SlideShare a Scribd company logo
1 of 19
Download to read offline
Evidence-Based Systematic Review: 
Effects of Intensity of Treatment 
and Constraint-Induced Language 
Therapy for Individuals With 
Stroke-Induced Aphasia 
Purpose: This systematic review summarizes evidence for intensity of treatment and 
constraint-induced language therapy (CILT) on measures of language impairment and 
communication activity/participation in individuals with stroke-induced aphasia. 
Method: A systematic search of the aphasia literature using 15 electronic databases 
(e.g., PubMed, CINAHL) identified 10 studies meeting inclusion/exclusion criteria. A 
review panel evaluated studies for methodological quality. Studies were characterized 
by research stage (i.e., discovery, efficacy, effectiveness, cost–benefit/public policy 
research), and effect sizes (ESs) were calculated wherever possible. 
Results: In chronic aphasia, studies provided modest evidence for more intensive 
treatment and the positive effects of CILT. In acute aphasia, 1 study evaluated 
high-intensity treatment positively; no studies examined CILT. Four studies reported 
discovery research, with quality scores ranging from 3 to 6 of 8 possible markers. Five 
treatment efficacy studies had quality scores ranging from 5 to 7 of 9 possible markers. 
One study of treatment effectiveness received a score of 4 of 8 possible markers. 
Conclusion: Although modest evidence exists for more intensive treatment and CILT for 
individuals with stroke-induced aphasia, the results of this review should be considered 
preliminary and, when making treatment decisions, should be used in conjunction 
with clinical expertise and the client’s individual values. 
KEY WORDS: aphasia, intensity, constraint, rehabilitation 
Constraint-induced language therapy (CILT), a relatively new apha-sia 
treatment, (Pulvermuller et al., 2001), has garnered consid-erable 
interest from consumer groups and clients with aphasia. 
The major components of CILT involve both forced use of verbal language 
and massed practice (i.e., high intensity of treatment). Both are of con-siderable 
importance in light of recent work in neuroscience demonstrat-ing 
that the neuroplasticity of the adult brain can be impacted by several 
experience-dependent principles, including intensity of training and forced 
use of cognitive capacities (Kleim & Jones, 2008). Because intensity of 
treatment is an important variable that affects neuroplasticity, any dis-cussion 
of CILT must consider the impact not only of constraint but also 
of treatment intensity on outcomes. Therefore, the aim of this evidence-based 
systematic review (EBSR) was to examine the current state of the 
evidence that used one or both principles of CILT for individuals with 
stroke-induced aphasia: (a) constraining treatment responses to the verbal 
Leora R. Cherney 
Rehabilitation Institute of Chicago, IL 
Janet P. Patterson 
California State University, 
East Bay, Hayward, CA 
Anastasia Raymer 
Old Dominion University, Norfolk, VA 
Tobi Frymark 
Tracy Schooling 
American Speech-Language-Hearing 
Association, Rockville, MD 
Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research • Vol. 51 • 1282–1299 • October 2008 • D American Speech-Language-Hearing Association 
1092-4388/08/5105-1282 
1282
modality only and (b) providing treatment on an inten-sive 
delivery schedule. 
The neurobiological principles and rehabilitation 
techniques surrounding CILT were derived from animal 
studies (Taub, 1977) as well as from a series of investi-gations 
of limbmovements in patients with chronic stroke 
and hemiplegia (Taub,Miller, Novack,&Cook, 1993; Taub 
&Wolf, 1997). These studies found that motor behavior of 
an impaired limb was modifiable with intensive practice 
over a short period of time. The techniques collectively 
known as constraint-induced movement therapy (CIMT) 
involve restraining the use of the less affected arm for 
the majority of waking hours to counteract the presumed 
learned nonuse of the affected arm. Repetitive training 
and shaping of movements of the affected arm take place 
in intensive treatment (6 hr per day) over 2 consecutive 
weeks. 
In the past 20 years, a large body of evidence, ap-plying 
either the original CIMT technique or a variant of 
this approach, has accumulated in support of the efficacy 
of constraint-induced (CI) therapy for rehabilitating 
hemiparetic arm use in individuals with chronic stroke 
(Hakkennes & Keating, 2005; Taub, Uswatte, & Piclikiti, 
1999). To date, there are more than 120 published stud-ies, 
including a single-site, placebo-controlled trial (Taub 
et al., 2006) and a large multisite randomized trial (Wolf 
et al., 2006). Although the magnitude of the treatment 
effects have varied throughout the CIMT literature, the 
generally positive outcomes have encouraged investigators 
to examine the effects of CI and their potential benefits in 
other domains of stroke rehabilitation, including aphasia. 
Since being introduced to the field of aphasiology by 
Pulvermuller and colleagues (2001), CILT has been met 
with great interest. Pulvermuller et al. argued that pa-tients 
with aphasia often use the communication chan-nel 
that is accessible to them with the least amount of 
effort, such as gestures and drawing, rather than spoken 
language. A constrained approach for aphasia is incor-porated 
in the therapeutic setting by forcing the patient 
to communicate only through verbal channels while lim-iting 
the use of all other communication channels. The 
implementation of CILT in patients with aphasia con-sisted 
of intensive practice through a series of language 
games requiring verbal production of phrases and sen-tences 
of increasing length that were shaped into succes-sively 
better approximations.Results comparing language 
improvements after 2 weeks of CILT to approximately 
4 weeks of traditional therapy indicated significant im-provements 
in the CI group on several standard clinical 
tests, self-ratings, and blinded-observer ratings of the 
patients’ communicative effectiveness in everyday life. 
Two principles of CILT need to be considered when 
interpreting the findings. The first principle, forced use 
of verbal language, requires that all responses provided 
during language treatment activities be constrained or 
restricted to the spoken modality. Nonverbal communi-cation 
attempts such as gesturing, drawing, or writing 
are not allowed and are not considered as communica-tion 
intents because participants are on opposite sides of 
a barrier. The second principle, massed practice, involves 
a high-intensity treatment schedule consisting of 3–4 hr 
of treatment per day for 2 weeks. Participants in the 
Pulvermuller et al. (2001) study were randomized to a 
CILT group and a comparison group that received con-ventional 
speech treatment provided on a distributed 
schedule over 3–5 weeks. That is, the groups differed in 
two dimensions of treatment: the type of treatment ad-ministered 
and the intensity of the treatment provided. 
Thus, it is difficult to determine whether the positive 
treatment results reported for the CILT group as com-pared 
to the conventional treatment group emanate from 
the constrained forced language use, the intensity of the 
treatment schedule, or a combination of these two factors. 
Intensity of treatment has been a topic of interest for 
some time in aphasia treatment studies (Poeck, Huber, 
& Willmes, 1989). Findings from Robey’s (1998) meta-analysis 
of the aphasia literature reported large effect 
sizes (ESs) associated with treatment provided for 2 or 
more hr per week. In a review focused on intensity and 
outcomes, Bhogal, Teasell, and Speechley (2003) reported 
better treatment outcomes in studies that provided in-tensive 
treatment schedules. On average, the more in-tensive 
treatment schedules equaled 8.8 hr per week for 
11 weeks, compared with the less intensive schedules of 
2 hr per week for 23 weeks of treatment. Although both 
reviews examined the intensity literature, the schedule 
of treatment provided in the cited studies is far less than 
the intensity of services given in CILT. 
This review examines the current state of the evi-dence 
employing the dual principles of CILT for individ-uals 
with stroke-induced aphasia. In addition to analyzing 
and synthesizing the scientific research in this area, the 
authors piloted the use of a newly created levels-of-evidence 
scheme developed by the American Speech- 
Language-Hearing Association (ASHA) National Center 
for Evidence-Based Practice in Communication Disor-ders 
(N-CEP), along with its advisory committee. This 
scheme classifies treatment studies according to several 
quality indicators and the phase of research (Robey, 
2004). We piloted this system to determine its applica-bility 
to treatment studies in the area of communication 
sciences and disorders and provided feedback to N-CEP 
and its advisory committee prior to the widespread 
adoption of this system. 
Clinical Questions 
Ten clinical questions were identified by the authors 
for review (see Table 1). In constructing these questions, 
Cherney et al.: EBSR: Effects of Intensity of Treatment and CILT 1283
the nature of outcome measurements and stage of re-covery 
from aphasia were considered. In keeping with 
current ASHA policy documents, such as the Scope of 
Practice in Speech-Language Pathology (ASHA, 2001), 
the World Health Organization’s (2001) International 
Classification of Functioning, Disability and Health: ICF 
was selected to categorize the various outcome measures 
representing language impairment (e.g., standard apha-sia 
tests of phonology, syntax, and semantics) or commu-nication 
activity/participation (e.g., functional real-life 
use of language in connected speech measures or com-munication 
ratings scales). Because the stage of recovery 
from stroke-induced aphasia—acute or chronic—influences 
recovery patterns (Robey, 1998) and studies typically 
include participants in only one of these stages, the clin-ical 
questions also included participants with chronic 
and acute aphasia (distinguished by mean time post 
onset [TPO]≤ 3months). Therefore, eight clinical ques-tions 
reflect combinations of the following parameters: 
(a) CILT principles—response constraint and treatment 
intensity; (b) aphasia chronicity—acute and chronic; and 
(c) outcome measures identified from the ICF—language 
impairment and communication activity/participation. 
Two additional questions were constructed to address 
maintenance of treatment behavior in chronic aphasia 
for a total of 10 clinical questions (see Table 1). 
Method 
A systematic search of the aphasia literature was 
conducted to identify studies that directly investigated 
CILT as the specific intervention for aphasia or directly 
compared conditions of higher and lower intensity treat-ment 
for aphasia. Fifteen electronic databases were 
searched: PubMed, CINAHL, PsycINFO, PsycArticles, 
CSA Linguistics and Language Behaviour Abstracts, 
Combined Health Information Database, Health Source: 
Nursing, Science Citation Index, ScienceDirect, NeLH, 
REHABDATA, Social ScienceCitation Index, SUMSearch, 
TRIP Database, and Cochrane Database of Systematic 
Reviews. Additional searches were also performed on all 
ASHA journals, the University of Pittsburgh Clinical 
AphasiologyConference Proceedings,National Institutes 
of Health (NIH) abstracts, and Google Scholar. In addi-tion, 
references from all relevant articles were examined 
to identify other applicable studies. A total of 25 ex-panded 
search terms included those related to stroke-induced 
aphasia, amount and intensity of treatment, and 
CILT. Studies were initially considered for the review if 
theywere published in a peer-reviewed journal from1990 
to 20061, were written in English, and contained original 
data addressing 1 ormore of the 10 clinical questions. For 
studies addressing a clinical question related to intensity, 
the keyword intensity or amount of treatment had to be 
included in the abstract or the title of the article. In ad-dition, 
studies were limited to those that included adults 
18 years of age or older with stroke-induced aphasia. 
Studies that included individuals with underlying cog-nitive 
deficits or any other primary medical diagnoses 
were excluded. Studies that used pharmacological inter-vention 
for aphasia as the comparison treatment were 
also excluded. 
The initial search schematicized in Figure 1 yielded 
441 citations. Two authors (TF and TS), blind to one an-other’s 
results, reviewed each abstract and identified 
36 abstracts as meeting the initial inclusion criteria with 
91% agreement. Of those preliminarily accepted, 26 were 
Table 1. Clinical questions. 
Question No. Clinical Question—Intensity 
1. For stroke-induced chronic aphasia, what is the influence of treatment intensity on measures of language impairment? 
2. For stroke-induced chronic aphasia, what is the influence of treatment intensity on measures of communication activity/participation? 
3. For stroke-induced acute aphasia, what is the influence of treatment intensity on measures of language impairment? 
4. For stroke-induced acute aphasia, what is the influence of treatment intensity on measures of communication activity/participation? 
5. For stroke-induced chronic aphasia, what treatment outcomes are maintained following intensive language treatment? 
Clinical Question—CILT 
6. For stroke-induced chronic aphasia, what is the influence of constraint-induced language therapy on measures of language impairment? 
7. For stroke-induced chronic aphasia, what is the influence of constraint-induced language therapy on measures of communication 
activity/participation? 
8. For stroke-induced acute aphasia, what is the influence of constraint-induced language therapy on measures of language impairment? 
9. For stroke-induced acute aphasia, what is the influence of constraint-induced language therapy on measures of communication 
activity/participation? 
10. For stroke-induced chronic aphasia, what treatment outcomes are maintained following constraint-induced language therapy? 
1The original purpose of the systematic review was to examine the effects 
of CILT. It became apparent that it would be impossible to adequately 
complete this review independent of addressing treatment intensity, which 
is a key principle of CILT. The search date was determined based on the 
emergence of CI literature in the early 1990’s. 
1284 Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research • Vol. 51 • 1282–1299 • October 2008
rejected upon review of the full text by the author panel, 
as they did not report original data or include direct evi-dence 
of a contrast between conditions (e.g., a more in-tensive 
condition vs. a less intensive condition) pertaining 
to one or more of the clinical questions. Ten studies met 
the final inclusion. 
Each of these studies was independently reviewed 
and evaluated for methodological quality by two of three 
authors (LC, JP, AR). One study written by an author 
(AR) was reviewed by two other panel authors (LC, JP). 
The authors, blind to one another’s results, appraised 
each study on the basis of quality indicators described in 
Table 2. For each indicator, quality markers are ordered 
from highest to lowest level of quality. A study received 
1 point for each quality indicator if the highest level of 
quality was incorporated. All nine indicators were rel-evant 
to studies incorporating controlled trials, leading 
to amaximum quality score of 9. For other study designs, 
where intent-to-treat analysis was not relevant, the 
maximum quality score was 8. Credit for study design 
varied by stage of research (see Table 3). For example, 
efficacy studies using controlled trial designs received 
1 point. Discovery studies received 1 point for research 
design if reporting retrospective case controls or single-participant 
studies. Reliability of scores was assessed, 
and any discrepancies in scoring were sent back to the 
full author panel for resolution. All disagreementswere 
resolved via consensus. 
In addition to assessing methodological quality, the 
phase of clinical research was determined using the 
decision tree depicted in Figure 2. The four stages of 
research—discovery, efficacy, effectiveness, and cost– 
benefit/public policy research—are defined in Table 3. 
A final synthesis of the body of scientific literature was 
compiled into an evidence table on the basis of the study 
qualitymarker score and corresponding stage of research. 
ESs were calculated for outcomemeasureswhenever 
possible. For group studies, Cohen’s d (Cohen, 1988) was 
calculated (corrected for bias following Hedges & Olkin, 
1985). ESs were calculated from group means and stan-dard 
deviations or were estimated from results of anal-yses 
of variance (ANOVAs) or t tests. We used Cohen’s 
benchmarks for small, medium, and large ESs as .2, .5, 
and .8, respectively. Whether these benchmarks are ap-plicable 
to aphasia treatment studies has yet to be de-termined. 
In the one single-participant experiment 
(Raymer, Kohen,&Saffell, 2006), the authors calculated 
theweighted ESs using the methods described by Beeson 
and Robey (2006). Although Busk and Serlin (1992) 
reported that large ESs in single-participant studies sur-pass 
2.5, Beeson and Robey noted benchmarks for small, 
medium, and large ESs in single-participant designs as 
2.6, 3.9, and 5.8. 
Results 
Of the 10 studies that met inclusion criteria for the 
systematic review, 5 studies investigated treatment in-tensity 
(Basso & Caporali, 2001; Denes, Perazzolo, Piani, 
& Piccione, 1996; Hinckley & Carr, 2005; Hinckley & 
Craig, 1998; Raymer et al., 2006), 4 studies evaluated 
Figure 1. Process for identification of included studies. 
Cherney et al.: EBSR: Effects of Intensity of Treatment and CILT 1285
CILT (Maher et al, 2006; Meinzer et al, 2004; Meinzer, 
Djundja,Barthel,Elbert,&Rockstroth, 2005; Pulvermuller, 
Hauk, Zohsel, Neininger, & Mohr, 2005), and 1 study ex-amined 
both (Pulvermuller et al., 2001). These studies 
allowed us to address 7 of the 10 clinical questions (see 
Table 1): Questions 1, 2, 5, 6, 7, and 10, which are related 
to language impairment and communication activity/ 
participation in individuals with chronic aphasia, and 
Question 3, which is related to the effects of treatment 
intensity on measures of language impairment for in-dividuals 
with acute aphasia. No study investigated 
CILT or intensive treatment effects for measures of 
Table 2. Quality indicators in the ASHA levels-of-evidence scheme. 
Indicator Description Quality marker 
Study design The type of design used in the study. Controlled trial (Efficacy research)* 
Retrospective case control (Effectiveness or 
cost–benefit research)* 
Single participant study (Discovery research)* 
Case series 
Case study 
Blinding The practice of keeping investigators or participants ignorant to 
the group to which participants are assigned. For the purposes 
of the critical appraisal, blinding refers to assessors only. 
Assessors blinded* 
Assessors not blinded or not stated 
Sampling The method(s) used to choose and assign participants to the 
experimental conditions in the study. 
Random sample adequately described* 
Random sample inadequately described 
Convenience sample adequately described 
Convenience sample inadequately described or 
hand-picked sample or not stated 
Group/participant 
comparability 
How similar the participants/groups were at the start of the 
study or how adequately they were described. 
Groups/participants comparable at baseline 
on important factors (between-subject design) 
or participant(s) adequately described 
(within-subject design)* 
Groups/participants not comparable at baseline 
or comparability not reported or participant(s) 
not adequately described 
Treatment fidelity The procedure used to ensure that the treatment 
was delivered as intended. 
Evidence of treatment fidelity* 
No evidence of treatment fidelity 
Outcomes The measure(s) used in the study to quantify improvement. At least one primary outcome measure is 
valid and reliable* 
Validity unknown, but appears reasonable; 
measure is reliable 
Invalid and/or unreliable 
Significance The likelihood that the study findings occurred by chance. p value reported or calculable* 
p value neither reported nor calculable 
Precision The size or magnitude of any difference found between the 
treatment under investigation and the control condition. 
Effect size and confidence interval 
reported or calculable* 
Effect size or confidence interval, but not 
both, reported or calculable 
Neither effect size nor confidence 
interval reported or calculable 
Intent-to-treat 
(controlled trials only) 
Participants are analyzed according to the group to which they were 
initially assigned, regardless of whether or not they dropped out, 
fully complied with the treatment, or crossed over and received 
the other treatment. If number of participants at pre-treatment = 
number at post-treatment, study received credit. 
Analyzed by intent-to-treat* 
Not analyzed by intent-to-treat* 
*Highest level of quality needed to receive 1 point. 
1286 Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research • Vol. 51 • 1282–1299 • October 2008
Table 3. Continuum of research stages. 
Stage Description 
Discovery Treatment approaches are developed and assessed in the context of whether 
communication activity/participation in acute aphasia 
(covered in Questions 4, 8, and 9). 
Participants 
Table 4 provides a detailed description of the 141 par-ticipants 
in the 10 studies (e.g., age, education level, 
gender, handedness, TPO, aphasia type, and severity). 
The total sample size (N) of participants reported in the 
studies does not account for the possible overlap of par-ticipants 
across four studies: Pulvermuller et al. (2001 
and 2005) and Meinzer et al. (2004 and 2005). 
For the intensity studies, type of aphasia was pro-vided 
in five studies; 81% (55 of 68) of the participants 
were nonfluent. Severity of aphasia was provided in four 
studies; 40% (23 of 57) of participants tended to have 
more severe aphasia, with 30% (19 of 63) of participants 
being described as having global aphasia. The mean TPO 
was considered acute in one study (3.1 months in Denes 
et al., 1996); the other studies included participants with 
chronic aphasia, ranging from 11.2 to 67.6 months postonset. 
Similarly for the CILT studies, 60% of participants 
(42 of 70) had nonfluent forms of aphasia, typically de-scribed 
as Broca’s aphasia. Most had moderate aphasia 
