SlideShare a Scribd company logo
~ 1 ~
The effect of anthropogenic noise on shoaling cohesion in three-
spined sticklebacks (Gasterosteus aculeatus)
A report submitted in part fulfilment of the examination requirements for the award of a B.Sc.
(Hons) Animal Behaviour and Welfare awarded by the University of Lincoln, June 2014,
supervised by Tom Pike.
Thomas Trew
12357944
~ 2 ~
CERTIFICATE OF ORIGINALITY
This is to certify that I am responsible for the work submitted in this thesis, that the original
work is my own, except as specified in the acknowledgements and in references, and that
neither the thesis nor the original work contained therein has been previously submitted to any
institution for a degree.
Signature:
Name:
Date:
CERTIFICATE OF COMPLIANCE
This is to certify that this project has been carried out in accordance with University principles
regarding ethics and health and safety. Forms are available to view on request.
Signature:
Name:
Date:
~ 3 ~
Abstract
Noise pollution in both terrestrial and aquatic habitats has considerably increased over the
last few decades, and there is growing evidence that anthropogenic noise can affect
behaviour in a variety of species. There are increasing concerns that this may affect the
fitness of many animals. A majority of fish species spend a part of their life in shoals,
which are important for both predator protection and group foraging. Therefore the aim of
this study was to assess the effect on anthropogenic noise on shoaling cohesion. To do this,
three-spined sticklebacks (Gasterosteus aculeatus) were used in a tank based playback
experiment. Results showed that during exposure to anthropogenic noise, sticklebacks
would group closer together, essentially mirroring behaviour seen in shoals when
threatened by a predator. The findings of this study suggested anthropogenic noise has the
potential to influence anti-predatory behaviour and vigilance of shoals. Future research is
required for more in depth analyses of the impact anthropogenic noise has on direct fitness.
Keywords
Anthropogenic, noise, pollution, Three-spined stickleback, Gasterosteus aculeatus, shoal,
cohesion, behaviour.
Word count
3464.
~ 4 ~
Introduction
The human population is ever growing, and so is its impact on wildlife (Chan et al., 2010).
One factor being the amount of noise pollution we produce, from activities such as urban
development, expansion of transport networks and resource extraction (Wale et al., 2013).
Past studies have shown increasing evidence that anthropogenic (man made) noise can
affect the behaviour of a wide and diverse range of animals (Normandeau Associates, Inc,
2012; Morely et al., 2014; Slabbekoorn and Ripmeester, 2008; Barber et al., 2010).
Anthropogenic noise, especially when loud, persistent, unexpected or novel, has the
potential to cause stress (Wysocki et al., 2006; Wright et al., 2007), to distract (Chan et al.,
2010) and to mask important sounds (Wale et al., 2013; Siemers and Schaub, 2011; Lowry
et al., 2012).
For example, Habib et al. (2007) conducted a study on male ovenbirds (Seiurus
aurocapilla); their results showed that males on louder territories were less likely to attract
a mate than those based at quieter locations. They hypothesized that noise pollution was
interfering with male ovenbirds songs, which could reduce the distance to which females
can hear the songs, or give the perception the male is of lower quality due to distortions of
song characteristics. If female ovenbirds were then to base mate choice on noise conditions
of the males’ territory over size, age and body condition of the male ovenbird; then male
mate pairing success may be negatively affected by anthropogenic noise.
Another example of animal communication being affected by increasing noise pollution
can be seen in the Sun and Narins (2005) study on frogs. They observed that certain
species of pond-dwelling frogs, which were particularly acoustically active, would reduce
their call rate when exposed to playbacks of traffic noise. These results suggest that the
frogs changed their calling behaviour to avoid auditory masking.
Anti-predator vigilance may also be increased by animals when exposed to loud noises.
Rabin et al. (2006) observed that in California ground squirrels (Spermophilus beecheyi)
the individuals located in areas around loud wind turbines exhibited higher rates of
vigilance when hearing alarm calls of conspecifics than those in quieter areas.
It has also been suggested that anti-predator behaviour may be affected by anthropogenic
noise (Chan et al., 2010; Karp and Root, 2009; Wale et al., 2013). Predator avoidance is
crucial for animals survivability and reproduction success (Caro, 2005); and yet there are
few studies that have investigated the potential effect of anthropogenic noise on anti-
predatory behaviour (Voellmy et al., 2014). Chan et al. (2010) observed that whilst
~ 5 ~
exposed to anthropogenic noise, simulated predators could get significantly closer to the
Caribbean hermit crabs (Coenobita clypeatus) before they retreated into their shells, in
comparison to the silent trials. They proposed the ‘Distracted prey hypothesis’, which
states that any stimulus an animal can perceive is capable of distracting it by reallocating
part of its finite attention and thus preventing it from responding accordingly to an
approaching threat. Attention is the process by which an individual filters out several
stimuli from the surrounding environment, letting in only as much as it can process
(Bushnell, 1998). Chan et al. (2010) then went on to suggest that anthropogenic noise
could be a distracting stimulus, and could reduce an animal’s fitness by increased
vulnerability to predators.
Another theory as to how anthropogenic noise may affect anti-predatory behaviour is that
the additional noise could mask important acoustic cues produced by predators, or warning
calls from conspecifics (Wale et al., 2013). Prey are often alerted to the presence of
predators by auditory cues, from acts such as intraspecific communication or the
inadvertent sound produced from movement (Barrera et al., 2011; Siemers and Schaub,
2011). Furthermore, alarm calls have evolved into a wide range of animals to warn others
of imminent danger (Hollen and Radford, 2009); other vocalizations can be used to
provide information on the level of risk (Hollen et al., 2008; Bell et al., 2009). Masking
can either be described as ‘energetic’, whereby the masking is complete and the signal is
not detected at all, or ‘informational’, where the signal can be detected by the listener,
however the content is hard to understand (Wale et al., 2013). Either way, there is likely
consequences to foraging and anti-predator behaviour from a reduced ability to detect
valuable auditory information (Brumm and Slabbekoorn, 2005; Siemers and Schaub, 2011;
Lowry et al., 2012).
Fish populations have been negatively impacted by human population growth for a number
of well known reasons (Slabbekoorn et al., 2010) such as fisheries (Jackson et al., 2001),
habitat degradation (Munday, 2004) and chemical pollution (Van Der Oost et al., 2003).
Underwater anthropogenic noise pollution could potentially be another big threat to fish
populations (McDonald et al., 2006). The amount of fresh water and marine noise
pollution produced by humans has been increasing significantly over the last few decades
(Codarin et al., 2009), not just around coastal areas, but also in the open ocean (Andrew et
al., 2002; Tyack, 2008). Sound travels approximately 5 times faster in water than it does in
air (around 1500m/s in water – 300m/s in air), meaning that sound wavelengths are also
~ 6 ~
about 5 times longer in water than they are in air (e.g. 100Hz signal = 15m in water - 3m in
air). This results in sound traveling greater distances and at higher amplitude levels in
water, enabling long distance communication, but also resulting in long distance noise
pollution (Slabbekoorn et al., 2010). For example, anthropogenic noise produced during
pile-driving for wind turbines can be detected over background underwater noise levels
from as far as 70km away (Bailey et al., 2010). This increase of sound amplitude and area
of effect makes underwater anthropogenic noise pollution a real cause for concern
(Slabbekoorn et al., 2010). To this date there’s only one study I am aware of that assesses
the impact of underwater anthropogenic noise on anti-predatory behaviour in fish.
Voellmy et al. (2014) used both European minnow (Phoxinus phoxinus) and three-spined
sticklebacks (Gasterosteus aculeatus) in their study. They observed increased vigilance in
sticklebacks and faster response rates to a simulated predator when exposed to
anthropogenic noise; whereas minnows did not significantly differ in response rate
depending on noise exposure. However, both three-spined sticklebacks and European
minnow are shoaling fish (Huntingford and Ruiz-Gomez, 2009; Ward and Krause, 2001).
Voellmy et al. (2014) only tested the effect of anthropogenic noise on individual anti-
predatory behaviour, even though both species of fish spend a large proportion of their
lives as part of a shoal.
A majority of fish species spend some part of their lives in groups (Shaw, 1978); these
groups of fish are commonly termed as ‘shoals’ (Miller and Gerlai, 2012; Kenedy and
Pitcher, 1975). Predators and food have been said to be the two fundamental keys of
understanding fish shoals (Pitcher, 1993). In order to survive and reproduce successfully,
animals must minimize the risk of starvation and predation (Wale et al., 2013). Living in
shoals is very important for defence against predation; synchronized co-operation confuses
predators and reduces the threat of a successful attack (Pitcher, 1993). Past research has
already shown that anthropogenic noise can have a negative impact on shoaling fish’s
foraging efficiency (Purser and Radford, 2011); however no research that I am aware of
has been done on the effect it has on shoal anti-predatory behaviour. Once a predator is
detected, predator defence takes precedence over food (Pitcher, 1993); suggesting the
results of Purser and Radford’s (2011) study with three-spined sticklebacks was due to the
fish choosing predator defence over food. In the presence of a predator, shoal cohesion
increases (distance between each fish reduces) (Ryer and Olla, 1998); which is why in this
study, I aim to test the effects on anthropogenic noise on shoaling cohesion using three-
~ 7 ~
spined sticklebacks, a small, robust and common fish species which acts as an excellent
model for the scientific study of behaviour and acclimates well to laboratory conditions
(Purse and Radford, 2011; Huntingford and Ruiz-Gomez, 2009). If Purse and Radford’s
(2011) results were, as I suggested, due to the fish displaying anti-predator behaviour; then
I would expect to see that the exposure of anthropogenic noise will result in increase shoal
cohesion.
Methods
Thirty adult three-spined sticklebacks were used as subjects in this study. The Fish used
were sourced from a local drainage ditch off the river Till (grid reference: -53.309418, -
0.651873) in October 2014 and housed in University of Lincolns Minster House aquatics
laboratory, in an 8+/-1°C chilled room; the light timer for this room was set to match the
natural daylight times at the river from which the fish were caught. Prior to the study all
