SlideShare a Scribd company logo
1 of 8
Download to read offline
ISBN 88-7395-155-4 Ā© 2006 ICMPC 605
Alma Mater Studiorum University of Bologna, August 22-26
2006
Age differences in listening to music while studying
Anastasia Kotsopoulou
Institute of Education, University of London,
UK
Susan Hallam
Institute of Education, University of London,
UK s.hallam@ioe.ac.uk
ABSTRACT
To date, there has been little research specifically
focusing on age differences and the perceived effects of
music on different types of studying tasks. This research
aims to explore age differences in the perceived effects
of playing music in the background while studying a
range of different tasks. Rating scale questionnaires
were administered to six hundred students in three age
groups, 12-13, 15-16 and 20-21. The questionnaires
explored the use of music in everyday life, the extent of
listening to music while studying, the kinds of tasks
where background music was played, the perceived
effects of music on studying, the types of music listened
to while studying and the factors that influenced the
decision to listen to music while working. Repeated
measures and factorial analyses of variance revealed
age significant differences in relation to most aspects of
listening to music in everyday life, the moods which
stimulated listening to music while studying, and
factors which triggered music being turned off. There
were few differences in relation to the kinds of tasks
where music was played. Overall, the youngest students
listened to music the least in their everyday lives, and
while studying. The university students were least likely
to agree that it aided concentration and helped them
learn faster, while the youngest were the least likely to
agree that it kept them company, alleviated boredom,
relaxed them, interfered so they could not concentrate
and interfered because they sang along with the music.
They were also less likely to turn off the music when
they could not concentrate, when they were unable to
learn, and were less discriminating in when they played
music in the background. The findings suggest that
older students perceive that music plays a increasing
role in their everyday lives. They also become more
aware of its effects on their studying. Experimental
research is now needed to verify these findings.
Keywords: Studying, homework, effects of music
INTRODUCTION
The effect of music on the moods, emotions and
behaviour of both individuals and groups has been
noted throughout history. A number of writers have
discussed the functions of music (e.g. Merriam, 1964;
Gaston, 1968), while others have researched both the
physiological and psychological effects (for reviews see
Radocy & Boyle, 1988; North and Hargreaves, 1997;
Hallam, 2001a; 2006). As a result of this research,
music has come to be considered as lying on a
continuum from highly stimulating and invigorating to
soothing or calming (Gaston, 1968). There is strong
evidence from a variety of sources that people respond
differently to stimulative and sedative music (Radocy
In: M. Baroni, A. R. Addessi, R. Caterina, M. Costa (2006) Proceedings
of the 9th International Conference on Music Perception & Cognition
(ICMPC9), Bologna/Italy, August 22-26 2006.Ā©2006 The Society for
Music Perception & Cognition (SMPC) and European Society for the
Cognitive Sciences of Music (ESCOM). Copyright of the content of an
individual paper is held by the primary (first-named) author of that
paper. All rights reserved. No paper from this proceedings may be
reproduced or transmitted in any form or by any means, electronic or
mechanical, including photocopying, recording, or by any information
retrieval systems, without permission in writing from the paper's
primary author. No other part of this proceedings may be reproduced or
transmitted in any form or by any means, electronic or mechanical,
including photocopying, recording, or by any information retrieval
system, without permission in writing from SMPC and ESCOM.
ISBN 88-7395-155-4 Ā© 2006 ICMPC 606
and Boyle, 1988; Hallam, 2001a). In the modern
western world where music is readily available to
everyone through radio, recordings, TV and videos and
where recorded background music is routinely played in
many public places the need to understand the effects of
music on our behaviour and cognitive processing has
become increasingly important. While, research has
been undertaken on the effects of music on a variety of
activities it has been relatively unsystematic and much
of it has been inconclusive. This is in part because
music can be processed in different ways (Hallam,
2001a). While there may be general trends in our
responses to stimulating or relaxing music these are
overlaid by individual cognitions which may mediate
the immediate effects, for instance, associations of
particular pieces of music with particular events or
dislike of particular musical genres.
With regard to young people, we know little about the
extent to which they are exposed to music in their
everyday lives or its effects on them. Given the overall
consumption of music within the wider community we
might expect that their exposure is substantial (Hallam,
2001a). We know that in the adolescent years music
becomes increasingly important with most teenagers
listening to music for approximately three hours a day,
its role being perceived in relation to portraying an
image to the outside world and satisfying their
emotional needs (North et al., 2001). We also know that
much studying undertaken at home is accompanied by
music or the TV playing (Patton, et al., 1983;
Kotsopoulou, 1997). What is as yet relatively
unexplored is the effect that the music may have on the
behaviour or task performance of young people.
Savan (1999) has demonstrated improved behaviour
and a greater concentration on school work when
Mozart was played during the science lessons of 10
children with learning, emotional and behavioural
difficulties. Other studies have explored the effects of
music on children in mainstream schools but the type of
music played and the ways in which it was expected to
influence behaviour have not always been specified.
Mitchell (1949) compared performance on a
comprehension task of listening to a variety show, a
musical show or silence and found that reading
achievement was not adversely affected by the musical
programme. In a later study, Hall (1952) found that
performance on reading comprehension tests was
significantly improved when background music was
playing. There were also ā€˜settling down periodsā€™ at the
beginning of the morning and afternoon sessions and a
mid-afternoon fatigue period when music was of
greatest assistance. However, no details were given of
the kind of background music which was used.
Not all of the research has found positive effects of
background music. Fogelson (1973), assessing the
reading test performance of 8th graders when an
instrumental version of Mantovaniā€™s ā€˜Favourite Show
Tunesā€™ was playing found that performance was
adversely affected for all the students. The music had a
more detrimental effect on the children of higher ability
than those of low ability. Kiger (1989), studying older
pupils aged 15 years who read a 1450 word passage on
Japanese history with high or low information load
music or no music in the background, found that
reading comprehension scores were significantly higher
in the low information load condition than in the silent
or high information load conditions.
Other work has focused on tasks which involve recall.
Henderson, Crews and Barlow (1945) explored the
effect of music as a source of distraction during the
taking of a test. Type of music and the complexity of
the testing material both affected performance.
Mowsesian and Heyer (1973) studied the effects of
music on test taking in 15 year olds using a control, and
groups listening to rock, folk, symphonic music and
opera. They were tested on mathematics, language and
spelling and also completed a self-concept of ability
scale. The music had no significant effect on
performance. Other tasks have been explored, for
instance, reading (McDonald, 1975; Harp, 1988; Hall,
1952; Douglas and Willat, 1994), writing (Karnowski,
1986; Koppelman and Imig, 1995), mathematics (Scott,
1970; Hallam and Price, 1998; Hallam et al., 2002),
memorizing (Morton, Kershner, and Sigel, 1990),
science (Savan, 1999) and learning a foreign language
(Failoni, 1993).
Much of the early work in the field was unsystematic
not taking account of the type of music being played -
whether it stimulated or calmed - making it impossible
to interpret the findings. Furthermore, factors such as
the characteristics of the individual (age, ability,
personality, metacognitive strategy use, musical
expertise, familiarity with the music, frequency of use
of background music, nationality, previous experiences
of listening to music), and the nature of the music (type,
stimulating/relaxing, complexity, volume, familiarity,
like/dislike of the music, and whether it was self
selected) were not taken into account. Typically,
students were asked to complete a task while music was
playing in the background, their performance was then
measured. Each study was undertaken in isolation and
there was no sense of progression or overall direction.
Students generally did not have the opportunity to select
or evaluate the music, they were not asked what they
perceived the effects of the music to be on their
behavior and studying, and they were not asked if they
normally listened to music while they were studying.
Explanations of the effect of music on learning and
behaviour have tended to be in terms of arousal and
mood. The Yerkes-Dodson law states that the arousal
ISBN 88-7395-155-4 Ā© 2006 ICMPC 607
level of the individual increases performance up to an
optimal level beyond which over arousal leads to a
deterioration in performance. The law also states that
the deterioration occurs more quickly when the task to
be performed is complex or under-learned. A simple
task will require a higher level of arousal for
concentration to be maintained. Stimulating music is
expected to increase arousal and improve performance
on simple tasks but if the task is complex the level of
arousal may become too great and performance
deteriorate. Arousal levels may also be linked to
personality factors (Eysenck, 1967) and are responsive
to a range of environmental stimuli. They are not
consistent over time. Some research has shown that
positive emotions may adversely affect cognitive
reasoning tasks (Oaksford, Morris, Grainger and
Williams, 1996) so music inducing a positive mood
might interfere with task performance.
Most of this research has been quasi-experimental and
has not reflected the ways that students listen to music
while doing homework. In that context, while students
do have to undertake tasks set by others, they normally
have control over whether they listen to music while
completing the tasks and also the type of music that
they listen to. The present study explores age
differences in studentsā€™ use of music in their everyday
lives and in their studying, and their perceptions of its
effects on them and their work.
METHODOLOGY
Three age groups were identified for study: 12-13 year
olds (secondary), 15-16 year olds (advanced secondary)
and 20-21 year olds (university students). These ages
were selected as representing distinct periods in young
peopleā€™s lives when their lifestyles and the importance
of studying would differ. Four different nationalities
were selected exemplifying different musical traditions.
Those included were the USA, the UK, Greece and
Japan. The USA was selected because being a relatively
ā€˜youngā€™ country, it lacked a long, well established
musical tradition; it was the birth place of modern
popular music, i.e. rock and roll and its world wide
political and commercial influence has provided it with
a key role in the music industry. The UK, although it
shares much of its current popular music with the USA
is part of a long European musical heritage. The music
of Greece links the cultures of East and West. Although
it is a European country, people have maintained their
own musical traditions despite the influx of Western
ā€˜popā€™ music. Japan offers a completely contrasting
cultural setting, with its own traditional music and only
relatively recent Western influences. Although, overall,
the students were randomly selected, an attempt was
made to recruit similar numbers of males and females in
each age group and from each nationality. Finally, the
sample was divided on the basis of active involvement
in making music; i.e. did the participants play a musical
instrument or sing in a choir, or group. Overall, 150
students from each country participated in the study, 50
from each age group. The students came from major (in
most cases the capital) cities in each country. They were
selected from schools and universities, which had a
broad intake representing a wide range of socio-
economic status.
A five point rating scale questionnaire was developed to
explore the listening habits of the students from the
different cultures. This was devised based on interviews
undertaken with young people regarding their use of
music while studying. Section 1 required respondents to
provide information about their gender, age, nationality,
and their active involvement in any musical pursuit.
Section 2 included statements about the role of music in
everyday life. Section 3 included statements relating to
listening to music while studying. Respondents were
asked if they listened to music when they were revising
for exams, writing, memorizing texts, reading, doing
course work, editing work previously completed,
solving problems, developing ideas, thinking or
learning a foreign language. Questions were also posed
about listening to music while studying their most or
least favorite subject. The fourth section explored issues
relating to the perceived effects of music on them and
their studying. The fifth section explored the different
kinds of music, which they listened to, the
characteristics of the music and the particular musical
genres. The statements about musical genre varied from
country to country to take account of national variation.
The categories were developed from those that appeared
in the shops selling music recordings in each country.
This ensured ecological validity and was also likely to
provide respondents with categorizations that were
familiar to them. Section 6 explored issues relating to
decisions taken with regard to listening to music or not.
Statements included: ā€œI listen to music while studying:
when I am happy, when I am bored, when I like/ donā€™t
like the subject, when I am disturbed by other noises
around meā€; ā€œI turn the music off when: I canā€™t
concentrate, I am unable to learn, someone suggests I
shouldā€; ā€œI choose to listen to music because I am
influenced by: the type of music, the subject I am
studying, the nature of the subject, the mood I am inā€.
Each variable where the rating scale was used was
coded from 1 to 5. One signified the response always, 2
frequently, 3 occasionally, 4, rarely, and five never. The
findings reported here focus on differences as a function
of age.
RESULTS
Listening to music in everyday life
A series of multivariate and repeated measures analysis
of variance were undertaken. Table 1 gives the means
ISBN 88-7395-155-4 Ā© 2006 ICMPC 608
and standard deviations for the questions related to
listening in everyday life. To summarise the overall age
differences, the secondary school students tended to
listen to music less often than their older counterparts,
although there was some variability depending on the
particular time of day. University students reported
listening to music more in their free time, as children,
when they were waking up, when they were at home in
the morning, and when they were eating. Advanced
secondary school students reported listening to music
more when they went to sleep and when they were at
home in the evening. There were no significant
differences in relation to travelling. A repeated
measures analysis of variance indicated significant
overall differences between the reported listening
activities (F=162.086, DF=8, p=.05). The most common
times for listening to music were in free time and while
travelling. The least common times were when taking a
bath and while eating. There were significant
interactions between age and listening to music with
everyday activities (F=8.302, df=14, p=.00001).
Listening to music while studying
Table 2 summarises the means for each age group,
regarding their responses to questions about listening to
music while studying. There are very few significant
differences between the age groups. Those observed are
for doing course work and solving problems. A repeated
measures analysis of variance found significant
differences between the means across questions
demonstrating that the students listen to music
differentially depending on the type of studying they are
undertaking (F = 13.01, df = 11, p = .0001). The activity
where music was most often played in the background
was thinking followed by writing. The least common
were memorizing texts and learning a foreign language.
There were significant interactions between age and
type of studying in relation to the different kinds of
studying (F=39.888, df=2, p=.035).
Table 1: Listening to music in everyday life
Age
12-
13
Age
15-
16
Age
20-
21
Overall
mean
SIG
Music
listening in
free time
2.29 1.91 1.91 2.04 .0001
Listening
to music as
a child
2.54 2.35 2.26 2.38 .015
Listening
to music
when wake
up
3.65 2.95 2.45 3.02 .0001
Listening
