SlideShare a Scribd company logo
1 of 192
Chapter Eleven
Intercultural Communication and the Organization
Chapter Objectives
Explain why intercultural communication is essential for
organizational success and communication.
Describe what is meant by culture in intercultural
communication.
Identify barriers to effective intercultural communication.
Identify steps that can be used to overcome these barriers.
Discuss the Hofstede dimensions as they apply to organizational
contexts.
Organizational Communication: Foundations for Business and
Management, 2e Thomson, 2006 ©
Reasons to Study Intercultural CommunicationWe are a global
village.Information is readily available anywhere on earth.The
workplace is more diverse than ever before. We must be able to
interact with others here and abroad efficiently and
appropriately.Bottom line: Effective intercultural
communication has become essential.
Organizational Communication: Foundations for Business and
Management, 2e Thomson, 2006 ©
Three Reasons Effective Intercultural Communication is
Essential
Mobility.Time and distance are no longer factors in
communication.
Economic and political interdependence.The world’s markets
and global concerns are dependent on effective communication.
Communication TechnologyIncreased technology has allowed us
freedoms to communicate quickly, efficiently, and cheaply.
Organizational Communication: Foundations for Business and
Management, 2e Thomson, 2006 ©
Defining CultureMultiple perspectives are apparent in all of the
following categoriesNormsRolesBelief SystemsValuesLaws
Organizational Communication: Foundations for Business and
Management, 2e Thomson, 2006 ©
Culture refers to the multiple perspectives a group has on the
world and worldly phenomena.
Defining CultureCulture is learned, not biologically transmitted.
Enculturation occurs through:LanguageNonverbal
messagesSpace and time orientationPatterns for thinkingSelf-
imagesAestheticsCulture is also modified by exposure to other
cultures, which is called acculturation.
Organizational Communication: Foundations for Business and
Management, 2e Thomson, 2006 ©
(continued)
Intercultural Communication
Refers to communication exchanges between people whose
cultural perceptions and symbols must be managed for effective
communication.
Organizational Communication: Foundations for Business and
Management, 2e Thomson, 2006 ©
Intercultural CommunicationSome examples of intercultural
communication include:An African-American speaking to a
Caucasian.A Jew speaking to a MuslimA Cajun speaking to a
CalifornianA man communicating to a woman
Organizational Communication: Foundations for Business and
Management, 2e Thomson, 2006 ©
Barriers to Effective Intercultural CommunicationPerceptual
disparitySelective perceptions may lead to confusing or
conflicting opinions.What is taken for granted behavior in one
culture could be considered disrespectful in another.
Organizational Communication: Foundations for Business and
Management, 2e Thomson, 2006 ©
Barriers to Effective Intercultural
CommunicationEthnocentrismPeople tend to judge the behaviors
of others based on their own cultural values
that they believe are “right.”An ethnocentric individual
believes his or her ethnicity or ethnic perspective is superior to
others.
Organizational Communication: Foundations for Business and
Management, 2e Thomson, 2006 ©
Barriers to Effective Intercultural CommunicationLanguage
dissimilaritySome researchers suggest that language shapes
reality. Slang, jargon, and other language barriers can
impact message reception and understanding.
Organizational Communication: Foundations for Business and
Management, 2e Thomson, 2006 ©
(continued)
Barriers to Effective Intercultural CommunicationNonverbal
dissimilarityGestures, emblems, and other nonverbal behaviors
may be judged differently or have different meanings in
different cultures.
Organizational Communication: Foundations for Business and
Management, 2e Thomson, 2006 ©
(continued)
Approaches to Multinational ExpansionMultinational
organizations can be structured in different ways towards the
cultures they are expanding into.Adler 5 structural orientations
multinational organizations use
Organizational Communication: Foundations for Business and
Management, 2e Thomson, 2006 ©
Adler Structural Orientations of Multinational Organizations
Cultural dominanceSuperimposing the culture to subsidiary
offices.
Cultural accommodationAttempting to accept and assume the
cultural values of the host company.
Cultural compromiseAttempting to identify the divergent
cultural orientations, but using only the similarities in the
formation of policy.
Organizational Communication: Foundations for Business and
Management, 2e Thomson, 2006 ©
Adler Structural Orientations of Multinational Organizations
Cultural avoidancePretending there are no differences between
cultures.
Cultural synergyA synergistic approach.Policies are not
superimposed by the parent organization.Values of both cultures
are managed, effectively creating a new culture.
Organizational Communication: Foundations for Business and
Management, 2e Thomson, 2006 ©
The Hofstede StudiesSuggests that there are perceptual
differences based on culture.Implies that these perceptual
differences can account for misunderstanding and intercultural
tensions in organizational contexts.
Organizational Communication: Foundations for Business and
Management, 2e Thomson, 2006 ©
The Hofstede StudiesHofstede surveyed 116,000 employees in
72 nations to see if perceptions about work and work processes
varied according to country and culture.Differences were
categorized in four groups.
Organizational Communication: Foundations for Business and
Management, 2e Thomson, 2006 ©
(continued)
The Hofstede StudiesUncertainty avoidanceHigh uncertainty
avoidance cultures seek more clarity and less abstraction.
Organizational Communication: Foundations for Business and
Management, 2e Thomson, 2006 ©
(continued)
The Hofstede StudiesPower distanceEmployees in
high power distant culturesrespect superiors and disdain those
who challenge them.
Organizational Communication: Foundations for Business and
Management, 2e Thomson, 2006 ©
(continued)
The Hofstede StudiesIndividualism vs. CollectivismRelates to
whether persons in the group value free-
spirited independence, orcommunity cooperation and
compromise.
Organizational Communication: Foundations for Business and
Management, 2e Thomson, 2006 ©
(continued)
The Hofstede Studies
Task vs. Social OrientationPertains to whether the dominant
values of the society emphasize assertiveness
or nurture.
Organizational Communication: Foundations for Business and
Management, 2e Thomson, 2006 ©
(continued)
Cultural Extremes
Organizational Communication: Foundations for Business and
Management, 2e Thomson, 2006 ©
Adapted from tables in Hofstede, Geert, Culture’s
Consequences: Second Edition, Comparing Values, Behaviors,
Instituations and Organizations Across Nations, Sage
Publications, 2001, pp. 87, 151, 215, 286.
Suggestions for
Overcoming BarriersFollow PrescriptionsSometimes we have an
idea about what to do, but find it hard to actually follow
through.
Organizational Communication: Foundations for Business and
Management, 2e Thomson, 2006 ©
Suggestions for
Overcoming BarriersLearn about other cultures
Acquire multicultural competencies and skills including foreign
languages.
Become students of worldwide human relations and values.
Think beyond local perceptions and transform stereotypes into
positive views of people.
Become open and flexible in dealing with diversity.
Organizational Communication: Foundations for Business and
Management, 2e Thomson, 2006 ©
Suggestions for
Overcoming BarriersRecognize diversity within cultural
groups.People within cultures are also diverse.Question
stereotypes you have about people from a specific culture.
Organizational Communication: Foundations for Business and
Management, 2e Thomson, 2006 ©
Suggestions for
Overcoming BarriersAssume an egalitarian frame. Be open to
cultural differences.Be aware of and question your own
ethnocentric attitudes and beliefs.
Organizational Communication: Foundations for Business and
Management, 2e Thomson, 2006 ©
Central ConceptsBecause of the ease of travel, the
pervasiveness of communication technology, and
political/economic reliance on other countries,
it has become increasingly important to understand the effects
of intercultural communication on organizational success.
Organizational Communication: Foundations for Business and
Management, 2e Thomson, 2006 ©
Central ConceptsOrganizational men and women need to
examine the communication noises that affect intercultural
interactions andbecome dedicated to overcoming these obstacles
when they communicate.
Organizational Communication: Foundations for Business and
Management, 2e Thomson, 2006 ©
PURPOSEFUL READING (3-2-1) REPORT Version 2.0
Lightly Adapted from a template by Geraldine Van Gyn.
Question 1: In your own words, what are the 3 most important
concepts, ideas or issues in the
reading? Briefly explain why you chose them.
Concept 1 (In your own words) (2 marks)
Concept 2 (In your own words) (2 marks)
Concept 3 (In your own words) (2 marks)
Question 2: What are 2 concepts, ideas or issues in the article
that you had difficulty
understanding, or that are missing but should have been
included? In your own words, briefly
explain what you did to correct the situation (e.g. looked up an
unfamiliar word or a missing
fact), and the result. Cite any sites or sources used in APA
format.
Issue 1 (In your own words) (1 mark)
Citation 1 (in APA format) (1 mark)
Issue 2 (In your own words) (1 mark)
Citation 2 (in APA format) (1 mark)
Question 3: What is the main economic story of the reading?
(Economics studies the allocation
of scarce resources.)
Story (In your own words) (2 marks)
1
ECON 317 SPRING 2010 – INDIVIDUAL ASSIGNMENT 1
TO BE SUBMITTED VIA COURSESPACES BY 11:59 PM ON
JANUARY 21st, 2020
Name (First, Family)
Last 3 digits of SID
TO SPEED UP MARKING, PLEASE ANSWER THE
QUESTIONS IN THE FORMS AND SPACES PROVIDED. THE
T.A. RESERVES THE RIGHT TO NOT MARK ANY
QUESTIONS THAT ARE NOT ANSWERED IN THE
EXPECTED LOCATIONS.
By submitting this assignment you agree to the following honor
code, and understand that any violation of the honor code may
lead to penalties including but not limited to a non-negotiable
mark of zero on the assignment:
Honor Code: I guarantee that all the answers in this assignment
are my own work. I have cited any outside sources that I used
to create these answers in correct APA style.
Marking scheme – Make sure you answer all the questions
before handing this in!
Question
Marks
1
a
12
2
a
3
b
3
3
a
3
Total
21
1. [Critique] Read the following article:
Marchildon, G. P. & Allin, S. (2016). The Public-Private Mix in
the Delivery of Health-Care Services: Its Relevance for Lower-
Income Canadians. Global Social Welfare, 3, pp. 161-170.
Retrieved from https://link-springer-
com.ezproxy.library.uvic.ca/article/10.1007%2Fs40609-016-
0070-4
(The above link allows you to access the article for free
whether you are on campus or not. If off-campus, log in to UVic
when prompted.)
a. Write a 3-2-1 report on the article using the form provided on
the course web site. (12 marks)
A note about 3-2-1 reports in this course:
For the ‘2’ part (2 things you didn’t understand and what you
did to fix it), we need to see both what you found out, and the
source you used to find that out, properly cited in APA format.
The paper you are asked to read for the assignment CANNOT be
used as the source: it’s understood that you will make sure to
understand everything within the article you are asked to read,
by consulting the article itself. This part of the 3-2-1 report is
meant to encourage you to consult outside sources to find things
out that you CANNOT find out from the article itself.
While I would prefer that you use sources that, unlike
Wikipedia, aren’t subject to constant editing and change, it’s
fine to use Wikipedia or other such sources as long as you also
include the date and time of access, so that readers can look up
the state of the page as it was when you looked at it. (So, you’d
add to the standard ‘Retrieved from’ part, ‘on January 3, 2020
at 11:59 AM’.)
Also, remember to include a summary, in your own words, of
what you found. It’s not enough to say, ‘I didn’t know what a
raccoon was, so I looked it up on Wikipedia.’ You need to tell
us what you discovered about raccoons, in your own words, and
you need to cite the source you looked the information up on.
2. [Theory] A 2016 article[footnoteRef:0] begins, “Extra-billing
in Ontario, private MRIs in Saskatchewan and user fees in
Quebec: violations of the Canada Health Act are on the rise
across the country.” [0: Meili, R. (2016, April 4). It’s Time
For The Federal Government To Enforce The Canada Health
Act. The Huffington Post. Retrieved from
http://www.huffingtonpost.ca/ryan-meili/canada-health-
act_b_9610424.html
]
a. What pillar(s) of the Canada Health Act is/are violated by
private MRIs in Saskatchewan, and in what way? Be specific. (3
marks)
Pillar(s) Violated:
___________________________________________________
Reasoning:
_____________________________________________________
____
b. What pillar(s) of the Canada Health Act is/are violated by
user fees for health care in Quebec, and in what way? Be
specific. (3 marks)
Pillar(s) Violated:
___________________________________________________
Reasoning:
_____________________________________________________
____
3. [Mini Meta Analysis] This is a simple question meant to help
you understand what is expected of you in the group project.
Group Assignment 1, due on January 31, will ask you to
conduct a search for articles to use in your own mini meta
analysis. This question asks you to familiarize yourself with
how such searches are reported in published, peer-reviewed
mini meta-analyses.
Start by accessing the following article:
Wiradhany, W. & Koerts, J. (2019). Everyday functioning-
related cognitive correlates of media multitasking: a mini meta-
analysis. Media Psychology. Retrieved from
https://doi.org/10.1080/15213269.2019.1685393
(The article is open-access, so you should have no trouble
accessing it, whether on or off campus.)
a. (3 marks) The published mini meta-analysis searched for
studies in PsycInfo, ERIC, MEDLINE, SocINDEX, CMMC and
DOAJ. For this question, use the search methods listed in the
‘Study selection’ section of the mini meta analysis to search for
articles fitting the search criteria in Google Scholar (
https://scholar.google.ca/ ). Google Scholar is a very useful
tool that searches through peer-reviewed academic literature.
In the space below, cite, in APA format, the first study in your
search results that fits the criteria specified in the mini meta
analysis. (Hint: This question can be answered VERY quickly.
The method described in the very first paragraph of the ‘study
selection’ section, on pages 4 and 5, should be enough for this
assignment question – just make sure your top result is a study,
and not a summary of studies, such as a review article,
systematic summary or meta-analysis, and that it studies media
multitasking, rather than just media OR multitasking.)
Full Terms & Conditions of access and use can be found at
https://www.tandfonline.com/action/journalInformation?journal
Code=hmep20
Media Psychology
ISSN: 1521-3269 (Print) 1532-785X (Online) Journal
homepage: https://www.tandfonline.com/loi/hmep20
Everyday functioning-related cognitive correlates
of media multitasking: a mini meta-analysis
Wisnu Wiradhany & Janneke Koerts
To cite this article: Wisnu Wiradhany & Janneke Koerts (2019):
Everyday functioning-related
cognitive correlates of media multitasking: a mini meta-
analysis, Media Psychology, DOI:
10.1080/15213269.2019.1685393
To link to this article:
https://doi.org/10.1080/15213269.2019.1685393
© 2019 The Author(s). Published with
license by Taylor & Francis Group, LLC.
Published online: 03 Nov 2019.
Submit your article to this journal
Article views: 328
View related articles
View Crossmark data
https://www.tandfonline.com/action/journalInformation?journal
Code=hmep20
https://www.tandfonline.com/loi/hmep20
https://www.tandfonline.com/action/showCitFormats?doi=10.10
80/15213269.2019.1685393
https://doi.org/10.1080/15213269.2019.1685393
https://www.tandfonline.com/action/authorSubmission?journalC
ode=hmep20&show=instructions
https://www.tandfonline.com/action/authorSubmission?journalC
ode=hmep20&show=instructions
https://www.tandfonline.com/doi/mlt/10.1080/15213269.2019.1
685393
https://www.tandfonline.com/doi/mlt/10.1080/15213269.2019.1
685393
http://crossmark.crossref.org/dialog/?doi=10.1080/15213269.20
19.1685393&domain=pdf&date_stamp=2019-11-03
http://crossmark.crossref.org/dialog/?doi=10.1080/15213269.20
19.1685393&domain=pdf&date_stamp=2019-11-03
Everyday functioning-related cognitive correlates of media
multitasking: a mini meta-analysis
Wisnu Wiradhany a and Janneke Koerts b
aDepartment of Experimental Psychology, University of
Groningen, Groningen, the Netherlands;
bDepartment of Clinical and Developmental Neuropsychology,
University of Groningen, Groningen, the
Netherlands
ABSTRACT
A recent meta-analysis has shown that media multitasking beha-
vior, or consuming multiple streams of media simultaneously,
might not be associated with less efficient cognitive processing,
as measured with objective tests. Nevertheless, a growing num-
ber of studies have reported that media multitasking is corre-
lated with cognitive functioning in everyday situations, as
measured in self-reports. Here, in a series of mini meta-
analyses, we show that the self-reported correlates of media
multitasking can be categorized in at least four major themes.
Heavy media multitasking was associated with increasing pro-
blems with attention regulation (e.g., increased mind-wandering
and distractibility), behavior regulation (e.g., emotion
regulation
and self-monitor), inhibition/impulsiveness (e.g., higher level of
impulsiveness and lower level of inhibition), and memory.
However, the pooled effect sizes were small (z =.16 to z = .22),
indicating that a large proportion of variance of media multi-
tasking behavior is still unaccounted for. Additionally, we wit-
nessed a high level of heterogeneity in the attention regulation
theme, which might indicate the presence of the risk of study
bias.
In recent years, the number of studies investigating the
correlates of
habitual media multitasking behavior, i.e., consuming multiple
streams of
media-related information simultaneously, have increased.
These studies
investigate correlates of media multitaskers using both
performance-based
and self-reported measures, and have presented an interesting
contradic-
tion. On the one hand, the group of studies using performance-
based
measures, that is, highly controlled psychophysics experiments
with clear
instructions (e.g., to perform as quickly and as accurately as
possible) and
clear beginning and end, has shown mixed results. Specifically,
some
studies showed that Heavy Media Multitaskers (HMMs),
compared to
Light Media Multitaskers (LMMs) displayed worse performance
in differ-
ent objective, performance-based measures of cognition (Cain,
Leonard,
CONTACT Wisnu Wiradhany [email protected] Department of
Experimental Psychology, University of
Groningen, Grote Kruisstraat 2/1, 9712 TS Groningen, the
Netherlands
MEDIA PSYCHOLOGY
https://doi.org/10.1080/15213269.2019.1685393
© 2019 The Author(s). Published with license by Taylor &
Francis Group, LLC.
This is an Open Access article distributed under the terms of the
Creative Commons Attribution-NonCommercial-NoDerivatives
License (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by-nc-nd/4.0/),
which permits non-commercial re-use, distribution, and
reproduction
in any medium, provided the original work is properly cited,
and is not altered, transformed, or built upon in any way.
http://orcid.org/0000-0001-8707-3146
http://orcid.org/0000-0002-2317-0171
https://crossmark.crossref.org/dialog/?doi=10.1080/15213269.2
019.1685393&domain=pdf&date_stamp=2019-11-02
Gabrieli, & Finn, 2016; Ophir, Nass, & Wagner, 2009; Ralph &
Smilek,
2016), while others reported that HMMs performed better than
LMMs
(Alzahabi & Becker, 2013; Baumgartner, Weeda, van der
Heijden, &
Huizinga, 2014). Yet, others reported mixed findings and/or
null results
(Cardoso-Leite et al., 2015; Gorman & Green, 2016; Minear,
Brasher,
McCurdy, Lewis, & Younggren, 2013; Murphy, McLauchlan, &
Lee, 2017;
Ralph, Thomson, Seli, Carriere, & Smilek, 2015; Wiradhany &
Nieuwenstein, 2017). With this mixed evidence, it is not
surprising that
a recent review (van der Schuur, Baumgartner, Sumter, &
Valkenburg,
2015) and a meta-analysis (Wiradhany & Nieuwenstein, 2017)
have
shown that pooled together, the association between media
multitasking
and performances on performance-based measures of cognition
is weak.
Furthermore, the meta-analysis has shown that upon applying
meta-
analytic correction, the pooled association between media
multitasking
and performances on performance-based measures of cognition
turned
out to be null.
On the other hand, there have been a growing number of studies
showing
associations between frequent media multitasking and problems
reported on
rating scales of cognition. Specifically, frequent media
multitasking has been
associated with more self-reported attention lapses and mind-
wandering
(Ralph, Thomson, Cheyne, & Smilek, 2013), higher levels of
impulsiveness
(Cain et al., 2016; Magen, 2017; Minear et al., 2013;
Sanbonmatsu, Strayer,
Medeiros-Ward, & Watson, 2013; Schutten, Stokes, & Arnell,
2017; Uncapher,
Thieu, & Wagner, 2016), a higher number of problems with
executive functions
(Baumgartner et al., 2014; Magen, 2017), and more (severe)
symptoms of
Attention Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorders or ADHD (Magen,
2017; Uncapher
et al., 2016). Together, these findings suggest that media
multitasking is not
associated with performances on objective measures of
cognition, but never-
theless, is associated with different aspects of everyday
cognitive functioning.
In cognition research, it is common to use both performance-
based and self-
reported methods for assessment as these two types of
assessment often comple-
ment one another (e.g., Chan, Shum, Toulopoulou, & Chen,
2008). At the same
time, findings from both types of measurement might disagree
with one another
for several reasons. To start, the two measures arguably
estimate one’s ability to
function on different levels. Performance-based measures
estimate one’s optimal
performance: These measures have explicit instructions and are
administered
under highly standardized conditions. Accordingly, the results
of these measures
would reflect the efficiency of cognitive processing of an
individual (Stanovich,
2009; Toplak, West, & Stanovich, 2013). In contrast, self-
reported measures of
the same construct estimate one’s typical performance: These
measures probe
a wide range of everyday behaviors which are related with the
construct which is
being estimated. Accordingly, the results of these measures
would reflect the
2 W. WIRADHANY AND J. KOERTS
ability of an individual to execute a task in conditions in which
no explicit
instructions or goals are given (Stanovich, 2009; Toplak et al.,
2013).
Critically, it is possible for an individual to score low in one
type of
measure but high in the other type and vice versa. In this light,
the
International Classification of Functioning, Disability, and
Health (ICF),
which is developed by the World Health Organization (World
Health
Organization, 2001), draws a distinction between functions (i.e.,
the struc-
tural integrity of the body to allow for optimal use) and
activities (i.e., the
life areas, tasks, and actions associated with an individual). One
can have
an impairment on the activity level, but might perform well on
the func-
tional level. For instance, individuals with dysexecutive
symptoms might
report frequent problems in everyday situations, yet they
perform relatively
well in an executive function test (Burgess et al., 2006).
Similarly, impair-
ments on a functional level do not always necessarily result in
impairments
on the activity level due to compensation and adaptation. For
instance,
individuals with mild Alzheimer’s disease might perform poorly
in an
objective test, yet they are able to perform their daily activities
using
support from their environments (Farias, Harrell, Neumann, &
Houtz,
2003). Accordingly, people who frequently media multitask
might not
perform worse in performance-based measures of cognition, yet
report
everyday problems associated with cognition due to the fact that
laboratory
measures might capture some, but not all aspects of cognition or
that they
measure cognition on a different level compared to self-reported
measures.
Correlates of media multitasking
Due to the ubiquity of media devices in recent years (Lenhart,
2015;
Marius & Anggoro, 2014), the frequency and duration of media
multi-
tasking behavior, consuming multiple streams of media
information simul-
taneously, have increased dramatically (Carrier, Cheever,
Rosen, Benitez, &
Chang, 2009; Rideout, Foehr, & Roberts, 2010; Roberts &
Foehr, 2008).
This behavior is mainly characterized by rapid switches of
attention
between different media streams. An observational study of
concurrent
television and computer usage showed that, on average,
participants
switched their attention 120 times within 27.5 min (Brasel &
Gips, 2011).
Similarly, another observational study reported that
contemporary office
workers spent on average 3 min on a task before switching to
another
(González & Mark, 2004). Switching does not only happen
between media
devices, but also between different media activities. For
instance, Judd
(2013) reported from computer session logs that college
students switched
between different tasks in a computer about 70% of the time and
spent on
average 2.3 min on one task before switching to another.
MEDIA PSYCHOLOGY 3
With the high frequency of switching between different media
streams, it is
likely for media multitasking behavior to disrupt other ongoing
cognitive and
behavioral processes. With regard to cognitive processes, media
multitasking
might disrupt one’s current train of thoughts, which may result
in worse task
performance. In a study in which participants were asked to
study an article
about influenza, participants recalled less information about the
article in con-
ditions in which they were either forced to check their Facebook
account or
allowed to check their Facebook account while studying the
article (Kononova,
Joo, & Yuan, 2016). Other studies have shown that media-
induced interruptions
might have no significant impact on task performance (Fox,
Rosen, & Crawford,
2009; Mark, Gudith, & Klocke, 2008), but nevertheless, people
who experienced
constant interruptions during work reported more stress and
frustration at the
end of the day (Mark et al., 2008). With regard to behavioral
processes, media
multitasking behavior might disrupt other everyday behavior
patterns. For
instance, adolescents who reported higher level of media
multitasking also
reported having fewer hours of sleep per night (Calamaro,
Mason, & Ratcliffe,
2009). Similarly, in a longitudinal study, adolescents with a
higher level of media
multitasking reported more sleeping problems at the time of the
data collection,
3 months, and 6 months later (van der Schuur, Baumgartner,
Sumter, &
Valkenburg, 2018).
The current study
Media multitasking behavior might interfere with different
ongoing processes in
everyday situations. This behavior might not be correlated with
performances
on objective measures of cognition (van der Schuur et al., 2015;
Wiradhany &
Nieuwenstein, 2017), but nevertheless, it might have profound
impact on every-
day cognitive functioning, as indicated by self-reported
measures of cognition.
This article aims to examine and summarize the current body of
literature on
media multitasking in order to create an overview of the
different domains of
everyday cognitive functioning which might be correlated with
media multi-
tasking behavior. The evidence was synthesized in a series of
mini meta-analyses.
Additionally, we also examined the risk of bias across the
findings and per-
formed a moderator analysis if risk of bias occurred.
Methods
Study selection
All studies which investigated correlates of self-reported
measures of media multi-
tasking and cognition were considered for inclusion. Studies
were identified in the
PsycInfo, ERIC, MEDLINE, SocINDEX, and CMMC databases,
as well as
the Directory of Open Access Journals (DOAJ) database. A
combination of the
4 W. WIRADHANY AND J. KOERTS
following keywords was entered in the search terms: media
multitask* AND
(problem* OR executive* OR impuls* OR attention*)1.
Together, the search yielded
130 results from the first set of databases and 68 results from
the DOAJ database.
As Figure 1 shows, of the 198 studies identified, 40 were
duplicates and
therefore removed. Of the 158 studies, only 43 pertained to the
term “media
multitasking” (i.e., not only pertained to “media” or
“multitasking” exclu-
sively) and were therefore considered for further screening. Of
43 studies
screened, we removed studies which did not meet the criteria
below.
First, studies must have examined the association between
measures of
media multitasking and self-report measures of cognition, or
psychological
traits or mental-health issues related to cognition. Therefore,
four review
articles (Aagaard, 2015; Carrier, Rosen, Cheever, & Lim, 2015;
Lin, 2009; van
der Schuur et al., 2015), two meta-analysis (Jeong & Hwang,
2016;
Figure 1. A flow diagram showing the selection of study
process.
MEDIA PSYCHOLOGY 5
Wiradhany & Nieuwenstein, 2017), one measurement validity
article
(Baumgartner, Lemmens, Weeda, & Huizinga, 2017), 12 articles
which only
included laboratory task performance measures (Alzahabi &
Becker, 2013;
Alzahabi, Becker, & Hambrick, 2017; Cain & Mitroff, 2011;
Edwards & Shin,
2017; Gorman & Green, 2016; Lui & Wong, 2012; Moisala et
al., 2016;
Murphy et al., 2017; Ophir et al., 2009; Ralph & Smilek, 2016;
Ralph et al.,
2015; Yap & Lim, 2013), two articles in which the level of
media multitasking
was manipulated (Kazakova, Cauberghe, Pandelaere, & De
Pelsmacker, 2015;
Lin, Robertson, & Lee, 2009), one article in which only a brain
imaging
measure was used (Loh & Kanai, 2014) and two articles in
which only media
multitasking behavior was observed (Loh, Tan, & Lim, 2016;
Rigby, Brumby,
Gould, & Cox, 2017) were excluded from further eligibility
assessment.
Second, since this study pertains to general media multitasking
behavior
(i.e., not a specific combination of two media), only studies
using a general
media multitasking measure were included. Therefore, one
article in which
only a specific combination of media multitasking was used
(Kononova,
Zasorina, Diveeva, Kokoeva, & Chelokyan, 2014) and one
article (Wu,
2017) which measured the perception of media multitasking
ability instead
of actual media multitasking frequency were removed. Thirdly,
we removed
two articles that measured the association between media
multitasking and
well-being or constructs which are related to well-being
(Hatchel, Negriff, &
Subrahmanyam, 2018; Pea et al., 2012). Lastly, one article was
excluded since
the relevant effect sizes could not be extracted from the
published article
(Shih, 2013)2. In all, a total of 13 articles containing 15
independent studies3
were included for synthesis (Baumgartner, van der Schuur et al.,
2017, 2014;
Cain et al., 2016; Cardoso-Leite et al., 2015; Duff, Yoon, Wang,
& Anghelcev,
2014; Hadlington & Murphy, 2018; Magen, 2017; Minear et al.,
2013; Ralph
et al., 2013; Sanbonmatsu et al., 2013; Schutten et al., 2017;
Uncapher et al.,
2016; Yang & Zhu, 2016). Table 1 shows the measures of self-
reported
functioning included in each study and the number of
participants assessed.
Effect size selection and calculation
Effect sizes were selected from reported outcome measures
which reflect
distinguishable constructs. For instance, a study examining the
association
between media multitasking and measures of executive function
would
report measures of attentional shifting, working memory, and
inhibition,
which are separate constructs. Study findings related to these
measures
would be regarded as individual effect sizes. In total, 48 unique
effect sizes
were extracted from the studies listed in Table 1 and included in
the final
series of mini meta-analysis.
Effect sizes were calculated in Fisher’s z, indicating the
normalized correla-
tion coefficients between self-reported measures of media
multitasking and
6 W. WIRADHANY AND J. KOERTS
Ta
b
le
1.
O
ve
rv
ie
w
of
in
cl
ud
ed
st
ud
ie
s
in
m
et
a-
an
al
ys
is
,i
n
cl
ud
in
g
th
e
n
um
b
er
of
p
ar
ti
ci
p
an
ts
,a
n
d
th
e
m
ea
su
re
s
of
se
lf-