fish were housed in the same 64-L tank, fitted with an Eheim water filter, filled with 45L
of filtered, aerated and chilled tap water and containing artificial plastic plants for shelter.
They were fed on a frozen bloodworm (Chironomid larvae) diet till satiation daily. Testing
shoals consisted of 3 fish; each fish was tested only once, allowing for 10 tests, all with
unique shoals. Three fish would be randomly selected and transferred, using a net, into a
circular (31cm diameter) grey testing tub, filled with 7cm of water. Once in the testing tub,
the fish were left for a 30 minute habituation period. After habituation, the testing period
began, which lasted 1 hour and 30 minutes; consisting of a 30 minute ‘silent’ control trial,
followed by a 30 minute noise exposure trial and ending with another 30 minute ‘silent’
control trail. The silent control trials gave a baseline measure of distance between fish in
ambient conditions (Purser and Radford, 2011; Chan et al., 2010). During the noise
exposure trial the fish were exposed to a playback of white noise for the full 30 minutes;
played through two 10 watt Mila 2.0 speakers, placed opposite each other facing towards
the centre of the testing tub; bandwidth was limited between 100 and 1000 HZ, presenting
frequencies falling under the sticklebacks likely hearing range (Purser and Radford, 2011).
The volume was consistent across all trials and was, to my hearing, set to a level that was
not completely biologically unrealistic to be heard around shorelines with boat activity.
Throughout the whole testing period a Microsoft 2.0 megapixel camera mounted 55cm
directly over the grey testing tub was being used to manually take pictures (via a Dell
latitude x300 notebook) approximately every 60 seconds, giving an aerial view of the fish
~ 8 ~
and their individual locations in the tub. This resulted in 90 images for each shoal, and 900
images in total. After each test, the 3 fish used were moved to another 64-L tank so as to
ensure the same fish were not used more than once; the testing tub would then be emptied
and refilled with fresh water, in preparation for the next shoal.
All 900 images taken during the 10 tests were analysed on Windows 7 Microsoft Paint,
allowing each fish to be allocated coordinates on each picture. The coordinates used were
manually worked out and based at the tip of each individual fish snout. The distance (D)
between two individual fish from each other could be calculated in every image using the
following formula:
where x1 and y1 stands for the X and Y axis coordinates of one fish, x2 and y2 represents
the X and Y axis coordinates for the second fish of which you calculate the distance
between. The average distance between all 3 individuals during the first control, noise
exposure, and second control trials were calculated for each shoal, resulting in 10 averages
of distance between fish for each trial. If my prediction that anthropogenic noise effects
shoaling cohesion is correct, we would expect to see a significant difference in distance
between individuals during the sound trial, when compared to the two control trials. To
examine this I conducted a general linear model with the average distance between fish as
the dependent variable, trial type (pre control, noise exposure and post control) as a fixed
factor, whilst controlling for non independence between the 10 shoals by including shoal
identity as a random effect. I also conducted a post-hoc Tukey test to examine pairwise
differences between trial types.
~ 9 ~
Results
Throughout the whole 90 minutes, the cohesiveness of the shoals differed significantly
(General linear model, F2,18 = 8.70, P = 0.002; Figure 1). Specifically, the post-hoc Tukey
tests revealed that control 1 differed from the sound treatment (T = -3.917, df = 18, P =
0.0028), control 2 differed from the sound treatment (T = -3.199, df = 18, P = 0.0131),
whereas control 1 and control 2 did not differ from each other (T = -0.718, df = 18, P =
0.7563).
Fig 1. Mean ± SE distance (mm) between fish during the three trials types.
0
20
40
60
80
100
120
140
Distance(mm)
Control 1 Sound Control 2
Trial type
~ 10 ~
Discussion
The results of this study show that anthropogenic noise impacts shoaling cohesion of three-
spined stickleback shoals. The addition of white noise elicited the same anti-predatory
defensive behaviour (increased shoal cohesion) that is shown in shoals when in the
presence of a predator (Ryer and Olla, 1998). By using two control trials, before and after
the noise exposure trial, the results also showed that there was no difference in shoal
cohesion between the two controls; suggesting that the noise treatment had no lasting
effect on the fish.
The increase in shoal cohesion when exposed to anthropogenic noise mirrors the grouping
behaviour seen in shoals under predatory threat (Ryer and Olla, 1998). Grouping together
has several mechanisms that reduce predation risk (Ioannou et al., 2008), including the
confusion effect (Miller, 1922), the dilution effect (Foster and Treherne, 1981) and the
‘many eyes’ hypothesis (Lima, 1995). The ‘many eyes’ hypothesis states that as part of a
group, the task of scanning the environment for predators is spread out to multiple
individuals; this collaborate effort results in higher levels of vigilance (Lima, 1995). An
increase in vigilance is a result that has been seen in previous studies of noise effect on
terrestrial wildlife (Quinn et al., 2006; Rabin et al., 2006), and in individual sticklebacks
(Voellmy et al., 2014). The increase in shoal cohesion and general alertness of the fish
may be the result of a stress response caused by the noise exposure (Wright et al., 2007;
Charmandari et al., 2005). Alternatively, grouping together may be to benefit from the
‘many eyes’ hypothesis in compensation for potential masking of predator auditory cues
(such as intraspecific communication and inadvertent sound produced from movement)
(Siemers and Schaub, 2011), by relying more on visual stimulus of the environment to
detect possible threats (Rabin et al., 2006). Increased vigilance often results in faster anti-
predatory responses (Voellmy et al., 2014); a reduced latency response could be a direct
benefit to fitness (Siemers and Schuab, 2011). Krause and Godin (1996) observed this
increase in fitness in Guppies (Poecilia reticulate) when prayed upon by blue acara
cichlids (Aequidens pulcher). They found that vigilant guppies detected cichlids sooner
and initiated flight responses earlier than foraging individuals. The vigilant guppies were
more likely to escape predation, resulting in greater fitness for the individuals responding
fastest, because after all ‘an animal captured and consumed by a predator has its
cumulative fitness abruptly terminated’ Ryer and Olla (1998). However, responding faster
reduces the time spent on risk assessment, which may result in suboptimal decision
~ 11 ~
making, such as unnecessary flight response to a low/no threat situation (Voellmy et al.,
2014). In the case of fleeing individuals seeking shelter, and subsequently spending time
hiding from a stimuli of low/no threat, not only is unnecessary energy spent fleeing, but
the time wasted hiding may also lead to lost opportunities to forage or reproduce
(Ydenberg and Dill, 1986). Additionally, constant forgoing of food for unnecessary anti-
predatory behaviour will eventually result in the individual weakening, resulting in them
being more vulnerable to predation. Furthermore, in fish, predation risk is typically
negatively correlated with the size of an individual (Sogard, 1997), slow growing
individuals may be under more intense predation for a longer period of time, than those
that are well fed and therefore grow more rapidly (Houde, 1987). Attempts to compensate
for lost foraging time may also results in increased predation risk, if animals are then
forced to forage during times with increased predatory threat (Lima and Dill, 1990).
Chan et al. (2010) suggested anthropogenic noise could reallocate the attention of prey,
increasing their susceptibility to predation. The Caribbean hermit crabs (Chan et al., 2010)
and the shore crab (Carcinus maenas) (Wale et al., 2013) have exhibited this ‘distracted’
behaviour when exposed to anthropogenic noise; the noise exposure caused delayed anti-
predator response in simulated predator attacks. Krause and Godin (1996) suggested that
predators take advantage of less vigilant prey and are more likely to attack individuals that
exhibit signs of distraction. Predator bias of less vigilant prey, along with delayed response
caused by anthropogenic noise, may increase vulnerability to predation (Chan et al., 2010).
However, past studies have shown evidence that the same anthropogenic noise can have
different impacts on different species (Francis et al., 2011; Rios-Chelen et al., 2012). The
results of my study and Voellmy et al. (2014) suggest three-spined sticklebacks increase
their vigilance when exposed to anthropogenic noise. Therefore whilst the distraction
hypothesis is a valid theory for some species, sticklebacks may be one of many fish
species that this theory does not apply to. On the other hand, as anthropogenic noise
becomes more frequent, animals response to it may weaken due to habituation (Thompson
and Spencer, 1966), a common process to many animals (Chance and Wash, 2006;
Nowacek et al., 2007). If habituation was to occur in sticklebacks, then I would presume
their vigilance to noise stimuli would weaken, perhaps not only to anthropogenic noise, but
the auditory cues of predators as well, increasing their vulnerability to predation. Species
specific habituation to anthropogenic noise could be a good area for future research.
~ 12 ~
Whilst the results of this study showed a significant difference in distance between fish
during the noise trial in comparison to both control trials; it is also important to note that
there was no significant difference between the pre and post controls, suggesting that there
was no lasting effect of the noise treatment. This could mean if underwater anthropogenic
noise pollution was reduced, there would be rapid changes to fish anti-predatory
behaviour. However, as mentioned before, anthropogenic noise impacts species in
different ways (Francis et al., 2011; Rios-Chelen et al., 2012), and three-spined
sticklebacks have been described as behaviourally robust, in the sense that they settle
quickly after a disturbance (Huntingford and Ruiz-Gomez, 2009); just because
sticklebacks show no lasting behavioural changes, does not mean the same can be said for
all fish species.
Although it is clear anthropogenic sound has an effect on shoaling cohesion, I can only
speculate the impact this change in shoaling behaviour has on predator vulnerability and
individual fitness. In order to gain a better understanding on how anthropogenic noise
effects anti-predator defence in shoals, further research needs to be conducted including a
simulated predator and assessing change of response latency in shoals when exposed to
noise. Additionally, care is always needed when interpreting results from a tank based
playback experiment into a real world context (Wale et al., 2013). The use of tanks is to
ensure tight control of potential confounding factors; however tank playbacks cannot
replicate natural sound fields perfectly (Okumura et al., 2002). This being said, given the
