SlideShare a Scribd company logo
1 of 86
Emotional Intelligence in Young and Middle Adulthood:
Cross-Sectional Analysis of Latent Structure and Means
Benjamin P. Chapman
University of Rochester Medical Center
Bert Hayslip Jr.
University of North Texas
Differentiation of the construct of emotional intelligence was
investigated in young and middle-aged
adults, on the basis of hypotheses generated from differential
emotions theory, discrete emotions
functionalist theory, and empirical literature on age-related
changes in affective complexity and differ-
entiation of abilities. Both age groups were characterized by the
same set of comparably related
dimensions. However, midlife adults reported significantly
greater use of optimism as a mood-regulation
strategy than was reported by young adults. This study
considers implications of possible structural
continuity in emotional intelligence in conjunction with mean
increases in the use of optimism as a
strategy for managing affect.
Keywords: emotional intelligence, midlife, differentiation
Since its introduction by Salovey and Mayer (1990) and popu-
larization by Goleman (1995), emotional intelligence (EI) has
been
a heavily researched individual difference construct. The EI
field
has more recently focused on two putatively distinct forms of
EI.
Trait EI is hypothesized to be a component of personality space
that is distinct from other traits such as the Big Five (Petrides &
Furnham, 2001), and it is measured by self-report inventories.
Ability EI is hypothesized to be a mental ability that is distinct
from other abilities (Mayer, Caruso, & Salovey, 1999), and it is
measured by performance tests.1
The issue of whether EI “behaves” more like a personality
trait or a cognitive ability has implications for its lifespan
trajectory, which with only a few exceptions, has been ne-
glected by researchers. Focusing mainly on the period from
infancy to young adulthood, Mayer, Salovey, Caruso, and Si-
tarenios (2001) proposed a “developmental criterion” for EI,
predicting that EI will increase with age, because it is an
adaptive function that develops in concert with cognitive and
social skills. Schaie (2001) pinpointed midlife as a time of peak
emotional functioning and called for cross-sectional compari-
sons involving samples of older adults. Kafetsios (2004) re-
ported that middle-aged persons scored higher than young per-
sons on performance EI tasks involving the facilitation,
understanding, and management of emotion. However, Ka-
fetsios’s primary concern was EI’s relationship to attachment
patterns, rather than its relationship to existing adult theories of
emotion related to development and aging.
The developmental criterion for EI implies mean-level
change in one or more dimensions of EI, but another question
prefigures this issue: Do the same specific dimensions charac-
terize the construct and/or exhibit similar interrelationships at
midlife as in young adulthood? Schaie (2001) raised this con-
cern on the basis of the literature on differentiation of abilities.
Unfortunately, it is difficult to make a strong prediction about
EI’s differentiation on the basis of this literature alone, for two
reasons: (1) Zelinski and Lewis (2003) have called into question
whether such a pattern is universally true for abilities, and (2)
EI appears to be a construct “somewhere at the intersection
between the domains of intelligence and personality” (Schaie,
2001, p. 202). Schaie’s observation suggests that the structural
configuration of EI might remain constant, because the struc-
ture of personality generally remains stable across time (cf.,
McCrae & Costa, 2003).
At least two theories may provide some basis for speculation
about the development of EI across the life span. Differential
emotions theory (DET) (Izard, 1991) posits that 11 basic emo-
tions function as the primary motivational units of human
personality (Izard & Malatesta, 1987) and suggests that the
1 Measurement poses a serious and unresolved problem for EI
research-
ers, because self-report items reflecting EI may or may not
elicit accurate
depictions, and “objectively correct” answers to items dealing
with emo-
tional experience are difficult to justify due to cultural and
contextual
differences in what may be “emotionally intelligent.” (For a
review of these
details, see MacCann, Roberts, Mathews, & Zeidner, 2004;
Mathews,
Roberts, & Zeidner, 2004; Mathews, Zeidner, & Roberts, 2002;
Roberts,
Zeidner, & Mathews, 2001; Zeidner, Mathews, & Roberts, 2001;
vs.
Mayer, Salovey, & Caruso, 2004a, 2004b; Mayer, Caruso, &
Salovey,
1999; Mayer, Salovey, Caruso, & Sitarenios, 2001).
Benjamin P. Chapman, Department of Psychiatry, Laboratory of
Per-
sonality and Development, University of Rochester Medical
Center, and
Bert Hayslip Jr., Department of Psychology, University of
North Texas.
This project was part of Benjamin P. Chapman’s dissertation
research,
conducted under the supervision of Bert Hayslip Jr. Portions of
this
research were presented at the 113th Annual Convention of the
American
Psychological Association, in Washington, DC, August 20,
2005.
Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to
Ben Chapman, University of Rochester Medical Center,
Department
of Psychiatry, Laboratory of Personality and Development. E-
mail:
[email protected]
Psychology and Aging Copyright 2006 by the American
Psychological Association
2006, Vol. 21, No. 2, 411– 418 0882-7974/06/$12.00 DOI:
10.1037/0882-7974.21.2.411
411
T
hi
s
do
cu
m
en
t i
s
co
py
ri
gh
te
d
by
th
e
A
m
er
ic
an
P
sy
ch
ol
og
ic
al
A
ss
oc
ia
tio
n
or
o
ne
o
f i
ts
a
lli
ed
p
ub
lis
he
rs
.
T
hi
s
ar
tic
le
is
in
te
nd
ed
s
ol
el
y
fo
r t
he
p
er
so
na
l u
se
o
f t
he
in
di
vi
du
al
u
se
r a
nd
is
n
ot
to
b
e
di
ss
em
in
at
ed
b
ro
ad
ly
.
basic quality of emotional experience remains constant over the
life span, which preserves the coherence of personality (Dough-
erty, Abe, & Izard, 1996). Malatesta and Wilson (1988) ex-
panded this notion with discrete emotions functionalist theory
(DEFT), arguing from the perspective of dynamic systems
(Magai & Nussbaum, 1996) that the habitual experience of
basic emotions drives crystallization of personality, but that a
period of acute high emotion may also lead to sudden per-
sonality change. Empirical literature supports both strong
temporal stability of individual differences in basic emotions
and close longitudinal links between discrete emotional expe-
riences and personality traits (Costa & McCrae, 1996; Costa,
McCrae, & Zonderman, 1987; Izard, Libero, Putnam, &
Haynes, 1993).
However, DET also proposes that the complexity of
affective– cognitive structures increases with age, through
growth in the associational network of images and thoughts
associated with each basic emotion (Dougherty et al., 1996).
This hypothesis is consistent with the cognitive–affective de-
velopmental theory (CADT) of Labouvie-Vief (1996;
Labouvie-Vief, DeVoe, & Bulka, 1989; Labouvie-Vief, Hakin-
Larson, DeVoe, & Schoeberlein, 1989), which proposes that
midlife is a time of increasing complexity in immediate emo-
tional experience. A growing body of empirical work on age-
related differences in the complexity of affect has produced
mixed results. In an experience-sampling study, Carstensen,
Pasupathi, Mayr, and Nesselroade (2000) found a correlation
between age and the number of eigenvalues extracted from
intraindividual correlation matrices of ratings of affect over
time. Lawton, Kleban, Rajagopal, and Dean (1992), in explor-
atory factor analyses of several dimensions of affect across age
groups, found an additional factor in midlife adults not evident
in their sample of young adults that was related to the damp-
ening of positive affect. Labouvie-Vief, DeVoe, & Bulka
(1989) also found evidence of greater cognitive–affective com-
plexity in narratives of midlife adults than in those of young
adults.
Other evidence suggests similarity, rather than differences, in
the experience of affect across age. Lawton, Kleban, Dean, Ra-
jagopal, and Parmelee (1992) found no differences between
young
and middle-aged adults in the correlation of factors related to
positive and negative affect; the loadings of emotion adjectives
were similar in the two groups. In another analysis of differenti-
ation of affect, Terracciano, McCrae, Hagemann, and Costa
(2003)
found that young adults produced a near-circular structure of
affect
while terms, midlife adults produced more clustered (i.e., less
differentiated) affect ratings in circumplex space. More recent
work suggests greater emotional heterogeneity in older versus
young adults (Charles, 2005), greater emotional specificity in
reaction to emotionally arousing stimuli among older adults
(Kun-
zmann & Gruhn, 2005), but less amplification and more
suppres-
sion of emotions in older persons (Kunzmann, Kupperbusch, &
Levenson, 2005).
Although these findings are mixed,2 one implication for the
EI construct—and one consistent with the stability of basic
emotional patterns implied by DET and DEFT—is that any
differentiation that does occur in the emotion system happens at
the molecular level of immediate affective experience, not at
the molar level of the higher emotional skills or traits. Terrac-
ciano et al. (2003) speculated that the differentiation of specific
emotions was a function of EI or its components, suggesting
that dimensions of EI may be organizational elements of per-
sonality structure. This hypothesis is consistent with DET,
which proposes that increasing, complexity in cognitive–
affective units occur against a backdrop of basic stability
(Dougherty et al., 1996); presumably, such stability would
require invariant organizational components of the emotion
system itself. From the standpoint of DEFT, the stability of
such underlying structures would contribute to the continuity in
emotional functioning requisite for personality coherence
(Malatesta & Wilson, 1988).
In this study, we sought to address Schaie’s (2001) question of
midlife differentiation of EI dimensions. On the basis of the
basic
continuity in emotional systems suggested by DET and DEFT,
we
hypothesized that the exploratorily derived factor structure of a
self-report measure of EI in young adults would be replicable in
midlife adults. This hypothesis was also informed by a
molecular–
molar distinction between basic affective experience, which
may
grow more complex and differentiated by midlife, and the EI
dimensions overseeing such experience, which would be
expected
to remain stable.
Another aim was to explore mean differences likely to
emerge along dimensions of EI. One reliably occurring dimen-
sion in EI factor analyses is emotion regulation; Kafetsios
(2004) found age-related increases in this component of EI.
This finding is consistent with several theories also suggesting
greater emotion-regulation at midlife. CADT proposes that
emotion regulation may improve at midlife as a function of
increasing cognitive–affective complexity and sensitivity to
context and that regulation efforts seek to maximize positive
affect and minimize negative affect (e.g., Labouvie-Vief,
Hakin-Larson et al., 1989). Maximization of positive affect is
also seen as an age-related shift tied to interpersonal patterns in
socioemotional selectivity theory (SST) (Carstensen, 1992,
1995). SST predicts that with age, individuals shift the breadth
and depth of their social networks in an effort to regulate
emotion, which becomes increasingly important relative to
other goals such as knowledge acquisition. Because emotion
regulation in our measure of EI taps the use of expectations of
positive outcome, findings on optimism in middle age are also
relevant. Chang & Sanna (2001) developed a model in which
optimism’s effects on life satisfaction were partially mediated
by positive affect, suggesting that optimism may be a mood-
regulation tool. Isaacowitz’s (2005) results reveal that midlife
adults are also more likely to use an optimistic explanatory
style
than are young adults. John and Gross (2004) also suggest that
reappraisal strategies, which involve positive construal of
emotion-eliciting events, are used more in midlife than in young
adulthood. We thus expected that midlife adults would report
better mood regulation, particularly to the extent that this di-
mension taps optimism.
2 These mixed findings may also be artifacts of the methods
used to
assess differentiation.
412 BRIEF REPORTS
T
hi
s
do
cu
m
en
t i
s
co
py
ri
gh
te
d
by
th
e
A
m
er
ic
an
P
sy
ch
ol
og
ic
al
A
ss
oc
ia
tio
n
or
o
ne
o
f i
ts
a
lli
ed
p
ub
lis
he
rs
.
T
hi
s
ar
tic
le
is
in
te
nd
ed
s
ol
el
y
fo
r t
he
p
er
so
na
l u
se
o
f t
he
in
di
vi
du
al
u
se
r a
nd
is
n
ot
to
b
e
di
ss
em
in
at
ed
b
ro
ad
ly
.
We offered no a priori hypotheses about mean differences
likely to emerge across other dimensions of EI, but we explored
these to the extent possible. Finally, on the basis of Schaie’s
(2001) speculation that EI may be differently related to person-
ality and intelligence at different points in the life span, we also
explored EI’s convergent and discriminant validity with per-
sonality traits and intellectual abilities in each study sample.
Method
Participants and Procedure
The sample of young adults consisted of 308 undergraduates
from a
large public university in the southern United States (74%
women; M �
20 years of age, SD � 2.2; 66% Anglo American) who were
recruited
through large courses in introductory psychology. The midlife
sample
consisted of 256 adults from the community (62% women; M �
49
years of age, SD � 5.4; 76% Anglo American), who were
reached
through community organizations and contacts in undergraduate
courses. The sample of midlife adults contained proportionally
more
women and Anglo Americans than the sample of college
students and
the midlife adults had more education than the college sample
did
(15.23 years vs. 13.58 years). Study participants completed a
battery of
measures for the North Texas Personality–Ability–Emotional
Intelli-
gence Project (Chapman, 2005), and these results were
confidential.
Emotional Intelligence Measure
EI was assessed by the Schutte Self-Report Inventory of
Emotional
Intelligence (SSRI) (Schutte et al., 1998), which is the most
commonly
used short measure of EI. It contains 33 five-point Likert-type
items
sampling equally from the domains of the original Salovey and
Mayer
(1990) model of EI: emotional appraisal, emotion regulation,
and utiliza-
tion. The SSRI correlates strongly in the expected direction
with measures
of alexithymia, depression, mood awareness and repair,
interpersonal func-
tioning, and cognitive task persistence (Schutte et al., 2001;
1998; Schutte,
Schuettpelz, & Malouff, 2002); discriminates between prisoners
and ther-
apists (Schutte et al., 1998); predicts partner satisfaction in
intimate dyads
(Schutte et al., 2001); and predicts emotional well-being
(Schutte, Malouff,
Simunek, McKenley, & Hollander, 2002). Internal consistency
ranges from
.84 to .90 (Austin, Saklofske, Huang, & McKinney, 2004;
Saklofske,
Austin, & Minski, 2003), and 2-week test–retest reliability is
.78 (Schutte
et al., 1998).
One criticism of the scale has been its potential overlap with
five-
factor instruments (Brackett & Mayer, 2003), although it
diverges more
from these constructs than other self-report measures such as
Bar-On’s
(1997) EQi (Mathews, Zeidner, & Roberts, 2002). Another
criticism has
been the scale’s unclear dimensionality. Petrides and Furnham
(2000a)
encouraged researchers to factor analyze the scale each time it
is used.
Results have consistently yielded factors for Emotion Appraisal,
Opti-
mism/Emotion Regulation, and Emotion Utilization (Austin et
al.,
2004; Petrides & Furnham 2000a; Saklofske et al., 2003); an
occasional
fourth dimension is Social Skills. These four factors have
differential
incremental validity in predicting adjustment to college
(Chapman &
Hayslip, 2005), and they differentially moderate the relationship
be-
tween stress and mental health (Ciarrochi, Dean, & Anderson,
2002).
Personality and Intelligence
Personality was assessed with the NEO-Five-Factor Inventory
(Costa &
McCrae, 1992), and intelligence was assessed with the matrices
and
synonyms subtests of Horn’s (1975) Fluid-Crystallized Sampler.
The
NEO-Five-Factor Inventory is a well-established and frequently
used in-
strument assessing the Big Five personality traits. The Fluid-
Crystallized
Sampler comprises objective tests of fluid ability and
crystallized ability
(Hayslip, 1989; Hayslip & Brookshire, 1985; Hayslip & Sterns,
1979;
Horn, 1977, 1975).
Results
Exploratory Analysis
Consistent with Petrides and Furnham’s (2000a) recommen-
dations, a principal axis exploratory factor analysis (EFA) was
conducted in the young adults, to determine the number of
dimensions underlying EI. A Minimum Average Partial Corre-
lation Test and Parallel Analysis (Zwick & Velicier, 1986)
revealed the lowest average partial correlations in the residual
correlation matrix after the extraction of the third factor, and
that only the first three Eigenvalues (8.09, 2.21, and 2.08) were
higher than the average corresponding Eigenvalues (1.67, 1.58,
1.52) of random matrices, respectively. These factors explained
38% of the common variance, and this result was comparable to
the findings of Austin et al. (2004); Petrides and Furnham
(2000a); and Saklofske et al. (2003).
Rotation proceeded obliquely and a stepwise variable selection
program (Kano & Harada, 2000, http://koko16.hus.osaka-
u.ac.jp/
�harada/sefa2002/stepwise/) was employed in order to provide
an
empirical evaluation of the items salient to each factor. The
first
factor—Appraising Others’ Emotions—was defined by four
items
(18, 25, 29, and 32) that dealt with perceiving and Appraising
Others’ Emotions, for example, “I am aware of the nonverbal
messages other people send.” The second factor—Optimistic
Mood Regulation—was defined by five items (3, 10, 12, 22, and
23), which dealt with maintaining an optimistic mood, for
exam-
ple, “I motivate myself by imagining a good outcome to tasks I
take on” and “When I experience a positive emotion, I know
how
to make it last.” The final factor—Emotion Utilization—was de-
fined by four items (7, 8, 26, and 27), which dealt with using
emotions for problem solving, motivation, or empathy, for
exam-
ple, “When I feel a change in emotions, I tend to come up with
new ideas.” These factors were consistent with those reported in
other analyses (cf., Austin et al., 2004; Petrides & Furnham,
2000b; Saklofske et al., 2003). When computed as unweighted
composites of constituent items, the factors correlated .30 –.40
(see
Table 1).
Confirmatory Analyses
The factor model was then tested in the midlife sample by using
CFA, fixing loadings of marker items on each factor, and
estimat-
ing all other parameters of the model with robust maximum-
likelihood estimation, which produces the Satorra-Bentler chi-
square (Satorra & Bentler, 2001) and scaled fit indices to
correct
for non-normality. The model was a good fit for the data on
the midlife adults, SBX2 (62) � 87.35, p � .02, CFI � .946,
413BRIEF REPORTS
T
hi
s
do
cu
m
en
t i
s
co
py
ri
gh
te
d
by
th
e
A
m
er
ic
an
P
sy
ch
ol
og
ic
al
A
ss
oc
ia
tio
n
or
o
ne
o
f i
ts
a
lli
ed
p
ub
lis
he
rs
.
T
hi
s
ar
tic
le
is
in
te
nd
ed
s
ol
el
y
fo
r t
he
p
er
so
na
l u
se
o
f t
he
in
di
vi
du
al
u
se
r a
nd
is
n
ot
to
b
e
di
ss
em
in
at
ed
b
ro
ad
ly
.
RMSEA � .040.3 A series of nested models then followed in
which elements of the factor structure were constrained to
equality
across groups and degradations in fit evaluated by difference
SBX2
(Satorra & Bentler, 2001) and difference CFI (Cheung & Rens-
vold, 2002), releasing offending constraints before proceeding
to
more restrictive models. Equality tests proceeding in the
following
order (Horn & McCardle, 1992; Hertzog & Schaie, 1988): factor
loadings (metric invariance), item residuals (equal
uniquenesses),
factor variances (equal individual differences), and factor
covari-
ances (differentiation). The model achieved metric invariance,
partial uniqueness invariance, and invariant factor variances and
covariances, yielding no evidence of greater differentiation
among
the midlife adults than among the young adults. The multigroup
factor model and its standardized estimates are shown in Figure
1.
After these tests, mean and covariance structure modeling (cf.,
Hertzog & Schaie, 1988) revealed no differences in latent means
for the Appraising Others’ emotions or Emotion Utilization
factors
in the two age groups. However, the latent mean for Optimistic
Mood Regulation in midlife adults was significantly higher than
that for young adults (d � .34). To control for potential
confound-
ing by gender (Petrides & Furnham, 2000b), the series of nested
models and the Mean and Covariance Structure Analysis
(MACS)
model were repeated using a subset of the young adult sample in
which females were randomly eliminated until gender
proportions
were equal (Ns � 203 young, 246 midlife adults). Results were
nearly identical, but the differences in latent means for
Optimistic
Mood Regulation increased (d � .48).
Finally, the convergent and discriminant correlation matrices of
EI dimensions, the Big Five, and fluid ability and crystallized
ability in each group were submitted to an omnibus test of
corre-
lation matrix equality. This process revealed that five
correlations
differed significantly across groups, including three involving
di-
mensions of EI and the Big Five (see Table 1).
Discussion
Overall, this cross-sectional analysis yields preliminary evi-
dence that self-reported EI is configured similarly in midlife
and
young adulthood. This finding has several implications. First,
dimensions of EI, at least those measured by the SSRI, may be
more associated with the structurally invariant personality
system
than with differentiating cognitive abilities. Low or
nonsignificant
EI correlations with measures of fluid ability and crystallized
ability in Table 1 support such an interpretation. Second, the
dimensions characterizing EI may correspond more closely to
higher order emotional tendencies that constitute part of the
stable
backdrop of emotional continuity posited by DET (Dougherty et
3 Because our goal was to test the equivalence of various
elements of the
factor structure, (particularly factor covariances), the model was
tested
individually via CFA in the young adult sample in which it was
derived
preliminary to multiple group analyses. As expected, it fit well,
SBX2
(62) � 75.26, p � .12, CFI � .975, RMSEA � .027. Analyses
used only
complete cases in each sample (young, N � 305; midlife, N �
246).
Table 1
Convergent and Discriminant Correlations Between Dimensions
