SlideShare a Scribd company logo
Writing Assignment Answer Sheet
When answering these questions, please use your own words.
Do not copy and paste and do not cite verbatim from the article.
Part 1: Article summary
· Make sure to answer only in the space provided. If you go
beyond it only part of your text will be visible and this will be
the only part we will grade. DO NOT change he font size to fit
more into the allotted space. DO NOT change the size of the
text box to increase the allotted space. EACH SUCH
ATTEMPT WILL BE CONSIDERED A BREACH OF ETHICAL
GUIDELINES AND WILL RESULT IN A GRADE OF 0
(ZERO) IN THE ASSIGNMENT and additional steps as I see
fit.
· If the answer is similar across studies, just say so rather than
elaborate (for example, X was measured like in Study 1; the
source of data was the one in Study 1…)
1. (a) What are the major factual reasons and theoretical
arguments for this research? (b) What gap in the
literature/knowledge do these researchers attempt to fill? (c)
What are the three overall research questions the researchers try
to answer?
2. Define the two types of bias discussed and how can they
contribute to health disparities?
3. What are the specific research questions the researchers
examined in each study?
Study 2:
Study 1:
4. (a) What are the data sources in each study? (b) How many
participants were included in the final analyses? (you do not
need to discuss the various ways to calculate and weight the
various types of data).
Study 1:
Study 2:
5. What were the major variables examined in each study? How
were they measured? (you do not need to discuss the
Correlates).
Study 1 (4 major variables):
Study 2 (4 major variables):
6. What were the major results in each study? (you do not need
to discuss the analyses. Refer only to the major variables and
research questions as indicated below).
Study 1:
a. Access to healthcare as related to race AND the different
types of bias:
b. Circulatory disease diagnosis as related to race AND the
different types of bias:
Study 2:
a. Death rate due to circulatory diseases as related to race AND
the different types of bias:
7. What was the research design used in these studies
(descriptive, correlational, or experimental?) explain your
answer: (a) why do you think this is the particular design? (b)
why didn’t you chose the other two designs? (c) can you make
causal inferences based on these studies? Why? (make sure to
refer to Chapter 2 when answering this question).
In the next page, you will answer Part 2: Critical evaluation and
conclusions. Remember it should not be longer than a page.
Use 12-point Times New Roman font and 1” margins on all
sides. When answering these questions, use the materials you
have learned in the course that might be relevant to this type of
research topic. See more details about this answer in the
separate instructions sheet.
Part 2 – A critical review of the paper
Psychological Science
2016, Vol. 27(10) 1299 –1311
© The Author(s) 2016
Reprints and permissions:
sagepub.com/journalsPermissions.nav
DOI: 10.1177/0956797616658450
pss.sagepub.com
Research Article
Blacks die at a higher rate than Whites from circulatory-
related diseases (e.g., heart disease; U.S. Department of
Health and Human Services, 2014). Prominent theories
have suggested that one cause of this disparity is that
Blacks experience more discrimination, which leads to
stress, which in turn has negative health consequences
(Clark, Anderson, Clark, & Williams, 1999; Hatzenbuehler,
Phelan, & Link, 2013; Major, Mendes, & Dovidio, 2013).
Studies supporting this view have found that the percep-
tion of discrimination is associated with anxiety, cardio-
vascular threat response, hypertension, and mortality
(Barnes et al., 2008; Mendoza-Denton, Downey, Purdie,
Davis, & Pietrzak, 2002; Pascoe & Richman, 2009; Sawyer,
Major, Casad, Townsend, & Mendes, 2012; Williams &
Mohammed, 2009).
Although this previous work suggests that perceived
racial bias contributes to disparities between Blacks’ and
Whites’ health, less is known about whether the actual
racial bias of Whites contributes to these disparities. A
deeper understanding of links between Whites’ racial
bias and racial health disparities could help identify com-
munities in greatest need of prejudice-prevention and
health-promotion interventions. Therefore, the aim of the
current research was to establish whether Black-White
disparities in risk of circulatory disease and in circula-
tory-disease-related death rate are greater in communi-
ties where Whites harbor more racial bias.
658450PSSXXX10.1177/0956797616658450Leitner et
al.Blacks’ Death Rate and Whites’ Bias
research-article2016
Corresponding Author:
Jordan B. Leitner, University of California, Berkeley–
Psychology, 3210
Tolman Hall, Berkeley, CA 94720
E-mail: [email protected]
Blacks’ Death Rate Due to Circulatory
Diseases Is Positively Related to Whites’
Explicit Racial Bias: A Nationwide
Investigation Using Project Implicit
Jordan B. Leitner1, Eric Hehman2, Ozlem Ayduk1, and
Rodolfo Mendoza-Denton1
1Department of Psychology, University of California, Berkeley,
and 2Department of Psychology,
Ryerson University
Abstract
Perceptions of racial bias have been linked to poorer circulatory
health among Blacks compared with Whites. However,
little is known about whether Whites’ actual racial bias
contributes to this racial disparity in health. We compiled
racial-
bias data from 1,391,632 Whites and examined whether racial
bias in a given county predicted Black-White disparities
in circulatory-disease risk (access to health care, diagnosis of a
circulatory disease; Study 1) and circulatory-disease-
related death rate (Study 2) in the same county. Results revealed
that in counties where Whites reported greater racial
bias, Blacks (but not Whites) reported decreased access to
health care (Study 1). Furthermore, in counties where
Whites reported greater racial bias, both Blacks and Whites
showed increased death rates due to circulatory diseases,
but this relationship was stronger for Blacks than for Whites
(Study 2). These results indicate that racial disparities in
risk of circulatory disease and in circulatory-disease-related
death rate are more pronounced in communities where
Whites harbor more explicit racial bias.
Keywords
racial and ethnic attitudes and relations, prejudice, health,
sociocultural factors, open data, open materials
Received 11/30/15; Revision accepted 6/16/16
1300 Leitner et al.
Whites’ Racial Biases and Health
Disparities
Previous research has contrasted two forms of racial
bias: explicit and implicit bias. Explicit bias refers to
deliberate, consciously controlled biases, whereas
implicit bias refers to more automatic biases that are
difficult to control (Greenwald, Poehlman, Uhlmann, &
Banaji, 2009). Explicit and implicit racial biases are posi-
tively correlated, though research suggests that they are
relatively independent constructs (Hofmann, Gawronski,
Gschwendner, Le, & Schmitt, 2005). The distinction
between explicit and implicit biases is supported by
research showing that explicit bias predicts intentional
behaviors, whereas implicit bias predicts relatively unin-
tentional behaviors (Dovidio, Kawakami, & Gaertner,
2002).
Both explicit and implicit racial biases might contribute
to racial health disparities. For instance, Whites’ explicit or
implicit racial bias may hinder Blacks’ access to health
care and thus decrease the likelihood that Blacks will
receive diagnosis of and treatment for health problems.
Additionally, Whites’ explicit or implicit bias may contrib-
ute to hostile community environments that evoke psy-
chological stress in Blacks, and stress has been linked to
circulatory disease (Black & Garbutt, 2002). Studies con-
sistent with these possibilities have demonstrated that
Blacks show increased death rates in regions where pop-
ulation-level measures (i.e., survey responses from multi-
racial samples) reveal more explicit anti-Black attitudes
(Kennedy, Kawachi, Lochner, Jones, & Prothrow-Stith,
1997; Lee, Kawachi, Muennig, & Hatzenbuehler, 2015).
Similarly, Blacks die at a higher rate in regions where
more racist Internet searches are conducted (Chae et al.,
2015), and sexual minorities have shorter life expectan-
cies in regions where population-level measures reveal
more antigay attitudes (Hatzenbuehler, Bellatorre, et al.,
2014).
Though these studies provide evidence for the perva-
sive effects of explicit bias, several important questions
remain. First, is there a relationship between the domi-
nant group’s (e.g., Whites’) negative attitudes toward a
targeted group (e.g., Blacks) and the targeted group’s
health? Addressing this question would be important, as
previous research has not disaggregated the bias of tar-
geted and nontargeted groups. For instance, Lee et al.
(2015) examined the effects of population-level anti-
Black attitudes that were aggregated across all respon-
dents (including Blacks). Similarly, Hatzenbuehler,
Bellatorre, et al. (2014) examined the effects of popula-
tion-level antigay attitudes that were aggregated across
straight and gay respondents. Accordingly, these studies
leave open the possibility that effects of population-level
bias on the target group’s health are driven by that group’s
bias against itself (e.g., Blacks’ anti-Black attitudes), as
opposed to nontargeted groups’ bias against the target
group (e.g., Whites’ anti-Black attitudes).
Second, does explicit or implicit bias play a stronger
role in predicting Black-White health disparities? Given
that explicit and implicit biases are positively correlated
(Hofmann et al., 2005), it would be important to deter-
mine whether they independently predict health dispari-
ties. Indeed, understanding the independent relationships
between the various forms of bias and health could pro-
vide insight into whether explicit or implicit bias should
be the target of interventions aimed at reducing health
disparities.
Finally, would a relationship between racial bias and
health emerge across a large number of small geographic
areas? Previous research has examined relationships
between bias and health outcomes in a relatively limited
number of large geographic areas (Chae et al., 2015: n =
196; Hatzenbuehler, Bellatorre, et al., 2014: n = 170;
Kennedy et al., 1997: n = 39; Lee et al., 2015: n = 100).
Given that bias might vary within geographic units (e.g.,
county-to-county variation within a state), examining the
effects of bias across smaller geographic areas may pro-
vide a more sensitive analysis.
To address these issues, we examined whether Black-
White disparities in risk of circulatory disease (Study 1)
and in rate of death due to circulatory disease (Study 2)
were more pronounced in counties where Whites har-
bored more racial bias. We pitted explicit and implicit
biases against one another to determine the stronger pre-
dictor of health outcomes.
Study 1
Data sources
Racial bias. To assess explicit and implicit racial bias,
we compiled data from Project Implicit (Xu, Nosek, &
Greenwald, 2014), an initiative that has measured racial
bias from millions of respondents over the Internet since
2002. Project Implicit has generated the largest known
repository of data on explicit and implicit bias. Though
respondents are self-selected, a major strength of the data
set is that their general geographic locations can be
ascertained from their Internet protocol (IP) addresses.
At the time when we obtained the Project Implicit data
set, it included responses made from 2002 through 2013,
though only five responses (0.0001%) were from 2002.
Because we examined changes in bias over time and
sought reliable estimates of bias in each year, we omitted
2002 responses. Thus, our analyses included responses in
the time window from 2003 through 2013. Respondents
completed measures of both explicit and implicit racial
bias. For the measure of explicit racial bias, community
Blacks’ Death Rate and Whites’ Bias 1301
members rated how warm they felt toward European
Americans and toward African Americans, on scales from
0 (coldest feelings) to 10 (warmest feelings). We com-
puted the difference between these responses (warmth
toward European Americans minus warmth toward
African Americans) and operationalized pro-White expli-
cit bias as more reported warmth toward European
Americans compared with African Americans.
The measure of implicit racial bias was an Implicit
Association Test (IAT), a speeded dual-categorization
task in which respondents categorized social targets (e.g.,
Black and White faces) and verbal targets (e.g., words
referring to “good” and “bad” things) by key press. Faster
responses when White faces and “good” things (and
Black faces and “bad” things) required the same key
press, as compared with the inverse pairing, reflect pro-
White implicit attitudes (Greenwald et al., 2009). The IAT
was scored according to the D measure (Greenwald,
Nosek, & Banaji, 2003). Explicit and implicit bias at the
county level were positively related, r = .25, p < .0001.
Within the Project Implicit data set, we identified
White respondents for whom the county where they
completed the measures was known: 1,391,632 responses
from 1,836 counties met these inclusion criteria (M =
759.57 responses per county, SD = 1,766.10). Counties
were defined according to the February 2013 Federal
Information Processing Standard (FIPS) county codes of
the U.S. Census Bureau’s (2013) Metropolitan and Micro-
politan Delineation Files.
A limitation of the Project Implicit data set is that it
was obtained with convenience sampling, and thus the
sample for a given county may not be representative of
all individuals in that county. One dimension on which
the Project Implicit sample was likely nonrepresentative
was age: Whereas the median age for all Project Implicit
respondents was 23, the national median age was 37,
according to the Median Age by Sex report from the U.S.
Census Bureau’s 2009–2013 American Community Survey
(ACS).1 Estimating the racial bias of older nonrespon-
dents would be important, given that racial bias is signifi-
cantly greater among older individuals (Gonsalkorale,
Sherman, & Klauer, 2009). Accordingly, we used post-
stratification weights that assigned greater weight to
respondents the more representative they were of the
age distribution of their community’s population (cf.
Lohr, 2009). We employed the following poststratification
weighting scheme. First, separately for explicit and
implicit bias, we computed average racial-bias scores for
five age groups in each county: 15–24, 25–34, 35–54, 55–
75, and 75+. Second, we determined the population
count of Whites in each of these age groups in each
county using the Sex by Age (White Only) reports from
the U.S. Census Bureau’s 2005–2009 and 2009–2013 ACSs.
Third, for each county, we computed estimates for explicit
and implicit racial bias that were weighted by the White
population count in each age group in that county. The
result of this weighting scheme was that respondents
were assigned a greater weight when they belonged to
an age group that was more populous in their county. We
report findings obtained using this weighting scheme,
but our conclusions were identical when we used
unweighted county averages (see the Supplemental
Material available online).
Although the racial-bias measure was limited to an
11-year window, we believe that it served as a reasonable
proxy for prejudice before and after this time period.
Supporting this notion, fixed-effects models revealed that
year (treated as a factor) accounted for only 1% of the
variance in explicit bias and 2% of the variance in implicit
bias. In contrast, county (treated as a factor) accounted
for 42% of the variance in explicit bias and 27% of the
variance in implicit bias. Thus, year-to-year change in
bias was minimal, whereas counties differed consider-
ably in their levels of bias. Moreover, previous work has
demonstrated that racial bias is highly stable over time,
even following a historic event (i.e., Barack Obama’s rise
to the presidency; Schmidt & Nosek, 2010).
Accordingly, we included racial-bias estimates from
2013, even though data from the previous year were used
to calculate our outcome measures of risk of circulatory
disease (see the next section). The advantage of includ-
ing racial-bias responses from 2013 was that it maximized
the sample (i.e., 8% of all responses in the data set were
from 2013), thereby increasing the reliability of the racial-
bias estimates. Nevertheless, when we used racial-bias
data from 2003 through 2012 only, the pattern of findings
was extremely similar to what we report here (see the
Supplemental Material).
Risk of circulatory disease. To estimate risk of circu-
latory disease, we compiled data from the 2012 Selected
Metropolitan/Micropolitan Area Risk Trends (SMART) of
the Behavioral Risk Factor Surveillance System survey, a
telephone survey in which participants were asked about
their health risk factors (Centers for Disease Control and
Prevention, CDC, 2013). We targeted two factors related
to circulatory-disease risk.
First, we examined participants’ response to the ques-
tion, “Was there a time in the past 12 months when you
needed to see a doctor but could not because of cost?” We
coded “yes” responses as 0 and “no” responses as 1. Thus,
when aggregated at the county level, responses to this
question provided an estimate of the percentage of county
residents who had access to affordable health care.
Second, to determine the percentage of respondents
who had been diagnosed with circulatory diseases, we
examined responses to two questions: “Has a doctor,
nurse, or other health professional ever told you that you
1302 Leitner et al.
had a heart attack, also called a myocardial infarction?”
and “Has a doctor, nurse, or other health professional
ever told you that you had angina or coronary heart dis-
ease?” We coded “yes” responses as 1 and “no” responses
as 0. Responses to these items were strongly related for
both Blacks, r = .66, p < .0001, and Whites, r = .77,
p < .0001. For parsimony, we averaged responses to these
two items. When aggregated at the county level, these
averages provided an estimate of the percentage of
county residents who had received these diagnoses.
In total, 23,522 Blacks from 208 counties and 175,637
Whites from 210 counties completed the health survey
(White responses per county: M = 836.37, SD = 591.07;
Black responses per county: M = 113.09, SD = 193.83).
We aggregated responses by race and county to arrive at
separate point estimates for the percentages of Whites
and Blacks with access to health care and with circula-
tory-disease diagnoses. Figure 1a shows the location of
the 205 counties for which we had estimates of Whites’
racial bias and SMART-survey responses from both Blacks
and Whites.
Race. Participants reported their race on the SMART sur-
vey. Race was coded as 0 for Black and 1 for White.
Covariates
Sex and age. Participants reported their sex and age
(in years) on the SMART survey. Sex was coded as −1 for
male and 1 for female.
Population. Population estimates by race and county
were derived from the modified counts for 2010 from the
following U.S. Census Bureau report: Annual Estimates
of the Resident Population by Sex, Race Alone or in
Combination, and Hispanic Origin for the United States,
States, and Counties: April 1, 2010 to July 1, 2013. From
these data, we computed the total population (the sum
of Blacks and Whites) and the Black-to-White ratio of the
population for each county in 2010. To produce unstan-
dardized regression coefficients that were interpretable,
we log-transformed the total-population estimates (but
not the Black-to-White ratios).
Education. County-level education was assessed by
averaging the high school graduation rates for Blacks
and Whites (data taken from the U.S. Census Bureau’s
2009–2013 ACS: report titled Sex by Educational Attain-
ment for the Population 25 Years and Over).
Income, unemployment, and poverty. County-level
income, unemployment, and poverty were assessed by
compiling data for Whites and Blacks from the 2005–2009
and 2009–2013 ACSs (income data were taken from the
reports titled Median Household Income in the Past 12
Months (Householder), unemployment data were taken
from the reports titled Employment Status, and poverty
data were taken from the reports titled Poverty Status
in the Past 12 Months by Sex and Age). County-level
income was estimated by averaging the median house-
hold income for Whites and Blacks. County-level unem-
ployment was estimated by averaging the unemployment
rates for Whites and Blacks. County-level poverty was
estimated by averaging the poverty rates for Whites and
Blacks. So that unstandardized coefficients would be
interpretable, we divided income values by 10,000.
Segregation. County-level racial segregation was
indexed via dissimilarity, an estimate of the percentage
of non-Hispanic Whites who would have to move to
another census tract within the same county in order to
achieve racial integration with non-Hispanic Blacks. Dis-
similarity indices at the county level were provided by
J. Dewitt (personal communication, December 15, 2015)
and were based on methods described in Frey and Mey-
ers (2005). We averaged dissimilarities from 2000 and
2010, so that each county had one value of dissimilarity.
Geographic mobility. A relationship between racial