to music
3.19 2.56 2.85 2.87 .0001
when go to
sleep
Listen to
music at
home in the
morning
3.08 2.76 2.55 2.80 .0001
Listen to
music at
home in the
evening
2.39 2.25 2.66 2.43 .002
Listening
to music
while eating
3.83 3.55 3.29 3.56 .0001
Listening
to music
when I am
taking a
bath
3.91 3.60 3.32 3.62 .0001
Listening
to music
when I am
travelling
2.28 2.19 2.23 2.23 NS
The highest responses are in bold, and the lowest are in italic. The
highest responses mean the least agreement
Table 2: Listening to music while studying
Listening to
music while:
Age
12-
13
Age
15-
16
Age
20-
21
Overall
mean
SIG
Studying 3.19 3.06 3.02 3.09 .NS
Revising for
exams
4.04 3.67 3.45 3.72 .0001
Writing 3.31 3.00 3.06 3.12 .05
Memorizing
texts
4.18 4.08 4.03 4.10 NS
Reading 3.63 3.47 3.54 3.54 NS
Doing course
work
3.63 3.29 3.27 3.39 .014
Editing work
previously
completed
3.67 3.45 3.35 3.49 NS
Solving
problems
3.61 3.29 3.61 3.50 .032
Developing
ideas
3.56 3.31 3.32 3.40 NS
Thinking 3.27 2.73 2.79 2.93 NS
Studying my
favourite
subject
3.30 3.30 3.46 3.35 NS
Studying my
least favourite
subject
3.33 3.24 3.38 3.31 NS
Learning a 3.89 3.86 4.13 3.95 .05
ISBN 88-7395-155-4 Ā© 2006 ICMPC 609
foreign
language
The highest responses are in bold, and the lowest are in italics. The
highest responses mean the least agreement
Effects of listening to music on studying
Students from each age group were asked to report the
perceived effects of listening to music on their studying.
There were no statistical significant differences between
secondary, advanced secondary and university students,
in relation to their perceptions of whether music helped
them to concentrate, kept them company, alleviated
boredom, helped them learn faster, interfered because
they ā€˜sang alongā€™ or interfered because it developed a
too high level of arousal. There were significant
differences in relation to the extent to which music was
seen to be relaxing and to interfere with concentration.
The university (mean = 1.73, SD = .77) and advanced
students (mean = 1.72, SD = 1.06) perceived that music
was more relaxing than their younger counterparts
(mean 1.97, SD = 1.21). These differences were
statistically significant (F = 3.5, df = 2, p = .03). The
university (mean = 2.89,SD = 1.19) and advanced
secondary students (mean 3.38, SD = 1.26) also
believed that music interfered with their concentration
more than their younger counterparts (mean = 3.55, SD
= 1.46). These differences were statistically significant
(F = 13. 96, df = 2, p = .00001). These differences may
reflect the type of music being listened to or increasing
metacognitive awareness with age. Table 3 gives an
overview of the means and standard deviations for the
findings reported in this section. A repeated measures
analysis of variance indicated that these differences
were statistically significant (F = 1. 057, df = 6, p =
.05). However, there were no statistically significant
interactions between the different perceived effects of
listening to background music while studying and age.
Table 3: Perceived effects of listening to background
music
I believe
that music:
Age
12-
13
Age
15-
16
Age
20-
21
Overall
mean
SIG
helps
concentrate
3.03 3.00 3.26 3.10 NS
keeps
company
2.34 2.21 2.13 2.23 NS
alleviates
boredom
2.07 1.91 1.95 1.98 NS
relaxes me 1.97 1.72 1.73 1.81 .03
helps me
learn faster
3.52 3.45 3.67 3.54 NS
interferes so
I can't
concentrate
3.55 3.38 2.89 3.27 .0001
interferes 3.34 3.15 3.09 3.19 NS
because I
sing along
interferes
because it
makes me
too aroused
3.99 4.04 4.00 4.01 NS
The highest responses are in bold, and the lowest are in italic. The
highest responses mean the least agreement
Characteristics of the music listened to while
studying
Only one aspect of the characteristics of the music
listened to while studying was different for the three age
groups. This was whether the students listened to music
with a fast tempo. The most negative response came
from the advanced secondary students (mean = 3.22,
SD = 1.36). The most positive response came from the
youngest students (mean 2.81, SD = 1.45). These
differences were statistically significant (F = 5.14, df =
2, p = .006). A repeated measures analysis of variance
showed no difference in the overall means between the
various categories which tends to suggest that no
particular musical characteristics determine the music
which students play in the background. Overall, young
people most often listened to songs that they knew
which were their favourites. Least often listened to was
instrumental music and arousing music. There were no
significant interactions between age and music
characteristics.
Table 4: Characteristics of the music listened to
while studying
While
studying I
listen to:
Age
12-
13
Age
15-
16
Age
20-
21
Overall
mean
SIG
my favourite
music
2.68 2.60 2.76 2.68 .
NS
songs that I
know
2.50 2.61 2.67 2.59 NS
music with
fast tempo
2.81 3.22 3.13 3.05 .006
music with
slow tempo
3.29 3.28 3.19 3.25 NS
Songs 2.57 2.80 2.88 2.75 NS
loud music 3.25 3.49 3.60 3.45 NS
instrumental
music
4.09 3.85 3.86 3.93 NS
calming
music
3.59 3.51 3.56 3.55 NS
Arousing
music
3.57 3.75 3.64 3.65 NS
The highest responses are in bold, and the lowest are in italics. The
highest responses mean the least agreement
ISBN 88-7395-155-4 Ā© 2006 ICMPC 610
Decisions to play background music while studying
Table 5 shows that in taking decisions about having
background music playing while studying there were
age related differences. The older students were more
prepared to take action to reduce any negative effects of
the music than the younger students. Younger students
tended to turn on music while studying, when they were
disturbed by other noises around them, and turned it off
if someone suggested they should. Advanced secondary
school students were more influenced than the others
about deciding to listen to music depending on the
subject they were studying, and its nature. University
students, turned the music off more than the school
students when they were unable to concentrate, when it
made them nervous, and when they were unable to
learn.
Overall, young people most strongly agreed that they
turned the music off when they couldnā€™t concentrate
and that listening to music depended on the mood they
were in. They most strongly disagreed that they turned
music off when someone suggested that they should and
that listening depended on the nature of the subject that
they were studying.
Type of music listened to while studying
Table 6 summarizes the studentsā€™ responses for
listening to different kinds of music while studying.
Overall, there were relatively few age differences in the
kinds of music selected to be listened to while studying.
There were some specific differences for the Greek and
Japanese populations but for the population as a whole
it was the variety of music, which was listened to,
which seemed to increase with age. The most common
type of music listened to across all nationalities was
recorded pop music.
Table 5: Decisions about when to listen to music
Age
12-
13
Age
15-
16
Age
20-
21
Overall
mean
SIG
I listen to music
when I am happy
2.68 2.57 2.90 2.71 NS
I listen to music
when I am bored
2.56 2.83 2.76 2.71 NS
I listen to music
when I like the
subject
3.16 3.19 3.29 3.21 NS
I listen to music
when I dislike the
subject
3.30 3.14 3.29 3.25 NS
I turn the music off
when I am disturbed
by other noises
2.89 3.32 3.15 3.12 .017
I turn the music off
when I am can't
concentrate
3.29 2.16 1.86 2.14 .001
I turn the music off
when it makes me
3.50 3.18 3.04 3.24 .015
nervous
I turn the music off
when I am unable to
learn
2.94 2.70 2.29 2.65 .0001
I turn the music off
when I someone
suggests I should
3.08 3.56 3.57 3.40 .0001
Listening to music
depends on the type
of music
2.64 2.50 2.56 2.57 NS
Listening to music
depends on the
subject I am studying
3.39 2.99 3.35 3.24 .009
Listening to music
depends on the
nature of the subject
3.60 3.04 3.46 3.37 .0001
Listening to music
depends on the mood
I am in
2.24 2.13 2.16 2.18 NS
The highest responses are in bold, and the lowest are in italics. The
highest responses mean the least agreement
Table 6: Type of music listened to while studying
While
studying I
listen to:
Age
12-
13
Age
15-
16
Age
20-
21
Overall
mean
SIG
Classical 4.15 4.26 4.16 4.19 NS
Rock 3.31 3.49 3.35 3.39 NS
Blues 4.09 4.20 3.92 4.07 NS
Country 4.30 4.15 4.17 4.21 NS
Easy listening 3.77 3.94 3.18 3.84 NS
folk/world
music
4.35 4.44 4.51 4.43 NS
Reggae 3.87 3.97 4.13 4.05 .023
Pop 2.77 3.43 3.29 3.16 .0001
Soul 3.80 3.64 3.71 3.72 NS
Dance 3.06 3.59 3.65 3.43 .005
Gospel 3.95 4.28 4.68 4.30 .0001
Jazz 3.81 4.30 4.17 4.09 .0001
Popular music
(JP)
3.95 3.02 3.04 3.34 .002
Enka (JP) 4.57 4.85 4.79 4.73 NS
New music (JP) 3.34 3.55 3.02 3.30 NS
Folk songs (JP) 4.64 4.42 4.38 4.48 NS
Kayoh kyoku
(JP)
3.88 3.65 3.25 3.59 NS
Commercial
songs (JP)
3.71 3.87 4.00 3.86 NS
TV music
themes (JP)
3.12 3.63 3.40 3.37 NS
Greek pop 3.04 3.81 3.20 3.36 .012
Greek
orchestral
3.76 3.36 2.91 3.36 .011
Greek popular 3.80 3.64 3.07 3.52 .031
Greek folk 4.08 4.53 4.00 4.21 .020
ISBN 88-7395-155-4 Ā© 2006 ICMPC 611
Radio 3.42 3.08 3.22 3.24 NS
Recorded
music
3.27 2.98 3.22 3.16 NS
The highest responses are in bold, and the lowest are in italics. The
highest responses mean the least agreement
DISCUSSION
The findings of this study reveal communality and
difference between students of different ages in
listening to music while studying. In everyday life there
is an increase in reported listening as young people get
older. This may reflect the increasingly unsupervised
time that university students have in contrast to those
still at school. The greatest listening time for the
youngest students is in the evening, when they have free
time. In contrast for the older students this is their least
favoured time, probably because they have wider social
interests and greater freedom. Listening while traveling
was the most reported across all age groups. This
requires further investigation.
In relation to the types of studying undertaken when
music was played there were few age related
differences. Where there were differences they may
have reflected the different types of studying required in
different educational environments. The students
seemed to be aware of the extent to which having
background music playing interfered with different
activities. Students reported rarely listening to music
while revising for examinations, memorizing material,
or learning a foreign language. They most reported
listening while thinking or writing. This suggests that
different studying activities share processing
mechanisms with musical listening to different degrees.
While the students may not have been able to articulate
these differences they were sufficiently aware to know
what tasks music most interfered with. The nature of the
subject being studied did not appear to be a factor in
whether music was played or not. The nature of the
actual task was more important.
There were few differences between secondary,
advanced secondary and university students, in relation
to their perceptions of whether music helped them to
concentrate, kept them company, alleviated boredom,
helped them learn faster, interfered because they ā€˜sang
along,ā€™ or interfered because it developed a too high
level of arousal. The university and advanced students
perceived that music was more relaxing than their
younger counterparts. This may reflect the increased
pressure on them as they progress through their
academic courses or may indicate that they are more
self-aware. The university and advanced secondary
students also believed that music interfered with their
concentration more than their younger counterparts
suggesting increased self-awareness. The nature of the
tasks set for private study may also play a part. The
older students may be given tasks which require greater
focus and concentration.
There were almost no differences in the characteristics
of the music listened to while studying with the
exception that the younger students tended to listen to
music with a fast tempo. Overall, young people most
often listened to songs that they knew which were their
favourites. Least often listened to was instrumental
music and arousing music. Overall, there were
relatively few age differences in the kinds of music
selected to be listened to while studying. There were
some specific differences for the Greek and Japanese
populations but for the population as a whole it was the
variety of music, which was listened to, which seemed
to increase with age. This supports earlier research
(LeBlanc and McCrary, 1983). The most common type
of music listened to across all nationalities was recorded
pop music.
The older students were more prepared to take action to
reduce any negative effects of the music than the
younger students. Younger students tended to turn on
music while studying, and when they were disturbed by
other noises around them. They turned it off if someone
suggested they should. Advanced secondary school
students were more influenced than the others about
deciding to listen to music depending on the subject
they were studying, and the nature of the subject.
University students, turned the music off more than the
school students when they were unable to concentrate,
when it made them nervous, and when they were unable
to learn. Overall, young people most strongly agreed
that they turned the music off when they couldnā€™t
concentrate and that listening to music depended on the
mood they were in. They most strongly disagreed that
they turned music off when someone suggested that
they should. This is important in relation to parental
supervision of music listening while studying as it
suggests that young people resent interference and may
ignore any instructions given.
REFERENCES
Douglas, S., and Willatts, P. (1994) The relationship
between musical ability and literacy skills, Journal of
Research in Reading, 17(2), 99-107.
Eysenck, H.J. (1967) The biological basis of
personality. Springfield, Illinois: Thomas
Failoni, J. (1993) Music means to enhance cultural
awareness and literacy in the foreign language
classroom, Mid-Atlantic Journal of Foreign Language
Pedagogy, 1, 97-108.
ISBN 88-7395-155-4 Ā© 2006 ICMPC 612
Fogelson, S. (1973) Music as a distracter on reading-
test performance of eighth grade students. Perceptual
and Motor Skills, 36, 1265-1266.
Gaston, E.T. (Ed) (1968) Music in Therapy. New York:
MacMillan
Hall, J. (1952) The effect of background music on the
reading comprehension of 278 eighth and ninth grade
students, Journal of Educational Research, 45(6), 451-
458, 1952.
Hallam, S. (2001) The power of music, London: The
Performing Rights Society. www.thepowerofmusic.co)
Hallam, S. (2006) Music Psychology in Education.
London: Institute of Education, University of London
Hallam, S. and Price, J, (1998) Can the use of
background music improve the behavior and academic
performance of children with emotional and behavioral
difficulties? British Journal of Special Education,
25(2), 87-90. .
Hallam, S., Price, J., and Katsarou, G. (2002) The
Effects of Background Music on Primary School
Pupilsā€™ Task Performance, Educational Studies, Vol.
28(2), 111-122.
Harp, B. (1988) When the principal asks: ā€˜Why are your kids
singing during reading time?ā€™ Reading Teacher, Vol. 41 (4),
454-456.
Henderson, M.T., Crews, A. & Barlow, J. (1945) A
study of the effect of music distraction on reading
efficiency, Journal of Applied Psychology, 29, 313-317.
Karnowski, L. (1986) How young writerā€™s
communicate, Educational Leadership, 44 (3), 58-60,
1986.
Kiger, D.M. (1989) Effects of music information load
on a reading comprehension task, Perceptual and Motor
Skills, 69, 531-534.
Koppelman, D. and Imig, S. (1995) The effect of music
on childrenā€™s writing content. Virginia: US.
Kotsopoulou A. (1997) Music in Studentā€™s lives,
Unpublished MA Dissertation, Institute of Education,
University of London.
LeBlanc, A. & McCrary, J. (1983) Effect of tempo on
childrenā€™s music preference, Journal of Research in
Music Education, 31 (4) 283-294
McDonald, D. (1975) Music and reading readiness,
Language Arts, 52 (6), 872-876.
Merriam, A.P. (1964) The Anthropology of Music.
USA: Northwestern University Press.
Mitchell, A.H. (1949) The effect of radio programs on
silent reading achievement of ninety-one sixth grade
students, Journal of Educational Research, 42(6), 460-
470.
Morton, L. et al (1990) The potential for therapeutic
applications of music on problems related to memory
and attention, Journal of Music Therapy, 27(4).
Mowsesian, R. And Heyer, M.R. (1973) The effect of
music as a distraction on test-taking performance,
Measurement and Evaluation in Guidance, 6(2), 104-
110.
North, A. C. & Hargreaves, D.J. (1997) Music and
consumer behaviour. In A.C. North, & D.J.
Hargreaves, Social Psychology of Music. Oxford:
Oxford University Press
North, A.C., Hargreaves, D.J. And Oā€™neill, S.A. (2000)
The importance of music to adolescents, British Journal
of Educational Psychology, 70, 255-272.
Oaksford, M., Morris, F., Grainger, B. & Williams,
J.M.G. (1996) Mood reasoning and central executive
processes, Journal of experimental Psychology,
Learning, Memory and Cognition, 22(2), 477-493.
Patton, J.E. Stinard, T.A. & Routh, D.K. (1983) Where
do children study? Journal of Educational Research,
76(5), 280-286.
Radocy, R.E. & Boyle, J.D. (1988) Psychological
Foundations of Musical Behaviour Springfield,
Illinois: Charles Thomas
Savan, A. (1999) The effect of background music on
learning, Psychology of Music, 27(2), 138-146.
Scott, T. (1970) The use of music to reduce
hyperactivity in children, American Journal of
Orthopsychiatry, 4, 677-680.

