SlideShare a Scribd company logo
1 of 23
Download to read offline
A Practical Guide to the Comparative Case Study
Method in Political Psychology
Juliet Kaarbo
University of Kansas
Ryan K. Beasley
Baker University
The case study, as a method of inquiry, is particularly suited to the field of political
psychology. Yet there is little training in political science, and even less in psychology, on
how to do case study research. Furthermore, misconceptions about case studies contribute
to the methodological barrier that exists within and between the two parent disciplines. This
paper reviews the various definitions and uses of case studies and integrates a number of
recent insights and advances into a practical guide for conducting case study research. To
this end, the paper discusses various stereotypes of the case study and offers specific steps
aimed at addressing these criticisms.
KEY WORDS: case study, comparative method, research design.
The field of political psychology has become increasingly populated with
individuals capable of speaking both the language of political science and the
language of psychology. This is valuable for building theory and understanding the
nature of the empirical domains of interest. Methodologically, however, there
remain some persistent obstacles between the two parent disciplines that, in our
view, unnecessarily hinder empirical cross-fertilization and advancement. Both
social psychologists and political scientists, for example, study the structure and
content of mass belief systems, but tend to use different methods (experiments and
surveys) and different statistical techniques (analyses of variance and regressions).
Margaret Hermann (1989) has indicated that methodological differences between
political science and psychology threaten to make the promise of a field of political
psychologymerelyafantasy.Ifwearegoingtocontinuetodevelopaninterdisciplinary
Political Psychology, Vol. 20, No. 2, 1999
369
0162-895X Š 1999 International Society of Political Psychology
Published by Blackwell Publishers, 350 Main Street, Malden, MA 02148, USA, and 108 Cowley Road, Oxford, OX4 1JF, UK.
language, methodological dialogue must replace the current tendency toward
simultaneous monologues.
Nowhere is the methodological gulf in understanding wider than when politi-
cal psychologists contemplate case study research (Tetlock, 1983). Indeed, a
review of the articles published in Political Psychology over the past decade reveals
that the case study is one of the most popular methods, but is most often used by
political scientists—especially those who study American leadership, international
relations, and comparative politics.1 Psychologists who publish in this journal tend
to use survey, experimental, or content-analytic techniques.2 Psychologists only
occasionally use the case study method.3
Practitioners of case study research, whether political scientists or psy-
chologists, lack a common language for their investigation. Alexander George’s
(George, 1979; George & Bennett, in press; George & McKeown, 1985; George
& Smoke, 1974) method of “structured-focused” comparison is perhaps the most
used resource for those engaging in this methodology. His articulation of the
procedures involved in the case study method have provided a sturdy methodologi-
cal foundation for hosts of empirical investigations. Yet many are less than
forthright in articulating the rationale of their chosen methodology. Indeed, Ragin
(1987) suggested that “most investigators who use case-oriented strategies . . . are
not self-consciously methodological; that is, they do not regard the case-oriented
strategies they use as formal methodologies” (p. 34).
Although there has been a resurgence of attention to the case study method
within a variety of disciplines (see, e.g., Orum, Feagin, & Sjoberg, 1991), we argue
that there has been insufficient attention to and internalization of these develop-
ments within political psychologists’ parent disciplines. This is true despite the
degree to which many political scientists embrace this technique and the tendency
for many psychologists toeschewit. Our purpose here is tosummarize and integrate
a number of these recent insights and advances into a practical guide for conducting
case study research, thereby providing greater direction and a common language
for those engaging in this type of research and highlighting the merits and value of
this technique for those more inclined to avoid it. Toward this end, we draw on
1 Recent examples of case study research in these subfields include Fishel’s (1992) case study of a
California legislator’s development over time, Gilbert’s (1995) case study of President Johnson for
the political effects of illness, Steinberg’s (1991) case study of the Cuban Missile Crisis for the role
of shame and humiliation in conflict deterrence, Farnham’s (1992) case study of Roosevelt during the
Munich Crisis as an example of prospect theory, and Groth and Britton’s (1993) case study of the
Soviet Union for the psychopolitical foundations of a garrison state.
2 Recent examples of research with these techniques include Fife-Schaw and Breakwell’s (1990) survey
of teenagers to predict the intention not to vote, Milburn and McGrail’s (1992) experiment on the
effects of cognitive complexity of news presentation, and Suedfeld’s (1992) content analysis of
newspaper editorials during times of positive and negative relations between pairs of countries.
3 Exceptions include Raven’s (1990) case study of the MacArthur-Truman relationship as an example
of interpersonal influence and Tiedens’ (1997) comparative case study oftwoPolish ghettos for violent
resistance during the Second World War.
370 Kaarbo and Beasley
numerous writings on the case study and comparative case study methods. We are
not intending to provide an array of novel reasons to use the case study method.
Rather, we are attempting to integrate observations regarding this method into a
coherent program for its use in a research effort, with particular attention to the
field of political psychology. In this sense, we are not comparing the case study
method to other methodologies, but instead are attempting to address various
perceived shortcomings and stereotypically founded criticisms within a practical
guide for actually doing case study research.
In this effort, we explicitly avoid any direct discussion of numerous and
engaging epistemological issues that are part of the current debate in political
science on the value of interpretation and the role of positivism in the social
sciences.4 The case study method is particularly prone to evoke discussions of the
nature of theories, the possibility of general laws governing human behavior, and
other fundamental issues of investigation. Although important, these issues are not
central to our discussion. Instead, our effort concerns the basic decisions and
choices that must be made in the practical application of the case study method.
In both political science and psychology, there exists a stereotype of the case
study that includes an idiosyncratic, historically specific, and atheoretical charac-
ter; a lack of control; an inability to generate a sufficient number of data points to
test theory; and a highly suspect “interpretive” character that allows the analyst to
draw favorable conclusions about hypotheses and to find support where no such
support exists. Certainly, the ways in which case studies have been performed and
used have frequently been rather divergent from the standards of systematic inquiry
currently widely accepted in both political science and psychology.
The reasons that the case study has fallen between the stools of methodological
rigor and empirical value are numerous. Among political scientists, the compara-
tive case study is often equated with cross-national comparison. The writings on
case study research tend to be associated with the subfield of comparative politics
and are not widely read in other subfields. Further, political scientists have paid
insufficient attention to recent discussions of the comparative method and case
study research found in public administration and sociology. Psychologists, on the
other hand, tend to associate case study research with the clinical “case” or patient
analysis and are generally unaware of the political science scholarship on the use
of case studies. Overall, we believe that political psychologists could be made more
aware of the different meanings of case study, the different types or purposes of
case study research, and the steps involved in case study research design, and that
they can make good use of the method as part of a systematic comparative
methodology without necessarily succumbing to its traditional pitfalls.
4 See, for example, the “Review Symposium: The Qualitative-Quantitative Disputation: Gary King,
Robert O. Keohane, and Sidney Verba’s Designing Social Inquiry: Scientific Inference in Qualitative
Research” in the American Political Science Review (1995); George and Bennett (in press).
Comparative Case Study 371
Defining “Case Study”
The terminology pertaining to “case studies” is frequently confusing. Ragin
(1987) noted that “the term ‘case’ and the various terms linked to the idea of case
analysis are not well defined in social science, despite their widespread usage and
centrality to social scientific discourse” (p. 1). For example, a “case” may be a
single instance or data point, such as a subject in an experiment, a survey respon-
dent, or the non-occurrence of war between belligerents (King, Keohane, & Verba,
1994). Alternatively, it may be a uniquely bounded phenomenon in a historical or
geographical sense, such as the case of Munich, the Soviet invasion of Afghanistan,
or the Watts riots. Indeed, the term “case” can have a variety of meanings, each
with important implications regarding empirical investigation.
When the term “case” is joined by other terms such as “study,” “analysis,” or
“method,” the picture does not become any clearer. For example, Orum et al. (1991)
defined a case study as “an in-depth, multifaceted investigation, using qualitative
research methods, of a single social phenomenon” (p. 2). Lijphart (1971, 1975) saw
the case study as a single case that is closely associated with the comparative
method, as contrasted with experimental and statistical methods. George and
McKeown (1985) indicated that a case study focuses on within-case analysis to
evaluate claims about causal process. Finally, Yin (1994) defined a case study as
“an empirical inquiry that: investigates a contemporary phenomenon within its
real-life context, especially when the boundaries between phenomenon and context
are not clearly evident; and . . . [that] relies on multiple sources of evidence”
(p. 13). Clearly there are different definitions of cases and case studies, and this
has made systematic analyses of the value and purposes of this technique difficult.
We recognize that coming to some consensual understanding of the terms
“case,” “case study,” or, more generally, the “case method” is extremely difficult
(see Ragin & Becker, 1992), yet without some understanding of what these terms
are meant to convey, it is even more difficult to offer any practical guidance in
carrying out an investigation. For our current purposes, we offer basic definitions
of several terms. We consider a case to be an instance, or a data point, and in this
sense we do not draw any distinctions about how a “case” is further defined or
arrived at in an investigation. Cases can be experimentally derived measurements,
survey responses, or classifications of historical events (such as “war/no war”). We
define a case study to be a method of obtaining a “case” or a number of “cases”
through an empirical examination of a real-world phenomenon within its naturally
occurring context, without directly manipulating either the phenomenon or the
context. The comparative case study is the systematic comparison of two or more
data points (“cases”) obtained through use of the case study method.
This definition has a number of implications. First, we do not assume a
particular purpose of the investigation, such as developing causal inferences or
detailing historical occurrences. A case study can be used for a variety of purposes,
several of which we articulate below. Second, we do not assume that multiple
372 Kaarbo and Beasley
sources of evidence must be used (as does Yin’s definition above), although we
suspect that this will frequently be necessary. The case study, in other words, often
uses a number of techniques for gathering information—from interviews to surveys
to content analysis—but it is not necessary to use multiple sources or types of
evidence in order to perform a case study. Third, we offer no particular distinction
between qualitative and quantitative aspects of a case study, nor do we draw a
particular distinction between narrative and analytic aspects (see Abbot, 1992;
King et al., 1994). Indeed, we feel that case studies can be very qualitative and
narrative in form, and they can be very quantitative and analytic in form. Fourth,
the issues of context and manipulation are central to our definition. The phenome-
non is studied as it occurs within its context, and neither the phenomenon nor the
context are directly manipulated by the researcher. Finally, our definition allows
for comparison of several case studies and is not limited to single case studies.5
Types of Case Studies
Our definition of the case study does not imply a single purpose for an
investigation. Indeed, there are many uses of case studies that have been outlined
by a variety of scholars (e.g., Eckstein, 1975; Lijphart, 1971; Van Evera, 1996;
Yin, 1994). In our effort to offer a practical guide for the case study method, we
condense these previous discussions into a single, accessible form. In doing so, we
hope to highlight the potential value of case studies for a variety of purposes as
well as lay the foundation for our arguments regarding the need for greater
systematicity in comparative case studies when theory development or testing is
the goal. We present the different uses or types of case studies as though the
investigator will be using a single case study, although this typology in no way
limits analysts to investigate only one case at a time.
Using Cases for Description
The first type of case study is what Lijphart (1971) referred to as “atheoretical”
and what Eckstein (1975) called “configurative-idiographic.” Here the analyst is
principally interested in the case itself, and is typically attempting to gain a Gestalt
or holistic picture of the event or phenomenon. Generally, with this use of case
studies, the analyst is more interested in the case than in some theory or hypothesis.
Certainly description requires some preconceived ideas or points of reference that
might be considered the foundation of theory, but the principal purpose of the case
5 Several scholars within the social sciences have written extensively about the importance and purpose
of the comparative method (see, for example, Collier, 1993; George, 1979; Lijphart, 1971, 1975;
Ragin, 1987). The comparative method should, in our view, be distinguished and disentangled from
the issues surrounding case studies and the case study method. Comparison is the very foundation of
scientific methodology. In other words, comparison need not be distinguished from other methods,
but can be considered to be a foundation of methodology itself.
Comparative Case Study 373
study does not directly involve that set of preconceived ideas. Rather, the case is
meant to be an end in and of itself. With this use of the case study we often observe
analysts using intuitive and descriptive accounts that attempt to lead to some
general “understanding” of the case.6 The use of the term idiographic (as contrasted
with nomothetic) quite directly indicates that the case will not be generalizable or
that the case study will not seek to establish general rules of behavior.
Using Theory to Explore Cases
In contrast to the “configurative-idiographic” or “atheoretical” case study,
a researcher may choose to use some theory or set of hypotheses to direct his
examination of a particular case. This use of the case study is “disciplined-
configurative” (Eckstein, 1975) or “interpretive” (Lijphart, 1971). Here, the focus
is still on the case, but the analyst explicitly uses some theoretical foundation in
order to examine or interpret the case. Although there may be some “feedback”
from the case to the theory—such as whether the theory is inappropriate for
understanding the case, or whether no theory currently exists that effectively
illuminates the case—the focus is still mainly on the case. After such an endeavor,
we should know the case in a “theoretically familiar” form. Here, however, the
purpose of the analyst may be to provide a mechanism whereby changes can be
effected on a particular organization, institution, or actor that was the subject of the
“interpretive” case study. In other words, the interpretation of the case may be
designed to effect changes along the lines of the given theory. The interest remains
on the case (or the actor or actorsof the given case), but the theoreticalinterpretation
of the case may provide some prescriptive leverage.7
Using Cases to Develop Theory
The third type or use of case studies is what has been termed “hypothesis-
generating” (Lijphart, 1971) or “heuristic” (Eckstein, 1975) case studies. Here the
analyst specifically investigates a case in an effort to develop testable hypotheses.
In this sense, the case study is being used to build theory; the analyst examines a
6 Some might argue that this type of case study should be further divided into “historical” and
“interpretive,” with the former being an attempt to present events or circumstances in an effort to
reveal the case, and the latter being an attempt to gain an understanding of the case from the perspective
of the actors involved. We, however, have not opted to divide this particular use of the case study
along these lines, as our intention is to provide a more general set of uses of the case study. Further,
following Ragin (1987), we consider that interpretive approaches to case studies are not in competition
with other types of approaches.
7 Freudian analysis applied to a patient, for example, interprets the patient’s past behaviors and activities
according to a more or less defined theory, often for the purpose of changing the patient according to
that interpretation. Decision analysts may evaluate the past behavior of an organization according to
theories about group performance specifically to offer modifications in the organization’s composition
or behavior according to that theory’s prescriptive implications for the case.
374 Kaarbo and Beasley
specific set of concepts in order to develop generalizable theory from particular
instances. The selection of cases is based on the nature of the theory that is being
constructed, rather than on a particular case that is otherwise of some interest to
the researcher. The focus, in other words, shifts from being explicitly on the case
to being explicitly on the theory. In these types of case studies, the analyst is
typically looking at fewer variables than would be true for the configurative-
idiographic case study, which uses a more Gestalt approach to the investigation,
and specifically attempts to develop a broad and informed picture of the case. This
is very similar to the sort of “introspective examination” that psychologists often
engage in as they are constructing a hypothesis regarding individual behavior or
cognition. They might, for example, ask themselves “how do I, as an expert, think
about things” when they are attempting to develop a theory of expert cognition.8
Using Cases to Explore and Refine Theory
Another use or type of case study increasingly focuses the analyst on a specific
theory. This has been termed a “plausibility probe” by Eckstein (1975), who
suggested that plausibility “means something more than a belief in potential
validity plain and simple . . . [and] something less than actual validity, for which
rigorous testing is required” (p. 108). Here the analyst is presumably interested in
performing some labor- or cost-intensive study, and thus is benefited by examining
certain aspects of the theory at stake within the confines of an empirical instance.
It is an inductive feedback device within the context of the particular empirical
domain to which the analyst’s theory speaks. Further, this use of the case study is
particularly important with novel hypotheses or theories, such that an existing body
of evidence cannot be referenced in speculating about the plausibility of a suspected
relationship. This assists the analyst in determining whether an empirical instance
of a particular phenomenon can be reasonably found, whether the operationaliza-
tions of key variables are in concert with the data available, and/or whether the
posited relationship or relationships between and among variables are consistent
with expectations.9 In this latter form, the plausibility probe helps to avoid a costly
exploration of an implausible explanation.
8 Using a case study as a “deviant case” (Lijphart, 1971) offers the potential for expanding the nature
or scope of an existing theory. Here the analyst specifically chooses a case that deviates from the
pattern predicted by theory, and then attempts to examine the case in an effort to uncover additional
variables that may be lacking in the original theory. In this sense, the deviant case differs from a
heuristic case study in that a theory already exists that is being examined. A deviant case, however, is
similar to a heuristic case in that both are attempting to establish new hypotheses or propositions. In
other words, the deviant case can elaborate a theory by suggesting new hypotheses or additional
conditions under which a certain relationship may or may not hold. In this sense, the deviant case and
the heuristic case are similar.
9 Goldmann (1988), in an appendix entitled “A Note on the Utility of Weak Theory and Weak Tests,”
suggested that using the case study in a fashion similar to the plausibility probe is of value for “testing”
weak theory, in that it can “lead to an improved understanding of the problems and possibilities of
operationalization” (p. 227).
Comparative Case Study 375
This is very similar to many aspects of the pilot study in experimental methods.
It is rare that an experiment is constructed in the office, run in the laboratory, and
published in a journal in swift order. Experimental psychology is replete with
efforts to probe the plausibility of an expected relationship, and the experimental
method is an iterative cycle that effectively incorporates numerous steps designed
to refine theories and hypotheses before testing them.
Using Cases as Tests of Theory
Case studies can also be used as single tests of an established theory, either to
confirm or infirm (Lijphart, 1971) or to investigate a crucial case (Eckstein, 1975).
The distinction between confirming and infirming rests principally in the nature of
the findings, that is, whether they support the predictions of an existing theory
(confirming) or whether they call that theory into question (infirming). In either
event, the process involves specifying in exacting fashion the variables to be
measured and the hypotheses to be tested. Further, a case must be selected such
that it fits the parameters of the theory that is being investigated. Once a population
is specified (in more or less precise form), then a sampling procedure that maxi-
mizes random selection will tend to offer the most compelling interpretation to the
given results of the study.10 Once performed, the case study will stand as a single
“test” of a given theory, and the weight afforded that test in terms of its commentary
on that theory will be a function of the way in which the case was chosen, the theory
specified, the variables operationalized and measured, and the conclusions drawn.
