SlideShare a Scribd company logo
EVALUATING THE SEPARATE AND COMBINED EFFECTS OF
POSITIVE AND NEGATIVE REINFORCEMENT ON TASK COMPLIANCE
KELLY J. BOUXSEIN, HENRY S. ROANE, AND TARA HARPER
MUNROE-MEYER INSTITUTE
UNIVERSITY OF NEBRASKA MEDICAL CENTER
Positive and negative reinforcement are effective for treating escape-maintained destructive
behavior. The current study evaluated the separate and combined effects of these contingencies
to increase task compliance. Results showed that a combination of positive and negative
reinforcement was most effective for increasing compliance.
Key words: compliance, negative reinforcement, positive reinforcement
_______________________________________________________________________________
Teachers consider compliance to be a critical
skill for success in a classroom (Walker, 1986).
Lack of consistent compliance (i.e., noncom-
pliance) can lead to significant deficits in skill
development (e.g., self-help skills), negatively
affect family life and parental stress levels
(Wierson & Forehand, 1994), and lead to
more severe behavior problems (e.g., delinquen-
cy, aggression; Merchant, Young, & West,
2004). Given the potential adverse impact of
noncompliance, interventions for increasing
compliance are warranted.
Positive reinforcement alone (DeLeon, Nei-
dert, Anders, & Rodriguez-Catter, 2001; Lalli
et al., 1999) or in combination with negative
reinforcement (Kodak, Lerman, Volkert, &
Trosclair, 2007; Piazza et al., 1997) has been
demonstrated to be effective for increasing
compliance while resulting in reduced levels of
destructive behavior. However, little research
has evaluated the effects of negative and positive
reinforcement on compliance independent of
treating destructive behavior. In addition, the
existing research has not evaluated the relative
effects of positive and negative reinforcement
alone versus in combination. Thus, the current
study evaluated the separate and combined
effects of positive and negative reinforcement
on task compliance.
METHOD
Participant and Setting
Nate, a 14-year-old boy who had been
diagnosed with Down syndrome, displayed
noncompliance with caregiver demands (e.g.,
self-help skills, academic tasks, daily chores). All
sessions were conducted in a room (4 m by 4 m)
that contained a CD player (during those
conditions in which the positive reinforcement
contingency was in place) and instructional
materials. The instructional materials consisted
of five to 10 separate pieces of paper that were
each formed into a ball (hereafter referred to as
trash) and a trash can.
Response Measurement and
Interobserver Agreement
Throwing away trash was selected as the
target task for this evaluation based on a review
of Nate’s existing self-help goals and because the
response could be physically guided. Compli-
ance was defined as completion of the task
within 5 s of a vocal or modeled prompt and
prior to a physical prompt. Task presentation
Address correspondence to Henry S. Roane, who is now
at the Departments of Pediatrics and Psychiatry, SUNY
Upstate Medical University, 750 E. Adams St. TU#3,
Syracuse, New York 13210 (e-mail: roaneh@upstate.edu).
doi: 10.1901/jaba.2011.44-175
This project was supported by Grant R03MH083193
from the National Institute of Mental Health. The content
is solely the responsibility of the authors and does not
necessarily represent the official views of the National
Institute of Mental Health or the National Institutes of
Health. We thank Ty Starks for his assistance with data
collection and Heather Kadey for her comments on an
earlier version of this manuscript.
JOURNAL OF APPLIED BEHAVIOR ANALYSIS 2011, 44, 175–179 NUMBER 1 (SPRING 2011)
175
was scored at the initiation of a prompting
hierarchy, not the initiation of each separate
prompt. Observers collected data on the
frequency of task compliance and task presen-
tation using a computer-based data-collection
program. Percentage of compliance was calcu-
lated by dividing the frequency of task
compliance by the frequency of tasks presenta-
tions and multiplying by 100%.
Interobserver agreement data were collected
for 34% of sessions. To calculate interobserver
agreement, a computer program partitioned
each session into successive 10-s intervals. Exact
agreement coefficients were calculated by divid-
ing the number of agreements by the number of
agreements plus disagreements and multiplying
by 100%. An agreement was defined as both
observers scoring the same frequency of a
response during the same 10-s interval. Agree-
ment averaged 95% (range, 68% to 100%) for
task compliance and 94% (range, 67% to
100%) for task presentations.
Procedure
Throughout all conditions, trash was
dumped on the ground next to the trash can
and the can was placed within arm’s reach of the
participant each time the trash was emptied.
When Nate placed all of the trash into the can,
the therapist subsequently dumped out the can
such that the target response was continuously
available throughout the session. Tasks were
presented using a graduated prompting hierar-
chy that consisted of sequential vocal, modeled,
and physical prompts in which the vocal
prompt was presented immediately following
the preceding task (or at the beginning of the
session; i.e., a 0-s delay to vocal prompts).
Following the vocal prompt, the subsequent
prompts were delivered every 5 s unless Nate
complied with the task or was in the process of
completing the task. The consequences ar-
ranged for compliance varied across conditions,
and Nate was provided with rules prior to each
session regarding the consequence in place for
compliance. All sessions were 10 min in length,
with the exception of the combined (break plus
music) sessions conducted in the second
analysis, in which session length was increased
due to yoking the number of tasks presented
during sessions (described below). Session
length in the yoked combined condition ranged
from 8 min to about 33 min.
During baseline of the initial analysis, the
experimenter provided brief praise for compli-
ance (e.g., ‘‘nice job, Nate’’) and presented tasks
continuously using the prompting hierarchy
described above (i.e., there were no breaks for
compliance). Following baseline, we compared
a break condition, a music condition, and a
combined contingency (break plus music).
During the break condition, compliance result-
ed in brief praise and 60-s break from task
presentations (i.e., task materials were removed
for 60 s). During the music condition, com-
pliance resulted in brief praise and 60-s access to
music (identified as preferred in a preference
assessment; Roane, Vollmer, Ringdahl, &
Marcus, 1998) while task presentation contin-
ued using the prompting procedure. In the
combined condition, compliance resulted in
brief praise and a break from task presentations
with simultaneous access to music for 60 s. The
initial analysis was conducted in a reversal
(ABCBCDCDC) design.
After the initial comparison of the various
contingencies, we conducted a second analysis
to evaluate whether the number of task
presentations altered the effectiveness of the
different reinforcement contingencies. That is,
it was possible that the observed increase in
compliance during the combined condition
relative to the music condition was due to the
overall lower amount of task presentations in
the former condition. During the second
analysis, the baseline, break, and music condi-
tions were conducted in a manner identical to
that described in the initial analysis. The yoked
combined condition included the same contin-
gencies as described above (i.e., brief praise, 60-
s break from task presentations with access to
176 KELLY J. BOUXSEIN et al.
music); however, the number of tasks presented
during this condition was yoked to match the
number of tasks presented in the first two
phases of the music condition of the second
analysis. Specifically, the total number of tasks
