SlideShare a Scribd company logo
1 of 6
Download to read offline
Behavior Research Methods, Instruments, & Computers
1998,30 (4),637-642
Testing the validity of the paddle method
for the kinesthetic and visual-kinesthetic
perception of inclination
CATINA FERESIN
University ofPadua, Padua, Italy
TIZIANO AGOSTINI
University of Trieste, Trieste, Italy
and
NILANEGRIN-SAVIOLO
University ofPadua, Padua, Italy
Wetested the validity ofthe paddle method for measuring both the kinesthetic and visual-kinesthetic
perception of inclination. In three conditions, subjects performed three different tasks: (1) rotating
a manual paddle to a set of verbally given inclinations (blindfolded subjects), (2) rotating a manual
paddle to the same set of verbally given inclinations after specific kinesthetic training (blindfolded
subjects), and (3) rotating the paddle to a set of fixed visual inclinations after the kinesthetic train-
ing. The results showed a high degree of accuracy and precision in the second and third task but not
in the first one. When subjects were asked to rotate a manual paddle to a set of verbally given incli-
nations, they used three main anchors (00
, 450
, 900
) . Furthermore, the paddle method is biased by a
kinesthetic deficiency, namely a rotational problem of the wrist that can be corrected by means of
specific training.
In a celebrated work on the role of texture in the per-
ception of inclination, Gibson (1950) used, for the first
time, a visual-kinesthetic method, which he called "the
method ofreproduction." During that experiment, the sub-
jects adjusted a rotatable surface (a manual paddle) with
their palm until the kinesthetic impression of inclination
was the same as the visual impression of inclination.'
The "method ofreproduction" can also be called "the pad-
dle method."
After Gibson (1950), many scientists used "purely vi-
sual methods." The first of these consists of a compari-
son line or a surface in the frontal plane used by subjects
to match visually the inclination of another surface in
depth (e.g., Harris, Freeman, & Hughes, 1992; Ryan &
Gillam, 1994; van Ee & Erkelens, 1995). The second con-
sists of a monocular comparison between two surfaces,
indicating which has the greater inclination in depth (see,
e.g., Braunstein & Payne, 1969; Freeman, 1966; Phillips,
1970). The third visual method consists of asking sub-
jects to set the inclination ofa comparison line presented
after a test pattern has been removed (Epstein & Morgan-
Paap, 1974).
Quite recently, the visual-kinesthetic method has again
been used for measuring the visual perceived inclination
ofa surface. This is because some researchers recognized
Correspondence should be addressed to C. Feresin, Dipartimento di
Psicologia Generale, Universita di Padova, via Venezia, 8 35131 Padova,
Italy (e-mail: paola@psicoserver.univ.trieste.it).
that this method does not introduce other visual stimuli
that might have an effect upon the measurement (Carrozzo
& Lacquaniti, 1994; Howard & Kaneko, 1994; Ohmi,
1993; Proffitt, Bhalla, Gossweiler, & Midgett, 1995). For
example, the intersensory method cannot be affected by
visual aftereffects from the test pattern, or by the depth
contrast effect.
Howard and Kaneko (1994) and Proffitt et al. (1995)
addressed the validity ofthe paddle method. Howard and
Kaneko found an underestimation of a large range of in-
clinations ofvisually perceived surfaces having a full range
ofbinocular and monocular depth cues. To calibrate their
unseen paddle before starting a set of experiments, they
asked the subjects to match the inclination of the paddle
to a very big cardboard surface. This surface was covered
with random dots and was inclined at 100
intervals be-
tween +800
and -800
• They used a third-order polyno-
mial function, which best fit the underestimation they
found, as a baseline for comparing the results ofthe cal-
ibration with the results ofthe experimental conditions.
Proffitt et al. (1995) used many psychophysical meth-
ods to measure the geographical inclination ofhills. Their
subjects gave verbal estimates of the inclination of a set
of hills, adjusted a representation of the cross section of
those hills, and, finally, used the visual-kinesthetic
method, setting a tilt board with their hands unseen. Prof-
fitt et al. found a small underestimation of the visually
perceived inclination of a set of hills when using the tilt
board and, overall, this measure was different from the
637 Copyright 1998 Psychonomic Society, Inc.
638 FERESIN, AGOSTINI, AND NEGRIN-SAVIOLO
Figure 1. (A) Kinesthetic deficiency ofthe wrist at the vertical p0-
sition when the forearm is orthogonal with respect to the arm. (B) To
solve the kinesthetic deficiency at the vertical position, the observer
has to lift up his/her forearm and incline the arm.
actual inclinations of the hills. In their paper, Proffitt
et al. measured the internal consistency among the three
already mentioned psychophysical methods they used.
They compared the kinesthetic responses the subjects
made by looking at some inclined hills with the kines-
thetic responses that the same subjects made in response
to an almost equivalent set of verbally given angles. The
subjects displayed an internal consistency for both the
verbal-kinesthetic and the visual-kinesthetic measures.
We decided to approach the problem ofthe validity of
the paddle method in a different way than did Howard and
Kaneko (1994) and Proffitt et al. (1995). Ifa person is ob-
served rotating his/her wrist, it is quite clear that there is
one main motor deficiency: The wrist cannot rotate com-
pletely to achieve the vertical position when the forearm
is orthogonal with respect to the arm (Figure IA). To
avoid this rotational problem close to the vertical, the
person has to lift his/her forearm up with respect to the
arm and incline the arm and the shoulder by a small
amount (Figure IB). However, close to the horizontal, the
subject's wrist may have an impediment in its rotation
due to the inclined position of the forearm with respect
to the arm (Figure 2B). Because ofthis, the subject can-
not really achieve a correct position of the wrist and the
forearm at the horizontal position (Figure 1A). As a con-
sequence ofthis possible motor problem around the ver-
tical, it is best for the person to choose a position for the
forearm and the wrist that allows him/her to freely rotate
the paddle from the vertical to the horizontal. This posi-
tion results in a compromise between the best position for
ROTATABLE
PADDLE
ROTATABLE
PADDLE
HAND
HAND
WRIST
rthogonal
FOREARM
B
rotating the wrist to the vertical and the best position for
rotating the wrist to the horizontal.
Because of this compromise, we suppose that when a
person is asked to kinesthetically set a paddle to the ver-
tical position (0°), he/she will rotate the wrist more than
0°, stopping the setting forward and then overestimating
the perceived inclination (Figure 3A). On the contrary,
when a person is asked to kinesthetically set a paddle to the
horizontal(90°),wesuppose thathe/she will rotatethe wrist
less than 90°, stopping the setting backward and then un-
derestimating the perceived inclination (Figure 3B).
Furthermore, we think that it is a reasonable strategy
for subjects to find and use spontaneously some kind of
kinesthetic anchor. This is because we know from the lit-
erature that the vertical and the horizontal are the main
visual and haptic orientational anchors or norms (e.g.,
Cecala & Garner, 1986;Gentaz & Hatwell, 1995;Howard,
1982). When people are asked to judge the visual and the
haptic inclination ofa surface or a rod, they use the hor-
izontal and the vertical plane as a frame ofreference. So,
why should it be different in the kinesthetic domain?
The first aim of our research was to investigate the
purely kinesthetic perception of a range of inclined sur-
faces to test whether the paddle method is biased in some
way by motor problems or strategies used by the sub-
jects. The second purpose was to measure the perception
of the visual inclination of a line by means of the paddle
kinesthetic method. If the paddle method is influenced
by motor problems, as we suppose, it should be possible
ARM
Figure 2. (A) Correct position ofthe wrist and the forearm at the
horizontal position. (B) Uncomfortable position of the wrist to
achieve the horizontal position due to the inclination ofthe forearm
and arm.
A
ROTATABLE
PADDLE
HAND
HAN
WRIST
WRIST
FOREARM
ROTATABLE
~======-i PADDLE
ARM
A
B
KINESTHETIC PERCEPTION OF SURFACE INCLINATION 639
A
horizontal, and finally they found the middle position
corresponding to 45°. When the subjects were asked to
set the paddle to 40° or 50°, they first looked for 0° and
for 90°, then they reached 45°, and finally went from it
to reach 40° or 50°.
At the end ofthe preliminary study, we asked the sub-
jects ifthey had used any anchor as a reference point. All
ofthem told us that they were using the three anchors we
had observed while watching them doing the kinesthetic
task.
EXPERIMENT
B
HAND 2
-'-FOREARM
Figure 3. (AI) Correctinclination ofthe paddle at the vertical: un-
comfortable inclination ofthe wrist. (A2) Overestimated inclination
of the paddle at the vertical: comfortable inclination of the wrist.
(81) Correct inclination ofthe paddle at the horizontal: uncomfort-
able inclination of the wrist. (82) Underestimated inclination of the
paddle at the horizontal: comfortable inclinationofthe wrist.
to compensate for such motor biases with an appropriate
kinesthetic training. When the motor biases are avoided,
we can use the paddle method to test the perception of
the visual inclination ofa surface or a line. In this way it
is possible to separate the kinesthetic outcome from the
visual outcome.
PRELIMINARY STUDY
During a preliminary study, we observed that, while
doing the kinesthetic task of rotating the manual paddle
to a set of II inclinations going from the vertical (0°) to
the horizontal (90°), subjects were limited in the extent
of rotation of the wrist. The subject's wrist could rotate
completely to achieve the vertical or horizontal position
only by forcing it. But since the subjects were not asked
to do that, they stopped their adjustment of the vertical
and the horizontal position whenever they started to feel
the impediment of rotating the wrist.
We also noticed that the subjects used three main
kinesthetic anchors to refer their paddle adjustment: 0°
(gravitational vertical), 45°, and 90° (horizontal). The
subjects' use ofthe vertical and the horizontal anchors is
logical for the reason explained in the introduction; but
the subjects also used 45° as an anchor. This anchor was
derived in that it was achieved by the subjects as the mid-
dle position between the vertical and the horizontal. For
example, when the subjects had to set the paddle to 45°,
they first started from the vertical, then they went to the
In two conditions, we measured the accuracy and pre-
cision (Ono, 1993)ofadjusting the inclination ofa manual
paddle to a set of verbally given angles. In a third condi-
tion, we measured the accuracy and precision ofadjusting
the inclination of the same manual paddle to a set of vi-
sually given inclinations.
The measure ofaccuracy was the mean ofthe point of
subjective equality (PSE) compared with the point ofob-
jective equality (POE). The measure ofprecision was the
standard error.
The task performed by the subjects in the first and sec-
ond conditions was a purely kinesthetic task in that the
inclination ofthe manual paddle was not compared with
a visual surface and the observer was blindfolded. We
also investigated the subject's possible strategies in per-
forming the task. The task performed by the subjects in
the third condition was a visual-kinesthetic task in that
the inclination ofthe manual paddle was compared with
a set of inclinations ofa luminous line.
Method
Subjects
Eighteen subjects took part in the experiment-6 for each con-
dition. They were undergraduate and graduate psychology students
of the University of Trieste. All were volunteers and naive regard-
ing the aim of this work.
Apparatus
A cubic chamber, 150 em long on each side, was placed in the
middle ofthe laboratory. A rotatable paddle (24 em long X 16 em
wide X 3 em thick) was connected to the frame of the chamber by
means of a small table. The paddle was connected to an electronic
protractor (Emaco Angle Star protractor system, Montreal, Que-
bec, Canada). During the third condition, the chamber was illumi-
nated by two projectors placed outside the right and the left sides
of the chamber itself. A thick luminous line (35 cm long X 1.5 cm
wide) was mounted on a wooden rotatable bar and fixed to the
cubic chamber in front of the subject at a viewing distance of
150 cm. The center of the line was at eye level (Figure 4).
Procedure
Condition 1. The blindfolded subject was placed at the center of
the chamber and seated on an adjustable chair. After the subject
had chosen the most comfortable position, which remained con-
stant during all the trials, the experimenter taped the hand to the
paddle. The tape did not interfere with the free movement of the
wrist. The taping was done to prevent movements of the fingers
that could have influenced the inclination response of the wrist.
The paddle was not a weight on the hand, and ofcourse there was
640 FERESIN, AGOSTINI, AND NEGRIN-SAVIOLO
