SlideShare a Scribd company logo
1 of 534
Description of Dataset for HW1
Each month the Bureau of Labor Statistics in the U.S.
Department of Labor conducts the
“Current Population Survey” (CPS), which provides data on
labor force characteristics of the
population, including the level of employment, unemployment,
and earnings. Approximately
65,000 randomly selected U.S. households are surveyed each
month. The sample is chosen
by randomly selecting addresses from a database comprised of
addresses from the most
recent decennial census augmented with data on new housing
units constructed after the last
census. The exact random sampling scheme is rather
complicated (first small geographical
areas are randomly selected, then housing units within these
areas randomly selected); details
can be found in the Handbook of Labor Statistics and is
described on the Bureau of Labor
Statistics website (www.bls.gov).
The survey conducted each March is more detailed than in other
months and asks questions
about earnings during the previous year. The file HW1 contains
the data for 2012 (from the
March 2013 survey). These data are for full-time workers,
defined as workers employed more
than 35 hours per week for at least 48 weeks in the previous
year. Data are provided for
workers whose highest educational achievement is (1) a high
school diploma, and (2) a
bachelor’s degree.
Series in Data Set:
FEMALE: 1 if female; 0 if male
YEAR: Year
AHE : Average Hourly Earnings
BACHELOR: 1 if worker has a bachelor’s degree; 0 if worker
has a high school degree
AGE: Age
Title:
Database:
Pavlovian conditioning. By: Sparzo, Frank J., Salem Press
Encyclopedia of Health, 2019
Research Starters
Pavlovian conditioning
Date: 1890s forward
Type of psychology: Learning
Pavlovian conditioning is a basic process of learning that
relates especially to reflexes and
emotional behavior. Interest in this form of learning has been
long-standing and continues to the
present day. Pavlovian principles apply to a very wide range of
organisms, situations, and
events.
Introduction
Pavlovian conditioning, also known as respondent conditioning
and classical conditioning (to
distinguish it from instrumental or operant conditioning), is an
elementary learning process and
has been of major interest to psychologists ever since the
Russian physiologist Ivan Petrovich
Pavlov discovered that a dog could learn to salivate to a neutral
stimulus after the stimulus was
paired repeatedly with food.
Pavlov’s early career focused on the study of heart circulation
and digestion in animals (usually
dogs), for which he received the Nobel Prize in Physiology or
Medicine in 1904. However, by
UAGC Library-test
Listen American Accent
One of the many
dogs Pavlov used in his
experiments (possibly
Baikal[1]), Pavlov
Museum Ryazan, Russia. Note
the saliva catch container and
tube surgically implanted in the
dog's muzzle. By Rklawton
(English Wikipedia, see below)
[GFDL
(http://www.gnu.org/copyleft/fdl.html)
or CC-BY-SA-3.0
(http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by-
sa/3.0/)], via Wikimedia
Commons
The Pavlov memorial museum,
Ryazan; former home of the
physiologist I. P. Pavlov (built in
the early 19th century) By Ceroi
(Own work) [CC-BY-SA-3.0
(http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by-
sa/3.0)], via Wikimedia
Commons
that time Pavlov had already turned his attention to
experiments on conditioned reflexes, from which flowed a
new psychological nomenclature.
Conditioning
The core of Pavlovian conditioning is the pairing
(association) of stimuli to elicit responses. Food (meat
powder) placed in a dog’s mouth naturally produces
salivation. Pavlov called the food an unconditioned stimulus
(US) and salivation, elicited by the food, the unconditioned
response (UR). When a neutral stimulus—for example, a
tone that does not naturally elicit salivation—is repeatedly
followed by food, the tone alone eventually evokes
salivation. Pavlov labeled the tone a conditioned stimulus
(CS) and the response (salivation) elicited by it the
conditioned response (CR).
Pavlov’s formulation can be summarized as follows:
Before conditioning:
Food (US) elicits Salivation (UR)
Conditioning procedure:
Neutral Stimulus (Tone) plus Food (US) elicits Salivation (UR)
After conditioning:
Tone (CS) elicits Salivation (CR)
Pavlov believed that conditioned responses were identical
to unconditioned responses. That is usually not the case.
For example, conditioned responses may be less
pronounced (weaker) or a bit more lethargic than
unconditioned responses.
Several phenomena turn up in studies of Pavlovian
conditioning. Extinction, generalization, and discrimination
are among the most important. Extinction refers to the
procedure as well as to the elimination of a CR. If the CS is
repeatedly presented without the US, extinction occurs: The dog
stops salivating to the tone.
During the course of extinction, the CR may return from time to
time until it is finally
extinguished. Pavlov called the occasional return of the CR
“spontaneous recovery.”
Stimulus generalization refers to responding not only to a
particular CS but also to similar but
different stimuli. Further, the magnitude (amount of salivation)
of a generalized response tends
to decline as stimuli become less and less like the CS. For
example, a dog trained to salivate to
a 5,000-cycle-per-second (cps) tone is likely to salivate also to
5,300 cps and 4,700 cps tones
without specific training to do so (stimulus generalization).
Responses tend to weaken in an
orderly way as tones become more and more unlike the CS—
that is, as the tones move away
from the CS in both directions, say, to 4,400 cps from 4,100
cps, and 5,600 cps to 5,900 cps, the
flow of salivation becomes less and less.
Stimulus generalization in effect extends the number of stimuli
that elicit a conditioned response.
Discrimination procedures restrict that number by conditioning
a subject not to generalize across
stimuli. The procedure involves two processes: acquisition and
extinction. The CS is paired
repeatedly with the US (acquisition) while the US is withheld as
generalized stimuli are
presented repeatedly (extinction). If the dog now salivates to
the CS and not to the generalized
stimuli, the dog has learned to discriminate or to act
discriminatively. Pavlov reported that some
dogs displayed a general breakdown in behavior patterns
(experimental neurosis) when called
on to make discriminations that were too difficult for them.
Pavlov’s work on what he called the second-signal system
implies that conditioning principles
are relevant to human as well as to animal learning. Once, say, a
tone is established as a CS in
first-order conditioning, the tone can be paired with a neutral
stimulus to establish a second-
order CS. Thus, in the absence of food, a light might precede
the tone (CS) several times until
the light itself begins to function as a CS. Second-order
conditioning appears to follow many of
the same rules as first-order conditioning.
Pavlov’s work has clearly provided one way to study the
learning process in great detail. It has
also provided the kind of data and theory that have affected
research in other areas of learning,
such as instrumental conditioning and, subsequently, cognitive
science and neuroscience.
In late 2015, neuroscientists at Johns Hopkins University
conducted an experiment in the hope
of finally determining how this learning process occurs, or
exactly how Pavlov's dogs were
conditioned to drool. For the first time, the scientists were able
to prove in a lab the link between
neurotransmitters and the conditioned response by studying
brain cells of mice. After stimulating
the cells with neurotransmitters, the scientists analyzed the
significance of the brain's chemical
reward system in terms of conditioning. This research prompted
discussion about whether this
knowledge could be used to enhance learning processes or
possibly treat cognitive issues.
Range of Pavlovian Conditioning
Pavlovian phenomena have been demonstrated with different
kinds of organisms and a wide
variety of stimuli and responses far beyond those studi ed by
Pavlov. Stimuli that precede such
unconditioned stimuli as sudden loud noises (leading to rapid
heart rate), a puff of air delivered
to the eye (evoking blinking), or a large temperature increase
(eliciting sweating) may become
conditioned stimuli capable of eliciting conditioned responses
on their own. The idea of second-
order (higher-order) conditioning is profoundly important
because it suggests how rewards such
as words of praise and money are established apart from
primary (biologically necessary)
rewards, such as food and water. It also may in part explain the
power of films, plays, novels,
and advertisements to evoke strong emotion in the absence of
direct experience with primary
(unconditioned) stimuli. Studies concerned with conditioned
emotional reactions (CER),
especially fear and anxiety in people—a subject much more
complex than simple reflexes—
have been of special interest to researchers and therapists for
many years.
Additional Research Findings
Studies of conditioning essentially look at how various
unconditioned and conditioned stimuli
influence responses under different arrangements of time and
space. Following are a few
general findings.
Pavlovian conditioning tends to be readily established when
stimuli or responses or both are
strong rather than weak. For example, in response to a near-
drowning experience, some people
promptly learn to fear such conditioned stimuli as the sight of
water, boats, palm trees, bathing
suits, and so on. In such cases, relevant stimuli and responses
(panic) are presumably quite
strong.
Conditioned stimuli are most likely to elicit conditioned
responses when unconditioned and
conditioned stimuli are paired consistently. If a mother always
hums when she rocks her infant
daughter to sleep, humming is likely to become a potent and
reliable CS, which soothes and
comforts her daughter. This outcome is less likely if the mother
hums only occasionally.
When several stimuli precede a US, the one most often paired
with the US will likely emerge as
the strongest CS. If, for example, both parents threaten to
punish their young son, but only
father always carries out the threats, father’s threats are more
likely than mother’s to evoke
apprehension in the child.
For some responses, such as eye blinking, conditioned stimuli
tend to be strongest when they
precede the US by about one-half second. The optimal interval
for other responses varies from
seconds to fractions of seconds: A neighbor’s dog barks
immediately before little Sophie falls
from her swing, bumping her nose very hard. She cries. If the
dog’s bark subsequently makes
Sophie feel uneasy, the bark is functioning as a CS. This
outcome becomes less and less likely
as the bark and fall increasingly separate in time.
Conditioned responses are usually not established if a US and
CS occur together (simultaneous
conditioning)—the potency of the UC overshadows the potential
CS—or when a neutral stimulus
follows the US (backward conditioning).
Some Practical Applications
In a widely cited study reported in 1920, American researchers
John B. Watson and Rosalie
Rayner conditioned a phobic reaction in an eleven-month-old
infant named Albert. The
researchers discovered that Albert feared loud noises but
seemed unafraid of a number of other
things, including small animals.
Watson and Rayner subsequently placed a white rat in Albert’s
crib. When Albert reached for it,
the researchers struck a piece of resonant metal with a hammer,
making a “loud sound.” After a
few such presentations, presenting the rat alone elicited crying
and various avoidance reactions.
Albert also showed signs of fear to similar things, such as a
rabbit, a furry object, and fluffy
clumps of cotton (stimulus generalization). Thus, Watson and
Rayner provided early
experimental evidence that Pavlovian principles are involved in
the acquisition of human
emotional reactions.
While this study induced a phobic reaction in the subject,
systematic desensitization is a
procedure designed to eliminate phobias and anxieties. The
procedure was largely developed
and named by South African-born therapist Joseph Wolpe.
Noting that it is very difficult to have
pleasant and anxious feelings simultaneously, Wolpe fashioned
a systematic technique to teach
clients to engage in behavior (relaxation) that competes with
anxiety.
Therapy typically begins with an interview designed to identify
specific sources of the client’s
fears. The therapist helps the client assemble a list of items that
elicit fear. Items associated with
the least amount of fear are positioned at the bottom of the list;
most feared items are placed
near the top. For example, if a client has a strong fear of dogs,
the therapist and client would
develop a list of scenes that make the client fearful. Situations
may vary from hearing the word
“dog” to seeing pictures of dogs, being in the vicinity of a dog,
hearing a dog bark, being close to
dogs, and patting a dog.
The client is next taught to relax by tensing and releasing
various groups of muscles—
shoulders, face, arms, neck, and so on. This phase of treatment
ends when the client has
learned to fully relax on his or her own in a matter of minutes.
The client and therapist now move on to the next phase of
therapy. While remaining fully
relaxed, the client is asked to imagine being in the first
situation at the bottom of the list. The
image is held for several seconds. The client then relaxes for
about twenty seconds before
imagining the same situation again for several seconds. When
the client is able to imagine an
item and remain fully relaxed, the therapist presents a slightly
more fearful situation to imagine.
This procedure continues until an image causes distress, at
which time the session ends. The
next session begins with relaxation, followed by the client
slowly moving up the list. As before,
the client stops at the point of distress. Therapy is successful
when the client can imagine all the
items on the list while remaining fully relaxed. The technique is
less helpful when clients have
difficulty identifying fearful situations or calling up vivid
images.
In the hands of a skillful therapist, systematic desensitization is
an effective technique for
reducing a wide variety of fears. Its Pavlovian features involve
pairing imagined fearful scenes
with relaxation. When relaxation successfully competes with
fear, it becomes a new CR to the
imagined scenes. As relaxation becomes sufficiently strong as a
CR, anxiety is replaced by
calmness in the face of earlier aversive stimuli.
Extinction offers a more direct route to the reduction of fear
than systematic desensitization. The
technique called flooding makes use of extinction. Flooding
exposes the client to fear-arousing
stimuli for a prolonged period of time. Suppose a child is afraid
of snakes. Although fear is likely
to increase initially, flooding would require the child to
confront the snake directly and
continuously—to be “flooded” by various stimuli associated
with the snake—until the conditioned
stimuli lose their power to elicit fear. Some therapists think that
the application of this technique
is probably best left to professionals.
Some Everyday Examples
Pavlovian principles may be plausibly applied to daily life, as
the following examples illustrate.
Couples sometimes refer to a certain tune as “our song.” A
plausible interpretation is that
Pavlovian conditioning has been at work. The favored tune may
have been popular and
repeated often at the time of the couple’s courtship and
marriage. The tune has since become a
CS that evokes a variety of pleasant feelings associated with
initial love.
A babysitter notes that giving a young child a blue blanket in
the absence of his mother markedly
reduces his irritability. Most likely the blanket has been
sufficiently associated with the soothing
actions of his mother (US) and now functions as a calming
stimulus (CS).
An adolescent steadfastly avoids the location where he was
seriously injured in an automobile
accident. He says that just thinking about the highway makes
him nervous. The location
doubtless contains a number of conditioned aversive stimuli that
now trigger unpleasant feelings
(CR) and avoidance.
After a bitter divorce, a woman finds that the sight of household
items (CS) associated with her
former husband is terribly upsetting (CR). She has reduced her
resentment by getting rid of the
offending items.
A wife often places flower arrangements in her husband’s den.
The flowers (CS) now bring him
a measure of comfort (CR) when she is away on trips.
Respondent Conditioning and Reinforcement
Pavlovian behaviors are principally elicited by antecedent
events (just as low temperatures elicit
shivering), while many behaviors are strengthened (in
reinforcement) or weakened (in
punishment) by what follows behavior. In Pavlovian
conditioning, two stimuli are presented, one
following another, regardless of what a subject does. What
follows behavior is usually not
important in this form of conditioning. In studying the role of
reinforcement on behavior
(instrumental or operant conditioning), the consequences that
follow a person’s actions often
determine what the person is likely to do under similar
circumstances in the future. What follows
behavior is important in this type of conditioning.
The topic of reinforcement is introduced here because Pavlovian
conditioning and reinforcement
are intricately related in that any Pavlovian conditioning is
likely to contain elements of
instrumental conditioning, and vice versa. For example, if
someone has a near-drowning
experience and now avoids bodies of water, it is plausible to say
that conditioned stimuli
associated with the experience evoke unsettling feelings. The
person reduces the unpleasant
feelings by avoiding bodies of water. In this example, negative
feelings are conditioned
according to Pavlovian principles. The avoidance reaction is
maintained by (negative)
reinforcement and involves instrumental learning. Virtually all
the previous examples can be
analyzed similarly.
Bibliography
Baldwin, John D., and Janice I. Baldwin. Behavior Principles in
Everyday Life. 4th ed. Upper
Saddle River: Prentice Hall, 2001. Print.
Dance, Scott. "Johns Hopkins Neuroscientists Trace What Made
Pavlov's Dog Salivate."
Baltimore Sun. Tribune, 6 Dec. 2015. Web. 23 Feb. 2016.
Hergenhahn, B. R. An Introduction to the History of
Psychology. 6th ed. Belmont: Wadsworth,
2009. Print.
Levis, Donald J. Foundations of Behavioral Therapy. New
Brunswick: Transaction, 2010. Print.
"Pavlovian Test Finds Sleeping Consciousness." New Scientist
26 Sept. 2009: 18. Print.
Ramnerö, Jonas, and Niklas Törneke. ABCs of Human
Behavior: Behavioral Principles for the
Practicing Clinician. Oakland: New Harbinger, 2008. Print.
Redish, A. David. The Mind Within the Brain. Oxford: Oxford
UP, 2013. Print.
Rescorla, Robert A. “Pavlovian Conditioning: It’s Not What
You Think It Is.” American
Psychologist 43.3 (1988): 151–60. Print
Watson, J. B., and R. Rayner. “Conditioned Emotional
Reactions.” Journal of Experimental
Psychology 3 (1920): 1–14. Print.
Wolpe, Joseph. The Practice of Behavior Therapy. 4th ed.
Boston: Allyn, 2008. Print.
Copyright of Salem Press Encyclopedia of Health is the
property of Salem Press. The
copyright in an individual article may be maintained by the
author in certain cases. Content may
not be copied or emailed to multiple sites or posted to a listserv
without the copyright holder's
express written permission. However, users may print,
download, or email articles for individual
use. Source: Salem Press Encyclopedia of Health, 2019, 5p
Item: 93872139
Google Book Preview
Goodreads- User Reviews
Plum Print
Title:
Database:
Operant conditioning. By: Rholetter, Wylene, MEd, Salem Press
Encyclopedia, 2019
Research Starters
Operant conditioning
Export Manager Save E-mail
Number of items to be saved: 1
Save citations to a file formatted for:
Direct Export in RIS Format (e.g. CITAVI, EasyBib, EndNote,
ProCite, Reference Manager,
Zotero)
Generic bibliographic management software
Citations in XML format
Citations in BibTeX format
Citations in MARC21 format
Direct Export to RefWorks
Direct Export to EndNote Web
Direct Export to EasyBib
Download CSV
Direct Export to NoodleTools
Save Cancel
x
UAGC Library-test
Listen
The hierarchy of operant
conditioning. By Studentne
[GFDL
(http://www.gnu.org/copyleft/fdl.html)
or CC-BY-SA-3.0
(http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by-
sa/3.0)], via Wikimedia
Commons
Operant conditioning, a term coined by B. F. Skinner, American
psychologist and radical
behaviorist, is the idea that behavior is the learned result of
consequences. Skinner, who
introduced the concept in his 1938 book The Behavior of
Organisms: An Experimental Analysis,
theorized that operant conditioning in the form of
reinforcements and punishments leads to an
association between a behavior and its consequence. Positive
reinforcement increases a
desirable behavior by following it with a favorable stimulus.
Negative reinforcement increases a
desirable behavior by removing an unfavorable stimulus after
the behavior is performed. Both
positive and negative reinforcement seek to increase a desirable
behavior. Punishment, like
reinforcement, also has positive and negative varieties. Positive
punishment is adding an
unfavorable stimulus in an effort to eradicate an undesirable
behavior. Negative punishment is
removing an unpleasant stimulus in order to decrease
undesirable behavior. Both positive and
negative punishment seek to decrease an undesirable behavior.
Overview
Skinner designed an operant conditioning chamber, which
came to be known as the Skinner box, to test his theory of
operant conditioning on animals. The Skinner box
prevented human interruption of the experimental session
and allowed the experimenter to study the behavior of an
animal as a continuous process. The box includes at least
one lever or key that the animal can manipulate to release
food, water, or some other reward or to avoid punishment
such as an electric shock. Skinner’s experiments with rats
and pigeons showed that the animals first hit the lever and
released food accidentally; after a few accidental releases,
the reinforcement of manipulating the lever ensured that the
behavior would be repeated. Skinner believed that operant
conditioning could be used in similar ways with human
beings.
Modifying behavior through operant conditioning has been used
in the treatment of phobias,
obsessive-compulsive disorders, substance-abuse problems, and
some sexual disorders, but
the impact of Skinner’s theories about operant conditioning has
proved to be immense, reaching
far beyond the field of psychology. Zoos and other animal
facilities routinely use food as a
positive reinforcement to train animals to move within enclosed
areas and to increase safety
during veterinary examinations. With human subjects, operant
conditioning has been used to
control absenteeism in the workplace (such as when employers
offer staff members with no
absences a chance to win cash rewards), to increase sales
(coupons), and to manage agitation
in older adults with dementia. Perhaps no field has been more
influenced by operant
conditioning than education. Skinner’s assertion that positive
reinforcement is more effective
than punishment at changing and establishing desirable behavior
led to the discrediting of
punitive punishment in schools and the common application of
timeouts (negative
reinforcement) and a token economy (i.e., rewarding good
behavior with gold stars that can be
accumulated for prizes) instead.
Critics of operant conditioning have been vehement in pointing
out its detriments. As early as
1959, American linguist and cognitive scientist Noam Chomsky
argued that what worked in
Skinner’s laboratory could be applied to complex human
behavior only in a superficial way. In
1960 progressive educator A. S. Neil insisted that rewarding
good behavior taught that the
behavior was not worth doing for reasons other than the reward.
Other critics were even more
severe, charging that operant conditioning was dangerous and
inhumane. Gradually, the
influence of Skinner’s ideas declined, and by the twenty-first
century, some declared that
operant conditioning had become peripheral in psychology and
related fields. However, in 2002
a list of ninety-nine top psychologists was published in the
Review of General Psychology and
B. F. Skinner topped the list.
Bibliography
Bunzli, Samantha, David Gillham, and Adrian Esterman.
“Physiotherapy-Provided Operant
Conditioning in the Management of Low Back Pain Disability:
A Systematic
Review.” Physiotherapy Research International 16.1 (2011): 4–
19. Academic Search Premier.
Web. 7 Aug. 2013.
Davey, Graham, and Chris Cullen. Human Operant Conditioning
and Behavior Modification.
New York: Wiley, 1988. Print.
Dayan, Peter. “Instrumental Vigour in Punishment and Reward.”
European Journal of
Neuroscience 35.7 (2012): 1152–68. Academic Search Premier.
Web. 7 Aug. 2013.
Dayan, Peter, et al. “Disentangling the Roles of Approach,
Activation and Valence in
Instrumental and Pavlovian Responding.” PLoS Computational
Biology 7.4 (2011): 1–
28. Academic Search Premier. Web. 7 Aug. 2013.
Edwards, Darren J. Integrating Behavioural and Cognitive
Psychology: A Modern Categorization
Theoretical Approach. Hauppage: Nova, 2015. Print.
Fonseca, Amilcar Rodrigues, Maria Cristina Zago Castelli, and
Emileane Costa Assis de
Oliveira. "Effects of Chronic Mild Stress on Operant
Discrimination Learning." Behavior Analysis:
Research and Practice 15.1 (2015): 20–27. Print.
Iversen, Iver H. “Skinner’s Early Research: From Reflexology
to Operant
Conditioning.” American Psychologist 47.11 (1992): 1318–28.
PsycINFO. Web. 24 July 2013.
Miller, Harold L., Jr., and E. Benjamin H. Heuston. “Recent
Trends in Operant Conditioning.”
21st Century Psychology: A Reference Handbook. Eds. Stephen
F. Davis et al. Los Angeles:
Sage, 2008, 340–50. Print.
Murphy, Eric S., and Frances K. McSweeney. The Wiley-
Blackwell Handbook of Operant and
Classical Conditioning. Hoboken: Wiley, 2014. Print.
Parrish, Margaret. “Behaviorism.” Social Work Perspectives on
Human Behavior. Maidenhead:
Open UP, 2010, 98–109. Print.
Rapanelli, Maximiliano, Luciana Romina Frick, and Bonifacio
Silvano Zanutto. “Learning an
Operant Conditioning Task Differentially Induces Gliogenesis
in the Medial Prefrontal Cortex and
Neurogenesis in the Hippocampus.” PLoS ONE 6.2 (2011): 1–
12. Academic Search Premier.
Web. 7 Aug. 2013.
Reynolds, George Stanley. A Primer of Operant Conditioning.
Rev. ed. Glenview: Scott, 1975.
Print.
Staddon, J. E. R., and D. T. Cerutti. “Operant Conditioning.”
Annual Review of Psychology 54
(2003): 115–44. PsycINFO. Web. 24 July 2013.
Copyright of Salem Press Encyclopedia is the property of Salem
Press. The copyright in an
individual article may be maintained by the author in certain
cases. Content may not be copied
or emailed to multiple sites or posted to a listserv without the
copyright holder's express written
permission. However, users may print, download, or email
articles for individual use. Source:
Salem Press Encyclopedia, 2019, 2p
Item: 89677602
Google Book Preview
Goodreads- User Reviews
Plum Print
172 asrt.org/publications
Editorial
Learning Theories: Behaviorism
Kevin R Clark, EdD, R.T.(R)(QM)
I
n its simplest form, learning is defined as gaining
knowledge through study, teaching, instruction, or
experience.1 Interestingly, learning is described and
viewed differently by theorists, researchers, and
practitioners who have spent time investigating and
experimenting in the educational psychology field.1,2
The differences in how educational theorists believe
individuals acquire, retain, and recall knowledge result-
ed in the development of multiple learning theories.1-3
Based on the context of the theorists’ work and other
factors at the time of investigation, these theories
explain how learning occurs, what internal or external
factors inf luence learning, how memory affects learn-
ing, and how transfer of knowledge occurs.1-3 In addi-
tion, the roles of the instructors and learners are
described according to each theory of learning. A basic
understanding of the various learning theories is essen-
tial for educators who strive to lead a classroom that is
conducive to learning and success.
The ideas of behaviorism date back to the late
19th and early 20th centuries when John Watson, an
American psychologist, believed the general public
would accept and recognize the new philosophy of psy-
chology as a true science only if it involved processes
of objective observation and scientific measurement.1
This notion of detailed observation and measurement
became central to the work of behaviorists.1
Behaviorism emphasizes that learning occurs when
an individual responds favorably to some type of
external stimuli.1-4 Behaviorism sometimes is referred
to as the stimulus-response theory.1 For example, when
presented with a math f lashcard showing the equation
6 3 8, the learner responds with the answer 48. The
equation is the stimulus, and the answer is the associ-
ated response.2 Essential elements with behaviorism
include the stimulus, the response, and the association
between these 2 elements.2 Of particular importance
is how the association between the stimulus and the
response is made, strengthened, and maintained.2
Behaviorists define learning as nothing more than
the acquisition of new behaviors. Behaviorists do not
emphasize thinking or other mental activities as a part
of the learning process because such variables are not
observable behaviors.1-4 Although the behaviorism
theory discounts any mental activity, other educational
theorists considered these processes to be a vital part of
learning and cognition, which resulted in the develop-
ment of other theories of learning.1,4 Behaviorists do not
address memory and how new behaviors or changes
in behaviors are stored or recalled for future use.2
Behaviorists refer to this type of learning, where a reac-
tion is made to a particular stimulus, as conditioning.1
Two main types of conditioning include Pavlov’s classi -
cal conditioning and Skinner’s operant conditioning.
Classical Conditioning
Ivan Pavlov, a Russian physiologist, noticed that dogs
salivated every time they ate or saw food and believed
173RADIOLOGIC TECHNOLOGY, November/December
2018, Volume 90, Number 2
Editorial
Clark
bowling alley.1 Skinner made generalizations about his
findings with rats and pigeons to humans.1 In addition,
he noted that operant conditioning also worked in a
negative way: stopping a behavior from occurring by
punishing it.1,3
Reinforcement and Punishment
Key aspects of operant conditioning include rein-
