SlideShare a Scribd company logo
1 of 86
CURRENT ISSUES - PERSPECTIVES AND REVIEWS
Tribute to Tinbergen: Public Engagement in Ethology
Julie Hecht* & Caren B. Cooper†
* Doctoral Program in Psychology, The Graduate Center, City
University of New York, New York, NY, USA
† Cornell Lab of Ornithology, Ithaca, NY, USA
(Invited Review)
Correspondence
Julie Hecht, Department of Psychology,
Hunter College and The Graduate Center,
City University of New York, 695 Park
Avenue, New York, NY 10065, USA.
E-mail: [email protected]
Received: October 4, 2013
Initial acceptance: October 19, 2013
Final acceptance: November 25, 2013
(M. Hauber)
doi: 10.1111/eth.12199
Keywords: citizen science, data quality,
animal behavior, informal science education
Abstract
Public engagement in research, called citizen science, has led to
advances
in a range of fields like astronomy, ornithology, and public
health. While
volunteers have been making and sharing observations
according to pro-
tocols set by researchers in numerous disciplines, citizen
science practices
are less common in the field of animal behavior. We consider
how citizen
science might be used to address animal behavior questions at
Tinbergen’s
four levels of analysis. We briefly review resources and
methods for
addressing technical issues surrounding volunteer
participation—such as
data quality—so that citizen science can make long-standing
contributions
to the field of animal behavior.
Introduction
‘Citizen science’ describes the various ways that mem-
bers of the public participate in genuine scientific
research (Cooper et al. 2007a; Silvertown 2009; Shirk
et al. 2012). Many citizen science projects arise from
communities with specific concerns (such as long-
term environmental monitoring), and some are
initiated by scientists to address specific research
objectives. Other projects have additional goals to
increase science literacy (Bonney et al. 2009a).
Research via public engagement is possible because
people enjoy making natural history observations and
sharing these observations with professionals and
peers. In addition, Web 2.0 and mobile technologies
facilitate mass participation. Consequently, citizen
science projects have arisen in a myriad of disciplines,
including ecology, phenology, macroecology, public
health, natural resource management, hydrology,
urban planning, meteorology, math, volcanology, and
various taxon-specific fields such as entomology,
ornithology, and herpetology. In recent years, citizen
science has enabled substantial contributions to the
fields of astronomy (Lintott et al. 2008), medicine
(Khatib et al. 2011a,b), and climate change (Morisette
et al. 2009).
Currently, the field of animal behavior is under-
represented among citizen science projects. Animal
behavior research integrates diverse methodological
approaches to investigate a wide array of scientific
questions about the behavior of wild and domesti-
cated animals in natural and captive settings. Public
observations of occurrence data (e.g., wildlife sight-
ings; Sn€all et al. 2011), count data (e.g., number of
birds; Cooper et al. 2007b), simple measurements
(e.g., rainfall; Cifelli et al. 2005), and even phenologi-
cal stages (e.g., budburst; Morisette et al. 2009) are
more common applications of citizen science, in part
because such data likely require simpler protocols
than most animal behavior data.
The dearth of citizen science research in animal
behavior is not necessarily due to lack of general pub-
lic interest. Pet-keeping is a global phenomenon
(Serpell 1996); wildlife tourism and wildlife docu-
mentaries are popular (Reynolds & Braithwaite 2001;
Nelson & Fijn 2013); zoo attendance is on the rise in
parts of the world (Davey 2007); and people are
already expressing interest in animal behavior citizen
Ethology 120 (2014) 207–214 © 2013 Blackwell Verlag GmbH
207
Ethology
ethology
international journal of behavioural biology
science projects (Foster et al. 2011; Williams et al.
2012). In the USA, one in four adults watches wildlife
(USFWS 2011), and the numerous websites that
stream live video of pets (e.g., SPCA cams) and wild-
life (e.g., nesting birds) are extremely popular (e.g.,
the Cornell Lab of Ornithology nest cams receive
millions of unique viewers from 175 countries,
including up to 9,000 simultaneous views of live-
streaming video).
Citizen science contributions could enhance animal
behavior investigations by increasing observations on
time and geographic scales (e.g., latitudinal gradients,
urbanization gradients, comparing populations, etc),
and these projects also have the potential to process
massive amounts of data (e.g., video or photo tag-
ging). In essence, citizen scientists provide an extra
pair of hands—or extra thousands of pairs. While a
small team of researchers documented the behavior of
the colonial orb-weaving spider during a solar eclipse
in Veracruz, Mexico (Uetz et al. 1994), the team
might have used citizen scientists to capture behav-
ioral data on additional populations or species during
the short-lived event. Without the help of citizen sci-
entists, researchers are effectively ‘missing’ variation
among animals.
Field research, behavior observation, and behavior
coding can be time-consuming, challenging to coordi-
nate, and expensive. Trained volunteers might offer
lower-cost options for collecting and/or scoring
behavior (Williams et al. 2012). Depending on the
study design and complexity, volunteers can be used
to ensure blind coding.
Citizen science methods could facilitate access to a
wide variety of species in different contexts and foster
integration of studies of natural populations and pop-
ulations under artificial selection. Additionally, an
expanded research platform offers the possibility of
capturing animals in novel contexts. Video and social
media, particularly YouTube, are rife with examples
of uncommon (Burn 2011) or previously unexplored
behaviors, such as interspecies play or object play
(Nelson & Fijn 2013). Behaviors captured in real-
world contexts can lead to new research directions,
and citizen scientists could provide behavioral video
footage.
We examine the realized and potential application
of citizen science to research animal behavior—the
inquiry about what animals do, how they do it, and
why they do it. Tinbergen (1963) framed the disci-
pline as encompassing four levels of analysis: causa-
tion, development, function, and phylogeny.
We use Tinbergen’s levels of analysis as a frame-
work to discuss a possible partnership between citizen
science and animal behavior research. We note that
citizen scientists could examine a particular behavior,
for example bird song, to generate data for answering
questions at all four levels (e.g., causation: ‘What day
length triggers bird song?’; development: ‘Do offspring
imitate songs of parents?’; function: ‘Do females
prefer certain song types?’; and phylogeny: ‘Which
species have similar songs?’). Professional scientists
can obtain a more robust understanding of an
observed behavior through investigations of non-
competing hypotheses at multiple levels. Other
questions might be best suited for investigation at one
particular level of analysis. We discuss a variety of
applications below, with the common theme that citi-
zen science participants can scale-up and advance
research that individual scientists could not do alone.
Additionally, we review the technical facets of a
union between animal behavior and citizen science,
giving attention to crucial details in training volun-
teers and handling volunteer data.
Citizen Science at Four Levels of Analysis
In the 1963 paper, On aims and methods of Ethology,
Tinbergen proposed an integrated approach to the
study of ethology to address the lack of a unified
public image of ethology and to resolve conceptual
differences between researchers. Since then, “how
questions,” exploring the immediate causes of behav-
ior, and “why questions,” exploring the evolutionary
forces behind behavior, have been pursued as
complementary approaches.
Causation
The causal basis of behavior focuses on stimuli and
mechanisms that trigger, cue, or precede a behavior.
For early ethologists, physiological and neural mecha-
nisms occurred in a ‘black box’ upon which they
could only speculate. Today, inquiry into causation
draws from genetics, endocrinology, physiology, and
neurobiology. Direct participation and input from citizen
scientists is already underway in neurobiology (Eyewire,
https://eyewire.org/signup) and genetics (Phylo) (Coo-
per et al. 2013). For domesticated and wild populations,
the collection of saliva and fecal samples could contrib-
ute to studies of stress physiology and measurements of
well-being to ultimately address a wide variety of behav-
ioral endocrinological questions.
Participant involvement could complement labo-
ratory findings by collecting behavioral data on
pre-screened or pre-selected populations. For exam-
ple, volunteers might assist research into relationships
Ethology 120 (2014) 207–214 © 2013 Blackwell Verlag
GmbH208
Public Engagement in Ethology J. Hecht & C. B. Cooper
between genetic markers and behavior by collecting
behavioral data on pre-selected animals (e.g., through
a web portal for annotating video) whose genes have
been mapped. Researchers recently determined that
particular allelic qualities of DRD4 and TH gene poly-
morphisms were associated with activity–impulsivity
in German Shepherds and Siberian Huskies, with
behavioral analysis stemming from approx. 250 dogs
(Kubinyi et al. 2012; Wan et al. 2013). Continued
exploration of gene variants in dogs could be coupled
with citizen science projects collecting behavioral data
on dogs with known genetic composition to generate
a more comprehensive understanding of behavioral
traits. Citizen science allows researchers to expand the
size of their data set without requiring the participants
to have any knowledge of the DNA sampling and gen-
otyping that go into pre-screening decisions.
The addition of a citizen science component can
increase the rigor of the research in at least two ways.
First, the volunteer contributions could ensure a dou-
ble-blind experiment. Second, the volunteers provide
observations from multiple observers, and researchers
can use a consensus tool to score behavior, analogous
to consensus tools (replication by multiple partici-
pants) common to online crowdsourcing efforts for
other research (Wiggins et al. 2011). Additionally,
volunteers can install cameras to record animal
behavior and facilitate remote coding by professional
researchers. For example, scientists used footage from
volunteer nest cameras to examine proximate control
of egg-laying behaviors in nestbox-dwelling bird
species (Cooper et al. 2009).
Development
Approaches to animal behavior from a developmental
perspective are also mechanistic, but with a focus on
changes in an organism’s capacity for expressing or
acquiring behaviors at different stages over the course
of a lifetime.
Citizen science offers a unique opportunity to inves-
tigate ontogenetic questions in a wide range of
species. For example, citizen scientists might assist in
the study of social behavior and document the devel-
opment of particular behavioral patterns—such as fac-
tors affecting the onset and development of play in
various species—or personality changes (Stamps &
Groothuis 2010). Volunteers could collect and report
data on single organisms—whether subjects be a com-
panion animal, a backyard nestbox or an annually
returning species—and generate substantial data
points for each individual. Ultimately, with many
volunteers, the increased sample sizes could make
full-scale statistical analysis possible (Dickinson et al.
2010).
Citizen scientists could also examine the develop-
ment of animal sensorium, such as in relation to mor-
phological traits. For example, assumptions about the
different sensory abilities in dog breeds abound, and
such differences might be expected given physiologi-
cal differences (McGreevy et al. 2003), but actual
behavioral and development data are wanting.
Researchers could design and implement a project
where participants conduct simple, in-home tests
examining behavior responses toward different stim-
uli at different ages, thereby offering additional data
on breed (and even species) sensory development.
These behavioral measures of development are com-
parable to traditional laboratory experiments (Lord
2013). But, with the help of citizen scientists,
researchers can obtain data on a much larger scale.
Zoo visitors and wildlife tourists can play a role in
monitoring individuals’ development. Zoo visitors
express interest in animal behavior studies (Bowler
et al. 2012) and could collect many more hours (or
years) of observations than possible by resource-
limited staff with many other care-taking duties. Such
efforts might elucidate the onset or development of
behaviors such as stereotypic motor patterns as well as
factors affecting the development of these behaviors.
Zoo visitors, however, can have bias toward collecting
observations of active animals (Altman 1998; Williams
et al. 2012).
Function
The analysis of function focuses on the pressures that
shape behaviors. The pervasive influence of the built
environment means that many organisms reside in
environmental conditions that differ in novel ways
from the conditions under which traits evolved. For
example, the anthropogenic sound levels near high-
ways and flight paths create novel selection pressures,
and some animals might be able to adjust their behav-
iors within a range of plasticity, such as birds singing
at higher pitches near roads (Slabbekoorn & Peet
2003; Slabbekoorn & den Boer-Visser 2006). Other
traits might exhibit limited phenotypic plasticity, and
disturbances may result in population decline. Fur-
thermore, both domesticated and non-domesticated
animals residing in homes or zoos will demonstrate
behaviors that might no longer be adaptive. Research-
ers can design participatory projects in which volun-
teers collect data on species in different environments
and investigate different hypotheses behind complex
behaviors. In an online project called CamClickr, par-
Ethology 120 (2014) 207–214 © 2013 Blackwell Verlag GmbH
209
J. Hecht & C. B. Cooper Public Engagement in Ethology
ticipants added behavior-category tags to images
archived from nest cameras to help investigate varia-
tions in nest attendance (Voss & Cooper 2010).
Because funding cycles offered by granting institu-
tions tend to span three- to five-year periods, collect-
ing long-term data sets can be challenging for
research scientists. The collection of such data sets
could be made possible through citizen science activi-
ties. Depending on the initiative, initial investment to
set up the project, and maintenance costs, could be
low with free online tools for managing projects (e.g.,
www.citsci.org), recruiting volunteers (e.g., www.
scistarter.com), and data archiving (e.g., www.data-
one.org). Citizen scientists could measure different
components of fitness such as survival to maturity,
longevity, mating success, and reproductive success.
Phylogeny
Behaviors can also be understood from a phylogenetic
perspective. By considering ancestry, researchers can
identify traits that are highly conserved and those that
are more recently evolved within the same lineage.
Because closely related species are found in everyday
environments, a citizen science project could collect a
wide variety of behavioral data on closely related spe-
cies in different environments that have adopted dif-
ferent survival strategies. By comparing closely
related species occurring on different continents (e.g.,
black-billed bagpie Pica hudsonia in America and the
European magpie Pica pica in Eurasia), citizen scien-
tists can provide simultaneous data sets without the
confound of temporal displacement typical of smaller-
scale research projects.
Not all citizen engagement in animal behavior pro-
jects fits neatly into Tinbergen’s levels of analysis.
Recently, Project: Play With Your Dog (Horowitz Dog
Cognition Lab, Barnard College) requested short vid-
eos depicting play between a dog and a person, with
the aim of capturing the wide array of behaviors and
conditions that encompass interspecific play. Video
submissions drew from around the world, and
although participants did not contribute directly to
behavioral analysis, citizen scientists had to think
about behavior in terms of how they and their dog
play together and provide behavioral descriptors in an
accompanying questionnaire.
Similarly, an online survey by Animal Welfare Indi-
cators asked participants to assist in developing a con-
tinuous behavioral scale of lameness for goats.
Participants watched nine videos and scored abnormal
gait, head-nodding, and spine curvature on a visual
analogue scale. Before participants began, each
behavior had a clear description, and participants
were schooled in measuring behavior as per the speci-
fications of the project.
Merging Ethology and Citizen Science Methods
Creating Successful Citizen Science Animal Behavior
Projects
Like all successful research endeavors, the design of
citizen science projects follows the steps of the scien-
tific method; the only difference is the additional layer
of complexity introduced by the involvement of the
lay public. Projects start with the formulation of scien-
tific questions, organization of a team of skilled
experts in relevant areas, and determination of appro-
priate data collection (Bonney et al. 2009b). Depend-
ing on the legal status and various protections offered
to different species, researchers might need to present
protocols to Institutional Animal Care and Use
Committees (Bayne 1998). Citizen scientists can learn
behavior-coding techniques—or relevant project-
related skills—in online videos or in person. The
research team can control quality before the project
begins by testing each prospective participant’s
accuracy and ability to collect meaningful data.
Behavioral data accuracy is a key concern, and pro-
jects incorporating citizen science data should con-
sider topics like observation quality, sampling bias,
and interobserver reliability (Dickinson et al. 2010;
Burghardt et al. 2012; Hart et al. 2012). Williams
et al. (2012) found that although zoo visitor interest
in the project was high, the activity budget data col-
lected on a group of captive otters were not accurate,
mostly because visitors did not adhere to the observa-
tion period; the researchers suggest that certain
sampling methods might be easier—or harder—for
observers.
Meanwhile, there are numerous ways to improve
data quality in citizen science (Cohn 2008; Silvertown
2009; Wiggins et al. 2011; Bonter & Cooper 2012).
Technologies like smartphones, multimedia, or inter-
active video could assist in time-keeping and behav-
ioral data collection (Parr et al. 2004; Aanensen et al.
2009). Data management tools, often through cyber-
infrastructure, improve efficiency (Newman et al.
2011). Additionally, some citizen science projects
demonstrate that certain types of people collect more
accurate data than others (Delaney et al. 2008), and
similar lessons can make their way into citizen science
projects in animal behavior.
Finally, researchers can be creative in where and
how volunteers access animals and how behavior will
Ethology 120 (2014) 207–214 © 2013 Blackwell Verlag
GmbH210
Public Engagement in Ethology J. Hecht & C. B. Cooper
be observed or documented. In some cases, behaviors
are not observed directly but are inferred. Phenology
research addresses the timing of life-cycle events,
including behaviors such as egg laying and migration
in birds, insects, and some reptiles and amphibians
(Head et al. 2013). Limited access to animals in situa-
tions where volunteers can obtain unbiased observa-
tions of life-cycle events may have been one of the
largest impediments to citizen science in animal
behavior. Now, Internet and remote-camera techno-
logies combined with crowdsourcing infrastructure
offer new opportunities to collect data on animals that
are difficult to access (Deng et al. 2012; Head et al.
2013; Loos & Ernst 2013). Live-streaming cameras
can be set up at areas with known activity—animal
nesting or gathering sites—so that people can docu-
ment behaviors at a distance. In addition to remote/
web-mediated studies, possibilities abound for studies
of companion animals (e.g., dogs, cats, rabbits, birds,
ferrets, fish, gerbils, and hamsters), zoo animals, ani-
mals viewed during nature tourism, and urban wild-
life such as squirrels, pigeons, rats, foxes, and coyotes.
Volunteers could document the behavior of animals
at zoos or wildlife tourism destinations, especially ani-
mals that handlers and keepers do not have time to
record. Special interest groups—such as the House
Rabbit Society, backyard chicken groups, and pet
breeders—offer significant potential for citizen science
projects.
Training and Resources for Animal Behavior Inquiry
Skill in observing and measuring animal behavior
does not necessarily follow from an appreciation of or
even cohabitation with animals. Members of the pub-
lic are generally interested in animals yet have a pro-
clivity not toward Tinbergen’s four levels of behavior,
but often toward anthropomorphic explanations of
behavior.
While the study of animal behavior is an attempt to
investigate ultimate and proximate questions about
behavior, humans often characterize animal behavior
through an anthropocentric lens (Foster et al. 2011).
People readily ascribe human characteristics to non-
humans. Subjective assessments of non-human
behavior can be consistent across observers, often
reifying existing beliefs regardless of whether they
have been substantiated (Bahlig-Pieren & Turner
1999; Morris et al. 2008; Horowitz 2011). Seminal
work by Heider & Simmel (1944) found that people
consistently attribute human emotions, aims, and
objectives to inanimate objects moving in a ‘human-
like’ manner. Companion animal species, mammals,
and non-human primates are more readily anthropo-
morphized than other species (Eddy et al. 1993), even
though seemingly ‘alien’ species, like cephalopods,
display complex behaviors and cognitive processes
(Fiorito & Scotto 1992; Moriyama & Gunji 1997).
Importantly, the appearance of behavioral homologies
should not imply similar meaning, such as the ‘smil-
ing’ chimpanzee or dolphin (Horowitz 2007).
Tendencies toward anthropomorphism are perhaps
a useful ‘hook’ when engaging the general public
in animal behavior projects. Recently, an online
video titled ‘Lol cats like stroking too’ (http://www.
youtube.com/watch?v=OFCRvjle2o8) accompanied a
publication on the neurons activated by massage-like
stroking to assist in public understanding of the
research (Vrontou et al. 2013). The video received
over 200,000 views in the first year.
Clearly defined project expectations and training
materials can minimize the effects of anthropomor-
phic inferences (Voss & Cooper 2010). Scientists can
provide a detailed ethogram, an explicit coding
scheme, and a clear behavior-sampling technique
(Martin & Bateson 2007). Behaviors in an ethogram
can be distinguished based on structural or empirical
characteristics, such as ‘raised lips at edges’, or conse-
quence-based descriptors, such as ‘threat’. Empirical
descriptors help create unambiguous behavioral
parameters (Lehner 1987) but might offer challenges
for unpracticed observers. Volunteers need a clear
understanding of the terms in which they are
expected to view and document behavior and have
the technology, such as a timer, to code appropriately.
Researchers determine who is being watched and
when and how to watch them. Some coding schemes
provide information on how much time an animal
spends performing various behaviors, while other
schemes monitor behavior frequencies (Altmann
1974). Researchers must match the skill level of
observers with the scientific question. The role of vol-
unteers in the study of animal behavior will largely
involve the production of meaningful behavioral data.
The challenge will be to offer effective guidance and
instruction to avoid anthropocentric assessments.
A number of institutions have created behavior-
observation instructional platforms. Ethosearch (www.
ethosearch.org), created by the Lincoln Park Zoo, is a
resource for researchers and students to store and view
ethograms and practice measuring animal behavior.
Living Links (www.living-links.org)—a joint enclosure
for squirrel monkeys (Saimiri sciureus) and capuchins
(Sapajus apella) at the Edinburgh Zoo—offers a
public-learning resource to observe and score the behav-
ior of both species (Macdonald & Whiten 2011). Cam-
Ethology 120 (2014) 207–214 © 2013 Blackwell Verlag GmbH
211
J. Hecht & C. B. Cooper Public Engagement in Ethology
Clickr was an online citizen science project to code the
behaviors of nesting birds from still images as a way of
