SlideShare a Scribd company logo
1 of 19
Download to read offline
0
US. Department
- - i
of Transportation
Nalionai Highway
Traffic Saferfy Pe" 81111111" Pe"
Adminishafion ^iMnABLIBV
DOT HS 809188 January 2001
Age of Drinking Onset, Driving
After Drinking, and Involvement in
Alcohol-Related Motor
Vehicle Crashes
This document is available to the public from the National Technical Information Service, Springfield, Virginia 22161
This publication is distributed by the U.S. Department of
Transportation, National Highway Traffic Safety Administration,
in the interest of information exchange. The opinions, findings
and conclusions expressed in this publication are those of the
author(s) and not necessarily those of the Department of
Transportation or the National Highway Traffic Safety
Administration. The United States Government assumes no
liability for its content or use thereof. If trade or manufacturer s
names or products are mentioned, it is because they are considered
essential to the object of the publication and should not be construed
as an endorsement. The United States Government does not
endorse products or manufacturers.
Technical Report Documentation Page
1. Report No. 2. Government Accession No. 3. Recipient's Catalog No.
DOT HS 809 188
4. Title and Subtitle 5. Report Date
January 2001
Age of Drinking Onset, Driving After Drinking, and Involvement in 6. Performing Organization Code
Alcohol-Related Motor Vehicle Crashes
NTS-31
7. Author(s) 8. Performing Organization Report No.
Hingson, R., Sc.D.; Heeren, T., Ph.D.; Levenson, S., M.Ed., M.P.H.
Jamanka, A., M.P.H.; Voas, R. Ph.D.
9. Performing Organization Name and Address 10. Work Unit No. (TRAIS)
Social and Behavioral Sciences Department
Boston University School of Public Health
11. Contract or Grant No.
715 Albany
y Street,
, T 2W DTNH22-98-D-15079
Boston, Massachusetts 02118
12. Sponsoring Agency Name and Address 13. Type of Report and Period Covered
National Highway Traffic Safety Administration Final Report
Office of Program Development and Evaluation 14. Sponsoring Agency Code
400 7th Street, S.W., Washington, DC 20590
15. Supplementary Notes
Amy Berning was the Contracting Officer's Technical Representative for this project.
16. Abstract
This study assessed whether persons who begin drinking at younger ages are more likely to report drunk
driving and alcohol-related crash involvement over the life course, even after controlling analytically for
diagnosis of alcohol dependence and other personal characteristics associated with the age respondents started
drinking. A national survey conducted for the National Institute on Alcohol Abuse and Alcoholism in 1992,
asked 42,862 respondents the age they started drinking, whether they drove after drinking too much, and
whether they were in motor vehicle crash(s) because of their drinking. The current study found that, the
earlier the age respondents started drinking, the more likely they were to report driving after drinking too
much and being in a motor vehicle crash because of their drinking, even after adjusting for current/ever
diagnosis of alcohol dependence and other characteristics and behaviors associated with the age respondents
started drinking. Even among persons who were never alcohol dependent, those who began drinking in each
age group under 21, relative to those starting at age 21 or older, were more likely to report "ever" and "in the
past year" being in a crash after drinking too much. The traffic safety benefits of delaying drinking may
extend well beyond the legal drinking age of 21.
17. Key Words 18. Distribution Statement
This report is available from the National Technical Information
alcohol, drinking onset, driving after drinking Service, Springfield, Virginia 22161 (703) 605-6000. It is also
motor vehicle crashes available, free of charge, on the NHTSA web site at
www.nhtsa.dot.gov
19. Security Classif. 20. Security Classif. 21. No. of 22. Price
(of this report) (of this page) Pages
Unclassified Unclassified
Form DOT F 1700.7 (8-72) Reproduction of completed page authorized
ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS
The National Longitudinal Alcohol Epidemiology Study (NLAES) was sponsored by the
National Institute on Alcohol Abuse and Alcoholism (NIAAA). We would like to thank
Dr. Bridgett Grant at NIAAA for her help on this project. The National Highway Traffic
Safety Administration (NHTSA) sponsored the data analysis for this paper.
2
INTRODUCTION
To reduce alcohol-related fatal crashes among youth, all states have adopted a
minimum legal drinking age (MLDA) of 21. States adopting MLDAs of 21 in the early
1980s experienced a 10 - 15 percent decline in alcohol-related traffic deaths among
drivers in the targeted ages compared with states that did not adopt such laws (General
Accounting Office, 1987). The National Highway Traffic Safety Administration
(NHTSA) has estimated that the MLDA of 21 prevents 700-1,000 traffic deaths annually
among persons under 21, and that over 18,000 traffic deaths among persons under 21
have been prevented since 1975 (National Highway Traffic Safety Administration, 1999).
MLDA laws not only decreased drinking among persons under age 21, they also
lowered drinking among people age 21 to 25 who grew up in states with MLDAs of 21
relative to those who grew up in other states (O'Malley & Wagenaar, 1991). However, it
is not known whether these laws, and other efforts to reduce underage drinking, lower
both driving after drinking and alcohol-related motor vehicle crash involvement later in
life.
A recent analysis of the National Longitudinal Alcohol Epidemiologic Survey
(NLAES) found that age of drinking onset was strongly related to having experienced
alcohol dependence during one's life as defined by the diagnostic criteria of the
Diagnostic and Statistical Manual, Fourth Edition (DSM-IV) (American Psychiatric
Association, 1994). Among both males and females and persons with and without a
family history of alcoholism, persons who began drinking regularly before age 14 were at
least 3 times more likely than those who did not drink alcohol until they were over age 21
to experience diagnosable alcohol dependence during their life (Grant, 1998). Questions
in that survey permit us to explore whether age of drinking onset is similarly related to
whether respondents, ever in their life, and specifically during the year prior to the
survey, reported having:
- driven a motor vehicle after having too much to drink; and
- been in a motor vehicle crash because of their drinking
Independent of factors such as alcohol dependence that are related to early age of
drinking onset.
METHOD
The National Longitudinal Alcohol Epidemiologic Survey (NLAES) was a
national probability survey sponsored by the National Institute on Alcohol Abuse and
Alcoholism. In 1992, the U.S. Bureau of the Census administered face to face interviews
with 42,862 respondents age 18 and older, mean age 44, residing in the non-
institutionalized population of the contiguous states including the District of Columbia.
Sample Design:
The multi-stage sampling approach used for NLAES was described by Massey et.
al., (1989). Primary sampling units (PSUs) were stratified according to socioeconomic
criteria and were selected with a probability proportional to their population size. Within
PSUs, geographically defined secondary sampling units, referred to as segments, were
selected systematically for the sample. The African American population was
oversampled at this stage of the sample selection to secure adequate numbers for analytic
purposes. Segments were then divided into clusters of 4-8 housing units, and all
3
occupied housing units were included in the survey. Within each household, one
randomly selected person age 18 and older was selected to participate. Young adults age
18-29 were oversampled at a ratio of 2.25:1.00 at this stage of sample selection to include
a greater representation of this heavier drinking population subgroup. Weighting using
SUDAAN (Shah, 1996) adjusted for the deliberate oversampling of African American
and persons age 18-29 and accounted for the complex sampling design of NLAES. The
household response rate for this representative sample of the U.S. population was 91.9%
and the sample person response rate was 97.4%. The overall response rate was 90%. (A
longitudinal follow up was initially planned but not completed because of financial
constraints. A repeat cross sectional survey is currently being considered).
Outcome measures
Driving after drinking was explored by asking respondents, "In your entire life,
did you ever drive a car, motorcycle, truck or boat, or other vehicle after having too much
to drink? Did that happen in the past 12 months?" Alcohol-related crash involvement
was explored by asking respondents, "In your entire life, did you ever have a car,
motorcycle, truck, boat, or other accident because of your drinking? Did that happen in
the past 12 months?"
Predictor Variables
Measures of alcohol use and dependence were derived from the Alcohol Use
Disorder and Associated Disabilities Interview Schedule (AUDADIS) (Grant and Hasin,
1992); a fully structured diagnostic psychiatric interview designed to be administered by
trained interviewers who were not clinicians.
The age of drinking onset was ascertained by asking respondents how old they
were when they first started drinking, not counting small tastes or sips of alcohol.
Drinking onset data was collected from respondents who were classified as current
drinkers (persons who had consumed at least 12 drinks in the past 12 months) and former
drinkers (persons who had consumed at least 12 drinks in any one year of their lives but
not during the year prior to the interview). Only current and former drinkers were
included in the analysis. In an independent test retest study before the full NLAES was
conducted, the test retest reliability of the drinking onset variable was good with a Kappa
of .72 (Grant, Harford, Dawson et. al., 1995).
The definition of lifetime alcohol dependence was based on the diagnostic criteria
of the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual, Fourth Edition (DSM-IV) (American
Psychiatric Association, 1994). The AUDADIS interview included an extensive list of
symptom questions that operationalize the DSM-IV criteria for alcohol dependence.
Diagnosis of alcohol dependence required that in any one year a respondent meet at least
three of the following seven criteria for dependence: 1) tolerance, 2) withdrawal or
avoidance of withdrawal, 3) persistent desire or unsuccessful attempts to cut down or stop
drinking, 4) spending much time drinking, obtaining alcohol, or recovering from its
effects, 5) giving up or reducing occupational, social, or recreational activities in favor of
drinking, 6) impaired control over drinking and 7) continuing to drink despite a physical
or psychological problem caused or exacerbated by drinking. Respondents were
classified as showing a life time alcohol dependence diagnosis if they experienced an
episode of dependence in the past year or at any time before the past year. The
4
independent test retest study determined good reliabilities with Kappas of 0.76, and .0.73
for past year and prior to past.year dependence diagnoses respectively (Grant, Harford,
Dawson et. al., 1995).
The following demographic and behavior variables were examined as potential
confounders of the association between age of drinking onset and later behavior: current
age, sex, race/ethnicity (white non-hispanic, black non-hispanic, hispanic, other),
education, marital status, smoking status (current, former, never), illicit drug use status
(current, former, never) and alcohol dependency (current, former, never).
Statistical Analysis
Like other reports of the NLAES, our statistical analyses were conducted using
the SUDAAN statistical package (Shah, 1996) to account for the complex survey design
and oversampling of NLAES in the estimation of both effects and their standard errors
(the pattern of results was very similar with and without use of the SUDAAN weighting).
We focused on respondents who reported drinking ever in their lifetime (n=27,081). The
univariate associations between age of drinking onset and drinking and driving outcomes
and demographic and background behavior characteristics was tested using a modified
test of independence that adjusts for the sampling design. The test for independence in
SUDAAN is based on the Wald statistic comparing observed and expected values and
follows the strategy proposed by Koch, Freeman, and Freeman (1975) for analyzing
complex survey data. The p-value for this test is based on transforming the Wald
Statistic to an F-statistic where degrees of freedom represent the size of the contingency
table and the number of PSU's and strata in the study design. Logistic regression
explored whether age of drinking onset was associated with each of the drinking and
driving outcomes, controlling for potential confounding from demographic and behavior
characteristics including alcohol dependency. The overall significance of the relations
between different ages of drinking onset and study outcomes in the logistic models were
tested through a chi-square statistic comparing models with and without the set of
indicator variables representing age of onset. We examined whether the potential
association between age of drinking onset and drinking and driving outcomes persisted
after controlling for alcohol dependence because of the established relationships between
alcohol dependence and drinking and driving and between age of drinking onset and
alcohol dependency (Grant, 1998). We also examined the potential relationships between
age of drinking onset and drinking and driving behavior in the subset of respondents who
were never alcohol dependent (n=21,713).
RESULTS
Sixty-five percent of respondents ever drank alcohol, 49 percent before age 21 and
3 percent before age 14. The mean drinking onset age was 19.0 years. Four percent were
classified as alcohol dependent in the past year and 13 percent ever in their life.
Twenty-three percent of respondents reported driving a motor vehicle after having
too much to drink ever in their life and five percent in the past year. Four percent of
respondents reported ever having been in a motor vehicle crash because of their drinking,
0.2 percent in the past year. The earlier the age respondents began drinking, the greater
the proportions who reported driving after drinking too much and motor vehicle crash
involvement because of drinking ever in their lives and during the year prior to the
5
survey. As can be seen in Figure 1, persons who began drinking before age 14 were 3
times more likely than those who began drinking after age 21 to report ever driving after
drinking too much, 53 percent vs. 18 percent, and 4 times more likely to report doing so
in the past year, 13 percent vs. 3 percent. As seen in Figure 2, respondents who began
drinking before age fourteen were 7 times more likely than those who began drinking
after age 21 to report ever being in a motor vehicle crash because of their drinking, 14
percent vs. 2 percent, and during the past year, 0.7 percent vs. 0.1 percent. The
magnitude of differences tended to become smaller as the age of drinking onset became
closer to 21. As seen in Figures 1 and 2, even after age 21, the older respondents were
when they began to drink the less likely they were to drive after drinking or be in traffic
crashes after drinking.
Several subgroups in the study were significantly more likely to have begun
drinking at younger ages: male respondents, younger respondents, those with less than a
high school education, persons never married, and persons who currently or ever smoked
or used illicit drugs and respondents with a current (past year) or lifetime diagnoses of
alcohol dependence (all relations p<.001; data available upon request).
To control for these potentially confounding characteristics, we entered those
background characteristics into a series of multiple logistic regression analyses examining
the following outcomes as dichotomous variables: whether or not respondents drove after
drinking too much, or were in a motor vehicle crash because of their drinking. Each of
these outcomes was examined ever during the respondent's life and during the year prior
to the interview.
Respondents who began drinking at an earlier age were significantly more likely
to report that they drove after drinking too much ever and in the past year. Further, they