bias and Black-White health disparities could be driven
by social selection forces. Specifically, rather than reflect-
ing an effect of racial bias, health disparities could be
due to more healthy Blacks selecting to live in environ-
ments with lower bias or to sick Blacks selecting to live
in high-bias environments. Accordingly, we accounted
for geographic mobility, as has been done in previous
work (Hatzenbuehler, Jun, Corliss, & Austin, 2014). The
U.S. Census Bureau’s 2005–2009 and 2009–2013 ACSs
provided estimates of the percentage of Blacks in each
county who moved into that county during the previous
year (a) from a different county in the same state, (b)
from a different state, and (c) from abroad (data taken
from the reports titled Geographic Mobility by Selected
Characteristics in the United States). For each county, we
summed these three percentages in each time window
and then averaged the two sums to obtain a single index
of geographic mobility.
Housing density. To capture where each county fell
on the continuum from rural to urban, we assessed hous-
ing density, the number of housing units per square mile.
For each county, we averaged housing-density values
across the U.S. Census Bureau’s 2000 and 2010 Popula-
tion, Housing Units, Area, and Density reports.
Age bias. To examine whether any effects were spe-
cific to racial bias, or generalized to bias on nonracial
dimensions, we used Project Implicit’s age-bias data from
2003 through 2013. Project Implicit’s indices of age bias
Blacks’ Death Rate and Whites’ Bias 1303
paralleled those for race bias. Implicit age bias was
indexed with an IAT (respondents categorized young
and old faces and “good” and “bad” words), and explicit
age bias was indexed with feeling thermometers for
young and old people (explicit age bias was computed
as warmth toward young people minus warmth toward
old people). This data set contained 585,242 geo-coded
responses from 1,850 counties (M = 316.35 responses per
county, SD = 777.29). We estimated county-level implicit
age bias and explicit age bias following the same proce-
dures used to compute race bias.
Results
To determine whether Whites’ explicit or implicit racial
bias (as measured by Project Implicit) independently pre-
dicted health risk for Blacks and Whites, we employed
generalized estimating equations (GEEs). We analyzed
the data using GEEs with robust standard errors in light
of the minimal distributional assumptions they require
and their robustness to misspecification in large samples
(Hubbard et al., 2010). Our conclusions do not depend
on the decision to use GEEs; effects for all analyses were
Fig. 1. Results for racial disparities in (a) access to affordable
health care (Study 1) and (b) death rate (per 100,000) due to
circulatory diseases (Study
2). In (a), darker colors indicate counties where Whites reported
greater access to health care than Blacks; in (b), darker colors
indicate counties where
Blacks died from circulatory diseases at a higher rate than
Whites did. For interactive versions of these maps, visit
www.jordanbleitner.com/maps.
1304 Leitner et al.
similar when estimated with mixed linear models. All
predictors were mean-centered unless otherwise noted.
The number of responses on the health survey varied
across counties, and we conjectured that our county-level
health estimates were more accurate in counties where
more participants completed the survey. Therefore, we
weighted counties by the number of responses on the
health survey. To isolate the effects of race and bias, we
controlled for age, sex, and all the county-level covariates
described in the Method section (total population, Black-
to-White ratio of the population, education, income,
unemployment, poverty, segregation, geographic mobil-
ity, housing density explicit age bias, and implicit age
bias).
Access to health care. Figure 2a shows the relationship
between Whites’ explicit racial bias and Whites’ and
Blacks’ access to affordable health care before account-
ing for covariates. To test whether Black-White disparities
in access to health care were more pronounced in coun-
ties where White respondents showed more racial bias,
even after controlling for county-level covariates, we
0
5
10
15
20
25
30
35
0.0 0.5 1.0 1.5 2.0
R
es
po
nd
en
ts
W
ith
C
irc
ul
at
or
y-
D
is
ea
se
D
ia
gn
os
es
(%
)
Whites’ Explicit Racial Bias
100
150
200
250
300
350
400
450
500
550
600
0.0 0.5 1.0 1.5 2.0
C
irc
ul
at
or
y-
D
is
ea
se
D
ea
th
R
at
e
Whites’ Explicit Racial Bias
a
b c
50
60
70
80
90
100
0.0 0.5 1.0 1.5 2.0
R
es
po
nd
en
ts
W
ith
A
cc
es
s
to
H
ea
lth
C
ar
e
(%
)
Whites’ Explicit Racial Bias
Whites
Blacks
Fig. 2. Bubble plots showing the county-level relationships
between average explicit racial bias of White respon-
dents and (a) the percentage of respondents with access to
affordable health care (Study 1), (b) the percentage
of respondents with diagnoses of circulatory disease (Study 1),
and (c) the age-adjusted rate of death due to
circulatory diseases (deaths per 100,000; Study 2). The size of
each plotted circle is proportionate to the number
of respondents in that county. The lines are the best-fitting
regression lines before covariates were entered in the
model. For visualization purposes only, counties with values
that deviated from the mean by more than 2.5 SD are
not shown in the graphs in (a) and (c) (2% of counties, but see
the Supplemental Material for plots that include
these counties), and the graph in (c) does not show counties that
had fewer than 50 responses on the racial-bias
measure (the bubbles would be too small to visualize).
Blacks’ Death Rate and Whites’ Bias 1305
regressed the percentage of respondents who had access
to affordable health care on race, explicit racial bias,
implicit racial bias, the Race × Explicit Racial Bias interac-
tion, the Race × Implicit Racial Bias interaction, all covari-
ates, and the interactions between the covariates and
race (Table 1). The significant effect of explicit racial bias
was qualified by the Race × Explicit Racial Bias interac-
tion. Simple-slopes analyses indicated that increased
explicit racial bias predicted decreased access to health
care for Blacks, b = −0.064, SE = 0.025, z = 2.563, p =
.0103, but this relationship was not significant for Whites,
b = −0.003, SE = 0.011, z = 0.300, p = .7587. With all
covariates included in the analysis, in counties with high
(1 SD above the mean) explicit racial bias, Blacks were
8% less likely than Whites to report access to affordable
health care. In contrast, in counties with low (1 SD below
the mean) explicit racial bias, Blacks were 3% less likely
than Whites to report access to affordable health care.
Notably, these analyses controlled for multiple socio-
economic indices, so socioeconomic status did not
appear to account for the link between explicit racial bias
and the racial disparity in access to health care. In con-
trast to explicit racial bias, implicit racial bias was unre-
lated to access to health care. Furthermore, given that the
effects of age bias were not significant, the results suggest
that racial bias specifically predicted Black-White dispari-
ties in access to health care. We report simple slopes
for the significant interactions with covariates in the Sup-
plemental Material. Sex did not further moderate the
Race × Explicit Race Bias interaction, p = .4964.
Table 1. Results of the Generalized Estimating Equation
Analysis
Predicting Access to Affordable Health Care in Study 1
Effect b SE z p
Race (Black = 0, White = 1) 0.057 0.008 7.479 < .0001
Explicit racial bias –0.064 0.025 2.563 .0103
Implicit racial bias 0.237 0.153 1.546 .1220
Race × Explicit Racial Bias 0.061 0.023 2.668 .0076
Race × Implicit Racial Bias –0.148 0.140 1.058 .2909
Sex (male = −1, female = 1) –0.010 0.003 3.211 .0013
Age 0.006 0.001 5.256 < .0001
Total population –0.012 0.014 0.894 .3711
Black-to-white ratio –0.037 0.036 1.025 .3066
Education –0.164 0.161 1.020 .3078
Income 0.018 0.007 2.458 .0140
Segregation 0.051 0.056 0.906 .3657
Geographic mobility 0.001 0.002 0.436 .6637
Unemployment –0.001 0.002 0.387 .6989
Poverty –0.002 0.002 0.860 .3899
Housing density < 0.001 < 0.001 1.249 .2117
Explicit age bias –0.023 0.030 0.762 .4456
Implicit age bias 0.103 0.112 0.922 .3575
Race × Sex –0.004 0.003 1.245 .2139
Race × Age –0.002 0.001 1.939 .0524
Race × Total Population 0.003 0.014 0.224 .8197
Race × Black-to-White Ratio 0.028 0.033 0.860 .3912
Race × Education 0.322 0.158 2.042 .0411
Race × Income < 0.001 < 0.001 < 0.001 .9661
Race × Segregation –0.057 0.051 1.122 .2609
Race × Geographic Mobility –0.001 0.002 0.735 .4624
Race × Unemployment –0.001 0.002 0.520 .6033
Race × Poverty 0.005 0.002 2.648 .0081
Race × Housing Density < 0.001 < 0.001 < 0.001 .9952
Race × Explicit Age Bias 0.047 0.028 1.688 .0916
Race × Implicit Age Bias –0.032 0.110 0.300 .7691
Note: Data from 204 counties were included in the analysis.
Pseudo-R2 for the
model was .62.
1306 Leitner et al.
Circulatory-disease diagnoses. Figure 2b shows the
relationship between Whites’ explicit racial bias and
Blacks’ and Whites’ rate of self-reported circulatory dis-
eases before accounting for covariates. To test whether,
after accounting for covariates, Black-White disparities in
diagnoses of circulatory diseases were more pronounced
in counties where White respondents showed more racial
bias, we regressed the percentage of participants in each
county who reported receiving such diagnoses on the
same predictors as in the model for access to health care
(Table 2). The model with all covariates included showed
that Blacks, compared with Whites, were more likely to
receive diagnoses of circulatory diseases. However, nei-
ther explicit nor implicit racial bias was significantly
related to the percentage of respondents with diagnoses
of circulatory diseases. Moreover, neither the Race ×
Explicit Racial Bias interaction nor the Race × Implicit
Racial Bias interaction was significant. Thus, even though
greater explicit racial bias corresponded with increased
Black-White disparities in access to affordable health care,
greater explicit racial bias was not related to increased
Black-White disparities in diagnoses of circulatory
diseases.
Discussion
The Black-White disparity in access to affordable health care
was more pronounced in counties where Whites harbored
more explicit racial bias. However, explicit racial bias did not
predict racial disparities in the percentage of respondents
with circulatory-disease diagnoses. One potential explana-
tion for this null effect is that Blacks in more biased counties
Table 2. Results of the Generalized Estimating Equation
Analysis
Predicting Circulatory-Disease Diagnoses in Study 1
Effect b SE z p
Race (Black = 0, White = 1) –0.008 0.003 2.890 .0038
Explicit racial bias –0.003 0.007 0.436 .6642
Implicit racial bias 0.023 0.038 0.592 .5534
Race × Explicit Racial Bias 0.010 0.007 1.327 .1847
Race × Implicit Racial Bias –0.019 0.040 0.480 .6314
Sex (male = −1, female = 1) –0.006 0.001 4.157 < .0001
Age 0.003 < 0.001 9.501 < .0001
Total population 0.002 0.004 0.346 .7248
Black-to-White ratio 0.004 0.009 0.412 .6772
Education –0.001 0.052 < 0.001 .9899
Income –0.004 0.002 2.345 .0190
Segregation 0.015 0.014 1.072 .2830
Geographic mobility 0.001 < 0.001 1.817 .0693
Unemployment < 0.001 0.001 0.346 .7324
Poverty < 0.001 0.001 0.693 .4896
Housing density < 0.001 < 0.001 1.400 .1615
Explicit age bias 0.007 0.007 0.964 .3356
Implicit age bias –0.022 0.036 0.600 .5467
Race × Sex –0.015 0.002 9.221 < .0001
Race × Age < 0.001 < 0.001 0.917 .3600
Race × Total Population –0.008 0.005 1.775 .0761
Race × Black-to-White Ratio 0.004 0.012 0.300 .7609
Race × Education –0.065 0.052 1.249 .2115
Race × Income 0.001 0.002 0.520 .6051
Race × Segregation –0.014 0.014 1.005 .3145
Race × Geographic Mobility –0.001 < 0.001 2.406 .0161
Race × Unemployment < 0.001 0.001 < 0.001 .9708
Race × Poverty –0.001 0.001 1.459 .1444
Race × Housing Density < 0.001 < 0.001 0.300 .7661
Race × Explicit Age Bias –0.008 0.008 1.077 .2810
Race × Implicit Age Bias 0.036 0.041 0.872 .3821
Note: Data from 204 counties were included in the analysis.
Pseudo-R2 for the
model was .73.
Blacks’ Death Rate and Whites’ Bias 1307
were less likely to see a health-care provider even if they
were sick. If this were the case, it is possible that Blacks
would have a higher death rate due to circulatory diseases in
communities where Whites harbored more explicit racial
bias. We tested this possibility in Study 2.
Study 2
In Study 2, we examined whether the Black-White dis-
parity in rate of death due to circulatory diseases was
more pronounced in counties where Whites harbored
more racial bias.
Data sources
Racial bias. Explicit and implicit racial biases were
estimated with the methods described for Study 1.
Death rate. Death records for Blacks and Whites were
obtained from the CDC (2014). Specifically, we examined
rates (per 100,000) of death from circulatory diseases (e.g.,
heart disease; Internal Statistical Classification of Diseases
and Related Health Problems codes I00–I99) in 2003
through 2013. Circulatory diseases are the leading cause of
death in the United States and have shown pervasive racial
disparities in prevalence over time (U.S. Department of
Health and Human Services, 2014). To account for poten-
tial age differences between counties and racial groups
and allow for more meaningful comparisons, we used age-
adjusted death rates. To derive these age-adjusted rates,
we used the default 2000 U.S. standard population detailed
in Anderson and Rosenberg (1998).
We aggregated the data across males and females,
given that sex did not moderate the effects of bias in
Study 1, and aggregation minimized missing data (i.e.,
aggregated data were less likely to be suppressed by the
CDC than were separated data for males and females).
We were able to obtain death rates for Whites in 3,110
counties and death rates for Blacks in 1,490 counties.
Data were unavailable for counties recording fewer than
10 deaths for a given group. Figure 1b shows the location
of the 1,149 counties for which we obtained racial-bias
data and death-rate data for both Blacks and Whites.
To determine whether racial bias predicted Black-
White disparities in deaths from causes other than circu-
latory diseases, we used the CDC (2014) data set to
compile county-level data on age-adjusted rates of death
due to neoplasm (e.g., cancer) for 2003 through 2013. We
focused on death due to neoplasm because neoplasms
are the second most prevalent cause of death in the
United States (U.S. Department of Health and Human
Services, 2014).
To isolate effects of interest, we incorporated the same
set of county-level covariates used in Study 1, except for
sex (because death rates were aggregated across males
and females) and age (because death rates were already
age adjusted).
Results
As in Study 1, we used GEEs to estimate all effects. Data
from a given county were weighted in the analyses by
the number of respondents who completed the racial-
bias measure in that county. As in Study 1, race was
coded as 0 for Black and 1 for White, and all other pre-
dictors were mean-centered.
Figure 2c shows the relationship between Whites’
explicit racial bias and Blacks’ and Whites’ death rates
due to circulatory diseases before accounting for covari-
ates. To test whether racial disparities in death rate due to
circulatory diseases were more pronounced in counties
where White respondents showed greater racial bias,
even after controlling for a set of county-level covariates,
we regressed circulatory-disease-related death rate on
race, explicit racial bias, implicit racial bias, the Race ×
Explicit Racial Bias interaction, the Race × Implicit Racial
Bias interaction, all covariates, and the interactions
between race and the covariates (Table 3). The effect of
explicit racial bias was qualified by the Race × Explicit
Racial Bias interaction. Simple-slopes analyses indicated
that the relationship between explicit racial bias and rate
of death due to circulatory diseases was positive for both
Blacks and Whites, but stronger for Blacks, b = 43.200,
SE = 12.100, z = 3.559, p = .0004, than for Whites, b =
13.900, SE = 4.970, z = 2.795, p = .0052. As in Study 1, the
Race × Explicit Racial Bias interaction was significant
over and above the effects of age bias, which suggests
that racial bias specifically was related to Black-White
disparities in death rate. Neither the main effect for
implicit racial bias nor the Race × Implicit Racial Bias
interaction was significant.
With all covariates included in the model, in counties
with high (1 SD above the mean) explicit racial bias, the
difference between Blacks’ and Whites’ death rates was
62 deaths per 100,000. In contrast, in counties with low (1
SD below the mean) explicit bias, the difference was 35
deaths per 100,000. Furthermore, adjusting for all covari-
ates, we estimated how many more Blacks died of circu-
latory-related diseases annually in counties that were high
(1 SD above the mean), rather than low (1 SD below the
mean), in explicit racial bias. We made this estimate at the
average Black population level in counties for which we
had death-rate data for Blacks (average Black popula-
tion = 28,598); 11 more Blacks per county were predicted
to die annually in high-explicit-bias counties (95 deaths)
than in low-explicit-bias counties (84 deaths).
To determine whether similar effects would emerge for
death rate not due to circulatory diseases, we regressed
1308 Leitner et al.
rate of death due to neoplasm on the same predictors as
in our analyses of deaths due to circulatory diseases. Nei-
ther the main effect of explicit racial bias nor the Race ×
Explicit Racial Bias interaction was significant, ps > .14.
Moreover, when we modeled neoplasm death rate as a
covariate in the model predicting death rate due to circu-
latory diseases, the Race × Explicit Racial Bias interaction
remained significant, b = −21.100, SE = 9.240, z = 2.283,
p = .0224. These findings suggest that that the relationship
between explicit racial bias and death rate was specific to
circulatory-related, and not neoplasm-related, disease.
(See the Supplemental Material for additional analyses.)
Discussion
In counties where White respondents harbored more
explicit racial bias, the rate of death from circulatory dis-
ease was increased for both Whites and Blacks. However,
Whites’ explicit racial bias predicted this death rate more
strongly for Blacks than for Whites. As in Study 1, explicit
racial bias, compared with implicit racial bias, was a
stronger predictor of Black-White health disparity.
General Discussion
In counties where White Project Implicit respondents
harbored more explicit racial bias, Black-White dispari-
ties in access to affordable health care (Study 1) and rate
of death due to circulatory diseases (Study 2) were more
pronounced. The robustness of these findings was evi-
denced by the replication of the same pattern across two
independent data sets that both included a large number
of counties. Although the effects could have been driven
by an unmeasured third variable, the fact that racial bias
was a significant predictor in models with a large set of
covariates supports the notion of a direct relationship
between Whites’ racial bias and Black-White health
disparities.
Table 3. Results of the Generalized Estimating Equation
Analysis
Predicting Death Rate Due to Circulatory Diseases in Study 2
Effect b SE z p
Race (Black = 0, White = 1) –48.400 7.000 6.917 < .0001
Explicit racial bias 43.200 12.100 3.559 .0004
Implicit racial bias 64.400 56.100 1.149 .2513
Race × Explicit Racial Bias –29.300 11.700 2.500 .0124
Race × Implicit Racial Bias –3.810 54.800 0.000 .9445
Total population –9.390 10.300 0.917 .3602
Black-to-White Ratio 92.300 25.400 3.632 .0003
Education –176.000 99.700 1.769 .0769
Income –8.200 4.140 1.982 .0475
Segregation 19.000 12.700 1.493 .1350
Geographic mobility –2.500 0.485 5.146 < .0001
Unemployment 2.140 1.180 1.817 .0695
Poverty 1.570 0.935 1.676 .0934
Housing density < 0.001 < 0.001 < 0.001 .9762
Explicit age bias –16.400 8.870 1.849 .0645
Implicit age bias 24.600 47.000 0.520 .6005
Race × Total Population –2.560 8.560 0.300 .7647
Race × Black-to-White Ratio –63.900 32.700 1.954 .0506
Race × Education –17.600 88.700 0.200 .8431
Race × Income 0.313 4.140 0.100 .9397
Race × Segregation –6.610 11.700 0.566 .5730
Race × Geographic Mobility 2.020 0.462 4.374 < .0001
Race × Unemployment –0.852 1.130 0.755 .4518
Race × Poverty –2.180 0.868 2.508 .0122
Race × Housing Density < 0.001 0.001 0.583 .5580
Race × Explicit Age Bias –1.170 8.480 0.141 .8903
Race × Implicit Age Bias 34.000 40.000 0.849 .3955
Note: Data from 1,776 counties were included in the analysis.
Pseudo-R2 for the
model was .42.
Blacks’ Death Rate and Whites’ Bias 1309
To our knowledge, this is the first research to show
that racial bias from a dominant group (e.g., Whites) pre-
dicts negative health outcomes more strongly for the tar-
get group (e.g., Blacks) than for the dominant group.
These results are consistent with research that has shown
that population-level bias (i.e., antigay attitudes), when
aggregated across targeted and nontargeted groups, pre-
dicts negative health outcomes more strongly for the
targeted than the nontargeted group (Hatzenbuehler,
Bellatorre, et al., 2014). However, the current results
extend this previous work, which did not directly address