presented in each session of the initial music
phase (e.g., 61 and 54; M 5 57 tasks presented
per music session) was recorded, and each total
was randomly assigned to a session in the yoked
combined condition (some totals were used
across multiple sessions due the different
number of sessions conducted in the music
and combined conditions; M 5 56.1 tasks
presented per yoked combined session). The
baseline, break, music, and yoked combined
conditions were compared in a reversal
(ABCBCDCD) design.
RESULTS AND DISCUSSION
The results of the initial analysis are shown in
Figure 1 (top). During baseline, compliance
was low (M 5 9.1%). Levels of compliance
decreased when the break contingency was
implemented alone (M 5 2.3%). Compliance
was higher when the music contingency was
implemented alone (M 5 46.9%). When the
contingencies were combined, levels of compli-
ance were highest (M 5 86.4%) and most
stable, with 100% compliance for the last five
sessions. Figure 1 (bottom) depicts the results
of the second analysis in which low levels of
compliance were observed during baseline
(M 5 9.7%) and break alone (compliance was
not observed). Compliance increased slightly in
the music condition (M 5 26.2%). Initially,
compliance was low in the yoked combined
condition but increased to near 100% toward
the end of this condition (M 5 67.8%).
In sum, the combined contingency was most
effective for increasing task compliance. When
implemented alone, neither contingency in-
creased compliance above 50%; however, the
music contingency implemented alone was
generally more effective than the break contin-
gency implemented alone. It is possible that
carryover effects between the combined condi-
tion and music condition may have altered
levels of compliance in the music condition of
the initial analysis. In other words, Nate first
experienced access to music for compliance in
the combined condition, which might have
affected levels of compliance in the subsequent
music condition. Different levels of compliance
may have been observed if the music condition
directly followed the break condition. This
possibility was addressed in the second analysis
by implementing the music and break condi-
tions in this order; levels of compliance during
the music condition of the second analysis
(M 5 26.2%) were lower than those observed
in the initial analysis (M 5 46.9%).
The purpose of the second analysis was to
address the possibility that the number of tasks
presented during each session influenced the
differences in compliance observed between the
initial music and the combined conditions.
During the first analysis, all sessions were
10 min in length. Therefore, more tasks were
presented throughout the session when the
music contingency was implemented alone
(M 5 34.9 tasks presented during the music
conditions of the initial analysis) than when the
negative reinforcement contingency was also in
effect (M 5 9.8 tasks presented during the
combined conditions of the initial analysis).
Although compliance was more variable in the
yoked combined condition (relative to the
combined condition of the initial analysis),
the overall results were consistent across all
exposures to the combined condition. That is,
the results of the second analysis suggested that
the increase in compliance observed under the
combined contingency (relative to music alone)
in the first analysis was not a function of the
number of tasks presented.
Results of the current study suggest that a
combination of both positive and negative
reinforcement was most effective for increasing
one participant’s compliance to simple tasks.
One possible explanation for these results is that
EFFECTS OF REINFORCEMENT ON TASK COMPLIANCE 177
the combination of both contingencies in-
creased the individual value of each reinforcer.
This may also resemble naturally occurring
consequences that maintain noncompliance.
For instance, when a child is noncompliant
following a parent instruction and the parent
allows escape from that instruction, the child
likely has access to preferred items rather than
sitting without engaging in any activity (as
arranged in the break condition). These results
are consistent with previous research in which a
combination of positive and negative reinforce-
ment was most effective for reaching treatment
goals (e.g., DeLeon et al., 2001; Kodak et al.,
2007; Lalli et al., 1999; Piazza et al., 1997).
However, the current study is noteworthy in
that previous research evaluated these contin-
gencies for affecting levels of escape-maintained
Figure 1. Nate’s percentage of task compliance across the baseline (BL), break (BK), music (MU), and combined
(CO) conditions of the first analysis (top) and second analysis (bottom).
178 KELLY J. BOUXSEIN et al.
problem behavior, whereas the current investi-
gation examined the effects of these contingen-
cies on compliance.
Nonetheless, results are limited by the
inclusion of only one task. Future studies would
benefit from the assessment of a variety of tasks
across a variety of settings with less intrusive
stimuli (e.g., music delivered via headphones).
Likewise, noncontingent (e.g., continuous)
access to positive reinforcement might increase
compliance by functioning as an abolishing
operation (cf. Lomas, Fisher, & Kelley, 2010).
It is also possible that the participant encoun-
tered more physical prompting in the condi-
tions associated with lower levels of compliance.
To the extent that physical guidance may be an
aversive stimulus, it is possible that the greater
likelihood of physical prompting produced an
overall suppression of behavior in these condi-
tions (i.e., a generalized punishment effect).
Thus, future research might evaluate these
potential influences on interventions for in-
creasing compliance.
REFERENCES
DeLeon, I. G., Neidert, P. L., Anders, B. M., &
Rodriguez-Catter, V. (2001). Choices between posi-
tive and negative reinforcement during treatment for
escape-maintained behavior. Journal of Applied Be-
havior Analysis, 34, 521–525.
Kodak, T., Lerman, D. C., Volkert, V. M., & Trosclair,
N. (2007). Further examination of factors that
influence preference for positive versus negative
reinforcement. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis,
40, 25–44.
Lalli, J. S., Vollmer, T. R., Progar, P. R., Wright, C.,
Borrero, J., Daniel, D., et al. (1999). Competition
between positive and negative reinforcement in the
treatment of escape behavior. Journal of Applied
Behavior Analysis, 32, 285–296.
Lomas, J. E., Fisher, W. W., & Kelley, M. E. (2010). The
effects of variable-time delivery of food items and
praise on problem behavior reinforced by escape.
Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 43, 425–435.
Merchant, M., Young, K. R., & West, R. P. (2004). The
effects of parental teaching on compliance behavior of
children. Psychology in the Schools, 41, 337–350.
Piazza, C. C., Fisher, W. W., Hanley, G. P., Remick,
M. L., Contrucci, S. A., & Aitken, T. L. (1997). The
use of positive and negative reinforcement in the
treatment of escape-maintained destructive behavior.
Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 30, 279–298.
Roane, H. S., Vollmer, T. R., Ringdahl, J. E., & Marcus,
B. A. (1998). Evaluation of a brief stimulus preference
assessment. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 31,
605–620.
Walker, H. M. (1986). The assessments for integration
into mainstream settings (AIMS) assessment system:
Rationale, instruments, procedures, and outcomes.
Journal of Clinical Child Psychology, 15, 55–63.
Wierson, M., & Forehand, R. (1994). Parent behavioral
training for child noncompliance: Rationale, con-
cepts, and effectiveness. Current Directions in Psycho-
logical Science, 3, 146–151.
Received November 20, 2009
Final acceptance May 25, 2010
Action Editor, Joel Ringdahl
EFFECTS OF REINFORCEMENT ON TASK COMPLIANCE 179

