SlideShare a Scribd company logo
HMSV 346 Diversity Issues in Human Services / Spring 2021
1/2
(George Floyd) Advocacy Project
Objective: Students will understand the importance of advocacy
and develop the attitude, knowledge, and
skills needed to effectively advocate on behalf of oppressed or
marginalized groups.
Background: A few months ago, George Floyd died when four
Minneapolis police officers were trying to
subdue and arrest him. His death, which was caught on video,
raised the now common concerns about
police brutality towards African Americans, and ignited
protests, unrest, and debate throughout the country.
Personal and Professional Relevance: Such incidents (the death
of Floyd and others) and subsequent
chain of events impact you in two distinct ways:
• As an individual bound by your personal identities, thoughts,
values, etc.,
• As a soon-to-be human service professional bound by a
professional code of ethics.
While your reactions to these events as an individual are quite
likely to be emotional and subjective, it is
essential that as a human service professional you maintain
objectivity in understanding and addressing
these events. The primary objective of this project is to develop
fundamental skills necessary to advocate
on behalf of an oppressed or marginalized group. Successful
completion of this project/assignment requires
you to answer the following questions and complete the
following parts:
Part I
1. What is your personal reaction, thoughts, feelings, etc. about
such incidents?
2. There are various perspectives about who is responsible for
such incidents:
a. Some claim that incidents such as these are caused by those
(some African Americans) who violate
the law or disobey the lawful orders of cops.
b. Others claim that such situations are caused by those racists
(some cops) who are ready to inflict
violence on African Americans at the slightest pretext or for no
reason whatsoever.
c. Some believe that such incidents happen because of a
combination of both (a) and (b).
d. A few believe that it is neither a, b, nor c, but there are other
or more reasons for why such
incidents happen.
What is your personal opinion about the reason(s) for such
incidents? Choose only one of the above (a, b, c,
or d) and elaborate.
Part II
3. Are such incidents relevant to human services practice with
clients? In other words, are incidents such
as these likely to impact certain groups of clients?
• If yes, then elaborate upon these impacts/consequences.
• If no, then provide a strong justification for your answer.
4. Human service professionals are required to consider the
National Organization of Human Services’
ethical standards in professional decision making. Identify
which ethical standards may apply to
understanding and addressing such incidents (Hint: Check out
the standards pertaining to
Responsibility to the Public and Society). Provide a brief
justification as to why you believe these
standards may apply to such incidents.
5. What steps would you take as a human service practitioner to
ensure that people-in-power (police
commissioners, lawmakers, funders, community leaders, etc.)
take appropriate action to prevent such
incidents?
• Ideally, your response to this question (5) are influenced
solely by the NOHS ethical standards. If
you believe that the standards are not sufficient enough to
address such incidents, then state so and
clarify what would be your course of action as a human service
professional to ensure that people-
in-power do what is needed.
HMSV 346 Diversity Issues in Human Services / Spring 2021
2/2
• Ideally, your response to this question (5) is based on what
you believe to be the reason for such
incidents (so keep in mind your choice for question 2), but this
is not a requirement. So if you do
not want to be restricted by your choice for question 2, then that
is acceptable.
• Clearly specify what needs to change or what the change
would look like.
Part III
6. Identify a person (example: member of congress, U.S.
senator, governor, state legislator, police chief,
CSB manager, county supervisor, human services director, etc.)
who has the power to make the
changes you seek.
• The person(s) you identify does not have to be specific to
Hampton Roads, Minneapolis, or where
you live. You can choose any person anywhere in the country.
• Permission from the course instructor is required before
selecting a person/persons for
advocacy.
• Send an “advocacy email” to this identified person.
o In this email, briefly state the importance of the issue (why
things need to change) and how the
change would look like.
o Use the NAMI toolkit to develop your email, which should be
to the point.
o It is highly recommended that you cite credible journal
articles, books, reports, organizations,
etc., in support of your position.
o Submission of Part III in Blackboard requires posting a copy
of your advocacy email and a
snapshot of a confirmation of receipt of your advocacy email.
Not all recipients will provide
confirmation of receipt, so a snapshot of your advocacy email
(taken before you send it!) will
suffice in lieu of a confirmation of receipt.
o Sample advocacy emails are available in the “Advocacy
Project” folder under “Assignments &
Submissions” on Blackboard.
Part IV
7. Reflection:
a. Was your advocacy effort effective? In other words, did it
bring about the change you were
seeking? (Do not exaggerate! You will lose points if you do).
Share the reasons for your answer.
b. What did you learn about the importance of advocacy? Do
you see it as an effective tool?
c. What would you do differently in the future to be a more
effective advocate?
8. Submit your reflection (answers to question 7 above) on
Blackboard along with parts II and III (not
part I).
9. Respond meaningfully to the postings of three of your
classmates. State what you liked about their
advocacy effort and why, and state what would you do
differently and why. [Important: Please do not
scold or humiliate anyone for their opinion about this issue or
what they want to change. Very politely
state your disagreement, if any, by simply saying what you
would do differently and why].
Note
• This assignment is worth 50 points. Parts I & II are worth 10
points each. Parts III & IV are worth 15
points each.
• Your answers to all of the above questions (excluding
responses to classmates in discussion board)
must be at least 2 pages or 1000 words long, but should not
exceed 5 pages or 2500 words.
• Use only single-spaced lines.
• APA style is not required, but all parts and your advocacy
email must be professionally written and
formatted (ex: Aligned left, free from grammatical,
compositional, and spelling errors, etc.)
• Citations will add value to your submissions especially to your
advocacy email. If citations are used,
then references are required.
ARTICLE
Exercise enhances motor skill learning by
neurotransmitter switching in the adult midbrain
Hui-quan Li 1,2✉ & Nicholas C. Spitzer 1,2✉
Physical exercise promotes motor skill learning in normal
individuals and those with neu-
rological disorders but its mechanism of action is unclear. We
find that one week of voluntary
wheel running enhances the acquisition of motor skills in
normal adult mice. One week of
running also induces switching from ACh to GABA expression
in neurons in the caudal
pedunculopontine nucleus (cPPN). Consistent with regulation of
motor skills, we show that
the switching neurons make projections to the substantia nigra
(SN), ventral tegmental area
(VTA) and ventrolateral-ventromedial nuclei of the thalamus
(VL-VM). Use of viral vectors
to override transmitter switching blocks the beneficial effect of
running on motor skill
learning. We suggest that neurotransmitter switching provides
the basis by which sustained
running benefits motor skill learning, presenting a target for
clinical treatment of movement
disorders.
https://doi.org/10.1038/s41467-020-16053-7 OPEN
1 Neurobiology Section, Division of Biological Sciences and
Center for Neural Circuits and Behavior, La Jolla, CA 92093-
0357, USA. 2 Kavli Institute for Brain
and Mind, University of California San Diego, La Jolla, CA
92093-0357, USA. ✉email: [email protected]; [email protected]
NATURE COMMUNICATIONS | (2020) 11:2195 |
https://doi.org/10.1038/s41467-020-16053-7 |
www.nature.com/naturecommunications 1
12
3
4
5
6
7
8
9
0
()
:,;
http://crossmark.crossref.org/dialog/?doi=10.1038/s41467-020-
16053-7&domain=pdf
http://crossmark.crossref.org/dialog/?doi=10.1038/s41467-020-
16053-7&domain=pdf
http://crossmark.crossref.org/dialog/?doi=10.1038/s41467-020-
16053-7&domain=pdf
http://crossmark.crossref.org/dialog/?doi=10.1038/s41467-020-
16053-7&domain=pdf
http://orcid.org/0000-0001-9552-2191
http://orcid.org/0000-0001-9552-2191
http://orcid.org/0000-0001-9552-2191
http://orcid.org/0000-0001-9552-2191
http://orcid.org/0000-0001-9552-2191
http://orcid.org/0000-0002-3523-1103
http://orcid.org/0000-0002-3523-1103
http://orcid.org/0000-0002-3523-1103
http://orcid.org/0000-0002-3523-1103
http://orcid.org/0000-0002-3523-1103
mailto:[email protected]
mailto:[email protected]
www.nature.com/naturecommunications
www.nature.com/naturecommunications
M
otor skill learning is fundamental to everyday life and is
regulated by a neural network involving the cortex,
thalamus, basal ganglia, brain stem, cerebellum, and
spinal cord1,2. Both neuronal and glial plasticity are essential
for
motor skill learning and disruption of this plasticity causes
motor
deficits3,4. Aerobic physical exercise promotes the abil ity to
acquire new motor skills5 and serves as a therapy for many
motor
disorders6–8, but its basis of action is not well understood.
Run-
ning is a natural motor activity for mice9 and generates
plasticity
in multiple brain regions4,10,11. Neurotransmitter switching is
a
newly appreciated form of plasticity that refers to the ability of
neurons to change their transmitter identity in response to sus-
tained stimuli, typically leading to changes in behavior12. We
hypothesized that chronic running induces neurotransmitter
switching in a circuit that is important for motor skill learning.
We find that mice that have run on a wheel for a week have an
enhanced ability to acquire motor skill on the rotarod and
balance beam. This enhancement in learning is temporally cor-
related with an activity-dependent transmitter switch of
midbrain
cholinergic neurons that now express GABA. The change in
transmitters corresponds to changes in the levels of transcripts
of
their transmitter synthetic enzymes. The switching neurons
project to nuclei that regulate motor skill learning, including the
SN, VTA, and VL-VM. Overriding the switch using viral tools
to
restore the loss of the synthetic enzyme for ACh or prevent the
gain of the synthetic enzyme for GABA blocks enhancement of
the ability to acquire motor skills.
Results
Sustained running enhances motor skill learning. We investi -
gated the impact of chronic voluntary running on motor skill
learning by exposing adult mice to running wheels for one week
(Fig. 1a). Each mouse spent consistent time on the wheel every
Ctrl
0
10
20
30
40
50
1 2 3 1 2 3
Ctrl
Run
Test
Run 1-wk Rest 1-wk
Retest
Retest
Retest
Rest 2-wkRun 1-wk
Run 1-wk Rest 4-wk
Test
Test
**
Rest
Rotarod
S
p
e
e
d
a
t
fa
ll
(R
P
M
)
0
20
40
60
80
1 wk 2 wks 4 wks0
h
****
Ctrl Run + reste
d
S
p
e
e
d
a
t
fa
ll
(R
P
M
)
0
20
60
40
80 ***
Rotarod
Ctrl Run
a
D7 D8 D9
R R&B B
R: Rotarod; B: Balance beams
D1
Start to run
Run
g
Ctrl Run Ctrl Run
b
S
p
e
e
d
(
km
/h
)
E
p
is
o
d
e
d
u
ra
tio
n
(
s)
0
0.5
1.0
1.5
2.0
2.5
Wheel running
80
0
160
240
400
320
D1 D7 D1 D7
***
***
5 RPM
80 RPM
6 min
Balance beam
** **
***
Trial Test
4 mm rod
T
im
e
t
o
c
ro
ss
t
h
e
b
e
a
m
(
s)
f
Ti
m
e
t
o
c
ro
ss
t
h
e
b
e
a
m
(
s)
***
Beam
Ctrl Run
** **
****
*
**
**
**
S
p
e
e
d
a
t
fa
ll
(R
P
M
)
c
**
Trial Test
Rotarod
0
10
30
20
50
40
p = 0.71
j k
Beam
T
im
e
t
o
c
ro
ss
(
s)
Rest 1 wk 2 wks 4 wks0
0
10
20
30
40
50
** **
**
i
Ctrl Run + rest
Beam
Test
Rest 1-wkRun 1-wk
0
20
80
40
S
p
e
e
d
a
t
fa
ll
(R
P
M
)
Rotarod
T
im
e
t
o
c
ro
ss
(
s)
30
0
10
20
50
4060
p = 0.41
p = 0.67 p = 0.77
0
20
40
60
80
1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 1 2 3
Ctrl
Run
Rest 1-wkRun 1-wk
Training Training
Enhanced motor
skill learning
Enhanced
performance
continues
Rest 2-wk
No enhancement
or
Fig. 1 Sustained running enhances motor skill learning. a Top:
Runner mice were housed with running wheels and control mice
with wheelbases. Bottom:
timeline for running and behavioral tests. b Mean speed (left, n
= 6 animals per group) and mean episode duration (right, n = 7
animals per group) of
running on day 1 (naive) or day 7 (1-week trained). c Speed at
fall on an accelerative rotarod of each trial during training or
each test the day after training.
d Mean speed at fall on this rotarod in three tests on the day
after training. e Time to cross a 1-m long, 4-mm diameter rod
balance beam during each trial
of training or each test the day after training. f Mean time to
cross this balance beam in three tests the day after training. For
c–f, n = 19 Ctrls and 20
Runners. g Timelines for immediate behavioral testing and
retesting. h, i Mice that had run for 1 week or non-runner
controls were tested with (h) rotarod
and (i) balance beam (4 mm rod). After rest for 1, 2 or 4 weeks,
mice were retested. For h, n = 12 animals per group for 0 and 1
week, 7 Ctrls and 6 Runners
for 2 weeks, and 7 per group for 4 weeks. For i, n = 12 animals
per group for 0 week, 10 for each group for 1 week, 7 per each
group for 2 and 4 weeks.
j Top: timelines for delayed behavioral testing. Bottom:
performances on rotarod (left) and balance beam (right, 4 mm
rod) after 1 week of running followed
by 1 week of rest compared to controls that never ran. n = 8
animals for Ctrl and 9 for Run+Rest. k Time dependence of
enhanced motor skill learning.
When motor skills are trained (green) immediately after 1 week
of running, runner mice show enhanced learning. Enhancement
of acquired motor skills is
sustained for at least two weeks. When motor skills are trained
(red) after 1 week of running followed by 1 week of rest, no
enhancement in learning is
observed. Statistical significance *p < 0.05, **p < 0.01, ***p <
0.001 was assessed by two-sided paired t-test (b) or two-sided
Welch’s t-test (other panels).
Data shown are mean ± SEM.
ARTICLE NATURE COMMUNICATIONS |
https://doi.org/10.1038/s41467-020-16053-7
2 NATURE COMMUNICATIONS | (2020) 11:2195 |
https://doi.org/10.1038/s41467-020-16053-7 |
www.nature.com/naturecommunications
www.nature.com/naturecommunications
day, indicating continuous interest, and their running skill
improved as measured by increased running speed, increased
duration of running episodes and more stable movement on the
wheel (Fig. 1b and Supplementary Fig. 1a, b). At the end of the
running period we assessed performance on the accelerating
rotarod and narrow balance beam to evaluate motor skill acqui -
sition13–15. In comparison to mice without running wheels,
mice
that ran for one week demonstrated enhanced learning of motor
skills, mastering an accelerating rotarod more rapidly and
accommodating to balance beams more quickly (Fig. 1c–f and
Supplementary Fig. 1c, d).
We calculated the slopes of learning curves to assess the speed
of motor skill learning. Mean data points for each training trial
were plotted and fitted and the coefficient of determination (R2)
was used to justify the fit. The mean speed at fall from a rotarod
for the nine trials on the training day was fitted by a one-phase
association model (R2 > 0.96 for controls; R2 > 0.95 for
runners).
yðtÞ ¼ y0 þ ðA � y0Þ 1 � e�kt
� �
ð1Þ
y(t) is the speed at fall on trial number t, y0 is the speed at fall
for
the first trial (t = 0), A is the plateau and k is a rate
constant.The
slopes were calculated with
y0ðtÞ ¼ dy
dt
¼ ke�ktðA � y0Þ ð2Þ
To calculate the initial slope, y′(0), we used
y0ð0Þ ¼ g k; Að Þ ¼ kðA � y0Þ ð3Þ
The standard errors were calculated with
∂g
∂k
¼ ðA � y0Þ ð4Þ
∂g
∂A
¼ k ð5Þ
δg ¼
ffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffi
ffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffi
∂g
∂k
δk
� �2
þ ∂g
∂A
δA
� �2
s
¼
ffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffi ffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffi
ffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffi
A � y0ð Þ2δk2 þ k2δA2
q
ð6Þ
The values of the slopes are presented as g ± δg (mean ± sem).
Initial slopes were 17 ± 4 rpm/trial for runners and 5 ± 2
rpm/trial
for controls, significantly steeper for the runners (p = 0.01,
Welch’s t-test) (Fig. 1c).
The mean time to cross a 4 mm rod beam for the three trials on
the training day was fitted by linear regression (R2 > 0.98 for
controls; R2 > 0.99 for runners)
yðtÞ ¼ y0 þ kt ð7Þ
y(t) is the time to cross the beam on trial number t, y0 is the
time
for the first trial (t = 0) and k is the slope
y0ðtÞ ¼ k ð8Þ
The values of the slopes are presented as k ± δk (mean ± sem).
The
slopes were −4.7 ± 0.5 s/trial for runners and −2.4 ± 0.3 s/trial
for
controls, again significantly steeper for runners (p < 0.001,
Welch’s
t-test) (Fig. 1e). Additionally, sustained running improved
rotorod
and balance beam test performance (Fig. 1d, f and
Supplementary
Fig. 1d) but did not affect basal locomotor activity as measured
by
infrared beam crossings in home cages (Supplementary Fig. 1e,
f).
The steeper slopes of the behavioral acquisition curves demon-
strate enhanced learning relative to the controls, in accordance
with previous studies16,17. If the change in behavior induced
by
wheel running were one of performance, these curves would be
parallel and the slopes would be equal.
To explore further the effect of running on enhancement of
motor skill learning, we removed running wheels from mouse
cages after the first motor skill tests and retested the mice after
different resting periods (Fig. 1g). Enhanced performance on
both
rotarod and balance beam was sustained up to 2 weeks but not
4 weeks (Fig. 1h,i). Moreover, when mice were not trained on
the
rotarod and balance beam until 1 week after running, their
motor
skill learning was not enhanced (Fig. 1j). This result suggests
that
running creates a sensitive period in the adult brain during
which
motor skill learning is improved and shows that the gain in
motor
skills persists longer than the duration of this period (Fig. 1k).
Running induces transmitter switching in the midbrain cPPN.
Because transmitter switching is activity-dependent18–20, we
first
searched for c-fos expression (Fig. 2a) to determine sites of
increased brain activity associated with running and to identify
neurons likely to switch their transmitter. Mice that ran for
1 week exhibited a six-fold increase in the number of c-fos+
neurons in the pedunculopontine nucleus (PPN) of the midbrain
compared to non-runner controls when examined immediately
after the end of the last running period (Fig. 2b, c). The dentate
gyrus also showed increased c-fos expression in runners, as
expected21. The PPN was an attractive candidate for running-
induced transmitter switching because it regulates gait and bal -
ance control in health22,23 and disease24. However, the rostral
and
caudal PPN (rPPN and cPPN) are distinct. Both contain gluta-
matergic, GABAergic and cholinergic neurons, but there are
important differences in the proportions of their transmitter
phenotypes25, their projection targets and activity during
movement23,26, and their roles in behavior27,28. Accordingly
we
looked for differences in the activation of the rPPN and cPPN.
Specifically, we asked whether running activates cholinergic
neurons (marked by choline acetyltransferase, ChAT, which
synthesizes ACh) differently in the rPPN and cPPN, because
these neurons are involved in the gait and postural disorders of
Parkinson’s disease29. One week of running increased the
number
of c-fos+ neurons in both rPPN and cPPN (Supplementary
Fig. 2a, b). However, running increased the percentage of c-
fos+
neurons in the ChAT+ neuron population by 22-fold (from 21 of
984 to 315 of 670) in the cPPN but increased the percentage by
only two-fold (from 62 of 347 to 127 of 321) in the rPPN (Fig.
2d,
e, the left panel of 2f, and Supplementary Fig. 2c). Moreover,
the
percentage of ChAT+ neurons in the c-fos+ neuron population
was not different between controls and runners in the rPPN (62
of 309 vs. 127 of 644) but increased six-fold from controls to
runners in the cPPN (from 21 of 261 to 315 of 673) (Fig. 2f,
right). Significantly, running increased the number of cfos+
non-
cholinergic (ChAT-) neurons in the cPPN by only 1.5-fold (from
240 to 358), much less than the 15-fold (21–315) increase in the
number of c-fos+ ChAT+ neurons (Supplementary Fig. 2c).
These results show that cPPN cholinergic neurons are more
strongly activated by sustained running than rPPN cholinergic
neurons or cPPN non-cholinergic neurons, identifying cPPN
cholinergic neurons as candidates for transmitter switching. The
increase in the proportion of c-fos+ neurons in the ChAT+
cPPN neurons occurred after as early as 3 days of running
(Supplementary Fig. 2d, e).
Indeed, chronic running for one week was accompanied by a
decrease in the number of cPPN neurons expressing both ChAT
(605 ± 19; Fig. 2g, h) and the vesicular acetylcholine
transporter
(VAChT; 459 ± 51; Supplementary Fig. 2i). This change was
accompanied by an equal increase in the number of cPPN
neurons expressing the gene encoding glutamic acid decarbox-
ylase (GAD1; 551 ± 95), the enzyme that generates GABA
(Fig. 2i, j). No neurogenesis or apoptosis was observed in
the cPPN of either control or runner mice (Supplementary
Fig. 3a–f). These results suggest that ~600 cPPN neurons
switched their transmitter from ACh to GABA. There was no
NATURE COMMUNICATIONS |
https://doi.org/10.1038/s41467-020-16053-7 ARTICLE
NATURE COMMUNICATIONS | (2020) 11:2195 |
https://doi.org/10.1038/s41467-020-16053-7 |
www.nature.com/naturecommunications 3
www.nature.com/naturecommunications
www.nature.com/naturecommunications
change in the number of ChAT+ neurons in the rPPN or the
adjacent lateral dorsotegmental nucleus and no difference in the
number of neurons expressing the vesicular glutamate transpor -
ter 2 (vGluT2) in the cPPN (Supplementary Fig. 3g–l).
To determine whether cholinergic cPPN activity plays a role in
the running-induced ACh-to-GABA switch, we expressed Kir2.1
inward-rectifier potassium channels specifically in cPPN choli -
nergic neurons to suppress their activity20 and examined the
impact on running-induced gain of GAD1 and loss of ChAT. We
used a well-characterized ChAT-Cre transgenic mouse line30,31
that exhibited the same running-dependent loss of ChAT and
gain of GAD1 expression as wild-type mice (Supplementary
Fig. 2f). We found that runner mice that had received AAV-
DIO-
Kir2.1 showed no difference in the number of ChAT+ or
GAD1+ neurons in the cPPN vs non-runner control mice and
were significantly different from the runner mice that received a
control AAV construct (Supplementary Fig. 2g, h). These
results
suggest that cholinergic cPPN activity is required for
transmitter
switching. Because one-third of cholinergic cPPN neurons lose
ChAT, the larger fold change in c-fos expression in the
cholinergic population of the cPPN compared to the rPPN
(Fig. 2d, e) is caused by both an increase of c-fos and decrease
of
ChAT expression. Considering that two-thirds of cholinergic
cPPN neurons do not switch their transmitter, the predominant
increase in c-fos expression in ChAT+ neurons (Supplementary
Fig. 2c) is consistent with cell-population-autonomous and non-
cell-autonomous activity-dependence of transmitter switching
previously reported19.
Mice that had run for 1 week, not subjected to motor skill
training, and allowed 1 week of rest now exhibited the same
number of ChAT+ and GAD1+ neurons as control mice that
had never run on a running wheel (Fig. 2k). No apoptosis or
neurogenesis was detected in the cPPN of these mice
(Supplementary Fig. 3c–f), indicating that the transmitter
switch had spontaneously reversed. The time during which
the transmitter switch reversed (Fig. 2k) corresponds to the
time during which the benefit of running on motor skill
acquisition disappeared (Fig. 1j, k), indicating a temporal
correlation between transmitter switching and the ability for
enhanced motor skill learning. This finding raised the
possibility that the transmitter switch is necessary for running
to enhance motor skill learning. Note that the transmitter
switch had reversed one week after running (Supplementary
Fig. 2j), even when training and testing on the rotorod and
balance beam occurred immediately after running, while the
acquired motor skills persisted for at least 2 weeks (Fig. 1g–i).
These results suggest that the transmitter switch is not
necessary for maintaining the acquired motor skills.
cPPNrPPN
*
cPPNrPPN
c-
fo
s+
/C
h
A
T
+
(
%
)
*
p = 0.99
p = 0.09
60
0
20
80
40
60
0
20
80
40
1000
G
A
D
1
+
n
e
u
ro
n
n
u
m
b
e
r
3000
0
**
2000
0
1000
2000
3000Runner
C
h
A
T
I
H
C
Control
D
M
*
**
0
100
200
300
a
c-fos screening
PPN
SNr
SNc
STR
GPe
STN
Glu(+) GABA(–) Dopamine
M1
M2
b c
Runner
scp
scp
PPN
PPN
Control
c-fos NeuN c-fos NeuN c-fos
D
M
g h
Control Runner
G
A
D
1
I
S
H
D
M
GAD1+ChAT+
0
1000
2000
3000
cP
P
N
n
e
u
ro
n
n
u
m
b
e
r
j
C
h
A
T
+
n
e
u
ro
n
n
u
m
b
e
r
**
Ctrl Run
i k
Ctrl Run
C
h
A
T
+
/c
-f
o
s+
(
%
)
fe
c-fos
Runner cPPN
ChAT
d
c-fos
Runner rPPN
ChAT MERGE MERGE
D
M
c-
fo
s+
c
e
ll
co
u
n
ts
p
e
r
m
m
2
p = 0.66
p = 0.93
cPPN
DG M1 M2 STN GPe SNc SNr PPN LDT
p = 0.25
p = 0.18
p = 0.33
p = 0.41
*
p = 0.13
cPPN
RunCtrl
RunCtrl
Run + RestCtrl
p = 0.07
Fig. 2 Running activates the pedunculopontine nucleus (PPN)
and triggers neurotransmitter switching in the caudal PPN. a
Motor circuitry screened
for activity. b Double staining of neuronal marker NeuN and
activity marker c-fos in a control (upper) and 1-week runner
(lower). Middle and right panels
are higher magnifications of the boxes in the left panels. Scp,
superior cerebellar peduncle. Dorsal (D) and medial (M) axes
are shown at top. Scale bar,
(left) 200 μm, (middle) 50 μm. c c-fos immunoreactive neuron
number in each region or nucleus of controls and 1-week runner
mice. DG, dentate gyrus;
M1, primary motor cortex; M2, secondary motor cortex; STN,
subthalamic nucleus; GPe, globus pallidus external; STR,
striatum; SNc, substantia nigra pars
compacta; SNr, substantia nigra pars reticulata; PPN,
pedunculopontine nucleus; LDT, laterodorsal tegmental nucleus.
For b, c, n = 12 sections from 4
animals per group. d, e Double staining of ChAT and c-fos in
the rostral (d) or caudal (e) PPN of a 1-week runner. White
arrowheads identify examples of c-
fos+ChAT+ cells. Scale bar, 100 μm. f Percentage of the c-
fos+ChAT+ neurons in the total ChAT+ neurons (left) or in the
total c-fos+ neurons (right).
For d–f, n = 3 animals per group. g 3,3′-diaminobenzidine
(DAB) staining of ChAT in the cPPNs of a control and 1-week
runner. Dotted lines outline the
PPN. Dark brown stain identifies ChAT+ neurons. Scale bar,
100 μm. h Stereological counts of g. n = 6 animals per group. i
In situ hybridization of GAD1 in
the cPPN of a control and 1-week runner. Scale bar, 100 μm. j
Stereological counts of i. n = 7 animals per group. k
Stereological counts of ChAT+ or GAD1+
neurons in the cPPN of mice that experienced 1 week of running
followed by 1 week of rest and of control mice that never ran on
a running wheel. n = 5
animals per group. Statistical significance *p < 0.05, **p < 0.01
was assessed by two-sided Welch’s t-test (c, f) or Mann–
Whitney U test (other panels).
Data shown are mean ± SEM.
ARTICLE NATURE COMMUNICATIONS |
https://doi.org/10.1038/s41467-020-16053-7
4 NATURE COMMUNICATIONS | (2020) 11:2195 |
https://doi.org/10.1038/s41467-020-16053-7 |
www.nature.com/naturecommunications
www.nature.com/naturecommunications
The number of neurons expressing ChAT was inversely
correlated with the number of neurons expressing GAD1 and
directly correlated with the enhanced learning of motor skills
(Supplementary Fig. 4a–e), suggesting that the level of
transmitter
switch determines the enhancement level of motor skill
learning.
The amount of running was not correlated with the motor skill
acquisition (Supplementary Fig. 4f–i), perhaps because the
variation in running duration is small (coefficient of variation,
0.17). Running is a categorical rather than a continuous variable
and enables individual variability in transmitter switching and
motor skill learning.
To seek more direct evidence for neurotransmitter switching,
we selectively tagged cholinergic neurons with genetic markers
to
reveal their change in transmitter identity following the exercise
challenge. We injected a Cre-dependent AAV vector (AAV-
DIO-
mRuby2) into the cPPN of ChAT-Cre mice to permanently label
cholinergic neurons with mRuby2, including those that subse-
quently lose ChAT after running (Fig. 3a, b). We then scored
the
number of mRuby2+ neurons that express ChAT and/or GABA
immunofluorescence. The anti-GABA antibodies were validated
in the superior colliculus where immunostaining of GAD67
identifies cell bodies (Supplementary Fig. 5). The colocalization
between GABA and GAD67 demonstrates the specificity of the
anti-GABA antibodies to detect GABAergic neurons. Co-
labeling
of mRuby2, ChAT and GABA revealed that in control mice,
65%
(548/844) of cPPN cholinergic neurons tagged by mRuby2
expressed only ChAT, while 29% (245/844) of them co-
expressed
ChAT and GABA, 4% (34/844) expressed neither and 2% (17/
844) expressed only GABA (Fig. 3c). In runners, 24% (177/738)
of mRuby2+ neurons expressed only ChAT, 31% (229/738) co-
expressed ChAT and GABA, 12% (89/738) expressed neither
and
33% (243/738) expressed only GABA (Fig. 3c). The decrease in
neurons expressing only ChAT and increase in neurons
expressing only GABA in the ChAT-Cre line identify the
expression of GABA in formerly cholinergic neurons. The
increase in the number of neurons that express neither ChAT
nor GABA suggests that switching neurons lose ChAT before
gaining GABA. The apparent constancy of the percent of co-
expressing neurons may indicate that switching and co-
expressing populations are distinct.
Transcript levels of transmitter synthetic enzymes change. To
understand whether the loss of ChAT and gain of GAD1 occur
in
the same population of neurons at the transcript level, we used
sensitive fluorescence in situ hybridization to analyze the levels
of
mRNA of transmitter synthetic enzymes in cPPN neurons from
runners and controls. We used the expression of neuronal nitric
oxide synthase (nNOS) as a biomarker restricted to ChAT+
neurons in the PPN32 (Fig. 4a, b). Although the number of
ChAT+ neurons decreased with one week of running (Fig. 2g,
h),
