SlideShare a Scribd company logo
Adults’ use of subtraction by addition
Greet Peters a,
⁎, Bert De Smedt b
, Joke Torbeyns a,c
, Pol Ghesquière b
, Lieven Verschaffel a
a
Centre for Instructional Psychology and Technology, K.U.Leuven, Belgium
b
Centre for Parenting, Child Welfare, and Disabilities, K.U.Leuven, Belgium
c
GROEP T – Leuven Education College, Belgium
a b s t r a c t
a r t i c l e i n f o
Article history:
Received 17 March 2010
Received in revised form 23 August 2010
Accepted 25 August 2010
Keywords:
Multi-digit subtraction
Strategy use
Subtraction by addition
The present study investigates adults’ use of addition to solve two-digit subtractions. Inspired by research on
single-digit arithmetic, we first examined regression models in which different problem characteristics predicted
participants’ reaction times. Second, we compared performance on two-digit subtractions presented in 2
presentation formats, i.e., the standard subtraction format (81–37=.) and an addition format (37+.=81). Both
methods lead to the conclusion that the participants switched between direct subtraction and subtraction by
addition depending on the relative size of the subtrahend: If the subtrahend was smaller than the difference,
direct subtraction was mainly used; if the subtrahend was larger than the difference, subtraction by addition was
the dominant strategy. However, this performance pattern was only observed when the distance between the
subtrahend and the difference was large; when the subtrahend and the difference were close to each other, there
was no subtrahend-dependent selection of direct subtraction vs. subtraction by addition. These data indicate that
theoretical models of people's strategy choices in subtraction should include the relative size of the subtrahend as
an important factor in the strategy selection process.
© 2010 Elsevier B.V. All rights reserved.
1. Introduction
For a long time, researchers have investigated how people solve
symbolically presented subtraction problems of the type M−S=.
(Barrouillet, Mignon, & Thevenot, 2008; Blöte, Klein, & Beishuizen,
2000; Fuson, 1992; Robinson, 2001; Selter, 2001; Verschaffel, Greer, &
De Corte, 2007; Woods, Resnick, & Groen, 1975). The bulk of this
research relates to single-digit subtraction, whereas only few studies
have looked at people's strategy use on multi-digit subtraction. This
is surprising, given the considerable differences between single-digit
and multi-digit arithmetic in the procedures and underlying arithmetic
concepts being used (Fuson, 1992; Verschaffel et al., 2007). This study
therefore aimed at examining people's strategy use in multi-digit
subtraction, by applying – for the first time – research methods that
have been frequently used in studies on single-digit arithmetic. In the
remainder of this introduction, we review the research on strategy use in
single-digit and multi-digit subtraction, together with the applied
research methods.
Woods et al. (1975) investigated how children solved symbolically
presented single-digit subtractions of the form M−S=. by fitting their
reaction times (RTs) to different linear regression models. They
observed that children solved these problems by counting up or down,
whichever involved the fewest counting steps (i.e., counting down for
9−2 vs. counting up for 9−7). After a period wherein accuracy and RTs
were the main data sources to investigate strategy use (Ashcraft, 1982;
Groen & Poll, 1973), verbal protocols became an increasingly
important research method (Campbell & Xue, 2001; Geary, Frensch,
& Wiley, 1993; LeFevre, DeStefano, Penner-Wilger, & Daley, 2006;
Seyler, Kirk, & Ashcraft, 2003). These verbal protocol data showed that
adults did not rely exclusively on direct retrieval when solving single-
digit subtractions, which was assumed to be the expert way of doing
single-digit arithmetic (e.g., Ashcraft, 1992; Siegler & Shrager, 1984).
Adults sometimes used procedural strategies, such as counting or
decomposing the problem into smaller facts, particularly on the so-
called large single-digit subtractions, i.e., with a minuend N10. Most
interestingly within the context of the present study is the observation
that adults sometimes used addition-based strategies to solve single-
digit subtractions (e.g., solving 9−7 by means of the known addition
fact 7+2=9, or solving 13−9 by counting or adding up from 9 to 13:
9+1=10 and 10+3=13, so the answer is 1+3=4) (Campbell &
Xue, 2001; Geary et al., 1993; LeFevre et al., 2006). However, these
addition-based strategies are mostly executed in a very fast and quasi-
automated way, which makes them difficult to investigate by means of
verbal protocols (Kirk & Ashcraft, 2001).
Campbell (2008) recently introduced another method to investigate
whether people use addition to solve single-digit subtractions. Based on
a design originally developed for multiplication and division by Mauro,
LeFevre, and Morris (2003), he presented single-digit subtractions in
subtraction (e.g., 12−9=.) and addition format (e.g., 9+.=12). He
Acta Psychologica 135 (2010) 323–329
⁎ Corresponding author. Andreas Vesaliusstraat 2, box 3770, 3000 Leuven, Belgium.
Tel.: +32 16 32 62 47; fax: +32 16 32 62 74.
E-mail address: Greet.Peters@ped.kuleuven.be (G. Peters).
0001-6918/$ – see front matter © 2010 Elsevier B.V. All rights reserved.
doi:10.1016/j.actpsy.2010.08.007
Contents lists available at ScienceDirect
Acta Psychologica
journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/ locate/actpsy
reasoned that if subtractions are solved by addition, the problems
presented in addition format should be solved faster than those in
subtraction format. This should be so because a subtraction presented in
addition format does not require the mental re-representation of the
subtraction into an addition,whereassuch a re-representation is needed
when the problem is presented in subtraction format. In other words,
the addition format might be a better cue for retrieving the addition fact
than the subtraction format and/or it might be a facilitator for executing
an addition-based procedure. Campbell successfully demonstrated that
large single-digit subtractions were solved significantly faster in
addition than in subtraction format, which indicated that adults used
addition to solve large single-digit subtractions. No such format
differences were observed on small single-digit subtractions.
Peters, De Smedt, Torbeyns, Ghesquière, and Verschaffel (2010)
recently refined Campbell's findings, by showing that this use of
subtraction by addition in large single-digit subtractions depends on
the relative size of the subtrahend. They suggested that adults solve
large single-digit subtractions by switching between subtraction by
addition and direct subtraction, taking into account which strategy is
most efficient. This mirrors the above-mentioned finding of Woods et
al. (1975) about children's flexible switches between counting up or
down on small single-digit subtractions.
Can these findings on strategy use in single-digit subtraction be
generalised to multi-digit subtraction? Within the mathematics
education literature, several authors have reported the use of
addition-based strategies on multi-digit subtractions (e.g., 71−57
by 57+10=67, 67+4=71; so the answer is 10+4=14), thereby
using different terms to denote this strategy, such as the forward
strategy (Brissiaud, 1994), solving subtractions by means of addition
(Beishuizen, 1997), the short jump strategy (Blöte et al., 2000), the
adding up strategy (Selter, 2001), or indirect addition (Torbeyns,
Ghesquière, & Verschaffel, 2009). In this article, we will call this
strategy subtraction by addition, whereas direct subtraction will be
used to refer to the common subtraction strategy where the
subtrahend is directly subtracted from the minuend.
Torbeyns et al. (2009) investigated adults’ use of subtraction by
addition on three-digit subtractions with the choice/no-choice para-
digm andsystematically manipulated therelative sizeof thesubtrahend.
Accuracy and speed data from the two no-choice conditions (wherein
participants had to solve all items first by means of direct subtraction
and afterwards via subtraction by addition, or vice versa) indicated that
subtraction by addition was a highly efficient strategy for three-digit
subtraction, irrespective of the size of the subtrahend. The verbal
protocol data from the choice condition indicated that this subtraction
by addition strategy was mainly selected on problems with a relatively
large subtrahend(e.g., 812−783),whichsuggests that participants took
into account the size of the subtrahend when selecting their strategy.
The use of subtraction by addition in multi-digit subtraction has
also been explored in children, again with verbal protocols. Different
from adults, children appeared to hardly apply this strategy
spontaneously, not even when they were confronted with problems
with a very large subtrahend as in 81−79 (Selter, 2001; Torbeyns, De
Smedt, Ghesquière, & Verschaffel, 2009b), when they were explicitly
stimulated to generate a strategy other than direct subtraction
(Torbeyns, De Smedt, Ghesquière, & Verschaffel, 2009a), or after
they received explicit training in subtraction by addition (De Smedt,
Torbeyns, Stassens, Ghesquière, & Verschaffel, 2010).
We remind that the available studies on subtraction by addition in
multi-digit arithmetic relied mainly on verbal protocol data. As for
research in single-digit arithmetic, it can be argued that verbal protocols
may be less suited to identify the subtraction by addition strategy in
multi-digit subtraction because this strategy may be executed very fast
and quasi-automatic. For example, when solving a problem such as
81−79, people may be unaware of the calculation steps they executed,
or they may have difficulties in articulatingprecisely how they foundthe
answer. This may have resulted in an underestimation of the number of
subtraction by addition strategies in the available studies on multi-digit
subtraction. In this respect, we point out that both the data of Torbeyns
et al. (2009) and of De Smedt et al. (2010) suggest that participants
sometimes used subtraction by addition while reporting the direct
subtraction strategy. If participants only used direct subtraction, an
increase in RTs should have been observed from items with small
subtrahends over items with medium-sized subtrahends to items with
large subtrahends because subtracting a larger subtrahend from a given
minuend requires more and/or larger calculation steps. However, both
studies showed that not only problems with small but also with large
subtrahends were solved faster than problems with medium-sized
subtrahends. This indicates that participants did not always use direct
subtraction,and it stronglysuggests that the verbal protocol data did not
always capture the actually applied strategy. Therefore, it justifies the
application of other methods of inferring participants’ strategy use that
have been successfully used in single-digit arithmetic, namely the
regression-based approach (Groen & Poll, 1973; Woods et al., 1975) and
the manipulation of presentation formats (Campbell, 2008; Peters et al.,
2010).
2. The present study
We used a stepwise regression analysis and the manipulation of
presentation formats to investigate the use of subtraction by addition
in multi-digit subtraction. We presented adult participants with two-
digit subtractions1
in the standard subtraction format (83−4=.) and
in its corresponding addition format (4+.=83), manipulating the
relative size of the subtrahend.
Following Groen and Poll (1973) and Woods et al. (1975), we first
calculated a stepwise regression model in which participants' RTs
on two-digit subtractions were predicted by the presentation format
(M−S=. or S+.=M), three variables referring to strategy use, and
their respective interactions. The variables referring to strategy use
were the to-be-determined difference (D), the known subtrahend (S),
and the minimum of difference and subtrahend (min[D, S]), which
reflect the use of subtraction by addition, direct subtraction, and
switching between both strategies depending on the size of the
subtrahend, respectively. In view of Peters et al. (2010) findings in
single-digit subtraction, we expected that a model including the min
(D, S) predictor as the variable that explains most of the variability in
RTs would provide the best fit, suggesting the mixed use of subtraction
by addition and direct subtraction in both presentation formats.
We also compared participants’ performance on different problem
types in the two presentation formats. We predicted an interaction
between thepresentationformat(M−S=. vs. S+.=M), themagnitude
of S compared to D (SND vs. SbD), and the numerical distance between
S and D (large vs. small). For large-distance problems, we predicted that
subtraction by addition would be used to solve SND problems and that
the direct subtraction strategy would be used to solve SbD problems.
The SND subtractions presented in addition format are thus expected
to be solved faster than the corresponding problems in subtraction
format, whereas SbD subtractions are expected to be solved faster in
subtraction than in addition format, this because the addition format
may facilitate the execution of the subtraction by addition strategy
more than the subtraction format (and vice versa for direct subtraction).
For small-distance problems, we predicted no interaction between
the magnitude of S compared to D and presentation format because
the computational advantage of one of the two strategies is less clear.
1
‘Two-digit subtraction’ has different meanings in the research literature. According
to some authors, it refers only to the subtraction of a two-digit number from a two-
digit number yielding a two-digit result (e.g., 75−36=.). However, it can also be used
to refer to problems with a single-digit subtrahend subtracted from a two-digit
minuend resulting in a two-digit difference (e.g., 71−2=.) or to refer to problems
with a two-digit subtrahend subtracted from a two-digit minuend resulting in a
single-digit difference (e.g., 71−69=.), as we did in the current manuscript (see also
Brissiaud, 1994; Van Mulken, 1992).
324 G. Peters et al. / Acta Psychologica 135 (2010) 323–329
3. Method
3.1. Participants
Participants were 25 university students (5 men, 20 women;
Mage =26 years, SDage =7 years) from the Department of Educational
Sciences of the Katholieke Universiteit Leuven. All of them had at least
a bachelor degree. They had normal or corrected-to-normal vision and
none of them reported learning difficulties.
3.2. Materials and procedure
Participants were asked to mentally solve 32 subtraction problems,
which had a two-digit minuend larger than 30 and required borrowing.
Problems were presented in the horizontal subtraction format2
(52−4=.) and in their corresponding addition format (4+.=52),
yielding a total set of 64 trials. They were divided into four problem types
based on the combination of the magnitude of S compared to D (SND or
SbD) and the numerical distancebetweenS and D (small or large). Small-
distance problems were defined by S and D differing by less than 10,
whereas in the large-distance problems S and D differed by at
least 10 and either S or D was a one-digit number. This resulted
in the following problem types: (a)large-distance SbD problems,
with subtrahends smaller than 10 (e.g., 83−4=. and 8+.=34);
(b)large-distance SND problems, with differences smaller than 10
(e.g., 77−68=. and 37+.=42); (c)small-distance SbD problems
(e.g., 92−44=. and 36+.=75); and (d)small-distance SND
problems (e.g., 32−17=. and 29+.=53) (see Appendix).