lack of studies examining the impact of anthropogenic noise on shoaling behaviours, even
preliminary evidence is potentially valuable.
Conclusion
This study has provided evidence that anthropogenic noise pollution influences shoal
cohesion in three-spined sticklebacks. The results showed that individuals grouped closer
together during noise exposure, which is similar behaviour to that seen in fish under
predatory threat. A possible implication of this may be reduced latency in flight response
to predators, at the cost of time spent assessing the level of risk, potentially resulting in
wasted energy and reduced foraging efficiency; which mirrors results seen in previous
studies assessing the effects of anthropogenic noise on sticklebacks (Voellmy et al., 2014;
Purser and Radford, 2011). However, without a simulated predator threat, this evidence
allows us to only predict how shoaling fish may react to a predator whilst exposed to
~ 13 ~
anthropogenic sounds. What is clear from this study is anthropogenic noise does have an
impact on shoaling behaviour in fish and that further research is required in order to truly
quantify the direct fitness consequences. Furthermore, laboratory and tank based studies
create an artificial scenario, so care is needed when translating results into real world
context. The ideal context for future studies would be to assess potential effects of
anthropogenic noise that have direct consequences to fitness, but in the wild with real
noise sources and natural sound fields, allowing the spatial scale of impact and severity of
fitness consequences to be determined.
Acknowledgements
I’d like to thank Tom Pike for all his help and supervision as my tutor. Many warm thanks
to Helen Edwards for making the cold hours of data collection fly by. Finally, thanks to
Danielle Derrick and Matthew Hale for constant moral support and assistance with data
input.
~ 14 ~
References
Andrew, R.K. Howe, B.M. Mercer, J.A. and Dzieciuch, M.A. (2002) Ocean ambient
sound: comparing the 1960s with the 1990s for a receiver off the California coast.
Acoustics Research Letters Online, 3(2) 65-70.
Bailey, H. Senior, B. Simmons, D. Rusin, J. Picken, G. and Thompson, P.M. (2010)
Assessing underwater noise levels during pile-driving at an offshore windfarm and its
potential effects on marine mammals. Marine Pollution Bulletin, 60, 888-897.
Barber, J.R. Crooks, K.R. and Fristrup, K.M. (2010) The costs of chronic noise exposure
for terrestrial organisms. Trends in Ecology and Evolution, 25(3) 180-189.
Barrera, J.P. Chong, L. Judy, K.N. and Blumstein, D.T. (2011) Reliability of public
information: predators provide more information about risk than conspecifics. Animal
Behaviour, 81(4) 779-787.
Bell, M.B.V. Radford, A.N. Rose, R. Wade, H.M. and Ridley, A.R. (2009) The value of
constant surveillance in risky environment. Proceedings of the Royal Society B, 276, 2997-
3005.
Brumm, H. and Slabbekoorn, H. (2005) Acoustic communication in noise. Advances in the
Study of Behavior, 35, 151-209.
Bushnell, P.J. (1998) Behavioral approaches to the assessment of attention in animals.
Psychopharmacology, 138(3) 231-259.
Caro, T. (2005) Antipredator defenses in birds and mammals. Chicago: The University of
Chicago press.
Chan, A.A.Y-H. Giraldo-Perez, P. Smith, S. and Blumstein, D.T. (2010) Anthropogenic
noise affects risk assessment and attention: the distracted prey hypothesis. Biology Letters,
6, 458-461.
Charmandari, E. Tsigos, C. and Chrousos, G. (2005) Endocrinology of the stress response.
Annu Rev Pyschol, 67, 259-284.
~ 15 ~
Codarin, A. Wysocki, L.E. Ladich, F. and Picciulin, M. (2009) Effects of ambient and boat
noise on hearing and communication in three fish species living in a marine protected area
(Miramare, Italy). Marine Pollution Bulletin, 58, 1880-1887.
Foster, W.A. and Treherne, J.E. (1981) Evidence for the dilution effect in the selfish herd
from fish predation on a marine insect. Nature, 293, 466-467.
Francis, C.D. Ortega, C.P. and Cruz, A. (2011) Different behavioural responses to
anthropogenic noise by two closely related passerine birds. Biology Letters, 7, 850-852.
Habib, L. Bayne, E.B. and Boutin, S. (2007) Chronic industrial noise affects pairing
success and age structure of ovenbirds Seiurus aurocapilla. Journal of Applied Ecology,
44(1) 176-184.
Hollen, L.I. Bell, M.B.V. and Radford, A.N. (2008) Cooperative sentinel calling? Foragers
gain increased biomass intake. Current Biology, 18, 576-579.
Hollen, L.I. and Radford, A.N. (2009) The development of alarm call behaviour in
mammals and birds. Animal Behaviour, 78, 791-800.
Houde, E.D. (1987) Fish early life dynamics and recruitment variability. American
Fisheries Society Symposium Series, 2, 17-29.
Huntingford, F.A. and Ruiz-Gomez, M.L. (2009) Three-spined sticklebacks Gasterosteus
aculeatus as a model for exploring behavioural biology. Journal of Fish Biology, 75(8)
1943-1976.
Ioannou, C.C. Tosh, C.R. Neville, L. and Krause, J. (2008) The confusion effect – from
neural networks to reduce predation risk. Behavioural Ecology, 19, 126-130.
~ 16 ~
Jackson, J.B.C. Kirby, M.X. Berger, W.H., Bjorndal, K.A. Botsford, L.W. Bourque, B.J.
Bradbury, R.H. Cooke, R. Erlandson, J. Estes, J.A. Hughes, T.P. Kidwell, S. Lange, C.B.
Lenihan, H.S. Pandolfi, J.M. Peterson, C.H. Steneck, R.S. Tegner, M.J. and Warner, R.R.
(2001) Historical overfishing and the recent collapse of coastal ecosystems. Science,
293(5530) 629-638.
Karp, D.S. and Root, T.L. (2009) Sound the stressor: how hoatzins (Opisthocomus hoazin)
react to ecotourist conversation. Biodiversity and Conservation, 18(14) 3733-3742
Kenedy, G.J.A. and Pitcher, T.J. (1975) Experiments on homing in shoals of European
Minnow, Phoxinus Phoxinus (L.). Transactions of the American Fisheries Society, 104,
452-5.
Krause, J. and Godin, J.J. (1996) Influence of prey foraging posture on flight behaviour
and predation risk: predators take advantage of unwary prey. Behav Ecol. 7, 264-271.
Lima, S.L. (1995) Back to the basics of anti-predatory vigilance: The group size effect.
Animal behaviour, 49(1) 11-20.
Lima, S.L. and Dill, L.M. (1990) Behavioral decisions made under the risk of predation: a
review and prospectus. Canadian Journal of Zoology, 68(4) 619-640.
Lowry, H. Lill, A. and Wong, B.B.M. (2012) How noisy does a noisy miner have to be?
Amplitude adjustments of alarm calls in an avian urban ‘adaptor’. PLoS ONE, 7(1)
e29960.
McDonald, M.A. Hildebrand, J.A. and Wiggins, S.M. (2006) Increases in deep ocean
ambient noise in the Northeast Pacific west of San Nicolas Island, California. Acoustical
Society of America, 120(2) 711-718.
Miller, R.C. (1922) The significance of the gregarious habit. Ecology, 3, 122-126.
Miller, N. and Gerlai, R. (2012) From schooling to shoaling: Patterns of collective motion
in Zebrafish (Danio rerio). PLoS ONE, 7(11) e48865.
~ 17 ~
Morely, E.L. Jones, G. and Radford, A.N. (2014) The importance of invertebrates when
considering the impacts of anthropogenic noise. Proceedings of Royal Society B,
281(1776) 20132683.
Munday, P.L. (2004) Habitat loss, resource specialization, and extinction on coral reefs.
Global Change Biology, 10(10) 1642-1647.
Normandeau Associates, Inc. 2012. Effects of Noise on Fish, Fisheries, and Invertebrates
in the U.S. Atlantic and Arctic from Energy Industry Sound-Generating Activities. A
Workshop Report for the U.S. Dept. of the Interior, Bureau of Ocean Energy Management.
Contract # M11PC00031. 72 pp. plus Appendices. Available:
http://www.cbd.int/doc/meetings/mar/mcbem-2014-01/other/mcbem-2014-01-submission-
boem-04-en.pdf [Accessed 14 March 2015].
Okumura, T. Akamatsu, T. and Yan, H.Y. (2002) Analyses of small tank acoustics:
empirical and theoretical approaches. Bioacoustics, 12, 330-332.
Pitcher, T.J. (1993). The behaviour of Teleost fishes. 2nd edition. London: Chapman &
Hall.
Purser, J. and Radford, A.N. (2011) Acoustic noise induces attention shifts and reduces
foraging performance in Three-spined Sticklebacks (Gasterosteus aculeatus). PLoS ONE,
6(2) e17478.
Quinn, J.L Whittingham, M.J. Butler, S.J. and Cresswell, W. (2006) Noise, predation risk
compensation and vigilance in the chaffinch Fringilla coelebs. Journal of Avian Biology,
37(6) 601-608.
Rabin, L.A. Cross, R.G. and Owings, D.H. (2006) The effects of wind turbines on
antipredator behavior in California ground squirrels (Spermophilus beecheyi). Biology
Conservation, 131, 410-420
Rios-Chelen, A.A. Salaberria, C. Barbosa, I. Macias, G.C. and Gil, D. (2012) The learning
advantage: bird species that learn their song show a tighter adjustment of song to noisy
environments than those that do not learn. Journal of Evolutionary Biology, 25(11) 2171-
2180.
~ 18 ~
Ryer, C.H. and Olla, B.L. (1998) Shifting the balance between foraging and predator
avoidance: the importance of food distribution for a schooling pelagic forager.
Environmental Biology of Fish, 52, 467-475.
Shaw, E. (1978) Schooling fishes. Animal Sciences, 66, 166-175.
Siemers, B.M. and Schaub, A. (2011) Hunting at the highway: traffic noise reduces
foraging efficiency in acoustic predators. Proceedings of the Royal Society B, 282(1805)
1646-1652.
Slabbekoorn, H. Bouton, N. Opzeeland, I.V. Coers, A. Cate, C.T. and Popper, A.N. (2010)
A noisy spring: the impact of globally rising underwater sound levels on fish. Trends Ecol
Evol, 25, 419-427.
Slabbekoorn, H. and Ripmeester, E.A.P (2008) Birdsong and anthropogenic noise:
implications and applications for conservation. Molecular Ecology, 17(1) 72-83.
Sogard, S.M. (1997) Size-selective mortality in the juvenile stage of teleost fishes: a
review. Bulletin of Marine Science, 60(29) 1129-1157.
Sun, J.W.C. and Narins, P.M. (2005) Anthropogenic sounds differentially affect amphibian
call rate. Biology Conservation, 121, 419-427.
Thompson, R.F. and Spencer, W.A. (1966) Habituation: a model phenomenon for the
study of neuronal substrates of behaviour. Psychol Rev, 73, 16-43.
Tyack, P.L. (2008) Implication for marine mammals of large-scale changes in the marine
acoustic environment. Journal of Mammalogy, 89(3) 549-558.
Van der Oost, R. Beyer, J. and Vermeulen, N.P.E. (2003) Fish bioaccumulation and
biomarkers in environmental risk assessment: a review. Environmental Toxicology and
Pharmacology, 13(2) 57-149.
Voellmy, I.K. Purser, J. Simpson, S.D, and Radford, A.N. (2014) Increased noise levels
have different impacts on the anti-predatory behaviour of two sympatric fish species. PLoS
ONE, 9(7) e102946.
Wale, M.A. Simpson, S.D. and Radford, A.N. (2013) Noise negatively affects foraging
and antipredator behaviour in shore crabs. Animal Behaviour, 86, 111-118.
~ 19 ~
Ward, A.J.W. and Krause, J. (2001) Body length assortative shoaling in European
minnow, Phoxinus phoxinus. Animal Behaviour, 62(4) 617-621.
Wright, A.J. Soto, N.A. Baldwin, A.I. Bateson, M. Beal, C.M. Charlotte, C. Deak, T.
Edwards, E.F. Fernadez, A. Godinho, A. Hatch, L.T. Kakuschke, A. Lusseau, D.
Martineau, D. Romero, L.M. Weilgart, L.S. Wintle, B.A. Notarbartolo-di-Sciara, G. and
Martin, V. (2007) Anthropogenic noise as a stressor in animals: a multidisciplinary
perspective. International Journal of Comparative Pyschology, 20, 250-273.
Wysocki, L.E. Dittami, J.P. and Ladich, F. Ship noise and cortisol secretion in European
freshwater fishes. Biological Conservation, 128, 501-508.
Ydenber, R.C. and Dill, L.M. (1986) The economics of fleeing from predators. Advances
in the Study of Behavior, 16, 229-249.