the issue of whether the effects were driven by the domi-
nant group’s stigmatization of the targeted group or the
targeted group’s self-stigmatization. Furthermore, by
demonstrating a relationship between Whites’ racial
biases and health outcomes of Blacks in the same com-
munity, these results support previous findings that
Blacks’ subjective perceptions of racism are linked to
their own health (Pascoe & Richman, 2009; Williams &
Mohammed, 2009). However, because perceptions of
racism can be shaped by race-based rejection sensitivity
(Mendoza-Denton et al., 2002), we extended this previ-
ous work by measuring racial bias directly from Whites.
Additionally, to our knowledge, this is the first research
to use geo-coded data on implicit bias to examine
whether racial health disparities are more pronounced in
communities where Whites show more implicit bias.
Notably, when implicit bias was modeled without explicit
bias, it showed patterns similar to those for explicit bias
(see Tables S4 and S6 in the Supplemental Material), but
when explicit and implicit biases were modeled together,
only explicit bias predicted health outcomes. The null
effects of implicit bias are informative, as research is
increasingly focusing on the predictive utility of implicit
bias in medical contexts (e.g., Hall et al., 2015), and it is
important to identify the outcomes that are more strongly
related to explicit than to implicit bias. One potential
explanation for why Whites’ explicit bias was a stronger
predictor than their implicit bias in the current work is
that explicit bias has historically exerted stronger effects
on the structural factors (e.g., regulation of environmen-
tal pollution) and psychological factors (e.g., overtly neg-
ative interracial interactions) that ultimately shape health
outcomes.
One limitation of this research is that the respondents
who completed Project Implicit’s racial-bias measures
might not have been representative of their counties on
all dimensions. For instance, Project Implicit respondents
might not reflect racial biases of older community mem-
bers. Though we employed a poststratification weighting
scheme designed to circumvent this limitation, no amount
of poststratification weighting can make a sample truly
representative on all dimensions. Thus, future research
should examine whether the observed effects remain
when bias is measured with full probability sampling.
Another limitation is that we did not have data on the
geographic mobility of specific individuals for whom we
assessed health outcomes. It is possible that the deceased
in Study 2 had moved to their communities soon before
their deaths and had died before the racial bias of their
communities had any effect on their health. However,
two lines of evidence suggest that this scenario occurs
relatively infrequently. First, 95% of the people who died
from circulatory-disease-related causes were over age 55
(CDC, 2014), and the median duration of residency for
people in this age group is more than 11 years (Mateyka
& Marlay, 2011). Second, when people of this age do
move, they are more likely to move to another residence
within the same county than to a different county (data
from the U.S. Census Bureau’s 2009–2013 ACS: report
titled Geographic Mobility by Selected Characteristics in
the United States). Together, this evidence suggests that
many people are geographically stable in the years lead-
ing to their death. Nevertheless, future research might
further examine the role of geographic mobility in the
relationship between racial bias and health.
The finding that Whites’ circulatory-disease-related
death rate was increased in counties where White respon-
dents harbored greater explicit bias is consistent with
research showing that racial bias is linked to negative
health outcomes for Whites (Kennedy et al., 1997; Lee
et al., 2015; Mendes, Gray, Mendoza-Denton, Major, &
Epel, 2007). One explanation for this finding, suggested
by recent research (Lee et al., 2015), is that highly biased
communities have decreased social capital (i.e., trust and
bonding between community members), which in turn
predicts negative health outcomes.
Though the current research does not establish that
White respondents’ racial bias caused the observed
health disparities between Whites and Blacks, the rela-
tionships between Whites’ racial bias and Black-White
health disparities were independent of a large set of
county-level socio-demographic characteristics. Thus,
our findings raise compelling questions about the mecha-
nisms through which Whites’ racial bias can be related to
health outcomes. On the basis of existing theoretical
frameworks (Clark et al., 1999; Hatzenbuehler et al.,
2013; Major et al., 2013), we posit that multiple causal
pathways might account for this relationship. These path-
ways might include structural (e.g., discrimination in
health care), interpersonal (e.g., hostile interactions),
emotional (e.g., stress), and behavioral (e.g., maladaptive
coping) processes that catalyze biological systems that
increase disease risk. We hope that the current work
serves to generate future research examining why there
is a relationship between racial bias and health.
1310 Leitner et al.
Action Editor
Jamin Halberstadt served as action editor for this article.
Author Contributions
J. B. Leitner, E. Hehman, and R. Mendoza-Denton developed
the theoretical framework. J. B. Leitner conducted the analyses
and drafted the manuscript. All the authors provided critical
revisions and approved the final version of the manuscript.
Acknowledgments
We are grateful to Doc Edge for providing insight into our
statistical analyses.
Declaration of Conflicting Interests
The authors declared that they had no conflicts of interest with
respect to their authorship or the publication of this article.
Funding
This research was supported by the National Science Founda-
tion under Award Numbers 1306709 (to R. Mendoza-Denton
and O. Ayduk) and 1514510 (to O. Ayduk and J. B. Leitner),
and by a Social Sciences and Humanities Research Council
Institutional Grant (to E. Hehman).
Supplemental Material
Additional supporting information can be found at http://
pss.sagepub.com/content/by/supplemental-data
Open Practices
All data and materials have been made publicly available via
the Open Science Framework and can be accessed at https://
osf.io/5ybhd/. The complete Open Practices Disclosure for this
article can be found at http://pss.sagepub.com/content/by/
supplemental-data. This article has received badges for Open
Data and Open Materials. More information about the Open
Practices badges can be found at https://osf.io/tvyxz/wiki/
1.%20View%20the%20Badges/ and http://pss.sagepub.com/
content/25/1/3.full.
Note
1. Unless otherwise noted, all data from the U.S. Census Bureau
were downloaded from factfinder.census.gov.
References
Anderson, R. N., & Rosenberg, H. M. (1998). Age standardiza-
tion of death rates: Implementation of the year 2000 stan-
dard. National Vital Statistics Reports, 47(3), 1–17.
Barnes, L. L., Mendes De Leon, C. F., Lewis, T. T., Bienias,
J. L.,
Wilson, R. S., & Evans, D. A. (2008). Perceived discrimi-
nation and mortality in a population-based study of older
adults. American Journal of Public Health, 98, 1241–1247.
doi:10.2105/AJPH.2007.114397
Black, P. H., & Garbutt, L. D. (2002). Stress, inflammation and
cardiovascular disease. Journal of Psychosomatic Research,
52, 1–23. doi:10.1016/S0022-3999(01)00302-6
Centers for Disease Control and Prevention. (2013). Behavioral
Risk Factor Surveillance System survey data. Atlanta, GA:
U.S. Department of Health and Human Services, Centers for
Disease Control and Prevention.
Centers for Disease Control and Prevention. (2014). Under-
lying cause of death [Data set]. Retrieved from http://
wonder.cdc.gov/ucd-icd10.html
Chae, D. H., Clouston, S., Hatzenbuehler, M. L., Kramer, M. R.,
Cooper, H. L. F., Wilson, S. M., . . . Link, B. G. (2015).
Association between an Internet-based measure of area rac-
ism and Black mortality. PLoS ONE, 10(4), Article e0122963.
doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0122963
Clark, R., Anderson, N. B., Clark, V. R., & Williams, D. R.
(1999).
Racism as a stressor for African Americans. American
Psychologist, 54, 805–816.
Dovidio, J. F., Kawakami, K., & Gaertner, S. L. (2002).
Implicit and explicit prejudice and interracial interaction.
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 82, 62–68.
doi:10.1037/0022-3514.82.1.62
Frey, W. H., & Myers, D. (2005). Racial segregation in U.S.
met-
ropolitan areas and cities, 1990–2000: Patterns, trends, and
explanations (Population Studies Center Research Report
05-573). Retrieved from http://www.frey-demographer.org/
reports/R-2005-2_RacialSegragationTrends.pdf
Gonsalkorale, K., Sherman, J. W., & Klauer, K. C. (2009).
Aging
and prejudice: Diminished regulation of automatic race
bias among older adults. Journal of Experimental Social
Psychology, 45, 410–414.
Greenwald, A. G., Nosek, B. A., & Banaji, M. R. (2003).
Understanding and using the Implicit Association Test:
I. An improved scoring algorithm. Journal of Personality
and Social Psychology, 85, 197–216. doi:10.1037/0022-
3514.85.2.197
Greenwald, A. G., Poehlman, T. A., Uhlmann, E. L., & Banaji,
M. R. (2009). Understanding and using the Implicit
Association Test: III. Meta-analysis of predictive validity.
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 97, 17–41.
doi:10.1037/a0015575
Hall, W. J., Chapman, M. V., Lee, K. M., Merino, Y. M.,
Thomas, T. W., Payne, B. K., . . . Coyne-Beasley, T. (2015).
Implicit racial/ethnic bias among health care professionals
and its influence on health care outcomes: A systematic
review. American Journal of Public Health, 105, e60–e76.
doi:10.2105/AJPH.2015.302903
Hatzenbuehler, M. L., Bellatorre, A., Lee, Y., Finch, B.,
Muennig,
P., & Fiscella, K. (2014). Structural stigma and all-cause
mortality in sexual minority populations. Social Science &
Medicine, 103, 33–41.
Hatzenbuehler, M. L., Jun, H.-J., Corliss, H. L., & Austin, S. B.
(2014). Structural stigma and cigarette smoking in a pro-
spective cohort study of sexual minority and heterosexual
youth. Annals of Behavioral Medicine, 47, 48–56.
https://osf.io/tvyxz/wiki/1.%20View%20the%20Badges/
https://osf.io/tvyxz/wiki/1.%20View%20the%20Badges/
http://www.frey-demographer.org/reports/R-2005-
2_RacialSegragationTrends.pdf
http://www.frey-demographer.org/reports/R-2005-
2_RacialSegragationTrends.pdf
http://pss.sagepub.com/content/by/supplemental-data
http://pss.sagepub.com/content/by/supplemental-data
http://pss.sagepub.com/content/25/1/3.full
http://pss.sagepub.com/content/25/1/3.full
Blacks’ Death Rate and Whites’ Bias 1311
Hatzenbuehler, M. L., Phelan, J. C., & Link, B. G. (2013).
Stigma
as a fundamental cause of population health inequalities.
American Journal of Public Health, 103, 813–821.
Hofmann, W., Gawronski, B., Gschwendner, T., Le, H.,
& Schmitt, M. (2005). A meta-analysis on the correla-
tion between the Implicit Association Test and explicit
self-report measures. Personality and Social Psychology
Bulletin, 31, 1369–1385.
Hubbard, A. E., Ahern, J., Fleischer, N. L., Van der Laan, M.,
Lippman, S. A., Jewell, N., . . . Satariano, W. A. (2010). To
GEE or not to GEE: Comparing population average and
mixed models for estimating the associations between neigh-
borhood risk factors and health. Epidemiology, 21, 467–474.
Kennedy, B. P., Kawachi, I., Lochner, K., Jones, C., &
Prothrow-
Stith, D. (1997). (Dis)respect and black mortality. Ethnicity
& Disease, 7(3), 207–214.
Lee, Y., Kawachi, I., Muennig, P., & Hatzenbuehler, M. L.
(2015). Effects of racial prejudice on the health of commu-
nities: A multilevel survival analysis. American Journal of
Public Health, 105, 2349–2355.
Lohr, S. (2009). Sampling: Design and analysis. Boston, MA:
Brooks/Cole.
Major, B., Mendes, W. B., & Dovidio, J. F. (2013). Intergroup
relations and health disparities: A social psychological
perspective. Health Psychology, 32, 514–524. doi:10.1038/
nsmb.2010
Mateyka, P., & Marlay, M. (2011). Residential duration by ten-
ure, race, and ethnicity: 2009. Retrieved from https://
www.census.gov/content/dam/Census/library/working-
papers/2011/demo/2009-Duration.pdf
Mendes, W. B., Gray, H. M., Mendoza-Denton, R., Major, B., &
Epel, E. S. (2007). Why egalitarianism might be good for
your health: Physiological thriving during stressful encoun-
ters. Psychological Science, 18, 991–998. doi:10.1111/j.1467-
9280.2007.02014.x
Mendoza-Denton, R., Downey, G., Purdie, V. J., Davis, A., &
Pietrzak, J. (2002). Sensitivity to status-based rejection:
Implications for African American students’ college expe-
rience. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 83,
896–918. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.83.4.896
Pascoe, E. A., & Richman, L. S. (2009). Perceived
discrimination
and health: A meta-analytic review. Psychological Bulletin,
135, 531–554. doi:10.1037/a0016059
Sawyer, P. J., Major, B., Casad, B. J., Townsend, S. S. M., &
Mendes, W. B. (2012). Discrimination and the stress
response: Psychological and physiological consequences of
anticipating prejudice in interethnic interactions. American
Journal of Public Health, 102, 1020–1026. doi:10.2105/
AJPH.2011.300620
Schmidt, K., & Nosek, B. A. (2010). Implicit (and explicit)
racial attitudes barely changed during Barack Obama’s
presidential campaign and early presidency. Journal of
Experimental Social Psychology, 46, 308–314. doi:10.1016/
j.jesp.2009.12.003
U.S. Census Bureau. (2013). Metropolitan and Micropolitan
Delineation Files [Data set]. Retrieved from http://www
.census.gov/population/metro/data/def.html
U.S. Department of Health and Human Services, Centers for
Disease Control and Prevention, National Center for Health
Statistics. (2014). Health, United States, 2014. Retrieved
from http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/books/NBK299348/
pdf/Bookshelf_NBK299348.pdf
Williams, D. R., & Mohammed, S. A. (2009). Discrimination
and
racial disparities in health: Evidence and needed research.
Journal of Behavioral Medicine, 32, 20–47. doi:10.1007/
s10865-008-9185-0
Xu, K., Nosek, B., & Greenwald, A. G. (2014). Data from the
Race Implicit Association Test on the Project Implicit Demo
Website. Journal of Open Psychology Data, 2(1), Article e3.
doi:10.5334/jopd.ac
http://www.census.gov/population/metro/data/def.html
http://www.census.gov/population/metro/data/def.html
http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/books/NBK299348/pdf/Bookshelf_
NBK299348.pdf
http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/books/NBK299348/pdf/Bookshelf_
NBK299348.pdf
Fall 2017 General Psychology: Writing Assignment
Your assignment is to write a paper reacting to a scholarly
journal article. The purpose of this
assignment is to give you practice in reading, understanding,
and critically evaluating research.
You are also expected to demonstrate the ability to synthesize
current research with our course
materials. This paper requires more than a simple summary of
what you have read. Please
allow yourself plenty of time to complete this assignment.
You are going to write about the following article:
Leitner, J. B., Hehman, E., Ayduk, O., & Mendoza-Denton, R.
(2016). Blacks’ death rate due to
circulatory diseases is positively related to whites’ explicit
racial bias: A nationwide
investigation using project implicit. Psychological Science, 27,
1299-1311. doi:
10.1177/0956797616658450
You can download the article here: http://bit.ly/2xIrOlR
Your paper should include two parts:
1. Part 1 - A brief summary of the article that answers the
questions in the attached answer
sheet. Please note:
a. You should not cite verbatim from the paper. You MUST use
your own words.
b. If there is no clear answer in the paper to any of the
questions, you can answer as
you see fit, but you MUST explain your answers. Otherwise,
you will not
receive points for that particular answer. You can also say that
there is no clear
answer in the article.
c. If there are few studies reported in the paper, please answer
the questions for
each study.
d. You are not responsible for knowing or reporting the
statistical analyses, the
details of the manipulations, or the procedures.
e. Answers to Part 1 should be ONLY within the text-boxes
provided, using
12point Times New Roman font.
2. Part 2 - Your critical reaction to the article and your
conclusions. You might critique the
research (for example, sample, method) and/or the conclusions
the authors arrived at; you
can identify unresolved issues, suggest future research, and/or
reflect on the study’s
implications. In your discussion, you must clearly connect the
material you are learning
in class with the article’s content area, theories, methodology,
and/or findings.
Part 2 of the paper should not be longer than one page. Use
double-spaced, 12-point
Times New Roman font, with 1-inch margins on the bottom,
top, and sides. We will
deduct points for deviating from these parameters.
For clarification, Part 2 must include:
a. At least one justified original critique (that is, critique that is
not based on the
limitations the authors discussed in the paper)
b. At least one suggestion for future research
c. At least one connection to class materials
d. At least one practical or theoretical implication of this study
e. Your overall impression of the article
To successfully complete this assignment, you should carefully
read the article, more than once.
When read the article, go beyond highlighting important facts
and interesting findings. Ask
yourself questions as you read: What are the researchers’
assumptions? How does the article
contribute to the field? Are the findings generalizable, and to
whom?
We strongly suggest consulting Chapter 2 (research methods)
and the relevant class notes when
working on this paper.
Please submit the assignment to TurnitIn in a WORD file. Do
not submit a PDF (we will deduct
points for that). See relevant instructions page.
The complete assignment should be submitted as one file.
Please use the Word file provided on
BB.
The assignment is worth 10 points of your overall class grade.
Grading criteria:
1. Each part is worth 50% of your grade. These refer to (a)
accuracy and clarity of answers;
(b) justifications of your answers; (c) answering all the parts of
each question as appear in
the answer sheet and in the instruction on this document; (d)
Grammatically correct
writing and adherence to length and formatting requirements.
For issues of grammar,
syntax, and style, we encourage you to use the services of the
Writing Center.
The paper is due by Wednesday, November 1st at 11:59PM.
You must submit the paper via Turnitin ONLY in a Word file.
We will provide you the
details in a separate document, to be found in BB under the
Writing Assignment tab. You must
load only one document, as Turnitin does not accept more than
one document.
TIPS FOR SCIENTIFIC WRITING
Psychology is a behavioral science, and writing in psychology
is similar to writing in the hard
sciences.
Avoid anthropomorphism: Anthropomorphism occurs when
human characteristics are
attributed to animals or inanimate entities. Anthropomorphism
can make your writing
awkward. Some examples include: “The experiment attempted
to demonstrate…,” and “The
tables compare…” Reword such sentences so that a person
performs the action: “The
experimenter attempted to demonstrate…” The verbs “show” or
“indicate” can also be used:
“The tables show…”
Verb tenses: Select verb tenses carefully. Use the past tense
when expressing actions or
conditions that occurred at a specific time in the past, when
discussing other people’s work, and
when reporting results. Use the present perfect tense to express
past actions or conditions that did
not occur at a specific time, or to describe an action beginning
in the past and continuing in the
present.
Pronoun agreement: Be consistent within and across sentences
with pronouns that refer to a
noun introduced earlier (antecedent). A common error is a
construction such as “Each child
responded to questions about their favorite toys.” The sentence
should have either a plural subject
(children) or a singular pronoun (his or her). Vague pronouns,
such as “this” or “that,” without a
clear antecedent can confuse readers: “This shows that girls are
more likely than boys …” could
be rewritten as “These results show that girls are more likely
than boys…”
Avoid figurative language and superlatives: Scientific writing
should be as concise and
specific as possible. Emotional language and superlatives, such
as “very,” “highly,”
“astonishingly,” “extremely,” “quite,” and even “exactly,” are
imprecise or unnecessary. A line
that is “exactly 100 centimeters” is, simply, 100 centimeters.
Avoid colloquial expressions and informal language: Use
“children” rather than “kids;”
“many” rather than “a lot;” “acquire” rather than “get;”
“prepare for” rather than “get ready;” etc.
Labels: Do not equate people with their physical or mental
conditions or categorize people
broadly as objects. For example, adjectival forms like “older
adults” are preferable to labels such
as “the elderly” or “the schizophrenics.” Another option is to
mention the person first, followed
by a descriptive phrase. For example, “people diagnosed with
schizophrenia.” Be careful using
the label “normal,” as it may imply that others are abnormal.

