SlideShare a Scribd company logo
1 of 9
Download to read offline
International Journal of Psychology, 2016
DOI: 10.1002/ijop.12249
Emotion regulation and psychopathological symptoms
of Chinese school-age children: A person-centred
and multi-informant approach
Peipei Li and Zhuo Rachel Han
Beijing Key Laboratory of Applied Experimental Psychology, School of Psychology, Beijing
Normal University, Beijing, China
Emotion regulation (ER) is a critical component of children’s development. Many previous studies have utilised a
single-assessment method to reflect child ER, which might result in losing important information regarding the unique
contribution of each informant. With a person-centred approach and multi-informant reports (mother, teacher and child),
the current study examined 196 children’s (age M = 9.21, SD = 1.10, range = 7–11 years; 51% girls) ER patterns and
their associations with psychopathological symptoms in a Chinese sample. A model-based clustering procedure resulted
in 3 ER groups: the poor family ER group (n = 36), poor school ER group (n = 120), and overall good ER group (n = 40).
Significant differences were found among ER clusters on teacher-reported child psychopathological symptoms compared
on the levels of withdrawn depression, somatic complain, thought problems and attention problems. No significant
differences were found on the mother-reported psychopathological symptoms. Compared with children in the poor school
or poor family ER clusters, children in the overall good ER group demonstrated fewer psychopathological symptoms at
school. Our results confirmed the advantage of adopting multi-informant assessments to fully capture children’s emotional
profiles and linked these profiles with children’s emotional and behavioural functioning at school.
Keywords: Child emotion regulation; Cross-informant; Person-centred; Psychopathological symptoms.
Although no consensus has been reached, many
researchers (e.g. Thompson, 1994, p. 27) agree that there
are shared, implicit notions of what is meant by ER: “it
consists of internal and external processes involved in
initiating, maintaining and modulating the occurrence,
intensity and expression of emotions.” The critical role of
ER in children’s development has been demonstrated by
numerous empirical studies (e.g. Han & Shaffer, 2013;
Kim & Cicchetti, 2010). Of particular interest in recent
years is the association of emotion dysregulation with
children’s psychopathological symptoms. First, emotion
dysregulation is regarded as a core aspect of most forms of
psychopathology. For example, mood and anxiety disor-
ders are primarily defined on the basis of disturbed emo-
tions (e.g. Mineka & Sutton, 1992). Second, most psy-
chopathological disorders are associated with many forms
of emotional disturbances. For example, dating back to
Abraham (1911), depressive disorder has been associated
with sadness, guilt and anger. Additionally, emotion
Correspondence should be addressed to Zhuo Rachel Han, Ph.D., Beijing Key Laboratory of Applied Experimental Psychology, School of
Psychology, Beijing Normal University, Beijing 100875, China. (E-mail: rachhan@bnu.edu.cn).
Rachel Han designed the study, collected data and revised the article. Peipei Li analysed the data and drafted the article. This study received support
from the National Natural Science Foundation of China (31500898).
dysregulation is also a risk factor for psychopathology
because emotional deficits predict subsequent changes in
the symptoms of anxiety, aggressive behaviour and other
types of psychopathology (McLaughlin, Hatzenbuehler,
Mennin, & Nolen-Hoeksema, 2011). Children who show
deficits in ER appear to be more vulnerable to many
forms of internalising and externalising problems. For
example, Folk, Zeman, Poon, and Dallaire (2014) linked
children’s ER with depressive symptoms, indicating that
poor ER ability may potentially deteriorate depressive
symptoms. Roberton, Daffern, and Bucks (2012) found
an association between aggression and anger regulation
among adolescents.
Although the critical role of ER and its effect on
children’s psychopathological symptoms has been rela-
tively well studied, several issues still remain unclear.
The first is cross-culture validity. Culture can impact both
ER-related behaviours as well as the relation between
ER and psychopathology. First, cultural values may have
© 2016 International Union of Psychological Science
2 LI AND HAN
different effects on people’s ER-related behaviours. It
has been argued that in collective cultures (e.g. Asian
countries), which tend to foster interdependent selves
thus encourage relatedness and communal relationships,
expressive suppression may be more encouraged. In con-
trast, in individualistic cultures (e.g. the United States),
which foster the development of independent construal of
self, open emotion expression might be preferred (Mat-
sumoto, Yoo, & Nakagawa, 2008). Second, the relation
between ER and psychopathology might also be affected
by cultural factors. Recent evidence has shown that the
frequency and psychological consequences of ER strate-
gies differ in ethnic/cultural groups, depending on differ-
ent cultural values. For instance, one study showed that
the habitual use of expressive suppression was associ-
ated with adverse psychological functioning in European
American, but not in Chinese participants (Soto, Perez,
Kim, Lee, & Minnick, 2011).
Although it has been postulated that culture may affect
the development and implementation of ER, many pre-
vious studies employed primarily European American,
middle-class samples. Given the recent research finding
indicating that the more normative a behaviour is in a spe-
cific culture, the fewer negative relations that exist with
negative child outcomes, and vice versa (Lansford et al.,
2006), there is reason to investigate whether and how
children’s ER in a specific cultural context affects their
behavioural outcomes.
In addition to the cultural context, other contextual fac-
tors may affect children’s ER and related psychopatho-
logical symptoms are micro-contexts (Bronfenbrenner’s,
1992; e.g. school or home). Micro-contexts are differ-
ent from macro-contexts (e.g. culture) in that most chil-
dren only live in one culture throughout development,
but constantly switch their micro-contexts (i.e. entering a
new school). Both dimensions (i.e. macro/micro-context)
require our attention when studying the children’s psy-
chological well-being.
When rating the emotional/behavioural problems of
children and adolescents in different micro-contexts,
Achenbach, McConaughy, and Howell (1987) found that
there were only modest correlations between different
informants (parents or teachers), which indicated that
child and adolescent problems might not be effectively
captured by a single informant under a specific situation.
However, a large proportion of studies on child ER only
used mothers as the single informant (see Adrian, Zeman,
& Veits, 2011). Although maternal reports of children’s
ER can be informative and convenient, they might not be
sufficient for school-age children. This is because most
mother–child interaction occurs in the family context
but school-age children spend most of their time at
school, that their ER in the school setting would be less
accessible for their mothers. Compared to the utilisa-
tion of family informants, less investigation has been
performed to explore children’s emotional competence
from informants at school (e.g. teacher reports). Teachers’
reports could be particularly important because they pro-
vide an opportunity to understand children’s ER in school
settings (Adrian et al., 2011). Research studies have indi-
cated that although some very deviant children may be
less affected by situational and informant variables (see
Loeber & Dishion, 1984), an assessment of most chil-
dren must account for the variance in the situations and
informants. Thus, it is necessary to incorporate teachers’
reports when examining the ER of school-age children.
In addition, many aspects of children’s emo-
tional world remain private, particularly during
middle-childhood, because they become more aware
of their own cultural rules for acceptable emotional
expression and begin to better regulate their own emo-
tions (Saarni, Campos, Camras, & Witherington, 2006).
Emotional activities of children at this age might become
more implicit, whereas maternal or teachers’ reports
largely rely on their own observation of their children’s
explicit behaviours. Thus, the self-report methodologies
for children appear to be indispensable because individ-
uals often have private access to internal feelings and
experiences. Recent emerging studies have emphasised a
multi-method approach in assessing children’s ER (e.g.
Adrian et al., 2011), and these studies have agreed upon
the advantage of multi-informant assessments.
Utilising multiple methods directly leads to another
question: how to better synthesise these multiple pieces
of information. Traditional analysis is mostly variable
centred, and researchers tend to average the sum of the
standardised scores reported by each informant, which
requires significant positive correlations among the stan-
dardised scores as a prerequisite. However, disagreement
among informants usually exists (e.g. De Los Reyes &
Kazdin, 2005) and these discrepancies might possess
useful information for the better understanding of child
ER. Researchers have recently begun to utilise more
person-oriented techniques to incorporate multiple ER
elements (e.g. Han & Shaffer, 2013); however, few studies
have incorporated teacher-report data from the school set-
ting to demonstrate children’s ER across micro-contexts.
If patterns of the children’s ER could be identi-
fied from a person-centred manner, then it should be
determined whether there is a linkage between chil-
dren’s psychopathological outcomes and their ER pro-
files. Many previous studies have only focused on broad
categories (e.g. externalising or internalising) or a spe-
cific type of problems (e.g. anxious, depressed). Some
psychopathological symptoms are relatively understudied
(e.g. somatic problems, attention problems), which might
result in the loss of important information regarding the
connection between ER and more nuanced psychopatho-
logical symptoms. The existing literature is potentially
important, but does not provide sufficient information for
us to understand how children’s ER is associated with
their psychopathological symptoms.
© 2016 International Union of Psychological Science
CHINESE CHILDREN’S EMOTION REGULATION 3
Taken together, the current study applied
person-oriented techniques to incorporate informa-
tion from multiple sources—children, mothers and
teachers—in a Chinese sample. Specifically, it had sev-
eral goals: (a) To utilise model-based cluster analysis to
identify the profiles of child ER based on ratings from
multiple informants (i.e. mother, teacher and child). On
the basis of the potential and reasonable existence of dis-
agreement among different informants (Achenbach et al.,
1987) as well as the children’s potential inconsistent per-
formance in different contexts, we expect the emergence
of at least two types of clusters: the consistent ER pattern
(in which children received all negative or all positive
evaluations from all informants) and the inconsistent ER
cluster pattern (in which children received inconsistent
evaluations from various informants). (b) To examine
the associations between children’s ER profiles and
psychopathological symptoms in this group of Chinese
children. It was expected that Chinese children with more
at-risk ER profiles might present more psychopatholog-
ical problems in both family and school settings.
METHODS
Participants
Participants were recruited from an elementary school
of a major city in northeastern China. A total of 208
two-parent families voluntarily participated in this study.
Twelve families were excluded due to a misunderstanding
of the instructions or a failure to return all scales. The rates
of the remaining missing data ranged from 0 to 2.9%, and
the expectation-maximisation (EM) imputation was used
to replace the missing values.
The remaining families included 196 fathers, (age
M = 40.32; SD = 3.46), 196 mothers, (age M = 37.54;
SD = 3.04), 96 boys (age M = 9.23, SD = 1.02) and 100
girls (age M = 9.06; SD = 1.02). In addition, 74.9% of
families reported having an annual household income at
or above average (i.e. approximately 10,000 USD) for
dual-income families in the city in which the information
was collected. All of the fathers and 194 of the mothers
were the children’s biological parents.
Procedure
Parents received the introductory and invitation letter
from their children’s head teachers at a school-wide
parental conference. For the parents who agreed to
participate, their children were asked to take home a
series of questionnaires with informed written consent
forms for the parents and children (the children’s written
consent forms were asked to be signed by their primary
caregivers on behalf of the children), which were sealed
in large envelopes. Parents returned the questionnaires
to their child’s head teacher after they had completed all
of the questionnaires. Participant ID numbers were used
to match the maternal and teacher’s reports. All of the
participating families were provided with two books on
parenting and child-rearing, which were selected by the
school president. In addition, each participating child’s
head teacher received a package of questionnaires and
was asked to respond to the questions for each child
based on their behaviours at school. All of the procedures
were approved by the authors’ university Institutional
Review Board.
Measures
Emotion Regulation Checklist (ERC)
First, the children’s emotion regulation (ER) from the
teacher’s and mother’s perspectives was measured using
the Chinese translated version of the ERC (Shields &
Cicchetti, 1997). The ERC consisted of 24 items that
aimed to assess the child’s emotionality and regulation.
Mothers and teachers were asked to report on how often
the children emotionally reacted in a specific way using a
4-point Likert scale. The ERC originally consisted of two
subscales (i.e. the Emotion Regulation scale and the Lia-
bility/Negativity scale). Because this study targeted the
