SlideShare a Scribd company logo
1 of 91
, Phonological systems are rule-governed; that is, they operate
according to certain rules and are
: manifested as patterns.The word used for individual speech
sounds is phones, and the study of the
; characteristics, or features, of phones of all languages is called
phonetics (Yule, 2010). Although the
I focus is on the English sound system, it is important to note
that each language is systematic in its
patterning, and that although similarities exist across all
languages, differences abound.
Phonology
The study of the sound system of languages, called phonology,
helps teachers understand many
challenges English learners (ELs) face, both in hearing and
producing the sounds of a new language.
This knowledge also assists teachers in diagnosing errors
second language (L2) readers typically
make when reading aloud and in predicting how this affects
comprehension, accuracy, and fluency.
This section is fundamental to an understanding of linguistics
because it introduces a number of
important concepts that are revisited at other levels of language.
The first section is on the basic con-
cepts of phonology; the second is about the consonants of
English; the third provides an overview of
the English vowels; and the fourth is about suprasegmentals, the
phonological phenomena affecting
pronunciation at word and phrasal levels. An examination of the
learning processes involved when
a learner encounters a new language is presented along with
activities to support educators and
students in discovering the characteristics of how the sound
systems function, as well as ways to
apply knowledge of phonology to help students overcome
difficulties. See Figure 5.1.
g
"i,':
.§
~
_;;
Sounds
l--- --L-..-.
~
~'------........-'
=
j
_;;
..... = = "' @
Intonation
Word stress
Rhythm
Features of
connected speech
Figure S.1. Phonology.
-[ill-
A uniYersal concept across languages is the phone, or sound, as
represe:-.?.:: ::-- .:. ..=~ o:::- 0::.~er 5;-::-.::... "-
between brackets, such as [p ]. Note that [pl between brackets
represents ti-.E s.:::. ~ 2..:'".i ~~ 'p ' in si.-.~
quotation marks represents the letter. The concept of phone is a
uni·ersal o:-.e: a _e::cr or other syrr.x_
in brackets indicates thatit is part ofa system that includes all
the world's languages. The Intemationa..
Phonetic Alphabet (IPA) includes all these phones using a
unique symbol for each sound.
The sound of [p] in English actually has three different variants,
the aspirated [p] in 'pit', fr.:c
unaspirated [p] in 'shopping' and the unreleased [p] in 'stop'.
Even though English has these ya::-
ants, called allophones, of [p ], they are still the same phoneme.
That is, the same symbol is used.::-
represent all the variants of [p] for English. A phoneme is
represented by a symbol that includes L
possible variants (allophones) of a particular sound in a
particular language, and is written ben..,·ee:-
slashes, as in / p /. Aspiration of a sound involves the
accompaniment of a puffofair on the sounc. ::
onset. You can detect this by putting your finger in front of
your lips when starting to say 'pif. =:
you say the word 'pit' twice, once with aspirated /p/ and once
with unaspirated /p/, the meani:;
of the word does not change. However, in Hindi and Arabic
there are two [p] phonemes becauE-c
speakers produce both aspirated and unaspirated sounds in the
same environment, and they can:- ·
be used interchangeably. They are different at the phonemic
level. Other languages, such as Korea:-
do not make a distinction between [p] and [f], so there would be
only one phoneme represent::-;
those Korean allophones. A specific instance of phonemic
difference is in Spanish, which has r -
/r/ phonemes, one written as 'r', and the other as 'rr', as in 'pero',
'but', and 'perro', 'dog' . As y~ -
can see, a phonemic difference indicates a meaning difference at
the word level.
The concepts of -etic and -emic are important for teachers to
understand because they are used.=
the fields ofboth anthropology and linguistics; both fields
contribute crucially to the teaching of :.a.:--
guages. The term -etic refers to universal characteristics of
languages or cultures, while -emic r~e:-
to language- or culture-specific characteristics. The whole idea
of universal properties of languat~
and cultures is a powerful one; within the bounds of our own
cultural and linguistic cocoons, · =
often assume that our way of experiencing the world is
universal, and are often surprised wz
we learn that it is not. When non-native English speakers cannot
hear or produce the 'th' sounc: =
'three', and produce 'tree' or 'free', we may wonder why they do
not say 'three', which is so sim;· -
The fact is that 'th' is quite rare across the world's languages, so
it is no wonder that ELs have c._:-
ficulty with this pronunciation.
At the word/concept level where culture and language meet, it is
surprising to learn that :-.a-
ther Vietnamese nor Swahili has the generic word 'aunt'. In
addition, mother's sister, father 's sis:2:
uncle's wife, and so on must all be referred to by different
words; the relationships and roles des.:.;-
nated by these different words are culturally determined. This
will be addressed in the section::.-
semantics. In becoming more aware of the needs of ELs in the
classroom, educators should prep~-
themselves to be surprised rather than judgmental, for what they
say and do can be a cons-z..-
source of wonder and learning for the whole class.
Systematicity of English Consonants
In this overview of sounds and the rules that underlie them,
consonants are examined. Eng·:.-
consonants are formed by affecting the passage of air through
the vocal tract in various ways. -:--
chart in Figure 5.2 provides information on the following: the
place of articulation for each soc
from front to back in the mouth, the pairing of sounds via their
voicing properties, and whether ::...:
passing through the vocal tract stops or continues.
In studying the chart, note that most consonant phonemes are
represented by letters of =
alphabet, while other symbols represent sounds that are written
with two letters. For the systerr. ·
be useful and clear, a symbol must uniquely represent only one
phoneme. A key word below e=--·
non-alphabetic symbol on the chart helps to identify the sound
it represents.
67 Chapter Five: The Fundamentals of Applied Linguistics:
Communication through Language
English Consonanis
Manner Bilabial Labio- hlter- Alveolar Palatal Velar Glottal
Place <lental dental+
Stop
/pl - top /ti -two /kl - carVoiceless
/bl - bee !di-do /g/- goVoiced
Nasal /ml-me /n/-no /rj/- ring
Voiced
only
Flicative
/fl -fun /e /- /s/ - so I f / - shoe /h/have Voiceless lltick
/vi- vote lzl - zooVoiced /of - /3/visionthe
Affticate
ltfl- watc/1Voiceless
Voiced Id!, /- joy
Glide lwl why /j/-yes /wi/
Voiced
only
Liquid /I/ love
Voiced only /r/ rot
Figure S.2. The consonant system of the English Language
Pause and Reflect
1. With a partner, produce each sound to become more familiar
with the overall system.
2. English sounds are produced using three articulators: the lips,
teeth, and tongue. Consult the
chart to identify which articulators are used to produce each
phoneme.
3. The place of articulation from front to back in the vocal tract
is where an articulator touches
a location in the mouth. As you pronounce each sound again, try
to describe its point of
articulation.
4. Compare the labels of the place of articulation on the chart in
Figure 5.2 with the positions
in the vocal tract shown in Figure 5.3 .
It, d, s, z, I, n I
[ j, z, tj, d, y, r ] Ik, g, IJ ]
[ p, b, m, w] [ A]
[ f, v ] Labiodental
Alveolar ridge:
alveolars
Alveopalatal ~'i:f~::d---
[ h ] Glottis: glottals
I ? l
Velum: velars
Uvula: uvulars
Pharnyx: pharnygeals
Esophagus
Part Two: Language and Literacy (Applied Linguistics)
-"-
Figure 5.3. Organs of Speech Production.
From Why TESOL? Theories & Issues in Teaching English to
Speakers of Other Languages in K-12 Classrooms by Ariza et
al.
Copyright© 2010 by Kendall Hunt Publishing Company.
Reprinted by permission.
It is important to note that all English phonemes, except for / h
/, are made in the area from
the lips to the velum. But each language has a different system
of phonemes, some more, some less
:han those of English. For example, Arabic speakers produce
uvular, pharyngeal, laryngeal, and glottal
sounds. So, you can imagine how difficult it is for English
learners of Arabic to master such sounds.
1. Look at the voiced and voiceless pairs on the chart in Figure
5.4. Produce each
voiced sound in the pair first, placing your index finger across
your 'voice box'
and trying to feel the vocal cords vibrate.
2. Now produce the voiceless member of each pair and note that
there is no
vibration.
Word-Initial Word-Medial Word-Final
-:oiced zap phasing buzz
~. ·oiceless ~ap fa.!:,;ing bu~
Figure S.4. Voiced/Voiceless Pairs.
The chart, along with Figure 5.2, illustrates that voicing is an
extremely important feature of
English.
Except for nasals, /r/, /1/, /w/, /y/, and /h/, all consonants in the
language occur in voiced
a.~d ,·oiceless pairs, which is not a universal feature of
languages in general. The voicing feature of
-=:.nglish is so prominent that voicing contrasts occur in word-
initial, word-medial, and word-final
;ositions, as shown in Figure 5.4 for the phonemes /z/ and /s/.
Note that English spelling often
ciscures the realities of sound similarities.
69 Chapter Five: The Fundamentals of Applied Linguistics:
Communication through Language
Althoughmany languages have voicing pairs word-initially,
several such as Spanish andGerman
devoice consonants word-finally. Thus, Spanish- and German-
speaking ELs tend to pronounce 'pig'
as 'pick', 'cab' as 'cap', and 'fuzz' as 'fuss'. The process of
devoicing in L2 learning is one kind of
transfer or interference from a first language (Ll).
~ 1. Locate the pairs (lbIpI, I diti, and I glkl). Do they stop the
airflow, or continue it?
2. Locate the other pairs, called fricatives, and answer the same
question. ~
Another important aspect of the English consonant system is
how airflow is impeded. Stops
completely do so, while fricatives obstruct the air flow, but
allow it to continue. As you studied the
chart and pronounced the various sounds, you might have
noticed that the three pairs of stops were
interspersed with pairs of fricatives. The pair lbIpI is produced
in the mouth very close to IvI fl
and Idltl is close to ltJId3I and also to lzlsl. Speakers of some
languages have difficulty hearing
and producing the difference between the lbl in 'cab' and the IvI
in 'calve'. Although Spanish has
both lbI and IvI phonemes, they may both be pronounced the
same, namely as bilabial fricatives,
where the sound is produced with air passing between the lips
instead of stopping. The words 'very'
and 'berry' are often pronounced almost identically by Spanish
speakers. (It should be noted that
generalizations made aboutvarious languages are not always
accurate for all dialects of a language.)
Speakers of some languages spoken in Ethiopia, as well as
Korean, have difficulty with the IpIfI
distinction. A Ethiopian friend once reported that his student
from a rural area told him, "Flease, Sir,
I am foor. I have no any money." A further example is that
French speakers, who lack the voiced 'th'
sound in their language, are likely to say' /zisl for 'this';
speakers of some West African languages
might say 'dat' for 'that'; and speakers of yet another language
might say 'tree' for 'three'. As noted
above, the two sounds that represent English 'th' are uncommon
among the world's languages and
so ELs will produce sounds from their own collection of
phonemes that are closest to the English
sounds. Thus, a second kind of Ll transfer, or interference, may
occur for speakers whose languages
lack the phonemic contrasts across the stopIfricative divide that
English has.
Pause and Reflect
1. One pair of sounds, /d3/- /tJ/ (as in 'jerry' and 'cherry') are
stop/continuants (called affri-
cates); /d3/ starts with /d/ and ends with /3/; /tJ/ starts with /t/
and ends with /f/.
Look at the nasals and explain why they are placed where they
are. Can you speculate why
there are no nasals in the fricative columns?
2. Where do you find the semi-vowels (also called glides) on
the chart in Figure 5.2?
70 Part Two: Language and Literacy (Applied Linguistics)
The two affricate sounds can be difficult for non-native English
speakers. For example, many
Spanish speakers have difficulty distinguishing between /tJ/ and
/J/ ('cherry' and 'sherry') in all
environments, and between /d3/ and /tJ/ word finally ('badge'
and 'batch'). These sounds may
also be late-learned by English Ll children during language
development. In fact, some other Eng-
lish sounds are difficult for both children and L2 learners.
Nasals are found only in the stop columns in English and they
are produced in the same manner
as their counterparts, but with the impeding of air through the
nasal passage. When that passage is
blocked, as with a cold or a pinched nose, the voiced stop is
produced, "by dose is all plugged up."
Some Spanish speakers may have difficulty producing /m/ word-
finally, either omitting it ("My
nay is_.") or substituting with /n/ or /11./ ("Hong Depot") . In
consulting an IPA chart you will find
that there are languages in the world with nasal fricatives. Can
you produce any of them?
Each semi-vowel and liquid is shown on the chart in Figure 5.2
in the column beneath its place
of articulation. It might seem quite odd to see /1/ placed in the
/dz/tf/ column until you experi-
ment a bit and find that it is formed by touching the tongue tip
at the alveolar ridge, although it
could also be formed further forward or further back (Try it!).
Speakers from Taiwan may produce
/1/ for / oI as in 'raler' for 'rather'. Speakers of some dialects of
English produce a palatalized /1/,
touching the blade of the tongue at the palate rather than using
the tongue tip.
It might also seem strange to see /w/ in the bilabial column. But
when /w/ is formed, as in
'wonder', the lips are round, thus, a bilabial sound. On the other
hand, /y/, formed in the palatal
area, has vowel-like properties as /w/ does. The two sounds are
vowel-like because the airflow is
not impeded in their production as with stops and fricatives.
Both L2 learners and English speaking children have difficulty
with liquids and semi-vowels
(glides). Thus, a second type of learning process results in
developmental phenomena. Examples
of both developmental and Ll transfer errors with liquids and
glides are shown in the chart in
Figure 5.5.
English Ll Development (and L2 Leaming) L2 Leaming
/r/ vs /w/
rabbit ➔ wabbit
/1/ vs /r/ rice ➔ lice
Japanese, Korean, and some East and Central
African languages
/1/ vs /w/ (line, wine)
/ lait/, /wait/ or / ait/
/wI vs /v/Wine ➔ vine
Farsi (Iran) & Dari (Afghanistan)
/f/ vs /wh/ fuji ➔ whuji
/y/ vs /j/ yet ➔ jet
Spanish
/w/ vs / gw/ wet ➔ gwet
Figure S.S. Developmental and Ll Transfer Errors.
Minimal Pairs: Listening and Speaking
When a learner has difficulty producing an English sound, an
experienced teacher analyzes the error
in comparison to the target pronunciation. For example a
student may say "I want a baby 'pick/JI
instead of 1pig1 because in the Ll voicing of consonants does
not occur word-finally. The words 'pig
and pick' constitute a minimal pair, which consists of two words
having two minimally contrastive
sounds in the same position in a word. In the pig/pick example
the word-final sounds are formed in the
same place, and are both stops; they differ only minimally-in
voicing. Figure 5.6 portrays pairs of
words that do not constitute minimal pairs.
71 Chapter Five: The Fundamentals of Applied Linguistics:
Communication through Language
pick/pill The word-final sounds are not produced at the same
location in the
mouth and differ in terms of voicing.
Also, /kl is a stop and /1/ is a continuant.
trip/dip Although / t/ and / d/ are minimally contrastive, there is
an extra
sound, /r/.
fight/night Rhyming words are not necessarily considered
minimal pairs.
Though they are both continuants, / f/ and / n/ differ in terms of
manner (nasal vs fricative) and place of articulation.
crutch/ grouch The velar stops/g/ and /k/ are minimally
contrastive, but the vowel
difference indicates not one contrast, but two.
Figure S.6. Non-Minimal Pairs
Pause and Reflect
1. Study the pairs of words in Figure 5.7, say them aloud, and
underline pairs that are mini-
mally contrastive. Remember, only one contrast in the same
position in both words constitutes
a minimal pair. Don't be fooled by spelling. For example, the
words in the first pair do not
use the same spelling pattern, but they do constitute a minimal
pair.
buy/pie fan/vat catch/ glitch Sue/zoo
rush/rouge bank/bag lip/lib than/thin
tan/Dan fuss/fuzz cheap/jeep red/rent
half/halve gum/come veal/feel puck/pug
Figure S.7. Identifying Minimal Pairs in English.
To listen to speakers of many different languages reading the
same passage in
English, go to this website: www.accent.gmu.edu, click on
browse; click on any
language, then find the list of speakers reading the same short
paragraph.
2. In pairs or small groups, identify errors you heard and create
minimal pairs
to help the speaker(s) hear the difference between the two
sounds. Consider
consulting the consonant chart or your personal experiences in
listening to Els
to complete this activity.
There are two ways to assist Els with new sounds, through
listening activities and visual pres-
entations. A teacher begins with a listening, a minimal pair
drill, which helps students hear the differ-
ence between two sounds as in the case of the learner who says
'pick' for 'pig'. The teacher presents
two minimally contrastive words orally so that the learner(s)
can hear the difference. Figure 5.8 shows
how to conduct a minimal pair drill to help Els distinguish
between the sound /z/ and /s/.
Once students can hear the differences, they are ready to say the
words. You can use your hands,
videos, or drawings of the vocal tract to demonstrate. Avoid
drawing attention to your own mouth,
as there may be taboos about doing so. There are numerous
websites with lists of minimal pairs for
teaching the sounds. Visit the ESOl website for suggestions.
www.accent.gmu.edu
-
72 Part Two: Language and Literacy (Applied Linguistics)
Teacher: Student(s):
Listen carefully as I say two words that sound similar,
'zap and sap'.
(Do not show the words in writing.)
Say slowly: They do not repeat,
Zap, zap, zap, sap, sap, sap, listen only.
Zap,sap,zap,sap,zap,sap
(Do not vary the intonation pattern as some will
focus on tonal distinctions).
When I say zap, put up one finger.
When I say 'sap' put up two fingers.
Say the words randomly and watch as students Students show
fingers as
show fingers, directed.
ending the drill when students comply consistently.
Do the same drill, but with the sounds in
final or medial position: 'buzz, bus; phasing, facing.'
Figure S.8. Leading a Minimal Pair Drill.
The Systematicity ofEnglish Vowels
Vowels are produced with no airflow obstruction, but always
involve the tongue and vocal folds in
their production. Among the simplest vowel systems is found in
some dialects of Arabic, with only
three vowels. Spanish has five, a very common pattern.
1. Study the five-vowel system chart in Figure 5.9 with the
Arabic vowels shown
in boldface type, as they are produced by moving the tongue in
the mouth.
Vowels change according to the vertical and horizontal position
of the tongue
in the mouth.
~
~
2. The Spanish vowel phonemes are pronounced as follows: /i/,
as in 'neat', /e/,
pronounced halfway between the vowels sounds in 'net' and
'Nate', /a/ as in
'not', /u/ as in 'newt', and/o/ pronounced halfway between the
vowel sounds
in 'note' and 'naught'. Pronounce the vowels and notice how
your tongue
position changes slightly as you move from front to back, then
high to low in
your mouth. Note that the labels represent the tongue positions.
Tongue Front Central Back
Position
Tongue
Height
High Ii/ /ul lu/
Mid /e/ / of
Low la/
Figure S.9. The Three-Vowel System of Arabic and Five-Vowel
System of Spanish
It should be noted that both systems are symmetrical. It is rare
to find asymmetrical vowel sys-
tems in languages. Just as you learned about consonants, you
can see that vowels usually pattern in
pairs. When examining the English vowel system, there is
greater complexity but, still with vowels
73 Chapter Five: The Fundamentals ofApplied Linguistics:
Communication through Language
in pairs, the distinction is between a tense or a lax tongue, as
seen in Figure 5.10. For ELs who speak
languages with few vowels, such as Spanish, there is
considerable transfer of the system from the
Ll, and this is often carried into late stage L2 development,
even fossilized (permanently fixed)
in late learners. However, for speakers of languages having
many different vowel sounds, such as
Vietnamese, English vowels present fewer problems. Those
speakers are already attuned to distin-
guishing subtle differences across their own vowel systems.
Tongue Front Central Back
Position
Tongue
Height
Tongue
Height
Tense Lax Lax Tense
High /i/
'neat'
/ I/
'knit'I
/u/
'nook'
/u/
'newt'
High
Mid /ey/
'Nate'
/el
'net
/A/
'nut'
hi
'naught'
/ow/
'note'
Mid
hi
'about'
/£/
'gnat'
Low /a/ Low
'not'
Figure S.1 0. The Vowel System of English.
You have probably already noticed that the /r_/ sound in 'gnat'
lacks a partner. Similarly, /a/ as in
'not' stands alone. Because the chart distorts the actual shape of
the mouth, as you can see in the vocal
tract diagram in Figure 5.3, the larger space in the high front
and central areas can simply accommo-
date more sounds than can the central and low back space, thus
the asymmetry of the system.
What makes English vowels difficult to focus on is that
speakers of different regional or social
dialects produce diphthongs rather than simple vowels in some
contexts, or they produce simple
sounds rather than diphthongs in other contexts. For example,
many speakers from Brooklyn in
New York City produce / u"/ instead of hi in 'coffee'; speakers
of some Southern dialects produce
/ a/ instead of / ay / in 'sight'. Of course, dialect differences are
even more profound across the
English-speaking world ofBritain, Canada, the United States,
Australia, and New Zealand. But speak-
ers of English as their first language exist in many of the former
colonies in Africa and Asia as well;
thus, this greatly expands the possibilities of standard
pronunciation across the English language. The
concept of World Englishes was developed by Braj Kachru
(1990, 2006), who argues that the notion of a
'standard' dialect of English must be expanded beyond the core
English-speaking area.
Suprasegmentals
Phonemes are not the only focus of interest in the field of
linguistics. Tone, intonation, pitch, length,
and stress, called suprasegmentals, are other properties
ofpronunciation that affect accent and other
communication difficulties experienced by ELs. According to
Hudson (2000) these sound qualities
occur, not at the phonetic level, but "spread over more than one
phone" (26). Also called prosodic
phenomena, these are length, stress, and pitch.
74 Part Two: Language and Literacy (Applied Linguistics)
Pause and Reflect
1. Now study the vowel system of English with its tense and lax
vowel pairs, simple vowels, and
diphthongs in Figure 5.10.
2. As you do so, note the key words in each position; say each
word and listen to the very subtle
difference between the two phonemes in each tense/lax pair.
3. Holding one hand, palm up, and pretending that it is your
tongue, relax it, then tense it a
few times. Pronounce the two words in each tense and lax pair
as you relax and tense your
hand. This kinesthetic activity is an effective way in helping Els
feel the difference between
the two sounds.
Phonemes Minimal Pairs Examples
/ii neat feel, teach, meat, cede, people,
receive, believe,
/I/ knit sit, mix, pick, film, rid, build
/ey/ Nate bait, rate, plain, may, weigh, favor
bait, rate, plain, may,
/e/ net weigh, favor
bet, said, rent, fence, bread
/El
/a/
/u/
gnat
not
newt
tack, sap, fan, magic
mop, father, calm, sock, Tom, jolly
luke, tune, fruit, room, blue, fool,
tour, move, do, two, blew
/u/
/owl
nook
note
look, put, foot
vote, poll, coat, broke, holy, so,
sew, Roman, sold, though,
low, beau
hi naught caught, song, crawl, fought, ball,
paw
Figure 5.11. The Pronunciation and Spelling of English Vowels
and Diphthongs.
75 Chapter Five: The Fundamentals of Applied Linguistics:
Communication through Language
Phonemes Minimal Pairs Examples
/A/ nut sun, luck, mother, enough, rung
hi/ noise toy, moist, Freud, foyle
/aw/ noun round, house, cow, now
/ai/ night fight, kind, mine, type, sign, fry,
buy
/ar/ narc farm, park, sorry, heart,
/Ar/ nerd clerk, bird, learn, worry, first, hurt
/or/ norm storm, for, tore, born, boar
Figure 5.11. The Pronunciation and Spelling of English Vowels
and Diphthongs. (Continued)
Pause and Reflect
1. The chart in Figure 5. 11 illustrates the system, the key
words as minimal pairs, and other
words having the same vowel phonemes as each key word.
Pronounce each of the words
again, and note the additional diphthongs and the words
influenced by /r/. Note the move-
ment of the tongue from one position to another as you produce
the diphthongs.
2. Discuss with a partner or in a small group the reading,
listening, and speaking difficulties
you might expect Els to face given both the subtle tense/lax
differences and the spelling
variations.
3. Devise ways to help Els confront the realities of learning
English pronunciation of vowels,
using your knowledge of the specific difficulties experienced by
l 1 speakers of languages
you know.
Phone lengthening can be a natural occurrence, demonstrated by
the words 'hat' and 'had'. If you
say these two words one after the other a few times, you will
notice that the vowel sound is slightly
longer in 'had'. The same occurs with'cab'/'cap','fuss'/'fuzz',
'half'/ 'halve' and 'bag'/'back'. As you
may have discovered, this subtle lengthening occurs when the
final consonant is voiced. In some lan-
guages however, there is a phonemic difference between 'short'
and 'long' vowel sounds. For exam-
ple, in Swahili there is a phonemic difference between the
words 'choo' (toilet), and 'cho' (a locative
76 Part Two: Language and Literacy (Applied Linguistics)
particle within a verb). Vowel length produces a difference in
meaning, though the vowel is produceci
in the same place in the mouth in both words. ELs who speak
languages having the lengthening fea-
ture might be taught to use lengthening, a case of positive
transfer, if they have difficulties with word-
final devoicing. As American teachers well know, the terms
short and long vowels refer to differences
in sound, not actual vowel length, so they will need to help their
ELs understand the difference.
Stress, "the intensity or loudness of the airstream" (27), occurs
at the syllable level. In the fol-
lowing three-syllable English words the stress occurs on
different syllables in each one: Canada (1s:
aroma (2nd), disappear (3rd
). The underlying rules of syllable stress are complex and can
be difficU:.:
to master. Many languages of the world have more regular
stress patterns, such as Spanish where
stress on most words is on the next-to-the-last syllable. Other
languages, such as Japanese, haYc
minimal syllable stress. For such speakers, learning the various
possible stress patterns of Eng~
can be a daunting undertaking.
"Pitch is the frequency of vibration of the vocal folds" (27).
More frequent vibrations produce 2.
higher pitch. Patterns of frequencies at the phrasal or sentence
level are called intonation. In Eng-
lish, falling intonation sentence-finally is often associated with
statements, rising intonation wit:-
yes/no questions. Saying the same phrase such as "we don't"
using several different intonatio:-
patterns results in subtle meaning differences. The rising and
falling intonation patterns of Eng~
statements and questions are not universal; so, ELs benefit from
some direct teaching of these pa:-
tems. Tonal languages are those where a difference in pitch on a
vowel in a syllable represents a:-
altogether different phoneme. Chinese, Vietnamese, Thai, Lao,
and some African and Scandanavia:-
languages are tonal languages. Vietnamese, a mono-syllabic
tone language, has a high tone, a lo, -
tone, a falling tone, and a contour (rising/falling) tone.
Changing the tone of a syllable produces c.
difference in meaning. In Mandarin Chinese, the word 'ma'
means scold when pronounced with c.
falling tone, and 'hemp' when pronounced with a rising tone.
Speakers of tone languages may bE
able to transfer their sensitivity to tones to learning English
intonation patterns.
Phonology and Second Language (L2) Learning
In sum, monolingual children acquiring their first language (Ll )
are capable of mastering any soun~
of any language; they are not born with the sounds of their
parents, but rather with the capacity tr-
acquire those sounds. We use the term acquire to refer to
subconscious learning. Children are no:
really taught language; they 'pick it up' from the linguistic
environment of their homes. If childre::'.
grow up in a bilingual or trilingual environment, they acquire
all the sounds of the languages the:-
hear and begin to speak. If children are in the environment of a
new language in school, for example
they will experience interference or transfer from the home
language at first. But because the br~
of children before puberty are quite flexible, they generally lose
their accents early in the language
learning process. If individuals meet a new language after the
age of puberty, however, it is quite
likely that first language effects will occur in the way they
speak the new language. Spanish speak~
learning English, for example, may have difficulty producing
the difference between the / i/ in 'bee:
and the /I/ in 'bit, orbetween the initial sounds in'cherry' and
'sherry'. They will almost always haYE
an accent from the Ll, although over time the accent may move
closer to that of a native speaker o:
the target language.
Morphology
As was discussed in the phonology section, languages have
sound systems and these systems arE
uniquely language-specific, but sounds themselves are not
associated with meaning. Morpholo~
is the study of words and word forms and the processes by
which words become created anci
modified. Morphology does not entail the study of word
meaning, but of word form and functio,:
Words and word parts are stored and accessed from the lexicon,
where they are associated witl-.
meaning studied in the field of semantics.
Morphemes
Teach-er-s
Non-lexicalLexical
Teach-
77 Chapter Five: The Fundamentals of Applied Linguistics:
Communication through Language
Classification ofMorphemes
Figure S.12. Morphology.
Morphemes are the smallest units of meaning in a language.
Note the etymology of the word mor-
phemic: morph (form) and -emic pertaining to language specific
phenomena. This is parallel to the
term phonemic, pertaining to a language-specific sound. A
classification of English morphemes is
shown in Figure 5.13 with examples.
Morphemes
Free
( occurs alone)
Bound (occurs
with
A free morpheme
or other bound
morpheme[s])
Lexical (content
words)
Non-lexical
(grammatical
or functional
words)
Lexical
Stems/roots
Non-lexical
Derivational
Non-lexical
Inflectional
book, family,
pleasantness
and, only, of, now,
our
-vis- (invisible)
-volv-, volu-
(revolution)
-ish (bookish)
-ness (bookishness)
-s (books)
-ed (booked)
Figure 5.13. Classification of English Morphemes.
Lexical items, also called content words, include nouns, verbs,
and adjectives, the traditional
parts of speech. Although for most languages the characteristics
of meaning are similar, the formal
characteristics are widely varied. Some languages, such as
Vietnamese, are monosyllabic, so infor-
mation about number, tense, and the like occurs as separate
morphemes. Thus, identifying and dis-
tinguishing among parts of speech is difficult for ELs without
morphological marking in their Lls.
Yet, form and meaning characteristics of the lexical categories
for English are fairly well-defined, as
shown in Table 5.1 (with characteristics from Hudson, 2000).
,..
Part Two: Language and Literacy (Applied Linguistics) 78
Table 5.1. Lexical Morphemes (Parts of Speech).
Parts of Speech Formal Characteristics Meaning Characteristics
Nouns: (many) can be pluralized: -s are usually names of
animate
puppy, house, imagination, puppies beings, events, places,
concrete
birthday, Tallahassee (some types) can be made objects, or
abstract concepts
possessive:
-s puppy's bone
can be made definite (with 'the'
or indefinite (with 'a')