More Related Content

Similar to Age Differences In Listening To Music While Studying

Penelitian Tentang Musik dan Memori
Penelitian Tentang Musik dan MemoriPenelitian Tentang Musik dan Memori
Penelitian Tentang Musik dan MemoriCIkumparan
Ā 
Karl Asumen Poster
Karl Asumen PosterKarl Asumen Poster
Karl Asumen PosterKarl Asumen
Ā 
'Tuned Inside-Out': Mediating engagement experiences with music on-the-go"
'Tuned Inside-Out': Mediating engagement experiences with music on-the-go"'Tuned Inside-Out': Mediating engagement experiences with music on-the-go"
'Tuned Inside-Out': Mediating engagement experiences with music on-the-go"Stefan
Ā 
Research Manuscript (Final)
Research Manuscript (Final)Research Manuscript (Final)
Research Manuscript (Final)Ashley Maher
Ā 
Primary School Music Education and Test Performance
Primary School Music Education and Test PerformancePrimary School Music Education and Test Performance
Primary School Music Education and Test PerformancePaul Larson
Ā 
Eduardo Coutinho - Psychoacoustic cues to emotion in speech prosody and music
Eduardo Coutinho - Psychoacoustic cues to emotion in speech prosody and musicEduardo Coutinho - Psychoacoustic cues to emotion in speech prosody and music
Eduardo Coutinho - Psychoacoustic cues to emotion in speech prosody and musicswissnex San Francisco
Ā 
How Music Primes the Brain for Learning.pdf
How Music Primes the Brain for Learning.pdfHow Music Primes the Brain for Learning.pdf
How Music Primes the Brain for Learning.pdfFuture Education Magazine
Ā 
Music in Schools
Music in SchoolsMusic in Schools
Music in SchoolsSamuel Ennis
Ā 
Tesar EDUC 523 final paper
Tesar EDUC 523 final paperTesar EDUC 523 final paper
Tesar EDUC 523 final paperKathleen Tesar
Ā 
The effect of music listening on work performance.
The effect of music listening on work performance.The effect of music listening on work performance.
The effect of music listening on work performance.PeacefulNature
Ā 
impact_of_music_on_literacy
impact_of_music_on_literacyimpact_of_music_on_literacy
impact_of_music_on_literacySimone Ribke
Ā 
Research Methods Paper
Research Methods PaperResearch Methods Paper
Research Methods PaperMadalynn Lauria
Ā 
The benefits of incorporating music
The benefits of incorporating musicThe benefits of incorporating music
The benefits of incorporating musicvickytg123
Ā 
Presentation1.3 (1)
Presentation1.3 (1)Presentation1.3 (1)
Presentation1.3 (1)Tran Kiet
Ā 
CREATIVE EDUCATION, MUSIC PRACTICES, AND GENDER DIFFERENCES AMONG YOUNG CHINE...
CREATIVE EDUCATION, MUSIC PRACTICES, AND GENDER DIFFERENCES AMONG YOUNG CHINE...CREATIVE EDUCATION, MUSIC PRACTICES, AND GENDER DIFFERENCES AMONG YOUNG CHINE...
CREATIVE EDUCATION, MUSIC PRACTICES, AND GENDER DIFFERENCES AMONG YOUNG CHINE...John1Lorcan
Ā 
CREATIVE EDUCATION, MUSIC PRACTICES, AND GENDER DIFFERENCES AMONG YOUNG CHINE...
CREATIVE EDUCATION, MUSIC PRACTICES, AND GENDER DIFFERENCES AMONG YOUNG CHINE...CREATIVE EDUCATION, MUSIC PRACTICES, AND GENDER DIFFERENCES AMONG YOUNG CHINE...
CREATIVE EDUCATION, MUSIC PRACTICES, AND GENDER DIFFERENCES AMONG YOUNG CHINE...John1Lorcan
Ā 