Threepracticeproblemsperpresentationformatwereadministeredto
familiarise participants with the task administration. The experimental
items were presented into six blocks, assigned in a pseudo-random
manner with the following constraints: (a) items from the same problem
typedid notoccuronmorethan twoconsecutive trials; (b)itemsthatonly
differed in presentation format (e.g., 31−28=. and 28+.=31) and
complementary items (e.g., 31−28=. and 31−3=.) were not
presented in the same block. Task instructions stressed both accuracy
and speed.
Each trial started with a fixation asterisk that appeared for 1000 ms
in the centre of a DELL personal computer screen. Next, the item was
presented horizontally in the middle of the screen. The characters were
2×2 cm large, black, separated by adjacent spaces and presented
against a white background. Time started to run when the item
appeared, and ended when the verbal response triggered the sound-
activated voice key. The item remained on the screen until the voice key
was triggered, with a maximum of 30 s. Participants were instructed to
articulate only the final answer and not the intermediate solution steps.
A computer connected to the voice key registered RT with millisecond
precision. The experimenter then entered the answer via the keyboard.
After that, the next trial was initiated. No feedback was given. Trials with
incorrect voice key registrations were presented again at the end of each
block, to avoid losing too many data3
.
We instructed participants on how to answer during the practice
trials, asking them to say the numerical answer to the problem as soon
as they knew the answer and without any preceding words or
phrases, such as “that makes…” or “the outcome is”. Furthermore, we
started the experiment by asking participants to name a given set of
numbers in order to make sure that the voice key reliably detected the
different initial phonemes of potential answers. Participants com-
pleted the task in a quiet room. They solved the task individually on a
computer in the presence of an experimenter.
4. Results
A total of 1600 items was solved. Seventeen items were excluded
because of voice key errors (1.06%). For RT analysis, 131 items were
deleted because of incorrect answers (8.49%) and 39 items (2.44%)
were additionally discarded as outliers (i.e., more than 2 standard
deviations from a participant's cell mean for the correct items per
problem type).
Results are presented in two parts. The first part involves the
evaluation of the regression models that predict participants’ RTs. In
the second part, we compare performance on both presentation
formats.
4.1. Regression analysis
Descriptive analyses showed that, for both presentation formats,
problems with a large numerical distance between S and D were
solved considerably faster than those with a small numerical distance
(Mlarge =2362 ms; SD=1208 ms vs. Msmall =4978 ms; SD=2379 ms
for the subtraction format; and Mlarge =2424 ms; SD=1196 ms vs.
Msmall =4774 ms; SD=2567 ms for the addition format). This was
observed in SbD problems as well as in SND problems (Table 1). These
descriptive data suggest that participants did not use the same
strategy on all problem types of the same presentation format, but
potentially switched between subtraction by addition and direct
subtraction depending on the relative size of the subtrahend.
To test this strategy switch hypothesis more formally, we carried
out a stepwise multiple regression analysis based on the work of
Groen and Poll (1973) and Woods et al. (1975), using the stepwise
selection option for regression analysis in the statistical program SAS
(version 9.1), with the criterion for inclusion of a parameter in the
model at p=0.05. We predicted the RTs of the 64 administered
problems by presentation format (coded as subtraction format=1
and addition format=−1). Additional predictors were the size the
subtrahend, the size of the difference, the minimum of D and S, and
the interactions between these predictors and presentation format.
If participants only used direct subtraction, RTs should be best
predicted by the size of the subtrahend because it takes longer to
subtract 84 from a given number than to subtract 3 from that number.
If participants only used subtraction by addition, RTs should be best
predicted by the size of the difference because it takes longer to
determine how much needs to be added to get at a given number
when the difference between both numbers is large (“How much
needs to be added to 3 to have 91?”) than when it is small (“How
much needs to be added to 88 to have 91?”). If participants switched
2
The horizontal format is the standard format for doing subtraction in Flanders,
Belgium, as in many other European countries (e.g., The Netherlands, Germany).
Flemish children learn to subtract in this way in the first two grades of elementary
school before they are introduced to the ‘vertical’ columnar algorithm, which they only
learn after mastering subtractions in the horizontal format in the number domain up
to 100. This differs from North America, where the vertical format is the dominant one.
3
About 5% of all administered trials were repeated at the end of a block (125/2400).
The distribution of incorrect voice key registrations did not differ across problem
types, χ²(3)=2.93, p=0.40. We repeated the analyses without the trials with
incorrect voice key registrations and the results remained generally the same,
indicating no main effect of format [F(1,24)=1.13, p=0.30], a significant format×-
magnitude interaction [F(1,96)=14.02, pb0.01], and a format × magnitude×distance
interaction [F(1,96)=3.92, p=0.05]. Since repeating problems did not affect our
findings, we did not to exclude these trials from the results.
Table 1
Mean reaction times of the 4 problem types in both presentation formats.
S b D S N D
Presentation
format
Large
distance
Small
distance
Large
distance
Small
distance
Subtraction format 2201 ms 4752 ms 2530 ms 5216 ms
Addition format 2735 ms 4612 ms 2120 ms 4942 ms
325
G. Peters et al. / Acta Psychologica 135 (2010) 323–329
between both strategies depending on the most efficient strategy, RTs
should be best predicted by the minimum of subtrahend and
difference: Short RTs are then expected for problems with a small
subtrahend (91−3=.) because they are easily solved by direct
subtraction, and for problems with a small difference (91−88=.)
because they can be easily solved by subtraction by addition. If
participants use different strategies on the two presentation formats,
there should be an interaction between presentation format and (one
of) the other predictors. If their strategy use was similar across both
formats, no such interactions are expected.
The results of the stepwise multiple regression analysis revealed
that the model containing only the minimum of S and D as predictor
provided the best fit to the data (Table 2), F(1,62)=222.65, pb0.01,
R²=0.78. This is in line with our hypothesis that, for both presentation
formats, participants switch between subtraction by addition and
direct subtraction depending on the relative size of the subtrahend.
This regression analysis aggregated performance over the various
participants and may mask performance patterns at the individual level.
We therefore also analysed the RTs for each participant individually. All
but two participants solved all types of large-distance problems faster
than the corresponding small-distance problems (Subjects 22 and 23, see
Fig. 1). We repeated the abovementioned stepwise regression analysis
for each participant individually as well. The model with min(D, S) as the
only predictor provided the best fit of the RTs for all 25 participants,
indicating at the individual level that all participants switched between
direct subtraction and subtraction by addition.
4.2. The effect of presentation format
The mean RT and accuracy per problem type and presentation
format are depicted in Fig. 2. We performed a 2×2 × 2 repeated
measures ANOVA on the RT and accuracy data with magnitude (SbD
vs. SND), numerical distance (small vs. large) and presentation format
(subtraction vs. addition) as within-subject factors. Tukey-Kramer
adjustments were used for post hoc comparisons.
4.2.1. Reaction time
There was a main effect of numerical distance, F(1,24)=122.23,
pb0.01, with large-distance problems being solved 2512 ms faster
than small-distance problems. There were no main effects of format,
F(1,24)=1.03, p=0.32, or magnitude, F(1,24)=2.98, p=0.10. The
magnitude×distance interaction was significant, F(1,96)=11.46,
pb0.01: Small-distance SbD problems were solved 484 ms faster than
small-distance SND problems, t(96)=-3.47, pb0.01, whereas large-
distance S N D problems were solved faster than large-distance S b D
problems, but this latter difference was not significant, t(96)=0.87,
p=0.44. More importantly, a significant format × magnitude interac-
tion was found, F(1,96)=9.29, pb0.01. S N D problems were solved
faster in addition than in subtraction format, t(96)=2.87, pb0.01, while
for the S b D problems the subtraction format tended to elicit shorter
RTs, but this latter effect was not significant, t(96)=-1.44, p=0.15.
Finally, the expected magnitude × distance × format interaction was
significant, F(1,96)=7.02, pb0.01. Post hoc t-tests showed that only for
large-distance subtractions, S b D problems were solved significantly
faster in subtraction than in addition format, t(96)=-3.09, pb0.01, and
S N D problems were solved faster in addition than in subtraction format,
t(96)=2.60, p=0.01. When thedistancebetween S and D wassmall, no
format effect was found.
4.2.2. Accuracy
Accuracy levels were high for all problem types. There was only a
main effect of numerical distance, F(1,24)=23.63, pb0.01, with
large-distance problems (M=0.96) being solved more accurately
than small-distance problems (M=0.87). No significant magnitude ×
distance × format interaction was found, F(1,96)=0.53, p=0.47.
Table 2
Outcome of the stepwise regression analysis in which all 7 predictors were
incorporated.
Predictor Regression weight t Changes
in R²
p
Raw Standardized
min(S, D) 98.65 0.92 14.46 0.72 b0.01
Format* size Subtrahend S 1.79 0.05 0.35 0.73
Format* min(S, D) -3.68 -0.05 -0.54 0.59
Size of difference -5.23 -0.08 -1.03 0.31
Format -317.51 -0.21 -1.16 0.25
Size of subtrahend -8.69 -0.13 -1.71 0.09
Format*size difference D 9.50 0.24 1.87 0.07
The predictors given in italics were not considered in the final model.
Subj.22
Subj.23
0
1000
2000
3000
4000
5000
6000
7000
8000
9000
10000
large
distance
small
distance
small
distance
large
distance
large
distance
small
distance
small
distance
large
distance
S < D S > D S < D S > D
Problem Type
RT
(ms)
Fig. 1. Mean RTs per participant per problem type for the problems presented in addition format (left panel) and subtraction format (right panel). S = subtrahend; D = difference.
326 G. Peters et al. / Acta Psychologica 135 (2010) 323–329
5. Discussion
The present study examined adults’ use of the subtraction by
addition strategy in two-digit subtraction by means of a stepwise
regression analysis and by contrasting performance on problems
presented in subtraction and addition format. Both methods con-
verged to the conclusion that adults solve two-digit subtractions by
switching between subtraction by addition and direct subtraction
depending on the relative size of the subtrahend.
These findings fit with previous research on adults’ use of
subtraction by addition in multi-digit subtraction (e.g., Torbeyns et
al., 2009), wherein verbal reports were the major data source. Because
the validity of such protocols has been questioned (Kirk & Ashcraft,
2001), we started from other methods that have been successfully
applied in research on single-digit subtraction (Campbell, 2008;
Woods et al., 1975) but not in research on multi-digit problems.
Based on Groen and Poll (1973) and Woods et al. (1975), we first
carried out a stepwise multiple regression analysis that predicted RTs by
the problem characteristics: presentation format, size of the difference,
size of the subtrahend, the minimum of difference and subtrahend, and
their respective interactions. The model involving the minimum of
difference and subtrahend, referring to a switch between direct
subtraction and subtraction by addition depending on the most efficient
strategy, emerged as the best predictor of the RT data. The absence of an
interaction between presentation format and the other predictors
indicates that this is thesame for bothpresentation formats. This pattern
was replicated at the individual level, as all participants’ RTs were best
predicted by that model. This all indicates that participants did not apply
the same strategy on all problem types, but switched between direct
subtraction and subtraction by addition. Our findings extend those
obtained by Groen and Poll (1973) and Woods et al. (1975) by showing
that their observations also apply to numbers beyond 10.
We also compared performance on problems presented in
standard subtraction format and in its corresponding addition format.
Our findings revealed that there was an interaction between the
relative size of the subtrahend and the presentation format: When the
numerical distance between the subtrahend and difference was large,
S N D problems were solved faster in addition than in subtraction
format, while S b D problems were solved faster in subtraction than in
addition format. When the numerical distance was small, there were
no format effects. These findings echo those of Peters et al. (2010) for
single-digit subtraction and further indicate that adults switch
between subtraction by addition and direct subtraction to solve
multi-digit subtractions depending on the relative size of subtrahend.
The observation that small-distance problems (e.g., 75−36) were
solved considerably slower and less accurately than large-distance
problems (e.g., 71−2 or 71−69) merits further comment. A first
possible explanation for this finding is that the solution process of the
former problems is less efficient, regardless whether direct subtraction
or subtraction by addition is used. Another possible explanation might
be that the process of strategy selection is more difficult in cases where
the subtrahend and the difference are close to each other. Indeed, it
might be that the strategy selection process on such small-distance
problems takes more time and/or may lead to more errors than on large-
distance problems because it is less clear whether direct subtraction or
subtraction by addition will be the most efficient (Siegler & Shrager,
1984). Further research focusing on both strategy selection and strategy
execution in multi-digit subtraction is needed to address this issue.
At a more general level, the above findings are in accordance with
Siegler's SCADS* model (Siegler & Araya, 2005; see also Verschaffel,
Luwel, Torbeyns, & Van Dooren, 2009), which postulates that when
confronted with cognitive tasks people make adaptive strategy
choices and take into account knowledge about the efficiency of a
particular strategy for a particular problem type. Indeed, the current
findings reveal that adults rely on two number features involved in
the task when choosing between the direct subtraction strategy and
subtraction by addition to solve multi-digit subtractions, namely the
magnitude of the subtrahend and the numerical distance between
subtrahend and difference. These task features seem to involve a
comparison between the numbers in the problem. This comparison
0
10
20
30
40
50
60
70
80
90
100
Subtraction
Format
Addition
Format
Subtraction
Format
Addition
Format
Subtraction
Format
Addition
Format
Subtraction
Format
Addition
Format
