SlideShare a Scribd company logo
1 of 49
Work Motivation and Performance: A Social
Identity Perspective
Daan van Knippenberg
University of Amsterdam, The Netherlands
La motivation au travail et les performances ont e teÂ
analyse es dans la per-
spective de la the orie de l'identite sociale et de celle de
l'auto-cate gorisation.
Centrale aÁ cette analyse se trouve la relation entre
l'identification organisa-
tionnelle et la motivation d'exercer un effort pour le compte de
la collectivite .
Une analyse the orique et une revue d' e tudes empiriques
relatives aux relations
entre cette identification organisationnelle et la motivation et
les performances
conduisent aÁ la conclusion: l'identification est lie e
positivement aÁ la motivation
au travail, avec les performances dans la re alisation des taÃ
ches et dans le
contexte, pour autant a) que l'identite sociale soit saillante et
que b) les
performances e leve es soient percË ues comme e tant dans
l'inte reà t du groupe ou
de l'organisation.
Work motivation and performance were analysed from the
perspective of
social identity theory and self-categorisation theory. Central in
this analysis
is the relation of organisational identification with the
motivation to exert
effort on behalf of the collective. A theoretical analysis as well
as a review
of empirical studies of the relationship of organisational
identification with
motivation and performance leads to the conclusion that
identification is
positively related to work motivation, task performance, and
contextual
performance to the extent that (a) social identity is salient, and
(b) high
performance is perceived to be in the group's or organisation's
interest.
INTRODUCTION
The study of work motivation and performance forms one of the
key issues
in research in organisational behaviour. Although a variety of
motivations
may affect performance, and performance may be contingent on
a multitude
of other factors, one of the more important factors affecting
performance
________________
* Address for correspondence: Daan van Knippenberg,
University of Amsterdam, Work &
Organizational Psychology, Roetersstraat 15, 1018 WB
Amsterdam, The Netherlands. Email:
[email protected]
I wish to thank Alexander Haslam, Moshe Krausz, Barbara van
Knippenberg, Robert
Wood, and an anonymous reviewer for their comments on
previous drafts of this paper.
APPLIED PSYCHOLOGY: AN INTERNATIONAL REVIEW,
2000, 49 (3), 357±371
# International Association for Applied Psychology, 2000.
Published by Blackwell Publishers,
108 Cowley Road, Oxford OX4 1JF, UK and 350 Main Street,
Malden, MA 02148, USA.
arguably is the motivation to perform well on the job. The
present study
focuses on work motivation, and analyses it from the
perspective of social
identity theory (Hogg & Abrams, 1988; Tajfel, 1978; Tajfel &
Turner, 1986)
and self-categorisation theory, which is an elaboration and
extension of social
identity theory (Turner, 1985; Turner, Hogg, Oakes, Reicher, &
Wetherell,
1987). In the following, the theoretical backgrounds to the
social identity
approach (i.e. including both social identity theory and self-
categorisation
theory) to motivation and performance are presented, the
proposition that
organisational identification affects work motivation and
performance is
derived from this approach, and empirical evidence in support
of this
proposition is discussed.
THEORETICAL BACKGROUNDS
The social identity approach outlines how membership in social
groups affects
the self-concept. Central to the approach is the proposition that
through
social identification, the perception of oneness with or
belongingness to a
group (see Mael & Ashforth, 1992), individuals define
themselves in terms of
their group membership and ascribe characteristics that are
typical of the
group to the self. The concept of social identity, `̀ that part of
an individual's
self-concept which derives from his knowledge of his
membership of a group
(or groups) together with the value and the emotional
significance attached
to the membership'' (Tajfel, 1978, p. 63), reflects this
internalisation of
group membership as part of `̀ who you are'' (Ashforth & Mael,
1989;
Haslam, in press). Identification leads individuals to perceive
themselves
in terms of the characteristics they share with other members of
their
ingroupsÐtheir shared social identityÐrather than in terms of
the
idiosyncratic characteristics that differentiate them from other
indivi-
dualsÐtheir personal identity (Turner et al., 1987).
Identification thus blurs
the distinction between self and group, and turns the group,
psychologically,
into a part of the self (Smith & Henry, 1996). This `̀ social'' or `̀
collective''
self lies at the heart of the perceptual, attitudinal, and
behavioural effects of
group membership. The more one conceives of oneself in terms
of one's
membership in a group (i.e. the more one identifies with the
group), the
more likely one is to act in accordance with the group's beliefs,
norms, and
values, and generally to act in `̀ group-typical'' ways (Ashforth
& Mael,
1989; Dutton, Dukerich, & Harquail, 1994; Hogg & Abrams,
1988; Turner
et al., 1987).
Even though identification with a group may lead individuals to
act in
group-typical ways, this does not mean that individuals who
identify with a
group always act in accordance with the social identity based in
that group
membership. The influence of identification is contingent on
social identity
being salient or cognitively activated. That is, even if an
individual identifies
358 VAN KNIPPENBERG
# International Association for Applied Psychology, 2000.
with a group, that does not mean the individual is always highly
aware
of this group membership, and group membership only affects
attitudes
and behaviour to the extent that the individual is `̀ made aware''
of the
membership in the group. Although identification itself may
contribute to
social identity salience (Haslam, in press), contextual factors
affect salience
as well. Roughly speaking, any `̀ event'' that speaks to a group
membership
(i.e. rather than to the individual qua individual) may make the
social
identity based in that group membership salient. The prospect of
a merger
may, for instance, render organisational identity salient (van
Knippenberg,
van Knippenberg, Monden, & de Lima, 1999), conflict or
competition
between work groups may render work group identity salient
(Kramer,
1991), and the addition of individuals from another ethnicity to
a previously
ethnicly homogenous work group may focus on ethnic identity.
Identity
salience may endure for longer periods of time or may change in
a matter of
moments, for instance when departmental identity is salient in
an inter-
departmental meeting, and a telephone call from home
immediately after the
meeting focuses on family identity (for a more elaborate
discussion of the
salience of social identities in organisations, see e.g. Haslam, in
this issue;
Hogg & Terry, 2000; Kramer, 1991).
As the group seems to be where organisational behaviour
primarily takes
place, and organisations themselves may, from a social
psychological per-
spective, be viewed as social groups, the relevance of the social
identity
approach to the study of organisational behaviour is readily
apparent.
Social identity theory has, more or less from its conception,
been applied to
the study of organisational behaviour (e.g. Brown, 1978), but
the study of
social identity processes in organisations has only gained
momentum in
recent years. The social identity approach has been applied in
several areas
of organisational behaviour research, such as intergroup
relations within the
organisation (Brown, Condor, Mathews, Wade, & Williams,
1986; Kramer,
1991), mergers and acquisitions (Terry & Callan, 1998; van
Knippenberg et al.,
1999), group cohesiveness (Hogg, 1993; Hogg, Cooper-Shaw, &
Holzworth,
1993), organisational demography (Ely, 1994; Tsui, Egan, &
O'Reilly, 1992),
leadership (Haslam, McGarty, Brown, Eggins, Morrison, &
Reynolds, 1998;
Hogg, Hains, & Mason, 1998), promotion decisions (Fajak &
Haslam,
1998), and turnover (Mael & Ashforth, 1995). Most relevant to
the present
discussion, the social identity approach also has clear
implications for work
motivation and performance.
IDENTIFICATION, MOTIVATION, AND PERFORMANCE
Membership in an organisation implies multiple group
memberships:
membership in the organisation as a whole, in one's own
department, in
one's own team or work unit, and so forth (see Ashforth &
Mael, 1989;
IDENTIFICATION, MOTIVATION, AND PERFORMANCE 359
# International Association for Applied Psychology, 2000.
van Knippenberg & van Schie, in press; for a discussion of the
multiple foci
of organisational identification). An obvious question from the
point of
view of the social identity approach is how these identifications
affect
organisational behaviour, and especiallyÐgiven that this is so
central to
organisational lifeÐwork motivation and performance. The
answer to this
question is that identification motivates group members to work
for the
group's interests, which in turn may affect performance. These
arguments
are elaborated below.
Identification elicits a sense of oneness with the target of
identification.
This psychological oneness with the group (or organisation)
induces indi-
viduals to take the group's perspective and to experience the
group's goals
and interests as their own (see Dutton et al., 1994). Thus,
identification will
generally be associated with a motivation to achieve group
goals and work
for the group's interest. Yet, whether identification with the
organisation
or within-organisation subunit (i.e. team, work group) actually
results in
higher performance at work is contingent on a number of other
factors.
First, as outlined above, identification with the group or
organisation will
only affect attitudes and behaviour to the extent that social
identity is
salient. Thus, identification will only result in the motivation to
exert effort
on behalf of the collective to the extent that group or
organisational identity
is salient (see also Haslam, in this issue).
Second, the relationship between identification and motivation
to exert
effort on behalf of the collective may be positive (although the
strength of
the relationship will be contingent on social identity salience),
but this does
not necessarily mean that identification results in work
motivation. That is,
we should distinguish between the motivation to exert effort on
behalf of the
collective and the motivation to perform well on the job (i.e.
work moti-
vation). Even though identification may be associated with the
willingness
to exert effort on behalf of the collective, whether this results in
work
motivation is contingent on what the goals and interests of that
collective
are perceived to be. Identification may only be expected to be
positively
related to work motivation if high performance is perceived to
be in the
collective's interest. Only then will motivation to exert effort on
behalf of the
collective translate to motivation to perform well on the job.
Performance
standards may, however, not be particularly high, clear, or
salient as it is not
always clear in organisations or jobs what is expected or desired
in terms
of performance, and high performance may not be a goal.
Moreover,
performance standards may be different at the organisational
and subunit
level, as arises when the organisation advocates high
performance, while
informal work group norms promote minimisation of work
efforts or a
focus on interpersonal relations. If the primary target of
identification is
not perceived to have a commitment to high performance,
identification is
unlikely to result in striving to enhance performance.
360 VAN KNIPPENBERG
# International Association for Applied Psychology, 2000.
To the extent that identification results in work motivation (i.e.
con-
tingent on social identity salience and the perceived goals and
interests of
the target of identification) it will have a positive effect on
performance.
This should hold to the extent that performance is under
volitional control
(e.g. to the extent that performance is contingent on effort and
persistence)
rather than on, for example, knowledge, skills, abilities, or
available resources
(because this qualification is not specific for the social identity
analysis, but
holds for the relationship between work motivation and
performance in
general, it receives no further attention here).
The analysis presented above is modelled in Fig. 1. In this
model, identi-
fication affects the motivation to exert effort on behalf of the
collective to
the extent that social identity is salient, and this motivation
affects work
motivation to the extent that the collective's interests are
perceived to be
performance-related. Finally, work motivation is related to
performance to
the extent that performance is under volitional control. Note
that whereas
the relationships between identification and the motivation to
exert effort on
behalf of the collective, and between work motivation and
performance, are
assumed to be positive (although moderated by social identity
salience and
the extent to which performance is under volitional control,
respectively),
the relationship between the motivation to exert effort on behalf
of the
collective and work motivation may be either positive, negative,
or zero
contingent on the perceived goals and interests of the collective.
In the following, I review empirical studies to evaluate the
model. In order
to unambiguously assess the merits of the model and to avoid
confusion
with the effects of other concepts reflecting psychological
attachment, like
organisational commitment (Allen & Meyer, 1990; Mowday,
Steers, &
Porter, 1979) or dispositional group loyalty (James &
Cropanzano, 1994), or
with other conceptualisations of identification (e.g. O'Reilly &
Chatman,
1986), this review is limited to studies that explicitly focus on
identification
as defined in the social identity approach. Moreover, it is
limited to studies
that measured work motivation or performance and does not
address
studies of related attitudes or behaviour (e.g. Mael &
Ashforth's, 1992,
study of organisational identification and support for the
organisation; see
Haslam, in press, for a review of studies of the relationship of
identification
with a broader range of work attitudes and behaviour).
Where performance is concerned, a distinction is made between
`̀ task
performance'' and `̀ contextual performance''. Whereas task
performance
refers to performance on the job that `̀ you were hired to do'',
contextual
performance (cf. organisational citizenship behaviour; Organ,
1997) refers
to behaviour that supports the organisational, social, and
psychological
environment in which task performance takes place, like helping
others,
taking others' interests into consideration, and presenting a
positive image of
the organisation to outsiders (e.g. Borman & Motowidlo, 1993;
Moorman &
IDENTIFICATION, MOTIVATION, AND PERFORMANCE 361
# International Association for Applied Psychology, 2000.
FIGURE 1. A social identity model of work motivation and
performance.
3
6
2
V
A
N
K
N
IP
P
E
N
B
E
R
G
#
In
tern
a
tio
n
a
l
A
sso
cia
tio
n
fo
r
A
p
p
lied
P
sy
ch
o
lo
g
y
,
2
0
0
0
.
Blakely, 1995; Organ, 1988). This distinction is of interest for
two reasons.
First, contextual performance may be assumed to be more under
volitional
control than task performance (Organ, 1988), because
behaviours like
helping others and taking others' interests into consideration are
generally
less contingent on skills, ability, resources, and so on, than task
perform-
ance. Moreover, contextual performance more or less by
definition refers to
behaviours that individuals are not required to perform, whereas
task
performance is part of job requirements. As a consequence, an
employee can
more easily refrain from contextual than from task behaviours.
Second, task
performance is more likely to benefit the self (e.g. in terms of
career
opportunities or bonuses) than contextual performance, and may
therefore
be less contingent on group-oriented motivations. These
considerations
suggest that the effects of identification may be more apparent
on con-
textual than on task performance. Moreover, contextual
performance may
benefit the task performance of the work unit as a whole
(Podsakoff,
Ahearne, & MacKenzie, 1997), even if it does not benefit the
task perform-
ance of the individual engaging in the contextual behaviours.
Even so, it
should be noted that although contextual performance may be in
the
collective's best interest, individuals need not perceive it as
such, and may
engage in contextual behaviours for other reasons than wanting
to contri-
bute to the collective interest (e.g. because they want to help a
friend). Thus,
the relationship between identification and contextual
performance may not
be straightforward either.
Identification andWork Motivation
To the author's knowledge, only one study has assessed the
relationship
between organisational identification and work motivation (van
Knippen-
berg & van Schie, in press). In a survey of work attitudes of the
employees of
a university, van Knippenberg and van Schie assessed work
motivation
(with Hackman & Lawler's, 1971, internal motivation scale) and
both work
group identification and organisational identification (with
Mael and Ash-
forth's, 1992, organisational identification questionnaire). In
addition, the
study included a measure of job involvement with items like `̀ I
am always
prepared to do my best'', which seems to come close to a
measure of work
motivation as defined here (i.e. as the willingness to exert effort
to perform
well). Work group identification was found to be related to
work motivation
(r = 0.30, P50.001) and both work group identification (r =
0.46,
P50.001) and organisational identification (r = 0.27, P50.01)
were related
to the job involvement measure. Although only a cross-sectional
study in
which all data were gathered with the same questionnaire, this
does
corroborate the proposition that identification may be associated
with the
motivation to exert effort on the job.
IDENTIFICATION, MOTIVATION, AND PERFORMANCE 363
# International Association for Applied Psychology, 2000.
Identification andTask Performance
Evidence regarding the relationship between identification and
task
performance comes from a series of laboratory studies that all
used simple
tasks on which performance may be assumed to be highly
dependent on
effort and persistence rather than skills, knowledge, or ability.
Performance
on these tasks may thus be regarded as indicative of work
motivation.
Concerning the role of social identity salience and the
perception that high
performance is in the group's best interest, a study by James and
Greenberg
(1989) is especially relevant. James and Greenberg
experimentally manipu-
lated the salience of students' membership in their university by
presenting
half of the participants with cues that rendered their university
membership
salient (the university colours in the first experiment, reference
to the uni-
versity mascot in the second study). In addition, James and
Greenberg told
their participants that the performance of students from their
university
would be compared to the performance of students from another
university,
thus implicitly suggesting that high performance would be
beneficial to
the university's image (Experiment 1), or manipulated whether
such inter-
group comparison was implied or not (Experiment 2). Under
these different
experimental conditions, participants performed a simple
anagram task. As
predicted, participants in the high identification condition
solved more
anagrams, but only when intergroup comparison was salient.
James and
Greenberg did not assess identification, but Worchel,
Rothgerber, Day,
Hart, and Butemeyer (1998) included a measure of
identification in a study
that basically replicated the James and Greenberg study.
Worchel et al.
(Exp. 3) let participants work on an assembly task (making a