More Related Content

What's hot

Keeley et al_02-15
Keeley et al_02-15Keeley et al_02-15
Keeley et al_02-15Kate
 
Ecology and Epidemiology of Ranaviruses
Ecology and Epidemiology of RanavirusesEcology and Epidemiology of Ranaviruses
Ecology and Epidemiology of Ranaviruses
mgray11
 
Biology in Focus - Chapter 41
Biology in Focus - Chapter 41Biology in Focus - Chapter 41
Biology in Focus - Chapter 41
mpattani
 
Biology in Focus - Chapter 23
Biology in Focus - Chapter 23Biology in Focus - Chapter 23
Biology in Focus - Chapter 23
mpattani
 
Effect of anthropogenic noise
Effect of anthropogenic noiseEffect of anthropogenic noise
Effect of anthropogenic noiseIlaria Spiga
 
Biology in Focus - Chapter 26
Biology in Focus - Chapter 26Biology in Focus - Chapter 26
Biology in Focus - Chapter 26
mpattani
 
Kaiser & O'Keefe.2015.Comparison of detectors
Kaiser & O'Keefe.2015.Comparison of detectorsKaiser & O'Keefe.2015.Comparison of detectors
Kaiser & O'Keefe.2015.Comparison of detectorsZach Kaiser
 
Ocean Noise: Science Findings and Regulatory Developments in 2007
Ocean Noise: Science Findings and Regulatory Developments in 2007Ocean Noise: Science Findings and Regulatory Developments in 2007
Ocean Noise: Science Findings and Regulatory Developments in 2007
Acoustic Ecology Institute
 
Blumstein et al_2016
Blumstein et al_2016Blumstein et al_2016
Blumstein et al_2016Kate
 
Biology in Focus - Chapter 27
Biology in Focus - Chapter 27Biology in Focus - Chapter 27
Biology in Focus - Chapter 27
mpattani
 
Biology in Focus - Chapter 20
Biology in Focus - Chapter 20Biology in Focus - Chapter 20
Biology in Focus - Chapter 20
mpattani
 
Jen McIntyre, New science documenting toxic impacts on salmon and other aquat...
Jen McIntyre, New science documenting toxic impacts on salmon and other aquat...Jen McIntyre, New science documenting toxic impacts on salmon and other aquat...
Jen McIntyre, New science documenting toxic impacts on salmon and other aquat...
Northwest Indian Fisheries Commission
 
Protazioetal2014online
Protazioetal2014onlineProtazioetal2014online
Protazioetal2014online
Arielson Protázio
 
Diversity copepods in deep sea coral
Diversity copepods in deep sea coralDiversity copepods in deep sea coral
Diversity copepods in deep sea coralFiddy Prasetiya
 
Bioacoustic
BioacousticBioacoustic
Bioacoustic
abhishek144
 
Biology in Focus - Chapter 24
Biology in Focus - Chapter 24Biology in Focus - Chapter 24
Biology in Focus - Chapter 24
mpattani
 
Ranaviral disease pathology and physiology
Ranaviral disease pathology and physiologyRanaviral disease pathology and physiology
Ranaviral disease pathology and physiology
mgray11
 
sea lion Olfactor y
sea lion Olfactor ysea lion Olfactor y
sea lion Olfactor yTim Hoffland
 

What's hot (20)

Keeley et al_02-15
Keeley et al_02-15Keeley et al_02-15
Keeley et al_02-15
 
Ecology and Epidemiology of Ranaviruses
Ecology and Epidemiology of RanavirusesEcology and Epidemiology of Ranaviruses
Ecology and Epidemiology of Ranaviruses
 
Biology in Focus - Chapter 41
Biology in Focus - Chapter 41Biology in Focus - Chapter 41
Biology in Focus - Chapter 41
 
Biology in Focus - Chapter 23
Biology in Focus - Chapter 23Biology in Focus - Chapter 23
Biology in Focus - Chapter 23
 
Effect of anthropogenic noise
Effect of anthropogenic noiseEffect of anthropogenic noise
Effect of anthropogenic noise
 
Biology in Focus - Chapter 26
Biology in Focus - Chapter 26Biology in Focus - Chapter 26
Biology in Focus - Chapter 26
 
Kaiser & O'Keefe.2015.Comparison of detectors
Kaiser & O'Keefe.2015.Comparison of detectorsKaiser & O'Keefe.2015.Comparison of detectors
Kaiser & O'Keefe.2015.Comparison of detectors
 
Ocean Noise: Science Findings and Regulatory Developments in 2007
Ocean Noise: Science Findings and Regulatory Developments in 2007Ocean Noise: Science Findings and Regulatory Developments in 2007
Ocean Noise: Science Findings and Regulatory Developments in 2007
 
Bird report
Bird reportBird report
Bird report
 
Blumstein et al_2016
Blumstein et al_2016Blumstein et al_2016
Blumstein et al_2016
 
Biology in Focus - Chapter 27
Biology in Focus - Chapter 27Biology in Focus - Chapter 27
Biology in Focus - Chapter 27
 
Biology in Focus - Chapter 20
Biology in Focus - Chapter 20Biology in Focus - Chapter 20
Biology in Focus - Chapter 20
 
Jen McIntyre, New science documenting toxic impacts on salmon and other aquat...
Jen McIntyre, New science documenting toxic impacts on salmon and other aquat...Jen McIntyre, New science documenting toxic impacts on salmon and other aquat...
Jen McIntyre, New science documenting toxic impacts on salmon and other aquat...
 
Protazioetal2014online
Protazioetal2014onlineProtazioetal2014online
Protazioetal2014online
 
Diversity copepods in deep sea coral
Diversity copepods in deep sea coralDiversity copepods in deep sea coral
Diversity copepods in deep sea coral
 
Bioacoustic
BioacousticBioacoustic
Bioacoustic
 
Biology in Focus - Chapter 24
Biology in Focus - Chapter 24Biology in Focus - Chapter 24
Biology in Focus - Chapter 24
 
Ranaviral disease pathology and physiology
Ranaviral disease pathology and physiologyRanaviral disease pathology and physiology
Ranaviral disease pathology and physiology
 
India paper
India paperIndia paper
India paper
 
sea lion Olfactor y
sea lion Olfactor ysea lion Olfactor y
sea lion Olfactor y
 

Similar to Dissertation_final_submission

Effects of chronic moderate noise on animal behavior and distribution
Effects of chronic moderate noise on animal behavior and distributionEffects of chronic moderate noise on animal behavior and distribution
Effects of chronic moderate noise on animal behavior and distribution
Acoustic Ecology Institute
 
Comparison of Echolocation by Sperm Whales and Spinner Dolphins
Comparison of Echolocation by Sperm Whales and Spinner DolphinsComparison of Echolocation by Sperm Whales and Spinner Dolphins
Comparison of Echolocation by Sperm Whales and Spinner DolphinsVictoria Nguyen
 
Acoustic Monitoring: Assoc for Env Studies 2009
Acoustic Monitoring: Assoc for Env Studies 2009Acoustic Monitoring: Assoc for Env Studies 2009
Acoustic Monitoring: Assoc for Env Studies 2009
Acoustic Ecology Institute
 
, 20130104, published 27 March 20139 2013 Biol. Lett. Matt.docx
, 20130104, published 27 March 20139 2013 Biol. Lett. Matt.docx, 20130104, published 27 March 20139 2013 Biol. Lett. Matt.docx
, 20130104, published 27 March 20139 2013 Biol. Lett. Matt.docx
mercysuttle
 
