SlideShare a Scribd company logo
1 of 48
Download to read offline
Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org
Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org
Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org
Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org
Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org
Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org
Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org
Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org
Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org
Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org
Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org
Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org
Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org
Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org
Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org
Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org
Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org
Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org
Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org
Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org
Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org
Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org
Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org
Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org
Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org
Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org
Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org
Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org
Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org
Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org
Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org
Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org
Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org
Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org
Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org
Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org
Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org
Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org
Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org
Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org
Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org
Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org
Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org
Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org
Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org
Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org
Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org
Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org
Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org
Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org
Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org
Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org
Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org
Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org
Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org
Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org
Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org
Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org
Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org
Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org
Aajhss.org
International
Journal of
Humanities &
Social Sciences
Vol. 8, No. 5
IJHSS.NET
e-ISSN: 1694-2639
p-ISSN: 1694-2620
October 2016
Vol 8, No 5 – October 2016
Table of Contents
Effects of personal characteristics on susceptibility to decision bias: a
literature study
1
Alexander Toet, Anne-Marie Brouwer, Karel van den Bosch
and J.E. (Hans) Korteling
Likert Scale Development: Construction and Evaluation of Home
Environment Scale
18
Mukhtar Ahmad Wani & Aejaz Masih
Iqbal’s Response to Modern Western Thought: A Critical Analysis 27
Dr. Mohammad Nayamat Ullah and Abdullah Al Masud
Is more BENELUX cooperation the future for the low countries ? 37
Prof. dr. Herman Matthijs
AAJHSS.ORG
1 http://aajhss.org/index.php/ijhss
International Journal of Humanities and Social Sciences
p-ISSN: 1694-2620
e-ISSN: 1694-2639
Vol. 8 No. 5, pp. 1-17, ©IJHSS
Effects of personal characteristics on susceptibility to
decision bias: a literature study
Alexander Toet, Anne-Marie Brouwer, Karel van den Bosch, J.E. (Hans) Korteling
TNO
Kampweg 5, 3769 DE Soesterberg, The Netherlands
Abstract
Cognitive biases and heuristics are pervasive simplifications and distortions in judgement and
reasoning that systematically affect human decision making. Knowledge in this area may enable
us to foresee and reduce detrimental effects of biases or to influence others more effectively. We
therefore performed a literature study to assess the influence of personal characteristics
(cognitive abilities, expertise, personality, cultural background) on the occurrence of cognitive
biases. We found that each of the aforementioned factors can affect cognitive biases, though not
much is known about the effects of culture. Also, factors that appear to reduce a cognitive bias
may in fact mitigate (suppress or override) its behavioral effect rather than preventing the bias
from occurring at all. The general picture that arises is that bias susceptibility and the occurrence
of biases depend on thinking style (heuristic versus deliberate), where thinking style is associated
with an individual‟s personal characteristics. In general, biases are reduced when a deliberate
(analytical) thinking style is applied. However, whether a specific (heuristic or deliberate)
thinking style actually reduces or enhances a given type of bias also depends on the context.
Keywords: bias, heuristics, personal characteristics, decision making, judgement.
Introduction
People constantly make judgments and decisions, either conscious or unconscious. Most
decisions are made without knowing for sure what the effects of the decision will be and whether
the outcome will be positive or not. Making decisions in uncertainty is based upon at least two
factors: the desirability and the likelihood of the outcome. Decision theory studies the
development of algorithms that, for a given problem and likelihoods, produce the best outcome.
Decision theory can thus, in principle, provide recommendations how to best make decisions.
However, in real life people often make decisions that deviate from the best solution (Haselton,
Nettle, & Andrews, 2005).These deviations are the outcome of systematic distortions in human
judgment and reasoning.
Subjective estimations play a significant part in our daily life. Our decisions, conclusions
and explanations are based upon our beliefs about the likelihood of uncertain events but not on
their objective likelihood. For example, the decision to stay away from a particular neighborhood is
not driven by the actual likelihood of being robbed, but by our perception of the likelihood of being
robbed. The important conclusion of an immense body of research on decision making is that
2 http://aajhss.org/index.php/ijhss
people do not follow the laws of probability, but instead use relatively simple rules (heuristics).
These heuristics often perform well, but under certain conditions may lead to systematic and
serious errors. The distortion of human judgment and decision making is called „cognitive bias‟,
or shortly „bias‟.
The study reported here was performed to acquire further insight into biased (pre-
cognitive, automatic, heuristic) judgment and decision processes (that are influenced by
intuitions, emotions, biases, or associations) and to investigate whether biased thinking depends
on the personal characteristics of individuals. If there are indeed psychological characteristics
that predict biased thinking, this knowledge could in principle effectively be used to either
mitigate or deploy biased thinking. Knowledge of cognitive biases can for instance be used to:
 develop selection procedures (to develop tests that estimate an individual‟s susceptibility to
biases),
 reduce susceptibility to biases and ameliorate their effects (by training personnel and
institutions to recognize and cope with biases appropriately, and by developing fast and
frugal decision protocols),
 effectively deploy knowledge of biases against relevant actors (both on a strategic level as part
of a doctrine and operationally in the field).
The literature study reported here contributes to this goal by identifying characteristics of
individuals (e.g., cognitive abilities, expertise, personality, cultural background) that may predict a
person‟s susceptibility to cognitive biases, and in particular those leading to decision and
judgment biases.
The following section discusses the literature search strategy. Next we briefly discuss the
origins of human decision biases and factors that may affect the susceptibility to these biases.
Then we present a literature review on some personal characteristics that may predict an
individual‟s susceptibility to judgment and decision bias. Finally, we will present the conclusions
of this study.
Literature search strategy
Electronic searches were carried out using the databases ScienceDirect, PubMed and PsycINFO.
In addition, lliterature searches were also performed with Google Scholar. As search terms we
used the names of several well-known biases (Anchoring, Anchoring bias, Attribution error, Base
rate neglect, Belief bias, Confirmation bias, Conjunction fallacy, Framing, Halo effect, Hindsight
bias, Imaginability bias, Omission bias, Negativity bias, Outcome bias, Over-confidence bias,
Sunk cost effect) as well as some general terms (Bias, Cognitive bias, Decision bias, Decision
making, Judgement, Heuristic bias, Heuristics: for definitions see Appendix A) and combined
them with (conjunction: AND) terms related to personal characteristics (cognitive abilities OR
expertise OR personality OR individual differences OR culture OR reasoning OR thinking OR
trait) factors. The searches were restricted to articles reporting empirical studies in peer reviewed
journals. The relevant papers that were found in this initial search served as a starting point for
subsequent searches, that included all later papers referring to papers from the initial set (found
by using the „Cited by‟ function in Google Scholar. We included studies found using this method
in the review only if they involved influences of personal factors on human decision making. The
literature searches were performed in the second half of 2016.
3 http://aajhss.org/index.php/ijhss
Cognitive biases and their origins
The human mind is limited in its capacity to render judgments in a way that is perfectly rational
and fully informed. While rational thinking serves well to solve decision problems that allow
comprehensive analysis, it may fail in complex real life situations, where it is often very difficult
to have access to every relevant piece of information, and where a decision often has to be made
quickly. Even if such access were possible, our brains do not operate like computer algorithms,
capable of complex and multiple calculations in order to reach logically sound conclusions – not
to mention that we hardly have time to perform such rigorous analysis for every judgment that
we make. As a result, human decision making tends to rely on a variety of simple heuristic
decision rules that can be executed quickly. Oftentimes, heuristics produce judgements and
decisions that are „good enough‟ when measured against an acceptable cognitive load. However,
heuristics can also lead to irrational thinking and problem-solving in ways that produce errors or
illogical decisions, known as „cognitive biases‟. This is most likely to occur in complex situations
(when relevant information is ignored and/or irrelevant information interferes) or in situations
that are mistakenly perceived as familiar (while they are actually unknown). Cognitive biases are
pervasive in human reasoning and have important practical implications.
Dual-process heuristic-deliberate theories postulate a distinction between fast, intuitive,
automatic, heuristic, emotionally charged and fallible (heuristic or „Type 1‟ ) processes versus
slow, conscious, controlled, deliberate and analytic (deliberate or „Type 2‟) processes (e.g., Evans,
2006; Kahneman, 2003; Kahneman & Frederick, 2002; Sloman, 1996). When fast responses are
required, performance is based on low-effort heuristic processes. Deliberate processes supervise
and control the output of the heuristic system. In this view biases occur when deliberate
processing either (1) fails to successfully engage (Kahneman, 2003) or (2) fails to override the
biased heuristic response (De Neys, 2012). The serial deliberate processes are slower and require
more working memory and are therefore constrained by the limited capacity of the brain.
Conversely, heuristic processes function implicitly and in parallel and do not claim executive
working memory resources (De Neys, 2006). It is generally assumed that heuristic processes
result in biased outcomes, unless the analytical system takes over (Evans, 1984, 1989). In this
study we adopt the dual-process heuristic-deliberate theory of human decision making as a
framework to unify and understand our current findings.
Intuitive (heuristic) decision-making produces quick solutions based on general heuristics
(the Fast and Frugal view: Gigerenzer & Gaissmaier, 2010) or on experience-based (pattern-
matching) evaluations (Naturalistic Decision Making: G. A. Klein, 1993; see e.g. Kahneman &
Klein, 2009; G. Klein, 2015 and Davis, Kulick, & Egner, 2005 for a discussion on the differences
and similarities between both views). Intuition can provide access to information that would not
be beliedfble through deliberate thinking (Hogarth, 2010). However, intuition cannot generate or
acquire new knowledge. Also, since intuition is bias prone, it may lead to potentially dangerous
inaccurate perceptions of reality (Dane & Pratt, 2007; Kahneman & Frederick, 2002; Amos
Tversky & Kahneman, 1974).
In contrast to intuitive decision-making, deliberation uses abstract thinking and
generalizations, and allows the acquisition of additional information in the decision-making
process (Söllner, Broeder, & Hilbig, 2013).This is a clear advantage over intuition when problem
solving requires the application of complex rules (Kahneman & Frederick, 2002). In the real
world, decision makers often have to deal with three critical constraints that limit their
opportunities for deliberation: (1) limited access to information, (2) cognitive limitations inherent
in the human mind, and (3) limited time. These constraints result in „bounded rationality‟ (H. A.
Simon, 1972). Deliberation is time consuming and requires cognitive effort. Cognitive resources
claimed by deliberation cannot be deployed for other tasks (Kurzban, Duckworth, Kable, &
4 http://aajhss.org/index.php/ijhss
Myers, 2013). Decision-makers will therefore typically use their intuition when there is no
obvious need to use deliberate reasoning to make a good decision.
From a behavioral standpoint cognitive biases may be seen as systematic errors in
rational reasoning (Amos Tversky & Kahneman, 1974). However, many cognitive biases that
appear irrational from the viewpoint of rational choice theory may in fact be quite rational from
the perspective of evolutionary biology (Santos & Rosati, 2015). They may optimize decision
making in a given environment (context) by optimally using the available information (ecological
rationality: Goldstein & Gigerenzer, 2002). However, decision rules that are completely adapted
to a given (natural) environment may of course lead to maladaptive („biased‟) behavior in
different settings (Fawcett et al., 2014).
Factors affecting human cognitive bias
From a practical viewpoint, it would be useful to understand the factors that predict the
occurrence of cognitive biases. This literature review addresses the question whether
characteristics of individuals affect susceptibility to cognitive biases. The dual-process heuristic-
deliberate theory of human decision making (described above) is adopted as a framework to
unify and understand our current findings. This theory states that individuals are less prone to
biases if they apply the thinking style (deliberate or heuristic) that is most appropriate for the
problem context. Furthermore, an individual‟s tendency to make biased judgments and decisions
may be related to personal characteristics (e.g., cognitive ability, expertise, personality).
Cognitive ability
People differ in their cognitive abilities like intelligence, training (level of expertise), and thinking
styles. Studies investigating the correlation between measures of intelligence and a wide range of
different cognitive biases have shown that cognitive ability (both fluid and crystalized
intelligence) does not predict bias-proneness in general (Stanovich & West, 2008; Teovanovic,
Knezevic, & Stankov, 2015). Intelligent people are just as prone to cognitive bias as less
intelligent ones. However, highly intelligent people are more able than less intelligent ones to
avoid cognitive bias once they have been warned about the bias in advance and are instructed
how to avoid it (Stanovich & West, 2008).
Significant negative correlations have been observed between fluid intelligence and
several biases. Fluid intelligence is an individual‟s capacity to think in a logical way and to find
solutions for new problems, independent of acquired knowledge (Cattell, 1987). Fluid
intelligence has been found to correlate negatively with belief bias, over-confidence bias and base
rate neglect and also with the Cognitive Reflection Test (CRT: Frederick, 2005). The CRT is a
widely used tool to assess individual differences in intuitive–analytic cognitive styles. The CRT
has been found to correlate negatively with the sunk cost effect, belief bias and base rate neglect;
Teovanovic et al., 2015).Thus it seems that people with high reflective abilities and a high fluid
intelligence are less prone to these cognitive biases.
The literature shows mixed results on the relation between analytic intelligence and
proneness to the anchoring bias (for a review see: Furnham & Boo, 2011). While some studies
found that individuals with higher cognitive abilities are less susceptible to anchoring (Bergman,
Ellingsen, Johannesson, & Svensson, 2010), others observed no - or even the opposite - effect
(Oechssler, Roider, & Schmitz, 2009).
There is evidence that an individual‟s susceptibility to bias relates to structures and
emotional processes in the brain. Two key brain structures mediating emotional information
5 http://aajhss.org/index.php/ijhss
processing are the amygdala and the orbitofrontal cortex (OFC) (e.g., Kim & Hamann, 2007;
Zald, 2003). The amygdala can be seen as a primitive structure linking immediate threat with
rapid survival responses (Sander, Grafman, & Zalla, 2003). The OFC is associated with
deliberate thinking and has the function to gather and update information and use it to predict
possible outcomes of-, and to steer, human behavior (Rolls, 2004; Rolls & Grabenhorst, 2008).
De Martino et al. (De Martino, Kumaran, Seymour, & Dolan, 2006) examined the neural
mechanisms mediating the framing effect and the ability to control it. While placed inside an
fMRI scanner their participants performed a financial decision making task. In line with the dual-
process theory (i.e., the view that choices are typically affectively loaded and involve heuristic
thinking; see Kahneman & Frederick, 2007) this study showed that framing bias during financial
decision making correlated with a higher activity in the greater amygdala. In addition, they found
that subjects who acted more rationally also exhibited stronger OFC activation. Interestingly,
they also found a strong inter-individual variability in susceptibility to framing, which did not
correlate with amygdala activity. Instead, they observed a positive correlation between the ability
to control framing bias and OFC activation: increased orbital and medial prefrontal cortex
activity correlated with a reduced susceptibility to the framing effect (De Martino et al., 2006).
Although enhanced OFC activity does not necessarily imply the inhibition of emotional
processes (Aron, 2007), this result agrees with the view that controlling decision bias depends on
engagement of deliberate, rational thinking.
People who score high on the Need for Cognition (NFC: an individual‟s propensity to enjoy
and engage in thought: Cacioppo & Petty, 1982) are just as likely to be „framed‟ as anyone else.
However, compared to people scoring low on NFC, they are more consistent across different
frames of a problem. In accordance with the abovementioned results from brain research, the
magnitude of the framing effect is significantly reduced when decision makers are encouraged to
reflect on the options and to motivate their choice (Miller & Fagley, 1991; Sieck & Yates, 1997;
Takemura, 1993). For people who score high on the Need for Cognition this manipulation even
eliminates the framing effect altogether (A. F. Simon, Fagley, & Halleran, 2004). NFC has also
been found to moderate hindsight bias: hindsight bias is found for persons with low and
medium NFC scores, but not for people with high NFC scores (Verplanken & Pieters, 1988).
An individual‟s thinking style has often been associated with proneness to bias. An
individual‟s preference for an analytic-rational (deliberate processing) or an intuitive-experiential
(heuristic processing) thinking style can be assessed through the Rational-Experiential Inventory
(REI: Epstein, Pacini, Denes-Raj, & Heier, 1996). The relationship between thinking style and
various biases has been investigated in several studies. Persons that dominantly use an
analytic/rational thinking style tend to be less susceptible to the base rate neglect bias than
people using an intuitive/experiental thinking style (Ohlert & Weißenberger, 2015). There seems
to be no relation between thinking style and susceptibility to the conjunction fallacy (Lu, 2015).
Thinking style and belief bias appear to be linked: in contrast to people with an intuitive-
experiential thinking style, people with an analytic-rational thinking style are less susceptible to
belief bias (Svedholm-Häkkinen, 2015; Trippas, Pennycook, Verde, & Handley, 2015).
A specific cognitive ability that appears to be linked with thinking style is numeracy, or the
proficiency in basic probability and numerical concepts (Peters et al., 2006). An individual‟s
numeracy-competency is determined by (1) the degree of information processing (heuristic or
deep elaborative processing), (2) affective numerical intuition (e.g., framing); and (3) intuitive
understanding (e.g., gist-based representation and reasoning; see Ghazal, Cokely, & Garcia-
Retamero, 2014). Low numeracy is typically linked with intuitive (heuristic) thinking, whereas
high numeracy is typically linked with deliberate (analytical) processing (Brust-Renck, Reyna,
Corbin, Royer, & Weldon, 2014). In this view people with high numeracy are less susceptible to
6 http://aajhss.org/index.php/ijhss
biases because they show an analytical thinking style and information seeking behavior (Ghazal
et al., 2014). Numeracy has indeed been found to determine decision making quality across a
wide range of tasks (Sinayev & Peters, 2015). For instance, people with high numeracy are less
susceptible to framing bias (Gamliel, Kreiner, & Garcia-Retamero, 2015; Peters et al., 2006) and
conjunctions fallacies (Sinayev & Peters, 2015), are less over/under confident about their
decisions (Sinayev & Peters, 2015), and take less risks (Jasper, Bhattacharya, Levin, Jones, &
Bossard, 2013). People with higher numeracy are better able to extract the affective „gist‟ of a
problem and use it to determine the quality of a particular choice (Jasper et al., 2013). These
findings cannot be attributed to differences in general intelligence (Peters et al., 2006).
Somewhat in contrast, people using a combination of thinking styles (high
deliberate/high heuristic (also called „complementary thinking‟) and low deliberate/low heuristic
(also called „poor thinking‟) are found to be more susceptible to framing than those using a
dominant (either rational or intuitive) thinking style (Shiloh, Salton, & Sharabi, 2002). Thus, it
seems that people with a clearly dominant heuristic or deliberate thinking style are more resistant
to framing. This may be because both decision styles use strong internal guides (either logical or
experiential) to process information. In contrast, people with a more uniform thinking style
(either „complementary‟ or „poor‟) appear to depend more on coincidental external situational cues
(e.g., the way the information is formulated) when processing information.
Individuals with high emotional intelligence (the ability to recognize and distinguish
between emotions and to identify their causes) are able to reduce (or even eliminate) the effects
of decision bias by recognizing that the emotions they experience (for instance anxiety: Yip &
Côté, 2013) are irrelevant for the decisions they have to make. For example, military officers with
high emotional intelligence make better tactical decisions under stressful condition because they
are able to maintain a higher state of attentiveness for social cues and perform a more exhaustive
(deliberate) analysis of situational cues (Fallon et al., 2014).
Summarizing, several types of cognitive ability, as well as the ability to engage in
deliberate information processing at appropriate times, seem to protect an individual from
several cognitive biases (in particular, the sunk cost bias, the base rate fallacy, the over-
confidence bias, the belief bias, and framing). For anchoring and conjunction bias the evidence is
mixed. Emotional intelligence has been found to reduce the likelihood of falling prey to decision
bias. In general it can be concluded that cognitive ability does not safeguard an individual against
bias, but it may in some cases help in deploying countering mechanisms that reduce, or prevent
subsequent behavioral effects.
Expertise
Whether people deploy a heuristic or a more deliberate decision making mode depends for a
large part on the decision-maker‟s expertise (Fuchs, Steigenberger, & Lübcke, 2015). An expert is
an individual who has acquired special skills in a given domain (Chi, Glaser, & Farr, 1988). The
main distinction between experts and novices is the extent of their domain-specific knowledge
(Chi et al., 1988). Less experienced decision-makers increasingly use deliberative thinking to
solve subjectively complex problems while mainly following their preferred (either heuristic or
deliberate) decision style. In addition, the thoroughness of their information processing is
affected by their mood: a happy mood leads to more superficial (heuristic) processing, while a
sad mood leads to more thorough (deliberate) processing (Englich & Soder, 2009). In contrast,
more experienced decision makers use deliberation independent of their decision preferences,
subjective environmental complexity or mood (Englich & Soder, 2009; Fuchs et al., 2015).
Moreover, experts rely more on intuition than on deliberation. They can probably do so because
7 http://aajhss.org/index.php/ijhss
they have learned to match tools and strategies to problem structures (similar to chess players:
Sauter, 1999). For example, expert handball players are more intuitive than non-experts and tend
to rely on their first intuitively generated decision option (Raab & Laborde, 2011).
Expertise generally does not significantly reduce the anchoring bias effect (for a review
see: Furnham & Boo, 2011). However, expertise in a specific estimation context (task) may
reduce susceptibility to anchoring: experience in a card game was inversely correlated with
susceptibility to anchoring (Welsh, Delfabbro, Burns, & Begg, 2014). This implies that the exact
nature of the expertise should be clearly defined before being able to assess whether „experts‟ are
less affected by anchors than „non-experts‟. Individual differences in various traits may be more
useful for predicting the rate of learning, where it is their level of expertise that indirectly reduces
susceptibility to anchoring, rather than direct susceptibility to biases (Welsh et al., 2014).
Summarizing, expertise affects sensitivity to biases since it determines thinking style and
the way that information is processed in combination with its context. Expertise can stimulate
both deliberate and heuristic thinking. When the context is not appropriate, the latter may lead to
biases. Experts are less likely to misinterpret the context than novices. They are therefore more
likely to select the thinking style appropriate for the context, and are subsequently less prone to
bias than non-experts.
Personality
Personality is often defined in terms of five main personality traits (the „Big Five‟: John &
Srivastava, 1999): openness, conscientiousness, extraversion, agreeableness, and neuroticism.
The literature shows that individuals with high conscientiousness, agreeableness and
openness to experience or with low extraversion are more susceptible to the anchoring bias
(Caputo, 2014; Eroglu & Croxton, 2010; McElroy & Dowd, 2007; Teovanovic et al., 2015). It
has been suggested that because individuals with high conscientiousness engage in more
deliberate thinking when making decisions, they are more likely to perform a confirmatory search
for anchor consistent information. Individuals with high agreeableness tend to be more affected
by anchors than less agreeable persons. This is probably because individuals with high openness
to experience easily „adjust‟ their beliefs when considering situational information.
There are some indications that introverts are more susceptible to anchoring bias than
extraverts (Eroglu & Croxton, 2010; Furnham, Boo, & McClelland, 2012), but this relation is not
robust (Furnham et al., 2012). It has been suggested that low extraversion may be associated with
negative affect (Eroglu & Croxton, 2010), which may stimulate more deliberate thinking and
thereby activate a confirmatory search for anchor consistent information (Bodenhausen, Gabriel,
& Lineberger, 2000; Englich & Soder, 2009).
People scoring high on trait optimism (people who tend to believe in a bright future)
(Scheier, Carver, & Bridges, 1994) were more likely to update their judgments in response to
desirable information than to undesirable information, particularly for judgments that apply to
themselves (Kuzmanovic, Jefferson, & Vogeley, 2015). In other words, people with high trait
optimism show a pronounced self-specific optimism bias. It has been argued that the evolution
of the healthy mind to (optimistically) mis-predict future occurrences has led to an increased
resilience, improved coping behavior and reduced anxiety, resulting in overall improvements of
both physical and mental health (Dolcos, Hu, Iordan, Moore, & Dolcos, 2015; Sharot, 2011).
Optimism bias may sometimes even lead to better outcomes than do unbiased beliefs (Sharot,
2011). Recent brain studies have identified the OFC with trait optimism: higher OFC gray
matter volume (GMV) correlates with increased optimism (Dolcos et al., 2015).
8 http://aajhss.org/index.php/ijhss
Despite the hypotheses suggested above, it is not easy to determine the mechanisms
underlying the relationships between personality traits and susceptibility to cognitive bias.
Summarizing, personality appears to relate to bias susceptibility because it determines how
people weigh and process information.
Culture
Members of different social cultures may have different ways of thinking, because they have
been socialized from birth into different world views. Some researchers hypothesize that these
cultural differences affect an individual‟s susceptibility for bias. For instance, East Asians are
believed to have a holistic world view, attending more to contextual factors and assigning
causality to them, while they are less inclined to categorize and use formal logic. Westerners, on
the other hand, are typically more analytic and are more inclined to use pay attention to objects
of interest, to categorize them, and to use rules and formal logic to understand their behavior