forcement and punishment, both of which can be
positive or negative. Reinforcement refers to anything
that has the effect of strengthening a particular behav-
ior for it to occur again.1,3 Positive reinforcement is the
addition of a rewarding stimulus to get the behavior to
happen again (eg, rewarding learners for making a high
grade on an exam in hopes they study harder for future
assessments and score high again). Negative reinforce-
ment is the removal of an unpleasant stimulus to get the
behavior to continue (eg, students learning the rules to
solve a particular problem so their instructor quits nag-
ging them about the importance of it). The unpleasant
behavior of the instructor’s nagging is removed when
students learn the rules, solve the problem correctly,
and continue the action so the nagging does not return.
Conversely, punishment refers to anything that
has an effect of lessening or discouraging a particular
behavior so that it does not occur again.1,3 Positive pun-
ishment is the addition of an unpleasant stimulus to get
the behavior to stop; any type of disciplinary action is
considered positive punishment. Negative punishment
is the removal of a rewarding stimulus to get the behav-
ior to stop (eg, not offering extra credit opportunities in
hopes the behavior stops so that the learners can receive
these beneficial opportunities in the future). Skinner
maintained that rewards and punishments control most
human behaviors.1-3
In addition to Watson, Pavlov, and Skinner, other
theorists were associated with the behaviorist move-
ment. The Table summarizes their contributions to the
theory of behaviorism.
Implications in Teaching and Learning
Behaviorists believe learning begins when a cue
or stimulus from the environment is presented, and
the learner reacts to the stimulus with some type of
response.1-3 Those responses are reinforced or punished,
he could condition the dogs to salivate at the sound of
a bell.1 Initially, Pavlov sounded a bell at the time food
was presented to the dogs and repeated this process
frequently.1 Eventually, the sound of the bell became
an indication to the dogs that food was about to be pre-
sented, and they responded by salivating at the sound
of the bell regardless of whether food was presented.1
This type of reinforcement of a natural ref lex or some
involuntary behavior that occurs as a response to a par-
ticular stimulus is called classical conditioning.1 Pavlov
was able to condition the dogs to salivate in response to
the sound of the bell.
Pavlov identified 4 stages of classical conditioning:
acquisition, extinction, generalization, and discrimina-
tion.1 The acquisition stage is the initial learning of
the conditioned response (the dogs salivating at the
sound of the bell).1 Pavlov believed the conditioned
response would not remain indefinitely, so he used the
term extinction to describe the disappearance of a con-
ditioned response.1 Pavlov demonstrated extinction by
repeatedly sounding the bell without presenting food
to the dogs.1 The final 2 stages, generalization and dis-
crimination, are opposites and explain how behaviorists
believe knowledge is transferred within learners.2 The
generalization stage implies that a conditioned response
might occur with similar stimuli without further train-
ing (the dogs salivating at the sound of something
similar to a bell).1 In contrast, the discrimination stage
indicates that a conditioned response might occur with
1 stimulus but not with another (the dogs not salivating
at the sound of something similar to a bell).1
Operant Conditioning
BF Skinner, a psychologist working in the United
States in the 1930s, established the theory of oper-
ant conditioning: a process of reinforcing a voluntary
behavior by rewarding it.1,3 Studying the behaviors of
rats, Skinner used a device (now called a Skinner box)
that contained a lever.1 W henever the rats pressed the
lever (an action Skinner considered normal, random,
and voluntary), a pellet of food was presented.1 As the
food rewards continued during the repetition of the
action, the rats learned that they had to press the lever
to be fed.1 Skinner also used reinforcement techniques
to teach pigeons to dance and to roll a ball down a mini
174 asrt.org/publications
Editorial
Learning Theories: Behaviorism
the reinforcement of appropriate classroom behaviors,
which can create a more orderly classroom environment
that is conducive to learning and success for all.1
Learning Activities
Classroom learning activities connected to the
behaviorism theory include1-3:
� lecturing
� recalling facts
� defining and illustrating concepts
� applying explanations
� participating in rote learning (ie, memorization
based on repetition)
� completing drill and practice exercises
� establishing classroom management policies
� using rewards and punishments
Implications in Medical Imaging Education
In medical imaging education, lecturing is a domi-
nant approach to presenting information because of
the complexity of the content. Considering time man-
agement issues and restrictions in higher education,
lecturing affords instructors an opportunity to pres-
ent a large amount of information to a large audience.
Often, medical imaging students memorize some of the
content presented and recall that knowledge during an
exam. The role of repetition aids in the learning of new
and challenging content. Medical imaging students
benefit from drill and practice exercises when working
with formulas, including the Inverse-Square Law, the
milliampere-seconds–distance compensation formula,
and this process is repeated so that the responses
become automatic.3 Ultimately, the change in behav-
ior indicates learning has occurred.3 As revealed,
behaviorism has little regard for mental processes or
understanding and, therefore, does not prepare learners
for problem-solving or critical-thinking skills.1-3
The instructor plays a dominant role in behaviorism
by leading the learning environment, using positive and
negative reinforcement to shape learners’ behaviors,
and presenting the content.1 With behaviorism, learn-
ers are described as passive individuals who voluntarily
respond to external stimuli.1 Other behaviorist implica-
tions in teaching and learning include1:
� creating procedures and expectations to manage
the classroom
� using rewards as incentives for learners to work
hard and behave
� using punishments (eg, loss of privileges or with-
holding of rewards) effectively and sparingly to
change learners’ behaviors
Critics of behaviorism argue that rewards can belittle
or demean a learning experience and, therefore, should
be used with caution.1 Often, rewards can evoke feel-
ings of unfairness or competition, and some learners
might become distracted from the real issue involved
in completing a task or learning new material.1 Using a
rewards system or giving 1 learner increased attention
might have a detrimental effect on others in the class or
cause them to feel isolated.1 Not surprisingly, rewards
do not always lead to higher-quality work; however,
using a behaviorist approach, rewards can result in
Table
Key Theorists and Their Contributions to Behaviorism1
Theorists Contribution
Ivan Pavlov Classical conditioning
Edward Thorndike Connectionism (emphasized the role of
experience in the strengthening and weakening of stimulus-
response
connections)
John Watson Scientific objectivity; Law of frequency (the more
frequent a stimulus and response occur in association with
each other, the stronger the habit will become)
Edwin Guthrie Contiguity (the same response to a stimulus most
likely will occur over and over again during repeated expo-
sures)
BF Skinner Operant conditioning
175RADIOLOGIC TECHNOLOGY, November/December
2018, Volume 90, Number 2
Editorial
Clark
and the grid conversion formula, as well as calculations
involving skin dose. Medical imaging instructors ben-
efit from using a behaviorist approach by implementing
a classroom management plan to lead a classroom con-
ducive to learning and success.
Conclusion
The theory of behaviorism can be illustrated by
the adage, “practice makes perfect.” Behaviorists see
learning as an observable change in behavior as a result
of experience and repetition. This stimulus-response
theory makes no attempt to assess the mental processes
necessary for learners to acquire, retain, and recall
information. The change in behavior is simply achieved
through a conditioning process using reinforcement
and punishment. Even though little importance is
placed on mental activity, concept formation, or under-
standing, there is a place for behaviorism in today’s
classrooms, especially in medical imaging education, in
the areas of rote learning and classroom management.
Kevin R Clark, EdD, R.T.(R)(QM), is assistant
professor and graduate coordinator for the School of Health
Professions at The University of Texas MD Anderson
Cancer Center in Houston. He serves on the Radiologic
Technology Editorial Review Board and can be reached at
[email protected]
References
1. Pritchard A. Behaviourism and the beginnings of theory. In:
Ways of Learning – Learning Theories and Learning Styles in
the Classroom. 3rd ed. New York, NY: Routledge; 2014:6-17.
2. Ertmer PA, Newby TJ. Behaviorism, cognitivism, construc-
tivism: comparing critical features from an instructional
design perspective. Perform Improv Q. 2013;26(2):43-71.
doi:10.1002/piq.21143.
3. Kelly J. Learning theories. The Peak Performance Center
website. http://thepeakperformancecenter.com/education
al-learning/learning/theories/. Published September 2012.
Accessed June 10, 2017.
4. David L. Behaviorism. Learning Theories website. https://
www.learning-theories.com/behaviorism.html. Published
January 31, 2007. Accessed June 10, 2017.
https://doi.org/10.1002/piq.21143
Copyright of Radiologic Technology is the property of
American Society of Radiologic
Technologists and its content may not be copied or emailed to
multiple sites or posted to a
listserv without the copyright holder's express written
permission. However, users may print,
download, or email articles for individual use.
ORIGINAL ARTICLE
If Everyone Is Doing It, It Must Be Safe: College Students’
Development of
Attitudes toward Poly-Substance Use
Erin Willisa , Robyn Adamsb, and Justin Keeneb
aAdvertising, Public Relations & Media Design, University of
Colorado Boulder, Boulder, Colorado, USA; bCreative Media
Industries,
College of Media and Communication, Texas Tech University,
Lubbock, Texas, USA
ABSTRACT
Background: While binge drinking on college campuses has
been a topic of concern for dec-
ades, especially among fraternity and sorority members,
recreational drug use is on the rise
and mixing alcohol and drugs is now more of a concern than
ever. Objective: Social learning
theory was used as a framework for understanding how students
develop attitudes regard-
ing the possible risks and rewards of various behaviors such as
binge drinking and drug
use. Method: This research reports the results of 13 focus group
discussions with 63 college
students. A thematic approach was used and revealed several
themes: participating in col-
lege culture, experimenting is expected, ignoring risk-taking,
and resisting peer pressure.
Findings: Participants felt as if it was expected that college
students would experiment with
alcohol and drugs, and that it was just “part of going away to
college.” Students reported
ignoring the known risks of mixing alcohol and drugs use
despite prior education efforts.
Conclusions: The findings of this study suggest that alcohol and
drug use on college cam-
puses is, at least in part, driven by a perception of college
culture and a poor balancing of
the risks and rewards associated with these behaviors.
KEYWORDS
Binge drinking; social
learning theory; focus
groups; non-medical use of
prescription medication;
risky behavior
Over one-third of full-time college students (18–22)
engaged in binge drinking in the past month; about
one in five used an illicit drug in the past month
(Center for Behavioral Health Statistics & Quality,
2015). The Centers for Disease Control and
Prevention (2017) characterize substance use as the
foremost public health hazard facing college students.
Substance use creates negative health, social, and eco-
nomic consequences for students, their families, and
their communities (National Institute on Drug Abuse,
2017; Substance Abuse and Mental Health Services,
2006). While binge drinking on college campuses has
been a topic of concern for decades (Hahm, Kolaczyk,
Jang, Swenson, & Bhindarwala, 2012; Wechsler, Lee,
Kuo, & Lee, 2000; White & Hingson, 2013), recre-
ational drug use is on the rise and poly-substance use
(mixing alcohol and drugs) is now more of a concern
than ever (CDC, 2017). In addition, much of this
poly-substance use involves prescription medication in
lieu of more illicit drugs (e.g., cocaine, marijuana).
Young people who engage in the non-medical use of
prescription medications have an increased risk of
using other drugs (e.g., alcohol, marijuana), suffering
from health issues (e.g., weight gain, mental health
problems), and engaging in risky behaviors (e.g.,
unprotected sex, criminal activity) (Ford &
Arrastia, 2008).
Exposure and access to prescription medication is
high on college campuses with 61% of college students
being offered these medications at least once, and 31%
using them non-medically (Garnier-Dykstra, Caldeira,
Vincent, O’Grady, & Arria, 2012). In addition, stu-
dents often overestimate the risky behaviors of their
peers; they overestimate stimulant use by 12.2% and
pain medication use by 8.8%, whereas marijuana use
by only 2.9% (McCabe, 2008). Such an overestimation
of risky behavior likely influences students’ percep-
tions of the risks associated with particular behaviors
and shifts their likelihood of partaking in such behav-
ior in the future.
Many universities and colleges now require stu-
dents to complete alcohol education programs prior
to arriving on campus (Croom et al., 2009). Previous
research notes that these brief alcohol interventions
yield only modest results, and that education alone is
not effective (Carey, Scott-Sheldon, Garey, Elliott, &
Carey, 2016; Tanner-Smith & Lipsey, 2015). Thus, this
study has two goals: (1) to better understand the role
CONTACT Erin Willis [email protected] University of Colorado
Boulder, 1511 University Ave, Boulder, CO, 478 UCB, 80309,
USA.
� 2019 Taylor & Francis Group, LLC
SUBSTANCE USE & MISUSE
2019, VOL. 54, NO. 11, 1886–1893
https://doi.org/10.1080/10826084.2019.1618334
http://crossmark.crossref.org/dialog/?doi=10.1080/10826084.20
19.1618334&domain=pdf&date_stamp=2019-06-28
http://orcid.org/0000-0002-1582-0867
http://orcid.org/0000-0002-1404-0025
https://doi.org./10.1080/10826084.2019.1618334
http://www.tandfonline.com
of social influence and peer behavior on the creation
and maintenance of attitudes toward various sub-
stance use behaviors, like mixing alcohol and drugs;
(2) to better understand how these attitudes interact
with perceptions of rewarding outcomes and risky
consequences to influence planned behavior. The goals
of this study add to our understanding of college stu-
dents’ decision-making processes, and better inform
health education and promotion targeted at new and
incoming students. We employed qualitative method-
ology to gain insight into college students’ perspec-
tives specific to the social learning theory. The results
are discussed within the context of this theory with
particular emphasis on the practical outcomes associ-
ated with cessation efforts on college campuses related
to education and intervention.
Social learning theory
Social learning theory provides a theoretical frame-
work for understanding risk-taking behaviors among
college students. The theory posits that people can
learn by observing and modeling others’ behaviors
(Bandura, 1977). Deviant behavior is learned and pri -
mary groups, such as peer groups, play a central role
in this learning. One place where the influence of
peers is prevalent is college campuses. Indeed, collegi -
ate peer use of alcohol often determines individual
use, and peer norms predict binge drinking (Bandura,
1977; Crawford & Novak, 2010; Read, Wood, Kahler,
Maddock, & Palfai, 2003; Sher, Bartholow, & Nanda,
2001; Tyler, Schmitz, Ray, Adams, & Gordon Simons,
2017). Previous research also notes that many college
students have positive attitudes toward alcohol use
(Peralta & Steele, 2010; Schultz, Nolan, Cialdini,
Goldstein, & Griskevicius, 2007; Wechsler et al., 2003),
have well-defined reasons for drinking, for instance,
mood enhancement or reducing stress (O’Connor &
Colder, 2005; O’Hara, Armeli, & Tennen, 2015).
In addition, the perceived benefits (e.g., social
interaction, fun/enjoyment) of drinking are significant
predictors of alcohol use (Brooks-Russel, Simons-
Morton, Haynie, Farhat, & Wang, 2014). Prior work
has demonstrated that first-year students are highly
susceptible to modeling the behavior of their older
peers, and are at the highest risk for the negative con-
sequences of alcohol use (Armeli, Conner, Cullum, &
Tennen, 2010; Maggs, Williams, & Lee, 2011).
Students with the highest likelihood of engaging in
multiple health-risk behaviors reported poorer mental
health, particularly related to stress and anxiety
(Martinez, Klanecky, & McChargue, 2018). Perceived
norms influence college students’ level of drinking
through the observation and comparison of their
peers’ drinking levels (Fournier, Hall, Ricke, & Storey,
2013; Stappenbeck, Quinn, Wetherill, & Fromme,
2010). The prediction here is that risky substance use
on college campuses is, at least in part, a product of
social learning processes that lead to attitudes regard-
ing specific substances and situations.
Differential association and reinforcement
There are several key elements to the learning process,
including differential association and differential
reinforcement (Akers, 2011). Differential association is
the association with individuals who engage in certain
types of conduct, as well as the exposure to different
sets of values and norms as a consequence of such
associations (Akers, 2011). For example, over 70% of
students nationwide overestimated the quantity of
alcohol consumed by their peers; further, the percep-
tion of campus drinking norms was by far the stron-
gest predictor of personal consumption, stronger even
than the actual campus drinking norm (Wesley
Perkins, Haines, & Rice, 2005).
Differential reinforcement is “the balance of antici-
pated and actual rewards and punishments that follow
or are consequences of behavior” (Akers, 2000, p. 78).
Within the context of alcohol use, this could take the
form of several different outcomes (e.g., hangovers,
Driving While Impaired (DWIs), alcohol poisoning).
However, as Durkin, Wolfe, and Clark (2005) demon-
strated, college-aged binge drinkers reported that alco-
hol consumption has more rewarding outcomes than
negative consequences.
In addition to the prediction related to the role of
social learning processes in risky behaviors, the cur-
rent study also seeks to understand how these social
learning processes interact with prior knowledge
regarding the risky nature of certain behaviors. By
concentrating on the differential associations and rein-
forcements regarding binge drinking and drug use
among college students, this study explores how atti-
tudes are formed and how behaviors are reinforced by
perceptions of normative behavior within peer groups.
This research fills a gap in the literature related to the
qualitative exploration of college students’ perceptions
of poly-substance use and risk-taking behaviors.
Method
This study used focus groups drawn from a larger stu-
dent population at a southwestern university. Focus
SUBSTANCE USE & MISUSE 1887
groups provide insights into a target audience’s per-
ceptions and motivations (Krueger & Casey, 2015),
and can capture the complexities of attitude and
behavioral intentions (Kitzinger, 1994). Enrollment at
the southwestern university was approximately 37,000
students. The college students were recruited from
general media and communication studies courses via
an online recruitment system, and they received extra
course credit for their participation. This research
study was approved by the southwestern university’s
institutional review board. The key ethical considera-
tions reviewed for this study relate to informed con-
sent, confidentiality, and the right to withdraw.
Participants had to be at least 18 years old to register
for the study and be enrolled at the southwestern uni-
versity. The recruitment procedures, discussion guide,
transcription process, and data analysis were approved
by the IRB. Age and university enrollment were the
only exclusion criterion; participants were not
excluded based on substance use history.
Thirteen group discussions were held in October
2017 with a total of 63 college students (27 men, 36
women; 3–8 per group) who were between 18 and
25 years old. Prior to the focus group discussions, a
trained moderator reviewed the goals of the study,
consent forms, and the right to withdraw with the
participants. One author, who had received training in
conducting focus group discussions, moderated each
semi-structured focus group discussion. The duration
of the focus groups ranged from 45 to 80 min. A dis-
cussion guide was developed to probe participants’ per -
ceptions of the college “party scene” and substance use
on campus. Open-ended questions helped minimize
researchers’ bias and allow participants to respond.
Following open-ended questions, probing questions
focused on participants’ feedback. Sample questions
included: What role does alcohol play in college for
you and your friends? When drinking alcohol, are there
times when drugs are around? In your experience, is it
common for your peers to do drugs? What social pres-
sures do you feel regarding drinking/drug use?
The focus groups were audio-recorded, transcribed
verbatim by an online transcription service, and then
analyzed using a thematic approach (Miles &
Huberman, 1994). Thematic analysis identifies themes
that emerge as being important to the description of a
phenomenon; it is a form of pattern recognition
within the data, while emerging themes become cate-
gories for analysis (Miles & Huberman, 1994). The
theoretical proposition that led this study – social
learning theory, specifically differential associations
and reinforcements – was used to assist in the
development of themes. This research used Miles and
Huberman’s data-analysis procedures. The first author
reviewed all transcripts and derived a set of themes
from the discussions, and the second and third authors
independently reviewed all transcripts and the appro-
priateness of the themes derived by the first author.
Disagreements on themes were resolved through dis-
cussion. The first author then coded all of the tran-
scripts by categorizing relevant statements in the
transcripts under themes (Stappenbeck et al., 2010).
The second and third authors then reviewed the coded
statements, and disagreements on the codes were
resolved. This rigorous analysis by multiple researchers
enhanced the reliability of the themes (Miles &
Huberman, 1994). The number of focus groups needed
was determined by saturation, or the point where no
new themes emerged (Krueger & Casey, 2015).
Findings
The current study used the social learning theory as
the analytical framework for identifying themes.
Focusing on differential associations and differential
reinforcements related to “partying” and substance
use, the following themes emerged: participating in
college culture, experimenting is expected, ignoring
risk-taking, and resisting peer pressure, and are
described below.
Participating in college culture
The majority of participants felt that alcohol and drug
use is part of attending college, and that most college
students engage in this type of behavior as part of
socializing. Five participants reported not drinking
due to various factors including religion, addiction, or
hiatus. Several participants mentioned that there was
“nothing else to do” or that drinking “is just – you
don’t think about it, it’s what you do.” The partici-
pants felt as if “partying” was expected of them since
they were in college, “that’s what college students do.”
I mean in general I feel like when you want to do
something, it’s always like centered around drinking.
Like it’s kind of like the social thing to do. I think it’s
just sort of the college thing. (Female, 19, Group #5)
You feel more inclined to drink since everybody, or a
lot of people, are doing it in this demographic area.
(Male, 21, Group #1)
I just can’t imagine being here without alcohol.
(Female, 22, Group #2)
Some participants even chose to attend this particu-
lar university because of its “party school” reputation.
1888 E. WILLIS ET AL.
Many participants reported binge drinking more while
underage than after they turned age 21 due to several
factors, including being new to alcohol and not know -
ing their limits or lack of access. However, many par-
ticipants reported “having a drink at dinner” and “just
staying home on the weekends and drinking” rather
than frequenting bars after they were legally allowed
to drink.
I mean, I’m not 21 anymore. I don’t go out and get
lit every weekend. Yeah, we might drink at home, but
it’s not like going to the bars. (Female, 22, Group #6)
We will just kick back and chill sometimes at the
house … might have a few people stop by, but it’s
very chill. (Male, 22, Group #10)
All of the participants mentioned football games
and tailgating specifically as times where “drinking is
taken to the next level,” meaning that people are
drinking heavily (some reported “up to 22 drinks on
game day”). “Pre-gaming,” by student definition is
where five to eight drinks are consumed before going
out, was commonly reported. While out at the bar,
participants reported drinking more alcohol and
sometimes using drugs such as cocaine, Molly, or
Xanax. Participants reported keeping a running list of
what types of alcohol mixed best with specific types of
available drugs. While many reported experimenting
with alcohol and drugs prior to college, that experi-
mentation drastically increased while in college.
Yeah, you get here and you see everyone drinking.
Eventually, you start drinking as much as everyone
else. (Female, 21, Group #9)
Participants reported having more opportunities to
binge drink in college, and because alcohol seemed to
be at most social functions, drinking (and often, binge
drinking) was expected behavior.
Experimenting is expected
Participants felt experimentation with combining
drugs and alcohol was normal, and most agreed that
marijuana use was very common among college stu-
dents. All of the focus group participants reported
seeing or using marijuana while at college. Six partici-
pants reported being regular users of marijuana.
I smoke weed every single day. I do everything high
and I have great grades. (Female, 19, Group #8)
Other drugs were reported to be popular among
some crowds or in “bathrooms everywhere in the
bars.” For example, half of participants reported being
offered cocaine at a party.
I was at a party. We’ve had [fraternity] events where
a couple of my friends, they’d just be like, “hey, you
want a bump or something? I’d be like, ‘yeah, why
not’, it’s free, it’s nothing I would pay for because it’s
not worth it to me. (Male, 21, Group #7)
It was just at a gathering and some girl just had some
cocaine and she was like, ‘who wants a free line?’
And I was like, ‘why not?’. (Female, 20, Group #10)
Being in a social environment and having the
opportunity to try the drug facilitated participants’
willingness to act. “It’s like since so many others are
doing it, I just can’t see the harm, I guess.” The
majority of participants reported seeing others mix
drugs and alcohol. The participants who reported
common drug use also reported having prior desires
and positive attitudes toward such risky behavior.
Ten of the participants reported actively using
drugs recreationally, sometimes in combination with
alcohol. For example, many participants reported
using Xanax prior to drinking or cocaine after a long
night “to sober you up.” Half of the participants
reported experimenting with drugs once or twice, but
then after the experience, refraining from future drug
use because “it just isn’t my thing.” The remaining
participants had never used drugs, but felt like it was
okay for others who wanted to experiment.
Ignoring risk-taking
All of the participants discussed “blacking out from
alcohol,” either having personal experience or hearing
first-hand from friends. Participants reported
“blacking out” because they either did not yet know
their limits or as a planned behavior.
Usually I feel like alcohol plays the biggest part in
blacking out. I think people do plan to black out,
because I have so many friends that are like, ‘Oh, my
God, I had four tests this week. I just want to go out
tonight and not remember anything’. They completely
black out. I’ve blacked out before. Personally, I don’t
really like it because I don’t like not remembering
anything, because I feel like I did something stupid
and you just don’t remember it … (Female, 21,
Group #5)
In terms of drug use, one participant noted that
‘everyone is medicated’ and that ‘prescription pills are
everywhere’. Many reported the use of prescription
pills specifically. For example, some reported friends
‘stockpiling’ prescription Xanax to use recreationally,
or purchasing Adderall from friends with prescrip-
tions to use while studying or ‘partying’. Participants
reported prescription drugs as being ‘very accessible
around here’.
SUBSTANCE USE & MISUSE 1889
Most of the participants reported not associating
marijuana with any risk, ‘probably because it’s legal in
most states’. Other drugs were thought of as being
dangerous, including cocaine and prescription medica-
tions such as Adderall, Ambien, and Xanax, when
taken with alcohol. Additionally, when asked to rank
substances by most dangerous to least dangerous,
most participants agreed that alcohol was the most
dangerous drug, followed by prescription pills
and cocaine.
I probably would just say that alcohol is one of the
worst because you don’t see it as a drug, but it really
is because people get addicted to it every day.
(Female, 24, Group #11)
I’d say I think alcohol is worse [than prescription
drugs]. So I would really say … of course the
cocaine, most definitely, and then alcohol after that,
you know what I’m saying? Not because of the long-
term effects or whatever. You got to drink a whole lot
to actually mess your kidneys, but just the fact that
people get messed up on it, drive and stuff like that.
(Male, 19, Group #2)
