SlideShare a Scribd company logo
Journulof zyxwvutsr
Sociallssues, Vol. zyxwvuts
38, No. 4, 1982, zyxwvu
pp. 121-136 zyxwv
Attributions and Assignment of Responsibility
in Sexual Harassment
Inger W. Jensen
University zyxwvutsr
of Culifornia uf Los Angeles zyxwvu
Barbara A. Gutek
Clarernonr Gruduute School
Although research has investigated the extent to which sexual harassment
exists and what people are or are no1 doing about it, little has been written
about the personal attributions made for these behaviors by either those
who have or those who have not been sexually harassed. Research on
rape and the assignment of responsiblity for a rape may be useful as a base
from which to generalize about sexual harassment and the various ways in
which victims and viewers respond to it. This study consists of data from
an initial survey investigating the occurrence of sexual harassment at the
workplace and follow-up interviews with female workers who reported in
the initial survey having been sexually harassed. The research focused on
the application of attribution theory in four areas: 1) sex differences in
assignment of responsibilityfor sexual harassment, 2) effects of having ex-
perienced sexual harassment on assignment of responsibility, 3) self-blame
in cases of sexual harassment and its affectiveand work-related effects, 4)
the role of sex-role beliefs on assignment of responsibility to self and others.
One of the stronger findings of the study concerned the role of sex-role
beliefs. Women who have traditional sex-role beliefs are more likely to
blame themselves and other victimsfor being sexually harassed. zyx
The research reported here was partially supported by a National lnrtitute of Mental
Health (NIMH) grant UPHS-MH-32606-01 lo the second author. We would like to thank
Marilynn Brewer, Joe McGrath, and an anonymous reviewer for their comments and sug-
gestions. We would also like lo acknowledge the help of Tom Wickens in providing advice
on the log linear analyses.
Correspondence regarding this article should be addressed to 1. W. Jensen, Cedars Sinai
Medical Office Towers, 8635 W. Third zyxwvu
St., Suite 770W, Los Angeles, California 90048.
121
0022~S37/82/1200-0121$03.00/1(s) 1982 Society for the Psychological Study of Social Issuer
122 zyxwvutsrq
lnger W. Jensen and Barbara A. Gulek zy
Although research has investigated the extent to which sexual harass-
ment exists and what people are or are not doing about it, little has been
written about the personal attributions made for these behaviors by either
those who have or those who have not experienced incidents of sexual
harassment.
Benson and Thomson (1982) have suggested that a significant gap
appears to exist between men and women in their perception of the fre-
quency and type of harassment that actually occurs at the workplace.
They also reported considerable sex differences in the perception of the
difficulty a woman would have in handling an unwanted sexual advance.
Vogelmann-Sine, Ervin, Christensen, Warmsun, and Ulmann (1979) noted
the importance of knowing how behaviors are interpreted in complex
interpersonal situations in order to avoid possible misunderstanding of
intent.
The literature on sexual harassment indicates that sex differences
exist in the perception of what constitutes harassing behaviors (Gutek,
Nakamura, Gahart, Handschumacher Jensen, zyxw
& Russell, 1980; Gutek,
Note 1). Although the theoretical bases for these different perceptions of
sexual harassment have not yet been addressed, the rape literature suggests
a variety of theories for such sex differences in attributions of causality
and assignment of responsiblity.
Although sexual harassment and rape are certainly not the same, they
do have some common characteristics. In each case, one person is trying
to force his sexual attentions on an unwilling other. While in rape the
coercion usually is the threat of bodily harm, in sexual harassment it
usually is the threat of depriving individuals of their jobs or job op-
portunities.
Groth (1979) suggested that there are three basic ways in which a
person gains sexual access to another person: 1) through consent (negotia-
tion); 2) through pressure (exploitation); or 3) through force (intimidation).
He further noted that the difference between pressure and force is that
in the pressured situation, the victim is sexually harassed or exploited,
while in the forced assault, the victim is raped. He suggested that there
is enough similarity in the underlying factors of the two situations to discuss
both meaningfully under the single term of rape. In further support of this
notion, Medea and Thompson (1974) asserted that rape could be construed
as a continuum of various degrees of violence with varying degrees of
sexual intimacy.
Although the various theories about attributional processes make
some conflicting predictions, they do have a number of things in com-
Attributions zyxwvutsrq
123 zy
mon. Most predict that sex differences will exist in attributions of locus
of responsibility as well as in how the incidents will be viewed. The study
described in this article addressed these issues. It investigates how well
various attribution theories account for the assignment of responsibility
for sexual harassment incidents, and it explores the impact of sex-role
beliefs on such assignment of responsibility.
DESIGN OF THE STUDY
The data addressing sex differences in assignment of responsibility
were derived from interviews with a representative sample of workers in
Los Angeles County with women oversampled; 827 women and 405 men
were interviewed (see Gutek, Note 1, for a description of this survey).
All other analyses use data obtained from 135 of the female respondents
who were re-contacted and who will hereafter be referred to as the victims
of sexual harassment.
The pool of respondents for the follow-up interviews consisted of
293 women who, in the initial survey, agreed to be recontacted and who
had reported experiences of one or more of the following sexually harassing
behaviors at their present or previous jobs:
a. having been touched by a man in a sexual way on the job;
b. being expected to go out with a man with the understanding that it
would hurt her job situation if she refused, or would help if she accepted;
c. being expected to engage in sexual relations with a man with the
understanding that it would hurt her job situation if she refused, or
help if she accepted.
The telephone interviews in this follow-up study were conducted
with the help of a computerized telephone system that enabled direct
programming of responses (Shure zyxwv
& Meeker, 1978). Due to the lapse in time
since the original contact (approximately 15 months), a substantial number
of potential respondents could not be contacted; 135 interviews were com-
pleted, close to a completion rate of 50%.
The interviews were conducted by college seniors who had received
prior training. In general, the interviews lasted between 15 and 20 minutes.
Once the subject’s consent to be reinterviewed was obtained, the interviewer
described the incident cited previously by the subject to see that the subject
still remembered the incident. All subjects were asked the same questions.
The first seven items dealt with feelings the respondent may have had
124 zyxwvutsrq
lnger W. Jensen and Barbara A. Gutek zy
following the incident. Then, some questions designed to ascertain locus of
responsibility were presented, followed by a number of questions about
reporting the incident and possible reasons for not reporting it. This was
followed by questions about attitudes towards sexual harassment. Finally,
a shortened version (17 items) of a scale measuring feminist attitudes
(Smith, Ferree, zyxwvu
& Miller, 1975) was presented. The scale contained items
assessing beliefs about women’s personality traits and what constitutes
appropriate role behavior in women.
The main classes of variables from the follow-up study that are
relevant to this paper are: 1) attribution items measuring self-blame and
other-woman blame, 2) behavior related to reporting the incident, 3)
affect variables (reports of feelings of anger, sadness, hurt, anxiety,
disgust), 4) job-related outcomes, 5) health-related outcomes, 6) sex-role
beliefs (Smith et al., 1975).
RESULTS
The results are divided into four sections: sex differences in assign-
ment of responsibility to female victims of sexual harassment; the influence
of being a victim on the assignment of responsibility; an exploration of
the phenomenon of self-blame among victims; the relation between sex-
role beliefs and assignment of responsibility.
Sex Differences in Assignment of Responsibility
Several authors (Shaver, 1970; Walster, 1966) have suggested that
the differential motivations of an actor and an observer may be an im-
portant factor in assignment of responsibility to a victim. Walster (1966)
found that as the consequences of an accident grew more severe, there
was a tendency for observers to assign increasing responsibility for the
event to the victim rather than to chance. She hypothesized that in the
case of a minor accident, the observer may feel sympathetic toward the
victim and attribute the responsibility for the outcome to either chance or
other persons. However, in the case of a severe catastrophe, ascribing its
occurrence to chance would leave observers with the uncomfortable thought
that a similar thing might happen to them. By stressing the dissimilarity
of the victim to themselves, observers may disassociate themselves from
a similar tragedy.
Shaver (1970) introduced the notion that such a desire to avoid fear
of unfortunate outcomes may be altered by the need for observers to
Attributions zyxwvuts
125 zy
avoid blame for such an incident and consequently may lead to differential
assignment of causal attributions and responsibility depending on the ob-
server’s perceived likelihood of being involved in a similar incident. He
further made a distinction between relevance due to personal similarity
between the observer and the victim of the accident, and relevance due
to situational similarities between them. He suggested that situational
similarity was a necessary factor, and once it was present, the degree of
personal identification would determine which attributional system would
take place. In “harm avoidance,” indicative of low personal relevance,
increased personal responsibility is assigned to the victim; in “blame
avoidance,” indicative of high personal relevance, decreased responsibility
is assigned in order for the observer to avoid being blamed should a similar
unfortunateincident happen to hcr.
A different motivational theory was suggested by Lerner (1965, 1971,
1978). He postulated that people were motivated to believe in a “just
world” where people get what they deserve, good or bad. To believe other-
wise would raise the question of whether the person can trust his or her
own environment. This “just world’’ notion suggests that when people
hear of an unfortunate incident, they would want to believe that the
victim deserved whatever suffering he or she experienced.
When applied to incidents of sexual harassment, the theories make
differential predictions about the assignment of responsibility by male and
female observers. According to Shaver’s defensive notion, male observers
should assign more responsibility for such incidents to female victims than
would female observers. Walster’s defensive notion predicts that female
respondents should view the female victim of sexual harassment as more
responsible than should the male respondents. In contrast to both of those
theories, Lerner’s “just world” would suggest no differences between male
and female respondents in the assignment of responsibility.
A review of the research investigating attribution theory and rape
suggests that Shaver’s defensive notion best accounts for sex differences in
assignment of responsibility for such incidents (cf. Calhoun, Selby, Cann, zy
& Keller, 1978; Selby, Calhoun, & Brock, 1977). It is reasonable to suggest
that male respondents, not wanting to be blamed in the future, would
assign more responsibility to the women, and similarly, the women, not
wanting to be blamed should such an event happen to them in the future,
would assign less responsibility to the women and more to the men. Shaver’s
version of attribution theory also best explains the data about the sex
differences in assignment of responsibility in our initial survey on sexual
harassment. More responsibility was assigned to the victim of sexual
harassment by the male than the female respondents in the large survey.
Significantly more of the male than female respondents endorsed the state-
ment, “When a woman is asked by a man at work to engage in sexual
126 Inyer zyxwv
W. Jensen and Barbara A. Gutek zy
relations, it’s usually because she did something to bring it about” (54.5%
of males versus 44.7% of females; zyxw
x’ zyxwv
= 9.41, p < .001).
The Influence of Being a Victim on the Assignment of
Responsibility
Attribution theory also addresses the differential assignment of
responsibility as a function of having been a victim. When applied to
incidents of sexual harassment, attribution theory suggests that women in
general will attribute less blame than men to a victim of sexual harassment.
However, significant differences in assignment of responsibility should
still emerge in a comparison between those women who had and those
who had not themselves been sexually harassed.
This notion was supported by the responses of the women inter-
viewed in the larger study. Although overall the majority of the female
victims agreed with the statement, “Women who are asked by men at work
to engage in sexual relations could have done something to prevent it”
(62% agreed), the agreement was significantly greater among those women
who had zyxwvuts
not been sexually harassed (74% agreed) (x’ = 10.8, df = 1,
p < .01). A difference in the same direction, although not statistically
significant, was found in respondents’ endorsement of the statement,