Avian Vocalization Adjustment to Urbanization and Anthropogenic Noise_ A Revi...
Avian Vocalization Adjustment to Urbanization and Anthropogenic Noise_ A Revi...Avian Vocalization Adjustment to Urbanization and Anthropogenic Noise_ A Revi...
Avian Vocalization Adjustment to Urbanization and Anthropogenic Noise_ A Revi...Maxson Jarecki
 
Extrapolating beyond chinchillas: ocean noise behavioral response ambiguity a...
Extrapolating beyond chinchillas: ocean noise behavioral response ambiguity a...Extrapolating beyond chinchillas: ocean noise behavioral response ambiguity a...
Extrapolating beyond chinchillas: ocean noise behavioral response ambiguity a...
Acoustic Ecology Institute
 
PHY101 Concepts In Physics.docx
PHY101 Concepts In Physics.docxPHY101 Concepts In Physics.docx
PHY101 Concepts In Physics.docx
write5
 
Ocean Noise: What we Learned in 2006
Ocean Noise: What we Learned in 2006Ocean Noise: What we Learned in 2006
Ocean Noise: What we Learned in 2006
Acoustic Ecology Institute
 
David Bryson Masters Thesis
David Bryson Masters ThesisDavid Bryson Masters Thesis
David Bryson Masters ThesisDavid Bryson
 
Effects of environmental noise on human health
Effects of environmental noise on human healthEffects of environmental noise on human health
Effects of environmental noise on human health
D_Petri
 
on January 22, 2018httprspb.royalsocietypublishing.orgDow.docx
 on January 22, 2018httprspb.royalsocietypublishing.orgDow.docx on January 22, 2018httprspb.royalsocietypublishing.orgDow.docx
on January 22, 2018httprspb.royalsocietypublishing.orgDow.docx
aryan532920
 
MASTER THESIS, Munoz, Ref 20140536
MASTER THESIS, Munoz, Ref 20140536MASTER THESIS, Munoz, Ref 20140536
MASTER THESIS, Munoz, Ref 20140536Josefine Mu
 
EFFECT OF SOUND POLLUTION ON FISHES
EFFECT OF SOUND POLLUTION ON FISHESEFFECT OF SOUND POLLUTION ON FISHES
EFFECT OF SOUND POLLUTION ON FISHES
Sailesh Mahapatra
 
SoundSmart: A Brief Survey of Sound, Noise and their Effects
SoundSmart: A Brief Survey of Sound, Noise and their EffectsSoundSmart: A Brief Survey of Sound, Noise and their Effects
SoundSmart: A Brief Survey of Sound, Noise and their Effects
WordCityStudio, Inc
 
Noise pollution(3).pptx
Noise pollution(3).pptxNoise pollution(3).pptx
Noise pollution(3).pptx
Sanjoy32
 
How noise effects animals.pptx
How noise effects animals.pptxHow noise effects animals.pptx
How noise effects animals.pptx
HammadWali1
 
Noelle Dunne BSc Thesis
Noelle Dunne BSc ThesisNoelle Dunne BSc Thesis
Noelle Dunne BSc ThesisNoelle Dunne
 
9 Acoustic Communication in Fishes and Frogs RANDY ZEL.docx
9 Acoustic Communication in Fishes and Frogs RANDY ZEL.docx9 Acoustic Communication in Fishes and Frogs RANDY ZEL.docx
9 Acoustic Communication in Fishes and Frogs RANDY ZEL.docx
ransayo
 

Similar to Dissertation_final_submission (20)

Effects of chronic moderate noise on animal behavior and distribution
Effects of chronic moderate noise on animal behavior and distributionEffects of chronic moderate noise on animal behavior and distribution
Effects of chronic moderate noise on animal behavior and distribution
 
Comparison of Echolocation by Sperm Whales and Spinner Dolphins
Comparison of Echolocation by Sperm Whales and Spinner DolphinsComparison of Echolocation by Sperm Whales and Spinner Dolphins
Comparison of Echolocation by Sperm Whales and Spinner Dolphins
 
Acoustic Monitoring: Assoc for Env Studies 2009
Acoustic Monitoring: Assoc for Env Studies 2009Acoustic Monitoring: Assoc for Env Studies 2009
Acoustic Monitoring: Assoc for Env Studies 2009
 
, 20130104, published 27 March 20139 2013 Biol. Lett. Matt.docx
, 20130104, published 27 March 20139 2013 Biol. Lett. Matt.docx, 20130104, published 27 March 20139 2013 Biol. Lett. Matt.docx
, 20130104, published 27 March 20139 2013 Biol. Lett. Matt.docx
 
Avian Vocalization Adjustment to Urbanization and Anthropogenic Noise_ A Revi...
Avian Vocalization Adjustment to Urbanization and Anthropogenic Noise_ A Revi...Avian Vocalization Adjustment to Urbanization and Anthropogenic Noise_ A Revi...
Avian Vocalization Adjustment to Urbanization and Anthropogenic Noise_ A Revi...
 
Extrapolating beyond chinchillas: ocean noise behavioral response ambiguity a...
Extrapolating beyond chinchillas: ocean noise behavioral response ambiguity a...Extrapolating beyond chinchillas: ocean noise behavioral response ambiguity a...
Extrapolating beyond chinchillas: ocean noise behavioral response ambiguity a...
 
PHY101 Concepts In Physics.docx
PHY101 Concepts In Physics.docxPHY101 Concepts In Physics.docx
PHY101 Concepts In Physics.docx
 
Ocean Noise: What we Learned in 2006
Ocean Noise: What we Learned in 2006Ocean Noise: What we Learned in 2006
Ocean Noise: What we Learned in 2006
 
David Bryson Masters Thesis
David Bryson Masters ThesisDavid Bryson Masters Thesis
David Bryson Masters Thesis
 
Effects of environmental noise on human health
Effects of environmental noise on human healthEffects of environmental noise on human health
Effects of environmental noise on human health
 
on January 22, 2018httprspb.royalsocietypublishing.orgDow.docx
 on January 22, 2018httprspb.royalsocietypublishing.orgDow.docx on January 22, 2018httprspb.royalsocietypublishing.orgDow.docx
on January 22, 2018httprspb.royalsocietypublishing.orgDow.docx
 
MASTER THESIS, Munoz, Ref 20140536
MASTER THESIS, Munoz, Ref 20140536MASTER THESIS, Munoz, Ref 20140536
MASTER THESIS, Munoz, Ref 20140536
 
EFFECT OF SOUND POLLUTION ON FISHES
EFFECT OF SOUND POLLUTION ON FISHESEFFECT OF SOUND POLLUTION ON FISHES
EFFECT OF SOUND POLLUTION ON FISHES
 
RGeorgeUSFS14
RGeorgeUSFS14RGeorgeUSFS14
RGeorgeUSFS14
 
SoundSmart: A Brief Survey of Sound, Noise and their Effects
SoundSmart: A Brief Survey of Sound, Noise and their EffectsSoundSmart: A Brief Survey of Sound, Noise and their Effects
SoundSmart: A Brief Survey of Sound, Noise and their Effects
 
Noise pollution(3).pptx
Noise pollution(3).pptxNoise pollution(3).pptx
Noise pollution(3).pptx
 
How noise effects animals.pptx
How noise effects animals.pptxHow noise effects animals.pptx
How noise effects animals.pptx
 
DISSERTATION
DISSERTATIONDISSERTATION
DISSERTATION
 
Noelle Dunne BSc Thesis
Noelle Dunne BSc ThesisNoelle Dunne BSc Thesis
Noelle Dunne BSc Thesis
 
9 Acoustic Communication in Fishes and Frogs RANDY ZEL.docx
9 Acoustic Communication in Fishes and Frogs RANDY ZEL.docx9 Acoustic Communication in Fishes and Frogs RANDY ZEL.docx
9 Acoustic Communication in Fishes and Frogs RANDY ZEL.docx
 