large distance small distance small distance large distance
D
>
S
D
<
S
Error
Rate
(%)
0
1000
2000
3000
4000
5000
6000
Reaction
Time
(ms)
Fig. 2. Presentation format × problem type interactions for accuracy (in error rates; bars represented on the left y-axis) and RT (lines depicted on the right y-axis). Error bars depict 1
SE of the mean. S = subtrahend; D = difference.
327
G. Peters et al. / Acta Psychologica 135 (2010) 323–329
stage probably occurs during the orienting or planning phase of the
solution process and relies on fast (quasi-)automatic processes of
estimation and number sense rather than precise calculations of
differences between the numbers. Several studies have highlighted
the importance of magnitude comparison skills (De Smedt, Verschaf-
fel, & Ghesquière, 2009; Holloway, & Ansari, 2009) and number sense
(Gilmore, McCarthy, & Spelke, 2007) for successful mathematical
development. Future research should therefore investigate more
systematically the role of this comparison process in the execution of
strategies in multi-digit subtraction.
It should be noted that multi-digit subtractions can also be solved by
the so-called indirect subtraction strategy, in which people determine
how much needs to be subtracted from the minuend to get to the
subtrahend (e.g., 75−43 by 75−30=45 and 45–2=43; so the
answer is 30+2=32) (De Corte & Verschaffel, 1987). This indirect
subtraction strategy may be particularly efficient on large-distance SND
problems (71−69). However, previous studies on people's strategy use
in subtraction revealed that participants use this strategy only very
rarely or not even at all (Beishuizen,VanPutten, & VanMulken, 1997;De
Smedt et al., 2010; Torbeyns et al., 2009b; Van Lieshout, 1997).
Moreover, our data of the format manipulation indicate that it is very
unlikely that participants applied this strategy. If they had used indirect
subtraction regularly, we should have observed a smaller speed
difference between the two formats; or we should have found at least
a difference in favor of the subtraction format for the large-distance SND
problems because the S+.=M format is not facilitating indirect
subtraction. This was not the case.
After having discussed the main research findings and their theoretical
implications, we now address some limitations of the present study and
some suggestions for further research. A first limitation deals with the
specific set of problem types administered in this study. All the large-
distance problems involved a single-digit number (either the subtrahend
as in 71−2=. or the difference as in 71−69=.) because we wanted to
test the hypothesised format effects for problems where the numerical
distance between subtrahend and difference was as large as possible. If
our participants did not use subtraction by addition on these extreme
large-difference problems, they probably would not use it for problems
with less extreme differences between subtrahend and difference, for
which the subtraction by addition strategy is – compared to the direct
subtraction strategy – clearly less efficient. As a consequence, trials having
a relatively small subtrahend or difference compared to the minuend, i.e.,
a single-digit subtrahend or difference, were eligible to be included in the
stimulus set. However, this limits the generalisability of our findings to
subtraction in the number domain 20-100 with single-digit subtrahends
or differences. It remains to be determined whether our findings can be
applied to the entire domain 20–100 an issue that should be investigated
in future studies. These studies should include large-distance problems in
which the minuend, subtrahend and difference are two-digit numbers
(e.g., 71−15=.) (see Klein et al. (2010) for such a design in their study
about two-digit addition). Furthermore, we cannot exclude that the
exclusive use of these single-digits in the large-distance problems of the
present study might have biased the results, since it may have led to an
increased use of fact retrieval rather than procedural strategies.
A second limitation of our problem set is that we only used carry
problems. Although the effect of the carry operation has been studied for
addition (e.g., Klein et al., 2010), it has not been systematically addressed
in subtraction. It seems plausible that people would use more direct
subtraction for problems without borrowing, such as 84−81 (for
example, transposing the problem into 4−1=3 by subtracting 80 from
bothnumbersintheproblem),butthispossibilityneedstobeinvestigated
in future studies.
A third limitation of the study is the number of presented problems.
Due to time restrictions, we only could present students with 64 sub-
traction problems. To obtain data that would provide the most optimal
comparison of RTs and accuracies on the different types of problems,
we decided to select only 16 problems and present them in the four
different presentation formats (e.g., 31−28=., 31−3=., 28+.=31
and 3+.=31). Since each problem was presented four times, we cannot
exclude the possibility that some participants memorised the answer
to particular problems, which in turn might have affected their solutions
on subsequent items. By assigning the problems in a pseudo-random
manner to six blocks, we tried to reduce these possible memory effects
as much as possible.
Fourth, the present study applied two methods to investigate
participants’ strategic behaviour without relying on verbal protocols for
the first time in the domain of multi-digit arithmetic. As argued before,
verbal protocols may be of questionable validity when quick and quasi-
automatic processes are examined (Kirk & Ashcraft, 2001). This
particularly applies to the subtractions with a very small or very large
difference between the two numbers. Participants may be unable to
accurately report their strategy use on these problems because (a) they
are not aware of how they calculate the answer, (b) they do not find the
appropriate words to articulate their strategy use, or (c) they may even
hide the use of a particular strategy because they think that the
experimenter considers it as an inappropriate or even unacceptable
method. These issues disappear when participants are not asked to report
on their solution process. We used two different methods, which both
converged to the same conclusion and thus enhanced the reliability of the
obtained results. Future research should, however, include other non-
verbal methods, such as eye-movement data. Such data could help to
further triangulate the data about people's strategy use. Eye-movement
data might also provide a unique way to investigate peoples’ orienting or
planning processes that precede the strategy execution phase.
Fifth, the current findings only involved adults. Previous work in
children has shown that they rarely use subtraction by addition
spontaneously in symbolically presented two-digit subtractions (De
Smedt et al., 2010; Dowker, 2009; Torbeyns et al., 2009a,b). Because
these findings are mainly based on verbal protocol methods, future
research should replicate the current study with children to find out
whether the previous findings indicating the scarcity of subtraction by
addition in children remain when non-verbal methods to infer strategy
use are used. Developmental studies are needed to examine the origin
and development of the subtraction by addition strategy, particularly
the point at which children start using this strategy. The task, subject,
and context characteristics that influence this change in children's
strategy choice process should be the focus of investigation as well.
Appendix
Overview of the 64 subtraction problems, categorised by numer-
ical distance between subtrahend(S) and difference(D), magnitude of
the subtrahend, and presentation format.
SbD problems S N D problems
Numerical
distance
Subtraction
format
Addition
format
Subtraction
format
Addition
format
Large distance 31−3=. 3+.=31 31−28=. 28+.=31
34−8=. 8+.=34 34−26=. 26+.=34
42−5=. 5+.=42 42−37=. 37+.=42
52−4=. 4+.=52 52−48=. 48+.=52
71−2=. 2+.=71 71−69=. 69+.=71
77−9=. 9+.=77 77−68=. 68+.=77
83−4=. 4+.=83 83−79=. 79+.=83
93−5=. 5+.=93 93−88=. 88+.=93
Small distance 32−15=. 15+.=32 32−17=. 17+.=32
43−18=. 18+.=43 43−25=. 25+.=43
51−25=. 25+.=51 51−26=. 26+.=51
53−24=. 24+.=53 53−29=. 29+.=53
75−36=. 36+.=75 75−39=. 39+.=75
81−37=. 37+.=81 81−44=. 44+.=81
84−38=. 38+.=84 84−46=. 46+.=84
92−44=. 44+.=92 92−48=. 48+.=92
328 G. Peters et al. / Acta Psychologica 135 (2010) 323–329
References
Ashcraft, M. H. (1982). The development of mental arithmetic: A chronometric
approach. Developmental Review, 2, 213−236.
Ashcraft, M. H. (1992). Cognitive arithmetic: A review of data and theory. Cognition, 44,
75−106.
Barrouillet, P., Mignon, M., & Thevenot, C. (2008). Strategies in subtraction problem
solving in children. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 99, 233−251.
Beishuizen, M. (1997). Development of mathematical strategies and procedures up to
100. In M. Beishuizen, K. P. E. Gravemeijer, & E. C. D. M. van Lieshout (Eds.), The role
of contexts and models in the development of mathematical strategies and procedures
(pp. 127−162). Utrecht, The Netherlands: Beta.
Beishuizen, M., Van Putten, C. M., & Van Mulken, F. (1997). Mental arithmetic and
strategy use with indirect number problems up to one hundred. Learning and
Instruction, 7, 87−106.
Blöte, A. W., Klein, A. S., & Beishuizen, M. (2000). Mental computation and conceptual
understanding. Learning and Instruction, 10, 221−247.
Brissiaud, R. (1994). Teaching and development: Solving "missing addend" problems
using subtraction. European Journal of Psychology of Education, 9, 343−365.
Campbell, J. I. D. (2008). Subtraction by addition. Memory & Cognition, 36, 1094−1102.
Campbell, J. I. D., & Xue, Q. (2001). Cognitive arithmetic across cultures. Journal of
Experimental Psychology: General, 130, 299−315.
De Corte, E., & Verschaffel, L. (1987). The effect of semantic structure on first graders’
strategies for solving addition and subtraction word problems. Journal for Research
in Mathematics Education, 18, 363−381.
De Smedt, B., Torbeyns, J., Stassens, N., Ghesquière, P., & Verschaffel, L. (2010). Frequency,
efficiency and flexibility of indirect addition in two learning environments. Learning
and Instruction, 20, 205−215. doi:10.1016/j.learninstruc.2009.02.020
De Smedt, B., Verschaffel, L., & Ghesquière, P. (2009). The predictive value of numerical
magnitude comparison for individual differences in mathematics achievement.
Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 103, 469−479.
Dowker, A. (2009). Use of derived fact strategies by children with mathematical
difficulties. Cognitive Development, 24, 401−410.
Fuson, K. C. (1992). Research on whole number addition and subtraction. In D. A.
Grouws (Ed.), Handbook of research on mathematical teaching and learning
(pp. 243−275). New York, NY: MacMillan.
Geary, D. C., Frensch, P. A., & Wiley, J. G. (1993). Simple and complex mental
subtraction: Strategy choice and speed-of-processing differences in younger and
older adults. Psychology and Aging, 8, 242−256.
Gilmore, C. K., McCarthy, S. E., & Spelke, E. S. (2007). Symbolic arithmetic knowledge
without instruction. Nature, 447(7144), 589−591.
Groen, G. J., & Poll, M. (1973). Subtraction and the solution of open sentence problems.
Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 16, 292−302.
Holloway, I. D., & Ansari, D. (2009). Mapping numerical magnitudes onto symbols: The
numerical distance effect and individual differences in children's mathematics
achievement. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 103, 17−29.
Kirk, E. P., & Ashcraft, M. H. (2001). Telling stories: The perils and promise of using
verbal reports to study math strategies. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Learning,
Memory, and Cognition, 27, 157−175.
Klein, E., Moeller, K., Dressel, K., Domahs, F., Wood, G., Willmes, K., et al. (2010). To carry
or not to carry—Is this the question? Disentangling the carry effect in multi-digit
addition. Acta Psychologica, 135, 67−76. doi:10.1016/j.actpsy.2010.06.002
LeFevre, J. -A., DeStefano, D., Penner-Wilger, M., & Daley, K. E. (2006). Selection of
procedures in mental subtraction. Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology, 60,
209−220.
Mauro, D. G., LeFevre, J. -A., & Morris, J. (2003). Effects of problem format on division
and multiplication performance: Division facts are mediated via multiplication-
based representations. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Learning, Memory, and
Cognition, 29, 163−170.
Peters, G., De Smedt, B., Torbeyns, J., Ghesquière, P., & Verschaffel, L. (2010). Using
addition to solve large subtractions in the number domain up to 20. Acta
Psychologica, 133, 163−169. doi:10.1016/j.actpsy.2009.10.012
Robinson, K. M. (2001). The validity of verbal reports in children's subtraction. Journal
of Educational Psychology, 93, 211−222.
Selter, C. (2001). Addition and subtraction of three-digit numbers: German elementary
children's success, methods, and strategies. Educational Studies in Mathematics, 47,
145−173.
Seyler, D. J., Kirk, E. P., & Ashcraft, M. H. (2003). Elementary subtraction. Journal of
Experimental Psychology. Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 29, 1339−1352.
Siegler, R. S., & Araya, R. (2005). A computational model of conscious and unconscious
strategy discovery. In R. V. Kail (Ed.), Advances in child development and behaviour.
(pp, 33. (pp. 1−42) Oxford, UK: Elsevier.
Siegler, R. S., & Shrager, J. (1984). Strategy choices in addition and subtraction: how
do children know what to do? In C. Sophian (Ed.), Origins of cognitive skills
(pp. 229−293). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Torbeyns, J., De Smedt, B., Ghesquière, P., & Verschaffel, L. (2009a). Acquisition and use
of shortcut strategies by traditionally schooled children. Educational Studies in
Mathematics, 71, 1−17.
Torbeyns, J., De Smedt, B., Ghesquière, P., & Verschaffel, L. (2009b). Solving subtractions
adaptively by means of indirect addition: Influence of task, subject, and
instructional factors. Mediterranean Journal for Research in Mathematics Education,
8(2), 1−30.
Torbeyns, J., Ghesquière, P., & Verschaffel, L. (2009). Efficiency and flexibility of
indirect addition in the domain of multi-digit subtraction. Learning and Instruction,
19, 1−12.
Van Lieshout, E. C. D. M. (1997). What can research on word and context problems tell
us about effective strategies to solve subtraction problems? In M. Beishuizen, K.
Gravemeijer, & E. C. D. M. Van Lieshout (Eds.), The role of contexts and models in the
development of mathematical strategies and procedures (pp. 79−111). Utrecht: CDβ
Press.
Van Mulken, F. (1992). Hoofdrekenen en strategisch handelen. Het gevarieerd gebruik van
twee grondvormen van optellen en aftrekken tot honderd. [Mental arithmetic and
strategic acting. The varied use of two basic types of addition and subtraction up to
one hundred.] Doctoral thesis, Leiden University.
Verschaffel, L., Greer, B., & De Corte, E. (2007). Whole number concepts and operations.
In F. K. Lester (Ed.), Second handbook of research in mathematics teaching and
learning (pp. 557−628). New York, NY: MacMillan.
Verschaffel, L., Luwel, K., Torbeyns, J., & Van Dooren, W. (2009). Conceptualizing,
investigating, and enhancing adaptive expertise in elementary mathematics
education. European Journal of Psychology of Education, 24, 335−359.
Woods, S. S., Resnick, L. B., & Groen, G. J. (1975). An experimental test of five process
models for subtraction. Journal of Educational Psychology, 67, 17−21.
329
G. Peters et al. / Acta Psychologica 135 (2010) 323–329

