SlideShare a Scribd company logo
Full Terms & Conditions of access and use can be found at
http://www.tandfonline.com/action/journalInformation?journalCode=mmtp20
Download by: [Institute of Professional Studies] Date: 01 March 2017, At: 05:45
Journal of Marketing Theory and Practice
ISSN: 1069-6679 (Print) 1944-7175 (Online) Journal homepage: http://www.tandfonline.com/loi/mmtp20
Attributions and Outcomes of the Service
Recovery Process
Scott R. Swanson & Scott W. Kelley
To cite this article: Scott R. Swanson & Scott W. Kelley (2001) Attributions and Outcomes
of the Service Recovery Process, Journal of Marketing Theory and Practice, 9:4, 50-65, DOI:
10.1080/10696679.2001.11501903
To link to this article: http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/10696679.2001.11501903
Published online: 08 Dec 2015.
Submit your article to this journal
Article views: 60
View related articles
Citing articles: 14 View citing articles
ATTRIBUTIONS AND OUTCOMES OF THE
SERVICE RECOVERY PROCESS
Scott R. Swanson
University of Wisconsin-Whitewater
Scott W. Kelley
University of Kentucky
Drawing on attribution theory and the services marketing literature, the authors examine how the allocation ofcausality and length
ofthe service recovery process impact post-recovery consumer perceptions ofservice quality, customer satisfaction and behavioral
intentions for word-of-mouth and repurchase. Results of a scenario-based repeated measures design suggest that 1) customer
behavioral intentions are more favorable in stable service recoveries, 2) an employee based service recovery results in more
favorable evaluations and word-of-mouth intentions, and 3) customer evaluations and behavioral intentions will be more positive
for service failures remedied by expeditious and less complicated recovery processes. Managerial implications and future research
directions are presented.
INTRODUCTION
It is widely recognized that no service system is perfect.
Mistakes do occur. Fortunately, accepting the inevitability of
service failures does not imply the automatic loss of
customers. When a service failure occurs, "the customer's
confidence in the firm hangs in the balance. The company can
make things better with the customer - at least to some extent -
or make things worse" (Berry, Parasuraman, and Zeithaml
1994, p. 38). It has even been suggested that, through a
phenomenon known as the service recovery paradox, a
successful recovery can result in a more favorable encounter
than if the transaction had been performed correctly the first
time (e.g., Hart, Heskett, and Sasser 1990). In order to better
retain service customers, it is essential, therefore, that
marketers understand the manner by which customers come to
accept (or reject) service recovery attempts.
50 Journal ofMarketing THEORY AND PRACTICE
There are substantial economic benefits to those that can
master the art of service recovery. For example, it has been
noted that a 5% decrease in the customer defection rate can
boost profits from 25% to 95% (Jacob 1994). Other
researchers have suggested that long-term customers generate
increasingly more profits year after year (Reichheld and Sasser
1990). This increase in profits manifests itselfthrough several
sources. First, costs decline due to the reduced expense of
replacing defecting customers. Second, repeat customers often
make fewer demands on employee time due to realistic
expectations. At the same time, employees may become more
efficient due to familiarity with the customer's needs. Third,
the costs ofpromotion, credit verification, and new account set
up to attract a new customer can be as much as five times the
cost of retaining the original customer (Peters 1988). In
addition to the economic benefits of retaining customers
through effective recoveries, the less tangible losses associated
with dissatisfied customersgenerating negative word-of-mouth
about the service provider and the service firm should be
considered (Keaveney 1995). As a result, many companies are
now viewing customers as valuable assets with the realization
that their customer portfolio is the "ultimate source of their
companies' growth and profitability" (McDougall 1995).
The impact of the service recovery process on customer
evaluations and repurchase intentions is an important topic for
both services marketing researchers and managers. There have
been relatively few theoretically based empirical studies on
service recovery to date (e.g., Smith and Bolton 1998; Smith,
Bolton, and Wagner 1998; Tax, Brown, and Chandrashekaran
1998). A great deal of the service recovery research has
primarilyconsisted ofidentifying and classifying recovery types
(e.g., Bitner, Booms, and Tetreault 1990; Kelley, Hoffinan, and
Davis 1993). More systematic theoretically based empirical
research is necessary to advance our knowledge ofthe service
failure/recovery phenomenon. Itisproposed inthis studythatthe
perceived causes ofa recovery can have a significant impact on
post-service recovery evaluations and behaviors. Attribution
theory is proposed as a theoretical basis that can provide
additional insight into the factors that determine customer
perceptions ofan organization's recovery efforts in response to
a service failure.
Subsequently we provide a briefoverview ofattribution theory,
present hypotheses based on attribution theory and services
literature, and then test our hypotheses in three different service
industries using a scenario-based experimental design. Finally,
we report our results, present implications for managers, and
discuss limitations and future research directions.
ATTRIBUTION THEORY
Attribution theory is a collection of several theories that are
concerned with the assignment of causal inferences and how
these interpretations influence evaluations and behavior. The
attribution field has grown from a variety of research streams.
These works include: Heider's seminal writing on naive
psychology (1958), Bem's self-perception theory (1965,1967,
1972), Jones and associates' correspondence of inference work
(Jones and Davis 1965; Jones and McGillis 1976), Kelley's
theory ofexternalattribution(1967, 1971, 1972, 1973),and more
recently, the research ofWeiner(1980, 1985a, 1985b). The wide
array ofsocial interaction phenomenato which attributiontheory
can beappliedhas made thistheory oneofthe primary paradigms
insocial psychology. As aresult, attributiontheory has alsobeen
widely adoptedbymarketingscholars(e.g., Folkes 1984;Curren
and Folkes 1987; Wofford and Goodwin 1990; Gooding and
Kinicki 1995).
Heider(1958) recognized that some attributional characteristics
fluctuate (e.g., effort and luck), while others are relatively fixed
(e.g., ability). This stability of a cause determines shifts in
expectancies (Weiner 1980). Thus, if circumstances are
perceived to remain the same (i.e., stable), then outcomes
experienced will be presumed to continue. However, if the
causal conditions are perceived as being likely to change (i.e.,
unstable), there will be uncertainty about subsequent outcomes.
Heider (1958) also identified an internal-external causal
dimension as fundamental to the attribution process. He noted
that outcomes ofany action depend on two sets ofconditions,
"factorswithinthe person and factors withintheenvironment" (p.
82). Weiner(1980) classifiedthis internal-externaldistinctionas
the locus ofcausality dimension.
Attributiontheory alsorecognizesthat individualsarenotalways
constrained by environmental factors, but make choices. Heider
(1958) related controllabilityto creditand blame. Forexample,
ifan organization has control in preventing a service failure, but
fails to do so, consumers may blame the firm. Conversely, the
company may be more likely to be given credit for positive
actions. In sum, individuals experience and/or witness events
and make inferences about the causes of these occurrences in
order to exercise control of their world. These causes can be
classifiedwithin thethreeprincipaldimensions ofstability(Isthe
cause likely to recur?), locus (Who is responsible?), and
controllability (Did the responsible party have control over the
cause?) (Bitner 1990).
Attribution theory has previously provided significant insights
into product failure experiences. For example, failure
attributed to a seller is more likely to 1) elicit complaints to the
firm and warnings to others (Richins 1983; Curren and Folkes
1987); 2) lead to less satisfaction (Oliver and DeSarbo 1988);
and 3) impact beliefs that the customer is owed apologies
and/or refunds (Folks 1984; Kelley, Hoffman, and Davis
1993). Ifa customer determines that the responsible party for
a failure had control over the cause they will be angrier, have
lower repurchase intentions, and have a greater desire to
complain (Folkes, Koletsky, and Graham 1987). Stability has
been found to influence the type of redress preferred when a
product fails; "compared to unstable reasons, stable
attributions lead consumers to more strongly prefer refunds
rather than exchanges" (Folkes 1988, p. 557). When product
failure is perceived as due to stable factors the customer
believes failure will re-occur and they express a desire for a
monetary refund. Ifthe failure is perceived as due to unstable
causes, then subsequent product satisfaction is expected and
product exchange is preferred (Folkes 1984).
In sum, attributions have been found to influence how
consumers communicate (Richins 1983; Curren and Folkes
1987; Folkes, Koletsky, and Graham 1987), satisfaction or
dissatisfaction (Oliverand DeSarbo 1988), preferred recovery
(Folkes 1984; 1988), and future repurchase intentions (Folkes,
Koletsky, and Graham 1987). As with product failures,
attribution theory may also provide insights into consumer
perceptions and intentions relative to service recovery
experiences. Specific attribution based hypotheses are
presented next.
Fall 2001 51
HYPOTHESES
Service recovery involves the specific actions taken in
response to a service failure (Gronroos 1988a). The service
recovery actions resulting from a service failure might be
implemented consistently or inconsistently (stability).
Previous research also indicates the locus of these actions
might be the service organization, the service employee, or the
service customer (Kelley, Hoffman, and Davis 1993). In
addition, the length of time required to execute the recovery
may vary (Hart, Heskett, and Sasser 1990; Kelley, Hoffman,
and Davis 1993). In the following attribution-based
hypotheses we specifically address the causal dimensions of
stability and locus, with controllability held constant. We also
consider the time involved in the recovery across three
different services.
HI -- Stability and Service FaiIurelRecovery Outcomes
Researchers have demonstrated that consumers engage in
attribution search for various product failures (e.g., Folkes
1984; Curren and Folkes 1987; Wofford and Goodwin 1990;
Gooding and Kinicki 1995). Generally, if the causes of an
outcome are expected to remain unchanged then an increased
degree of certainty is associated with evaluations and future
behaviors. The higher degree of certainty should result in
more favorable evaluations and future behaviors, assuming the
encounter in question was acceptable. But, ifthe causes ofan
outcome are expected to change, then the lack ofcertainty has
an adverse impact on evaluations and future behaviors (Weiner
1980). In this fashion, the stability of a causal attribution
associated with a service recovery has an impact on customer
evaluations and future behaviors.
Further supporting evidence of this relationship is offered
through services research that demonstrates customers greatly
value consistency and reliability in service delivery (Berry,
Parasuraman, and ZeithamI1994). For example, a dry cleaner
damages a customer's shirt. After the dry cleaner apologizes,
the customer is told to purchase a replacement and the
company will reimburse him for the cost of the shirt. This is
always the procedure followed for any damage to a customer's
clothing (i.e., stable). However, if over a number ofyears the
customer finds that in similar situations he one time gets
reimbursed, another time an in-house repair is attempted, and
yet in another incident only an explanation is offered, he can
no longer anticipate with any degree ofcertainty what outcome
will be received if there is a service failure (i.e., unstable).
Based on research from the attribution theory and services
literature, recoveries perceived as stable should lead to more
favorable customer evaluations and behaviors. Thus, the
following hypothesis is proposed:
HI: Stable service recovery attributions will result in more
favorable recovery evaluations and behavioral intentions than
unstable service recovery attributions.
52 Journal ofMarketing THEORY AND PRACTICE
H2 -- Locus and Service FaiIurelRecovery Outcomes
Consumerand marketing researchers (e.g., Richins 1983; Folkes
1984; Curren and Folkes 1987; Oliver and DeSarbo 1988) have
investigated internal and external customer locus attributions.
Valle and Wallendorfs (1977) content analysis of customer
attributions for causes of satisfaction and dissatisfaction with
products suggests the importance of utilizing an expanded
external locus dimension in consumer research. Subsequent
services research conducted using the critical incidenttechnique
(CIT) indicates that service customers perceive that their
experiences can be primarily attributed to eitherthe service fum,
the service employee, or the service customer (Bitner, Booms,
and Tetreault 1990; Kelley, Hoffman, and Davis 1993). For
example, service failures may be attributed by the customer to
him- orherself("l should have made myselfclearer"), the contact
employee ("that was the rudest person I ever met"), or the
company ("ifthat is their policy I will go elsewhere"). Service
recovery can also be attributed to the customer ("I really know
how to get things straightened out"), the contact person ("that
young woman sure took care ofthat problem promptly"), or the
fum ("that company really stands behind its work"). Although
attribution theory does not specifically address the impact of
mUltiple external locus dimensions, it is expected that locus
attributions to the customer, service employee, and service fum
will be differentially related to recovery outcomes (c.f., Folkes,
Koletsky, and Graham 1987; Bitner 1990).
In one ofthe few studies that directly examined consequences of
locus attributions on customer evaluations, Oliver and DeSarbo
(1988) found that successful outcomes resulting from an external
locus lead to greater satisfaction than when success is attributed
to the self. In the context ofthe present study, one would expect
customers to evaluate service recoveries attributed to either the
service employee or firm more favorably than those attributed to
themselves (i.e., customers). Further, previous research suggests
that recoveries enacted by frontline personnel may be evaluated
more favorably than recoveries attributed to the organization or
its higher-level representatives (Hart, Heskett, and Sasser 1990;
Kelley, Hoffman, and Davis 1993). Specifically, the following
hypothesis is suggested:
H2: Service recoveries attributed to the service employee will
result in the most favorable recovery evaluations and behavioral
intentions followed by service recoveries attributedto the service
firm and customer, respectively.
H3 - Recovery Time Differences and Service FailurelRecovery
Outcomes
The three sets ofscenarios used in this research (Airline, Cable
TV, Credit Card) considervarying service recovery times. These
scenarios are presented in Appendix Table 1 and their
development is subsequently discussed in detail. Previous
research (e.g., Hart, Heskett, and Sasser 1990; Kelley, Hoffman,
and Davis 1993) has suggested that customer evaluations of
recovery in complex and lengthy service recovery processes may
be less positive than for service failures remedied by less
complex and shorter recovery processes. The varying levels of
complexity and length ofthe service recoveries investigated are
expected to lead to differences across the three industries
considered.
The Airline failurelrecovery scenario involves a relatively
complex and lengthy recovery process related to lost luggage.
For instance, the lost luggage recovery might be enacted in the
following way: I) the airline employee receives the customer
complaint and completes the forms necessary to begin the
recovery process, 2) the airline baggage personnel locate the
luggage and place it on the right plane, 3) baggage personnel at
the destination unloadthe luggage, and4)otherairline personnel
deliverthe luggage to you the next day. The airline scenario has
the longest recovery time ofthe scenarios utilized (i.e., the next
aftemoon).
In contrast, the Cable TV service recovery process is less
complex and much shorter. In this case the customer calls the
CableTV company and arepairperson is there to fix the problem
within twohours. The numberofpeople involved in the recovery
process is smaller and the time involved in the recovery process
is much shorter. In the CreditCardscenariothe service recovery
process is even less complex and shorter. The customercalls the
CreditCardcompany, speaks with one individualandthe account
is correctedimmediately. The simplicityand limitedeffortonthe
part of the customer in this case may increase the satisfaction
level derived from the recovery. Therefore, based on scenario
recovery complexity and time differences the following
hypothesis is proposed:
H3: Service recovery evaluations and the resulting behavioral
intentions will be more favorable in recoveries that are less
complex and more timely.
Service FailurelRecovery Outcomes
In this study we consider four principle outcomes of service
failure/recovery encounters. These outcomes includeevaluative
outcomes and behavioral outcomes. Thetwo principleevaluative
outcomes of the service failurelrecovery encounter that we
considerare perceived service quality and customersatisfaction.
The behavioral outcomes considered include word-of-mouth
intentions and repurchase intentions. Subsequently, all four of
these outcomes are briefly discussed.
PerceivedService Quality. Several authors distinguish between
two basic types of service quality (e.g., Gronroos 1983b;
Parasuraman, Zeithaml, and Berry 1985; Kelley, Hoffman, and
Davis 1993). The first quality dimension is technical quality,
which relates to what is delivered, and isjudged by the customer
after the service is performed. Technical or outcome quality is
the culmination of having a service need met. The second
dimension focuses on how the service is delivered (fUnctional or
processquality), and relatestothe experiencethatacustomerhas
while the need is being met. The focus of our research is on
servicerecoveryprocess quality. Causal attributions associated
with service recoveries are predicted to influence customer
perceptions ofservice recovery process quality, even when the
service recovery outcome is held constant.
Customer Satisfaction. Satisfied customers experience "a
pleasurable level of consumption-related fulfillment" (Oliver
1997, p. 13). Consumers rely on customerexpectationsto reach
a judgment regarding the fulfillment response associated with
customersatisfaction(e.g., DrogeandHalstead 1991;Oliverand
DeSarbo 1988). A customer is assumed to have expectations
regarding service performance, and these expectations are
compared with actual perceptions ofperformance as the service
is consumed. It is reasonable to assume that this same internal
process occurs regarding service failures and recoveries.
Word-ol-Mouth Intentions. Several services marketing
researchers have considered the word-of-mouth intentions
associatedwithserviceencounters(e.g., Parasuraman,Zeithaml,
and Berry 1988; Parasuraman, Berry, and Zeithaml 1991;
Boulding et al. 1993). Word-of-mouth communications are
recognized as a very common and important form of
communication for services marketers and customers. The
majority of dissatisfied customers will participate in private
word-of-mouth as opposed to either taking no action, or
registering a formal complaint of some form (Richins 1983).
This study focuses on consumers' intended word-of-mouth
communications following aservice failure/recovery encounter.
Repurchase Intentions. The benefits of maintaining a base of
long-term customersare widely recognizedbyservicesmarketers
(e.g., Reichheld and Sasser 1990). One means of assessing
customerloyaltyand hencethe likelihoodofcustomers returning
is through their repurchase intentions (Jones and Sasser 1995).
In this study we specifically consider the post-service
failurelrecovery repurchase intentions ofconsumers.
THE STUDY
Research Methods
Two levelsofservice recoverystability (stableand unstable) and
three levels of service recovery locus (customer, service
employee, and service firm) were manipulated in a scenario
format. The design was replicated within subjects across three
service industries in which the complexity and length of the
service recovery process were manipulated (Airline, Cable TV
and Credit Card). Subjects were randomly assigned to receive
one of the six experimental conditions in each of the three
services.
Scenario Development
The scenarios used in this study were developed in several
separate stages using independent samples. In order to obtain a
better understanding of common, naturally occurring service
failures and their outcomes, the Critical IncidentTechnique was
Fall 2001 53
utilized. This procedure adhered to the suggestions of other
authors (e.g., Elig and Frieze 1979; Lichtenstein and Bearden
1986) by using open-ended elicitation to determine realistic
service failures and their outcomes.
In the fIrst stage, seventy-six service failure/recovery incidents
were collected early in the semester from student subjects (43
male, 33 female) enrolled in two Marketing Principles courses
taught by one of the authors at a large southeastern university.
Twenty-two different types ofservices with a full range ofpoor
to excellent recoveries were cited in the collected critical
incidents. Failure incidents from eightdifferenttypes ofservices
were chosen for further developmentbased on their frequency of
occurrence in the pool(Airline-lost luggage; Auto Repair-work
delay; Cable Television-loss ofservice; Credit Card Company-
incorrect billing; Dry Cleaners-damaged shirt; Fast Food-cold
meal; Hotel-overbooked on rooms; Theater-movie projector
breakdown). Each ofthese failure incidents could be classified
as a core service failure, as they were due to mistakes or other
technical problems with the service itself(Keaveney 1995). Core
service failures were one of the major critical incident groups
identified by Bitner, Booms, and Tetreault(1990) and have been
subsequently considered by several other researchers (e.g.,
Kelley, Hoffman, and Davis 1993; Bitner, Booms, and Mohr
1994; Keaveney 1995).
In the secondstage, we identifIed satisfactory service recoveries.
A number of alternative service recovery resolutions were
generated for each of the eight retained failures. Four expert
judges (faculty members and doctoral students familiar with the
services literature) and a sample of forty-one undergraduate
business students (23 male, 18 female) enrolled in an
introductory marketing course were presented with the eight
incidents and asked to write down three possible resolutions to
each situation: one poor, one satisfactory, and one excellent
resolution. The subjects had not participated in the reporting of
the original critical incidents. The responses ofthe expertjudges
and forty-one participants were then utilized to identifY a range
ofpossible recoveries for each incident.
In the third stage, fifty-eight undergraduate businessstudents (30
male, 28 female) that had not participated in the previous
scenario development stages were asked to scale the service
recoveries for each incidentusinga9-pointLikert-type scale with
the labelsofPoorResolution(1),SatisfactoryResolution(5), and
Excellent Resolution (9). The subjects were from an advanced
marketing course and were ーイ・、ッュゥョ。エセケ@ seniors (87.9%) that
ranged in age from 20 to 34 years ( x = 22.1 years). This
procedure provided a means for quantifYing the ratings of the
service recoveries being considered. Service recoveries with
wide variances in theirratings were discarded (standarddeviation
of 2.0 or more). Based on the computed mean values, one
satisfactoryrecovery for each servicefailure incidentwas chosen
for inclusion in the study. In this stage of the scenario
development process, subjects also rated the realism ofeach of
the eight service failure incidents on a 9-point Likert-type scale
with anchors ofVery Realistic (I) and Very Unrealistic (9). The
54 Journal ofMarketing THEORY AND PRACTICE
mean values for the realism scale ranged from 2.31 to 3.80.
Thus, all eight ofthe service failure incidents were perceived as
realistic.
Pre-Test I ofScenario Manipulations. Scenarios were further
developed and refined through the following procedures. First,
recoveries associated with eachoftheeightfailure incidentswere
developed by the authors to fIt each ofthe six cells included in
the design. Then fIve expert judges familiar with the services
and attribution theory literature (faculty members and doctoral
students) reviewed each of the failure/recovery scenarios and
classified causes of the recoveries into discrete attribution
categories (i.e., stable or unstable; customer-attributed, service
employee-attributed, orservice fIrm-attributed; andcontrolledor
uncontrolled). After two rounds ofrevisions, the scenarios were
reviewed by a second group of three faculty members also
familiar with both the services and attribution theory literature.