More Related Content

Viewers also liked

Bribery
BriberyBribery
Bribery
Azhar Alias
 
Bribery ppt (1) our presentation
Bribery ppt (1) our presentationBribery ppt (1) our presentation
Bribery ppt (1) our presentation
JamiLa Khan
 
Reinforcement (Behavioral Learning)
Reinforcement (Behavioral Learning)Reinforcement (Behavioral Learning)
Reinforcement (Behavioral Learning)
Emman Chavez
 
Reinforcement motivation theory
Reinforcement motivation theoryReinforcement motivation theory
Reinforcement motivation theory
Avinash default
 
Reinforcement ppt assignment
Reinforcement ppt assignmentReinforcement ppt assignment
Reinforcement ppt assignmenthailnur1794
 

Viewers also liked (6)

Bribery
BriberyBribery
Bribery
 
Bribery ppt (1) our presentation
Bribery ppt (1) our presentationBribery ppt (1) our presentation
Bribery ppt (1) our presentation
 
Microteaching reinforcement
Microteaching reinforcementMicroteaching reinforcement
Microteaching reinforcement
 
Reinforcement (Behavioral Learning)
Reinforcement (Behavioral Learning)Reinforcement (Behavioral Learning)
Reinforcement (Behavioral Learning)
 
Reinforcement motivation theory
Reinforcement motivation theoryReinforcement motivation theory
Reinforcement motivation theory
 
Reinforcement ppt assignment
Reinforcement ppt assignmentReinforcement ppt assignment
Reinforcement ppt assignment
 

Similar to Ej931945

THE EFFECTS OF A HIGH-PROBABILITY INSTRUCTION SEQUENCEAND RE.docx
THE EFFECTS OF A HIGH-PROBABILITY INSTRUCTION SEQUENCEAND RE.docxTHE EFFECTS OF A HIGH-PROBABILITY INSTRUCTION SEQUENCEAND RE.docx
THE EFFECTS OF A HIGH-PROBABILITY INSTRUCTION SEQUENCEAND RE.docx
rhetttrevannion
 
Research Proposal.edited
Research Proposal.editedResearch Proposal.edited
Research Proposal.editedKendrik Jones
 
A PROCEDURE FOR IDENTIFYING PRECURSORS TOPROBLEM BEHAVIOR.docx
A PROCEDURE FOR IDENTIFYING PRECURSORS TOPROBLEM BEHAVIOR.docxA PROCEDURE FOR IDENTIFYING PRECURSORS TOPROBLEM BEHAVIOR.docx
A PROCEDURE FOR IDENTIFYING PRECURSORS TOPROBLEM BEHAVIOR.docx
bartholomeocoombs
 
Group Estimation Lab Report
Group Estimation Lab ReportGroup Estimation Lab Report
Group Estimation Lab ReportWilliam Teng
 
Psychology IA
Psychology IAPsychology IA
Psychology IAfuyuki31
 
Efectos de entrenar con música en la fuerza, expresividad y humor
Efectos de entrenar con música en la fuerza, expresividad y humor Efectos de entrenar con música en la fuerza, expresividad y humor
Efectos de entrenar con música en la fuerza, expresividad y humor
EscuelaNacionalEntrenadoresFER
 
MUSIC MNEMONICS - Glee El Blanco.pptx
MUSIC MNEMONICS - Glee El Blanco.pptxMUSIC MNEMONICS - Glee El Blanco.pptx
MUSIC MNEMONICS - Glee El Blanco.pptx
JulevieBlanco
 
ASPR 2009 Presentation on 02.12.09 (Tony Machin)
ASPR 2009 Presentation on 02.12.09 (Tony Machin)ASPR 2009 Presentation on 02.12.09 (Tony Machin)
ASPR 2009 Presentation on 02.12.09 (Tony Machin)
Tony Machin
 
Aspr 2009 Presentation (Tony Machin)
Aspr 2009 Presentation (Tony Machin)Aspr 2009 Presentation (Tony Machin)
Aspr 2009 Presentation (Tony Machin)guestaedf29
 
