SlideShare a Scribd company logo
Circadian-temporal context and latent inhibition of conditioned
taste aversion: Effect of restriction in the intake of the
conditioned
taste stimulus
Andrés Molero-Chamizo1,2
# Psychonomic Society, Inc. 2016
Abstract Latent inhibition of conditioned taste aversion
(CTA) is sensitive to changes in the temporal context. A
change in the time of day of conditioning with respect to the
time of day of the preexposure can disrupt the latent inhibi-
tion. This contextual change in the time of day may reveal a
temporal specificity of latent inhibition. The optimum proce-
dure to induce this temporal specificity is not well established.
For example, it has been shown that a long period of habitu-
ation to temporal contexts is one factor that can determine the
effect. However, the experimental conditions on the condi-
tioning day that facilitate this phenomenon are unknown.
The aim of this study is to elucidate whether a restriction in
the intake of the conditioned taste stimulus affects the tempo-
ral specificity of latent inhibition. Two main groups of Wistar
rats were tested in a latent inhibition of CTA paradigm, in
which the temporal specificity of this phenomenon was ana-
lyzed by a change in the time of day of conditioning. The
intake of the taste stimulus was restricted in the conditioning
day in one of the groups, but this restriction was not applied in
the other group. The results indicated temporal specificity of
latent inhibition only in the group without restriction, but not
in the group with limitation in the intake of the taste stimulus
during conditioning. These findings can help to elucidate the
characteristics of the procedure to induce temporal specificity
of latent inhibition.
Keywords Conditioned taste aversion . Latent inhibition .
Taste stimulus restriction . Temporal context
The magnitude of a conditioned taste aversion (CTA) can be
modulated by a change in the time of day between condition-
ing and test (Manrique, Gámiz, Morón, Ballesteros, & Gallo,
2009; Morón et al., 2002). This procedure shows that animals
tested in temporal contexts different from that of the condi-
tioning acquire less aversion, which is similar to what has
been consistently described by changing the spatial context
(Boakes, Elliot, Swinbourne, & Westbrook, 1997; Bonardi,
Honey, & Hall, 1990; Bouton, 1993; Bouton, Westbrook,
Corcoran, & Maren, 2006; González, Garcia-Burgos, &
Hall, 2012; Pearce & Bouton, 2001; Rosas & Bouton 1997).
Similarly, the latent inhibition of CTA can be disrupted by
changes in the time of day between preexposure and condi-
tioning (Manrique et al., 2004; Molero et al., 2005).
Nevertheless, the characteristics of the procedure that facilitate
this temporal specificity of the latent inhibition (or dependen-
cy of latent inhibition on the temporal context—time of day—
of preexposure and conditioning) are not entirely known.
Long periods of habituation to different temporal contexts
seem to facilitate this phenomenon (Molero-Chamizo,
2013). This effect might mean that contextual familiarity and
associative strength of the conditioned stimulus are relevant
factors for the specificity of latent inhibition, as has been de-
scribed in contextual learning (Pearce & Bouton, 2001).
Moreover, a restriction in the intake of the conditioned taste
stimulus may also influence the conditioning process and
therefore may affect the temporal specificity of latent
inhibition. As a result, a reduced processing of the taste
stimulus during conditioning would prevent the effect of
temporal specificity. For example, Morón et al. (2002) applied
a restriction in the intake of the taste stimulus during
* Andrés Molero-Chamizo
[email protected]
1 Present address: Department of Psychobiology, University of
Granada, Campus Cartuja, Granada 18071, Spain
2 Department of Psychology, Psychobiology Area, University of
Huelva, Campus El Carmen, 21071 Huelva, Spain
Learn Behav
DOI 10.3758/s13420-016-0251-0
http://crossmark.crossref.org/dialog/?doi=10.3758/s13420-016-
0251-0&domain=pdf
conditioning, resulting in a modulation of the taste aversion
when tested in a different time of day from that of the condi-
tioning. However, by 4 days of habituation to the temporal
context, Molero-Chamizo (2013) found temporal specificity
of the latent inhibition of CTAwithout considering any restric-
tion in the amount of taste stimulus during conditioning.
Although in both cases different processes were measured
(CTA or latent inhibition of CTA), it seems necessary to clar-
ify whether a restriction in the amount of taste stimulus during
conditioning (and therefore an external control of the intake)
facilitates or hinders the temporal specificity of the latent in-
hibition of CTA, similar to that described in other learning
processes (De la Casa & Lubow, 1995; González, Morillas,
& Hall, 2015; Lukoyanov, Pereira, Mesquita, & Andrade,
2002). Thus, the aim of this study is to analyze the effects of
restriction versus no restriction in the intake of the taste stim-
ulus at the stage of conditioning on the temporal specificity of
the latent inhibition of CTA.
A similar procedure that has previously shown such tem-
poral specificity (Molero-Chamizo, 2013) was used in this
study. However, some of the animals had limited access to
the intake of the taste stimulus in the day of conditioning
(group with restriction), and the remaining animals had no
restriction in the intake of the taste stimulus during condition-
ing (group without restriction), except the time for recording
that was common to all animals. If the temporal specificity of
the latent inhibition of CTA depends on the free control of the
intake during conditioning, then in animals with restriction in
the taste stimulus (external control of the intake) the latent
inhibition will not be specific of the temporal context; that
is, these animals will show latent inhibition regardless of a
change in the time of day between preexposure and
conditioning.
Method
Subjects
Fifty adult male Wistar rats, weighing between 280 and 300 g,
were individually housed in boxes measuring 30 cm × 15 cm
× 30 cm. All of the animals were exposed to a daily 12 hours
light/dark cycle (lights on from 9:00 to 21:00), and the tem-
perature conditions were kept constant at 23 °C. Throughout
the procedure, food was provided ad libitum, and the avail-
ability of fluid was restricted to two daily 15 min sessions, one
in the morning (10:00) and one in the evening (20:00), for all
animals. Twenty-four of them were exposed to a limited
amount of taste stimulus (5 ml) for 15 min during conditioning
(group with restriction), and 26 rats had free access to the
intake of the taste stimulus for 15 min during conditioning
(group without restriction). The procedure was approved by
the Ethics Committee for Animal Research of the University
of Granada and was conducted in accordance with both the
NIH Publications (No. 80-23) of the National Institutes of
Health Guide (United States) for the Care and Use of
Laboratory Animals (1996 revision) and the European
Community Council Directive of November 24, 1986 (86/
609/EEC). The National Legislation, in agreement with this
Directive, is defined in Royal Decree No. 1201/2005.
Procedure
In the group with restriction in the intake of the taste stimulus,
rats were in turn distributed among the following four sub-
groups: PE-S (preexposed to the taste, PE, and same time of
day, S, for conditioning and testing, n = 8), PE-D (preexposed
to the taste, PE, and different time of day, D, for conditioning
and testing, n = 8), NPE-S (nonpreexposed to the taste, NPE,
and same time of day, S, for conditioning and testing, n = 4),
NPE-D (nonpreexposed to the taste, NPE, and different time
of day, D, for conditioning and testing, n = 4). In the group
without restriction, animals were distributed among the same
four subgroups: PE-S (n = 8), PE-D (n = 8), NPE-S (n = 5),
NPE-D (n = 5).
All animals were water restricted and received two 15-min
sessions of access to water per day, as described above, over 4
days to facilitate the differentiation of the temporal contexts
(morning vs. evening). After this period, the procedure had
three main stages (preexposure, conditioning, and testing).
With respect to the preexposure stage, all rats had access to
water in the morning session (15 min) for 2 days. The
nonpreexposed subgroups (NPE-S and NPE-D) also received
water in the evening session (15 min) of these 2 days, whereas
the preexposed subgroups (PE-S and PE-D) were exposed to a
sodium chloride solution dissolved in water (saline 1%) for 15
min. On the day following the last preexposure, conditioning
occurred in the morning session for the PE-D and NPE-D
subgroups. These subgroups of the group without restriction
in the intake of the taste stimulus were exposed to saline for
15 min in the morning, and the amounts ingested were record-
ed. The PE-D and NPE-D subgroups of the group with restric-
tion were exposed to 5 ml of saline for 15 min in the morning,
and the amounts ingested were recorded. This was the only
difference in the procedure between the groups with and with-
out restriction in the intake of the taste stimulus. Twenty mi-
nutes later, all animals received an injection of lithium chlo-
ride (LiCl 0.15 M, 2% of body weight, intraperitoneally).
Water was available for 15 min in the evening session. In
contrast, the PE-S and NPE-S subgroups received condition-
ing in the evening session (with saline restriction to 5 ml only
in the group with restriction) and water (15 min) in the morn-
ing session. After 1 day of recovery with water (15 min) in the
morning and evening sessions, the response to saline (testing
stage) was tested in all animals in the evening session for 5
Learn Behav
days. Water was available for 15 min in the morning sessions.
Table 1 summarizes the behavioral procedure.
Statistical analysis
The saline consumption on the conditioning day was analyzed
using a 2 × 2 × 2 factorial design with three between-subjects
factors, the first being preexposure (preexposure vs.
nonpreexposure), the second factor being context (same BS^
vs. different BD^ temporal context of conditioning), and the
third factor being restriction in the intake of the taste stimulus
(restriction vs. no restriction). The saline consumption across
the test days was analyzed by a 2 × 2 × 2 × 5 repeated-
measures ANOVA. Bayesian model comparison was conduct-
ed because of the sample size of some subgroups. When the
interactions were significant, Newman–Keuls post hoc tests
were applied to analyze differences. The critical level of sig-
nificance in all tests was set to p < .05. The analyses were
carried out using SPSS software.
Results
The results indicate that a change of the time of day between
preexposure and conditioning (PE-D subgroup) disrupts the
latent inhibition of CTA in animals without restriction in the
intake of the taste stimulus. In contrast, latent inhibition was
acquired when preexposure and conditioning were performed
at the same time of day (PE-S subgroup) in these animals.
Figure 1 represents the mean consumption by subgroups on
the preexposure, conditioning, and test days for animals with-
out restriction.
Regarding the group with restriction in the intake of the
taste stimulus during conditioning, unlike the group without
restriction, the results indicate that a change of the time of day
