SlideShare a Scribd company logo
1 of 359
PAGE
Running head: School Readiness and Later Achievement
1
School Readiness and Later Achievement
School Name
Course:
Date: 13th March 2013
School Readiness and Later Achievement
The linkages between school-entry academic, attention, and
socioemotional skills and later school reading and math
achievement suggest that math skills have the greatest
predictive power, followed by reading and then attention skills
(Japel, 2007). In contrast, socioemotional behaviors, including
internalizing and externalizing problems and social skills, are
insignificant predictors of later academic performance, even
among children with relatively high levels of problem behavior
(Japel, 2007). These patterns are usually associated with boys
and girls from children from low and high socioeconomic
backgrounds.
Assessing “Research-Based” Curricula
Most early childhood programs are being asked to choose
curricula that are “research based.” This requirement is the
result of increased attention to children’s academic needs as
they enter kindergarten, and has the potential of improving our
delivery of curricula to young children (Japel, 2007). However,
the meaning of “research based” has not been delineated.
Therefore, most publishers of curricula for young children have
adopted the language, and identify their programs as “research
based.” A careful analysis of the underlying research of three
important math curricula could help practitioners make more
informed choices. This analysis will also provide a list of
criteria for selection of other early childhood curricula, which
will require practitioners to take a brief look at the type of
research that purports to provide the research base for the
curricula (Japel, 2007).
For Example, the Pre-K Mathematics is a scripted math program
for four year olds. Its primary goal has been to close the gap in
math achievement between low-income children and middle
class children. Much research has documented this gap, which
exists as children enter school and grows as children progress
through school. Such kind of research demonstrates years of
careful research that their curriculum can start to close this gap.
Pre-K Mathematics has a clearly delineated scope and sequence.
The scope and sequence is carefully connected to the
development of mathematical concepts that are needed in formal
math education in elementary school, concepts that low-income
children often lack. The lessons are designed to be presented to
very small groups of children for short periods of time. The
lessons are supported with daily math activities that are
plentiful in the children’s environment. These researchers have
many years’ experience and long list of published research that
document achievement gaps, math concept development, and
demonstration projects in math achievement.
Conclusion
The research-based curricula described here have the potential
to provide preschoolers with a math curriculum that can prepare
them for the more structured lessons of elementary school. Most
professionals agree that a curriculum is only the beginning of
the process. Children also need teachers who are sensitive,
responsive, and knowledgeable about development and the
concepts that children need to be successful in kindergarten.
Therefore, teachers who have a deep gratitude for
developmentally appropriate practices can be able to employ
these curricula to the advantage of the children in their
classrooms.
References
Japel, Crista (2007). School Readiness and Later Achievement.
Developmental Psychology., Vol. 43, No. 6, 1428 –1446
PAGE
Running head: Assessing “Research-Based” Curricula
1
Assessing “Research-Based” Curricula
School Name
Course:
Date: 13th March 2013
Assessing “Research-Based” Curricula
The linkages between school-entry academic, attention, and
socioemotional skills and later school reading and math
achievement suggest that math skills have the greatest
predictive power, followed by reading and then attention skills
(Japel, 2007). In contrast, socioemotional behaviors, including
internalizing and externalizing problems and social skills, are
insignificant predictors of later academic performance, even
among children with relatively high levels of problem behavior
(Japel, 2007). These patterns are usually associated with boys
and girls from children from low and high socioeconomic
backgrounds.
Assessing “Research-Based” Curricula
Most early childhood programs are being asked to choose
curricula that are “research based.” This requirement is the
result of increased attention to children’s academic needs as
they enter kindergarten, and has the potential of improving our
delivery of curricula to young children (Japel, 2007). However,
the meaning of “research based” has not been delineated.
Therefore, most publishers of curricula for young children have
adopted the language, and identify their programs as “research
based.” A careful analysis of the underlying research of three
important math curricula could help practitioners make more
informed choices. This analysis will also provide a list of
criteria for selection of other early childhood curricula, which
will require practitioners to take a brief look at the type of
research that purports to provide the research base for the
curricula (Japel, 2007).
Pre-K Mathematics (Klein, A., Starkey, P., and Ramirez, M.,
2003) is a scripted math program for four year olds. Its primary
goal has been to close the gap in math achievement between
low-income children and middle class children. Much research
has documented this gap, which exists as children enter school
and grows as children progress through school. These
researchers have demonstrated through several years of careful
research that their curriculum can begin to close this gap
(Starkey, Klein, and Wakeley, 2004). Pre-K Mathematics has a
clearly delineated scope and sequence. The scope and sequence
is carefully connected to the development of mathematical
concepts that are needed in formal math education in elementary
school, concepts that low-income children often lack. The
lessons are designed to be presented to very small groups of
children for short periods of time. The lessons are supported
with daily math activities that are plentiful in the children’s
environment. These researchers have many years’ experience
and long list of published research that document achievement
gaps, math concept development, and demonstration projects in
math achievement. Starkey, Klein and Wakeley (2004) have
field-tested this curriculum for at least five years. The
classroom teachers in their studies have had continuous
training, close supervision, and much success with children. In
one year of instruction children using the Pre-K Mathematics
Program have nearly closed the conceptual gap between
themselves and middle class children entering kindergarten
(Starkey, Klein and Wakeley, 2004).
Conclusion
The research-based curricula described here have the potential
to provide preschoolers with a math curriculum that can prepare
them for the more structured lessons of elementary school. Most
professionals agree that a curriculum is only the beginning of
the process. Children also need teachers who are sensitive,
responsive, and knowledgeable about development and the
concepts that children need to be successful in kindergarten.
Teachers who have a deep appreciation for developmentally
appropriate practices will be able to employ these curricula to
the advantage of the children in their classes (Bredekamp &
Copple, 1997).
References
Klein, A., Starkey, P., and Ramirez, A. (2003). Pre-K
Mathematics Curriculum. Glendale, Il:
ScottForesman.
Starkey, P., Klein, A. and Wakeley, D. (2004). Enahancing
young children’s mathematical
knowledge through a pre-kindergarten mathematics
intervention. Early childhood
research Quarterly, 19, 99-120.
Japel, Crista (2007). School Readiness and Later Achievement.
Developmental Psychology., Vol. 43, No. 6, 1428 –1446
PAGE
Running head: TYPE ABBREVIATED TITLE HERE
1
Title of the Research Proposal in Full Goes Here
Group Names Go Here
Southwestern Oregon Community College
HDFS 247 Preschool Child Development
Date
Title of the Paper
Double space, indent each paragraph l ½ inch, and start typing.
Your introduction does not have a heading – just the title of the
paper.
You will need to write a thesis statement in your introduction,
the main idea of a paper. Click here to visit a website about
thesis statements.
Once you’ve developed the thesis, you can then begin
composing your introduction. Click here for information on
writing introductions.
Part One: Hypothesis
This will be the beginning of the body of the essay. Even
though it has a new heading, you want to make sure you link
this to your previous section so your reader can follow and
understand. Remember to make sure your first sentence in each
paragraph both transitions from your previous paragraph and
summarizes the main point in your paragraph. Stick to one
topic per paragraph, and avoid long paragraphs so you can hold
the reader’s attention. A paragraph should be a minimum of
three sentences. In this section you will address: What is your
hypothesis? Research questions you want to answer?
LOGICAL RELATIONSHIP
TRANSITIONAL EXPRESSION
Similarity
also, in the same way, just as … so too, likewise, similarly
Exception/Contrast
but, however, in spite of, on the one hand … on the other hand,
nevertheless, nonetheless, notwithstanding, in contrast, on the
contrary, still, yet
Sequence/Order
first, second, third, … next, then, finally
Time
after, afterward, at last, before, currently, during, earlier,
immediately, later, meanwhile, now, recently, simultaneously,
subsequently, then
Example
for example, for instance, namely, specifically, to illustrate
Emphasis
even, indeed, in fact, of course, truly
Place/Position
above, adjacent, below, beyond, here, in front, in back, nearby,
there
Cause and Effect
accordingly, consequently, hence, so, therefore, thus
Additional Support or Evidence
additionally, again, also, and, as well, besides, equally
important, further, furthermore, in addition, moreover, then
Conclusion / summary
finally, in a word, in brief, briefly, in conclusion, in the end, in
the final analysis, on the whole, thus, to conclude, to
summarize, in sum, to sum up, in summary
From http://writingcenter.unc.edu/handouts/transitions/
Next section Here, answering the next question or questions
from the list
It I OK to combine questions from the list below into sections,
but just be sure that each section heading clearly labels what the
section contains. For Example, it might say: Type of
Resaearch and Population. The next section could include
Sample, Groups, etc. Here is the list of what needs to be
included in your proposal
What is your hypothesis? Research questions you want to
answer?
What type of research will you use to answer the research
questions? (Quantitative or qualitative? Experiment, case
study, longitudinal, etc.?)
What is your population?
What is your sample?
Define your experimental group.
Define your control group.
How will you assign participants to groups?
What is your independent variable?
What is your dependent variable?
How will you collect data?
How will you operationalize these terms?
Are there any confounds or nuisance variables operating?
What ethical concerns may be present to consider in doing this
study?
Another Heading….
And so forth until the conclusion.
Conclusion
Your conclusion section should recap the major points you have
made in your work. Click here for information on writing a
conclusion.
References
Berger, R., Miller, A., Seifer, R., Cares, S., & Lebourgeois, M.
(2012). Acute sleep restriction effects on emotion responses in
30- to 36-month-old children. Journal Of Sleep Research,
21(3), 235-246. doi:10.1111/j.1365-2869.2011.00962.x
Feak, C. and Swales, J. (2009). Telling a research story:
Writing a literature review. Ann Arbor, MI: University of
Michigan Press.
Kavanagh, K., Absalom, K., Beil, W., & Schliessmann, L.
(2008). Connecting and becoming culturally competent: A
Lakota example. Advances in Nursing Science, 21, 9-31.
Retrieved March 26, 2012 from ProQuest/Nursing Journals
database.
Learn how to write a review of literature. (n.d.). Retrieved
September 12, 2012 from The Writer’s Handbook Website. site:
http://writing.wisc.edu/Handbook/ReviewofLiterature.html
"Review." The Oxford Pocket Dictionary of Current English.
2009. Retrieved September 18, 2012 from Encyclopedia.com:
http://www.encyclopedia.com/doc/1O999-review.html
Minnesota School Readiness Study:
Developmental Assessment at Kindergarten Entrance
Fall 2010
Acknowledgements
Minnesota School Readiness Study:
Developmental Assessment at Kindergarten Entrance
The Minnesota School Readiness Study: Developmental
Assessment at Kindergarten
Entrance Fall 2010 was planned, implemented, and the report
prepared by the Minnesota
Department of Education (MDE).
Special thanks to the 108 elementary schools involved in the
study, their principals,
kindergarten teachers, support staff and superintendents. The
observation and collection
of developmental information by teachers on kindergarten
children in the classroom was
essential to the study and is much appreciated.
All analyses in this report were conducted by the Human Capital
Research Collaborative
(HCRC), a partnership between the University of Minnesota
and the Federal Reserve
Bank of Minneapolis.
For more information, contact Avisia Whiteman at
[email protected] or
651-582-8329 or Eileen Nelson at [email protected] or 651-
582-8464. Ama nda
Varley, University of Minnesota Graduate School intern, also
provided significant
support to t he project.
Date of Report: November 2011
mailto:[email protected]
mailto:[email protected]
Background
Minnesota School Readiness Study: Developmental
Assessment at Kindergarten Entrance - Fall 2010
Research has shown, and continues to show, that t here is a
critical relationship between
early childhood experiences, school success, and positive life-
long outcomes. This
research has been a focal point for many states as they strive to
reduce the growing
achievement gap between less advantaged students and their
same-aged peers in the
educational system.
With no systematic process in place to assess children’s
school readiness, the Min nesota Department of Education
(MDE) in 2002 initiated a series of three yearly studies
focused on obtaining a picture of the school readiness of a
representative sample of Minnesota entering
kindergartners. Also, the series of studies was to evaluate
changes in the percentage of children fully prepared for
school at kindergarten entrance. The studies were well-
received by the public, and during the 2006 Minnesota
state legislative session, funding was appropriated for the
study to be continued on an annual basis.
This report describes findings from the assessment of
school readiness usin g a representative sample of children
entering kindergarten in Minnesota in Fall 2010. The data
provide a picture of the ratings of entering kindergartners
across five domains of child
development. The study provides information on school
readiness for parents; school
teachers and administrators; early childhood education and care
teachers, pr oviders and
administrators; policymakers; and the public.
Definition of School Readiness
For purposes of the study, “school readiness” is defined as the
skills, knowledge,
behaviors and accomplishments that children should know and
be able to do as they enter
kindergarten in the following areas of child development:
physical development; the arts;
personal and social development; language and literacy; and
mathematical thinking.
Assessing School Readiness
The study is designed to capture a picture of the readiness of
Minnesota children as they
enter kindergarten and track readiness trends over time. To
ensure that results are reliable
and can be generalized to the entire population of Minnesota
kindergartners, the study
uses a 10 percent sample of schools with entering
kindergartners. This sample size
generates data from approximately 6,000 kindergartners
annually.
Minnesota School Readiness Study 2010
The study uses the Work Sampling System (WSS®), a
developmentally appropriate,
standards-based observational assessment that allows children
to demonstrate their
knowledge and skills in various ways and across
developmental domains.
WSS® is aligned with the state’s early learning standards,
Minnesota Early Childhood
Indicators of Progress, and the K-12 Academic Standards. S ee
Appendix A.
Each domain and developmental indicator within the WSS®
Developmental Checklist
includes expected behaviors for children at that age or grade
level. For each indicator,
teachers used the following guidelines to rate the child's
performance:
Proficient — indicating that the child can reliably and
consistently demonstrate the skill,
knowledge, behavior or accomplishment represented by the
performance indicator.
In Process — indicating that the skill, knowledge, behavior
or accomplishment
represented by the indicator are intermittent or emergent, and
are not demonstrated
reliably or consistently.
Not Yet — indicating that the child cannot perform the
indicator (i.e., the performance
indicator represents a skill, knowledge, behavior or
accomplishment not yet acquired).
Because children’s rate of development is variable, the study
assesses children’s
proficiency within and across the developmental domains.
Rubrics for each rating level were distributed to teachers at the
start of the study. The
rubrics, provided by the publisher and revised in 2009, provide
additional detail for each
indicator for a Not Yet, In Process or Proficient rating.
Partnership with the Human Capital Research Collaborative
Throughout 2010, MDE worked in
partnership with the Human Capital
Research Collaborative (HCRC) to better
understand the relationship between
kindergarten entry results and future
academic achievement. HCRC is a
partnership of the University of Minnesota
and the Federal Reserve Bank of
Minneapolis. It was important to assess the
predictive validity of Minnesota’s school
readiness indicators and determine the
degree to which the School Readiness Study checklist added
additional weight beyond
demographics towards the likelihood of passing Grade 3 MCAs.
Work was conducted to
determine which type of measure from the checklist best
predicted Grade 3 MCA results.
Findings centered on children who reach 75 percent of the total
possible points on the
checklist having a greater likelihood in passing Grade 3 MCAs.
W hile national research
2
Minnesota School Readiness Study 2010
over decades has pointed to the relationship between early
experiences and academic
success, it is instructive to have a reference standard within the
existing checklist.
Based on data from Kindergarten cohorts in 2003, 2004, and
2006 who had available
achievement test scores in third grade or information on
remedial education, HCRC
found that the School Readiness Study checklist, including the
75 percent standard,
significantly and consistently predicted third-grade MCA
reading and math test scores
and the need for school remedial services (special education or
grade retention) above
and beyond the influence of child and family background
characteristics. The strength of
prediction was consistent across a range of child and family
characteristics (e.g., family
income, gender, and race/ethnicity). For more information on
this report, go to:
http://www.humancapitalrc.org/mn_school_readiness_indicators
.pdf
2010 Recr uitment
MDE contacted superintendents, principals and teachers
beginning mid-winter to build
the sample for the coming fall. A list of all public schools
with kindergartners as of
October 1 the previous year was compiled. The list was
divided into eight strata which
accounts for proximity to population centers and population
density and separated
charter and magnet schools. A representative sample of
schools within each strata was
invited to participate via a mailed invitation to the
superintendent and principal of each
site. Follow-up calls were made to each site to answer
questions. In 2010, 55 percent
(495/900) of all schools were invited to participate.
Approximately 24 pe rcent (120/495)
of those invited responded positively to the initial invitation.
In late spring, schools are
selected to be released from the cohort when student counts
exceed the sample amount.
In 2010, no s chools were released. By November, 12 pe rcent
of all elementary schools
(108/900) submitted child-level data.
The following table shows the total kindergarten population
compared to the sample
population. The sample seeks to be representative of all public
schools including charters
and magnets across federally mandated demographic categories.
(See Table 1.)
3
http://www.humancapitalrc.org/mn_school_readiness_indicators
.pdf
Table 1 - Kindergarten Population Compared to the
Sample
State
Study
Kindergarten
Sample
Enrollment
American Indian 2.3% 5.4%
Asian 7.1% 5.6%
Hispanic 8.5% 7.0%
Black 10.9% 8.8%
White 71.1% 71.7%
Limited English Proficiency 11.7% 6%
Special Education 10.4% 7%
2010 Res ults
A total of 5,838 kinder gartners from 108 se lected elementary
schools across the state
were included in the Fall 2010 cohort. This reflects 9.2 pe rcent
of the entering
kindergartners for the 2010-2011 sc hool year. Of
these children, 5,654 students had all WSS
indicators completed for analysis. For the Fall of
2010, 60 percent of Minnesota’s kindergartners
reached the 75 percent standard. For selected
categories, see Chart 1. The selected categories
in Chart 1 are based on the statistically
significant categories from the regression. The
regression is discussed in more detail on page 9.
The domain rankings by proficiency for the 2010 cohort are
reordered with previous
years of the study. (See Table 2 and Chart 2.) Physical
Development had the highest
percentage of children assessed Proficient on average, followed
in order by Language &
Literacy; The Arts; Personal and Social Development and
Mathematical Thinking.
Indicator order within each domain changed only slightly from
2009 in Mathematical
Thinking; Personal and Social Development and Language and
Literacy. ( See Table 3.)
Proficiency by domain is defined as the average percent
proficient across indicators
within each domain.
It is important to note that while there are trends towards
increases in estimates of
Proficient results, the trends are not outside the margin of
error. Also, the existing data
set does not allow for examination of potential reasons for
shifts.
Minnesota School Readiness Study 2010
4
Table 2 - Results By Domain
Margin
Domain/Result Proficient of Error
Physical Development 70%
2.7%
Language & Literacy 59% 2.9%
The Arts 56% 2.9%
Personal & Social
Development 56% 2.9%
Mathematical Thinking 52% 2.9%
Note categories are adjusted for stratified cluster sampling.
75 Percent Standard 60% 2.9%
Chart 1 – Percent of Students Reaching 75 Percent Standard by
Selected Sub-Cate
Minnesota School Readiness Study 2010
The 75 percent standard is defined as the percent reaching at
least 75
percent of the possible points on the checklist, a predictor of
grade 3
MCAs.
gories
5
Minnesota School Readiness Study 2010
Table 3 Domain & Indicator Results
Ranked by Proficiency
Percent
Physical Development Proficient
Physical Development
Average Score Summary 70%
Performs some self-care tasks independently. 73%
Coordinates movements to perform simple tasks. 71%
Uses eye-hand coordination to perform tasks. 67%
The Arts
The Arts Domain
Average Score Summary 56%
Participates in group music experiences. 63%
Participates in creative movement, dance and drama. 60%
Uses a variety of art materials for tactile experience and
exploration. 59%
Responds to artistic creations or events. 56%
Personal and Social Development
Personal and Social Development Domain
Average Score Summary 56%
Interacts easily with familiar adults. 63%
Shows eagerness and curiosity as a learner. 62%
Interacts easily with one or more children. 62%
Shows empathy and caring for others. 60%
Follows simple classroom rules and routines. 58%
Manages transitions. 57%
Shows some self-direction. 56%
Seeks adult help when needed to resolve conflicts. 53%
Attends to tasks and seeks help when encountering a
problem. 52%
Approaches tasks with flexibility and inventiveness. 50%
-
6
Minnesota School Readiness Study 2010
Language and Literacy
Language and Literacy Domain Average Score Summary 59%
Shows appreciation for books and reading. 66%
Speaks clearly enough to be understood without contextual
clues. 65%
Shows beginning understanding of concepts about print.
61%
Comprehends and responds to stories read aloud. 60%
Begins to develop knowledge about letters. 60%
Gains meaning by listening. 59%
Represents ideas and stories through pictures, dictation and
play. 57%
Follows two- or three-step directions. 55%
Uses expanded vocabulary and language arts for a variety
of purposes. 52%
Uses letter-like shapes, symbols and letters to convey
meaning. 52%
Demonstrates phonological awareness. 21%
Mathematical Thinking
Mathematical Thinking Domain Average Score Summary 52%
Begins to recognize and describe the attributes of shapes.
60%
Shows beginning understanding of number and quantity.
58%
Shows understanding of and uses several positional words.
57%
Begins to use simple strategies to solve mathematical
problems. 50%
7
Chart 2 – Proficiency Rates by Domain