411JOURNAL OF APPLIED BEHAVIOR ANALYSIS 2004, 37, 411–415
411JOURNAL OF APPLIED BEHAVIOR ANALYSIS 2004, 37, 411–415 411JOURNAL OF APPLIED BEHAVIOR ANALYSIS 2004, 37, 411–415
411JOURNAL OF APPLIED BEHAVIOR ANALYSIS 2004, 37, 411–415
maple8qvlisbey
 
Two Hemispheres are Better Than One for Motor Learning_NCM 2015 Conference Po...
Two Hemispheres are Better Than One for Motor Learning_NCM 2015 Conference Po...Two Hemispheres are Better Than One for Motor Learning_NCM 2015 Conference Po...
Two Hemispheres are Better Than One for Motor Learning_NCM 2015 Conference Po...Kalpish Shah
 
1. Elaborate on the coca-cocaine commodity value chain and the ill.docx
1. Elaborate on the coca-cocaine commodity value chain and the ill.docx1. Elaborate on the coca-cocaine commodity value chain and the ill.docx
1. Elaborate on the coca-cocaine commodity value chain and the ill.docx
SONU61709
 
Constraint induced language therapy
Constraint induced language therapyConstraint induced language therapy
Constraint induced language therapy
Arshelle Kibs
 
Increasing responding to vocal directions
Increasing responding to vocal directionsIncreasing responding to vocal directions
Increasing responding to vocal directionsParisa Foroughi
 
Introduction. unrelieved postoperative pain remains a common probl
Introduction. unrelieved postoperative pain remains a common problIntroduction. unrelieved postoperative pain remains a common probl
Introduction. unrelieved postoperative pain remains a common probl
ssuser337fce
 
Coburn Rojo 2016 SWACSM (1)
Coburn Rojo 2016 SWACSM (1)Coburn Rojo 2016 SWACSM (1)
Coburn Rojo 2016 SWACSM (1)Vanessa Rojo
 

Similar to Ej931945 (20)

THE EFFECTS OF A HIGH-PROBABILITY INSTRUCTION SEQUENCEAND RE.docx
THE EFFECTS OF A HIGH-PROBABILITY INSTRUCTION SEQUENCEAND RE.docxTHE EFFECTS OF A HIGH-PROBABILITY INSTRUCTION SEQUENCEAND RE.docx
THE EFFECTS OF A HIGH-PROBABILITY INSTRUCTION SEQUENCEAND RE.docx
 
Research Proposal.edited
Research Proposal.editedResearch Proposal.edited
Research Proposal.edited
 
A PROCEDURE FOR IDENTIFYING PRECURSORS TOPROBLEM BEHAVIOR.docx
A PROCEDURE FOR IDENTIFYING PRECURSORS TOPROBLEM BEHAVIOR.docxA PROCEDURE FOR IDENTIFYING PRECURSORS TOPROBLEM BEHAVIOR.docx
A PROCEDURE FOR IDENTIFYING PRECURSORS TOPROBLEM BEHAVIOR.docx
 
WhittemorePoster_Final
WhittemorePoster_FinalWhittemorePoster_Final
WhittemorePoster_Final
 
Group Estimation Lab Report
Group Estimation Lab ReportGroup Estimation Lab Report
Group Estimation Lab Report
 
Revised Poster.pptx
Revised Poster.pptxRevised Poster.pptx
Revised Poster.pptx
 
Psychology IA
Psychology IAPsychology IA
Psychology IA
 
Efectos de entrenar con música en la fuerza, expresividad y humor
Efectos de entrenar con música en la fuerza, expresividad y humor Efectos de entrenar con música en la fuerza, expresividad y humor
Efectos de entrenar con música en la fuerza, expresividad y humor
 
MUSIC MNEMONICS - Glee El Blanco.pptx
MUSIC MNEMONICS - Glee El Blanco.pptxMUSIC MNEMONICS - Glee El Blanco.pptx
MUSIC MNEMONICS - Glee El Blanco.pptx
 
ASPR 2009 Presentation on 02.12.09 (Tony Machin)
ASPR 2009 Presentation on 02.12.09 (Tony Machin)ASPR 2009 Presentation on 02.12.09 (Tony Machin)
ASPR 2009 Presentation on 02.12.09 (Tony Machin)
 
Aspr 2009 Presentation (Tony Machin)
Aspr 2009 Presentation (Tony Machin)Aspr 2009 Presentation (Tony Machin)
Aspr 2009 Presentation (Tony Machin)
 
411JOURNAL OF APPLIED BEHAVIOR ANALYSIS 2004, 37, 411–415
411JOURNAL OF APPLIED BEHAVIOR ANALYSIS 2004, 37, 411–415 411JOURNAL OF APPLIED BEHAVIOR ANALYSIS 2004, 37, 411–415
411JOURNAL OF APPLIED BEHAVIOR ANALYSIS 2004, 37, 411–415
 
Two Hemispheres are Better Than One for Motor Learning_NCM 2015 Conference Po...
Two Hemispheres are Better Than One for Motor Learning_NCM 2015 Conference Po...Two Hemispheres are Better Than One for Motor Learning_NCM 2015 Conference Po...
Two Hemispheres are Better Than One for Motor Learning_NCM 2015 Conference Po...
 
1. Elaborate on the coca-cocaine commodity value chain and the ill.docx
1. Elaborate on the coca-cocaine commodity value chain and the ill.docx1. Elaborate on the coca-cocaine commodity value chain and the ill.docx
1. Elaborate on the coca-cocaine commodity value chain and the ill.docx
 
RDSA II Paper Final Draft
RDSA II Paper Final Draft  RDSA II Paper Final Draft
RDSA II Paper Final Draft
 
Sources of Avoidance Motivation
Sources of Avoidance MotivationSources of Avoidance Motivation
Sources of Avoidance Motivation
 
Constraint induced language therapy
Constraint induced language therapyConstraint induced language therapy
Constraint induced language therapy
 
Increasing responding to vocal directions
Increasing responding to vocal directionsIncreasing responding to vocal directions
Increasing responding to vocal directions
 
