SlideShare a Scribd company logo
ForPeerReview
Only
Olfactory enrichment in California sea lions (Zalophus
californianus): An effective tool for captive welfare?
Journal: Journal of Applied Animal Welfare Science
Manuscript ID HAAW-2016-0004
Manuscript Type: Zoo/Wildlife
Keywords: Animal Welfare, California sea lions, Enrichment, Olfaction, Pinnipeds
URL: http://mc.manuscriptcentral.com/jaaws Email: ken.shapiro@animalsandsociety.org
Journal of Applied Animal Welfare Science
ForPeerReview
Only
Running head: OLFACTORY ENRICHMENT IN CALIFORNIA SEA LIONS 1
Olfactory Enrichment in California Sea Lions (Zalophus californianus): An
Effective Tool for Captive Welfare?
In the wild, California sea lions (Zalophus californianus) are exposed to a wide variety of
sensory information, which cannot be replicated in captive environments. Therefore,
unique procedures are necessary for the maintenance of physiological and psychological
health in captive animals. The effects of introducing of natural scents to captive
enclosures has been investigated in a variety of species, yet this has not been examined in
marine mammals. This project explores the behavioral effect of scent added to the
environment, with the goal of improving the welfare of captive sea lions. By introducing
two scent types: 1.) Natural scents, found in their native environment, and 2.) Non-
natural scents, not found in their native environment; this study examines not only scent
enrichment but the possible evolutionary underpinnings of pinniped olfaction. Scent
enrichment was found to significantly impact sea lion behavior as demonstrated by a
reduction in pattern swimming, an increase in habitat utilization, and a reduction in
stereotypical behavior. However, there were no differences in behavior between natural
and non-natural scent conditions.
Keywords: animal welfare, California sea lions, enrichment, olfaction, pinnipeds
Page 1 of 21
URL: http://mc.manuscriptcentral.com/jaaws Email: ken.shapiro@animalsandsociety.org
Journal of Applied Animal Welfare Science
1
2
3
4
5
6
7
8
9
10
11
12
13
14
15
16
17
18
19
20
21
22
23
24
25
26
27
28
29
30
31
32
33
34
35
36
37
38
39
40
41
42
43
44
45
46
47
48
49
50
51
52
53
54
55
56
57
58
59
60
ForPeerReview
Only
OLFACTORY ENRICHMENT IN CALIFORNIA SEA LIONS 2
Olfactory stimulation in California sea lions (Zalophus californianus): An effective
tool for captive welfare?
Introduction
In nature, wild animals encounter environmental stimulation from nearly infinite
sources. However, captive environments simply cannot compete with this diversity. As a
result, the environment must become more complex in order to maintain both the
physiological and psychological wellbeing of captive wildlife such as California sea lions
(Zalophus californianus; Carlstead, Seidensticker & Baldwin, 1991).
Scent is critical for effective foraging (Apfelbach, 1992; Gittleman, 1991;
Hughes, Price & Banks, 2010; Schwartz, Miller & Haroldson, 2003; Ylönen, Sundell,
Tiilikainen, Eccard, & Horne, 2003), navigation (Rogers, 1988), and social behaviors
(Beckoff, 1981; Rothman & Mech, 1979) within the caniform taxon, to which sea lions
belong. For the majority of caniform species, environmental enrichment has focused on
scent (i.e., Andrews & Ha, 2014; Kitchener & Asa, 2010; Leonard, 2008; Nelson, 2009;
Price, 2010; Rafacz & Santymire, 2014; Schneider, Nogge, & Kolter, 2014). Despite
extensive exploration of olfaction and scent enrichment in other caniform species, the
behavioral effects of scent enrichment has not previously been examined in pinnipeds.
Sea lions have maintained genes necessary for olfaction (Kishida, Kubota,
Shirayama, & Fukami, 2007), as well as extensive nasal turbinates (Van Valkenburgh,
Samuels, Bird, Fulkerson, Meachen Samuels, & Slater, 2011) indicating some need for
scent detection to survive in the wild. Therefore, it is likely that sea lions are biologically
prepared to obtain meaningful information from scents in their natural environment. For
example, South African fur seals (Arctocephalus pusillus), closely related to California
sea lions, have been shown to possess the capability to distinguish between chemically
Page 2 of 21
URL: http://mc.manuscriptcentral.com/jaaws Email: ken.shapiro@animalsandsociety.org
Journal of Applied Animal Welfare Science
1
2
3
4
5
6
7
8
9
10
11
12
13
14
15
16
17
18
19
20
21
22
23
24
25
26
27
28
29
30
31
32
33
34
35
36
37
38
39
40
41
42
43
44
45
46
47
48
49
50
51
52
53
54
55
56
57
58
59
60
ForPeerReview
Only
OLFACTORY ENRICHMENT IN CALIFORNIA SEA LIONS 3
similar odorants when relevant to their natural environment (Laska, Lord, Selin, &
Amundin, 2010). Thus, the introduction of natural scents into the environment may have
a greater impact on behavior than would non-natural scents.
The present study investigated whether scent can be utilized to enhance the
captive environment for California sea lions by introducing both natural and non-natural
scents and recording instances of abnormal and repetitive behavioral repertoires, called
stereotypical behaviors. In this study, the following hypotheses were examined: (1) Scent
enrichment will increase habitat utilization, indicated by a change in habitat usage
patterns, and reduce stereotypic behavior patterns; (1a) If present, these trends will
continue to be evident at both the enclosure and individual level; (2) Natural scents will
have a greater effect on behavior than non-natural scents, as indicated by a stronger effect
on habitat utilization patterns and a statistically significant difference in instances of
stereotypical behaviors; (2a) These trends will continue to be evident at both the
enclosure and individual level.
Methodology
Research was conducted at the Institute for Marine Mammal Studies (IMMS) in
Gulfport, Mississippi. Subjects included four, two-year-old female California sea lions
(Zalophus californianus). The sea lions were housed in pairs, with each enclosure
displaying differences in pattern swims and stereotypical behaviors (Table 1).
Scent enrichment was of two types, natural scents and non-natural scents. Natural
scents were those that are likely to be present in the animals’ natural environment. Non-
natural odorants were those that would be introduced through exposure to humans (Table
2).
Page 3 of 21
URL: http://mc.manuscriptcentral.com/jaaws Email: ken.shapiro@animalsandsociety.org
Journal of Applied Animal Welfare Science
1
2
3
4
5
6
7
8
9
10
11
12
13
14
15
16
17
18
19
20
21
22
23
24
25
26
27
28
29
30
31
32
33
34
35
36
37
38
39
40
41
42
43
44
45
46
47
48
49
50
51
52
53
54
55
56
57
58
59
60
ForPeerReview
Only
OLFACTORY ENRICHMENT IN CALIFORNIA SEA LIONS 4
Baseline Procedure
To begin this study, researchers collected baseline data over the span of one week
(six days) at IMMS. Researchers filmed one thirty-minute session in the morning, and
one in the afternoon (to parallel the experimental pre- and post- sessions). To eliminate
the confounding effect of a visual cue, this process included the introduction of a chalk
mark on the wall (with no added scent) during baseline pre-sessions. No scent treatment
was applied during the baseline post-session. In addition obtaining baseline data, the
animals were able to habituate to the researchers’ presence prior to the experimental
trials.
Experimental Procedure: Natural and Non-Natural Scents
Each trial consisted of a pre- and post-session. The pre-session data collection
period consisted of video data taken thirty minutes prior to the introduction of scent
stimulus. Similarly, the post-session data collection period consisted of video data taken
thirty minutes after the introduction of the scent stimulus. This approach allowed for the
assessment of the animals’ behavioral state prior to the introduction of scent enrichment,
and any potential changes as a result of this intervention.
Experimental trials were split into two scent types: natural and non-natural scents.
A total of 8 scents were presented three times each, resulting in twenty-four trials for per
animal (n = 4). Each trial consisted of a baseline pre-session in which a chalk mark was
placed on the wall and a post-session in which the scent was applied on top of the chalk
mark.
The order of presentation and placement of these scents within the enclosure were
pseudo-randomized. Each scent was presented once per cycle, and excluded placement
Page 4 of 21
URL: http://mc.manuscriptcentral.com/jaaws Email: ken.shapiro@animalsandsociety.org
Journal of Applied Animal Welfare Science
1
2
3
4
5
6
7
8
9
10
11
12
13
14
15
16
17
18
19
20
21
22
23
24
25
26
27
28
29
30
31
32
33
34
35
36
37
38
39
40
41
42
43
44
45
46
47
48
49
50
51
52
53
54
55
56
57
58
59
60
ForPeerReview
Only
OLFACTORY ENRICHMENT IN CALIFORNIA SEA LIONS 5
on the fourth wall (which was located on the same wall as the researcher, and was
obscured from view due to the film angle; see Figure 1). Trials were run simultaneously
in both enclosures in order to avoid the scent being detected by animals in the adjacent
enclosure. After the trial, the scent location was cleaned with a bleach solution to ensure
the enclosure was free of the scent for the subsequent trial. One trial was executed each
day so that the scent had been eliminated prior to the subsequent scent trial.
Video data was collected on site by two researchers stationed outside of each
enclosure (Figure 1). Data was then coded onto a Microsoft Excel spreadsheet in fifteen-
second intervals to examine habitat usage, behavioral state, and stereotypic behaviors
before and after the introduction of the enrichment (Table 3). To guarantee reliability, an
independent coder was employed, reaching 92% agreement on 20% of the video data.
In order to assess the general impact of the study on individual animals, the
researchers planned to examine overall habitat usage and the exhibition of stereotypical
behaviors in all four sea lions housed at IMMS using an ANOVA with Tukey’s Post-Hoc
Test. Following this analysis, independent t-tests were utilized to examine pre-session
and post-session changes in behavior between in each scent condition. Data was
interpreted using an alpha level of .05.
Hypothesis I Results
Hypothesis one was examined to determine if scent enrichment was effective in
increasing habitat utilization, decreasing pattern swimming, and reducing stereotypical
behaviors, for these captive California sea lions.
To assess the differences in the means for habitat utilization during the pre-
session and post-session experimental sessions (including both natural and non-natural
Page 5 of 21
URL: http://mc.manuscriptcentral.com/jaaws Email: ken.shapiro@animalsandsociety.org
Journal of Applied Animal Welfare Science
1
2
3
4
5
6
7
8
9
10
11
12
13
14
15
16
17
18
19
20
21
22
23
24
25
26
27
28
29
30
31
32
33
34
35
36
37
38
39
40
41
42
43
44
45
46
47
48
49
50
51
52
53
54
55
56
57
58
59
60
ForPeerReview
Only
OLFACTORY ENRICHMENT IN CALIFORNIA SEA LIONS 6
scent sessions) a one-way ANOVA was utilized to compare the main effect of scent on
habitat utilization across the six locations (locations 0-5). This analysis indicated that
there was a main effect of scent in all locations except location 5 (center), with the sea
lions spending more time out of the pool and less time in the pool quadrants after the
scent enrichment was in place (Figure 2).
Both enclosures showed similar effects of scent on habitat utilization. Sea lions in
both enclosure one and two spent more time out of the water (enclosure one location 0
t(94)=-4.361, p = .000; enclosure two location 0 t(94) = -3.549, p =.001) and less time at
locations 1, 2, 3, and 4 (enclosure one location 1, t(94) = 5.335, p = .000, location 2 t(94)
= 2.506, p = .014, location 3 t(94) = 2.333, p = .022, location 4 t(94) = 3.875, p = .000;
enclosure two location 1 t(94) = 4.386, p = .000, 2 t(94) = 3.846, p = .000, 3 t(94) =
3.854, p = .000, and 4 t(94) = 4.303, p = .000). Time spent in the center of the pool
(location 5) did not differ significantly in either enclosure (see Figure 3).
These general findings held true for individual sea lions as well. Gabby, Sage, and
Kaytee all spent significantly more time out of the water (location 0) and less time in
locations 1-4. In contrast, Maya decreased time spent in location 1 (t(46) = 2.729, p =
.009), evenly dispersing that time spent across the other areas of the enclosure. However,
none of the animals significantly altered the amount of time spent in the center of the