Minnesota School Readiness Study 2010
Descriptive Results
The 2010 c ohort was also analyzed for descriptive results
based on single demographic
categories. For example, to report under the income charts, all
parents are included in the
under 100 percent Federal Poverty Guidelines grouping
without controlling for education
status, home language or race/ethnicity. The family survey asks
parents to select all
race/ethnicity categories that are relevant for their child. If
multiple categories are
selected, the child will be represented in the
appropriate categories. A similar process was
followed for primary home languages. The
percent within each demographic category
reaching the 75 percent standard are reported in
Appendix B.
Family Survey Results
As part of the study process, families are asked to
complete a voluntary survey. This information is
8
Minnesota School Readiness Study 2010
combined with the Work Sampling System® checklist results
(see Appendix C). I n total,
4,932 pa rents (84 p ercent) completed the survey. Of this
group, 4,695 responses (95
percent) were usable for analysis. (A parent survey may not
be usable for analysis
because it was incomplete, the student information strip was
incomplete or the survey
lacked coordinating information in Work Sampling Online
(WSO).) After matching the
family survey data with Work Sampling Online results, 4,168 re
cords remained for
regression analysis. This is 85 pe rcent of all submitted parent
surveys and 89 pe rcent of
those available to match.
Logistic Regression Results
The analysis of the data included examining how a particular
child or family
characteristic may affect that child’s ratings while controlling
for the effects of other
demographic variables with which it may be confounded (e.g.,
a child from a family with
a lower household income is more likely to have a parent with
a lower education level).
The result of reaching the 75 percent proficiency standard
across all domains was
analyzed with respect to the demographic characteristics of
gender, parent education
level, household income, primary home language and race and
ethnicity collected from
parent surveys. (See Table 4 and Appendix D.) For
comparison to previous years, see
Appendix E.
All 2010 a nalyses reported involved statistical estimation
procedures that reflect the
stratified cluster sampling design used (with school as the
primary sampling unit), and
include correction for finite population sampling. Observations
within each stratum were
weighted to reflect the statewide proportion of students in the
stratum.
Table 4 - Statistically Significant Factors in Reaching the 75
Percent Standard
Household Income
Parent Education Level
Gender
Note: predictors significant at p < .05
Household Income
The odds of reaching the 75 percent standard for a student
whose household income was
at or above 400 percent of the Federal Poverty Guidelines
(FPG) were more than one and
a half times as great as compared to a
student whose household income was less
than 250 pe rcent FPG when holding all
other variables constant. The odds of
reaching the 75 percent standard for a
student whose household income was 250
400 percent FPG are nearly one and half
times as great as compared to a student
whose household income is up to 250
-
9
percent FPG. This result is statistically
significant.
Parent Education Level
Parent education level was found to be
statistically significant in reaching the 75 percent
standard. Students whose parents have a high
school degree a re twice as likely to reach the 75
percent standard as compared to students whose parents have
less than a high school
degree. Students with parents who have a an Associate degree,
Bachelor or graduate
degree are approximately one and a half ti mes as likely to
reach the 75 percent standard
as compared to students whose parents who have a high school
diploma or GED.
Primary Home Language
Primary home language was not found to be statistically
significant in reaching the 75
percent standard when holding all other variables constant.
Race and Ethnicity
Parent-report of race and ethnicity was not a statistically
significant factor in reaching the
75 percent standard when holding all other variables constant.
Minority status as an
overall category was marginally significant.
Gender
Gender continues to be a statistically significant factor. The
odds of reaching the 75
percent standard for females were up to one and a third times g
reater, as compared to
males.
Principal and Teacher Surveys
As in previous years, the success of the study rested with the
willingness of school
principals and kindergarten teachers to participate.
Participating school principals and
kindergarten teachers w ere again given surveys to complete
regarding their decision to
participate, barriers to participation, and the associated
workload and benefits. The
following information is based upon the response of 35 pr
incipals (108 possible
responses or 32 p ercent) and 165 kinder garten teachers (288
potential responses or 57
percent).
Principal Perspectives
Principals reported two primary benefits of participating in the
study: helping influence
statewide policy (100 percent) and gaining information about
where students are at the
beginning of the school year (69 percent). Reported barriers
for participation included
Minnesota School Readiness Study 2010
10
adding to existing teacher workloads (63 p ercent). Principals
balanced the need of the
project with competing needs by having more experienced
teachers mentor newer
teachers, paying teachers for their extra time and shifting staff
development resources.
Principals will use the information gained from the study to
identify children’s needs
earlier in the year (50 pe rcent). Principals using Work
Sampling Online (WSO) reported
that the online training was easy to access. A m ajority of
principals (84 pe rcent) reported
receiving the appropriate amount of information prior to and
during their participation.
Teacher Perspectives
A vast majority of teachers (86 pe rcent) responded that
contributing to a study that will
influence statewide early childhood policy was of benefit to
them. The same percent
reported receiving a $200 stipend as a benefit. Others reported
the benefit of gaining
information about where students are at the beginning of the
school year (68 percent). A
little over one-third of the teachers reported that collecting the
parent surveys was a
challenge for them (37 percent). On a follow-up question, 80 pe
rcent responded that they
were able to implement the parent survey with great to
moderate ease. Thirty-one percent
had no challenges implementing the study. Teachers reported
that the study took a
minimal (12 pe rcent) to average (72 pe rcent)
amount of work for a special project.
Teachers report planning to use the
information to identify children’s needs
earlier in the year (46 pe rcent) and helping
them target instruction (47 percent).
Regarding the use of technology, 96 percent
report great to moderate ease in accessing
WSO and the Web-based orientation.
Teachers report receiving adequate levels of information prior
to (95 pe rcent) and during
the study (98 p ercent). They also report receiving adequate
support from MDE (92
percent) throughout the study period. Currently, 28 percent of
teachers use Work
Sampling in their schools, 35 pe rcent report planning to
continue using WSO after the
study period. Approximately one-third of all teachers report
using locally designed
assessment tools in additional to the Work Sampling System®.
Limitations
Because children develop and grow along a continuum but at
varied ra tes, the goal of the
study is to assess children’s proficiency within and across these
developmental domains
over time and not establish whether or not children,
individually or in small groups, are
ready for school with the use of a “ready” or “not ready”
score. Nor is the study’s goal to
provide information on the history or the future of an
individual student.
Recent national reports have discussed the complexities in the
development of state-level
accountability systems. Taking Stock: Assessing and Improving
Early Childhood
Minnesota School Readiness Study 2010
11
Learning and Program Quality (2007) and The National
Academy of Science report Early
Childhood Assessment: Why, What and How? (2008) details
the necessary steps to use
authentic assessment results, also referred to as instructional
assessments, in
accountability initiatives. The National Academy of Science
reports that even in upper
grades, e xtreme caution is needed in relying exclusively on
child assessment and that for
children birth to five “even more extreme caution is needed.”
Discussion
In line with national research, family household income and
parent education was found
to be predictive in reaching the 75 percent standard.
Race/Ethnicity as an overall category
was marginally significant but not significant for individual
groups and G ender is
predictive in reaching the 75 percent standard.
Recommendations
1. Continue to work toward improving the quality of early
childhood education and care
programs in Minnesota by emphasizing the importance of
teacher-child interactions and
content-driven, intentional curriculum and instruction. Build on
the 10 Essential Elements
of Effective Early Childhood Programs and Governor Dayton’s
7-Point Plan for
Achieving Excellence.
2. Target intervention strategies to children assessed as Not
Proficient, especially in the
areas of literacy and mathematics. Implement compensatory
strategies as soon as a
child’s need is identified. Work with the Governor’s Early
Learning Council to identify
staged implementation strategies to maximize resources.
3. Support more children in their efforts to read well by third
grade by focusing state
policies on young children’s language and literacy
development.
4. Strengthen teacher-child interactions to improve
learning by implementing professional development
that includes teacher observation and development.
5. Individualize instruction by using assessment
information to design classroom experiences.
6. Use child progress assessment information when
teachers talk with parents about setting goals for
children.
7. Increase collaborations from early childhood
through Grade 3 at the teacher, director, principal and
superintendent levels. Identify district and state
policy opportunities to promote this work.
12
Minnesota School Readiness Study 2010
8. Consider collecting information on prior early care and
education experiences and
incorporating that information into the early childhood
longitudinal data system. Results
from the 2010 prior experience data pilot need to be considered
when planning for the
future.
Early Learning Council
T he Early Childhood Advisory Council (ECAC), seated from
December 2008 to January
2011, looked to the a nnual School Readiness study as one
measure of state progress on
early learning. The Council was reauthorized and renamed the
Early Learning Council by
Governor Dayton’s Executive Order 11-05. Read the Executive
Order on the Governor’s
website. The newly formed Early Learning Council (ELC)
may continue to look to the
results of the study to guide school readiness policy.
Minnesota School Readiness Study 2010
13
http://mn.gov/governor/multimedia/pdf/Executive-Order-11-
05.pdf
http://mn.gov/governor/multimedia/pdf/Executive-Order-11-
05.pdf
Minnesota School Readiness Study 2010
For further reading
Campbell, F. A., Ramey, C. T., Pungello, E., Sparling, J.,
& Miller-Johnson, S. (2002).Early childhood
education: Young adult outcomes from the Abecedarian
project. Applied Developmental Science, 6(1), 42
57.
Coley, R. J. (2002). An uneven start: Indicators of
inequality in school readiness. Princeton, NJ:
Educational Testing Service.
Dichtelmiller, M. L., Jablon, J. R., Marsden, D. B., &
Meisels, S. J. (2001). Preschool-4 developmental
guidelines (4th Ed.). New York: Rebus.
Gershoff, E. (November 2003). Living at the edge research
brief no.4: Low income and the development of
America’s kindergartners. New York: National Center for
Children in Poverty.
Meisels, S.J. & Atkins-Burnett, S. (2006). Evaluating early
childhood assessments: A differential Analysis.
In K. McCartney & D. Phillips (Eds.), The Blackwell
handbook of early childhood development (pp. 533
549). Malden, MA: Blackwell Publishing.
Minnesota Department of Education (2003). Minnesota
School Readiness Initiative: Developmental
Assessment at Kindergarten Entrance. Roseville: Minnesota
Department of Education.
Minnesota Department of Education. (2004). Minnesota
School Readiness Year Two Study: Developmental
Assessment at Kindergarten Entrance Fall 2003. Roseville:
Minnesota Department of Education.
Minnesota Department of Education. (2005). Minnesota
School Readiness Year Three Study:
Developmental Assessment at Kindergarten Entrance Fall
2004. Roseville: Minnesota Department of
Education.
Minnesota Department of Education (2007). Minnesota
School Readiness Study: Developmental
Assessment at Kindergarten Entrance Fall 2006. Roseville:
Minnesota Department of Education.
Minnesota Department of Education (2008). Minnesota
School Readiness Study: Developmental
Assessment at Kindergarten Entrance Fall 2007. Roseville:
Minnesota Department of Education.
Minnesota Department of Education and Minnesota
Department of Human Services. (2005). Early
childhood indicators of progress: Minnesota’s early learning
standards. Roseville: Minnesota Department
of Education.
National Early Childhood Accountability Task Force. (2007)
Taking Stock: Assessing and Improving Early
Childhood Learning and Program Quality. Washington DC:
The Pew Charitable Trusts.
National Research Council. (2008). Early Childhood
Assessment: Why, What, and How. Committee on
Developmental Outcomes and Assessments for Young
Children, C.E. Snow and S.B. Van Hemel, Editors.
Board on Children, Youth, and Families, Board on Testing
and Assessment, Division of Behavioral and
Social Sciences and Education. Washington, DC: The
National Academies Press.
National Research Council & Institute of Medicine. (2000).
From neurons to neighborhoods:
The science of early childhood development. Washington,
DC: National Academy Press.
Reynolds, A., Englund, M., Hayakawa, C., Hendricks, M.,
Ou, S., Rosenberger, A., Smerillo, N., Warner-
Richter, M. Assessing the Validity of Minnesota School
Readiness Indicators: Summary Report. Human
Capital Research Collaborative. January 2011. Retrieved
May 2011,
http://www.humancapitalrc.org/mn_school_readiness_indicators
.pdf
-
-
http://www.humancapitalrc.org/mn_school_readiness_indicators
.pdf
Minnesota School Readiness Study 2010
Reynolds, A. J., Temple, J. A., Robertson, D. L., &
Mann, E. A. (2001). Long-term effects of an early
childhood intervention on educational achievement and
juvenile arrest: A 15-year follow-up of low-income
children in public schools. Journal of the American Medical
Association, 285(18), 2339-2346.
Schweinhart, L. J., Montie, J., Xiang, Z., Barnett, W. S.,
Belfield, C. R., & Nores, M. (2005). Lifetime
effects: The high/scope perry preschool study through age 40.
Ypsilanti, MI: High/Scope Press.
U.S. Department of Education, U.S. National Center for
Education Statistics, Home Literacy Activities and
Signs of Children’s Emerging Literacy, 1993, NCES 2000-
026, November 1999; and the Early Childhood
Program Participation Survey, National Household Education
Surveys Program, 2005, unpublished data.
http://www.census.gov/compendia/statab/tables/09s0229.xls
U.S. Department of Health and Human Services. (2009). The
2009 HHS Poverty Guidelines. Retrieved
January 8, 2011, from
http://aspe.hhs.gov/poverty/09Poverty.shtml.
Wertheimer, R., & Croan, T. (December 2003). Attending
kindergarten and already behind: A statistical
portrait of vulnerable young children. Washington, DC: Child
Trends.
Zill, N., & West, J. (2000). Entering kindergarten: A
portrait of American children when they begin school.
Washington, DC: U.S. Department of Education, National
Center for Education Statistics.
http://www.census.gov/compendia/statab/tables/09s0229.xls
http://aspe.hhs.gov/poverty/09Poverty.shtml
A. Sample Work Sampling System® Developmental
Checklist (Minnesota P4)
B. Work Sampling System Subgroup Analysis with
Sampling Weight (2010)
C. Family Survey (English)
D. Logistic Regression Predicting Proficiency at the 75
Percent Standard
(Weighted)
E. Statistically Significant Factors from Logistic
Regression
Minnesota School Readiness Study 2010
Appendices
Minnesota School Readiness Study 2010
Appendix B
Work Sampling System Subgroup Analysis with Sampling
Weight (2010)
75% Overall
Proficiency
(weighted)
All children 59.9
Race/ethnicity
White (N=2841) 62.7
Asian/ Native Hawaiian/Pacific Islander (N=221) 62.0
Black/African/African American (N=349) 57.0
Other (N=64) 53.8
American Indian/Alaskan Native (N=203) 44.4
Hispanic/Latino (N=278) 43.6
Gender
Female (N=2754) 65.4
Male (N=2902) 54.5
IEP Status (Special education)
No (N=5258) 61.9
Yes (N=398) 29.9
Family Income
Over 250% Federal Poverty Guideline (N=1554) 69.2
250% Federal Poverty Guideline and under
52.3
(N=1735)
Parent Education
Less than high school (N=200) 32.4
High School Diploma/GED (N=671) 48.7
Trade school or some college (N=1013) 55.7
Associate’s degree (N=581) 61.2
Bachelor’s degree (N=1024) 67.6
Graduate or professional degree (N=466) 70.7
Strata
1 – Minneapolis and St. Paul (N=655) 57.4
2 – 7 country metro excluding MSP
1
(N=1551) 69.3
3 – Outstate enrollment 2,000+ (N=1306) 51.5
4 – Outstate enrollment 1,000-1,999 (N=1092) 45.9
5 - Outstate enrollment 500-999 (N=605) 52.4
6 - Outstate enrollment <500 (N=445) 63.6
* Note, 250% FPG for a family of four for this time period is
$55,125.
1
The seven county metro area includes Anoka, Carver,
Dakota, Hennepin, Ramsey, Scott and Washington Counties.
Minnesota School Readiness Study 2010
Appendix C
Parent Survey - Minnesota School Readiness Study
1. Please indicate whether you are this child’s:
___ Mother ___ Father ___ Other
2. Your highest level of school completed? Mark only
one.
___ Less than high school
___ High school diploma/GED
___Trade school or some college beyond high school
___ Associate degree
___ Bachelor’s degree
___ Graduate or professional school degree
3. Your household’s total yearly income before taxes from
January-December last year? Round to
the nearest thousand.
$________________________
4. How many people are currently in your household?
1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 Indicate:_____________
5. Race/ethnicity of your kindergarten child? Mark all
that apply.
___ Black/African/African American
___ American Indian/Alaskan Native
___ Asian
___ Native Hawaiian or other Pacific Islander
___ Hispanic or Latino
___ White/Caucasian
___ Other
6. What language does your family speak most at home?
___ English __ Vietnamese
___ Spanish __ Russian
___ Hmong __ Other
___ Somali
Thank you for your time in working with us on this
study.
For school use only:
Dist #_______ School #________ Gender: M F DoB:
____/____/____ MARSS:
_______________________________________
(include all 13 digits, including leading zeros)
Appendix D
Logistic Regression Predicting Proficiency at the 75 Percent
Standard
(Weighted)
VARIABLES b se(b) Wald df p Odds Ratio
Parent Education 38.12*** 5 0.000
Less than High School -0.67*** 0.23 8.09 1 0.004 0.51
High School or GED #
Some Post High
School 0.14 0.13 1.28 1 0.258 1.16
Associate Degree 0.37** 0.15 6.47 1 0.011 1.45
Bachelor Degree 0.54*** 0.14 15.12 1 0.000 1.71
Grad/Prof Degree 0.60*** 0.17 12.54 1 0.000 1.82
Percent of Federal
Poverty Guidelines 20.23*** 2 0.000
0-250 #
>250-400 0.37*** 0.11 11.49 1 0.001 1.44
>400 0.49*** 0.12 16.95 1 0.000 1.63
Home Language 1.24 1 0.266
Non-English #
English Only 0.21 0.19 1.24 1 0.266 1.24
Minority Status 5.07* 2 0.079
Minority Only -0.18 0.12 2.22 1 0.136 0.84
White and Minority 0.21 0.15 1.98 1 0.160 1.24
White Only #
Gender 15.15*** 1 0.000
Male #
Female 0.32*** 0.08 15.15 1 0.000 1.37
Intercept -0.35 0.22 2.52 1 0.113
Number of observations: 3246
# indicates reference category
*** p<0.01, ** p<0.05, * p<0.1
Minnesota School Readiness Study 2010
Appendix E
Statistically Significant Factors from Logistic Regression
Domain/Year
Parent Percent Primary Race and Gender
Education of FPG* Home Ethnicity
Language
Physical Development and
Health
2006 -- *** -- -- ***
-- *** -- -- ***
-- *** *** -- ***
*** *** -- -- --
*** -- -- -- ***
-- *** -- -- ***
-- *** -- -- ***
-- *** -- *** --
*** *** -- -- ***
--- *** -- -- ***
-- *** -- *** ***
-- *** -- -- ***
*** *** -- -- --
-- *** *** -- ***
-- *** *** -- ***
-- *** -- -- --
*** *** -- -- ***
*** *** *** -- ***
-- *** *** -- ***
-- *** -- -- ***
2007
2008
2009
The Arts
2006
2007
2008
2009
Personal and Social
Development
2006
2007
2008
2009
Mathematical Thinking
2006
2007
2008
2009
Language and Literacy
2006
2007
2008
2009
75 Percent Standard
2010 *** *** -- -- ***
*** Noted demographic is significant for specified domain
and year.
* Federal Poverty Guideline is used from 2007 forward.
2006 income asked categorically.
Note – Analysis 2010 forward focused on 75 percent
standard.
Minnesota School Readiness Study 2010
- - -
- - -
- -
- - -
- - -
- - -
- - -
- - -
- -
- - -
- -
- - -
- - -
- -
- -
- - - -
- -
-
- -
- - -
- -
School Readiness and Later Achievement
Greg J. Duncan
Northwestern University
Chantelle J. Dowsett
University of Texas at Austin
Amy Claessens
Northwestern University
Katherine Magnuson
University of Wisconsin–Madison
Aletha C. Huston
University of Texas at Austin
Pamela Klebanov
Princeton University
Linda S. Pagani
Université de Montréal
Leon Feinstein
University of London
Mimi Engel
Northwestern University
Jeanne Brooks-Gunn
Columbia University
Holly Sexton
University of Michigan
Kathryn Duckworth
University of London
Crista Japel
Université de Québec à Montréal
Using 6 longitudinal data sets, the authors estimate links
between three key elements of school
readiness—school-entry academic, attention, and
socioemotional skills—and later school reading and
math achievement. In an effort to isolate the effects of these
school-entry skills, the authors ensured that
most of their regression models control for cognitive, attention,
and socioemotional skills measured prior
to school entry, as well as a host of family background
measures. Across all 6 studies, the strongest
predictors of later achievement are school-entry math, reading,
and attention skills. A meta-analysis of
the results shows that early math skills have the greatest
predictive power, followed by reading and then
attention skills. By contrast, measures of socioemotional
behaviors, including internalizing and exter-
nalizing problems and social skills, were generally insignificant
predictors of later academic perfor-
mance, even among children with relatively high levels of
problem behavior. Patterns of association were
similar for boys and girls and for children from high and low
socioeconomic backgrounds.
Keywords: school readiness, socioemotional behaviors,
attention, early academic skills
Supplemental materials: http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/[0012-
1649.43.6.1428].supp
Greg J. Duncan, Amy Claessens, and Mimi Engel, School of
Education
and Social Policy, Northwestern University; Chantelle J.
Dowsett and
Aletha C. Huston, Department of Human Ecology, University of
Texas at
Austin; Katherine Magnuson, Department of Social Work,
University of
Wisconsin–Madison; Pamela Klebanov, Center for Research on
Child
Wellbeing, Princeton University; Linda S. Pagani, Department
of Psycho-
education, Université de Montréal, Québec, Canada; Leon
Feinstein and
Kathryn Duckworth, Department of Quantitative Social Science,
Institute
of Education, University of London, London, England; Jeanne
Brooks-
Gunn, Department of Pediatrics, Columbia University; Holly
Sexton, Re-
seach Center for Group Dynamics, University of Michigan;
Crista Japel,
Département d’éducation et formation spécialisées, Université
de Québec
à Montréal, Québec, Canada.
A preliminary version of this article was presented at the
biennial
meetings of the Society for Research on Child Development,
Atlanta,
Georgia, April 2005. We are grateful to the National Science
Foundation-supported Center for the Analysis of Pathways from
Child-
hood to Adulthood (CAPCA; Grant 0322356) for research
support. We
thank Larry Aber, Mark Appelbaum, Avshalom Caspi, David
Cordray,
Herbert Ginsburg, David Grissmer, Mark Lipsey, Derek Neal,
Cybele
Raver, Arnold Sameroff, Robert Siegler, Ross Thompson,
Sandra Jo
Wilson, Nicholas Zill, and other members of CAPCA and the
MacArthur Network on Families and the Economy for helpful
com-
ments.
Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to
Greg J.
Duncan, School of Education and Social Policy, Northwestern
University,
2046 Sheridan Road, Evanston, IL 60208. E-mail: greg-
[email protected]
Developmental Psychology Copyright 2007 by the American
Psychological Association
2007, Vol. 43, No. 6, 1428–1446 0012-1649/07/$12.00 DOI:
10.1037/0012-1649.43.6.1428
1428
Early childhood programs and policies that promote academic
skills have been gaining popularity among politicians and re-
searchers. For example, President George W. Bush (2002) en-
dorsed Head Start reforms in 2002 that focus on building early
academic skills, observing that “on the first day of school,
children
need to know letters and numbers. They need a strong
vocabulary.