were significantly more likely to have ever been involved in a motor vehicle crash after
drinking too much. For each study outcome, the odds ratio and 95% confidence intervals
for respondents who began drinking each year from less than age 14 through 20 relative
to those who began at age 21 or older was examined separately. Persons starting at age
21 and older were analyzed as a group for comparison because the minimum legal
drinking age is 21. Results are summarized in Figures 3 and 4.
The strongest relationships were observed when comparing respondents who
started drinking at age 14 relative to those starting at age 21 and older. Persons who
began drinking when they were age 14 were 3.4 (95% CI 2.7, 4.3) times more likely than
those who began after age 21 to report ever driving after drinking too much and 2.33 (1.7,
3.2) times more likely to do so in the year prior to the survey. Further, they were 4.1
times more likely to report being in a motor vehicle crash after drinking too much ever
(95% CI 2.9, 5.8) and in the past year (95% CI 1.5, 10.9). As can be seen in the figures,
these relationships tended to be stronger for those whose age of drinking onset was
younger. Also the relations were stronger when we examined whether respondents ever
in their life drove after drinking too much or were ever in a motor vehicle crash because
of drinking as compared with those who experienced these outcomes in the past year.
Respondents who began drinking in each age group under 21 relative to those starting
after age 21 were significantly more likely to report ever driving after drinking too much
and ever being in a motor vehicle crash because of their drinking.
We also repeated the logistic regression analysis examining only the subgroup of
respondents who drank but never experienced alcohol dependence (n=21,462). Just over
6
half the sample and 79% of persons who ever drank this group represented 35% of those
who reported motor vehicle crashes after too much drinking ever during their life and
28% of those reporting such crashes in the year prior to the interview. Those who started
drinking in each age group under 21 were significantly more likely than those who started
drinking after age 21 to have ever and in the past year been in a motor vehicle crash after
drinking too much. (Figure 5).
DISCUSSION
The relative risk of involvement in fatal crashes increases with increasing driver
blood alcohol content and the risks increase more steeply for drivers under age 21 than
for older drivers (Zador, Krawchuk and Voas, 2000). Previous studies have shown that
raising the legal drinking age to 21 reduced drinking and alcohol related motor vehicle
crash involvement among persons under age 21 (General Accounting Office, 1987;
National Highway Traffic Safety Administration, 1999). Prior research also indicated that
persons who grew up in states with a legal drinking age of 21 relative to those who grew
up in states with lower legal drinking ages, not only drank less when they were under age
21, but also when they were age 21 to 25 (O'Malley & Wagenaar, 199 1). However, it is
not known whether efforts to reduce underage drinking lowers driving after drinking and
alcohol related motor vehicle crash involvement later in life.
This national study of persons 18 and older, mean age 44, found that the earlier
the age respondents started drinking, the more likely they were to report driving after
drinking too much ever in their life and in the past year. Also the earlier the age of
drinking onset, the greater the likelihood the respondents reported being in a motor
vehicle crash because of their drinking, even after we analytically controlled for
characteristics and behaviors associated with earlier drinking onset: whether or not
respondents ever or currently had an alcohol dependence diagnosis, age, gender,
education, race/ethnicity, marital status, smoking, or illicit drug use. A 4-fold increased
alcohol-related crash risk for those who began drinking at age 14 relative to those who
started after age 21 was found both ever in a respondent's life and during the year of the
survey suggesting the relationship may not be purely a function of having more years of
alcohol exposure. Also, the relationships were not solely a function of people who start
drinking at an early age being more likely to become alcohol dependent. Among persons
who were never alcohol dependent, relative to respondents who started drinking at age 21
or older, those who began drinking at age 14, and in each succeeding age up through age
20 were significantly more likely to report being in a motor vehicle crash after drinking
ever and during the year prior to the survey.
Several methodological factors should be considered when interpreting the study's
results. First, the distribution of responses on key drinking variables was similar in the
NLAES sample when compared to other large national surveys. The National
Longitudinal Survey of Labor Market Experience in Youth (NLSY)(N=12,686) was a
follow up study of a survey designed to represent the non institutionalized population of
young people age 14-21 as well as person 17-21 serving in the military in 1979. Grant,
Harford and Stinson (In Review) reported the ages respondents in that sample started
drinking. Compared to respondents in the NLAES who were in those age groups -in 1979
(N=4210) the proportion of drinkers in the NLSY who started drinking at <14, and each
consecutive year up to age 21 and older were virtually identical (data available upon
7
request). When compared to the 1998 National Health Interview study, an area
probability sample of over 100,000 persons nationwide, similar proportions of the
NLAES sample were classified as ever having drunk alcohol 65% vs. 71% or as having
met DSM IV alcohol dependence criteria 4.4% vs. 5.9%. In NLAES to be classified as
ever drinking a person had to drink at least 12 drinks in a given year. In the National
Alcohol Survey conducted in 1990 the proportion who met DSM N dependence criteria
was 3.9% (for data from the National Health Interview Study and the National Alcohol
Survey see U.S. Department of Health and Human Services, 1997). The proportion who
drove a motor vehicle after having too much to drink in the past year 5% in NLAES was
similar to the percentage reporting they drove while impaired by alcohol in the past
month 2.5% in CDC's 1993 national Behavioral Risk Factor Survey of over 102,263
adults 18 or older (Lui et.al., 1997).
Second, even though respondents said the motor vehicle crashes they experienced
were because of their drinking, it is impossible to be certain that alcohol was the sole
cause of the crash. However, alcohol use was present and the crash can be considered
alcohol-related.
Third questions were not asked about the amount that respondents drove in
general or crash involvement when they had not been drinking. Driving amounts could
have influenced the key findings of this study. Also it would be of value to assess
whether age of drinking onset was more closely related to alcohol related crash
involvement than involvement in crashes where alcohol was not a factor.
Fourth, questions regarding driving after drinking and motor vehicle crash
involvement after drinking could have been more objectively worded. In the NLAES
survey, respondents were asked if they drove after drinking too much ever and in the past
year and if they were in a motor vehicle crash because of their drinking ever and in the
past year. A more objective approach would have been to ask respondents if they drove
or were in a motor vehicle crash within an hour after consuming a specific number of
drinks. The way questions were worded in the NLAES suggests the relations observed
may have been stronger than reported. Persons who drive after drinking tend to believe
they can consume more drinks and still drive safely than people who do not drive after
drinking (National Highway Traffic Safety Administration, 1996). Consequently, they
may under report the actual frequency with which they drove after drinking or were in a
crash because of their drinking.
Fifth, there is the possibility that social desirability biases may have influenced
respondents. Those least likely to admit drinking at an early age may be least likely to
admit driving after drinking too much and being in a motor vehicle crash because of
drinking.
Sixth, the survey was cross sectional and older adults may have difficulty
remembering the age they first started to drink. Finally, because the survey was
confidential, efforts were not made to link self-reports: of crash involvement to driver
records. Longitudinal studies asking adolescents when they first began to drink with
more objective questions about driving after drinking and crash involvement and which
explore actual traffic records need to be conducted.
The results of this study point to a need to explore why people who start drinking
at an early age are more likely to report driving after drinking in the past year even among
persons who are not alcohol dependent. Several factors may contribute to the relation.
8
First, it is possible that people who engage in a variety of deviant or illegal behaviors at
an early age are more likely to engage in several such behaviors later in life. However,
the relationships in this study between early drinking onset and driving after drinking and
motor vehicle crash involvement were independent of current or past use of illegal drugs
or tobacco. Second, persons who start drinking at an early age more frequently drink
heavily than those who start consuming alcohol later, in life even if they are not alcohol
dependent (Hingson, Heeren, Jamanka, & Howland, In Press). This could increase the
likelihood that they would also drive more frequently after drinking. Also, persons who
begin drinking at an early age may be less likely to believe that driving after drinking
increases crash risks. They may believe for example, that the risk of traffic crashes and
other injuries increase only for people who are visibly intoxicated. They may derive
pleasure or a sense of self-esteem by taking driving risks. It is well known that people
who drive after drinking are more likely to speed and are less likely to wear seat belts.
Also, heavy consumption of alcohol may further impair the judgement of those who start
drinking at an earlier age. After drinking they may be less likely to appreciate their
increased crash risk than when sober. Whether those who start drinking at a younger age
are as aware of traffic laws and as likely to believe those laws will be enforced also
warrants exploration.
A recent report indicated that after a decade of decline, the percentage of high
school seniors who drink and drive has increased the last two years (O'Malley &
Johnston, 1999). The study reported here identifies another important reason to step up
enforcement of the legal drinking age of 21 and to expand educational and community
programs that delay the onset of drinking. The potential traffic safety benefits of
delaying underage drinking may extend well beyond age 21.
9
REFERENCES
American Psychiatric Association. (1994) Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental
Disorders (DSM IV). Washington, D.C.
General Accounting Office (1987) Drinking age laws: an evaluation synthesis of their
impact on highway safety. GAO PEMD 87-100, Washington, DC.
Grant B.F.; Harford T.C. and Stinson F. Age at onset of alcohol use and DSM IV alcohol
abuse and dependence, a twelve-year follow up, Journal of Substance Abuse (under
review).
Grant B.F., Harford F.C., Dawson D.A., Chou P.S. and Pickerling R.P. (1995) The
alcohol use disorder and associated disabilities interview schedule: reliability of alcohol
and drug modules in a general population sample, Drug and Alcohol Dependence. 39,
37-44.
Grant, B. (1998) The impact of family history of alcoholism on the relationship between
age at onset of alcohol use and DSM-III alcohol dependence, Alcohol Health and
Research World 22, 2, 144 - 147.
Grant, B. F. and Hasin, D.S. (1992) The alcohol use disorders and associated disabilities
interview schedule. National Institute on Alcohol Abuse and Alcoholism, Rockville,
Maryland.
Hingson R., Heeren T., Jamanka A., and Howland J., Age of drinking onset and
unintentional injury involvement after drinking, Journal of the American Medical
Association (In Press).
Koch G.G., Freeman D.H. and Freeman J.L. (1975) Strategies in the multi variate
analysis of data from complex surveys, International Statistical Review. 43, 59-78.
Lui S., Seigel P., Brewer R., Mokdad A, Sleet D., and Serduda M., (1997) Prevalence of
alcohol impaired driving: results from a national self reported survey, Journal of the
American Medical Association. 272, 2, 123-125.
Massey, J.T., Parsons, V.L., and Tadros W. (1989) Design and estimation for the
national health interview survey, 1985 - 1994, Vital and Health Statistics Report Series
2(10), National Center for Health Statistics, U.S. Department of Health and Human
Services, Hyattsville, MD
National Highway Traffic Safety Administration (1996) National Survey of Drinking and
Driving Attitudes and Behavior, DOT HS 808 438. U.S. Department of Transportation,
Washington, DC.
National Highway Traffic Safety Administration (1999) Traffic Safety Facts, 1998:
Alcohol, DOT HS 808 950. U.S. Department of Transportation, Washington, DC.
10
O'Malley, P. and Wagenaar, A. (1991) Effects of minimum drinking age laws on alcohol
use, related behaviors, and traffic crash involvement among American youth., J. of
Alcohol Studies. 52,478 - 491.
O'Malley, P.M. and Johnston, L.D. (1999) Drinking and driving among high school
seniors. American Journal of Public Health. 89, 5, 678-684.
Shah, B.V., Barnwell, B.G., and Bieler, G.S. (1996) SUDAAN User's Manual, Release
7.0,. Research Triangle Institute, Research Triangle Park, NC.
U.S. Department of Health and Human Services Office of the Secretary (1997) Ninth
special report to the U.S. Congress on alcohol and health June 1997, Page 19 NIH
Publication Number 97-4017, NIH, NIAAA.
Zador P., Krawchuck S., and Was R. (2000) Alcohol related relative risk of driver
fatalities and driver involvement in fatal crashes in relation to driver age and gender,
Journal Studies on Alcohol, 61: 387-395.
11
*
Figure I
Drove a Motor Vehicle After Drinking Too
Much According to Age of Drinking Onset
*
Percent
60
60 54 *
53
*
-^- Ever
50
50
*
Past Year
40
*
35
33
30
*
26
20 *
20 -
13 16 18
13 *
11
10 ^
3
0---
Age Started<14 14 15 16 17 18 19 20 21+
DrinWg
1131 844 1507 3161 2866 5699 2213 2082. 2975
P<.001
*
Figure 2
In a Motor Vehicle Crash Because of
Drinking According to Age of Drinking Onset
15 la 14
14
13
12
Ever
ll
10
-^- Past Year
9 *
8
7
6
5 _ .
a
3
1.8 2
0.7 0.8 0.6 0.3 u 0.3 0.2
Age SParted
14 15 16 17 18 19 20 21+
Drinkingl4
N= 1131 844 1507 3161 2866 5699 2213 2082 2975
<.001
*
Figure 3: Drove After Drinking Too Much
According to Age of Drinking Onset (NLAES)
Odds Ratio and
Confidence Intervals
7T
6+ Ever Past Year
*
*
5
4 4
3
5
2 2
1
0i i
0
'
1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1
1
<14 14 15 16 17 18 19 20 21+ <14 14 15 16 17 18 19 20 21+
p<.00'
Age Started p<.001
Drinking •Each group compared to those who started drinking at age 21+.
•Controlling for: ever/past year/never alcohol dependent, age, sex, race/ethnicity,
education, marital status, smoking, and illicit drug use.
*
Figure 4: In a Motor Vehicle Accident Because
of Drinking Too Much According to Age of
Drinking Onset (NLAES)
Odds Ratio and 10,91
7T Confidence Intervals
7T
6 Ever 6f Past Year
5
*
5f
*
4+ 4,1
3+ 3f
2+ 2+
1.4 ,4 •
1+ 1+
0
0 I }
1 I I I I I i +
<14 14 15 16 17 18 19 20 21+ <14 14 15 16 17 18 19 20 21+
p<.OOI
Age Started N.S.
Drinking Each group compared to those who started drinking at age 21+.
•Controlling for: ever/past year/never alcohol dependent, age, sex, race/ethnicity,
education, marital status, smoking, and illicit drug use.
*
Figure 5: In a Motor Vehicle Crash After Drinking Too Much
According to Age of Drinking Onset (Subjects Who Were
Never Alcohol Dependent)
Odds Ratio and 84a
Confidence Intervals 5&8
24,.6
20--
*
*
18,9
16--
Past Year
4 Ever 14.3
124 * *
to--
*
8+
6 6+
4+
3,4
2 2+
0 I-------f
I I I
0 I I I
^ I I 1 ' I I I I
<14 14 15 16 17 18 19 20 21+ 14 15 16 17 18 19 20 21+
Age Started p<,po1 P<.001
'Controlling for: age, gender, race/ethnicity, education, marital status, smoking, and drug use.
Drinking
*Each group compared to those who started drinking at age 21+.
0
U.S. Department
of Transportation
National Hisl way
Administration
DOT HS 809 188
January 2001