re
p
or
te
d
fu
n
ct
io
n
in
g
us
ed
in
ea
ch
st
ud
y.
A
ut
h
or
s
(y
ea
r)
N
to
ta
l
M
ea
su
re
(s
)
of
se
lf-
re
p
or
te
d
fu
n
ct
io
n
in
g
Sa
m
p
le
d
es
cr
ip
ti
on
Ba
um
g
ar
tn
er
et
al
.
(2
01
4)
52
3
Be
h
av
io
r
Ra
ti
n
g
In
ve
n
to
ry
of
Ex
ec
ut
iv
e
Fu
n
ct
io
n
s
(B
RI
EF
):
W
or
ki
n
g
M
em
or
y,
In
h
ib
it
io
n
,a
n
d
Sh
ift
in
g
su
b
sc
al
es
48
%
fe
m
al
es
,
M
ag
e
=
13
.0
9
Ba
um
g
ar
tn
er
,
va
n
d
er
Sc
h
uu
r
et
al
.
(2
01
7,
St
ud
y
1,
w
av
e
1)
12
41
In
at
te
n
ti
ve
n
es
s
sc
al
e-
b
as
ed
on
D
SM
-V
cr
it
er
ia
fo
r
A
D
H
D
49
%
fe
m
al
es
,
M
ag
e
=
12
.6
1*
Ba
um
g
ar
tn
er
,
va
n
d
er
Sc
h
uu
r
et
al
.
(2
01
7,
St
ud
y
1,
w
av
e
2)
12
16
In
at
te
n
ti
ve
n
es
s
sc
al
e-
b
as
ed
on
D
SM
-V
cr
it
er
ia
fo
r
A
D
H
D
49
%
fe
m
al
es
,
M
ag
e
=
12
.6
1*
Ba
um
g
ar
tn
er
,
va
n
d
er
Sc
h
uu
r
et
al
.
(2
01
7,
St
ud
y
1,
w
av
e
3)
11
03
In
at
te
n
ti
ve
n
es
s
sc
al
e-
b
as
ed
on
D
SM
-V
cr
it
er
ia
fo
r
A
D
H
D
49
%
fe
m
al
es
,
M
ag
e
=
12
.6
1*
Ba
um
g
ar
tn
er
,
va
n
d
er
Sc
h
uu
r
et
al
.
(2
01
7,
St
ud
y
2,
w
av
e
1)
10
83
In
at
te
n
ti
ve
n
es
s
sc
al
e-
b
as
ed
on
D
SM
-V
cr
it
er
ia
fo
r
A
D
H
D
-
Ba
um
g
ar
tn
er
,
va
n
d
er
Sc
h
uu
r
et
al
.
(2
01
7,
St
ud
y
2,
w
av
e
2)
93
9
In
at
te
n
ti
ve
n
es
s
sc
al
e-
b
as
ed
on
D
SM
-V
cr
it
er
ia
fo
r
A
D
H
D
-
Ba
um
g
ar
tn
er
,
va
n
d
er
Sc
h
uu
r
et
al
.
(2
01
7,
St
ud
y
2,
w
av
e
3)
43
9
In
at
te
n
ti
ve
n
es
s
sc
al
e-
b
as
ed
on
D
SM
-V
cr
it
er
ia
fo
r
A
D
H
D
59
%
fe
m
al
es
,
M
ag
e
=
14
.3
7
C
ai
n
et
al
.
(2
01
6)
70
D
om
ai
n
-s
p
ec
ifi
c
im
p
ul
si
vi
ty
in
sc
h
oo
l-
ag
e
ch
ild
re
n
(D
iS
C
)
49
.3
1%
fe
m
al
es
,
M
ag
e
=
14
.4
C
ar
d
os
o-
Le
it
e
et
al
.
(2
01
5)
60
C
og
n
it
iv
e
Fa
ilu
re
Q
ue
st
io
n
n
ai
re
(C
FQ
),
A
tt
en
ti
on
D
ef
ic
it
/H
yp
er
ac
ti
vi
ty
D
is
or
d
er
Se
lf-
Re
p
or
t
Sc
al
e
(A
D
H
D
-A
SR
S)
13
.3
3%
fe
m
al
es
,
M
ag
e
=
20
.6
8
D
uf
f
et
al
.
(2
01
4,
St
ud
y,
p
.
1)
30
8
C
og
n
it
iv
e
Fa
ilu
re
s
Q
ue
st
io
n
n
ai
re
(C
FQ
),
Pe
rs
on
al
C
on
tr
ol
Sc
al
e
(P
C
S)
,
Br
ie
f
Se
n
sa
ti
on
-s
ee
ki
n
g
Sc
al
e
(B
-S
SS
)
58
.1
2%
fe
m
al
es
,
M
ag
e
=
20
.3
7
D
uf
f
et
al
.
(2
01
4,
St
ud
y,
p
.
2)
50
1
C
og
n
it
iv
e
Fa
ilu
re
s
Q
ue
st
io
n
n
ai
re
(C
FQ
),
Pe
rs
on
al
C
on
tr
ol
Sc
al
e
(P
C
S)
,
Br
ie
f
Se
n
sa
ti
on
-s
ee
ki
n
g
Sc
al
e
(B
-S
SS
)
51
.0
9%
fe
m
al
es
,
M
ag
e
=
34
.4
3
H
ad
lin
g
to
n
an
d
M
ur
p
h
y
(2
01
8)
14
4
Ri
sk
y
C
yb
er
se
cu
ri
ty
Be
h
av
io
r
(R
cS
B)
,
C
og
n
it
iv
e
Fa
ilu
re
Q
ue
st
io
n
n
ai
re
(C
FQ
)
77
.7
7%
fe
m
al
es
,
M
ag
e
=
20
.6
3
M
ag
en
(2
01
7)
19
6
Be
h
av
io
r
Ra
ti
n
g
In
ve
n
to
ry
of
Ex
ec
ut
iv
e
Fu
n
ct
io
n
s
(B
RI
EF
):
al
l
su
b
sc
al
es
,
A
tt
en
ti
on
D
ef
ic
it
/
H
yp
er
ac
ti
vi
ty
D
is
or
d
er
Se
lf-
Re
p
or
t
Sc
al
e
(A
D
H
D
-A
SR
S)
74
%
fe
m
al
es
,
M
ag
e
=
23
.4
4
M
in
ea
r
et
al
.
(2
01
3)
22
1
Ba
rr
at
t
Im
p
ul
si
ve
n
es
s
Sc
al
e
(B
IS
)
68
.3
2%
fe
m
al
es
,
M
ag
e
=
19
.8
Ra
lp
h
et
al
.
(2
01
3)
20
2
M
in
d
fu
l
A
tt
en
ti
on
A
w
ar
en
es
s
Sc
al
e
–
La
p
se
s
O
n
ly
(M
A
A
S-
LO
),
A
tt
en
ti
on
-r
el
at
ed
C
og
n
it
iv
e
Er
ro
rs
Sc
al
e
(A
RC
ES
),
M
em
or
y
Fa
ilu
re
s
Sc
al
e
(M
FS
),
M
in
d
W
an
d
er
in
g
-S
p
on
ta
n
eo
us
(M
W
-S
),
M
in
d
W
an
d
er
in
g
-D
el
ib
er
at
e
(M
W
-D
),
A
tt
en
ti
on
al
C
on
tr
ol
-S
w
it
ch
in
g
(A
C
-S
),
A
tt
en
ti
on
al
C
on
tr
ol
-
D
is
tr
ac
ti
b
ili
ty
(A
C
-D
)
72
.2
8%
fe
m
al
es
,
un
d
er
g
ra
d
ua
te
st
ud
en
ts
Sa
n
b
on
m
at
su
et
al
.
(2
01
3)
27
7
Ba
rr
at
t
Im
p
ul
si
ve
n
es
s
Sc
al
e
(B
IS
),
Se
n
sa
ti
on
-s
ee
ki
n
g
Sc
al
e
(S
SS
)
56
.7
7%
fe
m
al
es
,
M
ed
ia
n
ag
e
=
21
Sc
h
ut
te
n
et
al
.
(2
01
7)
30
3
Ba
rr
at
t
Im
p
ul
si
ve
n
es
s
Sc
al
e
(B
IS
)
83
.2
3%
fe
m
al
es
,
M
ag
e
=
19
.6
3
U
n
ca
p
h
er
et
al
.
(2
01
6)
13
9
Ba
rr
at
t
Im
p
ul
si
ve
n
es
s
Sc
al
e
(B
IS
),
A
tt
en
ti
on
D
ef
ic
it
/H
yp
er
ac
ti
vi
ty
D
is
or
d
er
Se
lf-
Re
p
or
t
Sc
al
e
(A
D
H
D
-
A
SR
S)
58
.0
4%
fe
m
al
es
,
M
ag
e
=
22
.1
Ya
n
g
an
d
Zh
u
(2
01
6)
31
0
Ba
rr
at
t
Im
p
ul
si
ve
n
es
s
Sc
al
e
(B
IS
),
Br
ie
f
Se
n
sa
ti
on
-s
ee
ki
n
g
Sc
al
e
(B
-S
SS
)
49
.3
5%
fe
m
al
es
,
M
ag
e
=
15
.3
*T
h
e
se
x
p
ro
p
or
ti
on
an
d
M
ea
n
of
ag
e
re
fe
rs
to
th
e
co
m
b
in
ed
sa
m
p
le
s
of
St
ud
y
1
ac
ro
ss
th
e
th
re
e
st
ud
y
w
av
es
.
MEDIA PSYCHOLOGY 7
self-reported measures of cognition. A positive z indicates that
frequent
media multitasking is associated with more (severe) issues and a
negative
z indicates that frequent media multitasking is associated with
less (severe)
issues. In most cases, the included studies reported Pearson’s
product-
moment correlations (r) as measures of effect sizes. These r’s
were converted
into Fisher’s z using formula 1 below (Borenstein, Hedges,
Higgins, &
Rothstein, 2009):
z ¼ 0:5 � ln 1 þ r
1 � r
� �
(1)
In which r is the Pearson’s product-moment correlation.
Analysis
Categorization of findings
Since different studies featured in the meta-analysis and the
featured rating
scales measured different domains of cognition, we grouped the
respective
effect sizes into different categories based on the similarity and
dissimilarity
between constructs. To illustrate, the Mindful Attention
Awareness Scale
(MAAS; Brown & Ryan, 2003) and the self-monitoring
subscales of the
Behavioral Ratings of Executive Functions (BRIEF; Gioia,
Isquith, Guy, &
Kenworthy, 2000; Gioia, Isquith, Retzlaff, & Espy, 2002) infer
a relatively
similar construct related to attention regulation, which is
relatively dissimilar
to the construct related to forming precise information in
memory inferred
by the Memory Failures Scale (MFS; Carriere, Cheyne, &
Smilek, 2008).
To guide the categorization of our effect sizes, we first made a
table of self-
report measures in Table 1 along with the goal of each
individual measure
and some examples of its items. Considering the goal of the
measure and its
items, we then looked into the literature to gain insights on how
to group
them in a meaningful way. For instance, measures related to
impulsiveness
such as the Barratt Impulsiveness Scale (BIS; Patton, Stanford,
& Barratt,
1995), measures related to inhibition such as the BRIEF-
Inhibition (Gioia
et al., 2002), and measures related to sensation seeking such as
the Sensation-
Seeking Scale (SSS; Zuckerman, 1996) were group together
under the over-
arching construct of impulsiveness/inhibition (e.g., Dalley,
Everitt, &
Robbins, 2011). Some measures, such as the ADHD-Adult Self
Report
Scale (ADHD-ASRS; Kessler et al., 2005) and the Cognitive
Failure
Questionnaire (CFQ; Broadbent & Cooper, 1982) might have
two or more
distinct underlying constructs; the former has attention and
impulsiveness
components and the latter has attention, memory, and other
components.
To ensure that our resulting categories were as independent
from each other
as they can be, in the case of the ADHD-ASRS, we contacted
the authors to
request additional data regarding the correlation between media
8 W. WIRADHANY AND J. KOERTS
multitasking and attention deficit and between media
multitasking and
hyperactivity/impulsiveness separately. In the case of the CFQ,
considering
that studies, especially recent ones were in disagreement with
regard to the
underlying dimensions of CFQ (Bridger, Johnsen, & Brasher,
2013; Larson,
Alderton, Neideffer, & Underhill, 2011; Wallace, Kass, &
Stanny, 2002), we
decided to categorize the effect sizes pertained to the general
CFQ scores
twice: once in the attention regulation category and once in the
memory
category. For all categories, the first author performed the
categorizations
and the second author checked the resulted categories.
Disagreements
between authors were resolved by consensus.
Using the categorization processes above, we identified four
different
themes for correlates between media multitasking and self-
reports of every-
day cognitive functioning, namely attention regulation, behavior
regulation,
impulsiveness/inhibition, and memory. The attention regulation
theme per-
tained to the set of cognitive abilities which help to boost
information
processing. Traditionally, this includes the ability to react to
important cues
in the environment (alerting of attention), select relevant from
irrelevant
information (orienting of attention), and switch from one
stimulus-response
task rule to another (executive attention; Petersen & Posner,
2012; Posner &
Petersen, 1990). More recently, our ability to suppress
internally generated
task-unrelated thoughts (Christoff, Irving, Fox, Spreng, &
Andrews-Hanna,
2016; Smallwood & Schooler, 2006) was also considered in this
array of
cognitive processing-boosting ability. Accordingly, in this
theme, we
included measures of attention orientation/selection (e.g.,
ADHD-ASRS –
Inattention subscale), distractibility (e.g., CFQ – distractibility
subscale and
CFQ – Total), switching from one task to another (e.g., BRIEF
– Shifting
subscale), and mind-wandering (e.g., MAAS, Mind-Wandering
scale).
The impulsiveness/inhibition theme pertained to the ability to
inhibit
premature thoughts and actions, and difficulties with this ability
can be
exhibited behaviorally in one’s tendency to seek additional
stimulations and
take risks (Dalley et al., 2011; Dalley & Robbins, 2017). Here,
we included
measures which were related to inhibition (BRIEF-Inhibition),
behavior
impulsiveness (e.g., BIS, ADHD-ASRS –
Hyperactivity/impulsivity subscale),
and sensation-seeking (e.g., SSS).
The memory theme pertained to the ability to retain information
do
mental work with information in memory (e.g., Diamond, 2013).
This ability
has been considered to be relatively independent to attentional
regulation, yet
it has been considered to play an important role in executive
functioning
(Diamond, 2013; Engle, 2002; Engle & Kane, 2004). In this
meta-analysis, this
theme included measures of working memory (e.g., BRIEF –
Working
memory subscale), memory failures (e.g., Memory Failures
Scale), and gen-
eral cognitive failures (e.g., CFQ – Total score).
MEDIA PSYCHOLOGY 9
Lastly, the behavior regulation theme pertained to the set of
abilities which
is related to the volitional control of action. According to one
taxonomy of
executive function (BRIEF; Gioia et al., 2002; Huizinga &
Smidts, 2010),
behavior regulation is an umbrella term which includes task-
switching and
inhibition as well. However, as discussed above, task-switching
appears to be
more related to attention regulation (e.g., Petersen & Posner,
2012) while
inhibition, which is more internally driven, appears to be more
related to
impulsiveness and risk-taking (e.g., Dalley et al., 2011).
Accordingly, we
categorized task-switching and inhibition in the attention
regulation and
impulsiveness/inhibition categories, respectively. Thus, what
remains in our
behavior regulation theme were abilities that relate to volitional
control of
action which are driven by external demands and situational
factors (e.g., see
Tsukayama, Duckworth, & Kim, 2013). This theme included
measures of
self-control (e.g., Domain-specific Impulsivity in School-age
Children4;
Personal Problem-solving Inventory – Self-control subscale),
emotion regu-
lation (e.g., BRIEF – Emotion regulation subscale), and self-
monitoring (e.g.,
BRIEF – Self monitoring subscale).
Note that while we sought out to minimize overlaps between the
themes and
categorize the findings as accurately as possible, the
categorization remained
somewhat arbitrary as different theoretical models of cognitive
function would
have both overlaps and distinctions of different sets of
cognitive ability (e.g., see
Chan et al., 2008). For each theme, a random-effect model and a
pooled effect
size were calculated to provide estimates of the magnitude of
the correlation.
Random-effect model
Since the current meta-analysis featured different rating scales
and outcome
measures, we constructed a random-effect model to estimate the
pooled
effect size. This model assumes that the different scales had
comparable,
but not identical effect sizes which are distributed around some
mean that
reflected the true effect (Borenstein et al., 2009). In our case,
we assumed that
the different outcomes measured different subsets of cognitive
functioning.
Thus, the effects might vary from one function to another.
The random-effect model was constructed in R (R Core team,
2015) using
the metafor package (Viechtbauer, 2010). To account for
variance inflation of
the pooled effect size due to the dependency of multiple
outcome measures
from one study, we calculated the robust variance estimation
(RVE; Hedges,
Tipton, & Johnson, 2010). RVE works by estimating the
correlations between
dependent outcome measures and adjusting the standard error of
the pooled
effect size based on these correlations (Hedges et al., 2010;
Scammacca,
Roberts, & Stuebing, 2013).
10 W. WIRADHANY AND J. KOERTS
Heterogeneity and risk of bias
When significant between-studies heterogeneity was detected,
we performed
a moderator analysis and a risk of bias analysis. The moderator
analysis
assesses whether the between-studies heterogeneity can be
explained by
shared characteristics of different subgroups of studies (Hedges
& Pigott,
2004).
The risk of bias analysis tested whether the heterogeneity was
stemming
from bias coming from the level of precision in each study.
Under a presence
of bias, it is common for studies with smaller sample sizes to
show an
overestimation of effect sizes due to sampling errors compared
with studies
with bigger sample sizes, a phenomenon called small-study
effect (Sterne,
Gavaghan, & Egger, 2000). A small-study effect might indicate
the presence
of publication bias, since other studies with smaller sample
sizes showing
underestimation of the effect ended up not being published
(Ioannidis, 2005;
Ioannidis, Munafò, Fusar-Poli, Nosek, & David, 2014). As a
formal inspec-
tion of small-study effects, we conducted an Egger’s test
(Egger, Davey Smith,
Schneider, & Minder, 1997), in which a simple linear regression
with effect
sizes as a measure of magnitude of study effect and sample
sizes or standard
errors as measures of study precision is constructed.
Results
Attention regulation
Random-effect model
Figure 2 shows a forest plot for a group of self-report scales
which measured
the association between media multitasking and constructs
related to the
ability to regulate attention. The scales categorized in this
theme included
Attentional Control (AC)-switching (e.g., “I am slow to switch
from one task
to another,” Carriere, Seli, & Smilek, 2013), Attentional
Control (AC)-
distractibility (e.g., “I have difficulties concentrating when
there is music in
the room around me”, Carriere et al., 2013), ADHD-ASRS –
Inattention (e.g.,
“How often do you have difficulty concentrating on what people
are saying
to you even when they are speaking to you directly?”, Kessler et
al., 2005),
MW – Spontaneous (e.g., “I find my thoughts wandering
spontaneously”,
Carriere et al., 2008), MW – Deliberate (e.g., “I allow my
thoughts to wander
on purpose”, Carriere et al., 2008), MAAS – Lapses only (e.g.,
“I do jobs or
tasks automatically, without being aware of what I’m doing”,
Brown & Ryan,
2003), ARCES (e.g.,“I have gone to the fridge to get one thing
(e.g., milk) and
taken something else (e.g., juice),” Carriere et al., 2008),
BRIEF-Shift (e.g., “I
get stuck on one topic or activity,” Gioia et al., 2002); CFQ –
Distractibility,
also with CFQ – total score (e.g., “Do you read something and
find you
MEDIA PSYCHOLOGY 11
haven’t been thinking about it and must read it again?,”
Broadbent &
Cooper, 1982).
Overall, the pooled effect size of the correlates between media
multitasking
and self-reported problems related to attention regulation was
small, yet
statistically significant, z = .162, 95% CI [.160, .164], p < .001.
At the same
time, however, a significant heterogeneity between the effect
sizes was
detected, I2 = 86.76%, Q(21) = 118.93, p < .001.
Heterogeneity & risk of bias analysis
To address the heterogeneity in the model, we performed
moderator analyses
with two moderators. First, we added sex, as indicated by the
proportion of
females in the study samples as a moderator. Second, we added
age, as
indicated by the mean age of the study samples as a moderator.
The two
moderators did not contribute to the unexplained variance in the
model, F(1,
17) = 2.59, p = .125; F(1, 11) = 3.08, p = .107, respectively,
indicating that the
heterogeneity could not be explained by differences in sex, and
age.
As for the risk of bias, the Egger’s test showed no relationship
between
effect size and study precision, z = −1.46, p = .144. This
indicates that under
the presence of heterogeneity, effect sizes were stable across
different studies
with different sample sizes.
Figure 2. Forest plot of the effect sizes (Fisher’s z) for studies
measuring the association between
media multitasking and attention regulation. Error bars indicate
95% confidence intervals of the
means. AC: Attentional Control scale; ADHD-ASRS: Attention
Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder Adult
Self Report Scale; ARCES: Attention-Related Cognitive Error
Scale; BRIEF: Behavior Rating
Inventory of Executive Function; CFQ: Cognitive Failure
Questionnaire; MAAS: Mindful
Awareness Attention Scale; MW: Mind-Wandering scale.
12 W. WIRADHANY AND J. KOERTS
Impulsiveness – inhibition
Random-effect model
Figure 3 shows a forest plot for a group of self-report scales
which measured
the association between media multitasking and constructs
related to impul-
siveness and/or inhibition. The scales categorized in this theme
included
ADHD-ASRS – hyperactivity (e.g., “How often do you fidget or
squirm
with your hands or your feet when you have to sit down for a
long time?”,
Kessler et al., 2005), BIS (e.g. “I do things without thinking”,
Patton et al.,
1995), BRIEF-Inhibit (e.g., “I do not think before doing,” Gioia
et al., 2002),
SSS, also the brief version; B-SSS (e.g., “I sometimes like to do
things that are
a little frightening”, Zuckerman, 1996), and RCsB (e.g.,
“Sharing passwords
with friends and colleagues”).
Overall, the pooled effect size of the correlates between media
multitasking
and self-reported problems related to impulsiveness and/or
inhibition was
small, yet statistically significant, z = .219, 95% CI [.218,
.219], p < .001. The
between-studies heterogeneity was low, I2 < .001%, Q(14) =
9.82, p = .775,
indicating that the effect was consistent across different studies.
Memory
Random-effect model
Figure 4 shows a forest plot for a group of self-report scales
which measured
the association between media multitasking and constructs
related to mem-
ory. The scales categorized in this theme included BRIEF-
Working Memory
Figure 3. Forest plot of the effect sizes (Fisher’s z) for studies
measuring the association between
media multitasking and impulsiveness and/or inhibition. Error
bars indicate 95% confidence
intervals of the means. ADHD-ASRS: Attention
Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder – Adult Self Report
Scales; B-SSS: Brief Sensation-Seeking Scale; BIS: Barratt
Impulsiveness Scale; BRIEF: Behavior
Rating Inventory of Executive Function; RCsB: Risky
Cybersecurity Behavior scale; SSS: Sensation-
seeking Scale.
MEDIA PSYCHOLOGY 13
(e.g., “I have trouble remembering things, even for a few
minutes,” Gioia
et al., 2002), CFQ (e.g., “Do you find you forget people’s
names?”, Broadbent
& Cooper, 1982), and MFS (e.g., “I forget what I went to the
supermarket to
buy”, Carriere et al., 2008).
Overall, the pooled effect size of the correlates between media
multitasking
and self-reported problems related to memory was small, yet
statistically
significant, z = .158, 95% CI [.156, .161], p < .001. The
between-studies
heterogeneity was low, I2 = 16.49%, Q(4) = 4.67, p = .323,
indicating that
the effect was consistent across different studies.
Behavior regulation
Random-effect model
Figure 5 shows a forest plot for a group of self-report scales
which measured
the association between media multitasking and constructs
related to the
ability to regulate behavior. The scales categorized in this
theme included
Emotional Control (e.g. “Has outburst for little reason”, Gioia
et al., 2002),
BRIEF-Initiate (e.g., “I need to be told to begin a task even
when willing”),
Figure 4. Forest plot of the effect sizes (Fisher’s z) for studies
measuring the association between
media multitasking and memory. Error bars indicate 95%
confidence intervals of the means.
BRIEF: Behavior Rating Inventory of Executive Function; CFQ:
Cognitive Failures Questionnaire;
MFS: Memory Failures Scale.
Figure 5. Forest plot of the effect sizes (Fisher’s z) for studies
measuring the association between
media multitasking and behavior regulation. Error bars indicate
95% confidence intervals of the
means. BRIEF: Behavior Rating Inventory of Executive
Function; DISC: Domain-specific Impulsivity
in School-age Children; PPSI: Personal Problem-solving
Inventory.
14 W. WIRADHANY AND J. KOERTS
BRIEF-Organization of Materials (e.g., “I cannot find things in
room or
school desk”), Plan/Organize, (e.g., “I become overwhelmed by
large assign-
ments”), BRIEF-Self-monitor (e.g. “I am unaware of how my
behavior affects
or bothers others”), BRIEF-Task-Monitor (e.g., “I make careless
errors”),
DiSC (e.g., “I interrupted other people” Tsukayama et al.,
2013), and PPSI-
Personal control (e.g., “Sometimes I do not stop and take time
to deal with
my problems, but just kind of muddle ahead” Heppner &
Petersen, 1982).
Overall, the pooled effect size of the correlates between media
multitasking
and self-reported problems related to behavior regulation was
small, yet
statistically significant, z = .192, 95% CI [.190, .193], p < .001.
The between-
studies heterogeneity was low, I2 = 6.67%, Q(8) = 5.69, p =
.683, indicating
that the effect was consistent across different studies.
General discussion
In this meta-analysis, we examined the correlates of media
multitasking
behavior with different domains of everyday cognitive
functioning in a series
of mini meta-analysis. The effect sizes were categorized into
different themes
reflecting different domains of everyday cognitive functioning,
based on the
similarities and dissimilarities between the reflected constructs.
Overall, the
effect sizes can be categorized into four distinct themes. Pooled
together,
frequent media multitasking had weak, but significant
associations with
a decrease of attention regulation (z = .16), lower levels of
inhibition/higher
levels of impulsiveness (z = .22), an increase of memory
problems (z = .16), and
a decreased behavior regulation (z = .19).
Regarding the association between media multitasking and
attention reg-
ulation, we found that heavy media multitasking was associated
with higher
frequency of mind-wandering, higher distractibility, and more
problems with
task switching. With regard to mind-wandering, this finding was
somewhat
consistent with other findings in the literature which used
objective mea-
sures. In an experiment in which participants were asked to
memorize
materials from a video-recorded lecture, Loh et al. (2016) found
that heavy
media multitaskers retained less information from the lecture,
and this effect
could be explained by the increased tendency to mind-wander in
this group.
However, at least one study showed a null correlation between
media multi-
tasking and mind-wandering: heavy media multitaskers
performed worse in
a metronome-response task, but they did not show a tendency to
have
increased mind-wandering during the experiment (Ralph et al.,
2015). With
regard to distractibility, other studies which used objective
measures showed
that, for instance, heavy media multitaskers were less able to
filter out
irrelevant information from their immediate environment (Cain
& Mitroff,
2011; Ophir et al., 2009). However, a recent meta-analysis
(Wiradhany &
Nieuwenstein, 2017) has shown that other studies have failed to
replicate this
MEDIA PSYCHOLOGY 15
finding. Lastly, with regard to task-switching, other studies
using objective
tests showed mixed evidence for the correlation between media
multitasking
and task switching. Some studies found a negative correlation
between media
multitasking and task performance (Ophir et al., 2009;
Wiradhany &
Nieuwenstein, 2017, Exp., p. 1), others found positive
correlations
(Alzahabi & Becker, 2013), yet others found null results (see
Wiradhany &
Nieuwenstein, 2017, for a meta-analysis). Thus, it can be said
that while
frequent media multitasking is associated with more problems
with atten-
tional control in everyday situations, media multitasking might
not directly
influence one’s ability to regulate attention, as measured by
objective tests,
per se.
Heavier media multitasking was associated with increased
impulsiveness/
decreased inhibition; heavier media multitaskers were
associated with higher
scores in impulsiveness traits and they reported more (severe)
symptoms of
hyperactivity/impulsivity. Heavier media multitasking was also
associated
with higher scores in other traits which are related to
impulsiveness, such
as sensation-seeking and risk-taking (Dalley et al., 2011;
Whiteside & Lynam,
2001). This finding was consistent with findings using objective
measures.
For instance, heavy media multitaskers were more likely to
choose smaller,
immediate rewards instead of later, larger ones and they
endorsed intuitive,
but incorrect answers of the Cognitive Reflection Test (Schutten
et al., 2017).
Additionally, another study indicated that HMMs scored lower
in a fluid
intelligence test due to them giving up earlier in the test
(Minear et al., 2013).
Individuals with higher levels of sensation-seeking trait are
characterized by
a higher stimulation threshold for optimal behavioral
performance (Hoyle,
Stephenson, Palmgreen, Lorch, & Donohew, 2002; Zuckerman,
2007) and
a higher likelihood to act prematurely without foresight, which
at times lead
to risk-taking behaviors (Dalley et al., 2011; Hoyle et al., 2002;
Zuckerman,
2007). Indeed, consuming multiple streams of information has
been shown
to promote a higher level of engagement (Bardhi, Rohm, &
Sultan, 2010;
Wang & Tchernev, 2009) and to provide gratifications (Hwang,
Kim, &
Jeong, 2014) which together provide stimulations for those who
seek them.
Accordingly, people with higher levels of sensation-seeking and
risk-taking
might media multitask to seek for additional stimulations.
Heavier media multitasking was associated with increased
problems
related to memory. In this regard, one study using an objective
measure,
namely a change-detection task has shown that HMMs had
difficulties
retaining specific information in working memory, regardless of
the presence
of distractors, and importantly, they performed more poorly in a
later long-
term memory test for both relevant and irrelevant objects
compared to
LMMs (Uncapher et al., 2016; but see Wiradhany, van Vugt, &
Nieuwenstein, 2019 for null results). Other studies have shown
that
HMMs, compared to LMMs performed worse in a complex
working memory
16 W. WIRADHANY AND J. KOERTS
test (Sanbonmatsu et al., 2013) and an N-back test (Cain et al.,
2016; Ophir
et al., 2009; Ralph & Smilek, 2016; but see Cardoso-Leite et al.,
2015;
Wiradhany & Nieuwenstein, 2017 for null results).
Lastly, regarding the association between media multitasking
and behavior
regulation, we found that heavier media multitaskers had more
difficulties to
adjust their thoughts, emotions, and actions to the situational
demands. This
finding is in line with its counterpart in objective tasks, where
studies have
found that HMMs performed worse in tasks in which they have
to respond
to different cue-probe contingencies, such as the AXE-CPT task
(Ophir et al.,
2009; Wiradhany & Nieuwenstein, 2017; but see Cardoso-Leite
et al., 2015
for null results). However, there were mixed findings with
regard to perfor-
mance of HMMs in a change-detection task in which distractor
filtering was
involved, with one study showed that HMMs performed worse
(Ophir et al.,
2009), while other, recent ones showed null findings (Cardoso-
Leite et al.,
2015; Uncapher et al., 2016; Wiradhany & Nieuwenstein, 2017;
Wiradhany
et al., 2019).
Collectively, we witnessed a discrepancy between the findings
in this meta-
analysis and the findings in a previous meta-analysis
(Wiradhany &
Nieuwenstein, 2017). In this meta-analysis, we found overall
weak, but stable-
pooled correlations between media multitasking and self-reports
of cognitive
functioning in everyday situations whereas in the previous
meta-analysis, we
found an overall weak-pooled correlation, but the correlation
became null
upon corrections. This discrepancy, as we previously
mentioned, might exist
for several reasons. First, performance-based measures might
capture some,
but not all aspects of everyday cognitive functioning. Consider
the tests for
one’s ability to regulate attention, for instance. One group of
researcher may
assess attention regulation using the perspective of mind-
wandering to inves-
tigate the waxing and waning of attention (e.g., Christoff et al.,
2016). Yet,
others assess attention regulation using the perspective of
divided attention
(e.g., Moisala et al., 2016). The two perspectives might cover
some, but not all
aspects of attention regulation, and in everyday situations one
might need to
suppress both mind-wandering and distraction to regulate
attention
properly. Second, performance-based measures are often
designed to assess
one’s ability at a pathological level (Chan et al., 2008). For
instance, as
a diagnostic tool to inquire whether one’s ability to regulate
attention is clearly
impaired. Therefore, one interpretation of the weak correlations
across all
themes would be that media multitasking behavior is associated
with the
increased number of everyday problems related to cognition, but
this does
not mean that media multitasking is associated with the
presence of an
impairment in cognitive abilities. The weak correlations
between media multi-
tasking and everyday cognitive functioning as shown in self-
reports suggest
that performance-based measures might not be adequately
sensitive to detect
everyday cognitive problems in media multitaskers.
MEDIA PSYCHOLOGY 17
Notes on causality
Media multitasking behavior might precede, occur as a
consequence, or have
a reciprocal relationship with everyday cognitive functioning.
Currently, this
meta-analysis does not allow for disentangling the causal
relationship
between the two. Preceding problems with cognition, media
multitasking
behavior may promote a specific mode of processing
information in the
environment (Lin, 2009. Specifically, heavy media multitaskers
might develop
a breadth-biased focus of attention, due to constant exposures to
media-
saturated environments. That is, they prefer to skim a large
quantity of
information rather than deeply processing a small amount of
information.
Consequently, adopting this mode of information processing
might lead
media multitaskers to apply cognitive control processes such as
thought-
monitoring and attention regulation less strictly. This might
have profound
consequences. In an fMRI study; Moisala et al. (2016) found
that in addition
to worse task performance in which participants had to attend to
sentences in
one modality (e.g. auditory) while they had to ignore distractor
sentences
presented in another modality (e.g. visual), HMMs, compared to
LMMs also
have higher activations in the right superior and medial frontal
gyri, and the
medial frontal gyrus. Increased activations in these areas have
been linked to,
among others, increased top-down attentional control.
Therefore, heavy
media multitaskers might require more effort in filtering
distracting informa-
tion than light media multitaskers. Alternatively, it could also
be the case that
media multitasking behavior leads to overreliance of exogenous
control of
attention (i.e. from incoming notifications from media; Ralph et
al., 2013).
Consequently, heavy media multitaskers train their endogenous
control less
often and thus, experience more problems related to cognitive
control.
Media multitasking behavior might also occur as a consequence
of existing
problems with cognition. People with ADHD and people with
problems with
behavior regulation and metacognition are more easily
distracted and therefore
have a higher propensity to media multitask. Similarly, people
with high levels
of sensation-seeking are more inclined to media multitask for
stimulation-
seeking purposes. Relatedly, indicating that excessive media
multitasking beha-
vior might be a result from a preexisting condition, studies have
also shown
that individuals with smaller grey matter volumes in the
Anterior Cingulate
Cortex (ACC) – a brain region which has been shown to be
more active during
error and conflict detections (Botvinick, Braver, Barch, Carter,
& Cohen, 2001;
Botvinick, Cohen, & Carter, 2004) – reported higher levels of
media multi-
tasking (Loh & Kanai, 2014). Similarly, the increased