Figure 5. Mean results of Condition 1: kinesthetic task.
Results
----- Subjects' means
y =5,1008 + 0,87917x R"2 =0,99(jj"
GI
90GI...Cl
GI 80'tl
C 70~
'E 60
GI
E 50
-III
;:,
40:g
l'lI
.!!! 30
'tl
20'tl
l'lI
a.
10
w
II)
0a.
0 10 20 30 40 50 60 70 80 90
POE: Verbal angle (in degrees)
Condition 1
As Figure 5 shows, the 6 subjects overestimated (for-
ward direction) the kinesthetic inclinations relative to the
verbal angle from 0° to 20°; settings were very accurate
but not very precise at 30°, quite accurate and precise
(small standard errors) at 40°, and very accurate and pre-
cise at 50°. Finally, the subjects underestimated (back-
ward direction) the kinesthetic inclinations relative to the
verbal angle from 60° to 90°.
When the verbal angle was close to the gravitational
vertical (from 10° to 30°), subjects always started their
settings from the vertical. This means that ifthey tended
to slightly overestimate the vertical itself, stopping their
used by the subject as a frame of reference to incline the luminous
line. This line was thick enough for linear perspective. We mea-
sured this visually perceived inclination by means of the paddle.
In this final condition, we trained the subjects to pay attention
to the motor deficiency of the wrist and to use the three anchors,
as we had done in Condition 2. This task was a visual-kinesthetic
task in that the inclination of the manual paddle was compared
with inclinations of the luminous line.
The independent variable was the set of visual inclinations of
the line-that is, the POE-whereas the dependent variable was
the manual adjustment of the inclination of the paddle-that is, the
PSE. The levels of the independent variable varied in steps of 10°
(45° included) between gravitational vertical (0°) and 80°. We did
not use the 90° inclination (horizontal) because it was perceived
by the subject as a point in space and not as an inclination. The
kinesthetic training was the same as in Condition 2.
The subject was placed at the center of the cubic chamber, as in
Conditions 1 and 2, resting his/her head on a chinrest, and was
asked to rotate the manual paddle to each of a set ofinclinations of
the luminous line. During this condition, the subject stopped each
trial when the kinesthetic inclination of the paddle was judged to
be the same as the visual inclination of the luminous line.
The line was inclined by the experimenter to 1 of 10 angles pre-
sented in random order. Each angle was repeated in random order
five times, yielding 50 total trials for every subject.
ba
~
rotatable
adjustable chair
Figure 4. Cubic chamber.
no change of one of the limb's eigenvectors (Pagano & Turvey,
1995).
The subject was asked to set the manual paddle to a set ofver-
bally given inclinations relative to gravity. On each trial, the sub-
ject stopped when the kinesthetic inclination of the wrist felt the
same as the verbal inclination.
Before performing Condition 1, the subjects were tested for
their understanding of the entire set of angles. Indeed, they were
asked to reproduce the entire set of angles by rotating the wrist.
Eleven verbal angles went from the gravitational vertical (0°) to
the horizontal (90°) in steps of 10°(45° included). The 11 settings
were performed in random order, and every setting was repeated in
random order five times, yielding a total of 55 trials for each sub-
ject. Subjects started all their settings from a random inclination of
the paddle.
Condition 2. The aim ofCondition 2 was to investigate the sub-
jects' performance after we had trained them to use the three an-
chors strategy and to overcome the rotational deficiency of the
wrist that we had noticed during the pilot study. This task, like the
one in the first condition, was a purely kinesthetic task in that the
inclination of the manual paddle was not compared with any visual
surface inclination and the subject was blindfolded.
The procedure followed by the subjects in the second condition
was the same as the procedure of Condition 1 except for training.
Indeed, we trained the subjects to use the three main anchors (0°,
45°, and 90°) that had been used spontaneously by the group of dur-
ing in the pilot study. First, we taught them to rely on the vertical
anchor (0°)when they had to set the paddle to inclinations from 10°
to 30° verbally indicated, to rely on the 45° anchor for inclinations
from 40° to 50°, and finally to rely on the horizontal anchor (90°)
for inclinations from 60° to 80°.
The training method we used was the basic one of demonstra-
tion and feedback for the whole set of inclinations. Furthermore,
we told the subjects about the rotational deficiency, and we trained
them not to stop their adjustment when they started to feel the im-
pediment in rotating the wrist. We expected an increase of accu-
racy and precision as a result of this training compared with the ac-
curacy and precision obtained in Condition I.
Condition3. The purpose ofCondition 3 was to test subjects vi-
sually perceived inclination of a line stimulus characterized by
most of the monocular and binocular depth cues. All the external
frames were visible; indeed, the edges of the cubic chamber were
KINESTHETIC PERCEPTION OF SURFACE INCLINATION 641
Iil
90Gl
Gl
..Cl 80Gl
'tl
.= 70
--60
e
Gl
50E
-UI
40~
:cIII
30
.!!
'tl 20
'tl
ca
Q. 10
Iii
enQ.
---- Subjects' means
y = 0,95741 + 0,99270x R"2 = 0,99
DISCUSSION
A one-group t test showed no significant difference
between the paddle settings and the visual inclinations
for either the 0°-40° distribution [t(4) = -0.71 ,p < .51]
or the 50°-80° distribution [t(4) = 0.05, p < .96].
Figure 7. Mean results of Condition 3: training + visual-
kinesthetic task.
10 20 30 40 50 60 70 80 90
POE: Inclination of the luminous line (in degrees)
In the first condition, in which the subjects were not
trained to use the paddle, we found an underestimation
of the perceived kinesthetic inclination when the tested
angles were close to the horizontal, an overestimation
when they were close to the gravitational vertical, and a
quite good level ofaccuracy and precision around 45°. In
the second condition, in which subjects were trained to
use three anchors (spontaneously used in the pilot study
and in the first condition) and received feedback to com-
pensate for the wrist rotational problem, we observed a
good level of accuracy and precision of the responses
without any under- or overestimation. In the third condi-
tion, the subjects had the same kinesthetic training as in
Condition 2 before performing a visual-kinesthetic task.
The results of this final condition showed a high degree
ofaccuracy and precision of the responses.
The main point emerging from this research is that the
use of the paddle method presents a wrist rotational bias
due to the position of the subject's forearm while he/she
is performing a kinesthetic task. However, the underes-
timation and overestimation found in the first condition
nearly disappears when the rotational bias is compen-
sated for by giving the subject kinesthetic feedback con-
cerning the veridical inclination ofthe paddle.
We emphasize that, when the subjects were asked to
perform a kinesthetic task by rotating a manual paddle,
they spontaneously used three anchors-that is, the per-
ceived 0°, 45°, and 90°. Wesuppose that training the sub-
- - i : ] - - Subjects' means
y = 1,6938 + 0,98101x R"2 = 0,99iii
:l 90...----.:--------------.
..Cl
~ 80
c: 70
~
w
enQ.
O~.........J.~-L...........I.-~L.....---L.--'--I.-~..l..-........J--.L.-J
o 10 20 30 40 50 60 70 80 90
POE: Verbal angle (in degrees)
Figure 6. Mean results of Condition 2: training + kinesthetic task.
Condition 3
Figure 7 shows the mean responses for each visual in-
clination. The 6 subjects were very accurate and precise
when they kinesthetically adjusted the paddle to the 10
different visual inclinations.
'E 60
Gl
E 50
Ui
:ij- 40
III
30
!!
:g 20
III
C. 10
Condition 2
Figure 6 shows mean responses for each verbal angle.
The 6 subjects were more precise than in Condition 1, as
the small standard errors show. Furthermore, there was
no over- or underestimation of the perceived kinesthetic
inclination. Indeed, the subjects showed high accuracy
from 0° to 90°.
We analyzed the data in the same way as in Condi-
tion I. A one-group t test showed no significant differ-
ence between the paddle settings and the verbal values
for either the 0°-40° distribution [t(4) = -1.50, p < .20]
or the 50°-90° distribution [t(4) = -0.20, p < .85].
setting forward, they carried this initial overestimation
with them and also showed this bias for angles close to
the vertical.
It was the same for the horizontal. Indeed, the subjects
tended to underestimate the horizontal, stopping their set-
ting backward, and carried this underestimationwith them,
also showing it for angles close to 90° (from 60° to 80°).
We divided the distribution into two halves-from 0°
to 40° and from 50° to 90°, excluding the 45° since it is
the inversion point between positive and negative PSE.
Then we subtracted the paddle settings from the verbally
defined values; finally, we analyzed the two distributions
by using a one-group t test. The statistical analysis showed
a significant difference between the paddle settings and
the verbal values for the 0°-40° distribution [t(4) = -2.8,
p < .05] and a tendency to significance in the opposite di-
rection for the 50°-90° distribution [t(4) = 2.4, p < .07].
642 FERESIN, AGOSTINI, AND NEGRIN-SAVIOLO
jects to consciously use these anchors reduced data vari-
ability by preventing the use of uncontrolled strategies.
Furthermore, we suppose that the kinesthetic outcome
could be affected by the use of strategies different from
those we found in our first condition, and we hope to in-
vestigate them in a future experiment. Most important,
we think that kinesthetic variables such as the effect of
wrist and forearm position should be taken into account
when the paddle method is used.
Our results show that in a visual-kinesthetic task, an
inclined line is matched correctly when subjects are pre-
viously trained in the kinesthetic domain. This suggests
that use of the intersensory method without training
could measure a motor problem and the presence ofhet-
erogeneous strategies rather than a visual misperception.
In conclusion, we think the intersensory paddle method
is a valid one but only when precautions are taken by
training the subjects to overcome the rotational problem
of the wrist and by verifying their strategies.
REFERENCES
BRAUNSTEIN, M. L., & PAYNE, I. W. (1969). Perspective and form ratio
as determinants of relative slant judgments. Journal ofExperimen-
tal Psychology. 81,584-590.
CARROZZO, M., & LACQUANITI, F. (1994). A hybrid frame of reference
for visuo-manual coordination. NeuroReport, 5, 453-456.
CECALA, A.I., & GARNER, W. R. (1986). Internal frame ofreference as
a determinant of the oblique effect. Journal ofExperimental Psy-
chology: Human Perception & Performance, 12, 314-323.
EpSTEIN, W., & MORGAN-PAAP, C. L. (1974). Aftereffect ofinspection
of a perspectival stimulus for slant depth: A new normalization ef-
fect. Perception & Psychophysics, 16, 299-302.
FREEMAN, R. B. (1966). Effect of size on visual slant. Journal ofEx-
perimental Psychology, 71, 96-103.
GENTAZ, E., & HATWELL, Y. (1995). The haptic "oblique effect" in chil-
dren's and adults' perception oforientation. Perception, 24, 631-646.
GIBSON, I. I. (1950). The perception ofvisual surfaces. American Jour-
nal ofPsychology, 63, 367-384.
HARRIS, M., FREEMAN, T., & HUGHES, I. (1992). Retinal speed gradient
and the perception of surface slant. Vision Research, 32, 587-590.
HOWARD, I. P. (1982). Human visual orientation. New York: Wiley.
HOWARD, I. P., & KANEKO, H. (1994). Relative shear disparities and the
perception of surface inclination. Vision Research, 34, 2505-2517.
KLATZKY, R. L., & LEDERMAN, S. J. (1995). Identifying objects from a
haptic glance. Perception & Psychophysics, 57,1111-1123.
OHMI,M. (1993). Depth perception of wide-angle surface [Abstracts
of the Sixteenth European Conference on Visual Perception]. Per-
ception, 22 (Supp!.), 114.
ONO, H. (1993). Precision and accuracy in perception [Educational
Computer Program Package]. Santa Barbara, CA: Intellimation.
PAGANO, C. P., & TuRVEY, M. T. (1995). The inertia tensor as a basis
for the perception oflimb orientation. Journal ofExperimental Psy-
chology: Human Perception & Performance, 21,1070-1087.
PHILLIPS, R.I. (1970). Stationary visual texture and the estimation of
slant angle. QuarterlyJournal ofExperimental Psychology, 22, 389-
397.
PROFFITT, D. R., BHALLA, M., GOSSWEILER, R., & MIDGETT, J. (1995).
Perceiving geographical slant. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 2,
409-428.
RYAN, C., & GILLAM, B. (1994). Cue conflict and stereoscopic surface
slant about horizontal and vertical axes. Perception, 23, 645-658.
VAN EE, R., & ERKELENS, C. J. (1995). Binocular perception of slant
about oblique axes relative to a visual frame of reference. Percep-
tion, 24, 299-314.
NOfE
I. Similar to Klatzky and Lederman (1995), we define kinesthesis as
perceptual information coming from muscles, tendons, and joints,
whereas cutaneous perceptual information derives from impressions
coming from somatosensory inputs of the skin. Kinesthetic and cuta-
neous perceptual information are both parts of the haptic system.
(Manuscript received January 13, 1997;
revision accepted for publication May 14, 1997.)

