SlideShare a Scribd company logo
1 of 22
D
ow
nloaded
from
http://journals.lw
w
.com
/nsca-jscr
by
B
hD
M
f5eP
H
K
av1zE
oum
1tQ
fN
4a+kJLhE
ZgbsIH
o4X
M
i0hC
yw
C
X
1A
W
nY
Q
p/IlQ
rH
D
3i3D
0O
dR
yi7TvS
Fl4C
f3V
C
4/O
A
V
pD
D
a8K
K
G
K
V
0Y
m
y+78=
on
05/21/2021
Downloadedfromhttp://journals.lww.com/nsca-
jscrbyBhDMf5ePHKav1zEoum1tQfN4a+kJLhEZgbsIHo4XMi0h
CywCX1AWnYQp/IlQrHD3i3D0OdRyi7TvSFl4Cf3VC4/OAVp
DDa8KKGKV0Ymy+78=on05/21/2021
Journal of Strength and Conditioning Research, 2006, 20(1),
145–150
q 2006 National Strength & Conditioning Association
THE EFFECT OF THE DIRECTION OF GAZE ON THE
KINEMATICS OF THE SQUAT EXERCISE
DAVID V. DONNELLY,1 WILLIAM P. BERG,2 AND
DARRYN M. FISKE3
1Department of Intercollegiate Athletics, Miami University,
Oxford, Ohio 45056; 2Department of Physical
Education, Health and Sport Studies, Miami University, Oxford,
Ohio 45056; 3Department of Athletics, St.
Bonaventure University, St. Bonaventure, New York 14778.
ABSTRACT. Donnelly, D., W.P. Berg, and D. Fiske. The effect
of
the direction of gaze on the kinematics of the squat exercise. J.
Strength Cond. Res. 20(1):145–150. 2006.—The purpose of this
study was to determine whether the direction of gaze influences
the kinematics of the squat exercise. Ten men experienced in
the squat exercise performed a total of 30 repetitions of the
squat in the form of 2 sets of 5 repetitions under 3 different
conditions. Conditions varied with respect to the direction of
the
subjects’ gaze as they performed the exercise. Condition D en-
tailed gazing downward at the intersection of the facing wall
and
the floor throughout the exercise. Condition S required subjects
to gaze straight ahead at their own reflection (eyes) in the
mirror
on the wall directly in front of them. Condition U involved gaz-
ing upward at the intersection of the facing wall and the ceiling
throughout the exercise. Dependent variables included the lin-
ear displacement of the bar and hip, linear velocity of the bar,
and the angular displacement/position and velocity of the head,
trunk, hip, and knee. The mean data were subjected to a re-
peated measures analysis of variance, and, where appropriate,
pairwise comparisons using Tukey’s Studentized Range Test.
The results revealed overall similarity in movement kinematics
when performing the squat exercise using the 3 different gaze
directions. In particular, the upward and straight gaze condi -
tions were not differentiated by the analysis. Conversely, the
downward gaze was shown to increase the extent of hip flexion
(F[2, 9] 5 4.82, p , .05), especially relative to the upward gaze,
and possibly trunk flexion as well (F[2, 9] 5 3.02, p 5 .07). In
terms of the practical application, because excessive hip and
trunk flexion in the squat are contraindicated, cautioning ath-
letes against allowing the head or direction of gaze to drop
below
a neutral position appears to be warranted.
KEY WORDS. weight training, vision, stability
INTRODUCTION
T
he squat exercise involves most major muscle
groups of the trunk and legs, and is one of the
most beneficial muscle strengthening exercises.
The squat not only enhances performance in
many sports, but has become an important component of
general-purpose weight training programs as well. Be-
cause of the number of muscle groups involved, squats
also can positively stimulate the cardiovascular system
(8, 9, 10). Moreover, when performed correctly, the squat
can help prevent injuries (2, 6).
To obtain the desired training effects and to ensure
safety, proper technique should be used when performing
the squat. There is general agreement about what con-
stitutes appropriate squat technique, with at least 1 ex-
ception. There is some lack of clarity about how the head
should be positioned during the squat, or more precisely,
where a performer’s gaze should be directed—upward,
downward, or straight ahead? According to Fry, using an
incorrect head position can cause poor body alignment
and predispose an athlete to injury (5). What is the cor-
rect head position and/or direction of gaze in the squat?
According to Francoeur, lifters should focus on a spot
on the wall or ceiling, as this will help to keep the head
up throughout the entire motion (4). Global Health and
Fitness also recommends looking upward throughout the
exercise (7), whereas O’Shea simply recommends keeping
the head up (16). Westcott (17) and other professionals
instruct exercisers to keep the head neutral throughout
the exercise. Indeed, Kelso et al. advise that the head
should face straight forward throughout the exercise (12).
Some recommendations are more specific, such as that
the head and neck should be kept straight with the eyes
looking straight ahead (3, 5, 11,).
Some professionals warn against allowing the head
and eye focus to drop from a neutral position, arguing
that this could result in excessive forward lean as the
lifter descends (3). Forward lean is a nontechnical de-
scriptor of what we will refer to as trunk flexion. Trunk
flexion is the forward deviation of the trunk relative to
the vertical and can result from hip and knee flexion, as
well as ankle dorsiflexion. In short, a head-down position
during the squat exercise may increase the likelihood of
excessive trunk flexion. Several authors have suggested
that excessive trunk flexion during the squat can cause
undue stress to be applied to the lumbar spine, and thus
increase the chance of injury (3, 5, 14, 15).
On the contrary, Hill warns against using a head-up,
eyes-up technique, arguing that balance is compromised,
increasingly so during descent, making lower back strain
more likely (3). Instead, Hill argues that the head and
eyes should be straight or slightly down throughout the
exercise. The latter recommendation is the only one, to
our knowledge, suggesting that a downwardly directed
gaze or head position is acceptable. Nonetheless, in spite
of the general bias among experts against looking down-
ward during the squat, athletes and weight trainers
sometimes can be observed doing so.
Adding to the uncertainty about appropriate head po-
sition and direction of gaze during the squat exercise is
the fact that head position and direction of gaze are not
synonymous. Ignoring the movement of the body as a
whole, humans can redirect gaze by (a) moving the head
with the eyes fixed in the head, (b) moving the eyes with
the head fixed, or (c) moving the eyes and the head si-
multaneously. A recommendation such as O’Shea’s to
keep the head up during the squat (16) does not ensure
that gaze will be directed upward as well. Of course, head
position and direction of gaze likely are related; concern-
146 DONNELLY, BERG, AND FISKE
ing recommendations for squat technique, however, it is
a mistake to assume they are equivalent. What is it that
matters: head position, direction of gaze, both, or neither?
Theoretically, the direction of gaze during the squat
exercise is potentially important, because it could affect
posture and stability. Vision can be critical for movement
control during the squat, particularly regarding balance/
stability (1). Accurate perception of self-motion is funda-
mental to maintaining stability, and the direction of gaze
could influence a lifter’s perception of self-motion. For ex-
ample, looking upward toward the ceiling during the
squat exercise could decrease sensitivity to information
about the position of one’s body relative to the proximal
support surface (floor). Information about one’s position
relative to the floor is salient because it is useful for de -
termining when to terminate the descent portion of the
exercise and to begin the ascent. Selecting the correct
point to change direction is critical, because a failure to
do so could result in a loss of balance, leading to injury
of the knee or back, or even causing the athlete to fall.
In contrast, directing one’s gaze downward toward the
floor during the squat could decrease sensitivity to infor-
mation about the position of one’s body relative to a wall
or objects directly in front of the lifter. Perceiving ap-
proach to or withdrawal from objects and surfaces direct-
ly in front of us allows us to perceive forward and back-
ward sway, respectively, and is critical for maintaining
an upright posture (13). By looking at the floor, a per-
former might fail to perceive postural sway in time to
retard the forward or backward rotation of the body,
which could result in a fall.
What about head position during the squat exercise?
Theoretically, head position is important because it could
possibly affect posture and stability, though more
straightforwardly so than direction of gaze influences sta-
bility. For example, Fry contends that the head and neck
can be considered continuations of the vertebral column
(5), implying that head position has a direct effect on body
alignment, irrespective of the direction of gaze. It is ob-
vious that improper body alignment, such as allowing
one’s center of gravity to escape the confines of the base
of support, could be detrimental to performance or even
dangerous.
Because of the lack of agreement concerning head po-
sition and direction of gaze in the squat exercise, and giv-
en its probable importance to stability and safety during
the performance of that exercise, the purpose of this
study was to determine whether direction of gaze influ-
ences the kinematics of the squat. As previously ex-
plained, when referring to the squat exercise, strength
and conditioning professionals appear to consider head
position and direction of gaze to be synonymous or nearly
so, but they are not. We chose to manipulate the direction
of gaze rather than head position, because the former is
far more easily and precisely controlled. Naturally, anal-
yses of the results included the extent to which direction
of gaze and head position were associated.
We also chose to evaluate anterior/posterior displace-
ment of the bar, as well as hip and linear velocity of the
bar, as measures of postural sway, and thus, indicators
of stability. We also chose to characterize the angular ki -
nematics of the trunk, hip, and knee to determine if the
direction of gaze affects the overall coordination dynamics
of the squat. Because of the potential link between exces-
sive trunk flexion, undue stress to the lumbar spine, and
injury risk (3, 6, 14, 15), we were particularly interested
in evaluating trunk flexion across different gaze direc-
tions. Our hypotheses were that a downward gaze would
result in a greater trunk flexion, as well as greater pos-
tural sway (instability), than either the straight or up-
ward gaze conditions. We also anticipated that the up-
ward and straight gaze directions would yield similar lin-
ear and angular squat kinematics.
METHODS
Experimental Approach to the Problem
A repeated-measures design was used to determine the
effect of the direction of gaze (downward, straight ahead,
or upward) on the kinematics of the squat exercise. De-
pendent variables included the linear displacement of the
bar and hip, linear velocity of the bar, and the angular
displacement/position and velocity of the head, trunk, hip,
and knee.
Subjects
The subjects for this study consisted of 10 young men
(mean age 5 20; SD 5 1.3; range, 18–22 years) who were
members of the Miami University football team. Each
subject had at least 1 year of experience performing the
squat exercise. The subjects ranged in height from 1.78–
1.96 m and ranged in weight from 84–136.2 kg. The in-
vestigation was approved by the Miami University Insti-
tutional Review Board for Human Subjects.
Procedures
The apparatus used to perform the squat exercise con-
sisted of a power rack, a York Olympic 20.43-kg bar (York
Barbell Co., York, PA), and Olympic-sized weight plates.
The rack was positioned such that subjects faced a mir-
rored wall (at a distance of 1.5 m.) while performing the
exercise. Subjects performed a total of 30 repetitions of
the squat exercise in the form of 2 sets of 5 repetitions
under 3 different conditions. The conditions varied with
respect to the direction of the subjects’ gaze as they per -
formed the squat. Condition D entailed gazing downward
at the intersection of the facing wall and the floor
throughout the exercise. Condition S required subjects to
gaze straight ahead at their own reflection (eyes) in the
mirror on the wall directly in front of them. Condition U
involved gazing upward at the intersection of the facing
wall and the ceiling (height 5 2.79 m) throughout the
exercise. The order in which the conditions were per-
formed was counterbalanced across subjects. The inten-
sity for each subject was set at 25% of his 1 repetition
maximum (1RM) in the squat exercise. The purpose of
choosing a relatively light load was to minimize the ex-
tent to which fatigue would influence squat kinematics.
Normally, one could expect counterbalancing the order in
which conditions are presented to completely control for
any order effect resulting from fatigue. However, given
that the differences in kinematics we could foresee as a
result of manipulating the direction gaze were not dra-
matic, we choose to try to minimize the effect of fatigue.
Three minutes of recovery was afforded between each set
of 5 repetitions.
A Panasonic WV-D5100HS SVHS video camera (Pan-
asonic, Secaucus, NJ) operating at 60 Hz and with a shut-
ter speed of 1/250 second was positioned 5.5 m to the right
of the subject and was used to record each trial. The cam-
DIRECTION OF GAZE IN THE SQUAT 147
FIGURE 1. Reflective marker locations and angle
designations.
TABLE 1. Mean total linear displacement of the bar and hip.*
Condition
Downward gaze Straight gaze Upward gaze
Mean total horizontal displacement of the bar (m) 0.12 0.11
0.10
SD 0.04 0.05 0.04
Mean total vertical displacement of the bar (m) 0.75 0.74 0.73
SD 0.08 0.09 0.08
Mean total horizontal displacement of the hip (m) 0.14 0.14
0.13
SD 0.02 0.02 0.03
Mean total vertical displacement of the hip (m) 0.39 0.38 0.38
SD 0.08 0.08 0.08
* Results were not statistically significant.
era was level, 90 cm from the floor, and was oriented with
its optical axis perpendicular to a subject’s sagittal plane
of motion. The camera was equipped with a halogen flood
lamp that illuminated reflective markers on the subject.
As illustrated in Figure 1, the reflective markers were
located (a) just below the superciliary arch near the right
eye, (b) at the greater trochanter of the right hip, (c) at