can be made negative with prefix
(non-believer)
Verbs: can be marked for tense (cook~ usually express actions,
being,
cook, take, identify, become, is/ coo keg) or aspect ( cooking,
states of being
are, reside have cooked)
can add suffixes to indicate agent
(bak§:, pianist)
can be made negative with prefix
(disagree, misunderstand)
Adjectives: can be marked with -(i)er word- often modifies
nouns or noun
tall, round, hungry, beautiful, finally to indicate contrast,
phrases
interesting and with -(i)est to indicate
superlative
can be used with 'more' or 'most'
can occur after 'very' or other
intensifiers
1. You have probably noted that meaning characteristics
described suggest that
there are exceptions to the 'rules'. Identify ways in which some
modifiers and
some actions can be nouns and some nouns can be actions.
2. Can you add any formal characteristics to any of the lexical
categories?
3. Another characteristic of the three lexical categories (also
called open class
words) is that new words are added to lexicon all the time, and
some words
drop out over time. Go to the Internet and search for obsolete
words, as well as
new words in the English language.
Not all words are lexical; that is, not all have both formal
characteristics and meaning proper-
ties. A limited number of words "express a grammatical or
structural relationship with other words
in a sentence" (Nordquist at http:/
/grammar.about.com/od/fh/g/functionword.htm). These
words belong to a closed class. No new words enter this class;
they have only one form, and they
do not change over time. Non-lexical or functional morphemes
in English include prepositions
(of, to), determiners (the, a), temporal adverbs (now, soon),
conjunctions (but, and), pronouns
(we, them), auxiliaries (be, have), modals (can should) and
quantifiers (some, both). Although
many prepositions do have lexical content, and they pattern like
nouns, verbs, and adjectives
at the phrasal level; they are often placed in the non-lexical
category because they behave like
other closed class words in lacking formal characteristics, not
changing over time or increasing
in number.
https://grammar.about.com/od/fh/g/functionword.htm
79 Chapter Five: The Fundamentals of Applied Linguistics:
Communication through Language
Derivational Morphemes
Derivation is a process involving the addition of a bound
morpheme, called an affix, to the beginning
or ending of an existing word. Derivational affixes are both
prefixes and suffixes in English (although
some languages also have infixes).* Examples of prefixes and
suffixes are found in Tables 5.2 and
5.3 respectively. Note that Table 5.2 shows that a prefix retains
the grammatical category of the root
word, but changes its meaning.
Prefix Change of meaning Examples
ex- former ex-wife, ex-boyfriend
deactivate, deconstruct
disappear, discontinue, dislike
de- not, or opposite
opposite dis-
mis- wrong misunderstand, misrepresent
re- again reconsider, redo
un- reverse action, or not uncover, unlock, unreliable
inconvenient, impossible -in/-im not
-con
-sub
with convene, conduct
under submarine, subterranean
Table 5.3 shows that a suffix changes the grammatical category
(form) of a word, but retains its
meaning. This distinction holds for most cases.
Learning derivational morphemes is difficult for second
language learners, especially for those
whose languages lack prefixes and suffixes. But because these
morphemes are taught to native
speakers as part of the language arts curriculum, elementary,
language arts, and English teachers
already have effective ways of teaching them.
Table 5.3. Suffixes, Change of Grammatical Category, and
Examples.
Suffix Change of Grammatical Category Examples
-able Adjective from Verb movable, doable, readable
-ing Adjective or Noun from Verb reading (class), moving
(target), moving is hard,
swimming is fun
-ive Adjective from Verb impressive, subjective
-al Noun form Verb refusal, denial, arrival
-ant Noun from Verb defendant, applicant, informant
-dorn Noun from Noun or Adjective kingdom, freedom
-ful Adjective from Noun fearful, hopeful, stressful, blissful
-ous Adjective from Noun poisonous, ruinous
-ize Verb from Noun or Adjective capitalize, hospitalize,
polarize, nationalize
-en Verb from Adjective blacken, harden
-ly Adverb from Adjective slowly, lightly
-ity Noun from Adjective superiority, priority, ability
• Tagalog, a language spoken in the Philippines uses infixes.
For example -um is the infix to form the infinitive of a
verb; sulat (write)-sumulat (to write)
80 Part Two: Language and Literacy (Applied Linguistics)
Roots and Stems
Not all bound morphemes are affixes. Some originate in Latin
or Greek languages, from which a
very great number of English words have been borrowed, often
through French or Middle English.
These stems or roots are bound lexical morphemes and they
form the primary meaning of a word.
Although some are words in their own right, others are not, as
shown in Table 5.4.
A number of resources are available to promote rapid and
systematic vocabulary development
for ELs, among them dictionaries and texts specifically devoted
to stems and affixes. However, since
ELs may overgeneralize their knowledge of a particular root or
stem and misunderstand the word's
meaning, educators should provide counter-examples to support
learning.
Table 5.4. Latin- (L) and Greek-(G)Based Stems/Roots.
Stems/Roots (language of origin, meaning) Examples
-port- (from L. portare, carry) transport, export, portable
-due- or -duct- (from L. ducere, lead) conduct, deduct, produce,
educate, inductive
-vis- (from L. videre, see) visage, invisible, visionary, visitor,
revise
-volve-, -volu- (from L. volvere, roll) involve, revolution,
voluble
-cred- (from L. credere, believe) incredible, credulous, credit,
credential
-fin- (from L. finir, end, conclusion) finish, infinite, refined
aero- (from G. akros, topmost, extreme) acrophobia, acrobat,
acronym
Inflectional Morphemes
Some of the world's languages have a great many inflectional
affixes. Romance languages such
as Spanish, Italian, and French use suffixes to indicate gender
(masculine/feminine) and number
(singular/plural) on nouns; determiners and adjectives must
agree with their corresponding nouns
by adding the same suffix, as shown in the following sentences
in Table 5.Sa. These languages also
indicate number, person, and tense on verbs using suffixes, as
shown in Table 5.Sb.
Table 5.Sa. Spanish Nouns: Number and Gender Agreement.
El libro esta
verb
en
preposition
la
determiner
mesa.
noun Determiner noun
(masculine, (masculine, (feminine, (feminine,
singular) singular)
book
libros
is located
est.in
on
singular)
the
singular)
table The
Los en las
determiner
mesas
noun Determiner noun verb preposition
(masculine, plural) (masculine, plural) (feminine, (feminine,
plural) _Elural)
The books are located on the tables.
Table S.Sb. Spanish Verb (hablar, to speak), Person, Number
and Tense Marking.
Present tense hablo(lps) hablas(2ps) habla(3ps) hablamos(lpp)
hablais(2pp) hablan(3pp)
Preterite
tense (past)
hable hablaste habl6 hablamos hablasteis hablaron
(Note: ps=person singular; pp=person plural) (http:/
/www.studyspanish.com/lessons)
81 Chapter Five: The Fundamentals of Applied Linguistics:
Communication through Language
Pause and Reflect
1. Elementary teachers: With a partner or in a small group,
address the issues in this scenario:
You have a class with Els some of whom speak Romance
languages, but several of whom do
not. How would you organize instruction of a unit on
derivational morphology that would be
fair for speakers from both backgrounds? What learning aids or
activities would you assign
to help Els learn these morphemes independently?
2. Language arts teachers: You have strategies and resources for
teaching root words and
affixes. Suggest ways you might augment your teaching of these
affixes for Els.
3. Secondary teachers: All academic areas utilize affixes as well
as root words. List several
examples in your academic content and suggest ways you might
help Els to learn them.
Swahili and other Bantu languages spoken across much of
Africa make use of even more affixes
on nouns and verbs, as demonstrated in this example:
Tulimwendeshia Dar es Salaam garini letu in
Table 5.6. We drove him to Dar es Salaam in our car. Prefixes
and infixes precede the verb stem (go),
while suffixes augment the verb's meaning. Modifiers of nouns
must agree with their noun classes,
or genders, of which there are several in Bantu languages, most
having singular and plural forms.
Table S.6. Explication of Swahili Sentence
Tu- -li- -m(w)- end(a)- -eshia Dares gari- -ni 1- -etu
Salaam
lps past 3ps go cause to car in Agr, li/ lpp our
We him/ ma
her class
sing
For English speakers, the rich inflectional morphology of
suchlanguages is usually quite difficult
to learn because English makes so few distinctions. So, should
learning the English system be easy
because of its few inflectional morphemes? Unfortunately, it
turns out that inflectional morphemes are
quite problematic for ELs, so teachers must be aware that
inflectional morphemes need to be given
a lot of attention. The chart in Table 5.7 lists these morphemes.
82 Part Two: Language and Literacy (Applied Linguistics)
Table S.7. Inflectional Morphemes of English.
Part of Speech Morphemes
Nouns Plural -s the cars;
Possessive -s Mary's car
Verbs 3rd Person Singular -s; Mary likes you.
Present Participle -ing; Mary is reading.
Past -ed; Mary liked the book.
Past Participle -ed/-en; Mary has cooked.
The eggs were broken.
Adjectives Comparative -er big - bigger
Adverbs
Superlative -est big - the biggest
-ly slowly, quickly
I
Developmental Phenomena in L2 Learning
To explain why these morphemes are so difficult for learners of
English, we must look at the facts
of a child's first language development. As you have read, in
addition to Ll transfer as a factor in
L2 learning, learner errors can also be due to the complexity of
the target language. All L2 learn-
ers, like child Ll learners, produce developmental errors that
arise when encountering difficult
aspects of a target language. Research conducted with Spanish
and Italian children learning thei:
first language showed that they acquire the functions and forms
of tense, person, gender, and num-
ber marking earlier than English speaking children learn their
inflectional system with far fewer
morphemes. This is because of the regularity and salience in the
inflectional systems of Spanish and.
Italian. Although some verb roots display irregularity with and
across tense and aspect, the ending5
(-o, -as, -amos, etc.) do not change. Note also that those endings
are perceptually salient; that is, eacl-.
one is an added syllable at the end of the verb root. It is easily
heard; each suffix either is a vowel, or
begins with a vowel, so it is processed easily. The learnability
of the Spanish and Italian systems is
less complex than that of English. Slobin (2007) and others
analyzed the learning sequences of gram-
matical morphemes by children from many different language
backgrounds and have suggested a
number of cognitive principles to explain ease and difficulty of
learning.
The learnability rule is a two-part explanation of why Spanish
and Italian inflectional systems are
easier to learn that of English. Part A: One form to one meaning
is learned earlier than one form to man:
meanings. With reference to Table 5.7, the inflectional
morpheme '-ing' has one function,; the presen:
participle indicates the progressive aspect. It also has one
invariant form and is learned early. On the
other hand, when spoken, the morpheme'-s' ( one form) can
indicate three different functions: a plura:.
a possessive, and a third person singular verb. Though the
plural form is learned somewhat early, the
other two are late. Similarly, the morpheme '-ed' has two
functions, to mark the regular past tense anc.
to indicate the regular past participle form, which can be used
with both the auxiliary 'have' and 'be
forms, in the case of the perfective aspect and the passive voice
respectively. Again, native English-
speaking children learn these forms late, especially the past
participle form. These developmental fa~
about verbs are made more difficult by the existence of both
regular and irregular past and past parti-
ciple forms. It is often surprising to hear English-speaking
children produce common irregular ver~
before the regular forms. This is because the '-ed' form, as in
'worked' or 'finished' is not as perceptual::
salient as an irregular form, as in 'broke' or 'took'.
The fact that two stop consonants occur together at the end of
the word is very difficult for man~-
non-native speakers whose languages do not allow such
clusters; the / t/ ending, as in /workt/, ~
not salient. As for verbs like 'need' and 'plant' (those that end in
/ t/ or / d/), adding -ed creates
an extra syllable, making the ending salient. In fact, when
learning to read English aloud, man:-
learners overgeneralize the /Id/ form, producing' /laikld/ (lik-
ed) or /muvld/ (mov-ed), assum-
ing these verbs pattern with /nidld/ (needed). Although for
learners the '-ed' form is usually no:
salient, the irregular forms are.
83 Chapter Five: The Fundamentals of Applied Linguistics:
Communication through Language
Pause and Reflect
1. To understand the concept of perceptual salience, pronounce
the words 'like' and 'liked' to
yourself. Note that both have only one syllable and that the /t/
follows /k/, forming a cluster
of two voiceless stops.
2. Why do you think the /t/ is difficult to hear in rapid speech?
For example, learners might produce "I broke it" or "I ate it"
before they produce "I pushed it"
or "I cooked it." Later, when they begin to understand the
function of'-ed', they overgeneralize with
irregular verbs, producing such utterances as "I breaked it," "I
broked it," or "I eated it." Language
learners, noticing that recurring patterns are found in language,
create rules to apply to new situa-
tions. They use these rules all the time; generalization is a
highly effective process in learning. Edu-
cators must recognize that learners have created a rule when
they overgeneralize, or extend the rule.
Part B of the learnability rule is that one meaning to one form is
learned earlier flum one meaning to
many forms. A good example pertains to the pronunciation of
the past tense '-ed' endings of regular
verbs. Table 5.8 lists examples of this language phenomenon at
the intersection of phonology and
morphology.
Table S.S. Regular Past Tense Verb Forms.
liked fixed missed jailed picked
buzzed banned inferred laughed moved
begged finished dropped freed formed
watched lobbed judged hanged stayed
(V
9 Pause and Reflect
1. With a partner or in a small group pronounce the regular past
tense forms in Table 5.8, and
note whether you produce /t/ or/d/ for the regular '-ed' ending.
Do not distort the pronun-
ciation, but produce it as you would in normal conversation.
2. Put the verbs into either a /t/ list or a / d/ list; then, explain
your reasoning. Hint: Note the
voicing of /t/ and /d/.
3. Do you think native English-speaking children have to learn
how to pronounce these verb
endings correctly? Or does this come 'naturally' to them?
84 Part Two: Language and Literacy (Applied Linguistics)
d,Je 5.10. Sentence Types .
.~--=:nr~-itic:e Negative Yes/No Wh- question Emphasis Tag
question Passive
question
-~ buys
groceries
Ari doesn't Does Ari buy Where does Ari does buy Ari buys A
lot of
buy groceries Ari buy groceries in groceries groceries
□ Tampa. groceries in Tampa? groceries? Tampa. in Tampa, are
bought
in Tampa. doesn't he? in Tampa.
:.-.c ··o..:::-
~O".!: ~
- ~--.-..- ......... ,.,;- ~ _;...,;,., -
~crru.a~
-.e: :a:-..g
For ELs, particularly adolescents and adults, learning three
separate morpho-phonemes for the
-ed' ending (the It/ in 'liked', the /d/ in 'loved', and the /'-Id/ in
'needed') presents the one mean-
:ng, many forms learning problem. English-speaking children
probably pick up this pronunciation
2asily - they do not have to be taught how to do it. You may
recall from the phonology reading
:hat some languages have a 'devoicing' feature on consonants at
the ends of words, or have no
consonant clusters, or lack voiced stops or fricatives word-
finally. So, it is clear that learning to hear,
then produce these endings will probably take some focused
explicit instruction. Note that the plural-s morpheme also has
three forms: /s/ as in 'pipes', /z/ as in 'cars', and /Iz/ as in
'churches'. The following minimal pairs of noun plurals can
be used to help learners hear, then produce the voiced/voice-
less distinction: pigs/picks, buzzes/buses, mates/maids,
calfs/calves, mobs/mops. In sum, in the case of one mean-
ing/many forms, first language effects join with develop-
mental effects to produce significant learnability difficulties.
:::::
Problems in English Verb Morphology
Co~ared to Spanish, the English inflectional morphological
system is 'impoverished, there being
:e'•,- endings or auxiliaries included in the English verb phrase.
However, the system can be quite
:::.ii.'.ficult for all learners and may not be mastered at all by
non-native speakers. In fact, the order
0: learning English morphemes forms the basis of a claim (by
some second language acquisition
:-oe.archers) that developmental phenomenon, as opposed to
transfer, better predicts the production
Gf :-1 English inflectional morphology across time because of
its complexity.
a:ble 5.9. Auxiliaries and Modals and their Functions.
I' re used with have used with do used with modals used with
I a::-.., is, V + ing; have, V + -ed or do, does, V (unmark-ed)
can, will, V (unmark-ed)
a.re, progressive has, -en did does eat, did shall, can see, might
~.• •asl running, had cooked, go could, go
;;·ere eating eaten, would,
V +-ed or -en broken should,
Passive may,
are seen, i§ might,
eaten must,
ought to
Tne lists in Table 5.9 portray the auxiliaries (helping verbs) of
English, the forms of 'be', 'have',
.::n .and the modals in columns 1, 3, 5, and 7 respectively, with
the verb forms they 'help' in columns
:. ..;,, 6, and 8 respectively. Each of these (usually) behaves in
the same way in affirmative statements,
?~es::ions (yes/no, wh-), negative statements, emphatic
statements, tags, and passives.
Table 5.10 portrays seven main sentence types using the 3<<l
person verb 'buy' in the present
::c:-.se. ::-(ote how the verb is altered across the sentence types.
j_
3. -
85 Chapter Five: The Fundamentals of Applied Linguistics:
Communication through Language
Pause and Reflect
1. Describe what happens to the verb with each type,
particularly the placement of the auxiliary
or modal in question formation (linguists call this aux-
movement).
2. Change the same verb to the past tense for each sentence type
and describe what happens.
3. Try completing the seven columns of the chart with the 1st
person present perfect aspect of the
verb; for example, 'We have already eaten lunch.' and the verb
with a modal, for example,
'They should finish by noon.' and the 2nd person past
progressive aspect of the verb, 'You
were running the race.' What differences in the patterns do you
observe?
In sum, we have learned that morphemes are the smallest
meaningful units of a language, that
:hey can be free or bound, and that bound morphemes are either
derivational or inflectional. Deriva-
:ional morphology is taught in language arts curricula because
all students need to understand and
:ise the morphemes. But students whose languages lack
derivations may need more explicit instruc-
:ion; L1 effects occur because such morphemes are absent in the
Ll, not because something is dif-
.:erent. Inflectional morphology is quite problematic because of
both Ll and developmental effects.
:-he basis for this claim is to be found by examining several
examples of the leamability rule: in the
sequence of learning for all learners of English one form/one
meaning precedes and is easier than
one form/many meanings, or one meaning/many forms.
The examples should guide teachers to become more aware that
L2 learning often needs a boost
°:)ecause of Lleffects, developmental effects, or both. Some
theories of second language learning pro-
:note the idea that input alone is enough to initiate the process
and keep it going 'naturally.' How-
e·er, errors in reception and production may persist if not
addressed early on, especially for all but
:he youngest learners. Whether you agree with the habit
formation theory or not, explicit instruction
about English morphology and its phonological manifestations
is essential for all students.
Syntax
:n 1959 Noam Chomsky, the most important linguist of the 20th
century, challenged the claims of
3. F. Skinner, a noted behaviorist, that language learning can be
explained as a series of stimulus/
~ sponse events. Chomsky presented an alternative idea, namely
that language is innate, and that
~anguage learning, or acquisition, is possible because a child is
already predisposed to activate his/
ier language organ. Over the years Chomsky and his followers,
using data from many different
-
86 Part Two: Language and Literacy (Applied Linguistics)
languages, have developed and constantly revised a theory to
explain
an innate disposition for language, and what was once referred
to as
the language acquisition device (LAD). These linguists prefer to
use the term acquire, as opposed to learn, to refer to
subconscious
processes that occur during the early years of life. Chomsky's
major
work focuses on the structural level of language called syntax,
which
entails the units beyond the word level- phrases and sentences.
Theoretical Issues with Syntax
So far in this chapter, you have read about phonology, the sound
system, and morphology, word forms and functions. These
systems
are also structural components of language; they are systematic
and
rule-governed, as is syntax. It is important to point out that
Chom-
skians attempt to understand language more at the level of
structure
than of meaning. Meaning, as well as communication, he has
claimed,
should be beyond the purview of structural linguistics.
In any language, a sequence of words may represent a sentence.
But a sentence in a lan-
guage cannot simply be just any sequence of words; it must
conform to the rules underlying it.
For example, the rules of English syntax do not generate, or
describe, the sequence 'girl book
see.' The elements are out of order, the verb does not indicate
tense or number and articles are
missing. Native speakers are able to recognize word
combinations that violate these rules as
ungrammatical, thus incorrect. This is not necessarily based on
a speaker's familiarity with
prescriptive language rules, but comes from knowledge often
outside of awareness, acquired as
we grow up. Moreover, the grammaticality of a sentence is not
necessarily linked to meaning.
Chomsky's (1957) famous sentence colorless green ideas sleep
furiously doesn't make sense, but it
conforms to the correct ordering of English and illustrates the
separation of grammaticality and
meaning.
Lexical and Functional Categories
In order to analyze the structure of phrases and sentences, a
review of word-level categories is
essential. Languages have two major groups of words: lexical
and functional . These categories are
listed in Table 5.11 (or refer back to Table 5.1). Linguists
assume that all languages have the same
categories. Functional categories are listed in Table 5.12.
Universals in Word Ordering
What is important about these categories of words is that in any
given language they are ordered
in specific ways; this is a universal aspect of all languages. The
ordering rules of English word
order are quite strict, and the order subject/verb/ (object) (SVO)
is the canonical, or typical,
order (The object category is parenthesized because not all
sentences have objects). However,
SVO is not the only possible word order, though it is
widespread throughout the world, espe-
cially in Western Europe. Languages of Korea, Japan, North
Africa, and the Inda-Iranian family
are predominantly subject/ (object)/verb (SOV). Verb-first
orders are less common (though they
include Arabic and several Celtic languages); object-first
languages are rarer still, with American
Sign Language (ASL) being among the very few of the OVS
type, and Klingon the only OS"
language.
87 Chapter Five: The Fundamentals of Applied Linguistics:
Communication through Language
Table S,11. Lexical Categories.
Lexical categories Examples
Noun book, city, girl, the playground, idea
Verb write, read, sing, become, live, be
Adjective lazy, slow, large interesting, young
Preposition on, under, to, of, at
Table 5.12. Functional Categories.
Functional category Examples
Determiners (Det)
Article
Possessive
Demonstrative
a, the
her, our
this, those
Auxiliaries (Aux)
Modal
Non-modal
will, can, may, might, must, could, etc.
be,have,do
Conjunctions (Con) and, or, but
Degree word (Deg) too, quite, only, etc.
Pause and Reflect
1. Create some sequences of words that do not constitute a
sentence, and any other sequences
that 'obey the rules' of English, but do not make sense.
2. Because Els come from a variety of L2 backgrounds, not all
of whom share the English word
order in sentences, they benefit from tasks that require putting
words in the correct order. You
can cut up sentences and mix the words from each sentence so
that students can reassemble
the sentences. Or you can provide specific Apps to support Els'
learning by going to the
ESOL website for suggestions.
Case-Marking
In examining Japanese and Korean, we find that the word order
may be a bit looser than it
is in English. That is because in these languages nouns are
assigned case according to their
functions in the sentence, such as subject, object, possessive,
instrumental, and so on. In each
88 Part Two: Language and Literacy (Applied Linguistics)
.:
..
situation a case marker indicates the function of the noun. For
example, a grammatical sentence
in Japanese might be expressed as 'boy' (subject marker) 'horse'
(object marker) kick'. Or it
might read as 'horse (object marker) boy (subject marker) kick'.
Both sentences have the same
meaning because the case, not the word order, indicates who is
doing what to whom. Because
Japanese is a head-final word order language, however, the verb
must come last, no matter what
the order of the noun phrase. The verb phrase is the core of
sentence structure, and, as noted
previously, there are two options for the majority of world
languages: verb first (English) and
Yerb last (Japanese).
Although many of the world's languages have no case-marking
at all, others have 10 or more.
The most common case is nominative (subject); accusative
(direct object) is also common. In
addition to Japanese and Korean, other case-marked languages
are Latin, Slavic (Russian, Ukarainian, Polish, Czech, Bulgar-
ian, Serbo-Croatian, etc.), Baltic (Lithuanian and Latvian), and
Finno-Urgaic, (Finnish, Hungarian, Estonian). Luthuanian has
14 cases. Old and Middle English had case-marking on nouns,
but Modern English retains case only on pronouns. Each per-
son and number has subject, object, and possessive forms. For
example the first person singular forms are 'I', 'me', and 'my'
respectively; the third person plural forms are 'they', 'them',
and 'their' respectively. These forms are usually learned late
in first language acquisition, and may also pose difficulties
for ELs.
Most educators can only imagine that English speakers
learning case-marked languages would find this aspect quite
difficult. However, for speakers of these languages learn-
ing English, the lack of case marking poses problems in ways
that affect the ordering of phrases within an English sentence.
Speakers of case-marked languages have the freedom to order
elements to provide focus where they want it and do not neces-
sarily follow a strict word order. The following examples from a
narrative about a naughty baby owlet told by a native bilingual
speaker of Russian and Czech, both case-marked languages,
demonstrate this difficulty. Note that
me speaker, Natasha, places focused information at the end of
the sentence in ways different from
;:he way the information would be placed by English speakers.
1. And then first egg, braked and was born Frankie. And
afterward second, from second egg
was born Marushka. And after another while was born, Bedjik.
2. When she wanted to show her husband their new babies, it
was in the bed only Frankie and
Marushka.
3. After summer start school.
4. After this movie Bedjik played with Marushka and Frankie
that he is bandit and he wants
to shoot them.
.:>. Marushka and Frankie saw ... that came, and spoke with
Bedjik this big animal, red animal
Lishka (fox).
6. And Lishka saw that he is so non-educated and he doesn't
know who she is and then she
w ants to play with him game.
1. After carefully examining the ways in which Natasha orders
her sentences,
imagine that she is a student in your classroom. With a partner
or in a small
group, discuss ways to help Natasha with word order problems
(ignore for the
moment some other problems such as omission of articles).
89 Chapter Five: The Fundamentals of Applied Linguistics:
Communication through Language
Order of Elements in Phrases
A very interesting aspect of languages is that by and large the
noun, verb, adjective, and prepo-
sitional phrases usually follow the same pattern. For example,
in the English verb phrase 'kicked
:he horse', the verb precedes its object, while in the Korean
phrase 'horse (object marker) kicked',
:he verb follows the object. The verb is called the head of the
phrase, and its object is called a com-
plement, because it completes the phrase (phrases may also
include specifiers which function to
make the head precise*). At the very least, the obligatory
element in a phrase is the head. There can-
not be a phrase without a head in its category. Examining the
other phrases in English or Korean,
we find that all phrase types follow the same order as the verb
displays. The examples in Table 5.13
illustrate how specifiers, heads, and complements are ordered in
English.
Pause and Reflect
1. Using a reference, or based on your own experiences,
construct a chart of phrases from
another language similar to those in Table 5. 13.
Table 5.13. English Phrase Structure, Head First.
Phrase Types Specifier Head Complement
>Joun Phrase (NP) The
A
boy
pig
wearing a tie
Verb Phrase (VP) operates
goes
a machine
Adverb Phrase (AP) very clean
hard as a rock
Prepositional Phrase (PP) just under
around
the trees
the comer
Although basic phrase structure word orders for most languages
are either head-first or head-last,
rules for arrangements within these basic structures may be
more complicated. For example, exam-
ine the string of words in Table 5.14 and place them in
grammatical order.
Table S.14, Complex NP.
ICereal IThose IYellow IBowls ILarge IThree
This string is not a sentence, but a noun phrase with several
adjectives. Begin by identifying the noun
that is the head of the phrase. Native English speakers almost
always agree on the order, but often cannot
explain the 'rule' that describes it. When given this string of
words to put in order, non-native speakers
experience difficulties because ofits complexity. This example
demonstrates that as the second language
(L2) develops, all learners have to deal with ways in which
English is challenging to learn.
* Specifier type is determined by the phrase head. For example,
in the noun phrase (NP) 'the book' the specifier can be a
determiner (Det), in this case 'the'. But an adverb cannot be a
specifier in an NP: 'slowly book' is not grammatical.
___ _
?O Part Two: Language and Literacy (Applied Linguistics)
d®
Pause and Reflect
.._,
l. Now that you have seen how English orders phrases, translate
each of the words in the
phrase above into Spanish or another language you may know.
2. Arrange them in their correct grammatical order. Is the order
the same as that for English
noun phrases? Do you have to add any words to make a good
translation?
1. Construct similar examples based on the same pattern.
2. When you recognize what kinds of adjectives occur in which
position, create
a chart like Table 5.13 and label each column to show the order
of the various
types of modifiers.
ELs will not always have a 'feel' for the correct order, and will
need to be taught. By providing a
5eneralized ordering rule, teachers will be able to guide
students in overcoming English word order
;:ii.-=ficulties. You may have noticed that the head of the noun
phrase comes, not at the beginning, but
a': the end when this string of words is put in order. This is
because single word adjectives occur
::-eiore the noun in English, while adjectival phrases and
clauses follow the noun, thus preserving
::.."-le normal head-first phrase structure pattern.