of Emotional Intelligence, the Big Five, and Crystallized Skills
and
Fluid Skills
Variable
Young adults
Mean (SD)
Midlife adults
Mean (SD) 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10
1. Neuroticism 24.14 (8.09) 20.00 (8.41) .86 �.43** .11*
�.30** �.34** �.14** �.07 �.07 �.46** �.03
.84
2. Extraversion 27.74 (7.37) 27.35 (7.15) �.43** .82 .08 .29**
.44** .00 .03 .27** .58** .14**
.83
3. Openness 27.78 (5.64) 24.75 (5.97) .11* .08 .69 .16** �.05
.27** .22** .20** .12** .14**
.63
4. Agreeableness 29.01 (5.65) 31.52 (5.94) �.30** .29** .16**
.76 .52* .17** .21** .21** .30** .24**
.68
5. Conscientiousness 28.29 (6.22) 33.71 (6.43) �.34** .44**
.12* .32** .83 .10* .06 .16** .49** .12**
.75
6. Crystallized skills 8.63 (3.18) 8.33 (3.26) �.14** .00 .27**
.17** .10* .95 .34** .05 .09 �.04
.78
7. Fluid skills 10.88 (2.51) 10.41 (5.03) �.07 .03 .22** .21**
.06 .34** .74 .03 .09 �.02
.73
8. Appraising others’
emotions 14.47 (2.72) 14.31 (2.57) �.07 .27** .20** .21**
.16** .05 .03 .76 .30** .40**
.77
9. Optimistic mood
regulation 14.54 (2.58) 15.21 (2.30) �.46** .58** .12** .46**
.49** .02 .09 .30** .69 .32**
.67
10. Emotion utilization 13.78 (2.62) 13.66 (2.56) �.03 .28**
.14** .17** .12** �.04 �.02 .40** .32** .62
.62
Note. Means and standard deviations of raw scores for each
variable. Correlations above diagonal for subset of midlife
sample (N � 204), correlations
below diagonal are for subset of young adult sample (N � 242).
Initial omnibus test of equality of correlation matrices, SBX2
(45) � 74.38, p � .0038,
CFI � .959, RMSEA � .038, revealed that corresponding
bolded off diagonal elements differed significantly between
samples. Freeing these correlations
to vary in each sample resulted in near perfect model fit, SBX2
(40) � 39.18, p � .507, CFI � 1.00, RMSEA � .000, �SBX2 �
33.45, �df � 5, p �
.001, �CFI � �.041.
* p � .05. ** p � .01 Diagonal shows Cronbach’s alpha internal
consistency estimates.
414 BRIEF REPORTS
T
hi
s
do
cu
m
en
t i
s
co
py
ri
gh
te
d
by
th
e
A
m
er
ic
an
P
sy
ch
ol
og
ic
al
A
ss
oc
ia
tio
n
or
o
ne
o
f i
ts
a
lli
ed
p
ub
lis
he
rs
.
T
hi
s
ar
tic
le
is
in
te
nd
ed
s
ol
el
y
fo
r t
he
p
er
so
na
l u
se
o
f t
he
in
di
vi
du
al
u
se
r a
nd
is
n
ot
to
b
e
di
ss
em
in
at
ed
b
ro
ad
ly
.
al., 1996). Both the theoretical differentiation of cognitive–
affective structures supposed by DET and the increasing
complex-
ity of immediate emotional experience (e.g., Carstensen et al.,
2000) may represent a more molecular level of emotional
experi-
ence, whereas broad EI dimensions function as molar,
individual
difference constructs.
The EI factors that emerged were consistent with those reported
in previous research, and midlife adults’ higher latent means for
Optimistic Mood Regulation echo Kafetsios’ (2004) findings of
better emotion management in midlife adults. This factor dealt
primarily with maintaining hope and optimism. Isaacowtiz’s
(2005) results also indicate that optimism tends to increase with
age. Thus, optimism may be used as a strategy to maximize
positive affect, consistent with Diehl, Coyle, and Labouvie-
Vief’s
(1996) findings that older adults may regulate mood with more
cheerful interpretations of conflict situations.4 Finally, “looking
on
the bright side” may be a form of what Shulz and Heckhausen
(1998) have deemed secondary control. Primary control
involves
instrumental action on the environment to regulate emotion;
sec-
ondary control involves altering one’s internal perspective to
reg-
4 As one reviewer pointed out, Optimistic Mood Regulation
shares
empirical and conceptual overlap with Extraversion and
Neuroticism, two
affectively loaded Big Five traits. However, midlife adults’
observed
scores were still significantly higher than young adults’ scores
in an
analysis of covariance controlling for Extraversion and
Neuroticism, F(1,
504) � 5.01, p � .026.
Figure 1. Fit of final multiple groups covariance structure
model � SBX2 (150) � 182.22, p � .038, CFI �
.969, RMSEA � .020. Parameters with two estimates indicate
elements of model that differ across groups;
boldfaced estimates are for midlife adults (N � 246). Young
adults N � 305.
415BRIEF REPORTS
T
hi
s
do
cu
m
en
t i
s
co
py
ri
gh
te
d
by
th
e
A
m
er
ic
an
P
sy
ch
ol
og
ic
al
A
ss
oc
ia
tio
n
or
o
ne
o
f i
ts
a
lli
ed
p
ub
lis
he
rs
.
T
hi
s
ar
tic
le
is
in
te
nd
ed
s
ol
el
y
fo
r t
he
p
er
so
na
l u
se
o
f t
he
in
di
vi
du
al
u
se
r a
nd
is
n
ot
to
b
e
di
ss
em
in
at
ed
b
ro
ad
ly
.
ulate emotion; and midlife marks the beginning of a shift
between
relative preference for primary over secondary control
strategies.
John and Gross’ (2004) notion of age-related increases in reap-
praisal and decreases in suppression as emotional regulatory
mech-
anisms is similarly commensurate with greater use of optimism
among midlife adults.
With respect to the other two dimensions of EI, midlife adults
did not report better appraisal of others’ emotions. This finding
is
inconsistent with DET predictions of improved emotion percep-
tion; the increased attention to context in emotion regulation,
suggested by CADT (Labouvie-Vief, 1996); and the notion that
better perception of others’ emotions may facilitate increased
interpersonal intimacy in significant relationships, derived from
SST. This null finding may not be an artifact of self-report,
because Kafetsios (2004) also failed to find age differences on
an
emotion perception task. One explanation may rest in the link
between emotion perception and the facial displays of others.
Midlife adults may interact more with older individuals, whose
facial expressions may be harder to judge, whereas younger
indi-
viduals may interact primarily with each other and with midlife
parents. Midlife adults also did not report greater use of
emotions
to solve problems or empathize with others.
However, both Emotion Utilization and Emotion Regulation
were differentially correlated with Agreeableness and
Extraversion
in the two groups. Because Agreeableness and Extraversion
have
sometimes been conceptualized as the dominance (vs.
submissive-
ness) and friendliness (vs. hostility) axes of the interpersonal
circumplex (Trapnell & Wiggins, 1990), these results may in
part
reflect age-related changes in the interplay between emotional
functioning and interpersonal relationships supposed by SST.
Op-
timistic Mood Regulation may be more tied to Agreeableness or
friendliness in young adulthood than in middle adulthood,
because
as SST posits, young adults use broader social networks in the
service of emotion regulation and for knowledge acquisition
(Carstensen, 1992, 1995). The finding that Emotion Utilization
is
more tied to Agreeableness or friendliness in midlife than in
young
adulthood is consistent with the findings of previous work sug-
gesting that midlife adults attend more to interpersonal
information
in problem solving than young adults do (Strough, Berg, & San-
sone, 1996; see also Blanchard-Fields & Abeles, 1996). On the
other hand, Emotion Utilization is slightly more tied to
Extraver-
sion or dominance in young adults than in midlife adults. Con-
ceivably, declines in the activity and excitement-seeking
compo-
nents of Extraversion (cf., Terracciano, McCrae, Brant, &
Costa,
2005) could alter this correlation in middle age. These
interpreta-
tions are preliminary, and links between EI and personality
across
the life span deserve further investigation in their own right, as
Schaie (2001) pointed out.
On balance, this study provided important initial information on
self-reported EI in young adulthood and middle age. However,
results must be qualified by the limitations of self-report.
Individ-
uals do not always reliably report their characteristic behavior
and
experience, and they may be even less able to accurately judge
skills of the sort implied by the term emotional “intelligence.”
Similarly, the stability of such self-reports is uncertain, because
test–retest intervals for the SSRI are limited. The use of a cross-
sectional design precludes statements about developmental
change, and limitations are compounded by variations in sample
demographics. Future work might use multiple methods
sampling
broader emotional functions and cohort sequential strategies to
disentangle age-related change, cohort effects, and antecedents
and
consequents of the change or stability of individual differences
in EI.
References
Austin, E. J., Saklofske, D. H., Huang, S. H. S., & McKinney,
D. (2004).
Measurement of trait-emotional intelligence: Testing and cross-
validating a modified version of Schutte et al.’s (1998) measure.
Per-
sonality and Individual Differences, 36, 555–562.
Bar-On, R. (1997). The Emotional Quotient Inventory technical
manual.
Toronto: Multihealth Systems.
Blanchard-Fields, F., & Abeles, R. (1996). Social cognition and
aging. In
J. E. Buren & K. W. Schaie (Eds.), Handbook of the psychology
of aging
(4th ed., pp. 150 –161). San Diego, CA: Academic Press.
Brackett, M. A., & Mayer, J. D. (2003). Convergent,
discriminant and
incremental validity of competing measures of emotional
intelligence.
Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 29, 1147–1158.
Carstensen, L. L. (1992). Social and emotional patterns in
adulthood:
Support for socioemotional selectivity theory. Psychology and
Aging, 7,
331–338.
Carstensen, L. L. (1995). Evidence for a life span theory of
socioemotional
selectivity. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 4,
151–156.
Carstensen, L. L., Pasupathi, M., Mayr, U., & Nesselroade, J. R.
(2000).
Emotional experience in everyday life across the adult life span.
Journal
of Personality and Social Psychology, 79, 644 – 655.
Chang, E. C., & Sanna, L. J. (2001). Optimism, pessimism, and
positive
and negative affectivity in middle-aged adults: A test of a
cognitive-
affective model of psychological adjustment. Psychology and
Aging, 16,
524 –531.
Chapman, B., & Hayslip, B. (2005). Incremental validity of a
measure of
emotional intelligence. Journal of Personality Assessment, 85,
154 –169.
Chapman, B. P. (2005). Emotional intelligence at mid life: A
cross sec-
tional investigation of factorial variance, social correlates, and
rela-
tionships to established personality and ability taxonomies.
Unpublished
doctoral dissertation, University of North Texas, Denton.
Charles, S. T. (2005). Viewing injustice: Greater emotion
heterogeneity
with age. Psychology and Aging, 20, 159 –164.
Cheung, G. W., & Rensvold, R. B. (2002). Evaluating goodness-
of-fit
indexes for testing measurement invariance. Structural Equation
Mod-
eling, 9, 233–255.
Ciarrochi, J., Dean, F. P., & Anderson, S. (2002). Emotional
intelligence
moderates the relationship between stress and mental health.
Personality
and Individual Differences, 32, 197–209.
Costa, P. T., & McCrae, R. R. (1992). Neo-PI, revised:
Technical manual.
Odessa, FL: Psychological Assessment Resources, Inc.
Costa, P. T., & McCrae, R. R. (1996). Mood and personality in
adulthood.
In C. Magai & S. H. McFadden (Eds.), Handbook of emotion,
adult
development, and aging (pp. 369 –381). San Diego, CA:
Academic
Press.
Costa, P. T., McCrae, R. R., & Zonderman, A. B. (1987).
Environmental
and dispositional influences on well-being: Longitudinal
followup of an
American national sample. British Journal of Psychology, 78,
299 –306.
Diehl, Coyle, & Labouvie-Vief (1996)
Dougherty, L. M., Abe, J. A., & Izard, C. E. (1996). Differential
emotions
theory and emotional development in adulthood and later life.
In C.
Magai & S. H. McFadden (Eds.), Handbook of emotion, adult
develop-
ment, and aging (pp. 27– 41). San Diego, CA: Academic Press.
Goleman, D. (1995). Emotional intelligence. New York: Bantam
Books,
Inc.
416 BRIEF REPORTS
T
hi
s
do
cu
m
en
t i
s
co
py
ri
gh
te
d
by
th
e
A
m
er
ic
an
P
sy
ch
ol
og
ic
al
A
ss
oc
ia
tio
n
or
o
ne
o
f i
ts
a
lli
ed
p
ub
lis
he
rs
.
T
hi
s
ar
tic
le
is
in
te
nd
ed
s
ol
el
y
fo
r t
he
p
er
so
na
l u
se
o
f t
he
in
di
vi
du
al
u
se
r a
nd
is
n
ot
to
b
e
di
ss
em
in
at
ed
b
ro
ad
ly
.
Hayslip, B., Jr. (1989). Alternative mechanisms for
improvements in fluid
ability performances among the aged. Psychology and Aging, 3,
122–
124.
Hayslip, B., & Brookshire, R. G. (1985). Relationships among
abilities in
elderly adults: A time lag analysis. Journal of Gerontology, 6,
748 –750.
Hayslip, B., & Sterns, H. L. (1979). Age differences in
relationships
between crystallized and fluid intelligences and problem
solving. Jour-
nal of Gerontology, 34, 404 – 414.
Hertzog, C., & Schaie, K. W. (1988). Stability and change in
adult
intelligence: II. Simultaneous analysis of longitudinal means
and covari-
ance structures. Psychology and Aging, 3, 122–130.
Horn, J. (1977). The aging of human abilities. In E. Wolman
(Ed.),
Handbook of developmental psychology. Englewood Cliffs, NJ:
Prentice
Hall.
Horn, J. L. (1975). Psychometric studies of aging and
intelligence. In S.
Gershon & A. Kankin (Eds.), Aging: Genesis and treatment of
psycho-
logical disorders in the elderly (Vol. 2). New York: Raven
Press.
Horn, J. L., & McCardle, J. J. (1992). A practical and
theoretical guide to
measurement invariance in aging research. Experimental Aging
Re-
search, 18, 117–144.
Isaacowitz, D. M. 2005. The gaze of the optimist. Personality
and Social
Psychology Bulletin, 31, 407– 415.
Izard, C. E. (1991). The psychology of emotions. New York:
Plenum Press.
Izard, C. E., Libero, D. Z., Putnam, P., & Haynes, M. O. (1993).
Stability
of emotion experiences and their relations to traits of
personality. Jour-
nal of Personality and Social Psychology, 64, 847– 860.
Izard, C. E., & Malatesta, C. Z. (1987). Perspectives on
emotional devel-
opment: I. Differential emotions theory of early emotional
development.
In J. D. Osofsy (Ed.), Handbook of infant development (2nd ed.,
pp.
494 –554). New York: Wiley-Interscience.
John, O., & Gross, J. (2004). Healthy and unhealthy emotion
regulation:
Personality processes, individual differences, and life span
development.
Journal of Personality, 72, 1301–1333.
Kafetsios, K. (2004). Attachment and emotional intelligence
abilities
across the life course. Personality and Individual Differences,
37, 129 –
145.
Kano, Y., & Harada, A. (2000). Stepwise variable selection in
factor
analysis [computer software]. Retrieved May 1, 2006, from
http://
koko16.hus.osaka-u.ac.jp/~harada/sefa2002/stepwise.
Kano, Y., & Harada, A. (2000). Stepwise variable selection in
factor
analysis. Psychometrika, 65, 7–22.
Kunzmann, U., & Gruhn, D. (2005). Age differences in
emotional reac-
tivity: The sample case of sadness. Psychology and Aging, 20,
47–59.
Kunzmann, U., Kupperbusch, C. S., & Levenson, R. W. (2005).
Behavioral
inhibition and amplification during emotional arousal: A
comparison of
two age groups. Psychology and Aging, 20, 144 –158.
Labouvie-Vief, G. 1996. Emotion, thought, and gender. In C.
Magai & S.
H. McFadaden (Eds.), Handbook of emotion, adult development,
and
aging (pp. 101–117). New York: Academic Press.
Labouvie-Vief, G., DeVoe, M., & Bulka, D. (1989). Speaking
about
feelings: Conceptions of emotions across the life span.
Psychology and
Aging, 4, 425– 437.
Labouvie-Vief, G., Hakin-Larson, J., DeVoe, M., &
Schoeberlein, S.
(1989). Emotions and self-regulation: A lifespan view. Human
Devel-
opment, 32, 279 –299.
Lawton, M. P., Kleban, M. H., Dean, J., Rajagopal, D., &
Parmelee, P. A.
(1992). The factorial generality of brief positive and negative
affect
measures. Journal of Gerontology, 47, 228 –237.
Lawton, M. P., Kleban, M. H., Rajagopal, D., & Dean, J.
(1992). Dimen-
sions of affective experience in three age groups. Psychology
and Aging,
7, 171–184.
MacCann, C., Roberts, R. D., Mathews, G., & Zeidner, M.
(2004). Con-
sensus scoring and empirical option weighting of performance-
based
Emotional Intelligence (EI) tests. Personality and Individual
Differ-
ences, 36, 645– 662.
Magai, C., & Halpern, B. (2001). Emotional development during
the
middle years. In Lachman, M. (Ed.), The handbook of midlife
develop-
ment, (pp. 310 –344). New York: Wiley.
Magai, C., & Nusbaum, B. (1996). Personality change in
adulthood:
Dynamic systems, emotions, and the transformed self. In C.
Magai &
S. H. McFadden (Eds.), Handbook of emotion, adult
development, and
aging (pp. 403– 419). San Diego, CA: Academic Press.
Malatesta, C. Z., & Wilson, A. (1988). Emotion cognition
interaction in
personality development: A discrete emotions, functionalist
analysis.
British Journal of Social Psychology, 27, 91–112.
Mathews, G., Roberts, R. D., & Zeidner, M. (2004). Seven
myths about
emotional intelligence. Psychological Inquiry, 15, 179 –196.
Mathews, G., Zeidner, M., & Roberts, R. D. (2002). Emotional
intelli-
gence: Science and myth. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Mayer, J., Salovey, P., & Caruso, D. (2004a). A further
consideration of the
issues of emotional intelligence. Psychological Inquiry, 15, 249
–255.
Mayer, J., Salovey, P., & Caruso, D. (2004b). Emotional
intelligence:
Theory, findings, and implications. Psychological Inquiry, 15,
197–215.
Mayer, J. D., Caruso, D., & Salovey, P. (1999). Emotional
intelligence
meets traditional standards for an intelligence. Intelligence, 27,
267–298.
Mayer, J. D., & Salovey, P. (1997). What is emotional
intelligence? In P.
Salovey, & D. J. Sluyter (Eds.), Emotional development and
emotional
intelligence: Educational implications (pp. 3–34). New York:
Basic
Books.
Mayer, J. D., Salovey, P. S., Caruso, D. R., & Sitarenios, G.
(2001).
Emotional intelligence as a standard intelligence. Emotion, 1,
232–242.
McCrae, R. R., & Costa, P. T. (2003). Personality in adulthood:
A
five-factor theory perspective. New York: The Guilford Press.
Petrides, K. A., & Furnham, A. (2000a). On the dimensional
structure of
emotional intelligence. Personality and Individual Differences,
29, 313–
320.
Petrides, K. A., & Furnham, A. (2000b). Sex differences in trait
emotional
intelligence. Sex Roles, 42, 449 – 461.
Petrides, K. A., & Furnham, A. (2001). Trait emotional
intelligence:
Psychometric investigation with reference to established trait
taxono-
mies. European Journal of Personality, 15, 423– 448.
Roberts, R. D., Zeidner, M., & Mathews, G. (2001). Does
emotional
intelligence meet traditional standards for an intelligence? Some
new
data and conclusions. Emotion, 1, 196 –231.
Saklofske, D. H., Austin, E. J., & Minski, P. S. (2003). Factor
structure and
validity of a trait emotional intelligence measure. Personality
and Indi-
vidual Differences, 34, 707–721.
Salovey, P., & Mayer, J. D. (1990). Emotional intelligence.
Imagination,
cognition, and Personality, 9, 185–211.
Satorra, A., & Bentler, P. M. (2001). A scaled chi-square test
statistic for
moment structure analysis. Psychometrika, 66, 507–514.
Schaie, K. W. (2001). Emotional intelligence: Psychometric
status and
developmental characteristics–Comment on Roberts, Zeidner,
and Mat-
thews (2001). Emotion, 1, 243–248.
Schutte, N. S., Malouff, J. M., Bobick, C., Coston, T. D.,
Greeson, C.,
Jedlicka, C., et al. (2001). Emotional intelligence and
interpersonal
relations. The Journal of Social Psychology, 121, 523–536.
Schutte, N. S., Malouff, J. M., Hall, L. E., Haggerty, D. J.,
Cooper, J. T.,
Golden, C. J., et al. (1998). Development and validation of a
measure of
emotional intelligence. Personality and Individual Differences,
25, 167–
177.
Schutte, N. S., Malouff, J. M., Simunek, M., McKenley, J., &
Hollander,
S. (2002). Characteristic emotional intelligence and emotional
well-
being. Cognition and Emotion, 16, 769 –785.
417BRIEF REPORTS
T
hi
s
do
cu
m
en
t i
s
co
py
ri
gh
te
d
by
th
e
A
m
er
ic
an
P
sy
ch
ol
og
ic
al
A
ss
oc
ia
tio
n
or
o
ne
o
f i
ts
a
lli
ed
p
ub
lis
he
rs
.
T
hi
s
ar
tic
le
is
in
te
nd
ed
s
ol
el
y
fo
r t
he
p
er
so
na
l u
se
o
f t
he
in
di
vi
du
al
u
se
r a
nd
is
n
ot
to
b
e
di
ss
em
in
at
ed
b
ro
ad
ly
.
Schutte, N. S., Schuettpelz, E., & Malouff, J. M. (2002).
Emotional
intelligence and task performance. Imagination, Cognition, and
Person-
ality, 20, 347–354.
Shulz, R., & Heckhausen, J. (1997). Emotion and control: A
lifespan
perspective. In K. W. Schaie & M. P. Lawton (Eds.), Annual
review of
gerontology and geriatrics: Focus on emotion and adult
development
(Vol. 17, pp. 185–205). New York: Springer.
Strough, J., Berg, C., & Sansone, C. (1996). Goals for solving
everyday
problems across the life span: Age and gender differences in the
salience
of interpersonal concerns. Developmental Psychology, 32, 1106
–1115.
Terracciano, A., McCrae, R. R., Brant. L. J., & Costa, P. T.
(2005).
Hierarchical linear modeling analyses of the NEO-PI-R scales in
the
Baltimore Longitudinal Study of Aging. Psychology and Aging,
20,
493–506.
Terracciano, A., McCrae, R. R., Hagemann, D., & Costa, P. T.
(2003).
Individual difference variables, affective differentiation, and
the struc-
tures of affect. Journal of Personality, 71, 669 –703.
Trapnell, P. D., & Wiggins, J. S. (1990). Extension of the
interpersonal
adjectives scales to include the Big Five dimensions of
personality.
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 59, 781–790.
Zeidner, M., Mathews, G., & Roberts, R. D. (2001). Slow down,
you move
too fast: Emotional intelligence remains an “elusive”
intelligence. Emo-
tion, 1, 265–275.
Zelinski, E. M., & Lewis, K. (2003). Adult age differences in
multiple
cognitive functions: Differentiation, dedifferentiation, or
process-
specific change? Psychology and Aging, 18, 727–745.
Zwick, W. R., & Velicier, W. F. (1986). Comparison of five
rules for
determining the number of components to retain. Psychological
Bulletin,
99, 432– 442.
Received January 27, 2005
Revision received December 2, 2005
Accepted December 28, 2005 �
418 BRIEF REPORTS
T
hi
s
do
cu
m
en
t i
s
co
py
ri
gh
te
d
by
th
e
A
m
er
ic
an
P
sy
ch
ol
og
ic
al
A
ss
oc
ia
tio
n
or
o
ne
o
f i
ts
a
lli
ed
p
ub
lis
he
rs
.
T
hi
s
ar
tic
le
is
in
te
nd
ed
s
ol
el
y
fo
r t
he
p
er
so
na
l u
se
o
f t
he
in
di
vi
du
al
u
se
r a
nd
is
n
ot
to
b
e
di
ss
em
in
at
ed
b
ro
ad
ly
.
fairly well satisfied before safety needs become im-
portant, and so on. As applied to social work prac-
tice, Maslow’s theory indicates that social workers
must first help clients meet basic needs (e.g., physio-
logical needs). Once clients’ basic needs are met,
higher-level needs can be dealt with.
Maslow did not offer an age-stage approach to
development. Striving for self-actualization is seen
as a universal process that can be observed at
nearly all ages. However, it is likely that there is
some progression among age groups. Infants prob-
ably have a strong emphasis on physiological
needs. As a person gradually grows older, safety
needs are emphasized, and then belongingness
and love needs, and so on. Because middle-aged
adults have had a variety of learning experiences