between preexposure and conditioning (PE-D subgroup) does
not disrupt the latent inhibition of CTA. Latent inhibition was
acquired by all subgroups regardless of a change in the tem-
poral context. For the group with restriction, Fig. 2 represents
the mean consumption by subgroups on the pre-exposure,
conditioning and test days.
An ANOVA of the mean consumption on the conditioning
day indicated no significant effect of the interaction between
preexposure, context, and restriction, F(1, 42) = 2.77, p = .1.
The repeated-measures ANOVA conducted to evaluate the
Table 1 Behavioral procedure (preexposure, conditioning, and
testing
stages)
GROUP PE1-2/Wpm CTA T1–T5pm
PE-D Water am
Saline 1% pm
Saline + LiCl ip am Saline pm
PE-S Water am
Saline 1% pm
Saline + LiCl ip pm Saline pm
NPE-D Water am–pm Saline + LiCl ip am Saline pm
NPE-S Water am–pm Saline + LiCl ip pm Saline pm
Note. PE1-2/Wpm = Days 1 and 2 of water or preexposure to
saline in the
evening session; CTA = conditioning day; T1–T5pm = tests
days in the
evening session; PE-D = preexposed subgroup in the Bdifferent
(D)^
context (CTA in the morning session); PE-S = preexposed
subgroup in
the Bsame (S)^ context (CTA in the evening session); NPE-D =
nonpreexposed subgroup in the Bdifferent^ context; NPE-S =
nonpreexposed subgroup in the Bsame^ context. For brevity, the
morning
consumption after CTA is not represented. Animals with saline
restriction
in the conditioning day (group with restriction) were exposed to
5 ml of
saline on the conditioning day, and the remaining of the
procedure was
identical to that of animals without saline restriction on the
conditioning
day (group without restriction).
0
2
4
6
8
10
12
14
16
PE-D PE-S
NPE-D NPE-S
M
ill
ili
te
rs
Fig. 1 Mean consumption of saline/water (in ml) by subgroups
without
restriction in the intake of the taste stimulus at different stages
of the
behavioral procedure, and standard deviation. BL4 = last of the
4 days
of baseline; am = morning session; pm = evening session; PE1-
2/Wpm =
Days 1 and 2 of water or preexposure to saline in the evening
session;
CTA = conditioning day; T1–T5pm = tests days in the evening
session;
PE-D = preexposed subgroup in the Bdifferent (D)^ context
(CTA in the
morning session); PE-S = preexposed subgroup in the Bsame
(S)^ context
(CTA in the evening session); NPE-D = nonpreexposed
subgroup in the
Bdifferent (D)^ context; NPE-S = nonpreexposed subgroup in
the Bsame
(S)^ context. For brevity, the morning consumption after CTA is
not
represented. The analysis of the main interaction shows that the
PE-S,
but not the PE-D, subgroup without restriction acquired latent
inhibition.
Learn Behav
mean consumption of each group throughout the 5 test days
revealed a significant effect of the interaction between
preexposure, context, restriction, and test F(4, 39) = 21.72, p
= .001. Newman–Keuls post hoc tests indicated no significant
effect of the preexposure factor in the different context for
animals without restriction (p > .05), indicating that the PE-
D subgroup did not show latent inhibition. There was a sig-
nificant effect of the preexposure factor in the same context (p
< .05) for these animals, which indicates that the PE-S sub-
group without restriction acquired latent inhibition. Newman–
Keuls post hoc tests also showed a significant effect of the
context factor in the preexposed animals without restriction,
indicating that the mean consumption of the PE-S subgroup
was higher than that of the PE-D subgroup (p < .05). In con-
trast, there was a significant effect of the preexposure factor
for animals with restriction (p < .05), which indicates that all
preexposed subgroups (PE-D and PE-S) with restriction ac-
quired latent inhibition. Data from Bayesian model compari-
son conducted to evaluate statistical power of the results are
shown in Table 2. Bayesian model comparison was imple-
mented for the repeated-measures ANOVA, including test
days as repeated measures (5 days) and group as between-
subjects factor (with comparisons for the resulting groups
from the Preexposure × Context × Restriction interaction).
These comparisons support the findings of the repeated-
measures ANOVA, in showing that a model including a Test
Days × Group interaction provides a better fit to the data than a
model with main effects but no interaction: the Bayes Factor
in favor of the interaction model is given by 2.710e199/
2.743e100 = 9.88 e98.
0
2
4
6
8
10
12
14
16
PE-D PE-S
NPE-D NPE-S
M
ill
ili
te
rs
Fig. 2 Mean consumption of saline/water (in ml) by subgroups
with
restriction in the intake of the taste stimulus at different stages
of the
behavioral procedure, and standard deviation. BL4 = last of the
4 days
of baseline; am = morning session; pm = evening session; PE1-
2/Wpm =
Days 1 and 2 of water or preexposure to saline in the evening
session;
CTA = conditioning day (saline restriction to 5 ml); T1–T5pm =
tests
days in the evening session; PE-D = preexposed subgroup in the
Bdifferent (D)^ context (CTA in the morning session); PE-S =
preexposed subgroup in the Bsame (S)^ context (CTA in the
evening
session); NPE-D = nonpreexposed subgroup in the Bdifferent
(D)^
condition; NPE-S = nonpreexposed subgroup in the Bsame (S)^
condition. The analysis of the main interaction indicates that all
preexposed subgroups (PE-D and PE-S) with restriction
acquired latent
inhibition.
Table 2 Bayesian model comparison for the repeated-measures
ANOVA
Models P(M) P(M|data) BF M BF 10 % error
Null model (incl. subject) 0.250 4.092e - 74 1.228e - 73 1.000
Test 0.250 0.427 2.236 1.044e + 73 0.438
CTA 0.250 3.669e - 74 1.101e -73 0.897 0.952
Test + CTA 0.250 0.573 4.025 1.400e + 73 3.498
Null model (incl. subject) 0.200 3.690e - 200 1.476e - 199
1.000
Test 0.200 3.879e - 127 1.551e - 126 1.051e + 73 0.630
Group 0.200 1.316e - 188 5.265e - 188 3.567e + 11 0.495
Test + Group 0.200 1.012e - 99 4.048e - 99 2.743e + 100 0.565
Test + Group + Test × Group 0.200 1.000 3.952e + 99 2.710e +
199 0.889
Note. BF = Bayesian F; CTA = conditioning day; Group =
resulting groups from the Preexposure × Context × Restriction
interaction; M = comparison
model; P = probability for models. All models include subject.
Considering null model as 0, main effects as 1 and interaction
effects as 2: BF1,2 = BF1,0 *
BF0,2 = BF1,0/BF2,0 = 2.743e +100/2.710e + 199 = 1.012e -
99. These comparisons support the findings of the repeated-
measures ANOVA, in showing
that a model including a Test Days × Group interaction provides
a better fit to the data than a model with main effects but no
interaction: the Bayes factor
in favor of the interaction model is given by
2.710e199/2.743e100 = 9.88 e98.
Learn Behav
Discussion
The results of this study indicate that the latent inhibition
of CTA may be disrupted by changes in the temporal
context between preexposure and conditioning. This tem-
poral specificity of the latent inhibition seems only to
occur when animals have free access to the taste stimulus
during the exposure interval, but not when the taste stim-
ulus is restricted and limits the intake behavior of animals.
Throughout the test days, the consumption of the PE-S
subgroup in the group without restriction in the intake
of the taste stimulus was higher than that of the PE-D
and NPE-S subgroups. However, the consumption of the
PE-D subgroup was not different from that of the NPE-D
subgroup. Thus, only animals in which the temporal con-
text remained the same between preexposure and condi-
tioning (PE-S subgroup) showed latent inhibition in the
group without restriction. Although the sample size in
some subgroups was limited, statistical analyses of the
data suggest that the temporal context has an effect on
latent inhibition when no restriction in the intake is ap-
plied during conditioning. It can be argued that, assuming
the statistical power of the main interaction shown by the
Bayesian model comparison, these results support the
possibility that a free control of the intake during condi-
tioning facilitates the temporal specificity of the latent
inhibition in the CTA paradigm, and an external control
of the intake during conditioning affects the temporal
specificity of this phenomenon. Considering these limita-
tions, these findings seem to be consistent with previous
studies in which different contextual cues have been used.
For example, latent inhibition was reduced in a study in
which external cues (and other variables such as the num-
ber of stimulus preexposures) were changed between
preexposure and testing (De la Casa & Lubow, 2001),
indicating that latent inhibition is sensitive to contextual
changes in different stages of the experimental procedure.
In the CTA paradigm, latent inhibition attenuation after an
external context change between preexposure and condi-
tioning/testing, but not between pre-exposure/condition-
ing and testing, also has been described (Quintero et al.,
2011). Therefore, different contextual changes (both ex-
ternal and internal) applied in different stages of the be-
havioral procedure may affect the acquisition of latent
inhibition (Hall & Channell, 1986; Quintero et al., 2014;
Revillo et al., 2014).
In contrast, no temporal specificity of latent inhibition of
CTA was detected when the intake of the taste stimulus was
restricted during conditioning. In this condition, both the PE-S
and PE-D subgroups showed latent inhibition, and the magni-
tude of taste aversion over the test days was lower in these
subgroups with respect to the nonpreexposed animals, regard-
less of a change in the temporal context. All these findings can
hardly be attributed to a contextual effect on the retrieval of
learning (Killcross, 2001; Maren & Holt, 2000) because the
context in the test days (evening session) should not point to
the conditioned stimulus–unconditioned stimulus association
in the PE-D subgroup without restriction in the intake of the
taste stimulus (and therefore this subgroup should express
latent inhibition). Rather, the effect of a change of the temporal
context on latent inhibition may be due to contextual influ-
ences on the association between stimuli (Hall, 1991; Kwok &
Boakes, 2015; Lubow & De la Casa, 2005; Schmajuk, Lam, &
Gray, 1996).
On the other hand, the behavioral procedure of this study
resulted in the delay between the final preexposure trial and
conditioning being different for Bdifferent^ and Bsame^ sub-
groups. In particular, the PE-D subgroups had a shorter delay
compared with the PE-S subgroups, which could influence the
results. Nevertheless, because greater temporal proximity be-
tween preexposure and conditioning should strengthen the
effect of preexposures (De la Casa & Lubow, 2001), greater
latent inhibition should be expected in the Bdifferent^ sub-
groups. The results indicate, however, that the subgroup with
greater temporal proximity between preexposure and condi-
tioning (PE-D subgroup) showed a disruption of latent inhibi-
tion. In addition, this only happened in the subgroup without
restriction in the intake of the taste stimulus during condition-
ing (PE-D and no restriction), although there was also this
greater temporal proximity in the restricted subgroup (PE-D
and restriction).
In general, we can conclude that the behavioral procedure
to induce temporal specificity of latent inhibition of CTA is
sensitive to the exposure conditions of the taste stimulus dur-
ing conditioning. In this experiment, we used 4 days of habit-