“When a woman is asked by a man at work to engage in sexual relations,
it’s usually because she did something to bring it about”; 46.4% of non-
victims and 42.2% of victims agreed.
The data further show that if the victims believed that a woman
does something to bring about an incident of sexual harassment, they
were also highly likely to believe that the woman could do something to
prevent such incidents. The relationship between the two variables (women
could prevent and women bring about) was significant (x’ = 290, df =
9, p < .001). It was, however, asymmetric. The female victims who strongly
agreed with the statement, “Women do something to bring it about,”
were highly likely to also agree with the statement, “Women could do
something to prevent it” (92%). The same strong relationship was not
found the other way around (59%). That a woman is expected to prevent
things from “going too far” is consistent with traditional sex-role beliefs,
which will be discussed later in this paper.
Self-Blame
The attributions made by victims about their own situation has been
a focus of interest in the rape research and will be the focm of the remainder
Attributions 127 zy
of this paper. Attribution theory applied to rape situations suggests that
victims experience self-blame (e.g., Bryant zyxw
& Cirel, 1977; Burges &
Holshom, 1974). Janoff-Bulman (1979) suggested that two different kinds
of self-blame exist: control related self-blame and esteem related self-
blame. With the former self-attributional strategy, persons blame the
unfortunate incident on their own behavior, such as having engaged in
a certain activity, or inversely, having failed to do so (e.g., I should not
have gone down this street alone at night; I should have kept my door
bolted). In the latter type of self-blame, the persons fault their own
character (i.e., I am just a careless person). While the self-blame focused
on behavior is termed “behavioral,” the esteem related self-blame is
termed “characterological” by Janoff-Bulman.
While there has been a scarcity of research dealing with this issue,
it seemed plausible that there may also be such assignment of respon-
sibility to self by victims of sexual harassment. Our follow-up study ad-
dressed several questions regarding self-blame: 1) will a victim of sexual
harassment engage in self-blame? 2) will a victim who engages in self-blame
be less likely than other victims to seek help? 3) what are the affective
consequences of self-blame and what further effects does self-blame have?
Three statements designed to measure the presence of self-blame
were presented to the women in the follow-up survey: zyx
1) “Perhaps
something in my behavior brought it about,” 2) “Perhaps something in
my behavior may have encouraged the man,’’ and 3) “I am the sort of
person these kinds of incidents are likely to happen to.” The first two
statements differ somewhat from the latter, in that they dealt with the
respondents’ behavior; the correlation between the two is .53. The “sort
of person” statement, on the other hand, ascribed the incident to more
characterological qualities of the respondent and yielded low correlations
with the two behavioral attributions (.17and .12 respectively). The majority
of victims did not evidence self-blame; only 25.4% of the 135 victims
agreed with the first statement, 29.3% with the second statement, and
20.9% with the third. It is possible, of course, that greater self-blame
may be evidenced when assessed soon after an incident; all of these in-
cidents occurred at least 15months before the interview. zyx
Self-Blame and Incident Reporting
It was hypothesized that a victim displaying self-blame would be
less likely to have reported the incident to someone in authority. To in-
vestigate the relationship between self-blame and help-seeking behavior,
responses to the following questions were examined: 1) “Did you report
128 zyxwvutsrq
lnger W. Jensen and Barbara A. Culek zy
your experience to anyone in authority like a supervisor, personnel director,
attorney, etc.?” and 2) “Did you talk to anyone you were close to about
the experience, like family, a friend, or a co-worker?” As predicted, those
who endorsed the two behavioral self-blame items were less likely to
have reported the incident (Table 1). The Pearson correlation coefficients
show significant relationships between the two behavioral self-blame items
and reporting the incident, but failed to support such a relationship for
the characterological self-blame item. The same relation was found between
the self-blame items and the item investigating whether victims had talked
to friends about the incident. Table 1 indicates that respondents who had
endorsed the behavioral self-blame statements tended not to have talked
to anyone about the incident of sexual harassment.
One possible reason for the lack of a relationship between the
characterological self-blame item and reporting the incident may be that
this item does not adequately measure self-blame but rather may reflect
feelings of being a victim. Another factor is that the characterological
self-blame item is psychometrically weak because the response distribu-
tion is so skewed.
One of the ways of explaining the non-report of incidents among the
women who engage in behavioral self-blame is the process of victimiza-
tion of women suggested by Weis and Borges (1973) and reported ex-
tensively in the rape literature. This notion proposes that other people
will hold a victim responsible for her rape or sexual harassment. As part of
this process, the victim internalizes the blame and will try to find some-
thing in her behavior to which she can attribute the incident. It is then
not surprising that she will hesitate to report an incident to authorities or
even to friends. As further support of this notion, more than one-third
of the victims endorsed the following statement as a reason for non-report
of the incident: “I thought it would be held against me or that I would
be blamed.”
Table 1. The Relationship Between Self-Blame and Help-Seeking Behavioi
Agreement with statements Reported incident Talked about incident
Behavioral self-blame:
behavior brought it
about. zyxwvutsr
. .
may have encouraged the man. . .
Perhaps something in my -0.22** -0.25**
Somethingabout my behavior -0.19** -0.30***
Characterological self-blame:
I am the sort of person these
kinds zyxwvutsr
of incidents are likely to
happen to . . .
0.04 --0.10 zyx
*p < zyxwvuts
O.O5;**p < 0.01; ***p < 0.001.
Allribulions zyxwvutsr
129 zy
The data in the follow-up survey also show that if a victim blames
herself, she is concerned about protecting the harasser. Positive correla-
tions were obtained for the relationships between all three self-blame items
and endorsement of the statement, “I did not want to hurt the person
who bothered me.” (Correlations for “behavior brought it about,” “be-
havior encouraged,” and “sort of person” were .19, .33, and .23 respec-
tively.) zyxwvuts
SelfBlame zyxwvut
and Affect
Janoff-Bulman (1979) suggested the efficacy of distinguishing be-
tween characterological and behavioral self-blame because she felt they had
different consequences. She contended that characterological self-blame
leads to feelings of worthlessness, of deserving one’s fate, and especially,
to depression. The hypothesized link between characterological self-
blame and reports of depression was not upheld in this study. Neither of
the two types of self-blame were related to reports zyx
of depression. The
coefficients between the self-blame items and depression were all under .lo.
A relatively small percentage of respondents (20%) gave reports of
depression in response to the sexual harassment incident, in contrast to
reports of other types of affect such as anger (68%) and disgust (80%).
Possible explanations may be that feelings of depression are more difficult
to relate to a stranger and/or harder to remember after more than a year
has passed. It is possible that the college students who evidenced depres-
sion in Janoff-Bulman’s study might not have revealed having experienced
such affect in a telephone interview over one year later.
In this study, seven different affective responses of victims to their
sexual harassment were assessed. A factor analysis of the items yielded
three factors. One included the affective responses of hurt, sadness, and
depression, and was labeled inward-directed affect; the second, labeled
outward-directed affect, was formed by the two responses, anger and
disgust. The third factor consisted of the single item, anxiety. Reliability
analyses supported combining the inward-directed affect items (Cronbach’s
alpha zyxwvut
= .72) as well as the two outward-directed affect items, anger
and disgust (Cronbach’s alpha = .80).
Janoff-Bulman’s (1979) research would suggest that a positive rela-
tionship might exist between behavioral self-blame and outward-directed
affect and also between characterological self-blame and inward-directed
affect as well as anxiety. In order to test this, the two items measuring
behavioral self-blame were combined into a single index (Cronbach’s
alpha = .64). There were significant relationships between behavioral
self-blame and outward-directed affect, and also between characterological
130 zyxwvutsr
lnger zyxwv
W. Jensen and Barbara A. Gutek
Table 2. zyxwvuts
The Relationship Between Type of Blame for Incidents
of Sexual Harassment and Type of Affect Experienced
Pearson correlation coefficients
Type of affect
T w e of blame Inward Outward Anxiety zyx
~~ ~
Behavioral self-blame 0.02 0.27*** 0.04
Characterological self-blame 0.01 0.03 .15** zyxw
*p < 0.05; zyxwvuts
**p < 0.01; ***p < 0.001.
self-blame and anxiety. However, the expected relationship between
characterological self-blame and inward-directed affect was not obtained
(Table 2). This may be because the characterological self-blame item
is not normally distributed and apparently has low reliability.
Effects zyxwvu
of Negative Affect Resultingfrom Sexual Harassment
In the case of sexual harassment, the knowledge that victims have
negative emotional reactions is not as likely to impress employers as the
fact that those emotional reactions have work-related consequences. Several
questions were asked in the follow-up interview to ascertain whether
feelings towards work had been adversely affected. Analyses revealed
significant relationships between reports of feelings of the negative affect
items-hurt, sadness, depression, anxiety, anger, and disgust -and an item
measuring negative attitude toward the job -loss of motivation, feeling
distracted, and dreading to go to work.
If a victim reported having experienced any of these types of negative
affect, she was also likely to report having experienced loss of motivation
and/or feeling distracted at the workplace zyxw
(F = .27). There also was a
significant relationship between negative affect and an item measuring
respondents’ physical well-being after the incident. If she had experienced
any of the types of negative affect, she was also likely to have experienced
negative physical effects (headaches, nervousness and sleeplessness) (f =
.26).
Sex-Role Beliefs and Assignment of Responsibility
Several studies on rape (Brownmiller, 1975; Field, 1978; Medea &
Thompson, 1974; Smith, Keating, Hester, & Mitchell, 1976; Krulewitz &
Payne, Note 2) conclude that general attitudes toward women, as in-
corporated in traditional versus pro-feminist views, are the best predictors
Atlributions 131 zy
of attitudes about rape. Krulewitz and Payne (Note zyx
2) found support
for the notion that subjects’ attitudes toivard traditional sex-roles may
influence their perceptions about rape. While normative behavior by
women is often described as “submissive, passive, not aggressive, and
fearful,” male behavior is frequently described as “dominant, aggressive,
adventurous, opportunistic, and proud of sexual ability” (Heilbrun, 1976).
Krulewitz and Payne (Note 2) suggested that the rapist could be viewed
as displaying an extreme version of such stereotypically masculine sex-
role behavior; i.e., sexually active, aggressive, and opportunistic. They
suggested that this may lead to perceiving the rape as normative male-
female sexual behavior, especially when force and physical violence is
low. Thus, an incident will be characterized as rape only when the per-
petrator uses substantial physical force. They proposed, however, that
this would only hold true for subjects incorporating more traditional
sex-role beliefs; those with nonstereotypical attitudes towards women
would be likely to view an incident as rape irrespective of the amount of
force used or the subject’s physical condition after the rape. Labeling
those holding the more traditional views towards women non-feminist
and those with stronger positive attitudes towards feminism pro-feminist,
Krulewitz and Payne found that while the amount of force depicted did
not affect the attributions of certainty by the pro-feminist female subjects,
non-feminist female subjects required substantial evidence of force before
they were likely to judge an incident as a rape. As the level of force in-
creased, victims were viewed as more helpless, but also, as more respect-
able. This indication that a respectable woman is expected to withstand
rape unless violent force is exerted by the rapist has been supported by
other studies (Brownmiller, 1975; Medea zyxw
& Thompson, 1974). They argue
that commonly held societal attitudes dictate that women should be
responsible for protecting their sexual purity. In further support, Field
(1978) found that pro-feminist subjects assigned less responsibility to the
victim than did subjects who embraced more traditional, or conservative
sex-role beliefs.
The possible impact of sex-role beliefs on perceptions of sexual
harassment has not been addressed. Earlier the victimization process was
suggested as one possible explanation for self-blame. Another exp1anatio:i
is that sex-role beliefs affect self-blame. In order to investigate whether