activations of the brain
areas associated with top-down control in heavy media
multitaskers (Moisala
et al., 2016) might also indicate that these areas function less
efficiently in
heavy media multitaskers, compared to light media multitaskers.
18 W. WIRADHANY AND J. KOERTS
Lastly, media multitasking behavior might have a reciprocal
relationship
with problems with cognition and vice versa. On this
reciprocity, several
longitudinal studies have attempted to examine whether media
multitasking
behavior and everyday-related problems are reinforcing each
other over
a longer time period. The results of these studies showed that
media multi-
tasking did not appear to have a reciprocal relationship with the
occurrence
attentional problems (Baumgartner, van der Schuur, Lemmens,
& Te Poel,
2017) 3 and 6 months later. Nevertheless, these studies showed
that the
associations between media multitasking and attentional
problems were
stable over time. That is, the correlation remained significant
during the
first, second, and third periods of data collection. Together, this
might
indicate that individuals have a stable level of media
multitasking behavior
over time and similarly, the occurrence of some everyday-
related problems is
also stable over time.
Limitation and future directions
The findings in our set of mini-meta-analyses are limited in
several ways.
First, while the effects found in different groups of findings
were somewhat
reliable across different studies, critically, the overall pooled
effects were
weak, with z ranging from .16 to .22. Thus, most of the variance
underlying
the media multitasking behavior is still unaccounted for.
Additionally, while
we refer to the literature, our categorization of effect sizes
remained some-
what subjective. This subjectivity might introduce bias and or
contribute to
our level of within-theme heterogeneity. We witnessed a high
level of hetero-
geneity in the attention regulation theme, and this high
heterogeneity could
not be explained by our moderators. Arguably, this high level of
heteroge-
neity might be driven by the effect sizes related to CFQ, which
dimension-
ality is still being argued for in recent studies (e.g., Bridger et
al., 2013). It
might be that the null and negative correlations in the CFQ-
related effect
sizes were driven by the other dimensions of CFQ.
Third, the current meta-analysis focuses on studies pertained to
everyday
cognitive functioning. However, media multitasking studies
have gone
beyond the cognition-related themes, as some studies have
investigated the
correlates between media multitasking and depression (Becker,
Alzahabi, &
Hopwood, 2013), anxiety (Becker et al., 2013; Hatchel et al.,
2018), creativity
and imagination (Duff et al., 2014), and well-being (Pea et al.,
2012; Shih,
2013; Xu, Wang, & David, 2016). With regards to depression
and anxiety,
heavy media multitasking was correlated with more (severe)
depressive and
anxiety symptoms (Becker et al., 2013; Hatchel et al., 2018).
This finding was
somewhat consistent with a recent nation-wide study which also
showed that
individuals who use multiple social media platforms in daily
life had higher
odds of having increased levels of depression and anxiety
(Primack et al.,
MEDIA PSYCHOLOGY 19
2017). Furthermore, media multitasking was negatively
correlated with ima-
gination, but it was positively correlated with creativity (Duff et
al., 2014).
Lastly, the correlations between media multitasking and well-
being were
somewhat mixed. In a large-scale study which involved 3461 8-
12-year-old
girls, Pea et al. (2012) found that media multitasking was
positively corre-
lated with social success, but it was negatively correlated with
normalcy
feelings, positive feelings, and social stress. Shih (2013) found
that media
multitasking was not correlated with well-being as assessed
using two ver-
sions of self-report questionnaires which focused on well-being.
While part
of these findings was discouraging, suggesting that media
multitasking beha-
vior might have potential ramifications on other aspects of
everyday func-
tioning beyond cognition, the other part, namely the positive
correlations
with social success and creativity suggests that media
multitasking behavior
might be beneficial as well. It could be interesting for future
studies to further
examine the adaptive values of everyday media multitasking
behavior, espe-
cially given that several longitudinal studies have indicated that
media multi-
tasking behavior is stable over time (Baumgartner, van der
Schuur et al.,
2017; van der Schuur et al., 2018).
Fourth and lastly, since all findings we synthesized in the meta-
analysis
were correlational, it is still an open question whether media
multitasking
behavior leads to, is an effect, or have a reciprocal relationship
with the
occurrence of cognitive problems in everyday situations.
Futures studies
might be interested in disentangling this association in a more
controlled
manner.
Conclusion
In a series of mini meta-analyses, we categorized the correlates
between
media multitasking and everyday cognitive functioning, as
assessed using
self-reports, in four different themes. Heavier media
multitasking was asso-
ciated with increased of levels of self-reported problems with
attention
regulation, behavior regulation, impulsiveness/inhibition, and
memory.
Together, media multitasking appears to be correlated with
increasing pro-
blems everyday cognitive functioning. However, the overall
small effects were
small, a high level of heterogeneity was detected in one theme,
and a large
proportion of variance of media multitasking behavior is still
unaccounted
for. Additionally, since most studies reported correlations, the
causality
direction is still unclear.
Notes
1. To ensure that all possible relevant results have been
included in the meta-analysis, in
addition to these keywords, we performed a search using more
general keywords,
20 W. WIRADHANY AND J. KOERTS
namely media multitask* AND (cognition OR emotion OR
trait). This search yielded no
additional results. Lists of the references found using our search
terms can be found in
the supplementary materials of this article.
2. The author was contacted for requesting the relevant zero-
order correlations not
reported in the article. Unfortunately, due to unforeseen
circumstances, the original
dataset was no longer available. Nevertheless, we are thankful
to Dr. Shui-I Shih for her
cooperation.
3. Two of the studies (Baumgartner, van der Schuur, et al.,
2017) were longitudinal
studies with three waves each. All study waves were included
(see Table 1).
4. Note that while this measure has impulsivity on its name, the
scale was intended to
measure how an individual may act (e.g., suppressing their
impulse) in situational
contexts (Tsukayama et al., 2013, p. 880). These authors also
proposed a distinction
between domain-specific impulsivity, which is externally driven
and measured by their
scale and domain-general impulsivity, which is more internally
driven and measured by
other impulsivity scales such as the BIS.
Acknowledgments
This article was written as a part of the Ph. D. project of the
first author, which is funded by
the Endowment Fund for Education (LPDP), Ministry of
Finance, the Republic of Indonesia.
We thank Prof. Pedro Cardoso-Leite and Dr. Melina Uncapher
for providing us additional
data regarding their ADHD findings.
Disclosure statement
No potential conflict of interest was reported by the authors.
Funding
This work was supported by the Indonesia Endowment Fund for
Education;
ORCID
Wisnu Wiradhany http://orcid.org/0000-0001-8707-3146
Janneke Koerts http://orcid.org/0000-0002-2317-0171
References
Aagaard, J. (2015). Media multitasking, attention, and
distraction: A critical discussion.
Phenomenology and the Cognitive Sciences, 14(4), 885–896.
doi:10.1007/s11097-014-9375-x
Alzahabi, R., & Becker, M. W. (2013). The association between
media multitasking,
task-switching, and dual-task performance. Journal of
Experimental Psychology: Human
Perception and Performance, 39(5), 1485–1495.
doi:10.1037/a0031208
Alzahabi, R., Becker, M. W., & Hambrick, D. Z. (2017).
Investigating the relationship between
media multitasking and processes involved in task-switching.
Journal of Experimental
Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 43(11), 1872–
1894. doi:10.1037/
xhp0000412
MEDIA PSYCHOLOGY 21
https://doi.org/10.1007/s11097-014-9375-x
https://doi.org/10.1037/a0031208
https://doi.org/10.1037/xhp0000412
https://doi.org/10.1037/xhp0000412
Bardhi, F., Rohm, A. J., & Sultan, F. (2010). Tuning in and
tuning out: Media multitasking
among young consumers. Journal of Consumer Behaviour, 9,
316–332. doi:10.1002/cb
Baumgartner, S. E., Lemmens, J. S., Weeda, W. D., & Huizinga,
M. (2017). Measuring media
multitasking. Journal of Media Psychology, 29, 188–197.
doi:10.1027/1864-1105/a000167
Baumgartner, S. E., van der Schuur, W. A., Lemmens, J. S., &
Te Poel, F. (2017). The
relationship between media multitasking and attention problems
in adolescents: Results
of two longitudinal studies. Human Communication Research,
1–27. doi:10.1111/
hcre.12111
Baumgartner, S. E., Weeda, W. D., van der Heijden, L. L., &
Huizinga, M. (2014). The
relationship between media multitasking and executive function
in early adolescents. The
Journal of Early Adolescence, 34(8), 1120–1144.
doi:10.1177/0272431614523133
Becker, M. W., Alzahabi, R., & Hopwood, C. J. (2013). Media
multitasking is associated with
symptoms of depression and social anxiety. Cyberpsychology,
Behavior and Social
Networking, 16(2), 132–135. doi:10.1089/cyber.2012.0291
Borenstein, M., Hedges, L. V., Higgins, J. P. T., & Rothstein,
H. R. (2009). Introduction to
meta-analysis. West Sussex, UK: John Wiley & Sons.
Botvinick, M. M., Braver, Barch, D. M., Carter, C. S., & Cohen,
J. D. (2001). Conflict
monitoring and cognitive control. Psychological Review,
108(3), 624–652. doi:10.1037//
0033-295X.I08.3.624
Botvinick, M. M., Cohen, J. D., & Carter, C. S. (2004). Conflict
monitoring and anterior
cingulate cortex: An update. Trends in Cognitive Sciences,
8(12), 539–546. doi:10.1016/j.
tics.2004.10.003
Brasel, S. A., & Gips, J. (2011). Media multitasking behavior:
Concurrent television and
computer usage. Cyberpsychology, Behavior and Social
Networking, 14(9), 527–534.
doi:10.1089/cyber.2010.0350
Bridger, R. S., Johnsen, S. Å. K., & Brasher, K. (2013).
Psychometric properties of the cognitive
failures questionnaire†. Ergonomics, 56(10), 1515–1524.
doi:10.1080/00140139.2013.821172
Broadbent, D., & Cooper, P. (1982). The cognitive failures
questionnaire (CFQ) and its
correlates. British Journal of Clinical Psychology, 21(1), 1–16.
Retrieved from http://online
library.wiley.com/doi/10.1111/j.2044-8260.1982.tb01421.x/full
Brown, K. W., & Ryan, R. M. (2003). The benefits of being
present: Mindfulness and its role
in psychological well-being. Jounal of Personality and Social
Psychology, 84(4), 822–848.
doi:10.1037/0022-3514.84.4.822
Burgess, P. W., Alderman, N., Forbes, C., Costello, A., Coates,
L. M., Dawson, D. R., … Channon, S.
(2006). The case for the development and use of “ecologically
valid” measures of executive
function in experimental and clinical neuropsychology. Journal
of the International
Neuropsychological Society : JINS, 12(2), 194–209.
doi:10.1017/S1355617706060310
Cain, M. S., Leonard, J. A., Gabrieli, J. D. E., & Finn, A. S.
(2016). Media multitasking in
adolescence. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 23(6), 1932–
1941. doi:10.3758/s13423-016-
1036-3
Cain, M. S., & Mitroff, S. R. (2011). Distractor filtering in
media multitaskers. Perception, 40
(10), 1183–1192. doi:10.1068/p7017
Calamaro, C. J., Mason, T. B. A., & Ratcliffe, S. J. (2009).
Adolescents living the 24/7 lifestyle:
Effects of caffeine and technology on sleep duration and
daytime functioning. Pediatrics,
123(6), e1005–e1010. doi:10.1542/peds.2008-3641
Cardoso-Leite, P., Kludt, R., Vignola, G., Ma, W. J., Green, C.
S., & Bavelier, D. (2015).
Technology consumption and cognitive control: Contrasting
action video game experience
with media multitasking. Attention, Perception, &
Psychophysics, 78(1), 218–241.
doi:10.3758/s13414-015-0988-0
22 W. WIRADHANY AND J. KOERTS
https://doi.org/10.1002/cb
https://doi.org/10.1027/1864-1105/a000167
https://doi.org/10.1111/hcre.12111
https://doi.org/10.1111/hcre.12111
https://doi.org/10.1177/0272431614523133
https://doi.org/10.1089/cyber.2012.0291
https://doi.org/10.1037//0033-295X.I08.3.624
https://doi.org/10.1037//0033-295X.I08.3.624
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2004.10.003
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2004.10.003
https://doi.org/10.1089/cyber.2010.0350
https://doi.org/10.1080/00140139.2013.821172
http://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1111/j.2044-
8260.1982.tb01421.x/full
http://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1111/j.2044-
8260.1982.tb01421.x/full
https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.84.4.822
https://doi.org/10.1017/S1355617706060310
https://doi.org/10.3758/s13423-016-1036-3
https://doi.org/10.3758/s13423-016-1036-3
https://doi.org/10.1068/p7017
https://doi.org/10.1542/peds.2008-3641
https://doi.org/10.3758/s13414-015-0988-0
Carrier, L. M., Cheever, N. A., Rosen, L. D., Benitez, S., &
Chang, J. (2009). Multitasking
across generations: Multitasking choices and difficulty ratings
in three generations of
Americans. Computers in Human Behavior, 25(2), 483–489.
doi:10.1016/j.chb.2008.10.012
Carrier, L. M., Rosen, L. D., Cheever, N. A., & Lim, A. F.
(2015). Causes, effects, and
practicalities of everyday multitasking. Developmental Review,
35, 64–78. doi:10.1016/j.
dr.2014.12.005
Carriere, J. S. A., Cheyne, J. A., & Smilek, D. (2008). Everyday
attention lapses and memory
failures: The affective consequences of mindlessness.
Consciousness and Cognition, 17(3),
835–847. doi:10.1016/j.concog.2007.04.008
Carriere, J. S. A., Seli, P., & Smilek, D. (2013). Wandering in
both mind and body: Individual
differences in mind wandering and inattention predict fidgeting.
Canadian Journal of
Experimental Psychology/Revue Canadienne De Psychologie
Expérimentale, 67(1), 19–31.
doi:10.1037/a0031438
Chan, R. C. K., Shum, D., Toulopoulou, T., & Chen, E. Y. H.
(2008). Assessment of executive
functions: Review of instruments and identification of critical
issues. Archives of Clinical
Neuropsychology, 23(2), 201–216.
doi:10.1016/j.acn.2007.08.010
Christoff, K., Irving, Z. C., Fox, K. C. R., Spreng, R. N., &
Andrews-Hanna, J. R. (2016).
Mind-wandering as spontaneous thought: A dynamic framework.
Nature Reviews
Neuroscience, 17(11), 1–44. doi:10.1038/nrn.2016.113
Dalley, J. W., Everitt, B. J., & Robbins, T. W. (2011).
Impulsivity, compulsivity, and top-down
cognitive control. Neuron, 69(4), 680–694.
doi:10.1016/j.neuron.2011.01.020
Dalley, J. W., & Robbins, T. W. (2017). Fractionating
impulsivity: Neuropsychiatric
implications. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 18(3), 158–171.
doi:10.1038/nrn.2017.8
Diamond, A. (2013). Executive functions. The Annual Review
of Psychology, 64, 135–168.
doi:10.1146/annurev-psych-113011-143750
Duff, B. R.-L., Yoon, G., Wang, Z., (Glenn), & Anghelcev, G.
(2014). Doing it all: An
exploratory study of predictors of media multitasking. Journal
of Interactive Advertising,
14(1), 11–23. 10.1080/15252019.2014.884480
Edwards, K. S., & Shin, M. (2017). Media multitasking and
implicit learning. Attention,
Perception, & Psychophysics, 79(5), 1535–1549.
doi:10.3758/s13414-017-1319-4
Egger, M., Davey Smith, G., Schneider, M., & Minder, C.
(1997). Bias in meta-analysis
detected by a simple, graphical test. BMJ (Clinical Research
ed.), 315(7109), 629–634.
doi:10.1136/bmj.316.7129.469
Engle, R. W. (2002). Working memory capacity as executive
attention. Psychological Science,
11(1), 19–23. doi:10.1111/1467-8721.00160
Engle, R. W., & Kane, M. J. (2004). Executive attention,
working memory capacity, and a
two-factor theory of cognitive control. The Psychology of
Learning and Motivation, 44,
145–199. doi:10.1016/S0079-7421(03)44005-X
Farias, S. T., Harrell, E., Neumann, C., & Houtz, A. (2003). The
relationship between
neuropsychological performance and daily functioning in
individuals with Alzheimer’s
disease: Ecological validity of neuropsychological tests.
Archives of Clinical
Neuropsychology, 18(6), 655–672. doi:10.1016/S0887-
6177(02)00159-2
Fox, A. B., Rosen, J., & Crawford, M. (2009). Distractions,
distractions: Does instant messa-
ging affect college students’ performance on a concurrent
reading comprehension task?
Cyberpsychology & Behavior : the Impact of the Internet,
Multimedia and Virtual Reality on
Behavior and Society, 12(1), 51–53. doi:10.1089/cpb.2008.0107
Gioia, G. A., Isquith, P. K., Guy, S. C., & Kenworthy, L.
(2000). TEST REVIEW behavior
rating inventory of executive function behavior rating inventory
of executive function.
Child Neuropsychology, 6(3), 235–238.
doi:10.1076/chin.6.3.235.3152
MEDIA PSYCHOLOGY 23
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.chb.2008.10.012
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.dr.2014.12.005
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.dr.2014.12.005
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.concog.2007.04.008
https://doi.org/10.1037/a0031438
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.acn.2007.08.010
https://doi.org/10.1038/nrn.2016.113
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuron.2011.01.020
https://doi.org/10.1038/nrn.2017.8
https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev-psych-113011-143750
http://10.1080/15252019.2014.884480
https://doi.org/10.3758/s13414-017-1319-4
https://doi.org/10.1136/bmj.316.7129.469
https://doi.org/10.1111/1467-8721.00160
https://doi.org/10.1016/S0079-7421(03)44005-X
https://doi.org/10.1016/S0887-6177(02)00159-2
https://doi.org/10.1089/cpb.2008.0107
https://doi.org/10.1076/chin.6.3.235.3152
Gioia, G. A., Isquith, P. K., Retzlaff, P. D., & Espy, K. A.
(2002). Confirmatory factor analysis
of the behavior rating inventory of executive function (BRIEF)
in a clinical sample. Child
Neuropsychology, 8(4), 249–257.
doi:10.1076/chin.8.4.249.13513
González, V. M., & Mark, G. (2004). “Constant, constant,
multi-tasking craziness”: Managing
multiple working spheres. CHI ’04 Proceedings of the SIGCHI
Conference on Human
Factors in Computing Systems, 6(1), 113–120.
doi:10.1145/985692.985707
Gorman, T. E., & Green, C. S. (2016). Short-term mindfulness
intervention reduces the
negative attentional effects associated with heavy media
multitasking. Scientific Reports, 6
(April), 24542. doi:10.1038/srep24542
Hadlington, L., & Murphy, K. (2018). Is media multitasking
good for cybersecurity?
Exploring the relationship between media multitasking and
everyday cognitive failures
on self-reported risky cybersecurity behaviors.
Cyberpsychology, Behavior and Social
Networking, 21(3), 168–172. doi:10.1089/cyber.2017.0524
Hatchel, T., Negriff, S., & Subrahmanyam, K. (2018). The
relation between media multi-
tasking, intensity of use, and well-being in a sample of
ethnically diverse emerging adults.
Computers in Human Behavior, 81, 115–123.
doi:10.1016/j.chb.2017.12.012
Hedges, L. V., & Pigott, T. D. (2004). The power of statistical
tests for moderators in
meta-analysis. Psychological Methods, 9(4), 426–445.
doi:10.1037/1082-989X.9.4.426
Hedges, L. V., Tipton, E., & Johnson, M. C. (2010). Robust
variance estimation in
meta-regression with dependent effect size estimates. Research
Synthesis Methods, 1(1),
39–65. doi:10.1002/jrsm.5
Heppner, P. P., & Petersen, C. H. (1982). The development and
implications of a personal
problem-solving inventory. Journal of Counseling Psychology,
29(1), 66–75. doi:10.1037/
0022-0167.29.1.66
Hoyle, R. H., Stephenson, M. T., Palmgreen, P., Lorch, E. P., &
Donohew, R. L. (2002).
Reliability and validity of a brief measure of sensation seeking.
Personality and Individual
Differences, 32(3), 401–414. doi:10.1016/S0191-
8869(01)00032-0
Huizinga, M., & Smidts, D. P. (2010). Age-related changes in
executive function: A normative
study with the Dutch version of the Behavior Rating Inventory
of Executive Function
(BRIEF). Child Neuropsychology, 17(1), 51–66.
doi:10.1080/09297049.2010.509715
Hwang, Y., Kim, H., & Jeong, S. H. (2014). Why do media
users multitask?: Motives for
general, medium-specific, and content-specific types of
multitasking. Computers in Human
Behavior, 36, 542–548. doi:10.1016/j.chb.2014.04.040
Ioannidis, J. P. A. (2005). Why most published research
findings are false. PLoS Medicine, 2
(8), e124. doi:10.1371/journal.pmed.0020124
Ioannidis, J. P. A., Munafò, M. R., Fusar-Poli, P., Nosek, B. A.,
& David, S. P. (2014).
Publication and other reporting biases in cognitive sciences:
Detection, prevalence, and
prevention. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 18(5), 235–241.
doi:10.1016/j.tics.2014.02.010
Jeong, S.-H., & Hwang, Y. (2016). Media multitasking effects
on cognitive vs. Attitudinal
outcomes: A meta-analysis. Human Communication Research,
42(4), 599–618. doi:10.1111/
hcre.12089
Judd, T. (2013). Making sense of multitasking: Key behaviours.
Computers and Education, 63,
358–367. doi:10.1016/j.compedu.2012.12.017
Kazakova, S., Cauberghe, V., Pandelaere, M., & De Pelsmacker,
P. (2015). Can’t see the forest
for the trees? The effect of media multitasking on cognitive
processing style. Media
Psychology, 18(4), 425–450.
doi:10.1080/15213269.2015.1006789
Kessler, R. C., Adler, L., Ames, M., Demler, O., Faraone, S.,
Hiripi, E., … Walters, E. E.
(2005). The world health organization adult ADHD Self-Report
Scale (ASRS): A short
screening scale for use in the general population. Psychological
Medicine, 35(2), 245–256.
doi:10.1017/S0033291704002892
24 W. WIRADHANY AND J. KOERTS
https://doi.org/10.1076/chin.8.4.249.13513
https://doi.org/10.1145/985692.985707
https://doi.org/10.1038/srep24542
https://doi.org/10.1089/cyber.2017.0524
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.chb.2017.12.012
https://doi.org/10.1037/1082-989X.9.4.426
https://doi.org/10.1002/jrsm.5
https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-0167.29.1.66
https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-0167.29.1.66
https://doi.org/10.1016/S0191-8869(01)00032-0
https://doi.org/10.1080/09297049.2010.509715
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.chb.2014.04.040
https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pmed.0020124
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2014.02.010
https://doi.org/10.1111/hcre.12089
https://doi.org/10.1111/hcre.12089
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.compedu.2012.12.017
https://doi.org/10.1080/15213269.2015.1006789
https://doi.org/10.1017/S0033291704002892
Kononova, A., Joo, E., & Yuan, S. (2016). If I choose when to
switch: Heavy multitaskers
remember online content better than light multitaskers when
they have the freedom to
multitask. Computers in Human Behavior, 65, 567–575.
doi:10.1016/j.chb.2016.09.011
Kononova, A., Zasorina, T., Diveeva, N., Kokoeva, A., &
Chelokyan, A. (2014). Multitasking
goes global: Multitasking with traditional and new electronic
media and attention to media
messages among college students in Kuwait, Russia, and the
USA. International
Communication Gazette, 76(8), 617–640.
doi:10.1177/1748048514548533
Larson, G. E., Alderton, D. L., Neideffer, M., & Underhill, E.
(2011). Further evidence on
dimensionality and correlates of the cognitive failures
questionnaire. British Journal of
Psychology, 88(1), 29–38. doi:10.1111/j.2044-
8295.1997.tb02618.x
Lenhart, A. (2015). Teens, social media & technology overview
2015. In Pew Research Center’s
Internet & American Life Project. Retrieved from
http://www.pewinternet.org/2015/04/09/
teens-social-media-technology-2015/
Lin, L. (2009). Breadth-biased versus focused cognitive control
in media multitasking
behaviors. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of
the United States of
America, 106(37), 15521–15522. doi:10.1073/pnas.0908642106
Lin, L., Robertson, T., & Lee, J. (2009). Reading performances
between novices and experts in
different media multitasking environments. Computers in the
Schools, 26(3), 169–186.
doi:10.1080/07380560903095162
Loh, K. K., & Kanai, R. (2014). Higher media multi-tasking
activity is associated with smaller
gray-matter density in the anterior cingulate cortex. PloS One,
9(9), e106698. doi:10.1371/
journal.pone.0106698
Loh, K. K., Tan, B. Z. H., & Lim, S. W. H. (2016). Media
multitasking predicts video-recorded
lecture learning performance through mind wandering
tendencies. Computers in Human
Behavior, 63, 943–947. doi:10.1016/j.chb.2016.06.030
Lui, K. F. H., & Wong, A. C.-N. (2012). Does media
multitasking always hurt? A positive
correlation between multitasking and multisensory integration.
Psychonomic Bulletin &
Review, 19(4), 647–653. doi:10.3758/s13423-012-0245-7
Magen, H. (2017). The relations between executive functions,
media multitasking and
polychronicity. Computers in Human Behavior, 67, 1–9.
doi:10.1016/j.chb.2016.10.011
Marius, P., & Anggoro, S. (2014). Profil Pengguna Internet
Indonesia 2014. Jakarta, Indonesia:
Asosiasi Penyelenggara Jasa Internet Indonesia.
Mark, G., Gudith, D., & Klocke, U. (2008). The cost of
interrupted work: More speed and
stress. Proceedings of the SIGCHI Conference on Human
Factors in Computing Systems,
107–110. doi:10.1145/1357054.1357072
Minear, M., Brasher, F., McCurdy, M., Lewis, J., & Younggren,
A. (2013). Working memory,
fluid intelligence, and impulsiveness in heavy media
multitaskers. Psychonomic Bulletin &
Review, 20, 1274–1281. doi:10.3758/s13423-013-0456-6
Moisala, M., Salmela, V., Hietajärvi, L., Salo, E., Carlson, S.,
Salonen, O., … Alho, K. (2016).
Media multitasking is associated with distractibility and
increased prefrontal activity in
adolescents and young adults. NeuroImage, 134, 113–121.
doi:10.1016/j.
neuroimage.2016.04.011
Murphy, K., McLauchlan, S., & Lee, M. (2017). Is there a link
between media-multitasking
and the executive functions of filtering and response inhibition?
Computers in Human
Behavior, 75, 667–677. doi:10.1016/j.chb.2017.06.001
Ophir, E., Nass, C., & Wagner, A. D. (2009). Cognitive control
in media multitaskers.
Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United
States of America, 106(37),
15583–15587. doi:10.1073/pnas.0903620106
MEDIA PSYCHOLOGY 25
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.chb.2016.09.011
https://doi.org/10.1177/1748048514548533
https://doi.org/10.1111/j.2044-8295.1997.tb02618.x
http://www.pewinternet.org/2015/04/09/teens-social-media-
technology-2015/
http://www.pewinternet.org/2015/04/09/teens-social-media-
technology-2015/
https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.0908642106
https://doi.org/10.1080/07380560903095162
https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0106698
https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0106698
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.chb.2016.06.030
https://doi.org/10.3758/s13423-012-0245-7
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.chb.2016.10.011
https://doi.org/10.1145/1357054.1357072
https://doi.org/10.3758/s13423-013-0456-6
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2016.04.011
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2016.04.011
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.chb.2017.06.001
https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.0903620106
Patton, J. H., Stanford, M. S., & Barratt, E. S. (1995). Factor
structure of the barratt
impulsiveness scale. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 51, 768–
774. doi:10.1002/(ISSN)1097-
4679
Pea, R., Nass, C., Meheula, L., Rance, M., Kumar, A., Bamford,
H., … Zhou, M. (2012). Media
use, face-to-face communication, media multitasking, and social
well-being among 8- to
12-year-old girls. Developmental Psychology, 48(2), 327–336.
doi:10.1037/a0027030
Petersen, S. E., & Posner, M. I. (2012). The attention system of
the human brain: 20 years
after. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 35(1), 73–89.
doi:10.1146/annurev-neuro-062111-
150525
Posner, M. I., & Petersen, S. E. (1990). The attention system of
the human brain. Annual
Review of Neuroscience, 13, 25–42.
doi:10.1146/annurev.ne.13.030190.000325
Primack, B. A., Shensa, A., Escobar-Viera, C. G., Barrett, E. L.,
Sidani, J. E., Colditz, J. B., &
James, A. E. (2017). Use of multiple social media platforms and
symptoms of depression
and anxiety: A nationally-representative study among U.S.
young adults. Computers in
Human Behavior, 69, 1–9. doi:10.1016/j.chb.2016.11.013
R Core team. (2015). R Core Team. R: A Language and
Environment for Statistical
Computing., 55, 275–286. Retrieved from http://www.r-
project.org/
Ralph, B. C. W., & Smilek, D. (2016). Individual differences in
media multitasking and
performance on the n-back. Attention, Perception, &
Psychophysics, 79(2), 582–592.
doi:10.3758/s13414-016-1260-y
Ralph, B. C. W., Thomson, D. R., Cheyne, J. A., & Smilek, D.
(2013). Media multitasking and
failures of attention in everyday life. Psychological Research,
78(5), 661–669. doi:10.1007/
s00426-013-0523-7
Ralph, B. C. W., Thomson, D. R., Seli, P., Carriere, J. S. A., &
Smilek, D. (2015). Media
multitasking and behavioral measures of sustained attention.
Attention, Perception, &
Psychophysics, 77(2), 390–401. doi:10.3758/s13414-014-0771-7
Rideout, V. J., Foehr, U. G., & Roberts, D. F. (2010).
Generation M2: Media in the Lives of 8
to 18 Year-Olds. Retrieved from
https://files.eric.ed.gov/fulltext/ED527859.pdf
Rigby, J. M., Brumby, D. P., Gould, S. J. J., & Cox, A. L.
(2017). Media multitasking at home:
A video observation study of concurrent TV and mobile device
usage. Proceedings of the
2017 ACM International Conference on Interactive Experiences
for TV and Online Video,
3–10. doi:10.1145/3077548.3077560
Roberts, D. F., & Foehr, U. G. (2008). Trends in media use.
Future of Children, 18(1), 11–37.
doi:10.1353/foc.0.0000
Sanbonmatsu, D. M., Strayer, D. L., Medeiros-Ward, N., &
Watson, J. M. (2013). Who
multi-tasks and why? Multi-tasking ability, perceived multi-
tasking ability, impulsivity,
Intercultural Communication in Organizations
Intercultural Communication in Organizations
Intercultural Communication in Organizations
Intercultural Communication in Organizations
Intercultural Communication in Organizations
Intercultural Communication in Organizations
Intercultural Communication in Organizations
Intercultural Communication in Organizations
Intercultural Communication in Organizations
Intercultural Communication in Organizations
Intercultural Communication in Organizations
Intercultural Communication in Organizations
Intercultural Communication in Organizations
Intercultural Communication in Organizations
Intercultural Communication in Organizations
Intercultural Communication in Organizations
Intercultural Communication in Organizations
Intercultural Communication in Organizations
Intercultural Communication in Organizations
Intercultural Communication in Organizations
Intercultural Communication in Organizations
Intercultural Communication in Organizations
Intercultural Communication in Organizations
Intercultural Communication in Organizations
Intercultural Communication in Organizations
Intercultural Communication in Organizations
Intercultural Communication in Organizations
Intercultural Communication in Organizations
Intercultural Communication in Organizations
Intercultural Communication in Organizations
Intercultural Communication in Organizations
Intercultural Communication in Organizations
Intercultural Communication in Organizations
Intercultural Communication in Organizations
Intercultural Communication in Organizations
Intercultural Communication in Organizations
Intercultural Communication in Organizations
Intercultural Communication in Organizations
Intercultural Communication in Organizations
Intercultural Communication in Organizations
Intercultural Communication in Organizations
Intercultural Communication in Organizations
Intercultural Communication in Organizations
Intercultural Communication in Organizations
Intercultural Communication in Organizations
Intercultural Communication in Organizations
Intercultural Communication in Organizations
Intercultural Communication in Organizations
Intercultural Communication in Organizations
Intercultural Communication in Organizations
Intercultural Communication in Organizations
Intercultural Communication in Organizations
Intercultural Communication in Organizations
Intercultural Communication in Organizations
Intercultural Communication in Organizations
Intercultural Communication in Organizations
Intercultural Communication in Organizations
Intercultural Communication in Organizations
Intercultural Communication in Organizations
Intercultural Communication in Organizations
Intercultural Communication in Organizations

