SlideShare a Scribd company logo
Workplace Emotions: The Role of Supervision and Leadership
Joyce E. Bono, Hannah Jackson Foldes, Gregory Vinson, and John P. Muros
University of Minnesota
In this experience sampling study, the authors examined the role of organizational leaders in employees’
emotional experiences. Data were collected from health care workers 4 times a day for 2 weeks. Results
indicate supervisors were associated with employee emotions in 3 ways: (a) Employees experienced
fewer positive emotions when interacting with their supervisors as compared with interactions with
coworkers and customers; (b) employees with supervisors high on transformational leadership experi-
enced more positive emotions throughout the workday, including interactions with coworkers and
customers; and (c) employees who regulated their emotions experienced decreased job satisfaction and
increased stress, but those with supervisors high on transformational leadership were less likely to
experience decreased job satisfaction. The results also suggest that the effects of emotional regulation on
stress are long lasting (up to 2 hr) and not easily reduced by leadership behaviors.
Keywords: emotional regulation, mood, stress, job satisfaction, leadership
Over the past 2 decades, there has been a growing interest in
affective and emotional experiences at work (Brief & Weiss,
2002), including interest in the role of mood and emotions in
employee motivation (Erez & Isen, 2002), job performance (Law,
Wong, & Song, 2004), creativity (George & Zhou, 2002), and job
attitudes (Weiss & Cropanzano, 1996). Emotional regulation at
work has also received attention, especially given evidence that
regulating emotions is associated with cardiovascular system ac-
tivation (Gross & Levenson, 1993, 1997), stress, emotional ex-
haustion (Pugliesi, 1999), and physical symptoms such as head-
aches (Schaubroeck & Jones, 2000).
There has also been interest in emotions in the leadership
domain. Most influential theories of transformational and charis-
matic leadership (e.g., Bass, 1985; Conger & Kanungo, 1998;
House, 1977; Shamir, House, & Arthur, 1993) posit emotional
links between leaders and followers, yet there is little empirical
research linking managers and their leadership behaviors to em-
ployees’ emotions. A few studies have been conducted, but these
studies have not fully elucidated emotional links between leaders
and followers because they either manipulated leader emotions
(Sy, Coˆte´, & Saavedra, 2005); used simulated leaders and follow-
ers (Bono & Ilies, 2006); or used a single-time, single-source
survey to assess follower emotions and leader behavior (McColl-
Kennedy & Anderson, 2002). Therefore, the primary purpose of
our study was to examine the effects of supervisors and managers
on employees’ emotions in a natural work setting. First, we ex-
amined the direct effects of supervisors and managers and their
leadership behaviors on employees’ emotional experiences (i.e.,
experienced emotions, expressed emotions, and emotional regula-
tion). Second, we examined the extent to which managers’ lead-
ership behaviors can buffer employees from the negative conse-
quences of emotional regulation. We used an experience sampling
methodology and within-person analyses, which allowed us to
focus on within-person covariations and the effects of leaders’
behaviors on these covariations. Figure 1 displays the associations
of interest in this research.
Leaders and Employee Emotions
Despite widespread beliefs that supervisors are a key source of
bad moods at work, there is little empirical research documenting
these effects. There is an extensive literature on workplace factors
associated with employee well-being and stress (see Danna &
Griffin, 1999, for a review), and a key assumption of this research
is that supervisors affect employees’ emotional experiences. From
a theoretical standpoint, there are at least two reasons why em-
ployees may feel increased anxiety during supervisory interac-
tions. First, supervisors are the individuals who directly evaluate
performance, and thus interactions with supervisors may increase
anxiety about performance, which can be directly evaluated by the
supervisor during interactions. Second, Ryan and Deci (2000)
provided compelling evidence that individuals have a need for
autonomy, which tends to be limited in the workplace by super-
visors. Interactions with supervisors allows for closer observations
of employee behavior, which may leave employees feeling mon-
itored and controlled (George & Zhou, 2001), leading to feelings
of irritation. Furthermore, research by Diefendorff and Richard
(2003) suggests that supervisors’ expectations may lead employees
to constrain their emotional expressions, which also leads to neg-
ative affect.
Empirical research is also consistent with the notion that super-
visors may negatively influence employee emotions. Glaso and
Einarsen (2006) found four affective factors relevant to the
Joyce E. Bono, Hannah Jackson Foldes, Gregory Vinson, and John P.