uation to the temporal context. Temporal specificity of latent
inhibition of CTA under long periods of contextual habitua-
tion has been described (Molero-Chamizo, 2013), but a short
period seems to be a factor that hinders the contextual effect
on taste aversion (Morón et al., 2002). The results of this study
complement our knowledge of this phenomenon because they
show that an exposure without restriction to the taste stimulus
under a long period of contextual habituation facilitates the
temporal specificity of the latent inhibition of CTA. However,
an external control of the intake behavior during conditioning
(a restriction in the intake of the conditioned taste stimulus)
under the same period of habituation disrupts this contextual
control of the latent inhibition. Thus, long periods of
temporal-contextual habituation might facilitate the temporal
specificity phenomenon when animals experience a determi-
nate exposure of the conditioned stimulus during
conditioning.
Because of the three-stage procedure of the study, the effects
of contextual changes on latent inhibition can be attributed to
associative mechanisms occurring during conditioning, which
may result in changes in the associative strength between
Learn Behav
stimuli (Escobar, Arcediano, & Miller, 2002; Westbrook, Jones,
Bailey, & Harris, 2000). A restriction in the intake of the con-
ditioned taste stimulus during conditioning might affect the
temporal specificity of the latent inhibition of CTA via a mod-
ulation of these associative processes. Regarding this modula-
tion, one possibility is that the experience with the taste
stimulus
is able to modify the way in which the time of day is treated by
the memory systems. It is also possible that the arousal level
associated with a particular intake can alter the attention direct-
ed to contextual cues such as the time of day. Because much
remains unknown about the processes of latent inhibition, these
results may guide further studies to elucidate the mechanisms
involved in the latent inhibition of CTA and the contextual and
temporal modulation of this learning.
Acknowledgments This work was supported by the Ministry of
Science and Technology (MICYT), Spain, under Grant Number
BSO2002-01215, CICYT. The author declares that there is no
conflict
of interest. The author is grateful to M. Gallo, I. Morón, and M.
A.
Ballesteros, who contributed to the execution of the
experiments. The
author wishes to thank the anonymous reviewers for their
valuable com-
ments and suggestions, and Francisco Herrero Machaconses for
helpful
comments on earlier drafts of the manuscript.
References
Boakes, R. A., Elliot, M., Swinbourne, A. L., & Westbrook, R.
F. (1997).
Context dependency of conditioned aversions to water and
sweet
tastes. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior
Processes, 230, 56–57. doi:10.1037/0097-7403.23.1.56
Bonardi, C., Honey, R. C., & Hall, G. (1990). Context-
specificity of
conditioning in flavor-aversion learning: Extinction and
blocking
tests. Animal Learning & Behaviour, 18, 229–237. doi:10.3758
/BF03205280
Bouton, M. E. (1993). Context, time, and memory retrieval in
the inter-
ference paradigms of Pavlovian learning. Psychological
Bulletin,
114, 80–99. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.114.1.80
Bouton, M. E., Westbrook, R. F., Corcoran, K. A., & Maren, S.
(2006).
Contextual and temporal modulations of extinction: Behavioral
and
biological mechanisms. Biological Psychiatry, 60, 352–360.
doi:10.1016/j.biopsych.2005.12
De la Casa, L. G., & Lubow, R. E. (1995). Latent inhibition in
condi-
tioned taste aversion: The roles of stimulus frequency and
duration
and the amount of fluid ingested during preexposure.
Neurobiology
of Learning and Memory, 64, 125–132. doi:10.1006
/nlme.1995.1051
De la Casa, L. G., & Lubow, R. E. (2001). Latent inhibition
with a
response time measure from a within-subject design: Effects of
num-
ber of preexposures, masking task, context change, and delay.
Neuropsychology, 15, 244–253. doi:10.1037/0894-
4105.15.2.244
Escobar, M., Arcediano, F., & Miller, R. R. (2002). Latent
inhibition and
contextual associations. Journal of Experimental Psychology:
Animal Behavior Processes, 28, 123–136. doi:10.1037/0097-
7403.28.2.123
González, F., Garcia-Burgos, D., & Hall, G. (2012). Associative
and
occasion-setting properties of contextual cues in flavor-nutrient
learning in rats. Appetite, 59, 898–904. doi:10.1016/j.
appet.2012.09.004
González, F., Morillas, E., & Hall, G. (2015). Latent inhibition
in flavor-
preference conditioning: Effects of motivational state and the
nature
of the reinforcer. Learning & Behavior, 43(4), 376–383.
doi:10.3758
/s13420-015-0185-y
Hall, G. (1991). Perceptual and associative learning. Oxford,
UK:
Clarendon Press.
Hall, G., & Channell, S. (1986). Context specificity of latent
inhibition in
taste aversion learning. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental
Psychology, 38B, 121–139.
Killcross, S. (2001). Loss of latent inhibition in conditioned
taste aversion
following exposure to a novel flavour before test. The Quarterly
Journal of Experimental Psychology, 54B, 271–288.
doi:10.1080
/713932758
Kwok, D. W., & Boakes, R. A. (2015). Proximal, but not distal,
pre-
exposure reduces serial overshadowing in one-trial taste
aversion
learning. Behavioural Processes, 118, 111–114. doi:10.1016/j.
beproc.2015.06.006
Lubow, R. E., & De la Casa, L. G. (2005). There is a time and a
place for
everything: Bidirectional modulations of latent inhibition by
time-
induced context differentiation. Psychonomic Bulletin &
Review,
12(5), 806–821. doi:10.3758/BF03196774
Lukoyanov, N. V., Pereira, P. A., Mesquita, R. M., & Andrade,
J. P.
(2002). Restricted feeding facilitates time-place learning in
adult
rats. Behavioural Brain Research, 134, 283–290. doi:10.1016
/S0166-4328(02)00036-0
Manrique, T., Molero, A., Ballesteros, M. A., Morón, I., Gallo,
M., &
Fenton, A. (2004). Time of day-dependent latent inhibition of
con-
ditioned taste aversions in rats. Neurobiology of Learning and
Memory, 82, 77–80. doi:10.1016/j.nlm.2004.04.003
Manrique, T., Gámiz, F., Morón, I., Ballesteros, M. A., &
Gallo, M.
(2009). Peculiar modulation of taste aversion learning by the
time
of day in developing rats. Developmental Psychobiology, 51,
147–
157. doi:10.1002/dev.20354
Maren, S., & Holt, W. (2000). The hippocampus and contextual
memory
retrieval in Pavlovian conditioning. Behavioural Brain
Research,
110, 97–108. doi:10.1016/S0166-4328(99)00188-6
Molero, A., Morón, I., Ballesteros, M. A., Manrique, T., Fenton,
A., &
Gallo, M. (2005). Hippocampus, temporal context and taste
memo-
ries. Chemical Senses, 30, 160–161. doi:10.1093/chemse/bjh163
Molero-Chamizo, A. (2013). Excitotoxic lesion of the
hippocampus of
Wistar rats disrupts the circadian control of the latent inhibition
of
taste aversion learning. Brain Research, 1533, 105–113.
doi:10.1016/j.brainres.2013.08.030
Morón, I., Manrique, T., Molero, A., Ballesteros, M. A., Gallo,
M., &
Fenton, A. (2002). The contextual modulation of conditioned
taste
aversions by the physical environment and time of day is
similar.
Learning & Memory, 9, 18–223. doi:10.1101/lm.52202
Pearce, J. M., & Bouton, M. E. (2001). Theories of associative
learning in
animals. Annual Review of Psychology, 52, 111–139.
doi:10.1146
/annurev.psych.52.1.111
Quintero, E., Díaz, E., Vargas, J. P., Schmajuk, N., López, J.
C., & De la
Casa, L. G. (2011). Effects of context novelty vs. familiarity on
latent inhibition with a conditioned taste aversion procedure.
Behavioural Processes, 86, 242–249. doi:10.1016/j.
beproc.2010.12.011
Quintero, E., Vargas, J. P., Diaz, E., Escarabajal, M. D.,
Carrasco, M., &
López, J. C. (2014). c-Fos positive nucleus reveals that
contextual
specificity of latent inhibition is dependent of insular cortex.
Brain
R e s e a rc h B u l l e t i n , 1 0 8 , 7 4 – 7 9 . d o i : 1 0 . 1 0 1
6 / j .
brainresbull.2014.08.008
Revillo, D. A., Gaztañaga, M., Aranda, E., Paglini, M. G.,
Chotro, M. G.,
& Arias, C. (2014). Context-dependent latent inhibition in
preweanling rats. Developmental Psychobiology, 56(7), 1507–
1517. doi:10.1002/dev.21236
Rosas, J. M., & Bouton, M. E. (1997). Renewal of a conditioned
taste
aversion upon return to the conditioning context after extinction
in
another one. Learning and Motivation, 28, 216–229.
doi:10.1006
/lmot.1996.0960
Learn Behav
http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0097-7403.23.1.56
http://dx.doi.org/10.3758/BF03205280
http://dx.doi.org/10.3758/BF03205280
http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.114.1.80
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsych.2005.12
http://dx.doi.org/10.1006/nlme.1995.1051
http://dx.doi.org/10.1006/nlme.1995.1051
http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0894-4105.15.2.244
http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0097-7403.28.2.123
http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0097-7403.28.2.123
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.appet.2012.09.004
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.appet.2012.09.004
http://dx.doi.org/10.3758/s13420-015-0185-y
http://dx.doi.org/10.3758/s13420-015-0185-y
http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/713932758
http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/713932758
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.beproc.2015.06.006
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.beproc.2015.06.006
http://dx.doi.org/10.3758/BF03196774
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0166-4328(02)00036-0
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0166-4328(02)00036-0
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.nlm.2004.04.003
http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/dev.20354
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0166-4328(99)00188-6
http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/chemse/bjh163
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.brainres.2013.08.030
http://dx.doi.org/10.1101/lm.52202
http://dx.doi.org/10.1146/annurev.psych.52.1.111
http://dx.doi.org/10.1146/annurev.psych.52.1.111
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.beproc.2010.12.011
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.beproc.2010.12.011
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.brainresbull.2014.08.008
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.brainresbull.2014.08.008
http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/dev.21236
http://dx.doi.org/10.1006/lmot.1996.0960
http://dx.doi.org/10.1006/lmot.1996.0960
Schmajuk, N. A., Lam, Y. W., & Gray, J. A. (1996). Latent
inhibition: A
neural network approach. Journal of Experimental Psychology:
Animal Behavior Processes, 22, 321–349. doi:10.1037/0097-
7403.22.3.321
Westbrook, R. F., Jones, M. L., Bailey, G. K., & Harris, J. A.
(2000).
Contextual control over conditioning responding in a latent
inhibi-
tion paradigm. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal
Behavior Processes, 26, 157–173. doi:10.1037/0097-
7403.26.2.157
Learn Behav
http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0097-7403.22.3.321
http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0097-7403.22.3.321
http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0097-7403.26.2.157Circadian-
temporal...AbstractMethodSubjectsProcedureStatistical
analysisResultsDiscussionReferences