More Related Content

Similar to Writing Assignment Answer SheetWhen answering these questions,.docx

Everyday Discrimination, Gender Differences in Major Lifetime Discrimination,...
Everyday Discrimination, Gender Differences in Major Lifetime Discrimination,...Everyday Discrimination, Gender Differences in Major Lifetime Discrimination,...
Everyday Discrimination, Gender Differences in Major Lifetime Discrimination,...
Wayne State University College of Liberal Arts and Sciences
 
Race medicine
Race medicineRace medicine
Race medicineantone04
 
Discussion Gender Bias in ResearchHistorically, men have do
Discussion Gender Bias in ResearchHistorically, men have doDiscussion Gender Bias in ResearchHistorically, men have do
Discussion Gender Bias in ResearchHistorically, men have do
widdowsonerica
 
Racism as Experienced by Physicians of Color in the Health Care Setting
Racism as Experienced by Physicians of Color in the Health Care Setting Racism as Experienced by Physicians of Color in the Health Care Setting
Racism as Experienced by Physicians of Color in the Health Care Setting
MiraMed: A Global Services Company
 
Physical Activity and White Matter Hyperintensities, A Systematic Review of Q...
Physical Activity and White Matter Hyperintensities, A Systematic Review of Q...Physical Activity and White Matter Hyperintensities, A Systematic Review of Q...
Physical Activity and White Matter Hyperintensities, A Systematic Review of Q...Emily Strack
 
CH30 Ethics and the Advanced Practice Nurse Essay.pdf
CH30 Ethics and the Advanced Practice Nurse Essay.pdfCH30 Ethics and the Advanced Practice Nurse Essay.pdf
CH30 Ethics and the Advanced Practice Nurse Essay.pdf
bkbk37
 
Running Head ADVANCED NURSING RESEARCH1ADVANCED NURSING RES.docx
Running Head ADVANCED NURSING RESEARCH1ADVANCED NURSING RES.docxRunning Head ADVANCED NURSING RESEARCH1ADVANCED NURSING RES.docx
Running Head ADVANCED NURSING RESEARCH1ADVANCED NURSING RES.docx
toddr4
 
Final PaperThis Final Paper involves the critical review and ana.docx
Final PaperThis Final Paper involves the critical review and ana.docxFinal PaperThis Final Paper involves the critical review and ana.docx
Final PaperThis Final Paper involves the critical review and ana.docx
ssuser454af01
 
1 Clinical Problem Social Anxiety is described by .docx
1  Clinical Problem  Social Anxiety is described by .docx1  Clinical Problem  Social Anxiety is described by .docx
1 Clinical Problem Social Anxiety is described by .docx
croftsshanon
 
1 Clinical Problem Social Anxiety is described by .docx
1  Clinical Problem  Social Anxiety is described by .docx1  Clinical Problem  Social Anxiety is described by .docx
1 Clinical Problem Social Anxiety is described by .docx
jeremylockett77
 
Running head RESEARCH PROPOSAL10RESEARCH PROPOSAL 8.docx
Running head RESEARCH PROPOSAL10RESEARCH PROPOSAL 8.docxRunning head RESEARCH PROPOSAL10RESEARCH PROPOSAL 8.docx
Running head RESEARCH PROPOSAL10RESEARCH PROPOSAL 8.docx
toltonkendal
 
2Complete AssignmentStudent’s NameInstitutio.docx
2Complete AssignmentStudent’s NameInstitutio.docx2Complete AssignmentStudent’s NameInstitutio.docx
2Complete AssignmentStudent’s NameInstitutio.docx
robert345678
 
A systematic literature review on outcomes associated with believing in free ...
A systematic literature review on outcomes associated with believing in free ...A systematic literature review on outcomes associated with believing in free ...
A systematic literature review on outcomes associated with believing in free ...
Will Edwards
 
Tangible Needs and External Stressors Faced by Chinese Ameri.docx
Tangible Needs and External Stressors Faced by Chinese Ameri.docxTangible Needs and External Stressors Faced by Chinese Ameri.docx
Tangible Needs and External Stressors Faced by Chinese Ameri.docx
perryk1
 
Substance Use & Misuse, 46808–818, 2011Copyright C© 2011 In.docx
Substance Use & Misuse, 46808–818, 2011Copyright C© 2011 In.docxSubstance Use & Misuse, 46808–818, 2011Copyright C© 2011 In.docx
Substance Use & Misuse, 46808–818, 2011Copyright C© 2011 In.docx
james891
 
SOCW 04 week 7 discussion #3 professor questionUSW1_SOCW_6090_.docx
SOCW 04 week 7 discussion #3 professor questionUSW1_SOCW_6090_.docxSOCW 04 week 7 discussion #3 professor questionUSW1_SOCW_6090_.docx
SOCW 04 week 7 discussion #3 professor questionUSW1_SOCW_6090_.docx
whitneyleman54422
 
Final Project Sampling 2FINAL PROJECT SAMPL.docx
Final Project Sampling  2FINAL PROJECT SAMPL.docxFinal Project Sampling  2FINAL PROJECT SAMPL.docx
Final Project Sampling 2FINAL PROJECT SAMPL.docx
voversbyobersby
 
A Naturalistic Study Of Dissociative Identity Disorder And Dissociative Disor...
A Naturalistic Study Of Dissociative Identity Disorder And Dissociative Disor...A Naturalistic Study Of Dissociative Identity Disorder And Dissociative Disor...
A Naturalistic Study Of Dissociative Identity Disorder And Dissociative Disor...
Sara Alvarez
 

Similar to Writing Assignment Answer SheetWhen answering these questions,.docx (20)

Everyday Discrimination, Gender Differences in Major Lifetime Discrimination,...
Everyday Discrimination, Gender Differences in Major Lifetime Discrimination,...Everyday Discrimination, Gender Differences in Major Lifetime Discrimination,...
Everyday Discrimination, Gender Differences in Major Lifetime Discrimination,...
 
Race medicine
Race medicineRace medicine
Race medicine
 
Discussion Gender Bias in ResearchHistorically, men have do
Discussion Gender Bias in ResearchHistorically, men have doDiscussion Gender Bias in ResearchHistorically, men have do
Discussion Gender Bias in ResearchHistorically, men have do
 
Racism as Experienced by Physicians of Color in the Health Care Setting
Racism as Experienced by Physicians of Color in the Health Care Setting Racism as Experienced by Physicians of Color in the Health Care Setting
Racism as Experienced by Physicians of Color in the Health Care Setting
 
Physical Activity and White Matter Hyperintensities, A Systematic Review of Q...
Physical Activity and White Matter Hyperintensities, A Systematic Review of Q...Physical Activity and White Matter Hyperintensities, A Systematic Review of Q...
Physical Activity and White Matter Hyperintensities, A Systematic Review of Q...
 
CH30 Ethics and the Advanced Practice Nurse Essay.pdf
CH30 Ethics and the Advanced Practice Nurse Essay.pdfCH30 Ethics and the Advanced Practice Nurse Essay.pdf
CH30 Ethics and the Advanced Practice Nurse Essay.pdf
 
Running Head ADVANCED NURSING RESEARCH1ADVANCED NURSING RES.docx
Running Head ADVANCED NURSING RESEARCH1ADVANCED NURSING RES.docxRunning Head ADVANCED NURSING RESEARCH1ADVANCED NURSING RES.docx
Running Head ADVANCED NURSING RESEARCH1ADVANCED NURSING RES.docx
 
Final PaperThis Final Paper involves the critical review and ana.docx
Final PaperThis Final Paper involves the critical review and ana.docxFinal PaperThis Final Paper involves the critical review and ana.docx
Final PaperThis Final Paper involves the critical review and ana.docx
 
1 Clinical Problem Social Anxiety is described by .docx
1  Clinical Problem  Social Anxiety is described by .docx1  Clinical Problem  Social Anxiety is described by .docx
1 Clinical Problem Social Anxiety is described by .docx
 
1 Clinical Problem Social Anxiety is described by .docx
1  Clinical Problem  Social Anxiety is described by .docx1  Clinical Problem  Social Anxiety is described by .docx
1 Clinical Problem Social Anxiety is described by .docx
 
Running head RESEARCH PROPOSAL10RESEARCH PROPOSAL 8.docx
Running head RESEARCH PROPOSAL10RESEARCH PROPOSAL 8.docxRunning head RESEARCH PROPOSAL10RESEARCH PROPOSAL 8.docx
Running head RESEARCH PROPOSAL10RESEARCH PROPOSAL 8.docx
 
2Complete AssignmentStudent’s NameInstitutio.docx
2Complete AssignmentStudent’s NameInstitutio.docx2Complete AssignmentStudent’s NameInstitutio.docx
2Complete AssignmentStudent’s NameInstitutio.docx
 
A systematic literature review on outcomes associated with believing in free ...
A systematic literature review on outcomes associated with believing in free ...A systematic literature review on outcomes associated with believing in free ...
A systematic literature review on outcomes associated with believing in free ...
 
SWPA Presentation
SWPA PresentationSWPA Presentation
SWPA Presentation
 
Tangible Needs and External Stressors Faced by Chinese Ameri.docx
Tangible Needs and External Stressors Faced by Chinese Ameri.docxTangible Needs and External Stressors Faced by Chinese Ameri.docx
Tangible Needs and External Stressors Faced by Chinese Ameri.docx
 
FioreFinalProject
FioreFinalProjectFioreFinalProject
FioreFinalProject
 
Substance Use & Misuse, 46808–818, 2011Copyright C© 2011 In.docx
Substance Use & Misuse, 46808–818, 2011Copyright C© 2011 In.docxSubstance Use & Misuse, 46808–818, 2011Copyright C© 2011 In.docx
Substance Use & Misuse, 46808–818, 2011Copyright C© 2011 In.docx
 
SOCW 04 week 7 discussion #3 professor questionUSW1_SOCW_6090_.docx
SOCW 04 week 7 discussion #3 professor questionUSW1_SOCW_6090_.docxSOCW 04 week 7 discussion #3 professor questionUSW1_SOCW_6090_.docx
SOCW 04 week 7 discussion #3 professor questionUSW1_SOCW_6090_.docx
 
Final Project Sampling 2FINAL PROJECT SAMPL.docx
Final Project Sampling  2FINAL PROJECT SAMPL.docxFinal Project Sampling  2FINAL PROJECT SAMPL.docx
Final Project Sampling 2FINAL PROJECT SAMPL.docx
 
A Naturalistic Study Of Dissociative Identity Disorder And Dissociative Disor...
A Naturalistic Study Of Dissociative Identity Disorder And Dissociative Disor...A Naturalistic Study Of Dissociative Identity Disorder And Dissociative Disor...
A Naturalistic Study Of Dissociative Identity Disorder And Dissociative Disor...
 

More from ambersalomon88660

1. Lists crimes and crime involvement on the Mendez brothers.2.I.docx
1. Lists crimes and crime involvement on the Mendez brothers.2.I.docx1. Lists crimes and crime involvement on the Mendez brothers.2.I.docx
1. Lists crimes and crime involvement on the Mendez brothers.2.I.docx
ambersalomon88660
 
1. Lists and analyzes strengths and weaknesses based on each of th.docx
1. Lists and analyzes strengths and weaknesses based on each of th.docx1. Lists and analyzes strengths and weaknesses based on each of th.docx
1. Lists and analyzes strengths and weaknesses based on each of th.docx
ambersalomon88660
 
1. List eight basic initiatives that companies can use to gain c.docx
1. List eight basic initiatives that companies can use to gain c.docx1. List eight basic initiatives that companies can use to gain c.docx
1. List eight basic initiatives that companies can use to gain c.docx
ambersalomon88660
 
1. Koffman Corporation is trying to raise capital. What method wou.docx
1. Koffman Corporation is trying to raise capital. What method wou.docx1. Koffman Corporation is trying to raise capital. What method wou.docx
1. Koffman Corporation is trying to raise capital. What method wou.docx
ambersalomon88660
 
1. List all the entities that interact with the TIMS system. Start b.docx
1. List all the entities that interact with the TIMS system. Start b.docx1. List all the entities that interact with the TIMS system. Start b.docx
1. List all the entities that interact with the TIMS system. Start b.docx
ambersalomon88660
 
1. Know the terminology flash cards.2. Know the hist.docx
1. Know the terminology flash cards.2. Know the hist.docx1. Know the terminology flash cards.2. Know the hist.docx
1. Know the terminology flash cards.2. Know the hist.docx
ambersalomon88660
 
1. Journal Entry The attached (BUROS Center for Testing).docx
1. Journal Entry  The attached (BUROS Center for Testing).docx1. Journal Entry  The attached (BUROS Center for Testing).docx
1. Journal Entry The attached (BUROS Center for Testing).docx
ambersalomon88660
 
1. Introduction and thesisThrough extensive research I hope to f.docx
1. Introduction and thesisThrough extensive research I hope to f.docx1. Introduction and thesisThrough extensive research I hope to f.docx
1. Introduction and thesisThrough extensive research I hope to f.docx
ambersalomon88660
 
1. Is it important the hospital to have a licensure to ensure that.docx
1. Is it important the hospital to have a licensure to ensure that.docx1. Is it important the hospital to have a licensure to ensure that.docx
1. Is it important the hospital to have a licensure to ensure that.docx
ambersalomon88660
 
1. INTRODUCTION In recent years, energy harvesting fro.docx
1. INTRODUCTION In recent years, energy harvesting fro.docx1. INTRODUCTION In recent years, energy harvesting fro.docx
1. INTRODUCTION In recent years, energy harvesting fro.docx
ambersalomon88660
 
1. INTRODUCTIONThe rapid of economic growth in China, is a fou.docx
1. INTRODUCTIONThe rapid of economic growth in China, is a fou.docx1. INTRODUCTIONThe rapid of economic growth in China, is a fou.docx
1. INTRODUCTIONThe rapid of economic growth in China, is a fou.docx
ambersalomon88660
 
1. Introduction to the Topica. What is outsourcingi. Ty.docx
1. Introduction to the Topica. What is outsourcingi. Ty.docx1. Introduction to the Topica. What is outsourcingi. Ty.docx
1. Introduction to the Topica. What is outsourcingi. Ty.docx
ambersalomon88660
 
1. Introduction 1. Technology and communication 1. Technology .docx
1. Introduction 1. Technology and communication 1. Technology .docx1. Introduction 1. Technology and communication 1. Technology .docx
1. Introduction 1. Technology and communication 1. Technology .docx
ambersalomon88660
 
1. In your definition of a well-run company, how important a.docx
1. In your definition of a well-run company, how important a.docx1. In your definition of a well-run company, how important a.docx
1. In your definition of a well-run company, how important a.docx
ambersalomon88660
 
1. In Chapter four titled Academy Training you learned about academi.docx
1. In Chapter four titled Academy Training you learned about academi.docx1. In Chapter four titled Academy Training you learned about academi.docx
1. In Chapter four titled Academy Training you learned about academi.docx
ambersalomon88660
 