When students see several examples of the same pattern, they
can recognize, and later follow,
::..'--;.e rules to create a well-ordered phrase. Verb, adjective,
and prepositional phrases may also be
complex, so learners need practice putting them in order as
well.
Prescriptive Grammar Rules
K."owledge of English word order is typically not taught in
American schools because native speak-
2r5 acquire it as children. More often than not, however,
grammar is formally taught in foreign
:.a....-1guage courses in order to present the structure of the
language. Frequently the grammatical rules
taught in upper elementary language arts and
secondary English courses in American schools
are prescriptive, rather than descriptive rules,
those that portray how the language actually
works. Many so-called 'rules' are actually quite
old and were proposed by English grammarians
based on a study of Latin. For example, the 'rule'
do not end a sentence with a preposition works for
Latin, but not for English. We accept "who are
you going with?" even though the question
ends with a preposition. Another incorrect rule
is use a singular verb with a plural subject and a plu-
ral verb with a singular subject. In examining the
....,, .....::.RuLcS
(}) ___
(f)®·-----
@
91 Chapter Five: The Fundamentals of Applied Linguistics:
Communication through Language
;e.ntences in Table 5.15, you can see that the rule is not correct.
Neither 'I' nor 'you', both singular,
::-equires the verb to be marked with '-s'.
In fact, only one person (3rd) takes a so-called 'plural' verb.
But, as noted in the morphology sec-
jon, the '-s' does not represent a plural, but simply denotes the
third person singular form of the
? resent tense verb. In older versions of English, verbs were
marked for person and number, just as in
Spanish and other European languages. Only the 3rd person
singular form has remained in English. In
some dialects of English it, too, is not used, and may someday
disappear from English all together. All
students need to understand how a language really works in
normal spoken and written discourse.
Table S.1 S. The third person singular rule.
Person, Number,
Singular
Subject Verb Person, Number,
Plural
Subject Verb
1st I read }'I we read
2nd you read 2nd you read
3rt1 he, she, it reads 3,ct they read
In sum, linguist Noam Chomsky claimed that language is an
organ, just as the heart and lungs
are organs, providing us with an innate linguistic capacity that
allows young children to acquire
:canguage out of awareness. Because language is an organ, he
argues, we are born with the capac-
:ty to use the organ already partially pre-programmed, such as
with the knowledge that language
obeys certain ordering rules, with all phrase types (noun, verb,
etc.) being consistently either head-
:irst or head-final across the board. Korean children are not
born with a head-last word order, nor
are Spanish-speaking children born with a head-first order.
Instead, as children hear their first
:anguage, they figure out the phrase structure rules and apply
them when learning to speak.
With respect to second language learners, the ordering rules of
the first language may produce
:ransfer effects on later-learned languages, though they might
not result in errors as transparent as
those at the phonological level. In Natasha's case, the fact that
her two Lls are case-marked allowed
arrangement of elements less constrained than those of English.
On the other hand, the example of
the complex noun phrase (about the cereal bowls) illustrates
that developmental effects are also at
play. It takes a long period for such complex structures to be
fully learned.
It is important to note that the work of Chomsky, his students,
and his other followers is formi-
dable inn its detail and depth of insight about the workings of
language. Nonetheless, his theory
has been challenged by other linguists, including developmental
psycholinguists, whose data raise
questions about the notion of a pre-programmed language
template. Yet, no matter whose theory
educators espouse, access to the right kinds of information
about how languages really work in
conversation and writing is essential. To appropriately and
adequately understand and address
ELs' needs, educators must acknowledge a variety of factors and
different theoretical explanations.
Semantics
You have read about phonology (system of sounds), morphology
(word forms and formation pro-
cesses), and syntax (how words join to form larger units), and
the three levels of language considered to
be innate, according to the Chomskian perspective; the next two
levels are semantics and pragmatics,
both seen as learned aspects of language. "Semantics is the
branch of linguistics thatdeals with the study
of meaning, changes in meaning, and the principles that govern
the relationship between sentences or
words and their meanings" (http:/
/www.thefreedictionary.com/semantics). In this section, you
will
find information about the lexicon, ideas for teaching
vocabulary, assigning vs. containing meaning,
denotation and connotation semantic relations at word and
sentence level, and ambiguity. Throughout
this section are suggestions for discussion and for ways to assist
English learners from PreK-12.
www.thefreedictionary.com/semantics
92 Part Two: Language and Literacy (Applied Linguistics)
Experimental research has revealed that words are organized
and stored in many different ways
in the mind. For example, if you can't remember a word, you
may remember its beginning sound,
form a mental representation of the word (such as 'dog'), or
locate the word according to its cat-
egory. Table 5.16 provides a list of categories of word
knowledge (Nation, 1990) which help to illus-
trate how words are stored in the mental lexicon.
Table 5.16. Types of Word Knowledge.
Examples - deep
Spoken form of a word
Types of Word Knowledge
/dip/ I
d-e-e-p
Morphemes possible
Written form of a word
deep (adjective); also depth (noun), deepen (verb), deep
-er, -er -est (comparative, superlative forms) deeply
(adverb)
Grammatical behavior of a word The pool is deep (predicate
adjective).
He's going into deep water (adjective before noun).
It's too deep for wading (adjective with specifier &
complement).
Collocational behavior of a word (as) deep (as the ocean), often
used with words pertaining to
bodies of water or places below !!round
Frequency of a word how often it occurs in speech or writing
Stylistic register constraints; when a word is
inappropriate to use
It would be inappropriate to use slang or swear words when
speaking with a person who is interviewing you for a job.
Conceptual meaning of a word Representation of a small figure
with a large amount of
water, for example, beneath it.
Associations with related words Deep: shallow (antonym),
profound (synonym), far down
in water or underground place (paraphrase)
Table 5.17 provides a list of principles of vocabulary learning
(Schmitt and Schmitt, 1995) whirr
are also based on an understanding of the mental lexicon.
Table S.17. Principles of Vocabulary Learning.
1. The best way to remember new words is to incorporate them
in (into) to language that is already known.
2. Organized material is easier to learn.
3. Words which are very similar should not be taught at the
same time.
-!. Word pairs can be used to learn a great number of words in a
short time .
.J. Knowing a word entails more than just knowing the meaning.
6. The deeper the mental processing used when learning a word,
the more likely a student will
remember it.
7. The act of recalling a word makes it more likely that the
learner will recall it again later.
8. Learners must pay attention in order to learn more
effectively.
9. Words need to be recycled to be learned.
10. Learners are individuals with different learning styles.
Vocabulary learning in the classroom can be done in many
different ways. Mendoza (2003) cre-
ated suggestions for university level English learners based on
her doctoral research. Some of these
suggestions are well-suited for younger learners, as well as
middle and secondary ELs. She advise.::
learners to make new words on their own in at least three of the
following ways:
a . draw a picture
b. write a translation
c. use it in a sentence
d . write its part of speech
Chapter Five: The Fundamentals of Applied Linguistics:
Communication through Language ~-
e. write its various forms
f. write a synonym and an antonym
g. say the word aloud several times
h. use it with your friends
i. put a new word list on a bulletin board or chart
j. keep an alphabetized list of words in your notebook
k. organize your words by topic or by part of speech
1. make a list of words with similar meanings
m. organize words according to their Greek or Latin roots
n. locate a word in the dictionary and analyze its parts
(prefixes, suffixes, roots).
Pause and Reflect
1. Recall from the morphology section that all words, or lexical
and non-lexical items, are free
morphemes, and that word parts are bound. Explain whether you
think bound morphemes
are also stored in the mental lexicon w ith their meanings/uses,
or whether they are stored
with individual free morphemes.
2. For information about language and the brain, visit:
http://emedia.leeward.hawaii.edu/
hurley/Ling 102web/mod5_Llearning/5mod5.1 _neuro.htm.
What It Means to Mean: Do Words Contain Meaning?
Early in thenineteenthcenturyinCentral Europe, semantics
emerged as a separate subfield of linguis-
tics to study meaning (http:/ /www.studymode.com/ essays/
The-History-Of-Semantics-1550608.
html). According to the Western Rationalist position, meanings
are not contained in words (or larger
units), but are assigned to these units by speakers and hearers
over time in public discourse. If mean-
ings were contained in language, they would not change over
time, although we know that they
do. Similarly, because there are so many meanings assigned to
some words in the lexicon, a single
conceptualization can be difficult to pin down. For example, the
meaning of run in 'run a race' is not
the same as run in 'run a business'.
The core meaning of run is 'to move quickly on foot' because
speakers of English understand
it that way. Yet we might instead have started using the word
'bleb' (a non-word), or 'correr' (the
word in Spanish) instead of 'run'. Specific words are simply
arbitrary combinations of sounds that
are assigned meanings by speakers. Whenever a new concept
emerges, somebody names it, the
name spreads, and soon it becomes a part of the lexicon of
speakers. When a new edition of a dic-
tionary appears, it contains these new words in the communal
lexicon.
Adults are able to distinguish between a word and its referent.
Yet, developmental studies have
shown that very young children do not always conceptually
separate words from their referents.
Some research has determined that bilingual children have an
advantage over monolinguals in this
regard. When shown a picture of the moon, told it is a 'bleb,'
and asked if the picture is still of a moon,
the very young monolingual English-speaking child will say
"no." But because Spanish/ English
www.studymode.com
http://emedia.leeward.hawaii.edu
94 Part Two: Language and Literacy (Applied Linguistics)
bilingual children of the same age have already learned that the
moon can be called luna and moon,
they are likely to say that the picture is still the moon, despite
its being called 'bleb.' They know that
two different words can have the same referent.
6®
Pause and Reflect
l. With a partner or in a small group discuss ways you have
observed vocabulary learning in
the classroom.
2. Have you had success with any of the methods listed above?
3. How would you encourage Els to identify and use their
individual preferences for learning
new words?
4. Mendoza encouraged her students to personalize their
vocabularies by dividing a 5x8 card
into four sections and writi ng one of the options listed above
on each section, as shown in
Figure 5.14. How can the use of technology support this
strategy?
Definition and Part of Speech:
credible (adjective), believable
Use in a sentence:
His story about the accident was not credible.
I think he was lying.
Translation:
Words with same root:
incredible, credit, credulous, creditor
(from Latin, credere), accreditation
Figure 5.14. Individual Vocabulary Learning Technique.
Denotation and Connotation
Two terms are important to address at this point, connotation
and denotation, each of which is an
aspect of meaning. Word denotation is presented in dictionary
descriptions; for example, the
word 'rat' is defined in Merriam-Webster's Online Dictionary as
"any of numerous rodents (rat-
tus and related genera) differing from the related mice
especially by considerably larger size."
This is its denotative meaning; however, this does not constitute
the variety of associations this
word can evoke. For many people, a rat calls to mind feelings of
disgust, fear, or even plague.
In the Chinese culture, however, the rat is the first sign of the
zodiac and has several positive
associations; " .... attributes of an animal with spirit, wit,
alertness, delicacy, flexibility and vital-
ity" (http:/
/www.travelchinaguide.com/intro/social_customs/zodiac/rat.htm
). The word rat
connotes a variety of associations in one's mind; these are both
culturally and individually influ-
enced, or they may be personal or restricted to small groups of
people, such as a family or a
sub-culture.
www.travelchinaguide.com/intro/social_customs/zodiac/rat.htm
95 Chapter Five: The Fundamentals of Applied Linguistics:
Communication through Language
6(1}
Pause and Reflect
1. Check dictionary definitions for five common words, for
example, 'marriage.' Write the deno-
tation and connotations for each (refer to the 'rat' example).
2. Find examples from different cultural backgrounds. In
mainstream American culture a couple
usually makes the major decisions regarding their future life
together; but, elsewhere mar-
riages may be arranged by parents. What do you think 'marriage'
connotes for a prospec-
tive bride who is only twelve or thirteen years old?
Semantic Relations
Several kinds of semantic relations exist among words, as
shown in Table 5.19. Categorizing words
in these ways assists learners to extend their vocabularies;
flashcards and a variety of games enable
students to learn words independently or in small groups.
The terms subject and predicate name syntactic roles, but they
are not so useful when it comes to
meaning, and will not take ELs far when struggling with
sentence meanings. Noun phrases in Eng-
lish fulfill various kinds of semantic roles. Four are listed in
Table 5.18, along with definitions and
examples (from Hudson, 2000). Recognizing the semantic role
of noun phrases can help readers,
including ELs, better understand the connections among various
parts of a sentence.
Table S.18. Semantic Roles.
Agent doer, actor The woman came here.
Patient entity affected by what agent or
causal agent does
The police punished the
teen-agers.
Recipient receiver of deed of agent I bought the gift for my
mother.
Experiencer preceiver of a stimulus Rain pleased the farmers.
Six additional roles are location, instrument, time, stimulus,
cause, and goal. Recognizing the
semantic role of noun phrases can help second language readers
better understand connections
among various parts of a sentence, as will be shown in the
section on ambiguity.
Concepts
Word meaning is closely related to how concepts are
represented in the mind. An earlier way of
thinking about concepts is that necessary and sufficient
conditions exist for an object to be called a
certain thing. For example, the defining conditions of 'bird' are
generally thought to be something
like [animal, small, able to fly]; although, we would have to
admit that ostriches arebirds though they
are neither small, nor do they fly. Amore recent view is that
concepts lack clear-cut boundaries; there
are 'fuzzy' boundaries around them. Thus, if you try to state the
necessary and sufficient conditions
96 Part Two: Language and Literacy (Applied Linguistics)
for something to be a chair or a game or a bird, you will find
some chairs, games, or birds that don't
fit the core definition. Certain fossil forms demonstrate a
biological link between ancient reptiles and
birds. So, are they birds? Are 'Ring around the Rosy' or 'London
Bridges' games in the same way that
Monopoly and football are games? Is a beanbag a chair, even
though it doesn't have legs?
Table S.19. Semantic Relations at the Word Level (From Yule,
2010).
1. With a partner or in a small group, make a short list of
necessary and sufficient
conditions for two or three of the following: dog, cup, table,
bachelor, cushion,
coat, friend.
2. Identify counter-examples for your conceptual descriptions.
What 'fuzzy areas'
did you encounter? Were some of the examples 'fuzzier' than
others?
3. Make a Venn diagram to represent the relationship between
the concepts
'language' and 'communication.' Are they two separate
concepts? Or does one
concept include the other? Or do they overlap, or mean the same
thing?
Although concepts might be fuzzy within a language, they
become even more problematic across
languages. For example, the word 'aunt' in English may
represent any of five relationships: mother's
sister, father's sister, mother's brother's wife, father's brother's
wife, or close family female friend of the
parents' generation. However, no such generic word as 'aunt'
exists in Vietnamese, for there are differ-
ent words for various people in familial relationships according
to the following criteria: the side of the
family (mother's or father's), birth status in the parents'
respective families (older or younger sibling of
the parent), and relationships by marriage. Although it may
seem strange to be required to learn all of
these possible relationships, it is essential for the young child to
do so, for every persons/he meets is
addressed, not by using a word for 'you', but by the label of one
of these relationships ("Good morning,
mom's younger sister word, Thuy"; or, Greetings, father's elder
brother word, Tam). The point to be made
here is that semantics, including word meaning, creates the
bridge between language and culture,
for it is at this intersection where meaning is ascribed to events,
objects, roles, and relationships among
members of a culture. Meanings are culture-bound in
immeasurable and often surprising ways.
1. If you come from a culture other than the so-called
'mainstream' American
culture, identify two concepts that exist in your (sub-) culture,
or your region
of the country, that are absent in the generic North American
context. It may
be that the other cultural background influences culture in the
United States in
ways that havebecome 'mainstream' such as, having pinatas
atbirthday parties.
~
~
2. Among the overriding concepts that define Americans are the
related ideas
of individualism and independence. What do you think may be
the origins of
Semantic relation
Synonomy, same meaning
Polysemy, refers to words having multiple
Examples
big/large, fat/thin, wrong/mistaken
bank, chair, foot
meanings
Homophony, same sound bear/ bare, hear/ here, two/ too
Antonymy Opposites: wide/narrow; gradable: long stick/long
car; complementary: single/ married; relational:
Hyponomy, meaning of one word included in
fold/ unfold
dog/animal, carrot/vegetable, rose/flower
meaning of another; pertains to subordinate &
superordinate categories
97 Chapter Five: The Fundamentals of Applied Linguistics:
Communication through Language
these concepts? If your own background, or that of some of your
students,
highlights the value of different concepts, how do these
influence behavior and
performance in the classroom?
Metaphors
Another important aspect of word meaning is metaphor, not
simply a literary device used by writ-
ers, but a pervasive feature of all languages. Examples in
English include the commodity metaphor
for talking about time (we waste, save, spend, and lose time),
the conduit metaphor for 'packaging'
mental activities (we get points across, put our thoughts into
words, and get through to each other),
and the gun metaphor (shooting for next week, no silver bullet,
parting shot, targeting). In learning
another language, metaphors are fun without even recognizing
that one's own language has them
as well; however, metaphors are often difficult for second
language learners.
Pause and Reflect
1. Make a list of metaphors using the names of body parts in
English (the head of the bed, the
leg of a chair, the long arm of justice).
2. After composing a substantial list, make generalizations
about some of them. Do you know
of other languages that use body part words metaphorically?
Semantic Relations: Meaning beyond Word Level
Semantic relations also exist among sentences as shown in
Table 5.20. When a person 'knows' a
language well, these various relationships within and among
sentences are usually quite obvious,
but they are not so obvious for ELs. For example, indirect
speech acts can be difficult for learners.
If you say "It's really hot in here" and the person does not
respond by offering to open the window,
he may not have understood your indirect request. Non-native
speakers may also appear rude when
they say something in a direct manner that native speakers
express indirectly, such as "I want you
to give me x" rather than "I could certainly use x." In addition,
other direct requests most Americans
consider inappropriate are: "How much money do you make?"
and "How old are you?"
1. With a partner or in a small group, write five indirect
statements by English
speakers.
2. Check with people from other cultural backgrounds, or use
the Internet, to
search for examples of indirectness.
98 Part Two: Language and Literacy (Applied Linguistics)
Table 5.20. Semantic Relations within/Across Sentences.
Semantic relations Definition Examples
Paraphrase one statement with a meaning
similar to another statement
The cat chased the mouse.
The mouse was chased by the cat.
Entailment
Contradiction
Presupposition
Deixis
one statement entailing the
consequence of the second
two contrary statements
a statement assuming a pre-
existing truth
elements in a statement implying
the location of the speaker
The hunter killed the bear.
The bear is dead.
Charles is a bachelor.
Charles is married.
When did you stop beating your
wife?
A bear is going into the tent.
A bear is coming into the tent!
Speech Acts associations with the utterance of
a statement
'I can't reach the salt' being an
indirect way of telling someone
to pass it.
'I pronounce you man and wife'
being an act that produces a
concrete result.
Ambiguity
Sentences or phrases in a language can be ambiguous; that is,
they may be understood in more than
one way. For example, the simple phrase 'a large hat box' may
refer to a box for large hats or a large
box for hats (of any size). This is a structurally ambiguous
phrase because we don't know whether
the referent of 'large' is a hat or a box. Ambiguity may also be
lexical. For example, in the phrase
'put the money in the bank' the word 'bank' might refer to a
place where people go to exchange
money, or it could refer to the area beside a river. Plays on
words, such as puns are a wonderful source
of humor, but can also challenge ELs. Lucas studied how
intensive ELs disambiguated puns; for
example, the ambiguity in these two plays on words:
1. One morning I shot an elephant in my pajamas. How he got
into my pajamas I'll never
know."
2. Groucho Marx: Let's talk about rights and lefts. You're right,
so I left.
Ambiguity is found in phrases where words are left out, as in
newspaper headlines. The ambi-
guity could be caused by omission of words or by modifiers in
the wrong place. For example, in the
phrase 'miners refuse to work after death;' it would helpful to
resolve the ambiguity if it had said
'another miner's death.' Table 5.21 provides a list of several
humorous headlines. As you can well
imagine, many of these pose considerable difficulties for ELs.
Table 5.21. Ambiguous Phrases.
1. Panda Mating Fails; Veterinarian Takes Over
2. Juvenile Court to Try Shooting Defendant
3. Miners Refuse to Work after Death
4. British Left Waffles on Falkland Island
5. Typhoon Rips Through Cemetery; Hw1dreds Dead
6. Man Struck By Lightning Faces Battery Charge
7. Include Your Children When Baking Cookies
8. Something Went Wrong in Jet Crash, Expert Says
9. Police Begin Campaign to Run Down Jaywalkers, Safety
Experts Say
10. School Bus Passengers Should be Belted
11. Drwlk Gets Nine Months in Violin Case
99 Chapter Five: The Fundamentals of Applied Linguistics:
Communication through Language
Table 5.21. Ambiguous Phrases. (Continued)
12. Enraged Cow Injures Farmer with Ax
13. Two Sisters Reunite After 18 Years in Checkout Counter
14. Enfield Couple Slain; Police Suspect Homicide
15. Kids Make Nutritious Snacks
Pause and Reflect
1. With a partner or in a small group, find the ambiguity in the
statements in Table 5 .21. Identify
whether the ambiguity is lexical, structural (the way the
sentence is arranged), contextual, or
some other aspect. Hint: You may find that if you can identify
the semantic role of problematic
noun phrases, you will be successful.
2. Once you figure out the speaker's intention from the
headlines, explain how you would
resolve the ambiguity.
Semantics is the study of the meaning of words, phrases, and
sentences. Word meanings are
stored in the lexicon, which is located both in the head and
among members of a speech community,
in the form of an abstract list of words organized in a number of
different ways. However, meaning
is not in the word. Meanings are assigned to words, concepts,
and events by speakers of a language
within a particular culture milieu; they are not contained in
words or sentences. Semantic relations
hold between words; synonyms, antonyms, and homonyms are
examples. Semantic relations also
hold at the sentence level. Examples are deixis, entailment,
andpresuppositions. It is possible to create
ambiguity in statements; ambiguity makes it possible for
speakers to say one thing and mean another.
Pragmatics
So far in this chapter, you have proceeded from learning about
the more structural aspects of lan-
guage, those claimed to be innate and universal, to those more
closely associated with communication
and culture. You have learned that in semantics the focus is on
meaning, where universal claims are
not so easy to make. As found by looking at the word ' aunt,'
concepts are not fixed across cultures. In
this section, the focus is on aspects of pragmatics that are
particularly relevant to the teaching of ELs.
The Scope ofPragmatics
According to Davis, 1991, "pragmatics will have as its domain
speakers' communicative intentions,
the uses of language that require such intentions, and the
strategies that hearers employ to deter-
mine what these intentions and acts are, so that they can
understand what the speaker intends to
communicate" http:/ /plato.stanford.edu/entries/pragmatics/.
Pragmatics, or 'speaker meaning'
is often called invisible meaning (Yule, 2010) because
comprehension of an utterance may involve not
just the spoken words but also the context in which two people
interact. This context can include
https://plato.stanford.edu/entries/pragmatics
100 Part Two: Language and Literacy (Applied Linguistics)
setting, roles and relationships of the participants, and shared
history and assumptions. When
speakers do not share the same assumptions, communication
problems arise, as is often the case
between a teacher and an EL, and/or a student who does not
know the norms ofparticipation in the
situation. These norms are often unspoken, and native English
speakers may be aware of them only
when they are not followed. Thus, in studying about pragmatics,
the notion of shared context should
be presented. Specific topics addressed in this section are
dialects and bilingualism, child language
socialization, language functions (speech acts), BICS and
CALP, and written discourse.
Dialects and Bilingualism
An interesting problem with Chomsky's theory of a single
language in a speaker's head, sometimes
called a language template, is the fact that mostpeople in the
world speak atleast two languages, or two
dialects of the same language. This means that people can have
two or more grammars in their heads at
the same time. When speakers of Jamaican English understand
and speak several dialects of the same
language, it is difficult to know whatgrammar rules are in their
heads, because the different dialects oper-
ate according to different rules. Bilingualism and
bidialectalism pose a problem for a Chomskian-
only approach to language. Speakers use the avail-
able linguistic resources in the many contexts
where they interact with others or express them-
selves in writing. For example, many bilinguals
freely switch, often mid-sentence, between their
two languages, thus mixing rules as well.
Although teachers should be providing ELs
with grammar and vocabulary instruction in Eng-
lish, teaching is incomplete without also help-
ing them to function in communicative settings.
Those who have grown up speaking the English
language from childhood participate in a wider
speech community of American English, and control
the various dialects of their ethnic and geographi-
cal backgrounds. They also reflect the social register(s) shaped
by their home upbringing and in their
occupational or educational endeavors, and they can shift the
way they speak depending on whether
the interlocutor is a three-year old, an old high school friend, or
a bureaucrat in an office. Minority stu-
dents are expected to control Standard English, whether they
actually know it or not, and to code-switch
in settings that demandit. To persist in using home dialects in
school or other mainstream environments
may invoke disapproval or lower marks on written work. A
minority speaker may be seen as willfully
refusing to know the dialect, or simply ignorant, inarticulate, or
incapable of clear thought. Those who
disparage dialects such as African American Vernacular English
are usually unaware that dialects, like
languages, are patterned phenomena with their own grammar
rules, meanings, and contexts of use.
Pause and Reflect
1. Go to the ESOL website or search the Internet to find out
more about language varieties and
dialects that describe dialects of American English or other
languages.
101 Chapter Five: The Fundamentals of Applied Linguistics:
Communication through Language
2. With a partner or in a small group, explore the wider speech
community of your neighbor-
hood, and listen for dialect differences. Use voice memo, Voice
Thread, or other technologies
to record speech.
3. Note similarities and differences across your data, summarize
your findings about each
dialect, their approximate age, and their educational and
socioeconomic level.
4. Share the results of your research w ith your classmates and
discuss general findings.
Child Language Socialization
Cross-cultural studies of child language development have
shown that even with a wide range
of inputs, young children grow up speaking the language spoken
around them. Although child-
rearing practices concerning language vary greatly across
cultures, similar types of language
phenomena are found across child Ll learners (one-word stage,
two-word stage, etc.). Even
deaf children who grow up in homes with hearing parents create
their own 'home sign,' an indi-
vidual language having structural properties similar to those of
spoken languages. But children
also learn to participate in the communicative practices of their
homes and neighborhoods, and
these vary quite widely.
Shirley Brice Heath's ethnography (1983) described two
contrasting working class communities
in a rural area of the Carolinas to demonstrate how primary
language socialization practices learned
at home later influence interaction patterns and performance
expectations in the classroom when
children begin school. Compared with middle class children
from the city, these children, though
mastering the valued practices in their home communities, were
not prepared to interact in the
'mainstream classroom' at school, where secondary language
socialization occurs. An example of a
ubiquitous practice in mainstream classrooms is the 'firetruck'
dialogue between a teacher and a
student; this is a typical stimulus-response-evaluation routine.
, Phonological systems are rule-governed; that is, they operat.docx
, Phonological systems are rule-governed; that is, they operat.docx
, Phonological systems are rule-governed; that is, they operat.docx
, Phonological systems are rule-governed; that is, they operat.docx
, Phonological systems are rule-governed; that is, they operat.docx
, Phonological systems are rule-governed; that is, they operat.docx