the number of nNOS+ neurons did not change (Fig. 4c). We
then
combined immunofluorescent labeling of nNOS with RNAscope
to detect mRNA encoding ChAT and GAD67 in nNOS+ neu-
rons. By measuring the number of transcript puncta (Fig. 4d–g)
and the total fluorescent area and fluorescence intensity per cell
(Supplementary Fig. 6), we demonstrated a decrease in the
number of ChAT transcripts and an increase in the number of
GAD1 transcripts in neurons expressing nNOS in runners.
While
RNAscope revealed the presence of ChAT transcripts in all
nNOS
neurons in controls (Fig. 4e), immunostaining identified 11% of
nNOS neurons that did not show detectable ChAT immunor-
eactivity (Fig. 4b). These results imply that in nNOS neurons of
controls, the bottom 11% of the distribution of ChAT mRNA
puncta (Fig. 4e; expressing <8 puncta) lack detectable ChAT
immunostaining. We next grouped neurons into the four cate-
gories (as in Fig. 3) on the basis of ChAT and GAD1 mRNA
expression (see Methods). In control mice, 45% of the cPPN
nNOS+ cells were classic cholinergic neurons, while 44% of
them
co-expressed ChAT and GAD1, 6% expressed neither and 5%
expressed GAD1 (Fig. 4e). However, in runners, the percentages
changed to 18% for classic cholinergic neurons, 43% for co-
expressing cells, 12% for neither and 27% that expressed GAD1
(Fig. 4e). These changes in the four categories are comparable
to
those identified by immunocytochemistry (Fig. 3c), further sup-
porting a running-induced switch from ChAT to GAD1 in nNOS
neurons. More modest co-expression of ChAT and GAD tran-
scripts has been observed in the adult rodent PPN33–35; the dif-
ference is likely to result from different ways to detect and
score a
positive neuron using thresholding probes (see Methods). The
RNAscope assay reveals that switching involves up and down
regulation of transcript levels and may not entail complete
GABA mRuby2ChAT MERGE
Run
Ctrl
CtrlCtrl
*
Classic Neither Switched
**
%
o
f
m
R
u
b
y2
+
n
e
u
ro
n
s
100
80
40
0
60
20
Ctrl Run Ctrl Run Run Run
**
Co-expressing
a b
c
AAV-hSyn-DIO-mRuby2
4-wk virus expression
ChAT-IRES-Cre
1-wk run
Start
running
GABA-IHC
ChAT-IHC
GABA mRuby2ChAT
p = 0.99
MERGE
Fig. 3 A subset of cholinergic cPPN neurons lose ChAT and
gain GABA. a Experimental strategy to permanently label
cholinergic cPPN neurons with
mRuby2 fluorescent protein by expressing Cre-dependent AAV-
DIO-mRuby2 in ChAT-Cre mice and determine whether the loss
of ChAT and gain of
GABA occur in mRuby2+ neurons. The coronal brain section
was drawn according to the Franklin & Paxinos brain atlas57. b
Double immunostaining of
GABA and ChAT in the cPPN of a control and a 1-week runner
ChAT-Cre mouse expressing AAV-DIO-mRuby2 in the cPPN.
Purple arrows, mRuby2
neurons that express ChAT but not GABA (classic ChAT
neurons). Gray arrows, mRuby2 neurons that express both
ChAT and GABA (co-expressing
neurons). Yellow arrows, mRuby2 neurons that express GABA
but not ChAT (switched neurons). Cyan arrow, a mRuby2
neuron that expresses neither
GABA nor ChAT. Scale bar, 50 μm. c The percentage of each
class of mRuby2+ neurons (n = 844 for controls and 738 cells
for runners). For b, c, n = 3
animals per group. Statistical significance *p < 0.05, **p < 0.01
was assessed by Mann–Whitney U test. Data shown are mean ±
SEM.
NATURE COMMUNICATIONS |
https://doi.org/10.1038/s41467-020-16053-7 ARTICLE
NATURE COMMUNICATIONS | (2020) 11:2195 |
https://doi.org/10.1038/s41467-020-16053-7 |
www.nature.com/naturecommunications 5
www.nature.com/naturecommunications
www.nature.com/naturecommunications
disappearance and de novo appearance of transcripts of trans-
mitter synthetic enzymes.
Switching neurons project to the SN, VTA, and thalamus. To
further test the link between neurotransmitter switching and
motor skill learning, we identified targets innervated by the
switching cholinergic neurons. PPN neurons project to the sub-
stantia nigra (SN), the ventral tegmental area (VTA) and the
ventrolateral-ventromedial nuclei of the thalamus (VL-VM), all
of
which regulate motor skill learning36–41. Anterograde tracing
of
mRuby2 in ChAT-Cre neurons (see Methods) followed by ret-
rograde tracing with retrobeads demonstrated synaptic connec-
tions between cholinergic cPPN neurons and neurons in the SN,
VTA and VL-VM (Fig. 5a–c and Supplementary Fig. 7a–c).
cPPN
neurons originating projections to all three targets had lower
mean numbers of ChAT transcript puncta in runners compared
to controls, as demonstrated by triple labeling of retrograde
beads, ChAT transcripts and nNOS protein (Fig. 5d–f and Sup-
plementary Fig. 7d, e). As noted above, nNOS neurons
expressing
<8 puncta appear to be those lacking ChAT immunoreactivity.
In
controls, the percentages of neurons that express less than 8
ChAT transcript puncta and project to the SN, VTA, and VL-
VM
were 11%, 8 and 6%, and increased to 42%, 11 and 19% in
runners. Neurons in the increased percentages of cells
expressing
low numbers of ChAT-transcript puncta are likely to include
those that switch transmitters. The greater increase in number of
the low-transcript neurons projecting to the SN (37 for Run vs.
9
for Ctrl) suggests that cPPN neurons switching transmitters
make
a major projection to the SN with smaller projections to the
VTA
(10 for Run vs. 7 for Ctrl) and VL-VM (15 for Run vs. 5 for
Ctrl)
(Fig. 5f).
To determine whether running up-regulates the level of
vesicular GABA transporter at the cholinergic cPPN terminals
in the SN, we injected an AAV vector expressing Cre-dependent
synaptophysin fused with EGFP (AAV8-phSyn1(S)-FLEX-tdTo-
mato-T2A-SypEGFP-WPRE) into the cPPN of ChAT-Cre mice
to label the cholinergic cPPN terminals. Immunostaining of the
cPPN with anti-tdTomato and anti-ChAT antibodies demon-
strated expression of the construct in the cholinergic cPPN
neurons (Supplementary Fig. 7f). Immunostaining the SN with
anti-VGAT or anti-VAChT antibodies identified an increase in
the number of EGFP+ putative terminals labeled with VGAT
and
a decrease in those labeled with VAChT in runners compared to
control mice (Fig. 5g–j). These results suggest that the ACh-to-
GABA switch observed in cPPN neuronal cell bodies extends to
their presynaptic terminals.
Enhanced motor skill learning requires transmitter switch. To
determine whether neurotransmitter switching is necessary for
the beneficial effect of running on motor skill learning, we
injected AAV-DIO-ChAT into the cPPN of ChAT-Cre mice to
continuously express ChAT in all cholinergic neurons (Fig. 6a,
b
and Supplementary Fig. 8a–d). The ChAT-Cre line30,31 demon-
strated the same running-dependent transmitter switch as wild-
type mice, even with expression of control constructs (AAV-
DIO-
mRuby2, Fig. 6c; AAV-DIO-shScr, Fig. 7b). Overexpression of
ChAT did not change the number of ChAT+ neurons in the
cPPN of control ChAT-Cre mice and maintained the number of
ChAT+ neurons at control levels after sustained running
(Fig. 6c). Although the overexpression of ChAT caused a 3-fold
increase in the level of ChAT expression in control mice (Fig.
6d
and Supplementary Fig. 8c), it did not affect their basal motor
skill learning (Fig. 6f, g and Supplementary Fig. 9e, f) or
running
activity in runner mice (Fig. 6e and Supplementary Fig. 9a, b).
This may result from feedback inhibition of ChAT by ACh42,43
that is likely to maintain ACh levels of the non-switching
neurons
Ctrl Run
e
Ctrl Run
g
10
15
20
0
5
P
u
n
ct
a
n
u
m
b
e
r
/n
e
u
ro
n
GAD1
***
ChAT-ISH GAD1-ISH
ChAT-ISH GAD1-ISH
nNOS-IHC
nNOS-IHC
Run
Ctrl
d
a b c
p = 0.94
nNOS ChAT
N
u
m
b
e
r
o
f
n
N
O
S
+
n
e
u
ro
n
s
0
2000
3000
1000
ChAT
86%
ChAT + nNOS
3% 11%
nNOS
≥105
GAD1 transcript puncta number
T
C
h
A
t
ra
n
sc
ri
p
t
p
u
n
ct
a
n
u
m
b
e
r
0
10
20
30
≥40
0 2 3 4 6 7 8 91
Ctrl Run
f
P
u
n
ct
a
n
u
m
b
e
r
/n
e
u
ro
n
0
20
40
60
ChAT
Ctrl Run
***
80
MERGE
MERGE
MERGE
MERGE-ZOOM
MERGE-ZOOM
Fig. 4 A subset of cPPN nNOS neurons of runner mice lose
ChAT transcripts and gain GAD1 transcripts. a Double staining
of the cPPN in a non-runner
control mouse for nNOS (green) and ChAT (red). Scale bar, 100
μm. b Co-localization of nNOS and ChAT in the cPPN. For a, b,
n = 1068 cells from three
non-runner control mice. c Stereological counts of DAB
staining of nNOS in control and 1-week runner cPPNs. n = 6
animals per group. Mann–Whitney U
test. NS, not significant. d Triple staining of ChAT and GAD1
transcripts and nNOS protein in the cPPN of a control and a 1-
week runner mouse. Right
panels are boxed regions in merged images at higher
magnification. Scale bar, 20 μm. e Scatterplot of numbers of in
situ stained ChAT puncta (y-axis)
against numbers of in situ stained GAD1 puncta (x-axis). Each
dot represents one neuron. The vertical line divides neurons that
contain zero from those
containing more GAD1 transcript puncta and the horizontal line
divides neurons that contain less from those containing more
than eight ChAT transcript
puncta. f, g Y-axes are the mean number of ChAT (f) and GAD1
(g) fluorescent puncta in single nNOS+ neurons. For d–g, n = 4
animals per group; n = 123
cells for Ctrl and 137 cells for Run. Statistical significance *p <
0.05, ***p < 0.001 was assessed by two-sided Welch’s t-test.
Data shown are mean ± SEM.
ARTICLE NATURE COMMUNICATIONS |
https://doi.org/10.1038/s41467-020-16053-7
6 NATURE COMMUNICATIONS | (2020) 11:2195 |
https://doi.org/10.1038/s41467-020-16053-7 |
www.nature.com/naturecommunications
www.nature.com/naturecommunications
in the physiological range. Mice that had received AAV-DIO-
ChAT acquired the same running skill as wild-type mice or
ChAT-Cre mice injected with AAV-DIO-mRuby2 but their
motor learning on the rotarod and balance beam, tested directly
after 1 week of running, was not enhanced (Fig. 6f, g). The
slopes
of the learning curves for both rotarod and balance beam beha-
viors were significantly steeper for runner mice expressing
AAV-
DIO-mRuby2 (10 ± 2 rpm/trial and −3.9 ± 1.0 s/trial) than for
runners expressing the AAV-DIO-ChAT (6 ± 1 rpm/trial, −1.3 ±
0.4 s/trial; p = 0.037 and p = 0.035) and test performances of
runners expressing AAV-DIO-mRuby2 were significantly better
(Supplementary Fig. 9e, f). Overriding the loss of ChAT also
prevented enhancement of motor skill learning when mice ran,
were trained and tested, rested for one week and then re-tested
(Fig. 6f, g). This finding makes it unlikely that exogenous
expression of ChAT had simply delayed the improvement in
motor skill learning.
Expression of shRNA for GAD1 (AAV-DIO-shGAD1) to
suppress the gain of GAD67 in cholinergic cPPN neurons
similarly prevented improved motor learning following one
week
of running, compared to mice expressing a Cre-dependent
scrambled shRNA sequence (AAV-DIO-shScr; Fig. 7a–f,
Supple-
mentary Figs. 8 and 9). Knockdown of GAD1 maintained the
number of GAD1+ neurons at control levels in the cPPN after
sustained running (Fig. 7b). Because immunostaining of GAD67
in the PPN detects a large number of synaptic puncta that makes
Ctrl Run
0.50
0.75
1.00
0
0.25
VGAT
**
Beads
Beads
SN
SN
cPPN
ChAT-IHC
Beads ChAT
Beads
SN
cPPN
ChAT ISH
nNOS IHC
a b c
d e f
C
o
n
tr
o
l
R
u
n
n
e
r
** *** ***
C
h
A
T
m
R
N
A
p
u
n
ct
a
n
u
m
b
e
r
0
20
40
60
VTA SN VL-VM
Ctrl Run Ctrl Run Ctrl Run
Beads Merge & nNOSChAT
VTA
75%
17%
1%
7%
Neither
Both
Ipsi
Contra
9.8%
SN
0.2%
30%
Ipsi
Both
Contra
60%
Neither
VL-VM
60%
28%
4%
Contra
Both
8%
Neither
Ipsi
ig
AAV-FLEX-SypEGFP-T2A-TdTomato
SN
cPPN
EGFP IHC
VAChT or VGAT IHC
R
u
n
synaptophysin-EGFP MERGEVAChT
C
tr
l
MERGEVGAT synaptophysin-EGFP
R
u
n
C
tr
l
h
0
25
75
100
VAChT
Ctrl Run
*
%
o
f
p
u
ta
tiv
e
t
e
rm
in
a
ls
Ctrl Run
50
75
100
0
25
VGAT
**
0
0.50
0.75
1.00
VAChT
Ctrl Run
*
C
o
lo
ca
liz
a
tio
n
c
o
e
ffi
ci
e
n
t
M
1
j
50
0.25
Fig. 5 Running reduces the number of ChAT transcripts in a
subset of cholinergic cPPN neurons that project to the VTA, SN
and VL-VM. a
Experimental design to validate cholinergic innervation of
target nuclei by the cPPN. Red or green retrobeads (beads) were
injected bilaterally into target
nuclei with one color per side. Substantia nigra (SN) provides
an example. b Triple labeling of retrobeads and ChAT in a
coronal section of cPPN in a mouse
injected with retrobeads. Scale bar, 50 μm. c Summary of the
percentage of cholinergic neurons (ChAT+) that project to
corresponding nuclei. Ipsi,
ipsilaterally. Contra, contralaterally. Both, both ipsi - and
contralaterally. Neither, no retrobeads. n = 3 animals examined
per region. n = 829 cells for the
VTA, 806 cells for the SN, and 812 for the VL-VM. d
Experimental design to identify the target(s) of neurons with
decreased numbers of ChAT transcripts.
SN is shown as an example. e Triple-labeling of retrobeads,
ChAT mRNA transcripts, and nNOS proteins in both control and
runner cPPNs. Scale bar,
20 μm. f Y-axis is the mean number of ChAT fluorescent puncta
in single nNOS+ cells. Each dot represents one neuron. n = 4
animals per group. n = 89
cells for VTA-Ctrl, 91 for VTA-Run, 81 for SN-Ctrl, 87 for SN-
Run, 82 for VL-VM- Ctrl, 80 for VL-VM-Run. g Experimental
design to identify presynaptic
changes in transporter expression of cholinergic cPPN axons in
the SN. AAV8-phSyn1-FLEX-tdTomato-T2A-Syptophysin-
EGFP-WPRE (AAV-FLEX-
SypEGFP-T2A-TdTomato) was injected into the cPPN of ChAT-
Cre mice. Coronal sections containing the SN were
immunostained for EGFP and VAChT or
EGFP and VGAT. h Double immunostaining of VAChT and
EGFP or VGAT and EGFP in the SN of non-runner controls or
1-week runners injected with AAV-
FLEX-SypEGFP-T2A-TdTomato. Scale bar, 10 μm. i Percentage
of VAChT+ or VGAT+ terminals in putatively cholinergic
terminals in the SN. j The
coefficient M1 of antibody colocalization with putative
cholinergic presynaptic terminals. n = 10139 EGFP+ terminals
for VAChT-Ctrl, 9629 for VAChT-
Run, 10174 for VGAT-Ctrl and 10611 for VGAT-Run. For each
group, data were quantified from 12 z-stack images from three
animals. Statistical significance
*p < 0.05, **p < 0.01, ***p < 0.001 was assessed by two-sided
Welch’s t-test. Data shown are mean ± SEM.
NATURE COMMUNICATIONS |
https://doi.org/10.1038/s41467-020-16053-7 ARTICLE
NATURE COMMUNICATIONS | (2020) 11:2195 |
https://doi.org/10.1038/s41467-020-16053-7 |
www.nature.com/naturecommunications 7
www.nature.com/naturecommunications
www.nature.com/naturecommunications
it challenging to analyze cell bodies, the knockdown efficiency
of
GAD67 expression by shGAD1 was tested in the motor cortex
and measured to be 65% (Supplementary Fig. 8e, f). Moreover,
shGAD1 blocked the increase in the expression of GABA in
cholinergic cPPN neurons of runner mice (Fig. 7c), indicating
that GAD1 has the dominant role in regulating the level of
GABA
in these neurons and that shGAD1 effectively prevents the gain
in
GABA caused by running. Suppressing the gain of GAD1 did
not
affect acquisition of the wheel running skill (Fig. 7d and
Supplementary Fig. 9c, d) but motor learning on the rotarod
and balance beam, tested directly after one week of running,
was
not enhanced for a period that was extended to one week of rest
(Fig. 7e,f). The slopes of the learning curves were again steeper
for
runner mice expressing AAV-DIO-shScr (9 ± 1 rpm/trial, −6.7 ±
0.9 s/trial) than for runners expressing AAV-DIO-shGAD1 (5 ±
2 rpm/trial, −3.7 ± 1.0 s/trial; p = 0.073 and p = 0.041) and test
performances for runners expressing AAV-DIO-shScr were
again
significantly better (Supplementary Fig. 9g, h). These results
suggest that both the loss of ACh and gain of GABA are
required
for enhancement of motor skill learning. Overriding the loss of
ChAT or suppressing the gain of GAD1 did not affect running-
induced c-fos expression in cholinergic cPPN neurons (Supple-
mentary Fig. 10). We tested for population-level control of the
change in expression of one transmitter by overriding the
change
in expression of the other. Notably, suppressing the gain in
GAD1
expression did not affect the loss of ChAT expression and vice
versa (Fig. 7g). Although expression of these two transmitter
synthetic enzymes is inversely correlated, the two are not
reciprocally regulated.
Discussion
Our findings provide insight into the mechanism by which sus-
tained running improves acquisition of motor skills (Fig. 8).
The functional significance of ACh-to-GABA transmitter
switching is demonstrated by changes in behavior that are
reversed by overriding the switch. Switching involves changes
in
levels of transcripts of transmitter synthetic enzymes. Activity-
dependent transcription factor phosphorylation18,19 and micro-
RNA regulation44, which have been implicated in transmitter
switching in the developing nervous system, are candidates for
implementing the switch in the adult CNS. Transmitter
switching
in the cPPN, particularly when it may change the sign of the
synapse from excitatory to inhibitory, appears to rewire motor
circuitry to enhance motor skills. Acquisition of motor skills
requires many components including balance, coordination,
Balance beam
T
im
e
t
o
c
ro
ss
(
s)
Immediate test Retest
Rotarod
Immediate test Retest
80
S
p
e
e
d
a
t
fa
ll
(R
P
M
)
60
0
20
40
e f g
cPPN neuron counts
a c
Wheel running
d
1-wk rest
Immediate test
4-wk virus expression
ChAT-IRES-Cre
1-wk run
Start running
AAV-hSyn-DIO-ChAT-flag-P2A-mRuby2
or AAV-hSyn-DIO-mRuby2
Retest
ChAT-Run
ChAT-Ctrl
mRuby2-Run
mRuby2-Ctrl
b
mRuby2
nNOS
LDT LDTPPN PPN
bregma -4.8
AQ
mRuby2 DRAQ5
C
h
A
T
+
in
te
n
si
ty
r
e
la
tiv
e
t
o
n
o
n
-t
ra
n
sf
e
ct
e
d
c
e
lls
(
%
)
m
Ru
by
2
Ch
AT
800
600
400
200
0
***
3000
2000
0
C
h
A
T
+
n
e
u
ro
n
n
u
m
b
e
r
*** *
1000
Day
S
p
e
e
d
(
km
/h
)
ChAT
mRuby2
1 2 3 4 5 6 7
2.0
1.5
1.0
0.5
0.0
0
30
10
20
50
40
** *** * *
* ** * **
p = 0.73 p = 0.95
p = 0.99 p = 0.99 p = 0.96 p = 0.93
p = 0.94 p = 0.97 p = 0.99 p = 0.99
p = 0.86
Fig. 6 Loss of ChAT in cholinergic cPPN neurons is necessary
for running-enhanced motor skill learning. a Experimental
design to override the loss of
ChAT in ChAT-Cre neurons and determine behavioral
relevance. The coronal brain section was drawn according to the
Franklin and Paxinos brain atlas57.
b Top: mRuby2 and nuclear marker DRAQ-5 show mRuby2
expression in a coronal cPPN section of an animal bilaterally
injected with AAV-DIO-ChAT-
P2A-mRuby2 constructs. Coordinates adapted to Allen Brain
Atlas. AQ, aqueduct. LDT, laterodorsal tegmental nucleus.
Scale bar, 500 μm. Bottom:
double-labeled image of nNOS and mRuby2 in boxed region of
the upper image. Scale bar, 200 μm. n = 34 animals. c ChAT+
neuron number in the cPPN
of runners and non-running controls that were injected with
AAV-DIO- mRuby2 or AAV-DIO-ChAT. n = 5 animals per
group. Nonparametric
Kruskal–Wallis test followed by Dunn’s correction. d ChAT
fluorescence intensity for mRuby2-expressing cells in ChAT-
Cre mice injected with AAV-DIO-
ChAT or AAV-DIO-mRuby2. Fluorescence intensity was
normalized by non-transfected cells (mRuby2 negative). n = 56
and 58 cells for mRuby2 and
ChAT. n = 3 animals per group. Welch’s t-test. e Wheel running
speed of mice that were injected with AAV-DIO-ChAT or AAV-
DIO-mRuby2 at the cPPN.
n = 10 animals per group. Linear regression followed by two-
sided Welch’s t-test. f, g Rotarod (f) and balance beam (g, 4 mm
rod) tests show that
expression of mRuby2 did not affect enhancement of acquisition
and maintenance of motor skills gained by running (mRuby2-
Run vs. mRuby2-Ctrl),
whereas exogenous ChAT expression blocked the enhancement
(ChAT-Run vs. mRuby2-Run). The blockade was sustained after
rest for 1 week. For
immediate rotarod test, numbers of animals are 9 for mRuby2-
Ctrl, 16 for ChAT-Ctrl, 9 for mRuby2-Run, 18 for ChAT-Run.
For rotarod retest, numbers of
animals are 9 for mRuby2-Ctrl, 6 for ChAT-Ctrl, 9 for mRuby2-
Run, 7 for ChAT-Run. For immediate test of balance beam,
numbers of animals are 10 for
mRuby2-Ctrl, 9 for ChAT-Ctrl, 10 for mRuby2-Run, 9 for
ChAT-Run. For balance beam retest, numbers of animals are 6
for mRuby2-Ctrl, ChAT-Ctrl,
mRuby2-Run and 7 for ChAT-Run. ANOVA followed by
Tukey’s test. *p < 0.05, **p < 0.01, ***p < 0.001. Data shown
are mean ± SEM.
ARTICLE NATURE COMMUNICATIONS |
https://doi.org/10.1038/s41467-020-16053-7
8 NATURE COMMUNICATIONS | (2020) 11:2195 |
https://doi.org/10.1038/s41467-020-16053-7 |
www.nature.com/naturecommunications
www.nature.com/naturecommunications
motivation, attention and muscle strength. Interestingly, the
PPN
regulates many of them23,45, and transmitter switching in the
cPPN may contribute to motor skill learning through regulating
one, several or even all of these aspects. The persistence of
learned
behaviors after the transmitter switch has reversed implies that
there is continued capacity for plasticity in locomotor circuitry.
We examined three targets of the cPPN and found that cho-
linergic cPPN neurons projecting to each of the three targets
showed a significant reduction of ChAT transcripts (Fig. 5).
This
suggests that the reduction of ChAT expression might be
observed in cholinergic cPPN neurons projecting to other
targets
as well. Multi-target regulation is not surprising because choli-
nergic PPN neurons have an average of five axonal collaterals
that
allow a single cholinergic neuron to project to many targets23.
Cholinergic cPPN neurons may regulate motor function and
other behaviors by gating the activity of downstream targets
through transmitter switching.
Dopaminergic and noradrenergic neurons are implicated in
fine motor function plasticity that regulates paw and digit
movements46–49. Our results unexpectedly indicate that
acquisi-
tion of high-demand rotarod and balance beam performance
requires transmitter switching in cholinergic cPPN neurons.
Overriding transmitter switching did not affect the ability to run
on a running wheel (Figs. 6 and 7), perhaps because running is
not an exceptionally taxing motor skill. Consistent with these
results, selective lesions of cholinergic PPN neurons impair
b Wheel runninga
1-wk rest
AAV-CAG-DIO-shGAD1 -EGFP
or AAV-CAG-DIO-shScr-EGFP
Immediate test
4-wk virus expression
ChAT-IRES-Cre
1-wk run
Start running Retest
d
e f g
3000
2000
0
N
e
u
ro
n
n
u
m
b
e
r **
ChAT+
shGAD1-RunChAT-Run
ChAT-Ctrl shGAD1-Ctrl
GAD1+
T
im
e
t
o
c
ro
ss
(
s)
Immediate test Retest
40
10
20
30
0
50
Balance beam
S
p
e
e
d
a
t
fa
ll
(R
P
M
)
20
40
0
60
80
Rotarod
Immediate test Retest
cPPN neuron counts
*
2000
3000
0
G
A
D
1
+
n
e
u
ro
n
n
u
m
b
e
r
1000
S
p
e
e
d
(
km
/h
)
shGAD1
shScr
Day
1000
2.0
1.5
1.0
0.5
0.0
1 2 3 4 5 6 7
10
0
40
50A
T
-
G
A
B
A
+
E
G
F
P
+
C
h
n
e
u
ro
n
n
u
m
b
e
r
(%
E
G
F
P
)
20
cPPN neuron
counts
cPPN neuron
counts
c
shGAD1-Run
shGAD1-Ctrl
shScr-Run
shScr-Ctrl
30
*** ***
p = 0.20 p = 0.75
*** ***
p = 0.95 p = 0.93 p = 0.28
*** *
p = 0.12 p = 0.99
** *
p = 0.87 p = 0.97
* **
p = 0.98 p = 0.79
** *
p = 0.59 p = 0.96
Fig. 7 Gain of GAD1 in cholinergic cPPN neurons is also
necessary for running-enhanced motor skill learning. a
Experimental design to override the gain
of GAD1 in ChAT-Cre neurons and examine behavioral
relevance. The coronal brain section was drawn according to the
Franklin and Paxinos brain atlas57.
b Numbers of GAD1 in situ stained neurons in the cPPNs of
runners and non-running controls that were injected with AAV-
DIO-shScr (scramble shRNA)
or AAV-DIO-shGAD1 (shRNA for GAD1). n = 7 for shGAD1-
Run, and five animals for the other three groups. Nonparametric
Kruskal–Wallis test followed
by Dunn’s correction. c Percentage of neurons immunostained
as GABA+, EGFP+ and ChAT- in the total EGFP+ neurons in
cPPNs of ChAT-Cre runners
and non-running controls that were injected with AAV-DIO-
shScr or AAV-DIO-shGAD1. n = 1243 cells for shScr-Ctrl,
1258 for shScr-Run, 957 for shGAD1-
Ctrl, and 1226 for shGAD1-Run from three animals per group.
Nonparametric Kruskal–Wallis test followed by Dunn’s
correction. d Wheel running speed of
ChAT-Cre mice that express AAV-DIO-shScr or AAV-DIO-
shGAD1 in the cPPN. n = 8 animals per group. Two-sided
Welch’s t-test. e, f Rotarod (e) and
balance beam (f, 4-mm rod) tests show that expression of shScr
did not affect the enhancement of acquisition and maintenance
of motor skills gained by
running (shScr-Ctrl vs. shScr-Run) whereas knocking down
GAD1 blocked the enhancement (shGAD1-Run vs. shScr-Run).
Blockade was sustained after
rest for 1 week. For e, animal numbers are 9 for shScr-Ctrl, 7
for shGAD1-Ctrl, 9 for shScr-Run and 9 for shGAD1-Run for
immediate test and 8 for shScr-
Ctrl, 7 for shGAD1-Ctrl, 8 for shScr-Run and 9 for shGAD1-
Run for retest. For f, numbers of animals are 8 for shScr -Ctrl, 7
for shGAD1-Ctrl, 8 for shScr-Run,
and 9 for shGAD1-Run for both immediate test and retest. g
Numbers of GAD1 in situ stained neurons in the cPPN of 1-
week runners and non-runner
controls that express AAV-DIO-ChAT and numbers of ChAT
immunostained neurons in 1-week runners and non-runner
controls that express AAV-DIO-
shGAD1. n = 6 animals per group for ChAT-Run and ChAT-Ctrl
and 5 for shGAD1-Run and shGAD1-Ctrl. Mann–Whitney U
test. *p < 0.05, **p < 0.01,
***p < 0.001. Data shown are mean ± SEM.
RunnerNaive
cPPN
Targets
cPPN
Targets
Basic
motor skill
learning
Runner with transmitter switching overridden
or
a b
c
ChAT expression
cPPN
Targets
Enhanced
motor skill
learning
cPPN
Targets
Basic
motor skill
learning nNOS
ChAT
GAD1
ChAT and GAD1
GAD1 knockdown
Fig. 8 Transmitter switching in the cPPN regulates motor skill
learning. a,
b Chronic running induces neurotransmitter switching from ACh
to GABA
in the cPPN and enhances motor skill learning. c Both ChAT
loss and GAD1
gain are necessary for running-enhanced motor skill learning.
NATURE COMMUNICATIONS |
https://doi.org/10.1038/s41467-020-16053-7 ARTICLE
NATURE COMMUNICATIONS | (2020) 11:2195 |
https://doi.org/10.1038/s41467-020-16053-7 |
www.nature.com/naturecommunications 9
www.nature.com/naturecommunications
www.nature.com/naturecommunications
learning of high-demand running on an accelerating-speed
rotarod but do not affect learning of low-demand running on a
fixed-speed rotarod or basal locomotion50. In contrast, glutama-
tergic and GABAergic PPN neurons regulate gait and speed of
locomotion51–53.
Control of voluntary movement and procedural learning are
core functions of the basal ganglia. SNr inhibitory neurons
serve
as the major output of the basal ganglia and project to the PPN;
in
turn, PPN ascending axons project to the SNc and SNr to
modulate the function of the basal ganglia23. We propose a
model
in which conversion of cPPN excitatory cholinergic input to
inhibitory GABAergic input of inhibitory neurons in the SN
enables feedback control of SN neurons regulating motor coor-
dination and skill learning, as supported by their decrease in c -
fos
expression (Fig. 2c). Gait speed, stride and balance of
Parkinson’s
disease and stroke patients are improved following sustained
treadmill training54–56. Our finding that transmitter switching
after running is a critical event for improving motor skill
learning
suggests that transmitter switching may be important in many
circumstances where sustained exercise benefits behavior.
Methods
Mice. All animal procedures were carried out in accordance
with NIH guidelines
and approved by the University of California, San Diego
Institutional Animal Care
and Use Committee or Scripps Institutional Animal Care and
Use Committee.
C57BL/6J (JAX#000664) mice were obtained from Jackson
Laboratories. ChAT-
IRES-Cre (JAX#006410) mice were obtained from the
Byungkook Lim lab and
Jackson Laboratories. PV-IRES-Cre (JAX#008069) mice were
provided by the
Stefan Leutgeb lab. Animals were maintained on a 12 h:12 h
light:dark cycle (light
on: 10:00 pm–10:00 am) with food and water ad libitum.
Vivarium temperature
was between 65 and 75 °F (~18 and 23 °C) with 40–60%
humidity. The ChAT-
IRES-Cre colony was maintained by breeding homozygous male
ChAT-Cre mice
with female wild-type C57BL/6J mice. Heterozygous ChAT-Cre
offspring were
used in the study. Both heterozygous and homozygous PV-
IRES-Cre mice were
used. All experiments were performed on 8- to 12-week-old
male mice.
Wheel running. Mice were single-housed in hamster cages and
provided with
FastTrac (Bio-serv, K3250) or digital (Med Associates ENV-
044) running wheels
that are identical in shape and size. Mice were allowed
voluntary running for one
week and were continuously recorded with Swann DVR4-2600
infrared video
cameras. Control mice were housed with running wheelbases
without wheels.
Episode duration and time with the wheel were hand-scored
using JWatcher
software. The maximum angular excursion of mouse movements
on the running
wheels was plotted using Image J and measured with a
protractor. The digital
running wheels recorded running distance and running speed
was calculated as
distance divided by time.
Rotarod. Training and tests were performed as the rotarod (Ugo
Basile, 57624)
accelerated from 5 rpm to 80 rpm in 6 min. The rpm at which
mice fell off was
recorded by the rotarod. Mice were trained for nine trials on the
first day, followed
by three tests the next day and three retests after 1, 2, or 4-
weeks rest. There was a
10-min interval between each trial/test. The single digit
accuracy of the slopes of
learning curves reflects the accuracy of recording the rpm at fall
by the rotarod.
Balance beam. Mice were trained and tested with balance beams
1 meter long and
0.75 meters above the floor. Twelve millimeters and 6 mm
square beams (S-12 and
S-6) were single plastic horizontal bars while 6 and 4 mm rod
beams (R-6 and R-4)
consisted of two stainless steel parallel bars 5 cm apart. On the
training day mice
were trained to cross S-12 for three trials, S-6 for three trials,
R-6 for three trials
and then R-4 for three trials, each 10 min apart. On the test and
retest day mice
were allowed to cross all four beams in the same order, three
times for each, 10 min
apart. A video camera was installed at the end of the beam
where mice started
walking. At the other end of the beam, a black box with an entry
facing the beam
was installed to attract mice to cross the beams. Time to cross
the beam and enter
the escape box was hand-scored and the investigator was
double-blinded to the
history of the mice. The slopes of learning curves are accurate
to one decimal place
because the time to cross the beam was hand scored and human
response time is
0.1~0.2 s.
Motor skill learning. The mean speed at fall from a rotarod for
the nine trials on
the training day was fitted by a one-phase association model.
Mean data points for
each training trial were plotted and fitted using GraphPad Prism
7 software and the
coefficient of determination (R2) was used to justify the fit (R2
> 0.96 for controls
and R2 > 0.95 for runners, with MATLAB). The mean time to
cross a 4 mm rod
beam for the three trials on the training day was fitted by linear
regression (R2 >
0.98 for controls and R2 > 0.99 for runners, with MATLAB).