Their review resulted in additional wording changes.
The scenarios were pre-tested with 91 (51 male, 40 female)
undergraduate business students to determine realism and ifthe
attribution manipulations were being ー・イ」・ゥカ・セ@ as anticipated.
Subjects ranged in age from 18 to 33 years ( x = 21.8 years),
were predominately juniors (n = 26) and seniors (n = 59), and
were drawn from classes that had not participated in the earlier
scenario development stages. After reading each scenario,
subjects completed a fifteen-item modifIed Causal Dimensions
Scale (Russell 1982) designed to assess causal perceptions ofa
particular situation in terms of the locus, stability, and
controllability dimensions. The original scale consisted ofnine
randomly presentedsemanticdifferentialstatementsthatrelate to
aparticularsituation,three for each causaldimension. Due to the
enlarged locus dimension utilized in this study, the locus scale
items were adapted to capture customer perceptions of
attributions forthe self(i.e., customer),the serviceemployee,and
the service firm. For each locus dimension respondents were
asked if I) taking action was something that was "Outside" or
"Inside" of"You", "The Employee", or "The Firm", 2) taking
action was something about "Other(s)" or "You", "The
Employee", or "The Firm", and 3) if the action taken reflected
"The Situation" or "You", "The Employee", or "The Firm".
Control was assessed by asking subjects if the outcome of a
scenario was I) "Intended" or "Unintended", 2) "Controllable"
or "Uncontrollable", and if3) "Someone was Responsible" or
"No One was Responsible". Stability items asked subjects ifthe
action taken in a particular scenario was perceived as I)
"Permanent" or"Temporary", 2) "Stable" or"Unstable", and 3)
"Unchanging" or"Changing". Asingle-item 9-pointLikert-type
scale was again utilized to assess scenario realism (l="Very
Realistic" and 9="Very Unrealistic").
The modifiedCausal Dimensions Scalewas factoranalyzedwith
varimax rotation. FindingsconfIrmedasingle-factorstructure for
each ofthe five causal dimensions. Coefficient alphas for each
causal dimension were as follows: stability (a = .64); control (a
= .38); customer (a = .73); employee (a = .70); and firm (a =
.74). The manipulations were tested further via ANOVA. All of
the treatment manipulations, with the exception ofthe stability
manipulation for one scenario were found to be statistically
significant(p< .05). Significantinteractionsofmanipulationson
manipulation check scales were not identified in any of the
scenarios and Duncan's Multiple Range post-hoc test revealed
significant differences between expected group means (Perdue
and Summers 1986).
In addition to demonstrating that the intended impact of the
manipulations was successful, the manipulation effects should
be of a sufficient magnitude (Perdue and Summers 1986). A
statistic developed to provide a conservative estimate of the
strength ofassociation or relation between the independentand
dependent variables (in this situation, the manipulation check
measure is being analyzed) is the omega-squared statistic (w).
Omega-squared is suggested as an appropriate indicator of
effect size in the context of ANOVA models (Green 1978;
Kerlinger 1986; Perdue and Summers 1986; Tabachnick and
Fidell 1989). The function of omega-squared in ANOVA is
similar to R2 in the context ofmultiple regression (Green 1978).
"Omega-squared represents the proportion of variance in the
dependent variable accounted for by agiven main or interaction
effect" (Perdue and Summers 1986, p. 323). Effect sizes in this
pretestrangedfrom .06to .21. Significantdifferences andmeans
falling at the appropriate scale ends suggested the service
recovery causes were being properly classified. Findings
generally indicated that the independent variable manipulations
were effective. However, due to modest effectsizes and the low
coefficientalphaassociatedwith the control dimension, asecond
pre-test was conducted.
Pre-Test I! of Scenario Manipulations. Failure/recovery
scenarios depicting each of the six treatment cells from five
service industries (i.e., Airline-lost luggage; Auto Repair-work
delay; Cable Television-loss ofservice; CreditCard Company-
incorrect billing; Hotel-overbooked on rooms) were retained
based on the results of the first pre-test. These five sets of
scenarios were pre-tested a second time after the following
changes were made: 1) the manipulations were integrated into
the cover page ofthe questionnaire, 2) the service failure was
more clearly delineated from the recovery in the scenarios, and
3) the wording of the stability manipulation statements was
strengthened by changing "rarely" and "always" to
"inconsistent" and "consistent", respectively. Business
students from classes not previously utilized in the scenario
development process (52 male, 33 female) were randomly
assigned to one ofthe six manipulation conditions, scenarioorder
was randomized for each subject. Subjectsranged in age from 20
to 39 years ( x= 24.0 years) and consisted ofseniors (n = 65)
and MBA students (n = 20). Factor analysis with varimax
rotation again confirmed a single-factor structure for each ofthe
five proposed causal dimension sub-scales. Coefficient alphas
for each ofthe causal dimensions were as follows: stability (a =
.79); control (a =.64); customer (a =.77); employee (a =.82);
and firm (a = .84). One significant interaction was found
(Stability with Locus) in one ofthe service industries (i.e., Auto
Repair-work delay). This service industry scenario was
eliminated from further consideration. All of the treatment
manipulations were statistically significant. Duncan's Multiple
Range post-hoc procedure revealed significant differences
betweengroupmeans in all cases. Thefmdings indicatedthatthe
causes were properly classified, and the independent variable
manipUlations were effective. Based on effect sizes and realism
scores, scenarios from the Airline, Cable TV, and Credit Card
industries were retained. All three scenarios were perceived as
both controlled (Airline: x= 1.84, SD = .80; Cable TV: x=
2.07, SD =.86; Credit Card: x=2.16, SD =1.07), and realistic
(Airline: x=1.96, SD =1.32; Cable TV: x=2.84, SD =1.80;
Credit Card: x= 2.20, SD = 1.29). The hotel scenarios were
eliminated from further study as locus effect sizes were lower in
every category relative to the other three services. To further
strengthen the stability and locusmanipulations, wordingchanges
were again made and a third pre-test was conducted.
Pre-Test II! ofScenario Manipulations. Similar to the earlier
pre-tests, subjects were randomly assigned to one of the six
experimentalconditionswith scenarioindustryorderrandomized.
Subjects included86 (49 male, 37 female) businessstudentsfrom
classes that had notparticipated in the earliere!"e-tests. Students
ranged in age from 19 to 50 years ( x = 23.9 years)
Dimensionality ofthe manipulation check items was examined
via factor analysis with varimax rotation. Coefficient alphas for
the causal dimensions were as follows: stability (a = .90);
customer (a = .83); employee (a = .82); and firm (a = .82).
Utilizing ANOVA, all of the treatments were found to be
statistically significant. ANOVA results and omega-squared
statistics are presented in Table 1. Duncan's MUltiple Range
post-hoc test revealed significant differences between group
means in all cases. The fmdings indicated that the scenario
manipulationswere beingproperly classifiedandwere effective.
The three failure scenarios and descriptions for each of the
treatment cells retained for the fmal study are presented in
Appendix Table 1.
Data Collection Method
A convenience sample of customers of four child-care centers
located in a large southeasterncity servedas the samplingframe.
Packets containing a cover letter and two identical surveys were
distributed to each potential respondent at all four centers. The
second survey in the packet was included with a request that
"anyone else in your home over the age of 18" fill out the
questionnaire as well. The packets were placed in a location
(either a file or box depending on the particular center) that was
checked daily by each child's parent or guardian. If siblings
attended a particular center, a single packet was placed in the
youngest child's file or box. A reminder letter was distributed
five days after the initial survey distribution. A survey drop box
was provided and placed prominently near the front entrance
desk of each child-care center. For each center's cooperation,
frozen flavored treats were provided for all ofthe children.
Fall 2001 55
TABLE 1
ANOVA RESULTS OF PRE-TEST III SCENARIO MANIPULATIONS
Dependent Variable F-Ratio' W' F-Ratio'
Airline
Stability
Locus
Customer
Employee
Firm
147.06 .63
34.93
30.15
45.64
.44
.40
.51
Cable TV
Stability
Locus
Customer
Employee
Firm
100.55
7.97
.54
.06 32.18
22.45
27.84
.42
.34
.39
Credit Card
Stability
Locus
Customer
Employee
Firm
'All effects significant at the p<.OOllevei
182.87
Subjects randomly received only one of the six experimental
conditions. The order of presentation of the industries was
randomized for each subject as well. A total 0[332 survey
packets (i.e., 664 surveys) were distributed. There were 212
surveys returned (response rate = 32%), ofwhich 29 were blank,
resulting in a useable response rate of28% (n = 183).
Subjects. Respondents ranged in age from 18 to 71 years ( x=
32.7), with females making up 64.4% of the sample. Most
respondents attended some college (36.9%) or were college
graduates (27.9%). Respondents were familiar with services, as
139 (77.2%Lhad worked in a service business ranging from 1to
40 years ( x = 8.57 years). About one-half of the subjects
(50.2%) had experienced problems similarto those presented in
the scenarios (Airline = 51.4%, Cable TV = 43.9%, and Credit
Card = 55.2%).
Measurement
Service recovery process quality was defined as the customer's
subjective evaluation of 'how' the service recovery was
delivered, relative to the level ofperformance that this particular
type ofservice company can and should deliver (i.e., the desired
service level) (Zeithaml, Berry, and Parasuraman 1993). An
attribute based seven-item, 9-point Likert-type scale applicableto
service recovery process quality was utilized. The SERVQUAL
instrument (Parasuraman, Berry, and Zeithaml 1991, 1993;
Parasuraman, Zeithaml, and Berry 1994)provided the foundation
for the service recovery process quality measure used in this
study.
Service recovery satisfaction was defmed as the customer's
overall psychological state resulting from his/her comparison of
expectations ofwhata service provider 'will' offer, and the actual
recovery experience (Zeithaml, Berry, and Parasuraman 1993).
56 Journal ofMarketing THEORY AND PRACTICE
.68
40.58
41.57
35.07
.48
.49
.45
Similar to other studies (e.g., Kelley and Davis 1994; Oliva,
Oliver, and MacMillan 1992), satisfaction/dissatisfaction is
assumed to be unidimensional ranging from Lower Than
Expected (1) to Higher Than Expected (9). An attribute based
nine-item, 9-point Likert-type scale derived from appropriate
SERVQUAL items was used to measure service recovery
satisfaction (Parasuraman, Berry, and Zeithaml 1991, 1993;
Parasuraman, Zeithaml, and Berry 1994). Word-of-mouth
intentions were defmed as the customer's belief that he or she
would discuss the incident either favorably, unfavorably, or
neutrally with at least one person within the customer's social net
(i.e., family member, friend, acquaintance), who was not directly
involved in the service failure/recovery encounter. This
definition is consistent with Day and Landon's (1977) private
response classification, as well as the word-of-mouth
conceptualization ofRichins (1983). Word-of-mouth intentions
were measured through a four-item, 7-point scale. Subjects
indicated their likelihood of praising/criticizing and
recommending/warning others about the service firm. Finally,
repurchase intentions were defined as a customer's beliefthat he
or she would purchase from the same service firm at some future
date. Repurchase intentions were operationalizedthrough a four-
item, 7-point scale similar to that utilized by Halstead and Page
(1992). The complete perceptions and behavioral intentions
scales are provided in Appendix Table 2.
RESULTS
Factor analysis ofthe evaluative and behavioral intentions items
was conducted using oblique rotation due to correlations among
the constructs (Pedhazur and Schmelkin 1991; Tabachnick and
FideJl 1989). Items loaded well on the appropriate factors.
Word-of-mouth in the Credit Card industry was the only variable
with potential cross-loading concerns (see Table 2). Although
WOM ICredit Card and WOM2Credit Card cross-loaded with the
repurchase intentions factor (F4) they were kept with the word-
of-mouth factor (F3) to be consistent with the Airline and Cable
TV industries. None ofthe items were deleted based on an item-
to-total correlation criterion ofr < .25 (Nunnally 1978). All of
the scales demonstrated strong internal consistency with alphas
ranging from .87 to .97 (see Table 2). The total variance
extracted by the four factors was 80.5%, 79.l%, and 78.0% for
the Airline, Cable TV, and Credit Card scenarios, respectively.
Hypotheses were tested utilizing a repeated measures design of
the multivariate analysis of variance (MANOVA) procedure.
The Mauchly test of sphericity was used to determine if an
adjustment in the degrees of freedom was necessary, thus
affecting critical Fvalues. The Mauchly test findings indicated
rejection ofthe null hypothesis thatthe error covariance matrix
ofthe orthonormalized transformed dependent variables were
proportional to an identity matrix for both quality (W = .960,
P< .05) and satisfaction (W =.961, P< .05). The Greenhouse-
Geisser epsilon was used to adjust the degrees of freedom for
the averaged tests of significance for these variables.
The repeated measures MANOVA indicated significant
between-subjects effects for both stability (F4,456 = 3.65, P <
.01), and locus (Fg916= 2.34, P< .05), but not their interaction
(Fg916 = 1.21, P >..05). In addition, there was a significant
within-subjects effect (i.e., recovery time) (Fg694 = 19.88, P <
.001). Significant interactions also resulted for recovery time
by stability (Fg694 = 3.21, P < .01) and recovery time by locus
(F16 060= 1.71, P< .05). Univariate tests indicated 1) recovery
quaiity was statistically significant (p < .05) in the recovery
time by stability interaction, and 2) satisfaction (p < .05) and
word-of-mouth intentions (p < .05) were statistically
significant in the recovery time by locus interaction. In the
following discussion of the findings relative to the specific
hypotheses, significant findings were based on a .05 (or less)
probability level.
HI - Stability
Findings suggest (see Tables 3-6) that for service recovery
quality the anticipated effect of stability was present for the
Airline industry ( Xstable = 5.13; Xunstable = 4.56), but was not
present in either the Cable TV ( Xstable = 5.51; Xunstable = 5.69)
or Credit Card ( Xstable = 6.14; Xunstable = 6.13) industries.
Examination of the means across the three service settings
considered indicated that stable recoveries result in enhanced
positive word-of-mouth intentions ( Xstable = 4.58; Xunstable =
4}5), and more favorable repurchase intentions ( Xstable= 4.90;
Xunstable =4.53). These findings provide partial support for H1.
H2 - Locus
In the Airline industry, the Duncan's post-hoc test indicated
satisfaction with the recovery attributed primarily to the
customer was rated significantly lower ( x= 4.45) than
satisfaction with the recovery attributed either primarily to the
firm ( x=5.11) or employee ( x=4.99). In the Credit Card
industry recoveries primarily attributed to the !lrm were
associated with the lowest levels of satisfaction ( x = 5.93),
which significan.!.ly differed from those primarily attributed to
the employee ( x = 6.44). Recovery satisfaction evaluations
ヲッセ@ the Cable TV Jndustry ヲッャャセキ・、@ those predicted in H2
( xcustomer = 5.46; xfirm = 5.61; Xemployee = 5.95), but were not
significantly different.
The mean distribution pattern for word-of-mouth intentions in
the recovery time by locus interaction was very similar to
those observed for recovery satisfaction. In the Airline
industry, recoveries primarily attributed to the customer
resulted in significantly lower word-of-mouth intentions ( x
= 3.66). The Duncan's Multiple Range post-hoc test further
revealed no significant group mean differenc:.s between
recoveries primarQy attributed to either the firm ( x =3.93) or
the employee ( x = 3.91). For the Cable TV industry,
recoveries primarily attributed to the employee were
significantly_more likely to elicit favorable セッイ、MッヲMュッオエィ@
intentions ( セ・ューャッケ・・@ = 5.15) relative to firm ( Xfirm =4.48) or
customer ( xcustomer = 4.71) attributed recoveries. Further
examination of the means across the three service recovery
times indicated partial support for H2. Specifically, recoveries
primarily attributed to the employee lead to ィゥァセ・イ@ levels of
ーセイ」・ゥカ・、@ recovery quality ( Xcustomer = 5.29; xfirm = 5.40;
Xemployee = 5.73) across service industries.
H3 - Recovery Time Differences
In H3 it was expected that service recovery evaluations and
intentions would be more favorable in recoveries that were
less complex and more timely. In the context ofour study it
was expected that evaluations and intentions would be more
favorable for the Credit Card scenario followed by the Cable
TV and Airline scenarios, respectively. The Duncan'sMUltiple
Range post-hoc testrevealed ウゥァョゥヲゥ」セエ@ differences 「・エキセ・ョ@ all
group means for recovery quality ( XCreditCard = 6.14; xCableTV
= 5.59; セaゥイャゥョ・@ =4.86), satisfaction ( XCreditCard = 6.2,!! XCableTv
= 5.68; xAirline =4.85), and repurchase intentions ( xCreditCard =
5.37; Xcable TV = 5.01; XAirline = 4.06). Word-of-mouth
ゥョセョエゥッョウ@ were also significantly セゥァィ・イ@ for the Credit Card
( Xwom = 4.94) and Cable TV ( xwo..!!) = 4.80) scenarios in
comparison to the Airline recovery ( xwom = 3.83) (see Tables
3-6). In summary, H3 was supported.
DISCUSSION AND MANAGERIAL IMPLICATIONS
By definition, stability attributions represent customer
perceptions of the likelihood of having the same recovery
experience if the circumstance occurred again in the future.
The findings associated with HI suggest customer behavioral
intentions (i.e., word-of-mouth and repurchase intentions) are
more favorable when customers believe that the recovery they
received is consistently implemented (i.e., is stable) when
failures do occur. Managers might take this into consideration
by making organizational policies regarding service recovery
Fall 2001 57
TABLE 2
ED FACTOR PATTERN AND RELlABn..rry COEFFICIENTS (ALPHAS) FOR MAIN STUDY BEHAVIORAL PERCEPTIONS AND
INTENTIONS SCALES'
Airline
FI F2 F3 F4
(.97)
.86
.82
.81
.82
.81
.78
.74
.94
.93
(.93)
.86
.88
.82
.74
(.95)
.55
.78
.86
.93
.89
.82
.93
(.89)
.73
.74
.81
.92
Cable TV
Fl . F2 F3 F4
(.97)
.89
.94
.92
.83
.78
.78
.81
.93
.85
(.90)
.76
.79
.79
.65
(.95)
.62
.69
.80
.91
.87
.86
.83
(.88)
.72
•.72
.82
.80
Fl
(.97)
.88
.90
.85
.87
.96
.88
.79
.89
.83
Credit Card
F2 F3
(.91)
.87
.91
.86
.61
(.95)
.84
.84
.87
.90
.84
.79
.89
.55
.65
hmlJarentheses are reliability coefficients. The other numbers are factor loadings -obtained after oblique rotation of the initial solutions. Loadin
e been omitted. The total variance extracted by the four factors was 80.5%, 78.0%, and 79.1% for the Airline, Credit Card, and Cable TV sa
Eigenvalues for each of the factors were as follows: Fl == 14.42 airline, 14.00 Cable TV, 12.73 OeditCard; F2 = 3.43 airline, 3.24 Cable TV. 4.44 Credit Card; F3
e TV, 1.21 OeditCard; F4 = .766 airline•.939 Cable TV, .819 Cr,ditCard·
nd behavioral intentions labels correspond to those of the items listed in Appendix Table 2.
Marketing THEORY AND PRACTICE
TABLE 3
DESCRITPTIVE STATISTICS FOR SERVICE RECOVERY PROCESS QUALITY
Airline Cable TV Credit Card Total
Standard Standard Standard Stan
Stability Mean Deviation Mean Deviation Mean Deviation Mean Devi
Stable 4.94 1.43 5.11 .86 5.87 .98 5.29 1.
Unstable 4.27 1.12 5.53 1.51 6.73 1.37 5.30 1.
Total 4.62 QNセ@ 5.30 1.21 6.27 1.24 5.29 1.
Stable 5.18 1.92 5.75 1.34 6.40 1.35 5.80 1.
Unstable 4.89 1.60 6.05 1.03 6.03 1.13 5.65 1.
Total 5.04 1.76 5:90 1.21 6.23 1.25 5.73 1.
Stable 5.30 1.46 5.67 1.52 6.13 1.10 5.72 1.
Unstable 4.48 1.34 5.43 1.32 5.64 1.13 5.06 1.
Total 4.90 1.45 5.55 1.42 5.89 1.13 5.40 1.
Stable 5.13 1.62 5.51 1.28 6.14 1.17 5.59 1.
Unstable 4.56 1.39 5.69 1.31 6.13 1.28 5.35 1.
Total 4.86 1.54 5.59 1.29 6.14 1.22 5.48 1.
TABLE 4
DESCRIPTIVE STATISTICS FOR CUSTOMER SATISFACTION WITH TIlE RECOVERY
Airline Cable TV Credit Card Total
Standard Standard Standard Stan
Stability Mean Deviation Mean Deviation Mean Deviation Mean- Devi
Stable 4.52 1.26 5.16 1.28 5.84 1.06 5.13 1.
Unstable 4.38 .99 5.81 1.63 6.83 1.36 5.42 '1:6
Total 4.45 1.14 5.46 1.48 6.30 1.30 5.26 1.4
Stable 5.00 1.90 5.99 1.28 6.70 1.40 5.92 1.
Unstable 4.99 1.63 5.90 1.07 6.15 1.03 5.74 1.
Total 4.99 1.76 5.95 1.18 6.44 1.26 5.83 1.
Stable 5.24 1.57 5.66 1.61 6.11 1.16 5.73 1.
Unstable 4.97 1.40 5.57 1.18 5.75 1.05 5.35 1.
Total 5.11 1.48 5.61 1.41 5.93 1.11 5.55 1.4
Stable 4.91 1.61 5.61 1.42 6.23 1.26 5.58 1.
Unstable 4.78 1.39 5.77 1.30 6.24 1.22 5.51 1.
Total 4.85 1.51 5.68 1.36 6.24 1.24 5.55 1.5
Fall 2001 59
clear to both employees and customers, and ensuring that
policies are implemented in a consistent manner. In addition,
when service recoveries are implemented it might be good
practice to clearly convey to the customer that the recovery
received is consistently implemented by the representatives of
the service organization in the rare instances when a service
failure occurs.
Within the stability manipulation, recovery qualityperceptions
varied, likely due to subtle differences in the failures
associated with each scenario. In the Cable TV scenarios the
service provider simply performed the service incorrectly.
Similarly, in the Credit Card scenarios the service provider
was wrongfully "demanding" possession of something
belonging to the customer (money). In the Airline scenarios
the airline was in possession ofsomething rightfully belonging
to the customer (luggage). When a service organization
wrongfully has possession of something belonging to the
customer (e.g., lost luggage, lost dry cleaning, misplaced
jewelry), the customer's primary focus when evaluating the
recovery is on having this property returned. In situations
where tangible objects are involved, customers place added
importance on knowing that the problem will be resolved
consistently (Le., stable recovery).
A second generalization drawn from our results concerns who
should execute the service recovery. Based on the findings it
seems safe to recommend that service failures should be
resolved by front-line service personnel whenever possible.
The constant across the three service settings investigated was
the key role of the service employee in perceived recovery
process quality. Our findings provide empirical support of
earlier contentions that proper training and empowerment of
front-line service employees is extremely important to
successfully carry out a service recovery program (Hart,
Heskett, and Sasser 1990).
Antithetically, a service recovery locus - repurchase intentions
relationship was not supported in H2. Although our findings
indicated an employee-based recovery was important for
quality and satisfaction evaluations and word-of-mouth
intentions, who the recovery was attributed to was not
significantly related to customer repurchase intentions.
Overall, the results pertaining to H2 can be interpreted as
follows. First, it seems prudent to have contact employees
resolve service failure situations whenever possible due to the
favorable impact this has on quality, satisfaction and word-of-
mouth intentions. However, depending on their goals for the
recovery process, service managers should not be overly
discouraged when it is not possible for a failure to be resolved
by contact personnel. Future repurchase intentions are not
significantly different whetherthe employee, firm, or customer
is deemed primarily responsible for the recovery. For
managers primarily interested in getting customers who have
experienced a service failure to repurchase in the future it
seems to be less important as to who is primarily responsible
60 Journal ofMarketing THEORY AND PRACTICE
for the recovery process and more important that an
appropriate recovery outcome is provided.
H3 considered the impact of service recovery time and
complexity on customer evaluations and intentions. Service
recovery time and complexity was found to result in
significant differences for the four recovery outcomes as
predicted. The greater complexity and length of the Airline
recovery process resulted in the least favorable recovery
outcomes. The mid-level amount of time involved in the
Cable TV service recovery process resulted in that recovery
having outcomes at a mid-range level. Customers evaluated
the simple and quick Credit Card recovery the most favorably.
It is interesting to note that these differences emerged despite
the fact that our pre-testing results indicated that the recovery
outcomes were perceived as equivalently satisfactory. These
findings lend support to previous suggestions that customer
evaluations will be more positive for service failures remedied
by less complicated and expeditious recovery processes (Hart,
Heskett, and Sasser 1990; Kelley, Hoffman, and Davis 1993).
In the scenarios both time and complexity move together. In
order to better understand these relationships, future
researches may wish to further test these findings by varying