pool (location 5; Figure 4).
Pattern Swimming
There was a reduction in pattern swimming (circle swimming t(189) = 6.728, p =
.000 and diagonal swimming t(189) = 2.338, p = .020) when scents were present for all
four animals. There was a significant reduction in both circle (t(94) = 4.548, p =.000) and
Page 6 of 21
URL: http://mc.manuscriptcentral.com/jaaws Email: ken.shapiro@animalsandsociety.org
Journal of Applied Animal Welfare Science
1
2
3
4
5
6
7
8
9
10
11
12
13
14
15
16
17
18
19
20
21
22
23
24
25
26
27
28
29
30
31
32
33
34
35
36
37
38
39
40
41
42
43
44
45
46
47
48
49
50
51
52
53
54
55
56
57
58
59
60
ForPeerReview
Only
OLFACTORY ENRICHMENT IN CALIFORNIA SEA LIONS 7
diagonal swimming (t(94) = 2.647, p = .010) in enclosure one and sharp reduction in
circle swimming in enclosure two (t(94) = 5.214, p = .000).
Within enclosure one, the predominant swim pattern was diagonal swimming,
which decreased when scents are present (t(94) = 2.676, p = .009). Circle swimming,
although rarely observed, did reduce when scents are present (t(94) = 4.619, p = .000).
Circle swimming in enclosure two also decreased significantly in the experimental
sessions (t(94) = 5.214, p = .000). Diagonal swimming was not observed in enclosure two
throughout the duration of this study, and was therefore not investigated at the individual
level.
In enclosure one, both Gabby and Maya showed a reduction in circle swimming
(Gabby t(44) = 4.048, p = .000; Maya t(44) = 2.829, p = .007). However, only Gabby
showed a significant reduction in diagonal swimming (Gabby t(44) = . 2.657, p = .011;
Maya t(44) = 1.143, p = .260, d = .345). When scents were present, in enclosure two,
both sea lions exhibited reduced circle swimming (Sage t(44) = 3.098, p = .003; Kaytee
t(44) = 3.875, p = .000). This was the only pattern swim to show a reduction at the
enclosure level as diagonal swims were not observed in this enclosure.
Stereotypical Behaviors
Both Gabby and Sage exhibited unique stereotypies, as well as individualized
reactions to scent. Gabby exhibited an increase in pool wall sucking (t(44) = -2.733, p =
.012) in the presence of scent. Sage exhibited a slight increase in floor sucking (t(45) = -
.719, p =.476, d = .212), while reducing tail sucking (t(45) = .130, p = .898, d = .038),
regurgitation (t(45) = 1.026, p = .310, d = .302), and reingestion (t(45) = .864, p = .392, d
= .254); for operational definitions of these behaviors refer to Table 3).
Page 7 of 21
URL: http://mc.manuscriptcentral.com/jaaws Email: ken.shapiro@animalsandsociety.org
Journal of Applied Animal Welfare Science
1
2
3
4
5
6
7
8
9
10
11
12
13
14
15
16
17
18
19
20
21
22
23
24
25
26
27
28
29
30
31
32
33
34
35
36
37
38
39
40
41
42
43
44
45
46
47
48
49
50
51
52
53
54
55
56
57
58
59
60
ForPeerReview
Only
OLFACTORY ENRICHMENT IN CALIFORNIA SEA LIONS 8
Hypothesis II Results
In evaluation of hypothesis two, the analysis demonstrated that the sea lions’
habitat usage did not significantly differ between natural scents and non-natural scents. In
addition, independent sample t-tests showed no differences for the enclosures, or for the
individual animals.
In the natural scent sessions, sea lions exhibited a reduction in both circle (natural
scent session t(94) = 5.047, p = .000; non-natural scent session t(94) = 4.528, p = .000)
and diagonal swimming (natural scent sessions t(94) = 1.986, p = .051, d = .41; non-
natural scent conditions, t(94) = 1.251, p = .215, d = .26). Within enclosure one, animals
were found to alter their habitat usage patterns in the presence of a natural scent, although
not significantly. Similarly, in enclosure two, a non-significant reduction in circle
swimming was found for both Sage and Kaytee when natural scents were present in the
enclosure. Although not statistically significant, it was noteworthy that diagonal
swimming did decrease in both sessions and was very near significance in the natural
scent sessions. This lack of significance may simply be due to the fact that this behavior
was only observed in one enclosure.
Pattern swimming in animals within enclosure one were not found to differ
significantly between natural and non-natural scent sessions (circle swimming, t(46) =
.499, p = .621, d = .147) and diagonal swimming (t(46) = -.089, p = .929, d = .026). In
enclosure two, animals were not found to differ significantly between natural and non-
natural scent sessions (t(46) = -.500, p = .617, d =.147). However, at the individual level
slight changes in behavior are evident.
Page 8 of 21
URL: http://mc.manuscriptcentral.com/jaaws Email: ken.shapiro@animalsandsociety.org
Journal of Applied Animal Welfare Science
1
2
3
4
5
6
7
8
9
10
11
12
13
14
15
16
17
18
19
20
21
22
23
24
25
26
27
28
29
30
31
32
33
34
35
36
37
38
39
40
41
42
43
44
45
46
47
48
49
50
51
52
53
54
55
56
57
58
59
60
ForPeerReview
Only
OLFACTORY ENRICHMENT IN CALIFORNIA SEA LIONS 9
Although not significant, Gabby, Sage, and Kaytee demonstrated a reduction in
circle swimming when natural scents were present (Gabby t(22) = .834, p = .419, d =
.356; Sage t(22) = -.276, p = .785, d = .117; Kaytee t(22) = -.134, p = .895, d = .057).
Maya, alternately, spent more time circle swimming in the natural sessions (t(22) = -.502,
p = .621, d = .214). Similarly, Gabby and Maya both spent less time diagonal swimming
in the natural sessions, although these results were not significant (Gabby t(22) = -.398, p
=.694, d = .169; Maya t(22) = .327, p = .747, d = .139). Neither Sage nor Kaytee engaged
in diagonal pattern swimming during the course of the experimental session, therefore
statistical analysis for this behavior cannot be provided.
Individual variation was evident when stereotypical behaviors were observed in
Gabby and Sage, who exhibited non-significant decreases in individualized stereotypies
in natural scent sessions. Specifically, Gabby’s pool wall sucking behavior decreased,
albeit not significantly, in the natural scent sessions when compared with the non-natural
scent sessions (t(21) = -.334, p = .742, d = .142). Similarly, Sage exhibited non-
significant decreases in floor sucking behavior (t(22) = -.593, p = .559, d = .252), tail
sucking (t(22) = -.250, p =.806, d = .106), regurgitation (t(22) = -1.517, p = .147, d =
.646), and reingestion (t(22) =. 928, p = .373, d = .396).
Discussion
Overall, scent was found to be enriching as it caused an increase in habitat
utilization, a reduction in pattern swimming, and a reduction in the majority of
stereotypical behaviors both across and within individuals. While the second hypothesis -
that sea lions would react more strongly to natural scents than non-natural scents - was
not supported, sea lions responded to both types of scents in their environment in a
Page 9 of 21
URL: http://mc.manuscriptcentral.com/jaaws Email: ken.shapiro@animalsandsociety.org
Journal of Applied Animal Welfare Science
1
2
3
4
5
6
7
8
9
10
11
12
13
14
15
16
17
18
19
20
21
22
23
24
25
26
27
28
29
30
31
32
33
34
35
36
37
38
39
40
41
42
43
44
45
46
47
48
49
50
51
52
53
54
55
56
57
58
59
60
ForPeerReview
Only
OLFACTORY ENRICHMENT IN CALIFORNIA SEA LIONS 10
positive manner by exhibiting a reduction in stereotypical behavior (e.g., pattern
swimming, oral stereotypies, and voluntary emesis). Although individual stereotypies,
such as regurgitation, were not found to be statistically significant, the large effect sizes
found in this analysis suggest that there was indeed a practical reduction in these
problematic behaviors.
Additionally, sea lions were observed by researchers to explore old scent markers
immediately upon discovering new scent markers, indicating that they may recall where
previous scents were placed. This unique observation supports the idea that sea lions are
responding to their environment using cognitive mechanisms, and are not solely attending
to scents pertinent to their natural environment. This finding suggests that there are many
unexplored avenues of research beyond the realms of environmental salient stimuli that
could be investigated for this species.
In addition to zoos and aquaria, rehabilitation facilities may benefit from this
research. Pinniped rehabilitation centers are often comprised of sterile environments that
do not allow for naturalistic enclosures and extensive enrichment procedures. This may
be due, in part, to the staff’s attempts to reduce human-animal interactions in order to
discourage habituation, facilitate disease control, and reduce cost. However scent
enrichment encourages species-typical behaviors, through increased habitat utilization,
(e.g. Fay & Miller, 2015). Moreover, this effect can be achieved at little or no cost to the
center and may be used to keep the animals cognitively engaged during their stay at the
rehabilitation center. Additionally, environmental enrichment has been demonstrated to
assist in the rehabilitation of animals suffering from a variety of conditions including:
brain lesions (De Bartolo, Leggio, Mandolesi, Foti, Ferlazzo & Pertosini, 2008; Paban,
Page 10 of 21
URL: http://mc.manuscriptcentral.com/jaaws Email: ken.shapiro@animalsandsociety.org
Journal of Applied Animal Welfare Science
1
2
3
4
5
6
7
8
9
10
11
12
13
14
15
16
17
18
19
20
21
22
23
24
25
26
27
28
29
30
31
32
33
34
35
36
37
38
39
40
41
42
43
44
45
46
47
48
49
50
51
52
53
54
55
56
57
58
59
60
ForPeerReview
Only
OLFACTORY ENRICHMENT IN CALIFORNIA SEA LIONS 11
Jaffard, Chambon, Malafosse & Alescio-Lautier, 2004), lead poisoning (Guilarte,
Toscano, McGlothan & Weaver, 2002), domoic acid toxicosis (Cook, Bernard &
Reichmuth, 2011), and malnutrition (Rose, 1988).
Although currently classified as a species of least concern by the IUCN Redlist,
sea lion strandings are highly influenced by the effects of climate change (Greig,
Gulland, & Kreuder, 2005; Hui, 2011; Keledjian & Mesnick, 2013). As these effects
continue to escalate, sea lion rehabilitation programs will likely continue to be a necessity
for the maintenance of a healthy California sea lion population. Thus, information
relating to low-cost enrichment that may increase survival rates post-release will only
assist rehabilitators in these endeavors.
Page 11 of 21
URL: http://mc.manuscriptcentral.com/jaaws Email: ken.shapiro@animalsandsociety.org
Journal of Applied Animal Welfare Science
1
2
3
4
5
6
7
8
9
10
11
12
13
14
15
16
17
18
19
20
21
22
23
24
25
26
27
28
29
30
31
32
33
34
35
36
37
38
39
40
41
42
43
44
45
46
47
48
49
50
51
52
53
54
55
56
57
58
59
60
ForPeerReview
Only
OLFACTORY ENRICHMENT IN CALIFORNIA SEA LIONS 12
References
Andrews, N. L. P. & Ha, J. C. (2014). The effects of automated scatter feeders on captive
grizzly bear activity budgets. Journal of Applied Animal Welfare Science, 17(2)
148-156.
Apfelbach, R. (1992). Ontogenetic olfactory experience and adult searching behavior
in the carnivorous ferret. In R. L. Doty & D. Müller-Schwarze (Eds.) Chemical
Signals in Vertebrates (6th
Ed.). New York, NY: Springer.
Beckoff, M. (1981). Mammalian sibling interactions: Genes, facilitative environments
and the coefficient of familiarity (Pp. 307-346). In D. J. Gubernick & P. H.
Klopfer (Eds.) Parental Care in Mammals, New York, NY: Plenum Press.
Carlstead, K., Seidensticker, J. & Baldwin, R. (1991). Environmental enrichment for zoo
housed bears. Zoo Biology, 10(3), 3-16.
Cook, P., Bernard, A. K., & Reichmuth, C. (2011). Which way did I go?: Remote
training of a spatial memory task to assess the effects of domoic acid exposure in
stranded California sea lions (Zalophus californianus), Soundings, 36(2), 16-19.
De Bartolo, P., Leggio, M. G., Mandolesi, L., Foti, F., Gelfo, F., Ferlazzo, F., &
Pertosini, L. (2008). Environmental enrichment mitigates the effects of basal
forebrain lesions on cognitive flexibility. Neuroscience, 154, 444-453.
Fay, C. & Miller, L. J. (2015). Utilizing scents as environmental enrichment: Preference
assessment and application with Rothschild Giraffe. Animal Behavior and
Cognition, 2(3), 285-291.
Gittleman, J. L. (1991). Carnivore olfactory bulb size: Allometry, phylogeny and
ecology. Journal of Zoology, 225(2), 253-272.
Greig, D. J., Gulland, F. M. D., & Kreuder, C. (2005). A decade of live sea lions
(Zalophus californianus) strandings along the central California coast: Causes
and trends, 1991-2000. Aquatic Mammals, 31(1), 11-22.
Guilarte, T. R., Toscano, C. D., McGlothan, J. L., & Weaver, S. A. (2002).
Environmentalenrichment reverses cognitive and molecular deficits induced by
developmentallead exposure. Annals of Neurology, 53(1), 50-56.