children’s regulation of emotions, only the emotion reg-
ulation subscale was selected (e.g. “Responds positively
to neutral or friendly overtures by adults”). Previous
studies using the Chinese version of the questionnaire
demonstrated acceptable internal reliabilities (e.g. Suveg
et al., 2014). The internal consistencies for the emotion
regulation subscale of the maternal report and teacher
report in the current sample were 𝛼 = .674 and 𝛼 = .804,
respectively.
Children’s Emotion Management Scales
(CEMS)
The children’s self-report measure of ER was mea-
sured using the Children’s Emotion Management Scales
(Zeman, Casino, Suveg, & Shipman, 2010). The Chinese
version of CEMS was translated and back-translated by
four bilingual translators. Differences in the original and
back-translated versions were discussed and resolved
through joint agreement. The CEMS consisted of 36
items that measured the children’s management of anger,
sadness and worry. The coping subscale of the CEMS
was used to ensure that all three measures captured
regulatory aspects of ER. This coping subscale measured
the children’s culturally appropriate and constructive
strategies to manage worry, sadness and anger (i.e. “I try
to calmly settle the problem when I feel worried”). Items
were rated on a 3-point Likert scale. Previous studies
adopting the CEMS with Chinese school-age children
demonstrated acceptable reliability, 𝛼 = .70 (Suveg et al.,
© 2016 International Union of Psychological Science
4 LI AND HAN
2014). The reliability for the coping subscale in the
current sample was good (𝛼 = .88).
The Child Behaviour Checklist (CBCL)
The Child Behaviour Checklist (Achenbach, 1991)
was used as an index of the children’s psychopatho-
logical symptoms. The CBCL was a 118-item measure
of parent-reported psychosocial functioning of children
over the past 6 months that yielded eight subscales in
different behavioural domains: withdrawal, somatic
problems, anxious/depressed, social problems, thought
problems, attention problems, rule breaking and aggres-
sive behaviours. All of the items were rated on a 3-point
Likert scale. The Chinese version of the CBCL has been
widely used and has shown good test–retest reliabilities
and validity (Leung et al., 2006). The internal consisten-
cies of maternal-reported CBCL subscales in this study
were as follows: withdrawn/depressed, 𝛼 = .89; somatic
problems, 𝛼 = .97; anxious/depressed, 𝛼 = .91; social
problems, 𝛼 = .88; thought problems, 𝛼 = .83; attention
problems, 𝛼 = .91; rule breaking, 𝛼 = .92; and aggressive
behaviours, 𝛼 = .94.
Child Behaviour Checklist Teacher Report
Form (TRF)
The TRF was CBCL’s parallel form (Achenbach,
1991). A teacher familiar with the participating child
completed the Chinese version of the Teacher’s Report
Form for each youth. Similar to the other informant
versions, the TRF-generated broad scores and more spe-
cific subscale scores of the children’s anxious/depressed,
withdrawn problems, somatic complaints, social prob-
lems, thought problems, attention problems, aggressive
behaviours and delinquent behaviours. The Chinese
version of the TRF has been previously used within
Chinese samples and has proven to be reliable and valid
(Leung et al., 2006). The internal consistencies of all
of the subscales in this study were as follows: with-
drawn/depressed, 𝛼 = .86; somatic problems, 𝛼 = .97;
anxious/depressed, 𝛼 = .79; social problems, 𝛼 = .72;
thought problems, 𝛼 = .73; attention problems, 𝛼 = .74;
rule breaking, 𝛼 = .87 and aggressive behaviours, 𝛼 = .79.
Analytical plan
Descriptive statistics as well as correlational tests among
the study variables were examined as preliminary analy-
ses. Next, potential group differences in study variables
based on demographic characteristics were performed to
provide a basic index for further analysis. A model-based
cluster analysis using the Mclust program developed for
R software (Fraley & Raftery, 2006) was performed
on the multiple measurements of the children’s ER.
Model-based cluster analysis overcomes some of the
inherent liabilities of traditional cluster analysis. Specif-
ically, this type of cluster analysis is more data-driven,
thereby reducing difficulties in determining the number
of clusters by providing a fit index (the Bayesian Informa-
tion Criterion, BIC). Higher BIC values indicate a better
fit of the model. When comparing models, a difference in
the BIC of 0–2 is considered weak support for the better
fitting model; 2–6 is considered positive support; 6–10
is considered strong support and over 10 is considered
very strong support (Raftery, 1995). In this study, the ER
scores from mothers, teachers and children were entered
into the analysis. The best fitting model was selected for
further analysis. Finally, a series of one-way analyses of
variance (ANOVAs) were performed to examine potential
differences across the identified clusters on the children’s
psychopathological symptoms.
RESULTS
Correlational tests were performed among the children’s
ER as reported by different informants. No significant
correlations were found, including that between child and
mother, child and teacher and mother and teacher. As
for psychopathological symptoms reported by mothers
and teachers, the results demonstrated low concordance
between both sources on all of the subscale scores. Only
low to moderate correlations were found for thought
problems, r(196) = .23, p = .001, and attention problems,
r(196) = .20, p = .006. Correlations between the rest pairs
of subscales were not significant.
Model-based cluster analysis: Cluster results
As shown in Figure 1, the best fitting model (BIC
value = −1619.83) yielded a four-class solution with
diagonal clusters with a variable volume and equal shape
(characteristics of the distribution). The next best fit-
ting model (BIC value = −1630.09) yielded a two-class
solution with spherical clusters with a variable volume
and equal shape. According to the rule of thumb for
interpreting BIC value differences proposed by Raftery
(1995), the best fitting model was strongly supported
(ΔBIC = 10.26). Thus, the four diagonal clusters with a
variable volume and equal shape were selected. However,
because Cluster 4 only had five participants and the mean
was within 1 SD of Cluster 2, the samples in Cluster 2 and
Cluster 4 demonstrated the same trend of evaluation from
different informants (i.e. three informants all reported
positive ER abilities for children in these two clusters).
We recoded the children in Cluster 4 into Cluster 2 for
ease of discussion. Each participant was classified into an
appropriate group based on their ER profiles. The results
of the final three clusters were presented in Figure 1,
which displayed each of their score’s deviation from the
sample mean on standardised units.
© 2016 International Union of Psychological Science
CHINESE CHILDREN’S EMOTION REGULATION 5
Figure 1. Standard deviations from the overall sample mean of each cluster for all measures on child emotion regulation from the best fitting model
with four-cluster solution.
Three groups identified by the clustering procedure
were differentiated according to the ER information col-
lected from different informants. The majority of children
in the current sample (n = 120; 61.22% of the sample)
was classified as belonging to the overall good ER group
(Cluster 2), in which the individuals received, on average,
moderately positive evaluations from all three informants.
A number of children (n = 40; 20.41% of the sample) fell
into the poor school ER group (Cluster 3). The ER pat-
terns of the children in this cluster were characterised by
the most negative evaluations from their teachers, slightly
negative self-evaluations and moderately positive assess-
ments from their mothers. In contrast, the poor family
ER group (Cluster 1) consisted of 36 children (18.37%
of the sample) whose ER abilities were rated most nega-
tively by their mothers, slightly negative by themselves,
but were slightly above average based on their teacher’s
reports. Importantly, when giving low scores, informants
appeared to disagree with each other. For example, for
children in Cluster 1, their teachers reported significantly
higher scores than their mothers, t(35) = 8.08, p < .001,
whereas for children in Cluster 3, their mothers’ scores
were significantly higher than those of their teachers,
t(39) = 13.14, p < .001.
Cluster differences
Demographic variables
After the profiles were identified, subgroups were
first examined on a number of demographic variables,
including gender, age and annual household income.
These results showed that these groups did not differ on
maternal age, F(2, 186) =. 89, p = .412; child age, F(2,
191) = .89, p = .410; or annual household income, F(2,
188) = 1.88, p = .156. Gender differences were found in
Cluster 1, 𝜒2(1) = 4.00, p = .046, with more boys than
girls belonging to this cluster. No gender differences were
found in the other two clusters. The descriptive statistics
for the clusters and demographic variables are shown in
Table 1.
Clustering variables
A series of ANOVAs among the clustering variables
showed statistically significant differences across the
three subgroups on each informant’s report, including
maternally reported ER, F(2, 193) = 152.24, p < .001;
teacher-reported ER, F(2, 193) = 192.44, p < .001; and
children’s self-reported ER, F(2, 193) = 10.16, p < .001.
Post-hoc analyses indicated that on the teacher reports,
differences existed between any two groups; on the mater-
nal reports, differences existed between any two groups,
except between Cluster 2 and Cluster 3; whereas on the
child’s self-report, group differences only existed between
Cluster 1 and Cluster 2.
Psychopathological problems
The clusters were associated with psychopathological
symptom information collected from parents and teachers
to identify the associations between particular ER pro-
files and the children’s psychosocial functioning. Because
© 2016 International Union of Psychological Science
6 LI AND HAN
TABLE 1
One-way analysis of variance (ANOVA) examining cluster differences on ER variables, demographic variables and teacher-reported
children’s emotional and behavioural problems
ER cluster
Cluster 1 (n = 36) Mean (SD) Cluster 2 (n = 120) Mean (SD) Cluster 3 (n = 40) Mean (SD) F
ER variables
Maternal report 3.17 (0.33) 3.99 (0.21) 3.93 (0.28) 151.24**
Teacher report 3.73 (0.28) 3.92 (0.21) 2.98 (0.37) 192.44**
Child self-report 1.93 (0.53) 2.32 (0.47) 2.04 (0.56) 10.16**
Demographic variables
Child age 9.29 (1.10) 9.16 (1.04) 8.98 (0.89) 0.90
Mother age 37.97 (3.85) 37.58 (2.74) 37.05 (3.03) 0.89
SES 6.47 (1.83) 6.02 (1.91) 5.59 (2.10) 1.88
TRF subscales
Anxious/depressed 54.15 (7.80) 53.05 (5.46) 54.23 (6.35) 0.81
Withdrawn 55.84 (7.24) 52.93 (4.70) 54.55 (7.19) 3.92**
Somatic complains 52.85 (5.84) 50.68 (3.30) 51.42 (4.37) 3.93**
Social problems 57.08 (9.13) 54.68 (6.40) 57.56 (10.29) 2.67*
Thought problems 55.69 (8.91) 52.19 (5.85) 52.13 (5.32) 4.40**
Attention problems 55.26 (8.28) 53.51 (5.68) 57.10 (10.37) 3.77**
Delinquent behavior 55.21 (8.16) 53.95 (6.03) 56.03 (9.13) 1.41
Aggressive behavior 56.25 (9.28) 53.65 (5.71) 55.21 (8.13) 2.19
*p < .05. **p < .01.
SES = Socleconomic
one goal of this study was to shed light on children’s
psychosocial behaviours under different contexts (home
or school), we performed ANOVAs separately among
mother-reported data and teacher-reported data. First, we
performed one-way ANOVAs on the family-reported psy-
chopathological information of their children. Unexpect-
edly, no significant group differences emerged on each
subscale in mother-reported CBCL. Next, we explored
whether the ER clusters generated from the data would
differ at the level of the children’s psychopathologi-
cal symptoms reported by teachers based on the chil-
dren’s behaviours at school. Significant differences were
found among the ER profiles when compared at the level
of withdrawal depression, F(2, 193) = 3.92, p = .022;
somatic complains, F(2, 193) = 3.93, p = .021; thought
problems, F(2, 193) = 4.40, p = .014; and attention prob-
lems, F(2, 193) = 3.77, p = .025. These group differences
are shown in Table 1.
Further explorations indicated that children who
belonged to the poor family ER group (Cluster 1) were
reported to have the highest levels of somatic complaints
(M = 52.85, SD = 5.84); thought problems (M = 55.69,
SD = 8.91) and withdrawal depression (M = 55.84,
SD = 7.24) by their teachers. Attention problems in
this subgroup were moderately higher than average
(M = 55.26, SD = 8.28). As for the individuals in the
other ER-inconsistent groups, children in the poor school
ER group (Cluster 3) were rated slightly more positively
than children in the poor family ER group (Cluster 1) by
their teachers, but they still were rated more negatively
than children in the Cluster 2. In Cluster 3, children were
reported to have the highest levels of attention problems
(M = 57.10, SD = 10.37), and they had the second highest
levels of withdrawal problems (M = 54.55, SD = 7.19)
and somatic complains (M = 51.42, SD = 4.37).
DISCUSSION
The current study utilised person-centred analyses to
characterise ER patterns in a Chinese school-age sample
as well as to examine how these profiles are associated
with children’s psychopathological symptoms demon-
strated at home and at school. This study has advanced
the literature by incorporating cross-informants reports on
children’s ER and dual perspectives on children’s emo-
tional and behavioural problems at home and at school in a
Chinese sample. Specifically, we adopted person-oriented
analyses to minimise problems with the heterogeneity
of child ER information from various sources. Com-
pared with the traditional variable-oriented approach,
the employment of model-based cluster analyses echoed
the call for multi-assessment of child ER in the litera-
ture (Adrian et al., 2011). Specifically, emotion and ER
are complex constructs that can be assessed at multi-
ple levels, including physiological and behavioural lev-
els. From a behavioural aspect, both reported obtained
from other studies and self-reports can provide mean-
ingful information on youth’s emotional functioning, but
either reporter per se can draw a full picture of children’s
ER profiles. Thus, the multifaceted nature of emotion