(48%, 30 of 62), whereas 19 participants (31%) had mild 
aphasia and 12 participants (19%) had severe aphasia. 
Mean TPO ranged from 36.8 months to 90 months, all in 
the chronic range. 
Four of the 10 studies were conducted in Germany, 
4 in the United States (U.S.), and 2 in Italy. Two German 
studies (Pulvermuller et al., 2001, 2005) included mono-lingual 
native speakers of German. The other two Ger-man 
studies (Meinzer et al., 2004, 2005) did not mention 
linguality of the participants. Two U.S. studies (Hinckley 
& Carr, 2005; Hinckley & Craig, 1998) noted that all par-ticipants 
were native speakers of English, whereas two 
Figure 2. Stage of research. 
they show promise of being efficacious. 
Efficacy Promising interventions are tested in a rigorous way under ideal, highly 
controlled conditions to determine the outcome that results. 
Effectiveness The intervention is tested in a “real-world” clinical setting. This phase 
is often conducted if the intervention demonstrates positive outcomes in a 
highly controlled setting of a clinical trial. 
Cost–benefit/public policy A study is conducted of the political and economic environment in which the 
intervention is best delivered. This phase is often conducted once it has 
been shown that an intervention is both efficacious and effective. 
Cherney et al.: EBSR: Effects of Intensity of Treatment and CILT 1287
Table 4. Patient characteristics. 
Study N Age Education Gender Handedness TPO Etiology Aphasia type 
Aphasia 
severity 
Aphasia 
severity 
rating 
Basso & Caporali, 
2001 (Intensity) 
6 35–48 yrs 8–17 yrs 4 M R 2–22 months 2 hemorrhagic 2 mixed nonfluent NR Test scores 
M = 41 yrs M = 12.5 yrs 2 F M = 11.2 months 4 ischemic 2 global 
2 nonfluent agrammatic 
Denes et al., 
1996 (Intensity) 
17 60.2 yrs 7.2 yrs 8 M R 2.4–4.5 months 4 hemorrhagic global Severe Test scores 
9 F M = 3.1 months 13 ischemic 
Hinckley & Craig, 
1998, Study 3 
(Intensity) 
10 25–78 yrs 12+ yrs 6 M 9 R 7–47 months Thromboembolic 8 nonfluent 1 severe Subjective 
M = 48 yrs 4 F 1 L M = 20 months 2 fluent 7 moderate 
2 mild 
Hinckley & Carr, 
2005 (Intensity) 
13 19–72 yrs NR NR R 6–99 months L CVA 12 Broca BDAE severity 
score 1–3 
Test scores 
M = 50 yrs M = 27 months intensive 1 transcortical motor 
M = 40 months nonintensive 
Maher et al., 2006 (CILT) 9 41–73 yrs 12–16+ yrs 6 M 8 R 14–72 months L CVA NR 4 severe Test scores 
M = 58 yrs 3 F 1 R+ M = 36.8 months 1 moderate 
3 mild 
1 NS 
Meinzer et al., 2004 (CILT) 28 35–80 yrs NR 14 M R 12–156 months 20 ischemic 13 Broca NR NR 
M = 54.6 yrs 14 F M = 43.8 months 8 hemorrhagic 4 Wernicke 
3 global 
6 not classified 
2 amnesic 
Meinzer et al., 2005 (CILT) 27 18–80 yrs NR 16 M NR 13–116 months 11 hemorrhagic 10 Broca 2 severe Test scores 
M = 51.5 yrs 11 F M = 43.1 months 16 ischemic 8 Wernicke 15 moderate 
3 amnesic 10 mild 
1 global 
5 not classified 
Pulvermuller et al., 2001 
(CILT and Intensity) 
17 42–72 yrs 9–13 years 12 M 14 R 2–223 months L CVA 10 Broca 4 severe Test scores 
M = 54.9 yrs M = 11 yrs 5 F 3 R+ M = 67.6 months 4 Wernicke 9 moderate 
1 amnesic 4 mild 
1 transcortical 
1 conduction 
Pulvermuller et al., 
2005 (CILT) 
9 39–72 yrs 10–13 yrs 6 M 7 R 16–223 months L CVA 5 Broca 2 severe Test scores 
M = 54.4 yrs M = 11.3 yrs 3 F 2 R+ M = 90 months 2 Wernicke 2 moderate 
1 amnesic 2 mild 
1 transcortical 
Raymer et al., 
2006 (Intensity) 
5 51–82 yrs 8–14 yrs 2 M 5 R 4–42 months L CVA 2 Broca NR Test scores 
M = 70.8 yrs M = 11.4 yrs 5 F M = 18.4 months 2 conduction 
1 mixed transcortical 
Note. TPO = time postonset; yrs = years; NR = not reported; R+ = ambidextrous; L CVA = left cerebrovascular accident; NS = not severe; BDAE = Boston Diagnostic Aphasia Examination (Goodglass & 
Kaplan, 1983). 
1288 Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research • Vol. 51 • 1282–1299 • October 2008
other U.S. studies (Maher et al., 2006; Raymer et al., 
2006) did not address linguality.One Italian study (Denes 
et al., 1996) included only native speakers of Italian. The 
second Italian study (Basso & Caporali, 2001) included 
5 participants who were native speakers of Italian and 1 
who was a native of Sweden and completely bilingual. 
The educational level of the participants was reported 
in only 6 of the 10 studies. The mean years of educa-tion 
varied from 10.9 years (Pulvermuller et al., 2001) to 
15 years (Maher et al., 2006). One study (Hinckley & 
Craig, 1998) reported that all participants completed 
high school or above, and the Basso and Caporali (2001) 
study reported educational attainment on only 4 of 6 par-ticipants. 
Only one study (Hinckley&Carr, 2005) reported 
mean data on socioeconomic status. Those participants 
had a mean score of 2.3 on the Hollingshead Four Factor 
Index of Social Status (Hollingshead, 1975). 
Study Characteristics and Quality 
Table 5 summarizes information regarding the in-tervention, 
including schedule, amount and duration of 
treatment, and outcomemeasures.Most studies provided 
24–30 hr of treatment; several studies reported more 
than 100 hr of treatment (e.g., Hinckley & Craig, 1998). 
Treatment schedules varied across studies, as did the 
nature of treatment provided. 
Table 6 displays quality marker ratings obtained for 
the studies. All 10 studies described participants suf-ficiently 
and reported data in a manner in which statis-tical 
significance was available. Studies were lacking in 
several areas, however, including use of a clearly described 
randomized assignment scheme for participants in group 
designs, provided in only one study; blinding of assessors 
to treatment conditions, indicated in only two studies; and 
providing evidence of treatment fidelity, also noted in 
only two studies. 
Four studies were judged as discovery phase re-search 
and five studies as efficacy research. Only one 
study fell into the effectiveness stage of research, and 
none addressed cost–benefit or public policy. 
Intensity Results 
Table 7 displays stage of research, quality scores, 
and ESs for the six studies examining treatment inten-sity. 
These studies addressed Questions 1, 2, 3, and 5 
(see Table 1). No data were available to answer Ques-tion 
4. Five of these studies contained sufficient data 
for calculation of treatment ESs. The effect of intensity 
in Denes et al. (1996), Pulvermuller et al. (2001), and 
Hinckley andCarr (2005) was derived from between-group 
comparisons for groups receiving intensive and non-intensive 
treatment. The effect of intensity in Study 3 of 
Hinckley andCraig (1998)was derived fromwithin-group 
comparisons of the pre- and post difference scores from 
each intensive 6-week training session compared with the 
nonintensive 6-week training session. In Raymer et al. 
(2006), the effects came from within-subject comparisons 
across the individual participants. 
Clinical Question 1: For stroke-induced chronic apha-sia, 
what is the influence of treatment intensity on mea-sures 
of language impairment? Four group studies used 
impairment outcome measures for which eight ESs were 
calculable, including seven large ESs, all in favor of more 
intensive treatment. In the single-participant design of 
Raymer et al. (2006), ESs were larger in the more inten-sive 
condition for picture-naming acquisition and larger 
in the less intensive condition for word/picture verifica-tion. 
ESs could not be calculated for Basso and Caporali 
(2001), who described case studies of three pairs of indi-viduals. 
Individuals receiving more intensive treatment 
showed greater gains on language impairment tasks than 
did the comparison individualswho received a less intensive 
schedule. Thus, the language impairment outcome mea-sures 
favored more intensive treatment for all language 
measures except one in participants with chronic aphasia. 
Clinical Question 2: For stroke-induced chronic apha-sia, 
what is the influence of treatment intensity on mea-sures 
of communication activity/participation? Three 
group studies used communication measures for which 
nine ESs were calculable, including content unit (CU) 
analysis, oral and written responses on a catalogue order-ing 
task administered in quiet and concurrent noise con-ditions, 
and the CommunicationActivities of Daily Living 
(CADL–2; Holland,Frattali,&Fromm, 1999). ESs ranged 
from –1.15 to 3.78,with five favoringmore intensive treat-ment 
(four large effects) and four favoring less intensive 
treatment (two large effects). 
Basso and Caporali (2001) also contributed data to 
this clinical question, as they described greater changes 
in participants’ conversational abilities in all persons re-ceiving 
more intensive treatment as compared with the 
counterpart receiving less intensive treatment. Overall, 
findings for outcome measures of communication activity/ 
participation were mixed, with some favoring less in-tensive 
treatment and others favoring more intensive 
treatment. 
Clinical Question 3: For stroke-induced acute aphasia, 
what is the influence of treatment intensity on measures of 
language impairment? In one study of 17 individuals, 
(Denes et al., 1996), the ESs for the Aachen Aphasia Test 
(AAT; Huber, Poeck, & Williams, 1984) ranged from 0.39 
(repetition tasks) to 1.20 (written language tasks), all fa-voring 
more intensive treatment. 
Clinical Question 5: For stroke-induced chronic apha-sia, 
what treatment outcomes are maintained following 
intensive language treatment? In the single-participant 
design of Raymer et al. (2006), ESs for picture-naming 
Cherney et al.: EBSR: Effects of Intensity of Treatment and CILT 1289
Table 5. Intervention variables. 
Study Treatment program Treatment schedule Total amount Duration 
Impairment 
measure 
Activity/participation 
measure 
Basso & Caporali, 
2001 (Intensity) 
All participants—individual treatment; conventional 
aphasia treatment; varied techniques. 
More intensive 
treatment 
NR All participants TT Picture description 
(no analysis) 
Goal areas = auditory comprehension, repetition, 
naming, and conversation. 
3 participants 5 hr/wk; 
Home program 
14–40 months Raven’s 
Matrices 
Treatment program—more intensive = clinic + home 
program; less intensive = clinic. 
2–3 hr/day 
Less intensive 
treatment 
3 participants; 
5 hr/wk 
Denes et al., 1996 
(Intensity) 
All participants—individual treatment; ecological, 
based on stimulation approach to restore efficient use 
of language mainly in conversation. 
All participants Intensive treatment 
group 
All participants AAT profile None 
Goal areas = auditory comprehension and verbal 
production. 
45- to 60-min sessions 
M = 130 sessions, 
SD = 36, 
range = 94–160 
M = 6 months, 
range = 5.2–7.0 
TT 
Technique = picture identification and conversation context. 
Intensive treatment 
group 
Regular treatment 
group 
Repetition 
6–7 sessions/wk 
M = 60 sessions, 
SD = 14, 
range = 56–70 
Written Language 
Regular treatment 
group 
Naming 
About 3 sessions/wk 
Comprehension 
Hinckley & Craig, 
1998, Study 3 
(Intensity) 
All participants—individual, group and computer treatment; 
functionalist/pragmatic approach. Standard functional 
treatments (i.e., PACE and cueing hierarchy). 
Intensive treatment 
phase 
All participants All participants BNT CU analysis from 
picture description 
23 hr/wk 
294–306 hrs 18 wks 
Nonintensive 
treatment phase 
6 wks intensive 
phase 
3–5 hr/wk 
6 wks nonintensive 
phase 
6 wks intensive 
phase 
Hinckley & Carr, 
2005 (Intensity) 
All participants—context-based treatment that is based 
on whole-task training and ecological validity. 
Intensive treatment 
group 
Total number of 
sessions per 
participant-NR 
NR PALPA CADL 
Tasks = problem-solving catalogue ordering task 
to develop compensatory strategies. 
20 hrs individual 
treatment/wk; 5 hr 
group treatment/wk 
All participants 
Oral Naming Catalogue ordering 
task 
Techniques = role play, strategy development, 
and context-specific cues. Nonintensive 
treatment group 
Minutes to reach 
criterion on 
catalogue ordering task 
Written Naming 
Treatment group—intensive treatment = individual + group; 
nonintensive treatment = individual. 4 hr individual 
treatment/wk 
M = 233, 
range = 29–597 
(1–10 sessions) 
(Continued on the following page) 
1290 Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research • Vol. 51 • 1282–1299 • October 2008
Table 5 Continued. Intervention variables. 
Study Treatment program Treatment schedule Total amount Duration 
Impairment 
measure 
Activity/participation 
measure 
Maher et al., 
2006 (CILT) 
All participants—task = primarily dual card task. All participants All participants All participants WAB Story retelling 
Clinician ratings 
of post-treatment 
narrative 
Technique = shaping and cueing. 3-hr sessions 24 hr 2 wks BNT 
Treatment group—CILT = verbal response required. 4 sessions/wk ANT 
PACE = all communication modalities permitted. 
Meinzer et al., 
2004 (CILT) 
Treatment group—CIAT Verbal response required. Dual 
card task (object drawings) 2–3 participants per group. 
All participants All participants All participants AAT None 
Technique = shaping and cueing. Model-based aphasia 
treatment; deficit-specific approach. 
3 hr/day for 10 days 30 hr 2 wks TT 
NR = individual or group treatment. 
Meinzer et al., 
2005 (CILT) 
Treatment group—CIAT = verbal response required; dual 
card task (object drawings); 2–3 participants per group. 
Technique = shaping. All participants All participants All participants AAT CAL 
CIAT plus = CIAT as above, plus dual card task (object 3 hr/day for 10 days 30 hr 2 wks CETI 
drawings, written words, and photographs); daily 
home program; daily communication practice with 
family members. 
Pulvermuller et al., 
2001 (CILT and 
Intensity) 
Treatment group CIAT group CIAT group CIAT group TT CAL 
CIAT = verbal response required; dual card task 
3 hr/day for 10 days M = 31.5 hr, 
2 weeks AAT 
(object drawings); 2–3 participants per group. 
range = 23–33 
Technique = Shaping and reinforcement. 
Conventional aphasia treatment—syndrome-specific 
standard approach; naming, repetition, 
sentence completion, and conversation tasks. 
Conventional 
treatment group Conventional 
treatment group 
Conventional 
treatment group 
Session length NR 3–5 wks 
M = 33.9 hr, 
range = 20–54 
3–5 wks 
(Continued on the following page) 
Cherney et al.: EBSR: Effects of Intensity of Treatment and CILT 1291
Table 5 Continued. Intervention variables. 
Study Treatment program Treatment schedule Total amount Duration 
Impairment 
measure 
Activity/participation 
measure 
Pulvermuller et al., 
2005 (CILT) 
All participants—CIAT; verbal response required; dual card 
task (object drawings); 3 participants per group. 
All participants All participants All participants Lexical decision 
Reaction time 
None 
Technique = shaping and reinforcement. 
3 hr/day for 10 days M = 31.3 hr, SD = 3.5, 
range = 23–33 
2 wks 
AAT 
TT 
Repetition 
Naming 
Comprehension 
Raymer et al., 
2006 (Intensity) 
All participants—Moss-talk multimode matching exercises; 
spoken and written word/picture matching. 
All participants All participants NR WAB None 
Alternating treatment 
schedules 
3–5 baseline sessions BNT 
3–4 sessions/wk 
12 training sessions Picture naming 
1–2 sessions/wk 
1-month break Word/picture 
12 training sessions verification 
4 participants = 24 hr 
1 participant = 22 hr 
Note. NR = not reported; AAT = Aachen Aphasia Test (Huber, Poeck, & Weniger, 1984); ANT = Action Naming Test (Nicholas, Obler, Albert, & Goodglass, 1985); BNT = Boston Naming Test (Kaplan, 
Goodglass, & Weintraub, 2001); CADL = Communication Activities of Daily Living (Holland, Frattali, & Fromm, 1999); CAL = Communication Activity Log (Pulvermuller et al., 2001); CETI = Communicative 
Effectiveness Index (Lomas et al., 1989); CU = content unit; PACE = Promoting Aphasics’ Communicative Effectiveness (Davis & Wilcox, 1985); PALPA = Psycholinguistic Assessment of Language Processing in 
Aphasia (Kay, Lesser, & Coltheart, 1992); TT = Token Test (De Renzi & Vignolo, 1962); WAB = Western Aphasia Battery (Kertesz, 1982). 
1292 Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research • Vol. 51 • 1282–1299 • October 2008
maintenance were 7.45 in the more intensive condi-tion 
and 4.85 in the less intensive condition. ESs for a 
word/picture verification task were 1.75 in the more 
intensive condition and 2.14 in the less intensive con-dition. 
Thus, maintenance effects of treatment inten-sity 
were mixed in this study. 
Stage of research and quality. Table 7 also shows the 
number of quality markers for each study and the stage 
of research. Of the two studies in the discovery phase of 
research, one received a quality marker score of 5 out of 
8 (Raymer et al., 2006) and the other received a quality 
marker score of 3 out of 8 (Basso&Caporali, 2001). Three 
studieswere efficacy studies (Denes et al., 1996;Hinckley 
& Carr, 2005; Pulvermuller et al., 2001), each of which 
attained a quality marker score of 6 or 7 out of 9. One 
study (Hinckley & Craig, 1998) fell into the effectiveness 
stage of research, achieving a quality score of 4 out of 8. 
CILT Results 
CILT was examined in five studies as shown in 
Table 8. These studies addressed Questions 6, 7, and 10. 
No studies of CILT in acute aphasia were available, so 
Questions 8 and 9 could not be addressed. Cohen’s d val-ues 
could be calculated for all five CILTstudies. The ESs 
for Meinzer et al. (2004, 2005), and Pulvermuller et al. 
(2005) represent within-subject effects, whereas those in 
Maher et al. (2006) and Pulvermuller et al. (2001) come 
frombetween-group comparisons of CILTwith a contrast-ing 
treatment. 
Clinical Question 6: For stroke-induced chronic apha-sia, 
what is the influence of constraint-induced language 
therapy on measures of language impairment? Across the 
five group studies, 16 ESs were calculable, including 9 
large ESs in favor of CILT. In individuals with chronic 
aphasia, CILT had positive effects for overall aphasia bat-tery 
scores (Maher et al., 2006; Meinzer et al., 2004) and 
subtests of auditory comprehension, word retrieval, repe-tition, 
and lexical decision (Maher et al., 2006; Meinzer 
et al., 2004; Pulvermuller et al., 2005). 
Meinzer et al. (2005) examined a modification of 
CILT (described as CIAT) in which additional home ac-tivities 
were included. There was no difference between 
CILT and CIAT plus on outcome scores immediately 
Table 6. Study quality marker variables. 
Study Design 
Assessor 
blinding 
Random 
sampling 
described 
Participants 
comparable/ 
described 
Treatment 
fidelity reported 
Valid outcome 
measure Significance 
Intent-to- 
treat Precision 
Basso & Caporali, 
2001 (Intensity) 
Case study No No Yes No Yes Yes N/A No 
Denes et al., 1996 
(Intensity) 
Controlled 
trial 
Yes No Yes No Yes Yes Yes Yes 
Hinckley & Craig, 
1998, Study 3 
(Intensity) 
Case series No No Yes No Yes Yes N/A Yes 
Hinckley & Carr, 
2005 (Intensity) 
Controlled 
trial 
No No Yes Yes Yes Yes Yes Yes 
Maher et al., 
2006 (CILT) 
Controlled 
trial 
No No Yes Yes Yes Yes No Yes 
Meinzer et al., 
2004 (CILT) 
Case series No No Yes No Yes Yes N/A Yes 
Meinzer et al., 
2005 (CILT) 
Controlled 
trial 
No No Yes No Yes Yes Yes No 
Pulvermuller et al., 
2001 (CILT 
and Intensity) 
Controlled 
trial 
Yes Yes Yes No Yes Yes No No 
Pulvermuller et al., 
2005 (CILT) 
Case series No No Yes No Yes Yes N/A Yes 
Raymer et al., 
2006 (Intensity) 
Single subject No No Yes No Yes Yes N/A Yes 
Note. N/A = not applicable. 
Cherney et al.: EBSR: Effects of Intensity of Treatment and CILT 1293
following training. Both groups reported significant im-provements. 
For language impairment measures, CILT 
consistently led to positive outcomes. 
Clinical Question 7: For stroke-induced chronic 
aphasia, what is the influence of constraint-induced lan-guage 
therapy on measures of communication activity/ 
participation? Three of the 5 CILT studies included mea-sures 
of communication activity/participation; ESs could 
be calculated for 11 outcome measures incorporated in the 
studies (e.g.,Communicative Activity Log, story retelling, 
Communicative Effectiveness Index; Lomas et al., 1989). 
ESs ranged from –.82 to 3.77, with eight favoringCILT (six 
large effects) and three favoring a comparison treatment 
(one large effect). Thus, CILT had positive outcomes for 
severalmeasures of communication activity/participation, 
although some favored a comparison condition. 
ClinicalQuestion 10: For stroke-induced chronic apha-sia, 
what treatment outcomes are maintained following 
Table 7. Intensity studies, effect sizes, and methodologic quality. 
Study Outcome measure(s) ICF Intensity effect size 
Research 
stage 
Quality 
score 
Clinical 
question 
Basso & Caporali, 2001 AAT TT I Not calculable Discovery 3 of 8 1, 2 
Raven’s matricesa I 
Picture description I 
Denes et al., 1996 AAT: TT I 0.60 Efficacy 7 of 9 3 
Repetition I 0.39 
Written Language I 1.20 
Naming I 0.73 
Comprehension I 0.91 
Profile Level I 0.83 
Hinckley & Craig, 1998, Study 3 Intensive Treatment I vs. Nonintensive Effectiveness 4 of 8 1, 2 
BNT I 1.12 
Content Unit Analysis A/P 0.53 
Intensive Treatment II vs. Nonintensive 
BNT I 0.95 
Content Unit Analysis A/P 0.74 
Hinckley & Carr, 2005 Catalogue ordering Efficacy 7 of 9 1,2 
Oral (quiet) A/P –0.81 
Oral (concurrent) A/P –0.05 
Written (quiet) A/P –0.54 
Written (concurrent) A/P 0.18 
CADL-2 A/P –1.15 
PALPA Oral Naming I 0.16 
PALPA Written Naming I 1.48 
Pulvermuller et al., 2001 AAT: Overall Test Scores I 2.18 Efficacy 6 of 9 1, 2 
TT I 0.92 
Naming I 1.12 
Language Comprehension I 1.12 
Repetition I Not calculable 
Communicative Activity Log: Patients A/P 3.78 
SLPs A/P 2.64 
Raymer et al., 2006 WAB Aphasia Quotient I Not calculable Discovery 5 of 8 1, 5 
BNT I Not calculable 
Picture Naming Acquisition I 4.35 (lw), 11.37 (hw) 
Picture Naming Maintenance I 4.85 (lw), 7.45 (hw) 
Word/Picture Verification Acquisition I 2.72 (lw), 2.14 (hw) 
Word/Picture Verification Maintenance I 2.14 (lw), 1.75 (hw) 
Note. lw = low weighted intensity; hw = high weighted intensity; ICF = International Classification System; I = impairment; A/P = activity/participation; 
SLP = speech-language pathologist. 
aRaven’s Coloured Progressive Matrices (Raven, Court, & Raven, 1986) 
1294 Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research • Vol. 51 • 1282–1299 • October 2008
constraint-induced language therapy? Two studies ad-dressed 
Question 10. Meinzer et al. (2005) reported that 
effects of CILT on measures of language impairment 
and communication activity/participation were main-tained 
at 6months compared with baseline performance. 