These are the building blocks of learning, and this nation must
provide them” (p. 12). The National Research Council’s
Commit-
tee on the Prevention of Reading Difficulties in Young Children
recommends providing environments that promote preliteracy
skills for all preschool children (Snow, Burns, & Griffin, 1998).
Similarly, the National Association for the Education of Young
Children and the National Council of Teachers of Mathematics
(2002) issued a joint statement that advocated for high-quality
mathematics education for children ages 3–6.
Others, however, maintain that a broad constellation of behav-
iors and skills enables children to learn in school. Asked to
identify
factors associated with a difficult transition to school,
kindergarten
teachers frequently mentioned weaknesses in academic skills,
problems with social skills, trouble following directions, and
dif-
ficulty with independent and group work (Rimm-Kaufman,
Pianta,
& Cox, 2000). Researchers too have made this point. The
National
Research Council and Institute on Medicine argued that “the
elements of early intervention programs that enhance social and
emotional development are just as important as the components
that enhance linguistic and cognitive competence” (Shonkoff &
Phillips, 2000, pp. 398–399).
These two views have emerged in the current debate about what
constitutes school readiness and in particular about what skills
predict school achievement. Many early education programs, in-
cluding Head Start, are designed to enhance children’s physical,
intellectual, and social competencies on the grounds that each
domain contributes to a child’s overall developmental
competence
and readiness for school. However, if early acquisition of
specific
academic skills or learning-enhancing behaviors forecasts later
achievement, it may be beneficial to add domain-specific early
skills to the definition of school readiness and to encourage
inter-
ventions aimed at promoting these skills prior to elementary
school. Thus, understanding which skills are linked to
children’s
academic achievement has important implications for early edu-
cation programs.
We adopted a child-centered model of school transition, which
is nested within a broader ecological framework but focuses on
the
set of individual skills and behaviors that children have
acquired
prior to school entry (Rimm-Kaufman & Pianta, 2000). A
child’s
individual characteristics contribute to the environments in
which
the child interacts and the rate at which the child may learn new
skills; in turn, the child receives feedback from others in the
environment (Meisels, 1998). Thus, because they affect both the
child and the social environment, early academic skills and
socio-
emotional behaviors are linked to subsequent academic achieve-
ment because they provide the foundation for positive classroom
adaptation (Cunha, Heckman, Lochner, & Masterov, 2006;
Entwisle, Alexander, & Olson, 2005).
For example, a child who enters kindergarten with rudimentary
academic skills may be poised to learn from formal reading and
mathematics instruction, receive positive reinforcement from
the
teacher, or be placed in a higher ability group that facilitates the
acquisition of additional skills. Similarly, a child who can pay
attention, inhibit impulsive behavior, and relate appropriately to
adults and peers may be able to take advantage of the learning
opportunities in the classroom, thus more easily mastering
reading
and math concepts taught in elementary school. For these
reasons,
the skills children possess when entering school might result in
different achievement patterns in later life. If achievement at
older
ages is the product of a sequential process of skill acquisition,
then
strengthening skills prior to school entry might lead children to
master more advanced skills at an earlier age and perhaps even
increase their ultimate level of achievement.
Although there are strong theoretical reasons to expect that
individual differences in children’s early academic skills and
be-
havior are linked to subsequent behavior and achievement, sur-
prisingly little rigorous research has been conducted to test this
hypothesis. Consequently, the purpose of this article is to assess
as
precisely as possible, using six longitudinal, nonexperimental
data
sets, the association between skills and behaviors that emerge
during the preschool years and later academic achievement. As
with Robins’s (1978) classic study of adult antisocial behavior,
our
approach consists of comparable analyses of a number of
different
longitudinal developmental studies.1 We are especially
interested
in identifying academic, attention, and socioemotional skills
and
behaviors that may be learned or improved through experiences
prior to school entry. In the following sections, we draw from
developmental literature to identify key dimensions of school
readiness and to derive theoretical predictions about how chil-
dren’s school-entry skills and behaviors contribute to short- and
long-term academic success.
Associations Between Early Skills and Later Achievement
Academic achievement is a cumulative process involving both
mastering new skills and improving already existing skills
(Entwisle & Alexander, 1990; Pungello, Kuperschmidt,
Burchinal,
& Patterson, 1996). Information about how children acquire
read-
ing and math skills points to the importance of specific
academic
skills but also indicates that more general cognitive skills,
partic-
ularly oral language and conceptual ability, may be increasingly
important for later mastery of more complex reading and mathe-
matical tasks. Basic oral language skills become critical for un-
derstanding texts as the level of difficulty of reading passages
increases (NICHD Early Child Care Research Network, 2005b;
Scarborough, 2001; Snow et al, 1998; Storch & Whitehurst,
2002;
Whitehurst & Lonigan, 1998). Likewise, mastery of
foundational
concepts of numbers allows for a deeper understanding of more
complex mathematical problems and flexible problem-solving
techniques (Baroody, 2003; Ferrari & Sternberg, 1998; Hiebert
&
Wearne, 1996).
Although children’s academic achievement is largely stable
throughout childhood, children do demonstrate both transitory
fluctuations and fundamental shifts in their achievement
trajecto-
ries (Kowaleski-Jones & Duncan, 1998; Pungello et al., 1996).
Nonexperimental data show that children’s achievement test
1 Robins (1978) justified her approach as follows: “In the long
run, the
best evidence for the truth of any observation lies in its
replicability across
studies. The more the populations studied differ, the wider the
historical
eras they span; the more the details of the methods vary, the
more
convincing becomes that replication” (p. 611).
1429SCHOOL READINESS AND LATER ACHIEVEMENT
scores are related to prior cognitive functioning and the
attainment
of basic skills in math and literacy such as number and letter
recognition (Stevenson & Newman, 1986). In their meta-
analysis,
La Paro and Pianta (2000) found middle-range correlations in
cognitive/academic skills both from preschool to kindergarten
(.43) and from kindergarten to first or second grade (.48).
Attention-related skills such as task persistence and self-
regulation are expected to increase the time during which
children
are engaged and participating in academic endeavors. Research
has
shown that signs of attention and impulsivity can be detected as
early as age 2.5 but continue to develop until reaching relative
stability between ages 6 and 8 (Olson, Sameroff, Kerr, Lopez, &
Wellman, 2005; Posner & Rothbart, 2000). Studies linking
atten-
tion with later achievement are less common, but consistent evi-
dence suggests that the ability to control and sustain attention
as
well as participate in classroom activities predicts achievement
test
scores and grades during preschool and the early elementary
grades (Alexander, Entwisle, & Dauber, 1993; Raver, Smith-
Donald, Hayes, & Jones, 2005). These attention skills, which
are
conceptually distinct from other types of interpersonal
behaviors,
are associated with later academic achievement, independent of
initial cognitive ability (McClelland, Morrison, & Holmes,
2000;
Yen, Konold, & McDermott, 2004) and of prior reading ability
and
current vocabulary (Howse, Lange, Farran, & Boyles, 2003).
Ex-
amining attention separately from externalizing problems has
clar-
ified the role of each in achievement, suggesting that attention
is
more predictive of later achievement than more general problem
behaviors (Barriga et al., 2002; Hinshaw, 1992; Konold &
Pianta,
2005; Ladd, Birch, & Buhs, 1999; Normandeau & Guay, 1998;
Trzesniewski, Moffitt, Caspi, Taylor, & Maughan, 2006).
Children’s socioemotional skills and behaviors are also ex-
pected to affect both individual learning and classroom
dynamics.
Inadequate interpersonal skills promote child–teacher conflict
and
social exclusion (Newcomb, Bukowski, & Pattee, 1993; Parker
&
Asher, 1987), and these stressors may reduce children’s
participa-
tion in collaborative learning activities and adversely affect
achievement (Ladd et al., 1999; Pianta & Stuhlman, 2004). Cor-
relational evidence linking problem behaviors to academic
achievement is found in the Beginning School Study. First-
grade
ratings on items describing a cheerful, outgoing temperament
(roughly the opposite of internalizing problems) predicted adult
educational attainment better than preschool or first-grade
achieve-
ment scores (Entwisle et al., 2005). Other studies yield similar
results. For example, children with consistently high levels of
aggression from ages 2–9 were more likely than other children
to
have achievement problems in third grade (NICHD Early Child
Care Research Network, 2004).
Experimental Evidence and Crossover Effects
Many nonexperimental studies find associations between early
achievement, attention, and behavior and later achievement, yet
few of these studies are designed to determine which of these
skills
can be modified prior to school entry or whether these changes
predict later achievement. In theory, intervention research
should
shed light on this gap by demonstrating ways to improve
children’s
skills and by testing whether improvements in early skills are
associated with better adjustment in the long term. Indeed, a
small
number of experimental interventions provide encouraging evi-
dence that high-quality programs for preschool children “at
risk”
for school failure produce gains in cognitive and academic
skills
and reduce behavior problems (Conduct Problems Prevention
Re-
search Group, 2002; Karoly, Kilburn, & Cannon, 2005; Love et
al.,
2003). Early educational interventions have also been found to
result in long-term reductions in special education services,
grade
retention, and increases in educational attainment (Campbell,
Ramey, Pungello, Sparling, & Miller-Johnson, 2002; Lazar et
al.,
1982; Reynolds & Temple, 1998).
As is the case with nonexperimental studies, few intervention
studies are designed to isolate the relative contributions of
changes
in achievement, attention, and behavior to later school achieve-
ment. A first problem is that behavioral interventions tend to
measure behavioral but not achievement outcomes, whereas
read-
ing and math interventions tend to measure achievement but not
behavioral outcomes. Interesting exceptions are a small number
of
experimental behavior-based interventions that tested for
achieve-
ment impacts (Coie & Krehbiel, 1984; Dolan et al., 1993). For
example, a random-assignment evaluation of a behavioral inter-
vention targeting both aggressive and shy behaviors among first
graders found short-run improvements in both teacher and peer
reports of aggressive and shy behavior but no crossover impacts
on
reading achievement (Dolan et al., 1993; Kellam, Mayer,
Rebok,
& Hawkins, 1998). Given evidence, albeit limited, that
behavioral
interventions succeed at improving behavior but not
achievement,
behavior would appear to play a limited role in academic
success.
A second problem is that many intervention programs target
both children’s academic skills and their socioemotional behav-
iors, rendering it impossible to assess their separate impacts
through simple experimental contrasts. For example, the Fast
Track prevention program provided a number of services to
chil-
dren who were identified as disruptive in kindergarten,
including
direct tutoring in reading skills in first grade (Conduct
Problems
Prevention Research Group, 1992; 2002). It is possible to
estimate
nonexperimental mediated models to determine whether
program
effects are more likely to be due to children’s improved
achieve-
ment, attention, or behavior skills (e.g., Reynolds, Ou, &
Topitzes,
2004). This is rarely done, however.
The Present Study
This study builds on previous school readiness research in
several ways. First, the scope of the study is unprecedented. We
estimated a carefully specified set of models with data from six
large-scale longitudinal studies, two of which were nationally
representative of U.S. children, whereas two were drawn from
multisite studies of U.S. children, with one each focusing on
children from Great Britain and Canada. Second, we included as
predictors a wide representation of school readiness indicators
used in previous research and carefully distinguished between
related but conceptually distinct skills (e.g., oral language vs.
preliteracy skills, attention vs. externalizing problems)
wherever
possible. Third, we examined multiple dimensions of academic
achievement outcomes, including grade completion and math
and
reading achievement as assessed by both teacher ratings and test
scores. Fourth, we implemented rigorous analytic methods that
attempted to isolate the effects of school-entry academic,
attention,
and socioemotional skills by controlling for an extensive set of
prior child, family, and contextual influences that may have
been
1430 DUNCAN ET AL.
related to children’s achievement. Finally, we assessed whether
the
predictive power of school readiness components differs by
gender
or socioeconomic status, which would indicate that some
children
are at heightened risk of low achievement.
We tested a number of hypotheses related to how school-entry
academic, attention, and socioemotional skills are associated
with
later school achievement. Developmental theory suggests that
chil-
dren’s informal, intuitive knowledge of early language and math
concepts plays an important role in the acquisition of more
com-
plex skills formally taught in elementary school (Adams,
Treiman,
& Pressley, 1998; Baroody, 2003; Griffin, Case, & Capodilupo,
1995; Tunmer & Nesdale, 1998). Theoretically, children’s atten-
tion and socioemotional skills should also affect achievement
because they influence children’s engagement in learning
activities
and facilitate (or disrupt) classroom processes (Ladd, Birch, &
Buhs, 1999; Pianta & Stuhlman, 2004). However, some scholars
point out that it is important to distinguish between behaviors
that
are directly relevant for learning, such as attention, and those
that
may be correlated with attention but are less likely to be
directly
linked with achievement, such as interpersonal skills and
problem
behavior (Alexander et al., 1993; Cooper & Farran, 1991;
McClel-
land et al., 2000; McWayne, Fantuzzo, & McDermott, 2004).
Therefore, we expected early academic and attention-related
skills
to predict subsequent test scores and teacher achievement
ratings,
and we expected attention skills to predict achievement more
consistently than do socioemotional behaviors.
In seeking a better understanding of the extent to which our
broad set of early skills is associated with later achievement, it
is
important to consider how outcomes are being measured.
Although
test performance provides a key independent assessment of aca-
demic achievement, teacher ratings also lend insight into chil-
dren’s everyday performance in the classroom. Teachers’
evalua-
tions are probably based on a broad picture of children’s
accomplishments, which include their academic skills as well as
whether they complete assignments on time, work
independently,
get along with others, and show involvement in the learning
agenda of the classroom. Moreover, previous research has found
that children’s behavior can play a role that is equal to, if not
greater than, prior cognitive ability in predicting teacher-rated
attainment or achievement (Lin, Lawrence, & Gorrell, 2003;
Schaefer & McDermott, 1999) and academic skills (National
Cen-
ter for Education Statistics, 1993). Consequently, we expected a
stronger relationship between school-entry socioemotional
behav-
iors and subsequent teacher-rated achievement than with subse-
quent test scores.
Although many previous studies have examined the association
between early academic, attention, and socioemotional skills
and
subsequent achievement, few have systematically considered the
extent to which these associations differ by gender
(Trzesniewski
et al., 2006). On average, boys receive poorer grades and have
more difficulties related to school progress (e.g., grade
retention,
special education, and drop out) than do girls (Dauber,
Alexander,
& Entwisle, 1993; McCoy & Reynolds, 1999), and these differ-
ences are especially pronounced among low-income children
(Hin-
shaw, 1992). Children from low-income families enter school
with
lower mean academic skills, and the gap tends to increase
during
the school years (Lee & Burkam, 2002). These groups also have
higher rates of problems with attention and externalizing
behavior
(Entwisle et al., 2005; Miech, Essex, & Goldsmith, 2001; Raver,
2004).
Despite differences in children’s behavior linked to gender and
family socioeconomic status, few studies have considered
whether
gender and socioeconomic status moderate the association be-
tween these early skills and behaviors and subsequent achieve-
ment. We expected early academic skills, attention, and
socioemo-
tional behaviors to matter more for these subgroups, particularly
when children enter school with very low levels of these skills.
To estimate the associations between early academic skills and
socioemotional behaviors and later school achievement, we
sum-
marize results from a coordinated series of analyses across six
longitudinal data sets in two ways. First, we relate early
academic,
attention, and socioemotional skills to later achievement in each
of
the six data sets and provide a basic summary of these results.
Second, we formally summarize the findings from these studies
in
a meta-analysis, again focusing on the extent to which this
collec-
tion of early skills predicts later achievement.
Method
In this section, we describe the data sets used in this study and
the common analytic procedures that were implemented across
studies. Detailed information about the measures, descriptive
sta-
tistics, and regression results from each study is presented in
Appendices A–F, which can be found online. As the goal of our
study was to relate early academic, attention, and
socioemotional
skills and behaviors to later achievement, each data set has mea-
sures of these constructs, although there is variation across the
studies with respect to when and how each skill or behavior is
assessed.
Table 1 provides an overview of data sources and measures
available in each study. All six data sets provide measures of
children’s academic skills as well as assessments of attention
and
socioemotional behaviors at about age 5 or 6. Because most
children enter elementary school at this age, we refer to the
timing
of these measures as school entry but alert the reader that the
precise timing varies considerably across studies. To facilitate
comparison of findings across studies, we standardized all mea-
sures to have a mean of 0 and standard deviation of 1.
We measured achievement outcomes using teachers’ reports,
test scores, and grade retention in early elementary school and,
in
some studies, middle childhood. In terms of the timing of the
measurement of achievement outcomes, the children of the Na-
tional Longitudinal Survey of Youth (NLSY) measures are as-
sessed as late as early adolescence, the National Institute of
Child
Health and Human Development Study of Early Child Care and
Youth Development (NICHD SECCYD) as late as fifth grade,
and
the 1970 British Birth Cohort Study (BCS) at age 10, whereas
none
of the other studies measures achievement beyond third grade.
As
for measurement methods, two studies have both test-score-
based
and teacher reports of reading and mathematics achievement
(the
Early Childhood Longitudinal Study–Kindergarten Cohort
[ECLS-
K] and NICHD SECCYD).
We measured attention and socioemotional behaviors on the
basis of mothers’ reports, teachers’ reports, and observation.
Table
1 provides an overview of the similarities and differences in
measurement across the six studies. One of our data sets, the
Infant
Health and Development Program (IHDP), has observer reports
of
1431SCHOOL READINESS AND LATER ACHIEVEMENT
T
ab
le
1
St
ud
y
M
ea
su
re
s
of
O
ut
co
m
es
,
Sc
ho
ol
E
nt
ry
an
d
P
re
sc
ho
ol
A
ch
ie
ve
m
en
t,
A
tt
en
ti
on
an
d
So
ci
oe
m
ot
io
na
l
B
eh
av
io
rs
M
ea
su
re
E
C
L
S-
K
G
ra
de
3
N
L
SY
ag
e
13
–1
4
N
IC
H
D
SE
C
C
Y
D
G
ra
de
5
IH
D
P
ag
e
8
M
L
E
PS
G
ra
de
3
B
C
S
ag
e
10
O
ut
co
m
es
R
ea
di
ng
A
ch
ie
ve
m
en
t
T
es
t
R
ea
di
ng
ite
m
re
sp
on
se
th
eo
ry
(I
R
T
)
“a
dv
an
ce
d”
su
bs
ca
le
s:
E
xt
ra
po
la
tio
n,
E
va
lu
at
io
n
A
ca
de
m
ic
R
at
in
g
Sc
al
e
(�
�
.9
5)
:
T
ea
ch
er
R
ep
or
t
Pe
ab
od
y
In
di
vi
du
al
A
ch
ie
ve
m
en
tT
es
ts
(P
IA
T
)
R
ea
di
ng
R
ec
og
ni
tio
n:
M
at
ch
in
g
le
tte
rs
,
na
m
in
g
na
m
es
,a
nd
re
ad
in
g
si
ng
le
w
or
ds
ou
tl
ou
d
(te
st
–r
et
es
t�
.8
9)
W
oo
dc
oc
k–
Jo
hn
so
n
Ps
yc
ho
-
E
du
ca
tio
na
lB
at
te
ry
—
R
ev
is
ed
(W
J-
R
)
R
ea
di
ng
:
L
et
te
r-
W
or
d,
W
or
d
A
tta
ck
A
ca
de
m
ic
Sk
ill
s
R
at
in
gs
Sc
al
e:
T
ea
ch
er
R
ep
or
t
W
oo
dc
oc
k–
Jo
hn
so
n
T
es
ts
of
A
ch
ie
ve
m
en
t—
R
ev
is
ed
:B
ro
ad
R
ea
di
ng
(�
�
.9
0s
)
V
er
ba
lS
ki
lls
:U
se
/
un
de
rs
ta
nd
in
g
of
Fr
en
ch
,
ch
ar
ac
te
ris
tic
s
of
or
al
co
m
m
un
ic
at
io
n
(�
�
.9
4)
:
T
ea
ch
er
R
ep
or
t
E
di
nb
ur
gh
R
ea
di
ng
T
es
t:
Se
lf
-c
om
pl
et
io
n,
w
or
d
re
co
gn
iti
on
,u
se
an
d
un
de
rs
ta
nd
in
g
of
sy
nt
ax
,s
eq
ue
nc
in
g,
co
m
pr
eh
en
si
on
,a
nd
re
te
nt
io
n
(�
�
.9
6)
M
at
h
A
ch
ie
ve
m
en
t
T
es
t
M
at
h
IR
T
“a
dv
an
ce
d”
su
bs
ca
le
s:
M
ul
tip
lic
at
io
n/
di
vi
si
on
,
Pl
ac
e-
va
lu
e,
W
or
d
pr
ob
le
m
s
A
ca
de
m
ic
R
at
in
g
Sc
al
e
(�
�
.9
4)
:
T
ea
ch
er
R
ep
or
t
PI
A
T
M
at
h:
A
pp
lic
at
io
n
of
m
at
he
m
at
ic
al
co
nc
ep
ts
,n
um
be
r
re
co
gn
iti
on
,c
ou
nt
in
g,
m
ul
tip
lic
at
io
n,
di
vi
si
on
,f
ra
ct
io
ns
,a
nd
ad
va
nc
ed
al
ge
br
a
an
d
ge
om
et
ry
(te
st
/re
te
st
�
.7
4)
W
J-
R
M
at
h:
A
pp
lie
d
pr
ob
le
m
s
A
ca
de
m
ic
Sk
ill
s
R
at
in
gs
Sc
al
e:
T
ea
ch
er
R
ep
or
t
W
oo
dc
oc
k–
Jo
hn
so
n
T
es
ts
of
A
ch
ie
ve
m
en
t—
R
ev
is
ed
:
B
ro
ad
M
at
h
(�
�
.9
0s
)
N
um
be
r
K
no
w
le
dg
e
T
es
t:
N
um
be
r
se
qu
en
ce
,
ad
di
tio
n,
su
bt
ra
ct
io
n,
m
ul
tip
lic
at
io
n,
di
vi
si
on
,
fr
ac
tio
ns
,d
ec
im
al
s
U
ni
ve
rs
ity
of
B
ris
to
lM
at
h
T
es
t:
R
ul
es
of
ar
ith
m
et
ic
,
pl
ac
e
va
lu
e,
fr
ac
tio
ns
,
m
ea
su
re
m
en
t,
al
ge
br
a,
ge
om
et
ry
,a
nd
st
at
is
tic
s
(�
�
.9
3)
Sc
ho
ol
en
tr
y
sk
ill
s
Fa
ll
of
ki
nd
er
ga
rt
en
A
ge
5–
6
A
ge
4.
5
A
ge
5
Ju
ni
or
an
d
se
ni
or
ki
nd
er
ga
rte
n
A
ge
5
A
ch
ie
ve
m
en
t
R
ea
di
ng
A
ch
ie
ve
m
en
tT
es
tR
ea
di
ng
IR
T
“e
ar
ly
”
su
bs
ca
le
s:
L
et
te
r
re
co
gn
iti
on
,
be
gi
nn
in
g
an
d
en
di
ng
w
or
d
so
un
ds
PI
A
T
R
ea
di
ng
R
ec
og
ni
tio
n
W
J-
R
R
ea
di
ng
:L
et
te
r-
W
or
d
Id
en
tif
ic
at
io
n
(�
�
.8
4)
L
an
gu
ag
e/
ve
rb
al
ab
ili
ty
Pr
es
ch
oo
l
L
an
gu
ag
e
Sc
al
e–
3:
E
xp
re
ss
iv
e
co
m
m
un
ic
at
io
n
W
ec
hs
le
r
Pr
es
ch
oo
l
an
d
Pr
im
ar
y
Sc
al
e
of
In
te
lli
ge
nc
e
(W
PP
SI
):
V
er
ba
l
IQ
(�
�
.9
4)
Pe
ab
od
y
Pi
ct
ur
e
V
oc
ab
ul
ar
y
T
es
t(
PP
V
T
),
Fo
rm
s
A
an
d
B
,F
re
nc
h
ad
ap
ta
tio
n:
Sp
lit
-
ha
lf
re
lia
bi
lit
y
.6
6
an
d
.8
5
fo
r
A
an
d
B
,r
es
pe
ct
iv
el
y;
te
st
–r
et
es
ta
t1
w
ee
k
�
.7
2
E
ng
lis
h
Pi
ct
ur
e
V
oc
ab
ul
ar
y
T
es
t:
V
er
ba
l
in
te
lli
ge
nc
e
M
at
h
A
ch
ie
ve
m
en
t
T
es
t
M
at
h
IR
T
“e
ar
ly
”
su
bs
ca
le
s:
C
ou
nt
in
g,
or
di
na
lit
y
an
d
re
la
tiv
e
si
ze
PI
A
T
M
at
h
W
J-
R
M
at
h:
A
pp
lie
d
pr
ob
le
m
s
(�
�
.8
4)
N
um
be
r
K
no
w
le
dg
e
T
es
t:
In
fo
rm
al
nu
m
be
r
kn
ow
le
dg
e,
co
un
tin
g,
ad
di
tio
n,
nu
m
be
r
se
qu
en
ce
A
tte
nt
io
n
sk
ill
s
A
tte
nt
io
n
sk
ill
s
A
pp
ro
ac
he
s
to
L
ea
rn
in
g
(�
�
.8
9)
:T
ea
ch
er
R
ep
or
t
C
on
tin
uo
us
Pe
rf
or
m
an
ce
T
as
k:
A
tte
nt
io
n
A
tte
nt
io
n:
C
hi
ld
co
nc
en
tr
at
es
,
lis
te
ns
at
te
nt
iv
el
y,
et
c.
(�
�
.8
2)
:
T
ea
ch
er
R
ep
or
t
A
tte
nt
io
n
pr
ob
le
m
s
H
yp
er
ac
tiv
ity
(d
if
fi
cu
lty
co
nc
en
tr
at
in
g,
re
st
le
ss
,e
tc
.):
M
at
er
na
l
R
ep
or
t
(I
)
C
hi
ld
B
eh
av
io
r
C
he
ck
lis
t
(C
B
C
L
),
A
tte
nt
io
n
pr
ob
le
m
s
(�
�
.9
3)
:F
al
l
of
K
in
de
rg
ar
te
n
T
ea
ch
er
R
ep
or
t
(I
I)
C
on
tin
uo
us
Pe
rf
or
m
an
ce
T
as
k:
Im
pu
ls
iv
ity
A
ch
en
ba
ch
C
hi
ld
B
eh
av
io
r
Pr
of
ile
,
A
tte
nt
io
n
pr
ob
le
m
s
(�
�
.6
1)
:M
at
er
na
l
R
ep
or
t
H
yp
er
ac
tiv
e:
C
hi
ld
se
em
s
ag
ita
te
d,
im
pu
ls
iv
e,
et
c.
(�
�
.9
0)
:
T
ea
ch
er
R
ep
or
t
R
ut
te
r
Sc
al
e:
In
at
te
nt
io
n
(�
�
.6
7)
:
M
at
er
na
l
R
ep
or
t