Similar to Age Differences In Listening To Music While Studying (20)

Penelitian Tentang Musik dan Memori
Penelitian Tentang Musik dan MemoriPenelitian Tentang Musik dan Memori
Penelitian Tentang Musik dan Memori
Ā 
H0361055060
H0361055060H0361055060
H0361055060
Ā 
F373239.pdf
F373239.pdfF373239.pdf
F373239.pdf
Ā 
F373239.pdf
F373239.pdfF373239.pdf
F373239.pdf
Ā 
Karl Asumen Poster
Karl Asumen PosterKarl Asumen Poster
Karl Asumen Poster
Ā 
'Tuned Inside-Out': Mediating engagement experiences with music on-the-go"
'Tuned Inside-Out': Mediating engagement experiences with music on-the-go"'Tuned Inside-Out': Mediating engagement experiences with music on-the-go"
'Tuned Inside-Out': Mediating engagement experiences with music on-the-go"
Ā 
Research Manuscript (Final)
Research Manuscript (Final)Research Manuscript (Final)
Research Manuscript (Final)
Ā 
Primary School Music Education and Test Performance
Primary School Music Education and Test PerformancePrimary School Music Education and Test Performance
Primary School Music Education and Test Performance
Ā 
Eduardo Coutinho - Psychoacoustic cues to emotion in speech prosody and music
Eduardo Coutinho - Psychoacoustic cues to emotion in speech prosody and musicEduardo Coutinho - Psychoacoustic cues to emotion in speech prosody and music
Eduardo Coutinho - Psychoacoustic cues to emotion in speech prosody and music
Ā 
How Music Primes the Brain for Learning.pdf
How Music Primes the Brain for Learning.pdfHow Music Primes the Brain for Learning.pdf
How Music Primes the Brain for Learning.pdf
Ā 
Music in Schools
Music in SchoolsMusic in Schools
Music in Schools
Ā 
Tesar EDUC 523 final paper
Tesar EDUC 523 final paperTesar EDUC 523 final paper
Tesar EDUC 523 final paper
Ā 
The effect of music listening on work performance.
The effect of music listening on work performance.The effect of music listening on work performance.
The effect of music listening on work performance.
Ā 
impact_of_music_on_literacy
impact_of_music_on_literacyimpact_of_music_on_literacy
impact_of_music_on_literacy
Ā 
Opinion Essay
Opinion EssayOpinion Essay
Opinion Essay
Ā 
Research Methods Paper
Research Methods PaperResearch Methods Paper
Research Methods Paper
Ā 
The benefits of incorporating music
The benefits of incorporating musicThe benefits of incorporating music
The benefits of incorporating music
Ā 
Presentation1.3 (1)
Presentation1.3 (1)Presentation1.3 (1)
Presentation1.3 (1)
Ā 
CREATIVE EDUCATION, MUSIC PRACTICES, AND GENDER DIFFERENCES AMONG YOUNG CHINE...
CREATIVE EDUCATION, MUSIC PRACTICES, AND GENDER DIFFERENCES AMONG YOUNG CHINE...CREATIVE EDUCATION, MUSIC PRACTICES, AND GENDER DIFFERENCES AMONG YOUNG CHINE...
CREATIVE EDUCATION, MUSIC PRACTICES, AND GENDER DIFFERENCES AMONG YOUNG CHINE...
Ā 
CREATIVE EDUCATION, MUSIC PRACTICES, AND GENDER DIFFERENCES AMONG YOUNG CHINE...
CREATIVE EDUCATION, MUSIC PRACTICES, AND GENDER DIFFERENCES AMONG YOUNG CHINE...CREATIVE EDUCATION, MUSIC PRACTICES, AND GENDER DIFFERENCES AMONG YOUNG CHINE...
CREATIVE EDUCATION, MUSIC PRACTICES, AND GENDER DIFFERENCES AMONG YOUNG CHINE...
Ā 

More from Cheryl Brown

Newspaper Report Writing - Examples, Format, Pdf Exa
Newspaper Report Writing - Examples, Format, Pdf ExaNewspaper Report Writing - Examples, Format, Pdf Exa
Newspaper Report Writing - Examples, Format, Pdf ExaCheryl Brown
Ā 
How To Use A Kindle Paperwhite On And Off -
How To Use A Kindle Paperwhite On And Off -How To Use A Kindle Paperwhite On And Off -
How To Use A Kindle Paperwhite On And Off -Cheryl Brown
Ā 
Bat Writing Template Bat Template, Writing Templat
Bat Writing Template Bat Template, Writing TemplatBat Writing Template Bat Template, Writing Templat
Bat Writing Template Bat Template, Writing TemplatCheryl Brown
Ā 
Contoh Proposal Fotografi Pigura
Contoh Proposal Fotografi PiguraContoh Proposal Fotografi Pigura
Contoh Proposal Fotografi PiguraCheryl Brown
Ā 
Family Tree Introduction Essay. My Family History E
Family Tree Introduction Essay. My Family History EFamily Tree Introduction Essay. My Family History E
Family Tree Introduction Essay. My Family History ECheryl Brown
Ā 
Thesis Statement For Research Paper On Drea
Thesis Statement For Research Paper On DreaThesis Statement For Research Paper On Drea
Thesis Statement For Research Paper On DreaCheryl Brown
Ā 
Good Short Stories To Write Textual Analysis On - Herolas
Good Short Stories To Write Textual Analysis On - HerolasGood Short Stories To Write Textual Analysis On - Herolas
Good Short Stories To Write Textual Analysis On - HerolasCheryl Brown
Ā 
Where Can I Buy Parchment Writing Pape
Where Can I Buy Parchment Writing PapeWhere Can I Buy Parchment Writing Pape
Where Can I Buy Parchment Writing PapeCheryl Brown
Ā 
Pin By Emily Harris On Teach McTeacherson
Pin By Emily Harris On Teach McTeachersonPin By Emily Harris On Teach McTeacherson
Pin By Emily Harris On Teach McTeachersonCheryl Brown
Ā 
My Favourite Author Worksheet (Teacher Made)
My Favourite Author Worksheet (Teacher Made)My Favourite Author Worksheet (Teacher Made)
My Favourite Author Worksheet (Teacher Made)Cheryl Brown
Ā 
Dialogue Essay Example For 4 Person - Interpreting Success
Dialogue Essay Example For 4 Person - Interpreting SuccessDialogue Essay Example For 4 Person - Interpreting Success
Dialogue Essay Example For 4 Person - Interpreting SuccessCheryl Brown
Ā 
Pin On Report Template
Pin On Report TemplatePin On Report Template
Pin On Report TemplateCheryl Brown
Ā 
Essay-Writing.Org Discount Code 2021 Coupons Fres
Essay-Writing.Org Discount Code 2021 Coupons FresEssay-Writing.Org Discount Code 2021 Coupons Fres
Essay-Writing.Org Discount Code 2021 Coupons FresCheryl Brown
Ā 
Images For Graffiti Words On Paper Graffiti Words
Images For  Graffiti Words On Paper  Graffiti WordsImages For  Graffiti Words On Paper  Graffiti Words
Images For Graffiti Words On Paper Graffiti WordsCheryl Brown
Ā 
Pin By Personal Statement Review O
Pin By Personal Statement Review OPin By Personal Statement Review O
Pin By Personal Statement Review OCheryl Brown
Ā 
Reasons For Attending College Or University. Reas
Reasons For Attending College Or University. ReasReasons For Attending College Or University. Reas
Reasons For Attending College Or University. ReasCheryl Brown
Ā 
Sample College Paper Format ~ Writing An Essa
Sample College Paper Format ~ Writing An EssaSample College Paper Format ~ Writing An Essa
Sample College Paper Format ~ Writing An EssaCheryl Brown
Ā 
SUNFLOWERS Personalised Writing Paper Set Of 20 Pers
SUNFLOWERS Personalised Writing Paper Set Of 20 PersSUNFLOWERS Personalised Writing Paper Set Of 20 Pers
SUNFLOWERS Personalised Writing Paper Set Of 20 PersCheryl Brown
Ā 
Should We Reduce The Am
Should We Reduce The AmShould We Reduce The Am
Should We Reduce The AmCheryl Brown
Ā 
Can AI Read And Rate College Essays More Fairly Than Hu
Can AI Read And Rate College Essays More Fairly Than HuCan AI Read And Rate College Essays More Fairly Than Hu
Can AI Read And Rate College Essays More Fairly Than HuCheryl Brown
Ā 

More from Cheryl Brown (20)