Locomotor activity. Mice were tested for 120 min in
polycarbonate cages (42 ×
22 × 20 cm) placed in frames (25.5 × 47 cm) mounted with two
levels of photocell
beams at 2 and 7 cm above the bottom of the cage (San Diego
Instruments, San
Diego, CA). The two sets of beams detect both horizontal
(roaming) and vertical
(rearing) behavior. A thin layer of bedding material covered the
bottom of the cage.
Data were collected in 1-min epochs.
Histology and immunocytochemistry. Animals were perfused
transcardially with
phosphate-buffered saline (PBS) followed by 4%
paraformaldehyde (PFA) in PBS
right after the last episode of running, i.e., mice started to run
at 10 am at light-off
and kept running until they were perfused between 2 and 3 pm.
Brains were
dissected and post-fixed in 4% PFA for 16 to 24 h at 4 °C,
washed in PBS for 1 min
and transferred to 30% sucrose in PBS for 2 days at 4 °C. Forty-
micrometers
coronal sections were cut on a microtome (Leica SM2010R) and
stained.
For immunostaining, sections were permeabilized and blocked
in 24-well
culture plates for 2 h in a blocking solution (5% normal horse
serum, 0.3% Triton
X-100 in PBS) at 22–24 °C. Primary and secondary antibodies
were diluted in the
blocking solution. Incubation with primary antibodies was
performed for 48 h on a
rotator at 4 °C. After washing in PBS (three times, 15 min
each), secondary
antibodies were added for 2 h at 22–24 °C. For
immunofluorescence, sections were
mounted with Fluoromount-G (Southern Biotech) or ProLong
Gold Antifade
Mountant (Life Technologies) containing DRAQ-5 (Thermo
Fisher, 62251, 1:1000
dilution; when nuclear staining was needed) after washes in
PBS (three times,
15 min each).
For DAB (3,3′-Diaminobenzidine) staining, sections were
treated with 0.3%
hydrogen peroxide for 30 min, washed in PBS (three times, 5
min each), incubated
with Vectastain Elite ABC HRP mixture (Vector Laboratories,
PK-6100) for
45 min, washed in PBS (three times, 15 min each), and signals
were developed
using the DAB Peroxidase Substrate kit (Vector Laboratories,
SK-4100).
Primary antibodies used in this study were goat anti-ChAT
(Millipore, AB144P,
1:500), rabbit-anti-nNOS (Thermo Fisher, 61-7000, 1:500),
goat-anti-cFos (Santa
Cruz, sc-52G, 1:300), rabbit-anti-cFos (Santa Cruz, sc-52,
1:300), mouse-anti-cFos
(Abcam, ab208942, 1:500), rabbit-anti-PV (Swant, PV25,
1:2000), goat-anti-
VAChT (Millipore, ABN100, 1:500), goat-anti-VGAT (Synaptic
system, 131004,
1:1000), mouse-anti-NeuN (Millipore, MAB377, 1:500), rabbit-
anti-GABA (Sigma-
Aldrich, A2052, 1:1000) rabbit-anti-GFP (Thermo Fisher,
A11122, 1:1000), chicken
anti-GFP (Abcam, ab13970, 1:1000), guinea pig anti-GFP
(Synaptic Systems,
132005, 1:3000), rabbit-anti-zsGreen (Takara, 632474, 1:500),
goat-anti-
doublecortin (Santa Cruz, sc-8066, 1:300) and rabbit-anti-Ki67
(Cell Signaling,
9129, 1:300). Secondary antibodies for immunofluorescence
were from Jackson
ImmunoResearch Labs and used at a concentration of 1:600:
Alexa Fluor-488
donkey-anti-rabbit (705-545-003), Alexa Fluor-488 donkey-
anti-guinea pig (706-
545-148), Alexa Fluor-488 donkey-anti-mouse (715-545-150),
Alexa Fluor-488
donkey-anti-goat (705-545-147), Alexa Fluor-594 donkey-anti-
goat (705-585-147),
Alexa Fluor-594 donkey-anti-mouse (715-585-150), Alexa
Fluor-647 donkey-anti-
goat (705-605-147) and Alexa Fluor-647 donkey-anti-rabbit
(711-605-152).
Biotinylated goat anti-rabbit (BA-1000) and horse anti-goat
(BA-9500) secondary
antibodies for DAB staining were from Vector Laboratories and
used at a
concentration of 1:300.
In situ hybridization PFA fixed brains were dissected, post-
fixed in 4% PFA for
16 to 24 h at 4 °C and transferred to 30% DEPC-sucrose in PBS
for 2 days.
Subsequently, brains were embedded in 30% DEPC-sucrose and
frozen with dry
ice. Forty-micrometers cryosections were collected on
Superfrost Plus slides (VWR,
48311-703) and used for mRNA in situ hybridization.
Complementary DNAs
(cDNAs) of gad1 or slc17a6 (sequences from Allen Brain Atlas:
gad1,
RP_040324_01_F01; slc17a6, RP_050921_01_E03) were cloned
from mouse cDNA
library in ~800-base-pair segments into a pGEM vector and
sequenced to verify
that they were correct. Antisense complementary RNA (cRNA)
probes were
synthesized with T7 (Promega, P2075) or Sp6 polymerases
(Promega, P1085) and
labeled with digoxigenin (Roche, 11175025910). Hybridization
was performed with
1 to 5 μg/ml cRNA probes at 65 °C for 20 to 24 h. Probes were
detected using Anti-
Digoxigenin-AP Fab fragments (Roche, 11093274910, 1:5000).
Signals were
developed for 8 h using a mixture of 4-Nitro blue tetrazolium
chloride (Roche,
11383213001) and BCIP 4-toluidine salt solution (Roche,
11383221001). We
counted all GAD1+ neurons in the cPPN, including those that
looked intense,
moderate or faint.
Fluorescent RNAscope in situ hybridization was performed
according to the
manufacturer’s instructions (Advanced Cell Diagnostics) with
some modifications:
In an RNase-free environment, 12-μm fixed brain sections were
mounted on
Superfrost Plus slides immediately after microtome sectioning
and air-dried in a
60 °C oven for 30 min. Sections were rehydrated in PBS for 2
min and incubated for
5 min in 1× target retrieval solution at 95 °C. Sections were
then rinsed with
distilled water for 5 s and rinsed in 100% ethanol for 5 seconds.
After air-drying,
sections were incubated with the following solutions in a
HybEZ humidified oven
at 40 °C with three rinsing steps in between each: protease III,
30 min; probes, 2 h;
amplification (Amp) 1-fluorescence (FL), 30 min; Amp 2-FL,
15 min; Amp 3-FL,
30 min; and Amp 4-FL, 15 min. Ready-to use Amp 1-FL, Amp
2-FL, Amp 3-FL,
ARTICLE NATURE COMMUNICATIONS |
https://doi.org/10.1038/s41467-020-16053-7
10 NATURE COMMUNICATIONS | (2020) 11:2195 |
https://doi.org/10.1038/s41467-020-16053-7 |
www.nature.com/naturecommunications
www.nature.com/naturecommunications
Amp 4-FL and 50× washing solution for rinsing steps were
included in the
RNAscope Multiplex Fluorescent Reagent kit. Standard
immunofluorescent
staining was subsequently performed in the dark15. Probes for
mouse chat mRNA
(Catalog number: 408731-C2) and gad1 mRNA (Catalog
number: 400951) were
from Advanced Cell Diagnostics. Sections were 6–7 μm thick
post-processing. Six
optical sections (1 µm Z step) of each physical section were
examined and regions
of interest (ROIs) were drawn around the boundaries of nNOS
cells on the optical
section that showed the best focal plane for each cell (largest
cross-sectional area).
These ROIs were scored for ChAT and GAD1 transcript puncta
using Image J. The
average area of ROIs was consistent between the control and
runner groups. We
included negative controls using a probe targeting a bacterial-
origin gene dapB
(Catalog number: 310043; GeneBank: EF191515) in each batch
of experiments and
validated that the negative control group showed <1 punctum in
100 cells.
Based on the expression of ChAT and GAD1 transcript puncta,
the neurons
were grouped into four categories (1) classic cholinergic
neurons: ≥8 ChAT
transcript puncta, no GAD1 transcript puncta; (2) co-expressing
neurons: ≥8
ChAT puncta, ≥1 GAD1 puncta); (3) neurons expressing
neither: <8 ChAT puncta,
no GAD1 puncta; (4) switched neurons: <8 ChAT puncta, ≥1
GAD1 puncta. We
are aware that other studies have arrived at a lower percent of
costaining than
reported here. The difference is likely to be caused by different
ways to detect and
score a positive neuron. Detection by IHC and ISH can be
greatly affected and is
ultimately determined by the sensitivity and thresholding of the
method. From the
distribution of GAD1 puncta number (Fig. 4e) we infer that
scoring only neurons
that contain 5 or more GAD1 puncta, would yield an incidence
of 4% of ChAT
neurons that co-express GAD1, which would be consistent with
percentages
previously reported33–35. We have not found descriptions of
counting thresholds in
other reports and believe that in most cases investigators count
neurons that
contain moderate to intense GAD1 and have regarded those with
a low level of
transcripts as negative.
Birthdating. Mice were intraperitoneally injected with BrdU (50
mg/kg) once every
12 h for 1 week. PFA-fixed brains were dissected, post-fixed
and dehydrated as
described above. Forty-micrometers cryosections were collected
and treated with
1 M HCl for 30 min at 45 °C for DNA denaturation. After
rinsing in PBS (three
times, 5 min each), sections were incubated in a mixture of
0.3% Triton X-100 and
5% horse serum in PBS for 1 h, incubated in rat-anti BrdU
antibody (AbD Serotec,
MCA2060, 1:300) at 4 °C overnight, rinsed in PBS (three times,
5 min each) and
amplified by Alexa Fluor-488 donkey anti-rat antibody (Life
Technologies, A21208,
1:600). Sections were mounted with Fluoromount containing
DRAQ-5 (1:1000).
TUNEL assay. The In Situ Cell Death Detection (TUNEL) Kit
with TMR Red
(Roche, 12156792910) was used to detect in situ apoptosis.
Forty-micrometers
cryosections were re-fixed with 1% PFA for 20 min at 22–24 °C
and rinsed with
PBS (three times, 5 min each). Sections were then
permeabilized in 0.1% sodium
citrate and 1% Triton X-100 for 1 h at 22–24 °C. After rinsing
in PBS (three times,
5 min each), sections were incubated with TUNEL reaction
solution according to
the vendor’s instruction, i.e., incubated in a mixture of 25 μL of
terminal-
deoxynucleotidyl transferase solution and 225 μL of label
solution. Incubation was
performed in a humidified chamber for 3 h at 37 °C in the dark.
Sections were
rinsed and mounted with Fluoromount containing DRAQ-5
(1:1000). For a
positive control, sections were treated with DNase I (10 U/mL,
New England
Biolabs, M0303S) for 1 h at 37 °C and rinsed in PBS (three
times, 5 min each),
followed by incubation with the TUNEL mixture.
Imaging and data analysis. Fluorescent images were acquired
with a Leica SP5
confocal microscope with a 25×/0.95 water-immersion objective
and a z resolution
of 1 μm. Leica Application Suite X software was used for
fluorescent cell counting
and all sections within the confocal stacks were examined
without maximal pro-
jection. RNAscope in situ signals were quantified using Image
J. Exemplar images
are maximum intensity projections of 6 consecutive confocal
sections when
RNAscope signals are included (Figs. 4d and 5e) and 16–24
confocal sections for
other immunofluorescence images. For the synaptophysin-EGFP
signals, images
were acquired with a 63×/0.90 water-immersion objective at
zoom 2 magnification
with a z resolution of 1 μm. Colocalization was quantified using
Image J with the
JACoP plugin. Exemplar images for DAB staining and in situ
hybridization were
acquired with a NanoZoomer Slide scanner (Hamamatsu S360)
with a 20×/0.75 air
objective. Counting DAB-stained c-fos neurons per unit area
was performed with
Image J on images harvested from the NanoZoomer. To delimit
the PPN in sec-
tions stained for c-fos (Supplementary Fig. 2a, b), or for
TUNEL and Ki67/DCX
(apoptosis and neurogenesis, Supplementary Fig. 3a–f), an
adjacent section was
stained for ChAT to define the boundaries of the PPN.
Images were analyzed exhaustively or by sampling. Exhaustive
analysis entailed
scoring all neurons in every other section through the entire
brain region of interest
and reporting the number of cells for the number of mice
examined. Sampling
involved scoring and reporting a specific number of cells from a
specified number
of sections from a particular number of mice. In these cases,
exact cell and/or
section numbers are shown in the figure legends.
Stereological counting. Stereo Investigator software (MBF
Bioscience) was used to
exhaustively count DAB immunostained or in situ
hybridization-stained cells.
Counting was carried out using optical fractionator sampling on
a Zeiss Axioskop 2
microscope (40×/0.65 Ph2 objective) equipped with a motorized
stage. The
population of midbrain PPN neurons was outlined on the basis
of ChAT immu-
nolabeling, with reference to a coronal atlas of the mouse
brain57 and anatomical
landmarks such as fiber tracts. Caudal PPN refers to the caudal
half of PPN with
reference to a transverse line normal to and halfway along the
rostrocaudal axis;
rostral PPN refers to the rostral half. To count GABAergic and
glutamatergic
neurons in the sections stained by in situ hybridization, an
adjacent section was
stained for ChAT to define the ROI for the boundaries of PPN.
The area of the ROI
and the cell density were compared between the control and
experimental groups
to ensure that the counted areas were comparable between the
two groups and that
changes in cell density were consistent with changes in the
absolute counts. Pilot
experiments with continuous counting determined that counting
every other sec-
tion was sufficient to estimate the number of cholinergic,
GABAergic, glutama-
tergic and nitroxidergic neurons in the cPPN of each brain.
Consequently, five to
six sections were counted for each mouse brain. The average
section thickness was
measured prior to counting. Sections shrank from 40 μm to 22–
24 μm after
staining and dehydration. Exhaustive counting was performed
and no sampling
grid was skipped because the distribution of neurons in the
cPPN is uneven.
Counting was performed by two investigators double-blinded to
the origin of
the sections.
Viral constructs and injection. Seven to eight-week-old male
mice were anesthe-
tized with a mixture of 120 mg/kg ketamine and 16 mg/kg
xylazine and head-fixed on
a stereotaxic apparatus (David Kopf Instruments Model 1900)
for all stereotaxic
surgeries. The caudal PPN was targeted bilaterally (cPPN:
anterior–posterior (AP),
−4.80 mm from bregma; mediolateral (ML), ±1.25 mm; dorsal–
ventral (DV),
−3.25 mm from the dura). A total of 300 nL of the Cre-on viral
vectors were injected
into each cPPN of ChAT-Cre mice at a rate of 100 nL/min using
a syringe pump
(PHD Ultra™, Harvard apparatus, no. 70-3007) installed with a
microliter syringe
(Hampton, no.1482452A) and capillary glass pipettes with
filament (Warner
Instruments, no. G150TF-4). Pipettes were left in place for 5
min after injection. Titers
of recombinant AAV vectors ranged from 7.1 × 1012 to 2.2 ×
1013 viral particles/ml,
based on quantitative PCR analysis. AAV8-phSyn1(S)-FLEX-
tdTomato-T2A-
SypEGFP-WPRE was purchased from the Salk Viral Vector
Core and used to trace
the axonal terminals58. AAV-DJ-CMV-DIO-Kir2.1 and AAV-
DJ-CMV-DIO-EGFP
were both ordered from the Stanford Viral Vector Core and used
to suppress neu-
ronal activity or as a control20,59. pAAV-hSyn-DIO-ChAT-
P2A-mRuby2 and pAAV-
hSyn-DIO-mRuby2 plasmids were constructed in our lab and
AAV2/8 vectors were
packaged in the Salk Viral Vector Core. Vector Biolabs
produced AAV8-CAG-DIO-
shRNAmir-mGAD1-EGFP and AAV8-CAG-DIO-shRNAmir-
Scramble-EGFP vec-
tors. The shRNA sequence for mouse GAD1 is
5′-
GTCTACAGTCAACCAGGATCTGGTTTTGGCCACTGACTGA
CCAGATCC
TTTGACTGTAGA-3′.
Tract tracing. For anterograde tracing, 300 nL of Cre-on AAV8-
DIO-mRuby2
vectors were unilaterally injected into the cPPN of ChAT-Cre
mice as described
above and axonal tracts were imaged by Leica confocal
microscope. For retrograde
tracing, a volume of 100 nL of retrobeads (Red Retrobeads™ IX
or Green Retro-
beads™ IX, Lumafluor) was injected at 100 nL/min into the SN
or VTA and VL-
VM. Pipettes were left in place for 10 min after injection. Three
weeks after
injection, mice were sacrificed for brain examination or housed
with running
wheels to examine running-induced loss of ChAT transcripts
using RNAscope
combined with nNOS immunostaining. ChAT transcripts in
neurons that con-
tained retrobeads were quantified using Image J. Retrobead
infusion coordinates
were SN (from bregma AP −3.20 mm; ML ±1.50 mm; and DV
from the dura,
−4.00 mm), VTA (AP, −3.10 mm; ML, ±0.50 mm; and DV,
−4.25 mm), and VL-
VM (AP, −2.20 mm; ML, ±1.75 mm; and DV, −3.25 mm).
Statistics and reproducibility. For comparisons between two
groups, data were
analyzed by Welch’s t-test for normally distributed data and the
Mann–Whitney U
test for data not normally distributed, using Graph Pad Prism 7.
For repeated
samples, data were analyzed by paired t-test. Correlation was
analyzed by the
Pearson correlation using Graph Pad Prism 7. All tests are two-
tailed. Statistical
analyzes of the data were performed using Prism 7 software for
the number of
animals for each experiment indicated in the figure legends.
Means and SEMs are
reported for all experiments. For comparisons between multiple
groups, ANOVA
followed by Tukey’s test was used for normally distributed
data. The nonpara-
metric Kruskal–Wallis test followed by Dunn’s correction was
used for data that
were not normally distributed. Experiments were carried out
independently twice
(Figs. 2b, c, 4a, b, 5b, c, h–j, 7b and Supplementary Figs. 1f,
3a–f, 5, 7f, 8f), three
times (Fig. 1b, h–j, 2d–k, 3, 4c–h, 5e–f, 6c–d, 7c, g, and
Supplementary Figs. 1a–d,
2b–f, i–j, 3g–l, 4a–c, f–i, 6, 7d–e, 8, 9a–d and 10), or four or
more times (Figs. 1c–f,
6e–g, 7d–f and Supplementary Figs. 2g, i, 4d, e, 9e–h).
Reporting summary. Further information on research design is
available in
the Nature Research Reporting Summary linked to this article.
NATURE COMMUNICATIONS |
https://doi.org/10.1038/s41467-020-16053-7 ARTICLE
NATURE COMMUNICATIONS | (2020) 11:2195 |
https://doi.org/10.1038/s41467-020-16053-7 |
www.nature.com/naturecommunications 11
https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/nuccore/EF191515
www.nature.com/naturecommunications
www.nature.com/naturecommunications
Data availability
All data generated or analyzed during this study are included in
this published article and
its supplementary information files. Source data are provided as
a Source data file.
Received: 30 May 2019; Accepted: 7 April 2020;
References
1. Bostan, A. C. & Strick, P. L. The basal ganglia and the
cerebellum: nodes in an
integrated network. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 19, 338–350 (2018).
2. Esposito, M. S. et al. Brainstem nucleus MdV mediates
skilled forelimb motor
tasks. Nature 508, 351–356 (2014).
3. Dayan, E. & Cohen, L. G. Neuroplasticity subserving motor
skill learning.
Neuron 72, 443–454 (2011).
4. McKenzie, I. A. et al. Motor skill learning requires active
central myelination.
Science 346, 318–322 (2014).
5. Statton, M. A. et al. A single bout of moderate aerobic
exercise improves
motor skill acquisition. PLoS ONE 10, e0141393 (2015).
6. Palmer, S. S. et al. Exercise therapy for Parkinson’s dis ease.
Arch. Phys. Med.
Rehabil. 67, 741–745 (1986).
7. Kane, K. & Bell, A. A. core stability group program for
children with
developmental coordination disorder: 3 clinical case reports.
Pediatr. Phys.
Ther. 21, 375–382 (2009).
8. Rafie, F. et al. Physical exercises and motor skills in autistic
children. Iran. J.
Public Health 44, 724–725 (2015).
9. Meijer, J. H. & Robbers, Y. Wheel running in the wild. Proc.
Roy. Soc. B 281,
20140210 (2014).
10. Murphy, T. H. & Corbett, D. Plasticity during stroke
recovery: from synapse
to behaviour. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 10, 861–872 (2009).
11. Petzinger, G. M. et al. Exercise-enhanced neuroplasticity
targeting motor and
cognitive circuitry in Parkinson’s disease. Lancet Neurol. 12,
716–726 (2013).
12. Spitzer, N. C. Neurotransmitter switching in the developing
and adult brain.
Ann. Rev. Neurosci. 40, 1–19 (2017).
13. Shiotsuki, H. et al. A rotarod test for evaluation of motor
skill learning. J.
Neurosci. Methods 189, 180–185 (2010).
14. Buitrago, M. M. et al. Short and long-term motor skill
learning in an
accelerated rotarod training paradigm. Neurobiol. Learn. Mem.
81, 211–216
(2004).
15. Pothakos, K., Kurz, M. J. & Lau, Y. S. Restorative effect of
endurance exercise
on behavioral deficits in the chronic mouse model of
Parkinson’s disease with
severe neurodegeneration. BMC Neurosci. 10, 6 (2009).
16. Mackintosh, N. J. A theory of attention: variations in the
associability of
stimuli with reinforcement. Psychological Rev. 82, 276–298
(1975).
17. Morris, R. G. et al. Selective impairment of learning and
blockade of long-term
potentiation by an N-methyl-D-aspartate receptor antagonist,
AP5. Nature
319, 774–776 (1986).
18. Marek, K. W., Kurtz, L. M. & Spitzer, N. C. cJun integrates
calcium activity
and tlx3 expression to regulate neurotransmitter specification.
Nat. Neurosci.
13, 944–950 (2010).
19. Güemez-Gamboa, A. et al. Non-cell-autonomous mechanism
of activity-
dependent neurotransmitter switching. Neuron 82, 1004–1016
(2014).
20. Meng, D. et al. Neuronal activity regulates neurotransmitter
switching in the
adult brain following light-induced stress. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci.
USA 115,
5064–5071 (2018).
21. Clark, P. J. et al. Adult hippocampal neurogenesis and c-Fos
induction during
escalation of voluntary wheel running in C57BL/6J mice.
Behav. Brain Res.
213, 246–252 (2010).
22. Boisgontier, M. P. et al. Individual differences in brainstem
and basal ganglia
structure predict postural control and balance loss in young and
older adults.
Neurobiol. Aging 50, 47–59 (2017).
23. Mena-Segovia, J. & Bolam, J. P. Rethinking the
pedunculopontine nucleus:
from cellular organization to function. Neuron 94, 7–18 (2017).
24. Smith, Y. et al. Parkinson’s disease therapeutics: new
developments and
challenges since the introduction of levodopa.
Neuropsychopharmacol 37,
213–246 (2012).
25. Mena-Segovia, J. et al. GABAergic neuron distribution in
the
pedunculopontine nucleus defines functional subterritories. J.
Comp. Neurol.
515, 397–408 (2009).
26. Tattersall, T. L. et al. Imagined gait modulates neuronal
network dynamics in
the human pedunculopontine nucleus. Nat. Neurosci. 17, 449–
454 (2014).
27. Thevathasan, W. et al. Alpha oscillations in the
pedunculopontine nucleus
correlate with gait performance in parkinsonism. Brain 135,
148–160 (2012).
28. Gut, N. K. & Winn, P. Deep brain stimulation of different
pedunculopontine
targets in a novel rodent model of parkinsonism. J. Neurosci.
35, 4792–4803
(2015).
29. Karachi, C. et al. Cholinergic mesencephalic neurons are
involved in gait
and postural disorders in Parkinson disease. J. Clin. Invest. 120,
2745–2754
(2010).
30. Madisen, L. et al. A robust and high-throughput Cre
reporting and
characterization system for the whole mouse brain. Nat.
Neurosci. 13, 133–140
(2010).
31. Chen, E. et al. Altered baseline and nicotine-mediated
behavioral and
cholinergic profiles in ChAT-Cre mouse lines. J. Neurosci. 38,
2177–2188
(2018).
32. Vincent, S. R. et al. NADPH-diaphorase: a selective
histochemical marker for
the cholinergic neurons of the pontine reticular formation.
Neurosci. Lett. 43,
31–36 (1983).
33. Saunders, A., Granger, A. J. & Sabatini, B. L. Corelease of
acetylcholine and
GABA from cholinergic forebrain neurons. elife 27, 4 (2015).
34. Wang, H. L. & Morales, M. Pedunculopontine and
laterodorsal tegmental
nuclei contain distinct populations of cholinergic, glutamatergic
and
GABAergic neurons in the rat. Eur. J. Neurosci. 29, 340–358
(2009).
35. Luquin, E. et al. Stereological estimates of glutamatergic,
GABAergic, and
cholinergic neurons in the pedunculopontine and laterodorsal
tegmental
nuclei in the rat. Front. Neuroanat. 12, 34 (2018).
36. Xiao, C. et al. Cholinergic mesopontine signals govern
locomotion and reward
through dissociable midbrain pathways. Neuron 90, 333–347
(2016).
37. Dautan, D. et al. Segregated cholinergic transmission
modulates dopamine
neurons integrated in distinct functional circuits. Nat. Neurosci.
19,
1025–1033 (2016).
38. Gambhir, H., Mathur, R. & Behari, M. Progressive
impairment in motor skill
learning at 12 and 20 weeks post 6-OHDA- SNc lesion in rats.
Parkinsonism
Relat. Disord. 17, 476–478 (2011).
39. Leemburg., S. et al. Motor skill learning and reward
consumption differentially
affect VTA activation. Sci. Rep. 8, 687 (2018).
40. Steriade, M. et al. Fast oscillations (20-40 Hz) in
thalamocortical systems and
their potentiation by mesopontine cholinergic nuclei in the cat.
Proc. Natl
Acad. Sci. USA 88, 4396–4400 (1991).
41. Jeljeli, M. et al. Effects of ventrolateral-ventromedial
thalamic lesions on
motor coordination and spatial orientation in rats. Neurosci.
Res. 47, 309–316
(2003).
42. Kaita, A. A. & Goldberg, A. M. Control of acetylcholine
synthesis—the
inhibition of choline acetyltransferase by acetylcholine. J.
Neurochem. 16,
1185–1191 (1969).
43. Morris, D., Maneckjee, A. & Hebb, C. The kinetic
properties of human
placental choline acetyltransferase. Biochem. J. 125, 857–863
(1971).
44. Dulcis, D. et al. Neurotransmitter switching regulated by
miRNAs controls
changes in social preference. Neuron 95, 1319–1333 (2017).
45. Perez-Lloret, S. & Barrantes, F. J. Deficits in cholinergic
neurotransmission
and their clinical correlates in Parkinson’s disease, N.P.J.
Parkinsons Dis. 2,
16001 (2016).
46. Hosp, J. A. et al. Dopaminergic projections from midbrain
to primary
motor cortex mediate motor skill learning. J. Neurosci. 31,
2481–2487
(2011).
47. Guo, L. et al. Dynamic rewiring of neural circuits in the
motor cortex in
mouse models of Parkinson’s disease. Nat. Neurosci. 18, 1299–
1309 (2015).
48. Vitrac, C. & Benoit-Marand, M. Monoaminergic modulation
of motor cortex
function. Front. Neural Circuits 11, 72 (2017).
49. Chandler, D. J., Gao, W. J. & Waterhouse, B. D.
Heterogeneous organization
of the locus coeruleus projections to prefrontal and motor
cortices. Proc. Natl
Acad. Sci. USA 111, 6816–6821 (2014).
50. MacLaren, D. A. et al. Deficits in motor performance after
pedunculopontine
lesions in rats – impairment depends on demands of task. Eur. J.
Neurosci. 40,
3224–3236 (2014).
51. Caggiano, V. et al. Midbrain circuits that set locomotor
speed and gait
selection. Nature 553, 455–460 (2018).
52. Roseberry, T. K. et al. Cell-type-specific control of
brainstem locomotor
circuits by basal ganglia. Cell 164, 526–537 (2016).
53. Lee, A. M. et al. Identification of a brainstem circuit
regulating visual cortical
state in parallel with locomotion. Neuron 83, 455–466 (2014).
54. Mehrholz, J. et al. Treadmill training for patients with
Parkinson’s disease.
Cochrane Database Syst. Rev. 8, CD007830 (2015).
55. Polese, J. C. et al. Treadmill training is effective for
ambulatory adults with
stroke: a systematic review. J. Physiother. 59, 73–80 (2013).
56. Gaßner, H. et al. Perturbation treadmill training improves
clinical
characteristics of gait and balance in Parkinson’s Disease. J.
Parkinsons Dis. 9,
413–426 (2019).
57. Franklin, K. & Paxinos, G. The Mouse Brain In Stereotaxic
Coordinates 3rd
edn (Elsevier, New York, 2008).
58. Abs, E. et al. Learning-related plasticity in dendrite-
targeting layer 1
interneurons. Neuron 100, 684–699 (2018).
59. Knowland, D. et al. Distinct ventral pallidal neural
populations mediate
separate symptoms of depression. Cell 170, 284–297 (2017).
ARTICLE NATURE COMMUNICATIONS |
https://doi.org/10.1038/s41467-020-16053-7
12 NATURE COMMUNICATIONS | (2020) 11:2195 |
https://doi.org/10.1038/s41467-020-16053-7 |
www.nature.com/naturecommunications
www.nature.com/naturecommunications
Acknowledgements
We thank Kyle Jackson, Vaidehi Gupta, and Wuji Jiang for
experimental assistance, Alex
Glavis-Bloom for technical support and Larry Squire and Takaki
Komiyama for com-
ments on the manuscript. We thank all members of the Spitzer
lab for suggestions on
experimental design and the manuscript. We thank Stefan
Leutgeb and Byungkook Lim
for providing transgenic mice. We thank Jennifer Santini and
the UCSD Microscopy
Core for technical support with imaging and Amanda Roberts
and the Scripps Animal
Models Core for suggestions on behavior experiments. This
research was supported by
grants to N.C.S from the Ellison Medical Foundation, the W. M.
Keck Foundation, NIH
NS047101 and the Overland Foundation.
Author contributions
H.-q.L. and N.C.S. conceived the study, designed the
experiments, interpreted the results
and wrote the paper. H.-q.L. performed the experiments and
analyzes.
Competing interests
The authors declare no competing interests.
Additional information
Supplementary information is available for this paper at
https://doi.org/10.1038/s41467-
020-16053-7.
Correspondence and requests for materials should be addressed
to H.-q.L. or N.C.S.
Peer review information Nature Communications thanks
Konstantinos Ampatzis,
Michael Nusbaum and the other anonymous reviewer(s) for their
contribution to the
peer review of this work. Peer reviewer reports are available.
Reprints and permission information is available at
http://www.nature.com/reprints
Publisher’s note Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to
jurisdictional claims in
published maps and institutional affiliations.
Open Access This article is licensed under a Creative Commons
Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use,
sharing,
adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or
format, as long as you give
appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source,
provide a link to the Creative
Commons license, and indicate if changes were made. The
images or other third party
material in this article are included in the article’s Creative
Commons license, unless
indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is
not included in the
article’s Creative Commons license and your intended use is not
permitted by statutory
regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain
permission directly from
the copyright holder. To view a copy of this license, visit
http://creativecommons.org/
licenses/by/4.0/.
© The Author(s) 2020
NATURE COMMUNICATIONS |
https://doi.org/10.1038/s41467-020-16053-7 ARTICLE
NATURE COMMUNICATIONS | (2020) 11:2195 |
https://doi.org/10.1038/s41467-020-16053-7 |
www.nature.com/naturecommunications 13
https://doi.org/10.1038/s41467-020-16053-7
https://doi.org/10.1038/s41467-020-16053-7
http://www.nature.com/reprints
http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/
http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/
www.nature.com/naturecommunications
www.nature.com/naturecommunicationsExercise enhances
motor skill learning by neurotransmitter switching in the adult
midbrainResultsSustained running enhances motor skill
learningRunning induces transmitter switching in the midbrain
cPPNTranscript levels of transmitter synthetic enzymes
changeSwitching neurons project to the SN, VTA, and
thalamusEnhanced motor skill learning requires transmitter
switchDiscussionMethodsMiceWheel runningRotarodBalance
beamMotor skill learningLocomotor activityHistology and
immunocytochemistryBirthdatingTUNEL assayImaging and data
analysisStereological countingViral constructs and
injectionTract tracingStatistics and reproducibilityReporting
summaryData availabilityReferencesAcknowledgementsAuthor
contributionsCompeting interestsAdditional information