carrying out scan sampling. Voss & Cooper (2010) cre-
ated a college-level laboratory unit in which students
can use CamClickr to create ethograms while learning
about observer bias, observation techniques, and defin-
ing and quantifying observations.
Conclusion
Animal behavior researchers might consider whether
and how citizen science could support and amplify
their studies. Volunteers can expand the scope and
scale of animal behavior studies by bringing attention
to novel or rare behaviors or providing access to
additional variation in animal behaviors. Volunteers,
too, can benefit from personal experiences engaging
in hands-on scientific interactions with animals.
Undoubtedly, challenges arise with the involve-
ment of citizen science in animal behavior, yet the
possibilities are great. Altmann’s (1974, p. 229) text
on behavior sampling methods stresses that there
should not be a disconnect between the laboratory
and the real world: ‘Such a restriction on research
would mean that the behavioral sciences would for-
ever forsake any hope of knowing whether their most
powerful theories have any relevance in the world of
behavior outside the laboratory.’ Citizen scientists
offer an additional, and untapped, opportunity for
ethological exploration.
Acknowledgements
Thanks to Felicity Muth, Mark Hauber, and Andrew
Fulmer for conceptual conversations. Many thanks to
reviewers for comments, which improved the manu-
script.
Literature cited
Aanensen, D. M., Huntley, D. M., Feil, E. J., al-Own, F. &
Spratt, B. G. 2009: EpiCollect: linking smartphones to
web applications for epidemiology, ecology and
community data collection. PLoS One 4, e6968.
Altman, J. D. 1998: Animal activity and visitor learning at
the zoo. Anthrozoos 11, 12—21.
Altmann, J. 1974: Observational study of behavior:
sampling methods. Behaviour 49, 227—267.
Bahlig-Pieren, Z. & Turner, D. C. 1999: Anthropomorphic
interpretations and ethological descriptions of dog and
cat behavior by lay people. Anthrozoos 12, 205—210.
Bayne, K. 1998: Developing guidelines on the care and use
of animals. Ann. N.Y. Acad. Sci. 862, 105—110.
Bonney, R., Ballard, H., Jordan, R., McCallie, E., Phillips,
T., Shirk, J. & Wilderman, C. C. 2009a: Public participa-
tion in scientific research: defining the field and assess-
ing its potential for informal science education. A CAISE
Inquiry Group Report. Center for Advancement of
Informal Science Education (CAISE), Washington, D.C.
Bonney, R., Cooper, C. B., Dickinson, J., Kelling, S.,
Phillips, T., Rosenberg, K. V. & Shirk, J. 2009b: Citizen
science: a developing tool for expanding science knowl-
edge and scientific literacy. Bioscience 59, 977—984.
Bonter, D. N. & Cooper, C. B. 2012: Data validation in
citizen science: a case study from Project FeederWatch.
Front. Ecol. Environ. 10, 305—307.
Bowler, M. T., Buchanan-Smith, H. M. & Whiten, A.
2012: Assessing public engagement with science in a
university primate research centre in a national zoo.
PLoS One 7, e34505.
Burghardt, G. M., Bartmess-LeVasseur, J. N., Browning, S.
A., Morrison, K. E., Stec, C. L., Zachau, C. E. & Free-
berg, T. M. 2012: Perspectives—Minimizing observer
bias in behavioral studies: a review and recommenda-
tions. Ethology 118, 511—517.
Burn, C. C. 2011: A vicious cycle: A cross-sectional study
of canine tail-chasing and human responses to it, using
a free video-sharing website. PLoS One 6, e26553.
Cifelli, R., Doesken, N., Kennedy, P., Carey, L. D.,
Rutledge, S. A., Gimmestad, C. & Depue, T. 2005: The
community collaborative rain, hail, and snow network:
informal education for scientists and citizens. Bull.
Amer. Meteor. Soc. 86, 1069—1077.
Cohn, J. P. 2008: Citizen science: can volunteers do real
research? Bioscience 58, 192—197.
Cooper, C. B., Dickinson, J., Phillips, T. & Bonney, R.
2007a: Citizen science as a tool for conservation in
residential ecosystems. Ecol. Soc. 12, 11.
Cooper, C. B., Hochachka, W. M. & Dhondt, A. A. 2007b:
Contrasting natural experiments confirm competition
between house finches and house sparrows. Ecology 88,
864—870.
Cooper, C. B., Voss, M. A. & Zivkovic, B. 2009: Extended
laying interval of ultimate eggs of the Eastern Bluebird.
Condor 11, 752—755.
Cooper, S., Khatib, F. & Baker, D. 2013: Increasing public
involvement in structural biology. Structure 21, 1482—
1484.
Davey, G. 2007: An analysis of country, socio-economic
and time factors on worldwide zoo attendance during a
40 year period. Int. Zoo Yearb. 41, 217—225.
Delaney, D. G., Sperling, C. D., Adams, C. S. & Leung, B.
2008: Marine invasive species: validation of citizen
science and implications for national monitoring
networks. Biol. Invasions 10, 117—128.
Deng, D., Chuang, T., Shao, K., Mai, G., Lin, T., Lemmens,
R., Hsu, C., Lin, H. & Kraak, M. 2012: Using social media
for collaborative species identification and occurrence:
Ethology 120 (2014) 207–214 © 2013 Blackwell Verlag
GmbH212
Public Engagement in Ethology J. Hecht & C. B. Cooper
issues, methods, and tools. SIGSPATIAL 2012 Interna-
tional Conference on Advances in Geographic Informa-
tion Systems, ACM, 22—29.
Dickinson, J. L., Zuckerberg, B. & Bonter, D. N. 2010:
Citizen science as an ecological research tool: challenges
and benefits. Annu. Rev. Ecol. Evol. Syst. 41, 149—172.
Eddy, T. J., Gallup, G. G. Jr & Povinelli, D. J. 1993:
Attribution of cognitive states to animals:
anthropomorphism in comparative perspective. J. Soc.
Issues 49, 87—101.
Fiorito, G. & Scotto, P. 1992: Observational learning in
Octopus vulgaris. Science 256, 545—547.
Foster, D., Kirman, B., Linehan, C., Lawson, S., Mills, D.,
Ellis, S. & Zulch, H. 2011: “I can haz emoshuns?”:
understanding anthropomorphosis of cats among
internet users. 3rd International Conference on Social
Computing, 9-11 October 2011, MIT Boston.
Hart, A., Stafford, R., Goodenough, A. & Morgan, S.
2012: The role of citizen science and volunteer data
collection in zoological research. Int. J. Zool., Article
ID 105345, doi:10.1155/2012/105345.
Head, J. S., Boesch, C., Robbins, M. M., Rabanal, L. I.,
Makaga, L. & K€uhl, H. S. 2013: Effective sociodemo-
graphic population assessment of elusive species in
ecology and conservation management. Ecol. Evol. 3,
2903—2916.
Heider, F. & Simmel, M. 1944: An experimental study of
apparent behavior. Am. J. Psychol. 57, 243—259.
Horowitz, A. 2007: Anthropomorphism. In. M.
Encyclopedia of Human-Animal Relationships (Bekoff,
M. ed). Greenwood Publishing Group, Westport, CT. pp.
60—66.
Horowitz, A. 2011: Empirically testing anthropomor-
phisms of the domestic dog. J. Vet. Behav. 6, 75.
Khatib, F., Cooper, S., Tyka, M. D., Xu, K., Makedon, I.,
Popovi�c, Z., Baker, D. & Foldit Players. 2011a: Algo-
rithm discovery by protein folding game players. Proc.
Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 108, 18949—18953.
Khatib, F., DiMaio, F., Cooper, S., Kazmierczyk, M., Gilski,
M., Krzywda, S., Z�abransk�a, H., Pichov�a, I., Thompson,
J., Popovi�c, Z., Jaskolski, M. & Baker, D. Foldit Contend-
ers Group, Foldit Void Crushers Group 2011b: Crystal
structure of a monomeric retroviral protease solved by
protein folding game players. Nat. Struct. Mol. Biol. 18,
1175—1177.
Kubinyi, E., Vas, J., Hejjas, K., Ronai, Z., Br�uder, I.,
Turcs�an, B., Sasvari-Szekely, M. & Mikl�osi, �A. 2012:
Polymorphism in the tyrosine hydroxylase (TH) gene is
associated with activity-impulsivity in German
Shepherd dogs. PLoS One 7, e30271.
Lehner, P. N. 1987: Design and execution of animal
behavior research: an overview. J. Anim. Sci. 65, 1213
—1219.
Lintott, C. J., Schawinski, K., Slosar, A., Land, K., Bam-
ford, S., Thomas, D., Raddick, M. J., Nichol, R. C.,
Szalay, A., Andreescu, D., Murray, P. & Vandenberg, J.
2008: Galaxy Zoo: morphologies derived from visual
inspection of galaxies from the Sloan Digital Sky Sur-
vey. Mon. Not. R. Astron. Soc. 389, 1179—1189.
Loos, A. & Ernst, A. 2013: An automated chimpanzee
identification system using face detection and recogni-
tion. EURASIP J. Image Video Process 49, 1—17.
Lord, K. 2013: A comparison of the sensory development
of wolves (Canis lupus lupus) and dogs (Canis lupus
familiaris). Ethology 119, 110—120.
Macdonald, C. & Whiten, A. 2011: The ‘Living Links to
Human Evolution’ Research Centre in Edinburgh Zoo:
a new endeavour in collaboration. Int. Zoo Yearb. 45,
7—17.
Martin, P. & Bateson, P. 2007: Measuring Behaviour: An
Introductory Guide. Cambridge University Press, Cam-
bridge.
McGreevy, P., Grassi, T. D. & Harman, A. M. 2003: A
strong correlation exists between the distribution of ret-
inal ganglion cells and nose length in the dog. Brain
Behav. Evol. 63, 13—22.
Morisette, J. T., Richardson, A. D., Knapp, A. K., Fisher, J.
I., Graham, E. A., Abatzoglou, J., Wilson, B. E., Bres-
hears, D. D., Henebry, G. M., Hanes, J. M. & Liang, L.
2009: Tracking the rhythm of the seasons in the face of
global change: phenological research in the 21st cen-
tury. Front. Ecol. Environ. 7, 253—260.
Moriyama, T. & Gunji, Y. 1997: Autonomous learning in
maze solution by Octopus. Ethology 103, 499—513.
Morris, P. H., Doe, C. & Godsell, E. 2008: Secondary
emotions in non-primate species? Behavioural reports
and subjective claims by animal owners. Cogn. Emot.
22, 3—20.
Nelson, X. J. & Fijn, N. 2013: The use of visual media as a
tool for investigating animal behaviour. Anim. Behav.
85, 525—536.
Newman, G., Graham, J., Crall, A. & Laituri, M. 2011: The
art and science of multi-scale citizen science support.
Ecol. Inform. 6, 217—227.
Parr, C. S., Jones, T. & Songer, N. B. 2004: Evaluation of a
handheld data collection interface for science learning.
J. Sci. Educ. Technol. 13, 233—242.
Reynolds, P. C. & Braithwaite, D. 2001: Towards a concep-
tual framework for wildlife tourism. Tour. Manage. 22,
31—42.
Serpell, J. 1996: In the Company of Animals: A Study of
Human-Animal Relationships. Cambridge University
Press, Cambridge.
Shirk, J. L., Ballard, H. L., Wilderman, C. C., Phillips, T.,
Wiggins, A., Jordan, R., McCallie, E., Minarchek, M.,
Lewenstein, B. V., Krasny, M. E. & Bonney, R. 2012:
Public participation in scientific research: a framework
for deliberate design. Ecol. Soc. 17, 29.
Silvertown, J. 2009: A new dawn for citizen science.
Trends Ecol. Evol. 24, 467—471.
Ethology 120 (2014) 207–214 © 2013 Blackwell Verlag GmbH
213
J. Hecht & C. B. Cooper Public Engagement in Ethology
Slabbekoorn, H. & den Boer-Visser, A. 2006: Cities change
the songs of birds. Curr. Biol. 16, 2326—2331.
Slabbekoorn, H. & Peet, M. 2003: Ecology: birds sing at a
higher pitch in urban noise. Nature 424, 267.
Sn€all, T., Kindvall, O., Nilsson, J. & P€art, T. 2011: Evaluat-
ing citizen-based presence data for bird monitoring.
Biol. Cons. 144, 804—810.
Stamps, J. & Groothuis, T. G. 2010: The development
of animal personality: relevance, concepts and per-
spectives. Biol. Rev. Camb. Philos. Soc. 85, 301—
325.
Tinbergen, N. 1963: On aims and methods of ethology.
Zeitschrift f€ur Tierpsychologie 20, 410—433.
Uetz, G. W., Hieber, C. S., Jakob, E. M., Wilcox, R. S.,
Kroeger, D., McCrate, A. & Mostrom, A. M. 1994:
Behavior of colonial orb-weaving spiders during a solar
eclipse. Ethology 96, 24—32.
USFWS. 2011: U.S. Fish & Wildlife Service 2011 National
Survey of Fishing, Hunting, and Wildlife-Associated
Recreation. http://www.doi.gov/news/pressreleases/
upload/FWS-National-Preliminary-Report-2011.pdf.
Voss, M. A. & Cooper, C. B. 2010: Using a free online
citizen-science project to teach observation & quantifi-
cation of animal behavior. Am. Biol. Teach. 72, 437—
443.
Vrontou, S., Wong, A. M., Rau, K. K., Koerber, H. R. &
Anderson, D. J. 2013: Genetic identification of C fibres
that detect massage-like stroking of hairy skin in vivo.
Nature 493, 669—673.
Wan, M., Hejjas, K., Ronai, Z., Elek, Z., Sasvari-Szekely,
M., Champagne, F. A., Mikl�osi, �A. & Kubinyi, E. 2013:
DRD4 and TH gene polymorphisms are associated with
activity, impulsivity and inattention in Siberian Husky
dogs. Anim. Genet. 44, 717—727.
Wiggins, A., Newman, G., Stevenson, R. D. & Crowston,
K. 2011: Mechanisms for data quality and validation in
citizen science. eScience Workshops, 14—19.
Williams, R. L., Porter, S. K., Hart, A. G. & Goodenough,
A. E. 2012: The accuracy of behavioural data collected
by visitors in a zoo environment: can visitors collect
meaningful data? Int. J. Zool., Article ID 724835,
doi:10.1155/2012/724835.
Ethology 120 (2014) 207–214 © 2013 Blackwell Verlag
GmbH214
Public Engagement in Ethology J. Hecht & C. B. Cooper
CURRENT ISSUES - PERSPECTIVES AND REVIEWS
Tribute to Tinbergen: Public Engagement in Ethology
Julie Hecht* & Caren B. Cooper†
* Doctoral Program in Psychology, The Graduate Center, City
University of New York, New York, NY, USA
† Cornell Lab of Ornithology, Ithaca, NY, USA
(Invited Review)
Correspondence
Julie Hecht, Department of Psychology,
Hunter College and The Graduate Center,
City University of New York, 695 Park
Avenue, New York, NY 10065, USA.
E-mail: [email protected]
Received: October 4, 2013
Initial acceptance: October 19, 2013
Final acceptance: November 25, 2013
(M. Hauber)
doi: 10.1111/eth.12199
Keywords: citizen science, data quality,
animal behavior, informal science education
Abstract
Public engagement in research, called citizen science, has led to
advances
in a range of fields like astronomy, ornithology, and public
health. While
volunteers have been making and sharing observations
according to pro-
tocols set by researchers in numerous disciplines, citizen
science practices
are less common in the field of animal behavior. We consider
how citizen
science might be used to address animal behavior questions at
Tinbergen’s
four levels of analysis. We briefly review resources and
methods for
addressing technical issues surrounding volunteer
participation—such as
data quality—so that citizen science can make long-standing
contributions
to the field of animal behavior.
Introduction
‘Citizen science’ describes the various ways that mem-
bers of the public participate in genuine scientific
research (Cooper et al. 2007a; Silvertown 2009; Shirk
et al. 2012). Many citizen science projects arise from
communities with specific concerns (such as long-
term environmental monitoring), and some are
initiated by scientists to address specific research
objectives. Other projects have additional goals to
increase science literacy (Bonney et al. 2009a).
Research via public engagement is possible because
people enjoy making natural history observations and
sharing these observations with professionals and
peers. In addition, Web 2.0 and mobile technologies
facilitate mass participation. Consequently, citizen
science projects have arisen in a myriad of disciplines,
including ecology, phenology, macroecology, public
health, natural resource management, hydrology,
urban planning, meteorology, math, volcanology, and
various taxon-specific fields such as entomology,
ornithology, and herpetology. In recent years, citizen
science has enabled substantial contributions to the
fields of astronomy (Lintott et al. 2008), medicine
(Khatib et al. 2011a,b), and climate change (Morisette
et al. 2009).
Currently, the field of animal behavior is under-
represented among citizen science projects. Animal
behavior research integrates diverse methodological
approaches to investigate a wide array of scientific
questions about the behavior of wild and domesti-
cated animals in natural and captive settings. Public
observations of occurrence data (e.g., wildlife sight-
ings; Sn€all et al. 2011), count data (e.g., number of
birds; Cooper et al. 2007b), simple measurements
(e.g., rainfall; Cifelli et al. 2005), and even phenologi-
cal stages (e.g., budburst; Morisette et al. 2009) are
more common applications of citizen science, in part
because such data likely require simpler protocols
than most animal behavior data.
The dearth of citizen science research in animal
behavior is not necessarily due to lack of general pub-
lic interest. Pet-keeping is a global phenomenon
(Serpell 1996); wildlife tourism and wildlife docu-
mentaries are popular (Reynolds & Braithwaite 2001;
Nelson & Fijn 2013); zoo attendance is on the rise in
parts of the world (Davey 2007); and people are
already expressing interest in animal behavior citizen
Ethology 120 (2014) 207–214 © 2013 Blackwell Verlag GmbH
207
Ethology
ethology
international journal of behavioural biology
science projects (Foster et al. 2011; Williams et al.
2012). In the USA, one in four adults watches wildlife
(USFWS 2011), and the numerous websites that
stream live video of pets (e.g., SPCA cams) and wild-
life (e.g., nesting birds) are extremely popular (e.g.,
the Cornell Lab of Ornithology nest cams receive
millions of unique viewers from 175 countries,
including up to 9,000 simultaneous views of live-
streaming video).
Citizen science contributions could enhance animal
behavior investigations by increasing observations on
time and geographic scales (e.g., latitudinal gradients,
urbanization gradients, comparing populations, etc),
and these projects also have the potential to process
massive amounts of data (e.g., video or photo tag-
ging). In essence, citizen scientists provide an extra
pair of hands—or extra thousands of pairs. While a
small team of researchers documented the behavior of
the colonial orb-weaving spider during a solar eclipse
in Veracruz, Mexico (Uetz et al. 1994), the team
might have used citizen scientists to capture behav-
ioral data on additional populations or species during
the short-lived event. Without the help of citizen sci-
entists, researchers are effectively ‘missing’ variation
among animals.
Field research, behavior observation, and behavior
coding can be time-consuming, challenging to coordi-
nate, and expensive. Trained volunteers might offer
lower-cost options for collecting and/or scoring
behavior (Williams et al. 2012). Depending on the
study design and complexity, volunteers can be used
to ensure blind coding.
Citizen science methods could facilitate access to a
wide variety of species in different contexts and foster
integration of studies of natural populations and pop-
ulations under artificial selection. Additionally, an
expanded research platform offers the possibility of
capturing animals in novel contexts. Video and social
media, particularly YouTube, are rife with examples
of uncommon (Burn 2011) or previously unexplored
behaviors, such as interspecies play or object play
(Nelson & Fijn 2013). Behaviors captured in real-
world contexts can lead to new research directions,
and citizen scientists could provide behavioral video
footage.
We examine the realized and potential application
of citizen science to research animal behavior—the
inquiry about what animals do, how they do it, and
why they do it. Tinbergen (1963) framed the disci-
pline as encompassing four levels of analysis: causa-
tion, development, function, and phylogeny.
We use Tinbergen’s levels of analysis as a frame-
work to discuss a possible partnership between citizen
science and animal behavior research. We note that
citizen scientists could examine a particular behavior,
for example bird song, to generate data for answering
questions at all four levels (e.g., causation: ‘What day
length triggers bird song?’; development: ‘Do offspring
imitate songs of parents?’; function: ‘Do females
prefer certain song types?’; and phylogeny: ‘Which
species have similar songs?’). Professional scientists
can obtain a more robust understanding of an
observed behavior through investigations of non-
competing hypotheses at multiple levels. Other
questions might be best suited for investigation at one
particular level of analysis. We discuss a variety of
applications below, with the common theme that citi-
zen science participants can scale-up and advance
research that individual scientists could not do alone.
Additionally, we review the technical facets of a
union between animal behavior and citizen science,
giving attention to crucial details in training volun-
teers and handling volunteer data.
Citizen Science at Four Levels of Analysis
In the 1963 paper, On aims and methods of Ethology,
Tinbergen proposed an integrated approach to the
study of ethology to address the lack of a unified
public image of ethology and to resolve conceptual
differences between researchers. Since then, “how
questions,” exploring the immediate causes of behav-
ior, and “why questions,” exploring the evolutionary
forces behind behavior, have been pursued as
complementary approaches.
Causation
The causal basis of behavior focuses on stimuli and
mechanisms that trigger, cue, or precede a behavior.
For early ethologists, physiological and neural mecha-
nisms occurred in a ‘black box’ upon which they
could only speculate. Today, inquiry into causation
draws from genetics, endocrinology, physiology, and
neurobiology. Direct participation and input from citizen
scientists is already underway in neurobiology (Eyewire,
https://eyewire.org/signup) and genetics (Phylo) (Coo-
per et al. 2013). For domesticated and wild populations,
the collection of saliva and fecal samples could contrib-
ute to studies of stress physiology and measurements of
well-being to ultimately address a wide variety of behav-
ioral endocrinological questions.
Participant involvement could complement labo-
ratory findings by collecting behavioral data on
pre-screened or pre-selected populations. For exam-
ple, volunteers might assist research into relationships
Ethology 120 (2014) 207–214 © 2013 Blackwell Verlag
GmbH208
Public Engagement in Ethology J. Hecht & C. B. Cooper
between genetic markers and behavior by collecting
behavioral data on pre-selected animals (e.g., through
a web portal for annotating video) whose genes have
been mapped. Researchers recently determined that
particular allelic qualities of DRD4 and TH gene poly-
morphisms were associated with activity–impulsivity
in German Shepherds and Siberian Huskies, with
behavioral analysis stemming from approx. 250 dogs
(Kubinyi et al. 2012; Wan et al. 2013). Continued
exploration of gene variants in dogs could be coupled
with citizen science projects collecting behavioral data
on dogs with known genetic composition to generate
a more comprehensive understanding of behavioral
traits. Citizen science allows researchers to expand the
size of their data set without requiring the participants
to have any knowledge of the DNA sampling and gen-
otyping that go into pre-screening decisions.
The addition of a citizen science component can
increase the rigor of the research in at least two ways.
First, the volunteer contributions could ensure a dou-
ble-blind experiment. Second, the volunteers provide
observations from multiple observers, and researchers
can use a consensus tool to score behavior, analogous
to consensus tools (replication by multiple partici-
pants) common to online crowdsourcing efforts for
other research (Wiggins et al. 2011). Additionally,
volunteers can install cameras to record animal
behavior and facilitate remote coding by professional
researchers. For example, scientists used footage from
volunteer nest cameras to examine proximate control
of egg-laying behaviors in nestbox-dwelling bird
species (Cooper et al. 2009).
Development
Approaches to animal behavior from a developmental
perspective are also mechanistic, but with a focus on
changes in an organism’s capacity for expressing or
acquiring behaviors at different stages over the course
of a lifetime.
Citizen science offers a unique opportunity to inves-
tigate ontogenetic questions in a wide range of
species. For example, citizen scientists might assist in
the study of social behavior and document the devel-
opment of particular behavioral patterns—such as fac-
tors affecting the onset and development of play in
various species—or personality changes (Stamps &
Groothuis 2010). Volunteers could collect and report
data on single organisms—whether subjects be a com-
panion animal, a backyard nestbox or an annually
returning species—and generate substantial data
points for each individual. Ultimately, with many
volunteers, the increased sample sizes could make
full-scale statistical analysis possible (Dickinson et al.
2010).
Citizen scientists could also examine the develop-
ment of animal sensorium, such as in relation to mor-
phological traits. For example, assumptions about the
different sensory abilities in dog breeds abound, and
such differences might be expected given physiologi-
cal differences (McGreevy et al. 2003), but actual
behavioral and development data are wanting.
Researchers could design and implement a project
where participants conduct simple, in-home tests
examining behavior responses toward different stim-
uli at different ages, thereby offering additional data
on breed (and even species) sensory development.
These behavioral measures of development are com-
parable to traditional laboratory experiments (Lord
2013). But, with the help of citizen scientists,
researchers can obtain data on a much larger scale.