chain out of
strips of paper) either in the presence of another work group or
not and
either wearing group uniforms (same-colour lab coats) or not.
Individual
performance was higher when group members were wearing
group uniforms
(i.e. social identity was salient), but only when the other group
was present.
Moreover, mediation analysis indicated that the effects of the
experimental
manipulations were partly mediated by identification. Results of
these studies
support the proposition that identification results in increased
work motivation
and task performance, provided social identity is salient (see
also Pilegge &
Holtz, 1997), and high performance is seen as serving the
group's interests.
Evidence that identification may induce individuals to
experience the
group's interest as self-interest (i.e. social self-interest rather
than personal
self-interest) comes from a study by van Leeuwen and van
Knippenberg
(1999). Van Leeuwen and van Knippenberg investigated the
effects on
performance of work group identification, prosocial versus
proself value
orientations (an individual difference variable), and
expectations about
other group members' effort. Participants worked on a simple
group task
(putting sheets of paper in envelopes). Identification with the
work group
364 VAN KNIPPENBERG
# International Association for Applied Psychology, 2000.
and value orientations were measured and expectations about
other group
members' task effort were manipulated. Van Leeuwen and van
Knippenberg
predicted that identification would motivate group members to
exert effort
on the group task, especially for group members who were not
disposi-
tionally inclined to take others' interests into account (i.e.
individuals with
proself as compared with prosocial value orientations). Results
supported
these predictions. Performance (number of envelopes filled) was
higher
for participants who identified more with their work group, and
this effect
was stronger for group members with a proself rather than a
prosocial
orientation. Performance was also affected by expectations
about the effort
other group members would invest in the group task. Higher
expected effort
from others resulted in higher own performance, but more so for
partici-
pants with relatively low identification. High identifiers were
motivated
to perform more or less irrespective of the effort and
performance they
expected from their fellow group members. These results show
that identi-
fication is positively related to performance, even if it means
exerting effort
on behalf of the group while other group members are taking it
easy.
Moreover, and of more importance to the present discussion, the
fact that
identification was primarily related to performance of
individuals who
dispositionally focus on personal self-interest showed that
identification
may elicit the motivation to exert effort on behalf of the
collective because it
leads individuals to experience the collective's interests as self-
interest, albeit
social self-interest and not personal self-interest.
Identification and Contextual Performance
Only one study provides an empirical test of the relationship
between
identification as defined in the social identity approach and
contextual
performance (van Knippenberg & Sleebos, 1999). In a survey of
university
faculty, van Knippenberg and Sleebos measured identification
(using the
Mael & Ashforth scale), individualism±collectivism as an
individual differ-
ence variable (with Wagner & Moch's, 1986, measure), and
affective commit-
ment (with Allen & Meyer's, 1990, scale). Two weeks later they
assessed
self-reported contextual performance. This second survey
focused on two
dimensions: altruism, the helping of coworkers in their job
when needed,
and generalised compliance with norms defining `̀ what a good
employee
ought to do'' (Organ, 1988). Identification was found to be
positively related
to self-reports of both altruism and generalised compliance,
although for
generalised compliance this was only true for more individually
oriented as
compared with more collectively oriented employees.
The fact that identification was positively related to self-reports
of con-
textual performance obviously supports the social identity
analysis of moti-
vation and performance. It should, however, be noted that the
relationship
IDENTIFICATION, MOTIVATION, AND PERFORMANCE 365
# International Association for Applied Psychology, 2000.
established in the van Knippenberg and Sleebos study is one
between
identification and self-reports of behaviour, and not between
identification
and actual performance (e.g. as in the previous section). Thus,
this relation-
ship still awaits a more conclusive test. The finding that the
relationship
between identification and (self-reported) contextual
performance was in
part moderated by individualism±collectivism is also highly
relevant to the
present discussion. Individualism±collectivism as an individual
difference
variable refers to a dimension that distinguishes individuals
who consider
their personal interests more important than the interests of the
group
(individualists) from individuals who are primarily focused on
the interests
of the group and would pursue group goals at the possible
expense of their
personal interests (collectivists). In other words, more
collectivistically oriented
individuals are dispositionally more inclined to exert effort on
behalf of
the group. Indeed, individualism±collectivism as an individual
difference
variable was found to be related to both task performance on
group tasks
(e.g. Wagner, 1995) and to contextual performance ratings
(Moorman
& Blakely, 1995). Analogous to van Leeuwen and van
Knippenberg's (1999)
reasoning concerning the interaction between identification and
social value
orientation, van Knippenberg and Sleebos argued that
individualism±
collectivism moderated the relationship between identification
and perform-
ance ratings, because identification leads individuals to
experience the
collective interest as self-interest (i.e. again, social rather than
personal self-
interest). This should affect individualists, who primarily focus
on self-
interest, to a greater extent than collectivists, for whom group
interest
generally prevails over personal interest.
Although not directly related to the social identity analysis of
motivation
and performance, one other finding from the van Knippenberg
and Sleebos
study is of interest here. Identification is conceptually similar to
the concept
of affective commitment, `̀ the employee's emotional attachment
to, identi-
fication with, and involvement in, the organisation'' (Allen &
Meyer, 1990,
p. 1; cf. Mowday, Steers, & Porter, 1979). In view of this
conceptual simi-
larity (especially the reference to identification), one may
wonder whether
identification as defined in the social identity approach is not
just a `̀ new
name for an old concept''. Addressing this issue, Ashforth and
Mael (1989)
argue that despite the apparent similarity, commitment and
identification
are different, because identification has a cognitive, self-
definitional com-
ponent (in addition to identification's affective component;
Karasawa, 1991;
Tajfel, 1978) that affective commitment has not (see also Pratt,
1998).
Corroborating this conceptual distinction, Mael (1988, in
Ashforth & Mael,
1989; Mael & Tetrick, 1992) showed that identification as
measured by
Mael and Ashforth's (1992) organisational identification
questionnaire is
empirically distinguishable from commitment measured with
Mowday
et al.'s (1979) Organisational Commitment Questionnaire.
Extending these
366 VAN KNIPPENBERG
# International Association for Applied Psychology, 2000.
findings, results from Confirmatory Factor Analysis in the van
Knippen-
berg and Sleebos study showed that identification is also
empirically dis-
tinguishable from commitment as assessed with Allen and
Meyer's (1990)
Affective Commitment Scale. The fit of a model with two
correlated factors
(w2 (76) = 125.71, P50.0001, NFI = 0.850, NNFI = 0.920, CFI
= 0.934)
was superior to that of a model with one single
identification/commitment
factor (w2 (77) = 162.25, P50.001, NFI = 0.807, NNFI = 0.865,
CFI =
0.886). Obviously, this is no proof that the difference between
the two lies
in the self-definitional aspect of identification, but it does show
that
identification and commitment are different concepts.
CONCLUSIONS
The number of studies of the relationship of identification with
motivation
and performance is as yet rather small, but these studies do
yield converging
evidence in support of the proposed social identity model of
motivation and
performance, and corroborate the conclusion that identification
is positively
related to work motivation, task performance, and contextual
performance
to the extent that (a) social identity is salient, and (b) high
performance is
perceived to be in the group's or organisation's interest. Even
so, some of
the relationships proposed by the model are not tested yet, only
implicated
by the results of the studies reviewed, or only established
correlationally
rather than experimentally. Therefore, more extensive testing of
the rela-
tions proposed in the model seems in order before more definite
conclusions
about the merits of the social identity approach to motivation
and perform-
ance may be drawn.
The relationship between social identity processes and
performance on
simple tasks (i.e. where performance is highly dependent on
motivation)
seems to be well established in the experimental research
reviewed. As
proposed in the model, but not addressed in the studies
reviewed here,
performance on more complex tasks (i.e. tasks on which
performance is less
contingent on motivation alone) may only be contingent on
social identity
processes to the extent that the individual possesses the
necessary skills,
knowledge, and so on, to render performance on the task
primarily an issue
of motivation. Yet, one may argue that especially on such tasks,
where
individuals may be more dependent on help, cooperation, and
information
from others, social identity processes may affect group
performance through
contextual performance (cf. Podsakoff et al., 1997) and
interpersonal co-
operation (cf. Kramer, 1991). The study of these more complex
relationships
would seem to be an obvious next step both from a theoretical
and from an
applied point of view.
Although the discussion has primarily focused on the positive
effect
identification may have on performance, there is a definite
potential for
IDENTIFICATION, MOTIVATION, AND PERFORMANCE 367
# International Association for Applied Psychology, 2000.
social identity processes to result in lowered performance. Work
groups may
develop counter-productive norms, for instance as a result of a
conflict with
management (e.g. if the organisation is seen as exploitative,
nonproductivity
may be an obvious response) or because within the informal
network of the
work group doing as little as possible and getting away with it
is regarded as
an accomplishment. In such cases, increased identification and
increased
social identity salience may result in lowered productivity (note
that, in this
example, low productivity is undesirable from the organisation's
point of
view, but not from the point of view of the target of
identification, the work
group). In addition, for individuals who do not identify with the
group
or organisation, an emphasis on their group membership (which
would
enhance social identity salience) may actually elicit responses
aimed at
underscoring the individual's personal (i.e. as opposed to social)
identity
(van Prooijen & van Knippenberg, 2000) and thus engender
behaviour that
is incongruent with the implied social identity (e.g.
nonproductivity; see
Haslam, in press, for a more elaborate discussion of these
issues). Thus, it
would seem crucial for a proper understanding of the effects of
social
identity processes on motivation and performance not only to
study the
factors that affect organisational identifications and the salience
of organ-
isational identities, but also to study the factors that affect
perceptions of
the group's goals and interests.
Factors that affect organisational identification and identity
salience are
extensively discussed elsewhere (e.g. Ashforth & Mael, 1989;
Dutton et al.,
1994; Haslam, in press; Hogg & Terry, 2000; Pratt, 1998), and
although
factors that affect the extent to which high performance is
perceived to be in
the group's or organisation's interest have not received much
attention in
social identity research, they are addressed elsewhere (e.g. in
research on
organisational culture; cf. van Vianen, in press). A discussion
of these issues
is beyond the scope of the present study, and the conclusion of
the previous
paragraph is meant as a suggested agenda for research rather
than as an
issue to be resolved here. Even so, to conclude I may highlight
a factor that
may be especially relevant in this respect: group goal setting. A
large body of
evidence shows that setting clear and specific performance
goals has a
positive effect on the performance of individuals as well as
groups (Locke
& Latham, 1990; O'Leary-Kelly, Martocchio, & Frink, 1994). In
a recent
discussion of the effects of group goal setting, Wegge, in this
issue, argues
that setting group goals, especially setting group goals
participatively,
enhances social identity salience and increases group
identification. Because
group goal setting also provides a clear performance standard,
Wegge's
discussion implies that group goal setting may affect the three
variables
highlighted above: identification, identity salience, and group
goals. Thus,
group goal setting in particular may engender a social identity-
based
motivation to perform well. More definite conclusions about
this proposi-
368 VAN KNIPPENBERG
# International Association for Applied Psychology, 2000.
tion, and other propositions made here, will have to await the
results of
future research.
REFERENCES
Allen, N.J., & Meyer, J.P. (1990). The measurement and
antecedents of affective,
continuance and normative commitment to the organization.
Journal of Occu-
pational Psychology, 63, 1±18.
Ashforth, B.E., & Mael, F. (1989). Social identity theory and
the organization.
Academy of Management Review, 14, 20±39.
Borman, W.C., & Motowidlo, S.J. (1993). Expanding the
criterion domain to include
elements of contextual performance. In N. Schmitt & W.C.
Borman (Eds.),
Personnel selection in organizations (pp. 71±98). New York:
Jossey-Bass.
Brown, R.J. (1978). Divided we fall: An analysis of relations
between sections of a
factory workforce. In H. Tajfel (Ed.), Differentiation between
social groups: Studies in
the social psychology of intergroup relations (pp. 395±429).
London: Academic Press.
Brown, R., Condor, S., Mathews, A., Wade, G., & Williams, J.
(1986). Explaining
intergroup differentiation in an industrial organization. Journal
of Occupational
Psychology, 59, 273±286.
Dutton, J.E., Dukerich, J.M., & Harquail, C.V. (1994).
Organizational images and
member identification. Administrative Science Quarterly, 39,
239±263.
Ely, R. (1994). The effects of organizational demographics and
social identity on rela-
tionships among professional women. Administrative Science
Quarterly, 39, 203±238.
Fajak, A., & Haslam, S.A. (1998). Gender solidarity in
hierarchical organizations.
British Journal of Social Psychology, 37, 73±94.
Hackman, J.R., & Lawler, E.E. (1971). Employee reactions to
job characteristics.
Journal of Applied Psychology, 55, 259±286.
Haslam, S.A. (in press). The psychology of organizations: A
social identity approach.
London & Newbury Park, CA: Sage.
Haslam, S.A. (2000). Social identity, self-categorization and
work motivation:
Rethinking the contribution of the group to positive and
sustainable organiz-
ational outcomes. Applied Psychology: An International
Review.
Haslam, S.A., McGarty, C., Brown, P.M., Eggins, R.A.,
Morrison, B.E., &
Reynolds, K.J. (1998). Inspecting the emperor's clothes:
Evidence that random
selection of leaders can enhance group performance. Group
Dynamics: Theory,
Research, & Practice, 2, 168±184.
Hogg, M.A. (1993). Group cohesiveness: A critical review and
some new directions.
European Review of Social Psychology, 4, 85±111.
Hogg, M.A., & Abrams, D. (1988). Social identifications: A
social psychology of
intergroup relations and group processes. London: Routledge.
Hogg, M.A., Cooper-Shaw, L., & Holzworth, D.W. (1993).
Group prototypicality
and depersonalized attraction in small interactive groups.
Personality and Social
Psychology Bulletin, 19, 452±465.
Hogg, M.A., Hains, S.C., & Mason, I. (1998). Identification and
leadership in small
groups: Salience, frame of reference, and leader stereotypicality
effects on leader
evaluations. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 75,
1248±1263.
IDENTIFICATION, MOTIVATION, AND PERFORMANCE 369
# International Association for Applied Psychology, 2000.
Hogg, M.A., & Terry, D.J. (2000). Social identity and self-
categorization processes in
organizational contexts. Academy of Management Review, 25,
121±140.
James, K., & Cropanzano, R. (1994). Dispositional group
loyalty and individual
action for the benefit of an ingroup: Experimental and
correlational evidence.
Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 60,
179±205.
James, K., & Greenberg, J. (1989). In-group salience,
intergroup comparison, and
individual performance and self-esteem. Personality and Social
Psychology
Bulletin, 15, 604±616.
Karasawa, M. (1991). Toward an assessment of social identity:
The structure of
group identification and its effects on in-group evaluations.
British Journal of
Social Psychology, 30, 293±307.
Kramer, R. (1991). Intergroup relations and organizational
dilemmas. Research in
Organizational Behavior, 13, 191±228.
Locke, E.A., & Latham, G.P. (1990). A theory of goal setting
and task performance.
Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall.
Mael, F., & Ashforth, B.E. (1992). Alumni and their alma
mater: A partial test of the
reformulated model of organizational identification. Journal of
Organizational
Behavior, 13, 103±123.
Mael, F., & Ashforth, B.E. (1995). Loyal from day one:
Biodata, organizational
identification, and turnover among newcomers. Personnel
Psychology, 48, 309±333.
Mael, F.A., & Tetrick, L.E. (1992). Identifying organizational
identification.
Educational and Psychological Measurement, 52, 813±824.
Moorman, R.H., & Blakely, G.L. (1995). Individualism-
collectivism as an individual
difference predictor of organizational citizenship behavior.
Journal of Organiza-
tional Behavior, 16, 127±142.
Mowday, R.T., Steers, R.M., & Porter, L.W. (1979). The
measurement of organ-
izational commitment. Journal of Vocational Behavior, 14,
224±247.
O'Leary-Kelly, A.M., Martocchio, J.J., & Frink, D.D. (1994). A
review of the
influence of group goals on group performance. Academy of
Management
Journal, 37, 1285±1301.
O'Reilly, C.A., & Chatman, J. (1986). Organizational
commitment and psycholo-
gical attachment: The effects of compliance, identification, and
internalization on
prosocial behavior. Journal of Applied Psychology, 71,
492±499.
Organ, D.W. (1988). Organizational citizenship behavior: The
good soldier syndrome.
Lexington, MA: Lexington Books.
Organ, D.W. (1997). Organizational citizenship behavior: It's
construct clean-up
time. Human Performance, 10, 85±97.
Pilegge, A.J., & Holtz, R. (1997). The effects of social identity
on the self-set goals
and task performance of high and low self-esteem individuals.
Organizational
Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 70, 17±26.
Podsakoff, P.M., Ahearne, M., & MacKenzie, S.B. (1997).
Organizational citizen-
ship behavior and the quality and quantity of work group
performance. Journal
of Applied Psychology, 82, 262±270.
Pratt, M.G. (1998). To be or not to be? Central questions in
organizational
identification. In D.A. Whetten & P.C. Godfrey (Eds.), Identity
in organiz-
ations: Building theory through conversations (pp. 171±207).