1. In 200 words, describe how Hamlet promotes andor subverts th.docx
1. In 200 words, describe how Hamlet promotes andor subverts th.docx1. In 200 words, describe how Hamlet promotes andor subverts th.docx
1. In 200 words, describe how Hamlet promotes andor subverts th.docx
ambersalomon88660
 
1. Image 1 courtesy of httpswww.virginiahospitalcenter.com.docx
1. Image 1 courtesy of httpswww.virginiahospitalcenter.com.docx1. Image 1 courtesy of httpswww.virginiahospitalcenter.com.docx
1. Image 1 courtesy of httpswww.virginiahospitalcenter.com.docx
ambersalomon88660
 
1. If I were to create an SEL program, I would focus on self-awar.docx
1. If I were to create an SEL program, I would focus on self-awar.docx1. If I were to create an SEL program, I would focus on self-awar.docx
1. If I were to create an SEL program, I would focus on self-awar.docx
ambersalomon88660
 
1. Identify and discuss the factors that contribute to heritage cons.docx
1. Identify and discuss the factors that contribute to heritage cons.docx1. Identify and discuss the factors that contribute to heritage cons.docx
1. Identify and discuss the factors that contribute to heritage cons.docx
ambersalomon88660
 
1. I think that the top three management positions in a health pla.docx
1. I think that the top three management positions in a health pla.docx1. I think that the top three management positions in a health pla.docx
1. I think that the top three management positions in a health pla.docx
ambersalomon88660
 

More from ambersalomon88660 (20)

1. Lists crimes and crime involvement on the Mendez brothers.2.I.docx
1. Lists crimes and crime involvement on the Mendez brothers.2.I.docx1. Lists crimes and crime involvement on the Mendez brothers.2.I.docx
1. Lists crimes and crime involvement on the Mendez brothers.2.I.docx
 
1. Lists and analyzes strengths and weaknesses based on each of th.docx
1. Lists and analyzes strengths and weaknesses based on each of th.docx1. Lists and analyzes strengths and weaknesses based on each of th.docx
1. Lists and analyzes strengths and weaknesses based on each of th.docx
 
1. List eight basic initiatives that companies can use to gain c.docx
1. List eight basic initiatives that companies can use to gain c.docx1. List eight basic initiatives that companies can use to gain c.docx
1. List eight basic initiatives that companies can use to gain c.docx
 
1. Koffman Corporation is trying to raise capital. What method wou.docx
1. Koffman Corporation is trying to raise capital. What method wou.docx1. Koffman Corporation is trying to raise capital. What method wou.docx
1. Koffman Corporation is trying to raise capital. What method wou.docx
 
1. List all the entities that interact with the TIMS system. Start b.docx
1. List all the entities that interact with the TIMS system. Start b.docx1. List all the entities that interact with the TIMS system. Start b.docx
1. List all the entities that interact with the TIMS system. Start b.docx
 
1. Know the terminology flash cards.2. Know the hist.docx
1. Know the terminology flash cards.2. Know the hist.docx1. Know the terminology flash cards.2. Know the hist.docx
1. Know the terminology flash cards.2. Know the hist.docx
 
1. Journal Entry The attached (BUROS Center for Testing).docx
1. Journal Entry  The attached (BUROS Center for Testing).docx1. Journal Entry  The attached (BUROS Center for Testing).docx
1. Journal Entry The attached (BUROS Center for Testing).docx
 
1. Introduction and thesisThrough extensive research I hope to f.docx
1. Introduction and thesisThrough extensive research I hope to f.docx1. Introduction and thesisThrough extensive research I hope to f.docx
1. Introduction and thesisThrough extensive research I hope to f.docx
 
1. Is it important the hospital to have a licensure to ensure that.docx
1. Is it important the hospital to have a licensure to ensure that.docx1. Is it important the hospital to have a licensure to ensure that.docx
1. Is it important the hospital to have a licensure to ensure that.docx
 
1. INTRODUCTION In recent years, energy harvesting fro.docx
1. INTRODUCTION In recent years, energy harvesting fro.docx1. INTRODUCTION In recent years, energy harvesting fro.docx
1. INTRODUCTION In recent years, energy harvesting fro.docx
 
1. INTRODUCTIONThe rapid of economic growth in China, is a fou.docx
1. INTRODUCTIONThe rapid of economic growth in China, is a fou.docx1. INTRODUCTIONThe rapid of economic growth in China, is a fou.docx
1. INTRODUCTIONThe rapid of economic growth in China, is a fou.docx
 
1. Introduction to the Topica. What is outsourcingi. Ty.docx
1. Introduction to the Topica. What is outsourcingi. Ty.docx1. Introduction to the Topica. What is outsourcingi. Ty.docx
1. Introduction to the Topica. What is outsourcingi. Ty.docx
 
1. Introduction 1. Technology and communication 1. Technology .docx
1. Introduction 1. Technology and communication 1. Technology .docx1. Introduction 1. Technology and communication 1. Technology .docx
1. Introduction 1. Technology and communication 1. Technology .docx
 
1. In your definition of a well-run company, how important a.docx
1. In your definition of a well-run company, how important a.docx1. In your definition of a well-run company, how important a.docx
1. In your definition of a well-run company, how important a.docx
 
1. In Chapter four titled Academy Training you learned about academi.docx
1. In Chapter four titled Academy Training you learned about academi.docx1. In Chapter four titled Academy Training you learned about academi.docx
1. In Chapter four titled Academy Training you learned about academi.docx
 
1. In 200 words, describe how Hamlet promotes andor subverts th.docx
1. In 200 words, describe how Hamlet promotes andor subverts th.docx1. In 200 words, describe how Hamlet promotes andor subverts th.docx
1. In 200 words, describe how Hamlet promotes andor subverts th.docx
 
1. Image 1 courtesy of httpswww.virginiahospitalcenter.com.docx
1. Image 1 courtesy of httpswww.virginiahospitalcenter.com.docx1. Image 1 courtesy of httpswww.virginiahospitalcenter.com.docx
1. Image 1 courtesy of httpswww.virginiahospitalcenter.com.docx
 
1. If I were to create an SEL program, I would focus on self-awar.docx
1. If I were to create an SEL program, I would focus on self-awar.docx1. If I were to create an SEL program, I would focus on self-awar.docx
1. If I were to create an SEL program, I would focus on self-awar.docx
 
1. Identify and discuss the factors that contribute to heritage cons.docx
1. Identify and discuss the factors that contribute to heritage cons.docx1. Identify and discuss the factors that contribute to heritage cons.docx
1. Identify and discuss the factors that contribute to heritage cons.docx
 
1. I think that the top three management positions in a health pla.docx
1. I think that the top three management positions in a health pla.docx1. I think that the top three management positions in a health pla.docx
1. I think that the top three management positions in a health pla.docx
 

Recently uploaded

PART A. Introduction to Costumer Service
PART A. Introduction to Costumer ServicePART A. Introduction to Costumer Service
PART A. Introduction to Costumer Service
PedroFerreira53928
 
How to Create Map Views in the Odoo 17 ERP
How to Create Map Views in the Odoo 17 ERPHow to Create Map Views in the Odoo 17 ERP
How to Create Map Views in the Odoo 17 ERP
Celine George
 
GIÁO ÁN DẠY THÊM (KẾ HOẠCH BÀI BUỔI 2) - TIẾNG ANH 8 GLOBAL SUCCESS (2 CỘT) N...
GIÁO ÁN DẠY THÊM (KẾ HOẠCH BÀI BUỔI 2) - TIẾNG ANH 8 GLOBAL SUCCESS (2 CỘT) N...GIÁO ÁN DẠY THÊM (KẾ HOẠCH BÀI BUỔI 2) - TIẾNG ANH 8 GLOBAL SUCCESS (2 CỘT) N...
GIÁO ÁN DẠY THÊM (KẾ HOẠCH BÀI BUỔI 2) - TIẾNG ANH 8 GLOBAL SUCCESS (2 CỘT) N...
Nguyen Thanh Tu Collection
 
How libraries can support authors with open access requirements for UKRI fund...
How libraries can support authors with open access requirements for UKRI fund...How libraries can support authors with open access requirements for UKRI fund...
How libraries can support authors with open access requirements for UKRI fund...
Jisc
 
Supporting (UKRI) OA monographs at Salford.pptx
Supporting (UKRI) OA monographs at Salford.pptxSupporting (UKRI) OA monographs at Salford.pptx
Supporting (UKRI) OA monographs at Salford.pptx
Jisc
 
Model Attribute Check Company Auto Property
Model Attribute  Check Company Auto PropertyModel Attribute  Check Company Auto Property
Model Attribute Check Company Auto Property
Celine George
 
2024.06.01 Introducing a competency framework for languag learning materials ...
2024.06.01 Introducing a competency framework for languag learning materials ...2024.06.01 Introducing a competency framework for languag learning materials ...
2024.06.01 Introducing a competency framework for languag learning materials ...
Sandy Millin
 
How to Make a Field invisible in Odoo 17
How to Make a Field invisible in Odoo 17How to Make a Field invisible in Odoo 17
How to Make a Field invisible in Odoo 17
Celine George
 
Mule 4.6 & Java 17 Upgrade | MuleSoft Mysore Meetup #46
Mule 4.6 & Java 17 Upgrade | MuleSoft Mysore Meetup #46Mule 4.6 & Java 17 Upgrade | MuleSoft Mysore Meetup #46
Mule 4.6 & Java 17 Upgrade | MuleSoft Mysore Meetup #46
MysoreMuleSoftMeetup
 
Palestine last event orientationfvgnh .pptx
Palestine last event orientationfvgnh .pptxPalestine last event orientationfvgnh .pptx
Palestine last event orientationfvgnh .pptx
RaedMohamed3
 
Phrasal Verbs.XXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXX
Phrasal Verbs.XXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXPhrasal Verbs.XXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXX
Phrasal Verbs.XXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXX
MIRIAMSALINAS13
 
Language Across the Curriculm LAC B.Ed.
Language Across the  Curriculm LAC B.Ed.Language Across the  Curriculm LAC B.Ed.
Language Across the Curriculm LAC B.Ed.
Atul Kumar Singh
 
ESC Beyond Borders _From EU to You_ InfoPack general.pdf
ESC Beyond Borders _From EU to You_ InfoPack general.pdfESC Beyond Borders _From EU to You_ InfoPack general.pdf
ESC Beyond Borders _From EU to You_ InfoPack general.pdf
Fundacja Rozwoju Społeczeństwa Przedsiębiorczego
 
TESDA TM1 REVIEWER FOR NATIONAL ASSESSMENT WRITTEN AND ORAL QUESTIONS WITH A...
TESDA TM1 REVIEWER  FOR NATIONAL ASSESSMENT WRITTEN AND ORAL QUESTIONS WITH A...TESDA TM1 REVIEWER  FOR NATIONAL ASSESSMENT WRITTEN AND ORAL QUESTIONS WITH A...
TESDA TM1 REVIEWER FOR NATIONAL ASSESSMENT WRITTEN AND ORAL QUESTIONS WITH A...
EugeneSaldivar
 
aaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaa
aaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaa
aaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaa
siemaillard
 
aaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaa
aaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaa
aaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaa
siemaillard
 
Unit 2- Research Aptitude (UGC NET Paper I).pdf
Unit 2- Research Aptitude (UGC NET Paper I).pdfUnit 2- Research Aptitude (UGC NET Paper I).pdf
Unit 2- Research Aptitude (UGC NET Paper I).pdf
Thiyagu K
 
Basic phrases for greeting and assisting costumers
Basic phrases for greeting and assisting costumersBasic phrases for greeting and assisting costumers
Basic phrases for greeting and assisting costumers
PedroFerreira53928
 
The French Revolution Class 9 Study Material pdf free download
The French Revolution Class 9 Study Material pdf free downloadThe French Revolution Class 9 Study Material pdf free download
The French Revolution Class 9 Study Material pdf free download
Vivekanand Anglo Vedic Academy
 
Unit 8 - Information and Communication Technology (Paper I).pdf
Unit 8 - Information and Communication Technology (Paper I).pdfUnit 8 - Information and Communication Technology (Paper I).pdf
Unit 8 - Information and Communication Technology (Paper I).pdf
Thiyagu K
 

Recently uploaded (20)

PART A. Introduction to Costumer Service
PART A. Introduction to Costumer ServicePART A. Introduction to Costumer Service
PART A. Introduction to Costumer Service
 
How to Create Map Views in the Odoo 17 ERP
How to Create Map Views in the Odoo 17 ERPHow to Create Map Views in the Odoo 17 ERP
How to Create Map Views in the Odoo 17 ERP
 
GIÁO ÁN DẠY THÊM (KẾ HOẠCH BÀI BUỔI 2) - TIẾNG ANH 8 GLOBAL SUCCESS (2 CỘT) N...
GIÁO ÁN DẠY THÊM (KẾ HOẠCH BÀI BUỔI 2) - TIẾNG ANH 8 GLOBAL SUCCESS (2 CỘT) N...GIÁO ÁN DẠY THÊM (KẾ HOẠCH BÀI BUỔI 2) - TIẾNG ANH 8 GLOBAL SUCCESS (2 CỘT) N...
GIÁO ÁN DẠY THÊM (KẾ HOẠCH BÀI BUỔI 2) - TIẾNG ANH 8 GLOBAL SUCCESS (2 CỘT) N...
 
How libraries can support authors with open access requirements for UKRI fund...
How libraries can support authors with open access requirements for UKRI fund...How libraries can support authors with open access requirements for UKRI fund...
How libraries can support authors with open access requirements for UKRI fund...
 
Supporting (UKRI) OA monographs at Salford.pptx
Supporting (UKRI) OA monographs at Salford.pptxSupporting (UKRI) OA monographs at Salford.pptx
Supporting (UKRI) OA monographs at Salford.pptx
 
Model Attribute Check Company Auto Property
Model Attribute  Check Company Auto PropertyModel Attribute  Check Company Auto Property
Model Attribute Check Company Auto Property
 
2024.06.01 Introducing a competency framework for languag learning materials ...
2024.06.01 Introducing a competency framework for languag learning materials ...2024.06.01 Introducing a competency framework for languag learning materials ...
2024.06.01 Introducing a competency framework for languag learning materials ...
 
How to Make a Field invisible in Odoo 17
How to Make a Field invisible in Odoo 17How to Make a Field invisible in Odoo 17
How to Make a Field invisible in Odoo 17
 
Mule 4.6 & Java 17 Upgrade | MuleSoft Mysore Meetup #46
Mule 4.6 & Java 17 Upgrade | MuleSoft Mysore Meetup #46Mule 4.6 & Java 17 Upgrade | MuleSoft Mysore Meetup #46
Mule 4.6 & Java 17 Upgrade | MuleSoft Mysore Meetup #46
 
Palestine last event orientationfvgnh .pptx
Palestine last event orientationfvgnh .pptxPalestine last event orientationfvgnh .pptx
Palestine last event orientationfvgnh .pptx
 
Phrasal Verbs.XXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXX
Phrasal Verbs.XXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXPhrasal Verbs.XXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXX
Phrasal Verbs.XXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXX
 
Language Across the Curriculm LAC B.Ed.
Language Across the  Curriculm LAC B.Ed.Language Across the  Curriculm LAC B.Ed.
Language Across the Curriculm LAC B.Ed.
 
ESC Beyond Borders _From EU to You_ InfoPack general.pdf
ESC Beyond Borders _From EU to You_ InfoPack general.pdfESC Beyond Borders _From EU to You_ InfoPack general.pdf
ESC Beyond Borders _From EU to You_ InfoPack general.pdf
 
TESDA TM1 REVIEWER FOR NATIONAL ASSESSMENT WRITTEN AND ORAL QUESTIONS WITH A...
TESDA TM1 REVIEWER  FOR NATIONAL ASSESSMENT WRITTEN AND ORAL QUESTIONS WITH A...TESDA TM1 REVIEWER  FOR NATIONAL ASSESSMENT WRITTEN AND ORAL QUESTIONS WITH A...
TESDA TM1 REVIEWER FOR NATIONAL ASSESSMENT WRITTEN AND ORAL QUESTIONS WITH A...
 
aaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaa
aaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaa
aaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaa
 
aaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaa
aaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaa
aaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaa
 
Unit 2- Research Aptitude (UGC NET Paper I).pdf
Unit 2- Research Aptitude (UGC NET Paper I).pdfUnit 2- Research Aptitude (UGC NET Paper I).pdf
Unit 2- Research Aptitude (UGC NET Paper I).pdf
 
Basic phrases for greeting and assisting costumers
Basic phrases for greeting and assisting costumersBasic phrases for greeting and assisting costumers
Basic phrases for greeting and assisting costumers
 
The French Revolution Class 9 Study Material pdf free download
The French Revolution Class 9 Study Material pdf free downloadThe French Revolution Class 9 Study Material pdf free download
The French Revolution Class 9 Study Material pdf free download
 
Unit 8 - Information and Communication Technology (Paper I).pdf
Unit 8 - Information and Communication Technology (Paper I).pdfUnit 8 - Information and Communication Technology (Paper I).pdf
Unit 8 - Information and Communication Technology (Paper I).pdf
 