More Related Content

Similar to , Phonological systems are rule-governed; that is, they operat.docx

Acc4300 6 learning unit pronunciation
Acc4300 6 learning unit pronunciation Acc4300 6 learning unit pronunciation
Acc4300 6 learning unit pronunciation April Pei
 
Phonetic and Phonology with images
Phonetic and Phonology with imagesPhonetic and Phonology with images
Phonetic and Phonology with imagesNamo Kim
 
Phonetic and Phonology
Phonetic and PhonologyPhonetic and Phonology
Phonetic and PhonologyNamo Kim
 
Kirchner on phonology
Kirchner on phonologyKirchner on phonology
Kirchner on phonologyHina Honey
 
03- Phonetics and Phonology- with GLOSSARY.pdf
03- Phonetics and Phonology- with GLOSSARY.pdf03- Phonetics and Phonology- with GLOSSARY.pdf
03- Phonetics and Phonology- with GLOSSARY.pdfDrTawas1
 
PPT PHONETICS(Place of Articulation).pptx
PPT PHONETICS(Place of Articulation).pptxPPT PHONETICS(Place of Articulation).pptx
PPT PHONETICS(Place of Articulation).pptxLenlyPMoya
 
Nurlen Power point Phoneme
Nurlen Power point PhonemeNurlen Power point Phoneme
Nurlen Power point Phonemenurlennurlen30
 
44 english phon.docx
44 english phon.docx44 english phon.docx
44 english phon.docxAdjepole
 
Phonology
PhonologyPhonology
Phonologyhasv31
 
Phonology by vikner
Phonology by viknerPhonology by vikner
Phonology by viknerHina Honey
 
Allophone and phoneme. persentation
Allophone and phoneme. persentationAllophone and phoneme. persentation
Allophone and phoneme. persentationDessy Restu Restu
 
Phonetics & Phonology Mine.pptx
Phonetics & Phonology Mine.pptxPhonetics & Phonology Mine.pptx
Phonetics & Phonology Mine.pptxKoukabKhan
 
3 phonology slides
3 phonology slides3 phonology slides
3 phonology slidesJasmine Wong
 
3 phonology slides
3 phonology slides3 phonology slides
3 phonology slidesJasmine Wong
 
Intro to Linguistic.pptx
Intro to Linguistic.pptxIntro to Linguistic.pptx
Intro to Linguistic.pptxErvinaNur1
 

Similar to , Phonological systems are rule-governed; that is, they operat.docx (20)

Acc4300 6 learning unit pronunciation
Acc4300 6 learning unit pronunciation Acc4300 6 learning unit pronunciation
Acc4300 6 learning unit pronunciation
 
Phonetic and Phonology with images
Phonetic and Phonology with imagesPhonetic and Phonology with images
Phonetic and Phonology with images
 
Phonetic and Phonology
Phonetic and PhonologyPhonetic and Phonology
Phonetic and Phonology
 
ALISAHIL.pdf
ALISAHIL.pdfALISAHIL.pdf
ALISAHIL.pdf
 
Phonetics
Phonetics Phonetics
Phonetics
 
Kirchner on phonology
Kirchner on phonologyKirchner on phonology
Kirchner on phonology
 
03- Phonetics and Phonology- with GLOSSARY.pdf
03- Phonetics and Phonology- with GLOSSARY.pdf03- Phonetics and Phonology- with GLOSSARY.pdf
03- Phonetics and Phonology- with GLOSSARY.pdf
 
EPENTHESIS TERM PAPERS
EPENTHESIS TERM PAPERSEPENTHESIS TERM PAPERS
EPENTHESIS TERM PAPERS
 
PPT PHONETICS(Place of Articulation).pptx
PPT PHONETICS(Place of Articulation).pptxPPT PHONETICS(Place of Articulation).pptx
PPT PHONETICS(Place of Articulation).pptx
 
Nurlen Power point Phoneme
Nurlen Power point PhonemeNurlen Power point Phoneme
Nurlen Power point Phoneme
 
44 english phon.docx
44 english phon.docx44 english phon.docx
44 english phon.docx
 
Phonology
PhonologyPhonology
Phonology
 
Phonology
PhonologyPhonology
Phonology
 
Phonology by vikner
Phonology by viknerPhonology by vikner
Phonology by vikner
 
Phonology
PhonologyPhonology
Phonology
 
Allophone and phoneme. persentation
Allophone and phoneme. persentationAllophone and phoneme. persentation
Allophone and phoneme. persentation
 
Phonetics & Phonology Mine.pptx
Phonetics & Phonology Mine.pptxPhonetics & Phonology Mine.pptx
Phonetics & Phonology Mine.pptx
 
3 phonology slides
3 phonology slides3 phonology slides
3 phonology slides
 
3 phonology slides
3 phonology slides3 phonology slides
3 phonology slides
 
Intro to Linguistic.pptx
Intro to Linguistic.pptxIntro to Linguistic.pptx
Intro to Linguistic.pptx
 

More from durantheseldine

Angela’s Ashes​ - Murasaki Shikibu said that the novel happens be.docx
Angela’s Ashes​ - Murasaki Shikibu said that the novel happens be.docxAngela’s Ashes​ - Murasaki Shikibu said that the novel happens be.docx
Angela’s Ashes​ - Murasaki Shikibu said that the novel happens be.docxdurantheseldine
 
ANG1922, Winter 2016Essay 02 InstructionsYour second e.docx
ANG1922, Winter 2016Essay 02 InstructionsYour second e.docxANG1922, Winter 2016Essay 02 InstructionsYour second e.docx
ANG1922, Winter 2016Essay 02 InstructionsYour second e.docxdurantheseldine
 
Anecdotal Records Anecdotal Record Developmental Domain__ _.docx
Anecdotal Records Anecdotal Record Developmental Domain__      _.docxAnecdotal Records Anecdotal Record Developmental Domain__      _.docx
Anecdotal Records Anecdotal Record Developmental Domain__ _.docxdurantheseldine
 
Andy and Beth are neighbors in a small duplex. In the evenings after.docx
Andy and Beth are neighbors in a small duplex. In the evenings after.docxAndy and Beth are neighbors in a small duplex. In the evenings after.docx
Andy and Beth are neighbors in a small duplex. In the evenings after.docxdurantheseldine
 
Andrew John De Los SantosPUP 190SOS 111 Sustainable CitiesMar.docx
Andrew John De Los SantosPUP 190SOS 111 Sustainable CitiesMar.docxAndrew John De Los SantosPUP 190SOS 111 Sustainable CitiesMar.docx
Andrew John De Los SantosPUP 190SOS 111 Sustainable CitiesMar.docxdurantheseldine
 
Android Permissions DemystifiedAdrienne Porter Felt, Erika.docx
Android Permissions DemystifiedAdrienne Porter Felt, Erika.docxAndroid Permissions DemystifiedAdrienne Porter Felt, Erika.docx
Android Permissions DemystifiedAdrienne Porter Felt, Erika.docxdurantheseldine
 
ANDREW CARNEGIE PRINCE OF STEELNARRATOR On November 25th, 1835 i.docx
ANDREW CARNEGIE PRINCE OF STEELNARRATOR On November 25th, 1835 i.docxANDREW CARNEGIE PRINCE OF STEELNARRATOR On November 25th, 1835 i.docx
ANDREW CARNEGIE PRINCE OF STEELNARRATOR On November 25th, 1835 i.docxdurantheseldine
 
Andrew CassidySaint Leo UniversityContemporary Issues in Crimina.docx
Andrew CassidySaint Leo UniversityContemporary Issues in Crimina.docxAndrew CassidySaint Leo UniversityContemporary Issues in Crimina.docx
Andrew CassidySaint Leo UniversityContemporary Issues in Crimina.docxdurantheseldine
 
andrea lunsfordstanford universitymichal brodysono.docx
andrea lunsfordstanford universitymichal brodysono.docxandrea lunsfordstanford universitymichal brodysono.docx
andrea lunsfordstanford universitymichal brodysono.docxdurantheseldine
 
Andrea Azpiazo – Review One. Little Havana Multifamily Developme.docx
Andrea Azpiazo – Review One.  Little Havana Multifamily Developme.docxAndrea Azpiazo – Review One.  Little Havana Multifamily Developme.docx
Andrea Azpiazo – Review One. Little Havana Multifamily Developme.docxdurantheseldine
 
And what we students of history always learn is that the human bein.docx
And what we students of history always learn is that the human bein.docxAnd what we students of history always learn is that the human bein.docx
And what we students of history always learn is that the human bein.docxdurantheseldine
 
and Contradiction in Architecture Robert Venturi .docx
and Contradiction in Architecture Robert Venturi .docxand Contradiction in Architecture Robert Venturi .docx
and Contradiction in Architecture Robert Venturi .docxdurantheseldine
 
Ancient Egypt1The Civilization of the Nile River V.docx
Ancient Egypt1The Civilization of the Nile River V.docxAncient Egypt1The Civilization of the Nile River V.docx
Ancient Egypt1The Civilization of the Nile River V.docxdurantheseldine
 
Anayze a landmark case. The assesment should include a full discussi.docx
Anayze a landmark case. The assesment should include a full discussi.docxAnayze a landmark case. The assesment should include a full discussi.docx
Anayze a landmark case. The assesment should include a full discussi.docxdurantheseldine
 
Anatomy and Physiology of the Digestive SystemObjectives· Iden.docx
Anatomy and Physiology of the Digestive SystemObjectives· Iden.docxAnatomy and Physiology of the Digestive SystemObjectives· Iden.docx
Anatomy and Physiology of the Digestive SystemObjectives· Iden.docxdurantheseldine
 
ANAThe Article Review by Jeanette Keith on Book by Stephanie McCu.docx
ANAThe Article  Review by Jeanette Keith on Book by Stephanie McCu.docxANAThe Article  Review by Jeanette Keith on Book by Stephanie McCu.docx
ANAThe Article Review by Jeanette Keith on Book by Stephanie McCu.docxdurantheseldine
 
Analyzing workers social networking behavior – an invasion of priva.docx
Analyzing workers social networking behavior – an invasion of priva.docxAnalyzing workers social networking behavior – an invasion of priva.docx
Analyzing workers social networking behavior – an invasion of priva.docxdurantheseldine
 
Analyzing and Visualizing Data Chapter 6Data Represent.docx
Analyzing and Visualizing Data Chapter 6Data Represent.docxAnalyzing and Visualizing Data Chapter 6Data Represent.docx
Analyzing and Visualizing Data Chapter 6Data Represent.docxdurantheseldine
 
Analyzing and Visualizing Data Chapter 1The .docx
Analyzing and Visualizing Data Chapter 1The .docxAnalyzing and Visualizing Data Chapter 1The .docx
Analyzing and Visualizing Data Chapter 1The .docxdurantheseldine
 
Analyzing a Primary Source RubricName ______________________.docx
Analyzing a Primary Source RubricName ______________________.docxAnalyzing a Primary Source RubricName ______________________.docx
Analyzing a Primary Source RubricName ______________________.docxdurantheseldine
 

More from durantheseldine (20)

Angela’s Ashes​ - Murasaki Shikibu said that the novel happens be.docx
Angela’s Ashes​ - Murasaki Shikibu said that the novel happens be.docxAngela’s Ashes​ - Murasaki Shikibu said that the novel happens be.docx
Angela’s Ashes​ - Murasaki Shikibu said that the novel happens be.docx
 
ANG1922, Winter 2016Essay 02 InstructionsYour second e.docx
ANG1922, Winter 2016Essay 02 InstructionsYour second e.docxANG1922, Winter 2016Essay 02 InstructionsYour second e.docx
ANG1922, Winter 2016Essay 02 InstructionsYour second e.docx
 