Family members of the group receiving CIATplus with 
the modified home program reported further gains in 
communication effectiveness over the 6-month period. 
Maher et al. (2006) reported follow-up testing at 1 month 
post-CILT completion. Three of 4 CILT participants and 
1 of 3 participants in Promoting Aphasics’ Communica-tive 
Effectiveness (PACE) demonstrated continued in-creases 
on aphasia tests over the 1-month period. 
Stage of research and quality. Table 8 indicates that 
two studies that fell into the discovery phase of re-search 
received quality scores of 4 out of 8. Three studies 
represented efficacy research, two of which received a 
qualitymarker score of 6 out of 9 and one ofwhich received 
a score of 5 out of 9. 
Discussion 
The purpose of this EBSR was to assess the in-fluence 
of intensity of language treatment and CILT on 
language and communication outcomes of individuals 
with stroke-induced aphasia. A systematic search of the 
literature from 1990 to 2006 yielded only 10 studies that 
met predetermined inclusion criteria, with 5 studies ad-dressing 
treatment intensity, 4 studies addressing CILT, 
and 1 study addressing both. Although few studies were 
expected to be found for CILT—a technique that was 
Table 8. CILT studies, effect sizes, and methodologic quality. 
Study Outcome measure(s) ICF CILT effect size Research stage Quality score Clinical question 
Maher et al., 2006 Aphasia tests Efficacy 6 of 9 6, 7, 10 
WAB Aphasia Quotient I 1.01 
BNT I –0.16 
Action Naming Test I 0.14 
Linguistic measures in story retelling 
Number of words A/P –0.72 
Number of utterances A/P –0.82 
Number of sentences A/P –0.19 
Mean length of utterance A/P 0.33 
Clinician ratings A/P Not calculable 
Meinzer et al., 2004 AAT: Profile I 0.34 Discovery 4 of 8 6 
Token Test I 0.81 
Meinzer et al., 2005 AAT: Profile I 1.63 Efficacy 5 of 9 6, 7, 10 
Subtests I Not calculable 
CETI: Overall – relatives A/P 1.86 
Communication Activity Log 
Quantity: Patients A/P 1.99 
Relatives A/P 2.35 
Comprehension: Patients A/P .47 
Relatives A/P 1.13 
Pulvermuller et al., 2001 AAT: Overall Test Scores I 2.18 Efficacy 6 of 9 6, 7 
TT I 0.92 
Naming I 1.12 
Language Comprehension I 1.12 
Repetition I Not calculable 
Communicative Activity Log: Patients A/P 3.77 
SLPs A/P 2.64 
Pulvermuller et al., 2005 AAT: TT I 0.25 Discovery 4 of 8 6 
Repetition I 0.11 
Naming I 0.25 
Comprehension I 0.46 
Lexical decision reaction times 
Response time I 2.39 
Pseudowords x time I 3.32 
Words I Not calculable 
Cherney et al.: EBSR: Effects of Intensity of Treatment and CILT 1295
introduced only in 2001—the paucity of studies directly 
addressing treatment intensity was surprising in view of 
the overall large number of studies examining language 
treatment for stroke-induced aphasia. Given the small 
number of studies, the conclusions of the present sys-tematic 
reviewmust be considered preliminary. Further-more, 
interpretation of ESs and their applicability to the 
aphasia literature is not certain at this time. Neverthe-less, 
certain trends in the literature from 1990 to 2006 
may usefully inform both clinical practice and future re-search 
in this area. 
Increased treatment intensity was associated with 
positive changes in outcome measures of language im-pairment 
in 68 persons with chronic and acute aphasia. 
All ESs calculated for group comparisons on language 
impairment measures favored more intensive treatment 
over less intensive treatment, including seven large ef-fect 
sizes. In the single-participant study (Raymer et al., 
2006), ESs for one measure favored more intensive treat-ment 
and one favored less intensive. The data for Clin-ical 
Question 2 addressing change in communication 
activity/participation were mixed, with some favoring 
more intensive treatment and some favoring less inten-sive 
treatment for persons with chronic aphasia. These 
observations suggest that there can be complex interac-tions 
among intensity of treatment schedule, type of 
treatment, and type of outcome measure. 
Maintenance of treatment intensity effects were 
reported in one study of persons with chronic aphasia 
(Raymer et al., 2006). Findings also were equivocal, fa-voring 
more intensive treatment for one outcome mea-sure 
and less intensive treatment for the other. However, 
there was only one data point for each maintenance ob-servation. 
Beeson and Robey (2006) suggest a minimum 
of two data points for calculation of an ES. Therefore, this 
result should be taken with caution. 
Overall, five studies involving 90 participants 
reported that CILT resulted in positive changes on mea-sures 
of language impairment and communication activity/ 
participation in individuals with chronic aphasia, in-cluding 
large ESs for 9 of 16 impairment measures and 
6 of 11 activity/participation measures. Meinzer et al. 
(2005) reported that effects weremaintained for as long 
as 6 months following completion of CILT in 27 patients. 
No data addressed the effect of CILT in patients with 
acute aphasia, however. Finally, it is important to note 
that Maher et al. (2006) also found significant improve-ments 
on language measures for participants in their 
control group, who participated in an intensive version 
of PACE in which use of compensatory modalities is al-lowed. 
This finding implies that intensity is a key com-ponent 
of CILT. 
Interpretation of the results of this EBSR and its 
implications for clinical practice must be considered in 
conjunction with the information about characteristics 
of the participants. For the intensity studies, the major-ity 
of participants had nonfluent forms of aphasia. When 
severity of aphasia was provided, the majority of partic-ipants 
tended to have more severe aphasia, with many 
described as having a global aphasia. Therefore, conclu-sions 
regarding intensity of treatment are most applica-ble 
to individuals with severe nonfluent aphasia. Future 
research needs to address, in particular, the impact of 
treatment intensity for participants presenting with a 
milder aphasia as well as participants who are catego-rized 
as fluent. Similarly for the CILT studies, the ma-jority 
of participants were nonfluent and moderately 
impaired, thereby limiting the generalizability of the re-sults 
in individuals with fluent aphasia and individuals 
with mild and severe aphasia. Chronicity of aphasia is 
another participant characteristic that requires atten-tion 
in future studies. No study of CILT included in-dividuals 
with acute aphasia, and only one treatment 
intensity study addressed the impact of intensity on 
acute aphasia. 
One of the difficulties encountered in this system-atic 
review was that of comparing results across studies 
when there were differences in the outcome measures 
used. Therefore, we attempted to find commonalities 
by categorizing the types of outcome measures accord-ing 
to the WHO (2001) ICF levels of body function (lan-guage 
reception and production impairment) and activity/ 
participation. Overall, all of the studies included at least 
one outcome measure at the language impairment level, 
and most of these measures were standardized, valid as-sessment 
instruments. Although all of the CILT studies 
and four of the six intensity studies included at least 
onemeasure at the communication activity/participation 
level, there were fewer of these measures, they were typ-ically 
individualized, and information on their validity 
and reliability was often lacking. Future studies of CILT 
and clinical practice should consider inclusion of more 
measures at the communication activity/participation 
level as well as studies that address quality of life in 
patients with aphasia. 
The variety of primary and secondary outcome mea-suresmade 
comparisons across studies difficult and com-plicated 
the determination of the validity and reliability 
as they contribute to the quality of the study. Therefore, 
our critical appraisal used a quality marker related to 
outcome measures that represented only a minimal level 
of quality in these areas. All studies achieved this min-imal 
level of quality that related to use of a valid and 
reliable primary outcome measure. 
A number of methodological weaknesses in the 10 
studies included in the EBSR were identified during the 
critical appraisal of the quality of each study. The short-comings 
across the studies related to primarily three 
1296 Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research • Vol. 51 • 1282–1299 • October 2008
quality indicators: random assignment of subjects, as-sessor 
blinding, and evidence of treatment fidelity. In 
most of the studies, regardless of stage of research and 
research design, participants were selected as a conve-nience 
sample. Randomization is critical for future re-search, 
as many evidence-based review organizations 
(e.g., The Cochrane Collaboration) include primarily ran-domized 
trials in their systematic reviews of health care 
interventions. It is also essential that investigators doc-ument 
procedures to ensure fidelity of the actual treat-ment 
techniques and that some measure of procedural 
reliability is provided. Clear documentation of the treat-ment 
technique will facilitate the transference of the tech-nique 
to the clinical setting. 
Examination of the studies in relation to stage of re-search 
clearly shows where the strengths of the current 
research lie and the direction of future research. In-cluded 
studies in the present EBSR were three efficacy 
studies evaluating the impact of intensity on stroke-related 
chronic aphasia and three efficacy studies eval-uating 
the impact of CILT on chronic aphasia. Only one 
intensity study evaluated effectiveness, and no study 
evaluated effectiveness of CILT. No studies in either cat-egory 
assessed policy change or cost effectiveness. There-fore, 
as evidence for efficacy of more intensive treatment 
and CILT continues to grow, future research must ad-dress 
issues of effectiveness and cost effectiveness with 
studies that are designed according to the criteria in-cluded 
in the ASHA levels-of-evidence scheme to ensure 
that the studies are of high quality. 
Regardless of type of treatment, our general con-clusions 
indicate that more treatment is better over a re-stricted 
time interval. No studies have addressed what 
might be the optimum amount and intensity of treat-ment; 
however, it is possible that no single answer exists 
and that optimum dosage of high-intensity treatment 
varies depending on the type of treatment and the char-acteristics 
of the participant’s aphasia, including chro-nicity. 
Future studies, beginning at the discovery phase, 
are needed to address the many confounds inherent in 
resolving the issue of optimum intensity of treatment for 
individuals with stroke-induced aphasia. 
Although this systematic review has highlighted the 
potential efficacy of CILT on the basis of its dual prin-ciples, 
future studies need to be designed with the aim of 
teasing out the impact of constraint and intensity on out-come. 
This task is particularly difficult because high in-tensity 
is such an integral part of CILT. However Maher 
et al. (2006) have demonstrated that it is possible to ad-dress 
this issue methodologically by providing an iden-tical 
schedule of CILTand its control treatment. Although 
results suggest that efficacy is related to the nature of the 
treatment (i.e., constraint) rather than to intensity alone, 
given the small number of participants, results are not 
yet conclusive. More definitive evidence will be derived 
only from (a) conducting randomized studies comparing 
CILT with other treatment procedures given at a simi-lar 
intensity schedule using participants of similar type 
and severity of aphasia and (b) measuring outcomes at 
the levels of language impairment and communication 
activity/participation. Additionally, the optimum inten-sity 
of treatment for CILT has yet to be determined. Fur-ther 
studies comparing constraints at different levels of 
intensity are needed. 
Although the early evidence surrounding the efficacy 
of CILT for individuals with aphasia shows somepromise, 
the effects are similar to other intensive treatments that 
do not employ the use of constraint. Therefore, these re-sults 
should be taken under advisement. Clear and con-vincing 
evidence supporting the effectiveness of CILT 
over other aphasia treatments has not yet been estab-lished, 
and further research comparing aphasia treat-ments 
is warranted. Clinicians should interpret the 
findings from this EBSR conservatively and always in 
conjunction with their patients’ unique characteristics, 
circumstances, and preferences when considering the 
best course of treatment for and with their clients. 
Finally, some observations need to be made following 
this pilot implementation of ASHA’s levels-of-evidence 
scheme. The indicators used to judge the quality of re-search 
studies tend to focus on issues that are most per-tinent 
to group study designs. Therefore, the usefulness 
of the levels-of-evidence scheme in evaluating single-participant 
research designs, which are used commonly 
in the speech-language pathology literature, is in need of 
further development. Although eight of the nine indica-tors 
were applicable to single-participant design research, 
other quality indicators related to the documentation of 
experimental control and threats to external validity (e.g., 
stability of baselinemeasures) that are unique to this type 
of research were not included. These additional issues of 
design quality that arise in single-participant research 
will need to be studied. 
Another notable aspect of the current systematic re-view 
is that all studies addressing the clinical questions, 
including discovery studies, are included. Although 
some systematic reviews include only group designs or 
randomized clinical trials, we elected to include all stud-ies, 
regardless of design. Ultimately, we need to give 
greater weight to well-designed randomized trials. How-ever, 
given the relative paucity of those studies in our 
current literature base, the inclusion of all possible stud-ies 
gives a broader perspective of the state of knowledge. 
It is our hope that this systematic review will incite ad-ditional 
rigorous clinical trials addressing the questions 
of intensity of aphasia treatment and CILT, as well as 
other areas of interest to the fields of speech-language 
pathology and audiology. 
Cherney et al.: EBSR: Effects of Intensity of Treatment and CILT 1297
Acknowledgments 
This EBSR was supported by ASHA’s National Center 
for Evidence-Based Practice in Communication Disorders 
(N-CEP), the Advisory Committee on Evidence-Based Practice 
in Communication Disorders, and ASHA’s Special Interest 
Division 2: Neurophysiology and Neurogenic Speech and 
Language Disorders. We thank the following individuals who 
contributed to the preparation of this article: Rob Mullen, 
N-CEP Director; BeverlyWang, N-CEP Information Manager; 
and Floyd Roye, N-CEP Project Administrator. All members 
of this evidence-based review panel agreed to declare no 
competing interests in relation to this article. No author had 
any paid consultancy or any other conflict of interest with this 
document. The first three authors contributed equally to the 
preparation of this article, and the order does not reflect any 
differences in contribution. 
References 
ASHA. (2001). Scope of practice in speech-language pathology 
[Scope of Practice]. Available from www.asha.org /policy. 
Basso, A. A., & Caporali, A. (2001). Aphasia therapy or the 
importance of being earnest. Aphasiology, 15, 307–332. 
Beeson, P., & Robey, R. (2006). Evaluating single-subject 
treatment research: Lessons learned from the aphasia 
literature. Neuropsychological Review, 16, 161–169. 
Bhogal, S.K., Teasell, R.,&Speechley, M. (2003). Intensity 
of aphasia therapy, impact on recovery. Stroke, 34, 987–993. 
Busk, P. L., & Serlin, R. (1992). Meta-analysis for single case 
research. In T. R. Kratochwill & J. R. Levin (Eds.), Single-case 
research design and analysis: New directions for psy-chology 
and education. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. 
Cohen, J. (1988). Statistical power analysis for the behavioral 
sciences (2nd ed.). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. 
Davis, G. A., & Wilcox, M. (1985). Adult aphasia rehabilita-tion: 
Applied pragmatics. San Diego, CA: College Hill Press. 
Denes, G., Perazzolo, C., Piani, A., & Piccione, F. (1996). 
Intensive versus regular speech therapy in global aphasia: 
A controlled study. Aphasiology, 10, 385–394. 
De Renzi, E., & Vignolo, L. (1962). The Token Test: A 
sensitive test to detect receptive disturbances in aphasics. 
Brain, 85, 665–678. 
Goodglass, H., & Kaplan, E. (1983). The Boston Diagnostic 
Aphasia Examination. Philadelphia: Lea & Febiger. 
Hakkennes, S., & Keating, J. (2005). Constraint-induced 
movement therapy following stroke: A systematic review of 
randomized controlled trials. Australian Journal of Physio-therapy, 
51, 221–231. 
Hedges, L.V.,&Olkin, I. (1985). Statistical methods for meta-analysis. 
Orlando, FL: Academic Press. 
Hinckley, J. J., & Craig, H. K. (1998). Influence of rate of 
treatment on the naming abilities of adults with chronic 
aphasia. Aphasiology, 12, 989–1006. 
Hinckley, J. J.,& Carr, T. (2005). Comparing the outcomes of 
intensive and non-intensive context-based aphasia treat-ment. 
Aphasiology, 19, 965–974. 
Holland, A., Frattali, C., & Fromm, D. (1999). Communica-tion 
activities of daily living (2nd ed.). Austin, TX: Pro-Ed. 
Hollingshead, A. B. (1975). Four Factor Index of Social 
Status. New Haven, CT: Yale University. 
Huber, H. P., Poeck, K., & Williams, K. (1984). The Aachen 
Aphasia Test. In F. C. Rose (Ed.), Progress in aphasiology 
(pp. 291–303). New York: Raven Press. 
Huber, W., Poeck, K., & Weniger, D. (1984). The Aachen 
AphasiaTest. InF. C. Rose (Ed.), Advances in neurology,Vol. 42: 
Progress in aphasiology. New York: Raven Press. 
Kaplan, E., Goodglass, H., & Weintraub, S. (2001). The 
Boston Naming Test. Philadelphia: Lea & Febiger. 
Kay, J., Lesser, R., & Coltheart, M. (1992). Psycholinguistic 
assessments of language processing in aphasia. Hove, United 
Kingdom: Psychology Press. 
Kertesz, A. (1982). Western Aphasia Battery. Orlando, FL: 
Grune & Stratton. 
Kleim, J. A., & Jones, T. A. (2008). Principles of experience-dependent 
neural plasticity: Implications for rehabilitation 
after brain damage. Journal of Speech-Language-Hearing 
Research, 51(Suppl.), S225–S239. 
Lomas, J., Pickard, L., Bester, S., Elbard, H., Finlayson, 
A., & Zoghaib, C. (1989). The Communicative Effective-ness 
Index: Development and psychometric evaluation of 
a functional communication measure for adult aphasia. 
Journal of Speech Hearing Disorders, 54, 113–124. 
Maher, L., Kendall, D., Swearengin, J., Rodriguez, A., 
Leon, S., Pingel, K., Holland, A., & Rothi, L. (2006). 
A pilot study of use-dependent learning in the context of 
constraint induced language therapy. Journal of the Inter-national 
Neuropsychological Society, 12, 843–852. 
Meinzer, M., Djundja, D., Barthel, G., Elbert, T., & 
Rockstroth, B. (2005). Long-term stability of improved 
language functions in chronic aphasia after constraint-induced 
aphasia therapy. Stroke, 36, 1462–1466. 
Meinzer, M., Elbert, C., Wienbruch, C., Djundja, D., 
Barthel, G., & Rockstroh, B. (2004). Intensive language 
training enhances brain plasticity in chronic aphasia. BMC 
Biology, 2, 1–9. 
Nicholas, M., Obler, L., Albert, M., & Goodglass, H. (1985). 
Lexical retrieval in healthy aging. Cortex, 21, 595–606. 
Poek, K., Huber, W., & Willmes, K. (1989). Outcomes of 
intensive language treatment in aphasia. Journal of Speech 
and Hearing Disorders, 54, 471–479. 
Pulvermuller, F. B., Neininger, B., Elbert, T., Mohr, B., 
Rockstroh, B., Koebbel, P.,&Taub, E. (2001). Constraint-induced 
therapy of chronic aphasia after stroke. Stroke, 32, 
1621–1626. 
Pulvermuller, F., Hauk, O., Zohsel, K., Neininger, B., & 
Mohr, B. (2005). Therapy-related reorganization of lan-guage 
in both hemispheres of patients with chronic aphasia. 
Neuroimage, 28, 481–489. 
Raymer, A., Kohen, F., & Saffell, D. (2006). Computerised 
training for impairments for word comprehension and 
retrieval in aphasia. Aphasiology, 20, 257–268. 
Raven, J. C., Court, J. H., & Raven, J. (1986). Raven’s 
Coloured Progressive Matrices. London: H. K. Lewis & Co. 
Robey, R. R. (1998). A meta-analysis of clinical outcomes in 
the treatment of aphasia. Journal of Speech, Language, and 
Hearing Research, 41, 172–187. 
Robey, R. R. (2004). A five-phase model for clinical-outcomes 
research. Journal of Communication Disorders, 5, 401–411. 
1298 Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research • Vol. 51 • 1282–1299 • October 2008
Taub, E. (1977). Movement in nonhuman primates deprived 
of somatosensory feedback. Exercise and Sports Sciences 
Review, 4, 335–374. 
Taub, E., Miller, N., Novack, T., & Cook, E. (1993). Tech-nique 
to improve chronic motor deficit after stroke. Archives 
of Physical Medicine and Rehabilitation, 74, 347–354. 
Taub, E., Uswatte, G., King, D. K., Morris, D. M., Crago, 
J. E., & Chatterjee, A. (2006). A placebo controlled trial of 
constraint-induced movement therapy for upper extremity 
after stroke. Stroke, 37, 1045–1049. 
Taub, E., Uswatte, G., & Piclikiti, R. (1999). Constraint-induced 
movement therapy: A new family of techniques with 
broad application to physical rehabilitation—A clinical 
review. Journal of Rehabilitation Research and Develop-ment, 
36, 237–251. 
Taub, E., & Wolf, S. (1997). Constraint-induced movement 
techniques to facilitate upper extremity use in stroke 
patients. Topics in Stroke Rehabilitation, 3, 38–61. 
Wolf, S., Winstein, C. J., Miller, J., Taub, E., Uswatte, G., 
Morris, D., et al. (2006). Effect of constraint-induced move-ment 
therapy on upper extremity function 3 to 9 months 
after stroke: The EXCITE randomized clinical trial. Jour-nal 
of the American Medical Association, 296, 2095–3104. 
World Health Organization. (2001). International Classifi-cation 
of Functioning, Disability and Health: ICF. Geneva, 
Switzerland: Author. 
Received September 4, 2007 
Accepted February 24, 2008 
DOI: 10.1044/1092-4388(2008/07-0206) 
Contact author: Tobi Frymark, National Center for Evidence- 
Based Practice in Communication Disorders, American 
Speech-Language-Hearing Association, 2200 Research 
Boulevard, Rockville, MD 20850. E-mail: tfrymark@asha.org. 
Cherney et al.: EBSR: Effects of Intensity of Treatment and CILT 1299
Constraint induced language therapy