1432 DUNCAN ET AL.
T
ab
le
1
(c
on
ti
nu
ed
)
Sc
ho
ol
en
tr
y
sk
ill
s
Fa
ll
of
ki
nd
er
ga
rt
en
A
ge
5–
6
A
ge
4.
5
A
ge
5
Ju
ni
or
an
d
se
ni
or
ki
nd
er
ga
rt
en
A
ge
5
So
ci
oe
m
ot
io
na
l
be
ha
vi
or
s
E
xt
er
na
liz
in
g
pr
ob
le
m
s
E
xt
er
na
liz
in
g
Pr
ob
le
m
s
(�
�
.9
0)
:
T
ea
ch
er
R
ep
or
t
(I
)
H
ea
d
St
ro
ng
(s
tu
bb
or
n,
st
ro
ng
te
m
pe
r,
et
c.
):
M
at
er
na
l
R
ep
or
t
(I
I)
A
nt
is
oc
ia
l
(c
he
at
s,
bu
lli
es
,e
tc
.):
M
at
er
na
l
R
ep
or
t
C
B
C
L
,A
gg
re
ss
iv
e
be
ha
vi
or
(�
�
.9
3)
:
Fa
ll
of
K
in
de
rg
ar
te
n
T
ea
ch
er
R
ep
or
t
A
gg
re
ss
io
n:
C
hi
ld
fi
gh
ts
,
bu
lli
es
ot
he
rs
,e
tc
.(
�
�
.7
2)
:
T
ea
ch
er
R
ep
or
t
R
ut
te
r
Sc
al
e,
E
xt
er
na
liz
in
g:
C
hi
ld
bu
lli
es
ot
he
rs
,i
s
di
so
be
di
en
t,
et
c.
(�
�
.7
2)
:M
at
er
na
lR
ep
or
t
In
te
rn
al
iz
in
g
pr
ob
le
m
s
In
te
rn
al
iz
in
g
Pr
ob
le
m
s
(�
�
.8
0)
:
T
ea
ch
er
R
ep
or
t
A
nx
io
us
/d
ep
re
ss
ed
(f
ee
ls
un
lo
ve
d,
sa
d)
:
M
at
er
na
l
R
ep
or
t
C
B
C
L
:
In
te
rn
al
iz
in
g
(�
�
.9
3)
:
Fa
ll
of
K
in
de
rg
ar
te
n
T
ea
ch
er
R
ep
or
t
A
nx
io
us
/D
ep
re
ss
ed
:
C
hi
ld
w
or
ri
es
,c
ri
es
of
te
n,
et
c.
(�
�
.8
0)
:
T
ea
ch
er
R
ep
or
t
R
ut
te
r
Sc
al
e,
In
te
rn
al
iz
in
g:
C
hi
ld
w
or
ri
es
,s
ee
m
s
m
is
er
ab
le
,e
tc
.(
�
�
.5
4)
:
M
at
er
na
l
R
ep
or
t
So
ci
al
sk
ill
s
(I
)
Se
lf
C
on
tr
ol
(�
�
.7
9)
:
T
ea
ch
er
R
ep
or
t
(I
I)
In
te
rp
er
so
na
l
Sk
ill
s
(�
�
.8
9)
:
T
ea
ch
er
R
ep
or
t
So
ci
al
Sk
ill
s
R
at
in
g
Sy
st
em
(S
SR
S)
:C
oo
pe
ra
tio
n,
as
se
rti
on
,s
el
f-
co
nt
ro
l(
�
�
.9
3)
:F
al
lo
f
K
in
de
rg
ar
te
n
T
ea
ch
er
Pr
os
oc
ia
l:
C
hi
ld
is
he
lp
fu
l,
sy
m
pa
th
et
ic
to
ot
he
rs
,e
tc
.
(�
�
.9
2)
:
T
ea
ch
er
R
ep
or
t
Pr
io
r
ch
ild
co
nt
ro
ls
A
ge
3–
4
A
ge
3
A
ge
3
Ju
ni
or
an
d
se
ni
or
ki
nd
er
ga
rt
en
A
ge
42
m
on
th
s
Pr
io
r
co
gn
iti
ve
/
ac
hi
ev
em
en
t
Pe
ab
od
y
Pi
ct
ur
e
V
oc
ab
ul
ar
y
T
es
t—
R
ev
is
ed
(P
PV
T
-R
;
sp
lit
-h
al
f
re
lia
bi
lit
y
of
.8
0)
B
ra
ck
en
B
as
ic
Sk
ill
s
(c
ol
or
s,
le
tte
rs
,n
um
be
rs
,
et
c.
,�
�
.9
3)
R
ey
ne
ll
D
ev
el
op
m
en
ta
l
L
an
gu
ag
e
Sc
al
e
(v
oc
ab
ul
ar
y
co
m
pr
eh
en
si
on
,�
�
.9
3;
ex
pr
es
si
ve
la
ng
ua
ge
,�
�
.8
6)
St
an
fo
rd
-B
in
et
IQ
T
es
t
Pe
ab
od
y
Pi
ct
ur
e
V
oc
ab
ul
ar
y
T
es
t(
PP
V
T
),
Fo
rm
s
A
an
d
B
,F
re
nc
h
ad
ap
ta
tio
n:
Sp
lit
-
ha
lf
re
lia
bi
lit
y
.6
6
an
d
.8
5
fo
r
A
an
d
B
,r
es
pe
ct
iv
el
y;
te
st
–r
et
es
ta
t1
w
ee
k
�
.7
2
N
um
be
r
K
no
w
le
dg
e
T
es
t:
In
fo
rm
al
nu
m
be
r
kn
ow
le
dg
e,
sh
ap
es
,c
ol
or
s,
co
un
tin
g,
nu
m
be
r
se
qu
en
ce
,
ad
di
tio
n
C
ou
nt
in
g
(�
�
.9
5)
Sp
ea
ki
ng
&
V
oc
ab
ul
ar
y
(�
�
.7
9)
C
op
yi
ng
de
si
gn
s
(�
�
.8
3)
A
ge
22
m
on
th
s:
C
ub
e
st
ac
ki
ng
,L
an
gu
ag
e
(�
�
.9
3)
,C
op
yi
ng
de
si
gn
s
te
st
(�
�
.4
6)
Pr
io
r
at
te
nt
io
n
sk
ill
s/
so
ci
oe
m
ot
io
na
l
be
ha
vi
or
C
om
pl
ia
nc
e
(�
�
.5
9)
;
M
at
er
na
l
R
ep
or
t
So
ci
ab
ili
ty
(�
�
.9
3)
:
In
te
rv
ie
w
er
ra
tin
gs
of
ch
ild
’s
co
op
er
at
io
n
du
ri
ng
th
e
as
se
ss
m
en
t
C
B
C
L
,E
xt
er
na
liz
in
g,
in
te
rn
al
iz
in
g:
M
at
er
na
l
R
ep
or
t
Fo
rb
id
de
n
to
y
la
bo
ra
to
ry
ta
sk
:
Im
pu
ls
iv
ity
C
B
C
L
,O
ve
ra
ll
be
ha
vi
or
pr
ob
le
m
s
(�
�
.9
4)
:
M
at
er
na
lR
ep
or
t
A
tte
nt
io
n
(�
�
.9
4)
:
T
es
to
bs
er
ve
r’
s
re
po
rt
C
oo
pe
ra
tio
n:
In
te
rv
ie
w
er
ra
tin
gs
of
ch
ild
’s
co
op
er
at
io
n
du
ri
ng
th
e
as
se
ss
m
en
t
A
ge
22
m
on
th
s:
Pe
rs
on
al
de
ve
lo
pm
en
t
(�
�
.8
2)
N
ot
e.
E
ar
ly
C
hi
ld
ho
od
L
on
gi
tu
di
na
l
St
ud
y-
K
in
de
rg
ar
te
n
C
oh
or
t
(E
C
L
S-
K
)
in
cl
ud
es
a
sc
ho
ol
en
tr
y
te
st
of
ge
ne
ra
l
kn
ow
le
dg
e.
N
at
io
na
l
In
st
itu
te
of
C
hi
ld
H
ea
lth
an
d
H
um
an
D
ev
el
op
m
en
t
St
ud
y
of
E
ar
ly
C
hi
ld
C
ar
e
an
d
Y
ou
th
D
ev
el
op
m
en
t
(N
IC
H
D
SE
C
C
Y
D
)
in
cl
ud
es
ag
e
4.
5
W
J-
R
C
og
ni
tiv
e
A
bi
lit
y
m
ea
su
re
.I
nf
an
t
H
ea
lth
an
d
D
ev
el
op
m
en
t
Pr
og
ra
m
(I
H
D
P)
in
cl
ud
es
ag
e
5
W
ec
hs
le
r
Pr
es
ch
oo
l
an
d
Pr
im
ar
y
Sc
al
e
of
In
te
lli
ge
nc
e
(W
PP
SI
)
Pe
rf
or
m
an
ce
IQ
(�
�
.9
3)
an
d
ag
e
5
A
ch
en
ba
ch
C
hi
ld
B
eh
av
io
r
Pr
of
ile
M
at
er
na
l
R
ep
or
t:
O
ve
ra
ll
be
ha
vi
or
pr
ob
le
m
s
(�
�
.9
3)
.B
ri
tis
h
B
ir
th
C
oh
or
t
St
ud
y
(B
C
S)
in
cl
ud
es
ag
e
5
H
um
an
Fi
gu
re
D
ra
w
in
g,
C
op
yi
ng
D
es
ig
ns
T
es
t,
an
d
Pr
of
ile
D
ra
w
in
g.
R
om
an
nu
m
er
al
s
in
de
x
m
ea
su
re
s
co
ns
is
te
nt
ly
in
T
ab
le
s
1
an
d
2.
N
L
SY
�
N
at
io
na
l
L
on
gi
tu
di
na
l
Su
rv
ey
of
Y
ou
th
;
M
L
E
PS
�
M
on
tr
ea
l
L
on
gi
tu
di
na
l-
E
xp
er
im
en
ta
l
Pr
es
ch
oo
l
St
ud
y.
1433SCHOOL READINESS AND LATER ACHIEVEMENT
attention, another (NICHD SECCYD) has both test-based and
teacher-rated measures of attention, and three (NLSY, IHDP,
and
BCS) have parent rather than teacher reports of socioemotional
behaviors. In addition, two of the studies (NICHD SECCYD and
the Montreal Longitudinal-Experimental Preschool Study
[MLEPS]) measure both attention skills and problems, whereas
three (NLSY, IHDP, and BCS) have measures of attention prob-
lems but not skills, and one study (ECLS-K) has a measure of
attention skills but not attention problems. In addition, with one
exception, all of the studies provide measures of academic,
atten-
tion, and socioemotional skills prior to the point of school
entry,
which we used as key control variables in our analyses.
The Studies and Samples
The Early Childhood Longitudinal Study–Kindergarten Cohort
(ECLS-K). The ECLS-K follows a nationally representative
sam-
ple of 21,260 children who were in kindergarten in 1998–1999.
We used data from kindergarten, first grade, and third grade.
Data
were collected from multiple sources, including direct achieve-
ment tests of children and surveys of parents, teachers, and
school
administrators (see Table 1; National Center for Education
Statis-
tics, 2001).
Achievement tests were administered in the fall of kindergarten
and in the spring of kindergarten, first grade, and third grade.
We
used teacher reports of children’s “approaches to learning”
(which
measure both attention skills and achievement motivation) and
socioemotional behaviors, including internalizing and
externaliz-
ing problems, self-control with peers, and interpersonal skills,
collected in the fall and spring of kindergarten.
The battery of achievement tests given as part of the ECLS-K
kindergarten and first-grade assessments covered three subject
areas: language and literacy, mathematical thinking, and general
knowledge. For third grade, the achievement tests included
math-
ematics, reading, and science. We used item response theory
scores for the first two of these as key dependent variables.
These
third-grade assessments required students to complete
workbooks
and open-ended mathematics problems. As detailed in Appendix
A, a host of family- and some child-level controls are available
in
the data.
The children of the National Longitudinal Survey of Youth
(NLSY). The NLSY is a multistage stratified random sample of
12,686 individuals age 14 to 21 in 1979 (Center for Human
Resource Research, 2004). Black, Hispanic, and low-income
youth
were overrepresented in the sample. Annual (through 1994) and
biennial (between 1994 and 2000) interviews with sample mem-
bers and very low cumulative attrition in the study contribute to
the
quality of the study’s data.
Beginning in 1986, the children born to NLSY female partici-
pants were tracked through biennial mother interview
supplements
and direct child assessments. Given the nature of the sample, it
is
important to note that early cohorts of the child sample were
born
disproportionately to young mothers. With each additional
cohort,
the children become more representative of all children, and
NLSY
children younger than age 14 in 2000 share many demographic
characteristics of their broader set of age mates.
The sample used in the present analysis consists of 1,756 chil-
dren whose academic achievement was tracked from age 7–8 to
age 13–14 and whose achievement and behavior was assessed at
age 5–6. Consequently, our sample comprises children who
were
age 5 or 6 in 1986, 1988, 1990, or 1992. The age 13–14
achieve-
ment and behavior of these children were assessed in the
respec-
tive 1994, 1996, 1998, and 2000 interviews.
School readiness measures, including math and reading test
scores (Peabody Individual Achievement Test; Dunn & Mark-
wardt, 1970) and maternal reports of children’s behavior
problems
(adapted from the Achenbach Behavior Problems Checklist;
Baker, Keck, Mott, & Quinlan, 1993) were collected at age 5 or
age 6. Academic achievement outcome measures were collected
biennially for children between the ages of 5 and 14. In
addition,
key control variables include children’s receptive vocabulary
(Pea-
body Picture Vocabulary Test—Revised; Dunn & Dunn, 1981)
and children’s temperament (compliance and sociability) at age
3
or 4. Additional family- and child-level control variables are
described in Appendix B.
The NICHD Study of Early Child Care and Youth Development
(SECCYD). Longitudinal data from the NICHD SECCYD are
drawn from a multisite study of births in 1991 (NICHD Early
Child Care Research Network, 2005a). Participants were
recruited
from hospitals located at 10 sites across the United States.
During
24-hr sampling periods, 5,265 new mothers met the selection
criteria and agreed to be contacted after returning home from
the
hospital. At 1 month of age, 1,364 healthy newborns were
enrolled
in the study. Although it is not nationally representative, the
study
sample closely matches national and census tract records with
respect to demographic variables such as ethnicity and
household
income. The majority of children in the sample are White, 12%
are
African American, and 11% are Hispanic or of another
ethnicity.
About 30% of mothers had a high school education or less, and
14% were single parents (NICHD Early Child Care Research
Network, 1997). The analysis sample had valid data on the
achievement outcome measures and at least three sources of in-
formation on the key independent variables (approximately 981
at
first grade, 928 at third grade, and 907 at fifth grade).
School readiness measures, including achievement tests and
attention/impulsivity tasks, were administered in a controlled
lab-
oratory setting at age 4.5, and attention problems, aggression,
internalizing behavior, and social skills were measured by
teacher
report in the fall of the kindergarten year. Outcomes at first,
third,
and fifth grades include achievement in math and reading
accord-
ing to teacher ratings and Woodcock–Johnson Tests of Achieve-
ment—Revised test scores (Woodcock & Johnson, 1990; see
Table
1). Key control variables at age 3 include children’s cognitive
ability, language skills, impulsivity, externalizing problems,
and
internalizing problems. The NICHD SECCYD also collects
infor-
mation from infancy about children’s early environments,
includ-
ing child-care type and quality, home environment, and
parenting;
these and other child- and family-level covariates are described
in
Appendix C.
The Infant Health and Development Program (IHDP). The
IHDP is an eight-site randomized clinical trial designed to
evaluate
the efficacy of a comprehensive early-intervention program for
low birth weight (LBW) premature infants. Infants weighing
2,500
g (5.51 lb) or less at birth were screened for eligibility if their
postconceptional age between January and October 1985 was 37
weeks or less and if they were born in one of eight participating
medical institutions. A total of 985 infants was randomly
assigned
either to a medical follow-up only or to a comprehensive early
1434 DUNCAN ET AL.
childhood intervention group immediately following hospital
dis-
charge.
Infants in both the comprehensive early childhood intervention
and medical follow-up only groups participated in a pediatric
follow-up program of periodic medical, developmental, and
famil-
ial assessments from 40 weeks of conceptional age (when they
would have been born if they had been full term) to 36 months
of
age corrected for prematurity. The intervention program, lasting
from hospital discharge until 36 months, consisted of home
visits,
child-care services, and parent group meetings. A coordinated
educational curriculum of learning games and activities was
used
both during home visits and at the center.
The primary analysis group consisted of 985 infants. Of these
985 infants, cognitive assessments are available for 843
children at
age 3, 745 children at age 5, and 787 children at age 8. In
addition,
76 children who were born at an extremely low birth weight
(ELBW; 1,000 g [3.27 lb] or less) were excluded from the
sample
because ELBW children differ markedly from other LBW
children
in cognitive and behavioral functioning (Klebanov, Brooks-
Gunn,
& McCormick, 1994a, 1994b). Thus, this study focuses on a
subsample of 690 children who were not born ELBW and for
whom cognitive assessment and family background data were
available.
Data come from a variety of sources: questionnaires, home
visits, and laboratory tests (see Table 1). School readiness mea-
sures include preschool performance and verbal test scores,
paren-
tal reports of children’s mental health and aggressive behavior,
and
observer reports of children’s attention and task persistence. We
assessed reading and math achievement using the Woodcock–
Johnson Tests of Achievement—Revised broad reading and
math
tests and the Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children—Third
Edition (Wechsler, 1991) performance and verbal tests at 8
years
of age. Key control variables include cognitive ability,
sustained
attention, and behavior problems at age 3. Additional family-
and
child-level control variables are described in Appendix D.
The Montreal Longitudinal-Experimental Preschool Study
(MLEPS). The MLEPS comprises several consecutive cohorts
launched from 1997 to 2000. The original sample of 4- and
5-year-old children (N � 1,928), representing one third of its
population base, was obtained after a multilevel consent process
involving school board administrators, local school committees,
parents, and teachers. Given that its final cohort (2000) does not
meet all the data requirements for the research objective
examined
here, we limited ourselves to the sample of children beginning
kindergarten in the fall of 1998 and the fall of 1999.
Incomplete data reduced the sample from 1,369 to 767 children.
Students in the final sample had a valid value on any of the four
outcome measures of interest (first- and third-grade
achievement
measures) and on at least four of the six socioemotional
measures.
Of the 767 participants in the final sample, 439 began
kindergarten
in 1998 and 328 began kindergarten in the fall of 1999.
Addition-
ally, for 350 of the 767 students, initial data were collected
during
the fall of junior kindergarten (332 who began junior
kindergarten
in 1997 and 18 who began junior kindergarten in 1998).
Initial and follow-up data were collected from multiple sources,
including direct cognitive assessments of children and surveys
of
parents and teachers. Early academic assessments include
individ-
ually administered number knowledge and receptive vocabulary
tests at the end of senior kindergarten. Teachers rated children’s
behavioral development, including physically aggressive,
anxious,
depressive, hyperactive, inattentive, and prosocial behavior.
Third-
grade assessments include a group-administered math test and
teacher ratings of children’s French language skills (see Table
1).
Key control variables include number knowledge and
vocabulary
measured on entry into junior kindergarten (age 4) for Cohort 1
and on entry into senior kindergarten (age 5) for Cohort 2.
Addi-
tional family- and child-level control variables are detailed in
Appendix E.
The 1970 British Birth Cohort Study (BCS). The U.K. 1970
BCS, a nationally representative longitudinal study, has
followed
into adulthood a cohort of children born in Great Britain during
1
week in 1970 (Bynner, Ferri, & Shepherd, 1997). The birth
sample
of 17,196 infants was approximately 97% of the target birth
population. Attrition has reduced the original sample to 11,200
participants. Nevertheless, the representativeness of the original
birth cohort has largely been maintained, although the current
sample is disproportionately female and highly educated (Ferri
&
Smith, 2003). Missing data on key variables reduce the sample
size
for most analyses to between 9,000 and 10,000 cases.
At each wave, cohort members were given a battery of tests of
intellectual and behavioral development (see Table 1). School
readiness measures include vocabulary and copying skills tests,
and maternal reports of attention, externalizing behavior, and
internalizing behavior were collected when the children were 5
years of age. Reading and mathematics achievement tests were
administered at age 10. Key control variables include measures
of
basic skills and behavior at ages 22 and 42 months for a 10%
subsample of the data. Additional family- and child-level
controls
are described in Appendix F.
PAGE  Running head School Readiness and Later Achievement1.docx
PAGE  Running head School Readiness and Later Achievement1.docx
PAGE  Running head School Readiness and Later Achievement1.docx
PAGE  Running head School Readiness and Later Achievement1.docx
PAGE  Running head School Readiness and Later Achievement1.docx
PAGE  Running head School Readiness and Later Achievement1.docx
PAGE  Running head School Readiness and Later Achievement1.docx
PAGE  Running head School Readiness and Later Achievement1.docx
PAGE  Running head School Readiness and Later Achievement1.docx
PAGE  Running head School Readiness and Later Achievement1.docx
PAGE  Running head School Readiness and Later Achievement1.docx
PAGE  Running head School Readiness and Later Achievement1.docx
PAGE  Running head School Readiness and Later Achievement1.docx
PAGE  Running head School Readiness and Later Achievement1.docx
PAGE  Running head School Readiness and Later Achievement1.docx
PAGE  Running head School Readiness and Later Achievement1.docx
PAGE  Running head School Readiness and Later Achievement1.docx
PAGE  Running head School Readiness and Later Achievement1.docx
PAGE  Running head School Readiness and Later Achievement1.docx
PAGE  Running head School Readiness and Later Achievement1.docx
PAGE  Running head School Readiness and Later Achievement1.docx
PAGE  Running head School Readiness and Later Achievement1.docx
PAGE  Running head School Readiness and Later Achievement1.docx
PAGE  Running head School Readiness and Later Achievement1.docx
PAGE  Running head School Readiness and Later Achievement1.docx
PAGE  Running head School Readiness and Later Achievement1.docx
PAGE  Running head School Readiness and Later Achievement1.docx
PAGE  Running head School Readiness and Later Achievement1.docx
PAGE  Running head School Readiness and Later Achievement1.docx
PAGE  Running head School Readiness and Later Achievement1.docx
PAGE  Running head School Readiness and Later Achievement1.docx
PAGE  Running head School Readiness and Later Achievement1.docx
PAGE  Running head School Readiness and Later Achievement1.docx
PAGE  Running head School Readiness and Later Achievement1.docx
PAGE  Running head School Readiness and Later Achievement1.docx
PAGE  Running head School Readiness and Later Achievement1.docx
PAGE  Running head School Readiness and Later Achievement1.docx
PAGE  Running head School Readiness and Later Achievement1.docx
PAGE  Running head School Readiness and Later Achievement1.docx
PAGE  Running head School Readiness and Later Achievement1.docx
PAGE  Running head School Readiness and Later Achievement1.docx
PAGE  Running head School Readiness and Later Achievement1.docx
PAGE  Running head School Readiness and Later Achievement1.docx
PAGE  Running head School Readiness and Later Achievement1.docx
PAGE  Running head School Readiness and Later Achievement1.docx
PAGE  Running head School Readiness and Later Achievement1.docx
PAGE  Running head School Readiness and Later Achievement1.docx
PAGE  Running head School Readiness and Later Achievement1.docx
PAGE  Running head School Readiness and Later Achievement1.docx
PAGE  Running head School Readiness and Later Achievement1.docx
PAGE  Running head School Readiness and Later Achievement1.docx
PAGE  Running head School Readiness and Later Achievement1.docx
PAGE  Running head School Readiness and Later Achievement1.docx
PAGE  Running head School Readiness and Later Achievement1.docx
PAGE  Running head School Readiness and Later Achievement1.docx
PAGE  Running head School Readiness and Later Achievement1.docx
PAGE  Running head School Readiness and Later Achievement1.docx
PAGE  Running head School Readiness and Later Achievement1.docx
PAGE  Running head School Readiness and Later Achievement1.docx
PAGE  Running head School Readiness and Later Achievement1.docx
PAGE  Running head School Readiness and Later Achievement1.docx
PAGE  Running head School Readiness and Later Achievement1.docx
PAGE  Running head School Readiness and Later Achievement1.docx
PAGE  Running head School Readiness and Later Achievement1.docx
PAGE  Running head School Readiness and Later Achievement1.docx
PAGE  Running head School Readiness and Later Achievement1.docx
PAGE  Running head School Readiness and Later Achievement1.docx
PAGE  Running head School Readiness and Later Achievement1.docx
PAGE  Running head School Readiness and Later Achievement1.docx
PAGE  Running head School Readiness and Later Achievement1.docx
PAGE  Running head School Readiness and Later Achievement1.docx
PAGE  Running head School Readiness and Later Achievement1.docx
PAGE  Running head School Readiness and Later Achievement1.docx
PAGE  Running head School Readiness and Later Achievement1.docx
PAGE  Running head School Readiness and Later Achievement1.docx
PAGE  Running head School Readiness and Later Achievement1.docx
PAGE  Running head School Readiness and Later Achievement1.docx
PAGE  Running head School Readiness and Later Achievement1.docx
PAGE  Running head School Readiness and Later Achievement1.docx
PAGE  Running head School Readiness and Later Achievement1.docx
PAGE  Running head School Readiness and Later Achievement1.docx
PAGE  Running head School Readiness and Later Achievement1.docx
PAGE  Running head School Readiness and Later Achievement1.docx
PAGE  Running head School Readiness and Later Achievement1.docx
PAGE  Running head School Readiness and Later Achievement1.docx
PAGE  Running head School Readiness and Later Achievement1.docx
PAGE  Running head School Readiness and Later Achievement1.docx
PAGE  Running head School Readiness and Later Achievement1.docx
PAGE  Running head School Readiness and Later Achievement1.docx
PAGE  Running head School Readiness and Later Achievement1.docx
PAGE  Running head School Readiness and Later Achievement1.docx
PAGE  Running head School Readiness and Later Achievement1.docx
PAGE  Running head School Readiness and Later Achievement1.docx
PAGE  Running head School Readiness and Later Achievement1.docx
PAGE  Running head School Readiness and Later Achievement1.docx
PAGE  Running head School Readiness and Later Achievement1.docx
PAGE  Running head School Readiness and Later Achievement1.docx
PAGE  Running head School Readiness and Later Achievement1.docx
PAGE  Running head School Readiness and Later Achievement1.docx
PAGE  Running head School Readiness and Later Achievement1.docx
PAGE  Running head School Readiness and Later Achievement1.docx
PAGE  Running head School Readiness and Later Achievement1.docx
PAGE  Running head School Readiness and Later Achievement1.docx
PAGE  Running head School Readiness and Later Achievement1.docx
PAGE  Running head School Readiness and Later Achievement1.docx
PAGE  Running head School Readiness and Later Achievement1.docx
PAGE  Running head School Readiness and Later Achievement1.docx
PAGE  Running head School Readiness and Later Achievement1.docx
PAGE  Running head School Readiness and Later Achievement1.docx
PAGE  Running head School Readiness and Later Achievement1.docx
PAGE  Running head School Readiness and Later Achievement1.docx
PAGE  Running head School Readiness and Later Achievement1.docx
PAGE  Running head School Readiness and Later Achievement1.docx
PAGE  Running head School Readiness and Later Achievement1.docx
PAGE  Running head School Readiness and Later Achievement1.docx
PAGE  Running head School Readiness and Later Achievement1.docx
PAGE  Running head School Readiness and Later Achievement1.docx
PAGE  Running head School Readiness and Later Achievement1.docx
PAGE  Running head School Readiness and Later Achievement1.docx
PAGE  Running head School Readiness and Later Achievement1.docx
PAGE  Running head School Readiness and Later Achievement1.docx
PAGE  Running head School Readiness and Later Achievement1.docx
PAGE  Running head School Readiness and Later Achievement1.docx
PAGE  Running head School Readiness and Later Achievement1.docx
PAGE  Running head School Readiness and Later Achievement1.docx
PAGE  Running head School Readiness and Later Achievement1.docx
PAGE  Running head School Readiness and Later Achievement1.docx
PAGE  Running head School Readiness and Later Achievement1.docx
PAGE  Running head School Readiness and Later Achievement1.docx
PAGE  Running head School Readiness and Later Achievement1.docx
PAGE  Running head School Readiness and Later Achievement1.docx
PAGE  Running head School Readiness and Later Achievement1.docx