the time and complexity within rather than between services.
For example, by manipulating recovery time within services
it would be possible to account for the expectations that
customers have for the performance in the industry, as well as
account for how the type of failure (i.e., lost luggage, versus
cable problems, versus an incorrect charge) impacts
assessment ofthe recovery.
Based on the findings here, managers will want to take into
account the complexity and length of the serviCe recovery
process in their particular industry. Customer evaluations of
recovery in complex and lengthy service recovery processes
are less positive than for service failures remedied by less
complex and shorter service recovery processes. As a result,
failures encumbered by lengthy orcomplex recovery processes
may likely require more elaborate forms of compensation
during the course ofthe recovery. In addition, providing quick
and simple recoveries in the event of a service failure can
provide a strategic advantage in positioning the firm relative
to competitors.
Limitations and Future Research Directions
Our study provides an experimental investigation ofcustomer
attributions associated with service recovery. Our research
also extends existing service recovery literature and
knowledge by utilizing an expanded locus dimension.
Expanding the locus dimension beyond the traditional view
that dichotomizes locus into internal and external dimensions
enhances our understanding of service recovery. In addition,
our findings provide empirical support for previous conceptual
propositions related to the importance of service recovery
complexity and time. Systematic replication of this study in
other service settings is recommended in order to develop a
deeper understanding of the differences identified in this
research.
In order to establish the soundness of the methodology we
utilized mUltiple pre-tests in developing the scenarios. We
started with a qualitative approach (i.e., Critical Incident
Technique) and rigorously applied the methodology to ensure
that the independentvariable manipUlations were both properly
classified and effective. In the main study a repeated measures
design with multivariate analysis ofvariance was utilized with
established and well-recognized dependent measures. In
addition, independent samples were utilized for each of the
research phases in an effort to improve internal validity.
Convenience samples of college students were utilized in the
development of the scenarios, while the main study used a
convenience sample of day care customers. An important
issue is the appropriateness ofthe subjects given the nature of
the task at hand. In the development and early testing of the
proposed model, it seems apparent that students do experience
service failures and recoveries. In addition, there was not an
a priori reason to conclude that students do not attribute causes
to explain what happens to them, do not have perceptions, or
fail to develop intentions. However, our use of convenience
samples throughout the study introduces the possibility of
selection bias and must be noted as a limitation. Future
researchers may wish to utilize non-student based and
probability samples to address this potential threat to validity.
In addition, this study only included core service failures and
a limited number ofloci in the scenarios tested. In the future,
similar research methods could be used to assess the effects of
attributions on post-recovery evaluations and behaviors
associated with different service failure types (c.f., Bitner,
Booms, and Tetreault 1990; Kelley, Hoffman, and Davis 1993)
across additional locus categories. Case studies could also be
utilized to provide a richer understanding ofthese relationships
in service recovery. Finally, the use of Likert scales for this
type of study also has limitations. Other measurement tools
could be utilized in the future to address this issue.
An alternative interpretation of our findings also merits the
consideration of additional research. Specifically, the effects
we found may be influenced by the competitive nature ofthe
industries included in this study. The three industries
considered in the scenarios vary on several dimensions but in
particular their level of competition. The Airline industry is
characterized by varying levels of competition on routes.
Flight routes to some destinations are virtual monopolies,
while other flight routes are highly competitive and allow the
consumer to pick from several carriers. Local Cable TV
markets are generally characterized by very limited
competition. In fact, this industry might be termed a virtual
monopoly. While there are some competing options available,
such as satellite dishes and digital dish networks, often the
consumer perceives them as either impractical or cost
prohibitive. The Credit Card industry is highly competitive.
Consumers are bombarded with Credit Card offers through
direct mail and telephone. As a result, it is relatively easy for
customers to switch from one service provider to another.
Overall, the three industries considered cover three different
levels of competitiveness. The Airline industry might be
characterized as an industry with moderate levels of
competition and consumer choice. The Cable TV industry is
characterized by very limited levels of competition and
consumer choice, and the Credit Card industry provides an
example of an industry characterized by high levels of
competition and consumer choice. Thus, the three sets of
scenarios used in this research consider a cross-section of
service industries with regard to competition and the extent to
which customers have choices within the industry.
It is difficult to draw any well-grounded industry-specific
conclusions regarding service recovery based on this research.
Therefore, the following speculative interpretation of our
results is presented. Perhaps Airline customers recognize that
while they do have some choices, the choices they have are
limited. As a result, customers may take their limited choice
set into account and evaluate a stable recovery more favorably.
In the essentially noncompetitive Cable TV industry the locus
of the service recovery was a significant factor in customer
evaluations and intentions. In this case, customers may take
into account that they have essentially no viable alternatives.
This lack ofviable alternatives may lead to a customer thought
process something like this: "Even though I have limited
alternatives in this situation, it is nice to know that someone at
my local Cable TV company cares enough to quickly respond
to my request."
Finally, in the highly competitive Credit Card industry
customers valued stability in the recoveries attributed to the
service employee and firm. However, customers rated
unstable recoveries attributed to themselves most favorably of
all the treatments considered in this industry. Credit Card
customers have many choices and in many cases they are
bombarded with these choices on an almost daily basis. First,
as suggested by attribution theory, stable recoveries attributed
to employees and the firm were evaluated more favorably than
unstable recoveries attributed to the same loci. One possible
explanation for the finding concerning the unstable customer
attributed recovery might focus on the level ofrisk and control
associated with the customer's recovery effort. As noted in
the scenarios, customers in the unstable-customer locus cell
treatment rarely take charge in service failure/recovery
situations. Perhaps one reason these customers don't take
control ofthese situations is that there is some risk involved in
that their demands may not be net. However, in the highly
competitive Credit Card industry it might be argued that it is
relatively low risk to take charge in these situations. Credit
Card customers have many choices available. The result is
that it is a "no risk" setting in which to take charge as a
customer - you have many other options ifthings do not work
out. As a result, the customer that is unaccustomed to taking
charge in such situations may do so and feel good about the
Fall 2001 61
fact that he/she obtained some results when taking charge of
the failure situation.
time constraints, among others, may all affect the outcome
variables investigated here. Finally, this research restricted its
focus to recovery attributions. How service failure attributions
and recovery attributions interact to impact post-recovery
evaluations and behaviors needs investigation. Future
examination ofthese types of questions may provide a better
understanding of how marketers can use the service recovery
process as a strategic customer retention tool
Further research that extends our knowledge of the
relationships considered in this study will be beneficial. For
example, mood states and personality traits may impact
perceived quality and satisfaction with a particular service
recovery. In addition, lack ofalternatives, switching costs, and
APPENDIX TABLE I
RETAINED SERVICE FAILURE AND CORRESPONDING RECOVERY SCENARIOS
Airline Cable TV Credit Card
The Service Problem
While traveling on your usual airline, you arrive at
your final destination. You wait at the baggage
claim area, but your luggage does not appear with
the other passengers' items. After checking at the
customer service desk, you are told your luggage
has been mistakenly put on adifferent flight and is
expected to arrive at the airport tomorrow
afternoon.
The Service Outcomes
Stable - Customer:
You demand action. You receive an apology and
the luggage is delivered to you the next afternoon.
You consistently take the initiative to get your
complaints addressed.
Unstable - Customer:
You demand action. You receive an apology and
the luggage is delivered to you the next afternoon.
You inconsistently take the initiative to get your
complaints addressed.
Stable - Employee:
The service employee takes action. You receive
an apology and the luggage is delivered to you the
next afternoon. You have heard that this airlines'
employees consistently take the initiative to
address customer complaints.
Unstable - Employee:
The service employee takes action. You receive
an apology and the luggage is delivered to you the
next afternoon. You have heard that this airlines'
employees inconSistently take the initiative to
address customer complaints.
Stable - Firm:
The airline takes action. You receive an apology
and the luggage is delivered to you the next
afternoon. You have heard that this airline
consistentlytakes the initiative toaddress customer
complaints.
Unstable - Firm:
The airline takes action. You receive an apology
and the luggage is delivered to you the next
afternoon. You have heard that this airline
inconsistently takes the initiative to address
customer complaints.
In order to watch more ofyour favorite television
shows, you decide to have cable installed. Soon
afterthe cable representative hooksyourtelevision
up, your screen goes blank. You call the cable
company about the problem.
You demand action. A repair person shows up
two hours later and corrects the problem. You
consistently take the initiative to get your
complaints addressed.
You demand action. A repair person shows up
two hours later and corrects the problem. You
inconsistently take the initiative to get your
complaints addressed.
The employeetakes action. The employee returns
two hours later and corrects the problem. You
have heard that this cable companys' employees
consis-tentlytake the initiativeto addresscustomer
complaints.
The employee takes action. The employee returns
two hours later and corrects the problem. You
have heard that this cable companys' employees
inconsistently take the initiative to address
customer complaints.
The cable company takes action. A repair person
shows up two hours laterand corrects the problem.
You have heard that this cable company
consistentlytakes the initiativeto addresscustomer
complaints.
The cable company takes action. A repair person
shows up two hours laterand corrects the problem.
You have heard that this cable company
inconsistently takes the initiative to address
customer complaints.
62 Journal ofMarketing THEORY AND PRACTICE
You receive your credit card bill and it includes a
charge that you did not make.
You contact the creditcard company and demand
action. Your account is corrected immediately
andyoureceive an apology for any inconvenience.
You consistently take the initiative to get your
complaints addressed.
You contact the creditcard company and demand
action. Your account is corrected immediately
andyou receive an apologyfor any inconvenience.
You inconsistently take the initiative to get your
complaints addressed.
Aftercontactingthe creditcard company, aservice
employee takes action by apologizing for any
inconvenience, and immediately correcting your
account. You have heard that this credit card
companys' employees consistently take the
initiative to address customer complaints.
Aftercontactingthe creditcard company, aservice
employee takes action by apologizing for any
inconvenience, and immediately correcting your
account. You have heard that this credit card
companys' employees inconsistently take the
initiative to address customer complaints.
After contacting the credit card company, the
company takes action by apologizing for any
inconvenience, and immediately correcting your
account. You have heard that this credit card
companyconSistentlytakesthe initiativetoaddress
customer complaints.
After contacting the credit card company, the
company takes action by apologizing for any
inconvenience, and immediately correcting your
account. You have heard that this credit card
company inconsistently takes the initiative to
address customer complaints.
APPENDIX TABLE 2
PERCEPTIONS AND BEHAVIORAL INTENTIONS BATTERY
Item
Label Item Wording
QUAL I
QUAL2
QUAL3
QUAL4
QUAL5
QUAL6
QUAL7
Dependability in handling customer service problems.
Willingness to handle customer problems.
Ability ofemployees to handle customer complaints.
Courteousness ofemployees.
Employees who have the knowledge to answer customer questions.
Company has the customer's best interest at heart.
Employees treat customers in a caring manner.
Satisfactionb
SAT!
SAT2
SAD
SAT4
SAT5
SAT6
SAT7
SAT8
SAT9
Dependability in handling customer service problems.
Willingness to handle customer problems.
Ability ofemployees to handle customer complaints.
Courteousness ofemployees.
Employees who have the knowledge to answer customer questions.
Company has the customer's best interest at heart.
Employees treat customers in a caring manner.
How the service problem was corrected.
My feelings towards this service outcome can be described as.
WOMI
WOM2
WOM3
WOM4
Word-of-Mouth Intentions'
I would try to convince my friends and relatives to use thtL. .
I would be likely to recommend thiL to others.
I would be likely to convince my friends and relatives not to use thiL . (-)
I would warn others about using thiL . (-)
Repurchase Intentions'
BUYI Would you use this_ again ifyou had a choice?
BUY2 What is the likelihood that you will go back to thtL. next time you need this service?
BUY3 How likely would you be to repurchase from thiL in the future?
BUY4 What is the likelihood that you will switch to 。ョッエィセ@ for this service? (-)
Items identified with a "-" were reverse scored.
'Each item was accompanied by a 9-point Likert-type scale with the labels: I-"Lower Than", 5-"The Same As", and 9 -"Higher Than" My Desired Service
Level.
bEach item was accompanied by a 9-point Likert-type scale with the labels: 1="Lower Than", 5="The Same As", and 9="Higher Than" I Would Have Expected.
'Each item was accompanied by a 7-point Likert-type scale with the anchors: 1="Definitey", 7="Definitely Not".
REFERENCES
Bem, Daryl 1. (1965), "An Experimental Analysis ofSelf-Persuasion, "
Journal 0/Experimental Social Psychology, 1 (August): 199-
218.
__(1967), "Self-Perception: An Alternative Interpretation ofCognitive
Dissonance Phenomena," Psychological Review, 74 (May): 183-
200.
__(1972), "Self-Perception Theory," in Advances in Experimental Social
Psychology, Vol. 6, L. Berkowitz, ed. New York: Academic Press,
1-62.
Berry, Leonard L., A. Parasuraman, and Valarie A. Zeithaml (1994),
"Improving Service Quality in America: Lessons Learned,"
Academy 0/Management Executive, 8 (May): 32-52.
Bitner, Mary Jo (1990), "Evaluating Service Encounters: The Effects of
Physical Surroundings and Employee Responses," Journal 0/
Marketing, 54 (April): 69-82.
- ' Bernard H. Booms, and Mary Stanfield Tetreault (1990), "The
Service Encounter: Diagnosing Favorable and Unfavorable
Incidents," Journal 0/Marketing, 54 (January): 71-84.
Boulding, Williarn, Ajay Kalra, Richard Staelin, and Valarie A. Zeithaml
(1993), "A Dynamic Process Model ofService Quality: From
Expectations to Behavioral Intentions," Journal o/Marketing
Research, 30 (February): 7-27.
Curren, Mary T. and Valerie S. Folkes (1987), "Attributionallnfluences on
Consumers' Desires to Communicate About Products,"
Psychology and Marketing, 4 (Spring): 31-45.
Day, Ralph L. and E. Laird Landon, Jr. (1977), "Toward a Theory of
Consumer Complaining Behavior," in Consumer and Industrial
Buying Behavior, Arch G. Woodside, Jagdish Sheth, and Peter
Bennett, eds. Elsevier North-Holland, Inc., 425-437.
DrOge, Cornelia and Diane Halstead, (1991), "Postpurchase Hierarchies of
Effects: The Antecedents and Consequences ofSatisfaction for
Complainers Versus Non-Complainers," International Journal
o/Research in Marketing, 8: 315-328.
Elig, Timothy W. and Irene Hanson Frieze, (1979), "Measuring Causal
Attributions for Success and Failure," Journal 0/Personality
and Social Psychology, 37 (April): 621-634.
Folkes, Valerie S. (1984), "Consumer Reactions to Product Failure: An
Attributional Approach," Journal o/Consumer Research, 10
(March): 398-409.
__(1998), "Recent Attribution Research in Consumer Behavior: A
Review and New Directions," Journal o/Consumer Research,
14 (March): 548-565.
__, Susan Koletsky, and John L. Graham (1987), "A Field Study of
Causal Inferences and Consumer Reaction: The View from the
Airport," Journal o/Consumer Research, 13 (March): 534-539.
Fall 2001 63
and Barbara Kotsos (1986), "Buyers' and Sellers' Explanations for
- - Product Failure: Who Done It," Journal ofMarketing, 50
(April): 74-80
Gooding, Richard Z. and Angelo J. Kinicki (1995), "Interpreting Event
Causes: The Complementary role of Categorization and
Attribution Processes," Journal ofManagement Studies, 32
(January): 1-22.
Green, Paul E. (1978), Analyzing Multivariate Data, Hinsdale: The Dryden
Press.
Gronroos, Christian (I 988a), Strategic Management and Marketing in the
Service Sector. Report No. 83-104. Cambridge: Marketing
Science Institute.
__(1988b), "Service Quality: The Six Criteria ofGood Perceived
Service Quality," Review ofBusiness, 9 (Winter): 10-13.
Halstead, Diane and Thomas J. Page, Jr. (1992), "The Effects ofSatisfaction
and Complaining Behavior on Consumer Repurchase Intentions,"
Journal ofConsumer Satisfaction, Dissatisfaction and Complaining
Behavior, 5: I-II.
Hart, Christopher W. L., James L. Heskett, and W. Earl Sasser, Jr. (1990),
"The Profitable Art of Service Recovery," Harvard Business
ReView, 68 (July-August): 148-56.
Heider, Fritz(1958), The PsychologyofInterpersonalRelations,New York: John
Wiley and Sons, Inc.
Jacob, Rahul (1994), "Why Some Customers are More Equal Than Others,"
Fortune, 130 (September 19): 215-224.
Jones, Edward E. and Keith Davis (1965), "From Acts to Dispositions: The
Attribution Process in Person Perception," in Advances in
ExperimentalSocialPsychology, Vol. 2. LeonardBerkowitz,ed. New
York, NY: Academic Press, 219-266.
__and Daniel McGillis (1976), "CorrespondentInferences andthe Attribution
Cube: AComparative Reappraisal," in New Directions in Attribution
Research, Vol. I, John Harvey, William Ickes, and Robert Kidd, eds.
Hillsdale, N1: Lawrence Erlbaum, 389-420.
Jones, Thomas O. and W. Earl Sasser, Jr. (1995), "Why Satisfied Customers
Defect," Harvard Business ReView, 73 (November-December): 88-
99.
Keaveney, Susan M. (1995), "Customer Switching Behavior in Service
Industries: An Exploratory Study," Journal of Marketing, 59
(April): 71-82.
Kelley, Harold H. (1967), "AttributionTheory in Social Psychology," inNebraska
Symposium on Motivation, Vol. 15. David Levine, ed. Lincoln, NB:
University ofNebraska Press, 192-238.
__(1971), Attribution in Social Interaction, New York: General Learning
Press.
__(1972), CausalSchemataandtheAttribution Process, Morristown: General
Learning Press.
__(1973), "The Processes ofCausal Attribution," American Psychologist,
28 (February): 107-128.
Kelley, Scott W., K. Douglas Hoffman, and Mark A. Davis (1993), "A Typology
ofRetail Failures and Recoveries," Journal ofRetailing, 69 (Winter):
429-452.
Kerlinger, Fred N. (1986), Foundations ofBehavioral Research Third Edition,
Fort Worth: Holt, Rinehart, and Winston, Inc.
64 Journal ofMarketing THEORY AND PRACTICE
Lichtenstein, Donald R. and William O. Bearden (1986), "Measurement and
Structure of Kelley's Covariance Theory," Journal of Consumer
Research, 13 (September): 290-296.
McDougall, Duncan (1995), "Know thy Customer," The Wall Street Journal,
(August 7): A14.
Nunnally, Jum C. (1978), Psychometric Theory, New York: McGraw-Hili Book
Company.
Oliver, Richard L. (1997), Satisfaction: A Behavioral Perspective on the
Consumer, New York: The McGraw-Hili Companies, Inc.
__and Wayne S. DeSarbo (1988), "Response Determinants in Satisfaction
Judgments," Journal ofConsumer Research, 14 (March): 495-507.
Parasuraman, A., Leonard L. Berry, and Valarie A. Zeithaml (1991),
"Refinement and Reassessment of the SERVQUAL Scale,"
Journal ofRetailing, 67 (Winter): 420-450.
__, - ' and __ (1993), "More on Improving Service Quality
Measurement," Journal ofRetailing, 69 (Spring): 140-147.
__, Valarie A. Zeithaml, and Leonard L. Berry, (1985), "A Conceptual
Model of Service Quality and Its Implications for Future
Research," Journal ofMarketing, 49 (Fall): 41-50.
- ' __, and __, (1994), "Reassessment of Expectations as a
Comparison Standard in Measuring Service Quality: Implications
for Further Research," Journal of Marketing, 58 (January): 111-
124.
Pedhazur, Elazar J. and Liora Pedhazur Schmelkin (1991), Measurement,
DeSign, and Analysis: An Integrated Approach, Hillsdale NJ:
Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Inc.
Perdue, Barbara C. and John O. Summers (1986), "Checking the Success of
Manipulations in Marketing Experiments," Journal ofMarketing
Research, 23 (November): 317-326.
Peters, Tom (1988), Thriving on Chaos, New York: Alfred A. Knopf.
Reichheld, Frederick F. and W. Earl Sasser, Jr. (1990), "Zero Defections: Quality
Comes to Services," Harvard Business Review, 68 (September-
October): 105-111.
Richins, Marsha (1983), "Negative Word-of-Mouth by Dissatisfied Consumers:
A Pilot Study," Journal ofMarketing, 47 (Winter): 68-78.
Russell, Dan (1982), "The Causal Dimension Scale: A Measure of How
Individuals Perceive Causes," Journal ofPersonality and Social
Psychology, 42 (June): 1137-1145.
Rust, Roland T. and Anthony 1. Zahorik (1993), "Customer Satisfaction,
Customer Retention, and Market Share," Journal ofRetailing, 69
(Summer): 193-215.
Smith, Amy K. and Ruth N. Bolton (1998), "An Experimental Investigation
of Service Failure and Recovery: Paradox or Peril?" Journal of
Service Research, I (August): 65-81.
__, __ and Janet Wagner (1998), "A Model of Customer Satisfaction
With Service Encounters Involving Failure and Recovery,"
Marketing Science Institute Report #98-100.
Tabachnick, Barbara G. and Linda S. Fidell (1989), Using Multivariate
Statistics, Second Edition. New York: Harper Collins Publishers,
Inc.
Tax, Stephen S., Stephen W. Brown, and Murali Chandrashekaran (1998),
"Customer Evaluations of Service Complaint Experiences:
Implications for Relationship Marketing," Journal ofMarketing,
62 (April): 60·76.
Valle, Valerie and Melanie Wallendorf(I977), "Consumers' Attributions ofthe
CauseofTheirProductSatisfactionand Dissatisfaction," in Consumer
Satisfaction/DissatisfactionandComplainingBehavior, Ralph L. Day,
ed. Bloomington, IN: Indiana University, 26·30.
Weiner, Bernard (1980), Human Motivation, New York: Holt, Rinehart, and
Winston.
__(I 985a), "An Attributional Theory of Achievement Motivation and
Emotion," Psychological Review, 92 (October): 548·573.
__(1985b), "Spontaneous Causal Thinking," Psychological Bulletin, 97
(January): 74·84.
Wofford J. C. and Vicki L. Goodwin (1990), "Effects ofFeedback on Cognitive
Processing and Choice Decision Style," Journal of Applied
Psychology, 75 (December): 603-612.
Zeithaml, Valarie A., Leonard L. Berry, and A. Parasuraman (1993), "TheNature
and Determinants ofCustomer Expectations ofService," Journal of
the Academy ofMarketing Science, 21 (Winter): 1·12.
AUTHOR BIOGRAPHY
Scott R. Swanson (Ph.D., University of Kentucky) is an assistant professor of marketing. His research interests include
issues related to services marketing, sports sponsorship, and pedagogy. His research has been published in the Journal
ofthe Academy ofMarketing Science, the Journal ofBusiness to Business Marketing, the European Journal of
Marketing, and Psychological Reports, among others.
AUTHOR BIOGRAPHY
Scott W. Kelley (D.B.A., University of Kentucky) is an associate professor ofmarketing and the Director ofthe UK
Center for Sports Marketing. His research has been published in the Journal ofthe Academy ofMarketing Science, the
Journal ofRetailing, the Journal ofBusiness Research, the Journal ofAdvertising, and the Journal ofPersonal Selling
and Sales Management, among others. His research interests include issues concerning services marketing and sports
marketing.
Fall 2001 65