Writing Assignment Answer SheetWhen answering these questions,.docx

  • 1. Writing Assignment Answer Sheet When answering these questions, please use your own words. Do not copy and paste and do not cite verbatim from the article. Part 1: Article summary · Make sure to answer only in the space provided. If you go beyond it only part of your text will be visible and this will be the only part we will grade. DO NOT change he font size to fit more into the allotted space. DO NOT change the size of the text box to increase the allotted space. EACH SUCH ATTEMPT WILL BE CONSIDERED A BREACH OF ETHICAL GUIDELINES AND WILL RESULT IN A GRADE OF 0 (ZERO) IN THE ASSIGNMENT and additional steps as I see fit. · If the answer is similar across studies, just say so rather than elaborate (for example, X was measured like in Study 1; the source of data was the one in Study 1…) 1. (a) What are the major factual reasons and theoretical arguments for this research? (b) What gap in the literature/knowledge do these researchers attempt to fill? (c) What are the three overall research questions the researchers try to answer? 2. Define the two types of bias discussed and how can they contribute to health disparities?
  • 2. 3. What are the specific research questions the researchers examined in each study? Study 2: Study 1: 4. (a) What are the data sources in each study? (b) How many participants were included in the final analyses? (you do not need to discuss the various ways to calculate and weight the various types of data). Study 1: Study 2: 5. What were the major variables examined in each study? How were they measured? (you do not need to discuss the Correlates). Study 1 (4 major variables): Study 2 (4 major variables): 6. What were the major results in each study? (you do not need to discuss the analyses. Refer only to the major variables and research questions as indicated below).
  • 3. Study 1: a. Access to healthcare as related to race AND the different types of bias: b. Circulatory disease diagnosis as related to race AND the different types of bias: Study 2: a. Death rate due to circulatory diseases as related to race AND the different types of bias: 7. What was the research design used in these studies (descriptive, correlational, or experimental?) explain your answer: (a) why do you think this is the particular design? (b) why didn’t you chose the other two designs? (c) can you make causal inferences based on these studies? Why? (make sure to refer to Chapter 2 when answering this question). In the next page, you will answer Part 2: Critical evaluation and conclusions. Remember it should not be longer than a page. Use 12-point Times New Roman font and 1” margins on all sides. When answering these questions, use the materials you have learned in the course that might be relevant to this type of research topic. See more details about this answer in the separate instructions sheet. Part 2 – A critical review of the paper Psychological Science
  • 4. 2016, Vol. 27(10) 1299 –1311 © The Author(s) 2016 Reprints and permissions: sagepub.com/journalsPermissions.nav DOI: 10.1177/0956797616658450 pss.sagepub.com Research Article Blacks die at a higher rate than Whites from circulatory- related diseases (e.g., heart disease; U.S. Department of Health and Human Services, 2014). Prominent theories have suggested that one cause of this disparity is that Blacks experience more discrimination, which leads to stress, which in turn has negative health consequences (Clark, Anderson, Clark, & Williams, 1999; Hatzenbuehler, Phelan, & Link, 2013; Major, Mendes, & Dovidio, 2013). Studies supporting this view have found that the percep- tion of discrimination is associated with anxiety, cardio- vascular threat response, hypertension, and mortality (Barnes et al., 2008; Mendoza-Denton, Downey, Purdie, Davis, & Pietrzak, 2002; Pascoe & Richman, 2009; Sawyer, Major, Casad, Townsend, & Mendes, 2012; Williams & Mohammed, 2009). Although this previous work suggests that perceived racial bias contributes to disparities between Blacks’ and Whites’ health, less is known about whether the actual racial bias of Whites contributes to these disparities. A deeper understanding of links between Whites’ racial bias and racial health disparities could help identify com- munities in greatest need of prejudice-prevention and health-promotion interventions. Therefore, the aim of the current research was to establish whether Black-White disparities in risk of circulatory disease and in circula-
  • 5. tory-disease-related death rate are greater in communi- ties where Whites harbor more racial bias. 658450PSSXXX10.1177/0956797616658450Leitner et al.Blacks’ Death Rate and Whites’ Bias research-article2016 Corresponding Author: Jordan B. Leitner, University of California, Berkeley– Psychology, 3210 Tolman Hall, Berkeley, CA 94720 E-mail: [email protected] Blacks’ Death Rate Due to Circulatory Diseases Is Positively Related to Whites’ Explicit Racial Bias: A Nationwide Investigation Using Project Implicit Jordan B. Leitner1, Eric Hehman2, Ozlem Ayduk1, and Rodolfo Mendoza-Denton1 1Department of Psychology, University of California, Berkeley, and 2Department of Psychology, Ryerson University Abstract Perceptions of racial bias have been linked to poorer circulatory health among Blacks compared with Whites. However, little is known about whether Whites’ actual racial bias contributes to this racial disparity in health. We compiled racial- bias data from 1,391,632 Whites and examined whether racial bias in a given county predicted Black-White disparities in circulatory-disease risk (access to health care, diagnosis of a circulatory disease; Study 1) and circulatory-disease- related death rate (Study 2) in the same county. Results revealed that in counties where Whites reported greater racial bias, Blacks (but not Whites) reported decreased access to
  • 6. health care (Study 1). Furthermore, in counties where Whites reported greater racial bias, both Blacks and Whites showed increased death rates due to circulatory diseases, but this relationship was stronger for Blacks than for Whites (Study 2). These results indicate that racial disparities in risk of circulatory disease and in circulatory-disease-related death rate are more pronounced in communities where Whites harbor more explicit racial bias. Keywords racial and ethnic attitudes and relations, prejudice, health, sociocultural factors, open data, open materials Received 11/30/15; Revision accepted 6/16/16 1300 Leitner et al. Whites’ Racial Biases and Health Disparities Previous research has contrasted two forms of racial bias: explicit and implicit bias. Explicit bias refers to deliberate, consciously controlled biases, whereas implicit bias refers to more automatic biases that are difficult to control (Greenwald, Poehlman, Uhlmann, & Banaji, 2009). Explicit and implicit racial biases are posi- tively correlated, though research suggests that they are relatively independent constructs (Hofmann, Gawronski, Gschwendner, Le, & Schmitt, 2005). The distinction between explicit and implicit biases is supported by research showing that explicit bias predicts intentional behaviors, whereas implicit bias predicts relatively unin- tentional behaviors (Dovidio, Kawakami, & Gaertner, 2002).
  • 7. Both explicit and implicit racial biases might contribute to racial health disparities. For instance, Whites’ explicit or implicit racial bias may hinder Blacks’ access to health care and thus decrease the likelihood that Blacks will receive diagnosis of and treatment for health problems. Additionally, Whites’ explicit or implicit bias may contrib- ute to hostile community environments that evoke psy- chological stress in Blacks, and stress has been linked to circulatory disease (Black & Garbutt, 2002). Studies con- sistent with these possibilities have demonstrated that Blacks show increased death rates in regions where pop- ulation-level measures (i.e., survey responses from multi- racial samples) reveal more explicit anti-Black attitudes (Kennedy, Kawachi, Lochner, Jones, & Prothrow-Stith, 1997; Lee, Kawachi, Muennig, & Hatzenbuehler, 2015). Similarly, Blacks die at a higher rate in regions where more racist Internet searches are conducted (Chae et al., 2015), and sexual minorities have shorter life expectan- cies in regions where population-level measures reveal more antigay attitudes (Hatzenbuehler, Bellatorre, et al., 2014). Though these studies provide evidence for the perva- sive effects of explicit bias, several important questions remain. First, is there a relationship between the domi- nant group’s (e.g., Whites’) negative attitudes toward a targeted group (e.g., Blacks) and the targeted group’s health? Addressing this question would be important, as previous research has not disaggregated the bias of tar- geted and nontargeted groups. For instance, Lee et al. (2015) examined the effects of population-level anti- Black attitudes that were aggregated across all respon- dents (including Blacks). Similarly, Hatzenbuehler, Bellatorre, et al. (2014) examined the effects of popula- tion-level antigay attitudes that were aggregated across
  • 8. straight and gay respondents. Accordingly, these studies leave open the possibility that effects of population-level bias on the target group’s health are driven by that group’s bias against itself (e.g., Blacks’ anti-Black attitudes), as opposed to nontargeted groups’ bias against the target group (e.g., Whites’ anti-Black attitudes). Second, does explicit or implicit bias play a stronger role in predicting Black-White health disparities? Given that explicit and implicit biases are positively correlated (Hofmann et al., 2005), it would be important to deter- mine whether they independently predict health dispari- ties. Indeed, understanding the independent relationships between the various forms of bias and health could pro- vide insight into whether explicit or implicit bias should be the target of interventions aimed at reducing health disparities. Finally, would a relationship between racial bias and health emerge across a large number of small geographic areas? Previous research has examined relationships between bias and health outcomes in a relatively limited number of large geographic areas (Chae et al., 2015: n = 196; Hatzenbuehler, Bellatorre, et al., 2014: n = 170; Kennedy et al., 1997: n = 39; Lee et al., 2015: n = 100). Given that bias might vary within geographic units (e.g., county-to-county variation within a state), examining the effects of bias across smaller geographic areas may pro- vide a more sensitive analysis. To address these issues, we examined whether Black- White disparities in risk of circulatory disease (Study 1) and in rate of death due to circulatory disease (Study 2) were more pronounced in counties where Whites har- bored more racial bias. We pitted explicit and implicit
  • 9. biases against one another to determine the stronger pre- dictor of health outcomes. Study 1 Data sources Racial bias. To assess explicit and implicit racial bias, we compiled data from Project Implicit (Xu, Nosek, & Greenwald, 2014), an initiative that has measured racial bias from millions of respondents over the Internet since 2002. Project Implicit has generated the largest known repository of data on explicit and implicit bias. Though respondents are self-selected, a major strength of the data set is that their general geographic locations can be ascertained from their Internet protocol (IP) addresses. At the time when we obtained the Project Implicit data set, it included responses made from 2002 through 2013, though only five responses (0.0001%) were from 2002. Because we examined changes in bias over time and sought reliable estimates of bias in each year, we omitted 2002 responses. Thus, our analyses included responses in the time window from 2003 through 2013. Respondents completed measures of both explicit and implicit racial bias. For the measure of explicit racial bias, community Blacks’ Death Rate and Whites’ Bias 1301 members rated how warm they felt toward European Americans and toward African Americans, on scales from 0 (coldest feelings) to 10 (warmest feelings). We com- puted the difference between these responses (warmth toward European Americans minus warmth toward
  • 10. African Americans) and operationalized pro-White expli- cit bias as more reported warmth toward European Americans compared with African Americans. The measure of implicit racial bias was an Implicit Association Test (IAT), a speeded dual-categorization task in which respondents categorized social targets (e.g., Black and White faces) and verbal targets (e.g., words referring to “good” and “bad” things) by key press. Faster responses when White faces and “good” things (and Black faces and “bad” things) required the same key press, as compared with the inverse pairing, reflect pro- White implicit attitudes (Greenwald et al., 2009). The IAT was scored according to the D measure (Greenwald, Nosek, & Banaji, 2003). Explicit and implicit bias at the county level were positively related, r = .25, p < .0001. Within the Project Implicit data set, we identified White respondents for whom the county where they completed the measures was known: 1,391,632 responses from 1,836 counties met these inclusion criteria (M = 759.57 responses per county, SD = 1,766.10). Counties were defined according to the February 2013 Federal Information Processing Standard (FIPS) county codes of the U.S. Census Bureau’s (2013) Metropolitan and Micro- politan Delineation Files. A limitation of the Project Implicit data set is that it was obtained with convenience sampling, and thus the sample for a given county may not be representative of all individuals in that county. One dimension on which the Project Implicit sample was likely nonrepresentative was age: Whereas the median age for all Project Implicit respondents was 23, the national median age was 37, according to the Median Age by Sex report from the U.S. Census Bureau’s 2009–2013 American Community Survey
  • 11. (ACS).1 Estimating the racial bias of older nonrespon- dents would be important, given that racial bias is signifi- cantly greater among older individuals (Gonsalkorale, Sherman, & Klauer, 2009). Accordingly, we used post- stratification weights that assigned greater weight to respondents the more representative they were of the age distribution of their community’s population (cf. Lohr, 2009). We employed the following poststratification weighting scheme. First, separately for explicit and implicit bias, we computed average racial-bias scores for five age groups in each county: 15–24, 25–34, 35–54, 55– 75, and 75+. Second, we determined the population count of Whites in each of these age groups in each county using the Sex by Age (White Only) reports from the U.S. Census Bureau’s 2005–2009 and 2009–2013 ACSs. Third, for each county, we computed estimates for explicit and implicit racial bias that were weighted by the White population count in each age group in that county. The result of this weighting scheme was that respondents were assigned a greater weight when they belonged to an age group that was more populous in their county. We report findings obtained using this weighting scheme, but our conclusions were identical when we used unweighted county averages (see the Supplemental Material available online). Although the racial-bias measure was limited to an 11-year window, we believe that it served as a reasonable proxy for prejudice before and after this time period. Supporting this notion, fixed-effects models revealed that year (treated as a factor) accounted for only 1% of the variance in explicit bias and 2% of the variance in implicit bias. In contrast, county (treated as a factor) accounted for 42% of the variance in explicit bias and 27% of the variance in implicit bias. Thus, year-to-year change in
  • 12. bias was minimal, whereas counties differed consider- ably in their levels of bias. Moreover, previous work has demonstrated that racial bias is highly stable over time, even following a historic event (i.e., Barack Obama’s rise to the presidency; Schmidt & Nosek, 2010). Accordingly, we included racial-bias estimates from 2013, even though data from the previous year were used to calculate our outcome measures of risk of circulatory disease (see the next section). The advantage of includ- ing racial-bias responses from 2013 was that it maximized the sample (i.e., 8% of all responses in the data set were from 2013), thereby increasing the reliability of the racial- bias estimates. Nevertheless, when we used racial-bias data from 2003 through 2012 only, the pattern of findings was extremely similar to what we report here (see the Supplemental Material). Risk of circulatory disease. To estimate risk of circu- latory disease, we compiled data from the 2012 Selected Metropolitan/Micropolitan Area Risk Trends (SMART) of the Behavioral Risk Factor Surveillance System survey, a telephone survey in which participants were asked about their health risk factors (Centers for Disease Control and Prevention, CDC, 2013). We targeted two factors related to circulatory-disease risk. First, we examined participants’ response to the ques- tion, “Was there a time in the past 12 months when you needed to see a doctor but could not because of cost?” We coded “yes” responses as 0 and “no” responses as 1. Thus, when aggregated at the county level, responses to this question provided an estimate of the percentage of county residents who had access to affordable health care. Second, to determine the percentage of respondents
  • 13. who had been diagnosed with circulatory diseases, we examined responses to two questions: “Has a doctor, nurse, or other health professional ever told you that you 1302 Leitner et al. had a heart attack, also called a myocardial infarction?” and “Has a doctor, nurse, or other health professional ever told you that you had angina or coronary heart dis- ease?” We coded “yes” responses as 1 and “no” responses as 0. Responses to these items were strongly related for both Blacks, r = .66, p < .0001, and Whites, r = .77, p < .0001. For parsimony, we averaged responses to these two items. When aggregated at the county level, these averages provided an estimate of the percentage of county residents who had received these diagnoses. In total, 23,522 Blacks from 208 counties and 175,637 Whites from 210 counties completed the health survey (White responses per county: M = 836.37, SD = 591.07; Black responses per county: M = 113.09, SD = 193.83). We aggregated responses by race and county to arrive at separate point estimates for the percentages of Whites and Blacks with access to health care and with circula- tory-disease diagnoses. Figure 1a shows the location of the 205 counties for which we had estimates of Whites’ racial bias and SMART-survey responses from both Blacks and Whites. Race. Participants reported their race on the SMART sur- vey. Race was coded as 0 for Black and 1 for White. Covariates Sex and age. Participants reported their sex and age
  • 14. (in years) on the SMART survey. Sex was coded as −1 for male and 1 for female. Population. Population estimates by race and county were derived from the modified counts for 2010 from the following U.S. Census Bureau report: Annual Estimates of the Resident Population by Sex, Race Alone or in Combination, and Hispanic Origin for the United States, States, and Counties: April 1, 2010 to July 1, 2013. From these data, we computed the total population (the sum of Blacks and Whites) and the Black-to-White ratio of the population for each county in 2010. To produce unstan- dardized regression coefficients that were interpretable, we log-transformed the total-population estimates (but not the Black-to-White ratios). Education. County-level education was assessed by averaging the high school graduation rates for Blacks and Whites (data taken from the U.S. Census Bureau’s 2009–2013 ACS: report titled Sex by Educational Attain- ment for the Population 25 Years and Over). Income, unemployment, and poverty. County-level income, unemployment, and poverty were assessed by compiling data for Whites and Blacks from the 2005–2009 and 2009–2013 ACSs (income data were taken from the reports titled Median Household Income in the Past 12 Months (Householder), unemployment data were taken from the reports titled Employment Status, and poverty data were taken from the reports titled Poverty Status in the Past 12 Months by Sex and Age). County-level income was estimated by averaging the median house- hold income for Whites and Blacks. County-level unem- ployment was estimated by averaging the unemployment