More Related Content

Similar to Age of drinking onset, driving after drinking, and involvement in alcohol related motor-vehicle crashes.pdf

State profile -_new_york_0
State profile -_new_york_0State profile -_new_york_0
State profile -_new_york_0satoriwatersfl
 
Driving Home the Point: Medicines, Impaired Driving, and You
Driving Home the Point: Medicines, Impaired Driving, and YouDriving Home the Point: Medicines, Impaired Driving, and You
Driving Home the Point: Medicines, Impaired Driving, and YouRADDOrg
 
Seminar paper 5
Seminar paper 5Seminar paper 5
Seminar paper 5juilice
 
242019 Download DocumentfileCUsersCarolynDocumen.docx
242019 Download DocumentfileCUsersCarolynDocumen.docx242019 Download DocumentfileCUsersCarolynDocumen.docx
242019 Download DocumentfileCUsersCarolynDocumen.docxvickeryr87
 
ECON104PaperRoughDraft2
ECON104PaperRoughDraft2ECON104PaperRoughDraft2
ECON104PaperRoughDraft2John Nguyen
 
Drunk Driving Accidents in the U.S.
Drunk Driving Accidents in the U.S.Drunk Driving Accidents in the U.S.
Drunk Driving Accidents in the U.S.Mark_Carnell
 
Substance Abuse Midland, Michigan
Substance Abuse Midland, MichiganSubstance Abuse Midland, Michigan
Substance Abuse Midland, Michiganrecoveryrestart2
 
Effectiveness of the minimum legal drinking age
Effectiveness of the minimum legal drinking ageEffectiveness of the minimum legal drinking age
Effectiveness of the minimum legal drinking ageChristopher Christensen
 
The effects of alcohol advertising on youth attitudes toward drinking and dri...
The effects of alcohol advertising on youth attitudes toward drinking and dri...The effects of alcohol advertising on youth attitudes toward drinking and dri...
The effects of alcohol advertising on youth attitudes toward drinking and dri...Peachy Essay
 