More Related Content

Similar to Attributions And Outcomes Of The Service Recovery Process

The Effects of Customer Expectation and Perceived Service Quality on Custome...
	The Effects of Customer Expectation and Perceived Service Quality on Custome...	The Effects of Customer Expectation and Perceived Service Quality on Custome...
The Effects of Customer Expectation and Perceived Service Quality on Custome...
inventionjournals
 
Chỉ số hài lòng của khách hàng (Customer Satisfaction Index – CSI) - Cơ sở lý...
Chỉ số hài lòng của khách hàng (Customer Satisfaction Index – CSI) - Cơ sở lý...Chỉ số hài lòng của khách hàng (Customer Satisfaction Index – CSI) - Cơ sở lý...
Chỉ số hài lòng của khách hàng (Customer Satisfaction Index – CSI) - Cơ sở lý...
Jackie Nguyen
 
4 faraz javed 1-2
4  faraz javed 1-24  faraz javed 1-2
4 faraz javed 1-2
Umangdeep Sharma
 
The Effects of the Determinants of Customer Satisfaction on Brand Loyalty
The Effects of the Determinants of Customer Satisfaction on Brand LoyaltyThe Effects of the Determinants of Customer Satisfaction on Brand Loyalty
The Effects of the Determinants of Customer Satisfaction on Brand Loyalty
Samaan Al-Msallam
 
Customer satisfaction and brand loyalty in the hotel industry
Customer satisfaction and brand loyalty in the hotel industryCustomer satisfaction and brand loyalty in the hotel industry
Customer satisfaction and brand loyalty in the hotel industry
Samaan Al-Msallam
 
Understanding the customer base of service providers: An examination of the D...
Understanding the customer base of service providers: An examination of the D...Understanding the customer base of service providers: An examination of the D...
Understanding the customer base of service providers: An examination of the D...
Dr. Larry Pino
 
A sample applied to Future Group : Drivers Of Customer Loyalty In A Retail St...
A sample applied to Future Group : Drivers Of Customer Loyalty In A Retail St...A sample applied to Future Group : Drivers Of Customer Loyalty In A Retail St...
A sample applied to Future Group : Drivers Of Customer Loyalty In A Retail St...
Ramesh Godabole
 
Association For Consumer Research
Association For Consumer ResearchAssociation For Consumer Research
Association For Consumer Research
Amy Roman
 
Benefits of customer retention and reichheld’s loyalty management strategy
Benefits of customer retention and reichheld’s loyalty management strategyBenefits of customer retention and reichheld’s loyalty management strategy
Benefits of customer retention and reichheld’s loyalty management strategy
Miraziz Bazarov
 
Salesperson Role Model in Creating Customer Loyalty at Department Store
Salesperson Role Model in Creating Customer Loyalty at Department StoreSalesperson Role Model in Creating Customer Loyalty at Department Store
Salesperson Role Model in Creating Customer Loyalty at Department Store
inventionjournals
 
Commitment and customer loyalty in business to-business context
Commitment and customer loyalty in business to-business contextCommitment and customer loyalty in business to-business context
Commitment and customer loyalty in business to-business context
Alexander Decker
 
Paper industrial enginering
Paper industrial engineringPaper industrial enginering
Paper industrial enginering
Jugala Raya, CV.
 
Module#2 Peter Giardini
Module#2 Peter GiardiniModule#2 Peter Giardini
Module#2 Peter GiardiniPeter Giardini
 
The_Long_Term_Stock_Market_Valuation_of.pdf
The_Long_Term_Stock_Market_Valuation_of.pdfThe_Long_Term_Stock_Market_Valuation_of.pdf
The_Long_Term_Stock_Market_Valuation_of.pdf
ismail981530
 
The relationship between customer satisfaction and customer loyalty in the ba...
The relationship between customer satisfaction and customer loyalty in the ba...The relationship between customer satisfaction and customer loyalty in the ba...
The relationship between customer satisfaction and customer loyalty in the ba...
Samaan Al-Msallam
 
How To Shift Consumer Behaviors to be more sustainable; a literature review a...
How To Shift Consumer Behaviors to be more sustainable; a literature review a...How To Shift Consumer Behaviors to be more sustainable; a literature review a...
How To Shift Consumer Behaviors to be more sustainable; a literature review a...
Nicha Tatsaneeyapan
 

Similar to Attributions And Outcomes Of The Service Recovery Process (20)

The Effects of Customer Expectation and Perceived Service Quality on Custome...
	The Effects of Customer Expectation and Perceived Service Quality on Custome...	The Effects of Customer Expectation and Perceived Service Quality on Custome...
The Effects of Customer Expectation and Perceived Service Quality on Custome...
 
I038079084
I038079084I038079084
I038079084
 
Ostrom dan iacobucci
Ostrom dan iacobucciOstrom dan iacobucci
Ostrom dan iacobucci
 
Chỉ số hài lòng của khách hàng (Customer Satisfaction Index – CSI) - Cơ sở lý...
Chỉ số hài lòng của khách hàng (Customer Satisfaction Index – CSI) - Cơ sở lý...Chỉ số hài lòng của khách hàng (Customer Satisfaction Index – CSI) - Cơ sở lý...
Chỉ số hài lòng của khách hàng (Customer Satisfaction Index – CSI) - Cơ sở lý...
 
4 faraz javed 1-2
4  faraz javed 1-24  faraz javed 1-2
4 faraz javed 1-2
 
The Effects of the Determinants of Customer Satisfaction on Brand Loyalty
The Effects of the Determinants of Customer Satisfaction on Brand LoyaltyThe Effects of the Determinants of Customer Satisfaction on Brand Loyalty
The Effects of the Determinants of Customer Satisfaction on Brand Loyalty
 
Customer satisfaction and brand loyalty in the hotel industry
Customer satisfaction and brand loyalty in the hotel industryCustomer satisfaction and brand loyalty in the hotel industry
Customer satisfaction and brand loyalty in the hotel industry
 
Understanding the customer base of service providers: An examination of the D...
Understanding the customer base of service providers: An examination of the D...Understanding the customer base of service providers: An examination of the D...
Understanding the customer base of service providers: An examination of the D...
 
A sample applied to Future Group : Drivers Of Customer Loyalty In A Retail St...
A sample applied to Future Group : Drivers Of Customer Loyalty In A Retail St...A sample applied to Future Group : Drivers Of Customer Loyalty In A Retail St...
A sample applied to Future Group : Drivers Of Customer Loyalty In A Retail St...
 
Association For Consumer Research
Association For Consumer ResearchAssociation For Consumer Research
Association For Consumer Research
 
Benefits of customer retention and reichheld’s loyalty management strategy
Benefits of customer retention and reichheld’s loyalty management strategyBenefits of customer retention and reichheld’s loyalty management strategy
Benefits of customer retention and reichheld’s loyalty management strategy
 
Salesperson Role Model in Creating Customer Loyalty at Department Store
Salesperson Role Model in Creating Customer Loyalty at Department StoreSalesperson Role Model in Creating Customer Loyalty at Department Store
Salesperson Role Model in Creating Customer Loyalty at Department Store
 
Commitment and customer loyalty in business to-business context
Commitment and customer loyalty in business to-business contextCommitment and customer loyalty in business to-business context
Commitment and customer loyalty in business to-business context
 
Satisfaction
SatisfactionSatisfaction
Satisfaction
 
Paper industrial enginering
Paper industrial engineringPaper industrial enginering
Paper industrial enginering
 
Module#2 Peter Giardini
Module#2 Peter GiardiniModule#2 Peter Giardini
Module#2 Peter Giardini
 
The_Long_Term_Stock_Market_Valuation_of.pdf
The_Long_Term_Stock_Market_Valuation_of.pdfThe_Long_Term_Stock_Market_Valuation_of.pdf
The_Long_Term_Stock_Market_Valuation_of.pdf
 
The relationship between customer satisfaction and customer loyalty in the ba...
The relationship between customer satisfaction and customer loyalty in the ba...The relationship between customer satisfaction and customer loyalty in the ba...
The relationship between customer satisfaction and customer loyalty in the ba...
 
Tucci dan talaga
Tucci dan talagaTucci dan talaga
Tucci dan talaga
 
How To Shift Consumer Behaviors to be more sustainable; a literature review a...
How To Shift Consumer Behaviors to be more sustainable; a literature review a...How To Shift Consumer Behaviors to be more sustainable; a literature review a...
How To Shift Consumer Behaviors to be more sustainable; a literature review a...
 

More from Jim Jimenez

My School Essay Writing - College Homework Help A
My School Essay Writing - College Homework Help AMy School Essay Writing - College Homework Help A
My School Essay Writing - College Homework Help A
Jim Jimenez
 
017 Difference Between Paragraph And Essay Ppt
017 Difference Between Paragraph And Essay Ppt017 Difference Between Paragraph And Essay Ppt
017 Difference Between Paragraph And Essay Ppt
Jim Jimenez
 
40 Can You Use The Same Essay For Different
40 Can You Use The Same Essay For Different40 Can You Use The Same Essay For Different
40 Can You Use The Same Essay For Different
Jim Jimenez
 
Printable Frog Writing Paper Curbeu Co Uk
Printable Frog Writing Paper Curbeu Co UkPrintable Frog Writing Paper Curbeu Co Uk
Printable Frog Writing Paper Curbeu Co Uk
Jim Jimenez
 
013 Essay Example Historiographical Glamoro
013 Essay Example Historiographical Glamoro013 Essay Example Historiographical Glamoro
013 Essay Example Historiographical Glamoro
Jim Jimenez
 
Scholarship Essay Sample About Why I Deserve The
Scholarship Essay Sample About Why I Deserve TheScholarship Essay Sample About Why I Deserve The
Scholarship Essay Sample About Why I Deserve The
Jim Jimenez
 
Lined Printable A4 Paper Letter Writing Personal Us
Lined Printable A4 Paper Letter Writing Personal UsLined Printable A4 Paper Letter Writing Personal Us
Lined Printable A4 Paper Letter Writing Personal Us
Jim Jimenez
 
College Pressures Essay 1 VOL.1 .Docx - Economic Se
College Pressures Essay 1 VOL.1 .Docx - Economic SeCollege Pressures Essay 1 VOL.1 .Docx - Economic Se
College Pressures Essay 1 VOL.1 .Docx - Economic Se
Jim Jimenez
 
Mla Format Double Spaced Essay - Term Paper Doubl
Mla Format Double Spaced Essay - Term Paper DoublMla Format Double Spaced Essay - Term Paper Doubl
Mla Format Double Spaced Essay - Term Paper Doubl
Jim Jimenez
 
012 Essay Example College Application Examples Th
012 Essay Example College Application Examples Th012 Essay Example College Application Examples Th
012 Essay Example College Application Examples Th
Jim Jimenez
 
Critical Review Research Papers
Critical Review Research PapersCritical Review Research Papers
Critical Review Research Papers
Jim Jimenez
 
Samples Of Dissertation Proposals. Writing A Disser
Samples Of Dissertation Proposals. Writing A DisserSamples Of Dissertation Proposals. Writing A Disser
Samples Of Dissertation Proposals. Writing A Disser
Jim Jimenez
 
Sample National Junior Honor Society Essay Tel
Sample National Junior Honor Society Essay TelSample National Junior Honor Society Essay Tel
Sample National Junior Honor Society Essay Tel
Jim Jimenez
 
Papers 9 Essays Research Essay Example Apa Template Microsoft Wor
Papers 9 Essays Research Essay Example Apa Template Microsoft WorPapers 9 Essays Research Essay Example Apa Template Microsoft Wor
Papers 9 Essays Research Essay Example Apa Template Microsoft Wor
Jim Jimenez
 
Personalised Luxury Writing Paper By Able Labels Not
Personalised Luxury Writing Paper By Able Labels NotPersonalised Luxury Writing Paper By Able Labels Not
Personalised Luxury Writing Paper By Able Labels Not
Jim Jimenez
 
Homework Help Best Topics For An Argumentative Essa
Homework Help Best Topics For An Argumentative EssaHomework Help Best Topics For An Argumentative Essa
Homework Help Best Topics For An Argumentative Essa
Jim Jimenez
 
🌈 Essay Writing My Teacher. Essay On My
🌈 Essay Writing My Teacher. Essay On My🌈 Essay Writing My Teacher. Essay On My
🌈 Essay Writing My Teacher. Essay On My
Jim Jimenez
 
Guide To The 2019-20 Columbia University Suppl
Guide To The 2019-20 Columbia University SupplGuide To The 2019-20 Columbia University Suppl
Guide To The 2019-20 Columbia University Suppl
Jim Jimenez
 