More Related Content

Similar to HMSV 346 Diversity Issues in Human Services Spring 2021

There are two Discussion Boards and a Reflection Discussion for a .docx
There are two Discussion Boards and a Reflection Discussion for a .docxThere are two Discussion Boards and a Reflection Discussion for a .docx
There are two Discussion Boards and a Reflection Discussion for a .docx
randymartin91030
 
An explanation of the Doing Ethics Technique Graham R Seach .docx
An explanation of the Doing Ethics Technique Graham R Seach .docxAn explanation of the Doing Ethics Technique Graham R Seach .docx
An explanation of the Doing Ethics Technique Graham R Seach .docx
nettletondevon
 
Deliverable 6 - Making Contacts for the FutureTop of FormBotto.docx
Deliverable 6 - Making Contacts for the FutureTop of FormBotto.docxDeliverable 6 - Making Contacts for the FutureTop of FormBotto.docx
Deliverable 6 - Making Contacts for the FutureTop of FormBotto.docx
alisondakintxt
 
etting StartedRevised Final Proposal - Internal and External
etting StartedRevised Final Proposal - Internal and Externaletting StartedRevised Final Proposal - Internal and External
etting StartedRevised Final Proposal - Internal and External
BetseyCalderon89
 
Charles Hackner, Felicia Anderson 6212-ECON-2302-Principles of Mic
Charles Hackner, Felicia Anderson 6212-ECON-2302-Principles of MicCharles Hackner, Felicia Anderson 6212-ECON-2302-Principles of Mic
Charles Hackner, Felicia Anderson 6212-ECON-2302-Principles of Mic
JinElias52
 
Notifications My CommunityHome BSL 4060-14F-6A19-S1, Team .docx
Notifications My CommunityHome BSL 4060-14F-6A19-S1, Team .docxNotifications My CommunityHome BSL 4060-14F-6A19-S1, Team .docx
Notifications My CommunityHome BSL 4060-14F-6A19-S1, Team .docx
vannagoforth
 
Assignment 1 Reviewing Research and Making Connections (Student.docx
Assignment 1 Reviewing Research and Making Connections (Student.docxAssignment 1 Reviewing Research and Making Connections (Student.docx
Assignment 1 Reviewing Research and Making Connections (Student.docx
deanmtaylor1545
 
Sample Title Page For Thesis Proposal - How To
Sample Title Page For Thesis Proposal - How ToSample Title Page For Thesis Proposal - How To
Sample Title Page For Thesis Proposal - How To
Beth Johnson
 
Casepresentations
CasepresentationsCasepresentations
Casepresentationsguestc30dd3
 
Unit 12 assignment 1 – job market research
 Unit 12  assignment 1 – job market research Unit 12  assignment 1 – job market research
Unit 12 assignment 1 – job market research
Bluecare
 
Write An Essay On My Dream House
Write An Essay On My Dream HouseWrite An Essay On My Dream House
Write An Essay On My Dream House
Carolyn Smith
 
Cja 425 Success Begins / snaptutorial.com
Cja 425  Success Begins / snaptutorial.comCja 425  Success Begins / snaptutorial.com
Cja 425 Success Begins / snaptutorial.com
WilliamsTaylorza41
 
Blank Paper To Type On FileWrite Your Story Blan
Blank Paper To Type On FileWrite Your Story BlanBlank Paper To Type On FileWrite Your Story Blan
Blank Paper To Type On FileWrite Your Story Blan
Dotha Keller
 

Similar to HMSV 346 Diversity Issues in Human Services Spring 2021 (13)

There are two Discussion Boards and a Reflection Discussion for a .docx
There are two Discussion Boards and a Reflection Discussion for a .docxThere are two Discussion Boards and a Reflection Discussion for a .docx
There are two Discussion Boards and a Reflection Discussion for a .docx
 
An explanation of the Doing Ethics Technique Graham R Seach .docx
An explanation of the Doing Ethics Technique Graham R Seach .docxAn explanation of the Doing Ethics Technique Graham R Seach .docx
An explanation of the Doing Ethics Technique Graham R Seach .docx
 
Deliverable 6 - Making Contacts for the FutureTop of FormBotto.docx
Deliverable 6 - Making Contacts for the FutureTop of FormBotto.docxDeliverable 6 - Making Contacts for the FutureTop of FormBotto.docx
Deliverable 6 - Making Contacts for the FutureTop of FormBotto.docx
 
etting StartedRevised Final Proposal - Internal and External
etting StartedRevised Final Proposal - Internal and Externaletting StartedRevised Final Proposal - Internal and External
etting StartedRevised Final Proposal - Internal and External
 
Charles Hackner, Felicia Anderson 6212-ECON-2302-Principles of Mic
Charles Hackner, Felicia Anderson 6212-ECON-2302-Principles of MicCharles Hackner, Felicia Anderson 6212-ECON-2302-Principles of Mic
Charles Hackner, Felicia Anderson 6212-ECON-2302-Principles of Mic
 
Notifications My CommunityHome BSL 4060-14F-6A19-S1, Team .docx
Notifications My CommunityHome BSL 4060-14F-6A19-S1, Team .docxNotifications My CommunityHome BSL 4060-14F-6A19-S1, Team .docx
Notifications My CommunityHome BSL 4060-14F-6A19-S1, Team .docx
 
Assignment 1 Reviewing Research and Making Connections (Student.docx
Assignment 1 Reviewing Research and Making Connections (Student.docxAssignment 1 Reviewing Research and Making Connections (Student.docx
Assignment 1 Reviewing Research and Making Connections (Student.docx
 
Sample Title Page For Thesis Proposal - How To
Sample Title Page For Thesis Proposal - How ToSample Title Page For Thesis Proposal - How To
Sample Title Page For Thesis Proposal - How To
 
Casepresentations
CasepresentationsCasepresentations
Casepresentations
 
Unit 12 assignment 1 – job market research
 Unit 12  assignment 1 – job market research Unit 12  assignment 1 – job market research
Unit 12 assignment 1 – job market research
 
Write An Essay On My Dream House
Write An Essay On My Dream HouseWrite An Essay On My Dream House
Write An Essay On My Dream House
 
Cja 425 Success Begins / snaptutorial.com
Cja 425  Success Begins / snaptutorial.comCja 425  Success Begins / snaptutorial.com
Cja 425 Success Begins / snaptutorial.com
 
Blank Paper To Type On FileWrite Your Story Blan
Blank Paper To Type On FileWrite Your Story BlanBlank Paper To Type On FileWrite Your Story Blan
Blank Paper To Type On FileWrite Your Story Blan
 

More from SusanaFurman449

Please read the description of the Religion ethnography carefully an.docx
Please read the description of the Religion ethnography carefully an.docxPlease read the description of the Religion ethnography carefully an.docx
Please read the description of the Religion ethnography carefully an.docx
SusanaFurman449
 
PLEASE read the question carefully.  The creation of teen ido.docx
PLEASE read the question carefully.  The creation of teen ido.docxPLEASE read the question carefully.  The creation of teen ido.docx
PLEASE read the question carefully.  The creation of teen ido.docx
SusanaFurman449
 
Please reflect on the relationship between faith, personal disciplin.docx
Please reflect on the relationship between faith, personal disciplin.docxPlease reflect on the relationship between faith, personal disciplin.docx
Please reflect on the relationship between faith, personal disciplin.docx
SusanaFurman449
 
Please read the following questions and answer the questions.docx
Please read the following questions and answer the questions.docxPlease read the following questions and answer the questions.docx
Please read the following questions and answer the questions.docx
SusanaFurman449
 
PRAISE FOR CRUCIAL CONVERSATIONS Relationships ar.docx
PRAISE FOR CRUCIAL CONVERSATIONS Relationships ar.docxPRAISE FOR CRUCIAL CONVERSATIONS Relationships ar.docx
PRAISE FOR CRUCIAL CONVERSATIONS Relationships ar.docx
SusanaFurman449
 
Must Be a hip-hop concert!!!!attend a .docx
Must Be a             hip-hop concert!!!!attend a           .docxMust Be a             hip-hop concert!!!!attend a           .docx
Must Be a hip-hop concert!!!!attend a .docx
SusanaFurman449
 
Mini-Paper #3 Johnson & Johnson and a Tale of Two Crises - An Eth.docx
Mini-Paper #3 Johnson & Johnson and a Tale of Two Crises - An Eth.docxMini-Paper #3 Johnson & Johnson and a Tale of Two Crises - An Eth.docx
Mini-Paper #3 Johnson & Johnson and a Tale of Two Crises - An Eth.docx
SusanaFurman449
 
Please write these 2 assignments in first person.docx
Please write these 2 assignments in                 first person.docxPlease write these 2 assignments in                 first person.docx
Please write these 2 assignments in first person.docx
SusanaFurman449
 
Personal Leadership Training plan AttributesColumbia South.docx
Personal Leadership Training plan  AttributesColumbia South.docxPersonal Leadership Training plan  AttributesColumbia South.docx
Personal Leadership Training plan AttributesColumbia South.docx
SusanaFurman449
 
Need help on researching why women join gangs1.How does anxi.docx
Need help on researching why women join gangs1.How does anxi.docxNeed help on researching why women join gangs1.How does anxi.docx
Need help on researching why women join gangs1.How does anxi.docx
SusanaFurman449
 
Jung Typology AssessmentThe purpose of this assignment is to ass.docx
Jung Typology AssessmentThe purpose of this assignment is to ass.docxJung Typology AssessmentThe purpose of this assignment is to ass.docx
Jung Typology AssessmentThe purpose of this assignment is to ass.docx
SusanaFurman449
 
Journal of Organizational Behavior J. Organiz. Behav. 31, .docx
Journal of Organizational Behavior J. Organiz. Behav. 31, .docxJournal of Organizational Behavior J. Organiz. Behav. 31, .docx
Journal of Organizational Behavior J. Organiz. Behav. 31, .docx
SusanaFurman449
 
LDR535 v4Organizational Change ChartLDR535 v4Page 2 of 2.docx
LDR535 v4Organizational Change ChartLDR535 v4Page 2 of 2.docxLDR535 v4Organizational Change ChartLDR535 v4Page 2 of 2.docx
LDR535 v4Organizational Change ChartLDR535 v4Page 2 of 2.docx
SusanaFurman449
 