The majority of participants reported engaging in
binge drinking and/or drug use, but did not discuss
the risks with friends due to several factors men-
tioned, including “we learned about that in high
school” and “that’s her life, whatever.” Participants
reported drug users as not wanting to think about the
risks, or “they’re not going to be proactive with it.
You’ve got to ask them.” Five participants reported
knowing friends who passed away from drug overdose
but still reported engaging in drinking and drug use,
just “in moderation.” In addition, most agreed that
they would not warn strangers of the risks of sub-
stance use. Participants felt as if strangers would not
heed their warning, and, therefore, agreed that they
would not try to dissuade a stranger from using drugs
in combination with alcohol, despite the obvious risks.
Resisting peer pressure
The participants agreed that they felt social pressure
to drink but not to do drugs.
I’ve never been pressured to take any drugs. I’ve been
asked, but they weren’t really as persistent as with
alcohol, because I think they know that drugs is a
little more than just alcohol. (Female, 20, Group #12)
No one’s like, ‘Shove this [drink] down or you’re not
part of the [name of school] family!’ I don’t think it’s
like that, it’s just we all do it [binge drink]. (Female,
19, Group #4)
There’s no pressure to do drugs … it’s just, it’s just
if you want it. (Male, 18, Group #10)
If participants were offered drugs and they did not
want to partake, many felt as if it would be easy to
decline the offer. Additionally, participants noted that
drugs were common among ‘certain crowds’ and that
peer pressure was about choice.
I think there’s always going to be that social pressure.
The kind of friend they’re going to be like, ‘Oh,
smoke this, smoke this, smoke this’. But it’s really like
at the end of the day, it’s your own decision to make.
(Male, 23, Group #7)
I feel that it’s like a lot who you surround yourself
with and like who your friends are, and like who you
choose to be around and stuff. (Female, 20,
Group #11)
Although it would seem drugs are commonly
offered with no pressure, alcohol carries a greater
social influence, “I mean, it’s hard because everyone is
doing it.” The majority of participants reported drink-
ing frequently, and when opting not to drink – did
not mention being pressured by their peers. Most
reported that their friends did not pressure them to
do anything they didn’t already want to do.
Well, I guess you are on Snapchat and you see all
your friends out, you’re like, oh, crap. I want to go
out drinking with them. I don’t want to be stuck at
home. (Female, 19, Group #8)
Participants reported fearing missing out on the
social scene if they were not out drinking with their
friends. Specifically, seeing friends having fun on
social media platforms created a desire for participants
to engage in the same activities. However, educational
duties often dictated when participants engaged in
heavy drinking, and most agreed that “drinking
shouldn’t get in the way of why I’m here.”
For those participants who did not drink at the
time of the focus groups, all reported not having any
difficulty resisting peer pressure in social settings
whether related to drugs and alcohol. After “pre-
gaming,” many participants reported drinking two to
four drinks while out at the bar unless “someone buys
me a drink, and yeah, I’ll drink it.” One participant
said: “I’ve never seen anyone be pressured to start
drinking, but I have seen people pressured to
keep drinking.”
Discussion
This study had two goals: (1) to understand how both
social influence and peer behavior influences attitudes
toward various substance use behaviors, and (2) to
understand how perceptions of rewarding outcomes
and risky consequences interact with these attitudes to
1890 E. WILLIS ET AL.
influence planned behavior. Focus groups were used
to discuss binge drinking and substance use with col-
lege students to understand their perceptions of these
risky behaviors; specifically, the differential associa-
tions and differential reinforcements that students
perceive of these behaviors was probed with direct
questioning. Although previous research has demon-
strated that experimentation with drugs and alcohol is
common and increasing among college students
(Behavioral Health Coordinating Committee:
Prescription Drug Abuse Subcommittee, Department
of Health & Human Services, 2013), this study
revealed how perceptions of peer norms and the bal -
ancing of risk with reward influence behavior.
Four themes were identified within the data: partic-
ipating in college culture, experimenting is expected,
ignoring risk-taking, and resisting peer pressure.
Students believed they were expected to experiment
with alcohol and drugs, and that sometimes included
use. In every focus group, it was said at least once,
“everybody’s doing it” in regard to drinking. Students
also noted that their university had a “party school”
reputation and that was attractive during recruitment.
Marijuana was considered “normal” amongst stu-
dents because it was often reported as “always being
around.” Interestingly, however, students reported
feeling pressure to partake in alcohol but not drugs.
Both illicit and prescription drugs were common and
available to students, and some reported accepting
while others declined. Regardless of the type, drug use
was highest in conjunction with alcohol. Although
students reported knowing the risks of combining
alcohol with prescription medications and illicit drugs,
these risks did not outweigh the perceived rewards.
Few students reported avoiding discussing the risks
with their friends because “it’s their life, you know?.”
Theoretical implications
Theoretically, the findings from this study offer
insight into the differential associations and reinforce-
ments among college students related to alcohol and
drug use. About half of the participants reported
abstaining from drug use, while only five reported not
drinking alcohol. The reported variance in drinking
behavior was evidence of different peer groups at this
university and how these associations influence the
perception of norms. Several students reported seeing
“hard drugs” at parties and making the decision to
leave because they did not feel comfortable around
illicit drug use. However, when it came to drinking,
excessive consumption was permitted because “that’s
what everyone else is doing.” Students reported “pre-
gaming” as a common behavior prior to drinking
more while out at a bar where drug use might also
occur. Many students perceive alcohol and drug use
as part of the college experience and thus, they per-
ceive this to be the norm. Because of their desire to
be part of the “in-group,” they report more reward
than risk due to their perception that “everybody’s
doing it.” Additionally, participants also mentioned
seeing “partying” on social media applications –
sometimes from peers at their same university, some-
times from peers at different universities – contribu-
ting to students’ perceptions that indeed, “everybody’s
doing it.”
In terms of differential reinforcement, students
reported learning their limits “the hard way,” for
example, drinking until sick, “blacking out,” and then
adjusting their behavior as to avoid negative conse-
quences in the future. Most of the students reported
socialization and “having fun” as benefits to engaging
in risk-taking behaviors, but they also acknowledge
the negative consequences, for example, hangovers,
sexual assault. Akers notes that differential reinforce-
ment is the “balance” of rewards and consequences of
engaging in a particular behavior (Akers, 2000; Maggs
et al., 2011). It is clear that over time, students find a
balance in the rewards and consequences of particu-
larly risky behavior that is common to their perceived
college experience. Interestingly, students’ evaluations
of their risk-taking behaviors are determined by their
performance in classes. If drinking or drug use was
interfering with attendance or their personal (or par-
ental) definition of success, then it was perceived as a
problem. However, health issues were not cited as a
reason for the cessation of the risky behavior.
Public health implications
The findings from this study inform public health
interventions targeted at high school and college stu-
dents specifically. Although the current study focused
on college students, many said they used drugs and/or
alcohol prior to attending college. Therefore, predis-
positions to alcohol and drugs start prior to college,
as does students’ perception of college culture; thus
attempts to prevent or cessate must start earlier and
grow as college progresses. More health education
should be targeted to this age group to help them
form an understanding of the risks of substance use,
especially mixing alcohol and drugs. Realistic, targeted
health messaging could help set this population’s
expectations of college “partying” and demonstrate the
SUBSTANCE USE & MISUSE 1891
consequences of risky behaviors. Findings from this
study might be used by college university’s health cen-
ters to design messages that highlight the consequen-
ces of substance abuse. Additionally, many universities
require students to complete alcohol education prior
to their first year on campus. Often, university educa-
tion includes bystander training which focuses mainly
on sexual misconduct, but perhaps should also include
lessons for students on substance use and discourag-
ing peers (not just friends!) from mixing drugs and
alcohol. Our research offers insight into substance
abuse topics that is informed by students and should
be considered when designing health messages to this
audience. This research demonstrates that students are
likely ignoring the negative consequences to risky
behaviors in order to be accepted by their peer group
and to achieve the perceived benefits of alcohol and
drug use. Future public health messaging related to
the use of drugs and alcohol concurrently should con-
centrate on accentuating the rewards of avoiding such
behavior rather than simply focusing on the risks as
these risks seem to be easily dismissed by this
age group.
Limitations
This study used a qualitative approach; focus groups
cannot be generalized to a general population of col-
lege students. The lack of independent coding by
more than one author is a limitation. In addition, the
students in the sample are from one university and,
thus, future work should include other campuses
across the country. Additionally, the university’s IRB
limited the demographic and behavioral information
that could be collected from students. Future work
should attempt to probe the relationships between
these differential associations and reinforcements
through quantitative measures such as large-scale sur-
veys or experimental designs intended to present mes-
sages that might appeal to one or both of the social
learning processes.
Taken together, the findings of this study suggest
that poly-substance use on college campuses is, at
least in part, driven by a perception of college culture
and a poor balancing of the risks and rewards associ -
ated with these behaviors.
Declaration of interest
The authors declare that they have no conflict of interest.
The authors alone are responsible for the content and writ-
ing of the article.
ORCID
Erin Willis http://orcid.org/0000-0002-1582-0867
Justin Keene http://orcid.org/0000-0002-1404-0025
References
Akers, R. L. (2000). Criminological theories: Introduction,
evaluation, and application (pp. 17–21). Los Angeles, CA:
Roxbury.
Akers, R. L. (2011). Social learning and social structure: A
general theory of crime and deviance (pp. 34–38).
Piscataway, NJ: Rutgers.
Armeli, S., Conner, T. S., Cullum, J., & Tennen, H. (2010).
A longitudinal analysis of drinking motives moderating
the negative affect-drinking association among college
students. Psychology of Addictive Behaviors, 24(1), 38–47.
doi:10.1037/a0017530
Bandura, A. (1977). Self-efficacy: Toward a unifying theory
of behavioral change. Psychology Review, 84(2), 191–215.
doi:10.1037/0033-295X.84.2.191
Behavioral Health Coordinating Committee: Prescription
Drug Abuse Subcommittee, Department of Health and
Human Services. (2013). Addressing prescription drug
abuse in the United States: Current activities and future
opportunities. Retrieved Jan 8, 2018 from http://www.cdc.
gov/HomeandRecreationalSafety/pdf/HHS_Prescription_
Drug_Abuse_Report_09.2013.pdf.
Brooks-Russel, A., Simons-Morton, B., Haynie, D., Farhat,
T., & Wang, J. (2014). Longitudinal relationship between
drinking with peers, descriptive norms, and adolescent
alcohol use. Prevention Science, 15, 497–505. doi:10.1007/
s11121-013-0391-9
Carey, K. B., Scott-Sheldon, L. A. J., Garey, L., Elliott, J. C.,
& Carey, M. P. (2016). Alcohol interventions for man-
dated college students: A meta-analytic review. Journal of
Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 84(7), 619–632. doi:
10.1037/a0040275
Center for Behavioral Health Statistics and Quality. (2015).
Results from the 2014 national survey on drug use and
health: Detailed tables (pp. 6–12). Rockville, MD:
Substance Abuse and Mental Health Services
Administration.
Centers for Disease Control and Prevention. (2017). Fact
sheets – Binge drinking. Retrieved Dec 15, 2017 from
https://www.cdc.gov/alcohol/fact-sheets/binge-drinking.htm.
Crawford, L. A., & Novak, K. B. (2010). Beliefs about alco-
hol and the college experience as moderators of the
effects of perceived drinking norms on student alcohol
use. Journal of Alcohol and Drug Education, 54, 69–86.
Croom, K., Lewis, D., Marchell, T., Lesser, M. L., Reyna,
V. F., Kubicki-Bedford, L., … Staiano-Coico, L. (2009).
Impact of an online alcohol education course on behavior
and harm for incoming first-year college students: Short-
term evaluation of a randomized trial. Journal of
American College Health, 57(4), 445–454. doi:10.3200/
JACH.57.4.445-454
Durkin, K. F., Wolfe, T. W., & Clark, G. A. (2005). College
students and binge drinking: An evaluation of the social
learning theory. Sociological Spectrum, 25(3), 255–272.
doi:10.1080/027321790518681
1892 E. WILLIS ET AL.
https://doi.org/10.1037/a0017530
https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-295X.84.2.191
http://www.cdc.gov/HomeandRecreationalSafety/pdf/HHS_Pres
cription_Drug_Abuse_Report_09.2013.pdf
http://www.cdc.gov/HomeandRecreationalSafety/pdf/HHS_Pres
cription_Drug_Abuse_Report_09.2013.pdf
http://www.cdc.gov/HomeandRecreationalSafety/pdf/HHS_Pres
cription_Drug_Abuse_Report_09.2013.pdf
https://doi.org/10.1007/s11121-013-0391-9
https://doi.org/10.1007/s11121-013-0391-9
https://doi.org/10.1037/a0040275
https://www.cdc.gov/alcohol/fact-sheets/binge-drinking.htm
https://doi.org/10.3200/JACH.57.4.445-454
https://doi.org/10.3200/JACH.57.4.445-454
https://doi.org/10.1080/027321790518681
Ford, J. A., & Arrastia, M. C. (2008). Pill-poppers and dop-
ers: A comparison of non-medical prescription drug use
and illicit/street drug use among college students.
Addictive Behaviors, 33(7), 934–941. doi:10.1016/
j.addbeh.2008.02.016
Fournier, A. K., Hall, E., Ricke, P., & Storey, B. (2013).
Alcohol and the social network: Online social networking
sites and college students’ perceived drinking norms.
Psychology of Popular Media Culture, 2(2), 86–95. doi:
10.1037/a0032097
Garnier-Dykstra, L. M., Caldeira, K. M., Vincent, K. B.,
O’Grady, K. E., & Arria, A. M. (2012). Nonmedical use
of prescription stimulants during college: Four-year
trends in exposure opportunity, use, motives, and sour-
ces. Journal of American College Health, 60(3), 226–236.
doi:10.1080/07448481.2011.589876
Hahm, H. C., Kolaczyk, E., Jang, J., Swenson, T., &
Bhindarwala, A. M. (2012). Binge drinking trajectories
from adolescence to young adulthood: The effects of peer
social network. Substance Use & Misuse, 47, 745–756.
doi:10.3109/10826084.2012.666313
Kitzinger, J. (1994). The methodology of focus groups: The
importance of interaction between research participants.
Sociology of Health and Illness, 16, 103–121. doi:10.1111/
1467-9566.ep11347023
Krueger, R. A., & Casey, M. A. (2015). Focus groups: A
practical guide for applied research (5th ed.; pp. 18–23).
Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage Publications, Inc.
Maggs, J. L., Williams, L. R., & Lee, C. M. (2011). Ups and
downs of alcohol use among first-year college students:
Numbers of drinks, heavy drinking, and stumble and
pass out drinking days. Addictive Behaviors, 36(3),
197–202. doi:10.1016/j.addbeh.2010.10.005
Martinez, H. S., Klanecky, A. K., & McChargue, D. E.
(2018). Problem drinking among at-risk college students:
The examination of Greek involvement, freshmen status,
and history of mental health problems. Journal of
American College Health. doi:10.1080/07448481.2018.
1432625
McCabe, S. E. (2008). Misperceptions of non-medical pre-
scription drug use: A web survey of college students.
Addictive Behaviors, 33(5), 713–724. doi:10.1016/
j.addbeh.2007.12.008
Miles, M. B., & Huberman, A. M. (1994). Qualitative data
analysis: An expanded sourcebook (pp. 98–101).
Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage Publications, Inc.
National Institute on Drug Abuse. (2017). Medical conse-
quences of drug abuse. Retreived Dec 6, 2017 from
http://www.drugabuse.gov/related-topics/medical-conse-
quences-drug-abuse.
O’Connor, R. M., & Colder, C. R. (2005). Predicting alcohol
patterns in first-year college students through motiv-
ational systems and reasons for drinking. Psychology of
Addictive Behaviors, 19, 10–20. doi:10.1037/0893-
164X.19.1.10
O’Hara, R. E., Armeli, S., & Tennen, H. (2015). College stu-
dents’ drinking motives and social-contextual factors:
Comparing associations across levels of analysis.
Psychology of Addictive Behaviors, 29, 420–429. doi:
10.1037/adb0000046
Peralta, R. L., & Steele, J. L. (2010). Nonmedical prescrip-
tion drug use among US college students at a midwest
university: A partial test of social learning theory.
Substance Use & Misuse, 45, 865–887. doi:10.3109/
10826080903443610
Read, J. P., Wood, M. D., Kahler, C. W., Maddock, J. E., &
Palfai, T. P. (2003). Examining the role of drinking
motives in college student alcohol use and problems.
Psychology of Addictive Behaviors, 17(1), 13–23. doi:
10.1037/0893-164X.17.1.13
Schultz, P. W., Nolan, J. M., Cialdini, R. B., Goldstein, N. J.,
& Griskevicius, V. (2007). The constructive, destructive,
and reconstructive power of social norms. Psychological
Science, 18(5), 429–434. doi:10.1111/j.1467-
9280.2007.01917.x
Sher, K. J., Bartholow, B. D., & Nanda, S. (2001). Short-
and long-term effects of fraternity and sorority member-
ship on heavy drinking: A social norms perspective.
Psychology of Addictive Behaviors, 15(1), 42–51. doi:
10.1037//0893-164X.15.1.42
Stappenbeck, C. A., Quinn, P. D., Wetherill, R. R., &
Fromme, K. (2010). Perceived norms for drinking in the
transition from high school to college and beyond.
Journal of Studies on Alcohol and Drugs, 71(6), 895–903.
Substance Abuse and Mental Health Services
Administration. (2006). A comprehensive plan for pre-
venting and reducing underage drinking. Retrieved Dec
29, 2017 from: http://www.stopalcoholabuse.gov/media/
pdf/underagerpttocongress.pdf.
Tanner-Smith, E. E., & Lipsey, M. W. (2015). Brief alcohol
interventions for adolescents and young adults: A system-
atic review and meta-analysis. Journal of Substance Abuse
Treatment, 51, 1–18. doi:10.1016/j.jsat.2014.09.001
Tyler, K. A., Schmitz, R. M., Ray, C. M., Adams, S. A., &
Gordon Simons, L. (2017). The role of protective behav-
ioral strategies, social environment, and housing type on
heavy drinking among college students. Substance Use
and Misuse. doi:10.1080/10826084.2017.1363235
Wechsler, H., Lee, J. E., Kuo, M., & Lee, H. (2000). College
binge drinking in the 1990s: A continuing problem
results of the Harvard School of Public Health 1999
College Alcohol Study. Journal of American College
Health, 48(5), 199–210. doi:10.1080/07448480009599305
Wechsler, H., Nelson, T. E., Lee, J. E., Seibring, M., Lewis,
C., & Keeling, R. P. (2003). Perception and reality: A
national evaluation of social norms and marketing inter-
ventions to reduce college students’ heavy alcohol use.
Journal of Studies on Alcohol, 64(4), 484–494. doi:
10.15288/jsa.2003.64.484
Wesley Perkins, H., Haines, M. P., & Rice, R. (2005).
Misperceiving the college drinking norm and related
problems: A nationwide study of exposure to prevention
information, perceived norms and student alcohol mis-
use. Journal of Studies on Alcohol, 66(4), 470–478. doi:
10.15288/jsa.2005.66.470
White, A., & Hingson, R. (2013). The burden of alcohol
use: Excessive alcohol consumption and related conse-
quences among college students. Alcohol Research:
Current Reviews, 35, 201–218.
SUBSTANCE USE & MISUSE 1893
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.addbeh.2008.02.016
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.addbeh.2008.02.016
https://doi.org/10.1037/a0032097
https://doi.org/10.1080/07448481.2011.589876
https://doi.org/10.3109/10826084.2012.666313
https://doi.org/10.1111/1467-9566.ep11347023
https://doi.org/10.1111/1467-9566.ep11347023
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.addbeh.2010.10.005
https://doi.org/10.1080/07448481.2018.1432625
https://doi.org/10.1080/07448481.2018.1432625
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.addbeh.2007.12.008
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.addbeh.2007.12.008
http://www.drugabuse.gov/related-topics/medical-consequences-
drug-abuse
http://www.drugabuse.gov/related-topics/medical-consequences-
drug-abuse
https://doi.org/10.1037/0893-164X.19.1.10
https://doi.org/10.1037/0893-164X.19.1.10
https://doi.org/10.1037/adb0000046
https://doi.org/10.3109/10826080903443610
https://doi.org/10.3109/10826080903443610
https://doi.org/10.1037/0893-164X.17.1.13
https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9280.2007.01917.x
https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9280.2007.01917.x
https://doi.org/10.1037//0893-164X.15.1.42
http://www.stopalcoholabuse.gov/media/pdf/underagerpttocongr
ess.pdf
http://www.stopalcoholabuse.gov/media/pdf/underagerpttocongr
ess.pdf
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jsat.2014.09.001
https://doi.org/10.1080/10826084.2017.1363235
https://doi.org/10.1080/07448480009599305
https://doi.org/10.15288/jsa.2003.64.484
https://doi.org/10.15288/jsa.2005.66.470
Copyright of Substance Use & Misuse is the property of Taylor
& Francis Ltd and its content
may not be copied or emailed to multiple sites or posted to a
listserv without the copyright
holder's express written permission. However, users may print,
download, or email articles for
individual use.
AbstractSocial learning theoryDifferential association and
reinforcementMethodFindingsParticipating in college
cultureExperimenting is expectedIgnoring risk-takingResisting
peer pressureDiscussionTheoretical implicationsPublic health
implicationsLimitationsDeclaration of interestReferences
The Rise and Fall of Behaviorism:
The Narrative and the Numbers
Michiel Braat, Jan Engelen, Ties van Gemert, and Sander
Verhaegh
Tilburg University
The history of 20th-century American psychology is often
depicted as a history of the rise
and fall of behaviorism. Although historians disagree about the
theoretical and social factors
that have contributed to the development of experimental
psychology, there is widespread
consensus about the growing and (later) declining influence of
behaviorism between
approximately 1920 and 1970. Because such wide-scope claims
about the development of
American psychology are typically based on small and
unrepresentative samples of histor-
ical data, however, the question arises to what extent the
received view is justified. This
article aims to answer this question in two ways. First, we use
advanced scientometric tools
(e.g., bibliometric mapping, cocitation analysis, and term co-
occurrence analysis) to quan-
titatively analyze the metadata of 119,278 articles published in
American journals between
1920 and 1970. We reconstruct the development and structure of
American psychology
using cocitation and co-occurrence networks and argue that the
standard story needs
reappraising. Second, we argue that the question whether
behaviorism was the “dominant”
school of American psychology is historically misleading to
begin with. Using the results of
our bibliometric analyses, we argue that questions about the
development of American
psychology deserve more fine-grained answers.
Keywords: behaviorism, American psychology, bibliometric
mapping, cocitation
analysis, co-occurrence analysis
The history of 20th-century American psychology is often
depicted as a history of the rise and
fall of behaviorism, the view that psychology should become “a
purely objective experimental
branch of natural science” (Watson, 1913, p. 248). Although
early 20th-century psychologists
aimed to redefine their discipline as a science of behavior, the
popularity of behaviorism
declined from the late 1950s onward, when psychologis ts,
linguists, and computer scientists
joined forces and developed empirical approaches to the study
of mind and cognition.
This article was published Online First March 19, 2020.
Michiel Braat, Tilburg School of Humanities and Digital
Sciences, Tilburg University; Jan Engelen, Depart-
ment of Communication and Cognition, Tilburg School of
Humanities and Digital Sciences, Tilburg University;
Ties van Gemert, Tilburg School of Humanities and Digital
Sciences, Tilburg University; Sander Verhaegh,
Tilburg Center for Logic, Ethics, and Philosophy of Science,
Tilburg School of Humanities and Digital Sciences,
Tilburg University.
All authors contributed equally to this work. This research is
funded by the Tilburg School of Humanities and
Digital Sciences Research Traineeships Programme. In addition,
Verhaegh’s research is funded by The
Netherlands Organization for Scientific Research (grant 275–20
– 064). We would like to thank Nees Jan van
Eck, members of the History of Behavior Analysis mailing list
(especially Nicole L. Banks and François
Tonneau) and audiences at conferences at the University of
Amsterdam, the CEU Institute for Advanced Study
Budapest, and the Tilburg School of Humanities and Digital
Sciences for their valuable comments and feedback.
Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to
Jan Engelen or Sander Verhaegh, Tilburg
School of Humanities and Digital Sciences, Tilburg University,
Warandelaan 2, 5037 AB, Tilburg, the
Netherlands. E-mail: [email protected] or [email protected]
T
hi
s
do
cu
m
en
t
is
co
py
ri
gh
te
d
by
th
e
A
m
er
ic
an
P
sy
ch
ol
og
ic
al
A
ss
oc
ia
ti
on
or
on
e
of
it
s
al
li
ed
pu
bl
is
he
rs
.
T
hi
s
ar
ti
cl
e
is
in
te
nd
ed
so
le
ly
fo
r
th
e
pe
rs
on
al
us
e
of
th
e
in
di
vi
du
al
us
er
an
d
is
no
t
to
be
di
ss
em
in
at
ed
br
oa
dl
y.
History of Psychology
© 2020 American Psychological Association 2020, Vol. 23, No.
3, 252–280
ISSN: 1093-4510 http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/hop0000146
252
mailto:[email protected]
mailto:[email protected]
http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/hop0000146
Although historians of science disagree about the theoretical
and social factors that have
contributed to the development of experimental psychology
(Cohen-Cole, 2014; Greenwood,
2009; Leahey, 2001), there is a widespread consensus about the
growing and declining
influence of behaviorism in 20th-century American psychology
(DeGrandpré & Buskist,
2000; O’Donohue & Kitchener, 1999; Staddon, 1999; Zuriff,
1985).
From a methodological perspective, these claims are
contentious, because wide-scope
claims about the development of American psychology are
rarely backed up by representative
empirical data. Although nobody will deny that many of today’s
best-known behaviorists
produced their most influential work between 1920 and 1960, it
is unclear what proportion of
American psychologists embraced a behavioristic conception of
psychology both during and
after the heyday of behaviorism. Similar worries can be raised
about the historians’ conclu-
sions about developments internal to behaviorism. Although
most scholars identify Watson,
Hull, Tolman, and Skinner as the most influential (neo-
)behaviorists,1 these claims are seldom
supported by balanced empirical evidence. In general, it is
unclear to what extent the list of
psychologists who have been most extensively studied by
historians of behaviorism forms a
faithful representation of the scholars that actually influenced
the development of behaviorism
in the first half of the 20th century.
The problem that wide-scope claims about the history of
American psychology are rarely
backed up by balanced empirical data is especially pressing
because it seems likely that
historians overestimate the influence of canonized schools and
scholars (whether it is behav-
iorism in the 1930s or cognitive psychology in the 1970s). Not
only do historians tend to
ignore psychologists who did not belong to any school (Green,
Feinerer, & Burman, 2013); in
studying the histories of influential schools and scholars,
historians also tend to limit them-
selves to the writings “of a few known authors that are
transmitted from generation to
generation [. . .] as the authors to read” (Betti, van den Berg,
Oortwijn, & Treijtel, 2019,
p. 296). More particularly, historical claims about the
development of American psychology
are often based on corpora that are (a) not clearly specified
(which texts exactly did the
historian study in arriving at their conclusions?), (b) very small
(how many of the tens of
thousands of texts produced by psychologists did the historian
actually read?), and (c) not
representative (how can the historian guarantee that the texts he
or she studied form a
representative sample of the total body of texts produced by
psychologists?). Although
in-depth text analyses and archive studies can provide historians
with a first indication of the
influence of a psychologist, a method, or a school of
psychology, their conclusions will always
be shaped by the texts and archives they choose to study.
In this article, we aim to transcend the received view about the
development of 20th-century
American psychology in two ways. First, we use advanced text
analysis tools (e.g., biblio-
metric mapping, cocitation analysis, and co-occurrence
analysis) to quantitatively analyze the
metadata of 119,278 articles published in American psychology
journals between 1920 and
1970. We analyze both the citations and the title terms of these
articles and generate cocitation
and co-occurrence networks for every consecutive decade
between 1920 and 1970 to recon-
struct the structure and development of mid-20th-century
American psychology and the