(Nisbett, Peng, Choi, & Norenzayan, 2001; Strutton & Carter, 2013). As a result, East Asians
may for instance be less susceptible to attribution errors, since they see behavior primarily as a
product of external factors and not merely of the actor's dispositions. For the same reason, they
may be more susceptible to hindsight bias because they are readily able to find some explanation
for a given event since everything is connected in their world view (Choi & Nisbett, 2000; Yama
et al., 2010). Following this line of thinking, Westerners may be better able to withstand the
hindsight bias because they have a more rule-based thinking style.
Only a few studies address the influence of culture on bias susceptibility. Studies on the
effects of culture on hindsight bias show mixed results: while some studies confirmed the
abovementioned hypotheses (Choi & Nisbett, 2000; Yama et al., 2010) others found no cultural
differences in the sensitivity to hindsight bias (Pohl, Bender, & Lachmann, 2002). One study
(Scott, Christopher, & John, 1998) found evidence for the hypothesis that self-centered
Westerners, with their personal desire to be correct and to fortify one‟s choices, are more
susceptible to the sunk cost bias than collectivist East Asians who are more focused on
optimizing outcomes for the group. However, other studies found opposite results (Yoder,
Mancha, & Agrawal, 2014). They propose that East Asians may also be prone to sunk cost bias
because they are more concerned about saving face, resulting in more commitment to prior
decisions.
A recent study on choice framing (Haerem, Kuvaas, Bakken, & Karlsen, 2011) compared
military decision makers with business students (difference in organizational cultures). It was
found that business students showed the classic framing bias (risk avoidance behavior in the gain
frame an risk seeking behavior in the loss frame: A. Tversky & Kahneman, 1981), while military
decision makers consistently showed a risk-seeking behavior for both (gain and loss) choice
frames. In addition, military officers showed significantly higher levels of self-efficacy than
business school students. Self-efficacy correlated with risk seeking in the military group but not
in the civil group. This result agrees with the finding that people with little confidence in their
own competence do not like to gamble (Heath & Tversky, 1991). Military decision makers, on
the other hand, are probably so self-confident (or even over-confident) that they believe that
they can beat the odds.
Summarizing, while it is possible that culture affects sensitivity to bias (e.g., as a result of
different preferred thinking styles or levels of self-efficacy), only a few studies on this topic have
been conducted and their results are mixed.
9 http://aajhss.org/index.php/ijhss
Discussion and conclusions
The objective of this literature review was to investigate to what extent individual characteristics
(cognitive abilities, expertise, personality, cultural background) affect a person‟s susceptibility to
judgment and decision biases. This knowledge may for instance be used to develop strategies to
mitigate biased thinking of own personnel, or to deploy strategies to evoke biased thinking in
opponent parties.
Our findings indicate that each of the reviewed aspects can affect cognitive biases under
certain conditions (though with respect to culture the evidence is scarce). Note that the factors
found to reduce cognitive bias may in fact merely mitigate the behavioral effects rather than
preventing the bias from occurring at all.
Several types of cognitive ability, as well as employing deliberate information processing
may mitigate various cognitive biases (in particular: sunk cost, base rate, over confidence, belief,
framing). For anchoring and conjunction bias, the evidence is mixed. Emotional intelligence has
been found to reduce some biases. In general, cognitive ability does, by itself, not prevent biases
from occurring, but it may help to learn how to prevent or reduce its effects on behavior.
Expertise affects sensitivity to biases, because expertise largely determines thinking style
and contextual information processing. Depending on the problem context, expertise may
stimulate both deliberate thinking as well as heuristic thinking. Experts are more likely to select
the thinking style appropriate for the context, and are subsequently less prone to bias than non-
experts.
Personality affects the way people weigh and processes information, which in turn affects
the susceptibility to certain (but not all) biases. However, the relationship between personality
and susceptibility to cognitive bias is not fully clear. Predicting the occurrence and direction of
cognitive bias based on personality traits is therefore very hard.
Limitations of the present study
The results reviewed in this study have mostly been obtained in laboratory conditions involving
simplified tests that have specially been designed to induce cognitive biases. It is therefore not
clear how these results translate into real-life practice.
We used the dual-process heuristic-deliberate framework to summarize, unify and
understand our findings. However, not all reported effects fit into this framework. For instance,
effects reported on emotional priming are different from studies that demonstrate effects of
emotion, indicating a different underlying mechanism. Also, some of the effects upon judgment
and decision making seem to be working against each other. For example, people who need to
perform under pressure and time constraints are expected to apply heuristics which makes them
vulnerable for bias. However, at the same time, it is known that pressure induces a negative
mood which often elicits deliberate thinking strategies. And although deliberate thinking tends to
guard people against the risk of bias, this is not always the case. Exactly when it does, and when
not, is still difficult to quantify. This underlines the need to more fully understand the
mechanisms underlying cognitive bias.
Future research
The fact that the effects of cognitive biases on decision making are not completely fixed within
and between individuals suggests opportunities to apply this knowledge for selecting (bias free)
and training (de-biasing) personnel. Implicit association tests may be effective selection tools (De
10 http://aajhss.org/index.php/ijhss
Houwer, 2006). How these selection tests should be worked out for a given context (e.g.,
medical, financial, military, police) requires analysis of the characteristics and requirements of the
candidates, the nature of the task, and the contextual demands. The information and insights
resulting from the present study may also stimulate the development of new de-biasing
techniques to protect own personnel against (self- or externally- induced) cognitive bias in
concrete situations. Strategies that promote deeper information processing and thereby stimulate
the recruitment of the medial and lateral orbitofrontal cortex (regions associated with the
integration of affective and contextual information in decision making) may constrain decision
bias (Hughes & Zaki, 2015). Computer games that immerse the user into bias-invoking situations
that provide the experience to identify cognitive bias and to practice mitigation strategies, may
serve as effective debiasing tools. It has been shown that these types of “serious games” can
provide an effective method to train adults how to recognize and mitigate several cognitive
biases (confirmation, attribution and blind spot; Clegg et al., 2015; Dunbar et al., 2014;
Symborski et al., 2014). Models and decision support systems that provide tools and explicit rules
to guide decisions, may also help to counteract the adverse effects of judgement bias. This can
for instance be achieved by allowing the user to employ heuristics while warning for the likely
biases, and by anticipating the likely use of heuristics and providing information that offsets the
effects of such use (Larrick, 2004). In addition, the current findings may initiate innovative
methods to exploit biases to manipulate the behavior of opponents and other groups.
References
Aron, A. R. (2007). The neural basis of inhibition in cognitive control. The Neuroscientist, 13(3), 214-228.
doi:10.1177/1073858407299288
Bergman, O., Ellingsen, T., Johannesson, M., & Svensson, C. (2010). Anchoring and cognitive ability.
Economics Letters, 107(1), 66-68. doi:10.1016/j.econlet.2009.12.028
Bodenhausen, G. V., Gabriel, S., & Lineberger, M. (2000). Sadness and susceptibility to judgmental bias:
The case of anchoring. Psychological Science, 11(4), 320-323. doi:10.1111/1467-9280.00263
Brust-Renck, P. G., Reyna, V. F., Corbin, J. C., Royer, C. E., & Weldon, R. B. (2014). The role of
numeracy in risk communication. In H. Cho, T. Reimer, & K. A. McComas (Eds.), The SAGE
Handbook of Risk Communication (pp. 134-145). Thousand Oaks, CA, USA: Sage.
Cacioppo, J. T., & Petty, R. E. (1982). The need for cognition. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,
42(1), 116-131. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.42.1.116
Caputo, A. (2014). Relevant information, personality traits and anchoring effect. International Journal of
Management and Decision Making, 13(1), 62-76. doi:10.1504/IJMDM.2014.058470
Cattell, R. B. (1987). Intelligence: Its structure, growth, and action. New York, USA: Elsevier Science Publishing
Comapny, Inc.
Chi, M. T., Glaser, R., & Farr, M. J. (1988). The nature of expertise. Hillsdale, NJ, USA: Lawrence Erlbaum
Assoiates, Inc.
Choi, I., & Nisbett, R. E. (2000). Cultural psychology of surprise: Holistic theories and recognition of
contradiction. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 79(6), 890-905. doi:10.1037/0022-
3514.79.6.890
Dane, E., & Pratt, M. G. (2007). Exploring intuition and its role in managerial decision making. Academy of
Management Review, 32(1), 33-54. doi:10.5465/AMR.2007.23463682
Davis, P. K., Kulick, J., & Egner, M. (2005). Implications of modern decision science for military decision-support
systems. Santa Monica, CA, USA: RAND Corporation.
De Houwer, J. (2006). What are implicit measures and why are we using them. In R. W. Wiers & A. W.
RStacy (Eds.), The handbook of implicit cognition and addiction (pp. 11-28). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage
Publishers.
De Martino, B., Kumaran, D., Seymour, B., & Dolan, R. J. (2006). Frames, biases, and rational decision-
making in the human brain. Science, 313(5787), 684-687. doi:10.1126/science.1128356
11 http://aajhss.org/index.php/ijhss
Chronometric and dual-task considerations. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 59(6),
1070-1100. doi:10.1080/02724980543000123
De Neys, W. (2012). Bias and conflict: A case for logical intuitions. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 7(1),
28-38. doi:10.1177/1745691611429354
Dolcos, S., Hu, Y., Iordan, A., Moore, M., & Dolcos, F. (2015). Optimism and the brain: Trait optimism
mediates the protective role of the orbitofrontal cortex gray matter volume against anxiety. Social
Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 11(2), 263-271. doi:10.1093/scan/nsv106
anchoring. Judgment and Decision Making, 4(1), 41-50.
Epstein, S., Pacini, R., Denes-
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 71(2), 390-405.
doi:10.1037/0022-3514.71.2.390
Eroglu, C., & Croxton, K. L. (2010). Biases in judgmental adjustments of statistical forecasts: The role of
individual differences. International Journal of Forecasting, 26(1), 116-133.
doi:10.1016/j.ijforecast.2009.02.005
Evans, J. S. B. T. (1984). Heuristic and analytic processes in reasoning. British Journal of Psychology, 75(4),
451-468. doi:10.1111/j.2044-8295.1984.tb01915.x
Evans, J. S. B. T. (1989). Bias in human reasoning: Causes and consequences. London, UK: Lawrence Erlbaum
Associates, Inc.
Evans, J. S. B. T. (2006). The heuristic-analytic theory of reasoning: Extension and evaluation. Psychonomic
Bulletin & Review, 13(3), 378-395. doi:10.3758/BF03193858
Fallon, C. K., Panganiban, A. R., Wohleber, R., Matthews, G., Kustubayeva, A. M., & Roberts, R. (2014).
Emotional intelligence, cognitive ability and information search in tactical decision-making.
Personality and Individual Differences, 65(0), 24-29. doi:10.1016/j.paid.2014.01.029
Fawcett, T. W., Fallenstein, B., Higginson, A. D., Houston, A. I., Mallpress, D. E. W., Trimmer, P. C., &
McNamara, J. M. (2014). The evolution of decision rules in complex environments. Trends in
Cognitive Sciences, 18(3), 153-161. doi:10.1016/j.tics.2013.12.012
Frederick, S. (2005). Cognitive reflection and decision making. Journal of Economic Perspectives, 19(4), 25-42.
doi:10.1257/089533005775196732
Fuchs, H. M., Steigenberger, N., & Lübcke, T. (2015).
decisions in action? Paper presented at the Proceedings of the 12th International Conference on
Information Systems for Crisis Response & Management (ISCRAM 2015), Kristiansand,
Denmark.
Furnham, A., & Boo, H. C. (2011). A literature review of the anchoring effect. The Journal of Socio-
Economics, 40(1), 35-42. doi:10.1016/j.socec.2010.10.008
Furnham, A., Boo, H. C., & McClelland, A. (2012). Individual differences and the susceptibility to the
influence of anchoring cues. Journal of Individual Differences, 33(2), 89-93. doi:10.1027/1614-
0001/a000076
Gamliel, E., Kreiner, H., & Garcia-Retamero, R. (2015). The moderating role of objective and subjective
numeracy in attribute framing. International Journal of Psychology, 51(2), 109-116.
doi:10.1002/ijop.12138
Ghazal, S., Cokely, E. T., & Garcia-Retamero, R. (2014). Predicting biases in very highly educated
samples: Numeracy and metacognition. Judgment and Decision Making, 9(1), 15-34.
Gigerenzer, G., & Gaissmaier, W. (2010). Heuristic decision making. Annual Review of Psychology, 62(1),
451-482. doi:10.1146/annurev-psych-120709-145346
Goldstein, D. G., & Gigerenzer, G. (2002). Models of ecological rationality: The recognition heuristic.
Psychological Review, 109(1), 75-90. doi:10.1037//0033-295X.109.1.75
Haerem, T., Kuvaas, B., Bakken, B. T., & Karlsen, T. (2011). Do military decision makers behave as
predicted by prospect theory? Journal of Behavioral Decision Making, 24(5), 482-497.
doi:10.1002/bdm.704
Haselton, M. G., Nettle, D., & Andrews, P. W. (2005). The evolution of cognitive bias. In D. M. Buss
(Ed.), The handbook of evolutionary psychology (pp. 724-746). Hoboken, NJ, USA: John Wiley & Sons
Inc.
Heath, C., & Tversky, A. (1991). Preference and belief: Ambiguity and competence in choice under
uncertainty. Journal of Risk and Uncertainty, 4(1), 5-28. doi:10.1007/BF00057884
12 http://aajhss.org/index.php/ijhss
Hogarth, R. M. (2010). Intuition: A challenge for psychological research on decision making. Psychological
Inquiry, 21(4), 338-353. doi:10.1080/1047840X.2010.520260
Jasper, J. D., Bhattacharya, C., Levin, I. P., Jones, L., & Bossard, E. (2013). Numeracy as a predictor of
adaptive risky decision making. Journal of Behavioral Decision Making, 26(2), 164-173.
doi:10.1002/bdm.1748
John, O. P., & Srivastava, S. (1999). The big five trait taxonomy: History, measurement, and theoretical
perspectives. In L. A. Pervin & O. P. John (Eds.), Handbook of personality: Theory and research (2nd
ed.) (Vol. 2, pp. 102-138). New York, NY: Guilford Press.
Kahneman, D. (2003). A perspective on judgment and choice: Mapping bounded rationality. American
Psychologist, 58(9), 697-720. doi:10.1037/0003-066X.58.9.697
Kahneman, D., & Frederick, S. (2002). Representativeness revisited: Attribute substitution in intuitive
judgment. In T. Gilovich, D. Griffin, & D. Kahneman (Eds.), Heuristics and biases: The psychology of
intuitive judgment (pp. 49-81). New York, NY, US: Cambridge University Press.
Kahneman, D., & Frederick, S. (2007). Frames and brains: Elicitation and control of response tendencies.
Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 11(2), 45-46. doi:10.1016/j.tics.2006.11.007
Kahneman, D., & Klein, G. (2009). Conditions for intuitive expertise: A failure to disagree. American
Psychologist, 64(6), 515-526. doi:10.1037/a0016755
Kim, S. H., & Hamann, S. (2007). Neural correlates of positive and negative emotion regulation. Journal of
Cognitive Neuroscience, 19(5), 776-798. doi:10.1162/jocn.2007.19.5.776
Klein, G. (2015). Naturalistic decision making suggestions for improving intuitive decision making. Journal
of Applied Research in Memory and Cognition, 4(3), 164-168. doi:10.1016/j.jarmac.2015.07.001
Klein, G. A. (1993). A recognition-primed decision (rpd) model of rapid decision making. In G. A. Klein,
J. Orasanu, R. Calderwood, & C. Zsambok (Eds.), Decision making in action: Models and methods (pp.
138-147). Norwood, NJ: Ablex Publishing Corporation.
Kurzban, R., Duckworth, A., Kable, J. W., & Myers, J. (2013). An opportunity cost model of subjective
effort and task performance. The Behavioral and brain sciences, 36(6), 661-679.
doi:10.1017/S0140525X12003196
Kuzmanovic, B., Jefferson, A., & Vogeley, K. (2015). Self-specific optimism bias in belief updating is
associated with high trait optimism. Journal of Behavioral Decision Making, 28(3), 281-293.
doi:10.1002/bdm.1849
Lu, Y. (2015). Is experiential-intuitive cognitive style more inclined to err on conjunction fallacy than
analytical-rational cognitive style? Frontiers in Psychology, 6(Article nr. 85), 1-8.
doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2015.00085
McElroy, T., & Dowd, K. (2007). Susceptibility to anchoring effects: How openness-to-experience
influences responses to anchoring cues. Judgment and Decision Making, 2(1), 48-53.
Miller, P. M., & Fagley, N. S. (1991). The effects of framing, problem variations, and providing rationale
on choice. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 17(5), 517-522. doi:10.1177/0146167291175006
Nisbett, R. E., Peng, K., Choi, I., & Norenzayan, A. (2001). Culture and systems of thought: Holistic
versus analytic cognition. Psychological Review, 108(2), 291-310. doi:10.1037//0033-295X.108.2.291
Oechssler, J., Roider, A., & Schmitz, P. W. (2009). Cognitive abilities and behavioral biases. Journal of
Economic Behavior & Organization, 72(1), 147-152.
Ohlert, C. R., & Weißenberger, B. E. (2015). Beating the base-rate fallacy: An experimental approach on
the effectiveness of different information presentation formats. Journal of Management Control,
26(1), 51-80. doi:10.1007/s00187-015-0205-2
Peters, E., Västfjäll, D., Slovic, P., Mertz, C. K., Mazzocco, K., & Dickert, S. (2006). Numeracy and
decision making. Psychological Science, 17(5), 407-413. doi:10.1111/j.1467-9280.2006.01720.x
Pohl, R. F., Bender, M., & Lachmann, G. (2002). Hindsight bias around the world. Experimental Psychology,
49(4), 270-282. doi:10.1026//1618-3169.49.4.270
Raab, M., & Laborde, S. (2011). When to blink and when to think. Research Quarterly for Exercise and Sport,
82(1), 89-98. doi:10.1080/02701367.2011.10599725
Rolls, E. T. (2004). The functions of the orbitofrontal cortex. Brain and Cognition, 55(1), 11-29.
doi:10.1016/S0278-2626(03)00277-X
Rolls, E. T., & Grabenhorst, F. (2008). The orbitofrontal cortex and beyond: From affect to decision-
making. Progress in Neurobiology, 86(3), 216-244. doi:10.1016/j.pneurobio.2008.09.001
Sander, D., Grafman, J., & Zalla, T. (2003). The human amygdala: An evolved system for relevance
detection. Reviews in the Neurosciences, 14(4), 303-316. doi:10.1515/REVNEURO.2003.14.4.303
13 http://aajhss.org/index.php/ijhss
Santos, L. R., & Rosati, A. G. (2015). The evolutionary roots of human decision making. Annual Review of
Psychology, 66(1), 321-347. doi:10.1146/annurev-psych-010814-015310
Sauter, V. L. (1999). Intuitive decision-making. Communications of the ACM, 42(6), 109-115.
doi:10.1145/303849.303869
Scheier, M. F., Carver, C. S., & Bridges, M. W. (1994). Distinguishing optimism from neuroticism (and
trait anxiety, self-mastery, and self-esteem): A reevaluation of the life orientation test. Journal of
Personality and Social Psychology, 67(6), 1063-1078. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.67.6.1063
Scott, W. G., Christopher, J. R., & John, G., I. (1998). The impact of cultural values on escalation of
commitment. The International Journal of Organizational Analysis, 6(2), 165-176.
doi:10.1108/eb028883
Sharot, T. (2011). The optimism bias. Current Biology, 21(23), R941-R945. doi:10.1016/j.cub.2011.10.030
Shiloh, S., Salton, E., & Sharabi, D. (2002). Individual differences in rational and intuitive thinking styles
as predictors of heuristic responses and framing effects. Personality and Individual Differences, 32(3),
415-429. doi:10.1016/S0191-8869(01)00034-4
Sieck, W., & Yates, J. F. (1997). Exposition effects on decision making: Choice and confidence in choice.
Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 70(3), 207-219. doi:10.1006/obhd.1997.2706
Simon, A. F., Fagley, N. S., & Halleran, J. G. (2004). Decision framing: Moderating effects of individual
differences and cognitive processing. Journal of Behavioral Decision Making, 17(2), 77-93.
doi:10.1002/bdm.463
Simon, H. A. (1972). Theories of bounded rationality. In C. B. McGuire & R. Radner (Eds.), Decision and
organization (pp. 161-176). Amsterdam, The Netherlands: North-Holland Publishing Company.
Sinayev, A., & Peters, E. (2015). Cognitive reflection vs. Calculation in decision making. Front Psychol,
6(Article 532), 1-16. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2015.00532
Sloman, S. A. (1996). The empirical case for two systems of reasoning. Psychological Bulletin, 119(1), 3-22.
doi:10.1037/0033-2909.119.1.3
Söllner, A., Broeder, A., & Hilbig, B. E. (2013). Deliberation versus automaticity in decision making:
Which presentation format features facilitate automatic decision making? Judgment and Decision
Making, 8(3), 278-298.
Stanovich, K. E., & West, R. F. (2008). On the relative independence of thinking biases and cognitive
ability. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 94(4), 672-695. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.94.4.672
Strutton, D., & Carter, W. (2013). Reducing biases in cross-cultural top management team decision-
making processes. International Journal of Business Administration, 4(3), 1-13. doi:10.5430/ijba.v4n3p1
Svedholm-Häkkinen, A. M. (2015). Highly reflective reasoners show no signs of belief inhibition. Acta
Psychologica, 154(0), 69-76. doi:10.1016/j.actpsy.2014.11.008
Takemura, K. (1993). The effect of decision frame and decision justification on risky choice. Japanese
Psychological Research, 35(1), 36-40. doi:10.4992/psycholres1954.35.36
Teovanovic, P., Knezevic, G., & Stankov, L. (2015). Individual differences in cognitive biases: Evidence
against one-factor theory of rationality. Intelligence, 50, 75-86. doi:10.1016/j.intell.2015.02.008
Trippas, D., Pennycook, G., Verde, M. F., & Handley, S. J. (2015). Better but still biased: Analytic
cognitive style and belief bias. Thinking & Reasoning, online, 1-15.
doi:10.1080/13546783.2015.1016450
Tversky, A., & Kahneman, D. (1974). Judgment under uncertainty: Heuristics and biases. Science,
185(4157), 1124-1131. doi:10.1126/science.185.4157.1124
Tversky, A., & Kahneman, D. (1981). The framing of decisions and the psychology of choice. Science,
211(4481), 453-458. doi:10.1126/science.7455683
Verplanken, B., & Pieters, R. G. M. (1988). Individual differences in reverse hindsight bias: I never
thought something like chernobyl would happen. Did I? Journal of Behavioral Decision Making, 1(3),
131-147. doi:10.1002/bdm.3960010302
Welsh, M. B., Delfabbro, P. H., Burns, N. R., & Begg, S. H. (2014). Individual differences in anchoring:
Traits and experience. Learning and Individual Differences, 29, 131-140.
doi:10.1016/j.lindif.2013.01.002
Yama, H., Manktelow, K. I., Mercier, H., Henst, J.-B. V. d., Soo Do, K., Kawasaki, Y., & Adachi, K.
(2010). A cross-cultural study of hindsight bias and conditional probabilistic reasoning. Thinking
& Reasoning, 16(4), 346-371. doi:10.1080/13546783.2010.526786
14 http://aajhss.org/index.php/ijhss
Yip, J. A., & Côté, S. (2013). The emotionally intelligent decision maker: Emotion-understanding ability
reduces the effect of incidental anxiety on risk taking. Psychological Science, 24(1), 48-55.
doi:10.1177/0956797612450031
Yoder, C. Y., Mancha, R., & Agrawal, N. (2014). Culture-related factors affect sunk cost bias. Behavioral
Development Bulletin, 19(4), 105-118. doi:10.1037/h0101086
Zald, D. H. (2003). The human amygdala and the emotional evaluation of sensory stimuli. Brain Research
Reviews, 41(1), 88-123. doi:10.1016/S0165-0173(02)00248-5
Appendix A : Terminology
Anchoring: a tendency to make decisions biased toward previously presented information (the
"anchor").
Attribution error: the tendency to see behavior as a product of the actor's dispositions and to ignore
important situational determinants of the behavior.
Base rate neglect: a tendency to ignore statistical information (prior probabilities) and focus on
information only pertaining to a certain case.
Belief bias: a tendency to draw conclusions that agree with one‟s own beliefs - i.e., to evaluate the logical
strength of an argument on the basis of the believability of the conclusion.
Bias: errors in decisions that arise due to limitations of cognitive processing. Biases are often explained
using dual-process theory, which states that we have two cognitive systems, one that is fast and intuitive,
and another that is slow and deliberate. Biases occur when our fast system operates without the oversight
of the slow system.
Cognitive bias: a consistent deviation from an accurate perception or
judgmentjudgment of the world. Inferences about other people and situations may be drawn in an
illogical fashion. Individuals create their own "subjective social reality" from their perception of the input.
Cognitive reflection test: test to assess the ability or disposition to resist reporting the response that first
comes to mind.
Confirmation bias: a tendency to search for, interpret, focus on and remember information in a way that
confirms one's preconceptions.
Conjunction fallacy: a combination of conditions is considered more likely than a general condition.
Crystallized intelligence: the ability to use skills, knowledge, and experience.
Fluid intelligence: the capacity to think logically and solve problems in novel situations, independent of
acquired knowledge. It is the ability to analyze novel problems, identify patterns and relationships that
underpin these problems and the extrapolation of these using logic.
Framing: a bias in decision making depending on the way in the information is presented (e.g., whether
options are presented in terms of gains or loss).
Halo effect: a tendency to let the perceived valence of a single aspect dominate the overall judgment of a
person or situation.
Heuristics: simple decision rules (rules of thumb) that ignore part of the available information but work
well in a given environment.
Hindsight bias: the tendency to erroneously perceive events as inevitable or more likely once they have
occurred.
15 http://aajhss.org/index.php/ijhss
Imaginability bias: the tendency to use our imagination to make a subjective premonition of a future
event for which no memories of actual instances come to mind.
Need for cognition: an individual‟s propensity to enjoy and engage in thought.
Negativity bias: a tendency to weigh negative information more heavily than positive information.
Numeracy: the ability to process basic probability and numerical concepts.
Omission bias: the tendency to prefer harm caused by omissions over equal or lesser harm caused by
acts.
Optimism bias: the tendency to overestimate the likelihood of positive events (and to underestimate the
likelihood of negative events) happening to oneself, compared to others.
Outcome bias: a tendency to evaluate the quality of a decision based on its outcome rather than on what
factors led to the decision.
Over-confidence bias: an inclination of individuals to overestimate their own abilities to successfully
perform a particular task.
Sunk cost effect: a tendency to persist in an endeavor once an investment of money, effort, or time has
already been made.
18 http://aajhss.org/index.php/ijhss
International Journal of Humanities and Social Sciences
p-ISSN: 1694-2620
e-ISSN: 1694-2639
Vol. 8 No. 5, pp. 18-26, ©IJHSS
Likert Scale Development: Construction and Evaluation of
Home Environment Scale
Mukhtar Ahmad Wani & Aejaz Masih
Department of Educational Studies
Jamia Millia Islamia (A Central University)
New Delhi, India- 110025
Abstract
The purpose of present paper is the construction and evaluation of home environment scale. An
attempt was made in this study to evaluate validity, reliability and to determine the appropriate
standards to interpret the results of home environment scale. The final form of the scale includes
ten sub-scales (dimensions) with seventy one (71) items/statements. The scale was validated for
its content by more than 20 experts from the related and allied fields. The construct validity was
calculated through factor analysis for items, and by correlating the score of each sub-scale with
the total score of the sale. t test for two independent samples was used (high group 27% and low
group27%) to find the discrimination validity for each sub-scale. The reliability for the whole
scale as well as for each sub-scale was calculated through Alpha coefficients. From the reliability
coefficients, it can be concluded that the scale is reliable as all the values are above .70. Finally,
the appropriate standards for interpretation of scores based on stanine procedures are
highlighted. However, interpreting the scores through stanine the investigator has to calculate z
values of raw scores after the data have been collected from all the sample participants. The
stanine procedure is the standardized technique for the categorization of the scores for
meaningful interpretation of Likert scale responses.
Keywords: Construction, Evaluation, Home Environment, Likert Scale, Reliability, Validity,
Stanine
Introduction
Various instruments have been developed for assessing the home environment in foreign as well
as in India. Some foreign scales are developed by Watson 1957, Williamson 1961, Moos 1974,
Bradley 1981 and some Indian scales by Prerna Mohete, Beena Shah, Reena Sharma, K. S. Mishra etc.
As we know the foreign tools are not culture fair and this makes them unsuitable for use in the
particular context. Another issue with these foreign scales (Moos) is that they lack reliability with
respect to adolescent sample (Boyd, C. P. et al, 1997). Other issue is their language of the items
in which they have been originally developed and their lack of measuring all the major
dimensions, that are universally acceptable on which home as a social institution is based.
Similarly, Indian scales measuring home environment are not without the pitfalls like that of
assessment of home environment for the age group of 20 to 50 years (Reena Sharma) and with
only five areas of home environment and others only devised for measuring home environment
for I-VIII class students (Prerna Mohete). While others for high school students (Beena Shah)
and others assessing only the psychological nature of the home (Mishra). Hence a complete tool
to assess the major parameters on which any institutions is based seems to be lacking in these
19 http://aajhss.org/index.php/ijhss
scales and this demands a new scale to be developed, which measures broad three parameters of
home environment which are interrelationship parameter, individual development parameter and
system organization parameter.
Objectives
1. To construct a Likert scale measuring the attitude of senior secondary school students
towards their home environment.
2. To evaluate the validity of home environment scale.
3. To evaluate the reliability of home environment scale.
4. To formulate appropriate standards to interpret the results of home environment scale.
Methodology
Descriptive statistical method was employed in the present study. The process of description as
employed in this research study goes beyond mere gathering and tabulation of data. It involves
an element of interpretation of the meaning or significance of what is described. Thus,
description is combined with comparison or contrast involving measurement, classification,
interpretation and evaluation.
Sample