Newspaper Report Writing - Examples, Format, Pdf Exa
Newspaper Report Writing - Examples, Format, Pdf ExaNewspaper Report Writing - Examples, Format, Pdf Exa
Newspaper Report Writing - Examples, Format, Pdf Exa
Ā 
How To Use A Kindle Paperwhite On And Off -
How To Use A Kindle Paperwhite On And Off -How To Use A Kindle Paperwhite On And Off -
How To Use A Kindle Paperwhite On And Off -
Ā 
Bat Writing Template Bat Template, Writing Templat
Bat Writing Template Bat Template, Writing TemplatBat Writing Template Bat Template, Writing Templat
Bat Writing Template Bat Template, Writing Templat
Ā 
Contoh Proposal Fotografi Pigura
Contoh Proposal Fotografi PiguraContoh Proposal Fotografi Pigura
Contoh Proposal Fotografi Pigura
Ā 
Family Tree Introduction Essay. My Family History E
Family Tree Introduction Essay. My Family History EFamily Tree Introduction Essay. My Family History E
Family Tree Introduction Essay. My Family History E
Ā 
Thesis Statement For Research Paper On Drea
Thesis Statement For Research Paper On DreaThesis Statement For Research Paper On Drea
Thesis Statement For Research Paper On Drea
Ā 
Good Short Stories To Write Textual Analysis On - Herolas
Good Short Stories To Write Textual Analysis On - HerolasGood Short Stories To Write Textual Analysis On - Herolas
Good Short Stories To Write Textual Analysis On - Herolas
Ā 
Where Can I Buy Parchment Writing Pape
Where Can I Buy Parchment Writing PapeWhere Can I Buy Parchment Writing Pape
Where Can I Buy Parchment Writing Pape
Ā 
Pin By Emily Harris On Teach McTeacherson
Pin By Emily Harris On Teach McTeachersonPin By Emily Harris On Teach McTeacherson
Pin By Emily Harris On Teach McTeacherson
Ā 
My Favourite Author Worksheet (Teacher Made)
My Favourite Author Worksheet (Teacher Made)My Favourite Author Worksheet (Teacher Made)
My Favourite Author Worksheet (Teacher Made)
Ā 
Dialogue Essay Example For 4 Person - Interpreting Success
Dialogue Essay Example For 4 Person - Interpreting SuccessDialogue Essay Example For 4 Person - Interpreting Success
Dialogue Essay Example For 4 Person - Interpreting Success
Ā 
Pin On Report Template
Pin On Report TemplatePin On Report Template
Pin On Report Template
Ā 
Essay-Writing.Org Discount Code 2021 Coupons Fres
Essay-Writing.Org Discount Code 2021 Coupons FresEssay-Writing.Org Discount Code 2021 Coupons Fres
Essay-Writing.Org Discount Code 2021 Coupons Fres
Ā 
Images For Graffiti Words On Paper Graffiti Words
Images For  Graffiti Words On Paper  Graffiti WordsImages For  Graffiti Words On Paper  Graffiti Words
Images For Graffiti Words On Paper Graffiti Words
Ā 
Pin By Personal Statement Review O
Pin By Personal Statement Review OPin By Personal Statement Review O
Pin By Personal Statement Review O
Ā 
Reasons For Attending College Or University. Reas
Reasons For Attending College Or University. ReasReasons For Attending College Or University. Reas
Reasons For Attending College Or University. Reas
Ā 
Sample College Paper Format ~ Writing An Essa
Sample College Paper Format ~ Writing An EssaSample College Paper Format ~ Writing An Essa
Sample College Paper Format ~ Writing An Essa
Ā 
SUNFLOWERS Personalised Writing Paper Set Of 20 Pers
SUNFLOWERS Personalised Writing Paper Set Of 20 PersSUNFLOWERS Personalised Writing Paper Set Of 20 Pers
SUNFLOWERS Personalised Writing Paper Set Of 20 Pers
Ā 
Should We Reduce The Am
Should We Reduce The AmShould We Reduce The Am
Should We Reduce The Am
Ā 
Can AI Read And Rate College Essays More Fairly Than Hu
Can AI Read And Rate College Essays More Fairly Than HuCan AI Read And Rate College Essays More Fairly Than Hu
Can AI Read And Rate College Essays More Fairly Than Hu
Ā 

Recently uploaded

Employee wellbeing at the workplace.pptx
Employee wellbeing at the workplace.pptxEmployee wellbeing at the workplace.pptx
Employee wellbeing at the workplace.pptxNirmalaLoungPoorunde1
Ā 
CELL CYCLE Division Science 8 quarter IV.pptx
CELL CYCLE Division Science 8 quarter IV.pptxCELL CYCLE Division Science 8 quarter IV.pptx
CELL CYCLE Division Science 8 quarter IV.pptxJiesonDelaCerna
Ā 
Incoming and Outgoing Shipments in 1 STEP Using Odoo 17
Incoming and Outgoing Shipments in 1 STEP Using Odoo 17Incoming and Outgoing Shipments in 1 STEP Using Odoo 17
Incoming and Outgoing Shipments in 1 STEP Using Odoo 17Celine George
Ā 
Roles & Responsibilities in Pharmacovigilance
Roles & Responsibilities in PharmacovigilanceRoles & Responsibilities in Pharmacovigilance
Roles & Responsibilities in PharmacovigilanceSamikshaHamane
Ā 
Historical philosophical, theoretical, and legal foundations of special and i...
Historical philosophical, theoretical, and legal foundations of special and i...Historical philosophical, theoretical, and legal foundations of special and i...
Historical philosophical, theoretical, and legal foundations of special and i...jaredbarbolino94
Ā 
internship ppt on smartinternz platform as salesforce developer
internship ppt on smartinternz platform as salesforce developerinternship ppt on smartinternz platform as salesforce developer
internship ppt on smartinternz platform as salesforce developerunnathinaik
Ā 
Capitol Tech U Doctoral Presentation - April 2024.pptx
Capitol Tech U Doctoral Presentation - April 2024.pptxCapitol Tech U Doctoral Presentation - April 2024.pptx
Capitol Tech U Doctoral Presentation - April 2024.pptxCapitolTechU
Ā 
Introduction to ArtificiaI Intelligence in Higher Education
Introduction to ArtificiaI Intelligence in Higher EducationIntroduction to ArtificiaI Intelligence in Higher Education
Introduction to ArtificiaI Intelligence in Higher Educationpboyjonauth
Ā 
DATA STRUCTURE AND ALGORITHM for beginners
DATA STRUCTURE AND ALGORITHM for beginnersDATA STRUCTURE AND ALGORITHM for beginners
DATA STRUCTURE AND ALGORITHM for beginnersSabitha Banu
Ā 
Painted Grey Ware.pptx, PGW Culture of India
Painted Grey Ware.pptx, PGW Culture of IndiaPainted Grey Ware.pptx, PGW Culture of India
Painted Grey Ware.pptx, PGW Culture of IndiaVirag Sontakke
Ā 
Proudly South Africa powerpoint Thorisha.pptx
Proudly South Africa powerpoint Thorisha.pptxProudly South Africa powerpoint Thorisha.pptx
Proudly South Africa powerpoint Thorisha.pptxthorishapillay1
Ā 
Earth Day Presentation wow hello nice great
Earth Day Presentation wow hello nice greatEarth Day Presentation wow hello nice great
Earth Day Presentation wow hello nice greatYousafMalik24
Ā 
ECONOMIC CONTEXT - PAPER 1 Q3: NEWSPAPERS.pptx
ECONOMIC CONTEXT - PAPER 1 Q3: NEWSPAPERS.pptxECONOMIC CONTEXT - PAPER 1 Q3: NEWSPAPERS.pptx
ECONOMIC CONTEXT - PAPER 1 Q3: NEWSPAPERS.pptxiammrhaywood
Ā 
call girls in Kamla Market (DELHI) šŸ” >ą¼’9953330565šŸ” genuine Escort Service šŸ”āœ”ļøāœ”ļø
call girls in Kamla Market (DELHI) šŸ” >ą¼’9953330565šŸ” genuine Escort Service šŸ”āœ”ļøāœ”ļøcall girls in Kamla Market (DELHI) šŸ” >ą¼’9953330565šŸ” genuine Escort Service šŸ”āœ”ļøāœ”ļø
call girls in Kamla Market (DELHI) šŸ” >ą¼’9953330565šŸ” genuine Escort Service šŸ”āœ”ļøāœ”ļø9953056974 Low Rate Call Girls In Saket, Delhi NCR
Ā 
Enzyme, Pharmaceutical Aids, Miscellaneous Last Part of Chapter no 5th.pdf
Enzyme, Pharmaceutical Aids, Miscellaneous Last Part of Chapter no 5th.pdfEnzyme, Pharmaceutical Aids, Miscellaneous Last Part of Chapter no 5th.pdf
Enzyme, Pharmaceutical Aids, Miscellaneous Last Part of Chapter no 5th.pdfSumit Tiwari
Ā 
Solving Puzzles Benefits Everyone (English).pptx
Solving Puzzles Benefits Everyone (English).pptxSolving Puzzles Benefits Everyone (English).pptx
Solving Puzzles Benefits Everyone (English).pptxOH TEIK BIN
Ā 
Full Stack Web Development Course for Beginners
Full Stack Web Development Course  for BeginnersFull Stack Web Development Course  for Beginners
Full Stack Web Development Course for BeginnersSabitha Banu
Ā 
POINT- BIOCHEMISTRY SEM 2 ENZYMES UNIT 5.pptx
POINT- BIOCHEMISTRY SEM 2 ENZYMES UNIT 5.pptxPOINT- BIOCHEMISTRY SEM 2 ENZYMES UNIT 5.pptx
POINT- BIOCHEMISTRY SEM 2 ENZYMES UNIT 5.pptxSayali Powar
Ā 
Introduction to AI in Higher Education_draft.pptx
Introduction to AI in Higher Education_draft.pptxIntroduction to AI in Higher Education_draft.pptx
Introduction to AI in Higher Education_draft.pptxpboyjonauth
Ā 
18-04-UA_REPORT_MEDIALITERAŠ”Y_INDEX-DM_23-1-final-eng.pdf
18-04-UA_REPORT_MEDIALITERAŠ”Y_INDEX-DM_23-1-final-eng.pdf18-04-UA_REPORT_MEDIALITERAŠ”Y_INDEX-DM_23-1-final-eng.pdf
18-04-UA_REPORT_MEDIALITERAŠ”Y_INDEX-DM_23-1-final-eng.pdfssuser54595a
Ā 

Recently uploaded (20)