More Related Content

Similar to Circadian-temporal context and latent inhibition of conditione.docx

JAPS project published
JAPS project publishedJAPS project published
JAPS project published
Rahul MK
 
Effect of tulsi on some biochemical parameters
Effect of tulsi on some biochemical parametersEffect of tulsi on some biochemical parameters
Effect of tulsi on some biochemical parameters
Dilruba Siraji
 
Effect of dietary cholesterol on shrimp growth and salinity stress.pptx
Effect of dietary cholesterol on shrimp growth and salinity stress.pptxEffect of dietary cholesterol on shrimp growth and salinity stress.pptx
Effect of dietary cholesterol on shrimp growth and salinity stress.pptx
Emmerik Motte
 
Hypothalamic tanycyte sensing of amino acids in fed and fasted rats
Hypothalamic tanycyte sensing of amino acids in fed and fasted ratsHypothalamic tanycyte sensing of amino acids in fed and fasted rats
Hypothalamic tanycyte sensing of amino acids in fed and fasted rats
Libby Copsey
 
CSETAC poster 2016- 2nd draft
CSETAC poster 2016- 2nd draftCSETAC poster 2016- 2nd draft
CSETAC poster 2016- 2nd draft
Lauren Stoczynski
 
Effects of Noise Stress on Body Weight and Adrenal Gland Weight of Male Wista...
Effects of Noise Stress on Body Weight and Adrenal Gland Weight of Male Wista...Effects of Noise Stress on Body Weight and Adrenal Gland Weight of Male Wista...
Effects of Noise Stress on Body Weight and Adrenal Gland Weight of Male Wista...
BRNSS Publication Hub
 

Similar to Circadian-temporal context and latent inhibition of conditione.docx (20)

33 35
33 3533 35
33 35
 
Nebivolol prevents indomethacin induced gastric ulcer in rats
Nebivolol prevents indomethacin induced gastric ulcer in ratsNebivolol prevents indomethacin induced gastric ulcer in rats
Nebivolol prevents indomethacin induced gastric ulcer in rats
 
Bioavailability Studies of Ketorolac Tromethamine Fast Dissolving Tablets Pre...
Bioavailability Studies of Ketorolac Tromethamine Fast Dissolving Tablets Pre...Bioavailability Studies of Ketorolac Tromethamine Fast Dissolving Tablets Pre...
Bioavailability Studies of Ketorolac Tromethamine Fast Dissolving Tablets Pre...
 
JAPS project published
JAPS project publishedJAPS project published
JAPS project published
 
Effect of tulsi on some biochemical parameters
Effect of tulsi on some biochemical parametersEffect of tulsi on some biochemical parameters
Effect of tulsi on some biochemical parameters
 
We are what we eat - The role of diets in the gut-microbiota-health interaction
We are what we eat - The role of diets in the gut-microbiota-health interactionWe are what we eat - The role of diets in the gut-microbiota-health interaction
We are what we eat - The role of diets in the gut-microbiota-health interaction
 
Mesexclor
MesexclorMesexclor
Mesexclor
 
Effect of dietary cholesterol on shrimp growth and salinity stress.pptx
Effect of dietary cholesterol on shrimp growth and salinity stress.pptxEffect of dietary cholesterol on shrimp growth and salinity stress.pptx
Effect of dietary cholesterol on shrimp growth and salinity stress.pptx
 
Hypothalamic tanycyte sensing of amino acids in fed and fasted rats
Hypothalamic tanycyte sensing of amino acids in fed and fasted ratsHypothalamic tanycyte sensing of amino acids in fed and fasted rats
Hypothalamic tanycyte sensing of amino acids in fed and fasted rats
 
CSETAC poster 2016- 2nd draft
CSETAC poster 2016- 2nd draftCSETAC poster 2016- 2nd draft
CSETAC poster 2016- 2nd draft
 
Garlic paper
Garlic paperGarlic paper
Garlic paper
 
SAFETY PHARMACOLOGY STUDIES final .pptx
SAFETY PHARMACOLOGY STUDIES final .pptxSAFETY PHARMACOLOGY STUDIES final .pptx
SAFETY PHARMACOLOGY STUDIES final .pptx
 
Examining Neurobehavioral Toxicity of Patulin in Adult Zebrafish
Examining Neurobehavioral Toxicity of Patulin in Adult ZebrafishExamining Neurobehavioral Toxicity of Patulin in Adult Zebrafish
Examining Neurobehavioral Toxicity of Patulin in Adult Zebrafish
 
Tocixity studies
Tocixity studiesTocixity studies
Tocixity studies
 
Gastro Retentive DDS
Gastro Retentive DDSGastro Retentive DDS
Gastro Retentive DDS
 
04_IJPBA_1847_20.pdf
04_IJPBA_1847_20.pdf04_IJPBA_1847_20.pdf
04_IJPBA_1847_20.pdf
 
Effects of Noise Stress on Body Weight and Adrenal Gland Weight of Male Wista...
Effects of Noise Stress on Body Weight and Adrenal Gland Weight of Male Wista...Effects of Noise Stress on Body Weight and Adrenal Gland Weight of Male Wista...
Effects of Noise Stress on Body Weight and Adrenal Gland Weight of Male Wista...
 
04_IJPBA_1847_20.pdf
04_IJPBA_1847_20.pdf04_IJPBA_1847_20.pdf
04_IJPBA_1847_20.pdf
 
Ijet v5 i6p17
Ijet v5 i6p17Ijet v5 i6p17
Ijet v5 i6p17
 
Gastroretentive Drug Delivery System
Gastroretentive Drug Delivery SystemGastroretentive Drug Delivery System
Gastroretentive Drug Delivery System
 

More from christinemaritza

ENG315                                    Professional Scenari.docx
ENG315                                    Professional Scenari.docxENG315                                    Professional Scenari.docx
ENG315                                    Professional Scenari.docx
christinemaritza
 
ENG115ASSIGNMENT2STANCEESSAYDRAFTDueWeek.docx
ENG115ASSIGNMENT2STANCEESSAYDRAFTDueWeek.docxENG115ASSIGNMENT2STANCEESSAYDRAFTDueWeek.docx
ENG115ASSIGNMENT2STANCEESSAYDRAFTDueWeek.docx
christinemaritza
 
ENG 510 Final Project Milestone Three Guidelines and Rubric .docx
ENG 510 Final Project Milestone Three Guidelines and Rubric .docxENG 510 Final Project Milestone Three Guidelines and Rubric .docx
ENG 510 Final Project Milestone Three Guidelines and Rubric .docx
christinemaritza
 
ENG-105 Peer Review Worksheet Rhetorical Analysis of a Public.docx
ENG-105 Peer Review Worksheet Rhetorical Analysis of a Public.docxENG-105 Peer Review Worksheet Rhetorical Analysis of a Public.docx
ENG-105 Peer Review Worksheet Rhetorical Analysis of a Public.docx
christinemaritza
 
ENG 272-0Objective The purpose of this essay is t.docx
ENG 272-0Objective  The purpose of this essay is t.docxENG 272-0Objective  The purpose of this essay is t.docx
ENG 272-0Objective The purpose of this essay is t.docx
christinemaritza
 
ENG 360 01 American PoetrySpring 2019TuesdayFriday 800 –.docx
ENG 360 01 American PoetrySpring 2019TuesdayFriday 800 –.docxENG 360 01 American PoetrySpring 2019TuesdayFriday 800 –.docx
ENG 360 01 American PoetrySpring 2019TuesdayFriday 800 –.docx
christinemaritza
 
ENG 4034AHamlet Final AssessmentDUE DATE WEDNESDAY, 1220, 1.docx
ENG 4034AHamlet Final AssessmentDUE DATE WEDNESDAY, 1220, 1.docxENG 4034AHamlet Final AssessmentDUE DATE WEDNESDAY, 1220, 1.docx
ENG 4034AHamlet Final AssessmentDUE DATE WEDNESDAY, 1220, 1.docx
christinemaritza
 
ENG 3107 Writing for the Professions—Business & Social Scienc.docx
ENG 3107 Writing for the Professions—Business & Social Scienc.docxENG 3107 Writing for the Professions—Business & Social Scienc.docx
ENG 3107 Writing for the Professions—Business & Social Scienc.docx
christinemaritza
 
ENG 271Plato and Aristotlea Classical Greek philosophe.docx
ENG 271Plato and Aristotlea Classical Greek philosophe.docxENG 271Plato and Aristotlea Classical Greek philosophe.docx
ENG 271Plato and Aristotlea Classical Greek philosophe.docx
christinemaritza
 
ENG 315 Professional Communication Week 4 Discussion Deliver.docx
ENG 315 Professional Communication Week 4 Discussion Deliver.docxENG 315 Professional Communication Week 4 Discussion Deliver.docx
ENG 315 Professional Communication Week 4 Discussion Deliver.docx
christinemaritza
 
ENG 315 Professional Communication Week 9Professional Exp.docx
ENG 315 Professional Communication Week 9Professional Exp.docxENG 315 Professional Communication Week 9Professional Exp.docx
ENG 315 Professional Communication Week 9Professional Exp.docx
christinemaritza
 
ENG 202 Questions about Point of View in Ursula K. Le Guin’s .docx
ENG 202 Questions about Point of View in Ursula K. Le Guin’s .docxENG 202 Questions about Point of View in Ursula K. Le Guin’s .docx
ENG 202 Questions about Point of View in Ursula K. Le Guin’s .docx
christinemaritza
 
ENG 220250 Lab Report Requirements Version 0.8 -- 0813201.docx
ENG 220250 Lab Report Requirements Version 0.8 -- 0813201.docxENG 220250 Lab Report Requirements Version 0.8 -- 0813201.docx
ENG 220250 Lab Report Requirements Version 0.8 -- 0813201.docx
christinemaritza
 
ENG 203 Short Article Response 2 Sample Answer (Worth 13 mark.docx
ENG 203 Short Article Response 2 Sample Answer (Worth 13 mark.docxENG 203 Short Article Response 2 Sample Answer (Worth 13 mark.docx
ENG 203 Short Article Response 2 Sample Answer (Worth 13 mark.docx
christinemaritza
 
ENG 130 Literature and Comp ENG 130 Argumentative Resear.docx
ENG 130 Literature and Comp ENG 130 Argumentative Resear.docxENG 130 Literature and Comp ENG 130 Argumentative Resear.docx
ENG 130 Literature and Comp ENG 130 Argumentative Resear.docx
christinemaritza
 
ENG 130- Literature and Comp Literary Response for Setting.docx
ENG 130- Literature and Comp Literary Response for Setting.docxENG 130- Literature and Comp Literary Response for Setting.docx
ENG 130- Literature and Comp Literary Response for Setting.docx
christinemaritza
 