More Related Content

What's hot

Frenkel's Exercise (Bangladesh Health Profession Institute) CRP
Frenkel's  Exercise (Bangladesh Health Profession Institute) CRPFrenkel's  Exercise (Bangladesh Health Profession Institute) CRP
Frenkel's Exercise (Bangladesh Health Profession Institute) CRPBipul Debnath
 
6 general measures of walking (nov 2014)
6 general measures of walking (nov 2014)6 general measures of walking (nov 2014)
6 general measures of walking (nov 2014)Richard Baker
 
Clinical evaluation of spine
Clinical evaluation of spineClinical evaluation of spine
Clinical evaluation of spineorthoprince
 
Kinematic gait analysis 1
Kinematic gait analysis 1Kinematic gait analysis 1
Kinematic gait analysis 1rraja durai
 
Gait_ Introduction, Analysis & Re-education Principles
Gait_ Introduction, Analysis & Re-education PrinciplesGait_ Introduction, Analysis & Re-education Principles
Gait_ Introduction, Analysis & Re-education PrinciplesVivek Ramanandi
 
P pt final sagittal plane analysis of gait
P pt  final   sagittal plane analysis of gaitP pt  final   sagittal plane analysis of gait
P pt final sagittal plane analysis of gaitDrTabassumAzmi
 
Sung488 JRRD Slideshow Project
Sung488 JRRD Slideshow ProjectSung488 JRRD Slideshow Project
Sung488 JRRD Slideshow Projectstacieyuhasz
 
Nice Legs, Great Squat
Nice Legs, Great SquatNice Legs, Great Squat
Nice Legs, Great SquatMark McKean
 
Crimson Publishers-Assessment of the Muscle Strength and Range of Motion Ankl...
Crimson Publishers-Assessment of the Muscle Strength and Range of Motion Ankl...Crimson Publishers-Assessment of the Muscle Strength and Range of Motion Ankl...
Crimson Publishers-Assessment of the Muscle Strength and Range of Motion Ankl...Crimsonpublishers-Sportsmedicine
 
Elbow joint Mobilization Techniques (Darlene Hertling)
Elbow joint Mobilization Techniques (Darlene Hertling)Elbow joint Mobilization Techniques (Darlene Hertling)
Elbow joint Mobilization Techniques (Darlene Hertling)Qandeel Hamidullah Khan
 
After shoulder-replacement - POST-SURGICAL SHOULDER REPLACEMENT REHABILITATIO...
After shoulder-replacement - POST-SURGICAL SHOULDER REPLACEMENT REHABILITATIO...After shoulder-replacement - POST-SURGICAL SHOULDER REPLACEMENT REHABILITATIO...
After shoulder-replacement - POST-SURGICAL SHOULDER REPLACEMENT REHABILITATIO...priyaakumarr
 
Gait analysis definitions
Gait analysis definitionsGait analysis definitions
Gait analysis definitionseappel
 
Cga ifa 2015 10 impairment focussed interpretation
Cga ifa 2015 10 impairment focussed interpretationCga ifa 2015 10 impairment focussed interpretation
Cga ifa 2015 10 impairment focussed interpretationRichard Baker
 
Biomechanics concepts
Biomechanics conceptsBiomechanics concepts
Biomechanics conceptsRatan Khuman
 
9.21 ascending stairs analysis presentation-jess
9.21 ascending stairs analysis presentation-jess9.21 ascending stairs analysis presentation-jess
9.21 ascending stairs analysis presentation-jessJill Stephenson
 
Pre-habilitation exercises 'The HAPI Study'
Pre-habilitation exercises 'The HAPI Study'Pre-habilitation exercises 'The HAPI Study'
Pre-habilitation exercises 'The HAPI Study'Louise Davies Grant
 
Shoulder Injury Prevention in MMA
Shoulder Injury Prevention in MMAShoulder Injury Prevention in MMA
Shoulder Injury Prevention in MMAspartanova
 
Hamstring Avulsion Repair Rehabilitation
Hamstring Avulsion Repair RehabilitationHamstring Avulsion Repair Rehabilitation
Hamstring Avulsion Repair RehabilitationRoss Nakaji
 

What's hot (20)

Frenkel's Exercise (Bangladesh Health Profession Institute) CRP
Frenkel's  Exercise (Bangladesh Health Profession Institute) CRPFrenkel's  Exercise (Bangladesh Health Profession Institute) CRP
Frenkel's Exercise (Bangladesh Health Profession Institute) CRP
 
6 general measures of walking (nov 2014)
6 general measures of walking (nov 2014)6 general measures of walking (nov 2014)
6 general measures of walking (nov 2014)
 
Clinical evaluation of spine
Clinical evaluation of spineClinical evaluation of spine
Clinical evaluation of spine
 
Kinematic gait analysis 1
Kinematic gait analysis 1Kinematic gait analysis 1
Kinematic gait analysis 1
 
Gait_ Introduction, Analysis & Re-education Principles
Gait_ Introduction, Analysis & Re-education PrinciplesGait_ Introduction, Analysis & Re-education Principles
Gait_ Introduction, Analysis & Re-education Principles
 
P pt final sagittal plane analysis of gait
P pt  final   sagittal plane analysis of gaitP pt  final   sagittal plane analysis of gait
P pt final sagittal plane analysis of gait
 
Sung488 JRRD Slideshow Project
Sung488 JRRD Slideshow ProjectSung488 JRRD Slideshow Project
Sung488 JRRD Slideshow Project
 
hip arthroscopy rehabilitation part one
hip arthroscopy rehabilitation part onehip arthroscopy rehabilitation part one
hip arthroscopy rehabilitation part one
 
Nice Legs, Great Squat
Nice Legs, Great SquatNice Legs, Great Squat
Nice Legs, Great Squat
 
Crimson Publishers-Assessment of the Muscle Strength and Range of Motion Ankl...
Crimson Publishers-Assessment of the Muscle Strength and Range of Motion Ankl...Crimson Publishers-Assessment of the Muscle Strength and Range of Motion Ankl...
Crimson Publishers-Assessment of the Muscle Strength and Range of Motion Ankl...
 