Thousand Oaks, CA:
Sage.
370 VAN KNIPPENBERG
# International Association for Applied Psychology, 2000.
Smith, E.R., & Henry, S. (1996). An in-group becomes part of
the self: Response
time evidence. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 22,
635±642.
Tajfel, H. (1978). Differentiation between social groups:
Studies in the social
psychology of intergroup relations. London: Academic Press.
Tajfel, H., & Turner, J.C. (1986). The social identity theory of
intergroup behaviour.
In S. Worchel & W.G. Austin (Eds.), Psychology of intergroup
relations (2nd edn.,
pp. 7±24). Chicago: Nelson-Hall.
Terry, D.J., & Callan, V.J. (1998). In-group bias in response to
an organizational
merger. Group Dynamics: Theory, Research, and Practice, 2,
67±81.
Tsui, A., Egan, T., & O'Reilly, C. (1992). Being different:
Relational demography
and organizational attachment. Administrative Science
Quarterly, 37, 549±579.
Turner, J.C. (1985). Social categorization and the self-concept:
A social cognitive
theory of group behavior. In E.J. Lawler (Ed.), Advances in
group processes, 2
(pp. 77±122). Greenwich, CT: JAI Press.
Turner, J.C., Hogg, M.A., Oakes, P.J., Reicher, S.D., &
Wetherell, M.S. (1987).
Rediscovering the social group. A self-categorization theory.
Oxford: Blackwell.
van Knippenberg, D., & Sleebos, E. (1999). Identification,
commitment, and
individualism-collectivism: Their interrelations and relationship
with organiza-
tional citizenship behavior. Unpublished manuscript, University
of Amsterdam.
van Knippenberg, D., van Knippenberg, B., Monden, L., & de
Lima, F. (1999). A
social categorization perspective on mergers and acquisitions.
Unpublished
manuscript, University of Amsterdam.
van Knippenberg, D., & van Schie, E.C.M. (in press). Foci and
correlates of organ-
izational identification. Journal of Occupational and
Organizational Psychology.
van Leeuwen, E., & van Knippenberg, D. (1999). Social value
orientations and group
performance: The role of expectations of other group members'
effort. Un-
published manuscript, Leiden University.
van Prooijen, J.W., & van Knippenberg, D. (2000).
Individuation or depersonal-
ization: The influence of personal status position. Group
Processes & Intergroup
Relations, 3, 63-77.
van Vianen, A. (in press). Person-organization fit: The match
between newcomers'
and recruiters' preferences for organizational cultures.
Personnel Psychology.
Wagner, J.A. (1995). Studies of individualism-collectivism:
Effects on cooperation in
groups. Academy of Management Journal, 38, 152±172.
Wagner, J.A., & Moch, M.K. (1986). Individualism-
collectivism: Concept and
measure. Group and Organization Studies, 11, 280±304.
Worchel, S., Rothgerber, H., Day, E.A., Hart, D., & Butemeyer,
J. (1998). Social
identity and individual productivity within groups. British
Journal of Social
Psychology, 37, 389±413.
IDENTIFICATION, MOTIVATION, AND PERFORMANCE 371
# International Association for Applied Psychology, 2000.
Top of Form
Implementation of the IOM Future of Nursing Report
1
Unsatisfactory
0.00%
2
Less than Satisfactory
75.00%
3
Satisfactory
79.00%
4
Good
89.00%
5
Excellent
100.00%
80.0 %Content
5.0 %Provided an original summary of the key messages of the
IOM report, Future of Nursing: Leading Change, Advancing
Health. Any specific references should be cited.
Did not attempt to provide a summary of the key messages of
the IOM report, Future of Nursing: Leading Change, Advancing
Health, or failed to cite specific references to the IOM report.
Provided a skeletal summary of the key messages of the IOM
report, Future of Nursing: Leading Change, Advancing Health.
Some of the specific references to the IOM report were cited or
were done incorrectly.
Demonstrates a moderate knowledge of the subject. Recognizes
the basic ideas. Misinterprets evidence on the committee's
initiative.
Demonstrates good knowledge of the subject. Correctly
describes the committee's initiative. Justifies some of the
impacts on the Future of Nursing.
Provided an original summary of the key messages of the IOM
report, Future of Nursing: Leading Change, Advancing Health.
References specific to the IOM report were properly cited.
15.0 %Identify the role of the Robert Wood Johnson Foundation
Initiative and the American Association of Retired Persons on
the Future of Nursing Campaign for Action and the State Based
Action Coalitions
Does not demonstrate knowledge of role. Fails to identify the
impact of the Robert Wood Johnson Foundation Initiative on the
Future of Nursing.
Demonstrates minimal knowledge of subject. Does not
adequately visualize or justify the work of the Committee of the
Robert Wood Johnson Foundation Initiative on the Future of
Nursing.
Demonstrates a moderate knowledge of the subject. Recognizes
the basic ideas. Misinterprets evidence on the committee's
initiative.
Demonstrates good knowledge of the subject. Correctly
describes the committee's initiative. Justifies some of the
impacts on the Future of Nursing.
Demonstrates a full and deep knowledge of subject. Develops
and explains an informed position on the committee's initiative,
integrates and justifies the impact on the Future of Nursing
15.0 %Identify the importance of the IOM FON report related to
the nursing workforce
Does not demonstrate knowledge of the concept or its role. Fails
to identify the importance of the IOM FON report related to the
nursing workforce.
Demonstrates minimal knowledge of the subject. Does not
adequately visualize or justify the importance of the IOM FON
report related to the nursing workforce.
Demonstrates a moderate knowledge of the subject. Recognizes
the basic ideas. Misinterprets evidence on the importance of the
IOM FON report related to the nursing workforce.
Demonstrates good knowledge of the subject. Correctly
describes importance of the IOM FON report related to the
nursing workforce.
Demonstrates a full and deep knowledge of the subject.
Develops and explains the importance of the IOM FON report,
integrates and justifies the importance of the IOM FON report
related to the nursing workforce.
15.0 %Discuss the intent of the Future of Nursing Campaign for
Action
Does not demonstrate knowledge of the concept or its role. Fails
to identify the intent of the Future of Nursing Campaign for
Action.
Demonstrates minimal knowledge of the subject. Does not
adequately visualize or identify the intent of the Future of
Nursing Campaign for Action.
Demonstrates a moderate knowledge of the subject. Recognizes
the basic ideas. Misinterprets evidence on the intent of the
Future of Nursing Campaign for Action.
Demonstrates good knowledge of the subject. Correctly
describes the intent of the Future of Nursing Campaign for
Action.
Demonstrates a full and deep knowledge of the subject.
Develops and explains the intent of the Future of Nursing
Campaign for Action, integrates and justifies the intent of the
Future of Nursing Campaign for Action.
15.0 %Identify the rationale of state-based action coalitions
Does not demonstrate knowledge of the concept or its role. Fails
to identify the rationale of state-based action coalitions.
Demonstrates minimal knowledge of the subject. Does not
adequately identify the rationale of state-based action
coalitions.
Demonstrates a moderate knowledge of the subject. Recognizes
the basic ideas. Misinterprets evidence on the rationale of state-
based action coalitions.
Demonstrates good knowledge of the subject. Correctly
identifies the rationale of state-based action coalitions.
Demonstrates a full and deep knowledge of the subject.
Develops and explains and identifies the rationale of state-based
action coalitions and justifies a rationale for state-based action
coalitions.
15.0 %Discuss one state-based action coalition and two
initiatives
Does not demonstrate knowledge of the concept or its role. Fails
to identify one state-based action coalition and two initiatives.
Demonstrates minimal knowledge of the subject. Does not
adequately identify one state-based action coalition and two
initiatives.
Demonstrates a moderate knowledge of the subject. Recognizes
the basic ideas. Identifies but misinterprets one state-based
action coalition and two initiatives.
Demonstrates good knowledge of the subject. Correctly
identifies one state-based action coalition and two initiatives.
Demonstrates a full and deep knowledge of the subject.
Develops and explains the one state-based action coalition and
two initiatives, integrates and justifies one state-based action
coalition and two initiatives.
15.0 %Organization and Effectiveness
5.0 %Thesis Development and Purpose
Paper lacks any discernible overall purpose or organizing claim.
Thesis and/or main claim are insufficiently developed and/or
vague; purpose is not clear.
Thesis and/or main claim are apparent and appropriate to
purpose.
Thesis and/or main claim are clear and forecast the development
of the paper. Is descriptive and reflective of the arguments and
appropriate to purpose.
Thesis and/or main claim are comprehensive; contained within
the thesis is the essence of the paper. Thesis statement makes
the purpose of the paper clear.
15.0 %Organization and Effectiveness
5.0 %Paragraph Development and Transitions
Paragraphs and transitions consistently lack unity and
coherence. No apparent connections between paragraphs are
established. Transitions are inappropriate to purpose and scope.
Organization is disjointed.
Some paragraphs and transitions may lack logical progression of
ideas, unity, coherence, and/or cohesiveness.Some degree of
organization is evident.
Paragraphs are generally competent, but ideas may show some
inconsistency in organization and/or in their relationships to
each other.
A logical progression of ideas between paragraphs is apparent.
Paragraphs exhibit a unity, coherence, and cohesiveness. Topic
sentences and concluding remarks are appropriate to purpose.
There is a sophisticated construction of paragraphs and
transitions. Ideas progress and relate to each other. Paragraph
and transition construction guide the reader. Paragraph structure
is seamless.
15.0 %Organization and Effectiveness
5.0 %Mechanics of Writing (includes spelling, punctuation,
grammar, language use)
Surface errors are pervasive enough that they impede
communication of meaning. Inappropriate word choice and/or
sentence construction are used.
Frequent and repetitive mechanical errors distract the reader.
Inconsistencies in language choice (register), sentence
structure, and/or word choice are present.
Some mechanical errors or typos are present, but are not overly
distracting to the reader. Correct sentence structure and
audience-appropriate language are used.
Prose is largely free of mechanical errors, although a few may
be present. A variety of sentence structures and effective
figures of speech are used.
Writer is clearly in command of standard, written, academic
English.
5.0 %Format
2.0 %Paper Format (1- inch margins;12-point-font;double-
spaced;Times New Roman, Arial, or Courier)
Template is not used appropriately or documentation format is
rarely followed correctly.
Template is used, but some elements are missing or mistaken;
lack of control with formatting is apparent.
Template is used, and formatting is correct, although some
minor errors may be present.
Template is fully used; There are virtually no errors in
formatting style.
All format elements are correct.
3.0 %Research Citations (In-text citations for paraphrasing and
direct quotes, and reference page listing and formatting, as
appropriate to assignment)
No reference page is included. No citations are used.
Reference page is present. Citations are inconsistently used.
Reference page is included and lists sources used in the paper.
Sources are appropriately documented, although some errors
may be present.
Reference page is present and fully inclusive of all cited
sources. Documentation is appropriate and GCU style is usually
correct.
In-text citations and a reference page are complete. The
documentation of cited sources is free of error.
100 %Total Weightage
Bottom of Form
Identity
The term identity implies a construct that gives the self a
meaning or purpose. Weigert, Teitge, and Teitge (1986) gave
another brief definition: "identity is a definition that transforms
a mere biological individual into a human person" (p. 31). To
explicate further, identity is how people define themselves via
many different expressions such as autobiographical memories
(personal identity) and identification with groups (social
identity). Social and personal identity are not completely
separable but overlap and interact with one another very closely
to guide a person's perceptions, attitudes, and behaviors as well
as form a general identity. See the figure below.
We will briefly explore the concept of personal identity before
returning to the main concept of social identity.
Personal Identity
Mayer, Greenbaum, Kuenzi, and Shteynberg (2009) state
"personal identity consists of moral sensibility and conscience,
and also a desire for achievement, mastery, and competence."
Personal identity is the combination of objective biosocial
markers such as age, race, sex, and so on, and the personal life
history of the individual (Brewer & Gardner, 1996; Weigert et
al., 1986). These factors are often considered part of social
identity as well because they place the individual into a group.
That said, they involve attributes of individuals (even if they
are shared within groups characterized by values of those
markers), so the biosocial markers also create a personal
identity. Personal identity is those aspects of the self that are
unique to the individual and are used to define the individual
(Brewer & Gardner, 1996).
Social Identity
The most widely accepted definition of social identity comes
from Tajfel (1981): "that part of an individual's self-concept
which derives from his [sic] knowledge of his membership in a
social group (or groups) together with the value and emotional
significance attached with that membership" (p. 255).
According to Tajfel, social identity has three components:
· Self-categorization: Belief of group membership
· Group self-esteem: Importance of group membership to one's
self-concept
· Group commitment: Desire for group membership
Therefore, Tajfel (1981) theorizes that it is not enough to
belong to an organization; a person needs to feel connected to
the group, and some sort of social-esteem must also be derived
from that group membership. When a person categorizes
her/himself as part of the group, feels committed to the group,
and gains some esteem from the group, then that group has
become the person's in-group. All other groups are potential
rivals/competitors for resources and are known as out-groups.
Identity, Threat, and Behavior
There is convincing evidence that under certain conditions, such
as various types of group membership and threat conditions, one
of the major components of identity is likely to emerge as the
primary determinant of behavior (Ellemers et al., 2002; Nario-
Redmond, Biernat, Eidelman, & Palenske, 2004). For example,
it has been demonstrated that under threat to a high-status
group, the behavior of a group member is likely to be
determined by her/his social identity (Ellemers et al., 2002;
Haslam, O'Brien, & Jetten, Vormedal, & Penna, 2005). In
contrast, while under personal threat, the same person's personal
identity is likely to emerge as the primary determinant of
behavior (Ellemers et al., 2002; Nario-Redmond et al., 2004).
See the table below.
Threat and Behavioral Determinants
THREATENED ENTITIES PRIMARY BEHAVIORAL
DETERMINANTS SECONDARY BEHAVIORAL
DETERMINANTS
Person Personal identity General identity, social identity
High-status in-group Social identity General identity,
personal identity
Group Status and Self-Esteem
Social identity theory explains a person's behavior in a group
using that person's status within the group as a social
comparison. Every group has a certain amount of social status
attached to its membership (Morton, Postmes, Haslam, &
Hornsey, 2009; Tajfel & Turner, 1979, 1986), which gives it
value for the individual who is a member of that particular
group. Individual group members use the status from their group
membership to gain and maintain self-esteem (Derks, van Laar,
& Ellemers, 2009; Morton, Postmes, Haslam, & Hornsey, 2009).
Status provides self-esteem for group members because it
fulfills the need for a positive social identity (Hogg, 1992;
Hogg & Abrams, 1988; 1990; 1993). The motivations to
enhance self-esteem and gain status cause individuals to behave
in ways that maintain the group and their membership in the
group (Derks, van Laar, & Ellemers, 2009; Hogg & Abrams,
1990; Morton, Postmes, Haslam, & Hornsey, 2009). The better
one's own group looks in comparison to other groups, the more
status the group gains, and the more self-esteem it can provide
for its members (Morton, Postmes, Haslam, & Hornsey, 2009;
Tajfel & Turner, 1979, 1986). See the figure below.
Self-esteem is not necessarily the mechanism by which a
positive social identity is achieved. Status itself can also be
enough to trigger a positive social identity. In this case,
individuals seek to join a high-status group to improve their
own social identity in general rather than to improve their self-
esteem per se (Bettencourt, Dorr, Charlton, & Hume, 2001). But
in either case the motivation is a positive social identity from
which the person gains benefits (e.g. self-esteem, higher status,
possibly power or wealth) through self-identification with the
high-status group. The benefits of social identity have recently
been demonstrated by Peeters and Oerlemans (2009) who
studied two different organizations and found that those with
strong social identification with their organization had greater
feelings of well-being than those with weaker social
identification.
In summary, social identity theory proposes that self-esteem and
status are the main reasons that social identity motivates
individuals. But remember that a person must also first describe
her/himself as part of the group (self-categorization) as well as
want to be a part of the group (commitment).
Organizational Applications
Application of social identity theory in organizations is still
relatively new (since about the turn of the century). However,
because of the success of the theory and the large amounts of
empirical evidence, several books have been written (e.g.
Haslam, 2004; Hogg & Terry, 2001) that not only synthesize the
vast amount of research but also provide many useful tips on
how to manage certain aspects of organizations.
The theory has been applied in many different areas of
organizations, and it has been used to not only explain why
some groups are motivated to experience conflict with groups
within their own organization but also why their organization
may experience conflict with other organizations. It has also
been used to resolve those conflicts or avoid them altogether.
Specifically, it has been used to manage group relationships
with regards to the following: majority-minority relations,
corporate mergers, people with multiple roles in an
organization, workgroup socialization, power usage, and
leadership, as well as others. Experienced success of
organizations in motivating their people through those often
difficult tasks reflects the relative easiness of implementation of
the theory's basic components as described in Tajfel's
definition. For instance, during a corporate merger where both
organizations have strong corporate identities, leadership in
organizations with knowledge of this theory will know that
there will be a great resistance to the merger as neither
organization will want to give up its social identity. As such,
they may want to proceed slowly with the merger and work on
creating a shared identity before an official merger occurs so
that all involved will be motivated to join the new group. See
the figure below. You may have noticed that when corporations
merge, a new name may be chosen for the new combined
organization.