Some cases, it can be argued, offer the potential to make a particularly compelling
statement about the theory involved. This has been termed the “crucial case” study
(Eckstein, 1975) and purportedly offers a critical test of a theory, in contrast to the
“uncritical”or“routine”testofferedbyothercaseexaminationsofagiventheory.Here,
the analyst intentionally selects a case where alternative explanations of a negative
finding(afindingotherthanthatpredictedbythetheory)arenotplausible.Thisrequires
both that the theory can be completely specified in terms of empirical expectations,
and that a case exists such that mitigating factors do not exist. As Eckstein himself
acknowledged, this is rare within the social sciences, although how crucial a case is
can be considered along a continuum that allows more or less boisterous claims on the
part of the researcher regarding the support or lack of support afforded by the test.
Steps to Comparative Case Study Research
Much of the writing in political science on the case study method has focused
on justifying the case study or on the different purposes of case studies. There has
10 For a discussion on the important and deceptively difficult question of populations and case studies,
see Ragin and Becker (1992). See also Yin (1994) for a discussion of statistical generalization
(generalizing to a population) versus analytical generalization (generalizing to a theory).
376 Kaarbo and Beasley
been less attention paid to how to do case study research. Even in research methods
textbooks, chapters devoted to the case study give the student few guidelines for
how to construct a research design for the case study method (e.g., Baker, 1988;
Isaak, 1985; Johnson & Joslyn, 1991). Our purpose in this section is to outline for
researchers who choose the case study method the important steps for case study
research. What issues are likely to arise in the design of case study research? What
decisions must the researcher face? What types of evidence are compatible with
case study research? These are the questions addressed in the following guidelines.
In doing so, we want to integrate the advice from a number of fields and offer a
practical guide for conducting case study research.
Although most of these steps to case study research would apply to many of
the above uses or types of case studies and to single as well as multiple case studies,
our treatment focuses on comparative case studies with the purpose of examining
hypotheses as critically and systematically as possible. In other words, although
there is an important role for case studies in interpreting a single historical case and
in serving as a heuristic for theory construction, we discuss the steps researchers
will take when they use the case study method to compare two or more cases in
order to draw implications about an existing theory or single hypothesis. We focus
on this type of case study because it is the furthest afield from the “stereotype” of
case studies as idiosyncratic, historically specific, and atheoretical. We do not
intend to suggest that the other uses of the case study are not of value, or that many
of these steps could not be applied to improving the other types of case studies.
Rather, we find it least confusing to present these steps with a particular use of the
case study in mind. However, many of the issues we discuss are not necessarily
limited to this purpose of the case study.
To date, the best guide for comparative case study research is Alexander
George’s “method of structured, focused comparison” (George, 1979; George &
Bennett, in press; George & McKeown, 1985; George & Smoke, 1974).11 Accord-
ing to George (1979), the comparison is “focused because it deals selectively with
only certain aspects of the historical case . . . and structured because it employs
general questions to guide the data collection analysis in that historical case”
(pp. 61–62; emphasis added). George and his colleagues have outlined several
tasks for researchers who choose the structured, focused case comparison. These
guidelines overlap significantly with the steps we discuss below, although we
consolidate some tasks, divide others, and integrate other scholars’ advice for
conducting case study research in our discussion. Our aim is both to incorporate
previous discussions of case study research from other disciplines (e.g., public
administration and sociology) and to offer guidelines for conducting case studies
that are accessible and meaningful for political scientists, psychologists, and
political psychologists.
11 In his recently published collection of “memos,” Stephen Van Evera (1996) also offered step-by-step
advice for case study research.
Comparative Case Study 377
Of course, these steps are fairly standard in research design. They are the
traditional steps researchers follow when designing research using almost any
method, yet they are rarely followed for case study research. The form that these
steps take in comparative case study research differs somewhat from that for other
research methods. For example, how reliability is enhanced in a research design is
different for case studies than for experiments, although the same logic underlies
reliability issues in both types of research. Thus, what follows is a discussion of
how traditional research design issues play out in the comparative case study
method.
Step 1: Identify Specific Research Question for Focused Comparison
The first step in comparative case study research, just as for any other method,
is to formulate the specific research question. What phenomena do you want to
explain? What is the dependent variable? The type of research questions most
appropriate for case studies are “how” and “why” questions—that is, questions
focusing on the underlying process, on the causal nexus between the independent
variables and the phenomena to be explained. Examples are “How do voters choose
candidates?,” “Why do unanimity decision rules produce compromise outcomes
in group decision-making?,” and “How do leaders manage information inconsis-
tent with their beliefs?” Frequency questions (“How often do different values of
the dependent variable occur?”) are not appropriate for comparisons of a small
number of cases (Yin, 1994).
At this point, the researcher wants to focus the research on a specific phenome-
non and be clear what class of events the research will address. Focusing the
research question is an essential part of George’s “focused comparison” and is
Lijphart’s (1971) advice for dealing with the “too many variables” problem. As
Lijphart (1971) argued, researchers comparing case studies “must avoid the danger
of being overwhelmed by large numbers of variables and, as a result, losing the
possibility of discovering controlled relationships” (p. 690). Developing a focused
research question is the best way to identify the most important variables. The
identification of the class of phenomena is also a crucial part of this first step and
will help the researcher both to identify cases for selection and to answer the
question “What is this a case of?” For example, suppose you are interested in the
relationship between leaders’ personalities and decision-making outcomes. It is
important at the outset to focus on what leaders you are interested in. In other words,
does the research question apply to all leaders, or to leaders of particular countries
or types of political systems? Focusing on particular types of leaders would make
the identification of explanatory variables and case selection much more manageable.
378 Kaarbo and Beasley
Step 2: Identify Variables From Existing Theory
The next question the researcher asks is “What independent or explanatory
variables or conditions have been found or hypothesized to explain the dependent
variable?” This is usually accomplished by an extensive literature review. The
researcher should then specify which aspects of existing theory will be singled out
for assessment, again focusing the research question. A large list of potential
explanatory variables investigated in a small number of cases yields an indetermi-
nate research design, and thus the case study researcher must choose a focused set
of candidate explanatory variables to investigate (King et al., 1994). One advantage
of case study research, however, is the ability to track other variables in the cases
that may not be the primary focused explanatory variable(s), but may be important
later in the interpretation of the relationships discovered. This is not uncommon in
experimental studies, wherein measures are frequently taken for a host of variables
whether or not they are of central concern to the study.
The researcher should also make explicit which of the variables to be investi-
gated are hypothesized to be most important for explaining the phenomenon. In
other words, does the theory suggest any ordering of the possible independent
variables? Are any conditions hypothesized to be necessary or sufficient? Hypothe-
sized intervening relationships should also be specified at this point. Although
many research methods are aided by this a priori inventory of relationships among
variables, the case study comparison requires it because this will drive the next step
of case selection.
Step 3: Case Selection
The most important (and difficult) stage in comparative case study research is
the selection of cases. Cases can, of course, be drawn randomly from a population
to eliminate the danger of selection bias. Yet drawing a random sample is often not
possible because the universe of cases is not known or is not accessible.12 At the
heart of systematic investigations is the idea of control. To establish the relationship
between two or more variables, it is necessary to minimize variability in other
variables that may affect the investigated relationship. In the experimental method,
this is approximated by dividing subjects into experimental and control groups
through some form of random assignment. With the use of certain statistical
methods, the same type of control can be achieved through the use of partial
correlations. Although here no “assignment” of subjects to conditions occurs, the
influence of certain variables on the relationship being examined can be controlled
for statistically. Control for the comparative case study method is achieved through
12 As King et al. (1994) indicated, random selection does not always solve the problem of selection bias.
See Collier and Mahoney (1996) for an excellent and detailed discussion of the issues surrounding
selection bias in qualitative research.
Comparative Case Study 379
case selection. There are three important tasks in selecting cases that involve
control: selecting comparable cases, selecting cases that vary on the dependent
variable, and selecting cases across subgroups of the population to address alter-
native explanations.
3a. Comparable cases. Comparability is the heart of any scientific investiga-
tion and thus is not unique to case study research (see Ragin, 1987, p. 1). Compa-
rability is the basis of Przeworski and Teune’s (1970) “most similar systems
design” in which the researcher chooses cases as similar as possible to minimize
the number of explanatory variables.13 Without comparability, of course, the
researcher will not know if the variation seen in the cases is due to the
explanatory variable under consideration or to the other differences between
the cases (Lijphart, 1971). For example, suppose you are interested in how
juries aggregate individual preferences into a group decision and you hypothe-
size that the size of the jury is the best explanation for this process. If your cases
include juries from different cultures, different time periods, and different legal
systems, you do not know whether the differences in the group decision are due
to the explanatory variable (jury size) or to the other differences between the
cases (culture, time period, and legal system). Thus, if you have reason to
believe culture, time period, and legal system affect how juries make decisions,
you would want to choose cases that are comparable on these dimensions in
order to focus on the effect of size.
Comparability depends on the theoretical basis of the study. Cases do not have
to be comparable on dimensions that do not (or are assumed not to) impinge on the
relationship under investigation. Just as experimentalists are generally not con-
cerned about (or at least tolerate) differing weather conditions on the multiple days
that an experiment takes place (a problem not corrected by random assignment of
subjects to groups), comparative case study researchers should not be concerned
about irrelevant differences in their cases. Building on the previous example, if
your theory or previous evidence suggests that jury size affects the group aggrega-
tion process regardless of culture, time period, or political system, these variables
do not have to be considered to make cases comparable. This stands in contrast to
Diesing’s (1971, pp. 187–188) advice to pick cases that are geographically similar
and that have similar histories if the theory does not suggest relevant dimensions
for comparability. The theory should be more capable of distinguishing impor-
tant factors. Cases should not be chosen for comparability on non-theoretically
derived properties.14 Instead, researchers should choose cases in an effort to
13 Although Przeworski and Teune’s “most similar systems design” focuses on choosing comparable
systems, the same principle applies to any level of analysis.
14 A prerequisite for choosing comparable cases is to define what a “case” or the unit of analysis is for
the investigation. Thus, choosing comparative cases directly follows from the first step of focusing
the research question and identifying the class of phenomena that the question addresses. Is a “case”
a case of jury decision-making (for which any case of a jury decision would yield a comparable case),
or is a “case” a case of jury decision-making in Western cultures, in the late 20th century, in
380 Kaarbo and Beasley
control for known or suspected alternative causes of the relationship under
investigation.
3b. Cases with variation in the values of the dependent variable. The most
basic rule in case selection is that selection should allow for the possibility of
variation in the values of the dependent variable (King et al., 1994). Without
variation in values of the dependent variable, the researcher cannot make any causal
inference about the phenomenon because the same explanatory variables may be
present in cases in which the phenomenon is absent. This problem led John Stuart
Mill (1843/1974) to caution scholars against this “method of agreement,” and
Mill’s caution has been echoed by many later writers on the case study (e.g., Achen
& Snidal, 1989; Collier, 1993; Geddes, 1990). Despite such advice, case study
researchers continue to examine cases of deterrence failure (and not cases of
deterrence success), cases of revolution (and not cases in which a revolution did
not occur), and cases of crisis decision-making (and not cases of routine decision-
making). Such studies can eliminate a hypothesized explanation if it is not present
in the cases (Collier, 1995; Collier & Mahoney, 1996), but cannot conclude that a
factor present in the cases is related to the dependent variable.
To ensure the possibility of variation in the values of the dependent variable,
case study researchers have two choices: selecting cases based on variation in the
values of an explanatory variable, and selecting cases based on variation in the
values of the dependent variable (the latter being Mill’s “method of difference”).
The first option, selecting cases based on categories of an explanatory variable,
causes no inference problems and is therefore considered to be the best solution
(King et al., 1994). With this strategy, for example, the researcher interested in the
relationship between stress and the quality of decision-making would choose cases
in which stress was present (or high) and cases in which stress was absent (or low)
and then proceed to investigate how these independent variables related to the
quality of decision-making.
One of the drawbacks to this strategy (and hence one argument for choosing
the second strategy of selecting cases based on variation in the values of the
dependent variable) is that often it is the value of the dependent variable (i.e.,
that a revolution occurred) that is already known to the researcher. If the
researcher already knows the values of the dependent variable, then this is the
same as selecting the cases on both the independent and dependent variables.
This might cause bias in the case selection if the cases are chosen in such a way
that the independent variable matches up with the known value of the dependent
variable in the hypothesized direction (King et al., 1994). Furthermore, re-
searchers are often inherently interested in explaining the dependent variable
common-law systems (for which cases must be comparable on these dimensions)? The definition of
the unit of analysis must come from the researcher’s theoretical focus or from the population to which
the hypothesis applies.
Comparative Case Study 381
(e.g., poor decision-making or revolutions); that is what motivates them to do the
study. Choosing cases based on variation in the values of the dependent variable
hastheadvantageofallowingtheresearchertosaysomethingaboutthedependent
variable under investigation upon its completion. If, using the previous example,
casesinwhichstress(theexplanatoryvariable)waspresentandabsentarechosen
and stress turns out to be unrelated to decision-making quality (the dependent
variable), then this is all the research can conclude. If, on the other hand, cases of
good and poordecision-making(the dependentvariable)are chosen,then,even if
stressturnsouttobeunrelated,theresearchercaninvestigateothervariablesinthe
casesthatmightberelatedtodecision-makingquality.15
3c. Choosing cases across population subgroups. In addition to choosing
comparable cases that vary on the values of the dependent variable, case study
researchers should consider choosing cases with an alternative explanation in mind.
In other words, the case selection design may include variation across some
dimension associated with an alternative explanation of the relationship being
investigated. What the researcher attempts to do in this step is to demonstrate that
the relationship holds across different subgroups of a population. This gives the
researcher, upon the conclusion of the study, the opportunity to challenge an
alternative explanation.16
For example, if you are interested in how personality characteristics affect the
maintenance of enemy images, in addition to choosing cases in which an enemy
image was maintained and cases in which an enemy image changed, you might
consciously choose cases before and after the end of the Cold War. This case
selection would ensure that you could say something about the relationship under
investigation, regardless of the time period. If cases only from the Cold War era or
only from the post–Cold War period were examined, you would be open to the
criticism that the study’s results only apply to that particular era. An alternative
theory might suggest that the nature of enemy images is completely different in
these two historical periods of the international system—that enemy images, for
example, are much more malleable in the post–Cold War world regardless of
personality characteristics. Of course, choosing cases across different subgroups
of a population proliferates the number of cases needed.17
The difference between choosing comparable cases (step 3a) and choosing
cases across population subgroups (step 3c) concerns the expectations about the
15 A final point in favor of choosing cases based on variation in values of the dependent variable is that,
in many cases, researchers hypothesize multiple (often competing) explanatory variables. Thus,
choosing casesbased on the (single)dependentvariable toensurevariationwouldbea simpler formula
for case selection. See Collier and Mahoney (1996) and Van Evera (1996) for additional issues
concerning case selection and variation in the dependent variable.
16 Choosing cases across subgroups associated with an alternative explanation to show the maximum
leverage of the hypothesis is consistent with Przeworski and Teune’s (1970) “most different systems
design.”
17 See King et al. (1994), chapter 5, for more specific advice on this issue.
382 Kaarbo and Beasley
applicability of the hypothesized relationship. In choosing comparable cases, the
researcher is recognizing the limits of the hypothesis, arguing that it only
applies to cases that are comparable on a certain number of theoretically derived
dimensions. In choosing cases across subgroups, the researcher is purposefully
stretching the investigation of the hypothesis across a variable derived from an
alternative explanation.
Step 4: Operationalize Variables and Construct a Case Codebook
The fourth major task in designing a comparative case study, and one that is
often overlooked, is to operationalize the variables under investigation. The re-
searcher is essentially constructing a case “codebook” to guide the collection of
evidence for the variables in the study. Indeed, King et al. (1994) argued that such
recording and reporting the way the data were collected is even more important in
case studies than is case selection.
George (1979) conceived of the variables in the form of structured questions
that the researcher asks in each case:
Using a standardized set of questions in the controlled comparison is
necessary to assure acquisition of comparable data from the several cases.
In this way, the method of structured, focused comparison will avoid the
all-too-familiar and disappointing experience of traditional, intensive
single case studies in the past which, even when they were instances of a
single class of events, were not performed in a comparable way and hence
did not contribute to an orderly, cumulative development of knowledge
and theory about the phenomenon in question. Instead, as conducted in
the past, each case study tended to go its own way, reflecting the special
interests of each investigator and often, somewhat opportunistically,
being guided by the readily available historical data rather than by a well
defined theoretical focus. As a result, the idiosyncratic features of each
case tended to shape the research questions differently. (p. 62)
George’s structured questions—the “structured” element of his “structured, fo-
cused comparison”—are similar to what Yin (1994, p. 70) called a “case study
protocol,” a list of case study questions and identification of potential sources of
evidence for answers.
Although we enthusiastically endorse George’s advice to investigate the same
variables across the set of cases in the form of structured questions, we argue that
the case study researcher needs to go beyond structuring questions and to structure
answers as well. In other words, the same set of general categories of answers, or
values of each variable, should guide the researcher across the cases. The key is to
specify in advance what is necessary for the case analyst to see in the evidence to
code a variable in a particular way, to make a judgment of its value. This allows
for enhanced intracoder reliability (the case study analyst uses the same set of
Comparative Case Study 383
criteria to assign variables particular values) and provides a basis for intercoder
reliability (the same set of criteria to assign variables particular values can be
used by different analysts). In many case studies, of course, variables will be
qualitatively judged on the basis of subjective interpretations of transcripts, diaries,
etc., but even in this way, a priori categories of the possible variable values can
increase systematicity and reliability.
Just as in the construction of answers to surveys, the analyst should treat the
operationalizations of the possible values of the variables seriously. Operationali-