the lateral articulation of the right knee, (d) at the lateral
malleolous of the right ankle, and (e) at the end of the
squatting bar.
The third trial from each set of 5 repetitions was dig-
itized using a motion measurement system from Peak
Performance Inc (Vicon-Peak, Centennial, CO). Thus, 6
trials were digitized for each subject (2 for each condi-
tion), for a total of 60 trials. The landmarks denoted by
the reflective markers were automatically digitized begin-
ning with the video frame displaying the first downward
movement of the bar, and continued for each of 176 con-
secutive frames (2.93 seconds). Once digitizing was com-
pleted, the data was smoothed using a Butterworth dig-
ital filter, and the dependent variables were computed.
Statistical Analyses
Variables included the linear displacement of the bar and
hip, linear velocity of the bar, and the angular displace-
ment/position and velocity of the head, trunk, hip, and
knee (refer to Figure 1 for angle designations). To ascer -
tain whether there were differences among the 3 condi-
tions, the mean data were subjected to a repeated mea-
sures analysis of variance (ANOVA), and where appro-
priate, pairwise comparisons using Tukey’s Studentized
Range Test. The correlation between the direction of gaze
and mean head position was computed using a Spear-
man’s rho (rank) correlation. An alpha level of 0.05 was
used for the statistical tests.
RESULTS
Linear Kinematics of the Bar and Hip
As shown in Table 1, a repeated measures ANOVA failed
to reveal differences in the mean total horizontal and ver -
tical bar displacements or the mean total horizontal and
vertical hip displacements: F(2, 9) 5 0.81, p . 0.05; F(2,
9) 5 1.18, p . 0.05; F(2, 9) 5 0.77, p . 0.05; and F(2, 9)
5 0.87, p . 0.05, respectively. As is evident from the data
presented in Table 2, there were likewise no significant
differences in peak linear velocity of the bar in the down-
ward, upward, forward, or backward directions: F(2, 9) 5
0.74, p . 0.05; F(2, 9) 5 0.53, p . 0.05; F(2, 9) 5 1.04, p
. 0.05; and F(2, 9) 5 0.41, p . 0.05, respectively. The
lack of differences in the data characterizing horizontal
(i.e., anterior-posterior) motion indicates that postural
sway, and thus stability, was not affected by the direction
of gaze.
Angular Kinematics
The mean angular head position was computed to deter-
mine the extent to which the different gaze conditions
influenced head position. The mean head angles are
shown in Table 3. The repeated measures ANOVA for
head angle was significant (F[2, 9] 5 35.77, p , 0.0001).
Pairwise comparisons using Tukey’s Studentized Range
Test revealed significant differences between condition D
and each of the other conditions (S: difference 5 22.72,
95% confidence limits [CL] 5 13.02, 32.42; U: difference
5 31.05, 95% CL 5 21.35, 40.75). However, the difference
148 DONNELLY, BERG, AND FISKE
TABLE 2. Mean peak linear velocity of the bar for 3 gaze
directions.*
Condition
Downward gaze Straight gaze Upward gaze
Mean peak velocity of the bar in downward direction (m·s21)
0.92 0.94 0.91
SD 0.15 0.13 0.14
Mean peak velocity of the bar in upward direction (m·s21) 1.06
1.09 1.08
SD 0.09 0.08 0.10
Mean peak velocity of the bar in forward direction (m·s21) 0.15
0.15 0.13
SD 0.04 0.06 0.04
Mean peak velocity of the bar in backward direction (m·s21)
0.16 0.15 0.16
SD 0.05 0.06 0.07
* Results were not statistically significant.
TABLE 3. Mean maximum angular displacement of the trunk,
hip, knee, and mean angular position of the head for 3 gaze
directions.*
Condition
Downward gaze Straight gaze Upward gaze
Mean maximum trunk flexion† (8) 217 213 212
SD 7 12 12
Mean maximum hip flexion‡ (8) 77 84 86
SD 7 15 14
Mean maximum knee flexion (8) 82 83 85
SD 11 12 12
Mean head angle§ (8) 97 75 66
SD 12 5 10
* For the trunk and head, larger values represent greater
flexion. For the hip and knee, smaller values represent greater
flexion.
See Figure 1 for angle designations.
† P 5 0.07.
‡ p , 0.05 (pairwise comparisons revealed a single significant
difference between conditions D and U).
§ p , 0.0001 (pairwise comparisons revealed significant
differences between conditions D and S, and D and U).
between conditions S and U did not achieve significance
(difference 5 8.33, 95% CL 5 21.37, 18.03). The corre-
lation coefficient between direction of gaze and mean
head position was 0.79 (p , 0.0001). The results indicate
that although direction of gaze and head position were
strongly related, they should not be considered equiva-
lent.
Also apparent in Table 3 is the fact that mean maxi-
mum trunk, hip, and knee flexion were each greatest
when the direction of gaze was directed downward (con-
dition D). (Note that for the trunk, larger values repre-
sent greater flexion, whereas for the hip and knee, small -
er values represent greater flexion.) The repeated mea-
sures ANOVA for mean maximum hip flexion was signif-
icant (F[2, 9] 5 4.82, p , 0.05), with the lone significant
difference found between conditions D and U. In other
words, hip flexion was more severe when using the down-
ward gaze than the upward gaze (difference 5 8.32, 95%
CL 5 1.05, 15.58). The repeated measures ANOVA for
mean maximum trunk flexion approached significance
(F[2, 9] 5 3.02, p 5 0.07), with condition D exceeding both
conditions S and U by 4 and 58, respectively. In other
words, trunk flexion demonstrated a tendency to be more
severe when using the downward gaze than when using
the straight or upward conditions, but the difference was
not statistically significant. The repeated measures AN-
OVA for mean maximum knee flexion was not significant
(F[2, 9] 5 0.48, p . 0.05), indicating that knee flexion
was unaffected by gaze direction.
As shown in Table 4, the repeated measures ANOVAs
failed to reveal statistically significant differences among
conditions for peak trunk, hip, and knee flexion velocity:
F(2, 9) 5 1.81, p . 0.05; F(2, 9) 5 0.15, p . 0.05; and
F(2, 9) 5 0.28, p . 0.05, respectively. There were likewise
no significant differences among conditions for peak
trunk, hip, and knee extension velocity: F(2, 9) 5 0.58, p
. 0.05; F(2, 9) 5 0.36, p . 0.05; and F(2, 9) 5 2.02, p .
0.05, respectively.
DISCUSSION
The purpose of this study was to begin to address the lack
of clarity about head position or gaze direction during the
squat exercise. Based on current technical standards for
performing the squat exercise, as well as theoretical im-
plications of head position and direction of gaze on pos-
tural stability, we hypothesized that a downward gaze
(condition D) would result in a greater maximum trunk
flexion, as well as greater instability as determined by the
total anterior-posterior displacement of the bar and hip.
Conversely, we anticipated that an upward and straight-
ahead gaze (conditions U and S, respectively) would re-
sult in similar linear and angular kinematics.
First, however, we evaluated the extent to which the
direction of gaze and head position were equivalent. As
expected, there was a strong relationship between direc-
tion of gaze and mean head position, yet it was not perfect
(r 5 0.79, R2 5 62.4%). Strength and conditioning profes-
sionals should be aware of the lack of a one-to-one map-
ping between direction of gaze and head position, and
DIRECTION OF GAZE IN THE SQUAT 149
TABLE 4. Mean peak angular velocity of the trunk, hip, and
knee for three gaze directions.
Condition
Downward gaze Straight gaze Upward gaze
Mean peak trunk flexion velocity (8·s21) 45 42 40
SD 8 10 6
Mean peak hip flexion velocity (8·s21) 102 101 100
SD 15 15 13
Mean peak knee flexion velocity (8·s21) 111 108 107
SD 22 21 19
Mean peak trunk extension velocity (8·s21 46 46 44
SD 5 9 7
Mean peak hip extension velocity (8·s21) 114 116 117
SD 14 10 18
Mean peak knee extension velocity (8·s21) 113 116 122
SD 24 23 29
* Results were not statistically significant.
should develop instructional cues for the squat exercise
accordingly. Because we directly manipulated only the
gaze direction in this study, we will refer only to it in the
discussion.
Our hypotheses that condition D would result in
greater total anterior-posterior motion of the bar and hip
(postural sway) were not supported. Likewise, mean peak
linear velocity of the bar in the downward, upward, for-
ward, or backward direction did not differ across condi -
tions. In sum, based on measures of total bar and hip
displacement, as well as peak bar velocity, we must con-
clude that direction of gaze did not influence postural
sway, and thus stability, in the squat exercise.
Mean maximum trunk flexion was an average of 4.58
greater in condition D than in condition S or U. The sta-
tistical analysis on this variable approached significance
(p 5 0.07) and thus there is reason to believe that the
downward gaze did result in greater trunk flexion than
did conditions U and S. Moreover, under condition D,
mean maximum hip flexion was 9 and 78 greater than
conditions U and S, respectively, with the difference be-
tween D and U achieving statistical significance. It is
noteworthy that squat depth did not differ among condi-
tions (as shown in Table 1—vertical displacement of the
hip), nor did maximum knee flexion differ, indicating that
the increase in hip flexion observed in condition D was
not offset by greater knee flexion (i.e., a deeper squat).
Our findings appear to confirm the efficacy of the rec-
ommendation made by some strength and conditioning
professionals against allowing the head and eye focus to
drop below a neutral position as a means of preventing
excessive trunk flexion (3). What are the implications of
greater trunk flexion in the squat? Of course, some trunk
flexion is necessary, but excessive flexion could put an
increased torque on the lower back musculature, possibly
putting the athlete at greater risk of injuries such as mus-
cle strains, disc herniations, and spondylolysis (stress
fracture of the vertebral column) (3, 5, 14, 15). The injury
risk that is speculated to accompany excessive trunk flex-
ion resulting from a downward gaze might be exacerbated
if accompanied by a concomitant increase in flexion ve-
locity. However, analyses failed to reveal statistically sig-
nificant differences among conditions for peak trunk flex-
ion velocity.
Our study had 2 noteworthy limitations. First, sub-
jects in the study were all college football players with a
minimum of 1 year of experience performing the squat
exercise. Therefore, the results should not necessarily be
generalized to younger athletes or those with less expe-
rience performing the squat. Second, subjects performed
the squat using 25% of their 1RM. It is possible that the
effect of gaze direction on movement kinematics could dif-
fer from that observed in this study when using a higher
intensity exercise. This question will need to be addressed
in future research, but for now we must emphasize that
the results of this study apply only to low-intensity squat-
ting. It is also important to emphasize that the significant
and nearly significant differences in mean maximum hip
and trunk flexion, respectively, were found between con-
ditions D and U only. Thus, it is important to be precise
in our conclusion that the downward gaze resulted in
greater hip and trunk flexion than the upward gaze, but
not greater than the straight gaze.
PRACTICAL APPLICATIONS
This study of male collegiate football players revealed
overall similarity in movement kinematics when perform-
ing the squat exercise using 3 different gaze directions:
upward, straight, and downward. Our study was unable
to differentiate between the straight and upward gaze di -
rections. Conversely, the downward gaze was shown to
increase the extent of hip flexion and possibly trunk flex-
ion as well, especially relative to the upward gaze. Be-
cause excessive hip and trunk flexion in the squat are
contraindicated, cautioning athletes against allowing the
head or direction of gaze to drop below a neutral position
appears to be warranted.
REFERENCES
1. BENNETT, S., AND K. DAVIDS. The manipulation of vision
dur-
ing the powerlift squat: Exploring the boundaries of the spec-
ificity of learning hypothesis. Res. Q. Exerc. Sport 66:210–218.
1995.
2. CHANDLER, T.J., AND M.H. STONE. The squat exercise in
ath-
letic conditioning: A position statement and review of litera-
ture. Strength Cond. J. 13(5):51–60. 1991.
3. FAIRCHILD, D., B. HILL, M. RITCHIE, AND D. SOCHOR.
Round-
table: Common technique errors in the back squat. Strength
Cond. J. 15(3):20–27. 1993.
4. FRANCOEUR, J.P. Squat. Available at: http://www.jeanpaul.
com/squat.html. Accessed July 14, 2004.
5. FRY, A. Coaching considerations for the barbell squat—Part
I.
Strength Cond. J. 15(2):60–65. 1993.
150 DONNELLY, BERG, AND FISKE
6. FRY, A.C., C. SMITH, AND B.K. SCHILLING. Effect of
knee posi-
tion on hip and knee torques during the barbell squat. J.
Strength Cond. Res.. 17:629–633. 2003.
7. GLOBAL HEALTH AND FITNESS. Legs. Available at:
http://
www.global-fitness.com/musclemap/mmpquads.html. Accessed
July 12, 2004.
8. GOLDBERG, L., D.L. ELLIOT, AND K.S. KUEHL.
Cardiovascular
changes at rest and during mixed static and dynamic exercises
after weight training. J. Appl. Sport Sci. Res. 2(3):42–45. 1988.
9. GOLDBERG, L., D.L. ELLIOT, AND K.S. KUEHL. A
comparison of
the cardiovascular effects of running and weight training. J.
Strength Cond. Res. 8:219–224. 1994.
10. GOTSHALK, L., R. BERGER, AND W. KRAEMER.
Cardiovascular
responses to a high-volume continuous circuit resistance train-
ing protocol. J. Strength Cond. Res. 18:760–764. 2004.
11. GRAHAM, J. Back squat. Strength Cond. J. 23(5):28–29.
2001.
12. KELSO, T., J. GAMBLE, B. HILL, K. BARNES, AND D.
PASANELLA.
The back squat. Strength Cond. J. 11(2):18–24. 1989.
13. LEE, D.N., AND J.R. LISHMAN. Visual proprioceptive
control of
stance. J. Hum. Mov. Stud. 1(2):87–95. 1975.
14. MCLAUGHLIN, T., R. LARDNER, AND C. DILLMAN.
Kinetics of the
parallel squat. Res. Quart. 49:175–189. 1978.
15. NEITZEL, J., AND G. DAVIES. The benefits and
controversy of
the parallel squat in strength training and rehabilitation.
Strength Cond. J. 22(3):30–37. 2000.
16. O’SHEA, P. The parallel squat. Strength Cond. J. 7(1):4–6.
1985.
17. WESTCOTT, W.L. Strength Fitness: Physiological
Principles and
Training Techniques (4th ed.). Quincy, MA: McGraw-Hill.
1994.
Acknowledgments
We would like to thank Michael Hughes for his contribution to
this study.
Address correspondence to William P. Berg, [email protected]
muohio.edu.