and ER lend themselves well to a multi-method assess-
ment. When information from multiple recourses were
© 2016 International Union of Psychological Science
CHINESE CHILDREN’S EMOTION REGULATION 7
collected, the utilisation of person-centred statistical tech-
niques became key, as it contributed to the interpretation
of these data without removing the unique contribution
from each reporter.
In this study, person-oriented analyses identified four
distinct groups of children based on the different infor-
mants’ assessments of the children’s ER (i.e. mother,
teacher and child). We combined two clusters with the
same trend for ease of discussion. More than half of
the participating children were placed into the overall
good ER group (Cluster 2), indicating that the major-
ity of our sample (i.e. a community sample of Chi-
nese school-age children) consistently displayed good
ER skills across contexts. Expectedly, there were also
children who received inconsistent ER assessments from
family and school raters (children in Cluster 1 and Clus-
ter 3). Such inconsistency between raters further con-
firmed the necessity of employing a multi-informant and
person-centred approach.
Individuals from these inconsistent ER groups together
occupied 38.78% of the overall sample. Although the
number was smaller than individuals in the consistent ER
cluster, these individuals occupied a considerable propor-
tion of the current sample. Special attention should be
paid to children in these two inconsistent clusters because
their emotional problems might only be observable under
a specific micro-context (family or school). For instance,
mothers of children in the poor school ER (Cluster 3)
group may miss their children’s difficulties with ER at
home, whereas teachers of children in the poor family ER
group (Cluster 1) may not notice the children’s problems
with ER at school.
Specifically, individuals in the poor family ER group
received the lowest ER scores from their mothers, which
were not confirmed by either the children themselves
or the teachers. In contrast, children in the poor school
ER group obtained the poorest ER scores from their
teachers and an average evaluation from their mothers.
These results suggest that many children performed dif-
ferently at home and at school in terms of using ER strate-
gies. However, another potential explanation for these
diverging results might also come from the mothers’
biased reports. Previous studies have shown that moth-
ers with higher levels of depression or anxiety were more
likely to provide biased reports on children’s emotional
problems, which were not consistent with other infor-
mants (Briggs-Gowan, Carter, & Schwab-Stone, 1996).
Future studies are strongly encouraged to include mater-
nal depressive symptoms in the analyses as well as to
incorporate paternal ratings and peer ratings, as fathers
may provide an evaluation under the family context in
addition to mothers and peers could rate the children’s
behaviours under school settings in addition to teachers.
In sum, consistent with our expectation, all three clus-
tering variables demonstrated statistically significant dif-
ferences across the final three clusters. These findings
helped to confirm the validity of the three profiles: indi-
viduals of each cluster showed distinct ER patterns when
incorporating the perspectives from the mother, teacher
and child per se. When investigating the demographic
variables, we found that the groups did not differ on
the child’s age, maternal age or paternal age. However,
girls appeared to slightly outperform boys on ER abili-
ties. There were more girls than boys in the overall good
ER group, whereas there were more boys than girls in the
poor family ER group and poor school ER group. This
finding was consistent with those obtained from Western
school-age samples (e.g. Larsson & Frisk, 1999), such
that girls appeared to be more emotionally competent than
boys at this developmental stage.
As for our hypotheses regarding the associations
between ER profiles and children’s psychopatholog-
ical symptoms, group differences were found only
on the teacher-reported but not the parent-reported
psychopathological symptoms of the children, which
suggested that ER patterns appeared to be more infor-
mative of psychological and behavioural adjustments
in the school context. It could be that when a teacher
was observing the child in the school context, she would
automatically regard other children as her references for
evaluation. Thus, teachers might be more likely to obtain
an integrative perspective when making evaluations.
Such overall perspectives might enable the teachers
to better differentiate the characteristics of one child
from another. In contrast, the majority of parents in this
Chinese sample were parents of an only child (89.80%),
such that these Chinese parents did not have easily
accessible children for reference in order for them to rate
their child’s behaviours. Thus, the differences between
having compared to not having available comparisons
may contribute to these distinct findings regarding the
association between ER patterns with teacher-reported
and family-reported psychopathological outcomes of
children.
As for the associations between ER patterns and
teacher-reported psychopathological symptoms, we
found a positive trend in the overall good ER group.
Specifically, children in the inconsistent subgroups were
reported as having more somatic complaints, thought
problems and withdrawal depressive symptoms as well
as attention problems compared to children in the over-
all good ER group. These results were consistent with
findings from Western samples, such that children with
more effective ER strategies demonstrated more adaptive
behaviours (e.g. Rubin, Coplan, Fox, & Calkins, 1995)
and fewer psychopathological problems (e.g. Kim & Cic-
chetti, 2010). More specifically, Schmeichel (2007) found
that maladaptive emotional expressions undermined per-
formance on subsequent tests of working memory span,
which was related to child attention problems and thought
problems (Mezzacappa & Buckner, 2010). Rubin et al.
(1995) discovered that children with poor ER abilities
© 2016 International Union of Psychological Science
8 LI AND HAN
displayed more wary and anxious behaviours during free
play and interaction with others, which could be observed
as typical symptoms of withdrawal depression. Lundh
and Simonsson-Sarnecki (2001) also linked somatic com-
plaints and emotional difficulties in youth samples. Our
results showed consistent findings in Chinese school-age
children with that obtained from Western samples; they
also confirmed that when we aim to describe the chil-
dren’s emotional world, a single informant might not be
sufficient.
In addition, it should be noted that consistent with
existing evidence (De Los Reyes & Kazdin, 2005), the
results of this study also indicated that there was a
fair amount of disagreement among various informants
regarding the children’s emotional competence and psy-
chopathological problems. There were no significant cor-
relations between any rater pairs on children’s emotional
competence. Similarly, teachers’ reports showed diverg-
ing results from those of their parents on most ratings on
psychopathological symptoms.
Although this study is among the first to depict
the emotional profiles of Chinese children with a
multi-assessment and person-centred approach, sev-
eral limitations must be considered. The first limitation
of this study is the lack of behavioural observation.
Although family- and school-reported data have previ-
ously demonstrated a relatively comprehensive picture
of children’s emotional and behavioural status across
contexts, behavioural observation from the professional
perspective of researchers would provide more useful
information. Second, the finding about the inconsistent
reports of mothers and teachers on children’s behavioural
and emotional functioning should be interpreted with
caution because it might also be due to rater difference
rather than the true context-specific performance of
children. In addition, it is important to note that no
conclusions about causal pathways or developmental
patterns can be drawn from the results of this study due
to its cross-sectional nature. Future studies on the pre-
dictive power of children’s ER abilities on their clinical
outcomes might significantly benefit from a longitudinal
design.
Manuscript received May 2015
Revised manuscript accepted November 2015
REFERENCES
Abraham, K. (1911). Notes on the psycho-analytical investiga-
tion and treatment of manic-depressive insanity and allied
conditions. In Jones E (ed)., Selected papers on psychoanal-
ysis (pp. 137–156). London, England: Hogarth.
Achenbach, T. M. (1991). Integrative guide for the 1991
CBCL/4-18, YSR, and TRF profiles. Department of Psychia-
try, University of Vermont Burlington, VT.
Achenbach, T. M., McConaughy, S. H., & Howell, C. T. (1987).
Child/adolescent behavioral and emotional problems: Impli-
cations of cross-informant correlations for situational speci-
ficity. Psychological Bulletin, 101, 213–232.
Adrian, M., Zeman, J., & Veits, G. (2011). Methodological
implications of the affect revolution: A 35-year review of
emotion regulation assessment in children. Journal of Exper-
imental Child Psychology, 110, 171–197.
Briggs-Gowan, M. J., Carter, A. S., & Schwab-Stone, M. (1996).
Discrepancies among mother, child, and teacher reports:
Examining the contributions of maternal depression and anx-
iety. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 24, 749–765.
Bronfenbrenner, U. (1992). Ecological systems theory. Jessica
Kingsley.
De Los Reyes, A., & Kazdin, A. E. (2005). Informant discrep-
ancies in the assessment of childhood psychopathology: a
critical review, theoretical framework, and recommendations
for further study. Psychological Bulletin, 131, 483–509.
Folk, J. B., Zeman, J., Poon, J. A., & Dallaire, D. H. (2014). A
longitudinal examination of emotion regulation: pathways to
anxiety and depressive symptoms in urban minority youth.
Child and Adolescent Mental Health, 19, 243–250.
Fraley, C., & Raftery, A. E. (2006). MCLUST version 3: An
R package for normal mixture modeling and model-based
clustering. Washington University Department of Statistics.
Han, Z. R., & Shaffer, A. (2013). The relation of parental emo-
tion dysregulation to children’s psychopathology symptoms:
The moderating role of child emotion dysregulation. Child
Psychiatry and Human Development, 44, 591–601.
Kim, J., & Cicchetti, D. (2010). Longitudinal pathways linking
child maltreatment, emotion regulation, peer relations, and
psychopathology. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychia-
try, 51, 706–716.
Lansford, J. E., Malone, P. S., Stevens, K. I., Dodge, K. A.,
Bates, J. E., & Pettit, G. S. (2006). Developmental trajec-
tories of externalizing and internalizing behaviors: Factors
underlying resilience in physically abused children. Devel-
opment and Psychopathology, 18, 35–55.
Larsson, B., & Frisk, M. (1999). Social competence and
emotional/behavior problems in 6–16 year-old Swedish
school children. European Child & Adolescent Psychiatry,
8, 24–33.
Leung, P. W. L., Kwong, S. L., Tang, C. P., Ho, T. P., Hung, S.
F., Lee, C. C., … Liu, W. S. (2006). Test-retest reliability
and criterion validity of the Chinese version of CBCL, TRF,
and YSR. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, and
Allied Disciplines, 47, 970–973.
Loeber, R., & Dishion, T. J. (1984). Boys who fight at home
and school: Family conditions influencing cross-setting con-
sistency. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 52,
759–768.
Lundh, L. G., & Simonsson-Sarnecki, M. (2001). Alexithymia,
emotion, and somatic complaints. Journal of Personality, 69,
483–510.
Matsumoto, D., Yoo, S. H., & Nakagawa, S. (2008). Culture,
emotion regulation, and adjustment. Journal of Personality
and Social Psychology, 94, 925–937.
McLaughlin, K. A., Hatzenbuehler, M. L., Mennin, D. S., &
Nolen-Hoeksema, S. (2011). Emotion dysregulation and ado-
lescent psychopathology: A prospective study. Behaviour
Research and Therapy, 49, 544–554.
© 2016 International Union of Psychological Science
CHINESE CHILDREN’S EMOTION REGULATION 9
Mezzacappa, E., & Buckner, J. C. (2010). Working memory
training for children with attention problems or hyperactiv-
ity: A school-based pilot study. School Mental Health, 2,
202–208.
Mineka, S., & Sutton, S. K. (1992). Cognitive biases and the
emotional disorders. Psychological Science, 3, 65–69.
Raftery, A. E. (1995). Bayesian model selection in social
research. Sociological Methodology, 25, 111–164.
Roberton, T., Daffern, M., & Bucks, R. S. (2012). Emotion
regulation and aggression. Aggression and Violent Behavior,
17, 72–82.
Rubin, K. H., Coplan, R. J., Fox, N. A., & Calkins, S. D. (1995).
Emotionality, emotion regulation, and preschoolers’ social
adaptation. Development and Psychopathology, 7, 49–62.
Saarni, C., Campos, J. J., Camras, L. A., & Witherington,
D. (2006). Handbook of child psychology. New York, NY:
Wiley.
Schmeichel, B. J. (2007). Attention control, memory updating,
and emotion regulation temporarily reduce the capacity for
executive control. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Gen-
eral, 136, 241–255.
Shields, A., & Cicchetti, D. (1997). Emotion regulation among
school-age children: The development and validation of a
new criterion Q-sort scale. Developmental Psychology, 33,
906–916.
Soto, J. A., Perez, C. R., Kim, Y. H., Lee, E. A., & Minnick, M.
R. (2011). Is expressive suppression always associated with
poorer psychological functioning? A cross-cultural compar-
ison between European Americans and Hong Kong Chinese.
Emotion, 11, 1450–1455.
Suveg, C., Raley, J. N., Morelen, D., Wang, W., Han, R. Z., &
Campion, S. (2014). Child and family emotional functioning:
A cross-national examination of families from China and
the United States. Journal of Child and Family Studies, 23,
1444–1454.
Thompson, R. A. (1994). Emotion regulation: A theme in search
of definition. Monographs of the Society for Research in
Child Development, 59, 25–52.
Zeman, J., Casino, M., Suveg, C., & Shipman, K. (2010). Initial
validation of the children’s worry management scale. Journal
of Child and Family Studies, 19, 381–392.
© 2016 International Union of Psychological Science