In this paper, you will select an ethics issue from among the topics.docx
In this paper, you will select an ethics issue from among the topics.docxIn this paper, you will select an ethics issue from among the topics.docx
In this paper, you will select an ethics issue from among the topics.docx
SusanaFurman449
 
In the past few weeks, you practiced observation skills by watchin.docx
In the past few weeks, you practiced observation skills by watchin.docxIn the past few weeks, you practiced observation skills by watchin.docx
In the past few weeks, you practiced observation skills by watchin.docx
SusanaFurman449
 
Overview After analyzing your public health issue in Milestone On.docx
Overview After analyzing your public health issue in Milestone On.docxOverview After analyzing your public health issue in Milestone On.docx
Overview After analyzing your public health issue in Milestone On.docx
SusanaFurman449
 
Judicial OpinionsOverview After the simulation, justices writ.docx
Judicial OpinionsOverview After the simulation, justices writ.docxJudicial OpinionsOverview After the simulation, justices writ.docx
Judicial OpinionsOverview After the simulation, justices writ.docx
SusanaFurman449
 
IntroductionReview the Vila Health scenario and complete the int.docx
IntroductionReview the Vila Health scenario and complete the int.docxIntroductionReview the Vila Health scenario and complete the int.docx
IntroductionReview the Vila Health scenario and complete the int.docx
SusanaFurman449
 
In studying Social Problems, sociologists (and historians) identify .docx
In studying Social Problems, sociologists (and historians) identify .docxIn studying Social Problems, sociologists (and historians) identify .docx
In studying Social Problems, sociologists (and historians) identify .docx
SusanaFurman449
 
I need help correcting an integrative review.This was the profes.docx
I need help correcting an integrative review.This was the profes.docxI need help correcting an integrative review.This was the profes.docx
I need help correcting an integrative review.This was the profes.docx
SusanaFurman449
 

More from SusanaFurman449 (20)

Please read the description of the Religion ethnography carefully an.docx
Please read the description of the Religion ethnography carefully an.docxPlease read the description of the Religion ethnography carefully an.docx
Please read the description of the Religion ethnography carefully an.docx
 
PLEASE read the question carefully.  The creation of teen ido.docx
PLEASE read the question carefully.  The creation of teen ido.docxPLEASE read the question carefully.  The creation of teen ido.docx
PLEASE read the question carefully.  The creation of teen ido.docx
 
Please reflect on the relationship between faith, personal disciplin.docx
Please reflect on the relationship between faith, personal disciplin.docxPlease reflect on the relationship between faith, personal disciplin.docx
Please reflect on the relationship between faith, personal disciplin.docx
 
Please read the following questions and answer the questions.docx
Please read the following questions and answer the questions.docxPlease read the following questions and answer the questions.docx
Please read the following questions and answer the questions.docx
 
PRAISE FOR CRUCIAL CONVERSATIONS Relationships ar.docx
PRAISE FOR CRUCIAL CONVERSATIONS Relationships ar.docxPRAISE FOR CRUCIAL CONVERSATIONS Relationships ar.docx
PRAISE FOR CRUCIAL CONVERSATIONS Relationships ar.docx
 
Must Be a hip-hop concert!!!!attend a .docx
Must Be a             hip-hop concert!!!!attend a           .docxMust Be a             hip-hop concert!!!!attend a           .docx
Must Be a hip-hop concert!!!!attend a .docx
 
Mini-Paper #3 Johnson & Johnson and a Tale of Two Crises - An Eth.docx
Mini-Paper #3 Johnson & Johnson and a Tale of Two Crises - An Eth.docxMini-Paper #3 Johnson & Johnson and a Tale of Two Crises - An Eth.docx
Mini-Paper #3 Johnson & Johnson and a Tale of Two Crises - An Eth.docx
 
Please write these 2 assignments in first person.docx
Please write these 2 assignments in                 first person.docxPlease write these 2 assignments in                 first person.docx
Please write these 2 assignments in first person.docx
 
Personal Leadership Training plan AttributesColumbia South.docx
Personal Leadership Training plan  AttributesColumbia South.docxPersonal Leadership Training plan  AttributesColumbia South.docx
Personal Leadership Training plan AttributesColumbia South.docx
 
Need help on researching why women join gangs1.How does anxi.docx
Need help on researching why women join gangs1.How does anxi.docxNeed help on researching why women join gangs1.How does anxi.docx
Need help on researching why women join gangs1.How does anxi.docx
 
Jung Typology AssessmentThe purpose of this assignment is to ass.docx
Jung Typology AssessmentThe purpose of this assignment is to ass.docxJung Typology AssessmentThe purpose of this assignment is to ass.docx
Jung Typology AssessmentThe purpose of this assignment is to ass.docx
 
Journal of Organizational Behavior J. Organiz. Behav. 31, .docx
Journal of Organizational Behavior J. Organiz. Behav. 31, .docxJournal of Organizational Behavior J. Organiz. Behav. 31, .docx
Journal of Organizational Behavior J. Organiz. Behav. 31, .docx
 
LDR535 v4Organizational Change ChartLDR535 v4Page 2 of 2.docx
LDR535 v4Organizational Change ChartLDR535 v4Page 2 of 2.docxLDR535 v4Organizational Change ChartLDR535 v4Page 2 of 2.docx
LDR535 v4Organizational Change ChartLDR535 v4Page 2 of 2.docx
 
In this paper, you will select an ethics issue from among the topics.docx
In this paper, you will select an ethics issue from among the topics.docxIn this paper, you will select an ethics issue from among the topics.docx
In this paper, you will select an ethics issue from among the topics.docx
 
In the past few weeks, you practiced observation skills by watchin.docx
In the past few weeks, you practiced observation skills by watchin.docxIn the past few weeks, you practiced observation skills by watchin.docx
In the past few weeks, you practiced observation skills by watchin.docx
 
Overview After analyzing your public health issue in Milestone On.docx
Overview After analyzing your public health issue in Milestone On.docxOverview After analyzing your public health issue in Milestone On.docx
Overview After analyzing your public health issue in Milestone On.docx
 
Judicial OpinionsOverview After the simulation, justices writ.docx
Judicial OpinionsOverview After the simulation, justices writ.docxJudicial OpinionsOverview After the simulation, justices writ.docx
Judicial OpinionsOverview After the simulation, justices writ.docx
 
IntroductionReview the Vila Health scenario and complete the int.docx
IntroductionReview the Vila Health scenario and complete the int.docxIntroductionReview the Vila Health scenario and complete the int.docx
IntroductionReview the Vila Health scenario and complete the int.docx
 
In studying Social Problems, sociologists (and historians) identify .docx
In studying Social Problems, sociologists (and historians) identify .docxIn studying Social Problems, sociologists (and historians) identify .docx
In studying Social Problems, sociologists (and historians) identify .docx
 
I need help correcting an integrative review.This was the profes.docx
I need help correcting an integrative review.This was the profes.docxI need help correcting an integrative review.This was the profes.docx
I need help correcting an integrative review.This was the profes.docx
 

Recently uploaded

Language Across the Curriculm LAC B.Ed.
Language Across the  Curriculm LAC B.Ed.Language Across the  Curriculm LAC B.Ed.
Language Across the Curriculm LAC B.Ed.
Atul Kumar Singh
 
June 3, 2024 Anti-Semitism Letter Sent to MIT President Kornbluth and MIT Cor...
June 3, 2024 Anti-Semitism Letter Sent to MIT President Kornbluth and MIT Cor...June 3, 2024 Anti-Semitism Letter Sent to MIT President Kornbluth and MIT Cor...
June 3, 2024 Anti-Semitism Letter Sent to MIT President Kornbluth and MIT Cor...
Levi Shapiro
 
CLASS 11 CBSE B.St Project AIDS TO TRADE - INSURANCE
CLASS 11 CBSE B.St Project AIDS TO TRADE - INSURANCECLASS 11 CBSE B.St Project AIDS TO TRADE - INSURANCE
CLASS 11 CBSE B.St Project AIDS TO TRADE - INSURANCE
BhavyaRajput3
 
Overview on Edible Vaccine: Pros & Cons with Mechanism
Overview on Edible Vaccine: Pros & Cons with MechanismOverview on Edible Vaccine: Pros & Cons with Mechanism
Overview on Edible Vaccine: Pros & Cons with Mechanism
DeeptiGupta154
 
Model Attribute Check Company Auto Property
Model Attribute  Check Company Auto PropertyModel Attribute  Check Company Auto Property
Model Attribute Check Company Auto Property
Celine George
 
Supporting (UKRI) OA monographs at Salford.pptx
Supporting (UKRI) OA monographs at Salford.pptxSupporting (UKRI) OA monographs at Salford.pptx
Supporting (UKRI) OA monographs at Salford.pptx
Jisc
 
Francesca Gottschalk - How can education support child empowerment.pptx
Francesca Gottschalk - How can education support child empowerment.pptxFrancesca Gottschalk - How can education support child empowerment.pptx
Francesca Gottschalk - How can education support child empowerment.pptx
EduSkills OECD
 
Adversarial Attention Modeling for Multi-dimensional Emotion Regression.pdf
Adversarial Attention Modeling for Multi-dimensional Emotion Regression.pdfAdversarial Attention Modeling for Multi-dimensional Emotion Regression.pdf
Adversarial Attention Modeling for Multi-dimensional Emotion Regression.pdf
Po-Chuan Chen
 
Additional Benefits for Employee Website.pdf
Additional Benefits for Employee Website.pdfAdditional Benefits for Employee Website.pdf
Additional Benefits for Employee Website.pdf
joachimlavalley1
 
The Accursed House by Émile Gaboriau.pptx
The Accursed House by Émile Gaboriau.pptxThe Accursed House by Émile Gaboriau.pptx
The Accursed House by Émile Gaboriau.pptx
DhatriParmar
 
"Protectable subject matters, Protection in biotechnology, Protection of othe...
"Protectable subject matters, Protection in biotechnology, Protection of othe..."Protectable subject matters, Protection in biotechnology, Protection of othe...
"Protectable subject matters, Protection in biotechnology, Protection of othe...
SACHIN R KONDAGURI
 
1.4 modern child centered education - mahatma gandhi-2.pptx
1.4 modern child centered education - mahatma gandhi-2.pptx1.4 modern child centered education - mahatma gandhi-2.pptx
1.4 modern child centered education - mahatma gandhi-2.pptx
JosvitaDsouza2
 
Embracing GenAI - A Strategic Imperative
Embracing GenAI - A Strategic ImperativeEmbracing GenAI - A Strategic Imperative
Embracing GenAI - A Strategic Imperative
Peter Windle
 
Chapter 3 - Islamic Banking Products and Services.pptx
Chapter 3 - Islamic Banking Products and Services.pptxChapter 3 - Islamic Banking Products and Services.pptx
Chapter 3 - Islamic Banking Products and Services.pptx
Mohd Adib Abd Muin, Senior Lecturer at Universiti Utara Malaysia
 
Guidance_and_Counselling.pdf B.Ed. 4th Semester
Guidance_and_Counselling.pdf B.Ed. 4th SemesterGuidance_and_Counselling.pdf B.Ed. 4th Semester
Guidance_and_Counselling.pdf B.Ed. 4th Semester
Atul Kumar Singh
 
How to Make a Field invisible in Odoo 17
How to Make a Field invisible in Odoo 17How to Make a Field invisible in Odoo 17
How to Make a Field invisible in Odoo 17
Celine George
 
TESDA TM1 REVIEWER FOR NATIONAL ASSESSMENT WRITTEN AND ORAL QUESTIONS WITH A...
TESDA TM1 REVIEWER  FOR NATIONAL ASSESSMENT WRITTEN AND ORAL QUESTIONS WITH A...TESDA TM1 REVIEWER  FOR NATIONAL ASSESSMENT WRITTEN AND ORAL QUESTIONS WITH A...
TESDA TM1 REVIEWER FOR NATIONAL ASSESSMENT WRITTEN AND ORAL QUESTIONS WITH A...
EugeneSaldivar
 
Home assignment II on Spectroscopy 2024 Answers.pdf
Home assignment II on Spectroscopy 2024 Answers.pdfHome assignment II on Spectroscopy 2024 Answers.pdf
Home assignment II on Spectroscopy 2024 Answers.pdf
Tamralipta Mahavidyalaya
 
2024.06.01 Introducing a competency framework for languag learning materials ...
2024.06.01 Introducing a competency framework for languag learning materials ...2024.06.01 Introducing a competency framework for languag learning materials ...
2024.06.01 Introducing a competency framework for languag learning materials ...
Sandy Millin
 
How libraries can support authors with open access requirements for UKRI fund...
How libraries can support authors with open access requirements for UKRI fund...How libraries can support authors with open access requirements for UKRI fund...
How libraries can support authors with open access requirements for UKRI fund...
Jisc
 

Recently uploaded (20)

Language Across the Curriculm LAC B.Ed.
Language Across the  Curriculm LAC B.Ed.Language Across the  Curriculm LAC B.Ed.
Language Across the Curriculm LAC B.Ed.
 
June 3, 2024 Anti-Semitism Letter Sent to MIT President Kornbluth and MIT Cor...
June 3, 2024 Anti-Semitism Letter Sent to MIT President Kornbluth and MIT Cor...June 3, 2024 Anti-Semitism Letter Sent to MIT President Kornbluth and MIT Cor...
June 3, 2024 Anti-Semitism Letter Sent to MIT President Kornbluth and MIT Cor...
 
CLASS 11 CBSE B.St Project AIDS TO TRADE - INSURANCE
CLASS 11 CBSE B.St Project AIDS TO TRADE - INSURANCECLASS 11 CBSE B.St Project AIDS TO TRADE - INSURANCE
CLASS 11 CBSE B.St Project AIDS TO TRADE - INSURANCE
 
Overview on Edible Vaccine: Pros & Cons with Mechanism
Overview on Edible Vaccine: Pros & Cons with MechanismOverview on Edible Vaccine: Pros & Cons with Mechanism
Overview on Edible Vaccine: Pros & Cons with Mechanism
 
Model Attribute Check Company Auto Property
Model Attribute  Check Company Auto PropertyModel Attribute  Check Company Auto Property
Model Attribute Check Company Auto Property
 
Supporting (UKRI) OA monographs at Salford.pptx
Supporting (UKRI) OA monographs at Salford.pptxSupporting (UKRI) OA monographs at Salford.pptx
Supporting (UKRI) OA monographs at Salford.pptx
 
Francesca Gottschalk - How can education support child empowerment.pptx
Francesca Gottschalk - How can education support child empowerment.pptxFrancesca Gottschalk - How can education support child empowerment.pptx
Francesca Gottschalk - How can education support child empowerment.pptx
 
Adversarial Attention Modeling for Multi-dimensional Emotion Regression.pdf
Adversarial Attention Modeling for Multi-dimensional Emotion Regression.pdfAdversarial Attention Modeling for Multi-dimensional Emotion Regression.pdf
Adversarial Attention Modeling for Multi-dimensional Emotion Regression.pdf
 
Additional Benefits for Employee Website.pdf
Additional Benefits for Employee Website.pdfAdditional Benefits for Employee Website.pdf
Additional Benefits for Employee Website.pdf
 
The Accursed House by Émile Gaboriau.pptx
The Accursed House by Émile Gaboriau.pptxThe Accursed House by Émile Gaboriau.pptx
The Accursed House by Émile Gaboriau.pptx
 
"Protectable subject matters, Protection in biotechnology, Protection of othe...
"Protectable subject matters, Protection in biotechnology, Protection of othe..."Protectable subject matters, Protection in biotechnology, Protection of othe...
"Protectable subject matters, Protection in biotechnology, Protection of othe...
 
1.4 modern child centered education - mahatma gandhi-2.pptx
1.4 modern child centered education - mahatma gandhi-2.pptx1.4 modern child centered education - mahatma gandhi-2.pptx
1.4 modern child centered education - mahatma gandhi-2.pptx
 
Embracing GenAI - A Strategic Imperative
Embracing GenAI - A Strategic ImperativeEmbracing GenAI - A Strategic Imperative
Embracing GenAI - A Strategic Imperative
 
Chapter 3 - Islamic Banking Products and Services.pptx
Chapter 3 - Islamic Banking Products and Services.pptxChapter 3 - Islamic Banking Products and Services.pptx
Chapter 3 - Islamic Banking Products and Services.pptx
 
Guidance_and_Counselling.pdf B.Ed. 4th Semester
Guidance_and_Counselling.pdf B.Ed. 4th SemesterGuidance_and_Counselling.pdf B.Ed. 4th Semester
Guidance_and_Counselling.pdf B.Ed. 4th Semester
 
How to Make a Field invisible in Odoo 17
How to Make a Field invisible in Odoo 17How to Make a Field invisible in Odoo 17
How to Make a Field invisible in Odoo 17
 
TESDA TM1 REVIEWER FOR NATIONAL ASSESSMENT WRITTEN AND ORAL QUESTIONS WITH A...
TESDA TM1 REVIEWER  FOR NATIONAL ASSESSMENT WRITTEN AND ORAL QUESTIONS WITH A...TESDA TM1 REVIEWER  FOR NATIONAL ASSESSMENT WRITTEN AND ORAL QUESTIONS WITH A...
TESDA TM1 REVIEWER FOR NATIONAL ASSESSMENT WRITTEN AND ORAL QUESTIONS WITH A...
 
Home assignment II on Spectroscopy 2024 Answers.pdf
Home assignment II on Spectroscopy 2024 Answers.pdfHome assignment II on Spectroscopy 2024 Answers.pdf
Home assignment II on Spectroscopy 2024 Answers.pdf
 
2024.06.01 Introducing a competency framework for languag learning materials ...
2024.06.01 Introducing a competency framework for languag learning materials ...2024.06.01 Introducing a competency framework for languag learning materials ...
2024.06.01 Introducing a competency framework for languag learning materials ...
 
How libraries can support authors with open access requirements for UKRI fund...
How libraries can support authors with open access requirements for UKRI fund...How libraries can support authors with open access requirements for UKRI fund...
How libraries can support authors with open access requirements for UKRI fund...
 