Zoo visitors and wildlife tourists can play a role in
monitoring individuals’ development. Zoo visitors
express interest in animal behavior studies (Bowler
et al. 2012) and could collect many more hours (or
years) of observations than possible by resource-
limited staff with many other care-taking duties. Such
efforts might elucidate the onset or development of
behaviors such as stereotypic motor patterns as well as
factors affecting the development of these behaviors.
Zoo visitors, however, can have bias toward collecting
observations of active animals (Altman 1998; Williams
et al. 2012).
Function
The analysis of function focuses on the pressures that
shape behaviors. The pervasive influence of the built
environment means that many organisms reside in
environmental conditions that differ in novel ways
from the conditions under which traits evolved. For
example, the anthropogenic sound levels near high-
ways and flight paths create novel selection pressures,
and some animals might be able to adjust their behav-
iors within a range of plasticity, such as birds singing
at higher pitches near roads (Slabbekoorn & Peet
2003; Slabbekoorn & den Boer-Visser 2006). Other
traits might exhibit limited phenotypic plasticity, and
disturbances may result in population decline. Fur-
thermore, both domesticated and non-domesticated
animals residing in homes or zoos will demonstrate
behaviors that might no longer be adaptive. Research-
ers can design participatory projects in which volun-
teers collect data on species in different environments
and investigate different hypotheses behind complex
behaviors. In an online project called CamClickr, par-
Ethology 120 (2014) 207–214 © 2013 Blackwell Verlag GmbH
209
J. Hecht & C. B. Cooper Public Engagement in Ethology
ticipants added behavior-category tags to images
archived from nest cameras to help investigate varia-
tions in nest attendance (Voss & Cooper 2010).
Because funding cycles offered by granting institu-
tions tend to span three- to five-year periods, collect-
ing long-term data sets can be challenging for
research scientists. The collection of such data sets
could be made possible through citizen science activi-
ties. Depending on the initiative, initial investment to
set up the project, and maintenance costs, could be
low with free online tools for managing projects (e.g.,
www.citsci.org), recruiting volunteers (e.g., www.
scistarter.com), and data archiving (e.g., www.data-
one.org). Citizen scientists could measure different
components of fitness such as survival to maturity,
longevity, mating success, and reproductive success.
Phylogeny
Behaviors can also be understood from a phylogenetic
perspective. By considering ancestry, researchers can
identify traits that are highly conserved and those that
are more recently evolved within the same lineage.
Because closely related species are found in everyday
environments, a citizen science project could collect a
wide variety of behavioral data on closely related spe-
cies in different environments that have adopted dif-
ferent survival strategies. By comparing closely
related species occurring on different continents (e.g.,
black-billed bagpie Pica hudsonia in America and the
European magpie Pica pica in Eurasia), citizen scien-
tists can provide simultaneous data sets without the
confound of temporal displacement typical of smaller-
scale research projects.
Not all citizen engagement in animal behavior pro-
jects fits neatly into Tinbergen’s levels of analysis.
Recently, Project: Play With Your Dog (Horowitz Dog
Cognition Lab, Barnard College) requested short vid-
eos depicting play between a dog and a person, with
the aim of capturing the wide array of behaviors and
conditions that encompass interspecific play. Video
submissions drew from around the world, and
although participants did not contribute directly to
behavioral analysis, citizen scientists had to think
about behavior in terms of how they and their dog
play together and provide behavioral descriptors in an
accompanying questionnaire.
Similarly, an online survey by Animal Welfare Indi-
cators asked participants to assist in developing a con-
tinuous behavioral scale of lameness for goats.
Participants watched nine videos and scored abnormal
gait, head-nodding, and spine curvature on a visual
analogue scale. Before participants began, each
behavior had a clear description, and participants
were schooled in measuring behavior as per the speci-
fications of the project.
Merging Ethology and Citizen Science Methods
Creating Successful Citizen Science Animal Behavior
Projects
Like all successful research endeavors, the design of
citizen science projects follows the steps of the scien-
tific method; the only difference is the additional layer
of complexity introduced by the involvement of the
lay public. Projects start with the formulation of scien-
tific questions, organization of a team of skilled
experts in relevant areas, and determination of appro-
priate data collection (Bonney et al. 2009b). Depend-
ing on the legal status and various protections offered
to different species, researchers might need to present
protocols to Institutional Animal Care and Use
Committees (Bayne 1998). Citizen scientists can learn
behavior-coding techniques—or relevant project-
related skills—in online videos or in person. The
research team can control quality before the project
begins by testing each prospective participant’s
accuracy and ability to collect meaningful data.
Behavioral data accuracy is a key concern, and pro-
jects incorporating citizen science data should con-
sider topics like observation quality, sampling bias,
and interobserver reliability (Dickinson et al. 2010;
Burghardt et al. 2012; Hart et al. 2012). Williams
et al. (2012) found that although zoo visitor interest
in the project was high, the activity budget data col-
lected on a group of captive otters were not accurate,
mostly because visitors did not adhere to the observa-
tion period; the researchers suggest that certain
sampling methods might be easier—or harder—for
observers.
Meanwhile, there are numerous ways to improve
data quality in citizen science (Cohn 2008; Silvertown
2009; Wiggins et al. 2011; Bonter & Cooper 2012).
Technologies like smartphones, multimedia, or inter-
active video could assist in time-keeping and behav-
ioral data collection (Parr et al. 2004; Aanensen et al.
2009). Data management tools, often through cyber-
infrastructure, improve efficiency (Newman et al.
2011). Additionally, some citizen science projects
demonstrate that certain types of people collect more
accurate data than others (Delaney et al. 2008), and
similar lessons can make their way into citizen science
projects in animal behavior.
Finally, researchers can be creative in where and
how volunteers access animals and how behavior will
Ethology 120 (2014) 207–214 © 2013 Blackwell Verlag
GmbH210
Public Engagement in Ethology J. Hecht & C. B. Cooper
be observed or documented. In some cases, behaviors
are not observed directly but are inferred. Phenology
research addresses the timing of life-cycle events,
including behaviors such as egg laying and migration
in birds, insects, and some reptiles and amphibians
(Head et al. 2013). Limited access to animals in situa-
tions where volunteers can obtain unbiased observa-
tions of life-cycle events may have been one of the
largest impediments to citizen science in animal
behavior. Now, Internet and remote-camera techno-
logies combined with crowdsourcing infrastructure
offer new opportunities to collect data on animals that
are difficult to access (Deng et al. 2012; Head et al.
2013; Loos & Ernst 2013). Live-streaming cameras
can be set up at areas with known activity—animal
nesting or gathering sites—so that people can docu-
ment behaviors at a distance. In addition to remote/
web-mediated studies, possibilities abound for studies
of companion animals (e.g., dogs, cats, rabbits, birds,
ferrets, fish, gerbils, and hamsters), zoo animals, ani-
mals viewed during nature tourism, and urban wild-
life such as squirrels, pigeons, rats, foxes, and coyotes.
Volunteers could document the behavior of animals
at zoos or wildlife tourism destinations, especially ani-
mals that handlers and keepers do not have time to
record. Special interest groups—such as the House
Rabbit Society, backyard chicken groups, and pet
breeders—offer significant potential for citizen science
projects.
Training and Resources for Animal Behavior Inquiry
Skill in observing and measuring animal behavior
does not necessarily follow from an appreciation of or
even cohabitation with animals. Members of the pub-
lic are generally interested in animals yet have a pro-
clivity not toward Tinbergen’s four levels of behavior,
but often toward anthropomorphic explanations of
behavior.
While the study of animal behavior is an attempt to
investigate ultimate and proximate questions about
behavior, humans often characterize animal behavior
through an anthropocentric lens (Foster et al. 2011).
People readily ascribe human characteristics to non-
humans. Subjective assessments of non-human
behavior can be consistent across observers, often
reifying existing beliefs regardless of whether they
have been substantiated (Bahlig-Pieren & Turner
1999; Morris et al. 2008; Horowitz 2011). Seminal
work by Heider & Simmel (1944) found that people
consistently attribute human emotions, aims, and
objectives to inanimate objects moving in a ‘human-
like’ manner. Companion animal species, mammals,
and non-human primates are more readily anthropo-
morphized than other species (Eddy et al. 1993), even
though seemingly ‘alien’ species, like cephalopods,
display complex behaviors and cognitive processes
(Fiorito & Scotto 1992; Moriyama & Gunji 1997).
Importantly, the appearance of behavioral homologies
should not imply similar meaning, such as the ‘smil-
ing’ chimpanzee or dolphin (Horowitz 2007).
Tendencies toward anthropomorphism are perhaps
a useful ‘hook’ when engaging the general public
in animal behavior projects. Recently, an online
video titled ‘Lol cats like stroking too’ (http://www.
youtube.com/watch?v=OFCRvjle2o8) accompanied a
publication on the neurons activated by massage-like
stroking to assist in public understanding of the
research (Vrontou et al. 2013). The video received
over 200,000 views in the first year.
Clearly defined project expectations and training
materials can minimize the effects of anthropomor-
phic inferences (Voss & Cooper 2010). Scientists can
provide a detailed ethogram, an explicit coding
scheme, and a clear behavior-sampling technique
(Martin & Bateson 2007). Behaviors in an ethogram
can be distinguished based on structural or empirical
characteristics, such as ‘raised lips at edges’, or conse-
quence-based descriptors, such as ‘threat’. Empirical
descriptors help create unambiguous behavioral
parameters (Lehner 1987) but might offer challenges
for unpracticed observers. Volunteers need a clear
understanding of the terms in which they are
expected to view and document behavior and have
the technology, such as a timer, to code appropriately.
Researchers determine who is being watched and
when and how to watch them. Some coding schemes
provide information on how much time an animal
spends performing various behaviors, while other
schemes monitor behavior frequencies (Altmann
1974). Researchers must match the skill level of
observers with the scientific question. The role of vol-
unteers in the study of animal behavior will largely
involve the production of meaningful behavioral data.
The challenge will be to offer effective guidance and
instruction to avoid anthropocentric assessments.
A number of institutions have created behavior-
observation instructional platforms. Ethosearch (www.
ethosearch.org), created by the Lincoln Park Zoo, is a
resource for researchers and students to store and view
ethograms and practice measuring animal behavior.
Living Links (www.living-links.org)—a joint enclosure
for squirrel monkeys (Saimiri sciureus) and capuchins
(Sapajus apella) at the Edinburgh Zoo—offers a
public-learning resource to observe and score the behav-
ior of both species (Macdonald & Whiten 2011). Cam-
Ethology 120 (2014) 207–214 © 2013 Blackwell Verlag GmbH
211
J. Hecht & C. B. Cooper Public Engagement in Ethology
Clickr was an online citizen science project to code the
behaviors of nesting birds from still images as a way of
carrying out scan sampling. Voss & Cooper (2010) cre-
ated a college-level laboratory unit in which students
can use CamClickr to create ethograms while learning
about observer bias, observation techniques, and defin-
ing and quantifying observations.
Conclusion
Animal behavior researchers might consider whether
and how citizen science could support and amplify
their studies. Volunteers can expand the scope and
scale of animal behavior studies by bringing attention
to novel or rare behaviors or providing access to
additional variation in animal behaviors. Volunteers,
too, can benefit from personal experiences engaging
in hands-on scientific interactions with animals.
Undoubtedly, challenges arise with the involve-
ment of citizen science in animal behavior, yet the
possibilities are great. Altmann’s (1974, p. 229) text
on behavior sampling methods stresses that there
should not be a disconnect between the laboratory
and the real world: ‘Such a restriction on research
would mean that the behavioral sciences would for-
ever forsake any hope of knowing whether their most
powerful theories have any relevance in the world of
behavior outside the laboratory.’ Citizen scientists
offer an additional, and untapped, opportunity for
ethological exploration.
Acknowledgements
Thanks to Felicity Muth, Mark Hauber, and Andrew
Fulmer for conceptual conversations. Many thanks to
reviewers for comments, which improved the manu-
script.
Literature cited
Aanensen, D. M., Huntley, D. M., Feil, E. J., al-Own, F. &
Spratt, B. G. 2009: EpiCollect: linking smartphones to
web applications for epidemiology, ecology and
community data collection. PLoS One 4, e6968.
Altman, J. D. 1998: Animal activity and visitor learning at
the zoo. Anthrozoos 11, 12—21.
Altmann, J. 1974: Observational study of behavior:
sampling methods. Behaviour 49, 227—267.
Bahlig-Pieren, Z. & Turner, D. C. 1999: Anthropomorphic
interpretations and ethological descriptions of dog and
cat behavior by lay people. Anthrozoos 12, 205—210.
Bayne, K. 1998: Developing guidelines on the care and use
of animals. Ann. N.Y. Acad. Sci. 862, 105—110.
Bonney, R., Ballard, H., Jordan, R., McCallie, E., Phillips,
T., Shirk, J. & Wilderman, C. C. 2009a: Public participa-
tion in scientific research: defining the field and assess-
ing its potential for informal science education. A CAISE
Inquiry Group Report. Center for Advancement of
Informal Science Education (CAISE), Washington, D.C.
Bonney, R., Cooper, C. B., Dickinson, J., Kelling, S.,
Phillips, T., Rosenberg, K. V. & Shirk, J. 2009b: Citizen
science: a developing tool for expanding science knowl-
edge and scientific literacy. Bioscience 59, 977—984.
Bonter, D. N. & Cooper, C. B. 2012: Data validation in
citizen science: a case study from Project FeederWatch.
Front. Ecol. Environ. 10, 305—307.
Bowler, M. T., Buchanan-Smith, H. M. & Whiten, A.
2012: Assessing public engagement with science in a
university primate research centre in a national zoo.
PLoS One 7, e34505.
Burghardt, G. M., Bartmess-LeVasseur, J. N., Browning, S.
A., Morrison, K. E., Stec, C. L., Zachau, C. E. & Free-
berg, T. M. 2012: Perspectives—Minimizing observer
bias in behavioral studies: a review and recommenda-
tions. Ethology 118, 511—517.
Burn, C. C. 2011: A vicious cycle: A cross-sectional study
of canine tail-chasing and human responses to it, using
a free video-sharing website. PLoS One 6, e26553.
Cifelli, R., Doesken, N., Kennedy, P., Carey, L. D.,
Rutledge, S. A., Gimmestad, C. & Depue, T. 2005: The
community collaborative rain, hail, and snow network:
informal education for scientists and citizens. Bull.
Amer. Meteor. Soc. 86, 1069—1077.
Cohn, J. P. 2008: Citizen science: can volunteers do real
research? Bioscience 58, 192—197.
Cooper, C. B., Dickinson, J., Phillips, T. & Bonney, R.
2007a: Citizen science as a tool for conservation in
residential ecosystems. Ecol. Soc. 12, 11.
Cooper, C. B., Hochachka, W. M. & Dhondt, A. A. 2007b:
Contrasting natural experiments confirm competition
between house finches and house sparrows. Ecology 88,
864—870.
Cooper, C. B., Voss, M. A. & Zivkovic, B. 2009: Extended
laying interval of ultimate eggs of the Eastern Bluebird.
Condor 11, 752—755.
Cooper, S., Khatib, F. & Baker, D. 2013: Increasing public
involvement in structural biology. Structure 21, 1482—
1484.
Davey, G. 2007: An analysis of country, socio-economic
and time factors on worldwide zoo attendance during a
40 year period. Int. Zoo Yearb. 41, 217—225.
Delaney, D. G., Sperling, C. D., Adams, C. S. & Leung, B.
2008: Marine invasive species: validation of citizen
science and implications for national monitoring
networks. Biol. Invasions 10, 117—128.
Deng, D., Chuang, T., Shao, K., Mai, G., Lin, T., Lemmens,
R., Hsu, C., Lin, H. & Kraak, M. 2012: Using social media
for collaborative species identification and occurrence:
Ethology 120 (2014) 207–214 © 2013 Blackwell Verlag
GmbH212
Public Engagement in Ethology J. Hecht & C. B. Cooper
issues, methods, and tools. SIGSPATIAL 2012 Interna-
tional Conference on Advances in Geographic Informa-
tion Systems, ACM, 22—29.
Dickinson, J. L., Zuckerberg, B. & Bonter, D. N. 2010:
Citizen science as an ecological research tool: challenges
and benefits. Annu. Rev. Ecol. Evol. Syst. 41, 149—172.
Eddy, T. J., Gallup, G. G. Jr & Povinelli, D. J. 1993:
Attribution of cognitive states to animals:
anthropomorphism in comparative perspective. J. Soc.
Issues 49, 87—101.
Fiorito, G. & Scotto, P. 1992: Observational learning in
Octopus vulgaris. Science 256, 545—547.
Foster, D., Kirman, B., Linehan, C., Lawson, S., Mills, D.,
Ellis, S. & Zulch, H. 2011: “I can haz emoshuns?”:
understanding anthropomorphosis of cats among
internet users. 3rd International Conference on Social
Computing, 9-11 October 2011, MIT Boston.
Hart, A., Stafford, R., Goodenough, A. & Morgan, S.
2012: The role of citizen science and volunteer data
collection in zoological research. Int. J. Zool., Article
ID 105345, doi:10.1155/2012/105345.
Head, J. S., Boesch, C., Robbins, M. M., Rabanal, L. I.,
Makaga, L. & K€uhl, H. S. 2013: Effective sociodemo-
graphic population assessment of elusive species in
ecology and conservation management. Ecol. Evol. 3,
2903—2916.
Heider, F. & Simmel, M. 1944: An experimental study of
apparent behavior. Am. J. Psychol. 57, 243—259.
Horowitz, A. 2007: Anthropomorphism. In. M.
Encyclopedia of Human-Animal Relationships (Bekoff,
M. ed). Greenwood Publishing Group, Westport, CT. pp.
60—66.
Horowitz, A. 2011: Empirically testing anthropomor-
phisms of the domestic dog. J. Vet. Behav. 6, 75.
Khatib, F., Cooper, S., Tyka, M. D., Xu, K., Makedon, I.,
Popovi�c, Z., Baker, D. & Foldit Players. 2011a: Algo-
rithm discovery by protein folding game players. Proc.
Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 108, 18949—18953.
Khatib, F., DiMaio, F., Cooper, S., Kazmierczyk, M., Gilski,
M., Krzywda, S., Z�abransk�a, H., Pichov�a, I., Thompson,
J., Popovi�c, Z., Jaskolski, M. & Baker, D. Foldit Contend-
ers Group, Foldit Void Crushers Group 2011b: Crystal
structure of a monomeric retroviral protease solved by
protein folding game players. Nat. Struct. Mol. Biol. 18,
1175—1177.
Kubinyi, E., Vas, J., Hejjas, K., Ronai, Z., Br�uder, I.,
Turcs�an, B., Sasvari-Szekely, M. & Mikl�osi, �A. 2012:
Polymorphism in the tyrosine hydroxylase (TH) gene is
associated with activity-impulsivity in German
Shepherd dogs. PLoS One 7, e30271.
Lehner, P. N. 1987: Design and execution of animal
behavior research: an overview. J. Anim. Sci. 65, 1213
—1219.
Lintott, C. J., Schawinski, K., Slosar, A., Land, K., Bam-
ford, S., Thomas, D., Raddick, M. J., Nichol, R. C.,
Szalay, A., Andreescu, D., Murray, P. & Vandenberg, J.
2008: Galaxy Zoo: morphologies derived from visual
inspection of galaxies from the Sloan Digital Sky Sur-
vey. Mon. Not. R. Astron. Soc. 389, 1179—1189.
Loos, A. & Ernst, A. 2013: An automated chimpanzee
identification system using face detection and recogni-
tion. EURASIP J. Image Video Process 49, 1—17.
Lord, K. 2013: A comparison of the sensory development
of wolves (Canis lupus lupus) and dogs (Canis lupus
familiaris). Ethology 119, 110—120.
Macdonald, C. & Whiten, A. 2011: The ‘Living Links to
Human Evolution’ Research Centre in Edinburgh Zoo:
a new endeavour in collaboration. Int. Zoo Yearb. 45,
7—17.
Martin, P. & Bateson, P. 2007: Measuring Behaviour: An
Introductory Guide. Cambridge University Press, Cam-
bridge.
McGreevy, P., Grassi, T. D. & Harman, A. M. 2003: A
strong correlation exists between the distribution of ret-
inal ganglion cells and nose length in the dog. Brain
Behav. Evol. 63, 13—22.
Morisette, J. T., Richardson, A. D., Knapp, A. K., Fisher, J.
I., Graham, E. A., Abatzoglou, J., Wilson, B. E., Bres-
hears, D. D., Henebry, G. M., Hanes, J. M. & Liang, L.
2009: Tracking the rhythm of the seasons in the face of
global change: phenological research in the 21st cen-
tury. Front. Ecol. Environ. 7, 253—260.
Moriyama, T. & Gunji, Y. 1997: Autonomous learning in
maze solution by Octopus. Ethology 103, 499—513.
Morris, P. H., Doe, C. & Godsell, E. 2008: Secondary
emotions in non-primate species? Behavioural reports
and subjective claims by animal owners. Cogn. Emot.
22, 3—20.
Nelson, X. J. & Fijn, N. 2013: The use of visual media as a
tool for investigating animal behaviour. Anim. Behav.
85, 525—536.
Newman, G., Graham, J., Crall, A. & Laituri, M. 2011: The
art and science of multi-scale citizen science support.
Ecol. Inform. 6, 217—227.
Parr, C. S., Jones, T. & Songer, N. B. 2004: Evaluation of a
handheld data collection interface for science learning.
J. Sci. Educ. Technol. 13, 233—242.
Reynolds, P. C. & Braithwaite, D. 2001: Towards a concep-
tual framework for wildlife tourism. Tour. Manage. 22,
31—42.
Serpell, J. 1996: In the Company of Animals: A Study of
Human-Animal Relationships. Cambridge University
Press, Cambridge.
Shirk, J. L., Ballard, H. L., Wilderman, C. C., Phillips, T.,
Wiggins, A., Jordan, R., McCallie, E., Minarchek, M.,
Lewenstein, B. V., Krasny, M. E. & Bonney, R. 2012:
Public participation in scientific research: a framework
for deliberate design. Ecol. Soc. 17, 29.
Silvertown, J. 2009: A new dawn for citizen science.
Trends Ecol. Evol. 24, 467—471.
Ethology 120 (2014) 207–214 © 2013 Blackwell Verlag GmbH
213
J. Hecht & C. B. Cooper Public Engagement in Ethology
Slabbekoorn, H. & den Boer-Visser, A. 2006: Cities change
the songs of birds. Curr. Biol. 16, 2326—2331.
Slabbekoorn, H. & Peet, M. 2003: Ecology: birds sing at a
higher pitch in urban noise. Nature 424, 267.
Sn€all, T., Kindvall, O., Nilsson, J. & P€art, T. 2011: Evaluat-
ing citizen-based presence data for bird monitoring.
Biol. Cons. 144, 804—810.
Stamps, J. & Groothuis, T. G. 2010: The development
of animal personality: relevance, concepts and per-
spectives. Biol. Rev. Camb. Philos. Soc. 85, 301—
325.
Tinbergen, N. 1963: On aims and methods of ethology.
Zeitschrift f€ur Tierpsychologie 20, 410—433.
Uetz, G. W., Hieber, C. S., Jakob, E. M., Wilcox, R. S.,
Kroeger, D., McCrate, A. & Mostrom, A. M. 1994:
Behavior of colonial orb-weaving spiders during a solar
eclipse. Ethology 96, 24—32.
USFWS. 2011: U.S. Fish & Wildlife Service 2011 National
Survey of Fishing, Hunting, and Wildlife-Associated
Recreation. http://www.doi.gov/news/pressreleases/
upload/FWS-National-Preliminary-Report-2011.pdf.
Voss, M. A. & Cooper, C. B. 2010: Using a free online
citizen-science project to teach observation & quantifi-
cation of animal behavior. Am. Biol. Teach. 72, 437—
443.
Vrontou, S., Wong, A. M., Rau, K. K., Koerber, H. R. &
Anderson, D. J. 2013: Genetic identification of C fibres
that detect massage-like stroking of hairy skin in vivo.
Nature 493, 669—673.
Wan, M., Hejjas, K., Ronai, Z., Elek, Z., Sasvari-Szekely,
M., Champagne, F. A., Mikl�osi, �A. & Kubinyi, E. 2013:
DRD4 and TH gene polymorphisms are associated with
activity, impulsivity and inattention in Siberian Husky
dogs. Anim. Genet. 44, 717—727.
Wiggins, A., Newman, G., Stevenson, R. D. & Crowston,
K. 2011: Mechanisms for data quality and validation in
citizen science. eScience Workshops, 14—19.
Williams, R. L., Porter, S. K., Hart, A. G. & Goodenough,
A. E. 2012: The accuracy of behavioural data collected
by visitors in a zoo environment: can visitors collect
meaningful data? Int. J. Zool., Article ID 724835,
doi:10.1155/2012/724835.
Ethology 120 (2014) 207–214 © 2013 Blackwell Verlag
GmbH214
Public Engagement in Ethology J. Hecht & C. B. Cooper
CURRENT ISSUES - PERSPECTIVES AND REVIEWSTribute to Tinber.docx