ENG 130 Literature and Comp Literary Response for Point o.docx
ENG 130 Literature and Comp Literary Response for Point o.docxENG 130 Literature and Comp Literary Response for Point o.docx
ENG 130 Literature and Comp Literary Response for Point o.docx
christinemaritza
 

More from christinemaritza (20)

ENG315                                    Professional Scenari.docx
ENG315                                    Professional Scenari.docxENG315                                    Professional Scenari.docx
ENG315                                    Professional Scenari.docx
 
ENG122 – Research Paper Peer Review InstructionsApply each of .docx
ENG122 – Research Paper Peer Review InstructionsApply each of .docxENG122 – Research Paper Peer Review InstructionsApply each of .docx
ENG122 – Research Paper Peer Review InstructionsApply each of .docx
 
ENG122 – Research Paper Peer Review InstructionsApply each of th.docx
ENG122 – Research Paper Peer Review InstructionsApply each of th.docxENG122 – Research Paper Peer Review InstructionsApply each of th.docx
ENG122 – Research Paper Peer Review InstructionsApply each of th.docx
 
ENG115ASSIGNMENT2STANCEESSAYDRAFTDueWeek.docx
ENG115ASSIGNMENT2STANCEESSAYDRAFTDueWeek.docxENG115ASSIGNMENT2STANCEESSAYDRAFTDueWeek.docx
ENG115ASSIGNMENT2STANCEESSAYDRAFTDueWeek.docx
 
ENG 510 Final Project Milestone Three Guidelines and Rubric .docx
ENG 510 Final Project Milestone Three Guidelines and Rubric .docxENG 510 Final Project Milestone Three Guidelines and Rubric .docx
ENG 510 Final Project Milestone Three Guidelines and Rubric .docx
 
ENG-105 Peer Review Worksheet Rhetorical Analysis of a Public.docx
ENG-105 Peer Review Worksheet Rhetorical Analysis of a Public.docxENG-105 Peer Review Worksheet Rhetorical Analysis of a Public.docx
ENG-105 Peer Review Worksheet Rhetorical Analysis of a Public.docx
 
ENG 272-0Objective The purpose of this essay is t.docx
ENG 272-0Objective  The purpose of this essay is t.docxENG 272-0Objective  The purpose of this essay is t.docx
ENG 272-0Objective The purpose of this essay is t.docx
 
ENG 360 01 American PoetrySpring 2019TuesdayFriday 800 –.docx
ENG 360 01 American PoetrySpring 2019TuesdayFriday 800 –.docxENG 360 01 American PoetrySpring 2019TuesdayFriday 800 –.docx
ENG 360 01 American PoetrySpring 2019TuesdayFriday 800 –.docx
 
ENG 4034AHamlet Final AssessmentDUE DATE WEDNESDAY, 1220, 1.docx
ENG 4034AHamlet Final AssessmentDUE DATE WEDNESDAY, 1220, 1.docxENG 4034AHamlet Final AssessmentDUE DATE WEDNESDAY, 1220, 1.docx
ENG 4034AHamlet Final AssessmentDUE DATE WEDNESDAY, 1220, 1.docx
 
ENG 3107 Writing for the Professions—Business & Social Scienc.docx
ENG 3107 Writing for the Professions—Business & Social Scienc.docxENG 3107 Writing for the Professions—Business & Social Scienc.docx
ENG 3107 Writing for the Professions—Business & Social Scienc.docx
 
ENG 271Plato and Aristotlea Classical Greek philosophe.docx
ENG 271Plato and Aristotlea Classical Greek philosophe.docxENG 271Plato and Aristotlea Classical Greek philosophe.docx
ENG 271Plato and Aristotlea Classical Greek philosophe.docx
 
ENG 315 Professional Communication Week 4 Discussion Deliver.docx
ENG 315 Professional Communication Week 4 Discussion Deliver.docxENG 315 Professional Communication Week 4 Discussion Deliver.docx
ENG 315 Professional Communication Week 4 Discussion Deliver.docx
 
ENG 315 Professional Communication Week 9Professional Exp.docx
ENG 315 Professional Communication Week 9Professional Exp.docxENG 315 Professional Communication Week 9Professional Exp.docx
ENG 315 Professional Communication Week 9Professional Exp.docx
 
ENG 202 Questions about Point of View in Ursula K. Le Guin’s .docx
ENG 202 Questions about Point of View in Ursula K. Le Guin’s .docxENG 202 Questions about Point of View in Ursula K. Le Guin’s .docx
ENG 202 Questions about Point of View in Ursula K. Le Guin’s .docx
 
ENG 220250 Lab Report Requirements Version 0.8 -- 0813201.docx
ENG 220250 Lab Report Requirements Version 0.8 -- 0813201.docxENG 220250 Lab Report Requirements Version 0.8 -- 0813201.docx
ENG 220250 Lab Report Requirements Version 0.8 -- 0813201.docx
 
ENG 203 Short Article Response 2 Sample Answer (Worth 13 mark.docx
ENG 203 Short Article Response 2 Sample Answer (Worth 13 mark.docxENG 203 Short Article Response 2 Sample Answer (Worth 13 mark.docx
ENG 203 Short Article Response 2 Sample Answer (Worth 13 mark.docx
 
ENG 130 Literature and Comp ENG 130 Argumentative Resear.docx
ENG 130 Literature and Comp ENG 130 Argumentative Resear.docxENG 130 Literature and Comp ENG 130 Argumentative Resear.docx
ENG 130 Literature and Comp ENG 130 Argumentative Resear.docx
 
ENG 132What’s Wrong With HoldenHere’s What You Should Do, .docx
ENG 132What’s Wrong With HoldenHere’s What You Should Do, .docxENG 132What’s Wrong With HoldenHere’s What You Should Do, .docx
ENG 132What’s Wrong With HoldenHere’s What You Should Do, .docx
 
ENG 130- Literature and Comp Literary Response for Setting.docx
ENG 130- Literature and Comp Literary Response for Setting.docxENG 130- Literature and Comp Literary Response for Setting.docx
ENG 130- Literature and Comp Literary Response for Setting.docx
 
ENG 130 Literature and Comp Literary Response for Point o.docx
ENG 130 Literature and Comp Literary Response for Point o.docxENG 130 Literature and Comp Literary Response for Point o.docx
ENG 130 Literature and Comp Literary Response for Point o.docx
 

Recently uploaded

ppt your views.ppt your views of your college in your eyes
ppt your views.ppt your views of your college in your eyesppt your views.ppt your views of your college in your eyes
ppt your views.ppt your views of your college in your eyes
ashishpaul799
 
Industrial Training Report- AKTU Industrial Training Report
Industrial Training Report- AKTU Industrial Training ReportIndustrial Training Report- AKTU Industrial Training Report
Industrial Training Report- AKTU Industrial Training Report
Avinash Rai
 

Recently uploaded (20)

ppt your views.ppt your views of your college in your eyes
ppt your views.ppt your views of your college in your eyesppt your views.ppt your views of your college in your eyes
ppt your views.ppt your views of your college in your eyes
 
NCERT Solutions Power Sharing Class 10 Notes pdf
NCERT Solutions Power Sharing Class 10 Notes pdfNCERT Solutions Power Sharing Class 10 Notes pdf
NCERT Solutions Power Sharing Class 10 Notes pdf
 
Industrial Training Report- AKTU Industrial Training Report
Industrial Training Report- AKTU Industrial Training ReportIndustrial Training Report- AKTU Industrial Training Report
Industrial Training Report- AKTU Industrial Training Report
 
slides CapTechTalks Webinar May 2024 Alexander Perry.pptx
slides CapTechTalks Webinar May 2024 Alexander Perry.pptxslides CapTechTalks Webinar May 2024 Alexander Perry.pptx
slides CapTechTalks Webinar May 2024 Alexander Perry.pptx
 
50 ĐỀ LUYỆN THI IOE LỚP 9 - NĂM HỌC 2022-2023 (CÓ LINK HÌNH, FILE AUDIO VÀ ĐÁ...
50 ĐỀ LUYỆN THI IOE LỚP 9 - NĂM HỌC 2022-2023 (CÓ LINK HÌNH, FILE AUDIO VÀ ĐÁ...50 ĐỀ LUYỆN THI IOE LỚP 9 - NĂM HỌC 2022-2023 (CÓ LINK HÌNH, FILE AUDIO VÀ ĐÁ...
50 ĐỀ LUYỆN THI IOE LỚP 9 - NĂM HỌC 2022-2023 (CÓ LINK HÌNH, FILE AUDIO VÀ ĐÁ...
 
Operations Management - Book1.p - Dr. Abdulfatah A. Salem
Operations Management - Book1.p  - Dr. Abdulfatah A. SalemOperations Management - Book1.p  - Dr. Abdulfatah A. Salem
Operations Management - Book1.p - Dr. Abdulfatah A. Salem
 
Introduction to Quality Improvement Essentials
Introduction to Quality Improvement EssentialsIntroduction to Quality Improvement Essentials
Introduction to Quality Improvement Essentials
 
The Last Leaf, a short story by O. Henry
The Last Leaf, a short story by O. HenryThe Last Leaf, a short story by O. Henry
The Last Leaf, a short story by O. Henry
 
Morse OER Some Benefits and Challenges.pptx
Morse OER Some Benefits and Challenges.pptxMorse OER Some Benefits and Challenges.pptx
Morse OER Some Benefits and Challenges.pptx
 
How to Break the cycle of negative Thoughts
How to Break the cycle of negative ThoughtsHow to Break the cycle of negative Thoughts
How to Break the cycle of negative Thoughts
 
PART A. Introduction to Costumer Service
PART A. Introduction to Costumer ServicePART A. Introduction to Costumer Service
PART A. Introduction to Costumer Service
 
The impact of social media on mental health and well-being has been a topic o...
The impact of social media on mental health and well-being has been a topic o...The impact of social media on mental health and well-being has been a topic o...
The impact of social media on mental health and well-being has been a topic o...
 