HMSV 346 Diversity Issues in Human Services Spring 2021

  • 1. HMSV 346 Diversity Issues in Human Services / Spring 2021 1/2 (George Floyd) Advocacy Project Objective: Students will understand the importance of advocacy and develop the attitude, knowledge, and skills needed to effectively advocate on behalf of oppressed or marginalized groups. Background: A few months ago, George Floyd died when four Minneapolis police officers were trying to subdue and arrest him. His death, which was caught on video, raised the now common concerns about police brutality towards African Americans, and ignited protests, unrest, and debate throughout the country. Personal and Professional Relevance: Such incidents (the death of Floyd and others) and subsequent chain of events impact you in two distinct ways: • As an individual bound by your personal identities, thoughts, values, etc.,
  • 2. • As a soon-to-be human service professional bound by a professional code of ethics. While your reactions to these events as an individual are quite likely to be emotional and subjective, it is essential that as a human service professional you maintain objectivity in understanding and addressing these events. The primary objective of this project is to develop fundamental skills necessary to advocate on behalf of an oppressed or marginalized group. Successful completion of this project/assignment requires you to answer the following questions and complete the following parts: Part I 1. What is your personal reaction, thoughts, feelings, etc. about such incidents? 2. There are various perspectives about who is responsible for such incidents: a. Some claim that incidents such as these are caused by those (some African Americans) who violate the law or disobey the lawful orders of cops. b. Others claim that such situations are caused by those racists (some cops) who are ready to inflict violence on African Americans at the slightest pretext or for no reason whatsoever. c. Some believe that such incidents happen because of a
  • 3. combination of both (a) and (b). d. A few believe that it is neither a, b, nor c, but there are other or more reasons for why such incidents happen. What is your personal opinion about the reason(s) for such incidents? Choose only one of the above (a, b, c, or d) and elaborate. Part II 3. Are such incidents relevant to human services practice with clients? In other words, are incidents such as these likely to impact certain groups of clients? • If yes, then elaborate upon these impacts/consequences. • If no, then provide a strong justification for your answer. 4. Human service professionals are required to consider the National Organization of Human Services’ ethical standards in professional decision making. Identify which ethical standards may apply to understanding and addressing such incidents (Hint: Check out the standards pertaining to Responsibility to the Public and Society). Provide a brief justification as to why you believe these standards may apply to such incidents. 5. What steps would you take as a human service practitioner to
  • 4. ensure that people-in-power (police commissioners, lawmakers, funders, community leaders, etc.) take appropriate action to prevent such incidents? • Ideally, your response to this question (5) are influenced solely by the NOHS ethical standards. If you believe that the standards are not sufficient enough to address such incidents, then state so and clarify what would be your course of action as a human service professional to ensure that people- in-power do what is needed. HMSV 346 Diversity Issues in Human Services / Spring 2021 2/2 • Ideally, your response to this question (5) is based on what you believe to be the reason for such incidents (so keep in mind your choice for question 2), but this is not a requirement. So if you do not want to be restricted by your choice for question 2, then that is acceptable. • Clearly specify what needs to change or what the change would look like.
  • 5. Part III 6. Identify a person (example: member of congress, U.S. senator, governor, state legislator, police chief, CSB manager, county supervisor, human services director, etc.) who has the power to make the changes you seek. • The person(s) you identify does not have to be specific to Hampton Roads, Minneapolis, or where you live. You can choose any person anywhere in the country. • Permission from the course instructor is required before selecting a person/persons for advocacy. • Send an “advocacy email” to this identified person. o In this email, briefly state the importance of the issue (why things need to change) and how the change would look like. o Use the NAMI toolkit to develop your email, which should be to the point. o It is highly recommended that you cite credible journal articles, books, reports, organizations, etc., in support of your position. o Submission of Part III in Blackboard requires posting a copy of your advocacy email and a snapshot of a confirmation of receipt of your advocacy email. Not all recipients will provide confirmation of receipt, so a snapshot of your advocacy email
  • 6. (taken before you send it!) will suffice in lieu of a confirmation of receipt. o Sample advocacy emails are available in the “Advocacy Project” folder under “Assignments & Submissions” on Blackboard. Part IV 7. Reflection: a. Was your advocacy effort effective? In other words, did it bring about the change you were seeking? (Do not exaggerate! You will lose points if you do). Share the reasons for your answer. b. What did you learn about the importance of advocacy? Do you see it as an effective tool? c. What would you do differently in the future to be a more effective advocate? 8. Submit your reflection (answers to question 7 above) on Blackboard along with parts II and III (not part I). 9. Respond meaningfully to the postings of three of your classmates. State what you liked about their advocacy effort and why, and state what would you do differently and why. [Important: Please do not scold or humiliate anyone for their opinion about this issue or what they want to change. Very politely state your disagreement, if any, by simply saying what you would do differently and why].
  • 7. Note • This assignment is worth 50 points. Parts I & II are worth 10 points each. Parts III & IV are worth 15 points each. • Your answers to all of the above questions (excluding responses to classmates in discussion board) must be at least 2 pages or 1000 words long, but should not exceed 5 pages or 2500 words. • Use only single-spaced lines. • APA style is not required, but all parts and your advocacy email must be professionally written and formatted (ex: Aligned left, free from grammatical, compositional, and spelling errors, etc.) • Citations will add value to your submissions especially to your advocacy email. If citations are used, then references are required. ARTICLE Exercise enhances motor skill learning by neurotransmitter switching in the adult midbrain Hui-quan Li 1,2✉ & Nicholas C. Spitzer 1,2✉ Physical exercise promotes motor skill learning in normal individuals and those with neu- rological disorders but its mechanism of action is unclear. We find that one week of voluntary
  • 8. wheel running enhances the acquisition of motor skills in normal adult mice. One week of running also induces switching from ACh to GABA expression in neurons in the caudal pedunculopontine nucleus (cPPN). Consistent with regulation of motor skills, we show that the switching neurons make projections to the substantia nigra (SN), ventral tegmental area (VTA) and ventrolateral-ventromedial nuclei of the thalamus (VL-VM). Use of viral vectors to override transmitter switching blocks the beneficial effect of running on motor skill learning. We suggest that neurotransmitter switching provides the basis by which sustained running benefits motor skill learning, presenting a target for clinical treatment of movement disorders. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41467-020-16053-7 OPEN 1 Neurobiology Section, Division of Biological Sciences and Center for Neural Circuits and Behavior, La Jolla, CA 92093- 0357, USA. 2 Kavli Institute for Brain and Mind, University of California San Diego, La Jolla, CA 92093-0357, USA. ✉email: [email protected]; [email protected] NATURE COMMUNICATIONS | (2020) 11:2195 | https://doi.org/10.1038/s41467-020-16053-7 |
  • 9. www.nature.com/naturecommunications 1 12 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 0 () :,; http://crossmark.crossref.org/dialog/?doi=10.1038/s41467-020- 16053-7&domain=pdf http://crossmark.crossref.org/dialog/?doi=10.1038/s41467-020- 16053-7&domain=pdf http://crossmark.crossref.org/dialog/?doi=10.1038/s41467-020- 16053-7&domain=pdf http://crossmark.crossref.org/dialog/?doi=10.1038/s41467-020- 16053-7&domain=pdf http://orcid.org/0000-0001-9552-2191 http://orcid.org/0000-0001-9552-2191 http://orcid.org/0000-0001-9552-2191 http://orcid.org/0000-0001-9552-2191 http://orcid.org/0000-0001-9552-2191 http://orcid.org/0000-0002-3523-1103 http://orcid.org/0000-0002-3523-1103 http://orcid.org/0000-0002-3523-1103 http://orcid.org/0000-0002-3523-1103 http://orcid.org/0000-0002-3523-1103 mailto:[email protected] mailto:[email protected] www.nature.com/naturecommunications www.nature.com/naturecommunications
  • 10. M otor skill learning is fundamental to everyday life and is regulated by a neural network involving the cortex, thalamus, basal ganglia, brain stem, cerebellum, and spinal cord1,2. Both neuronal and glial plasticity are essential for motor skill learning and disruption of this plasticity causes motor deficits3,4. Aerobic physical exercise promotes the abil ity to acquire new motor skills5 and serves as a therapy for many motor disorders6–8, but its basis of action is not well understood. Run- ning is a natural motor activity for mice9 and generates plasticity in multiple brain regions4,10,11. Neurotransmitter switching is a newly appreciated form of plasticity that refers to the ability of neurons to change their transmitter identity in response to sus- tained stimuli, typically leading to changes in behavior12. We hypothesized that chronic running induces neurotransmitter switching in a circuit that is important for motor skill learning. We find that mice that have run on a wheel for a week have an enhanced ability to acquire motor skill on the rotarod and balance beam. This enhancement in learning is temporally cor- related with an activity-dependent transmitter switch of midbrain cholinergic neurons that now express GABA. The change in transmitters corresponds to changes in the levels of transcripts of their transmitter synthetic enzymes. The switching neurons
  • 11. project to nuclei that regulate motor skill learning, including the SN, VTA, and VL-VM. Overriding the switch using viral tools to restore the loss of the synthetic enzyme for ACh or prevent the gain of the synthetic enzyme for GABA blocks enhancement of the ability to acquire motor skills. Results Sustained running enhances motor skill learning. We investi - gated the impact of chronic voluntary running on motor skill learning by exposing adult mice to running wheels for one week (Fig. 1a). Each mouse spent consistent time on the wheel every Ctrl 0 10 20 30 40 50 1 2 3 1 2 3 Ctrl Run Test Run 1-wk Rest 1-wk Retest
  • 12. Retest Retest Rest 2-wkRun 1-wk Run 1-wk Rest 4-wk Test Test ** Rest Rotarod S p e e d a t fa ll (R P M )
  • 13. 0 20 40 60 80 1 wk 2 wks 4 wks0 h **** Ctrl Run + reste d S p e e d a t fa ll (R P M
  • 14. ) 0 20 60 40 80 *** Rotarod Ctrl Run a D7 D8 D9 R R&B B R: Rotarod; B: Balance beams D1 Start to run Run g Ctrl Run Ctrl Run b
  • 16. 1.5 2.0 2.5 Wheel running 80 0 160 240 400 320 D1 D7 D1 D7 *** *** 5 RPM 80 RPM 6 min Balance beam ** ** ***
  • 17. Trial Test 4 mm rod T im e t o c ro ss t h e b e a m ( s) f Ti m e t o c
  • 20. j k Beam T im e t o c ro ss ( s) Rest 1 wk 2 wks 4 wks0 0 10 20 30 40 50 ** ** ** i Ctrl Run + rest
  • 22. e t o c ro ss ( s) 30 0 10 20 50 4060 p = 0.41 p = 0.67 p = 0.77 0 20 40 60 80
  • 23. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 1 2 3 Ctrl Run Rest 1-wkRun 1-wk Training Training Enhanced motor skill learning Enhanced performance continues Rest 2-wk No enhancement or Fig. 1 Sustained running enhances motor skill learning. a Top: Runner mice were housed with running wheels and control mice with wheelbases. Bottom: timeline for running and behavioral tests. b Mean speed (left, n = 6 animals per group) and mean episode duration (right, n = 7 animals per group) of running on day 1 (naive) or day 7 (1-week trained). c Speed at fall on an accelerative rotarod of each trial during training or each test the day after training. d Mean speed at fall on this rotarod in three tests on the day after training. e Time to cross a 1-m long, 4-mm diameter rod balance beam during each trial of training or each test the day after training. f Mean time to
  • 24. cross this balance beam in three tests the day after training. For c–f, n = 19 Ctrls and 20 Runners. g Timelines for immediate behavioral testing and retesting. h, i Mice that had run for 1 week or non-runner controls were tested with (h) rotarod and (i) balance beam (4 mm rod). After rest for 1, 2 or 4 weeks, mice were retested. For h, n = 12 animals per group for 0 and 1 week, 7 Ctrls and 6 Runners for 2 weeks, and 7 per group for 4 weeks. For i, n = 12 animals per group for 0 week, 10 for each group for 1 week, 7 per each group for 2 and 4 weeks. j Top: timelines for delayed behavioral testing. Bottom: performances on rotarod (left) and balance beam (right, 4 mm rod) after 1 week of running followed by 1 week of rest compared to controls that never ran. n = 8 animals for Ctrl and 9 for Run+Rest. k Time dependence of enhanced motor skill learning. When motor skills are trained (green) immediately after 1 week of running, runner mice show enhanced learning. Enhancement of acquired motor skills is sustained for at least two weeks. When motor skills are trained (red) after 1 week of running followed by 1 week of rest, no enhancement in learning is observed. Statistical significance *p < 0.05, **p < 0.01, ***p < 0.001 was assessed by two-sided paired t-test (b) or two-sided Welch’s t-test (other panels). Data shown are mean ± SEM. ARTICLE NATURE COMMUNICATIONS | https://doi.org/10.1038/s41467-020-16053-7 2 NATURE COMMUNICATIONS | (2020) 11:2195 | https://doi.org/10.1038/s41467-020-16053-7 | www.nature.com/naturecommunications www.nature.com/naturecommunications
  • 25. day, indicating continuous interest, and their running skill improved as measured by increased running speed, increased duration of running episodes and more stable movement on the wheel (Fig. 1b and Supplementary Fig. 1a, b). At the end of the running period we assessed performance on the accelerating rotarod and narrow balance beam to evaluate motor skill acqui - sition13–15. In comparison to mice without running wheels, mice that ran for one week demonstrated enhanced learning of motor skills, mastering an accelerating rotarod more rapidly and accommodating to balance beams more quickly (Fig. 1c–f and Supplementary Fig. 1c, d). We calculated the slopes of learning curves to assess the speed of motor skill learning. Mean data points for each training trial were plotted and fitted and the coefficient of determination (R2) was used to justify the fit. The mean speed at fall from a rotarod for the nine trials on the training day was fitted by a one-phase association model (R2 > 0.96 for controls; R2 > 0.95 for runners). yðtÞ ¼ y0 þ ðA � y0Þ 1 � e�kt � � ð1Þ y(t) is the speed at fall on trial number t, y0 is the speed at fall for the first trial (t = 0), A is the plateau and k is a rate constant.The slopes were calculated with y0ðtÞ ¼ dy dt
  • 26. ¼ ke�ktðA � y0Þ ð2Þ To calculate the initial slope, y′(0), we used y0ð0Þ ¼ g k; Að Þ ¼ kðA � y0Þ ð3Þ The standard errors were calculated with ∂g ∂k ¼ ðA � y0Þ ð4Þ ∂g ∂A ¼ k ð5Þ δg ¼ ffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffi ffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffi ∂g ∂k δk � �2 þ ∂g ∂A δA � �2 s ¼ ffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffi ffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffi ffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffi
  • 27. A � y0ð Þ2δk2 þ k2δA2 q ð6Þ The values of the slopes are presented as g ± δg (mean ± sem). Initial slopes were 17 ± 4 rpm/trial for runners and 5 ± 2 rpm/trial for controls, significantly steeper for the runners (p = 0.01, Welch’s t-test) (Fig. 1c). The mean time to cross a 4 mm rod beam for the three trials on the training day was fitted by linear regression (R2 > 0.98 for controls; R2 > 0.99 for runners) yðtÞ ¼ y0 þ kt ð7Þ y(t) is the time to cross the beam on trial number t, y0 is the time for the first trial (t = 0) and k is the slope y0ðtÞ ¼ k ð8Þ The values of the slopes are presented as k ± δk (mean ± sem). The slopes were −4.7 ± 0.5 s/trial for runners and −2.4 ± 0.3 s/trial for controls, again significantly steeper for runners (p < 0.001, Welch’s t-test) (Fig. 1e). Additionally, sustained running improved rotorod and balance beam test performance (Fig. 1d, f and Supplementary Fig. 1d) but did not affect basal locomotor activity as measured by infrared beam crossings in home cages (Supplementary Fig. 1e, f). The steeper slopes of the behavioral acquisition curves demon-
  • 28. strate enhanced learning relative to the controls, in accordance with previous studies16,17. If the change in behavior induced by wheel running were one of performance, these curves would be parallel and the slopes would be equal. To explore further the effect of running on enhancement of motor skill learning, we removed running wheels from mouse cages after the first motor skill tests and retested the mice after different resting periods (Fig. 1g). Enhanced performance on both rotarod and balance beam was sustained up to 2 weeks but not 4 weeks (Fig. 1h,i). Moreover, when mice were not trained on the rotarod and balance beam until 1 week after running, their motor skill learning was not enhanced (Fig. 1j). This result suggests that running creates a sensitive period in the adult brain during which motor skill learning is improved and shows that the gain in motor skills persists longer than the duration of this period (Fig. 1k). Running induces transmitter switching in the midbrain cPPN. Because transmitter switching is activity-dependent18–20, we first searched for c-fos expression (Fig. 2a) to determine sites of increased brain activity associated with running and to identify neurons likely to switch their transmitter. Mice that ran for 1 week exhibited a six-fold increase in the number of c-fos+ neurons in the pedunculopontine nucleus (PPN) of the midbrain compared to non-runner controls when examined immediately after the end of the last running period (Fig. 2b, c). The dentate gyrus also showed increased c-fos expression in runners, as
  • 29. expected21. The PPN was an attractive candidate for running- induced transmitter switching because it regulates gait and bal - ance control in health22,23 and disease24. However, the rostral and caudal PPN (rPPN and cPPN) are distinct. Both contain gluta- matergic, GABAergic and cholinergic neurons, but there are important differences in the proportions of their transmitter phenotypes25, their projection targets and activity during movement23,26, and their roles in behavior27,28. Accordingly we looked for differences in the activation of the rPPN and cPPN. Specifically, we asked whether running activates cholinergic neurons (marked by choline acetyltransferase, ChAT, which synthesizes ACh) differently in the rPPN and cPPN, because these neurons are involved in the gait and postural disorders of Parkinson’s disease29. One week of running increased the number of c-fos+ neurons in both rPPN and cPPN (Supplementary Fig. 2a, b). However, running increased the percentage of c- fos+ neurons in the ChAT+ neuron population by 22-fold (from 21 of 984 to 315 of 670) in the cPPN but increased the percentage by only two-fold (from 62 of 347 to 127 of 321) in the rPPN (Fig. 2d, e, the left panel of 2f, and Supplementary Fig. 2c). Moreover, the percentage of ChAT+ neurons in the c-fos+ neuron population was not different between controls and runners in the rPPN (62 of 309 vs. 127 of 644) but increased six-fold from controls to runners in the cPPN (from 21 of 261 to 315 of 673) (Fig. 2f, right). Significantly, running increased the number of cfos+ non- cholinergic (ChAT-) neurons in the cPPN by only 1.5-fold (from 240 to 358), much less than the 15-fold (21–315) increase in the number of c-fos+ ChAT+ neurons (Supplementary Fig. 2c).
  • 30. These results show that cPPN cholinergic neurons are more strongly activated by sustained running than rPPN cholinergic neurons or cPPN non-cholinergic neurons, identifying cPPN cholinergic neurons as candidates for transmitter switching. The increase in the proportion of c-fos+ neurons in the ChAT+ cPPN neurons occurred after as early as 3 days of running (Supplementary Fig. 2d, e). Indeed, chronic running for one week was accompanied by a decrease in the number of cPPN neurons expressing both ChAT (605 ± 19; Fig. 2g, h) and the vesicular acetylcholine transporter (VAChT; 459 ± 51; Supplementary Fig. 2i). This change was accompanied by an equal increase in the number of cPPN neurons expressing the gene encoding glutamic acid decarbox- ylase (GAD1; 551 ± 95), the enzyme that generates GABA (Fig. 2i, j). No neurogenesis or apoptosis was observed in the cPPN of either control or runner mice (Supplementary Fig. 3a–f). These results suggest that ~600 cPPN neurons switched their transmitter from ACh to GABA. There was no NATURE COMMUNICATIONS | https://doi.org/10.1038/s41467-020-16053-7 ARTICLE NATURE COMMUNICATIONS | (2020) 11:2195 | https://doi.org/10.1038/s41467-020-16053-7 | www.nature.com/naturecommunications 3 www.nature.com/naturecommunications www.nature.com/naturecommunications change in the number of ChAT+ neurons in the rPPN or the adjacent lateral dorsotegmental nucleus and no difference in the number of neurons expressing the vesicular glutamate transpor - ter 2 (vGluT2) in the cPPN (Supplementary Fig. 3g–l).
  • 31. To determine whether cholinergic cPPN activity plays a role in the running-induced ACh-to-GABA switch, we expressed Kir2.1 inward-rectifier potassium channels specifically in cPPN choli - nergic neurons to suppress their activity20 and examined the impact on running-induced gain of GAD1 and loss of ChAT. We used a well-characterized ChAT-Cre transgenic mouse line30,31 that exhibited the same running-dependent loss of ChAT and gain of GAD1 expression as wild-type mice (Supplementary Fig. 2f). We found that runner mice that had received AAV- DIO- Kir2.1 showed no difference in the number of ChAT+ or GAD1+ neurons in the cPPN vs non-runner control mice and were significantly different from the runner mice that received a control AAV construct (Supplementary Fig. 2g, h). These results suggest that cholinergic cPPN activity is required for transmitter switching. Because one-third of cholinergic cPPN neurons lose ChAT, the larger fold change in c-fos expression in the cholinergic population of the cPPN compared to the rPPN (Fig. 2d, e) is caused by both an increase of c-fos and decrease of ChAT expression. Considering that two-thirds of cholinergic cPPN neurons do not switch their transmitter, the predominant increase in c-fos expression in ChAT+ neurons (Supplementary Fig. 2c) is consistent with cell-population-autonomous and non- cell-autonomous activity-dependence of transmitter switching previously reported19. Mice that had run for 1 week, not subjected to motor skill training, and allowed 1 week of rest now exhibited the same number of ChAT+ and GAD1+ neurons as control mice that had never run on a running wheel (Fig. 2k). No apoptosis or
  • 32. neurogenesis was detected in the cPPN of these mice (Supplementary Fig. 3c–f), indicating that the transmitter switch had spontaneously reversed. The time during which the transmitter switch reversed (Fig. 2k) corresponds to the time during which the benefit of running on motor skill acquisition disappeared (Fig. 1j, k), indicating a temporal correlation between transmitter switching and the ability for enhanced motor skill learning. This finding raised the possibility that the transmitter switch is necessary for running to enhance motor skill learning. Note that the transmitter switch had reversed one week after running (Supplementary Fig. 2j), even when training and testing on the rotorod and balance beam occurred immediately after running, while the acquired motor skills persisted for at least 2 weeks (Fig. 1g–i). These results suggest that the transmitter switch is not necessary for maintaining the acquired motor skills. cPPNrPPN * cPPNrPPN c- fo s+ /C h A T + (
  • 33. % ) * p = 0.99 p = 0.09 60 0 20 80 40 60 0 20 80 40 1000 G A D
  • 36. GPe STN Glu(+) GABA(–) Dopamine M1 M2 b c Runner scp scp PPN PPN Control c-fos NeuN c-fos NeuN c-fos D M g h Control Runner G A D 1
  • 40. fo s+ c e ll co u n ts p e r m m 2 p = 0.66 p = 0.93 cPPN DG M1 M2 STN GPe SNc SNr PPN LDT p = 0.25 p = 0.18 p = 0.33
  • 41. p = 0.41 * p = 0.13 cPPN RunCtrl RunCtrl Run + RestCtrl p = 0.07 Fig. 2 Running activates the pedunculopontine nucleus (PPN) and triggers neurotransmitter switching in the caudal PPN. a Motor circuitry screened for activity. b Double staining of neuronal marker NeuN and activity marker c-fos in a control (upper) and 1-week runner (lower). Middle and right panels are higher magnifications of the boxes in the left panels. Scp, superior cerebellar peduncle. Dorsal (D) and medial (M) axes are shown at top. Scale bar, (left) 200 μm, (middle) 50 μm. c c-fos immunoreactive neuron number in each region or nucleus of controls and 1-week runner mice. DG, dentate gyrus; M1, primary motor cortex; M2, secondary motor cortex; STN, subthalamic nucleus; GPe, globus pallidus external; STR, striatum; SNc, substantia nigra pars compacta; SNr, substantia nigra pars reticulata; PPN, pedunculopontine nucleus; LDT, laterodorsal tegmental nucleus. For b, c, n = 12 sections from 4 animals per group. d, e Double staining of ChAT and c-fos in the rostral (d) or caudal (e) PPN of a 1-week runner. White arrowheads identify examples of c-
  • 42. fos+ChAT+ cells. Scale bar, 100 μm. f Percentage of the c- fos+ChAT+ neurons in the total ChAT+ neurons (left) or in the total c-fos+ neurons (right). For d–f, n = 3 animals per group. g 3,3′-diaminobenzidine (DAB) staining of ChAT in the cPPNs of a control and 1-week runner. Dotted lines outline the PPN. Dark brown stain identifies ChAT+ neurons. Scale bar, 100 μm. h Stereological counts of g. n = 6 animals per group. i In situ hybridization of GAD1 in the cPPN of a control and 1-week runner. Scale bar, 100 μm. j Stereological counts of i. n = 7 animals per group. k Stereological counts of ChAT+ or GAD1+ neurons in the cPPN of mice that experienced 1 week of running followed by 1 week of rest and of control mice that never ran on a running wheel. n = 5 animals per group. Statistical significance *p < 0.05, **p < 0.01 was assessed by two-sided Welch’s t-test (c, f) or Mann– Whitney U test (other panels). Data shown are mean ± SEM. ARTICLE NATURE COMMUNICATIONS | https://doi.org/10.1038/s41467-020-16053-7 4 NATURE COMMUNICATIONS | (2020) 11:2195 | https://doi.org/10.1038/s41467-020-16053-7 | www.nature.com/naturecommunications www.nature.com/naturecommunications The number of neurons expressing ChAT was inversely correlated with the number of neurons expressing GAD1 and directly correlated with the enhanced learning of motor skills (Supplementary Fig. 4a–e), suggesting that the level of transmitter switch determines the enhancement level of motor skill
  • 43. learning. The amount of running was not correlated with the motor skill acquisition (Supplementary Fig. 4f–i), perhaps because the variation in running duration is small (coefficient of variation, 0.17). Running is a categorical rather than a continuous variable and enables individual variability in transmitter switching and motor skill learning. To seek more direct evidence for neurotransmitter switching, we selectively tagged cholinergic neurons with genetic markers to reveal their change in transmitter identity following the exercise challenge. We injected a Cre-dependent AAV vector (AAV- DIO- mRuby2) into the cPPN of ChAT-Cre mice to permanently label cholinergic neurons with mRuby2, including those that subse- quently lose ChAT after running (Fig. 3a, b). We then scored the number of mRuby2+ neurons that express ChAT and/or GABA immunofluorescence. The anti-GABA antibodies were validated in the superior colliculus where immunostaining of GAD67 identifies cell bodies (Supplementary Fig. 5). The colocalization between GABA and GAD67 demonstrates the specificity of the anti-GABA antibodies to detect GABAergic neurons. Co- labeling of mRuby2, ChAT and GABA revealed that in control mice, 65% (548/844) of cPPN cholinergic neurons tagged by mRuby2 expressed only ChAT, while 29% (245/844) of them co- expressed ChAT and GABA, 4% (34/844) expressed neither and 2% (17/ 844) expressed only GABA (Fig. 3c). In runners, 24% (177/738) of mRuby2+ neurons expressed only ChAT, 31% (229/738) co- expressed ChAT and GABA, 12% (89/738) expressed neither and 33% (243/738) expressed only GABA (Fig. 3c). The decrease in
  • 44. neurons expressing only ChAT and increase in neurons expressing only GABA in the ChAT-Cre line identify the expression of GABA in formerly cholinergic neurons. The increase in the number of neurons that express neither ChAT nor GABA suggests that switching neurons lose ChAT before gaining GABA. The apparent constancy of the percent of co- expressing neurons may indicate that switching and co- expressing populations are distinct. Transcript levels of transmitter synthetic enzymes change. To understand whether the loss of ChAT and gain of GAD1 occur in the same population of neurons at the transcript level, we used sensitive fluorescence in situ hybridization to analyze the levels of mRNA of transmitter synthetic enzymes in cPPN neurons from runners and controls. We used the expression of neuronal nitric oxide synthase (nNOS) as a biomarker restricted to ChAT+ neurons in the PPN32 (Fig. 4a, b). Although the number of ChAT+ neurons decreased with one week of running (Fig. 2g, h), the number of nNOS+ neurons did not change (Fig. 4c). We then combined immunofluorescent labeling of nNOS with RNAscope to detect mRNA encoding ChAT and GAD67 in nNOS+ neu- rons. By measuring the number of transcript puncta (Fig. 4d–g) and the total fluorescent area and fluorescence intensity per cell (Supplementary Fig. 6), we demonstrated a decrease in the number of ChAT transcripts and an increase in the number of GAD1 transcripts in neurons expressing nNOS in runners. While RNAscope revealed the presence of ChAT transcripts in all nNOS neurons in controls (Fig. 4e), immunostaining identified 11% of nNOS neurons that did not show detectable ChAT immunor- eactivity (Fig. 4b). These results imply that in nNOS neurons of