More Related Content

Similar to PAGE Running head School Readiness and Later Achievement1.docx

IntroductionIt is not unusual for the teachers of students wit.docx
IntroductionIt is not unusual for the teachers of students wit.docxIntroductionIt is not unusual for the teachers of students wit.docx
IntroductionIt is not unusual for the teachers of students wit.docx
mariuse18nolet
 
Running head EDUCATIONAL RESEARCH 1ED.docx
Running head EDUCATIONAL RESEARCH                      1ED.docxRunning head EDUCATIONAL RESEARCH                      1ED.docx
Running head EDUCATIONAL RESEARCH 1ED.docx
todd271
 
INTRODUCTIONModule 3 Week 6 The Purpose StatementIn thi
INTRODUCTIONModule 3 Week 6 The Purpose StatementIn thiINTRODUCTIONModule 3 Week 6 The Purpose StatementIn thi
INTRODUCTIONModule 3 Week 6 The Purpose StatementIn thi
TatianaMajor22
 
The value of integration2
The value of integration2The value of integration2
The value of integration2
huutrinh
 
Final Using Peer Tutoring Strategies to Increase Mathematic Achievement
Final Using Peer Tutoring Strategies to Increase Mathematic AchievementFinal Using Peer Tutoring Strategies to Increase Mathematic Achievement
Final Using Peer Tutoring Strategies to Increase Mathematic Achievement
Beth Csiszer
 
An Analysis of Teachers’ Pedagogical Content Knowledge on Number Sense Learni...
An Analysis of Teachers’ Pedagogical Content Knowledge on Number Sense Learni...An Analysis of Teachers’ Pedagogical Content Knowledge on Number Sense Learni...
An Analysis of Teachers’ Pedagogical Content Knowledge on Number Sense Learni...
AJHSSR Journal
 
Professional Journal PowerPoint
Professional Journal PowerPointProfessional Journal PowerPoint
Professional Journal PowerPoint
nqueisi
 
Inquiry based teaching
Inquiry based teaching Inquiry based teaching
Inquiry based teaching
Silas Spence
 

Similar to PAGE Running head School Readiness and Later Achievement1.docx (20)

IntroductionIt is not unusual for the teachers of students wit.docx
IntroductionIt is not unusual for the teachers of students wit.docxIntroductionIt is not unusual for the teachers of students wit.docx
IntroductionIt is not unusual for the teachers of students wit.docx
 
Running head EDUCATIONAL RESEARCH 1ED.docx
Running head EDUCATIONAL RESEARCH                      1ED.docxRunning head EDUCATIONAL RESEARCH                      1ED.docx
Running head EDUCATIONAL RESEARCH 1ED.docx
 
INTRODUCTIONModule 3 Week 6 The Purpose StatementIn thi
INTRODUCTIONModule 3 Week 6 The Purpose StatementIn thiINTRODUCTIONModule 3 Week 6 The Purpose StatementIn thi
INTRODUCTIONModule 3 Week 6 The Purpose StatementIn thi
 
Problem Centered Approach
Problem Centered ApproachProblem Centered Approach
Problem Centered Approach
 
The value of integration2
The value of integration2The value of integration2
The value of integration2
 
Melody - compreensão na leitura- INGLÊS
Melody -  compreensão na leitura- INGLÊSMelody -  compreensão na leitura- INGLÊS
Melody - compreensão na leitura- INGLÊS
 
e12tim33
e12tim33e12tim33
e12tim33
 
Analytical Term Project
Analytical Term ProjectAnalytical Term Project
Analytical Term Project
 
Reading Book Report 1. Chapters 1 and 2
Reading Book Report 1. Chapters 1 and 2Reading Book Report 1. Chapters 1 and 2
Reading Book Report 1. Chapters 1 and 2
 
Intro to Science teacher guide
Intro to Science teacher guide Intro to Science teacher guide
Intro to Science teacher guide
 
A Planning And Problem-Solving Preschool Model The Methodology Of Being A Go...
A Planning And Problem-Solving Preschool Model  The Methodology Of Being A Go...A Planning And Problem-Solving Preschool Model  The Methodology Of Being A Go...
A Planning And Problem-Solving Preschool Model The Methodology Of Being A Go...
 
Final Using Peer Tutoring Strategies to Increase Mathematic Achievement
Final Using Peer Tutoring Strategies to Increase Mathematic AchievementFinal Using Peer Tutoring Strategies to Increase Mathematic Achievement
Final Using Peer Tutoring Strategies to Increase Mathematic Achievement
 
Four families-of-teaching
Four families-of-teachingFour families-of-teaching
Four families-of-teaching
 
Put Understanding First.pdf
Put Understanding First.pdfPut Understanding First.pdf
Put Understanding First.pdf
 
Pods write up
Pods write up Pods write up
Pods write up
 
Some methods of effective teaching and learning of mathematics
Some methods of effective teaching and learning of mathematicsSome methods of effective teaching and learning of mathematics
Some methods of effective teaching and learning of mathematics
 
An Analysis of Teachers’ Pedagogical Content Knowledge on Number Sense Learni...
An Analysis of Teachers’ Pedagogical Content Knowledge on Number Sense Learni...An Analysis of Teachers’ Pedagogical Content Knowledge on Number Sense Learni...
An Analysis of Teachers’ Pedagogical Content Knowledge on Number Sense Learni...
 
Professional Journal PowerPoint
Professional Journal PowerPointProfessional Journal PowerPoint
Professional Journal PowerPoint
 
Inquiry based teaching
Inquiry based teaching Inquiry based teaching
Inquiry based teaching
 
Transition
TransitionTransition
Transition
 

More from alfred4lewis58146

For this assignment, select a human service organization from .docx
For this assignment, select a human service organization from .docxFor this assignment, select a human service organization from .docx
For this assignment, select a human service organization from .docx
alfred4lewis58146
 
For this assignment, download the A6 code pack. This zip fil.docx
For this assignment, download the A6 code pack. This zip fil.docxFor this assignment, download the A6 code pack. This zip fil.docx
For this assignment, download the A6 code pack. This zip fil.docx
alfred4lewis58146
 
For the shortanswer questions,you will need to respo.docx
For the shortanswer questions,you will need to respo.docxFor the shortanswer questions,you will need to respo.docx
For the shortanswer questions,you will need to respo.docx
alfred4lewis58146
 
For the project, you will be expected to apply the key concepts of p.docx
For the project, you will be expected to apply the key concepts of p.docxFor the project, you will be expected to apply the key concepts of p.docx
For the project, you will be expected to apply the key concepts of p.docx
alfred4lewis58146
 
For the past several weeks you have addressed several different area.docx
For the past several weeks you have addressed several different area.docxFor the past several weeks you have addressed several different area.docx
For the past several weeks you have addressed several different area.docx
alfred4lewis58146
 

More from alfred4lewis58146 (20)

For this assignment, students will need to observe the activities th.docx
For this assignment, students will need to observe the activities th.docxFor this assignment, students will need to observe the activities th.docx
For this assignment, students will need to observe the activities th.docx
 
For this assignment, select a human service organization from .docx
For this assignment, select a human service organization from .docxFor this assignment, select a human service organization from .docx
For this assignment, select a human service organization from .docx
 
For this Assignment, read the case study for Claudia and find tw.docx
For this Assignment, read the case study for Claudia and find tw.docxFor this Assignment, read the case study for Claudia and find tw.docx
For this Assignment, read the case study for Claudia and find tw.docx
 
For this assignment, download the A6 code pack. This zip fil.docx
For this assignment, download the A6 code pack. This zip fil.docxFor this assignment, download the A6 code pack. This zip fil.docx
For this assignment, download the A6 code pack. This zip fil.docx
 
For this assignment, create infographic using the Canva website..docx
For this assignment, create infographic using the Canva website..docxFor this assignment, create infographic using the Canva website..docx
For this assignment, create infographic using the Canva website..docx
 
For this assignment, compare  California during the Great Depression.docx
For this assignment, compare  California during the Great Depression.docxFor this assignment, compare  California during the Great Depression.docx
For this assignment, compare  California during the Great Depression.docx
 
For this assignment, create a 10- to 12-slide presentation in Mi.docx
For this assignment, create a 10- to 12-slide presentation in Mi.docxFor this assignment, create a 10- to 12-slide presentation in Mi.docx
For this assignment, create a 10- to 12-slide presentation in Mi.docx
 
For this assignment, begin by reading chapters 12-15 in Dr. Bells t.docx
For this assignment, begin by reading chapters 12-15 in Dr. Bells t.docxFor this assignment, begin by reading chapters 12-15 in Dr. Bells t.docx
For this assignment, begin by reading chapters 12-15 in Dr. Bells t.docx
 
For this assignment, assume you are the new Secretary of Homelan.docx
For this assignment, assume you are the new Secretary of Homelan.docxFor this assignment, assume you are the new Secretary of Homelan.docx
For this assignment, assume you are the new Secretary of Homelan.docx
 
For this assignment, address the following promptsIntroductor.docx
For this assignment, address the following promptsIntroductor.docxFor this assignment, address the following promptsIntroductor.docx
For this assignment, address the following promptsIntroductor.docx
 
For this assignment, analyze the play by focusing on one of the .docx
For this assignment, analyze the play by focusing on one of the .docxFor this assignment, analyze the play by focusing on one of the .docx
For this assignment, analyze the play by focusing on one of the .docx
 
For this assignment I would like you to answer these questions.docx
For this assignment I would like you to answer these questions.docxFor this assignment I would like you to answer these questions.docx
For this assignment I would like you to answer these questions.docx
 
For the Weekly Reports I need 2 reports. For the First two weeks the.docx
For the Weekly Reports I need 2 reports. For the First two weeks the.docxFor the Weekly Reports I need 2 reports. For the First two weeks the.docx
For the Weekly Reports I need 2 reports. For the First two weeks the.docx
 
For the shortanswer questions,you will need to respo.docx
For the shortanswer questions,you will need to respo.docxFor the shortanswer questions,you will need to respo.docx
For the shortanswer questions,you will need to respo.docx
 
For the sake of argument (this essay in particular), lets prete.docx
For the sake of argument (this essay in particular), lets prete.docxFor the sake of argument (this essay in particular), lets prete.docx
For the sake of argument (this essay in particular), lets prete.docx
 
For the proposal, each student must describe an interface they a.docx
For the proposal, each student must describe an interface they a.docxFor the proposal, each student must describe an interface they a.docx
For the proposal, each student must describe an interface they a.docx
 
For the project, you will be expected to apply the key concepts of p.docx
For the project, you will be expected to apply the key concepts of p.docxFor the project, you will be expected to apply the key concepts of p.docx
For the project, you will be expected to apply the key concepts of p.docx
 
For the past several weeks you have addressed several different area.docx
For the past several weeks you have addressed several different area.docxFor the past several weeks you have addressed several different area.docx
For the past several weeks you have addressed several different area.docx
 
For the Mash it Up assignment, we experimented with different ways t.docx
For the Mash it Up assignment, we experimented with different ways t.docxFor the Mash it Up assignment, we experimented with different ways t.docx
For the Mash it Up assignment, we experimented with different ways t.docx
 
For the first time in modern history, the world is experiencing a he.docx
For the first time in modern history, the world is experiencing a he.docxFor the first time in modern history, the world is experiencing a he.docx
For the first time in modern history, the world is experiencing a he.docx
 

Recently uploaded

SPLICE Working Group: Reusable Code Examples
SPLICE Working Group:Reusable Code ExamplesSPLICE Working Group:Reusable Code Examples
SPLICE Working Group: Reusable Code Examples
Peter Brusilovsky
 
Personalisation of Education by AI and Big Data - Lourdes Guàrdia
Personalisation of Education by AI and Big Data - Lourdes GuàrdiaPersonalisation of Education by AI and Big Data - Lourdes Guàrdia
Personalisation of Education by AI and Big Data - Lourdes Guàrdia
EADTU
 
Orientation Canvas Course Presentation.pdf
Orientation Canvas Course Presentation.pdfOrientation Canvas Course Presentation.pdf
Orientation Canvas Course Presentation.pdf
Elizabeth Walsh
 
QUATER-1-PE-HEALTH-LC2- this is just a sample of unpacked lesson
QUATER-1-PE-HEALTH-LC2- this is just a sample of unpacked lessonQUATER-1-PE-HEALTH-LC2- this is just a sample of unpacked lesson
QUATER-1-PE-HEALTH-LC2- this is just a sample of unpacked lesson
httgc7rh9c
 
Transparency, Recognition and the role of eSealing - Ildiko Mazar and Koen No...
Transparency, Recognition and the role of eSealing - Ildiko Mazar and Koen No...Transparency, Recognition and the role of eSealing - Ildiko Mazar and Koen No...
Transparency, Recognition and the role of eSealing - Ildiko Mazar and Koen No...
EADTU
 
MuleSoft Integration with AWS Textract | Calling AWS Textract API |AWS - Clou...
MuleSoft Integration with AWS Textract | Calling AWS Textract API |AWS - Clou...MuleSoft Integration with AWS Textract | Calling AWS Textract API |AWS - Clou...
MuleSoft Integration with AWS Textract | Calling AWS Textract API |AWS - Clou...
MysoreMuleSoftMeetup
 

Recently uploaded (20)

UGC NET Paper 1 Unit 7 DATA INTERPRETATION.pdf
UGC NET Paper 1 Unit 7 DATA INTERPRETATION.pdfUGC NET Paper 1 Unit 7 DATA INTERPRETATION.pdf
UGC NET Paper 1 Unit 7 DATA INTERPRETATION.pdf
 
HMCS Vancouver Pre-Deployment Brief - May 2024 (Web Version).pptx
HMCS Vancouver Pre-Deployment Brief - May 2024 (Web Version).pptxHMCS Vancouver Pre-Deployment Brief - May 2024 (Web Version).pptx
HMCS Vancouver Pre-Deployment Brief - May 2024 (Web Version).pptx
 
How to setup Pycharm environment for Odoo 17.pptx
How to setup Pycharm environment for Odoo 17.pptxHow to setup Pycharm environment for Odoo 17.pptx
How to setup Pycharm environment for Odoo 17.pptx
 
Unit 3 Emotional Intelligence and Spiritual Intelligence.pdf
Unit 3 Emotional Intelligence and Spiritual Intelligence.pdfUnit 3 Emotional Intelligence and Spiritual Intelligence.pdf
Unit 3 Emotional Intelligence and Spiritual Intelligence.pdf
 
21st_Century_Skills_Framework_Final_Presentation_2.pptx
21st_Century_Skills_Framework_Final_Presentation_2.pptx21st_Century_Skills_Framework_Final_Presentation_2.pptx
21st_Century_Skills_Framework_Final_Presentation_2.pptx
 
SPLICE Working Group: Reusable Code Examples
SPLICE Working Group:Reusable Code ExamplesSPLICE Working Group:Reusable Code Examples
SPLICE Working Group: Reusable Code Examples
 
FSB Advising Checklist - Orientation 2024
FSB Advising Checklist - Orientation 2024FSB Advising Checklist - Orientation 2024
FSB Advising Checklist - Orientation 2024
 
NO1 Top Black Magic Specialist In Lahore Black magic In Pakistan Kala Ilam Ex...
NO1 Top Black Magic Specialist In Lahore Black magic In Pakistan Kala Ilam Ex...NO1 Top Black Magic Specialist In Lahore Black magic In Pakistan Kala Ilam Ex...
NO1 Top Black Magic Specialist In Lahore Black magic In Pakistan Kala Ilam Ex...
 
FICTIONAL SALESMAN/SALESMAN SNSW 2024.pdf
FICTIONAL SALESMAN/SALESMAN SNSW 2024.pdfFICTIONAL SALESMAN/SALESMAN SNSW 2024.pdf
FICTIONAL SALESMAN/SALESMAN SNSW 2024.pdf
 
Personalisation of Education by AI and Big Data - Lourdes Guàrdia
Personalisation of Education by AI and Big Data - Lourdes GuàrdiaPersonalisation of Education by AI and Big Data - Lourdes Guàrdia
Personalisation of Education by AI and Big Data - Lourdes Guàrdia
 
Observing-Correct-Grammar-in-Making-Definitions.pptx
Observing-Correct-Grammar-in-Making-Definitions.pptxObserving-Correct-Grammar-in-Making-Definitions.pptx
Observing-Correct-Grammar-in-Making-Definitions.pptx
 
Play hard learn harder: The Serious Business of Play
Play hard learn harder:  The Serious Business of PlayPlay hard learn harder:  The Serious Business of Play
Play hard learn harder: The Serious Business of Play
 
Wellbeing inclusion and digital dystopias.pptx
Wellbeing inclusion and digital dystopias.pptxWellbeing inclusion and digital dystopias.pptx
Wellbeing inclusion and digital dystopias.pptx
 
How to Add New Custom Addons Path in Odoo 17
How to Add New Custom Addons Path in Odoo 17How to Add New Custom Addons Path in Odoo 17
How to Add New Custom Addons Path in Odoo 17
 
COMMUNICATING NEGATIVE NEWS - APPROACHES .pptx
COMMUNICATING NEGATIVE NEWS - APPROACHES .pptxCOMMUNICATING NEGATIVE NEWS - APPROACHES .pptx
COMMUNICATING NEGATIVE NEWS - APPROACHES .pptx
 
diagnosting testing bsc 2nd sem.pptx....
diagnosting testing bsc 2nd sem.pptx....diagnosting testing bsc 2nd sem.pptx....
diagnosting testing bsc 2nd sem.pptx....
 