The samples of the study is comprised of 106 senior secondary school students currently
enrolled in class 11th of different (Govt./Private) schools of South Kashmir of Jammu and
Kashmir during the year 2015. This study was delimited to students of class 11th. Secondly the
age range of the members of the population is 16-17 years.
Stages of tool construction
As with the tool construction, there is no total agreement of experts about the precise steps for
tool construction. Nevertheless, when constructing a tool, it is necessary to go through a number
of stages in order to ensure its good quality (Alderson, 1995). Although their needs a proper
procedure for tool construction. The graphical representation for the stages of tool construction
as depicts in figure 1.
Fig. 1: Stages of tool construction
20 http://aajhss.org/index.php/ijhss
Preparation of the preliminary draft
Having gone through the literature and previous tools as mentioned above in introduction, ten
dimensions based on three mentioned broad parameters were selected which are related to the
overall environment of home. Interrelation parameter includes dimension as Family integration,
Conflict, Self-expression and Social climate of the family. Individual development parameter
includes dimension as Guidance provided at home, Emotional support, Success orientation and
Independence and System organization parameter includes dimensions like Organization and
management of family and Control. Then the items associated with ten dimensions were selected
and each item was selected according to the nature of the dimension. For the selection of the
items, previous tools and studies related with home environment were consulted along with the
available literature. While selecting items, the nature of item measured the desired dimension of
home environment were taken into consideration. In this way the initial draft was prepared and
110 items (11 in each dimension) were included in the scale. Then, draft items were given to
experts from different universities who were well versed in the field and scale construction with a
request to review the statements and evaluate their content accuracy coverage, editorial quality
and suggestions for additions, deletion and modification of items. On the basis of 80% of
unanimously 30 items were deleted and 80 items were retained, which are reported below with
the number of items:
Table 1: Dimensions with number items in HES
Dimension No. of Items
A. Family integration 8
B. Social climate of the family 8
C. Guidance (Assistance) provided at home 8
D. Organization and management of the family 8
E. Conflict 8
F. Emotional Support 8
G. Success Orientation 8
H. Control 8
I. Self-expression 8
J. Independence 8
Try-out of the tool
The initial format with 80 items was administered on the sample of 106 higher secondary school
students from Kashmir (J&K). This is an attitude scale measuring the children‟s attitude towards
their family environment. The scale requires pupils to tell the favorableness or unfavorableness
with which a particular behavior has been observed by them in their homes, i.e., he/she is
requested to tell whether they are Strongly Agree, Agree, Undecided, Disagree and Strongly
Disagree respectively with the items in the scale.
Scoring of the Responses to HES Items
There are five options namely, “Strongly Agree”, “Agree, “Undecided, “Disagree” and “Strongly
Disagree”, for each statement of the scale. 5 marks were assigned to 'Strongly Agree', 4 marks to
'Agree', 3 marks to 'Undecided', 2 mark to „Disagree', and 1 marks to „Strongly Disagree'
responses and for negative items scoring is done in reverse order like 1,2,3,4,and 5 for Strongly
Agree, Agree, Undecided, Disagree and Strongly Agree respectively. Then the marks were
counted which were assigned to A, B, C, D, E, F, G, H, I and J dimension statements and then
they were added to get the total composite score on the particular dimension. Ten composite
21 http://aajhss.org/index.php/ijhss
scores for ten dimensions of the scale consists the children‟s attitude towards their home
environment.
Item Analysis
For assessing the item analysis bi-serial correlation was used to sharpen the scale. The responses
were collected and scored. Individual item score was correlated with the total score of the tool.
Item analysis was done for the 106 response sheets by using Item Vs Whole correlation method.
The sum of the scores on each dimension of value was calculated. Then „r‟ was calculated by
correlating the individual item and the corresponding component score. The correlation
coefficient at the 5% level of significance is 0.196 when the degree of freedom is 100 (Best, J. W.
2006). So the items having „r‟ values 0.196 and above were selected. It was found that out of the
total 80 items, there are 71 items which are having significant correlations with the total score of
the scale except 9 items which are having no significant correlation with the total score of the
tool. The correlation table is given below:
Table 2: r Values for HES
Item No. r value Item r value item r value
1 .323**
29 .298**
57 .410**
2 .510**
30 .258**
58 .302**
3 .200*
31 .219*
59 .380**
4 .590**
32 .214*
60 .543**
5 .527**
33 .349**
61 .262**
6 .574**
34 .218*
62 .431**
7 .493**
35 .106 63 .287**
8 .355**
36 .268**
64 .409**
9 .476**
37 .191*
65 .341**
10 .096 38 .289**
66 .281**
11 .372**
39 .119 67 .036
12 .559**
40 .235*
68 .436**
13 .465**
41 .331**
69 .260**
14 .629**
42 .365**
70 .315**
15 .327**
43 .230*
71 .423**
16 .299**
44 .389**
72 .309**
17 .412**
45 .334**
73 .301**
18 .238*
46 .130 74 .262**
19 .349**
47 .332**
75 .378**
20 .028 48 .226*
76 .249**
21 .487**
49 .377**
77 .343**
22 .407**
50 .435**
78 .153
23 .329**
51 .301**
79 .357**
24 .384**
52 .209*
80 .342**
25 .214*
53 .380**
Bold Italic items not selected
26 .212*
54 .322**
27 .245*
55 .141
28 .181 56 .205*
**
Correlation is Significant at 0.01
*
Correlation is Significant at 0.05
22 http://aajhss.org/index.php/ijhss
From the perusal of table 2 above, it is clearly reflected that some of the item (bold and
italics) were not having a significant correlation with the total scores of the scale and
hence were deleted. After the rejection of 9 unsuitable items from the scale, a total of 71
items in ten dimensions of home environment scale were selected which are shown
below in table 3 along with the possible range of scores:
Table 3: No. of items and range of scores in each dimension of HES
Evaluation of tool validity
When a test measures what it has been suposed to measure, is said to be valid. To determine the
validity of the test, the researchers tested face validity, construct validity and discrimination
validity.
Face validity or content validity
The content validity of the „Home Environment Scale‟ was tested by more than 20 experts. It is
evident from the assessment of experts that items of the test are directly related to the different
dimensions of Home Environment.
Construct validity
In order to find out the construct validity, the researcher calculated correlation between each
sub-scale‟s score and total score of the scale.
Table 4: Correlation between Each Dimension and Total Score
From the perusal of the table 4 above, it can be concluded that the correlation
coefficient of all dimensions (.712, .684, .609, .379, .431, .519, .513, .616, .550, and .515
respectively) are significant at .01. This indicates that all dimensions are related to
home environment and the tool has good construct validity.
Dimension No. of Items Possible range of scores
A Family integration 8 8-40
B Social climate of the family 7 7-35
C Guidance (Assistance) provided at home 7 7-35
D Organization and management of the family 7 7-35
E Conflict 6 6-30
F Emotional Support 7 7-35
G Success Orientation 7 7-35
H Control 7 7-35
I Self-expression 8 8-40
J Independence 7 7-35
Total 71 1-355
Dimension ‘r’ values Dimension ‘r’ values
A 0.712**
F 0.519**
B 0.684**
G 0.513**
C 0.609**
H 0.616**
D 0.379**
I 0.550**
E 0.431**
J 0.515**
23 http://aajhss.org/index.php/ijhss
Factor Analysis
However the scale was also subjected to exploratory factor analysis as the minimum
number of cases required for factor analysis is 100 (Kline, 1986). All the ten
components were retained as the eigenvalues are above 1. From the exploratory factor
analysis items loading .4 were selected and items below .4 were dropped from the scale.
From the factor analysis it can be concluded that all the items are measuring the same
construct.
Discrimination validity
To find out the discrimination validity of the items the researchers used item analysis
(difficulty level value and discrimination value). For knowing the level of discrimination
validity for each dimension of the scale, ‘t’ test for two independent samples was used
(high group 27% and low group 27%). Finally the discrimination validity of whole test
was also determined by using ‘t’ test. Discrimination validity for each domain and whole
test is given in the table below. It indicates that all ‘t’ values are significant at level 0.01
and the means of high group are also higher than low group which support the high
validity of home environment scale.
Table 5: t values for each dimension of the HES
**
Significant at 0.01 level
Reliability of the Home Environment Scale
The degree of consistency among test scores is called reliability. The values of reliability
coefficients (Cronbach alpha) for each sub-scale and for the whole scale are shown below:
Dimensions Group N Mean Std. D Df t value
A High
Low
28
28
28.32
14.71
1.44
1.38
54 36.03**
B High
Low
28
28
24.17
11.57
1.94
0.92
54 31.00**
C High
Low
28
28
24.17
12.50
1.18
1.47
54 32.58**
D High
Low
28
28
24.46
12.46
2.31
1.34
54 23.69**
E High
Low
28
28
19.85
9.32
2.64
1.46
54 18.41**
F High
Low
28
28
24.39
12.57
1.49
1.28
54 31.64**
G High
Low
28
28
24.32
13.21
1.46
1.47
54 28.25**
H High
Low
28
28
27.03
14.39
1.66
1.19
54 32.60**
I High
Low
28
28
24.85
13.53
1.64
1.83
54 24.27**
J High
Low
28
28
23.78
12.67
1.37
1.18
54 32.40**
TOTAL High
Low
28
28
245.39
126.64
16.60
12.93
54 29.70**
24 http://aajhss.org/index.php/ijhss
Table 6: Reliability coefficients of HES
Dimensions Alpha Dimensions Alpha
A .753 G .747
B .774 H .736
C .708 I .706
D .729 J .725
E .781 Total Reliability
of the Scale .895F .719
Final form
The final form of the scale along with serial no. of items for affirmative and negative statements
is presented in the below table 7.
Table 7: Item Presentation in the final form of HES:
Dimension Affirmative items Negative items
A Family integration 1, 2, 3, 4, 5 6, 7, 8
B Social climate of the family 10, 11, 12, 14, 15 9, 13
C Guidance (Assistance) provided at home 16, 18, 20, 21, 22 17, 18
D Organization and management of the family 23, 24, 26, 26, 29 27, 28
E Conflict 30, 31, 32 33, 34, 35
F Emotional Support 36, 37, 38, 39, 40 41, 42
G Success Orientation 43, 44, 46, 46, 47 48, 49
H Control 50, 51, 52, 54, 55 53, 56,
I Self-expression 57, 58, 59, 60, 61,
62
63, 64
J Independence 65, 66, 67, 69, 71 68, 70
Table 8: Scoring table for all dimensions of HES
Dimensions A B C D E F G H I J
Score
Mean
Stanine
The table 8 above represents the scoring for each dimension of the scale. The table is blank,
because the raw scores, mean and stanines will differ from sample to sample. The table is to be
filled after the data have been collected. The interpretation of the scores is done separately for
each dimension based on the Z value. Then the nine levels based on Z values ranging from -1.75
25 http://aajhss.org/index.php/ijhss
to +1.75 are to be divided. The stanine procedure is the standardized technique for the
categorization of the scores for meaningful interpretation.
Results
After following these steps to construct the scale and after analyzing the data from the first and
the last application by using adequate statistical methods, it has been concluded that:
 The study has produced a scale measuring the attitude of senior secondary school
students towards their home environment. This scale includes 71 items which measures
ten dimensions of home environment viz, Family integration, Social climate of the
family, Guidance (Assistance) provided at home, Organization and management of the
family, Conflict, Emotional Support, Success Orientation, Control, Self-expression and
Independence.
 The scale has been validated through content, construct and discrimination validity. The
content validity has been evaluated by experts, construct validity has been calculated by
Pearson‟s correlation. The correlation coefficients of all dimensions are (.712, .684, .609,
.379, .431, .519, .513, .616, .550, and .515 respectively) which are significant at .01 level.
This indicates that all dimensions are related to home environment and the scale has
good construct validity. The discrimination validity has been evaluated through „t‟ test
between high group 27% and low group 27%. All „t‟ values are significant at level 0.01
and the means of high group are also higher than low group which support the high
validity of HES.
 The reliability of the scale was evaluated by calculating Alpha Cronbach Coefficient. All
reliability coefficient values are above .70. Thus home environment scale is a reliable
scale whose reliability is 0.89 and the reliability for each dimension of HES is .75, .77, .70,
.72, .78, .71, .74, .73, .70, & .72 respectively.
 To categorize the students into different categories with respect to their attitude towards
home environment, the researchers used the stanine procedures.
References
Anastasi, A. (1987). Psychological Testing. New York: Macmillan Co.
Bandhana, & Sharma, D. (2012). A Study of Home Environment and Reasoning Ability among
Secondary School Students. Developing Country Studies, 2 (1), 73-80. Retrieved from www.iiste.org.
Best, J. W., & Kahn, J. V. (2010). Research in Education. New Delhi: PHI Learning Ltd.
Biel, A. (1986). Childrens Spatial Knowledge about Their Home Environment. Children's Environments
Quarterly, 3(4), 2-9.
Boyd, C. P. et al, (1997). The Family Environment Scale: Reliability and Normative Data for an
Adolescent Sample. Family Process36:369-373, retrieved from
https://www.researchgate.net/publication/51334514
Cohen, L., Manion, L., & Morrison, K. (2007). Research Methods in Education (6th ed.). New York:
Routledge.
Cunningham, J. B., & Aldrich, J. O. (2012). Using SPSS: An Interactive Hands-On Approach. New Delhi: Sage
Publications India Pvt Ltd.
Ebel, R. L., & Frisbie. (2004). Essentials of Educational Measurement. New Delhi: PHI Learning Pvt Ltd.
Ferguson, G. A., & Yashio, T. (1989). Statistical Analysis in Psychology and Education. New York: McGraw
Hill Book Co.
Field, A. (2014). Discoevring Statistics using IBM SPSS Statiatics (4th ed.). New Delhi:Sage Publications India
Pvt Ltd.
Garret, H. E. (1973). Statistics in Psychology and Education. Bombay Vakils: Feffer & Simons Pvt Ltd.
Guilford, J. P. (1987). Psychometric Methods. New York: McGraw Hill.
Guilford, J. P., & Beryamin, F. (1946). Fundamental Statistics in Psychology and Education. Singapore:
McGraw Hill Book Co.
26 http://aajhss.org/index.php/ijhss
Gupta, N., Joshi, R. & Pasbola, H. (2008). Effect of Home Environment upon Emotional Maturity
among female adolescents of Joint and Nuclear Family Structure. Behavioural Scientist, 9(2): 87-
92.
Kundu, G. (1975). A New Technique of Attitude Measurement. Calcutta: Annupurna Publishing House
McLeod, S. A. (2008). Likert Scale. Retrieved from www.simplypsychology.org/likertscale.htm
Mishra, K. S. (1989). Manual of Home Environment Inventory. Allahabad: Ankur Psychological
Agency, India.
Mishra, S., & Bamba, V. (2012). Impact of family environment on academic achievement of
secondary school students in science subject. International Journal of Research in Economics & Social
Sciences, 2(5), 42-49.
Mohanraj, R and Latha (2005). Perceived family environment in relation of adjustment and academic
achievement. Journal of the Indian Academy of Applied Psychology: 31(1-2): 18-23.
Thondike, R. M., & Tracy, C. T. (2011). Measurement and Evaluation in Psychology. New Delhi: PHI Learning
Pvt Ltd.
Wani, M. A., & Masih, A. (2015). Facilitating Learning by linking the two Environments: Interface
between Home and School through Technology. International Education Conference on Learning
Technologies in Education (pp. 334-342). New Delhi: Excel India Publishers.
27 http://aajhss.org/index.php/ijhss
International Journal of Humanities and Social Sciences
p-ISSN: 1694-2620
e-ISSN: 1694-2639
Vol. 8 No. 5, pp. 27-36, ©IJHSS
Iqbal’s Response to Modern Western Thought: A Critical
Analysis
Dr. Mohammad Nayamat Ullah
Associate Professor
Department of Arabic
University of Chittagong, Bangladesh
Abdullah Al Masud
PhD Researcher
Dept. of Usuluddin and Comparative Religion
International Islamic University Malaysia (IIUM)
ABSTRACT
Muhammad Iqbal (1873-1938) is a prominent philosopher and great thinker in Indian Sub-
continent as well as a dominant figure in the literary history of the East. His thought and
literature are not simply for his countrymen or for the Muslim Ummah alone but for the whole
of humanity. He explores his distinctive thoughts on several issues related to Western concepts
and ideologies. Iqbal had made precious contribution to the reconstruction of political thoughts.
The main purpose of the study is to present Iqbal‟s distinctive thoughts and to evaluate the
merits and demerits of modern political thoughts. The analytical, descriptive and criticism
methods have been applied in conducting the research through comprehensive study of his
writings both in the form of prose and poetry in various books, articles, and conferences. It is
expected that the study would identify distinctive political thought by Iqbal. It also demonstrates
differences between modern thoughts and Iqbalic thoughts of politics.
Keywords: Iqbal, western thoughts, democracy, nationalism, secularism
1. INTRODUCTION
Iqbal was not only a great poet-philosopher of the East but was also among the profound,
renowned scholars and a brilliant political thinker in the twentieth century of the world. The
contemporary scholars acknowledge his influence in modern movement. His literature was a
source of inspiration for the hopeless Muslims who had subtle ability, potential and intellectual
capability. He endeavoured substantially for the sake of mankind. He can be best described as
the embodiment of “being a beacon of light in the dark night of the wilderness” (Khalil,
2000:182), a characteristic most needed for the Muslim Ummah. Iqbal was a „warner‟ who made a
clarion call to the oppressed of the world to rediscover the infinite sources of their creative
power and develops their faith. His message was not simply for his countrymen, nor for the
Muslim Ummah alone but for the whole of humanity (Munawwar: 1982). This is study
investigates modern western thoughts of Iqbal from Islamic perspective. It also attempts to
clarify Iqbal‟s position on the modern Western political philosophy. In particular, his views on
democracy, nationalism, secularism, capitalism, socialism and Liberalism are analyzed.
28 http://aajhss.org/index.php/ijhss
2. BIOGRAPHICAL SKETCH OF IQBAL AND HIS WORKS
Iqbal was born at Sialkot in the Punjab province of present Pakistan on 9 November, 1877. He
hailed from a family descended from Kashmiri Brahmins of Supra-caste1
, who had embraced
Islam in 17th Century (Mustansir: 2006). Iqbal refers to his Brahmin ancestry in several writings.
His father Shaikh Noor Mohammad was a skilled and enterprising businessman but a pious Sufi
saint and a God loving man (Munawwar: 1982). His mother Imam Bibi was a generous and kind
but also a deeply religious woman.
Iqbal completed his early education in Sialkot and migrated to Lahore, Pakistan in 1895. He
studied under a teacher named Maulvi Mir Hasan (1844-1929), who was a renowned scholar of
Islamic Studies, Persian and Arabic. His teacher was impressed by the inborn poetic talent of
Iqbal and encouraged him to continue his writing (Munawwar: 1982). On May 5, 1893, Iqbal was
admitted to the Scotch Mission College2
in Sialkot and successfully passed his intermediate
examination in1895. At the same year he got enrolled at B.A. in the Lahore Government College.
In 1897 he graduated and got admit in the same collage for Master‟s Program in the Department
of Philosophy. Iqbal‟s academic performance was excellent and continued brilliant record result
at all level and won many gold medals (Mir, 2006).
At Lahore Iqbal came under the influence of Sir Thomas Arnold, who was a profound professor
in Philosophy and had a echoing insight in Arabic and Islamic Studies. Thomas‟s role influenced
him to study the Western thoughts and instigated him into the modern methods of criticism. In
1899, he obtained the Master‟s degree and was appointed as a Macleod Reader in the Oriental
College, Lahore, whereas in March 1904, he joined as an assistance Professor in the Department
of Philosophy and English at the Government College, Lahore (Hafeez: 1971). As Iqbal was
advised by Thomas Arnold, he intended to go Europe for higher education in 1905. Iqbal
studied both in England and Germany. He studied in England at the Lincoln‟s Inn qualifying for
Bar (Hilal: 1995) as a barrister and at the Trinity College of Cambridge, where he studied with R.
A. Nicolson, a renowned orientalist and John M. E. McTaggart, idealist Metaphysician. At the
same time he went to Germany and got admitted in the University of Munich, where he was
awarded his PhD degree on his dissertation entitled “The Development of Metaphysics in
Persia” on November 4, 1907 (Hilal: 1995).
Iqbal returned from Europe to Lahore in August 1908 (Vahid: 1948) and began his professional
career as Professor, lawyer and poet. During his study in Europe, he developed his ideas and
thoughts, where “he was vivacious, gregarious, eager to engage himself in scholarly discussion
with his juniors and superiors and politely self-assertive” (Hilal: 1995:36). His stay in Europe
provided him with the opportunity to observe carefully the realities of Western societies. At his
time, the modern methods of criticism in true sense had not been developed in India. As Iqbal
employed the method of comparison between Islamic thoughts and Western ones, critical
analysis of then several philosophical aspects and so on, he was regarded as one of the pioneers
of the modern methods of criticism. He realized the shortcomings and dark aspects of modern
Western civilization, where chaos and crisis are visible in human life more than ever. That is why,
1
According to Vanina, in mediaeval Indian regions there existed big supra-caste bound by status, corporate
interests and common socio-ethical and moral values (2012: 139) which oversaws the political, economic and
juridical spheres of everyday existence (Gadgil & Guha, 1993: 113). This supra-caste presupposed a certain
hierarchy, and contradictions and conflicts within each of them (Vanina, 2012: 134). There are various castes;
however, in Indian societies who are peculiar in terms of beliefs, culture and social phenomenon which
explanation is not relevant to this article. As long time ago, Iqbal’s ancestors embraced Islam, generation after
generation the influence and traditions of castes gradually disappeared
2
This college was later renamed Murray Collage, which exists under that name.
29 http://aajhss.org/index.php/ijhss
he highly criticized Western political thoughts and civilization as well. Although, Iqbal spent his
time in law practice, cultural activities and literary works, he later took part in politics and
became an elected Legislative Council of the Punjab in 1926. In 1930, he was appointed
president of the annual session of the Muslim League and delivered a remarkable address which
led to a new idea of a separate Muslim state in Indian subcontinent. (Mujahid: 1986). He also
represented India at the Round Table Conference in London in 1931 and 1932 (Vahid: 1948).
Iqbal was a creative author in both poetry and prose a he left prolific intellectual works for the
next generation. He wrote 17 books in Persian, Urdu and English. Among his 12,000 verses of
poetry, about 7,000 verses are in Persian3
. Asrar-e-Khudi (Secrets of the Self) was the first
work of Iqbal in Persian composed in 1919, which deals with human personality or individual
self. Bang-i Dara (The Call of the Caravan Bell) was the first work in Urdu which was written in
1924 (Hilal: 1995). Iqbal also composed two books in English on the topic of “The Department of
Metaphysics in Persia”, which is a valuable contribution in the history of Muslim Philosophy, and
“The Reconstruction of Religious Thought in Islam”, where he attempted to restructure the history of
Islamic thought. Besides these, he wrote hundreds of articles, short essays, and letters as well as
issued several statements. All these publications would help researchers to understand the real
perception of Iqbal in various subjects. It is stated by Ali (1978:17) that, “… a leading exponent
of recent interpretations of Islam, Iqbal was in correspondence with people from all walks of life.
Religious leaders, journalists, politicians and scholars were his life-long correspondents with
whom he exchanged views on various subjects”. William O. Douglas, justice of the Supreme
Court of USA says: “that (Iqbal's) simple tomb is a place of pilgrimage for me. For Iqbal was a
man who belonged to all races; his concepts had universal appeal. He spoke to the conscience of
men of goodwill whatever their tongue, whatever their creed” (Hafeez, 1971).
Among the Muslim scholars of South Asia, Iqbal was the first who defined the concept of state
in Islam. In 1930, Iqbal delivered a famous presidential speech at Allahabad where he pointed
out a way for the political deadlock in the Indian subcontinent. He also stated: “I would like to
see the Punjab, North West Frontier Province, Sindh and Baluchistan amalgamated into a single
state of self- government within the British Empire or without the British Empire...I therefore,
demand the formation of a consolidated Muslim state in the best interests of India and Islam”
(Sing & Roy, 2011: 174). This statement proves that Iqbal wanted a Muslim state for the Muslims
in North- West of India where Muslim could develop and nurture their culture, religion,
language, literature besides other religious communities of India. For long time of the past, two
religions (Muslims and Hindus) were dominating. To the great extent peace, social security and
justice in India were dependent on their mutual understanding. Islamic ideologies of unity,
equity, humanity and justice played an important role through ages all over the globe in
alleviating any differences among human beings. Indeed, Iqbal emphasized this fact. Thus, he
also believed that the peace and prosperity of India would depend upon Muslims and Hindus
when the spirit of mutual understanding, love and unity in diversity will be maintained. However,
he sought a separate state for Muslim; to maintain the spiritual life of Muslim community; to
protect the Muslim culture from harmful influences4
and rapid invasion of un-Islamic elements.
In the beginning of 1938, Iqbal‟s health sharply declined and it took a serious turn for the worse
on March 25, 1938. Finally, he passed away at 5:15 in the morning on April 21, 1938.
Rabindronath Tagore sent a condolence message as soon as he heard the demise news of Allama
Iqbal that, “The death of Sir Mohammad Iqbal has created a void in literature which like a deep wound, will
3
2 http://www.poemhunter.com/allama-muhammad-iqbal/biography/ Retrieved on 20-07-2016, and
https://en.wikipedia.org/wiki/Muhammad_Iqbal Retrieved on 07-10-2016
4
Harmful influences or behaviours that are not allowed in Islam like polytheism, injustice and any unethical
behaviour such as free mixing, drug addiction, illegal sexual relation and so on.
30 http://aajhss.org/index.php/ijhss
take a long time to heal. India which occupies but a limited place in the world can ill afford to lose a poet whose
poetry has such appeal” (Maire, 1981: 4).
3. IQBAL ON MODERN WESTERN THOUGHTS
Muhammad Iqbal was not only a prominent scholar, writer, poet and philosopher but also he
was a political and social reformer of the Indian subcontinent. He was the first personality who
generated a unique idea of an independent Muslim state in Indian subcontinent. Through His
scholarly writings he more detailed his philosophical thoughts and ideas, especially his political
ideas. His political thoughts are deeply rooted in Islamic values which are derived from the Holy
Quran and the Sunnah. According to Iqbal, “Islam believes in a universal polity- a politico-
religious system or social polity- based on fundamentals that were revealed to the Prophet” (Sh.
Muhammad Ashraf, 1952:238) His attention was to protect the Muslim Ummah from the attack
of Western thought and to encourage and preserve their own cultural heritage and thoughts.
3.1 DEMOCRACY
Democracy is a political system of a state and government where sovereignty and power belong
5to the majority people and the government is elected by people‟s opinion through election.
That is why democracy as a political ideal was defined by Abraham Lincoln as “Democracy is a
government of the people, for the people, by the people”, while Alex Woolf (2007:4) described it
as “a system in which people decide matters together, or collectively”. Haneef Nadvi (1973:205)
explains that, “Democracy is composed of two Greek components; one means the people and
the other means government and law”. Technically, democracy is applied to a system of
government for the majority.
Iqbal was a democrat, who was actively involved with the politics of Indian subcontinent. In
1926, Iqbal participated in the election, where he was elected to the Punjab Legislative council.
L.S. May (1974:179) states that Iqbal was “an active member of this Council, speaking often on
land revenue and taxation, demanding greater justice in land assessment and even land revenue
deductions in hardship cases”. Although Iqbal preferred democracy, he has pointed out some
demerits in this regard. Iqbal 1opposed the Western democracy as it is a methodology rather
than an ideology or philosophy (Qureshi, 1983). He had his own observations and he suggested a
new term known as „spiritual democracy‟5
(Iqbal, 2011a:180), which is based on the Quranic
teaching. This spiritual democracy means, “A democracy where laws of God Almighty are
observed and enforced” (Munawwar, 2001:142). In Western democracy, sovereignty belongs to
the people. As opposed to this, sovereignty in Iqbal‟s “spiritual democracy” belongs to God
alone, which is an inseparable part in the Islamic world-view.
Iqbal was a lover of innovations. That‟s why when democracy evolved as a system of
government; he welcomed it as thought that the new system might be helpful in alleviating the
sufferings of the exploited and oppressed people (Abbas, 1997: xxiii). But he soon realized its
serious drawbacks and became a strong critic of the Western democratic form of government
where persons are counted, not weighted; it is a material fact but not of personality, which is a
spiritual fact. He says that, Democracy is a form of government in which People are counted but
their worth is not assessed (Iqbal, 1941:150). According to him, humanity needs three things
today “a spiritual interpretation of the universe, spiritual emancipation of mankind and basic
5
The Spiritual democracy is based on the principle of Tawhid, which speaks bout unity of thoughts, action and
value across humanity (Begum, 2001: 21). In case of spiritual democracy the Quran, Prophet’s Tradition, Ijma
and Qiyas are the Sources of the Islamic law, which would be interpreted according to the demand of ages. This
kind of democracy distinguishes man with significance that is sacred kind. ‘This democracy also recognizes
value of individual and rejects blood relationship as a basis of human unity’ (Iqbal, 2011a:116).
Vol 8 No 5 - October 2016
Vol 8 No 5 - October 2016
Vol 8 No 5 - October 2016
Vol 8 No 5 - October 2016
Vol 8 No 5 - October 2016
Vol 8 No 5 - October 2016
Vol 8 No 5 - October 2016
Vol 8 No 5 - October 2016
Vol 8 No 5 - October 2016
Vol 8 No 5 - October 2016
Vol 8 No 5 - October 2016
Vol 8 No 5 - October 2016
Vol 8 No 5 - October 2016
Vol 8 No 5 - October 2016
Vol 8 No 5 - October 2016
Vol 8 No 5 - October 2016
Vol 8 No 5 - October 2016
Vol 8 No 5 - October 2016