More Related Content

Similar to Work Motivation and Performance A SocialIdentity Perspectiv.docx

BMIS 570Case Study Instructions You will write a 4–5-page pape.docx
BMIS 570Case Study Instructions You will write a 4–5-page pape.docxBMIS 570Case Study Instructions You will write a 4–5-page pape.docx
BMIS 570Case Study Instructions You will write a 4–5-page pape.docx
moirarandell
 
Social Identity and Wellbeing in Orgs_vFINAL_DOC
Social Identity and Wellbeing in Orgs_vFINAL_DOCSocial Identity and Wellbeing in Orgs_vFINAL_DOC
Social Identity and Wellbeing in Orgs_vFINAL_DOC
Athanasia Varvitsioti
 
Work & Stress Vol. 22, No. 3, JulySeptember 2008, 187200.docx
Work & Stress Vol. 22, No. 3, JulySeptember 2008, 187200.docxWork & Stress Vol. 22, No. 3, JulySeptember 2008, 187200.docx
Work & Stress Vol. 22, No. 3, JulySeptember 2008, 187200.docx
troutmanboris
 
Organizational Effectiveness and Staheli West
Organizational Effectiveness and Staheli WestOrganizational Effectiveness and Staheli West
Organizational Effectiveness and Staheli West
Quinn Thurman
 
Journal of Public Affairs Education 545Expressing concern.docx
 Journal of Public Affairs Education 545Expressing concern.docx Journal of Public Affairs Education 545Expressing concern.docx
Journal of Public Affairs Education 545Expressing concern.docx
aryan532920
 
Organizational behaviour_Team Development
Organizational behaviour_Team DevelopmentOrganizational behaviour_Team Development
Organizational behaviour_Team Development
Kanav N. Sahgal
 
Mediating Effect of Reputation on the Relationship between Interpersonal Skil...
Mediating Effect of Reputation on the Relationship between Interpersonal Skil...Mediating Effect of Reputation on the Relationship between Interpersonal Skil...
Mediating Effect of Reputation on the Relationship between Interpersonal Skil...
paperpublications3
 
Personality and Career Choice
Personality and Career ChoicePersonality and Career Choice
Personality and Career Choice
Margarit Davtian
 

Similar to Work Motivation and Performance A SocialIdentity Perspectiv.docx (20)

Literature Review
Literature ReviewLiterature Review
Literature Review
 
BMIS 570Case Study Instructions You will write a 4–5-page pape.docx
BMIS 570Case Study Instructions You will write a 4–5-page pape.docxBMIS 570Case Study Instructions You will write a 4–5-page pape.docx
BMIS 570Case Study Instructions You will write a 4–5-page pape.docx
 
Kanika homer meeting presentation
Kanika homer meeting presentationKanika homer meeting presentation
Kanika homer meeting presentation
 
Social Identity and Wellbeing in Orgs_vFINAL_DOC
Social Identity and Wellbeing in Orgs_vFINAL_DOCSocial Identity and Wellbeing in Orgs_vFINAL_DOC
Social Identity and Wellbeing in Orgs_vFINAL_DOC
 
Work & Stress Vol. 22, No. 3, JulySeptember 2008, 187200.docx
Work & Stress Vol. 22, No. 3, JulySeptember 2008, 187200.docxWork & Stress Vol. 22, No. 3, JulySeptember 2008, 187200.docx
Work & Stress Vol. 22, No. 3, JulySeptember 2008, 187200.docx
 
Acting As If Differentiating Entrepreneurial From Organizational Behavior
Acting As If  Differentiating Entrepreneurial From Organizational BehaviorActing As If  Differentiating Entrepreneurial From Organizational Behavior
Acting As If Differentiating Entrepreneurial From Organizational Behavior
 
Change organization
Change organizationChange organization
Change organization
 
Perception of civil servants on performance : An Emperical Analysis of Indone...
Perception of civil servants on performance : An Emperical Analysis of Indone...Perception of civil servants on performance : An Emperical Analysis of Indone...
Perception of civil servants on performance : An Emperical Analysis of Indone...
 
Social Identity
Social IdentitySocial Identity
Social Identity
 
Coursepack obii pg09
Coursepack obii pg09Coursepack obii pg09
Coursepack obii pg09
 
Groups and their effects.....pdf
Groups and their effects.....pdfGroups and their effects.....pdf
Groups and their effects.....pdf
 
Organizational Effectiveness and Staheli West
Organizational Effectiveness and Staheli WestOrganizational Effectiveness and Staheli West
Organizational Effectiveness and Staheli West
 
Arvind saini y19282008
Arvind saini y19282008Arvind saini y19282008
Arvind saini y19282008
 
Attitudes, Values and Organizational Culture: Disentangling the Concepts
Attitudes, Values and Organizational Culture: Disentangling the ConceptsAttitudes, Values and Organizational Culture: Disentangling the Concepts
Attitudes, Values and Organizational Culture: Disentangling the Concepts
 
Journal of Public Affairs Education 545Expressing concern.docx
 Journal of Public Affairs Education 545Expressing concern.docx Journal of Public Affairs Education 545Expressing concern.docx
Journal of Public Affairs Education 545Expressing concern.docx
 
Organizational behaviour_Team Development
Organizational behaviour_Team DevelopmentOrganizational behaviour_Team Development
Organizational behaviour_Team Development
 
To stay or not to stay can organizational culture provide the staying power
To stay or not to stay can organizational culture provide the staying powerTo stay or not to stay can organizational culture provide the staying power
To stay or not to stay can organizational culture provide the staying power
 
What is organizational development
What is organizational developmentWhat is organizational development
What is organizational development
 
Mediating Effect of Reputation on the Relationship between Interpersonal Skil...
Mediating Effect of Reputation on the Relationship between Interpersonal Skil...Mediating Effect of Reputation on the Relationship between Interpersonal Skil...
Mediating Effect of Reputation on the Relationship between Interpersonal Skil...
 
Personality and Career Choice
Personality and Career ChoicePersonality and Career Choice
Personality and Career Choice
 

More from ambersalomon88660

1. Koffman Corporation is trying to raise capital. What method wou.docx
1. Koffman Corporation is trying to raise capital. What method wou.docx1. Koffman Corporation is trying to raise capital. What method wou.docx
1. Koffman Corporation is trying to raise capital. What method wou.docx
ambersalomon88660
 
1. Know the terminology flash cards.2. Know the hist.docx
1. Know the terminology flash cards.2. Know the hist.docx1. Know the terminology flash cards.2. Know the hist.docx
1. Know the terminology flash cards.2. Know the hist.docx
ambersalomon88660
 
1. Journal Entry The attached (BUROS Center for Testing).docx
1. Journal Entry  The attached (BUROS Center for Testing).docx1. Journal Entry  The attached (BUROS Center for Testing).docx
1. Journal Entry The attached (BUROS Center for Testing).docx
ambersalomon88660
 
1. Is it important the hospital to have a licensure to ensure that.docx
1. Is it important the hospital to have a licensure to ensure that.docx1. Is it important the hospital to have a licensure to ensure that.docx
1. Is it important the hospital to have a licensure to ensure that.docx
ambersalomon88660
 
1. INTRODUCTION In recent years, energy harvesting fro.docx
1. INTRODUCTION In recent years, energy harvesting fro.docx1. INTRODUCTION In recent years, energy harvesting fro.docx
1. INTRODUCTION In recent years, energy harvesting fro.docx
ambersalomon88660
 
1. INTRODUCTIONThe rapid of economic growth in China, is a fou.docx
1. INTRODUCTIONThe rapid of economic growth in China, is a fou.docx1. INTRODUCTIONThe rapid of economic growth in China, is a fou.docx
1. INTRODUCTIONThe rapid of economic growth in China, is a fou.docx
ambersalomon88660
 
1. Introduction 1. Technology and communication 1. Technology .docx
1. Introduction 1. Technology and communication 1. Technology .docx1. Introduction 1. Technology and communication 1. Technology .docx
1. Introduction 1. Technology and communication 1. Technology .docx
ambersalomon88660
 
1. In your definition of a well-run company, how important a.docx
1. In your definition of a well-run company, how important a.docx1. In your definition of a well-run company, how important a.docx
1. In your definition of a well-run company, how important a.docx
ambersalomon88660
 
1. In Chapter four titled Academy Training you learned about academi.docx
1. In Chapter four titled Academy Training you learned about academi.docx1. In Chapter four titled Academy Training you learned about academi.docx
1. In Chapter four titled Academy Training you learned about academi.docx
ambersalomon88660
 
1. In 200 words, describe how Hamlet promotes andor subverts th.docx
1. In 200 words, describe how Hamlet promotes andor subverts th.docx1. In 200 words, describe how Hamlet promotes andor subverts th.docx
1. In 200 words, describe how Hamlet promotes andor subverts th.docx
ambersalomon88660
 