zations should be general—applicable to the class of phenomena, not just to the
specific case under investigation—so that the case codebook could be applied to
cases not in the study. Just as for any codebook, categories, to the extent possible,
should be exhaustive for all possible answers in all cases. Of course, categories
should be mutually exclusive. If some variables have multiple indicators in their
operational definitions, the researcher should specify a meta-decision rule for
deciding the value of the variable. For example, if there are several indicators for
coding a leader as a crusader personality and there are several indicators for coding
a leader as a pragmatist personality, the codebook should specify how to deal with
mixed evidence when indicators of both personalitytypesare found inthe evidence.
The exact nature of the operationalizations of the variables will, of course,
depend on the data sources to be used in the investigation. One advantage of a
comparative case study research design is that a variety of types of data and
techniques for extracting data can be used (Yin, 1994). Content analysis, survey
data, archive data, descriptive statistical analysis, focus groups, and interviews are
all compatible, not competitive, with case study research. The use of multiple
sources is one way to enhance the construct validity of operational definitions (Yin,
1994, p. 34).18
In addition to specifying in advance the operationalizations of the variables,
case study researchers can enhance the reliability and validity of case studies in
other ways. Case study researchers can use judgments made for entirely different
purposes by other researchers to avoid having measurements be influenced by the
hypotheses (King et al., 1994). King et al. also advised using qualified experts to
code the variables who do not know the theory being tested.Even non-expertcoders
who are blind to the hypotheses could demonstrate the intercoder reliability of the
variable measurements, although this may be difficult with certain types of
evidence.
Case study researchers may also consider conducting a pilot study: “The pilot
case is used more formatively, assisting an investigator to develop relevant lines
of questions—possibly even providing some conceptual clarification for the re-
search design as well” (Yin, 1994, p. 74). Pilot studies may be necessary when
18 Yin (1994) also argued that the use of multiple sources of evidence allows the researcher to develop
“converging lines of inquiry, [or] a process of triangulation” of data sources that makes conclusions
from case studies much more convincing (p. 92; emphasis in original).
384 Kaarbo and Beasley
operationalizations do not exist from previous studies or are not specified com-
pletely by the theory.19 After looking at a similar case with similar evidence that
will not be included in the main study, the case analyst is often better able to
fine-tune the operationalizations.
The tasks involved in the construction of a codebook—constructing structured
answers and structured questions, identifying multiple sources of evidence, testing
for intercoder reliability, investigating pilot cases—may seem to be tedious work
for a small number of cases. Yet this step addresses the most often heard criticism
of the case study: unreliability. Following rules for systematic extraction of data,
and reporting these rules to readers, enhances the credibility of conclusions from
case studies. Unfortunately, this step is rarely taken in case study research (George,
1979).20
Step 5: Code-Write Cases
Once the codebook is constructed and the evidence is accumulated, the
variable should be coded and the results presented. These are actually two separate
tasks. Indeed, Yin (1994) suggested creating a case study database, different and
separate from the report, arguing that “too often, the case study data are synony-
mous with the evidence presented in the case study report, and a critical reader has
no recourse if he or she wants to inspect the database that led to the case study
conclusions” (p. 95).
In terms of presentation, the narrative remains the most used and preferred
form (Yin, 1994). The narrative case study tells a story based on the variables and
coded values and allows the researcher to show the variables in their context.21 This
is a way to demonstrate the realism and temporal progression that is an advantage
of the case study method.22 In the narrative, any uncertainty on the part of the
analyst should, of course, also be presented (King et al., 1994). The length and
detail of the narratives depends on the research question, the number of variables,
and the number of cases.
Although the narrative form of presentation demonstrates the richness of the
data, it is not the only form available to the case study researcher and indeed can
become impossible when a large number of case studies are involved. Alterna-
tively, the case analyst can present a question-and-answer format or present only
19 This is one of the uses of a case study to explore and refine theory discussed earlier.
20 Analysts are, of course, still free to observe variables not included in their codebook, and even to
question the particular operationalizations that they have chosen. A codebook should not hinder the
valuable insights afforded analysts as they examine phenomena in their naturally occurring context.
These, however, should be noted as such when researchers report their findings.
21 For a discussion of different purposes of case narratives, see Abbott (1992, pp. 62–80).
22 The narrative form also has the advantage of conforming to the way people think about events. As
Pennington and Hastie’s (1986) research demonstrates, people think in terms of stories, not in terms
of comparing the values of variables in isolation.
Comparative Case Study 385
coded results in table form with short case descriptions in an appendix (Yin, 1994).
Combinations of narratives and short descriptions might be used, depending on the
results. For example, the researcher might want to demonstrate in more detail the
cases that fit or do not fit the hypotheses the best. In whichever form the case analyst
chooses to present the cases, the presentation of results should include more explicit
discussion of method than is usually found in case studies (George & McKeown,
1985).
Step 6: Comparison and Implications for Theory
After the cases have been coded for the variables, the case study analyst looks
for patterns within and across cases. In the congruence procedure, the analyst
determines whether any independent variables differentiate across the different
outcomes of the dependent variable (George & McKeown, 1985). In other words,
the analyst asks: Does the value of x relate to the value of y in the predicted way?
The analysis stage can follow another form of logic in which a pattern
predicted by the theory is matched against the pattern seen in the cases. In other
words, a number of different explanatory variables must be present and related to
each other in the right form for the hypothesis to be accepted. This technique of
“pattern matching” is a key advantage of the case study method (Campbell, 1975;
Collier, 1993). In pattern matching, a single predicted pattern can be compared to
the pattern observed in the cases or, alternatively, mutually exclusive rival patterns
can be compared for their consistency with the pattern observed in the cases (Yin,
1994, p. 108). This essentially increases the number of observations made within
each case study. In recanting his earlier “strong rejection” of the case study method,
the well-known psychologist Donald Campbell (1975) applied the statistical con-
cept of degrees of freedom to case studies. According to Campbell, theories usually
have a number of predictions that the social scientist uses to “pattern match” against
a single case. In other words, degrees of freedom in a case study means “testing”
multiple implications of the theory.
When the pattern that is investigated is temporal in character, the case analyst
engages in what George and McKeown (1985) termed “process tracing” or inves-
tigating a “decision process by which various initial conditions are translated into
outcomes” (p. 35).23 As Yin (1994) argued, “the ability to trace changes over time
is a major strength of case studies—which are not limited to cross-sectional or static
assessments of a particular situation” (p. 113). However, the data requirements for
process tracing are considerably greater than for the simple congruence procedure
and thus may not be practical for some case study research.
23 For George and McKeown (1985), process tracing means more than the analysis of temporal patterns
within cases; it also involves interpretivism in that it is “an attempt to reconstruct actors’ definitions
of the situation” (p. 35).
386 Kaarbo and Beasley
Examining the multiple implications of the theory or engaging in process
tracing within a single case study also results in certain limitations that should be
made clear by the case analyst. Much like choosing comparable cases by minimiz-
ing variation on alternative explanatory variables, examining patterns and pro-
cesses through time within a single case study does not allow for any between-cases
variation. Although a particular observed pattern may or may not conform to
theoretical predictions, it is difficult within a single case study to determine whether
a particular set of observations is idiosyncratic to that case or not. Further, a
particular pattern of observations within a single case study could be due to the
impact of a causal factor that cumulates through time but which is thought to be
independent of the causal process revealed in the case. For example, a particular
pattern of activity of presidential initiatives to secure approval of legislation may
reflect a predicted pattern of intensification through time due to a particular
personality trait, but for a given president regarding a particular initiative it may
also reflect a gradual accumulation of initial conditions such as public opinion,
economic prosperity, or other non–personality-based factors. Of course, investi-
gating more than one case study at a time helps to address some of these concerns,
as patterns and processes noted within one case study can then be systematically
compared to the other case studies that have been comparably performed.
Once the initial assessment of the theoretical implications is made, the case
study analyst may want to “return” to the cases to investigate candidate explana-
tions for the relationships discovered. In other words, certain interpretations arising
from the initial conclusions can often be discounted or suggested by analyzing the
“extra” material included in the case studies. Although the researcher may need to
design a new research plan and select new cases to better “test” these implications,
case study research can offer the researcher more material on which to build
speculative interpretations of results. This is a heuristic value built into every case
study research design.
Conclusions: Why Should Political Psychologists Care About
the Case Study Method?
It is our hope that this examination of the case study method will help to guide
researchers in their use of this methodology and will begin to dispel several
stereotypical conceptions of the case study method. As we suggested at the outset,
the case study has been plagued by various misconceptions, attributable more to
the way it has been used than to its actual potential. We have suggested that,
although valuable as a tool for understanding idiosyncratic historical events, the
method is also valuable as a tool for generating and exploring theories and testing
hypotheses. Further, we have offered various techniques for dealing with the issue
of the ratio of observations to hypotheses. We have also offered, by way of a review
of general design procedures, a set of steps for gaining some measure of control in
the comparative case study method. Although by definition our conception of the
Comparative Case Study 387
case study method disavows direct manipulation of either context or phenomenon,
case selection procedures do allow a degree of discrimination in determining
relationships between and among variables that is similar to that afforded by
experimental manipulation or statistical control. Finally, the resounding critique of
subjectivity on the part of the researcher has been addressed by suggesting
procedures that can significantly improve the systematicity and reliability of the
case study method.
Beyond the general value of case study research, the case study method is
particularly suitable for political psychology. We base this proposition on Margaret
Hermann’s (1986) identification of the tenets of the field of political psychology.
In her answer to the question “What is political psychology?” Hermann proposed
five common themes that characterize research in this area:
1. Focus Is on the Interaction of Political and Psychological Phenomena
2. Research Is Responsive and Relevant to Societal Problems
3. Context Can Make a Difference
4. Emphasis Is on Process as Well as Outcome
5. There Is a Tolerance of Multiple Methods for Gathering Data
Comparative case study research, we believe, is particularly relevant to the last
three of Hermann’s tenets.24
First, case study research is relevant to the tenet that context can make a
difference. Hermann (1986) wrote that “within political psychology there is a
healthy dialogue and debate” on the importance of contextual factors such as time,
culture, and political structures, but that “there is a growing recognition that such
contextual factors help not only to shape what an individual is like, but also to limit
what an individual can do politically” (p. 3). Case study research internalizes this
tenet, as “context” is part of the definition of the case study offered earlier in this
paper. Case study research means that a real-world phenomenon is investigated
within its naturally occurring context, with the recognition that context can make
a difference.
Second, the case study method is consistent with the tenet that the process is
as important as the outcome. Hermann (1986) argued that
psychological and political phenomena often seem to have their most
direct impact on one another in determining political processesas opposed
24 This is not to suggest that the case study method is not relevant to the first two tenets. Case study
research is certainly appropriate for the study of the interaction of political and psychological
phenomena and for research that is responsive and relevant to societal problems. Indeed, our
discussion in this paper of case studies whose goal is intervention and the desirability of choosing
cases based on variation in the values of an interesting dependent variable reflects this second tenet.
Our point in this section, however, is that there is something about the case study method in particular
that is consistent with the last three of Hermann’s tenets.
388 Kaarbo and Beasley
to political outcomes. Once delineated in one political setting, descrip-
tions of processes offer the possibility of generalization to other political
contexts with somewhat similar characteristics. By ascertaining what
some of these cross-cutting processes are, political psychologists can
begin to build more general explanations and theories. (p. 3)
Case study research is often directed at identifying the underlying process. As
discussed above, the most appropriate research questions for case study research
are “how” and “why” questions. Furthermore, pattern matching (especially tem-
poral pattern matching or process tracing), as a technique for analysis in case study
research, emphasizes the process by which initial conditions are translated into
outcomes. In this sense, process is central to most case study research.
Finally, case study research is compatible with political psychology’s toler-
ance of multiple methods for gathering data. As Hermann (1986) wrote, “political
psychology appears not to be wedded to one type of methodology. Its practitioners
employ a variety of methods” (p. 3). Not only can the case study method serve to
complement other research methods, it can also be multimethodological in itself,
as a number of techniques (e.g., content analysis, archival analysis, interviews) can
be incorporated into a case study research design.25
Clearly, then, the case study method can be a valuable research tool and is
particularly suited to the needs and interests of those studying within the field of
political psychology. In this light, it is unfortunate that some of the misconceptions
about case studies have contributed to the methodological barrier between political
science and psychology. It has not been our argument here that the case study
should supplant other methods, nor have we tried to systematically articulate each
advantage and disadvantage associated with case study research relative to other
methods. Rather, we hope to have provided a practical guide to doing case study
research and in the process helped to dispel certain misconceptions and thereby
further the development of a common language for political psychology.
ACKNOWLEDGMENTS
Earlier versions of this paper were presented as part of the methods workshop
series at the Summer Institute of Political Psychology at The Ohio State University,
1992–1997. We would like to thank the participants in those workshops, as their
questions and comments helped to refine our thinking about this topic over the
years. We also thank Alexander George as well as the editors and anonymous
reviewers for suggestions on earlier drafts. Correspondence concerning this article
should be addressed to Juliet Kaarbo, University of Kansas, Department of Political
Science, 504 Blake, Lawrence, KS 66045. E-mail: kaarbo@falcon.cc.ukans.edu
25 See Tetlock (1983) on the advantages of multimethod convergence in political psychology.
Comparative Case Study 389
REFERENCES
Abbott, A. (1992). What do cases do? Some notes on activity in sociological analysis. In C. C. Ragin
& H. S. Becker (Eds.), What is a case?: Exploring the foundations of social inquiry (pp. 53–82).
Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Achen, C.,&Snidal, D. (1989). Rational deterrence theoryand comparativecase studies.World Politics,
41, 143–169.
Baker, T. L. (1988). Doing social research. New York: McGraw-Hill.
Campbell, D. T. (1975). Degrees of freedom and the case study. Comparative Political Studies, 8,
178–193.
Collier, D. (1993). The comparative method. In A. W. Finifter (Ed.), Political science: The state of the
discipline II (pp. 105–119). Washington, DC: American Political Science Association.
Collier, D. (1995). Translating quantitative methods for qualitative researchers: The case of selection
bias. American Political Science Review, 89, 461–466.
Collier, D., & Mahoney, J. (1996). Insights and pitfalls: Selection bias in qualitative research. World
Politics, 49, 56–91.
Diesing, P. (1971). Patterns of discovery in the social sciences. New York: Aldine.
Eckstein, H. (1975). Case study and theory in political science. In F. I. Greenstein & N. W. Polsby
(Eds.), Handbook of political science (pp. 79–138). Reading, MA: Addison-Wesley.
Farnham, B. (1992). Roosevelt and the Munich crisis: Insights from prospect theory. Political
Psychology, 13, 205–235.
Fife-Schaw, C., & Breakwell, G. M. (1990). Predicting the intention not to vote in late teenage: A U.K.
study of 17- and 18-year-olds. Political Psychology, 11, 739–755.
Fishel, J. (1992). Leadership for social change: John Vasconcellos (D-CA) and the promise of
humanistic psychology in public life. Political Psychology, 13, 663–692.
Geddes, B. (1990). How the cases you choose affect the answers you get: Selection bias in comparative
politics. In J. A. Stimson (Ed.), Political analysis (vol. 2, pp. 131–150). Ann Arbor, MI: University
of Michigan Press.
George, A. L. (1979). Case studies and theory development: The method of structured, focused
comparison. In P. G. Lauren (Ed.), Diplomacy: New approaches in history, theory, and policy
(pp. 43–68). New York: Free Press.
George, A. L., & Bennett, A. (in press). Case studies and theory development. BCSJA Security Studies
Series. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
George, A. L., & McKeown, T. J. (1985). Case studies and theories of organizational decision making.
In R. F. Coulam & R. A. Smith (Eds.), Advances in information processing in organizations (vol.
2, pp. 21–58). Greenwich, CT: JAI Press.
George, A. L., & Smoke, R. (1974). Deterrence in American foreign policy. New York: Columbia
University Press.
Gilbert, R. E. (1995). The political effects of presidential illness: The case of Lyndon B. Johnson.
Political Psychology, 16, 761–776.
Goldmann, K. (1988). Stability and change in foreign policy. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press.
Groth, A. J., & Britton, S. (1993). Gorbachev and Lenin: The psychological walls of the Soviet “garrison
state.” Political Psychology, 14, 627–650.
Hermann, M. G. (1986). What is political psychology? In M. G. Hermann (Ed.), Political psychology
(pp. 1–10). San Francisco: Jossey-Bass.
Hermann, M. G. (1989, August). Political psychology: Fad, fantasy, or field. Paper presented at the
annual meeting of the American Psychological Association, New Orleans.
Isaak, A. C. (1985). Scope and methods of political science. Chicago: Dorsey.
390 Kaarbo and Beasley
Johnson, J. B., & Joslyn, R. A. (1991). Political science research methods. Washington, DC:
Congressional Quarterly Press.
King, G., Keohane, R. O., & Verba, S. (1994). Designing social inquiry: Scientific inference in
qualitative research. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press.
Lijphart, A. (1971). Comparative politics and the comparative method. American Political Science
Review, 65, 682–693.
Lijphart, A. (1975). The comparable-case strategy in comparative research. Comparative Political
Studies, 8, 158–177.
Milburn, M. A., & McGrail, A. B. (1992). The dramatic presentation of news and its effects on cognitive
complexity. Political Psychology, 13, 613–632.
Mill, J. S. (1974). A system of logic. Toronto: University of Toronto Press. (Original work published
1843)
Orum, A. M., Feagin, J. R., & Sjoberg, G. (1991). Introduction: The nature of the case study. In J. R.
Feagin, A. M. Orum, & G. Sjoberg (Eds.), A case for the case study (pp. 1–26). Chapel Hill, NC:
University of North Carolina Press.
Pennington, N., & Hastie, R. (1986). Evidence evaluation in complex decision making. Journal of
Personality and Social Psychology, 51, 242–258.
Przeworski, A., & Teune, H. (1970). The logic of comparative social inquiry. New York: Wiley.
Ragin, C. C. (1987). The comparative method: Moving beyond qualitative and quantitative strategies.
Berkeley, CA: University of California Press.
Ragin, C. C., & Becker, H. S. (Eds.) (1992). What is a case?: Exploring the foundationsof social inquiry.
Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Raven, B. H. (1990). Political applications of the psychology of interpersonal influence and social
power. Political Psychology, 11, 493–520.
Steinberg, B. S. (1991). Shame and humiliation in the Cuban missile crisis: A psychoanalytic
perspective. Political Psychology, 12, 653–690.
Suedfeld, P. (1992). Bilateral relations between countries and the complexity of newspaper editorials.
Political Psychology, 13, 601–612.
Tetlock, P. E. (1983). Psychological research on foreign policy: A methodological review. Review of
Personality and Social Psychology, 4, 45–78.
Tiedens, L. Z. (1997). Optimism and revolt of the oppressed: A comparison of two Polish Jewish ghettos
of World War II. Political Psychology, 18, 45–69.
Van Evera, S. (1996). Guide to methodology for students of political science. Cambridge, MA: Defense
and Arms Control Studies Program, Massachusetts Institute of Technology.
Yin, R. (1994). Case study research. Beverly Hills, CA: Sage.
Comparative Case Study 391