Introduction. unrelieved postoperative pain remains a common probl
Introduction. unrelieved postoperative pain remains a common problIntroduction. unrelieved postoperative pain remains a common probl
Introduction. unrelieved postoperative pain remains a common probl
 
Coburn Rojo 2016 SWACSM (1)
Coburn Rojo 2016 SWACSM (1)Coburn Rojo 2016 SWACSM (1)
Coburn Rojo 2016 SWACSM (1)
 

Recently uploaded

Language Across the Curriculm LAC B.Ed.
Language Across the  Curriculm LAC B.Ed.Language Across the  Curriculm LAC B.Ed.
Language Across the Curriculm LAC B.Ed.
Atul Kumar Singh
 
Best Digital Marketing Institute In NOIDA
Best Digital Marketing Institute In NOIDABest Digital Marketing Institute In NOIDA
Best Digital Marketing Institute In NOIDA
deeptiverma2406
 
Synthetic Fiber Construction in lab .pptx
Synthetic Fiber Construction in lab .pptxSynthetic Fiber Construction in lab .pptx
Synthetic Fiber Construction in lab .pptx
Pavel ( NSTU)
 
Thesis Statement for students diagnonsed withADHD.ppt
Thesis Statement for students diagnonsed withADHD.pptThesis Statement for students diagnonsed withADHD.ppt
Thesis Statement for students diagnonsed withADHD.ppt
EverAndrsGuerraGuerr
 
aaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaa
aaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaa
aaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaa
siemaillard
 
Supporting (UKRI) OA monographs at Salford.pptx
Supporting (UKRI) OA monographs at Salford.pptxSupporting (UKRI) OA monographs at Salford.pptx
Supporting (UKRI) OA monographs at Salford.pptx
Jisc
 
2024.06.01 Introducing a competency framework for languag learning materials ...
2024.06.01 Introducing a competency framework for languag learning materials ...2024.06.01 Introducing a competency framework for languag learning materials ...
2024.06.01 Introducing a competency framework for languag learning materials ...
Sandy Millin
 
BÀI TẬP BỔ TRỢ TIẾNG ANH GLOBAL SUCCESS LỚP 3 - CẢ NĂM (CÓ FILE NGHE VÀ ĐÁP Á...
BÀI TẬP BỔ TRỢ TIẾNG ANH GLOBAL SUCCESS LỚP 3 - CẢ NĂM (CÓ FILE NGHE VÀ ĐÁP Á...BÀI TẬP BỔ TRỢ TIẾNG ANH GLOBAL SUCCESS LỚP 3 - CẢ NĂM (CÓ FILE NGHE VÀ ĐÁP Á...
BÀI TẬP BỔ TRỢ TIẾNG ANH GLOBAL SUCCESS LỚP 3 - CẢ NĂM (CÓ FILE NGHE VÀ ĐÁP Á...
Nguyen Thanh Tu Collection
 
The Accursed House by Émile Gaboriau.pptx
The Accursed House by Émile Gaboriau.pptxThe Accursed House by Émile Gaboriau.pptx
The Accursed House by Émile Gaboriau.pptx
DhatriParmar
 
How libraries can support authors with open access requirements for UKRI fund...
How libraries can support authors with open access requirements for UKRI fund...How libraries can support authors with open access requirements for UKRI fund...
How libraries can support authors with open access requirements for UKRI fund...
Jisc
 
Group Presentation 2 Economics.Ariana Buscigliopptx
Group Presentation 2 Economics.Ariana BuscigliopptxGroup Presentation 2 Economics.Ariana Buscigliopptx
Group Presentation 2 Economics.Ariana Buscigliopptx
ArianaBusciglio
 
The approach at University of Liverpool.pptx
The approach at University of Liverpool.pptxThe approach at University of Liverpool.pptx
The approach at University of Liverpool.pptx
Jisc
 
Home assignment II on Spectroscopy 2024 Answers.pdf
Home assignment II on Spectroscopy 2024 Answers.pdfHome assignment II on Spectroscopy 2024 Answers.pdf
Home assignment II on Spectroscopy 2024 Answers.pdf
Tamralipta Mahavidyalaya
 
Honest Reviews of Tim Han LMA Course Program.pptx
Honest Reviews of Tim Han LMA Course Program.pptxHonest Reviews of Tim Han LMA Course Program.pptx
Honest Reviews of Tim Han LMA Course Program.pptx
timhan337
 
A Strategic Approach: GenAI in Education
A Strategic Approach: GenAI in EducationA Strategic Approach: GenAI in Education
A Strategic Approach: GenAI in Education
Peter Windle
 
June 3, 2024 Anti-Semitism Letter Sent to MIT President Kornbluth and MIT Cor...
June 3, 2024 Anti-Semitism Letter Sent to MIT President Kornbluth and MIT Cor...June 3, 2024 Anti-Semitism Letter Sent to MIT President Kornbluth and MIT Cor...
June 3, 2024 Anti-Semitism Letter Sent to MIT President Kornbluth and MIT Cor...
Levi Shapiro
 
The French Revolution Class 9 Study Material pdf free download
The French Revolution Class 9 Study Material pdf free downloadThe French Revolution Class 9 Study Material pdf free download
The French Revolution Class 9 Study Material pdf free download
Vivekanand Anglo Vedic Academy
 
Azure Interview Questions and Answers PDF By ScholarHat
Azure Interview Questions and Answers PDF By ScholarHatAzure Interview Questions and Answers PDF By ScholarHat
Azure Interview Questions and Answers PDF By ScholarHat
Scholarhat
 
S1-Introduction-Biopesticides in ICM.pptx
S1-Introduction-Biopesticides in ICM.pptxS1-Introduction-Biopesticides in ICM.pptx
S1-Introduction-Biopesticides in ICM.pptx
tarandeep35
 
Embracing GenAI - A Strategic Imperative
Embracing GenAI - A Strategic ImperativeEmbracing GenAI - A Strategic Imperative
Embracing GenAI - A Strategic Imperative
Peter Windle
 

Recently uploaded (20)

Language Across the Curriculm LAC B.Ed.
Language Across the  Curriculm LAC B.Ed.Language Across the  Curriculm LAC B.Ed.
Language Across the Curriculm LAC B.Ed.
 