More Related Content

Similar to Vol 8 No 5 - October 2016

Lovely Bones Essay.pdf
Lovely Bones Essay.pdfLovely Bones Essay.pdf
Lovely Bones Essay.pdfAngel Morris
 
DRUGS New agreement to tackle pharmaceutical pollution p.1
DRUGS New agreement to tackle pharmaceutical pollution p.1DRUGS New agreement to tackle pharmaceutical pollution p.1
DRUGS New agreement to tackle pharmaceutical pollution p.1AlyciaGold776
 
The Outsider Essay. The Outsider. - GCSE English - Marked by Teachers.com
The Outsider Essay. The Outsider. - GCSE English - Marked by Teachers.comThe Outsider Essay. The Outsider. - GCSE English - Marked by Teachers.com
The Outsider Essay. The Outsider. - GCSE English - Marked by Teachers.comElizabeth Montes
 
Collective mind futurist perspective
Collective mind  futurist perspectiveCollective mind  futurist perspective
Collective mind futurist perspectiveAziz Alizadeh
 
In Essay Writing Looking For Ideas. Online assignment writing service.
In Essay Writing Looking For Ideas. Online assignment writing service.In Essay Writing Looking For Ideas. Online assignment writing service.
In Essay Writing Looking For Ideas. Online assignment writing service.Sandy Rodriguez
 
Causal Argument Essay Examples
Causal Argument Essay ExamplesCausal Argument Essay Examples
Causal Argument Essay ExamplesSandra Hughes
 
COGNITIVE REPAIRSHOW ORGANIZATIONAL PRACTICESCANCOMPENS.docx
COGNITIVE REPAIRSHOW ORGANIZATIONAL PRACTICESCANCOMPENS.docxCOGNITIVE REPAIRSHOW ORGANIZATIONAL PRACTICESCANCOMPENS.docx
COGNITIVE REPAIRSHOW ORGANIZATIONAL PRACTICESCANCOMPENS.docxmary772
 
COGNITIVE REPAIRSHOW ORGANIZATIONAL PRACTICESCANCOMPENS.docx
COGNITIVE REPAIRSHOW ORGANIZATIONAL PRACTICESCANCOMPENS.docxCOGNITIVE REPAIRSHOW ORGANIZATIONAL PRACTICESCANCOMPENS.docx
COGNITIVE REPAIRSHOW ORGANIZATIONAL PRACTICESCANCOMPENS.docxmccormicknadine86
 
The Psychology of Creative Thinking
The Psychology of Creative ThinkingThe Psychology of Creative Thinking
The Psychology of Creative ThinkingJoseph Knox
 
Vol 6 No 1 - November 2015
Vol 6 No 1 - November 2015Vol 6 No 1 - November 2015
Vol 6 No 1 - November 2015ijhssnet
 
Value Education Essay. Importance of Value Education: Essay amp; Speech Leve...
Value Education Essay. Importance of Value Education: Essay amp; Speech  Leve...Value Education Essay. Importance of Value Education: Essay amp; Speech  Leve...
Value Education Essay. Importance of Value Education: Essay amp; Speech Leve...Shannon Bennett
 
Power Of Positive Thinking Essay. The Power Of Positive Thinking Summary, PDF...
Power Of Positive Thinking Essay. The Power Of Positive Thinking Summary, PDF...Power Of Positive Thinking Essay. The Power Of Positive Thinking Summary, PDF...
Power Of Positive Thinking Essay. The Power Of Positive Thinking Summary, PDF...Heather Hotovec
 

Similar to Vol 8 No 5 - October 2016 (14)

Lovely Bones Essay.pdf
Lovely Bones Essay.pdfLovely Bones Essay.pdf
Lovely Bones Essay.pdf
 
DRUGS New agreement to tackle pharmaceutical pollution p.1
DRUGS New agreement to tackle pharmaceutical pollution p.1DRUGS New agreement to tackle pharmaceutical pollution p.1
DRUGS New agreement to tackle pharmaceutical pollution p.1
 
The Outsider Essay. The Outsider. - GCSE English - Marked by Teachers.com
The Outsider Essay. The Outsider. - GCSE English - Marked by Teachers.comThe Outsider Essay. The Outsider. - GCSE English - Marked by Teachers.com
The Outsider Essay. The Outsider. - GCSE English - Marked by Teachers.com
 
Collective mind futurist perspective
Collective mind  futurist perspectiveCollective mind  futurist perspective
Collective mind futurist perspective
 
In Essay Writing Looking For Ideas. Online assignment writing service.
In Essay Writing Looking For Ideas. Online assignment writing service.In Essay Writing Looking For Ideas. Online assignment writing service.
In Essay Writing Looking For Ideas. Online assignment writing service.
 
Causal Argument Essay Examples
Causal Argument Essay ExamplesCausal Argument Essay Examples
Causal Argument Essay Examples
 
O Behave! Issue 25
O Behave! Issue 25O Behave! Issue 25
O Behave! Issue 25
 
COGNITIVE REPAIRSHOW ORGANIZATIONAL PRACTICESCANCOMPENS.docx
COGNITIVE REPAIRSHOW ORGANIZATIONAL PRACTICESCANCOMPENS.docxCOGNITIVE REPAIRSHOW ORGANIZATIONAL PRACTICESCANCOMPENS.docx
COGNITIVE REPAIRSHOW ORGANIZATIONAL PRACTICESCANCOMPENS.docx
 
COGNITIVE REPAIRSHOW ORGANIZATIONAL PRACTICESCANCOMPENS.docx
COGNITIVE REPAIRSHOW ORGANIZATIONAL PRACTICESCANCOMPENS.docxCOGNITIVE REPAIRSHOW ORGANIZATIONAL PRACTICESCANCOMPENS.docx
COGNITIVE REPAIRSHOW ORGANIZATIONAL PRACTICESCANCOMPENS.docx
 
The Psychology of Creative Thinking
The Psychology of Creative ThinkingThe Psychology of Creative Thinking
The Psychology of Creative Thinking
 
Vol 6 No 1 - November 2015
Vol 6 No 1 - November 2015Vol 6 No 1 - November 2015
Vol 6 No 1 - November 2015
 
Chapter 1 Ap Psych- Research Methods
Chapter 1 Ap Psych- Research MethodsChapter 1 Ap Psych- Research Methods
Chapter 1 Ap Psych- Research Methods
 
Value Education Essay. Importance of Value Education: Essay amp; Speech Leve...
Value Education Essay. Importance of Value Education: Essay amp; Speech  Leve...Value Education Essay. Importance of Value Education: Essay amp; Speech  Leve...
Value Education Essay. Importance of Value Education: Essay amp; Speech Leve...
 
Power Of Positive Thinking Essay. The Power Of Positive Thinking Summary, PDF...
Power Of Positive Thinking Essay. The Power Of Positive Thinking Summary, PDF...Power Of Positive Thinking Essay. The Power Of Positive Thinking Summary, PDF...
Power Of Positive Thinking Essay. The Power Of Positive Thinking Summary, PDF...
 

More from ijhssnet

Vol 9 No 6 December 2017
Vol 9 No 6 December 2017Vol 9 No 6 December 2017
Vol 9 No 6 December 2017ijhssnet
 
Special Issue - April 2015
Special Issue - April 2015Special Issue - April 2015
Special Issue - April 2015ijhssnet
 
Vol 2 No 1 - March 2015
Vol 2 No 1 - March 2015Vol 2 No 1 - March 2015
Vol 2 No 1 - March 2015ijhssnet
 
Vol 3 No 1 - May 2015
Vol 3 No 1 - May 2015Vol 3 No 1 - May 2015
Vol 3 No 1 - May 2015ijhssnet
 
Vol 4 No 1 - July 2015
Vol 4 No 1 - July 2015Vol 4 No 1 - July 2015
Vol 4 No 1 - July 2015ijhssnet
 
Vol 5 No 1 - September 2015
Vol 5 No 1 - September 2015Vol 5 No 1 - September 2015
Vol 5 No 1 - September 2015ijhssnet
 
Vol 7 No 1 - January 2016
Vol 7 No 1 - January 2016Vol 7 No 1 - January 2016
Vol 7 No 1 - January 2016ijhssnet
 
Vol 8 No 1 - March 2016
Vol 8 No 1 - March 2016Vol 8 No 1 - March 2016
Vol 8 No 1 - March 2016ijhssnet
 
Vol 8 No 2 - Special Issue
Vol 8 No 2 - Special IssueVol 8 No 2 - Special Issue
Vol 8 No 2 - Special Issueijhssnet
 
Vol 8 No 3 - June 2016
Vol 8 No 3 - June 2016Vol 8 No 3 - June 2016
Vol 8 No 3 - June 2016ijhssnet
 
Vol 8 No 4 - August 2016
Vol 8 No 4 - August 2016Vol 8 No 4 - August 2016
Vol 8 No 4 - August 2016ijhssnet
 
Vol 8 No 6 - December 2016
Vol 8 No 6 - December 2016Vol 8 No 6 - December 2016
Vol 8 No 6 - December 2016ijhssnet
 
Vol 9 No 1 - February 2017
Vol 9 No 1 - February 2017Vol 9 No 1 - February 2017
Vol 9 No 1 - February 2017ijhssnet
 
Vol 9 No 2 - April 2017
Vol 9 No 2 - April 2017Vol 9 No 2 - April 2017
Vol 9 No 2 - April 2017ijhssnet
 
Vol 9 No 3 - June 2017
Vol 9 No 3 - June 2017Vol 9 No 3 - June 2017
Vol 9 No 3 - June 2017ijhssnet
 
Vol 9 No 5 - October 2017
Vol 9 No 5 - October 2017Vol 9 No 5 - October 2017
Vol 9 No 5 - October 2017ijhssnet
 

More from ijhssnet (16)

Vol 9 No 6 December 2017
Vol 9 No 6 December 2017Vol 9 No 6 December 2017
Vol 9 No 6 December 2017
 
Special Issue - April 2015
Special Issue - April 2015Special Issue - April 2015
Special Issue - April 2015
 
Vol 2 No 1 - March 2015
Vol 2 No 1 - March 2015Vol 2 No 1 - March 2015
Vol 2 No 1 - March 2015
 
Vol 3 No 1 - May 2015
Vol 3 No 1 - May 2015Vol 3 No 1 - May 2015
Vol 3 No 1 - May 2015
 
Vol 4 No 1 - July 2015
Vol 4 No 1 - July 2015Vol 4 No 1 - July 2015
Vol 4 No 1 - July 2015
 
Vol 5 No 1 - September 2015
Vol 5 No 1 - September 2015Vol 5 No 1 - September 2015
Vol 5 No 1 - September 2015
 
Vol 7 No 1 - January 2016
Vol 7 No 1 - January 2016Vol 7 No 1 - January 2016
Vol 7 No 1 - January 2016
 
Vol 8 No 1 - March 2016
Vol 8 No 1 - March 2016Vol 8 No 1 - March 2016
Vol 8 No 1 - March 2016
 
Vol 8 No 2 - Special Issue
Vol 8 No 2 - Special IssueVol 8 No 2 - Special Issue
Vol 8 No 2 - Special Issue
 