More Related Content

Similar to CURRENT ISSUES - PERSPECTIVES AND REVIEWSTribute to Tinber.docx

Latent life: Concepts and practices of human tissue preservation in the In...
Latent life: Concepts  and practices of human  tissue preservation in the  In...Latent life: Concepts  and practices of human  tissue preservation in the  In...
Latent life: Concepts and practices of human tissue preservation in the In...JssicaBley
 
An Inventory Of Peer-Reviewed Articles On Killer Whales (Orcinus Orca) With A...
An Inventory Of Peer-Reviewed Articles On Killer Whales (Orcinus Orca) With A...An Inventory Of Peer-Reviewed Articles On Killer Whales (Orcinus Orca) With A...
An Inventory Of Peer-Reviewed Articles On Killer Whales (Orcinus Orca) With A...Steven Wallach
 
Mesa Redonda Ciencia Ciudadana Campus Gutenberg 2015
Mesa Redonda Ciencia Ciudadana Campus Gutenberg 2015Mesa Redonda Ciencia Ciudadana Campus Gutenberg 2015
Mesa Redonda Ciencia Ciudadana Campus Gutenberg 2015Josep Perelló
 
Collective experimentation on human behaviour using citizen science practices
Collective experimentation on human behaviour using citizen science practices Collective experimentation on human behaviour using citizen science practices
Collective experimentation on human behaviour using citizen science practices Josep Perelló
 
Harold garfinkel - studies in ethnomethodology
Harold garfinkel - studies in ethnomethodologyHarold garfinkel - studies in ethnomethodology
Harold garfinkel - studies in ethnomethodologyTrisna Nurdiaman
 
A review of pedestrian group dynamics and methodologies in modelling pedestri...
A review of pedestrian group dynamics and methodologies in modelling pedestri...A review of pedestrian group dynamics and methodologies in modelling pedestri...
A review of pedestrian group dynamics and methodologies in modelling pedestri...Premier Publishers
 
Citizen Science: An applied research designed for amateurs & volunteers - A ...
Citizen Science: An applied research designed for amateurs & volunteers - A ...Citizen Science: An applied research designed for amateurs & volunteers - A ...
Citizen Science: An applied research designed for amateurs & volunteers - A ...Peri Kourakli
 
Alternatives To Animal Dissection In School Science Classes
Alternatives To Animal Dissection In School Science ClassesAlternatives To Animal Dissection In School Science Classes
Alternatives To Animal Dissection In School Science ClassesClaudia Acosta
 
Fifty shades of evidence: A transdisciplinary research project on changing cl...
Fifty shades of evidence: A transdisciplinary research project on changing cl...Fifty shades of evidence: A transdisciplinary research project on changing cl...
Fifty shades of evidence: A transdisciplinary research project on changing cl...Carina van Rooyen
 
Annotated bibliography
Annotated bibliographyAnnotated bibliography
Annotated bibliographyHMENI
 
Contributions of Animal Research to Public Health FINAL
Contributions of Animal Research to Public Health FINALContributions of Animal Research to Public Health FINAL
Contributions of Animal Research to Public Health FINALEva Quintero
 
Animals And Sociology
Animals And SociologyAnimals And Sociology
Animals And SociologySara Alvarez
 
The Lesson on Lyme Classroom Activity
The Lesson on Lyme Classroom ActivityThe Lesson on Lyme Classroom Activity
The Lesson on Lyme Classroom ActivityVeronica Nelson
 

Similar to CURRENT ISSUES - PERSPECTIVES AND REVIEWSTribute to Tinber.docx (20)

Latent life: Concepts and practices of human tissue preservation in the In...
Latent life: Concepts  and practices of human  tissue preservation in the  In...Latent life: Concepts  and practices of human  tissue preservation in the  In...
Latent life: Concepts and practices of human tissue preservation in the In...
 
Ethno-Report.doc
Ethno-Report.docEthno-Report.doc
Ethno-Report.doc
 
An Inventory Of Peer-Reviewed Articles On Killer Whales (Orcinus Orca) With A...
An Inventory Of Peer-Reviewed Articles On Killer Whales (Orcinus Orca) With A...An Inventory Of Peer-Reviewed Articles On Killer Whales (Orcinus Orca) With A...
An Inventory Of Peer-Reviewed Articles On Killer Whales (Orcinus Orca) With A...
 
Mesa Redonda Ciencia Ciudadana Campus Gutenberg 2015
Mesa Redonda Ciencia Ciudadana Campus Gutenberg 2015Mesa Redonda Ciencia Ciudadana Campus Gutenberg 2015
Mesa Redonda Ciencia Ciudadana Campus Gutenberg 2015
 
Collective experimentation on human behaviour using citizen science practices
Collective experimentation on human behaviour using citizen science practices Collective experimentation on human behaviour using citizen science practices
Collective experimentation on human behaviour using citizen science practices
 
Harold garfinkel - studies in ethnomethodology
Harold garfinkel - studies in ethnomethodologyHarold garfinkel - studies in ethnomethodology
Harold garfinkel - studies in ethnomethodology
 
Amlc
AmlcAmlc
Amlc
 
Qualitative research types
Qualitative research typesQualitative research types
Qualitative research types
 
A review of pedestrian group dynamics and methodologies in modelling pedestri...
A review of pedestrian group dynamics and methodologies in modelling pedestri...A review of pedestrian group dynamics and methodologies in modelling pedestri...
A review of pedestrian group dynamics and methodologies in modelling pedestri...
 
Citizen Science: An applied research designed for amateurs & volunteers - A ...
Citizen Science: An applied research designed for amateurs & volunteers - A ...Citizen Science: An applied research designed for amateurs & volunteers - A ...
Citizen Science: An applied research designed for amateurs & volunteers - A ...
 
Behavioral Poster Spring 2013
Behavioral Poster Spring 2013Behavioral Poster Spring 2013
Behavioral Poster Spring 2013
 
Alternatives To Animal Dissection In School Science Classes
Alternatives To Animal Dissection In School Science ClassesAlternatives To Animal Dissection In School Science Classes
Alternatives To Animal Dissection In School Science Classes
 
Growing Physical, Social and Cognitive Capacity: Engaging with Natural Enviro...
Growing Physical, Social and Cognitive Capacity: Engaging with Natural Enviro...Growing Physical, Social and Cognitive Capacity: Engaging with Natural Enviro...
Growing Physical, Social and Cognitive Capacity: Engaging with Natural Enviro...
 
Science
ScienceScience
Science
 
lesson 1.pptx
lesson 1.pptxlesson 1.pptx
lesson 1.pptx
 
Fifty shades of evidence: A transdisciplinary research project on changing cl...
Fifty shades of evidence: A transdisciplinary research project on changing cl...Fifty shades of evidence: A transdisciplinary research project on changing cl...
Fifty shades of evidence: A transdisciplinary research project on changing cl...
 
Annotated bibliography
Annotated bibliographyAnnotated bibliography
Annotated bibliography
 
Contributions of Animal Research to Public Health FINAL
Contributions of Animal Research to Public Health FINALContributions of Animal Research to Public Health FINAL
Contributions of Animal Research to Public Health FINAL
 
Animals And Sociology
Animals And SociologyAnimals And Sociology
Animals And Sociology
 
The Lesson on Lyme Classroom Activity
The Lesson on Lyme Classroom ActivityThe Lesson on Lyme Classroom Activity
The Lesson on Lyme Classroom Activity
 

More from dorishigh

Cyber War versus Cyber Realities Cyber War v.docx
Cyber War versus Cyber Realities Cyber War v.docxCyber War versus Cyber Realities Cyber War v.docx
Cyber War versus Cyber Realities Cyber War v.docxdorishigh
 
Cyber terrorism, by definition, is the politically motivated use.docx
Cyber terrorism, by definition, is the politically motivated use.docxCyber terrorism, by definition, is the politically motivated use.docx
Cyber terrorism, by definition, is the politically motivated use.docxdorishigh
 
Cyber Security ThreatsYassir NourDr. Fonda IngramETCS-690 .docx
Cyber Security ThreatsYassir NourDr. Fonda IngramETCS-690 .docxCyber Security ThreatsYassir NourDr. Fonda IngramETCS-690 .docx
Cyber Security ThreatsYassir NourDr. Fonda IngramETCS-690 .docxdorishigh
 
Cyber Security in Industry 4.0Cyber Security in Industry 4.0 (.docx
Cyber Security in Industry 4.0Cyber Security in Industry 4.0 (.docxCyber Security in Industry 4.0Cyber Security in Industry 4.0 (.docx
Cyber Security in Industry 4.0Cyber Security in Industry 4.0 (.docxdorishigh
 
Cyber Security and the Internet of ThingsVulnerabilities, T.docx
Cyber Security and the Internet of ThingsVulnerabilities, T.docxCyber Security and the Internet of ThingsVulnerabilities, T.docx
Cyber Security and the Internet of ThingsVulnerabilities, T.docxdorishigh
 
Cyber Security Gone too farCarlos Diego LimaExce.docx
Cyber Security Gone too farCarlos Diego LimaExce.docxCyber Security Gone too farCarlos Diego LimaExce.docx
Cyber Security Gone too farCarlos Diego LimaExce.docxdorishigh
 
CW 1R Checklist and Feedback Sheet Student Copy Go through this.docx
CW 1R Checklist and Feedback Sheet Student Copy Go through this.docxCW 1R Checklist and Feedback Sheet Student Copy Go through this.docx
CW 1R Checklist and Feedback Sheet Student Copy Go through this.docxdorishigh
 
CW 1 Car Industry and AIby Victoria StephensonSubmission.docx
CW 1 Car Industry and AIby Victoria StephensonSubmission.docxCW 1 Car Industry and AIby Victoria StephensonSubmission.docx
CW 1 Car Industry and AIby Victoria StephensonSubmission.docxdorishigh
 
CWTS CWFT Module 7 Chapter 2 Eco-maps 1 ECO-MAPS .docx
CWTS CWFT Module 7 Chapter 2 Eco-maps 1 ECO-MAPS .docxCWTS CWFT Module 7 Chapter 2 Eco-maps 1 ECO-MAPS .docx
CWTS CWFT Module 7 Chapter 2 Eco-maps 1 ECO-MAPS .docxdorishigh
 
Cw2 Marking Rubric Managerial Finance0Fail2(1-29) Fail.docx
Cw2 Marking Rubric Managerial Finance0Fail2(1-29) Fail.docxCw2 Marking Rubric Managerial Finance0Fail2(1-29) Fail.docx
Cw2 Marking Rubric Managerial Finance0Fail2(1-29) Fail.docxdorishigh
 
Cyber AttacksProtecting National Infrastructure, 1st ed.Ch.docx
Cyber AttacksProtecting National Infrastructure, 1st ed.Ch.docxCyber AttacksProtecting National Infrastructure, 1st ed.Ch.docx
Cyber AttacksProtecting National Infrastructure, 1st ed.Ch.docxdorishigh
 
CVPSales price per unit$75.00Variable Cost per unit$67.00Fixed C.docx
CVPSales price per unit$75.00Variable Cost per unit$67.00Fixed C.docxCVPSales price per unit$75.00Variable Cost per unit$67.00Fixed C.docx
CVPSales price per unit$75.00Variable Cost per unit$67.00Fixed C.docxdorishigh
 
CYB207 v2Wk 4 – Assignment TemplateCYB205 v2Page 2 of 2.docx
CYB207 v2Wk 4 – Assignment TemplateCYB205 v2Page 2 of 2.docxCYB207 v2Wk 4 – Assignment TemplateCYB205 v2Page 2 of 2.docx
CYB207 v2Wk 4 – Assignment TemplateCYB205 v2Page 2 of 2.docxdorishigh
 
CUSTOMERSERVICE-TRAINIGPROGRAM 2 TA.docx
CUSTOMERSERVICE-TRAINIGPROGRAM 2  TA.docxCUSTOMERSERVICE-TRAINIGPROGRAM 2  TA.docx
CUSTOMERSERVICE-TRAINIGPROGRAM 2 TA.docxdorishigh
 
Customer Service Test (Chapter 6 - 10)Name Multiple Choice.docx
Customer Service Test (Chapter 6 - 10)Name Multiple Choice.docxCustomer Service Test (Chapter 6 - 10)Name Multiple Choice.docx
Customer Service Test (Chapter 6 - 10)Name Multiple Choice.docxdorishigh
 
Customer Value Funnel Questions1. Identify the relevant .docx
Customer Value Funnel Questions1. Identify the relevant .docxCustomer Value Funnel Questions1. Identify the relevant .docx
Customer Value Funnel Questions1. Identify the relevant .docxdorishigh
 
Customer service is something that we have all heard of and have som.docx
Customer service is something that we have all heard of and have som.docxCustomer service is something that we have all heard of and have som.docx
Customer service is something that we have all heard of and have som.docxdorishigh
 
Customer requests areProposed Cloud Architecture (5 pages n.docx
Customer requests areProposed Cloud Architecture (5 pages n.docxCustomer requests areProposed Cloud Architecture (5 pages n.docx
Customer requests areProposed Cloud Architecture (5 pages n.docxdorishigh
 
Customer Relationship Management Presented ByShan Gu Cris.docx
Customer Relationship Management Presented ByShan Gu Cris.docxCustomer Relationship Management Presented ByShan Gu Cris.docx
Customer Relationship Management Presented ByShan Gu Cris.docxdorishigh
 