the effects of sex-role beliefs could be generalized from research on rape
to sexual harassment, the 135 women in the follow-up survey who had
reported their experiences of sexual harassment were administered a sex-
role scale (Smith et al., 1975) along with the other questions. This scale
used 5-point Likert-type items to measure acceptance or rejection of central
beliefs of feminism and the acceptance or rejection of traditional sex-
role beliefs.
132 lnger zyxwv
W. Jensen and Barbara A. Gutek zy
The literature on rape suggests that more assignment of respon-
sibility to victims will be made by those who adhere to traditional sex-
role beliefs. Generalizing this to incidents of sexual harassment suggested
that of those women who reported experiences of sexual harassment, those
who adhere to traditional stereotypic sex-role concepts would assign greater
responsibility to women in general for incidents of sexual harassment.
Three questions dealing with assignment of responsibility to women
in general for sexual harassment were fermed into a single index (Cronbach’s
alpha zyxwvut
= .60). The three items concerned whether women provoke ad-
vances, do something to bring them about, and could do something to
prevent them. There was a significant correlation between the responses to
this index and to the sex-role scale zyxwv
(r = .37), indicating that victims who
obtain traditional scores on the sex-role scale are more likely than those
with non-traditional scores to hold women responsible for incidents of
sexual harassment.
Sex-Role Beliefs and Self-Blame
Sex-role beliefs should also have an impact on whether or not a
victim of sexual harassment will blame herself. In support of this con-
tention, there were significant correlations between sex-role beliefs and the
index containing the two behavioral self-blame items (r = .26) and be-
tween sex-role beliefs and characterological self-blame (r = .33).
Relation Between Self-Blame and Blaming Others
Sex-role beliefs of victims are related to both self-blame and blaming
other women, suggesting that there may be a relationship between self-
blame and other-woman blame. We predicted a stronger tendency to blame
others for the women who engaged in self-blame (behavioral and character-
ological) in contrast to those who did not evidence self-blame. Con-
sistent with this prediction were significant positive correlations between
the index assessing blame to other women and both characterological
self-blame (r = .23) and the two-item index measuring behavioral self-
blame (r = .32).
Thus, sex-role beliefs, blaming others, and self-blame are all related.
What are the causal or sequential processes by which they are linked?
One possibility is that both other-woman-blame and self-blame are con-
sequences of sex-role beliefs. This possibility can be directly tested by
examining them in a three-dimensional test of frequencies, that gives the
difference in the fit between two models (cf. Fienberg, 1980). One model
contains only direct relationships between self-blame and sex-role beliefs
Allribuliuns 133 zy
sex-role belief
Fig. zyxwvuts
1. Models of relationships among sex-role beliefs, self-blame, and other-
woman-blame.
and between sex-role beliefs and other-woman-blame (Figure zyx
1, excluding
dashed line); the other model contains all three relationships -sex-role
beliefs and other-woman-blame, sex-role beliefs and self-blame, and
self-blame and other-woman-blame (Figure 1 with the dashed line).
The results of the model testing indicate that the relationship between
the two forms of blame can be attributed to their common association
with sex-role beliefs, and not to any direct causal links between them.'
These results are not consistent with the belief that victimization is a
consequence of internalizing others' perceptions. If that were the case,
there should have been an independent effect for the relation between
other-woman-blame and self-blame. The results of this analysis do suggest
a mechanism for reducing self-blame -acquiring more feminist sex-role
beliefs.
CONCLUSIONSAND IMPLICATIONS
The hypothesis that men and women differ in their assignment of
responsibility regarding sexual harassment at the workplace is strongly
supported by our survey data. Men are more likely than women to blame
women for being sexually harassed. Among women, victims are less
likely than others to blame women for being sexually harassed.
Our data from the follow-up survey supported the research on rape
showing that victims engage in self-blame. Although the follow-up data
were collected over a year after the incident, between 20% and 30% of
women endorsed each of three self-blame statements. These victims showed
less characterological than behavioral self-blame.
'Formally, the test is of the conditional independence of self-blame [S] and other-
woman-blame [ 0 ] in the presence of the sex-role variable [R] (see Fienberg, 1980, chapter 3).
The model [RS] [RO] tests the relationships of sex-role to each of the two blame variables.
A fit of the model [RS] [RO] to the data gave a likelihood chi-square of 7.54 on 8 degrees
of freedom; a fit of the model [RS] [RO] [SO] gave a likelihood chi-square of 4.18 on
4 degrees of freedom. The difference between these two statistics tests the strength of the
self-other association. The difference gave a non-significant chi-square statistic ofzyx
3.36 with zy
4degrees of freedom.
134 zyxwvutsrq
lnger W. Jensen and Barbara A. Gulek zy
Victims who evidenced behavioral self-blame were less likely than
other victims to report the incident or talk to someone about it. They
were also more likely to feel anger and disgust as a result of the incident,
yet they (as well as women who experienced characterological self-blame)
were more concerned about protecting the harasser than other victims
who did not report self-blame.
Moreover, women who had experienced negative affect were more
likely to report having experienced adverse physical reactions, as well as
such adverse job reactions as distraction and loss of motivation at the
workplace. This indicates that the occurrence of sexual harassment may
well be of substantital economic importance to the employer. In this regard,
these findings parallel those of a recent large-scale government study
investigating incidents of sexual harassment within the federal government
(U.S. Merit Systems Protection Board, 1981).
The results of the follow-up study also suggest that in trying to ac-
count for attributions and assignment of responsibility for experiences like
sexual harassment and rape, a salient factor to consider may be the accep-
tance of traditional versus progressive sex-role beliefs. The results of this
study suggest that sex-role beliefs will dictate how a woman who has
been sexually harassed is going to feel about the incident. If she has in-
corporated the traditional sex-role beliefs, she will be highly likely to blame
other women, as well as herself, for incidents of sexual harassment. Such
an attribution process, in return, will affect whether she reports the incident
to someone in authority or talks to co-workers or friends about it.
Furthermore, the data presented here show that sex-role beliefs have
direct effects on both blaming self and blaming others. These data are
in contrast to a model that shows a direct relationship between the two
kinds of self-blame. One common model proposes that self-blame is an
internalization of holding other women responsible for rape or sexual
harassment (cf. Weis zyxwvu
& Borges, 1973). In contrast, our data suggest that a
woman who is a victim of sexual harassment might engage in self-blame
because of her traditional sex-role beliefs rather than because she blames
other victims for being sexually harassed.
One implication of the impact of sex-role beliefs on attitudes towards
sexual harassment as well as rape is that a different approach toward
change may be warranted. Theorists in the past have suggested that at-
titudes could be changed by providing ample information about what
really happens in such incidents, and by changing specific beliefs about the
act and its consequences. It is possible that such an approach is not the
method of choice for these conditions, and that only by changing people’s
general sex-role beliefs can one effect a change in the attitudes towards
victims of harassment. Such a theory of attitudes and attitude change has
an immediate application in the legal profession, where much of the theory
Attributions 135 zy
about sexual harassment and debate about how it should be treated has
taken place (cf. MacKinnon, 1979). Our data suggest that, to the lawyer
who represents a victim of an incident, the most salient information to
obtain about the judge or the jury may not be their personal experiences,
or even their sex, but rather whether they endorse traditional or progressive
sex-role beliefs.
In general, this research project has demonstrated that sexual harass-
ment is sufficiently similar to rape to borrow theories and hypotheses.
In this case, the research on attribution theory that has been applied to
situations of rape proved to be a fruitful starting point for generating
hypotheses about sexual harassment. This ability to borrow from the
literature on rape is especially helpful since research and theory on rape is
better developed than research and theory on sexual harassment. A
continued interchange of research results between the two areas will even-
tually reveal crucial differences between rape and sexual harassment, as well
as the similarities here considered. This will yield benefits to both areas of
inquiry.
REFERENCE NOTES zyxw
1. Gutek, B. A. zyxwvutsr
Experiences zyxwvuts
of sexual hurussment: Resulis Jrotn a representutive survey.
Paper presented at the annual meeting of the American Psychological Association, LOS
Angeles, August 1981.
2. Krulewitz, J. E., & Payne, E. J. Sex differences in attributions about rupe, ruprsts, and
rape viclims. Paper presented at the meeting of the American Psychological Associa-
tion, San Francisco, 1977.
REFERENCES
Benson, D. J., & Thomson, G. Sexual harassment on a university campus: The confluence of
authority relations, sexual interest, and gender stratification. Social Problems, 1982,
29(3),236-251.
Brownmiller, S. Against our will: Men, women and rape. New York: Simon and Schuster,
197s.
Bryant, G., & Cirel, P. A communily response to rape: An exemplary projeci. (Polk County
Rape/Sexual Assault Case Center). Washington, D.C.: National Institute of Law
Enforcement and Criminal Justice, 1977.
Burgess, A. W., & Holmstrom, L. L. Rape trauma syndrome. American Journal of Psy-
chiatry,' 1974, 131,981-986.
Calhoun, L. G., Selby, J. W., Cann, A., & Keller, G. T. The effects of victim physical
attractiveness and sex of respondents on social reactions to victims of rape. The Brilish
Journalof Socialand Clinical Psychology, 1978, zyxwv
17, 191-192.
Field, H. S. Attitudes toward rape: A comparative analysis of police, rapists, crisis counselors,
and citizens. Journal of Personalily and Social Psychology, 1978, 36, 156-179.
Fienberg, S. E. The analysis of cross-classified culegorical data (2nd Ed.). Cambridge, MA:
MIT Press, 1980.
Groth, N. A. Men who rape: The psychology of the offender. New York: Plenum
Publishing Co., 1979.
136 zyxwvuts
lnger W. Jensen and Barbara A. Gutek zy
Gutek, B. A,, Nakamura, zyxwvuts
C . Y., Gahart, M., Handschumacher Jensen, I. W., zyx
& Russell, D.
Sexuality in the workplace. zyxwvut
Basic and Applied Social Psychology, 1980, 1(3), 255-265.
Heilbrun, A. B., Jr. Measurement of masculine and feminine sex role identities as inde-
pendent dimensions. Journal zyxwvu
of Consulting and Clinical Psycliology, 1976, 44(2),
183-190.
Janoff-Bulman, R. Characterological versus behavioral self-blame: Inquiries into depression
and rape. Journal zyxwvutsr
of Personality and Social Psychology, 1979, 37, 1798-1809.
Lerner, M. J. Evaluation of performance as a function of performer’s reward and attractive-
ness. Journal ofPersonality and Social Psychology, 1965, I , 355-360.
Lerner, M. J. Observer’s evaluation of a victim: Justice, guilt, and verdical perception.
Jourrial of Personality and Social Psychology, 1971,20, 127-135.
Lerner, M. J., & Miller, D. T. Just world research and the attribution process: Looking
back and ahead. Psychological Bulletin, 1978,85, 1030-1051.
MacKinnon, C. Sexual harassment of working wornen. New Haven: Yale University Press,
1979.
Medea, A., & Thompson, K. Against rape. New York: Farrar, Strauss, and Giroux, 1974.
Selby, J. N., Calhoun, L. G. & Brock, T. A. Sex differences in the social perceptions of a
rape victim. Personalityand Social Psychology Bulletin, 1977,3, 412-415.
Shaver, K. G. Defensive attribution: Effects of severity and relevance on the responsibility
assigned to accidents. Journal ofPersonality and Social Psychology, 1970, 14, 101-113.
Shure, G. H., & Meeker, R. J. A minicomputer system for multiperson computer-
assisted telephone interviewing. Behavior Research Methods and lnstruriientation,
Smith, E. R., Ferree, M. M., & Miller, F. D. A short scale of attitudes towards feminism.
RepresentativeResearcli in Social Psychology, 1975, 6, 5 1-56.
Smith, R. E., Keating, J. P., Hester, R. K., & Mitchell, H. E. Role and justice considera-
tions in the attribution of responsibility to a rape victim. Journal of Researcl7 in
Personality, 1976, 10, 346-357.
U. S. Merit Systems Protection Board, Office of Merit Systems Review and Studies. Sexual
horassnient zyxwvuts
in thefederal workplace: Is it oproble~ii?
March 1981.
Volgelniann-Sine, S., Ervin, E. D., Christensen, R., Warmsun, C. H., & Ullmann, L. P.
Sex differences in feelings attributed to a woman in situations involving coercion and
sexual advances. Journal oJPersonality, 1979, 47(3),420-431.
Walster, E. Assignment of responsibility for an accident. Journal of Personaliry and Social
Weis, K., & Borges, S. S. Victiniology and rape: The case of the legitimate victim. Issues in
1978, 10(2), 196-202.
PS,YCI~OIOXY,
1966.3, 73-79.
Crin~inology,1973, 8, 71-115.