Muros, Department of Psychology, University of Minnesota.
Hannah Jackson Foldes is now employed at Personnel Decisions Inter-
national, and Gregory Vinson is now employed at The Center for Victims
of Torture.
This article was supported in part by a Grant-in-Aid of Research and
Artistry, University of Minnesota.
Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to Joyce E.
Bono, Department of Psychology, University of Minnesota, 75 East River
Road, Minneapolis, MN 55455. E-mail: jbono@umn.edu
Journal of Applied Psychology Copyright 2007 by the American Psychological Association
2007, Vol. 92, No. 5, 1357–1367 0021-9010/07/$12.00 DOI: 10.1037/0021-9010.92.5.1357
1357
supervisor–subordinate relationship, three of which were negative
(i.e., frustration, violation, and uncertainty). Fitness (2000) inter-
viewed employees about their experiences of anger and found that
unfair treatment by supervisors, which tended to remain unre-
solved, was a key source of employee anger. A recent experience
sampling study directly examined the link between mood and
supervisory interactions (Miner, Glomb, & Hulin, 2005). This
research revealed that employees rated 80% of their interactions
with their supervisors as positive and only 20% as negative;
however, the effects of negative interactions on employee mood
were, in general, 5 times stronger than the effects of positive
interactions. These findings suggest that even though most super-
visory interactions are positive, the overall net effect of interac-
tions with supervisors may be slightly negative because of the
stronger effects of negative interactions on employee mood.1
Hypothesis 1: Employees experience more negative and
fewer positive emotions when interacting with supervisors
than when interacting with coworkers or clients and custom-
ers.
In addition to the general mood effects of interactions with
supervisors, we also expected supervisors’ typical behavioral pat-
terns (i.e., leadership behaviors) to influence employees’ emo-
tional experiences. Abusive supervisors (Tepper, 2000) might be
expected to elicit frustration, anxiety, and anger. In contrast, su-
pervisors who use transformational leadership behaviors may elicit
feelings of happiness and enthusiasm in employees. Transforma-
tional and charismatic leadership theories (Bass, 1985; Conger &
Kanungo, 1998; House, 1977) are unique in the emphasis they
place on the emotional aspects of leadership. Shamir et al. (1993)
focused on emotional attachment to the leader and the emotional
arousal of followers, and Bass (1985) suggested that transforma-
tional leadership “has an intense emotional component” (p. 36).
Testing these notions, Bono and Ilies (2006) recently linked ratings
of transformational leadership to managers’ use of positive affect
words (e.g., good, happy) in their written and verbal communica-
tions. Lab studies also suggest that positive emotions experienced
and expressed by leaders may be transferred to employees through
the process of emotional contagion. Sy et al. (2005) reported that
leader’s positive mood, which was induced in their study, influ-
enced group affect and performance. Bono and Ilies extended this
work by linking leaders’ expressions of enthusiasm to followers’
positive mood states. Whereas these results are promising, there
has been no research examining the effects of leadership behaviors
on employee mood throughout the day in a natural work setting.
Hypothesis 2: There is a positive association between super-
visors’ transformational leadership behaviors and employees’
experiences of positive emotions throughout the workday.
Leaders and Employee Emotional Regulation
The preceding arguments imply direct effects of supervisors and
their leadership behaviors on employees’ experienced emotions. A
second potential influence of leader behaviors involves leaders as
a coping resource for employees who regulate their emotions at
work. Emotional regulation refers to processes by which individ-
uals choose which emotions they express, relative to those they
experience, in either a controlled or an automatic way (Gross,
1998). People habitually regulate their emotions and emotional
displays to conform with norms and expectations of the workplace,
as well as job role demands. Initial theoretical work by Hochschild
(1979) and the more recent free trait theory (Little, 2000) both
view emotional regulation as harmful to employees because it
involves acting without authenticity. Empirical evidence supports
this notion, as researchers have found that suppressing emotions
has both physiological and cognitive costs, including cardiovascu-
lar activation and decreased memory for social information (see
Gross, Richards, & John, 2006, for a review), and is also associ-
ated with psychological strain (e.g., stress, emotional exhaustion,
and burnout) and physical complaints (Schaubroeck & Jones,
2000). Therefore, we expected the following:
Hypothesis 3a: Emotional regulation (both hiding negative
emotions and faking positive emotions) is negatively associ-
ated with job satisfaction within individuals.