Custom Vans Inc. Custom Vans Inc. specializes in converting st.docx
Custom Vans Inc. Custom Vans Inc. specializes in converting st.docxCustom Vans Inc. Custom Vans Inc. specializes in converting st.docx
Custom Vans Inc. Custom Vans Inc. specializes in converting st.docxdorishigh
 

More from dorishigh (20)

Cyber War versus Cyber Realities Cyber War v.docx
Cyber War versus Cyber Realities Cyber War v.docxCyber War versus Cyber Realities Cyber War v.docx
Cyber War versus Cyber Realities Cyber War v.docx
 
Cyber terrorism, by definition, is the politically motivated use.docx
Cyber terrorism, by definition, is the politically motivated use.docxCyber terrorism, by definition, is the politically motivated use.docx
Cyber terrorism, by definition, is the politically motivated use.docx
 
Cyber Security ThreatsYassir NourDr. Fonda IngramETCS-690 .docx
Cyber Security ThreatsYassir NourDr. Fonda IngramETCS-690 .docxCyber Security ThreatsYassir NourDr. Fonda IngramETCS-690 .docx
Cyber Security ThreatsYassir NourDr. Fonda IngramETCS-690 .docx
 
Cyber Security in Industry 4.0Cyber Security in Industry 4.0 (.docx
Cyber Security in Industry 4.0Cyber Security in Industry 4.0 (.docxCyber Security in Industry 4.0Cyber Security in Industry 4.0 (.docx
Cyber Security in Industry 4.0Cyber Security in Industry 4.0 (.docx
 
Cyber Security and the Internet of ThingsVulnerabilities, T.docx
Cyber Security and the Internet of ThingsVulnerabilities, T.docxCyber Security and the Internet of ThingsVulnerabilities, T.docx
Cyber Security and the Internet of ThingsVulnerabilities, T.docx
 
Cyber Security Gone too farCarlos Diego LimaExce.docx
Cyber Security Gone too farCarlos Diego LimaExce.docxCyber Security Gone too farCarlos Diego LimaExce.docx
Cyber Security Gone too farCarlos Diego LimaExce.docx
 
CW 1R Checklist and Feedback Sheet Student Copy Go through this.docx
CW 1R Checklist and Feedback Sheet Student Copy Go through this.docxCW 1R Checklist and Feedback Sheet Student Copy Go through this.docx
CW 1R Checklist and Feedback Sheet Student Copy Go through this.docx
 
CW 1 Car Industry and AIby Victoria StephensonSubmission.docx
CW 1 Car Industry and AIby Victoria StephensonSubmission.docxCW 1 Car Industry and AIby Victoria StephensonSubmission.docx
CW 1 Car Industry and AIby Victoria StephensonSubmission.docx
 
CWTS CWFT Module 7 Chapter 2 Eco-maps 1 ECO-MAPS .docx
CWTS CWFT Module 7 Chapter 2 Eco-maps 1 ECO-MAPS .docxCWTS CWFT Module 7 Chapter 2 Eco-maps 1 ECO-MAPS .docx
CWTS CWFT Module 7 Chapter 2 Eco-maps 1 ECO-MAPS .docx
 
Cw2 Marking Rubric Managerial Finance0Fail2(1-29) Fail.docx
Cw2 Marking Rubric Managerial Finance0Fail2(1-29) Fail.docxCw2 Marking Rubric Managerial Finance0Fail2(1-29) Fail.docx
Cw2 Marking Rubric Managerial Finance0Fail2(1-29) Fail.docx
 
Cyber AttacksProtecting National Infrastructure, 1st ed.Ch.docx
Cyber AttacksProtecting National Infrastructure, 1st ed.Ch.docxCyber AttacksProtecting National Infrastructure, 1st ed.Ch.docx
Cyber AttacksProtecting National Infrastructure, 1st ed.Ch.docx
 
CVPSales price per unit$75.00Variable Cost per unit$67.00Fixed C.docx
CVPSales price per unit$75.00Variable Cost per unit$67.00Fixed C.docxCVPSales price per unit$75.00Variable Cost per unit$67.00Fixed C.docx
CVPSales price per unit$75.00Variable Cost per unit$67.00Fixed C.docx
 
CYB207 v2Wk 4 – Assignment TemplateCYB205 v2Page 2 of 2.docx
CYB207 v2Wk 4 – Assignment TemplateCYB205 v2Page 2 of 2.docxCYB207 v2Wk 4 – Assignment TemplateCYB205 v2Page 2 of 2.docx
CYB207 v2Wk 4 – Assignment TemplateCYB205 v2Page 2 of 2.docx
 
CUSTOMERSERVICE-TRAINIGPROGRAM 2 TA.docx
CUSTOMERSERVICE-TRAINIGPROGRAM 2  TA.docxCUSTOMERSERVICE-TRAINIGPROGRAM 2  TA.docx
CUSTOMERSERVICE-TRAINIGPROGRAM 2 TA.docx
 
Customer Service Test (Chapter 6 - 10)Name Multiple Choice.docx
Customer Service Test (Chapter 6 - 10)Name Multiple Choice.docxCustomer Service Test (Chapter 6 - 10)Name Multiple Choice.docx
Customer Service Test (Chapter 6 - 10)Name Multiple Choice.docx
 
Customer Value Funnel Questions1. Identify the relevant .docx
Customer Value Funnel Questions1. Identify the relevant .docxCustomer Value Funnel Questions1. Identify the relevant .docx
Customer Value Funnel Questions1. Identify the relevant .docx
 
Customer service is something that we have all heard of and have som.docx
Customer service is something that we have all heard of and have som.docxCustomer service is something that we have all heard of and have som.docx
Customer service is something that we have all heard of and have som.docx
 
Customer requests areProposed Cloud Architecture (5 pages n.docx
Customer requests areProposed Cloud Architecture (5 pages n.docxCustomer requests areProposed Cloud Architecture (5 pages n.docx
Customer requests areProposed Cloud Architecture (5 pages n.docx
 
Customer Relationship Management Presented ByShan Gu Cris.docx
Customer Relationship Management Presented ByShan Gu Cris.docxCustomer Relationship Management Presented ByShan Gu Cris.docx
Customer Relationship Management Presented ByShan Gu Cris.docx
 
Custom Vans Inc. Custom Vans Inc. specializes in converting st.docx
Custom Vans Inc. Custom Vans Inc. specializes in converting st.docxCustom Vans Inc. Custom Vans Inc. specializes in converting st.docx
Custom Vans Inc. Custom Vans Inc. specializes in converting st.docx
 

Recently uploaded

Separation of Lanthanides/ Lanthanides and Actinides
Separation of Lanthanides/ Lanthanides and ActinidesSeparation of Lanthanides/ Lanthanides and Actinides
Separation of Lanthanides/ Lanthanides and ActinidesFatimaKhan178732
 
How to Configure Email Server in Odoo 17
How to Configure Email Server in Odoo 17How to Configure Email Server in Odoo 17
How to Configure Email Server in Odoo 17Celine George
 
MENTAL STATUS EXAMINATION format.docx
MENTAL     STATUS EXAMINATION format.docxMENTAL     STATUS EXAMINATION format.docx
MENTAL STATUS EXAMINATION format.docxPoojaSen20
 
Sanyam Choudhary Chemistry practical.pdf
Sanyam Choudhary Chemistry practical.pdfSanyam Choudhary Chemistry practical.pdf
Sanyam Choudhary Chemistry practical.pdfsanyamsingh5019
 
Hybridoma Technology ( Production , Purification , and Application )
Hybridoma Technology  ( Production , Purification , and Application  ) Hybridoma Technology  ( Production , Purification , and Application  )
Hybridoma Technology ( Production , Purification , and Application ) Sakshi Ghasle
 
mini mental status format.docx
mini    mental       status     format.docxmini    mental       status     format.docx
mini mental status format.docxPoojaSen20
 
Kisan Call Centre - To harness potential of ICT in Agriculture by answer farm...
Kisan Call Centre - To harness potential of ICT in Agriculture by answer farm...Kisan Call Centre - To harness potential of ICT in Agriculture by answer farm...
Kisan Call Centre - To harness potential of ICT in Agriculture by answer farm...Krashi Coaching
 
Incoming and Outgoing Shipments in 1 STEP Using Odoo 17
Incoming and Outgoing Shipments in 1 STEP Using Odoo 17Incoming and Outgoing Shipments in 1 STEP Using Odoo 17
Incoming and Outgoing Shipments in 1 STEP Using Odoo 17Celine George
 
Alper Gobel In Media Res Media Component
Alper Gobel In Media Res Media ComponentAlper Gobel In Media Res Media Component
Alper Gobel In Media Res Media ComponentInMediaRes1
 
CARE OF CHILD IN INCUBATOR..........pptx
CARE OF CHILD IN INCUBATOR..........pptxCARE OF CHILD IN INCUBATOR..........pptx
CARE OF CHILD IN INCUBATOR..........pptxGaneshChakor2
 
Class 11 Legal Studies Ch-1 Concept of State .pdf
Class 11 Legal Studies Ch-1 Concept of State .pdfClass 11 Legal Studies Ch-1 Concept of State .pdf
Class 11 Legal Studies Ch-1 Concept of State .pdfakmcokerachita
 
Interactive Powerpoint_How to Master effective communication
Interactive Powerpoint_How to Master effective communicationInteractive Powerpoint_How to Master effective communication
Interactive Powerpoint_How to Master effective communicationnomboosow
 
Organic Name Reactions for the students and aspirants of Chemistry12th.pptx
Organic Name Reactions  for the students and aspirants of Chemistry12th.pptxOrganic Name Reactions  for the students and aspirants of Chemistry12th.pptx
Organic Name Reactions for the students and aspirants of Chemistry12th.pptxVS Mahajan Coaching Centre
 
The Most Excellent Way | 1 Corinthians 13
The Most Excellent Way | 1 Corinthians 13The Most Excellent Way | 1 Corinthians 13
The Most Excellent Way | 1 Corinthians 13Steve Thomason
 
KSHARA STURA .pptx---KSHARA KARMA THERAPY (CAUSTIC THERAPY)————IMP.OF KSHARA ...
KSHARA STURA .pptx---KSHARA KARMA THERAPY (CAUSTIC THERAPY)————IMP.OF KSHARA ...KSHARA STURA .pptx---KSHARA KARMA THERAPY (CAUSTIC THERAPY)————IMP.OF KSHARA ...
KSHARA STURA .pptx---KSHARA KARMA THERAPY (CAUSTIC THERAPY)————IMP.OF KSHARA ...M56BOOKSTORE PRODUCT/SERVICE
 

Recently uploaded (20)

Separation of Lanthanides/ Lanthanides and Actinides
Separation of Lanthanides/ Lanthanides and ActinidesSeparation of Lanthanides/ Lanthanides and Actinides
Separation of Lanthanides/ Lanthanides and Actinides
 
How to Configure Email Server in Odoo 17
How to Configure Email Server in Odoo 17How to Configure Email Server in Odoo 17
How to Configure Email Server in Odoo 17
 
MENTAL STATUS EXAMINATION format.docx
MENTAL     STATUS EXAMINATION format.docxMENTAL     STATUS EXAMINATION format.docx
MENTAL STATUS EXAMINATION format.docx
 
Sanyam Choudhary Chemistry practical.pdf
Sanyam Choudhary Chemistry practical.pdfSanyam Choudhary Chemistry practical.pdf
Sanyam Choudhary Chemistry practical.pdf
 
Hybridoma Technology ( Production , Purification , and Application )
Hybridoma Technology  ( Production , Purification , and Application  ) Hybridoma Technology  ( Production , Purification , and Application  )
Hybridoma Technology ( Production , Purification , and Application )
 
Model Call Girl in Bikash Puri Delhi reach out to us at 🔝9953056974🔝
Model Call Girl in Bikash Puri  Delhi reach out to us at 🔝9953056974🔝Model Call Girl in Bikash Puri  Delhi reach out to us at 🔝9953056974🔝
Model Call Girl in Bikash Puri Delhi reach out to us at 🔝9953056974🔝
 
Staff of Color (SOC) Retention Efforts DDSD
Staff of Color (SOC) Retention Efforts DDSDStaff of Color (SOC) Retention Efforts DDSD
Staff of Color (SOC) Retention Efforts DDSD
 
mini mental status format.docx
mini    mental       status     format.docxmini    mental       status     format.docx
mini mental status format.docx
 
9953330565 Low Rate Call Girls In Rohini Delhi NCR
9953330565 Low Rate Call Girls In Rohini  Delhi NCR9953330565 Low Rate Call Girls In Rohini  Delhi NCR
9953330565 Low Rate Call Girls In Rohini Delhi NCR
 
Kisan Call Centre - To harness potential of ICT in Agriculture by answer farm...
Kisan Call Centre - To harness potential of ICT in Agriculture by answer farm...Kisan Call Centre - To harness potential of ICT in Agriculture by answer farm...
Kisan Call Centre - To harness potential of ICT in Agriculture by answer farm...
 
Código Creativo y Arte de Software | Unidad 1
Código Creativo y Arte de Software | Unidad 1Código Creativo y Arte de Software | Unidad 1
Código Creativo y Arte de Software | Unidad 1
 
Incoming and Outgoing Shipments in 1 STEP Using Odoo 17
Incoming and Outgoing Shipments in 1 STEP Using Odoo 17Incoming and Outgoing Shipments in 1 STEP Using Odoo 17
Incoming and Outgoing Shipments in 1 STEP Using Odoo 17
 
Alper Gobel In Media Res Media Component
Alper Gobel In Media Res Media ComponentAlper Gobel In Media Res Media Component
Alper Gobel In Media Res Media Component
 
CARE OF CHILD IN INCUBATOR..........pptx
CARE OF CHILD IN INCUBATOR..........pptxCARE OF CHILD IN INCUBATOR..........pptx
CARE OF CHILD IN INCUBATOR..........pptx
 
Class 11 Legal Studies Ch-1 Concept of State .pdf
Class 11 Legal Studies Ch-1 Concept of State .pdfClass 11 Legal Studies Ch-1 Concept of State .pdf
Class 11 Legal Studies Ch-1 Concept of State .pdf
 
Interactive Powerpoint_How to Master effective communication
Interactive Powerpoint_How to Master effective communicationInteractive Powerpoint_How to Master effective communication
Interactive Powerpoint_How to Master effective communication
 
Organic Name Reactions for the students and aspirants of Chemistry12th.pptx
Organic Name Reactions  for the students and aspirants of Chemistry12th.pptxOrganic Name Reactions  for the students and aspirants of Chemistry12th.pptx
Organic Name Reactions for the students and aspirants of Chemistry12th.pptx
 
The Most Excellent Way | 1 Corinthians 13
The Most Excellent Way | 1 Corinthians 13The Most Excellent Way | 1 Corinthians 13
The Most Excellent Way | 1 Corinthians 13
 
TataKelola dan KamSiber Kecerdasan Buatan v022.pdf
TataKelola dan KamSiber Kecerdasan Buatan v022.pdfTataKelola dan KamSiber Kecerdasan Buatan v022.pdf
TataKelola dan KamSiber Kecerdasan Buatan v022.pdf
 
KSHARA STURA .pptx---KSHARA KARMA THERAPY (CAUSTIC THERAPY)————IMP.OF KSHARA ...
KSHARA STURA .pptx---KSHARA KARMA THERAPY (CAUSTIC THERAPY)————IMP.OF KSHARA ...KSHARA STURA .pptx---KSHARA KARMA THERAPY (CAUSTIC THERAPY)————IMP.OF KSHARA ...
KSHARA STURA .pptx---KSHARA KARMA THERAPY (CAUSTIC THERAPY)————IMP.OF KSHARA ...
 