Dissertation_final_submission

  • 1. ~ 1 ~ The effect of anthropogenic noise on shoaling cohesion in three- spined sticklebacks (Gasterosteus aculeatus) A report submitted in part fulfilment of the examination requirements for the award of a B.Sc. (Hons) Animal Behaviour and Welfare awarded by the University of Lincoln, June 2014, supervised by Tom Pike. Thomas Trew 12357944
  • 2. ~ 2 ~ CERTIFICATE OF ORIGINALITY This is to certify that I am responsible for the work submitted in this thesis, that the original work is my own, except as specified in the acknowledgements and in references, and that neither the thesis nor the original work contained therein has been previously submitted to any institution for a degree. Signature: Name: Date: CERTIFICATE OF COMPLIANCE This is to certify that this project has been carried out in accordance with University principles regarding ethics and health and safety. Forms are available to view on request. Signature: Name: Date:
  • 3. ~ 3 ~ Abstract Noise pollution in both terrestrial and aquatic habitats has considerably increased over the last few decades, and there is growing evidence that anthropogenic noise can affect behaviour in a variety of species. There are increasing concerns that this may affect the fitness of many animals. A majority of fish species spend a part of their life in shoals, which are important for both predator protection and group foraging. Therefore the aim of this study was to assess the effect on anthropogenic noise on shoaling cohesion. To do this, three-spined sticklebacks (Gasterosteus aculeatus) were used in a tank based playback experiment. Results showed that during exposure to anthropogenic noise, sticklebacks would group closer together, essentially mirroring behaviour seen in shoals when threatened by a predator. The findings of this study suggested anthropogenic noise has the potential to influence anti-predatory behaviour and vigilance of shoals. Future research is required for more in depth analyses of the impact anthropogenic noise has on direct fitness. Keywords Anthropogenic, noise, pollution, Three-spined stickleback, Gasterosteus aculeatus, shoal, cohesion, behaviour. Word count 3464.
  • 4. ~ 4 ~ Introduction The human population is ever growing, and so is its impact on wildlife (Chan et al., 2010). One factor being the amount of noise pollution we produce, from activities such as urban development, expansion of transport networks and resource extraction (Wale et al., 2013). Past studies have shown increasing evidence that anthropogenic (man made) noise can affect the behaviour of a wide and diverse range of animals (Normandeau Associates, Inc, 2012; Morely et al., 2014; Slabbekoorn and Ripmeester, 2008; Barber et al., 2010). Anthropogenic noise, especially when loud, persistent, unexpected or novel, has the potential to cause stress (Wysocki et al., 2006; Wright et al., 2007), to distract (Chan et al., 2010) and to mask important sounds (Wale et al., 2013; Siemers and Schaub, 2011; Lowry et al., 2012). For example, Habib et al. (2007) conducted a study on male ovenbirds (Seiurus aurocapilla); their results showed that males on louder territories were less likely to attract a mate than those based at quieter locations. They hypothesized that noise pollution was interfering with male ovenbirds songs, which could reduce the distance to which females can hear the songs, or give the perception the male is of lower quality due to distortions of song characteristics. If female ovenbirds were then to base mate choice on noise conditions of the males’ territory over size, age and body condition of the male ovenbird; then male mate pairing success may be negatively affected by anthropogenic noise. Another example of animal communication being affected by increasing noise pollution can be seen in the Sun and Narins (2005) study on frogs. They observed that certain species of pond-dwelling frogs, which were particularly acoustically active, would reduce their call rate when exposed to playbacks of traffic noise. These results suggest that the frogs changed their calling behaviour to avoid auditory masking. Anti-predator vigilance may also be increased by animals when exposed to loud noises. Rabin et al. (2006) observed that in California ground squirrels (Spermophilus beecheyi) the individuals located in areas around loud wind turbines exhibited higher rates of vigilance when hearing alarm calls of conspecifics than those in quieter areas. It has also been suggested that anti-predator behaviour may be affected by anthropogenic noise (Chan et al., 2010; Karp and Root, 2009; Wale et al., 2013). Predator avoidance is crucial for animals survivability and reproduction success (Caro, 2005); and yet there are few studies that have investigated the potential effect of anthropogenic noise on anti- predatory behaviour (Voellmy et al., 2014). Chan et al. (2010) observed that whilst
  • 5. ~ 5 ~ exposed to anthropogenic noise, simulated predators could get significantly closer to the Caribbean hermit crabs (Coenobita clypeatus) before they retreated into their shells, in comparison to the silent trials. They proposed the ‘Distracted prey hypothesis’, which states that any stimulus an animal can perceive is capable of distracting it by reallocating part of its finite attention and thus preventing it from responding accordingly to an approaching threat. Attention is the process by which an individual filters out several stimuli from the surrounding environment, letting in only as much as it can process (Bushnell, 1998). Chan et al. (2010) then went on to suggest that anthropogenic noise could be a distracting stimulus, and could reduce an animal’s fitness by increased vulnerability to predators. Another theory as to how anthropogenic noise may affect anti-predatory behaviour is that the additional noise could mask important acoustic cues produced by predators, or warning calls from conspecifics (Wale et al., 2013). Prey are often alerted to the presence of predators by auditory cues, from acts such as intraspecific communication or the inadvertent sound produced from movement (Barrera et al., 2011; Siemers and Schaub, 2011). Furthermore, alarm calls have evolved into a wide range of animals to warn others of imminent danger (Hollen and Radford, 2009); other vocalizations can be used to provide information on the level of risk (Hollen et al., 2008; Bell et al., 2009). Masking can either be described as ‘energetic’, whereby the masking is complete and the signal is not detected at all, or ‘informational’, where the signal can be detected by the listener, however the content is hard to understand (Wale et al., 2013). Either way, there is likely consequences to foraging and anti-predator behaviour from a reduced ability to detect valuable auditory information (Brumm and Slabbekoorn, 2005; Siemers and Schaub, 2011; Lowry et al., 2012). Fish populations have been negatively impacted by human population growth for a number of well known reasons (Slabbekoorn et al., 2010) such as fisheries (Jackson et al., 2001), habitat degradation (Munday, 2004) and chemical pollution (Van Der Oost et al., 2003). Underwater anthropogenic noise pollution could potentially be another big threat to fish populations (McDonald et al., 2006). The amount of fresh water and marine noise pollution produced by humans has been increasing significantly over the last few decades (Codarin et al., 2009), not just around coastal areas, but also in the open ocean (Andrew et al., 2002; Tyack, 2008). Sound travels approximately 5 times faster in water than it does in air (around 1500m/s in water – 300m/s in air), meaning that sound wavelengths are also
  • 6. ~ 6 ~ about 5 times longer in water than they are in air (e.g. 100Hz signal = 15m in water - 3m in air). This results in sound traveling greater distances and at higher amplitude levels in water, enabling long distance communication, but also resulting in long distance noise pollution (Slabbekoorn et al., 2010). For example, anthropogenic noise produced during pile-driving for wind turbines can be detected over background underwater noise levels from as far as 70km away (Bailey et al., 2010). This increase of sound amplitude and area of effect makes underwater anthropogenic noise pollution a real cause for concern (Slabbekoorn et al., 2010). To this date there’s only one study I am aware of that assesses the impact of underwater anthropogenic noise on anti-predatory behaviour in fish. Voellmy et al. (2014) used both European minnow (Phoxinus phoxinus) and three-spined sticklebacks (Gasterosteus aculeatus) in their study. They observed increased vigilance in sticklebacks and faster response rates to a simulated predator when exposed to anthropogenic noise; whereas minnows did not significantly differ in response rate depending on noise exposure. However, both three-spined sticklebacks and European minnow are shoaling fish (Huntingford and Ruiz-Gomez, 2009; Ward and Krause, 2001). Voellmy et al. (2014) only tested the effect of anthropogenic noise on individual anti- predatory behaviour, even though both species of fish spend a large proportion of their lives as part of a shoal. A majority of fish species spend some part of their lives in groups (Shaw, 1978); these groups of fish are commonly termed as ‘shoals’ (Miller and Gerlai, 2012; Kenedy and Pitcher, 1975). Predators and food have been said to be the two fundamental keys of understanding fish shoals (Pitcher, 1993). In order to survive and reproduce successfully, animals must minimize the risk of starvation and predation (Wale et al., 2013). Living in shoals is very important for defence against predation; synchronized co-operation confuses predators and reduces the threat of a successful attack (Pitcher, 1993). Past research has already shown that anthropogenic noise can have a negative impact on shoaling fish’s foraging efficiency (Purser and Radford, 2011); however no research that I am aware of has been done on the effect it has on shoal anti-predatory behaviour. Once a predator is detected, predator defence takes precedence over food (Pitcher, 1993); suggesting the results of Purser and Radford’s (2011) study with three-spined sticklebacks was due to the fish choosing predator defence over food. In the presence of a predator, shoal cohesion increases (distance between each fish reduces) (Ryer and Olla, 1998); which is why in this study, I aim to test the effects on anthropogenic noise on shoaling cohesion using three-