1. Image 1 courtesy of httpswww.virginiahospitalcenter.com.docx
1. Image 1 courtesy of httpswww.virginiahospitalcenter.com.docx1. Image 1 courtesy of httpswww.virginiahospitalcenter.com.docx
1. Image 1 courtesy of httpswww.virginiahospitalcenter.com.docx
ambersalomon88660
 
1. If I were to create an SEL program, I would focus on self-awar.docx
1. If I were to create an SEL program, I would focus on self-awar.docx1. If I were to create an SEL program, I would focus on self-awar.docx
1. If I were to create an SEL program, I would focus on self-awar.docx
ambersalomon88660
 
1. I think that the top three management positions in a health pla.docx
1. I think that the top three management positions in a health pla.docx1. I think that the top three management positions in a health pla.docx
1. I think that the top three management positions in a health pla.docx
ambersalomon88660
 

More from ambersalomon88660 (20)

1. Lists crimes and crime involvement on the Mendez brothers.2.I.docx
1. Lists crimes and crime involvement on the Mendez brothers.2.I.docx1. Lists crimes and crime involvement on the Mendez brothers.2.I.docx
1. Lists crimes and crime involvement on the Mendez brothers.2.I.docx
 
1. Lists and analyzes strengths and weaknesses based on each of th.docx
1. Lists and analyzes strengths and weaknesses based on each of th.docx1. Lists and analyzes strengths and weaknesses based on each of th.docx
1. Lists and analyzes strengths and weaknesses based on each of th.docx
 
1. List eight basic initiatives that companies can use to gain c.docx
1. List eight basic initiatives that companies can use to gain c.docx1. List eight basic initiatives that companies can use to gain c.docx
1. List eight basic initiatives that companies can use to gain c.docx
 
1. Koffman Corporation is trying to raise capital. What method wou.docx
1. Koffman Corporation is trying to raise capital. What method wou.docx1. Koffman Corporation is trying to raise capital. What method wou.docx
1. Koffman Corporation is trying to raise capital. What method wou.docx
 
1. List all the entities that interact with the TIMS system. Start b.docx
1. List all the entities that interact with the TIMS system. Start b.docx1. List all the entities that interact with the TIMS system. Start b.docx
1. List all the entities that interact with the TIMS system. Start b.docx
 
1. Know the terminology flash cards.2. Know the hist.docx
1. Know the terminology flash cards.2. Know the hist.docx1. Know the terminology flash cards.2. Know the hist.docx
1. Know the terminology flash cards.2. Know the hist.docx
 
1. Journal Entry The attached (BUROS Center for Testing).docx
1. Journal Entry  The attached (BUROS Center for Testing).docx1. Journal Entry  The attached (BUROS Center for Testing).docx
1. Journal Entry The attached (BUROS Center for Testing).docx
 
1. Introduction and thesisThrough extensive research I hope to f.docx
1. Introduction and thesisThrough extensive research I hope to f.docx1. Introduction and thesisThrough extensive research I hope to f.docx
1. Introduction and thesisThrough extensive research I hope to f.docx
 
1. Is it important the hospital to have a licensure to ensure that.docx
1. Is it important the hospital to have a licensure to ensure that.docx1. Is it important the hospital to have a licensure to ensure that.docx
1. Is it important the hospital to have a licensure to ensure that.docx
 
1. INTRODUCTION In recent years, energy harvesting fro.docx
1. INTRODUCTION In recent years, energy harvesting fro.docx1. INTRODUCTION In recent years, energy harvesting fro.docx
1. INTRODUCTION In recent years, energy harvesting fro.docx
 
1. INTRODUCTIONThe rapid of economic growth in China, is a fou.docx
1. INTRODUCTIONThe rapid of economic growth in China, is a fou.docx1. INTRODUCTIONThe rapid of economic growth in China, is a fou.docx
1. INTRODUCTIONThe rapid of economic growth in China, is a fou.docx
 
1. Introduction to the Topica. What is outsourcingi. Ty.docx
1. Introduction to the Topica. What is outsourcingi. Ty.docx1. Introduction to the Topica. What is outsourcingi. Ty.docx
1. Introduction to the Topica. What is outsourcingi. Ty.docx
 
1. Introduction 1. Technology and communication 1. Technology .docx
1. Introduction 1. Technology and communication 1. Technology .docx1. Introduction 1. Technology and communication 1. Technology .docx
1. Introduction 1. Technology and communication 1. Technology .docx
 
1. In your definition of a well-run company, how important a.docx
1. In your definition of a well-run company, how important a.docx1. In your definition of a well-run company, how important a.docx
1. In your definition of a well-run company, how important a.docx
 
1. In Chapter four titled Academy Training you learned about academi.docx
1. In Chapter four titled Academy Training you learned about academi.docx1. In Chapter four titled Academy Training you learned about academi.docx
1. In Chapter four titled Academy Training you learned about academi.docx
 
1. In 200 words, describe how Hamlet promotes andor subverts th.docx
1. In 200 words, describe how Hamlet promotes andor subverts th.docx1. In 200 words, describe how Hamlet promotes andor subverts th.docx
1. In 200 words, describe how Hamlet promotes andor subverts th.docx
 
1. Image 1 courtesy of httpswww.virginiahospitalcenter.com.docx
1. Image 1 courtesy of httpswww.virginiahospitalcenter.com.docx1. Image 1 courtesy of httpswww.virginiahospitalcenter.com.docx
1. Image 1 courtesy of httpswww.virginiahospitalcenter.com.docx
 
1. If I were to create an SEL program, I would focus on self-awar.docx
1. If I were to create an SEL program, I would focus on self-awar.docx1. If I were to create an SEL program, I would focus on self-awar.docx
1. If I were to create an SEL program, I would focus on self-awar.docx
 
1. Identify and discuss the factors that contribute to heritage cons.docx
1. Identify and discuss the factors that contribute to heritage cons.docx1. Identify and discuss the factors that contribute to heritage cons.docx
1. Identify and discuss the factors that contribute to heritage cons.docx
 
1. I think that the top three management positions in a health pla.docx
1. I think that the top three management positions in a health pla.docx1. I think that the top three management positions in a health pla.docx
1. I think that the top three management positions in a health pla.docx
 

Recently uploaded

會考英文會考英文會考英文會考英文會考英文會考英文會考英文會考英文會考英文會考英文會考英文
會考英文會考英文會考英文會考英文會考英文會考英文會考英文會考英文會考英文會考英文會考英文會考英文會考英文會考英文會考英文會考英文會考英文會考英文會考英文會考英文會考英文會考英文
會考英文會考英文會考英文會考英文會考英文會考英文會考英文會考英文會考英文會考英文會考英文
中 央社
 

Recently uploaded (20)

Open Educational Resources Primer PowerPoint
Open Educational Resources Primer PowerPointOpen Educational Resources Primer PowerPoint
Open Educational Resources Primer PowerPoint
 
An overview of the various scriptures in Hinduism
An overview of the various scriptures in HinduismAn overview of the various scriptures in Hinduism
An overview of the various scriptures in Hinduism
 
2024_Student Session 2_ Set Plan Preparation.pptx
2024_Student Session 2_ Set Plan Preparation.pptx2024_Student Session 2_ Set Plan Preparation.pptx
2024_Student Session 2_ Set Plan Preparation.pptx
 
Morse OER Some Benefits and Challenges.pptx
Morse OER Some Benefits and Challenges.pptxMorse OER Some Benefits and Challenges.pptx
Morse OER Some Benefits and Challenges.pptx
 
size separation d pharm 1st year pharmaceutics
size separation d pharm 1st year pharmaceuticssize separation d pharm 1st year pharmaceutics
size separation d pharm 1st year pharmaceutics
 
“O BEIJO” EM ARTE .
“O BEIJO” EM ARTE                       .“O BEIJO” EM ARTE                       .
“O BEIJO” EM ARTE .
 
Championnat de France de Tennis de table/
Championnat de France de Tennis de table/Championnat de France de Tennis de table/
Championnat de France de Tennis de table/
 
Navigating the Misinformation Minefield: The Role of Higher Education in the ...
Navigating the Misinformation Minefield: The Role of Higher Education in the ...Navigating the Misinformation Minefield: The Role of Higher Education in the ...
Navigating the Misinformation Minefield: The Role of Higher Education in the ...
 
TỔNG HỢP HƠN 100 ĐỀ THI THỬ TỐT NGHIỆP THPT VẬT LÝ 2024 - TỪ CÁC TRƯỜNG, TRƯ...
TỔNG HỢP HƠN 100 ĐỀ THI THỬ TỐT NGHIỆP THPT VẬT LÝ 2024 - TỪ CÁC TRƯỜNG, TRƯ...TỔNG HỢP HƠN 100 ĐỀ THI THỬ TỐT NGHIỆP THPT VẬT LÝ 2024 - TỪ CÁC TRƯỜNG, TRƯ...
TỔNG HỢP HƠN 100 ĐỀ THI THỬ TỐT NGHIỆP THPT VẬT LÝ 2024 - TỪ CÁC TRƯỜNG, TRƯ...
 
The Ball Poem- John Berryman_20240518_001617_0000.pptx
The Ball Poem- John Berryman_20240518_001617_0000.pptxThe Ball Poem- John Berryman_20240518_001617_0000.pptx
The Ball Poem- John Berryman_20240518_001617_0000.pptx
 
Operations Management - Book1.p - Dr. Abdulfatah A. Salem
Operations Management - Book1.p  - Dr. Abdulfatah A. SalemOperations Management - Book1.p  - Dr. Abdulfatah A. Salem
Operations Management - Book1.p - Dr. Abdulfatah A. Salem
 
factors influencing drug absorption-final-2.pptx
factors influencing drug absorption-final-2.pptxfactors influencing drug absorption-final-2.pptx
factors influencing drug absorption-final-2.pptx
 
Basic Civil Engineering notes on Transportation Engineering, Modes of Transpo...
Basic Civil Engineering notes on Transportation Engineering, Modes of Transpo...Basic Civil Engineering notes on Transportation Engineering, Modes of Transpo...
Basic Civil Engineering notes on Transportation Engineering, Modes of Transpo...
 
MichaelStarkes_UncutGemsProjectSummary.pdf
MichaelStarkes_UncutGemsProjectSummary.pdfMichaelStarkes_UncutGemsProjectSummary.pdf
MichaelStarkes_UncutGemsProjectSummary.pdf
 
How to Manage Closest Location in Odoo 17 Inventory
How to Manage Closest Location in Odoo 17 InventoryHow to Manage Closest Location in Odoo 17 Inventory
How to Manage Closest Location in Odoo 17 Inventory
 
An Overview of the Odoo 17 Discuss App.pptx
An Overview of the Odoo 17 Discuss App.pptxAn Overview of the Odoo 17 Discuss App.pptx
An Overview of the Odoo 17 Discuss App.pptx
 
會考英文會考英文會考英文會考英文會考英文會考英文會考英文會考英文會考英文會考英文會考英文
會考英文會考英文會考英文會考英文會考英文會考英文會考英文會考英文會考英文會考英文會考英文會考英文會考英文會考英文會考英文會考英文會考英文會考英文會考英文會考英文會考英文會考英文
會考英文會考英文會考英文會考英文會考英文會考英文會考英文會考英文會考英文會考英文會考英文
 
Features of Video Calls in the Discuss Module in Odoo 17
Features of Video Calls in the Discuss Module in Odoo 17Features of Video Calls in the Discuss Module in Odoo 17
Features of Video Calls in the Discuss Module in Odoo 17
 
....................Muslim-Law notes.pdf
....................Muslim-Law notes.pdf....................Muslim-Law notes.pdf
....................Muslim-Law notes.pdf
 
Exploring Gemini AI and Integration with MuleSoft | MuleSoft Mysore Meetup #45
Exploring Gemini AI and Integration with MuleSoft | MuleSoft Mysore Meetup #45Exploring Gemini AI and Integration with MuleSoft | MuleSoft Mysore Meetup #45
Exploring Gemini AI and Integration with MuleSoft | MuleSoft Mysore Meetup #45
 