More Related Content

Similar to Intercultural Communication in Organizations

Communication Is The Most Fundamental Element. Cross...
Communication Is The Most Fundamental Element. Cross...Communication Is The Most Fundamental Element. Cross...
Communication Is The Most Fundamental Element. Cross...Lindsey Jones
 
1Running head LITERATURE REVIEWLITERATURE REVIEW .docx
1Running head LITERATURE REVIEWLITERATURE REVIEW             .docx1Running head LITERATURE REVIEWLITERATURE REVIEW             .docx
1Running head LITERATURE REVIEWLITERATURE REVIEW .docxfelicidaddinwoodie
 
Cross-Cultural Misunderstanding in Businesses Language Misinterpr
Cross-Cultural Misunderstanding in Businesses Language MisinterprCross-Cultural Misunderstanding in Businesses Language Misinterpr
Cross-Cultural Misunderstanding in Businesses Language MisinterprMargenePurnell14
 
Cross Cultural Communication
Cross Cultural CommunicationCross Cultural Communication
Cross Cultural Communicationrehan012
 
MGM316-1401B-01Quesadra D. GoodrumClass Discussion Phase2.docx
MGM316-1401B-01Quesadra D. GoodrumClass Discussion Phase2.docxMGM316-1401B-01Quesadra D. GoodrumClass Discussion Phase2.docx
MGM316-1401B-01Quesadra D. GoodrumClass Discussion Phase2.docxandreecapon
 
Cross administrative culture and values
Cross administrative culture and valuesCross administrative culture and values
Cross administrative culture and valuesANWARUL WADUD
 
atify and describe the cult alue dimensions that help ural profile of.pdf
atify and describe the cult alue dimensions that help ural profile of.pdfatify and describe the cult alue dimensions that help ural profile of.pdf
atify and describe the cult alue dimensions that help ural profile of.pdfjeeteshmalani1
 
Cultural Intelligence: Bridging the Cultural Differences in the Emerging Markets
Cultural Intelligence: Bridging the Cultural Differences in the Emerging MarketsCultural Intelligence: Bridging the Cultural Differences in the Emerging Markets
Cultural Intelligence: Bridging the Cultural Differences in the Emerging MarketsJIANGUANGLUNG DANGMEI
 
Effect of Culture on Entrepreneurship in Nigeria
Effect of Culture on Entrepreneurship in NigeriaEffect of Culture on Entrepreneurship in Nigeria
Effect of Culture on Entrepreneurship in Nigeriainventionjournals
 
PSB510 Organisational Culture.docx
PSB510 Organisational Culture.docxPSB510 Organisational Culture.docx
PSB510 Organisational Culture.docxwrite5
 
The role of diplomacy & its effects in multicultural conflict management
The role of diplomacy & its effects in multicultural conflict managementThe role of diplomacy & its effects in multicultural conflict management
The role of diplomacy & its effects in multicultural conflict managementAquatix Pharma
 
Intercultural communication presentation
Intercultural communication presentation Intercultural communication presentation
Intercultural communication presentation Hasan BİLOKCUOGLU
 
The cultural environments_facing_business
The cultural environments_facing_businessThe cultural environments_facing_business
The cultural environments_facing_businessIsrar Khan Raja
 
Unit I Organisational Behaviour
Unit I Organisational BehaviourUnit I Organisational Behaviour
Unit I Organisational BehaviourSachin MK
 

Similar to Intercultural Communication in Organizations (15)

Communication Is The Most Fundamental Element. Cross...
Communication Is The Most Fundamental Element. Cross...Communication Is The Most Fundamental Element. Cross...
Communication Is The Most Fundamental Element. Cross...
 