structure and development of behaviorism. Second, we argue
that the question whether
behaviorism was the “dominant” school of American
psychology is historically misleading to
begin with. Using the results of our bibliometric analyses, we
argue that questions about the
development of American psychology deserve more fine-grained
answers.
This article is organized as follows. After a brief outline of
what might be called the received
view about the history of behaviorism, we explain the main
methodological challenges
1
See, for example, Boakes (1984, p. 237), Smith (1986, pp. 21–
22) and Mills (2012, pp. 104 –108). Most
historians paint a picture of the development of behaviorism in
which first Watson and later Hull and Tolman
offered the “dominant orientation in American departments of
psychology until after the end of the Second War,
when it was displaced by [Skinner’s] radical behaviorism”
(Greenwood, 2009, p. 477).
T
hi
s
do
cu
m
en
t
is
co
py
ri
gh
te
d
by
th
e
A
m
er
ic
an
P
sy
ch
ol
og
ic
al
A
ss
oc
ia
ti
on
or
on
e
of
it
s
al
li
ed
pu
bl
is
he
rs
.
T
hi
s
ar
ti
cl
e
is
in
te
nd
ed
so
le
ly
fo
r
th
e
pe
rs
on
al
us
e
of
th
e
in
di
vi
du
al
us
er
an
d
is
no
t
to
be
di
ss
em
in
at
ed
br
oa
dl
y.
253RISE AND FALL OF BEHAVIORISM
surrounding quantitative representations of the development of
American psychology. Next,
we describe our data and methods and provide an overview of
our findings about the structure
and development of American psychology between 1920 and
1970. Finally, we discuss the
implications of our findings and argue that the standard story
about the development of
American psychology needs reappraising.
Behaviorism: The Narrative
Behaviorism is a complex of methodological, epistemological,
and sometimes ontological
assumptions about the foundations of psychology. Where
psychology is traditionally defined
as the study of mental phenomena, behaviorists typically argue
that psychology should
become a science of behavior. More specifically, most
behaviorists agree that (a) psychology
is or should become a branch of natural science, (b)
psychologists should study behavior
instead of mental phenomena, and (c) a science of behavior
should be built exclusively on
publicly available evidence (thereby dismissing the use of
introspection in psychological
research). Usually, behaviorists combine this view about the
nature of psychology with a set
of empirical assumptions—for instance, the assumption that the
behavior of an organism is
determined by the organism’s reinforcement history. Outside
these shared philosophical and
empirical commitments, behaviorists also strongly disagree
about a wide range of issues. Most
importantly, they disagree about the domain for psychology
(should a study of behavior
include or exclude physiological variables?), the nature of the
observation language (should
behaviorists tolerate intensional descriptions or purposive
language?), and the types of theo-
retical concepts allowed in the construction of behaviorist
theories (are intervening variables
or hypothetical constructs acceptable?).
Historians often distinguish between two types of behaviorism
in psychology: meth-
odological and radical behaviorism (Day, 1983; Mills, 2012;
Moore, 1981).2 Method-
ological behaviorists view (a)–(c) as a set of methodological
prescriptions; they do not
believe that psychologists should say something about the
ontological status of mental
states. According to the methodological view, psychologists
should aim to scientifically
describe and explain behavior without referring to mental states,
images, or processes.
Radical behaviorists, on the other hand, deny that mental
entities exist and argue that
private events should be included in the analysis of behavior —
that is, that private events
should be analyzed in terms of the same principles that have
been used to study overt
behavior (Day, 1983; Ringen, 1999; Skinner, 1974).
Although behaviorism is generally viewed as a distinctively
American psychology,
most historians recognize that its roots can be traced back to the
work of the Russian
objective psychologists or reflexologists (Boakes, 1984; Fuchs
& Milar, 2003; Hergen-
hahn, 2005).3 Ivan Sechenov, often credited as the founder of
this school, worked on the
(excitatory and inhibitory) mediational role of the cerebral
cortex in reflex actions and
extrapolated his findings to the concepts of psychology. In
Reflexes of the Brain, Sechenov
(1863/1965) defended the view that every mental process is
reducible to a physiological
reflex: “only physiology holds the key to the scientific analysis
of psychical phenomena”
(p. 351). Ivan Pavlov and Vladimir Bekhterev extended
Sechenov’s work by indepen-
dently discovering the principles of classical conditioning.
Indeed, Sechenov’s call for an
objective psychology seems to have played an important role in
Pavlov’s conclusion that
2
We include the qualification “in psychology” because the
present overview excludes the role behaviorist
theories played outside psychology (e.g. philosophy, economics,
and sociology). See, for example, Pooley and
Solovey (2010) and Hauser (2015).
3
In addition, historians generally recognize that behaviorism also
affected the development of psychology
outside the United States. See, for example, Ardilla (2009).
T
hi
s
do
cu
m
en
t
is
co
py
ri
gh
te
d
by
th
e
A
m
er
ic
an
P
sy
ch
ol
og
ic
al
A
ss
oc
ia
ti
on
or
on
e
of
it
s
al
li
ed
pu
bl
is
he
rs
.
T
hi
s
ar
ti
cl
e
is
in
te
nd
ed
so
le
ly
fo
r
th
e
pe
rs
on
al
us
e
of
th
e
in
di
vi
du
al
us
er
an
d
is
no
t
to
be
di
ss
em
in
at
ed
br
oa
dl
y.
254 BRAAT, ENGELEN, VAN GEMERT, AND VERHAEGH
his experimental results (dogs salivate in response to food
stimuli at a distance) could be
Description of Dataset for HW1 Each month the Bureau of
Description of Dataset for HW1 Each month the Bureau of
Description of Dataset for HW1 Each month the Bureau of
Description of Dataset for HW1 Each month the Bureau of
Description of Dataset for HW1 Each month the Bureau of
Description of Dataset for HW1 Each month the Bureau of
Description of Dataset for HW1 Each month the Bureau of
Description of Dataset for HW1 Each month the Bureau of
Description of Dataset for HW1 Each month the Bureau of
Description of Dataset for HW1 Each month the Bureau of
Description of Dataset for HW1 Each month the Bureau of
Description of Dataset for HW1 Each month the Bureau of
Description of Dataset for HW1 Each month the Bureau of
Description of Dataset for HW1 Each month the Bureau of
Description of Dataset for HW1 Each month the Bureau of
Description of Dataset for HW1 Each month the Bureau of
Description of Dataset for HW1 Each month the Bureau of
Description of Dataset for HW1 Each month the Bureau of
Description of Dataset for HW1 Each month the Bureau of
Description of Dataset for HW1 Each month the Bureau of
Description of Dataset for HW1 Each month the Bureau of
Description of Dataset for HW1 Each month the Bureau of
Description of Dataset for HW1 Each month the Bureau of
Description of Dataset for HW1 Each month the Bureau of
Description of Dataset for HW1 Each month the Bureau of
Description of Dataset for HW1 Each month the Bureau of
Description of Dataset for HW1 Each month the Bureau of
Description of Dataset for HW1 Each month the Bureau of
Description of Dataset for HW1 Each month the Bureau of
Description of Dataset for HW1 Each month the Bureau of
Description of Dataset for HW1 Each month the Bureau of
Description of Dataset for HW1 Each month the Bureau of
Description of Dataset for HW1 Each month the Bureau of
Description of Dataset for HW1 Each month the Bureau of
Description of Dataset for HW1 Each month the Bureau of
Description of Dataset for HW1 Each month the Bureau of
Description of Dataset for HW1 Each month the Bureau of
Description of Dataset for HW1 Each month the Bureau of
Description of Dataset for HW1 Each month the Bureau of
Description of Dataset for HW1 Each month the Bureau of
Description of Dataset for HW1 Each month the Bureau of
Description of Dataset for HW1 Each month the Bureau of
Description of Dataset for HW1 Each month the Bureau of
Description of Dataset for HW1 Each month the Bureau of
Description of Dataset for HW1 Each month the Bureau of
Description of Dataset for HW1 Each month the Bureau of
Description of Dataset for HW1 Each month the Bureau of
Description of Dataset for HW1 Each month the Bureau of
Description of Dataset for HW1 Each month the Bureau of
Description of Dataset for HW1 Each month the Bureau of
Description of Dataset for HW1 Each month the Bureau of
Description of Dataset for HW1 Each month the Bureau of
Description of Dataset for HW1 Each month the Bureau of
Description of Dataset for HW1 Each month the Bureau of
Description of Dataset for HW1 Each month the Bureau of
Description of Dataset for HW1 Each month the Bureau of
Description of Dataset for HW1 Each month the Bureau of
Description of Dataset for HW1 Each month the Bureau of
Description of Dataset for HW1 Each month the Bureau of
Description of Dataset for HW1 Each month the Bureau of
Description of Dataset for HW1 Each month the Bureau of
Description of Dataset for HW1 Each month the Bureau of
Description of Dataset for HW1 Each month the Bureau of
Description of Dataset for HW1 Each month the Bureau of
Description of Dataset for HW1 Each month the Bureau of
Description of Dataset for HW1 Each month the Bureau of
Description of Dataset for HW1 Each month the Bureau of
Description of Dataset for HW1 Each month the Bureau of
Description of Dataset for HW1 Each month the Bureau of
Description of Dataset for HW1 Each month the Bureau of
Description of Dataset for HW1 Each month the Bureau of
Description of Dataset for HW1 Each month the Bureau of
Description of Dataset for HW1 Each month the Bureau of
Description of Dataset for HW1 Each month the Bureau of
Description of Dataset for HW1 Each month the Bureau of
Description of Dataset for HW1 Each month the Bureau of
Description of Dataset for HW1 Each month the Bureau of
Description of Dataset for HW1 Each month the Bureau of
Description of Dataset for HW1 Each month the Bureau of
Description of Dataset for HW1 Each month the Bureau of
Description of Dataset for HW1 Each month the Bureau of
Description of Dataset for HW1 Each month the Bureau of
Description of Dataset for HW1 Each month the Bureau of
Description of Dataset for HW1 Each month the Bureau of
Description of Dataset for HW1 Each month the Bureau of
Description of Dataset for HW1 Each month the Bureau of
Description of Dataset for HW1 Each month the Bureau of
Description of Dataset for HW1 Each month the Bureau of
Description of Dataset for HW1 Each month the Bureau of
Description of Dataset for HW1 Each month the Bureau of
Description of Dataset for HW1 Each month the Bureau of
Description of Dataset for HW1 Each month the Bureau of
Description of Dataset for HW1 Each month the Bureau of
Description of Dataset for HW1 Each month the Bureau of
Description of Dataset for HW1 Each month the Bureau of
Description of Dataset for HW1 Each month the Bureau of
Description of Dataset for HW1 Each month the Bureau of
Description of Dataset for HW1 Each month the Bureau of
Description of Dataset for HW1 Each month the Bureau of
Description of Dataset for HW1 Each month the Bureau of
Description of Dataset for HW1 Each month the Bureau of
Description of Dataset for HW1 Each month the Bureau of
Description of Dataset for HW1 Each month the Bureau of
Description of Dataset for HW1 Each month the Bureau of
Description of Dataset for HW1 Each month the Bureau of
Description of Dataset for HW1 Each month the Bureau of
Description of Dataset for HW1 Each month the Bureau of
Description of Dataset for HW1 Each month the Bureau of
Description of Dataset for HW1 Each month the Bureau of
Description of Dataset for HW1 Each month the Bureau of
Description of Dataset for HW1 Each month the Bureau of
Description of Dataset for HW1 Each month the Bureau of
Description of Dataset for HW1 Each month the Bureau of
Description of Dataset for HW1 Each month the Bureau of
Description of Dataset for HW1 Each month the Bureau of
Description of Dataset for HW1 Each month the Bureau of
Description of Dataset for HW1 Each month the Bureau of
Description of Dataset for HW1 Each month the Bureau of
Description of Dataset for HW1 Each month the Bureau of
Description of Dataset for HW1 Each month the Bureau of
Description of Dataset for HW1 Each month the Bureau of
Description of Dataset for HW1 Each month the Bureau of
Description of Dataset for HW1 Each month the Bureau of
Description of Dataset for HW1 Each month the Bureau of
Description of Dataset for HW1 Each month the Bureau of
Description of Dataset for HW1 Each month the Bureau of
Description of Dataset for HW1 Each month the Bureau of
Description of Dataset for HW1 Each month the Bureau of
Description of Dataset for HW1 Each month the Bureau of
Description of Dataset for HW1 Each month the Bureau of
Description of Dataset for HW1 Each month the Bureau of
Description of Dataset for HW1 Each month the Bureau of
Description of Dataset for HW1 Each month the Bureau of
Description of Dataset for HW1 Each month the Bureau of
Description of Dataset for HW1 Each month the Bureau of
Description of Dataset for HW1 Each month the Bureau of
Description of Dataset for HW1 Each month the Bureau of
Description of Dataset for HW1 Each month the Bureau of
Description of Dataset for HW1 Each month the Bureau of
Description of Dataset for HW1 Each month the Bureau of
Description of Dataset for HW1 Each month the Bureau of
Description of Dataset for HW1 Each month the Bureau of
Description of Dataset for HW1 Each month the Bureau of
Description of Dataset for HW1 Each month the Bureau of
Description of Dataset for HW1 Each month the Bureau of
Description of Dataset for HW1 Each month the Bureau of
Description of Dataset for HW1 Each month the Bureau of
Description of Dataset for HW1 Each month the Bureau of
Description of Dataset for HW1 Each month the Bureau of
Description of Dataset for HW1 Each month the Bureau of
Description of Dataset for HW1 Each month the Bureau of
Description of Dataset for HW1 Each month the Bureau of
Description of Dataset for HW1 Each month the Bureau of
Description of Dataset for HW1 Each month the Bureau of
Description of Dataset for HW1 Each month the Bureau of
Description of Dataset for HW1 Each month the Bureau of
Description of Dataset for HW1 Each month the Bureau of
Description of Dataset for HW1 Each month the Bureau of
Description of Dataset for HW1 Each month the Bureau of
Description of Dataset for HW1 Each month the Bureau of
Description of Dataset for HW1 Each month the Bureau of
Description of Dataset for HW1 Each month the Bureau of
Description of Dataset for HW1 Each month the Bureau of
Description of Dataset for HW1 Each month the Bureau of
Description of Dataset for HW1 Each month the Bureau of
Description of Dataset for HW1 Each month the Bureau of
Description of Dataset for HW1 Each month the Bureau of
Description of Dataset for HW1 Each month the Bureau of
Description of Dataset for HW1 Each month the Bureau of
Description of Dataset for HW1 Each month the Bureau of
Description of Dataset for HW1 Each month the Bureau of
Description of Dataset for HW1 Each month the Bureau of
Description of Dataset for HW1 Each month the Bureau of
Description of Dataset for HW1 Each month the Bureau of
Description of Dataset for HW1 Each month the Bureau of
Description of Dataset for HW1 Each month the Bureau of
Description of Dataset for HW1 Each month the Bureau of
Description of Dataset for HW1 Each month the Bureau of
Description of Dataset for HW1 Each month the Bureau of
Description of Dataset for HW1 Each month the Bureau of
Description of Dataset for HW1 Each month the Bureau of
Description of Dataset for HW1 Each month the Bureau of
Description of Dataset for HW1 Each month the Bureau of
Description of Dataset for HW1 Each month the Bureau of
Description of Dataset for HW1 Each month the Bureau of
Description of Dataset for HW1 Each month the Bureau of
Description of Dataset for HW1 Each month the Bureau of
Description of Dataset for HW1 Each month the Bureau of
Description of Dataset for HW1 Each month the Bureau of
Description of Dataset for HW1 Each month the Bureau of
Description of Dataset for HW1 Each month the Bureau of
Description of Dataset for HW1 Each month the Bureau of
Description of Dataset for HW1 Each month the Bureau of
Description of Dataset for HW1 Each month the Bureau of
Description of Dataset for HW1 Each month the Bureau of
Description of Dataset for HW1 Each month the Bureau of
Description of Dataset for HW1 Each month the Bureau of
Description of Dataset for HW1 Each month the Bureau of
Description of Dataset for HW1 Each month the Bureau of
Description of Dataset for HW1 Each month the Bureau of
Description of Dataset for HW1 Each month the Bureau of
Description of Dataset for HW1 Each month the Bureau of
Description of Dataset for HW1 Each month the Bureau of
Description of Dataset for HW1 Each month the Bureau of
Description of Dataset for HW1 Each month the Bureau of
Description of Dataset for HW1 Each month the Bureau of
Description of Dataset for HW1 Each month the Bureau of
Description of Dataset for HW1 Each month the Bureau of
Description of Dataset for HW1 Each month the Bureau of
Description of Dataset for HW1 Each month the Bureau of
Description of Dataset for HW1 Each month the Bureau of
Description of Dataset for HW1 Each month the Bureau of
Description of Dataset for HW1 Each month the Bureau of
Description of Dataset for HW1 Each month the Bureau of
Description of Dataset for HW1 Each month the Bureau of
Description of Dataset for HW1 Each month the Bureau of
Description of Dataset for HW1 Each month the Bureau of
Description of Dataset for HW1 Each month the Bureau of
Description of Dataset for HW1 Each month the Bureau of
Description of Dataset for HW1 Each month the Bureau of
Description of Dataset for HW1 Each month the Bureau of
Description of Dataset for HW1 Each month the Bureau of
Description of Dataset for HW1 Each month the Bureau of
Description of Dataset for HW1 Each month the Bureau of
Description of Dataset for HW1 Each month the Bureau of
Description of Dataset for HW1 Each month the Bureau of
Description of Dataset for HW1 Each month the Bureau of
Description of Dataset for HW1 Each month the Bureau of
Description of Dataset for HW1 Each month the Bureau of
Description of Dataset for HW1 Each month the Bureau of
Description of Dataset for HW1 Each month the Bureau of
Description of Dataset for HW1 Each month the Bureau of
Description of Dataset for HW1 Each month the Bureau of
Description of Dataset for HW1 Each month the Bureau of
Description of Dataset for HW1 Each month the Bureau of
Description of Dataset for HW1 Each month the Bureau of
Description of Dataset for HW1 Each month the Bureau of
Description of Dataset for HW1 Each month the Bureau of
Description of Dataset for HW1 Each month the Bureau of
Description of Dataset for HW1 Each month the Bureau of
Description of Dataset for HW1 Each month the Bureau of
Description of Dataset for HW1 Each month the Bureau of
Description of Dataset for HW1 Each month the Bureau of
Description of Dataset for HW1 Each month the Bureau of
Description of Dataset for HW1 Each month the Bureau of
Description of Dataset for HW1 Each month the Bureau of
Description of Dataset for HW1 Each month the Bureau of
Description of Dataset for HW1 Each month the Bureau of
Description of Dataset for HW1 Each month the Bureau of
Description of Dataset for HW1 Each month the Bureau of
Description of Dataset for HW1 Each month the Bureau of
Description of Dataset for HW1 Each month the Bureau of
Description of Dataset for HW1 Each month the Bureau of
Description of Dataset for HW1 Each month the Bureau of
Description of Dataset for HW1 Each month the Bureau of
Description of Dataset for HW1 Each month the Bureau of
Description of Dataset for HW1 Each month the Bureau of
Description of Dataset for HW1 Each month the Bureau of
Description of Dataset for HW1 Each month the Bureau of
Description of Dataset for HW1 Each month the Bureau of
Description of Dataset for HW1 Each month the Bureau of
Description of Dataset for HW1 Each month the Bureau of
Description of Dataset for HW1 Each month the Bureau of
Description of Dataset for HW1 Each month the Bureau of
Description of Dataset for HW1 Each month the Bureau of
Description of Dataset for HW1 Each month the Bureau of
Description of Dataset for HW1 Each month the Bureau of
Description of Dataset for HW1 Each month the Bureau of
Description of Dataset for HW1 Each month the Bureau of
Description of Dataset for HW1 Each month the Bureau of
Description of Dataset for HW1 Each month the Bureau of
Description of Dataset for HW1 Each month the Bureau of
Description of Dataset for HW1 Each month the Bureau of
Description of Dataset for HW1 Each month the Bureau of
Description of Dataset for HW1 Each month the Bureau of
Description of Dataset for HW1 Each month the Bureau of
Description of Dataset for HW1 Each month the Bureau of
Description of Dataset for HW1 Each month the Bureau of
Description of Dataset for HW1 Each month the Bureau of
Description of Dataset for HW1 Each month the Bureau of
Description of Dataset for HW1 Each month the Bureau of
Description of Dataset for HW1 Each month the Bureau of
Description of Dataset for HW1 Each month the Bureau of
Description of Dataset for HW1 Each month the Bureau of
Description of Dataset for HW1 Each month the Bureau of
Description of Dataset for HW1 Each month the Bureau of
Description of Dataset for HW1 Each month the Bureau of
Description of Dataset for HW1 Each month the Bureau of
Description of Dataset for HW1 Each month the Bureau of
Description of Dataset for HW1 Each month the Bureau of
Description of Dataset for HW1 Each month the Bureau of
Description of Dataset for HW1 Each month the Bureau of
Description of Dataset for HW1 Each month the Bureau of
Description of Dataset for HW1 Each month the Bureau of
Description of Dataset for HW1 Each month the Bureau of
Description of Dataset for HW1 Each month the Bureau of
Description of Dataset for HW1 Each month the Bureau of
Description of Dataset for HW1 Each month the Bureau of
Description of Dataset for HW1 Each month the Bureau of
Description of Dataset for HW1 Each month the Bureau of
Description of Dataset for HW1 Each month the Bureau of
Description of Dataset for HW1 Each month the Bureau of
Description of Dataset for HW1 Each month the Bureau of
Description of Dataset for HW1 Each month the Bureau of
Description of Dataset for HW1 Each month the Bureau of
Description of Dataset for HW1 Each month the Bureau of
Description of Dataset for HW1 Each month the Bureau of
Description of Dataset for HW1 Each month the Bureau of
Description of Dataset for HW1 Each month the Bureau of
Description of Dataset for HW1 Each month the Bureau of
Description of Dataset for HW1 Each month the Bureau of
Description of Dataset for HW1 Each month the Bureau of
Description of Dataset for HW1 Each month the Bureau of
Description of Dataset for HW1 Each month the Bureau of
Description of Dataset for HW1 Each month the Bureau of
Description of Dataset for HW1 Each month the Bureau of
Description of Dataset for HW1 Each month the Bureau of
Description of Dataset for HW1 Each month the Bureau of
Description of Dataset for HW1 Each month the Bureau of
Description of Dataset for HW1 Each month the Bureau of
Description of Dataset for HW1 Each month the Bureau of
Description of Dataset for HW1 Each month the Bureau of
Description of Dataset for HW1 Each month the Bureau of
Description of Dataset for HW1 Each month the Bureau of
Description of Dataset for HW1 Each month the Bureau of
Description of Dataset for HW1 Each month the Bureau of
Description of Dataset for HW1 Each month the Bureau of
Description of Dataset for HW1 Each month the Bureau of
Description of Dataset for HW1 Each month the Bureau of
Description of Dataset for HW1 Each month the Bureau of
Description of Dataset for HW1 Each month the Bureau of
Description of Dataset for HW1 Each month the Bureau of
Description of Dataset for HW1 Each month the Bureau of
Description of Dataset for HW1 Each month the Bureau of
Description of Dataset for HW1 Each month the Bureau of
Description of Dataset for HW1 Each month the Bureau of
Description of Dataset for HW1 Each month the Bureau of
Description of Dataset for HW1 Each month the Bureau of
Description of Dataset for HW1 Each month the Bureau of
Description of Dataset for HW1 Each month the Bureau of
Description of Dataset for HW1 Each month the Bureau of
Description of Dataset for HW1 Each month the Bureau of
Description of Dataset for HW1 Each month the Bureau of
Description of Dataset for HW1 Each month the Bureau of
Description of Dataset for HW1 Each month the Bureau of
Description of Dataset for HW1 Each month the Bureau of
Description of Dataset for HW1 Each month the Bureau of
Description of Dataset for HW1 Each month the Bureau of
Description of Dataset for HW1 Each month the Bureau of
Description of Dataset for HW1 Each month the Bureau of
Description of Dataset for HW1 Each month the Bureau of
Description of Dataset for HW1 Each month the Bureau of
Description of Dataset for HW1 Each month the Bureau of
Description of Dataset for HW1 Each month the Bureau of
Description of Dataset for HW1 Each month the Bureau of
Description of Dataset for HW1 Each month the Bureau of
Description of Dataset for HW1 Each month the Bureau of
Description of Dataset for HW1 Each month the Bureau of
Description of Dataset for HW1 Each month the Bureau of
Description of Dataset for HW1 Each month the Bureau of
Description of Dataset for HW1 Each month the Bureau of
Description of Dataset for HW1 Each month the Bureau of
Description of Dataset for HW1 Each month the Bureau of
Description of Dataset for HW1 Each month the Bureau of
Description of Dataset for HW1 Each month the Bureau of
Description of Dataset for HW1 Each month the Bureau of
Description of Dataset for HW1 Each month the Bureau of
Description of Dataset for HW1 Each month the Bureau of
Description of Dataset for HW1 Each month the Bureau of
Description of Dataset for HW1 Each month the Bureau of
Description of Dataset for HW1 Each month the Bureau of
Description of Dataset for HW1 Each month the Bureau of
Description of Dataset for HW1 Each month the Bureau of
Description of Dataset for HW1 Each month the Bureau of
Description of Dataset for HW1 Each month the Bureau of
Description of Dataset for HW1 Each month the Bureau of
Description of Dataset for HW1 Each month the Bureau of
Description of Dataset for HW1 Each month the Bureau of
Description of Dataset for HW1 Each month the Bureau of
Description of Dataset for HW1 Each month the Bureau of
Description of Dataset for HW1 Each month the Bureau of
Description of Dataset for HW1 Each month the Bureau of
Description of Dataset for HW1 Each month the Bureau of
Description of Dataset for HW1 Each month the Bureau of
Description of Dataset for HW1 Each month the Bureau of
Description of Dataset for HW1 Each month the Bureau of
Description of Dataset for HW1 Each month the Bureau of
Description of Dataset for HW1 Each month the Bureau of
Description of Dataset for HW1 Each month the Bureau of
Description of Dataset for HW1 Each month the Bureau of
Description of Dataset for HW1 Each month the Bureau of
Description of Dataset for HW1 Each month the Bureau of
Description of Dataset for HW1 Each month the Bureau of
Description of Dataset for HW1 Each month the Bureau of
Description of Dataset for HW1 Each month the Bureau of
Description of Dataset for HW1 Each month the Bureau of
Description of Dataset for HW1 Each month the Bureau of
Description of Dataset for HW1 Each month the Bureau of
Description of Dataset for HW1 Each month the Bureau of
Description of Dataset for HW1 Each month the Bureau of
Description of Dataset for HW1 Each month the Bureau of
Description of Dataset for HW1 Each month the Bureau of
Description of Dataset for HW1 Each month the Bureau of
Description of Dataset for HW1 Each month the Bureau of
Description of Dataset for HW1 Each month the Bureau of
Description of Dataset for HW1 Each month the Bureau of
Description of Dataset for HW1 Each month the Bureau of
Description of Dataset for HW1 Each month the Bureau of
Description of Dataset for HW1 Each month the Bureau of
Description of Dataset for HW1 Each month the Bureau of
Description of Dataset for HW1 Each month the Bureau of
Description of Dataset for HW1 Each month the Bureau of
Description of Dataset for HW1 Each month the Bureau of
Description of Dataset for HW1 Each month the Bureau of
Description of Dataset for HW1 Each month the Bureau of
Description of Dataset for HW1 Each month the Bureau of
Description of Dataset for HW1 Each month the Bureau of
Description of Dataset for HW1 Each month the Bureau of
Description of Dataset for HW1 Each month the Bureau of
Description of Dataset for HW1 Each month the Bureau of
Description of Dataset for HW1 Each month the Bureau of
Description of Dataset for HW1 Each month the Bureau of
Description of Dataset for HW1 Each month the Bureau of
Description of Dataset for HW1 Each month the Bureau of
Description of Dataset for HW1 Each month the Bureau of
Description of Dataset for HW1 Each month the Bureau of
Description of Dataset for HW1 Each month the Bureau of
Description of Dataset for HW1 Each month the Bureau of
Description of Dataset for HW1 Each month the Bureau of
Description of Dataset for HW1 Each month the Bureau of
Description of Dataset for HW1 Each month the Bureau of
Description of Dataset for HW1 Each month the Bureau of
Description of Dataset for HW1 Each month the Bureau of
Description of Dataset for HW1 Each month the Bureau of
Description of Dataset for HW1 Each month the Bureau of
Description of Dataset for HW1 Each month the Bureau of
Description of Dataset for HW1 Each month the Bureau of
Description of Dataset for HW1 Each month the Bureau of
Description of Dataset for HW1 Each month the Bureau of
Description of Dataset for HW1 Each month the Bureau of
Description of Dataset for HW1 Each month the Bureau of
Description of Dataset for HW1 Each month the Bureau of
Description of Dataset for HW1 Each month the Bureau of
Description of Dataset for HW1 Each month the Bureau of
Description of Dataset for HW1 Each month the Bureau of
Description of Dataset for HW1 Each month the Bureau of
Description of Dataset for HW1 Each month the Bureau of
Description of Dataset for HW1 Each month the Bureau of
Description of Dataset for HW1 Each month the Bureau of
Description of Dataset for HW1 Each month the Bureau of