Best Digital Marketing Institute In NOIDA
Best Digital Marketing Institute In NOIDABest Digital Marketing Institute In NOIDA
Best Digital Marketing Institute In NOIDA
 
Synthetic Fiber Construction in lab .pptx
Synthetic Fiber Construction in lab .pptxSynthetic Fiber Construction in lab .pptx
Synthetic Fiber Construction in lab .pptx
 
Thesis Statement for students diagnonsed withADHD.ppt
Thesis Statement for students diagnonsed withADHD.pptThesis Statement for students diagnonsed withADHD.ppt
Thesis Statement for students diagnonsed withADHD.ppt
 
aaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaa
aaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaa
aaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaa
 
Supporting (UKRI) OA monographs at Salford.pptx
Supporting (UKRI) OA monographs at Salford.pptxSupporting (UKRI) OA monographs at Salford.pptx
Supporting (UKRI) OA monographs at Salford.pptx
 
2024.06.01 Introducing a competency framework for languag learning materials ...
2024.06.01 Introducing a competency framework for languag learning materials ...2024.06.01 Introducing a competency framework for languag learning materials ...
2024.06.01 Introducing a competency framework for languag learning materials ...
 
BÀI TẬP BỔ TRỢ TIẾNG ANH GLOBAL SUCCESS LỚP 3 - CẢ NĂM (CÓ FILE NGHE VÀ ĐÁP Á...
BÀI TẬP BỔ TRỢ TIẾNG ANH GLOBAL SUCCESS LỚP 3 - CẢ NĂM (CÓ FILE NGHE VÀ ĐÁP Á...BÀI TẬP BỔ TRỢ TIẾNG ANH GLOBAL SUCCESS LỚP 3 - CẢ NĂM (CÓ FILE NGHE VÀ ĐÁP Á...
BÀI TẬP BỔ TRỢ TIẾNG ANH GLOBAL SUCCESS LỚP 3 - CẢ NĂM (CÓ FILE NGHE VÀ ĐÁP Á...
 
The Accursed House by Émile Gaboriau.pptx
The Accursed House by Émile Gaboriau.pptxThe Accursed House by Émile Gaboriau.pptx
The Accursed House by Émile Gaboriau.pptx
 
How libraries can support authors with open access requirements for UKRI fund...
How libraries can support authors with open access requirements for UKRI fund...How libraries can support authors with open access requirements for UKRI fund...
How libraries can support authors with open access requirements for UKRI fund...
 
Group Presentation 2 Economics.Ariana Buscigliopptx
Group Presentation 2 Economics.Ariana BuscigliopptxGroup Presentation 2 Economics.Ariana Buscigliopptx
Group Presentation 2 Economics.Ariana Buscigliopptx
 
The approach at University of Liverpool.pptx
The approach at University of Liverpool.pptxThe approach at University of Liverpool.pptx
The approach at University of Liverpool.pptx
 
Home assignment II on Spectroscopy 2024 Answers.pdf
Home assignment II on Spectroscopy 2024 Answers.pdfHome assignment II on Spectroscopy 2024 Answers.pdf
Home assignment II on Spectroscopy 2024 Answers.pdf
 
Honest Reviews of Tim Han LMA Course Program.pptx
Honest Reviews of Tim Han LMA Course Program.pptxHonest Reviews of Tim Han LMA Course Program.pptx
Honest Reviews of Tim Han LMA Course Program.pptx
 
A Strategic Approach: GenAI in Education
A Strategic Approach: GenAI in EducationA Strategic Approach: GenAI in Education
A Strategic Approach: GenAI in Education
 
June 3, 2024 Anti-Semitism Letter Sent to MIT President Kornbluth and MIT Cor...
June 3, 2024 Anti-Semitism Letter Sent to MIT President Kornbluth and MIT Cor...June 3, 2024 Anti-Semitism Letter Sent to MIT President Kornbluth and MIT Cor...
June 3, 2024 Anti-Semitism Letter Sent to MIT President Kornbluth and MIT Cor...
 
The French Revolution Class 9 Study Material pdf free download
The French Revolution Class 9 Study Material pdf free downloadThe French Revolution Class 9 Study Material pdf free download
The French Revolution Class 9 Study Material pdf free download
 
Azure Interview Questions and Answers PDF By ScholarHat
Azure Interview Questions and Answers PDF By ScholarHatAzure Interview Questions and Answers PDF By ScholarHat
Azure Interview Questions and Answers PDF By ScholarHat
 
S1-Introduction-Biopesticides in ICM.pptx
S1-Introduction-Biopesticides in ICM.pptxS1-Introduction-Biopesticides in ICM.pptx
S1-Introduction-Biopesticides in ICM.pptx
 
Embracing GenAI - A Strategic Imperative
Embracing GenAI - A Strategic ImperativeEmbracing GenAI - A Strategic Imperative
Embracing GenAI - A Strategic Imperative
 