Age of Responsibility Reformation
Age of Responsibility ReformationAge of Responsibility Reformation
Age of Responsibility ReformationChristopher Kemmett
 
Magellan Strategies EPA Regulation Survey Press Release 061214
Magellan Strategies EPA Regulation Survey Press Release 061214Magellan Strategies EPA Regulation Survey Press Release 061214
Magellan Strategies EPA Regulation Survey Press Release 061214Magellan Strategies
 
PR Binge Drinking Campaign
PR Binge Drinking CampaignPR Binge Drinking Campaign
PR Binge Drinking CampaignJessica Grocock
 
The wichita anti drunk driving campaign-final version
The wichita anti drunk driving campaign-final versionThe wichita anti drunk driving campaign-final version
The wichita anti drunk driving campaign-final versionWilkes University
 
(Alsaab) 5Mohammed AlsaabENG 1225Professor Lee Hinds4 Ma.docx
(Alsaab) 5Mohammed AlsaabENG 1225Professor Lee Hinds4 Ma.docx(Alsaab) 5Mohammed AlsaabENG 1225Professor Lee Hinds4 Ma.docx
(Alsaab) 5Mohammed AlsaabENG 1225Professor Lee Hinds4 Ma.docxkatherncarlyle
 

Similar to Age of drinking onset, driving after drinking, and involvement in alcohol related motor-vehicle crashes.pdf (20)

State profile -_new_york_0
State profile -_new_york_0State profile -_new_york_0
State profile -_new_york_0
 
Driving Home the Point: Medicines, Impaired Driving, and You
Driving Home the Point: Medicines, Impaired Driving, and YouDriving Home the Point: Medicines, Impaired Driving, and You
Driving Home the Point: Medicines, Impaired Driving, and You
 
FINAL POSTER
FINAL POSTERFINAL POSTER
FINAL POSTER
 
drug_toxicology_for_prosecutors_04
drug_toxicology_for_prosecutors_04drug_toxicology_for_prosecutors_04
drug_toxicology_for_prosecutors_04
 
AAA Driving & THC
AAA Driving & THCAAA Driving & THC
AAA Driving & THC
 
Seminar paper 5
Seminar paper 5Seminar paper 5
Seminar paper 5
 
Smoking and kids
Smoking and kidsSmoking and kids
Smoking and kids
 
Causal Argument Essay
Causal Argument EssayCausal Argument Essay
Causal Argument Essay
 
242019 Download DocumentfileCUsersCarolynDocumen.docx
242019 Download DocumentfileCUsersCarolynDocumen.docx242019 Download DocumentfileCUsersCarolynDocumen.docx
242019 Download DocumentfileCUsersCarolynDocumen.docx
 
ECON104PaperRoughDraft2
ECON104PaperRoughDraft2ECON104PaperRoughDraft2
ECON104PaperRoughDraft2
 
Drunk Driving Accidents in the U.S.
Drunk Driving Accidents in the U.S.Drunk Driving Accidents in the U.S.
Drunk Driving Accidents in the U.S.
 
Substance Abuse Midland, Michigan
Substance Abuse Midland, MichiganSubstance Abuse Midland, Michigan
Substance Abuse Midland, Michigan
 
Effectiveness of the minimum legal drinking age
Effectiveness of the minimum legal drinking ageEffectiveness of the minimum legal drinking age
Effectiveness of the minimum legal drinking age
 
The effects of alcohol advertising on youth attitudes toward drinking and dri...
The effects of alcohol advertising on youth attitudes toward drinking and dri...The effects of alcohol advertising on youth attitudes toward drinking and dri...
The effects of alcohol advertising on youth attitudes toward drinking and dri...
 
Age of Responsibility Reformation
Age of Responsibility ReformationAge of Responsibility Reformation
Age of Responsibility Reformation
 
Magellan Strategies EPA Regulation Survey Press Release 061214
Magellan Strategies EPA Regulation Survey Press Release 061214Magellan Strategies EPA Regulation Survey Press Release 061214
Magellan Strategies EPA Regulation Survey Press Release 061214
 
PR Binge Drinking Campaign
PR Binge Drinking CampaignPR Binge Drinking Campaign
PR Binge Drinking Campaign
 
The wichita anti drunk driving campaign-final version
The wichita anti drunk driving campaign-final versionThe wichita anti drunk driving campaign-final version
The wichita anti drunk driving campaign-final version
 
(Alsaab) 5Mohammed AlsaabENG 1225Professor Lee Hinds4 Ma.docx
(Alsaab) 5Mohammed AlsaabENG 1225Professor Lee Hinds4 Ma.docx(Alsaab) 5Mohammed AlsaabENG 1225Professor Lee Hinds4 Ma.docx
(Alsaab) 5Mohammed AlsaabENG 1225Professor Lee Hinds4 Ma.docx
 
Wishahy, Sarah CPM Capstone
Wishahy, Sarah CPM CapstoneWishahy, Sarah CPM Capstone
Wishahy, Sarah CPM Capstone
 

More from Amber Ford

Writing Paper Background - Crumpled Writing Paper B
Writing Paper Background - Crumpled Writing Paper BWriting Paper Background - Crumpled Writing Paper B
Writing Paper Background - Crumpled Writing Paper BAmber Ford
 
022 Essay Example Page 4 Narrative Thatsnotus
022 Essay Example Page 4 Narrative Thatsnotus022 Essay Example Page 4 Narrative Thatsnotus
022 Essay Example Page 4 Narrative ThatsnotusAmber Ford
 
WDLJ Why DonT You Learn Japanese () Kanji
WDLJ Why DonT You Learn Japanese () KanjiWDLJ Why DonT You Learn Japanese () Kanji
WDLJ Why DonT You Learn Japanese () KanjiAmber Ford
 
Essay Title - Center For Student Success
Essay Title - Center For Student SuccessEssay Title - Center For Student Success
Essay Title - Center For Student SuccessAmber Ford
 
Begin Your Essay With A. Online assignment writing service.
Begin Your Essay With A. Online assignment writing service.Begin Your Essay With A. Online assignment writing service.
Begin Your Essay With A. Online assignment writing service.Amber Ford
 
2Nd Grade Writing Worksheets - Best Colorin
2Nd Grade Writing Worksheets - Best Colorin2Nd Grade Writing Worksheets - Best Colorin
2Nd Grade Writing Worksheets - Best ColorinAmber Ford
 
002 Essay Example Rogerian Thatsnotus. Online assignment writing service.
002 Essay Example Rogerian Thatsnotus. Online assignment writing service.002 Essay Example Rogerian Thatsnotus. Online assignment writing service.
002 Essay Example Rogerian Thatsnotus. Online assignment writing service.Amber Ford
 
How To Format A Narrative Essay - Neuro Gastron
How To Format A Narrative Essay - Neuro GastronHow To Format A Narrative Essay - Neuro Gastron
How To Format A Narrative Essay - Neuro GastronAmber Ford
 
Be Your Ghost Writer, Paper Back Writer, Non Fiction Writer And Editor
Be Your Ghost Writer, Paper Back Writer, Non Fiction Writer And EditorBe Your Ghost Writer, Paper Back Writer, Non Fiction Writer And Editor
Be Your Ghost Writer, Paper Back Writer, Non Fiction Writer And EditorAmber Ford
 
Scientific Writing. Online assignment writing service.
Scientific Writing. Online assignment writing service.Scientific Writing. Online assignment writing service.
Scientific Writing. Online assignment writing service.Amber Ford
 
Essay Speech Format Spm E. Online assignment writing service.
Essay Speech Format Spm E. Online assignment writing service.Essay Speech Format Spm E. Online assignment writing service.
Essay Speech Format Spm E. Online assignment writing service.Amber Ford
 
How To Write An ACT Essay Writing Tips With Great Examples
How To Write An ACT Essay Writing Tips With Great ExamplesHow To Write An ACT Essay Writing Tips With Great Examples
How To Write An ACT Essay Writing Tips With Great ExamplesAmber Ford
 
Cheap Essay Writing Service Writing Services, Paper Writing Service
Cheap Essay Writing Service Writing Services, Paper Writing ServiceCheap Essay Writing Service Writing Services, Paper Writing Service
Cheap Essay Writing Service Writing Services, Paper Writing ServiceAmber Ford
 
Magic Essay Typer Plagiarism EssayTyper,
Magic Essay Typer Plagiarism EssayTyper,Magic Essay Typer Plagiarism EssayTyper,
Magic Essay Typer Plagiarism EssayTyper,Amber Ford
 
Opinion Paper Topics. 70 Interesting Opinion Essay
Opinion Paper Topics. 70 Interesting Opinion EssayOpinion Paper Topics. 70 Interesting Opinion Essay
Opinion Paper Topics. 70 Interesting Opinion EssayAmber Ford
 
Best Free Essay Sites. Online assignment writing service.
Best Free Essay Sites. Online assignment writing service.Best Free Essay Sites. Online assignment writing service.
Best Free Essay Sites. Online assignment writing service.Amber Ford
 
Reflection Essay How To Write An Argument Essay O
Reflection Essay How To Write An Argument Essay OReflection Essay How To Write An Argument Essay O
Reflection Essay How To Write An Argument Essay OAmber Ford
 
College Essay Review Service 48-Hour Essay R
College Essay Review Service 48-Hour Essay RCollege Essay Review Service 48-Hour Essay R
College Essay Review Service 48-Hour Essay RAmber Ford
 
Descriptive Essay Political Science Quantitative Resear
Descriptive Essay Political Science Quantitative ResearDescriptive Essay Political Science Quantitative Resear
Descriptive Essay Political Science Quantitative ResearAmber Ford
 
StudentS Guide To Writing College Papers Fourth Ed
StudentS Guide To Writing College Papers Fourth EdStudentS Guide To Writing College Papers Fourth Ed
StudentS Guide To Writing College Papers Fourth EdAmber Ford
 

More from Amber Ford (20)

Writing Paper Background - Crumpled Writing Paper B
Writing Paper Background - Crumpled Writing Paper BWriting Paper Background - Crumpled Writing Paper B
Writing Paper Background - Crumpled Writing Paper B
 
022 Essay Example Page 4 Narrative Thatsnotus
022 Essay Example Page 4 Narrative Thatsnotus022 Essay Example Page 4 Narrative Thatsnotus
022 Essay Example Page 4 Narrative Thatsnotus
 
WDLJ Why DonT You Learn Japanese () Kanji
WDLJ Why DonT You Learn Japanese () KanjiWDLJ Why DonT You Learn Japanese () Kanji
WDLJ Why DonT You Learn Japanese () Kanji
 
Essay Title - Center For Student Success
Essay Title - Center For Student SuccessEssay Title - Center For Student Success
Essay Title - Center For Student Success
 
Begin Your Essay With A. Online assignment writing service.
Begin Your Essay With A. Online assignment writing service.Begin Your Essay With A. Online assignment writing service.
Begin Your Essay With A. Online assignment writing service.
 