Hui, T. C. Y. (2011). Steller sea lions and fisheries: Competition at sea? M.S. Thesis.
The University of British Columbia, CA.
Hughes, N. K., Price, C. J., & Banks, P. B. (2010). Predators are attracted to olfactory
signals of prey. PLOS ONE 5(9), 1-4
Page 12 of 21
URL: http://mc.manuscriptcentral.com/jaaws Email: ken.shapiro@animalsandsociety.org
Journal of Applied Animal Welfare Science
1
2
3
4
5
6
7
8
9
10
11
12
13
14
15
16
17
18
19
20
21
22
23
24
25
26
27
28
29
30
31
32
33
34
35
36
37
38
39
40
41
42
43
44
45
46
47
48
49
50
51
52
53
54
55
56
57
58
59
60
ForPeerReview
Only
OLFACTORY ENRICHMENT IN CALIFORNIA SEA LIONS 13
IUCN, Conservation International and NatureServe. (2014). California sea lions. IUCN
Red List. http://www.iucnredlist.org/details/41666/0
Keledjian, A. J. & Mesnick, S. (2013). The impacts of El Niño conditions on California
sea lions (Zalophus californianus) fisheries interactions: Predicting spatial and
temporal hotspots along the California coast. Aquatic Mammals, 39(3), 221-232.
Kishida, T., Kubota, S., Shirayama Y., & Fukami, H. (2007). The olfactory receptor
gene repertoires in secondary-adapted marine vertebrates: Evidence for
reduction of the functional proportions in cetaceans. Biology Letters, 3, 428-
430.
Kitchener, A. C. & Asa, C. S. (2010). Editorial: Bears and canids. International Zoo
Yearbook, 44(1), 7-15.
Leonard, D. A. (2008). An evaluation of environmental enrichment for two highlysocial
and endangered canid species, the African wild dog (Lycaon pictus) and Mexican
wolf (Canis lupus) (Doctoral Dissertation). Saint Louis University, St. Louis MO.
Nelson, K. O. (2009). Environmental enrichment effects on the activity of a nearactic
river otter. Master’s Thesis. The Rochester Institute of Technology.
Paban, V., Jaffard, M., Chambon, M., Malafosse, M., & Alescio-Lautier, B. (2004). Time
course of behavioral changes following basal forebrain cholinergic damage in
rats: Environmental enrichment as a therapeutic intervention. Neuroscience, 132,
13-32.
Peterson, R.S. & Bartholomew, G. A. (1969). Airborne vocal communication in the
California sea lion (Zalophus californianus). Animal Behaviour, 17(1), 17-18.
Price, L. J. (2010). A preliminary study of the effects of environmental enrichment on
the behaviour of captive African wild dogs (Lycaon pictus). Bioscience
Horizons, 0(0), 1-9.
Rafacz, M. L. & Santymire, R. M. (2014). Using odor cues to elicit a behavioral and
hormonal response in zoo-housed African wild dogs. Zoo Biology, 3(2), 144
149.
Rogers, L. L. (1988). Homing tendencies of large mammals: A review. U.S. Forest
Service North Central Forest Experiment Station. St Paul, MN.
Rose, F. D. (1988). Environmental enrichment and recovery of function following brain
damage in the rat. Medical Science Research, 16(6), 257-263.
Rothman, R. J. & Mech, L. D. (1979). Scent-making in lone wolves and newly formed
pairs. Animal Behaviour, 27(3), 750-752.
Page 13 of 21
URL: http://mc.manuscriptcentral.com/jaaws Email: ken.shapiro@animalsandsociety.org
Journal of Applied Animal Welfare Science
1
2
3
4
5
6
7
8
9
10
11
12
13
14
15
16
17
18
19
20
21
22
23
24
25
26
27
28
29
30
31
32
33
34
35
36
37
38
39
40
41
42
43
44
45
46
47
48
49
50
51
52
53
54
55
56
57
58
59
60
ForPeerReview
Only
OLFACTORY ENRICHMENT IN CALIFORNIA SEA LIONS 14
Schneider, M., Nogge, G., & Kolter, L. (2014). Implementing unpredictability in feeding
enrichment for Malayan sun bears (Helarctos malayanus). Zoo Biology, 33(1),
54-62.
Schwartz, C. C., Miller S. D., & Haroldson, M. A. (2003). Grizzly bear (Pp. 556-586). In
G. A. Feldhammer, B.C. Thompson, & A. J. Champman (Eds). Wild Mammals
of North America: Biology, Management, and Conservation (2nd
Ed.). Baltimore,
MD: Johns Hopkins University Press.
Steele, R. M. & Steele, T. (2005). The use of canid urine and related scents for the
enrichment of captive grey wolves (Canis lupus). Regional Meeting
Proceedings: Association of Zoos and Aquariums Proceedings.
Szokalski, M. S., Litchfied, C. A., & Foster, W. K. (2012). Enrichment for captive tigers
(Panthera tigris): Current knowledge and future directions. Applied Animal
Behavior Science, 139(1-2), 1-9.
Van Valkenburgh, B., Curtis, A., Samuels, J. X., Bird, D., Fulkerson, B., Meachen
Samuels, J., & Slater, G. J. (2011). Aquatic adaptations in the nose of
carnivorans: Evidence from the turbinates. Journal of Anatomy, 218(3), 298-
310.
Wells, D. L. (2004). Review of environmental enrichment for kenneled dogs (Canis
familiaris). Applied Animal Behaviour Science, 85 (3-4), 307-317.
Wells, D. L. (2009). Sensory stimulation as environmental enrichment for captive
animals: A review. Applied Animal Behaviour Science, 118(1-2), 1-11.
Ylönen, H., Sundell, J., Tiilikainen, R., Eccard, J. A., & Horne, T. (2003). Weasel’s
(Mustela nivalis nivalis) preference for olfactory cues of the vole
(Clethrionomys glareolus). Ecology, 84(6), 1447-1452.
Page 14 of 21
URL: http://mc.manuscriptcentral.com/jaaws Email: ken.shapiro@animalsandsociety.org
Journal of Applied Animal Welfare Science
1
2
3
4
5
6
7
8
9
10
11
12
13
14
15
16
17
18
19
20
21
22
23
24
25
26
27
28
29
30
31
32
33
34
35
36
37
38
39
40
41
42
43
44
45
46
47
48
49
50
51
52
53
54
55
56
57
58
59
60
ForPeerReview
Only
OLFACTORY ENRICHMENT IN CALIFORNIA SEA LIONS 15
Table 1. Summary of Subjects
Animal Age Sex Enclosure Dominant
Swim Pattern
Individual
Stereotypies
Maya 2 Female 1 Diagonal N/A
Gabby 2 Female 1 Diagonal Pool wall
sucking
Sage 2 Female 2 Circle Tail & Floor
Sucking
Kaytee 2 Female 2 Circle N/A
Page 15 of 21
URL: http://mc.manuscriptcentral.com/jaaws Email: ken.shapiro@animalsandsociety.org
Journal of Applied Animal Welfare Science
1
2
3
4
5
6
7
8
9
10
11
12
13
14
15
16
17
18
19
20
21
22
23
24
25
26
27
28
29
30
31
32
33
34
35
36
37
38
39
40
41
42
43
44
45
46
47
48
49
50
51
52
53
54
55
56
57
58
59
60
ForPeerReview
Only
OLFACTORY ENRICHMENT IN CALIFORNIA SEA LIONS 16
Table 2. List of Scents
Scent Type Application Method
Sterilized potting soil (no
fertilizer added)
Natural 1 cup was rubbed on the
wall.
Sterilized playground sand Natural 1 cup was rubbed on the
wall.
Kelp Natural 100% human food grade
dried kelp: approximately 1
cup of dried sea kelp was
wet down with saltwater and
then rubbed on the wall.
Sardine oil Natural 100% biodegradable, water-
soluble, sardine bait oil scent
(1 tsp.) was applied to a
wall.
Orange Non-natural One large orange was cut in
half and rubbed on the wall.
Banana Non-natural One large banana was cut in
half and rubbed on the wall.
Vanilla extract Non-natural 1 tsp. of 100% food grade
vanilla extract was applied to
a wall.
Cinnamon Non-natural 1 tsp. of 100% dried food-
grade cinnamon was applied
to a wall.
Page 16 of 21
URL: http://mc.manuscriptcentral.com/jaaws Email: ken.shapiro@animalsandsociety.org
Journal of Applied Animal Welfare Science
1
2
3
4
5
6
7
8
9
10
11
12
13
14
15
16
17
18
19
20
21
22
23
24
25
26
27
28
29
30
31
32
33
34
35
36
37
38
39
40
41
42
43
44
45
46
47
48
49
50
51
52
53
54
55
56
57
58
59
60
ForPeerReview
Only
OLFACTORY ENRICHMENT IN CALIFORNIA SEA LIONS 17
Table 3. Behavioral Ethogram
Behavior Description
Circle Swimming Swimming in a fixed pattern, which is the same at all points, in a clockwise or
counterclockwise fashion.
Diagonal
Swimming
Floor Sucking
Pacing
Pool Wall Sucking
Tail Sucking
Swimming in a fixed pattern, which is the same at all points, crossing diagonally across
the pool (including locations 1, 5, and 4 or 2, 5, and 3).
Placing the mouth directly on the floor in order to suck, audibly and visually, off of the
floor.
Walking in a fixed pattern, which is the same at all points.
Placing mouth directly on pool wall, and remaining in one location for more than a two
second increment.
Curling into an “o” shape to make contact with the tail using one’s mouth followed by an
audible sucking sound.
Page 17 of 21
URL: http://mc.manuscriptcentral.com/jaaws Email: ken.shapiro@animalsandsociety.org
Journal of Applied Animal Welfare Science
1
2
3
4
5
6
7
8
9
10
11
12
13
14
15
16
17
18
19
20
21
22
23
24
25
26
27
28
29
30
31
32
33
34
35
36
37
38
39
40
41
42
43
44
45
46
47
48
49
50
51
52
53
54
55
56
57
58
59
60
ForPeerReview
Only
OLFACTORY ENRICHMENT IN CALIFORNIA SEA LIONS 18
Figure 1. Enclosure layout
Page 18 of 21
URL: http://mc.manuscriptcentral.com/jaaws Email: ken.shapiro@animalsandsociety.org
Journal of Applied Animal Welfare Science
1
2
3
4
5
6
7
8
9
10
11
12
13
14
15
16
17
18
19
20
21
22
23
24
25
26
27
28
29
30
31
32
33
34
35
36
37
38
39
40
41
42
43
44
45
46
47
48
49
50
51
52
53
54
55
56
57
58
59
60
ForPeerReview
Only
OLFACTORY ENRICHMENT IN CALIFORNIA SEA LIONS 19
Figure 2. Overall mean location frequencies in experimental pre- and post-sessions
for the effect of scent enrichment on habitat utilizationPre-session
Location
0
Post-session
Location
0
Pre-session
Location
1
Post-session
Location
1
Pre-session
Location
2
Post-session
Location
2
Pre-session
Location
3
Post-session
Location
3
Pre-session
Location
4
Post-session
Location
4
Pre-session
Location
5
Post-session
Location
5
0
50
100
150
Locations
MeanFrequenciesinLocations
*
**
*
*
Habitat Utilization in All Four Animals
Page 19 of 21
URL: http://mc.manuscriptcentral.com/jaaws Email: ken.shapiro@animalsandsociety.org
Journal of Applied Animal Welfare Science
1
2
3
4
5
6
7
8
9
10
11
12
13
14
15
16
17
18
19
20
21
22
23
24
25
26
27
28
29
30
31
32
33
34
35
36
37
38
39
40
41
42
43
44
45
46
47
48
49
50
51
52
53
54
55
56
57
58
59
60
ForPeerReview
Only
OLFACTORY ENRICHMENT IN CALIFORNIA SEA LIONS 20
Figure 3. Changes in location between experimental pre- and post-sessions by
enclosurePre-session
Location
0
Post-session
Location
0
Pre-session
Location
1
Post-session
Location
1
Pre-session
Location
2
Post-session
Location
2
Pre-session
Location
3
Post-session
Location
3
Pre-session
Location
4
Post-session
Location
4
Pre-session
Location
5
Post-session
Location
5
Pre-session
Location
0
Post-session
Location
0
Pre-session
Location
1
Post-session
Location
1
Pre-session
Location
2
Post-session
Location
2
Pre-session
Location
3
Post-session
Location
3
Pre-session
Location
4
Post-session
Location
4
Pre-session
Location
5
Post-session
Location
5
0
50
100
150
Locations
MeanFrequenciesinLocations
Habitat Utilization in Enclosures 1 and 2
*
*
* * *
*
*
* *
*
Enclosure 1 Enclosure 2
Page 20 of 21
URL: http://mc.manuscriptcentral.com/jaaws Email: ken.shapiro@animalsandsociety.org
Journal of Applied Animal Welfare Science
1
2
3
4
5
6
7
8
9
10
11
12
13
14
15
16
17
18
19
20
21
22
23
24
25
26
27
28
29
30
31
32
33
34
35
36
37
38
39
40
41
42
43
44
45
46
47
48
49
50
51
52
53
54
55
56
57
58
59
60
ForPeerReview
Only
OLFACTORY ENRICHMENT IN CALIFORNIA SEA LIONS 21
Figure 4. Pre-session and post-session changes in habitat utilization by animal
Pre-session
Location
0
Post-session
Location
0
Pre-session
Location
1
Post-session
Location
1
Pre-session
Location
2
Post-session
Location
2
Pre-session
Location
3
Post-session
Location
3
Pre-session
Location
4
Post-session
Location
4
Pre-session
Location
5
Post-session
Location
5
0
50
100
150
Locations
MeanFrequenciesinLocations
Gabby
*
*
*
*
*
Pre-session
Location
0
Post-session
Location
0
Pre-session
Location
1
Post-session
Location
1
Pre-session
Location
2
Post-session
Location
2
Pre-session
Location
3
Post-session
Location
3
Pre-session
Location
4
Post-session
Location
4
Pre-session
Location
5
Post-session
Location
5
0
50
100
150
Locations
MeanFrequenciesinLocations
Maya
* *
Pre-session
Location
0
Post-session
Location
0
Pre-session
Location
1
Post-session
Location
1
Pre-session
Location
2
Post-session
Location
2
Pre-session
Location
3
Post-session
Location
3
Pre-session
Location
4
Post-session
Location
4
Pre-session
Location
5
Post-session
Location
5
0
50
100
150
Locations
MeanFrequenciesinLocations
Sage
*
*
* * *
Pre-session
Location
0
Post-session
Location
0
Pre-session
Location
1
Post-session
Location
1
Pre-session
Location
2
Post-session
Location
2
Pre-session
Location
3
Post-session
Location
3
Pre-session
Location
4
Post-session
Location
4
Pre-session
Location
5
Post-session
Location
5
0
50
100
150
Locations
MeanFrequenciesinLocations
Kaytee
*
* * * *
Page 21 of 21
URL: http://mc.manuscriptcentral.com/jaaws Email: ken.shapiro@animalsandsociety.org
Journal of Applied Animal Welfare Science
1
2
3
4
5
6
7
8
9
10
11
12
13
14
15
16
17
18
19
20
21
22
23
24
25
26
27
28
29
30
31
32
33
34
35
36
37
38
39
40
41
42
43
44
45
46
47
48
49
50
51
52
53
54
55
56
57
58
59
60