CURRENT ISSUES - PERSPECTIVES AND REVIEWSTribute to Tinber.docx

  • 1. CURRENT ISSUES - PERSPECTIVES AND REVIEWS Tribute to Tinbergen: Public Engagement in Ethology Julie Hecht* & Caren B. Cooper† * Doctoral Program in Psychology, The Graduate Center, City University of New York, New York, NY, USA † Cornell Lab of Ornithology, Ithaca, NY, USA (Invited Review) Correspondence Julie Hecht, Department of Psychology, Hunter College and The Graduate Center, City University of New York, 695 Park Avenue, New York, NY 10065, USA. E-mail: [email protected] Received: October 4, 2013 Initial acceptance: October 19, 2013 Final acceptance: November 25, 2013 (M. Hauber) doi: 10.1111/eth.12199
  • 2. Keywords: citizen science, data quality, animal behavior, informal science education Abstract Public engagement in research, called citizen science, has led to advances in a range of fields like astronomy, ornithology, and public health. While volunteers have been making and sharing observations according to pro- tocols set by researchers in numerous disciplines, citizen science practices are less common in the field of animal behavior. We consider how citizen science might be used to address animal behavior questions at Tinbergen’s four levels of analysis. We briefly review resources and methods for addressing technical issues surrounding volunteer participation—such as data quality—so that citizen science can make long-standing contributions to the field of animal behavior. Introduction
  • 3. ‘Citizen science’ describes the various ways that mem- bers of the public participate in genuine scientific research (Cooper et al. 2007a; Silvertown 2009; Shirk et al. 2012). Many citizen science projects arise from communities with specific concerns (such as long- term environmental monitoring), and some are initiated by scientists to address specific research objectives. Other projects have additional goals to increase science literacy (Bonney et al. 2009a). Research via public engagement is possible because people enjoy making natural history observations and sharing these observations with professionals and peers. In addition, Web 2.0 and mobile technologies facilitate mass participation. Consequently, citizen science projects have arisen in a myriad of disciplines, including ecology, phenology, macroecology, public health, natural resource management, hydrology, urban planning, meteorology, math, volcanology, and
  • 4. various taxon-specific fields such as entomology, ornithology, and herpetology. In recent years, citizen science has enabled substantial contributions to the fields of astronomy (Lintott et al. 2008), medicine (Khatib et al. 2011a,b), and climate change (Morisette et al. 2009). Currently, the field of animal behavior is under- represented among citizen science projects. Animal behavior research integrates diverse methodological approaches to investigate a wide array of scientific questions about the behavior of wild and domesti- cated animals in natural and captive settings. Public observations of occurrence data (e.g., wildlife sight- ings; Sn€all et al. 2011), count data (e.g., number of birds; Cooper et al. 2007b), simple measurements (e.g., rainfall; Cifelli et al. 2005), and even phenologi- cal stages (e.g., budburst; Morisette et al. 2009) are more common applications of citizen science, in part
  • 5. because such data likely require simpler protocols than most animal behavior data. The dearth of citizen science research in animal behavior is not necessarily due to lack of general pub- lic interest. Pet-keeping is a global phenomenon (Serpell 1996); wildlife tourism and wildlife docu- mentaries are popular (Reynolds & Braithwaite 2001; Nelson & Fijn 2013); zoo attendance is on the rise in parts of the world (Davey 2007); and people are already expressing interest in animal behavior citizen Ethology 120 (2014) 207–214 © 2013 Blackwell Verlag GmbH 207 Ethology ethology international journal of behavioural biology science projects (Foster et al. 2011; Williams et al. 2012). In the USA, one in four adults watches wildlife (USFWS 2011), and the numerous websites that
  • 6. stream live video of pets (e.g., SPCA cams) and wild- life (e.g., nesting birds) are extremely popular (e.g., the Cornell Lab of Ornithology nest cams receive millions of unique viewers from 175 countries, including up to 9,000 simultaneous views of live- streaming video). Citizen science contributions could enhance animal behavior investigations by increasing observations on time and geographic scales (e.g., latitudinal gradients, urbanization gradients, comparing populations, etc), and these projects also have the potential to process massive amounts of data (e.g., video or photo tag- ging). In essence, citizen scientists provide an extra pair of hands—or extra thousands of pairs. While a small team of researchers documented the behavior of the colonial orb-weaving spider during a solar eclipse in Veracruz, Mexico (Uetz et al. 1994), the team might have used citizen scientists to capture behav- ioral data on additional populations or species during
  • 7. the short-lived event. Without the help of citizen sci- entists, researchers are effectively ‘missing’ variation among animals. Field research, behavior observation, and behavior coding can be time-consuming, challenging to coordi- nate, and expensive. Trained volunteers might offer lower-cost options for collecting and/or scoring behavior (Williams et al. 2012). Depending on the study design and complexity, volunteers can be used to ensure blind coding. Citizen science methods could facilitate access to a wide variety of species in different contexts and foster integration of studies of natural populations and pop- ulations under artificial selection. Additionally, an expanded research platform offers the possibility of capturing animals in novel contexts. Video and social media, particularly YouTube, are rife with examples of uncommon (Burn 2011) or previously unexplored
  • 8. behaviors, such as interspecies play or object play (Nelson & Fijn 2013). Behaviors captured in real- world contexts can lead to new research directions, and citizen scientists could provide behavioral video footage. We examine the realized and potential application of citizen science to research animal behavior—the inquiry about what animals do, how they do it, and why they do it. Tinbergen (1963) framed the disci- pline as encompassing four levels of analysis: causa- tion, development, function, and phylogeny. We use Tinbergen’s levels of analysis as a frame- work to discuss a possible partnership between citizen science and animal behavior research. We note that citizen scientists could examine a particular behavior, for example bird song, to generate data for answering questions at all four levels (e.g., causation: ‘What day length triggers bird song?’; development: ‘Do offspring
  • 9. imitate songs of parents?’; function: ‘Do females prefer certain song types?’; and phylogeny: ‘Which species have similar songs?’). Professional scientists can obtain a more robust understanding of an observed behavior through investigations of non- competing hypotheses at multiple levels. Other questions might be best suited for investigation at one particular level of analysis. We discuss a variety of applications below, with the common theme that citi- zen science participants can scale-up and advance research that individual scientists could not do alone. Additionally, we review the technical facets of a union between animal behavior and citizen science, giving attention to crucial details in training volun- teers and handling volunteer data. Citizen Science at Four Levels of Analysis In the 1963 paper, On aims and methods of Ethology, Tinbergen proposed an integrated approach to the
  • 10. study of ethology to address the lack of a unified public image of ethology and to resolve conceptual differences between researchers. Since then, “how questions,” exploring the immediate causes of behav- ior, and “why questions,” exploring the evolutionary forces behind behavior, have been pursued as complementary approaches. Causation The causal basis of behavior focuses on stimuli and mechanisms that trigger, cue, or precede a behavior. For early ethologists, physiological and neural mecha- nisms occurred in a ‘black box’ upon which they could only speculate. Today, inquiry into causation draws from genetics, endocrinology, physiology, and neurobiology. Direct participation and input from citizen scientists is already underway in neurobiology (Eyewire, https://eyewire.org/signup) and genetics (Phylo) (Coo- per et al. 2013). For domesticated and wild populations,
  • 11. the collection of saliva and fecal samples could contrib- ute to studies of stress physiology and measurements of well-being to ultimately address a wide variety of behav- ioral endocrinological questions. Participant involvement could complement labo- ratory findings by collecting behavioral data on pre-screened or pre-selected populations. For exam- ple, volunteers might assist research into relationships Ethology 120 (2014) 207–214 © 2013 Blackwell Verlag GmbH208 Public Engagement in Ethology J. Hecht & C. B. Cooper between genetic markers and behavior by collecting behavioral data on pre-selected animals (e.g., through a web portal for annotating video) whose genes have been mapped. Researchers recently determined that particular allelic qualities of DRD4 and TH gene poly- morphisms were associated with activity–impulsivity in German Shepherds and Siberian Huskies, with
  • 12. behavioral analysis stemming from approx. 250 dogs (Kubinyi et al. 2012; Wan et al. 2013). Continued exploration of gene variants in dogs could be coupled with citizen science projects collecting behavioral data on dogs with known genetic composition to generate a more comprehensive understanding of behavioral traits. Citizen science allows researchers to expand the size of their data set without requiring the participants to have any knowledge of the DNA sampling and gen- otyping that go into pre-screening decisions. The addition of a citizen science component can increase the rigor of the research in at least two ways. First, the volunteer contributions could ensure a dou- ble-blind experiment. Second, the volunteers provide observations from multiple observers, and researchers can use a consensus tool to score behavior, analogous to consensus tools (replication by multiple partici- pants) common to online crowdsourcing efforts for
  • 13. other research (Wiggins et al. 2011). Additionally, volunteers can install cameras to record animal behavior and facilitate remote coding by professional researchers. For example, scientists used footage from volunteer nest cameras to examine proximate control of egg-laying behaviors in nestbox-dwelling bird species (Cooper et al. 2009). Development Approaches to animal behavior from a developmental perspective are also mechanistic, but with a focus on changes in an organism’s capacity for expressing or acquiring behaviors at different stages over the course of a lifetime. Citizen science offers a unique opportunity to inves- tigate ontogenetic questions in a wide range of species. For example, citizen scientists might assist in the study of social behavior and document the devel- opment of particular behavioral patterns—such as fac- tors affecting the onset and development of play in
  • 14. various species—or personality changes (Stamps & Groothuis 2010). Volunteers could collect and report data on single organisms—whether subjects be a com- panion animal, a backyard nestbox or an annually returning species—and generate substantial data points for each individual. Ultimately, with many volunteers, the increased sample sizes could make full-scale statistical analysis possible (Dickinson et al. 2010). Citizen scientists could also examine the develop- ment of animal sensorium, such as in relation to mor- phological traits. For example, assumptions about the different sensory abilities in dog breeds abound, and such differences might be expected given physiologi- cal differences (McGreevy et al. 2003), but actual behavioral and development data are wanting. Researchers could design and implement a project where participants conduct simple, in-home tests examining behavior responses toward different stim-
  • 15. uli at different ages, thereby offering additional data on breed (and even species) sensory development. These behavioral measures of development are com- parable to traditional laboratory experiments (Lord 2013). But, with the help of citizen scientists, researchers can obtain data on a much larger scale. Zoo visitors and wildlife tourists can play a role in monitoring individuals’ development. Zoo visitors express interest in animal behavior studies (Bowler et al. 2012) and could collect many more hours (or years) of observations than possible by resource- limited staff with many other care-taking duties. Such efforts might elucidate the onset or development of behaviors such as stereotypic motor patterns as well as factors affecting the development of these behaviors. Zoo visitors, however, can have bias toward collecting observations of active animals (Altman 1998; Williams et al. 2012).
  • 16. Function The analysis of function focuses on the pressures that shape behaviors. The pervasive influence of the built environment means that many organisms reside in environmental conditions that differ in novel ways from the conditions under which traits evolved. For example, the anthropogenic sound levels near high- ways and flight paths create novel selection pressures, and some animals might be able to adjust their behav- iors within a range of plasticity, such as birds singing at higher pitches near roads (Slabbekoorn & Peet 2003; Slabbekoorn & den Boer-Visser 2006). Other traits might exhibit limited phenotypic plasticity, and disturbances may result in population decline. Fur- thermore, both domesticated and non-domesticated animals residing in homes or zoos will demonstrate behaviors that might no longer be adaptive. Research- ers can design participatory projects in which volun-
  • 17. teers collect data on species in different environments and investigate different hypotheses behind complex behaviors. In an online project called CamClickr, par- Ethology 120 (2014) 207–214 © 2013 Blackwell Verlag GmbH 209 J. Hecht & C. B. Cooper Public Engagement in Ethology ticipants added behavior-category tags to images archived from nest cameras to help investigate varia- tions in nest attendance (Voss & Cooper 2010). Because funding cycles offered by granting institu- tions tend to span three- to five-year periods, collect- ing long-term data sets can be challenging for research scientists. The collection of such data sets could be made possible through citizen science activi- ties. Depending on the initiative, initial investment to set up the project, and maintenance costs, could be low with free online tools for managing projects (e.g., www.citsci.org), recruiting volunteers (e.g., www.
  • 18. scistarter.com), and data archiving (e.g., www.data- one.org). Citizen scientists could measure different components of fitness such as survival to maturity, longevity, mating success, and reproductive success. Phylogeny Behaviors can also be understood from a phylogenetic perspective. By considering ancestry, researchers can identify traits that are highly conserved and those that are more recently evolved within the same lineage. Because closely related species are found in everyday environments, a citizen science project could collect a wide variety of behavioral data on closely related spe- cies in different environments that have adopted dif- ferent survival strategies. By comparing closely related species occurring on different continents (e.g., black-billed bagpie Pica hudsonia in America and the European magpie Pica pica in Eurasia), citizen scien- tists can provide simultaneous data sets without the
  • 19. confound of temporal displacement typical of smaller- scale research projects. Not all citizen engagement in animal behavior pro- jects fits neatly into Tinbergen’s levels of analysis. Recently, Project: Play With Your Dog (Horowitz Dog Cognition Lab, Barnard College) requested short vid- eos depicting play between a dog and a person, with the aim of capturing the wide array of behaviors and conditions that encompass interspecific play. Video submissions drew from around the world, and although participants did not contribute directly to behavioral analysis, citizen scientists had to think about behavior in terms of how they and their dog play together and provide behavioral descriptors in an accompanying questionnaire. Similarly, an online survey by Animal Welfare Indi- cators asked participants to assist in developing a con- tinuous behavioral scale of lameness for goats.
  • 20. Participants watched nine videos and scored abnormal gait, head-nodding, and spine curvature on a visual analogue scale. Before participants began, each behavior had a clear description, and participants were schooled in measuring behavior as per the speci- fications of the project. Merging Ethology and Citizen Science Methods Creating Successful Citizen Science Animal Behavior Projects Like all successful research endeavors, the design of citizen science projects follows the steps of the scien- tific method; the only difference is the additional layer of complexity introduced by the involvement of the lay public. Projects start with the formulation of scien- tific questions, organization of a team of skilled experts in relevant areas, and determination of appro- priate data collection (Bonney et al. 2009b). Depend- ing on the legal status and various protections offered
  • 21. to different species, researchers might need to present protocols to Institutional Animal Care and Use Committees (Bayne 1998). Citizen scientists can learn behavior-coding techniques—or relevant project- related skills—in online videos or in person. The research team can control quality before the project begins by testing each prospective participant’s accuracy and ability to collect meaningful data. Behavioral data accuracy is a key concern, and pro- jects incorporating citizen science data should con- sider topics like observation quality, sampling bias, and interobserver reliability (Dickinson et al. 2010; Burghardt et al. 2012; Hart et al. 2012). Williams et al. (2012) found that although zoo visitor interest in the project was high, the activity budget data col- lected on a group of captive otters were not accurate, mostly because visitors did not adhere to the observa- tion period; the researchers suggest that certain sampling methods might be easier—or harder—for
  • 22. observers. Meanwhile, there are numerous ways to improve data quality in citizen science (Cohn 2008; Silvertown 2009; Wiggins et al. 2011; Bonter & Cooper 2012). Technologies like smartphones, multimedia, or inter- active video could assist in time-keeping and behav- ioral data collection (Parr et al. 2004; Aanensen et al. 2009). Data management tools, often through cyber- infrastructure, improve efficiency (Newman et al. 2011). Additionally, some citizen science projects demonstrate that certain types of people collect more accurate data than others (Delaney et al. 2008), and similar lessons can make their way into citizen science projects in animal behavior. Finally, researchers can be creative in where and how volunteers access animals and how behavior will Ethology 120 (2014) 207–214 © 2013 Blackwell Verlag GmbH210 Public Engagement in Ethology J. Hecht & C. B. Cooper
  • 23. be observed or documented. In some cases, behaviors are not observed directly but are inferred. Phenology research addresses the timing of life-cycle events, including behaviors such as egg laying and migration in birds, insects, and some reptiles and amphibians (Head et al. 2013). Limited access to animals in situa- tions where volunteers can obtain unbiased observa- tions of life-cycle events may have been one of the largest impediments to citizen science in animal behavior. Now, Internet and remote-camera techno- logies combined with crowdsourcing infrastructure offer new opportunities to collect data on animals that are difficult to access (Deng et al. 2012; Head et al. 2013; Loos & Ernst 2013). Live-streaming cameras can be set up at areas with known activity—animal nesting or gathering sites—so that people can docu- ment behaviors at a distance. In addition to remote/ web-mediated studies, possibilities abound for studies
  • 24. of companion animals (e.g., dogs, cats, rabbits, birds, ferrets, fish, gerbils, and hamsters), zoo animals, ani- mals viewed during nature tourism, and urban wild- life such as squirrels, pigeons, rats, foxes, and coyotes. Volunteers could document the behavior of animals at zoos or wildlife tourism destinations, especially ani- mals that handlers and keepers do not have time to record. Special interest groups—such as the House Rabbit Society, backyard chicken groups, and pet breeders—offer significant potential for citizen science projects. Training and Resources for Animal Behavior Inquiry Skill in observing and measuring animal behavior does not necessarily follow from an appreciation of or even cohabitation with animals. Members of the pub- lic are generally interested in animals yet have a pro- clivity not toward Tinbergen’s four levels of behavior, but often toward anthropomorphic explanations of behavior.
  • 25. While the study of animal behavior is an attempt to investigate ultimate and proximate questions about behavior, humans often characterize animal behavior through an anthropocentric lens (Foster et al. 2011). People readily ascribe human characteristics to non- humans. Subjective assessments of non-human behavior can be consistent across observers, often reifying existing beliefs regardless of whether they have been substantiated (Bahlig-Pieren & Turner 1999; Morris et al. 2008; Horowitz 2011). Seminal work by Heider & Simmel (1944) found that people consistently attribute human emotions, aims, and objectives to inanimate objects moving in a ‘human- like’ manner. Companion animal species, mammals, and non-human primates are more readily anthropo- morphized than other species (Eddy et al. 1993), even though seemingly ‘alien’ species, like cephalopods, display complex behaviors and cognitive processes
  • 26. (Fiorito & Scotto 1992; Moriyama & Gunji 1997). Importantly, the appearance of behavioral homologies should not imply similar meaning, such as the ‘smil- ing’ chimpanzee or dolphin (Horowitz 2007). Tendencies toward anthropomorphism are perhaps a useful ‘hook’ when engaging the general public in animal behavior projects. Recently, an online video titled ‘Lol cats like stroking too’ (http://www. youtube.com/watch?v=OFCRvjle2o8) accompanied a publication on the neurons activated by massage-like stroking to assist in public understanding of the research (Vrontou et al. 2013). The video received over 200,000 views in the first year. Clearly defined project expectations and training materials can minimize the effects of anthropomor- phic inferences (Voss & Cooper 2010). Scientists can provide a detailed ethogram, an explicit coding scheme, and a clear behavior-sampling technique
  • 27. (Martin & Bateson 2007). Behaviors in an ethogram can be distinguished based on structural or empirical characteristics, such as ‘raised lips at edges’, or conse- quence-based descriptors, such as ‘threat’. Empirical descriptors help create unambiguous behavioral parameters (Lehner 1987) but might offer challenges for unpracticed observers. Volunteers need a clear understanding of the terms in which they are expected to view and document behavior and have the technology, such as a timer, to code appropriately. Researchers determine who is being watched and when and how to watch them. Some coding schemes provide information on how much time an animal spends performing various behaviors, while other schemes monitor behavior frequencies (Altmann 1974). Researchers must match the skill level of observers with the scientific question. The role of vol- unteers in the study of animal behavior will largely
  • 28. involve the production of meaningful behavioral data. The challenge will be to offer effective guidance and instruction to avoid anthropocentric assessments. A number of institutions have created behavior- observation instructional platforms. Ethosearch (www. ethosearch.org), created by the Lincoln Park Zoo, is a resource for researchers and students to store and view ethograms and practice measuring animal behavior. Living Links (www.living-links.org)—a joint enclosure for squirrel monkeys (Saimiri sciureus) and capuchins (Sapajus apella) at the Edinburgh Zoo—offers a public-learning resource to observe and score the behav- ior of both species (Macdonald & Whiten 2011). Cam- Ethology 120 (2014) 207–214 © 2013 Blackwell Verlag GmbH 211 J. Hecht & C. B. Cooper Public Engagement in Ethology Clickr was an online citizen science project to code the behaviors of nesting birds from still images as a way of