2Nd Grade Writing Worksheets - Best Colorin
2Nd Grade Writing Worksheets - Best Colorin2Nd Grade Writing Worksheets - Best Colorin
2Nd Grade Writing Worksheets - Best Colorin
 
002 Essay Example Rogerian Thatsnotus. Online assignment writing service.
002 Essay Example Rogerian Thatsnotus. Online assignment writing service.002 Essay Example Rogerian Thatsnotus. Online assignment writing service.
002 Essay Example Rogerian Thatsnotus. Online assignment writing service.
 
How To Format A Narrative Essay - Neuro Gastron
How To Format A Narrative Essay - Neuro GastronHow To Format A Narrative Essay - Neuro Gastron
How To Format A Narrative Essay - Neuro Gastron
 
Be Your Ghost Writer, Paper Back Writer, Non Fiction Writer And Editor
Be Your Ghost Writer, Paper Back Writer, Non Fiction Writer And EditorBe Your Ghost Writer, Paper Back Writer, Non Fiction Writer And Editor
Be Your Ghost Writer, Paper Back Writer, Non Fiction Writer And Editor
 
Scientific Writing. Online assignment writing service.
Scientific Writing. Online assignment writing service.Scientific Writing. Online assignment writing service.
Scientific Writing. Online assignment writing service.
 
Essay Speech Format Spm E. Online assignment writing service.
Essay Speech Format Spm E. Online assignment writing service.Essay Speech Format Spm E. Online assignment writing service.
Essay Speech Format Spm E. Online assignment writing service.
 
How To Write An ACT Essay Writing Tips With Great Examples
How To Write An ACT Essay Writing Tips With Great ExamplesHow To Write An ACT Essay Writing Tips With Great Examples
How To Write An ACT Essay Writing Tips With Great Examples
 
Cheap Essay Writing Service Writing Services, Paper Writing Service
Cheap Essay Writing Service Writing Services, Paper Writing ServiceCheap Essay Writing Service Writing Services, Paper Writing Service
Cheap Essay Writing Service Writing Services, Paper Writing Service
 
Magic Essay Typer Plagiarism EssayTyper,
Magic Essay Typer Plagiarism EssayTyper,Magic Essay Typer Plagiarism EssayTyper,
Magic Essay Typer Plagiarism EssayTyper,
 
Opinion Paper Topics. 70 Interesting Opinion Essay
Opinion Paper Topics. 70 Interesting Opinion EssayOpinion Paper Topics. 70 Interesting Opinion Essay
Opinion Paper Topics. 70 Interesting Opinion Essay
 
Best Free Essay Sites. Online assignment writing service.
Best Free Essay Sites. Online assignment writing service.Best Free Essay Sites. Online assignment writing service.
Best Free Essay Sites. Online assignment writing service.
 
Reflection Essay How To Write An Argument Essay O
Reflection Essay How To Write An Argument Essay OReflection Essay How To Write An Argument Essay O
Reflection Essay How To Write An Argument Essay O
 
College Essay Review Service 48-Hour Essay R
College Essay Review Service 48-Hour Essay RCollege Essay Review Service 48-Hour Essay R
College Essay Review Service 48-Hour Essay R
 
Descriptive Essay Political Science Quantitative Resear
Descriptive Essay Political Science Quantitative ResearDescriptive Essay Political Science Quantitative Resear
Descriptive Essay Political Science Quantitative Resear
 
StudentS Guide To Writing College Papers Fourth Ed
StudentS Guide To Writing College Papers Fourth EdStudentS Guide To Writing College Papers Fourth Ed
StudentS Guide To Writing College Papers Fourth Ed
 

Recently uploaded

Unit-IV- Pharma. Marketing Channels.pptx
Unit-IV- Pharma. Marketing Channels.pptxUnit-IV- Pharma. Marketing Channels.pptx
Unit-IV- Pharma. Marketing Channels.pptxVishalSingh1417
 
Disha NEET Physics Guide for classes 11 and 12.pdf
Disha NEET Physics Guide for classes 11 and 12.pdfDisha NEET Physics Guide for classes 11 and 12.pdf
Disha NEET Physics Guide for classes 11 and 12.pdfchloefrazer622
 
BAG TECHNIQUE Bag technique-a tool making use of public health bag through wh...
BAG TECHNIQUE Bag technique-a tool making use of public health bag through wh...BAG TECHNIQUE Bag technique-a tool making use of public health bag through wh...
BAG TECHNIQUE Bag technique-a tool making use of public health bag through wh...Sapna Thakur
 
Measures of Central Tendency: Mean, Median and Mode
Measures of Central Tendency: Mean, Median and ModeMeasures of Central Tendency: Mean, Median and Mode
Measures of Central Tendency: Mean, Median and ModeThiyagu K
 
Nutritional Needs Presentation - HLTH 104
Nutritional Needs Presentation - HLTH 104Nutritional Needs Presentation - HLTH 104
Nutritional Needs Presentation - HLTH 104misteraugie
 
microwave assisted reaction. General introduction
microwave assisted reaction. General introductionmicrowave assisted reaction. General introduction
microwave assisted reaction. General introductionMaksud Ahmed
 
Explore beautiful and ugly buildings. Mathematics helps us create beautiful d...
Explore beautiful and ugly buildings. Mathematics helps us create beautiful d...Explore beautiful and ugly buildings. Mathematics helps us create beautiful d...
Explore beautiful and ugly buildings. Mathematics helps us create beautiful d...christianmathematics
 
Activity 01 - Artificial Culture (1).pdf
Activity 01 - Artificial Culture (1).pdfActivity 01 - Artificial Culture (1).pdf
Activity 01 - Artificial Culture (1).pdfciinovamais
 
Arihant handbook biology for class 11 .pdf
Arihant handbook biology for class 11 .pdfArihant handbook biology for class 11 .pdf
Arihant handbook biology for class 11 .pdfchloefrazer622
 
Web & Social Media Analytics Previous Year Question Paper.pdf
Web & Social Media Analytics Previous Year Question Paper.pdfWeb & Social Media Analytics Previous Year Question Paper.pdf
Web & Social Media Analytics Previous Year Question Paper.pdfJayanti Pande
 
Introduction to Nonprofit Accounting: The Basics
Introduction to Nonprofit Accounting: The BasicsIntroduction to Nonprofit Accounting: The Basics
Introduction to Nonprofit Accounting: The BasicsTechSoup
 
APM Welcome, APM North West Network Conference, Synergies Across Sectors
APM Welcome, APM North West Network Conference, Synergies Across SectorsAPM Welcome, APM North West Network Conference, Synergies Across Sectors
APM Welcome, APM North West Network Conference, Synergies Across SectorsAssociation for Project Management
 
Interactive Powerpoint_How to Master effective communication
Interactive Powerpoint_How to Master effective communicationInteractive Powerpoint_How to Master effective communication
Interactive Powerpoint_How to Master effective communicationnomboosow
 
Q4-W6-Restating Informational Text Grade 3
Q4-W6-Restating Informational Text Grade 3Q4-W6-Restating Informational Text Grade 3
Q4-W6-Restating Informational Text Grade 3JemimahLaneBuaron
 
Advanced Views - Calendar View in Odoo 17
Advanced Views - Calendar View in Odoo 17Advanced Views - Calendar View in Odoo 17
Advanced Views - Calendar View in Odoo 17Celine George
 
General AI for Medical Educators April 2024
General AI for Medical Educators April 2024General AI for Medical Educators April 2024
General AI for Medical Educators April 2024Janet Corral
 
Measures of Dispersion and Variability: Range, QD, AD and SD
Measures of Dispersion and Variability: Range, QD, AD and SDMeasures of Dispersion and Variability: Range, QD, AD and SD
Measures of Dispersion and Variability: Range, QD, AD and SDThiyagu K
 
Software Engineering Methodologies (overview)
Software Engineering Methodologies (overview)Software Engineering Methodologies (overview)
Software Engineering Methodologies (overview)eniolaolutunde
 

Recently uploaded (20)

Unit-IV- Pharma. Marketing Channels.pptx
Unit-IV- Pharma. Marketing Channels.pptxUnit-IV- Pharma. Marketing Channels.pptx
Unit-IV- Pharma. Marketing Channels.pptx
 
Advance Mobile Application Development class 07
Advance Mobile Application Development class 07Advance Mobile Application Development class 07
Advance Mobile Application Development class 07
 
Disha NEET Physics Guide for classes 11 and 12.pdf
Disha NEET Physics Guide for classes 11 and 12.pdfDisha NEET Physics Guide for classes 11 and 12.pdf
Disha NEET Physics Guide for classes 11 and 12.pdf
 
Mattingly "AI & Prompt Design: Structured Data, Assistants, & RAG"
Mattingly "AI & Prompt Design: Structured Data, Assistants, & RAG"Mattingly "AI & Prompt Design: Structured Data, Assistants, & RAG"
Mattingly "AI & Prompt Design: Structured Data, Assistants, & RAG"
 
BAG TECHNIQUE Bag technique-a tool making use of public health bag through wh...
BAG TECHNIQUE Bag technique-a tool making use of public health bag through wh...BAG TECHNIQUE Bag technique-a tool making use of public health bag through wh...
BAG TECHNIQUE Bag technique-a tool making use of public health bag through wh...
 