More Related Content

Similar to Attributions And Assignment Of Responsibility In Sexual Harassment

Final-Research-Report
Final-Research-ReportFinal-Research-Report
Final-Research-ReportLucas Liddell
 
Carney 2007 women who perpetrate ipv. review of lit
Carney 2007  women who perpetrate ipv. review of litCarney 2007  women who perpetrate ipv. review of lit
Carney 2007 women who perpetrate ipv. review of litAzarelita Enriquez
 
Senior Thesis (Ryan Callahan) (1)
Senior Thesis (Ryan Callahan) (1)Senior Thesis (Ryan Callahan) (1)
Senior Thesis (Ryan Callahan) (1)Ryan Callahan
 
Yellow Group Final Draft 2
Yellow Group Final Draft 2Yellow Group Final Draft 2
Yellow Group Final Draft 2Courtney Michael
 
AGGRESSIVE BEHAVIORVolume 29, pages 366—380 (2003)Perpet.docx
AGGRESSIVE BEHAVIORVolume 29, pages 366—380 (2003)Perpet.docxAGGRESSIVE BEHAVIORVolume 29, pages 366—380 (2003)Perpet.docx
AGGRESSIVE BEHAVIORVolume 29, pages 366—380 (2003)Perpet.docxnettletondevon
 
Poster for Spring Research Fair 2014
Poster for Spring Research Fair 2014Poster for Spring Research Fair 2014
Poster for Spring Research Fair 2014Rachel O'Hanlon
 
Juror Perception of Intimate Partner Violence Cases Involving Victims with Di...
Juror Perception of Intimate Partner Violence Cases Involving Victims with Di...Juror Perception of Intimate Partner Violence Cases Involving Victims with Di...
Juror Perception of Intimate Partner Violence Cases Involving Victims with Di...Amanda Langley
 
Sexual Harassment On Latinos
Sexual Harassment On Latinos Sexual Harassment On Latinos
Sexual Harassment On Latinos Fakru Bashu
 
PO 450 Final Research Project - Alejandro Luna
PO 450 Final Research Project - Alejandro LunaPO 450 Final Research Project - Alejandro Luna
PO 450 Final Research Project - Alejandro LunaAlejandro Luna
 
The Effect of Gender and Type of Force on Perceptions of Sex Trafficking
The Effect of Gender and Type of Force on Perceptions of Sex TraffickingThe Effect of Gender and Type of Force on Perceptions of Sex Trafficking
The Effect of Gender and Type of Force on Perceptions of Sex TraffickingAmanda Langley
 
The association between personal history of dating violence and bystander int...
The association between personal history of dating violence and bystander int...The association between personal history of dating violence and bystander int...
The association between personal history of dating violence and bystander int...William Woods
 
MPA Bystander Poster Final
MPA Bystander Poster FinalMPA Bystander Poster Final
MPA Bystander Poster FinalKasey Jerioski
 

Similar to Attributions And Assignment Of Responsibility In Sexual Harassment (18)

Final-Research-Report
Final-Research-ReportFinal-Research-Report
Final-Research-Report
 
Carney 2007 women who perpetrate ipv. review of lit
Carney 2007  women who perpetrate ipv. review of litCarney 2007  women who perpetrate ipv. review of lit
Carney 2007 women who perpetrate ipv. review of lit
 
Senior Thesis (Ryan Callahan) (1)
Senior Thesis (Ryan Callahan) (1)Senior Thesis (Ryan Callahan) (1)
Senior Thesis (Ryan Callahan) (1)
 
Case studies
Case studiesCase studies
Case studies
 
Yellow Group Final Draft 2
Yellow Group Final Draft 2Yellow Group Final Draft 2
Yellow Group Final Draft 2
 
10.1.1.470.9865
10.1.1.470.986510.1.1.470.9865
10.1.1.470.9865
 
AGGRESSIVE BEHAVIORVolume 29, pages 366—380 (2003)Perpet.docx
AGGRESSIVE BEHAVIORVolume 29, pages 366—380 (2003)Perpet.docxAGGRESSIVE BEHAVIORVolume 29, pages 366—380 (2003)Perpet.docx
AGGRESSIVE BEHAVIORVolume 29, pages 366—380 (2003)Perpet.docx
 
Poster for Spring Research Fair 2014
Poster for Spring Research Fair 2014Poster for Spring Research Fair 2014
Poster for Spring Research Fair 2014
 
IVS Final Paper
IVS Final PaperIVS Final Paper
IVS Final Paper
 
Juror Perception of Intimate Partner Violence Cases Involving Victims with Di...
Juror Perception of Intimate Partner Violence Cases Involving Victims with Di...Juror Perception of Intimate Partner Violence Cases Involving Victims with Di...
Juror Perception of Intimate Partner Violence Cases Involving Victims with Di...
 
Sexual Harassment On Latinos
Sexual Harassment On Latinos Sexual Harassment On Latinos
Sexual Harassment On Latinos
 
Axiom Proposal
Axiom ProposalAxiom Proposal
Axiom Proposal
 
PO 450 Final Research Project - Alejandro Luna
PO 450 Final Research Project - Alejandro LunaPO 450 Final Research Project - Alejandro Luna
PO 450 Final Research Project - Alejandro Luna
 
The Effect of Gender and Type of Force on Perceptions of Sex Trafficking
The Effect of Gender and Type of Force on Perceptions of Sex TraffickingThe Effect of Gender and Type of Force on Perceptions of Sex Trafficking
The Effect of Gender and Type of Force on Perceptions of Sex Trafficking
 
FinalLitReview
FinalLitReviewFinalLitReview
FinalLitReview
 
The association between personal history of dating violence and bystander int...
The association between personal history of dating violence and bystander int...The association between personal history of dating violence and bystander int...
The association between personal history of dating violence and bystander int...
 
MPA Bystander Poster Final
MPA Bystander Poster FinalMPA Bystander Poster Final
MPA Bystander Poster Final
 
APS Poster
APS PosterAPS Poster
APS Poster
 

More from Sean Flores

Importance Of Top-Rated Essay Writing Services - A Helpful Tool For
Importance Of Top-Rated Essay Writing Services - A Helpful Tool ForImportance Of Top-Rated Essay Writing Services - A Helpful Tool For
Importance Of Top-Rated Essay Writing Services - A Helpful Tool ForSean Flores
 
Linking Words For Essay Telegraph. Online assignment writing service.
Linking Words For Essay Telegraph. Online assignment writing service.Linking Words For Essay Telegraph. Online assignment writing service.
Linking Words For Essay Telegraph. Online assignment writing service.Sean Flores
 
Research Paper Outline Template Sample Room S
Research Paper Outline Template Sample Room SResearch Paper Outline Template Sample Room S
Research Paper Outline Template Sample Room SSean Flores
 
Research Paper On College Athletes Getting Paid. Gold Essay Research
Research Paper On College Athletes Getting Paid. Gold Essay ResearchResearch Paper On College Athletes Getting Paid. Gold Essay Research
Research Paper On College Athletes Getting Paid. Gold Essay ResearchSean Flores
 
Essay On Basketball Basketball Essay For Stude
Essay On Basketball  Basketball Essay For StudeEssay On Basketball  Basketball Essay For Stude
Essay On Basketball Basketball Essay For StudeSean Flores
 
Impressive Research Paper With Cover Page Example
Impressive Research Paper With Cover Page ExampleImpressive Research Paper With Cover Page Example
Impressive Research Paper With Cover Page ExampleSean Flores
 
Opinion Essay Telegraph. Online assignment writing service.
Opinion Essay   Telegraph. Online assignment writing service.Opinion Essay   Telegraph. Online assignment writing service.
Opinion Essay Telegraph. Online assignment writing service.Sean Flores
 
Football-Themed Creative Writing Prompts Printabl
Football-Themed Creative Writing Prompts  PrintablFootball-Themed Creative Writing Prompts  Printabl
Football-Themed Creative Writing Prompts PrintablSean Flores
 
Funny Story Essay - Latest. Online assignment writing service.
Funny Story Essay - Latest. Online assignment writing service.Funny Story Essay - Latest. Online assignment writing service.
Funny Story Essay - Latest. Online assignment writing service.Sean Flores
 
The Truth About Homework Essay. Online assignment writing service.
The Truth About Homework Essay. Online assignment writing service.The Truth About Homework Essay. Online assignment writing service.
The Truth About Homework Essay. Online assignment writing service.Sean Flores
 
10 Spelman College Graduate Entrepreneur
10 Spelman College Graduate Entrepreneur10 Spelman College Graduate Entrepreneur
10 Spelman College Graduate EntrepreneurSean Flores
 
College Essay Editing. Essay Writing Servic
College Essay Editing. Essay Writing ServicCollege Essay Editing. Essay Writing Servic
College Essay Editing. Essay Writing ServicSean Flores
 
Think Twice Before You Pa. Online assignment writing service.
Think Twice Before You Pa. Online assignment writing service.Think Twice Before You Pa. Online assignment writing service.
Think Twice Before You Pa. Online assignment writing service.Sean Flores
 
Free Printable Thanksgiving Stationery. Online assignment writing service.
Free Printable Thanksgiving Stationery. Online assignment writing service.Free Printable Thanksgiving Stationery. Online assignment writing service.
Free Printable Thanksgiving Stationery. Online assignment writing service.Sean Flores
 
Observation Paper. Online assignment writing service.
Observation Paper. Online assignment writing service.Observation Paper. Online assignment writing service.
Observation Paper. Online assignment writing service.Sean Flores
 
Writing A Procedure - Studyladder Interactive Le
Writing A Procedure - Studyladder Interactive LeWriting A Procedure - Studyladder Interactive Le
Writing A Procedure - Studyladder Interactive LeSean Flores
 
Reflective Essay On Presentation And D. Online assignment writing service.
Reflective Essay On Presentation And D. Online assignment writing service.Reflective Essay On Presentation And D. Online assignment writing service.
Reflective Essay On Presentation And D. Online assignment writing service.Sean Flores
 
Hamburger Writing Template For Teachers Perfect For G
Hamburger Writing Template For Teachers  Perfect For GHamburger Writing Template For Teachers  Perfect For G
Hamburger Writing Template For Teachers Perfect For GSean Flores
 
How To Write Thematic Statement. How To Write A
How To Write Thematic Statement. How To Write AHow To Write Thematic Statement. How To Write A
How To Write Thematic Statement. How To Write ASean Flores
 
Financial Need Scholarship Essay Examples Scholar
Financial Need Scholarship Essay Examples  ScholarFinancial Need Scholarship Essay Examples  Scholar
Financial Need Scholarship Essay Examples ScholarSean Flores
 

More from Sean Flores (20)

Importance Of Top-Rated Essay Writing Services - A Helpful Tool For
Importance Of Top-Rated Essay Writing Services - A Helpful Tool ForImportance Of Top-Rated Essay Writing Services - A Helpful Tool For
Importance Of Top-Rated Essay Writing Services - A Helpful Tool For
 
Linking Words For Essay Telegraph. Online assignment writing service.
Linking Words For Essay Telegraph. Online assignment writing service.Linking Words For Essay Telegraph. Online assignment writing service.
Linking Words For Essay Telegraph. Online assignment writing service.
 
Research Paper Outline Template Sample Room S
Research Paper Outline Template Sample Room SResearch Paper Outline Template Sample Room S
Research Paper Outline Template Sample Room S
 
Research Paper On College Athletes Getting Paid. Gold Essay Research
Research Paper On College Athletes Getting Paid. Gold Essay ResearchResearch Paper On College Athletes Getting Paid. Gold Essay Research
Research Paper On College Athletes Getting Paid. Gold Essay Research
 
Essay On Basketball Basketball Essay For Stude
Essay On Basketball  Basketball Essay For StudeEssay On Basketball  Basketball Essay For Stude
Essay On Basketball Basketball Essay For Stude
 
Impressive Research Paper With Cover Page Example
Impressive Research Paper With Cover Page ExampleImpressive Research Paper With Cover Page Example
Impressive Research Paper With Cover Page Example
 
Opinion Essay Telegraph. Online assignment writing service.
Opinion Essay   Telegraph. Online assignment writing service.Opinion Essay   Telegraph. Online assignment writing service.
Opinion Essay Telegraph. Online assignment writing service.
 