Vol 8 No 3 - June 2016
Vol 8 No 3 - June 2016Vol 8 No 3 - June 2016
Vol 8 No 3 - June 2016
 
Vol 8 No 4 - August 2016
Vol 8 No 4 - August 2016Vol 8 No 4 - August 2016
Vol 8 No 4 - August 2016
 
Vol 8 No 6 - December 2016
Vol 8 No 6 - December 2016Vol 8 No 6 - December 2016
Vol 8 No 6 - December 2016
 
Vol 9 No 1 - February 2017
Vol 9 No 1 - February 2017Vol 9 No 1 - February 2017
Vol 9 No 1 - February 2017
 
Vol 9 No 2 - April 2017
Vol 9 No 2 - April 2017Vol 9 No 2 - April 2017
Vol 9 No 2 - April 2017
 
Vol 9 No 3 - June 2017
Vol 9 No 3 - June 2017Vol 9 No 3 - June 2017
Vol 9 No 3 - June 2017
 
Vol 9 No 5 - October 2017
Vol 9 No 5 - October 2017Vol 9 No 5 - October 2017
Vol 9 No 5 - October 2017
 

Recently uploaded

Andreas Schleicher presents at the launch of What does child empowerment mean...
Andreas Schleicher presents at the launch of What does child empowerment mean...Andreas Schleicher presents at the launch of What does child empowerment mean...
Andreas Schleicher presents at the launch of What does child empowerment mean...EduSkills OECD
 
Transparency, Recognition and the role of eSealing - Ildiko Mazar and Koen No...
Transparency, Recognition and the role of eSealing - Ildiko Mazar and Koen No...Transparency, Recognition and the role of eSealing - Ildiko Mazar and Koen No...
Transparency, Recognition and the role of eSealing - Ildiko Mazar and Koen No...EADTU
 
Basic Civil Engineering notes on Transportation Engineering & Modes of Transport
Basic Civil Engineering notes on Transportation Engineering & Modes of TransportBasic Civil Engineering notes on Transportation Engineering & Modes of Transport
Basic Civil Engineering notes on Transportation Engineering & Modes of TransportDenish Jangid
 
ĐỀ THAM KHẢO KÌ THI TUYỂN SINH VÀO LỚP 10 MÔN TIẾNG ANH FORM 50 CÂU TRẮC NGHI...
ĐỀ THAM KHẢO KÌ THI TUYỂN SINH VÀO LỚP 10 MÔN TIẾNG ANH FORM 50 CÂU TRẮC NGHI...ĐỀ THAM KHẢO KÌ THI TUYỂN SINH VÀO LỚP 10 MÔN TIẾNG ANH FORM 50 CÂU TRẮC NGHI...
ĐỀ THAM KHẢO KÌ THI TUYỂN SINH VÀO LỚP 10 MÔN TIẾNG ANH FORM 50 CÂU TRẮC NGHI...Nguyen Thanh Tu Collection
 
TỔNG HỢP HƠN 100 ĐỀ THI THỬ TỐT NGHIỆP THPT TOÁN 2024 - TỪ CÁC TRƯỜNG, TRƯỜNG...
TỔNG HỢP HƠN 100 ĐỀ THI THỬ TỐT NGHIỆP THPT TOÁN 2024 - TỪ CÁC TRƯỜNG, TRƯỜNG...TỔNG HỢP HƠN 100 ĐỀ THI THỬ TỐT NGHIỆP THPT TOÁN 2024 - TỪ CÁC TRƯỜNG, TRƯỜNG...
TỔNG HỢP HƠN 100 ĐỀ THI THỬ TỐT NGHIỆP THPT TOÁN 2024 - TỪ CÁC TRƯỜNG, TRƯỜNG...Nguyen Thanh Tu Collection
 
The Story of Village Palampur Class 9 Free Study Material PDF
The Story of Village Palampur Class 9 Free Study Material PDFThe Story of Village Palampur Class 9 Free Study Material PDF
The Story of Village Palampur Class 9 Free Study Material PDFVivekanand Anglo Vedic Academy
 
Graduate Outcomes Presentation Slides - English (v3).pptx
Graduate Outcomes Presentation Slides - English (v3).pptxGraduate Outcomes Presentation Slides - English (v3).pptx
Graduate Outcomes Presentation Slides - English (v3).pptxneillewis46
 
會考英文會考英文會考英文會考英文會考英文會考英文會考英文會考英文會考英文會考英文會考英文
會考英文會考英文會考英文會考英文會考英文會考英文會考英文會考英文會考英文會考英文會考英文會考英文會考英文會考英文會考英文會考英文會考英文會考英文會考英文會考英文會考英文會考英文
會考英文會考英文會考英文會考英文會考英文會考英文會考英文會考英文會考英文會考英文會考英文中 央社
 
DEMONSTRATION LESSON IN ENGLISH 4 MATATAG CURRICULUM
DEMONSTRATION LESSON IN ENGLISH 4 MATATAG CURRICULUMDEMONSTRATION LESSON IN ENGLISH 4 MATATAG CURRICULUM
DEMONSTRATION LESSON IN ENGLISH 4 MATATAG CURRICULUMELOISARIVERA8
 
Sternal Fractures & Dislocations - EMGuidewire Radiology Reading Room
Sternal Fractures & Dislocations - EMGuidewire Radiology Reading RoomSternal Fractures & Dislocations - EMGuidewire Radiology Reading Room
Sternal Fractures & Dislocations - EMGuidewire Radiology Reading RoomSean M. Fox
 
24 ĐỀ THAM KHẢO KÌ THI TUYỂN SINH VÀO LỚP 10 MÔN TIẾNG ANH SỞ GIÁO DỤC HẢI DƯ...
24 ĐỀ THAM KHẢO KÌ THI TUYỂN SINH VÀO LỚP 10 MÔN TIẾNG ANH SỞ GIÁO DỤC HẢI DƯ...24 ĐỀ THAM KHẢO KÌ THI TUYỂN SINH VÀO LỚP 10 MÔN TIẾNG ANH SỞ GIÁO DỤC HẢI DƯ...
24 ĐỀ THAM KHẢO KÌ THI TUYỂN SINH VÀO LỚP 10 MÔN TIẾNG ANH SỞ GIÁO DỤC HẢI DƯ...Nguyen Thanh Tu Collection
 
Personalisation of Education by AI and Big Data - Lourdes Guàrdia
Personalisation of Education by AI and Big Data - Lourdes GuàrdiaPersonalisation of Education by AI and Big Data - Lourdes Guàrdia
Personalisation of Education by AI and Big Data - Lourdes GuàrdiaEADTU
 
Trauma-Informed Leadership - Five Practical Principles
Trauma-Informed Leadership - Five Practical PrinciplesTrauma-Informed Leadership - Five Practical Principles
Trauma-Informed Leadership - Five Practical PrinciplesPooky Knightsmith
 
diagnosting testing bsc 2nd sem.pptx....
diagnosting testing bsc 2nd sem.pptx....diagnosting testing bsc 2nd sem.pptx....
diagnosting testing bsc 2nd sem.pptx....Ritu480198
 
SURVEY I created for uni project research
SURVEY I created for uni project researchSURVEY I created for uni project research
SURVEY I created for uni project researchCaitlinCummins3
 
Observing-Correct-Grammar-in-Making-Definitions.pptx
Observing-Correct-Grammar-in-Making-Definitions.pptxObserving-Correct-Grammar-in-Making-Definitions.pptx
Observing-Correct-Grammar-in-Making-Definitions.pptxAdelaideRefugio
 
An overview of the various scriptures in Hinduism
An overview of the various scriptures in HinduismAn overview of the various scriptures in Hinduism
An overview of the various scriptures in HinduismDabee Kamal
 
How To Create Editable Tree View in Odoo 17
How To Create Editable Tree View in Odoo 17How To Create Editable Tree View in Odoo 17
How To Create Editable Tree View in Odoo 17Celine George
 

Recently uploaded (20)

Andreas Schleicher presents at the launch of What does child empowerment mean...
Andreas Schleicher presents at the launch of What does child empowerment mean...Andreas Schleicher presents at the launch of What does child empowerment mean...
Andreas Schleicher presents at the launch of What does child empowerment mean...
 
Transparency, Recognition and the role of eSealing - Ildiko Mazar and Koen No...
Transparency, Recognition and the role of eSealing - Ildiko Mazar and Koen No...Transparency, Recognition and the role of eSealing - Ildiko Mazar and Koen No...
Transparency, Recognition and the role of eSealing - Ildiko Mazar and Koen No...
 
Basic Civil Engineering notes on Transportation Engineering & Modes of Transport
Basic Civil Engineering notes on Transportation Engineering & Modes of TransportBasic Civil Engineering notes on Transportation Engineering & Modes of Transport
Basic Civil Engineering notes on Transportation Engineering & Modes of Transport
 
Mattingly "AI & Prompt Design: Named Entity Recognition"
Mattingly "AI & Prompt Design: Named Entity Recognition"Mattingly "AI & Prompt Design: Named Entity Recognition"
Mattingly "AI & Prompt Design: Named Entity Recognition"
 
ĐỀ THAM KHẢO KÌ THI TUYỂN SINH VÀO LỚP 10 MÔN TIẾNG ANH FORM 50 CÂU TRẮC NGHI...
ĐỀ THAM KHẢO KÌ THI TUYỂN SINH VÀO LỚP 10 MÔN TIẾNG ANH FORM 50 CÂU TRẮC NGHI...ĐỀ THAM KHẢO KÌ THI TUYỂN SINH VÀO LỚP 10 MÔN TIẾNG ANH FORM 50 CÂU TRẮC NGHI...
ĐỀ THAM KHẢO KÌ THI TUYỂN SINH VÀO LỚP 10 MÔN TIẾNG ANH FORM 50 CÂU TRẮC NGHI...
 
TỔNG HỢP HƠN 100 ĐỀ THI THỬ TỐT NGHIỆP THPT TOÁN 2024 - TỪ CÁC TRƯỜNG, TRƯỜNG...
TỔNG HỢP HƠN 100 ĐỀ THI THỬ TỐT NGHIỆP THPT TOÁN 2024 - TỪ CÁC TRƯỜNG, TRƯỜNG...TỔNG HỢP HƠN 100 ĐỀ THI THỬ TỐT NGHIỆP THPT TOÁN 2024 - TỪ CÁC TRƯỜNG, TRƯỜNG...
TỔNG HỢP HƠN 100 ĐỀ THI THỬ TỐT NGHIỆP THPT TOÁN 2024 - TỪ CÁC TRƯỜNG, TRƯỜNG...
 
The Story of Village Palampur Class 9 Free Study Material PDF
The Story of Village Palampur Class 9 Free Study Material PDFThe Story of Village Palampur Class 9 Free Study Material PDF
The Story of Village Palampur Class 9 Free Study Material PDF
 
Graduate Outcomes Presentation Slides - English (v3).pptx
Graduate Outcomes Presentation Slides - English (v3).pptxGraduate Outcomes Presentation Slides - English (v3).pptx
Graduate Outcomes Presentation Slides - English (v3).pptx
 
會考英文會考英文會考英文會考英文會考英文會考英文會考英文會考英文會考英文會考英文會考英文
會考英文會考英文會考英文會考英文會考英文會考英文會考英文會考英文會考英文會考英文會考英文會考英文會考英文會考英文會考英文會考英文會考英文會考英文會考英文會考英文會考英文會考英文
會考英文會考英文會考英文會考英文會考英文會考英文會考英文會考英文會考英文會考英文會考英文
 
DEMONSTRATION LESSON IN ENGLISH 4 MATATAG CURRICULUM
DEMONSTRATION LESSON IN ENGLISH 4 MATATAG CURRICULUMDEMONSTRATION LESSON IN ENGLISH 4 MATATAG CURRICULUM
DEMONSTRATION LESSON IN ENGLISH 4 MATATAG CURRICULUM
 
Sternal Fractures & Dislocations - EMGuidewire Radiology Reading Room
Sternal Fractures & Dislocations - EMGuidewire Radiology Reading RoomSternal Fractures & Dislocations - EMGuidewire Radiology Reading Room
Sternal Fractures & Dislocations - EMGuidewire Radiology Reading Room
 
24 ĐỀ THAM KHẢO KÌ THI TUYỂN SINH VÀO LỚP 10 MÔN TIẾNG ANH SỞ GIÁO DỤC HẢI DƯ...
24 ĐỀ THAM KHẢO KÌ THI TUYỂN SINH VÀO LỚP 10 MÔN TIẾNG ANH SỞ GIÁO DỤC HẢI DƯ...24 ĐỀ THAM KHẢO KÌ THI TUYỂN SINH VÀO LỚP 10 MÔN TIẾNG ANH SỞ GIÁO DỤC HẢI DƯ...
24 ĐỀ THAM KHẢO KÌ THI TUYỂN SINH VÀO LỚP 10 MÔN TIẾNG ANH SỞ GIÁO DỤC HẢI DƯ...
 
ESSENTIAL of (CS/IT/IS) class 07 (Networks)
ESSENTIAL of (CS/IT/IS) class 07 (Networks)ESSENTIAL of (CS/IT/IS) class 07 (Networks)
ESSENTIAL of (CS/IT/IS) class 07 (Networks)
 
Personalisation of Education by AI and Big Data - Lourdes Guàrdia
Personalisation of Education by AI and Big Data - Lourdes GuàrdiaPersonalisation of Education by AI and Big Data - Lourdes Guàrdia
Personalisation of Education by AI and Big Data - Lourdes Guàrdia
 
Trauma-Informed Leadership - Five Practical Principles
Trauma-Informed Leadership - Five Practical PrinciplesTrauma-Informed Leadership - Five Practical Principles
Trauma-Informed Leadership - Five Practical Principles
 
diagnosting testing bsc 2nd sem.pptx....
diagnosting testing bsc 2nd sem.pptx....diagnosting testing bsc 2nd sem.pptx....
diagnosting testing bsc 2nd sem.pptx....
 
SURVEY I created for uni project research
SURVEY I created for uni project researchSURVEY I created for uni project research
SURVEY I created for uni project research
 
Observing-Correct-Grammar-in-Making-Definitions.pptx
Observing-Correct-Grammar-in-Making-Definitions.pptxObserving-Correct-Grammar-in-Making-Definitions.pptx
Observing-Correct-Grammar-in-Making-Definitions.pptx
 
An overview of the various scriptures in Hinduism
An overview of the various scriptures in HinduismAn overview of the various scriptures in Hinduism
An overview of the various scriptures in Hinduism
 
How To Create Editable Tree View in Odoo 17
How To Create Editable Tree View in Odoo 17How To Create Editable Tree View in Odoo 17
How To Create Editable Tree View in Odoo 17
 