More Related Content

What's hot

Emotionality and social behaviour
Emotionality and social behaviourEmotionality and social behaviour
Emotionality and social behaviourSFragoso
 
Childhood Trauma: The impact of Childhood Adversity on Education, Learning an...
Childhood Trauma: The impact of Childhood Adversity on Education, Learning an...Childhood Trauma: The impact of Childhood Adversity on Education, Learning an...
Childhood Trauma: The impact of Childhood Adversity on Education, Learning an...Michael Changaris
 
Effects of trauma on implicit emotion regulation within a family system a res...
Effects of trauma on implicit emotion regulation within a family system a res...Effects of trauma on implicit emotion regulation within a family system a res...
Effects of trauma on implicit emotion regulation within a family system a res...Michael Changaris
 
Lit review guide to nwriting literature review
Lit review guide to nwriting literature reviewLit review guide to nwriting literature review
Lit review guide to nwriting literature reviewroxcine
 
A Comparative Study of Students Engaged in General versus Professional Course...
A Comparative Study of Students Engaged in General versus Professional Course...A Comparative Study of Students Engaged in General versus Professional Course...
A Comparative Study of Students Engaged in General versus Professional Course...paperpublications3
 
Anger management
Anger managementAnger management
Anger managementsimonaarens
 
Association Between Relationship Quality and Childhood Trauma in Emerging Ad...
Association Between Relationship Quality  and Childhood Trauma in Emerging Ad...Association Between Relationship Quality  and Childhood Trauma in Emerging Ad...
Association Between Relationship Quality and Childhood Trauma in Emerging Ad...John Mattscheck, M.A.
 
Childhood Maltreatment and PTSD Literature Review and Proposed Study
Childhood Maltreatment and PTSD Literature Review and Proposed StudyChildhood Maltreatment and PTSD Literature Review and Proposed Study
Childhood Maltreatment and PTSD Literature Review and Proposed StudyAlexandraPerkins5
 
The effects Childhood Trauma and PTSD on Education and Learning (Guide to Cla...
The effects Childhood Trauma and PTSD on Education and Learning (Guide to Cla...The effects Childhood Trauma and PTSD on Education and Learning (Guide to Cla...
The effects Childhood Trauma and PTSD on Education and Learning (Guide to Cla...Michael Changaris
 
Au Psy492 M7 A2 Broyer J
Au Psy492 M7 A2 Broyer JAu Psy492 M7 A2 Broyer J
Au Psy492 M7 A2 Broyer Jjbroyer
 
Summers, Ashley Final Research Paper
Summers, Ashley Final Research PaperSummers, Ashley Final Research Paper
Summers, Ashley Final Research PaperAshley Summers
 
Chapter 1 introduction to psychology
Chapter 1 introduction to psychologyChapter 1 introduction to psychology
Chapter 1 introduction to psychologykrupamodhiya
 
LauraSasha Final Paper
LauraSasha Final PaperLauraSasha Final Paper
LauraSasha Final PaperSasha Albrecht
 

What's hot (18)

LS Honors Thesis 5.1.15
LS Honors Thesis 5.1.15 LS Honors Thesis 5.1.15
LS Honors Thesis 5.1.15
 
Emotionality and social behaviour
Emotionality and social behaviourEmotionality and social behaviour
Emotionality and social behaviour
 
Childhood Trauma: The impact of Childhood Adversity on Education, Learning an...
Childhood Trauma: The impact of Childhood Adversity on Education, Learning an...Childhood Trauma: The impact of Childhood Adversity on Education, Learning an...
Childhood Trauma: The impact of Childhood Adversity on Education, Learning an...
 
Effects of trauma on implicit emotion regulation within a family system a res...
Effects of trauma on implicit emotion regulation within a family system a res...Effects of trauma on implicit emotion regulation within a family system a res...
Effects of trauma on implicit emotion regulation within a family system a res...
 
Lit review guide to nwriting literature review
Lit review guide to nwriting literature reviewLit review guide to nwriting literature review
Lit review guide to nwriting literature review
 
Honors Thesis
Honors ThesisHonors Thesis
Honors Thesis
 
A Comparative Study of Students Engaged in General versus Professional Course...
A Comparative Study of Students Engaged in General versus Professional Course...A Comparative Study of Students Engaged in General versus Professional Course...
A Comparative Study of Students Engaged in General versus Professional Course...
 
Dissertation to print
Dissertation to printDissertation to print
Dissertation to print
 
Anger management
Anger managementAnger management
Anger management
 
aids and behavior
aids and behavioraids and behavior
aids and behavior
 
Association Between Relationship Quality and Childhood Trauma in Emerging Ad...
Association Between Relationship Quality  and Childhood Trauma in Emerging Ad...Association Between Relationship Quality  and Childhood Trauma in Emerging Ad...
Association Between Relationship Quality and Childhood Trauma in Emerging Ad...
 
Childhood Maltreatment and PTSD Literature Review and Proposed Study
Childhood Maltreatment and PTSD Literature Review and Proposed StudyChildhood Maltreatment and PTSD Literature Review and Proposed Study
Childhood Maltreatment and PTSD Literature Review and Proposed Study
 
The effects Childhood Trauma and PTSD on Education and Learning (Guide to Cla...
The effects Childhood Trauma and PTSD on Education and Learning (Guide to Cla...The effects Childhood Trauma and PTSD on Education and Learning (Guide to Cla...
The effects Childhood Trauma and PTSD on Education and Learning (Guide to Cla...
 
Au Psy492 M7 A2 Broyer J
Au Psy492 M7 A2 Broyer JAu Psy492 M7 A2 Broyer J
Au Psy492 M7 A2 Broyer J
 
Summers, Ashley Final Research Paper
Summers, Ashley Final Research PaperSummers, Ashley Final Research Paper
Summers, Ashley Final Research Paper
 
senior sem paper
senior sem papersenior sem paper
senior sem paper
 
Chapter 1 introduction to psychology
Chapter 1 introduction to psychologyChapter 1 introduction to psychology
Chapter 1 introduction to psychology
 
LauraSasha Final Paper
LauraSasha Final PaperLauraSasha Final Paper
LauraSasha Final Paper
 

Viewers also liked

Transform Your Vaping Experience
Transform Your Vaping ExperienceTransform Your Vaping Experience
Transform Your Vaping Experiencevapingman
 
Debt Management Strategies presentation
Debt Management Strategies presentationDebt Management Strategies presentation
Debt Management Strategies presentationMatt Newlin, Ed.D.
 
C.V Updated 2016 Mark Daniels[1]
C.V Updated 2016 Mark Daniels[1]C.V Updated 2016 Mark Daniels[1]
C.V Updated 2016 Mark Daniels[1]mark daniels
 
Schnell Idea newer brochure
Schnell Idea newer brochureSchnell Idea newer brochure
Schnell Idea newer brochureLee Disbury
 
The life cycle of a chicken
The life cycle of a chickenThe life cycle of a chicken
The life cycle of a chickenJ_M_Tornero
 
Secuencia narrativa de imagenes 2-Trianas
Secuencia narrativa de imagenes 2-TrianasSecuencia narrativa de imagenes 2-Trianas
Secuencia narrativa de imagenes 2-TrianasDaniela Bargnia
 
Tarea i de tecnologia aplicada a la educacion
Tarea i de tecnologia aplicada a la educacionTarea i de tecnologia aplicada a la educacion
Tarea i de tecnologia aplicada a la educacionDanilka Hernandez
 
Structural Mechanism for the Fidelity Modulation of DNA Polymerase λ
Structural Mechanism for the Fidelity Modulation of DNA Polymerase λStructural Mechanism for the Fidelity Modulation of DNA Polymerase λ
Structural Mechanism for the Fidelity Modulation of DNA Polymerase λMu-Sen Liu
 

Viewers also liked (18)

Seguridad Informática
Seguridad InformáticaSeguridad Informática
Seguridad Informática
 
Transform Your Vaping Experience
Transform Your Vaping ExperienceTransform Your Vaping Experience
Transform Your Vaping Experience
 
Debt Management Strategies presentation
Debt Management Strategies presentationDebt Management Strategies presentation
Debt Management Strategies presentation
 
C.V Updated 2016 Mark Daniels[1]
C.V Updated 2016 Mark Daniels[1]C.V Updated 2016 Mark Daniels[1]
C.V Updated 2016 Mark Daniels[1]
 
Forte Company Profile reg (2)
Forte Company Profile reg (2)Forte Company Profile reg (2)
Forte Company Profile reg (2)
 
Schnell Idea newer brochure
Schnell Idea newer brochureSchnell Idea newer brochure
Schnell Idea newer brochure
 
The life cycle of a chicken
The life cycle of a chickenThe life cycle of a chicken
The life cycle of a chicken
 
GK Kaanazine issue spring 2009
GK Kaanazine issue spring 2009GK Kaanazine issue spring 2009
GK Kaanazine issue spring 2009
 