1Running head LITERATURE REVIEWLITERATURE REVIEW .docx
1Running head LITERATURE REVIEWLITERATURE REVIEW             .docx1Running head LITERATURE REVIEWLITERATURE REVIEW             .docx
1Running head LITERATURE REVIEWLITERATURE REVIEW .docx
 
Cross-Cultural Misunderstanding in Businesses Language Misinterpr
Cross-Cultural Misunderstanding in Businesses Language MisinterprCross-Cultural Misunderstanding in Businesses Language Misinterpr
Cross-Cultural Misunderstanding in Businesses Language Misinterpr
 
Cross Cultural Communication
Cross Cultural CommunicationCross Cultural Communication
Cross Cultural Communication
 
MGM316-1401B-01Quesadra D. GoodrumClass Discussion Phase2.docx
MGM316-1401B-01Quesadra D. GoodrumClass Discussion Phase2.docxMGM316-1401B-01Quesadra D. GoodrumClass Discussion Phase2.docx
MGM316-1401B-01Quesadra D. GoodrumClass Discussion Phase2.docx
 
Cross administrative culture and values
Cross administrative culture and valuesCross administrative culture and values
Cross administrative culture and values
 
Ccdm ppt 2011
Ccdm ppt 2011Ccdm ppt 2011
Ccdm ppt 2011
 
atify and describe the cult alue dimensions that help ural profile of.pdf
atify and describe the cult alue dimensions that help ural profile of.pdfatify and describe the cult alue dimensions that help ural profile of.pdf
atify and describe the cult alue dimensions that help ural profile of.pdf
 
Cultural Intelligence: Bridging the Cultural Differences in the Emerging Markets
Cultural Intelligence: Bridging the Cultural Differences in the Emerging MarketsCultural Intelligence: Bridging the Cultural Differences in the Emerging Markets
Cultural Intelligence: Bridging the Cultural Differences in the Emerging Markets
 
Effect of Culture on Entrepreneurship in Nigeria
Effect of Culture on Entrepreneurship in NigeriaEffect of Culture on Entrepreneurship in Nigeria
Effect of Culture on Entrepreneurship in Nigeria
 
PSB510 Organisational Culture.docx
PSB510 Organisational Culture.docxPSB510 Organisational Culture.docx
PSB510 Organisational Culture.docx
 
The role of diplomacy & its effects in multicultural conflict management
The role of diplomacy & its effects in multicultural conflict managementThe role of diplomacy & its effects in multicultural conflict management
The role of diplomacy & its effects in multicultural conflict management
 
Intercultural communication presentation
Intercultural communication presentation Intercultural communication presentation
Intercultural communication presentation
 
The cultural environments_facing_business
The cultural environments_facing_businessThe cultural environments_facing_business
The cultural environments_facing_business
 
Unit I Organisational Behaviour
Unit I Organisational BehaviourUnit I Organisational Behaviour
Unit I Organisational Behaviour
 

More from mccormicknadine86

Option #2Researching a Leader Complete preliminary rese.docx
Option #2Researching a Leader Complete preliminary rese.docxOption #2Researching a Leader Complete preliminary rese.docx
Option #2Researching a Leader Complete preliminary rese.docxmccormicknadine86
 
Option 1 ImperialismThe exploitation of  colonial resources.docx
Option 1 ImperialismThe exploitation of  colonial resources.docxOption 1 ImperialismThe exploitation of  colonial resources.docx
Option 1 ImperialismThe exploitation of  colonial resources.docxmccormicknadine86
 
Option Wireless LTD v. OpenPeak, Inc.Be sure to save an elec.docx
Option Wireless LTD v. OpenPeak, Inc.Be sure to save an elec.docxOption Wireless LTD v. OpenPeak, Inc.Be sure to save an elec.docx
Option Wireless LTD v. OpenPeak, Inc.Be sure to save an elec.docxmccormicknadine86
 
Option A Land SharkWhen is a shark just a shark Consider the.docx
Option A Land SharkWhen is a shark just a shark Consider the.docxOption A Land SharkWhen is a shark just a shark Consider the.docx
Option A Land SharkWhen is a shark just a shark Consider the.docxmccormicknadine86
 
Option 3 Discuss your thoughts on drugs and deviance. Do you think .docx
Option 3 Discuss your thoughts on drugs and deviance. Do you think .docxOption 3 Discuss your thoughts on drugs and deviance. Do you think .docx
Option 3 Discuss your thoughts on drugs and deviance. Do you think .docxmccormicknadine86
 
OPTION 2 Can we make the changes we need to make After the pandemi.docx
OPTION 2 Can we make the changes we need to make After the pandemi.docxOPTION 2 Can we make the changes we need to make After the pandemi.docx
OPTION 2 Can we make the changes we need to make After the pandemi.docxmccormicknadine86
 
Option 1 You will create a PowerPoint (or equivalent) of your p.docx
Option 1 You will create a PowerPoint (or equivalent) of your p.docxOption 1 You will create a PowerPoint (or equivalent) of your p.docx
Option 1 You will create a PowerPoint (or equivalent) of your p.docxmccormicknadine86
 
Option A Description of Dance StylesSelect two styles of danc.docx
Option A Description of Dance StylesSelect two styles of danc.docxOption A Description of Dance StylesSelect two styles of danc.docx
Option A Description of Dance StylesSelect two styles of danc.docxmccormicknadine86
 
Option #2Provide several slides that explain the key section.docx
Option #2Provide several slides that explain the key section.docxOption #2Provide several slides that explain the key section.docx
Option #2Provide several slides that explain the key section.docxmccormicknadine86
 
Option 2 Slavery vs. Indentured ServitudeExplain how and wh.docx
Option 2 Slavery vs. Indentured ServitudeExplain how and wh.docxOption 2 Slavery vs. Indentured ServitudeExplain how and wh.docx
Option 2 Slavery vs. Indentured ServitudeExplain how and wh.docxmccormicknadine86
 
Option 2 ArtSelect any 2 of works of art about the Holocaus.docx
Option 2 ArtSelect any 2 of works of art about the Holocaus.docxOption 2 ArtSelect any 2 of works of art about the Holocaus.docx
Option 2 ArtSelect any 2 of works of art about the Holocaus.docxmccormicknadine86
 
Option #1 Stanford University Prison Experiment Causality, C.docx
Option #1 Stanford University Prison Experiment Causality, C.docxOption #1 Stanford University Prison Experiment Causality, C.docx
Option #1 Stanford University Prison Experiment Causality, C.docxmccormicknadine86
 
Option A  Gender CrimesCriminal acts occur against individu.docx
Option A  Gender CrimesCriminal acts occur against individu.docxOption A  Gender CrimesCriminal acts occur against individu.docx
Option A  Gender CrimesCriminal acts occur against individu.docxmccormicknadine86
 
opic 4 Discussion Question 1 May students express religious bel.docx
opic 4 Discussion Question 1 May students express religious bel.docxopic 4 Discussion Question 1 May students express religious bel.docx
opic 4 Discussion Question 1 May students express religious bel.docxmccormicknadine86
 
Option 1Choose a philosopher who interests you. Research that p.docx
Option 1Choose a philosopher who interests you. Research that p.docxOption 1Choose a philosopher who interests you. Research that p.docx
Option 1Choose a philosopher who interests you. Research that p.docxmccormicknadine86
 
Option #1The Stanford University Prison Experiment Structu.docx
Option #1The Stanford University Prison Experiment Structu.docxOption #1The Stanford University Prison Experiment Structu.docx
Option #1The Stanford University Prison Experiment Structu.docxmccormicknadine86
 
Operationaland Organizational SecurityChapter 3Princ.docx
Operationaland Organizational SecurityChapter 3Princ.docxOperationaland Organizational SecurityChapter 3Princ.docx
Operationaland Organizational SecurityChapter 3Princ.docxmccormicknadine86
 
Open the file (Undergrad Reqt_Individual In-Depth Case Study) for in.docx
Open the file (Undergrad Reqt_Individual In-Depth Case Study) for in.docxOpen the file (Undergrad Reqt_Individual In-Depth Case Study) for in.docx
Open the file (Undergrad Reqt_Individual In-Depth Case Study) for in.docxmccormicknadine86
 
onsider whether you think means-tested programs, such as the Tem.docx
onsider whether you think means-tested programs, such as the Tem.docxonsider whether you think means-tested programs, such as the Tem.docx
onsider whether you think means-tested programs, such as the Tem.docxmccormicknadine86
 
Operations security - PPT should cover below questions (chapter 1 to.docx
Operations security - PPT should cover below questions (chapter 1 to.docxOperations security - PPT should cover below questions (chapter 1 to.docx
Operations security - PPT should cover below questions (chapter 1 to.docxmccormicknadine86
 

More from mccormicknadine86 (20)

Option #2Researching a Leader Complete preliminary rese.docx
Option #2Researching a Leader Complete preliminary rese.docxOption #2Researching a Leader Complete preliminary rese.docx
Option #2Researching a Leader Complete preliminary rese.docx
 
Option 1 ImperialismThe exploitation of  colonial resources.docx
Option 1 ImperialismThe exploitation of  colonial resources.docxOption 1 ImperialismThe exploitation of  colonial resources.docx
Option 1 ImperialismThe exploitation of  colonial resources.docx
 
Option Wireless LTD v. OpenPeak, Inc.Be sure to save an elec.docx
Option Wireless LTD v. OpenPeak, Inc.Be sure to save an elec.docxOption Wireless LTD v. OpenPeak, Inc.Be sure to save an elec.docx
Option Wireless LTD v. OpenPeak, Inc.Be sure to save an elec.docx
 
Option A Land SharkWhen is a shark just a shark Consider the.docx
Option A Land SharkWhen is a shark just a shark Consider the.docxOption A Land SharkWhen is a shark just a shark Consider the.docx
Option A Land SharkWhen is a shark just a shark Consider the.docx
 
Option 3 Discuss your thoughts on drugs and deviance. Do you think .docx
Option 3 Discuss your thoughts on drugs and deviance. Do you think .docxOption 3 Discuss your thoughts on drugs and deviance. Do you think .docx
Option 3 Discuss your thoughts on drugs and deviance. Do you think .docx
 
OPTION 2 Can we make the changes we need to make After the pandemi.docx
OPTION 2 Can we make the changes we need to make After the pandemi.docxOPTION 2 Can we make the changes we need to make After the pandemi.docx
OPTION 2 Can we make the changes we need to make After the pandemi.docx
 
Option 1 You will create a PowerPoint (or equivalent) of your p.docx
Option 1 You will create a PowerPoint (or equivalent) of your p.docxOption 1 You will create a PowerPoint (or equivalent) of your p.docx
Option 1 You will create a PowerPoint (or equivalent) of your p.docx
 
Option A Description of Dance StylesSelect two styles of danc.docx
Option A Description of Dance StylesSelect two styles of danc.docxOption A Description of Dance StylesSelect two styles of danc.docx
Option A Description of Dance StylesSelect two styles of danc.docx
 
Option #2Provide several slides that explain the key section.docx
Option #2Provide several slides that explain the key section.docxOption #2Provide several slides that explain the key section.docx
Option #2Provide several slides that explain the key section.docx
 
Option 2 Slavery vs. Indentured ServitudeExplain how and wh.docx
Option 2 Slavery vs. Indentured ServitudeExplain how and wh.docxOption 2 Slavery vs. Indentured ServitudeExplain how and wh.docx
Option 2 Slavery vs. Indentured ServitudeExplain how and wh.docx
 
Option 2 ArtSelect any 2 of works of art about the Holocaus.docx
Option 2 ArtSelect any 2 of works of art about the Holocaus.docxOption 2 ArtSelect any 2 of works of art about the Holocaus.docx
Option 2 ArtSelect any 2 of works of art about the Holocaus.docx
 
Option #1 Stanford University Prison Experiment Causality, C.docx
Option #1 Stanford University Prison Experiment Causality, C.docxOption #1 Stanford University Prison Experiment Causality, C.docx
Option #1 Stanford University Prison Experiment Causality, C.docx
 
Option A  Gender CrimesCriminal acts occur against individu.docx
Option A  Gender CrimesCriminal acts occur against individu.docxOption A  Gender CrimesCriminal acts occur against individu.docx
Option A  Gender CrimesCriminal acts occur against individu.docx
 
opic 4 Discussion Question 1 May students express religious bel.docx
opic 4 Discussion Question 1 May students express religious bel.docxopic 4 Discussion Question 1 May students express religious bel.docx
opic 4 Discussion Question 1 May students express religious bel.docx
 
Option 1Choose a philosopher who interests you. Research that p.docx
Option 1Choose a philosopher who interests you. Research that p.docxOption 1Choose a philosopher who interests you. Research that p.docx
Option 1Choose a philosopher who interests you. Research that p.docx
 
Option #1The Stanford University Prison Experiment Structu.docx
Option #1The Stanford University Prison Experiment Structu.docxOption #1The Stanford University Prison Experiment Structu.docx
Option #1The Stanford University Prison Experiment Structu.docx
 
Operationaland Organizational SecurityChapter 3Princ.docx
Operationaland Organizational SecurityChapter 3Princ.docxOperationaland Organizational SecurityChapter 3Princ.docx
Operationaland Organizational SecurityChapter 3Princ.docx
 
Open the file (Undergrad Reqt_Individual In-Depth Case Study) for in.docx
Open the file (Undergrad Reqt_Individual In-Depth Case Study) for in.docxOpen the file (Undergrad Reqt_Individual In-Depth Case Study) for in.docx
Open the file (Undergrad Reqt_Individual In-Depth Case Study) for in.docx
 
onsider whether you think means-tested programs, such as the Tem.docx
onsider whether you think means-tested programs, such as the Tem.docxonsider whether you think means-tested programs, such as the Tem.docx
onsider whether you think means-tested programs, such as the Tem.docx
 
Operations security - PPT should cover below questions (chapter 1 to.docx
Operations security - PPT should cover below questions (chapter 1 to.docxOperations security - PPT should cover below questions (chapter 1 to.docx
Operations security - PPT should cover below questions (chapter 1 to.docx
 

Recently uploaded

ECONOMIC CONTEXT - PAPER 1 Q3: NEWSPAPERS.pptx
ECONOMIC CONTEXT - PAPER 1 Q3: NEWSPAPERS.pptxECONOMIC CONTEXT - PAPER 1 Q3: NEWSPAPERS.pptx
ECONOMIC CONTEXT - PAPER 1 Q3: NEWSPAPERS.pptxiammrhaywood
 
Difference Between Search & Browse Methods in Odoo 17
Difference Between Search & Browse Methods in Odoo 17Difference Between Search & Browse Methods in Odoo 17
Difference Between Search & Browse Methods in Odoo 17Celine George
 
ACC 2024 Chronicles. Cardiology. Exam.pdf
ACC 2024 Chronicles. Cardiology. Exam.pdfACC 2024 Chronicles. Cardiology. Exam.pdf
ACC 2024 Chronicles. Cardiology. Exam.pdfSpandanaRallapalli
 
Keynote by Prof. Wurzer at Nordex about IP-design
Keynote by Prof. Wurzer at Nordex about IP-designKeynote by Prof. Wurzer at Nordex about IP-design
Keynote by Prof. Wurzer at Nordex about IP-designMIPLM
 
Influencing policy (training slides from Fast Track Impact)
Influencing policy (training slides from Fast Track Impact)Influencing policy (training slides from Fast Track Impact)
Influencing policy (training slides from Fast Track Impact)Mark Reed
 
ENGLISH 7_Q4_LESSON 2_ Employing a Variety of Strategies for Effective Interp...
ENGLISH 7_Q4_LESSON 2_ Employing a Variety of Strategies for Effective Interp...ENGLISH 7_Q4_LESSON 2_ Employing a Variety of Strategies for Effective Interp...
ENGLISH 7_Q4_LESSON 2_ Employing a Variety of Strategies for Effective Interp...JhezDiaz1
 
Hierarchy of management that covers different levels of management
Hierarchy of management that covers different levels of managementHierarchy of management that covers different levels of management
Hierarchy of management that covers different levels of managementmkooblal
 
Grade 9 Q4-MELC1-Active and Passive Voice.pptx
Grade 9 Q4-MELC1-Active and Passive Voice.pptxGrade 9 Q4-MELC1-Active and Passive Voice.pptx
Grade 9 Q4-MELC1-Active and Passive Voice.pptxChelloAnnAsuncion2
 
How to do quick user assign in kanban in Odoo 17 ERP
How to do quick user assign in kanban in Odoo 17 ERPHow to do quick user assign in kanban in Odoo 17 ERP
How to do quick user assign in kanban in Odoo 17 ERPCeline George
 
Introduction to AI in Higher Education_draft.pptx
Introduction to AI in Higher Education_draft.pptxIntroduction to AI in Higher Education_draft.pptx
Introduction to AI in Higher Education_draft.pptxpboyjonauth
 
MULTIDISCIPLINRY NATURE OF THE ENVIRONMENTAL STUDIES.pptx
MULTIDISCIPLINRY NATURE OF THE ENVIRONMENTAL STUDIES.pptxMULTIDISCIPLINRY NATURE OF THE ENVIRONMENTAL STUDIES.pptx
MULTIDISCIPLINRY NATURE OF THE ENVIRONMENTAL STUDIES.pptxAnupkumar Sharma
 
ROOT CAUSE ANALYSIS PowerPoint Presentation
ROOT CAUSE ANALYSIS PowerPoint PresentationROOT CAUSE ANALYSIS PowerPoint Presentation
ROOT CAUSE ANALYSIS PowerPoint PresentationAadityaSharma884161
 
Proudly South Africa powerpoint Thorisha.pptx
Proudly South Africa powerpoint Thorisha.pptxProudly South Africa powerpoint Thorisha.pptx
Proudly South Africa powerpoint Thorisha.pptxthorishapillay1
 
Gas measurement O2,Co2,& ph) 04/2024.pptx
Gas measurement O2,Co2,& ph) 04/2024.pptxGas measurement O2,Co2,& ph) 04/2024.pptx
Gas measurement O2,Co2,& ph) 04/2024.pptxDr.Ibrahim Hassaan
 
DATA STRUCTURE AND ALGORITHM for beginners
DATA STRUCTURE AND ALGORITHM for beginnersDATA STRUCTURE AND ALGORITHM for beginners
DATA STRUCTURE AND ALGORITHM for beginnersSabitha Banu
 
AmericanHighSchoolsprezentacijaoskolama.
AmericanHighSchoolsprezentacijaoskolama.AmericanHighSchoolsprezentacijaoskolama.
AmericanHighSchoolsprezentacijaoskolama.arsicmarija21
 
Employee wellbeing at the workplace.pptx
Employee wellbeing at the workplace.pptxEmployee wellbeing at the workplace.pptx
Employee wellbeing at the workplace.pptxNirmalaLoungPoorunde1
 
Planning a health career 4th Quarter.pptx
Planning a health career 4th Quarter.pptxPlanning a health career 4th Quarter.pptx
Planning a health career 4th Quarter.pptxLigayaBacuel1
 

Recently uploaded (20)

ECONOMIC CONTEXT - PAPER 1 Q3: NEWSPAPERS.pptx
ECONOMIC CONTEXT - PAPER 1 Q3: NEWSPAPERS.pptxECONOMIC CONTEXT - PAPER 1 Q3: NEWSPAPERS.pptx
ECONOMIC CONTEXT - PAPER 1 Q3: NEWSPAPERS.pptx
 
Difference Between Search & Browse Methods in Odoo 17
Difference Between Search & Browse Methods in Odoo 17Difference Between Search & Browse Methods in Odoo 17
Difference Between Search & Browse Methods in Odoo 17
 
ACC 2024 Chronicles. Cardiology. Exam.pdf
ACC 2024 Chronicles. Cardiology. Exam.pdfACC 2024 Chronicles. Cardiology. Exam.pdf
ACC 2024 Chronicles. Cardiology. Exam.pdf
 
TataKelola dan KamSiber Kecerdasan Buatan v022.pdf
TataKelola dan KamSiber Kecerdasan Buatan v022.pdfTataKelola dan KamSiber Kecerdasan Buatan v022.pdf
TataKelola dan KamSiber Kecerdasan Buatan v022.pdf
 
Keynote by Prof. Wurzer at Nordex about IP-design
Keynote by Prof. Wurzer at Nordex about IP-designKeynote by Prof. Wurzer at Nordex about IP-design
Keynote by Prof. Wurzer at Nordex about IP-design
 
Influencing policy (training slides from Fast Track Impact)
Influencing policy (training slides from Fast Track Impact)Influencing policy (training slides from Fast Track Impact)
Influencing policy (training slides from Fast Track Impact)
 
Model Call Girl in Bikash Puri Delhi reach out to us at 🔝9953056974🔝
Model Call Girl in Bikash Puri  Delhi reach out to us at 🔝9953056974🔝Model Call Girl in Bikash Puri  Delhi reach out to us at 🔝9953056974🔝
Model Call Girl in Bikash Puri Delhi reach out to us at 🔝9953056974🔝
 
ENGLISH 7_Q4_LESSON 2_ Employing a Variety of Strategies for Effective Interp...
ENGLISH 7_Q4_LESSON 2_ Employing a Variety of Strategies for Effective Interp...ENGLISH 7_Q4_LESSON 2_ Employing a Variety of Strategies for Effective Interp...
ENGLISH 7_Q4_LESSON 2_ Employing a Variety of Strategies for Effective Interp...
 
Hierarchy of management that covers different levels of management
Hierarchy of management that covers different levels of managementHierarchy of management that covers different levels of management
Hierarchy of management that covers different levels of management
 
Grade 9 Q4-MELC1-Active and Passive Voice.pptx
Grade 9 Q4-MELC1-Active and Passive Voice.pptxGrade 9 Q4-MELC1-Active and Passive Voice.pptx
Grade 9 Q4-MELC1-Active and Passive Voice.pptx
 
How to do quick user assign in kanban in Odoo 17 ERP
How to do quick user assign in kanban in Odoo 17 ERPHow to do quick user assign in kanban in Odoo 17 ERP
How to do quick user assign in kanban in Odoo 17 ERP
 
Introduction to AI in Higher Education_draft.pptx
Introduction to AI in Higher Education_draft.pptxIntroduction to AI in Higher Education_draft.pptx
Introduction to AI in Higher Education_draft.pptx
 
MULTIDISCIPLINRY NATURE OF THE ENVIRONMENTAL STUDIES.pptx
MULTIDISCIPLINRY NATURE OF THE ENVIRONMENTAL STUDIES.pptxMULTIDISCIPLINRY NATURE OF THE ENVIRONMENTAL STUDIES.pptx
MULTIDISCIPLINRY NATURE OF THE ENVIRONMENTAL STUDIES.pptx
 
ROOT CAUSE ANALYSIS PowerPoint Presentation
ROOT CAUSE ANALYSIS PowerPoint PresentationROOT CAUSE ANALYSIS PowerPoint Presentation
ROOT CAUSE ANALYSIS PowerPoint Presentation
 
Proudly South Africa powerpoint Thorisha.pptx
Proudly South Africa powerpoint Thorisha.pptxProudly South Africa powerpoint Thorisha.pptx
Proudly South Africa powerpoint Thorisha.pptx
 
Gas measurement O2,Co2,& ph) 04/2024.pptx
Gas measurement O2,Co2,& ph) 04/2024.pptxGas measurement O2,Co2,& ph) 04/2024.pptx
Gas measurement O2,Co2,& ph) 04/2024.pptx
 
DATA STRUCTURE AND ALGORITHM for beginners
DATA STRUCTURE AND ALGORITHM for beginnersDATA STRUCTURE AND ALGORITHM for beginners
DATA STRUCTURE AND ALGORITHM for beginners
 
AmericanHighSchoolsprezentacijaoskolama.
AmericanHighSchoolsprezentacijaoskolama.AmericanHighSchoolsprezentacijaoskolama.
AmericanHighSchoolsprezentacijaoskolama.
 
Employee wellbeing at the workplace.pptx
Employee wellbeing at the workplace.pptxEmployee wellbeing at the workplace.pptx
Employee wellbeing at the workplace.pptx
 
Planning a health career 4th Quarter.pptx
Planning a health career 4th Quarter.pptxPlanning a health career 4th Quarter.pptx
Planning a health career 4th Quarter.pptx
 