Measures of Central Tendency: Mean, Median and Mode
Measures of Central Tendency: Mean, Median and ModeMeasures of Central Tendency: Mean, Median and Mode
Measures of Central Tendency: Mean, Median and Mode
 
Nutritional Needs Presentation - HLTH 104
Nutritional Needs Presentation - HLTH 104Nutritional Needs Presentation - HLTH 104
Nutritional Needs Presentation - HLTH 104
 
microwave assisted reaction. General introduction
microwave assisted reaction. General introductionmicrowave assisted reaction. General introduction
microwave assisted reaction. General introduction
 
Explore beautiful and ugly buildings. Mathematics helps us create beautiful d...
Explore beautiful and ugly buildings. Mathematics helps us create beautiful d...Explore beautiful and ugly buildings. Mathematics helps us create beautiful d...
Explore beautiful and ugly buildings. Mathematics helps us create beautiful d...
 
Activity 01 - Artificial Culture (1).pdf
Activity 01 - Artificial Culture (1).pdfActivity 01 - Artificial Culture (1).pdf
Activity 01 - Artificial Culture (1).pdf
 
Arihant handbook biology for class 11 .pdf
Arihant handbook biology for class 11 .pdfArihant handbook biology for class 11 .pdf
Arihant handbook biology for class 11 .pdf
 
Web & Social Media Analytics Previous Year Question Paper.pdf
Web & Social Media Analytics Previous Year Question Paper.pdfWeb & Social Media Analytics Previous Year Question Paper.pdf
Web & Social Media Analytics Previous Year Question Paper.pdf
 
Introduction to Nonprofit Accounting: The Basics
Introduction to Nonprofit Accounting: The BasicsIntroduction to Nonprofit Accounting: The Basics
Introduction to Nonprofit Accounting: The Basics
 
APM Welcome, APM North West Network Conference, Synergies Across Sectors
APM Welcome, APM North West Network Conference, Synergies Across SectorsAPM Welcome, APM North West Network Conference, Synergies Across Sectors
APM Welcome, APM North West Network Conference, Synergies Across Sectors
 
Interactive Powerpoint_How to Master effective communication
Interactive Powerpoint_How to Master effective communicationInteractive Powerpoint_How to Master effective communication
Interactive Powerpoint_How to Master effective communication
 
Q4-W6-Restating Informational Text Grade 3
Q4-W6-Restating Informational Text Grade 3Q4-W6-Restating Informational Text Grade 3
Q4-W6-Restating Informational Text Grade 3
 
Advanced Views - Calendar View in Odoo 17
Advanced Views - Calendar View in Odoo 17Advanced Views - Calendar View in Odoo 17
Advanced Views - Calendar View in Odoo 17
 
General AI for Medical Educators April 2024
General AI for Medical Educators April 2024General AI for Medical Educators April 2024
General AI for Medical Educators April 2024
 
Measures of Dispersion and Variability: Range, QD, AD and SD
Measures of Dispersion and Variability: Range, QD, AD and SDMeasures of Dispersion and Variability: Range, QD, AD and SD
Measures of Dispersion and Variability: Range, QD, AD and SD
 
Software Engineering Methodologies (overview)
Software Engineering Methodologies (overview)Software Engineering Methodologies (overview)
Software Engineering Methodologies (overview)
 

Age of drinking onset, driving after drinking, and involvement in alcohol related motor-vehicle crashes.pdf