More Related Content

Similar to Description of Dataset for HW1 Each month the Bureau of

Theory of behavior notes @ mba
Theory of behavior  notes @ mbaTheory of behavior  notes @ mba
Theory of behavior notes @ mbaBabasab Patil
 
Behaviorism
Behaviorism Behaviorism
Behaviorism anmolayaz
 
Quality Recruitment & Retention
Quality Recruitment & RetentionQuality Recruitment & Retention
Quality Recruitment & RetentionShawn C. Petty
 
Ep psych - presentation behaviorism
Ep psych - presentation behaviorismEp psych - presentation behaviorism
Ep psych - presentation behaviorismaboula1991
 
Classical Conditioning according to Pavlov and J.b Waston
Classical Conditioning according to Pavlov and J.b Waston Classical Conditioning according to Pavlov and J.b Waston
Classical Conditioning according to Pavlov and J.b Waston UmairaNasim
 
Principles and Applications of Classical and operant conditioningppt.pptx
Principles and Applications of Classical and operant conditioningppt.pptxPrinciples and Applications of Classical and operant conditioningppt.pptx
Principles and Applications of Classical and operant conditioningppt.pptxsarahfauzna
 
Learning in Psychological Perspectives.pdf
Learning in Psychological Perspectives.pdfLearning in Psychological Perspectives.pdf
Learning in Psychological Perspectives.pdfKhemraj Subedi
 
Theory of Behaviorism
Theory of BehaviorismTheory of Behaviorism
Theory of BehaviorismHiren Patel
 
PAVLOV’S CLASSICAL CONDITIONING .pptx
PAVLOV’S CLASSICAL CONDITIONING .pptxPAVLOV’S CLASSICAL CONDITIONING .pptx
PAVLOV’S CLASSICAL CONDITIONING .pptxRadhika Verma
 
CLASSICAL CONDITIONING.pptx
CLASSICAL CONDITIONING.pptxCLASSICAL CONDITIONING.pptx
CLASSICAL CONDITIONING.pptxKSiva21
 

Similar to Description of Dataset for HW1 Each month the Bureau of (18)

Theory of behavior notes @ mba
Theory of behavior  notes @ mbaTheory of behavior  notes @ mba
Theory of behavior notes @ mba
 
Chapter8
Chapter8Chapter8
Chapter8
 
Behaviorism
Behaviorism Behaviorism
Behaviorism
 
Quality Recruitment & Retention
Quality Recruitment & RetentionQuality Recruitment & Retention
Quality Recruitment & Retention
 
Learning
LearningLearning
Learning
 
MS IQRAT JAAN.pptx
MS IQRAT JAAN.pptxMS IQRAT JAAN.pptx
MS IQRAT JAAN.pptx
 
Ep psych - presentation behaviorism
Ep psych - presentation behaviorismEp psych - presentation behaviorism
Ep psych - presentation behaviorism
 
Classical Conditioning according to Pavlov and J.b Waston
Classical Conditioning according to Pavlov and J.b Waston Classical Conditioning according to Pavlov and J.b Waston
Classical Conditioning according to Pavlov and J.b Waston
 
Principles and Applications of Classical and operant conditioningppt.pptx
Principles and Applications of Classical and operant conditioningppt.pptxPrinciples and Applications of Classical and operant conditioningppt.pptx
Principles and Applications of Classical and operant conditioningppt.pptx
 
APPENDIX.docx
APPENDIX.docxAPPENDIX.docx
APPENDIX.docx
 
Learning in Psychological Perspectives.pdf
Learning in Psychological Perspectives.pdfLearning in Psychological Perspectives.pdf
Learning in Psychological Perspectives.pdf
 
Behaviorism
BehaviorismBehaviorism
Behaviorism
 
Classical conditioning
Classical conditioningClassical conditioning
Classical conditioning
 
Classical conditioning
Classical conditioningClassical conditioning
Classical conditioning
 
Theory of Behaviorism
Theory of BehaviorismTheory of Behaviorism
Theory of Behaviorism
 
ppt on Ivan pavlov
ppt on Ivan pavlovppt on Ivan pavlov
ppt on Ivan pavlov
 
PAVLOV’S CLASSICAL CONDITIONING .pptx
PAVLOV’S CLASSICAL CONDITIONING .pptxPAVLOV’S CLASSICAL CONDITIONING .pptx
PAVLOV’S CLASSICAL CONDITIONING .pptx
 
CLASSICAL CONDITIONING.pptx
CLASSICAL CONDITIONING.pptxCLASSICAL CONDITIONING.pptx
CLASSICAL CONDITIONING.pptx
 

More from LinaCovington707

ESSAY #4In contrast to thinking of poor people as deserving of bei.docx
ESSAY #4In contrast to thinking of poor people as deserving of bei.docxESSAY #4In contrast to thinking of poor people as deserving of bei.docx
ESSAY #4In contrast to thinking of poor people as deserving of bei.docxLinaCovington707
 
Essay # 3 Instructions Representations of War and Genocide .docx
Essay # 3 Instructions Representations of War and Genocide .docxEssay # 3 Instructions Representations of War and Genocide .docx
Essay # 3 Instructions Representations of War and Genocide .docxLinaCovington707
 
Essay 1 What is the role of the millennial servant leader on Capito.docx
Essay 1 What is the role of the millennial servant leader on Capito.docxEssay 1 What is the role of the millennial servant leader on Capito.docx
Essay 1 What is the role of the millennial servant leader on Capito.docxLinaCovington707
 
ESSAY #6Over the course of the quarter, you have learned to apply .docx
ESSAY #6Over the course of the quarter, you have learned to apply .docxESSAY #6Over the course of the quarter, you have learned to apply .docx
ESSAY #6Over the course of the quarter, you have learned to apply .docxLinaCovington707
 
ErrorsKeyboarding ErrorsCapitlalization ErrorsAbbreviation err.docx
ErrorsKeyboarding ErrorsCapitlalization ErrorsAbbreviation err.docxErrorsKeyboarding ErrorsCapitlalization ErrorsAbbreviation err.docx
ErrorsKeyboarding ErrorsCapitlalization ErrorsAbbreviation err.docxLinaCovington707
 
Epidemiological ApplicationsDescribe how the concept of multifacto.docx
Epidemiological ApplicationsDescribe how the concept of multifacto.docxEpidemiological ApplicationsDescribe how the concept of multifacto.docx
Epidemiological ApplicationsDescribe how the concept of multifacto.docxLinaCovington707
 
Epidemic, Endemic, and Pandemic Occurrence of Disease(s)One aspect.docx
Epidemic, Endemic, and Pandemic Occurrence of Disease(s)One aspect.docxEpidemic, Endemic, and Pandemic Occurrence of Disease(s)One aspect.docx
Epidemic, Endemic, and Pandemic Occurrence of Disease(s)One aspect.docxLinaCovington707
 
ENVIRONMENTShould the US support initiatives that restrict carbo.docx
ENVIRONMENTShould the US support initiatives that restrict carbo.docxENVIRONMENTShould the US support initiatives that restrict carbo.docx
ENVIRONMENTShould the US support initiatives that restrict carbo.docxLinaCovington707
 
ePortfolio CompletionResourcesDiscussion Participation Scoring.docx
ePortfolio CompletionResourcesDiscussion Participation Scoring.docxePortfolio CompletionResourcesDiscussion Participation Scoring.docx
ePortfolio CompletionResourcesDiscussion Participation Scoring.docxLinaCovington707
 
eproduction and Animal BehaviorReproduction Explain why asexually.docx
eproduction and Animal BehaviorReproduction Explain why asexually.docxeproduction and Animal BehaviorReproduction Explain why asexually.docx
eproduction and Animal BehaviorReproduction Explain why asexually.docxLinaCovington707
 
Envisioning LeadershipIdentifying a challenge that evokes your pas.docx
Envisioning LeadershipIdentifying a challenge that evokes your pas.docxEnvisioning LeadershipIdentifying a challenge that evokes your pas.docx
Envisioning LeadershipIdentifying a challenge that evokes your pas.docxLinaCovington707
 
EnvironmentOur environment is really important. We need to under.docx
EnvironmentOur environment is really important. We need to under.docxEnvironmentOur environment is really important. We need to under.docx
EnvironmentOur environment is really important. We need to under.docxLinaCovington707
 
Environmental Awareness and Organizational Sustainability  Please .docx
Environmental Awareness and Organizational Sustainability  Please .docxEnvironmental Awareness and Organizational Sustainability  Please .docx
Environmental Awareness and Organizational Sustainability  Please .docxLinaCovington707
 
EnterobacteriaceaeThe family Enterobacteriaceae contains some or.docx
EnterobacteriaceaeThe family Enterobacteriaceae contains some or.docxEnterobacteriaceaeThe family Enterobacteriaceae contains some or.docx
EnterobacteriaceaeThe family Enterobacteriaceae contains some or.docxLinaCovington707
 
Ensuring your local region is prepared for any emergency is a comp.docx
Ensuring your local region is prepared for any emergency is a comp.docxEnsuring your local region is prepared for any emergency is a comp.docx
Ensuring your local region is prepared for any emergency is a comp.docxLinaCovington707
 
ENG 2480 Major Assignment #3Essay #2 CharacterAnaly.docx
ENG 2480 Major Assignment #3Essay #2 CharacterAnaly.docxENG 2480 Major Assignment #3Essay #2 CharacterAnaly.docx
ENG 2480 Major Assignment #3Essay #2 CharacterAnaly.docxLinaCovington707
 
English EssayMLA format500 words or moreThis is Caue types of .docx
English EssayMLA format500 words or moreThis is Caue types of .docxEnglish EssayMLA format500 words or moreThis is Caue types of .docx
English EssayMLA format500 words or moreThis is Caue types of .docxLinaCovington707
 
Eng 2480 British Literature after 1790NameApplying Wilde .docx
Eng 2480 British Literature after 1790NameApplying Wilde .docxEng 2480 British Literature after 1790NameApplying Wilde .docx
Eng 2480 British Literature after 1790NameApplying Wilde .docxLinaCovington707
 
English 1C Critical Thinking Essay (6 - 6 12 pages, MLA 12pt font .docx
English 1C Critical Thinking Essay (6 - 6 12 pages, MLA 12pt font .docxEnglish 1C Critical Thinking Essay (6 - 6 12 pages, MLA 12pt font .docx
English 1C Critical Thinking Essay (6 - 6 12 pages, MLA 12pt font .docxLinaCovington707
 
ENGL 227World FictionEssay #2Write a 2-3 page essay (with work.docx
ENGL 227World FictionEssay #2Write a 2-3 page essay (with work.docxENGL 227World FictionEssay #2Write a 2-3 page essay (with work.docx
ENGL 227World FictionEssay #2Write a 2-3 page essay (with work.docxLinaCovington707
 

More from LinaCovington707 (20)

ESSAY #4In contrast to thinking of poor people as deserving of bei.docx
ESSAY #4In contrast to thinking of poor people as deserving of bei.docxESSAY #4In contrast to thinking of poor people as deserving of bei.docx
ESSAY #4In contrast to thinking of poor people as deserving of bei.docx
 
Essay # 3 Instructions Representations of War and Genocide .docx
Essay # 3 Instructions Representations of War and Genocide .docxEssay # 3 Instructions Representations of War and Genocide .docx
Essay # 3 Instructions Representations of War and Genocide .docx
 
Essay 1 What is the role of the millennial servant leader on Capito.docx
Essay 1 What is the role of the millennial servant leader on Capito.docxEssay 1 What is the role of the millennial servant leader on Capito.docx
Essay 1 What is the role of the millennial servant leader on Capito.docx
 
ESSAY #6Over the course of the quarter, you have learned to apply .docx
ESSAY #6Over the course of the quarter, you have learned to apply .docxESSAY #6Over the course of the quarter, you have learned to apply .docx
ESSAY #6Over the course of the quarter, you have learned to apply .docx
 
ErrorsKeyboarding ErrorsCapitlalization ErrorsAbbreviation err.docx
ErrorsKeyboarding ErrorsCapitlalization ErrorsAbbreviation err.docxErrorsKeyboarding ErrorsCapitlalization ErrorsAbbreviation err.docx
ErrorsKeyboarding ErrorsCapitlalization ErrorsAbbreviation err.docx
 
Epidemiological ApplicationsDescribe how the concept of multifacto.docx
Epidemiological ApplicationsDescribe how the concept of multifacto.docxEpidemiological ApplicationsDescribe how the concept of multifacto.docx
Epidemiological ApplicationsDescribe how the concept of multifacto.docx
 
Epidemic, Endemic, and Pandemic Occurrence of Disease(s)One aspect.docx
Epidemic, Endemic, and Pandemic Occurrence of Disease(s)One aspect.docxEpidemic, Endemic, and Pandemic Occurrence of Disease(s)One aspect.docx
Epidemic, Endemic, and Pandemic Occurrence of Disease(s)One aspect.docx
 
ENVIRONMENTShould the US support initiatives that restrict carbo.docx
ENVIRONMENTShould the US support initiatives that restrict carbo.docxENVIRONMENTShould the US support initiatives that restrict carbo.docx
ENVIRONMENTShould the US support initiatives that restrict carbo.docx
 
ePortfolio CompletionResourcesDiscussion Participation Scoring.docx
ePortfolio CompletionResourcesDiscussion Participation Scoring.docxePortfolio CompletionResourcesDiscussion Participation Scoring.docx
ePortfolio CompletionResourcesDiscussion Participation Scoring.docx
 
eproduction and Animal BehaviorReproduction Explain why asexually.docx
eproduction and Animal BehaviorReproduction Explain why asexually.docxeproduction and Animal BehaviorReproduction Explain why asexually.docx
eproduction and Animal BehaviorReproduction Explain why asexually.docx
 
Envisioning LeadershipIdentifying a challenge that evokes your pas.docx
Envisioning LeadershipIdentifying a challenge that evokes your pas.docxEnvisioning LeadershipIdentifying a challenge that evokes your pas.docx
Envisioning LeadershipIdentifying a challenge that evokes your pas.docx
 
EnvironmentOur environment is really important. We need to under.docx
EnvironmentOur environment is really important. We need to under.docxEnvironmentOur environment is really important. We need to under.docx
EnvironmentOur environment is really important. We need to under.docx
 
Environmental Awareness and Organizational Sustainability  Please .docx
Environmental Awareness and Organizational Sustainability  Please .docxEnvironmental Awareness and Organizational Sustainability  Please .docx
Environmental Awareness and Organizational Sustainability  Please .docx
 
EnterobacteriaceaeThe family Enterobacteriaceae contains some or.docx
EnterobacteriaceaeThe family Enterobacteriaceae contains some or.docxEnterobacteriaceaeThe family Enterobacteriaceae contains some or.docx
EnterobacteriaceaeThe family Enterobacteriaceae contains some or.docx
 
Ensuring your local region is prepared for any emergency is a comp.docx
Ensuring your local region is prepared for any emergency is a comp.docxEnsuring your local region is prepared for any emergency is a comp.docx
Ensuring your local region is prepared for any emergency is a comp.docx
 
ENG 2480 Major Assignment #3Essay #2 CharacterAnaly.docx
ENG 2480 Major Assignment #3Essay #2 CharacterAnaly.docxENG 2480 Major Assignment #3Essay #2 CharacterAnaly.docx
ENG 2480 Major Assignment #3Essay #2 CharacterAnaly.docx
 
English EssayMLA format500 words or moreThis is Caue types of .docx
English EssayMLA format500 words or moreThis is Caue types of .docxEnglish EssayMLA format500 words or moreThis is Caue types of .docx
English EssayMLA format500 words or moreThis is Caue types of .docx
 
Eng 2480 British Literature after 1790NameApplying Wilde .docx
Eng 2480 British Literature after 1790NameApplying Wilde .docxEng 2480 British Literature after 1790NameApplying Wilde .docx
Eng 2480 British Literature after 1790NameApplying Wilde .docx
 
English 1C Critical Thinking Essay (6 - 6 12 pages, MLA 12pt font .docx
English 1C Critical Thinking Essay (6 - 6 12 pages, MLA 12pt font .docxEnglish 1C Critical Thinking Essay (6 - 6 12 pages, MLA 12pt font .docx
English 1C Critical Thinking Essay (6 - 6 12 pages, MLA 12pt font .docx
 
ENGL 227World FictionEssay #2Write a 2-3 page essay (with work.docx
ENGL 227World FictionEssay #2Write a 2-3 page essay (with work.docxENGL 227World FictionEssay #2Write a 2-3 page essay (with work.docx
ENGL 227World FictionEssay #2Write a 2-3 page essay (with work.docx
 

Recently uploaded

Personalisation of Education by AI and Big Data - Lourdes Guàrdia
Personalisation of Education by AI and Big Data - Lourdes GuàrdiaPersonalisation of Education by AI and Big Data - Lourdes Guàrdia
Personalisation of Education by AI and Big Data - Lourdes GuàrdiaEADTU
 
AIM of Education-Teachers Training-2024.ppt
AIM of Education-Teachers Training-2024.pptAIM of Education-Teachers Training-2024.ppt
AIM of Education-Teachers Training-2024.pptNishitharanjan Rout
 
COMMUNICATING NEGATIVE NEWS - APPROACHES .pptx
COMMUNICATING NEGATIVE NEWS - APPROACHES .pptxCOMMUNICATING NEGATIVE NEWS - APPROACHES .pptx
COMMUNICATING NEGATIVE NEWS - APPROACHES .pptxannathomasp01
 
Transparency, Recognition and the role of eSealing - Ildiko Mazar and Koen No...
Transparency, Recognition and the role of eSealing - Ildiko Mazar and Koen No...Transparency, Recognition and the role of eSealing - Ildiko Mazar and Koen No...
Transparency, Recognition and the role of eSealing - Ildiko Mazar and Koen No...EADTU
 
OSCM Unit 2_Operations Processes & Systems
OSCM Unit 2_Operations Processes & SystemsOSCM Unit 2_Operations Processes & Systems
OSCM Unit 2_Operations Processes & SystemsSandeep D Chaudhary
 
DEMONSTRATION LESSON IN ENGLISH 4 MATATAG CURRICULUM
DEMONSTRATION LESSON IN ENGLISH 4 MATATAG CURRICULUMDEMONSTRATION LESSON IN ENGLISH 4 MATATAG CURRICULUM
DEMONSTRATION LESSON IN ENGLISH 4 MATATAG CURRICULUMELOISARIVERA8
 
The Story of Village Palampur Class 9 Free Study Material PDF
The Story of Village Palampur Class 9 Free Study Material PDFThe Story of Village Palampur Class 9 Free Study Material PDF
The Story of Village Palampur Class 9 Free Study Material PDFVivekanand Anglo Vedic Academy
 
How to Send Pro Forma Invoice to Your Customers in Odoo 17
How to Send Pro Forma Invoice to Your Customers in Odoo 17How to Send Pro Forma Invoice to Your Customers in Odoo 17
How to Send Pro Forma Invoice to Your Customers in Odoo 17Celine George
 
Sternal Fractures & Dislocations - EMGuidewire Radiology Reading Room
Sternal Fractures & Dislocations - EMGuidewire Radiology Reading RoomSternal Fractures & Dislocations - EMGuidewire Radiology Reading Room
Sternal Fractures & Dislocations - EMGuidewire Radiology Reading RoomSean M. Fox
 
Stl Algorithms in C++ jjjjjjjjjjjjjjjjjj
Stl Algorithms in C++ jjjjjjjjjjjjjjjjjjStl Algorithms in C++ jjjjjjjjjjjjjjjjjj
Stl Algorithms in C++ jjjjjjjjjjjjjjjjjjMohammed Sikander
 
會考英文會考英文會考英文會考英文會考英文會考英文會考英文會考英文會考英文會考英文會考英文
會考英文會考英文會考英文會考英文會考英文會考英文會考英文會考英文會考英文會考英文會考英文會考英文會考英文會考英文會考英文會考英文會考英文會考英文會考英文會考英文會考英文會考英文
會考英文會考英文會考英文會考英文會考英文會考英文會考英文會考英文會考英文會考英文會考英文中 央社
 
ĐỀ THAM KHẢO KÌ THI TUYỂN SINH VÀO LỚP 10 MÔN TIẾNG ANH FORM 50 CÂU TRẮC NGHI...
ĐỀ THAM KHẢO KÌ THI TUYỂN SINH VÀO LỚP 10 MÔN TIẾNG ANH FORM 50 CÂU TRẮC NGHI...ĐỀ THAM KHẢO KÌ THI TUYỂN SINH VÀO LỚP 10 MÔN TIẾNG ANH FORM 50 CÂU TRẮC NGHI...
ĐỀ THAM KHẢO KÌ THI TUYỂN SINH VÀO LỚP 10 MÔN TIẾNG ANH FORM 50 CÂU TRẮC NGHI...Nguyen Thanh Tu Collection
 
Spring gala 2024 photo slideshow - Celebrating School-Community Partnerships
Spring gala 2024 photo slideshow - Celebrating School-Community PartnershipsSpring gala 2024 photo slideshow - Celebrating School-Community Partnerships
Spring gala 2024 photo slideshow - Celebrating School-Community Partnershipsexpandedwebsite
 
UChicago CMSC 23320 - The Best Commit Messages of 2024
UChicago CMSC 23320 - The Best Commit Messages of 2024UChicago CMSC 23320 - The Best Commit Messages of 2024
UChicago CMSC 23320 - The Best Commit Messages of 2024Borja Sotomayor
 
Observing-Correct-Grammar-in-Making-Definitions.pptx
Observing-Correct-Grammar-in-Making-Definitions.pptxObserving-Correct-Grammar-in-Making-Definitions.pptx
Observing-Correct-Grammar-in-Making-Definitions.pptxAdelaideRefugio
 
Analyzing and resolving a communication crisis in Dhaka textiles LTD.pptx
Analyzing and resolving a communication crisis in Dhaka textiles LTD.pptxAnalyzing and resolving a communication crisis in Dhaka textiles LTD.pptx
Analyzing and resolving a communication crisis in Dhaka textiles LTD.pptxLimon Prince
 
When Quality Assurance Meets Innovation in Higher Education - Report launch w...
When Quality Assurance Meets Innovation in Higher Education - Report launch w...When Quality Assurance Meets Innovation in Higher Education - Report launch w...
When Quality Assurance Meets Innovation in Higher Education - Report launch w...Gary Wood
 
Improved Approval Flow in Odoo 17 Studio App
Improved Approval Flow in Odoo 17 Studio AppImproved Approval Flow in Odoo 17 Studio App
Improved Approval Flow in Odoo 17 Studio AppCeline George
 
24 ĐỀ THAM KHẢO KÌ THI TUYỂN SINH VÀO LỚP 10 MÔN TIẾNG ANH SỞ GIÁO DỤC HẢI DƯ...
24 ĐỀ THAM KHẢO KÌ THI TUYỂN SINH VÀO LỚP 10 MÔN TIẾNG ANH SỞ GIÁO DỤC HẢI DƯ...24 ĐỀ THAM KHẢO KÌ THI TUYỂN SINH VÀO LỚP 10 MÔN TIẾNG ANH SỞ GIÁO DỤC HẢI DƯ...
24 ĐỀ THAM KHẢO KÌ THI TUYỂN SINH VÀO LỚP 10 MÔN TIẾNG ANH SỞ GIÁO DỤC HẢI DƯ...Nguyen Thanh Tu Collection
 

Recently uploaded (20)

Personalisation of Education by AI and Big Data - Lourdes Guàrdia
Personalisation of Education by AI and Big Data - Lourdes GuàrdiaPersonalisation of Education by AI and Big Data - Lourdes Guàrdia
Personalisation of Education by AI and Big Data - Lourdes Guàrdia
 
AIM of Education-Teachers Training-2024.ppt
AIM of Education-Teachers Training-2024.pptAIM of Education-Teachers Training-2024.ppt
AIM of Education-Teachers Training-2024.ppt
 
COMMUNICATING NEGATIVE NEWS - APPROACHES .pptx
COMMUNICATING NEGATIVE NEWS - APPROACHES .pptxCOMMUNICATING NEGATIVE NEWS - APPROACHES .pptx
COMMUNICATING NEGATIVE NEWS - APPROACHES .pptx
 
Transparency, Recognition and the role of eSealing - Ildiko Mazar and Koen No...
Transparency, Recognition and the role of eSealing - Ildiko Mazar and Koen No...Transparency, Recognition and the role of eSealing - Ildiko Mazar and Koen No...
Transparency, Recognition and the role of eSealing - Ildiko Mazar and Koen No...
 
OSCM Unit 2_Operations Processes & Systems
OSCM Unit 2_Operations Processes & SystemsOSCM Unit 2_Operations Processes & Systems
OSCM Unit 2_Operations Processes & Systems
 
DEMONSTRATION LESSON IN ENGLISH 4 MATATAG CURRICULUM
DEMONSTRATION LESSON IN ENGLISH 4 MATATAG CURRICULUMDEMONSTRATION LESSON IN ENGLISH 4 MATATAG CURRICULUM
DEMONSTRATION LESSON IN ENGLISH 4 MATATAG CURRICULUM
 
The Story of Village Palampur Class 9 Free Study Material PDF
The Story of Village Palampur Class 9 Free Study Material PDFThe Story of Village Palampur Class 9 Free Study Material PDF
The Story of Village Palampur Class 9 Free Study Material PDF
 
How to Send Pro Forma Invoice to Your Customers in Odoo 17
How to Send Pro Forma Invoice to Your Customers in Odoo 17How to Send Pro Forma Invoice to Your Customers in Odoo 17
How to Send Pro Forma Invoice to Your Customers in Odoo 17
 
Sternal Fractures & Dislocations - EMGuidewire Radiology Reading Room
Sternal Fractures & Dislocations - EMGuidewire Radiology Reading RoomSternal Fractures & Dislocations - EMGuidewire Radiology Reading Room
Sternal Fractures & Dislocations - EMGuidewire Radiology Reading Room
 
Stl Algorithms in C++ jjjjjjjjjjjjjjjjjj
Stl Algorithms in C++ jjjjjjjjjjjjjjjjjjStl Algorithms in C++ jjjjjjjjjjjjjjjjjj
Stl Algorithms in C++ jjjjjjjjjjjjjjjjjj
 
會考英文會考英文會考英文會考英文會考英文會考英文會考英文會考英文會考英文會考英文會考英文
會考英文會考英文會考英文會考英文會考英文會考英文會考英文會考英文會考英文會考英文會考英文會考英文會考英文會考英文會考英文會考英文會考英文會考英文會考英文會考英文會考英文會考英文
會考英文會考英文會考英文會考英文會考英文會考英文會考英文會考英文會考英文會考英文會考英文
 
Mattingly "AI and Prompt Design: LLMs with NER"
Mattingly "AI and Prompt Design: LLMs with NER"Mattingly "AI and Prompt Design: LLMs with NER"
Mattingly "AI and Prompt Design: LLMs with NER"
 
ĐỀ THAM KHẢO KÌ THI TUYỂN SINH VÀO LỚP 10 MÔN TIẾNG ANH FORM 50 CÂU TRẮC NGHI...
ĐỀ THAM KHẢO KÌ THI TUYỂN SINH VÀO LỚP 10 MÔN TIẾNG ANH FORM 50 CÂU TRẮC NGHI...ĐỀ THAM KHẢO KÌ THI TUYỂN SINH VÀO LỚP 10 MÔN TIẾNG ANH FORM 50 CÂU TRẮC NGHI...
ĐỀ THAM KHẢO KÌ THI TUYỂN SINH VÀO LỚP 10 MÔN TIẾNG ANH FORM 50 CÂU TRẮC NGHI...
 