Vol 8 No 5 - October 2016

  • 1. Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org Aajhss.org International Journal of Humanities & Social Sciences Vol. 8, No. 5 IJHSS.NET e-ISSN: 1694-2639 p-ISSN: 1694-2620 October 2016
  • 2. Vol 8, No 5 – October 2016 Table of Contents Effects of personal characteristics on susceptibility to decision bias: a literature study 1 Alexander Toet, Anne-Marie Brouwer, Karel van den Bosch and J.E. (Hans) Korteling Likert Scale Development: Construction and Evaluation of Home Environment Scale 18 Mukhtar Ahmad Wani & Aejaz Masih Iqbal’s Response to Modern Western Thought: A Critical Analysis 27 Dr. Mohammad Nayamat Ullah and Abdullah Al Masud Is more BENELUX cooperation the future for the low countries ? 37 Prof. dr. Herman Matthijs AAJHSS.ORG
  • 3. 1 http://aajhss.org/index.php/ijhss International Journal of Humanities and Social Sciences p-ISSN: 1694-2620 e-ISSN: 1694-2639 Vol. 8 No. 5, pp. 1-17, ©IJHSS Effects of personal characteristics on susceptibility to decision bias: a literature study Alexander Toet, Anne-Marie Brouwer, Karel van den Bosch, J.E. (Hans) Korteling TNO Kampweg 5, 3769 DE Soesterberg, The Netherlands Abstract Cognitive biases and heuristics are pervasive simplifications and distortions in judgement and reasoning that systematically affect human decision making. Knowledge in this area may enable us to foresee and reduce detrimental effects of biases or to influence others more effectively. We therefore performed a literature study to assess the influence of personal characteristics (cognitive abilities, expertise, personality, cultural background) on the occurrence of cognitive biases. We found that each of the aforementioned factors can affect cognitive biases, though not much is known about the effects of culture. Also, factors that appear to reduce a cognitive bias may in fact mitigate (suppress or override) its behavioral effect rather than preventing the bias from occurring at all. The general picture that arises is that bias susceptibility and the occurrence of biases depend on thinking style (heuristic versus deliberate), where thinking style is associated with an individual‟s personal characteristics. In general, biases are reduced when a deliberate (analytical) thinking style is applied. However, whether a specific (heuristic or deliberate) thinking style actually reduces or enhances a given type of bias also depends on the context. Keywords: bias, heuristics, personal characteristics, decision making, judgement. Introduction People constantly make judgments and decisions, either conscious or unconscious. Most decisions are made without knowing for sure what the effects of the decision will be and whether the outcome will be positive or not. Making decisions in uncertainty is based upon at least two factors: the desirability and the likelihood of the outcome. Decision theory studies the development of algorithms that, for a given problem and likelihoods, produce the best outcome. Decision theory can thus, in principle, provide recommendations how to best make decisions. However, in real life people often make decisions that deviate from the best solution (Haselton, Nettle, & Andrews, 2005).These deviations are the outcome of systematic distortions in human judgment and reasoning. Subjective estimations play a significant part in our daily life. Our decisions, conclusions and explanations are based upon our beliefs about the likelihood of uncertain events but not on their objective likelihood. For example, the decision to stay away from a particular neighborhood is not driven by the actual likelihood of being robbed, but by our perception of the likelihood of being robbed. The important conclusion of an immense body of research on decision making is that
  • 4. 2 http://aajhss.org/index.php/ijhss people do not follow the laws of probability, but instead use relatively simple rules (heuristics). These heuristics often perform well, but under certain conditions may lead to systematic and serious errors. The distortion of human judgment and decision making is called „cognitive bias‟, or shortly „bias‟. The study reported here was performed to acquire further insight into biased (pre- cognitive, automatic, heuristic) judgment and decision processes (that are influenced by intuitions, emotions, biases, or associations) and to investigate whether biased thinking depends on the personal characteristics of individuals. If there are indeed psychological characteristics that predict biased thinking, this knowledge could in principle effectively be used to either mitigate or deploy biased thinking. Knowledge of cognitive biases can for instance be used to:  develop selection procedures (to develop tests that estimate an individual‟s susceptibility to biases),  reduce susceptibility to biases and ameliorate their effects (by training personnel and institutions to recognize and cope with biases appropriately, and by developing fast and frugal decision protocols),  effectively deploy knowledge of biases against relevant actors (both on a strategic level as part of a doctrine and operationally in the field). The literature study reported here contributes to this goal by identifying characteristics of individuals (e.g., cognitive abilities, expertise, personality, cultural background) that may predict a person‟s susceptibility to cognitive biases, and in particular those leading to decision and judgment biases. The following section discusses the literature search strategy. Next we briefly discuss the origins of human decision biases and factors that may affect the susceptibility to these biases. Then we present a literature review on some personal characteristics that may predict an individual‟s susceptibility to judgment and decision bias. Finally, we will present the conclusions of this study. Literature search strategy Electronic searches were carried out using the databases ScienceDirect, PubMed and PsycINFO. In addition, lliterature searches were also performed with Google Scholar. As search terms we used the names of several well-known biases (Anchoring, Anchoring bias, Attribution error, Base rate neglect, Belief bias, Confirmation bias, Conjunction fallacy, Framing, Halo effect, Hindsight bias, Imaginability bias, Omission bias, Negativity bias, Outcome bias, Over-confidence bias, Sunk cost effect) as well as some general terms (Bias, Cognitive bias, Decision bias, Decision making, Judgement, Heuristic bias, Heuristics: for definitions see Appendix A) and combined them with (conjunction: AND) terms related to personal characteristics (cognitive abilities OR expertise OR personality OR individual differences OR culture OR reasoning OR thinking OR trait) factors. The searches were restricted to articles reporting empirical studies in peer reviewed journals. The relevant papers that were found in this initial search served as a starting point for subsequent searches, that included all later papers referring to papers from the initial set (found by using the „Cited by‟ function in Google Scholar. We included studies found using this method in the review only if they involved influences of personal factors on human decision making. The literature searches were performed in the second half of 2016.
  • 5. 3 http://aajhss.org/index.php/ijhss Cognitive biases and their origins The human mind is limited in its capacity to render judgments in a way that is perfectly rational and fully informed. While rational thinking serves well to solve decision problems that allow comprehensive analysis, it may fail in complex real life situations, where it is often very difficult to have access to every relevant piece of information, and where a decision often has to be made quickly. Even if such access were possible, our brains do not operate like computer algorithms, capable of complex and multiple calculations in order to reach logically sound conclusions – not to mention that we hardly have time to perform such rigorous analysis for every judgment that we make. As a result, human decision making tends to rely on a variety of simple heuristic decision rules that can be executed quickly. Oftentimes, heuristics produce judgements and decisions that are „good enough‟ when measured against an acceptable cognitive load. However, heuristics can also lead to irrational thinking and problem-solving in ways that produce errors or illogical decisions, known as „cognitive biases‟. This is most likely to occur in complex situations (when relevant information is ignored and/or irrelevant information interferes) or in situations that are mistakenly perceived as familiar (while they are actually unknown). Cognitive biases are pervasive in human reasoning and have important practical implications. Dual-process heuristic-deliberate theories postulate a distinction between fast, intuitive, automatic, heuristic, emotionally charged and fallible (heuristic or „Type 1‟ ) processes versus slow, conscious, controlled, deliberate and analytic (deliberate or „Type 2‟) processes (e.g., Evans, 2006; Kahneman, 2003; Kahneman & Frederick, 2002; Sloman, 1996). When fast responses are required, performance is based on low-effort heuristic processes. Deliberate processes supervise and control the output of the heuristic system. In this view biases occur when deliberate processing either (1) fails to successfully engage (Kahneman, 2003) or (2) fails to override the biased heuristic response (De Neys, 2012). The serial deliberate processes are slower and require more working memory and are therefore constrained by the limited capacity of the brain. Conversely, heuristic processes function implicitly and in parallel and do not claim executive working memory resources (De Neys, 2006). It is generally assumed that heuristic processes result in biased outcomes, unless the analytical system takes over (Evans, 1984, 1989). In this study we adopt the dual-process heuristic-deliberate theory of human decision making as a framework to unify and understand our current findings. Intuitive (heuristic) decision-making produces quick solutions based on general heuristics (the Fast and Frugal view: Gigerenzer & Gaissmaier, 2010) or on experience-based (pattern- matching) evaluations (Naturalistic Decision Making: G. A. Klein, 1993; see e.g. Kahneman & Klein, 2009; G. Klein, 2015 and Davis, Kulick, & Egner, 2005 for a discussion on the differences and similarities between both views). Intuition can provide access to information that would not be beliedfble through deliberate thinking (Hogarth, 2010). However, intuition cannot generate or acquire new knowledge. Also, since intuition is bias prone, it may lead to potentially dangerous inaccurate perceptions of reality (Dane & Pratt, 2007; Kahneman & Frederick, 2002; Amos Tversky & Kahneman, 1974). In contrast to intuitive decision-making, deliberation uses abstract thinking and generalizations, and allows the acquisition of additional information in the decision-making process (Söllner, Broeder, & Hilbig, 2013).This is a clear advantage over intuition when problem solving requires the application of complex rules (Kahneman & Frederick, 2002). In the real world, decision makers often have to deal with three critical constraints that limit their opportunities for deliberation: (1) limited access to information, (2) cognitive limitations inherent in the human mind, and (3) limited time. These constraints result in „bounded rationality‟ (H. A. Simon, 1972). Deliberation is time consuming and requires cognitive effort. Cognitive resources claimed by deliberation cannot be deployed for other tasks (Kurzban, Duckworth, Kable, &
  • 6. 4 http://aajhss.org/index.php/ijhss Myers, 2013). Decision-makers will therefore typically use their intuition when there is no obvious need to use deliberate reasoning to make a good decision. From a behavioral standpoint cognitive biases may be seen as systematic errors in rational reasoning (Amos Tversky & Kahneman, 1974). However, many cognitive biases that appear irrational from the viewpoint of rational choice theory may in fact be quite rational from the perspective of evolutionary biology (Santos & Rosati, 2015). They may optimize decision making in a given environment (context) by optimally using the available information (ecological rationality: Goldstein & Gigerenzer, 2002). However, decision rules that are completely adapted to a given (natural) environment may of course lead to maladaptive („biased‟) behavior in different settings (Fawcett et al., 2014). Factors affecting human cognitive bias From a practical viewpoint, it would be useful to understand the factors that predict the occurrence of cognitive biases. This literature review addresses the question whether characteristics of individuals affect susceptibility to cognitive biases. The dual-process heuristic- deliberate theory of human decision making (described above) is adopted as a framework to unify and understand our current findings. This theory states that individuals are less prone to biases if they apply the thinking style (deliberate or heuristic) that is most appropriate for the problem context. Furthermore, an individual‟s tendency to make biased judgments and decisions may be related to personal characteristics (e.g., cognitive ability, expertise, personality). Cognitive ability People differ in their cognitive abilities like intelligence, training (level of expertise), and thinking styles. Studies investigating the correlation between measures of intelligence and a wide range of different cognitive biases have shown that cognitive ability (both fluid and crystalized intelligence) does not predict bias-proneness in general (Stanovich & West, 2008; Teovanovic, Knezevic, & Stankov, 2015). Intelligent people are just as prone to cognitive bias as less intelligent ones. However, highly intelligent people are more able than less intelligent ones to avoid cognitive bias once they have been warned about the bias in advance and are instructed how to avoid it (Stanovich & West, 2008). Significant negative correlations have been observed between fluid intelligence and several biases. Fluid intelligence is an individual‟s capacity to think in a logical way and to find solutions for new problems, independent of acquired knowledge (Cattell, 1987). Fluid intelligence has been found to correlate negatively with belief bias, over-confidence bias and base rate neglect and also with the Cognitive Reflection Test (CRT: Frederick, 2005). The CRT is a widely used tool to assess individual differences in intuitive–analytic cognitive styles. The CRT has been found to correlate negatively with the sunk cost effect, belief bias and base rate neglect; Teovanovic et al., 2015).Thus it seems that people with high reflective abilities and a high fluid intelligence are less prone to these cognitive biases. The literature shows mixed results on the relation between analytic intelligence and proneness to the anchoring bias (for a review see: Furnham & Boo, 2011). While some studies found that individuals with higher cognitive abilities are less susceptible to anchoring (Bergman, Ellingsen, Johannesson, & Svensson, 2010), others observed no - or even the opposite - effect (Oechssler, Roider, & Schmitz, 2009). There is evidence that an individual‟s susceptibility to bias relates to structures and emotional processes in the brain. Two key brain structures mediating emotional information
  • 7. 5 http://aajhss.org/index.php/ijhss processing are the amygdala and the orbitofrontal cortex (OFC) (e.g., Kim & Hamann, 2007; Zald, 2003). The amygdala can be seen as a primitive structure linking immediate threat with rapid survival responses (Sander, Grafman, & Zalla, 2003). The OFC is associated with deliberate thinking and has the function to gather and update information and use it to predict possible outcomes of-, and to steer, human behavior (Rolls, 2004; Rolls & Grabenhorst, 2008). De Martino et al. (De Martino, Kumaran, Seymour, & Dolan, 2006) examined the neural mechanisms mediating the framing effect and the ability to control it. While placed inside an fMRI scanner their participants performed a financial decision making task. In line with the dual- process theory (i.e., the view that choices are typically affectively loaded and involve heuristic thinking; see Kahneman & Frederick, 2007) this study showed that framing bias during financial decision making correlated with a higher activity in the greater amygdala. In addition, they found that subjects who acted more rationally also exhibited stronger OFC activation. Interestingly, they also found a strong inter-individual variability in susceptibility to framing, which did not correlate with amygdala activity. Instead, they observed a positive correlation between the ability to control framing bias and OFC activation: increased orbital and medial prefrontal cortex activity correlated with a reduced susceptibility to the framing effect (De Martino et al., 2006). Although enhanced OFC activity does not necessarily imply the inhibition of emotional processes (Aron, 2007), this result agrees with the view that controlling decision bias depends on engagement of deliberate, rational thinking. People who score high on the Need for Cognition (NFC: an individual‟s propensity to enjoy and engage in thought: Cacioppo & Petty, 1982) are just as likely to be „framed‟ as anyone else. However, compared to people scoring low on NFC, they are more consistent across different frames of a problem. In accordance with the abovementioned results from brain research, the magnitude of the framing effect is significantly reduced when decision makers are encouraged to reflect on the options and to motivate their choice (Miller & Fagley, 1991; Sieck & Yates, 1997; Takemura, 1993). For people who score high on the Need for Cognition this manipulation even eliminates the framing effect altogether (A. F. Simon, Fagley, & Halleran, 2004). NFC has also been found to moderate hindsight bias: hindsight bias is found for persons with low and medium NFC scores, but not for people with high NFC scores (Verplanken & Pieters, 1988). An individual‟s thinking style has often been associated with proneness to bias. An individual‟s preference for an analytic-rational (deliberate processing) or an intuitive-experiential (heuristic processing) thinking style can be assessed through the Rational-Experiential Inventory (REI: Epstein, Pacini, Denes-Raj, & Heier, 1996). The relationship between thinking style and various biases has been investigated in several studies. Persons that dominantly use an analytic/rational thinking style tend to be less susceptible to the base rate neglect bias than people using an intuitive/experiental thinking style (Ohlert & Weißenberger, 2015). There seems to be no relation between thinking style and susceptibility to the conjunction fallacy (Lu, 2015). Thinking style and belief bias appear to be linked: in contrast to people with an intuitive- experiential thinking style, people with an analytic-rational thinking style are less susceptible to belief bias (Svedholm-Häkkinen, 2015; Trippas, Pennycook, Verde, & Handley, 2015). A specific cognitive ability that appears to be linked with thinking style is numeracy, or the proficiency in basic probability and numerical concepts (Peters et al., 2006). An individual‟s numeracy-competency is determined by (1) the degree of information processing (heuristic or deep elaborative processing), (2) affective numerical intuition (e.g., framing); and (3) intuitive understanding (e.g., gist-based representation and reasoning; see Ghazal, Cokely, & Garcia- Retamero, 2014). Low numeracy is typically linked with intuitive (heuristic) thinking, whereas high numeracy is typically linked with deliberate (analytical) processing (Brust-Renck, Reyna, Corbin, Royer, & Weldon, 2014). In this view people with high numeracy are less susceptible to
  • 8. 6 http://aajhss.org/index.php/ijhss biases because they show an analytical thinking style and information seeking behavior (Ghazal et al., 2014). Numeracy has indeed been found to determine decision making quality across a wide range of tasks (Sinayev & Peters, 2015). For instance, people with high numeracy are less susceptible to framing bias (Gamliel, Kreiner, & Garcia-Retamero, 2015; Peters et al., 2006) and conjunctions fallacies (Sinayev & Peters, 2015), are less over/under confident about their decisions (Sinayev & Peters, 2015), and take less risks (Jasper, Bhattacharya, Levin, Jones, & Bossard, 2013). People with higher numeracy are better able to extract the affective „gist‟ of a problem and use it to determine the quality of a particular choice (Jasper et al., 2013). These findings cannot be attributed to differences in general intelligence (Peters et al., 2006). Somewhat in contrast, people using a combination of thinking styles (high deliberate/high heuristic (also called „complementary thinking‟) and low deliberate/low heuristic (also called „poor thinking‟) are found to be more susceptible to framing than those using a dominant (either rational or intuitive) thinking style (Shiloh, Salton, & Sharabi, 2002). Thus, it seems that people with a clearly dominant heuristic or deliberate thinking style are more resistant to framing. This may be because both decision styles use strong internal guides (either logical or experiential) to process information. In contrast, people with a more uniform thinking style (either „complementary‟ or „poor‟) appear to depend more on coincidental external situational cues (e.g., the way the information is formulated) when processing information. Individuals with high emotional intelligence (the ability to recognize and distinguish between emotions and to identify their causes) are able to reduce (or even eliminate) the effects of decision bias by recognizing that the emotions they experience (for instance anxiety: Yip & Côté, 2013) are irrelevant for the decisions they have to make. For example, military officers with high emotional intelligence make better tactical decisions under stressful condition because they are able to maintain a higher state of attentiveness for social cues and perform a more exhaustive (deliberate) analysis of situational cues (Fallon et al., 2014). Summarizing, several types of cognitive ability, as well as the ability to engage in deliberate information processing at appropriate times, seem to protect an individual from several cognitive biases (in particular, the sunk cost bias, the base rate fallacy, the over- confidence bias, the belief bias, and framing). For anchoring and conjunction bias the evidence is mixed. Emotional intelligence has been found to reduce the likelihood of falling prey to decision bias. In general it can be concluded that cognitive ability does not safeguard an individual against bias, but it may in some cases help in deploying countering mechanisms that reduce, or prevent subsequent behavioral effects. Expertise Whether people deploy a heuristic or a more deliberate decision making mode depends for a large part on the decision-maker‟s expertise (Fuchs, Steigenberger, & Lübcke, 2015). An expert is an individual who has acquired special skills in a given domain (Chi, Glaser, & Farr, 1988). The main distinction between experts and novices is the extent of their domain-specific knowledge (Chi et al., 1988). Less experienced decision-makers increasingly use deliberative thinking to solve subjectively complex problems while mainly following their preferred (either heuristic or deliberate) decision style. In addition, the thoroughness of their information processing is affected by their mood: a happy mood leads to more superficial (heuristic) processing, while a sad mood leads to more thorough (deliberate) processing (Englich & Soder, 2009). In contrast, more experienced decision makers use deliberation independent of their decision preferences, subjective environmental complexity or mood (Englich & Soder, 2009; Fuchs et al., 2015). Moreover, experts rely more on intuition than on deliberation. They can probably do so because
  • 9. 7 http://aajhss.org/index.php/ijhss they have learned to match tools and strategies to problem structures (similar to chess players: Sauter, 1999). For example, expert handball players are more intuitive than non-experts and tend to rely on their first intuitively generated decision option (Raab & Laborde, 2011). Expertise generally does not significantly reduce the anchoring bias effect (for a review see: Furnham & Boo, 2011). However, expertise in a specific estimation context (task) may reduce susceptibility to anchoring: experience in a card game was inversely correlated with susceptibility to anchoring (Welsh, Delfabbro, Burns, & Begg, 2014). This implies that the exact nature of the expertise should be clearly defined before being able to assess whether „experts‟ are less affected by anchors than „non-experts‟. Individual differences in various traits may be more useful for predicting the rate of learning, where it is their level of expertise that indirectly reduces susceptibility to anchoring, rather than direct susceptibility to biases (Welsh et al., 2014). Summarizing, expertise affects sensitivity to biases since it determines thinking style and the way that information is processed in combination with its context. Expertise can stimulate both deliberate and heuristic thinking. When the context is not appropriate, the latter may lead to biases. Experts are less likely to misinterpret the context than novices. They are therefore more likely to select the thinking style appropriate for the context, and are subsequently less prone to bias than non-experts. Personality Personality is often defined in terms of five main personality traits (the „Big Five‟: John & Srivastava, 1999): openness, conscientiousness, extraversion, agreeableness, and neuroticism. The literature shows that individuals with high conscientiousness, agreeableness and openness to experience or with low extraversion are more susceptible to the anchoring bias (Caputo, 2014; Eroglu & Croxton, 2010; McElroy & Dowd, 2007; Teovanovic et al., 2015). It has been suggested that because individuals with high conscientiousness engage in more deliberate thinking when making decisions, they are more likely to perform a confirmatory search for anchor consistent information. Individuals with high agreeableness tend to be more affected by anchors than less agreeable persons. This is probably because individuals with high openness to experience easily „adjust‟ their beliefs when considering situational information. There are some indications that introverts are more susceptible to anchoring bias than extraverts (Eroglu & Croxton, 2010; Furnham, Boo, & McClelland, 2012), but this relation is not robust (Furnham et al., 2012). It has been suggested that low extraversion may be associated with negative affect (Eroglu & Croxton, 2010), which may stimulate more deliberate thinking and thereby activate a confirmatory search for anchor consistent information (Bodenhausen, Gabriel, & Lineberger, 2000; Englich & Soder, 2009). People scoring high on trait optimism (people who tend to believe in a bright future) (Scheier, Carver, & Bridges, 1994) were more likely to update their judgments in response to desirable information than to undesirable information, particularly for judgments that apply to themselves (Kuzmanovic, Jefferson, & Vogeley, 2015). In other words, people with high trait optimism show a pronounced self-specific optimism bias. It has been argued that the evolution of the healthy mind to (optimistically) mis-predict future occurrences has led to an increased resilience, improved coping behavior and reduced anxiety, resulting in overall improvements of both physical and mental health (Dolcos, Hu, Iordan, Moore, & Dolcos, 2015; Sharot, 2011). Optimism bias may sometimes even lead to better outcomes than do unbiased beliefs (Sharot, 2011). Recent brain studies have identified the OFC with trait optimism: higher OFC gray matter volume (GMV) correlates with increased optimism (Dolcos et al., 2015).
  • 10. 8 http://aajhss.org/index.php/ijhss Despite the hypotheses suggested above, it is not easy to determine the mechanisms underlying the relationships between personality traits and susceptibility to cognitive bias. Summarizing, personality appears to relate to bias susceptibility because it determines how people weigh and process information. Culture Members of different social cultures may have different ways of thinking, because they have been socialized from birth into different world views. Some researchers hypothesize that these cultural differences affect an individual‟s susceptibility for bias. For instance, East Asians are believed to have a holistic world view, attending more to contextual factors and assigning causality to them, while they are less inclined to categorize and use formal logic. Westerners, on the other hand, are typically more analytic and are more inclined to use pay attention to objects of interest, to categorize them, and to use rules and formal logic to understand their behavior (Nisbett, Peng, Choi, & Norenzayan, 2001; Strutton & Carter, 2013). As a result, East Asians may for instance be less susceptible to attribution errors, since they see behavior primarily as a product of external factors and not merely of the actor's dispositions. For the same reason, they may be more susceptible to hindsight bias because they are readily able to find some explanation for a given event since everything is connected in their world view (Choi & Nisbett, 2000; Yama et al., 2010). Following this line of thinking, Westerners may be better able to withstand the hindsight bias because they have a more rule-based thinking style. Only a few studies address the influence of culture on bias susceptibility. Studies on the effects of culture on hindsight bias show mixed results: while some studies confirmed the abovementioned hypotheses (Choi & Nisbett, 2000; Yama et al., 2010) others found no cultural differences in the sensitivity to hindsight bias (Pohl, Bender, & Lachmann, 2002). One study (Scott, Christopher, & John, 1998) found evidence for the hypothesis that self-centered Westerners, with their personal desire to be correct and to fortify one‟s choices, are more susceptible to the sunk cost bias than collectivist East Asians who are more focused on optimizing outcomes for the group. However, other studies found opposite results (Yoder, Mancha, & Agrawal, 2014). They propose that East Asians may also be prone to sunk cost bias because they are more concerned about saving face, resulting in more commitment to prior decisions. A recent study on choice framing (Haerem, Kuvaas, Bakken, & Karlsen, 2011) compared military decision makers with business students (difference in organizational cultures). It was found that business students showed the classic framing bias (risk avoidance behavior in the gain frame an risk seeking behavior in the loss frame: A. Tversky & Kahneman, 1981), while military decision makers consistently showed a risk-seeking behavior for both (gain and loss) choice frames. In addition, military officers showed significantly higher levels of self-efficacy than business school students. Self-efficacy correlated with risk seeking in the military group but not in the civil group. This result agrees with the finding that people with little confidence in their own competence do not like to gamble (Heath & Tversky, 1991). Military decision makers, on the other hand, are probably so self-confident (or even over-confident) that they believe that they can beat the odds. Summarizing, while it is possible that culture affects sensitivity to bias (e.g., as a result of different preferred thinking styles or levels of self-efficacy), only a few studies on this topic have been conducted and their results are mixed.