More Related Content

Similar to Downloadedfromhttpjournals.lww.comnsca

Conventional Strength: Squat Progressions for Variety, Form, and Function
Conventional Strength: Squat Progressions for Variety, Form, and FunctionConventional Strength: Squat Progressions for Variety, Form, and Function
Conventional Strength: Squat Progressions for Variety, Form, and FunctionPrecor
 
Function and core
Function and coreFunction and core
Function and coreVivi Sary
 
Active self correction of back posture
Active self correction of back postureActive self correction of back posture
Active self correction of back postureMeziat
 
Safe and Proper Way to Perform a Barbell Back Squat
Safe and Proper Way to Perform a Barbell Back Squat Safe and Proper Way to Perform a Barbell Back Squat
Safe and Proper Way to Perform a Barbell Back Squat Kody Springsteen
 
UK snowsports ACL injury
UK snowsports ACL injuryUK snowsports ACL injury
UK snowsports ACL injurynw conditioning
 
Field Work Project Proposal
Field Work Project ProposalField Work Project Proposal
Field Work Project ProposalThom Manning
 
Sports Science Congress at Al-Ahli Saudi
Sports Science Congress at Al-Ahli SaudiSports Science Congress at Al-Ahli Saudi
Sports Science Congress at Al-Ahli SaudiFitnessInSoccer
 
Respond to the 5 post below.100-200 wordsAPA FORMAT (NO TITLE PA.docx
Respond to the 5 post below.100-200 wordsAPA FORMAT (NO TITLE PA.docxRespond to the 5 post below.100-200 wordsAPA FORMAT (NO TITLE PA.docx
Respond to the 5 post below.100-200 wordsAPA FORMAT (NO TITLE PA.docxaudeleypearl
 
Promoting-Back-Health
Promoting-Back-HealthPromoting-Back-Health
Promoting-Back-HealthStephen Brown
 
Muscle ativaction during four pilates core stability exercises in quadrupede ...
Muscle ativaction during four pilates core stability exercises in quadrupede ...Muscle ativaction during four pilates core stability exercises in quadrupede ...
Muscle ativaction during four pilates core stability exercises in quadrupede ...Dra. Welker Fisioterapeuta
 
12 Sports Science - Combat Juggling
12 Sports Science - Combat Juggling12 Sports Science - Combat Juggling
12 Sports Science - Combat JugglingBCSPS
 
Amanda Gray Technical Report
Amanda Gray Technical ReportAmanda Gray Technical Report
Amanda Gray Technical ReportAmanda Gray
 
Lesson-1-Physical-Fitness Concept Pathfit
Lesson-1-Physical-Fitness Concept PathfitLesson-1-Physical-Fitness Concept Pathfit
Lesson-1-Physical-Fitness Concept Pathfitnmb5kzw4sr
 
Analyse the Performance of a Sport Skill: Rugby Line-out Throw
Analyse the Performance of a Sport Skill: Rugby Line-out ThrowAnalyse the Performance of a Sport Skill: Rugby Line-out Throw
Analyse the Performance of a Sport Skill: Rugby Line-out ThrowCarl Page
 