Work Motivation and Performance A SocialIdentity Perspectiv.docx

  • 1. Work Motivation and Performance: A Social Identity Perspective Daan van Knippenberg University of Amsterdam, The Netherlands La motivation au travail et les performances ont e te analyse es dans la per- spective de la the orie de l'identite sociale et de celle de l'auto-cate gorisation. Centrale aÁ cette analyse se trouve la relation entre l'identification organisa- tionnelle et la motivation d'exercer un effort pour le compte de la collectivite . Une analyse the orique et une revue d' e tudes empiriques relatives aux relations entre cette identification organisationnelle et la motivation et les performances conduisent aÁ la conclusion: l'identification est lie e positivement aÁ la motivation au travail, avec les performances dans la re alisation des taà ches et dans le contexte, pour autant a) que l'identite sociale soit saillante et que b) les performances e leve es soient percË ues comme e tant dans l'inte reà t du groupe ou de l'organisation. Work motivation and performance were analysed from the perspective of social identity theory and self-categorisation theory. Central in this analysis
  • 2. is the relation of organisational identification with the motivation to exert effort on behalf of the collective. A theoretical analysis as well as a review of empirical studies of the relationship of organisational identification with motivation and performance leads to the conclusion that identification is positively related to work motivation, task performance, and contextual performance to the extent that (a) social identity is salient, and (b) high performance is perceived to be in the group's or organisation's interest. INTRODUCTION The study of work motivation and performance forms one of the key issues in research in organisational behaviour. Although a variety of motivations may affect performance, and performance may be contingent on a multitude of other factors, one of the more important factors affecting performance ________________ * Address for correspondence: Daan van Knippenberg, University of Amsterdam, Work & Organizational Psychology, Roetersstraat 15, 1018 WB Amsterdam, The Netherlands. Email: [email protected] I wish to thank Alexander Haslam, Moshe Krausz, Barbara van
  • 3. Knippenberg, Robert Wood, and an anonymous reviewer for their comments on previous drafts of this paper. APPLIED PSYCHOLOGY: AN INTERNATIONAL REVIEW, 2000, 49 (3), 357±371 # International Association for Applied Psychology, 2000. Published by Blackwell Publishers, 108 Cowley Road, Oxford OX4 1JF, UK and 350 Main Street, Malden, MA 02148, USA. arguably is the motivation to perform well on the job. The present study focuses on work motivation, and analyses it from the perspective of social identity theory (Hogg & Abrams, 1988; Tajfel, 1978; Tajfel & Turner, 1986) and self-categorisation theory, which is an elaboration and extension of social identity theory (Turner, 1985; Turner, Hogg, Oakes, Reicher, & Wetherell, 1987). In the following, the theoretical backgrounds to the social identity approach (i.e. including both social identity theory and self- categorisation theory) to motivation and performance are presented, the proposition that organisational identification affects work motivation and performance is derived from this approach, and empirical evidence in support of this proposition is discussed.
  • 4. THEORETICAL BACKGROUNDS The social identity approach outlines how membership in social groups affects the self-concept. Central to the approach is the proposition that through social identification, the perception of oneness with or belongingness to a group (see Mael & Ashforth, 1992), individuals define themselves in terms of their group membership and ascribe characteristics that are typical of the group to the self. The concept of social identity, `̀ that part of an individual's self-concept which derives from his knowledge of his membership of a group (or groups) together with the value and the emotional significance attached to the membership'' (Tajfel, 1978, p. 63), reflects this internalisation of group membership as part of `̀ who you are'' (Ashforth & Mael, 1989; Haslam, in press). Identification leads individuals to perceive themselves in terms of the characteristics they share with other members of their ingroupsÐtheir shared social identityÐrather than in terms of the idiosyncratic characteristics that differentiate them from other indivi- dualsÐtheir personal identity (Turner et al., 1987). Identification thus blurs the distinction between self and group, and turns the group, psychologically, into a part of the self (Smith & Henry, 1996). This `̀ social'' or `̀
  • 5. collective'' self lies at the heart of the perceptual, attitudinal, and behavioural effects of group membership. The more one conceives of oneself in terms of one's membership in a group (i.e. the more one identifies with the group), the more likely one is to act in accordance with the group's beliefs, norms, and values, and generally to act in `̀ group-typical'' ways (Ashforth & Mael, 1989; Dutton, Dukerich, & Harquail, 1994; Hogg & Abrams, 1988; Turner et al., 1987). Even though identification with a group may lead individuals to act in group-typical ways, this does not mean that individuals who identify with a group always act in accordance with the social identity based in that group membership. The influence of identification is contingent on social identity being salient or cognitively activated. That is, even if an individual identifies 358 VAN KNIPPENBERG # International Association for Applied Psychology, 2000. with a group, that does not mean the individual is always highly aware of this group membership, and group membership only affects attitudes
  • 6. and behaviour to the extent that the individual is `̀ made aware'' of the membership in the group. Although identification itself may contribute to social identity salience (Haslam, in press), contextual factors affect salience as well. Roughly speaking, any `̀ event'' that speaks to a group membership (i.e. rather than to the individual qua individual) may make the social identity based in that group membership salient. The prospect of a merger may, for instance, render organisational identity salient (van Knippenberg, van Knippenberg, Monden, & de Lima, 1999), conflict or competition between work groups may render work group identity salient (Kramer, 1991), and the addition of individuals from another ethnicity to a previously ethnicly homogenous work group may focus on ethnic identity. Identity salience may endure for longer periods of time or may change in a matter of moments, for instance when departmental identity is salient in an inter- departmental meeting, and a telephone call from home immediately after the meeting focuses on family identity (for a more elaborate discussion of the salience of social identities in organisations, see e.g. Haslam, in this issue; Hogg & Terry, 2000; Kramer, 1991). As the group seems to be where organisational behaviour primarily takes
  • 7. place, and organisations themselves may, from a social psychological per- spective, be viewed as social groups, the relevance of the social identity approach to the study of organisational behaviour is readily apparent. Social identity theory has, more or less from its conception, been applied to the study of organisational behaviour (e.g. Brown, 1978), but the study of social identity processes in organisations has only gained momentum in recent years. The social identity approach has been applied in several areas of organisational behaviour research, such as intergroup relations within the organisation (Brown, Condor, Mathews, Wade, & Williams, 1986; Kramer, 1991), mergers and acquisitions (Terry & Callan, 1998; van Knippenberg et al., 1999), group cohesiveness (Hogg, 1993; Hogg, Cooper-Shaw, & Holzworth, 1993), organisational demography (Ely, 1994; Tsui, Egan, & O'Reilly, 1992), leadership (Haslam, McGarty, Brown, Eggins, Morrison, & Reynolds, 1998; Hogg, Hains, & Mason, 1998), promotion decisions (Fajak & Haslam, 1998), and turnover (Mael & Ashforth, 1995). Most relevant to the present discussion, the social identity approach also has clear implications for work motivation and performance. IDENTIFICATION, MOTIVATION, AND PERFORMANCE
  • 8. Membership in an organisation implies multiple group memberships: membership in the organisation as a whole, in one's own department, in one's own team or work unit, and so forth (see Ashforth & Mael, 1989; IDENTIFICATION, MOTIVATION, AND PERFORMANCE 359 # International Association for Applied Psychology, 2000. van Knippenberg & van Schie, in press; for a discussion of the multiple foci of organisational identification). An obvious question from the point of view of the social identity approach is how these identifications affect organisational behaviour, and especiallyÐgiven that this is so central to organisational lifeÐwork motivation and performance. The answer to this question is that identification motivates group members to work for the group's interests, which in turn may affect performance. These arguments are elaborated below. Identification elicits a sense of oneness with the target of identification. This psychological oneness with the group (or organisation) induces indi- viduals to take the group's perspective and to experience the group's goals and interests as their own (see Dutton et al., 1994). Thus,
  • 9. identification will generally be associated with a motivation to achieve group goals and work for the group's interest. Yet, whether identification with the organisation or within-organisation subunit (i.e. team, work group) actually results in higher performance at work is contingent on a number of other factors. First, as outlined above, identification with the group or organisation will only affect attitudes and behaviour to the extent that social identity is salient. Thus, identification will only result in the motivation to exert effort on behalf of the collective to the extent that group or organisational identity is salient (see also Haslam, in this issue). Second, the relationship between identification and motivation to exert effort on behalf of the collective may be positive (although the strength of the relationship will be contingent on social identity salience), but this does not necessarily mean that identification results in work motivation. That is, we should distinguish between the motivation to exert effort on behalf of the collective and the motivation to perform well on the job (i.e. work moti- vation). Even though identification may be associated with the willingness to exert effort on behalf of the collective, whether this results in work motivation is contingent on what the goals and interests of that
  • 10. collective are perceived to be. Identification may only be expected to be positively related to work motivation if high performance is perceived to be in the collective's interest. Only then will motivation to exert effort on behalf of the collective translate to motivation to perform well on the job. Performance standards may, however, not be particularly high, clear, or salient as it is not always clear in organisations or jobs what is expected or desired in terms of performance, and high performance may not be a goal. Moreover, performance standards may be different at the organisational and subunit level, as arises when the organisation advocates high performance, while informal work group norms promote minimisation of work efforts or a focus on interpersonal relations. If the primary target of identification is not perceived to have a commitment to high performance, identification is unlikely to result in striving to enhance performance. 360 VAN KNIPPENBERG # International Association for Applied Psychology, 2000. To the extent that identification results in work motivation (i.e. con- tingent on social identity salience and the perceived goals and
  • 11. interests of the target of identification) it will have a positive effect on performance. This should hold to the extent that performance is under volitional control (e.g. to the extent that performance is contingent on effort and persistence) rather than on, for example, knowledge, skills, abilities, or available resources (because this qualification is not specific for the social identity analysis, but holds for the relationship between work motivation and performance in general, it receives no further attention here). The analysis presented above is modelled in Fig. 1. In this model, identi- fication affects the motivation to exert effort on behalf of the collective to the extent that social identity is salient, and this motivation affects work motivation to the extent that the collective's interests are perceived to be performance-related. Finally, work motivation is related to performance to the extent that performance is under volitional control. Note that whereas the relationships between identification and the motivation to exert effort on behalf of the collective, and between work motivation and performance, are assumed to be positive (although moderated by social identity salience and the extent to which performance is under volitional control, respectively), the relationship between the motivation to exert effort on behalf
  • 12. of the collective and work motivation may be either positive, negative, or zero contingent on the perceived goals and interests of the collective. In the following, I review empirical studies to evaluate the model. In order to unambiguously assess the merits of the model and to avoid confusion with the effects of other concepts reflecting psychological attachment, like organisational commitment (Allen & Meyer, 1990; Mowday, Steers, & Porter, 1979) or dispositional group loyalty (James & Cropanzano, 1994), or with other conceptualisations of identification (e.g. O'Reilly & Chatman, 1986), this review is limited to studies that explicitly focus on identification as defined in the social identity approach. Moreover, it is limited to studies that measured work motivation or performance and does not address studies of related attitudes or behaviour (e.g. Mael & Ashforth's, 1992, study of organisational identification and support for the organisation; see Haslam, in press, for a review of studies of the relationship of identification with a broader range of work attitudes and behaviour). Where performance is concerned, a distinction is made between `̀ task performance'' and `̀ contextual performance''. Whereas task performance refers to performance on the job that `̀ you were hired to do'',
  • 13. contextual performance (cf. organisational citizenship behaviour; Organ, 1997) refers to behaviour that supports the organisational, social, and psychological environment in which task performance takes place, like helping others, taking others' interests into consideration, and presenting a positive image of the organisation to outsiders (e.g. Borman & Motowidlo, 1993; Moorman & IDENTIFICATION, MOTIVATION, AND PERFORMANCE 361 # International Association for Applied Psychology, 2000. FIGURE 1. A social identity model of work motivation and performance. 3 6 2 V A N K N IP P E N B E
  • 15. 0 0 0 . Blakely, 1995; Organ, 1988). This distinction is of interest for two reasons. First, contextual performance may be assumed to be more under volitional control than task performance (Organ, 1988), because behaviours like helping others and taking others' interests into consideration are generally less contingent on skills, ability, resources, and so on, than task perform- ance. Moreover, contextual performance more or less by definition refers to behaviours that individuals are not required to perform, whereas task performance is part of job requirements. As a consequence, an employee can more easily refrain from contextual than from task behaviours. Second, task performance is more likely to benefit the self (e.g. in terms of career opportunities or bonuses) than contextual performance, and may therefore be less contingent on group-oriented motivations. These considerations suggest that the effects of identification may be more apparent on con- textual than on task performance. Moreover, contextual performance may benefit the task performance of the work unit as a whole
  • 16. (Podsakoff, Ahearne, & MacKenzie, 1997), even if it does not benefit the task perform- ance of the individual engaging in the contextual behaviours. Even so, it should be noted that although contextual performance may be in the collective's best interest, individuals need not perceive it as such, and may engage in contextual behaviours for other reasons than wanting to contri- bute to the collective interest (e.g. because they want to help a friend). Thus, the relationship between identification and contextual performance may not be straightforward either. Identification andWork Motivation To the author's knowledge, only one study has assessed the relationship between organisational identification and work motivation (van Knippen- berg & van Schie, in press). In a survey of work attitudes of the employees of a university, van Knippenberg and van Schie assessed work motivation (with Hackman & Lawler's, 1971, internal motivation scale) and both work group identification and organisational identification (with Mael and Ash- forth's, 1992, organisational identification questionnaire). In addition, the study included a measure of job involvement with items like `̀ I am always prepared to do my best'', which seems to come close to a
  • 17. measure of work motivation as defined here (i.e. as the willingness to exert effort to perform well). Work group identification was found to be related to work motivation (r = 0.30, P50.001) and both work group identification (r = 0.46, P50.001) and organisational identification (r = 0.27, P50.01) were related to the job involvement measure. Although only a cross-sectional study in which all data were gathered with the same questionnaire, this does corroborate the proposition that identification may be associated with the motivation to exert effort on the job. IDENTIFICATION, MOTIVATION, AND PERFORMANCE 363 # International Association for Applied Psychology, 2000. Identification andTask Performance Evidence regarding the relationship between identification and task performance comes from a series of laboratory studies that all used simple tasks on which performance may be assumed to be highly dependent on effort and persistence rather than skills, knowledge, or ability. Performance on these tasks may thus be regarded as indicative of work motivation. Concerning the role of social identity salience and the
  • 18. perception that high performance is in the group's best interest, a study by James and Greenberg (1989) is especially relevant. James and Greenberg experimentally manipu- lated the salience of students' membership in their university by presenting half of the participants with cues that rendered their university membership salient (the university colours in the first experiment, reference to the uni- versity mascot in the second study). In addition, James and Greenberg told their participants that the performance of students from their university would be compared to the performance of students from another university, thus implicitly suggesting that high performance would be beneficial to the university's image (Experiment 1), or manipulated whether such inter- group comparison was implied or not (Experiment 2). Under these different experimental conditions, participants performed a simple anagram task. As predicted, participants in the high identification condition solved more anagrams, but only when intergroup comparison was salient. James and Greenberg did not assess identification, but Worchel, Rothgerber, Day, Hart, and Butemeyer (1998) included a measure of identification in a study that basically replicated the James and Greenberg study. Worchel et al. (Exp. 3) let participants work on an assembly task (making a