  • 29. carrying out scan sampling. Voss & Cooper (2010) cre- ated a college-level laboratory unit in which students can use CamClickr to create ethograms while learning about observer bias, observation techniques, and defin- ing and quantifying observations. Conclusion Animal behavior researchers might consider whether and how citizen science could support and amplify their studies. Volunteers can expand the scope and scale of animal behavior studies by bringing attention to novel or rare behaviors or providing access to additional variation in animal behaviors. Volunteers, too, can benefit from personal experiences engaging in hands-on scientific interactions with animals. Undoubtedly, challenges arise with the involve- ment of citizen science in animal behavior, yet the possibilities are great. Altmann’s (1974, p. 229) text on behavior sampling methods stresses that there
  • 30. should not be a disconnect between the laboratory and the real world: ‘Such a restriction on research would mean that the behavioral sciences would for- ever forsake any hope of knowing whether their most powerful theories have any relevance in the world of behavior outside the laboratory.’ Citizen scientists offer an additional, and untapped, opportunity for ethological exploration. Acknowledgements Thanks to Felicity Muth, Mark Hauber, and Andrew Fulmer for conceptual conversations. Many thanks to reviewers for comments, which improved the manu- script. Literature cited Aanensen, D. M., Huntley, D. M., Feil, E. J., al-Own, F. & Spratt, B. G. 2009: EpiCollect: linking smartphones to web applications for epidemiology, ecology and community data collection. PLoS One 4, e6968.
  • 31. Altman, J. D. 1998: Animal activity and visitor learning at the zoo. Anthrozoos 11, 12—21. Altmann, J. 1974: Observational study of behavior: sampling methods. Behaviour 49, 227—267. Bahlig-Pieren, Z. & Turner, D. C. 1999: Anthropomorphic interpretations and ethological descriptions of dog and cat behavior by lay people. Anthrozoos 12, 205—210. Bayne, K. 1998: Developing guidelines on the care and use of animals. Ann. N.Y. Acad. Sci. 862, 105—110. Bonney, R., Ballard, H., Jordan, R., McCallie, E., Phillips, T., Shirk, J. & Wilderman, C. C. 2009a: Public participa- tion in scientific research: defining the field and assess- ing its potential for informal science education. A CAISE Inquiry Group Report. Center for Advancement of Informal Science Education (CAISE), Washington, D.C. Bonney, R., Cooper, C. B., Dickinson, J., Kelling, S., Phillips, T., Rosenberg, K. V. & Shirk, J. 2009b: Citizen science: a developing tool for expanding science knowl-
  • 32. edge and scientific literacy. Bioscience 59, 977—984. Bonter, D. N. & Cooper, C. B. 2012: Data validation in citizen science: a case study from Project FeederWatch. Front. Ecol. Environ. 10, 305—307. Bowler, M. T., Buchanan-Smith, H. M. & Whiten, A. 2012: Assessing public engagement with science in a university primate research centre in a national zoo. PLoS One 7, e34505. Burghardt, G. M., Bartmess-LeVasseur, J. N., Browning, S. A., Morrison, K. E., Stec, C. L., Zachau, C. E. & Free- berg, T. M. 2012: Perspectives—Minimizing observer bias in behavioral studies: a review and recommenda- tions. Ethology 118, 511—517. Burn, C. C. 2011: A vicious cycle: A cross-sectional study of canine tail-chasing and human responses to it, using a free video-sharing website. PLoS One 6, e26553. Cifelli, R., Doesken, N., Kennedy, P., Carey, L. D., Rutledge, S. A., Gimmestad, C. & Depue, T. 2005: The
  • 33. community collaborative rain, hail, and snow network: informal education for scientists and citizens. Bull. Amer. Meteor. Soc. 86, 1069—1077. Cohn, J. P. 2008: Citizen science: can volunteers do real research? Bioscience 58, 192—197. Cooper, C. B., Dickinson, J., Phillips, T. & Bonney, R. 2007a: Citizen science as a tool for conservation in residential ecosystems. Ecol. Soc. 12, 11. Cooper, C. B., Hochachka, W. M. & Dhondt, A. A. 2007b: Contrasting natural experiments confirm competition between house finches and house sparrows. Ecology 88, 864—870. Cooper, C. B., Voss, M. A. & Zivkovic, B. 2009: Extended laying interval of ultimate eggs of the Eastern Bluebird. Condor 11, 752—755. Cooper, S., Khatib, F. & Baker, D. 2013: Increasing public involvement in structural biology. Structure 21, 1482— 1484.
  • 34. Davey, G. 2007: An analysis of country, socio-economic and time factors on worldwide zoo attendance during a 40 year period. Int. Zoo Yearb. 41, 217—225. Delaney, D. G., Sperling, C. D., Adams, C. S. & Leung, B. 2008: Marine invasive species: validation of citizen science and implications for national monitoring networks. Biol. Invasions 10, 117—128. Deng, D., Chuang, T., Shao, K., Mai, G., Lin, T., Lemmens, R., Hsu, C., Lin, H. & Kraak, M. 2012: Using social media for collaborative species identification and occurrence: Ethology 120 (2014) 207–214 © 2013 Blackwell Verlag GmbH212 Public Engagement in Ethology J. Hecht & C. B. Cooper issues, methods, and tools. SIGSPATIAL 2012 Interna- tional Conference on Advances in Geographic Informa- tion Systems, ACM, 22—29. Dickinson, J. L., Zuckerberg, B. & Bonter, D. N. 2010: Citizen science as an ecological research tool: challenges
  • 35. and benefits. Annu. Rev. Ecol. Evol. Syst. 41, 149—172. Eddy, T. J., Gallup, G. G. Jr & Povinelli, D. J. 1993: Attribution of cognitive states to animals: anthropomorphism in comparative perspective. J. Soc. Issues 49, 87—101. Fiorito, G. & Scotto, P. 1992: Observational learning in Octopus vulgaris. Science 256, 545—547. Foster, D., Kirman, B., Linehan, C., Lawson, S., Mills, D., Ellis, S. & Zulch, H. 2011: “I can haz emoshuns?”: understanding anthropomorphosis of cats among internet users. 3rd International Conference on Social Computing, 9-11 October 2011, MIT Boston. Hart, A., Stafford, R., Goodenough, A. & Morgan, S. 2012: The role of citizen science and volunteer data collection in zoological research. Int. J. Zool., Article ID 105345, doi:10.1155/2012/105345. Head, J. S., Boesch, C., Robbins, M. M., Rabanal, L. I., Makaga, L. & K€uhl, H. S. 2013: Effective sociodemo-
  • 36. graphic population assessment of elusive species in ecology and conservation management. Ecol. Evol. 3, 2903—2916. Heider, F. & Simmel, M. 1944: An experimental study of apparent behavior. Am. J. Psychol. 57, 243—259. Horowitz, A. 2007: Anthropomorphism. In. M. Encyclopedia of Human-Animal Relationships (Bekoff, M. ed). Greenwood Publishing Group, Westport, CT. pp. 60—66. Horowitz, A. 2011: Empirically testing anthropomor- phisms of the domestic dog. J. Vet. Behav. 6, 75. Khatib, F., Cooper, S., Tyka, M. D., Xu, K., Makedon, I., Popovi�c, Z., Baker, D. & Foldit Players. 2011a: Algo- rithm discovery by protein folding game players. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 108, 18949—18953. Khatib, F., DiMaio, F., Cooper, S., Kazmierczyk, M., Gilski, M., Krzywda, S., Z�abransk�a, H., Pichov�a, I., Thompson, J., Popovi�c, Z., Jaskolski, M. & Baker, D. Foldit Contend-
  • 37. ers Group, Foldit Void Crushers Group 2011b: Crystal structure of a monomeric retroviral protease solved by protein folding game players. Nat. Struct. Mol. Biol. 18, 1175—1177. Kubinyi, E., Vas, J., Hejjas, K., Ronai, Z., Br�uder, I., Turcs�an, B., Sasvari-Szekely, M. & Mikl�osi, �A. 2012: Polymorphism in the tyrosine hydroxylase (TH) gene is associated with activity-impulsivity in German Shepherd dogs. PLoS One 7, e30271. Lehner, P. N. 1987: Design and execution of animal behavior research: an overview. J. Anim. Sci. 65, 1213 —1219. Lintott, C. J., Schawinski, K., Slosar, A., Land, K., Bam- ford, S., Thomas, D., Raddick, M. J., Nichol, R. C., Szalay, A., Andreescu, D., Murray, P. & Vandenberg, J. 2008: Galaxy Zoo: morphologies derived from visual inspection of galaxies from the Sloan Digital Sky Sur- vey. Mon. Not. R. Astron. Soc. 389, 1179—1189.
  • 38. Loos, A. & Ernst, A. 2013: An automated chimpanzee identification system using face detection and recogni- tion. EURASIP J. Image Video Process 49, 1—17. Lord, K. 2013: A comparison of the sensory development of wolves (Canis lupus lupus) and dogs (Canis lupus familiaris). Ethology 119, 110—120. Macdonald, C. & Whiten, A. 2011: The ‘Living Links to Human Evolution’ Research Centre in Edinburgh Zoo: a new endeavour in collaboration. Int. Zoo Yearb. 45, 7—17. Martin, P. & Bateson, P. 2007: Measuring Behaviour: An Introductory Guide. Cambridge University Press, Cam- bridge. McGreevy, P., Grassi, T. D. & Harman, A. M. 2003: A strong correlation exists between the distribution of ret- inal ganglion cells and nose length in the dog. Brain Behav. Evol. 63, 13—22. Morisette, J. T., Richardson, A. D., Knapp, A. K., Fisher, J.
  • 39. I., Graham, E. A., Abatzoglou, J., Wilson, B. E., Bres- hears, D. D., Henebry, G. M., Hanes, J. M. & Liang, L. 2009: Tracking the rhythm of the seasons in the face of global change: phenological research in the 21st cen- tury. Front. Ecol. Environ. 7, 253—260. Moriyama, T. & Gunji, Y. 1997: Autonomous learning in maze solution by Octopus. Ethology 103, 499—513. Morris, P. H., Doe, C. & Godsell, E. 2008: Secondary emotions in non-primate species? Behavioural reports and subjective claims by animal owners. Cogn. Emot. 22, 3—20. Nelson, X. J. & Fijn, N. 2013: The use of visual media as a tool for investigating animal behaviour. Anim. Behav. 85, 525—536. Newman, G., Graham, J., Crall, A. & Laituri, M. 2011: The art and science of multi-scale citizen science support. Ecol. Inform. 6, 217—227. Parr, C. S., Jones, T. & Songer, N. B. 2004: Evaluation of a
  • 40. handheld data collection interface for science learning. J. Sci. Educ. Technol. 13, 233—242. Reynolds, P. C. & Braithwaite, D. 2001: Towards a concep- tual framework for wildlife tourism. Tour. Manage. 22, 31—42. Serpell, J. 1996: In the Company of Animals: A Study of Human-Animal Relationships. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge. Shirk, J. L., Ballard, H. L., Wilderman, C. C., Phillips, T., Wiggins, A., Jordan, R., McCallie, E., Minarchek, M., Lewenstein, B. V., Krasny, M. E. & Bonney, R. 2012: Public participation in scientific research: a framework for deliberate design. Ecol. Soc. 17, 29. Silvertown, J. 2009: A new dawn for citizen science. Trends Ecol. Evol. 24, 467—471. Ethology 120 (2014) 207–214 © 2013 Blackwell Verlag GmbH 213 J. Hecht & C. B. Cooper Public Engagement in Ethology
  • 41. Slabbekoorn, H. & den Boer-Visser, A. 2006: Cities change the songs of birds. Curr. Biol. 16, 2326—2331. Slabbekoorn, H. & Peet, M. 2003: Ecology: birds sing at a higher pitch in urban noise. Nature 424, 267. Sn€all, T., Kindvall, O., Nilsson, J. & P€art, T. 2011: Evaluat- ing citizen-based presence data for bird monitoring. Biol. Cons. 144, 804—810. Stamps, J. & Groothuis, T. G. 2010: The development of animal personality: relevance, concepts and per- spectives. Biol. Rev. Camb. Philos. Soc. 85, 301— 325. Tinbergen, N. 1963: On aims and methods of ethology. Zeitschrift f€ur Tierpsychologie 20, 410—433. Uetz, G. W., Hieber, C. S., Jakob, E. M., Wilcox, R. S., Kroeger, D., McCrate, A. & Mostrom, A. M. 1994: Behavior of colonial orb-weaving spiders during a solar eclipse. Ethology 96, 24—32.
  • 42. USFWS. 2011: U.S. Fish & Wildlife Service 2011 National Survey of Fishing, Hunting, and Wildlife-Associated Recreation. http://www.doi.gov/news/pressreleases/ upload/FWS-National-Preliminary-Report-2011.pdf. Voss, M. A. & Cooper, C. B. 2010: Using a free online citizen-science project to teach observation & quantifi- cation of animal behavior. Am. Biol. Teach. 72, 437— 443. Vrontou, S., Wong, A. M., Rau, K. K., Koerber, H. R. & Anderson, D. J. 2013: Genetic identification of C fibres that detect massage-like stroking of hairy skin in vivo. Nature 493, 669—673. Wan, M., Hejjas, K., Ronai, Z., Elek, Z., Sasvari-Szekely, M., Champagne, F. A., Mikl�osi, �A. & Kubinyi, E. 2013: DRD4 and TH gene polymorphisms are associated with activity, impulsivity and inattention in Siberian Husky dogs. Anim. Genet. 44, 717—727. Wiggins, A., Newman, G., Stevenson, R. D. & Crowston,
  • 43. K. 2011: Mechanisms for data quality and validation in citizen science. eScience Workshops, 14—19. Williams, R. L., Porter, S. K., Hart, A. G. & Goodenough, A. E. 2012: The accuracy of behavioural data collected by visitors in a zoo environment: can visitors collect meaningful data? Int. J. Zool., Article ID 724835, doi:10.1155/2012/724835. Ethology 120 (2014) 207–214 © 2013 Blackwell Verlag GmbH214 Public Engagement in Ethology J. Hecht & C. B. Cooper CURRENT ISSUES - PERSPECTIVES AND REVIEWS Tribute to Tinbergen: Public Engagement in Ethology Julie Hecht* & Caren B. Cooper† * Doctoral Program in Psychology, The Graduate Center, City University of New York, New York, NY, USA † Cornell Lab of Ornithology, Ithaca, NY, USA (Invited Review) Correspondence
  • 44. Julie Hecht, Department of Psychology, Hunter College and The Graduate Center, City University of New York, 695 Park Avenue, New York, NY 10065, USA. E-mail: [email protected] Received: October 4, 2013 Initial acceptance: October 19, 2013 Final acceptance: November 25, 2013 (M. Hauber) doi: 10.1111/eth.12199 Keywords: citizen science, data quality, animal behavior, informal science education Abstract Public engagement in research, called citizen science, has led to advances in a range of fields like astronomy, ornithology, and public health. While volunteers have been making and sharing observations according to pro- tocols set by researchers in numerous disciplines, citizen science practices
  • 45. are less common in the field of animal behavior. We consider how citizen science might be used to address animal behavior questions at Tinbergen’s four levels of analysis. We briefly review resources and methods for addressing technical issues surrounding volunteer participation—such as data quality—so that citizen science can make long-standing contributions to the field of animal behavior. Introduction ‘Citizen science’ describes the various ways that mem- bers of the public participate in genuine scientific research (Cooper et al. 2007a; Silvertown 2009; Shirk et al. 2012). Many citizen science projects arise from communities with specific concerns (such as long- term environmental monitoring), and some are initiated by scientists to address specific research objectives. Other projects have additional goals to increase science literacy (Bonney et al. 2009a).
  • 46. Research via public engagement is possible because people enjoy making natural history observations and sharing these observations with professionals and peers. In addition, Web 2.0 and mobile technologies facilitate mass participation. Consequently, citizen science projects have arisen in a myriad of disciplines, including ecology, phenology, macroecology, public health, natural resource management, hydrology, urban planning, meteorology, math, volcanology, and various taxon-specific fields such as entomology, ornithology, and herpetology. In recent years, citizen science has enabled substantial contributions to the fields of astronomy (Lintott et al. 2008), medicine (Khatib et al. 2011a,b), and climate change (Morisette et al. 2009). Currently, the field of animal behavior is under- represented among citizen science projects. Animal behavior research integrates diverse methodological
  • 47. approaches to investigate a wide array of scientific questions about the behavior of wild and domesti- cated animals in natural and captive settings. Public observations of occurrence data (e.g., wildlife sight- ings; Sn€all et al. 2011), count data (e.g., number of birds; Cooper et al. 2007b), simple measurements (e.g., rainfall; Cifelli et al. 2005), and even phenologi- cal stages (e.g., budburst; Morisette et al. 2009) are more common applications of citizen science, in part because such data likely require simpler protocols than most animal behavior data. The dearth of citizen science research in animal behavior is not necessarily due to lack of general pub- lic interest. Pet-keeping is a global phenomenon (Serpell 1996); wildlife tourism and wildlife docu- mentaries are popular (Reynolds & Braithwaite 2001; Nelson & Fijn 2013); zoo attendance is on the rise in parts of the world (Davey 2007); and people are
  • 48. already expressing interest in animal behavior citizen Ethology 120 (2014) 207–214 © 2013 Blackwell Verlag GmbH 207 Ethology ethology international journal of behavioural biology science projects (Foster et al. 2011; Williams et al. 2012). In the USA, one in four adults watches wildlife (USFWS 2011), and the numerous websites that stream live video of pets (e.g., SPCA cams) and wild- life (e.g., nesting birds) are extremely popular (e.g., the Cornell Lab of Ornithology nest cams receive millions of unique viewers from 175 countries, including up to 9,000 simultaneous views of live- streaming video). Citizen science contributions could enhance animal behavior investigations by increasing observations on time and geographic scales (e.g., latitudinal gradients,
  • 49. urbanization gradients, comparing populations, etc), and these projects also have the potential to process massive amounts of data (e.g., video or photo tag- ging). In essence, citizen scientists provide an extra pair of hands—or extra thousands of pairs. While a small team of researchers documented the behavior of the colonial orb-weaving spider during a solar eclipse in Veracruz, Mexico (Uetz et al. 1994), the team might have used citizen scientists to capture behav- ioral data on additional populations or species during the short-lived event. Without the help of citizen sci- entists, researchers are effectively ‘missing’ variation among animals. Field research, behavior observation, and behavior coding can be time-consuming, challenging to coordi- nate, and expensive. Trained volunteers might offer lower-cost options for collecting and/or scoring behavior (Williams et al. 2012). Depending on the study design and complexity, volunteers can be used
  • 50. to ensure blind coding. Citizen science methods could facilitate access to a wide variety of species in different contexts and foster integration of studies of natural populations and pop- ulations under artificial selection. Additionally, an expanded research platform offers the possibility of capturing animals in novel contexts. Video and social media, particularly YouTube, are rife with examples of uncommon (Burn 2011) or previously unexplored behaviors, such as interspecies play or object play (Nelson & Fijn 2013). Behaviors captured in real- world contexts can lead to new research directions, and citizen scientists could provide behavioral video footage. We examine the realized and potential application of citizen science to research animal behavior—the inquiry about what animals do, how they do it, and why they do it. Tinbergen (1963) framed the disci-
  • 51. pline as encompassing four levels of analysis: causa- tion, development, function, and phylogeny. We use Tinbergen’s levels of analysis as a frame- work to discuss a possible partnership between citizen science and animal behavior research. We note that citizen scientists could examine a particular behavior, for example bird song, to generate data for answering questions at all four levels (e.g., causation: ‘What day length triggers bird song?’; development: ‘Do offspring imitate songs of parents?’; function: ‘Do females prefer certain song types?’; and phylogeny: ‘Which species have similar songs?’). Professional scientists can obtain a more robust understanding of an observed behavior through investigations of non- competing hypotheses at multiple levels. Other questions might be best suited for investigation at one particular level of analysis. We discuss a variety of applications below, with the common theme that citi-
  • 52. zen science participants can scale-up and advance research that individual scientists could not do alone. Additionally, we review the technical facets of a union between animal behavior and citizen science, giving attention to crucial details in training volun- teers and handling volunteer data. Citizen Science at Four Levels of Analysis In the 1963 paper, On aims and methods of Ethology, Tinbergen proposed an integrated approach to the study of ethology to address the lack of a unified public image of ethology and to resolve conceptual differences between researchers. Since then, “how questions,” exploring the immediate causes of behav- ior, and “why questions,” exploring the evolutionary forces behind behavior, have been pursued as complementary approaches. Causation The causal basis of behavior focuses on stimuli and
  • 53. mechanisms that trigger, cue, or precede a behavior. For early ethologists, physiological and neural mecha- nisms occurred in a ‘black box’ upon which they could only speculate. Today, inquiry into causation draws from genetics, endocrinology, physiology, and neurobiology. Direct participation and input from citizen scientists is already underway in neurobiology (Eyewire, https://eyewire.org/signup) and genetics (Phylo) (Coo- per et al. 2013). For domesticated and wild populations, the collection of saliva and fecal samples could contrib- ute to studies of stress physiology and measurements of well-being to ultimately address a wide variety of behav- ioral endocrinological questions. Participant involvement could complement labo- ratory findings by collecting behavioral data on pre-screened or pre-selected populations. For exam- ple, volunteers might assist research into relationships Ethology 120 (2014) 207–214 © 2013 Blackwell Verlag GmbH208
  • 54. Public Engagement in Ethology J. Hecht & C. B. Cooper between genetic markers and behavior by collecting behavioral data on pre-selected animals (e.g., through a web portal for annotating video) whose genes have been mapped. Researchers recently determined that particular allelic qualities of DRD4 and TH gene poly- morphisms were associated with activity–impulsivity in German Shepherds and Siberian Huskies, with behavioral analysis stemming from approx. 250 dogs (Kubinyi et al. 2012; Wan et al. 2013). Continued exploration of gene variants in dogs could be coupled with citizen science projects collecting behavioral data on dogs with known genetic composition to generate a more comprehensive understanding of behavioral traits. Citizen science allows researchers to expand the size of their data set without requiring the participants to have any knowledge of the DNA sampling and gen-
  • 55. otyping that go into pre-screening decisions. The addition of a citizen science component can increase the rigor of the research in at least two ways. First, the volunteer contributions could ensure a dou- ble-blind experiment. Second, the volunteers provide observations from multiple observers, and researchers can use a consensus tool to score behavior, analogous to consensus tools (replication by multiple partici- pants) common to online crowdsourcing efforts for other research (Wiggins et al. 2011). Additionally, volunteers can install cameras to record animal behavior and facilitate remote coding by professional researchers. For example, scientists used footage from volunteer nest cameras to examine proximate control of egg-laying behaviors in nestbox-dwelling bird species (Cooper et al. 2009). Development Approaches to animal behavior from a developmental
  • 56. perspective are also mechanistic, but with a focus on changes in an organism’s capacity for expressing or acquiring behaviors at different stages over the course of a lifetime. Citizen science offers a unique opportunity to inves- tigate ontogenetic questions in a wide range of species. For example, citizen scientists might assist in the study of social behavior and document the devel- opment of particular behavioral patterns—such as fac- tors affecting the onset and development of play in various species—or personality changes (Stamps & Groothuis 2010). Volunteers could collect and report data on single organisms—whether subjects be a com- panion animal, a backyard nestbox or an annually returning species—and generate substantial data points for each individual. Ultimately, with many volunteers, the increased sample sizes could make full-scale statistical analysis possible (Dickinson et al. 2010). Citizen scientists could also examine the develop-
  • 57. ment of animal sensorium, such as in relation to mor- phological traits. For example, assumptions about the different sensory abilities in dog breeds abound, and such differences might be expected given physiologi- cal differences (McGreevy et al. 2003), but actual behavioral and development data are wanting. Researchers could design and implement a project where participants conduct simple, in-home tests examining behavior responses toward different stim- uli at different ages, thereby offering additional data on breed (and even species) sensory development. These behavioral measures of development are com- parable to traditional laboratory experiments (Lord 2013). But, with the help of citizen scientists, researchers can obtain data on a much larger scale. Zoo visitors and wildlife tourists can play a role in monitoring individuals’ development. Zoo visitors express interest in animal behavior studies (Bowler