Informatica 3
Informatica 3Informatica 3
Informatica 3
 
Secuencia narrativa de imagenes 2-Trianas
Secuencia narrativa de imagenes 2-TrianasSecuencia narrativa de imagenes 2-Trianas
Secuencia narrativa de imagenes 2-Trianas
 
Tarea i de tecnologia aplicada a la educacion
Tarea i de tecnologia aplicada a la educacionTarea i de tecnologia aplicada a la educacion
Tarea i de tecnologia aplicada a la educacion
 
Pereira
PereiraPereira
Pereira
 
Top Floor Penthouse Apartment For Rent
Top Floor Penthouse Apartment For RentTop Floor Penthouse Apartment For Rent
Top Floor Penthouse Apartment For Rent
 
Ds new
Ds newDs new
Ds new
 
Casa Museo Jesús Escobar
Casa Museo Jesús EscobarCasa Museo Jesús Escobar
Casa Museo Jesús Escobar
 
Gunar Professional profile
Gunar Professional profileGunar Professional profile
Gunar Professional profile
 
Structural Mechanism for the Fidelity Modulation of DNA Polymerase λ
Structural Mechanism for the Fidelity Modulation of DNA Polymerase λStructural Mechanism for the Fidelity Modulation of DNA Polymerase λ
Structural Mechanism for the Fidelity Modulation of DNA Polymerase λ
 
Alamo apratment medical center
Alamo apratment medical centerAlamo apratment medical center
Alamo apratment medical center
 

Similar to Emotion Regulation and Psychopathological Symptoms in Chinese Children

Mothers with eating disorders, depression and anxietyArticolo diagnosi
Mothers with eating disorders, depression and anxietyArticolo diagnosiMothers with eating disorders, depression and anxietyArticolo diagnosi
Mothers with eating disorders, depression and anxietyArticolo diagnosilucacerniglia
 
A six years prospective study
A six years prospective studyA six years prospective study
A six years prospective studylucacerniglia
 
JIPED_22_1_2018_4-1-1.pdf
JIPED_22_1_2018_4-1-1.pdfJIPED_22_1_2018_4-1-1.pdf
JIPED_22_1_2018_4-1-1.pdfAGNESSANTOS19
 
Early maternal relational traumatic experiences and psychopathological sympto...
Early maternal relational traumatic experiences and psychopathological sympto...Early maternal relational traumatic experiences and psychopathological sympto...
Early maternal relational traumatic experiences and psychopathological sympto...lucacerniglia
 
Early maternal relational traumatic experiences and psychopathological sympto...
Early maternal relational traumatic experiences and psychopathological sympto...Early maternal relational traumatic experiences and psychopathological sympto...
Early maternal relational traumatic experiences and psychopathological sympto...lucacerniglia
 
Critical analysis-bio-psycho
Critical analysis-bio-psychoCritical analysis-bio-psycho
Critical analysis-bio-psychojaysoncajate1
 
Adolescent Depression 1Running Head ADOLESCENT DEPRESSI.docx
Adolescent Depression   1Running Head ADOLESCENT DEPRESSI.docxAdolescent Depression   1Running Head ADOLESCENT DEPRESSI.docx
Adolescent Depression 1Running Head ADOLESCENT DEPRESSI.docxnettletondevon
 
Annual Research Review Enduring Neurobiological Effects Of Childhood Abuse A...
Annual Research Review  Enduring Neurobiological Effects Of Childhood Abuse A...Annual Research Review  Enduring Neurobiological Effects Of Childhood Abuse A...
Annual Research Review Enduring Neurobiological Effects Of Childhood Abuse A...Justin Knight
 
Culture, Socialization, and Children's EF
Culture, Socialization, and Children's EFCulture, Socialization, and Children's EF
Culture, Socialization, and Children's EFMarissa Schneider
 
A Relational Perspective On PTSD In Early Childhood
A Relational Perspective On PTSD In Early ChildhoodA Relational Perspective On PTSD In Early Childhood
A Relational Perspective On PTSD In Early ChildhoodKarla Adamson
 
Nonverbal Communication and Autism April Priest
Nonverbal Communication and Autism April PriestNonverbal Communication and Autism April Priest
Nonverbal Communication and Autism April PriestApril Priest
 
Positive Emotions and Cognition
Positive Emotions and CognitionPositive Emotions and Cognition
Positive Emotions and CognitionDokka Srinivasu
 
mental health reviewd file
mental health reviewd filemental health reviewd file
mental health reviewd fileSurojit Saha
 
Early occuring maternal deppression and maternal negativity in predicting you...
Early occuring maternal deppression and maternal negativity in predicting you...Early occuring maternal deppression and maternal negativity in predicting you...
Early occuring maternal deppression and maternal negativity in predicting you...hamsulbasri
 
School Effects on Psychological Outcomes During Adolescence.docx
School Effects on Psychological Outcomes During Adolescence.docxSchool Effects on Psychological Outcomes During Adolescence.docx
School Effects on Psychological Outcomes During Adolescence.docxWilheminaRossi174
 
poster example 1
poster example 1poster example 1
poster example 1Peipei li
 
Personality Factors as Related to Stresses among Parents of Mentally Sub-Norm...
Personality Factors as Related to Stresses among Parents of Mentally Sub-Norm...Personality Factors as Related to Stresses among Parents of Mentally Sub-Norm...
Personality Factors as Related to Stresses among Parents of Mentally Sub-Norm...inventionjournals
 
1 7Annotated Bib
1                                         7Annotated Bib1                                         7Annotated Bib
1 7Annotated BibVannaJoy20
 
CHILDREN’S EMOTION REGULATION AND ATTACHMENT TO PARENTS PAR.docx
CHILDREN’S EMOTION REGULATION AND ATTACHMENT TO PARENTS PAR.docxCHILDREN’S EMOTION REGULATION AND ATTACHMENT TO PARENTS PAR.docx
CHILDREN’S EMOTION REGULATION AND ATTACHMENT TO PARENTS PAR.docxbissacr
 
CHILDREN’S EMOTION REGULATION AND ATTACHMENT TO PARENTS PAR
CHILDREN’S EMOTION REGULATION AND ATTACHMENT TO PARENTS PARCHILDREN’S EMOTION REGULATION AND ATTACHMENT TO PARENTS PAR
CHILDREN’S EMOTION REGULATION AND ATTACHMENT TO PARENTS PARJinElias52
 

Similar to Emotion Regulation and Psychopathological Symptoms in Chinese Children (20)

Mothers with eating disorders, depression and anxietyArticolo diagnosi
Mothers with eating disorders, depression and anxietyArticolo diagnosiMothers with eating disorders, depression and anxietyArticolo diagnosi
Mothers with eating disorders, depression and anxietyArticolo diagnosi
 
A six years prospective study
A six years prospective studyA six years prospective study
A six years prospective study
 
JIPED_22_1_2018_4-1-1.pdf
JIPED_22_1_2018_4-1-1.pdfJIPED_22_1_2018_4-1-1.pdf
JIPED_22_1_2018_4-1-1.pdf
 
Early maternal relational traumatic experiences and psychopathological sympto...
Early maternal relational traumatic experiences and psychopathological sympto...Early maternal relational traumatic experiences and psychopathological sympto...
Early maternal relational traumatic experiences and psychopathological sympto...
 
Early maternal relational traumatic experiences and psychopathological sympto...
Early maternal relational traumatic experiences and psychopathological sympto...Early maternal relational traumatic experiences and psychopathological sympto...
Early maternal relational traumatic experiences and psychopathological sympto...
 
Critical analysis-bio-psycho
Critical analysis-bio-psychoCritical analysis-bio-psycho
Critical analysis-bio-psycho
 
Adolescent Depression 1Running Head ADOLESCENT DEPRESSI.docx
Adolescent Depression   1Running Head ADOLESCENT DEPRESSI.docxAdolescent Depression   1Running Head ADOLESCENT DEPRESSI.docx
Adolescent Depression 1Running Head ADOLESCENT DEPRESSI.docx
 
Annual Research Review Enduring Neurobiological Effects Of Childhood Abuse A...
Annual Research Review  Enduring Neurobiological Effects Of Childhood Abuse A...Annual Research Review  Enduring Neurobiological Effects Of Childhood Abuse A...
Annual Research Review Enduring Neurobiological Effects Of Childhood Abuse A...
 
Culture, Socialization, and Children's EF
Culture, Socialization, and Children's EFCulture, Socialization, and Children's EF
Culture, Socialization, and Children's EF
 
A Relational Perspective On PTSD In Early Childhood
A Relational Perspective On PTSD In Early ChildhoodA Relational Perspective On PTSD In Early Childhood
A Relational Perspective On PTSD In Early Childhood
 
Nonverbal Communication and Autism April Priest
Nonverbal Communication and Autism April PriestNonverbal Communication and Autism April Priest
Nonverbal Communication and Autism April Priest
 
Positive Emotions and Cognition
Positive Emotions and CognitionPositive Emotions and Cognition
Positive Emotions and Cognition
 
mental health reviewd file
mental health reviewd filemental health reviewd file
mental health reviewd file
 
Early occuring maternal deppression and maternal negativity in predicting you...
Early occuring maternal deppression and maternal negativity in predicting you...Early occuring maternal deppression and maternal negativity in predicting you...
Early occuring maternal deppression and maternal negativity in predicting you...
 
School Effects on Psychological Outcomes During Adolescence.docx
School Effects on Psychological Outcomes During Adolescence.docxSchool Effects on Psychological Outcomes During Adolescence.docx
School Effects on Psychological Outcomes During Adolescence.docx
 
poster example 1
poster example 1poster example 1
poster example 1
 
Personality Factors as Related to Stresses among Parents of Mentally Sub-Norm...
Personality Factors as Related to Stresses among Parents of Mentally Sub-Norm...Personality Factors as Related to Stresses among Parents of Mentally Sub-Norm...
Personality Factors as Related to Stresses among Parents of Mentally Sub-Norm...
 
1 7Annotated Bib
1                                         7Annotated Bib1                                         7Annotated Bib
1 7Annotated Bib
 
CHILDREN’S EMOTION REGULATION AND ATTACHMENT TO PARENTS PAR.docx
CHILDREN’S EMOTION REGULATION AND ATTACHMENT TO PARENTS PAR.docxCHILDREN’S EMOTION REGULATION AND ATTACHMENT TO PARENTS PAR.docx
CHILDREN’S EMOTION REGULATION AND ATTACHMENT TO PARENTS PAR.docx
 
CHILDREN’S EMOTION REGULATION AND ATTACHMENT TO PARENTS PAR
CHILDREN’S EMOTION REGULATION AND ATTACHMENT TO PARENTS PARCHILDREN’S EMOTION REGULATION AND ATTACHMENT TO PARENTS PAR
CHILDREN’S EMOTION REGULATION AND ATTACHMENT TO PARENTS PAR
 