Hypothesis 3b: Emotional regulation (both hiding negative
emotions and faking positive emotions) is positively associ-
ated with experienced stress within individuals.
Whereas these hypotheses are not unique in linking emotional
regulation to stress and job satisfaction, existing research has
examined only the association between employees’ general ten-
1
Although there is considerable evidence that positive and negative
mood represent independent dimensions of affect experienced by individ-
uals in general (Watson & Clark, 1997), when participants are asked to
report their current mood, a single dimension (i.e., happiness vs. unhappi-
ness) best represents mood data (Diener & Emmons, 1984). However,
because we wished to examine the potential differential effects of regulat-
ing positive and negative emotions, we considered positive and negative
mood separately, expecting them to be negatively correlated.
Figure 1. Model linking supervision to employees’ experienced emotions, emotional regulation, stress, and job
satisfaction. H ϭ Hypothesis.
1358 BONO, FOLDES, VINSON, AND MUROS
dencies to regulate emotions at work and their general tendencies
to experience stress and decreased job satisfaction. In contrast, we
hypothesized and tested direct links between individual episodes
of emotional regulation, job stress, and job satisfaction and their
covariation within individuals. Using a repeated measures, within-
subjects design, we sought to determine whether individuals’ lev-
els of stress and job satisfaction fluctuate throughout the course of
the workday in conjunction with emotional regulation. By exam-
ining variability around each individual’s mean level, we con-
trolled for differences between individuals in the tendency to
experience, remember, and report emotional experiences and
stress. This design also allowed us to examine the association
between emotional regulation and stress over time.
In linking supervisors’ and managers’ leadership behaviors to
emotional regulation, we drew from two theoretical perspectives.
Self-determination theory (Ryan & Deci, 2000) and the goal self-
concordance model (Sheldon & Elliott, 1999) both posit links
between authentic self-expression and individual well-being.
Hochschild (1983) suggested that when employees regulate their
emotions at work, they experience feelings of depersonalization
and separation from self; however, it appears that this is not always
the case, as certain conditions may weaken or eliminate the neg-
ative effects of emotional regulation. In a review of the literature,
John and Gross (2004) reported that healthier patterns of social
functioning and greater well-being were linked to emotional reg-
ulation for individuals who changed the way they thought about an
emotional event as compared with individuals who changed only
their emotional expressions. In addition, Ashforth and Humphrey
(1993) suggested that employees who identify with their work are
more likely to feel authentic even when conforming to role expec-
tations, such as demands for emotional regulation. This is where
leadership can play a role. In both lab and field studies, Bono and
Judge (2003) demonstrated that managers’ transformational lead-
ership behaviors can influence employees’ identification with their
work. They also found that transformational leadership was a
positive predictor of the extent to which employees felt that their
work activities were self-congruent and consistent with their own
interests and values. This suggests that individuals who report to
transformational leaders, compared with those who do not, will be
more likely to identify with their work and to change the way they
think about role-required emotional regulation, thereby leading
them to experience less stress and greater job satisfaction.
Managers who engage in transformational leadership behaviors
may also provide greater social support for their employees. Social
support has been recognized as important to the experience of
emotional management at work because providers of social sup-
port (e.g., family, coworkers, and supervisors) are thought to
enable employees to cope better with job stressors and increase
their sense of control (Abraham, 1998; Zapf, 2002). Abraham
reported that when employees experienced high social support, no
association between emotional regulation and job satisfaction was
found; however, under conditions of low social support, emotional
labor negatively affected satisfaction. Transformational leaders are
typically characterized as empathetic, and the behavioral dimen-
sion of individualized consideration explicitly describes leaders as
attending to and supporting the individual needs of followers. Such
leaders may be able to help employees cope with emotional
regulation in more effective and less psychologically draining
ways by helping employees to understand why and how positive
emotional expressions contribute to work goals (e.g., Shamir et al.,
1993). Because they engender trust, transformational leaders are
also uniquely positioned to assist employees in coping with emo-
tional labor. Therefore, we expected the following:
Hypothesis 4a: Supervisors’ transformational leadership be-
haviors moderate the link between emotional regulation and
job satisfaction, such that when supervisors engage in more
transformational leadership behaviors, the negative associa-
tion between emotional regulation and job satisfaction is
weaker than when supervisors engage in less transformational
leadership behaviors.