Football-Themed Creative Writing Prompts Printabl
Football-Themed Creative Writing Prompts  PrintablFootball-Themed Creative Writing Prompts  Printabl
Football-Themed Creative Writing Prompts Printabl
 
Funny Story Essay - Latest. Online assignment writing service.
Funny Story Essay - Latest. Online assignment writing service.Funny Story Essay - Latest. Online assignment writing service.
Funny Story Essay - Latest. Online assignment writing service.
 
The Truth About Homework Essay. Online assignment writing service.
The Truth About Homework Essay. Online assignment writing service.The Truth About Homework Essay. Online assignment writing service.
The Truth About Homework Essay. Online assignment writing service.
 
10 Spelman College Graduate Entrepreneur
10 Spelman College Graduate Entrepreneur10 Spelman College Graduate Entrepreneur
10 Spelman College Graduate Entrepreneur
 
College Essay Editing. Essay Writing Servic
College Essay Editing. Essay Writing ServicCollege Essay Editing. Essay Writing Servic
College Essay Editing. Essay Writing Servic
 
Think Twice Before You Pa. Online assignment writing service.
Think Twice Before You Pa. Online assignment writing service.Think Twice Before You Pa. Online assignment writing service.
Think Twice Before You Pa. Online assignment writing service.
 
Free Printable Thanksgiving Stationery. Online assignment writing service.
Free Printable Thanksgiving Stationery. Online assignment writing service.Free Printable Thanksgiving Stationery. Online assignment writing service.
Free Printable Thanksgiving Stationery. Online assignment writing service.
 
Observation Paper. Online assignment writing service.
Observation Paper. Online assignment writing service.Observation Paper. Online assignment writing service.
Observation Paper. Online assignment writing service.
 
Writing A Procedure - Studyladder Interactive Le
Writing A Procedure - Studyladder Interactive LeWriting A Procedure - Studyladder Interactive Le
Writing A Procedure - Studyladder Interactive Le
 
Reflective Essay On Presentation And D. Online assignment writing service.
Reflective Essay On Presentation And D. Online assignment writing service.Reflective Essay On Presentation And D. Online assignment writing service.
Reflective Essay On Presentation And D. Online assignment writing service.
 
Hamburger Writing Template For Teachers Perfect For G
Hamburger Writing Template For Teachers  Perfect For GHamburger Writing Template For Teachers  Perfect For G
Hamburger Writing Template For Teachers Perfect For G
 
How To Write Thematic Statement. How To Write A
How To Write Thematic Statement. How To Write AHow To Write Thematic Statement. How To Write A
How To Write Thematic Statement. How To Write A
 
Financial Need Scholarship Essay Examples Scholar
Financial Need Scholarship Essay Examples  ScholarFinancial Need Scholarship Essay Examples  Scholar
Financial Need Scholarship Essay Examples Scholar
 

Recently uploaded

Benefits and Challenges of Using Open Educational Resources
Benefits and Challenges of Using Open Educational ResourcesBenefits and Challenges of Using Open Educational Resources
Benefits and Challenges of Using Open Educational Resourcesdimpy50
 
MARUTI SUZUKI- A Successful Joint Venture in India.pptx
MARUTI SUZUKI- A Successful Joint Venture in India.pptxMARUTI SUZUKI- A Successful Joint Venture in India.pptx
MARUTI SUZUKI- A Successful Joint Venture in India.pptxbennyroshan06
 
The basics of sentences session 4pptx.pptx
The basics of sentences session 4pptx.pptxThe basics of sentences session 4pptx.pptx
The basics of sentences session 4pptx.pptxheathfieldcps1
 
How to Create Map Views in the Odoo 17 ERP
How to Create Map Views in the Odoo 17 ERPHow to Create Map Views in the Odoo 17 ERP
How to Create Map Views in the Odoo 17 ERPCeline George
 
Basic Civil Engg Notes_Chapter-6_Environment Pollution & Engineering
Basic Civil Engg Notes_Chapter-6_Environment Pollution & EngineeringBasic Civil Engg Notes_Chapter-6_Environment Pollution & Engineering
Basic Civil Engg Notes_Chapter-6_Environment Pollution & EngineeringDenish Jangid
 
Application of Matrices in real life. Presentation on application of matrices
Application of Matrices in real life. Presentation on application of matricesApplication of Matrices in real life. Presentation on application of matrices
Application of Matrices in real life. Presentation on application of matricesRased Khan
 
Advances in production technology of Grapes.pdf
Advances in production technology of Grapes.pdfAdvances in production technology of Grapes.pdf
Advances in production technology of Grapes.pdfDr. M. Kumaresan Hort.
 
Pragya Champions Chalice 2024 Prelims & Finals Q/A set, General Quiz
Pragya Champions Chalice 2024 Prelims & Finals Q/A set, General QuizPragya Champions Chalice 2024 Prelims & Finals Q/A set, General Quiz
Pragya Champions Chalice 2024 Prelims & Finals Q/A set, General QuizPragya - UEM Kolkata Quiz Club
 
The Last Leaf, a short story by O. Henry
The Last Leaf, a short story by O. HenryThe Last Leaf, a short story by O. Henry
The Last Leaf, a short story by O. HenryEugene Lysak
 
slides CapTechTalks Webinar May 2024 Alexander Perry.pptx
slides CapTechTalks Webinar May 2024 Alexander Perry.pptxslides CapTechTalks Webinar May 2024 Alexander Perry.pptx
slides CapTechTalks Webinar May 2024 Alexander Perry.pptxCapitolTechU
 
[GDSC YCCE] Build with AI Online Presentation
[GDSC YCCE] Build with AI Online Presentation[GDSC YCCE] Build with AI Online Presentation
[GDSC YCCE] Build with AI Online PresentationGDSCYCCE
 
ppt your views.ppt your views of your college in your eyes
ppt your views.ppt your views of your college in your eyesppt your views.ppt your views of your college in your eyes
ppt your views.ppt your views of your college in your eyesashishpaul799
 
Salient features of Environment protection Act 1986.pptx
Salient features of Environment protection Act 1986.pptxSalient features of Environment protection Act 1986.pptx
Salient features of Environment protection Act 1986.pptxakshayaramakrishnan21
 
Industrial Training Report- AKTU Industrial Training Report
Industrial Training Report- AKTU Industrial Training ReportIndustrial Training Report- AKTU Industrial Training Report
Industrial Training Report- AKTU Industrial Training ReportAvinash Rai
 
An Overview of the Odoo 17 Discuss App.pptx
An Overview of the Odoo 17 Discuss App.pptxAn Overview of the Odoo 17 Discuss App.pptx
An Overview of the Odoo 17 Discuss App.pptxCeline George
 
Phrasal Verbs.XXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXX
Phrasal Verbs.XXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXPhrasal Verbs.XXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXX
Phrasal Verbs.XXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXMIRIAMSALINAS13
 
Sha'Carri Richardson Presentation 202345
Sha'Carri Richardson Presentation 202345Sha'Carri Richardson Presentation 202345
Sha'Carri Richardson Presentation 202345beazzy04
 
Gyanartha SciBizTech Quiz slideshare.pptx
Gyanartha SciBizTech Quiz slideshare.pptxGyanartha SciBizTech Quiz slideshare.pptx
Gyanartha SciBizTech Quiz slideshare.pptxShibin Azad
 
50 ĐỀ LUYỆN THI IOE LỚP 9 - NĂM HỌC 2022-2023 (CÓ LINK HÌNH, FILE AUDIO VÀ ĐÁ...
50 ĐỀ LUYỆN THI IOE LỚP 9 - NĂM HỌC 2022-2023 (CÓ LINK HÌNH, FILE AUDIO VÀ ĐÁ...50 ĐỀ LUYỆN THI IOE LỚP 9 - NĂM HỌC 2022-2023 (CÓ LINK HÌNH, FILE AUDIO VÀ ĐÁ...
50 ĐỀ LUYỆN THI IOE LỚP 9 - NĂM HỌC 2022-2023 (CÓ LINK HÌNH, FILE AUDIO VÀ ĐÁ...Nguyen Thanh Tu Collection
 

Recently uploaded (20)

Benefits and Challenges of Using Open Educational Resources
Benefits and Challenges of Using Open Educational ResourcesBenefits and Challenges of Using Open Educational Resources
Benefits and Challenges of Using Open Educational Resources
 
MARUTI SUZUKI- A Successful Joint Venture in India.pptx
MARUTI SUZUKI- A Successful Joint Venture in India.pptxMARUTI SUZUKI- A Successful Joint Venture in India.pptx
MARUTI SUZUKI- A Successful Joint Venture in India.pptx
 
The basics of sentences session 4pptx.pptx
The basics of sentences session 4pptx.pptxThe basics of sentences session 4pptx.pptx
The basics of sentences session 4pptx.pptx
 
NCERT Solutions Power Sharing Class 10 Notes pdf
NCERT Solutions Power Sharing Class 10 Notes pdfNCERT Solutions Power Sharing Class 10 Notes pdf
NCERT Solutions Power Sharing Class 10 Notes pdf
 
How to Create Map Views in the Odoo 17 ERP
How to Create Map Views in the Odoo 17 ERPHow to Create Map Views in the Odoo 17 ERP
How to Create Map Views in the Odoo 17 ERP
 
Basic Civil Engg Notes_Chapter-6_Environment Pollution & Engineering
Basic Civil Engg Notes_Chapter-6_Environment Pollution & EngineeringBasic Civil Engg Notes_Chapter-6_Environment Pollution & Engineering
Basic Civil Engg Notes_Chapter-6_Environment Pollution & Engineering
 
Application of Matrices in real life. Presentation on application of matrices
Application of Matrices in real life. Presentation on application of matricesApplication of Matrices in real life. Presentation on application of matrices
Application of Matrices in real life. Presentation on application of matrices
 
Advances in production technology of Grapes.pdf
Advances in production technology of Grapes.pdfAdvances in production technology of Grapes.pdf
Advances in production technology of Grapes.pdf
 
Pragya Champions Chalice 2024 Prelims & Finals Q/A set, General Quiz
Pragya Champions Chalice 2024 Prelims & Finals Q/A set, General QuizPragya Champions Chalice 2024 Prelims & Finals Q/A set, General Quiz
Pragya Champions Chalice 2024 Prelims & Finals Q/A set, General Quiz
 
The Last Leaf, a short story by O. Henry
The Last Leaf, a short story by O. HenryThe Last Leaf, a short story by O. Henry
The Last Leaf, a short story by O. Henry
 
slides CapTechTalks Webinar May 2024 Alexander Perry.pptx
slides CapTechTalks Webinar May 2024 Alexander Perry.pptxslides CapTechTalks Webinar May 2024 Alexander Perry.pptx
slides CapTechTalks Webinar May 2024 Alexander Perry.pptx
 
[GDSC YCCE] Build with AI Online Presentation
[GDSC YCCE] Build with AI Online Presentation[GDSC YCCE] Build with AI Online Presentation
[GDSC YCCE] Build with AI Online Presentation
 
ppt your views.ppt your views of your college in your eyes
ppt your views.ppt your views of your college in your eyesppt your views.ppt your views of your college in your eyes
ppt your views.ppt your views of your college in your eyes
 
Salient features of Environment protection Act 1986.pptx
Salient features of Environment protection Act 1986.pptxSalient features of Environment protection Act 1986.pptx
Salient features of Environment protection Act 1986.pptx
 
Industrial Training Report- AKTU Industrial Training Report
Industrial Training Report- AKTU Industrial Training ReportIndustrial Training Report- AKTU Industrial Training Report
Industrial Training Report- AKTU Industrial Training Report
 
An Overview of the Odoo 17 Discuss App.pptx
An Overview of the Odoo 17 Discuss App.pptxAn Overview of the Odoo 17 Discuss App.pptx
An Overview of the Odoo 17 Discuss App.pptx
 
Phrasal Verbs.XXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXX
Phrasal Verbs.XXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXPhrasal Verbs.XXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXX
Phrasal Verbs.XXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXX
 
Sha'Carri Richardson Presentation 202345
Sha'Carri Richardson Presentation 202345Sha'Carri Richardson Presentation 202345
Sha'Carri Richardson Presentation 202345
 
Gyanartha SciBizTech Quiz slideshare.pptx
Gyanartha SciBizTech Quiz slideshare.pptxGyanartha SciBizTech Quiz slideshare.pptx
Gyanartha SciBizTech Quiz slideshare.pptx
 
50 ĐỀ LUYỆN THI IOE LỚP 9 - NĂM HỌC 2022-2023 (CÓ LINK HÌNH, FILE AUDIO VÀ ĐÁ...
50 ĐỀ LUYỆN THI IOE LỚP 9 - NĂM HỌC 2022-2023 (CÓ LINK HÌNH, FILE AUDIO VÀ ĐÁ...50 ĐỀ LUYỆN THI IOE LỚP 9 - NĂM HỌC 2022-2023 (CÓ LINK HÌNH, FILE AUDIO VÀ ĐÁ...
50 ĐỀ LUYỆN THI IOE LỚP 9 - NĂM HỌC 2022-2023 (CÓ LINK HÌNH, FILE AUDIO VÀ ĐÁ...
 