and tend to be at the peak of their earning poten-
tial, they tend to have a greater opportunity to fo-
cus on meeting self-actualization needs. However,
such crises as unemployment, prolonged illness,
and broken relationships can switch the emphasis
to a lower level of need.
Emotional Intelligence
Psychologists Peter Salovey and John Mayer coined
the term emotional intelligence (EI) in 1990 (Papalia
et al., 2009). It refers to the ability to recognize and
deal with one’s own feelings as well as the feelings of
others. Daniel Goleman (1995) popularized the EI
concept and expanded it to include such qualities
as empathy, motivation, social competence, optimism,
and conscientiousness.
McClelland had done some earlier work on
factors related to emotional intelligence (Papalia
et al., 2009). In the 1960s, the U.S. State Depart-
ment concluded that a test of general knowledge
was a poor predictor of how well those applying
to be foreign service officers would perform. In
addition, the test tended to screen out women
and people of color. McClelland devised a selec-
tion process that had nothing to do with general
knowledge. He found that the best foreign service
officers had positive expectations of others, were
perceptive of the needs of others, and were skillful
in forming social networks. His selection process
emphasized these characteristics, and led to the ap-
pointment of effective foreign service officers. It
also ended the discrimination against women and
people of color.
Goleman (1995) developed an EI test. Studying
nearly 500 corporations, Goleman found that those
who rose to the top of the corporate ladder tended to
score highest on EI. Goleman (1998, 2001) found the
following competencies to be most closely associated
with effective work performance:
• Self-awareness (accurate self-assessment, emotional
self-awareness, and self-confidence)
• Self-management (trustworthiness, achievement drive,
initiative, adaptability, and self-control)
• Social awareness (empathy, organizational aware-
ness, and service orientation)
• Relationship management (exerting influence,
conflict management, leadership, communication,
building bonds, teamwork and collaboration, be-
ing a catalyst for change, and developing others
Goleman (1998) found that excelling in at least one
competency in each of these four areas appears to be
a key to success in almost any job.
Emotional intelligence is not the opposite of cog-
nitive intelligence. Some very bright people score
high in EI, whereas others score lower. Some less-
than-average scorers on IQ tests score high on EI,
whereas others score lower.
Emotional intelligence is not easy to measure.
Papalia, Olds, and Feldman (2004, p. 478) note:
Hard as it is to assess cognitive intelligence, EI
may be even harder to measure. For one thing,
lumping the emotions together can be misleading.
How do we assess someone who can handle fear
but not guilt, or who can face stress better than
boredom? Then too, the usefulness of a certain
emotion may depend on the circumstances. Anger,
for example, can lead to either destructive or con-
structive behavior.
Self-
actualization
needs
Self-esteem needs
Belongingness and love needs
Safety needs
Physiological needs
FIGURE 11.1 Maslow’s Hierarchy of Needs
©
Ce
ng
ag
e
Le
ar
ni
ng
20
13
Psychological Aspects of Young and Middle Adulthood 475
Copyright 2012 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May
not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due
to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed
from the eBook and/or eChapter(s).
Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does
not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage
Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any
time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.
Social Intelligence
Closely related to emotional intelligence is social in-
telligence. Social Intelligence (SI) has been defined
in a variety of ways.
According to the original definition of Edward
Thorndike (1920, p. 228), social intelligence is “the
ability to understand and manage men and women,
boys and girls, to act wisely in human relations.”
According to this definition, it is equivalent to inter-
personal intelligence.
Some authors have restricted the definition of SI
to deal only with knowledge of social situations.
With this perspective, SI is synonymous with social
cognition or social marketing intelligence.
Daniel Goleman (2006) has drawn on social neu-
roscience research to propose that social intelligence
is made up of social awareness (including social cog-
nition, empathy, attunement, and empathic accu-
racy) and social facility (including self-presentation,
influence, concern, and synchrony).
There are various types of intelligence, including
intellectual competencies, emotional intelligence, and
social intelligence. Counseling/psychotherapy often
involves helping people to modify their patterns of
social intelligence, particularly those that cause clients
to have problems in their interpersonal relationships.
Some tests have been developed to measure SI
(Goleman, 2006). Like IQ tests, SI tests are usually
based on a 100-point scale in which 100 is the aver-
age score. Most people score between 85–115. Scores
of 140 are considered to be very high. People with SI
scores below 80 may have an autism spectrum disor-
der. These people are apt to have trouble making
friends, and with communication. They might need
social skill training. People with SI scores over 120
are considered to be very socially skilled and well
adjusted, and probably will excel in jobs that involve
direct contact and communication with people.
Mezzo-System Interactions:
Nonverbal Communication
In seeking to assess human behavior, it is also im-
portant to attend to nonverbal communication. Sig-
mund Freud (quoted in Knapp & Hall, 1992) noted,
“He that has eyes to see and ears to hear may con-
vince himself that no mortal can keep a secret. If his
lips are silent, he chatters with his finger tips;
betrayal oozes out of him at every pore” (p. 391).
It is impossible not to communicate. No matter
what we do, we transmit information about our-
selves. Even an expressionless face communicates
messages. As you are reading this, stop for a minute
and analyze what nonverbal messages you would be
sending if someone were observing you. Are your
eyes wide open or half closed? Is your posture
According to Maslow, we have a basic need to “belong” and a
basic need to be “loved.”
D
av
id
Yo
un
g-
W
ol
ff
/P
ho
to
Ed
it
476 Understanding Human Behavior and the Social Environment
Copyright 2012 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May
not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due
to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed
from the eBook and/or eChapter(s).
Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does
not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage
Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any
time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.
When psychological dependence occurs, the user
feels psychological discomfort if use is terminated.
Dependent users tend to believe that they will use
the chemical for the rest of their lives as a regular
part of social or recreational activities. They question
whether the desired emotional state can be achieved
without the use of the chemical, and they have a pre-
occupation with thinking and talking about the chem-
ical and activities associated with using it.
Users also generally develop a tolerance for some
drugs, which means they have to take increasing
amounts over time to achieve a given level of effect.
Tolerance depends partly on the type of drug, be-
cause some drugs (such as aspirin) do not create
tolerance.
Drug addiction is difficult to define. In a broad
sense, addiction refers to an intense craving for a
particular substance. The problem is that this defini-
tion could be applied to an intense craving for a
variety of substances—pickles, ice cream, potato
chips, strawberry shortcake. To avoid this problem,
we will define addiction as an intense craving for a
drug that develops after a period of heavy use.
Why Do People Use and Abuse Alcohol and
Other Drugs?
The effects of using drugs are numerous, ranging
from feeling light-headed to death through overdos-
ing. Drug abuse may lead to deterioration in health,
relationship problems, automobile accidents, child
abuse, spouse abuse, loss of job, low self-esteem, loss
of social status, financial disaster, divorce, and arrests
and convictions.
A distinction needs to be made between responsi-
ble drug use and drug abuse. Many drugs do have
beneficial effects when used responsibly; aspirin re-
lieves pain, alcohol helps people relax, tranquilizers
reduce anxiety, antidepressant drugs reduce depres-
sion, amphetamines increase alertness, morphine is a
painkiller, and marijuana is useful in treating glau-
coma. Irresponsible drug use is abuse, which was
defined earlier in this chapter.
Why do people abuse drugs? The reasons are nu-
merous. Drug companies widely advertise the bene-
ficial effects of their products. The media (such as
television and movies) glamorize the mind-altering
effects. Many popular songs highlight drinking.
Bars and cocktail lounges have become centers for
socializing, and promote drinking. Through such
channels, Americans have become socialized to ac-
cept drug usage as a part of daily living. Socializa-
tion patterns lead many people to use drugs, and for
some the use is a stepping-stone to abuse.
Attitudes toward drug use also encourage abuse.
For example, some college students believe they
should get blitzed or stoned after a tough exam.
Ryne Duren (1985), former pitcher for the New
York Yankees, asked this question: “I started becom-
ing an alcoholic at age four, even though I had my
first drink at age nine—how can this be?” Duren went
on to explain that at a very young age he became
socialized to believe that a real man was “someone
who could drink others under the table,” and that
the way to have fun was to get high on alcohol.
People abuse drugs for a variety of reasons. Some
people build up a tolerance to a drug and then
HIGHLIGHT 11.6
Steroid Use in Baseball
On March 30, 2006, baseball commissioner Bud Selig asked
former Senator George Mitchell to investigate steroid use in
baseball. On December 13, 2007, Mitchell released his report.
The report found steroid use to be rampant among former
and current players.
Eighty-six former and current players were named in the
report. (It is thought that there are many other users among
baseball players who have not yet been identified.) Steroids
have been on baseball’s banned substance list since 1991; how-
ever, testing of Major League players did not begin until 2003.
Seven Most Valuable Player Award winners were named
in the report, along with 31 All-Stars—at least one for every
position. Some of the biggest names in baseball are alleged to
have been users, including Barry Bonds, Roger Clemens, Mark
McGwire, David Justice, Jason Giambi, Gary Sheffield,
Miguel Tejada, Lenny Dykstra, Rafael Palmeiro, Andy
Pettitte, Chuck Knoblauch, and Alex Rodriguez. (Some
of these players have denied, under oath, that they used
steroids.)
To avoid testing positive for steroids, many athletes
looking for an edge have now turned to human growth hor-
mone (HGH) to build muscle. It is difficult to detect, and
the best test available has a window of detection of only
48–72 hours.
Psychological Aspects of Young and Middle Adulthood 505
Copyright 2012 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May
not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due
to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed
from the eBook and/or eChapter(s).
Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does
not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage
Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any
time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.
increase the dosage to obtain a high. Physical and
psychological dependence usually leads to abuse.
People with intense unwanted emotions (such as
loneliness, anxiety, feelings of inadequacy, guilt, de-
pression, insecurity, and resentment) may turn to
drugs. For many abusers, their drug of choice be-
comes their best friend because they tend to person-
alize it and value it more highly than they value their
friends. The drug is something that they can always
count on to relieve pain or give them the kind of
high they desire. Many abusers become so highly
attached to their drug that they choose to continue
using it even though it leads to deterioration of
health, divorce, discharges from jobs, automobile
accidents, alienation from children, loss of friends,
depletion of financial resources, and court appear-
ances. Drug abusers usually feel they need their
drug as a crutch to make it through the day.
Abusers develop an intimate relationship with
their drug of choice. Even though this relationship
is unhealthy, the drug plays a primary role in the
abuser’s life, dictates a certain lifestyle, fills a psycho-
logical need, and more often than not takes prece-
dence over family, friends, and work. Most abusers
deny their drug usage is creating problems for them,
because they know that admitting they have a drug
problem means they will have to end their relation-
ship with their best friend, and they deeply believe
they need their drug to handle their daily concerns
and pressures. Drug abusers are apt to use a number
of defense mechanisms in order to continue using
drugs. They rationalize adverse consequences of drug
abuse (such as the loss of a job) by twisting or dis-
torting reality to explain the consequences of their
behavior while under the influence. They minimize
the adverse consequences of their drug use. They
use projection to place the blame for their problems
on others; for example, “If you had a wife like mine,
you’d drink too.”
Theories About Drug Use
A variety of theories have been offered as to why
people use drugs. Biological theories assert that
physiological changes produced by the drugs eventu-
ally generate an irresistible craving for the drug.
Some biological theories also postulate that some
people are predisposed by their genetic structure to
abuse certain types of drugs. For example, some au-
thorities believe that genes play a role in predispos-
ing some people to alcoholism. Behavioral theories
hold that people use drugs because they find them
pleasurable and continue to use them because doing
so prevents withdrawal distress. Interactionist theo-
ries maintain that drug use is learned from interac-
tion with others in our culture. For example, people
drink alcohol because drinking is widely accepted.
Interactionist theories assert that those who use ille-
gal drugs such as marijuana or cocaine have contact
with a drug subculture that encourages them to ex-
periment with illegal drugs.
Interaction in Family Systems: A Theoretical
Approach to Drug Abuse
Wegscheider (1981) maintains that chemical depen-
dency is a family disease that involves and affects
each family member. Although she focuses on the
families of alcoholics, much of what she says may
also apply to the families of other types of chemical
substance abusers.
She cites several rules that tend to characterize
the families of drug abusers. First, the dependent
person’s alcohol use becomes “the most important
thing in the family’s life” (Wegscheider, 1981, p. 81).
The abuser’s top priority is getting enough alcohol,
and the family’s top priorities are the abuser, the abu-
ser’s behavior, and keeping the abuser away from al-
cohol. The goals of the abuser and of the rest of the
family are at completely opposite poles.
A second rule in an alcoholic family is that alco-
hol is not the cause of the problem. Denial is para-
mount. A third family rule maintains that the
dependent person is not responsible for his or her
behavior and that the alcohol causes the behavior.
There is always someone or something else to
blame. Another rule dictates that no one should
rock the boat, no matter what. Family members
strive to protect the family’s status quo, even when
the family is miserable. Yet other rules concern for-
bidding discussion of the family problem either
within or outside of the family, and consistently
avoiding stating one’s true feelings. Wegscheider
(1981) maintains that these rules protect the depen-
dent person from taking responsibility for his or her
behavior, and that the rules serve to maintain the
drinking problem.
Wegscheider (1981) goes on to identify several
roles that family members typically play. In addition
to the chemically dependent person, there is the chief
enabler, the family hero, the scapegoat, the lost
child, and the mascot.
The chief enabler’s main purpose is to assume the
primary responsibility for family functioning. The
506 Understanding Human Behavior and the Social Environment
Copyright 2012 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May
not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due
to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed
from the eBook and/or eChapter(s).
Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does
not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage
Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any
time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.
abuser typically continues to lose control and relin-
quishes responsibility. The chief enabler takes on
more and more responsibility and begins making
more and more of the family’s decisions. A chief
enabler is often the parent or spouse of the chemi-
cally dependent person.
Conditions often continue to deteriorate as the
chemically dependent person loses control. A posi-
tive influence is needed to offset the negative. The
family hero fulfills this role. The family hero is often
the person who does well at everything he or she
tries. The hero works hard at making the family
look as though it is functioning better than it is. In
this way, the family hero provides the family with
self-worth.
Another role typically to played by someone in
the family is that of scapegoat. Although the alco-
hol abuse is the real problem, a family rule man-
dates that this fact must be denied. Therefore, the
blame must be placed elsewhere. Frequently, an-
other family member is blamed for the problem.
The scapegoat often behaves in negative ways that
draw attention to him or her (for example, the per-
son gets caught stealing, runs away, or becomes ex-
tremely withdrawn). The scapegoat’s role is to
distract attention away from the chemically depen-
dent person and onto something else. This role helps
the family avoid addressing the problem of chemical
dependency.
Often, someone plays the role of lost child. This
is a person who seems relatively uninvolved with the
rest of the family and never causes any trouble. The
lost child’s purpose is to provide relief from some of
the pain the family is suffering. At least there is
someone in the family who neither requires much
attention nor causes any stress. The lost child is sim-
ply there.
Finally, chemically dependent families often have
someone playing the role of mascot. The mascot is
someone who has a good sense of humor and appears
not to take anything seriously. Despite how much the
mascot might be suffering inside, he or she provides a
little fun for the family.
In summary, chemical dependency is a problem
affecting the entire family. Each family member suf-
fers from the chemical dependency, yet each assumes
a role in order to maintain the family’s status quo
and help the family survive. Family members are
driven to maintain these roles no matter what hap-
pens. The roles eventually become associated with
survival.
The Application of Theory to
Client Situations: Treatment for
the Chemically Dependent
Person and His or Her Family
One of the first tasks in treatment is for
the chemically dependent person to
take responsibility for his or her own
behavior. The abuser must acknowl-
edge that he or she has a problem before beginning
to solve it. Several concepts are critical in working
with the family (Wegscheider, 1981). Family members
must first come to realize the extent of the problem.
They need to identify the chemical abuse as their ma-
jor problem. Additionally, they need to learn about
and evaluate their family dynamics. They need to eval-
uate their own behavior and break out of the roles that
have been maintaining the chemical abuse. The chief
enabler, in particular, must stop making excuses and
assuming the chemically dependent person’s responsi-
bilities. If the chemically dependent person is sick from
a hangover and cannot make it to school or work the
next day, it must be that person’s responsibility, not a
parent’s or spouse’s, to call in sick.
Family members eventually learn to confront the
chemically dependent person and give him or her
honest information about his or her behavior. For
instance, they are encouraged to tell the dependent
exactly how he or she behaved while having a black-
out. If the dependent person hit another family
member while drunk, this fact needs to be con-
fronted. The confrontation should occur not in an
emotional manner but rather in a factual one.
The family also needs to learn about the pro-
gression of the disease. We’ve already discussed
some characteristics of drug dependence. There is
a typical progression of an alcoholic’s feelings and
behavior. At first, only occasional relief drinking
occurs. Drinking becomes more constant. The de-
pendent person then begins to drink in secret and
to feel guilty about drinking. Memory blackouts
begin to occur and gradually increase in frequency.
The dependent person feels worse and worse about
his or her drinking behavior, but seems to have less
and less control over it. Finally, the drinking be-
gins to seriously affect the person’s work, family,
and social relationships. A job may be lost or all
school classes flunked. Perhaps family members
leave or throw the dependent person out. The de-
pendent person’s thinking becomes more and more
impaired.
EP 2.1.3b,
2.1.6b,
2.1.10a,
d, e, g, & j
Psychological Aspects of Young and Middle Adulthood 507
Copyright 2012 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May
not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due
to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed
from the eBook and/or eChapter(s).
Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does
not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage
Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any
time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.