More Related Content

Similar to Adults Use Of Subtraction By Addition

Question 1The Uniform Commercial Code incorporates some of the s.docx
Question 1The Uniform Commercial Code incorporates some of the s.docxQuestion 1The Uniform Commercial Code incorporates some of the s.docx
Question 1The Uniform Commercial Code incorporates some of the s.docx
makdul
 
Cesar working document 8 added value
Cesar working document 8 added valueCesar working document 8 added value
Cesar working document 8 added value
Marco
 

Similar to Adults Use Of Subtraction By Addition (20)

A General Analytical Model For Problem Solving Teaching BoS
A General Analytical Model For Problem Solving Teaching  BoSA General Analytical Model For Problem Solving Teaching  BoS
A General Analytical Model For Problem Solving Teaching BoS
 
Technological persuasive pedagogy a new way to persuade students in the compu...
Technological persuasive pedagogy a new way to persuade students in the compu...Technological persuasive pedagogy a new way to persuade students in the compu...
Technological persuasive pedagogy a new way to persuade students in the compu...
 
An ICT Environment To Assess And Support Students Mathematical Problem-Solvi...
An ICT Environment To Assess And Support Students  Mathematical Problem-Solvi...An ICT Environment To Assess And Support Students  Mathematical Problem-Solvi...
An ICT Environment To Assess And Support Students Mathematical Problem-Solvi...
 
Some Methods Used by Mathematics Teachers in Solving Equations
Some Methods Used by Mathematics Teachers in Solving EquationsSome Methods Used by Mathematics Teachers in Solving Equations
Some Methods Used by Mathematics Teachers in Solving Equations
 
Método Topsis - multiple decision makers
Método Topsis  - multiple decision makersMétodo Topsis  - multiple decision makers
Método Topsis - multiple decision makers
 
1. Understanding research and statistics.ppt
1. Understanding research and statistics.ppt1. Understanding research and statistics.ppt
1. Understanding research and statistics.ppt
 
A Developmental Scale Of Mental Computation With Part-Whole Numbers
A Developmental Scale Of Mental Computation With Part-Whole NumbersA Developmental Scale Of Mental Computation With Part-Whole Numbers
A Developmental Scale Of Mental Computation With Part-Whole Numbers
 
A Microgenetic Study Of Simple Division
A Microgenetic Study Of Simple DivisionA Microgenetic Study Of Simple Division
A Microgenetic Study Of Simple Division
 
Application of Graph Theory in an Intelligent Tutoring System for Solving Mat...
Application of Graph Theory in an Intelligent Tutoring System for Solving Mat...Application of Graph Theory in an Intelligent Tutoring System for Solving Mat...
Application of Graph Theory in an Intelligent Tutoring System for Solving Mat...
 
Understand addition through modelling and manipulation of concrete materials
Understand addition through modelling and manipulation of concrete materialsUnderstand addition through modelling and manipulation of concrete materials
Understand addition through modelling and manipulation of concrete materials
 
Question 1The Uniform Commercial Code incorporates some of the s.docx
Question 1The Uniform Commercial Code incorporates some of the s.docxQuestion 1The Uniform Commercial Code incorporates some of the s.docx
Question 1The Uniform Commercial Code incorporates some of the s.docx
 
Data Analysis
Data Analysis Data Analysis
Data Analysis
 
Urban biodiversity and social inequalities in built up cities new evidences, ...
Urban biodiversity and social inequalities in built up cities new evidences, ...Urban biodiversity and social inequalities in built up cities new evidences, ...
Urban biodiversity and social inequalities in built up cities new evidences, ...
 
Math
MathMath
Math
 
Uas efm
Uas efmUas efm
Uas efm
 
Effective feedback through learning analytics dashboards
Effective feedback through learning analytics dashboardsEffective feedback through learning analytics dashboards
Effective feedback through learning analytics dashboards
 
Cesar working document 8 added value
Cesar working document 8 added valueCesar working document 8 added value
Cesar working document 8 added value
 
Place Value
Place ValuePlace Value
Place Value
 
A Case Study of Teaching the Concept of Differential in Mathematics Teacher T...
A Case Study of Teaching the Concept of Differential in Mathematics Teacher T...A Case Study of Teaching the Concept of Differential in Mathematics Teacher T...
A Case Study of Teaching the Concept of Differential in Mathematics Teacher T...
 
Quantitative research paper
Quantitative research paperQuantitative research paper
Quantitative research paper
 

More from Dustin Pytko

More from Dustin Pytko (20)

Critique Response Sample Peer Review Feedback Fo
Critique Response Sample Peer Review Feedback FoCritique Response Sample Peer Review Feedback Fo
Critique Response Sample Peer Review Feedback Fo
 
How To Write Better Essays (12 Best Tips)
How To Write Better Essays (12 Best Tips)How To Write Better Essays (12 Best Tips)
How To Write Better Essays (12 Best Tips)
 
How To Write A 500-Word Essay About - Agnew Text
How To Write A 500-Word Essay About - Agnew TextHow To Write A 500-Word Essay About - Agnew Text
How To Write A 500-Word Essay About - Agnew Text
 
Sample On Project Management By Instant E
Sample On Project Management By Instant ESample On Project Management By Instant E
Sample On Project Management By Instant E
 
Gingerbread Stationary Stationary Printable Free,
Gingerbread Stationary Stationary Printable Free,Gingerbread Stationary Stationary Printable Free,
Gingerbread Stationary Stationary Printable Free,
 
The Creative Spirit Graffiti Challenge 55 Graffiti Art Lett
The Creative Spirit Graffiti Challenge 55 Graffiti Art LettThe Creative Spirit Graffiti Challenge 55 Graffiti Art Lett
The Creative Spirit Graffiti Challenge 55 Graffiti Art Lett
 
My First Day At College - GCSE English - Marked B
My First Day At College - GCSE English - Marked BMy First Day At College - GCSE English - Marked B
My First Day At College - GCSE English - Marked B
 
💋 The Help Movie Analysis Essay. The Help Film Anal.pdf
💋 The Help Movie Analysis Essay. The Help Film Anal.pdf💋 The Help Movie Analysis Essay. The Help Film Anal.pdf
💋 The Help Movie Analysis Essay. The Help Film Anal.pdf
 
Essay Writing Step-By-Step A Newsweek Education Pr
Essay Writing Step-By-Step A Newsweek Education PrEssay Writing Step-By-Step A Newsweek Education Pr
Essay Writing Step-By-Step A Newsweek Education Pr
 
Writing A Dialogue Paper. How To Format Dialogue (Includes Exampl
Writing A Dialogue Paper. How To Format Dialogue (Includes ExamplWriting A Dialogue Paper. How To Format Dialogue (Includes Exampl
Writing A Dialogue Paper. How To Format Dialogue (Includes Exampl
 
Sociology Essay Writing. Online assignment writing service.
Sociology Essay Writing. Online assignment writing service.Sociology Essay Writing. Online assignment writing service.
Sociology Essay Writing. Online assignment writing service.
 