Intercultural Communication in Organizations

  • 1. Chapter Eleven Intercultural Communication and the Organization Chapter Objectives Explain why intercultural communication is essential for organizational success and communication. Describe what is meant by culture in intercultural communication. Identify barriers to effective intercultural communication. Identify steps that can be used to overcome these barriers. Discuss the Hofstede dimensions as they apply to organizational contexts. Organizational Communication: Foundations for Business and Management, 2e Thomson, 2006 © Reasons to Study Intercultural CommunicationWe are a global village.Information is readily available anywhere on earth.The workplace is more diverse than ever before. We must be able to interact with others here and abroad efficiently and appropriately.Bottom line: Effective intercultural communication has become essential. Organizational Communication: Foundations for Business and Management, 2e Thomson, 2006 ©
  • 2. Three Reasons Effective Intercultural Communication is Essential Mobility.Time and distance are no longer factors in communication. Economic and political interdependence.The world’s markets and global concerns are dependent on effective communication. Communication TechnologyIncreased technology has allowed us freedoms to communicate quickly, efficiently, and cheaply. Organizational Communication: Foundations for Business and Management, 2e Thomson, 2006 © Defining CultureMultiple perspectives are apparent in all of the following categoriesNormsRolesBelief SystemsValuesLaws Organizational Communication: Foundations for Business and Management, 2e Thomson, 2006 © Culture refers to the multiple perspectives a group has on the world and worldly phenomena. Defining CultureCulture is learned, not biologically transmitted. Enculturation occurs through:LanguageNonverbal messagesSpace and time orientationPatterns for thinkingSelf- imagesAestheticsCulture is also modified by exposure to other cultures, which is called acculturation. Organizational Communication: Foundations for Business and Management, 2e Thomson, 2006 © (continued) Intercultural Communication Refers to communication exchanges between people whose
  • 3. cultural perceptions and symbols must be managed for effective communication. Organizational Communication: Foundations for Business and Management, 2e Thomson, 2006 © Intercultural CommunicationSome examples of intercultural communication include:An African-American speaking to a Caucasian.A Jew speaking to a MuslimA Cajun speaking to a CalifornianA man communicating to a woman Organizational Communication: Foundations for Business and Management, 2e Thomson, 2006 © Barriers to Effective Intercultural CommunicationPerceptual disparitySelective perceptions may lead to confusing or conflicting opinions.What is taken for granted behavior in one culture could be considered disrespectful in another. Organizational Communication: Foundations for Business and Management, 2e Thomson, 2006 © Barriers to Effective Intercultural CommunicationEthnocentrismPeople tend to judge the behaviors of others based on their own cultural values that they believe are “right.”An ethnocentric individual believes his or her ethnicity or ethnic perspective is superior to others. Organizational Communication: Foundations for Business and Management, 2e Thomson, 2006 ©
  • 4. Barriers to Effective Intercultural CommunicationLanguage dissimilaritySome researchers suggest that language shapes reality. Slang, jargon, and other language barriers can impact message reception and understanding. Organizational Communication: Foundations for Business and Management, 2e Thomson, 2006 © (continued) Barriers to Effective Intercultural CommunicationNonverbal dissimilarityGestures, emblems, and other nonverbal behaviors may be judged differently or have different meanings in different cultures. Organizational Communication: Foundations for Business and Management, 2e Thomson, 2006 © (continued) Approaches to Multinational ExpansionMultinational organizations can be structured in different ways towards the cultures they are expanding into.Adler 5 structural orientations multinational organizations use Organizational Communication: Foundations for Business and Management, 2e Thomson, 2006 © Adler Structural Orientations of Multinational Organizations Cultural dominanceSuperimposing the culture to subsidiary offices. Cultural accommodationAttempting to accept and assume the cultural values of the host company. Cultural compromiseAttempting to identify the divergent cultural orientations, but using only the similarities in the
  • 5. formation of policy. Organizational Communication: Foundations for Business and Management, 2e Thomson, 2006 © Adler Structural Orientations of Multinational Organizations Cultural avoidancePretending there are no differences between cultures. Cultural synergyA synergistic approach.Policies are not superimposed by the parent organization.Values of both cultures are managed, effectively creating a new culture. Organizational Communication: Foundations for Business and Management, 2e Thomson, 2006 © The Hofstede StudiesSuggests that there are perceptual differences based on culture.Implies that these perceptual differences can account for misunderstanding and intercultural tensions in organizational contexts. Organizational Communication: Foundations for Business and Management, 2e Thomson, 2006 © The Hofstede StudiesHofstede surveyed 116,000 employees in 72 nations to see if perceptions about work and work processes varied according to country and culture.Differences were categorized in four groups. Organizational Communication: Foundations for Business and Management, 2e Thomson, 2006 © (continued)
  • 6. The Hofstede StudiesUncertainty avoidanceHigh uncertainty avoidance cultures seek more clarity and less abstraction. Organizational Communication: Foundations for Business and Management, 2e Thomson, 2006 © (continued) The Hofstede StudiesPower distanceEmployees in high power distant culturesrespect superiors and disdain those who challenge them. Organizational Communication: Foundations for Business and Management, 2e Thomson, 2006 © (continued) The Hofstede StudiesIndividualism vs. CollectivismRelates to whether persons in the group value free- spirited independence, orcommunity cooperation and compromise. Organizational Communication: Foundations for Business and Management, 2e Thomson, 2006 © (continued) The Hofstede Studies Task vs. Social OrientationPertains to whether the dominant values of the society emphasize assertiveness or nurture. Organizational Communication: Foundations for Business and Management, 2e Thomson, 2006 © (continued)
  • 7. Cultural Extremes Organizational Communication: Foundations for Business and Management, 2e Thomson, 2006 © Adapted from tables in Hofstede, Geert, Culture’s Consequences: Second Edition, Comparing Values, Behaviors, Instituations and Organizations Across Nations, Sage Publications, 2001, pp. 87, 151, 215, 286. Suggestions for Overcoming BarriersFollow PrescriptionsSometimes we have an idea about what to do, but find it hard to actually follow through. Organizational Communication: Foundations for Business and Management, 2e Thomson, 2006 © Suggestions for Overcoming BarriersLearn about other cultures Acquire multicultural competencies and skills including foreign languages. Become students of worldwide human relations and values. Think beyond local perceptions and transform stereotypes into positive views of people. Become open and flexible in dealing with diversity. Organizational Communication: Foundations for Business and Management, 2e Thomson, 2006 ©
  • 8. Suggestions for Overcoming BarriersRecognize diversity within cultural groups.People within cultures are also diverse.Question stereotypes you have about people from a specific culture. Organizational Communication: Foundations for Business and Management, 2e Thomson, 2006 © Suggestions for Overcoming BarriersAssume an egalitarian frame. Be open to cultural differences.Be aware of and question your own ethnocentric attitudes and beliefs. Organizational Communication: Foundations for Business and Management, 2e Thomson, 2006 © Central ConceptsBecause of the ease of travel, the pervasiveness of communication technology, and political/economic reliance on other countries, it has become increasingly important to understand the effects of intercultural communication on organizational success. Organizational Communication: Foundations for Business and Management, 2e Thomson, 2006 © Central ConceptsOrganizational men and women need to examine the communication noises that affect intercultural interactions andbecome dedicated to overcoming these obstacles when they communicate.
  • 9. Organizational Communication: Foundations for Business and Management, 2e Thomson, 2006 © PURPOSEFUL READING (3-2-1) REPORT Version 2.0 Lightly Adapted from a template by Geraldine Van Gyn. Question 1: In your own words, what are the 3 most important concepts, ideas or issues in the reading? Briefly explain why you chose them. Concept 1 (In your own words) (2 marks) Concept 2 (In your own words) (2 marks) Concept 3 (In your own words) (2 marks)
  • 10. Question 2: What are 2 concepts, ideas or issues in the article that you had difficulty understanding, or that are missing but should have been included? In your own words, briefly explain what you did to correct the situation (e.g. looked up an unfamiliar word or a missing fact), and the result. Cite any sites or sources used in APA format. Issue 1 (In your own words) (1 mark) Citation 1 (in APA format) (1 mark)
  • 11. Issue 2 (In your own words) (1 mark) Citation 2 (in APA format) (1 mark) Question 3: What is the main economic story of the reading? (Economics studies the allocation of scarce resources.) Story (In your own words) (2 marks)
  • 12. 1 ECON 317 SPRING 2010 – INDIVIDUAL ASSIGNMENT 1 TO BE SUBMITTED VIA COURSESPACES BY 11:59 PM ON JANUARY 21st, 2020 Name (First, Family) Last 3 digits of SID TO SPEED UP MARKING, PLEASE ANSWER THE QUESTIONS IN THE FORMS AND SPACES PROVIDED. THE T.A. RESERVES THE RIGHT TO NOT MARK ANY QUESTIONS THAT ARE NOT ANSWERED IN THE EXPECTED LOCATIONS. By submitting this assignment you agree to the following honor code, and understand that any violation of the honor code may lead to penalties including but not limited to a non-negotiable mark of zero on the assignment: Honor Code: I guarantee that all the answers in this assignment are my own work. I have cited any outside sources that I used to create these answers in correct APA style. Marking scheme – Make sure you answer all the questions before handing this in!
  • 13. Question Marks 1 a 12 2 a 3 b 3 3 a 3 Total 21 1. [Critique] Read the following article: Marchildon, G. P. & Allin, S. (2016). The Public-Private Mix in the Delivery of Health-Care Services: Its Relevance for Lower- Income Canadians. Global Social Welfare, 3, pp. 161-170. Retrieved from https://link-springer- com.ezproxy.library.uvic.ca/article/10.1007%2Fs40609-016- 0070-4 (The above link allows you to access the article for free whether you are on campus or not. If off-campus, log in to UVic when prompted.) a. Write a 3-2-1 report on the article using the form provided on the course web site. (12 marks) A note about 3-2-1 reports in this course:
  • 14. For the ‘2’ part (2 things you didn’t understand and what you did to fix it), we need to see both what you found out, and the source you used to find that out, properly cited in APA format. The paper you are asked to read for the assignment CANNOT be used as the source: it’s understood that you will make sure to understand everything within the article you are asked to read, by consulting the article itself. This part of the 3-2-1 report is meant to encourage you to consult outside sources to find things out that you CANNOT find out from the article itself. While I would prefer that you use sources that, unlike Wikipedia, aren’t subject to constant editing and change, it’s fine to use Wikipedia or other such sources as long as you also include the date and time of access, so that readers can look up the state of the page as it was when you looked at it. (So, you’d add to the standard ‘Retrieved from’ part, ‘on January 3, 2020 at 11:59 AM’.) Also, remember to include a summary, in your own words, of what you found. It’s not enough to say, ‘I didn’t know what a raccoon was, so I looked it up on Wikipedia.’ You need to tell us what you discovered about raccoons, in your own words, and you need to cite the source you looked the information up on. 2. [Theory] A 2016 article[footnoteRef:0] begins, “Extra-billing in Ontario, private MRIs in Saskatchewan and user fees in Quebec: violations of the Canada Health Act are on the rise across the country.” [0: Meili, R. (2016, April 4). It’s Time For The Federal Government To Enforce The Canada Health Act. The Huffington Post. Retrieved from http://www.huffingtonpost.ca/ryan-meili/canada-health- act_b_9610424.html ]
  • 15. a. What pillar(s) of the Canada Health Act is/are violated by private MRIs in Saskatchewan, and in what way? Be specific. (3 marks) Pillar(s) Violated: ___________________________________________________ Reasoning: _____________________________________________________ ____ b. What pillar(s) of the Canada Health Act is/are violated by user fees for health care in Quebec, and in what way? Be specific. (3 marks) Pillar(s) Violated: ___________________________________________________ Reasoning: _____________________________________________________ ____ 3. [Mini Meta Analysis] This is a simple question meant to help you understand what is expected of you in the group project. Group Assignment 1, due on January 31, will ask you to conduct a search for articles to use in your own mini meta analysis. This question asks you to familiarize yourself with
  • 16. how such searches are reported in published, peer-reviewed mini meta-analyses. Start by accessing the following article: Wiradhany, W. & Koerts, J. (2019). Everyday functioning- related cognitive correlates of media multitasking: a mini meta- analysis. Media Psychology. Retrieved from https://doi.org/10.1080/15213269.2019.1685393 (The article is open-access, so you should have no trouble accessing it, whether on or off campus.) a. (3 marks) The published mini meta-analysis searched for studies in PsycInfo, ERIC, MEDLINE, SocINDEX, CMMC and DOAJ. For this question, use the search methods listed in the ‘Study selection’ section of the mini meta analysis to search for articles fitting the search criteria in Google Scholar ( https://scholar.google.ca/ ). Google Scholar is a very useful tool that searches through peer-reviewed academic literature. In the space below, cite, in APA format, the first study in your search results that fits the criteria specified in the mini meta analysis. (Hint: This question can be answered VERY quickly. The method described in the very first paragraph of the ‘study selection’ section, on pages 4 and 5, should be enough for this assignment question – just make sure your top result is a study, and not a summary of studies, such as a review article, systematic summary or meta-analysis, and that it studies media multitasking, rather than just media OR multitasking.)
  • 17. Full Terms & Conditions of access and use can be found at https://www.tandfonline.com/action/journalInformation?journal Code=hmep20 Media Psychology ISSN: 1521-3269 (Print) 1532-785X (Online) Journal homepage: https://www.tandfonline.com/loi/hmep20 Everyday functioning-related cognitive correlates of media multitasking: a mini meta-analysis Wisnu Wiradhany & Janneke Koerts To cite this article: Wisnu Wiradhany & Janneke Koerts (2019): Everyday functioning-related cognitive correlates of media multitasking: a mini meta- analysis, Media Psychology, DOI: 10.1080/15213269.2019.1685393 To link to this article: https://doi.org/10.1080/15213269.2019.1685393 © 2019 The Author(s). Published with license by Taylor & Francis Group, LLC. Published online: 03 Nov 2019. Submit your article to this journal Article views: 328 View related articles View Crossmark data
  • 18. https://www.tandfonline.com/action/journalInformation?journal Code=hmep20 https://www.tandfonline.com/loi/hmep20 https://www.tandfonline.com/action/showCitFormats?doi=10.10 80/15213269.2019.1685393 https://doi.org/10.1080/15213269.2019.1685393 https://www.tandfonline.com/action/authorSubmission?journalC ode=hmep20&show=instructions https://www.tandfonline.com/action/authorSubmission?journalC ode=hmep20&show=instructions https://www.tandfonline.com/doi/mlt/10.1080/15213269.2019.1 685393 https://www.tandfonline.com/doi/mlt/10.1080/15213269.2019.1 685393 http://crossmark.crossref.org/dialog/?doi=10.1080/15213269.20 19.1685393&domain=pdf&date_stamp=2019-11-03 http://crossmark.crossref.org/dialog/?doi=10.1080/15213269.20 19.1685393&domain=pdf&date_stamp=2019-11-03 Everyday functioning-related cognitive correlates of media multitasking: a mini meta-analysis Wisnu Wiradhany a and Janneke Koerts b aDepartment of Experimental Psychology, University of Groningen, Groningen, the Netherlands; bDepartment of Clinical and Developmental Neuropsychology, University of Groningen, Groningen, the Netherlands ABSTRACT A recent meta-analysis has shown that media multitasking beha- vior, or consuming multiple streams of media simultaneously, might not be associated with less efficient cognitive processing, as measured with objective tests. Nevertheless, a growing num-
  • 19. ber of studies have reported that media multitasking is corre- lated with cognitive functioning in everyday situations, as measured in self-reports. Here, in a series of mini meta- analyses, we show that the self-reported correlates of media multitasking can be categorized in at least four major themes. Heavy media multitasking was associated with increasing pro- blems with attention regulation (e.g., increased mind-wandering and distractibility), behavior regulation (e.g., emotion regulation and self-monitor), inhibition/impulsiveness (e.g., higher level of impulsiveness and lower level of inhibition), and memory. However, the pooled effect sizes were small (z =.16 to z = .22), indicating that a large proportion of variance of media multi- tasking behavior is still unaccounted for. Additionally, we wit- nessed a high level of heterogeneity in the attention regulation theme, which might indicate the presence of the risk of study bias. In recent years, the number of studies investigating the correlates of habitual media multitasking behavior, i.e., consuming multiple streams of media-related information simultaneously, have increased. These studies investigate correlates of media multitaskers using both performance-based and self-reported measures, and have presented an interesting contradic- tion. On the one hand, the group of studies using performance- based measures, that is, highly controlled psychophysics experiments with clear instructions (e.g., to perform as quickly and as accurately as possible) and clear beginning and end, has shown mixed results. Specifically, some
  • 20. studies showed that Heavy Media Multitaskers (HMMs), compared to Light Media Multitaskers (LMMs) displayed worse performance in differ- ent objective, performance-based measures of cognition (Cain, Leonard, CONTACT Wisnu Wiradhany [email protected] Department of Experimental Psychology, University of Groningen, Grote Kruisstraat 2/1, 9712 TS Groningen, the Netherlands MEDIA PSYCHOLOGY https://doi.org/10.1080/15213269.2019.1685393 © 2019 The Author(s). Published with license by Taylor & Francis Group, LLC. This is an Open Access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution-NonCommercial-NoDerivatives License (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by-nc-nd/4.0/), which permits non-commercial re-use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is properly cited, and is not altered, transformed, or built upon in any way. http://orcid.org/0000-0001-8707-3146 http://orcid.org/0000-0002-2317-0171 https://crossmark.crossref.org/dialog/?doi=10.1080/15213269.2 019.1685393&domain=pdf&date_stamp=2019-11-02 Gabrieli, & Finn, 2016; Ophir, Nass, & Wagner, 2009; Ralph & Smilek, 2016), while others reported that HMMs performed better than LMMs (Alzahabi & Becker, 2013; Baumgartner, Weeda, van der
  • 21. Heijden, & Huizinga, 2014). Yet, others reported mixed findings and/or null results (Cardoso-Leite et al., 2015; Gorman & Green, 2016; Minear, Brasher, McCurdy, Lewis, & Younggren, 2013; Murphy, McLauchlan, & Lee, 2017; Ralph, Thomson, Seli, Carriere, & Smilek, 2015; Wiradhany & Nieuwenstein, 2017). With this mixed evidence, it is not surprising that a recent review (van der Schuur, Baumgartner, Sumter, & Valkenburg, 2015) and a meta-analysis (Wiradhany & Nieuwenstein, 2017) have shown that pooled together, the association between media multitasking and performances on performance-based measures of cognition is weak. Furthermore, the meta-analysis has shown that upon applying meta- analytic correction, the pooled association between media multitasking and performances on performance-based measures of cognition turned out to be null. On the other hand, there have been a growing number of studies showing associations between frequent media multitasking and problems reported on rating scales of cognition. Specifically, frequent media multitasking has been associated with more self-reported attention lapses and mind- wandering (Ralph, Thomson, Cheyne, & Smilek, 2013), higher levels of impulsiveness
  • 22. (Cain et al., 2016; Magen, 2017; Minear et al., 2013; Sanbonmatsu, Strayer, Medeiros-Ward, & Watson, 2013; Schutten, Stokes, & Arnell, 2017; Uncapher, Thieu, & Wagner, 2016), a higher number of problems with executive functions (Baumgartner et al., 2014; Magen, 2017), and more (severe) symptoms of Attention Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorders or ADHD (Magen, 2017; Uncapher et al., 2016). Together, these findings suggest that media multitasking is not associated with performances on objective measures of cognition, but never- theless, is associated with different aspects of everyday cognitive functioning. In cognition research, it is common to use both performance- based and self- reported methods for assessment as these two types of assessment often comple- ment one another (e.g., Chan, Shum, Toulopoulou, & Chen, 2008). At the same time, findings from both types of measurement might disagree with one another for several reasons. To start, the two measures arguably estimate one’s ability to function on different levels. Performance-based measures estimate one’s optimal performance: These measures have explicit instructions and are administered under highly standardized conditions. Accordingly, the results of these measures would reflect the efficiency of cognitive processing of an individual (Stanovich, 2009; Toplak, West, & Stanovich, 2013). In contrast, self-
  • 23. reported measures of the same construct estimate one’s typical performance: These measures probe a wide range of everyday behaviors which are related with the construct which is being estimated. Accordingly, the results of these measures would reflect the 2 W. WIRADHANY AND J. KOERTS ability of an individual to execute a task in conditions in which no explicit instructions or goals are given (Stanovich, 2009; Toplak et al., 2013). Critically, it is possible for an individual to score low in one type of measure but high in the other type and vice versa. In this light, the International Classification of Functioning, Disability, and Health (ICF), which is developed by the World Health Organization (World Health Organization, 2001), draws a distinction between functions (i.e., the struc- tural integrity of the body to allow for optimal use) and activities (i.e., the life areas, tasks, and actions associated with an individual). One can have an impairment on the activity level, but might perform well on the func- tional level. For instance, individuals with dysexecutive symptoms might report frequent problems in everyday situations, yet they
  • 24. perform relatively well in an executive function test (Burgess et al., 2006). Similarly, impair- ments on a functional level do not always necessarily result in impairments on the activity level due to compensation and adaptation. For instance, individuals with mild Alzheimer’s disease might perform poorly in an objective test, yet they are able to perform their daily activities using support from their environments (Farias, Harrell, Neumann, & Houtz, 2003). Accordingly, people who frequently media multitask might not perform worse in performance-based measures of cognition, yet report everyday problems associated with cognition due to the fact that laboratory measures might capture some, but not all aspects of cognition or that they measure cognition on a different level compared to self-reported measures. Correlates of media multitasking Due to the ubiquity of media devices in recent years (Lenhart, 2015; Marius & Anggoro, 2014), the frequency and duration of media multi- tasking behavior, consuming multiple streams of media information simul- taneously, have increased dramatically (Carrier, Cheever, Rosen, Benitez, & Chang, 2009; Rideout, Foehr, & Roberts, 2010; Roberts & Foehr, 2008).
  • 25. This behavior is mainly characterized by rapid switches of attention between different media streams. An observational study of concurrent television and computer usage showed that, on average, participants switched their attention 120 times within 27.5 min (Brasel & Gips, 2011). Similarly, another observational study reported that contemporary office workers spent on average 3 min on a task before switching to another (González & Mark, 2004). Switching does not only happen between media devices, but also between different media activities. For instance, Judd (2013) reported from computer session logs that college students switched between different tasks in a computer about 70% of the time and spent on average 2.3 min on one task before switching to another. MEDIA PSYCHOLOGY 3 With the high frequency of switching between different media streams, it is likely for media multitasking behavior to disrupt other ongoing cognitive and behavioral processes. With regard to cognitive processes, media multitasking might disrupt one’s current train of thoughts, which may result in worse task performance. In a study in which participants were asked to study an article
  • 26. about influenza, participants recalled less information about the article in con- ditions in which they were either forced to check their Facebook account or allowed to check their Facebook account while studying the article (Kononova, Joo, & Yuan, 2016). Other studies have shown that media- induced interruptions might have no significant impact on task performance (Fox, Rosen, & Crawford, 2009; Mark, Gudith, & Klocke, 2008), but nevertheless, people who experienced constant interruptions during work reported more stress and frustration at the end of the day (Mark et al., 2008). With regard to behavioral processes, media multitasking behavior might disrupt other everyday behavior patterns. For instance, adolescents who reported higher level of media multitasking also reported having fewer hours of sleep per night (Calamaro, Mason, & Ratcliffe, 2009). Similarly, in a longitudinal study, adolescents with a higher level of media multitasking reported more sleeping problems at the time of the data collection, 3 months, and 6 months later (van der Schuur, Baumgartner, Sumter, & Valkenburg, 2018). The current study Media multitasking behavior might interfere with different ongoing processes in everyday situations. This behavior might not be correlated with performances
  • 27. on objective measures of cognition (van der Schuur et al., 2015; Wiradhany & Nieuwenstein, 2017), but nevertheless, it might have profound impact on every- day cognitive functioning, as indicated by self-reported measures of cognition. This article aims to examine and summarize the current body of literature on media multitasking in order to create an overview of the different domains of everyday cognitive functioning which might be correlated with media multi- tasking behavior. The evidence was synthesized in a series of mini meta-analyses. Additionally, we also examined the risk of bias across the findings and per- formed a moderator analysis if risk of bias occurred. Methods Study selection All studies which investigated correlates of self-reported measures of media multi- tasking and cognition were considered for inclusion. Studies were identified in the PsycInfo, ERIC, MEDLINE, SocINDEX, and CMMC databases, as well as the Directory of Open Access Journals (DOAJ) database. A combination of the 4 W. WIRADHANY AND J. KOERTS following keywords was entered in the search terms: media
  • 28. multitask* AND (problem* OR executive* OR impuls* OR attention*)1. Together, the search yielded 130 results from the first set of databases and 68 results from the DOAJ database. As Figure 1 shows, of the 198 studies identified, 40 were duplicates and therefore removed. Of the 158 studies, only 43 pertained to the term “media multitasking” (i.e., not only pertained to “media” or “multitasking” exclu- sively) and were therefore considered for further screening. Of 43 studies screened, we removed studies which did not meet the criteria below. First, studies must have examined the association between measures of media multitasking and self-report measures of cognition, or psychological traits or mental-health issues related to cognition. Therefore, four review articles (Aagaard, 2015; Carrier, Rosen, Cheever, & Lim, 2015; Lin, 2009; van der Schuur et al., 2015), two meta-analysis (Jeong & Hwang, 2016; Figure 1. A flow diagram showing the selection of study process. MEDIA PSYCHOLOGY 5 Wiradhany & Nieuwenstein, 2017), one measurement validity
  • 29. article (Baumgartner, Lemmens, Weeda, & Huizinga, 2017), 12 articles which only included laboratory task performance measures (Alzahabi & Becker, 2013; Alzahabi, Becker, & Hambrick, 2017; Cain & Mitroff, 2011; Edwards & Shin, 2017; Gorman & Green, 2016; Lui & Wong, 2012; Moisala et al., 2016; Murphy et al., 2017; Ophir et al., 2009; Ralph & Smilek, 2016; Ralph et al., 2015; Yap & Lim, 2013), two articles in which the level of media multitasking was manipulated (Kazakova, Cauberghe, Pandelaere, & De Pelsmacker, 2015; Lin, Robertson, & Lee, 2009), one article in which only a brain imaging measure was used (Loh & Kanai, 2014) and two articles in which only media multitasking behavior was observed (Loh, Tan, & Lim, 2016; Rigby, Brumby, Gould, & Cox, 2017) were excluded from further eligibility assessment. Second, since this study pertains to general media multitasking behavior (i.e., not a specific combination of two media), only studies using a general media multitasking measure were included. Therefore, one article in which only a specific combination of media multitasking was used (Kononova, Zasorina, Diveeva, Kokoeva, & Chelokyan, 2014) and one article (Wu, 2017) which measured the perception of media multitasking ability instead