Orientation Canvas Course Presentation.pdf
Orientation Canvas Course Presentation.pdfOrientation Canvas Course Presentation.pdf
Orientation Canvas Course Presentation.pdf
 
QUATER-1-PE-HEALTH-LC2- this is just a sample of unpacked lesson
QUATER-1-PE-HEALTH-LC2- this is just a sample of unpacked lessonQUATER-1-PE-HEALTH-LC2- this is just a sample of unpacked lesson
QUATER-1-PE-HEALTH-LC2- this is just a sample of unpacked lesson
 
Transparency, Recognition and the role of eSealing - Ildiko Mazar and Koen No...
Transparency, Recognition and the role of eSealing - Ildiko Mazar and Koen No...Transparency, Recognition and the role of eSealing - Ildiko Mazar and Koen No...
Transparency, Recognition and the role of eSealing - Ildiko Mazar and Koen No...
 
MuleSoft Integration with AWS Textract | Calling AWS Textract API |AWS - Clou...
MuleSoft Integration with AWS Textract | Calling AWS Textract API |AWS - Clou...MuleSoft Integration with AWS Textract | Calling AWS Textract API |AWS - Clou...
MuleSoft Integration with AWS Textract | Calling AWS Textract API |AWS - Clou...
 

PAGE Running head School Readiness and Later Achievement1.docx

  • 1. PAGE Running head: School Readiness and Later Achievement 1 School Readiness and Later Achievement School Name Course: Date: 13th March 2013 School Readiness and Later Achievement The linkages between school-entry academic, attention, and socioemotional skills and later school reading and math achievement suggest that math skills have the greatest predictive power, followed by reading and then attention skills (Japel, 2007). In contrast, socioemotional behaviors, including internalizing and externalizing problems and social skills, are insignificant predictors of later academic performance, even among children with relatively high levels of problem behavior (Japel, 2007). These patterns are usually associated with boys and girls from children from low and high socioeconomic backgrounds. Assessing “Research-Based” Curricula Most early childhood programs are being asked to choose curricula that are “research based.” This requirement is the result of increased attention to children’s academic needs as they enter kindergarten, and has the potential of improving our delivery of curricula to young children (Japel, 2007). However, the meaning of “research based” has not been delineated. Therefore, most publishers of curricula for young children have adopted the language, and identify their programs as “research based.” A careful analysis of the underlying research of three important math curricula could help practitioners make more informed choices. This analysis will also provide a list of
  • 2. criteria for selection of other early childhood curricula, which will require practitioners to take a brief look at the type of research that purports to provide the research base for the curricula (Japel, 2007). For Example, the Pre-K Mathematics is a scripted math program for four year olds. Its primary goal has been to close the gap in math achievement between low-income children and middle class children. Much research has documented this gap, which exists as children enter school and grows as children progress through school. Such kind of research demonstrates years of careful research that their curriculum can start to close this gap. Pre-K Mathematics has a clearly delineated scope and sequence. The scope and sequence is carefully connected to the development of mathematical concepts that are needed in formal math education in elementary school, concepts that low-income children often lack. The lessons are designed to be presented to very small groups of children for short periods of time. The lessons are supported with daily math activities that are plentiful in the children’s environment. These researchers have many years’ experience and long list of published research that document achievement gaps, math concept development, and demonstration projects in math achievement. Conclusion The research-based curricula described here have the potential to provide preschoolers with a math curriculum that can prepare them for the more structured lessons of elementary school. Most professionals agree that a curriculum is only the beginning of the process. Children also need teachers who are sensitive, responsive, and knowledgeable about development and the concepts that children need to be successful in kindergarten. Therefore, teachers who have a deep gratitude for developmentally appropriate practices can be able to employ these curricula to the advantage of the children in their
  • 3. classrooms. References Japel, Crista (2007). School Readiness and Later Achievement. Developmental Psychology., Vol. 43, No. 6, 1428 –1446 PAGE Running head: Assessing “Research-Based” Curricula 1 Assessing “Research-Based” Curricula School Name Course: Date: 13th March 2013 Assessing “Research-Based” Curricula The linkages between school-entry academic, attention, and socioemotional skills and later school reading and math achievement suggest that math skills have the greatest predictive power, followed by reading and then attention skills (Japel, 2007). In contrast, socioemotional behaviors, including internalizing and externalizing problems and social skills, are insignificant predictors of later academic performance, even among children with relatively high levels of problem behavior (Japel, 2007). These patterns are usually associated with boys and girls from children from low and high socioeconomic backgrounds. Assessing “Research-Based” Curricula Most early childhood programs are being asked to choose curricula that are “research based.” This requirement is the result of increased attention to children’s academic needs as
  • 4. they enter kindergarten, and has the potential of improving our delivery of curricula to young children (Japel, 2007). However, the meaning of “research based” has not been delineated. Therefore, most publishers of curricula for young children have adopted the language, and identify their programs as “research based.” A careful analysis of the underlying research of three important math curricula could help practitioners make more informed choices. This analysis will also provide a list of criteria for selection of other early childhood curricula, which will require practitioners to take a brief look at the type of research that purports to provide the research base for the curricula (Japel, 2007). Pre-K Mathematics (Klein, A., Starkey, P., and Ramirez, M., 2003) is a scripted math program for four year olds. Its primary goal has been to close the gap in math achievement between low-income children and middle class children. Much research has documented this gap, which exists as children enter school and grows as children progress through school. These researchers have demonstrated through several years of careful research that their curriculum can begin to close this gap (Starkey, Klein, and Wakeley, 2004). Pre-K Mathematics has a clearly delineated scope and sequence. The scope and sequence is carefully connected to the development of mathematical concepts that are needed in formal math education in elementary school, concepts that low-income children often lack. The lessons are designed to be presented to very small groups of children for short periods of time. The lessons are supported with daily math activities that are plentiful in the children’s environment. These researchers have many years’ experience and long list of published research that document achievement gaps, math concept development, and demonstration projects in math achievement. Starkey, Klein and Wakeley (2004) have field-tested this curriculum for at least five years. The classroom teachers in their studies have had continuous training, close supervision, and much success with children. In
  • 5. one year of instruction children using the Pre-K Mathematics Program have nearly closed the conceptual gap between themselves and middle class children entering kindergarten (Starkey, Klein and Wakeley, 2004). Conclusion The research-based curricula described here have the potential to provide preschoolers with a math curriculum that can prepare them for the more structured lessons of elementary school. Most professionals agree that a curriculum is only the beginning of the process. Children also need teachers who are sensitive, responsive, and knowledgeable about development and the concepts that children need to be successful in kindergarten. Teachers who have a deep appreciation for developmentally appropriate practices will be able to employ these curricula to the advantage of the children in their classes (Bredekamp & Copple, 1997). References Klein, A., Starkey, P., and Ramirez, A. (2003). Pre-K Mathematics Curriculum. Glendale, Il: ScottForesman. Starkey, P., Klein, A. and Wakeley, D. (2004). Enahancing young children’s mathematical knowledge through a pre-kindergarten mathematics intervention. Early childhood research Quarterly, 19, 99-120. Japel, Crista (2007). School Readiness and Later Achievement. Developmental Psychology., Vol. 43, No. 6, 1428 –1446
  • 6. PAGE Running head: TYPE ABBREVIATED TITLE HERE 1 Title of the Research Proposal in Full Goes Here Group Names Go Here Southwestern Oregon Community College HDFS 247 Preschool Child Development Date Title of the Paper Double space, indent each paragraph l ½ inch, and start typing. Your introduction does not have a heading – just the title of the paper. You will need to write a thesis statement in your introduction, the main idea of a paper. Click here to visit a website about thesis statements. Once you’ve developed the thesis, you can then begin composing your introduction. Click here for information on writing introductions. Part One: Hypothesis This will be the beginning of the body of the essay. Even though it has a new heading, you want to make sure you link this to your previous section so your reader can follow and understand. Remember to make sure your first sentence in each paragraph both transitions from your previous paragraph and summarizes the main point in your paragraph. Stick to one topic per paragraph, and avoid long paragraphs so you can hold the reader’s attention. A paragraph should be a minimum of three sentences. In this section you will address: What is your hypothesis? Research questions you want to answer? LOGICAL RELATIONSHIP
  • 7. TRANSITIONAL EXPRESSION Similarity also, in the same way, just as … so too, likewise, similarly Exception/Contrast but, however, in spite of, on the one hand … on the other hand, nevertheless, nonetheless, notwithstanding, in contrast, on the contrary, still, yet Sequence/Order first, second, third, … next, then, finally Time after, afterward, at last, before, currently, during, earlier, immediately, later, meanwhile, now, recently, simultaneously, subsequently, then Example for example, for instance, namely, specifically, to illustrate Emphasis even, indeed, in fact, of course, truly Place/Position above, adjacent, below, beyond, here, in front, in back, nearby, there Cause and Effect accordingly, consequently, hence, so, therefore, thus Additional Support or Evidence additionally, again, also, and, as well, besides, equally important, further, furthermore, in addition, moreover, then Conclusion / summary finally, in a word, in brief, briefly, in conclusion, in the end, in the final analysis, on the whole, thus, to conclude, to summarize, in sum, to sum up, in summary From http://writingcenter.unc.edu/handouts/transitions/ Next section Here, answering the next question or questions from the list It I OK to combine questions from the list below into sections, but just be sure that each section heading clearly labels what the section contains. For Example, it might say: Type of Resaearch and Population. The next section could include
  • 8. Sample, Groups, etc. Here is the list of what needs to be included in your proposal What is your hypothesis? Research questions you want to answer? What type of research will you use to answer the research questions? (Quantitative or qualitative? Experiment, case study, longitudinal, etc.?) What is your population? What is your sample? Define your experimental group. Define your control group. How will you assign participants to groups? What is your independent variable? What is your dependent variable? How will you collect data? How will you operationalize these terms? Are there any confounds or nuisance variables operating? What ethical concerns may be present to consider in doing this study? Another Heading…. And so forth until the conclusion. Conclusion Your conclusion section should recap the major points you have
  • 9. made in your work. Click here for information on writing a conclusion. References Berger, R., Miller, A., Seifer, R., Cares, S., & Lebourgeois, M. (2012). Acute sleep restriction effects on emotion responses in 30- to 36-month-old children. Journal Of Sleep Research, 21(3), 235-246. doi:10.1111/j.1365-2869.2011.00962.x Feak, C. and Swales, J. (2009). Telling a research story: Writing a literature review. Ann Arbor, MI: University of Michigan Press. Kavanagh, K., Absalom, K., Beil, W., & Schliessmann, L. (2008). Connecting and becoming culturally competent: A Lakota example. Advances in Nursing Science, 21, 9-31. Retrieved March 26, 2012 from ProQuest/Nursing Journals database. Learn how to write a review of literature. (n.d.). Retrieved September 12, 2012 from The Writer’s Handbook Website. site: http://writing.wisc.edu/Handbook/ReviewofLiterature.html "Review." The Oxford Pocket Dictionary of Current English. 2009. Retrieved September 18, 2012 from Encyclopedia.com: http://www.encyclopedia.com/doc/1O999-review.html
  • 10. Minnesota School Readiness Study: Developmental Assessment at Kindergarten Entrance Fall 2010
  • 11. Acknowledgements Minnesota School Readiness Study: Developmental Assessment at Kindergarten Entrance The Minnesota School Readiness Study: Developmental Assessment at Kindergarten Entrance Fall 2010 was planned, implemented, and the report prepared by the Minnesota Department of Education (MDE). Special thanks to the 108 elementary schools involved in the study, their principals, kindergarten teachers, support staff and superintendents. The observation and collection of developmental information by teachers on kindergarten children in the classroom was essential to the study and is much appreciated. All analyses in this report were conducted by the Human Capital Research Collaborative (HCRC), a partnership between the University of Minnesota and the Federal Reserve
  • 12. Bank of Minneapolis. For more information, contact Avisia Whiteman at [email protected] or 651-582-8329 or Eileen Nelson at [email protected] or 651- 582-8464. Ama nda Varley, University of Minnesota Graduate School intern, also provided significant support to t he project. Date of Report: November 2011 mailto:[email protected] mailto:[email protected] Background Minnesota School Readiness Study: Developmental Assessment at Kindergarten Entrance - Fall 2010
  • 13. Research has shown, and continues to show, that t here is a critical relationship between early childhood experiences, school success, and positive life- long outcomes. This research has been a focal point for many states as they strive to reduce the growing achievement gap between less advantaged students and their same-aged peers in the educational system. With no systematic process in place to assess children’s school readiness, the Min nesota Department of Education (MDE) in 2002 initiated a series of three yearly studies focused on obtaining a picture of the school readiness of a representative sample of Minnesota entering kindergartners. Also, the series of studies was to evaluate changes in the percentage of children fully prepared for school at kindergarten entrance. The studies were well- received by the public, and during the 2006 Minnesota state legislative session, funding was appropriated for the study to be continued on an annual basis.
  • 14. This report describes findings from the assessment of school readiness usin g a representative sample of children entering kindergarten in Minnesota in Fall 2010. The data provide a picture of the ratings of entering kindergartners across five domains of child development. The study provides information on school readiness for parents; school teachers and administrators; early childhood education and care teachers, pr oviders and administrators; policymakers; and the public. Definition of School Readiness For purposes of the study, “school readiness” is defined as the skills, knowledge, behaviors and accomplishments that children should know and be able to do as they enter kindergarten in the following areas of child development: physical development; the arts; personal and social development; language and literacy; and mathematical thinking.
  • 15. Assessing School Readiness The study is designed to capture a picture of the readiness of Minnesota children as they enter kindergarten and track readiness trends over time. To ensure that results are reliable and can be generalized to the entire population of Minnesota kindergartners, the study uses a 10 percent sample of schools with entering kindergartners. This sample size generates data from approximately 6,000 kindergartners annually. Minnesota School Readiness Study 2010 The study uses the Work Sampling System (WSS®), a developmentally appropriate, standards-based observational assessment that allows children to demonstrate their knowledge and skills in various ways and across developmental domains.
  • 16. WSS® is aligned with the state’s early learning standards, Minnesota Early Childhood Indicators of Progress, and the K-12 Academic Standards. S ee Appendix A. Each domain and developmental indicator within the WSS® Developmental Checklist includes expected behaviors for children at that age or grade level. For each indicator, teachers used the following guidelines to rate the child's performance: Proficient — indicating that the child can reliably and consistently demonstrate the skill, knowledge, behavior or accomplishment represented by the performance indicator. In Process — indicating that the skill, knowledge, behavior or accomplishment represented by the indicator are intermittent or emergent, and are not demonstrated reliably or consistently. Not Yet — indicating that the child cannot perform the indicator (i.e., the performance
  • 17. indicator represents a skill, knowledge, behavior or accomplishment not yet acquired). Because children’s rate of development is variable, the study assesses children’s proficiency within and across the developmental domains. Rubrics for each rating level were distributed to teachers at the start of the study. The rubrics, provided by the publisher and revised in 2009, provide additional detail for each indicator for a Not Yet, In Process or Proficient rating. Partnership with the Human Capital Research Collaborative Throughout 2010, MDE worked in partnership with the Human Capital Research Collaborative (HCRC) to better understand the relationship between kindergarten entry results and future academic achievement. HCRC is a partnership of the University of Minnesota
  • 18. and the Federal Reserve Bank of Minneapolis. It was important to assess the predictive validity of Minnesota’s school readiness indicators and determine the degree to which the School Readiness Study checklist added additional weight beyond demographics towards the likelihood of passing Grade 3 MCAs. Work was conducted to determine which type of measure from the checklist best predicted Grade 3 MCA results. Findings centered on children who reach 75 percent of the total possible points on the checklist having a greater likelihood in passing Grade 3 MCAs. W hile national research 2 Minnesota School Readiness Study 2010 over decades has pointed to the relationship between early experiences and academic
  • 19. success, it is instructive to have a reference standard within the existing checklist. Based on data from Kindergarten cohorts in 2003, 2004, and 2006 who had available achievement test scores in third grade or information on remedial education, HCRC found that the School Readiness Study checklist, including the 75 percent standard, significantly and consistently predicted third-grade MCA reading and math test scores and the need for school remedial services (special education or grade retention) above and beyond the influence of child and family background characteristics. The strength of prediction was consistent across a range of child and family characteristics (e.g., family income, gender, and race/ethnicity). For more information on this report, go to: http://www.humancapitalrc.org/mn_school_readiness_indicators .pdf 2010 Recr uitment
  • 20. MDE contacted superintendents, principals and teachers beginning mid-winter to build the sample for the coming fall. A list of all public schools with kindergartners as of October 1 the previous year was compiled. The list was divided into eight strata which accounts for proximity to population centers and population density and separated charter and magnet schools. A representative sample of schools within each strata was invited to participate via a mailed invitation to the superintendent and principal of each site. Follow-up calls were made to each site to answer questions. In 2010, 55 percent (495/900) of all schools were invited to participate. Approximately 24 pe rcent (120/495) of those invited responded positively to the initial invitation. In late spring, schools are selected to be released from the cohort when student counts exceed the sample amount. In 2010, no s chools were released. By November, 12 pe rcent of all elementary schools (108/900) submitted child-level data.
  • 21. The following table shows the total kindergarten population compared to the sample population. The sample seeks to be representative of all public schools including charters and magnets across federally mandated demographic categories. (See Table 1.) 3 http://www.humancapitalrc.org/mn_school_readiness_indicators .pdf Table 1 - Kindergarten Population Compared to the Sample State Study Kindergarten Sample Enrollment American Indian 2.3% 5.4% Asian 7.1% 5.6% Hispanic 8.5% 7.0%
  • 22. Black 10.9% 8.8% White 71.1% 71.7% Limited English Proficiency 11.7% 6% Special Education 10.4% 7% 2010 Res ults A total of 5,838 kinder gartners from 108 se lected elementary schools across the state were included in the Fall 2010 cohort. This reflects 9.2 pe rcent of the entering kindergartners for the 2010-2011 sc hool year. Of these children, 5,654 students had all WSS indicators completed for analysis. For the Fall of 2010, 60 percent of Minnesota’s kindergartners reached the 75 percent standard. For selected categories, see Chart 1. The selected categories in Chart 1 are based on the statistically significant categories from the regression. The regression is discussed in more detail on page 9.
  • 23. The domain rankings by proficiency for the 2010 cohort are reordered with previous years of the study. (See Table 2 and Chart 2.) Physical Development had the highest percentage of children assessed Proficient on average, followed in order by Language & Literacy; The Arts; Personal and Social Development and Mathematical Thinking. Indicator order within each domain changed only slightly from 2009 in Mathematical Thinking; Personal and Social Development and Language and Literacy. ( See Table 3.) Proficiency by domain is defined as the average percent proficient across indicators within each domain. It is important to note that while there are trends towards increases in estimates of Proficient results, the trends are not outside the margin of error. Also, the existing data set does not allow for examination of potential reasons for shifts.
  • 24. Minnesota School Readiness Study 2010 4 Table 2 - Results By Domain Margin Domain/Result Proficient of Error Physical Development 70% 2.7% Language & Literacy 59% 2.9% The Arts 56% 2.9% Personal & Social Development 56% 2.9% Mathematical Thinking 52% 2.9%
  • 25. Note categories are adjusted for stratified cluster sampling. 75 Percent Standard 60% 2.9% Chart 1 – Percent of Students Reaching 75 Percent Standard by Selected Sub-Cate Minnesota School Readiness Study 2010 The 75 percent standard is defined as the percent reaching at least 75 percent of the possible points on the checklist, a predictor of grade 3 MCAs. gories 5 Minnesota School Readiness Study 2010
  • 26. Table 3 Domain & Indicator Results Ranked by Proficiency Percent Physical Development Proficient Physical Development Average Score Summary 70% Performs some self-care tasks independently. 73% Coordinates movements to perform simple tasks. 71% Uses eye-hand coordination to perform tasks. 67% The Arts The Arts Domain Average Score Summary 56% Participates in group music experiences. 63% Participates in creative movement, dance and drama. 60% Uses a variety of art materials for tactile experience and exploration. 59%
  • 27. Responds to artistic creations or events. 56% Personal and Social Development Personal and Social Development Domain Average Score Summary 56% Interacts easily with familiar adults. 63% Shows eagerness and curiosity as a learner. 62% Interacts easily with one or more children. 62% Shows empathy and caring for others. 60% Follows simple classroom rules and routines. 58% Manages transitions. 57% Shows some self-direction. 56% Seeks adult help when needed to resolve conflicts. 53% Attends to tasks and seeks help when encountering a problem. 52% Approaches tasks with flexibility and inventiveness. 50% - 6
  • 28. Minnesota School Readiness Study 2010 Language and Literacy Language and Literacy Domain Average Score Summary 59% Shows appreciation for books and reading. 66% Speaks clearly enough to be understood without contextual clues. 65%
  • 29. Shows beginning understanding of concepts about print. 61% Comprehends and responds to stories read aloud. 60% Begins to develop knowledge about letters. 60% Gains meaning by listening. 59% Represents ideas and stories through pictures, dictation and play. 57% Follows two- or three-step directions. 55% Uses expanded vocabulary and language arts for a variety of purposes. 52% Uses letter-like shapes, symbols and letters to convey meaning. 52% Demonstrates phonological awareness. 21% Mathematical Thinking Mathematical Thinking Domain Average Score Summary 52% Begins to recognize and describe the attributes of shapes. 60% Shows beginning understanding of number and quantity. 58% Shows understanding of and uses several positional words. 57%
  • 30. Begins to use simple strategies to solve mathematical problems. 50% 7 Chart 2 – Proficiency Rates by Domain Minnesota School Readiness Study 2010 Descriptive Results The 2010 c ohort was also analyzed for descriptive results based on single demographic categories. For example, to report under the income charts, all parents are included in the under 100 percent Federal Poverty Guidelines grouping without controlling for education status, home language or race/ethnicity. The family survey asks parents to select all race/ethnicity categories that are relevant for their child. If multiple categories are selected, the child will be represented in the
  • 31. appropriate categories. A similar process was followed for primary home languages. The percent within each demographic category reaching the 75 percent standard are reported in Appendix B. Family Survey Results As part of the study process, families are asked to complete a voluntary survey. This information is 8 Minnesota School Readiness Study 2010 combined with the Work Sampling System® checklist results (see Appendix C). I n total, 4,932 pa rents (84 p ercent) completed the survey. Of this group, 4,695 responses (95
  • 32. percent) were usable for analysis. (A parent survey may not be usable for analysis because it was incomplete, the student information strip was incomplete or the survey lacked coordinating information in Work Sampling Online (WSO).) After matching the family survey data with Work Sampling Online results, 4,168 re cords remained for regression analysis. This is 85 pe rcent of all submitted parent surveys and 89 pe rcent of those available to match. Logistic Regression Results The analysis of the data included examining how a particular child or family characteristic may affect that child’s ratings while controlling for the effects of other demographic variables with which it may be confounded (e.g., a child from a family with a lower household income is more likely to have a parent with a lower education level). The result of reaching the 75 percent proficiency standard across all domains was