  • 19. chain out of strips of paper) either in the presence of another work group or not and either wearing group uniforms (same-colour lab coats) or not. Individual performance was higher when group members were wearing group uniforms (i.e. social identity was salient), but only when the other group was present. Moreover, mediation analysis indicated that the effects of the experimental manipulations were partly mediated by identification. Results of these studies support the proposition that identification results in increased work motivation and task performance, provided social identity is salient (see also Pilegge & Holtz, 1997), and high performance is seen as serving the group's interests. Evidence that identification may induce individuals to experience the group's interest as self-interest (i.e. social self-interest rather than personal self-interest) comes from a study by van Leeuwen and van Knippenberg (1999). Van Leeuwen and van Knippenberg investigated the effects on performance of work group identification, prosocial versus proself value orientations (an individual difference variable), and expectations about other group members' effort. Participants worked on a simple group task (putting sheets of paper in envelopes). Identification with the work group
  • 20. 364 VAN KNIPPENBERG # International Association for Applied Psychology, 2000. and value orientations were measured and expectations about other group members' task effort were manipulated. Van Leeuwen and van Knippenberg predicted that identification would motivate group members to exert effort on the group task, especially for group members who were not disposi- tionally inclined to take others' interests into account (i.e. individuals with proself as compared with prosocial value orientations). Results supported these predictions. Performance (number of envelopes filled) was higher for participants who identified more with their work group, and this effect was stronger for group members with a proself rather than a prosocial orientation. Performance was also affected by expectations about the effort other group members would invest in the group task. Higher expected effort from others resulted in higher own performance, but more so for partici- pants with relatively low identification. High identifiers were motivated to perform more or less irrespective of the effort and performance they expected from their fellow group members. These results show
  • 21. that identi- fication is positively related to performance, even if it means exerting effort on behalf of the group while other group members are taking it easy. Moreover, and of more importance to the present discussion, the fact that identification was primarily related to performance of individuals who dispositionally focus on personal self-interest showed that identification may elicit the motivation to exert effort on behalf of the collective because it leads individuals to experience the collective's interests as self- interest, albeit social self-interest and not personal self-interest. Identification and Contextual Performance Only one study provides an empirical test of the relationship between identification as defined in the social identity approach and contextual performance (van Knippenberg & Sleebos, 1999). In a survey of university faculty, van Knippenberg and Sleebos measured identification (using the Mael & Ashforth scale), individualism±collectivism as an individual differ- ence variable (with Wagner & Moch's, 1986, measure), and affective commit- ment (with Allen & Meyer's, 1990, scale). Two weeks later they assessed self-reported contextual performance. This second survey focused on two dimensions: altruism, the helping of coworkers in their job
  • 22. when needed, and generalised compliance with norms defining `̀ what a good employee ought to do'' (Organ, 1988). Identification was found to be positively related to self-reports of both altruism and generalised compliance, although for generalised compliance this was only true for more individually oriented as compared with more collectively oriented employees. The fact that identification was positively related to self-reports of con- textual performance obviously supports the social identity analysis of moti- vation and performance. It should, however, be noted that the relationship IDENTIFICATION, MOTIVATION, AND PERFORMANCE 365 # International Association for Applied Psychology, 2000. established in the van Knippenberg and Sleebos study is one between identification and self-reports of behaviour, and not between identification and actual performance (e.g. as in the previous section). Thus, this relation- ship still awaits a more conclusive test. The finding that the relationship between identification and (self-reported) contextual performance was in part moderated by individualism±collectivism is also highly relevant to the
  • 23. present discussion. Individualism±collectivism as an individual difference variable refers to a dimension that distinguishes individuals who consider their personal interests more important than the interests of the group (individualists) from individuals who are primarily focused on the interests of the group and would pursue group goals at the possible expense of their personal interests (collectivists). In other words, more collectivistically oriented individuals are dispositionally more inclined to exert effort on behalf of the group. Indeed, individualism±collectivism as an individual difference variable was found to be related to both task performance on group tasks (e.g. Wagner, 1995) and to contextual performance ratings (Moorman & Blakely, 1995). Analogous to van Leeuwen and van Knippenberg's (1999) reasoning concerning the interaction between identification and social value orientation, van Knippenberg and Sleebos argued that individualism± collectivism moderated the relationship between identification and perform- ance ratings, because identification leads individuals to experience the collective interest as self-interest (i.e. again, social rather than personal self- interest). This should affect individualists, who primarily focus on self- interest, to a greater extent than collectivists, for whom group interest
  • 24. generally prevails over personal interest. Although not directly related to the social identity analysis of motivation and performance, one other finding from the van Knippenberg and Sleebos study is of interest here. Identification is conceptually similar to the concept of affective commitment, `̀ the employee's emotional attachment to, identi- fication with, and involvement in, the organisation'' (Allen & Meyer, 1990, p. 1; cf. Mowday, Steers, & Porter, 1979). In view of this conceptual simi- larity (especially the reference to identification), one may wonder whether identification as defined in the social identity approach is not just a `̀ new name for an old concept''. Addressing this issue, Ashforth and Mael (1989) argue that despite the apparent similarity, commitment and identification are different, because identification has a cognitive, self- definitional com- ponent (in addition to identification's affective component; Karasawa, 1991; Tajfel, 1978) that affective commitment has not (see also Pratt, 1998). Corroborating this conceptual distinction, Mael (1988, in Ashforth & Mael, 1989; Mael & Tetrick, 1992) showed that identification as measured by Mael and Ashforth's (1992) organisational identification questionnaire is empirically distinguishable from commitment measured with Mowday
  • 25. et al.'s (1979) Organisational Commitment Questionnaire. Extending these 366 VAN KNIPPENBERG # International Association for Applied Psychology, 2000. findings, results from Confirmatory Factor Analysis in the van Knippen- berg and Sleebos study showed that identification is also empirically dis- tinguishable from commitment as assessed with Allen and Meyer's (1990) Affective Commitment Scale. The fit of a model with two correlated factors (w2 (76) = 125.71, P50.0001, NFI = 0.850, NNFI = 0.920, CFI = 0.934) was superior to that of a model with one single identification/commitment factor (w2 (77) = 162.25, P50.001, NFI = 0.807, NNFI = 0.865, CFI = 0.886). Obviously, this is no proof that the difference between the two lies in the self-definitional aspect of identification, but it does show that identification and commitment are different concepts. CONCLUSIONS The number of studies of the relationship of identification with motivation and performance is as yet rather small, but these studies do yield converging evidence in support of the proposed social identity model of
  • 26. motivation and performance, and corroborate the conclusion that identification is positively related to work motivation, task performance, and contextual performance to the extent that (a) social identity is salient, and (b) high performance is perceived to be in the group's or organisation's interest. Even so, some of the relationships proposed by the model are not tested yet, only implicated by the results of the studies reviewed, or only established correlationally rather than experimentally. Therefore, more extensive testing of the rela- tions proposed in the model seems in order before more definite conclusions about the merits of the social identity approach to motivation and perform- ance may be drawn. The relationship between social identity processes and performance on simple tasks (i.e. where performance is highly dependent on motivation) seems to be well established in the experimental research reviewed. As proposed in the model, but not addressed in the studies reviewed here, performance on more complex tasks (i.e. tasks on which performance is less contingent on motivation alone) may only be contingent on social identity processes to the extent that the individual possesses the necessary skills, knowledge, and so on, to render performance on the task
  • 27. primarily an issue of motivation. Yet, one may argue that especially on such tasks, where individuals may be more dependent on help, cooperation, and information from others, social identity processes may affect group performance through contextual performance (cf. Podsakoff et al., 1997) and interpersonal co- operation (cf. Kramer, 1991). The study of these more complex relationships would seem to be an obvious next step both from a theoretical and from an applied point of view. Although the discussion has primarily focused on the positive effect identification may have on performance, there is a definite potential for IDENTIFICATION, MOTIVATION, AND PERFORMANCE 367 # International Association for Applied Psychology, 2000. social identity processes to result in lowered performance. Work groups may develop counter-productive norms, for instance as a result of a conflict with management (e.g. if the organisation is seen as exploitative, nonproductivity may be an obvious response) or because within the informal network of the work group doing as little as possible and getting away with it is regarded as
  • 28. an accomplishment. In such cases, increased identification and increased social identity salience may result in lowered productivity (note that, in this example, low productivity is undesirable from the organisation's point of view, but not from the point of view of the target of identification, the work group). In addition, for individuals who do not identify with the group or organisation, an emphasis on their group membership (which would enhance social identity salience) may actually elicit responses aimed at underscoring the individual's personal (i.e. as opposed to social) identity (van Prooijen & van Knippenberg, 2000) and thus engender behaviour that is incongruent with the implied social identity (e.g. nonproductivity; see Haslam, in press, for a more elaborate discussion of these issues). Thus, it would seem crucial for a proper understanding of the effects of social identity processes on motivation and performance not only to study the factors that affect organisational identifications and the salience of organ- isational identities, but also to study the factors that affect perceptions of the group's goals and interests. Factors that affect organisational identification and identity salience are extensively discussed elsewhere (e.g. Ashforth & Mael, 1989; Dutton et al.,
  • 29. 1994; Haslam, in press; Hogg & Terry, 2000; Pratt, 1998), and although factors that affect the extent to which high performance is perceived to be in the group's or organisation's interest have not received much attention in social identity research, they are addressed elsewhere (e.g. in research on organisational culture; cf. van Vianen, in press). A discussion of these issues is beyond the scope of the present study, and the conclusion of the previous paragraph is meant as a suggested agenda for research rather than as an issue to be resolved here. Even so, to conclude I may highlight a factor that may be especially relevant in this respect: group goal setting. A large body of evidence shows that setting clear and specific performance goals has a positive effect on the performance of individuals as well as groups (Locke & Latham, 1990; O'Leary-Kelly, Martocchio, & Frink, 1994). In a recent discussion of the effects of group goal setting, Wegge, in this issue, argues that setting group goals, especially setting group goals participatively, enhances social identity salience and increases group identification. Because group goal setting also provides a clear performance standard, Wegge's discussion implies that group goal setting may affect the three variables highlighted above: identification, identity salience, and group goals. Thus,
  • 30. group goal setting in particular may engender a social identity- based motivation to perform well. More definite conclusions about this proposi- 368 VAN KNIPPENBERG # International Association for Applied Psychology, 2000. tion, and other propositions made here, will have to await the results of future research. REFERENCES Allen, N.J., & Meyer, J.P. (1990). The measurement and antecedents of affective, continuance and normative commitment to the organization. Journal of Occu- pational Psychology, 63, 1±18. Ashforth, B.E., & Mael, F. (1989). Social identity theory and the organization. Academy of Management Review, 14, 20±39. Borman, W.C., & Motowidlo, S.J. (1993). Expanding the criterion domain to include elements of contextual performance. In N. Schmitt & W.C. Borman (Eds.), Personnel selection in organizations (pp. 71±98). New York: Jossey-Bass. Brown, R.J. (1978). Divided we fall: An analysis of relations
  • 31. between sections of a factory workforce. In H. Tajfel (Ed.), Differentiation between social groups: Studies in the social psychology of intergroup relations (pp. 395±429). London: Academic Press. Brown, R., Condor, S., Mathews, A., Wade, G., & Williams, J. (1986). Explaining intergroup differentiation in an industrial organization. Journal of Occupational Psychology, 59, 273±286. Dutton, J.E., Dukerich, J.M., & Harquail, C.V. (1994). Organizational images and member identification. Administrative Science Quarterly, 39, 239±263. Ely, R. (1994). The effects of organizational demographics and social identity on rela- tionships among professional women. Administrative Science Quarterly, 39, 203±238. Fajak, A., & Haslam, S.A. (1998). Gender solidarity in hierarchical organizations. British Journal of Social Psychology, 37, 73±94. Hackman, J.R., & Lawler, E.E. (1971). Employee reactions to job characteristics. Journal of Applied Psychology, 55, 259±286. Haslam, S.A. (in press). The psychology of organizations: A social identity approach. London & Newbury Park, CA: Sage.
  • 32. Haslam, S.A. (2000). Social identity, self-categorization and work motivation: Rethinking the contribution of the group to positive and sustainable organiz- ational outcomes. Applied Psychology: An International Review. Haslam, S.A., McGarty, C., Brown, P.M., Eggins, R.A., Morrison, B.E., & Reynolds, K.J. (1998). Inspecting the emperor's clothes: Evidence that random selection of leaders can enhance group performance. Group Dynamics: Theory, Research, & Practice, 2, 168±184. Hogg, M.A. (1993). Group cohesiveness: A critical review and some new directions. European Review of Social Psychology, 4, 85±111. Hogg, M.A., & Abrams, D. (1988). Social identifications: A social psychology of intergroup relations and group processes. London: Routledge. Hogg, M.A., Cooper-Shaw, L., & Holzworth, D.W. (1993). Group prototypicality and depersonalized attraction in small interactive groups. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 19, 452±465. Hogg, M.A., Hains, S.C., & Mason, I. (1998). Identification and leadership in small
  • 33. groups: Salience, frame of reference, and leader stereotypicality effects on leader evaluations. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 75, 1248±1263. IDENTIFICATION, MOTIVATION, AND PERFORMANCE 369 # International Association for Applied Psychology, 2000. Hogg, M.A., & Terry, D.J. (2000). Social identity and self- categorization processes in organizational contexts. Academy of Management Review, 25, 121±140. James, K., & Cropanzano, R. (1994). Dispositional group loyalty and individual action for the benefit of an ingroup: Experimental and correlational evidence. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 60, 179±205. James, K., & Greenberg, J. (1989). In-group salience, intergroup comparison, and individual performance and self-esteem. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 15, 604±616. Karasawa, M. (1991). Toward an assessment of social identity: The structure of group identification and its effects on in-group evaluations. British Journal of Social Psychology, 30, 293±307.
  • 34. Kramer, R. (1991). Intergroup relations and organizational dilemmas. Research in Organizational Behavior, 13, 191±228. Locke, E.A., & Latham, G.P. (1990). A theory of goal setting and task performance. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall. Mael, F., & Ashforth, B.E. (1992). Alumni and their alma mater: A partial test of the reformulated model of organizational identification. Journal of Organizational Behavior, 13, 103±123. Mael, F., & Ashforth, B.E. (1995). Loyal from day one: Biodata, organizational identification, and turnover among newcomers. Personnel Psychology, 48, 309±333. Mael, F.A., & Tetrick, L.E. (1992). Identifying organizational identification. Educational and Psychological Measurement, 52, 813±824. Moorman, R.H., & Blakely, G.L. (1995). Individualism- collectivism as an individual difference predictor of organizational citizenship behavior. Journal of Organiza- tional Behavior, 16, 127±142. Mowday, R.T., Steers, R.M., & Porter, L.W. (1979). The measurement of organ- izational commitment. Journal of Vocational Behavior, 14, 224±247.
  • 35. O'Leary-Kelly, A.M., Martocchio, J.J., & Frink, D.D. (1994). A review of the influence of group goals on group performance. Academy of Management Journal, 37, 1285±1301. O'Reilly, C.A., & Chatman, J. (1986). Organizational commitment and psycholo- gical attachment: The effects of compliance, identification, and internalization on prosocial behavior. Journal of Applied Psychology, 71, 492±499. Organ, D.W. (1988). Organizational citizenship behavior: The good soldier syndrome. Lexington, MA: Lexington Books. Organ, D.W. (1997). Organizational citizenship behavior: It's construct clean-up time. Human Performance, 10, 85±97. Pilegge, A.J., & Holtz, R. (1997). The effects of social identity on the self-set goals and task performance of high and low self-esteem individuals. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 70, 17±26. Podsakoff, P.M., Ahearne, M., & MacKenzie, S.B. (1997). Organizational citizen- ship behavior and the quality and quantity of work group performance. Journal of Applied Psychology, 82, 262±270.