  • 15. rates for Whites and Blacks. County-level poverty was estimated by averaging the poverty rates for Whites and Blacks. So that unstandardized coefficients would be interpretable, we divided income values by 10,000. Segregation. County-level racial segregation was indexed via dissimilarity, an estimate of the percentage of non-Hispanic Whites who would have to move to another census tract within the same county in order to achieve racial integration with non-Hispanic Blacks. Dis- similarity indices at the county level were provided by J. Dewitt (personal communication, December 15, 2015) and were based on methods described in Frey and Mey- ers (2005). We averaged dissimilarities from 2000 and 2010, so that each county had one value of dissimilarity. Geographic mobility. A relationship between racial bias and Black-White health disparities could be driven by social selection forces. Specifically, rather than reflect- ing an effect of racial bias, health disparities could be due to more healthy Blacks selecting to live in environ- ments with lower bias or to sick Blacks selecting to live in high-bias environments. Accordingly, we accounted for geographic mobility, as has been done in previous work (Hatzenbuehler, Jun, Corliss, & Austin, 2014). The U.S. Census Bureau’s 2005–2009 and 2009–2013 ACSs provided estimates of the percentage of Blacks in each county who moved into that county during the previous year (a) from a different county in the same state, (b) from a different state, and (c) from abroad (data taken from the reports titled Geographic Mobility by Selected Characteristics in the United States). For each county, we summed these three percentages in each time window and then averaged the two sums to obtain a single index of geographic mobility.
  • 16. Housing density. To capture where each county fell on the continuum from rural to urban, we assessed hous- ing density, the number of housing units per square mile. For each county, we averaged housing-density values across the U.S. Census Bureau’s 2000 and 2010 Popula- tion, Housing Units, Area, and Density reports. Age bias. To examine whether any effects were spe- cific to racial bias, or generalized to bias on nonracial dimensions, we used Project Implicit’s age-bias data from 2003 through 2013. Project Implicit’s indices of age bias Blacks’ Death Rate and Whites’ Bias 1303 paralleled those for race bias. Implicit age bias was indexed with an IAT (respondents categorized young and old faces and “good” and “bad” words), and explicit age bias was indexed with feeling thermometers for young and old people (explicit age bias was computed as warmth toward young people minus warmth toward old people). This data set contained 585,242 geo-coded responses from 1,850 counties (M = 316.35 responses per county, SD = 777.29). We estimated county-level implicit age bias and explicit age bias following the same proce- dures used to compute race bias. Results To determine whether Whites’ explicit or implicit racial bias (as measured by Project Implicit) independently pre- dicted health risk for Blacks and Whites, we employed generalized estimating equations (GEEs). We analyzed the data using GEEs with robust standard errors in light of the minimal distributional assumptions they require
  • 17. and their robustness to misspecification in large samples (Hubbard et al., 2010). Our conclusions do not depend on the decision to use GEEs; effects for all analyses were Fig. 1. Results for racial disparities in (a) access to affordable health care (Study 1) and (b) death rate (per 100,000) due to circulatory diseases (Study 2). In (a), darker colors indicate counties where Whites reported greater access to health care than Blacks; in (b), darker colors indicate counties where Blacks died from circulatory diseases at a higher rate than Whites did. For interactive versions of these maps, visit www.jordanbleitner.com/maps. 1304 Leitner et al. similar when estimated with mixed linear models. All predictors were mean-centered unless otherwise noted. The number of responses on the health survey varied across counties, and we conjectured that our county-level health estimates were more accurate in counties where more participants completed the survey. Therefore, we weighted counties by the number of responses on the health survey. To isolate the effects of race and bias, we controlled for age, sex, and all the county-level covariates described in the Method section (total population, Black- to-White ratio of the population, education, income, unemployment, poverty, segregation, geographic mobil- ity, housing density explicit age bias, and implicit age bias). Access to health care. Figure 2a shows the relationship between Whites’ explicit racial bias and Whites’ and
  • 18. Blacks’ access to affordable health care before account- ing for covariates. To test whether Black-White disparities in access to health care were more pronounced in coun- ties where White respondents showed more racial bias, even after controlling for county-level covariates, we 0 5 10 15 20 25 30 35 0.0 0.5 1.0 1.5 2.0 R es po nd en ts W ith
  • 20. 300 350 400 450 500 550 600 0.0 0.5 1.0 1.5 2.0 C irc ul at or y- D is ea se D ea th R
  • 21. at e Whites’ Explicit Racial Bias a b c 50 60 70 80 90 100 0.0 0.5 1.0 1.5 2.0 R es po nd en ts W ith
  • 22. A cc es s to H ea lth C ar e (% ) Whites’ Explicit Racial Bias Whites Blacks Fig. 2. Bubble plots showing the county-level relationships between average explicit racial bias of White respon- dents and (a) the percentage of respondents with access to affordable health care (Study 1), (b) the percentage of respondents with diagnoses of circulatory disease (Study 1), and (c) the age-adjusted rate of death due to circulatory diseases (deaths per 100,000; Study 2). The size of each plotted circle is proportionate to the number of respondents in that county. The lines are the best-fitting regression lines before covariates were entered in the model. For visualization purposes only, counties with values
  • 23. that deviated from the mean by more than 2.5 SD are not shown in the graphs in (a) and (c) (2% of counties, but see the Supplemental Material for plots that include these counties), and the graph in (c) does not show counties that had fewer than 50 responses on the racial-bias measure (the bubbles would be too small to visualize). Blacks’ Death Rate and Whites’ Bias 1305 regressed the percentage of respondents who had access to affordable health care on race, explicit racial bias, implicit racial bias, the Race × Explicit Racial Bias interac- tion, the Race × Implicit Racial Bias interaction, all covari- ates, and the interactions between the covariates and race (Table 1). The significant effect of explicit racial bias was qualified by the Race × Explicit Racial Bias interac- tion. Simple-slopes analyses indicated that increased explicit racial bias predicted decreased access to health care for Blacks, b = −0.064, SE = 0.025, z = 2.563, p = .0103, but this relationship was not significant for Whites, b = −0.003, SE = 0.011, z = 0.300, p = .7587. With all covariates included in the analysis, in counties with high (1 SD above the mean) explicit racial bias, Blacks were 8% less likely than Whites to report access to affordable health care. In contrast, in counties with low (1 SD below the mean) explicit racial bias, Blacks were 3% less likely than Whites to report access to affordable health care. Notably, these analyses controlled for multiple socio- economic indices, so socioeconomic status did not appear to account for the link between explicit racial bias and the racial disparity in access to health care. In con- trast to explicit racial bias, implicit racial bias was unre-
  • 24. lated to access to health care. Furthermore, given that the effects of age bias were not significant, the results suggest that racial bias specifically predicted Black-White dispari- ties in access to health care. We report simple slopes for the significant interactions with covariates in the Sup- plemental Material. Sex did not further moderate the Race × Explicit Race Bias interaction, p = .4964. Table 1. Results of the Generalized Estimating Equation Analysis Predicting Access to Affordable Health Care in Study 1 Effect b SE z p Race (Black = 0, White = 1) 0.057 0.008 7.479 < .0001 Explicit racial bias –0.064 0.025 2.563 .0103 Implicit racial bias 0.237 0.153 1.546 .1220 Race × Explicit Racial Bias 0.061 0.023 2.668 .0076 Race × Implicit Racial Bias –0.148 0.140 1.058 .2909 Sex (male = −1, female = 1) –0.010 0.003 3.211 .0013 Age 0.006 0.001 5.256 < .0001 Total population –0.012 0.014 0.894 .3711 Black-to-white ratio –0.037 0.036 1.025 .3066 Education –0.164 0.161 1.020 .3078 Income 0.018 0.007 2.458 .0140 Segregation 0.051 0.056 0.906 .3657 Geographic mobility 0.001 0.002 0.436 .6637 Unemployment –0.001 0.002 0.387 .6989 Poverty –0.002 0.002 0.860 .3899 Housing density < 0.001 < 0.001 1.249 .2117 Explicit age bias –0.023 0.030 0.762 .4456 Implicit age bias 0.103 0.112 0.922 .3575 Race × Sex –0.004 0.003 1.245 .2139 Race × Age –0.002 0.001 1.939 .0524 Race × Total Population 0.003 0.014 0.224 .8197 Race × Black-to-White Ratio 0.028 0.033 0.860 .3912
  • 25. Race × Education 0.322 0.158 2.042 .0411 Race × Income < 0.001 < 0.001 < 0.001 .9661 Race × Segregation –0.057 0.051 1.122 .2609 Race × Geographic Mobility –0.001 0.002 0.735 .4624 Race × Unemployment –0.001 0.002 0.520 .6033 Race × Poverty 0.005 0.002 2.648 .0081 Race × Housing Density < 0.001 < 0.001 < 0.001 .9952 Race × Explicit Age Bias 0.047 0.028 1.688 .0916 Race × Implicit Age Bias –0.032 0.110 0.300 .7691 Note: Data from 204 counties were included in the analysis. Pseudo-R2 for the model was .62. 1306 Leitner et al. Circulatory-disease diagnoses. Figure 2b shows the relationship between Whites’ explicit racial bias and Blacks’ and Whites’ rate of self-reported circulatory dis- eases before accounting for covariates. To test whether, after accounting for covariates, Black-White disparities in diagnoses of circulatory diseases were more pronounced in counties where White respondents showed more racial bias, we regressed the percentage of participants in each county who reported receiving such diagnoses on the same predictors as in the model for access to health care (Table 2). The model with all covariates included showed that Blacks, compared with Whites, were more likely to receive diagnoses of circulatory diseases. However, nei- ther explicit nor implicit racial bias was significantly related to the percentage of respondents with diagnoses of circulatory diseases. Moreover, neither the Race × Explicit Racial Bias interaction nor the Race × Implicit
  • 26. Racial Bias interaction was significant. Thus, even though greater explicit racial bias corresponded with increased Black-White disparities in access to affordable health care, greater explicit racial bias was not related to increased Black-White disparities in diagnoses of circulatory diseases. Discussion The Black-White disparity in access to affordable health care was more pronounced in counties where Whites harbored more explicit racial bias. However, explicit racial bias did not predict racial disparities in the percentage of respondents with circulatory-disease diagnoses. One potential explana- tion for this null effect is that Blacks in more biased counties Table 2. Results of the Generalized Estimating Equation Analysis Predicting Circulatory-Disease Diagnoses in Study 1 Effect b SE z p Race (Black = 0, White = 1) –0.008 0.003 2.890 .0038 Explicit racial bias –0.003 0.007 0.436 .6642 Implicit racial bias 0.023 0.038 0.592 .5534 Race × Explicit Racial Bias 0.010 0.007 1.327 .1847 Race × Implicit Racial Bias –0.019 0.040 0.480 .6314 Sex (male = −1, female = 1) –0.006 0.001 4.157 < .0001 Age 0.003 < 0.001 9.501 < .0001 Total population 0.002 0.004 0.346 .7248 Black-to-White ratio 0.004 0.009 0.412 .6772 Education –0.001 0.052 < 0.001 .9899 Income –0.004 0.002 2.345 .0190 Segregation 0.015 0.014 1.072 .2830 Geographic mobility 0.001 < 0.001 1.817 .0693
  • 27. Unemployment < 0.001 0.001 0.346 .7324 Poverty < 0.001 0.001 0.693 .4896 Housing density < 0.001 < 0.001 1.400 .1615 Explicit age bias 0.007 0.007 0.964 .3356 Implicit age bias –0.022 0.036 0.600 .5467 Race × Sex –0.015 0.002 9.221 < .0001 Race × Age < 0.001 < 0.001 0.917 .3600 Race × Total Population –0.008 0.005 1.775 .0761 Race × Black-to-White Ratio 0.004 0.012 0.300 .7609 Race × Education –0.065 0.052 1.249 .2115 Race × Income 0.001 0.002 0.520 .6051 Race × Segregation –0.014 0.014 1.005 .3145 Race × Geographic Mobility –0.001 < 0.001 2.406 .0161 Race × Unemployment < 0.001 0.001 < 0.001 .9708 Race × Poverty –0.001 0.001 1.459 .1444 Race × Housing Density < 0.001 < 0.001 0.300 .7661 Race × Explicit Age Bias –0.008 0.008 1.077 .2810 Race × Implicit Age Bias 0.036 0.041 0.872 .3821 Note: Data from 204 counties were included in the analysis. Pseudo-R2 for the model was .73. Blacks’ Death Rate and Whites’ Bias 1307 were less likely to see a health-care provider even if they were sick. If this were the case, it is possible that Blacks would have a higher death rate due to circulatory diseases in communities where Whites harbored more explicit racial bias. We tested this possibility in Study 2. Study 2 In Study 2, we examined whether the Black-White dis-
  • 28. parity in rate of death due to circulatory diseases was more pronounced in counties where Whites harbored more racial bias. Data sources Racial bias. Explicit and implicit racial biases were estimated with the methods described for Study 1. Death rate. Death records for Blacks and Whites were obtained from the CDC (2014). Specifically, we examined rates (per 100,000) of death from circulatory diseases (e.g., heart disease; Internal Statistical Classification of Diseases and Related Health Problems codes I00–I99) in 2003 through 2013. Circulatory diseases are the leading cause of death in the United States and have shown pervasive racial disparities in prevalence over time (U.S. Department of Health and Human Services, 2014). To account for poten- tial age differences between counties and racial groups and allow for more meaningful comparisons, we used age- adjusted death rates. To derive these age-adjusted rates, we used the default 2000 U.S. standard population detailed in Anderson and Rosenberg (1998). We aggregated the data across males and females, given that sex did not moderate the effects of bias in Study 1, and aggregation minimized missing data (i.e., aggregated data were less likely to be suppressed by the CDC than were separated data for males and females). We were able to obtain death rates for Whites in 3,110 counties and death rates for Blacks in 1,490 counties. Data were unavailable for counties recording fewer than 10 deaths for a given group. Figure 1b shows the location of the 1,149 counties for which we obtained racial-bias data and death-rate data for both Blacks and Whites.
  • 29. To determine whether racial bias predicted Black- White disparities in deaths from causes other than circu- latory diseases, we used the CDC (2014) data set to compile county-level data on age-adjusted rates of death due to neoplasm (e.g., cancer) for 2003 through 2013. We focused on death due to neoplasm because neoplasms are the second most prevalent cause of death in the United States (U.S. Department of Health and Human Services, 2014). To isolate effects of interest, we incorporated the same set of county-level covariates used in Study 1, except for sex (because death rates were aggregated across males and females) and age (because death rates were already age adjusted). Results As in Study 1, we used GEEs to estimate all effects. Data from a given county were weighted in the analyses by the number of respondents who completed the racial- bias measure in that county. As in Study 1, race was coded as 0 for Black and 1 for White, and all other pre- dictors were mean-centered. Figure 2c shows the relationship between Whites’ explicit racial bias and Blacks’ and Whites’ death rates due to circulatory diseases before accounting for covari- ates. To test whether racial disparities in death rate due to circulatory diseases were more pronounced in counties where White respondents showed greater racial bias, even after controlling for a set of county-level covariates, we regressed circulatory-disease-related death rate on race, explicit racial bias, implicit racial bias, the Race × Explicit Racial Bias interaction, the Race × Implicit Racial
  • 30. Bias interaction, all covariates, and the interactions between race and the covariates (Table 3). The effect of explicit racial bias was qualified by the Race × Explicit Racial Bias interaction. Simple-slopes analyses indicated that the relationship between explicit racial bias and rate of death due to circulatory diseases was positive for both Blacks and Whites, but stronger for Blacks, b = 43.200, SE = 12.100, z = 3.559, p = .0004, than for Whites, b = 13.900, SE = 4.970, z = 2.795, p = .0052. As in Study 1, the Race × Explicit Racial Bias interaction was significant over and above the effects of age bias, which suggests that racial bias specifically was related to Black-White disparities in death rate. Neither the main effect for implicit racial bias nor the Race × Implicit Racial Bias interaction was significant. With all covariates included in the model, in counties with high (1 SD above the mean) explicit racial bias, the difference between Blacks’ and Whites’ death rates was 62 deaths per 100,000. In contrast, in counties with low (1 SD below the mean) explicit bias, the difference was 35 deaths per 100,000. Furthermore, adjusting for all covari- ates, we estimated how many more Blacks died of circu- latory-related diseases annually in counties that were high (1 SD above the mean), rather than low (1 SD below the mean), in explicit racial bias. We made this estimate at the average Black population level in counties for which we had death-rate data for Blacks (average Black popula- tion = 28,598); 11 more Blacks per county were predicted to die annually in high-explicit-bias counties (95 deaths) than in low-explicit-bias counties (84 deaths). To determine whether similar effects would emerge for death rate not due to circulatory diseases, we regressed
  • 31. 1308 Leitner et al. rate of death due to neoplasm on the same predictors as in our analyses of deaths due to circulatory diseases. Nei- ther the main effect of explicit racial bias nor the Race × Explicit Racial Bias interaction was significant, ps > .14. Moreover, when we modeled neoplasm death rate as a covariate in the model predicting death rate due to circu- latory diseases, the Race × Explicit Racial Bias interaction remained significant, b = −21.100, SE = 9.240, z = 2.283, p = .0224. These findings suggest that that the relationship between explicit racial bias and death rate was specific to circulatory-related, and not neoplasm-related, disease. (See the Supplemental Material for additional analyses.) Discussion In counties where White respondents harbored more explicit racial bias, the rate of death from circulatory dis- ease was increased for both Whites and Blacks. However, Whites’ explicit racial bias predicted this death rate more strongly for Blacks than for Whites. As in Study 1, explicit racial bias, compared with implicit racial bias, was a stronger predictor of Black-White health disparity. General Discussion In counties where White Project Implicit respondents harbored more explicit racial bias, Black-White dispari- ties in access to affordable health care (Study 1) and rate of death due to circulatory diseases (Study 2) were more pronounced. The robustness of these findings was evi- denced by the replication of the same pattern across two independent data sets that both included a large number
  • 32. of counties. Although the effects could have been driven by an unmeasured third variable, the fact that racial bias was a significant predictor in models with a large set of covariates supports the notion of a direct relationship between Whites’ racial bias and Black-White health disparities. Table 3. Results of the Generalized Estimating Equation Analysis Predicting Death Rate Due to Circulatory Diseases in Study 2 Effect b SE z p Race (Black = 0, White = 1) –48.400 7.000 6.917 < .0001 Explicit racial bias 43.200 12.100 3.559 .0004 Implicit racial bias 64.400 56.100 1.149 .2513 Race × Explicit Racial Bias –29.300 11.700 2.500 .0124 Race × Implicit Racial Bias –3.810 54.800 0.000 .9445 Total population –9.390 10.300 0.917 .3602 Black-to-White Ratio 92.300 25.400 3.632 .0003 Education –176.000 99.700 1.769 .0769 Income –8.200 4.140 1.982 .0475 Segregation 19.000 12.700 1.493 .1350 Geographic mobility –2.500 0.485 5.146 < .0001 Unemployment 2.140 1.180 1.817 .0695 Poverty 1.570 0.935 1.676 .0934 Housing density < 0.001 < 0.001 < 0.001 .9762 Explicit age bias –16.400 8.870 1.849 .0645 Implicit age bias 24.600 47.000 0.520 .6005 Race × Total Population –2.560 8.560 0.300 .7647 Race × Black-to-White Ratio –63.900 32.700 1.954 .0506 Race × Education –17.600 88.700 0.200 .8431 Race × Income 0.313 4.140 0.100 .9397 Race × Segregation –6.610 11.700 0.566 .5730 Race × Geographic Mobility 2.020 0.462 4.374 < .0001 Race × Unemployment –0.852 1.130 0.755 .4518
  • 33. Race × Poverty –2.180 0.868 2.508 .0122 Race × Housing Density < 0.001 0.001 0.583 .5580 Race × Explicit Age Bias –1.170 8.480 0.141 .8903 Race × Implicit Age Bias 34.000 40.000 0.849 .3955 Note: Data from 1,776 counties were included in the analysis. Pseudo-R2 for the model was .42. Blacks’ Death Rate and Whites’ Bias 1309 To our knowledge, this is the first research to show that racial bias from a dominant group (e.g., Whites) pre- dicts negative health outcomes more strongly for the tar- get group (e.g., Blacks) than for the dominant group. These results are consistent with research that has shown that population-level bias (i.e., antigay attitudes), when aggregated across targeted and nontargeted groups, pre- dicts negative health outcomes more strongly for the targeted than the nontargeted group (Hatzenbuehler, Bellatorre, et al., 2014). However, the current results extend this previous work, which did not directly address the issue of whether the effects were driven by the domi- nant group’s stigmatization of the targeted group or the targeted group’s self-stigmatization. Furthermore, by demonstrating a relationship between Whites’ racial biases and health outcomes of Blacks in the same com- munity, these results support previous findings that Blacks’ subjective perceptions of racism are linked to their own health (Pascoe & Richman, 2009; Williams & Mohammed, 2009). However, because perceptions of racism can be shaped by race-based rejection sensitivity (Mendoza-Denton et al., 2002), we extended this previ- ous work by measuring racial bias directly from Whites.