More Related Content

What's hot

Prophylactic poster 2015
Prophylactic poster 2015Prophylactic poster 2015
Prophylactic poster 2015Sarah Blakeman
 
Christy Starr Denman Poster
Christy Starr Denman PosterChristy Starr Denman Poster
Christy Starr Denman PosterChristy Starr
 
EFFICACY OF FIXED VERSUS REMOVAL RETAINER POST ORTHODONTIC TREATMENT: A COMP...
 EFFICACY OF FIXED VERSUS REMOVAL RETAINER POST ORTHODONTIC TREATMENT: A COMP... EFFICACY OF FIXED VERSUS REMOVAL RETAINER POST ORTHODONTIC TREATMENT: A COMP...
EFFICACY OF FIXED VERSUS REMOVAL RETAINER POST ORTHODONTIC TREATMENT: A COMP...DrHeena tiwari
 
Verbal presentation 05.09.2014
Verbal presentation 05.09.2014Verbal presentation 05.09.2014
Verbal presentation 05.09.2014NurseRaquelMurphy
 
Kinh Nghiệm Giảm Cân Hiệu Quả | Venus Global
Kinh Nghiệm Giảm Cân Hiệu Quả | Venus GlobalKinh Nghiệm Giảm Cân Hiệu Quả | Venus Global
Kinh Nghiệm Giảm Cân Hiệu Quả | Venus GlobalVENUS
 
Clinical practice critical_research_paper essay sample from assignmentsupport...
Clinical practice critical_research_paper essay sample from assignmentsupport...Clinical practice critical_research_paper essay sample from assignmentsupport...
Clinical practice critical_research_paper essay sample from assignmentsupport...https://writeessayuk.com/
 
Perioperative interventions
Perioperative interventionsPerioperative interventions
Perioperative interventionsmrcs89
 
EVALUATION OF EFFICIENCY IN CAST PARTIAL DENTURES VERSUS ACRYLIC PARTIAL DENT...
EVALUATION OF EFFICIENCY IN CAST PARTIAL DENTURES VERSUS ACRYLIC PARTIAL DENT...EVALUATION OF EFFICIENCY IN CAST PARTIAL DENTURES VERSUS ACRYLIC PARTIAL DENT...
EVALUATION OF EFFICIENCY IN CAST PARTIAL DENTURES VERSUS ACRYLIC PARTIAL DENT...DrHeena tiwari
 
Highlights eular hp 2012 berlin john verhoef def
Highlights eular hp 2012 berlin john verhoef defHighlights eular hp 2012 berlin john verhoef def
Highlights eular hp 2012 berlin john verhoef defjennyaboki
 
Quantitative Research Article Critique
Quantitative Research Article CritiqueQuantitative Research Article Critique
Quantitative Research Article CritiqueChelsea Zabala
 
Public involvement in the systematic review process in health and social care...
Public involvement in the systematic review process in health and social care...Public involvement in the systematic review process in health and social care...
Public involvement in the systematic review process in health and social care...HTAi Bilbao 2012
 
Analgesic effect of low level laser therapy
Analgesic effect of low level laser therapyAnalgesic effect of low level laser therapy
Analgesic effect of low level laser therapyTurgut Novruzlu
 
Patient centered-perspective-on-treatment-outcomes-in-chronic-pain
Patient centered-perspective-on-treatment-outcomes-in-chronic-painPatient centered-perspective-on-treatment-outcomes-in-chronic-pain
Patient centered-perspective-on-treatment-outcomes-in-chronic-painPaul Coelho, MD
 
EVIDENCE BASED PRACTICE
EVIDENCE BASED PRACTICEEVIDENCE BASED PRACTICE
EVIDENCE BASED PRACTICEBalkeej Sidhu
 
Pilates and cronic low-back pain: a systematic review
Pilates and cronic low-back pain: a systematic reviewPilates and cronic low-back pain: a systematic review
Pilates and cronic low-back pain: a systematic reviewDra. Welker Fisioterapeuta
 
Barriers to evidenced based practice Priyadarsini John
Barriers to evidenced based practice Priyadarsini JohnBarriers to evidenced based practice Priyadarsini John
Barriers to evidenced based practice Priyadarsini JohnPriya John
 

What's hot (20)

Prophylactic poster 2015
Prophylactic poster 2015Prophylactic poster 2015
Prophylactic poster 2015
 
Christy Starr Denman Poster
Christy Starr Denman PosterChristy Starr Denman Poster
Christy Starr Denman Poster
 
EFFICACY OF FIXED VERSUS REMOVAL RETAINER POST ORTHODONTIC TREATMENT: A COMP...
 EFFICACY OF FIXED VERSUS REMOVAL RETAINER POST ORTHODONTIC TREATMENT: A COMP... EFFICACY OF FIXED VERSUS REMOVAL RETAINER POST ORTHODONTIC TREATMENT: A COMP...
EFFICACY OF FIXED VERSUS REMOVAL RETAINER POST ORTHODONTIC TREATMENT: A COMP...
 
Introduction to EBP
Introduction to EBPIntroduction to EBP
Introduction to EBP
 
Verbal presentation 05.09.2014
Verbal presentation 05.09.2014Verbal presentation 05.09.2014
Verbal presentation 05.09.2014
 
Kinh Nghiệm Giảm Cân Hiệu Quả | Venus Global
Kinh Nghiệm Giảm Cân Hiệu Quả | Venus GlobalKinh Nghiệm Giảm Cân Hiệu Quả | Venus Global
Kinh Nghiệm Giảm Cân Hiệu Quả | Venus Global
 
Problem Based Integrated Teaching of Bronchial Asthma to Second MBBS Students
Problem Based Integrated Teaching of Bronchial Asthma to Second MBBS StudentsProblem Based Integrated Teaching of Bronchial Asthma to Second MBBS Students
Problem Based Integrated Teaching of Bronchial Asthma to Second MBBS Students
 
Clinical practice critical_research_paper essay sample from assignmentsupport...
Clinical practice critical_research_paper essay sample from assignmentsupport...Clinical practice critical_research_paper essay sample from assignmentsupport...
Clinical practice critical_research_paper essay sample from assignmentsupport...
 
Perioperative interventions
Perioperative interventionsPerioperative interventions
Perioperative interventions
 
EVALUATION OF EFFICIENCY IN CAST PARTIAL DENTURES VERSUS ACRYLIC PARTIAL DENT...
EVALUATION OF EFFICIENCY IN CAST PARTIAL DENTURES VERSUS ACRYLIC PARTIAL DENT...EVALUATION OF EFFICIENCY IN CAST PARTIAL DENTURES VERSUS ACRYLIC PARTIAL DENT...
EVALUATION OF EFFICIENCY IN CAST PARTIAL DENTURES VERSUS ACRYLIC PARTIAL DENT...
 
Highlights eular hp 2012 berlin john verhoef def
Highlights eular hp 2012 berlin john verhoef defHighlights eular hp 2012 berlin john verhoef def
Highlights eular hp 2012 berlin john verhoef def
 
Quantitative Research Article Critique
Quantitative Research Article CritiqueQuantitative Research Article Critique
Quantitative Research Article Critique
 
Public involvement in the systematic review process in health and social care...
Public involvement in the systematic review process in health and social care...Public involvement in the systematic review process in health and social care...
Public involvement in the systematic review process in health and social care...
 