Essay On Importance Of Education In 150 Words. Sh
Essay On Importance Of Education In 150 Words. ShEssay On Importance Of Education In 150 Words. Sh
Essay On Importance Of Education In 150 Words. Sh
 
Types Of Essays We Can Write For You Types Of Essay, E
Types Of Essays We Can Write For You Types Of Essay, ETypes Of Essays We Can Write For You Types Of Essay, E
Types Of Essays We Can Write For You Types Of Essay, E
 
Lined Paper For Writing Notebook Paper Template,
Lined Paper For Writing Notebook Paper Template,Lined Paper For Writing Notebook Paper Template,
Lined Paper For Writing Notebook Paper Template,
 
Research Paper Executive Summary Synopsis Writin
Research Paper Executive Summary Synopsis WritinResearch Paper Executive Summary Synopsis Writin
Research Paper Executive Summary Synopsis Writin
 
Uk Best Essay Service. Order Best Essays In UK
Uk Best Essay Service. Order Best Essays In UKUk Best Essay Service. Order Best Essays In UK
Uk Best Essay Service. Order Best Essays In UK
 
What Is The Body Of A Paragraph. How To Write A Body Paragraph For A
What Is The Body Of A Paragraph. How To Write A Body Paragraph For AWhat Is The Body Of A Paragraph. How To Write A Body Paragraph For A
What Is The Body Of A Paragraph. How To Write A Body Paragraph For A
 
My Handwriting , . Online assignment writing service.
My Handwriting , . Online assignment writing service.My Handwriting , . Online assignment writing service.
My Handwriting , . Online assignment writing service.
 
How To Stay Calm During Exam And Term Paper Writi
How To Stay Calm During Exam And Term Paper WritiHow To Stay Calm During Exam And Term Paper Writi
How To Stay Calm During Exam And Term Paper Writi
 
Image Result For Fundations Letter Formation Page Fundations
Image Result For Fundations Letter Formation Page FundationsImage Result For Fundations Letter Formation Page Fundations
Image Result For Fundations Letter Formation Page Fundations
 

Recently uploaded

plant breeding methods in asexually or clonally propagated crops
plant breeding methods in asexually or clonally propagated cropsplant breeding methods in asexually or clonally propagated crops
plant breeding methods in asexually or clonally propagated crops
parmarsneha2
 
The Roman Empire A Historical Colossus.pdf
The Roman Empire A Historical Colossus.pdfThe Roman Empire A Historical Colossus.pdf
The Roman Empire A Historical Colossus.pdf
kaushalkr1407
 

Recently uploaded (20)

Introduction to Quality Improvement Essentials
Introduction to Quality Improvement EssentialsIntroduction to Quality Improvement Essentials
Introduction to Quality Improvement Essentials
 
Basic_QTL_Marker-assisted_Selection_Sourabh.ppt
Basic_QTL_Marker-assisted_Selection_Sourabh.pptBasic_QTL_Marker-assisted_Selection_Sourabh.ppt
Basic_QTL_Marker-assisted_Selection_Sourabh.ppt
 
aaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaa
aaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaa
aaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaa
 
UNIT – IV_PCI Complaints: Complaints and evaluation of complaints, Handling o...
UNIT – IV_PCI Complaints: Complaints and evaluation of complaints, Handling o...UNIT – IV_PCI Complaints: Complaints and evaluation of complaints, Handling o...
UNIT – IV_PCI Complaints: Complaints and evaluation of complaints, Handling o...
 
The Challenger.pdf DNHS Official Publication
The Challenger.pdf DNHS Official PublicationThe Challenger.pdf DNHS Official Publication
The Challenger.pdf DNHS Official Publication
 
Embracing GenAI - A Strategic Imperative
Embracing GenAI - A Strategic ImperativeEmbracing GenAI - A Strategic Imperative
Embracing GenAI - A Strategic Imperative
 
Home assignment II on Spectroscopy 2024 Answers.pdf
Home assignment II on Spectroscopy 2024 Answers.pdfHome assignment II on Spectroscopy 2024 Answers.pdf
Home assignment II on Spectroscopy 2024 Answers.pdf
 
The approach at University of Liverpool.pptx
The approach at University of Liverpool.pptxThe approach at University of Liverpool.pptx
The approach at University of Liverpool.pptx
 
plant breeding methods in asexually or clonally propagated crops
plant breeding methods in asexually or clonally propagated cropsplant breeding methods in asexually or clonally propagated crops
plant breeding methods in asexually or clonally propagated crops
 
Phrasal Verbs.XXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXX
Phrasal Verbs.XXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXPhrasal Verbs.XXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXX
Phrasal Verbs.XXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXX
 
Template Jadual Bertugas Kelas (Boleh Edit)
Template Jadual Bertugas Kelas (Boleh Edit)Template Jadual Bertugas Kelas (Boleh Edit)
Template Jadual Bertugas Kelas (Boleh Edit)
 
The Art Pastor's Guide to Sabbath | Steve Thomason
The Art Pastor's Guide to Sabbath | Steve ThomasonThe Art Pastor's Guide to Sabbath | Steve Thomason
The Art Pastor's Guide to Sabbath | Steve Thomason
 
Basic phrases for greeting and assisting costumers
Basic phrases for greeting and assisting costumersBasic phrases for greeting and assisting costumers
Basic phrases for greeting and assisting costumers
 
Sectors of the Indian Economy - Class 10 Study Notes pdf
Sectors of the Indian Economy - Class 10 Study Notes pdfSectors of the Indian Economy - Class 10 Study Notes pdf
Sectors of the Indian Economy - Class 10 Study Notes pdf
 
The Roman Empire A Historical Colossus.pdf
The Roman Empire A Historical Colossus.pdfThe Roman Empire A Historical Colossus.pdf
The Roman Empire A Historical Colossus.pdf
 
1.4 modern child centered education - mahatma gandhi-2.pptx
1.4 modern child centered education - mahatma gandhi-2.pptx1.4 modern child centered education - mahatma gandhi-2.pptx
1.4 modern child centered education - mahatma gandhi-2.pptx
 
Students, digital devices and success - Andreas Schleicher - 27 May 2024..pptx
Students, digital devices and success - Andreas Schleicher - 27 May 2024..pptxStudents, digital devices and success - Andreas Schleicher - 27 May 2024..pptx
Students, digital devices and success - Andreas Schleicher - 27 May 2024..pptx
 
Chapter 3 - Islamic Banking Products and Services.pptx
Chapter 3 - Islamic Banking Products and Services.pptxChapter 3 - Islamic Banking Products and Services.pptx
Chapter 3 - Islamic Banking Products and Services.pptx
 
How to Break the cycle of negative Thoughts
How to Break the cycle of negative ThoughtsHow to Break the cycle of negative Thoughts
How to Break the cycle of negative Thoughts
 
MARUTI SUZUKI- A Successful Joint Venture in India.pptx
MARUTI SUZUKI- A Successful Joint Venture in India.pptxMARUTI SUZUKI- A Successful Joint Venture in India.pptx
MARUTI SUZUKI- A Successful Joint Venture in India.pptx
 