Similar to Downloadedfromhttpjournals.lww.comnsca (20)

4 Pics 1 Word.pptx
4 Pics 1 Word.pptx4 Pics 1 Word.pptx
4 Pics 1 Word.pptx
 
Conventional Strength: Squat Progressions for Variety, Form, and Function
Conventional Strength: Squat Progressions for Variety, Form, and FunctionConventional Strength: Squat Progressions for Variety, Form, and Function
Conventional Strength: Squat Progressions for Variety, Form, and Function
 
Function and core
Function and coreFunction and core
Function and core
 
Active self correction of back posture
Active self correction of back postureActive self correction of back posture
Active self correction of back posture
 
Safe and Proper Way to Perform a Barbell Back Squat
Safe and Proper Way to Perform a Barbell Back Squat Safe and Proper Way to Perform a Barbell Back Squat
Safe and Proper Way to Perform a Barbell Back Squat
 
Pilates and the Powerhouse
Pilates and the PowerhousePilates and the Powerhouse
Pilates and the Powerhouse
 
UK snowsports ACL injury
UK snowsports ACL injuryUK snowsports ACL injury
UK snowsports ACL injury
 
Field Work Project Proposal
Field Work Project ProposalField Work Project Proposal
Field Work Project Proposal
 
Sports Science Congress at Al-Ahli Saudi
Sports Science Congress at Al-Ahli SaudiSports Science Congress at Al-Ahli Saudi
Sports Science Congress at Al-Ahli Saudi
 
Respond to the 5 post below.100-200 wordsAPA FORMAT (NO TITLE PA.docx
Respond to the 5 post below.100-200 wordsAPA FORMAT (NO TITLE PA.docxRespond to the 5 post below.100-200 wordsAPA FORMAT (NO TITLE PA.docx
Respond to the 5 post below.100-200 wordsAPA FORMAT (NO TITLE PA.docx
 
LBP & Neck Pain in Aviation
LBP & Neck Pain in AviationLBP & Neck Pain in Aviation
LBP & Neck Pain in Aviation
 
Chop-Lift_Article.pdf
Chop-Lift_Article.pdfChop-Lift_Article.pdf
Chop-Lift_Article.pdf
 
Promoting-Back-Health
Promoting-Back-HealthPromoting-Back-Health
Promoting-Back-Health
 
Muscle ativaction during four pilates core stability exercises in quadrupede ...
Muscle ativaction during four pilates core stability exercises in quadrupede ...Muscle ativaction during four pilates core stability exercises in quadrupede ...
Muscle ativaction during four pilates core stability exercises in quadrupede ...
 
final posture.pptx
final posture.pptxfinal posture.pptx
final posture.pptx
 
Training Program
Training ProgramTraining Program
Training Program
 
12 Sports Science - Combat Juggling
12 Sports Science - Combat Juggling12 Sports Science - Combat Juggling
12 Sports Science - Combat Juggling
 
Amanda Gray Technical Report
Amanda Gray Technical ReportAmanda Gray Technical Report
Amanda Gray Technical Report
 
Lesson-1-Physical-Fitness Concept Pathfit
Lesson-1-Physical-Fitness Concept PathfitLesson-1-Physical-Fitness Concept Pathfit
Lesson-1-Physical-Fitness Concept Pathfit
 
Analyse the Performance of a Sport Skill: Rugby Line-out Throw
Analyse the Performance of a Sport Skill: Rugby Line-out ThrowAnalyse the Performance of a Sport Skill: Rugby Line-out Throw
Analyse the Performance of a Sport Skill: Rugby Line-out Throw
 

More from DustiBuckner14

Your new clientsThe Wagner’s – Scott and Ella are a young marri.docx
Your new clientsThe Wagner’s – Scott and Ella are a young marri.docxYour new clientsThe Wagner’s – Scott and Ella are a young marri.docx
Your new clientsThe Wagner’s – Scott and Ella are a young marri.docxDustiBuckner14
 
Writing Conclusions for Research PapersWhat is the purpose.docx
Writing Conclusions for Research PapersWhat is the purpose.docxWriting Conclusions for Research PapersWhat is the purpose.docx
Writing Conclusions for Research PapersWhat is the purpose.docxDustiBuckner14
 
What Is Septic TankSeptic or septic typically is used t.docx
What Is Septic TankSeptic or septic typically is used t.docxWhat Is Septic TankSeptic or septic typically is used t.docx
What Is Septic TankSeptic or septic typically is used t.docxDustiBuckner14
 
· You should respond to at least two of your peers by extending, r.docx
· You should respond to at least two of your peers by extending, r.docx· You should respond to at least two of your peers by extending, r.docx
· You should respond to at least two of your peers by extending, r.docxDustiBuckner14
 
You are a medical student working your way throughcollege and ar.docx
You are a medical student working your way throughcollege and ar.docxYou are a medical student working your way throughcollege and ar.docx
You are a medical student working your way throughcollege and ar.docxDustiBuckner14
 
[removed]THIS IEP INCLUDES FORMCHECKBOX Transitions.docx
[removed]THIS IEP INCLUDES     FORMCHECKBOX  Transitions.docx[removed]THIS IEP INCLUDES     FORMCHECKBOX  Transitions.docx
[removed]THIS IEP INCLUDES FORMCHECKBOX Transitions.docxDustiBuckner14
 
Using the Integrated Model of Work Motivation Figure 12.1 (Latham, 2.docx
Using the Integrated Model of Work Motivation Figure 12.1 (Latham, 2.docxUsing the Integrated Model of Work Motivation Figure 12.1 (Latham, 2.docx
Using the Integrated Model of Work Motivation Figure 12.1 (Latham, 2.docxDustiBuckner14
 
What We Can Afford” Poem By Shavar X. Seabrooks L.docx
What We Can Afford” Poem By Shavar  X. Seabrooks L.docxWhat We Can Afford” Poem By Shavar  X. Seabrooks L.docx
What We Can Afford” Poem By Shavar X. Seabrooks L.docxDustiBuckner14
 
What are the techniques in handling categorical attributesHow.docx
What are the techniques in handling categorical attributesHow.docxWhat are the techniques in handling categorical attributesHow.docx
What are the techniques in handling categorical attributesHow.docxDustiBuckner14
 
University of the CumberlandsSchool of Computer & Information .docx
University of the CumberlandsSchool of Computer & Information .docxUniversity of the CumberlandsSchool of Computer & Information .docx
University of the CumberlandsSchool of Computer & Information .docxDustiBuckner14
 
Theresa and Mike fully support creating a code of conduct for th.docx
Theresa and Mike fully support creating a code of conduct for th.docxTheresa and Mike fully support creating a code of conduct for th.docx
Theresa and Mike fully support creating a code of conduct for th.docxDustiBuckner14
 
Unit VII 1. Suppose a firm uses sugar in a product tha.docx
Unit VII         1. Suppose a firm uses sugar in a product tha.docxUnit VII         1. Suppose a firm uses sugar in a product tha.docx
Unit VII 1. Suppose a firm uses sugar in a product tha.docxDustiBuckner14
 
Title PageThis spreadsheet supports STUDENT analysis of the case .docx
Title PageThis spreadsheet supports STUDENT analysis of the case .docxTitle PageThis spreadsheet supports STUDENT analysis of the case .docx
Title PageThis spreadsheet supports STUDENT analysis of the case .docxDustiBuckner14
 
Title If a compensation system works well for one business, that .docx
Title If a compensation system works well for one business, that .docxTitle If a compensation system works well for one business, that .docx
Title If a compensation system works well for one business, that .docxDustiBuckner14
 
Review the Article Below Keller, J. G., Miller, C., LasDulce, C.docx
Review the Article Below Keller, J. G., Miller, C., LasDulce, C.docxReview the Article Below Keller, J. G., Miller, C., LasDulce, C.docx
Review the Article Below Keller, J. G., Miller, C., LasDulce, C.docxDustiBuckner14
 
Teachers reach diverse learners by scaffolding instruction in ways t.docx
Teachers reach diverse learners by scaffolding instruction in ways t.docxTeachers reach diverse learners by scaffolding instruction in ways t.docx
Teachers reach diverse learners by scaffolding instruction in ways t.docxDustiBuckner14
 
ScenarioThe HIT Innovation Steering Committee of a large.docx
ScenarioThe HIT Innovation Steering Committee of a large.docxScenarioThe HIT Innovation Steering Committee of a large.docx
ScenarioThe HIT Innovation Steering Committee of a large.docxDustiBuckner14
 
Space ... the final frontier.  So, as I am sure everyone knows, .docx
Space ... the final frontier.  So, as I am sure everyone knows, .docxSpace ... the final frontier.  So, as I am sure everyone knows, .docx
Space ... the final frontier.  So, as I am sure everyone knows, .docxDustiBuckner14
 
The Internal EnvironmentInstitutionStudent’s name.docx
The Internal EnvironmentInstitutionStudent’s name.docxThe Internal EnvironmentInstitutionStudent’s name.docx
The Internal EnvironmentInstitutionStudent’s name.docxDustiBuckner14
 
THE RESEARCH PROPOSAL BUS8100 8Chapter 2 - Literature ReviewTh.docx
THE RESEARCH PROPOSAL BUS8100 8Chapter 2 - Literature ReviewTh.docxTHE RESEARCH PROPOSAL BUS8100 8Chapter 2 - Literature ReviewTh.docx
THE RESEARCH PROPOSAL BUS8100 8Chapter 2 - Literature ReviewTh.docxDustiBuckner14
 

More from DustiBuckner14 (20)

Your new clientsThe Wagner’s – Scott and Ella are a young marri.docx
Your new clientsThe Wagner’s – Scott and Ella are a young marri.docxYour new clientsThe Wagner’s – Scott and Ella are a young marri.docx
Your new clientsThe Wagner’s – Scott and Ella are a young marri.docx
 
Writing Conclusions for Research PapersWhat is the purpose.docx
Writing Conclusions for Research PapersWhat is the purpose.docxWriting Conclusions for Research PapersWhat is the purpose.docx
Writing Conclusions for Research PapersWhat is the purpose.docx
 
What Is Septic TankSeptic or septic typically is used t.docx
What Is Septic TankSeptic or septic typically is used t.docxWhat Is Septic TankSeptic or septic typically is used t.docx
What Is Septic TankSeptic or septic typically is used t.docx
 