Gait pattern classification in children with cp
Gait pattern classification in children with cpGait pattern classification in children with cp
Gait pattern classification in children with cp
 
Elbow joint Mobilization Techniques (Darlene Hertling)
Elbow joint Mobilization Techniques (Darlene Hertling)Elbow joint Mobilization Techniques (Darlene Hertling)
Elbow joint Mobilization Techniques (Darlene Hertling)
 
After shoulder-replacement - POST-SURGICAL SHOULDER REPLACEMENT REHABILITATIO...
After shoulder-replacement - POST-SURGICAL SHOULDER REPLACEMENT REHABILITATIO...After shoulder-replacement - POST-SURGICAL SHOULDER REPLACEMENT REHABILITATIO...
After shoulder-replacement - POST-SURGICAL SHOULDER REPLACEMENT REHABILITATIO...
 
Gait analysis definitions
Gait analysis definitionsGait analysis definitions
Gait analysis definitions
 
Cga ifa 2015 10 impairment focussed interpretation
Cga ifa 2015 10 impairment focussed interpretationCga ifa 2015 10 impairment focussed interpretation
Cga ifa 2015 10 impairment focussed interpretation
 
Biomechanics concepts
Biomechanics conceptsBiomechanics concepts
Biomechanics concepts
 
9.21 ascending stairs analysis presentation-jess
9.21 ascending stairs analysis presentation-jess9.21 ascending stairs analysis presentation-jess
9.21 ascending stairs analysis presentation-jess
 
Pre-habilitation exercises 'The HAPI Study'
Pre-habilitation exercises 'The HAPI Study'Pre-habilitation exercises 'The HAPI Study'
Pre-habilitation exercises 'The HAPI Study'
 
Shoulder Injury Prevention in MMA
Shoulder Injury Prevention in MMAShoulder Injury Prevention in MMA
Shoulder Injury Prevention in MMA
 
Hamstring Avulsion Repair Rehabilitation
Hamstring Avulsion Repair RehabilitationHamstring Avulsion Repair Rehabilitation
Hamstring Avulsion Repair Rehabilitation
 

Viewers also liked

04 infeksi saluran napas
04 infeksi saluran napas04 infeksi saluran napas
04 infeksi saluran napasdickybagus
 
Benefits of Simulation - Presented by ASTEC
Benefits of Simulation - Presented by ASTECBenefits of Simulation - Presented by ASTEC
Benefits of Simulation - Presented by ASTECsabriiinaaa
 
MOND CV
MOND  CVMOND  CV
MOND CVR Com
 
Mocinic Snjezana and Feresin Catina (2012)
Mocinic Snjezana and Feresin Catina (2012)Mocinic Snjezana and Feresin Catina (2012)
Mocinic Snjezana and Feresin Catina (2012)Catina Feresin
 
Isharun ki zaban main Namaz
Isharun ki zaban main NamazIsharun ki zaban main Namaz
Isharun ki zaban main Namazalharmaintrustpk
 

Viewers also liked (13)

04 infeksi saluran napas
04 infeksi saluran napas04 infeksi saluran napas
04 infeksi saluran napas
 
Benefits of Simulation - Presented by ASTEC
Benefits of Simulation - Presented by ASTECBenefits of Simulation - Presented by ASTEC
Benefits of Simulation - Presented by ASTEC
 
MOND CV
MOND  CVMOND  CV
MOND CV
 
FenimorePortfolio_MURP
FenimorePortfolio_MURPFenimorePortfolio_MURP
FenimorePortfolio_MURP
 
DARTS
DARTSDARTS
DARTS
 
EDM_SETUP_REQUEST
EDM_SETUP_REQUESTEDM_SETUP_REQUEST
EDM_SETUP_REQUEST
 
Rajshri raj
Rajshri rajRajshri raj
Rajshri raj
 
Mocinic Snjezana and Feresin Catina (2012)
Mocinic Snjezana and Feresin Catina (2012)Mocinic Snjezana and Feresin Catina (2012)
Mocinic Snjezana and Feresin Catina (2012)
 
Aulas virtuales
Aulas virtualesAulas virtuales
Aulas virtuales
 
201 Neiman Marcus
201 Neiman Marcus201 Neiman Marcus
201 Neiman Marcus
 
Globus Asia 2016
Globus Asia 2016Globus Asia 2016
Globus Asia 2016
 
Isharun ki zaban main Namaz
Isharun ki zaban main NamazIsharun ki zaban main Namaz
Isharun ki zaban main Namaz
 
GLP HNW Presentation SB
GLP HNW Presentation SBGLP HNW Presentation SB
GLP HNW Presentation SB
 

Similar to Feresin Catina, Agostini Tiziano, Nila Saviolo Negrin, 1998

Feresin Catina and Agostini Tiziano (2007)
Feresin Catina and Agostini Tiziano (2007)Feresin Catina and Agostini Tiziano (2007)
Feresin Catina and Agostini Tiziano (2007)Catina Feresin
 
Confiabilidad evaluaciones hombro
Confiabilidad evaluaciones hombroConfiabilidad evaluaciones hombro
Confiabilidad evaluaciones hombroIsrael Kine Cortes
 
Face bow / dental lab technology courses
Face bow / dental lab technology coursesFace bow / dental lab technology courses
Face bow / dental lab technology coursesIndian dental academy
 
Face bow /certified fixed orthodontic courses by Indian dental academy
Face bow /certified fixed orthodontic courses by Indian dental academy Face bow /certified fixed orthodontic courses by Indian dental academy
Face bow /certified fixed orthodontic courses by Indian dental academy Indian dental academy
 
Final report wearable haptics
Final report   wearable hapticsFinal report   wearable haptics
Final report wearable hapticsApurva Gupta
 
Eva Bonda et al Amygdala & Biological Motion, Journal of Neuroscience, 1996
Eva Bonda et al Amygdala & Biological Motion, Journal of Neuroscience, 1996 Eva Bonda et al Amygdala & Biological Motion, Journal of Neuroscience, 1996
Eva Bonda et al Amygdala & Biological Motion, Journal of Neuroscience, 1996 Dr Eva Bonda, PhD
 
gait joint angles
gait joint anglesgait joint angles
gait joint anglestisurangaj
 
Seated Human Spine Response Prediction to Vertical Vibration via Artificial...
Seated Human Spine Response Prediction to Vertical Vibration via   Artificial...Seated Human Spine Response Prediction to Vertical Vibration via   Artificial...
Seated Human Spine Response Prediction to Vertical Vibration via Artificial...abdulaziznaser2012
 
Articulators-A Review Article
Articulators-A Review ArticleArticulators-A Review Article
Articulators-A Review ArticleMartha Brown
 

Similar to Feresin Catina, Agostini Tiziano, Nila Saviolo Negrin, 1998 (20)

HINGE AXIS.pptx
HINGE AXIS.pptxHINGE AXIS.pptx
HINGE AXIS.pptx
 
Feresin Catina and Agostini Tiziano (2007)
Feresin Catina and Agostini Tiziano (2007)Feresin Catina and Agostini Tiziano (2007)
Feresin Catina and Agostini Tiziano (2007)
 
Confiabilidad evaluaciones hombro
Confiabilidad evaluaciones hombroConfiabilidad evaluaciones hombro
Confiabilidad evaluaciones hombro
 
Face bo wfn/ dental courses
Face bo wfn/ dental coursesFace bo wfn/ dental courses
Face bo wfn/ dental courses
 
Face bow / dental lab technology courses
Face bow / dental lab technology coursesFace bow / dental lab technology courses
Face bow / dental lab technology courses
 
Face bow (2) / dental courses
Face bow (2) / dental coursesFace bow (2) / dental courses
Face bow (2) / dental courses
 
Face bow /certified fixed orthodontic courses by Indian dental academy
Face bow /certified fixed orthodontic courses by Indian dental academy Face bow /certified fixed orthodontic courses by Indian dental academy
Face bow /certified fixed orthodontic courses by Indian dental academy
 
Final report wearable haptics
Final report   wearable hapticsFinal report   wearable haptics
Final report wearable haptics
 
Eva Bonda et al Amygdala & Biological Motion, Journal of Neuroscience, 1996
Eva Bonda et al Amygdala & Biological Motion, Journal of Neuroscience, 1996 Eva Bonda et al Amygdala & Biological Motion, Journal of Neuroscience, 1996
Eva Bonda et al Amygdala & Biological Motion, Journal of Neuroscience, 1996
 
Articulators
ArticulatorsArticulators
Articulators
 
WISENC-15
WISENC-15WISENC-15
WISENC-15
 
gait joint angles
gait joint anglesgait joint angles
gait joint angles
 
Articulators new
Articulators newArticulators new
Articulators new
 
Hinge
HingeHinge
Hinge
 
ARTÍCULO sem 1 (2).pdf
ARTÍCULO sem 1 (2).pdfARTÍCULO sem 1 (2).pdf
ARTÍCULO sem 1 (2).pdf
 
Face bow
Face bowFace bow
Face bow
 
Ieeej 2010
Ieeej 2010Ieeej 2010
Ieeej 2010
 
Seated Human Spine Response Prediction to Vertical Vibration via Artificial...
Seated Human Spine Response Prediction to Vertical Vibration via   Artificial...Seated Human Spine Response Prediction to Vertical Vibration via   Artificial...
Seated Human Spine Response Prediction to Vertical Vibration via Artificial...
 