Adults Use Of Subtraction By Addition

  • 1. Adults’ use of subtraction by addition Greet Peters a, ⁎, Bert De Smedt b , Joke Torbeyns a,c , Pol Ghesquière b , Lieven Verschaffel a a Centre for Instructional Psychology and Technology, K.U.Leuven, Belgium b Centre for Parenting, Child Welfare, and Disabilities, K.U.Leuven, Belgium c GROEP T – Leuven Education College, Belgium a b s t r a c t a r t i c l e i n f o Article history: Received 17 March 2010 Received in revised form 23 August 2010 Accepted 25 August 2010 Keywords: Multi-digit subtraction Strategy use Subtraction by addition The present study investigates adults’ use of addition to solve two-digit subtractions. Inspired by research on single-digit arithmetic, we first examined regression models in which different problem characteristics predicted participants’ reaction times. Second, we compared performance on two-digit subtractions presented in 2 presentation formats, i.e., the standard subtraction format (81–37=.) and an addition format (37+.=81). Both methods lead to the conclusion that the participants switched between direct subtraction and subtraction by addition depending on the relative size of the subtrahend: If the subtrahend was smaller than the difference, direct subtraction was mainly used; if the subtrahend was larger than the difference, subtraction by addition was the dominant strategy. However, this performance pattern was only observed when the distance between the subtrahend and the difference was large; when the subtrahend and the difference were close to each other, there was no subtrahend-dependent selection of direct subtraction vs. subtraction by addition. These data indicate that theoretical models of people's strategy choices in subtraction should include the relative size of the subtrahend as an important factor in the strategy selection process. © 2010 Elsevier B.V. All rights reserved. 1. Introduction For a long time, researchers have investigated how people solve symbolically presented subtraction problems of the type M−S=. (Barrouillet, Mignon, & Thevenot, 2008; Blöte, Klein, & Beishuizen, 2000; Fuson, 1992; Robinson, 2001; Selter, 2001; Verschaffel, Greer, & De Corte, 2007; Woods, Resnick, & Groen, 1975). The bulk of this research relates to single-digit subtraction, whereas only few studies have looked at people's strategy use on multi-digit subtraction. This is surprising, given the considerable differences between single-digit and multi-digit arithmetic in the procedures and underlying arithmetic concepts being used (Fuson, 1992; Verschaffel et al., 2007). This study therefore aimed at examining people's strategy use in multi-digit subtraction, by applying – for the first time – research methods that have been frequently used in studies on single-digit arithmetic. In the remainder of this introduction, we review the research on strategy use in single-digit and multi-digit subtraction, together with the applied research methods. Woods et al. (1975) investigated how children solved symbolically presented single-digit subtractions of the form M−S=. by fitting their reaction times (RTs) to different linear regression models. They observed that children solved these problems by counting up or down, whichever involved the fewest counting steps (i.e., counting down for 9−2 vs. counting up for 9−7). After a period wherein accuracy and RTs were the main data sources to investigate strategy use (Ashcraft, 1982; Groen & Poll, 1973), verbal protocols became an increasingly important research method (Campbell & Xue, 2001; Geary, Frensch, & Wiley, 1993; LeFevre, DeStefano, Penner-Wilger, & Daley, 2006; Seyler, Kirk, & Ashcraft, 2003). These verbal protocol data showed that adults did not rely exclusively on direct retrieval when solving single- digit subtractions, which was assumed to be the expert way of doing single-digit arithmetic (e.g., Ashcraft, 1992; Siegler & Shrager, 1984). Adults sometimes used procedural strategies, such as counting or decomposing the problem into smaller facts, particularly on the so- called large single-digit subtractions, i.e., with a minuend N10. Most interestingly within the context of the present study is the observation that adults sometimes used addition-based strategies to solve single- digit subtractions (e.g., solving 9−7 by means of the known addition fact 7+2=9, or solving 13−9 by counting or adding up from 9 to 13: 9+1=10 and 10+3=13, so the answer is 1+3=4) (Campbell & Xue, 2001; Geary et al., 1993; LeFevre et al., 2006). However, these addition-based strategies are mostly executed in a very fast and quasi- automated way, which makes them difficult to investigate by means of verbal protocols (Kirk & Ashcraft, 2001). Campbell (2008) recently introduced another method to investigate whether people use addition to solve single-digit subtractions. Based on a design originally developed for multiplication and division by Mauro, LeFevre, and Morris (2003), he presented single-digit subtractions in subtraction (e.g., 12−9=.) and addition format (e.g., 9+.=12). He Acta Psychologica 135 (2010) 323–329 ⁎ Corresponding author. Andreas Vesaliusstraat 2, box 3770, 3000 Leuven, Belgium. Tel.: +32 16 32 62 47; fax: +32 16 32 62 74. E-mail address: Greet.Peters@ped.kuleuven.be (G. Peters). 0001-6918/$ – see front matter © 2010 Elsevier B.V. All rights reserved. doi:10.1016/j.actpsy.2010.08.007 Contents lists available at ScienceDirect Acta Psychologica journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/ locate/actpsy
  • 2. reasoned that if subtractions are solved by addition, the problems presented in addition format should be solved faster than those in subtraction format. This should be so because a subtraction presented in addition format does not require the mental re-representation of the subtraction into an addition,whereassuch a re-representation is needed when the problem is presented in subtraction format. In other words, the addition format might be a better cue for retrieving the addition fact than the subtraction format and/or it might be a facilitator for executing an addition-based procedure. Campbell successfully demonstrated that large single-digit subtractions were solved significantly faster in addition than in subtraction format, which indicated that adults used addition to solve large single-digit subtractions. No such format differences were observed on small single-digit subtractions. Peters, De Smedt, Torbeyns, Ghesquière, and Verschaffel (2010) recently refined Campbell's findings, by showing that this use of subtraction by addition in large single-digit subtractions depends on the relative size of the subtrahend. They suggested that adults solve large single-digit subtractions by switching between subtraction by addition and direct subtraction, taking into account which strategy is most efficient. This mirrors the above-mentioned finding of Woods et al. (1975) about children's flexible switches between counting up or down on small single-digit subtractions. Can these findings on strategy use in single-digit subtraction be generalised to multi-digit subtraction? Within the mathematics education literature, several authors have reported the use of addition-based strategies on multi-digit subtractions (e.g., 71−57 by 57+10=67, 67+4=71; so the answer is 10+4=14), thereby using different terms to denote this strategy, such as the forward strategy (Brissiaud, 1994), solving subtractions by means of addition (Beishuizen, 1997), the short jump strategy (Blöte et al., 2000), the adding up strategy (Selter, 2001), or indirect addition (Torbeyns, Ghesquière, & Verschaffel, 2009). In this article, we will call this strategy subtraction by addition, whereas direct subtraction will be used to refer to the common subtraction strategy where the subtrahend is directly subtracted from the minuend. Torbeyns et al. (2009) investigated adults’ use of subtraction by addition on three-digit subtractions with the choice/no-choice para- digm andsystematically manipulated therelative sizeof thesubtrahend. Accuracy and speed data from the two no-choice conditions (wherein participants had to solve all items first by means of direct subtraction and afterwards via subtraction by addition, or vice versa) indicated that subtraction by addition was a highly efficient strategy for three-digit subtraction, irrespective of the size of the subtrahend. The verbal protocol data from the choice condition indicated that this subtraction by addition strategy was mainly selected on problems with a relatively large subtrahend(e.g., 812−783),whichsuggests that participants took into account the size of the subtrahend when selecting their strategy. The use of subtraction by addition in multi-digit subtraction has also been explored in children, again with verbal protocols. Different from adults, children appeared to hardly apply this strategy spontaneously, not even when they were confronted with problems with a very large subtrahend as in 81−79 (Selter, 2001; Torbeyns, De Smedt, Ghesquière, & Verschaffel, 2009b), when they were explicitly stimulated to generate a strategy other than direct subtraction (Torbeyns, De Smedt, Ghesquière, & Verschaffel, 2009a), or after they received explicit training in subtraction by addition (De Smedt, Torbeyns, Stassens, Ghesquière, & Verschaffel, 2010). We remind that the available studies on subtraction by addition in multi-digit arithmetic relied mainly on verbal protocol data. As for research in single-digit arithmetic, it can be argued that verbal protocols may be less suited to identify the subtraction by addition strategy in multi-digit subtraction because this strategy may be executed very fast and quasi-automatic. For example, when solving a problem such as 81−79, people may be unaware of the calculation steps they executed, or they may have difficulties in articulatingprecisely how they foundthe answer. This may have resulted in an underestimation of the number of subtraction by addition strategies in the available studies on multi-digit subtraction. In this respect, we point out that both the data of Torbeyns et al. (2009) and of De Smedt et al. (2010) suggest that participants sometimes used subtraction by addition while reporting the direct subtraction strategy. If participants only used direct subtraction, an increase in RTs should have been observed from items with small subtrahends over items with medium-sized subtrahends to items with large subtrahends because subtracting a larger subtrahend from a given minuend requires more and/or larger calculation steps. However, both studies showed that not only problems with small but also with large subtrahends were solved faster than problems with medium-sized subtrahends. This indicates that participants did not always use direct subtraction,and it stronglysuggests that the verbal protocol data did not always capture the actually applied strategy. Therefore, it justifies the application of other methods of inferring participants’ strategy use that have been successfully used in single-digit arithmetic, namely the regression-based approach (Groen & Poll, 1973; Woods et al., 1975) and the manipulation of presentation formats (Campbell, 2008; Peters et al., 2010). 2. The present study We used a stepwise regression analysis and the manipulation of presentation formats to investigate the use of subtraction by addition in multi-digit subtraction. We presented adult participants with two- digit subtractions1 in the standard subtraction format (83−4=.) and in its corresponding addition format (4+.=83), manipulating the relative size of the subtrahend. Following Groen and Poll (1973) and Woods et al. (1975), we first calculated a stepwise regression model in which participants' RTs on two-digit subtractions were predicted by the presentation format (M−S=. or S+.=M), three variables referring to strategy use, and their respective interactions. The variables referring to strategy use were the to-be-determined difference (D), the known subtrahend (S), and the minimum of difference and subtrahend (min[D, S]), which reflect the use of subtraction by addition, direct subtraction, and switching between both strategies depending on the size of the subtrahend, respectively. In view of Peters et al. (2010) findings in single-digit subtraction, we expected that a model including the min (D, S) predictor as the variable that explains most of the variability in RTs would provide the best fit, suggesting the mixed use of subtraction by addition and direct subtraction in both presentation formats. We also compared participants’ performance on different problem types in the two presentation formats. We predicted an interaction between thepresentationformat(M−S=. vs. S+.=M), themagnitude of S compared to D (SND vs. SbD), and the numerical distance between S and D (large vs. small). For large-distance problems, we predicted that subtraction by addition would be used to solve SND problems and that the direct subtraction strategy would be used to solve SbD problems. The SND subtractions presented in addition format are thus expected to be solved faster than the corresponding problems in subtraction format, whereas SbD subtractions are expected to be solved faster in subtraction than in addition format, this because the addition format may facilitate the execution of the subtraction by addition strategy more than the subtraction format (and vice versa for direct subtraction). For small-distance problems, we predicted no interaction between the magnitude of S compared to D and presentation format because the computational advantage of one of the two strategies is less clear. 1 ‘Two-digit subtraction’ has different meanings in the research literature. According to some authors, it refers only to the subtraction of a two-digit number from a two- digit number yielding a two-digit result (e.g., 75−36=.). However, it can also be used to refer to problems with a single-digit subtrahend subtracted from a two-digit minuend resulting in a two-digit difference (e.g., 71−2=.) or to refer to problems with a two-digit subtrahend subtracted from a two-digit minuend resulting in a single-digit difference (e.g., 71−69=.), as we did in the current manuscript (see also Brissiaud, 1994; Van Mulken, 1992). 324 G. Peters et al. / Acta Psychologica 135 (2010) 323–329