Evidences
EvidencesEvidences
Evidences
 
Analgesic effect of low level laser therapy
Analgesic effect of low level laser therapyAnalgesic effect of low level laser therapy
Analgesic effect of low level laser therapy
 
Patient centered-perspective-on-treatment-outcomes-in-chronic-pain
Patient centered-perspective-on-treatment-outcomes-in-chronic-painPatient centered-perspective-on-treatment-outcomes-in-chronic-pain
Patient centered-perspective-on-treatment-outcomes-in-chronic-pain
 
EVIDENCE BASED PRACTICE
EVIDENCE BASED PRACTICEEVIDENCE BASED PRACTICE
EVIDENCE BASED PRACTICE
 
Turner stokes
Turner stokesTurner stokes
Turner stokes
 
Pilates and cronic low-back pain: a systematic review
Pilates and cronic low-back pain: a systematic reviewPilates and cronic low-back pain: a systematic review
Pilates and cronic low-back pain: a systematic review
 
Barriers to evidenced based practice Priyadarsini John
Barriers to evidenced based practice Priyadarsini JohnBarriers to evidenced based practice Priyadarsini John
Barriers to evidenced based practice Priyadarsini John
 

Similar to Constraint induced language therapy

McClelland_M_NU608_819_Qualitative_Paper__Final.pdf.pdf
McClelland_M_NU608_819_Qualitative_Paper__Final.pdf.pdfMcClelland_M_NU608_819_Qualitative_Paper__Final.pdf.pdf
McClelland_M_NU608_819_Qualitative_Paper__Final.pdf.pdfDrog3
 
6410 Application 3 Becoming a Leader in the Translation of Evide.docx
6410 Application 3  Becoming a Leader in the Translation of Evide.docx6410 Application 3  Becoming a Leader in the Translation of Evide.docx
6410 Application 3 Becoming a Leader in the Translation of Evide.docxtroutmanboris
 
RP_ L. SUNEETHA CHEST PHYSIOTHERAPY.pptx
RP_ L. SUNEETHA CHEST PHYSIOTHERAPY.pptxRP_ L. SUNEETHA CHEST PHYSIOTHERAPY.pptx
RP_ L. SUNEETHA CHEST PHYSIOTHERAPY.pptxLankeSuneetha
 
humanastatinarticle
humanastatinarticlehumanastatinarticle
humanastatinarticlenewtonsapple
 
Alt PDFThe Journal of the CanadianChiropractic Associati.docx
Alt PDFThe Journal of the CanadianChiropractic Associati.docxAlt PDFThe Journal of the CanadianChiropractic Associati.docx
Alt PDFThe Journal of the CanadianChiropractic Associati.docxdaniahendric
 
Corticosteroids for sore throat sr ma bmj 2018
Corticosteroids for sore throat sr ma bmj 2018Corticosteroids for sore throat sr ma bmj 2018
Corticosteroids for sore throat sr ma bmj 2018Mayra Serrano
 
Evidence based practice
Evidence based practiceEvidence based practice
Evidence based practicelecturerpt
 
REVIEW Open AccessWhat happens after treatment Asystema.docx
REVIEW Open AccessWhat happens after treatment Asystema.docxREVIEW Open AccessWhat happens after treatment Asystema.docx
REVIEW Open AccessWhat happens after treatment Asystema.docxmichael591
 
PhD thesis Berghout 2010
PhD thesis Berghout 2010PhD thesis Berghout 2010
PhD thesis Berghout 2010Caspar Berghout
 
Evidence based practice
Evidence based practiceEvidence based practice
Evidence based practiceamz741987
 
Evidence based practice
Evidence based practiceEvidence based practice
Evidence based practicezualias
 
Long-Term Effect of Exercise Therapyand Patient Education on.docx
Long-Term Effect of Exercise Therapyand Patient Education on.docxLong-Term Effect of Exercise Therapyand Patient Education on.docx
Long-Term Effect of Exercise Therapyand Patient Education on.docxwkyra78
 
Long-Term Effect of Exercise Therapyand Patient Education on.docx
Long-Term Effect of Exercise Therapyand Patient Education on.docxLong-Term Effect of Exercise Therapyand Patient Education on.docx
Long-Term Effect of Exercise Therapyand Patient Education on.docxcroysierkathey
 
Jiang, Allan_UROP Poster
Jiang, Allan_UROP PosterJiang, Allan_UROP Poster
Jiang, Allan_UROP PosterAllan Jiang
 

Similar to Constraint induced language therapy (20)

1
11
1
 
McClelland_M_NU608_819_Qualitative_Paper__Final.pdf.pdf
McClelland_M_NU608_819_Qualitative_Paper__Final.pdf.pdfMcClelland_M_NU608_819_Qualitative_Paper__Final.pdf.pdf
McClelland_M_NU608_819_Qualitative_Paper__Final.pdf.pdf
 
Chronic low-back pain
Chronic low-back painChronic low-back pain
Chronic low-back pain
 
Intensive Aphasia Program
Intensive Aphasia ProgramIntensive Aphasia Program
Intensive Aphasia Program
 
6410 Application 3 Becoming a Leader in the Translation of Evide.docx
6410 Application 3  Becoming a Leader in the Translation of Evide.docx6410 Application 3  Becoming a Leader in the Translation of Evide.docx
6410 Application 3 Becoming a Leader in the Translation of Evide.docx
 
RP_ L. SUNEETHA CHEST PHYSIOTHERAPY.pptx
RP_ L. SUNEETHA CHEST PHYSIOTHERAPY.pptxRP_ L. SUNEETHA CHEST PHYSIOTHERAPY.pptx
RP_ L. SUNEETHA CHEST PHYSIOTHERAPY.pptx
 
humanastatinarticle
humanastatinarticlehumanastatinarticle
humanastatinarticle
 
Alt PDFThe Journal of the CanadianChiropractic Associati.docx
Alt PDFThe Journal of the CanadianChiropractic Associati.docxAlt PDFThe Journal of the CanadianChiropractic Associati.docx
Alt PDFThe Journal of the CanadianChiropractic Associati.docx
 
Corticosteroids for sore throat sr ma bmj 2018
Corticosteroids for sore throat sr ma bmj 2018Corticosteroids for sore throat sr ma bmj 2018
Corticosteroids for sore throat sr ma bmj 2018
 
Evidence based practice
Evidence based practiceEvidence based practice
Evidence based practice
 
REVIEW Open AccessWhat happens after treatment Asystema.docx
REVIEW Open AccessWhat happens after treatment Asystema.docxREVIEW Open AccessWhat happens after treatment Asystema.docx
REVIEW Open AccessWhat happens after treatment Asystema.docx
 
PhD thesis Berghout 2010
PhD thesis Berghout 2010PhD thesis Berghout 2010
PhD thesis Berghout 2010
 
Evidence based practice
Evidence based practiceEvidence based practice
Evidence based practice
 
Evidence based practice
Evidence based practiceEvidence based practice
Evidence based practice
 
Exercícios controle motor e lombalgia
Exercícios controle motor e lombalgiaExercícios controle motor e lombalgia
Exercícios controle motor e lombalgia
 
Evidence based Practice in Emergency Medicine
Evidence based Practice in Emergency Medicine Evidence based Practice in Emergency Medicine
Evidence based Practice in Emergency Medicine
 
Long-Term Effect of Exercise Therapyand Patient Education on.docx
Long-Term Effect of Exercise Therapyand Patient Education on.docxLong-Term Effect of Exercise Therapyand Patient Education on.docx
Long-Term Effect of Exercise Therapyand Patient Education on.docx
 
Long-Term Effect of Exercise Therapyand Patient Education on.docx
Long-Term Effect of Exercise Therapyand Patient Education on.docxLong-Term Effect of Exercise Therapyand Patient Education on.docx
Long-Term Effect of Exercise Therapyand Patient Education on.docx
 
Tim McNamara
Tim McNamara   Tim McNamara
Tim McNamara
 
Jiang, Allan_UROP Poster
Jiang, Allan_UROP PosterJiang, Allan_UROP Poster
Jiang, Allan_UROP Poster
 

Recently uploaded

Activity 01 - Artificial Culture (1).pdf
Activity 01 - Artificial Culture (1).pdfActivity 01 - Artificial Culture (1).pdf
Activity 01 - Artificial Culture (1).pdfciinovamais
 
Beyond the EU: DORA and NIS 2 Directive's Global Impact
Beyond the EU: DORA and NIS 2 Directive's Global ImpactBeyond the EU: DORA and NIS 2 Directive's Global Impact
Beyond the EU: DORA and NIS 2 Directive's Global ImpactPECB
 
IGNOU MSCCFT and PGDCFT Exam Question Pattern: MCFT003 Counselling and Family...
IGNOU MSCCFT and PGDCFT Exam Question Pattern: MCFT003 Counselling and Family...IGNOU MSCCFT and PGDCFT Exam Question Pattern: MCFT003 Counselling and Family...
IGNOU MSCCFT and PGDCFT Exam Question Pattern: MCFT003 Counselling and Family...PsychoTech Services
 
SOCIAL AND HISTORICAL CONTEXT - LFTVD.pptx
SOCIAL AND HISTORICAL CONTEXT - LFTVD.pptxSOCIAL AND HISTORICAL CONTEXT - LFTVD.pptx
SOCIAL AND HISTORICAL CONTEXT - LFTVD.pptxiammrhaywood
 
1029-Danh muc Sach Giao Khoa khoi 6.pdf
1029-Danh muc Sach Giao Khoa khoi  6.pdf1029-Danh muc Sach Giao Khoa khoi  6.pdf
1029-Danh muc Sach Giao Khoa khoi 6.pdfQucHHunhnh
 
Holdier Curriculum Vitae (April 2024).pdf
Holdier Curriculum Vitae (April 2024).pdfHoldier Curriculum Vitae (April 2024).pdf
Holdier Curriculum Vitae (April 2024).pdfagholdier
 
Sanyam Choudhary Chemistry practical.pdf
Sanyam Choudhary Chemistry practical.pdfSanyam Choudhary Chemistry practical.pdf
Sanyam Choudhary Chemistry practical.pdfsanyamsingh5019
 
Paris 2024 Olympic Geographies - an activity
Paris 2024 Olympic Geographies - an activityParis 2024 Olympic Geographies - an activity
Paris 2024 Olympic Geographies - an activityGeoBlogs
 
Class 11th Physics NEET formula sheet pdf
Class 11th Physics NEET formula sheet pdfClass 11th Physics NEET formula sheet pdf
Class 11th Physics NEET formula sheet pdfAyushMahapatra5
 
Call Girls in Dwarka Mor Delhi Contact Us 9654467111
Call Girls in Dwarka Mor Delhi Contact Us 9654467111Call Girls in Dwarka Mor Delhi Contact Us 9654467111
Call Girls in Dwarka Mor Delhi Contact Us 9654467111Sapana Sha
 
9548086042 for call girls in Indira Nagar with room service
9548086042  for call girls in Indira Nagar  with room service9548086042  for call girls in Indira Nagar  with room service
9548086042 for call girls in Indira Nagar with room servicediscovermytutordmt
 
Interactive Powerpoint_How to Master effective communication
Interactive Powerpoint_How to Master effective communicationInteractive Powerpoint_How to Master effective communication
Interactive Powerpoint_How to Master effective communicationnomboosow
 
Presentation by Andreas Schleicher Tackling the School Absenteeism Crisis 30 ...
Presentation by Andreas Schleicher Tackling the School Absenteeism Crisis 30 ...Presentation by Andreas Schleicher Tackling the School Absenteeism Crisis 30 ...
Presentation by Andreas Schleicher Tackling the School Absenteeism Crisis 30 ...EduSkills OECD
 
Z Score,T Score, Percential Rank and Box Plot Graph
Z Score,T Score, Percential Rank and Box Plot GraphZ Score,T Score, Percential Rank and Box Plot Graph
Z Score,T Score, Percential Rank and Box Plot GraphThiyagu K
 
General AI for Medical Educators April 2024
General AI for Medical Educators April 2024General AI for Medical Educators April 2024
General AI for Medical Educators April 2024Janet Corral
 
Accessible design: Minimum effort, maximum impact
Accessible design: Minimum effort, maximum impactAccessible design: Minimum effort, maximum impact
Accessible design: Minimum effort, maximum impactdawncurless
 
Web & Social Media Analytics Previous Year Question Paper.pdf
Web & Social Media Analytics Previous Year Question Paper.pdfWeb & Social Media Analytics Previous Year Question Paper.pdf
Web & Social Media Analytics Previous Year Question Paper.pdfJayanti Pande
 
APM Welcome, APM North West Network Conference, Synergies Across Sectors
APM Welcome, APM North West Network Conference, Synergies Across SectorsAPM Welcome, APM North West Network Conference, Synergies Across Sectors
APM Welcome, APM North West Network Conference, Synergies Across SectorsAssociation for Project Management
 

Recently uploaded (20)

Mattingly "AI & Prompt Design: The Basics of Prompt Design"
Mattingly "AI & Prompt Design: The Basics of Prompt Design"Mattingly "AI & Prompt Design: The Basics of Prompt Design"
Mattingly "AI & Prompt Design: The Basics of Prompt Design"
 
Activity 01 - Artificial Culture (1).pdf
Activity 01 - Artificial Culture (1).pdfActivity 01 - Artificial Culture (1).pdf
Activity 01 - Artificial Culture (1).pdf
 
Beyond the EU: DORA and NIS 2 Directive's Global Impact
Beyond the EU: DORA and NIS 2 Directive's Global ImpactBeyond the EU: DORA and NIS 2 Directive's Global Impact
Beyond the EU: DORA and NIS 2 Directive's Global Impact
 
IGNOU MSCCFT and PGDCFT Exam Question Pattern: MCFT003 Counselling and Family...
IGNOU MSCCFT and PGDCFT Exam Question Pattern: MCFT003 Counselling and Family...IGNOU MSCCFT and PGDCFT Exam Question Pattern: MCFT003 Counselling and Family...
IGNOU MSCCFT and PGDCFT Exam Question Pattern: MCFT003 Counselling and Family...
 
SOCIAL AND HISTORICAL CONTEXT - LFTVD.pptx
SOCIAL AND HISTORICAL CONTEXT - LFTVD.pptxSOCIAL AND HISTORICAL CONTEXT - LFTVD.pptx
SOCIAL AND HISTORICAL CONTEXT - LFTVD.pptx
 
1029-Danh muc Sach Giao Khoa khoi 6.pdf
1029-Danh muc Sach Giao Khoa khoi  6.pdf1029-Danh muc Sach Giao Khoa khoi  6.pdf
1029-Danh muc Sach Giao Khoa khoi 6.pdf
 
Holdier Curriculum Vitae (April 2024).pdf
Holdier Curriculum Vitae (April 2024).pdfHoldier Curriculum Vitae (April 2024).pdf
Holdier Curriculum Vitae (April 2024).pdf
 
Sanyam Choudhary Chemistry practical.pdf
Sanyam Choudhary Chemistry practical.pdfSanyam Choudhary Chemistry practical.pdf
Sanyam Choudhary Chemistry practical.pdf
 
Paris 2024 Olympic Geographies - an activity
Paris 2024 Olympic Geographies - an activityParis 2024 Olympic Geographies - an activity
Paris 2024 Olympic Geographies - an activity
 
Class 11th Physics NEET formula sheet pdf
Class 11th Physics NEET formula sheet pdfClass 11th Physics NEET formula sheet pdf
Class 11th Physics NEET formula sheet pdf
 
Call Girls in Dwarka Mor Delhi Contact Us 9654467111
Call Girls in Dwarka Mor Delhi Contact Us 9654467111Call Girls in Dwarka Mor Delhi Contact Us 9654467111
Call Girls in Dwarka Mor Delhi Contact Us 9654467111
 
9548086042 for call girls in Indira Nagar with room service
9548086042  for call girls in Indira Nagar  with room service9548086042  for call girls in Indira Nagar  with room service
9548086042 for call girls in Indira Nagar with room service
 
Código Creativo y Arte de Software | Unidad 1
Código Creativo y Arte de Software | Unidad 1Código Creativo y Arte de Software | Unidad 1
Código Creativo y Arte de Software | Unidad 1
 
Interactive Powerpoint_How to Master effective communication
Interactive Powerpoint_How to Master effective communicationInteractive Powerpoint_How to Master effective communication
Interactive Powerpoint_How to Master effective communication
 
Presentation by Andreas Schleicher Tackling the School Absenteeism Crisis 30 ...
Presentation by Andreas Schleicher Tackling the School Absenteeism Crisis 30 ...Presentation by Andreas Schleicher Tackling the School Absenteeism Crisis 30 ...
Presentation by Andreas Schleicher Tackling the School Absenteeism Crisis 30 ...
 
Z Score,T Score, Percential Rank and Box Plot Graph
Z Score,T Score, Percential Rank and Box Plot GraphZ Score,T Score, Percential Rank and Box Plot Graph
Z Score,T Score, Percential Rank and Box Plot Graph
 
General AI for Medical Educators April 2024
General AI for Medical Educators April 2024General AI for Medical Educators April 2024
General AI for Medical Educators April 2024
 
Accessible design: Minimum effort, maximum impact
Accessible design: Minimum effort, maximum impactAccessible design: Minimum effort, maximum impact
Accessible design: Minimum effort, maximum impact
 
Web & Social Media Analytics Previous Year Question Paper.pdf
Web & Social Media Analytics Previous Year Question Paper.pdfWeb & Social Media Analytics Previous Year Question Paper.pdf
Web & Social Media Analytics Previous Year Question Paper.pdf
 
APM Welcome, APM North West Network Conference, Synergies Across Sectors
APM Welcome, APM North West Network Conference, Synergies Across SectorsAPM Welcome, APM North West Network Conference, Synergies Across Sectors
APM Welcome, APM North West Network Conference, Synergies Across Sectors
 