Face bow
Face bowFace bow
Face bow
 
Articulators-A Review Article
Articulators-A Review ArticleArticulators-A Review Article
Articulators-A Review Article
 

More from Catina Feresin

Do we need to train teachers and students to care about other living beings?
Do we need to train teachers and students to care about other living beings?Do we need to train teachers and students to care about other living beings?
Do we need to train teachers and students to care about other living beings?Catina Feresin
 
Il ruolo del processo di normalizzazione verso il piano frontoparallelo nella...
Il ruolo del processo di normalizzazione verso il piano frontoparallelo nella...Il ruolo del processo di normalizzazione verso il piano frontoparallelo nella...
Il ruolo del processo di normalizzazione verso il piano frontoparallelo nella...Catina Feresin
 
L'uso di stimoli ecologicamente validi nella percezione visiva di superfici i...
L'uso di stimoli ecologicamente validi nella percezione visiva di superfici i...L'uso di stimoli ecologicamente validi nella percezione visiva di superfici i...
L'uso di stimoli ecologicamente validi nella percezione visiva di superfici i...Catina Feresin
 
La normalizzazione dell'inclinazione dell'immagine consecutiva e il ruolo del...
La normalizzazione dell'inclinazione dell'immagine consecutiva e il ruolo del...La normalizzazione dell'inclinazione dell'immagine consecutiva e il ruolo del...
La normalizzazione dell'inclinazione dell'immagine consecutiva e il ruolo del...Catina Feresin
 
"Shear" verticali, orizzontali e percezione stereoscopica di superfici inclinate
"Shear" verticali, orizzontali e percezione stereoscopica di superfici inclinate"Shear" verticali, orizzontali e percezione stereoscopica di superfici inclinate
"Shear" verticali, orizzontali e percezione stereoscopica di superfici inclinateCatina Feresin
 
The World Health Charter
The World Health CharterThe World Health Charter
The World Health CharterCatina Feresin
 
The role of the school space in preschoolers's learning processes
The role of the school space in preschoolers's learning processesThe role of the school space in preschoolers's learning processes
The role of the school space in preschoolers's learning processesCatina Feresin
 
Mood disorders in preschool and primary school children
Mood disorders in preschool and primary school childrenMood disorders in preschool and primary school children
Mood disorders in preschool and primary school childrenCatina Feresin
 
Percezione della forma e principi della Gestalt.ppt
Percezione della forma e principi della Gestalt.pptPercezione della forma e principi della Gestalt.ppt
Percezione della forma e principi della Gestalt.pptCatina Feresin
 
1 problema e basi neuroanat
1 problema e basi neuroanat1 problema e basi neuroanat
1 problema e basi neuroanatCatina Feresin
 
Lezione disturbi della sfera cognitiva
Lezione disturbi della sfera cognitivaLezione disturbi della sfera cognitiva
Lezione disturbi della sfera cognitivaCatina Feresin
 
Presentazione plitvice 2008
Presentazione plitvice 2008Presentazione plitvice 2008
Presentazione plitvice 2008Catina Feresin
 
Feresin C. and Howard I. P. (1995) Disparità verticali e movimenti oculari di...
Feresin C. and Howard I. P. (1995) Disparità verticali e movimenti oculari di...Feresin C. and Howard I. P. (1995) Disparità verticali e movimenti oculari di...
Feresin C. and Howard I. P. (1995) Disparità verticali e movimenti oculari di...Catina Feresin
 
"Progetto ascolto" nelle scuole. Riflessioni su una ricerca presso il Comune ...
"Progetto ascolto" nelle scuole. Riflessioni su una ricerca presso il Comune ..."Progetto ascolto" nelle scuole. Riflessioni su una ricerca presso il Comune ...
"Progetto ascolto" nelle scuole. Riflessioni su una ricerca presso il Comune ...Catina Feresin
 

More from Catina Feresin (20)

Do we need to train teachers and students to care about other living beings?
Do we need to train teachers and students to care about other living beings?Do we need to train teachers and students to care about other living beings?
Do we need to train teachers and students to care about other living beings?
 
Leadership in azienda
Leadership in aziendaLeadership in azienda
Leadership in azienda
 
Filosofia della mente
Filosofia della menteFilosofia della mente
Filosofia della mente
 
Lezione metodologia
Lezione metodologiaLezione metodologia
Lezione metodologia
 
Il ruolo del processo di normalizzazione verso il piano frontoparallelo nella...
Il ruolo del processo di normalizzazione verso il piano frontoparallelo nella...Il ruolo del processo di normalizzazione verso il piano frontoparallelo nella...
Il ruolo del processo di normalizzazione verso il piano frontoparallelo nella...
 
L'uso di stimoli ecologicamente validi nella percezione visiva di superfici i...
L'uso di stimoli ecologicamente validi nella percezione visiva di superfici i...L'uso di stimoli ecologicamente validi nella percezione visiva di superfici i...
L'uso di stimoli ecologicamente validi nella percezione visiva di superfici i...
 
La normalizzazione dell'inclinazione dell'immagine consecutiva e il ruolo del...
La normalizzazione dell'inclinazione dell'immagine consecutiva e il ruolo del...La normalizzazione dell'inclinazione dell'immagine consecutiva e il ruolo del...
La normalizzazione dell'inclinazione dell'immagine consecutiva e il ruolo del...
 
"Shear" verticali, orizzontali e percezione stereoscopica di superfici inclinate
"Shear" verticali, orizzontali e percezione stereoscopica di superfici inclinate"Shear" verticali, orizzontali e percezione stereoscopica di superfici inclinate
"Shear" verticali, orizzontali e percezione stereoscopica di superfici inclinate
 
The World Health Charter
The World Health CharterThe World Health Charter
The World Health Charter
 
The role of the school space in preschoolers's learning processes
The role of the school space in preschoolers's learning processesThe role of the school space in preschoolers's learning processes
The role of the school space in preschoolers's learning processes
 
Colore nel web
Colore nel webColore nel web
Colore nel web
 
Teorie del colore
Teorie del coloreTeorie del colore
Teorie del colore
 
Percezione 3 D
Percezione 3 DPercezione 3 D
Percezione 3 D
 
Mood disorders in preschool and primary school children
Mood disorders in preschool and primary school childrenMood disorders in preschool and primary school children
Mood disorders in preschool and primary school children
 
Percezione della forma e principi della Gestalt.ppt
Percezione della forma e principi della Gestalt.pptPercezione della forma e principi della Gestalt.ppt
Percezione della forma e principi della Gestalt.ppt
 
1 problema e basi neuroanat
1 problema e basi neuroanat1 problema e basi neuroanat
1 problema e basi neuroanat
 
Lezione disturbi della sfera cognitiva
Lezione disturbi della sfera cognitivaLezione disturbi della sfera cognitiva
Lezione disturbi della sfera cognitiva
 
Presentazione plitvice 2008
Presentazione plitvice 2008Presentazione plitvice 2008
Presentazione plitvice 2008
 
Feresin C. and Howard I. P. (1995) Disparità verticali e movimenti oculari di...
Feresin C. and Howard I. P. (1995) Disparità verticali e movimenti oculari di...Feresin C. and Howard I. P. (1995) Disparità verticali e movimenti oculari di...
Feresin C. and Howard I. P. (1995) Disparità verticali e movimenti oculari di...
 
"Progetto ascolto" nelle scuole. Riflessioni su una ricerca presso il Comune ...
"Progetto ascolto" nelle scuole. Riflessioni su una ricerca presso il Comune ..."Progetto ascolto" nelle scuole. Riflessioni su una ricerca presso il Comune ...
"Progetto ascolto" nelle scuole. Riflessioni su una ricerca presso il Comune ...
 

Recently uploaded

Forensic Biology & Its biological significance.pdf
Forensic Biology & Its biological significance.pdfForensic Biology & Its biological significance.pdf
Forensic Biology & Its biological significance.pdfrohankumarsinghrore1
 
Pests of cotton_Borer_Pests_Binomics_Dr.UPR.pdf
Pests of cotton_Borer_Pests_Binomics_Dr.UPR.pdfPests of cotton_Borer_Pests_Binomics_Dr.UPR.pdf
Pests of cotton_Borer_Pests_Binomics_Dr.UPR.pdfPirithiRaju
 
Recombination DNA Technology (Nucleic Acid Hybridization )
Recombination DNA Technology (Nucleic Acid Hybridization )Recombination DNA Technology (Nucleic Acid Hybridization )
Recombination DNA Technology (Nucleic Acid Hybridization )aarthirajkumar25
 
Animal Communication- Auditory and Visual.pptx
Animal Communication- Auditory and Visual.pptxAnimal Communication- Auditory and Visual.pptx
Animal Communication- Auditory and Visual.pptxUmerFayaz5
 
Stunning ➥8448380779▻ Call Girls In Panchshil Enclave Delhi NCR
Stunning ➥8448380779▻ Call Girls In Panchshil Enclave Delhi NCRStunning ➥8448380779▻ Call Girls In Panchshil Enclave Delhi NCR
Stunning ➥8448380779▻ Call Girls In Panchshil Enclave Delhi NCRDelhi Call girls
 
Green chemistry and Sustainable development.pptx
Green chemistry  and Sustainable development.pptxGreen chemistry  and Sustainable development.pptx
Green chemistry and Sustainable development.pptxRajatChauhan518211
 
Hubble Asteroid Hunter III. Physical properties of newly found asteroids
Hubble Asteroid Hunter III. Physical properties of newly found asteroidsHubble Asteroid Hunter III. Physical properties of newly found asteroids
Hubble Asteroid Hunter III. Physical properties of newly found asteroidsSérgio Sacani
 
fundamental of entomology all in one topics of entomology
fundamental of entomology all in one topics of entomologyfundamental of entomology all in one topics of entomology
fundamental of entomology all in one topics of entomologyDrAnita Sharma
 
Formation of low mass protostars and their circumstellar disks
Formation of low mass protostars and their circumstellar disksFormation of low mass protostars and their circumstellar disks
Formation of low mass protostars and their circumstellar disksSérgio Sacani
 
Zoology 4th semester series (krishna).pdf
Zoology 4th semester series (krishna).pdfZoology 4th semester series (krishna).pdf
Zoology 4th semester series (krishna).pdfSumit Kumar yadav
 
Raman spectroscopy.pptx M Pharm, M Sc, Advanced Spectral Analysis
Raman spectroscopy.pptx M Pharm, M Sc, Advanced Spectral AnalysisRaman spectroscopy.pptx M Pharm, M Sc, Advanced Spectral Analysis
Raman spectroscopy.pptx M Pharm, M Sc, Advanced Spectral AnalysisDiwakar Mishra
 
Botany krishna series 2nd semester Only Mcq type questions
Botany krishna series 2nd semester Only Mcq type questionsBotany krishna series 2nd semester Only Mcq type questions
Botany krishna series 2nd semester Only Mcq type questionsSumit Kumar yadav
 