  • 3. 3. Method 3.1. Participants Participants were 25 university students (5 men, 20 women; Mage =26 years, SDage =7 years) from the Department of Educational Sciences of the Katholieke Universiteit Leuven. All of them had at least a bachelor degree. They had normal or corrected-to-normal vision and none of them reported learning difficulties. 3.2. Materials and procedure Participants were asked to mentally solve 32 subtraction problems, which had a two-digit minuend larger than 30 and required borrowing. Problems were presented in the horizontal subtraction format2 (52−4=.) and in their corresponding addition format (4+.=52), yielding a total set of 64 trials. They were divided into four problem types based on the combination of the magnitude of S compared to D (SND or SbD) and the numerical distancebetweenS and D (small or large). Small- distance problems were defined by S and D differing by less than 10, whereas in the large-distance problems S and D differed by at least 10 and either S or D was a one-digit number. This resulted in the following problem types: (a)large-distance SbD problems, with subtrahends smaller than 10 (e.g., 83−4=. and 8+.=34); (b)large-distance SND problems, with differences smaller than 10 (e.g., 77−68=. and 37+.=42); (c)small-distance SbD problems (e.g., 92−44=. and 36+.=75); and (d)small-distance SND problems (e.g., 32−17=. and 29+.=53) (see Appendix). Threepracticeproblemsperpresentationformatwereadministeredto familiarise participants with the task administration. The experimental items were presented into six blocks, assigned in a pseudo-random manner with the following constraints: (a) items from the same problem typedid notoccuronmorethan twoconsecutive trials; (b)itemsthatonly differed in presentation format (e.g., 31−28=. and 28+.=31) and complementary items (e.g., 31−28=. and 31−3=.) were not presented in the same block. Task instructions stressed both accuracy and speed. Each trial started with a fixation asterisk that appeared for 1000 ms in the centre of a DELL personal computer screen. Next, the item was presented horizontally in the middle of the screen. The characters were 2×2 cm large, black, separated by adjacent spaces and presented against a white background. Time started to run when the item appeared, and ended when the verbal response triggered the sound- activated voice key. The item remained on the screen until the voice key was triggered, with a maximum of 30 s. Participants were instructed to articulate only the final answer and not the intermediate solution steps. A computer connected to the voice key registered RT with millisecond precision. The experimenter then entered the answer via the keyboard. After that, the next trial was initiated. No feedback was given. Trials with incorrect voice key registrations were presented again at the end of each block, to avoid losing too many data3 . We instructed participants on how to answer during the practice trials, asking them to say the numerical answer to the problem as soon as they knew the answer and without any preceding words or phrases, such as “that makes…” or “the outcome is”. Furthermore, we started the experiment by asking participants to name a given set of numbers in order to make sure that the voice key reliably detected the different initial phonemes of potential answers. Participants com- pleted the task in a quiet room. They solved the task individually on a computer in the presence of an experimenter. 4. Results A total of 1600 items was solved. Seventeen items were excluded because of voice key errors (1.06%). For RT analysis, 131 items were deleted because of incorrect answers (8.49%) and 39 items (2.44%) were additionally discarded as outliers (i.e., more than 2 standard deviations from a participant's cell mean for the correct items per problem type). Results are presented in two parts. The first part involves the evaluation of the regression models that predict participants’ RTs. In the second part, we compare performance on both presentation formats. 4.1. Regression analysis Descriptive analyses showed that, for both presentation formats, problems with a large numerical distance between S and D were solved considerably faster than those with a small numerical distance (Mlarge =2362 ms; SD=1208 ms vs. Msmall =4978 ms; SD=2379 ms for the subtraction format; and Mlarge =2424 ms; SD=1196 ms vs. Msmall =4774 ms; SD=2567 ms for the addition format). This was observed in SbD problems as well as in SND problems (Table 1). These descriptive data suggest that participants did not use the same strategy on all problem types of the same presentation format, but potentially switched between subtraction by addition and direct subtraction depending on the relative size of the subtrahend. To test this strategy switch hypothesis more formally, we carried out a stepwise multiple regression analysis based on the work of Groen and Poll (1973) and Woods et al. (1975), using the stepwise selection option for regression analysis in the statistical program SAS (version 9.1), with the criterion for inclusion of a parameter in the model at p=0.05. We predicted the RTs of the 64 administered problems by presentation format (coded as subtraction format=1 and addition format=−1). Additional predictors were the size the subtrahend, the size of the difference, the minimum of D and S, and the interactions between these predictors and presentation format. If participants only used direct subtraction, RTs should be best predicted by the size of the subtrahend because it takes longer to subtract 84 from a given number than to subtract 3 from that number. If participants only used subtraction by addition, RTs should be best predicted by the size of the difference because it takes longer to determine how much needs to be added to get at a given number when the difference between both numbers is large (“How much needs to be added to 3 to have 91?”) than when it is small (“How much needs to be added to 88 to have 91?”). If participants switched 2 The horizontal format is the standard format for doing subtraction in Flanders, Belgium, as in many other European countries (e.g., The Netherlands, Germany). Flemish children learn to subtract in this way in the first two grades of elementary school before they are introduced to the ‘vertical’ columnar algorithm, which they only learn after mastering subtractions in the horizontal format in the number domain up to 100. This differs from North America, where the vertical format is the dominant one. 3 About 5% of all administered trials were repeated at the end of a block (125/2400). The distribution of incorrect voice key registrations did not differ across problem types, χ²(3)=2.93, p=0.40. We repeated the analyses without the trials with incorrect voice key registrations and the results remained generally the same, indicating no main effect of format [F(1,24)=1.13, p=0.30], a significant format×- magnitude interaction [F(1,96)=14.02, pb0.01], and a format × magnitude×distance interaction [F(1,96)=3.92, p=0.05]. Since repeating problems did not affect our findings, we did not to exclude these trials from the results. Table 1 Mean reaction times of the 4 problem types in both presentation formats. S b D S N D Presentation format Large distance Small distance Large distance Small distance Subtraction format 2201 ms 4752 ms 2530 ms 5216 ms Addition format 2735 ms 4612 ms 2120 ms 4942 ms 325 G. Peters et al. / Acta Psychologica 135 (2010) 323–329
  • 4. between both strategies depending on the most efficient strategy, RTs should be best predicted by the minimum of subtrahend and difference: Short RTs are then expected for problems with a small subtrahend (91−3=.) because they are easily solved by direct subtraction, and for problems with a small difference (91−88=.) because they can be easily solved by subtraction by addition. If participants use different strategies on the two presentation formats, there should be an interaction between presentation format and (one of) the other predictors. If their strategy use was similar across both formats, no such interactions are expected. The results of the stepwise multiple regression analysis revealed that the model containing only the minimum of S and D as predictor provided the best fit to the data (Table 2), F(1,62)=222.65, pb0.01, R²=0.78. This is in line with our hypothesis that, for both presentation formats, participants switch between subtraction by addition and direct subtraction depending on the relative size of the subtrahend. This regression analysis aggregated performance over the various participants and may mask performance patterns at the individual level. We therefore also analysed the RTs for each participant individually. All but two participants solved all types of large-distance problems faster than the corresponding small-distance problems (Subjects 22 and 23, see Fig. 1). We repeated the abovementioned stepwise regression analysis for each participant individually as well. The model with min(D, S) as the only predictor provided the best fit of the RTs for all 25 participants, indicating at the individual level that all participants switched between direct subtraction and subtraction by addition. 4.2. The effect of presentation format The mean RT and accuracy per problem type and presentation format are depicted in Fig. 2. We performed a 2×2 × 2 repeated measures ANOVA on the RT and accuracy data with magnitude (SbD vs. SND), numerical distance (small vs. large) and presentation format (subtraction vs. addition) as within-subject factors. Tukey-Kramer adjustments were used for post hoc comparisons. 4.2.1. Reaction time There was a main effect of numerical distance, F(1,24)=122.23, pb0.01, with large-distance problems being solved 2512 ms faster than small-distance problems. There were no main effects of format, F(1,24)=1.03, p=0.32, or magnitude, F(1,24)=2.98, p=0.10. The magnitude×distance interaction was significant, F(1,96)=11.46, pb0.01: Small-distance SbD problems were solved 484 ms faster than small-distance SND problems, t(96)=-3.47, pb0.01, whereas large- distance S N D problems were solved faster than large-distance S b D problems, but this latter difference was not significant, t(96)=0.87, p=0.44. More importantly, a significant format × magnitude interac- tion was found, F(1,96)=9.29, pb0.01. S N D problems were solved faster in addition than in subtraction format, t(96)=2.87, pb0.01, while for the S b D problems the subtraction format tended to elicit shorter RTs, but this latter effect was not significant, t(96)=-1.44, p=0.15. Finally, the expected magnitude × distance × format interaction was significant, F(1,96)=7.02, pb0.01. Post hoc t-tests showed that only for large-distance subtractions, S b D problems were solved significantly faster in subtraction than in addition format, t(96)=-3.09, pb0.01, and S N D problems were solved faster in addition than in subtraction format, t(96)=2.60, p=0.01. When thedistancebetween S and D wassmall, no format effect was found. 4.2.2. Accuracy Accuracy levels were high for all problem types. There was only a main effect of numerical distance, F(1,24)=23.63, pb0.01, with large-distance problems (M=0.96) being solved more accurately than small-distance problems (M=0.87). No significant magnitude × distance × format interaction was found, F(1,96)=0.53, p=0.47. Table 2 Outcome of the stepwise regression analysis in which all 7 predictors were incorporated. Predictor Regression weight t Changes in R² p Raw Standardized min(S, D) 98.65 0.92 14.46 0.72 b0.01 Format* size Subtrahend S 1.79 0.05 0.35 0.73 Format* min(S, D) -3.68 -0.05 -0.54 0.59 Size of difference -5.23 -0.08 -1.03 0.31 Format -317.51 -0.21 -1.16 0.25 Size of subtrahend -8.69 -0.13 -1.71 0.09 Format*size difference D 9.50 0.24 1.87 0.07 The predictors given in italics were not considered in the final model. Subj.22 Subj.23 0 1000 2000 3000 4000 5000 6000 7000 8000 9000 10000 large distance small distance small distance large distance large distance small distance small distance large distance S < D S > D S < D S > D Problem Type RT (ms) Fig. 1. Mean RTs per participant per problem type for the problems presented in addition format (left panel) and subtraction format (right panel). S = subtrahend; D = difference. 326 G. Peters et al. / Acta Psychologica 135 (2010) 323–329
  • 5. 5. Discussion The present study examined adults’ use of the subtraction by addition strategy in two-digit subtraction by means of a stepwise regression analysis and by contrasting performance on problems presented in subtraction and addition format. Both methods con- verged to the conclusion that adults solve two-digit subtractions by switching between subtraction by addition and direct subtraction depending on the relative size of the subtrahend. These findings fit with previous research on adults’ use of subtraction by addition in multi-digit subtraction (e.g., Torbeyns et al., 2009), wherein verbal reports were the major data source. Because the validity of such protocols has been questioned (Kirk & Ashcraft, 2001), we started from other methods that have been successfully applied in research on single-digit subtraction (Campbell, 2008; Woods et al., 1975) but not in research on multi-digit problems. Based on Groen and Poll (1973) and Woods et al. (1975), we first carried out a stepwise multiple regression analysis that predicted RTs by the problem characteristics: presentation format, size of the difference, size of the subtrahend, the minimum of difference and subtrahend, and their respective interactions. The model involving the minimum of difference and subtrahend, referring to a switch between direct subtraction and subtraction by addition depending on the most efficient strategy, emerged as the best predictor of the RT data. The absence of an interaction between presentation format and the other predictors indicates that this is thesame for bothpresentation formats. This pattern was replicated at the individual level, as all participants’ RTs were best predicted by that model. This all indicates that participants did not apply the same strategy on all problem types, but switched between direct subtraction and subtraction by addition. Our findings extend those obtained by Groen and Poll (1973) and Woods et al. (1975) by showing that their observations also apply to numbers beyond 10. We also compared performance on problems presented in standard subtraction format and in its corresponding addition format. Our findings revealed that there was an interaction between the relative size of the subtrahend and the presentation format: When the numerical distance between the subtrahend and difference was large, S N D problems were solved faster in addition than in subtraction format, while S b D problems were solved faster in subtraction than in addition format. When the numerical distance was small, there were no format effects. These findings echo those of Peters et al. (2010) for single-digit subtraction and further indicate that adults switch between subtraction by addition and direct subtraction to solve multi-digit subtractions depending on the relative size of subtrahend. The observation that small-distance problems (e.g., 75−36) were solved considerably slower and less accurately than large-distance problems (e.g., 71−2 or 71−69) merits further comment. A first possible explanation for this finding is that the solution process of the former problems is less efficient, regardless whether direct subtraction or subtraction by addition is used. Another possible explanation might be that the process of strategy selection is more difficult in cases where the subtrahend and the difference are close to each other. Indeed, it might be that the strategy selection process on such small-distance problems takes more time and/or may lead to more errors than on large- distance problems because it is less clear whether direct subtraction or subtraction by addition will be the most efficient (Siegler & Shrager, 1984). Further research focusing on both strategy selection and strategy execution in multi-digit subtraction is needed to address this issue. At a more general level, the above findings are in accordance with Siegler's SCADS* model (Siegler & Araya, 2005; see also Verschaffel, Luwel, Torbeyns, & Van Dooren, 2009), which postulates that when confronted with cognitive tasks people make adaptive strategy choices and take into account knowledge about the efficiency of a particular strategy for a particular problem type. Indeed, the current findings reveal that adults rely on two number features involved in the task when choosing between the direct subtraction strategy and subtraction by addition to solve multi-digit subtractions, namely the magnitude of the subtrahend and the numerical distance between subtrahend and difference. These task features seem to involve a comparison between the numbers in the problem. This comparison 0 10 20 30 40 50 60 70 80 90 100 Subtraction Format Addition Format Subtraction Format Addition Format Subtraction Format Addition Format Subtraction Format Addition Format large distance small distance small distance large distance D > S D < S Error Rate (%) 0 1000 2000 3000 4000 5000 6000 Reaction Time (ms) Fig. 2. Presentation format × problem type interactions for accuracy (in error rates; bars represented on the left y-axis) and RT (lines depicted on the right y-axis). Error bars depict 1 SE of the mean. S = subtrahend; D = difference. 327 G. Peters et al. / Acta Psychologica 135 (2010) 323–329