More Related Content

Similar to Emotional Intelligence in Young and Middle AdulthoodCross-S.docx

IJOP_12249_REV_EV
IJOP_12249_REV_EVIJOP_12249_REV_EV
IJOP_12249_REV_EVPeipei li
 
Running head EFFECTS OF AGE ON DETECTION OF EMOTION 1Effe.docx
Running head EFFECTS OF AGE ON DETECTION OF EMOTION 1Effe.docxRunning head EFFECTS OF AGE ON DETECTION OF EMOTION 1Effe.docx
Running head EFFECTS OF AGE ON DETECTION OF EMOTION 1Effe.docxsusanschei
 
Positive Emotions and Cognition
Positive Emotions and CognitionPositive Emotions and Cognition
Positive Emotions and CognitionDokka Srinivasu
 
A Review Study on Spiritual Intelligence, Adolescence and Spiritual Intellige...
A Review Study on Spiritual Intelligence, Adolescence and Spiritual Intellige...A Review Study on Spiritual Intelligence, Adolescence and Spiritual Intellige...
A Review Study on Spiritual Intelligence, Adolescence and Spiritual Intellige...Jonathan Dunnemann
 
Gender differences
Gender differencesGender differences
Gender differencesSamerYaqoob
 
Chronic Emotional Detachment, Disorders, and Treatment-Team B
Chronic Emotional Detachment, Disorders, and Treatment-Team BChronic Emotional Detachment, Disorders, and Treatment-Team B
Chronic Emotional Detachment, Disorders, and Treatment-Team BSarah M
 
Emotion Recognition and Emotional Resonance: Exploring the Relationship betwe...
Emotion Recognition and Emotional Resonance: Exploring the Relationship betwe...Emotion Recognition and Emotional Resonance: Exploring the Relationship betwe...
Emotion Recognition and Emotional Resonance: Exploring the Relationship betwe...Rebecca Noskeau
 
N0507233_Project Report 2016
N0507233_Project Report 2016N0507233_Project Report 2016
N0507233_Project Report 2016Daniel Horsley
 
A Review Study on Spiritual Intelligence, Adolescence and Spiritual Intellig...
A Review Study on Spiritual Intelligence, Adolescence and Spiritual  Intellig...A Review Study on Spiritual Intelligence, Adolescence and Spiritual  Intellig...
A Review Study on Spiritual Intelligence, Adolescence and Spiritual Intellig...Jonathan Dunnemann
 
Narcissism and vulnerability
Narcissism and vulnerabilityNarcissism and vulnerability
Narcissism and vulnerabilityveropabon
 
1Running head INTROVERSION-EXTRAVERSION AND MARRIAGE BLISS.docx
1Running head INTROVERSION-EXTRAVERSION AND MARRIAGE BLISS.docx1Running head INTROVERSION-EXTRAVERSION AND MARRIAGE BLISS.docx
1Running head INTROVERSION-EXTRAVERSION AND MARRIAGE BLISS.docxeugeniadean34240
 
ESE ANX Study Paper
ESE ANX Study PaperESE ANX Study Paper
ESE ANX Study PaperSharon Hui
 

Similar to Emotional Intelligence in Young and Middle AdulthoodCross-S.docx (20)

IJOP_12249_REV_EV
IJOP_12249_REV_EVIJOP_12249_REV_EV
IJOP_12249_REV_EV
 
Running head EFFECTS OF AGE ON DETECTION OF EMOTION 1Effe.docx
Running head EFFECTS OF AGE ON DETECTION OF EMOTION 1Effe.docxRunning head EFFECTS OF AGE ON DETECTION OF EMOTION 1Effe.docx
Running head EFFECTS OF AGE ON DETECTION OF EMOTION 1Effe.docx
 
Positive Emotions and Cognition
Positive Emotions and CognitionPositive Emotions and Cognition
Positive Emotions and Cognition
 
A Review Study on Spiritual Intelligence, Adolescence and Spiritual Intellige...
A Review Study on Spiritual Intelligence, Adolescence and Spiritual Intellige...A Review Study on Spiritual Intelligence, Adolescence and Spiritual Intellige...
A Review Study on Spiritual Intelligence, Adolescence and Spiritual Intellige...
 
Gender differences
Gender differencesGender differences
Gender differences
 
Chronic Emotional Detachment, Disorders, and Treatment-Team B
Chronic Emotional Detachment, Disorders, and Treatment-Team BChronic Emotional Detachment, Disorders, and Treatment-Team B
Chronic Emotional Detachment, Disorders, and Treatment-Team B
 
Emotion Recognition and Emotional Resonance: Exploring the Relationship betwe...
Emotion Recognition and Emotional Resonance: Exploring the Relationship betwe...Emotion Recognition and Emotional Resonance: Exploring the Relationship betwe...
Emotion Recognition and Emotional Resonance: Exploring the Relationship betwe...
 
Dr. William Allan Kritsonis
Dr. William Allan KritsonisDr. William Allan Kritsonis
Dr. William Allan Kritsonis
 
N0507233_Project Report 2016
N0507233_Project Report 2016N0507233_Project Report 2016
N0507233_Project Report 2016
 
199RA_TonyWeeda
199RA_TonyWeeda199RA_TonyWeeda
199RA_TonyWeeda
 
Mpq SCALE
Mpq SCALEMpq SCALE
Mpq SCALE
 
Jonathan Ketley Dissertation
Jonathan Ketley DissertationJonathan Ketley Dissertation
Jonathan Ketley Dissertation
 
A Review Study on Spiritual Intelligence, Adolescence and Spiritual Intellig...
A Review Study on Spiritual Intelligence, Adolescence and Spiritual  Intellig...A Review Study on Spiritual Intelligence, Adolescence and Spiritual  Intellig...
A Review Study on Spiritual Intelligence, Adolescence and Spiritual Intellig...
 
Narcissism and vulnerability
Narcissism and vulnerabilityNarcissism and vulnerability
Narcissism and vulnerability
 
1Running head INTROVERSION-EXTRAVERSION AND MARRIAGE BLISS.docx
1Running head INTROVERSION-EXTRAVERSION AND MARRIAGE BLISS.docx1Running head INTROVERSION-EXTRAVERSION AND MARRIAGE BLISS.docx
1Running head INTROVERSION-EXTRAVERSION AND MARRIAGE BLISS.docx
 
Emotion differentiation
Emotion differentiationEmotion differentiation
Emotion differentiation
 
Family Systems Theory
Family Systems TheoryFamily Systems Theory
Family Systems Theory
 
Essay On Emotional Intelligence In Leadership
Essay On Emotional Intelligence In LeadershipEssay On Emotional Intelligence In Leadership
Essay On Emotional Intelligence In Leadership
 
ESE ANX Study Paper
ESE ANX Study PaperESE ANX Study Paper
ESE ANX Study Paper
 
synp.pptx
synp.pptxsynp.pptx
synp.pptx
 

More from SALU18

AFRICAResearch Paper AssignmentInstructionsOverview.docx
AFRICAResearch Paper AssignmentInstructionsOverview.docxAFRICAResearch Paper AssignmentInstructionsOverview.docx
AFRICAResearch Paper AssignmentInstructionsOverview.docxSALU18
 
Adversarial ProceedingsCritically discuss with your classmates t.docx
Adversarial ProceedingsCritically discuss with your classmates t.docxAdversarial ProceedingsCritically discuss with your classmates t.docx
Adversarial ProceedingsCritically discuss with your classmates t.docxSALU18
 
Advances In Management .docx
Advances In Management                                        .docxAdvances In Management                                        .docx
Advances In Management .docxSALU18
 
African-American Literature An introduction to major African-Americ.docx
African-American Literature An introduction to major African-Americ.docxAfrican-American Literature An introduction to major African-Americ.docx
African-American Literature An introduction to major African-Americ.docxSALU18
 
African American Women and Healthcare I want to explain how heal.docx
African American Women and Healthcare I want to explain how heal.docxAfrican American Women and Healthcare I want to explain how heal.docx
African American Women and Healthcare I want to explain how heal.docxSALU18
 
Advocacy & Legislation in Early Childhood EducationAdvocacy & Le.docx
Advocacy & Legislation in Early Childhood EducationAdvocacy & Le.docxAdvocacy & Legislation in Early Childhood EducationAdvocacy & Le.docx
Advocacy & Legislation in Early Childhood EducationAdvocacy & Le.docxSALU18
 
Advertising is one of the most common forms of visual persuasion we .docx
Advertising is one of the most common forms of visual persuasion we .docxAdvertising is one of the most common forms of visual persuasion we .docx
Advertising is one of the most common forms of visual persuasion we .docxSALU18
 
Adult Health 1 Study GuideSensory Unit Chapters 63 & 64.docx
Adult Health 1 Study GuideSensory Unit Chapters 63 & 64.docxAdult Health 1 Study GuideSensory Unit Chapters 63 & 64.docx
Adult Health 1 Study GuideSensory Unit Chapters 63 & 64.docxSALU18
 
Advertising Campaign Management Part 3Jennifer Sundstrom-F.docx
Advertising Campaign Management Part 3Jennifer Sundstrom-F.docxAdvertising Campaign Management Part 3Jennifer Sundstrom-F.docx
Advertising Campaign Management Part 3Jennifer Sundstrom-F.docxSALU18
 
Adopt-a-Plant Project guidelinesOverviewThe purpose of this.docx
Adopt-a-Plant Project guidelinesOverviewThe purpose of this.docxAdopt-a-Plant Project guidelinesOverviewThe purpose of this.docx
Adopt-a-Plant Project guidelinesOverviewThe purpose of this.docxSALU18
 
ADM2302 M, N, P and Q Assignment # 4 Winter 2020 Page 1 .docx
ADM2302 M, N, P and Q  Assignment # 4 Winter 2020  Page 1 .docxADM2302 M, N, P and Q  Assignment # 4 Winter 2020  Page 1 .docx
ADM2302 M, N, P and Q Assignment # 4 Winter 2020 Page 1 .docxSALU18
 
Adlerian-Based Positive Group Counseling Interventions w ith.docx
Adlerian-Based Positive Group Counseling Interventions w ith.docxAdlerian-Based Positive Group Counseling Interventions w ith.docx
Adlerian-Based Positive Group Counseling Interventions w ith.docxSALU18
 
After completing the assessment, my Signature Theme Report produ.docx
After completing the assessment, my Signature Theme Report produ.docxAfter completing the assessment, my Signature Theme Report produ.docx
After completing the assessment, my Signature Theme Report produ.docxSALU18
 