  • 34. Additionally, to our knowledge, this is the first research to use geo-coded data on implicit bias to examine whether racial health disparities are more pronounced in communities where Whites show more implicit bias. Notably, when implicit bias was modeled without explicit bias, it showed patterns similar to those for explicit bias (see Tables S4 and S6 in the Supplemental Material), but when explicit and implicit biases were modeled together, only explicit bias predicted health outcomes. The null effects of implicit bias are informative, as research is increasingly focusing on the predictive utility of implicit bias in medical contexts (e.g., Hall et al., 2015), and it is important to identify the outcomes that are more strongly related to explicit than to implicit bias. One potential explanation for why Whites’ explicit bias was a stronger predictor than their implicit bias in the current work is that explicit bias has historically exerted stronger effects on the structural factors (e.g., regulation of environmen- tal pollution) and psychological factors (e.g., overtly neg- ative interracial interactions) that ultimately shape health outcomes. One limitation of this research is that the respondents who completed Project Implicit’s racial-bias measures might not have been representative of their counties on all dimensions. For instance, Project Implicit respondents might not reflect racial biases of older community mem- bers. Though we employed a poststratification weighting scheme designed to circumvent this limitation, no amount of poststratification weighting can make a sample truly representative on all dimensions. Thus, future research should examine whether the observed effects remain when bias is measured with full probability sampling.
  • 35. Another limitation is that we did not have data on the geographic mobility of specific individuals for whom we assessed health outcomes. It is possible that the deceased in Study 2 had moved to their communities soon before their deaths and had died before the racial bias of their communities had any effect on their health. However, two lines of evidence suggest that this scenario occurs relatively infrequently. First, 95% of the people who died from circulatory-disease-related causes were over age 55 (CDC, 2014), and the median duration of residency for people in this age group is more than 11 years (Mateyka & Marlay, 2011). Second, when people of this age do move, they are more likely to move to another residence within the same county than to a different county (data from the U.S. Census Bureau’s 2009–2013 ACS: report titled Geographic Mobility by Selected Characteristics in the United States). Together, this evidence suggests that many people are geographically stable in the years lead- ing to their death. Nevertheless, future research might further examine the role of geographic mobility in the relationship between racial bias and health. The finding that Whites’ circulatory-disease-related death rate was increased in counties where White respon- dents harbored greater explicit bias is consistent with research showing that racial bias is linked to negative health outcomes for Whites (Kennedy et al., 1997; Lee et al., 2015; Mendes, Gray, Mendoza-Denton, Major, & Epel, 2007). One explanation for this finding, suggested by recent research (Lee et al., 2015), is that highly biased communities have decreased social capital (i.e., trust and bonding between community members), which in turn predicts negative health outcomes. Though the current research does not establish that White respondents’ racial bias caused the observed
  • 36. health disparities between Whites and Blacks, the rela- tionships between Whites’ racial bias and Black-White health disparities were independent of a large set of county-level socio-demographic characteristics. Thus, our findings raise compelling questions about the mecha- nisms through which Whites’ racial bias can be related to health outcomes. On the basis of existing theoretical frameworks (Clark et al., 1999; Hatzenbuehler et al., 2013; Major et al., 2013), we posit that multiple causal pathways might account for this relationship. These path- ways might include structural (e.g., discrimination in health care), interpersonal (e.g., hostile interactions), emotional (e.g., stress), and behavioral (e.g., maladaptive coping) processes that catalyze biological systems that increase disease risk. We hope that the current work serves to generate future research examining why there is a relationship between racial bias and health. 1310 Leitner et al. Action Editor Jamin Halberstadt served as action editor for this article. Author Contributions J. B. Leitner, E. Hehman, and R. Mendoza-Denton developed the theoretical framework. J. B. Leitner conducted the analyses and drafted the manuscript. All the authors provided critical revisions and approved the final version of the manuscript. Acknowledgments We are grateful to Doc Edge for providing insight into our
  • 37. statistical analyses. Declaration of Conflicting Interests The authors declared that they had no conflicts of interest with respect to their authorship or the publication of this article. Funding This research was supported by the National Science Founda- tion under Award Numbers 1306709 (to R. Mendoza-Denton and O. Ayduk) and 1514510 (to O. Ayduk and J. B. Leitner), and by a Social Sciences and Humanities Research Council Institutional Grant (to E. Hehman). Supplemental Material Additional supporting information can be found at http:// pss.sagepub.com/content/by/supplemental-data Open Practices All data and materials have been made publicly available via the Open Science Framework and can be accessed at https:// osf.io/5ybhd/. The complete Open Practices Disclosure for this article can be found at http://pss.sagepub.com/content/by/ supplemental-data. This article has received badges for Open Data and Open Materials. More information about the Open Practices badges can be found at https://osf.io/tvyxz/wiki/ 1.%20View%20the%20Badges/ and http://pss.sagepub.com/ content/25/1/3.full. Note 1. Unless otherwise noted, all data from the U.S. Census Bureau were downloaded from factfinder.census.gov.
  • 38. References Anderson, R. N., & Rosenberg, H. M. (1998). Age standardiza- tion of death rates: Implementation of the year 2000 stan- dard. National Vital Statistics Reports, 47(3), 1–17. Barnes, L. L., Mendes De Leon, C. F., Lewis, T. T., Bienias, J. L., Wilson, R. S., & Evans, D. A. (2008). Perceived discrimi- nation and mortality in a population-based study of older adults. American Journal of Public Health, 98, 1241–1247. doi:10.2105/AJPH.2007.114397 Black, P. H., & Garbutt, L. D. (2002). Stress, inflammation and cardiovascular disease. Journal of Psychosomatic Research, 52, 1–23. doi:10.1016/S0022-3999(01)00302-6 Centers for Disease Control and Prevention. (2013). Behavioral Risk Factor Surveillance System survey data. Atlanta, GA: U.S. Department of Health and Human Services, Centers for Disease Control and Prevention. Centers for Disease Control and Prevention. (2014). Under- lying cause of death [Data set]. Retrieved from http:// wonder.cdc.gov/ucd-icd10.html Chae, D. H., Clouston, S., Hatzenbuehler, M. L., Kramer, M. R., Cooper, H. L. F., Wilson, S. M., . . . Link, B. G. (2015). Association between an Internet-based measure of area rac- ism and Black mortality. PLoS ONE, 10(4), Article e0122963. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0122963 Clark, R., Anderson, N. B., Clark, V. R., & Williams, D. R. (1999).
  • 39. Racism as a stressor for African Americans. American Psychologist, 54, 805–816. Dovidio, J. F., Kawakami, K., & Gaertner, S. L. (2002). Implicit and explicit prejudice and interracial interaction. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 82, 62–68. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.82.1.62 Frey, W. H., & Myers, D. (2005). Racial segregation in U.S. met- ropolitan areas and cities, 1990–2000: Patterns, trends, and explanations (Population Studies Center Research Report 05-573). Retrieved from http://www.frey-demographer.org/ reports/R-2005-2_RacialSegragationTrends.pdf Gonsalkorale, K., Sherman, J. W., & Klauer, K. C. (2009). Aging and prejudice: Diminished regulation of automatic race bias among older adults. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 45, 410–414. Greenwald, A. G., Nosek, B. A., & Banaji, M. R. (2003). Understanding and using the Implicit Association Test: I. An improved scoring algorithm. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 85, 197–216. doi:10.1037/0022- 3514.85.2.197 Greenwald, A. G., Poehlman, T. A., Uhlmann, E. L., & Banaji, M. R. (2009). Understanding and using the Implicit Association Test: III. Meta-analysis of predictive validity. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 97, 17–41. doi:10.1037/a0015575 Hall, W. J., Chapman, M. V., Lee, K. M., Merino, Y. M., Thomas, T. W., Payne, B. K., . . . Coyne-Beasley, T. (2015). Implicit racial/ethnic bias among health care professionals
  • 40. and its influence on health care outcomes: A systematic review. American Journal of Public Health, 105, e60–e76. doi:10.2105/AJPH.2015.302903 Hatzenbuehler, M. L., Bellatorre, A., Lee, Y., Finch, B., Muennig, P., & Fiscella, K. (2014). Structural stigma and all-cause mortality in sexual minority populations. Social Science & Medicine, 103, 33–41. Hatzenbuehler, M. L., Jun, H.-J., Corliss, H. L., & Austin, S. B. (2014). Structural stigma and cigarette smoking in a pro- spective cohort study of sexual minority and heterosexual youth. Annals of Behavioral Medicine, 47, 48–56. https://osf.io/tvyxz/wiki/1.%20View%20the%20Badges/ https://osf.io/tvyxz/wiki/1.%20View%20the%20Badges/ http://www.frey-demographer.org/reports/R-2005- 2_RacialSegragationTrends.pdf http://www.frey-demographer.org/reports/R-2005- 2_RacialSegragationTrends.pdf http://pss.sagepub.com/content/by/supplemental-data http://pss.sagepub.com/content/by/supplemental-data http://pss.sagepub.com/content/25/1/3.full http://pss.sagepub.com/content/25/1/3.full Blacks’ Death Rate and Whites’ Bias 1311 Hatzenbuehler, M. L., Phelan, J. C., & Link, B. G. (2013). Stigma as a fundamental cause of population health inequalities. American Journal of Public Health, 103, 813–821. Hofmann, W., Gawronski, B., Gschwendner, T., Le, H., & Schmitt, M. (2005). A meta-analysis on the correla-
  • 41. tion between the Implicit Association Test and explicit self-report measures. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 31, 1369–1385. Hubbard, A. E., Ahern, J., Fleischer, N. L., Van der Laan, M., Lippman, S. A., Jewell, N., . . . Satariano, W. A. (2010). To GEE or not to GEE: Comparing population average and mixed models for estimating the associations between neigh- borhood risk factors and health. Epidemiology, 21, 467–474. Kennedy, B. P., Kawachi, I., Lochner, K., Jones, C., & Prothrow- Stith, D. (1997). (Dis)respect and black mortality. Ethnicity & Disease, 7(3), 207–214. Lee, Y., Kawachi, I., Muennig, P., & Hatzenbuehler, M. L. (2015). Effects of racial prejudice on the health of commu- nities: A multilevel survival analysis. American Journal of Public Health, 105, 2349–2355. Lohr, S. (2009). Sampling: Design and analysis. Boston, MA: Brooks/Cole. Major, B., Mendes, W. B., & Dovidio, J. F. (2013). Intergroup relations and health disparities: A social psychological perspective. Health Psychology, 32, 514–524. doi:10.1038/ nsmb.2010 Mateyka, P., & Marlay, M. (2011). Residential duration by ten- ure, race, and ethnicity: 2009. Retrieved from https:// www.census.gov/content/dam/Census/library/working- papers/2011/demo/2009-Duration.pdf Mendes, W. B., Gray, H. M., Mendoza-Denton, R., Major, B., & Epel, E. S. (2007). Why egalitarianism might be good for your health: Physiological thriving during stressful encoun-
  • 42. ters. Psychological Science, 18, 991–998. doi:10.1111/j.1467- 9280.2007.02014.x Mendoza-Denton, R., Downey, G., Purdie, V. J., Davis, A., & Pietrzak, J. (2002). Sensitivity to status-based rejection: Implications for African American students’ college expe- rience. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 83, 896–918. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.83.4.896 Pascoe, E. A., & Richman, L. S. (2009). Perceived discrimination and health: A meta-analytic review. Psychological Bulletin, 135, 531–554. doi:10.1037/a0016059 Sawyer, P. J., Major, B., Casad, B. J., Townsend, S. S. M., & Mendes, W. B. (2012). Discrimination and the stress response: Psychological and physiological consequences of anticipating prejudice in interethnic interactions. American Journal of Public Health, 102, 1020–1026. doi:10.2105/ AJPH.2011.300620 Schmidt, K., & Nosek, B. A. (2010). Implicit (and explicit) racial attitudes barely changed during Barack Obama’s presidential campaign and early presidency. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 46, 308–314. doi:10.1016/ j.jesp.2009.12.003 U.S. Census Bureau. (2013). Metropolitan and Micropolitan Delineation Files [Data set]. Retrieved from http://www .census.gov/population/metro/data/def.html U.S. Department of Health and Human Services, Centers for Disease Control and Prevention, National Center for Health Statistics. (2014). Health, United States, 2014. Retrieved from http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/books/NBK299348/ pdf/Bookshelf_NBK299348.pdf
  • 43. Williams, D. R., & Mohammed, S. A. (2009). Discrimination and racial disparities in health: Evidence and needed research. Journal of Behavioral Medicine, 32, 20–47. doi:10.1007/ s10865-008-9185-0 Xu, K., Nosek, B., & Greenwald, A. G. (2014). Data from the Race Implicit Association Test on the Project Implicit Demo Website. Journal of Open Psychology Data, 2(1), Article e3. doi:10.5334/jopd.ac http://www.census.gov/population/metro/data/def.html http://www.census.gov/population/metro/data/def.html http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/books/NBK299348/pdf/Bookshelf_ NBK299348.pdf http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/books/NBK299348/pdf/Bookshelf_ NBK299348.pdf Fall 2017 General Psychology: Writing Assignment Your assignment is to write a paper reacting to a scholarly journal article. The purpose of this assignment is to give you practice in reading, understanding, and critically evaluating research. You are also expected to demonstrate the ability to synthesize current research with our course materials. This paper requires more than a simple summary of what you have read. Please allow yourself plenty of time to complete this assignment. You are going to write about the following article: Leitner, J. B., Hehman, E., Ayduk, O., & Mendoza-Denton, R.
  • 44. (2016). Blacks’ death rate due to circulatory diseases is positively related to whites’ explicit racial bias: A nationwide investigation using project implicit. Psychological Science, 27, 1299-1311. doi: 10.1177/0956797616658450 You can download the article here: http://bit.ly/2xIrOlR Your paper should include two parts: 1. Part 1 - A brief summary of the article that answers the questions in the attached answer sheet. Please note: a. You should not cite verbatim from the paper. You MUST use your own words. b. If there is no clear answer in the paper to any of the questions, you can answer as you see fit, but you MUST explain your answers. Otherwise, you will not receive points for that particular answer. You can also say that there is no clear answer in the article. c. If there are few studies reported in the paper, please answer the questions for each study. d. You are not responsible for knowing or reporting the statistical analyses, the details of the manipulations, or the procedures.
  • 45. e. Answers to Part 1 should be ONLY within the text-boxes provided, using 12point Times New Roman font. 2. Part 2 - Your critical reaction to the article and your conclusions. You might critique the research (for example, sample, method) and/or the conclusions the authors arrived at; you can identify unresolved issues, suggest future research, and/or reflect on the study’s implications. In your discussion, you must clearly connect the material you are learning in class with the article’s content area, theories, methodology, and/or findings. Part 2 of the paper should not be longer than one page. Use double-spaced, 12-point Times New Roman font, with 1-inch margins on the bottom, top, and sides. We will deduct points for deviating from these parameters. For clarification, Part 2 must include: a. At least one justified original critique (that is, critique that is not based on the limitations the authors discussed in the paper) b. At least one suggestion for future research c. At least one connection to class materials d. At least one practical or theoretical implication of this study e. Your overall impression of the article To successfully complete this assignment, you should carefully
  • 46. read the article, more than once. When read the article, go beyond highlighting important facts and interesting findings. Ask yourself questions as you read: What are the researchers’ assumptions? How does the article contribute to the field? Are the findings generalizable, and to whom? We strongly suggest consulting Chapter 2 (research methods) and the relevant class notes when working on this paper. Please submit the assignment to TurnitIn in a WORD file. Do not submit a PDF (we will deduct points for that). See relevant instructions page. The complete assignment should be submitted as one file. Please use the Word file provided on BB. The assignment is worth 10 points of your overall class grade. Grading criteria: 1. Each part is worth 50% of your grade. These refer to (a) accuracy and clarity of answers; (b) justifications of your answers; (c) answering all the parts of each question as appear in the answer sheet and in the instruction on this document; (d) Grammatically correct writing and adherence to length and formatting requirements. For issues of grammar, syntax, and style, we encourage you to use the services of the Writing Center. The paper is due by Wednesday, November 1st at 11:59PM.
  • 47. You must submit the paper via Turnitin ONLY in a Word file. We will provide you the details in a separate document, to be found in BB under the Writing Assignment tab. You must load only one document, as Turnitin does not accept more than one document. TIPS FOR SCIENTIFIC WRITING Psychology is a behavioral science, and writing in psychology is similar to writing in the hard sciences. Avoid anthropomorphism: Anthropomorphism occurs when human characteristics are attributed to animals or inanimate entities. Anthropomorphism can make your writing awkward. Some examples include: “The experiment attempted to demonstrate…,” and “The tables compare…” Reword such sentences so that a person performs the action: “The experimenter attempted to demonstrate…” The verbs “show” or “indicate” can also be used: “The tables show…” Verb tenses: Select verb tenses carefully. Use the past tense when expressing actions or conditions that occurred at a specific time in the past, when discussing other people’s work, and when reporting results. Use the present perfect tense to express past actions or conditions that did not occur at a specific time, or to describe an action beginning in the past and continuing in the present.
  • 48. Pronoun agreement: Be consistent within and across sentences with pronouns that refer to a noun introduced earlier (antecedent). A common error is a construction such as “Each child responded to questions about their favorite toys.” The sentence should have either a plural subject (children) or a singular pronoun (his or her). Vague pronouns, such as “this” or “that,” without a clear antecedent can confuse readers: “This shows that girls are more likely than boys …” could be rewritten as “These results show that girls are more likely than boys…” Avoid figurative language and superlatives: Scientific writing should be as concise and specific as possible. Emotional language and superlatives, such as “very,” “highly,” “astonishingly,” “extremely,” “quite,” and even “exactly,” are imprecise or unnecessary. A line that is “exactly 100 centimeters” is, simply, 100 centimeters. Avoid colloquial expressions and informal language: Use “children” rather than “kids;” “many” rather than “a lot;” “acquire” rather than “get;” “prepare for” rather than “get ready;” etc. Labels: Do not equate people with their physical or mental conditions or categorize people broadly as objects. For example, adjectival forms like “older adults” are preferable to labels such as “the elderly” or “the schizophrenics.” Another option is to mention the person first, followed by a descriptive phrase. For example, “people diagnosed with schizophrenia.” Be careful using
  • 49. the label “normal,” as it may imply that others are abnormal.