Benefits and Challenges of Using Open Educational Resources
Benefits and Challenges of Using Open Educational ResourcesBenefits and Challenges of Using Open Educational Resources
Benefits and Challenges of Using Open Educational Resources
 
Salient features of Environment protection Act 1986.pptx
Salient features of Environment protection Act 1986.pptxSalient features of Environment protection Act 1986.pptx
Salient features of Environment protection Act 1986.pptx
 
B.ed spl. HI pdusu exam paper-2023-24.pdf
B.ed spl. HI pdusu exam paper-2023-24.pdfB.ed spl. HI pdusu exam paper-2023-24.pdf
B.ed spl. HI pdusu exam paper-2023-24.pdf
 
An Overview of the Odoo 17 Discuss App.pptx
An Overview of the Odoo 17 Discuss App.pptxAn Overview of the Odoo 17 Discuss App.pptx
An Overview of the Odoo 17 Discuss App.pptx
 
Telling Your Story_ Simple Steps to Build Your Nonprofit's Brand Webinar.pdf
Telling Your Story_ Simple Steps to Build Your Nonprofit's Brand Webinar.pdfTelling Your Story_ Simple Steps to Build Your Nonprofit's Brand Webinar.pdf
Telling Your Story_ Simple Steps to Build Your Nonprofit's Brand Webinar.pdf
 
Phrasal Verbs.XXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXX
Phrasal Verbs.XXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXPhrasal Verbs.XXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXX
Phrasal Verbs.XXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXX
 
Gyanartha SciBizTech Quiz slideshare.pptx
Gyanartha SciBizTech Quiz slideshare.pptxGyanartha SciBizTech Quiz slideshare.pptx
Gyanartha SciBizTech Quiz slideshare.pptx
 
INU_CAPSTONEDESIGN_비밀번호486_업로드용 발표자료.pdf
INU_CAPSTONEDESIGN_비밀번호486_업로드용 발표자료.pdfINU_CAPSTONEDESIGN_비밀번호486_업로드용 발표자료.pdf
INU_CAPSTONEDESIGN_비밀번호486_업로드용 발표자료.pdf
 