More Related Content

What's hot

2002 sa r.o. & langone j.a. 2002 the tadpole of
2002 sa r.o. & langone j.a. 2002 the tadpole of2002 sa r.o. & langone j.a. 2002 the tadpole of
2002 sa r.o. & langone j.a. 2002 the tadpole of
Pedro Taucce
 
Protazioetal2014online
Protazioetal2014onlineProtazioetal2014online
Protazioetal2014online
Arielson Protázio
 
Keeley et al_02-15
Keeley et al_02-15Keeley et al_02-15
Keeley et al_02-15Kate
 
CV_NLS_November2015
CV_NLS_November2015CV_NLS_November2015
CV_NLS_November2015Nick Smith
 
J. Sequeira Final Sample Proposal
J. Sequeira Final Sample ProposalJ. Sequeira Final Sample Proposal
J. Sequeira Final Sample ProposalJohn Sequeira
 
Predation, habitat use and impact of Pterois
Predation, habitat use and impact of PteroisPredation, habitat use and impact of Pterois
Predation, habitat use and impact of Pterois
Loretta Roberson
 
Fuong et al._09-14_Animal Behaviour
Fuong et al._09-14_Animal BehaviourFuong et al._09-14_Animal Behaviour
Fuong et al._09-14_Animal BehaviourKate
 
X International conference on Behaviour, Physiology and Genetic of Wildlife -...
X International conference on Behaviour, Physiology and Genetic of Wildlife -...X International conference on Behaviour, Physiology and Genetic of Wildlife -...
X International conference on Behaviour, Physiology and Genetic of Wildlife -...Dalila Frasson
 
2014 Vianna et al. Acoustic telemetry validates citizen science for monitorin...
2014 Vianna et al. Acoustic telemetry validates citizen science for monitorin...2014 Vianna et al. Acoustic telemetry validates citizen science for monitorin...
2014 Vianna et al. Acoustic telemetry validates citizen science for monitorin...Gabriel Maciel de Souza Vianna
 
6626907 83-population-ecology
6626907 83-population-ecology6626907 83-population-ecology
6626907 83-population-ecologyNgaire Taylor
 
G1074651
G1074651G1074651
G1074651
IJERD Editor
 
Chapter 8.3 presentation
Chapter 8.3 presentationChapter 8.3 presentation
Chapter 8.3 presentationAngela Huey
 
Emily Shultz-Optimized Seperation of Estuarin Plankton to Determine Associati...
Emily Shultz-Optimized Seperation of Estuarin Plankton to Determine Associati...Emily Shultz-Optimized Seperation of Estuarin Plankton to Determine Associati...
Emily Shultz-Optimized Seperation of Estuarin Plankton to Determine Associati...Emily Shultz, M.S.
 
Ecological Monitoring Techniques
Ecological Monitoring TechniquesEcological Monitoring Techniques
Ecological Monitoring Techniques
Gururaja KV
 
Bachelor's thesis _ Current and historical distribution of the endemic Santa ...
Bachelor's thesis _ Current and historical distribution of the endemic Santa ...Bachelor's thesis _ Current and historical distribution of the endemic Santa ...
Bachelor's thesis _ Current and historical distribution of the endemic Santa ...
DeannaRhoades
 
RIVER NETWORKS AS ECOLOGICAL CORRIDORS FOR SPECIES POPULATIONS AND WATER-BORN...
RIVER NETWORKS AS ECOLOGICAL CORRIDORS FOR SPECIES POPULATIONS AND WATER-BORN...RIVER NETWORKS AS ECOLOGICAL CORRIDORS FOR SPECIES POPULATIONS AND WATER-BORN...
RIVER NETWORKS AS ECOLOGICAL CORRIDORS FOR SPECIES POPULATIONS AND WATER-BORN...
Riccardo Rigon
 
Wildlife Society Bulletin 2016 CraggTechniques for monitoring Brachyramphusmu...
Wildlife Society Bulletin 2016 CraggTechniques for monitoring Brachyramphusmu...Wildlife Society Bulletin 2016 CraggTechniques for monitoring Brachyramphusmu...
Wildlife Society Bulletin 2016 CraggTechniques for monitoring Brachyramphusmu...Jenna Cragg
 
Phylogeography and genetic diversity of Baetodes huaico (EPHEMEROPTERA: BAETI...
Phylogeography and genetic diversity of Baetodes huaico (EPHEMEROPTERA: BAETI...Phylogeography and genetic diversity of Baetodes huaico (EPHEMEROPTERA: BAETI...
Phylogeography and genetic diversity of Baetodes huaico (EPHEMEROPTERA: BAETI...
jcgjuancruz
 
Mello_SMM 2011 Poster 9 (2)
Mello_SMM 2011 Poster 9 (2)Mello_SMM 2011 Poster 9 (2)
Mello_SMM 2011 Poster 9 (2)Megan Mello
 

What's hot (20)

2002 sa r.o. & langone j.a. 2002 the tadpole of
2002 sa r.o. & langone j.a. 2002 the tadpole of2002 sa r.o. & langone j.a. 2002 the tadpole of
2002 sa r.o. & langone j.a. 2002 the tadpole of
 
Protazioetal2014online
Protazioetal2014onlineProtazioetal2014online
Protazioetal2014online
 
Tang_Sedgwick_Paper
Tang_Sedgwick_PaperTang_Sedgwick_Paper
Tang_Sedgwick_Paper
 
Keeley et al_02-15
Keeley et al_02-15Keeley et al_02-15
Keeley et al_02-15
 
CV_NLS_November2015
CV_NLS_November2015CV_NLS_November2015
CV_NLS_November2015
 
J. Sequeira Final Sample Proposal
J. Sequeira Final Sample ProposalJ. Sequeira Final Sample Proposal
J. Sequeira Final Sample Proposal
 
Predation, habitat use and impact of Pterois
Predation, habitat use and impact of PteroisPredation, habitat use and impact of Pterois
Predation, habitat use and impact of Pterois
 
Fuong et al._09-14_Animal Behaviour
Fuong et al._09-14_Animal BehaviourFuong et al._09-14_Animal Behaviour
Fuong et al._09-14_Animal Behaviour
 
X International conference on Behaviour, Physiology and Genetic of Wildlife -...
X International conference on Behaviour, Physiology and Genetic of Wildlife -...X International conference on Behaviour, Physiology and Genetic of Wildlife -...
X International conference on Behaviour, Physiology and Genetic of Wildlife -...
 
2014 Vianna et al. Acoustic telemetry validates citizen science for monitorin...
2014 Vianna et al. Acoustic telemetry validates citizen science for monitorin...2014 Vianna et al. Acoustic telemetry validates citizen science for monitorin...
2014 Vianna et al. Acoustic telemetry validates citizen science for monitorin...
 
6626907 83-population-ecology
6626907 83-population-ecology6626907 83-population-ecology
6626907 83-population-ecology
 
G1074651
G1074651G1074651
G1074651
 
Chapter 8.3 presentation
Chapter 8.3 presentationChapter 8.3 presentation
Chapter 8.3 presentation
 
Emily Shultz-Optimized Seperation of Estuarin Plankton to Determine Associati...
Emily Shultz-Optimized Seperation of Estuarin Plankton to Determine Associati...Emily Shultz-Optimized Seperation of Estuarin Plankton to Determine Associati...
Emily Shultz-Optimized Seperation of Estuarin Plankton to Determine Associati...
 
Ecological Monitoring Techniques
Ecological Monitoring TechniquesEcological Monitoring Techniques
Ecological Monitoring Techniques
 
Bachelor's thesis _ Current and historical distribution of the endemic Santa ...
Bachelor's thesis _ Current and historical distribution of the endemic Santa ...Bachelor's thesis _ Current and historical distribution of the endemic Santa ...
Bachelor's thesis _ Current and historical distribution of the endemic Santa ...
 
RIVER NETWORKS AS ECOLOGICAL CORRIDORS FOR SPECIES POPULATIONS AND WATER-BORN...
RIVER NETWORKS AS ECOLOGICAL CORRIDORS FOR SPECIES POPULATIONS AND WATER-BORN...RIVER NETWORKS AS ECOLOGICAL CORRIDORS FOR SPECIES POPULATIONS AND WATER-BORN...
RIVER NETWORKS AS ECOLOGICAL CORRIDORS FOR SPECIES POPULATIONS AND WATER-BORN...
 