Attributions And Assignment Of Responsibility In Sexual Harassment

  • 1. Journulof zyxwvutsr Sociallssues, Vol. zyxwvuts 38, No. 4, 1982, zyxwvu pp. 121-136 zyxwv Attributions and Assignment of Responsibility in Sexual Harassment Inger W. Jensen University zyxwvutsr of Culifornia uf Los Angeles zyxwvu Barbara A. Gutek Clarernonr Gruduute School Although research has investigated the extent to which sexual harassment exists and what people are or are no1 doing about it, little has been written about the personal attributions made for these behaviors by either those who have or those who have not been sexually harassed. Research on rape and the assignment of responsiblity for a rape may be useful as a base from which to generalize about sexual harassment and the various ways in which victims and viewers respond to it. This study consists of data from an initial survey investigating the occurrence of sexual harassment at the workplace and follow-up interviews with female workers who reported in the initial survey having been sexually harassed. The research focused on the application of attribution theory in four areas: 1) sex differences in assignment of responsibilityfor sexual harassment, 2) effects of having ex- perienced sexual harassment on assignment of responsibility, 3) self-blame in cases of sexual harassment and its affectiveand work-related effects, 4) the role of sex-role beliefs on assignment of responsibility to self and others. One of the stronger findings of the study concerned the role of sex-role beliefs. Women who have traditional sex-role beliefs are more likely to blame themselves and other victimsfor being sexually harassed. zyx The research reported here was partially supported by a National lnrtitute of Mental Health (NIMH) grant UPHS-MH-32606-01 lo the second author. We would like to thank Marilynn Brewer, Joe McGrath, and an anonymous reviewer for their comments and sug- gestions. We would also like lo acknowledge the help of Tom Wickens in providing advice on the log linear analyses. Correspondence regarding this article should be addressed to 1. W. Jensen, Cedars Sinai Medical Office Towers, 8635 W. Third zyxwvu St., Suite 770W, Los Angeles, California 90048. 121 0022~S37/82/1200-0121$03.00/1(s) 1982 Society for the Psychological Study of Social Issuer
  • 2. 122 zyxwvutsrq lnger W. Jensen and Barbara A. Gulek zy Although research has investigated the extent to which sexual harass- ment exists and what people are or are not doing about it, little has been written about the personal attributions made for these behaviors by either those who have or those who have not experienced incidents of sexual harassment. Benson and Thomson (1982) have suggested that a significant gap appears to exist between men and women in their perception of the fre- quency and type of harassment that actually occurs at the workplace. They also reported considerable sex differences in the perception of the difficulty a woman would have in handling an unwanted sexual advance. Vogelmann-Sine, Ervin, Christensen, Warmsun, and Ulmann (1979) noted the importance of knowing how behaviors are interpreted in complex interpersonal situations in order to avoid possible misunderstanding of intent. The literature on sexual harassment indicates that sex differences exist in the perception of what constitutes harassing behaviors (Gutek, Nakamura, Gahart, Handschumacher Jensen, zyxw & Russell, 1980; Gutek, Note 1). Although the theoretical bases for these different perceptions of sexual harassment have not yet been addressed, the rape literature suggests a variety of theories for such sex differences in attributions of causality and assignment of responsiblity. Although sexual harassment and rape are certainly not the same, they do have some common characteristics. In each case, one person is trying to force his sexual attentions on an unwilling other. While in rape the coercion usually is the threat of bodily harm, in sexual harassment it usually is the threat of depriving individuals of their jobs or job op- portunities. Groth (1979) suggested that there are three basic ways in which a person gains sexual access to another person: 1) through consent (negotia- tion); 2) through pressure (exploitation); or 3) through force (intimidation). He further noted that the difference between pressure and force is that in the pressured situation, the victim is sexually harassed or exploited, while in the forced assault, the victim is raped. He suggested that there is enough similarity in the underlying factors of the two situations to discuss both meaningfully under the single term of rape. In further support of this notion, Medea and Thompson (1974) asserted that rape could be construed as a continuum of various degrees of violence with varying degrees of sexual intimacy. Although the various theories about attributional processes make some conflicting predictions, they do have a number of things in com-
  • 3. Attributions zyxwvutsrq 123 zy mon. Most predict that sex differences will exist in attributions of locus of responsibility as well as in how the incidents will be viewed. The study described in this article addressed these issues. It investigates how well various attribution theories account for the assignment of responsibility for sexual harassment incidents, and it explores the impact of sex-role beliefs on such assignment of responsibility. DESIGN OF THE STUDY The data addressing sex differences in assignment of responsibility were derived from interviews with a representative sample of workers in Los Angeles County with women oversampled; 827 women and 405 men were interviewed (see Gutek, Note 1, for a description of this survey). All other analyses use data obtained from 135 of the female respondents who were re-contacted and who will hereafter be referred to as the victims of sexual harassment. The pool of respondents for the follow-up interviews consisted of 293 women who, in the initial survey, agreed to be recontacted and who had reported experiences of one or more of the following sexually harassing behaviors at their present or previous jobs: a. having been touched by a man in a sexual way on the job; b. being expected to go out with a man with the understanding that it would hurt her job situation if she refused, or would help if she accepted; c. being expected to engage in sexual relations with a man with the understanding that it would hurt her job situation if she refused, or help if she accepted. The telephone interviews in this follow-up study were conducted with the help of a computerized telephone system that enabled direct programming of responses (Shure zyxwv & Meeker, 1978). Due to the lapse in time since the original contact (approximately 15 months), a substantial number of potential respondents could not be contacted; 135 interviews were com- pleted, close to a completion rate of 50%. The interviews were conducted by college seniors who had received prior training. In general, the interviews lasted between 15 and 20 minutes. Once the subject’s consent to be reinterviewed was obtained, the interviewer described the incident cited previously by the subject to see that the subject still remembered the incident. All subjects were asked the same questions. The first seven items dealt with feelings the respondent may have had
  • 4. 124 zyxwvutsrq lnger W. Jensen and Barbara A. Gutek zy following the incident. Then, some questions designed to ascertain locus of responsibility were presented, followed by a number of questions about reporting the incident and possible reasons for not reporting it. This was followed by questions about attitudes towards sexual harassment. Finally, a shortened version (17 items) of a scale measuring feminist attitudes (Smith, Ferree, zyxwvu & Miller, 1975) was presented. The scale contained items assessing beliefs about women’s personality traits and what constitutes appropriate role behavior in women. The main classes of variables from the follow-up study that are relevant to this paper are: 1) attribution items measuring self-blame and other-woman blame, 2) behavior related to reporting the incident, 3) affect variables (reports of feelings of anger, sadness, hurt, anxiety, disgust), 4) job-related outcomes, 5) health-related outcomes, 6) sex-role beliefs (Smith et al., 1975). RESULTS The results are divided into four sections: sex differences in assign- ment of responsibility to female victims of sexual harassment; the influence of being a victim on the assignment of responsibility; an exploration of the phenomenon of self-blame among victims; the relation between sex- role beliefs and assignment of responsibility. Sex Differences in Assignment of Responsibility Several authors (Shaver, 1970; Walster, 1966) have suggested that the differential motivations of an actor and an observer may be an im- portant factor in assignment of responsibility to a victim. Walster (1966) found that as the consequences of an accident grew more severe, there was a tendency for observers to assign increasing responsibility for the event to the victim rather than to chance. She hypothesized that in the case of a minor accident, the observer may feel sympathetic toward the victim and attribute the responsibility for the outcome to either chance or other persons. However, in the case of a severe catastrophe, ascribing its occurrence to chance would leave observers with the uncomfortable thought that a similar thing might happen to them. By stressing the dissimilarity of the victim to themselves, observers may disassociate themselves from a similar tragedy. Shaver (1970) introduced the notion that such a desire to avoid fear of unfortunate outcomes may be altered by the need for observers to
  • 5. Attributions zyxwvuts 125 zy avoid blame for such an incident and consequently may lead to differential assignment of causal attributions and responsibility depending on the ob- server’s perceived likelihood of being involved in a similar incident. He further made a distinction between relevance due to personal similarity between the observer and the victim of the accident, and relevance due to situational similarities between them. He suggested that situational similarity was a necessary factor, and once it was present, the degree of personal identification would determine which attributional system would take place. In “harm avoidance,” indicative of low personal relevance, increased personal responsibility is assigned to the victim; in “blame avoidance,” indicative of high personal relevance, decreased responsibility is assigned in order for the observer to avoid being blamed should a similar unfortunateincident happen to hcr. A different motivational theory was suggested by Lerner (1965, 1971, 1978). He postulated that people were motivated to believe in a “just world” where people get what they deserve, good or bad. To believe other- wise would raise the question of whether the person can trust his or her own environment. This “just world’’ notion suggests that when people hear of an unfortunate incident, they would want to believe that the victim deserved whatever suffering he or she experienced. When applied to incidents of sexual harassment, the theories make differential predictions about the assignment of responsibility by male and female observers. According to Shaver’s defensive notion, male observers should assign more responsibility for such incidents to female victims than would female observers. Walster’s defensive notion predicts that female respondents should view the female victim of sexual harassment as more responsible than should the male respondents. In contrast to both of those theories, Lerner’s “just world” would suggest no differences between male and female respondents in the assignment of responsibility. A review of the research investigating attribution theory and rape suggests that Shaver’s defensive notion best accounts for sex differences in assignment of responsibility for such incidents (cf. Calhoun, Selby, Cann, zy & Keller, 1978; Selby, Calhoun, & Brock, 1977). It is reasonable to suggest that male respondents, not wanting to be blamed in the future, would assign more responsibility to the women, and similarly, the women, not wanting to be blamed should such an event happen to them in the future, would assign less responsibility to the women and more to the men. Shaver’s version of attribution theory also best explains the data about the sex differences in assignment of responsibility in our initial survey on sexual harassment. More responsibility was assigned to the victim of sexual harassment by the male than the female respondents in the large survey. Significantly more of the male than female respondents endorsed the state- ment, “When a woman is asked by a man at work to engage in sexual
  • 6. 126 Inyer zyxwv W. Jensen and Barbara A. Gutek zy relations, it’s usually because she did something to bring it about” (54.5% of males versus 44.7% of females; zyxw x’ zyxwv = 9.41, p < .001). The Influence of Being a Victim on the Assignment of Responsibility Attribution theory also addresses the differential assignment of responsibility as a function of having been a victim. When applied to incidents of sexual harassment, attribution theory suggests that women in general will attribute less blame than men to a victim of sexual harassment. However, significant differences in assignment of responsibility should still emerge in a comparison between those women who had and those who had not themselves been sexually harassed. This notion was supported by the responses of the women inter- viewed in the larger study. Although overall the majority of the female victims agreed with the statement, “Women who are asked by men at work to engage in sexual relations could have done something to prevent it” (62% agreed), the agreement was significantly greater among those women who had zyxwvuts not been sexually harassed (74% agreed) (x’ = 10.8, df = 1, p < .01). A difference in the same direction, although not statistically significant, was found in respondents’ endorsement of the statement, “When a woman is asked by a man at work to engage in sexual relations, it’s usually because she did something to bring it about”; 46.4% of non- victims and 42.2% of victims agreed. The data further show that if the victims believed that a woman does something to bring about an incident of sexual harassment, they were also highly likely to believe that the woman could do something to prevent such incidents. The relationship between the two variables (women could prevent and women bring about) was significant (x’ = 290, df = 9, p < .001). It was, however, asymmetric. The female victims who strongly agreed with the statement, “Women do something to bring it about,” were highly likely to also agree with the statement, “Women could do something to prevent it” (92%). The same strong relationship was not found the other way around (59%). That a woman is expected to prevent things from “going too far” is consistent with traditional sex-role beliefs, which will be discussed later in this paper. Self-Blame The attributions made by victims about their own situation has been a focus of interest in the rape research and will be the focm of the remainder