Constraint induced language therapy

  • 1. Evidence-Based Systematic Review: Effects of Intensity of Treatment and Constraint-Induced Language Therapy for Individuals With Stroke-Induced Aphasia Purpose: This systematic review summarizes evidence for intensity of treatment and constraint-induced language therapy (CILT) on measures of language impairment and communication activity/participation in individuals with stroke-induced aphasia. Method: A systematic search of the aphasia literature using 15 electronic databases (e.g., PubMed, CINAHL) identified 10 studies meeting inclusion/exclusion criteria. A review panel evaluated studies for methodological quality. Studies were characterized by research stage (i.e., discovery, efficacy, effectiveness, cost–benefit/public policy research), and effect sizes (ESs) were calculated wherever possible. Results: In chronic aphasia, studies provided modest evidence for more intensive treatment and the positive effects of CILT. In acute aphasia, 1 study evaluated high-intensity treatment positively; no studies examined CILT. Four studies reported discovery research, with quality scores ranging from 3 to 6 of 8 possible markers. Five treatment efficacy studies had quality scores ranging from 5 to 7 of 9 possible markers. One study of treatment effectiveness received a score of 4 of 8 possible markers. Conclusion: Although modest evidence exists for more intensive treatment and CILT for individuals with stroke-induced aphasia, the results of this review should be considered preliminary and, when making treatment decisions, should be used in conjunction with clinical expertise and the client’s individual values. KEY WORDS: aphasia, intensity, constraint, rehabilitation Constraint-induced language therapy (CILT), a relatively new apha-sia treatment, (Pulvermuller et al., 2001), has garnered consid-erable interest from consumer groups and clients with aphasia. The major components of CILT involve both forced use of verbal language and massed practice (i.e., high intensity of treatment). Both are of con-siderable importance in light of recent work in neuroscience demonstrat-ing that the neuroplasticity of the adult brain can be impacted by several experience-dependent principles, including intensity of training and forced use of cognitive capacities (Kleim & Jones, 2008). Because intensity of treatment is an important variable that affects neuroplasticity, any dis-cussion of CILT must consider the impact not only of constraint but also of treatment intensity on outcomes. Therefore, the aim of this evidence-based systematic review (EBSR) was to examine the current state of the evidence that used one or both principles of CILT for individuals with stroke-induced aphasia: (a) constraining treatment responses to the verbal Leora R. Cherney Rehabilitation Institute of Chicago, IL Janet P. Patterson California State University, East Bay, Hayward, CA Anastasia Raymer Old Dominion University, Norfolk, VA Tobi Frymark Tracy Schooling American Speech-Language-Hearing Association, Rockville, MD Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research • Vol. 51 • 1282–1299 • October 2008 • D American Speech-Language-Hearing Association 1092-4388/08/5105-1282 1282
  • 2. modality only and (b) providing treatment on an inten-sive delivery schedule. The neurobiological principles and rehabilitation techniques surrounding CILT were derived from animal studies (Taub, 1977) as well as from a series of investi-gations of limbmovements in patients with chronic stroke and hemiplegia (Taub,Miller, Novack,&Cook, 1993; Taub &Wolf, 1997). These studies found that motor behavior of an impaired limb was modifiable with intensive practice over a short period of time. The techniques collectively known as constraint-induced movement therapy (CIMT) involve restraining the use of the less affected arm for the majority of waking hours to counteract the presumed learned nonuse of the affected arm. Repetitive training and shaping of movements of the affected arm take place in intensive treatment (6 hr per day) over 2 consecutive weeks. In the past 20 years, a large body of evidence, ap-plying either the original CIMT technique or a variant of this approach, has accumulated in support of the efficacy of constraint-induced (CI) therapy for rehabilitating hemiparetic arm use in individuals with chronic stroke (Hakkennes & Keating, 2005; Taub, Uswatte, & Piclikiti, 1999). To date, there are more than 120 published stud-ies, including a single-site, placebo-controlled trial (Taub et al., 2006) and a large multisite randomized trial (Wolf et al., 2006). Although the magnitude of the treatment effects have varied throughout the CIMT literature, the generally positive outcomes have encouraged investigators to examine the effects of CI and their potential benefits in other domains of stroke rehabilitation, including aphasia. Since being introduced to the field of aphasiology by Pulvermuller and colleagues (2001), CILT has been met with great interest. Pulvermuller et al. argued that pa-tients with aphasia often use the communication chan-nel that is accessible to them with the least amount of effort, such as gestures and drawing, rather than spoken language. A constrained approach for aphasia is incor-porated in the therapeutic setting by forcing the patient to communicate only through verbal channels while lim-iting the use of all other communication channels. The implementation of CILT in patients with aphasia con-sisted of intensive practice through a series of language games requiring verbal production of phrases and sen-tences of increasing length that were shaped into succes-sively better approximations.Results comparing language improvements after 2 weeks of CILT to approximately 4 weeks of traditional therapy indicated significant im-provements in the CI group on several standard clinical tests, self-ratings, and blinded-observer ratings of the patients’ communicative effectiveness in everyday life. Two principles of CILT need to be considered when interpreting the findings. The first principle, forced use of verbal language, requires that all responses provided during language treatment activities be constrained or restricted to the spoken modality. Nonverbal communi-cation attempts such as gesturing, drawing, or writing are not allowed and are not considered as communica-tion intents because participants are on opposite sides of a barrier. The second principle, massed practice, involves a high-intensity treatment schedule consisting of 3–4 hr of treatment per day for 2 weeks. Participants in the Pulvermuller et al. (2001) study were randomized to a CILT group and a comparison group that received con-ventional speech treatment provided on a distributed schedule over 3–5 weeks. That is, the groups differed in two dimensions of treatment: the type of treatment ad-ministered and the intensity of the treatment provided. Thus, it is difficult to determine whether the positive treatment results reported for the CILT group as com-pared to the conventional treatment group emanate from the constrained forced language use, the intensity of the treatment schedule, or a combination of these two factors. Intensity of treatment has been a topic of interest for some time in aphasia treatment studies (Poeck, Huber, & Willmes, 1989). Findings from Robey’s (1998) meta-analysis of the aphasia literature reported large effect sizes (ESs) associated with treatment provided for 2 or more hr per week. In a review focused on intensity and outcomes, Bhogal, Teasell, and Speechley (2003) reported better treatment outcomes in studies that provided in-tensive treatment schedules. On average, the more in-tensive treatment schedules equaled 8.8 hr per week for 11 weeks, compared with the less intensive schedules of 2 hr per week for 23 weeks of treatment. Although both reviews examined the intensity literature, the schedule of treatment provided in the cited studies is far less than the intensity of services given in CILT. This review examines the current state of the evi-dence employing the dual principles of CILT for individ-uals with stroke-induced aphasia. In addition to analyzing and synthesizing the scientific research in this area, the authors piloted the use of a newly created levels-of-evidence scheme developed by the American Speech- Language-Hearing Association (ASHA) National Center for Evidence-Based Practice in Communication Disor-ders (N-CEP), along with its advisory committee. This scheme classifies treatment studies according to several quality indicators and the phase of research (Robey, 2004). We piloted this system to determine its applica-bility to treatment studies in the area of communication sciences and disorders and provided feedback to N-CEP and its advisory committee prior to the widespread adoption of this system. Clinical Questions Ten clinical questions were identified by the authors for review (see Table 1). In constructing these questions, Cherney et al.: EBSR: Effects of Intensity of Treatment and CILT 1283
  • 3. the nature of outcome measurements and stage of re-covery from aphasia were considered. In keeping with current ASHA policy documents, such as the Scope of Practice in Speech-Language Pathology (ASHA, 2001), the World Health Organization’s (2001) International Classification of Functioning, Disability and Health: ICF was selected to categorize the various outcome measures representing language impairment (e.g., standard apha-sia tests of phonology, syntax, and semantics) or commu-nication activity/participation (e.g., functional real-life use of language in connected speech measures or com-munication ratings scales). Because the stage of recovery from stroke-induced aphasia—acute or chronic—influences recovery patterns (Robey, 1998) and studies typically include participants in only one of these stages, the clin-ical questions also included participants with chronic and acute aphasia (distinguished by mean time post onset [TPO]≤ 3months). Therefore, eight clinical ques-tions reflect combinations of the following parameters: (a) CILT principles—response constraint and treatment intensity; (b) aphasia chronicity—acute and chronic; and (c) outcome measures identified from the ICF—language impairment and communication activity/participation. Two additional questions were constructed to address maintenance of treatment behavior in chronic aphasia for a total of 10 clinical questions (see Table 1). Method A systematic search of the aphasia literature was conducted to identify studies that directly investigated CILT as the specific intervention for aphasia or directly compared conditions of higher and lower intensity treat-ment for aphasia. Fifteen electronic databases were searched: PubMed, CINAHL, PsycINFO, PsycArticles, CSA Linguistics and Language Behaviour Abstracts, Combined Health Information Database, Health Source: Nursing, Science Citation Index, ScienceDirect, NeLH, REHABDATA, Social ScienceCitation Index, SUMSearch, TRIP Database, and Cochrane Database of Systematic Reviews. Additional searches were also performed on all ASHA journals, the University of Pittsburgh Clinical AphasiologyConference Proceedings,National Institutes of Health (NIH) abstracts, and Google Scholar. In addi-tion, references from all relevant articles were examined to identify other applicable studies. A total of 25 ex-panded search terms included those related to stroke-induced aphasia, amount and intensity of treatment, and CILT. Studies were initially considered for the review if theywere published in a peer-reviewed journal from1990 to 20061, were written in English, and contained original data addressing 1 ormore of the 10 clinical questions. For studies addressing a clinical question related to intensity, the keyword intensity or amount of treatment had to be included in the abstract or the title of the article. In ad-dition, studies were limited to those that included adults 18 years of age or older with stroke-induced aphasia. Studies that included individuals with underlying cog-nitive deficits or any other primary medical diagnoses were excluded. Studies that used pharmacological inter-vention for aphasia as the comparison treatment were also excluded. The initial search schematicized in Figure 1 yielded 441 citations. Two authors (TF and TS), blind to one an-other’s results, reviewed each abstract and identified 36 abstracts as meeting the initial inclusion criteria with 91% agreement. Of those preliminarily accepted, 26 were Table 1. Clinical questions. Question No. Clinical Question—Intensity 1. For stroke-induced chronic aphasia, what is the influence of treatment intensity on measures of language impairment? 2. For stroke-induced chronic aphasia, what is the influence of treatment intensity on measures of communication activity/participation? 3. For stroke-induced acute aphasia, what is the influence of treatment intensity on measures of language impairment? 4. For stroke-induced acute aphasia, what is the influence of treatment intensity on measures of communication activity/participation? 5. For stroke-induced chronic aphasia, what treatment outcomes are maintained following intensive language treatment? Clinical Question—CILT 6. For stroke-induced chronic aphasia, what is the influence of constraint-induced language therapy on measures of language impairment? 7. For stroke-induced chronic aphasia, what is the influence of constraint-induced language therapy on measures of communication activity/participation? 8. For stroke-induced acute aphasia, what is the influence of constraint-induced language therapy on measures of language impairment? 9. For stroke-induced acute aphasia, what is the influence of constraint-induced language therapy on measures of communication activity/participation? 10. For stroke-induced chronic aphasia, what treatment outcomes are maintained following constraint-induced language therapy? 1The original purpose of the systematic review was to examine the effects of CILT. It became apparent that it would be impossible to adequately complete this review independent of addressing treatment intensity, which is a key principle of CILT. The search date was determined based on the emergence of CI literature in the early 1990’s. 1284 Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research • Vol. 51 • 1282–1299 • October 2008
  • 4. rejected upon review of the full text by the author panel, as they did not report original data or include direct evi-dence of a contrast between conditions (e.g., a more in-tensive condition vs. a less intensive condition) pertaining to one or more of the clinical questions. Ten studies met the final inclusion. Each of these studies was independently reviewed and evaluated for methodological quality by two of three authors (LC, JP, AR). One study written by an author (AR) was reviewed by two other panel authors (LC, JP). The authors, blind to one another’s results, appraised each study on the basis of quality indicators described in Table 2. For each indicator, quality markers are ordered from highest to lowest level of quality. A study received 1 point for each quality indicator if the highest level of quality was incorporated. All nine indicators were rel-evant to studies incorporating controlled trials, leading to amaximum quality score of 9. For other study designs, where intent-to-treat analysis was not relevant, the maximum quality score was 8. Credit for study design varied by stage of research (see Table 3). For example, efficacy studies using controlled trial designs received 1 point. Discovery studies received 1 point for research design if reporting retrospective case controls or single-participant studies. Reliability of scores was assessed, and any discrepancies in scoring were sent back to the full author panel for resolution. All disagreementswere resolved via consensus. In addition to assessing methodological quality, the phase of clinical research was determined using the decision tree depicted in Figure 2. The four stages of research—discovery, efficacy, effectiveness, and cost– benefit/public policy research—are defined in Table 3. A final synthesis of the body of scientific literature was compiled into an evidence table on the basis of the study qualitymarker score and corresponding stage of research. ESs were calculated for outcomemeasureswhenever possible. For group studies, Cohen’s d (Cohen, 1988) was calculated (corrected for bias following Hedges & Olkin, 1985). ESs were calculated from group means and stan-dard deviations or were estimated from results of anal-yses of variance (ANOVAs) or t tests. We used Cohen’s benchmarks for small, medium, and large ESs as .2, .5, and .8, respectively. Whether these benchmarks are ap-plicable to aphasia treatment studies has yet to be de-termined. In the one single-participant experiment (Raymer, Kohen,&Saffell, 2006), the authors calculated theweighted ESs using the methods described by Beeson and Robey (2006). Although Busk and Serlin (1992) reported that large ESs in single-participant studies sur-pass 2.5, Beeson and Robey noted benchmarks for small, medium, and large ESs in single-participant designs as 2.6, 3.9, and 5.8. Results Of the 10 studies that met inclusion criteria for the systematic review, 5 studies investigated treatment in-tensity (Basso & Caporali, 2001; Denes, Perazzolo, Piani, & Piccione, 1996; Hinckley & Carr, 2005; Hinckley & Craig, 1998; Raymer et al., 2006), 4 studies evaluated Figure 1. Process for identification of included studies. Cherney et al.: EBSR: Effects of Intensity of Treatment and CILT 1285
  • 5. CILT (Maher et al, 2006; Meinzer et al, 2004; Meinzer, Djundja,Barthel,Elbert,&Rockstroth, 2005; Pulvermuller, Hauk, Zohsel, Neininger, & Mohr, 2005), and 1 study ex-amined both (Pulvermuller et al., 2001). These studies allowed us to address 7 of the 10 clinical questions (see Table 1): Questions 1, 2, 5, 6, 7, and 10, which are related to language impairment and communication activity/ participation in individuals with chronic aphasia, and Question 3, which is related to the effects of treatment intensity on measures of language impairment for in-dividuals with acute aphasia. No study investigated CILT or intensive treatment effects for measures of Table 2. Quality indicators in the ASHA levels-of-evidence scheme. Indicator Description Quality marker Study design The type of design used in the study. Controlled trial (Efficacy research)* Retrospective case control (Effectiveness or cost–benefit research)* Single participant study (Discovery research)* Case series Case study Blinding The practice of keeping investigators or participants ignorant to the group to which participants are assigned. For the purposes of the critical appraisal, blinding refers to assessors only. Assessors blinded* Assessors not blinded or not stated Sampling The method(s) used to choose and assign participants to the experimental conditions in the study. Random sample adequately described* Random sample inadequately described Convenience sample adequately described Convenience sample inadequately described or hand-picked sample or not stated Group/participant comparability How similar the participants/groups were at the start of the study or how adequately they were described. Groups/participants comparable at baseline on important factors (between-subject design) or participant(s) adequately described (within-subject design)* Groups/participants not comparable at baseline or comparability not reported or participant(s) not adequately described Treatment fidelity The procedure used to ensure that the treatment was delivered as intended. Evidence of treatment fidelity* No evidence of treatment fidelity Outcomes The measure(s) used in the study to quantify improvement. At least one primary outcome measure is valid and reliable* Validity unknown, but appears reasonable; measure is reliable Invalid and/or unreliable Significance The likelihood that the study findings occurred by chance. p value reported or calculable* p value neither reported nor calculable Precision The size or magnitude of any difference found between the treatment under investigation and the control condition. Effect size and confidence interval reported or calculable* Effect size or confidence interval, but not both, reported or calculable Neither effect size nor confidence interval reported or calculable Intent-to-treat (controlled trials only) Participants are analyzed according to the group to which they were initially assigned, regardless of whether or not they dropped out, fully complied with the treatment, or crossed over and received the other treatment. If number of participants at pre-treatment = number at post-treatment, study received credit. Analyzed by intent-to-treat* Not analyzed by intent-to-treat* *Highest level of quality needed to receive 1 point. 1286 Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research • Vol. 51 • 1282–1299 • October 2008