Employee wellbeing at the workplace.pptx
Employee wellbeing at the workplace.pptxEmployee wellbeing at the workplace.pptx
Employee wellbeing at the workplace.pptx
Ā 
CELL CYCLE Division Science 8 quarter IV.pptx
CELL CYCLE Division Science 8 quarter IV.pptxCELL CYCLE Division Science 8 quarter IV.pptx
CELL CYCLE Division Science 8 quarter IV.pptx
Ā 
Incoming and Outgoing Shipments in 1 STEP Using Odoo 17
Incoming and Outgoing Shipments in 1 STEP Using Odoo 17Incoming and Outgoing Shipments in 1 STEP Using Odoo 17
Incoming and Outgoing Shipments in 1 STEP Using Odoo 17
Ā 
Roles & Responsibilities in Pharmacovigilance
Roles & Responsibilities in PharmacovigilanceRoles & Responsibilities in Pharmacovigilance
Roles & Responsibilities in Pharmacovigilance
Ā 
Historical philosophical, theoretical, and legal foundations of special and i...
Historical philosophical, theoretical, and legal foundations of special and i...Historical philosophical, theoretical, and legal foundations of special and i...
Historical philosophical, theoretical, and legal foundations of special and i...
Ā 
internship ppt on smartinternz platform as salesforce developer
internship ppt on smartinternz platform as salesforce developerinternship ppt on smartinternz platform as salesforce developer
internship ppt on smartinternz platform as salesforce developer
Ā 
Capitol Tech U Doctoral Presentation - April 2024.pptx
Capitol Tech U Doctoral Presentation - April 2024.pptxCapitol Tech U Doctoral Presentation - April 2024.pptx
Capitol Tech U Doctoral Presentation - April 2024.pptx
Ā 
Introduction to ArtificiaI Intelligence in Higher Education
Introduction to ArtificiaI Intelligence in Higher EducationIntroduction to ArtificiaI Intelligence in Higher Education
Introduction to ArtificiaI Intelligence in Higher Education
Ā 
DATA STRUCTURE AND ALGORITHM for beginners
DATA STRUCTURE AND ALGORITHM for beginnersDATA STRUCTURE AND ALGORITHM for beginners
DATA STRUCTURE AND ALGORITHM for beginners
Ā 
Painted Grey Ware.pptx, PGW Culture of India
Painted Grey Ware.pptx, PGW Culture of IndiaPainted Grey Ware.pptx, PGW Culture of India
Painted Grey Ware.pptx, PGW Culture of India
Ā 
Proudly South Africa powerpoint Thorisha.pptx
Proudly South Africa powerpoint Thorisha.pptxProudly South Africa powerpoint Thorisha.pptx
Proudly South Africa powerpoint Thorisha.pptx
Ā 
Earth Day Presentation wow hello nice great
Earth Day Presentation wow hello nice greatEarth Day Presentation wow hello nice great
Earth Day Presentation wow hello nice great
Ā 
ECONOMIC CONTEXT - PAPER 1 Q3: NEWSPAPERS.pptx
ECONOMIC CONTEXT - PAPER 1 Q3: NEWSPAPERS.pptxECONOMIC CONTEXT - PAPER 1 Q3: NEWSPAPERS.pptx
ECONOMIC CONTEXT - PAPER 1 Q3: NEWSPAPERS.pptx
Ā 
call girls in Kamla Market (DELHI) šŸ” >ą¼’9953330565šŸ” genuine Escort Service šŸ”āœ”ļøāœ”ļø
call girls in Kamla Market (DELHI) šŸ” >ą¼’9953330565šŸ” genuine Escort Service šŸ”āœ”ļøāœ”ļøcall girls in Kamla Market (DELHI) šŸ” >ą¼’9953330565šŸ” genuine Escort Service šŸ”āœ”ļøāœ”ļø
call girls in Kamla Market (DELHI) šŸ” >ą¼’9953330565šŸ” genuine Escort Service šŸ”āœ”ļøāœ”ļø
Ā 
Enzyme, Pharmaceutical Aids, Miscellaneous Last Part of Chapter no 5th.pdf
Enzyme, Pharmaceutical Aids, Miscellaneous Last Part of Chapter no 5th.pdfEnzyme, Pharmaceutical Aids, Miscellaneous Last Part of Chapter no 5th.pdf
Enzyme, Pharmaceutical Aids, Miscellaneous Last Part of Chapter no 5th.pdf
Ā 
Solving Puzzles Benefits Everyone (English).pptx
Solving Puzzles Benefits Everyone (English).pptxSolving Puzzles Benefits Everyone (English).pptx
Solving Puzzles Benefits Everyone (English).pptx
Ā 
Full Stack Web Development Course for Beginners
Full Stack Web Development Course  for BeginnersFull Stack Web Development Course  for Beginners
Full Stack Web Development Course for Beginners
Ā 
POINT- BIOCHEMISTRY SEM 2 ENZYMES UNIT 5.pptx
POINT- BIOCHEMISTRY SEM 2 ENZYMES UNIT 5.pptxPOINT- BIOCHEMISTRY SEM 2 ENZYMES UNIT 5.pptx
POINT- BIOCHEMISTRY SEM 2 ENZYMES UNIT 5.pptx
Ā 
Introduction to AI in Higher Education_draft.pptx
Introduction to AI in Higher Education_draft.pptxIntroduction to AI in Higher Education_draft.pptx
Introduction to AI in Higher Education_draft.pptx
Ā 
18-04-UA_REPORT_MEDIALITERAŠ”Y_INDEX-DM_23-1-final-eng.pdf
18-04-UA_REPORT_MEDIALITERAŠ”Y_INDEX-DM_23-1-final-eng.pdf18-04-UA_REPORT_MEDIALITERAŠ”Y_INDEX-DM_23-1-final-eng.pdf
18-04-UA_REPORT_MEDIALITERAŠ”Y_INDEX-DM_23-1-final-eng.pdf
Ā 