· You should respond to at least two of your peers by extending, r.docx
· You should respond to at least two of your peers by extending, r.docx· You should respond to at least two of your peers by extending, r.docx
· You should respond to at least two of your peers by extending, r.docx
 
You are a medical student working your way throughcollege and ar.docx
You are a medical student working your way throughcollege and ar.docxYou are a medical student working your way throughcollege and ar.docx
You are a medical student working your way throughcollege and ar.docx
 
[removed]THIS IEP INCLUDES FORMCHECKBOX Transitions.docx
[removed]THIS IEP INCLUDES     FORMCHECKBOX  Transitions.docx[removed]THIS IEP INCLUDES     FORMCHECKBOX  Transitions.docx
[removed]THIS IEP INCLUDES FORMCHECKBOX Transitions.docx
 
Using the Integrated Model of Work Motivation Figure 12.1 (Latham, 2.docx
Using the Integrated Model of Work Motivation Figure 12.1 (Latham, 2.docxUsing the Integrated Model of Work Motivation Figure 12.1 (Latham, 2.docx
Using the Integrated Model of Work Motivation Figure 12.1 (Latham, 2.docx
 
What We Can Afford” Poem By Shavar X. Seabrooks L.docx
What We Can Afford” Poem By Shavar  X. Seabrooks L.docxWhat We Can Afford” Poem By Shavar  X. Seabrooks L.docx
What We Can Afford” Poem By Shavar X. Seabrooks L.docx
 
What are the techniques in handling categorical attributesHow.docx
What are the techniques in handling categorical attributesHow.docxWhat are the techniques in handling categorical attributesHow.docx
What are the techniques in handling categorical attributesHow.docx
 
University of the CumberlandsSchool of Computer & Information .docx
University of the CumberlandsSchool of Computer & Information .docxUniversity of the CumberlandsSchool of Computer & Information .docx
University of the CumberlandsSchool of Computer & Information .docx
 
Theresa and Mike fully support creating a code of conduct for th.docx
Theresa and Mike fully support creating a code of conduct for th.docxTheresa and Mike fully support creating a code of conduct for th.docx
Theresa and Mike fully support creating a code of conduct for th.docx
 
Unit VII 1. Suppose a firm uses sugar in a product tha.docx
Unit VII         1. Suppose a firm uses sugar in a product tha.docxUnit VII         1. Suppose a firm uses sugar in a product tha.docx
Unit VII 1. Suppose a firm uses sugar in a product tha.docx
 
Title PageThis spreadsheet supports STUDENT analysis of the case .docx
Title PageThis spreadsheet supports STUDENT analysis of the case .docxTitle PageThis spreadsheet supports STUDENT analysis of the case .docx
Title PageThis spreadsheet supports STUDENT analysis of the case .docx
 
Title If a compensation system works well for one business, that .docx
Title If a compensation system works well for one business, that .docxTitle If a compensation system works well for one business, that .docx
Title If a compensation system works well for one business, that .docx
 
Review the Article Below Keller, J. G., Miller, C., LasDulce, C.docx
Review the Article Below Keller, J. G., Miller, C., LasDulce, C.docxReview the Article Below Keller, J. G., Miller, C., LasDulce, C.docx
Review the Article Below Keller, J. G., Miller, C., LasDulce, C.docx
 
Teachers reach diverse learners by scaffolding instruction in ways t.docx
Teachers reach diverse learners by scaffolding instruction in ways t.docxTeachers reach diverse learners by scaffolding instruction in ways t.docx
Teachers reach diverse learners by scaffolding instruction in ways t.docx
 
ScenarioThe HIT Innovation Steering Committee of a large.docx
ScenarioThe HIT Innovation Steering Committee of a large.docxScenarioThe HIT Innovation Steering Committee of a large.docx
ScenarioThe HIT Innovation Steering Committee of a large.docx
 
Space ... the final frontier.  So, as I am sure everyone knows, .docx
Space ... the final frontier.  So, as I am sure everyone knows, .docxSpace ... the final frontier.  So, as I am sure everyone knows, .docx
Space ... the final frontier.  So, as I am sure everyone knows, .docx
 
The Internal EnvironmentInstitutionStudent’s name.docx
The Internal EnvironmentInstitutionStudent’s name.docxThe Internal EnvironmentInstitutionStudent’s name.docx
The Internal EnvironmentInstitutionStudent’s name.docx
 
THE RESEARCH PROPOSAL BUS8100 8Chapter 2 - Literature ReviewTh.docx
THE RESEARCH PROPOSAL BUS8100 8Chapter 2 - Literature ReviewTh.docxTHE RESEARCH PROPOSAL BUS8100 8Chapter 2 - Literature ReviewTh.docx
THE RESEARCH PROPOSAL BUS8100 8Chapter 2 - Literature ReviewTh.docx
 

Recently uploaded

Presentation by Andreas Schleicher Tackling the School Absenteeism Crisis 30 ...
Presentation by Andreas Schleicher Tackling the School Absenteeism Crisis 30 ...Presentation by Andreas Schleicher Tackling the School Absenteeism Crisis 30 ...
Presentation by Andreas Schleicher Tackling the School Absenteeism Crisis 30 ...EduSkills OECD
 
Paris 2024 Olympic Geographies - an activity
Paris 2024 Olympic Geographies - an activityParis 2024 Olympic Geographies - an activity
Paris 2024 Olympic Geographies - an activityGeoBlogs
 
Mastering the Unannounced Regulatory Inspection
Mastering the Unannounced Regulatory InspectionMastering the Unannounced Regulatory Inspection
Mastering the Unannounced Regulatory InspectionSafetyChain Software
 
The Most Excellent Way | 1 Corinthians 13
The Most Excellent Way | 1 Corinthians 13The Most Excellent Way | 1 Corinthians 13
The Most Excellent Way | 1 Corinthians 13Steve Thomason
 
Science 7 - LAND and SEA BREEZE and its Characteristics
Science 7 - LAND and SEA BREEZE and its CharacteristicsScience 7 - LAND and SEA BREEZE and its Characteristics
Science 7 - LAND and SEA BREEZE and its CharacteristicsKarinaGenton
 
Crayon Activity Handout For the Crayon A
Crayon Activity Handout For the Crayon ACrayon Activity Handout For the Crayon A
Crayon Activity Handout For the Crayon AUnboundStockton
 
Call Girls in Dwarka Mor Delhi Contact Us 9654467111
Call Girls in Dwarka Mor Delhi Contact Us 9654467111Call Girls in Dwarka Mor Delhi Contact Us 9654467111
Call Girls in Dwarka Mor Delhi Contact Us 9654467111Sapana Sha
 
Concept of Vouching. B.Com(Hons) /B.Compdf
Concept of Vouching. B.Com(Hons) /B.CompdfConcept of Vouching. B.Com(Hons) /B.Compdf
Concept of Vouching. B.Com(Hons) /B.CompdfUmakantAnnand
 
Sanyam Choudhary Chemistry practical.pdf
Sanyam Choudhary Chemistry practical.pdfSanyam Choudhary Chemistry practical.pdf
Sanyam Choudhary Chemistry practical.pdfsanyamsingh5019
 
Software Engineering Methodologies (overview)
Software Engineering Methodologies (overview)Software Engineering Methodologies (overview)
Software Engineering Methodologies (overview)eniolaolutunde
 
Hybridoma Technology ( Production , Purification , and Application )
Hybridoma Technology  ( Production , Purification , and Application  ) Hybridoma Technology  ( Production , Purification , and Application  )
Hybridoma Technology ( Production , Purification , and Application ) Sakshi Ghasle
 
Incoming and Outgoing Shipments in 1 STEP Using Odoo 17
Incoming and Outgoing Shipments in 1 STEP Using Odoo 17Incoming and Outgoing Shipments in 1 STEP Using Odoo 17
Incoming and Outgoing Shipments in 1 STEP Using Odoo 17Celine George
 
POINT- BIOCHEMISTRY SEM 2 ENZYMES UNIT 5.pptx
POINT- BIOCHEMISTRY SEM 2 ENZYMES UNIT 5.pptxPOINT- BIOCHEMISTRY SEM 2 ENZYMES UNIT 5.pptx
POINT- BIOCHEMISTRY SEM 2 ENZYMES UNIT 5.pptxSayali Powar
 
Solving Puzzles Benefits Everyone (English).pptx
Solving Puzzles Benefits Everyone (English).pptxSolving Puzzles Benefits Everyone (English).pptx
Solving Puzzles Benefits Everyone (English).pptxOH TEIK BIN
 
Presiding Officer Training module 2024 lok sabha elections
Presiding Officer Training module 2024 lok sabha electionsPresiding Officer Training module 2024 lok sabha elections
Presiding Officer Training module 2024 lok sabha electionsanshu789521
 
MENTAL STATUS EXAMINATION format.docx
MENTAL     STATUS EXAMINATION format.docxMENTAL     STATUS EXAMINATION format.docx
MENTAL STATUS EXAMINATION format.docxPoojaSen20
 
call girls in Kamla Market (DELHI) 🔝 >༒9953330565🔝 genuine Escort Service 🔝✔️✔️
call girls in Kamla Market (DELHI) 🔝 >༒9953330565🔝 genuine Escort Service 🔝✔️✔️call girls in Kamla Market (DELHI) 🔝 >༒9953330565🔝 genuine Escort Service 🔝✔️✔️
call girls in Kamla Market (DELHI) 🔝 >༒9953330565🔝 genuine Escort Service 🔝✔️✔️9953056974 Low Rate Call Girls In Saket, Delhi NCR
 

Recently uploaded (20)

Presentation by Andreas Schleicher Tackling the School Absenteeism Crisis 30 ...
Presentation by Andreas Schleicher Tackling the School Absenteeism Crisis 30 ...Presentation by Andreas Schleicher Tackling the School Absenteeism Crisis 30 ...
Presentation by Andreas Schleicher Tackling the School Absenteeism Crisis 30 ...
 