  • 30. of actual media multitasking frequency were removed. Thirdly, we removed two articles that measured the association between media multitasking and well-being or constructs which are related to well-being (Hatchel, Negriff, & Subrahmanyam, 2018; Pea et al., 2012). Lastly, one article was excluded since the relevant effect sizes could not be extracted from the published article (Shih, 2013)2. In all, a total of 13 articles containing 15 independent studies3 were included for synthesis (Baumgartner, van der Schuur et al., 2017, 2014; Cain et al., 2016; Cardoso-Leite et al., 2015; Duff, Yoon, Wang, & Anghelcev, 2014; Hadlington & Murphy, 2018; Magen, 2017; Minear et al., 2013; Ralph et al., 2013; Sanbonmatsu et al., 2013; Schutten et al., 2017; Uncapher et al., 2016; Yang & Zhu, 2016). Table 1 shows the measures of self- reported functioning included in each study and the number of participants assessed. Effect size selection and calculation Effect sizes were selected from reported outcome measures which reflect distinguishable constructs. For instance, a study examining the association between media multitasking and measures of executive function would report measures of attentional shifting, working memory, and inhibition,
  • 31. which are separate constructs. Study findings related to these measures would be regarded as individual effect sizes. In total, 48 unique effect sizes were extracted from the studies listed in Table 1 and included in the final series of mini meta-analysis. Effect sizes were calculated in Fisher’s z, indicating the normalized correla- tion coefficients between self-reported measures of media multitasking and 6 W. WIRADHANY AND J. KOERTS Ta b le 1. O ve rv ie w of in cl ud ed st ud
  • 84. ud y 1 ac ro ss th e th re e st ud y w av es . MEDIA PSYCHOLOGY 7 self-reported measures of cognition. A positive z indicates that frequent media multitasking is associated with more (severe) issues and a negative z indicates that frequent media multitasking is associated with less (severe) issues. In most cases, the included studies reported Pearson’s product- moment correlations (r) as measures of effect sizes. These r’s were converted
  • 85. into Fisher’s z using formula 1 below (Borenstein, Hedges, Higgins, & Rothstein, 2009): z ¼ 0:5 � ln 1 þ r 1 � r � � (1) In which r is the Pearson’s product-moment correlation. Analysis Categorization of findings Since different studies featured in the meta-analysis and the featured rating scales measured different domains of cognition, we grouped the respective effect sizes into different categories based on the similarity and dissimilarity between constructs. To illustrate, the Mindful Attention Awareness Scale (MAAS; Brown & Ryan, 2003) and the self-monitoring subscales of the Behavioral Ratings of Executive Functions (BRIEF; Gioia, Isquith, Guy, & Kenworthy, 2000; Gioia, Isquith, Retzlaff, & Espy, 2002) infer a relatively similar construct related to attention regulation, which is relatively dissimilar to the construct related to forming precise information in memory inferred by the Memory Failures Scale (MFS; Carriere, Cheyne, & Smilek, 2008).
  • 86. To guide the categorization of our effect sizes, we first made a table of self- report measures in Table 1 along with the goal of each individual measure and some examples of its items. Considering the goal of the measure and its items, we then looked into the literature to gain insights on how to group them in a meaningful way. For instance, measures related to impulsiveness such as the Barratt Impulsiveness Scale (BIS; Patton, Stanford, & Barratt, 1995), measures related to inhibition such as the BRIEF- Inhibition (Gioia et al., 2002), and measures related to sensation seeking such as the Sensation- Seeking Scale (SSS; Zuckerman, 1996) were group together under the over- arching construct of impulsiveness/inhibition (e.g., Dalley, Everitt, & Robbins, 2011). Some measures, such as the ADHD-Adult Self Report Scale (ADHD-ASRS; Kessler et al., 2005) and the Cognitive Failure Questionnaire (CFQ; Broadbent & Cooper, 1982) might have two or more distinct underlying constructs; the former has attention and impulsiveness components and the latter has attention, memory, and other components. To ensure that our resulting categories were as independent from each other as they can be, in the case of the ADHD-ASRS, we contacted the authors to request additional data regarding the correlation between media
  • 87. 8 W. WIRADHANY AND J. KOERTS multitasking and attention deficit and between media multitasking and hyperactivity/impulsiveness separately. In the case of the CFQ, considering that studies, especially recent ones were in disagreement with regard to the underlying dimensions of CFQ (Bridger, Johnsen, & Brasher, 2013; Larson, Alderton, Neideffer, & Underhill, 2011; Wallace, Kass, & Stanny, 2002), we decided to categorize the effect sizes pertained to the general CFQ scores twice: once in the attention regulation category and once in the memory category. For all categories, the first author performed the categorizations and the second author checked the resulted categories. Disagreements between authors were resolved by consensus. Using the categorization processes above, we identified four different themes for correlates between media multitasking and self- reports of every- day cognitive functioning, namely attention regulation, behavior regulation, impulsiveness/inhibition, and memory. The attention regulation theme per- tained to the set of cognitive abilities which help to boost information processing. Traditionally, this includes the ability to react to important cues
  • 88. in the environment (alerting of attention), select relevant from irrelevant information (orienting of attention), and switch from one stimulus-response task rule to another (executive attention; Petersen & Posner, 2012; Posner & Petersen, 1990). More recently, our ability to suppress internally generated task-unrelated thoughts (Christoff, Irving, Fox, Spreng, & Andrews-Hanna, 2016; Smallwood & Schooler, 2006) was also considered in this array of cognitive processing-boosting ability. Accordingly, in this theme, we included measures of attention orientation/selection (e.g., ADHD-ASRS – Inattention subscale), distractibility (e.g., CFQ – distractibility subscale and CFQ – Total), switching from one task to another (e.g., BRIEF – Shifting subscale), and mind-wandering (e.g., MAAS, Mind-Wandering scale). The impulsiveness/inhibition theme pertained to the ability to inhibit premature thoughts and actions, and difficulties with this ability can be exhibited behaviorally in one’s tendency to seek additional stimulations and take risks (Dalley et al., 2011; Dalley & Robbins, 2017). Here, we included measures which were related to inhibition (BRIEF-Inhibition), behavior impulsiveness (e.g., BIS, ADHD-ASRS – Hyperactivity/impulsivity subscale), and sensation-seeking (e.g., SSS).
  • 89. The memory theme pertained to the ability to retain information do mental work with information in memory (e.g., Diamond, 2013). This ability has been considered to be relatively independent to attentional regulation, yet it has been considered to play an important role in executive functioning (Diamond, 2013; Engle, 2002; Engle & Kane, 2004). In this meta-analysis, this theme included measures of working memory (e.g., BRIEF – Working memory subscale), memory failures (e.g., Memory Failures Scale), and gen- eral cognitive failures (e.g., CFQ – Total score). MEDIA PSYCHOLOGY 9 Lastly, the behavior regulation theme pertained to the set of abilities which is related to the volitional control of action. According to one taxonomy of executive function (BRIEF; Gioia et al., 2002; Huizinga & Smidts, 2010), behavior regulation is an umbrella term which includes task- switching and inhibition as well. However, as discussed above, task-switching appears to be more related to attention regulation (e.g., Petersen & Posner, 2012) while inhibition, which is more internally driven, appears to be more related to impulsiveness and risk-taking (e.g., Dalley et al., 2011).
  • 90. Accordingly, we categorized task-switching and inhibition in the attention regulation and impulsiveness/inhibition categories, respectively. Thus, what remains in our behavior regulation theme were abilities that relate to volitional control of action which are driven by external demands and situational factors (e.g., see Tsukayama, Duckworth, & Kim, 2013). This theme included measures of self-control (e.g., Domain-specific Impulsivity in School-age Children4; Personal Problem-solving Inventory – Self-control subscale), emotion regu- lation (e.g., BRIEF – Emotion regulation subscale), and self- monitoring (e.g., BRIEF – Self monitoring subscale). Note that while we sought out to minimize overlaps between the themes and categorize the findings as accurately as possible, the categorization remained somewhat arbitrary as different theoretical models of cognitive function would have both overlaps and distinctions of different sets of cognitive ability (e.g., see Chan et al., 2008). For each theme, a random-effect model and a pooled effect size were calculated to provide estimates of the magnitude of the correlation. Random-effect model Since the current meta-analysis featured different rating scales and outcome measures, we constructed a random-effect model to estimate the
  • 91. pooled effect size. This model assumes that the different scales had comparable, but not identical effect sizes which are distributed around some mean that reflected the true effect (Borenstein et al., 2009). In our case, we assumed that the different outcomes measured different subsets of cognitive functioning. Thus, the effects might vary from one function to another. The random-effect model was constructed in R (R Core team, 2015) using the metafor package (Viechtbauer, 2010). To account for variance inflation of the pooled effect size due to the dependency of multiple outcome measures from one study, we calculated the robust variance estimation (RVE; Hedges, Tipton, & Johnson, 2010). RVE works by estimating the correlations between dependent outcome measures and adjusting the standard error of the pooled effect size based on these correlations (Hedges et al., 2010; Scammacca, Roberts, & Stuebing, 2013). 10 W. WIRADHANY AND J. KOERTS Heterogeneity and risk of bias When significant between-studies heterogeneity was detected, we performed a moderator analysis and a risk of bias analysis. The moderator analysis
  • 92. assesses whether the between-studies heterogeneity can be explained by shared characteristics of different subgroups of studies (Hedges & Pigott, 2004). The risk of bias analysis tested whether the heterogeneity was stemming from bias coming from the level of precision in each study. Under a presence of bias, it is common for studies with smaller sample sizes to show an overestimation of effect sizes due to sampling errors compared with studies with bigger sample sizes, a phenomenon called small-study effect (Sterne, Gavaghan, & Egger, 2000). A small-study effect might indicate the presence of publication bias, since other studies with smaller sample sizes showing underestimation of the effect ended up not being published (Ioannidis, 2005; Ioannidis, Munafò, Fusar-Poli, Nosek, & David, 2014). As a formal inspec- tion of small-study effects, we conducted an Egger’s test (Egger, Davey Smith, Schneider, & Minder, 1997), in which a simple linear regression with effect sizes as a measure of magnitude of study effect and sample sizes or standard errors as measures of study precision is constructed. Results Attention regulation
  • 93. Random-effect model Figure 2 shows a forest plot for a group of self-report scales which measured the association between media multitasking and constructs related to the ability to regulate attention. The scales categorized in this theme included Attentional Control (AC)-switching (e.g., “I am slow to switch from one task to another,” Carriere, Seli, & Smilek, 2013), Attentional Control (AC)- distractibility (e.g., “I have difficulties concentrating when there is music in the room around me”, Carriere et al., 2013), ADHD-ASRS – Inattention (e.g., “How often do you have difficulty concentrating on what people are saying to you even when they are speaking to you directly?”, Kessler et al., 2005), MW – Spontaneous (e.g., “I find my thoughts wandering spontaneously”, Carriere et al., 2008), MW – Deliberate (e.g., “I allow my thoughts to wander on purpose”, Carriere et al., 2008), MAAS – Lapses only (e.g., “I do jobs or tasks automatically, without being aware of what I’m doing”, Brown & Ryan, 2003), ARCES (e.g.,“I have gone to the fridge to get one thing (e.g., milk) and taken something else (e.g., juice),” Carriere et al., 2008), BRIEF-Shift (e.g., “I get stuck on one topic or activity,” Gioia et al., 2002); CFQ – Distractibility, also with CFQ – total score (e.g., “Do you read something and find you
  • 94. MEDIA PSYCHOLOGY 11 haven’t been thinking about it and must read it again?,” Broadbent & Cooper, 1982). Overall, the pooled effect size of the correlates between media multitasking and self-reported problems related to attention regulation was small, yet statistically significant, z = .162, 95% CI [.160, .164], p < .001. At the same time, however, a significant heterogeneity between the effect sizes was detected, I2 = 86.76%, Q(21) = 118.93, p < .001. Heterogeneity & risk of bias analysis To address the heterogeneity in the model, we performed moderator analyses with two moderators. First, we added sex, as indicated by the proportion of females in the study samples as a moderator. Second, we added age, as indicated by the mean age of the study samples as a moderator. The two moderators did not contribute to the unexplained variance in the model, F(1, 17) = 2.59, p = .125; F(1, 11) = 3.08, p = .107, respectively, indicating that the heterogeneity could not be explained by differences in sex, and age. As for the risk of bias, the Egger’s test showed no relationship between
  • 95. effect size and study precision, z = −1.46, p = .144. This indicates that under the presence of heterogeneity, effect sizes were stable across different studies with different sample sizes. Figure 2. Forest plot of the effect sizes (Fisher’s z) for studies measuring the association between media multitasking and attention regulation. Error bars indicate 95% confidence intervals of the means. AC: Attentional Control scale; ADHD-ASRS: Attention Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder Adult Self Report Scale; ARCES: Attention-Related Cognitive Error Scale; BRIEF: Behavior Rating Inventory of Executive Function; CFQ: Cognitive Failure Questionnaire; MAAS: Mindful Awareness Attention Scale; MW: Mind-Wandering scale. 12 W. WIRADHANY AND J. KOERTS Impulsiveness – inhibition Random-effect model Figure 3 shows a forest plot for a group of self-report scales which measured the association between media multitasking and constructs related to impul- siveness and/or inhibition. The scales categorized in this theme included ADHD-ASRS – hyperactivity (e.g., “How often do you fidget or squirm with your hands or your feet when you have to sit down for a long time?”, Kessler et al., 2005), BIS (e.g. “I do things without thinking”,
  • 96. Patton et al., 1995), BRIEF-Inhibit (e.g., “I do not think before doing,” Gioia et al., 2002), SSS, also the brief version; B-SSS (e.g., “I sometimes like to do things that are a little frightening”, Zuckerman, 1996), and RCsB (e.g., “Sharing passwords with friends and colleagues”). Overall, the pooled effect size of the correlates between media multitasking and self-reported problems related to impulsiveness and/or inhibition was small, yet statistically significant, z = .219, 95% CI [.218, .219], p < .001. The between-studies heterogeneity was low, I2 < .001%, Q(14) = 9.82, p = .775, indicating that the effect was consistent across different studies. Memory Random-effect model Figure 4 shows a forest plot for a group of self-report scales which measured the association between media multitasking and constructs related to mem- ory. The scales categorized in this theme included BRIEF- Working Memory Figure 3. Forest plot of the effect sizes (Fisher’s z) for studies measuring the association between media multitasking and impulsiveness and/or inhibition. Error bars indicate 95% confidence intervals of the means. ADHD-ASRS: Attention Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder – Adult Self Report Scales; B-SSS: Brief Sensation-Seeking Scale; BIS: Barratt
  • 97. Impulsiveness Scale; BRIEF: Behavior Rating Inventory of Executive Function; RCsB: Risky Cybersecurity Behavior scale; SSS: Sensation- seeking Scale. MEDIA PSYCHOLOGY 13 (e.g., “I have trouble remembering things, even for a few minutes,” Gioia et al., 2002), CFQ (e.g., “Do you find you forget people’s names?”, Broadbent & Cooper, 1982), and MFS (e.g., “I forget what I went to the supermarket to buy”, Carriere et al., 2008). Overall, the pooled effect size of the correlates between media multitasking and self-reported problems related to memory was small, yet statistically significant, z = .158, 95% CI [.156, .161], p < .001. The between-studies heterogeneity was low, I2 = 16.49%, Q(4) = 4.67, p = .323, indicating that the effect was consistent across different studies. Behavior regulation Random-effect model Figure 5 shows a forest plot for a group of self-report scales which measured the association between media multitasking and constructs related to the ability to regulate behavior. The scales categorized in this theme included
  • 98. Emotional Control (e.g. “Has outburst for little reason”, Gioia et al., 2002), BRIEF-Initiate (e.g., “I need to be told to begin a task even when willing”), Figure 4. Forest plot of the effect sizes (Fisher’s z) for studies measuring the association between media multitasking and memory. Error bars indicate 95% confidence intervals of the means. BRIEF: Behavior Rating Inventory of Executive Function; CFQ: Cognitive Failures Questionnaire; MFS: Memory Failures Scale. Figure 5. Forest plot of the effect sizes (Fisher’s z) for studies measuring the association between media multitasking and behavior regulation. Error bars indicate 95% confidence intervals of the means. BRIEF: Behavior Rating Inventory of Executive Function; DISC: Domain-specific Impulsivity in School-age Children; PPSI: Personal Problem-solving Inventory. 14 W. WIRADHANY AND J. KOERTS BRIEF-Organization of Materials (e.g., “I cannot find things in room or school desk”), Plan/Organize, (e.g., “I become overwhelmed by large assign- ments”), BRIEF-Self-monitor (e.g. “I am unaware of how my behavior affects or bothers others”), BRIEF-Task-Monitor (e.g., “I make careless errors”), DiSC (e.g., “I interrupted other people” Tsukayama et al., 2013), and PPSI-
  • 99. Personal control (e.g., “Sometimes I do not stop and take time to deal with my problems, but just kind of muddle ahead” Heppner & Petersen, 1982). Overall, the pooled effect size of the correlates between media multitasking and self-reported problems related to behavior regulation was small, yet statistically significant, z = .192, 95% CI [.190, .193], p < .001. The between- studies heterogeneity was low, I2 = 6.67%, Q(8) = 5.69, p = .683, indicating that the effect was consistent across different studies. General discussion In this meta-analysis, we examined the correlates of media multitasking behavior with different domains of everyday cognitive functioning in a series of mini meta-analysis. The effect sizes were categorized into different themes reflecting different domains of everyday cognitive functioning, based on the similarities and dissimilarities between the reflected constructs. Overall, the effect sizes can be categorized into four distinct themes. Pooled together, frequent media multitasking had weak, but significant associations with a decrease of attention regulation (z = .16), lower levels of inhibition/higher levels of impulsiveness (z = .22), an increase of memory problems (z = .16), and a decreased behavior regulation (z = .19).
  • 100. Regarding the association between media multitasking and attention reg- ulation, we found that heavy media multitasking was associated with higher frequency of mind-wandering, higher distractibility, and more problems with task switching. With regard to mind-wandering, this finding was somewhat consistent with other findings in the literature which used objective mea- sures. In an experiment in which participants were asked to memorize materials from a video-recorded lecture, Loh et al. (2016) found that heavy media multitaskers retained less information from the lecture, and this effect could be explained by the increased tendency to mind-wander in this group. However, at least one study showed a null correlation between media multi- tasking and mind-wandering: heavy media multitaskers performed worse in a metronome-response task, but they did not show a tendency to have increased mind-wandering during the experiment (Ralph et al., 2015). With regard to distractibility, other studies which used objective measures showed that, for instance, heavy media multitaskers were less able to filter out irrelevant information from their immediate environment (Cain & Mitroff, 2011; Ophir et al., 2009). However, a recent meta-analysis (Wiradhany & Nieuwenstein, 2017) has shown that other studies have failed to
  • 101. replicate this MEDIA PSYCHOLOGY 15 finding. Lastly, with regard to task-switching, other studies using objective tests showed mixed evidence for the correlation between media multitasking and task switching. Some studies found a negative correlation between media multitasking and task performance (Ophir et al., 2009; Wiradhany & Nieuwenstein, 2017, Exp., p. 1), others found positive correlations (Alzahabi & Becker, 2013), yet others found null results (see Wiradhany & Nieuwenstein, 2017, for a meta-analysis). Thus, it can be said that while frequent media multitasking is associated with more problems with atten- tional control in everyday situations, media multitasking might not directly influence one’s ability to regulate attention, as measured by objective tests, per se. Heavier media multitasking was associated with increased impulsiveness/ decreased inhibition; heavier media multitaskers were associated with higher scores in impulsiveness traits and they reported more (severe) symptoms of hyperactivity/impulsivity. Heavier media multitasking was also associated
  • 102. with higher scores in other traits which are related to impulsiveness, such as sensation-seeking and risk-taking (Dalley et al., 2011; Whiteside & Lynam, 2001). This finding was consistent with findings using objective measures. For instance, heavy media multitaskers were more likely to choose smaller, immediate rewards instead of later, larger ones and they endorsed intuitive, but incorrect answers of the Cognitive Reflection Test (Schutten et al., 2017). Additionally, another study indicated that HMMs scored lower in a fluid intelligence test due to them giving up earlier in the test (Minear et al., 2013). Individuals with higher levels of sensation-seeking trait are characterized by a higher stimulation threshold for optimal behavioral performance (Hoyle, Stephenson, Palmgreen, Lorch, & Donohew, 2002; Zuckerman, 2007) and a higher likelihood to act prematurely without foresight, which at times lead to risk-taking behaviors (Dalley et al., 2011; Hoyle et al., 2002; Zuckerman, 2007). Indeed, consuming multiple streams of information has been shown to promote a higher level of engagement (Bardhi, Rohm, & Sultan, 2010; Wang & Tchernev, 2009) and to provide gratifications (Hwang, Kim, & Jeong, 2014) which together provide stimulations for those who seek them. Accordingly, people with higher levels of sensation-seeking and risk-taking
  • 103. might media multitask to seek for additional stimulations. Heavier media multitasking was associated with increased problems related to memory. In this regard, one study using an objective measure, namely a change-detection task has shown that HMMs had difficulties retaining specific information in working memory, regardless of the presence of distractors, and importantly, they performed more poorly in a later long- term memory test for both relevant and irrelevant objects compared to LMMs (Uncapher et al., 2016; but see Wiradhany, van Vugt, & Nieuwenstein, 2019 for null results). Other studies have shown that HMMs, compared to LMMs performed worse in a complex working memory 16 W. WIRADHANY AND J. KOERTS test (Sanbonmatsu et al., 2013) and an N-back test (Cain et al., 2016; Ophir et al., 2009; Ralph & Smilek, 2016; but see Cardoso-Leite et al., 2015; Wiradhany & Nieuwenstein, 2017 for null results). Lastly, regarding the association between media multitasking and behavior regulation, we found that heavier media multitaskers had more difficulties to adjust their thoughts, emotions, and actions to the situational demands. This
  • 104. finding is in line with its counterpart in objective tasks, where studies have found that HMMs performed worse in tasks in which they have to respond to different cue-probe contingencies, such as the AXE-CPT task (Ophir et al., 2009; Wiradhany & Nieuwenstein, 2017; but see Cardoso-Leite et al., 2015 for null results). However, there were mixed findings with regard to perfor- mance of HMMs in a change-detection task in which distractor filtering was involved, with one study showed that HMMs performed worse (Ophir et al., 2009), while other, recent ones showed null findings (Cardoso- Leite et al., 2015; Uncapher et al., 2016; Wiradhany & Nieuwenstein, 2017; Wiradhany et al., 2019). Collectively, we witnessed a discrepancy between the findings in this meta- analysis and the findings in a previous meta-analysis (Wiradhany & Nieuwenstein, 2017). In this meta-analysis, we found overall weak, but stable- pooled correlations between media multitasking and self-reports of cognitive functioning in everyday situations whereas in the previous meta-analysis, we found an overall weak-pooled correlation, but the correlation became null upon corrections. This discrepancy, as we previously mentioned, might exist for several reasons. First, performance-based measures might capture some,
  • 105. but not all aspects of everyday cognitive functioning. Consider the tests for one’s ability to regulate attention, for instance. One group of researcher may assess attention regulation using the perspective of mind- wandering to inves- tigate the waxing and waning of attention (e.g., Christoff et al., 2016). Yet, others assess attention regulation using the perspective of divided attention (e.g., Moisala et al., 2016). The two perspectives might cover some, but not all aspects of attention regulation, and in everyday situations one might need to suppress both mind-wandering and distraction to regulate attention properly. Second, performance-based measures are often designed to assess one’s ability at a pathological level (Chan et al., 2008). For instance, as a diagnostic tool to inquire whether one’s ability to regulate attention is clearly impaired. Therefore, one interpretation of the weak correlations across all themes would be that media multitasking behavior is associated with the increased number of everyday problems related to cognition, but this does not mean that media multitasking is associated with the presence of an impairment in cognitive abilities. The weak correlations between media multi- tasking and everyday cognitive functioning as shown in self- reports suggest that performance-based measures might not be adequately sensitive to detect
  • 106. everyday cognitive problems in media multitaskers. MEDIA PSYCHOLOGY 17 Notes on causality Media multitasking behavior might precede, occur as a consequence, or have a reciprocal relationship with everyday cognitive functioning. Currently, this meta-analysis does not allow for disentangling the causal relationship between the two. Preceding problems with cognition, media multitasking behavior may promote a specific mode of processing information in the environment (Lin, 2009. Specifically, heavy media multitaskers might develop a breadth-biased focus of attention, due to constant exposures to media- saturated environments. That is, they prefer to skim a large quantity of information rather than deeply processing a small amount of information. Consequently, adopting this mode of information processing might lead media multitaskers to apply cognitive control processes such as thought- monitoring and attention regulation less strictly. This might have profound consequences. In an fMRI study; Moisala et al. (2016) found that in addition to worse task performance in which participants had to attend to sentences in
  • 107. one modality (e.g. auditory) while they had to ignore distractor sentences presented in another modality (e.g. visual), HMMs, compared to LMMs also have higher activations in the right superior and medial frontal gyri, and the medial frontal gyrus. Increased activations in these areas have been linked to, among others, increased top-down attentional control. Therefore, heavy media multitaskers might require more effort in filtering distracting informa- tion than light media multitaskers. Alternatively, it could also be the case that media multitasking behavior leads to overreliance of exogenous control of attention (i.e. from incoming notifications from media; Ralph et al., 2013). Consequently, heavy media multitaskers train their endogenous control less often and thus, experience more problems related to cognitive control. Media multitasking behavior might also occur as a consequence of existing problems with cognition. People with ADHD and people with problems with behavior regulation and metacognition are more easily distracted and therefore have a higher propensity to media multitask. Similarly, people with high levels of sensation-seeking are more inclined to media multitask for stimulation- seeking purposes. Relatedly, indicating that excessive media multitasking beha- vior might be a result from a preexisting condition, studies have