  • 33. analyzed with respect to the demographic characteristics of gender, parent education level, household income, primary home language and race and ethnicity collected from parent surveys. (See Table 4 and Appendix D.) For comparison to previous years, see Appendix E. All 2010 a nalyses reported involved statistical estimation procedures that reflect the stratified cluster sampling design used (with school as the primary sampling unit), and include correction for finite population sampling. Observations within each stratum were weighted to reflect the statewide proportion of students in the stratum. Table 4 - Statistically Significant Factors in Reaching the 75 Percent Standard Household Income Parent Education Level Gender Note: predictors significant at p < .05
  • 34. Household Income The odds of reaching the 75 percent standard for a student whose household income was at or above 400 percent of the Federal Poverty Guidelines (FPG) were more than one and a half times as great as compared to a student whose household income was less than 250 pe rcent FPG when holding all other variables constant. The odds of reaching the 75 percent standard for a student whose household income was 250 400 percent FPG are nearly one and half times as great as compared to a student whose household income is up to 250 - 9
  • 35. percent FPG. This result is statistically significant. Parent Education Level Parent education level was found to be statistically significant in reaching the 75 percent standard. Students whose parents have a high school degree a re twice as likely to reach the 75 percent standard as compared to students whose parents have less than a high school degree. Students with parents who have a an Associate degree, Bachelor or graduate degree are approximately one and a half ti mes as likely to reach the 75 percent standard as compared to students whose parents who have a high school diploma or GED. Primary Home Language Primary home language was not found to be statistically significant in reaching the 75
  • 36. percent standard when holding all other variables constant. Race and Ethnicity Parent-report of race and ethnicity was not a statistically significant factor in reaching the 75 percent standard when holding all other variables constant. Minority status as an overall category was marginally significant. Gender Gender continues to be a statistically significant factor. The odds of reaching the 75 percent standard for females were up to one and a third times g reater, as compared to males. Principal and Teacher Surveys As in previous years, the success of the study rested with the willingness of school principals and kindergarten teachers to participate. Participating school principals and
  • 37. kindergarten teachers w ere again given surveys to complete regarding their decision to participate, barriers to participation, and the associated workload and benefits. The following information is based upon the response of 35 pr incipals (108 possible responses or 32 p ercent) and 165 kinder garten teachers (288 potential responses or 57 percent). Principal Perspectives Principals reported two primary benefits of participating in the study: helping influence statewide policy (100 percent) and gaining information about where students are at the beginning of the school year (69 percent). Reported barriers for participation included Minnesota School Readiness Study 2010 10
  • 38. adding to existing teacher workloads (63 p ercent). Principals balanced the need of the project with competing needs by having more experienced teachers mentor newer teachers, paying teachers for their extra time and shifting staff development resources. Principals will use the information gained from the study to identify children’s needs earlier in the year (50 pe rcent). Principals using Work Sampling Online (WSO) reported that the online training was easy to access. A m ajority of principals (84 pe rcent) reported receiving the appropriate amount of information prior to and during their participation. Teacher Perspectives A vast majority of teachers (86 pe rcent) responded that contributing to a study that will influence statewide early childhood policy was of benefit to them. The same percent reported receiving a $200 stipend as a benefit. Others reported the benefit of gaining information about where students are at the beginning of the
  • 39. school year (68 percent). A little over one-third of the teachers reported that collecting the parent surveys was a challenge for them (37 percent). On a follow-up question, 80 pe rcent responded that they were able to implement the parent survey with great to moderate ease. Thirty-one percent had no challenges implementing the study. Teachers reported that the study took a minimal (12 pe rcent) to average (72 pe rcent) amount of work for a special project. Teachers report planning to use the information to identify children’s needs earlier in the year (46 pe rcent) and helping them target instruction (47 percent). Regarding the use of technology, 96 percent report great to moderate ease in accessing WSO and the Web-based orientation. Teachers report receiving adequate levels of information prior to (95 pe rcent) and during
  • 40. the study (98 p ercent). They also report receiving adequate support from MDE (92 percent) throughout the study period. Currently, 28 percent of teachers use Work Sampling in their schools, 35 pe rcent report planning to continue using WSO after the study period. Approximately one-third of all teachers report using locally designed assessment tools in additional to the Work Sampling System®. Limitations Because children develop and grow along a continuum but at varied ra tes, the goal of the study is to assess children’s proficiency within and across these developmental domains over time and not establish whether or not children, individually or in small groups, are ready for school with the use of a “ready” or “not ready” score. Nor is the study’s goal to provide information on the history or the future of an individual student. Recent national reports have discussed the complexities in the
  • 41. development of state-level accountability systems. Taking Stock: Assessing and Improving Early Childhood Minnesota School Readiness Study 2010 11 Learning and Program Quality (2007) and The National Academy of Science report Early Childhood Assessment: Why, What and How? (2008) details the necessary steps to use authentic assessment results, also referred to as instructional assessments, in accountability initiatives. The National Academy of Science reports that even in upper grades, e xtreme caution is needed in relying exclusively on child assessment and that for children birth to five “even more extreme caution is needed.” Discussion
  • 42. In line with national research, family household income and parent education was found to be predictive in reaching the 75 percent standard. Race/Ethnicity as an overall category was marginally significant but not significant for individual groups and G ender is predictive in reaching the 75 percent standard. Recommendations 1. Continue to work toward improving the quality of early childhood education and care programs in Minnesota by emphasizing the importance of teacher-child interactions and content-driven, intentional curriculum and instruction. Build on the 10 Essential Elements of Effective Early Childhood Programs and Governor Dayton’s 7-Point Plan for Achieving Excellence. 2. Target intervention strategies to children assessed as Not Proficient, especially in the areas of literacy and mathematics. Implement compensatory strategies as soon as a
  • 43. child’s need is identified. Work with the Governor’s Early Learning Council to identify staged implementation strategies to maximize resources. 3. Support more children in their efforts to read well by third grade by focusing state policies on young children’s language and literacy development. 4. Strengthen teacher-child interactions to improve learning by implementing professional development that includes teacher observation and development. 5. Individualize instruction by using assessment information to design classroom experiences. 6. Use child progress assessment information when teachers talk with parents about setting goals for children. 7. Increase collaborations from early childhood through Grade 3 at the teacher, director, principal and
  • 44. superintendent levels. Identify district and state policy opportunities to promote this work. 12 Minnesota School Readiness Study 2010 8. Consider collecting information on prior early care and education experiences and incorporating that information into the early childhood longitudinal data system. Results from the 2010 prior experience data pilot need to be considered when planning for the future. Early Learning Council T he Early Childhood Advisory Council (ECAC), seated from December 2008 to January 2011, looked to the a nnual School Readiness study as one
  • 45. measure of state progress on early learning. The Council was reauthorized and renamed the Early Learning Council by Governor Dayton’s Executive Order 11-05. Read the Executive Order on the Governor’s website. The newly formed Early Learning Council (ELC) may continue to look to the results of the study to guide school readiness policy. Minnesota School Readiness Study 2010 13 http://mn.gov/governor/multimedia/pdf/Executive-Order-11- 05.pdf http://mn.gov/governor/multimedia/pdf/Executive-Order-11- 05.pdf Minnesota School Readiness Study 2010 For further reading Campbell, F. A., Ramey, C. T., Pungello, E., Sparling, J., & Miller-Johnson, S. (2002).Early childhood
  • 46. education: Young adult outcomes from the Abecedarian project. Applied Developmental Science, 6(1), 42 57. Coley, R. J. (2002). An uneven start: Indicators of inequality in school readiness. Princeton, NJ: Educational Testing Service. Dichtelmiller, M. L., Jablon, J. R., Marsden, D. B., & Meisels, S. J. (2001). Preschool-4 developmental guidelines (4th Ed.). New York: Rebus. Gershoff, E. (November 2003). Living at the edge research brief no.4: Low income and the development of America’s kindergartners. New York: National Center for Children in Poverty. Meisels, S.J. & Atkins-Burnett, S. (2006). Evaluating early childhood assessments: A differential Analysis. In K. McCartney & D. Phillips (Eds.), The Blackwell handbook of early childhood development (pp. 533 549). Malden, MA: Blackwell Publishing. Minnesota Department of Education (2003). Minnesota
  • 47. School Readiness Initiative: Developmental Assessment at Kindergarten Entrance. Roseville: Minnesota Department of Education. Minnesota Department of Education. (2004). Minnesota School Readiness Year Two Study: Developmental Assessment at Kindergarten Entrance Fall 2003. Roseville: Minnesota Department of Education. Minnesota Department of Education. (2005). Minnesota School Readiness Year Three Study: Developmental Assessment at Kindergarten Entrance Fall 2004. Roseville: Minnesota Department of Education. Minnesota Department of Education (2007). Minnesota School Readiness Study: Developmental Assessment at Kindergarten Entrance Fall 2006. Roseville: Minnesota Department of Education. Minnesota Department of Education (2008). Minnesota School Readiness Study: Developmental Assessment at Kindergarten Entrance Fall 2007. Roseville: Minnesota Department of Education.
  • 48. Minnesota Department of Education and Minnesota Department of Human Services. (2005). Early childhood indicators of progress: Minnesota’s early learning standards. Roseville: Minnesota Department of Education. National Early Childhood Accountability Task Force. (2007) Taking Stock: Assessing and Improving Early Childhood Learning and Program Quality. Washington DC: The Pew Charitable Trusts. National Research Council. (2008). Early Childhood Assessment: Why, What, and How. Committee on Developmental Outcomes and Assessments for Young Children, C.E. Snow and S.B. Van Hemel, Editors. Board on Children, Youth, and Families, Board on Testing and Assessment, Division of Behavioral and Social Sciences and Education. Washington, DC: The National Academies Press. National Research Council & Institute of Medicine. (2000). From neurons to neighborhoods: The science of early childhood development. Washington, DC: National Academy Press. Reynolds, A., Englund, M., Hayakawa, C., Hendricks, M.,
  • 49. Ou, S., Rosenberger, A., Smerillo, N., Warner- Richter, M. Assessing the Validity of Minnesota School Readiness Indicators: Summary Report. Human Capital Research Collaborative. January 2011. Retrieved May 2011, http://www.humancapitalrc.org/mn_school_readiness_indicators .pdf - - http://www.humancapitalrc.org/mn_school_readiness_indicators .pdf Minnesota School Readiness Study 2010 Reynolds, A. J., Temple, J. A., Robertson, D. L., & Mann, E. A. (2001). Long-term effects of an early childhood intervention on educational achievement and juvenile arrest: A 15-year follow-up of low-income children in public schools. Journal of the American Medical Association, 285(18), 2339-2346. Schweinhart, L. J., Montie, J., Xiang, Z., Barnett, W. S.,
  • 50. Belfield, C. R., & Nores, M. (2005). Lifetime effects: The high/scope perry preschool study through age 40. Ypsilanti, MI: High/Scope Press. U.S. Department of Education, U.S. National Center for Education Statistics, Home Literacy Activities and Signs of Children’s Emerging Literacy, 1993, NCES 2000- 026, November 1999; and the Early Childhood Program Participation Survey, National Household Education Surveys Program, 2005, unpublished data. http://www.census.gov/compendia/statab/tables/09s0229.xls U.S. Department of Health and Human Services. (2009). The 2009 HHS Poverty Guidelines. Retrieved January 8, 2011, from http://aspe.hhs.gov/poverty/09Poverty.shtml. Wertheimer, R., & Croan, T. (December 2003). Attending kindergarten and already behind: A statistical portrait of vulnerable young children. Washington, DC: Child Trends. Zill, N., & West, J. (2000). Entering kindergarten: A portrait of American children when they begin school. Washington, DC: U.S. Department of Education, National
  • 51. Center for Education Statistics. http://www.census.gov/compendia/statab/tables/09s0229.xls http://aspe.hhs.gov/poverty/09Poverty.shtml A. Sample Work Sampling System® Developmental Checklist (Minnesota P4) B. Work Sampling System Subgroup Analysis with Sampling Weight (2010) C. Family Survey (English) D. Logistic Regression Predicting Proficiency at the 75 Percent Standard (Weighted) E. Statistically Significant Factors from Logistic Regression Minnesota School Readiness Study 2010 Appendices
  • 52. Minnesota School Readiness Study 2010 Appendix B Work Sampling System Subgroup Analysis with Sampling Weight (2010) 75% Overall Proficiency (weighted) All children 59.9 Race/ethnicity White (N=2841) 62.7 Asian/ Native Hawaiian/Pacific Islander (N=221) 62.0 Black/African/African American (N=349) 57.0 Other (N=64) 53.8 American Indian/Alaskan Native (N=203) 44.4 Hispanic/Latino (N=278) 43.6
  • 53. Gender Female (N=2754) 65.4 Male (N=2902) 54.5 IEP Status (Special education) No (N=5258) 61.9 Yes (N=398) 29.9 Family Income Over 250% Federal Poverty Guideline (N=1554) 69.2 250% Federal Poverty Guideline and under 52.3 (N=1735) Parent Education Less than high school (N=200) 32.4 High School Diploma/GED (N=671) 48.7 Trade school or some college (N=1013) 55.7 Associate’s degree (N=581) 61.2 Bachelor’s degree (N=1024) 67.6 Graduate or professional degree (N=466) 70.7
  • 54. Strata 1 – Minneapolis and St. Paul (N=655) 57.4 2 – 7 country metro excluding MSP 1 (N=1551) 69.3 3 – Outstate enrollment 2,000+ (N=1306) 51.5 4 – Outstate enrollment 1,000-1,999 (N=1092) 45.9 5 - Outstate enrollment 500-999 (N=605) 52.4 6 - Outstate enrollment <500 (N=445) 63.6 * Note, 250% FPG for a family of four for this time period is $55,125. 1 The seven county metro area includes Anoka, Carver, Dakota, Hennepin, Ramsey, Scott and Washington Counties. Minnesota School Readiness Study 2010 Appendix C Parent Survey - Minnesota School Readiness Study
  • 55. 1. Please indicate whether you are this child’s: ___ Mother ___ Father ___ Other 2. Your highest level of school completed? Mark only one. ___ Less than high school ___ High school diploma/GED ___Trade school or some college beyond high school ___ Associate degree ___ Bachelor’s degree ___ Graduate or professional school degree 3. Your household’s total yearly income before taxes from January-December last year? Round to the nearest thousand. $________________________ 4. How many people are currently in your household?
  • 56. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 Indicate:_____________ 5. Race/ethnicity of your kindergarten child? Mark all that apply. ___ Black/African/African American ___ American Indian/Alaskan Native ___ Asian ___ Native Hawaiian or other Pacific Islander ___ Hispanic or Latino ___ White/Caucasian ___ Other 6. What language does your family speak most at home? ___ English __ Vietnamese ___ Spanish __ Russian ___ Hmong __ Other ___ Somali
  • 57. Thank you for your time in working with us on this study. For school use only: Dist #_______ School #________ Gender: M F DoB: ____/____/____ MARSS: _______________________________________ (include all 13 digits, including leading zeros) Appendix D Logistic Regression Predicting Proficiency at the 75 Percent Standard (Weighted) VARIABLES b se(b) Wald df p Odds Ratio
  • 58. Parent Education 38.12*** 5 0.000 Less than High School -0.67*** 0.23 8.09 1 0.004 0.51 High School or GED # Some Post High School 0.14 0.13 1.28 1 0.258 1.16 Associate Degree 0.37** 0.15 6.47 1 0.011 1.45 Bachelor Degree 0.54*** 0.14 15.12 1 0.000 1.71 Grad/Prof Degree 0.60*** 0.17 12.54 1 0.000 1.82 Percent of Federal Poverty Guidelines 20.23*** 2 0.000 0-250 # >250-400 0.37*** 0.11 11.49 1 0.001 1.44 >400 0.49*** 0.12 16.95 1 0.000 1.63 Home Language 1.24 1 0.266 Non-English # English Only 0.21 0.19 1.24 1 0.266 1.24 Minority Status 5.07* 2 0.079 Minority Only -0.18 0.12 2.22 1 0.136 0.84
  • 59. White and Minority 0.21 0.15 1.98 1 0.160 1.24 White Only # Gender 15.15*** 1 0.000 Male # Female 0.32*** 0.08 15.15 1 0.000 1.37 Intercept -0.35 0.22 2.52 1 0.113 Number of observations: 3246 # indicates reference category *** p<0.01, ** p<0.05, * p<0.1 Minnesota School Readiness Study 2010 Appendix E
  • 60. Statistically Significant Factors from Logistic Regression Domain/Year Parent Percent Primary Race and Gender Education of FPG* Home Ethnicity Language Physical Development and Health 2006 -- *** -- -- *** -- *** -- -- *** -- *** *** -- *** *** *** -- -- -- *** -- -- -- *** -- *** -- -- *** -- *** -- -- *** -- *** -- *** -- *** *** -- -- ***
  • 61. --- *** -- -- *** -- *** -- *** *** -- *** -- -- *** *** *** -- -- -- -- *** *** -- *** -- *** *** -- *** -- *** -- -- -- *** *** -- -- *** *** *** *** -- *** -- *** *** -- *** -- *** -- -- *** 2007 2008 2009 The Arts
  • 63. 2006 2007 2008 2009 75 Percent Standard 2010 *** *** -- -- *** *** Noted demographic is significant for specified domain and year. * Federal Poverty Guideline is used from 2007 forward. 2006 income asked categorically. Note – Analysis 2010 forward focused on 75 percent standard. Minnesota School Readiness Study 2010 - - - - - - - - - - - - - -
  • 64. - - - - - - - - - - - - - - - - - - - - - - - - - - - - - - - - - - - - - - - -
  • 65. School Readiness and Later Achievement Greg J. Duncan Northwestern University Chantelle J. Dowsett University of Texas at Austin Amy Claessens Northwestern University Katherine Magnuson University of Wisconsin–Madison Aletha C. Huston University of Texas at Austin Pamela Klebanov Princeton University Linda S. Pagani Université de Montréal Leon Feinstein University of London Mimi Engel Northwestern University Jeanne Brooks-Gunn Columbia University Holly Sexton University of Michigan Kathryn Duckworth
  • 66. University of London Crista Japel Université de Québec à Montréal Using 6 longitudinal data sets, the authors estimate links between three key elements of school readiness—school-entry academic, attention, and socioemotional skills—and later school reading and math achievement. In an effort to isolate the effects of these school-entry skills, the authors ensured that most of their regression models control for cognitive, attention, and socioemotional skills measured prior to school entry, as well as a host of family background measures. Across all 6 studies, the strongest predictors of later achievement are school-entry math, reading, and attention skills. A meta-analysis of the results shows that early math skills have the greatest predictive power, followed by reading and then attention skills. By contrast, measures of socioemotional behaviors, including internalizing and exter- nalizing problems and social skills, were generally insignificant predictors of later academic perfor- mance, even among children with relatively high levels of problem behavior. Patterns of association were similar for boys and girls and for children from high and low socioeconomic backgrounds. Keywords: school readiness, socioemotional behaviors, attention, early academic skills Supplemental materials: http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/[0012- 1649.43.6.1428].supp Greg J. Duncan, Amy Claessens, and Mimi Engel, School of Education
  • 67. and Social Policy, Northwestern University; Chantelle J. Dowsett and Aletha C. Huston, Department of Human Ecology, University of Texas at Austin; Katherine Magnuson, Department of Social Work, University of Wisconsin–Madison; Pamela Klebanov, Center for Research on Child Wellbeing, Princeton University; Linda S. Pagani, Department of Psycho- education, Université de Montréal, Québec, Canada; Leon Feinstein and Kathryn Duckworth, Department of Quantitative Social Science, Institute of Education, University of London, London, England; Jeanne Brooks- Gunn, Department of Pediatrics, Columbia University; Holly Sexton, Re- seach Center for Group Dynamics, University of Michigan; Crista Japel, Département d’éducation et formation spécialisées, Université de Québec à Montréal, Québec, Canada. A preliminary version of this article was presented at the biennial meetings of the Society for Research on Child Development, Atlanta, Georgia, April 2005. We are grateful to the National Science Foundation-supported Center for the Analysis of Pathways from Child- hood to Adulthood (CAPCA; Grant 0322356) for research support. We thank Larry Aber, Mark Appelbaum, Avshalom Caspi, David Cordray,
  • 68. Herbert Ginsburg, David Grissmer, Mark Lipsey, Derek Neal, Cybele Raver, Arnold Sameroff, Robert Siegler, Ross Thompson, Sandra Jo Wilson, Nicholas Zill, and other members of CAPCA and the MacArthur Network on Families and the Economy for helpful com- ments. Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to Greg J. Duncan, School of Education and Social Policy, Northwestern University, 2046 Sheridan Road, Evanston, IL 60208. E-mail: greg- [email protected] Developmental Psychology Copyright 2007 by the American Psychological Association 2007, Vol. 43, No. 6, 1428–1446 0012-1649/07/$12.00 DOI: 10.1037/0012-1649.43.6.1428 1428 Early childhood programs and policies that promote academic skills have been gaining popularity among politicians and re- searchers. For example, President George W. Bush (2002) en- dorsed Head Start reforms in 2002 that focus on building early academic skills, observing that “on the first day of school, children need to know letters and numbers. They need a strong vocabulary. These are the building blocks of learning, and this nation must provide them” (p. 12). The National Research Council’s Commit- tee on the Prevention of Reading Difficulties in Young Children
  • 69. recommends providing environments that promote preliteracy skills for all preschool children (Snow, Burns, & Griffin, 1998). Similarly, the National Association for the Education of Young Children and the National Council of Teachers of Mathematics (2002) issued a joint statement that advocated for high-quality mathematics education for children ages 3–6. Others, however, maintain that a broad constellation of behav- iors and skills enables children to learn in school. Asked to identify factors associated with a difficult transition to school, kindergarten teachers frequently mentioned weaknesses in academic skills, problems with social skills, trouble following directions, and dif- ficulty with independent and group work (Rimm-Kaufman, Pianta, & Cox, 2000). Researchers too have made this point. The National Research Council and Institute on Medicine argued that “the elements of early intervention programs that enhance social and emotional development are just as important as the components that enhance linguistic and cognitive competence” (Shonkoff & Phillips, 2000, pp. 398–399). These two views have emerged in the current debate about what constitutes school readiness and in particular about what skills predict school achievement. Many early education programs, in- cluding Head Start, are designed to enhance children’s physical, intellectual, and social competencies on the grounds that each domain contributes to a child’s overall developmental competence and readiness for school. However, if early acquisition of specific academic skills or learning-enhancing behaviors forecasts later achievement, it may be beneficial to add domain-specific early
  • 70. skills to the definition of school readiness and to encourage inter- ventions aimed at promoting these skills prior to elementary school. Thus, understanding which skills are linked to children’s academic achievement has important implications for early edu- cation programs. We adopted a child-centered model of school transition, which is nested within a broader ecological framework but focuses on the set of individual skills and behaviors that children have acquired prior to school entry (Rimm-Kaufman & Pianta, 2000). A child’s individual characteristics contribute to the environments in which the child interacts and the rate at which the child may learn new skills; in turn, the child receives feedback from others in the environment (Meisels, 1998). Thus, because they affect both the child and the social environment, early academic skills and socio- emotional behaviors are linked to subsequent academic achieve- ment because they provide the foundation for positive classroom adaptation (Cunha, Heckman, Lochner, & Masterov, 2006; Entwisle, Alexander, & Olson, 2005). For example, a child who enters kindergarten with rudimentary academic skills may be poised to learn from formal reading and mathematics instruction, receive positive reinforcement from the teacher, or be placed in a higher ability group that facilitates the acquisition of additional skills. Similarly, a child who can pay attention, inhibit impulsive behavior, and relate appropriately to adults and peers may be able to take advantage of the learning
  • 71. opportunities in the classroom, thus more easily mastering reading and math concepts taught in elementary school. For these reasons, the skills children possess when entering school might result in different achievement patterns in later life. If achievement at older ages is the product of a sequential process of skill acquisition, then strengthening skills prior to school entry might lead children to master more advanced skills at an earlier age and perhaps even increase their ultimate level of achievement. Although there are strong theoretical reasons to expect that individual differences in children’s early academic skills and be- havior are linked to subsequent behavior and achievement, sur- prisingly little rigorous research has been conducted to test this hypothesis. Consequently, the purpose of this article is to assess as precisely as possible, using six longitudinal, nonexperimental data sets, the association between skills and behaviors that emerge during the preschool years and later academic achievement. As with Robins’s (1978) classic study of adult antisocial behavior, our approach consists of comparable analyses of a number of different longitudinal developmental studies.1 We are especially interested in identifying academic, attention, and socioemotional skills and behaviors that may be learned or improved through experiences prior to school entry. In the following sections, we draw from developmental literature to identify key dimensions of school readiness and to derive theoretical predictions about how chil-