Circadian-temporal context and latent inhibition of conditione.docx

  • 1. Circadian-temporal context and latent inhibition of conditioned taste aversion: Effect of restriction in the intake of the conditioned taste stimulus Andrés Molero-Chamizo1,2 # Psychonomic Society, Inc. 2016 Abstract Latent inhibition of conditioned taste aversion (CTA) is sensitive to changes in the temporal context. A change in the time of day of conditioning with respect to the time of day of the preexposure can disrupt the latent inhibi- tion. This contextual change in the time of day may reveal a temporal specificity of latent inhibition. The optimum proce- dure to induce this temporal specificity is not well established. For example, it has been shown that a long period of habitu- ation to temporal contexts is one factor that can determine the effect. However, the experimental conditions on the condi- tioning day that facilitate this phenomenon are unknown. The aim of this study is to elucidate whether a restriction in the intake of the conditioned taste stimulus affects the tempo- ral specificity of latent inhibition. Two main groups of Wistar rats were tested in a latent inhibition of CTA paradigm, in which the temporal specificity of this phenomenon was ana- lyzed by a change in the time of day of conditioning. The intake of the taste stimulus was restricted in the conditioning day in one of the groups, but this restriction was not applied in the other group. The results indicated temporal specificity of latent inhibition only in the group without restriction, but not in the group with limitation in the intake of the taste stimulus during conditioning. These findings can help to elucidate the
  • 2. characteristics of the procedure to induce temporal specificity of latent inhibition. Keywords Conditioned taste aversion . Latent inhibition . Taste stimulus restriction . Temporal context The magnitude of a conditioned taste aversion (CTA) can be modulated by a change in the time of day between condition- ing and test (Manrique, Gámiz, Morón, Ballesteros, & Gallo, 2009; Morón et al., 2002). This procedure shows that animals tested in temporal contexts different from that of the condi- tioning acquire less aversion, which is similar to what has been consistently described by changing the spatial context (Boakes, Elliot, Swinbourne, & Westbrook, 1997; Bonardi, Honey, & Hall, 1990; Bouton, 1993; Bouton, Westbrook, Corcoran, & Maren, 2006; González, Garcia-Burgos, & Hall, 2012; Pearce & Bouton, 2001; Rosas & Bouton 1997). Similarly, the latent inhibition of CTA can be disrupted by changes in the time of day between preexposure and condi- tioning (Manrique et al., 2004; Molero et al., 2005). Nevertheless, the characteristics of the procedure that facilitate this temporal specificity of the latent inhibition (or dependen- cy of latent inhibition on the temporal context—time of day— of preexposure and conditioning) are not entirely known. Long periods of habituation to different temporal contexts seem to facilitate this phenomenon (Molero-Chamizo, 2013). This effect might mean that contextual familiarity and associative strength of the conditioned stimulus are relevant factors for the specificity of latent inhibition, as has been de- scribed in contextual learning (Pearce & Bouton, 2001). Moreover, a restriction in the intake of the conditioned taste stimulus may also influence the conditioning process and therefore may affect the temporal specificity of latent inhibition. As a result, a reduced processing of the taste stimulus during conditioning would prevent the effect of
  • 3. temporal specificity. For example, Morón et al. (2002) applied a restriction in the intake of the taste stimulus during * Andrés Molero-Chamizo [email protected] 1 Present address: Department of Psychobiology, University of Granada, Campus Cartuja, Granada 18071, Spain 2 Department of Psychology, Psychobiology Area, University of Huelva, Campus El Carmen, 21071 Huelva, Spain Learn Behav DOI 10.3758/s13420-016-0251-0 http://crossmark.crossref.org/dialog/?doi=10.3758/s13420-016- 0251-0&domain=pdf conditioning, resulting in a modulation of the taste aversion when tested in a different time of day from that of the condi- tioning. However, by 4 days of habituation to the temporal context, Molero-Chamizo (2013) found temporal specificity of the latent inhibition of CTAwithout considering any restric- tion in the amount of taste stimulus during conditioning. Although in both cases different processes were measured (CTA or latent inhibition of CTA), it seems necessary to clar- ify whether a restriction in the amount of taste stimulus during conditioning (and therefore an external control of the intake) facilitates or hinders the temporal specificity of the latent in- hibition of CTA, similar to that described in other learning processes (De la Casa & Lubow, 1995; González, Morillas, & Hall, 2015; Lukoyanov, Pereira, Mesquita, & Andrade, 2002). Thus, the aim of this study is to analyze the effects of restriction versus no restriction in the intake of the taste stim- ulus at the stage of conditioning on the temporal specificity of the latent inhibition of CTA.
  • 4. A similar procedure that has previously shown such tem- poral specificity (Molero-Chamizo, 2013) was used in this study. However, some of the animals had limited access to the intake of the taste stimulus in the day of conditioning (group with restriction), and the remaining animals had no restriction in the intake of the taste stimulus during condition- ing (group without restriction), except the time for recording that was common to all animals. If the temporal specificity of the latent inhibition of CTA depends on the free control of the intake during conditioning, then in animals with restriction in the taste stimulus (external control of the intake) the latent inhibition will not be specific of the temporal context; that is, these animals will show latent inhibition regardless of a change in the time of day between preexposure and conditioning. Method Subjects Fifty adult male Wistar rats, weighing between 280 and 300 g, were individually housed in boxes measuring 30 cm × 15 cm × 30 cm. All of the animals were exposed to a daily 12 hours light/dark cycle (lights on from 9:00 to 21:00), and the tem- perature conditions were kept constant at 23 °C. Throughout the procedure, food was provided ad libitum, and the avail- ability of fluid was restricted to two daily 15 min sessions, one in the morning (10:00) and one in the evening (20:00), for all animals. Twenty-four of them were exposed to a limited amount of taste stimulus (5 ml) for 15 min during conditioning (group with restriction), and 26 rats had free access to the intake of the taste stimulus for 15 min during conditioning (group without restriction). The procedure was approved by the Ethics Committee for Animal Research of the University
  • 5. of Granada and was conducted in accordance with both the NIH Publications (No. 80-23) of the National Institutes of Health Guide (United States) for the Care and Use of Laboratory Animals (1996 revision) and the European Community Council Directive of November 24, 1986 (86/ 609/EEC). The National Legislation, in agreement with this Directive, is defined in Royal Decree No. 1201/2005. Procedure In the group with restriction in the intake of the taste stimulus, rats were in turn distributed among the following four sub- groups: PE-S (preexposed to the taste, PE, and same time of day, S, for conditioning and testing, n = 8), PE-D (preexposed to the taste, PE, and different time of day, D, for conditioning and testing, n = 8), NPE-S (nonpreexposed to the taste, NPE, and same time of day, S, for conditioning and testing, n = 4), NPE-D (nonpreexposed to the taste, NPE, and different time of day, D, for conditioning and testing, n = 4). In the group without restriction, animals were distributed among the same four subgroups: PE-S (n = 8), PE-D (n = 8), NPE-S (n = 5), NPE-D (n = 5). All animals were water restricted and received two 15-min sessions of access to water per day, as described above, over 4 days to facilitate the differentiation of the temporal contexts (morning vs. evening). After this period, the procedure had three main stages (preexposure, conditioning, and testing). With respect to the preexposure stage, all rats had access to water in the morning session (15 min) for 2 days. The nonpreexposed subgroups (NPE-S and NPE-D) also received water in the evening session (15 min) of these 2 days, whereas the preexposed subgroups (PE-S and PE-D) were exposed to a sodium chloride solution dissolved in water (saline 1%) for 15 min. On the day following the last preexposure, conditioning occurred in the morning session for the PE-D and NPE-D
  • 6. subgroups. These subgroups of the group without restriction in the intake of the taste stimulus were exposed to saline for 15 min in the morning, and the amounts ingested were record- ed. The PE-D and NPE-D subgroups of the group with restric- tion were exposed to 5 ml of saline for 15 min in the morning, and the amounts ingested were recorded. This was the only difference in the procedure between the groups with and with- out restriction in the intake of the taste stimulus. Twenty mi- nutes later, all animals received an injection of lithium chlo- ride (LiCl 0.15 M, 2% of body weight, intraperitoneally). Water was available for 15 min in the evening session. In contrast, the PE-S and NPE-S subgroups received condition- ing in the evening session (with saline restriction to 5 ml only in the group with restriction) and water (15 min) in the morn- ing session. After 1 day of recovery with water (15 min) in the morning and evening sessions, the response to saline (testing stage) was tested in all animals in the evening session for 5 Learn Behav days. Water was available for 15 min in the morning sessions. Table 1 summarizes the behavioral procedure. Statistical analysis The saline consumption on the conditioning day was analyzed using a 2 × 2 × 2 factorial design with three between-subjects factors, the first being preexposure (preexposure vs. nonpreexposure), the second factor being context (same BS^ vs. different BD^ temporal context of conditioning), and the third factor being restriction in the intake of the taste stimulus (restriction vs. no restriction). The saline consumption across the test days was analyzed by a 2 × 2 × 2 × 5 repeated-
  • 7. measures ANOVA. Bayesian model comparison was conduct- ed because of the sample size of some subgroups. When the interactions were significant, Newman–Keuls post hoc tests were applied to analyze differences. The critical level of sig- nificance in all tests was set to p < .05. The analyses were carried out using SPSS software. Results The results indicate that a change of the time of day between preexposure and conditioning (PE-D subgroup) disrupts the latent inhibition of CTA in animals without restriction in the intake of the taste stimulus. In contrast, latent inhibition was acquired when preexposure and conditioning were performed at the same time of day (PE-S subgroup) in these animals. Figure 1 represents the mean consumption by subgroups on the preexposure, conditioning, and test days for animals with- out restriction. Regarding the group with restriction in the intake of the taste stimulus during conditioning, unlike the group without restriction, the results indicate that a change of the time of day between preexposure and conditioning (PE-D subgroup) does not disrupt the latent inhibition of CTA. Latent inhibition was acquired by all subgroups regardless of a change in the tem- poral context. For the group with restriction, Fig. 2 represents the mean consumption by subgroups on the pre-exposure, conditioning and test days. An ANOVA of the mean consumption on the conditioning day indicated no significant effect of the interaction between preexposure, context, and restriction, F(1, 42) = 2.77, p = .1. The repeated-measures ANOVA conducted to evaluate the Table 1 Behavioral procedure (preexposure, conditioning, and testing
  • 8. stages) GROUP PE1-2/Wpm CTA T1–T5pm PE-D Water am Saline 1% pm Saline + LiCl ip am Saline pm PE-S Water am Saline 1% pm Saline + LiCl ip pm Saline pm NPE-D Water am–pm Saline + LiCl ip am Saline pm NPE-S Water am–pm Saline + LiCl ip pm Saline pm Note. PE1-2/Wpm = Days 1 and 2 of water or preexposure to saline in the evening session; CTA = conditioning day; T1–T5pm = tests days in the evening session; PE-D = preexposed subgroup in the Bdifferent (D)^ context (CTA in the morning session); PE-S = preexposed subgroup in the Bsame (S)^ context (CTA in the evening session); NPE-D = nonpreexposed subgroup in the Bdifferent^ context; NPE-S = nonpreexposed subgroup in the Bsame^ context. For brevity, the morning consumption after CTA is not represented. Animals with saline restriction in the conditioning day (group with restriction) were exposed to 5 ml of saline on the conditioning day, and the remaining of the procedure was
  • 9. identical to that of animals without saline restriction on the conditioning day (group without restriction). 0 2 4 6 8 10 12 14 16 PE-D PE-S NPE-D NPE-S M ill ili te rs Fig. 1 Mean consumption of saline/water (in ml) by subgroups without restriction in the intake of the taste stimulus at different stages of the behavioral procedure, and standard deviation. BL4 = last of the
  • 10. 4 days of baseline; am = morning session; pm = evening session; PE1- 2/Wpm = Days 1 and 2 of water or preexposure to saline in the evening session; CTA = conditioning day; T1–T5pm = tests days in the evening session; PE-D = preexposed subgroup in the Bdifferent (D)^ context (CTA in the morning session); PE-S = preexposed subgroup in the Bsame (S)^ context (CTA in the evening session); NPE-D = nonpreexposed subgroup in the Bdifferent (D)^ context; NPE-S = nonpreexposed subgroup in the Bsame (S)^ context. For brevity, the morning consumption after CTA is not represented. The analysis of the main interaction shows that the PE-S, but not the PE-D, subgroup without restriction acquired latent inhibition. Learn Behav mean consumption of each group throughout the 5 test days revealed a significant effect of the interaction between preexposure, context, restriction, and test F(4, 39) = 21.72, p = .001. Newman–Keuls post hoc tests indicated no significant effect of the preexposure factor in the different context for animals without restriction (p > .05), indicating that the PE- D subgroup did not show latent inhibition. There was a sig- nificant effect of the preexposure factor in the same context (p < .05) for these animals, which indicates that the PE-S sub-
  • 11. group without restriction acquired latent inhibition. Newman– Keuls post hoc tests also showed a significant effect of the context factor in the preexposed animals without restriction, indicating that the mean consumption of the PE-S subgroup was higher than that of the PE-D subgroup (p < .05). In con- trast, there was a significant effect of the preexposure factor for animals with restriction (p < .05), which indicates that all preexposed subgroups (PE-D and PE-S) with restriction ac- quired latent inhibition. Data from Bayesian model compari- son conducted to evaluate statistical power of the results are shown in Table 2. Bayesian model comparison was imple- mented for the repeated-measures ANOVA, including test days as repeated measures (5 days) and group as between- subjects factor (with comparisons for the resulting groups from the Preexposure × Context × Restriction interaction). These comparisons support the findings of the repeated- measures ANOVA, in showing that a model including a Test Days × Group interaction provides a better fit to the data than a model with main effects but no interaction: the Bayes Factor in favor of the interaction model is given by 2.710e199/ 2.743e100 = 9.88 e98. 0 2 4 6 8 10 12
  • 12. 14 16 PE-D PE-S NPE-D NPE-S M ill ili te rs Fig. 2 Mean consumption of saline/water (in ml) by subgroups with restriction in the intake of the taste stimulus at different stages of the behavioral procedure, and standard deviation. BL4 = last of the 4 days of baseline; am = morning session; pm = evening session; PE1- 2/Wpm = Days 1 and 2 of water or preexposure to saline in the evening session; CTA = conditioning day (saline restriction to 5 ml); T1–T5pm = tests days in the evening session; PE-D = preexposed subgroup in the Bdifferent (D)^ context (CTA in the morning session); PE-S = preexposed subgroup in the Bsame (S)^ context (CTA in the evening session); NPE-D = nonpreexposed subgroup in the Bdifferent (D)^ condition; NPE-S = nonpreexposed subgroup in the Bsame (S)^ condition. The analysis of the main interaction indicates that all