After careful reading of the case material, consider and fully answe.docx
After careful reading of the case material, consider and fully answe.docxAfter careful reading of the case material, consider and fully answe.docx
After careful reading of the case material, consider and fully answe.docxSALU18
 
AffluentBe unique toConformDebatableDominantEn.docx
AffluentBe unique toConformDebatableDominantEn.docxAffluentBe unique toConformDebatableDominantEn.docx
AffluentBe unique toConformDebatableDominantEn.docxSALU18
 
Advocacy Advoc.docx
Advocacy Advoc.docxAdvocacy Advoc.docx
Advocacy Advoc.docxSALU18
 
Advanced persistent threats (APTs) have been thrust into the spotlig.docx
Advanced persistent threats (APTs) have been thrust into the spotlig.docxAdvanced persistent threats (APTs) have been thrust into the spotlig.docx
Advanced persistent threats (APTs) have been thrust into the spotlig.docxSALU18
 
Advanced persistent threatRecommendations for remediation .docx
Advanced persistent threatRecommendations for remediation .docxAdvanced persistent threatRecommendations for remediation .docx
Advanced persistent threatRecommendations for remediation .docxSALU18
 
Adultism refers to the oppression of young people by adults. The pop.docx
Adultism refers to the oppression of young people by adults. The pop.docxAdultism refers to the oppression of young people by adults. The pop.docx
Adultism refers to the oppression of young people by adults. The pop.docxSALU18
 
ADVANCE v.09212015 •APPLICANT DIVERSITY STATEMENT .docx
ADVANCE v.09212015 •APPLICANT DIVERSITY STATEMENT .docxADVANCE v.09212015 •APPLICANT DIVERSITY STATEMENT .docx
ADVANCE v.09212015 •APPLICANT DIVERSITY STATEMENT .docxSALU18
 

More from SALU18 (20)

AFRICAResearch Paper AssignmentInstructionsOverview.docx
AFRICAResearch Paper AssignmentInstructionsOverview.docxAFRICAResearch Paper AssignmentInstructionsOverview.docx
AFRICAResearch Paper AssignmentInstructionsOverview.docx
 
Adversarial ProceedingsCritically discuss with your classmates t.docx
Adversarial ProceedingsCritically discuss with your classmates t.docxAdversarial ProceedingsCritically discuss with your classmates t.docx
Adversarial ProceedingsCritically discuss with your classmates t.docx
 
Advances In Management .docx
Advances In Management                                        .docxAdvances In Management                                        .docx
Advances In Management .docx
 
African-American Literature An introduction to major African-Americ.docx
African-American Literature An introduction to major African-Americ.docxAfrican-American Literature An introduction to major African-Americ.docx
African-American Literature An introduction to major African-Americ.docx
 
African American Women and Healthcare I want to explain how heal.docx
African American Women and Healthcare I want to explain how heal.docxAfrican American Women and Healthcare I want to explain how heal.docx
African American Women and Healthcare I want to explain how heal.docx
 
Advocacy & Legislation in Early Childhood EducationAdvocacy & Le.docx
Advocacy & Legislation in Early Childhood EducationAdvocacy & Le.docxAdvocacy & Legislation in Early Childhood EducationAdvocacy & Le.docx
Advocacy & Legislation in Early Childhood EducationAdvocacy & Le.docx
 
Advertising is one of the most common forms of visual persuasion we .docx
Advertising is one of the most common forms of visual persuasion we .docxAdvertising is one of the most common forms of visual persuasion we .docx
Advertising is one of the most common forms of visual persuasion we .docx
 
Adult Health 1 Study GuideSensory Unit Chapters 63 & 64.docx
Adult Health 1 Study GuideSensory Unit Chapters 63 & 64.docxAdult Health 1 Study GuideSensory Unit Chapters 63 & 64.docx
Adult Health 1 Study GuideSensory Unit Chapters 63 & 64.docx
 
Advertising Campaign Management Part 3Jennifer Sundstrom-F.docx
Advertising Campaign Management Part 3Jennifer Sundstrom-F.docxAdvertising Campaign Management Part 3Jennifer Sundstrom-F.docx
Advertising Campaign Management Part 3Jennifer Sundstrom-F.docx
 
Adopt-a-Plant Project guidelinesOverviewThe purpose of this.docx
Adopt-a-Plant Project guidelinesOverviewThe purpose of this.docxAdopt-a-Plant Project guidelinesOverviewThe purpose of this.docx
Adopt-a-Plant Project guidelinesOverviewThe purpose of this.docx
 
ADM2302 M, N, P and Q Assignment # 4 Winter 2020 Page 1 .docx
ADM2302 M, N, P and Q  Assignment # 4 Winter 2020  Page 1 .docxADM2302 M, N, P and Q  Assignment # 4 Winter 2020  Page 1 .docx
ADM2302 M, N, P and Q Assignment # 4 Winter 2020 Page 1 .docx
 
Adlerian-Based Positive Group Counseling Interventions w ith.docx
Adlerian-Based Positive Group Counseling Interventions w ith.docxAdlerian-Based Positive Group Counseling Interventions w ith.docx
Adlerian-Based Positive Group Counseling Interventions w ith.docx
 
After completing the assessment, my Signature Theme Report produ.docx
After completing the assessment, my Signature Theme Report produ.docxAfter completing the assessment, my Signature Theme Report produ.docx
After completing the assessment, my Signature Theme Report produ.docx
 
After careful reading of the case material, consider and fully answe.docx
After careful reading of the case material, consider and fully answe.docxAfter careful reading of the case material, consider and fully answe.docx
After careful reading of the case material, consider and fully answe.docx
 
AffluentBe unique toConformDebatableDominantEn.docx
AffluentBe unique toConformDebatableDominantEn.docxAffluentBe unique toConformDebatableDominantEn.docx
AffluentBe unique toConformDebatableDominantEn.docx
 
Advocacy Advoc.docx
Advocacy Advoc.docxAdvocacy Advoc.docx
Advocacy Advoc.docx
 
Advanced persistent threats (APTs) have been thrust into the spotlig.docx
Advanced persistent threats (APTs) have been thrust into the spotlig.docxAdvanced persistent threats (APTs) have been thrust into the spotlig.docx
Advanced persistent threats (APTs) have been thrust into the spotlig.docx
 
Advanced persistent threatRecommendations for remediation .docx
Advanced persistent threatRecommendations for remediation .docxAdvanced persistent threatRecommendations for remediation .docx
Advanced persistent threatRecommendations for remediation .docx
 
Adultism refers to the oppression of young people by adults. The pop.docx
Adultism refers to the oppression of young people by adults. The pop.docxAdultism refers to the oppression of young people by adults. The pop.docx
Adultism refers to the oppression of young people by adults. The pop.docx
 
ADVANCE v.09212015 •APPLICANT DIVERSITY STATEMENT .docx
ADVANCE v.09212015 •APPLICANT DIVERSITY STATEMENT .docxADVANCE v.09212015 •APPLICANT DIVERSITY STATEMENT .docx
ADVANCE v.09212015 •APPLICANT DIVERSITY STATEMENT .docx
 

Recently uploaded

Employee wellbeing at the workplace.pptx
Employee wellbeing at the workplace.pptxEmployee wellbeing at the workplace.pptx
Employee wellbeing at the workplace.pptxNirmalaLoungPoorunde1
 
CELL CYCLE Division Science 8 quarter IV.pptx
CELL CYCLE Division Science 8 quarter IV.pptxCELL CYCLE Division Science 8 quarter IV.pptx
CELL CYCLE Division Science 8 quarter IV.pptxJiesonDelaCerna
 
Proudly South Africa powerpoint Thorisha.pptx
Proudly South Africa powerpoint Thorisha.pptxProudly South Africa powerpoint Thorisha.pptx
Proudly South Africa powerpoint Thorisha.pptxthorishapillay1
 
Alper Gobel In Media Res Media Component
Alper Gobel In Media Res Media ComponentAlper Gobel In Media Res Media Component
Alper Gobel In Media Res Media ComponentInMediaRes1
 
Introduction to AI in Higher Education_draft.pptx
Introduction to AI in Higher Education_draft.pptxIntroduction to AI in Higher Education_draft.pptx
Introduction to AI in Higher Education_draft.pptxpboyjonauth
 
Painted Grey Ware.pptx, PGW Culture of India
Painted Grey Ware.pptx, PGW Culture of IndiaPainted Grey Ware.pptx, PGW Culture of India
Painted Grey Ware.pptx, PGW Culture of IndiaVirag Sontakke
 
Presiding Officer Training module 2024 lok sabha elections
Presiding Officer Training module 2024 lok sabha electionsPresiding Officer Training module 2024 lok sabha elections
Presiding Officer Training module 2024 lok sabha electionsanshu789521
 
ECONOMIC CONTEXT - PAPER 1 Q3: NEWSPAPERS.pptx
ECONOMIC CONTEXT - PAPER 1 Q3: NEWSPAPERS.pptxECONOMIC CONTEXT - PAPER 1 Q3: NEWSPAPERS.pptx
ECONOMIC CONTEXT - PAPER 1 Q3: NEWSPAPERS.pptxiammrhaywood
 
18-04-UA_REPORT_MEDIALITERAСY_INDEX-DM_23-1-final-eng.pdf
18-04-UA_REPORT_MEDIALITERAСY_INDEX-DM_23-1-final-eng.pdf18-04-UA_REPORT_MEDIALITERAСY_INDEX-DM_23-1-final-eng.pdf
18-04-UA_REPORT_MEDIALITERAСY_INDEX-DM_23-1-final-eng.pdfssuser54595a
 
internship ppt on smartinternz platform as salesforce developer
internship ppt on smartinternz platform as salesforce developerinternship ppt on smartinternz platform as salesforce developer
internship ppt on smartinternz platform as salesforce developerunnathinaik
 
Crayon Activity Handout For the Crayon A
Crayon Activity Handout For the Crayon ACrayon Activity Handout For the Crayon A
Crayon Activity Handout For the Crayon AUnboundStockton
 
Biting mechanism of poisonous snakes.pdf
Biting mechanism of poisonous snakes.pdfBiting mechanism of poisonous snakes.pdf
Biting mechanism of poisonous snakes.pdfadityarao40181
 
Solving Puzzles Benefits Everyone (English).pptx
Solving Puzzles Benefits Everyone (English).pptxSolving Puzzles Benefits Everyone (English).pptx
Solving Puzzles Benefits Everyone (English).pptxOH TEIK BIN
 
Incoming and Outgoing Shipments in 1 STEP Using Odoo 17
Incoming and Outgoing Shipments in 1 STEP Using Odoo 17Incoming and Outgoing Shipments in 1 STEP Using Odoo 17
Incoming and Outgoing Shipments in 1 STEP Using Odoo 17Celine George
 
Historical philosophical, theoretical, and legal foundations of special and i...
Historical philosophical, theoretical, and legal foundations of special and i...Historical philosophical, theoretical, and legal foundations of special and i...
Historical philosophical, theoretical, and legal foundations of special and i...jaredbarbolino94
 
Pharmacognosy Flower 3. Compositae 2023.pdf
Pharmacognosy Flower 3. Compositae 2023.pdfPharmacognosy Flower 3. Compositae 2023.pdf
Pharmacognosy Flower 3. Compositae 2023.pdfMahmoud M. Sallam
 
Organic Name Reactions for the students and aspirants of Chemistry12th.pptx
Organic Name Reactions  for the students and aspirants of Chemistry12th.pptxOrganic Name Reactions  for the students and aspirants of Chemistry12th.pptx
Organic Name Reactions for the students and aspirants of Chemistry12th.pptxVS Mahajan Coaching Centre
 
“Oh GOSH! Reflecting on Hackteria's Collaborative Practices in a Global Do-It...
“Oh GOSH! Reflecting on Hackteria's Collaborative Practices in a Global Do-It...“Oh GOSH! Reflecting on Hackteria's Collaborative Practices in a Global Do-It...
“Oh GOSH! Reflecting on Hackteria's Collaborative Practices in a Global Do-It...Marc Dusseiller Dusjagr
 
ECONOMIC CONTEXT - LONG FORM TV DRAMA - PPT
ECONOMIC CONTEXT - LONG FORM TV DRAMA - PPTECONOMIC CONTEXT - LONG FORM TV DRAMA - PPT
ECONOMIC CONTEXT - LONG FORM TV DRAMA - PPTiammrhaywood
 

Recently uploaded (20)

Employee wellbeing at the workplace.pptx
Employee wellbeing at the workplace.pptxEmployee wellbeing at the workplace.pptx
Employee wellbeing at the workplace.pptx
 
CELL CYCLE Division Science 8 quarter IV.pptx
CELL CYCLE Division Science 8 quarter IV.pptxCELL CYCLE Division Science 8 quarter IV.pptx
CELL CYCLE Division Science 8 quarter IV.pptx
 
Proudly South Africa powerpoint Thorisha.pptx
Proudly South Africa powerpoint Thorisha.pptxProudly South Africa powerpoint Thorisha.pptx
Proudly South Africa powerpoint Thorisha.pptx
 
Alper Gobel In Media Res Media Component
Alper Gobel In Media Res Media ComponentAlper Gobel In Media Res Media Component
Alper Gobel In Media Res Media Component
 
Introduction to AI in Higher Education_draft.pptx
Introduction to AI in Higher Education_draft.pptxIntroduction to AI in Higher Education_draft.pptx
Introduction to AI in Higher Education_draft.pptx
 
Painted Grey Ware.pptx, PGW Culture of India
Painted Grey Ware.pptx, PGW Culture of IndiaPainted Grey Ware.pptx, PGW Culture of India
Painted Grey Ware.pptx, PGW Culture of India
 
Presiding Officer Training module 2024 lok sabha elections
Presiding Officer Training module 2024 lok sabha electionsPresiding Officer Training module 2024 lok sabha elections
Presiding Officer Training module 2024 lok sabha elections
 
ECONOMIC CONTEXT - PAPER 1 Q3: NEWSPAPERS.pptx
ECONOMIC CONTEXT - PAPER 1 Q3: NEWSPAPERS.pptxECONOMIC CONTEXT - PAPER 1 Q3: NEWSPAPERS.pptx
ECONOMIC CONTEXT - PAPER 1 Q3: NEWSPAPERS.pptx
 
18-04-UA_REPORT_MEDIALITERAСY_INDEX-DM_23-1-final-eng.pdf
18-04-UA_REPORT_MEDIALITERAСY_INDEX-DM_23-1-final-eng.pdf18-04-UA_REPORT_MEDIALITERAСY_INDEX-DM_23-1-final-eng.pdf
18-04-UA_REPORT_MEDIALITERAСY_INDEX-DM_23-1-final-eng.pdf
 
internship ppt on smartinternz platform as salesforce developer
internship ppt on smartinternz platform as salesforce developerinternship ppt on smartinternz platform as salesforce developer
internship ppt on smartinternz platform as salesforce developer
 
ESSENTIAL of (CS/IT/IS) class 06 (database)
ESSENTIAL of (CS/IT/IS) class 06 (database)ESSENTIAL of (CS/IT/IS) class 06 (database)
ESSENTIAL of (CS/IT/IS) class 06 (database)
 
Crayon Activity Handout For the Crayon A
Crayon Activity Handout For the Crayon ACrayon Activity Handout For the Crayon A
Crayon Activity Handout For the Crayon A
 
Biting mechanism of poisonous snakes.pdf
Biting mechanism of poisonous snakes.pdfBiting mechanism of poisonous snakes.pdf
Biting mechanism of poisonous snakes.pdf
 
Solving Puzzles Benefits Everyone (English).pptx
Solving Puzzles Benefits Everyone (English).pptxSolving Puzzles Benefits Everyone (English).pptx
Solving Puzzles Benefits Everyone (English).pptx
 
Incoming and Outgoing Shipments in 1 STEP Using Odoo 17
Incoming and Outgoing Shipments in 1 STEP Using Odoo 17Incoming and Outgoing Shipments in 1 STEP Using Odoo 17
Incoming and Outgoing Shipments in 1 STEP Using Odoo 17
 
Historical philosophical, theoretical, and legal foundations of special and i...
Historical philosophical, theoretical, and legal foundations of special and i...Historical philosophical, theoretical, and legal foundations of special and i...
Historical philosophical, theoretical, and legal foundations of special and i...
 
Pharmacognosy Flower 3. Compositae 2023.pdf
Pharmacognosy Flower 3. Compositae 2023.pdfPharmacognosy Flower 3. Compositae 2023.pdf
Pharmacognosy Flower 3. Compositae 2023.pdf
 
Organic Name Reactions for the students and aspirants of Chemistry12th.pptx
Organic Name Reactions  for the students and aspirants of Chemistry12th.pptxOrganic Name Reactions  for the students and aspirants of Chemistry12th.pptx
Organic Name Reactions for the students and aspirants of Chemistry12th.pptx
 
“Oh GOSH! Reflecting on Hackteria's Collaborative Practices in a Global Do-It...
“Oh GOSH! Reflecting on Hackteria's Collaborative Practices in a Global Do-It...“Oh GOSH! Reflecting on Hackteria's Collaborative Practices in a Global Do-It...
“Oh GOSH! Reflecting on Hackteria's Collaborative Practices in a Global Do-It...
 