Ej931945

  • 1. EVALUATING THE SEPARATE AND COMBINED EFFECTS OF POSITIVE AND NEGATIVE REINFORCEMENT ON TASK COMPLIANCE KELLY J. BOUXSEIN, HENRY S. ROANE, AND TARA HARPER MUNROE-MEYER INSTITUTE UNIVERSITY OF NEBRASKA MEDICAL CENTER Positive and negative reinforcement are effective for treating escape-maintained destructive behavior. The current study evaluated the separate and combined effects of these contingencies to increase task compliance. Results showed that a combination of positive and negative reinforcement was most effective for increasing compliance. Key words: compliance, negative reinforcement, positive reinforcement _______________________________________________________________________________ Teachers consider compliance to be a critical skill for success in a classroom (Walker, 1986). Lack of consistent compliance (i.e., noncom- pliance) can lead to significant deficits in skill development (e.g., self-help skills), negatively affect family life and parental stress levels (Wierson & Forehand, 1994), and lead to more severe behavior problems (e.g., delinquen- cy, aggression; Merchant, Young, & West, 2004). Given the potential adverse impact of noncompliance, interventions for increasing compliance are warranted. Positive reinforcement alone (DeLeon, Nei- dert, Anders, & Rodriguez-Catter, 2001; Lalli et al., 1999) or in combination with negative reinforcement (Kodak, Lerman, Volkert, & Trosclair, 2007; Piazza et al., 1997) has been demonstrated to be effective for increasing compliance while resulting in reduced levels of destructive behavior. However, little research has evaluated the effects of negative and positive reinforcement on compliance independent of treating destructive behavior. In addition, the existing research has not evaluated the relative effects of positive and negative reinforcement alone versus in combination. Thus, the current study evaluated the separate and combined effects of positive and negative reinforcement on task compliance. METHOD Participant and Setting Nate, a 14-year-old boy who had been diagnosed with Down syndrome, displayed noncompliance with caregiver demands (e.g., self-help skills, academic tasks, daily chores). All sessions were conducted in a room (4 m by 4 m) that contained a CD player (during those conditions in which the positive reinforcement contingency was in place) and instructional materials. The instructional materials consisted of five to 10 separate pieces of paper that were each formed into a ball (hereafter referred to as trash) and a trash can. Response Measurement and Interobserver Agreement Throwing away trash was selected as the target task for this evaluation based on a review of Nate’s existing self-help goals and because the response could be physically guided. Compli- ance was defined as completion of the task within 5 s of a vocal or modeled prompt and prior to a physical prompt. Task presentation Address correspondence to Henry S. Roane, who is now at the Departments of Pediatrics and Psychiatry, SUNY Upstate Medical University, 750 E. Adams St. TU#3, Syracuse, New York 13210 (e-mail: roaneh@upstate.edu). doi: 10.1901/jaba.2011.44-175 This project was supported by Grant R03MH083193 from the National Institute of Mental Health. The content is solely the responsibility of the authors and does not necessarily represent the official views of the National Institute of Mental Health or the National Institutes of Health. We thank Ty Starks for his assistance with data collection and Heather Kadey for her comments on an earlier version of this manuscript. JOURNAL OF APPLIED BEHAVIOR ANALYSIS 2011, 44, 175–179 NUMBER 1 (SPRING 2011) 175
  • 2. was scored at the initiation of a prompting hierarchy, not the initiation of each separate prompt. Observers collected data on the frequency of task compliance and task presen- tation using a computer-based data-collection program. Percentage of compliance was calcu- lated by dividing the frequency of task compliance by the frequency of tasks presenta- tions and multiplying by 100%. Interobserver agreement data were collected for 34% of sessions. To calculate interobserver agreement, a computer program partitioned each session into successive 10-s intervals. Exact agreement coefficients were calculated by divid- ing the number of agreements by the number of agreements plus disagreements and multiplying by 100%. An agreement was defined as both observers scoring the same frequency of a response during the same 10-s interval. Agree- ment averaged 95% (range, 68% to 100%) for task compliance and 94% (range, 67% to 100%) for task presentations. Procedure Throughout all conditions, trash was dumped on the ground next to the trash can and the can was placed within arm’s reach of the participant each time the trash was emptied. When Nate placed all of the trash into the can, the therapist subsequently dumped out the can such that the target response was continuously available throughout the session. Tasks were presented using a graduated prompting hierar- chy that consisted of sequential vocal, modeled, and physical prompts in which the vocal prompt was presented immediately following the preceding task (or at the beginning of the session; i.e., a 0-s delay to vocal prompts). Following the vocal prompt, the subsequent prompts were delivered every 5 s unless Nate complied with the task or was in the process of completing the task. The consequences ar- ranged for compliance varied across conditions, and Nate was provided with rules prior to each session regarding the consequence in place for compliance. All sessions were 10 min in length, with the exception of the combined (break plus music) sessions conducted in the second analysis, in which session length was increased due to yoking the number of tasks presented during sessions (described below). Session length in the yoked combined condition ranged from 8 min to about 33 min. During baseline of the initial analysis, the experimenter provided brief praise for compli- ance (e.g., ‘‘nice job, Nate’’) and presented tasks continuously using the prompting hierarchy described above (i.e., there were no breaks for compliance). Following baseline, we compared a break condition, a music condition, and a combined contingency (break plus music). During the break condition, compliance result- ed in brief praise and 60-s break from task presentations (i.e., task materials were removed for 60 s). During the music condition, com- pliance resulted in brief praise and 60-s access to music (identified as preferred in a preference assessment; Roane, Vollmer, Ringdahl, & Marcus, 1998) while task presentation contin- ued using the prompting procedure. In the combined condition, compliance resulted in brief praise and a break from task presentations with simultaneous access to music for 60 s. The initial analysis was conducted in a reversal (ABCBCDCDC) design. After the initial comparison of the various contingencies, we conducted a second analysis to evaluate whether the number of task presentations altered the effectiveness of the different reinforcement contingencies. That is, it was possible that the observed increase in compliance during the combined condition relative to the music condition was due to the overall lower amount of task presentations in the former condition. During the second analysis, the baseline, break, and music condi- tions were conducted in a manner identical to that described in the initial analysis. The yoked combined condition included the same contin- gencies as described above (i.e., brief praise, 60- s break from task presentations with access to 176 KELLY J. BOUXSEIN et al.