Spring gala 2024 photo slideshow - Celebrating School-Community Partnerships
Spring gala 2024 photo slideshow - Celebrating School-Community PartnershipsSpring gala 2024 photo slideshow - Celebrating School-Community Partnerships
Spring gala 2024 photo slideshow - Celebrating School-Community Partnerships
 
UChicago CMSC 23320 - The Best Commit Messages of 2024
UChicago CMSC 23320 - The Best Commit Messages of 2024UChicago CMSC 23320 - The Best Commit Messages of 2024
UChicago CMSC 23320 - The Best Commit Messages of 2024
 
Observing-Correct-Grammar-in-Making-Definitions.pptx
Observing-Correct-Grammar-in-Making-Definitions.pptxObserving-Correct-Grammar-in-Making-Definitions.pptx
Observing-Correct-Grammar-in-Making-Definitions.pptx
 
Analyzing and resolving a communication crisis in Dhaka textiles LTD.pptx
Analyzing and resolving a communication crisis in Dhaka textiles LTD.pptxAnalyzing and resolving a communication crisis in Dhaka textiles LTD.pptx
Analyzing and resolving a communication crisis in Dhaka textiles LTD.pptx
 
When Quality Assurance Meets Innovation in Higher Education - Report launch w...
When Quality Assurance Meets Innovation in Higher Education - Report launch w...When Quality Assurance Meets Innovation in Higher Education - Report launch w...
When Quality Assurance Meets Innovation in Higher Education - Report launch w...
 
Improved Approval Flow in Odoo 17 Studio App
Improved Approval Flow in Odoo 17 Studio AppImproved Approval Flow in Odoo 17 Studio App
Improved Approval Flow in Odoo 17 Studio App
 
24 ĐỀ THAM KHẢO KÌ THI TUYỂN SINH VÀO LỚP 10 MÔN TIẾNG ANH SỞ GIÁO DỤC HẢI DƯ...
24 ĐỀ THAM KHẢO KÌ THI TUYỂN SINH VÀO LỚP 10 MÔN TIẾNG ANH SỞ GIÁO DỤC HẢI DƯ...24 ĐỀ THAM KHẢO KÌ THI TUYỂN SINH VÀO LỚP 10 MÔN TIẾNG ANH SỞ GIÁO DỤC HẢI DƯ...
24 ĐỀ THAM KHẢO KÌ THI TUYỂN SINH VÀO LỚP 10 MÔN TIẾNG ANH SỞ GIÁO DỤC HẢI DƯ...
 