More Related Content

Similar to A Practical Guide To The Comparative Case Study Method In Political Psychology

128649542 case-study
128649542 case-study128649542 case-study
128649542 case-studyhomeworkping8
 
An Argument Analysis Contribution To Interpretive Public Policy Analysis
An Argument Analysis Contribution To Interpretive Public Policy AnalysisAn Argument Analysis Contribution To Interpretive Public Policy Analysis
An Argument Analysis Contribution To Interpretive Public Policy AnalysisKate Campbell
 
Approaches to International Relations.pdf
Approaches to International Relations.pdfApproaches to International Relations.pdf
Approaches to International Relations.pdfMuhammad Aamir
 
Week 6 introduction to case study
Week 6 introduction to case studyWeek 6 introduction to case study
Week 6 introduction to case studyDr. Russell Rodrigo
 
Qualitative Research and Family Psychology by Jane F. Gilgun
Qualitative Research and Family Psychology by Jane F. GilgunQualitative Research and Family Psychology by Jane F. Gilgun
Qualitative Research and Family Psychology by Jane F. GilgunJim Bloyd
 
Sale mixed methods
Sale mixed methodsSale mixed methods
Sale mixed methodspsdeeren
 
FINDING YOUR STORY DATA ANALYSISCH. 7 Finding Your Story Data
FINDING YOUR STORY DATA ANALYSISCH. 7 Finding Your Story Data FINDING YOUR STORY DATA ANALYSISCH. 7 Finding Your Story Data
FINDING YOUR STORY DATA ANALYSISCH. 7 Finding Your Story Data MerrileeDelvalle969
 
AUTHOR YIN CHAPTER 1Chapter 1 Plan¡ Identify the relev.docx
AUTHOR YIN CHAPTER 1Chapter 1 Plan¡ Identify the relev.docxAUTHOR YIN CHAPTER 1Chapter 1 Plan¡ Identify the relev.docx
AUTHOR YIN CHAPTER 1Chapter 1 Plan¡ Identify the relev.docxcelenarouzie
 
Article Summary • Introduction (1 – 2 Sentences) •.docx
Article Summary • Introduction (1 – 2 Sentences) •.docxArticle Summary • Introduction (1 – 2 Sentences) •.docx
Article Summary • Introduction (1 – 2 Sentences) •.docxfestockton
 
Methodological issues in cross.pdf
Methodological issues in cross.pdfMethodological issues in cross.pdf
Methodological issues in cross.pdfAsmar37
 
Unit 1 Comparative methods and Approaches
Unit 1 Comparative methods and ApproachesUnit 1 Comparative methods and Approaches
Unit 1 Comparative methods and ApproachesYash Agarwal
 
An Array Of Qualitative Data Analysis Tools A Call For Data Analysis Triangu...
An Array Of Qualitative Data Analysis Tools  A Call For Data Analysis Triangu...An Array Of Qualitative Data Analysis Tools  A Call For Data Analysis Triangu...
An Array Of Qualitative Data Analysis Tools A Call For Data Analysis Triangu...Angie Miller
 
CP 501 LECTURE ONE political science annavvsnsnvbshbsbbbshnsbhshjsjvshhsjsjbs...
CP 501 LECTURE ONE political science annavvsnsnvbshbsbbbshnsbhshjsjvshhsjsjbs...CP 501 LECTURE ONE political science annavvsnsnvbshbsbbbshnsbhshjsjvshhsjsjbs...
CP 501 LECTURE ONE political science annavvsnsnvbshbsbbbshnsbhshjsjvshhsjsjbs...DishuSingh8
 
ANYTIME YOU COMPLETE A PAPERESSAY in this course you must follow .docx
ANYTIME YOU COMPLETE A PAPERESSAY in this course you must follow .docxANYTIME YOU COMPLETE A PAPERESSAY in this course you must follow .docx
ANYTIME YOU COMPLETE A PAPERESSAY in this course you must follow .docxfestockton
 
Evaluating The Theoretical And Or Methodological Approach...
Evaluating The Theoretical And Or Methodological Approach...Evaluating The Theoretical And Or Methodological Approach...
Evaluating The Theoretical And Or Methodological Approach...Sandra Ahn
 
Page 291LEARNING OBJECTIVES¡ Discuss the issues created by.docx
Page 291LEARNING OBJECTIVES¡ Discuss the issues created by.docxPage 291LEARNING OBJECTIVES¡ Discuss the issues created by.docx
Page 291LEARNING OBJECTIVES¡ Discuss the issues created by.docxkarlhennesey
 
Copyright Š 2013, 2011, 2009 Pearson Education, Inc. All right.docx
Copyright Š 2013, 2011, 2009 Pearson Education, Inc. All right.docxCopyright Š 2013, 2011, 2009 Pearson Education, Inc. All right.docx
Copyright Š 2013, 2011, 2009 Pearson Education, Inc. All right.docxmaxinesmith73660
 
case study
case studycase study
case studySunny Shah
 
Eval Rev-2016-Valentine-0193841X16674421
Eval Rev-2016-Valentine-0193841X16674421Eval Rev-2016-Valentine-0193841X16674421
Eval Rev-2016-Valentine-0193841X16674421Lisa Foster
 

Similar to A Practical Guide To The Comparative Case Study Method In Political Psychology (20)

128649542 case-study
128649542 case-study128649542 case-study
128649542 case-study
 
An Argument Analysis Contribution To Interpretive Public Policy Analysis
An Argument Analysis Contribution To Interpretive Public Policy AnalysisAn Argument Analysis Contribution To Interpretive Public Policy Analysis
An Argument Analysis Contribution To Interpretive Public Policy Analysis
 
Approaches to International Relations.pdf
Approaches to International Relations.pdfApproaches to International Relations.pdf
Approaches to International Relations.pdf
 
Week 6 introduction to case study
Week 6 introduction to case studyWeek 6 introduction to case study
Week 6 introduction to case study
 
Qualitative Research and Family Psychology by Jane F. Gilgun
Qualitative Research and Family Psychology by Jane F. GilgunQualitative Research and Family Psychology by Jane F. Gilgun
Qualitative Research and Family Psychology by Jane F. Gilgun
 
Sale mixed methods
Sale mixed methodsSale mixed methods
Sale mixed methods
 
FINDING YOUR STORY DATA ANALYSISCH. 7 Finding Your Story Data
FINDING YOUR STORY DATA ANALYSISCH. 7 Finding Your Story Data FINDING YOUR STORY DATA ANALYSISCH. 7 Finding Your Story Data
FINDING YOUR STORY DATA ANALYSISCH. 7 Finding Your Story Data
 
The Nature And Future Of Comparative Politics
The Nature And Future Of Comparative PoliticsThe Nature And Future Of Comparative Politics
The Nature And Future Of Comparative Politics
 
AUTHOR YIN CHAPTER 1Chapter 1 Plan¡ Identify the relev.docx
AUTHOR YIN CHAPTER 1Chapter 1 Plan¡ Identify the relev.docxAUTHOR YIN CHAPTER 1Chapter 1 Plan¡ Identify the relev.docx
AUTHOR YIN CHAPTER 1Chapter 1 Plan¡ Identify the relev.docx
 
Article Summary • Introduction (1 – 2 Sentences) •.docx
Article Summary • Introduction (1 – 2 Sentences) •.docxArticle Summary • Introduction (1 – 2 Sentences) •.docx
Article Summary • Introduction (1 – 2 Sentences) •.docx
 
Methodological issues in cross.pdf
Methodological issues in cross.pdfMethodological issues in cross.pdf
Methodological issues in cross.pdf
 
Unit 1 Comparative methods and Approaches
Unit 1 Comparative methods and ApproachesUnit 1 Comparative methods and Approaches
Unit 1 Comparative methods and Approaches
 
An Array Of Qualitative Data Analysis Tools A Call For Data Analysis Triangu...
An Array Of Qualitative Data Analysis Tools  A Call For Data Analysis Triangu...An Array Of Qualitative Data Analysis Tools  A Call For Data Analysis Triangu...
An Array Of Qualitative Data Analysis Tools A Call For Data Analysis Triangu...
 
CP 501 LECTURE ONE political science annavvsnsnvbshbsbbbshnsbhshjsjvshhsjsjbs...
CP 501 LECTURE ONE political science annavvsnsnvbshbsbbbshnsbhshjsjvshhsjsjbs...CP 501 LECTURE ONE political science annavvsnsnvbshbsbbbshnsbhshjsjvshhsjsjbs...
CP 501 LECTURE ONE political science annavvsnsnvbshbsbbbshnsbhshjsjvshhsjsjbs...
 
ANYTIME YOU COMPLETE A PAPERESSAY in this course you must follow .docx
ANYTIME YOU COMPLETE A PAPERESSAY in this course you must follow .docxANYTIME YOU COMPLETE A PAPERESSAY in this course you must follow .docx
ANYTIME YOU COMPLETE A PAPERESSAY in this course you must follow .docx
 
Evaluating The Theoretical And Or Methodological Approach...
Evaluating The Theoretical And Or Methodological Approach...Evaluating The Theoretical And Or Methodological Approach...
Evaluating The Theoretical And Or Methodological Approach...
 
Page 291LEARNING OBJECTIVES¡ Discuss the issues created by.docx
Page 291LEARNING OBJECTIVES¡ Discuss the issues created by.docxPage 291LEARNING OBJECTIVES¡ Discuss the issues created by.docx
Page 291LEARNING OBJECTIVES¡ Discuss the issues created by.docx
 
Copyright Š 2013, 2011, 2009 Pearson Education, Inc. All right.docx
Copyright Š 2013, 2011, 2009 Pearson Education, Inc. All right.docxCopyright Š 2013, 2011, 2009 Pearson Education, Inc. All right.docx
Copyright Š 2013, 2011, 2009 Pearson Education, Inc. All right.docx
 
case study
case studycase study
case study
 
Eval Rev-2016-Valentine-0193841X16674421
Eval Rev-2016-Valentine-0193841X16674421Eval Rev-2016-Valentine-0193841X16674421
Eval Rev-2016-Valentine-0193841X16674421
 

More from Dustin Pytko

Critique Response Sample Peer Review Feedback Fo
Critique Response Sample Peer Review Feedback FoCritique Response Sample Peer Review Feedback Fo
Critique Response Sample Peer Review Feedback FoDustin Pytko
 
How To Write Better Essays (12 Best Tips)
How To Write Better Essays (12 Best Tips)How To Write Better Essays (12 Best Tips)
How To Write Better Essays (12 Best Tips)Dustin Pytko
 
How To Write A 500-Word Essay About - Agnew Text
How To Write A 500-Word Essay About - Agnew TextHow To Write A 500-Word Essay About - Agnew Text
How To Write A 500-Word Essay About - Agnew TextDustin Pytko
 
Sample On Project Management By Instant E
Sample On Project Management By Instant ESample On Project Management By Instant E
Sample On Project Management By Instant EDustin Pytko
 
Gingerbread Stationary Stationary Printable Free,
Gingerbread Stationary Stationary Printable Free,Gingerbread Stationary Stationary Printable Free,
Gingerbread Stationary Stationary Printable Free,Dustin Pytko
 
The Creative Spirit Graffiti Challenge 55 Graffiti Art Lett
The Creative Spirit Graffiti Challenge 55 Graffiti Art LettThe Creative Spirit Graffiti Challenge 55 Graffiti Art Lett
The Creative Spirit Graffiti Challenge 55 Graffiti Art LettDustin Pytko
 
My First Day At College - GCSE English - Marked B
My First Day At College - GCSE English - Marked BMy First Day At College - GCSE English - Marked B
My First Day At College - GCSE English - Marked BDustin Pytko
 
💋 The Help Movie Analysis Essay. The Help Film Anal.pdf
💋 The Help Movie Analysis Essay. The Help Film Anal.pdf💋 The Help Movie Analysis Essay. The Help Film Anal.pdf
💋 The Help Movie Analysis Essay. The Help Film Anal.pdfDustin Pytko
 
Essay Writing Step-By-Step A Newsweek Education Pr
Essay Writing Step-By-Step A Newsweek Education PrEssay Writing Step-By-Step A Newsweek Education Pr
Essay Writing Step-By-Step A Newsweek Education PrDustin Pytko
 
Writing A Dialogue Paper. How To Format Dialogue (Includes Exampl
Writing A Dialogue Paper. How To Format Dialogue (Includes ExamplWriting A Dialogue Paper. How To Format Dialogue (Includes Exampl
Writing A Dialogue Paper. How To Format Dialogue (Includes ExamplDustin Pytko
 
Sociology Essay Writing. Online assignment writing service.
Sociology Essay Writing. Online assignment writing service.Sociology Essay Writing. Online assignment writing service.
Sociology Essay Writing. Online assignment writing service.Dustin Pytko
 
Essay On Importance Of Education In 150 Words. Sh
Essay On Importance Of Education In 150 Words. ShEssay On Importance Of Education In 150 Words. Sh
Essay On Importance Of Education In 150 Words. ShDustin Pytko
 
Types Of Essays We Can Write For You Types Of Essay, E
Types Of Essays We Can Write For You Types Of Essay, ETypes Of Essays We Can Write For You Types Of Essay, E
Types Of Essays We Can Write For You Types Of Essay, EDustin Pytko
 
Lined Paper For Writing Notebook Paper Template,
Lined Paper For Writing Notebook Paper Template,Lined Paper For Writing Notebook Paper Template,
Lined Paper For Writing Notebook Paper Template,Dustin Pytko
 
Research Paper Executive Summary Synopsis Writin
Research Paper Executive Summary Synopsis WritinResearch Paper Executive Summary Synopsis Writin
Research Paper Executive Summary Synopsis WritinDustin Pytko
 
Uk Best Essay Service. Order Best Essays In UK
Uk Best Essay Service. Order Best Essays In UKUk Best Essay Service. Order Best Essays In UK
Uk Best Essay Service. Order Best Essays In UKDustin Pytko
 
What Is The Body Of A Paragraph. How To Write A Body Paragraph For A
What Is The Body Of A Paragraph. How To Write A Body Paragraph For AWhat Is The Body Of A Paragraph. How To Write A Body Paragraph For A
What Is The Body Of A Paragraph. How To Write A Body Paragraph For ADustin Pytko
 
My Handwriting , . Online assignment writing service.
My Handwriting , . Online assignment writing service.My Handwriting , . Online assignment writing service.
My Handwriting , . Online assignment writing service.Dustin Pytko
 
How To Stay Calm During Exam And Term Paper Writi
How To Stay Calm During Exam And Term Paper WritiHow To Stay Calm During Exam And Term Paper Writi
How To Stay Calm During Exam And Term Paper WritiDustin Pytko
 
Image Result For Fundations Letter Formation Page Fundations
Image Result For Fundations Letter Formation Page FundationsImage Result For Fundations Letter Formation Page Fundations
Image Result For Fundations Letter Formation Page FundationsDustin Pytko
 

More from Dustin Pytko (20)

Critique Response Sample Peer Review Feedback Fo
Critique Response Sample Peer Review Feedback FoCritique Response Sample Peer Review Feedback Fo
Critique Response Sample Peer Review Feedback Fo
 
How To Write Better Essays (12 Best Tips)
How To Write Better Essays (12 Best Tips)How To Write Better Essays (12 Best Tips)
How To Write Better Essays (12 Best Tips)
 
How To Write A 500-Word Essay About - Agnew Text
How To Write A 500-Word Essay About - Agnew TextHow To Write A 500-Word Essay About - Agnew Text
How To Write A 500-Word Essay About - Agnew Text
 
Sample On Project Management By Instant E
Sample On Project Management By Instant ESample On Project Management By Instant E
Sample On Project Management By Instant E
 
Gingerbread Stationary Stationary Printable Free,
Gingerbread Stationary Stationary Printable Free,Gingerbread Stationary Stationary Printable Free,
Gingerbread Stationary Stationary Printable Free,
 
The Creative Spirit Graffiti Challenge 55 Graffiti Art Lett
The Creative Spirit Graffiti Challenge 55 Graffiti Art LettThe Creative Spirit Graffiti Challenge 55 Graffiti Art Lett
The Creative Spirit Graffiti Challenge 55 Graffiti Art Lett
 
My First Day At College - GCSE English - Marked B
My First Day At College - GCSE English - Marked BMy First Day At College - GCSE English - Marked B
My First Day At College - GCSE English - Marked B
 
💋 The Help Movie Analysis Essay. The Help Film Anal.pdf
💋 The Help Movie Analysis Essay. The Help Film Anal.pdf💋 The Help Movie Analysis Essay. The Help Film Anal.pdf
💋 The Help Movie Analysis Essay. The Help Film Anal.pdf
 
Essay Writing Step-By-Step A Newsweek Education Pr
Essay Writing Step-By-Step A Newsweek Education PrEssay Writing Step-By-Step A Newsweek Education Pr
Essay Writing Step-By-Step A Newsweek Education Pr
 
Writing A Dialogue Paper. How To Format Dialogue (Includes Exampl
Writing A Dialogue Paper. How To Format Dialogue (Includes ExamplWriting A Dialogue Paper. How To Format Dialogue (Includes Exampl
Writing A Dialogue Paper. How To Format Dialogue (Includes Exampl
 
Sociology Essay Writing. Online assignment writing service.
Sociology Essay Writing. Online assignment writing service.Sociology Essay Writing. Online assignment writing service.
Sociology Essay Writing. Online assignment writing service.
 