Help Writing Papers For College - The Best Place T
Help Writing Papers For College - The Best Place THelp Writing Papers For College - The Best Place T
Help Writing Papers For College - The Best Place T
Jim Jimenez
 
Essay Def. What Is An Essay The Definition And Main Features Of
Essay Def. What Is An Essay The Definition And Main Features OfEssay Def. What Is An Essay The Definition And Main Features Of
Essay Def. What Is An Essay The Definition And Main Features Of
Jim Jimenez
 

More from Jim Jimenez (20)

My School Essay Writing - College Homework Help A
My School Essay Writing - College Homework Help AMy School Essay Writing - College Homework Help A
My School Essay Writing - College Homework Help A
 
017 Difference Between Paragraph And Essay Ppt
017 Difference Between Paragraph And Essay Ppt017 Difference Between Paragraph And Essay Ppt
017 Difference Between Paragraph And Essay Ppt
 
40 Can You Use The Same Essay For Different
40 Can You Use The Same Essay For Different40 Can You Use The Same Essay For Different
40 Can You Use The Same Essay For Different
 
Printable Frog Writing Paper Curbeu Co Uk
Printable Frog Writing Paper Curbeu Co UkPrintable Frog Writing Paper Curbeu Co Uk
Printable Frog Writing Paper Curbeu Co Uk
 
013 Essay Example Historiographical Glamoro
013 Essay Example Historiographical Glamoro013 Essay Example Historiographical Glamoro
013 Essay Example Historiographical Glamoro
 
Scholarship Essay Sample About Why I Deserve The
Scholarship Essay Sample About Why I Deserve TheScholarship Essay Sample About Why I Deserve The
Scholarship Essay Sample About Why I Deserve The
 
Lined Printable A4 Paper Letter Writing Personal Us
Lined Printable A4 Paper Letter Writing Personal UsLined Printable A4 Paper Letter Writing Personal Us
Lined Printable A4 Paper Letter Writing Personal Us
 
College Pressures Essay 1 VOL.1 .Docx - Economic Se
College Pressures Essay 1 VOL.1 .Docx - Economic SeCollege Pressures Essay 1 VOL.1 .Docx - Economic Se
College Pressures Essay 1 VOL.1 .Docx - Economic Se
 
Mla Format Double Spaced Essay - Term Paper Doubl
Mla Format Double Spaced Essay - Term Paper DoublMla Format Double Spaced Essay - Term Paper Doubl
Mla Format Double Spaced Essay - Term Paper Doubl
 
012 Essay Example College Application Examples Th
012 Essay Example College Application Examples Th012 Essay Example College Application Examples Th
012 Essay Example College Application Examples Th
 
Critical Review Research Papers
Critical Review Research PapersCritical Review Research Papers
Critical Review Research Papers
 
Samples Of Dissertation Proposals. Writing A Disser
Samples Of Dissertation Proposals. Writing A DisserSamples Of Dissertation Proposals. Writing A Disser
Samples Of Dissertation Proposals. Writing A Disser
 
Sample National Junior Honor Society Essay Tel
Sample National Junior Honor Society Essay TelSample National Junior Honor Society Essay Tel
Sample National Junior Honor Society Essay Tel
 
Papers 9 Essays Research Essay Example Apa Template Microsoft Wor
Papers 9 Essays Research Essay Example Apa Template Microsoft WorPapers 9 Essays Research Essay Example Apa Template Microsoft Wor
Papers 9 Essays Research Essay Example Apa Template Microsoft Wor
 
Personalised Luxury Writing Paper By Able Labels Not
Personalised Luxury Writing Paper By Able Labels NotPersonalised Luxury Writing Paper By Able Labels Not
Personalised Luxury Writing Paper By Able Labels Not
 
Homework Help Best Topics For An Argumentative Essa
Homework Help Best Topics For An Argumentative EssaHomework Help Best Topics For An Argumentative Essa
Homework Help Best Topics For An Argumentative Essa
 
🌈 Essay Writing My Teacher. Essay On My
🌈 Essay Writing My Teacher. Essay On My🌈 Essay Writing My Teacher. Essay On My
🌈 Essay Writing My Teacher. Essay On My
 
Guide To The 2019-20 Columbia University Suppl
Guide To The 2019-20 Columbia University SupplGuide To The 2019-20 Columbia University Suppl
Guide To The 2019-20 Columbia University Suppl
 
Help Writing Papers For College - The Best Place T
Help Writing Papers For College - The Best Place THelp Writing Papers For College - The Best Place T
Help Writing Papers For College - The Best Place T
 
Essay Def. What Is An Essay The Definition And Main Features Of
Essay Def. What Is An Essay The Definition And Main Features OfEssay Def. What Is An Essay The Definition And Main Features Of
Essay Def. What Is An Essay The Definition And Main Features Of
 

Recently uploaded

How to Make a Field invisible in Odoo 17
How to Make a Field invisible in Odoo 17How to Make a Field invisible in Odoo 17
How to Make a Field invisible in Odoo 17
Celine George
 
Digital Artifact 1 - 10VCD Environments Unit
Digital Artifact 1 - 10VCD Environments UnitDigital Artifact 1 - 10VCD Environments Unit
Digital Artifact 1 - 10VCD Environments Unit
chanes7
 
Biological Screening of Herbal Drugs in detailed.
Biological Screening of Herbal Drugs in detailed.Biological Screening of Herbal Drugs in detailed.
Biological Screening of Herbal Drugs in detailed.
Ashokrao Mane college of Pharmacy Peth-Vadgaon
 
Unit 8 - Information and Communication Technology (Paper I).pdf
Unit 8 - Information and Communication Technology (Paper I).pdfUnit 8 - Information and Communication Technology (Paper I).pdf
Unit 8 - Information and Communication Technology (Paper I).pdf
Thiyagu K
 
Unit 2- Research Aptitude (UGC NET Paper I).pdf
Unit 2- Research Aptitude (UGC NET Paper I).pdfUnit 2- Research Aptitude (UGC NET Paper I).pdf
Unit 2- Research Aptitude (UGC NET Paper I).pdf
Thiyagu K
 
Lapbook sobre os Regimes Totalitários.pdf
Lapbook sobre os Regimes Totalitários.pdfLapbook sobre os Regimes Totalitários.pdf
Lapbook sobre os Regimes Totalitários.pdf
Jean Carlos Nunes Paixão
 
Chapter 4 - Islamic Financial Institutions in Malaysia.pptx
Chapter 4 - Islamic Financial Institutions in Malaysia.pptxChapter 4 - Islamic Financial Institutions in Malaysia.pptx
Chapter 4 - Islamic Financial Institutions in Malaysia.pptx
Mohd Adib Abd Muin, Senior Lecturer at Universiti Utara Malaysia
 
Executive Directors Chat Leveraging AI for Diversity, Equity, and Inclusion
Executive Directors Chat  Leveraging AI for Diversity, Equity, and InclusionExecutive Directors Chat  Leveraging AI for Diversity, Equity, and Inclusion
Executive Directors Chat Leveraging AI for Diversity, Equity, and Inclusion
TechSoup
 
Best Digital Marketing Institute In NOIDA
Best Digital Marketing Institute In NOIDABest Digital Marketing Institute In NOIDA
Best Digital Marketing Institute In NOIDA
deeptiverma2406
 
STRAND 3 HYGIENIC PRACTICES.pptx GRADE 7 CBC
STRAND 3 HYGIENIC PRACTICES.pptx GRADE 7 CBCSTRAND 3 HYGIENIC PRACTICES.pptx GRADE 7 CBC
STRAND 3 HYGIENIC PRACTICES.pptx GRADE 7 CBC
kimdan468
 
The Diamonds of 2023-2024 in the IGRA collection
The Diamonds of 2023-2024 in the IGRA collectionThe Diamonds of 2023-2024 in the IGRA collection
The Diamonds of 2023-2024 in the IGRA collection
Israel Genealogy Research Association
 
The Challenger.pdf DNHS Official Publication
The Challenger.pdf DNHS Official PublicationThe Challenger.pdf DNHS Official Publication
The Challenger.pdf DNHS Official Publication
Delapenabediema
 
Natural birth techniques - Mrs.Akanksha Trivedi Rama University
Natural birth techniques - Mrs.Akanksha Trivedi Rama UniversityNatural birth techniques - Mrs.Akanksha Trivedi Rama University
Natural birth techniques - Mrs.Akanksha Trivedi Rama University
Akanksha trivedi rama nursing college kanpur.
 
A Survey of Techniques for Maximizing LLM Performance.pptx
A Survey of Techniques for Maximizing LLM Performance.pptxA Survey of Techniques for Maximizing LLM Performance.pptx
A Survey of Techniques for Maximizing LLM Performance.pptx
thanhdowork
 
1.4 modern child centered education - mahatma gandhi-2.pptx
1.4 modern child centered education - mahatma gandhi-2.pptx1.4 modern child centered education - mahatma gandhi-2.pptx
1.4 modern child centered education - mahatma gandhi-2.pptx
JosvitaDsouza2
 
S1-Introduction-Biopesticides in ICM.pptx
S1-Introduction-Biopesticides in ICM.pptxS1-Introduction-Biopesticides in ICM.pptx
S1-Introduction-Biopesticides in ICM.pptx
tarandeep35
 
CACJapan - GROUP Presentation 1- Wk 4.pdf
CACJapan - GROUP Presentation 1- Wk 4.pdfCACJapan - GROUP Presentation 1- Wk 4.pdf
CACJapan - GROUP Presentation 1- Wk 4.pdf
camakaiclarkmusic
 
June 3, 2024 Anti-Semitism Letter Sent to MIT President Kornbluth and MIT Cor...
June 3, 2024 Anti-Semitism Letter Sent to MIT President Kornbluth and MIT Cor...June 3, 2024 Anti-Semitism Letter Sent to MIT President Kornbluth and MIT Cor...
June 3, 2024 Anti-Semitism Letter Sent to MIT President Kornbluth and MIT Cor...
Levi Shapiro
 
Digital Artifact 2 - Investigating Pavilion Designs
Digital Artifact 2 - Investigating Pavilion DesignsDigital Artifact 2 - Investigating Pavilion Designs
Digital Artifact 2 - Investigating Pavilion Designs
chanes7
 
A Strategic Approach: GenAI in Education
A Strategic Approach: GenAI in EducationA Strategic Approach: GenAI in Education
A Strategic Approach: GenAI in Education
Peter Windle
 

Recently uploaded (20)

How to Make a Field invisible in Odoo 17
How to Make a Field invisible in Odoo 17How to Make a Field invisible in Odoo 17
How to Make a Field invisible in Odoo 17
 
Digital Artifact 1 - 10VCD Environments Unit
Digital Artifact 1 - 10VCD Environments UnitDigital Artifact 1 - 10VCD Environments Unit
Digital Artifact 1 - 10VCD Environments Unit
 
Biological Screening of Herbal Drugs in detailed.
Biological Screening of Herbal Drugs in detailed.Biological Screening of Herbal Drugs in detailed.
Biological Screening of Herbal Drugs in detailed.
 
Unit 8 - Information and Communication Technology (Paper I).pdf
Unit 8 - Information and Communication Technology (Paper I).pdfUnit 8 - Information and Communication Technology (Paper I).pdf
Unit 8 - Information and Communication Technology (Paper I).pdf
 
Unit 2- Research Aptitude (UGC NET Paper I).pdf
Unit 2- Research Aptitude (UGC NET Paper I).pdfUnit 2- Research Aptitude (UGC NET Paper I).pdf
Unit 2- Research Aptitude (UGC NET Paper I).pdf
 
Lapbook sobre os Regimes Totalitários.pdf
Lapbook sobre os Regimes Totalitários.pdfLapbook sobre os Regimes Totalitários.pdf
Lapbook sobre os Regimes Totalitários.pdf
 
Chapter 4 - Islamic Financial Institutions in Malaysia.pptx
Chapter 4 - Islamic Financial Institutions in Malaysia.pptxChapter 4 - Islamic Financial Institutions in Malaysia.pptx
Chapter 4 - Islamic Financial Institutions in Malaysia.pptx
 
Executive Directors Chat Leveraging AI for Diversity, Equity, and Inclusion
Executive Directors Chat  Leveraging AI for Diversity, Equity, and InclusionExecutive Directors Chat  Leveraging AI for Diversity, Equity, and Inclusion
Executive Directors Chat Leveraging AI for Diversity, Equity, and Inclusion
 
Best Digital Marketing Institute In NOIDA
Best Digital Marketing Institute In NOIDABest Digital Marketing Institute In NOIDA
Best Digital Marketing Institute In NOIDA
 
STRAND 3 HYGIENIC PRACTICES.pptx GRADE 7 CBC
STRAND 3 HYGIENIC PRACTICES.pptx GRADE 7 CBCSTRAND 3 HYGIENIC PRACTICES.pptx GRADE 7 CBC
STRAND 3 HYGIENIC PRACTICES.pptx GRADE 7 CBC
 
The Diamonds of 2023-2024 in the IGRA collection
The Diamonds of 2023-2024 in the IGRA collectionThe Diamonds of 2023-2024 in the IGRA collection
The Diamonds of 2023-2024 in the IGRA collection
 
The Challenger.pdf DNHS Official Publication
The Challenger.pdf DNHS Official PublicationThe Challenger.pdf DNHS Official Publication
The Challenger.pdf DNHS Official Publication
 
Natural birth techniques - Mrs.Akanksha Trivedi Rama University
Natural birth techniques - Mrs.Akanksha Trivedi Rama UniversityNatural birth techniques - Mrs.Akanksha Trivedi Rama University
Natural birth techniques - Mrs.Akanksha Trivedi Rama University
 
A Survey of Techniques for Maximizing LLM Performance.pptx
A Survey of Techniques for Maximizing LLM Performance.pptxA Survey of Techniques for Maximizing LLM Performance.pptx
A Survey of Techniques for Maximizing LLM Performance.pptx
 
1.4 modern child centered education - mahatma gandhi-2.pptx
1.4 modern child centered education - mahatma gandhi-2.pptx1.4 modern child centered education - mahatma gandhi-2.pptx
1.4 modern child centered education - mahatma gandhi-2.pptx
 
S1-Introduction-Biopesticides in ICM.pptx
S1-Introduction-Biopesticides in ICM.pptxS1-Introduction-Biopesticides in ICM.pptx
S1-Introduction-Biopesticides in ICM.pptx
 
CACJapan - GROUP Presentation 1- Wk 4.pdf
CACJapan - GROUP Presentation 1- Wk 4.pdfCACJapan - GROUP Presentation 1- Wk 4.pdf
CACJapan - GROUP Presentation 1- Wk 4.pdf
 
June 3, 2024 Anti-Semitism Letter Sent to MIT President Kornbluth and MIT Cor...
June 3, 2024 Anti-Semitism Letter Sent to MIT President Kornbluth and MIT Cor...June 3, 2024 Anti-Semitism Letter Sent to MIT President Kornbluth and MIT Cor...
June 3, 2024 Anti-Semitism Letter Sent to MIT President Kornbluth and MIT Cor...
 
Digital Artifact 2 - Investigating Pavilion Designs
Digital Artifact 2 - Investigating Pavilion DesignsDigital Artifact 2 - Investigating Pavilion Designs
Digital Artifact 2 - Investigating Pavilion Designs
 
A Strategic Approach: GenAI in Education
A Strategic Approach: GenAI in EducationA Strategic Approach: GenAI in Education
A Strategic Approach: GenAI in Education
 