Description of Dataset for HW1 Each month the Bureau of

  • 1. Description of Dataset for HW1 Each month the Bureau of Labor Statistics in the U.S. Department of Labor conducts the “Current Population Survey” (CPS), which provides data on labor force characteristics of the population, including the level of employment, unemployment, and earnings. Approximately 65,000 randomly selected U.S. households are surveyed each month. The sample is chosen by randomly selecting addresses from a database comprised of addresses from the most recent decennial census augmented with data on new housing units constructed after the last census. The exact random sampling scheme is rather complicated (first small geographical areas are randomly selected, then housing units within these areas randomly selected); details can be found in the Handbook of Labor Statistics and is described on the Bureau of Labor Statistics website (www.bls.gov).
  • 2. The survey conducted each March is more detailed than in other months and asks questions about earnings during the previous year. The file HW1 contains the data for 2012 (from the March 2013 survey). These data are for full-time workers, defined as workers employed more than 35 hours per week for at least 48 weeks in the previous year. Data are provided for workers whose highest educational achievement is (1) a high school diploma, and (2) a bachelor’s degree. Series in Data Set: FEMALE: 1 if female; 0 if male YEAR: Year AHE : Average Hourly Earnings BACHELOR: 1 if worker has a bachelor’s degree; 0 if worker has a high school degree AGE: Age
  • 3. Title: Database: Pavlovian conditioning. By: Sparzo, Frank J., Salem Press Encyclopedia of Health, 2019 Research Starters Pavlovian conditioning Date: 1890s forward Type of psychology: Learning Pavlovian conditioning is a basic process of learning that relates especially to reflexes and emotional behavior. Interest in this form of learning has been long-standing and continues to the present day. Pavlovian principles apply to a very wide range of organisms, situations, and events. Introduction Pavlovian conditioning, also known as respondent conditioning and classical conditioning (to distinguish it from instrumental or operant conditioning), is an elementary learning process and has been of major interest to psychologists ever since the Russian physiologist Ivan Petrovich Pavlov discovered that a dog could learn to salivate to a neutral stimulus after the stimulus was paired repeatedly with food.
  • 4. Pavlov’s early career focused on the study of heart circulation and digestion in animals (usually dogs), for which he received the Nobel Prize in Physiology or Medicine in 1904. However, by UAGC Library-test Listen American Accent One of the many dogs Pavlov used in his experiments (possibly Baikal[1]), Pavlov Museum Ryazan, Russia. Note the saliva catch container and tube surgically implanted in the dog's muzzle. By Rklawton (English Wikipedia, see below) [GFDL (http://www.gnu.org/copyleft/fdl.html) or CC-BY-SA-3.0 (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by- sa/3.0/)], via Wikimedia Commons The Pavlov memorial museum, Ryazan; former home of the physiologist I. P. Pavlov (built in
  • 5. the early 19th century) By Ceroi (Own work) [CC-BY-SA-3.0 (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by- sa/3.0)], via Wikimedia Commons that time Pavlov had already turned his attention to experiments on conditioned reflexes, from which flowed a new psychological nomenclature. Conditioning The core of Pavlovian conditioning is the pairing (association) of stimuli to elicit responses. Food (meat powder) placed in a dog’s mouth naturally produces salivation. Pavlov called the food an unconditioned stimulus (US) and salivation, elicited by the food, the unconditioned response (UR). When a neutral stimulus—for example, a tone that does not naturally elicit salivation—is repeatedly followed by food, the tone alone eventually evokes salivation. Pavlov labeled the tone a conditioned stimulus (CS) and the response (salivation) elicited by it the conditioned response (CR). Pavlov’s formulation can be summarized as follows: Before conditioning: Food (US) elicits Salivation (UR) Conditioning procedure: Neutral Stimulus (Tone) plus Food (US) elicits Salivation (UR) After conditioning: Tone (CS) elicits Salivation (CR) Pavlov believed that conditioned responses were identical
  • 6. to unconditioned responses. That is usually not the case. For example, conditioned responses may be less pronounced (weaker) or a bit more lethargic than unconditioned responses. Several phenomena turn up in studies of Pavlovian conditioning. Extinction, generalization, and discrimination are among the most important. Extinction refers to the procedure as well as to the elimination of a CR. If the CS is repeatedly presented without the US, extinction occurs: The dog stops salivating to the tone. During the course of extinction, the CR may return from time to time until it is finally extinguished. Pavlov called the occasional return of the CR “spontaneous recovery.” Stimulus generalization refers to responding not only to a particular CS but also to similar but different stimuli. Further, the magnitude (amount of salivation) of a generalized response tends to decline as stimuli become less and less like the CS. For example, a dog trained to salivate to a 5,000-cycle-per-second (cps) tone is likely to salivate also to 5,300 cps and 4,700 cps tones without specific training to do so (stimulus generalization). Responses tend to weaken in an orderly way as tones become more and more unlike the CS— that is, as the tones move away from the CS in both directions, say, to 4,400 cps from 4,100 cps, and 5,600 cps to 5,900 cps, the flow of salivation becomes less and less. Stimulus generalization in effect extends the number of stimuli
  • 7. that elicit a conditioned response. Discrimination procedures restrict that number by conditioning a subject not to generalize across stimuli. The procedure involves two processes: acquisition and extinction. The CS is paired repeatedly with the US (acquisition) while the US is withheld as generalized stimuli are presented repeatedly (extinction). If the dog now salivates to the CS and not to the generalized stimuli, the dog has learned to discriminate or to act discriminatively. Pavlov reported that some dogs displayed a general breakdown in behavior patterns (experimental neurosis) when called on to make discriminations that were too difficult for them. Pavlov’s work on what he called the second-signal system implies that conditioning principles are relevant to human as well as to animal learning. Once, say, a tone is established as a CS in first-order conditioning, the tone can be paired with a neutral stimulus to establish a second- order CS. Thus, in the absence of food, a light might precede the tone (CS) several times until the light itself begins to function as a CS. Second-order conditioning appears to follow many of the same rules as first-order conditioning. Pavlov’s work has clearly provided one way to study the learning process in great detail. It has also provided the kind of data and theory that have affected research in other areas of learning, such as instrumental conditioning and, subsequently, cognitive science and neuroscience. In late 2015, neuroscientists at Johns Hopkins University conducted an experiment in the hope
  • 8. of finally determining how this learning process occurs, or exactly how Pavlov's dogs were conditioned to drool. For the first time, the scientists were able to prove in a lab the link between neurotransmitters and the conditioned response by studying brain cells of mice. After stimulating the cells with neurotransmitters, the scientists analyzed the significance of the brain's chemical reward system in terms of conditioning. This research prompted discussion about whether this knowledge could be used to enhance learning processes or possibly treat cognitive issues. Range of Pavlovian Conditioning Pavlovian phenomena have been demonstrated with different kinds of organisms and a wide variety of stimuli and responses far beyond those studi ed by Pavlov. Stimuli that precede such unconditioned stimuli as sudden loud noises (leading to rapid heart rate), a puff of air delivered to the eye (evoking blinking), or a large temperature increase (eliciting sweating) may become conditioned stimuli capable of eliciting conditioned responses on their own. The idea of second- order (higher-order) conditioning is profoundly important because it suggests how rewards such as words of praise and money are established apart from primary (biologically necessary) rewards, such as food and water. It also may in part explain the power of films, plays, novels, and advertisements to evoke strong emotion in the absence of direct experience with primary
  • 9. (unconditioned) stimuli. Studies concerned with conditioned emotional reactions (CER), especially fear and anxiety in people—a subject much more complex than simple reflexes— have been of special interest to researchers and therapists for many years. Additional Research Findings Studies of conditioning essentially look at how various unconditioned and conditioned stimuli influence responses under different arrangements of time and space. Following are a few general findings. Pavlovian conditioning tends to be readily established when stimuli or responses or both are strong rather than weak. For example, in response to a near- drowning experience, some people promptly learn to fear such conditioned stimuli as the sight of water, boats, palm trees, bathing suits, and so on. In such cases, relevant stimuli and responses (panic) are presumably quite strong. Conditioned stimuli are most likely to elicit conditioned responses when unconditioned and conditioned stimuli are paired consistently. If a mother always hums when she rocks her infant daughter to sleep, humming is likely to become a potent and reliable CS, which soothes and comforts her daughter. This outcome is less likely if the mother hums only occasionally. When several stimuli precede a US, the one most often paired with the US will likely emerge as
  • 10. the strongest CS. If, for example, both parents threaten to punish their young son, but only father always carries out the threats, father’s threats are more likely than mother’s to evoke apprehension in the child. For some responses, such as eye blinking, conditioned stimuli tend to be strongest when they precede the US by about one-half second. The optimal interval for other responses varies from seconds to fractions of seconds: A neighbor’s dog barks immediately before little Sophie falls from her swing, bumping her nose very hard. She cries. If the dog’s bark subsequently makes Sophie feel uneasy, the bark is functioning as a CS. This outcome becomes less and less likely as the bark and fall increasingly separate in time. Conditioned responses are usually not established if a US and CS occur together (simultaneous conditioning)—the potency of the UC overshadows the potential CS—or when a neutral stimulus follows the US (backward conditioning). Some Practical Applications In a widely cited study reported in 1920, American researchers John B. Watson and Rosalie Rayner conditioned a phobic reaction in an eleven-month-old infant named Albert. The researchers discovered that Albert feared loud noises but seemed unafraid of a number of other things, including small animals.
  • 11. Watson and Rayner subsequently placed a white rat in Albert’s crib. When Albert reached for it, the researchers struck a piece of resonant metal with a hammer, making a “loud sound.” After a few such presentations, presenting the rat alone elicited crying and various avoidance reactions. Albert also showed signs of fear to similar things, such as a rabbit, a furry object, and fluffy clumps of cotton (stimulus generalization). Thus, Watson and Rayner provided early experimental evidence that Pavlovian principles are involved in the acquisition of human emotional reactions. While this study induced a phobic reaction in the subject, systematic desensitization is a procedure designed to eliminate phobias and anxieties. The procedure was largely developed and named by South African-born therapist Joseph Wolpe. Noting that it is very difficult to have pleasant and anxious feelings simultaneously, Wolpe fashioned a systematic technique to teach clients to engage in behavior (relaxation) that competes with anxiety. Therapy typically begins with an interview designed to identify specific sources of the client’s fears. The therapist helps the client assemble a list of items that elicit fear. Items associated with the least amount of fear are positioned at the bottom of the list; most feared items are placed near the top. For example, if a client has a strong fear of dogs,
  • 12. the therapist and client would develop a list of scenes that make the client fearful. Situations may vary from hearing the word “dog” to seeing pictures of dogs, being in the vicinity of a dog, hearing a dog bark, being close to dogs, and patting a dog. The client is next taught to relax by tensing and releasing various groups of muscles— shoulders, face, arms, neck, and so on. This phase of treatment ends when the client has learned to fully relax on his or her own in a matter of minutes. The client and therapist now move on to the next phase of therapy. While remaining fully relaxed, the client is asked to imagine being in the first situation at the bottom of the list. The image is held for several seconds. The client then relaxes for about twenty seconds before imagining the same situation again for several seconds. When the client is able to imagine an item and remain fully relaxed, the therapist presents a slightly more fearful situation to imagine. This procedure continues until an image causes distress, at which time the session ends. The next session begins with relaxation, followed by the client slowly moving up the list. As before, the client stops at the point of distress. Therapy is successful when the client can imagine all the items on the list while remaining fully relaxed. The technique is less helpful when clients have difficulty identifying fearful situations or calling up vivid images. In the hands of a skillful therapist, systematic desensitization is an effective technique for
  • 13. reducing a wide variety of fears. Its Pavlovian features involve pairing imagined fearful scenes with relaxation. When relaxation successfully competes with fear, it becomes a new CR to the imagined scenes. As relaxation becomes sufficiently strong as a CR, anxiety is replaced by calmness in the face of earlier aversive stimuli. Extinction offers a more direct route to the reduction of fear than systematic desensitization. The technique called flooding makes use of extinction. Flooding exposes the client to fear-arousing stimuli for a prolonged period of time. Suppose a child is afraid of snakes. Although fear is likely to increase initially, flooding would require the child to confront the snake directly and continuously—to be “flooded” by various stimuli associated with the snake—until the conditioned stimuli lose their power to elicit fear. Some therapists think that the application of this technique is probably best left to professionals. Some Everyday Examples Pavlovian principles may be plausibly applied to daily life, as the following examples illustrate. Couples sometimes refer to a certain tune as “our song.” A plausible interpretation is that Pavlovian conditioning has been at work. The favored tune may have been popular and repeated often at the time of the couple’s courtship and marriage. The tune has since become a CS that evokes a variety of pleasant feelings associated with
  • 14. initial love. A babysitter notes that giving a young child a blue blanket in the absence of his mother markedly reduces his irritability. Most likely the blanket has been sufficiently associated with the soothing actions of his mother (US) and now functions as a calming stimulus (CS). An adolescent steadfastly avoids the location where he was seriously injured in an automobile accident. He says that just thinking about the highway makes him nervous. The location doubtless contains a number of conditioned aversive stimuli that now trigger unpleasant feelings (CR) and avoidance. After a bitter divorce, a woman finds that the sight of household items (CS) associated with her former husband is terribly upsetting (CR). She has reduced her resentment by getting rid of the offending items. A wife often places flower arrangements in her husband’s den. The flowers (CS) now bring him a measure of comfort (CR) when she is away on trips. Respondent Conditioning and Reinforcement Pavlovian behaviors are principally elicited by antecedent events (just as low temperatures elicit shivering), while many behaviors are strengthened (in reinforcement) or weakened (in punishment) by what follows behavior. In Pavlovian conditioning, two stimuli are presented, one following another, regardless of what a subject does. What
  • 15. follows behavior is usually not important in this form of conditioning. In studying the role of reinforcement on behavior (instrumental or operant conditioning), the consequences that follow a person’s actions often determine what the person is likely to do under similar circumstances in the future. What follows behavior is important in this type of conditioning. The topic of reinforcement is introduced here because Pavlovian conditioning and reinforcement are intricately related in that any Pavlovian conditioning is likely to contain elements of instrumental conditioning, and vice versa. For example, if someone has a near-drowning experience and now avoids bodies of water, it is plausible to say that conditioned stimuli associated with the experience evoke unsettling feelings. The person reduces the unpleasant feelings by avoiding bodies of water. In this example, negative feelings are conditioned according to Pavlovian principles. The avoidance reaction is maintained by (negative) reinforcement and involves instrumental learning. Virtually all the previous examples can be analyzed similarly. Bibliography Baldwin, John D., and Janice I. Baldwin. Behavior Principles in Everyday Life. 4th ed. Upper Saddle River: Prentice Hall, 2001. Print.
  • 16. Dance, Scott. "Johns Hopkins Neuroscientists Trace What Made Pavlov's Dog Salivate." Baltimore Sun. Tribune, 6 Dec. 2015. Web. 23 Feb. 2016. Hergenhahn, B. R. An Introduction to the History of Psychology. 6th ed. Belmont: Wadsworth, 2009. Print. Levis, Donald J. Foundations of Behavioral Therapy. New Brunswick: Transaction, 2010. Print. "Pavlovian Test Finds Sleeping Consciousness." New Scientist 26 Sept. 2009: 18. Print. Ramnerö, Jonas, and Niklas Törneke. ABCs of Human Behavior: Behavioral Principles for the Practicing Clinician. Oakland: New Harbinger, 2008. Print. Redish, A. David. The Mind Within the Brain. Oxford: Oxford UP, 2013. Print. Rescorla, Robert A. “Pavlovian Conditioning: It’s Not What You Think It Is.” American Psychologist 43.3 (1988): 151–60. Print Watson, J. B., and R. Rayner. “Conditioned Emotional Reactions.” Journal of Experimental Psychology 3 (1920): 1–14. Print. Wolpe, Joseph. The Practice of Behavior Therapy. 4th ed. Boston: Allyn, 2008. Print. Copyright of Salem Press Encyclopedia of Health is the property of Salem Press. The
  • 17. copyright in an individual article may be maintained by the author in certain cases. Content may not be copied or emailed to multiple sites or posted to a listserv without the copyright holder's express written permission. However, users may print, download, or email articles for individual use. Source: Salem Press Encyclopedia of Health, 2019, 5p Item: 93872139 Google Book Preview Goodreads- User Reviews Plum Print Title: Database: Operant conditioning. By: Rholetter, Wylene, MEd, Salem Press Encyclopedia, 2019 Research Starters Operant conditioning Export Manager Save E-mail Number of items to be saved: 1 Save citations to a file formatted for: Direct Export in RIS Format (e.g. CITAVI, EasyBib, EndNote,
  • 18. ProCite, Reference Manager, Zotero) Generic bibliographic management software Citations in XML format Citations in BibTeX format Citations in MARC21 format Direct Export to RefWorks Direct Export to EndNote Web Direct Export to EasyBib Download CSV Direct Export to NoodleTools Save Cancel x UAGC Library-test Listen The hierarchy of operant conditioning. By Studentne [GFDL (http://www.gnu.org/copyleft/fdl.html)
  • 19. or CC-BY-SA-3.0 (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by- sa/3.0)], via Wikimedia Commons Operant conditioning, a term coined by B. F. Skinner, American psychologist and radical behaviorist, is the idea that behavior is the learned result of consequences. Skinner, who introduced the concept in his 1938 book The Behavior of Organisms: An Experimental Analysis, theorized that operant conditioning in the form of reinforcements and punishments leads to an association between a behavior and its consequence. Positive reinforcement increases a desirable behavior by following it with a favorable stimulus. Negative reinforcement increases a desirable behavior by removing an unfavorable stimulus after the behavior is performed. Both positive and negative reinforcement seek to increase a desirable behavior. Punishment, like reinforcement, also has positive and negative varieties. Positive punishment is adding an unfavorable stimulus in an effort to eradicate an undesirable behavior. Negative punishment is removing an unpleasant stimulus in order to decrease undesirable behavior. Both positive and negative punishment seek to decrease an undesirable behavior. Overview Skinner designed an operant conditioning chamber, which came to be known as the Skinner box, to test his theory of operant conditioning on animals. The Skinner box
  • 20. prevented human interruption of the experimental session and allowed the experimenter to study the behavior of an animal as a continuous process. The box includes at least one lever or key that the animal can manipulate to release food, water, or some other reward or to avoid punishment such as an electric shock. Skinner’s experiments with rats and pigeons showed that the animals first hit the lever and released food accidentally; after a few accidental releases, the reinforcement of manipulating the lever ensured that the behavior would be repeated. Skinner believed that operant conditioning could be used in similar ways with human beings. Modifying behavior through operant conditioning has been used in the treatment of phobias, obsessive-compulsive disorders, substance-abuse problems, and some sexual disorders, but the impact of Skinner’s theories about operant conditioning has proved to be immense, reaching far beyond the field of psychology. Zoos and other animal facilities routinely use food as a positive reinforcement to train animals to move within enclosed areas and to increase safety during veterinary examinations. With human subjects, operant conditioning has been used to control absenteeism in the workplace (such as when employers offer staff members with no absences a chance to win cash rewards), to increase sales (coupons), and to manage agitation in older adults with dementia. Perhaps no field has been more influenced by operant conditioning than education. Skinner’s assertion that positive reinforcement is more effective
  • 21. than punishment at changing and establishing desirable behavior led to the discrediting of punitive punishment in schools and the common application of timeouts (negative reinforcement) and a token economy (i.e., rewarding good behavior with gold stars that can be accumulated for prizes) instead. Critics of operant conditioning have been vehement in pointing out its detriments. As early as 1959, American linguist and cognitive scientist Noam Chomsky argued that what worked in Skinner’s laboratory could be applied to complex human behavior only in a superficial way. In 1960 progressive educator A. S. Neil insisted that rewarding good behavior taught that the behavior was not worth doing for reasons other than the reward. Other critics were even more severe, charging that operant conditioning was dangerous and inhumane. Gradually, the influence of Skinner’s ideas declined, and by the twenty-first century, some declared that operant conditioning had become peripheral in psychology and related fields. However, in 2002 a list of ninety-nine top psychologists was published in the Review of General Psychology and B. F. Skinner topped the list. Bibliography Bunzli, Samantha, David Gillham, and Adrian Esterman. “Physiotherapy-Provided Operant Conditioning in the Management of Low Back Pain Disability: A Systematic Review.” Physiotherapy Research International 16.1 (2011): 4– 19. Academic Search Premier.
  • 22. Web. 7 Aug. 2013. Davey, Graham, and Chris Cullen. Human Operant Conditioning and Behavior Modification. New York: Wiley, 1988. Print. Dayan, Peter. “Instrumental Vigour in Punishment and Reward.” European Journal of Neuroscience 35.7 (2012): 1152–68. Academic Search Premier. Web. 7 Aug. 2013. Dayan, Peter, et al. “Disentangling the Roles of Approach, Activation and Valence in Instrumental and Pavlovian Responding.” PLoS Computational Biology 7.4 (2011): 1– 28. Academic Search Premier. Web. 7 Aug. 2013. Edwards, Darren J. Integrating Behavioural and Cognitive Psychology: A Modern Categorization Theoretical Approach. Hauppage: Nova, 2015. Print. Fonseca, Amilcar Rodrigues, Maria Cristina Zago Castelli, and Emileane Costa Assis de Oliveira. "Effects of Chronic Mild Stress on Operant Discrimination Learning." Behavior Analysis: Research and Practice 15.1 (2015): 20–27. Print. Iversen, Iver H. “Skinner’s Early Research: From Reflexology to Operant Conditioning.” American Psychologist 47.11 (1992): 1318–28. PsycINFO. Web. 24 July 2013. Miller, Harold L., Jr., and E. Benjamin H. Heuston. “Recent Trends in Operant Conditioning.”
  • 23. 21st Century Psychology: A Reference Handbook. Eds. Stephen F. Davis et al. Los Angeles: Sage, 2008, 340–50. Print. Murphy, Eric S., and Frances K. McSweeney. The Wiley- Blackwell Handbook of Operant and Classical Conditioning. Hoboken: Wiley, 2014. Print. Parrish, Margaret. “Behaviorism.” Social Work Perspectives on Human Behavior. Maidenhead: Open UP, 2010, 98–109. Print. Rapanelli, Maximiliano, Luciana Romina Frick, and Bonifacio Silvano Zanutto. “Learning an Operant Conditioning Task Differentially Induces Gliogenesis in the Medial Prefrontal Cortex and Neurogenesis in the Hippocampus.” PLoS ONE 6.2 (2011): 1– 12. Academic Search Premier. Web. 7 Aug. 2013. Reynolds, George Stanley. A Primer of Operant Conditioning. Rev. ed. Glenview: Scott, 1975. Print. Staddon, J. E. R., and D. T. Cerutti. “Operant Conditioning.” Annual Review of Psychology 54 (2003): 115–44. PsycINFO. Web. 24 July 2013. Copyright of Salem Press Encyclopedia is the property of Salem Press. The copyright in an individual article may be maintained by the author in certain cases. Content may not be copied or emailed to multiple sites or posted to a listserv without the copyright holder's express written permission. However, users may print, download, or email articles for individual use. Source:
  • 24. Salem Press Encyclopedia, 2019, 2p Item: 89677602 Google Book Preview Goodreads- User Reviews Plum Print 172 asrt.org/publications Editorial Learning Theories: Behaviorism Kevin R Clark, EdD, R.T.(R)(QM) I n its simplest form, learning is defined as gaining knowledge through study, teaching, instruction, or experience.1 Interestingly, learning is described and viewed differently by theorists, researchers, and practitioners who have spent time investigating and experimenting in the educational psychology field.1,2 The differences in how educational theorists believe individuals acquire, retain, and recall knowledge result- ed in the development of multiple learning theories.1-3 Based on the context of the theorists’ work and other factors at the time of investigation, these theories explain how learning occurs, what internal or external
  • 25. factors inf luence learning, how memory affects learn- ing, and how transfer of knowledge occurs.1-3 In addi- tion, the roles of the instructors and learners are described according to each theory of learning. A basic understanding of the various learning theories is essen- tial for educators who strive to lead a classroom that is conducive to learning and success. The ideas of behaviorism date back to the late 19th and early 20th centuries when John Watson, an American psychologist, believed the general public would accept and recognize the new philosophy of psy- chology as a true science only if it involved processes of objective observation and scientific measurement.1 This notion of detailed observation and measurement became central to the work of behaviorists.1 Behaviorism emphasizes that learning occurs when an individual responds favorably to some type of external stimuli.1-4 Behaviorism sometimes is referred to as the stimulus-response theory.1 For example, when presented with a math f lashcard showing the equation 6 3 8, the learner responds with the answer 48. The equation is the stimulus, and the answer is the associ- ated response.2 Essential elements with behaviorism include the stimulus, the response, and the association between these 2 elements.2 Of particular importance is how the association between the stimulus and the response is made, strengthened, and maintained.2 Behaviorists define learning as nothing more than the acquisition of new behaviors. Behaviorists do not emphasize thinking or other mental activities as a part of the learning process because such variables are not observable behaviors.1-4 Although the behaviorism
  • 26. theory discounts any mental activity, other educational theorists considered these processes to be a vital part of learning and cognition, which resulted in the develop- ment of other theories of learning.1,4 Behaviorists do not address memory and how new behaviors or changes in behaviors are stored or recalled for future use.2 Behaviorists refer to this type of learning, where a reac- tion is made to a particular stimulus, as conditioning.1 Two main types of conditioning include Pavlov’s classi - cal conditioning and Skinner’s operant conditioning. Classical Conditioning Ivan Pavlov, a Russian physiologist, noticed that dogs salivated every time they ate or saw food and believed 173RADIOLOGIC TECHNOLOGY, November/December 2018, Volume 90, Number 2 Editorial Clark bowling alley.1 Skinner made generalizations about his findings with rats and pigeons to humans.1 In addition, he noted that operant conditioning also worked in a negative way: stopping a behavior from occurring by punishing it.1,3 Reinforcement and Punishment Key aspects of operant conditioning include rein- forcement and punishment, both of which can be positive or negative. Reinforcement refers to anything that has the effect of strengthening a particular behav-
  • 27. ior for it to occur again.1,3 Positive reinforcement is the addition of a rewarding stimulus to get the behavior to happen again (eg, rewarding learners for making a high grade on an exam in hopes they study harder for future assessments and score high again). Negative reinforce- ment is the removal of an unpleasant stimulus to get the behavior to continue (eg, students learning the rules to solve a particular problem so their instructor quits nag- ging them about the importance of it). The unpleasant behavior of the instructor’s nagging is removed when students learn the rules, solve the problem correctly, and continue the action so the nagging does not return. Conversely, punishment refers to anything that has an effect of lessening or discouraging a particular behavior so that it does not occur again.1,3 Positive pun- ishment is the addition of an unpleasant stimulus to get the behavior to stop; any type of disciplinary action is considered positive punishment. Negative punishment is the removal of a rewarding stimulus to get the behav- ior to stop (eg, not offering extra credit opportunities in hopes the behavior stops so that the learners can receive these beneficial opportunities in the future). Skinner maintained that rewards and punishments control most human behaviors.1-3 In addition to Watson, Pavlov, and Skinner, other theorists were associated with the behaviorist move- ment. The Table summarizes their contributions to the theory of behaviorism. Implications in Teaching and Learning Behaviorists believe learning begins when a cue or stimulus from the environment is presented, and the learner reacts to the stimulus with some type of
  • 28. response.1-3 Those responses are reinforced or punished, he could condition the dogs to salivate at the sound of a bell.1 Initially, Pavlov sounded a bell at the time food was presented to the dogs and repeated this process frequently.1 Eventually, the sound of the bell became an indication to the dogs that food was about to be pre- sented, and they responded by salivating at the sound of the bell regardless of whether food was presented.1 This type of reinforcement of a natural ref lex or some involuntary behavior that occurs as a response to a par- ticular stimulus is called classical conditioning.1 Pavlov was able to condition the dogs to salivate in response to the sound of the bell. Pavlov identified 4 stages of classical conditioning: acquisition, extinction, generalization, and discrimina- tion.1 The acquisition stage is the initial learning of the conditioned response (the dogs salivating at the sound of the bell).1 Pavlov believed the conditioned response would not remain indefinitely, so he used the term extinction to describe the disappearance of a con- ditioned response.1 Pavlov demonstrated extinction by repeatedly sounding the bell without presenting food to the dogs.1 The final 2 stages, generalization and dis- crimination, are opposites and explain how behaviorists believe knowledge is transferred within learners.2 The generalization stage implies that a conditioned response might occur with similar stimuli without further train- ing (the dogs salivating at the sound of something similar to a bell).1 In contrast, the discrimination stage indicates that a conditioned response might occur with 1 stimulus but not with another (the dogs not salivating at the sound of something similar to a bell).1 Operant Conditioning
  • 29. BF Skinner, a psychologist working in the United States in the 1930s, established the theory of oper- ant conditioning: a process of reinforcing a voluntary behavior by rewarding it.1,3 Studying the behaviors of rats, Skinner used a device (now called a Skinner box) that contained a lever.1 W henever the rats pressed the lever (an action Skinner considered normal, random, and voluntary), a pellet of food was presented.1 As the food rewards continued during the repetition of the action, the rats learned that they had to press the lever to be fed.1 Skinner also used reinforcement techniques to teach pigeons to dance and to roll a ball down a mini 174 asrt.org/publications Editorial Learning Theories: Behaviorism the reinforcement of appropriate classroom behaviors, which can create a more orderly classroom environment that is conducive to learning and success for all.1 Learning Activities Classroom learning activities connected to the behaviorism theory include1-3: � lecturing � recalling facts � defining and illustrating concepts � applying explanations � participating in rote learning (ie, memorization based on repetition)
  • 30. � completing drill and practice exercises � establishing classroom management policies � using rewards and punishments Implications in Medical Imaging Education In medical imaging education, lecturing is a domi- nant approach to presenting information because of the complexity of the content. Considering time man- agement issues and restrictions in higher education, lecturing affords instructors an opportunity to pres- ent a large amount of information to a large audience. Often, medical imaging students memorize some of the content presented and recall that knowledge during an exam. The role of repetition aids in the learning of new and challenging content. Medical imaging students benefit from drill and practice exercises when working with formulas, including the Inverse-Square Law, the milliampere-seconds–distance compensation formula, and this process is repeated so that the responses become automatic.3 Ultimately, the change in behav- ior indicates learning has occurred.3 As revealed, behaviorism has little regard for mental processes or understanding and, therefore, does not prepare learners for problem-solving or critical-thinking skills.1-3 The instructor plays a dominant role in behaviorism by leading the learning environment, using positive and negative reinforcement to shape learners’ behaviors, and presenting the content.1 With behaviorism, learn- ers are described as passive individuals who voluntarily respond to external stimuli.1 Other behaviorist implica- tions in teaching and learning include1: � creating procedures and expectations to manage
  • 31. the classroom � using rewards as incentives for learners to work hard and behave � using punishments (eg, loss of privileges or with- holding of rewards) effectively and sparingly to change learners’ behaviors Critics of behaviorism argue that rewards can belittle or demean a learning experience and, therefore, should be used with caution.1 Often, rewards can evoke feel- ings of unfairness or competition, and some learners might become distracted from the real issue involved in completing a task or learning new material.1 Using a rewards system or giving 1 learner increased attention might have a detrimental effect on others in the class or cause them to feel isolated.1 Not surprisingly, rewards do not always lead to higher-quality work; however, using a behaviorist approach, rewards can result in Table Key Theorists and Their Contributions to Behaviorism1 Theorists Contribution Ivan Pavlov Classical conditioning Edward Thorndike Connectionism (emphasized the role of experience in the strengthening and weakening of stimulus- response connections) John Watson Scientific objectivity; Law of frequency (the more frequent a stimulus and response occur in association with
  • 32. each other, the stronger the habit will become) Edwin Guthrie Contiguity (the same response to a stimulus most likely will occur over and over again during repeated expo- sures) BF Skinner Operant conditioning 175RADIOLOGIC TECHNOLOGY, November/December 2018, Volume 90, Number 2 Editorial Clark and the grid conversion formula, as well as calculations involving skin dose. Medical imaging instructors ben- efit from using a behaviorist approach by implementing a classroom management plan to lead a classroom con- ducive to learning and success. Conclusion The theory of behaviorism can be illustrated by the adage, “practice makes perfect.” Behaviorists see learning as an observable change in behavior as a result of experience and repetition. This stimulus-response theory makes no attempt to assess the mental processes necessary for learners to acquire, retain, and recall information. The change in behavior is simply achieved through a conditioning process using reinforcement and punishment. Even though little importance is placed on mental activity, concept formation, or under- standing, there is a place for behaviorism in today’s classrooms, especially in medical imaging education, in
  • 33. the areas of rote learning and classroom management. Kevin R Clark, EdD, R.T.(R)(QM), is assistant professor and graduate coordinator for the School of Health Professions at The University of Texas MD Anderson Cancer Center in Houston. He serves on the Radiologic Technology Editorial Review Board and can be reached at [email protected] References 1. Pritchard A. Behaviourism and the beginnings of theory. In: Ways of Learning – Learning Theories and Learning Styles in the Classroom. 3rd ed. New York, NY: Routledge; 2014:6-17. 2. Ertmer PA, Newby TJ. Behaviorism, cognitivism, construc- tivism: comparing critical features from an instructional design perspective. Perform Improv Q. 2013;26(2):43-71. doi:10.1002/piq.21143. 3. Kelly J. Learning theories. The Peak Performance Center website. http://thepeakperformancecenter.com/education al-learning/learning/theories/. Published September 2012. Accessed June 10, 2017. 4. David L. Behaviorism. Learning Theories website. https:// www.learning-theories.com/behaviorism.html. Published January 31, 2007. Accessed June 10, 2017. https://doi.org/10.1002/piq.21143 Copyright of Radiologic Technology is the property of American Society of Radiologic Technologists and its content may not be copied or emailed to multiple sites or posted to a listserv without the copyright holder's express written
  • 34. permission. However, users may print, download, or email articles for individual use. ORIGINAL ARTICLE If Everyone Is Doing It, It Must Be Safe: College Students’ Development of Attitudes toward Poly-Substance Use Erin Willisa , Robyn Adamsb, and Justin Keeneb aAdvertising, Public Relations & Media Design, University of Colorado Boulder, Boulder, Colorado, USA; bCreative Media Industries, College of Media and Communication, Texas Tech University, Lubbock, Texas, USA ABSTRACT Background: While binge drinking on college campuses has been a topic of concern for dec- ades, especially among fraternity and sorority members, recreational drug use is on the rise and mixing alcohol and drugs is now more of a concern than ever. Objective: Social learning theory was used as a framework for understanding how students develop attitudes regard- ing the possible risks and rewards of various behaviors such as binge drinking and drug use. Method: This research reports the results of 13 focus group discussions with 63 college students. A thematic approach was used and revealed several themes: participating in col- lege culture, experimenting is expected, ignoring risk-taking,
  • 35. and resisting peer pressure. Findings: Participants felt as if it was expected that college students would experiment with alcohol and drugs, and that it was just “part of going away to college.” Students reported ignoring the known risks of mixing alcohol and drugs use despite prior education efforts. Conclusions: The findings of this study suggest that alcohol and drug use on college cam- puses is, at least in part, driven by a perception of college culture and a poor balancing of the risks and rewards associated with these behaviors. KEYWORDS Binge drinking; social learning theory; focus groups; non-medical use of prescription medication; risky behavior Over one-third of full-time college students (18–22) engaged in binge drinking in the past month; about one in five used an illicit drug in the past month (Center for Behavioral Health Statistics & Quality, 2015). The Centers for Disease Control and Prevention (2017) characterize substance use as the foremost public health hazard facing college students. Substance use creates negative health, social, and eco- nomic consequences for students, their families, and their communities (National Institute on Drug Abuse, 2017; Substance Abuse and Mental Health Services, 2006). While binge drinking on college campuses has been a topic of concern for decades (Hahm, Kolaczyk, Jang, Swenson, & Bhindarwala, 2012; Wechsler, Lee, Kuo, & Lee, 2000; White & Hingson, 2013), recre- ational drug use is on the rise and poly-substance use
  • 36. (mixing alcohol and drugs) is now more of a concern than ever (CDC, 2017). In addition, much of this poly-substance use involves prescription medication in lieu of more illicit drugs (e.g., cocaine, marijuana). Young people who engage in the non-medical use of prescription medications have an increased risk of using other drugs (e.g., alcohol, marijuana), suffering from health issues (e.g., weight gain, mental health problems), and engaging in risky behaviors (e.g., unprotected sex, criminal activity) (Ford & Arrastia, 2008). Exposure and access to prescription medication is high on college campuses with 61% of college students being offered these medications at least once, and 31% using them non-medically (Garnier-Dykstra, Caldeira, Vincent, O’Grady, & Arria, 2012). In addition, stu- dents often overestimate the risky behaviors of their peers; they overestimate stimulant use by 12.2% and pain medication use by 8.8%, whereas marijuana use by only 2.9% (McCabe, 2008). Such an overestimation of risky behavior likely influences students’ percep- tions of the risks associated with particular behaviors and shifts their likelihood of partaking in such behav- ior in the future. Many universities and colleges now require stu- dents to complete alcohol education programs prior to arriving on campus (Croom et al., 2009). Previous research notes that these brief alcohol interventions yield only modest results, and that education alone is not effective (Carey, Scott-Sheldon, Garey, Elliott, & Carey, 2016; Tanner-Smith & Lipsey, 2015). Thus, this study has two goals: (1) to better understand the role
  • 37. CONTACT Erin Willis [email protected] University of Colorado Boulder, 1511 University Ave, Boulder, CO, 478 UCB, 80309, USA. � 2019 Taylor & Francis Group, LLC SUBSTANCE USE & MISUSE 2019, VOL. 54, NO. 11, 1886–1893 https://doi.org/10.1080/10826084.2019.1618334 http://crossmark.crossref.org/dialog/?doi=10.1080/10826084.20 19.1618334&domain=pdf&date_stamp=2019-06-28 http://orcid.org/0000-0002-1582-0867 http://orcid.org/0000-0002-1404-0025 https://doi.org./10.1080/10826084.2019.1618334 http://www.tandfonline.com of social influence and peer behavior on the creation and maintenance of attitudes toward various sub- stance use behaviors, like mixing alcohol and drugs; (2) to better understand how these attitudes interact with perceptions of rewarding outcomes and risky consequences to influence planned behavior. The goals of this study add to our understanding of college stu- dents’ decision-making processes, and better inform health education and promotion targeted at new and incoming students. We employed qualitative method- ology to gain insight into college students’ perspec- tives specific to the social learning theory. The results are discussed within the context of this theory with particular emphasis on the practical outcomes associ- ated with cessation efforts on college campuses related to education and intervention. Social learning theory
  • 38. Social learning theory provides a theoretical frame- work for understanding risk-taking behaviors among college students. The theory posits that people can learn by observing and modeling others’ behaviors (Bandura, 1977). Deviant behavior is learned and pri - mary groups, such as peer groups, play a central role in this learning. One place where the influence of peers is prevalent is college campuses. Indeed, collegi - ate peer use of alcohol often determines individual use, and peer norms predict binge drinking (Bandura, 1977; Crawford & Novak, 2010; Read, Wood, Kahler, Maddock, & Palfai, 2003; Sher, Bartholow, & Nanda, 2001; Tyler, Schmitz, Ray, Adams, & Gordon Simons, 2017). Previous research also notes that many college students have positive attitudes toward alcohol use (Peralta & Steele, 2010; Schultz, Nolan, Cialdini, Goldstein, & Griskevicius, 2007; Wechsler et al., 2003), have well-defined reasons for drinking, for instance, mood enhancement or reducing stress (O’Connor & Colder, 2005; O’Hara, Armeli, & Tennen, 2015). In addition, the perceived benefits (e.g., social interaction, fun/enjoyment) of drinking are significant predictors of alcohol use (Brooks-Russel, Simons- Morton, Haynie, Farhat, & Wang, 2014). Prior work has demonstrated that first-year students are highly susceptible to modeling the behavior of their older peers, and are at the highest risk for the negative con- sequences of alcohol use (Armeli, Conner, Cullum, & Tennen, 2010; Maggs, Williams, & Lee, 2011). Students with the highest likelihood of engaging in multiple health-risk behaviors reported poorer mental health, particularly related to stress and anxiety (Martinez, Klanecky, & McChargue, 2018). Perceived norms influence college students’ level of drinking