  • 7. Attributions 127 zy of this paper. Attribution theory applied to rape situations suggests that victims experience self-blame (e.g., Bryant zyxw & Cirel, 1977; Burges & Holshom, 1974). Janoff-Bulman (1979) suggested that two different kinds of self-blame exist: control related self-blame and esteem related self- blame. With the former self-attributional strategy, persons blame the unfortunate incident on their own behavior, such as having engaged in a certain activity, or inversely, having failed to do so (e.g., I should not have gone down this street alone at night; I should have kept my door bolted). In the latter type of self-blame, the persons fault their own character (i.e., I am just a careless person). While the self-blame focused on behavior is termed “behavioral,” the esteem related self-blame is termed “characterological” by Janoff-Bulman. While there has been a scarcity of research dealing with this issue, it seemed plausible that there may also be such assignment of respon- sibility to self by victims of sexual harassment. Our follow-up study ad- dressed several questions regarding self-blame: 1) will a victim of sexual harassment engage in self-blame? 2) will a victim who engages in self-blame be less likely than other victims to seek help? 3) what are the affective consequences of self-blame and what further effects does self-blame have? Three statements designed to measure the presence of self-blame were presented to the women in the follow-up survey: zyx 1) “Perhaps something in my behavior brought it about,” 2) “Perhaps something in my behavior may have encouraged the man,’’ and 3) “I am the sort of person these kinds of incidents are likely to happen to.” The first two statements differ somewhat from the latter, in that they dealt with the respondents’ behavior; the correlation between the two is .53. The “sort of person” statement, on the other hand, ascribed the incident to more characterological qualities of the respondent and yielded low correlations with the two behavioral attributions (.17and .12 respectively). The majority of victims did not evidence self-blame; only 25.4% of the 135 victims agreed with the first statement, 29.3% with the second statement, and 20.9% with the third. It is possible, of course, that greater self-blame may be evidenced when assessed soon after an incident; all of these in- cidents occurred at least 15months before the interview. zyx Self-Blame and Incident Reporting It was hypothesized that a victim displaying self-blame would be less likely to have reported the incident to someone in authority. To in- vestigate the relationship between self-blame and help-seeking behavior, responses to the following questions were examined: 1) “Did you report
  • 8. 128 zyxwvutsrq lnger W. Jensen and Barbara A. Culek zy your experience to anyone in authority like a supervisor, personnel director, attorney, etc.?” and 2) “Did you talk to anyone you were close to about the experience, like family, a friend, or a co-worker?” As predicted, those who endorsed the two behavioral self-blame items were less likely to have reported the incident (Table 1). The Pearson correlation coefficients show significant relationships between the two behavioral self-blame items and reporting the incident, but failed to support such a relationship for the characterological self-blame item. The same relation was found between the self-blame items and the item investigating whether victims had talked to friends about the incident. Table 1 indicates that respondents who had endorsed the behavioral self-blame statements tended not to have talked to anyone about the incident of sexual harassment. One possible reason for the lack of a relationship between the characterological self-blame item and reporting the incident may be that this item does not adequately measure self-blame but rather may reflect feelings of being a victim. Another factor is that the characterological self-blame item is psychometrically weak because the response distribu- tion is so skewed. One of the ways of explaining the non-report of incidents among the women who engage in behavioral self-blame is the process of victimiza- tion of women suggested by Weis and Borges (1973) and reported ex- tensively in the rape literature. This notion proposes that other people will hold a victim responsible for her rape or sexual harassment. As part of this process, the victim internalizes the blame and will try to find some- thing in her behavior to which she can attribute the incident. It is then not surprising that she will hesitate to report an incident to authorities or even to friends. As further support of this notion, more than one-third of the victims endorsed the following statement as a reason for non-report of the incident: “I thought it would be held against me or that I would be blamed.” Table 1. The Relationship Between Self-Blame and Help-Seeking Behavioi Agreement with statements Reported incident Talked about incident Behavioral self-blame: behavior brought it about. zyxwvutsr . . may have encouraged the man. . . Perhaps something in my -0.22** -0.25** Somethingabout my behavior -0.19** -0.30*** Characterological self-blame: I am the sort of person these kinds zyxwvutsr of incidents are likely to happen to . . . 0.04 --0.10 zyx *p < zyxwvuts O.O5;**p < 0.01; ***p < 0.001.
  • 9. Allribulions zyxwvutsr 129 zy The data in the follow-up survey also show that if a victim blames herself, she is concerned about protecting the harasser. Positive correla- tions were obtained for the relationships between all three self-blame items and endorsement of the statement, “I did not want to hurt the person who bothered me.” (Correlations for “behavior brought it about,” “be- havior encouraged,” and “sort of person” were .19, .33, and .23 respec- tively.) zyxwvuts SelfBlame zyxwvut and Affect Janoff-Bulman (1979) suggested the efficacy of distinguishing be- tween characterological and behavioral self-blame because she felt they had different consequences. She contended that characterological self-blame leads to feelings of worthlessness, of deserving one’s fate, and especially, to depression. The hypothesized link between characterological self- blame and reports of depression was not upheld in this study. Neither of the two types of self-blame were related to reports zyx of depression. The coefficients between the self-blame items and depression were all under .lo. A relatively small percentage of respondents (20%) gave reports of depression in response to the sexual harassment incident, in contrast to reports of other types of affect such as anger (68%) and disgust (80%). Possible explanations may be that feelings of depression are more difficult to relate to a stranger and/or harder to remember after more than a year has passed. It is possible that the college students who evidenced depres- sion in Janoff-Bulman’s study might not have revealed having experienced such affect in a telephone interview over one year later. In this study, seven different affective responses of victims to their sexual harassment were assessed. A factor analysis of the items yielded three factors. One included the affective responses of hurt, sadness, and depression, and was labeled inward-directed affect; the second, labeled outward-directed affect, was formed by the two responses, anger and disgust. The third factor consisted of the single item, anxiety. Reliability analyses supported combining the inward-directed affect items (Cronbach’s alpha zyxwvut = .72) as well as the two outward-directed affect items, anger and disgust (Cronbach’s alpha = .80). Janoff-Bulman’s (1979) research would suggest that a positive rela- tionship might exist between behavioral self-blame and outward-directed affect and also between characterological self-blame and inward-directed affect as well as anxiety. In order to test this, the two items measuring behavioral self-blame were combined into a single index (Cronbach’s alpha = .64). There were significant relationships between behavioral self-blame and outward-directed affect, and also between characterological
  • 10. 130 zyxwvutsr lnger zyxwv W. Jensen and Barbara A. Gutek Table 2. zyxwvuts The Relationship Between Type of Blame for Incidents of Sexual Harassment and Type of Affect Experienced Pearson correlation coefficients Type of affect T w e of blame Inward Outward Anxiety zyx ~~ ~ Behavioral self-blame 0.02 0.27*** 0.04 Characterological self-blame 0.01 0.03 .15** zyxw *p < 0.05; zyxwvuts **p < 0.01; ***p < 0.001. self-blame and anxiety. However, the expected relationship between characterological self-blame and inward-directed affect was not obtained (Table 2). This may be because the characterological self-blame item is not normally distributed and apparently has low reliability. Effects zyxwvu of Negative Affect Resultingfrom Sexual Harassment In the case of sexual harassment, the knowledge that victims have negative emotional reactions is not as likely to impress employers as the fact that those emotional reactions have work-related consequences. Several questions were asked in the follow-up interview to ascertain whether feelings towards work had been adversely affected. Analyses revealed significant relationships between reports of feelings of the negative affect items-hurt, sadness, depression, anxiety, anger, and disgust -and an item measuring negative attitude toward the job -loss of motivation, feeling distracted, and dreading to go to work. If a victim reported having experienced any of these types of negative affect, she was also likely to report having experienced loss of motivation and/or feeling distracted at the workplace zyxw (F = .27). There also was a significant relationship between negative affect and an item measuring respondents’ physical well-being after the incident. If she had experienced any of the types of negative affect, she was also likely to have experienced negative physical effects (headaches, nervousness and sleeplessness) (f = .26). Sex-Role Beliefs and Assignment of Responsibility Several studies on rape (Brownmiller, 1975; Field, 1978; Medea & Thompson, 1974; Smith, Keating, Hester, & Mitchell, 1976; Krulewitz & Payne, Note 2) conclude that general attitudes toward women, as in- corporated in traditional versus pro-feminist views, are the best predictors
  • 11. Atlributions 131 zy of attitudes about rape. Krulewitz and Payne (Note zyx 2) found support for the notion that subjects’ attitudes toivard traditional sex-roles may influence their perceptions about rape. While normative behavior by women is often described as “submissive, passive, not aggressive, and fearful,” male behavior is frequently described as “dominant, aggressive, adventurous, opportunistic, and proud of sexual ability” (Heilbrun, 1976). Krulewitz and Payne (Note 2) suggested that the rapist could be viewed as displaying an extreme version of such stereotypically masculine sex- role behavior; i.e., sexually active, aggressive, and opportunistic. They suggested that this may lead to perceiving the rape as normative male- female sexual behavior, especially when force and physical violence is low. Thus, an incident will be characterized as rape only when the per- petrator uses substantial physical force. They proposed, however, that this would only hold true for subjects incorporating more traditional sex-role beliefs; those with nonstereotypical attitudes towards women would be likely to view an incident as rape irrespective of the amount of force used or the subject’s physical condition after the rape. Labeling those holding the more traditional views towards women non-feminist and those with stronger positive attitudes towards feminism pro-feminist, Krulewitz and Payne found that while the amount of force depicted did not affect the attributions of certainty by the pro-feminist female subjects, non-feminist female subjects required substantial evidence of force before they were likely to judge an incident as a rape. As the level of force in- creased, victims were viewed as more helpless, but also, as more respect- able. This indication that a respectable woman is expected to withstand rape unless violent force is exerted by the rapist has been supported by other studies (Brownmiller, 1975; Medea zyxw & Thompson, 1974). They argue that commonly held societal attitudes dictate that women should be responsible for protecting their sexual purity. In further support, Field (1978) found that pro-feminist subjects assigned less responsibility to the victim than did subjects who embraced more traditional, or conservative sex-role beliefs. The possible impact of sex-role beliefs on perceptions of sexual harassment has not been addressed. Earlier the victimization process was suggested as one possible explanation for self-blame. Another exp1anatio:i is that sex-role beliefs affect self-blame. In order to investigate whether the effects of sex-role beliefs could be generalized from research on rape to sexual harassment, the 135 women in the follow-up survey who had reported their experiences of sexual harassment were administered a sex- role scale (Smith et al., 1975) along with the other questions. This scale used 5-point Likert-type items to measure acceptance or rejection of central beliefs of feminism and the acceptance or rejection of traditional sex- role beliefs.