Wildlife Society Bulletin 2016 CraggTechniques for monitoring Brachyramphusmu...
Wildlife Society Bulletin 2016 CraggTechniques for monitoring Brachyramphusmu...Wildlife Society Bulletin 2016 CraggTechniques for monitoring Brachyramphusmu...
Wildlife Society Bulletin 2016 CraggTechniques for monitoring Brachyramphusmu...
 
Phylogeography and genetic diversity of Baetodes huaico (EPHEMEROPTERA: BAETI...
Phylogeography and genetic diversity of Baetodes huaico (EPHEMEROPTERA: BAETI...Phylogeography and genetic diversity of Baetodes huaico (EPHEMEROPTERA: BAETI...
Phylogeography and genetic diversity of Baetodes huaico (EPHEMEROPTERA: BAETI...
 
Mello_SMM 2011 Poster 9 (2)
Mello_SMM 2011 Poster 9 (2)Mello_SMM 2011 Poster 9 (2)
Mello_SMM 2011 Poster 9 (2)
 

Similar to sea lion Olfactor y

Annals of Limnology and Oceanography
Annals of Limnology and OceanographyAnnals of Limnology and Oceanography
Annals of Limnology and Oceanography
peertechzpublication
 
Annals of Limnology and Oceanography
Annals of Limnology and OceanographyAnnals of Limnology and Oceanography
Annals of Limnology and Oceanography
peertechzpublication
 
Disrupting the Balance - Ecological Impacts of Pesticides in California
Disrupting the Balance - Ecological Impacts of Pesticides in California Disrupting the Balance - Ecological Impacts of Pesticides in California
Disrupting the Balance - Ecological Impacts of Pesticides in California
v2zq
 
Schmucker_WritingSample_American Glass Eels Respond to Conspecific Odor as a ...
Schmucker_WritingSample_American Glass Eels Respond to Conspecific Odor as a ...Schmucker_WritingSample_American Glass Eels Respond to Conspecific Odor as a ...
Schmucker_WritingSample_American Glass Eels Respond to Conspecific Odor as a ...Andrew Schmucker
 
Effect of lethal concentrations of rubber extract (hevea brasiliensis) on the...
Effect of lethal concentrations of rubber extract (hevea brasiliensis) on the...Effect of lethal concentrations of rubber extract (hevea brasiliensis) on the...
Effect of lethal concentrations of rubber extract (hevea brasiliensis) on the...
Alexander Decker
 
Diversity and dispersion patterns of echinoderms in Babanlagan, Talisayan, Mi...
Diversity and dispersion patterns of echinoderms in Babanlagan, Talisayan, Mi...Diversity and dispersion patterns of echinoderms in Babanlagan, Talisayan, Mi...
Diversity and dispersion patterns of echinoderms in Babanlagan, Talisayan, Mi...
Angelo Mark Walag
 
Student InstructionsIn this lab, you will determine how an inv.docx
Student InstructionsIn this lab, you will determine how an inv.docxStudent InstructionsIn this lab, you will determine how an inv.docx
Student InstructionsIn this lab, you will determine how an inv.docx
cpatriciarpatricia
 
Darcya_HonoursThesisFinal
Darcya_HonoursThesisFinalDarcya_HonoursThesisFinal
Darcya_HonoursThesisFinalAndrew Darcy
 
Wagner College Forum for Undergraduate Research, Vol. 17 No. 1
Wagner College Forum for Undergraduate Research, Vol. 17 No. 1Wagner College Forum for Undergraduate Research, Vol. 17 No. 1
Wagner College Forum for Undergraduate Research, Vol. 17 No. 1
Wagner College
 
Antimicrobial Activity Mucus D. Latifrons
Antimicrobial Activity Mucus D. LatifronsAntimicrobial Activity Mucus D. Latifrons
Antimicrobial Activity Mucus D. LatifronsWiner Daniel Reyes
 
Joshua Seidman Honors Thesis Rough Draft 2.4 enm
Joshua Seidman Honors Thesis Rough Draft 2.4 enmJoshua Seidman Honors Thesis Rough Draft 2.4 enm
Joshua Seidman Honors Thesis Rough Draft 2.4 enmJoshua Seidman
 
Global Journal of Ecology
Global Journal of Ecology Global Journal of Ecology
Global Journal of Ecology
peertechzpublication
 
A preliminary study on the toxic potentials of shea butter effluent using Cla...
A preliminary study on the toxic potentials of shea butter effluent using Cla...A preliminary study on the toxic potentials of shea butter effluent using Cla...
A preliminary study on the toxic potentials of shea butter effluent using Cla...
IOSR Journals
 
AsianCarpPoster_CraigFultonGreenLeiderman (1)
AsianCarpPoster_CraigFultonGreenLeiderman (1)AsianCarpPoster_CraigFultonGreenLeiderman (1)
AsianCarpPoster_CraigFultonGreenLeiderman (1)Chelsea Green
 
Courtney Emery
Courtney EmeryCourtney Emery
Courtney EmeryLigScience
 
Acute and sub acute toxicological evaluation of ethanolic leaves extract of p...
Acute and sub acute toxicological evaluation of ethanolic leaves extract of p...Acute and sub acute toxicological evaluation of ethanolic leaves extract of p...
Acute and sub acute toxicological evaluation of ethanolic leaves extract of p...
Alexander Decker
 
Use of Radioactive Isotope in Tropical Fish feeding
Use of Radioactive Isotope in Tropical Fish feedingUse of Radioactive Isotope in Tropical Fish feeding
Use of Radioactive Isotope in Tropical Fish feeding
Jesutofunmi Osunlana
 

Similar to sea lion Olfactor y (20)

Annals of Limnology and Oceanography
Annals of Limnology and OceanographyAnnals of Limnology and Oceanography
Annals of Limnology and Oceanography
 
Annals of Limnology and Oceanography
Annals of Limnology and OceanographyAnnals of Limnology and Oceanography
Annals of Limnology and Oceanography
 
Disrupting the Balance - Ecological Impacts of Pesticides in California
Disrupting the Balance - Ecological Impacts of Pesticides in California Disrupting the Balance - Ecological Impacts of Pesticides in California
Disrupting the Balance - Ecological Impacts of Pesticides in California
 
Schmucker_WritingSample_American Glass Eels Respond to Conspecific Odor as a ...
Schmucker_WritingSample_American Glass Eels Respond to Conspecific Odor as a ...Schmucker_WritingSample_American Glass Eels Respond to Conspecific Odor as a ...
Schmucker_WritingSample_American Glass Eels Respond to Conspecific Odor as a ...
 
Effect of lethal concentrations of rubber extract (hevea brasiliensis) on the...
Effect of lethal concentrations of rubber extract (hevea brasiliensis) on the...Effect of lethal concentrations of rubber extract (hevea brasiliensis) on the...
Effect of lethal concentrations of rubber extract (hevea brasiliensis) on the...
 
penguin paper
penguin paperpenguin paper
penguin paper
 
Love Lab Visit
Love Lab VisitLove Lab Visit
Love Lab Visit
 
Diversity and dispersion patterns of echinoderms in Babanlagan, Talisayan, Mi...
Diversity and dispersion patterns of echinoderms in Babanlagan, Talisayan, Mi...Diversity and dispersion patterns of echinoderms in Babanlagan, Talisayan, Mi...
Diversity and dispersion patterns of echinoderms in Babanlagan, Talisayan, Mi...
 
Student InstructionsIn this lab, you will determine how an inv.docx
Student InstructionsIn this lab, you will determine how an inv.docxStudent InstructionsIn this lab, you will determine how an inv.docx
Student InstructionsIn this lab, you will determine how an inv.docx
 
Darcya_HonoursThesisFinal
Darcya_HonoursThesisFinalDarcya_HonoursThesisFinal
Darcya_HonoursThesisFinal
 
Marina's thesis v1
Marina's thesis v1Marina's thesis v1
Marina's thesis v1
 
Wagner College Forum for Undergraduate Research, Vol. 17 No. 1
Wagner College Forum for Undergraduate Research, Vol. 17 No. 1Wagner College Forum for Undergraduate Research, Vol. 17 No. 1
Wagner College Forum for Undergraduate Research, Vol. 17 No. 1
 
Antimicrobial Activity Mucus D. Latifrons
Antimicrobial Activity Mucus D. LatifronsAntimicrobial Activity Mucus D. Latifrons
Antimicrobial Activity Mucus D. Latifrons
 
Joshua Seidman Honors Thesis Rough Draft 2.4 enm
Joshua Seidman Honors Thesis Rough Draft 2.4 enmJoshua Seidman Honors Thesis Rough Draft 2.4 enm
Joshua Seidman Honors Thesis Rough Draft 2.4 enm
 
Global Journal of Ecology
Global Journal of Ecology Global Journal of Ecology
Global Journal of Ecology
 
A preliminary study on the toxic potentials of shea butter effluent using Cla...
A preliminary study on the toxic potentials of shea butter effluent using Cla...A preliminary study on the toxic potentials of shea butter effluent using Cla...
A preliminary study on the toxic potentials of shea butter effluent using Cla...
 
AsianCarpPoster_CraigFultonGreenLeiderman (1)
AsianCarpPoster_CraigFultonGreenLeiderman (1)AsianCarpPoster_CraigFultonGreenLeiderman (1)
AsianCarpPoster_CraigFultonGreenLeiderman (1)
 
Courtney Emery
Courtney EmeryCourtney Emery
Courtney Emery
 
Acute and sub acute toxicological evaluation of ethanolic leaves extract of p...
Acute and sub acute toxicological evaluation of ethanolic leaves extract of p...Acute and sub acute toxicological evaluation of ethanolic leaves extract of p...
Acute and sub acute toxicological evaluation of ethanolic leaves extract of p...
 
Use of Radioactive Isotope in Tropical Fish feeding
Use of Radioactive Isotope in Tropical Fish feedingUse of Radioactive Isotope in Tropical Fish feeding
Use of Radioactive Isotope in Tropical Fish feeding
 