Essay On Importance Of Education In 150 Words. Sh
Essay On Importance Of Education In 150 Words. ShEssay On Importance Of Education In 150 Words. Sh
Essay On Importance Of Education In 150 Words. Sh
 
Types Of Essays We Can Write For You Types Of Essay, E
Types Of Essays We Can Write For You Types Of Essay, ETypes Of Essays We Can Write For You Types Of Essay, E
Types Of Essays We Can Write For You Types Of Essay, E
 
Lined Paper For Writing Notebook Paper Template,
Lined Paper For Writing Notebook Paper Template,Lined Paper For Writing Notebook Paper Template,
Lined Paper For Writing Notebook Paper Template,
 
Research Paper Executive Summary Synopsis Writin
Research Paper Executive Summary Synopsis WritinResearch Paper Executive Summary Synopsis Writin
Research Paper Executive Summary Synopsis Writin
 
Uk Best Essay Service. Order Best Essays In UK
Uk Best Essay Service. Order Best Essays In UKUk Best Essay Service. Order Best Essays In UK
Uk Best Essay Service. Order Best Essays In UK
 
What Is The Body Of A Paragraph. How To Write A Body Paragraph For A
What Is The Body Of A Paragraph. How To Write A Body Paragraph For AWhat Is The Body Of A Paragraph. How To Write A Body Paragraph For A
What Is The Body Of A Paragraph. How To Write A Body Paragraph For A
 
My Handwriting , . Online assignment writing service.
My Handwriting , . Online assignment writing service.My Handwriting , . Online assignment writing service.
My Handwriting , . Online assignment writing service.
 
How To Stay Calm During Exam And Term Paper Writi
How To Stay Calm During Exam And Term Paper WritiHow To Stay Calm During Exam And Term Paper Writi
How To Stay Calm During Exam And Term Paper Writi
 
Image Result For Fundations Letter Formation Page Fundations
Image Result For Fundations Letter Formation Page FundationsImage Result For Fundations Letter Formation Page Fundations
Image Result For Fundations Letter Formation Page Fundations
 

Recently uploaded

psychiatric nursing HISTORY COLLECTION .docx
psychiatric  nursing HISTORY  COLLECTION  .docxpsychiatric  nursing HISTORY  COLLECTION  .docx
psychiatric nursing HISTORY COLLECTION .docxPoojaSen20
 
Grant Readiness 101 TechSoup and Remy Consulting
Grant Readiness 101 TechSoup and Remy ConsultingGrant Readiness 101 TechSoup and Remy Consulting
Grant Readiness 101 TechSoup and Remy ConsultingTechSoup
 
Role Of Transgenic Animal In Target Validation-1.pptx
Role Of Transgenic Animal In Target Validation-1.pptxRole Of Transgenic Animal In Target Validation-1.pptx
Role Of Transgenic Animal In Target Validation-1.pptxNikitaBankoti2
 
Basic Civil Engineering first year Notes- Chapter 4 Building.pptx
Basic Civil Engineering first year Notes- Chapter 4 Building.pptxBasic Civil Engineering first year Notes- Chapter 4 Building.pptx
Basic Civil Engineering first year Notes- Chapter 4 Building.pptxDenish Jangid
 
Z Score,T Score, Percential Rank and Box Plot Graph
Z Score,T Score, Percential Rank and Box Plot GraphZ Score,T Score, Percential Rank and Box Plot Graph
Z Score,T Score, Percential Rank and Box Plot GraphThiyagu K
 
Measures of Dispersion and Variability: Range, QD, AD and SD
Measures of Dispersion and Variability: Range, QD, AD and SDMeasures of Dispersion and Variability: Range, QD, AD and SD
Measures of Dispersion and Variability: Range, QD, AD and SDThiyagu K
 
Explore beautiful and ugly buildings. Mathematics helps us create beautiful d...
Explore beautiful and ugly buildings. Mathematics helps us create beautiful d...Explore beautiful and ugly buildings. Mathematics helps us create beautiful d...
Explore beautiful and ugly buildings. Mathematics helps us create beautiful d...christianmathematics
 
Presentation by Andreas Schleicher Tackling the School Absenteeism Crisis 30 ...
Presentation by Andreas Schleicher Tackling the School Absenteeism Crisis 30 ...Presentation by Andreas Schleicher Tackling the School Absenteeism Crisis 30 ...
Presentation by Andreas Schleicher Tackling the School Absenteeism Crisis 30 ...EduSkills OECD
 
microwave assisted reaction. General introduction
microwave assisted reaction. General introductionmicrowave assisted reaction. General introduction
microwave assisted reaction. General introductionMaksud Ahmed
 
Mixin Classes in Odoo 17 How to Extend Models Using Mixin Classes
Mixin Classes in Odoo 17  How to Extend Models Using Mixin ClassesMixin Classes in Odoo 17  How to Extend Models Using Mixin Classes
Mixin Classes in Odoo 17 How to Extend Models Using Mixin ClassesCeline George
 
Energy Resources. ( B. Pharmacy, 1st Year, Sem-II) Natural Resources
Energy Resources. ( B. Pharmacy, 1st Year, Sem-II) Natural ResourcesEnergy Resources. ( B. Pharmacy, 1st Year, Sem-II) Natural Resources
Energy Resources. ( B. Pharmacy, 1st Year, Sem-II) Natural ResourcesShubhangi Sonawane
 
The basics of sentences session 2pptx copy.pptx
The basics of sentences session 2pptx copy.pptxThe basics of sentences session 2pptx copy.pptx
The basics of sentences session 2pptx copy.pptxheathfieldcps1
 
Micro-Scholarship, What it is, How can it help me.pdf
Micro-Scholarship, What it is, How can it help me.pdfMicro-Scholarship, What it is, How can it help me.pdf
Micro-Scholarship, What it is, How can it help me.pdfPoh-Sun Goh
 
Web & Social Media Analytics Previous Year Question Paper.pdf
Web & Social Media Analytics Previous Year Question Paper.pdfWeb & Social Media Analytics Previous Year Question Paper.pdf
Web & Social Media Analytics Previous Year Question Paper.pdfJayanti Pande
 
TỔNG ÔN TẬP THI VÀO LỚP 10 MÔN TIẾNG ANH NĂM HỌC 2023 - 2024 CÓ ĐÁP ÁN (NGỮ Â...
TỔNG ÔN TẬP THI VÀO LỚP 10 MÔN TIẾNG ANH NĂM HỌC 2023 - 2024 CÓ ĐÁP ÁN (NGỮ Â...TỔNG ÔN TẬP THI VÀO LỚP 10 MÔN TIẾNG ANH NĂM HỌC 2023 - 2024 CÓ ĐÁP ÁN (NGỮ Â...
TỔNG ÔN TẬP THI VÀO LỚP 10 MÔN TIẾNG ANH NĂM HỌC 2023 - 2024 CÓ ĐÁP ÁN (NGỮ Â...Nguyen Thanh Tu Collection
 
Food Chain and Food Web (Ecosystem) EVS, B. Pharmacy 1st Year, Sem-II
Food Chain and Food Web (Ecosystem) EVS, B. Pharmacy 1st Year, Sem-IIFood Chain and Food Web (Ecosystem) EVS, B. Pharmacy 1st Year, Sem-II
Food Chain and Food Web (Ecosystem) EVS, B. Pharmacy 1st Year, Sem-IIShubhangi Sonawane
 
Class 11th Physics NEET formula sheet pdf
Class 11th Physics NEET formula sheet pdfClass 11th Physics NEET formula sheet pdf
Class 11th Physics NEET formula sheet pdfAyushMahapatra5
 
Russian Escort Service in Delhi 11k Hotel Foreigner Russian Call Girls in Delhi
Russian Escort Service in Delhi 11k Hotel Foreigner Russian Call Girls in DelhiRussian Escort Service in Delhi 11k Hotel Foreigner Russian Call Girls in Delhi
Russian Escort Service in Delhi 11k Hotel Foreigner Russian Call Girls in Delhikauryashika82
 
1029-Danh muc Sach Giao Khoa khoi 6.pdf
1029-Danh muc Sach Giao Khoa khoi  6.pdf1029-Danh muc Sach Giao Khoa khoi  6.pdf
1029-Danh muc Sach Giao Khoa khoi 6.pdfQucHHunhnh
 

Recently uploaded (20)

psychiatric nursing HISTORY COLLECTION .docx
psychiatric  nursing HISTORY  COLLECTION  .docxpsychiatric  nursing HISTORY  COLLECTION  .docx
psychiatric nursing HISTORY COLLECTION .docx
 
Grant Readiness 101 TechSoup and Remy Consulting
Grant Readiness 101 TechSoup and Remy ConsultingGrant Readiness 101 TechSoup and Remy Consulting
Grant Readiness 101 TechSoup and Remy Consulting
 
Role Of Transgenic Animal In Target Validation-1.pptx
Role Of Transgenic Animal In Target Validation-1.pptxRole Of Transgenic Animal In Target Validation-1.pptx
Role Of Transgenic Animal In Target Validation-1.pptx
 
Basic Civil Engineering first year Notes- Chapter 4 Building.pptx
Basic Civil Engineering first year Notes- Chapter 4 Building.pptxBasic Civil Engineering first year Notes- Chapter 4 Building.pptx
Basic Civil Engineering first year Notes- Chapter 4 Building.pptx
 
Z Score,T Score, Percential Rank and Box Plot Graph
Z Score,T Score, Percential Rank and Box Plot GraphZ Score,T Score, Percential Rank and Box Plot Graph
Z Score,T Score, Percential Rank and Box Plot Graph
 
Measures of Dispersion and Variability: Range, QD, AD and SD
Measures of Dispersion and Variability: Range, QD, AD and SDMeasures of Dispersion and Variability: Range, QD, AD and SD
Measures of Dispersion and Variability: Range, QD, AD and SD
 
Explore beautiful and ugly buildings. Mathematics helps us create beautiful d...
Explore beautiful and ugly buildings. Mathematics helps us create beautiful d...Explore beautiful and ugly buildings. Mathematics helps us create beautiful d...
Explore beautiful and ugly buildings. Mathematics helps us create beautiful d...
 
Presentation by Andreas Schleicher Tackling the School Absenteeism Crisis 30 ...
Presentation by Andreas Schleicher Tackling the School Absenteeism Crisis 30 ...Presentation by Andreas Schleicher Tackling the School Absenteeism Crisis 30 ...
Presentation by Andreas Schleicher Tackling the School Absenteeism Crisis 30 ...
 
microwave assisted reaction. General introduction
microwave assisted reaction. General introductionmicrowave assisted reaction. General introduction
microwave assisted reaction. General introduction
 
Mixin Classes in Odoo 17 How to Extend Models Using Mixin Classes
Mixin Classes in Odoo 17  How to Extend Models Using Mixin ClassesMixin Classes in Odoo 17  How to Extend Models Using Mixin Classes
Mixin Classes in Odoo 17 How to Extend Models Using Mixin Classes
 
INDIA QUIZ 2024 RLAC DELHI UNIVERSITY.pptx
INDIA QUIZ 2024 RLAC DELHI UNIVERSITY.pptxINDIA QUIZ 2024 RLAC DELHI UNIVERSITY.pptx
INDIA QUIZ 2024 RLAC DELHI UNIVERSITY.pptx
 
Energy Resources. ( B. Pharmacy, 1st Year, Sem-II) Natural Resources
Energy Resources. ( B. Pharmacy, 1st Year, Sem-II) Natural ResourcesEnergy Resources. ( B. Pharmacy, 1st Year, Sem-II) Natural Resources
Energy Resources. ( B. Pharmacy, 1st Year, Sem-II) Natural Resources
 
The basics of sentences session 2pptx copy.pptx
The basics of sentences session 2pptx copy.pptxThe basics of sentences session 2pptx copy.pptx
The basics of sentences session 2pptx copy.pptx
 
Micro-Scholarship, What it is, How can it help me.pdf
Micro-Scholarship, What it is, How can it help me.pdfMicro-Scholarship, What it is, How can it help me.pdf
Micro-Scholarship, What it is, How can it help me.pdf
 
Web & Social Media Analytics Previous Year Question Paper.pdf
Web & Social Media Analytics Previous Year Question Paper.pdfWeb & Social Media Analytics Previous Year Question Paper.pdf
Web & Social Media Analytics Previous Year Question Paper.pdf
 
TỔNG ÔN TẬP THI VÀO LỚP 10 MÔN TIẾNG ANH NĂM HỌC 2023 - 2024 CÓ ĐÁP ÁN (NGỮ Â...
TỔNG ÔN TẬP THI VÀO LỚP 10 MÔN TIẾNG ANH NĂM HỌC 2023 - 2024 CÓ ĐÁP ÁN (NGỮ Â...TỔNG ÔN TẬP THI VÀO LỚP 10 MÔN TIẾNG ANH NĂM HỌC 2023 - 2024 CÓ ĐÁP ÁN (NGỮ Â...
TỔNG ÔN TẬP THI VÀO LỚP 10 MÔN TIẾNG ANH NĂM HỌC 2023 - 2024 CÓ ĐÁP ÁN (NGỮ Â...
 