  • 58. et al. 2012) and could collect many more hours (or years) of observations than possible by resource- limited staff with many other care-taking duties. Such efforts might elucidate the onset or development of behaviors such as stereotypic motor patterns as well as factors affecting the development of these behaviors. Zoo visitors, however, can have bias toward collecting observations of active animals (Altman 1998; Williams et al. 2012). Function The analysis of function focuses on the pressures that shape behaviors. The pervasive influence of the built environment means that many organisms reside in environmental conditions that differ in novel ways from the conditions under which traits evolved. For example, the anthropogenic sound levels near high- ways and flight paths create novel selection pressures, and some animals might be able to adjust their behav-
  • 59. iors within a range of plasticity, such as birds singing at higher pitches near roads (Slabbekoorn & Peet 2003; Slabbekoorn & den Boer-Visser 2006). Other traits might exhibit limited phenotypic plasticity, and disturbances may result in population decline. Fur- thermore, both domesticated and non-domesticated animals residing in homes or zoos will demonstrate behaviors that might no longer be adaptive. Research- ers can design participatory projects in which volun- teers collect data on species in different environments and investigate different hypotheses behind complex behaviors. In an online project called CamClickr, par- Ethology 120 (2014) 207–214 © 2013 Blackwell Verlag GmbH 209 J. Hecht & C. B. Cooper Public Engagement in Ethology ticipants added behavior-category tags to images archived from nest cameras to help investigate varia- tions in nest attendance (Voss & Cooper 2010).
  • 60. Because funding cycles offered by granting institu- tions tend to span three- to five-year periods, collect- ing long-term data sets can be challenging for research scientists. The collection of such data sets could be made possible through citizen science activi- ties. Depending on the initiative, initial investment to set up the project, and maintenance costs, could be low with free online tools for managing projects (e.g., www.citsci.org), recruiting volunteers (e.g., www. scistarter.com), and data archiving (e.g., www.data- one.org). Citizen scientists could measure different components of fitness such as survival to maturity, longevity, mating success, and reproductive success. Phylogeny Behaviors can also be understood from a phylogenetic perspective. By considering ancestry, researchers can identify traits that are highly conserved and those that are more recently evolved within the same lineage.
  • 61. Because closely related species are found in everyday environments, a citizen science project could collect a wide variety of behavioral data on closely related spe- cies in different environments that have adopted dif- ferent survival strategies. By comparing closely related species occurring on different continents (e.g., black-billed bagpie Pica hudsonia in America and the European magpie Pica pica in Eurasia), citizen scien- tists can provide simultaneous data sets without the confound of temporal displacement typical of smaller- scale research projects. Not all citizen engagement in animal behavior pro- jects fits neatly into Tinbergen’s levels of analysis. Recently, Project: Play With Your Dog (Horowitz Dog Cognition Lab, Barnard College) requested short vid- eos depicting play between a dog and a person, with the aim of capturing the wide array of behaviors and conditions that encompass interspecific play. Video
  • 62. submissions drew from around the world, and although participants did not contribute directly to behavioral analysis, citizen scientists had to think about behavior in terms of how they and their dog play together and provide behavioral descriptors in an accompanying questionnaire. Similarly, an online survey by Animal Welfare Indi- cators asked participants to assist in developing a con- tinuous behavioral scale of lameness for goats. Participants watched nine videos and scored abnormal gait, head-nodding, and spine curvature on a visual analogue scale. Before participants began, each behavior had a clear description, and participants were schooled in measuring behavior as per the speci- fications of the project. Merging Ethology and Citizen Science Methods Creating Successful Citizen Science Animal Behavior Projects
  • 63. Like all successful research endeavors, the design of citizen science projects follows the steps of the scien- tific method; the only difference is the additional layer of complexity introduced by the involvement of the lay public. Projects start with the formulation of scien- tific questions, organization of a team of skilled experts in relevant areas, and determination of appro- priate data collection (Bonney et al. 2009b). Depend- ing on the legal status and various protections offered to different species, researchers might need to present protocols to Institutional Animal Care and Use Committees (Bayne 1998). Citizen scientists can learn behavior-coding techniques—or relevant project- related skills—in online videos or in person. The research team can control quality before the project begins by testing each prospective participant’s accuracy and ability to collect meaningful data. Behavioral data accuracy is a key concern, and pro- jects incorporating citizen science data should con-
  • 64. sider topics like observation quality, sampling bias, and interobserver reliability (Dickinson et al. 2010; Burghardt et al. 2012; Hart et al. 2012). Williams et al. (2012) found that although zoo visitor interest in the project was high, the activity budget data col- lected on a group of captive otters were not accurate, mostly because visitors did not adhere to the observa- tion period; the researchers suggest that certain sampling methods might be easier—or harder—for observers. Meanwhile, there are numerous ways to improve data quality in citizen science (Cohn 2008; Silvertown 2009; Wiggins et al. 2011; Bonter & Cooper 2012). Technologies like smartphones, multimedia, or inter- active video could assist in time-keeping and behav- ioral data collection (Parr et al. 2004; Aanensen et al. 2009). Data management tools, often through cyber- infrastructure, improve efficiency (Newman et al.
  • 65. 2011). Additionally, some citizen science projects demonstrate that certain types of people collect more accurate data than others (Delaney et al. 2008), and similar lessons can make their way into citizen science projects in animal behavior. Finally, researchers can be creative in where and how volunteers access animals and how behavior will Ethology 120 (2014) 207–214 © 2013 Blackwell Verlag GmbH210 Public Engagement in Ethology J. Hecht & C. B. Cooper be observed or documented. In some cases, behaviors are not observed directly but are inferred. Phenology research addresses the timing of life-cycle events, including behaviors such as egg laying and migration in birds, insects, and some reptiles and amphibians (Head et al. 2013). Limited access to animals in situa- tions where volunteers can obtain unbiased observa- tions of life-cycle events may have been one of the
  • 66. largest impediments to citizen science in animal behavior. Now, Internet and remote-camera techno- logies combined with crowdsourcing infrastructure offer new opportunities to collect data on animals that are difficult to access (Deng et al. 2012; Head et al. 2013; Loos & Ernst 2013). Live-streaming cameras can be set up at areas with known activity—animal nesting or gathering sites—so that people can docu- ment behaviors at a distance. In addition to remote/ web-mediated studies, possibilities abound for studies of companion animals (e.g., dogs, cats, rabbits, birds, ferrets, fish, gerbils, and hamsters), zoo animals, ani- mals viewed during nature tourism, and urban wild- life such as squirrels, pigeons, rats, foxes, and coyotes. Volunteers could document the behavior of animals at zoos or wildlife tourism destinations, especially ani- mals that handlers and keepers do not have time to record. Special interest groups—such as the House Rabbit Society, backyard chicken groups, and pet
  • 67. breeders—offer significant potential for citizen science projects. Training and Resources for Animal Behavior Inquiry Skill in observing and measuring animal behavior does not necessarily follow from an appreciation of or even cohabitation with animals. Members of the pub- lic are generally interested in animals yet have a pro- clivity not toward Tinbergen’s four levels of behavior, but often toward anthropomorphic explanations of behavior. While the study of animal behavior is an attempt to investigate ultimate and proximate questions about behavior, humans often characterize animal behavior through an anthropocentric lens (Foster et al. 2011). People readily ascribe human characteristics to non- humans. Subjective assessments of non-human behavior can be consistent across observers, often reifying existing beliefs regardless of whether they have been substantiated (Bahlig-Pieren & Turner
  • 68. 1999; Morris et al. 2008; Horowitz 2011). Seminal work by Heider & Simmel (1944) found that people consistently attribute human emotions, aims, and objectives to inanimate objects moving in a ‘human- like’ manner. Companion animal species, mammals, and non-human primates are more readily anthropo- morphized than other species (Eddy et al. 1993), even though seemingly ‘alien’ species, like cephalopods, display complex behaviors and cognitive processes (Fiorito & Scotto 1992; Moriyama & Gunji 1997). Importantly, the appearance of behavioral homologies should not imply similar meaning, such as the ‘smil- ing’ chimpanzee or dolphin (Horowitz 2007). Tendencies toward anthropomorphism are perhaps a useful ‘hook’ when engaging the general public in animal behavior projects. Recently, an online video titled ‘Lol cats like stroking too’ (http://www. youtube.com/watch?v=OFCRvjle2o8) accompanied a
  • 69. publication on the neurons activated by massage-like stroking to assist in public understanding of the research (Vrontou et al. 2013). The video received over 200,000 views in the first year. Clearly defined project expectations and training materials can minimize the effects of anthropomor- phic inferences (Voss & Cooper 2010). Scientists can provide a detailed ethogram, an explicit coding scheme, and a clear behavior-sampling technique (Martin & Bateson 2007). Behaviors in an ethogram can be distinguished based on structural or empirical characteristics, such as ‘raised lips at edges’, or conse- quence-based descriptors, such as ‘threat’. Empirical descriptors help create unambiguous behavioral parameters (Lehner 1987) but might offer challenges for unpracticed observers. Volunteers need a clear understanding of the terms in which they are expected to view and document behavior and have
  • 70. the technology, such as a timer, to code appropriately. Researchers determine who is being watched and when and how to watch them. Some coding schemes provide information on how much time an animal spends performing various behaviors, while other schemes monitor behavior frequencies (Altmann 1974). Researchers must match the skill level of observers with the scientific question. The role of vol- unteers in the study of animal behavior will largely involve the production of meaningful behavioral data. The challenge will be to offer effective guidance and instruction to avoid anthropocentric assessments. A number of institutions have created behavior- observation instructional platforms. Ethosearch (www. ethosearch.org), created by the Lincoln Park Zoo, is a resource for researchers and students to store and view ethograms and practice measuring animal behavior. Living Links (www.living-links.org)—a joint enclosure
  • 71. for squirrel monkeys (Saimiri sciureus) and capuchins (Sapajus apella) at the Edinburgh Zoo—offers a public-learning resource to observe and score the behav- ior of both species (Macdonald & Whiten 2011). Cam- Ethology 120 (2014) 207–214 © 2013 Blackwell Verlag GmbH 211 J. Hecht & C. B. Cooper Public Engagement in Ethology Clickr was an online citizen science project to code the behaviors of nesting birds from still images as a way of carrying out scan sampling. Voss & Cooper (2010) cre- ated a college-level laboratory unit in which students can use CamClickr to create ethograms while learning about observer bias, observation techniques, and defin- ing and quantifying observations. Conclusion Animal behavior researchers might consider whether and how citizen science could support and amplify their studies. Volunteers can expand the scope and
  • 72. scale of animal behavior studies by bringing attention to novel or rare behaviors or providing access to additional variation in animal behaviors. Volunteers, too, can benefit from personal experiences engaging in hands-on scientific interactions with animals. Undoubtedly, challenges arise with the involve- ment of citizen science in animal behavior, yet the possibilities are great. Altmann’s (1974, p. 229) text on behavior sampling methods stresses that there should not be a disconnect between the laboratory and the real world: ‘Such a restriction on research would mean that the behavioral sciences would for- ever forsake any hope of knowing whether their most powerful theories have any relevance in the world of behavior outside the laboratory.’ Citizen scientists offer an additional, and untapped, opportunity for ethological exploration. Acknowledgements
  • 73. Thanks to Felicity Muth, Mark Hauber, and Andrew Fulmer for conceptual conversations. Many thanks to reviewers for comments, which improved the manu- script. Literature cited Aanensen, D. M., Huntley, D. M., Feil, E. J., al-Own, F. & Spratt, B. G. 2009: EpiCollect: linking smartphones to web applications for epidemiology, ecology and community data collection. PLoS One 4, e6968. Altman, J. D. 1998: Animal activity and visitor learning at the zoo. Anthrozoos 11, 12—21. Altmann, J. 1974: Observational study of behavior: sampling methods. Behaviour 49, 227—267. Bahlig-Pieren, Z. & Turner, D. C. 1999: Anthropomorphic interpretations and ethological descriptions of dog and cat behavior by lay people. Anthrozoos 12, 205—210. Bayne, K. 1998: Developing guidelines on the care and use of animals. Ann. N.Y. Acad. Sci. 862, 105—110.
  • 74. Bonney, R., Ballard, H., Jordan, R., McCallie, E., Phillips, T., Shirk, J. & Wilderman, C. C. 2009a: Public participa- tion in scientific research: defining the field and assess- ing its potential for informal science education. A CAISE Inquiry Group Report. Center for Advancement of Informal Science Education (CAISE), Washington, D.C. Bonney, R., Cooper, C. B., Dickinson, J., Kelling, S., Phillips, T., Rosenberg, K. V. & Shirk, J. 2009b: Citizen science: a developing tool for expanding science knowl- edge and scientific literacy. Bioscience 59, 977—984. Bonter, D. N. & Cooper, C. B. 2012: Data validation in citizen science: a case study from Project FeederWatch. Front. Ecol. Environ. 10, 305—307. Bowler, M. T., Buchanan-Smith, H. M. & Whiten, A. 2012: Assessing public engagement with science in a university primate research centre in a national zoo. PLoS One 7, e34505. Burghardt, G. M., Bartmess-LeVasseur, J. N., Browning, S.
  • 75. A., Morrison, K. E., Stec, C. L., Zachau, C. E. & Free- berg, T. M. 2012: Perspectives—Minimizing observer bias in behavioral studies: a review and recommenda- tions. Ethology 118, 511—517. Burn, C. C. 2011: A vicious cycle: A cross-sectional study of canine tail-chasing and human responses to it, using a free video-sharing website. PLoS One 6, e26553. Cifelli, R., Doesken, N., Kennedy, P., Carey, L. D., Rutledge, S. A., Gimmestad, C. & Depue, T. 2005: The community collaborative rain, hail, and snow network: informal education for scientists and citizens. Bull. Amer. Meteor. Soc. 86, 1069—1077. Cohn, J. P. 2008: Citizen science: can volunteers do real research? Bioscience 58, 192—197. Cooper, C. B., Dickinson, J., Phillips, T. & Bonney, R. 2007a: Citizen science as a tool for conservation in residential ecosystems. Ecol. Soc. 12, 11. Cooper, C. B., Hochachka, W. M. & Dhondt, A. A. 2007b:
  • 76. Contrasting natural experiments confirm competition between house finches and house sparrows. Ecology 88, 864—870. Cooper, C. B., Voss, M. A. & Zivkovic, B. 2009: Extended laying interval of ultimate eggs of the Eastern Bluebird. Condor 11, 752—755. Cooper, S., Khatib, F. & Baker, D. 2013: Increasing public involvement in structural biology. Structure 21, 1482— 1484. Davey, G. 2007: An analysis of country, socio-economic and time factors on worldwide zoo attendance during a 40 year period. Int. Zoo Yearb. 41, 217—225. Delaney, D. G., Sperling, C. D., Adams, C. S. & Leung, B. 2008: Marine invasive species: validation of citizen science and implications for national monitoring networks. Biol. Invasions 10, 117—128. Deng, D., Chuang, T., Shao, K., Mai, G., Lin, T., Lemmens, R., Hsu, C., Lin, H. & Kraak, M. 2012: Using social media
  • 77. for collaborative species identification and occurrence: Ethology 120 (2014) 207–214 © 2013 Blackwell Verlag GmbH212 Public Engagement in Ethology J. Hecht & C. B. Cooper issues, methods, and tools. SIGSPATIAL 2012 Interna- tional Conference on Advances in Geographic Informa- tion Systems, ACM, 22—29. Dickinson, J. L., Zuckerberg, B. & Bonter, D. N. 2010: Citizen science as an ecological research tool: challenges and benefits. Annu. Rev. Ecol. Evol. Syst. 41, 149—172. Eddy, T. J., Gallup, G. G. Jr & Povinelli, D. J. 1993: Attribution of cognitive states to animals: anthropomorphism in comparative perspective. J. Soc. Issues 49, 87—101. Fiorito, G. & Scotto, P. 1992: Observational learning in Octopus vulgaris. Science 256, 545—547. Foster, D., Kirman, B., Linehan, C., Lawson, S., Mills, D., Ellis, S. & Zulch, H. 2011: “I can haz emoshuns?”:
  • 78. understanding anthropomorphosis of cats among internet users. 3rd International Conference on Social Computing, 9-11 October 2011, MIT Boston. Hart, A., Stafford, R., Goodenough, A. & Morgan, S. 2012: The role of citizen science and volunteer data collection in zoological research. Int. J. Zool., Article ID 105345, doi:10.1155/2012/105345. Head, J. S., Boesch, C., Robbins, M. M., Rabanal, L. I., Makaga, L. & K€uhl, H. S. 2013: Effective sociodemo- graphic population assessment of elusive species in ecology and conservation management. Ecol. Evol. 3, 2903—2916. Heider, F. & Simmel, M. 1944: An experimental study of apparent behavior. Am. J. Psychol. 57, 243—259. Horowitz, A. 2007: Anthropomorphism. In. M. Encyclopedia of Human-Animal Relationships (Bekoff, M. ed). Greenwood Publishing Group, Westport, CT. pp. 60—66.
  • 79. Horowitz, A. 2011: Empirically testing anthropomor- phisms of the domestic dog. J. Vet. Behav. 6, 75. Khatib, F., Cooper, S., Tyka, M. D., Xu, K., Makedon, I., Popovi�c, Z., Baker, D. & Foldit Players. 2011a: Algo- rithm discovery by protein folding game players. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 108, 18949—18953. Khatib, F., DiMaio, F., Cooper, S., Kazmierczyk, M., Gilski, M., Krzywda, S., Z�abransk�a, H., Pichov�a, I., Thompson, J., Popovi�c, Z., Jaskolski, M. & Baker, D. Foldit Contend- ers Group, Foldit Void Crushers Group 2011b: Crystal structure of a monomeric retroviral protease solved by protein folding game players. Nat. Struct. Mol. Biol. 18, 1175—1177. Kubinyi, E., Vas, J., Hejjas, K., Ronai, Z., Br�uder, I., Turcs�an, B., Sasvari-Szekely, M. & Mikl�osi, �A. 2012: Polymorphism in the tyrosine hydroxylase (TH) gene is associated with activity-impulsivity in German Shepherd dogs. PLoS One 7, e30271.
  • 80. Lehner, P. N. 1987: Design and execution of animal behavior research: an overview. J. Anim. Sci. 65, 1213 —1219. Lintott, C. J., Schawinski, K., Slosar, A., Land, K., Bam- ford, S., Thomas, D., Raddick, M. J., Nichol, R. C., Szalay, A., Andreescu, D., Murray, P. & Vandenberg, J. 2008: Galaxy Zoo: morphologies derived from visual inspection of galaxies from the Sloan Digital Sky Sur- vey. Mon. Not. R. Astron. Soc. 389, 1179—1189. Loos, A. & Ernst, A. 2013: An automated chimpanzee identification system using face detection and recogni- tion. EURASIP J. Image Video Process 49, 1—17. Lord, K. 2013: A comparison of the sensory development of wolves (Canis lupus lupus) and dogs (Canis lupus familiaris). Ethology 119, 110—120. Macdonald, C. & Whiten, A. 2011: The ‘Living Links to Human Evolution’ Research Centre in Edinburgh Zoo: a new endeavour in collaboration. Int. Zoo Yearb. 45,
  • 81. 7—17. Martin, P. & Bateson, P. 2007: Measuring Behaviour: An Introductory Guide. Cambridge University Press, Cam- bridge. McGreevy, P., Grassi, T. D. & Harman, A. M. 2003: A strong correlation exists between the distribution of ret- inal ganglion cells and nose length in the dog. Brain Behav. Evol. 63, 13—22. Morisette, J. T., Richardson, A. D., Knapp, A. K., Fisher, J. I., Graham, E. A., Abatzoglou, J., Wilson, B. E., Bres- hears, D. D., Henebry, G. M., Hanes, J. M. & Liang, L. 2009: Tracking the rhythm of the seasons in the face of global change: phenological research in the 21st cen- tury. Front. Ecol. Environ. 7, 253—260. Moriyama, T. & Gunji, Y. 1997: Autonomous learning in maze solution by Octopus. Ethology 103, 499—513. Morris, P. H., Doe, C. & Godsell, E. 2008: Secondary emotions in non-primate species? Behavioural reports
  • 82. and subjective claims by animal owners. Cogn. Emot. 22, 3—20. Nelson, X. J. & Fijn, N. 2013: The use of visual media as a tool for investigating animal behaviour. Anim. Behav. 85, 525—536. Newman, G., Graham, J., Crall, A. & Laituri, M. 2011: The art and science of multi-scale citizen science support. Ecol. Inform. 6, 217—227. Parr, C. S., Jones, T. & Songer, N. B. 2004: Evaluation of a handheld data collection interface for science learning. J. Sci. Educ. Technol. 13, 233—242. Reynolds, P. C. & Braithwaite, D. 2001: Towards a concep- tual framework for wildlife tourism. Tour. Manage. 22, 31—42. Serpell, J. 1996: In the Company of Animals: A Study of Human-Animal Relationships. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge. Shirk, J. L., Ballard, H. L., Wilderman, C. C., Phillips, T.,
  • 83. Wiggins, A., Jordan, R., McCallie, E., Minarchek, M., Lewenstein, B. V., Krasny, M. E. & Bonney, R. 2012: Public participation in scientific research: a framework for deliberate design. Ecol. Soc. 17, 29. Silvertown, J. 2009: A new dawn for citizen science. Trends Ecol. Evol. 24, 467—471. Ethology 120 (2014) 207–214 © 2013 Blackwell Verlag GmbH 213 J. Hecht & C. B. Cooper Public Engagement in Ethology Slabbekoorn, H. & den Boer-Visser, A. 2006: Cities change the songs of birds. Curr. Biol. 16, 2326—2331. Slabbekoorn, H. & Peet, M. 2003: Ecology: birds sing at a higher pitch in urban noise. Nature 424, 267. Sn€all, T., Kindvall, O., Nilsson, J. & P€art, T. 2011: Evaluat- ing citizen-based presence data for bird monitoring. Biol. Cons. 144, 804—810. Stamps, J. & Groothuis, T. G. 2010: The development
  • 84. of animal personality: relevance, concepts and per- spectives. Biol. Rev. Camb. Philos. Soc. 85, 301— 325. Tinbergen, N. 1963: On aims and methods of ethology. Zeitschrift f€ur Tierpsychologie 20, 410—433. Uetz, G. W., Hieber, C. S., Jakob, E. M., Wilcox, R. S., Kroeger, D., McCrate, A. & Mostrom, A. M. 1994: Behavior of colonial orb-weaving spiders during a solar eclipse. Ethology 96, 24—32. USFWS. 2011: U.S. Fish & Wildlife Service 2011 National Survey of Fishing, Hunting, and Wildlife-Associated Recreation. http://www.doi.gov/news/pressreleases/ upload/FWS-National-Preliminary-Report-2011.pdf. Voss, M. A. & Cooper, C. B. 2010: Using a free online citizen-science project to teach observation & quantifi- cation of animal behavior. Am. Biol. Teach. 72, 437— 443. Vrontou, S., Wong, A. M., Rau, K. K., Koerber, H. R. &
  • 85. Anderson, D. J. 2013: Genetic identification of C fibres that detect massage-like stroking of hairy skin in vivo. Nature 493, 669—673. Wan, M., Hejjas, K., Ronai, Z., Elek, Z., Sasvari-Szekely, M., Champagne, F. A., Mikl�osi, �A. & Kubinyi, E. 2013: DRD4 and TH gene polymorphisms are associated with activity, impulsivity and inattention in Siberian Husky dogs. Anim. Genet. 44, 717—727. Wiggins, A., Newman, G., Stevenson, R. D. & Crowston, K. 2011: Mechanisms for data quality and validation in citizen science. eScience Workshops, 14—19. Williams, R. L., Porter, S. K., Hart, A. G. & Goodenough, A. E. 2012: The accuracy of behavioural data collected by visitors in a zoo environment: can visitors collect meaningful data? Int. J. Zool., Article ID 724835, doi:10.1155/2012/724835. Ethology 120 (2014) 207–214 © 2013 Blackwell Verlag GmbH214 Public Engagement in Ethology J. Hecht & C. B. Cooper