sea lion Olfactor y

  • 1. ForPeerReview Only Olfactory enrichment in California sea lions (Zalophus californianus): An effective tool for captive welfare? Journal: Journal of Applied Animal Welfare Science Manuscript ID HAAW-2016-0004 Manuscript Type: Zoo/Wildlife Keywords: Animal Welfare, California sea lions, Enrichment, Olfaction, Pinnipeds URL: http://mc.manuscriptcentral.com/jaaws Email: ken.shapiro@animalsandsociety.org Journal of Applied Animal Welfare Science
  • 2. ForPeerReview Only Running head: OLFACTORY ENRICHMENT IN CALIFORNIA SEA LIONS 1 Olfactory Enrichment in California Sea Lions (Zalophus californianus): An Effective Tool for Captive Welfare? In the wild, California sea lions (Zalophus californianus) are exposed to a wide variety of sensory information, which cannot be replicated in captive environments. Therefore, unique procedures are necessary for the maintenance of physiological and psychological health in captive animals. The effects of introducing of natural scents to captive enclosures has been investigated in a variety of species, yet this has not been examined in marine mammals. This project explores the behavioral effect of scent added to the environment, with the goal of improving the welfare of captive sea lions. By introducing two scent types: 1.) Natural scents, found in their native environment, and 2.) Non- natural scents, not found in their native environment; this study examines not only scent enrichment but the possible evolutionary underpinnings of pinniped olfaction. Scent enrichment was found to significantly impact sea lion behavior as demonstrated by a reduction in pattern swimming, an increase in habitat utilization, and a reduction in stereotypical behavior. However, there were no differences in behavior between natural and non-natural scent conditions. Keywords: animal welfare, California sea lions, enrichment, olfaction, pinnipeds Page 1 of 21 URL: http://mc.manuscriptcentral.com/jaaws Email: ken.shapiro@animalsandsociety.org Journal of Applied Animal Welfare Science 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 12 13 14 15 16 17 18 19 20 21 22 23 24 25 26 27 28 29 30 31 32 33 34 35 36 37 38 39 40 41 42 43 44 45 46 47 48 49 50 51 52 53 54 55 56 57 58 59 60
  • 3. ForPeerReview Only OLFACTORY ENRICHMENT IN CALIFORNIA SEA LIONS 2 Olfactory stimulation in California sea lions (Zalophus californianus): An effective tool for captive welfare? Introduction In nature, wild animals encounter environmental stimulation from nearly infinite sources. However, captive environments simply cannot compete with this diversity. As a result, the environment must become more complex in order to maintain both the physiological and psychological wellbeing of captive wildlife such as California sea lions (Zalophus californianus; Carlstead, Seidensticker & Baldwin, 1991). Scent is critical for effective foraging (Apfelbach, 1992; Gittleman, 1991; Hughes, Price & Banks, 2010; Schwartz, Miller & Haroldson, 2003; Ylönen, Sundell, Tiilikainen, Eccard, & Horne, 2003), navigation (Rogers, 1988), and social behaviors (Beckoff, 1981; Rothman & Mech, 1979) within the caniform taxon, to which sea lions belong. For the majority of caniform species, environmental enrichment has focused on scent (i.e., Andrews & Ha, 2014; Kitchener & Asa, 2010; Leonard, 2008; Nelson, 2009; Price, 2010; Rafacz & Santymire, 2014; Schneider, Nogge, & Kolter, 2014). Despite extensive exploration of olfaction and scent enrichment in other caniform species, the behavioral effects of scent enrichment has not previously been examined in pinnipeds. Sea lions have maintained genes necessary for olfaction (Kishida, Kubota, Shirayama, & Fukami, 2007), as well as extensive nasal turbinates (Van Valkenburgh, Samuels, Bird, Fulkerson, Meachen Samuels, & Slater, 2011) indicating some need for scent detection to survive in the wild. Therefore, it is likely that sea lions are biologically prepared to obtain meaningful information from scents in their natural environment. For example, South African fur seals (Arctocephalus pusillus), closely related to California sea lions, have been shown to possess the capability to distinguish between chemically Page 2 of 21 URL: http://mc.manuscriptcentral.com/jaaws Email: ken.shapiro@animalsandsociety.org Journal of Applied Animal Welfare Science 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 12 13 14 15 16 17 18 19 20 21 22 23 24 25 26 27 28 29 30 31 32 33 34 35 36 37 38 39 40 41 42 43 44 45 46 47 48 49 50 51 52 53 54 55 56 57 58 59 60
  • 4. ForPeerReview Only OLFACTORY ENRICHMENT IN CALIFORNIA SEA LIONS 3 similar odorants when relevant to their natural environment (Laska, Lord, Selin, & Amundin, 2010). Thus, the introduction of natural scents into the environment may have a greater impact on behavior than would non-natural scents. The present study investigated whether scent can be utilized to enhance the captive environment for California sea lions by introducing both natural and non-natural scents and recording instances of abnormal and repetitive behavioral repertoires, called stereotypical behaviors. In this study, the following hypotheses were examined: (1) Scent enrichment will increase habitat utilization, indicated by a change in habitat usage patterns, and reduce stereotypic behavior patterns; (1a) If present, these trends will continue to be evident at both the enclosure and individual level; (2) Natural scents will have a greater effect on behavior than non-natural scents, as indicated by a stronger effect on habitat utilization patterns and a statistically significant difference in instances of stereotypical behaviors; (2a) These trends will continue to be evident at both the enclosure and individual level. Methodology Research was conducted at the Institute for Marine Mammal Studies (IMMS) in Gulfport, Mississippi. Subjects included four, two-year-old female California sea lions (Zalophus californianus). The sea lions were housed in pairs, with each enclosure displaying differences in pattern swims and stereotypical behaviors (Table 1). Scent enrichment was of two types, natural scents and non-natural scents. Natural scents were those that are likely to be present in the animals’ natural environment. Non- natural odorants were those that would be introduced through exposure to humans (Table 2). Page 3 of 21 URL: http://mc.manuscriptcentral.com/jaaws Email: ken.shapiro@animalsandsociety.org Journal of Applied Animal Welfare Science 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 12 13 14 15 16 17 18 19 20 21 22 23 24 25 26 27 28 29 30 31 32 33 34 35 36 37 38 39 40 41 42 43 44 45 46 47 48 49 50 51 52 53 54 55 56 57 58 59 60
  • 5. ForPeerReview Only OLFACTORY ENRICHMENT IN CALIFORNIA SEA LIONS 4 Baseline Procedure To begin this study, researchers collected baseline data over the span of one week (six days) at IMMS. Researchers filmed one thirty-minute session in the morning, and one in the afternoon (to parallel the experimental pre- and post- sessions). To eliminate the confounding effect of a visual cue, this process included the introduction of a chalk mark on the wall (with no added scent) during baseline pre-sessions. No scent treatment was applied during the baseline post-session. In addition obtaining baseline data, the animals were able to habituate to the researchers’ presence prior to the experimental trials. Experimental Procedure: Natural and Non-Natural Scents Each trial consisted of a pre- and post-session. The pre-session data collection period consisted of video data taken thirty minutes prior to the introduction of scent stimulus. Similarly, the post-session data collection period consisted of video data taken thirty minutes after the introduction of the scent stimulus. This approach allowed for the assessment of the animals’ behavioral state prior to the introduction of scent enrichment, and any potential changes as a result of this intervention. Experimental trials were split into two scent types: natural and non-natural scents. A total of 8 scents were presented three times each, resulting in twenty-four trials for per animal (n = 4). Each trial consisted of a baseline pre-session in which a chalk mark was placed on the wall and a post-session in which the scent was applied on top of the chalk mark. The order of presentation and placement of these scents within the enclosure were pseudo-randomized. Each scent was presented once per cycle, and excluded placement Page 4 of 21 URL: http://mc.manuscriptcentral.com/jaaws Email: ken.shapiro@animalsandsociety.org Journal of Applied Animal Welfare Science 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 12 13 14 15 16 17 18 19 20 21 22 23 24 25 26 27 28 29 30 31 32 33 34 35 36 37 38 39 40 41 42 43 44 45 46 47 48 49 50 51 52 53 54 55 56 57 58 59 60
  • 6. ForPeerReview Only OLFACTORY ENRICHMENT IN CALIFORNIA SEA LIONS 5 on the fourth wall (which was located on the same wall as the researcher, and was obscured from view due to the film angle; see Figure 1). Trials were run simultaneously in both enclosures in order to avoid the scent being detected by animals in the adjacent enclosure. After the trial, the scent location was cleaned with a bleach solution to ensure the enclosure was free of the scent for the subsequent trial. One trial was executed each day so that the scent had been eliminated prior to the subsequent scent trial. Video data was collected on site by two researchers stationed outside of each enclosure (Figure 1). Data was then coded onto a Microsoft Excel spreadsheet in fifteen- second intervals to examine habitat usage, behavioral state, and stereotypic behaviors before and after the introduction of the enrichment (Table 3). To guarantee reliability, an independent coder was employed, reaching 92% agreement on 20% of the video data. In order to assess the general impact of the study on individual animals, the researchers planned to examine overall habitat usage and the exhibition of stereotypical behaviors in all four sea lions housed at IMMS using an ANOVA with Tukey’s Post-Hoc Test. Following this analysis, independent t-tests were utilized to examine pre-session and post-session changes in behavior between in each scent condition. Data was interpreted using an alpha level of .05. Hypothesis I Results Hypothesis one was examined to determine if scent enrichment was effective in increasing habitat utilization, decreasing pattern swimming, and reducing stereotypical behaviors, for these captive California sea lions. To assess the differences in the means for habitat utilization during the pre- session and post-session experimental sessions (including both natural and non-natural Page 5 of 21 URL: http://mc.manuscriptcentral.com/jaaws Email: ken.shapiro@animalsandsociety.org Journal of Applied Animal Welfare Science 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 12 13 14 15 16 17 18 19 20 21 22 23 24 25 26 27 28 29 30 31 32 33 34 35 36 37 38 39 40 41 42 43 44 45 46 47 48 49 50 51 52 53 54 55 56 57 58 59 60
  • 7. ForPeerReview Only OLFACTORY ENRICHMENT IN CALIFORNIA SEA LIONS 6 scent sessions) a one-way ANOVA was utilized to compare the main effect of scent on habitat utilization across the six locations (locations 0-5). This analysis indicated that there was a main effect of scent in all locations except location 5 (center), with the sea lions spending more time out of the pool and less time in the pool quadrants after the scent enrichment was in place (Figure 2). Both enclosures showed similar effects of scent on habitat utilization. Sea lions in both enclosure one and two spent more time out of the water (enclosure one location 0 t(94)=-4.361, p = .000; enclosure two location 0 t(94) = -3.549, p =.001) and less time at locations 1, 2, 3, and 4 (enclosure one location 1, t(94) = 5.335, p = .000, location 2 t(94) = 2.506, p = .014, location 3 t(94) = 2.333, p = .022, location 4 t(94) = 3.875, p = .000; enclosure two location 1 t(94) = 4.386, p = .000, 2 t(94) = 3.846, p = .000, 3 t(94) = 3.854, p = .000, and 4 t(94) = 4.303, p = .000). Time spent in the center of the pool (location 5) did not differ significantly in either enclosure (see Figure 3). These general findings held true for individual sea lions as well. Gabby, Sage, and Kaytee all spent significantly more time out of the water (location 0) and less time in locations 1-4. In contrast, Maya decreased time spent in location 1 (t(46) = 2.729, p = .009), evenly dispersing that time spent across the other areas of the enclosure. However, none of the animals significantly altered the amount of time spent in the center of the pool (location 5; Figure 4). Pattern Swimming There was a reduction in pattern swimming (circle swimming t(189) = 6.728, p = .000 and diagonal swimming t(189) = 2.338, p = .020) when scents were present for all four animals. There was a significant reduction in both circle (t(94) = 4.548, p =.000) and Page 6 of 21 URL: http://mc.manuscriptcentral.com/jaaws Email: ken.shapiro@animalsandsociety.org Journal of Applied Animal Welfare Science 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 12 13 14 15 16 17 18 19 20 21 22 23 24 25 26 27 28 29 30 31 32 33 34 35 36 37 38 39 40 41 42 43 44 45 46 47 48 49 50 51 52 53 54 55 56 57 58 59 60