GBSN - Microbiology (Unit 1)
GBSN - Microbiology (Unit 1)GBSN - Microbiology (Unit 1)
GBSN - Microbiology (Unit 1)Areesha Ahmad
 
All-domain Anomaly Resolution Office U.S. Department of Defense (U) Case: “Eg...
All-domain Anomaly Resolution Office U.S. Department of Defense (U) Case: “Eg...All-domain Anomaly Resolution Office U.S. Department of Defense (U) Case: “Eg...
All-domain Anomaly Resolution Office U.S. Department of Defense (U) Case: “Eg...Sérgio Sacani
 
Pests of cotton_Sucking_Pests_Dr.UPR.pdf
Pests of cotton_Sucking_Pests_Dr.UPR.pdfPests of cotton_Sucking_Pests_Dr.UPR.pdf
Pests of cotton_Sucking_Pests_Dr.UPR.pdfPirithiRaju
 
Physiochemical properties of nanomaterials and its nanotoxicity.pptx
Physiochemical properties of nanomaterials and its nanotoxicity.pptxPhysiochemical properties of nanomaterials and its nanotoxicity.pptx
Physiochemical properties of nanomaterials and its nanotoxicity.pptxAArockiyaNisha
 
Chemistry 4th semester series (krishna).pdf
Chemistry 4th semester series (krishna).pdfChemistry 4th semester series (krishna).pdf
Chemistry 4th semester series (krishna).pdfSumit Kumar yadav
 
Nanoparticles synthesis and characterization​ ​
Nanoparticles synthesis and characterization​  ​Nanoparticles synthesis and characterization​  ​
Nanoparticles synthesis and characterization​ ​kaibalyasahoo82800
 
Disentangling the origin of chemical differences using GHOST
Disentangling the origin of chemical differences using GHOSTDisentangling the origin of chemical differences using GHOST
Disentangling the origin of chemical differences using GHOSTSérgio Sacani
 

Recently uploaded (20)

Forensic Biology & Its biological significance.pdf
Forensic Biology & Its biological significance.pdfForensic Biology & Its biological significance.pdf
Forensic Biology & Its biological significance.pdf
 
Pests of cotton_Borer_Pests_Binomics_Dr.UPR.pdf
Pests of cotton_Borer_Pests_Binomics_Dr.UPR.pdfPests of cotton_Borer_Pests_Binomics_Dr.UPR.pdf
Pests of cotton_Borer_Pests_Binomics_Dr.UPR.pdf
 
Recombination DNA Technology (Nucleic Acid Hybridization )
Recombination DNA Technology (Nucleic Acid Hybridization )Recombination DNA Technology (Nucleic Acid Hybridization )
Recombination DNA Technology (Nucleic Acid Hybridization )
 
Animal Communication- Auditory and Visual.pptx
Animal Communication- Auditory and Visual.pptxAnimal Communication- Auditory and Visual.pptx
Animal Communication- Auditory and Visual.pptx
 
Stunning ➥8448380779▻ Call Girls In Panchshil Enclave Delhi NCR
Stunning ➥8448380779▻ Call Girls In Panchshil Enclave Delhi NCRStunning ➥8448380779▻ Call Girls In Panchshil Enclave Delhi NCR
Stunning ➥8448380779▻ Call Girls In Panchshil Enclave Delhi NCR
 
Green chemistry and Sustainable development.pptx
Green chemistry  and Sustainable development.pptxGreen chemistry  and Sustainable development.pptx
Green chemistry and Sustainable development.pptx
 
Hubble Asteroid Hunter III. Physical properties of newly found asteroids
Hubble Asteroid Hunter III. Physical properties of newly found asteroidsHubble Asteroid Hunter III. Physical properties of newly found asteroids
Hubble Asteroid Hunter III. Physical properties of newly found asteroids
 
fundamental of entomology all in one topics of entomology
fundamental of entomology all in one topics of entomologyfundamental of entomology all in one topics of entomology
fundamental of entomology all in one topics of entomology
 
Formation of low mass protostars and their circumstellar disks
Formation of low mass protostars and their circumstellar disksFormation of low mass protostars and their circumstellar disks
Formation of low mass protostars and their circumstellar disks
 
Zoology 4th semester series (krishna).pdf
Zoology 4th semester series (krishna).pdfZoology 4th semester series (krishna).pdf
Zoology 4th semester series (krishna).pdf
 
Raman spectroscopy.pptx M Pharm, M Sc, Advanced Spectral Analysis
Raman spectroscopy.pptx M Pharm, M Sc, Advanced Spectral AnalysisRaman spectroscopy.pptx M Pharm, M Sc, Advanced Spectral Analysis
Raman spectroscopy.pptx M Pharm, M Sc, Advanced Spectral Analysis
 
Botany krishna series 2nd semester Only Mcq type questions
Botany krishna series 2nd semester Only Mcq type questionsBotany krishna series 2nd semester Only Mcq type questions
Botany krishna series 2nd semester Only Mcq type questions
 
GBSN - Microbiology (Unit 1)
GBSN - Microbiology (Unit 1)GBSN - Microbiology (Unit 1)
GBSN - Microbiology (Unit 1)
 
All-domain Anomaly Resolution Office U.S. Department of Defense (U) Case: “Eg...
All-domain Anomaly Resolution Office U.S. Department of Defense (U) Case: “Eg...All-domain Anomaly Resolution Office U.S. Department of Defense (U) Case: “Eg...
All-domain Anomaly Resolution Office U.S. Department of Defense (U) Case: “Eg...
 
Pests of cotton_Sucking_Pests_Dr.UPR.pdf
Pests of cotton_Sucking_Pests_Dr.UPR.pdfPests of cotton_Sucking_Pests_Dr.UPR.pdf
Pests of cotton_Sucking_Pests_Dr.UPR.pdf
 
The Philosophy of Science
The Philosophy of ScienceThe Philosophy of Science
The Philosophy of Science
 
Physiochemical properties of nanomaterials and its nanotoxicity.pptx
Physiochemical properties of nanomaterials and its nanotoxicity.pptxPhysiochemical properties of nanomaterials and its nanotoxicity.pptx
Physiochemical properties of nanomaterials and its nanotoxicity.pptx
 
Chemistry 4th semester series (krishna).pdf
Chemistry 4th semester series (krishna).pdfChemistry 4th semester series (krishna).pdf
Chemistry 4th semester series (krishna).pdf
 
Nanoparticles synthesis and characterization​ ​
Nanoparticles synthesis and characterization​  ​Nanoparticles synthesis and characterization​  ​
Nanoparticles synthesis and characterization​ ​
 
Disentangling the origin of chemical differences using GHOST
Disentangling the origin of chemical differences using GHOSTDisentangling the origin of chemical differences using GHOST
Disentangling the origin of chemical differences using GHOST
 