  • 11. 9 http://aajhss.org/index.php/ijhss Discussion and conclusions The objective of this literature review was to investigate to what extent individual characteristics (cognitive abilities, expertise, personality, cultural background) affect a person‟s susceptibility to judgment and decision biases. This knowledge may for instance be used to develop strategies to mitigate biased thinking of own personnel, or to deploy strategies to evoke biased thinking in opponent parties. Our findings indicate that each of the reviewed aspects can affect cognitive biases under certain conditions (though with respect to culture the evidence is scarce). Note that the factors found to reduce cognitive bias may in fact merely mitigate the behavioral effects rather than preventing the bias from occurring at all. Several types of cognitive ability, as well as employing deliberate information processing may mitigate various cognitive biases (in particular: sunk cost, base rate, over confidence, belief, framing). For anchoring and conjunction bias, the evidence is mixed. Emotional intelligence has been found to reduce some biases. In general, cognitive ability does, by itself, not prevent biases from occurring, but it may help to learn how to prevent or reduce its effects on behavior. Expertise affects sensitivity to biases, because expertise largely determines thinking style and contextual information processing. Depending on the problem context, expertise may stimulate both deliberate thinking as well as heuristic thinking. Experts are more likely to select the thinking style appropriate for the context, and are subsequently less prone to bias than non- experts. Personality affects the way people weigh and processes information, which in turn affects the susceptibility to certain (but not all) biases. However, the relationship between personality and susceptibility to cognitive bias is not fully clear. Predicting the occurrence and direction of cognitive bias based on personality traits is therefore very hard. Limitations of the present study The results reviewed in this study have mostly been obtained in laboratory conditions involving simplified tests that have specially been designed to induce cognitive biases. It is therefore not clear how these results translate into real-life practice. We used the dual-process heuristic-deliberate framework to summarize, unify and understand our findings. However, not all reported effects fit into this framework. For instance, effects reported on emotional priming are different from studies that demonstrate effects of emotion, indicating a different underlying mechanism. Also, some of the effects upon judgment and decision making seem to be working against each other. For example, people who need to perform under pressure and time constraints are expected to apply heuristics which makes them vulnerable for bias. However, at the same time, it is known that pressure induces a negative mood which often elicits deliberate thinking strategies. And although deliberate thinking tends to guard people against the risk of bias, this is not always the case. Exactly when it does, and when not, is still difficult to quantify. This underlines the need to more fully understand the mechanisms underlying cognitive bias. Future research The fact that the effects of cognitive biases on decision making are not completely fixed within and between individuals suggests opportunities to apply this knowledge for selecting (bias free) and training (de-biasing) personnel. Implicit association tests may be effective selection tools (De
  • 12. 10 http://aajhss.org/index.php/ijhss Houwer, 2006). How these selection tests should be worked out for a given context (e.g., medical, financial, military, police) requires analysis of the characteristics and requirements of the candidates, the nature of the task, and the contextual demands. The information and insights resulting from the present study may also stimulate the development of new de-biasing techniques to protect own personnel against (self- or externally- induced) cognitive bias in concrete situations. Strategies that promote deeper information processing and thereby stimulate the recruitment of the medial and lateral orbitofrontal cortex (regions associated with the integration of affective and contextual information in decision making) may constrain decision bias (Hughes & Zaki, 2015). Computer games that immerse the user into bias-invoking situations that provide the experience to identify cognitive bias and to practice mitigation strategies, may serve as effective debiasing tools. It has been shown that these types of “serious games” can provide an effective method to train adults how to recognize and mitigate several cognitive biases (confirmation, attribution and blind spot; Clegg et al., 2015; Dunbar et al., 2014; Symborski et al., 2014). Models and decision support systems that provide tools and explicit rules to guide decisions, may also help to counteract the adverse effects of judgement bias. This can for instance be achieved by allowing the user to employ heuristics while warning for the likely biases, and by anticipating the likely use of heuristics and providing information that offsets the effects of such use (Larrick, 2004). In addition, the current findings may initiate innovative methods to exploit biases to manipulate the behavior of opponents and other groups. References Aron, A. R. (2007). The neural basis of inhibition in cognitive control. The Neuroscientist, 13(3), 214-228. doi:10.1177/1073858407299288 Bergman, O., Ellingsen, T., Johannesson, M., & Svensson, C. (2010). Anchoring and cognitive ability. Economics Letters, 107(1), 66-68. doi:10.1016/j.econlet.2009.12.028 Bodenhausen, G. V., Gabriel, S., & Lineberger, M. (2000). Sadness and susceptibility to judgmental bias: The case of anchoring. Psychological Science, 11(4), 320-323. doi:10.1111/1467-9280.00263 Brust-Renck, P. G., Reyna, V. F., Corbin, J. C., Royer, C. E., & Weldon, R. B. (2014). The role of numeracy in risk communication. In H. Cho, T. Reimer, & K. A. McComas (Eds.), The SAGE Handbook of Risk Communication (pp. 134-145). Thousand Oaks, CA, USA: Sage. Cacioppo, J. T., & Petty, R. E. (1982). The need for cognition. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 42(1), 116-131. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.42.1.116 Caputo, A. (2014). Relevant information, personality traits and anchoring effect. International Journal of Management and Decision Making, 13(1), 62-76. doi:10.1504/IJMDM.2014.058470 Cattell, R. B. (1987). Intelligence: Its structure, growth, and action. New York, USA: Elsevier Science Publishing Comapny, Inc. Chi, M. T., Glaser, R., & Farr, M. J. (1988). The nature of expertise. Hillsdale, NJ, USA: Lawrence Erlbaum Assoiates, Inc. Choi, I., & Nisbett, R. E. (2000). Cultural psychology of surprise: Holistic theories and recognition of contradiction. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 79(6), 890-905. doi:10.1037/0022- 3514.79.6.890 Dane, E., & Pratt, M. G. (2007). Exploring intuition and its role in managerial decision making. Academy of Management Review, 32(1), 33-54. doi:10.5465/AMR.2007.23463682 Davis, P. K., Kulick, J., & Egner, M. (2005). Implications of modern decision science for military decision-support systems. Santa Monica, CA, USA: RAND Corporation. De Houwer, J. (2006). What are implicit measures and why are we using them. In R. W. Wiers & A. W. RStacy (Eds.), The handbook of implicit cognition and addiction (pp. 11-28). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage Publishers. De Martino, B., Kumaran, D., Seymour, B., & Dolan, R. J. (2006). Frames, biases, and rational decision- making in the human brain. Science, 313(5787), 684-687. doi:10.1126/science.1128356
  • 13. 11 http://aajhss.org/index.php/ijhss Chronometric and dual-task considerations. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 59(6), 1070-1100. doi:10.1080/02724980543000123 De Neys, W. (2012). Bias and conflict: A case for logical intuitions. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 7(1), 28-38. doi:10.1177/1745691611429354 Dolcos, S., Hu, Y., Iordan, A., Moore, M., & Dolcos, F. (2015). Optimism and the brain: Trait optimism mediates the protective role of the orbitofrontal cortex gray matter volume against anxiety. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 11(2), 263-271. doi:10.1093/scan/nsv106 anchoring. Judgment and Decision Making, 4(1), 41-50. Epstein, S., Pacini, R., Denes- Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 71(2), 390-405. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.71.2.390 Eroglu, C., & Croxton, K. L. (2010). Biases in judgmental adjustments of statistical forecasts: The role of individual differences. International Journal of Forecasting, 26(1), 116-133. doi:10.1016/j.ijforecast.2009.02.005 Evans, J. S. B. T. (1984). Heuristic and analytic processes in reasoning. British Journal of Psychology, 75(4), 451-468. doi:10.1111/j.2044-8295.1984.tb01915.x Evans, J. S. B. T. (1989). Bias in human reasoning: Causes and consequences. London, UK: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Inc. Evans, J. S. B. T. (2006). The heuristic-analytic theory of reasoning: Extension and evaluation. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 13(3), 378-395. doi:10.3758/BF03193858 Fallon, C. K., Panganiban, A. R., Wohleber, R., Matthews, G., Kustubayeva, A. M., & Roberts, R. (2014). Emotional intelligence, cognitive ability and information search in tactical decision-making. Personality and Individual Differences, 65(0), 24-29. doi:10.1016/j.paid.2014.01.029 Fawcett, T. W., Fallenstein, B., Higginson, A. D., Houston, A. I., Mallpress, D. E. W., Trimmer, P. C., & McNamara, J. M. (2014). The evolution of decision rules in complex environments. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 18(3), 153-161. doi:10.1016/j.tics.2013.12.012 Frederick, S. (2005). Cognitive reflection and decision making. Journal of Economic Perspectives, 19(4), 25-42. doi:10.1257/089533005775196732 Fuchs, H. M., Steigenberger, N., & Lübcke, T. (2015). decisions in action? Paper presented at the Proceedings of the 12th International Conference on Information Systems for Crisis Response & Management (ISCRAM 2015), Kristiansand, Denmark. Furnham, A., & Boo, H. C. (2011). A literature review of the anchoring effect. The Journal of Socio- Economics, 40(1), 35-42. doi:10.1016/j.socec.2010.10.008 Furnham, A., Boo, H. C., & McClelland, A. (2012). Individual differences and the susceptibility to the influence of anchoring cues. Journal of Individual Differences, 33(2), 89-93. doi:10.1027/1614- 0001/a000076 Gamliel, E., Kreiner, H., & Garcia-Retamero, R. (2015). The moderating role of objective and subjective numeracy in attribute framing. International Journal of Psychology, 51(2), 109-116. doi:10.1002/ijop.12138 Ghazal, S., Cokely, E. T., & Garcia-Retamero, R. (2014). Predicting biases in very highly educated samples: Numeracy and metacognition. Judgment and Decision Making, 9(1), 15-34. Gigerenzer, G., & Gaissmaier, W. (2010). Heuristic decision making. Annual Review of Psychology, 62(1), 451-482. doi:10.1146/annurev-psych-120709-145346 Goldstein, D. G., & Gigerenzer, G. (2002). Models of ecological rationality: The recognition heuristic. Psychological Review, 109(1), 75-90. doi:10.1037//0033-295X.109.1.75 Haerem, T., Kuvaas, B., Bakken, B. T., & Karlsen, T. (2011). Do military decision makers behave as predicted by prospect theory? Journal of Behavioral Decision Making, 24(5), 482-497. doi:10.1002/bdm.704 Haselton, M. G., Nettle, D., & Andrews, P. W. (2005). The evolution of cognitive bias. In D. M. Buss (Ed.), The handbook of evolutionary psychology (pp. 724-746). Hoboken, NJ, USA: John Wiley & Sons Inc. Heath, C., & Tversky, A. (1991). Preference and belief: Ambiguity and competence in choice under uncertainty. Journal of Risk and Uncertainty, 4(1), 5-28. doi:10.1007/BF00057884
  • 14. 12 http://aajhss.org/index.php/ijhss Hogarth, R. M. (2010). Intuition: A challenge for psychological research on decision making. Psychological Inquiry, 21(4), 338-353. doi:10.1080/1047840X.2010.520260 Jasper, J. D., Bhattacharya, C., Levin, I. P., Jones, L., & Bossard, E. (2013). Numeracy as a predictor of adaptive risky decision making. Journal of Behavioral Decision Making, 26(2), 164-173. doi:10.1002/bdm.1748 John, O. P., & Srivastava, S. (1999). The big five trait taxonomy: History, measurement, and theoretical perspectives. In L. A. Pervin & O. P. John (Eds.), Handbook of personality: Theory and research (2nd ed.) (Vol. 2, pp. 102-138). New York, NY: Guilford Press. Kahneman, D. (2003). A perspective on judgment and choice: Mapping bounded rationality. American Psychologist, 58(9), 697-720. doi:10.1037/0003-066X.58.9.697 Kahneman, D., & Frederick, S. (2002). Representativeness revisited: Attribute substitution in intuitive judgment. In T. Gilovich, D. Griffin, & D. Kahneman (Eds.), Heuristics and biases: The psychology of intuitive judgment (pp. 49-81). New York, NY, US: Cambridge University Press. Kahneman, D., & Frederick, S. (2007). Frames and brains: Elicitation and control of response tendencies. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 11(2), 45-46. doi:10.1016/j.tics.2006.11.007 Kahneman, D., & Klein, G. (2009). Conditions for intuitive expertise: A failure to disagree. American Psychologist, 64(6), 515-526. doi:10.1037/a0016755 Kim, S. H., & Hamann, S. (2007). Neural correlates of positive and negative emotion regulation. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 19(5), 776-798. doi:10.1162/jocn.2007.19.5.776 Klein, G. (2015). Naturalistic decision making suggestions for improving intuitive decision making. Journal of Applied Research in Memory and Cognition, 4(3), 164-168. doi:10.1016/j.jarmac.2015.07.001 Klein, G. A. (1993). A recognition-primed decision (rpd) model of rapid decision making. In G. A. Klein, J. Orasanu, R. Calderwood, & C. Zsambok (Eds.), Decision making in action: Models and methods (pp. 138-147). Norwood, NJ: Ablex Publishing Corporation. Kurzban, R., Duckworth, A., Kable, J. W., & Myers, J. (2013). An opportunity cost model of subjective effort and task performance. The Behavioral and brain sciences, 36(6), 661-679. doi:10.1017/S0140525X12003196 Kuzmanovic, B., Jefferson, A., & Vogeley, K. (2015). Self-specific optimism bias in belief updating is associated with high trait optimism. Journal of Behavioral Decision Making, 28(3), 281-293. doi:10.1002/bdm.1849 Lu, Y. (2015). Is experiential-intuitive cognitive style more inclined to err on conjunction fallacy than analytical-rational cognitive style? Frontiers in Psychology, 6(Article nr. 85), 1-8. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2015.00085 McElroy, T., & Dowd, K. (2007). Susceptibility to anchoring effects: How openness-to-experience influences responses to anchoring cues. Judgment and Decision Making, 2(1), 48-53. Miller, P. M., & Fagley, N. S. (1991). The effects of framing, problem variations, and providing rationale on choice. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 17(5), 517-522. doi:10.1177/0146167291175006 Nisbett, R. E., Peng, K., Choi, I., & Norenzayan, A. (2001). Culture and systems of thought: Holistic versus analytic cognition. Psychological Review, 108(2), 291-310. doi:10.1037//0033-295X.108.2.291 Oechssler, J., Roider, A., & Schmitz, P. W. (2009). Cognitive abilities and behavioral biases. Journal of Economic Behavior & Organization, 72(1), 147-152. Ohlert, C. R., & Weißenberger, B. E. (2015). Beating the base-rate fallacy: An experimental approach on the effectiveness of different information presentation formats. Journal of Management Control, 26(1), 51-80. doi:10.1007/s00187-015-0205-2 Peters, E., Västfjäll, D., Slovic, P., Mertz, C. K., Mazzocco, K., & Dickert, S. (2006). Numeracy and decision making. Psychological Science, 17(5), 407-413. doi:10.1111/j.1467-9280.2006.01720.x Pohl, R. F., Bender, M., & Lachmann, G. (2002). Hindsight bias around the world. Experimental Psychology, 49(4), 270-282. doi:10.1026//1618-3169.49.4.270 Raab, M., & Laborde, S. (2011). When to blink and when to think. Research Quarterly for Exercise and Sport, 82(1), 89-98. doi:10.1080/02701367.2011.10599725 Rolls, E. T. (2004). The functions of the orbitofrontal cortex. Brain and Cognition, 55(1), 11-29. doi:10.1016/S0278-2626(03)00277-X Rolls, E. T., & Grabenhorst, F. (2008). The orbitofrontal cortex and beyond: From affect to decision- making. Progress in Neurobiology, 86(3), 216-244. doi:10.1016/j.pneurobio.2008.09.001 Sander, D., Grafman, J., & Zalla, T. (2003). The human amygdala: An evolved system for relevance detection. Reviews in the Neurosciences, 14(4), 303-316. doi:10.1515/REVNEURO.2003.14.4.303
  • 15. 13 http://aajhss.org/index.php/ijhss Santos, L. R., & Rosati, A. G. (2015). The evolutionary roots of human decision making. Annual Review of Psychology, 66(1), 321-347. doi:10.1146/annurev-psych-010814-015310 Sauter, V. L. (1999). Intuitive decision-making. Communications of the ACM, 42(6), 109-115. doi:10.1145/303849.303869 Scheier, M. F., Carver, C. S., & Bridges, M. W. (1994). Distinguishing optimism from neuroticism (and trait anxiety, self-mastery, and self-esteem): A reevaluation of the life orientation test. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 67(6), 1063-1078. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.67.6.1063 Scott, W. G., Christopher, J. R., & John, G., I. (1998). The impact of cultural values on escalation of commitment. The International Journal of Organizational Analysis, 6(2), 165-176. doi:10.1108/eb028883 Sharot, T. (2011). The optimism bias. Current Biology, 21(23), R941-R945. doi:10.1016/j.cub.2011.10.030 Shiloh, S., Salton, E., & Sharabi, D. (2002). Individual differences in rational and intuitive thinking styles as predictors of heuristic responses and framing effects. Personality and Individual Differences, 32(3), 415-429. doi:10.1016/S0191-8869(01)00034-4 Sieck, W., & Yates, J. F. (1997). Exposition effects on decision making: Choice and confidence in choice. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 70(3), 207-219. doi:10.1006/obhd.1997.2706 Simon, A. F., Fagley, N. S., & Halleran, J. G. (2004). Decision framing: Moderating effects of individual differences and cognitive processing. Journal of Behavioral Decision Making, 17(2), 77-93. doi:10.1002/bdm.463 Simon, H. A. (1972). Theories of bounded rationality. In C. B. McGuire & R. Radner (Eds.), Decision and organization (pp. 161-176). Amsterdam, The Netherlands: North-Holland Publishing Company. Sinayev, A., & Peters, E. (2015). Cognitive reflection vs. Calculation in decision making. Front Psychol, 6(Article 532), 1-16. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2015.00532 Sloman, S. A. (1996). The empirical case for two systems of reasoning. Psychological Bulletin, 119(1), 3-22. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.119.1.3 Söllner, A., Broeder, A., & Hilbig, B. E. (2013). Deliberation versus automaticity in decision making: Which presentation format features facilitate automatic decision making? Judgment and Decision Making, 8(3), 278-298. Stanovich, K. E., & West, R. F. (2008). On the relative independence of thinking biases and cognitive ability. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 94(4), 672-695. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.94.4.672 Strutton, D., & Carter, W. (2013). Reducing biases in cross-cultural top management team decision- making processes. International Journal of Business Administration, 4(3), 1-13. doi:10.5430/ijba.v4n3p1 Svedholm-Häkkinen, A. M. (2015). Highly reflective reasoners show no signs of belief inhibition. Acta Psychologica, 154(0), 69-76. doi:10.1016/j.actpsy.2014.11.008 Takemura, K. (1993). The effect of decision frame and decision justification on risky choice. Japanese Psychological Research, 35(1), 36-40. doi:10.4992/psycholres1954.35.36 Teovanovic, P., Knezevic, G., & Stankov, L. (2015). Individual differences in cognitive biases: Evidence against one-factor theory of rationality. Intelligence, 50, 75-86. doi:10.1016/j.intell.2015.02.008 Trippas, D., Pennycook, G., Verde, M. F., & Handley, S. J. (2015). Better but still biased: Analytic cognitive style and belief bias. Thinking & Reasoning, online, 1-15. doi:10.1080/13546783.2015.1016450 Tversky, A., & Kahneman, D. (1974). Judgment under uncertainty: Heuristics and biases. Science, 185(4157), 1124-1131. doi:10.1126/science.185.4157.1124 Tversky, A., & Kahneman, D. (1981). The framing of decisions and the psychology of choice. Science, 211(4481), 453-458. doi:10.1126/science.7455683 Verplanken, B., & Pieters, R. G. M. (1988). Individual differences in reverse hindsight bias: I never thought something like chernobyl would happen. Did I? Journal of Behavioral Decision Making, 1(3), 131-147. doi:10.1002/bdm.3960010302 Welsh, M. B., Delfabbro, P. H., Burns, N. R., & Begg, S. H. (2014). Individual differences in anchoring: Traits and experience. Learning and Individual Differences, 29, 131-140. doi:10.1016/j.lindif.2013.01.002 Yama, H., Manktelow, K. I., Mercier, H., Henst, J.-B. V. d., Soo Do, K., Kawasaki, Y., & Adachi, K. (2010). A cross-cultural study of hindsight bias and conditional probabilistic reasoning. Thinking & Reasoning, 16(4), 346-371. doi:10.1080/13546783.2010.526786
  • 16. 14 http://aajhss.org/index.php/ijhss Yip, J. A., & Côté, S. (2013). The emotionally intelligent decision maker: Emotion-understanding ability reduces the effect of incidental anxiety on risk taking. Psychological Science, 24(1), 48-55. doi:10.1177/0956797612450031 Yoder, C. Y., Mancha, R., & Agrawal, N. (2014). Culture-related factors affect sunk cost bias. Behavioral Development Bulletin, 19(4), 105-118. doi:10.1037/h0101086 Zald, D. H. (2003). The human amygdala and the emotional evaluation of sensory stimuli. Brain Research Reviews, 41(1), 88-123. doi:10.1016/S0165-0173(02)00248-5 Appendix A : Terminology Anchoring: a tendency to make decisions biased toward previously presented information (the "anchor"). Attribution error: the tendency to see behavior as a product of the actor's dispositions and to ignore important situational determinants of the behavior. Base rate neglect: a tendency to ignore statistical information (prior probabilities) and focus on information only pertaining to a certain case. Belief bias: a tendency to draw conclusions that agree with one‟s own beliefs - i.e., to evaluate the logical strength of an argument on the basis of the believability of the conclusion. Bias: errors in decisions that arise due to limitations of cognitive processing. Biases are often explained using dual-process theory, which states that we have two cognitive systems, one that is fast and intuitive, and another that is slow and deliberate. Biases occur when our fast system operates without the oversight of the slow system. Cognitive bias: a consistent deviation from an accurate perception or judgmentjudgment of the world. Inferences about other people and situations may be drawn in an illogical fashion. Individuals create their own "subjective social reality" from their perception of the input. Cognitive reflection test: test to assess the ability or disposition to resist reporting the response that first comes to mind. Confirmation bias: a tendency to search for, interpret, focus on and remember information in a way that confirms one's preconceptions. Conjunction fallacy: a combination of conditions is considered more likely than a general condition. Crystallized intelligence: the ability to use skills, knowledge, and experience. Fluid intelligence: the capacity to think logically and solve problems in novel situations, independent of acquired knowledge. It is the ability to analyze novel problems, identify patterns and relationships that underpin these problems and the extrapolation of these using logic. Framing: a bias in decision making depending on the way in the information is presented (e.g., whether options are presented in terms of gains or loss). Halo effect: a tendency to let the perceived valence of a single aspect dominate the overall judgment of a person or situation. Heuristics: simple decision rules (rules of thumb) that ignore part of the available information but work well in a given environment. Hindsight bias: the tendency to erroneously perceive events as inevitable or more likely once they have occurred.
  • 17. 15 http://aajhss.org/index.php/ijhss Imaginability bias: the tendency to use our imagination to make a subjective premonition of a future event for which no memories of actual instances come to mind. Need for cognition: an individual‟s propensity to enjoy and engage in thought. Negativity bias: a tendency to weigh negative information more heavily than positive information. Numeracy: the ability to process basic probability and numerical concepts. Omission bias: the tendency to prefer harm caused by omissions over equal or lesser harm caused by acts. Optimism bias: the tendency to overestimate the likelihood of positive events (and to underestimate the likelihood of negative events) happening to oneself, compared to others. Outcome bias: a tendency to evaluate the quality of a decision based on its outcome rather than on what factors led to the decision. Over-confidence bias: an inclination of individuals to overestimate their own abilities to successfully perform a particular task. Sunk cost effect: a tendency to persist in an endeavor once an investment of money, effort, or time has already been made.
  • 18. 18 http://aajhss.org/index.php/ijhss International Journal of Humanities and Social Sciences p-ISSN: 1694-2620 e-ISSN: 1694-2639 Vol. 8 No. 5, pp. 18-26, ©IJHSS Likert Scale Development: Construction and Evaluation of Home Environment Scale Mukhtar Ahmad Wani & Aejaz Masih Department of Educational Studies Jamia Millia Islamia (A Central University) New Delhi, India- 110025 Abstract The purpose of present paper is the construction and evaluation of home environment scale. An attempt was made in this study to evaluate validity, reliability and to determine the appropriate standards to interpret the results of home environment scale. The final form of the scale includes ten sub-scales (dimensions) with seventy one (71) items/statements. The scale was validated for its content by more than 20 experts from the related and allied fields. The construct validity was calculated through factor analysis for items, and by correlating the score of each sub-scale with the total score of the sale. t test for two independent samples was used (high group 27% and low group27%) to find the discrimination validity for each sub-scale. The reliability for the whole scale as well as for each sub-scale was calculated through Alpha coefficients. From the reliability coefficients, it can be concluded that the scale is reliable as all the values are above .70. Finally, the appropriate standards for interpretation of scores based on stanine procedures are highlighted. However, interpreting the scores through stanine the investigator has to calculate z values of raw scores after the data have been collected from all the sample participants. The stanine procedure is the standardized technique for the categorization of the scores for meaningful interpretation of Likert scale responses. Keywords: Construction, Evaluation, Home Environment, Likert Scale, Reliability, Validity, Stanine Introduction Various instruments have been developed for assessing the home environment in foreign as well as in India. Some foreign scales are developed by Watson 1957, Williamson 1961, Moos 1974, Bradley 1981 and some Indian scales by Prerna Mohete, Beena Shah, Reena Sharma, K. S. Mishra etc. As we know the foreign tools are not culture fair and this makes them unsuitable for use in the particular context. Another issue with these foreign scales (Moos) is that they lack reliability with respect to adolescent sample (Boyd, C. P. et al, 1997). Other issue is their language of the items in which they have been originally developed and their lack of measuring all the major dimensions, that are universally acceptable on which home as a social institution is based. Similarly, Indian scales measuring home environment are not without the pitfalls like that of assessment of home environment for the age group of 20 to 50 years (Reena Sharma) and with only five areas of home environment and others only devised for measuring home environment for I-VIII class students (Prerna Mohete). While others for high school students (Beena Shah) and others assessing only the psychological nature of the home (Mishra). Hence a complete tool to assess the major parameters on which any institutions is based seems to be lacking in these
  • 19. 19 http://aajhss.org/index.php/ijhss scales and this demands a new scale to be developed, which measures broad three parameters of home environment which are interrelationship parameter, individual development parameter and system organization parameter. Objectives 1. To construct a Likert scale measuring the attitude of senior secondary school students towards their home environment. 2. To evaluate the validity of home environment scale. 3. To evaluate the reliability of home environment scale. 4. To formulate appropriate standards to interpret the results of home environment scale. Methodology Descriptive statistical method was employed in the present study. The process of description as employed in this research study goes beyond mere gathering and tabulation of data. It involves an element of interpretation of the meaning or significance of what is described. Thus, description is combined with comparison or contrast involving measurement, classification, interpretation and evaluation. Sample The samples of the study is comprised of 106 senior secondary school students currently enrolled in class 11th of different (Govt./Private) schools of South Kashmir of Jammu and Kashmir during the year 2015. This study was delimited to students of class 11th. Secondly the age range of the members of the population is 16-17 years. Stages of tool construction As with the tool construction, there is no total agreement of experts about the precise steps for tool construction. Nevertheless, when constructing a tool, it is necessary to go through a number of stages in order to ensure its good quality (Alderson, 1995). Although their needs a proper procedure for tool construction. The graphical representation for the stages of tool construction as depicts in figure 1. Fig. 1: Stages of tool construction