Attributions And Outcomes Of The Service Recovery Process

  • 1. Full Terms & Conditions of access and use can be found at http://www.tandfonline.com/action/journalInformation?journalCode=mmtp20 Download by: [Institute of Professional Studies] Date: 01 March 2017, At: 05:45 Journal of Marketing Theory and Practice ISSN: 1069-6679 (Print) 1944-7175 (Online) Journal homepage: http://www.tandfonline.com/loi/mmtp20 Attributions and Outcomes of the Service Recovery Process Scott R. Swanson & Scott W. Kelley To cite this article: Scott R. Swanson & Scott W. Kelley (2001) Attributions and Outcomes of the Service Recovery Process, Journal of Marketing Theory and Practice, 9:4, 50-65, DOI: 10.1080/10696679.2001.11501903 To link to this article: http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/10696679.2001.11501903 Published online: 08 Dec 2015. Submit your article to this journal Article views: 60 View related articles Citing articles: 14 View citing articles
  • 2. ATTRIBUTIONS AND OUTCOMES OF THE SERVICE RECOVERY PROCESS Scott R. Swanson University of Wisconsin-Whitewater Scott W. Kelley University of Kentucky Drawing on attribution theory and the services marketing literature, the authors examine how the allocation ofcausality and length ofthe service recovery process impact post-recovery consumer perceptions ofservice quality, customer satisfaction and behavioral intentions for word-of-mouth and repurchase. Results of a scenario-based repeated measures design suggest that 1) customer behavioral intentions are more favorable in stable service recoveries, 2) an employee based service recovery results in more favorable evaluations and word-of-mouth intentions, and 3) customer evaluations and behavioral intentions will be more positive for service failures remedied by expeditious and less complicated recovery processes. Managerial implications and future research directions are presented. INTRODUCTION It is widely recognized that no service system is perfect. Mistakes do occur. Fortunately, accepting the inevitability of service failures does not imply the automatic loss of customers. When a service failure occurs, "the customer's confidence in the firm hangs in the balance. The company can make things better with the customer - at least to some extent - or make things worse" (Berry, Parasuraman, and Zeithaml 1994, p. 38). It has even been suggested that, through a phenomenon known as the service recovery paradox, a successful recovery can result in a more favorable encounter than if the transaction had been performed correctly the first time (e.g., Hart, Heskett, and Sasser 1990). In order to better retain service customers, it is essential, therefore, that marketers understand the manner by which customers come to accept (or reject) service recovery attempts. 50 Journal ofMarketing THEORY AND PRACTICE There are substantial economic benefits to those that can master the art of service recovery. For example, it has been noted that a 5% decrease in the customer defection rate can boost profits from 25% to 95% (Jacob 1994). Other researchers have suggested that long-term customers generate increasingly more profits year after year (Reichheld and Sasser 1990). This increase in profits manifests itselfthrough several sources. First, costs decline due to the reduced expense of replacing defecting customers. Second, repeat customers often make fewer demands on employee time due to realistic expectations. At the same time, employees may become more efficient due to familiarity with the customer's needs. Third, the costs ofpromotion, credit verification, and new account set up to attract a new customer can be as much as five times the cost of retaining the original customer (Peters 1988). In addition to the economic benefits of retaining customers through effective recoveries, the less tangible losses associated
  • 3. with dissatisfied customersgenerating negative word-of-mouth about the service provider and the service firm should be considered (Keaveney 1995). As a result, many companies are now viewing customers as valuable assets with the realization that their customer portfolio is the "ultimate source of their companies' growth and profitability" (McDougall 1995). The impact of the service recovery process on customer evaluations and repurchase intentions is an important topic for both services marketing researchers and managers. There have been relatively few theoretically based empirical studies on service recovery to date (e.g., Smith and Bolton 1998; Smith, Bolton, and Wagner 1998; Tax, Brown, and Chandrashekaran 1998). A great deal of the service recovery research has primarilyconsisted ofidentifying and classifying recovery types (e.g., Bitner, Booms, and Tetreault 1990; Kelley, Hoffinan, and Davis 1993). More systematic theoretically based empirical research is necessary to advance our knowledge ofthe service failure/recovery phenomenon. Itisproposed inthis studythatthe perceived causes ofa recovery can have a significant impact on post-service recovery evaluations and behaviors. Attribution theory is proposed as a theoretical basis that can provide additional insight into the factors that determine customer perceptions ofan organization's recovery efforts in response to a service failure. Subsequently we provide a briefoverview ofattribution theory, present hypotheses based on attribution theory and services literature, and then test our hypotheses in three different service industries using a scenario-based experimental design. Finally, we report our results, present implications for managers, and discuss limitations and future research directions. ATTRIBUTION THEORY Attribution theory is a collection of several theories that are concerned with the assignment of causal inferences and how these interpretations influence evaluations and behavior. The attribution field has grown from a variety of research streams. These works include: Heider's seminal writing on naive psychology (1958), Bem's self-perception theory (1965,1967, 1972), Jones and associates' correspondence of inference work (Jones and Davis 1965; Jones and McGillis 1976), Kelley's theory ofexternalattribution(1967, 1971, 1972, 1973),and more recently, the research ofWeiner(1980, 1985a, 1985b). The wide array ofsocial interaction phenomenato which attributiontheory can beappliedhas made thistheory oneofthe primary paradigms insocial psychology. As aresult, attributiontheory has alsobeen widely adoptedbymarketingscholars(e.g., Folkes 1984;Curren and Folkes 1987; Wofford and Goodwin 1990; Gooding and Kinicki 1995). Heider(1958) recognized that some attributional characteristics fluctuate (e.g., effort and luck), while others are relatively fixed (e.g., ability). This stability of a cause determines shifts in expectancies (Weiner 1980). Thus, if circumstances are perceived to remain the same (i.e., stable), then outcomes experienced will be presumed to continue. However, if the causal conditions are perceived as being likely to change (i.e., unstable), there will be uncertainty about subsequent outcomes. Heider (1958) also identified an internal-external causal dimension as fundamental to the attribution process. He noted that outcomes ofany action depend on two sets ofconditions, "factorswithinthe person and factors withintheenvironment" (p. 82). Weiner(1980) classifiedthis internal-externaldistinctionas the locus ofcausality dimension. Attributiontheory alsorecognizesthat individualsarenotalways constrained by environmental factors, but make choices. Heider (1958) related controllabilityto creditand blame. Forexample, ifan organization has control in preventing a service failure, but fails to do so, consumers may blame the firm. Conversely, the company may be more likely to be given credit for positive actions. In sum, individuals experience and/or witness events and make inferences about the causes of these occurrences in order to exercise control of their world. These causes can be classifiedwithin thethreeprincipaldimensions ofstability(Isthe cause likely to recur?), locus (Who is responsible?), and controllability (Did the responsible party have control over the cause?) (Bitner 1990). Attribution theory has previously provided significant insights into product failure experiences. For example, failure attributed to a seller is more likely to 1) elicit complaints to the firm and warnings to others (Richins 1983; Curren and Folkes 1987); 2) lead to less satisfaction (Oliver and DeSarbo 1988); and 3) impact beliefs that the customer is owed apologies and/or refunds (Folks 1984; Kelley, Hoffman, and Davis 1993). Ifa customer determines that the responsible party for a failure had control over the cause they will be angrier, have lower repurchase intentions, and have a greater desire to complain (Folkes, Koletsky, and Graham 1987). Stability has been found to influence the type of redress preferred when a product fails; "compared to unstable reasons, stable attributions lead consumers to more strongly prefer refunds rather than exchanges" (Folkes 1988, p. 557). When product failure is perceived as due to stable factors the customer believes failure will re-occur and they express a desire for a monetary refund. Ifthe failure is perceived as due to unstable causes, then subsequent product satisfaction is expected and product exchange is preferred (Folkes 1984). In sum, attributions have been found to influence how consumers communicate (Richins 1983; Curren and Folkes 1987; Folkes, Koletsky, and Graham 1987), satisfaction or dissatisfaction (Oliverand DeSarbo 1988), preferred recovery (Folkes 1984; 1988), and future repurchase intentions (Folkes, Koletsky, and Graham 1987). As with product failures, attribution theory may also provide insights into consumer perceptions and intentions relative to service recovery experiences. Specific attribution based hypotheses are presented next. Fall 2001 51
  • 4. HYPOTHESES Service recovery involves the specific actions taken in response to a service failure (Gronroos 1988a). The service recovery actions resulting from a service failure might be implemented consistently or inconsistently (stability). Previous research also indicates the locus of these actions might be the service organization, the service employee, or the service customer (Kelley, Hoffman, and Davis 1993). In addition, the length of time required to execute the recovery may vary (Hart, Heskett, and Sasser 1990; Kelley, Hoffman, and Davis 1993). In the following attribution-based hypotheses we specifically address the causal dimensions of stability and locus, with controllability held constant. We also consider the time involved in the recovery across three different services. HI -- Stability and Service FaiIurelRecovery Outcomes Researchers have demonstrated that consumers engage in attribution search for various product failures (e.g., Folkes 1984; Curren and Folkes 1987; Wofford and Goodwin 1990; Gooding and Kinicki 1995). Generally, if the causes of an outcome are expected to remain unchanged then an increased degree of certainty is associated with evaluations and future behaviors. The higher degree of certainty should result in more favorable evaluations and future behaviors, assuming the encounter in question was acceptable. But, ifthe causes ofan outcome are expected to change, then the lack ofcertainty has an adverse impact on evaluations and future behaviors (Weiner 1980). In this fashion, the stability of a causal attribution associated with a service recovery has an impact on customer evaluations and future behaviors. Further supporting evidence of this relationship is offered through services research that demonstrates customers greatly value consistency and reliability in service delivery (Berry, Parasuraman, and ZeithamI1994). For example, a dry cleaner damages a customer's shirt. After the dry cleaner apologizes, the customer is told to purchase a replacement and the company will reimburse him for the cost of the shirt. This is always the procedure followed for any damage to a customer's clothing (i.e., stable). However, if over a number ofyears the customer finds that in similar situations he one time gets reimbursed, another time an in-house repair is attempted, and yet in another incident only an explanation is offered, he can no longer anticipate with any degree ofcertainty what outcome will be received if there is a service failure (i.e., unstable). Based on research from the attribution theory and services literature, recoveries perceived as stable should lead to more favorable customer evaluations and behaviors. Thus, the following hypothesis is proposed: HI: Stable service recovery attributions will result in more favorable recovery evaluations and behavioral intentions than unstable service recovery attributions. 52 Journal ofMarketing THEORY AND PRACTICE H2 -- Locus and Service FaiIurelRecovery Outcomes Consumerand marketing researchers (e.g., Richins 1983; Folkes 1984; Curren and Folkes 1987; Oliver and DeSarbo 1988) have investigated internal and external customer locus attributions. Valle and Wallendorfs (1977) content analysis of customer attributions for causes of satisfaction and dissatisfaction with products suggests the importance of utilizing an expanded external locus dimension in consumer research. Subsequent services research conducted using the critical incidenttechnique (CIT) indicates that service customers perceive that their experiences can be primarily attributed to eitherthe service fum, the service employee, or the service customer (Bitner, Booms, and Tetreault 1990; Kelley, Hoffman, and Davis 1993). For example, service failures may be attributed by the customer to him- orherself("l should have made myselfclearer"), the contact employee ("that was the rudest person I ever met"), or the company ("ifthat is their policy I will go elsewhere"). Service recovery can also be attributed to the customer ("I really know how to get things straightened out"), the contact person ("that young woman sure took care ofthat problem promptly"), or the fum ("that company really stands behind its work"). Although attribution theory does not specifically address the impact of mUltiple external locus dimensions, it is expected that locus attributions to the customer, service employee, and service fum will be differentially related to recovery outcomes (c.f., Folkes, Koletsky, and Graham 1987; Bitner 1990). In one ofthe few studies that directly examined consequences of locus attributions on customer evaluations, Oliver and DeSarbo (1988) found that successful outcomes resulting from an external locus lead to greater satisfaction than when success is attributed to the self. In the context ofthe present study, one would expect customers to evaluate service recoveries attributed to either the service employee or firm more favorably than those attributed to themselves (i.e., customers). Further, previous research suggests that recoveries enacted by frontline personnel may be evaluated more favorably than recoveries attributed to the organization or its higher-level representatives (Hart, Heskett, and Sasser 1990; Kelley, Hoffman, and Davis 1993). Specifically, the following hypothesis is suggested: H2: Service recoveries attributed to the service employee will result in the most favorable recovery evaluations and behavioral intentions followed by service recoveries attributedto the service firm and customer, respectively. H3 - Recovery Time Differences and Service FailurelRecovery Outcomes The three sets ofscenarios used in this research (Airline, Cable TV, Credit Card) considervarying service recovery times. These scenarios are presented in Appendix Table 1 and their development is subsequently discussed in detail. Previous research (e.g., Hart, Heskett, and Sasser 1990; Kelley, Hoffman, and Davis 1993) has suggested that customer evaluations of recovery in complex and lengthy service recovery processes may
  • 5. be less positive than for service failures remedied by less complex and shorter recovery processes. The varying levels of complexity and length ofthe service recoveries investigated are expected to lead to differences across the three industries considered. The Airline failurelrecovery scenario involves a relatively complex and lengthy recovery process related to lost luggage. For instance, the lost luggage recovery might be enacted in the following way: I) the airline employee receives the customer complaint and completes the forms necessary to begin the recovery process, 2) the airline baggage personnel locate the luggage and place it on the right plane, 3) baggage personnel at the destination unloadthe luggage, and4)otherairline personnel deliverthe luggage to you the next day. The airline scenario has the longest recovery time ofthe scenarios utilized (i.e., the next aftemoon). In contrast, the Cable TV service recovery process is less complex and much shorter. In this case the customer calls the CableTV company and arepairperson is there to fix the problem within twohours. The numberofpeople involved in the recovery process is smaller and the time involved in the recovery process is much shorter. In the CreditCardscenariothe service recovery process is even less complex and shorter. The customercalls the CreditCardcompany, speaks with one individualandthe account is correctedimmediately. The simplicityand limitedeffortonthe part of the customer in this case may increase the satisfaction level derived from the recovery. Therefore, based on scenario recovery complexity and time differences the following hypothesis is proposed: H3: Service recovery evaluations and the resulting behavioral intentions will be more favorable in recoveries that are less complex and more timely. Service FailurelRecovery Outcomes In this study we consider four principle outcomes of service failure/recovery encounters. These outcomes includeevaluative outcomes and behavioral outcomes. Thetwo principleevaluative outcomes of the service failurelrecovery encounter that we considerare perceived service quality and customersatisfaction. The behavioral outcomes considered include word-of-mouth intentions and repurchase intentions. Subsequently, all four of these outcomes are briefly discussed. PerceivedService Quality. Several authors distinguish between two basic types of service quality (e.g., Gronroos 1983b; Parasuraman, Zeithaml, and Berry 1985; Kelley, Hoffman, and Davis 1993). The first quality dimension is technical quality, which relates to what is delivered, and isjudged by the customer after the service is performed. Technical or outcome quality is the culmination of having a service need met. The second dimension focuses on how the service is delivered (fUnctional or processquality), and relatestothe experiencethatacustomerhas while the need is being met. The focus of our research is on servicerecoveryprocess quality. Causal attributions associated with service recoveries are predicted to influence customer perceptions ofservice recovery process quality, even when the service recovery outcome is held constant. Customer Satisfaction. Satisfied customers experience "a pleasurable level of consumption-related fulfillment" (Oliver 1997, p. 13). Consumers rely on customerexpectationsto reach a judgment regarding the fulfillment response associated with customersatisfaction(e.g., DrogeandHalstead 1991;Oliverand DeSarbo 1988). A customer is assumed to have expectations regarding service performance, and these expectations are compared with actual perceptions ofperformance as the service is consumed. It is reasonable to assume that this same internal process occurs regarding service failures and recoveries. Word-ol-Mouth Intentions. Several services marketing researchers have considered the word-of-mouth intentions associatedwithserviceencounters(e.g., Parasuraman,Zeithaml, and Berry 1988; Parasuraman, Berry, and Zeithaml 1991; Boulding et al. 1993). Word-of-mouth communications are recognized as a very common and important form of communication for services marketers and customers. The majority of dissatisfied customers will participate in private word-of-mouth as opposed to either taking no action, or registering a formal complaint of some form (Richins 1983). This study focuses on consumers' intended word-of-mouth communications following aservice failure/recovery encounter. Repurchase Intentions. The benefits of maintaining a base of long-term customersare widely recognizedbyservicesmarketers (e.g., Reichheld and Sasser 1990). One means of assessing customerloyaltyand hencethe likelihoodofcustomers returning is through their repurchase intentions (Jones and Sasser 1995). In this study we specifically consider the post-service failurelrecovery repurchase intentions ofconsumers. THE STUDY Research Methods Two levelsofservice recoverystability (stableand unstable) and three levels of service recovery locus (customer, service employee, and service firm) were manipulated in a scenario format. The design was replicated within subjects across three service industries in which the complexity and length of the service recovery process were manipulated (Airline, Cable TV and Credit Card). Subjects were randomly assigned to receive one of the six experimental conditions in each of the three services. Scenario Development The scenarios used in this study were developed in several separate stages using independent samples. In order to obtain a better understanding of common, naturally occurring service failures and their outcomes, the Critical IncidentTechnique was Fall 2001 53
  • 6. utilized. This procedure adhered to the suggestions of other authors (e.g., Elig and Frieze 1979; Lichtenstein and Bearden 1986) by using open-ended elicitation to determine realistic service failures and their outcomes. In the fIrst stage, seventy-six service failure/recovery incidents were collected early in the semester from student subjects (43 male, 33 female) enrolled in two Marketing Principles courses taught by one of the authors at a large southeastern university. Twenty-two different types ofservices with a full range ofpoor to excellent recoveries were cited in the collected critical incidents. Failure incidents from eightdifferenttypes ofservices were chosen for further developmentbased on their frequency of occurrence in the pool(Airline-lost luggage; Auto Repair-work delay; Cable Television-loss ofservice; Credit Card Company- incorrect billing; Dry Cleaners-damaged shirt; Fast Food-cold meal; Hotel-overbooked on rooms; Theater-movie projector breakdown). Each ofthese failure incidents could be classified as a core service failure, as they were due to mistakes or other technical problems with the service itself(Keaveney 1995). Core service failures were one of the major critical incident groups identified by Bitner, Booms, and Tetreault(1990) and have been subsequently considered by several other researchers (e.g., Kelley, Hoffman, and Davis 1993; Bitner, Booms, and Mohr 1994; Keaveney 1995). In the secondstage, we identifIed satisfactory service recoveries. A number of alternative service recovery resolutions were generated for each of the eight retained failures. Four expert judges (faculty members and doctoral students familiar with the services literature) and a sample of forty-one undergraduate business students (23 male, 18 female) enrolled in an introductory marketing course were presented with the eight incidents and asked to write down three possible resolutions to each situation: one poor, one satisfactory, and one excellent resolution. The subjects had not participated in the reporting of the original critical incidents. The responses ofthe expertjudges and forty-one participants were then utilized to identifY a range ofpossible recoveries for each incident. In the third stage, fifty-eight undergraduate businessstudents (30 male, 28 female) that had not participated in the previous scenario development stages were asked to scale the service recoveries for each incidentusinga9-pointLikert-type scale with the labelsofPoorResolution(1),SatisfactoryResolution(5), and Excellent Resolution (9). The subjects were from an advanced marketing course and were ーイ・、ッュゥョ。エセケ@ seniors (87.9%) that ranged in age from 20 to 34 years ( x = 22.1 years). This procedure provided a means for quantifYing the ratings of the service recoveries being considered. Service recoveries with wide variances in theirratings were discarded (standarddeviation of 2.0 or more). Based on the computed mean values, one satisfactoryrecovery for each servicefailure incidentwas chosen for inclusion in the study. In this stage of the scenario development process, subjects also rated the realism ofeach of the eight service failure incidents on a 9-point Likert-type scale with anchors ofVery Realistic (I) and Very Unrealistic (9). The 54 Journal ofMarketing THEORY AND PRACTICE mean values for the realism scale ranged from 2.31 to 3.80. Thus, all eight ofthe service failure incidents were perceived as realistic. Pre-Test I ofScenario Manipulations. Scenarios were further developed and refined through the following procedures. First, recoveries associated with eachoftheeightfailure incidentswere developed by the authors to fIt each ofthe six cells included in the design. Then fIve expert judges familiar with the services and attribution theory literature (faculty members and doctoral students) reviewed each of the failure/recovery scenarios and classified causes of the recoveries into discrete attribution categories (i.e., stable or unstable; customer-attributed, service employee-attributed, orservice fIrm-attributed; andcontrolledor uncontrolled). After two rounds ofrevisions, the scenarios were reviewed by a second group of three faculty members also familiar with both the services and attribution theory literature. Their review resulted in additional wording changes. The scenarios were pre-tested with 91 (51 male, 40 female) undergraduate business students to determine realism and ifthe attribution manipulations were being ー・イ」・ゥカ・セ@ as anticipated. Subjects ranged in age from 18 to 33 years ( x = 21.8 years), were predominately juniors (n = 26) and seniors (n = 59), and were drawn from classes that had not participated in the earlier scenario development stages. After reading each scenario, subjects completed a fifteen-item modifIed Causal Dimensions Scale (Russell 1982) designed to assess causal perceptions ofa particular situation in terms of the locus, stability, and controllability dimensions. The original scale consisted ofnine randomly presentedsemanticdifferentialstatementsthatrelate to aparticularsituation,three for each causaldimension. Due to the enlarged locus dimension utilized in this study, the locus scale items were adapted to capture customer perceptions of attributions forthe self(i.e., customer),the serviceemployee,and the service firm. For each locus dimension respondents were asked if I) taking action was something that was "Outside" or "Inside" of"You", "The Employee", or "The Firm", 2) taking action was something about "Other(s)" or "You", "The Employee", or "The Firm", and 3) if the action taken reflected "The Situation" or "You", "The Employee", or "The Firm". Control was assessed by asking subjects if the outcome of a scenario was I) "Intended" or "Unintended", 2) "Controllable" or "Uncontrollable", and if3) "Someone was Responsible" or "No One was Responsible". Stability items asked subjects ifthe action taken in a particular scenario was perceived as I) "Permanent" or"Temporary", 2) "Stable" or"Unstable", and 3) "Unchanging" or"Changing". Asingle-item 9-pointLikert-type scale was again utilized to assess scenario realism (l="Very Realistic" and 9="Very Unrealistic"). The modifiedCausal Dimensions Scalewas factoranalyzedwith varimax rotation. FindingsconfIrmedasingle-factorstructure for each ofthe five causal dimensions. Coefficient alphas for each causal dimension were as follows: stability (a = .64); control (a = .38); customer (a = .73); employee (a = .70); and firm (a = .74). The manipulations were tested further via ANOVA. All of