  • 45. controls, the bottom 11% of the distribution of ChAT mRNA puncta (Fig. 4e; expressing <8 puncta) lack detectable ChAT immunostaining. We next grouped neurons into the four cate- gories (as in Fig. 3) on the basis of ChAT and GAD1 mRNA expression (see Methods). In control mice, 45% of the cPPN nNOS+ cells were classic cholinergic neurons, while 44% of them co-expressed ChAT and GAD1, 6% expressed neither and 5% expressed GAD1 (Fig. 4e). However, in runners, the percentages changed to 18% for classic cholinergic neurons, 43% for co- expressing cells, 12% for neither and 27% that expressed GAD1 (Fig. 4e). These changes in the four categories are comparable to those identified by immunocytochemistry (Fig. 3c), further sup- porting a running-induced switch from ChAT to GAD1 in nNOS neurons. More modest co-expression of ChAT and GAD tran- scripts has been observed in the adult rodent PPN33–35; the dif- ference is likely to result from different ways to detect and score a positive neuron using thresholding probes (see Methods). The RNAscope assay reveals that switching involves up and down regulation of transcript levels and may not entail complete GABA mRuby2ChAT MERGE Run Ctrl CtrlCtrl * Classic Neither Switched **
  • 47. Ctrl Run Ctrl Run Run Run ** Co-expressing a b c AAV-hSyn-DIO-mRuby2 4-wk virus expression ChAT-IRES-Cre 1-wk run Start running GABA-IHC ChAT-IHC GABA mRuby2ChAT p = 0.99 MERGE Fig. 3 A subset of cholinergic cPPN neurons lose ChAT and gain GABA. a Experimental strategy to permanently label cholinergic cPPN neurons with mRuby2 fluorescent protein by expressing Cre-dependent AAV- DIO-mRuby2 in ChAT-Cre mice and determine whether the loss of ChAT and gain of
  • 48. GABA occur in mRuby2+ neurons. The coronal brain section was drawn according to the Franklin & Paxinos brain atlas57. b Double immunostaining of GABA and ChAT in the cPPN of a control and a 1-week runner ChAT-Cre mouse expressing AAV-DIO-mRuby2 in the cPPN. Purple arrows, mRuby2 neurons that express ChAT but not GABA (classic ChAT neurons). Gray arrows, mRuby2 neurons that express both ChAT and GABA (co-expressing neurons). Yellow arrows, mRuby2 neurons that express GABA but not ChAT (switched neurons). Cyan arrow, a mRuby2 neuron that expresses neither GABA nor ChAT. Scale bar, 50 μm. c The percentage of each class of mRuby2+ neurons (n = 844 for controls and 738 cells for runners). For b, c, n = 3 animals per group. Statistical significance *p < 0.05, **p < 0.01 was assessed by Mann–Whitney U test. Data shown are mean ± SEM. NATURE COMMUNICATIONS | https://doi.org/10.1038/s41467-020-16053-7 ARTICLE NATURE COMMUNICATIONS | (2020) 11:2195 | https://doi.org/10.1038/s41467-020-16053-7 | www.nature.com/naturecommunications 5 www.nature.com/naturecommunications www.nature.com/naturecommunications disappearance and de novo appearance of transcripts of trans- mitter synthetic enzymes. Switching neurons project to the SN, VTA, and thalamus. To further test the link between neurotransmitter switching and motor skill learning, we identified targets innervated by the
  • 49. switching cholinergic neurons. PPN neurons project to the sub- stantia nigra (SN), the ventral tegmental area (VTA) and the ventrolateral-ventromedial nuclei of the thalamus (VL-VM), all of which regulate motor skill learning36–41. Anterograde tracing of mRuby2 in ChAT-Cre neurons (see Methods) followed by ret- rograde tracing with retrobeads demonstrated synaptic connec- tions between cholinergic cPPN neurons and neurons in the SN, VTA and VL-VM (Fig. 5a–c and Supplementary Fig. 7a–c). cPPN neurons originating projections to all three targets had lower mean numbers of ChAT transcript puncta in runners compared to controls, as demonstrated by triple labeling of retrograde beads, ChAT transcripts and nNOS protein (Fig. 5d–f and Sup- plementary Fig. 7d, e). As noted above, nNOS neurons expressing <8 puncta appear to be those lacking ChAT immunoreactivity. In controls, the percentages of neurons that express less than 8 ChAT transcript puncta and project to the SN, VTA, and VL- VM were 11%, 8 and 6%, and increased to 42%, 11 and 19% in runners. Neurons in the increased percentages of cells expressing low numbers of ChAT-transcript puncta are likely to include those that switch transmitters. The greater increase in number of the low-transcript neurons projecting to the SN (37 for Run vs. 9 for Ctrl) suggests that cPPN neurons switching transmitters make a major projection to the SN with smaller projections to the VTA (10 for Run vs. 7 for Ctrl) and VL-VM (15 for Run vs. 5 for Ctrl) (Fig. 5f).
  • 50. To determine whether running up-regulates the level of vesicular GABA transporter at the cholinergic cPPN terminals in the SN, we injected an AAV vector expressing Cre-dependent synaptophysin fused with EGFP (AAV8-phSyn1(S)-FLEX-tdTo- mato-T2A-SypEGFP-WPRE) into the cPPN of ChAT-Cre mice to label the cholinergic cPPN terminals. Immunostaining of the cPPN with anti-tdTomato and anti-ChAT antibodies demon- strated expression of the construct in the cholinergic cPPN neurons (Supplementary Fig. 7f). Immunostaining the SN with anti-VGAT or anti-VAChT antibodies identified an increase in the number of EGFP+ putative terminals labeled with VGAT and a decrease in those labeled with VAChT in runners compared to control mice (Fig. 5g–j). These results suggest that the ACh-to- GABA switch observed in cPPN neuronal cell bodies extends to their presynaptic terminals. Enhanced motor skill learning requires transmitter switch. To determine whether neurotransmitter switching is necessary for the beneficial effect of running on motor skill learning, we injected AAV-DIO-ChAT into the cPPN of ChAT-Cre mice to continuously express ChAT in all cholinergic neurons (Fig. 6a, b and Supplementary Fig. 8a–d). The ChAT-Cre line30,31 demon- strated the same running-dependent transmitter switch as wild- type mice, even with expression of control constructs (AAV- DIO- mRuby2, Fig. 6c; AAV-DIO-shScr, Fig. 7b). Overexpression of ChAT did not change the number of ChAT+ neurons in the cPPN of control ChAT-Cre mice and maintained the number of ChAT+ neurons at control levels after sustained running (Fig. 6c). Although the overexpression of ChAT caused a 3-fold increase in the level of ChAT expression in control mice (Fig. 6d
  • 51. and Supplementary Fig. 8c), it did not affect their basal motor skill learning (Fig. 6f, g and Supplementary Fig. 9e, f) or running activity in runner mice (Fig. 6e and Supplementary Fig. 9a, b). This may result from feedback inhibition of ChAT by ACh42,43 that is likely to maintain ACh levels of the non-switching neurons Ctrl Run e Ctrl Run g 10 15 20 0 5 P u n ct a n u
  • 54. ChAT + nNOS 3% 11% nNOS ≥105 GAD1 transcript puncta number T C h A t ra n sc ri p t p u n ct a n u m b e
  • 55. r 0 10 20 30 ≥40 0 2 3 4 6 7 8 91 Ctrl Run f P u n ct a n u m b e r /n e u
  • 56. ro n 0 20 40 60 ChAT Ctrl Run *** 80 MERGE MERGE MERGE MERGE-ZOOM MERGE-ZOOM Fig. 4 A subset of cPPN nNOS neurons of runner mice lose ChAT transcripts and gain GAD1 transcripts. a Double staining of the cPPN in a non-runner control mouse for nNOS (green) and ChAT (red). Scale bar, 100 μm. b Co-localization of nNOS and ChAT in the cPPN. For a, b, n = 1068 cells from three non-runner control mice. c Stereological counts of DAB
  • 57. staining of nNOS in control and 1-week runner cPPNs. n = 6 animals per group. Mann–Whitney U test. NS, not significant. d Triple staining of ChAT and GAD1 transcripts and nNOS protein in the cPPN of a control and a 1- week runner mouse. Right panels are boxed regions in merged images at higher magnification. Scale bar, 20 μm. e Scatterplot of numbers of in situ stained ChAT puncta (y-axis) against numbers of in situ stained GAD1 puncta (x-axis). Each dot represents one neuron. The vertical line divides neurons that contain zero from those containing more GAD1 transcript puncta and the horizontal line divides neurons that contain less from those containing more than eight ChAT transcript puncta. f, g Y-axes are the mean number of ChAT (f) and GAD1 (g) fluorescent puncta in single nNOS+ neurons. For d–g, n = 4 animals per group; n = 123 cells for Ctrl and 137 cells for Run. Statistical significance *p < 0.05, ***p < 0.001 was assessed by two-sided Welch’s t-test. Data shown are mean ± SEM. ARTICLE NATURE COMMUNICATIONS | https://doi.org/10.1038/s41467-020-16053-7 6 NATURE COMMUNICATIONS | (2020) 11:2195 | https://doi.org/10.1038/s41467-020-16053-7 | www.nature.com/naturecommunications www.nature.com/naturecommunications in the physiological range. Mice that had received AAV-DIO- ChAT acquired the same running skill as wild-type mice or ChAT-Cre mice injected with AAV-DIO-mRuby2 but their motor learning on the rotarod and balance beam, tested directly after 1 week of running, was not enhanced (Fig. 6f, g). The
  • 58. slopes of the learning curves for both rotarod and balance beam beha- viors were significantly steeper for runner mice expressing AAV- DIO-mRuby2 (10 ± 2 rpm/trial and −3.9 ± 1.0 s/trial) than for runners expressing the AAV-DIO-ChAT (6 ± 1 rpm/trial, −1.3 ± 0.4 s/trial; p = 0.037 and p = 0.035) and test performances of runners expressing AAV-DIO-mRuby2 were significantly better (Supplementary Fig. 9e, f). Overriding the loss of ChAT also prevented enhancement of motor skill learning when mice ran, were trained and tested, rested for one week and then re-tested (Fig. 6f, g). This finding makes it unlikely that exogenous expression of ChAT had simply delayed the improvement in motor skill learning. Expression of shRNA for GAD1 (AAV-DIO-shGAD1) to suppress the gain of GAD67 in cholinergic cPPN neurons similarly prevented improved motor learning following one week of running, compared to mice expressing a Cre-dependent scrambled shRNA sequence (AAV-DIO-shScr; Fig. 7a–f, Supple- mentary Figs. 8 and 9). Knockdown of GAD1 maintained the number of GAD1+ neurons at control levels in the cPPN after sustained running (Fig. 7b). Because immunostaining of GAD67 in the PPN detects a large number of synaptic puncta that makes Ctrl Run 0.50 0.75 1.00
  • 61. b e r 0 20 40 60 VTA SN VL-VM Ctrl Run Ctrl Run Ctrl Run Beads Merge & nNOSChAT VTA 75% 17% 1% 7% Neither Both Ipsi Contra 9.8%
  • 63. SN cPPN EGFP IHC VAChT or VGAT IHC R u n synaptophysin-EGFP MERGEVAChT C tr l MERGEVGAT synaptophysin-EGFP R u n C tr l h 0 25 75
  • 66. ci e n t M 1 j 50 0.25 Fig. 5 Running reduces the number of ChAT transcripts in a subset of cholinergic cPPN neurons that project to the VTA, SN and VL-VM. a Experimental design to validate cholinergic innervation of target nuclei by the cPPN. Red or green retrobeads (beads) were injected bilaterally into target nuclei with one color per side. Substantia nigra (SN) provides an example. b Triple labeling of retrobeads and ChAT in a coronal section of cPPN in a mouse injected with retrobeads. Scale bar, 50 μm. c Summary of the percentage of cholinergic neurons (ChAT+) that project to corresponding nuclei. Ipsi, ipsilaterally. Contra, contralaterally. Both, both ipsi - and contralaterally. Neither, no retrobeads. n = 3 animals examined per region. n = 829 cells for the VTA, 806 cells for the SN, and 812 for the VL-VM. d Experimental design to identify the target(s) of neurons with decreased numbers of ChAT transcripts. SN is shown as an example. e Triple-labeling of retrobeads, ChAT mRNA transcripts, and nNOS proteins in both control and runner cPPNs. Scale bar, 20 μm. f Y-axis is the mean number of ChAT fluorescent puncta
  • 67. in single nNOS+ cells. Each dot represents one neuron. n = 4 animals per group. n = 89 cells for VTA-Ctrl, 91 for VTA-Run, 81 for SN-Ctrl, 87 for SN- Run, 82 for VL-VM- Ctrl, 80 for VL-VM-Run. g Experimental design to identify presynaptic changes in transporter expression of cholinergic cPPN axons in the SN. AAV8-phSyn1-FLEX-tdTomato-T2A-Syptophysin- EGFP-WPRE (AAV-FLEX- SypEGFP-T2A-TdTomato) was injected into the cPPN of ChAT- Cre mice. Coronal sections containing the SN were immunostained for EGFP and VAChT or EGFP and VGAT. h Double immunostaining of VAChT and EGFP or VGAT and EGFP in the SN of non-runner controls or 1-week runners injected with AAV- FLEX-SypEGFP-T2A-TdTomato. Scale bar, 10 μm. i Percentage of VAChT+ or VGAT+ terminals in putatively cholinergic terminals in the SN. j The coefficient M1 of antibody colocalization with putative cholinergic presynaptic terminals. n = 10139 EGFP+ terminals for VAChT-Ctrl, 9629 for VAChT- Run, 10174 for VGAT-Ctrl and 10611 for VGAT-Run. For each group, data were quantified from 12 z-stack images from three animals. Statistical significance *p < 0.05, **p < 0.01, ***p < 0.001 was assessed by two-sided Welch’s t-test. Data shown are mean ± SEM. NATURE COMMUNICATIONS | https://doi.org/10.1038/s41467-020-16053-7 ARTICLE NATURE COMMUNICATIONS | (2020) 11:2195 | https://doi.org/10.1038/s41467-020-16053-7 | www.nature.com/naturecommunications 7 www.nature.com/naturecommunications www.nature.com/naturecommunications
  • 68. it challenging to analyze cell bodies, the knockdown efficiency of GAD67 expression by shGAD1 was tested in the motor cortex and measured to be 65% (Supplementary Fig. 8e, f). Moreover, shGAD1 blocked the increase in the expression of GABA in cholinergic cPPN neurons of runner mice (Fig. 7c), indicating that GAD1 has the dominant role in regulating the level of GABA in these neurons and that shGAD1 effectively prevents the gain in GABA caused by running. Suppressing the gain of GAD1 did not affect acquisition of the wheel running skill (Fig. 7d and Supplementary Fig. 9c, d) but motor learning on the rotarod and balance beam, tested directly after one week of running, was not enhanced for a period that was extended to one week of rest (Fig. 7e,f). The slopes of the learning curves were again steeper for runner mice expressing AAV-DIO-shScr (9 ± 1 rpm/trial, −6.7 ± 0.9 s/trial) than for runners expressing AAV-DIO-shGAD1 (5 ± 2 rpm/trial, −3.7 ± 1.0 s/trial; p = 0.073 and p = 0.041) and test performances for runners expressing AAV-DIO-shScr were again significantly better (Supplementary Fig. 9g, h). These results suggest that both the loss of ACh and gain of GABA are required for enhancement of motor skill learning. Overriding the loss of ChAT or suppressing the gain of GAD1 did not affect running- induced c-fos expression in cholinergic cPPN neurons (Supple- mentary Fig. 10). We tested for population-level control of the change in expression of one transmitter by overriding the change in expression of the other. Notably, suppressing the gain in
  • 69. GAD1 expression did not affect the loss of ChAT expression and vice versa (Fig. 7g). Although expression of these two transmitter synthetic enzymes is inversely correlated, the two are not reciprocally regulated. Discussion Our findings provide insight into the mechanism by which sus- tained running improves acquisition of motor skills (Fig. 8). The functional significance of ACh-to-GABA transmitter switching is demonstrated by changes in behavior that are reversed by overriding the switch. Switching involves changes in levels of transcripts of transmitter synthetic enzymes. Activity- dependent transcription factor phosphorylation18,19 and micro- RNA regulation44, which have been implicated in transmitter switching in the developing nervous system, are candidates for implementing the switch in the adult CNS. Transmitter switching in the cPPN, particularly when it may change the sign of the synapse from excitatory to inhibitory, appears to rewire motor circuitry to enhance motor skills. Acquisition of motor skills requires many components including balance, coordination, Balance beam T im e t o c ro ss
  • 70. ( s) Immediate test Retest Rotarod Immediate test Retest 80 S p e e d a t fa ll (R P M ) 60 0 20
  • 71. 40 e f g cPPN neuron counts a c Wheel running d 1-wk rest Immediate test 4-wk virus expression ChAT-IRES-Cre 1-wk run Start running AAV-hSyn-DIO-ChAT-flag-P2A-mRuby2 or AAV-hSyn-DIO-mRuby2 Retest ChAT-Run ChAT-Ctrl mRuby2-Run mRuby2-Ctrl b mRuby2
  • 72. nNOS LDT LDTPPN PPN bregma -4.8 AQ mRuby2 DRAQ5 C h A T + in te n si ty r e la tiv e t o n
  • 75. *** * 1000 Day S p e e d ( km /h ) ChAT mRuby2 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 2.0 1.5 1.0 0.5 0.0 0 30
  • 76. 10 20 50 40 ** *** * * * ** * ** p = 0.73 p = 0.95 p = 0.99 p = 0.99 p = 0.96 p = 0.93 p = 0.94 p = 0.97 p = 0.99 p = 0.99 p = 0.86 Fig. 6 Loss of ChAT in cholinergic cPPN neurons is necessary for running-enhanced motor skill learning. a Experimental design to override the loss of ChAT in ChAT-Cre neurons and determine behavioral relevance. The coronal brain section was drawn according to the Franklin and Paxinos brain atlas57. b Top: mRuby2 and nuclear marker DRAQ-5 show mRuby2 expression in a coronal cPPN section of an animal bilaterally injected with AAV-DIO-ChAT- P2A-mRuby2 constructs. Coordinates adapted to Allen Brain Atlas. AQ, aqueduct. LDT, laterodorsal tegmental nucleus. Scale bar, 500 μm. Bottom: double-labeled image of nNOS and mRuby2 in boxed region of the upper image. Scale bar, 200 μm. n = 34 animals. c ChAT+ neuron number in the cPPN of runners and non-running controls that were injected with AAV-DIO- mRuby2 or AAV-DIO-ChAT. n = 5 animals per
  • 77. group. Nonparametric Kruskal–Wallis test followed by Dunn’s correction. d ChAT fluorescence intensity for mRuby2-expressing cells in ChAT- Cre mice injected with AAV-DIO- ChAT or AAV-DIO-mRuby2. Fluorescence intensity was normalized by non-transfected cells (mRuby2 negative). n = 56 and 58 cells for mRuby2 and ChAT. n = 3 animals per group. Welch’s t-test. e Wheel running speed of mice that were injected with AAV-DIO-ChAT or AAV- DIO-mRuby2 at the cPPN. n = 10 animals per group. Linear regression followed by two- sided Welch’s t-test. f, g Rotarod (f) and balance beam (g, 4 mm rod) tests show that expression of mRuby2 did not affect enhancement of acquisition and maintenance of motor skills gained by running (mRuby2- Run vs. mRuby2-Ctrl), whereas exogenous ChAT expression blocked the enhancement (ChAT-Run vs. mRuby2-Run). The blockade was sustained after rest for 1 week. For immediate rotarod test, numbers of animals are 9 for mRuby2- Ctrl, 16 for ChAT-Ctrl, 9 for mRuby2-Run, 18 for ChAT-Run. For rotarod retest, numbers of animals are 9 for mRuby2-Ctrl, 6 for ChAT-Ctrl, 9 for mRuby2- Run, 7 for ChAT-Run. For immediate test of balance beam, numbers of animals are 10 for mRuby2-Ctrl, 9 for ChAT-Ctrl, 10 for mRuby2-Run, 9 for ChAT-Run. For balance beam retest, numbers of animals are 6 for mRuby2-Ctrl, ChAT-Ctrl, mRuby2-Run and 7 for ChAT-Run. ANOVA followed by Tukey’s test. *p < 0.05, **p < 0.01, ***p < 0.001. Data shown are mean ± SEM. ARTICLE NATURE COMMUNICATIONS | https://doi.org/10.1038/s41467-020-16053-7 8 NATURE COMMUNICATIONS | (2020) 11:2195 |
  • 78. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41467-020-16053-7 | www.nature.com/naturecommunications www.nature.com/naturecommunications motivation, attention and muscle strength. Interestingly, the PPN regulates many of them23,45, and transmitter switching in the cPPN may contribute to motor skill learning through regulating one, several or even all of these aspects. The persistence of learned behaviors after the transmitter switch has reversed implies that there is continued capacity for plasticity in locomotor circuitry. We examined three targets of the cPPN and found that cho- linergic cPPN neurons projecting to each of the three targets showed a significant reduction of ChAT transcripts (Fig. 5). This suggests that the reduction of ChAT expression might be observed in cholinergic cPPN neurons projecting to other targets as well. Multi-target regulation is not surprising because choli- nergic PPN neurons have an average of five axonal collaterals that allow a single cholinergic neuron to project to many targets23. Cholinergic cPPN neurons may regulate motor function and other behaviors by gating the activity of downstream targets through transmitter switching. Dopaminergic and noradrenergic neurons are implicated in fine motor function plasticity that regulates paw and digit movements46–49. Our results unexpectedly indicate that acquisi- tion of high-demand rotarod and balance beam performance requires transmitter switching in cholinergic cPPN neurons.
  • 79. Overriding transmitter switching did not affect the ability to run on a running wheel (Figs. 6 and 7), perhaps because running is not an exceptionally taxing motor skill. Consistent with these results, selective lesions of cholinergic PPN neurons impair b Wheel runninga 1-wk rest AAV-CAG-DIO-shGAD1 -EGFP or AAV-CAG-DIO-shScr-EGFP Immediate test 4-wk virus expression ChAT-IRES-Cre 1-wk run Start running Retest d e f g 3000 2000 0 N e u
  • 82. 40 0 60 80 Rotarod Immediate test Retest cPPN neuron counts * 2000 3000 0 G A D 1 + n e u ro
  • 84. 1.5 1.0 0.5 0.0 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 10 0 40 50A T - G A B A + E G F P + C
  • 86. shGAD1-Run shGAD1-Ctrl shScr-Run shScr-Ctrl 30 *** *** p = 0.20 p = 0.75 *** *** p = 0.95 p = 0.93 p = 0.28 *** * p = 0.12 p = 0.99 ** * p = 0.87 p = 0.97 * ** p = 0.98 p = 0.79 ** * p = 0.59 p = 0.96 Fig. 7 Gain of GAD1 in cholinergic cPPN neurons is also necessary for running-enhanced motor skill learning. a
  • 87. Experimental design to override the gain of GAD1 in ChAT-Cre neurons and examine behavioral relevance. The coronal brain section was drawn according to the Franklin and Paxinos brain atlas57. b Numbers of GAD1 in situ stained neurons in the cPPNs of runners and non-running controls that were injected with AAV- DIO-shScr (scramble shRNA) or AAV-DIO-shGAD1 (shRNA for GAD1). n = 7 for shGAD1- Run, and five animals for the other three groups. Nonparametric Kruskal–Wallis test followed by Dunn’s correction. c Percentage of neurons immunostained as GABA+, EGFP+ and ChAT- in the total EGFP+ neurons in cPPNs of ChAT-Cre runners and non-running controls that were injected with AAV-DIO- shScr or AAV-DIO-shGAD1. n = 1243 cells for shScr-Ctrl, 1258 for shScr-Run, 957 for shGAD1- Ctrl, and 1226 for shGAD1-Run from three animals per group. Nonparametric Kruskal–Wallis test followed by Dunn’s correction. d Wheel running speed of ChAT-Cre mice that express AAV-DIO-shScr or AAV-DIO- shGAD1 in the cPPN. n = 8 animals per group. Two-sided Welch’s t-test. e, f Rotarod (e) and balance beam (f, 4-mm rod) tests show that expression of shScr did not affect the enhancement of acquisition and maintenance of motor skills gained by running (shScr-Ctrl vs. shScr-Run) whereas knocking down GAD1 blocked the enhancement (shGAD1-Run vs. shScr-Run). Blockade was sustained after rest for 1 week. For e, animal numbers are 9 for shScr-Ctrl, 7 for shGAD1-Ctrl, 9 for shScr-Run and 9 for shGAD1-Run for immediate test and 8 for shScr- Ctrl, 7 for shGAD1-Ctrl, 8 for shScr-Run and 9 for shGAD1- Run for retest. For f, numbers of animals are 8 for shScr -Ctrl, 7 for shGAD1-Ctrl, 8 for shScr-Run, and 9 for shGAD1-Run for both immediate test and retest. g Numbers of GAD1 in situ stained neurons in the cPPN of 1-
  • 88. week runners and non-runner controls that express AAV-DIO-ChAT and numbers of ChAT immunostained neurons in 1-week runners and non-runner controls that express AAV-DIO- shGAD1. n = 6 animals per group for ChAT-Run and ChAT-Ctrl and 5 for shGAD1-Run and shGAD1-Ctrl. Mann–Whitney U test. *p < 0.05, **p < 0.01, ***p < 0.001. Data shown are mean ± SEM. RunnerNaive cPPN Targets cPPN Targets Basic motor skill learning Runner with transmitter switching overridden or a b c ChAT expression cPPN Targets
  • 89. Enhanced motor skill learning cPPN Targets Basic motor skill learning nNOS ChAT GAD1 ChAT and GAD1 GAD1 knockdown Fig. 8 Transmitter switching in the cPPN regulates motor skill learning. a, b Chronic running induces neurotransmitter switching from ACh to GABA in the cPPN and enhances motor skill learning. c Both ChAT loss and GAD1 gain are necessary for running-enhanced motor skill learning. NATURE COMMUNICATIONS | https://doi.org/10.1038/s41467-020-16053-7 ARTICLE NATURE COMMUNICATIONS | (2020) 11:2195 | https://doi.org/10.1038/s41467-020-16053-7 | www.nature.com/naturecommunications 9
  • 90. www.nature.com/naturecommunications www.nature.com/naturecommunications learning of high-demand running on an accelerating-speed rotarod but do not affect learning of low-demand running on a fixed-speed rotarod or basal locomotion50. In contrast, glutama- tergic and GABAergic PPN neurons regulate gait and speed of locomotion51–53. Control of voluntary movement and procedural learning are core functions of the basal ganglia. SNr inhibitory neurons serve as the major output of the basal ganglia and project to the PPN; in turn, PPN ascending axons project to the SNc and SNr to modulate the function of the basal ganglia23. We propose a model in which conversion of cPPN excitatory cholinergic input to inhibitory GABAergic input of inhibitory neurons in the SN enables feedback control of SN neurons regulating motor coor- dination and skill learning, as supported by their decrease in c - fos expression (Fig. 2c). Gait speed, stride and balance of Parkinson’s disease and stroke patients are improved following sustained treadmill training54–56. Our finding that transmitter switching after running is a critical event for improving motor skill learning suggests that transmitter switching may be important in many circumstances where sustained exercise benefits behavior. Methods Mice. All animal procedures were carried out in accordance with NIH guidelines and approved by the University of California, San Diego
  • 91. Institutional Animal Care and Use Committee or Scripps Institutional Animal Care and Use Committee. C57BL/6J (JAX#000664) mice were obtained from Jackson Laboratories. ChAT- IRES-Cre (JAX#006410) mice were obtained from the Byungkook Lim lab and Jackson Laboratories. PV-IRES-Cre (JAX#008069) mice were provided by the Stefan Leutgeb lab. Animals were maintained on a 12 h:12 h light:dark cycle (light on: 10:00 pm–10:00 am) with food and water ad libitum. Vivarium temperature was between 65 and 75 °F (~18 and 23 °C) with 40–60% humidity. The ChAT- IRES-Cre colony was maintained by breeding homozygous male ChAT-Cre mice with female wild-type C57BL/6J mice. Heterozygous ChAT-Cre offspring were used in the study. Both heterozygous and homozygous PV- IRES-Cre mice were used. All experiments were performed on 8- to 12-week-old male mice. Wheel running. Mice were single-housed in hamster cages and provided with FastTrac (Bio-serv, K3250) or digital (Med Associates ENV- 044) running wheels that are identical in shape and size. Mice were allowed voluntary running for one week and were continuously recorded with Swann DVR4-2600 infrared video cameras. Control mice were housed with running wheelbases without wheels. Episode duration and time with the wheel were hand-scored using JWatcher
  • 92. software. The maximum angular excursion of mouse movements on the running wheels was plotted using Image J and measured with a protractor. The digital running wheels recorded running distance and running speed was calculated as distance divided by time. Rotarod. Training and tests were performed as the rotarod (Ugo Basile, 57624) accelerated from 5 rpm to 80 rpm in 6 min. The rpm at which mice fell off was recorded by the rotarod. Mice were trained for nine trials on the first day, followed by three tests the next day and three retests after 1, 2, or 4- weeks rest. There was a 10-min interval between each trial/test. The single digit accuracy of the slopes of learning curves reflects the accuracy of recording the rpm at fall by the rotarod. Balance beam. Mice were trained and tested with balance beams 1 meter long and 0.75 meters above the floor. Twelve millimeters and 6 mm square beams (S-12 and S-6) were single plastic horizontal bars while 6 and 4 mm rod beams (R-6 and R-4) consisted of two stainless steel parallel bars 5 cm apart. On the training day mice were trained to cross S-12 for three trials, S-6 for three trials, R-6 for three trials and then R-4 for three trials, each 10 min apart. On the test and retest day mice were allowed to cross all four beams in the same order, three times for each, 10 min apart. A video camera was installed at the end of the beam
  • 93. where mice started walking. At the other end of the beam, a black box with an entry facing the beam was installed to attract mice to cross the beams. Time to cross the beam and enter the escape box was hand-scored and the investigator was double-blinded to the history of the mice. The slopes of learning curves are accurate to one decimal place because the time to cross the beam was hand scored and human response time is 0.1~0.2 s. Motor skill learning. The mean speed at fall from a rotarod for the nine trials on the training day was fitted by a one-phase association model. Mean data points for each training trial were plotted and fitted using GraphPad Prism 7 software and the coefficient of determination (R2) was used to justify the fit (R2 > 0.96 for controls and R2 > 0.95 for runners, with MATLAB). The mean time to cross a 4 mm rod beam for the three trials on the training day was fitted by linear regression (R2 > 0.98 for controls and R2 > 0.99 for runners, with MATLAB). Locomotor activity. Mice were tested for 120 min in polycarbonate cages (42 × 22 × 20 cm) placed in frames (25.5 × 47 cm) mounted with two levels of photocell beams at 2 and 7 cm above the bottom of the cage (San Diego Instruments, San Diego, CA). The two sets of beams detect both horizontal (roaming) and vertical