Paris 2024 Olympic Geographies - an activity
Paris 2024 Olympic Geographies - an activityParis 2024 Olympic Geographies - an activity
Paris 2024 Olympic Geographies - an activity
 
Staff of Color (SOC) Retention Efforts DDSD
Staff of Color (SOC) Retention Efforts DDSDStaff of Color (SOC) Retention Efforts DDSD
Staff of Color (SOC) Retention Efforts DDSD
 
Mastering the Unannounced Regulatory Inspection
Mastering the Unannounced Regulatory InspectionMastering the Unannounced Regulatory Inspection
Mastering the Unannounced Regulatory Inspection
 
The Most Excellent Way | 1 Corinthians 13
The Most Excellent Way | 1 Corinthians 13The Most Excellent Way | 1 Corinthians 13
The Most Excellent Way | 1 Corinthians 13
 
Science 7 - LAND and SEA BREEZE and its Characteristics
Science 7 - LAND and SEA BREEZE and its CharacteristicsScience 7 - LAND and SEA BREEZE and its Characteristics
Science 7 - LAND and SEA BREEZE and its Characteristics
 
Crayon Activity Handout For the Crayon A
Crayon Activity Handout For the Crayon ACrayon Activity Handout For the Crayon A
Crayon Activity Handout For the Crayon A
 
Código Creativo y Arte de Software | Unidad 1
Código Creativo y Arte de Software | Unidad 1Código Creativo y Arte de Software | Unidad 1
Código Creativo y Arte de Software | Unidad 1
 
Call Girls in Dwarka Mor Delhi Contact Us 9654467111
Call Girls in Dwarka Mor Delhi Contact Us 9654467111Call Girls in Dwarka Mor Delhi Contact Us 9654467111
Call Girls in Dwarka Mor Delhi Contact Us 9654467111
 
Concept of Vouching. B.Com(Hons) /B.Compdf
Concept of Vouching. B.Com(Hons) /B.CompdfConcept of Vouching. B.Com(Hons) /B.Compdf
Concept of Vouching. B.Com(Hons) /B.Compdf
 
Sanyam Choudhary Chemistry practical.pdf
Sanyam Choudhary Chemistry practical.pdfSanyam Choudhary Chemistry practical.pdf
Sanyam Choudhary Chemistry practical.pdf
 
Software Engineering Methodologies (overview)
Software Engineering Methodologies (overview)Software Engineering Methodologies (overview)
Software Engineering Methodologies (overview)
 
Hybridoma Technology ( Production , Purification , and Application )
Hybridoma Technology  ( Production , Purification , and Application  ) Hybridoma Technology  ( Production , Purification , and Application  )
Hybridoma Technology ( Production , Purification , and Application )
 
Incoming and Outgoing Shipments in 1 STEP Using Odoo 17
Incoming and Outgoing Shipments in 1 STEP Using Odoo 17Incoming and Outgoing Shipments in 1 STEP Using Odoo 17
Incoming and Outgoing Shipments in 1 STEP Using Odoo 17
 
POINT- BIOCHEMISTRY SEM 2 ENZYMES UNIT 5.pptx
POINT- BIOCHEMISTRY SEM 2 ENZYMES UNIT 5.pptxPOINT- BIOCHEMISTRY SEM 2 ENZYMES UNIT 5.pptx
POINT- BIOCHEMISTRY SEM 2 ENZYMES UNIT 5.pptx
 
TataKelola dan KamSiber Kecerdasan Buatan v022.pdf
TataKelola dan KamSiber Kecerdasan Buatan v022.pdfTataKelola dan KamSiber Kecerdasan Buatan v022.pdf
TataKelola dan KamSiber Kecerdasan Buatan v022.pdf
 
Solving Puzzles Benefits Everyone (English).pptx
Solving Puzzles Benefits Everyone (English).pptxSolving Puzzles Benefits Everyone (English).pptx
Solving Puzzles Benefits Everyone (English).pptx
 
Presiding Officer Training module 2024 lok sabha elections
Presiding Officer Training module 2024 lok sabha electionsPresiding Officer Training module 2024 lok sabha elections
Presiding Officer Training module 2024 lok sabha elections
 
MENTAL STATUS EXAMINATION format.docx
MENTAL     STATUS EXAMINATION format.docxMENTAL     STATUS EXAMINATION format.docx
MENTAL STATUS EXAMINATION format.docx
 
call girls in Kamla Market (DELHI) 🔝 >༒9953330565🔝 genuine Escort Service 🔝✔️✔️
call girls in Kamla Market (DELHI) 🔝 >༒9953330565🔝 genuine Escort Service 🔝✔️✔️call girls in Kamla Market (DELHI) 🔝 >༒9953330565🔝 genuine Escort Service 🔝✔️✔️
call girls in Kamla Market (DELHI) 🔝 >༒9953330565🔝 genuine Escort Service 🔝✔️✔️
 