  • 7. the treatment manipulations, with the exception ofthe stability manipulation for one scenario were found to be statistically significant(p< .05). Significantinteractionsofmanipulationson manipulation check scales were not identified in any of the scenarios and Duncan's Multiple Range post-hoc test revealed significant differences between expected group means (Perdue and Summers 1986). In addition to demonstrating that the intended impact of the manipulations was successful, the manipulation effects should be of a sufficient magnitude (Perdue and Summers 1986). A statistic developed to provide a conservative estimate of the strength ofassociation or relation between the independentand dependent variables (in this situation, the manipulation check measure is being analyzed) is the omega-squared statistic (w). Omega-squared is suggested as an appropriate indicator of effect size in the context of ANOVA models (Green 1978; Kerlinger 1986; Perdue and Summers 1986; Tabachnick and Fidell 1989). The function of omega-squared in ANOVA is similar to R2 in the context ofmultiple regression (Green 1978). "Omega-squared represents the proportion of variance in the dependent variable accounted for by agiven main or interaction effect" (Perdue and Summers 1986, p. 323). Effect sizes in this pretestrangedfrom .06to .21. Significantdifferences andmeans falling at the appropriate scale ends suggested the service recovery causes were being properly classified. Findings generally indicated that the independent variable manipulations were effective. However, due to modest effectsizes and the low coefficientalphaassociatedwith the control dimension, asecond pre-test was conducted. Pre-Test I! of Scenario Manipulations. Failure/recovery scenarios depicting each of the six treatment cells from five service industries (i.e., Airline-lost luggage; Auto Repair-work delay; Cable Television-loss ofservice; CreditCard Company- incorrect billing; Hotel-overbooked on rooms) were retained based on the results of the first pre-test. These five sets of scenarios were pre-tested a second time after the following changes were made: 1) the manipulations were integrated into the cover page ofthe questionnaire, 2) the service failure was more clearly delineated from the recovery in the scenarios, and 3) the wording of the stability manipulation statements was strengthened by changing "rarely" and "always" to "inconsistent" and "consistent", respectively. Business students from classes not previously utilized in the scenario development process (52 male, 33 female) were randomly assigned to one ofthe six manipulation conditions, scenarioorder was randomized for each subject. Subjectsranged in age from 20 to 39 years ( x= 24.0 years) and consisted ofseniors (n = 65) and MBA students (n = 20). Factor analysis with varimax rotation again confirmed a single-factor structure for each ofthe five proposed causal dimension sub-scales. Coefficient alphas for each ofthe causal dimensions were as follows: stability (a = .79); control (a =.64); customer (a =.77); employee (a =.82); and firm (a = .84). One significant interaction was found (Stability with Locus) in one ofthe service industries (i.e., Auto Repair-work delay). This service industry scenario was eliminated from further consideration. All of the treatment manipulations were statistically significant. Duncan's Multiple Range post-hoc procedure revealed significant differences betweengroupmeans in all cases. Thefmdings indicatedthatthe causes were properly classified, and the independent variable manipUlations were effective. Based on effect sizes and realism scores, scenarios from the Airline, Cable TV, and Credit Card industries were retained. All three scenarios were perceived as both controlled (Airline: x= 1.84, SD = .80; Cable TV: x= 2.07, SD =.86; Credit Card: x=2.16, SD =1.07), and realistic (Airline: x=1.96, SD =1.32; Cable TV: x=2.84, SD =1.80; Credit Card: x= 2.20, SD = 1.29). The hotel scenarios were eliminated from further study as locus effect sizes were lower in every category relative to the other three services. To further strengthen the stability and locusmanipulations, wordingchanges were again made and a third pre-test was conducted. Pre-Test II! ofScenario Manipulations. Similar to the earlier pre-tests, subjects were randomly assigned to one of the six experimentalconditionswith scenarioindustryorderrandomized. Subjects included86 (49 male, 37 female) businessstudentsfrom classes that had notparticipated in the earliere!"e-tests. Students ranged in age from 19 to 50 years ( x = 23.9 years) Dimensionality ofthe manipulation check items was examined via factor analysis with varimax rotation. Coefficient alphas for the causal dimensions were as follows: stability (a = .90); customer (a = .83); employee (a = .82); and firm (a = .82). Utilizing ANOVA, all of the treatments were found to be statistically significant. ANOVA results and omega-squared statistics are presented in Table 1. Duncan's MUltiple Range post-hoc test revealed significant differences between group means in all cases. The fmdings indicated that the scenario manipulationswere beingproperly classifiedandwere effective. The three failure scenarios and descriptions for each of the treatment cells retained for the fmal study are presented in Appendix Table 1. Data Collection Method A convenience sample of customers of four child-care centers located in a large southeasterncity servedas the samplingframe. Packets containing a cover letter and two identical surveys were distributed to each potential respondent at all four centers. The second survey in the packet was included with a request that "anyone else in your home over the age of 18" fill out the questionnaire as well. The packets were placed in a location (either a file or box depending on the particular center) that was checked daily by each child's parent or guardian. If siblings attended a particular center, a single packet was placed in the youngest child's file or box. A reminder letter was distributed five days after the initial survey distribution. A survey drop box was provided and placed prominently near the front entrance desk of each child-care center. For each center's cooperation, frozen flavored treats were provided for all ofthe children. Fall 2001 55
  • 8. TABLE 1 ANOVA RESULTS OF PRE-TEST III SCENARIO MANIPULATIONS Dependent Variable F-Ratio' W' F-Ratio' Airline Stability Locus Customer Employee Firm 147.06 .63 34.93 30.15 45.64 .44 .40 .51 Cable TV Stability Locus Customer Employee Firm 100.55 7.97 .54 .06 32.18 22.45 27.84 .42 .34 .39 Credit Card Stability Locus Customer Employee Firm 'All effects significant at the p<.OOllevei 182.87 Subjects randomly received only one of the six experimental conditions. The order of presentation of the industries was randomized for each subject as well. A total 0[332 survey packets (i.e., 664 surveys) were distributed. There were 212 surveys returned (response rate = 32%), ofwhich 29 were blank, resulting in a useable response rate of28% (n = 183). Subjects. Respondents ranged in age from 18 to 71 years ( x= 32.7), with females making up 64.4% of the sample. Most respondents attended some college (36.9%) or were college graduates (27.9%). Respondents were familiar with services, as 139 (77.2%Lhad worked in a service business ranging from 1to 40 years ( x = 8.57 years). About one-half of the subjects (50.2%) had experienced problems similarto those presented in the scenarios (Airline = 51.4%, Cable TV = 43.9%, and Credit Card = 55.2%). Measurement Service recovery process quality was defined as the customer's subjective evaluation of 'how' the service recovery was delivered, relative to the level ofperformance that this particular type ofservice company can and should deliver (i.e., the desired service level) (Zeithaml, Berry, and Parasuraman 1993). An attribute based seven-item, 9-point Likert-type scale applicableto service recovery process quality was utilized. The SERVQUAL instrument (Parasuraman, Berry, and Zeithaml 1991, 1993; Parasuraman, Zeithaml, and Berry 1994)provided the foundation for the service recovery process quality measure used in this study. Service recovery satisfaction was defmed as the customer's overall psychological state resulting from his/her comparison of expectations ofwhata service provider 'will' offer, and the actual recovery experience (Zeithaml, Berry, and Parasuraman 1993). 56 Journal ofMarketing THEORY AND PRACTICE .68 40.58 41.57 35.07 .48 .49 .45 Similar to other studies (e.g., Kelley and Davis 1994; Oliva, Oliver, and MacMillan 1992), satisfaction/dissatisfaction is assumed to be unidimensional ranging from Lower Than Expected (1) to Higher Than Expected (9). An attribute based nine-item, 9-point Likert-type scale derived from appropriate SERVQUAL items was used to measure service recovery satisfaction (Parasuraman, Berry, and Zeithaml 1991, 1993; Parasuraman, Zeithaml, and Berry 1994). Word-of-mouth intentions were defmed as the customer's belief that he or she would discuss the incident either favorably, unfavorably, or neutrally with at least one person within the customer's social net (i.e., family member, friend, acquaintance), who was not directly involved in the service failure/recovery encounter. This definition is consistent with Day and Landon's (1977) private response classification, as well as the word-of-mouth conceptualization ofRichins (1983). Word-of-mouth intentions were measured through a four-item, 7-point scale. Subjects indicated their likelihood of praising/criticizing and recommending/warning others about the service firm. Finally, repurchase intentions were defined as a customer's beliefthat he or she would purchase from the same service firm at some future date. Repurchase intentions were operationalizedthrough a four- item, 7-point scale similar to that utilized by Halstead and Page (1992). The complete perceptions and behavioral intentions scales are provided in Appendix Table 2. RESULTS Factor analysis ofthe evaluative and behavioral intentions items was conducted using oblique rotation due to correlations among the constructs (Pedhazur and Schmelkin 1991; Tabachnick and FideJl 1989). Items loaded well on the appropriate factors. Word-of-mouth in the Credit Card industry was the only variable with potential cross-loading concerns (see Table 2). Although WOM ICredit Card and WOM2Credit Card cross-loaded with the
  • 9. repurchase intentions factor (F4) they were kept with the word- of-mouth factor (F3) to be consistent with the Airline and Cable TV industries. None ofthe items were deleted based on an item- to-total correlation criterion ofr < .25 (Nunnally 1978). All of the scales demonstrated strong internal consistency with alphas ranging from .87 to .97 (see Table 2). The total variance extracted by the four factors was 80.5%, 79.l%, and 78.0% for the Airline, Cable TV, and Credit Card scenarios, respectively. Hypotheses were tested utilizing a repeated measures design of the multivariate analysis of variance (MANOVA) procedure. The Mauchly test of sphericity was used to determine if an adjustment in the degrees of freedom was necessary, thus affecting critical Fvalues. The Mauchly test findings indicated rejection ofthe null hypothesis thatthe error covariance matrix ofthe orthonormalized transformed dependent variables were proportional to an identity matrix for both quality (W = .960, P< .05) and satisfaction (W =.961, P< .05). The Greenhouse- Geisser epsilon was used to adjust the degrees of freedom for the averaged tests of significance for these variables. The repeated measures MANOVA indicated significant between-subjects effects for both stability (F4,456 = 3.65, P < .01), and locus (Fg916= 2.34, P< .05), but not their interaction (Fg916 = 1.21, P >..05). In addition, there was a significant within-subjects effect (i.e., recovery time) (Fg694 = 19.88, P < .001). Significant interactions also resulted for recovery time by stability (Fg694 = 3.21, P < .01) and recovery time by locus (F16 060= 1.71, P< .05). Univariate tests indicated 1) recovery quaiity was statistically significant (p < .05) in the recovery time by stability interaction, and 2) satisfaction (p < .05) and word-of-mouth intentions (p < .05) were statistically significant in the recovery time by locus interaction. In the following discussion of the findings relative to the specific hypotheses, significant findings were based on a .05 (or less) probability level. HI - Stability Findings suggest (see Tables 3-6) that for service recovery quality the anticipated effect of stability was present for the Airline industry ( Xstable = 5.13; Xunstable = 4.56), but was not present in either the Cable TV ( Xstable = 5.51; Xunstable = 5.69) or Credit Card ( Xstable = 6.14; Xunstable = 6.13) industries. Examination of the means across the three service settings considered indicated that stable recoveries result in enhanced positive word-of-mouth intentions ( Xstable = 4.58; Xunstable = 4}5), and more favorable repurchase intentions ( Xstable= 4.90; Xunstable =4.53). These findings provide partial support for H1. H2 - Locus In the Airline industry, the Duncan's post-hoc test indicated satisfaction with the recovery attributed primarily to the customer was rated significantly lower ( x= 4.45) than satisfaction with the recovery attributed either primarily to the firm ( x=5.11) or employee ( x=4.99). In the Credit Card industry recoveries primarily attributed to the !lrm were associated with the lowest levels of satisfaction ( x = 5.93), which significan.!.ly differed from those primarily attributed to the employee ( x = 6.44). Recovery satisfaction evaluations ヲッセ@ the Cable TV Jndustry ヲッャャセキ・、@ those predicted in H2 ( xcustomer = 5.46; xfirm = 5.61; Xemployee = 5.95), but were not significantly different. The mean distribution pattern for word-of-mouth intentions in the recovery time by locus interaction was very similar to those observed for recovery satisfaction. In the Airline industry, recoveries primarily attributed to the customer resulted in significantly lower word-of-mouth intentions ( x = 3.66). The Duncan's Multiple Range post-hoc test further revealed no significant group mean differenc:.s between recoveries primarQy attributed to either the firm ( x =3.93) or the employee ( x = 3.91). For the Cable TV industry, recoveries primarily attributed to the employee were significantly_more likely to elicit favorable セッイ、MッヲMュッオエィ@ intentions ( セ・ューャッケ・・@ = 5.15) relative to firm ( Xfirm =4.48) or customer ( xcustomer = 4.71) attributed recoveries. Further examination of the means across the three service recovery times indicated partial support for H2. Specifically, recoveries primarily attributed to the employee lead to ィゥァセ・イ@ levels of ーセイ」・ゥカ・、@ recovery quality ( Xcustomer = 5.29; xfirm = 5.40; Xemployee = 5.73) across service industries. H3 - Recovery Time Differences In H3 it was expected that service recovery evaluations and intentions would be more favorable in recoveries that were less complex and more timely. In the context ofour study it was expected that evaluations and intentions would be more favorable for the Credit Card scenario followed by the Cable TV and Airline scenarios, respectively. The Duncan'sMUltiple Range post-hoc testrevealed ウゥァョゥヲゥ」セエ@ differences 「・エキセ・ョ@ all group means for recovery quality ( XCreditCard = 6.14; xCableTV = 5.59; セaゥイャゥョ・@ =4.86), satisfaction ( XCreditCard = 6.2,!! XCableTv = 5.68; xAirline =4.85), and repurchase intentions ( xCreditCard = 5.37; Xcable TV = 5.01; XAirline = 4.06). Word-of-mouth ゥョセョエゥッョウ@ were also significantly セゥァィ・イ@ for the Credit Card ( Xwom = 4.94) and Cable TV ( xwo..!!) = 4.80) scenarios in comparison to the Airline recovery ( xwom = 3.83) (see Tables 3-6). In summary, H3 was supported. DISCUSSION AND MANAGERIAL IMPLICATIONS By definition, stability attributions represent customer perceptions of the likelihood of having the same recovery experience if the circumstance occurred again in the future. The findings associated with HI suggest customer behavioral intentions (i.e., word-of-mouth and repurchase intentions) are more favorable when customers believe that the recovery they received is consistently implemented (i.e., is stable) when failures do occur. Managers might take this into consideration by making organizational policies regarding service recovery Fall 2001 57
  • 10. TABLE 2 ED FACTOR PATTERN AND RELlABn..rry COEFFICIENTS (ALPHAS) FOR MAIN STUDY BEHAVIORAL PERCEPTIONS AND INTENTIONS SCALES' Airline FI F2 F3 F4 (.97) .86 .82 .81 .82 .81 .78 .74 .94 .93 (.93) .86 .88 .82 .74 (.95) .55 .78 .86 .93 .89 .82 .93 (.89) .73 .74 .81 .92 Cable TV Fl . F2 F3 F4 (.97) .89 .94 .92 .83 .78 .78 .81 .93 .85 (.90) .76 .79 .79 .65 (.95) .62 .69 .80 .91 .87 .86 .83 (.88) .72 •.72 .82 .80 Fl (.97) .88 .90 .85 .87 .96 .88 .79 .89 .83 Credit Card F2 F3 (.91) .87 .91 .86 .61 (.95) .84 .84 .87 .90 .84 .79 .89 .55 .65 hmlJarentheses are reliability coefficients. The other numbers are factor loadings -obtained after oblique rotation of the initial solutions. Loadin e been omitted. The total variance extracted by the four factors was 80.5%, 78.0%, and 79.1% for the Airline, Credit Card, and Cable TV sa Eigenvalues for each of the factors were as follows: Fl == 14.42 airline, 14.00 Cable TV, 12.73 OeditCard; F2 = 3.43 airline, 3.24 Cable TV. 4.44 Credit Card; F3 e TV, 1.21 OeditCard; F4 = .766 airline•.939 Cable TV, .819 Cr,ditCard· nd behavioral intentions labels correspond to those of the items listed in Appendix Table 2. Marketing THEORY AND PRACTICE
  • 11. TABLE 3 DESCRITPTIVE STATISTICS FOR SERVICE RECOVERY PROCESS QUALITY Airline Cable TV Credit Card Total Standard Standard Standard Stan Stability Mean Deviation Mean Deviation Mean Deviation Mean Devi Stable 4.94 1.43 5.11 .86 5.87 .98 5.29 1. Unstable 4.27 1.12 5.53 1.51 6.73 1.37 5.30 1. Total 4.62 QNセ@ 5.30 1.21 6.27 1.24 5.29 1. Stable 5.18 1.92 5.75 1.34 6.40 1.35 5.80 1. Unstable 4.89 1.60 6.05 1.03 6.03 1.13 5.65 1. Total 5.04 1.76 5:90 1.21 6.23 1.25 5.73 1. Stable 5.30 1.46 5.67 1.52 6.13 1.10 5.72 1. Unstable 4.48 1.34 5.43 1.32 5.64 1.13 5.06 1. Total 4.90 1.45 5.55 1.42 5.89 1.13 5.40 1. Stable 5.13 1.62 5.51 1.28 6.14 1.17 5.59 1. Unstable 4.56 1.39 5.69 1.31 6.13 1.28 5.35 1. Total 4.86 1.54 5.59 1.29 6.14 1.22 5.48 1. TABLE 4 DESCRIPTIVE STATISTICS FOR CUSTOMER SATISFACTION WITH TIlE RECOVERY Airline Cable TV Credit Card Total Standard Standard Standard Stan Stability Mean Deviation Mean Deviation Mean Deviation Mean- Devi Stable 4.52 1.26 5.16 1.28 5.84 1.06 5.13 1. Unstable 4.38 .99 5.81 1.63 6.83 1.36 5.42 '1:6 Total 4.45 1.14 5.46 1.48 6.30 1.30 5.26 1.4 Stable 5.00 1.90 5.99 1.28 6.70 1.40 5.92 1. Unstable 4.99 1.63 5.90 1.07 6.15 1.03 5.74 1. Total 4.99 1.76 5.95 1.18 6.44 1.26 5.83 1. Stable 5.24 1.57 5.66 1.61 6.11 1.16 5.73 1. Unstable 4.97 1.40 5.57 1.18 5.75 1.05 5.35 1. Total 5.11 1.48 5.61 1.41 5.93 1.11 5.55 1.4 Stable 4.91 1.61 5.61 1.42 6.23 1.26 5.58 1. Unstable 4.78 1.39 5.77 1.30 6.24 1.22 5.51 1. Total 4.85 1.51 5.68 1.36 6.24 1.24 5.55 1.5 Fall 2001 59
  • 12. clear to both employees and customers, and ensuring that policies are implemented in a consistent manner. In addition, when service recoveries are implemented it might be good practice to clearly convey to the customer that the recovery received is consistently implemented by the representatives of the service organization in the rare instances when a service failure occurs. Within the stability manipulation, recovery qualityperceptions varied, likely due to subtle differences in the failures associated with each scenario. In the Cable TV scenarios the service provider simply performed the service incorrectly. Similarly, in the Credit Card scenarios the service provider was wrongfully "demanding" possession of something belonging to the customer (money). In the Airline scenarios the airline was in possession ofsomething rightfully belonging to the customer (luggage). When a service organization wrongfully has possession of something belonging to the customer (e.g., lost luggage, lost dry cleaning, misplaced jewelry), the customer's primary focus when evaluating the recovery is on having this property returned. In situations where tangible objects are involved, customers place added importance on knowing that the problem will be resolved consistently (Le., stable recovery). A second generalization drawn from our results concerns who should execute the service recovery. Based on the findings it seems safe to recommend that service failures should be resolved by front-line service personnel whenever possible. The constant across the three service settings investigated was the key role of the service employee in perceived recovery process quality. Our findings provide empirical support of earlier contentions that proper training and empowerment of front-line service employees is extremely important to successfully carry out a service recovery program (Hart, Heskett, and Sasser 1990). Antithetically, a service recovery locus - repurchase intentions relationship was not supported in H2. Although our findings indicated an employee-based recovery was important for quality and satisfaction evaluations and word-of-mouth intentions, who the recovery was attributed to was not significantly related to customer repurchase intentions. Overall, the results pertaining to H2 can be interpreted as follows. First, it seems prudent to have contact employees resolve service failure situations whenever possible due to the favorable impact this has on quality, satisfaction and word-of- mouth intentions. However, depending on their goals for the recovery process, service managers should not be overly discouraged when it is not possible for a failure to be resolved by contact personnel. Future repurchase intentions are not significantly different whetherthe employee, firm, or customer is deemed primarily responsible for the recovery. For managers primarily interested in getting customers who have experienced a service failure to repurchase in the future it seems to be less important as to who is primarily responsible 60 Journal ofMarketing THEORY AND PRACTICE for the recovery process and more important that an appropriate recovery outcome is provided. H3 considered the impact of service recovery time and complexity on customer evaluations and intentions. Service recovery time and complexity was found to result in significant differences for the four recovery outcomes as predicted. The greater complexity and length of the Airline recovery process resulted in the least favorable recovery outcomes. The mid-level amount of time involved in the Cable TV service recovery process resulted in that recovery having outcomes at a mid-range level. Customers evaluated the simple and quick Credit Card recovery the most favorably. It is interesting to note that these differences emerged despite the fact that our pre-testing results indicated that the recovery outcomes were perceived as equivalently satisfactory. These findings lend support to previous suggestions that customer evaluations will be more positive for service failures remedied by less complicated and expeditious recovery processes (Hart, Heskett, and Sasser 1990; Kelley, Hoffman, and Davis 1993). In the scenarios both time and complexity move together. In order to better understand these relationships, future researches may wish to further test these findings by varying the time and complexity within rather than between services. For example, by manipulating recovery time within services it would be possible to account for the expectations that customers have for the performance in the industry, as well as account for how the type of failure (i.e., lost luggage, versus cable problems, versus an incorrect charge) impacts assessment ofthe recovery. Based on the findings here, managers will want to take into account the complexity and length of the serviCe recovery process in their particular industry. Customer evaluations of recovery in complex and lengthy service recovery processes are less positive than for service failures remedied by less complex and shorter service recovery processes. As a result, failures encumbered by lengthy orcomplex recovery processes may likely require more elaborate forms of compensation during the course ofthe recovery. In addition, providing quick and simple recoveries in the event of a service failure can provide a strategic advantage in positioning the firm relative to competitors. Limitations and Future Research Directions Our study provides an experimental investigation ofcustomer attributions associated with service recovery. Our research also extends existing service recovery literature and knowledge by utilizing an expanded locus dimension. Expanding the locus dimension beyond the traditional view that dichotomizes locus into internal and external dimensions enhances our understanding of service recovery. In addition, our findings provide empirical support for previous conceptual propositions related to the importance of service recovery complexity and time. Systematic replication of this study in