ECONOMIC CONTEXT - LONG FORM TV DRAMA - PPT
ECONOMIC CONTEXT - LONG FORM TV DRAMA - PPTECONOMIC CONTEXT - LONG FORM TV DRAMA - PPT
ECONOMIC CONTEXT - LONG FORM TV DRAMA - PPT
 

Emotional Intelligence in Young and Middle AdulthoodCross-S.docx

  • 1. Emotional Intelligence in Young and Middle Adulthood: Cross-Sectional Analysis of Latent Structure and Means Benjamin P. Chapman University of Rochester Medical Center Bert Hayslip Jr. University of North Texas Differentiation of the construct of emotional intelligence was investigated in young and middle-aged adults, on the basis of hypotheses generated from differential emotions theory, discrete emotions functionalist theory, and empirical literature on age-related changes in affective complexity and differ- entiation of abilities. Both age groups were characterized by the same set of comparably related dimensions. However, midlife adults reported significantly greater use of optimism as a mood-regulation strategy than was reported by young adults. This study considers implications of possible structural continuity in emotional intelligence in conjunction with mean increases in the use of optimism as a strategy for managing affect. Keywords: emotional intelligence, midlife, differentiation Since its introduction by Salovey and Mayer (1990) and popu- larization by Goleman (1995), emotional intelligence (EI) has been a heavily researched individual difference construct. The EI field
  • 2. has more recently focused on two putatively distinct forms of EI. Trait EI is hypothesized to be a component of personality space that is distinct from other traits such as the Big Five (Petrides & Furnham, 2001), and it is measured by self-report inventories. Ability EI is hypothesized to be a mental ability that is distinct from other abilities (Mayer, Caruso, & Salovey, 1999), and it is measured by performance tests.1 The issue of whether EI “behaves” more like a personality trait or a cognitive ability has implications for its lifespan trajectory, which with only a few exceptions, has been ne- glected by researchers. Focusing mainly on the period from infancy to young adulthood, Mayer, Salovey, Caruso, and Si- tarenios (2001) proposed a “developmental criterion” for EI, predicting that EI will increase with age, because it is an adaptive function that develops in concert with cognitive and social skills. Schaie (2001) pinpointed midlife as a time of peak emotional functioning and called for cross-sectional compari- sons involving samples of older adults. Kafetsios (2004) re- ported that middle-aged persons scored higher than young per- sons on performance EI tasks involving the facilitation, understanding, and management of emotion. However, Ka- fetsios’s primary concern was EI’s relationship to attachment patterns, rather than its relationship to existing adult theories of emotion related to development and aging. The developmental criterion for EI implies mean-level change in one or more dimensions of EI, but another question prefigures this issue: Do the same specific dimensions charac- terize the construct and/or exhibit similar interrelationships at midlife as in young adulthood? Schaie (2001) raised this con- cern on the basis of the literature on differentiation of abilities. Unfortunately, it is difficult to make a strong prediction about EI’s differentiation on the basis of this literature alone, for two
  • 3. reasons: (1) Zelinski and Lewis (2003) have called into question whether such a pattern is universally true for abilities, and (2) EI appears to be a construct “somewhere at the intersection between the domains of intelligence and personality” (Schaie, 2001, p. 202). Schaie’s observation suggests that the structural configuration of EI might remain constant, because the struc- ture of personality generally remains stable across time (cf., McCrae & Costa, 2003). At least two theories may provide some basis for speculation about the development of EI across the life span. Differential emotions theory (DET) (Izard, 1991) posits that 11 basic emo- tions function as the primary motivational units of human personality (Izard & Malatesta, 1987) and suggests that the 1 Measurement poses a serious and unresolved problem for EI research- ers, because self-report items reflecting EI may or may not elicit accurate depictions, and “objectively correct” answers to items dealing with emo- tional experience are difficult to justify due to cultural and contextual differences in what may be “emotionally intelligent.” (For a review of these details, see MacCann, Roberts, Mathews, & Zeidner, 2004; Mathews, Roberts, & Zeidner, 2004; Mathews, Zeidner, & Roberts, 2002; Roberts, Zeidner, & Mathews, 2001; Zeidner, Mathews, & Roberts, 2001; vs. Mayer, Salovey, & Caruso, 2004a, 2004b; Mayer, Caruso, & Salovey, 1999; Mayer, Salovey, Caruso, & Sitarenios, 2001). Benjamin P. Chapman, Department of Psychiatry, Laboratory of
  • 4. Per- sonality and Development, University of Rochester Medical Center, and Bert Hayslip Jr., Department of Psychology, University of North Texas. This project was part of Benjamin P. Chapman’s dissertation research, conducted under the supervision of Bert Hayslip Jr. Portions of this research were presented at the 113th Annual Convention of the American Psychological Association, in Washington, DC, August 20, 2005. Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to Ben Chapman, University of Rochester Medical Center, Department of Psychiatry, Laboratory of Personality and Development. E- mail: [email protected] Psychology and Aging Copyright 2006 by the American Psychological Association 2006, Vol. 21, No. 2, 411– 418 0882-7974/06/$12.00 DOI: 10.1037/0882-7974.21.2.411 411 T hi s do cu m
  • 9. ly . basic quality of emotional experience remains constant over the life span, which preserves the coherence of personality (Dough- erty, Abe, & Izard, 1996). Malatesta and Wilson (1988) ex- panded this notion with discrete emotions functionalist theory (DEFT), arguing from the perspective of dynamic systems (Magai & Nussbaum, 1996) that the habitual experience of basic emotions drives crystallization of personality, but that a period of acute high emotion may also lead to sudden per- sonality change. Empirical literature supports both strong temporal stability of individual differences in basic emotions and close longitudinal links between discrete emotional expe- riences and personality traits (Costa & McCrae, 1996; Costa, McCrae, & Zonderman, 1987; Izard, Libero, Putnam, & Haynes, 1993). However, DET also proposes that the complexity of affective– cognitive structures increases with age, through growth in the associational network of images and thoughts associated with each basic emotion (Dougherty et al., 1996). This hypothesis is consistent with the cognitive–affective de- velopmental theory (CADT) of Labouvie-Vief (1996; Labouvie-Vief, DeVoe, & Bulka, 1989; Labouvie-Vief, Hakin- Larson, DeVoe, & Schoeberlein, 1989), which proposes that midlife is a time of increasing complexity in immediate emo- tional experience. A growing body of empirical work on age- related differences in the complexity of affect has produced mixed results. In an experience-sampling study, Carstensen, Pasupathi, Mayr, and Nesselroade (2000) found a correlation between age and the number of eigenvalues extracted from intraindividual correlation matrices of ratings of affect over
  • 10. time. Lawton, Kleban, Rajagopal, and Dean (1992), in explor- atory factor analyses of several dimensions of affect across age groups, found an additional factor in midlife adults not evident in their sample of young adults that was related to the damp- ening of positive affect. Labouvie-Vief, DeVoe, & Bulka (1989) also found evidence of greater cognitive–affective com- plexity in narratives of midlife adults than in those of young adults. Other evidence suggests similarity, rather than differences, in the experience of affect across age. Lawton, Kleban, Dean, Ra- jagopal, and Parmelee (1992) found no differences between young and middle-aged adults in the correlation of factors related to positive and negative affect; the loadings of emotion adjectives were similar in the two groups. In another analysis of differenti- ation of affect, Terracciano, McCrae, Hagemann, and Costa (2003) found that young adults produced a near-circular structure of affect while terms, midlife adults produced more clustered (i.e., less differentiated) affect ratings in circumplex space. More recent work suggests greater emotional heterogeneity in older versus young adults (Charles, 2005), greater emotional specificity in reaction to emotionally arousing stimuli among older adults (Kun- zmann & Gruhn, 2005), but less amplification and more suppres- sion of emotions in older persons (Kunzmann, Kupperbusch, & Levenson, 2005). Although these findings are mixed,2 one implication for the EI construct—and one consistent with the stability of basic emotional patterns implied by DET and DEFT—is that any differentiation that does occur in the emotion system happens at the molecular level of immediate affective experience, not at
  • 11. the molar level of the higher emotional skills or traits. Terrac- ciano et al. (2003) speculated that the differentiation of specific emotions was a function of EI or its components, suggesting that dimensions of EI may be organizational elements of per- sonality structure. This hypothesis is consistent with DET, which proposes that increasing, complexity in cognitive– affective units occur against a backdrop of basic stability (Dougherty et al., 1996); presumably, such stability would require invariant organizational components of the emotion system itself. From the standpoint of DEFT, the stability of such underlying structures would contribute to the continuity in emotional functioning requisite for personality coherence (Malatesta & Wilson, 1988). In this study, we sought to address Schaie’s (2001) question of midlife differentiation of EI dimensions. On the basis of the basic continuity in emotional systems suggested by DET and DEFT, we hypothesized that the exploratorily derived factor structure of a self-report measure of EI in young adults would be replicable in midlife adults. This hypothesis was also informed by a molecular– molar distinction between basic affective experience, which may grow more complex and differentiated by midlife, and the EI dimensions overseeing such experience, which would be expected to remain stable. Another aim was to explore mean differences likely to emerge along dimensions of EI. One reliably occurring dimen- sion in EI factor analyses is emotion regulation; Kafetsios (2004) found age-related increases in this component of EI. This finding is consistent with several theories also suggesting
  • 12. greater emotion-regulation at midlife. CADT proposes that emotion regulation may improve at midlife as a function of increasing cognitive–affective complexity and sensitivity to context and that regulation efforts seek to maximize positive affect and minimize negative affect (e.g., Labouvie-Vief, Hakin-Larson et al., 1989). Maximization of positive affect is also seen as an age-related shift tied to interpersonal patterns in socioemotional selectivity theory (SST) (Carstensen, 1992, 1995). SST predicts that with age, individuals shift the breadth and depth of their social networks in an effort to regulate emotion, which becomes increasingly important relative to other goals such as knowledge acquisition. Because emotion regulation in our measure of EI taps the use of expectations of positive outcome, findings on optimism in middle age are also relevant. Chang & Sanna (2001) developed a model in which optimism’s effects on life satisfaction were partially mediated by positive affect, suggesting that optimism may be a mood- regulation tool. Isaacowitz’s (2005) results reveal that midlife adults are also more likely to use an optimistic explanatory style than are young adults. John and Gross (2004) also suggest that reappraisal strategies, which involve positive construal of emotion-eliciting events, are used more in midlife than in young adulthood. We thus expected that midlife adults would report better mood regulation, particularly to the extent that this di- mension taps optimism. 2 These mixed findings may also be artifacts of the methods used to assess differentiation. 412 BRIEF REPORTS T hi
  • 17. ed b ro ad ly . We offered no a priori hypotheses about mean differences likely to emerge across other dimensions of EI, but we explored these to the extent possible. Finally, on the basis of Schaie’s (2001) speculation that EI may be differently related to person- ality and intelligence at different points in the life span, we also explored EI’s convergent and discriminant validity with per- sonality traits and intellectual abilities in each study sample. Method Participants and Procedure The sample of young adults consisted of 308 undergraduates from a large public university in the southern United States (74% women; M � 20 years of age, SD � 2.2; 66% Anglo American) who were recruited through large courses in introductory psychology. The midlife sample consisted of 256 adults from the community (62% women; M � 49 years of age, SD � 5.4; 76% Anglo American), who were reached through community organizations and contacts in undergraduate
  • 18. courses. The sample of midlife adults contained proportionally more women and Anglo Americans than the sample of college students and the midlife adults had more education than the college sample did (15.23 years vs. 13.58 years). Study participants completed a battery of measures for the North Texas Personality–Ability–Emotional Intelli- gence Project (Chapman, 2005), and these results were confidential. Emotional Intelligence Measure EI was assessed by the Schutte Self-Report Inventory of Emotional Intelligence (SSRI) (Schutte et al., 1998), which is the most commonly used short measure of EI. It contains 33 five-point Likert-type items sampling equally from the domains of the original Salovey and Mayer (1990) model of EI: emotional appraisal, emotion regulation, and utiliza- tion. The SSRI correlates strongly in the expected direction with measures of alexithymia, depression, mood awareness and repair, interpersonal func- tioning, and cognitive task persistence (Schutte et al., 2001; 1998; Schutte, Schuettpelz, & Malouff, 2002); discriminates between prisoners and ther- apists (Schutte et al., 1998); predicts partner satisfaction in intimate dyads (Schutte et al., 2001); and predicts emotional well-being
  • 19. (Schutte, Malouff, Simunek, McKenley, & Hollander, 2002). Internal consistency ranges from .84 to .90 (Austin, Saklofske, Huang, & McKinney, 2004; Saklofske, Austin, & Minski, 2003), and 2-week test–retest reliability is .78 (Schutte et al., 1998). One criticism of the scale has been its potential overlap with five- factor instruments (Brackett & Mayer, 2003), although it diverges more from these constructs than other self-report measures such as Bar-On’s (1997) EQi (Mathews, Zeidner, & Roberts, 2002). Another criticism has been the scale’s unclear dimensionality. Petrides and Furnham (2000a) encouraged researchers to factor analyze the scale each time it is used. Results have consistently yielded factors for Emotion Appraisal, Opti- mism/Emotion Regulation, and Emotion Utilization (Austin et al., 2004; Petrides & Furnham 2000a; Saklofske et al., 2003); an occasional fourth dimension is Social Skills. These four factors have differential incremental validity in predicting adjustment to college (Chapman & Hayslip, 2005), and they differentially moderate the relationship be- tween stress and mental health (Ciarrochi, Dean, & Anderson, 2002).
  • 20. Personality and Intelligence Personality was assessed with the NEO-Five-Factor Inventory (Costa & McCrae, 1992), and intelligence was assessed with the matrices and synonyms subtests of Horn’s (1975) Fluid-Crystallized Sampler. The NEO-Five-Factor Inventory is a well-established and frequently used in- strument assessing the Big Five personality traits. The Fluid- Crystallized Sampler comprises objective tests of fluid ability and crystallized ability (Hayslip, 1989; Hayslip & Brookshire, 1985; Hayslip & Sterns, 1979; Horn, 1977, 1975). Results Exploratory Analysis Consistent with Petrides and Furnham’s (2000a) recommen- dations, a principal axis exploratory factor analysis (EFA) was conducted in the young adults, to determine the number of dimensions underlying EI. A Minimum Average Partial Corre- lation Test and Parallel Analysis (Zwick & Velicier, 1986) revealed the lowest average partial correlations in the residual correlation matrix after the extraction of the third factor, and that only the first three Eigenvalues (8.09, 2.21, and 2.08) were higher than the average corresponding Eigenvalues (1.67, 1.58, 1.52) of random matrices, respectively. These factors explained 38% of the common variance, and this result was comparable to the findings of Austin et al. (2004); Petrides and Furnham (2000a); and Saklofske et al. (2003).
  • 21. Rotation proceeded obliquely and a stepwise variable selection program (Kano & Harada, 2000, http://koko16.hus.osaka- u.ac.jp/ �harada/sefa2002/stepwise/) was employed in order to provide an empirical evaluation of the items salient to each factor. The first factor—Appraising Others’ Emotions—was defined by four items (18, 25, 29, and 32) that dealt with perceiving and Appraising Others’ Emotions, for example, “I am aware of the nonverbal messages other people send.” The second factor—Optimistic Mood Regulation—was defined by five items (3, 10, 12, 22, and 23), which dealt with maintaining an optimistic mood, for exam- ple, “I motivate myself by imagining a good outcome to tasks I take on” and “When I experience a positive emotion, I know how to make it last.” The final factor—Emotion Utilization—was de- fined by four items (7, 8, 26, and 27), which dealt with using emotions for problem solving, motivation, or empathy, for exam- ple, “When I feel a change in emotions, I tend to come up with new ideas.” These factors were consistent with those reported in other analyses (cf., Austin et al., 2004; Petrides & Furnham, 2000b; Saklofske et al., 2003). When computed as unweighted composites of constituent items, the factors correlated .30 –.40 (see Table 1). Confirmatory Analyses The factor model was then tested in the midlife sample by using CFA, fixing loadings of marker items on each factor, and estimat-
  • 22. ing all other parameters of the model with robust maximum- likelihood estimation, which produces the Satorra-Bentler chi- square (Satorra & Bentler, 2001) and scaled fit indices to correct for non-normality. The model was a good fit for the data on the midlife adults, SBX2 (62) � 87.35, p � .02, CFI � .946, 413BRIEF REPORTS T hi s do cu m en t i s co py ri gh te d by th e
  • 25. fo r t he p er so na l u se o f t he in di vi du al u se r a nd is n ot
  • 26. to b e di ss em in at ed b ro ad ly . RMSEA � .040.3 A series of nested models then followed in which elements of the factor structure were constrained to equality across groups and degradations in fit evaluated by difference SBX2 (Satorra & Bentler, 2001) and difference CFI (Cheung & Rens- vold, 2002), releasing offending constraints before proceeding to more restrictive models. Equality tests proceeding in the following order (Horn & McCardle, 1992; Hertzog & Schaie, 1988): factor loadings (metric invariance), item residuals (equal
  • 27. uniquenesses), factor variances (equal individual differences), and factor covari- ances (differentiation). The model achieved metric invariance, partial uniqueness invariance, and invariant factor variances and covariances, yielding no evidence of greater differentiation among the midlife adults than among the young adults. The multigroup factor model and its standardized estimates are shown in Figure 1. After these tests, mean and covariance structure modeling (cf., Hertzog & Schaie, 1988) revealed no differences in latent means for the Appraising Others’ emotions or Emotion Utilization factors in the two age groups. However, the latent mean for Optimistic Mood Regulation in midlife adults was significantly higher than that for young adults (d � .34). To control for potential confound- ing by gender (Petrides & Furnham, 2000b), the series of nested models and the Mean and Covariance Structure Analysis (MACS) model were repeated using a subset of the young adult sample in which females were randomly eliminated until gender proportions were equal (Ns � 203 young, 246 midlife adults). Results were nearly identical, but the differences in latent means for Optimistic Mood Regulation increased (d � .48). Finally, the convergent and discriminant correlation matrices of EI dimensions, the Big Five, and fluid ability and crystallized ability in each group were submitted to an omnibus test of corre- lation matrix equality. This process revealed that five correlations
  • 28. differed significantly across groups, including three involving di- mensions of EI and the Big Five (see Table 1). Discussion Overall, this cross-sectional analysis yields preliminary evi- dence that self-reported EI is configured similarly in midlife and young adulthood. This finding has several implications. First, dimensions of EI, at least those measured by the SSRI, may be more associated with the structurally invariant personality system than with differentiating cognitive abilities. Low or nonsignificant EI correlations with measures of fluid ability and crystallized ability in Table 1 support such an interpretation. Second, the dimensions characterizing EI may correspond more closely to higher order emotional tendencies that constitute part of the stable backdrop of emotional continuity posited by DET (Dougherty et 3 Because our goal was to test the equivalence of various elements of the factor structure, (particularly factor covariances), the model was tested individually via CFA in the young adult sample in which it was derived preliminary to multiple group analyses. As expected, it fit well, SBX2 (62) � 75.26, p � .12, CFI � .975, RMSEA � .027. Analyses used only complete cases in each sample (young, N � 305; midlife, N � 246).