  • 8. ForPeerReview Only OLFACTORY ENRICHMENT IN CALIFORNIA SEA LIONS 7 diagonal swimming (t(94) = 2.647, p = .010) in enclosure one and sharp reduction in circle swimming in enclosure two (t(94) = 5.214, p = .000). Within enclosure one, the predominant swim pattern was diagonal swimming, which decreased when scents are present (t(94) = 2.676, p = .009). Circle swimming, although rarely observed, did reduce when scents are present (t(94) = 4.619, p = .000). Circle swimming in enclosure two also decreased significantly in the experimental sessions (t(94) = 5.214, p = .000). Diagonal swimming was not observed in enclosure two throughout the duration of this study, and was therefore not investigated at the individual level. In enclosure one, both Gabby and Maya showed a reduction in circle swimming (Gabby t(44) = 4.048, p = .000; Maya t(44) = 2.829, p = .007). However, only Gabby showed a significant reduction in diagonal swimming (Gabby t(44) = . 2.657, p = .011; Maya t(44) = 1.143, p = .260, d = .345). When scents were present, in enclosure two, both sea lions exhibited reduced circle swimming (Sage t(44) = 3.098, p = .003; Kaytee t(44) = 3.875, p = .000). This was the only pattern swim to show a reduction at the enclosure level as diagonal swims were not observed in this enclosure. Stereotypical Behaviors Both Gabby and Sage exhibited unique stereotypies, as well as individualized reactions to scent. Gabby exhibited an increase in pool wall sucking (t(44) = -2.733, p = .012) in the presence of scent. Sage exhibited a slight increase in floor sucking (t(45) = - .719, p =.476, d = .212), while reducing tail sucking (t(45) = .130, p = .898, d = .038), regurgitation (t(45) = 1.026, p = .310, d = .302), and reingestion (t(45) = .864, p = .392, d = .254); for operational definitions of these behaviors refer to Table 3). Page 7 of 21 URL: http://mc.manuscriptcentral.com/jaaws Email: ken.shapiro@animalsandsociety.org Journal of Applied Animal Welfare Science 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 12 13 14 15 16 17 18 19 20 21 22 23 24 25 26 27 28 29 30 31 32 33 34 35 36 37 38 39 40 41 42 43 44 45 46 47 48 49 50 51 52 53 54 55 56 57 58 59 60
  • 9. ForPeerReview Only OLFACTORY ENRICHMENT IN CALIFORNIA SEA LIONS 8 Hypothesis II Results In evaluation of hypothesis two, the analysis demonstrated that the sea lions’ habitat usage did not significantly differ between natural scents and non-natural scents. In addition, independent sample t-tests showed no differences for the enclosures, or for the individual animals. In the natural scent sessions, sea lions exhibited a reduction in both circle (natural scent session t(94) = 5.047, p = .000; non-natural scent session t(94) = 4.528, p = .000) and diagonal swimming (natural scent sessions t(94) = 1.986, p = .051, d = .41; non- natural scent conditions, t(94) = 1.251, p = .215, d = .26). Within enclosure one, animals were found to alter their habitat usage patterns in the presence of a natural scent, although not significantly. Similarly, in enclosure two, a non-significant reduction in circle swimming was found for both Sage and Kaytee when natural scents were present in the enclosure. Although not statistically significant, it was noteworthy that diagonal swimming did decrease in both sessions and was very near significance in the natural scent sessions. This lack of significance may simply be due to the fact that this behavior was only observed in one enclosure. Pattern swimming in animals within enclosure one were not found to differ significantly between natural and non-natural scent sessions (circle swimming, t(46) = .499, p = .621, d = .147) and diagonal swimming (t(46) = -.089, p = .929, d = .026). In enclosure two, animals were not found to differ significantly between natural and non- natural scent sessions (t(46) = -.500, p = .617, d =.147). However, at the individual level slight changes in behavior are evident. Page 8 of 21 URL: http://mc.manuscriptcentral.com/jaaws Email: ken.shapiro@animalsandsociety.org Journal of Applied Animal Welfare Science 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 12 13 14 15 16 17 18 19 20 21 22 23 24 25 26 27 28 29 30 31 32 33 34 35 36 37 38 39 40 41 42 43 44 45 46 47 48 49 50 51 52 53 54 55 56 57 58 59 60
  • 10. ForPeerReview Only OLFACTORY ENRICHMENT IN CALIFORNIA SEA LIONS 9 Although not significant, Gabby, Sage, and Kaytee demonstrated a reduction in circle swimming when natural scents were present (Gabby t(22) = .834, p = .419, d = .356; Sage t(22) = -.276, p = .785, d = .117; Kaytee t(22) = -.134, p = .895, d = .057). Maya, alternately, spent more time circle swimming in the natural sessions (t(22) = -.502, p = .621, d = .214). Similarly, Gabby and Maya both spent less time diagonal swimming in the natural sessions, although these results were not significant (Gabby t(22) = -.398, p =.694, d = .169; Maya t(22) = .327, p = .747, d = .139). Neither Sage nor Kaytee engaged in diagonal pattern swimming during the course of the experimental session, therefore statistical analysis for this behavior cannot be provided. Individual variation was evident when stereotypical behaviors were observed in Gabby and Sage, who exhibited non-significant decreases in individualized stereotypies in natural scent sessions. Specifically, Gabby’s pool wall sucking behavior decreased, albeit not significantly, in the natural scent sessions when compared with the non-natural scent sessions (t(21) = -.334, p = .742, d = .142). Similarly, Sage exhibited non- significant decreases in floor sucking behavior (t(22) = -.593, p = .559, d = .252), tail sucking (t(22) = -.250, p =.806, d = .106), regurgitation (t(22) = -1.517, p = .147, d = .646), and reingestion (t(22) =. 928, p = .373, d = .396). Discussion Overall, scent was found to be enriching as it caused an increase in habitat utilization, a reduction in pattern swimming, and a reduction in the majority of stereotypical behaviors both across and within individuals. While the second hypothesis - that sea lions would react more strongly to natural scents than non-natural scents - was not supported, sea lions responded to both types of scents in their environment in a Page 9 of 21 URL: http://mc.manuscriptcentral.com/jaaws Email: ken.shapiro@animalsandsociety.org Journal of Applied Animal Welfare Science 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 12 13 14 15 16 17 18 19 20 21 22 23 24 25 26 27 28 29 30 31 32 33 34 35 36 37 38 39 40 41 42 43 44 45 46 47 48 49 50 51 52 53 54 55 56 57 58 59 60
  • 11. ForPeerReview Only OLFACTORY ENRICHMENT IN CALIFORNIA SEA LIONS 10 positive manner by exhibiting a reduction in stereotypical behavior (e.g., pattern swimming, oral stereotypies, and voluntary emesis). Although individual stereotypies, such as regurgitation, were not found to be statistically significant, the large effect sizes found in this analysis suggest that there was indeed a practical reduction in these problematic behaviors. Additionally, sea lions were observed by researchers to explore old scent markers immediately upon discovering new scent markers, indicating that they may recall where previous scents were placed. This unique observation supports the idea that sea lions are responding to their environment using cognitive mechanisms, and are not solely attending to scents pertinent to their natural environment. This finding suggests that there are many unexplored avenues of research beyond the realms of environmental salient stimuli that could be investigated for this species. In addition to zoos and aquaria, rehabilitation facilities may benefit from this research. Pinniped rehabilitation centers are often comprised of sterile environments that do not allow for naturalistic enclosures and extensive enrichment procedures. This may be due, in part, to the staff’s attempts to reduce human-animal interactions in order to discourage habituation, facilitate disease control, and reduce cost. However scent enrichment encourages species-typical behaviors, through increased habitat utilization, (e.g. Fay & Miller, 2015). Moreover, this effect can be achieved at little or no cost to the center and may be used to keep the animals cognitively engaged during their stay at the rehabilitation center. Additionally, environmental enrichment has been demonstrated to assist in the rehabilitation of animals suffering from a variety of conditions including: brain lesions (De Bartolo, Leggio, Mandolesi, Foti, Ferlazzo & Pertosini, 2008; Paban, Page 10 of 21 URL: http://mc.manuscriptcentral.com/jaaws Email: ken.shapiro@animalsandsociety.org Journal of Applied Animal Welfare Science 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 12 13 14 15 16 17 18 19 20 21 22 23 24 25 26 27 28 29 30 31 32 33 34 35 36 37 38 39 40 41 42 43 44 45 46 47 48 49 50 51 52 53 54 55 56 57 58 59 60
  • 12. ForPeerReview Only OLFACTORY ENRICHMENT IN CALIFORNIA SEA LIONS 11 Jaffard, Chambon, Malafosse & Alescio-Lautier, 2004), lead poisoning (Guilarte, Toscano, McGlothan & Weaver, 2002), domoic acid toxicosis (Cook, Bernard & Reichmuth, 2011), and malnutrition (Rose, 1988). Although currently classified as a species of least concern by the IUCN Redlist, sea lion strandings are highly influenced by the effects of climate change (Greig, Gulland, & Kreuder, 2005; Hui, 2011; Keledjian & Mesnick, 2013). As these effects continue to escalate, sea lion rehabilitation programs will likely continue to be a necessity for the maintenance of a healthy California sea lion population. Thus, information relating to low-cost enrichment that may increase survival rates post-release will only assist rehabilitators in these endeavors. Page 11 of 21 URL: http://mc.manuscriptcentral.com/jaaws Email: ken.shapiro@animalsandsociety.org Journal of Applied Animal Welfare Science 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 12 13 14 15 16 17 18 19 20 21 22 23 24 25 26 27 28 29 30 31 32 33 34 35 36 37 38 39 40 41 42 43 44 45 46 47 48 49 50 51 52 53 54 55 56 57 58 59 60
  • 13. ForPeerReview Only OLFACTORY ENRICHMENT IN CALIFORNIA SEA LIONS 12 References Andrews, N. L. P. & Ha, J. C. (2014). The effects of automated scatter feeders on captive grizzly bear activity budgets. Journal of Applied Animal Welfare Science, 17(2) 148-156. Apfelbach, R. (1992). Ontogenetic olfactory experience and adult searching behavior in the carnivorous ferret. In R. L. Doty & D. Müller-Schwarze (Eds.) Chemical Signals in Vertebrates (6th Ed.). New York, NY: Springer. Beckoff, M. (1981). Mammalian sibling interactions: Genes, facilitative environments and the coefficient of familiarity (Pp. 307-346). In D. J. Gubernick & P. H. Klopfer (Eds.) Parental Care in Mammals, New York, NY: Plenum Press. Carlstead, K., Seidensticker, J. & Baldwin, R. (1991). Environmental enrichment for zoo housed bears. Zoo Biology, 10(3), 3-16. Cook, P., Bernard, A. K., & Reichmuth, C. (2011). Which way did I go?: Remote training of a spatial memory task to assess the effects of domoic acid exposure in stranded California sea lions (Zalophus californianus), Soundings, 36(2), 16-19. De Bartolo, P., Leggio, M. G., Mandolesi, L., Foti, F., Gelfo, F., Ferlazzo, F., & Pertosini, L. (2008). Environmental enrichment mitigates the effects of basal forebrain lesions on cognitive flexibility. Neuroscience, 154, 444-453. Fay, C. & Miller, L. J. (2015). Utilizing scents as environmental enrichment: Preference assessment and application with Rothschild Giraffe. Animal Behavior and Cognition, 2(3), 285-291. Gittleman, J. L. (1991). Carnivore olfactory bulb size: Allometry, phylogeny and ecology. Journal of Zoology, 225(2), 253-272. Greig, D. J., Gulland, F. M. D., & Kreuder, C. (2005). A decade of live sea lions (Zalophus californianus) strandings along the central California coast: Causes and trends, 1991-2000. Aquatic Mammals, 31(1), 11-22. Guilarte, T. R., Toscano, C. D., McGlothan, J. L., & Weaver, S. A. (2002). Environmentalenrichment reverses cognitive and molecular deficits induced by developmentallead exposure. Annals of Neurology, 53(1), 50-56. Hui, T. C. Y. (2011). Steller sea lions and fisheries: Competition at sea? M.S. Thesis. The University of British Columbia, CA. Hughes, N. K., Price, C. J., & Banks, P. B. (2010). Predators are attracted to olfactory signals of prey. PLOS ONE 5(9), 1-4 Page 12 of 21 URL: http://mc.manuscriptcentral.com/jaaws Email: ken.shapiro@animalsandsociety.org Journal of Applied Animal Welfare Science 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 12 13 14 15 16 17 18 19 20 21 22 23 24 25 26 27 28 29 30 31 32 33 34 35 36 37 38 39 40 41 42 43 44 45 46 47 48 49 50 51 52 53 54 55 56 57 58 59 60