  • 13. other service settings is recommended in order to develop a deeper understanding of the differences identified in this research. In order to establish the soundness of the methodology we utilized mUltiple pre-tests in developing the scenarios. We started with a qualitative approach (i.e., Critical Incident Technique) and rigorously applied the methodology to ensure that the independentvariable manipUlations were both properly classified and effective. In the main study a repeated measures design with multivariate analysis ofvariance was utilized with established and well-recognized dependent measures. In addition, independent samples were utilized for each of the research phases in an effort to improve internal validity. Convenience samples of college students were utilized in the development of the scenarios, while the main study used a convenience sample of day care customers. An important issue is the appropriateness ofthe subjects given the nature of the task at hand. In the development and early testing of the proposed model, it seems apparent that students do experience service failures and recoveries. In addition, there was not an a priori reason to conclude that students do not attribute causes to explain what happens to them, do not have perceptions, or fail to develop intentions. However, our use of convenience samples throughout the study introduces the possibility of selection bias and must be noted as a limitation. Future researchers may wish to utilize non-student based and probability samples to address this potential threat to validity. In addition, this study only included core service failures and a limited number ofloci in the scenarios tested. In the future, similar research methods could be used to assess the effects of attributions on post-recovery evaluations and behaviors associated with different service failure types (c.f., Bitner, Booms, and Tetreault 1990; Kelley, Hoffman, and Davis 1993) across additional locus categories. Case studies could also be utilized to provide a richer understanding ofthese relationships in service recovery. Finally, the use of Likert scales for this type of study also has limitations. Other measurement tools could be utilized in the future to address this issue. An alternative interpretation of our findings also merits the consideration of additional research. Specifically, the effects we found may be influenced by the competitive nature ofthe industries included in this study. The three industries considered in the scenarios vary on several dimensions but in particular their level of competition. The Airline industry is characterized by varying levels of competition on routes. Flight routes to some destinations are virtual monopolies, while other flight routes are highly competitive and allow the consumer to pick from several carriers. Local Cable TV markets are generally characterized by very limited competition. In fact, this industry might be termed a virtual monopoly. While there are some competing options available, such as satellite dishes and digital dish networks, often the consumer perceives them as either impractical or cost prohibitive. The Credit Card industry is highly competitive. Consumers are bombarded with Credit Card offers through direct mail and telephone. As a result, it is relatively easy for customers to switch from one service provider to another. Overall, the three industries considered cover three different levels of competitiveness. The Airline industry might be characterized as an industry with moderate levels of competition and consumer choice. The Cable TV industry is characterized by very limited levels of competition and consumer choice, and the Credit Card industry provides an example of an industry characterized by high levels of competition and consumer choice. Thus, the three sets of scenarios used in this research consider a cross-section of service industries with regard to competition and the extent to which customers have choices within the industry. It is difficult to draw any well-grounded industry-specific conclusions regarding service recovery based on this research. Therefore, the following speculative interpretation of our results is presented. Perhaps Airline customers recognize that while they do have some choices, the choices they have are limited. As a result, customers may take their limited choice set into account and evaluate a stable recovery more favorably. In the essentially noncompetitive Cable TV industry the locus of the service recovery was a significant factor in customer evaluations and intentions. In this case, customers may take into account that they have essentially no viable alternatives. This lack ofviable alternatives may lead to a customer thought process something like this: "Even though I have limited alternatives in this situation, it is nice to know that someone at my local Cable TV company cares enough to quickly respond to my request." Finally, in the highly competitive Credit Card industry customers valued stability in the recoveries attributed to the service employee and firm. However, customers rated unstable recoveries attributed to themselves most favorably of all the treatments considered in this industry. Credit Card customers have many choices and in many cases they are bombarded with these choices on an almost daily basis. First, as suggested by attribution theory, stable recoveries attributed to employees and the firm were evaluated more favorably than unstable recoveries attributed to the same loci. One possible explanation for the finding concerning the unstable customer attributed recovery might focus on the level ofrisk and control associated with the customer's recovery effort. As noted in the scenarios, customers in the unstable-customer locus cell treatment rarely take charge in service failure/recovery situations. Perhaps one reason these customers don't take control ofthese situations is that there is some risk involved in that their demands may not be net. However, in the highly competitive Credit Card industry it might be argued that it is relatively low risk to take charge in these situations. Credit Card customers have many choices available. The result is that it is a "no risk" setting in which to take charge as a customer - you have many other options ifthings do not work out. As a result, the customer that is unaccustomed to taking charge in such situations may do so and feel good about the Fall 2001 61
  • 14. fact that he/she obtained some results when taking charge of the failure situation. time constraints, among others, may all affect the outcome variables investigated here. Finally, this research restricted its focus to recovery attributions. How service failure attributions and recovery attributions interact to impact post-recovery evaluations and behaviors needs investigation. Future examination ofthese types of questions may provide a better understanding of how marketers can use the service recovery process as a strategic customer retention tool Further research that extends our knowledge of the relationships considered in this study will be beneficial. For example, mood states and personality traits may impact perceived quality and satisfaction with a particular service recovery. In addition, lack ofalternatives, switching costs, and APPENDIX TABLE I RETAINED SERVICE FAILURE AND CORRESPONDING RECOVERY SCENARIOS Airline Cable TV Credit Card The Service Problem While traveling on your usual airline, you arrive at your final destination. You wait at the baggage claim area, but your luggage does not appear with the other passengers' items. After checking at the customer service desk, you are told your luggage has been mistakenly put on adifferent flight and is expected to arrive at the airport tomorrow afternoon. The Service Outcomes Stable - Customer: You demand action. You receive an apology and the luggage is delivered to you the next afternoon. You consistently take the initiative to get your complaints addressed. Unstable - Customer: You demand action. You receive an apology and the luggage is delivered to you the next afternoon. You inconsistently take the initiative to get your complaints addressed. Stable - Employee: The service employee takes action. You receive an apology and the luggage is delivered to you the next afternoon. You have heard that this airlines' employees consistently take the initiative to address customer complaints. Unstable - Employee: The service employee takes action. You receive an apology and the luggage is delivered to you the next afternoon. You have heard that this airlines' employees inconSistently take the initiative to address customer complaints. Stable - Firm: The airline takes action. You receive an apology and the luggage is delivered to you the next afternoon. You have heard that this airline consistentlytakes the initiative toaddress customer complaints. Unstable - Firm: The airline takes action. You receive an apology and the luggage is delivered to you the next afternoon. You have heard that this airline inconsistently takes the initiative to address customer complaints. In order to watch more ofyour favorite television shows, you decide to have cable installed. Soon afterthe cable representative hooksyourtelevision up, your screen goes blank. You call the cable company about the problem. You demand action. A repair person shows up two hours later and corrects the problem. You consistently take the initiative to get your complaints addressed. You demand action. A repair person shows up two hours later and corrects the problem. You inconsistently take the initiative to get your complaints addressed. The employeetakes action. The employee returns two hours later and corrects the problem. You have heard that this cable companys' employees consis-tentlytake the initiativeto addresscustomer complaints. The employee takes action. The employee returns two hours later and corrects the problem. You have heard that this cable companys' employees inconsistently take the initiative to address customer complaints. The cable company takes action. A repair person shows up two hours laterand corrects the problem. You have heard that this cable company consistentlytakes the initiativeto addresscustomer complaints. The cable company takes action. A repair person shows up two hours laterand corrects the problem. You have heard that this cable company inconsistently takes the initiative to address customer complaints. 62 Journal ofMarketing THEORY AND PRACTICE You receive your credit card bill and it includes a charge that you did not make. You contact the creditcard company and demand action. Your account is corrected immediately andyoureceive an apology for any inconvenience. You consistently take the initiative to get your complaints addressed. You contact the creditcard company and demand action. Your account is corrected immediately andyou receive an apologyfor any inconvenience. You inconsistently take the initiative to get your complaints addressed. Aftercontactingthe creditcard company, aservice employee takes action by apologizing for any inconvenience, and immediately correcting your account. You have heard that this credit card companys' employees consistently take the initiative to address customer complaints. Aftercontactingthe creditcard company, aservice employee takes action by apologizing for any inconvenience, and immediately correcting your account. You have heard that this credit card companys' employees inconsistently take the initiative to address customer complaints. After contacting the credit card company, the company takes action by apologizing for any inconvenience, and immediately correcting your account. You have heard that this credit card companyconSistentlytakesthe initiativetoaddress customer complaints. After contacting the credit card company, the company takes action by apologizing for any inconvenience, and immediately correcting your account. You have heard that this credit card company inconsistently takes the initiative to address customer complaints.
  • 15. APPENDIX TABLE 2 PERCEPTIONS AND BEHAVIORAL INTENTIONS BATTERY Item Label Item Wording QUAL I QUAL2 QUAL3 QUAL4 QUAL5 QUAL6 QUAL7 Dependability in handling customer service problems. Willingness to handle customer problems. Ability ofemployees to handle customer complaints. Courteousness ofemployees. Employees who have the knowledge to answer customer questions. Company has the customer's best interest at heart. Employees treat customers in a caring manner. Satisfactionb SAT! SAT2 SAD SAT4 SAT5 SAT6 SAT7 SAT8 SAT9 Dependability in handling customer service problems. Willingness to handle customer problems. Ability ofemployees to handle customer complaints. Courteousness ofemployees. Employees who have the knowledge to answer customer questions. Company has the customer's best interest at heart. Employees treat customers in a caring manner. How the service problem was corrected. My feelings towards this service outcome can be described as. WOMI WOM2 WOM3 WOM4 Word-of-Mouth Intentions' I would try to convince my friends and relatives to use thtL. . I would be likely to recommend thiL to others. I would be likely to convince my friends and relatives not to use thiL . (-) I would warn others about using thiL . (-) Repurchase Intentions' BUYI Would you use this_ again ifyou had a choice? BUY2 What is the likelihood that you will go back to thtL. next time you need this service? BUY3 How likely would you be to repurchase from thiL in the future? BUY4 What is the likelihood that you will switch to 。ョッエィセ@ for this service? (-) Items identified with a "-" were reverse scored. 'Each item was accompanied by a 9-point Likert-type scale with the labels: I-"Lower Than", 5-"The Same As", and 9 -"Higher Than" My Desired Service Level. bEach item was accompanied by a 9-point Likert-type scale with the labels: 1="Lower Than", 5="The Same As", and 9="Higher Than" I Would Have Expected. 'Each item was accompanied by a 7-point Likert-type scale with the anchors: 1="Definitey", 7="Definitely Not". REFERENCES Bem, Daryl 1. (1965), "An Experimental Analysis ofSelf-Persuasion, " Journal 0/Experimental Social Psychology, 1 (August): 199- 218. __(1967), "Self-Perception: An Alternative Interpretation ofCognitive Dissonance Phenomena," Psychological Review, 74 (May): 183- 200. __(1972), "Self-Perception Theory," in Advances in Experimental Social Psychology, Vol. 6, L. Berkowitz, ed. New York: Academic Press, 1-62. Berry, Leonard L., A. Parasuraman, and Valarie A. Zeithaml (1994), "Improving Service Quality in America: Lessons Learned," Academy 0/Management Executive, 8 (May): 32-52. Bitner, Mary Jo (1990), "Evaluating Service Encounters: The Effects of Physical Surroundings and Employee Responses," Journal 0/ Marketing, 54 (April): 69-82. - ' Bernard H. Booms, and Mary Stanfield Tetreault (1990), "The Service Encounter: Diagnosing Favorable and Unfavorable Incidents," Journal 0/Marketing, 54 (January): 71-84. Boulding, Williarn, Ajay Kalra, Richard Staelin, and Valarie A. Zeithaml (1993), "A Dynamic Process Model ofService Quality: From Expectations to Behavioral Intentions," Journal o/Marketing Research, 30 (February): 7-27. Curren, Mary T. and Valerie S. Folkes (1987), "Attributionallnfluences on Consumers' Desires to Communicate About Products," Psychology and Marketing, 4 (Spring): 31-45. Day, Ralph L. and E. Laird Landon, Jr. (1977), "Toward a Theory of Consumer Complaining Behavior," in Consumer and Industrial Buying Behavior, Arch G. Woodside, Jagdish Sheth, and Peter Bennett, eds. Elsevier North-Holland, Inc., 425-437. DrOge, Cornelia and Diane Halstead, (1991), "Postpurchase Hierarchies of Effects: The Antecedents and Consequences ofSatisfaction for Complainers Versus Non-Complainers," International Journal o/Research in Marketing, 8: 315-328. Elig, Timothy W. and Irene Hanson Frieze, (1979), "Measuring Causal Attributions for Success and Failure," Journal 0/Personality and Social Psychology, 37 (April): 621-634. Folkes, Valerie S. (1984), "Consumer Reactions to Product Failure: An Attributional Approach," Journal o/Consumer Research, 10 (March): 398-409. __(1998), "Recent Attribution Research in Consumer Behavior: A Review and New Directions," Journal o/Consumer Research, 14 (March): 548-565. __, Susan Koletsky, and John L. Graham (1987), "A Field Study of Causal Inferences and Consumer Reaction: The View from the Airport," Journal o/Consumer Research, 13 (March): 534-539. Fall 2001 63
  • 16. and Barbara Kotsos (1986), "Buyers' and Sellers' Explanations for - - Product Failure: Who Done It," Journal ofMarketing, 50 (April): 74-80 Gooding, Richard Z. and Angelo J. Kinicki (1995), "Interpreting Event Causes: The Complementary role of Categorization and Attribution Processes," Journal ofManagement Studies, 32 (January): 1-22. Green, Paul E. (1978), Analyzing Multivariate Data, Hinsdale: The Dryden Press. Gronroos, Christian (I 988a), Strategic Management and Marketing in the Service Sector. Report No. 83-104. Cambridge: Marketing Science Institute. __(1988b), "Service Quality: The Six Criteria ofGood Perceived Service Quality," Review ofBusiness, 9 (Winter): 10-13. Halstead, Diane and Thomas J. Page, Jr. (1992), "The Effects ofSatisfaction and Complaining Behavior on Consumer Repurchase Intentions," Journal ofConsumer Satisfaction, Dissatisfaction and Complaining Behavior, 5: I-II. Hart, Christopher W. L., James L. Heskett, and W. Earl Sasser, Jr. (1990), "The Profitable Art of Service Recovery," Harvard Business ReView, 68 (July-August): 148-56. Heider, Fritz(1958), The PsychologyofInterpersonalRelations,New York: John Wiley and Sons, Inc. Jacob, Rahul (1994), "Why Some Customers are More Equal Than Others," Fortune, 130 (September 19): 215-224. Jones, Edward E. and Keith Davis (1965), "From Acts to Dispositions: The Attribution Process in Person Perception," in Advances in ExperimentalSocialPsychology, Vol. 2. LeonardBerkowitz,ed. New York, NY: Academic Press, 219-266. __and Daniel McGillis (1976), "CorrespondentInferences andthe Attribution Cube: AComparative Reappraisal," in New Directions in Attribution Research, Vol. I, John Harvey, William Ickes, and Robert Kidd, eds. Hillsdale, N1: Lawrence Erlbaum, 389-420. Jones, Thomas O. and W. Earl Sasser, Jr. (1995), "Why Satisfied Customers Defect," Harvard Business ReView, 73 (November-December): 88- 99. Keaveney, Susan M. (1995), "Customer Switching Behavior in Service Industries: An Exploratory Study," Journal of Marketing, 59 (April): 71-82. Kelley, Harold H. (1967), "AttributionTheory in Social Psychology," inNebraska Symposium on Motivation, Vol. 15. David Levine, ed. Lincoln, NB: University ofNebraska Press, 192-238. __(1971), Attribution in Social Interaction, New York: General Learning Press. __(1972), CausalSchemataandtheAttribution Process, Morristown: General Learning Press. __(1973), "The Processes ofCausal Attribution," American Psychologist, 28 (February): 107-128. Kelley, Scott W., K. Douglas Hoffman, and Mark A. Davis (1993), "A Typology ofRetail Failures and Recoveries," Journal ofRetailing, 69 (Winter): 429-452. Kerlinger, Fred N. (1986), Foundations ofBehavioral Research Third Edition, Fort Worth: Holt, Rinehart, and Winston, Inc. 64 Journal ofMarketing THEORY AND PRACTICE Lichtenstein, Donald R. and William O. Bearden (1986), "Measurement and Structure of Kelley's Covariance Theory," Journal of Consumer Research, 13 (September): 290-296. McDougall, Duncan (1995), "Know thy Customer," The Wall Street Journal, (August 7): A14. Nunnally, Jum C. (1978), Psychometric Theory, New York: McGraw-Hili Book Company. Oliver, Richard L. (1997), Satisfaction: A Behavioral Perspective on the Consumer, New York: The McGraw-Hili Companies, Inc. __and Wayne S. DeSarbo (1988), "Response Determinants in Satisfaction Judgments," Journal ofConsumer Research, 14 (March): 495-507. Parasuraman, A., Leonard L. Berry, and Valarie A. Zeithaml (1991), "Refinement and Reassessment of the SERVQUAL Scale," Journal ofRetailing, 67 (Winter): 420-450. __, - ' and __ (1993), "More on Improving Service Quality Measurement," Journal ofRetailing, 69 (Spring): 140-147. __, Valarie A. Zeithaml, and Leonard L. Berry, (1985), "A Conceptual Model of Service Quality and Its Implications for Future Research," Journal ofMarketing, 49 (Fall): 41-50. - ' __, and __, (1994), "Reassessment of Expectations as a Comparison Standard in Measuring Service Quality: Implications for Further Research," Journal of Marketing, 58 (January): 111- 124. Pedhazur, Elazar J. and Liora Pedhazur Schmelkin (1991), Measurement, DeSign, and Analysis: An Integrated Approach, Hillsdale NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Inc. Perdue, Barbara C. and John O. Summers (1986), "Checking the Success of Manipulations in Marketing Experiments," Journal ofMarketing Research, 23 (November): 317-326. Peters, Tom (1988), Thriving on Chaos, New York: Alfred A. Knopf. Reichheld, Frederick F. and W. Earl Sasser, Jr. (1990), "Zero Defections: Quality Comes to Services," Harvard Business Review, 68 (September- October): 105-111. Richins, Marsha (1983), "Negative Word-of-Mouth by Dissatisfied Consumers: A Pilot Study," Journal ofMarketing, 47 (Winter): 68-78. Russell, Dan (1982), "The Causal Dimension Scale: A Measure of How Individuals Perceive Causes," Journal ofPersonality and Social Psychology, 42 (June): 1137-1145. Rust, Roland T. and Anthony 1. Zahorik (1993), "Customer Satisfaction, Customer Retention, and Market Share," Journal ofRetailing, 69 (Summer): 193-215. Smith, Amy K. and Ruth N. Bolton (1998), "An Experimental Investigation of Service Failure and Recovery: Paradox or Peril?" Journal of Service Research, I (August): 65-81. __, __ and Janet Wagner (1998), "A Model of Customer Satisfaction With Service Encounters Involving Failure and Recovery," Marketing Science Institute Report #98-100. Tabachnick, Barbara G. and Linda S. Fidell (1989), Using Multivariate Statistics, Second Edition. New York: Harper Collins Publishers, Inc.
  • 17. Tax, Stephen S., Stephen W. Brown, and Murali Chandrashekaran (1998), "Customer Evaluations of Service Complaint Experiences: Implications for Relationship Marketing," Journal ofMarketing, 62 (April): 60·76. Valle, Valerie and Melanie Wallendorf(I977), "Consumers' Attributions ofthe CauseofTheirProductSatisfactionand Dissatisfaction," in Consumer Satisfaction/DissatisfactionandComplainingBehavior, Ralph L. Day, ed. Bloomington, IN: Indiana University, 26·30. Weiner, Bernard (1980), Human Motivation, New York: Holt, Rinehart, and Winston. __(I 985a), "An Attributional Theory of Achievement Motivation and Emotion," Psychological Review, 92 (October): 548·573. __(1985b), "Spontaneous Causal Thinking," Psychological Bulletin, 97 (January): 74·84. Wofford J. C. and Vicki L. Goodwin (1990), "Effects ofFeedback on Cognitive Processing and Choice Decision Style," Journal of Applied Psychology, 75 (December): 603-612. Zeithaml, Valarie A., Leonard L. Berry, and A. Parasuraman (1993), "TheNature and Determinants ofCustomer Expectations ofService," Journal of the Academy ofMarketing Science, 21 (Winter): 1·12. AUTHOR BIOGRAPHY Scott R. Swanson (Ph.D., University of Kentucky) is an assistant professor of marketing. His research interests include issues related to services marketing, sports sponsorship, and pedagogy. His research has been published in the Journal ofthe Academy ofMarketing Science, the Journal ofBusiness to Business Marketing, the European Journal of Marketing, and Psychological Reports, among others. AUTHOR BIOGRAPHY Scott W. Kelley (D.B.A., University of Kentucky) is an associate professor ofmarketing and the Director ofthe UK Center for Sports Marketing. His research has been published in the Journal ofthe Academy ofMarketing Science, the Journal ofRetailing, the Journal ofBusiness Research, the Journal ofAdvertising, and the Journal ofPersonal Selling and Sales Management, among others. His research interests include issues concerning services marketing and sports marketing. Fall 2001 65