Food Chain and Food Web (Ecosystem) EVS, B. Pharmacy 1st Year, Sem-II
Food Chain and Food Web (Ecosystem) EVS, B. Pharmacy 1st Year, Sem-IIFood Chain and Food Web (Ecosystem) EVS, B. Pharmacy 1st Year, Sem-II
Food Chain and Food Web (Ecosystem) EVS, B. Pharmacy 1st Year, Sem-II
 
Class 11th Physics NEET formula sheet pdf
Class 11th Physics NEET formula sheet pdfClass 11th Physics NEET formula sheet pdf
Class 11th Physics NEET formula sheet pdf
 
Russian Escort Service in Delhi 11k Hotel Foreigner Russian Call Girls in Delhi
Russian Escort Service in Delhi 11k Hotel Foreigner Russian Call Girls in DelhiRussian Escort Service in Delhi 11k Hotel Foreigner Russian Call Girls in Delhi
Russian Escort Service in Delhi 11k Hotel Foreigner Russian Call Girls in Delhi
 
1029-Danh muc Sach Giao Khoa khoi 6.pdf
1029-Danh muc Sach Giao Khoa khoi  6.pdf1029-Danh muc Sach Giao Khoa khoi  6.pdf
1029-Danh muc Sach Giao Khoa khoi 6.pdf
 