  • 7. ~ 7 ~ spined sticklebacks, a small, robust and common fish species which acts as an excellent model for the scientific study of behaviour and acclimates well to laboratory conditions (Purse and Radford, 2011; Huntingford and Ruiz-Gomez, 2009). If Purse and Radford’s (2011) results were, as I suggested, due to the fish displaying anti-predator behaviour; then I would expect to see that the exposure of anthropogenic noise will result in increase shoal cohesion. Methods Thirty adult three-spined sticklebacks were used as subjects in this study. The Fish used were sourced from a local drainage ditch off the river Till (grid reference: -53.309418, - 0.651873) in October 2014 and housed in University of Lincolns Minster House aquatics laboratory, in an 8+/-1°C chilled room; the light timer for this room was set to match the natural daylight times at the river from which the fish were caught. Prior to the study all fish were housed in the same 64-L tank, fitted with an Eheim water filter, filled with 45L of filtered, aerated and chilled tap water and containing artificial plastic plants for shelter. They were fed on a frozen bloodworm (Chironomid larvae) diet till satiation daily. Testing shoals consisted of 3 fish; each fish was tested only once, allowing for 10 tests, all with unique shoals. Three fish would be randomly selected and transferred, using a net, into a circular (31cm diameter) grey testing tub, filled with 7cm of water. Once in the testing tub, the fish were left for a 30 minute habituation period. After habituation, the testing period began, which lasted 1 hour and 30 minutes; consisting of a 30 minute ‘silent’ control trial, followed by a 30 minute noise exposure trial and ending with another 30 minute ‘silent’ control trail. The silent control trials gave a baseline measure of distance between fish in ambient conditions (Purser and Radford, 2011; Chan et al., 2010). During the noise exposure trial the fish were exposed to a playback of white noise for the full 30 minutes; played through two 10 watt Mila 2.0 speakers, placed opposite each other facing towards the centre of the testing tub; bandwidth was limited between 100 and 1000 HZ, presenting frequencies falling under the sticklebacks likely hearing range (Purser and Radford, 2011). The volume was consistent across all trials and was, to my hearing, set to a level that was not completely biologically unrealistic to be heard around shorelines with boat activity. Throughout the whole testing period a Microsoft 2.0 megapixel camera mounted 55cm directly over the grey testing tub was being used to manually take pictures (via a Dell latitude x300 notebook) approximately every 60 seconds, giving an aerial view of the fish
  • 8. ~ 8 ~ and their individual locations in the tub. This resulted in 90 images for each shoal, and 900 images in total. After each test, the 3 fish used were moved to another 64-L tank so as to ensure the same fish were not used more than once; the testing tub would then be emptied and refilled with fresh water, in preparation for the next shoal. All 900 images taken during the 10 tests were analysed on Windows 7 Microsoft Paint, allowing each fish to be allocated coordinates on each picture. The coordinates used were manually worked out and based at the tip of each individual fish snout. The distance (D) between two individual fish from each other could be calculated in every image using the following formula: where x1 and y1 stands for the X and Y axis coordinates of one fish, x2 and y2 represents the X and Y axis coordinates for the second fish of which you calculate the distance between. The average distance between all 3 individuals during the first control, noise exposure, and second control trials were calculated for each shoal, resulting in 10 averages of distance between fish for each trial. If my prediction that anthropogenic noise effects shoaling cohesion is correct, we would expect to see a significant difference in distance between individuals during the sound trial, when compared to the two control trials. To examine this I conducted a general linear model with the average distance between fish as the dependent variable, trial type (pre control, noise exposure and post control) as a fixed factor, whilst controlling for non independence between the 10 shoals by including shoal identity as a random effect. I also conducted a post-hoc Tukey test to examine pairwise differences between trial types.
  • 9. ~ 9 ~ Results Throughout the whole 90 minutes, the cohesiveness of the shoals differed significantly (General linear model, F2,18 = 8.70, P = 0.002; Figure 1). Specifically, the post-hoc Tukey tests revealed that control 1 differed from the sound treatment (T = -3.917, df = 18, P = 0.0028), control 2 differed from the sound treatment (T = -3.199, df = 18, P = 0.0131), whereas control 1 and control 2 did not differ from each other (T = -0.718, df = 18, P = 0.7563). Fig 1. Mean ± SE distance (mm) between fish during the three trials types. 0 20 40 60 80 100 120 140 Distance(mm) Control 1 Sound Control 2 Trial type
  • 10. ~ 10 ~ Discussion The results of this study show that anthropogenic noise impacts shoaling cohesion of three- spined stickleback shoals. The addition of white noise elicited the same anti-predatory defensive behaviour (increased shoal cohesion) that is shown in shoals when in the presence of a predator (Ryer and Olla, 1998). By using two control trials, before and after the noise exposure trial, the results also showed that there was no difference in shoal cohesion between the two controls; suggesting that the noise treatment had no lasting effect on the fish. The increase in shoal cohesion when exposed to anthropogenic noise mirrors the grouping behaviour seen in shoals under predatory threat (Ryer and Olla, 1998). Grouping together has several mechanisms that reduce predation risk (Ioannou et al., 2008), including the confusion effect (Miller, 1922), the dilution effect (Foster and Treherne, 1981) and the ‘many eyes’ hypothesis (Lima, 1995). The ‘many eyes’ hypothesis states that as part of a group, the task of scanning the environment for predators is spread out to multiple individuals; this collaborate effort results in higher levels of vigilance (Lima, 1995). An increase in vigilance is a result that has been seen in previous studies of noise effect on terrestrial wildlife (Quinn et al., 2006; Rabin et al., 2006), and in individual sticklebacks (Voellmy et al., 2014). The increase in shoal cohesion and general alertness of the fish may be the result of a stress response caused by the noise exposure (Wright et al., 2007; Charmandari et al., 2005). Alternatively, grouping together may be to benefit from the ‘many eyes’ hypothesis in compensation for potential masking of predator auditory cues (such as intraspecific communication and inadvertent sound produced from movement) (Siemers and Schaub, 2011), by relying more on visual stimulus of the environment to detect possible threats (Rabin et al., 2006). Increased vigilance often results in faster anti- predatory responses (Voellmy et al., 2014); a reduced latency response could be a direct benefit to fitness (Siemers and Schuab, 2011). Krause and Godin (1996) observed this increase in fitness in Guppies (Poecilia reticulate) when prayed upon by blue acara cichlids (Aequidens pulcher). They found that vigilant guppies detected cichlids sooner and initiated flight responses earlier than foraging individuals. The vigilant guppies were more likely to escape predation, resulting in greater fitness for the individuals responding fastest, because after all ‘an animal captured and consumed by a predator has its cumulative fitness abruptly terminated’ Ryer and Olla (1998). However, responding faster reduces the time spent on risk assessment, which may result in suboptimal decision
  • 11. ~ 11 ~ making, such as unnecessary flight response to a low/no threat situation (Voellmy et al., 2014). In the case of fleeing individuals seeking shelter, and subsequently spending time hiding from a stimuli of low/no threat, not only is unnecessary energy spent fleeing, but the time wasted hiding may also lead to lost opportunities to forage or reproduce (Ydenberg and Dill, 1986). Additionally, constant forgoing of food for unnecessary anti- predatory behaviour will eventually result in the individual weakening, resulting in them being more vulnerable to predation. Furthermore, in fish, predation risk is typically negatively correlated with the size of an individual (Sogard, 1997), slow growing individuals may be under more intense predation for a longer period of time, than those that are well fed and therefore grow more rapidly (Houde, 1987). Attempts to compensate for lost foraging time may also results in increased predation risk, if animals are then forced to forage during times with increased predatory threat (Lima and Dill, 1990). Chan et al. (2010) suggested anthropogenic noise could reallocate the attention of prey, increasing their susceptibility to predation. The Caribbean hermit crabs (Chan et al., 2010) and the shore crab (Carcinus maenas) (Wale et al., 2013) have exhibited this ‘distracted’ behaviour when exposed to anthropogenic noise; the noise exposure caused delayed anti- predator response in simulated predator attacks. Krause and Godin (1996) suggested that predators take advantage of less vigilant prey and are more likely to attack individuals that exhibit signs of distraction. Predator bias of less vigilant prey, along with delayed response caused by anthropogenic noise, may increase vulnerability to predation (Chan et al., 2010). However, past studies have shown evidence that the same anthropogenic noise can have different impacts on different species (Francis et al., 2011; Rios-Chelen et al., 2012). The results of my study and Voellmy et al. (2014) suggest three-spined sticklebacks increase their vigilance when exposed to anthropogenic noise. Therefore whilst the distraction hypothesis is a valid theory for some species, sticklebacks may be one of many fish species that this theory does not apply to. On the other hand, as anthropogenic noise becomes more frequent, animals response to it may weaken due to habituation (Thompson and Spencer, 1966), a common process to many animals (Chance and Wash, 2006; Nowacek et al., 2007). If habituation was to occur in sticklebacks, then I would presume their vigilance to noise stimuli would weaken, perhaps not only to anthropogenic noise, but the auditory cues of predators as well, increasing their vulnerability to predation. Species specific habituation to anthropogenic noise could be a good area for future research.
  • 12. ~ 12 ~ Whilst the results of this study showed a significant difference in distance between fish during the noise trial in comparison to both control trials; it is also important to note that there was no significant difference between the pre and post controls, suggesting that there was no lasting effect of the noise treatment. This could mean if underwater anthropogenic noise pollution was reduced, there would be rapid changes to fish anti-predatory behaviour. However, as mentioned before, anthropogenic noise impacts species in different ways (Francis et al., 2011; Rios-Chelen et al., 2012), and three-spined sticklebacks have been described as behaviourally robust, in the sense that they settle quickly after a disturbance (Huntingford and Ruiz-Gomez, 2009); just because sticklebacks show no lasting behavioural changes, does not mean the same can be said for all fish species. Although it is clear anthropogenic sound has an effect on shoaling cohesion, I can only speculate the impact this change in shoaling behaviour has on predator vulnerability and individual fitness. In order to gain a better understanding on how anthropogenic noise effects anti-predator defence in shoals, further research needs to be conducted including a simulated predator and assessing change of response latency in shoals when exposed to noise. Additionally, care is always needed when interpreting results from a tank based playback experiment into a real world context (Wale et al., 2013). The use of tanks is to ensure tight control of potential confounding factors; however tank playbacks cannot replicate natural sound fields perfectly (Okumura et al., 2002). This being said, given the lack of studies examining the impact of anthropogenic noise on shoaling behaviours, even preliminary evidence is potentially valuable. Conclusion This study has provided evidence that anthropogenic noise pollution influences shoal cohesion in three-spined sticklebacks. The results showed that individuals grouped closer together during noise exposure, which is similar behaviour to that seen in fish under predatory threat. A possible implication of this may be reduced latency in flight response to predators, at the cost of time spent assessing the level of risk, potentially resulting in wasted energy and reduced foraging efficiency; which mirrors results seen in previous studies assessing the effects of anthropogenic noise on sticklebacks (Voellmy et al., 2014; Purser and Radford, 2011). However, without a simulated predator threat, this evidence allows us to only predict how shoaling fish may react to a predator whilst exposed to