Downloadedfromhttpjournals.lww.comnsca

  • 3. G K V 0Y m y+78= on 05/21/2021 Downloadedfromhttp://journals.lww.com/nsca- jscrbyBhDMf5ePHKav1zEoum1tQfN4a+kJLhEZgbsIHo4XMi0h CywCX1AWnYQp/IlQrHD3i3D0OdRyi7TvSFl4Cf3VC4/OAVp DDa8KKGKV0Ymy+78=on05/21/2021 Journal of Strength and Conditioning Research, 2006, 20(1), 145–150 q 2006 National Strength & Conditioning Association THE EFFECT OF THE DIRECTION OF GAZE ON THE KINEMATICS OF THE SQUAT EXERCISE DAVID V. DONNELLY,1 WILLIAM P. BERG,2 AND DARRYN M. FISKE3 1Department of Intercollegiate Athletics, Miami University, Oxford, Ohio 45056; 2Department of Physical Education, Health and Sport Studies, Miami University, Oxford, Ohio 45056; 3Department of Athletics, St. Bonaventure University, St. Bonaventure, New York 14778. ABSTRACT. Donnelly, D., W.P. Berg, and D. Fiske. The effect of the direction of gaze on the kinematics of the squat exercise. J. Strength Cond. Res. 20(1):145–150. 2006.—The purpose of this study was to determine whether the direction of gaze influences
  • 4. the kinematics of the squat exercise. Ten men experienced in the squat exercise performed a total of 30 repetitions of the squat in the form of 2 sets of 5 repetitions under 3 different conditions. Conditions varied with respect to the direction of the subjects’ gaze as they performed the exercise. Condition D en- tailed gazing downward at the intersection of the facing wall and the floor throughout the exercise. Condition S required subjects to gaze straight ahead at their own reflection (eyes) in the mirror on the wall directly in front of them. Condition U involved gaz- ing upward at the intersection of the facing wall and the ceiling throughout the exercise. Dependent variables included the lin- ear displacement of the bar and hip, linear velocity of the bar, and the angular displacement/position and velocity of the head, trunk, hip, and knee. The mean data were subjected to a re- peated measures analysis of variance, and, where appropriate, pairwise comparisons using Tukey’s Studentized Range Test. The results revealed overall similarity in movement kinematics when performing the squat exercise using the 3 different gaze directions. In particular, the upward and straight gaze condi - tions were not differentiated by the analysis. Conversely, the downward gaze was shown to increase the extent of hip flexion (F[2, 9] 5 4.82, p , .05), especially relative to the upward gaze, and possibly trunk flexion as well (F[2, 9] 5 3.02, p 5 .07). In terms of the practical application, because excessive hip and trunk flexion in the squat are contraindicated, cautioning ath- letes against allowing the head or direction of gaze to drop below a neutral position appears to be warranted. KEY WORDS. weight training, vision, stability INTRODUCTION
  • 5. T he squat exercise involves most major muscle groups of the trunk and legs, and is one of the most beneficial muscle strengthening exercises. The squat not only enhances performance in many sports, but has become an important component of general-purpose weight training programs as well. Be- cause of the number of muscle groups involved, squats also can positively stimulate the cardiovascular system (8, 9, 10). Moreover, when performed correctly, the squat can help prevent injuries (2, 6). To obtain the desired training effects and to ensure safety, proper technique should be used when performing the squat. There is general agreement about what con- stitutes appropriate squat technique, with at least 1 ex- ception. There is some lack of clarity about how the head should be positioned during the squat, or more precisely, where a performer’s gaze should be directed—upward, downward, or straight ahead? According to Fry, using an incorrect head position can cause poor body alignment and predispose an athlete to injury (5). What is the cor- rect head position and/or direction of gaze in the squat? According to Francoeur, lifters should focus on a spot on the wall or ceiling, as this will help to keep the head up throughout the entire motion (4). Global Health and Fitness also recommends looking upward throughout the exercise (7), whereas O’Shea simply recommends keeping the head up (16). Westcott (17) and other professionals instruct exercisers to keep the head neutral throughout the exercise. Indeed, Kelso et al. advise that the head should face straight forward throughout the exercise (12). Some recommendations are more specific, such as that
  • 6. the head and neck should be kept straight with the eyes looking straight ahead (3, 5, 11,). Some professionals warn against allowing the head and eye focus to drop from a neutral position, arguing that this could result in excessive forward lean as the lifter descends (3). Forward lean is a nontechnical de- scriptor of what we will refer to as trunk flexion. Trunk flexion is the forward deviation of the trunk relative to the vertical and can result from hip and knee flexion, as well as ankle dorsiflexion. In short, a head-down position during the squat exercise may increase the likelihood of excessive trunk flexion. Several authors have suggested that excessive trunk flexion during the squat can cause undue stress to be applied to the lumbar spine, and thus increase the chance of injury (3, 5, 14, 15). On the contrary, Hill warns against using a head-up, eyes-up technique, arguing that balance is compromised, increasingly so during descent, making lower back strain more likely (3). Instead, Hill argues that the head and eyes should be straight or slightly down throughout the exercise. The latter recommendation is the only one, to our knowledge, suggesting that a downwardly directed gaze or head position is acceptable. Nonetheless, in spite of the general bias among experts against looking down- ward during the squat, athletes and weight trainers sometimes can be observed doing so. Adding to the uncertainty about appropriate head po- sition and direction of gaze during the squat exercise is the fact that head position and direction of gaze are not synonymous. Ignoring the movement of the body as a whole, humans can redirect gaze by (a) moving the head with the eyes fixed in the head, (b) moving the eyes with the head fixed, or (c) moving the eyes and the head si-
  • 7. multaneously. A recommendation such as O’Shea’s to keep the head up during the squat (16) does not ensure that gaze will be directed upward as well. Of course, head position and direction of gaze likely are related; concern- 146 DONNELLY, BERG, AND FISKE ing recommendations for squat technique, however, it is a mistake to assume they are equivalent. What is it that matters: head position, direction of gaze, both, or neither? Theoretically, the direction of gaze during the squat exercise is potentially important, because it could affect posture and stability. Vision can be critical for movement control during the squat, particularly regarding balance/ stability (1). Accurate perception of self-motion is funda- mental to maintaining stability, and the direction of gaze could influence a lifter’s perception of self-motion. For ex- ample, looking upward toward the ceiling during the squat exercise could decrease sensitivity to information about the position of one’s body relative to the proximal support surface (floor). Information about one’s position relative to the floor is salient because it is useful for de - termining when to terminate the descent portion of the exercise and to begin the ascent. Selecting the correct point to change direction is critical, because a failure to do so could result in a loss of balance, leading to injury of the knee or back, or even causing the athlete to fall. In contrast, directing one’s gaze downward toward the floor during the squat could decrease sensitivity to infor- mation about the position of one’s body relative to a wall or objects directly in front of the lifter. Perceiving ap- proach to or withdrawal from objects and surfaces direct-
  • 8. ly in front of us allows us to perceive forward and back- ward sway, respectively, and is critical for maintaining an upright posture (13). By looking at the floor, a per- former might fail to perceive postural sway in time to retard the forward or backward rotation of the body, which could result in a fall. What about head position during the squat exercise? Theoretically, head position is important because it could possibly affect posture and stability, though more straightforwardly so than direction of gaze influences sta- bility. For example, Fry contends that the head and neck can be considered continuations of the vertebral column (5), implying that head position has a direct effect on body alignment, irrespective of the direction of gaze. It is ob- vious that improper body alignment, such as allowing one’s center of gravity to escape the confines of the base of support, could be detrimental to performance or even dangerous. Because of the lack of agreement concerning head po- sition and direction of gaze in the squat exercise, and giv- en its probable importance to stability and safety during the performance of that exercise, the purpose of this study was to determine whether direction of gaze influ- ences the kinematics of the squat. As previously ex- plained, when referring to the squat exercise, strength and conditioning professionals appear to consider head position and direction of gaze to be synonymous or nearly so, but they are not. We chose to manipulate the direction of gaze rather than head position, because the former is far more easily and precisely controlled. Naturally, anal- yses of the results included the extent to which direction of gaze and head position were associated. We also chose to evaluate anterior/posterior displace-
  • 9. ment of the bar, as well as hip and linear velocity of the bar, as measures of postural sway, and thus, indicators of stability. We also chose to characterize the angular ki - nematics of the trunk, hip, and knee to determine if the direction of gaze affects the overall coordination dynamics of the squat. Because of the potential link between exces- sive trunk flexion, undue stress to the lumbar spine, and injury risk (3, 6, 14, 15), we were particularly interested in evaluating trunk flexion across different gaze direc- tions. Our hypotheses were that a downward gaze would result in a greater trunk flexion, as well as greater pos- tural sway (instability), than either the straight or up- ward gaze conditions. We also anticipated that the up- ward and straight gaze directions would yield similar lin- ear and angular squat kinematics. METHODS Experimental Approach to the Problem A repeated-measures design was used to determine the effect of the direction of gaze (downward, straight ahead, or upward) on the kinematics of the squat exercise. De- pendent variables included the linear displacement of the bar and hip, linear velocity of the bar, and the angular displacement/position and velocity of the head, trunk, hip, and knee. Subjects The subjects for this study consisted of 10 young men (mean age 5 20; SD 5 1.3; range, 18–22 years) who were members of the Miami University football team. Each subject had at least 1 year of experience performing the squat exercise. The subjects ranged in height from 1.78– 1.96 m and ranged in weight from 84–136.2 kg. The in-
  • 10. vestigation was approved by the Miami University Insti- tutional Review Board for Human Subjects. Procedures The apparatus used to perform the squat exercise con- sisted of a power rack, a York Olympic 20.43-kg bar (York Barbell Co., York, PA), and Olympic-sized weight plates. The rack was positioned such that subjects faced a mir- rored wall (at a distance of 1.5 m.) while performing the exercise. Subjects performed a total of 30 repetitions of the squat exercise in the form of 2 sets of 5 repetitions under 3 different conditions. The conditions varied with respect to the direction of the subjects’ gaze as they per - formed the squat. Condition D entailed gazing downward at the intersection of the facing wall and the floor throughout the exercise. Condition S required subjects to gaze straight ahead at their own reflection (eyes) in the mirror on the wall directly in front of them. Condition U involved gazing upward at the intersection of the facing wall and the ceiling (height 5 2.79 m) throughout the exercise. The order in which the conditions were per- formed was counterbalanced across subjects. The inten- sity for each subject was set at 25% of his 1 repetition maximum (1RM) in the squat exercise. The purpose of choosing a relatively light load was to minimize the ex- tent to which fatigue would influence squat kinematics. Normally, one could expect counterbalancing the order in which conditions are presented to completely control for any order effect resulting from fatigue. However, given that the differences in kinematics we could foresee as a result of manipulating the direction gaze were not dra- matic, we choose to try to minimize the effect of fatigue. Three minutes of recovery was afforded between each set of 5 repetitions.
  • 11. A Panasonic WV-D5100HS SVHS video camera (Pan- asonic, Secaucus, NJ) operating at 60 Hz and with a shut- ter speed of 1/250 second was positioned 5.5 m to the right of the subject and was used to record each trial. The cam- DIRECTION OF GAZE IN THE SQUAT 147 FIGURE 1. Reflective marker locations and angle designations. TABLE 1. Mean total linear displacement of the bar and hip.* Condition Downward gaze Straight gaze Upward gaze Mean total horizontal displacement of the bar (m) 0.12 0.11 0.10 SD 0.04 0.05 0.04 Mean total vertical displacement of the bar (m) 0.75 0.74 0.73 SD 0.08 0.09 0.08 Mean total horizontal displacement of the hip (m) 0.14 0.14 0.13 SD 0.02 0.02 0.03 Mean total vertical displacement of the hip (m) 0.39 0.38 0.38 SD 0.08 0.08 0.08 * Results were not statistically significant. era was level, 90 cm from the floor, and was oriented with its optical axis perpendicular to a subject’s sagittal plane of motion. The camera was equipped with a halogen flood lamp that illuminated reflective markers on the subject. As illustrated in Figure 1, the reflective markers were