  • 20. 20 http://aajhss.org/index.php/ijhss Preparation of the preliminary draft Having gone through the literature and previous tools as mentioned above in introduction, ten dimensions based on three mentioned broad parameters were selected which are related to the overall environment of home. Interrelation parameter includes dimension as Family integration, Conflict, Self-expression and Social climate of the family. Individual development parameter includes dimension as Guidance provided at home, Emotional support, Success orientation and Independence and System organization parameter includes dimensions like Organization and management of family and Control. Then the items associated with ten dimensions were selected and each item was selected according to the nature of the dimension. For the selection of the items, previous tools and studies related with home environment were consulted along with the available literature. While selecting items, the nature of item measured the desired dimension of home environment were taken into consideration. In this way the initial draft was prepared and 110 items (11 in each dimension) were included in the scale. Then, draft items were given to experts from different universities who were well versed in the field and scale construction with a request to review the statements and evaluate their content accuracy coverage, editorial quality and suggestions for additions, deletion and modification of items. On the basis of 80% of unanimously 30 items were deleted and 80 items were retained, which are reported below with the number of items: Table 1: Dimensions with number items in HES Dimension No. of Items A. Family integration 8 B. Social climate of the family 8 C. Guidance (Assistance) provided at home 8 D. Organization and management of the family 8 E. Conflict 8 F. Emotional Support 8 G. Success Orientation 8 H. Control 8 I. Self-expression 8 J. Independence 8 Try-out of the tool The initial format with 80 items was administered on the sample of 106 higher secondary school students from Kashmir (J&K). This is an attitude scale measuring the children‟s attitude towards their family environment. The scale requires pupils to tell the favorableness or unfavorableness with which a particular behavior has been observed by them in their homes, i.e., he/she is requested to tell whether they are Strongly Agree, Agree, Undecided, Disagree and Strongly Disagree respectively with the items in the scale. Scoring of the Responses to HES Items There are five options namely, “Strongly Agree”, “Agree, “Undecided, “Disagree” and “Strongly Disagree”, for each statement of the scale. 5 marks were assigned to 'Strongly Agree', 4 marks to 'Agree', 3 marks to 'Undecided', 2 mark to „Disagree', and 1 marks to „Strongly Disagree' responses and for negative items scoring is done in reverse order like 1,2,3,4,and 5 for Strongly Agree, Agree, Undecided, Disagree and Strongly Agree respectively. Then the marks were counted which were assigned to A, B, C, D, E, F, G, H, I and J dimension statements and then they were added to get the total composite score on the particular dimension. Ten composite
  • 21. 21 http://aajhss.org/index.php/ijhss scores for ten dimensions of the scale consists the children‟s attitude towards their home environment. Item Analysis For assessing the item analysis bi-serial correlation was used to sharpen the scale. The responses were collected and scored. Individual item score was correlated with the total score of the tool. Item analysis was done for the 106 response sheets by using Item Vs Whole correlation method. The sum of the scores on each dimension of value was calculated. Then „r‟ was calculated by correlating the individual item and the corresponding component score. The correlation coefficient at the 5% level of significance is 0.196 when the degree of freedom is 100 (Best, J. W. 2006). So the items having „r‟ values 0.196 and above were selected. It was found that out of the total 80 items, there are 71 items which are having significant correlations with the total score of the scale except 9 items which are having no significant correlation with the total score of the tool. The correlation table is given below: Table 2: r Values for HES Item No. r value Item r value item r value 1 .323** 29 .298** 57 .410** 2 .510** 30 .258** 58 .302** 3 .200* 31 .219* 59 .380** 4 .590** 32 .214* 60 .543** 5 .527** 33 .349** 61 .262** 6 .574** 34 .218* 62 .431** 7 .493** 35 .106 63 .287** 8 .355** 36 .268** 64 .409** 9 .476** 37 .191* 65 .341** 10 .096 38 .289** 66 .281** 11 .372** 39 .119 67 .036 12 .559** 40 .235* 68 .436** 13 .465** 41 .331** 69 .260** 14 .629** 42 .365** 70 .315** 15 .327** 43 .230* 71 .423** 16 .299** 44 .389** 72 .309** 17 .412** 45 .334** 73 .301** 18 .238* 46 .130 74 .262** 19 .349** 47 .332** 75 .378** 20 .028 48 .226* 76 .249** 21 .487** 49 .377** 77 .343** 22 .407** 50 .435** 78 .153 23 .329** 51 .301** 79 .357** 24 .384** 52 .209* 80 .342** 25 .214* 53 .380** Bold Italic items not selected 26 .212* 54 .322** 27 .245* 55 .141 28 .181 56 .205* ** Correlation is Significant at 0.01 * Correlation is Significant at 0.05
  • 22. 22 http://aajhss.org/index.php/ijhss From the perusal of table 2 above, it is clearly reflected that some of the item (bold and italics) were not having a significant correlation with the total scores of the scale and hence were deleted. After the rejection of 9 unsuitable items from the scale, a total of 71 items in ten dimensions of home environment scale were selected which are shown below in table 3 along with the possible range of scores: Table 3: No. of items and range of scores in each dimension of HES Evaluation of tool validity When a test measures what it has been suposed to measure, is said to be valid. To determine the validity of the test, the researchers tested face validity, construct validity and discrimination validity. Face validity or content validity The content validity of the „Home Environment Scale‟ was tested by more than 20 experts. It is evident from the assessment of experts that items of the test are directly related to the different dimensions of Home Environment. Construct validity In order to find out the construct validity, the researcher calculated correlation between each sub-scale‟s score and total score of the scale. Table 4: Correlation between Each Dimension and Total Score From the perusal of the table 4 above, it can be concluded that the correlation coefficient of all dimensions (.712, .684, .609, .379, .431, .519, .513, .616, .550, and .515 respectively) are significant at .01. This indicates that all dimensions are related to home environment and the tool has good construct validity. Dimension No. of Items Possible range of scores A Family integration 8 8-40 B Social climate of the family 7 7-35 C Guidance (Assistance) provided at home 7 7-35 D Organization and management of the family 7 7-35 E Conflict 6 6-30 F Emotional Support 7 7-35 G Success Orientation 7 7-35 H Control 7 7-35 I Self-expression 8 8-40 J Independence 7 7-35 Total 71 1-355 Dimension ‘r’ values Dimension ‘r’ values A 0.712** F 0.519** B 0.684** G 0.513** C 0.609** H 0.616** D 0.379** I 0.550** E 0.431** J 0.515**
  • 23. 23 http://aajhss.org/index.php/ijhss Factor Analysis However the scale was also subjected to exploratory factor analysis as the minimum number of cases required for factor analysis is 100 (Kline, 1986). All the ten components were retained as the eigenvalues are above 1. From the exploratory factor analysis items loading .4 were selected and items below .4 were dropped from the scale. From the factor analysis it can be concluded that all the items are measuring the same construct. Discrimination validity To find out the discrimination validity of the items the researchers used item analysis (difficulty level value and discrimination value). For knowing the level of discrimination validity for each dimension of the scale, ‘t’ test for two independent samples was used (high group 27% and low group 27%). Finally the discrimination validity of whole test was also determined by using ‘t’ test. Discrimination validity for each domain and whole test is given in the table below. It indicates that all ‘t’ values are significant at level 0.01 and the means of high group are also higher than low group which support the high validity of home environment scale. Table 5: t values for each dimension of the HES ** Significant at 0.01 level Reliability of the Home Environment Scale The degree of consistency among test scores is called reliability. The values of reliability coefficients (Cronbach alpha) for each sub-scale and for the whole scale are shown below: Dimensions Group N Mean Std. D Df t value A High Low 28 28 28.32 14.71 1.44 1.38 54 36.03** B High Low 28 28 24.17 11.57 1.94 0.92 54 31.00** C High Low 28 28 24.17 12.50 1.18 1.47 54 32.58** D High Low 28 28 24.46 12.46 2.31 1.34 54 23.69** E High Low 28 28 19.85 9.32 2.64 1.46 54 18.41** F High Low 28 28 24.39 12.57 1.49 1.28 54 31.64** G High Low 28 28 24.32 13.21 1.46 1.47 54 28.25** H High Low 28 28 27.03 14.39 1.66 1.19 54 32.60** I High Low 28 28 24.85 13.53 1.64 1.83 54 24.27** J High Low 28 28 23.78 12.67 1.37 1.18 54 32.40** TOTAL High Low 28 28 245.39 126.64 16.60 12.93 54 29.70**
  • 24. 24 http://aajhss.org/index.php/ijhss Table 6: Reliability coefficients of HES Dimensions Alpha Dimensions Alpha A .753 G .747 B .774 H .736 C .708 I .706 D .729 J .725 E .781 Total Reliability of the Scale .895F .719 Final form The final form of the scale along with serial no. of items for affirmative and negative statements is presented in the below table 7. Table 7: Item Presentation in the final form of HES: Dimension Affirmative items Negative items A Family integration 1, 2, 3, 4, 5 6, 7, 8 B Social climate of the family 10, 11, 12, 14, 15 9, 13 C Guidance (Assistance) provided at home 16, 18, 20, 21, 22 17, 18 D Organization and management of the family 23, 24, 26, 26, 29 27, 28 E Conflict 30, 31, 32 33, 34, 35 F Emotional Support 36, 37, 38, 39, 40 41, 42 G Success Orientation 43, 44, 46, 46, 47 48, 49 H Control 50, 51, 52, 54, 55 53, 56, I Self-expression 57, 58, 59, 60, 61, 62 63, 64 J Independence 65, 66, 67, 69, 71 68, 70 Table 8: Scoring table for all dimensions of HES Dimensions A B C D E F G H I J Score Mean Stanine The table 8 above represents the scoring for each dimension of the scale. The table is blank, because the raw scores, mean and stanines will differ from sample to sample. The table is to be filled after the data have been collected. The interpretation of the scores is done separately for each dimension based on the Z value. Then the nine levels based on Z values ranging from -1.75
  • 25. 25 http://aajhss.org/index.php/ijhss to +1.75 are to be divided. The stanine procedure is the standardized technique for the categorization of the scores for meaningful interpretation. Results After following these steps to construct the scale and after analyzing the data from the first and the last application by using adequate statistical methods, it has been concluded that:  The study has produced a scale measuring the attitude of senior secondary school students towards their home environment. This scale includes 71 items which measures ten dimensions of home environment viz, Family integration, Social climate of the family, Guidance (Assistance) provided at home, Organization and management of the family, Conflict, Emotional Support, Success Orientation, Control, Self-expression and Independence.  The scale has been validated through content, construct and discrimination validity. The content validity has been evaluated by experts, construct validity has been calculated by Pearson‟s correlation. The correlation coefficients of all dimensions are (.712, .684, .609, .379, .431, .519, .513, .616, .550, and .515 respectively) which are significant at .01 level. This indicates that all dimensions are related to home environment and the scale has good construct validity. The discrimination validity has been evaluated through „t‟ test between high group 27% and low group 27%. All „t‟ values are significant at level 0.01 and the means of high group are also higher than low group which support the high validity of HES.  The reliability of the scale was evaluated by calculating Alpha Cronbach Coefficient. All reliability coefficient values are above .70. Thus home environment scale is a reliable scale whose reliability is 0.89 and the reliability for each dimension of HES is .75, .77, .70, .72, .78, .71, .74, .73, .70, & .72 respectively.  To categorize the students into different categories with respect to their attitude towards home environment, the researchers used the stanine procedures. References Anastasi, A. (1987). Psychological Testing. New York: Macmillan Co. Bandhana, & Sharma, D. (2012). A Study of Home Environment and Reasoning Ability among Secondary School Students. Developing Country Studies, 2 (1), 73-80. Retrieved from www.iiste.org. Best, J. W., & Kahn, J. V. (2010). Research in Education. New Delhi: PHI Learning Ltd. Biel, A. (1986). Childrens Spatial Knowledge about Their Home Environment. Children's Environments Quarterly, 3(4), 2-9. Boyd, C. P. et al, (1997). The Family Environment Scale: Reliability and Normative Data for an Adolescent Sample. Family Process36:369-373, retrieved from https://www.researchgate.net/publication/51334514 Cohen, L., Manion, L., & Morrison, K. (2007). Research Methods in Education (6th ed.). New York: Routledge. Cunningham, J. B., & Aldrich, J. O. (2012). Using SPSS: An Interactive Hands-On Approach. New Delhi: Sage Publications India Pvt Ltd. Ebel, R. L., & Frisbie. (2004). Essentials of Educational Measurement. New Delhi: PHI Learning Pvt Ltd. Ferguson, G. A., & Yashio, T. (1989). Statistical Analysis in Psychology and Education. New York: McGraw Hill Book Co. Field, A. (2014). Discoevring Statistics using IBM SPSS Statiatics (4th ed.). New Delhi:Sage Publications India Pvt Ltd. Garret, H. E. (1973). Statistics in Psychology and Education. Bombay Vakils: Feffer & Simons Pvt Ltd. Guilford, J. P. (1987). Psychometric Methods. New York: McGraw Hill. Guilford, J. P., & Beryamin, F. (1946). Fundamental Statistics in Psychology and Education. Singapore: McGraw Hill Book Co.
  • 26. 26 http://aajhss.org/index.php/ijhss Gupta, N., Joshi, R. & Pasbola, H. (2008). Effect of Home Environment upon Emotional Maturity among female adolescents of Joint and Nuclear Family Structure. Behavioural Scientist, 9(2): 87- 92. Kundu, G. (1975). A New Technique of Attitude Measurement. Calcutta: Annupurna Publishing House McLeod, S. A. (2008). Likert Scale. Retrieved from www.simplypsychology.org/likertscale.htm Mishra, K. S. (1989). Manual of Home Environment Inventory. Allahabad: Ankur Psychological Agency, India. Mishra, S., & Bamba, V. (2012). Impact of family environment on academic achievement of secondary school students in science subject. International Journal of Research in Economics & Social Sciences, 2(5), 42-49. Mohanraj, R and Latha (2005). Perceived family environment in relation of adjustment and academic achievement. Journal of the Indian Academy of Applied Psychology: 31(1-2): 18-23. Thondike, R. M., & Tracy, C. T. (2011). Measurement and Evaluation in Psychology. New Delhi: PHI Learning Pvt Ltd. Wani, M. A., & Masih, A. (2015). Facilitating Learning by linking the two Environments: Interface between Home and School through Technology. International Education Conference on Learning Technologies in Education (pp. 334-342). New Delhi: Excel India Publishers.
  • 27. 27 http://aajhss.org/index.php/ijhss International Journal of Humanities and Social Sciences p-ISSN: 1694-2620 e-ISSN: 1694-2639 Vol. 8 No. 5, pp. 27-36, ©IJHSS Iqbal’s Response to Modern Western Thought: A Critical Analysis Dr. Mohammad Nayamat Ullah Associate Professor Department of Arabic University of Chittagong, Bangladesh Abdullah Al Masud PhD Researcher Dept. of Usuluddin and Comparative Religion International Islamic University Malaysia (IIUM) ABSTRACT Muhammad Iqbal (1873-1938) is a prominent philosopher and great thinker in Indian Sub- continent as well as a dominant figure in the literary history of the East. His thought and literature are not simply for his countrymen or for the Muslim Ummah alone but for the whole of humanity. He explores his distinctive thoughts on several issues related to Western concepts and ideologies. Iqbal had made precious contribution to the reconstruction of political thoughts. The main purpose of the study is to present Iqbal‟s distinctive thoughts and to evaluate the merits and demerits of modern political thoughts. The analytical, descriptive and criticism methods have been applied in conducting the research through comprehensive study of his writings both in the form of prose and poetry in various books, articles, and conferences. It is expected that the study would identify distinctive political thought by Iqbal. It also demonstrates differences between modern thoughts and Iqbalic thoughts of politics. Keywords: Iqbal, western thoughts, democracy, nationalism, secularism 1. INTRODUCTION Iqbal was not only a great poet-philosopher of the East but was also among the profound, renowned scholars and a brilliant political thinker in the twentieth century of the world. The contemporary scholars acknowledge his influence in modern movement. His literature was a source of inspiration for the hopeless Muslims who had subtle ability, potential and intellectual capability. He endeavoured substantially for the sake of mankind. He can be best described as the embodiment of “being a beacon of light in the dark night of the wilderness” (Khalil, 2000:182), a characteristic most needed for the Muslim Ummah. Iqbal was a „warner‟ who made a clarion call to the oppressed of the world to rediscover the infinite sources of their creative power and develops their faith. His message was not simply for his countrymen, nor for the Muslim Ummah alone but for the whole of humanity (Munawwar: 1982). This is study investigates modern western thoughts of Iqbal from Islamic perspective. It also attempts to clarify Iqbal‟s position on the modern Western political philosophy. In particular, his views on democracy, nationalism, secularism, capitalism, socialism and Liberalism are analyzed.
  • 28. 28 http://aajhss.org/index.php/ijhss 2. BIOGRAPHICAL SKETCH OF IQBAL AND HIS WORKS Iqbal was born at Sialkot in the Punjab province of present Pakistan on 9 November, 1877. He hailed from a family descended from Kashmiri Brahmins of Supra-caste1 , who had embraced Islam in 17th Century (Mustansir: 2006). Iqbal refers to his Brahmin ancestry in several writings. His father Shaikh Noor Mohammad was a skilled and enterprising businessman but a pious Sufi saint and a God loving man (Munawwar: 1982). His mother Imam Bibi was a generous and kind but also a deeply religious woman. Iqbal completed his early education in Sialkot and migrated to Lahore, Pakistan in 1895. He studied under a teacher named Maulvi Mir Hasan (1844-1929), who was a renowned scholar of Islamic Studies, Persian and Arabic. His teacher was impressed by the inborn poetic talent of Iqbal and encouraged him to continue his writing (Munawwar: 1982). On May 5, 1893, Iqbal was admitted to the Scotch Mission College2 in Sialkot and successfully passed his intermediate examination in1895. At the same year he got enrolled at B.A. in the Lahore Government College. In 1897 he graduated and got admit in the same collage for Master‟s Program in the Department of Philosophy. Iqbal‟s academic performance was excellent and continued brilliant record result at all level and won many gold medals (Mir, 2006). At Lahore Iqbal came under the influence of Sir Thomas Arnold, who was a profound professor in Philosophy and had a echoing insight in Arabic and Islamic Studies. Thomas‟s role influenced him to study the Western thoughts and instigated him into the modern methods of criticism. In 1899, he obtained the Master‟s degree and was appointed as a Macleod Reader in the Oriental College, Lahore, whereas in March 1904, he joined as an assistance Professor in the Department of Philosophy and English at the Government College, Lahore (Hafeez: 1971). As Iqbal was advised by Thomas Arnold, he intended to go Europe for higher education in 1905. Iqbal studied both in England and Germany. He studied in England at the Lincoln‟s Inn qualifying for Bar (Hilal: 1995) as a barrister and at the Trinity College of Cambridge, where he studied with R. A. Nicolson, a renowned orientalist and John M. E. McTaggart, idealist Metaphysician. At the same time he went to Germany and got admitted in the University of Munich, where he was awarded his PhD degree on his dissertation entitled “The Development of Metaphysics in Persia” on November 4, 1907 (Hilal: 1995). Iqbal returned from Europe to Lahore in August 1908 (Vahid: 1948) and began his professional career as Professor, lawyer and poet. During his study in Europe, he developed his ideas and thoughts, where “he was vivacious, gregarious, eager to engage himself in scholarly discussion with his juniors and superiors and politely self-assertive” (Hilal: 1995:36). His stay in Europe provided him with the opportunity to observe carefully the realities of Western societies. At his time, the modern methods of criticism in true sense had not been developed in India. As Iqbal employed the method of comparison between Islamic thoughts and Western ones, critical analysis of then several philosophical aspects and so on, he was regarded as one of the pioneers of the modern methods of criticism. He realized the shortcomings and dark aspects of modern Western civilization, where chaos and crisis are visible in human life more than ever. That is why, 1 According to Vanina, in mediaeval Indian regions there existed big supra-caste bound by status, corporate interests and common socio-ethical and moral values (2012: 139) which oversaws the political, economic and juridical spheres of everyday existence (Gadgil & Guha, 1993: 113). This supra-caste presupposed a certain hierarchy, and contradictions and conflicts within each of them (Vanina, 2012: 134). There are various castes; however, in Indian societies who are peculiar in terms of beliefs, culture and social phenomenon which explanation is not relevant to this article. As long time ago, Iqbal’s ancestors embraced Islam, generation after generation the influence and traditions of castes gradually disappeared 2 This college was later renamed Murray Collage, which exists under that name.
  • 29. 29 http://aajhss.org/index.php/ijhss he highly criticized Western political thoughts and civilization as well. Although, Iqbal spent his time in law practice, cultural activities and literary works, he later took part in politics and became an elected Legislative Council of the Punjab in 1926. In 1930, he was appointed president of the annual session of the Muslim League and delivered a remarkable address which led to a new idea of a separate Muslim state in Indian subcontinent. (Mujahid: 1986). He also represented India at the Round Table Conference in London in 1931 and 1932 (Vahid: 1948). Iqbal was a creative author in both poetry and prose a he left prolific intellectual works for the next generation. He wrote 17 books in Persian, Urdu and English. Among his 12,000 verses of poetry, about 7,000 verses are in Persian3 . Asrar-e-Khudi (Secrets of the Self) was the first work of Iqbal in Persian composed in 1919, which deals with human personality or individual self. Bang-i Dara (The Call of the Caravan Bell) was the first work in Urdu which was written in 1924 (Hilal: 1995). Iqbal also composed two books in English on the topic of “The Department of Metaphysics in Persia”, which is a valuable contribution in the history of Muslim Philosophy, and “The Reconstruction of Religious Thought in Islam”, where he attempted to restructure the history of Islamic thought. Besides these, he wrote hundreds of articles, short essays, and letters as well as issued several statements. All these publications would help researchers to understand the real perception of Iqbal in various subjects. It is stated by Ali (1978:17) that, “… a leading exponent of recent interpretations of Islam, Iqbal was in correspondence with people from all walks of life. Religious leaders, journalists, politicians and scholars were his life-long correspondents with whom he exchanged views on various subjects”. William O. Douglas, justice of the Supreme Court of USA says: “that (Iqbal's) simple tomb is a place of pilgrimage for me. For Iqbal was a man who belonged to all races; his concepts had universal appeal. He spoke to the conscience of men of goodwill whatever their tongue, whatever their creed” (Hafeez, 1971). Among the Muslim scholars of South Asia, Iqbal was the first who defined the concept of state in Islam. In 1930, Iqbal delivered a famous presidential speech at Allahabad where he pointed out a way for the political deadlock in the Indian subcontinent. He also stated: “I would like to see the Punjab, North West Frontier Province, Sindh and Baluchistan amalgamated into a single state of self- government within the British Empire or without the British Empire...I therefore, demand the formation of a consolidated Muslim state in the best interests of India and Islam” (Sing & Roy, 2011: 174). This statement proves that Iqbal wanted a Muslim state for the Muslims in North- West of India where Muslim could develop and nurture their culture, religion, language, literature besides other religious communities of India. For long time of the past, two religions (Muslims and Hindus) were dominating. To the great extent peace, social security and justice in India were dependent on their mutual understanding. Islamic ideologies of unity, equity, humanity and justice played an important role through ages all over the globe in alleviating any differences among human beings. Indeed, Iqbal emphasized this fact. Thus, he also believed that the peace and prosperity of India would depend upon Muslims and Hindus when the spirit of mutual understanding, love and unity in diversity will be maintained. However, he sought a separate state for Muslim; to maintain the spiritual life of Muslim community; to protect the Muslim culture from harmful influences4 and rapid invasion of un-Islamic elements. In the beginning of 1938, Iqbal‟s health sharply declined and it took a serious turn for the worse on March 25, 1938. Finally, he passed away at 5:15 in the morning on April 21, 1938. Rabindronath Tagore sent a condolence message as soon as he heard the demise news of Allama Iqbal that, “The death of Sir Mohammad Iqbal has created a void in literature which like a deep wound, will 3 2 http://www.poemhunter.com/allama-muhammad-iqbal/biography/ Retrieved on 20-07-2016, and https://en.wikipedia.org/wiki/Muhammad_Iqbal Retrieved on 07-10-2016 4 Harmful influences or behaviours that are not allowed in Islam like polytheism, injustice and any unethical behaviour such as free mixing, drug addiction, illegal sexual relation and so on.
  • 30. 30 http://aajhss.org/index.php/ijhss take a long time to heal. India which occupies but a limited place in the world can ill afford to lose a poet whose poetry has such appeal” (Maire, 1981: 4). 3. IQBAL ON MODERN WESTERN THOUGHTS Muhammad Iqbal was not only a prominent scholar, writer, poet and philosopher but also he was a political and social reformer of the Indian subcontinent. He was the first personality who generated a unique idea of an independent Muslim state in Indian subcontinent. Through His scholarly writings he more detailed his philosophical thoughts and ideas, especially his political ideas. His political thoughts are deeply rooted in Islamic values which are derived from the Holy Quran and the Sunnah. According to Iqbal, “Islam believes in a universal polity- a politico- religious system or social polity- based on fundamentals that were revealed to the Prophet” (Sh. Muhammad Ashraf, 1952:238) His attention was to protect the Muslim Ummah from the attack of Western thought and to encourage and preserve their own cultural heritage and thoughts. 3.1 DEMOCRACY Democracy is a political system of a state and government where sovereignty and power belong 5to the majority people and the government is elected by people‟s opinion through election. That is why democracy as a political ideal was defined by Abraham Lincoln as “Democracy is a government of the people, for the people, by the people”, while Alex Woolf (2007:4) described it as “a system in which people decide matters together, or collectively”. Haneef Nadvi (1973:205) explains that, “Democracy is composed of two Greek components; one means the people and the other means government and law”. Technically, democracy is applied to a system of government for the majority. Iqbal was a democrat, who was actively involved with the politics of Indian subcontinent. In 1926, Iqbal participated in the election, where he was elected to the Punjab Legislative council. L.S. May (1974:179) states that Iqbal was “an active member of this Council, speaking often on land revenue and taxation, demanding greater justice in land assessment and even land revenue deductions in hardship cases”. Although Iqbal preferred democracy, he has pointed out some demerits in this regard. Iqbal 1opposed the Western democracy as it is a methodology rather than an ideology or philosophy (Qureshi, 1983). He had his own observations and he suggested a new term known as „spiritual democracy‟5 (Iqbal, 2011a:180), which is based on the Quranic teaching. This spiritual democracy means, “A democracy where laws of God Almighty are observed and enforced” (Munawwar, 2001:142). In Western democracy, sovereignty belongs to the people. As opposed to this, sovereignty in Iqbal‟s “spiritual democracy” belongs to God alone, which is an inseparable part in the Islamic world-view. Iqbal was a lover of innovations. That‟s why when democracy evolved as a system of government; he welcomed it as thought that the new system might be helpful in alleviating the sufferings of the exploited and oppressed people (Abbas, 1997: xxiii). But he soon realized its serious drawbacks and became a strong critic of the Western democratic form of government where persons are counted, not weighted; it is a material fact but not of personality, which is a spiritual fact. He says that, Democracy is a form of government in which People are counted but their worth is not assessed (Iqbal, 1941:150). According to him, humanity needs three things today “a spiritual interpretation of the universe, spiritual emancipation of mankind and basic 5 The Spiritual democracy is based on the principle of Tawhid, which speaks bout unity of thoughts, action and value across humanity (Begum, 2001: 21). In case of spiritual democracy the Quran, Prophet’s Tradition, Ijma and Qiyas are the Sources of the Islamic law, which would be interpreted according to the demand of ages. This kind of democracy distinguishes man with significance that is sacred kind. ‘This democracy also recognizes value of individual and rejects blood relationship as a basis of human unity’ (Iqbal, 2011a:116).