  • 13. preexposed subgroups (PE-D and PE-S) with restriction acquired latent inhibition. Table 2 Bayesian model comparison for the repeated-measures ANOVA Models P(M) P(M|data) BF M BF 10 % error Null model (incl. subject) 0.250 4.092e - 74 1.228e - 73 1.000 Test 0.250 0.427 2.236 1.044e + 73 0.438 CTA 0.250 3.669e - 74 1.101e -73 0.897 0.952 Test + CTA 0.250 0.573 4.025 1.400e + 73 3.498 Null model (incl. subject) 0.200 3.690e - 200 1.476e - 199 1.000 Test 0.200 3.879e - 127 1.551e - 126 1.051e + 73 0.630 Group 0.200 1.316e - 188 5.265e - 188 3.567e + 11 0.495 Test + Group 0.200 1.012e - 99 4.048e - 99 2.743e + 100 0.565 Test + Group + Test × Group 0.200 1.000 3.952e + 99 2.710e + 199 0.889 Note. BF = Bayesian F; CTA = conditioning day; Group = resulting groups from the Preexposure × Context × Restriction interaction; M = comparison model; P = probability for models. All models include subject. Considering null model as 0, main effects as 1 and interaction effects as 2: BF1,2 = BF1,0 * BF0,2 = BF1,0/BF2,0 = 2.743e +100/2.710e + 199 = 1.012e - 99. These comparisons support the findings of the repeated-
  • 14. measures ANOVA, in showing that a model including a Test Days × Group interaction provides a better fit to the data than a model with main effects but no interaction: the Bayes factor in favor of the interaction model is given by 2.710e199/2.743e100 = 9.88 e98. Learn Behav Discussion The results of this study indicate that the latent inhibition of CTA may be disrupted by changes in the temporal context between preexposure and conditioning. This tem- poral specificity of the latent inhibition seems only to occur when animals have free access to the taste stimulus during the exposure interval, but not when the taste stim- ulus is restricted and limits the intake behavior of animals. Throughout the test days, the consumption of the PE-S subgroup in the group without restriction in the intake of the taste stimulus was higher than that of the PE-D and NPE-S subgroups. However, the consumption of the PE-D subgroup was not different from that of the NPE-D subgroup. Thus, only animals in which the temporal con- text remained the same between preexposure and condi- tioning (PE-S subgroup) showed latent inhibition in the group without restriction. Although the sample size in some subgroups was limited, statistical analyses of the data suggest that the temporal context has an effect on latent inhibition when no restriction in the intake is ap- plied during conditioning. It can be argued that, assuming the statistical power of the main interaction shown by the Bayesian model comparison, these results support the possibility that a free control of the intake during condi-
  • 15. tioning facilitates the temporal specificity of the latent inhibition in the CTA paradigm, and an external control of the intake during conditioning affects the temporal specificity of this phenomenon. Considering these limita- tions, these findings seem to be consistent with previous studies in which different contextual cues have been used. For example, latent inhibition was reduced in a study in which external cues (and other variables such as the num- ber of stimulus preexposures) were changed between preexposure and testing (De la Casa & Lubow, 2001), indicating that latent inhibition is sensitive to contextual changes in different stages of the experimental procedure. In the CTA paradigm, latent inhibition attenuation after an external context change between preexposure and condi- tioning/testing, but not between pre-exposure/condition- ing and testing, also has been described (Quintero et al., 2011). Therefore, different contextual changes (both ex- ternal and internal) applied in different stages of the be- havioral procedure may affect the acquisition of latent inhibition (Hall & Channell, 1986; Quintero et al., 2014; Revillo et al., 2014). In contrast, no temporal specificity of latent inhibition of CTA was detected when the intake of the taste stimulus was restricted during conditioning. In this condition, both the PE-S and PE-D subgroups showed latent inhibition, and the magni- tude of taste aversion over the test days was lower in these subgroups with respect to the nonpreexposed animals, regard- less of a change in the temporal context. All these findings can hardly be attributed to a contextual effect on the retrieval of learning (Killcross, 2001; Maren & Holt, 2000) because the context in the test days (evening session) should not point to the conditioned stimulus–unconditioned stimulus association in the PE-D subgroup without restriction in the intake of the taste stimulus (and therefore this subgroup should express
  • 16. latent inhibition). Rather, the effect of a change of the temporal context on latent inhibition may be due to contextual influ- ences on the association between stimuli (Hall, 1991; Kwok & Boakes, 2015; Lubow & De la Casa, 2005; Schmajuk, Lam, & Gray, 1996). On the other hand, the behavioral procedure of this study resulted in the delay between the final preexposure trial and conditioning being different for Bdifferent^ and Bsame^ sub- groups. In particular, the PE-D subgroups had a shorter delay compared with the PE-S subgroups, which could influence the results. Nevertheless, because greater temporal proximity be- tween preexposure and conditioning should strengthen the effect of preexposures (De la Casa & Lubow, 2001), greater latent inhibition should be expected in the Bdifferent^ sub- groups. The results indicate, however, that the subgroup with greater temporal proximity between preexposure and condi- tioning (PE-D subgroup) showed a disruption of latent inhibi- tion. In addition, this only happened in the subgroup without restriction in the intake of the taste stimulus during condition- ing (PE-D and no restriction), although there was also this greater temporal proximity in the restricted subgroup (PE-D and restriction). In general, we can conclude that the behavioral procedure to induce temporal specificity of latent inhibition of CTA is sensitive to the exposure conditions of the taste stimulus dur- ing conditioning. In this experiment, we used 4 days of habit- uation to the temporal context. Temporal specificity of latent inhibition of CTA under long periods of contextual habitua- tion has been described (Molero-Chamizo, 2013), but a short period seems to be a factor that hinders the contextual effect on taste aversion (Morón et al., 2002). The results of this study complement our knowledge of this phenomenon because they show that an exposure without restriction to the taste stimulus under a long period of contextual habituation facilitates the
  • 17. temporal specificity of the latent inhibition of CTA. However, an external control of the intake behavior during conditioning (a restriction in the intake of the conditioned taste stimulus) under the same period of habituation disrupts this contextual control of the latent inhibition. Thus, long periods of temporal-contextual habituation might facilitate the temporal specificity phenomenon when animals experience a determi- nate exposure of the conditioned stimulus during conditioning. Because of the three-stage procedure of the study, the effects of contextual changes on latent inhibition can be attributed to associative mechanisms occurring during conditioning, which may result in changes in the associative strength between Learn Behav stimuli (Escobar, Arcediano, & Miller, 2002; Westbrook, Jones, Bailey, & Harris, 2000). A restriction in the intake of the con- ditioned taste stimulus during conditioning might affect the temporal specificity of the latent inhibition of CTA via a mod- ulation of these associative processes. Regarding this modula- tion, one possibility is that the experience with the taste stimulus is able to modify the way in which the time of day is treated by the memory systems. It is also possible that the arousal level associated with a particular intake can alter the attention direct- ed to contextual cues such as the time of day. Because much remains unknown about the processes of latent inhibition, these results may guide further studies to elucidate the mechanisms involved in the latent inhibition of CTA and the contextual and temporal modulation of this learning. Acknowledgments This work was supported by the Ministry of
  • 18. Science and Technology (MICYT), Spain, under Grant Number BSO2002-01215, CICYT. The author declares that there is no conflict of interest. The author is grateful to M. Gallo, I. Morón, and M. A. Ballesteros, who contributed to the execution of the experiments. The author wishes to thank the anonymous reviewers for their valuable com- ments and suggestions, and Francisco Herrero Machaconses for helpful comments on earlier drafts of the manuscript. References Boakes, R. A., Elliot, M., Swinbourne, A. L., & Westbrook, R. F. (1997). Context dependency of conditioned aversions to water and sweet tastes. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes, 230, 56–57. doi:10.1037/0097-7403.23.1.56 Bonardi, C., Honey, R. C., & Hall, G. (1990). Context- specificity of conditioning in flavor-aversion learning: Extinction and blocking tests. Animal Learning & Behaviour, 18, 229–237. doi:10.3758 /BF03205280 Bouton, M. E. (1993). Context, time, and memory retrieval in the inter- ference paradigms of Pavlovian learning. Psychological Bulletin, 114, 80–99. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.114.1.80 Bouton, M. E., Westbrook, R. F., Corcoran, K. A., & Maren, S.
  • 19. (2006). Contextual and temporal modulations of extinction: Behavioral and biological mechanisms. Biological Psychiatry, 60, 352–360. doi:10.1016/j.biopsych.2005.12 De la Casa, L. G., & Lubow, R. E. (1995). Latent inhibition in condi- tioned taste aversion: The roles of stimulus frequency and duration and the amount of fluid ingested during preexposure. Neurobiology of Learning and Memory, 64, 125–132. doi:10.1006 /nlme.1995.1051 De la Casa, L. G., & Lubow, R. E. (2001). Latent inhibition with a response time measure from a within-subject design: Effects of num- ber of preexposures, masking task, context change, and delay. Neuropsychology, 15, 244–253. doi:10.1037/0894- 4105.15.2.244 Escobar, M., Arcediano, F., & Miller, R. R. (2002). Latent inhibition and contextual associations. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes, 28, 123–136. doi:10.1037/0097- 7403.28.2.123 González, F., Garcia-Burgos, D., & Hall, G. (2012). Associative and occasion-setting properties of contextual cues in flavor-nutrient learning in rats. Appetite, 59, 898–904. doi:10.1016/j. appet.2012.09.004 González, F., Morillas, E., & Hall, G. (2015). Latent inhibition
  • 20. in flavor- preference conditioning: Effects of motivational state and the nature of the reinforcer. Learning & Behavior, 43(4), 376–383. doi:10.3758 /s13420-015-0185-y Hall, G. (1991). Perceptual and associative learning. Oxford, UK: Clarendon Press. Hall, G., & Channell, S. (1986). Context specificity of latent inhibition in taste aversion learning. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 38B, 121–139. Killcross, S. (2001). Loss of latent inhibition in conditioned taste aversion following exposure to a novel flavour before test. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 54B, 271–288. doi:10.1080 /713932758 Kwok, D. W., & Boakes, R. A. (2015). Proximal, but not distal, pre- exposure reduces serial overshadowing in one-trial taste aversion learning. Behavioural Processes, 118, 111–114. doi:10.1016/j. beproc.2015.06.006 Lubow, R. E., & De la Casa, L. G. (2005). There is a time and a place for everything: Bidirectional modulations of latent inhibition by time- induced context differentiation. Psychonomic Bulletin &
  • 21. Review, 12(5), 806–821. doi:10.3758/BF03196774 Lukoyanov, N. V., Pereira, P. A., Mesquita, R. M., & Andrade, J. P. (2002). Restricted feeding facilitates time-place learning in adult rats. Behavioural Brain Research, 134, 283–290. doi:10.1016 /S0166-4328(02)00036-0 Manrique, T., Molero, A., Ballesteros, M. A., Morón, I., Gallo, M., & Fenton, A. (2004). Time of day-dependent latent inhibition of con- ditioned taste aversions in rats. Neurobiology of Learning and Memory, 82, 77–80. doi:10.1016/j.nlm.2004.04.003 Manrique, T., Gámiz, F., Morón, I., Ballesteros, M. A., & Gallo, M. (2009). Peculiar modulation of taste aversion learning by the time of day in developing rats. Developmental Psychobiology, 51, 147– 157. doi:10.1002/dev.20354 Maren, S., & Holt, W. (2000). The hippocampus and contextual memory retrieval in Pavlovian conditioning. Behavioural Brain Research, 110, 97–108. doi:10.1016/S0166-4328(99)00188-6 Molero, A., Morón, I., Ballesteros, M. A., Manrique, T., Fenton, A., & Gallo, M. (2005). Hippocampus, temporal context and taste memo- ries. Chemical Senses, 30, 160–161. doi:10.1093/chemse/bjh163
  • 22. Molero-Chamizo, A. (2013). Excitotoxic lesion of the hippocampus of Wistar rats disrupts the circadian control of the latent inhibition of taste aversion learning. Brain Research, 1533, 105–113. doi:10.1016/j.brainres.2013.08.030 Morón, I., Manrique, T., Molero, A., Ballesteros, M. A., Gallo, M., & Fenton, A. (2002). The contextual modulation of conditioned taste aversions by the physical environment and time of day is similar. Learning & Memory, 9, 18–223. doi:10.1101/lm.52202 Pearce, J. M., & Bouton, M. E. (2001). Theories of associative learning in animals. Annual Review of Psychology, 52, 111–139. doi:10.1146 /annurev.psych.52.1.111 Quintero, E., Díaz, E., Vargas, J. P., Schmajuk, N., López, J. C., & De la Casa, L. G. (2011). Effects of context novelty vs. familiarity on latent inhibition with a conditioned taste aversion procedure. Behavioural Processes, 86, 242–249. doi:10.1016/j. beproc.2010.12.011 Quintero, E., Vargas, J. P., Diaz, E., Escarabajal, M. D., Carrasco, M., & López, J. C. (2014). c-Fos positive nucleus reveals that contextual specificity of latent inhibition is dependent of insular cortex. Brain R e s e a rc h B u l l e t i n , 1 0 8 , 7 4 – 7 9 . d o i : 1 0 . 1 0 1
  • 23. 6 / j . brainresbull.2014.08.008 Revillo, D. A., Gaztañaga, M., Aranda, E., Paglini, M. G., Chotro, M. G., & Arias, C. (2014). Context-dependent latent inhibition in preweanling rats. Developmental Psychobiology, 56(7), 1507– 1517. doi:10.1002/dev.21236 Rosas, J. M., & Bouton, M. E. (1997). Renewal of a conditioned taste aversion upon return to the conditioning context after extinction in another one. Learning and Motivation, 28, 216–229. doi:10.1006 /lmot.1996.0960 Learn Behav http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0097-7403.23.1.56 http://dx.doi.org/10.3758/BF03205280 http://dx.doi.org/10.3758/BF03205280 http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.114.1.80 http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsych.2005.12 http://dx.doi.org/10.1006/nlme.1995.1051 http://dx.doi.org/10.1006/nlme.1995.1051 http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0894-4105.15.2.244 http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0097-7403.28.2.123 http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0097-7403.28.2.123 http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.appet.2012.09.004 http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.appet.2012.09.004 http://dx.doi.org/10.3758/s13420-015-0185-y http://dx.doi.org/10.3758/s13420-015-0185-y http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/713932758 http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/713932758 http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.beproc.2015.06.006
  • 24. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.beproc.2015.06.006 http://dx.doi.org/10.3758/BF03196774 http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0166-4328(02)00036-0 http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0166-4328(02)00036-0 http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.nlm.2004.04.003 http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/dev.20354 http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0166-4328(99)00188-6 http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/chemse/bjh163 http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.brainres.2013.08.030 http://dx.doi.org/10.1101/lm.52202 http://dx.doi.org/10.1146/annurev.psych.52.1.111 http://dx.doi.org/10.1146/annurev.psych.52.1.111 http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.beproc.2010.12.011 http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.beproc.2010.12.011 http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.brainresbull.2014.08.008 http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.brainresbull.2014.08.008 http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/dev.21236 http://dx.doi.org/10.1006/lmot.1996.0960 http://dx.doi.org/10.1006/lmot.1996.0960 Schmajuk, N. A., Lam, Y. W., & Gray, J. A. (1996). Latent inhibition: A neural network approach. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes, 22, 321–349. doi:10.1037/0097- 7403.22.3.321 Westbrook, R. F., Jones, M. L., Bailey, G. K., & Harris, J. A. (2000). Contextual control over conditioning responding in a latent inhibi- tion paradigm. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes, 26, 157–173. doi:10.1037/0097- 7403.26.2.157 Learn Behav