Age Differences In Listening To Music While Studying

  • 1. ISBN 88-7395-155-4 Ā© 2006 ICMPC 605 Alma Mater Studiorum University of Bologna, August 22-26 2006 Age differences in listening to music while studying Anastasia Kotsopoulou Institute of Education, University of London, UK Susan Hallam Institute of Education, University of London, UK s.hallam@ioe.ac.uk ABSTRACT To date, there has been little research specifically focusing on age differences and the perceived effects of music on different types of studying tasks. This research aims to explore age differences in the perceived effects of playing music in the background while studying a range of different tasks. Rating scale questionnaires were administered to six hundred students in three age groups, 12-13, 15-16 and 20-21. The questionnaires explored the use of music in everyday life, the extent of listening to music while studying, the kinds of tasks where background music was played, the perceived effects of music on studying, the types of music listened to while studying and the factors that influenced the decision to listen to music while working. Repeated measures and factorial analyses of variance revealed age significant differences in relation to most aspects of listening to music in everyday life, the moods which stimulated listening to music while studying, and factors which triggered music being turned off. There were few differences in relation to the kinds of tasks where music was played. Overall, the youngest students listened to music the least in their everyday lives, and while studying. The university students were least likely to agree that it aided concentration and helped them learn faster, while the youngest were the least likely to agree that it kept them company, alleviated boredom, relaxed them, interfered so they could not concentrate and interfered because they sang along with the music. They were also less likely to turn off the music when they could not concentrate, when they were unable to learn, and were less discriminating in when they played music in the background. The findings suggest that older students perceive that music plays a increasing role in their everyday lives. They also become more aware of its effects on their studying. Experimental research is now needed to verify these findings. Keywords: Studying, homework, effects of music INTRODUCTION The effect of music on the moods, emotions and behaviour of both individuals and groups has been noted throughout history. A number of writers have discussed the functions of music (e.g. Merriam, 1964; Gaston, 1968), while others have researched both the physiological and psychological effects (for reviews see Radocy & Boyle, 1988; North and Hargreaves, 1997; Hallam, 2001a; 2006). As a result of this research, music has come to be considered as lying on a continuum from highly stimulating and invigorating to soothing or calming (Gaston, 1968). There is strong evidence from a variety of sources that people respond differently to stimulative and sedative music (Radocy In: M. Baroni, A. R. Addessi, R. Caterina, M. Costa (2006) Proceedings of the 9th International Conference on Music Perception & Cognition (ICMPC9), Bologna/Italy, August 22-26 2006.Ā©2006 The Society for Music Perception & Cognition (SMPC) and European Society for the Cognitive Sciences of Music (ESCOM). Copyright of the content of an individual paper is held by the primary (first-named) author of that paper. All rights reserved. No paper from this proceedings may be reproduced or transmitted in any form or by any means, electronic or mechanical, including photocopying, recording, or by any information retrieval systems, without permission in writing from the paper's primary author. No other part of this proceedings may be reproduced or transmitted in any form or by any means, electronic or mechanical, including photocopying, recording, or by any information retrieval system, without permission in writing from SMPC and ESCOM.
  • 2. ISBN 88-7395-155-4 Ā© 2006 ICMPC 606 and Boyle, 1988; Hallam, 2001a). In the modern western world where music is readily available to everyone through radio, recordings, TV and videos and where recorded background music is routinely played in many public places the need to understand the effects of music on our behaviour and cognitive processing has become increasingly important. While, research has been undertaken on the effects of music on a variety of activities it has been relatively unsystematic and much of it has been inconclusive. This is in part because music can be processed in different ways (Hallam, 2001a). While there may be general trends in our responses to stimulating or relaxing music these are overlaid by individual cognitions which may mediate the immediate effects, for instance, associations of particular pieces of music with particular events or dislike of particular musical genres. With regard to young people, we know little about the extent to which they are exposed to music in their everyday lives or its effects on them. Given the overall consumption of music within the wider community we might expect that their exposure is substantial (Hallam, 2001a). We know that in the adolescent years music becomes increasingly important with most teenagers listening to music for approximately three hours a day, its role being perceived in relation to portraying an image to the outside world and satisfying their emotional needs (North et al., 2001). We also know that much studying undertaken at home is accompanied by music or the TV playing (Patton, et al., 1983; Kotsopoulou, 1997). What is as yet relatively unexplored is the effect that the music may have on the behaviour or task performance of young people. Savan (1999) has demonstrated improved behaviour and a greater concentration on school work when Mozart was played during the science lessons of 10 children with learning, emotional and behavioural difficulties. Other studies have explored the effects of music on children in mainstream schools but the type of music played and the ways in which it was expected to influence behaviour have not always been specified. Mitchell (1949) compared performance on a comprehension task of listening to a variety show, a musical show or silence and found that reading achievement was not adversely affected by the musical programme. In a later study, Hall (1952) found that performance on reading comprehension tests was significantly improved when background music was playing. There were also ā€˜settling down periodsā€™ at the beginning of the morning and afternoon sessions and a mid-afternoon fatigue period when music was of greatest assistance. However, no details were given of the kind of background music which was used. Not all of the research has found positive effects of background music. Fogelson (1973), assessing the reading test performance of 8th graders when an instrumental version of Mantovaniā€™s ā€˜Favourite Show Tunesā€™ was playing found that performance was adversely affected for all the students. The music had a more detrimental effect on the children of higher ability than those of low ability. Kiger (1989), studying older pupils aged 15 years who read a 1450 word passage on Japanese history with high or low information load music or no music in the background, found that reading comprehension scores were significantly higher in the low information load condition than in the silent or high information load conditions. Other work has focused on tasks which involve recall. Henderson, Crews and Barlow (1945) explored the effect of music as a source of distraction during the taking of a test. Type of music and the complexity of the testing material both affected performance. Mowsesian and Heyer (1973) studied the effects of music on test taking in 15 year olds using a control, and groups listening to rock, folk, symphonic music and opera. They were tested on mathematics, language and spelling and also completed a self-concept of ability scale. The music had no significant effect on performance. Other tasks have been explored, for instance, reading (McDonald, 1975; Harp, 1988; Hall, 1952; Douglas and Willat, 1994), writing (Karnowski, 1986; Koppelman and Imig, 1995), mathematics (Scott, 1970; Hallam and Price, 1998; Hallam et al., 2002), memorizing (Morton, Kershner, and Sigel, 1990), science (Savan, 1999) and learning a foreign language (Failoni, 1993). Much of the early work in the field was unsystematic not taking account of the type of music being played - whether it stimulated or calmed - making it impossible to interpret the findings. Furthermore, factors such as the characteristics of the individual (age, ability, personality, metacognitive strategy use, musical expertise, familiarity with the music, frequency of use of background music, nationality, previous experiences of listening to music), and the nature of the music (type, stimulating/relaxing, complexity, volume, familiarity, like/dislike of the music, and whether it was self selected) were not taken into account. Typically, students were asked to complete a task while music was playing in the background, their performance was then measured. Each study was undertaken in isolation and there was no sense of progression or overall direction. Students generally did not have the opportunity to select or evaluate the music, they were not asked what they perceived the effects of the music to be on their behavior and studying, and they were not asked if they normally listened to music while they were studying. Explanations of the effect of music on learning and behaviour have tended to be in terms of arousal and mood. The Yerkes-Dodson law states that the arousal
  • 3. ISBN 88-7395-155-4 Ā© 2006 ICMPC 607 level of the individual increases performance up to an optimal level beyond which over arousal leads to a deterioration in performance. The law also states that the deterioration occurs more quickly when the task to be performed is complex or under-learned. A simple task will require a higher level of arousal for concentration to be maintained. Stimulating music is expected to increase arousal and improve performance on simple tasks but if the task is complex the level of arousal may become too great and performance deteriorate. Arousal levels may also be linked to personality factors (Eysenck, 1967) and are responsive to a range of environmental stimuli. They are not consistent over time. Some research has shown that positive emotions may adversely affect cognitive reasoning tasks (Oaksford, Morris, Grainger and Williams, 1996) so music inducing a positive mood might interfere with task performance. Most of this research has been quasi-experimental and has not reflected the ways that students listen to music while doing homework. In that context, while students do have to undertake tasks set by others, they normally have control over whether they listen to music while completing the tasks and also the type of music that they listen to. The present study explores age differences in studentsā€™ use of music in their everyday lives and in their studying, and their perceptions of its effects on them and their work. METHODOLOGY Three age groups were identified for study: 12-13 year olds (secondary), 15-16 year olds (advanced secondary) and 20-21 year olds (university students). These ages were selected as representing distinct periods in young peopleā€™s lives when their lifestyles and the importance of studying would differ. Four different nationalities were selected exemplifying different musical traditions. Those included were the USA, the UK, Greece and Japan. The USA was selected because being a relatively ā€˜youngā€™ country, it lacked a long, well established musical tradition; it was the birth place of modern popular music, i.e. rock and roll and its world wide political and commercial influence has provided it with a key role in the music industry. The UK, although it shares much of its current popular music with the USA is part of a long European musical heritage. The music of Greece links the cultures of East and West. Although it is a European country, people have maintained their own musical traditions despite the influx of Western ā€˜popā€™ music. Japan offers a completely contrasting cultural setting, with its own traditional music and only relatively recent Western influences. Although, overall, the students were randomly selected, an attempt was made to recruit similar numbers of males and females in each age group and from each nationality. Finally, the sample was divided on the basis of active involvement in making music; i.e. did the participants play a musical instrument or sing in a choir, or group. Overall, 150 students from each country participated in the study, 50 from each age group. The students came from major (in most cases the capital) cities in each country. They were selected from schools and universities, which had a broad intake representing a wide range of socio- economic status. A five point rating scale questionnaire was developed to explore the listening habits of the students from the different cultures. This was devised based on interviews undertaken with young people regarding their use of music while studying. Section 1 required respondents to provide information about their gender, age, nationality, and their active involvement in any musical pursuit. Section 2 included statements about the role of music in everyday life. Section 3 included statements relating to listening to music while studying. Respondents were asked if they listened to music when they were revising for exams, writing, memorizing texts, reading, doing course work, editing work previously completed, solving problems, developing ideas, thinking or learning a foreign language. Questions were also posed about listening to music while studying their most or least favorite subject. The fourth section explored issues relating to the perceived effects of music on them and their studying. The fifth section explored the different kinds of music, which they listened to, the characteristics of the music and the particular musical genres. The statements about musical genre varied from country to country to take account of national variation. The categories were developed from those that appeared in the shops selling music recordings in each country. This ensured ecological validity and was also likely to provide respondents with categorizations that were familiar to them. Section 6 explored issues relating to decisions taken with regard to listening to music or not. Statements included: ā€œI listen to music while studying: when I am happy, when I am bored, when I like/ donā€™t like the subject, when I am disturbed by other noises around meā€; ā€œI turn the music off when: I canā€™t concentrate, I am unable to learn, someone suggests I shouldā€; ā€œI choose to listen to music because I am influenced by: the type of music, the subject I am studying, the nature of the subject, the mood I am inā€. Each variable where the rating scale was used was coded from 1 to 5. One signified the response always, 2 frequently, 3 occasionally, 4, rarely, and five never. The findings reported here focus on differences as a function of age. RESULTS Listening to music in everyday life A series of multivariate and repeated measures analysis of variance were undertaken. Table 1 gives the means
  • 4. ISBN 88-7395-155-4 Ā© 2006 ICMPC 608 and standard deviations for the questions related to listening in everyday life. To summarise the overall age differences, the secondary school students tended to listen to music less often than their older counterparts, although there was some variability depending on the particular time of day. University students reported listening to music more in their free time, as children, when they were waking up, when they were at home in the morning, and when they were eating. Advanced secondary school students reported listening to music more when they went to sleep and when they were at home in the evening. There were no significant differences in relation to travelling. A repeated measures analysis of variance indicated significant overall differences between the reported listening activities (F=162.086, DF=8, p=.05). The most common times for listening to music were in free time and while travelling. The least common times were when taking a bath and while eating. There were significant interactions between age and listening to music with everyday activities (F=8.302, df=14, p=.00001). Listening to music while studying Table 2 summarises the means for each age group, regarding their responses to questions about listening to music while studying. There are very few significant differences between the age groups. Those observed are for doing course work and solving problems. A repeated measures analysis of variance found significant differences between the means across questions demonstrating that the students listen to music differentially depending on the type of studying they are undertaking (F = 13.01, df = 11, p = .0001). The activity where music was most often played in the background was thinking followed by writing. The least common were memorizing texts and learning a foreign language. There were significant interactions between age and type of studying in relation to the different kinds of studying (F=39.888, df=2, p=.035). Table 1: Listening to music in everyday life Age 12- 13 Age 15- 16 Age 20- 21 Overall mean SIG Music listening in free time 2.29 1.91 1.91 2.04 .0001 Listening to music as a child 2.54 2.35 2.26 2.38 .015 Listening to music when wake up 3.65 2.95 2.45 3.02 .0001 Listening to music 3.19 2.56 2.85 2.87 .0001 when go to sleep Listen to music at home in the morning 3.08 2.76 2.55 2.80 .0001 Listen to music at home in the evening 2.39 2.25 2.66 2.43 .002 Listening to music while eating 3.83 3.55 3.29 3.56 .0001 Listening to music when I am taking a bath 3.91 3.60 3.32 3.62 .0001 Listening to music when I am travelling 2.28 2.19 2.23 2.23 NS The highest responses are in bold, and the lowest are in italic. The highest responses mean the least agreement Table 2: Listening to music while studying Listening to music while: Age 12- 13 Age 15- 16 Age 20- 21 Overall mean SIG Studying 3.19 3.06 3.02 3.09 .NS Revising for exams 4.04 3.67 3.45 3.72 .0001 Writing 3.31 3.00 3.06 3.12 .05 Memorizing texts 4.18 4.08 4.03 4.10 NS Reading 3.63 3.47 3.54 3.54 NS Doing course work 3.63 3.29 3.27 3.39 .014 Editing work previously completed 3.67 3.45 3.35 3.49 NS Solving problems 3.61 3.29 3.61 3.50 .032 Developing ideas 3.56 3.31 3.32 3.40 NS Thinking 3.27 2.73 2.79 2.93 NS Studying my favourite subject 3.30 3.30 3.46 3.35 NS Studying my least favourite subject 3.33 3.24 3.38 3.31 NS Learning a 3.89 3.86 4.13 3.95 .05