Anecdotal Records Anecdotal Record Developmental Domain__ _.docx
Anecdotal Records Anecdotal Record Developmental Domain__      _.docxAnecdotal Records Anecdotal Record Developmental Domain__      _.docx
Anecdotal Records Anecdotal Record Developmental Domain__ _.docx
 
Andy and Beth are neighbors in a small duplex. In the evenings after.docx
Andy and Beth are neighbors in a small duplex. In the evenings after.docxAndy and Beth are neighbors in a small duplex. In the evenings after.docx
Andy and Beth are neighbors in a small duplex. In the evenings after.docx
 
Andrew John De Los SantosPUP 190SOS 111 Sustainable CitiesMar.docx
Andrew John De Los SantosPUP 190SOS 111 Sustainable CitiesMar.docxAndrew John De Los SantosPUP 190SOS 111 Sustainable CitiesMar.docx
Andrew John De Los SantosPUP 190SOS 111 Sustainable CitiesMar.docx
 
Android Permissions DemystifiedAdrienne Porter Felt, Erika.docx
Android Permissions DemystifiedAdrienne Porter Felt, Erika.docxAndroid Permissions DemystifiedAdrienne Porter Felt, Erika.docx
Android Permissions DemystifiedAdrienne Porter Felt, Erika.docx
 
ANDREW CARNEGIE PRINCE OF STEELNARRATOR On November 25th, 1835 i.docx
ANDREW CARNEGIE PRINCE OF STEELNARRATOR On November 25th, 1835 i.docxANDREW CARNEGIE PRINCE OF STEELNARRATOR On November 25th, 1835 i.docx
ANDREW CARNEGIE PRINCE OF STEELNARRATOR On November 25th, 1835 i.docx
 
Andrew CassidySaint Leo UniversityContemporary Issues in Crimina.docx
Andrew CassidySaint Leo UniversityContemporary Issues in Crimina.docxAndrew CassidySaint Leo UniversityContemporary Issues in Crimina.docx
Andrew CassidySaint Leo UniversityContemporary Issues in Crimina.docx
 
andrea lunsfordstanford universitymichal brodysono.docx
andrea lunsfordstanford universitymichal brodysono.docxandrea lunsfordstanford universitymichal brodysono.docx
andrea lunsfordstanford universitymichal brodysono.docx
 
Andrea Azpiazo – Review One. Little Havana Multifamily Developme.docx
Andrea Azpiazo – Review One.  Little Havana Multifamily Developme.docxAndrea Azpiazo – Review One.  Little Havana Multifamily Developme.docx
Andrea Azpiazo – Review One. Little Havana Multifamily Developme.docx
 
And what we students of history always learn is that the human bein.docx
And what we students of history always learn is that the human bein.docxAnd what we students of history always learn is that the human bein.docx
And what we students of history always learn is that the human bein.docx
 
and Contradiction in Architecture Robert Venturi .docx
and Contradiction in Architecture Robert Venturi .docxand Contradiction in Architecture Robert Venturi .docx
and Contradiction in Architecture Robert Venturi .docx
 
Ancient Egypt1The Civilization of the Nile River V.docx
Ancient Egypt1The Civilization of the Nile River V.docxAncient Egypt1The Civilization of the Nile River V.docx
Ancient Egypt1The Civilization of the Nile River V.docx
 
Anayze a landmark case. The assesment should include a full discussi.docx
Anayze a landmark case. The assesment should include a full discussi.docxAnayze a landmark case. The assesment should include a full discussi.docx
Anayze a landmark case. The assesment should include a full discussi.docx
 
Anatomy and Physiology of the Digestive SystemObjectives· Iden.docx
Anatomy and Physiology of the Digestive SystemObjectives· Iden.docxAnatomy and Physiology of the Digestive SystemObjectives· Iden.docx
Anatomy and Physiology of the Digestive SystemObjectives· Iden.docx
 
ANAThe Article Review by Jeanette Keith on Book by Stephanie McCu.docx
ANAThe Article  Review by Jeanette Keith on Book by Stephanie McCu.docxANAThe Article  Review by Jeanette Keith on Book by Stephanie McCu.docx
ANAThe Article Review by Jeanette Keith on Book by Stephanie McCu.docx
 
Analyzing workers social networking behavior – an invasion of priva.docx
Analyzing workers social networking behavior – an invasion of priva.docxAnalyzing workers social networking behavior – an invasion of priva.docx
Analyzing workers social networking behavior – an invasion of priva.docx
 
Analyzing and Visualizing Data Chapter 6Data Represent.docx
Analyzing and Visualizing Data Chapter 6Data Represent.docxAnalyzing and Visualizing Data Chapter 6Data Represent.docx
Analyzing and Visualizing Data Chapter 6Data Represent.docx
 
Analyzing and Visualizing Data Chapter 1The .docx
Analyzing and Visualizing Data Chapter 1The .docxAnalyzing and Visualizing Data Chapter 1The .docx
Analyzing and Visualizing Data Chapter 1The .docx
 
Analyzing a Primary Source RubricName ______________________.docx
Analyzing a Primary Source RubricName ______________________.docxAnalyzing a Primary Source RubricName ______________________.docx
Analyzing a Primary Source RubricName ______________________.docx
 

Recently uploaded

MULTIDISCIPLINRY NATURE OF THE ENVIRONMENTAL STUDIES.pptx
MULTIDISCIPLINRY NATURE OF THE ENVIRONMENTAL STUDIES.pptxMULTIDISCIPLINRY NATURE OF THE ENVIRONMENTAL STUDIES.pptx
MULTIDISCIPLINRY NATURE OF THE ENVIRONMENTAL STUDIES.pptxAnupkumar Sharma
 
Judging the Relevance and worth of ideas part 2.pptx
Judging the Relevance  and worth of ideas part 2.pptxJudging the Relevance  and worth of ideas part 2.pptx
Judging the Relevance and worth of ideas part 2.pptxSherlyMaeNeri
 
AmericanHighSchoolsprezentacijaoskolama.
AmericanHighSchoolsprezentacijaoskolama.AmericanHighSchoolsprezentacijaoskolama.
AmericanHighSchoolsprezentacijaoskolama.arsicmarija21
 
Hierarchy of management that covers different levels of management
Hierarchy of management that covers different levels of managementHierarchy of management that covers different levels of management
Hierarchy of management that covers different levels of managementmkooblal
 
How to Configure Email Server in Odoo 17
How to Configure Email Server in Odoo 17How to Configure Email Server in Odoo 17
How to Configure Email Server in Odoo 17Celine George
 
DATA STRUCTURE AND ALGORITHM for beginners
DATA STRUCTURE AND ALGORITHM for beginnersDATA STRUCTURE AND ALGORITHM for beginners
DATA STRUCTURE AND ALGORITHM for beginnersSabitha Banu
 
Introduction to ArtificiaI Intelligence in Higher Education
Introduction to ArtificiaI Intelligence in Higher EducationIntroduction to ArtificiaI Intelligence in Higher Education
Introduction to ArtificiaI Intelligence in Higher Educationpboyjonauth
 
Crayon Activity Handout For the Crayon A
Crayon Activity Handout For the Crayon ACrayon Activity Handout For the Crayon A
Crayon Activity Handout For the Crayon AUnboundStockton
 
Planning a health career 4th Quarter.pptx
Planning a health career 4th Quarter.pptxPlanning a health career 4th Quarter.pptx
Planning a health career 4th Quarter.pptxLigayaBacuel1
 
Quarter 4 Peace-education.pptx Catch Up Friday
Quarter 4 Peace-education.pptx Catch Up FridayQuarter 4 Peace-education.pptx Catch Up Friday
Quarter 4 Peace-education.pptx Catch Up FridayMakMakNepo
 
Computed Fields and api Depends in the Odoo 17
Computed Fields and api Depends in the Odoo 17Computed Fields and api Depends in the Odoo 17
Computed Fields and api Depends in the Odoo 17Celine George
 
Atmosphere science 7 quarter 4 .........
Atmosphere science 7 quarter 4 .........Atmosphere science 7 quarter 4 .........
Atmosphere science 7 quarter 4 .........LeaCamillePacle
 
Types of Journalistic Writing Grade 8.pptx
Types of Journalistic Writing Grade 8.pptxTypes of Journalistic Writing Grade 8.pptx
Types of Journalistic Writing Grade 8.pptxEyham Joco
 
EPANDING THE CONTENT OF AN OUTLINE using notes.pptx
EPANDING THE CONTENT OF AN OUTLINE using notes.pptxEPANDING THE CONTENT OF AN OUTLINE using notes.pptx
EPANDING THE CONTENT OF AN OUTLINE using notes.pptxRaymartEstabillo3
 
ECONOMIC CONTEXT - PAPER 1 Q3: NEWSPAPERS.pptx
ECONOMIC CONTEXT - PAPER 1 Q3: NEWSPAPERS.pptxECONOMIC CONTEXT - PAPER 1 Q3: NEWSPAPERS.pptx
ECONOMIC CONTEXT - PAPER 1 Q3: NEWSPAPERS.pptxiammrhaywood
 
HỌC TỐT TIẾNG ANH 11 THEO CHƯƠNG TRÌNH GLOBAL SUCCESS ĐÁP ÁN CHI TIẾT - CẢ NĂ...
HỌC TỐT TIẾNG ANH 11 THEO CHƯƠNG TRÌNH GLOBAL SUCCESS ĐÁP ÁN CHI TIẾT - CẢ NĂ...HỌC TỐT TIẾNG ANH 11 THEO CHƯƠNG TRÌNH GLOBAL SUCCESS ĐÁP ÁN CHI TIẾT - CẢ NĂ...
HỌC TỐT TIẾNG ANH 11 THEO CHƯƠNG TRÌNH GLOBAL SUCCESS ĐÁP ÁN CHI TIẾT - CẢ NĂ...Nguyen Thanh Tu Collection
 
ENGLISH6-Q4-W3.pptxqurter our high choom
ENGLISH6-Q4-W3.pptxqurter our high choomENGLISH6-Q4-W3.pptxqurter our high choom
ENGLISH6-Q4-W3.pptxqurter our high choomnelietumpap1
 
Influencing policy (training slides from Fast Track Impact)
Influencing policy (training slides from Fast Track Impact)Influencing policy (training slides from Fast Track Impact)
Influencing policy (training slides from Fast Track Impact)Mark Reed
 

Recently uploaded (20)

MULTIDISCIPLINRY NATURE OF THE ENVIRONMENTAL STUDIES.pptx
MULTIDISCIPLINRY NATURE OF THE ENVIRONMENTAL STUDIES.pptxMULTIDISCIPLINRY NATURE OF THE ENVIRONMENTAL STUDIES.pptx
MULTIDISCIPLINRY NATURE OF THE ENVIRONMENTAL STUDIES.pptx
 
Judging the Relevance and worth of ideas part 2.pptx
Judging the Relevance  and worth of ideas part 2.pptxJudging the Relevance  and worth of ideas part 2.pptx
Judging the Relevance and worth of ideas part 2.pptx
 
AmericanHighSchoolsprezentacijaoskolama.
AmericanHighSchoolsprezentacijaoskolama.AmericanHighSchoolsprezentacijaoskolama.
AmericanHighSchoolsprezentacijaoskolama.
 
Hierarchy of management that covers different levels of management
Hierarchy of management that covers different levels of managementHierarchy of management that covers different levels of management
Hierarchy of management that covers different levels of management
 
How to Configure Email Server in Odoo 17
How to Configure Email Server in Odoo 17How to Configure Email Server in Odoo 17
How to Configure Email Server in Odoo 17
 
DATA STRUCTURE AND ALGORITHM for beginners
DATA STRUCTURE AND ALGORITHM for beginnersDATA STRUCTURE AND ALGORITHM for beginners
DATA STRUCTURE AND ALGORITHM for beginners
 
Introduction to ArtificiaI Intelligence in Higher Education
Introduction to ArtificiaI Intelligence in Higher EducationIntroduction to ArtificiaI Intelligence in Higher Education
Introduction to ArtificiaI Intelligence in Higher Education
 
Crayon Activity Handout For the Crayon A
Crayon Activity Handout For the Crayon ACrayon Activity Handout For the Crayon A
Crayon Activity Handout For the Crayon A
 
Planning a health career 4th Quarter.pptx
Planning a health career 4th Quarter.pptxPlanning a health career 4th Quarter.pptx
Planning a health career 4th Quarter.pptx
 
Quarter 4 Peace-education.pptx Catch Up Friday
Quarter 4 Peace-education.pptx Catch Up FridayQuarter 4 Peace-education.pptx Catch Up Friday
Quarter 4 Peace-education.pptx Catch Up Friday
 
Computed Fields and api Depends in the Odoo 17
Computed Fields and api Depends in the Odoo 17Computed Fields and api Depends in the Odoo 17
Computed Fields and api Depends in the Odoo 17
 
Atmosphere science 7 quarter 4 .........
Atmosphere science 7 quarter 4 .........Atmosphere science 7 quarter 4 .........
Atmosphere science 7 quarter 4 .........
 
Types of Journalistic Writing Grade 8.pptx
Types of Journalistic Writing Grade 8.pptxTypes of Journalistic Writing Grade 8.pptx
Types of Journalistic Writing Grade 8.pptx
 
Rapple "Scholarly Communications and the Sustainable Development Goals"
Rapple "Scholarly Communications and the Sustainable Development Goals"Rapple "Scholarly Communications and the Sustainable Development Goals"
Rapple "Scholarly Communications and the Sustainable Development Goals"
 
EPANDING THE CONTENT OF AN OUTLINE using notes.pptx
EPANDING THE CONTENT OF AN OUTLINE using notes.pptxEPANDING THE CONTENT OF AN OUTLINE using notes.pptx
EPANDING THE CONTENT OF AN OUTLINE using notes.pptx
 
ECONOMIC CONTEXT - PAPER 1 Q3: NEWSPAPERS.pptx
ECONOMIC CONTEXT - PAPER 1 Q3: NEWSPAPERS.pptxECONOMIC CONTEXT - PAPER 1 Q3: NEWSPAPERS.pptx
ECONOMIC CONTEXT - PAPER 1 Q3: NEWSPAPERS.pptx
 
OS-operating systems- ch04 (Threads) ...
OS-operating systems- ch04 (Threads) ...OS-operating systems- ch04 (Threads) ...
OS-operating systems- ch04 (Threads) ...
 
HỌC TỐT TIẾNG ANH 11 THEO CHƯƠNG TRÌNH GLOBAL SUCCESS ĐÁP ÁN CHI TIẾT - CẢ NĂ...
HỌC TỐT TIẾNG ANH 11 THEO CHƯƠNG TRÌNH GLOBAL SUCCESS ĐÁP ÁN CHI TIẾT - CẢ NĂ...HỌC TỐT TIẾNG ANH 11 THEO CHƯƠNG TRÌNH GLOBAL SUCCESS ĐÁP ÁN CHI TIẾT - CẢ NĂ...
HỌC TỐT TIẾNG ANH 11 THEO CHƯƠNG TRÌNH GLOBAL SUCCESS ĐÁP ÁN CHI TIẾT - CẢ NĂ...
 
ENGLISH6-Q4-W3.pptxqurter our high choom
ENGLISH6-Q4-W3.pptxqurter our high choomENGLISH6-Q4-W3.pptxqurter our high choom
ENGLISH6-Q4-W3.pptxqurter our high choom
 
Influencing policy (training slides from Fast Track Impact)
Influencing policy (training slides from Fast Track Impact)Influencing policy (training slides from Fast Track Impact)
Influencing policy (training slides from Fast Track Impact)
 