  • 1. 0 US. Department - - i of Transportation Nalionai Highway Traffic Saferfy Pe" 81111111" Pe" Adminishafion ^iMnABLIBV DOT HS 809188 January 2001 Age of Drinking Onset, Driving After Drinking, and Involvement in Alcohol-Related Motor Vehicle Crashes This document is available to the public from the National Technical Information Service, Springfield, Virginia 22161
  • 2. This publication is distributed by the U.S. Department of Transportation, National Highway Traffic Safety Administration, in the interest of information exchange. The opinions, findings and conclusions expressed in this publication are those of the author(s) and not necessarily those of the Department of Transportation or the National Highway Traffic Safety Administration. The United States Government assumes no liability for its content or use thereof. If trade or manufacturer s names or products are mentioned, it is because they are considered essential to the object of the publication and should not be construed as an endorsement. The United States Government does not endorse products or manufacturers.
  • 3. Technical Report Documentation Page 1. Report No. 2. Government Accession No. 3. Recipient's Catalog No. DOT HS 809 188 4. Title and Subtitle 5. Report Date January 2001 Age of Drinking Onset, Driving After Drinking, and Involvement in 6. Performing Organization Code Alcohol-Related Motor Vehicle Crashes NTS-31 7. Author(s) 8. Performing Organization Report No. Hingson, R., Sc.D.; Heeren, T., Ph.D.; Levenson, S., M.Ed., M.P.H. Jamanka, A., M.P.H.; Voas, R. Ph.D. 9. Performing Organization Name and Address 10. Work Unit No. (TRAIS) Social and Behavioral Sciences Department Boston University School of Public Health 11. Contract or Grant No. 715 Albany y Street, , T 2W DTNH22-98-D-15079 Boston, Massachusetts 02118 12. Sponsoring Agency Name and Address 13. Type of Report and Period Covered National Highway Traffic Safety Administration Final Report Office of Program Development and Evaluation 14. Sponsoring Agency Code 400 7th Street, S.W., Washington, DC 20590 15. Supplementary Notes Amy Berning was the Contracting Officer's Technical Representative for this project. 16. Abstract This study assessed whether persons who begin drinking at younger ages are more likely to report drunk driving and alcohol-related crash involvement over the life course, even after controlling analytically for diagnosis of alcohol dependence and other personal characteristics associated with the age respondents started drinking. A national survey conducted for the National Institute on Alcohol Abuse and Alcoholism in 1992, asked 42,862 respondents the age they started drinking, whether they drove after drinking too much, and whether they were in motor vehicle crash(s) because of their drinking. The current study found that, the earlier the age respondents started drinking, the more likely they were to report driving after drinking too much and being in a motor vehicle crash because of their drinking, even after adjusting for current/ever diagnosis of alcohol dependence and other characteristics and behaviors associated with the age respondents started drinking. Even among persons who were never alcohol dependent, those who began drinking in each age group under 21, relative to those starting at age 21 or older, were more likely to report "ever" and "in the past year" being in a crash after drinking too much. The traffic safety benefits of delaying drinking may extend well beyond the legal drinking age of 21. 17. Key Words 18. Distribution Statement This report is available from the National Technical Information alcohol, drinking onset, driving after drinking Service, Springfield, Virginia 22161 (703) 605-6000. It is also motor vehicle crashes available, free of charge, on the NHTSA web site at www.nhtsa.dot.gov 19. Security Classif. 20. Security Classif. 21. No. of 22. Price (of this report) (of this page) Pages Unclassified Unclassified Form DOT F 1700.7 (8-72) Reproduction of completed page authorized
  • 4. ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS The National Longitudinal Alcohol Epidemiology Study (NLAES) was sponsored by the National Institute on Alcohol Abuse and Alcoholism (NIAAA). We would like to thank Dr. Bridgett Grant at NIAAA for her help on this project. The National Highway Traffic Safety Administration (NHTSA) sponsored the data analysis for this paper. 2
  • 5. INTRODUCTION To reduce alcohol-related fatal crashes among youth, all states have adopted a minimum legal drinking age (MLDA) of 21. States adopting MLDAs of 21 in the early 1980s experienced a 10 - 15 percent decline in alcohol-related traffic deaths among drivers in the targeted ages compared with states that did not adopt such laws (General Accounting Office, 1987). The National Highway Traffic Safety Administration (NHTSA) has estimated that the MLDA of 21 prevents 700-1,000 traffic deaths annually among persons under 21, and that over 18,000 traffic deaths among persons under 21 have been prevented since 1975 (National Highway Traffic Safety Administration, 1999). MLDA laws not only decreased drinking among persons under age 21, they also lowered drinking among people age 21 to 25 who grew up in states with MLDAs of 21 relative to those who grew up in other states (O'Malley & Wagenaar, 1991). However, it is not known whether these laws, and other efforts to reduce underage drinking, lower both driving after drinking and alcohol-related motor vehicle crash involvement later in life. A recent analysis of the National Longitudinal Alcohol Epidemiologic Survey (NLAES) found that age of drinking onset was strongly related to having experienced alcohol dependence during one's life as defined by the diagnostic criteria of the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual, Fourth Edition (DSM-IV) (American Psychiatric Association, 1994). Among both males and females and persons with and without a family history of alcoholism, persons who began drinking regularly before age 14 were at least 3 times more likely than those who did not drink alcohol until they were over age 21 to experience diagnosable alcohol dependence during their life (Grant, 1998). Questions in that survey permit us to explore whether age of drinking onset is similarly related to whether respondents, ever in their life, and specifically during the year prior to the survey, reported having: - driven a motor vehicle after having too much to drink; and - been in a motor vehicle crash because of their drinking Independent of factors such as alcohol dependence that are related to early age of drinking onset. METHOD The National Longitudinal Alcohol Epidemiologic Survey (NLAES) was a national probability survey sponsored by the National Institute on Alcohol Abuse and Alcoholism. In 1992, the U.S. Bureau of the Census administered face to face interviews with 42,862 respondents age 18 and older, mean age 44, residing in the non- institutionalized population of the contiguous states including the District of Columbia. Sample Design: The multi-stage sampling approach used for NLAES was described by Massey et. al., (1989). Primary sampling units (PSUs) were stratified according to socioeconomic criteria and were selected with a probability proportional to their population size. Within PSUs, geographically defined secondary sampling units, referred to as segments, were selected systematically for the sample. The African American population was oversampled at this stage of the sample selection to secure adequate numbers for analytic purposes. Segments were then divided into clusters of 4-8 housing units, and all 3
  • 6. occupied housing units were included in the survey. Within each household, one randomly selected person age 18 and older was selected to participate. Young adults age 18-29 were oversampled at a ratio of 2.25:1.00 at this stage of sample selection to include a greater representation of this heavier drinking population subgroup. Weighting using SUDAAN (Shah, 1996) adjusted for the deliberate oversampling of African American and persons age 18-29 and accounted for the complex sampling design of NLAES. The household response rate for this representative sample of the U.S. population was 91.9% and the sample person response rate was 97.4%. The overall response rate was 90%. (A longitudinal follow up was initially planned but not completed because of financial constraints. A repeat cross sectional survey is currently being considered). Outcome measures Driving after drinking was explored by asking respondents, "In your entire life, did you ever drive a car, motorcycle, truck or boat, or other vehicle after having too much to drink? Did that happen in the past 12 months?" Alcohol-related crash involvement was explored by asking respondents, "In your entire life, did you ever have a car, motorcycle, truck, boat, or other accident because of your drinking? Did that happen in the past 12 months?" Predictor Variables Measures of alcohol use and dependence were derived from the Alcohol Use Disorder and Associated Disabilities Interview Schedule (AUDADIS) (Grant and Hasin, 1992); a fully structured diagnostic psychiatric interview designed to be administered by trained interviewers who were not clinicians. The age of drinking onset was ascertained by asking respondents how old they were when they first started drinking, not counting small tastes or sips of alcohol. Drinking onset data was collected from respondents who were classified as current drinkers (persons who had consumed at least 12 drinks in the past 12 months) and former drinkers (persons who had consumed at least 12 drinks in any one year of their lives but not during the year prior to the interview). Only current and former drinkers were included in the analysis. In an independent test retest study before the full NLAES was conducted, the test retest reliability of the drinking onset variable was good with a Kappa of .72 (Grant, Harford, Dawson et. al., 1995). The definition of lifetime alcohol dependence was based on the diagnostic criteria of the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual, Fourth Edition (DSM-IV) (American Psychiatric Association, 1994). The AUDADIS interview included an extensive list of symptom questions that operationalize the DSM-IV criteria for alcohol dependence. Diagnosis of alcohol dependence required that in any one year a respondent meet at least three of the following seven criteria for dependence: 1) tolerance, 2) withdrawal or avoidance of withdrawal, 3) persistent desire or unsuccessful attempts to cut down or stop drinking, 4) spending much time drinking, obtaining alcohol, or recovering from its effects, 5) giving up or reducing occupational, social, or recreational activities in favor of drinking, 6) impaired control over drinking and 7) continuing to drink despite a physical or psychological problem caused or exacerbated by drinking. Respondents were classified as showing a life time alcohol dependence diagnosis if they experienced an episode of dependence in the past year or at any time before the past year. The 4
  • 7. independent test retest study determined good reliabilities with Kappas of 0.76, and .0.73 for past year and prior to past.year dependence diagnoses respectively (Grant, Harford, Dawson et. al., 1995). The following demographic and behavior variables were examined as potential confounders of the association between age of drinking onset and later behavior: current age, sex, race/ethnicity (white non-hispanic, black non-hispanic, hispanic, other), education, marital status, smoking status (current, former, never), illicit drug use status (current, former, never) and alcohol dependency (current, former, never). Statistical Analysis Like other reports of the NLAES, our statistical analyses were conducted using the SUDAAN statistical package (Shah, 1996) to account for the complex survey design and oversampling of NLAES in the estimation of both effects and their standard errors (the pattern of results was very similar with and without use of the SUDAAN weighting). We focused on respondents who reported drinking ever in their lifetime (n=27,081). The univariate associations between age of drinking onset and drinking and driving outcomes and demographic and background behavior characteristics was tested using a modified test of independence that adjusts for the sampling design. The test for independence in SUDAAN is based on the Wald statistic comparing observed and expected values and follows the strategy proposed by Koch, Freeman, and Freeman (1975) for analyzing complex survey data. The p-value for this test is based on transforming the Wald Statistic to an F-statistic where degrees of freedom represent the size of the contingency table and the number of PSU's and strata in the study design. Logistic regression explored whether age of drinking onset was associated with each of the drinking and driving outcomes, controlling for potential confounding from demographic and behavior characteristics including alcohol dependency. The overall significance of the relations between different ages of drinking onset and study outcomes in the logistic models were tested through a chi-square statistic comparing models with and without the set of indicator variables representing age of onset. We examined whether the potential association between age of drinking onset and drinking and driving outcomes persisted after controlling for alcohol dependence because of the established relationships between alcohol dependence and drinking and driving and between age of drinking onset and alcohol dependency (Grant, 1998). We also examined the potential relationships between age of drinking onset and drinking and driving behavior in the subset of respondents who were never alcohol dependent (n=21,713). RESULTS Sixty-five percent of respondents ever drank alcohol, 49 percent before age 21 and 3 percent before age 14. The mean drinking onset age was 19.0 years. Four percent were classified as alcohol dependent in the past year and 13 percent ever in their life. Twenty-three percent of respondents reported driving a motor vehicle after having too much to drink ever in their life and five percent in the past year. Four percent of respondents reported ever having been in a motor vehicle crash because of their drinking, 0.2 percent in the past year. The earlier the age respondents began drinking, the greater the proportions who reported driving after drinking too much and motor vehicle crash involvement because of drinking ever in their lives and during the year prior to the 5
  • 8. survey. As can be seen in Figure 1, persons who began drinking before age 14 were 3 times more likely than those who began drinking after age 21 to report ever driving after drinking too much, 53 percent vs. 18 percent, and 4 times more likely to report doing so in the past year, 13 percent vs. 3 percent. As seen in Figure 2, respondents who began drinking before age fourteen were 7 times more likely than those who began drinking after age 21 to report ever being in a motor vehicle crash because of their drinking, 14 percent vs. 2 percent, and during the past year, 0.7 percent vs. 0.1 percent. The magnitude of differences tended to become smaller as the age of drinking onset became closer to 21. As seen in Figures 1 and 2, even after age 21, the older respondents were when they began to drink the less likely they were to drive after drinking or be in traffic crashes after drinking. Several subgroups in the study were significantly more likely to have begun drinking at younger ages: male respondents, younger respondents, those with less than a high school education, persons never married, and persons who currently or ever smoked or used illicit drugs and respondents with a current (past year) or lifetime diagnoses of alcohol dependence (all relations p<.001; data available upon request). To control for these potentially confounding characteristics, we entered those background characteristics into a series of multiple logistic regression analyses examining the following outcomes as dichotomous variables: whether or not respondents drove after drinking too much, or were in a motor vehicle crash because of their drinking. Each of these outcomes was examined ever during the respondent's life and during the year prior to the interview. Respondents who began drinking at an earlier age were significantly more likely to report that they drove after drinking too much ever and in the past year. Further, they were significantly more likely to have ever been involved in a motor vehicle crash after drinking too much. For each study outcome, the odds ratio and 95% confidence intervals for respondents who began drinking each year from less than age 14 through 20 relative to those who began at age 21 or older was examined separately. Persons starting at age 21 and older were analyzed as a group for comparison because the minimum legal drinking age is 21. Results are summarized in Figures 3 and 4. The strongest relationships were observed when comparing respondents who started drinking at age 14 relative to those starting at age 21 and older. Persons who began drinking when they were age 14 were 3.4 (95% CI 2.7, 4.3) times more likely than those who began after age 21 to report ever driving after drinking too much and 2.33 (1.7, 3.2) times more likely to do so in the year prior to the survey. Further, they were 4.1 times more likely to report being in a motor vehicle crash after drinking too much ever (95% CI 2.9, 5.8) and in the past year (95% CI 1.5, 10.9). As can be seen in the figures, these relationships tended to be stronger for those whose age of drinking onset was younger. Also the relations were stronger when we examined whether respondents ever in their life drove after drinking too much or were ever in a motor vehicle crash because of drinking as compared with those who experienced these outcomes in the past year. Respondents who began drinking in each age group under 21 relative to those starting after age 21 were significantly more likely to report ever driving after drinking too much and ever being in a motor vehicle crash because of their drinking. We also repeated the logistic regression analysis examining only the subgroup of respondents who drank but never experienced alcohol dependence (n=21,462). Just over 6