  • 39. through the observation and comparison of their peers’ drinking levels (Fournier, Hall, Ricke, & Storey, 2013; Stappenbeck, Quinn, Wetherill, & Fromme, 2010). The prediction here is that risky substance use on college campuses is, at least in part, a product of social learning processes that lead to attitudes regard- ing specific substances and situations. Differential association and reinforcement There are several key elements to the learning process, including differential association and differential reinforcement (Akers, 2011). Differential association is the association with individuals who engage in certain types of conduct, as well as the exposure to different sets of values and norms as a consequence of such associations (Akers, 2011). For example, over 70% of students nationwide overestimated the quantity of alcohol consumed by their peers; further, the percep- tion of campus drinking norms was by far the stron- gest predictor of personal consumption, stronger even than the actual campus drinking norm (Wesley Perkins, Haines, & Rice, 2005). Differential reinforcement is “the balance of antici- pated and actual rewards and punishments that follow or are consequences of behavior” (Akers, 2000, p. 78). Within the context of alcohol use, this could take the form of several different outcomes (e.g., hangovers, Driving While Impaired (DWIs), alcohol poisoning). However, as Durkin, Wolfe, and Clark (2005) demon- strated, college-aged binge drinkers reported that alco- hol consumption has more rewarding outcomes than negative consequences. In addition to the prediction related to the role of
  • 40. social learning processes in risky behaviors, the cur- rent study also seeks to understand how these social learning processes interact with prior knowledge regarding the risky nature of certain behaviors. By concentrating on the differential associations and rein- forcements regarding binge drinking and drug use among college students, this study explores how atti- tudes are formed and how behaviors are reinforced by perceptions of normative behavior within peer groups. This research fills a gap in the literature related to the qualitative exploration of college students’ perceptions of poly-substance use and risk-taking behaviors. Method This study used focus groups drawn from a larger stu- dent population at a southwestern university. Focus SUBSTANCE USE & MISUSE 1887 groups provide insights into a target audience’s per- ceptions and motivations (Krueger & Casey, 2015), and can capture the complexities of attitude and behavioral intentions (Kitzinger, 1994). Enrollment at the southwestern university was approximately 37,000 students. The college students were recruited from general media and communication studies courses via an online recruitment system, and they received extra course credit for their participation. This research study was approved by the southwestern university’s institutional review board. The key ethical considera- tions reviewed for this study relate to informed con- sent, confidentiality, and the right to withdraw. Participants had to be at least 18 years old to register
  • 41. for the study and be enrolled at the southwestern uni- versity. The recruitment procedures, discussion guide, transcription process, and data analysis were approved by the IRB. Age and university enrollment were the only exclusion criterion; participants were not excluded based on substance use history. Thirteen group discussions were held in October 2017 with a total of 63 college students (27 men, 36 women; 3–8 per group) who were between 18 and 25 years old. Prior to the focus group discussions, a trained moderator reviewed the goals of the study, consent forms, and the right to withdraw with the participants. One author, who had received training in conducting focus group discussions, moderated each semi-structured focus group discussion. The duration of the focus groups ranged from 45 to 80 min. A dis- cussion guide was developed to probe participants’ per - ceptions of the college “party scene” and substance use on campus. Open-ended questions helped minimize researchers’ bias and allow participants to respond. Following open-ended questions, probing questions focused on participants’ feedback. Sample questions included: What role does alcohol play in college for you and your friends? When drinking alcohol, are there times when drugs are around? In your experience, is it common for your peers to do drugs? What social pres- sures do you feel regarding drinking/drug use? The focus groups were audio-recorded, transcribed verbatim by an online transcription service, and then analyzed using a thematic approach (Miles & Huberman, 1994). Thematic analysis identifies themes that emerge as being important to the description of a phenomenon; it is a form of pattern recognition within the data, while emerging themes become cate-
  • 42. gories for analysis (Miles & Huberman, 1994). The theoretical proposition that led this study – social learning theory, specifically differential associations and reinforcements – was used to assist in the development of themes. This research used Miles and Huberman’s data-analysis procedures. The first author reviewed all transcripts and derived a set of themes from the discussions, and the second and third authors independently reviewed all transcripts and the appro- priateness of the themes derived by the first author. Disagreements on themes were resolved through dis- cussion. The first author then coded all of the tran- scripts by categorizing relevant statements in the transcripts under themes (Stappenbeck et al., 2010). The second and third authors then reviewed the coded statements, and disagreements on the codes were resolved. This rigorous analysis by multiple researchers enhanced the reliability of the themes (Miles & Huberman, 1994). The number of focus groups needed was determined by saturation, or the point where no new themes emerged (Krueger & Casey, 2015). Findings The current study used the social learning theory as the analytical framework for identifying themes. Focusing on differential associations and differential reinforcements related to “partying” and substance use, the following themes emerged: participating in college culture, experimenting is expected, ignoring risk-taking, and resisting peer pressure, and are described below. Participating in college culture
  • 43. The majority of participants felt that alcohol and drug use is part of attending college, and that most college students engage in this type of behavior as part of socializing. Five participants reported not drinking due to various factors including religion, addiction, or hiatus. Several participants mentioned that there was “nothing else to do” or that drinking “is just – you don’t think about it, it’s what you do.” The partici- pants felt as if “partying” was expected of them since they were in college, “that’s what college students do.” I mean in general I feel like when you want to do something, it’s always like centered around drinking. Like it’s kind of like the social thing to do. I think it’s just sort of the college thing. (Female, 19, Group #5) You feel more inclined to drink since everybody, or a lot of people, are doing it in this demographic area. (Male, 21, Group #1) I just can’t imagine being here without alcohol. (Female, 22, Group #2) Some participants even chose to attend this particu- lar university because of its “party school” reputation. 1888 E. WILLIS ET AL. Many participants reported binge drinking more while underage than after they turned age 21 due to several factors, including being new to alcohol and not know - ing their limits or lack of access. However, many par- ticipants reported “having a drink at dinner” and “just staying home on the weekends and drinking” rather
  • 44. than frequenting bars after they were legally allowed to drink. I mean, I’m not 21 anymore. I don’t go out and get lit every weekend. Yeah, we might drink at home, but it’s not like going to the bars. (Female, 22, Group #6) We will just kick back and chill sometimes at the house … might have a few people stop by, but it’s very chill. (Male, 22, Group #10) All of the participants mentioned football games and tailgating specifically as times where “drinking is taken to the next level,” meaning that people are drinking heavily (some reported “up to 22 drinks on game day”). “Pre-gaming,” by student definition is where five to eight drinks are consumed before going out, was commonly reported. While out at the bar, participants reported drinking more alcohol and sometimes using drugs such as cocaine, Molly, or Xanax. Participants reported keeping a running list of what types of alcohol mixed best with specific types of available drugs. While many reported experimenting with alcohol and drugs prior to college, that experi- mentation drastically increased while in college. Yeah, you get here and you see everyone drinking. Eventually, you start drinking as much as everyone else. (Female, 21, Group #9) Participants reported having more opportunities to binge drink in college, and because alcohol seemed to be at most social functions, drinking (and often, binge drinking) was expected behavior. Experimenting is expected
  • 45. Participants felt experimentation with combining drugs and alcohol was normal, and most agreed that marijuana use was very common among college stu- dents. All of the focus group participants reported seeing or using marijuana while at college. Six partici- pants reported being regular users of marijuana. I smoke weed every single day. I do everything high and I have great grades. (Female, 19, Group #8) Other drugs were reported to be popular among some crowds or in “bathrooms everywhere in the bars.” For example, half of participants reported being offered cocaine at a party. I was at a party. We’ve had [fraternity] events where a couple of my friends, they’d just be like, “hey, you want a bump or something? I’d be like, ‘yeah, why not’, it’s free, it’s nothing I would pay for because it’s not worth it to me. (Male, 21, Group #7) It was just at a gathering and some girl just had some cocaine and she was like, ‘who wants a free line?’ And I was like, ‘why not?’. (Female, 20, Group #10) Being in a social environment and having the opportunity to try the drug facilitated participants’ willingness to act. “It’s like since so many others are doing it, I just can’t see the harm, I guess.” The majority of participants reported seeing others mix drugs and alcohol. The participants who reported common drug use also reported having prior desires and positive attitudes toward such risky behavior. Ten of the participants reported actively using
  • 46. drugs recreationally, sometimes in combination with alcohol. For example, many participants reported using Xanax prior to drinking or cocaine after a long night “to sober you up.” Half of the participants reported experimenting with drugs once or twice, but then after the experience, refraining from future drug use because “it just isn’t my thing.” The remaining participants had never used drugs, but felt like it was okay for others who wanted to experiment. Ignoring risk-taking All of the participants discussed “blacking out from alcohol,” either having personal experience or hearing first-hand from friends. Participants reported “blacking out” because they either did not yet know their limits or as a planned behavior. Usually I feel like alcohol plays the biggest part in blacking out. I think people do plan to black out, because I have so many friends that are like, ‘Oh, my God, I had four tests this week. I just want to go out tonight and not remember anything’. They completely black out. I’ve blacked out before. Personally, I don’t really like it because I don’t like not remembering anything, because I feel like I did something stupid and you just don’t remember it … (Female, 21, Group #5) In terms of drug use, one participant noted that ‘everyone is medicated’ and that ‘prescription pills are everywhere’. Many reported the use of prescription pills specifically. For example, some reported friends ‘stockpiling’ prescription Xanax to use recreationally, or purchasing Adderall from friends with prescrip- tions to use while studying or ‘partying’. Participants
  • 47. reported prescription drugs as being ‘very accessible around here’. SUBSTANCE USE & MISUSE 1889 Most of the participants reported not associating marijuana with any risk, ‘probably because it’s legal in most states’. Other drugs were thought of as being dangerous, including cocaine and prescription medica- tions such as Adderall, Ambien, and Xanax, when taken with alcohol. Additionally, when asked to rank substances by most dangerous to least dangerous, most participants agreed that alcohol was the most dangerous drug, followed by prescription pills and cocaine. I probably would just say that alcohol is one of the worst because you don’t see it as a drug, but it really is because people get addicted to it every day. (Female, 24, Group #11) I’d say I think alcohol is worse [than prescription drugs]. So I would really say … of course the cocaine, most definitely, and then alcohol after that, you know what I’m saying? Not because of the long- term effects or whatever. You got to drink a whole lot to actually mess your kidneys, but just the fact that people get messed up on it, drive and stuff like that. (Male, 19, Group #2) The majority of participants reported engaging in binge drinking and/or drug use, but did not discuss the risks with friends due to several factors men- tioned, including “we learned about that in high
  • 48. school” and “that’s her life, whatever.” Participants reported drug users as not wanting to think about the risks, or “they’re not going to be proactive with it. You’ve got to ask them.” Five participants reported knowing friends who passed away from drug overdose but still reported engaging in drinking and drug use, just “in moderation.” In addition, most agreed that they would not warn strangers of the risks of sub- stance use. Participants felt as if strangers would not heed their warning, and, therefore, agreed that they would not try to dissuade a stranger from using drugs in combination with alcohol, despite the obvious risks. Resisting peer pressure The participants agreed that they felt social pressure to drink but not to do drugs. I’ve never been pressured to take any drugs. I’ve been asked, but they weren’t really as persistent as with alcohol, because I think they know that drugs is a little more than just alcohol. (Female, 20, Group #12) No one’s like, ‘Shove this [drink] down or you’re not part of the [name of school] family!’ I don’t think it’s like that, it’s just we all do it [binge drink]. (Female, 19, Group #4) There’s no pressure to do drugs … it’s just, it’s just if you want it. (Male, 18, Group #10) If participants were offered drugs and they did not want to partake, many felt as if it would be easy to decline the offer. Additionally, participants noted that drugs were common among ‘certain crowds’ and that peer pressure was about choice.
  • 49. I think there’s always going to be that social pressure. The kind of friend they’re going to be like, ‘Oh, smoke this, smoke this, smoke this’. But it’s really like at the end of the day, it’s your own decision to make. (Male, 23, Group #7) I feel that it’s like a lot who you surround yourself with and like who your friends are, and like who you choose to be around and stuff. (Female, 20, Group #11) Although it would seem drugs are commonly offered with no pressure, alcohol carries a greater social influence, “I mean, it’s hard because everyone is doing it.” The majority of participants reported drink- ing frequently, and when opting not to drink – did not mention being pressured by their peers. Most reported that their friends did not pressure them to do anything they didn’t already want to do. Well, I guess you are on Snapchat and you see all your friends out, you’re like, oh, crap. I want to go out drinking with them. I don’t want to be stuck at home. (Female, 19, Group #8) Participants reported fearing missing out on the social scene if they were not out drinking with their friends. Specifically, seeing friends having fun on social media platforms created a desire for participants to engage in the same activities. However, educational duties often dictated when participants engaged in heavy drinking, and most agreed that “drinking shouldn’t get in the way of why I’m here.” For those participants who did not drink at the
  • 50. time of the focus groups, all reported not having any difficulty resisting peer pressure in social settings whether related to drugs and alcohol. After “pre- gaming,” many participants reported drinking two to four drinks while out at the bar unless “someone buys me a drink, and yeah, I’ll drink it.” One participant said: “I’ve never seen anyone be pressured to start drinking, but I have seen people pressured to keep drinking.” Discussion This study had two goals: (1) to understand how both social influence and peer behavior influences attitudes toward various substance use behaviors, and (2) to understand how perceptions of rewarding outcomes and risky consequences interact with these attitudes to 1890 E. WILLIS ET AL. influence planned behavior. Focus groups were used to discuss binge drinking and substance use with col- lege students to understand their perceptions of these risky behaviors; specifically, the differential associa- tions and differential reinforcements that students perceive of these behaviors was probed with direct questioning. Although previous research has demon- strated that experimentation with drugs and alcohol is common and increasing among college students (Behavioral Health Coordinating Committee: Prescription Drug Abuse Subcommittee, Department of Health & Human Services, 2013), this study revealed how perceptions of peer norms and the bal - ancing of risk with reward influence behavior.
  • 51. Four themes were identified within the data: partic- ipating in college culture, experimenting is expected, ignoring risk-taking, and resisting peer pressure. Students believed they were expected to experiment with alcohol and drugs, and that sometimes included use. In every focus group, it was said at least once, “everybody’s doing it” in regard to drinking. Students also noted that their university had a “party school” reputation and that was attractive during recruitment. Marijuana was considered “normal” amongst stu- dents because it was often reported as “always being around.” Interestingly, however, students reported feeling pressure to partake in alcohol but not drugs. Both illicit and prescription drugs were common and available to students, and some reported accepting while others declined. Regardless of the type, drug use was highest in conjunction with alcohol. Although students reported knowing the risks of combining alcohol with prescription medications and illicit drugs, these risks did not outweigh the perceived rewards. Few students reported avoiding discussing the risks with their friends because “it’s their life, you know?.” Theoretical implications Theoretically, the findings from this study offer insight into the differential associations and reinforce- ments among college students related to alcohol and drug use. About half of the participants reported abstaining from drug use, while only five reported not drinking alcohol. The reported variance in drinking behavior was evidence of different peer groups at this university and how these associations influence the perception of norms. Several students reported seeing
  • 52. “hard drugs” at parties and making the decision to leave because they did not feel comfortable around illicit drug use. However, when it came to drinking, excessive consumption was permitted because “that’s what everyone else is doing.” Students reported “pre- gaming” as a common behavior prior to drinking more while out at a bar where drug use might also occur. Many students perceive alcohol and drug use as part of the college experience and thus, they per- ceive this to be the norm. Because of their desire to be part of the “in-group,” they report more reward than risk due to their perception that “everybody’s doing it.” Additionally, participants also mentioned seeing “partying” on social media applications – sometimes from peers at their same university, some- times from peers at different universities – contribu- ting to students’ perceptions that indeed, “everybody’s doing it.” In terms of differential reinforcement, students reported learning their limits “the hard way,” for example, drinking until sick, “blacking out,” and then adjusting their behavior as to avoid negative conse- quences in the future. Most of the students reported socialization and “having fun” as benefits to engaging in risk-taking behaviors, but they also acknowledge the negative consequences, for example, hangovers, sexual assault. Akers notes that differential reinforce- ment is the “balance” of rewards and consequences of engaging in a particular behavior (Akers, 2000; Maggs et al., 2011). It is clear that over time, students find a balance in the rewards and consequences of particu- larly risky behavior that is common to their perceived college experience. Interestingly, students’ evaluations of their risk-taking behaviors are determined by their
  • 53. performance in classes. If drinking or drug use was interfering with attendance or their personal (or par- ental) definition of success, then it was perceived as a problem. However, health issues were not cited as a reason for the cessation of the risky behavior. Public health implications The findings from this study inform public health interventions targeted at high school and college stu- dents specifically. Although the current study focused on college students, many said they used drugs and/or alcohol prior to attending college. Therefore, predis- positions to alcohol and drugs start prior to college, as does students’ perception of college culture; thus attempts to prevent or cessate must start earlier and grow as college progresses. More health education should be targeted to this age group to help them form an understanding of the risks of substance use, especially mixing alcohol and drugs. Realistic, targeted health messaging could help set this population’s expectations of college “partying” and demonstrate the SUBSTANCE USE & MISUSE 1891 consequences of risky behaviors. Findings from this study might be used by college university’s health cen- ters to design messages that highlight the consequen- ces of substance abuse. Additionally, many universities require students to complete alcohol education prior to their first year on campus. Often, university educa- tion includes bystander training which focuses mainly on sexual misconduct, but perhaps should also include lessons for students on substance use and discourag-
  • 54. ing peers (not just friends!) from mixing drugs and alcohol. Our research offers insight into substance abuse topics that is informed by students and should be considered when designing health messages to this audience. This research demonstrates that students are likely ignoring the negative consequences to risky behaviors in order to be accepted by their peer group and to achieve the perceived benefits of alcohol and drug use. Future public health messaging related to the use of drugs and alcohol concurrently should con- centrate on accentuating the rewards of avoiding such behavior rather than simply focusing on the risks as these risks seem to be easily dismissed by this age group. Limitations This study used a qualitative approach; focus groups cannot be generalized to a general population of col- lege students. The lack of independent coding by more than one author is a limitation. In addition, the students in the sample are from one university and, thus, future work should include other campuses across the country. Additionally, the university’s IRB limited the demographic and behavioral information that could be collected from students. Future work should attempt to probe the relationships between these differential associations and reinforcements through quantitative measures such as large-scale sur- veys or experimental designs intended to present mes- sages that might appeal to one or both of the social learning processes. Taken together, the findings of this study suggest that poly-substance use on college campuses is, at least in part, driven by a perception of college culture
  • 55. and a poor balancing of the risks and rewards associ - ated with these behaviors. Declaration of interest The authors declare that they have no conflict of interest. The authors alone are responsible for the content and writ- ing of the article. ORCID Erin Willis http://orcid.org/0000-0002-1582-0867 Justin Keene http://orcid.org/0000-0002-1404-0025 References Akers, R. L. (2000). Criminological theories: Introduction, evaluation, and application (pp. 17–21). Los Angeles, CA: Roxbury. Akers, R. L. (2011). Social learning and social structure: A general theory of crime and deviance (pp. 34–38). Piscataway, NJ: Rutgers. Armeli, S., Conner, T. S., Cullum, J., & Tennen, H. (2010). A longitudinal analysis of drinking motives moderating the negative affect-drinking association among college students. Psychology of Addictive Behaviors, 24(1), 38–47. doi:10.1037/a0017530 Bandura, A. (1977). Self-efficacy: Toward a unifying theory of behavioral change. Psychology Review, 84(2), 191–215. doi:10.1037/0033-295X.84.2.191 Behavioral Health Coordinating Committee: Prescription Drug Abuse Subcommittee, Department of Health and
  • 56. Human Services. (2013). Addressing prescription drug abuse in the United States: Current activities and future opportunities. Retrieved Jan 8, 2018 from http://www.cdc. gov/HomeandRecreationalSafety/pdf/HHS_Prescription_ Drug_Abuse_Report_09.2013.pdf. Brooks-Russel, A., Simons-Morton, B., Haynie, D., Farhat, T., & Wang, J. (2014). Longitudinal relationship between drinking with peers, descriptive norms, and adolescent alcohol use. Prevention Science, 15, 497–505. doi:10.1007/ s11121-013-0391-9 Carey, K. B., Scott-Sheldon, L. A. J., Garey, L., Elliott, J. C., & Carey, M. P. (2016). Alcohol interventions for man- dated college students: A meta-analytic review. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 84(7), 619–632. doi: 10.1037/a0040275 Center for Behavioral Health Statistics and Quality. (2015). Results from the 2014 national survey on drug use and health: Detailed tables (pp. 6–12). Rockville, MD: Substance Abuse and Mental Health Services Administration. Centers for Disease Control and Prevention. (2017). Fact sheets – Binge drinking. Retrieved Dec 15, 2017 from https://www.cdc.gov/alcohol/fact-sheets/binge-drinking.htm. Crawford, L. A., & Novak, K. B. (2010). Beliefs about alco- hol and the college experience as moderators of the effects of perceived drinking norms on student alcohol use. Journal of Alcohol and Drug Education, 54, 69–86. Croom, K., Lewis, D., Marchell, T., Lesser, M. L., Reyna, V. F., Kubicki-Bedford, L., … Staiano-Coico, L. (2009). Impact of an online alcohol education course on behavior
  • 57. and harm for incoming first-year college students: Short- term evaluation of a randomized trial. Journal of American College Health, 57(4), 445–454. doi:10.3200/ JACH.57.4.445-454 Durkin, K. F., Wolfe, T. W., & Clark, G. A. (2005). College students and binge drinking: An evaluation of the social learning theory. Sociological Spectrum, 25(3), 255–272. doi:10.1080/027321790518681 1892 E. WILLIS ET AL. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0017530 https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-295X.84.2.191 http://www.cdc.gov/HomeandRecreationalSafety/pdf/HHS_Pres cription_Drug_Abuse_Report_09.2013.pdf http://www.cdc.gov/HomeandRecreationalSafety/pdf/HHS_Pres cription_Drug_Abuse_Report_09.2013.pdf http://www.cdc.gov/HomeandRecreationalSafety/pdf/HHS_Pres cription_Drug_Abuse_Report_09.2013.pdf https://doi.org/10.1007/s11121-013-0391-9 https://doi.org/10.1007/s11121-013-0391-9 https://doi.org/10.1037/a0040275 https://www.cdc.gov/alcohol/fact-sheets/binge-drinking.htm https://doi.org/10.3200/JACH.57.4.445-454 https://doi.org/10.3200/JACH.57.4.445-454 https://doi.org/10.1080/027321790518681 Ford, J. A., & Arrastia, M. C. (2008). Pill-poppers and dop- ers: A comparison of non-medical prescription drug use and illicit/street drug use among college students. Addictive Behaviors, 33(7), 934–941. doi:10.1016/ j.addbeh.2008.02.016 Fournier, A. K., Hall, E., Ricke, P., & Storey, B. (2013).
  • 58. Alcohol and the social network: Online social networking sites and college students’ perceived drinking norms. Psychology of Popular Media Culture, 2(2), 86–95. doi: 10.1037/a0032097 Garnier-Dykstra, L. M., Caldeira, K. M., Vincent, K. B., O’Grady, K. E., & Arria, A. M. (2012). Nonmedical use of prescription stimulants during college: Four-year trends in exposure opportunity, use, motives, and sour- ces. Journal of American College Health, 60(3), 226–236. doi:10.1080/07448481.2011.589876 Hahm, H. C., Kolaczyk, E., Jang, J., Swenson, T., & Bhindarwala, A. M. (2012). Binge drinking trajectories from adolescence to young adulthood: The effects of peer social network. Substance Use & Misuse, 47, 745–756. doi:10.3109/10826084.2012.666313 Kitzinger, J. (1994). The methodology of focus groups: The importance of interaction between research participants. Sociology of Health and Illness, 16, 103–121. doi:10.1111/ 1467-9566.ep11347023 Krueger, R. A., & Casey, M. A. (2015). Focus groups: A practical guide for applied research (5th ed.; pp. 18–23). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage Publications, Inc. Maggs, J. L., Williams, L. R., & Lee, C. M. (2011). Ups and downs of alcohol use among first-year college students: Numbers of drinks, heavy drinking, and stumble and pass out drinking days. Addictive Behaviors, 36(3), 197–202. doi:10.1016/j.addbeh.2010.10.005 Martinez, H. S., Klanecky, A. K., & McChargue, D. E. (2018). Problem drinking among at-risk college students: The examination of Greek involvement, freshmen status,
  • 59. and history of mental health problems. Journal of American College Health. doi:10.1080/07448481.2018. 1432625 McCabe, S. E. (2008). Misperceptions of non-medical pre- scription drug use: A web survey of college students. Addictive Behaviors, 33(5), 713–724. doi:10.1016/ j.addbeh.2007.12.008 Miles, M. B., & Huberman, A. M. (1994). Qualitative data analysis: An expanded sourcebook (pp. 98–101). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage Publications, Inc. National Institute on Drug Abuse. (2017). Medical conse- quences of drug abuse. Retreived Dec 6, 2017 from http://www.drugabuse.gov/related-topics/medical-conse- quences-drug-abuse. O’Connor, R. M., & Colder, C. R. (2005). Predicting alcohol patterns in first-year college students through motiv- ational systems and reasons for drinking. Psychology of Addictive Behaviors, 19, 10–20. doi:10.1037/0893- 164X.19.1.10 O’Hara, R. E., Armeli, S., & Tennen, H. (2015). College stu- dents’ drinking motives and social-contextual factors: Comparing associations across levels of analysis. Psychology of Addictive Behaviors, 29, 420–429. doi: 10.1037/adb0000046 Peralta, R. L., & Steele, J. L. (2010). Nonmedical prescrip- tion drug use among US college students at a midwest university: A partial test of social learning theory. Substance Use & Misuse, 45, 865–887. doi:10.3109/ 10826080903443610
  • 60. Read, J. P., Wood, M. D., Kahler, C. W., Maddock, J. E., & Palfai, T. P. (2003). Examining the role of drinking motives in college student alcohol use and problems. Psychology of Addictive Behaviors, 17(1), 13–23. doi: 10.1037/0893-164X.17.1.13 Schultz, P. W., Nolan, J. M., Cialdini, R. B., Goldstein, N. J., & Griskevicius, V. (2007). The constructive, destructive, and reconstructive power of social norms. Psychological Science, 18(5), 429–434. doi:10.1111/j.1467- 9280.2007.01917.x Sher, K. J., Bartholow, B. D., & Nanda, S. (2001). Short- and long-term effects of fraternity and sorority member- ship on heavy drinking: A social norms perspective. Psychology of Addictive Behaviors, 15(1), 42–51. doi: 10.1037//0893-164X.15.1.42 Stappenbeck, C. A., Quinn, P. D., Wetherill, R. R., & Fromme, K. (2010). Perceived norms for drinking in the transition from high school to college and beyond. Journal of Studies on Alcohol and Drugs, 71(6), 895–903. Substance Abuse and Mental Health Services Administration. (2006). A comprehensive plan for pre- venting and reducing underage drinking. Retrieved Dec 29, 2017 from: http://www.stopalcoholabuse.gov/media/ pdf/underagerpttocongress.pdf. Tanner-Smith, E. E., & Lipsey, M. W. (2015). Brief alcohol interventions for adolescents and young adults: A system- atic review and meta-analysis. Journal of Substance Abuse Treatment, 51, 1–18. doi:10.1016/j.jsat.2014.09.001 Tyler, K. A., Schmitz, R. M., Ray, C. M., Adams, S. A., & Gordon Simons, L. (2017). The role of protective behav-
  • 61. ioral strategies, social environment, and housing type on heavy drinking among college students. Substance Use and Misuse. doi:10.1080/10826084.2017.1363235 Wechsler, H., Lee, J. E., Kuo, M., & Lee, H. (2000). College binge drinking in the 1990s: A continuing problem results of the Harvard School of Public Health 1999 College Alcohol Study. Journal of American College Health, 48(5), 199–210. doi:10.1080/07448480009599305 Wechsler, H., Nelson, T. E., Lee, J. E., Seibring, M., Lewis, C., & Keeling, R. P. (2003). Perception and reality: A national evaluation of social norms and marketing inter- ventions to reduce college students’ heavy alcohol use. Journal of Studies on Alcohol, 64(4), 484–494. doi: 10.15288/jsa.2003.64.484 Wesley Perkins, H., Haines, M. P., & Rice, R. (2005). Misperceiving the college drinking norm and related problems: A nationwide study of exposure to prevention information, perceived norms and student alcohol mis- use. Journal of Studies on Alcohol, 66(4), 470–478. doi: 10.15288/jsa.2005.66.470 White, A., & Hingson, R. (2013). The burden of alcohol use: Excessive alcohol consumption and related conse- quences among college students. Alcohol Research: Current Reviews, 35, 201–218. SUBSTANCE USE & MISUSE 1893 https://doi.org/10.1016/j.addbeh.2008.02.016 https://doi.org/10.1016/j.addbeh.2008.02.016 https://doi.org/10.1037/a0032097 https://doi.org/10.1080/07448481.2011.589876 https://doi.org/10.3109/10826084.2012.666313
  • 62. https://doi.org/10.1111/1467-9566.ep11347023 https://doi.org/10.1111/1467-9566.ep11347023 https://doi.org/10.1016/j.addbeh.2010.10.005 https://doi.org/10.1080/07448481.2018.1432625 https://doi.org/10.1080/07448481.2018.1432625 https://doi.org/10.1016/j.addbeh.2007.12.008 https://doi.org/10.1016/j.addbeh.2007.12.008 http://www.drugabuse.gov/related-topics/medical-consequences- drug-abuse http://www.drugabuse.gov/related-topics/medical-consequences- drug-abuse https://doi.org/10.1037/0893-164X.19.1.10 https://doi.org/10.1037/0893-164X.19.1.10 https://doi.org/10.1037/adb0000046 https://doi.org/10.3109/10826080903443610 https://doi.org/10.3109/10826080903443610 https://doi.org/10.1037/0893-164X.17.1.13 https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9280.2007.01917.x https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9280.2007.01917.x https://doi.org/10.1037//0893-164X.15.1.42 http://www.stopalcoholabuse.gov/media/pdf/underagerpttocongr ess.pdf http://www.stopalcoholabuse.gov/media/pdf/underagerpttocongr ess.pdf https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jsat.2014.09.001 https://doi.org/10.1080/10826084.2017.1363235 https://doi.org/10.1080/07448480009599305 https://doi.org/10.15288/jsa.2003.64.484 https://doi.org/10.15288/jsa.2005.66.470 Copyright of Substance Use & Misuse is the property of Taylor & Francis Ltd and its content may not be copied or emailed to multiple sites or posted to a listserv without the copyright holder's express written permission. However, users may print,
  • 63. download, or email articles for individual use. AbstractSocial learning theoryDifferential association and reinforcementMethodFindingsParticipating in college cultureExperimenting is expectedIgnoring risk-takingResisting peer pressureDiscussionTheoretical implicationsPublic health implicationsLimitationsDeclaration of interestReferences The Rise and Fall of Behaviorism: The Narrative and the Numbers Michiel Braat, Jan Engelen, Ties van Gemert, and Sander Verhaegh Tilburg University The history of 20th-century American psychology is often depicted as a history of the rise and fall of behaviorism. Although historians disagree about the theoretical and social factors that have contributed to the development of experimental psychology, there is widespread consensus about the growing and (later) declining influence of behaviorism between approximately 1920 and 1970. Because such wide-scope claims about the development of American psychology are typically based on small and unrepresentative samples of histor- ical data, however, the question arises to what extent the received view is justified. This article aims to answer this question in two ways. First, we use advanced scientometric tools (e.g., bibliometric mapping, cocitation analysis, and term co- occurrence analysis) to quan- titatively analyze the metadata of 119,278 articles published in
  • 64. American journals between 1920 and 1970. We reconstruct the development and structure of American psychology using cocitation and co-occurrence networks and argue that the standard story needs reappraising. Second, we argue that the question whether behaviorism was the “dominant” school of American psychology is historically misleading to begin with. Using the results of our bibliometric analyses, we argue that questions about the development of American psychology deserve more fine-grained answers. Keywords: behaviorism, American psychology, bibliometric mapping, cocitation analysis, co-occurrence analysis The history of 20th-century American psychology is often depicted as a history of the rise and fall of behaviorism, the view that psychology should become “a purely objective experimental branch of natural science” (Watson, 1913, p. 248). Although early 20th-century psychologists aimed to redefine their discipline as a science of behavior, the popularity of behaviorism declined from the late 1950s onward, when psychologis ts, linguists, and computer scientists joined forces and developed empirical approaches to the study of mind and cognition. This article was published Online First March 19, 2020. Michiel Braat, Tilburg School of Humanities and Digital Sciences, Tilburg University; Jan Engelen, Depart- ment of Communication and Cognition, Tilburg School of Humanities and Digital Sciences, Tilburg University;
  • 65. Ties van Gemert, Tilburg School of Humanities and Digital Sciences, Tilburg University; Sander Verhaegh, Tilburg Center for Logic, Ethics, and Philosophy of Science, Tilburg School of Humanities and Digital Sciences, Tilburg University. All authors contributed equally to this work. This research is funded by the Tilburg School of Humanities and Digital Sciences Research Traineeships Programme. In addition, Verhaegh’s research is funded by The Netherlands Organization for Scientific Research (grant 275–20 – 064). We would like to thank Nees Jan van Eck, members of the History of Behavior Analysis mailing list (especially Nicole L. Banks and François Tonneau) and audiences at conferences at the University of Amsterdam, the CEU Institute for Advanced Study Budapest, and the Tilburg School of Humanities and Digital Sciences for their valuable comments and feedback. Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to Jan Engelen or Sander Verhaegh, Tilburg School of Humanities and Digital Sciences, Tilburg University, Warandelaan 2, 5037 AB, Tilburg, the Netherlands. E-mail: [email protected] or [email protected] T hi s do cu m en t
  • 70. © 2020 American Psychological Association 2020, Vol. 23, No. 3, 252–280 ISSN: 1093-4510 http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/hop0000146 252 mailto:[email protected] mailto:[email protected] http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/hop0000146 Although historians of science disagree about the theoretical and social factors that have contributed to the development of experimental psychology (Cohen-Cole, 2014; Greenwood, 2009; Leahey, 2001), there is a widespread consensus about the growing and declining influence of behaviorism in 20th-century American psychology (DeGrandpré & Buskist, 2000; O’Donohue & Kitchener, 1999; Staddon, 1999; Zuriff, 1985). From a methodological perspective, these claims are contentious, because wide-scope claims about the development of American psychology are rarely backed up by representative empirical data. Although nobody will deny that many of today’s best-known behaviorists produced their most influential work between 1920 and 1960, it is unclear what proportion of American psychologists embraced a behavioristic conception of psychology both during and after the heyday of behaviorism. Similar worries can be raised about the historians’ conclu- sions about developments internal to behaviorism. Although most scholars identify Watson,
  • 71. Hull, Tolman, and Skinner as the most influential (neo- )behaviorists,1 these claims are seldom supported by balanced empirical evidence. In general, it is unclear to what extent the list of psychologists who have been most extensively studied by historians of behaviorism forms a faithful representation of the scholars that actually influenced the development of behaviorism in the first half of the 20th century. The problem that wide-scope claims about the history of American psychology are rarely backed up by balanced empirical data is especially pressing because it seems likely that historians overestimate the influence of canonized schools and scholars (whether it is behav- iorism in the 1930s or cognitive psychology in the 1970s). Not only do historians tend to ignore psychologists who did not belong to any school (Green, Feinerer, & Burman, 2013); in studying the histories of influential schools and scholars, historians also tend to limit them- selves to the writings “of a few known authors that are transmitted from generation to generation [. . .] as the authors to read” (Betti, van den Berg, Oortwijn, & Treijtel, 2019, p. 296). More particularly, historical claims about the development of American psychology are often based on corpora that are (a) not clearly specified (which texts exactly did the historian study in arriving at their conclusions?), (b) very small (how many of the tens of thousands of texts produced by psychologists did the historian actually read?), and (c) not representative (how can the historian guarantee that the texts he or she studied form a
  • 72. representative sample of the total body of texts produced by psychologists?). Although in-depth text analyses and archive studies can provide historians with a first indication of the influence of a psychologist, a method, or a school of psychology, their conclusions will always be shaped by the texts and archives they choose to study. In this article, we aim to transcend the received view about the development of 20th-century American psychology in two ways. First, we use advanced text analysis tools (e.g., biblio- metric mapping, cocitation analysis, and co-occurrence analysis) to quantitatively analyze the metadata of 119,278 articles published in American psychology journals between 1920 and 1970. We analyze both the citations and the title terms of these articles and generate cocitation and co-occurrence networks for every consecutive decade between 1920 and 1970 to recon- struct the structure and development of mid-20th-century American psychology and the structure and development of behaviorism. Second, we argue that the question whether behaviorism was the “dominant” school of American psychology is historically misleading to begin with. Using the results of our bibliometric analyses, we argue that questions about the development of American psychology deserve more fine-grained answers. This article is organized as follows. After a brief outline of what might be called the received view about the history of behaviorism, we explain the main methodological challenges
  • 73. 1 See, for example, Boakes (1984, p. 237), Smith (1986, pp. 21– 22) and Mills (2012, pp. 104 –108). Most historians paint a picture of the development of behaviorism in which first Watson and later Hull and Tolman offered the “dominant orientation in American departments of psychology until after the end of the Second War, when it was displaced by [Skinner’s] radical behaviorism” (Greenwood, 2009, p. 477). T hi s do cu m en t is co py ri gh te d by th
  • 77. no t to be di ss em in at ed br oa dl y. 253RISE AND FALL OF BEHAVIORISM surrounding quantitative representations of the development of American psychology. Next, we describe our data and methods and provide an overview of our findings about the structure and development of American psychology between 1920 and 1970. Finally, we discuss the implications of our findings and argue that the standard story about the development of American psychology needs reappraising.
  • 78. Behaviorism: The Narrative Behaviorism is a complex of methodological, epistemological, and sometimes ontological assumptions about the foundations of psychology. Where psychology is traditionally defined as the study of mental phenomena, behaviorists typically argue that psychology should become a science of behavior. More specifically, most behaviorists agree that (a) psychology is or should become a branch of natural science, (b) psychologists should study behavior instead of mental phenomena, and (c) a science of behavior should be built exclusively on publicly available evidence (thereby dismissing the use of introspection in psychological research). Usually, behaviorists combine this view about the nature of psychology with a set of empirical assumptions—for instance, the assumption that the behavior of an organism is determined by the organism’s reinforcement history. Outside these shared philosophical and empirical commitments, behaviorists also strongly disagree about a wide range of issues. Most importantly, they disagree about the domain for psychology (should a study of behavior include or exclude physiological variables?), the nature of the observation language (should behaviorists tolerate intensional descriptions or purposive language?), and the types of theo- retical concepts allowed in the construction of behaviorist theories (are intervening variables or hypothetical constructs acceptable?). Historians often distinguish between two types of behaviorism in psychology: meth-
  • 79. odological and radical behaviorism (Day, 1983; Mills, 2012; Moore, 1981).2 Method- ological behaviorists view (a)–(c) as a set of methodological prescriptions; they do not believe that psychologists should say something about the ontological status of mental states. According to the methodological view, psychologists should aim to scientifically describe and explain behavior without referring to mental states, images, or processes. Radical behaviorists, on the other hand, deny that mental entities exist and argue that private events should be included in the analysis of behavior — that is, that private events should be analyzed in terms of the same principles that have been used to study overt behavior (Day, 1983; Ringen, 1999; Skinner, 1974). Although behaviorism is generally viewed as a distinctively American psychology, most historians recognize that its roots can be traced back to the work of the Russian objective psychologists or reflexologists (Boakes, 1984; Fuchs & Milar, 2003; Hergen- hahn, 2005).3 Ivan Sechenov, often credited as the founder of this school, worked on the (excitatory and inhibitory) mediational role of the cerebral cortex in reflex actions and extrapolated his findings to the concepts of psychology. In Reflexes of the Brain, Sechenov (1863/1965) defended the view that every mental process is reducible to a physiological reflex: “only physiology holds the key to the scientific analysis of psychical phenomena” (p. 351). Ivan Pavlov and Vladimir Bekhterev extended Sechenov’s work by indepen-
  • 80. dently discovering the principles of classical conditioning. Indeed, Sechenov’s call for an objective psychology seems to have played an important role in Pavlov’s conclusion that 2 We include the qualification “in psychology” because the present overview excludes the role behaviorist theories played outside psychology (e.g. philosophy, economics, and sociology). See, for example, Pooley and Solovey (2010) and Hauser (2015). 3 In addition, historians generally recognize that behaviorism also affected the development of psychology outside the United States. See, for example, Ardilla (2009). T hi s do cu m en t is co py ri
  • 84. us er an d is no t to be di ss em in at ed br oa dl y. 254 BRAAT, ENGELEN, VAN GEMERT, AND VERHAEGH his experimental results (dogs salivate in response to food stimuli at a distance) could be