  • 29. Table 1 Convergent and Discriminant Correlations Between Dimensions of Emotional Intelligence, the Big Five, and Crystallized Skills and Fluid Skills Variable Young adults Mean (SD) Midlife adults Mean (SD) 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 1. Neuroticism 24.14 (8.09) 20.00 (8.41) .86 �.43** .11* �.30** �.34** �.14** �.07 �.07 �.46** �.03 .84 2. Extraversion 27.74 (7.37) 27.35 (7.15) �.43** .82 .08 .29** .44** .00 .03 .27** .58** .14** .83 3. Openness 27.78 (5.64) 24.75 (5.97) .11* .08 .69 .16** �.05 .27** .22** .20** .12** .14** .63 4. Agreeableness 29.01 (5.65) 31.52 (5.94) �.30** .29** .16** .76 .52* .17** .21** .21** .30** .24** .68 5. Conscientiousness 28.29 (6.22) 33.71 (6.43) �.34** .44** .12* .32** .83 .10* .06 .16** .49** .12** .75 6. Crystallized skills 8.63 (3.18) 8.33 (3.26) �.14** .00 .27** .17** .10* .95 .34** .05 .09 �.04 .78
  • 30. 7. Fluid skills 10.88 (2.51) 10.41 (5.03) �.07 .03 .22** .21** .06 .34** .74 .03 .09 �.02 .73 8. Appraising others’ emotions 14.47 (2.72) 14.31 (2.57) �.07 .27** .20** .21** .16** .05 .03 .76 .30** .40** .77 9. Optimistic mood regulation 14.54 (2.58) 15.21 (2.30) �.46** .58** .12** .46** .49** .02 .09 .30** .69 .32** .67 10. Emotion utilization 13.78 (2.62) 13.66 (2.56) �.03 .28** .14** .17** .12** �.04 �.02 .40** .32** .62 .62 Note. Means and standard deviations of raw scores for each variable. Correlations above diagonal for subset of midlife sample (N � 204), correlations below diagonal are for subset of young adult sample (N � 242). Initial omnibus test of equality of correlation matrices, SBX2 (45) � 74.38, p � .0038, CFI � .959, RMSEA � .038, revealed that corresponding bolded off diagonal elements differed significantly between samples. Freeing these correlations to vary in each sample resulted in near perfect model fit, SBX2 (40) � 39.18, p � .507, CFI � 1.00, RMSEA � .000, �SBX2 � 33.45, �df � 5, p � .001, �CFI � �.041. * p � .05. ** p � .01 Diagonal shows Cronbach’s alpha internal consistency estimates.
  • 31. 414 BRIEF REPORTS T hi s do cu m en t i s co py ri gh te d by th e A m er ic an
  • 34. so na l u se o f t he in di vi du al u se r a nd is n ot to b e di ss
  • 35. em in at ed b ro ad ly . al., 1996). Both the theoretical differentiation of cognitive– affective structures supposed by DET and the increasing complex- ity of immediate emotional experience (e.g., Carstensen et al., 2000) may represent a more molecular level of emotional experi- ence, whereas broad EI dimensions function as molar, individual difference constructs. The EI factors that emerged were consistent with those reported in previous research, and midlife adults’ higher latent means for Optimistic Mood Regulation echo Kafetsios’ (2004) findings of better emotion management in midlife adults. This factor dealt primarily with maintaining hope and optimism. Isaacowtiz’s (2005) results also indicate that optimism tends to increase with age. Thus, optimism may be used as a strategy to maximize positive affect, consistent with Diehl, Coyle, and Labouvie- Vief’s
  • 36. (1996) findings that older adults may regulate mood with more cheerful interpretations of conflict situations.4 Finally, “looking on the bright side” may be a form of what Shulz and Heckhausen (1998) have deemed secondary control. Primary control involves instrumental action on the environment to regulate emotion; sec- ondary control involves altering one’s internal perspective to reg- 4 As one reviewer pointed out, Optimistic Mood Regulation shares empirical and conceptual overlap with Extraversion and Neuroticism, two affectively loaded Big Five traits. However, midlife adults’ observed scores were still significantly higher than young adults’ scores in an analysis of covariance controlling for Extraversion and Neuroticism, F(1, 504) � 5.01, p � .026. Figure 1. Fit of final multiple groups covariance structure model � SBX2 (150) � 182.22, p � .038, CFI � .969, RMSEA � .020. Parameters with two estimates indicate elements of model that differ across groups; boldfaced estimates are for midlife adults (N � 246). Young adults N � 305. 415BRIEF REPORTS T hi s
  • 41. b ro ad ly . ulate emotion; and midlife marks the beginning of a shift between relative preference for primary over secondary control strategies. John and Gross’ (2004) notion of age-related increases in reap- praisal and decreases in suppression as emotional regulatory mech- anisms is similarly commensurate with greater use of optimism among midlife adults. With respect to the other two dimensions of EI, midlife adults did not report better appraisal of others’ emotions. This finding is inconsistent with DET predictions of improved emotion percep- tion; the increased attention to context in emotion regulation, suggested by CADT (Labouvie-Vief, 1996); and the notion that better perception of others’ emotions may facilitate increased interpersonal intimacy in significant relationships, derived from SST. This null finding may not be an artifact of self-report, because Kafetsios (2004) also failed to find age differences on an emotion perception task. One explanation may rest in the link between emotion perception and the facial displays of others. Midlife adults may interact more with older individuals, whose facial expressions may be harder to judge, whereas younger indi-
  • 42. viduals may interact primarily with each other and with midlife parents. Midlife adults also did not report greater use of emotions to solve problems or empathize with others. However, both Emotion Utilization and Emotion Regulation were differentially correlated with Agreeableness and Extraversion in the two groups. Because Agreeableness and Extraversion have sometimes been conceptualized as the dominance (vs. submissive- ness) and friendliness (vs. hostility) axes of the interpersonal circumplex (Trapnell & Wiggins, 1990), these results may in part reflect age-related changes in the interplay between emotional functioning and interpersonal relationships supposed by SST. Op- timistic Mood Regulation may be more tied to Agreeableness or friendliness in young adulthood than in middle adulthood, because as SST posits, young adults use broader social networks in the service of emotion regulation and for knowledge acquisition (Carstensen, 1992, 1995). The finding that Emotion Utilization is more tied to Agreeableness or friendliness in midlife than in young adulthood is consistent with the findings of previous work sug- gesting that midlife adults attend more to interpersonal information in problem solving than young adults do (Strough, Berg, & San- sone, 1996; see also Blanchard-Fields & Abeles, 1996). On the other hand, Emotion Utilization is slightly more tied to Extraver- sion or dominance in young adults than in midlife adults. Con- ceivably, declines in the activity and excitement-seeking
  • 43. compo- nents of Extraversion (cf., Terracciano, McCrae, Brant, & Costa, 2005) could alter this correlation in middle age. These interpreta- tions are preliminary, and links between EI and personality across the life span deserve further investigation in their own right, as Schaie (2001) pointed out. On balance, this study provided important initial information on self-reported EI in young adulthood and middle age. However, results must be qualified by the limitations of self-report. Individ- uals do not always reliably report their characteristic behavior and experience, and they may be even less able to accurately judge skills of the sort implied by the term emotional “intelligence.” Similarly, the stability of such self-reports is uncertain, because test–retest intervals for the SSRI are limited. The use of a cross- sectional design precludes statements about developmental change, and limitations are compounded by variations in sample demographics. Future work might use multiple methods sampling broader emotional functions and cohort sequential strategies to disentangle age-related change, cohort effects, and antecedents and consequents of the change or stability of individual differences in EI. References Austin, E. J., Saklofske, D. H., Huang, S. H. S., & McKinney, D. (2004). Measurement of trait-emotional intelligence: Testing and cross-
  • 44. validating a modified version of Schutte et al.’s (1998) measure. Per- sonality and Individual Differences, 36, 555–562. Bar-On, R. (1997). The Emotional Quotient Inventory technical manual. Toronto: Multihealth Systems. Blanchard-Fields, F., & Abeles, R. (1996). Social cognition and aging. In J. E. Buren & K. W. Schaie (Eds.), Handbook of the psychology of aging (4th ed., pp. 150 –161). San Diego, CA: Academic Press. Brackett, M. A., & Mayer, J. D. (2003). Convergent, discriminant and incremental validity of competing measures of emotional intelligence. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 29, 1147–1158. Carstensen, L. L. (1992). Social and emotional patterns in adulthood: Support for socioemotional selectivity theory. Psychology and Aging, 7, 331–338. Carstensen, L. L. (1995). Evidence for a life span theory of socioemotional selectivity. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 4, 151–156. Carstensen, L. L., Pasupathi, M., Mayr, U., & Nesselroade, J. R. (2000). Emotional experience in everyday life across the adult life span. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 79, 644 – 655.
  • 45. Chang, E. C., & Sanna, L. J. (2001). Optimism, pessimism, and positive and negative affectivity in middle-aged adults: A test of a cognitive- affective model of psychological adjustment. Psychology and Aging, 16, 524 –531. Chapman, B., & Hayslip, B. (2005). Incremental validity of a measure of emotional intelligence. Journal of Personality Assessment, 85, 154 –169. Chapman, B. P. (2005). Emotional intelligence at mid life: A cross sec- tional investigation of factorial variance, social correlates, and rela- tionships to established personality and ability taxonomies. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, University of North Texas, Denton. Charles, S. T. (2005). Viewing injustice: Greater emotion heterogeneity with age. Psychology and Aging, 20, 159 –164. Cheung, G. W., & Rensvold, R. B. (2002). Evaluating goodness- of-fit indexes for testing measurement invariance. Structural Equation Mod- eling, 9, 233–255. Ciarrochi, J., Dean, F. P., & Anderson, S. (2002). Emotional intelligence moderates the relationship between stress and mental health. Personality
  • 46. and Individual Differences, 32, 197–209. Costa, P. T., & McCrae, R. R. (1992). Neo-PI, revised: Technical manual. Odessa, FL: Psychological Assessment Resources, Inc. Costa, P. T., & McCrae, R. R. (1996). Mood and personality in adulthood. In C. Magai & S. H. McFadden (Eds.), Handbook of emotion, adult development, and aging (pp. 369 –381). San Diego, CA: Academic Press. Costa, P. T., McCrae, R. R., & Zonderman, A. B. (1987). Environmental and dispositional influences on well-being: Longitudinal followup of an American national sample. British Journal of Psychology, 78, 299 –306. Diehl, Coyle, & Labouvie-Vief (1996) Dougherty, L. M., Abe, J. A., & Izard, C. E. (1996). Differential emotions theory and emotional development in adulthood and later life. In C. Magai & S. H. McFadden (Eds.), Handbook of emotion, adult develop- ment, and aging (pp. 27– 41). San Diego, CA: Academic Press. Goleman, D. (1995). Emotional intelligence. New York: Bantam Books, Inc. 416 BRIEF REPORTS
  • 50. na l u se o f t he in di vi du al u se r a nd is n ot to b e di ss em
  • 51. in at ed b ro ad ly . Hayslip, B., Jr. (1989). Alternative mechanisms for improvements in fluid ability performances among the aged. Psychology and Aging, 3, 122– 124. Hayslip, B., & Brookshire, R. G. (1985). Relationships among abilities in elderly adults: A time lag analysis. Journal of Gerontology, 6, 748 –750. Hayslip, B., & Sterns, H. L. (1979). Age differences in relationships between crystallized and fluid intelligences and problem solving. Jour- nal of Gerontology, 34, 404 – 414. Hertzog, C., & Schaie, K. W. (1988). Stability and change in adult intelligence: II. Simultaneous analysis of longitudinal means and covari-
  • 52. ance structures. Psychology and Aging, 3, 122–130. Horn, J. (1977). The aging of human abilities. In E. Wolman (Ed.), Handbook of developmental psychology. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall. Horn, J. L. (1975). Psychometric studies of aging and intelligence. In S. Gershon & A. Kankin (Eds.), Aging: Genesis and treatment of psycho- logical disorders in the elderly (Vol. 2). New York: Raven Press. Horn, J. L., & McCardle, J. J. (1992). A practical and theoretical guide to measurement invariance in aging research. Experimental Aging Re- search, 18, 117–144. Isaacowitz, D. M. 2005. The gaze of the optimist. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 31, 407– 415. Izard, C. E. (1991). The psychology of emotions. New York: Plenum Press. Izard, C. E., Libero, D. Z., Putnam, P., & Haynes, M. O. (1993). Stability of emotion experiences and their relations to traits of personality. Jour- nal of Personality and Social Psychology, 64, 847– 860. Izard, C. E., & Malatesta, C. Z. (1987). Perspectives on emotional devel-
  • 53. opment: I. Differential emotions theory of early emotional development. In J. D. Osofsy (Ed.), Handbook of infant development (2nd ed., pp. 494 –554). New York: Wiley-Interscience. John, O., & Gross, J. (2004). Healthy and unhealthy emotion regulation: Personality processes, individual differences, and life span development. Journal of Personality, 72, 1301–1333. Kafetsios, K. (2004). Attachment and emotional intelligence abilities across the life course. Personality and Individual Differences, 37, 129 – 145. Kano, Y., & Harada, A. (2000). Stepwise variable selection in factor analysis [computer software]. Retrieved May 1, 2006, from http:// koko16.hus.osaka-u.ac.jp/~harada/sefa2002/stepwise. Kano, Y., & Harada, A. (2000). Stepwise variable selection in factor analysis. Psychometrika, 65, 7–22. Kunzmann, U., & Gruhn, D. (2005). Age differences in emotional reac- tivity: The sample case of sadness. Psychology and Aging, 20, 47–59. Kunzmann, U., Kupperbusch, C. S., & Levenson, R. W. (2005). Behavioral inhibition and amplification during emotional arousal: A
  • 54. comparison of two age groups. Psychology and Aging, 20, 144 –158. Labouvie-Vief, G. 1996. Emotion, thought, and gender. In C. Magai & S. H. McFadaden (Eds.), Handbook of emotion, adult development, and aging (pp. 101–117). New York: Academic Press. Labouvie-Vief, G., DeVoe, M., & Bulka, D. (1989). Speaking about feelings: Conceptions of emotions across the life span. Psychology and Aging, 4, 425– 437. Labouvie-Vief, G., Hakin-Larson, J., DeVoe, M., & Schoeberlein, S. (1989). Emotions and self-regulation: A lifespan view. Human Devel- opment, 32, 279 –299. Lawton, M. P., Kleban, M. H., Dean, J., Rajagopal, D., & Parmelee, P. A. (1992). The factorial generality of brief positive and negative affect measures. Journal of Gerontology, 47, 228 –237. Lawton, M. P., Kleban, M. H., Rajagopal, D., & Dean, J. (1992). Dimen- sions of affective experience in three age groups. Psychology and Aging, 7, 171–184. MacCann, C., Roberts, R. D., Mathews, G., & Zeidner, M. (2004). Con- sensus scoring and empirical option weighting of performance-
  • 55. based Emotional Intelligence (EI) tests. Personality and Individual Differ- ences, 36, 645– 662. Magai, C., & Halpern, B. (2001). Emotional development during the middle years. In Lachman, M. (Ed.), The handbook of midlife develop- ment, (pp. 310 –344). New York: Wiley. Magai, C., & Nusbaum, B. (1996). Personality change in adulthood: Dynamic systems, emotions, and the transformed self. In C. Magai & S. H. McFadden (Eds.), Handbook of emotion, adult development, and aging (pp. 403– 419). San Diego, CA: Academic Press. Malatesta, C. Z., & Wilson, A. (1988). Emotion cognition interaction in personality development: A discrete emotions, functionalist analysis. British Journal of Social Psychology, 27, 91–112. Mathews, G., Roberts, R. D., & Zeidner, M. (2004). Seven myths about emotional intelligence. Psychological Inquiry, 15, 179 –196. Mathews, G., Zeidner, M., & Roberts, R. D. (2002). Emotional intelli- gence: Science and myth. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. Mayer, J., Salovey, P., & Caruso, D. (2004a). A further consideration of the
  • 56. issues of emotional intelligence. Psychological Inquiry, 15, 249 –255. Mayer, J., Salovey, P., & Caruso, D. (2004b). Emotional intelligence: Theory, findings, and implications. Psychological Inquiry, 15, 197–215. Mayer, J. D., Caruso, D., & Salovey, P. (1999). Emotional intelligence meets traditional standards for an intelligence. Intelligence, 27, 267–298. Mayer, J. D., & Salovey, P. (1997). What is emotional intelligence? In P. Salovey, & D. J. Sluyter (Eds.), Emotional development and emotional intelligence: Educational implications (pp. 3–34). New York: Basic Books. Mayer, J. D., Salovey, P. S., Caruso, D. R., & Sitarenios, G. (2001). Emotional intelligence as a standard intelligence. Emotion, 1, 232–242. McCrae, R. R., & Costa, P. T. (2003). Personality in adulthood: A five-factor theory perspective. New York: The Guilford Press. Petrides, K. A., & Furnham, A. (2000a). On the dimensional structure of emotional intelligence. Personality and Individual Differences, 29, 313– 320.
  • 57. Petrides, K. A., & Furnham, A. (2000b). Sex differences in trait emotional intelligence. Sex Roles, 42, 449 – 461. Petrides, K. A., & Furnham, A. (2001). Trait emotional intelligence: Psychometric investigation with reference to established trait taxono- mies. European Journal of Personality, 15, 423– 448. Roberts, R. D., Zeidner, M., & Mathews, G. (2001). Does emotional intelligence meet traditional standards for an intelligence? Some new data and conclusions. Emotion, 1, 196 –231. Saklofske, D. H., Austin, E. J., & Minski, P. S. (2003). Factor structure and validity of a trait emotional intelligence measure. Personality and Indi- vidual Differences, 34, 707–721. Salovey, P., & Mayer, J. D. (1990). Emotional intelligence. Imagination, cognition, and Personality, 9, 185–211. Satorra, A., & Bentler, P. M. (2001). A scaled chi-square test statistic for moment structure analysis. Psychometrika, 66, 507–514. Schaie, K. W. (2001). Emotional intelligence: Psychometric status and developmental characteristics–Comment on Roberts, Zeidner, and Mat- thews (2001). Emotion, 1, 243–248.
  • 58. Schutte, N. S., Malouff, J. M., Bobick, C., Coston, T. D., Greeson, C., Jedlicka, C., et al. (2001). Emotional intelligence and interpersonal relations. The Journal of Social Psychology, 121, 523–536. Schutte, N. S., Malouff, J. M., Hall, L. E., Haggerty, D. J., Cooper, J. T., Golden, C. J., et al. (1998). Development and validation of a measure of emotional intelligence. Personality and Individual Differences, 25, 167– 177. Schutte, N. S., Malouff, J. M., Simunek, M., McKenley, J., & Hollander, S. (2002). Characteristic emotional intelligence and emotional well- being. Cognition and Emotion, 16, 769 –785. 417BRIEF REPORTS T hi s do cu m en t i s co
  • 63. Schutte, N. S., Schuettpelz, E., & Malouff, J. M. (2002). Emotional intelligence and task performance. Imagination, Cognition, and Person- ality, 20, 347–354. Shulz, R., & Heckhausen, J. (1997). Emotion and control: A lifespan perspective. In K. W. Schaie & M. P. Lawton (Eds.), Annual review of gerontology and geriatrics: Focus on emotion and adult development (Vol. 17, pp. 185–205). New York: Springer. Strough, J., Berg, C., & Sansone, C. (1996). Goals for solving everyday problems across the life span: Age and gender differences in the salience of interpersonal concerns. Developmental Psychology, 32, 1106 –1115. Terracciano, A., McCrae, R. R., Brant. L. J., & Costa, P. T. (2005). Hierarchical linear modeling analyses of the NEO-PI-R scales in the Baltimore Longitudinal Study of Aging. Psychology and Aging, 20, 493–506. Terracciano, A., McCrae, R. R., Hagemann, D., & Costa, P. T. (2003). Individual difference variables, affective differentiation, and the struc- tures of affect. Journal of Personality, 71, 669 –703. Trapnell, P. D., & Wiggins, J. S. (1990). Extension of the
  • 64. interpersonal adjectives scales to include the Big Five dimensions of personality. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 59, 781–790. Zeidner, M., Mathews, G., & Roberts, R. D. (2001). Slow down, you move too fast: Emotional intelligence remains an “elusive” intelligence. Emo- tion, 1, 265–275. Zelinski, E. M., & Lewis, K. (2003). Adult age differences in multiple cognitive functions: Differentiation, dedifferentiation, or process- specific change? Psychology and Aging, 18, 727–745. Zwick, W. R., & Velicier, W. F. (1986). Comparison of five rules for determining the number of components to retain. Psychological Bulletin, 99, 432– 442. Received January 27, 2005 Revision received December 2, 2005 Accepted December 28, 2005 � 418 BRIEF REPORTS T hi s do