  • 13. ~ 13 ~ anthropogenic sounds. What is clear from this study is anthropogenic noise does have an impact on shoaling behaviour in fish and that further research is required in order to truly quantify the direct fitness consequences. Furthermore, laboratory and tank based studies create an artificial scenario, so care is needed when translating results into real world context. The ideal context for future studies would be to assess potential effects of anthropogenic noise that have direct consequences to fitness, but in the wild with real noise sources and natural sound fields, allowing the spatial scale of impact and severity of fitness consequences to be determined. Acknowledgements I’d like to thank Tom Pike for all his help and supervision as my tutor. Many warm thanks to Helen Edwards for making the cold hours of data collection fly by. Finally, thanks to Danielle Derrick and Matthew Hale for constant moral support and assistance with data input.
  • 14. ~ 14 ~ References Andrew, R.K. Howe, B.M. Mercer, J.A. and Dzieciuch, M.A. (2002) Ocean ambient sound: comparing the 1960s with the 1990s for a receiver off the California coast. Acoustics Research Letters Online, 3(2) 65-70. Bailey, H. Senior, B. Simmons, D. Rusin, J. Picken, G. and Thompson, P.M. (2010) Assessing underwater noise levels during pile-driving at an offshore windfarm and its potential effects on marine mammals. Marine Pollution Bulletin, 60, 888-897. Barber, J.R. Crooks, K.R. and Fristrup, K.M. (2010) The costs of chronic noise exposure for terrestrial organisms. Trends in Ecology and Evolution, 25(3) 180-189. Barrera, J.P. Chong, L. Judy, K.N. and Blumstein, D.T. (2011) Reliability of public information: predators provide more information about risk than conspecifics. Animal Behaviour, 81(4) 779-787. Bell, M.B.V. Radford, A.N. Rose, R. Wade, H.M. and Ridley, A.R. (2009) The value of constant surveillance in risky environment. Proceedings of the Royal Society B, 276, 2997- 3005. Brumm, H. and Slabbekoorn, H. (2005) Acoustic communication in noise. Advances in the Study of Behavior, 35, 151-209. Bushnell, P.J. (1998) Behavioral approaches to the assessment of attention in animals. Psychopharmacology, 138(3) 231-259. Caro, T. (2005) Antipredator defenses in birds and mammals. Chicago: The University of Chicago press. Chan, A.A.Y-H. Giraldo-Perez, P. Smith, S. and Blumstein, D.T. (2010) Anthropogenic noise affects risk assessment and attention: the distracted prey hypothesis. Biology Letters, 6, 458-461. Charmandari, E. Tsigos, C. and Chrousos, G. (2005) Endocrinology of the stress response. Annu Rev Pyschol, 67, 259-284.
  • 15. ~ 15 ~ Codarin, A. Wysocki, L.E. Ladich, F. and Picciulin, M. (2009) Effects of ambient and boat noise on hearing and communication in three fish species living in a marine protected area (Miramare, Italy). Marine Pollution Bulletin, 58, 1880-1887. Foster, W.A. and Treherne, J.E. (1981) Evidence for the dilution effect in the selfish herd from fish predation on a marine insect. Nature, 293, 466-467. Francis, C.D. Ortega, C.P. and Cruz, A. (2011) Different behavioural responses to anthropogenic noise by two closely related passerine birds. Biology Letters, 7, 850-852. Habib, L. Bayne, E.B. and Boutin, S. (2007) Chronic industrial noise affects pairing success and age structure of ovenbirds Seiurus aurocapilla. Journal of Applied Ecology, 44(1) 176-184. Hollen, L.I. Bell, M.B.V. and Radford, A.N. (2008) Cooperative sentinel calling? Foragers gain increased biomass intake. Current Biology, 18, 576-579. Hollen, L.I. and Radford, A.N. (2009) The development of alarm call behaviour in mammals and birds. Animal Behaviour, 78, 791-800. Houde, E.D. (1987) Fish early life dynamics and recruitment variability. American Fisheries Society Symposium Series, 2, 17-29. Huntingford, F.A. and Ruiz-Gomez, M.L. (2009) Three-spined sticklebacks Gasterosteus aculeatus as a model for exploring behavioural biology. Journal of Fish Biology, 75(8) 1943-1976. Ioannou, C.C. Tosh, C.R. Neville, L. and Krause, J. (2008) The confusion effect – from neural networks to reduce predation risk. Behavioural Ecology, 19, 126-130.
  • 16. ~ 16 ~ Jackson, J.B.C. Kirby, M.X. Berger, W.H., Bjorndal, K.A. Botsford, L.W. Bourque, B.J. Bradbury, R.H. Cooke, R. Erlandson, J. Estes, J.A. Hughes, T.P. Kidwell, S. Lange, C.B. Lenihan, H.S. Pandolfi, J.M. Peterson, C.H. Steneck, R.S. Tegner, M.J. and Warner, R.R. (2001) Historical overfishing and the recent collapse of coastal ecosystems. Science, 293(5530) 629-638. Karp, D.S. and Root, T.L. (2009) Sound the stressor: how hoatzins (Opisthocomus hoazin) react to ecotourist conversation. Biodiversity and Conservation, 18(14) 3733-3742 Kenedy, G.J.A. and Pitcher, T.J. (1975) Experiments on homing in shoals of European Minnow, Phoxinus Phoxinus (L.). Transactions of the American Fisheries Society, 104, 452-5. Krause, J. and Godin, J.J. (1996) Influence of prey foraging posture on flight behaviour and predation risk: predators take advantage of unwary prey. Behav Ecol. 7, 264-271. Lima, S.L. (1995) Back to the basics of anti-predatory vigilance: The group size effect. Animal behaviour, 49(1) 11-20. Lima, S.L. and Dill, L.M. (1990) Behavioral decisions made under the risk of predation: a review and prospectus. Canadian Journal of Zoology, 68(4) 619-640. Lowry, H. Lill, A. and Wong, B.B.M. (2012) How noisy does a noisy miner have to be? Amplitude adjustments of alarm calls in an avian urban ‘adaptor’. PLoS ONE, 7(1) e29960. McDonald, M.A. Hildebrand, J.A. and Wiggins, S.M. (2006) Increases in deep ocean ambient noise in the Northeast Pacific west of San Nicolas Island, California. Acoustical Society of America, 120(2) 711-718. Miller, R.C. (1922) The significance of the gregarious habit. Ecology, 3, 122-126. Miller, N. and Gerlai, R. (2012) From schooling to shoaling: Patterns of collective motion in Zebrafish (Danio rerio). PLoS ONE, 7(11) e48865.
  • 17. ~ 17 ~ Morely, E.L. Jones, G. and Radford, A.N. (2014) The importance of invertebrates when considering the impacts of anthropogenic noise. Proceedings of Royal Society B, 281(1776) 20132683. Munday, P.L. (2004) Habitat loss, resource specialization, and extinction on coral reefs. Global Change Biology, 10(10) 1642-1647. Normandeau Associates, Inc. 2012. Effects of Noise on Fish, Fisheries, and Invertebrates in the U.S. Atlantic and Arctic from Energy Industry Sound-Generating Activities. A Workshop Report for the U.S. Dept. of the Interior, Bureau of Ocean Energy Management. Contract # M11PC00031. 72 pp. plus Appendices. Available: http://www.cbd.int/doc/meetings/mar/mcbem-2014-01/other/mcbem-2014-01-submission- boem-04-en.pdf [Accessed 14 March 2015]. Okumura, T. Akamatsu, T. and Yan, H.Y. (2002) Analyses of small tank acoustics: empirical and theoretical approaches. Bioacoustics, 12, 330-332. Pitcher, T.J. (1993). The behaviour of Teleost fishes. 2nd edition. London: Chapman & Hall. Purser, J. and Radford, A.N. (2011) Acoustic noise induces attention shifts and reduces foraging performance in Three-spined Sticklebacks (Gasterosteus aculeatus). PLoS ONE, 6(2) e17478. Quinn, J.L Whittingham, M.J. Butler, S.J. and Cresswell, W. (2006) Noise, predation risk compensation and vigilance in the chaffinch Fringilla coelebs. Journal of Avian Biology, 37(6) 601-608. Rabin, L.A. Cross, R.G. and Owings, D.H. (2006) The effects of wind turbines on antipredator behavior in California ground squirrels (Spermophilus beecheyi). Biology Conservation, 131, 410-420 Rios-Chelen, A.A. Salaberria, C. Barbosa, I. Macias, G.C. and Gil, D. (2012) The learning advantage: bird species that learn their song show a tighter adjustment of song to noisy environments than those that do not learn. Journal of Evolutionary Biology, 25(11) 2171- 2180.
  • 18. ~ 18 ~ Ryer, C.H. and Olla, B.L. (1998) Shifting the balance between foraging and predator avoidance: the importance of food distribution for a schooling pelagic forager. Environmental Biology of Fish, 52, 467-475. Shaw, E. (1978) Schooling fishes. Animal Sciences, 66, 166-175. Siemers, B.M. and Schaub, A. (2011) Hunting at the highway: traffic noise reduces foraging efficiency in acoustic predators. Proceedings of the Royal Society B, 282(1805) 1646-1652. Slabbekoorn, H. Bouton, N. Opzeeland, I.V. Coers, A. Cate, C.T. and Popper, A.N. (2010) A noisy spring: the impact of globally rising underwater sound levels on fish. Trends Ecol Evol, 25, 419-427. Slabbekoorn, H. and Ripmeester, E.A.P (2008) Birdsong and anthropogenic noise: implications and applications for conservation. Molecular Ecology, 17(1) 72-83. Sogard, S.M. (1997) Size-selective mortality in the juvenile stage of teleost fishes: a review. Bulletin of Marine Science, 60(29) 1129-1157. Sun, J.W.C. and Narins, P.M. (2005) Anthropogenic sounds differentially affect amphibian call rate. Biology Conservation, 121, 419-427. Thompson, R.F. and Spencer, W.A. (1966) Habituation: a model phenomenon for the study of neuronal substrates of behaviour. Psychol Rev, 73, 16-43. Tyack, P.L. (2008) Implication for marine mammals of large-scale changes in the marine acoustic environment. Journal of Mammalogy, 89(3) 549-558. Van der Oost, R. Beyer, J. and Vermeulen, N.P.E. (2003) Fish bioaccumulation and biomarkers in environmental risk assessment: a review. Environmental Toxicology and Pharmacology, 13(2) 57-149. Voellmy, I.K. Purser, J. Simpson, S.D, and Radford, A.N. (2014) Increased noise levels have different impacts on the anti-predatory behaviour of two sympatric fish species. PLoS ONE, 9(7) e102946. Wale, M.A. Simpson, S.D. and Radford, A.N. (2013) Noise negatively affects foraging and antipredator behaviour in shore crabs. Animal Behaviour, 86, 111-118.
  • 19. ~ 19 ~ Ward, A.J.W. and Krause, J. (2001) Body length assortative shoaling in European minnow, Phoxinus phoxinus. Animal Behaviour, 62(4) 617-621. Wright, A.J. Soto, N.A. Baldwin, A.I. Bateson, M. Beal, C.M. Charlotte, C. Deak, T. Edwards, E.F. Fernadez, A. Godinho, A. Hatch, L.T. Kakuschke, A. Lusseau, D. Martineau, D. Romero, L.M. Weilgart, L.S. Wintle, B.A. Notarbartolo-di-Sciara, G. and Martin, V. (2007) Anthropogenic noise as a stressor in animals: a multidisciplinary perspective. International Journal of Comparative Pyschology, 20, 250-273. Wysocki, L.E. Dittami, J.P. and Ladich, F. Ship noise and cortisol secretion in European freshwater fishes. Biological Conservation, 128, 501-508. Ydenber, R.C. and Dill, L.M. (1986) The economics of fleeing from predators. Advances in the Study of Behavior, 16, 229-249.