Emotion Regulation and Psychopathological Symptoms in Chinese Children

  • 1. International Journal of Psychology, 2016 DOI: 10.1002/ijop.12249 Emotion regulation and psychopathological symptoms of Chinese school-age children: A person-centred and multi-informant approach Peipei Li and Zhuo Rachel Han Beijing Key Laboratory of Applied Experimental Psychology, School of Psychology, Beijing Normal University, Beijing, China Emotion regulation (ER) is a critical component of children’s development. Many previous studies have utilised a single-assessment method to reflect child ER, which might result in losing important information regarding the unique contribution of each informant. With a person-centred approach and multi-informant reports (mother, teacher and child), the current study examined 196 children’s (age M = 9.21, SD = 1.10, range = 7–11 years; 51% girls) ER patterns and their associations with psychopathological symptoms in a Chinese sample. A model-based clustering procedure resulted in 3 ER groups: the poor family ER group (n = 36), poor school ER group (n = 120), and overall good ER group (n = 40). Significant differences were found among ER clusters on teacher-reported child psychopathological symptoms compared on the levels of withdrawn depression, somatic complain, thought problems and attention problems. No significant differences were found on the mother-reported psychopathological symptoms. Compared with children in the poor school or poor family ER clusters, children in the overall good ER group demonstrated fewer psychopathological symptoms at school. Our results confirmed the advantage of adopting multi-informant assessments to fully capture children’s emotional profiles and linked these profiles with children’s emotional and behavioural functioning at school. Keywords: Child emotion regulation; Cross-informant; Person-centred; Psychopathological symptoms. Although no consensus has been reached, many researchers (e.g. Thompson, 1994, p. 27) agree that there are shared, implicit notions of what is meant by ER: “it consists of internal and external processes involved in initiating, maintaining and modulating the occurrence, intensity and expression of emotions.” The critical role of ER in children’s development has been demonstrated by numerous empirical studies (e.g. Han & Shaffer, 2013; Kim & Cicchetti, 2010). Of particular interest in recent years is the association of emotion dysregulation with children’s psychopathological symptoms. First, emotion dysregulation is regarded as a core aspect of most forms of psychopathology. For example, mood and anxiety disor- ders are primarily defined on the basis of disturbed emo- tions (e.g. Mineka & Sutton, 1992). Second, most psy- chopathological disorders are associated with many forms of emotional disturbances. For example, dating back to Abraham (1911), depressive disorder has been associated with sadness, guilt and anger. Additionally, emotion Correspondence should be addressed to Zhuo Rachel Han, Ph.D., Beijing Key Laboratory of Applied Experimental Psychology, School of Psychology, Beijing Normal University, Beijing 100875, China. (E-mail: rachhan@bnu.edu.cn). Rachel Han designed the study, collected data and revised the article. Peipei Li analysed the data and drafted the article. This study received support from the National Natural Science Foundation of China (31500898). dysregulation is also a risk factor for psychopathology because emotional deficits predict subsequent changes in the symptoms of anxiety, aggressive behaviour and other types of psychopathology (McLaughlin, Hatzenbuehler, Mennin, & Nolen-Hoeksema, 2011). Children who show deficits in ER appear to be more vulnerable to many forms of internalising and externalising problems. For example, Folk, Zeman, Poon, and Dallaire (2014) linked children’s ER with depressive symptoms, indicating that poor ER ability may potentially deteriorate depressive symptoms. Roberton, Daffern, and Bucks (2012) found an association between aggression and anger regulation among adolescents. Although the critical role of ER and its effect on children’s psychopathological symptoms has been rela- tively well studied, several issues still remain unclear. The first is cross-culture validity. Culture can impact both ER-related behaviours as well as the relation between ER and psychopathology. First, cultural values may have © 2016 International Union of Psychological Science
  • 2. 2 LI AND HAN different effects on people’s ER-related behaviours. It has been argued that in collective cultures (e.g. Asian countries), which tend to foster interdependent selves thus encourage relatedness and communal relationships, expressive suppression may be more encouraged. In con- trast, in individualistic cultures (e.g. the United States), which foster the development of independent construal of self, open emotion expression might be preferred (Mat- sumoto, Yoo, & Nakagawa, 2008). Second, the relation between ER and psychopathology might also be affected by cultural factors. Recent evidence has shown that the frequency and psychological consequences of ER strate- gies differ in ethnic/cultural groups, depending on differ- ent cultural values. For instance, one study showed that the habitual use of expressive suppression was associ- ated with adverse psychological functioning in European American, but not in Chinese participants (Soto, Perez, Kim, Lee, & Minnick, 2011). Although it has been postulated that culture may affect the development and implementation of ER, many pre- vious studies employed primarily European American, middle-class samples. Given the recent research finding indicating that the more normative a behaviour is in a spe- cific culture, the fewer negative relations that exist with negative child outcomes, and vice versa (Lansford et al., 2006), there is reason to investigate whether and how children’s ER in a specific cultural context affects their behavioural outcomes. In addition to the cultural context, other contextual fac- tors may affect children’s ER and related psychopatho- logical symptoms are micro-contexts (Bronfenbrenner’s, 1992; e.g. school or home). Micro-contexts are differ- ent from macro-contexts (e.g. culture) in that most chil- dren only live in one culture throughout development, but constantly switch their micro-contexts (i.e. entering a new school). Both dimensions (i.e. macro/micro-context) require our attention when studying the children’s psy- chological well-being. When rating the emotional/behavioural problems of children and adolescents in different micro-contexts, Achenbach, McConaughy, and Howell (1987) found that there were only modest correlations between different informants (parents or teachers), which indicated that child and adolescent problems might not be effectively captured by a single informant under a specific situation. However, a large proportion of studies on child ER only used mothers as the single informant (see Adrian, Zeman, & Veits, 2011). Although maternal reports of children’s ER can be informative and convenient, they might not be sufficient for school-age children. This is because most mother–child interaction occurs in the family context but school-age children spend most of their time at school, that their ER in the school setting would be less accessible for their mothers. Compared to the utilisa- tion of family informants, less investigation has been performed to explore children’s emotional competence from informants at school (e.g. teacher reports). Teachers’ reports could be particularly important because they pro- vide an opportunity to understand children’s ER in school settings (Adrian et al., 2011). Research studies have indi- cated that although some very deviant children may be less affected by situational and informant variables (see Loeber & Dishion, 1984), an assessment of most chil- dren must account for the variance in the situations and informants. Thus, it is necessary to incorporate teachers’ reports when examining the ER of school-age children. In addition, many aspects of children’s emo- tional world remain private, particularly during middle-childhood, because they become more aware of their own cultural rules for acceptable emotional expression and begin to better regulate their own emo- tions (Saarni, Campos, Camras, & Witherington, 2006). Emotional activities of children at this age might become more implicit, whereas maternal or teachers’ reports largely rely on their own observation of their children’s explicit behaviours. Thus, the self-report methodologies for children appear to be indispensable because individ- uals often have private access to internal feelings and experiences. Recent emerging studies have emphasised a multi-method approach in assessing children’s ER (e.g. Adrian et al., 2011), and these studies have agreed upon the advantage of multi-informant assessments. Utilising multiple methods directly leads to another question: how to better synthesise these multiple pieces of information. Traditional analysis is mostly variable centred, and researchers tend to average the sum of the standardised scores reported by each informant, which requires significant positive correlations among the stan- dardised scores as a prerequisite. However, disagreement among informants usually exists (e.g. De Los Reyes & Kazdin, 2005) and these discrepancies might possess useful information for the better understanding of child ER. Researchers have recently begun to utilise more person-oriented techniques to incorporate multiple ER elements (e.g. Han & Shaffer, 2013); however, few studies have incorporated teacher-report data from the school set- ting to demonstrate children’s ER across micro-contexts. If patterns of the children’s ER could be identi- fied from a person-centred manner, then it should be determined whether there is a linkage between chil- dren’s psychopathological outcomes and their ER pro- files. Many previous studies have only focused on broad categories (e.g. externalising or internalising) or a spe- cific type of problems (e.g. anxious, depressed). Some psychopathological symptoms are relatively understudied (e.g. somatic problems, attention problems), which might result in the loss of important information regarding the connection between ER and more nuanced psychopatho- logical symptoms. The existing literature is potentially important, but does not provide sufficient information for us to understand how children’s ER is associated with their psychopathological symptoms. © 2016 International Union of Psychological Science
  • 3. CHINESE CHILDREN’S EMOTION REGULATION 3 Taken together, the current study applied person-oriented techniques to incorporate informa- tion from multiple sources—children, mothers and teachers—in a Chinese sample. Specifically, it had sev- eral goals: (a) To utilise model-based cluster analysis to identify the profiles of child ER based on ratings from multiple informants (i.e. mother, teacher and child). On the basis of the potential and reasonable existence of dis- agreement among different informants (Achenbach et al., 1987) as well as the children’s potential inconsistent per- formance in different contexts, we expect the emergence of at least two types of clusters: the consistent ER pattern (in which children received all negative or all positive evaluations from all informants) and the inconsistent ER cluster pattern (in which children received inconsistent evaluations from various informants). (b) To examine the associations between children’s ER profiles and psychopathological symptoms in this group of Chinese children. It was expected that Chinese children with more at-risk ER profiles might present more psychopatholog- ical problems in both family and school settings. METHODS Participants Participants were recruited from an elementary school of a major city in northeastern China. A total of 208 two-parent families voluntarily participated in this study. Twelve families were excluded due to a misunderstanding of the instructions or a failure to return all scales. The rates of the remaining missing data ranged from 0 to 2.9%, and the expectation-maximisation (EM) imputation was used to replace the missing values. The remaining families included 196 fathers, (age M = 40.32; SD = 3.46), 196 mothers, (age M = 37.54; SD = 3.04), 96 boys (age M = 9.23, SD = 1.02) and 100 girls (age M = 9.06; SD = 1.02). In addition, 74.9% of families reported having an annual household income at or above average (i.e. approximately 10,000 USD) for dual-income families in the city in which the information was collected. All of the fathers and 194 of the mothers were the children’s biological parents. Procedure Parents received the introductory and invitation letter from their children’s head teachers at a school-wide parental conference. For the parents who agreed to participate, their children were asked to take home a series of questionnaires with informed written consent forms for the parents and children (the children’s written consent forms were asked to be signed by their primary caregivers on behalf of the children), which were sealed in large envelopes. Parents returned the questionnaires to their child’s head teacher after they had completed all of the questionnaires. Participant ID numbers were used to match the maternal and teacher’s reports. All of the participating families were provided with two books on parenting and child-rearing, which were selected by the school president. In addition, each participating child’s head teacher received a package of questionnaires and was asked to respond to the questions for each child based on their behaviours at school. All of the procedures were approved by the authors’ university Institutional Review Board. Measures Emotion Regulation Checklist (ERC) First, the children’s emotion regulation (ER) from the teacher’s and mother’s perspectives was measured using the Chinese translated version of the ERC (Shields & Cicchetti, 1997). The ERC consisted of 24 items that aimed to assess the child’s emotionality and regulation. Mothers and teachers were asked to report on how often the children emotionally reacted in a specific way using a 4-point Likert scale. The ERC originally consisted of two subscales (i.e. the Emotion Regulation scale and the Lia- bility/Negativity scale). Because this study targeted the children’s regulation of emotions, only the emotion reg- ulation subscale was selected (e.g. “Responds positively to neutral or friendly overtures by adults”). Previous studies using the Chinese version of the questionnaire demonstrated acceptable internal reliabilities (e.g. Suveg et al., 2014). The internal consistencies for the emotion regulation subscale of the maternal report and teacher report in the current sample were 𝛼 = .674 and 𝛼 = .804, respectively. Children’s Emotion Management Scales (CEMS) The children’s self-report measure of ER was mea- sured using the Children’s Emotion Management Scales (Zeman, Casino, Suveg, & Shipman, 2010). The Chinese version of CEMS was translated and back-translated by four bilingual translators. Differences in the original and back-translated versions were discussed and resolved through joint agreement. The CEMS consisted of 36 items that measured the children’s management of anger, sadness and worry. The coping subscale of the CEMS was used to ensure that all three measures captured regulatory aspects of ER. This coping subscale measured the children’s culturally appropriate and constructive strategies to manage worry, sadness and anger (i.e. “I try to calmly settle the problem when I feel worried”). Items were rated on a 3-point Likert scale. Previous studies adopting the CEMS with Chinese school-age children demonstrated acceptable reliability, 𝛼 = .70 (Suveg et al., © 2016 International Union of Psychological Science