  • 36. Pratt, M.G. (1998). To be or not to be? Central questions in organizational identification. In D.A. Whetten & P.C. Godfrey (Eds.), Identity in organiz- ations: Building theory through conversations (pp. 171±207). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage. 370 VAN KNIPPENBERG # International Association for Applied Psychology, 2000. Smith, E.R., & Henry, S. (1996). An in-group becomes part of the self: Response time evidence. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 22, 635±642. Tajfel, H. (1978). Differentiation between social groups: Studies in the social psychology of intergroup relations. London: Academic Press. Tajfel, H., & Turner, J.C. (1986). The social identity theory of intergroup behaviour. In S. Worchel & W.G. Austin (Eds.), Psychology of intergroup relations (2nd edn., pp. 7±24). Chicago: Nelson-Hall. Terry, D.J., & Callan, V.J. (1998). In-group bias in response to an organizational merger. Group Dynamics: Theory, Research, and Practice, 2,
  • 37. 67±81. Tsui, A., Egan, T., & O'Reilly, C. (1992). Being different: Relational demography and organizational attachment. Administrative Science Quarterly, 37, 549±579. Turner, J.C. (1985). Social categorization and the self-concept: A social cognitive theory of group behavior. In E.J. Lawler (Ed.), Advances in group processes, 2 (pp. 77±122). Greenwich, CT: JAI Press. Turner, J.C., Hogg, M.A., Oakes, P.J., Reicher, S.D., & Wetherell, M.S. (1987). Rediscovering the social group. A self-categorization theory. Oxford: Blackwell. van Knippenberg, D., & Sleebos, E. (1999). Identification, commitment, and individualism-collectivism: Their interrelations and relationship with organiza- tional citizenship behavior. Unpublished manuscript, University of Amsterdam. van Knippenberg, D., van Knippenberg, B., Monden, L., & de Lima, F. (1999). A social categorization perspective on mergers and acquisitions. Unpublished manuscript, University of Amsterdam. van Knippenberg, D., & van Schie, E.C.M. (in press). Foci and correlates of organ- izational identification. Journal of Occupational and
  • 38. Organizational Psychology. van Leeuwen, E., & van Knippenberg, D. (1999). Social value orientations and group performance: The role of expectations of other group members' effort. Un- published manuscript, Leiden University. van Prooijen, J.W., & van Knippenberg, D. (2000). Individuation or depersonal- ization: The influence of personal status position. Group Processes & Intergroup Relations, 3, 63-77. van Vianen, A. (in press). Person-organization fit: The match between newcomers' and recruiters' preferences for organizational cultures. Personnel Psychology. Wagner, J.A. (1995). Studies of individualism-collectivism: Effects on cooperation in groups. Academy of Management Journal, 38, 152±172. Wagner, J.A., & Moch, M.K. (1986). Individualism- collectivism: Concept and measure. Group and Organization Studies, 11, 280±304. Worchel, S., Rothgerber, H., Day, E.A., Hart, D., & Butemeyer, J. (1998). Social identity and individual productivity within groups. British Journal of Social Psychology, 37, 389±413. IDENTIFICATION, MOTIVATION, AND PERFORMANCE 371
  • 39. # International Association for Applied Psychology, 2000. Top of Form Implementation of the IOM Future of Nursing Report 1 Unsatisfactory 0.00% 2 Less than Satisfactory 75.00% 3 Satisfactory 79.00% 4 Good 89.00% 5 Excellent 100.00% 80.0 %Content 5.0 %Provided an original summary of the key messages of the IOM report, Future of Nursing: Leading Change, Advancing Health. Any specific references should be cited. Did not attempt to provide a summary of the key messages of the IOM report, Future of Nursing: Leading Change, Advancing Health, or failed to cite specific references to the IOM report. Provided a skeletal summary of the key messages of the IOM report, Future of Nursing: Leading Change, Advancing Health. Some of the specific references to the IOM report were cited or were done incorrectly. Demonstrates a moderate knowledge of the subject. Recognizes the basic ideas. Misinterprets evidence on the committee's
  • 40. initiative. Demonstrates good knowledge of the subject. Correctly describes the committee's initiative. Justifies some of the impacts on the Future of Nursing. Provided an original summary of the key messages of the IOM report, Future of Nursing: Leading Change, Advancing Health. References specific to the IOM report were properly cited. 15.0 %Identify the role of the Robert Wood Johnson Foundation Initiative and the American Association of Retired Persons on the Future of Nursing Campaign for Action and the State Based Action Coalitions Does not demonstrate knowledge of role. Fails to identify the impact of the Robert Wood Johnson Foundation Initiative on the Future of Nursing. Demonstrates minimal knowledge of subject. Does not adequately visualize or justify the work of the Committee of the Robert Wood Johnson Foundation Initiative on the Future of Nursing. Demonstrates a moderate knowledge of the subject. Recognizes the basic ideas. Misinterprets evidence on the committee's initiative. Demonstrates good knowledge of the subject. Correctly describes the committee's initiative. Justifies some of the impacts on the Future of Nursing. Demonstrates a full and deep knowledge of subject. Develops and explains an informed position on the committee's initiative, integrates and justifies the impact on the Future of Nursing 15.0 %Identify the importance of the IOM FON report related to the nursing workforce Does not demonstrate knowledge of the concept or its role. Fails to identify the importance of the IOM FON report related to the nursing workforce. Demonstrates minimal knowledge of the subject. Does not adequately visualize or justify the importance of the IOM FON
  • 41. report related to the nursing workforce. Demonstrates a moderate knowledge of the subject. Recognizes the basic ideas. Misinterprets evidence on the importance of the IOM FON report related to the nursing workforce. Demonstrates good knowledge of the subject. Correctly describes importance of the IOM FON report related to the nursing workforce. Demonstrates a full and deep knowledge of the subject. Develops and explains the importance of the IOM FON report, integrates and justifies the importance of the IOM FON report related to the nursing workforce. 15.0 %Discuss the intent of the Future of Nursing Campaign for Action Does not demonstrate knowledge of the concept or its role. Fails to identify the intent of the Future of Nursing Campaign for Action. Demonstrates minimal knowledge of the subject. Does not adequately visualize or identify the intent of the Future of Nursing Campaign for Action. Demonstrates a moderate knowledge of the subject. Recognizes the basic ideas. Misinterprets evidence on the intent of the Future of Nursing Campaign for Action. Demonstrates good knowledge of the subject. Correctly describes the intent of the Future of Nursing Campaign for Action. Demonstrates a full and deep knowledge of the subject. Develops and explains the intent of the Future of Nursing Campaign for Action, integrates and justifies the intent of the Future of Nursing Campaign for Action. 15.0 %Identify the rationale of state-based action coalitions Does not demonstrate knowledge of the concept or its role. Fails to identify the rationale of state-based action coalitions. Demonstrates minimal knowledge of the subject. Does not adequately identify the rationale of state-based action
  • 42. coalitions. Demonstrates a moderate knowledge of the subject. Recognizes the basic ideas. Misinterprets evidence on the rationale of state- based action coalitions. Demonstrates good knowledge of the subject. Correctly identifies the rationale of state-based action coalitions. Demonstrates a full and deep knowledge of the subject. Develops and explains and identifies the rationale of state-based action coalitions and justifies a rationale for state-based action coalitions. 15.0 %Discuss one state-based action coalition and two initiatives Does not demonstrate knowledge of the concept or its role. Fails to identify one state-based action coalition and two initiatives. Demonstrates minimal knowledge of the subject. Does not adequately identify one state-based action coalition and two initiatives. Demonstrates a moderate knowledge of the subject. Recognizes the basic ideas. Identifies but misinterprets one state-based action coalition and two initiatives. Demonstrates good knowledge of the subject. Correctly identifies one state-based action coalition and two initiatives. Demonstrates a full and deep knowledge of the subject. Develops and explains the one state-based action coalition and two initiatives, integrates and justifies one state-based action coalition and two initiatives. 15.0 %Organization and Effectiveness 5.0 %Thesis Development and Purpose Paper lacks any discernible overall purpose or organizing claim. Thesis and/or main claim are insufficiently developed and/or vague; purpose is not clear. Thesis and/or main claim are apparent and appropriate to purpose.
  • 43. Thesis and/or main claim are clear and forecast the development of the paper. Is descriptive and reflective of the arguments and appropriate to purpose. Thesis and/or main claim are comprehensive; contained within the thesis is the essence of the paper. Thesis statement makes the purpose of the paper clear. 15.0 %Organization and Effectiveness 5.0 %Paragraph Development and Transitions Paragraphs and transitions consistently lack unity and coherence. No apparent connections between paragraphs are established. Transitions are inappropriate to purpose and scope. Organization is disjointed. Some paragraphs and transitions may lack logical progression of ideas, unity, coherence, and/or cohesiveness.Some degree of organization is evident. Paragraphs are generally competent, but ideas may show some inconsistency in organization and/or in their relationships to each other. A logical progression of ideas between paragraphs is apparent. Paragraphs exhibit a unity, coherence, and cohesiveness. Topic sentences and concluding remarks are appropriate to purpose. There is a sophisticated construction of paragraphs and transitions. Ideas progress and relate to each other. Paragraph and transition construction guide the reader. Paragraph structure is seamless. 15.0 %Organization and Effectiveness 5.0 %Mechanics of Writing (includes spelling, punctuation, grammar, language use) Surface errors are pervasive enough that they impede communication of meaning. Inappropriate word choice and/or sentence construction are used. Frequent and repetitive mechanical errors distract the reader.
  • 44. Inconsistencies in language choice (register), sentence structure, and/or word choice are present. Some mechanical errors or typos are present, but are not overly distracting to the reader. Correct sentence structure and audience-appropriate language are used. Prose is largely free of mechanical errors, although a few may be present. A variety of sentence structures and effective figures of speech are used. Writer is clearly in command of standard, written, academic English. 5.0 %Format 2.0 %Paper Format (1- inch margins;12-point-font;double- spaced;Times New Roman, Arial, or Courier) Template is not used appropriately or documentation format is rarely followed correctly. Template is used, but some elements are missing or mistaken; lack of control with formatting is apparent. Template is used, and formatting is correct, although some minor errors may be present. Template is fully used; There are virtually no errors in formatting style. All format elements are correct. 3.0 %Research Citations (In-text citations for paraphrasing and direct quotes, and reference page listing and formatting, as appropriate to assignment) No reference page is included. No citations are used. Reference page is present. Citations are inconsistently used. Reference page is included and lists sources used in the paper. Sources are appropriately documented, although some errors may be present. Reference page is present and fully inclusive of all cited sources. Documentation is appropriate and GCU style is usually correct.
  • 45. In-text citations and a reference page are complete. The documentation of cited sources is free of error. 100 %Total Weightage Bottom of Form Identity The term identity implies a construct that gives the self a meaning or purpose. Weigert, Teitge, and Teitge (1986) gave another brief definition: "identity is a definition that transforms a mere biological individual into a human person" (p. 31). To explicate further, identity is how people define themselves via many different expressions such as autobiographical memories (personal identity) and identification with groups (social identity). Social and personal identity are not completely separable but overlap and interact with one another very closely to guide a person's perceptions, attitudes, and behaviors as well as form a general identity. See the figure below. We will briefly explore the concept of personal identity before returning to the main concept of social identity. Personal Identity Mayer, Greenbaum, Kuenzi, and Shteynberg (2009) state "personal identity consists of moral sensibility and conscience, and also a desire for achievement, mastery, and competence." Personal identity is the combination of objective biosocial markers such as age, race, sex, and so on, and the personal life history of the individual (Brewer & Gardner, 1996; Weigert et
  • 46. al., 1986). These factors are often considered part of social identity as well because they place the individual into a group. That said, they involve attributes of individuals (even if they are shared within groups characterized by values of those markers), so the biosocial markers also create a personal identity. Personal identity is those aspects of the self that are unique to the individual and are used to define the individual (Brewer & Gardner, 1996). Social Identity The most widely accepted definition of social identity comes from Tajfel (1981): "that part of an individual's self-concept which derives from his [sic] knowledge of his membership in a social group (or groups) together with the value and emotional significance attached with that membership" (p. 255). According to Tajfel, social identity has three components: · Self-categorization: Belief of group membership · Group self-esteem: Importance of group membership to one's self-concept · Group commitment: Desire for group membership Therefore, Tajfel (1981) theorizes that it is not enough to belong to an organization; a person needs to feel connected to the group, and some sort of social-esteem must also be derived from that group membership. When a person categorizes her/himself as part of the group, feels committed to the group, and gains some esteem from the group, then that group has become the person's in-group. All other groups are potential rivals/competitors for resources and are known as out-groups. Identity, Threat, and Behavior There is convincing evidence that under certain conditions, such as various types of group membership and threat conditions, one of the major components of identity is likely to emerge as the primary determinant of behavior (Ellemers et al., 2002; Nario- Redmond, Biernat, Eidelman, & Palenske, 2004). For example, it has been demonstrated that under threat to a high-status group, the behavior of a group member is likely to be determined by her/his social identity (Ellemers et al., 2002;
  • 47. Haslam, O'Brien, & Jetten, Vormedal, & Penna, 2005). In contrast, while under personal threat, the same person's personal identity is likely to emerge as the primary determinant of behavior (Ellemers et al., 2002; Nario-Redmond et al., 2004). See the table below. Threat and Behavioral Determinants THREATENED ENTITIES PRIMARY BEHAVIORAL DETERMINANTS SECONDARY BEHAVIORAL DETERMINANTS Person Personal identity General identity, social identity High-status in-group Social identity General identity, personal identity Group Status and Self-Esteem Social identity theory explains a person's behavior in a group using that person's status within the group as a social comparison. Every group has a certain amount of social status attached to its membership (Morton, Postmes, Haslam, & Hornsey, 2009; Tajfel & Turner, 1979, 1986), which gives it value for the individual who is a member of that particular group. Individual group members use the status from their group membership to gain and maintain self-esteem (Derks, van Laar, & Ellemers, 2009; Morton, Postmes, Haslam, & Hornsey, 2009). Status provides self-esteem for group members because it fulfills the need for a positive social identity (Hogg, 1992; Hogg & Abrams, 1988; 1990; 1993). The motivations to enhance self-esteem and gain status cause individuals to behave in ways that maintain the group and their membership in the group (Derks, van Laar, & Ellemers, 2009; Hogg & Abrams, 1990; Morton, Postmes, Haslam, & Hornsey, 2009). The better one's own group looks in comparison to other groups, the more status the group gains, and the more self-esteem it can provide for its members (Morton, Postmes, Haslam, & Hornsey, 2009; Tajfel & Turner, 1979, 1986). See the figure below. Self-esteem is not necessarily the mechanism by which a positive social identity is achieved. Status itself can also be
  • 48. enough to trigger a positive social identity. In this case, individuals seek to join a high-status group to improve their own social identity in general rather than to improve their self- esteem per se (Bettencourt, Dorr, Charlton, & Hume, 2001). But in either case the motivation is a positive social identity from which the person gains benefits (e.g. self-esteem, higher status, possibly power or wealth) through self-identification with the high-status group. The benefits of social identity have recently been demonstrated by Peeters and Oerlemans (2009) who studied two different organizations and found that those with strong social identification with their organization had greater feelings of well-being than those with weaker social identification. In summary, social identity theory proposes that self-esteem and status are the main reasons that social identity motivates individuals. But remember that a person must also first describe her/himself as part of the group (self-categorization) as well as want to be a part of the group (commitment). Organizational Applications Application of social identity theory in organizations is still relatively new (since about the turn of the century). However, because of the success of the theory and the large amounts of empirical evidence, several books have been written (e.g. Haslam, 2004; Hogg & Terry, 2001) that not only synthesize the vast amount of research but also provide many useful tips on how to manage certain aspects of organizations. The theory has been applied in many different areas of organizations, and it has been used to not only explain why some groups are motivated to experience conflict with groups within their own organization but also why their organization may experience conflict with other organizations. It has also been used to resolve those conflicts or avoid them altogether. Specifically, it has been used to manage group relationships with regards to the following: majority-minority relations, corporate mergers, people with multiple roles in an organization, workgroup socialization, power usage, and
  • 49. leadership, as well as others. Experienced success of organizations in motivating their people through those often difficult tasks reflects the relative easiness of implementation of the theory's basic components as described in Tajfel's definition. For instance, during a corporate merger where both organizations have strong corporate identities, leadership in organizations with knowledge of this theory will know that there will be a great resistance to the merger as neither organization will want to give up its social identity. As such, they may want to proceed slowly with the merger and work on creating a shared identity before an official merger occurs so that all involved will be motivated to join the new group. See the figure below. You may have noticed that when corporations merge, a new name may be chosen for the new combined organization.