  • 6. stage probably occurs during the orienting or planning phase of the solution process and relies on fast (quasi-)automatic processes of estimation and number sense rather than precise calculations of differences between the numbers. Several studies have highlighted the importance of magnitude comparison skills (De Smedt, Verschaf- fel, & Ghesquière, 2009; Holloway, & Ansari, 2009) and number sense (Gilmore, McCarthy, & Spelke, 2007) for successful mathematical development. Future research should therefore investigate more systematically the role of this comparison process in the execution of strategies in multi-digit subtraction. It should be noted that multi-digit subtractions can also be solved by the so-called indirect subtraction strategy, in which people determine how much needs to be subtracted from the minuend to get to the subtrahend (e.g., 75−43 by 75−30=45 and 45–2=43; so the answer is 30+2=32) (De Corte & Verschaffel, 1987). This indirect subtraction strategy may be particularly efficient on large-distance SND problems (71−69). However, previous studies on people's strategy use in subtraction revealed that participants use this strategy only very rarely or not even at all (Beishuizen,VanPutten, & VanMulken, 1997;De Smedt et al., 2010; Torbeyns et al., 2009b; Van Lieshout, 1997). Moreover, our data of the format manipulation indicate that it is very unlikely that participants applied this strategy. If they had used indirect subtraction regularly, we should have observed a smaller speed difference between the two formats; or we should have found at least a difference in favor of the subtraction format for the large-distance SND problems because the S+.=M format is not facilitating indirect subtraction. This was not the case. After having discussed the main research findings and their theoretical implications, we now address some limitations of the present study and some suggestions for further research. A first limitation deals with the specific set of problem types administered in this study. All the large- distance problems involved a single-digit number (either the subtrahend as in 71−2=. or the difference as in 71−69=.) because we wanted to test the hypothesised format effects for problems where the numerical distance between subtrahend and difference was as large as possible. If our participants did not use subtraction by addition on these extreme large-difference problems, they probably would not use it for problems with less extreme differences between subtrahend and difference, for which the subtraction by addition strategy is – compared to the direct subtraction strategy – clearly less efficient. As a consequence, trials having a relatively small subtrahend or difference compared to the minuend, i.e., a single-digit subtrahend or difference, were eligible to be included in the stimulus set. However, this limits the generalisability of our findings to subtraction in the number domain 20-100 with single-digit subtrahends or differences. It remains to be determined whether our findings can be applied to the entire domain 20–100 an issue that should be investigated in future studies. These studies should include large-distance problems in which the minuend, subtrahend and difference are two-digit numbers (e.g., 71−15=.) (see Klein et al. (2010) for such a design in their study about two-digit addition). Furthermore, we cannot exclude that the exclusive use of these single-digits in the large-distance problems of the present study might have biased the results, since it may have led to an increased use of fact retrieval rather than procedural strategies. A second limitation of our problem set is that we only used carry problems. Although the effect of the carry operation has been studied for addition (e.g., Klein et al., 2010), it has not been systematically addressed in subtraction. It seems plausible that people would use more direct subtraction for problems without borrowing, such as 84−81 (for example, transposing the problem into 4−1=3 by subtracting 80 from bothnumbersintheproblem),butthispossibilityneedstobeinvestigated in future studies. A third limitation of the study is the number of presented problems. Due to time restrictions, we only could present students with 64 sub- traction problems. To obtain data that would provide the most optimal comparison of RTs and accuracies on the different types of problems, we decided to select only 16 problems and present them in the four different presentation formats (e.g., 31−28=., 31−3=., 28+.=31 and 3+.=31). Since each problem was presented four times, we cannot exclude the possibility that some participants memorised the answer to particular problems, which in turn might have affected their solutions on subsequent items. By assigning the problems in a pseudo-random manner to six blocks, we tried to reduce these possible memory effects as much as possible. Fourth, the present study applied two methods to investigate participants’ strategic behaviour without relying on verbal protocols for the first time in the domain of multi-digit arithmetic. As argued before, verbal protocols may be of questionable validity when quick and quasi- automatic processes are examined (Kirk & Ashcraft, 2001). This particularly applies to the subtractions with a very small or very large difference between the two numbers. Participants may be unable to accurately report their strategy use on these problems because (a) they are not aware of how they calculate the answer, (b) they do not find the appropriate words to articulate their strategy use, or (c) they may even hide the use of a particular strategy because they think that the experimenter considers it as an inappropriate or even unacceptable method. These issues disappear when participants are not asked to report on their solution process. We used two different methods, which both converged to the same conclusion and thus enhanced the reliability of the obtained results. Future research should, however, include other non- verbal methods, such as eye-movement data. Such data could help to further triangulate the data about people's strategy use. Eye-movement data might also provide a unique way to investigate peoples’ orienting or planning processes that precede the strategy execution phase. Fifth, the current findings only involved adults. Previous work in children has shown that they rarely use subtraction by addition spontaneously in symbolically presented two-digit subtractions (De Smedt et al., 2010; Dowker, 2009; Torbeyns et al., 2009a,b). Because these findings are mainly based on verbal protocol methods, future research should replicate the current study with children to find out whether the previous findings indicating the scarcity of subtraction by addition in children remain when non-verbal methods to infer strategy use are used. Developmental studies are needed to examine the origin and development of the subtraction by addition strategy, particularly the point at which children start using this strategy. The task, subject, and context characteristics that influence this change in children's strategy choice process should be the focus of investigation as well. Appendix Overview of the 64 subtraction problems, categorised by numer- ical distance between subtrahend(S) and difference(D), magnitude of the subtrahend, and presentation format. SbD problems S N D problems Numerical distance Subtraction format Addition format Subtraction format Addition format Large distance 31−3=. 3+.=31 31−28=. 28+.=31 34−8=. 8+.=34 34−26=. 26+.=34 42−5=. 5+.=42 42−37=. 37+.=42 52−4=. 4+.=52 52−48=. 48+.=52 71−2=. 2+.=71 71−69=. 69+.=71 77−9=. 9+.=77 77−68=. 68+.=77 83−4=. 4+.=83 83−79=. 79+.=83 93−5=. 5+.=93 93−88=. 88+.=93 Small distance 32−15=. 15+.=32 32−17=. 17+.=32 43−18=. 18+.=43 43−25=. 25+.=43 51−25=. 25+.=51 51−26=. 26+.=51 53−24=. 24+.=53 53−29=. 29+.=53 75−36=. 36+.=75 75−39=. 39+.=75 81−37=. 37+.=81 81−44=. 44+.=81 84−38=. 38+.=84 84−46=. 46+.=84 92−44=. 44+.=92 92−48=. 48+.=92 328 G. Peters et al. / Acta Psychologica 135 (2010) 323–329
  • 7. References Ashcraft, M. H. (1982). The development of mental arithmetic: A chronometric approach. Developmental Review, 2, 213−236. Ashcraft, M. H. (1992). Cognitive arithmetic: A review of data and theory. Cognition, 44, 75−106. Barrouillet, P., Mignon, M., & Thevenot, C. (2008). Strategies in subtraction problem solving in children. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 99, 233−251. Beishuizen, M. (1997). Development of mathematical strategies and procedures up to 100. In M. Beishuizen, K. P. E. Gravemeijer, & E. C. D. M. van Lieshout (Eds.), The role of contexts and models in the development of mathematical strategies and procedures (pp. 127−162). Utrecht, The Netherlands: Beta. Beishuizen, M., Van Putten, C. M., & Van Mulken, F. (1997). Mental arithmetic and strategy use with indirect number problems up to one hundred. Learning and Instruction, 7, 87−106. Blöte, A. W., Klein, A. S., & Beishuizen, M. (2000). Mental computation and conceptual understanding. Learning and Instruction, 10, 221−247. Brissiaud, R. (1994). Teaching and development: Solving "missing addend" problems using subtraction. European Journal of Psychology of Education, 9, 343−365. Campbell, J. I. D. (2008). Subtraction by addition. Memory & Cognition, 36, 1094−1102. Campbell, J. I. D., & Xue, Q. (2001). Cognitive arithmetic across cultures. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 130, 299−315. De Corte, E., & Verschaffel, L. (1987). The effect of semantic structure on first graders’ strategies for solving addition and subtraction word problems. Journal for Research in Mathematics Education, 18, 363−381. De Smedt, B., Torbeyns, J., Stassens, N., Ghesquière, P., & Verschaffel, L. (2010). Frequency, efficiency and flexibility of indirect addition in two learning environments. Learning and Instruction, 20, 205−215. doi:10.1016/j.learninstruc.2009.02.020 De Smedt, B., Verschaffel, L., & Ghesquière, P. (2009). The predictive value of numerical magnitude comparison for individual differences in mathematics achievement. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 103, 469−479. Dowker, A. (2009). Use of derived fact strategies by children with mathematical difficulties. Cognitive Development, 24, 401−410. Fuson, K. C. (1992). Research on whole number addition and subtraction. In D. A. Grouws (Ed.), Handbook of research on mathematical teaching and learning (pp. 243−275). New York, NY: MacMillan. Geary, D. C., Frensch, P. A., & Wiley, J. G. (1993). Simple and complex mental subtraction: Strategy choice and speed-of-processing differences in younger and older adults. Psychology and Aging, 8, 242−256. Gilmore, C. K., McCarthy, S. E., & Spelke, E. S. (2007). Symbolic arithmetic knowledge without instruction. Nature, 447(7144), 589−591. Groen, G. J., & Poll, M. (1973). Subtraction and the solution of open sentence problems. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 16, 292−302. Holloway, I. D., & Ansari, D. (2009). Mapping numerical magnitudes onto symbols: The numerical distance effect and individual differences in children's mathematics achievement. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 103, 17−29. Kirk, E. P., & Ashcraft, M. H. (2001). Telling stories: The perils and promise of using verbal reports to study math strategies. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 27, 157−175. Klein, E., Moeller, K., Dressel, K., Domahs, F., Wood, G., Willmes, K., et al. (2010). To carry or not to carry—Is this the question? Disentangling the carry effect in multi-digit addition. Acta Psychologica, 135, 67−76. doi:10.1016/j.actpsy.2010.06.002 LeFevre, J. -A., DeStefano, D., Penner-Wilger, M., & Daley, K. E. (2006). Selection of procedures in mental subtraction. Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology, 60, 209−220. Mauro, D. G., LeFevre, J. -A., & Morris, J. (2003). Effects of problem format on division and multiplication performance: Division facts are mediated via multiplication- based representations. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 29, 163−170. Peters, G., De Smedt, B., Torbeyns, J., Ghesquière, P., & Verschaffel, L. (2010). Using addition to solve large subtractions in the number domain up to 20. Acta Psychologica, 133, 163−169. doi:10.1016/j.actpsy.2009.10.012 Robinson, K. M. (2001). The validity of verbal reports in children's subtraction. Journal of Educational Psychology, 93, 211−222. Selter, C. (2001). Addition and subtraction of three-digit numbers: German elementary children's success, methods, and strategies. Educational Studies in Mathematics, 47, 145−173. Seyler, D. J., Kirk, E. P., & Ashcraft, M. H. (2003). Elementary subtraction. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 29, 1339−1352. Siegler, R. S., & Araya, R. (2005). A computational model of conscious and unconscious strategy discovery. In R. V. Kail (Ed.), Advances in child development and behaviour. (pp, 33. (pp. 1−42) Oxford, UK: Elsevier. Siegler, R. S., & Shrager, J. (1984). Strategy choices in addition and subtraction: how do children know what to do? In C. Sophian (Ed.), Origins of cognitive skills (pp. 229−293). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. Torbeyns, J., De Smedt, B., Ghesquière, P., & Verschaffel, L. (2009a). Acquisition and use of shortcut strategies by traditionally schooled children. Educational Studies in Mathematics, 71, 1−17. Torbeyns, J., De Smedt, B., Ghesquière, P., & Verschaffel, L. (2009b). Solving subtractions adaptively by means of indirect addition: Influence of task, subject, and instructional factors. Mediterranean Journal for Research in Mathematics Education, 8(2), 1−30. Torbeyns, J., Ghesquière, P., & Verschaffel, L. (2009). Efficiency and flexibility of indirect addition in the domain of multi-digit subtraction. Learning and Instruction, 19, 1−12. Van Lieshout, E. C. D. M. (1997). What can research on word and context problems tell us about effective strategies to solve subtraction problems? In M. Beishuizen, K. Gravemeijer, & E. C. D. M. Van Lieshout (Eds.), The role of contexts and models in the development of mathematical strategies and procedures (pp. 79−111). Utrecht: CDβ Press. Van Mulken, F. (1992). Hoofdrekenen en strategisch handelen. Het gevarieerd gebruik van twee grondvormen van optellen en aftrekken tot honderd. [Mental arithmetic and strategic acting. The varied use of two basic types of addition and subtraction up to one hundred.] Doctoral thesis, Leiden University. Verschaffel, L., Greer, B., & De Corte, E. (2007). Whole number concepts and operations. In F. K. Lester (Ed.), Second handbook of research in mathematics teaching and learning (pp. 557−628). New York, NY: MacMillan. Verschaffel, L., Luwel, K., Torbeyns, J., & Van Dooren, W. (2009). Conceptualizing, investigating, and enhancing adaptive expertise in elementary mathematics education. European Journal of Psychology of Education, 24, 335−359. Woods, S. S., Resnick, L. B., & Groen, G. J. (1975). An experimental test of five process models for subtraction. Journal of Educational Psychology, 67, 17−21. 329 G. Peters et al. / Acta Psychologica 135 (2010) 323–329