, Phonological systems are rule-governed; that is, they operat.docx

  • 1. , Phonological systems are rule-governed; that is, they operate according to certain rules and are : manifested as patterns.The word used for individual speech sounds is phones, and the study of the ; characteristics, or features, of phones of all languages is called phonetics (Yule, 2010). Although the I focus is on the English sound system, it is important to note that each language is systematic in its patterning, and that although similarities exist across all languages, differences abound. Phonology The study of the sound system of languages, called phonology, helps teachers understand many challenges English learners (ELs) face, both in hearing and producing the sounds of a new language. This knowledge also assists teachers in diagnosing errors second language (L2) readers typically make when reading aloud and in predicting how this affects comprehension, accuracy, and fluency. This section is fundamental to an understanding of linguistics because it introduces a number of important concepts that are revisited at other levels of language. The first section is on the basic con- cepts of phonology; the second is about the consonants of English; the third provides an overview of the English vowels; and the fourth is about suprasegmentals, the phonological phenomena affecting pronunciation at word and phrasal levels. An examination of the learning processes involved when
  • 2. a learner encounters a new language is presented along with activities to support educators and students in discovering the characteristics of how the sound systems function, as well as ways to apply knowledge of phonology to help students overcome difficulties. See Figure 5.1. g "i,': .§ ~ _;; Sounds l--- --L-..-. ~ ~'------........-' = j _;; ..... = = "' @ Intonation Word stress Rhythm Features of connected speech Figure S.1. Phonology.
  • 3. -[ill- A uniYersal concept across languages is the phone, or sound, as represe:-.?.:: ::-- .:. ..=~ o:::- 0::.~er 5;-::-.::... "- between brackets, such as [p ]. Note that [pl between brackets represents ti-.E s.:::. ~ 2..:'".i ~~ 'p ' in si.-.~ quotation marks represents the letter. The concept of phone is a uni·ersal o:-.e: a _e::cr or other syrr.x_ in brackets indicates thatit is part ofa system that includes all the world's languages. The Intemationa.. Phonetic Alphabet (IPA) includes all these phones using a unique symbol for each sound. The sound of [p] in English actually has three different variants, the aspirated [p] in 'pit', fr.:c unaspirated [p] in 'shopping' and the unreleased [p] in 'stop'. Even though English has these ya::- ants, called allophones, of [p ], they are still the same phoneme. That is, the same symbol is used.::- represent all the variants of [p] for English. A phoneme is represented by a symbol that includes L possible variants (allophones) of a particular sound in a particular language, and is written ben..,·ee:- slashes, as in / p /. Aspiration of a sound involves the accompaniment of a puffofair on the sounc. :: onset. You can detect this by putting your finger in front of your lips when starting to say 'pif. =: you say the word 'pit' twice, once with aspirated /p/ and once with unaspirated /p/, the meani:; of the word does not change. However, in Hindi and Arabic there are two [p] phonemes becauE-c
  • 4. speakers produce both aspirated and unaspirated sounds in the same environment, and they can:- · be used interchangeably. They are different at the phonemic level. Other languages, such as Korea:- do not make a distinction between [p] and [f], so there would be only one phoneme represent::-; those Korean allophones. A specific instance of phonemic difference is in Spanish, which has r - /r/ phonemes, one written as 'r', and the other as 'rr', as in 'pero', 'but', and 'perro', 'dog' . As y~ - can see, a phonemic difference indicates a meaning difference at the word level. The concepts of -etic and -emic are important for teachers to understand because they are used.= the fields ofboth anthropology and linguistics; both fields contribute crucially to the teaching of :.a.:-- guages. The term -etic refers to universal characteristics of languages or cultures, while -emic r~e:- to language- or culture-specific characteristics. The whole idea of universal properties of languat~ and cultures is a powerful one; within the bounds of our own cultural and linguistic cocoons, · = often assume that our way of experiencing the world is universal, and are often surprised wz we learn that it is not. When non-native English speakers cannot hear or produce the 'th' sounc: = 'three', and produce 'tree' or 'free', we may wonder why they do not say 'three', which is so sim;· - The fact is that 'th' is quite rare across the world's languages, so it is no wonder that ELs have c._:- ficulty with this pronunciation. At the word/concept level where culture and language meet, it is surprising to learn that :-.a- ther Vietnamese nor Swahili has the generic word 'aunt'. In
  • 5. addition, mother's sister, father 's sis:2: uncle's wife, and so on must all be referred to by different words; the relationships and roles des.:.;- nated by these different words are culturally determined. This will be addressed in the section::.- semantics. In becoming more aware of the needs of ELs in the classroom, educators should prep~- themselves to be surprised rather than judgmental, for what they say and do can be a cons-z..- source of wonder and learning for the whole class. Systematicity of English Consonants In this overview of sounds and the rules that underlie them, consonants are examined. Eng·:.- consonants are formed by affecting the passage of air through the vocal tract in various ways. -:-- chart in Figure 5.2 provides information on the following: the place of articulation for each soc from front to back in the mouth, the pairing of sounds via their voicing properties, and whether ::...: passing through the vocal tract stops or continues. In studying the chart, note that most consonant phonemes are represented by letters of = alphabet, while other symbols represent sounds that are written with two letters. For the systerr. · be useful and clear, a symbol must uniquely represent only one phoneme. A key word below e=--· non-alphabetic symbol on the chart helps to identify the sound it represents. 67 Chapter Five: The Fundamentals of Applied Linguistics: Communication through Language
  • 6. English Consonanis Manner Bilabial Labio- hlter- Alveolar Palatal Velar Glottal Place <lental dental+ Stop /pl - top /ti -two /kl - carVoiceless /bl - bee !di-do /g/- goVoiced Nasal /ml-me /n/-no /rj/- ring Voiced only Flicative /fl -fun /e /- /s/ - so I f / - shoe /h/have Voiceless lltick /vi- vote lzl - zooVoiced /of - /3/visionthe Affticate ltfl- watc/1Voiceless Voiced Id!, /- joy Glide lwl why /j/-yes /wi/ Voiced only Liquid /I/ love Voiced only /r/ rot Figure S.2. The consonant system of the English Language Pause and Reflect 1. With a partner, produce each sound to become more familiar with the overall system.
  • 7. 2. English sounds are produced using three articulators: the lips, teeth, and tongue. Consult the chart to identify which articulators are used to produce each phoneme. 3. The place of articulation from front to back in the vocal tract is where an articulator touches a location in the mouth. As you pronounce each sound again, try to describe its point of articulation. 4. Compare the labels of the place of articulation on the chart in Figure 5.2 with the positions in the vocal tract shown in Figure 5.3 . It, d, s, z, I, n I [ j, z, tj, d, y, r ] Ik, g, IJ ] [ p, b, m, w] [ A] [ f, v ] Labiodental Alveolar ridge: alveolars Alveopalatal ~'i:f~::d--- [ h ] Glottis: glottals I ? l
  • 8. Velum: velars Uvula: uvulars Pharnyx: pharnygeals Esophagus Part Two: Language and Literacy (Applied Linguistics) -"- Figure 5.3. Organs of Speech Production. From Why TESOL? Theories & Issues in Teaching English to Speakers of Other Languages in K-12 Classrooms by Ariza et al. Copyright© 2010 by Kendall Hunt Publishing Company. Reprinted by permission. It is important to note that all English phonemes, except for / h /, are made in the area from the lips to the velum. But each language has a different system of phonemes, some more, some less :han those of English. For example, Arabic speakers produce uvular, pharyngeal, laryngeal, and glottal sounds. So, you can imagine how difficult it is for English learners of Arabic to master such sounds. 1. Look at the voiced and voiceless pairs on the chart in Figure 5.4. Produce each voiced sound in the pair first, placing your index finger across your 'voice box' and trying to feel the vocal cords vibrate. 2. Now produce the voiceless member of each pair and note that there is no
  • 9. vibration. Word-Initial Word-Medial Word-Final -:oiced zap phasing buzz ~. ·oiceless ~ap fa.!:,;ing bu~ Figure S.4. Voiced/Voiceless Pairs. The chart, along with Figure 5.2, illustrates that voicing is an extremely important feature of English. Except for nasals, /r/, /1/, /w/, /y/, and /h/, all consonants in the language occur in voiced a.~d ,·oiceless pairs, which is not a universal feature of languages in general. The voicing feature of -=:.nglish is so prominent that voicing contrasts occur in word- initial, word-medial, and word-final ;ositions, as shown in Figure 5.4 for the phonemes /z/ and /s/. Note that English spelling often ciscures the realities of sound similarities. 69 Chapter Five: The Fundamentals of Applied Linguistics: Communication through Language Althoughmany languages have voicing pairs word-initially, several such as Spanish andGerman devoice consonants word-finally. Thus, Spanish- and German- speaking ELs tend to pronounce 'pig' as 'pick', 'cab' as 'cap', and 'fuzz' as 'fuss'. The process of devoicing in L2 learning is one kind of transfer or interference from a first language (Ll).
  • 10. ~ 1. Locate the pairs (lbIpI, I diti, and I glkl). Do they stop the airflow, or continue it? 2. Locate the other pairs, called fricatives, and answer the same question. ~ Another important aspect of the English consonant system is how airflow is impeded. Stops completely do so, while fricatives obstruct the air flow, but allow it to continue. As you studied the chart and pronounced the various sounds, you might have noticed that the three pairs of stops were interspersed with pairs of fricatives. The pair lbIpI is produced in the mouth very close to IvI fl and Idltl is close to ltJId3I and also to lzlsl. Speakers of some languages have difficulty hearing and producing the difference between the lbl in 'cab' and the IvI in 'calve'. Although Spanish has both lbI and IvI phonemes, they may both be pronounced the same, namely as bilabial fricatives, where the sound is produced with air passing between the lips instead of stopping. The words 'very' and 'berry' are often pronounced almost identically by Spanish speakers. (It should be noted that generalizations made aboutvarious languages are not always accurate for all dialects of a language.) Speakers of some languages spoken in Ethiopia, as well as Korean, have difficulty with the IpIfI distinction. A Ethiopian friend once reported that his student from a rural area told him, "Flease, Sir, I am foor. I have no any money." A further example is that French speakers, who lack the voiced 'th' sound in their language, are likely to say' /zisl for 'this'; speakers of some West African languages might say 'dat' for 'that'; and speakers of yet another language
  • 11. might say 'tree' for 'three'. As noted above, the two sounds that represent English 'th' are uncommon among the world's languages and so ELs will produce sounds from their own collection of phonemes that are closest to the English sounds. Thus, a second kind of Ll transfer, or interference, may occur for speakers whose languages lack the phonemic contrasts across the stopIfricative divide that English has. Pause and Reflect 1. One pair of sounds, /d3/- /tJ/ (as in 'jerry' and 'cherry') are stop/continuants (called affri- cates); /d3/ starts with /d/ and ends with /3/; /tJ/ starts with /t/ and ends with /f/. Look at the nasals and explain why they are placed where they are. Can you speculate why there are no nasals in the fricative columns? 2. Where do you find the semi-vowels (also called glides) on the chart in Figure 5.2? 70 Part Two: Language and Literacy (Applied Linguistics) The two affricate sounds can be difficult for non-native English speakers. For example, many Spanish speakers have difficulty distinguishing between /tJ/ and /J/ ('cherry' and 'sherry') in all environments, and between /d3/ and /tJ/ word finally ('badge' and 'batch'). These sounds may also be late-learned by English Ll children during language development. In fact, some other Eng-
  • 12. lish sounds are difficult for both children and L2 learners. Nasals are found only in the stop columns in English and they are produced in the same manner as their counterparts, but with the impeding of air through the nasal passage. When that passage is blocked, as with a cold or a pinched nose, the voiced stop is produced, "by dose is all plugged up." Some Spanish speakers may have difficulty producing /m/ word- finally, either omitting it ("My nay is_.") or substituting with /n/ or /11./ ("Hong Depot") . In consulting an IPA chart you will find that there are languages in the world with nasal fricatives. Can you produce any of them? Each semi-vowel and liquid is shown on the chart in Figure 5.2 in the column beneath its place of articulation. It might seem quite odd to see /1/ placed in the /dz/tf/ column until you experi- ment a bit and find that it is formed by touching the tongue tip at the alveolar ridge, although it could also be formed further forward or further back (Try it!). Speakers from Taiwan may produce /1/ for / oI as in 'raler' for 'rather'. Speakers of some dialects of English produce a palatalized /1/, touching the blade of the tongue at the palate rather than using the tongue tip. It might also seem strange to see /w/ in the bilabial column. But when /w/ is formed, as in 'wonder', the lips are round, thus, a bilabial sound. On the other hand, /y/, formed in the palatal area, has vowel-like properties as /w/ does. The two sounds are vowel-like because the airflow is not impeded in their production as with stops and fricatives.
  • 13. Both L2 learners and English speaking children have difficulty with liquids and semi-vowels (glides). Thus, a second type of learning process results in developmental phenomena. Examples of both developmental and Ll transfer errors with liquids and glides are shown in the chart in Figure 5.5. English Ll Development (and L2 Leaming) L2 Leaming /r/ vs /w/ rabbit ➔ wabbit /1/ vs /r/ rice ➔ lice Japanese, Korean, and some East and Central African languages /1/ vs /w/ (line, wine) / lait/, /wait/ or / ait/ /wI vs /v/Wine ➔ vine Farsi (Iran) & Dari (Afghanistan) /f/ vs /wh/ fuji ➔ whuji /y/ vs /j/ yet ➔ jet Spanish /w/ vs / gw/ wet ➔ gwet Figure S.S. Developmental and Ll Transfer Errors. Minimal Pairs: Listening and Speaking When a learner has difficulty producing an English sound, an experienced teacher analyzes the error in comparison to the target pronunciation. For example a
  • 14. student may say "I want a baby 'pick/JI instead of 1pig1 because in the Ll voicing of consonants does not occur word-finally. The words 'pig and pick' constitute a minimal pair, which consists of two words having two minimally contrastive sounds in the same position in a word. In the pig/pick example the word-final sounds are formed in the same place, and are both stops; they differ only minimally-in voicing. Figure 5.6 portrays pairs of words that do not constitute minimal pairs. 71 Chapter Five: The Fundamentals of Applied Linguistics: Communication through Language pick/pill The word-final sounds are not produced at the same location in the mouth and differ in terms of voicing. Also, /kl is a stop and /1/ is a continuant. trip/dip Although / t/ and / d/ are minimally contrastive, there is an extra sound, /r/. fight/night Rhyming words are not necessarily considered minimal pairs. Though they are both continuants, / f/ and / n/ differ in terms of manner (nasal vs fricative) and place of articulation. crutch/ grouch The velar stops/g/ and /k/ are minimally contrastive, but the vowel difference indicates not one contrast, but two.
  • 15. Figure S.6. Non-Minimal Pairs Pause and Reflect 1. Study the pairs of words in Figure 5.7, say them aloud, and underline pairs that are mini- mally contrastive. Remember, only one contrast in the same position in both words constitutes a minimal pair. Don't be fooled by spelling. For example, the words in the first pair do not use the same spelling pattern, but they do constitute a minimal pair. buy/pie fan/vat catch/ glitch Sue/zoo rush/rouge bank/bag lip/lib than/thin tan/Dan fuss/fuzz cheap/jeep red/rent half/halve gum/come veal/feel puck/pug Figure S.7. Identifying Minimal Pairs in English. To listen to speakers of many different languages reading the same passage in English, go to this website: www.accent.gmu.edu, click on browse; click on any language, then find the list of speakers reading the same short paragraph. 2. In pairs or small groups, identify errors you heard and create minimal pairs to help the speaker(s) hear the difference between the two sounds. Consider consulting the consonant chart or your personal experiences in listening to Els
  • 16. to complete this activity. There are two ways to assist Els with new sounds, through listening activities and visual pres- entations. A teacher begins with a listening, a minimal pair drill, which helps students hear the differ- ence between two sounds as in the case of the learner who says 'pick' for 'pig'. The teacher presents two minimally contrastive words orally so that the learner(s) can hear the difference. Figure 5.8 shows how to conduct a minimal pair drill to help Els distinguish between the sound /z/ and /s/. Once students can hear the differences, they are ready to say the words. You can use your hands, videos, or drawings of the vocal tract to demonstrate. Avoid drawing attention to your own mouth, as there may be taboos about doing so. There are numerous websites with lists of minimal pairs for teaching the sounds. Visit the ESOl website for suggestions. www.accent.gmu.edu - 72 Part Two: Language and Literacy (Applied Linguistics) Teacher: Student(s): Listen carefully as I say two words that sound similar, 'zap and sap'. (Do not show the words in writing.) Say slowly: They do not repeat, Zap, zap, zap, sap, sap, sap, listen only.
  • 17. Zap,sap,zap,sap,zap,sap (Do not vary the intonation pattern as some will focus on tonal distinctions). When I say zap, put up one finger. When I say 'sap' put up two fingers. Say the words randomly and watch as students Students show fingers as show fingers, directed. ending the drill when students comply consistently. Do the same drill, but with the sounds in final or medial position: 'buzz, bus; phasing, facing.' Figure S.8. Leading a Minimal Pair Drill. The Systematicity ofEnglish Vowels Vowels are produced with no airflow obstruction, but always involve the tongue and vocal folds in their production. Among the simplest vowel systems is found in some dialects of Arabic, with only three vowels. Spanish has five, a very common pattern. 1. Study the five-vowel system chart in Figure 5.9 with the Arabic vowels shown in boldface type, as they are produced by moving the tongue in the mouth. Vowels change according to the vertical and horizontal position of the tongue in the mouth. ~ ~
  • 18. 2. The Spanish vowel phonemes are pronounced as follows: /i/, as in 'neat', /e/, pronounced halfway between the vowels sounds in 'net' and 'Nate', /a/ as in 'not', /u/ as in 'newt', and/o/ pronounced halfway between the vowel sounds in 'note' and 'naught'. Pronounce the vowels and notice how your tongue position changes slightly as you move from front to back, then high to low in your mouth. Note that the labels represent the tongue positions. Tongue Front Central Back Position Tongue Height High Ii/ /ul lu/ Mid /e/ / of Low la/ Figure S.9. The Three-Vowel System of Arabic and Five-Vowel System of Spanish It should be noted that both systems are symmetrical. It is rare to find asymmetrical vowel sys- tems in languages. Just as you learned about consonants, you can see that vowels usually pattern in pairs. When examining the English vowel system, there is greater complexity but, still with vowels
  • 19. 73 Chapter Five: The Fundamentals ofApplied Linguistics: Communication through Language in pairs, the distinction is between a tense or a lax tongue, as seen in Figure 5.10. For ELs who speak languages with few vowels, such as Spanish, there is considerable transfer of the system from the Ll, and this is often carried into late stage L2 development, even fossilized (permanently fixed) in late learners. However, for speakers of languages having many different vowel sounds, such as Vietnamese, English vowels present fewer problems. Those speakers are already attuned to distin- guishing subtle differences across their own vowel systems. Tongue Front Central Back Position Tongue Height Tongue Height Tense Lax Lax Tense High /i/ 'neat' / I/ 'knit'I /u/ 'nook' /u/ 'newt'
  • 20. High Mid /ey/ 'Nate' /el 'net /A/ 'nut' hi 'naught' /ow/ 'note' Mid hi 'about' /£/ 'gnat' Low /a/ Low 'not' Figure S.1 0. The Vowel System of English. You have probably already noticed that the /r_/ sound in 'gnat' lacks a partner. Similarly, /a/ as in 'not' stands alone. Because the chart distorts the actual shape of the mouth, as you can see in the vocal tract diagram in Figure 5.3, the larger space in the high front
  • 21. and central areas can simply accommo- date more sounds than can the central and low back space, thus the asymmetry of the system. What makes English vowels difficult to focus on is that speakers of different regional or social dialects produce diphthongs rather than simple vowels in some contexts, or they produce simple sounds rather than diphthongs in other contexts. For example, many speakers from Brooklyn in New York City produce / u"/ instead of hi in 'coffee'; speakers of some Southern dialects produce / a/ instead of / ay / in 'sight'. Of course, dialect differences are even more profound across the English-speaking world ofBritain, Canada, the United States, Australia, and New Zealand. But speak- ers of English as their first language exist in many of the former colonies in Africa and Asia as well; thus, this greatly expands the possibilities of standard pronunciation across the English language. The concept of World Englishes was developed by Braj Kachru (1990, 2006), who argues that the notion of a 'standard' dialect of English must be expanded beyond the core English-speaking area. Suprasegmentals Phonemes are not the only focus of interest in the field of linguistics. Tone, intonation, pitch, length, and stress, called suprasegmentals, are other properties ofpronunciation that affect accent and other communication difficulties experienced by ELs. According to Hudson (2000) these sound qualities occur, not at the phonetic level, but "spread over more than one phone" (26). Also called prosodic phenomena, these are length, stress, and pitch.
  • 22. 74 Part Two: Language and Literacy (Applied Linguistics) Pause and Reflect 1. Now study the vowel system of English with its tense and lax vowel pairs, simple vowels, and diphthongs in Figure 5.10. 2. As you do so, note the key words in each position; say each word and listen to the very subtle difference between the two phonemes in each tense/lax pair. 3. Holding one hand, palm up, and pretending that it is your tongue, relax it, then tense it a few times. Pronounce the two words in each tense and lax pair as you relax and tense your hand. This kinesthetic activity is an effective way in helping Els feel the difference between the two sounds. Phonemes Minimal Pairs Examples /ii neat feel, teach, meat, cede, people, receive, believe, /I/ knit sit, mix, pick, film, rid, build /ey/ Nate bait, rate, plain, may, weigh, favor bait, rate, plain, may, /e/ net weigh, favor bet, said, rent, fence, bread
  • 23. /El /a/ /u/ gnat not newt tack, sap, fan, magic mop, father, calm, sock, Tom, jolly luke, tune, fruit, room, blue, fool, tour, move, do, two, blew /u/ /owl nook note look, put, foot vote, poll, coat, broke, holy, so, sew, Roman, sold, though, low, beau hi naught caught, song, crawl, fought, ball, paw Figure 5.11. The Pronunciation and Spelling of English Vowels and Diphthongs. 75 Chapter Five: The Fundamentals of Applied Linguistics:
  • 24. Communication through Language Phonemes Minimal Pairs Examples /A/ nut sun, luck, mother, enough, rung hi/ noise toy, moist, Freud, foyle /aw/ noun round, house, cow, now /ai/ night fight, kind, mine, type, sign, fry, buy /ar/ narc farm, park, sorry, heart, /Ar/ nerd clerk, bird, learn, worry, first, hurt /or/ norm storm, for, tore, born, boar Figure 5.11. The Pronunciation and Spelling of English Vowels and Diphthongs. (Continued) Pause and Reflect 1. The chart in Figure 5. 11 illustrates the system, the key words as minimal pairs, and other words having the same vowel phonemes as each key word. Pronounce each of the words again, and note the additional diphthongs and the words influenced by /r/. Note the move- ment of the tongue from one position to another as you produce the diphthongs. 2. Discuss with a partner or in a small group the reading, listening, and speaking difficulties you might expect Els to face given both the subtle tense/lax differences and the spelling
  • 25. variations. 3. Devise ways to help Els confront the realities of learning English pronunciation of vowels, using your knowledge of the specific difficulties experienced by l 1 speakers of languages you know. Phone lengthening can be a natural occurrence, demonstrated by the words 'hat' and 'had'. If you say these two words one after the other a few times, you will notice that the vowel sound is slightly longer in 'had'. The same occurs with'cab'/'cap','fuss'/'fuzz', 'half'/ 'halve' and 'bag'/'back'. As you may have discovered, this subtle lengthening occurs when the final consonant is voiced. In some lan- guages however, there is a phonemic difference between 'short' and 'long' vowel sounds. For exam- ple, in Swahili there is a phonemic difference between the words 'choo' (toilet), and 'cho' (a locative 76 Part Two: Language and Literacy (Applied Linguistics) particle within a verb). Vowel length produces a difference in meaning, though the vowel is produceci in the same place in the mouth in both words. ELs who speak languages having the lengthening fea- ture might be taught to use lengthening, a case of positive transfer, if they have difficulties with word- final devoicing. As American teachers well know, the terms short and long vowels refer to differences in sound, not actual vowel length, so they will need to help their ELs understand the difference.
  • 26. Stress, "the intensity or loudness of the airstream" (27), occurs at the syllable level. In the fol- lowing three-syllable English words the stress occurs on different syllables in each one: Canada (1s: aroma (2nd), disappear (3rd ). The underlying rules of syllable stress are complex and can be difficU:.: to master. Many languages of the world have more regular stress patterns, such as Spanish where stress on most words is on the next-to-the-last syllable. Other languages, such as Japanese, haYc minimal syllable stress. For such speakers, learning the various possible stress patterns of Eng~ can be a daunting undertaking. "Pitch is the frequency of vibration of the vocal folds" (27). More frequent vibrations produce 2. higher pitch. Patterns of frequencies at the phrasal or sentence level are called intonation. In Eng- lish, falling intonation sentence-finally is often associated with statements, rising intonation wit:- yes/no questions. Saying the same phrase such as "we don't" using several different intonatio:- patterns results in subtle meaning differences. The rising and falling intonation patterns of Eng~ statements and questions are not universal; so, ELs benefit from some direct teaching of these pa:- tems. Tonal languages are those where a difference in pitch on a vowel in a syllable represents a:- altogether different phoneme. Chinese, Vietnamese, Thai, Lao, and some African and Scandanavia:- languages are tonal languages. Vietnamese, a mono-syllabic tone language, has a high tone, a lo, -
  • 27. tone, a falling tone, and a contour (rising/falling) tone. Changing the tone of a syllable produces c. difference in meaning. In Mandarin Chinese, the word 'ma' means scold when pronounced with c. falling tone, and 'hemp' when pronounced with a rising tone. Speakers of tone languages may bE able to transfer their sensitivity to tones to learning English intonation patterns. Phonology and Second Language (L2) Learning In sum, monolingual children acquiring their first language (Ll ) are capable of mastering any soun~ of any language; they are not born with the sounds of their parents, but rather with the capacity tr- acquire those sounds. We use the term acquire to refer to subconscious learning. Children are no: really taught language; they 'pick it up' from the linguistic environment of their homes. If childre::'. grow up in a bilingual or trilingual environment, they acquire all the sounds of the languages the:- hear and begin to speak. If children are in the environment of a new language in school, for example they will experience interference or transfer from the home language at first. But because the br~ of children before puberty are quite flexible, they generally lose their accents early in the language learning process. If individuals meet a new language after the age of puberty, however, it is quite likely that first language effects will occur in the way they speak the new language. Spanish speak~ learning English, for example, may have difficulty producing the difference between the / i/ in 'bee: and the /I/ in 'bit, orbetween the initial sounds in'cherry' and 'sherry'. They will almost always haYE
  • 28. an accent from the Ll, although over time the accent may move closer to that of a native speaker o: the target language. Morphology As was discussed in the phonology section, languages have sound systems and these systems arE uniquely language-specific, but sounds themselves are not associated with meaning. Morpholo~ is the study of words and word forms and the processes by which words become created anci modified. Morphology does not entail the study of word meaning, but of word form and functio,: Words and word parts are stored and accessed from the lexicon, where they are associated witl-. meaning studied in the field of semantics. Morphemes Teach-er-s Non-lexicalLexical Teach- 77 Chapter Five: The Fundamentals of Applied Linguistics: Communication through Language Classification ofMorphemes Figure S.12. Morphology. Morphemes are the smallest units of meaning in a language. Note the etymology of the word mor-
  • 29. phemic: morph (form) and -emic pertaining to language specific phenomena. This is parallel to the term phonemic, pertaining to a language-specific sound. A classification of English morphemes is shown in Figure 5.13 with examples. Morphemes Free ( occurs alone) Bound (occurs with A free morpheme or other bound morpheme[s]) Lexical (content words) Non-lexical (grammatical or functional words) Lexical Stems/roots Non-lexical Derivational Non-lexical Inflectional book, family,
  • 30. pleasantness and, only, of, now, our -vis- (invisible) -volv-, volu- (revolution) -ish (bookish) -ness (bookishness) -s (books) -ed (booked) Figure 5.13. Classification of English Morphemes. Lexical items, also called content words, include nouns, verbs, and adjectives, the traditional parts of speech. Although for most languages the characteristics of meaning are similar, the formal characteristics are widely varied. Some languages, such as Vietnamese, are monosyllabic, so infor- mation about number, tense, and the like occurs as separate morphemes. Thus, identifying and dis- tinguishing among parts of speech is difficult for ELs without morphological marking in their Lls. Yet, form and meaning characteristics of the lexical categories for English are fairly well-defined, as shown in Table 5.1 (with characteristics from Hudson, 2000). ,..