A Practical Guide To The Comparative Case Study Method In Political Psychology

  • 1. A Practical Guide to the Comparative Case Study Method in Political Psychology Juliet Kaarbo University of Kansas Ryan K. Beasley Baker University The case study, as a method of inquiry, is particularly suited to the field of political psychology. Yet there is little training in political science, and even less in psychology, on how to do case study research. Furthermore, misconceptions about case studies contribute to the methodological barrier that exists within and between the two parent disciplines. This paper reviews the various definitions and uses of case studies and integrates a number of recent insights and advances into a practical guide for conducting case study research. To this end, the paper discusses various stereotypes of the case study and offers specific steps aimed at addressing these criticisms. KEY WORDS: case study, comparative method, research design. The field of political psychology has become increasingly populated with individuals capable of speaking both the language of political science and the language of psychology. This is valuable for building theory and understanding the nature of the empirical domains of interest. Methodologically, however, there remain some persistent obstacles between the two parent disciplines that, in our view, unnecessarily hinder empirical cross-fertilization and advancement. Both social psychologists and political scientists, for example, study the structure and content of mass belief systems, but tend to use different methods (experiments and surveys) and different statistical techniques (analyses of variance and regressions). Margaret Hermann (1989) has indicated that methodological differences between political science and psychology threaten to make the promise of a field of political psychologymerelyafantasy.Ifwearegoingtocontinuetodevelopaninterdisciplinary Political Psychology, Vol. 20, No. 2, 1999 369 0162-895X Š 1999 International Society of Political Psychology Published by Blackwell Publishers, 350 Main Street, Malden, MA 02148, USA, and 108 Cowley Road, Oxford, OX4 1JF, UK.
  • 2. language, methodological dialogue must replace the current tendency toward simultaneous monologues. Nowhere is the methodological gulf in understanding wider than when politi- cal psychologists contemplate case study research (Tetlock, 1983). Indeed, a review of the articles published in Political Psychology over the past decade reveals that the case study is one of the most popular methods, but is most often used by political scientists—especially those who study American leadership, international relations, and comparative politics.1 Psychologists who publish in this journal tend to use survey, experimental, or content-analytic techniques.2 Psychologists only occasionally use the case study method.3 Practitioners of case study research, whether political scientists or psy- chologists, lack a common language for their investigation. Alexander George’s (George, 1979; George & Bennett, in press; George & McKeown, 1985; George & Smoke, 1974) method of “structured-focused” comparison is perhaps the most used resource for those engaging in this methodology. His articulation of the procedures involved in the case study method have provided a sturdy methodologi- cal foundation for hosts of empirical investigations. Yet many are less than forthright in articulating the rationale of their chosen methodology. Indeed, Ragin (1987) suggested that “most investigators who use case-oriented strategies . . . are not self-consciously methodological; that is, they do not regard the case-oriented strategies they use as formal methodologies” (p. 34). Although there has been a resurgence of attention to the case study method within a variety of disciplines (see, e.g., Orum, Feagin, & Sjoberg, 1991), we argue that there has been insufficient attention to and internalization of these develop- ments within political psychologists’ parent disciplines. This is true despite the degree to which many political scientists embrace this technique and the tendency for many psychologists toeschewit. Our purpose here is tosummarize and integrate a number of these recent insights and advances into a practical guide for conducting case study research, thereby providing greater direction and a common language for those engaging in this type of research and highlighting the merits and value of this technique for those more inclined to avoid it. Toward this end, we draw on 1 Recent examples of case study research in these subfields include Fishel’s (1992) case study of a California legislator’s development over time, Gilbert’s (1995) case study of President Johnson for the political effects of illness, Steinberg’s (1991) case study of the Cuban Missile Crisis for the role of shame and humiliation in conflict deterrence, Farnham’s (1992) case study of Roosevelt during the Munich Crisis as an example of prospect theory, and Groth and Britton’s (1993) case study of the Soviet Union for the psychopolitical foundations of a garrison state. 2 Recent examples of research with these techniques include Fife-Schaw and Breakwell’s (1990) survey of teenagers to predict the intention not to vote, Milburn and McGrail’s (1992) experiment on the effects of cognitive complexity of news presentation, and Suedfeld’s (1992) content analysis of newspaper editorials during times of positive and negative relations between pairs of countries. 3 Exceptions include Raven’s (1990) case study of the MacArthur-Truman relationship as an example of interpersonal influence and Tiedens’ (1997) comparative case study oftwoPolish ghettos for violent resistance during the Second World War. 370 Kaarbo and Beasley
  • 3. numerous writings on the case study and comparative case study methods. We are not intending to provide an array of novel reasons to use the case study method. Rather, we are attempting to integrate observations regarding this method into a coherent program for its use in a research effort, with particular attention to the field of political psychology. In this sense, we are not comparing the case study method to other methodologies, but instead are attempting to address various perceived shortcomings and stereotypically founded criticisms within a practical guide for actually doing case study research. In this effort, we explicitly avoid any direct discussion of numerous and engaging epistemological issues that are part of the current debate in political science on the value of interpretation and the role of positivism in the social sciences.4 The case study method is particularly prone to evoke discussions of the nature of theories, the possibility of general laws governing human behavior, and other fundamental issues of investigation. Although important, these issues are not central to our discussion. Instead, our effort concerns the basic decisions and choices that must be made in the practical application of the case study method. In both political science and psychology, there exists a stereotype of the case study that includes an idiosyncratic, historically specific, and atheoretical charac- ter; a lack of control; an inability to generate a sufficient number of data points to test theory; and a highly suspect “interpretive” character that allows the analyst to draw favorable conclusions about hypotheses and to find support where no such support exists. Certainly, the ways in which case studies have been performed and used have frequently been rather divergent from the standards of systematic inquiry currently widely accepted in both political science and psychology. The reasons that the case study has fallen between the stools of methodological rigor and empirical value are numerous. Among political scientists, the compara- tive case study is often equated with cross-national comparison. The writings on case study research tend to be associated with the subfield of comparative politics and are not widely read in other subfields. Further, political scientists have paid insufficient attention to recent discussions of the comparative method and case study research found in public administration and sociology. Psychologists, on the other hand, tend to associate case study research with the clinical “case” or patient analysis and are generally unaware of the political science scholarship on the use of case studies. Overall, we believe that political psychologists could be made more aware of the different meanings of case study, the different types or purposes of case study research, and the steps involved in case study research design, and that they can make good use of the method as part of a systematic comparative methodology without necessarily succumbing to its traditional pitfalls. 4 See, for example, the “Review Symposium: The Qualitative-Quantitative Disputation: Gary King, Robert O. Keohane, and Sidney Verba’s Designing Social Inquiry: Scientific Inference in Qualitative Research” in the American Political Science Review (1995); George and Bennett (in press). Comparative Case Study 371
  • 4. Defining “Case Study” The terminology pertaining to “case studies” is frequently confusing. Ragin (1987) noted that “the term ‘case’ and the various terms linked to the idea of case analysis are not well defined in social science, despite their widespread usage and centrality to social scientific discourse” (p. 1). For example, a “case” may be a single instance or data point, such as a subject in an experiment, a survey respon- dent, or the non-occurrence of war between belligerents (King, Keohane, & Verba, 1994). Alternatively, it may be a uniquely bounded phenomenon in a historical or geographical sense, such as the case of Munich, the Soviet invasion of Afghanistan, or the Watts riots. Indeed, the term “case” can have a variety of meanings, each with important implications regarding empirical investigation. When the term “case” is joined by other terms such as “study,” “analysis,” or “method,” the picture does not become any clearer. For example, Orum et al. (1991) defined a case study as “an in-depth, multifaceted investigation, using qualitative research methods, of a single social phenomenon” (p. 2). Lijphart (1971, 1975) saw the case study as a single case that is closely associated with the comparative method, as contrasted with experimental and statistical methods. George and McKeown (1985) indicated that a case study focuses on within-case analysis to evaluate claims about causal process. Finally, Yin (1994) defined a case study as “an empirical inquiry that: investigates a contemporary phenomenon within its real-life context, especially when the boundaries between phenomenon and context are not clearly evident; and . . . [that] relies on multiple sources of evidence” (p. 13). Clearly there are different definitions of cases and case studies, and this has made systematic analyses of the value and purposes of this technique difficult. We recognize that coming to some consensual understanding of the terms “case,” “case study,” or, more generally, the “case method” is extremely difficult (see Ragin & Becker, 1992), yet without some understanding of what these terms are meant to convey, it is even more difficult to offer any practical guidance in carrying out an investigation. For our current purposes, we offer basic definitions of several terms. We consider a case to be an instance, or a data point, and in this sense we do not draw any distinctions about how a “case” is further defined or arrived at in an investigation. Cases can be experimentally derived measurements, survey responses, or classifications of historical events (such as “war/no war”). We define a case study to be a method of obtaining a “case” or a number of “cases” through an empirical examination of a real-world phenomenon within its naturally occurring context, without directly manipulating either the phenomenon or the context. The comparative case study is the systematic comparison of two or more data points (“cases”) obtained through use of the case study method. This definition has a number of implications. First, we do not assume a particular purpose of the investigation, such as developing causal inferences or detailing historical occurrences. A case study can be used for a variety of purposes, several of which we articulate below. Second, we do not assume that multiple 372 Kaarbo and Beasley
  • 5. sources of evidence must be used (as does Yin’s definition above), although we suspect that this will frequently be necessary. The case study, in other words, often uses a number of techniques for gathering information—from interviews to surveys to content analysis—but it is not necessary to use multiple sources or types of evidence in order to perform a case study. Third, we offer no particular distinction between qualitative and quantitative aspects of a case study, nor do we draw a particular distinction between narrative and analytic aspects (see Abbot, 1992; King et al., 1994). Indeed, we feel that case studies can be very qualitative and narrative in form, and they can be very quantitative and analytic in form. Fourth, the issues of context and manipulation are central to our definition. The phenome- non is studied as it occurs within its context, and neither the phenomenon nor the context are directly manipulated by the researcher. Finally, our definition allows for comparison of several case studies and is not limited to single case studies.5 Types of Case Studies Our definition of the case study does not imply a single purpose for an investigation. Indeed, there are many uses of case studies that have been outlined by a variety of scholars (e.g., Eckstein, 1975; Lijphart, 1971; Van Evera, 1996; Yin, 1994). In our effort to offer a practical guide for the case study method, we condense these previous discussions into a single, accessible form. In doing so, we hope to highlight the potential value of case studies for a variety of purposes as well as lay the foundation for our arguments regarding the need for greater systematicity in comparative case studies when theory development or testing is the goal. We present the different uses or types of case studies as though the investigator will be using a single case study, although this typology in no way limits analysts to investigate only one case at a time. Using Cases for Description The first type of case study is what Lijphart (1971) referred to as “atheoretical” and what Eckstein (1975) called “configurative-idiographic.” Here the analyst is principally interested in the case itself, and is typically attempting to gain a Gestalt or holistic picture of the event or phenomenon. Generally, with this use of case studies, the analyst is more interested in the case than in some theory or hypothesis. Certainly description requires some preconceived ideas or points of reference that might be considered the foundation of theory, but the principal purpose of the case 5 Several scholars within the social sciences have written extensively about the importance and purpose of the comparative method (see, for example, Collier, 1993; George, 1979; Lijphart, 1971, 1975; Ragin, 1987). The comparative method should, in our view, be distinguished and disentangled from the issues surrounding case studies and the case study method. Comparison is the very foundation of scientific methodology. In other words, comparison need not be distinguished from other methods, but can be considered to be a foundation of methodology itself. Comparative Case Study 373
  • 6. study does not directly involve that set of preconceived ideas. Rather, the case is meant to be an end in and of itself. With this use of the case study we often observe analysts using intuitive and descriptive accounts that attempt to lead to some general “understanding” of the case.6 The use of the term idiographic (as contrasted with nomothetic) quite directly indicates that the case will not be generalizable or that the case study will not seek to establish general rules of behavior. Using Theory to Explore Cases In contrast to the “configurative-idiographic” or “atheoretical” case study, a researcher may choose to use some theory or set of hypotheses to direct his examination of a particular case. This use of the case study is “disciplined- configurative” (Eckstein, 1975) or “interpretive” (Lijphart, 1971). Here, the focus is still on the case, but the analyst explicitly uses some theoretical foundation in order to examine or interpret the case. Although there may be some “feedback” from the case to the theory—such as whether the theory is inappropriate for understanding the case, or whether no theory currently exists that effectively illuminates the case—the focus is still mainly on the case. After such an endeavor, we should know the case in a “theoretically familiar” form. Here, however, the purpose of the analyst may be to provide a mechanism whereby changes can be effected on a particular organization, institution, or actor that was the subject of the “interpretive” case study. In other words, the interpretation of the case may be designed to effect changes along the lines of the given theory. The interest remains on the case (or the actor or actorsof the given case), but the theoreticalinterpretation of the case may provide some prescriptive leverage.7 Using Cases to Develop Theory The third type or use of case studies is what has been termed “hypothesis- generating” (Lijphart, 1971) or “heuristic” (Eckstein, 1975) case studies. Here the analyst specifically investigates a case in an effort to develop testable hypotheses. In this sense, the case study is being used to build theory; the analyst examines a 6 Some might argue that this type of case study should be further divided into “historical” and “interpretive,” with the former being an attempt to present events or circumstances in an effort to reveal the case, and the latter being an attempt to gain an understanding of the case from the perspective of the actors involved. We, however, have not opted to divide this particular use of the case study along these lines, as our intention is to provide a more general set of uses of the case study. Further, following Ragin (1987), we consider that interpretive approaches to case studies are not in competition with other types of approaches. 7 Freudian analysis applied to a patient, for example, interprets the patient’s past behaviors and activities according to a more or less defined theory, often for the purpose of changing the patient according to that interpretation. Decision analysts may evaluate the past behavior of an organization according to theories about group performance specifically to offer modifications in the organization’s composition or behavior according to that theory’s prescriptive implications for the case. 374 Kaarbo and Beasley
  • 7. specific set of concepts in order to develop generalizable theory from particular instances. The selection of cases is based on the nature of the theory that is being constructed, rather than on a particular case that is otherwise of some interest to the researcher. The focus, in other words, shifts from being explicitly on the case to being explicitly on the theory. In these types of case studies, the analyst is typically looking at fewer variables than would be true for the configurative- idiographic case study, which uses a more Gestalt approach to the investigation, and specifically attempts to develop a broad and informed picture of the case. This is very similar to the sort of “introspective examination” that psychologists often engage in as they are constructing a hypothesis regarding individual behavior or cognition. They might, for example, ask themselves “how do I, as an expert, think about things” when they are attempting to develop a theory of expert cognition.8 Using Cases to Explore and Refine Theory Another use or type of case study increasingly focuses the analyst on a specific theory. This has been termed a “plausibility probe” by Eckstein (1975), who suggested that plausibility “means something more than a belief in potential validity plain and simple . . . [and] something less than actual validity, for which rigorous testing is required” (p. 108). Here the analyst is presumably interested in performing some labor- or cost-intensive study, and thus is benefited by examining certain aspects of the theory at stake within the confines of an empirical instance. It is an inductive feedback device within the context of the particular empirical domain to which the analyst’s theory speaks. Further, this use of the case study is particularly important with novel hypotheses or theories, such that an existing body of evidence cannot be referenced in speculating about the plausibility of a suspected relationship. This assists the analyst in determining whether an empirical instance of a particular phenomenon can be reasonably found, whether the operationaliza- tions of key variables are in concert with the data available, and/or whether the posited relationship or relationships between and among variables are consistent with expectations.9 In this latter form, the plausibility probe helps to avoid a costly exploration of an implausible explanation. 8 Using a case study as a “deviant case” (Lijphart, 1971) offers the potential for expanding the nature or scope of an existing theory. Here the analyst specifically chooses a case that deviates from the pattern predicted by theory, and then attempts to examine the case in an effort to uncover additional variables that may be lacking in the original theory. In this sense, the deviant case differs from a heuristic case study in that a theory already exists that is being examined. A deviant case, however, is similar to a heuristic case in that both are attempting to establish new hypotheses or propositions. In other words, the deviant case can elaborate a theory by suggesting new hypotheses or additional conditions under which a certain relationship may or may not hold. In this sense, the deviant case and the heuristic case are similar. 9 Goldmann (1988), in an appendix entitled “A Note on the Utility of Weak Theory and Weak Tests,” suggested that using the case study in a fashion similar to the plausibility probe is of value for “testing” weak theory, in that it can “lead to an improved understanding of the problems and possibilities of operationalization” (p. 227). Comparative Case Study 375
  • 8. This is very similar to many aspects of the pilot study in experimental methods. It is rare that an experiment is constructed in the office, run in the laboratory, and published in a journal in swift order. Experimental psychology is replete with efforts to probe the plausibility of an expected relationship, and the experimental method is an iterative cycle that effectively incorporates numerous steps designed to refine theories and hypotheses before testing them. Using Cases as Tests of Theory Case studies can also be used as single tests of an established theory, either to confirm or infirm (Lijphart, 1971) or to investigate a crucial case (Eckstein, 1975). The distinction between confirming and infirming rests principally in the nature of the findings, that is, whether they support the predictions of an existing theory (confirming) or whether they call that theory into question (infirming). In either event, the process involves specifying in exacting fashion the variables to be measured and the hypotheses to be tested. Further, a case must be selected such that it fits the parameters of the theory that is being investigated. Once a population is specified (in more or less precise form), then a sampling procedure that maxi- mizes random selection will tend to offer the most compelling interpretation to the given results of the study.10 Once performed, the case study will stand as a single “test” of a given theory, and the weight afforded that test in terms of its commentary on that theory will be a function of the way in which the case was chosen, the theory specified, the variables operationalized and measured, and the conclusions drawn. Some cases, it can be argued, offer the potential to make a particularly compelling statement about the theory involved. This has been termed the “crucial case” study (Eckstein, 1975) and purportedly offers a critical test of a theory, in contrast to the “uncritical”or“routine”testofferedbyothercaseexaminationsofagiventheory.Here, the analyst intentionally selects a case where alternative explanations of a negative finding(afindingotherthanthatpredictedbythetheory)arenotplausible.Thisrequires both that the theory can be completely specified in terms of empirical expectations, and that a case exists such that mitigating factors do not exist. As Eckstein himself acknowledged, this is rare within the social sciences, although how crucial a case is can be considered along a continuum that allows more or less boisterous claims on the part of the researcher regarding the support or lack of support afforded by the test. Steps to Comparative Case Study Research Much of the writing in political science on the case study method has focused on justifying the case study or on the different purposes of case studies. There has 10 For a discussion on the important and deceptively difficult question of populations and case studies, see Ragin and Becker (1992). See also Yin (1994) for a discussion of statistical generalization (generalizing to a population) versus analytical generalization (generalizing to a theory). 376 Kaarbo and Beasley
  • 9. been less attention paid to how to do case study research. Even in research methods textbooks, chapters devoted to the case study give the student few guidelines for how to construct a research design for the case study method (e.g., Baker, 1988; Isaak, 1985; Johnson & Joslyn, 1991). Our purpose in this section is to outline for researchers who choose the case study method the important steps for case study research. What issues are likely to arise in the design of case study research? What decisions must the researcher face? What types of evidence are compatible with case study research? These are the questions addressed in the following guidelines. In doing so, we want to integrate the advice from a number of fields and offer a practical guide for conducting case study research. Although most of these steps to case study research would apply to many of the above uses or types of case studies and to single as well as multiple case studies, our treatment focuses on comparative case studies with the purpose of examining hypotheses as critically and systematically as possible. In other words, although there is an important role for case studies in interpreting a single historical case and in serving as a heuristic for theory construction, we discuss the steps researchers will take when they use the case study method to compare two or more cases in order to draw implications about an existing theory or single hypothesis. We focus on this type of case study because it is the furthest afield from the “stereotype” of case studies as idiosyncratic, historically specific, and atheoretical. We do not intend to suggest that the other uses of the case study are not of value, or that many of these steps could not be applied to improving the other types of case studies. Rather, we find it least confusing to present these steps with a particular use of the case study in mind. However, many of the issues we discuss are not necessarily limited to this purpose of the case study. To date, the best guide for comparative case study research is Alexander George’s “method of structured, focused comparison” (George, 1979; George & Bennett, in press; George & McKeown, 1985; George & Smoke, 1974).11 Accord- ing to George (1979), the comparison is “focused because it deals selectively with only certain aspects of the historical case . . . and structured because it employs general questions to guide the data collection analysis in that historical case” (pp. 61–62; emphasis added). George and his colleagues have outlined several tasks for researchers who choose the structured, focused case comparison. These guidelines overlap significantly with the steps we discuss below, although we consolidate some tasks, divide others, and integrate other scholars’ advice for conducting case study research in our discussion. Our aim is both to incorporate previous discussions of case study research from other disciplines (e.g., public administration and sociology) and to offer guidelines for conducting case studies that are accessible and meaningful for political scientists, psychologists, and political psychologists. 11 In his recently published collection of “memos,” Stephen Van Evera (1996) also offered step-by-step advice for case study research. Comparative Case Study 377
  • 10. Of course, these steps are fairly standard in research design. They are the traditional steps researchers follow when designing research using almost any method, yet they are rarely followed for case study research. The form that these steps take in comparative case study research differs somewhat from that for other research methods. For example, how reliability is enhanced in a research design is different for case studies than for experiments, although the same logic underlies reliability issues in both types of research. Thus, what follows is a discussion of how traditional research design issues play out in the comparative case study method. Step 1: Identify Specific Research Question for Focused Comparison The first step in comparative case study research, just as for any other method, is to formulate the specific research question. What phenomena do you want to explain? What is the dependent variable? The type of research questions most appropriate for case studies are “how” and “why” questions—that is, questions focusing on the underlying process, on the causal nexus between the independent variables and the phenomena to be explained. Examples are “How do voters choose candidates?,” “Why do unanimity decision rules produce compromise outcomes in group decision-making?,” and “How do leaders manage information inconsis- tent with their beliefs?” Frequency questions (“How often do different values of the dependent variable occur?”) are not appropriate for comparisons of a small number of cases (Yin, 1994). At this point, the researcher wants to focus the research on a specific phenome- non and be clear what class of events the research will address. Focusing the research question is an essential part of George’s “focused comparison” and is Lijphart’s (1971) advice for dealing with the “too many variables” problem. As Lijphart (1971) argued, researchers comparing case studies “must avoid the danger of being overwhelmed by large numbers of variables and, as a result, losing the possibility of discovering controlled relationships” (p. 690). Developing a focused research question is the best way to identify the most important variables. The identification of the class of phenomena is also a crucial part of this first step and will help the researcher both to identify cases for selection and to answer the question “What is this a case of?” For example, suppose you are interested in the relationship between leaders’ personalities and decision-making outcomes. It is important at the outset to focus on what leaders you are interested in. In other words, does the research question apply to all leaders, or to leaders of particular countries or types of political systems? Focusing on particular types of leaders would make the identification of explanatory variables and case selection much more manageable. 378 Kaarbo and Beasley
  • 11. Step 2: Identify Variables From Existing Theory The next question the researcher asks is “What independent or explanatory variables or conditions have been found or hypothesized to explain the dependent variable?” This is usually accomplished by an extensive literature review. The researcher should then specify which aspects of existing theory will be singled out for assessment, again focusing the research question. A large list of potential explanatory variables investigated in a small number of cases yields an indetermi- nate research design, and thus the case study researcher must choose a focused set of candidate explanatory variables to investigate (King et al., 1994). One advantage of case study research, however, is the ability to track other variables in the cases that may not be the primary focused explanatory variable(s), but may be important later in the interpretation of the relationships discovered. This is not uncommon in experimental studies, wherein measures are frequently taken for a host of variables whether or not they are of central concern to the study. The researcher should also make explicit which of the variables to be investi- gated are hypothesized to be most important for explaining the phenomenon. In other words, does the theory suggest any ordering of the possible independent variables? Are any conditions hypothesized to be necessary or sufficient? Hypothe- sized intervening relationships should also be specified at this point. Although many research methods are aided by this a priori inventory of relationships among variables, the case study comparison requires it because this will drive the next step of case selection. Step 3: Case Selection The most important (and difficult) stage in comparative case study research is the selection of cases. Cases can, of course, be drawn randomly from a population to eliminate the danger of selection bias. Yet drawing a random sample is often not possible because the universe of cases is not known or is not accessible.12 At the heart of systematic investigations is the idea of control. To establish the relationship between two or more variables, it is necessary to minimize variability in other variables that may affect the investigated relationship. In the experimental method, this is approximated by dividing subjects into experimental and control groups through some form of random assignment. With the use of certain statistical methods, the same type of control can be achieved through the use of partial correlations. Although here no “assignment” of subjects to conditions occurs, the influence of certain variables on the relationship being examined can be controlled for statistically. Control for the comparative case study method is achieved through 12 As King et al. (1994) indicated, random selection does not always solve the problem of selection bias. See Collier and Mahoney (1996) for an excellent and detailed discussion of the issues surrounding selection bias in qualitative research. Comparative Case Study 379