Hypothesis 4b: Supervisors’ transformational leadership be-
haviors moderate the link between emotional regulation and
stress, such that when supervisors engage in more transfor-
mational leadership behaviors, the positive association be-
tween emotional regulation and stress is weaker than when
supervisors engage in less transformational leadership behav-
iors.
Method
Participants
Participants were 57 employees of an ambulatory health care
organization, randomly selected from nonmanagement employees
participating in an organization-wide survey. As is common in
health care organizations, participants were predominantly women
(94%). They were, on average, 41 years old (SD ϭ 10 years).
Eighty-six percent were Caucasian, 9% were African American,
and 5% were of Asian origin. Participants’ average job tenure was
5.24 years. Participants worked in family practice clinics (n ϭ 14),
administrative offices (n ϭ 10), and a billing office (n ϭ 33), and
held a broad variety of jobs ranging from nurse, medical assistant,
and lab technician in the family practice clinics; patient services
support, case analyst, and account follow-up specialist in the
billing office; and accountant, case manager, and human resource
specialist in the administrative office.
Procedure
Participants provided two types of data: survey data and expe-
rience sampling data. Paper surveys were used to gather general
job satisfaction and stress data. Experience sampling data were
obtained via a handheld computer that participants carried for 2
weeks and included momentary stress, job satisfaction, and affec-
tive experiences. A third type of data—leadership behaviors of
participants’ supervisors—was obtained from an organization-
wide survey.
As part of an organization-wide survey that we administered,
all employees (N ϭ 365; 309 nonmanagement and 56 manage-
ment) reported on the leadership behaviors of their direct su-
pervisor. Surveys, completed in small group sessions by 253
(73%) nonmanagement and 56 (100%) management employees,
were anonymous (employees recorded only the name of their
supervisor) and were returned to us in unmarked, sealed enve-
lopes. Only aggregate data were provided to organization ex-
ecutives. One month following the organizational survey, we
invited 73 employees to participate in the study. Participants
1359LEADERSHIP AND EMPLOYEE EMOTIONS
were quasi-randomly selected, proportionately from each loca-
tion, by entering the names of those who participated in the
organization-wide survey into a spreadsheet, sorting by first
name, and then choosing every other person on the list until the
desired number of participants was achieved.
Fifty-eight of the 73 employees contacted (79%) affirmed they
would attend an information session, 6 (8%) were interested but
could not make it to a scheduled information session, and 9 (12%)
indicated they were not interested in participating in this research.
At the orientation session, participants were informed that the
organization’s management had endorsed their participation in the
study and that they were allowed to respond to surveys during
normal working hours. They were also informed that they would
receive $25 as payment for participation. After obtaining this
information, 57 of the 58 employees agreed to participate.
After agreeing to enroll in the study, participants completed a
survey, after which they were trained in the use of a personal
digital assistant (PDA). For the experience sampling surveys,
participants were signaled four times each day, Monday through
Friday, for 2 weeks. The signal intervals were modified to accom-
modate participants’ regular working hours (e.g., 7 a.m. to 3 p.m.
or 8 a.m. to 4 p.m.). Participants were signaled once during each
2-hr segment of their 8-hr workday. Participants were given 10
min to respond to each signal, after which the survey was no longer
available. They were told to respond to the surveys based on the
emotions and attitudes they were experiencing “immediately be-
fore the beep went off.” In addition, they were instructed to
respond to as many signals as possible during the workday without
compromising their job performance or service to patients or
customers. Because we were not able to program the PDAs to stop
signaling on weekends (i.e., Saturday and Sunday), we asked
participants to leave the PDA either at work, in their car, or at
home in a location where the signals would not be annoying. Data
were collected for 10 working days.2
At each signal, participants were asked to report whether they
were at work. As we were attempting to collect data based spe-
cifically on emotional regulation in a social context, we next asked
whether they were interacting with others when the signal
sounded. Only surveys with yes responses to this item were in-
cluded in this research. Next, they were asked to report on their
level of stress and job satisfaction at that moment. Finally, they
were asked to report on their affective experiences at that moment,
including the extent to which they were feeling six emotions
(happiness, enthusiasm, optimism, anxiety, irritation, and anger)
and the extent to which they were faking the three positive emo-
tions (happiness, enthusiasm, and optimism) and hiding the three
negative emotions (anxiety, irritation, and anger). Hereinafter, we
refer to experienced emotions as emotions experienced or felt
emotions and to faking and hiding emotions as emotional regula-
tion. The 12 affective items (i.e., 6 experienced emotion items and
6 emotional regulation items) appeared in random order for each
survey.