  • 31. Part Two: Language and Literacy (Applied Linguistics) 78 Table 5.1. Lexical Morphemes (Parts of Speech). Parts of Speech Formal Characteristics Meaning Characteristics Nouns: (many) can be pluralized: -s are usually names of animate puppy, house, imagination, puppies beings, events, places, concrete birthday, Tallahassee (some types) can be made objects, or abstract concepts possessive: -s puppy's bone can be made definite (with 'the' or indefinite (with 'a') can be made negative with prefix (non-believer) Verbs: can be marked for tense (cook~ usually express actions, being, cook, take, identify, become, is/ coo keg) or aspect ( cooking, states of being are, reside have cooked) can add suffixes to indicate agent (bak§:, pianist) can be made negative with prefix (disagree, misunderstand)
  • 32. Adjectives: can be marked with -(i)er word- often modifies nouns or noun tall, round, hungry, beautiful, finally to indicate contrast, phrases interesting and with -(i)est to indicate superlative can be used with 'more' or 'most' can occur after 'very' or other intensifiers 1. You have probably noted that meaning characteristics described suggest that there are exceptions to the 'rules'. Identify ways in which some modifiers and some actions can be nouns and some nouns can be actions. 2. Can you add any formal characteristics to any of the lexical categories? 3. Another characteristic of the three lexical categories (also called open class words) is that new words are added to lexicon all the time, and some words drop out over time. Go to the Internet and search for obsolete words, as well as new words in the English language. Not all words are lexical; that is, not all have both formal characteristics and meaning proper- ties. A limited number of words "express a grammatical or structural relationship with other words in a sentence" (Nordquist at http:/ /grammar.about.com/od/fh/g/functionword.htm). These
  • 33. words belong to a closed class. No new words enter this class; they have only one form, and they do not change over time. Non-lexical or functional morphemes in English include prepositions (of, to), determiners (the, a), temporal adverbs (now, soon), conjunctions (but, and), pronouns (we, them), auxiliaries (be, have), modals (can should) and quantifiers (some, both). Although many prepositions do have lexical content, and they pattern like nouns, verbs, and adjectives at the phrasal level; they are often placed in the non-lexical category because they behave like other closed class words in lacking formal characteristics, not changing over time or increasing in number. https://grammar.about.com/od/fh/g/functionword.htm 79 Chapter Five: The Fundamentals of Applied Linguistics: Communication through Language Derivational Morphemes Derivation is a process involving the addition of a bound morpheme, called an affix, to the beginning or ending of an existing word. Derivational affixes are both prefixes and suffixes in English (although some languages also have infixes).* Examples of prefixes and suffixes are found in Tables 5.2 and 5.3 respectively. Note that Table 5.2 shows that a prefix retains the grammatical category of the root word, but changes its meaning. Prefix Change of meaning Examples ex- former ex-wife, ex-boyfriend
  • 34. deactivate, deconstruct disappear, discontinue, dislike de- not, or opposite opposite dis- mis- wrong misunderstand, misrepresent re- again reconsider, redo un- reverse action, or not uncover, unlock, unreliable inconvenient, impossible -in/-im not -con -sub with convene, conduct under submarine, subterranean Table 5.3 shows that a suffix changes the grammatical category (form) of a word, but retains its meaning. This distinction holds for most cases. Learning derivational morphemes is difficult for second language learners, especially for those whose languages lack prefixes and suffixes. But because these morphemes are taught to native speakers as part of the language arts curriculum, elementary, language arts, and English teachers already have effective ways of teaching them.
  • 35. Table 5.3. Suffixes, Change of Grammatical Category, and Examples. Suffix Change of Grammatical Category Examples -able Adjective from Verb movable, doable, readable -ing Adjective or Noun from Verb reading (class), moving (target), moving is hard, swimming is fun -ive Adjective from Verb impressive, subjective -al Noun form Verb refusal, denial, arrival -ant Noun from Verb defendant, applicant, informant -dorn Noun from Noun or Adjective kingdom, freedom -ful Adjective from Noun fearful, hopeful, stressful, blissful -ous Adjective from Noun poisonous, ruinous -ize Verb from Noun or Adjective capitalize, hospitalize, polarize, nationalize -en Verb from Adjective blacken, harden -ly Adverb from Adjective slowly, lightly -ity Noun from Adjective superiority, priority, ability • Tagalog, a language spoken in the Philippines uses infixes. For example -um is the infix to form the infinitive of a verb; sulat (write)-sumulat (to write)
  • 36. 80 Part Two: Language and Literacy (Applied Linguistics) Roots and Stems Not all bound morphemes are affixes. Some originate in Latin or Greek languages, from which a very great number of English words have been borrowed, often through French or Middle English. These stems or roots are bound lexical morphemes and they form the primary meaning of a word. Although some are words in their own right, others are not, as shown in Table 5.4. A number of resources are available to promote rapid and systematic vocabulary development for ELs, among them dictionaries and texts specifically devoted to stems and affixes. However, since ELs may overgeneralize their knowledge of a particular root or stem and misunderstand the word's meaning, educators should provide counter-examples to support learning. Table 5.4. Latin- (L) and Greek-(G)Based Stems/Roots. Stems/Roots (language of origin, meaning) Examples -port- (from L. portare, carry) transport, export, portable -due- or -duct- (from L. ducere, lead) conduct, deduct, produce, educate, inductive -vis- (from L. videre, see) visage, invisible, visionary, visitor, revise -volve-, -volu- (from L. volvere, roll) involve, revolution, voluble -cred- (from L. credere, believe) incredible, credulous, credit, credential
  • 37. -fin- (from L. finir, end, conclusion) finish, infinite, refined aero- (from G. akros, topmost, extreme) acrophobia, acrobat, acronym Inflectional Morphemes Some of the world's languages have a great many inflectional affixes. Romance languages such as Spanish, Italian, and French use suffixes to indicate gender (masculine/feminine) and number (singular/plural) on nouns; determiners and adjectives must agree with their corresponding nouns by adding the same suffix, as shown in the following sentences in Table 5.Sa. These languages also indicate number, person, and tense on verbs using suffixes, as shown in Table 5.Sb. Table 5.Sa. Spanish Nouns: Number and Gender Agreement. El libro esta verb en preposition la determiner mesa. noun Determiner noun (masculine, (masculine, (feminine, (feminine, singular) singular) book
  • 38. libros is located est.in on singular) the singular) table The Los en las determiner mesas noun Determiner noun verb preposition (masculine, plural) (masculine, plural) (feminine, (feminine, plural) _Elural) The books are located on the tables. Table S.Sb. Spanish Verb (hablar, to speak), Person, Number and Tense Marking. Present tense hablo(lps) hablas(2ps) habla(3ps) hablamos(lpp) hablais(2pp) hablan(3pp) Preterite tense (past) hable hablaste habl6 hablamos hablasteis hablaron (Note: ps=person singular; pp=person plural) (http:/ /www.studyspanish.com/lessons)
  • 39. 81 Chapter Five: The Fundamentals of Applied Linguistics: Communication through Language Pause and Reflect 1. Elementary teachers: With a partner or in a small group, address the issues in this scenario: You have a class with Els some of whom speak Romance languages, but several of whom do not. How would you organize instruction of a unit on derivational morphology that would be fair for speakers from both backgrounds? What learning aids or activities would you assign to help Els learn these morphemes independently? 2. Language arts teachers: You have strategies and resources for teaching root words and affixes. Suggest ways you might augment your teaching of these affixes for Els. 3. Secondary teachers: All academic areas utilize affixes as well as root words. List several examples in your academic content and suggest ways you might help Els to learn them. Swahili and other Bantu languages spoken across much of Africa make use of even more affixes on nouns and verbs, as demonstrated in this example: Tulimwendeshia Dar es Salaam garini letu in Table 5.6. We drove him to Dar es Salaam in our car. Prefixes and infixes precede the verb stem (go), while suffixes augment the verb's meaning. Modifiers of nouns must agree with their noun classes,
  • 40. or genders, of which there are several in Bantu languages, most having singular and plural forms. Table S.6. Explication of Swahili Sentence Tu- -li- -m(w)- end(a)- -eshia Dares gari- -ni 1- -etu Salaam lps past 3ps go cause to car in Agr, li/ lpp our We him/ ma her class sing For English speakers, the rich inflectional morphology of suchlanguages is usually quite difficult to learn because English makes so few distinctions. So, should learning the English system be easy because of its few inflectional morphemes? Unfortunately, it turns out that inflectional morphemes are quite problematic for ELs, so teachers must be aware that inflectional morphemes need to be given a lot of attention. The chart in Table 5.7 lists these morphemes. 82 Part Two: Language and Literacy (Applied Linguistics) Table S.7. Inflectional Morphemes of English. Part of Speech Morphemes Nouns Plural -s the cars; Possessive -s Mary's car Verbs 3rd Person Singular -s; Mary likes you.
  • 41. Present Participle -ing; Mary is reading. Past -ed; Mary liked the book. Past Participle -ed/-en; Mary has cooked. The eggs were broken. Adjectives Comparative -er big - bigger Adverbs Superlative -est big - the biggest -ly slowly, quickly I Developmental Phenomena in L2 Learning To explain why these morphemes are so difficult for learners of English, we must look at the facts of a child's first language development. As you have read, in addition to Ll transfer as a factor in L2 learning, learner errors can also be due to the complexity of the target language. All L2 learn- ers, like child Ll learners, produce developmental errors that arise when encountering difficult aspects of a target language. Research conducted with Spanish and Italian children learning thei: first language showed that they acquire the functions and forms of tense, person, gender, and num- ber marking earlier than English speaking children learn their inflectional system with far fewer morphemes. This is because of the regularity and salience in the inflectional systems of Spanish and. Italian. Although some verb roots display irregularity with and across tense and aspect, the ending5 (-o, -as, -amos, etc.) do not change. Note also that those endings are perceptually salient; that is, eacl-.
  • 42. one is an added syllable at the end of the verb root. It is easily heard; each suffix either is a vowel, or begins with a vowel, so it is processed easily. The learnability of the Spanish and Italian systems is less complex than that of English. Slobin (2007) and others analyzed the learning sequences of gram- matical morphemes by children from many different language backgrounds and have suggested a number of cognitive principles to explain ease and difficulty of learning. The learnability rule is a two-part explanation of why Spanish and Italian inflectional systems are easier to learn that of English. Part A: One form to one meaning is learned earlier than one form to man: meanings. With reference to Table 5.7, the inflectional morpheme '-ing' has one function,; the presen: participle indicates the progressive aspect. It also has one invariant form and is learned early. On the other hand, when spoken, the morpheme'-s' ( one form) can indicate three different functions: a plura:. a possessive, and a third person singular verb. Though the plural form is learned somewhat early, the other two are late. Similarly, the morpheme '-ed' has two functions, to mark the regular past tense anc. to indicate the regular past participle form, which can be used with both the auxiliary 'have' and 'be forms, in the case of the perfective aspect and the passive voice respectively. Again, native English- speaking children learn these forms late, especially the past participle form. These developmental fa~ about verbs are made more difficult by the existence of both regular and irregular past and past parti- ciple forms. It is often surprising to hear English-speaking children produce common irregular ver~ before the regular forms. This is because the '-ed' form, as in
  • 43. 'worked' or 'finished' is not as perceptual:: salient as an irregular form, as in 'broke' or 'took'. The fact that two stop consonants occur together at the end of the word is very difficult for man~- non-native speakers whose languages do not allow such clusters; the / t/ ending, as in /workt/, ~ not salient. As for verbs like 'need' and 'plant' (those that end in / t/ or / d/), adding -ed creates an extra syllable, making the ending salient. In fact, when learning to read English aloud, man:- learners overgeneralize the /Id/ form, producing' /laikld/ (lik- ed) or /muvld/ (mov-ed), assum- ing these verbs pattern with /nidld/ (needed). Although for learners the '-ed' form is usually no: salient, the irregular forms are. 83 Chapter Five: The Fundamentals of Applied Linguistics: Communication through Language Pause and Reflect 1. To understand the concept of perceptual salience, pronounce the words 'like' and 'liked' to yourself. Note that both have only one syllable and that the /t/ follows /k/, forming a cluster of two voiceless stops. 2. Why do you think the /t/ is difficult to hear in rapid speech? For example, learners might produce "I broke it" or "I ate it" before they produce "I pushed it" or "I cooked it." Later, when they begin to understand the function of'-ed', they overgeneralize with
  • 44. irregular verbs, producing such utterances as "I breaked it," "I broked it," or "I eated it." Language learners, noticing that recurring patterns are found in language, create rules to apply to new situa- tions. They use these rules all the time; generalization is a highly effective process in learning. Edu- cators must recognize that learners have created a rule when they overgeneralize, or extend the rule. Part B of the learnability rule is that one meaning to one form is learned earlier flum one meaning to many forms. A good example pertains to the pronunciation of the past tense '-ed' endings of regular verbs. Table 5.8 lists examples of this language phenomenon at the intersection of phonology and morphology. Table S.S. Regular Past Tense Verb Forms. liked fixed missed jailed picked buzzed banned inferred laughed moved begged finished dropped freed formed watched lobbed judged hanged stayed (V 9 Pause and Reflect 1. With a partner or in a small group pronounce the regular past tense forms in Table 5.8, and note whether you produce /t/ or/d/ for the regular '-ed' ending. Do not distort the pronun- ciation, but produce it as you would in normal conversation.
  • 45. 2. Put the verbs into either a /t/ list or a / d/ list; then, explain your reasoning. Hint: Note the voicing of /t/ and /d/. 3. Do you think native English-speaking children have to learn how to pronounce these verb endings correctly? Or does this come 'naturally' to them? 84 Part Two: Language and Literacy (Applied Linguistics) d,Je 5.10. Sentence Types . .~--=:nr~-itic:e Negative Yes/No Wh- question Emphasis Tag question Passive question -~ buys groceries Ari doesn't Does Ari buy Where does Ari does buy Ari buys A lot of buy groceries Ari buy groceries in groceries groceries □ Tampa. groceries in Tampa? groceries? Tampa. in Tampa, are bought in Tampa. doesn't he? in Tampa. :.-.c ··o..:::- ~O".!: ~ - ~--.-..- ......... ,.,;- ~ _;...,;,., -
  • 46. ~crru.a~ -.e: :a:-..g For ELs, particularly adolescents and adults, learning three separate morpho-phonemes for the -ed' ending (the It/ in 'liked', the /d/ in 'loved', and the /'-Id/ in 'needed') presents the one mean- :ng, many forms learning problem. English-speaking children probably pick up this pronunciation 2asily - they do not have to be taught how to do it. You may recall from the phonology reading :hat some languages have a 'devoicing' feature on consonants at the ends of words, or have no consonant clusters, or lack voiced stops or fricatives word- finally. So, it is clear that learning to hear, then produce these endings will probably take some focused explicit instruction. Note that the plural-s morpheme also has three forms: /s/ as in 'pipes', /z/ as in 'cars', and /Iz/ as in 'churches'. The following minimal pairs of noun plurals can be used to help learners hear, then produce the voiced/voice- less distinction: pigs/picks, buzzes/buses, mates/maids, calfs/calves, mobs/mops. In sum, in the case of one mean- ing/many forms, first language effects join with develop- mental effects to produce significant learnability difficulties. ::::: Problems in English Verb Morphology Co~ared to Spanish, the English inflectional morphological system is 'impoverished, there being :e'•,- endings or auxiliaries included in the English verb phrase. However, the system can be quite :::.ii.'.ficult for all learners and may not be mastered at all by
  • 47. non-native speakers. In fact, the order 0: learning English morphemes forms the basis of a claim (by some second language acquisition :-oe.archers) that developmental phenomenon, as opposed to transfer, better predicts the production Gf :-1 English inflectional morphology across time because of its complexity. a:ble 5.9. Auxiliaries and Modals and their Functions. I' re used with have used with do used with modals used with I a::-.., is, V + ing; have, V + -ed or do, does, V (unmark-ed) can, will, V (unmark-ed) a.re, progressive has, -en did does eat, did shall, can see, might ~.• •asl running, had cooked, go could, go ;;·ere eating eaten, would, V +-ed or -en broken should, Passive may, are seen, i§ might, eaten must, ought to Tne lists in Table 5.9 portray the auxiliaries (helping verbs) of English, the forms of 'be', 'have', .::n .and the modals in columns 1, 3, 5, and 7 respectively, with the verb forms they 'help' in columns :. ..;,, 6, and 8 respectively. Each of these (usually) behaves in the same way in affirmative statements, ?~es::ions (yes/no, wh-), negative statements, emphatic statements, tags, and passives. Table 5.10 portrays seven main sentence types using the 3<<l
  • 48. person verb 'buy' in the present ::c:-.se. ::-(ote how the verb is altered across the sentence types. j_ 3. - 85 Chapter Five: The Fundamentals of Applied Linguistics: Communication through Language Pause and Reflect 1. Describe what happens to the verb with each type, particularly the placement of the auxiliary or modal in question formation (linguists call this aux- movement). 2. Change the same verb to the past tense for each sentence type and describe what happens. 3. Try completing the seven columns of the chart with the 1st person present perfect aspect of the verb; for example, 'We have already eaten lunch.' and the verb with a modal, for example, 'They should finish by noon.' and the 2nd person past progressive aspect of the verb, 'You were running the race.' What differences in the patterns do you observe? In sum, we have learned that morphemes are the smallest meaningful units of a language, that :hey can be free or bound, and that bound morphemes are either derivational or inflectional. Deriva- :ional morphology is taught in language arts curricula because
  • 49. all students need to understand and :ise the morphemes. But students whose languages lack derivations may need more explicit instruc- :ion; L1 effects occur because such morphemes are absent in the Ll, not because something is dif- .:erent. Inflectional morphology is quite problematic because of both Ll and developmental effects. :-he basis for this claim is to be found by examining several examples of the leamability rule: in the sequence of learning for all learners of English one form/one meaning precedes and is easier than one form/many meanings, or one meaning/many forms. The examples should guide teachers to become more aware that L2 learning often needs a boost °:)ecause of Lleffects, developmental effects, or both. Some theories of second language learning pro- :note the idea that input alone is enough to initiate the process and keep it going 'naturally.' How- e·er, errors in reception and production may persist if not addressed early on, especially for all but :he youngest learners. Whether you agree with the habit formation theory or not, explicit instruction about English morphology and its phonological manifestations is essential for all students. Syntax :n 1959 Noam Chomsky, the most important linguist of the 20th century, challenged the claims of 3. F. Skinner, a noted behaviorist, that language learning can be explained as a series of stimulus/ ~ sponse events. Chomsky presented an alternative idea, namely that language is innate, and that ~anguage learning, or acquisition, is possible because a child is already predisposed to activate his/
  • 50. ier language organ. Over the years Chomsky and his followers, using data from many different - 86 Part Two: Language and Literacy (Applied Linguistics) languages, have developed and constantly revised a theory to explain an innate disposition for language, and what was once referred to as the language acquisition device (LAD). These linguists prefer to use the term acquire, as opposed to learn, to refer to subconscious processes that occur during the early years of life. Chomsky's major work focuses on the structural level of language called syntax, which entails the units beyond the word level- phrases and sentences. Theoretical Issues with Syntax So far in this chapter, you have read about phonology, the sound system, and morphology, word forms and functions. These systems are also structural components of language; they are systematic and rule-governed, as is syntax. It is important to point out that Chom- skians attempt to understand language more at the level of structure than of meaning. Meaning, as well as communication, he has claimed, should be beyond the purview of structural linguistics.
  • 51. In any language, a sequence of words may represent a sentence. But a sentence in a lan- guage cannot simply be just any sequence of words; it must conform to the rules underlying it. For example, the rules of English syntax do not generate, or describe, the sequence 'girl book see.' The elements are out of order, the verb does not indicate tense or number and articles are missing. Native speakers are able to recognize word combinations that violate these rules as ungrammatical, thus incorrect. This is not necessarily based on a speaker's familiarity with prescriptive language rules, but comes from knowledge often outside of awareness, acquired as we grow up. Moreover, the grammaticality of a sentence is not necessarily linked to meaning. Chomsky's (1957) famous sentence colorless green ideas sleep furiously doesn't make sense, but it conforms to the correct ordering of English and illustrates the separation of grammaticality and meaning. Lexical and Functional Categories In order to analyze the structure of phrases and sentences, a review of word-level categories is essential. Languages have two major groups of words: lexical and functional . These categories are listed in Table 5.11 (or refer back to Table 5.1). Linguists assume that all languages have the same categories. Functional categories are listed in Table 5.12. Universals in Word Ordering What is important about these categories of words is that in any given language they are ordered in specific ways; this is a universal aspect of all languages. The ordering rules of English word
  • 52. order are quite strict, and the order subject/verb/ (object) (SVO) is the canonical, or typical, order (The object category is parenthesized because not all sentences have objects). However, SVO is not the only possible word order, though it is widespread throughout the world, espe- cially in Western Europe. Languages of Korea, Japan, North Africa, and the Inda-Iranian family are predominantly subject/ (object)/verb (SOV). Verb-first orders are less common (though they include Arabic and several Celtic languages); object-first languages are rarer still, with American Sign Language (ASL) being among the very few of the OVS type, and Klingon the only OS" language. 87 Chapter Five: The Fundamentals of Applied Linguistics: Communication through Language Table S,11. Lexical Categories. Lexical categories Examples Noun book, city, girl, the playground, idea Verb write, read, sing, become, live, be Adjective lazy, slow, large interesting, young Preposition on, under, to, of, at Table 5.12. Functional Categories. Functional category Examples
  • 53. Determiners (Det) Article Possessive Demonstrative a, the her, our this, those Auxiliaries (Aux) Modal Non-modal will, can, may, might, must, could, etc. be,have,do Conjunctions (Con) and, or, but Degree word (Deg) too, quite, only, etc. Pause and Reflect 1. Create some sequences of words that do not constitute a sentence, and any other sequences that 'obey the rules' of English, but do not make sense. 2. Because Els come from a variety of L2 backgrounds, not all of whom share the English word order in sentences, they benefit from tasks that require putting words in the correct order. You can cut up sentences and mix the words from each sentence so that students can reassemble the sentences. Or you can provide specific Apps to support Els' learning by going to the ESOL website for suggestions.
  • 54. Case-Marking In examining Japanese and Korean, we find that the word order may be a bit looser than it is in English. That is because in these languages nouns are assigned case according to their functions in the sentence, such as subject, object, possessive, instrumental, and so on. In each 88 Part Two: Language and Literacy (Applied Linguistics) .: .. situation a case marker indicates the function of the noun. For example, a grammatical sentence in Japanese might be expressed as 'boy' (subject marker) 'horse' (object marker) kick'. Or it might read as 'horse (object marker) boy (subject marker) kick'. Both sentences have the same meaning because the case, not the word order, indicates who is doing what to whom. Because Japanese is a head-final word order language, however, the verb must come last, no matter what the order of the noun phrase. The verb phrase is the core of sentence structure, and, as noted previously, there are two options for the majority of world languages: verb first (English) and Yerb last (Japanese). Although many of the world's languages have no case-marking at all, others have 10 or more. The most common case is nominative (subject); accusative (direct object) is also common. In
  • 55. addition to Japanese and Korean, other case-marked languages are Latin, Slavic (Russian, Ukarainian, Polish, Czech, Bulgar- ian, Serbo-Croatian, etc.), Baltic (Lithuanian and Latvian), and Finno-Urgaic, (Finnish, Hungarian, Estonian). Luthuanian has 14 cases. Old and Middle English had case-marking on nouns, but Modern English retains case only on pronouns. Each per- son and number has subject, object, and possessive forms. For example the first person singular forms are 'I', 'me', and 'my' respectively; the third person plural forms are 'they', 'them', and 'their' respectively. These forms are usually learned late in first language acquisition, and may also pose difficulties for ELs. Most educators can only imagine that English speakers learning case-marked languages would find this aspect quite difficult. However, for speakers of these languages learn- ing English, the lack of case marking poses problems in ways that affect the ordering of phrases within an English sentence. Speakers of case-marked languages have the freedom to order elements to provide focus where they want it and do not neces- sarily follow a strict word order. The following examples from a narrative about a naughty baby owlet told by a native bilingual speaker of Russian and Czech, both case-marked languages, demonstrate this difficulty. Note that me speaker, Natasha, places focused information at the end of the sentence in ways different from ;:he way the information would be placed by English speakers. 1. And then first egg, braked and was born Frankie. And afterward second, from second egg was born Marushka. And after another while was born, Bedjik. 2. When she wanted to show her husband their new babies, it was in the bed only Frankie and
  • 56. Marushka. 3. After summer start school. 4. After this movie Bedjik played with Marushka and Frankie that he is bandit and he wants to shoot them. .:>. Marushka and Frankie saw ... that came, and spoke with Bedjik this big animal, red animal Lishka (fox). 6. And Lishka saw that he is so non-educated and he doesn't know who she is and then she w ants to play with him game. 1. After carefully examining the ways in which Natasha orders her sentences, imagine that she is a student in your classroom. With a partner or in a small group, discuss ways to help Natasha with word order problems (ignore for the moment some other problems such as omission of articles). 89 Chapter Five: The Fundamentals of Applied Linguistics: Communication through Language Order of Elements in Phrases A very interesting aspect of languages is that by and large the noun, verb, adjective, and prepo- sitional phrases usually follow the same pattern. For example, in the English verb phrase 'kicked :he horse', the verb precedes its object, while in the Korean phrase 'horse (object marker) kicked',
  • 57. :he verb follows the object. The verb is called the head of the phrase, and its object is called a com- plement, because it completes the phrase (phrases may also include specifiers which function to make the head precise*). At the very least, the obligatory element in a phrase is the head. There can- not be a phrase without a head in its category. Examining the other phrases in English or Korean, we find that all phrase types follow the same order as the verb displays. The examples in Table 5.13 illustrate how specifiers, heads, and complements are ordered in English. Pause and Reflect 1. Using a reference, or based on your own experiences, construct a chart of phrases from another language similar to those in Table 5. 13. Table 5.13. English Phrase Structure, Head First. Phrase Types Specifier Head Complement >Joun Phrase (NP) The A boy pig wearing a tie Verb Phrase (VP) operates goes a machine
  • 58. Adverb Phrase (AP) very clean hard as a rock Prepositional Phrase (PP) just under around the trees the comer Although basic phrase structure word orders for most languages are either head-first or head-last, rules for arrangements within these basic structures may be more complicated. For example, exam- ine the string of words in Table 5.14 and place them in grammatical order. Table S.14, Complex NP. ICereal IThose IYellow IBowls ILarge IThree This string is not a sentence, but a noun phrase with several adjectives. Begin by identifying the noun that is the head of the phrase. Native English speakers almost always agree on the order, but often cannot explain the 'rule' that describes it. When given this string of words to put in order, non-native speakers experience difficulties because ofits complexity. This example demonstrates that as the second language (L2) develops, all learners have to deal with ways in which English is challenging to learn. * Specifier type is determined by the phrase head. For example, in the noun phrase (NP) 'the book' the specifier can be a determiner (Det), in this case 'the'. But an adverb cannot be a specifier in an NP: 'slowly book' is not grammatical.
  • 59. ___ _ ?O Part Two: Language and Literacy (Applied Linguistics) d® Pause and Reflect .._, l. Now that you have seen how English orders phrases, translate each of the words in the phrase above into Spanish or another language you may know. 2. Arrange them in their correct grammatical order. Is the order the same as that for English noun phrases? Do you have to add any words to make a good translation? 1. Construct similar examples based on the same pattern. 2. When you recognize what kinds of adjectives occur in which position, create a chart like Table 5.13 and label each column to show the order of the various types of modifiers. ELs will not always have a 'feel' for the correct order, and will need to be taught. By providing a 5eneralized ordering rule, teachers will be able to guide students in overcoming English word order ;:ii.-=ficulties. You may have noticed that the head of the noun phrase comes, not at the beginning, but a': the end when this string of words is put in order. This is because single word adjectives occur