  • 72. dren’s school-entry skills and behaviors contribute to short- and long-term academic success. Associations Between Early Skills and Later Achievement Academic achievement is a cumulative process involving both mastering new skills and improving already existing skills (Entwisle & Alexander, 1990; Pungello, Kuperschmidt, Burchinal, & Patterson, 1996). Information about how children acquire read- ing and math skills points to the importance of specific academic skills but also indicates that more general cognitive skills, partic- ularly oral language and conceptual ability, may be increasingly important for later mastery of more complex reading and mathe- matical tasks. Basic oral language skills become critical for un- derstanding texts as the level of difficulty of reading passages increases (NICHD Early Child Care Research Network, 2005b; Scarborough, 2001; Snow et al, 1998; Storch & Whitehurst, 2002; Whitehurst & Lonigan, 1998). Likewise, mastery of foundational concepts of numbers allows for a deeper understanding of more complex mathematical problems and flexible problem-solving techniques (Baroody, 2003; Ferrari & Sternberg, 1998; Hiebert & Wearne, 1996). Although children’s academic achievement is largely stable throughout childhood, children do demonstrate both transitory fluctuations and fundamental shifts in their achievement trajecto- ries (Kowaleski-Jones & Duncan, 1998; Pungello et al., 1996). Nonexperimental data show that children’s achievement test
  • 73. 1 Robins (1978) justified her approach as follows: “In the long run, the best evidence for the truth of any observation lies in its replicability across studies. The more the populations studied differ, the wider the historical eras they span; the more the details of the methods vary, the more convincing becomes that replication” (p. 611). 1429SCHOOL READINESS AND LATER ACHIEVEMENT scores are related to prior cognitive functioning and the attainment of basic skills in math and literacy such as number and letter recognition (Stevenson & Newman, 1986). In their meta- analysis, La Paro and Pianta (2000) found middle-range correlations in cognitive/academic skills both from preschool to kindergarten (.43) and from kindergarten to first or second grade (.48). Attention-related skills such as task persistence and self- regulation are expected to increase the time during which children are engaged and participating in academic endeavors. Research has shown that signs of attention and impulsivity can be detected as early as age 2.5 but continue to develop until reaching relative stability between ages 6 and 8 (Olson, Sameroff, Kerr, Lopez, & Wellman, 2005; Posner & Rothbart, 2000). Studies linking atten- tion with later achievement are less common, but consistent evi- dence suggests that the ability to control and sustain attention
  • 74. as well as participate in classroom activities predicts achievement test scores and grades during preschool and the early elementary grades (Alexander, Entwisle, & Dauber, 1993; Raver, Smith- Donald, Hayes, & Jones, 2005). These attention skills, which are conceptually distinct from other types of interpersonal behaviors, are associated with later academic achievement, independent of initial cognitive ability (McClelland, Morrison, & Holmes, 2000; Yen, Konold, & McDermott, 2004) and of prior reading ability and current vocabulary (Howse, Lange, Farran, & Boyles, 2003). Ex- amining attention separately from externalizing problems has clar- ified the role of each in achievement, suggesting that attention is more predictive of later achievement than more general problem behaviors (Barriga et al., 2002; Hinshaw, 1992; Konold & Pianta, 2005; Ladd, Birch, & Buhs, 1999; Normandeau & Guay, 1998; Trzesniewski, Moffitt, Caspi, Taylor, & Maughan, 2006). Children’s socioemotional skills and behaviors are also ex- pected to affect both individual learning and classroom dynamics. Inadequate interpersonal skills promote child–teacher conflict and social exclusion (Newcomb, Bukowski, & Pattee, 1993; Parker & Asher, 1987), and these stressors may reduce children’s participa- tion in collaborative learning activities and adversely affect
  • 75. achievement (Ladd et al., 1999; Pianta & Stuhlman, 2004). Cor- relational evidence linking problem behaviors to academic achievement is found in the Beginning School Study. First- grade ratings on items describing a cheerful, outgoing temperament (roughly the opposite of internalizing problems) predicted adult educational attainment better than preschool or first-grade achieve- ment scores (Entwisle et al., 2005). Other studies yield similar results. For example, children with consistently high levels of aggression from ages 2–9 were more likely than other children to have achievement problems in third grade (NICHD Early Child Care Research Network, 2004). Experimental Evidence and Crossover Effects Many nonexperimental studies find associations between early achievement, attention, and behavior and later achievement, yet few of these studies are designed to determine which of these skills can be modified prior to school entry or whether these changes predict later achievement. In theory, intervention research should shed light on this gap by demonstrating ways to improve children’s skills and by testing whether improvements in early skills are associated with better adjustment in the long term. Indeed, a small number of experimental interventions provide encouraging evi- dence that high-quality programs for preschool children “at risk” for school failure produce gains in cognitive and academic skills and reduce behavior problems (Conduct Problems Prevention
  • 76. Re- search Group, 2002; Karoly, Kilburn, & Cannon, 2005; Love et al., 2003). Early educational interventions have also been found to result in long-term reductions in special education services, grade retention, and increases in educational attainment (Campbell, Ramey, Pungello, Sparling, & Miller-Johnson, 2002; Lazar et al., 1982; Reynolds & Temple, 1998). As is the case with nonexperimental studies, few intervention studies are designed to isolate the relative contributions of changes in achievement, attention, and behavior to later school achieve- ment. A first problem is that behavioral interventions tend to measure behavioral but not achievement outcomes, whereas read- ing and math interventions tend to measure achievement but not behavioral outcomes. Interesting exceptions are a small number of experimental behavior-based interventions that tested for achieve- ment impacts (Coie & Krehbiel, 1984; Dolan et al., 1993). For example, a random-assignment evaluation of a behavioral inter- vention targeting both aggressive and shy behaviors among first graders found short-run improvements in both teacher and peer reports of aggressive and shy behavior but no crossover impacts on reading achievement (Dolan et al., 1993; Kellam, Mayer, Rebok, & Hawkins, 1998). Given evidence, albeit limited, that behavioral interventions succeed at improving behavior but not achievement, behavior would appear to play a limited role in academic
  • 77. success. A second problem is that many intervention programs target both children’s academic skills and their socioemotional behav- iors, rendering it impossible to assess their separate impacts through simple experimental contrasts. For example, the Fast Track prevention program provided a number of services to chil- dren who were identified as disruptive in kindergarten, including direct tutoring in reading skills in first grade (Conduct Problems Prevention Research Group, 1992; 2002). It is possible to estimate nonexperimental mediated models to determine whether program effects are more likely to be due to children’s improved achieve- ment, attention, or behavior skills (e.g., Reynolds, Ou, & Topitzes, 2004). This is rarely done, however. The Present Study This study builds on previous school readiness research in several ways. First, the scope of the study is unprecedented. We estimated a carefully specified set of models with data from six large-scale longitudinal studies, two of which were nationally representative of U.S. children, whereas two were drawn from multisite studies of U.S. children, with one each focusing on children from Great Britain and Canada. Second, we included as predictors a wide representation of school readiness indicators used in previous research and carefully distinguished between related but conceptually distinct skills (e.g., oral language vs. preliteracy skills, attention vs. externalizing problems) wherever
  • 78. possible. Third, we examined multiple dimensions of academic achievement outcomes, including grade completion and math and reading achievement as assessed by both teacher ratings and test scores. Fourth, we implemented rigorous analytic methods that attempted to isolate the effects of school-entry academic, attention, and socioemotional skills by controlling for an extensive set of prior child, family, and contextual influences that may have been 1430 DUNCAN ET AL. related to children’s achievement. Finally, we assessed whether the predictive power of school readiness components differs by gender or socioeconomic status, which would indicate that some children are at heightened risk of low achievement. We tested a number of hypotheses related to how school-entry academic, attention, and socioemotional skills are associated with later school achievement. Developmental theory suggests that chil- dren’s informal, intuitive knowledge of early language and math concepts plays an important role in the acquisition of more com- plex skills formally taught in elementary school (Adams, Treiman, & Pressley, 1998; Baroody, 2003; Griffin, Case, & Capodilupo, 1995; Tunmer & Nesdale, 1998). Theoretically, children’s atten- tion and socioemotional skills should also affect achievement
  • 79. because they influence children’s engagement in learning activities and facilitate (or disrupt) classroom processes (Ladd, Birch, & Buhs, 1999; Pianta & Stuhlman, 2004). However, some scholars point out that it is important to distinguish between behaviors that are directly relevant for learning, such as attention, and those that may be correlated with attention but are less likely to be directly linked with achievement, such as interpersonal skills and problem behavior (Alexander et al., 1993; Cooper & Farran, 1991; McClel- land et al., 2000; McWayne, Fantuzzo, & McDermott, 2004). Therefore, we expected early academic and attention-related skills to predict subsequent test scores and teacher achievement ratings, and we expected attention skills to predict achievement more consistently than do socioemotional behaviors. In seeking a better understanding of the extent to which our broad set of early skills is associated with later achievement, it is important to consider how outcomes are being measured. Although test performance provides a key independent assessment of aca- demic achievement, teacher ratings also lend insight into chil- dren’s everyday performance in the classroom. Teachers’ evalua- tions are probably based on a broad picture of children’s accomplishments, which include their academic skills as well as whether they complete assignments on time, work independently, get along with others, and show involvement in the learning
  • 80. agenda of the classroom. Moreover, previous research has found that children’s behavior can play a role that is equal to, if not greater than, prior cognitive ability in predicting teacher-rated attainment or achievement (Lin, Lawrence, & Gorrell, 2003; Schaefer & McDermott, 1999) and academic skills (National Cen- ter for Education Statistics, 1993). Consequently, we expected a stronger relationship between school-entry socioemotional behav- iors and subsequent teacher-rated achievement than with subse- quent test scores. Although many previous studies have examined the association between early academic, attention, and socioemotional skills and subsequent achievement, few have systematically considered the extent to which these associations differ by gender (Trzesniewski et al., 2006). On average, boys receive poorer grades and have more difficulties related to school progress (e.g., grade retention, special education, and drop out) than do girls (Dauber, Alexander, & Entwisle, 1993; McCoy & Reynolds, 1999), and these differ- ences are especially pronounced among low-income children (Hin- shaw, 1992). Children from low-income families enter school with lower mean academic skills, and the gap tends to increase during the school years (Lee & Burkam, 2002). These groups also have higher rates of problems with attention and externalizing behavior (Entwisle et al., 2005; Miech, Essex, & Goldsmith, 2001; Raver, 2004).
  • 81. Despite differences in children’s behavior linked to gender and family socioeconomic status, few studies have considered whether gender and socioeconomic status moderate the association be- tween these early skills and behaviors and subsequent achieve- ment. We expected early academic skills, attention, and socioemo- tional behaviors to matter more for these subgroups, particularly when children enter school with very low levels of these skills. To estimate the associations between early academic skills and socioemotional behaviors and later school achievement, we sum- marize results from a coordinated series of analyses across six longitudinal data sets in two ways. First, we relate early academic, attention, and socioemotional skills to later achievement in each of the six data sets and provide a basic summary of these results. Second, we formally summarize the findings from these studies in a meta-analysis, again focusing on the extent to which this collec- tion of early skills predicts later achievement. Method In this section, we describe the data sets used in this study and the common analytic procedures that were implemented across studies. Detailed information about the measures, descriptive sta- tistics, and regression results from each study is presented in Appendices A–F, which can be found online. As the goal of our study was to relate early academic, attention, and socioemotional
  • 82. skills and behaviors to later achievement, each data set has mea- sures of these constructs, although there is variation across the studies with respect to when and how each skill or behavior is assessed. Table 1 provides an overview of data sources and measures available in each study. All six data sets provide measures of children’s academic skills as well as assessments of attention and socioemotional behaviors at about age 5 or 6. Because most children enter elementary school at this age, we refer to the timing of these measures as school entry but alert the reader that the precise timing varies considerably across studies. To facilitate comparison of findings across studies, we standardized all mea- sures to have a mean of 0 and standard deviation of 1. We measured achievement outcomes using teachers’ reports, test scores, and grade retention in early elementary school and, in some studies, middle childhood. In terms of the timing of the measurement of achievement outcomes, the children of the Na- tional Longitudinal Survey of Youth (NLSY) measures are as- sessed as late as early adolescence, the National Institute of Child Health and Human Development Study of Early Child Care and Youth Development (NICHD SECCYD) as late as fifth grade, and the 1970 British Birth Cohort Study (BCS) at age 10, whereas none of the other studies measures achievement beyond third grade. As for measurement methods, two studies have both test-score- based and teacher reports of reading and mathematics achievement (the
  • 83. Early Childhood Longitudinal Study–Kindergarten Cohort [ECLS- K] and NICHD SECCYD). We measured attention and socioemotional behaviors on the basis of mothers’ reports, teachers’ reports, and observation. Table 1 provides an overview of the similarities and differences in measurement across the six studies. One of our data sets, the Infant Health and Development Program (IHDP), has observer reports of 1431SCHOOL READINESS AND LATER ACHIEVEMENT T ab le 1 St ud y M ea su re s of
  • 150. er na l R ep or t 1432 DUNCAN ET AL. T ab le 1 (c on ti nu ed ) Sc ho ol en
  • 219. y. 1433SCHOOL READINESS AND LATER ACHIEVEMENT attention, another (NICHD SECCYD) has both test-based and teacher-rated measures of attention, and three (NLSY, IHDP, and BCS) have parent rather than teacher reports of socioemotional behaviors. In addition, two of the studies (NICHD SECCYD and the Montreal Longitudinal-Experimental Preschool Study [MLEPS]) measure both attention skills and problems, whereas three (NLSY, IHDP, and BCS) have measures of attention prob- lems but not skills, and one study (ECLS-K) has a measure of attention skills but not attention problems. In addition, with one exception, all of the studies provide measures of academic, atten- tion, and socioemotional skills prior to the point of school entry, which we used as key control variables in our analyses. The Studies and Samples The Early Childhood Longitudinal Study–Kindergarten Cohort (ECLS-K). The ECLS-K follows a nationally representative sam- ple of 21,260 children who were in kindergarten in 1998–1999. We used data from kindergarten, first grade, and third grade. Data were collected from multiple sources, including direct achieve- ment tests of children and surveys of parents, teachers, and school administrators (see Table 1; National Center for Education Statis- tics, 2001).
  • 220. Achievement tests were administered in the fall of kindergarten and in the spring of kindergarten, first grade, and third grade. We used teacher reports of children’s “approaches to learning” (which measure both attention skills and achievement motivation) and socioemotional behaviors, including internalizing and externaliz- ing problems, self-control with peers, and interpersonal skills, collected in the fall and spring of kindergarten. The battery of achievement tests given as part of the ECLS-K kindergarten and first-grade assessments covered three subject areas: language and literacy, mathematical thinking, and general knowledge. For third grade, the achievement tests included math- ematics, reading, and science. We used item response theory scores for the first two of these as key dependent variables. These third-grade assessments required students to complete workbooks and open-ended mathematics problems. As detailed in Appendix A, a host of family- and some child-level controls are available in the data. The children of the National Longitudinal Survey of Youth (NLSY). The NLSY is a multistage stratified random sample of 12,686 individuals age 14 to 21 in 1979 (Center for Human Resource Research, 2004). Black, Hispanic, and low-income youth were overrepresented in the sample. Annual (through 1994) and biennial (between 1994 and 2000) interviews with sample mem- bers and very low cumulative attrition in the study contribute to the
  • 221. quality of the study’s data. Beginning in 1986, the children born to NLSY female partici- pants were tracked through biennial mother interview supplements and direct child assessments. Given the nature of the sample, it is important to note that early cohorts of the child sample were born disproportionately to young mothers. With each additional cohort, the children become more representative of all children, and NLSY children younger than age 14 in 2000 share many demographic characteristics of their broader set of age mates. The sample used in the present analysis consists of 1,756 chil- dren whose academic achievement was tracked from age 7–8 to age 13–14 and whose achievement and behavior was assessed at age 5–6. Consequently, our sample comprises children who were age 5 or 6 in 1986, 1988, 1990, or 1992. The age 13–14 achieve- ment and behavior of these children were assessed in the respec- tive 1994, 1996, 1998, and 2000 interviews. School readiness measures, including math and reading test scores (Peabody Individual Achievement Test; Dunn & Mark- wardt, 1970) and maternal reports of children’s behavior problems (adapted from the Achenbach Behavior Problems Checklist; Baker, Keck, Mott, & Quinlan, 1993) were collected at age 5 or age 6. Academic achievement outcome measures were collected biennially for children between the ages of 5 and 14. In
  • 222. addition, key control variables include children’s receptive vocabulary (Pea- body Picture Vocabulary Test—Revised; Dunn & Dunn, 1981) and children’s temperament (compliance and sociability) at age 3 or 4. Additional family- and child-level control variables are described in Appendix B. The NICHD Study of Early Child Care and Youth Development (SECCYD). Longitudinal data from the NICHD SECCYD are drawn from a multisite study of births in 1991 (NICHD Early Child Care Research Network, 2005a). Participants were recruited from hospitals located at 10 sites across the United States. During 24-hr sampling periods, 5,265 new mothers met the selection criteria and agreed to be contacted after returning home from the hospital. At 1 month of age, 1,364 healthy newborns were enrolled in the study. Although it is not nationally representative, the study sample closely matches national and census tract records with respect to demographic variables such as ethnicity and household income. The majority of children in the sample are White, 12% are African American, and 11% are Hispanic or of another ethnicity. About 30% of mothers had a high school education or less, and 14% were single parents (NICHD Early Child Care Research Network, 1997). The analysis sample had valid data on the achievement outcome measures and at least three sources of in- formation on the key independent variables (approximately 981 at
  • 223. first grade, 928 at third grade, and 907 at fifth grade). School readiness measures, including achievement tests and attention/impulsivity tasks, were administered in a controlled lab- oratory setting at age 4.5, and attention problems, aggression, internalizing behavior, and social skills were measured by teacher report in the fall of the kindergarten year. Outcomes at first, third, and fifth grades include achievement in math and reading accord- ing to teacher ratings and Woodcock–Johnson Tests of Achieve- ment—Revised test scores (Woodcock & Johnson, 1990; see Table 1). Key control variables at age 3 include children’s cognitive ability, language skills, impulsivity, externalizing problems, and internalizing problems. The NICHD SECCYD also collects infor- mation from infancy about children’s early environments, includ- ing child-care type and quality, home environment, and parenting; these and other child- and family-level covariates are described in Appendix C. The Infant Health and Development Program (IHDP). The IHDP is an eight-site randomized clinical trial designed to evaluate the efficacy of a comprehensive early-intervention program for low birth weight (LBW) premature infants. Infants weighing 2,500 g (5.51 lb) or less at birth were screened for eligibility if their postconceptional age between January and October 1985 was 37
  • 224. weeks or less and if they were born in one of eight participating medical institutions. A total of 985 infants was randomly assigned either to a medical follow-up only or to a comprehensive early 1434 DUNCAN ET AL. childhood intervention group immediately following hospital dis- charge. Infants in both the comprehensive early childhood intervention and medical follow-up only groups participated in a pediatric follow-up program of periodic medical, developmental, and famil- ial assessments from 40 weeks of conceptional age (when they would have been born if they had been full term) to 36 months of age corrected for prematurity. The intervention program, lasting from hospital discharge until 36 months, consisted of home visits, child-care services, and parent group meetings. A coordinated educational curriculum of learning games and activities was used both during home visits and at the center. The primary analysis group consisted of 985 infants. Of these 985 infants, cognitive assessments are available for 843 children at age 3, 745 children at age 5, and 787 children at age 8. In addition, 76 children who were born at an extremely low birth weight (ELBW; 1,000 g [3.27 lb] or less) were excluded from the sample
  • 225. because ELBW children differ markedly from other LBW children in cognitive and behavioral functioning (Klebanov, Brooks- Gunn, & McCormick, 1994a, 1994b). Thus, this study focuses on a subsample of 690 children who were not born ELBW and for whom cognitive assessment and family background data were available. Data come from a variety of sources: questionnaires, home visits, and laboratory tests (see Table 1). School readiness mea- sures include preschool performance and verbal test scores, paren- tal reports of children’s mental health and aggressive behavior, and observer reports of children’s attention and task persistence. We assessed reading and math achievement using the Woodcock– Johnson Tests of Achievement—Revised broad reading and math tests and the Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children—Third Edition (Wechsler, 1991) performance and verbal tests at 8 years of age. Key control variables include cognitive ability, sustained attention, and behavior problems at age 3. Additional family- and child-level control variables are described in Appendix D. The Montreal Longitudinal-Experimental Preschool Study (MLEPS). The MLEPS comprises several consecutive cohorts launched from 1997 to 2000. The original sample of 4- and 5-year-old children (N � 1,928), representing one third of its population base, was obtained after a multilevel consent process involving school board administrators, local school committees, parents, and teachers. Given that its final cohort (2000) does not meet all the data requirements for the research objective
  • 226. examined here, we limited ourselves to the sample of children beginning kindergarten in the fall of 1998 and the fall of 1999. Incomplete data reduced the sample from 1,369 to 767 children. Students in the final sample had a valid value on any of the four outcome measures of interest (first- and third-grade achievement measures) and on at least four of the six socioemotional measures. Of the 767 participants in the final sample, 439 began kindergarten in 1998 and 328 began kindergarten in the fall of 1999. Addition- ally, for 350 of the 767 students, initial data were collected during the fall of junior kindergarten (332 who began junior kindergarten in 1997 and 18 who began junior kindergarten in 1998). Initial and follow-up data were collected from multiple sources, including direct cognitive assessments of children and surveys of parents and teachers. Early academic assessments include individ- ually administered number knowledge and receptive vocabulary tests at the end of senior kindergarten. Teachers rated children’s behavioral development, including physically aggressive, anxious, depressive, hyperactive, inattentive, and prosocial behavior. Third- grade assessments include a group-administered math test and teacher ratings of children’s French language skills (see Table 1). Key control variables include number knowledge and
  • 227. vocabulary measured on entry into junior kindergarten (age 4) for Cohort 1 and on entry into senior kindergarten (age 5) for Cohort 2. Addi- tional family- and child-level control variables are detailed in Appendix E. The 1970 British Birth Cohort Study (BCS). The U.K. 1970 BCS, a nationally representative longitudinal study, has followed into adulthood a cohort of children born in Great Britain during 1 week in 1970 (Bynner, Ferri, & Shepherd, 1997). The birth sample of 17,196 infants was approximately 97% of the target birth population. Attrition has reduced the original sample to 11,200 participants. Nevertheless, the representativeness of the original birth cohort has largely been maintained, although the current sample is disproportionately female and highly educated (Ferri & Smith, 2003). Missing data on key variables reduce the sample size for most analyses to between 9,000 and 10,000 cases. At each wave, cohort members were given a battery of tests of intellectual and behavioral development (see Table 1). School readiness measures include vocabulary and copying skills tests, and maternal reports of attention, externalizing behavior, and internalizing behavior were collected when the children were 5 years of age. Reading and mathematics achievement tests were administered at age 10. Key control variables include measures of basic skills and behavior at ages 22 and 42 months for a 10% subsample of the data. Additional family- and child-level controls are described in Appendix F.