  • 14. ForPeerReview Only OLFACTORY ENRICHMENT IN CALIFORNIA SEA LIONS 13 IUCN, Conservation International and NatureServe. (2014). California sea lions. IUCN Red List. http://www.iucnredlist.org/details/41666/0 Keledjian, A. J. & Mesnick, S. (2013). The impacts of El Niño conditions on California sea lions (Zalophus californianus) fisheries interactions: Predicting spatial and temporal hotspots along the California coast. Aquatic Mammals, 39(3), 221-232. Kishida, T., Kubota, S., Shirayama Y., & Fukami, H. (2007). The olfactory receptor gene repertoires in secondary-adapted marine vertebrates: Evidence for reduction of the functional proportions in cetaceans. Biology Letters, 3, 428- 430. Kitchener, A. C. & Asa, C. S. (2010). Editorial: Bears and canids. International Zoo Yearbook, 44(1), 7-15. Leonard, D. A. (2008). An evaluation of environmental enrichment for two highlysocial and endangered canid species, the African wild dog (Lycaon pictus) and Mexican wolf (Canis lupus) (Doctoral Dissertation). Saint Louis University, St. Louis MO. Nelson, K. O. (2009). Environmental enrichment effects on the activity of a nearactic river otter. Master’s Thesis. The Rochester Institute of Technology. Paban, V., Jaffard, M., Chambon, M., Malafosse, M., & Alescio-Lautier, B. (2004). Time course of behavioral changes following basal forebrain cholinergic damage in rats: Environmental enrichment as a therapeutic intervention. Neuroscience, 132, 13-32. Peterson, R.S. & Bartholomew, G. A. (1969). Airborne vocal communication in the California sea lion (Zalophus californianus). Animal Behaviour, 17(1), 17-18. Price, L. J. (2010). A preliminary study of the effects of environmental enrichment on the behaviour of captive African wild dogs (Lycaon pictus). Bioscience Horizons, 0(0), 1-9. Rafacz, M. L. & Santymire, R. M. (2014). Using odor cues to elicit a behavioral and hormonal response in zoo-housed African wild dogs. Zoo Biology, 3(2), 144 149. Rogers, L. L. (1988). Homing tendencies of large mammals: A review. U.S. Forest Service North Central Forest Experiment Station. St Paul, MN. Rose, F. D. (1988). Environmental enrichment and recovery of function following brain damage in the rat. Medical Science Research, 16(6), 257-263. Rothman, R. J. & Mech, L. D. (1979). Scent-making in lone wolves and newly formed pairs. Animal Behaviour, 27(3), 750-752. Page 13 of 21 URL: http://mc.manuscriptcentral.com/jaaws Email: ken.shapiro@animalsandsociety.org Journal of Applied Animal Welfare Science 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 12 13 14 15 16 17 18 19 20 21 22 23 24 25 26 27 28 29 30 31 32 33 34 35 36 37 38 39 40 41 42 43 44 45 46 47 48 49 50 51 52 53 54 55 56 57 58 59 60
  • 15. ForPeerReview Only OLFACTORY ENRICHMENT IN CALIFORNIA SEA LIONS 14 Schneider, M., Nogge, G., & Kolter, L. (2014). Implementing unpredictability in feeding enrichment for Malayan sun bears (Helarctos malayanus). Zoo Biology, 33(1), 54-62. Schwartz, C. C., Miller S. D., & Haroldson, M. A. (2003). Grizzly bear (Pp. 556-586). In G. A. Feldhammer, B.C. Thompson, & A. J. Champman (Eds). Wild Mammals of North America: Biology, Management, and Conservation (2nd Ed.). Baltimore, MD: Johns Hopkins University Press. Steele, R. M. & Steele, T. (2005). The use of canid urine and related scents for the enrichment of captive grey wolves (Canis lupus). Regional Meeting Proceedings: Association of Zoos and Aquariums Proceedings. Szokalski, M. S., Litchfied, C. A., & Foster, W. K. (2012). Enrichment for captive tigers (Panthera tigris): Current knowledge and future directions. Applied Animal Behavior Science, 139(1-2), 1-9. Van Valkenburgh, B., Curtis, A., Samuels, J. X., Bird, D., Fulkerson, B., Meachen Samuels, J., & Slater, G. J. (2011). Aquatic adaptations in the nose of carnivorans: Evidence from the turbinates. Journal of Anatomy, 218(3), 298- 310. Wells, D. L. (2004). Review of environmental enrichment for kenneled dogs (Canis familiaris). Applied Animal Behaviour Science, 85 (3-4), 307-317. Wells, D. L. (2009). Sensory stimulation as environmental enrichment for captive animals: A review. Applied Animal Behaviour Science, 118(1-2), 1-11. Ylönen, H., Sundell, J., Tiilikainen, R., Eccard, J. A., & Horne, T. (2003). Weasel’s (Mustela nivalis nivalis) preference for olfactory cues of the vole (Clethrionomys glareolus). Ecology, 84(6), 1447-1452. Page 14 of 21 URL: http://mc.manuscriptcentral.com/jaaws Email: ken.shapiro@animalsandsociety.org Journal of Applied Animal Welfare Science 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 12 13 14 15 16 17 18 19 20 21 22 23 24 25 26 27 28 29 30 31 32 33 34 35 36 37 38 39 40 41 42 43 44 45 46 47 48 49 50 51 52 53 54 55 56 57 58 59 60
  • 16. ForPeerReview Only OLFACTORY ENRICHMENT IN CALIFORNIA SEA LIONS 15 Table 1. Summary of Subjects Animal Age Sex Enclosure Dominant Swim Pattern Individual Stereotypies Maya 2 Female 1 Diagonal N/A Gabby 2 Female 1 Diagonal Pool wall sucking Sage 2 Female 2 Circle Tail & Floor Sucking Kaytee 2 Female 2 Circle N/A Page 15 of 21 URL: http://mc.manuscriptcentral.com/jaaws Email: ken.shapiro@animalsandsociety.org Journal of Applied Animal Welfare Science 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 12 13 14 15 16 17 18 19 20 21 22 23 24 25 26 27 28 29 30 31 32 33 34 35 36 37 38 39 40 41 42 43 44 45 46 47 48 49 50 51 52 53 54 55 56 57 58 59 60
  • 17. ForPeerReview Only OLFACTORY ENRICHMENT IN CALIFORNIA SEA LIONS 16 Table 2. List of Scents Scent Type Application Method Sterilized potting soil (no fertilizer added) Natural 1 cup was rubbed on the wall. Sterilized playground sand Natural 1 cup was rubbed on the wall. Kelp Natural 100% human food grade dried kelp: approximately 1 cup of dried sea kelp was wet down with saltwater and then rubbed on the wall. Sardine oil Natural 100% biodegradable, water- soluble, sardine bait oil scent (1 tsp.) was applied to a wall. Orange Non-natural One large orange was cut in half and rubbed on the wall. Banana Non-natural One large banana was cut in half and rubbed on the wall. Vanilla extract Non-natural 1 tsp. of 100% food grade vanilla extract was applied to a wall. Cinnamon Non-natural 1 tsp. of 100% dried food- grade cinnamon was applied to a wall. Page 16 of 21 URL: http://mc.manuscriptcentral.com/jaaws Email: ken.shapiro@animalsandsociety.org Journal of Applied Animal Welfare Science 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 12 13 14 15 16 17 18 19 20 21 22 23 24 25 26 27 28 29 30 31 32 33 34 35 36 37 38 39 40 41 42 43 44 45 46 47 48 49 50 51 52 53 54 55 56 57 58 59 60
  • 18. ForPeerReview Only OLFACTORY ENRICHMENT IN CALIFORNIA SEA LIONS 17 Table 3. Behavioral Ethogram Behavior Description Circle Swimming Swimming in a fixed pattern, which is the same at all points, in a clockwise or counterclockwise fashion. Diagonal Swimming Floor Sucking Pacing Pool Wall Sucking Tail Sucking Swimming in a fixed pattern, which is the same at all points, crossing diagonally across the pool (including locations 1, 5, and 4 or 2, 5, and 3). Placing the mouth directly on the floor in order to suck, audibly and visually, off of the floor. Walking in a fixed pattern, which is the same at all points. Placing mouth directly on pool wall, and remaining in one location for more than a two second increment. Curling into an “o” shape to make contact with the tail using one’s mouth followed by an audible sucking sound. Page 17 of 21 URL: http://mc.manuscriptcentral.com/jaaws Email: ken.shapiro@animalsandsociety.org Journal of Applied Animal Welfare Science 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 12 13 14 15 16 17 18 19 20 21 22 23 24 25 26 27 28 29 30 31 32 33 34 35 36 37 38 39 40 41 42 43 44 45 46 47 48 49 50 51 52 53 54 55 56 57 58 59 60
  • 19. ForPeerReview Only OLFACTORY ENRICHMENT IN CALIFORNIA SEA LIONS 18 Figure 1. Enclosure layout Page 18 of 21 URL: http://mc.manuscriptcentral.com/jaaws Email: ken.shapiro@animalsandsociety.org Journal of Applied Animal Welfare Science 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 12 13 14 15 16 17 18 19 20 21 22 23 24 25 26 27 28 29 30 31 32 33 34 35 36 37 38 39 40 41 42 43 44 45 46 47 48 49 50 51 52 53 54 55 56 57 58 59 60
  • 20. ForPeerReview Only OLFACTORY ENRICHMENT IN CALIFORNIA SEA LIONS 19 Figure 2. Overall mean location frequencies in experimental pre- and post-sessions for the effect of scent enrichment on habitat utilizationPre-session Location 0 Post-session Location 0 Pre-session Location 1 Post-session Location 1 Pre-session Location 2 Post-session Location 2 Pre-session Location 3 Post-session Location 3 Pre-session Location 4 Post-session Location 4 Pre-session Location 5 Post-session Location 5 0 50 100 150 Locations MeanFrequenciesinLocations * ** * * Habitat Utilization in All Four Animals Page 19 of 21 URL: http://mc.manuscriptcentral.com/jaaws Email: ken.shapiro@animalsandsociety.org Journal of Applied Animal Welfare Science 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 12 13 14 15 16 17 18 19 20 21 22 23 24 25 26 27 28 29 30 31 32 33 34 35 36 37 38 39 40 41 42 43 44 45 46 47 48 49 50 51 52 53 54 55 56 57 58 59 60
  • 21. ForPeerReview Only OLFACTORY ENRICHMENT IN CALIFORNIA SEA LIONS 20 Figure 3. Changes in location between experimental pre- and post-sessions by enclosurePre-session Location 0 Post-session Location 0 Pre-session Location 1 Post-session Location 1 Pre-session Location 2 Post-session Location 2 Pre-session Location 3 Post-session Location 3 Pre-session Location 4 Post-session Location 4 Pre-session Location 5 Post-session Location 5 Pre-session Location 0 Post-session Location 0 Pre-session Location 1 Post-session Location 1 Pre-session Location 2 Post-session Location 2 Pre-session Location 3 Post-session Location 3 Pre-session Location 4 Post-session Location 4 Pre-session Location 5 Post-session Location 5 0 50 100 150 Locations MeanFrequenciesinLocations Habitat Utilization in Enclosures 1 and 2 * * * * * * * * * * Enclosure 1 Enclosure 2 Page 20 of 21 URL: http://mc.manuscriptcentral.com/jaaws Email: ken.shapiro@animalsandsociety.org Journal of Applied Animal Welfare Science 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 12 13 14 15 16 17 18 19 20 21 22 23 24 25 26 27 28 29 30 31 32 33 34 35 36 37 38 39 40 41 42 43 44 45 46 47 48 49 50 51 52 53 54 55 56 57 58 59 60
  • 22. ForPeerReview Only OLFACTORY ENRICHMENT IN CALIFORNIA SEA LIONS 21 Figure 4. Pre-session and post-session changes in habitat utilization by animal Pre-session Location 0 Post-session Location 0 Pre-session Location 1 Post-session Location 1 Pre-session Location 2 Post-session Location 2 Pre-session Location 3 Post-session Location 3 Pre-session Location 4 Post-session Location 4 Pre-session Location 5 Post-session Location 5 0 50 100 150 Locations MeanFrequenciesinLocations Gabby * * * * * Pre-session Location 0 Post-session Location 0 Pre-session Location 1 Post-session Location 1 Pre-session Location 2 Post-session Location 2 Pre-session Location 3 Post-session Location 3 Pre-session Location 4 Post-session Location 4 Pre-session Location 5 Post-session Location 5 0 50 100 150 Locations MeanFrequenciesinLocations Maya * * Pre-session Location 0 Post-session Location 0 Pre-session Location 1 Post-session Location 1 Pre-session Location 2 Post-session Location 2 Pre-session Location 3 Post-session Location 3 Pre-session Location 4 Post-session Location 4 Pre-session Location 5 Post-session Location 5 0 50 100 150 Locations MeanFrequenciesinLocations Sage * * * * * Pre-session Location 0 Post-session Location 0 Pre-session Location 1 Post-session Location 1 Pre-session Location 2 Post-session Location 2 Pre-session Location 3 Post-session Location 3 Pre-session Location 4 Post-session Location 4 Pre-session Location 5 Post-session Location 5 0 50 100 150 Locations MeanFrequenciesinLocations Kaytee * * * * * Page 21 of 21 URL: http://mc.manuscriptcentral.com/jaaws Email: ken.shapiro@animalsandsociety.org Journal of Applied Animal Welfare Science 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 12 13 14 15 16 17 18 19 20 21 22 23 24 25 26 27 28 29 30 31 32 33 34 35 36 37 38 39 40 41 42 43 44 45 46 47 48 49 50 51 52 53 54 55 56 57 58 59 60