  • 12. 132 lnger zyxwv W. Jensen and Barbara A. Gutek zy The literature on rape suggests that more assignment of respon- sibility to victims will be made by those who adhere to traditional sex- role beliefs. Generalizing this to incidents of sexual harassment suggested that of those women who reported experiences of sexual harassment, those who adhere to traditional stereotypic sex-role concepts would assign greater responsibility to women in general for incidents of sexual harassment. Three questions dealing with assignment of responsibility to women in general for sexual harassment were fermed into a single index (Cronbach’s alpha zyxwvut = .60). The three items concerned whether women provoke ad- vances, do something to bring them about, and could do something to prevent them. There was a significant correlation between the responses to this index and to the sex-role scale zyxwv (r = .37), indicating that victims who obtain traditional scores on the sex-role scale are more likely than those with non-traditional scores to hold women responsible for incidents of sexual harassment. Sex-Role Beliefs and Self-Blame Sex-role beliefs should also have an impact on whether or not a victim of sexual harassment will blame herself. In support of this con- tention, there were significant correlations between sex-role beliefs and the index containing the two behavioral self-blame items (r = .26) and be- tween sex-role beliefs and characterological self-blame (r = .33). Relation Between Self-Blame and Blaming Others Sex-role beliefs of victims are related to both self-blame and blaming other women, suggesting that there may be a relationship between self- blame and other-woman blame. We predicted a stronger tendency to blame others for the women who engaged in self-blame (behavioral and character- ological) in contrast to those who did not evidence self-blame. Con- sistent with this prediction were significant positive correlations between the index assessing blame to other women and both characterological self-blame (r = .23) and the two-item index measuring behavioral self- blame (r = .32). Thus, sex-role beliefs, blaming others, and self-blame are all related. What are the causal or sequential processes by which they are linked? One possibility is that both other-woman-blame and self-blame are con- sequences of sex-role beliefs. This possibility can be directly tested by examining them in a three-dimensional test of frequencies, that gives the difference in the fit between two models (cf. Fienberg, 1980). One model contains only direct relationships between self-blame and sex-role beliefs
  • 13. Allribuliuns 133 zy sex-role belief Fig. zyxwvuts 1. Models of relationships among sex-role beliefs, self-blame, and other- woman-blame. and between sex-role beliefs and other-woman-blame (Figure zyx 1, excluding dashed line); the other model contains all three relationships -sex-role beliefs and other-woman-blame, sex-role beliefs and self-blame, and self-blame and other-woman-blame (Figure 1 with the dashed line). The results of the model testing indicate that the relationship between the two forms of blame can be attributed to their common association with sex-role beliefs, and not to any direct causal links between them.' These results are not consistent with the belief that victimization is a consequence of internalizing others' perceptions. If that were the case, there should have been an independent effect for the relation between other-woman-blame and self-blame. The results of this analysis do suggest a mechanism for reducing self-blame -acquiring more feminist sex-role beliefs. CONCLUSIONSAND IMPLICATIONS The hypothesis that men and women differ in their assignment of responsibility regarding sexual harassment at the workplace is strongly supported by our survey data. Men are more likely than women to blame women for being sexually harassed. Among women, victims are less likely than others to blame women for being sexually harassed. Our data from the follow-up survey supported the research on rape showing that victims engage in self-blame. Although the follow-up data were collected over a year after the incident, between 20% and 30% of women endorsed each of three self-blame statements. These victims showed less characterological than behavioral self-blame. 'Formally, the test is of the conditional independence of self-blame [S] and other- woman-blame [ 0 ] in the presence of the sex-role variable [R] (see Fienberg, 1980, chapter 3). The model [RS] [RO] tests the relationships of sex-role to each of the two blame variables. A fit of the model [RS] [RO] to the data gave a likelihood chi-square of 7.54 on 8 degrees of freedom; a fit of the model [RS] [RO] [SO] gave a likelihood chi-square of 4.18 on 4 degrees of freedom. The difference between these two statistics tests the strength of the self-other association. The difference gave a non-significant chi-square statistic ofzyx 3.36 with zy 4degrees of freedom.
  • 14. 134 zyxwvutsrq lnger W. Jensen and Barbara A. Gulek zy Victims who evidenced behavioral self-blame were less likely than other victims to report the incident or talk to someone about it. They were also more likely to feel anger and disgust as a result of the incident, yet they (as well as women who experienced characterological self-blame) were more concerned about protecting the harasser than other victims who did not report self-blame. Moreover, women who had experienced negative affect were more likely to report having experienced adverse physical reactions, as well as such adverse job reactions as distraction and loss of motivation at the workplace. This indicates that the occurrence of sexual harassment may well be of substantital economic importance to the employer. In this regard, these findings parallel those of a recent large-scale government study investigating incidents of sexual harassment within the federal government (U.S. Merit Systems Protection Board, 1981). The results of the follow-up study also suggest that in trying to ac- count for attributions and assignment of responsibility for experiences like sexual harassment and rape, a salient factor to consider may be the accep- tance of traditional versus progressive sex-role beliefs. The results of this study suggest that sex-role beliefs will dictate how a woman who has been sexually harassed is going to feel about the incident. If she has in- corporated the traditional sex-role beliefs, she will be highly likely to blame other women, as well as herself, for incidents of sexual harassment. Such an attribution process, in return, will affect whether she reports the incident to someone in authority or talks to co-workers or friends about it. Furthermore, the data presented here show that sex-role beliefs have direct effects on both blaming self and blaming others. These data are in contrast to a model that shows a direct relationship between the two kinds of self-blame. One common model proposes that self-blame is an internalization of holding other women responsible for rape or sexual harassment (cf. Weis zyxwvu & Borges, 1973). In contrast, our data suggest that a woman who is a victim of sexual harassment might engage in self-blame because of her traditional sex-role beliefs rather than because she blames other victims for being sexually harassed. One implication of the impact of sex-role beliefs on attitudes towards sexual harassment as well as rape is that a different approach toward change may be warranted. Theorists in the past have suggested that at- titudes could be changed by providing ample information about what really happens in such incidents, and by changing specific beliefs about the act and its consequences. It is possible that such an approach is not the method of choice for these conditions, and that only by changing people’s general sex-role beliefs can one effect a change in the attitudes towards victims of harassment. Such a theory of attitudes and attitude change has an immediate application in the legal profession, where much of the theory
  • 15. Attributions 135 zy about sexual harassment and debate about how it should be treated has taken place (cf. MacKinnon, 1979). Our data suggest that, to the lawyer who represents a victim of an incident, the most salient information to obtain about the judge or the jury may not be their personal experiences, or even their sex, but rather whether they endorse traditional or progressive sex-role beliefs. In general, this research project has demonstrated that sexual harass- ment is sufficiently similar to rape to borrow theories and hypotheses. In this case, the research on attribution theory that has been applied to situations of rape proved to be a fruitful starting point for generating hypotheses about sexual harassment. This ability to borrow from the literature on rape is especially helpful since research and theory on rape is better developed than research and theory on sexual harassment. A continued interchange of research results between the two areas will even- tually reveal crucial differences between rape and sexual harassment, as well as the similarities here considered. This will yield benefits to both areas of inquiry. REFERENCE NOTES zyxw 1. Gutek, B. A. zyxwvutsr Experiences zyxwvuts of sexual hurussment: Resulis Jrotn a representutive survey. Paper presented at the annual meeting of the American Psychological Association, LOS Angeles, August 1981. 2. Krulewitz, J. E., & Payne, E. J. Sex differences in attributions about rupe, ruprsts, and rape viclims. Paper presented at the meeting of the American Psychological Associa- tion, San Francisco, 1977. REFERENCES Benson, D. J., & Thomson, G. Sexual harassment on a university campus: The confluence of authority relations, sexual interest, and gender stratification. Social Problems, 1982, 29(3),236-251. Brownmiller, S. Against our will: Men, women and rape. New York: Simon and Schuster, 197s. Bryant, G., & Cirel, P. A communily response to rape: An exemplary projeci. (Polk County Rape/Sexual Assault Case Center). Washington, D.C.: National Institute of Law Enforcement and Criminal Justice, 1977. Burgess, A. W., & Holmstrom, L. L. Rape trauma syndrome. American Journal of Psy- chiatry,' 1974, 131,981-986. Calhoun, L. G., Selby, J. W., Cann, A., & Keller, G. T. The effects of victim physical attractiveness and sex of respondents on social reactions to victims of rape. The Brilish Journalof Socialand Clinical Psychology, 1978, zyxwv 17, 191-192. Field, H. S. Attitudes toward rape: A comparative analysis of police, rapists, crisis counselors, and citizens. Journal of Personalily and Social Psychology, 1978, 36, 156-179. Fienberg, S. E. The analysis of cross-classified culegorical data (2nd Ed.). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press, 1980. Groth, N. A. Men who rape: The psychology of the offender. New York: Plenum Publishing Co., 1979.
  • 16. 136 zyxwvuts lnger W. Jensen and Barbara A. Gutek zy Gutek, B. A,, Nakamura, zyxwvuts C . Y., Gahart, M., Handschumacher Jensen, I. W., zyx & Russell, D. Sexuality in the workplace. zyxwvut Basic and Applied Social Psychology, 1980, 1(3), 255-265. Heilbrun, A. B., Jr. Measurement of masculine and feminine sex role identities as inde- pendent dimensions. Journal zyxwvu of Consulting and Clinical Psycliology, 1976, 44(2), 183-190. Janoff-Bulman, R. Characterological versus behavioral self-blame: Inquiries into depression and rape. Journal zyxwvutsr of Personality and Social Psychology, 1979, 37, 1798-1809. Lerner, M. J. Evaluation of performance as a function of performer’s reward and attractive- ness. Journal ofPersonality and Social Psychology, 1965, I , 355-360. Lerner, M. J. Observer’s evaluation of a victim: Justice, guilt, and verdical perception. Jourrial of Personality and Social Psychology, 1971,20, 127-135. Lerner, M. J., & Miller, D. T. Just world research and the attribution process: Looking back and ahead. Psychological Bulletin, 1978,85, 1030-1051. MacKinnon, C. Sexual harassment of working wornen. New Haven: Yale University Press, 1979. Medea, A., & Thompson, K. Against rape. New York: Farrar, Strauss, and Giroux, 1974. Selby, J. N., Calhoun, L. G. & Brock, T. A. Sex differences in the social perceptions of a rape victim. Personalityand Social Psychology Bulletin, 1977,3, 412-415. Shaver, K. G. Defensive attribution: Effects of severity and relevance on the responsibility assigned to accidents. Journal ofPersonality and Social Psychology, 1970, 14, 101-113. Shure, G. H., & Meeker, R. J. A minicomputer system for multiperson computer- assisted telephone interviewing. Behavior Research Methods and lnstruriientation, Smith, E. R., Ferree, M. M., & Miller, F. D. A short scale of attitudes towards feminism. RepresentativeResearcli in Social Psychology, 1975, 6, 5 1-56. Smith, R. E., Keating, J. P., Hester, R. K., & Mitchell, H. E. Role and justice considera- tions in the attribution of responsibility to a rape victim. Journal of Researcl7 in Personality, 1976, 10, 346-357. U. S. Merit Systems Protection Board, Office of Merit Systems Review and Studies. Sexual horassnient zyxwvuts in thefederal workplace: Is it oproble~ii? March 1981. Volgelniann-Sine, S., Ervin, E. D., Christensen, R., Warmsun, C. H., & Ullmann, L. P. Sex differences in feelings attributed to a woman in situations involving coercion and sexual advances. Journal oJPersonality, 1979, 47(3),420-431. Walster, E. Assignment of responsibility for an accident. Journal of Personaliry and Social Weis, K., & Borges, S. S. Victiniology and rape: The case of the legitimate victim. Issues in 1978, 10(2), 196-202. PS,YCI~OIOXY, 1966.3, 73-79. Crin~inology,1973, 8, 71-115.