  • 6. Table 3. Continuum of research stages. Stage Description Discovery Treatment approaches are developed and assessed in the context of whether communication activity/participation in acute aphasia (covered in Questions 4, 8, and 9). Participants Table 4 provides a detailed description of the 141 par-ticipants in the 10 studies (e.g., age, education level, gender, handedness, TPO, aphasia type, and severity). The total sample size (N) of participants reported in the studies does not account for the possible overlap of par-ticipants across four studies: Pulvermuller et al. (2001 and 2005) and Meinzer et al. (2004 and 2005). For the intensity studies, type of aphasia was pro-vided in five studies; 81% (55 of 68) of the participants were nonfluent. Severity of aphasia was provided in four studies; 40% (23 of 57) of participants tended to have more severe aphasia, with 30% (19 of 63) of participants being described as having global aphasia. The mean TPO was considered acute in one study (3.1 months in Denes et al., 1996); the other studies included participants with chronic aphasia, ranging from 11.2 to 67.6 months postonset. Similarly for the CILT studies, 60% of participants (42 of 70) had nonfluent forms of aphasia, typically de-scribed as Broca’s aphasia. Most had moderate aphasia (48%, 30 of 62), whereas 19 participants (31%) had mild aphasia and 12 participants (19%) had severe aphasia. Mean TPO ranged from 36.8 months to 90 months, all in the chronic range. Four of the 10 studies were conducted in Germany, 4 in the United States (U.S.), and 2 in Italy. Two German studies (Pulvermuller et al., 2001, 2005) included mono-lingual native speakers of German. The other two Ger-man studies (Meinzer et al., 2004, 2005) did not mention linguality of the participants. Two U.S. studies (Hinckley & Carr, 2005; Hinckley & Craig, 1998) noted that all par-ticipants were native speakers of English, whereas two Figure 2. Stage of research. they show promise of being efficacious. Efficacy Promising interventions are tested in a rigorous way under ideal, highly controlled conditions to determine the outcome that results. Effectiveness The intervention is tested in a “real-world” clinical setting. This phase is often conducted if the intervention demonstrates positive outcomes in a highly controlled setting of a clinical trial. Cost–benefit/public policy A study is conducted of the political and economic environment in which the intervention is best delivered. This phase is often conducted once it has been shown that an intervention is both efficacious and effective. Cherney et al.: EBSR: Effects of Intensity of Treatment and CILT 1287
  • 7. Table 4. Patient characteristics. Study N Age Education Gender Handedness TPO Etiology Aphasia type Aphasia severity Aphasia severity rating Basso & Caporali, 2001 (Intensity) 6 35–48 yrs 8–17 yrs 4 M R 2–22 months 2 hemorrhagic 2 mixed nonfluent NR Test scores M = 41 yrs M = 12.5 yrs 2 F M = 11.2 months 4 ischemic 2 global 2 nonfluent agrammatic Denes et al., 1996 (Intensity) 17 60.2 yrs 7.2 yrs 8 M R 2.4–4.5 months 4 hemorrhagic global Severe Test scores 9 F M = 3.1 months 13 ischemic Hinckley & Craig, 1998, Study 3 (Intensity) 10 25–78 yrs 12+ yrs 6 M 9 R 7–47 months Thromboembolic 8 nonfluent 1 severe Subjective M = 48 yrs 4 F 1 L M = 20 months 2 fluent 7 moderate 2 mild Hinckley & Carr, 2005 (Intensity) 13 19–72 yrs NR NR R 6–99 months L CVA 12 Broca BDAE severity score 1–3 Test scores M = 50 yrs M = 27 months intensive 1 transcortical motor M = 40 months nonintensive Maher et al., 2006 (CILT) 9 41–73 yrs 12–16+ yrs 6 M 8 R 14–72 months L CVA NR 4 severe Test scores M = 58 yrs 3 F 1 R+ M = 36.8 months 1 moderate 3 mild 1 NS Meinzer et al., 2004 (CILT) 28 35–80 yrs NR 14 M R 12–156 months 20 ischemic 13 Broca NR NR M = 54.6 yrs 14 F M = 43.8 months 8 hemorrhagic 4 Wernicke 3 global 6 not classified 2 amnesic Meinzer et al., 2005 (CILT) 27 18–80 yrs NR 16 M NR 13–116 months 11 hemorrhagic 10 Broca 2 severe Test scores M = 51.5 yrs 11 F M = 43.1 months 16 ischemic 8 Wernicke 15 moderate 3 amnesic 10 mild 1 global 5 not classified Pulvermuller et al., 2001 (CILT and Intensity) 17 42–72 yrs 9–13 years 12 M 14 R 2–223 months L CVA 10 Broca 4 severe Test scores M = 54.9 yrs M = 11 yrs 5 F 3 R+ M = 67.6 months 4 Wernicke 9 moderate 1 amnesic 4 mild 1 transcortical 1 conduction Pulvermuller et al., 2005 (CILT) 9 39–72 yrs 10–13 yrs 6 M 7 R 16–223 months L CVA 5 Broca 2 severe Test scores M = 54.4 yrs M = 11.3 yrs 3 F 2 R+ M = 90 months 2 Wernicke 2 moderate 1 amnesic 2 mild 1 transcortical Raymer et al., 2006 (Intensity) 5 51–82 yrs 8–14 yrs 2 M 5 R 4–42 months L CVA 2 Broca NR Test scores M = 70.8 yrs M = 11.4 yrs 5 F M = 18.4 months 2 conduction 1 mixed transcortical Note. TPO = time postonset; yrs = years; NR = not reported; R+ = ambidextrous; L CVA = left cerebrovascular accident; NS = not severe; BDAE = Boston Diagnostic Aphasia Examination (Goodglass & Kaplan, 1983). 1288 Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research • Vol. 51 • 1282–1299 • October 2008
  • 8. other U.S. studies (Maher et al., 2006; Raymer et al., 2006) did not address linguality.One Italian study (Denes et al., 1996) included only native speakers of Italian. The second Italian study (Basso & Caporali, 2001) included 5 participants who were native speakers of Italian and 1 who was a native of Sweden and completely bilingual. The educational level of the participants was reported in only 6 of the 10 studies. The mean years of educa-tion varied from 10.9 years (Pulvermuller et al., 2001) to 15 years (Maher et al., 2006). One study (Hinckley & Craig, 1998) reported that all participants completed high school or above, and the Basso and Caporali (2001) study reported educational attainment on only 4 of 6 par-ticipants. Only one study (Hinckley&Carr, 2005) reported mean data on socioeconomic status. Those participants had a mean score of 2.3 on the Hollingshead Four Factor Index of Social Status (Hollingshead, 1975). Study Characteristics and Quality Table 5 summarizes information regarding the in-tervention, including schedule, amount and duration of treatment, and outcomemeasures.Most studies provided 24–30 hr of treatment; several studies reported more than 100 hr of treatment (e.g., Hinckley & Craig, 1998). Treatment schedules varied across studies, as did the nature of treatment provided. Table 6 displays quality marker ratings obtained for the studies. All 10 studies described participants suf-ficiently and reported data in a manner in which statis-tical significance was available. Studies were lacking in several areas, however, including use of a clearly described randomized assignment scheme for participants in group designs, provided in only one study; blinding of assessors to treatment conditions, indicated in only two studies; and providing evidence of treatment fidelity, also noted in only two studies. Four studies were judged as discovery phase re-search and five studies as efficacy research. Only one study fell into the effectiveness stage of research, and none addressed cost–benefit or public policy. Intensity Results Table 7 displays stage of research, quality scores, and ESs for the six studies examining treatment inten-sity. These studies addressed Questions 1, 2, 3, and 5 (see Table 1). No data were available to answer Ques-tion 4. Five of these studies contained sufficient data for calculation of treatment ESs. The effect of intensity in Denes et al. (1996), Pulvermuller et al. (2001), and Hinckley andCarr (2005) was derived from between-group comparisons for groups receiving intensive and non-intensive treatment. The effect of intensity in Study 3 of Hinckley andCraig (1998)was derived fromwithin-group comparisons of the pre- and post difference scores from each intensive 6-week training session compared with the nonintensive 6-week training session. In Raymer et al. (2006), the effects came from within-subject comparisons across the individual participants. Clinical Question 1: For stroke-induced chronic apha-sia, what is the influence of treatment intensity on mea-sures of language impairment? Four group studies used impairment outcome measures for which eight ESs were calculable, including seven large ESs, all in favor of more intensive treatment. In the single-participant design of Raymer et al. (2006), ESs were larger in the more inten-sive condition for picture-naming acquisition and larger in the less intensive condition for word/picture verifica-tion. ESs could not be calculated for Basso and Caporali (2001), who described case studies of three pairs of indi-viduals. Individuals receiving more intensive treatment showed greater gains on language impairment tasks than did the comparison individualswho received a less intensive schedule. Thus, the language impairment outcome mea-sures favored more intensive treatment for all language measures except one in participants with chronic aphasia. Clinical Question 2: For stroke-induced chronic apha-sia, what is the influence of treatment intensity on mea-sures of communication activity/participation? Three group studies used communication measures for which nine ESs were calculable, including content unit (CU) analysis, oral and written responses on a catalogue order-ing task administered in quiet and concurrent noise con-ditions, and the CommunicationActivities of Daily Living (CADL–2; Holland,Frattali,&Fromm, 1999). ESs ranged from –1.15 to 3.78,with five favoringmore intensive treat-ment (four large effects) and four favoring less intensive treatment (two large effects). Basso and Caporali (2001) also contributed data to this clinical question, as they described greater changes in participants’ conversational abilities in all persons re-ceiving more intensive treatment as compared with the counterpart receiving less intensive treatment. Overall, findings for outcome measures of communication activity/ participation were mixed, with some favoring less in-tensive treatment and others favoring more intensive treatment. Clinical Question 3: For stroke-induced acute aphasia, what is the influence of treatment intensity on measures of language impairment? In one study of 17 individuals, (Denes et al., 1996), the ESs for the Aachen Aphasia Test (AAT; Huber, Poeck, & Williams, 1984) ranged from 0.39 (repetition tasks) to 1.20 (written language tasks), all fa-voring more intensive treatment. Clinical Question 5: For stroke-induced chronic apha-sia, what treatment outcomes are maintained following intensive language treatment? In the single-participant design of Raymer et al. (2006), ESs for picture-naming Cherney et al.: EBSR: Effects of Intensity of Treatment and CILT 1289
  • 9. Table 5. Intervention variables. Study Treatment program Treatment schedule Total amount Duration Impairment measure Activity/participation measure Basso & Caporali, 2001 (Intensity) All participants—individual treatment; conventional aphasia treatment; varied techniques. More intensive treatment NR All participants TT Picture description (no analysis) Goal areas = auditory comprehension, repetition, naming, and conversation. 3 participants 5 hr/wk; Home program 14–40 months Raven’s Matrices Treatment program—more intensive = clinic + home program; less intensive = clinic. 2–3 hr/day Less intensive treatment 3 participants; 5 hr/wk Denes et al., 1996 (Intensity) All participants—individual treatment; ecological, based on stimulation approach to restore efficient use of language mainly in conversation. All participants Intensive treatment group All participants AAT profile None Goal areas = auditory comprehension and verbal production. 45- to 60-min sessions M = 130 sessions, SD = 36, range = 94–160 M = 6 months, range = 5.2–7.0 TT Technique = picture identification and conversation context. Intensive treatment group Regular treatment group Repetition 6–7 sessions/wk M = 60 sessions, SD = 14, range = 56–70 Written Language Regular treatment group Naming About 3 sessions/wk Comprehension Hinckley & Craig, 1998, Study 3 (Intensity) All participants—individual, group and computer treatment; functionalist/pragmatic approach. Standard functional treatments (i.e., PACE and cueing hierarchy). Intensive treatment phase All participants All participants BNT CU analysis from picture description 23 hr/wk 294–306 hrs 18 wks Nonintensive treatment phase 6 wks intensive phase 3–5 hr/wk 6 wks nonintensive phase 6 wks intensive phase Hinckley & Carr, 2005 (Intensity) All participants—context-based treatment that is based on whole-task training and ecological validity. Intensive treatment group Total number of sessions per participant-NR NR PALPA CADL Tasks = problem-solving catalogue ordering task to develop compensatory strategies. 20 hrs individual treatment/wk; 5 hr group treatment/wk All participants Oral Naming Catalogue ordering task Techniques = role play, strategy development, and context-specific cues. Nonintensive treatment group Minutes to reach criterion on catalogue ordering task Written Naming Treatment group—intensive treatment = individual + group; nonintensive treatment = individual. 4 hr individual treatment/wk M = 233, range = 29–597 (1–10 sessions) (Continued on the following page) 1290 Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research • Vol. 51 • 1282–1299 • October 2008
  • 10. Table 5 Continued. Intervention variables. Study Treatment program Treatment schedule Total amount Duration Impairment measure Activity/participation measure Maher et al., 2006 (CILT) All participants—task = primarily dual card task. All participants All participants All participants WAB Story retelling Clinician ratings of post-treatment narrative Technique = shaping and cueing. 3-hr sessions 24 hr 2 wks BNT Treatment group—CILT = verbal response required. 4 sessions/wk ANT PACE = all communication modalities permitted. Meinzer et al., 2004 (CILT) Treatment group—CIAT Verbal response required. Dual card task (object drawings) 2–3 participants per group. All participants All participants All participants AAT None Technique = shaping and cueing. Model-based aphasia treatment; deficit-specific approach. 3 hr/day for 10 days 30 hr 2 wks TT NR = individual or group treatment. Meinzer et al., 2005 (CILT) Treatment group—CIAT = verbal response required; dual card task (object drawings); 2–3 participants per group. Technique = shaping. All participants All participants All participants AAT CAL CIAT plus = CIAT as above, plus dual card task (object 3 hr/day for 10 days 30 hr 2 wks CETI drawings, written words, and photographs); daily home program; daily communication practice with family members. Pulvermuller et al., 2001 (CILT and Intensity) Treatment group CIAT group CIAT group CIAT group TT CAL CIAT = verbal response required; dual card task 3 hr/day for 10 days M = 31.5 hr, 2 weeks AAT (object drawings); 2–3 participants per group. range = 23–33 Technique = Shaping and reinforcement. Conventional aphasia treatment—syndrome-specific standard approach; naming, repetition, sentence completion, and conversation tasks. Conventional treatment group Conventional treatment group Conventional treatment group Session length NR 3–5 wks M = 33.9 hr, range = 20–54 3–5 wks (Continued on the following page) Cherney et al.: EBSR: Effects of Intensity of Treatment and CILT 1291
  • 11. Table 5 Continued. Intervention variables. Study Treatment program Treatment schedule Total amount Duration Impairment measure Activity/participation measure Pulvermuller et al., 2005 (CILT) All participants—CIAT; verbal response required; dual card task (object drawings); 3 participants per group. All participants All participants All participants Lexical decision Reaction time None Technique = shaping and reinforcement. 3 hr/day for 10 days M = 31.3 hr, SD = 3.5, range = 23–33 2 wks AAT TT Repetition Naming Comprehension Raymer et al., 2006 (Intensity) All participants—Moss-talk multimode matching exercises; spoken and written word/picture matching. All participants All participants NR WAB None Alternating treatment schedules 3–5 baseline sessions BNT 3–4 sessions/wk 12 training sessions Picture naming 1–2 sessions/wk 1-month break Word/picture 12 training sessions verification 4 participants = 24 hr 1 participant = 22 hr Note. NR = not reported; AAT = Aachen Aphasia Test (Huber, Poeck, & Weniger, 1984); ANT = Action Naming Test (Nicholas, Obler, Albert, & Goodglass, 1985); BNT = Boston Naming Test (Kaplan, Goodglass, & Weintraub, 2001); CADL = Communication Activities of Daily Living (Holland, Frattali, & Fromm, 1999); CAL = Communication Activity Log (Pulvermuller et al., 2001); CETI = Communicative Effectiveness Index (Lomas et al., 1989); CU = content unit; PACE = Promoting Aphasics’ Communicative Effectiveness (Davis & Wilcox, 1985); PALPA = Psycholinguistic Assessment of Language Processing in Aphasia (Kay, Lesser, & Coltheart, 1992); TT = Token Test (De Renzi & Vignolo, 1962); WAB = Western Aphasia Battery (Kertesz, 1982). 1292 Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research • Vol. 51 • 1282–1299 • October 2008
  • 12. maintenance were 7.45 in the more intensive condi-tion and 4.85 in the less intensive condition. ESs for a word/picture verification task were 1.75 in the more intensive condition and 2.14 in the less intensive con-dition. Thus, maintenance effects of treatment inten-sity were mixed in this study. Stage of research and quality. Table 7 also shows the number of quality markers for each study and the stage of research. Of the two studies in the discovery phase of research, one received a quality marker score of 5 out of 8 (Raymer et al., 2006) and the other received a quality marker score of 3 out of 8 (Basso&Caporali, 2001). Three studieswere efficacy studies (Denes et al., 1996;Hinckley & Carr, 2005; Pulvermuller et al., 2001), each of which attained a quality marker score of 6 or 7 out of 9. One study (Hinckley & Craig, 1998) fell into the effectiveness stage of research, achieving a quality score of 4 out of 8. CILT Results CILT was examined in five studies as shown in Table 8. These studies addressed Questions 6, 7, and 10. No studies of CILT in acute aphasia were available, so Questions 8 and 9 could not be addressed. Cohen’s d val-ues could be calculated for all five CILTstudies. The ESs for Meinzer et al. (2004, 2005), and Pulvermuller et al. (2005) represent within-subject effects, whereas those in Maher et al. (2006) and Pulvermuller et al. (2001) come frombetween-group comparisons of CILTwith a contrast-ing treatment. Clinical Question 6: For stroke-induced chronic apha-sia, what is the influence of constraint-induced language therapy on measures of language impairment? Across the five group studies, 16 ESs were calculable, including 9 large ESs in favor of CILT. In individuals with chronic aphasia, CILT had positive effects for overall aphasia bat-tery scores (Maher et al., 2006; Meinzer et al., 2004) and subtests of auditory comprehension, word retrieval, repe-tition, and lexical decision (Maher et al., 2006; Meinzer et al., 2004; Pulvermuller et al., 2005). Meinzer et al. (2005) examined a modification of CILT (described as CIAT) in which additional home ac-tivities were included. There was no difference between CILT and CIAT plus on outcome scores immediately Table 6. Study quality marker variables. Study Design Assessor blinding Random sampling described Participants comparable/ described Treatment fidelity reported Valid outcome measure Significance Intent-to- treat Precision Basso & Caporali, 2001 (Intensity) Case study No No Yes No Yes Yes N/A No Denes et al., 1996 (Intensity) Controlled trial Yes No Yes No Yes Yes Yes Yes Hinckley & Craig, 1998, Study 3 (Intensity) Case series No No Yes No Yes Yes N/A Yes Hinckley & Carr, 2005 (Intensity) Controlled trial No No Yes Yes Yes Yes Yes Yes Maher et al., 2006 (CILT) Controlled trial No No Yes Yes Yes Yes No Yes Meinzer et al., 2004 (CILT) Case series No No Yes No Yes Yes N/A Yes Meinzer et al., 2005 (CILT) Controlled trial No No Yes No Yes Yes Yes No Pulvermuller et al., 2001 (CILT and Intensity) Controlled trial Yes Yes Yes No Yes Yes No No Pulvermuller et al., 2005 (CILT) Case series No No Yes No Yes Yes N/A Yes Raymer et al., 2006 (Intensity) Single subject No No Yes No Yes Yes N/A Yes Note. N/A = not applicable. Cherney et al.: EBSR: Effects of Intensity of Treatment and CILT 1293
  • 13. following training. Both groups reported significant im-provements. For language impairment measures, CILT consistently led to positive outcomes. Clinical Question 7: For stroke-induced chronic aphasia, what is the influence of constraint-induced lan-guage therapy on measures of communication activity/ participation? Three of the 5 CILT studies included mea-sures of communication activity/participation; ESs could be calculated for 11 outcome measures incorporated in the studies (e.g.,Communicative Activity Log, story retelling, Communicative Effectiveness Index; Lomas et al., 1989). ESs ranged from –.82 to 3.77, with eight favoringCILT (six large effects) and three favoring a comparison treatment (one large effect). Thus, CILT had positive outcomes for severalmeasures of communication activity/participation, although some favored a comparison condition. ClinicalQuestion 10: For stroke-induced chronic apha-sia, what treatment outcomes are maintained following Table 7. Intensity studies, effect sizes, and methodologic quality. Study Outcome measure(s) ICF Intensity effect size Research stage Quality score Clinical question Basso & Caporali, 2001 AAT TT I Not calculable Discovery 3 of 8 1, 2 Raven’s matricesa I Picture description I Denes et al., 1996 AAT: TT I 0.60 Efficacy 7 of 9 3 Repetition I 0.39 Written Language I 1.20 Naming I 0.73 Comprehension I 0.91 Profile Level I 0.83 Hinckley & Craig, 1998, Study 3 Intensive Treatment I vs. Nonintensive Effectiveness 4 of 8 1, 2 BNT I 1.12 Content Unit Analysis A/P 0.53 Intensive Treatment II vs. Nonintensive BNT I 0.95 Content Unit Analysis A/P 0.74 Hinckley & Carr, 2005 Catalogue ordering Efficacy 7 of 9 1,2 Oral (quiet) A/P –0.81 Oral (concurrent) A/P –0.05 Written (quiet) A/P –0.54 Written (concurrent) A/P 0.18 CADL-2 A/P –1.15 PALPA Oral Naming I 0.16 PALPA Written Naming I 1.48 Pulvermuller et al., 2001 AAT: Overall Test Scores I 2.18 Efficacy 6 of 9 1, 2 TT I 0.92 Naming I 1.12 Language Comprehension I 1.12 Repetition I Not calculable Communicative Activity Log: Patients A/P 3.78 SLPs A/P 2.64 Raymer et al., 2006 WAB Aphasia Quotient I Not calculable Discovery 5 of 8 1, 5 BNT I Not calculable Picture Naming Acquisition I 4.35 (lw), 11.37 (hw) Picture Naming Maintenance I 4.85 (lw), 7.45 (hw) Word/Picture Verification Acquisition I 2.72 (lw), 2.14 (hw) Word/Picture Verification Maintenance I 2.14 (lw), 1.75 (hw) Note. lw = low weighted intensity; hw = high weighted intensity; ICF = International Classification System; I = impairment; A/P = activity/participation; SLP = speech-language pathologist. aRaven’s Coloured Progressive Matrices (Raven, Court, & Raven, 1986) 1294 Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research • Vol. 51 • 1282–1299 • October 2008