  • 9. half the sample and 79% of persons who ever drank this group represented 35% of those who reported motor vehicle crashes after too much drinking ever during their life and 28% of those reporting such crashes in the year prior to the interview. Those who started drinking in each age group under 21 were significantly more likely than those who started drinking after age 21 to have ever and in the past year been in a motor vehicle crash after drinking too much. (Figure 5). DISCUSSION The relative risk of involvement in fatal crashes increases with increasing driver blood alcohol content and the risks increase more steeply for drivers under age 21 than for older drivers (Zador, Krawchuk and Voas, 2000). Previous studies have shown that raising the legal drinking age to 21 reduced drinking and alcohol related motor vehicle crash involvement among persons under age 21 (General Accounting Office, 1987; National Highway Traffic Safety Administration, 1999). Prior research also indicated that persons who grew up in states with a legal drinking age of 21 relative to those who grew up in states with lower legal drinking ages, not only drank less when they were under age 21, but also when they were age 21 to 25 (O'Malley & Wagenaar, 199 1). However, it is not known whether efforts to reduce underage drinking lowers driving after drinking and alcohol related motor vehicle crash involvement later in life. This national study of persons 18 and older, mean age 44, found that the earlier the age respondents started drinking, the more likely they were to report driving after drinking too much ever in their life and in the past year. Also the earlier the age of drinking onset, the greater the likelihood the respondents reported being in a motor vehicle crash because of their drinking, even after we analytically controlled for characteristics and behaviors associated with earlier drinking onset: whether or not respondents ever or currently had an alcohol dependence diagnosis, age, gender, education, race/ethnicity, marital status, smoking, or illicit drug use. A 4-fold increased alcohol-related crash risk for those who began drinking at age 14 relative to those who started after age 21 was found both ever in a respondent's life and during the year of the survey suggesting the relationship may not be purely a function of having more years of alcohol exposure. Also, the relationships were not solely a function of people who start drinking at an early age being more likely to become alcohol dependent. Among persons who were never alcohol dependent, relative to respondents who started drinking at age 21 or older, those who began drinking at age 14, and in each succeeding age up through age 20 were significantly more likely to report being in a motor vehicle crash after drinking ever and during the year prior to the survey. Several methodological factors should be considered when interpreting the study's results. First, the distribution of responses on key drinking variables was similar in the NLAES sample when compared to other large national surveys. The National Longitudinal Survey of Labor Market Experience in Youth (NLSY)(N=12,686) was a follow up study of a survey designed to represent the non institutionalized population of young people age 14-21 as well as person 17-21 serving in the military in 1979. Grant, Harford and Stinson (In Review) reported the ages respondents in that sample started drinking. Compared to respondents in the NLAES who were in those age groups -in 1979 (N=4210) the proportion of drinkers in the NLSY who started drinking at <14, and each consecutive year up to age 21 and older were virtually identical (data available upon 7
  • 10. request). When compared to the 1998 National Health Interview study, an area probability sample of over 100,000 persons nationwide, similar proportions of the NLAES sample were classified as ever having drunk alcohol 65% vs. 71% or as having met DSM IV alcohol dependence criteria 4.4% vs. 5.9%. In NLAES to be classified as ever drinking a person had to drink at least 12 drinks in a given year. In the National Alcohol Survey conducted in 1990 the proportion who met DSM N dependence criteria was 3.9% (for data from the National Health Interview Study and the National Alcohol Survey see U.S. Department of Health and Human Services, 1997). The proportion who drove a motor vehicle after having too much to drink in the past year 5% in NLAES was similar to the percentage reporting they drove while impaired by alcohol in the past month 2.5% in CDC's 1993 national Behavioral Risk Factor Survey of over 102,263 adults 18 or older (Lui et.al., 1997). Second, even though respondents said the motor vehicle crashes they experienced were because of their drinking, it is impossible to be certain that alcohol was the sole cause of the crash. However, alcohol use was present and the crash can be considered alcohol-related. Third questions were not asked about the amount that respondents drove in general or crash involvement when they had not been drinking. Driving amounts could have influenced the key findings of this study. Also it would be of value to assess whether age of drinking onset was more closely related to alcohol related crash involvement than involvement in crashes where alcohol was not a factor. Fourth, questions regarding driving after drinking and motor vehicle crash involvement after drinking could have been more objectively worded. In the NLAES survey, respondents were asked if they drove after drinking too much ever and in the past year and if they were in a motor vehicle crash because of their drinking ever and in the past year. A more objective approach would have been to ask respondents if they drove or were in a motor vehicle crash within an hour after consuming a specific number of drinks. The way questions were worded in the NLAES suggests the relations observed may have been stronger than reported. Persons who drive after drinking tend to believe they can consume more drinks and still drive safely than people who do not drive after drinking (National Highway Traffic Safety Administration, 1996). Consequently, they may under report the actual frequency with which they drove after drinking or were in a crash because of their drinking. Fifth, there is the possibility that social desirability biases may have influenced respondents. Those least likely to admit drinking at an early age may be least likely to admit driving after drinking too much and being in a motor vehicle crash because of drinking. Sixth, the survey was cross sectional and older adults may have difficulty remembering the age they first started to drink. Finally, because the survey was confidential, efforts were not made to link self-reports: of crash involvement to driver records. Longitudinal studies asking adolescents when they first began to drink with more objective questions about driving after drinking and crash involvement and which explore actual traffic records need to be conducted. The results of this study point to a need to explore why people who start drinking at an early age are more likely to report driving after drinking in the past year even among persons who are not alcohol dependent. Several factors may contribute to the relation. 8
  • 11. First, it is possible that people who engage in a variety of deviant or illegal behaviors at an early age are more likely to engage in several such behaviors later in life. However, the relationships in this study between early drinking onset and driving after drinking and motor vehicle crash involvement were independent of current or past use of illegal drugs or tobacco. Second, persons who start drinking at an early age more frequently drink heavily than those who start consuming alcohol later, in life even if they are not alcohol dependent (Hingson, Heeren, Jamanka, & Howland, In Press). This could increase the likelihood that they would also drive more frequently after drinking. Also, persons who begin drinking at an early age may be less likely to believe that driving after drinking increases crash risks. They may believe for example, that the risk of traffic crashes and other injuries increase only for people who are visibly intoxicated. They may derive pleasure or a sense of self-esteem by taking driving risks. It is well known that people who drive after drinking are more likely to speed and are less likely to wear seat belts. Also, heavy consumption of alcohol may further impair the judgement of those who start drinking at an earlier age. After drinking they may be less likely to appreciate their increased crash risk than when sober. Whether those who start drinking at a younger age are as aware of traffic laws and as likely to believe those laws will be enforced also warrants exploration. A recent report indicated that after a decade of decline, the percentage of high school seniors who drink and drive has increased the last two years (O'Malley & Johnston, 1999). The study reported here identifies another important reason to step up enforcement of the legal drinking age of 21 and to expand educational and community programs that delay the onset of drinking. The potential traffic safety benefits of delaying underage drinking may extend well beyond age 21. 9
  • 12. REFERENCES American Psychiatric Association. (1994) Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (DSM IV). Washington, D.C. General Accounting Office (1987) Drinking age laws: an evaluation synthesis of their impact on highway safety. GAO PEMD 87-100, Washington, DC. Grant B.F.; Harford T.C. and Stinson F. Age at onset of alcohol use and DSM IV alcohol abuse and dependence, a twelve-year follow up, Journal of Substance Abuse (under review). Grant B.F., Harford F.C., Dawson D.A., Chou P.S. and Pickerling R.P. (1995) The alcohol use disorder and associated disabilities interview schedule: reliability of alcohol and drug modules in a general population sample, Drug and Alcohol Dependence. 39, 37-44. Grant, B. (1998) The impact of family history of alcoholism on the relationship between age at onset of alcohol use and DSM-III alcohol dependence, Alcohol Health and Research World 22, 2, 144 - 147. Grant, B. F. and Hasin, D.S. (1992) The alcohol use disorders and associated disabilities interview schedule. National Institute on Alcohol Abuse and Alcoholism, Rockville, Maryland. Hingson R., Heeren T., Jamanka A., and Howland J., Age of drinking onset and unintentional injury involvement after drinking, Journal of the American Medical Association (In Press). Koch G.G., Freeman D.H. and Freeman J.L. (1975) Strategies in the multi variate analysis of data from complex surveys, International Statistical Review. 43, 59-78. Lui S., Seigel P., Brewer R., Mokdad A, Sleet D., and Serduda M., (1997) Prevalence of alcohol impaired driving: results from a national self reported survey, Journal of the American Medical Association. 272, 2, 123-125. Massey, J.T., Parsons, V.L., and Tadros W. (1989) Design and estimation for the national health interview survey, 1985 - 1994, Vital and Health Statistics Report Series 2(10), National Center for Health Statistics, U.S. Department of Health and Human Services, Hyattsville, MD National Highway Traffic Safety Administration (1996) National Survey of Drinking and Driving Attitudes and Behavior, DOT HS 808 438. U.S. Department of Transportation, Washington, DC. National Highway Traffic Safety Administration (1999) Traffic Safety Facts, 1998: Alcohol, DOT HS 808 950. U.S. Department of Transportation, Washington, DC. 10
  • 13. O'Malley, P. and Wagenaar, A. (1991) Effects of minimum drinking age laws on alcohol use, related behaviors, and traffic crash involvement among American youth., J. of Alcohol Studies. 52,478 - 491. O'Malley, P.M. and Johnston, L.D. (1999) Drinking and driving among high school seniors. American Journal of Public Health. 89, 5, 678-684. Shah, B.V., Barnwell, B.G., and Bieler, G.S. (1996) SUDAAN User's Manual, Release 7.0,. Research Triangle Institute, Research Triangle Park, NC. U.S. Department of Health and Human Services Office of the Secretary (1997) Ninth special report to the U.S. Congress on alcohol and health June 1997, Page 19 NIH Publication Number 97-4017, NIH, NIAAA. Zador P., Krawchuck S., and Was R. (2000) Alcohol related relative risk of driver fatalities and driver involvement in fatal crashes in relation to driver age and gender, Journal Studies on Alcohol, 61: 387-395. 11
  • 14. * Figure I Drove a Motor Vehicle After Drinking Too Much According to Age of Drinking Onset * Percent 60 60 54 * 53 * -^- Ever 50 50 * Past Year 40 * 35 33 30 * 26 20 * 20 - 13 16 18 13 * 11 10 ^ 3 0--- Age Started<14 14 15 16 17 18 19 20 21+ DrinWg 1131 844 1507 3161 2866 5699 2213 2082. 2975 P<.001
  • 15. * Figure 2 In a Motor Vehicle Crash Because of Drinking According to Age of Drinking Onset 15 la 14 14 13 12 Ever ll 10 -^- Past Year 9 * 8 7 6 5 _ . a 3 1.8 2 0.7 0.8 0.6 0.3 u 0.3 0.2 Age SParted 14 15 16 17 18 19 20 21+ Drinkingl4 N= 1131 844 1507 3161 2866 5699 2213 2082 2975 <.001
  • 16. * Figure 3: Drove After Drinking Too Much According to Age of Drinking Onset (NLAES) Odds Ratio and Confidence Intervals 7T 6+ Ever Past Year * * 5 4 4 3 5 2 2 1 0i i 0 ' 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 <14 14 15 16 17 18 19 20 21+ <14 14 15 16 17 18 19 20 21+ p<.00' Age Started p<.001 Drinking •Each group compared to those who started drinking at age 21+. •Controlling for: ever/past year/never alcohol dependent, age, sex, race/ethnicity, education, marital status, smoking, and illicit drug use.
  • 17. * Figure 4: In a Motor Vehicle Accident Because of Drinking Too Much According to Age of Drinking Onset (NLAES) Odds Ratio and 10,91 7T Confidence Intervals 7T 6 Ever 6f Past Year 5 * 5f * 4+ 4,1 3+ 3f 2+ 2+ 1.4 ,4 • 1+ 1+ 0 0 I } 1 I I I I I i + <14 14 15 16 17 18 19 20 21+ <14 14 15 16 17 18 19 20 21+ p<.OOI Age Started N.S. Drinking Each group compared to those who started drinking at age 21+. •Controlling for: ever/past year/never alcohol dependent, age, sex, race/ethnicity, education, marital status, smoking, and illicit drug use.
  • 18. * Figure 5: In a Motor Vehicle Crash After Drinking Too Much According to Age of Drinking Onset (Subjects Who Were Never Alcohol Dependent) Odds Ratio and 84a Confidence Intervals 5&8 24,.6 20-- * * 18,9 16-- Past Year 4 Ever 14.3 124 * * to-- * 8+ 6 6+ 4+ 3,4 2 2+ 0 I-------f I I I 0 I I I ^ I I 1 ' I I I I <14 14 15 16 17 18 19 20 21+ 14 15 16 17 18 19 20 21+ Age Started p<,po1 P<.001 'Controlling for: age, gender, race/ethnicity, education, marital status, smoking, and drug use. Drinking *Each group compared to those who started drinking at age 21+.
  • 19. 0 U.S. Department of Transportation National Hisl way Administration DOT HS 809 188 January 2001