  • 3. music); however, the number of tasks presented during this condition was yoked to match the number of tasks presented in the first two phases of the music condition of the second analysis. Specifically, the total number of tasks presented in each session of the initial music phase (e.g., 61 and 54; M 5 57 tasks presented per music session) was recorded, and each total was randomly assigned to a session in the yoked combined condition (some totals were used across multiple sessions due the different number of sessions conducted in the music and combined conditions; M 5 56.1 tasks presented per yoked combined session). The baseline, break, music, and yoked combined conditions were compared in a reversal (ABCBCDCD) design. RESULTS AND DISCUSSION The results of the initial analysis are shown in Figure 1 (top). During baseline, compliance was low (M 5 9.1%). Levels of compliance decreased when the break contingency was implemented alone (M 5 2.3%). Compliance was higher when the music contingency was implemented alone (M 5 46.9%). When the contingencies were combined, levels of compli- ance were highest (M 5 86.4%) and most stable, with 100% compliance for the last five sessions. Figure 1 (bottom) depicts the results of the second analysis in which low levels of compliance were observed during baseline (M 5 9.7%) and break alone (compliance was not observed). Compliance increased slightly in the music condition (M 5 26.2%). Initially, compliance was low in the yoked combined condition but increased to near 100% toward the end of this condition (M 5 67.8%). In sum, the combined contingency was most effective for increasing task compliance. When implemented alone, neither contingency in- creased compliance above 50%; however, the music contingency implemented alone was generally more effective than the break contin- gency implemented alone. It is possible that carryover effects between the combined condi- tion and music condition may have altered levels of compliance in the music condition of the initial analysis. In other words, Nate first experienced access to music for compliance in the combined condition, which might have affected levels of compliance in the subsequent music condition. Different levels of compliance may have been observed if the music condition directly followed the break condition. This possibility was addressed in the second analysis by implementing the music and break condi- tions in this order; levels of compliance during the music condition of the second analysis (M 5 26.2%) were lower than those observed in the initial analysis (M 5 46.9%). The purpose of the second analysis was to address the possibility that the number of tasks presented during each session influenced the differences in compliance observed between the initial music and the combined conditions. During the first analysis, all sessions were 10 min in length. Therefore, more tasks were presented throughout the session when the music contingency was implemented alone (M 5 34.9 tasks presented during the music conditions of the initial analysis) than when the negative reinforcement contingency was also in effect (M 5 9.8 tasks presented during the combined conditions of the initial analysis). Although compliance was more variable in the yoked combined condition (relative to the combined condition of the initial analysis), the overall results were consistent across all exposures to the combined condition. That is, the results of the second analysis suggested that the increase in compliance observed under the combined contingency (relative to music alone) in the first analysis was not a function of the number of tasks presented. Results of the current study suggest that a combination of both positive and negative reinforcement was most effective for increasing one participant’s compliance to simple tasks. One possible explanation for these results is that EFFECTS OF REINFORCEMENT ON TASK COMPLIANCE 177
  • 4. the combination of both contingencies in- creased the individual value of each reinforcer. This may also resemble naturally occurring consequences that maintain noncompliance. For instance, when a child is noncompliant following a parent instruction and the parent allows escape from that instruction, the child likely has access to preferred items rather than sitting without engaging in any activity (as arranged in the break condition). These results are consistent with previous research in which a combination of positive and negative reinforce- ment was most effective for reaching treatment goals (e.g., DeLeon et al., 2001; Kodak et al., 2007; Lalli et al., 1999; Piazza et al., 1997). However, the current study is noteworthy in that previous research evaluated these contin- gencies for affecting levels of escape-maintained Figure 1. Nate’s percentage of task compliance across the baseline (BL), break (BK), music (MU), and combined (CO) conditions of the first analysis (top) and second analysis (bottom). 178 KELLY J. BOUXSEIN et al.
  • 5. problem behavior, whereas the current investi- gation examined the effects of these contingen- cies on compliance. Nonetheless, results are limited by the inclusion of only one task. Future studies would benefit from the assessment of a variety of tasks across a variety of settings with less intrusive stimuli (e.g., music delivered via headphones). Likewise, noncontingent (e.g., continuous) access to positive reinforcement might increase compliance by functioning as an abolishing operation (cf. Lomas, Fisher, & Kelley, 2010). It is also possible that the participant encoun- tered more physical prompting in the condi- tions associated with lower levels of compliance. To the extent that physical guidance may be an aversive stimulus, it is possible that the greater likelihood of physical prompting produced an overall suppression of behavior in these condi- tions (i.e., a generalized punishment effect). Thus, future research might evaluate these potential influences on interventions for in- creasing compliance. REFERENCES DeLeon, I. G., Neidert, P. L., Anders, B. M., & Rodriguez-Catter, V. (2001). Choices between posi- tive and negative reinforcement during treatment for escape-maintained behavior. Journal of Applied Be- havior Analysis, 34, 521–525. Kodak, T., Lerman, D. C., Volkert, V. M., & Trosclair, N. (2007). Further examination of factors that influence preference for positive versus negative reinforcement. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 40, 25–44. Lalli, J. S., Vollmer, T. R., Progar, P. R., Wright, C., Borrero, J., Daniel, D., et al. (1999). Competition between positive and negative reinforcement in the treatment of escape behavior. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 32, 285–296. Lomas, J. E., Fisher, W. W., & Kelley, M. E. (2010). The effects of variable-time delivery of food items and praise on problem behavior reinforced by escape. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 43, 425–435. Merchant, M., Young, K. R., & West, R. P. (2004). The effects of parental teaching on compliance behavior of children. Psychology in the Schools, 41, 337–350. Piazza, C. C., Fisher, W. W., Hanley, G. P., Remick, M. L., Contrucci, S. A., & Aitken, T. L. (1997). The use of positive and negative reinforcement in the treatment of escape-maintained destructive behavior. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 30, 279–298. Roane, H. S., Vollmer, T. R., Ringdahl, J. E., & Marcus, B. A. (1998). Evaluation of a brief stimulus preference assessment. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 31, 605–620. Walker, H. M. (1986). The assessments for integration into mainstream settings (AIMS) assessment system: Rationale, instruments, procedures, and outcomes. Journal of Clinical Child Psychology, 15, 55–63. Wierson, M., & Forehand, R. (1994). Parent behavioral training for child noncompliance: Rationale, con- cepts, and effectiveness. Current Directions in Psycho- logical Science, 3, 146–151. Received November 20, 2009 Final acceptance May 25, 2010 Action Editor, Joel Ringdahl EFFECTS OF REINFORCEMENT ON TASK COMPLIANCE 179