  • 94. (rearing) behavior. A thin layer of bedding material covered the bottom of the cage. Data were collected in 1-min epochs. Histology and immunocytochemistry. Animals were perfused transcardially with phosphate-buffered saline (PBS) followed by 4% paraformaldehyde (PFA) in PBS right after the last episode of running, i.e., mice started to run at 10 am at light-off and kept running until they were perfused between 2 and 3 pm. Brains were dissected and post-fixed in 4% PFA for 16 to 24 h at 4 °C, washed in PBS for 1 min and transferred to 30% sucrose in PBS for 2 days at 4 °C. Forty- micrometers coronal sections were cut on a microtome (Leica SM2010R) and stained. For immunostaining, sections were permeabilized and blocked in 24-well culture plates for 2 h in a blocking solution (5% normal horse serum, 0.3% Triton X-100 in PBS) at 22–24 °C. Primary and secondary antibodies were diluted in the blocking solution. Incubation with primary antibodies was performed for 48 h on a rotator at 4 °C. After washing in PBS (three times, 15 min each), secondary antibodies were added for 2 h at 22–24 °C. For immunofluorescence, sections were mounted with Fluoromount-G (Southern Biotech) or ProLong Gold Antifade Mountant (Life Technologies) containing DRAQ-5 (Thermo Fisher, 62251, 1:1000 dilution; when nuclear staining was needed) after washes in
  • 95. PBS (three times, 15 min each). For DAB (3,3′-Diaminobenzidine) staining, sections were treated with 0.3% hydrogen peroxide for 30 min, washed in PBS (three times, 5 min each), incubated with Vectastain Elite ABC HRP mixture (Vector Laboratories, PK-6100) for 45 min, washed in PBS (three times, 15 min each), and signals were developed using the DAB Peroxidase Substrate kit (Vector Laboratories, SK-4100). Primary antibodies used in this study were goat anti-ChAT (Millipore, AB144P, 1:500), rabbit-anti-nNOS (Thermo Fisher, 61-7000, 1:500), goat-anti-cFos (Santa Cruz, sc-52G, 1:300), rabbit-anti-cFos (Santa Cruz, sc-52, 1:300), mouse-anti-cFos (Abcam, ab208942, 1:500), rabbit-anti-PV (Swant, PV25, 1:2000), goat-anti- VAChT (Millipore, ABN100, 1:500), goat-anti-VGAT (Synaptic system, 131004, 1:1000), mouse-anti-NeuN (Millipore, MAB377, 1:500), rabbit- anti-GABA (Sigma- Aldrich, A2052, 1:1000) rabbit-anti-GFP (Thermo Fisher, A11122, 1:1000), chicken anti-GFP (Abcam, ab13970, 1:1000), guinea pig anti-GFP (Synaptic Systems, 132005, 1:3000), rabbit-anti-zsGreen (Takara, 632474, 1:500), goat-anti- doublecortin (Santa Cruz, sc-8066, 1:300) and rabbit-anti-Ki67 (Cell Signaling, 9129, 1:300). Secondary antibodies for immunofluorescence were from Jackson
  • 96. ImmunoResearch Labs and used at a concentration of 1:600: Alexa Fluor-488 donkey-anti-rabbit (705-545-003), Alexa Fluor-488 donkey- anti-guinea pig (706- 545-148), Alexa Fluor-488 donkey-anti-mouse (715-545-150), Alexa Fluor-488 donkey-anti-goat (705-545-147), Alexa Fluor-594 donkey-anti- goat (705-585-147), Alexa Fluor-594 donkey-anti-mouse (715-585-150), Alexa Fluor-647 donkey-anti- goat (705-605-147) and Alexa Fluor-647 donkey-anti-rabbit (711-605-152). Biotinylated goat anti-rabbit (BA-1000) and horse anti-goat (BA-9500) secondary antibodies for DAB staining were from Vector Laboratories and used at a concentration of 1:300. In situ hybridization PFA fixed brains were dissected, post- fixed in 4% PFA for 16 to 24 h at 4 °C and transferred to 30% DEPC-sucrose in PBS for 2 days. Subsequently, brains were embedded in 30% DEPC-sucrose and frozen with dry ice. Forty-micrometers cryosections were collected on Superfrost Plus slides (VWR, 48311-703) and used for mRNA in situ hybridization. Complementary DNAs (cDNAs) of gad1 or slc17a6 (sequences from Allen Brain Atlas: gad1, RP_040324_01_F01; slc17a6, RP_050921_01_E03) were cloned from mouse cDNA library in ~800-base-pair segments into a pGEM vector and sequenced to verify that they were correct. Antisense complementary RNA (cRNA) probes were
  • 97. synthesized with T7 (Promega, P2075) or Sp6 polymerases (Promega, P1085) and labeled with digoxigenin (Roche, 11175025910). Hybridization was performed with 1 to 5 μg/ml cRNA probes at 65 °C for 20 to 24 h. Probes were detected using Anti- Digoxigenin-AP Fab fragments (Roche, 11093274910, 1:5000). Signals were developed for 8 h using a mixture of 4-Nitro blue tetrazolium chloride (Roche, 11383213001) and BCIP 4-toluidine salt solution (Roche, 11383221001). We counted all GAD1+ neurons in the cPPN, including those that looked intense, moderate or faint. Fluorescent RNAscope in situ hybridization was performed according to the manufacturer’s instructions (Advanced Cell Diagnostics) with some modifications: In an RNase-free environment, 12-μm fixed brain sections were mounted on Superfrost Plus slides immediately after microtome sectioning and air-dried in a 60 °C oven for 30 min. Sections were rehydrated in PBS for 2 min and incubated for 5 min in 1× target retrieval solution at 95 °C. Sections were then rinsed with distilled water for 5 s and rinsed in 100% ethanol for 5 seconds. After air-drying, sections were incubated with the following solutions in a HybEZ humidified oven at 40 °C with three rinsing steps in between each: protease III, 30 min; probes, 2 h; amplification (Amp) 1-fluorescence (FL), 30 min; Amp 2-FL, 15 min; Amp 3-FL,
  • 98. 30 min; and Amp 4-FL, 15 min. Ready-to use Amp 1-FL, Amp 2-FL, Amp 3-FL, ARTICLE NATURE COMMUNICATIONS | https://doi.org/10.1038/s41467-020-16053-7 10 NATURE COMMUNICATIONS | (2020) 11:2195 | https://doi.org/10.1038/s41467-020-16053-7 | www.nature.com/naturecommunications www.nature.com/naturecommunications Amp 4-FL and 50× washing solution for rinsing steps were included in the RNAscope Multiplex Fluorescent Reagent kit. Standard immunofluorescent staining was subsequently performed in the dark15. Probes for mouse chat mRNA (Catalog number: 408731-C2) and gad1 mRNA (Catalog number: 400951) were from Advanced Cell Diagnostics. Sections were 6–7 μm thick post-processing. Six optical sections (1 µm Z step) of each physical section were examined and regions of interest (ROIs) were drawn around the boundaries of nNOS cells on the optical section that showed the best focal plane for each cell (largest cross-sectional area). These ROIs were scored for ChAT and GAD1 transcript puncta using Image J. The average area of ROIs was consistent between the control and runner groups. We included negative controls using a probe targeting a bacterial- origin gene dapB (Catalog number: 310043; GeneBank: EF191515) in each batch
  • 99. of experiments and validated that the negative control group showed <1 punctum in 100 cells. Based on the expression of ChAT and GAD1 transcript puncta, the neurons were grouped into four categories (1) classic cholinergic neurons: ≥8 ChAT transcript puncta, no GAD1 transcript puncta; (2) co-expressing neurons: ≥8 ChAT puncta, ≥1 GAD1 puncta); (3) neurons expressing neither: <8 ChAT puncta, no GAD1 puncta; (4) switched neurons: <8 ChAT puncta, ≥1 GAD1 puncta. We are aware that other studies have arrived at a lower percent of costaining than reported here. The difference is likely to be caused by different ways to detect and score a positive neuron. Detection by IHC and ISH can be greatly affected and is ultimately determined by the sensitivity and thresholding of the method. From the distribution of GAD1 puncta number (Fig. 4e) we infer that scoring only neurons that contain 5 or more GAD1 puncta, would yield an incidence of 4% of ChAT neurons that co-express GAD1, which would be consistent with percentages previously reported33–35. We have not found descriptions of counting thresholds in other reports and believe that in most cases investigators count neurons that contain moderate to intense GAD1 and have regarded those with a low level of transcripts as negative.
  • 100. Birthdating. Mice were intraperitoneally injected with BrdU (50 mg/kg) once every 12 h for 1 week. PFA-fixed brains were dissected, post-fixed and dehydrated as described above. Forty-micrometers cryosections were collected and treated with 1 M HCl for 30 min at 45 °C for DNA denaturation. After rinsing in PBS (three times, 5 min each), sections were incubated in a mixture of 0.3% Triton X-100 and 5% horse serum in PBS for 1 h, incubated in rat-anti BrdU antibody (AbD Serotec, MCA2060, 1:300) at 4 °C overnight, rinsed in PBS (three times, 5 min each) and amplified by Alexa Fluor-488 donkey anti-rat antibody (Life Technologies, A21208, 1:600). Sections were mounted with Fluoromount containing DRAQ-5 (1:1000). TUNEL assay. The In Situ Cell Death Detection (TUNEL) Kit with TMR Red (Roche, 12156792910) was used to detect in situ apoptosis. Forty-micrometers cryosections were re-fixed with 1% PFA for 20 min at 22–24 °C and rinsed with PBS (three times, 5 min each). Sections were then permeabilized in 0.1% sodium citrate and 1% Triton X-100 for 1 h at 22–24 °C. After rinsing in PBS (three times, 5 min each), sections were incubated with TUNEL reaction solution according to the vendor’s instruction, i.e., incubated in a mixture of 25 μL of terminal- deoxynucleotidyl transferase solution and 225 μL of label solution. Incubation was performed in a humidified chamber for 3 h at 37 °C in the dark.
  • 101. Sections were rinsed and mounted with Fluoromount containing DRAQ-5 (1:1000). For a positive control, sections were treated with DNase I (10 U/mL, New England Biolabs, M0303S) for 1 h at 37 °C and rinsed in PBS (three times, 5 min each), followed by incubation with the TUNEL mixture. Imaging and data analysis. Fluorescent images were acquired with a Leica SP5 confocal microscope with a 25×/0.95 water-immersion objective and a z resolution of 1 μm. Leica Application Suite X software was used for fluorescent cell counting and all sections within the confocal stacks were examined without maximal pro- jection. RNAscope in situ signals were quantified using Image J. Exemplar images are maximum intensity projections of 6 consecutive confocal sections when RNAscope signals are included (Figs. 4d and 5e) and 16–24 confocal sections for other immunofluorescence images. For the synaptophysin-EGFP signals, images were acquired with a 63×/0.90 water-immersion objective at zoom 2 magnification with a z resolution of 1 μm. Colocalization was quantified using Image J with the JACoP plugin. Exemplar images for DAB staining and in situ hybridization were acquired with a NanoZoomer Slide scanner (Hamamatsu S360) with a 20×/0.75 air objective. Counting DAB-stained c-fos neurons per unit area was performed with Image J on images harvested from the NanoZoomer. To delimit
  • 102. the PPN in sec- tions stained for c-fos (Supplementary Fig. 2a, b), or for TUNEL and Ki67/DCX (apoptosis and neurogenesis, Supplementary Fig. 3a–f), an adjacent section was stained for ChAT to define the boundaries of the PPN. Images were analyzed exhaustively or by sampling. Exhaustive analysis entailed scoring all neurons in every other section through the entire brain region of interest and reporting the number of cells for the number of mice examined. Sampling involved scoring and reporting a specific number of cells from a specified number of sections from a particular number of mice. In these cases, exact cell and/or section numbers are shown in the figure legends. Stereological counting. Stereo Investigator software (MBF Bioscience) was used to exhaustively count DAB immunostained or in situ hybridization-stained cells. Counting was carried out using optical fractionator sampling on a Zeiss Axioskop 2 microscope (40×/0.65 Ph2 objective) equipped with a motorized stage. The population of midbrain PPN neurons was outlined on the basis of ChAT immu- nolabeling, with reference to a coronal atlas of the mouse brain57 and anatomical landmarks such as fiber tracts. Caudal PPN refers to the caudal half of PPN with reference to a transverse line normal to and halfway along the rostrocaudal axis; rostral PPN refers to the rostral half. To count GABAergic and
  • 103. glutamatergic neurons in the sections stained by in situ hybridization, an adjacent section was stained for ChAT to define the ROI for the boundaries of PPN. The area of the ROI and the cell density were compared between the control and experimental groups to ensure that the counted areas were comparable between the two groups and that changes in cell density were consistent with changes in the absolute counts. Pilot experiments with continuous counting determined that counting every other sec- tion was sufficient to estimate the number of cholinergic, GABAergic, glutama- tergic and nitroxidergic neurons in the cPPN of each brain. Consequently, five to six sections were counted for each mouse brain. The average section thickness was measured prior to counting. Sections shrank from 40 μm to 22– 24 μm after staining and dehydration. Exhaustive counting was performed and no sampling grid was skipped because the distribution of neurons in the cPPN is uneven. Counting was performed by two investigators double-blinded to the origin of the sections. Viral constructs and injection. Seven to eight-week-old male mice were anesthe- tized with a mixture of 120 mg/kg ketamine and 16 mg/kg xylazine and head-fixed on a stereotaxic apparatus (David Kopf Instruments Model 1900) for all stereotaxic surgeries. The caudal PPN was targeted bilaterally (cPPN:
  • 104. anterior–posterior (AP), −4.80 mm from bregma; mediolateral (ML), ±1.25 mm; dorsal– ventral (DV), −3.25 mm from the dura). A total of 300 nL of the Cre-on viral vectors were injected into each cPPN of ChAT-Cre mice at a rate of 100 nL/min using a syringe pump (PHD Ultra™, Harvard apparatus, no. 70-3007) installed with a microliter syringe (Hampton, no.1482452A) and capillary glass pipettes with filament (Warner Instruments, no. G150TF-4). Pipettes were left in place for 5 min after injection. Titers of recombinant AAV vectors ranged from 7.1 × 1012 to 2.2 × 1013 viral particles/ml, based on quantitative PCR analysis. AAV8-phSyn1(S)-FLEX- tdTomato-T2A- SypEGFP-WPRE was purchased from the Salk Viral Vector Core and used to trace the axonal terminals58. AAV-DJ-CMV-DIO-Kir2.1 and AAV- DJ-CMV-DIO-EGFP were both ordered from the Stanford Viral Vector Core and used to suppress neu- ronal activity or as a control20,59. pAAV-hSyn-DIO-ChAT- P2A-mRuby2 and pAAV- hSyn-DIO-mRuby2 plasmids were constructed in our lab and AAV2/8 vectors were packaged in the Salk Viral Vector Core. Vector Biolabs produced AAV8-CAG-DIO- shRNAmir-mGAD1-EGFP and AAV8-CAG-DIO-shRNAmir- Scramble-EGFP vec- tors. The shRNA sequence for mouse GAD1 is 5′- GTCTACAGTCAACCAGGATCTGGTTTTGGCCACTGACTGA CCAGATCC
  • 105. TTTGACTGTAGA-3′. Tract tracing. For anterograde tracing, 300 nL of Cre-on AAV8- DIO-mRuby2 vectors were unilaterally injected into the cPPN of ChAT-Cre mice as described above and axonal tracts were imaged by Leica confocal microscope. For retrograde tracing, a volume of 100 nL of retrobeads (Red Retrobeads™ IX or Green Retro- beads™ IX, Lumafluor) was injected at 100 nL/min into the SN or VTA and VL- VM. Pipettes were left in place for 10 min after injection. Three weeks after injection, mice were sacrificed for brain examination or housed with running wheels to examine running-induced loss of ChAT transcripts using RNAscope combined with nNOS immunostaining. ChAT transcripts in neurons that con- tained retrobeads were quantified using Image J. Retrobead infusion coordinates were SN (from bregma AP −3.20 mm; ML ±1.50 mm; and DV from the dura, −4.00 mm), VTA (AP, −3.10 mm; ML, ±0.50 mm; and DV, −4.25 mm), and VL- VM (AP, −2.20 mm; ML, ±1.75 mm; and DV, −3.25 mm). Statistics and reproducibility. For comparisons between two groups, data were analyzed by Welch’s t-test for normally distributed data and the Mann–Whitney U test for data not normally distributed, using Graph Pad Prism 7. For repeated samples, data were analyzed by paired t-test. Correlation was analyzed by the
  • 106. Pearson correlation using Graph Pad Prism 7. All tests are two- tailed. Statistical analyzes of the data were performed using Prism 7 software for the number of animals for each experiment indicated in the figure legends. Means and SEMs are reported for all experiments. For comparisons between multiple groups, ANOVA followed by Tukey’s test was used for normally distributed data. The nonpara- metric Kruskal–Wallis test followed by Dunn’s correction was used for data that were not normally distributed. Experiments were carried out independently twice (Figs. 2b, c, 4a, b, 5b, c, h–j, 7b and Supplementary Figs. 1f, 3a–f, 5, 7f, 8f), three times (Fig. 1b, h–j, 2d–k, 3, 4c–h, 5e–f, 6c–d, 7c, g, and Supplementary Figs. 1a–d, 2b–f, i–j, 3g–l, 4a–c, f–i, 6, 7d–e, 8, 9a–d and 10), or four or more times (Figs. 1c–f, 6e–g, 7d–f and Supplementary Figs. 2g, i, 4d, e, 9e–h). Reporting summary. Further information on research design is available in the Nature Research Reporting Summary linked to this article. NATURE COMMUNICATIONS | https://doi.org/10.1038/s41467-020-16053-7 ARTICLE NATURE COMMUNICATIONS | (2020) 11:2195 | https://doi.org/10.1038/s41467-020-16053-7 | www.nature.com/naturecommunications 11 https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/nuccore/EF191515 www.nature.com/naturecommunications www.nature.com/naturecommunications
  • 107. Data availability All data generated or analyzed during this study are included in this published article and its supplementary information files. Source data are provided as a Source data file. Received: 30 May 2019; Accepted: 7 April 2020; References 1. Bostan, A. C. & Strick, P. L. The basal ganglia and the cerebellum: nodes in an integrated network. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 19, 338–350 (2018). 2. Esposito, M. S. et al. Brainstem nucleus MdV mediates skilled forelimb motor tasks. Nature 508, 351–356 (2014). 3. Dayan, E. & Cohen, L. G. Neuroplasticity subserving motor skill learning. Neuron 72, 443–454 (2011). 4. McKenzie, I. A. et al. Motor skill learning requires active central myelination. Science 346, 318–322 (2014). 5. Statton, M. A. et al. A single bout of moderate aerobic exercise improves motor skill acquisition. PLoS ONE 10, e0141393 (2015). 6. Palmer, S. S. et al. Exercise therapy for Parkinson’s dis ease. Arch. Phys. Med. Rehabil. 67, 741–745 (1986). 7. Kane, K. & Bell, A. A. core stability group program for
  • 108. children with developmental coordination disorder: 3 clinical case reports. Pediatr. Phys. Ther. 21, 375–382 (2009). 8. Rafie, F. et al. Physical exercises and motor skills in autistic children. Iran. J. Public Health 44, 724–725 (2015). 9. Meijer, J. H. & Robbers, Y. Wheel running in the wild. Proc. Roy. Soc. B 281, 20140210 (2014). 10. Murphy, T. H. & Corbett, D. Plasticity during stroke recovery: from synapse to behaviour. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 10, 861–872 (2009). 11. Petzinger, G. M. et al. Exercise-enhanced neuroplasticity targeting motor and cognitive circuitry in Parkinson’s disease. Lancet Neurol. 12, 716–726 (2013). 12. Spitzer, N. C. Neurotransmitter switching in the developing and adult brain. Ann. Rev. Neurosci. 40, 1–19 (2017). 13. Shiotsuki, H. et al. A rotarod test for evaluation of motor skill learning. J. Neurosci. Methods 189, 180–185 (2010). 14. Buitrago, M. M. et al. Short and long-term motor skill learning in an accelerated rotarod training paradigm. Neurobiol. Learn. Mem. 81, 211–216 (2004).
  • 109. 15. Pothakos, K., Kurz, M. J. & Lau, Y. S. Restorative effect of endurance exercise on behavioral deficits in the chronic mouse model of Parkinson’s disease with severe neurodegeneration. BMC Neurosci. 10, 6 (2009). 16. Mackintosh, N. J. A theory of attention: variations in the associability of stimuli with reinforcement. Psychological Rev. 82, 276–298 (1975). 17. Morris, R. G. et al. Selective impairment of learning and blockade of long-term potentiation by an N-methyl-D-aspartate receptor antagonist, AP5. Nature 319, 774–776 (1986). 18. Marek, K. W., Kurtz, L. M. & Spitzer, N. C. cJun integrates calcium activity and tlx3 expression to regulate neurotransmitter specification. Nat. Neurosci. 13, 944–950 (2010). 19. Güemez-Gamboa, A. et al. Non-cell-autonomous mechanism of activity- dependent neurotransmitter switching. Neuron 82, 1004–1016 (2014). 20. Meng, D. et al. Neuronal activity regulates neurotransmitter switching in the adult brain following light-induced stress. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 115, 5064–5071 (2018). 21. Clark, P. J. et al. Adult hippocampal neurogenesis and c-Fos
  • 110. induction during escalation of voluntary wheel running in C57BL/6J mice. Behav. Brain Res. 213, 246–252 (2010). 22. Boisgontier, M. P. et al. Individual differences in brainstem and basal ganglia structure predict postural control and balance loss in young and older adults. Neurobiol. Aging 50, 47–59 (2017). 23. Mena-Segovia, J. & Bolam, J. P. Rethinking the pedunculopontine nucleus: from cellular organization to function. Neuron 94, 7–18 (2017). 24. Smith, Y. et al. Parkinson’s disease therapeutics: new developments and challenges since the introduction of levodopa. Neuropsychopharmacol 37, 213–246 (2012). 25. Mena-Segovia, J. et al. GABAergic neuron distribution in the pedunculopontine nucleus defines functional subterritories. J. Comp. Neurol. 515, 397–408 (2009). 26. Tattersall, T. L. et al. Imagined gait modulates neuronal network dynamics in the human pedunculopontine nucleus. Nat. Neurosci. 17, 449– 454 (2014). 27. Thevathasan, W. et al. Alpha oscillations in the pedunculopontine nucleus correlate with gait performance in parkinsonism. Brain 135, 148–160 (2012).
  • 111. 28. Gut, N. K. & Winn, P. Deep brain stimulation of different pedunculopontine targets in a novel rodent model of parkinsonism. J. Neurosci. 35, 4792–4803 (2015). 29. Karachi, C. et al. Cholinergic mesencephalic neurons are involved in gait and postural disorders in Parkinson disease. J. Clin. Invest. 120, 2745–2754 (2010). 30. Madisen, L. et al. A robust and high-throughput Cre reporting and characterization system for the whole mouse brain. Nat. Neurosci. 13, 133–140 (2010). 31. Chen, E. et al. Altered baseline and nicotine-mediated behavioral and cholinergic profiles in ChAT-Cre mouse lines. J. Neurosci. 38, 2177–2188 (2018). 32. Vincent, S. R. et al. NADPH-diaphorase: a selective histochemical marker for the cholinergic neurons of the pontine reticular formation. Neurosci. Lett. 43, 31–36 (1983). 33. Saunders, A., Granger, A. J. & Sabatini, B. L. Corelease of acetylcholine and GABA from cholinergic forebrain neurons. elife 27, 4 (2015). 34. Wang, H. L. & Morales, M. Pedunculopontine and
  • 112. laterodorsal tegmental nuclei contain distinct populations of cholinergic, glutamatergic and GABAergic neurons in the rat. Eur. J. Neurosci. 29, 340–358 (2009). 35. Luquin, E. et al. Stereological estimates of glutamatergic, GABAergic, and cholinergic neurons in the pedunculopontine and laterodorsal tegmental nuclei in the rat. Front. Neuroanat. 12, 34 (2018). 36. Xiao, C. et al. Cholinergic mesopontine signals govern locomotion and reward through dissociable midbrain pathways. Neuron 90, 333–347 (2016). 37. Dautan, D. et al. Segregated cholinergic transmission modulates dopamine neurons integrated in distinct functional circuits. Nat. Neurosci. 19, 1025–1033 (2016). 38. Gambhir, H., Mathur, R. & Behari, M. Progressive impairment in motor skill learning at 12 and 20 weeks post 6-OHDA- SNc lesion in rats. Parkinsonism Relat. Disord. 17, 476–478 (2011). 39. Leemburg., S. et al. Motor skill learning and reward consumption differentially affect VTA activation. Sci. Rep. 8, 687 (2018). 40. Steriade, M. et al. Fast oscillations (20-40 Hz) in thalamocortical systems and their potentiation by mesopontine cholinergic nuclei in the cat.
  • 113. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 88, 4396–4400 (1991). 41. Jeljeli, M. et al. Effects of ventrolateral-ventromedial thalamic lesions on motor coordination and spatial orientation in rats. Neurosci. Res. 47, 309–316 (2003). 42. Kaita, A. A. & Goldberg, A. M. Control of acetylcholine synthesis—the inhibition of choline acetyltransferase by acetylcholine. J. Neurochem. 16, 1185–1191 (1969). 43. Morris, D., Maneckjee, A. & Hebb, C. The kinetic properties of human placental choline acetyltransferase. Biochem. J. 125, 857–863 (1971). 44. Dulcis, D. et al. Neurotransmitter switching regulated by miRNAs controls changes in social preference. Neuron 95, 1319–1333 (2017). 45. Perez-Lloret, S. & Barrantes, F. J. Deficits in cholinergic neurotransmission and their clinical correlates in Parkinson’s disease, N.P.J. Parkinsons Dis. 2, 16001 (2016). 46. Hosp, J. A. et al. Dopaminergic projections from midbrain to primary motor cortex mediate motor skill learning. J. Neurosci. 31, 2481–2487 (2011).
  • 114. 47. Guo, L. et al. Dynamic rewiring of neural circuits in the motor cortex in mouse models of Parkinson’s disease. Nat. Neurosci. 18, 1299– 1309 (2015). 48. Vitrac, C. & Benoit-Marand, M. Monoaminergic modulation of motor cortex function. Front. Neural Circuits 11, 72 (2017). 49. Chandler, D. J., Gao, W. J. & Waterhouse, B. D. Heterogeneous organization of the locus coeruleus projections to prefrontal and motor cortices. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 111, 6816–6821 (2014). 50. MacLaren, D. A. et al. Deficits in motor performance after pedunculopontine lesions in rats – impairment depends on demands of task. Eur. J. Neurosci. 40, 3224–3236 (2014). 51. Caggiano, V. et al. Midbrain circuits that set locomotor speed and gait selection. Nature 553, 455–460 (2018). 52. Roseberry, T. K. et al. Cell-type-specific control of brainstem locomotor circuits by basal ganglia. Cell 164, 526–537 (2016). 53. Lee, A. M. et al. Identification of a brainstem circuit regulating visual cortical state in parallel with locomotion. Neuron 83, 455–466 (2014). 54. Mehrholz, J. et al. Treadmill training for patients with Parkinson’s disease. Cochrane Database Syst. Rev. 8, CD007830 (2015).
  • 115. 55. Polese, J. C. et al. Treadmill training is effective for ambulatory adults with stroke: a systematic review. J. Physiother. 59, 73–80 (2013). 56. Gaßner, H. et al. Perturbation treadmill training improves clinical characteristics of gait and balance in Parkinson’s Disease. J. Parkinsons Dis. 9, 413–426 (2019). 57. Franklin, K. & Paxinos, G. The Mouse Brain In Stereotaxic Coordinates 3rd edn (Elsevier, New York, 2008). 58. Abs, E. et al. Learning-related plasticity in dendrite- targeting layer 1 interneurons. Neuron 100, 684–699 (2018). 59. Knowland, D. et al. Distinct ventral pallidal neural populations mediate separate symptoms of depression. Cell 170, 284–297 (2017). ARTICLE NATURE COMMUNICATIONS | https://doi.org/10.1038/s41467-020-16053-7 12 NATURE COMMUNICATIONS | (2020) 11:2195 | https://doi.org/10.1038/s41467-020-16053-7 | www.nature.com/naturecommunications www.nature.com/naturecommunications Acknowledgements We thank Kyle Jackson, Vaidehi Gupta, and Wuji Jiang for experimental assistance, Alex
  • 116. Glavis-Bloom for technical support and Larry Squire and Takaki Komiyama for com- ments on the manuscript. We thank all members of the Spitzer lab for suggestions on experimental design and the manuscript. We thank Stefan Leutgeb and Byungkook Lim for providing transgenic mice. We thank Jennifer Santini and the UCSD Microscopy Core for technical support with imaging and Amanda Roberts and the Scripps Animal Models Core for suggestions on behavior experiments. This research was supported by grants to N.C.S from the Ellison Medical Foundation, the W. M. Keck Foundation, NIH NS047101 and the Overland Foundation. Author contributions H.-q.L. and N.C.S. conceived the study, designed the experiments, interpreted the results and wrote the paper. H.-q.L. performed the experiments and analyzes. Competing interests The authors declare no competing interests. Additional information Supplementary information is available for this paper at https://doi.org/10.1038/s41467- 020-16053-7. Correspondence and requests for materials should be addressed to H.-q.L. or N.C.S. Peer review information Nature Communications thanks Konstantinos Ampatzis, Michael Nusbaum and the other anonymous reviewer(s) for their
  • 117. contribution to the peer review of this work. Peer reviewer reports are available. Reprints and permission information is available at http://www.nature.com/reprints Publisher’s note Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations. Open Access This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons license, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article’s Creative Commons license, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article’s Creative Commons license and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this license, visit http://creativecommons.org/ licenses/by/4.0/. © The Author(s) 2020 NATURE COMMUNICATIONS | https://doi.org/10.1038/s41467-020-16053-7 ARTICLE
  • 118. NATURE COMMUNICATIONS | (2020) 11:2195 | https://doi.org/10.1038/s41467-020-16053-7 | www.nature.com/naturecommunications 13 https://doi.org/10.1038/s41467-020-16053-7 https://doi.org/10.1038/s41467-020-16053-7 http://www.nature.com/reprints http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/ http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/ www.nature.com/naturecommunications www.nature.com/naturecommunicationsExercise enhances motor skill learning by neurotransmitter switching in the adult midbrainResultsSustained running enhances motor skill learningRunning induces transmitter switching in the midbrain cPPNTranscript levels of transmitter synthetic enzymes changeSwitching neurons project to the SN, VTA, and thalamusEnhanced motor skill learning requires transmitter switchDiscussionMethodsMiceWheel runningRotarodBalance beamMotor skill learningLocomotor activityHistology and immunocytochemistryBirthdatingTUNEL assayImaging and data analysisStereological countingViral constructs and injectionTract tracingStatistics and reproducibilityReporting summaryData availabilityReferencesAcknowledgementsAuthor contributionsCompeting interestsAdditional information