  • 12. case selection. There are three important tasks in selecting cases that involve control: selecting comparable cases, selecting cases that vary on the dependent variable, and selecting cases across subgroups of the population to address alter- native explanations. 3a. Comparable cases. Comparability is the heart of any scientific investiga- tion and thus is not unique to case study research (see Ragin, 1987, p. 1). Compa- rability is the basis of Przeworski and Teune’s (1970) “most similar systems design” in which the researcher chooses cases as similar as possible to minimize the number of explanatory variables.13 Without comparability, of course, the researcher will not know if the variation seen in the cases is due to the explanatory variable under consideration or to the other differences between the cases (Lijphart, 1971). For example, suppose you are interested in how juries aggregate individual preferences into a group decision and you hypothe- size that the size of the jury is the best explanation for this process. If your cases include juries from different cultures, different time periods, and different legal systems, you do not know whether the differences in the group decision are due to the explanatory variable (jury size) or to the other differences between the cases (culture, time period, and legal system). Thus, if you have reason to believe culture, time period, and legal system affect how juries make decisions, you would want to choose cases that are comparable on these dimensions in order to focus on the effect of size. Comparability depends on the theoretical basis of the study. Cases do not have to be comparable on dimensions that do not (or are assumed not to) impinge on the relationship under investigation. Just as experimentalists are generally not con- cerned about (or at least tolerate) differing weather conditions on the multiple days that an experiment takes place (a problem not corrected by random assignment of subjects to groups), comparative case study researchers should not be concerned about irrelevant differences in their cases. Building on the previous example, if your theory or previous evidence suggests that jury size affects the group aggrega- tion process regardless of culture, time period, or political system, these variables do not have to be considered to make cases comparable. This stands in contrast to Diesing’s (1971, pp. 187–188) advice to pick cases that are geographically similar and that have similar histories if the theory does not suggest relevant dimensions for comparability. The theory should be more capable of distinguishing impor- tant factors. Cases should not be chosen for comparability on non-theoretically derived properties.14 Instead, researchers should choose cases in an effort to 13 Although Przeworski and Teune’s “most similar systems design” focuses on choosing comparable systems, the same principle applies to any level of analysis. 14 A prerequisite for choosing comparable cases is to define what a “case” or the unit of analysis is for the investigation. Thus, choosing comparative cases directly follows from the first step of focusing the research question and identifying the class of phenomena that the question addresses. Is a “case” a case of jury decision-making (for which any case of a jury decision would yield a comparable case), or is a “case” a case of jury decision-making in Western cultures, in the late 20th century, in 380 Kaarbo and Beasley
  • 13. control for known or suspected alternative causes of the relationship under investigation. 3b. Cases with variation in the values of the dependent variable. The most basic rule in case selection is that selection should allow for the possibility of variation in the values of the dependent variable (King et al., 1994). Without variation in values of the dependent variable, the researcher cannot make any causal inference about the phenomenon because the same explanatory variables may be present in cases in which the phenomenon is absent. This problem led John Stuart Mill (1843/1974) to caution scholars against this “method of agreement,” and Mill’s caution has been echoed by many later writers on the case study (e.g., Achen & Snidal, 1989; Collier, 1993; Geddes, 1990). Despite such advice, case study researchers continue to examine cases of deterrence failure (and not cases of deterrence success), cases of revolution (and not cases in which a revolution did not occur), and cases of crisis decision-making (and not cases of routine decision- making). Such studies can eliminate a hypothesized explanation if it is not present in the cases (Collier, 1995; Collier & Mahoney, 1996), but cannot conclude that a factor present in the cases is related to the dependent variable. To ensure the possibility of variation in the values of the dependent variable, case study researchers have two choices: selecting cases based on variation in the values of an explanatory variable, and selecting cases based on variation in the values of the dependent variable (the latter being Mill’s “method of difference”). The first option, selecting cases based on categories of an explanatory variable, causes no inference problems and is therefore considered to be the best solution (King et al., 1994). With this strategy, for example, the researcher interested in the relationship between stress and the quality of decision-making would choose cases in which stress was present (or high) and cases in which stress was absent (or low) and then proceed to investigate how these independent variables related to the quality of decision-making. One of the drawbacks to this strategy (and hence one argument for choosing the second strategy of selecting cases based on variation in the values of the dependent variable) is that often it is the value of the dependent variable (i.e., that a revolution occurred) that is already known to the researcher. If the researcher already knows the values of the dependent variable, then this is the same as selecting the cases on both the independent and dependent variables. This might cause bias in the case selection if the cases are chosen in such a way that the independent variable matches up with the known value of the dependent variable in the hypothesized direction (King et al., 1994). Furthermore, re- searchers are often inherently interested in explaining the dependent variable common-law systems (for which cases must be comparable on these dimensions)? The definition of the unit of analysis must come from the researcher’s theoretical focus or from the population to which the hypothesis applies. Comparative Case Study 381
  • 14. (e.g., poor decision-making or revolutions); that is what motivates them to do the study. Choosing cases based on variation in the values of the dependent variable hastheadvantageofallowingtheresearchertosaysomethingaboutthedependent variable under investigation upon its completion. If, using the previous example, casesinwhichstress(theexplanatoryvariable)waspresentandabsentarechosen and stress turns out to be unrelated to decision-making quality (the dependent variable), then this is all the research can conclude. If, on the other hand, cases of good and poordecision-making(the dependentvariable)are chosen,then,even if stressturnsouttobeunrelated,theresearchercaninvestigateothervariablesinthe casesthatmightberelatedtodecision-makingquality.15 3c. Choosing cases across population subgroups. In addition to choosing comparable cases that vary on the values of the dependent variable, case study researchers should consider choosing cases with an alternative explanation in mind. In other words, the case selection design may include variation across some dimension associated with an alternative explanation of the relationship being investigated. What the researcher attempts to do in this step is to demonstrate that the relationship holds across different subgroups of a population. This gives the researcher, upon the conclusion of the study, the opportunity to challenge an alternative explanation.16 For example, if you are interested in how personality characteristics affect the maintenance of enemy images, in addition to choosing cases in which an enemy image was maintained and cases in which an enemy image changed, you might consciously choose cases before and after the end of the Cold War. This case selection would ensure that you could say something about the relationship under investigation, regardless of the time period. If cases only from the Cold War era or only from the post–Cold War period were examined, you would be open to the criticism that the study’s results only apply to that particular era. An alternative theory might suggest that the nature of enemy images is completely different in these two historical periods of the international system—that enemy images, for example, are much more malleable in the post–Cold War world regardless of personality characteristics. Of course, choosing cases across different subgroups of a population proliferates the number of cases needed.17 The difference between choosing comparable cases (step 3a) and choosing cases across population subgroups (step 3c) concerns the expectations about the 15 A final point in favor of choosing cases based on variation in values of the dependent variable is that, in many cases, researchers hypothesize multiple (often competing) explanatory variables. Thus, choosing casesbased on the (single)dependentvariable toensurevariationwouldbea simpler formula for case selection. See Collier and Mahoney (1996) and Van Evera (1996) for additional issues concerning case selection and variation in the dependent variable. 16 Choosing cases across subgroups associated with an alternative explanation to show the maximum leverage of the hypothesis is consistent with Przeworski and Teune’s (1970) “most different systems design.” 17 See King et al. (1994), chapter 5, for more specific advice on this issue. 382 Kaarbo and Beasley
  • 15. applicability of the hypothesized relationship. In choosing comparable cases, the researcher is recognizing the limits of the hypothesis, arguing that it only applies to cases that are comparable on a certain number of theoretically derived dimensions. In choosing cases across subgroups, the researcher is purposefully stretching the investigation of the hypothesis across a variable derived from an alternative explanation. Step 4: Operationalize Variables and Construct a Case Codebook The fourth major task in designing a comparative case study, and one that is often overlooked, is to operationalize the variables under investigation. The re- searcher is essentially constructing a case “codebook” to guide the collection of evidence for the variables in the study. Indeed, King et al. (1994) argued that such recording and reporting the way the data were collected is even more important in case studies than is case selection. George (1979) conceived of the variables in the form of structured questions that the researcher asks in each case: Using a standardized set of questions in the controlled comparison is necessary to assure acquisition of comparable data from the several cases. In this way, the method of structured, focused comparison will avoid the all-too-familiar and disappointing experience of traditional, intensive single case studies in the past which, even when they were instances of a single class of events, were not performed in a comparable way and hence did not contribute to an orderly, cumulative development of knowledge and theory about the phenomenon in question. Instead, as conducted in the past, each case study tended to go its own way, reflecting the special interests of each investigator and often, somewhat opportunistically, being guided by the readily available historical data rather than by a well defined theoretical focus. As a result, the idiosyncratic features of each case tended to shape the research questions differently. (p. 62) George’s structured questions—the “structured” element of his “structured, fo- cused comparison”—are similar to what Yin (1994, p. 70) called a “case study protocol,” a list of case study questions and identification of potential sources of evidence for answers. Although we enthusiastically endorse George’s advice to investigate the same variables across the set of cases in the form of structured questions, we argue that the case study researcher needs to go beyond structuring questions and to structure answers as well. In other words, the same set of general categories of answers, or values of each variable, should guide the researcher across the cases. The key is to specify in advance what is necessary for the case analyst to see in the evidence to code a variable in a particular way, to make a judgment of its value. This allows for enhanced intracoder reliability (the case study analyst uses the same set of Comparative Case Study 383
  • 16. criteria to assign variables particular values) and provides a basis for intercoder reliability (the same set of criteria to assign variables particular values can be used by different analysts). In many case studies, of course, variables will be qualitatively judged on the basis of subjective interpretations of transcripts, diaries, etc., but even in this way, a priori categories of the possible variable values can increase systematicity and reliability. Just as in the construction of answers to surveys, the analyst should treat the operationalizations of the possible values of the variables seriously. Operationali- zations should be general—applicable to the class of phenomena, not just to the specific case under investigation—so that the case codebook could be applied to cases not in the study. Just as for any codebook, categories, to the extent possible, should be exhaustive for all possible answers in all cases. Of course, categories should be mutually exclusive. If some variables have multiple indicators in their operational definitions, the researcher should specify a meta-decision rule for deciding the value of the variable. For example, if there are several indicators for coding a leader as a crusader personality and there are several indicators for coding a leader as a pragmatist personality, the codebook should specify how to deal with mixed evidence when indicators of both personalitytypesare found inthe evidence. The exact nature of the operationalizations of the variables will, of course, depend on the data sources to be used in the investigation. One advantage of a comparative case study research design is that a variety of types of data and techniques for extracting data can be used (Yin, 1994). Content analysis, survey data, archive data, descriptive statistical analysis, focus groups, and interviews are all compatible, not competitive, with case study research. The use of multiple sources is one way to enhance the construct validity of operational definitions (Yin, 1994, p. 34).18 In addition to specifying in advance the operationalizations of the variables, case study researchers can enhance the reliability and validity of case studies in other ways. Case study researchers can use judgments made for entirely different purposes by other researchers to avoid having measurements be influenced by the hypotheses (King et al., 1994). King et al. also advised using qualified experts to code the variables who do not know the theory being tested.Even non-expertcoders who are blind to the hypotheses could demonstrate the intercoder reliability of the variable measurements, although this may be difficult with certain types of evidence. Case study researchers may also consider conducting a pilot study: “The pilot case is used more formatively, assisting an investigator to develop relevant lines of questions—possibly even providing some conceptual clarification for the re- search design as well” (Yin, 1994, p. 74). Pilot studies may be necessary when 18 Yin (1994) also argued that the use of multiple sources of evidence allows the researcher to develop “converging lines of inquiry, [or] a process of triangulation” of data sources that makes conclusions from case studies much more convincing (p. 92; emphasis in original). 384 Kaarbo and Beasley
  • 17. operationalizations do not exist from previous studies or are not specified com- pletely by the theory.19 After looking at a similar case with similar evidence that will not be included in the main study, the case analyst is often better able to fine-tune the operationalizations. The tasks involved in the construction of a codebook—constructing structured answers and structured questions, identifying multiple sources of evidence, testing for intercoder reliability, investigating pilot cases—may seem to be tedious work for a small number of cases. Yet this step addresses the most often heard criticism of the case study: unreliability. Following rules for systematic extraction of data, and reporting these rules to readers, enhances the credibility of conclusions from case studies. Unfortunately, this step is rarely taken in case study research (George, 1979).20 Step 5: Code-Write Cases Once the codebook is constructed and the evidence is accumulated, the variable should be coded and the results presented. These are actually two separate tasks. Indeed, Yin (1994) suggested creating a case study database, different and separate from the report, arguing that “too often, the case study data are synony- mous with the evidence presented in the case study report, and a critical reader has no recourse if he or she wants to inspect the database that led to the case study conclusions” (p. 95). In terms of presentation, the narrative remains the most used and preferred form (Yin, 1994). The narrative case study tells a story based on the variables and coded values and allows the researcher to show the variables in their context.21 This is a way to demonstrate the realism and temporal progression that is an advantage of the case study method.22 In the narrative, any uncertainty on the part of the analyst should, of course, also be presented (King et al., 1994). The length and detail of the narratives depends on the research question, the number of variables, and the number of cases. Although the narrative form of presentation demonstrates the richness of the data, it is not the only form available to the case study researcher and indeed can become impossible when a large number of case studies are involved. Alterna- tively, the case analyst can present a question-and-answer format or present only 19 This is one of the uses of a case study to explore and refine theory discussed earlier. 20 Analysts are, of course, still free to observe variables not included in their codebook, and even to question the particular operationalizations that they have chosen. A codebook should not hinder the valuable insights afforded analysts as they examine phenomena in their naturally occurring context. These, however, should be noted as such when researchers report their findings. 21 For a discussion of different purposes of case narratives, see Abbott (1992, pp. 62–80). 22 The narrative form also has the advantage of conforming to the way people think about events. As Pennington and Hastie’s (1986) research demonstrates, people think in terms of stories, not in terms of comparing the values of variables in isolation. Comparative Case Study 385
  • 18. coded results in table form with short case descriptions in an appendix (Yin, 1994). Combinations of narratives and short descriptions might be used, depending on the results. For example, the researcher might want to demonstrate in more detail the cases that fit or do not fit the hypotheses the best. In whichever form the case analyst chooses to present the cases, the presentation of results should include more explicit discussion of method than is usually found in case studies (George & McKeown, 1985). Step 6: Comparison and Implications for Theory After the cases have been coded for the variables, the case study analyst looks for patterns within and across cases. In the congruence procedure, the analyst determines whether any independent variables differentiate across the different outcomes of the dependent variable (George & McKeown, 1985). In other words, the analyst asks: Does the value of x relate to the value of y in the predicted way? The analysis stage can follow another form of logic in which a pattern predicted by the theory is matched against the pattern seen in the cases. In other words, a number of different explanatory variables must be present and related to each other in the right form for the hypothesis to be accepted. This technique of “pattern matching” is a key advantage of the case study method (Campbell, 1975; Collier, 1993). In pattern matching, a single predicted pattern can be compared to the pattern observed in the cases or, alternatively, mutually exclusive rival patterns can be compared for their consistency with the pattern observed in the cases (Yin, 1994, p. 108). This essentially increases the number of observations made within each case study. In recanting his earlier “strong rejection” of the case study method, the well-known psychologist Donald Campbell (1975) applied the statistical con- cept of degrees of freedom to case studies. According to Campbell, theories usually have a number of predictions that the social scientist uses to “pattern match” against a single case. In other words, degrees of freedom in a case study means “testing” multiple implications of the theory. When the pattern that is investigated is temporal in character, the case analyst engages in what George and McKeown (1985) termed “process tracing” or inves- tigating a “decision process by which various initial conditions are translated into outcomes” (p. 35).23 As Yin (1994) argued, “the ability to trace changes over time is a major strength of case studies—which are not limited to cross-sectional or static assessments of a particular situation” (p. 113). However, the data requirements for process tracing are considerably greater than for the simple congruence procedure and thus may not be practical for some case study research. 23 For George and McKeown (1985), process tracing means more than the analysis of temporal patterns within cases; it also involves interpretivism in that it is “an attempt to reconstruct actors’ definitions of the situation” (p. 35). 386 Kaarbo and Beasley
  • 19. Examining the multiple implications of the theory or engaging in process tracing within a single case study also results in certain limitations that should be made clear by the case analyst. Much like choosing comparable cases by minimiz- ing variation on alternative explanatory variables, examining patterns and pro- cesses through time within a single case study does not allow for any between-cases variation. Although a particular observed pattern may or may not conform to theoretical predictions, it is difficult within a single case study to determine whether a particular set of observations is idiosyncratic to that case or not. Further, a particular pattern of observations within a single case study could be due to the impact of a causal factor that cumulates through time but which is thought to be independent of the causal process revealed in the case. For example, a particular pattern of activity of presidential initiatives to secure approval of legislation may reflect a predicted pattern of intensification through time due to a particular personality trait, but for a given president regarding a particular initiative it may also reflect a gradual accumulation of initial conditions such as public opinion, economic prosperity, or other non–personality-based factors. Of course, investi- gating more than one case study at a time helps to address some of these concerns, as patterns and processes noted within one case study can then be systematically compared to the other case studies that have been comparably performed. Once the initial assessment of the theoretical implications is made, the case study analyst may want to “return” to the cases to investigate candidate explana- tions for the relationships discovered. In other words, certain interpretations arising from the initial conclusions can often be discounted or suggested by analyzing the “extra” material included in the case studies. Although the researcher may need to design a new research plan and select new cases to better “test” these implications, case study research can offer the researcher more material on which to build speculative interpretations of results. This is a heuristic value built into every case study research design. Conclusions: Why Should Political Psychologists Care About the Case Study Method? It is our hope that this examination of the case study method will help to guide researchers in their use of this methodology and will begin to dispel several stereotypical conceptions of the case study method. As we suggested at the outset, the case study has been plagued by various misconceptions, attributable more to the way it has been used than to its actual potential. We have suggested that, although valuable as a tool for understanding idiosyncratic historical events, the method is also valuable as a tool for generating and exploring theories and testing hypotheses. Further, we have offered various techniques for dealing with the issue of the ratio of observations to hypotheses. We have also offered, by way of a review of general design procedures, a set of steps for gaining some measure of control in the comparative case study method. Although by definition our conception of the Comparative Case Study 387
  • 20. case study method disavows direct manipulation of either context or phenomenon, case selection procedures do allow a degree of discrimination in determining relationships between and among variables that is similar to that afforded by experimental manipulation or statistical control. Finally, the resounding critique of subjectivity on the part of the researcher has been addressed by suggesting procedures that can significantly improve the systematicity and reliability of the case study method. Beyond the general value of case study research, the case study method is particularly suitable for political psychology. We base this proposition on Margaret Hermann’s (1986) identification of the tenets of the field of political psychology. In her answer to the question “What is political psychology?” Hermann proposed five common themes that characterize research in this area: 1. Focus Is on the Interaction of Political and Psychological Phenomena 2. Research Is Responsive and Relevant to Societal Problems 3. Context Can Make a Difference 4. Emphasis Is on Process as Well as Outcome 5. There Is a Tolerance of Multiple Methods for Gathering Data Comparative case study research, we believe, is particularly relevant to the last three of Hermann’s tenets.24 First, case study research is relevant to the tenet that context can make a difference. Hermann (1986) wrote that “within political psychology there is a healthy dialogue and debate” on the importance of contextual factors such as time, culture, and political structures, but that “there is a growing recognition that such contextual factors help not only to shape what an individual is like, but also to limit what an individual can do politically” (p. 3). Case study research internalizes this tenet, as “context” is part of the definition of the case study offered earlier in this paper. Case study research means that a real-world phenomenon is investigated within its naturally occurring context, with the recognition that context can make a difference. Second, the case study method is consistent with the tenet that the process is as important as the outcome. Hermann (1986) argued that psychological and political phenomena often seem to have their most direct impact on one another in determining political processesas opposed 24 This is not to suggest that the case study method is not relevant to the first two tenets. Case study research is certainly appropriate for the study of the interaction of political and psychological phenomena and for research that is responsive and relevant to societal problems. Indeed, our discussion in this paper of case studies whose goal is intervention and the desirability of choosing cases based on variation in the values of an interesting dependent variable reflects this second tenet. Our point in this section, however, is that there is something about the case study method in particular that is consistent with the last three of Hermann’s tenets. 388 Kaarbo and Beasley
  • 21. to political outcomes. Once delineated in one political setting, descrip- tions of processes offer the possibility of generalization to other political contexts with somewhat similar characteristics. By ascertaining what some of these cross-cutting processes are, political psychologists can begin to build more general explanations and theories. (p. 3) Case study research is often directed at identifying the underlying process. As discussed above, the most appropriate research questions for case study research are “how” and “why” questions. Furthermore, pattern matching (especially tem- poral pattern matching or process tracing), as a technique for analysis in case study research, emphasizes the process by which initial conditions are translated into outcomes. In this sense, process is central to most case study research. Finally, case study research is compatible with political psychology’s toler- ance of multiple methods for gathering data. As Hermann (1986) wrote, “political psychology appears not to be wedded to one type of methodology. Its practitioners employ a variety of methods” (p. 3). Not only can the case study method serve to complement other research methods, it can also be multimethodological in itself, as a number of techniques (e.g., content analysis, archival analysis, interviews) can be incorporated into a case study research design.25 Clearly, then, the case study method can be a valuable research tool and is particularly suited to the needs and interests of those studying within the field of political psychology. In this light, it is unfortunate that some of the misconceptions about case studies have contributed to the methodological barrier between political science and psychology. It has not been our argument here that the case study should supplant other methods, nor have we tried to systematically articulate each advantage and disadvantage associated with case study research relative to other methods. Rather, we hope to have provided a practical guide to doing case study research and in the process helped to dispel certain misconceptions and thereby further the development of a common language for political psychology. ACKNOWLEDGMENTS Earlier versions of this paper were presented as part of the methods workshop series at the Summer Institute of Political Psychology at The Ohio State University, 1992–1997. We would like to thank the participants in those workshops, as their questions and comments helped to refine our thinking about this topic over the years. We also thank Alexander George as well as the editors and anonymous reviewers for suggestions on earlier drafts. Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to Juliet Kaarbo, University of Kansas, Department of Political Science, 504 Blake, Lawrence, KS 66045. E-mail: kaarbo@falcon.cc.ukans.edu 25 See Tetlock (1983) on the advantages of multimethod convergence in political psychology. Comparative Case Study 389
  • 22. REFERENCES Abbott, A. (1992). What do cases do? Some notes on activity in sociological analysis. In C. C. Ragin & H. S. Becker (Eds.), What is a case?: Exploring the foundations of social inquiry (pp. 53–82). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. Achen, C.,&Snidal, D. (1989). Rational deterrence theoryand comparativecase studies.World Politics, 41, 143–169. Baker, T. L. (1988). Doing social research. New York: McGraw-Hill. Campbell, D. T. (1975). Degrees of freedom and the case study. Comparative Political Studies, 8, 178–193. Collier, D. (1993). The comparative method. In A. W. Finifter (Ed.), Political science: The state of the discipline II (pp. 105–119). Washington, DC: American Political Science Association. Collier, D. (1995). Translating quantitative methods for qualitative researchers: The case of selection bias. American Political Science Review, 89, 461–466. Collier, D., & Mahoney, J. (1996). Insights and pitfalls: Selection bias in qualitative research. World Politics, 49, 56–91. Diesing, P. (1971). Patterns of discovery in the social sciences. New York: Aldine. Eckstein, H. (1975). Case study and theory in political science. In F. I. Greenstein & N. W. Polsby (Eds.), Handbook of political science (pp. 79–138). Reading, MA: Addison-Wesley. Farnham, B. (1992). Roosevelt and the Munich crisis: Insights from prospect theory. Political Psychology, 13, 205–235. Fife-Schaw, C., & Breakwell, G. M. (1990). Predicting the intention not to vote in late teenage: A U.K. study of 17- and 18-year-olds. Political Psychology, 11, 739–755. Fishel, J. (1992). Leadership for social change: John Vasconcellos (D-CA) and the promise of humanistic psychology in public life. Political Psychology, 13, 663–692. Geddes, B. (1990). How the cases you choose affect the answers you get: Selection bias in comparative politics. In J. A. Stimson (Ed.), Political analysis (vol. 2, pp. 131–150). Ann Arbor, MI: University of Michigan Press. George, A. L. (1979). Case studies and theory development: The method of structured, focused comparison. In P. G. Lauren (Ed.), Diplomacy: New approaches in history, theory, and policy (pp. 43–68). New York: Free Press. George, A. L., & Bennett, A. (in press). Case studies and theory development. BCSJA Security Studies Series. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. George, A. L., & McKeown, T. J. (1985). Case studies and theories of organizational decision making. In R. F. Coulam & R. A. Smith (Eds.), Advances in information processing in organizations (vol. 2, pp. 21–58). Greenwich, CT: JAI Press. George, A. L., & Smoke, R. (1974). Deterrence in American foreign policy. New York: Columbia University Press. Gilbert, R. E. (1995). The political effects of presidential illness: The case of Lyndon B. Johnson. Political Psychology, 16, 761–776. Goldmann, K. (1988). Stability and change in foreign policy. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press. Groth, A. J., & Britton, S. (1993). Gorbachev and Lenin: The psychological walls of the Soviet “garrison state.” Political Psychology, 14, 627–650. Hermann, M. G. (1986). What is political psychology? In M. G. Hermann (Ed.), Political psychology (pp. 1–10). San Francisco: Jossey-Bass. Hermann, M. G. (1989, August). Political psychology: Fad, fantasy, or field. Paper presented at the annual meeting of the American Psychological Association, New Orleans. Isaak, A. C. (1985). Scope and methods of political science. Chicago: Dorsey. 390 Kaarbo and Beasley
  • 23. Johnson, J. B., & Joslyn, R. A. (1991). Political science research methods. Washington, DC: Congressional Quarterly Press. King, G., Keohane, R. O., & Verba, S. (1994). Designing social inquiry: Scientific inference in qualitative research. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press. Lijphart, A. (1971). Comparative politics and the comparative method. American Political Science Review, 65, 682–693. Lijphart, A. (1975). The comparable-case strategy in comparative research. Comparative Political Studies, 8, 158–177. Milburn, M. A., & McGrail, A. B. (1992). The dramatic presentation of news and its effects on cognitive complexity. Political Psychology, 13, 613–632. Mill, J. S. (1974). A system of logic. Toronto: University of Toronto Press. (Original work published 1843) Orum, A. M., Feagin, J. R., & Sjoberg, G. (1991). Introduction: The nature of the case study. In J. R. Feagin, A. M. Orum, & G. Sjoberg (Eds.), A case for the case study (pp. 1–26). Chapel Hill, NC: University of North Carolina Press. Pennington, N., & Hastie, R. (1986). Evidence evaluation in complex decision making. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 51, 242–258. Przeworski, A., & Teune, H. (1970). The logic of comparative social inquiry. New York: Wiley. Ragin, C. C. (1987). The comparative method: Moving beyond qualitative and quantitative strategies. Berkeley, CA: University of California Press. Ragin, C. C., & Becker, H. S. (Eds.) (1992). What is a case?: Exploring the foundationsof social inquiry. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. Raven, B. H. (1990). Political applications of the psychology of interpersonal influence and social power. Political Psychology, 11, 493–520. Steinberg, B. S. (1991). Shame and humiliation in the Cuban missile crisis: A psychoanalytic perspective. Political Psychology, 12, 653–690. Suedfeld, P. (1992). Bilateral relations between countries and the complexity of newspaper editorials. Political Psychology, 13, 601–612. Tetlock, P. E. (1983). Psychological research on foreign policy: A methodological review. Review of Personality and Social Psychology, 4, 45–78. Tiedens, L. Z. (1997). Optimism and revolt of the oppressed: A comparison of two Polish Jewish ghettos of World War II. Political Psychology, 18, 45–69. Van Evera, S. (1996). Guide to methodology for students of political science. Cambridge, MA: Defense and Arms Control Studies Program, Massachusetts Institute of Technology. Yin, R. (1994). Case study research. Beverly Hills, CA: Sage. Comparative Case Study 391