  • 108. also shown that individuals with smaller grey matter volumes in the Anterior Cingulate Cortex (ACC) – a brain region which has been shown to be more active during error and conflict detections (Botvinick, Braver, Barch, Carter, & Cohen, 2001; Botvinick, Cohen, & Carter, 2004) – reported higher levels of media multi- tasking (Loh & Kanai, 2014). Similarly, the increased activations of the brain areas associated with top-down control in heavy media multitaskers (Moisala et al., 2016) might also indicate that these areas function less efficiently in heavy media multitaskers, compared to light media multitaskers. 18 W. WIRADHANY AND J. KOERTS Lastly, media multitasking behavior might have a reciprocal relationship with problems with cognition and vice versa. On this reciprocity, several longitudinal studies have attempted to examine whether media multitasking behavior and everyday-related problems are reinforcing each other over a longer time period. The results of these studies showed that media multi- tasking did not appear to have a reciprocal relationship with the occurrence attentional problems (Baumgartner, van der Schuur, Lemmens, & Te Poel, 2017) 3 and 6 months later. Nevertheless, these studies showed
  • 109. that the associations between media multitasking and attentional problems were stable over time. That is, the correlation remained significant during the first, second, and third periods of data collection. Together, this might indicate that individuals have a stable level of media multitasking behavior over time and similarly, the occurrence of some everyday- related problems is also stable over time. Limitation and future directions The findings in our set of mini-meta-analyses are limited in several ways. First, while the effects found in different groups of findings were somewhat reliable across different studies, critically, the overall pooled effects were weak, with z ranging from .16 to .22. Thus, most of the variance underlying the media multitasking behavior is still unaccounted for. Additionally, while we refer to the literature, our categorization of effect sizes remained some- what subjective. This subjectivity might introduce bias and or contribute to our level of within-theme heterogeneity. We witnessed a high level of hetero- geneity in the attention regulation theme, and this high heterogeneity could not be explained by our moderators. Arguably, this high level of heteroge- neity might be driven by the effect sizes related to CFQ, which
  • 110. dimension- ality is still being argued for in recent studies (e.g., Bridger et al., 2013). It might be that the null and negative correlations in the CFQ- related effect sizes were driven by the other dimensions of CFQ. Third, the current meta-analysis focuses on studies pertained to everyday cognitive functioning. However, media multitasking studies have gone beyond the cognition-related themes, as some studies have investigated the correlates between media multitasking and depression (Becker, Alzahabi, & Hopwood, 2013), anxiety (Becker et al., 2013; Hatchel et al., 2018), creativity and imagination (Duff et al., 2014), and well-being (Pea et al., 2012; Shih, 2013; Xu, Wang, & David, 2016). With regards to depression and anxiety, heavy media multitasking was correlated with more (severe) depressive and anxiety symptoms (Becker et al., 2013; Hatchel et al., 2018). This finding was somewhat consistent with a recent nation-wide study which also showed that individuals who use multiple social media platforms in daily life had higher odds of having increased levels of depression and anxiety (Primack et al., MEDIA PSYCHOLOGY 19
  • 111. 2017). Furthermore, media multitasking was negatively correlated with ima- gination, but it was positively correlated with creativity (Duff et al., 2014). Lastly, the correlations between media multitasking and well- being were somewhat mixed. In a large-scale study which involved 3461 8- 12-year-old girls, Pea et al. (2012) found that media multitasking was positively corre- lated with social success, but it was negatively correlated with normalcy feelings, positive feelings, and social stress. Shih (2013) found that media multitasking was not correlated with well-being as assessed using two ver- sions of self-report questionnaires which focused on well-being. While part of these findings was discouraging, suggesting that media multitasking beha- vior might have potential ramifications on other aspects of everyday func- tioning beyond cognition, the other part, namely the positive correlations with social success and creativity suggests that media multitasking behavior might be beneficial as well. It could be interesting for future studies to further examine the adaptive values of everyday media multitasking behavior, espe- cially given that several longitudinal studies have indicated that media multi- tasking behavior is stable over time (Baumgartner, van der Schuur et al., 2017; van der Schuur et al., 2018).
  • 112. Fourth and lastly, since all findings we synthesized in the meta- analysis were correlational, it is still an open question whether media multitasking behavior leads to, is an effect, or have a reciprocal relationship with the occurrence of cognitive problems in everyday situations. Futures studies might be interested in disentangling this association in a more controlled manner. Conclusion In a series of mini meta-analyses, we categorized the correlates between media multitasking and everyday cognitive functioning, as assessed using self-reports, in four different themes. Heavier media multitasking was asso- ciated with increased of levels of self-reported problems with attention regulation, behavior regulation, impulsiveness/inhibition, and memory. Together, media multitasking appears to be correlated with increasing pro- blems everyday cognitive functioning. However, the overall small effects were small, a high level of heterogeneity was detected in one theme, and a large proportion of variance of media multitasking behavior is still unaccounted for. Additionally, since most studies reported correlations, the causality direction is still unclear.
  • 113. Notes 1. To ensure that all possible relevant results have been included in the meta-analysis, in addition to these keywords, we performed a search using more general keywords, 20 W. WIRADHANY AND J. KOERTS namely media multitask* AND (cognition OR emotion OR trait). This search yielded no additional results. Lists of the references found using our search terms can be found in the supplementary materials of this article. 2. The author was contacted for requesting the relevant zero- order correlations not reported in the article. Unfortunately, due to unforeseen circumstances, the original dataset was no longer available. Nevertheless, we are thankful to Dr. Shui-I Shih for her cooperation. 3. Two of the studies (Baumgartner, van der Schuur, et al., 2017) were longitudinal studies with three waves each. All study waves were included (see Table 1). 4. Note that while this measure has impulsivity on its name, the scale was intended to measure how an individual may act (e.g., suppressing their impulse) in situational contexts (Tsukayama et al., 2013, p. 880). These authors also proposed a distinction
  • 114. between domain-specific impulsivity, which is externally driven and measured by their scale and domain-general impulsivity, which is more internally driven and measured by other impulsivity scales such as the BIS. Acknowledgments This article was written as a part of the Ph. D. project of the first author, which is funded by the Endowment Fund for Education (LPDP), Ministry of Finance, the Republic of Indonesia. We thank Prof. Pedro Cardoso-Leite and Dr. Melina Uncapher for providing us additional data regarding their ADHD findings. Disclosure statement No potential conflict of interest was reported by the authors. Funding This work was supported by the Indonesia Endowment Fund for Education; ORCID Wisnu Wiradhany http://orcid.org/0000-0001-8707-3146 Janneke Koerts http://orcid.org/0000-0002-2317-0171 References Aagaard, J. (2015). Media multitasking, attention, and distraction: A critical discussion. Phenomenology and the Cognitive Sciences, 14(4), 885–896. doi:10.1007/s11097-014-9375-x
  • 115. Alzahabi, R., & Becker, M. W. (2013). The association between media multitasking, task-switching, and dual-task performance. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 39(5), 1485–1495. doi:10.1037/a0031208 Alzahabi, R., Becker, M. W., & Hambrick, D. Z. (2017). Investigating the relationship between media multitasking and processes involved in task-switching. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 43(11), 1872– 1894. doi:10.1037/ xhp0000412 MEDIA PSYCHOLOGY 21 https://doi.org/10.1007/s11097-014-9375-x https://doi.org/10.1037/a0031208 https://doi.org/10.1037/xhp0000412 https://doi.org/10.1037/xhp0000412 Bardhi, F., Rohm, A. J., & Sultan, F. (2010). Tuning in and tuning out: Media multitasking among young consumers. Journal of Consumer Behaviour, 9, 316–332. doi:10.1002/cb Baumgartner, S. E., Lemmens, J. S., Weeda, W. D., & Huizinga, M. (2017). Measuring media multitasking. Journal of Media Psychology, 29, 188–197. doi:10.1027/1864-1105/a000167 Baumgartner, S. E., van der Schuur, W. A., Lemmens, J. S., & Te Poel, F. (2017). The
  • 116. relationship between media multitasking and attention problems in adolescents: Results of two longitudinal studies. Human Communication Research, 1–27. doi:10.1111/ hcre.12111 Baumgartner, S. E., Weeda, W. D., van der Heijden, L. L., & Huizinga, M. (2014). The relationship between media multitasking and executive function in early adolescents. The Journal of Early Adolescence, 34(8), 1120–1144. doi:10.1177/0272431614523133 Becker, M. W., Alzahabi, R., & Hopwood, C. J. (2013). Media multitasking is associated with symptoms of depression and social anxiety. Cyberpsychology, Behavior and Social Networking, 16(2), 132–135. doi:10.1089/cyber.2012.0291 Borenstein, M., Hedges, L. V., Higgins, J. P. T., & Rothstein, H. R. (2009). Introduction to meta-analysis. West Sussex, UK: John Wiley & Sons. Botvinick, M. M., Braver, Barch, D. M., Carter, C. S., & Cohen, J. D. (2001). Conflict monitoring and cognitive control. Psychological Review, 108(3), 624–652. doi:10.1037// 0033-295X.I08.3.624 Botvinick, M. M., Cohen, J. D., & Carter, C. S. (2004). Conflict monitoring and anterior cingulate cortex: An update. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 8(12), 539–546. doi:10.1016/j. tics.2004.10.003 Brasel, S. A., & Gips, J. (2011). Media multitasking behavior:
  • 117. Concurrent television and computer usage. Cyberpsychology, Behavior and Social Networking, 14(9), 527–534. doi:10.1089/cyber.2010.0350 Bridger, R. S., Johnsen, S. Å. K., & Brasher, K. (2013). Psychometric properties of the cognitive failures questionnaire†. Ergonomics, 56(10), 1515–1524. doi:10.1080/00140139.2013.821172 Broadbent, D., & Cooper, P. (1982). The cognitive failures questionnaire (CFQ) and its correlates. British Journal of Clinical Psychology, 21(1), 1–16. Retrieved from http://online library.wiley.com/doi/10.1111/j.2044-8260.1982.tb01421.x/full Brown, K. W., & Ryan, R. M. (2003). The benefits of being present: Mindfulness and its role in psychological well-being. Jounal of Personality and Social Psychology, 84(4), 822–848. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.84.4.822 Burgess, P. W., Alderman, N., Forbes, C., Costello, A., Coates, L. M., Dawson, D. R., … Channon, S. (2006). The case for the development and use of “ecologically valid” measures of executive function in experimental and clinical neuropsychology. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society : JINS, 12(2), 194–209. doi:10.1017/S1355617706060310 Cain, M. S., Leonard, J. A., Gabrieli, J. D. E., & Finn, A. S. (2016). Media multitasking in adolescence. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 23(6), 1932– 1941. doi:10.3758/s13423-016- 1036-3
  • 118. Cain, M. S., & Mitroff, S. R. (2011). Distractor filtering in media multitaskers. Perception, 40 (10), 1183–1192. doi:10.1068/p7017 Calamaro, C. J., Mason, T. B. A., & Ratcliffe, S. J. (2009). Adolescents living the 24/7 lifestyle: Effects of caffeine and technology on sleep duration and daytime functioning. Pediatrics, 123(6), e1005–e1010. doi:10.1542/peds.2008-3641 Cardoso-Leite, P., Kludt, R., Vignola, G., Ma, W. J., Green, C. S., & Bavelier, D. (2015). Technology consumption and cognitive control: Contrasting action video game experience with media multitasking. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 78(1), 218–241. doi:10.3758/s13414-015-0988-0 22 W. WIRADHANY AND J. KOERTS https://doi.org/10.1002/cb https://doi.org/10.1027/1864-1105/a000167 https://doi.org/10.1111/hcre.12111 https://doi.org/10.1111/hcre.12111 https://doi.org/10.1177/0272431614523133 https://doi.org/10.1089/cyber.2012.0291 https://doi.org/10.1037//0033-295X.I08.3.624 https://doi.org/10.1037//0033-295X.I08.3.624 https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2004.10.003 https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2004.10.003 https://doi.org/10.1089/cyber.2010.0350 https://doi.org/10.1080/00140139.2013.821172 http://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1111/j.2044- 8260.1982.tb01421.x/full http://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1111/j.2044-
  • 119. 8260.1982.tb01421.x/full https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.84.4.822 https://doi.org/10.1017/S1355617706060310 https://doi.org/10.3758/s13423-016-1036-3 https://doi.org/10.3758/s13423-016-1036-3 https://doi.org/10.1068/p7017 https://doi.org/10.1542/peds.2008-3641 https://doi.org/10.3758/s13414-015-0988-0 Carrier, L. M., Cheever, N. A., Rosen, L. D., Benitez, S., & Chang, J. (2009). Multitasking across generations: Multitasking choices and difficulty ratings in three generations of Americans. Computers in Human Behavior, 25(2), 483–489. doi:10.1016/j.chb.2008.10.012 Carrier, L. M., Rosen, L. D., Cheever, N. A., & Lim, A. F. (2015). Causes, effects, and practicalities of everyday multitasking. Developmental Review, 35, 64–78. doi:10.1016/j. dr.2014.12.005 Carriere, J. S. A., Cheyne, J. A., & Smilek, D. (2008). Everyday attention lapses and memory failures: The affective consequences of mindlessness. Consciousness and Cognition, 17(3), 835–847. doi:10.1016/j.concog.2007.04.008 Carriere, J. S. A., Seli, P., & Smilek, D. (2013). Wandering in both mind and body: Individual differences in mind wandering and inattention predict fidgeting. Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology/Revue Canadienne De Psychologie Expérimentale, 67(1), 19–31. doi:10.1037/a0031438
  • 120. Chan, R. C. K., Shum, D., Toulopoulou, T., & Chen, E. Y. H. (2008). Assessment of executive functions: Review of instruments and identification of critical issues. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology, 23(2), 201–216. doi:10.1016/j.acn.2007.08.010 Christoff, K., Irving, Z. C., Fox, K. C. R., Spreng, R. N., & Andrews-Hanna, J. R. (2016). Mind-wandering as spontaneous thought: A dynamic framework. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 17(11), 1–44. doi:10.1038/nrn.2016.113 Dalley, J. W., Everitt, B. J., & Robbins, T. W. (2011). Impulsivity, compulsivity, and top-down cognitive control. Neuron, 69(4), 680–694. doi:10.1016/j.neuron.2011.01.020 Dalley, J. W., & Robbins, T. W. (2017). Fractionating impulsivity: Neuropsychiatric implications. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 18(3), 158–171. doi:10.1038/nrn.2017.8 Diamond, A. (2013). Executive functions. The Annual Review of Psychology, 64, 135–168. doi:10.1146/annurev-psych-113011-143750 Duff, B. R.-L., Yoon, G., Wang, Z., (Glenn), & Anghelcev, G. (2014). Doing it all: An exploratory study of predictors of media multitasking. Journal of Interactive Advertising, 14(1), 11–23. 10.1080/15252019.2014.884480 Edwards, K. S., & Shin, M. (2017). Media multitasking and implicit learning. Attention,
  • 121. Perception, & Psychophysics, 79(5), 1535–1549. doi:10.3758/s13414-017-1319-4 Egger, M., Davey Smith, G., Schneider, M., & Minder, C. (1997). Bias in meta-analysis detected by a simple, graphical test. BMJ (Clinical Research ed.), 315(7109), 629–634. doi:10.1136/bmj.316.7129.469 Engle, R. W. (2002). Working memory capacity as executive attention. Psychological Science, 11(1), 19–23. doi:10.1111/1467-8721.00160 Engle, R. W., & Kane, M. J. (2004). Executive attention, working memory capacity, and a two-factor theory of cognitive control. The Psychology of Learning and Motivation, 44, 145–199. doi:10.1016/S0079-7421(03)44005-X Farias, S. T., Harrell, E., Neumann, C., & Houtz, A. (2003). The relationship between neuropsychological performance and daily functioning in individuals with Alzheimer’s disease: Ecological validity of neuropsychological tests. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology, 18(6), 655–672. doi:10.1016/S0887- 6177(02)00159-2 Fox, A. B., Rosen, J., & Crawford, M. (2009). Distractions, distractions: Does instant messa- ging affect college students’ performance on a concurrent reading comprehension task? Cyberpsychology & Behavior : the Impact of the Internet, Multimedia and Virtual Reality on Behavior and Society, 12(1), 51–53. doi:10.1089/cpb.2008.0107
  • 122. Gioia, G. A., Isquith, P. K., Guy, S. C., & Kenworthy, L. (2000). TEST REVIEW behavior rating inventory of executive function behavior rating inventory of executive function. Child Neuropsychology, 6(3), 235–238. doi:10.1076/chin.6.3.235.3152 MEDIA PSYCHOLOGY 23 https://doi.org/10.1016/j.chb.2008.10.012 https://doi.org/10.1016/j.dr.2014.12.005 https://doi.org/10.1016/j.dr.2014.12.005 https://doi.org/10.1016/j.concog.2007.04.008 https://doi.org/10.1037/a0031438 https://doi.org/10.1016/j.acn.2007.08.010 https://doi.org/10.1038/nrn.2016.113 https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuron.2011.01.020 https://doi.org/10.1038/nrn.2017.8 https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev-psych-113011-143750 http://10.1080/15252019.2014.884480 https://doi.org/10.3758/s13414-017-1319-4 https://doi.org/10.1136/bmj.316.7129.469 https://doi.org/10.1111/1467-8721.00160 https://doi.org/10.1016/S0079-7421(03)44005-X https://doi.org/10.1016/S0887-6177(02)00159-2 https://doi.org/10.1089/cpb.2008.0107 https://doi.org/10.1076/chin.6.3.235.3152 Gioia, G. A., Isquith, P. K., Retzlaff, P. D., & Espy, K. A. (2002). Confirmatory factor analysis of the behavior rating inventory of executive function (BRIEF) in a clinical sample. Child Neuropsychology, 8(4), 249–257. doi:10.1076/chin.8.4.249.13513
  • 123. González, V. M., & Mark, G. (2004). “Constant, constant, multi-tasking craziness”: Managing multiple working spheres. CHI ’04 Proceedings of the SIGCHI Conference on Human Factors in Computing Systems, 6(1), 113–120. doi:10.1145/985692.985707 Gorman, T. E., & Green, C. S. (2016). Short-term mindfulness intervention reduces the negative attentional effects associated with heavy media multitasking. Scientific Reports, 6 (April), 24542. doi:10.1038/srep24542 Hadlington, L., & Murphy, K. (2018). Is media multitasking good for cybersecurity? Exploring the relationship between media multitasking and everyday cognitive failures on self-reported risky cybersecurity behaviors. Cyberpsychology, Behavior and Social Networking, 21(3), 168–172. doi:10.1089/cyber.2017.0524 Hatchel, T., Negriff, S., & Subrahmanyam, K. (2018). The relation between media multi- tasking, intensity of use, and well-being in a sample of ethnically diverse emerging adults. Computers in Human Behavior, 81, 115–123. doi:10.1016/j.chb.2017.12.012 Hedges, L. V., & Pigott, T. D. (2004). The power of statistical tests for moderators in meta-analysis. Psychological Methods, 9(4), 426–445. doi:10.1037/1082-989X.9.4.426 Hedges, L. V., Tipton, E., & Johnson, M. C. (2010). Robust variance estimation in meta-regression with dependent effect size estimates. Research
  • 124. Synthesis Methods, 1(1), 39–65. doi:10.1002/jrsm.5 Heppner, P. P., & Petersen, C. H. (1982). The development and implications of a personal problem-solving inventory. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 29(1), 66–75. doi:10.1037/ 0022-0167.29.1.66 Hoyle, R. H., Stephenson, M. T., Palmgreen, P., Lorch, E. P., & Donohew, R. L. (2002). Reliability and validity of a brief measure of sensation seeking. Personality and Individual Differences, 32(3), 401–414. doi:10.1016/S0191- 8869(01)00032-0 Huizinga, M., & Smidts, D. P. (2010). Age-related changes in executive function: A normative study with the Dutch version of the Behavior Rating Inventory of Executive Function (BRIEF). Child Neuropsychology, 17(1), 51–66. doi:10.1080/09297049.2010.509715 Hwang, Y., Kim, H., & Jeong, S. H. (2014). Why do media users multitask?: Motives for general, medium-specific, and content-specific types of multitasking. Computers in Human Behavior, 36, 542–548. doi:10.1016/j.chb.2014.04.040 Ioannidis, J. P. A. (2005). Why most published research findings are false. PLoS Medicine, 2 (8), e124. doi:10.1371/journal.pmed.0020124 Ioannidis, J. P. A., Munafò, M. R., Fusar-Poli, P., Nosek, B. A., & David, S. P. (2014). Publication and other reporting biases in cognitive sciences:
  • 125. Detection, prevalence, and prevention. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 18(5), 235–241. doi:10.1016/j.tics.2014.02.010 Jeong, S.-H., & Hwang, Y. (2016). Media multitasking effects on cognitive vs. Attitudinal outcomes: A meta-analysis. Human Communication Research, 42(4), 599–618. doi:10.1111/ hcre.12089 Judd, T. (2013). Making sense of multitasking: Key behaviours. Computers and Education, 63, 358–367. doi:10.1016/j.compedu.2012.12.017 Kazakova, S., Cauberghe, V., Pandelaere, M., & De Pelsmacker, P. (2015). Can’t see the forest for the trees? The effect of media multitasking on cognitive processing style. Media Psychology, 18(4), 425–450. doi:10.1080/15213269.2015.1006789 Kessler, R. C., Adler, L., Ames, M., Demler, O., Faraone, S., Hiripi, E., … Walters, E. E. (2005). The world health organization adult ADHD Self-Report Scale (ASRS): A short screening scale for use in the general population. Psychological Medicine, 35(2), 245–256. doi:10.1017/S0033291704002892 24 W. WIRADHANY AND J. KOERTS https://doi.org/10.1076/chin.8.4.249.13513 https://doi.org/10.1145/985692.985707 https://doi.org/10.1038/srep24542 https://doi.org/10.1089/cyber.2017.0524 https://doi.org/10.1016/j.chb.2017.12.012
  • 126. https://doi.org/10.1037/1082-989X.9.4.426 https://doi.org/10.1002/jrsm.5 https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-0167.29.1.66 https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-0167.29.1.66 https://doi.org/10.1016/S0191-8869(01)00032-0 https://doi.org/10.1080/09297049.2010.509715 https://doi.org/10.1016/j.chb.2014.04.040 https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pmed.0020124 https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2014.02.010 https://doi.org/10.1111/hcre.12089 https://doi.org/10.1111/hcre.12089 https://doi.org/10.1016/j.compedu.2012.12.017 https://doi.org/10.1080/15213269.2015.1006789 https://doi.org/10.1017/S0033291704002892 Kononova, A., Joo, E., & Yuan, S. (2016). If I choose when to switch: Heavy multitaskers remember online content better than light multitaskers when they have the freedom to multitask. Computers in Human Behavior, 65, 567–575. doi:10.1016/j.chb.2016.09.011 Kononova, A., Zasorina, T., Diveeva, N., Kokoeva, A., & Chelokyan, A. (2014). Multitasking goes global: Multitasking with traditional and new electronic media and attention to media messages among college students in Kuwait, Russia, and the USA. International Communication Gazette, 76(8), 617–640. doi:10.1177/1748048514548533 Larson, G. E., Alderton, D. L., Neideffer, M., & Underhill, E. (2011). Further evidence on dimensionality and correlates of the cognitive failures questionnaire. British Journal of
  • 127. Psychology, 88(1), 29–38. doi:10.1111/j.2044- 8295.1997.tb02618.x Lenhart, A. (2015). Teens, social media & technology overview 2015. In Pew Research Center’s Internet & American Life Project. Retrieved from http://www.pewinternet.org/2015/04/09/ teens-social-media-technology-2015/ Lin, L. (2009). Breadth-biased versus focused cognitive control in media multitasking behaviors. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 106(37), 15521–15522. doi:10.1073/pnas.0908642106 Lin, L., Robertson, T., & Lee, J. (2009). Reading performances between novices and experts in different media multitasking environments. Computers in the Schools, 26(3), 169–186. doi:10.1080/07380560903095162 Loh, K. K., & Kanai, R. (2014). Higher media multi-tasking activity is associated with smaller gray-matter density in the anterior cingulate cortex. PloS One, 9(9), e106698. doi:10.1371/ journal.pone.0106698 Loh, K. K., Tan, B. Z. H., & Lim, S. W. H. (2016). Media multitasking predicts video-recorded lecture learning performance through mind wandering tendencies. Computers in Human Behavior, 63, 943–947. doi:10.1016/j.chb.2016.06.030 Lui, K. F. H., & Wong, A. C.-N. (2012). Does media multitasking always hurt? A positive correlation between multitasking and multisensory integration.
  • 128. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 19(4), 647–653. doi:10.3758/s13423-012-0245-7 Magen, H. (2017). The relations between executive functions, media multitasking and polychronicity. Computers in Human Behavior, 67, 1–9. doi:10.1016/j.chb.2016.10.011 Marius, P., & Anggoro, S. (2014). Profil Pengguna Internet Indonesia 2014. Jakarta, Indonesia: Asosiasi Penyelenggara Jasa Internet Indonesia. Mark, G., Gudith, D., & Klocke, U. (2008). The cost of interrupted work: More speed and stress. Proceedings of the SIGCHI Conference on Human Factors in Computing Systems, 107–110. doi:10.1145/1357054.1357072 Minear, M., Brasher, F., McCurdy, M., Lewis, J., & Younggren, A. (2013). Working memory, fluid intelligence, and impulsiveness in heavy media multitaskers. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 20, 1274–1281. doi:10.3758/s13423-013-0456-6 Moisala, M., Salmela, V., Hietajärvi, L., Salo, E., Carlson, S., Salonen, O., … Alho, K. (2016). Media multitasking is associated with distractibility and increased prefrontal activity in adolescents and young adults. NeuroImage, 134, 113–121. doi:10.1016/j. neuroimage.2016.04.011 Murphy, K., McLauchlan, S., & Lee, M. (2017). Is there a link between media-multitasking and the executive functions of filtering and response inhibition? Computers in Human
  • 129. Behavior, 75, 667–677. doi:10.1016/j.chb.2017.06.001 Ophir, E., Nass, C., & Wagner, A. D. (2009). Cognitive control in media multitaskers. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 106(37), 15583–15587. doi:10.1073/pnas.0903620106 MEDIA PSYCHOLOGY 25 https://doi.org/10.1016/j.chb.2016.09.011 https://doi.org/10.1177/1748048514548533 https://doi.org/10.1111/j.2044-8295.1997.tb02618.x http://www.pewinternet.org/2015/04/09/teens-social-media- technology-2015/ http://www.pewinternet.org/2015/04/09/teens-social-media- technology-2015/ https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.0908642106 https://doi.org/10.1080/07380560903095162 https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0106698 https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0106698 https://doi.org/10.1016/j.chb.2016.06.030 https://doi.org/10.3758/s13423-012-0245-7 https://doi.org/10.1016/j.chb.2016.10.011 https://doi.org/10.1145/1357054.1357072 https://doi.org/10.3758/s13423-013-0456-6 https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2016.04.011 https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2016.04.011 https://doi.org/10.1016/j.chb.2017.06.001 https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.0903620106 Patton, J. H., Stanford, M. S., & Barratt, E. S. (1995). Factor structure of the barratt impulsiveness scale. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 51, 768– 774. doi:10.1002/(ISSN)1097-
  • 130. 4679 Pea, R., Nass, C., Meheula, L., Rance, M., Kumar, A., Bamford, H., … Zhou, M. (2012). Media use, face-to-face communication, media multitasking, and social well-being among 8- to 12-year-old girls. Developmental Psychology, 48(2), 327–336. doi:10.1037/a0027030 Petersen, S. E., & Posner, M. I. (2012). The attention system of the human brain: 20 years after. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 35(1), 73–89. doi:10.1146/annurev-neuro-062111- 150525 Posner, M. I., & Petersen, S. E. (1990). The attention system of the human brain. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 13, 25–42. doi:10.1146/annurev.ne.13.030190.000325 Primack, B. A., Shensa, A., Escobar-Viera, C. G., Barrett, E. L., Sidani, J. E., Colditz, J. B., & James, A. E. (2017). Use of multiple social media platforms and symptoms of depression and anxiety: A nationally-representative study among U.S. young adults. Computers in Human Behavior, 69, 1–9. doi:10.1016/j.chb.2016.11.013 R Core team. (2015). R Core Team. R: A Language and Environment for Statistical Computing., 55, 275–286. Retrieved from http://www.r- project.org/ Ralph, B. C. W., & Smilek, D. (2016). Individual differences in media multitasking and performance on the n-back. Attention, Perception, &
  • 131. Psychophysics, 79(2), 582–592. doi:10.3758/s13414-016-1260-y Ralph, B. C. W., Thomson, D. R., Cheyne, J. A., & Smilek, D. (2013). Media multitasking and failures of attention in everyday life. Psychological Research, 78(5), 661–669. doi:10.1007/ s00426-013-0523-7 Ralph, B. C. W., Thomson, D. R., Seli, P., Carriere, J. S. A., & Smilek, D. (2015). Media multitasking and behavioral measures of sustained attention. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 77(2), 390–401. doi:10.3758/s13414-014-0771-7 Rideout, V. J., Foehr, U. G., & Roberts, D. F. (2010). Generation M2: Media in the Lives of 8 to 18 Year-Olds. Retrieved from https://files.eric.ed.gov/fulltext/ED527859.pdf Rigby, J. M., Brumby, D. P., Gould, S. J. J., & Cox, A. L. (2017). Media multitasking at home: A video observation study of concurrent TV and mobile device usage. Proceedings of the 2017 ACM International Conference on Interactive Experiences for TV and Online Video, 3–10. doi:10.1145/3077548.3077560 Roberts, D. F., & Foehr, U. G. (2008). Trends in media use. Future of Children, 18(1), 11–37. doi:10.1353/foc.0.0000 Sanbonmatsu, D. M., Strayer, D. L., Medeiros-Ward, N., & Watson, J. M. (2013). Who multi-tasks and why? Multi-tasking ability, perceived multi- tasking ability, impulsivity,