  • 4. 4 LI AND HAN 2014). The reliability for the coping subscale in the current sample was good (𝛼 = .88). The Child Behaviour Checklist (CBCL) The Child Behaviour Checklist (Achenbach, 1991) was used as an index of the children’s psychopatho- logical symptoms. The CBCL was a 118-item measure of parent-reported psychosocial functioning of children over the past 6 months that yielded eight subscales in different behavioural domains: withdrawal, somatic problems, anxious/depressed, social problems, thought problems, attention problems, rule breaking and aggres- sive behaviours. All of the items were rated on a 3-point Likert scale. The Chinese version of the CBCL has been widely used and has shown good test–retest reliabilities and validity (Leung et al., 2006). The internal consisten- cies of maternal-reported CBCL subscales in this study were as follows: withdrawn/depressed, 𝛼 = .89; somatic problems, 𝛼 = .97; anxious/depressed, 𝛼 = .91; social problems, 𝛼 = .88; thought problems, 𝛼 = .83; attention problems, 𝛼 = .91; rule breaking, 𝛼 = .92; and aggressive behaviours, 𝛼 = .94. Child Behaviour Checklist Teacher Report Form (TRF) The TRF was CBCL’s parallel form (Achenbach, 1991). A teacher familiar with the participating child completed the Chinese version of the Teacher’s Report Form for each youth. Similar to the other informant versions, the TRF-generated broad scores and more spe- cific subscale scores of the children’s anxious/depressed, withdrawn problems, somatic complaints, social prob- lems, thought problems, attention problems, aggressive behaviours and delinquent behaviours. The Chinese version of the TRF has been previously used within Chinese samples and has proven to be reliable and valid (Leung et al., 2006). The internal consistencies of all of the subscales in this study were as follows: with- drawn/depressed, 𝛼 = .86; somatic problems, 𝛼 = .97; anxious/depressed, 𝛼 = .79; social problems, 𝛼 = .72; thought problems, 𝛼 = .73; attention problems, 𝛼 = .74; rule breaking, 𝛼 = .87 and aggressive behaviours, 𝛼 = .79. Analytical plan Descriptive statistics as well as correlational tests among the study variables were examined as preliminary analy- ses. Next, potential group differences in study variables based on demographic characteristics were performed to provide a basic index for further analysis. A model-based cluster analysis using the Mclust program developed for R software (Fraley & Raftery, 2006) was performed on the multiple measurements of the children’s ER. Model-based cluster analysis overcomes some of the inherent liabilities of traditional cluster analysis. Specif- ically, this type of cluster analysis is more data-driven, thereby reducing difficulties in determining the number of clusters by providing a fit index (the Bayesian Informa- tion Criterion, BIC). Higher BIC values indicate a better fit of the model. When comparing models, a difference in the BIC of 0–2 is considered weak support for the better fitting model; 2–6 is considered positive support; 6–10 is considered strong support and over 10 is considered very strong support (Raftery, 1995). In this study, the ER scores from mothers, teachers and children were entered into the analysis. The best fitting model was selected for further analysis. Finally, a series of one-way analyses of variance (ANOVAs) were performed to examine potential differences across the identified clusters on the children’s psychopathological symptoms. RESULTS Correlational tests were performed among the children’s ER as reported by different informants. No significant correlations were found, including that between child and mother, child and teacher and mother and teacher. As for psychopathological symptoms reported by mothers and teachers, the results demonstrated low concordance between both sources on all of the subscale scores. Only low to moderate correlations were found for thought problems, r(196) = .23, p = .001, and attention problems, r(196) = .20, p = .006. Correlations between the rest pairs of subscales were not significant. Model-based cluster analysis: Cluster results As shown in Figure 1, the best fitting model (BIC value = −1619.83) yielded a four-class solution with diagonal clusters with a variable volume and equal shape (characteristics of the distribution). The next best fit- ting model (BIC value = −1630.09) yielded a two-class solution with spherical clusters with a variable volume and equal shape. According to the rule of thumb for interpreting BIC value differences proposed by Raftery (1995), the best fitting model was strongly supported (ΔBIC = 10.26). Thus, the four diagonal clusters with a variable volume and equal shape were selected. However, because Cluster 4 only had five participants and the mean was within 1 SD of Cluster 2, the samples in Cluster 2 and Cluster 4 demonstrated the same trend of evaluation from different informants (i.e. three informants all reported positive ER abilities for children in these two clusters). We recoded the children in Cluster 4 into Cluster 2 for ease of discussion. Each participant was classified into an appropriate group based on their ER profiles. The results of the final three clusters were presented in Figure 1, which displayed each of their score’s deviation from the sample mean on standardised units. © 2016 International Union of Psychological Science
  • 5. CHINESE CHILDREN’S EMOTION REGULATION 5 Figure 1. Standard deviations from the overall sample mean of each cluster for all measures on child emotion regulation from the best fitting model with four-cluster solution. Three groups identified by the clustering procedure were differentiated according to the ER information col- lected from different informants. The majority of children in the current sample (n = 120; 61.22% of the sample) was classified as belonging to the overall good ER group (Cluster 2), in which the individuals received, on average, moderately positive evaluations from all three informants. A number of children (n = 40; 20.41% of the sample) fell into the poor school ER group (Cluster 3). The ER pat- terns of the children in this cluster were characterised by the most negative evaluations from their teachers, slightly negative self-evaluations and moderately positive assess- ments from their mothers. In contrast, the poor family ER group (Cluster 1) consisted of 36 children (18.37% of the sample) whose ER abilities were rated most nega- tively by their mothers, slightly negative by themselves, but were slightly above average based on their teacher’s reports. Importantly, when giving low scores, informants appeared to disagree with each other. For example, for children in Cluster 1, their teachers reported significantly higher scores than their mothers, t(35) = 8.08, p < .001, whereas for children in Cluster 3, their mothers’ scores were significantly higher than those of their teachers, t(39) = 13.14, p < .001. Cluster differences Demographic variables After the profiles were identified, subgroups were first examined on a number of demographic variables, including gender, age and annual household income. These results showed that these groups did not differ on maternal age, F(2, 186) =. 89, p = .412; child age, F(2, 191) = .89, p = .410; or annual household income, F(2, 188) = 1.88, p = .156. Gender differences were found in Cluster 1, 𝜒2(1) = 4.00, p = .046, with more boys than girls belonging to this cluster. No gender differences were found in the other two clusters. The descriptive statistics for the clusters and demographic variables are shown in Table 1. Clustering variables A series of ANOVAs among the clustering variables showed statistically significant differences across the three subgroups on each informant’s report, including maternally reported ER, F(2, 193) = 152.24, p < .001; teacher-reported ER, F(2, 193) = 192.44, p < .001; and children’s self-reported ER, F(2, 193) = 10.16, p < .001. Post-hoc analyses indicated that on the teacher reports, differences existed between any two groups; on the mater- nal reports, differences existed between any two groups, except between Cluster 2 and Cluster 3; whereas on the child’s self-report, group differences only existed between Cluster 1 and Cluster 2. Psychopathological problems The clusters were associated with psychopathological symptom information collected from parents and teachers to identify the associations between particular ER pro- files and the children’s psychosocial functioning. Because © 2016 International Union of Psychological Science
  • 6. 6 LI AND HAN TABLE 1 One-way analysis of variance (ANOVA) examining cluster differences on ER variables, demographic variables and teacher-reported children’s emotional and behavioural problems ER cluster Cluster 1 (n = 36) Mean (SD) Cluster 2 (n = 120) Mean (SD) Cluster 3 (n = 40) Mean (SD) F ER variables Maternal report 3.17 (0.33) 3.99 (0.21) 3.93 (0.28) 151.24** Teacher report 3.73 (0.28) 3.92 (0.21) 2.98 (0.37) 192.44** Child self-report 1.93 (0.53) 2.32 (0.47) 2.04 (0.56) 10.16** Demographic variables Child age 9.29 (1.10) 9.16 (1.04) 8.98 (0.89) 0.90 Mother age 37.97 (3.85) 37.58 (2.74) 37.05 (3.03) 0.89 SES 6.47 (1.83) 6.02 (1.91) 5.59 (2.10) 1.88 TRF subscales Anxious/depressed 54.15 (7.80) 53.05 (5.46) 54.23 (6.35) 0.81 Withdrawn 55.84 (7.24) 52.93 (4.70) 54.55 (7.19) 3.92** Somatic complains 52.85 (5.84) 50.68 (3.30) 51.42 (4.37) 3.93** Social problems 57.08 (9.13) 54.68 (6.40) 57.56 (10.29) 2.67* Thought problems 55.69 (8.91) 52.19 (5.85) 52.13 (5.32) 4.40** Attention problems 55.26 (8.28) 53.51 (5.68) 57.10 (10.37) 3.77** Delinquent behavior 55.21 (8.16) 53.95 (6.03) 56.03 (9.13) 1.41 Aggressive behavior 56.25 (9.28) 53.65 (5.71) 55.21 (8.13) 2.19 *p < .05. **p < .01. SES = Socleconomic one goal of this study was to shed light on children’s psychosocial behaviours under different contexts (home or school), we performed ANOVAs separately among mother-reported data and teacher-reported data. First, we performed one-way ANOVAs on the family-reported psy- chopathological information of their children. Unexpect- edly, no significant group differences emerged on each subscale in mother-reported CBCL. Next, we explored whether the ER clusters generated from the data would differ at the level of the children’s psychopathologi- cal symptoms reported by teachers based on the chil- dren’s behaviours at school. Significant differences were found among the ER profiles when compared at the level of withdrawal depression, F(2, 193) = 3.92, p = .022; somatic complains, F(2, 193) = 3.93, p = .021; thought problems, F(2, 193) = 4.40, p = .014; and attention prob- lems, F(2, 193) = 3.77, p = .025. These group differences are shown in Table 1. Further explorations indicated that children who belonged to the poor family ER group (Cluster 1) were reported to have the highest levels of somatic complaints (M = 52.85, SD = 5.84); thought problems (M = 55.69, SD = 8.91) and withdrawal depression (M = 55.84, SD = 7.24) by their teachers. Attention problems in this subgroup were moderately higher than average (M = 55.26, SD = 8.28). As for the individuals in the other ER-inconsistent groups, children in the poor school ER group (Cluster 3) were rated slightly more positively than children in the poor family ER group (Cluster 1) by their teachers, but they still were rated more negatively than children in the Cluster 2. In Cluster 3, children were reported to have the highest levels of attention problems (M = 57.10, SD = 10.37), and they had the second highest levels of withdrawal problems (M = 54.55, SD = 7.19) and somatic complains (M = 51.42, SD = 4.37). DISCUSSION The current study utilised person-centred analyses to characterise ER patterns in a Chinese school-age sample as well as to examine how these profiles are associated with children’s psychopathological symptoms demon- strated at home and at school. This study has advanced the literature by incorporating cross-informants reports on children’s ER and dual perspectives on children’s emo- tional and behavioural problems at home and at school in a Chinese sample. Specifically, we adopted person-oriented analyses to minimise problems with the heterogeneity of child ER information from various sources. Com- pared with the traditional variable-oriented approach, the employment of model-based cluster analyses echoed the call for multi-assessment of child ER in the litera- ture (Adrian et al., 2011). Specifically, emotion and ER are complex constructs that can be assessed at multi- ple levels, including physiological and behavioural lev- els. From a behavioural aspect, both reported obtained from other studies and self-reports can provide mean- ingful information on youth’s emotional functioning, but either reporter per se can draw a full picture of children’s ER profiles. Thus, the multifaceted nature of emotion and ER lend themselves well to a multi-method assess- ment. When information from multiple recourses were © 2016 International Union of Psychological Science