  • 12. located (a) just below the superciliary arch near the right eye, (b) at the greater trochanter of the right hip, (c) at the lateral articulation of the right knee, (d) at the lateral malleolous of the right ankle, and (e) at the end of the squatting bar. The third trial from each set of 5 repetitions was dig- itized using a motion measurement system from Peak Performance Inc (Vicon-Peak, Centennial, CO). Thus, 6 trials were digitized for each subject (2 for each condi- tion), for a total of 60 trials. The landmarks denoted by the reflective markers were automatically digitized begin- ning with the video frame displaying the first downward movement of the bar, and continued for each of 176 con- secutive frames (2.93 seconds). Once digitizing was com- pleted, the data was smoothed using a Butterworth dig- ital filter, and the dependent variables were computed. Statistical Analyses Variables included the linear displacement of the bar and hip, linear velocity of the bar, and the angular displace- ment/position and velocity of the head, trunk, hip, and knee (refer to Figure 1 for angle designations). To ascer - tain whether there were differences among the 3 condi- tions, the mean data were subjected to a repeated mea- sures analysis of variance (ANOVA), and where appro- priate, pairwise comparisons using Tukey’s Studentized Range Test. The correlation between the direction of gaze and mean head position was computed using a Spear- man’s rho (rank) correlation. An alpha level of 0.05 was used for the statistical tests. RESULTS Linear Kinematics of the Bar and Hip
  • 13. As shown in Table 1, a repeated measures ANOVA failed to reveal differences in the mean total horizontal and ver - tical bar displacements or the mean total horizontal and vertical hip displacements: F(2, 9) 5 0.81, p . 0.05; F(2, 9) 5 1.18, p . 0.05; F(2, 9) 5 0.77, p . 0.05; and F(2, 9) 5 0.87, p . 0.05, respectively. As is evident from the data presented in Table 2, there were likewise no significant differences in peak linear velocity of the bar in the down- ward, upward, forward, or backward directions: F(2, 9) 5 0.74, p . 0.05; F(2, 9) 5 0.53, p . 0.05; F(2, 9) 5 1.04, p . 0.05; and F(2, 9) 5 0.41, p . 0.05, respectively. The lack of differences in the data characterizing horizontal (i.e., anterior-posterior) motion indicates that postural sway, and thus stability, was not affected by the direction of gaze. Angular Kinematics The mean angular head position was computed to deter- mine the extent to which the different gaze conditions influenced head position. The mean head angles are shown in Table 3. The repeated measures ANOVA for head angle was significant (F[2, 9] 5 35.77, p , 0.0001). Pairwise comparisons using Tukey’s Studentized Range Test revealed significant differences between condition D and each of the other conditions (S: difference 5 22.72, 95% confidence limits [CL] 5 13.02, 32.42; U: difference 5 31.05, 95% CL 5 21.35, 40.75). However, the difference 148 DONNELLY, BERG, AND FISKE TABLE 2. Mean peak linear velocity of the bar for 3 gaze directions.*
  • 14. Condition Downward gaze Straight gaze Upward gaze Mean peak velocity of the bar in downward direction (m·s21) 0.92 0.94 0.91 SD 0.15 0.13 0.14 Mean peak velocity of the bar in upward direction (m·s21) 1.06 1.09 1.08 SD 0.09 0.08 0.10 Mean peak velocity of the bar in forward direction (m·s21) 0.15 0.15 0.13 SD 0.04 0.06 0.04 Mean peak velocity of the bar in backward direction (m·s21) 0.16 0.15 0.16 SD 0.05 0.06 0.07 * Results were not statistically significant. TABLE 3. Mean maximum angular displacement of the trunk, hip, knee, and mean angular position of the head for 3 gaze directions.* Condition Downward gaze Straight gaze Upward gaze Mean maximum trunk flexion† (8) 217 213 212 SD 7 12 12 Mean maximum hip flexion‡ (8) 77 84 86 SD 7 15 14 Mean maximum knee flexion (8) 82 83 85 SD 11 12 12 Mean head angle§ (8) 97 75 66 SD 12 5 10
  • 15. * For the trunk and head, larger values represent greater flexion. For the hip and knee, smaller values represent greater flexion. See Figure 1 for angle designations. † P 5 0.07. ‡ p , 0.05 (pairwise comparisons revealed a single significant difference between conditions D and U). § p , 0.0001 (pairwise comparisons revealed significant differences between conditions D and S, and D and U). between conditions S and U did not achieve significance (difference 5 8.33, 95% CL 5 21.37, 18.03). The corre- lation coefficient between direction of gaze and mean head position was 0.79 (p , 0.0001). The results indicate that although direction of gaze and head position were strongly related, they should not be considered equiva- lent. Also apparent in Table 3 is the fact that mean maxi- mum trunk, hip, and knee flexion were each greatest when the direction of gaze was directed downward (con- dition D). (Note that for the trunk, larger values repre- sent greater flexion, whereas for the hip and knee, small - er values represent greater flexion.) The repeated mea- sures ANOVA for mean maximum hip flexion was signif- icant (F[2, 9] 5 4.82, p , 0.05), with the lone significant difference found between conditions D and U. In other words, hip flexion was more severe when using the down- ward gaze than the upward gaze (difference 5 8.32, 95% CL 5 1.05, 15.58). The repeated measures ANOVA for mean maximum trunk flexion approached significance (F[2, 9] 5 3.02, p 5 0.07), with condition D exceeding both conditions S and U by 4 and 58, respectively. In other words, trunk flexion demonstrated a tendency to be more
  • 16. severe when using the downward gaze than when using the straight or upward conditions, but the difference was not statistically significant. The repeated measures AN- OVA for mean maximum knee flexion was not significant (F[2, 9] 5 0.48, p . 0.05), indicating that knee flexion was unaffected by gaze direction. As shown in Table 4, the repeated measures ANOVAs failed to reveal statistically significant differences among conditions for peak trunk, hip, and knee flexion velocity: F(2, 9) 5 1.81, p . 0.05; F(2, 9) 5 0.15, p . 0.05; and F(2, 9) 5 0.28, p . 0.05, respectively. There were likewise no significant differences among conditions for peak trunk, hip, and knee extension velocity: F(2, 9) 5 0.58, p . 0.05; F(2, 9) 5 0.36, p . 0.05; and F(2, 9) 5 2.02, p . 0.05, respectively. DISCUSSION The purpose of this study was to begin to address the lack of clarity about head position or gaze direction during the squat exercise. Based on current technical standards for performing the squat exercise, as well as theoretical im- plications of head position and direction of gaze on pos- tural stability, we hypothesized that a downward gaze (condition D) would result in a greater maximum trunk flexion, as well as greater instability as determined by the total anterior-posterior displacement of the bar and hip. Conversely, we anticipated that an upward and straight- ahead gaze (conditions U and S, respectively) would re- sult in similar linear and angular kinematics. First, however, we evaluated the extent to which the direction of gaze and head position were equivalent. As expected, there was a strong relationship between direc- tion of gaze and mean head position, yet it was not perfect
  • 17. (r 5 0.79, R2 5 62.4%). Strength and conditioning profes- sionals should be aware of the lack of a one-to-one map- ping between direction of gaze and head position, and DIRECTION OF GAZE IN THE SQUAT 149 TABLE 4. Mean peak angular velocity of the trunk, hip, and knee for three gaze directions. Condition Downward gaze Straight gaze Upward gaze Mean peak trunk flexion velocity (8·s21) 45 42 40 SD 8 10 6 Mean peak hip flexion velocity (8·s21) 102 101 100 SD 15 15 13 Mean peak knee flexion velocity (8·s21) 111 108 107 SD 22 21 19 Mean peak trunk extension velocity (8·s21 46 46 44 SD 5 9 7 Mean peak hip extension velocity (8·s21) 114 116 117 SD 14 10 18 Mean peak knee extension velocity (8·s21) 113 116 122 SD 24 23 29 * Results were not statistically significant. should develop instructional cues for the squat exercise accordingly. Because we directly manipulated only the gaze direction in this study, we will refer only to it in the discussion. Our hypotheses that condition D would result in
  • 18. greater total anterior-posterior motion of the bar and hip (postural sway) were not supported. Likewise, mean peak linear velocity of the bar in the downward, upward, for- ward, or backward direction did not differ across condi - tions. In sum, based on measures of total bar and hip displacement, as well as peak bar velocity, we must con- clude that direction of gaze did not influence postural sway, and thus stability, in the squat exercise. Mean maximum trunk flexion was an average of 4.58 greater in condition D than in condition S or U. The sta- tistical analysis on this variable approached significance (p 5 0.07) and thus there is reason to believe that the downward gaze did result in greater trunk flexion than did conditions U and S. Moreover, under condition D, mean maximum hip flexion was 9 and 78 greater than conditions U and S, respectively, with the difference be- tween D and U achieving statistical significance. It is noteworthy that squat depth did not differ among condi- tions (as shown in Table 1—vertical displacement of the hip), nor did maximum knee flexion differ, indicating that the increase in hip flexion observed in condition D was not offset by greater knee flexion (i.e., a deeper squat). Our findings appear to confirm the efficacy of the rec- ommendation made by some strength and conditioning professionals against allowing the head and eye focus to drop below a neutral position as a means of preventing excessive trunk flexion (3). What are the implications of greater trunk flexion in the squat? Of course, some trunk flexion is necessary, but excessive flexion could put an increased torque on the lower back musculature, possibly putting the athlete at greater risk of injuries such as mus- cle strains, disc herniations, and spondylolysis (stress fracture of the vertebral column) (3, 5, 14, 15). The injury risk that is speculated to accompany excessive trunk flex-
  • 19. ion resulting from a downward gaze might be exacerbated if accompanied by a concomitant increase in flexion ve- locity. However, analyses failed to reveal statistically sig- nificant differences among conditions for peak trunk flex- ion velocity. Our study had 2 noteworthy limitations. First, sub- jects in the study were all college football players with a minimum of 1 year of experience performing the squat exercise. Therefore, the results should not necessarily be generalized to younger athletes or those with less expe- rience performing the squat. Second, subjects performed the squat using 25% of their 1RM. It is possible that the effect of gaze direction on movement kinematics could dif- fer from that observed in this study when using a higher intensity exercise. This question will need to be addressed in future research, but for now we must emphasize that the results of this study apply only to low-intensity squat- ting. It is also important to emphasize that the significant and nearly significant differences in mean maximum hip and trunk flexion, respectively, were found between con- ditions D and U only. Thus, it is important to be precise in our conclusion that the downward gaze resulted in greater hip and trunk flexion than the upward gaze, but not greater than the straight gaze. PRACTICAL APPLICATIONS This study of male collegiate football players revealed overall similarity in movement kinematics when perform- ing the squat exercise using 3 different gaze directions: upward, straight, and downward. Our study was unable to differentiate between the straight and upward gaze di - rections. Conversely, the downward gaze was shown to increase the extent of hip flexion and possibly trunk flex-
  • 20. ion as well, especially relative to the upward gaze. Be- cause excessive hip and trunk flexion in the squat are contraindicated, cautioning athletes against allowing the head or direction of gaze to drop below a neutral position appears to be warranted. REFERENCES 1. BENNETT, S., AND K. DAVIDS. The manipulation of vision dur- ing the powerlift squat: Exploring the boundaries of the spec- ificity of learning hypothesis. Res. Q. Exerc. Sport 66:210–218. 1995. 2. CHANDLER, T.J., AND M.H. STONE. The squat exercise in ath- letic conditioning: A position statement and review of litera- ture. Strength Cond. J. 13(5):51–60. 1991. 3. FAIRCHILD, D., B. HILL, M. RITCHIE, AND D. SOCHOR. Round- table: Common technique errors in the back squat. Strength Cond. J. 15(3):20–27. 1993. 4. FRANCOEUR, J.P. Squat. Available at: http://www.jeanpaul. com/squat.html. Accessed July 14, 2004. 5. FRY, A. Coaching considerations for the barbell squat—Part I. Strength Cond. J. 15(2):60–65. 1993. 150 DONNELLY, BERG, AND FISKE 6. FRY, A.C., C. SMITH, AND B.K. SCHILLING. Effect of
  • 21. knee posi- tion on hip and knee torques during the barbell squat. J. Strength Cond. Res.. 17:629–633. 2003. 7. GLOBAL HEALTH AND FITNESS. Legs. Available at: http:// www.global-fitness.com/musclemap/mmpquads.html. Accessed July 12, 2004. 8. GOLDBERG, L., D.L. ELLIOT, AND K.S. KUEHL. Cardiovascular changes at rest and during mixed static and dynamic exercises after weight training. J. Appl. Sport Sci. Res. 2(3):42–45. 1988. 9. GOLDBERG, L., D.L. ELLIOT, AND K.S. KUEHL. A comparison of the cardiovascular effects of running and weight training. J. Strength Cond. Res. 8:219–224. 1994. 10. GOTSHALK, L., R. BERGER, AND W. KRAEMER. Cardiovascular responses to a high-volume continuous circuit resistance train- ing protocol. J. Strength Cond. Res. 18:760–764. 2004. 11. GRAHAM, J. Back squat. Strength Cond. J. 23(5):28–29. 2001. 12. KELSO, T., J. GAMBLE, B. HILL, K. BARNES, AND D. PASANELLA. The back squat. Strength Cond. J. 11(2):18–24. 1989. 13. LEE, D.N., AND J.R. LISHMAN. Visual proprioceptive control of stance. J. Hum. Mov. Stud. 1(2):87–95. 1975. 14. MCLAUGHLIN, T., R. LARDNER, AND C. DILLMAN.
  • 22. Kinetics of the parallel squat. Res. Quart. 49:175–189. 1978. 15. NEITZEL, J., AND G. DAVIES. The benefits and controversy of the parallel squat in strength training and rehabilitation. Strength Cond. J. 22(3):30–37. 2000. 16. O’SHEA, P. The parallel squat. Strength Cond. J. 7(1):4–6. 1985. 17. WESTCOTT, W.L. Strength Fitness: Physiological Principles and Training Techniques (4th ed.). Quincy, MA: McGraw-Hill. 1994. Acknowledgments We would like to thank Michael Hughes for his contribution to this study. Address correspondence to William P. Berg, [email protected] muohio.edu.