  • 60. ::-eiore the noun in English, while adjectival phrases and clauses follow the noun, thus preserving ::.."-le normal head-first phrase structure pattern. When students see several examples of the same pattern, they can recognize, and later follow, ::..'--;.e rules to create a well-ordered phrase. Verb, adjective, and prepositional phrases may also be complex, so learners need practice putting them in order as well. Prescriptive Grammar Rules K."owledge of English word order is typically not taught in American schools because native speak- 2r5 acquire it as children. More often than not, however, grammar is formally taught in foreign :.a....-1guage courses in order to present the structure of the language. Frequently the grammatical rules taught in upper elementary language arts and secondary English courses in American schools are prescriptive, rather than descriptive rules, those that portray how the language actually works. Many so-called 'rules' are actually quite old and were proposed by English grammarians based on a study of Latin. For example, the 'rule' do not end a sentence with a preposition works for Latin, but not for English. We accept "who are you going with?" even though the question ends with a preposition. Another incorrect rule is use a singular verb with a plural subject and a plu- ral verb with a singular subject. In examining the ....,, .....::.RuLcS (}) ___
  • 61. (f)®·----- @ 91 Chapter Five: The Fundamentals of Applied Linguistics: Communication through Language ;e.ntences in Table 5.15, you can see that the rule is not correct. Neither 'I' nor 'you', both singular, ::-equires the verb to be marked with '-s'. In fact, only one person (3rd) takes a so-called 'plural' verb. But, as noted in the morphology sec- jon, the '-s' does not represent a plural, but simply denotes the third person singular form of the ? resent tense verb. In older versions of English, verbs were marked for person and number, just as in Spanish and other European languages. Only the 3rd person singular form has remained in English. In some dialects of English it, too, is not used, and may someday disappear from English all together. All students need to understand how a language really works in normal spoken and written discourse. Table S.1 S. The third person singular rule. Person, Number, Singular Subject Verb Person, Number, Plural Subject Verb
  • 62. 1st I read }'I we read 2nd you read 2nd you read 3rt1 he, she, it reads 3,ct they read In sum, linguist Noam Chomsky claimed that language is an organ, just as the heart and lungs are organs, providing us with an innate linguistic capacity that allows young children to acquire :canguage out of awareness. Because language is an organ, he argues, we are born with the capac- :ty to use the organ already partially pre-programmed, such as with the knowledge that language obeys certain ordering rules, with all phrase types (noun, verb, etc.) being consistently either head- :irst or head-final across the board. Korean children are not born with a head-last word order, nor are Spanish-speaking children born with a head-first order. Instead, as children hear their first :anguage, they figure out the phrase structure rules and apply them when learning to speak. With respect to second language learners, the ordering rules of the first language may produce :ransfer effects on later-learned languages, though they might not result in errors as transparent as those at the phonological level. In Natasha's case, the fact that her two Lls are case-marked allowed arrangement of elements less constrained than those of English. On the other hand, the example of the complex noun phrase (about the cereal bowls) illustrates that developmental effects are also at play. It takes a long period for such complex structures to be fully learned. It is important to note that the work of Chomsky, his students, and his other followers is formi-
  • 63. dable inn its detail and depth of insight about the workings of language. Nonetheless, his theory has been challenged by other linguists, including developmental psycholinguists, whose data raise questions about the notion of a pre-programmed language template. Yet, no matter whose theory educators espouse, access to the right kinds of information about how languages really work in conversation and writing is essential. To appropriately and adequately understand and address ELs' needs, educators must acknowledge a variety of factors and different theoretical explanations. Semantics You have read about phonology (system of sounds), morphology (word forms and formation pro- cesses), and syntax (how words join to form larger units), and the three levels of language considered to be innate, according to the Chomskian perspective; the next two levels are semantics and pragmatics, both seen as learned aspects of language. "Semantics is the branch of linguistics thatdeals with the study of meaning, changes in meaning, and the principles that govern the relationship between sentences or words and their meanings" (http:/ /www.thefreedictionary.com/semantics). In this section, you will find information about the lexicon, ideas for teaching vocabulary, assigning vs. containing meaning, denotation and connotation semantic relations at word and sentence level, and ambiguity. Throughout this section are suggestions for discussion and for ways to assist English learners from PreK-12. www.thefreedictionary.com/semantics
  • 64. 92 Part Two: Language and Literacy (Applied Linguistics) Experimental research has revealed that words are organized and stored in many different ways in the mind. For example, if you can't remember a word, you may remember its beginning sound, form a mental representation of the word (such as 'dog'), or locate the word according to its cat- egory. Table 5.16 provides a list of categories of word knowledge (Nation, 1990) which help to illus- trate how words are stored in the mental lexicon. Table 5.16. Types of Word Knowledge. Examples - deep Spoken form of a word Types of Word Knowledge /dip/ I d-e-e-p Morphemes possible Written form of a word deep (adjective); also depth (noun), deepen (verb), deep -er, -er -est (comparative, superlative forms) deeply (adverb) Grammatical behavior of a word The pool is deep (predicate adjective). He's going into deep water (adjective before noun). It's too deep for wading (adjective with specifier & complement).
  • 65. Collocational behavior of a word (as) deep (as the ocean), often used with words pertaining to bodies of water or places below !!round Frequency of a word how often it occurs in speech or writing Stylistic register constraints; when a word is inappropriate to use It would be inappropriate to use slang or swear words when speaking with a person who is interviewing you for a job. Conceptual meaning of a word Representation of a small figure with a large amount of water, for example, beneath it. Associations with related words Deep: shallow (antonym), profound (synonym), far down in water or underground place (paraphrase) Table 5.17 provides a list of principles of vocabulary learning (Schmitt and Schmitt, 1995) whirr are also based on an understanding of the mental lexicon. Table S.17. Principles of Vocabulary Learning. 1. The best way to remember new words is to incorporate them in (into) to language that is already known. 2. Organized material is easier to learn. 3. Words which are very similar should not be taught at the same time. -!. Word pairs can be used to learn a great number of words in a short time . .J. Knowing a word entails more than just knowing the meaning. 6. The deeper the mental processing used when learning a word, the more likely a student will
  • 66. remember it. 7. The act of recalling a word makes it more likely that the learner will recall it again later. 8. Learners must pay attention in order to learn more effectively. 9. Words need to be recycled to be learned. 10. Learners are individuals with different learning styles. Vocabulary learning in the classroom can be done in many different ways. Mendoza (2003) cre- ated suggestions for university level English learners based on her doctoral research. Some of these suggestions are well-suited for younger learners, as well as middle and secondary ELs. She advise.:: learners to make new words on their own in at least three of the following ways: a . draw a picture b. write a translation c. use it in a sentence d . write its part of speech Chapter Five: The Fundamentals of Applied Linguistics: Communication through Language ~- e. write its various forms f. write a synonym and an antonym g. say the word aloud several times h. use it with your friends i. put a new word list on a bulletin board or chart j. keep an alphabetized list of words in your notebook k. organize your words by topic or by part of speech 1. make a list of words with similar meanings
  • 67. m. organize words according to their Greek or Latin roots n. locate a word in the dictionary and analyze its parts (prefixes, suffixes, roots). Pause and Reflect 1. Recall from the morphology section that all words, or lexical and non-lexical items, are free morphemes, and that word parts are bound. Explain whether you think bound morphemes are also stored in the mental lexicon w ith their meanings/uses, or whether they are stored with individual free morphemes. 2. For information about language and the brain, visit: http://emedia.leeward.hawaii.edu/ hurley/Ling 102web/mod5_Llearning/5mod5.1 _neuro.htm. What It Means to Mean: Do Words Contain Meaning? Early in thenineteenthcenturyinCentral Europe, semantics emerged as a separate subfield of linguis- tics to study meaning (http:/ /www.studymode.com/ essays/ The-History-Of-Semantics-1550608. html). According to the Western Rationalist position, meanings are not contained in words (or larger units), but are assigned to these units by speakers and hearers over time in public discourse. If mean- ings were contained in language, they would not change over time, although we know that they do. Similarly, because there are so many meanings assigned to some words in the lexicon, a single conceptualization can be difficult to pin down. For example, the meaning of run in 'run a race' is not the same as run in 'run a business'.
  • 68. The core meaning of run is 'to move quickly on foot' because speakers of English understand it that way. Yet we might instead have started using the word 'bleb' (a non-word), or 'correr' (the word in Spanish) instead of 'run'. Specific words are simply arbitrary combinations of sounds that are assigned meanings by speakers. Whenever a new concept emerges, somebody names it, the name spreads, and soon it becomes a part of the lexicon of speakers. When a new edition of a dic- tionary appears, it contains these new words in the communal lexicon. Adults are able to distinguish between a word and its referent. Yet, developmental studies have shown that very young children do not always conceptually separate words from their referents. Some research has determined that bilingual children have an advantage over monolinguals in this regard. When shown a picture of the moon, told it is a 'bleb,' and asked if the picture is still of a moon, the very young monolingual English-speaking child will say "no." But because Spanish/ English www.studymode.com http://emedia.leeward.hawaii.edu 94 Part Two: Language and Literacy (Applied Linguistics) bilingual children of the same age have already learned that the moon can be called luna and moon, they are likely to say that the picture is still the moon, despite its being called 'bleb.' They know that two different words can have the same referent.
  • 69. 6® Pause and Reflect l. With a partner or in a small group discuss ways you have observed vocabulary learning in the classroom. 2. Have you had success with any of the methods listed above? 3. How would you encourage Els to identify and use their individual preferences for learning new words? 4. Mendoza encouraged her students to personalize their vocabularies by dividing a 5x8 card into four sections and writi ng one of the options listed above on each section, as shown in Figure 5.14. How can the use of technology support this strategy? Definition and Part of Speech: credible (adjective), believable Use in a sentence: His story about the accident was not credible. I think he was lying. Translation: Words with same root: incredible, credit, credulous, creditor (from Latin, credere), accreditation Figure 5.14. Individual Vocabulary Learning Technique.
  • 70. Denotation and Connotation Two terms are important to address at this point, connotation and denotation, each of which is an aspect of meaning. Word denotation is presented in dictionary descriptions; for example, the word 'rat' is defined in Merriam-Webster's Online Dictionary as "any of numerous rodents (rat- tus and related genera) differing from the related mice especially by considerably larger size." This is its denotative meaning; however, this does not constitute the variety of associations this word can evoke. For many people, a rat calls to mind feelings of disgust, fear, or even plague. In the Chinese culture, however, the rat is the first sign of the zodiac and has several positive associations; " .... attributes of an animal with spirit, wit, alertness, delicacy, flexibility and vital- ity" (http:/ /www.travelchinaguide.com/intro/social_customs/zodiac/rat.htm ). The word rat connotes a variety of associations in one's mind; these are both culturally and individually influ- enced, or they may be personal or restricted to small groups of people, such as a family or a sub-culture. www.travelchinaguide.com/intro/social_customs/zodiac/rat.htm 95 Chapter Five: The Fundamentals of Applied Linguistics: Communication through Language 6(1} Pause and Reflect
  • 71. 1. Check dictionary definitions for five common words, for example, 'marriage.' Write the deno- tation and connotations for each (refer to the 'rat' example). 2. Find examples from different cultural backgrounds. In mainstream American culture a couple usually makes the major decisions regarding their future life together; but, elsewhere mar- riages may be arranged by parents. What do you think 'marriage' connotes for a prospec- tive bride who is only twelve or thirteen years old? Semantic Relations Several kinds of semantic relations exist among words, as shown in Table 5.19. Categorizing words in these ways assists learners to extend their vocabularies; flashcards and a variety of games enable students to learn words independently or in small groups. The terms subject and predicate name syntactic roles, but they are not so useful when it comes to meaning, and will not take ELs far when struggling with sentence meanings. Noun phrases in Eng- lish fulfill various kinds of semantic roles. Four are listed in Table 5.18, along with definitions and examples (from Hudson, 2000). Recognizing the semantic role of noun phrases can help readers, including ELs, better understand the connections among various parts of a sentence. Table S.18. Semantic Roles. Agent doer, actor The woman came here. Patient entity affected by what agent or causal agent does
  • 72. The police punished the teen-agers. Recipient receiver of deed of agent I bought the gift for my mother. Experiencer preceiver of a stimulus Rain pleased the farmers. Six additional roles are location, instrument, time, stimulus, cause, and goal. Recognizing the semantic role of noun phrases can help second language readers better understand connections among various parts of a sentence, as will be shown in the section on ambiguity. Concepts Word meaning is closely related to how concepts are represented in the mind. An earlier way of thinking about concepts is that necessary and sufficient conditions exist for an object to be called a certain thing. For example, the defining conditions of 'bird' are generally thought to be something like [animal, small, able to fly]; although, we would have to admit that ostriches arebirds though they are neither small, nor do they fly. Amore recent view is that concepts lack clear-cut boundaries; there are 'fuzzy' boundaries around them. Thus, if you try to state the necessary and sufficient conditions 96 Part Two: Language and Literacy (Applied Linguistics) for something to be a chair or a game or a bird, you will find some chairs, games, or birds that don't
  • 73. fit the core definition. Certain fossil forms demonstrate a biological link between ancient reptiles and birds. So, are they birds? Are 'Ring around the Rosy' or 'London Bridges' games in the same way that Monopoly and football are games? Is a beanbag a chair, even though it doesn't have legs? Table S.19. Semantic Relations at the Word Level (From Yule, 2010). 1. With a partner or in a small group, make a short list of necessary and sufficient conditions for two or three of the following: dog, cup, table, bachelor, cushion, coat, friend. 2. Identify counter-examples for your conceptual descriptions. What 'fuzzy areas' did you encounter? Were some of the examples 'fuzzier' than others? 3. Make a Venn diagram to represent the relationship between the concepts 'language' and 'communication.' Are they two separate concepts? Or does one concept include the other? Or do they overlap, or mean the same thing? Although concepts might be fuzzy within a language, they become even more problematic across languages. For example, the word 'aunt' in English may represent any of five relationships: mother's sister, father's sister, mother's brother's wife, father's brother's wife, or close family female friend of the parents' generation. However, no such generic word as 'aunt' exists in Vietnamese, for there are differ-
  • 74. ent words for various people in familial relationships according to the following criteria: the side of the family (mother's or father's), birth status in the parents' respective families (older or younger sibling of the parent), and relationships by marriage. Although it may seem strange to be required to learn all of these possible relationships, it is essential for the young child to do so, for every persons/he meets is addressed, not by using a word for 'you', but by the label of one of these relationships ("Good morning, mom's younger sister word, Thuy"; or, Greetings, father's elder brother word, Tam). The point to be made here is that semantics, including word meaning, creates the bridge between language and culture, for it is at this intersection where meaning is ascribed to events, objects, roles, and relationships among members of a culture. Meanings are culture-bound in immeasurable and often surprising ways. 1. If you come from a culture other than the so-called 'mainstream' American culture, identify two concepts that exist in your (sub-) culture, or your region of the country, that are absent in the generic North American context. It may be that the other cultural background influences culture in the United States in ways that havebecome 'mainstream' such as, having pinatas atbirthday parties. ~ ~ 2. Among the overriding concepts that define Americans are the related ideas of individualism and independence. What do you think may be
  • 75. the origins of Semantic relation Synonomy, same meaning Polysemy, refers to words having multiple Examples big/large, fat/thin, wrong/mistaken bank, chair, foot meanings Homophony, same sound bear/ bare, hear/ here, two/ too Antonymy Opposites: wide/narrow; gradable: long stick/long car; complementary: single/ married; relational: Hyponomy, meaning of one word included in fold/ unfold dog/animal, carrot/vegetable, rose/flower meaning of another; pertains to subordinate & superordinate categories 97 Chapter Five: The Fundamentals of Applied Linguistics: Communication through Language these concepts? If your own background, or that of some of your students, highlights the value of different concepts, how do these
  • 76. influence behavior and performance in the classroom? Metaphors Another important aspect of word meaning is metaphor, not simply a literary device used by writ- ers, but a pervasive feature of all languages. Examples in English include the commodity metaphor for talking about time (we waste, save, spend, and lose time), the conduit metaphor for 'packaging' mental activities (we get points across, put our thoughts into words, and get through to each other), and the gun metaphor (shooting for next week, no silver bullet, parting shot, targeting). In learning another language, metaphors are fun without even recognizing that one's own language has them as well; however, metaphors are often difficult for second language learners. Pause and Reflect 1. Make a list of metaphors using the names of body parts in English (the head of the bed, the leg of a chair, the long arm of justice). 2. After composing a substantial list, make generalizations about some of them. Do you know of other languages that use body part words metaphorically? Semantic Relations: Meaning beyond Word Level Semantic relations also exist among sentences as shown in Table 5.20. When a person 'knows' a language well, these various relationships within and among sentences are usually quite obvious, but they are not so obvious for ELs. For example, indirect
  • 77. speech acts can be difficult for learners. If you say "It's really hot in here" and the person does not respond by offering to open the window, he may not have understood your indirect request. Non-native speakers may also appear rude when they say something in a direct manner that native speakers express indirectly, such as "I want you to give me x" rather than "I could certainly use x." In addition, other direct requests most Americans consider inappropriate are: "How much money do you make?" and "How old are you?" 1. With a partner or in a small group, write five indirect statements by English speakers. 2. Check with people from other cultural backgrounds, or use the Internet, to search for examples of indirectness. 98 Part Two: Language and Literacy (Applied Linguistics) Table 5.20. Semantic Relations within/Across Sentences. Semantic relations Definition Examples Paraphrase one statement with a meaning similar to another statement The cat chased the mouse. The mouse was chased by the cat. Entailment
  • 78. Contradiction Presupposition Deixis one statement entailing the consequence of the second two contrary statements a statement assuming a pre- existing truth elements in a statement implying the location of the speaker The hunter killed the bear. The bear is dead. Charles is a bachelor. Charles is married. When did you stop beating your wife? A bear is going into the tent. A bear is coming into the tent! Speech Acts associations with the utterance of a statement 'I can't reach the salt' being an indirect way of telling someone to pass it.
  • 79. 'I pronounce you man and wife' being an act that produces a concrete result. Ambiguity Sentences or phrases in a language can be ambiguous; that is, they may be understood in more than one way. For example, the simple phrase 'a large hat box' may refer to a box for large hats or a large box for hats (of any size). This is a structurally ambiguous phrase because we don't know whether the referent of 'large' is a hat or a box. Ambiguity may also be lexical. For example, in the phrase 'put the money in the bank' the word 'bank' might refer to a place where people go to exchange money, or it could refer to the area beside a river. Plays on words, such as puns are a wonderful source of humor, but can also challenge ELs. Lucas studied how intensive ELs disambiguated puns; for example, the ambiguity in these two plays on words: 1. One morning I shot an elephant in my pajamas. How he got into my pajamas I'll never know." 2. Groucho Marx: Let's talk about rights and lefts. You're right, so I left. Ambiguity is found in phrases where words are left out, as in newspaper headlines. The ambi- guity could be caused by omission of words or by modifiers in the wrong place. For example, in the phrase 'miners refuse to work after death;' it would helpful to resolve the ambiguity if it had said 'another miner's death.' Table 5.21 provides a list of several humorous headlines. As you can well
  • 80. imagine, many of these pose considerable difficulties for ELs. Table 5.21. Ambiguous Phrases. 1. Panda Mating Fails; Veterinarian Takes Over 2. Juvenile Court to Try Shooting Defendant 3. Miners Refuse to Work after Death 4. British Left Waffles on Falkland Island 5. Typhoon Rips Through Cemetery; Hw1dreds Dead 6. Man Struck By Lightning Faces Battery Charge 7. Include Your Children When Baking Cookies 8. Something Went Wrong in Jet Crash, Expert Says 9. Police Begin Campaign to Run Down Jaywalkers, Safety Experts Say 10. School Bus Passengers Should be Belted 11. Drwlk Gets Nine Months in Violin Case 99 Chapter Five: The Fundamentals of Applied Linguistics: Communication through Language Table 5.21. Ambiguous Phrases. (Continued) 12. Enraged Cow Injures Farmer with Ax 13. Two Sisters Reunite After 18 Years in Checkout Counter 14. Enfield Couple Slain; Police Suspect Homicide 15. Kids Make Nutritious Snacks Pause and Reflect 1. With a partner or in a small group, find the ambiguity in the statements in Table 5 .21. Identify whether the ambiguity is lexical, structural (the way the sentence is arranged), contextual, or
  • 81. some other aspect. Hint: You may find that if you can identify the semantic role of problematic noun phrases, you will be successful. 2. Once you figure out the speaker's intention from the headlines, explain how you would resolve the ambiguity. Semantics is the study of the meaning of words, phrases, and sentences. Word meanings are stored in the lexicon, which is located both in the head and among members of a speech community, in the form of an abstract list of words organized in a number of different ways. However, meaning is not in the word. Meanings are assigned to words, concepts, and events by speakers of a language within a particular culture milieu; they are not contained in words or sentences. Semantic relations hold between words; synonyms, antonyms, and homonyms are examples. Semantic relations also hold at the sentence level. Examples are deixis, entailment, andpresuppositions. It is possible to create ambiguity in statements; ambiguity makes it possible for speakers to say one thing and mean another. Pragmatics So far in this chapter, you have proceeded from learning about the more structural aspects of lan- guage, those claimed to be innate and universal, to those more closely associated with communication and culture. You have learned that in semantics the focus is on meaning, where universal claims are not so easy to make. As found by looking at the word ' aunt,' concepts are not fixed across cultures. In this section, the focus is on aspects of pragmatics that are
  • 82. particularly relevant to the teaching of ELs. The Scope ofPragmatics According to Davis, 1991, "pragmatics will have as its domain speakers' communicative intentions, the uses of language that require such intentions, and the strategies that hearers employ to deter- mine what these intentions and acts are, so that they can understand what the speaker intends to communicate" http:/ /plato.stanford.edu/entries/pragmatics/. Pragmatics, or 'speaker meaning' is often called invisible meaning (Yule, 2010) because comprehension of an utterance may involve not just the spoken words but also the context in which two people interact. This context can include https://plato.stanford.edu/entries/pragmatics 100 Part Two: Language and Literacy (Applied Linguistics) setting, roles and relationships of the participants, and shared history and assumptions. When speakers do not share the same assumptions, communication problems arise, as is often the case between a teacher and an EL, and/or a student who does not know the norms ofparticipation in the situation. These norms are often unspoken, and native English speakers may be aware of them only when they are not followed. Thus, in studying about pragmatics, the notion of shared context should be presented. Specific topics addressed in this section are dialects and bilingualism, child language socialization, language functions (speech acts), BICS and CALP, and written discourse.
  • 83. Dialects and Bilingualism An interesting problem with Chomsky's theory of a single language in a speaker's head, sometimes called a language template, is the fact that mostpeople in the world speak atleast two languages, or two dialects of the same language. This means that people can have two or more grammars in their heads at the same time. When speakers of Jamaican English understand and speak several dialects of the same language, it is difficult to know whatgrammar rules are in their heads, because the different dialects oper- ate according to different rules. Bilingualism and bidialectalism pose a problem for a Chomskian- only approach to language. Speakers use the avail- able linguistic resources in the many contexts where they interact with others or express them- selves in writing. For example, many bilinguals freely switch, often mid-sentence, between their two languages, thus mixing rules as well. Although teachers should be providing ELs with grammar and vocabulary instruction in Eng- lish, teaching is incomplete without also help- ing them to function in communicative settings. Those who have grown up speaking the English language from childhood participate in a wider speech community of American English, and control the various dialects of their ethnic and geographi- cal backgrounds. They also reflect the social register(s) shaped by their home upbringing and in their occupational or educational endeavors, and they can shift the way they speak depending on whether the interlocutor is a three-year old, an old high school friend, or a bureaucrat in an office. Minority stu-
  • 84. dents are expected to control Standard English, whether they actually know it or not, and to code-switch in settings that demandit. To persist in using home dialects in school or other mainstream environments may invoke disapproval or lower marks on written work. A minority speaker may be seen as willfully refusing to know the dialect, or simply ignorant, inarticulate, or incapable of clear thought. Those who disparage dialects such as African American Vernacular English are usually unaware that dialects, like languages, are patterned phenomena with their own grammar rules, meanings, and contexts of use. Pause and Reflect 1. Go to the ESOL website or search the Internet to find out more about language varieties and dialects that describe dialects of American English or other languages. 101 Chapter Five: The Fundamentals of Applied Linguistics: Communication through Language 2. With a partner or in a small group, explore the wider speech community of your neighbor- hood, and listen for dialect differences. Use voice memo, Voice Thread, or other technologies to record speech. 3. Note similarities and differences across your data, summarize your findings about each dialect, their approximate age, and their educational and socioeconomic level.
  • 85. 4. Share the results of your research w ith your classmates and discuss general findings. Child Language Socialization Cross-cultural studies of child language development have shown that even with a wide range of inputs, young children grow up speaking the language spoken around them. Although child- rearing practices concerning language vary greatly across cultures, similar types of language phenomena are found across child Ll learners (one-word stage, two-word stage, etc.). Even deaf children who grow up in homes with hearing parents create their own 'home sign,' an indi- vidual language having structural properties similar to those of spoken languages. But children also learn to participate in the communicative practices of their homes and neighborhoods, and these vary quite widely. Shirley Brice Heath's ethnography (1983) described two contrasting working class communities in a rural area of the Carolinas to demonstrate how primary language socialization practices learned at home later influence interaction patterns and performance expectations in the classroom when children begin school. Compared with middle class children from the city, these children, though mastering the valued practices in their home communities, were not prepared to interact in the 'mainstream classroom' at school, where secondary language socialization occurs. An example of a ubiquitous practice in mainstream classrooms is the 'firetruck' dialogue between a teacher and a student; this is a typical stimulus-response-evaluation routine.