  • 7. CHINESE CHILDREN’S EMOTION REGULATION 7 collected, the utilisation of person-centred statistical tech- niques became key, as it contributed to the interpretation of these data without removing the unique contribution from each reporter. In this study, person-oriented analyses identified four distinct groups of children based on the different infor- mants’ assessments of the children’s ER (i.e. mother, teacher and child). We combined two clusters with the same trend for ease of discussion. More than half of the participating children were placed into the overall good ER group (Cluster 2), indicating that the major- ity of our sample (i.e. a community sample of Chi- nese school-age children) consistently displayed good ER skills across contexts. Expectedly, there were also children who received inconsistent ER assessments from family and school raters (children in Cluster 1 and Clus- ter 3). Such inconsistency between raters further con- firmed the necessity of employing a multi-informant and person-centred approach. Individuals from these inconsistent ER groups together occupied 38.78% of the overall sample. Although the number was smaller than individuals in the consistent ER cluster, these individuals occupied a considerable propor- tion of the current sample. Special attention should be paid to children in these two inconsistent clusters because their emotional problems might only be observable under a specific micro-context (family or school). For instance, mothers of children in the poor school ER (Cluster 3) group may miss their children’s difficulties with ER at home, whereas teachers of children in the poor family ER group (Cluster 1) may not notice the children’s problems with ER at school. Specifically, individuals in the poor family ER group received the lowest ER scores from their mothers, which were not confirmed by either the children themselves or the teachers. In contrast, children in the poor school ER group obtained the poorest ER scores from their teachers and an average evaluation from their mothers. These results suggest that many children performed dif- ferently at home and at school in terms of using ER strate- gies. However, another potential explanation for these diverging results might also come from the mothers’ biased reports. Previous studies have shown that moth- ers with higher levels of depression or anxiety were more likely to provide biased reports on children’s emotional problems, which were not consistent with other infor- mants (Briggs-Gowan, Carter, & Schwab-Stone, 1996). Future studies are strongly encouraged to include mater- nal depressive symptoms in the analyses as well as to incorporate paternal ratings and peer ratings, as fathers may provide an evaluation under the family context in addition to mothers and peers could rate the children’s behaviours under school settings in addition to teachers. In sum, consistent with our expectation, all three clus- tering variables demonstrated statistically significant dif- ferences across the final three clusters. These findings helped to confirm the validity of the three profiles: indi- viduals of each cluster showed distinct ER patterns when incorporating the perspectives from the mother, teacher and child per se. When investigating the demographic variables, we found that the groups did not differ on the child’s age, maternal age or paternal age. However, girls appeared to slightly outperform boys on ER abili- ties. There were more girls than boys in the overall good ER group, whereas there were more boys than girls in the poor family ER group and poor school ER group. This finding was consistent with those obtained from Western school-age samples (e.g. Larsson & Frisk, 1999), such that girls appeared to be more emotionally competent than boys at this developmental stage. As for our hypotheses regarding the associations between ER profiles and children’s psychopatholog- ical symptoms, group differences were found only on the teacher-reported but not the parent-reported psychopathological symptoms of the children, which suggested that ER patterns appeared to be more infor- mative of psychological and behavioural adjustments in the school context. It could be that when a teacher was observing the child in the school context, she would automatically regard other children as her references for evaluation. Thus, teachers might be more likely to obtain an integrative perspective when making evaluations. Such overall perspectives might enable the teachers to better differentiate the characteristics of one child from another. In contrast, the majority of parents in this Chinese sample were parents of an only child (89.80%), such that these Chinese parents did not have easily accessible children for reference in order for them to rate their child’s behaviours. Thus, the differences between having compared to not having available comparisons may contribute to these distinct findings regarding the association between ER patterns with teacher-reported and family-reported psychopathological outcomes of children. As for the associations between ER patterns and teacher-reported psychopathological symptoms, we found a positive trend in the overall good ER group. Specifically, children in the inconsistent subgroups were reported as having more somatic complaints, thought problems and withdrawal depressive symptoms as well as attention problems compared to children in the over- all good ER group. These results were consistent with findings from Western samples, such that children with more effective ER strategies demonstrated more adaptive behaviours (e.g. Rubin, Coplan, Fox, & Calkins, 1995) and fewer psychopathological problems (e.g. Kim & Cic- chetti, 2010). More specifically, Schmeichel (2007) found that maladaptive emotional expressions undermined per- formance on subsequent tests of working memory span, which was related to child attention problems and thought problems (Mezzacappa & Buckner, 2010). Rubin et al. (1995) discovered that children with poor ER abilities © 2016 International Union of Psychological Science
  • 8. 8 LI AND HAN displayed more wary and anxious behaviours during free play and interaction with others, which could be observed as typical symptoms of withdrawal depression. Lundh and Simonsson-Sarnecki (2001) also linked somatic com- plaints and emotional difficulties in youth samples. Our results showed consistent findings in Chinese school-age children with that obtained from Western samples; they also confirmed that when we aim to describe the chil- dren’s emotional world, a single informant might not be sufficient. In addition, it should be noted that consistent with existing evidence (De Los Reyes & Kazdin, 2005), the results of this study also indicated that there was a fair amount of disagreement among various informants regarding the children’s emotional competence and psy- chopathological problems. There were no significant cor- relations between any rater pairs on children’s emotional competence. Similarly, teachers’ reports showed diverg- ing results from those of their parents on most ratings on psychopathological symptoms. Although this study is among the first to depict the emotional profiles of Chinese children with a multi-assessment and person-centred approach, sev- eral limitations must be considered. The first limitation of this study is the lack of behavioural observation. Although family- and school-reported data have previ- ously demonstrated a relatively comprehensive picture of children’s emotional and behavioural status across contexts, behavioural observation from the professional perspective of researchers would provide more useful information. Second, the finding about the inconsistent reports of mothers and teachers on children’s behavioural and emotional functioning should be interpreted with caution because it might also be due to rater difference rather than the true context-specific performance of children. In addition, it is important to note that no conclusions about causal pathways or developmental patterns can be drawn from the results of this study due to its cross-sectional nature. Future studies on the pre- dictive power of children’s ER abilities on their clinical outcomes might significantly benefit from a longitudinal design. Manuscript received May 2015 Revised manuscript accepted November 2015 REFERENCES Abraham, K. (1911). Notes on the psycho-analytical investiga- tion and treatment of manic-depressive insanity and allied conditions. In Jones E (ed)., Selected papers on psychoanal- ysis (pp. 137–156). London, England: Hogarth. Achenbach, T. M. (1991). Integrative guide for the 1991 CBCL/4-18, YSR, and TRF profiles. Department of Psychia- try, University of Vermont Burlington, VT. Achenbach, T. M., McConaughy, S. H., & Howell, C. T. (1987). Child/adolescent behavioral and emotional problems: Impli- cations of cross-informant correlations for situational speci- ficity. Psychological Bulletin, 101, 213–232. Adrian, M., Zeman, J., & Veits, G. (2011). Methodological implications of the affect revolution: A 35-year review of emotion regulation assessment in children. Journal of Exper- imental Child Psychology, 110, 171–197. Briggs-Gowan, M. J., Carter, A. S., & Schwab-Stone, M. (1996). Discrepancies among mother, child, and teacher reports: Examining the contributions of maternal depression and anx- iety. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 24, 749–765. Bronfenbrenner, U. (1992). Ecological systems theory. Jessica Kingsley. De Los Reyes, A., & Kazdin, A. E. (2005). Informant discrep- ancies in the assessment of childhood psychopathology: a critical review, theoretical framework, and recommendations for further study. Psychological Bulletin, 131, 483–509. Folk, J. B., Zeman, J., Poon, J. A., & Dallaire, D. H. (2014). A longitudinal examination of emotion regulation: pathways to anxiety and depressive symptoms in urban minority youth. Child and Adolescent Mental Health, 19, 243–250. Fraley, C., & Raftery, A. E. (2006). MCLUST version 3: An R package for normal mixture modeling and model-based clustering. Washington University Department of Statistics. Han, Z. R., & Shaffer, A. (2013). The relation of parental emo- tion dysregulation to children’s psychopathology symptoms: The moderating role of child emotion dysregulation. Child Psychiatry and Human Development, 44, 591–601. Kim, J., & Cicchetti, D. (2010). Longitudinal pathways linking child maltreatment, emotion regulation, peer relations, and psychopathology. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychia- try, 51, 706–716. Lansford, J. E., Malone, P. S., Stevens, K. I., Dodge, K. A., Bates, J. E., & Pettit, G. S. (2006). Developmental trajec- tories of externalizing and internalizing behaviors: Factors underlying resilience in physically abused children. Devel- opment and Psychopathology, 18, 35–55. Larsson, B., & Frisk, M. (1999). Social competence and emotional/behavior problems in 6–16 year-old Swedish school children. European Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 8, 24–33. Leung, P. W. L., Kwong, S. L., Tang, C. P., Ho, T. P., Hung, S. F., Lee, C. C., … Liu, W. S. (2006). Test-retest reliability and criterion validity of the Chinese version of CBCL, TRF, and YSR. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, and Allied Disciplines, 47, 970–973. Loeber, R., & Dishion, T. J. (1984). Boys who fight at home and school: Family conditions influencing cross-setting con- sistency. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 52, 759–768. Lundh, L. G., & Simonsson-Sarnecki, M. (2001). Alexithymia, emotion, and somatic complaints. Journal of Personality, 69, 483–510. Matsumoto, D., Yoo, S. H., & Nakagawa, S. (2008). Culture, emotion regulation, and adjustment. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 94, 925–937. McLaughlin, K. A., Hatzenbuehler, M. L., Mennin, D. S., & Nolen-Hoeksema, S. (2011). Emotion dysregulation and ado- lescent psychopathology: A prospective study. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 49, 544–554. © 2016 International Union of Psychological Science
  • 9. CHINESE CHILDREN’S EMOTION REGULATION 9 Mezzacappa, E., & Buckner, J. C. (2010). Working memory training for children with attention problems or hyperactiv- ity: A school-based pilot study. School Mental Health, 2, 202–208. Mineka, S., & Sutton, S. K. (1992). Cognitive biases and the emotional disorders. Psychological Science, 3, 65–69. Raftery, A. E. (1995). Bayesian model selection in social research. Sociological Methodology, 25, 111–164. Roberton, T., Daffern, M., & Bucks, R. S. (2012). Emotion regulation and aggression. Aggression and Violent Behavior, 17, 72–82. Rubin, K. H., Coplan, R. J., Fox, N. A., & Calkins, S. D. (1995). Emotionality, emotion regulation, and preschoolers’ social adaptation. Development and Psychopathology, 7, 49–62. Saarni, C., Campos, J. J., Camras, L. A., & Witherington, D. (2006). Handbook of child psychology. New York, NY: Wiley. Schmeichel, B. J. (2007). Attention control, memory updating, and emotion regulation temporarily reduce the capacity for executive control. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Gen- eral, 136, 241–255. Shields, A., & Cicchetti, D. (1997). Emotion regulation among school-age children: The development and validation of a new criterion Q-sort scale. Developmental Psychology, 33, 906–916. Soto, J. A., Perez, C. R., Kim, Y. H., Lee, E. A., & Minnick, M. R. (2011). Is expressive suppression always associated with poorer psychological functioning? A cross-cultural compar- ison between European Americans and Hong Kong Chinese. Emotion, 11, 1450–1455. Suveg, C., Raley, J. N., Morelen, D., Wang, W., Han, R. Z., & Campion, S. (2014). Child and family emotional functioning: A cross-national examination of families from China and the United States. Journal of Child and Family Studies, 23, 1444–1454. Thompson, R. A. (1994). Emotion regulation: A theme in search of definition. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 59, 25–52. Zeman, J., Casino, M., Suveg, C., & Shipman, K. (2010). Initial validation of the children’s worry management scale. Journal of Child and Family Studies, 19, 381–392. © 2016 International Union of Psychological Science