  • 69. ro ad ly . fairly well satisfied before safety needs become im- portant, and so on. As applied to social work prac- tice, Maslow’s theory indicates that social workers must first help clients meet basic needs (e.g., physio- logical needs). Once clients’ basic needs are met, higher-level needs can be dealt with. Maslow did not offer an age-stage approach to development. Striving for self-actualization is seen as a universal process that can be observed at nearly all ages. However, it is likely that there is some progression among age groups. Infants prob- ably have a strong emphasis on physiological needs. As a person gradually grows older, safety needs are emphasized, and then belongingness and love needs, and so on. Because middle-aged adults have had a variety of learning experiences and tend to be at the peak of their earning poten- tial, they tend to have a greater opportunity to fo- cus on meeting self-actualization needs. However, such crises as unemployment, prolonged illness, and broken relationships can switch the emphasis to a lower level of need. Emotional Intelligence Psychologists Peter Salovey and John Mayer coined the term emotional intelligence (EI) in 1990 (Papalia
  • 70. et al., 2009). It refers to the ability to recognize and deal with one’s own feelings as well as the feelings of others. Daniel Goleman (1995) popularized the EI concept and expanded it to include such qualities as empathy, motivation, social competence, optimism, and conscientiousness. McClelland had done some earlier work on factors related to emotional intelligence (Papalia et al., 2009). In the 1960s, the U.S. State Depart- ment concluded that a test of general knowledge was a poor predictor of how well those applying to be foreign service officers would perform. In addition, the test tended to screen out women and people of color. McClelland devised a selec- tion process that had nothing to do with general knowledge. He found that the best foreign service officers had positive expectations of others, were perceptive of the needs of others, and were skillful in forming social networks. His selection process emphasized these characteristics, and led to the ap- pointment of effective foreign service officers. It also ended the discrimination against women and people of color. Goleman (1995) developed an EI test. Studying nearly 500 corporations, Goleman found that those who rose to the top of the corporate ladder tended to score highest on EI. Goleman (1998, 2001) found the following competencies to be most closely associated with effective work performance: • Self-awareness (accurate self-assessment, emotional self-awareness, and self-confidence)
  • 71. • Self-management (trustworthiness, achievement drive, initiative, adaptability, and self-control) • Social awareness (empathy, organizational aware- ness, and service orientation) • Relationship management (exerting influence, conflict management, leadership, communication, building bonds, teamwork and collaboration, be- ing a catalyst for change, and developing others Goleman (1998) found that excelling in at least one competency in each of these four areas appears to be a key to success in almost any job. Emotional intelligence is not the opposite of cog- nitive intelligence. Some very bright people score high in EI, whereas others score lower. Some less- than-average scorers on IQ tests score high on EI, whereas others score lower. Emotional intelligence is not easy to measure. Papalia, Olds, and Feldman (2004, p. 478) note: Hard as it is to assess cognitive intelligence, EI may be even harder to measure. For one thing, lumping the emotions together can be misleading. How do we assess someone who can handle fear but not guilt, or who can face stress better than boredom? Then too, the usefulness of a certain emotion may depend on the circumstances. Anger, for example, can lead to either destructive or con- structive behavior. Self- actualization
  • 72. needs Self-esteem needs Belongingness and love needs Safety needs Physiological needs FIGURE 11.1 Maslow’s Hierarchy of Needs © Ce ng ag e Le ar ni ng 20 13 Psychological Aspects of Young and Middle Adulthood 475 Copyright 2012 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any
  • 73. time if subsequent rights restrictions require it. Social Intelligence Closely related to emotional intelligence is social in- telligence. Social Intelligence (SI) has been defined in a variety of ways. According to the original definition of Edward Thorndike (1920, p. 228), social intelligence is “the ability to understand and manage men and women, boys and girls, to act wisely in human relations.” According to this definition, it is equivalent to inter- personal intelligence. Some authors have restricted the definition of SI to deal only with knowledge of social situations. With this perspective, SI is synonymous with social cognition or social marketing intelligence. Daniel Goleman (2006) has drawn on social neu- roscience research to propose that social intelligence is made up of social awareness (including social cog- nition, empathy, attunement, and empathic accu- racy) and social facility (including self-presentation, influence, concern, and synchrony). There are various types of intelligence, including intellectual competencies, emotional intelligence, and social intelligence. Counseling/psychotherapy often involves helping people to modify their patterns of social intelligence, particularly those that cause clients to have problems in their interpersonal relationships. Some tests have been developed to measure SI
  • 74. (Goleman, 2006). Like IQ tests, SI tests are usually based on a 100-point scale in which 100 is the aver- age score. Most people score between 85–115. Scores of 140 are considered to be very high. People with SI scores below 80 may have an autism spectrum disor- der. These people are apt to have trouble making friends, and with communication. They might need social skill training. People with SI scores over 120 are considered to be very socially skilled and well adjusted, and probably will excel in jobs that involve direct contact and communication with people. Mezzo-System Interactions: Nonverbal Communication In seeking to assess human behavior, it is also im- portant to attend to nonverbal communication. Sig- mund Freud (quoted in Knapp & Hall, 1992) noted, “He that has eyes to see and ears to hear may con- vince himself that no mortal can keep a secret. If his lips are silent, he chatters with his finger tips; betrayal oozes out of him at every pore” (p. 391). It is impossible not to communicate. No matter what we do, we transmit information about our- selves. Even an expressionless face communicates messages. As you are reading this, stop for a minute and analyze what nonverbal messages you would be sending if someone were observing you. Are your eyes wide open or half closed? Is your posture According to Maslow, we have a basic need to “belong” and a basic need to be “loved.” D av
  • 75. id Yo un g- W ol ff /P ho to Ed it 476 Understanding Human Behavior and the Social Environment Copyright 2012 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it. When psychological dependence occurs, the user feels psychological discomfort if use is terminated. Dependent users tend to believe that they will use the chemical for the rest of their lives as a regular part of social or recreational activities. They question whether the desired emotional state can be achieved without the use of the chemical, and they have a pre- occupation with thinking and talking about the chem-
  • 76. ical and activities associated with using it. Users also generally develop a tolerance for some drugs, which means they have to take increasing amounts over time to achieve a given level of effect. Tolerance depends partly on the type of drug, be- cause some drugs (such as aspirin) do not create tolerance. Drug addiction is difficult to define. In a broad sense, addiction refers to an intense craving for a particular substance. The problem is that this defini- tion could be applied to an intense craving for a variety of substances—pickles, ice cream, potato chips, strawberry shortcake. To avoid this problem, we will define addiction as an intense craving for a drug that develops after a period of heavy use. Why Do People Use and Abuse Alcohol and Other Drugs? The effects of using drugs are numerous, ranging from feeling light-headed to death through overdos- ing. Drug abuse may lead to deterioration in health, relationship problems, automobile accidents, child abuse, spouse abuse, loss of job, low self-esteem, loss of social status, financial disaster, divorce, and arrests and convictions. A distinction needs to be made between responsi- ble drug use and drug abuse. Many drugs do have beneficial effects when used responsibly; aspirin re- lieves pain, alcohol helps people relax, tranquilizers reduce anxiety, antidepressant drugs reduce depres- sion, amphetamines increase alertness, morphine is a painkiller, and marijuana is useful in treating glau- coma. Irresponsible drug use is abuse, which was
  • 77. defined earlier in this chapter. Why do people abuse drugs? The reasons are nu- merous. Drug companies widely advertise the bene- ficial effects of their products. The media (such as television and movies) glamorize the mind-altering effects. Many popular songs highlight drinking. Bars and cocktail lounges have become centers for socializing, and promote drinking. Through such channels, Americans have become socialized to ac- cept drug usage as a part of daily living. Socializa- tion patterns lead many people to use drugs, and for some the use is a stepping-stone to abuse. Attitudes toward drug use also encourage abuse. For example, some college students believe they should get blitzed or stoned after a tough exam. Ryne Duren (1985), former pitcher for the New York Yankees, asked this question: “I started becom- ing an alcoholic at age four, even though I had my first drink at age nine—how can this be?” Duren went on to explain that at a very young age he became socialized to believe that a real man was “someone who could drink others under the table,” and that the way to have fun was to get high on alcohol. People abuse drugs for a variety of reasons. Some people build up a tolerance to a drug and then HIGHLIGHT 11.6 Steroid Use in Baseball On March 30, 2006, baseball commissioner Bud Selig asked former Senator George Mitchell to investigate steroid use in baseball. On December 13, 2007, Mitchell released his report.
  • 78. The report found steroid use to be rampant among former and current players. Eighty-six former and current players were named in the report. (It is thought that there are many other users among baseball players who have not yet been identified.) Steroids have been on baseball’s banned substance list since 1991; how- ever, testing of Major League players did not begin until 2003. Seven Most Valuable Player Award winners were named in the report, along with 31 All-Stars—at least one for every position. Some of the biggest names in baseball are alleged to have been users, including Barry Bonds, Roger Clemens, Mark McGwire, David Justice, Jason Giambi, Gary Sheffield, Miguel Tejada, Lenny Dykstra, Rafael Palmeiro, Andy Pettitte, Chuck Knoblauch, and Alex Rodriguez. (Some of these players have denied, under oath, that they used steroids.) To avoid testing positive for steroids, many athletes looking for an edge have now turned to human growth hor- mone (HGH) to build muscle. It is difficult to detect, and the best test available has a window of detection of only 48–72 hours. Psychological Aspects of Young and Middle Adulthood 505 Copyright 2012 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any
  • 79. time if subsequent rights restrictions require it. increase the dosage to obtain a high. Physical and psychological dependence usually leads to abuse. People with intense unwanted emotions (such as loneliness, anxiety, feelings of inadequacy, guilt, de- pression, insecurity, and resentment) may turn to drugs. For many abusers, their drug of choice be- comes their best friend because they tend to person- alize it and value it more highly than they value their friends. The drug is something that they can always count on to relieve pain or give them the kind of high they desire. Many abusers become so highly attached to their drug that they choose to continue using it even though it leads to deterioration of health, divorce, discharges from jobs, automobile accidents, alienation from children, loss of friends, depletion of financial resources, and court appear- ances. Drug abusers usually feel they need their drug as a crutch to make it through the day. Abusers develop an intimate relationship with their drug of choice. Even though this relationship is unhealthy, the drug plays a primary role in the abuser’s life, dictates a certain lifestyle, fills a psycho- logical need, and more often than not takes prece- dence over family, friends, and work. Most abusers deny their drug usage is creating problems for them, because they know that admitting they have a drug problem means they will have to end their relation- ship with their best friend, and they deeply believe they need their drug to handle their daily concerns and pressures. Drug abusers are apt to use a number of defense mechanisms in order to continue using
  • 80. drugs. They rationalize adverse consequences of drug abuse (such as the loss of a job) by twisting or dis- torting reality to explain the consequences of their behavior while under the influence. They minimize the adverse consequences of their drug use. They use projection to place the blame for their problems on others; for example, “If you had a wife like mine, you’d drink too.” Theories About Drug Use A variety of theories have been offered as to why people use drugs. Biological theories assert that physiological changes produced by the drugs eventu- ally generate an irresistible craving for the drug. Some biological theories also postulate that some people are predisposed by their genetic structure to abuse certain types of drugs. For example, some au- thorities believe that genes play a role in predispos- ing some people to alcoholism. Behavioral theories hold that people use drugs because they find them pleasurable and continue to use them because doing so prevents withdrawal distress. Interactionist theo- ries maintain that drug use is learned from interac- tion with others in our culture. For example, people drink alcohol because drinking is widely accepted. Interactionist theories assert that those who use ille- gal drugs such as marijuana or cocaine have contact with a drug subculture that encourages them to ex- periment with illegal drugs. Interaction in Family Systems: A Theoretical Approach to Drug Abuse Wegscheider (1981) maintains that chemical depen- dency is a family disease that involves and affects each family member. Although she focuses on the
  • 81. families of alcoholics, much of what she says may also apply to the families of other types of chemical substance abusers. She cites several rules that tend to characterize the families of drug abusers. First, the dependent person’s alcohol use becomes “the most important thing in the family’s life” (Wegscheider, 1981, p. 81). The abuser’s top priority is getting enough alcohol, and the family’s top priorities are the abuser, the abu- ser’s behavior, and keeping the abuser away from al- cohol. The goals of the abuser and of the rest of the family are at completely opposite poles. A second rule in an alcoholic family is that alco- hol is not the cause of the problem. Denial is para- mount. A third family rule maintains that the dependent person is not responsible for his or her behavior and that the alcohol causes the behavior. There is always someone or something else to blame. Another rule dictates that no one should rock the boat, no matter what. Family members strive to protect the family’s status quo, even when the family is miserable. Yet other rules concern for- bidding discussion of the family problem either within or outside of the family, and consistently avoiding stating one’s true feelings. Wegscheider (1981) maintains that these rules protect the depen- dent person from taking responsibility for his or her behavior, and that the rules serve to maintain the drinking problem. Wegscheider (1981) goes on to identify several roles that family members typically play. In addition to the chemically dependent person, there is the chief enabler, the family hero, the scapegoat, the lost
  • 82. child, and the mascot. The chief enabler’s main purpose is to assume the primary responsibility for family functioning. The 506 Understanding Human Behavior and the Social Environment Copyright 2012 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any time if subsequent rights restrictions require it. abuser typically continues to lose control and relin- quishes responsibility. The chief enabler takes on more and more responsibility and begins making more and more of the family’s decisions. A chief enabler is often the parent or spouse of the chemi- cally dependent person. Conditions often continue to deteriorate as the chemically dependent person loses control. A posi- tive influence is needed to offset the negative. The family hero fulfills this role. The family hero is often the person who does well at everything he or she tries. The hero works hard at making the family look as though it is functioning better than it is. In this way, the family hero provides the family with self-worth.
  • 83. Another role typically to played by someone in the family is that of scapegoat. Although the alco- hol abuse is the real problem, a family rule man- dates that this fact must be denied. Therefore, the blame must be placed elsewhere. Frequently, an- other family member is blamed for the problem. The scapegoat often behaves in negative ways that draw attention to him or her (for example, the per- son gets caught stealing, runs away, or becomes ex- tremely withdrawn). The scapegoat’s role is to distract attention away from the chemically depen- dent person and onto something else. This role helps the family avoid addressing the problem of chemical dependency. Often, someone plays the role of lost child. This is a person who seems relatively uninvolved with the rest of the family and never causes any trouble. The lost child’s purpose is to provide relief from some of the pain the family is suffering. At least there is someone in the family who neither requires much attention nor causes any stress. The lost child is sim- ply there. Finally, chemically dependent families often have someone playing the role of mascot. The mascot is someone who has a good sense of humor and appears not to take anything seriously. Despite how much the mascot might be suffering inside, he or she provides a little fun for the family. In summary, chemical dependency is a problem affecting the entire family. Each family member suf- fers from the chemical dependency, yet each assumes a role in order to maintain the family’s status quo and help the family survive. Family members are
  • 84. driven to maintain these roles no matter what hap- pens. The roles eventually become associated with survival. The Application of Theory to Client Situations: Treatment for the Chemically Dependent Person and His or Her Family One of the first tasks in treatment is for the chemically dependent person to take responsibility for his or her own behavior. The abuser must acknowl- edge that he or she has a problem before beginning to solve it. Several concepts are critical in working with the family (Wegscheider, 1981). Family members must first come to realize the extent of the problem. They need to identify the chemical abuse as their ma- jor problem. Additionally, they need to learn about and evaluate their family dynamics. They need to eval- uate their own behavior and break out of the roles that have been maintaining the chemical abuse. The chief enabler, in particular, must stop making excuses and assuming the chemically dependent person’s responsi- bilities. If the chemically dependent person is sick from a hangover and cannot make it to school or work the next day, it must be that person’s responsibility, not a parent’s or spouse’s, to call in sick. Family members eventually learn to confront the chemically dependent person and give him or her honest information about his or her behavior. For instance, they are encouraged to tell the dependent exactly how he or she behaved while having a black- out. If the dependent person hit another family member while drunk, this fact needs to be con- fronted. The confrontation should occur not in an
  • 85. emotional manner but rather in a factual one. The family also needs to learn about the pro- gression of the disease. We’ve already discussed some characteristics of drug dependence. There is a typical progression of an alcoholic’s feelings and behavior. At first, only occasional relief drinking occurs. Drinking becomes more constant. The de- pendent person then begins to drink in secret and to feel guilty about drinking. Memory blackouts begin to occur and gradually increase in frequency. The dependent person feels worse and worse about his or her drinking behavior, but seems to have less and less control over it. Finally, the drinking be- gins to seriously affect the person’s work, family, and social relationships. A job may be lost or all school classes flunked. Perhaps family members leave or throw the dependent person out. The de- pendent person’s thinking becomes more and more impaired. EP 2.1.3b, 2.1.6b, 2.1.10a, d, e, g, & j Psychological Aspects of Young and Middle Adulthood 507 Copyright 2012 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part. Due to electronic rights, some third party content may be suppressed from the eBook and/or eChapter(s). Editorial review has deemed that any suppressed content does not materially affect the overall learning experience. Cengage Learning reserves the right to remove additional content at any
  • 86. time if subsequent rights restrictions require it.