Feresin Catina, Agostini Tiziano, Nila Saviolo Negrin, 1998

  • 1. Behavior Research Methods, Instruments, & Computers 1998,30 (4),637-642 Testing the validity of the paddle method for the kinesthetic and visual-kinesthetic perception of inclination CATINA FERESIN University ofPadua, Padua, Italy TIZIANO AGOSTINI University of Trieste, Trieste, Italy and NILANEGRIN-SAVIOLO University ofPadua, Padua, Italy Wetested the validity ofthe paddle method for measuring both the kinesthetic and visual-kinesthetic perception of inclination. In three conditions, subjects performed three different tasks: (1) rotating a manual paddle to a set of verbally given inclinations (blindfolded subjects), (2) rotating a manual paddle to the same set of verbally given inclinations after specific kinesthetic training (blindfolded subjects), and (3) rotating the paddle to a set of fixed visual inclinations after the kinesthetic train- ing. The results showed a high degree of accuracy and precision in the second and third task but not in the first one. When subjects were asked to rotate a manual paddle to a set of verbally given incli- nations, they used three main anchors (00 , 450 , 900 ) . Furthermore, the paddle method is biased by a kinesthetic deficiency, namely a rotational problem of the wrist that can be corrected by means of specific training. In a celebrated work on the role of texture in the per- ception of inclination, Gibson (1950) used, for the first time, a visual-kinesthetic method, which he called "the method ofreproduction." During that experiment, the sub- jects adjusted a rotatable surface (a manual paddle) with their palm until the kinesthetic impression of inclination was the same as the visual impression of inclination.' The "method ofreproduction" can also be called "the pad- dle method." After Gibson (1950), many scientists used "purely vi- sual methods." The first of these consists of a compari- son line or a surface in the frontal plane used by subjects to match visually the inclination of another surface in depth (e.g., Harris, Freeman, & Hughes, 1992; Ryan & Gillam, 1994; van Ee & Erkelens, 1995). The second con- sists of a monocular comparison between two surfaces, indicating which has the greater inclination in depth (see, e.g., Braunstein & Payne, 1969; Freeman, 1966; Phillips, 1970). The third visual method consists of asking sub- jects to set the inclination ofa comparison line presented after a test pattern has been removed (Epstein & Morgan- Paap, 1974). Quite recently, the visual-kinesthetic method has again been used for measuring the visual perceived inclination ofa surface. This is because some researchers recognized Correspondence should be addressed to C. Feresin, Dipartimento di Psicologia Generale, Universita di Padova, via Venezia, 8 35131 Padova, Italy (e-mail: paola@psicoserver.univ.trieste.it). that this method does not introduce other visual stimuli that might have an effect upon the measurement (Carrozzo & Lacquaniti, 1994; Howard & Kaneko, 1994; Ohmi, 1993; Proffitt, Bhalla, Gossweiler, & Midgett, 1995). For example, the intersensory method cannot be affected by visual aftereffects from the test pattern, or by the depth contrast effect. Howard and Kaneko (1994) and Proffitt et al. (1995) addressed the validity ofthe paddle method. Howard and Kaneko found an underestimation of a large range of in- clinations ofvisually perceived surfaces having a full range ofbinocular and monocular depth cues. To calibrate their unseen paddle before starting a set of experiments, they asked the subjects to match the inclination of the paddle to a very big cardboard surface. This surface was covered with random dots and was inclined at 100 intervals be- tween +800 and -800 • They used a third-order polyno- mial function, which best fit the underestimation they found, as a baseline for comparing the results ofthe cal- ibration with the results ofthe experimental conditions. Proffitt et al. (1995) used many psychophysical meth- ods to measure the geographical inclination ofhills. Their subjects gave verbal estimates of the inclination of a set of hills, adjusted a representation of the cross section of those hills, and, finally, used the visual-kinesthetic method, setting a tilt board with their hands unseen. Prof- fitt et al. found a small underestimation of the visually perceived inclination of a set of hills when using the tilt board and, overall, this measure was different from the 637 Copyright 1998 Psychonomic Society, Inc.
  • 2. 638 FERESIN, AGOSTINI, AND NEGRIN-SAVIOLO Figure 1. (A) Kinesthetic deficiency ofthe wrist at the vertical p0- sition when the forearm is orthogonal with respect to the arm. (B) To solve the kinesthetic deficiency at the vertical position, the observer has to lift up his/her forearm and incline the arm. actual inclinations of the hills. In their paper, Proffitt et al. measured the internal consistency among the three already mentioned psychophysical methods they used. They compared the kinesthetic responses the subjects made by looking at some inclined hills with the kines- thetic responses that the same subjects made in response to an almost equivalent set of verbally given angles. The subjects displayed an internal consistency for both the verbal-kinesthetic and the visual-kinesthetic measures. We decided to approach the problem ofthe validity of the paddle method in a different way than did Howard and Kaneko (1994) and Proffitt et al. (1995). Ifa person is ob- served rotating his/her wrist, it is quite clear that there is one main motor deficiency: The wrist cannot rotate com- pletely to achieve the vertical position when the forearm is orthogonal with respect to the arm (Figure IA). To avoid this rotational problem close to the vertical, the person has to lift his/her forearm up with respect to the arm and incline the arm and the shoulder by a small amount (Figure IB). However, close to the horizontal, the subject's wrist may have an impediment in its rotation due to the inclined position of the forearm with respect to the arm (Figure 2B). Because ofthis, the subject can- not really achieve a correct position of the wrist and the forearm at the horizontal position (Figure 1A). As a con- sequence ofthis possible motor problem around the ver- tical, it is best for the person to choose a position for the forearm and the wrist that allows him/her to freely rotate the paddle from the vertical to the horizontal. This posi- tion results in a compromise between the best position for ROTATABLE PADDLE ROTATABLE PADDLE HAND HAND WRIST rthogonal FOREARM B rotating the wrist to the vertical and the best position for rotating the wrist to the horizontal. Because of this compromise, we suppose that when a person is asked to kinesthetically set a paddle to the ver- tical position (0°), he/she will rotate the wrist more than 0°, stopping the setting forward and then overestimating the perceived inclination (Figure 3A). On the contrary, when a person is asked to kinesthetically set a paddle to the horizontal(90°),wesuppose thathe/she will rotatethe wrist less than 90°, stopping the setting backward and then un- derestimating the perceived inclination (Figure 3B). Furthermore, we think that it is a reasonable strategy for subjects to find and use spontaneously some kind of kinesthetic anchor. This is because we know from the lit- erature that the vertical and the horizontal are the main visual and haptic orientational anchors or norms (e.g., Cecala & Garner, 1986;Gentaz & Hatwell, 1995;Howard, 1982). When people are asked to judge the visual and the haptic inclination ofa surface or a rod, they use the hor- izontal and the vertical plane as a frame ofreference. So, why should it be different in the kinesthetic domain? The first aim of our research was to investigate the purely kinesthetic perception of a range of inclined sur- faces to test whether the paddle method is biased in some way by motor problems or strategies used by the sub- jects. The second purpose was to measure the perception of the visual inclination of a line by means of the paddle kinesthetic method. If the paddle method is influenced by motor problems, as we suppose, it should be possible ARM Figure 2. (A) Correct position ofthe wrist and the forearm at the horizontal position. (B) Uncomfortable position of the wrist to achieve the horizontal position due to the inclination ofthe forearm and arm. A ROTATABLE PADDLE HAND HAN WRIST WRIST FOREARM ROTATABLE ~======-i PADDLE ARM A B
  • 3. KINESTHETIC PERCEPTION OF SURFACE INCLINATION 639 A horizontal, and finally they found the middle position corresponding to 45°. When the subjects were asked to set the paddle to 40° or 50°, they first looked for 0° and for 90°, then they reached 45°, and finally went from it to reach 40° or 50°. At the end ofthe preliminary study, we asked the sub- jects ifthey had used any anchor as a reference point. All ofthem told us that they were using the three anchors we had observed while watching them doing the kinesthetic task. EXPERIMENT B HAND 2 -'-FOREARM Figure 3. (AI) Correctinclination ofthe paddle at the vertical: un- comfortable inclination ofthe wrist. (A2) Overestimated inclination of the paddle at the vertical: comfortable inclination of the wrist. (81) Correct inclination ofthe paddle at the horizontal: uncomfort- able inclination of the wrist. (82) Underestimated inclination of the paddle at the horizontal: comfortable inclinationofthe wrist. to compensate for such motor biases with an appropriate kinesthetic training. When the motor biases are avoided, we can use the paddle method to test the perception of the visual inclination ofa surface or a line. In this way it is possible to separate the kinesthetic outcome from the visual outcome. PRELIMINARY STUDY During a preliminary study, we observed that, while doing the kinesthetic task of rotating the manual paddle to a set of II inclinations going from the vertical (0°) to the horizontal (90°), subjects were limited in the extent of rotation of the wrist. The subject's wrist could rotate completely to achieve the vertical or horizontal position only by forcing it. But since the subjects were not asked to do that, they stopped their adjustment of the vertical and the horizontal position whenever they started to feel the impediment of rotating the wrist. We also noticed that the subjects used three main kinesthetic anchors to refer their paddle adjustment: 0° (gravitational vertical), 45°, and 90° (horizontal). The subjects' use ofthe vertical and the horizontal anchors is logical for the reason explained in the introduction; but the subjects also used 45° as an anchor. This anchor was derived in that it was achieved by the subjects as the mid- dle position between the vertical and the horizontal. For example, when the subjects had to set the paddle to 45°, they first started from the vertical, then they went to the In two conditions, we measured the accuracy and pre- cision (Ono, 1993)ofadjusting the inclination ofa manual paddle to a set of verbally given angles. In a third condi- tion, we measured the accuracy and precision ofadjusting the inclination of the same manual paddle to a set of vi- sually given inclinations. The measure ofaccuracy was the mean ofthe point of subjective equality (PSE) compared with the point ofob- jective equality (POE). The measure ofprecision was the standard error. The task performed by the subjects in the first and sec- ond conditions was a purely kinesthetic task in that the inclination ofthe manual paddle was not compared with a visual surface and the observer was blindfolded. We also investigated the subject's possible strategies in per- forming the task. The task performed by the subjects in the third condition was a visual-kinesthetic task in that the inclination ofthe manual paddle was compared with a set of inclinations ofa luminous line. Method Subjects Eighteen subjects took part in the experiment-6 for each con- dition. They were undergraduate and graduate psychology students of the University of Trieste. All were volunteers and naive regard- ing the aim of this work. Apparatus A cubic chamber, 150 em long on each side, was placed in the middle ofthe laboratory. A rotatable paddle (24 em long X 16 em wide X 3 em thick) was connected to the frame of the chamber by means of a small table. The paddle was connected to an electronic protractor (Emaco Angle Star protractor system, Montreal, Que- bec, Canada). During the third condition, the chamber was illumi- nated by two projectors placed outside the right and the left sides of the chamber itself. A thick luminous line (35 cm long X 1.5 cm wide) was mounted on a wooden rotatable bar and fixed to the cubic chamber in front of the subject at a viewing distance of 150 cm. The center of the line was at eye level (Figure 4). Procedure Condition 1. The blindfolded subject was placed at the center of the chamber and seated on an adjustable chair. After the subject had chosen the most comfortable position, which remained con- stant during all the trials, the experimenter taped the hand to the paddle. The tape did not interfere with the free movement of the wrist. The taping was done to prevent movements of the fingers that could have influenced the inclination response of the wrist. The paddle was not a weight on the hand, and ofcourse there was