  • 14. constraint-induced language therapy? Two studies ad-dressed Question 10. Meinzer et al. (2005) reported that effects of CILT on measures of language impairment and communication activity/participation were main-tained at 6months compared with baseline performance. Family members of the group receiving CIATplus with the modified home program reported further gains in communication effectiveness over the 6-month period. Maher et al. (2006) reported follow-up testing at 1 month post-CILT completion. Three of 4 CILT participants and 1 of 3 participants in Promoting Aphasics’ Communica-tive Effectiveness (PACE) demonstrated continued in-creases on aphasia tests over the 1-month period. Stage of research and quality. Table 8 indicates that two studies that fell into the discovery phase of re-search received quality scores of 4 out of 8. Three studies represented efficacy research, two of which received a qualitymarker score of 6 out of 9 and one ofwhich received a score of 5 out of 9. Discussion The purpose of this EBSR was to assess the in-fluence of intensity of language treatment and CILT on language and communication outcomes of individuals with stroke-induced aphasia. A systematic search of the literature from 1990 to 2006 yielded only 10 studies that met predetermined inclusion criteria, with 5 studies ad-dressing treatment intensity, 4 studies addressing CILT, and 1 study addressing both. Although few studies were expected to be found for CILT—a technique that was Table 8. CILT studies, effect sizes, and methodologic quality. Study Outcome measure(s) ICF CILT effect size Research stage Quality score Clinical question Maher et al., 2006 Aphasia tests Efficacy 6 of 9 6, 7, 10 WAB Aphasia Quotient I 1.01 BNT I –0.16 Action Naming Test I 0.14 Linguistic measures in story retelling Number of words A/P –0.72 Number of utterances A/P –0.82 Number of sentences A/P –0.19 Mean length of utterance A/P 0.33 Clinician ratings A/P Not calculable Meinzer et al., 2004 AAT: Profile I 0.34 Discovery 4 of 8 6 Token Test I 0.81 Meinzer et al., 2005 AAT: Profile I 1.63 Efficacy 5 of 9 6, 7, 10 Subtests I Not calculable CETI: Overall – relatives A/P 1.86 Communication Activity Log Quantity: Patients A/P 1.99 Relatives A/P 2.35 Comprehension: Patients A/P .47 Relatives A/P 1.13 Pulvermuller et al., 2001 AAT: Overall Test Scores I 2.18 Efficacy 6 of 9 6, 7 TT I 0.92 Naming I 1.12 Language Comprehension I 1.12 Repetition I Not calculable Communicative Activity Log: Patients A/P 3.77 SLPs A/P 2.64 Pulvermuller et al., 2005 AAT: TT I 0.25 Discovery 4 of 8 6 Repetition I 0.11 Naming I 0.25 Comprehension I 0.46 Lexical decision reaction times Response time I 2.39 Pseudowords x time I 3.32 Words I Not calculable Cherney et al.: EBSR: Effects of Intensity of Treatment and CILT 1295
  • 15. introduced only in 2001—the paucity of studies directly addressing treatment intensity was surprising in view of the overall large number of studies examining language treatment for stroke-induced aphasia. Given the small number of studies, the conclusions of the present sys-tematic reviewmust be considered preliminary. Further-more, interpretation of ESs and their applicability to the aphasia literature is not certain at this time. Neverthe-less, certain trends in the literature from 1990 to 2006 may usefully inform both clinical practice and future re-search in this area. Increased treatment intensity was associated with positive changes in outcome measures of language im-pairment in 68 persons with chronic and acute aphasia. All ESs calculated for group comparisons on language impairment measures favored more intensive treatment over less intensive treatment, including seven large ef-fect sizes. In the single-participant study (Raymer et al., 2006), ESs for one measure favored more intensive treat-ment and one favored less intensive. The data for Clin-ical Question 2 addressing change in communication activity/participation were mixed, with some favoring more intensive treatment and some favoring less inten-sive treatment for persons with chronic aphasia. These observations suggest that there can be complex interac-tions among intensity of treatment schedule, type of treatment, and type of outcome measure. Maintenance of treatment intensity effects were reported in one study of persons with chronic aphasia (Raymer et al., 2006). Findings also were equivocal, fa-voring more intensive treatment for one outcome mea-sure and less intensive treatment for the other. However, there was only one data point for each maintenance ob-servation. Beeson and Robey (2006) suggest a minimum of two data points for calculation of an ES. Therefore, this result should be taken with caution. Overall, five studies involving 90 participants reported that CILT resulted in positive changes on mea-sures of language impairment and communication activity/ participation in individuals with chronic aphasia, in-cluding large ESs for 9 of 16 impairment measures and 6 of 11 activity/participation measures. Meinzer et al. (2005) reported that effects weremaintained for as long as 6 months following completion of CILT in 27 patients. No data addressed the effect of CILT in patients with acute aphasia, however. Finally, it is important to note that Maher et al. (2006) also found significant improve-ments on language measures for participants in their control group, who participated in an intensive version of PACE in which use of compensatory modalities is al-lowed. This finding implies that intensity is a key com-ponent of CILT. Interpretation of the results of this EBSR and its implications for clinical practice must be considered in conjunction with the information about characteristics of the participants. For the intensity studies, the major-ity of participants had nonfluent forms of aphasia. When severity of aphasia was provided, the majority of partic-ipants tended to have more severe aphasia, with many described as having a global aphasia. Therefore, conclu-sions regarding intensity of treatment are most applica-ble to individuals with severe nonfluent aphasia. Future research needs to address, in particular, the impact of treatment intensity for participants presenting with a milder aphasia as well as participants who are catego-rized as fluent. Similarly for the CILT studies, the ma-jority of participants were nonfluent and moderately impaired, thereby limiting the generalizability of the re-sults in individuals with fluent aphasia and individuals with mild and severe aphasia. Chronicity of aphasia is another participant characteristic that requires atten-tion in future studies. No study of CILT included in-dividuals with acute aphasia, and only one treatment intensity study addressed the impact of intensity on acute aphasia. One of the difficulties encountered in this system-atic review was that of comparing results across studies when there were differences in the outcome measures used. Therefore, we attempted to find commonalities by categorizing the types of outcome measures accord-ing to the WHO (2001) ICF levels of body function (lan-guage reception and production impairment) and activity/ participation. Overall, all of the studies included at least one outcome measure at the language impairment level, and most of these measures were standardized, valid as-sessment instruments. Although all of the CILT studies and four of the six intensity studies included at least onemeasure at the communication activity/participation level, there were fewer of these measures, they were typ-ically individualized, and information on their validity and reliability was often lacking. Future studies of CILT and clinical practice should consider inclusion of more measures at the communication activity/participation level as well as studies that address quality of life in patients with aphasia. The variety of primary and secondary outcome mea-suresmade comparisons across studies difficult and com-plicated the determination of the validity and reliability as they contribute to the quality of the study. Therefore, our critical appraisal used a quality marker related to outcome measures that represented only a minimal level of quality in these areas. All studies achieved this min-imal level of quality that related to use of a valid and reliable primary outcome measure. A number of methodological weaknesses in the 10 studies included in the EBSR were identified during the critical appraisal of the quality of each study. The short-comings across the studies related to primarily three 1296 Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research • Vol. 51 • 1282–1299 • October 2008
  • 16. quality indicators: random assignment of subjects, as-sessor blinding, and evidence of treatment fidelity. In most of the studies, regardless of stage of research and research design, participants were selected as a conve-nience sample. Randomization is critical for future re-search, as many evidence-based review organizations (e.g., The Cochrane Collaboration) include primarily ran-domized trials in their systematic reviews of health care interventions. It is also essential that investigators doc-ument procedures to ensure fidelity of the actual treat-ment techniques and that some measure of procedural reliability is provided. Clear documentation of the treat-ment technique will facilitate the transference of the tech-nique to the clinical setting. Examination of the studies in relation to stage of re-search clearly shows where the strengths of the current research lie and the direction of future research. In-cluded studies in the present EBSR were three efficacy studies evaluating the impact of intensity on stroke-related chronic aphasia and three efficacy studies eval-uating the impact of CILT on chronic aphasia. Only one intensity study evaluated effectiveness, and no study evaluated effectiveness of CILT. No studies in either cat-egory assessed policy change or cost effectiveness. There-fore, as evidence for efficacy of more intensive treatment and CILT continues to grow, future research must ad-dress issues of effectiveness and cost effectiveness with studies that are designed according to the criteria in-cluded in the ASHA levels-of-evidence scheme to ensure that the studies are of high quality. Regardless of type of treatment, our general con-clusions indicate that more treatment is better over a re-stricted time interval. No studies have addressed what might be the optimum amount and intensity of treat-ment; however, it is possible that no single answer exists and that optimum dosage of high-intensity treatment varies depending on the type of treatment and the char-acteristics of the participant’s aphasia, including chro-nicity. Future studies, beginning at the discovery phase, are needed to address the many confounds inherent in resolving the issue of optimum intensity of treatment for individuals with stroke-induced aphasia. Although this systematic review has highlighted the potential efficacy of CILT on the basis of its dual prin-ciples, future studies need to be designed with the aim of teasing out the impact of constraint and intensity on out-come. This task is particularly difficult because high in-tensity is such an integral part of CILT. However Maher et al. (2006) have demonstrated that it is possible to ad-dress this issue methodologically by providing an iden-tical schedule of CILTand its control treatment. Although results suggest that efficacy is related to the nature of the treatment (i.e., constraint) rather than to intensity alone, given the small number of participants, results are not yet conclusive. More definitive evidence will be derived only from (a) conducting randomized studies comparing CILT with other treatment procedures given at a simi-lar intensity schedule using participants of similar type and severity of aphasia and (b) measuring outcomes at the levels of language impairment and communication activity/participation. Additionally, the optimum inten-sity of treatment for CILT has yet to be determined. Fur-ther studies comparing constraints at different levels of intensity are needed. Although the early evidence surrounding the efficacy of CILT for individuals with aphasia shows somepromise, the effects are similar to other intensive treatments that do not employ the use of constraint. Therefore, these re-sults should be taken under advisement. Clear and con-vincing evidence supporting the effectiveness of CILT over other aphasia treatments has not yet been estab-lished, and further research comparing aphasia treat-ments is warranted. Clinicians should interpret the findings from this EBSR conservatively and always in conjunction with their patients’ unique characteristics, circumstances, and preferences when considering the best course of treatment for and with their clients. Finally, some observations need to be made following this pilot implementation of ASHA’s levels-of-evidence scheme. The indicators used to judge the quality of re-search studies tend to focus on issues that are most per-tinent to group study designs. Therefore, the usefulness of the levels-of-evidence scheme in evaluating single-participant research designs, which are used commonly in the speech-language pathology literature, is in need of further development. Although eight of the nine indica-tors were applicable to single-participant design research, other quality indicators related to the documentation of experimental control and threats to external validity (e.g., stability of baselinemeasures) that are unique to this type of research were not included. These additional issues of design quality that arise in single-participant research will need to be studied. Another notable aspect of the current systematic re-view is that all studies addressing the clinical questions, including discovery studies, are included. Although some systematic reviews include only group designs or randomized clinical trials, we elected to include all stud-ies, regardless of design. Ultimately, we need to give greater weight to well-designed randomized trials. How-ever, given the relative paucity of those studies in our current literature base, the inclusion of all possible stud-ies gives a broader perspective of the state of knowledge. It is our hope that this systematic review will incite ad-ditional rigorous clinical trials addressing the questions of intensity of aphasia treatment and CILT, as well as other areas of interest to the fields of speech-language pathology and audiology. Cherney et al.: EBSR: Effects of Intensity of Treatment and CILT 1297
  • 17. Acknowledgments This EBSR was supported by ASHA’s National Center for Evidence-Based Practice in Communication Disorders (N-CEP), the Advisory Committee on Evidence-Based Practice in Communication Disorders, and ASHA’s Special Interest Division 2: Neurophysiology and Neurogenic Speech and Language Disorders. We thank the following individuals who contributed to the preparation of this article: Rob Mullen, N-CEP Director; BeverlyWang, N-CEP Information Manager; and Floyd Roye, N-CEP Project Administrator. All members of this evidence-based review panel agreed to declare no competing interests in relation to this article. No author had any paid consultancy or any other conflict of interest with this document. The first three authors contributed equally to the preparation of this article, and the order does not reflect any differences in contribution. References ASHA. (2001). Scope of practice in speech-language pathology [Scope of Practice]. Available from www.asha.org /policy. Basso, A. A., & Caporali, A. (2001). Aphasia therapy or the importance of being earnest. Aphasiology, 15, 307–332. Beeson, P., & Robey, R. (2006). Evaluating single-subject treatment research: Lessons learned from the aphasia literature. Neuropsychological Review, 16, 161–169. Bhogal, S.K., Teasell, R.,&Speechley, M. (2003). Intensity of aphasia therapy, impact on recovery. Stroke, 34, 987–993. Busk, P. L., & Serlin, R. (1992). Meta-analysis for single case research. In T. R. Kratochwill & J. R. Levin (Eds.), Single-case research design and analysis: New directions for psy-chology and education. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. Cohen, J. (1988). Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences (2nd ed.). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. Davis, G. A., & Wilcox, M. (1985). Adult aphasia rehabilita-tion: Applied pragmatics. San Diego, CA: College Hill Press. Denes, G., Perazzolo, C., Piani, A., & Piccione, F. (1996). Intensive versus regular speech therapy in global aphasia: A controlled study. Aphasiology, 10, 385–394. De Renzi, E., & Vignolo, L. (1962). The Token Test: A sensitive test to detect receptive disturbances in aphasics. Brain, 85, 665–678. Goodglass, H., & Kaplan, E. (1983). The Boston Diagnostic Aphasia Examination. Philadelphia: Lea & Febiger. Hakkennes, S., & Keating, J. (2005). Constraint-induced movement therapy following stroke: A systematic review of randomized controlled trials. Australian Journal of Physio-therapy, 51, 221–231. Hedges, L.V.,&Olkin, I. (1985). Statistical methods for meta-analysis. Orlando, FL: Academic Press. Hinckley, J. J., & Craig, H. K. (1998). Influence of rate of treatment on the naming abilities of adults with chronic aphasia. Aphasiology, 12, 989–1006. Hinckley, J. J.,& Carr, T. (2005). Comparing the outcomes of intensive and non-intensive context-based aphasia treat-ment. Aphasiology, 19, 965–974. Holland, A., Frattali, C., & Fromm, D. (1999). Communica-tion activities of daily living (2nd ed.). Austin, TX: Pro-Ed. Hollingshead, A. B. (1975). Four Factor Index of Social Status. New Haven, CT: Yale University. Huber, H. P., Poeck, K., & Williams, K. (1984). The Aachen Aphasia Test. In F. C. Rose (Ed.), Progress in aphasiology (pp. 291–303). New York: Raven Press. Huber, W., Poeck, K., & Weniger, D. (1984). The Aachen AphasiaTest. InF. C. Rose (Ed.), Advances in neurology,Vol. 42: Progress in aphasiology. New York: Raven Press. Kaplan, E., Goodglass, H., & Weintraub, S. (2001). The Boston Naming Test. Philadelphia: Lea & Febiger. Kay, J., Lesser, R., & Coltheart, M. (1992). Psycholinguistic assessments of language processing in aphasia. Hove, United Kingdom: Psychology Press. Kertesz, A. (1982). Western Aphasia Battery. Orlando, FL: Grune & Stratton. Kleim, J. A., & Jones, T. A. (2008). Principles of experience-dependent neural plasticity: Implications for rehabilitation after brain damage. Journal of Speech-Language-Hearing Research, 51(Suppl.), S225–S239. Lomas, J., Pickard, L., Bester, S., Elbard, H., Finlayson, A., & Zoghaib, C. (1989). The Communicative Effective-ness Index: Development and psychometric evaluation of a functional communication measure for adult aphasia. Journal of Speech Hearing Disorders, 54, 113–124. Maher, L., Kendall, D., Swearengin, J., Rodriguez, A., Leon, S., Pingel, K., Holland, A., & Rothi, L. (2006). A pilot study of use-dependent learning in the context of constraint induced language therapy. Journal of the Inter-national Neuropsychological Society, 12, 843–852. Meinzer, M., Djundja, D., Barthel, G., Elbert, T., & Rockstroth, B. (2005). Long-term stability of improved language functions in chronic aphasia after constraint-induced aphasia therapy. Stroke, 36, 1462–1466. Meinzer, M., Elbert, C., Wienbruch, C., Djundja, D., Barthel, G., & Rockstroh, B. (2004). Intensive language training enhances brain plasticity in chronic aphasia. BMC Biology, 2, 1–9. Nicholas, M., Obler, L., Albert, M., & Goodglass, H. (1985). Lexical retrieval in healthy aging. Cortex, 21, 595–606. Poek, K., Huber, W., & Willmes, K. (1989). Outcomes of intensive language treatment in aphasia. Journal of Speech and Hearing Disorders, 54, 471–479. Pulvermuller, F. B., Neininger, B., Elbert, T., Mohr, B., Rockstroh, B., Koebbel, P.,&Taub, E. (2001). Constraint-induced therapy of chronic aphasia after stroke. Stroke, 32, 1621–1626. Pulvermuller, F., Hauk, O., Zohsel, K., Neininger, B., & Mohr, B. (2005). Therapy-related reorganization of lan-guage in both hemispheres of patients with chronic aphasia. Neuroimage, 28, 481–489. Raymer, A., Kohen, F., & Saffell, D. (2006). Computerised training for impairments for word comprehension and retrieval in aphasia. Aphasiology, 20, 257–268. Raven, J. C., Court, J. H., & Raven, J. (1986). Raven’s Coloured Progressive Matrices. London: H. K. Lewis & Co. Robey, R. R. (1998). A meta-analysis of clinical outcomes in the treatment of aphasia. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 41, 172–187. Robey, R. R. (2004). A five-phase model for clinical-outcomes research. Journal of Communication Disorders, 5, 401–411. 1298 Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research • Vol. 51 • 1282–1299 • October 2008
  • 18. Taub, E. (1977). Movement in nonhuman primates deprived of somatosensory feedback. Exercise and Sports Sciences Review, 4, 335–374. Taub, E., Miller, N., Novack, T., & Cook, E. (1993). Tech-nique to improve chronic motor deficit after stroke. Archives of Physical Medicine and Rehabilitation, 74, 347–354. Taub, E., Uswatte, G., King, D. K., Morris, D. M., Crago, J. E., & Chatterjee, A. (2006). A placebo controlled trial of constraint-induced movement therapy for upper extremity after stroke. Stroke, 37, 1045–1049. Taub, E., Uswatte, G., & Piclikiti, R. (1999). Constraint-induced movement therapy: A new family of techniques with broad application to physical rehabilitation—A clinical review. Journal of Rehabilitation Research and Develop-ment, 36, 237–251. Taub, E., & Wolf, S. (1997). Constraint-induced movement techniques to facilitate upper extremity use in stroke patients. Topics in Stroke Rehabilitation, 3, 38–61. Wolf, S., Winstein, C. J., Miller, J., Taub, E., Uswatte, G., Morris, D., et al. (2006). Effect of constraint-induced move-ment therapy on upper extremity function 3 to 9 months after stroke: The EXCITE randomized clinical trial. Jour-nal of the American Medical Association, 296, 2095–3104. World Health Organization. (2001). International Classifi-cation of Functioning, Disability and Health: ICF. Geneva, Switzerland: Author. Received September 4, 2007 Accepted February 24, 2008 DOI: 10.1044/1092-4388(2008/07-0206) Contact author: Tobi Frymark, National Center for Evidence- Based Practice in Communication Disorders, American Speech-Language-Hearing Association, 2200 Research Boulevard, Rockville, MD 20850. E-mail: tfrymark@asha.org. Cherney et al.: EBSR: Effects of Intensity of Treatment and CILT 1299