  • 5. ISBN 88-7395-155-4 Ā© 2006 ICMPC 609 foreign language The highest responses are in bold, and the lowest are in italics. The highest responses mean the least agreement Effects of listening to music on studying Students from each age group were asked to report the perceived effects of listening to music on their studying. There were no statistical significant differences between secondary, advanced secondary and university students, in relation to their perceptions of whether music helped them to concentrate, kept them company, alleviated boredom, helped them learn faster, interfered because they ā€˜sang alongā€™ or interfered because it developed a too high level of arousal. There were significant differences in relation to the extent to which music was seen to be relaxing and to interfere with concentration. The university (mean = 1.73, SD = .77) and advanced students (mean = 1.72, SD = 1.06) perceived that music was more relaxing than their younger counterparts (mean 1.97, SD = 1.21). These differences were statistically significant (F = 3.5, df = 2, p = .03). The university (mean = 2.89,SD = 1.19) and advanced secondary students (mean 3.38, SD = 1.26) also believed that music interfered with their concentration more than their younger counterparts (mean = 3.55, SD = 1.46). These differences were statistically significant (F = 13. 96, df = 2, p = .00001). These differences may reflect the type of music being listened to or increasing metacognitive awareness with age. Table 3 gives an overview of the means and standard deviations for the findings reported in this section. A repeated measures analysis of variance indicated that these differences were statistically significant (F = 1. 057, df = 6, p = .05). However, there were no statistically significant interactions between the different perceived effects of listening to background music while studying and age. Table 3: Perceived effects of listening to background music I believe that music: Age 12- 13 Age 15- 16 Age 20- 21 Overall mean SIG helps concentrate 3.03 3.00 3.26 3.10 NS keeps company 2.34 2.21 2.13 2.23 NS alleviates boredom 2.07 1.91 1.95 1.98 NS relaxes me 1.97 1.72 1.73 1.81 .03 helps me learn faster 3.52 3.45 3.67 3.54 NS interferes so I can't concentrate 3.55 3.38 2.89 3.27 .0001 interferes 3.34 3.15 3.09 3.19 NS because I sing along interferes because it makes me too aroused 3.99 4.04 4.00 4.01 NS The highest responses are in bold, and the lowest are in italic. The highest responses mean the least agreement Characteristics of the music listened to while studying Only one aspect of the characteristics of the music listened to while studying was different for the three age groups. This was whether the students listened to music with a fast tempo. The most negative response came from the advanced secondary students (mean = 3.22, SD = 1.36). The most positive response came from the youngest students (mean 2.81, SD = 1.45). These differences were statistically significant (F = 5.14, df = 2, p = .006). A repeated measures analysis of variance showed no difference in the overall means between the various categories which tends to suggest that no particular musical characteristics determine the music which students play in the background. Overall, young people most often listened to songs that they knew which were their favourites. Least often listened to was instrumental music and arousing music. There were no significant interactions between age and music characteristics. Table 4: Characteristics of the music listened to while studying While studying I listen to: Age 12- 13 Age 15- 16 Age 20- 21 Overall mean SIG my favourite music 2.68 2.60 2.76 2.68 . NS songs that I know 2.50 2.61 2.67 2.59 NS music with fast tempo 2.81 3.22 3.13 3.05 .006 music with slow tempo 3.29 3.28 3.19 3.25 NS Songs 2.57 2.80 2.88 2.75 NS loud music 3.25 3.49 3.60 3.45 NS instrumental music 4.09 3.85 3.86 3.93 NS calming music 3.59 3.51 3.56 3.55 NS Arousing music 3.57 3.75 3.64 3.65 NS The highest responses are in bold, and the lowest are in italics. The highest responses mean the least agreement
  • 6. ISBN 88-7395-155-4 Ā© 2006 ICMPC 610 Decisions to play background music while studying Table 5 shows that in taking decisions about having background music playing while studying there were age related differences. The older students were more prepared to take action to reduce any negative effects of the music than the younger students. Younger students tended to turn on music while studying, when they were disturbed by other noises around them, and turned it off if someone suggested they should. Advanced secondary school students were more influenced than the others about deciding to listen to music depending on the subject they were studying, and its nature. University students, turned the music off more than the school students when they were unable to concentrate, when it made them nervous, and when they were unable to learn. Overall, young people most strongly agreed that they turned the music off when they couldnā€™t concentrate and that listening to music depended on the mood they were in. They most strongly disagreed that they turned music off when someone suggested that they should and that listening depended on the nature of the subject that they were studying. Type of music listened to while studying Table 6 summarizes the studentsā€™ responses for listening to different kinds of music while studying. Overall, there were relatively few age differences in the kinds of music selected to be listened to while studying. There were some specific differences for the Greek and Japanese populations but for the population as a whole it was the variety of music, which was listened to, which seemed to increase with age. The most common type of music listened to across all nationalities was recorded pop music. Table 5: Decisions about when to listen to music Age 12- 13 Age 15- 16 Age 20- 21 Overall mean SIG I listen to music when I am happy 2.68 2.57 2.90 2.71 NS I listen to music when I am bored 2.56 2.83 2.76 2.71 NS I listen to music when I like the subject 3.16 3.19 3.29 3.21 NS I listen to music when I dislike the subject 3.30 3.14 3.29 3.25 NS I turn the music off when I am disturbed by other noises 2.89 3.32 3.15 3.12 .017 I turn the music off when I am can't concentrate 3.29 2.16 1.86 2.14 .001 I turn the music off when it makes me 3.50 3.18 3.04 3.24 .015 nervous I turn the music off when I am unable to learn 2.94 2.70 2.29 2.65 .0001 I turn the music off when I someone suggests I should 3.08 3.56 3.57 3.40 .0001 Listening to music depends on the type of music 2.64 2.50 2.56 2.57 NS Listening to music depends on the subject I am studying 3.39 2.99 3.35 3.24 .009 Listening to music depends on the nature of the subject 3.60 3.04 3.46 3.37 .0001 Listening to music depends on the mood I am in 2.24 2.13 2.16 2.18 NS The highest responses are in bold, and the lowest are in italics. The highest responses mean the least agreement Table 6: Type of music listened to while studying While studying I listen to: Age 12- 13 Age 15- 16 Age 20- 21 Overall mean SIG Classical 4.15 4.26 4.16 4.19 NS Rock 3.31 3.49 3.35 3.39 NS Blues 4.09 4.20 3.92 4.07 NS Country 4.30 4.15 4.17 4.21 NS Easy listening 3.77 3.94 3.18 3.84 NS folk/world music 4.35 4.44 4.51 4.43 NS Reggae 3.87 3.97 4.13 4.05 .023 Pop 2.77 3.43 3.29 3.16 .0001 Soul 3.80 3.64 3.71 3.72 NS Dance 3.06 3.59 3.65 3.43 .005 Gospel 3.95 4.28 4.68 4.30 .0001 Jazz 3.81 4.30 4.17 4.09 .0001 Popular music (JP) 3.95 3.02 3.04 3.34 .002 Enka (JP) 4.57 4.85 4.79 4.73 NS New music (JP) 3.34 3.55 3.02 3.30 NS Folk songs (JP) 4.64 4.42 4.38 4.48 NS Kayoh kyoku (JP) 3.88 3.65 3.25 3.59 NS Commercial songs (JP) 3.71 3.87 4.00 3.86 NS TV music themes (JP) 3.12 3.63 3.40 3.37 NS Greek pop 3.04 3.81 3.20 3.36 .012 Greek orchestral 3.76 3.36 2.91 3.36 .011 Greek popular 3.80 3.64 3.07 3.52 .031 Greek folk 4.08 4.53 4.00 4.21 .020
  • 7. ISBN 88-7395-155-4 Ā© 2006 ICMPC 611 Radio 3.42 3.08 3.22 3.24 NS Recorded music 3.27 2.98 3.22 3.16 NS The highest responses are in bold, and the lowest are in italics. The highest responses mean the least agreement DISCUSSION The findings of this study reveal communality and difference between students of different ages in listening to music while studying. In everyday life there is an increase in reported listening as young people get older. This may reflect the increasingly unsupervised time that university students have in contrast to those still at school. The greatest listening time for the youngest students is in the evening, when they have free time. In contrast for the older students this is their least favoured time, probably because they have wider social interests and greater freedom. Listening while traveling was the most reported across all age groups. This requires further investigation. In relation to the types of studying undertaken when music was played there were few age related differences. Where there were differences they may have reflected the different types of studying required in different educational environments. The students seemed to be aware of the extent to which having background music playing interfered with different activities. Students reported rarely listening to music while revising for examinations, memorizing material, or learning a foreign language. They most reported listening while thinking or writing. This suggests that different studying activities share processing mechanisms with musical listening to different degrees. While the students may not have been able to articulate these differences they were sufficiently aware to know what tasks music most interfered with. The nature of the subject being studied did not appear to be a factor in whether music was played or not. The nature of the actual task was more important. There were few differences between secondary, advanced secondary and university students, in relation to their perceptions of whether music helped them to concentrate, kept them company, alleviated boredom, helped them learn faster, interfered because they ā€˜sang along,ā€™ or interfered because it developed a too high level of arousal. The university and advanced students perceived that music was more relaxing than their younger counterparts. This may reflect the increased pressure on them as they progress through their academic courses or may indicate that they are more self-aware. The university and advanced secondary students also believed that music interfered with their concentration more than their younger counterparts suggesting increased self-awareness. The nature of the tasks set for private study may also play a part. The older students may be given tasks which require greater focus and concentration. There were almost no differences in the characteristics of the music listened to while studying with the exception that the younger students tended to listen to music with a fast tempo. Overall, young people most often listened to songs that they knew which were their favourites. Least often listened to was instrumental music and arousing music. Overall, there were relatively few age differences in the kinds of music selected to be listened to while studying. There were some specific differences for the Greek and Japanese populations but for the population as a whole it was the variety of music, which was listened to, which seemed to increase with age. This supports earlier research (LeBlanc and McCrary, 1983). The most common type of music listened to across all nationalities was recorded pop music. The older students were more prepared to take action to reduce any negative effects of the music than the younger students. Younger students tended to turn on music while studying, and when they were disturbed by other noises around them. They turned it off if someone suggested they should. Advanced secondary school students were more influenced than the others about deciding to listen to music depending on the subject they were studying, and the nature of the subject. University students, turned the music off more than the school students when they were unable to concentrate, when it made them nervous, and when they were unable to learn. Overall, young people most strongly agreed that they turned the music off when they couldnā€™t concentrate and that listening to music depended on the mood they were in. They most strongly disagreed that they turned music off when someone suggested that they should. This is important in relation to parental supervision of music listening while studying as it suggests that young people resent interference and may ignore any instructions given. REFERENCES Douglas, S., and Willatts, P. (1994) The relationship between musical ability and literacy skills, Journal of Research in Reading, 17(2), 99-107. Eysenck, H.J. (1967) The biological basis of personality. Springfield, Illinois: Thomas Failoni, J. (1993) Music means to enhance cultural awareness and literacy in the foreign language classroom, Mid-Atlantic Journal of Foreign Language Pedagogy, 1, 97-108.
  • 8. ISBN 88-7395-155-4 Ā© 2006 ICMPC 612 Fogelson, S. (1973) Music as a distracter on reading- test performance of eighth grade students. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 36, 1265-1266. Gaston, E.T. (Ed) (1968) Music in Therapy. New York: MacMillan Hall, J. (1952) The effect of background music on the reading comprehension of 278 eighth and ninth grade students, Journal of Educational Research, 45(6), 451- 458, 1952. Hallam, S. (2001) The power of music, London: The Performing Rights Society. www.thepowerofmusic.co) Hallam, S. (2006) Music Psychology in Education. London: Institute of Education, University of London Hallam, S. and Price, J, (1998) Can the use of background music improve the behavior and academic performance of children with emotional and behavioral difficulties? British Journal of Special Education, 25(2), 87-90. . Hallam, S., Price, J., and Katsarou, G. (2002) The Effects of Background Music on Primary School Pupilsā€™ Task Performance, Educational Studies, Vol. 28(2), 111-122. Harp, B. (1988) When the principal asks: ā€˜Why are your kids singing during reading time?ā€™ Reading Teacher, Vol. 41 (4), 454-456. Henderson, M.T., Crews, A. & Barlow, J. (1945) A study of the effect of music distraction on reading efficiency, Journal of Applied Psychology, 29, 313-317. Karnowski, L. (1986) How young writerā€™s communicate, Educational Leadership, 44 (3), 58-60, 1986. Kiger, D.M. (1989) Effects of music information load on a reading comprehension task, Perceptual and Motor Skills, 69, 531-534. Koppelman, D. and Imig, S. (1995) The effect of music on childrenā€™s writing content. Virginia: US. Kotsopoulou A. (1997) Music in Studentā€™s lives, Unpublished MA Dissertation, Institute of Education, University of London. LeBlanc, A. & McCrary, J. (1983) Effect of tempo on childrenā€™s music preference, Journal of Research in Music Education, 31 (4) 283-294 McDonald, D. (1975) Music and reading readiness, Language Arts, 52 (6), 872-876. Merriam, A.P. (1964) The Anthropology of Music. USA: Northwestern University Press. Mitchell, A.H. (1949) The effect of radio programs on silent reading achievement of ninety-one sixth grade students, Journal of Educational Research, 42(6), 460- 470. Morton, L. et al (1990) The potential for therapeutic applications of music on problems related to memory and attention, Journal of Music Therapy, 27(4). Mowsesian, R. And Heyer, M.R. (1973) The effect of music as a distraction on test-taking performance, Measurement and Evaluation in Guidance, 6(2), 104- 110. North, A. C. & Hargreaves, D.J. (1997) Music and consumer behaviour. In A.C. North, & D.J. Hargreaves, Social Psychology of Music. Oxford: Oxford University Press North, A.C., Hargreaves, D.J. And Oā€™neill, S.A. (2000) The importance of music to adolescents, British Journal of Educational Psychology, 70, 255-272. Oaksford, M., Morris, F., Grainger, B. & Williams, J.M.G. (1996) Mood reasoning and central executive processes, Journal of experimental Psychology, Learning, Memory and Cognition, 22(2), 477-493. Patton, J.E. Stinard, T.A. & Routh, D.K. (1983) Where do children study? Journal of Educational Research, 76(5), 280-286. Radocy, R.E. & Boyle, J.D. (1988) Psychological Foundations of Musical Behaviour Springfield, Illinois: Charles Thomas Savan, A. (1999) The effect of background music on learning, Psychology of Music, 27(2), 138-146. Scott, T. (1970) The use of music to reduce hyperactivity in children, American Journal of Orthopsychiatry, 4, 677-680.