  • 4. 640 FERESIN, AGOSTINI, AND NEGRIN-SAVIOLO Figure 5. Mean results of Condition 1: kinesthetic task. Results ----- Subjects' means y =5,1008 + 0,87917x R"2 =0,99(jj" GI 90GI...Cl GI 80'tl C 70~ 'E 60 GI E 50 -III ;:, 40:g l'lI .!!! 30 'tl 20'tl l'lI a. 10 w II) 0a. 0 10 20 30 40 50 60 70 80 90 POE: Verbal angle (in degrees) Condition 1 As Figure 5 shows, the 6 subjects overestimated (for- ward direction) the kinesthetic inclinations relative to the verbal angle from 0° to 20°; settings were very accurate but not very precise at 30°, quite accurate and precise (small standard errors) at 40°, and very accurate and pre- cise at 50°. Finally, the subjects underestimated (back- ward direction) the kinesthetic inclinations relative to the verbal angle from 60° to 90°. When the verbal angle was close to the gravitational vertical (from 10° to 30°), subjects always started their settings from the vertical. This means that ifthey tended to slightly overestimate the vertical itself, stopping their used by the subject as a frame of reference to incline the luminous line. This line was thick enough for linear perspective. We mea- sured this visually perceived inclination by means of the paddle. In this final condition, we trained the subjects to pay attention to the motor deficiency of the wrist and to use the three anchors, as we had done in Condition 2. This task was a visual-kinesthetic task in that the inclination of the manual paddle was compared with inclinations of the luminous line. The independent variable was the set of visual inclinations of the line-that is, the POE-whereas the dependent variable was the manual adjustment of the inclination of the paddle-that is, the PSE. The levels of the independent variable varied in steps of 10° (45° included) between gravitational vertical (0°) and 80°. We did not use the 90° inclination (horizontal) because it was perceived by the subject as a point in space and not as an inclination. The kinesthetic training was the same as in Condition 2. The subject was placed at the center of the cubic chamber, as in Conditions 1 and 2, resting his/her head on a chinrest, and was asked to rotate the manual paddle to each of a set ofinclinations of the luminous line. During this condition, the subject stopped each trial when the kinesthetic inclination of the paddle was judged to be the same as the visual inclination of the luminous line. The line was inclined by the experimenter to 1 of 10 angles pre- sented in random order. Each angle was repeated in random order five times, yielding 50 total trials for every subject. ba ~ rotatable adjustable chair Figure 4. Cubic chamber. no change of one of the limb's eigenvectors (Pagano & Turvey, 1995). The subject was asked to set the manual paddle to a set ofver- bally given inclinations relative to gravity. On each trial, the sub- ject stopped when the kinesthetic inclination of the wrist felt the same as the verbal inclination. Before performing Condition 1, the subjects were tested for their understanding of the entire set of angles. Indeed, they were asked to reproduce the entire set of angles by rotating the wrist. Eleven verbal angles went from the gravitational vertical (0°) to the horizontal (90°) in steps of 10°(45° included). The 11 settings were performed in random order, and every setting was repeated in random order five times, yielding a total of 55 trials for each sub- ject. Subjects started all their settings from a random inclination of the paddle. Condition 2. The aim ofCondition 2 was to investigate the sub- jects' performance after we had trained them to use the three an- chors strategy and to overcome the rotational deficiency of the wrist that we had noticed during the pilot study. This task, like the one in the first condition, was a purely kinesthetic task in that the inclination of the manual paddle was not compared with any visual surface inclination and the subject was blindfolded. The procedure followed by the subjects in the second condition was the same as the procedure of Condition 1 except for training. Indeed, we trained the subjects to use the three main anchors (0°, 45°, and 90°) that had been used spontaneously by the group of dur- ing in the pilot study. First, we taught them to rely on the vertical anchor (0°)when they had to set the paddle to inclinations from 10° to 30° verbally indicated, to rely on the 45° anchor for inclinations from 40° to 50°, and finally to rely on the horizontal anchor (90°) for inclinations from 60° to 80°. The training method we used was the basic one of demonstra- tion and feedback for the whole set of inclinations. Furthermore, we told the subjects about the rotational deficiency, and we trained them not to stop their adjustment when they started to feel the im- pediment in rotating the wrist. We expected an increase of accu- racy and precision as a result of this training compared with the ac- curacy and precision obtained in Condition I. Condition3. The purpose ofCondition 3 was to test subjects vi- sually perceived inclination of a line stimulus characterized by most of the monocular and binocular depth cues. All the external frames were visible; indeed, the edges of the cubic chamber were
  • 5. KINESTHETIC PERCEPTION OF SURFACE INCLINATION 641 Iil 90Gl Gl ..Cl 80Gl 'tl .= 70 --60 e Gl 50E -UI 40~ :cIII 30 .!! 'tl 20 'tl ca Q. 10 Iii enQ. ---- Subjects' means y = 0,95741 + 0,99270x R"2 = 0,99 DISCUSSION A one-group t test showed no significant difference between the paddle settings and the visual inclinations for either the 0°-40° distribution [t(4) = -0.71 ,p < .51] or the 50°-80° distribution [t(4) = 0.05, p < .96]. Figure 7. Mean results of Condition 3: training + visual- kinesthetic task. 10 20 30 40 50 60 70 80 90 POE: Inclination of the luminous line (in degrees) In the first condition, in which the subjects were not trained to use the paddle, we found an underestimation of the perceived kinesthetic inclination when the tested angles were close to the horizontal, an overestimation when they were close to the gravitational vertical, and a quite good level ofaccuracy and precision around 45°. In the second condition, in which subjects were trained to use three anchors (spontaneously used in the pilot study and in the first condition) and received feedback to com- pensate for the wrist rotational problem, we observed a good level of accuracy and precision of the responses without any under- or overestimation. In the third condi- tion, the subjects had the same kinesthetic training as in Condition 2 before performing a visual-kinesthetic task. The results of this final condition showed a high degree ofaccuracy and precision of the responses. The main point emerging from this research is that the use of the paddle method presents a wrist rotational bias due to the position of the subject's forearm while he/she is performing a kinesthetic task. However, the underes- timation and overestimation found in the first condition nearly disappears when the rotational bias is compen- sated for by giving the subject kinesthetic feedback con- cerning the veridical inclination ofthe paddle. We emphasize that, when the subjects were asked to perform a kinesthetic task by rotating a manual paddle, they spontaneously used three anchors-that is, the per- ceived 0°, 45°, and 90°. Wesuppose that training the sub- - - i : ] - - Subjects' means y = 1,6938 + 0,98101x R"2 = 0,99iii :l 90...----.:--------------. ..Cl ~ 80 c: 70 ~ w enQ. O~.........J.~-L...........I.-~L.....---L.--'--I.-~..l..-........J--.L.-J o 10 20 30 40 50 60 70 80 90 POE: Verbal angle (in degrees) Figure 6. Mean results of Condition 2: training + kinesthetic task. Condition 3 Figure 7 shows the mean responses for each visual in- clination. The 6 subjects were very accurate and precise when they kinesthetically adjusted the paddle to the 10 different visual inclinations. 'E 60 Gl E 50 Ui :ij- 40 III 30 !! :g 20 III C. 10 Condition 2 Figure 6 shows mean responses for each verbal angle. The 6 subjects were more precise than in Condition 1, as the small standard errors show. Furthermore, there was no over- or underestimation of the perceived kinesthetic inclination. Indeed, the subjects showed high accuracy from 0° to 90°. We analyzed the data in the same way as in Condi- tion I. A one-group t test showed no significant differ- ence between the paddle settings and the verbal values for either the 0°-40° distribution [t(4) = -1.50, p < .20] or the 50°-90° distribution [t(4) = -0.20, p < .85]. setting forward, they carried this initial overestimation with them and also showed this bias for angles close to the vertical. It was the same for the horizontal. Indeed, the subjects tended to underestimate the horizontal, stopping their set- ting backward, and carried this underestimationwith them, also showing it for angles close to 90° (from 60° to 80°). We divided the distribution into two halves-from 0° to 40° and from 50° to 90°, excluding the 45° since it is the inversion point between positive and negative PSE. Then we subtracted the paddle settings from the verbally defined values; finally, we analyzed the two distributions by using a one-group t test. The statistical analysis showed a significant difference between the paddle settings and the verbal values for the 0°-40° distribution [t(4) = -2.8, p < .05] and a tendency to significance in the opposite di- rection for the 50°-90° distribution [t(4) = 2.4, p < .07].
  • 6. 642 FERESIN, AGOSTINI, AND NEGRIN-SAVIOLO jects to consciously use these anchors reduced data vari- ability by preventing the use of uncontrolled strategies. Furthermore, we suppose that the kinesthetic outcome could be affected by the use of strategies different from those we found in our first condition, and we hope to in- vestigate them in a future experiment. Most important, we think that kinesthetic variables such as the effect of wrist and forearm position should be taken into account when the paddle method is used. Our results show that in a visual-kinesthetic task, an inclined line is matched correctly when subjects are pre- viously trained in the kinesthetic domain. This suggests that use of the intersensory method without training could measure a motor problem and the presence ofhet- erogeneous strategies rather than a visual misperception. In conclusion, we think the intersensory paddle method is a valid one but only when precautions are taken by training the subjects to overcome the rotational problem of the wrist and by verifying their strategies. REFERENCES BRAUNSTEIN, M. L., & PAYNE, I. W. (1969). Perspective and form ratio as determinants of relative slant judgments. Journal ofExperimen- tal Psychology. 81,584-590. CARROZZO, M., & LACQUANITI, F. (1994). A hybrid frame of reference for visuo-manual coordination. NeuroReport, 5, 453-456. CECALA, A.I., & GARNER, W. R. (1986). Internal frame ofreference as a determinant of the oblique effect. Journal ofExperimental Psy- chology: Human Perception & Performance, 12, 314-323. EpSTEIN, W., & MORGAN-PAAP, C. L. (1974). Aftereffect ofinspection of a perspectival stimulus for slant depth: A new normalization ef- fect. Perception & Psychophysics, 16, 299-302. FREEMAN, R. B. (1966). Effect of size on visual slant. Journal ofEx- perimental Psychology, 71, 96-103. GENTAZ, E., & HATWELL, Y. (1995). The haptic "oblique effect" in chil- dren's and adults' perception oforientation. Perception, 24, 631-646. GIBSON, I. I. (1950). The perception ofvisual surfaces. American Jour- nal ofPsychology, 63, 367-384. HARRIS, M., FREEMAN, T., & HUGHES, I. (1992). Retinal speed gradient and the perception of surface slant. Vision Research, 32, 587-590. HOWARD, I. P. (1982). Human visual orientation. New York: Wiley. HOWARD, I. P., & KANEKO, H. (1994). Relative shear disparities and the perception of surface inclination. Vision Research, 34, 2505-2517. KLATZKY, R. L., & LEDERMAN, S. J. (1995). Identifying objects from a haptic glance. Perception & Psychophysics, 57,1111-1123. OHMI,M. (1993). Depth perception of wide-angle surface [Abstracts of the Sixteenth European Conference on Visual Perception]. Per- ception, 22 (Supp!.), 114. ONO, H. (1993). Precision and accuracy in perception [Educational Computer Program Package]. Santa Barbara, CA: Intellimation. PAGANO, C. P., & TuRVEY, M. T. (1995). The inertia tensor as a basis for the perception oflimb orientation. Journal ofExperimental Psy- chology: Human Perception & Performance, 21,1070-1087. PHILLIPS, R.I. (1970). Stationary visual texture and the estimation of slant angle. QuarterlyJournal ofExperimental Psychology, 22, 389- 397. PROFFITT, D. R., BHALLA, M., GOSSWEILER, R., & MIDGETT, J. (1995). Perceiving geographical slant. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 2, 409-428. RYAN, C., & GILLAM, B. (1994). Cue conflict and stereoscopic surface slant about horizontal and vertical axes. Perception, 23, 645-658. VAN EE, R., & ERKELENS, C. J. (1995). Binocular perception of slant about oblique axes relative to a visual frame of reference. Percep- tion, 24, 299-314. NOfE I. Similar to Klatzky and Lederman (1995), we define kinesthesis as perceptual information coming from muscles, tendons, and joints, whereas cutaneous perceptual information derives from impressions coming from somatosensory inputs of the skin. Kinesthetic and cuta- neous perceptual information are both parts of the haptic system. (Manuscript received January 13, 1997; revision accepted for publication May 14, 1997.)