At the end of 2 weeks, participants attended a debriefing
session, completed a second paper survey regarding general
stress at work, returned their handheld computers, and were
paid $25. At this time, they were also asked the name of their
supervisor so that we could obtain reports on their supervisor’s
leadership behaviors from the organizational survey. Table 1
displays the source and format of the data, along with the data
collection timeline.
Measures
Supervisors’ leadership behaviors. The leadership behaviors
of participants’ supervisors were measured using the 20-item Mul-
tifactor Leadership Questionnaire (MLQ – Form 5x; Avolio, Bass,
& Jung, 1995).3
The MLQ is the most frequently used measure of
transformational leadership and has exhibited high reliability and
convergent and discriminant validity (Avolio et al., 1995). Re-
sponses were evaluated on a 5-point scale ranging from 1 (not at
all) to 5 (frequently, if not always). Consistent with other research-
ers whose interest is the general construct of transformational
leadership behaviors, we combined items to form a single score for
each supervisor (e.g., Lim & Ployhart, 2004).
Job satisfaction. We assessed momentary job satisfaction by
asking participants to rate their agreement with the statement “At
this very moment, I am fairly satisfied with my job” on a 5-point
scale (1 ϭ strongly disagree, 5 ϭ strongly agree). Ilies and Judge
2
The PDAs were programmed to signal twice during the evening hours
as well. The optional evening surveys were used to collect pilot data for
another project. Thirty-five individuals responded to evening surveys,
which were not included in this study.
3
The MLQ, Form 5x (Copyright 1995 by Bernard Bass and Bruce
Avolio) is used with permission of Mind Garden, 1690 Woodside Road,
Suite 202, Redwood City, CA 94061.
Table 1
Overview of Data Collection
Variable Source Format Timeline
Supervisors’ leadership behaviors Aggregate of employee responses
to organizational survey
Paper survey T1: One month prior to ESM
General job satisfaction Participant Paper survey T2: Immediately prior to ESM
Work status and interactions Participant Handheld computer T3: During ESM
Momentary job satisfaction Participant Handheld computer T3: During ESM
Momentary stress Participant Handheld computer T3: During ESM
Affective experiences Participant Handheld computer T3: During ESM
Overall stress Participant Paper survey T4: Immediately following ESM
Note. The Experience sampling method (ESM) period consisted of a 2-week time frame during which participants responded to multiple surveys
administered daily via a handheld computer. T1–T4 refer to the four distinct episodes of data collection.
1360 BONO, FOLDES, VINSON, AND MUROS
(2002; Judge & Ilies, 2004) have found considerable variation in
job satisfaction over the course of the workday, which our mo-
mentary measure was designed to capture. We also obtained a
measure of general job satisfaction, using five Brayfield–Rothe
items (e.g., “Most days I am enthusiastic about my work”; see
Bono & Judge, 2003; Brayfield & Rothe, 1951). Responses for
these five items were on the same 5-point scale used for momen-
tary job satisfaction.
Work status and interactions. Immediately after being sig-
naled by the PDA, participants were asked to report whether they
were currently at work (yes or no). Next, they responded to the
following question: “When the beep went off, were you interacting
with any of the following?” 1 (supervisor), 2 (coworker), 3 (cus-
tomer/client), 4 (family/friends), 5 (no one), 6 (other).
Affective experiences. Although many taxonomies of affect
and emotion exist in the literature, we were constrained by our
method (i.e., surveys of only 1–2 min in length) to select a small
number of affect terms in attempting to cover the full range of
work-relevant affect and emotion. After reviewing the literature
(Ortony & Turner, 1990; Russell & Feldman Barrett, 1999;
Shaver, Schwartz, Kirson, & O’Connor, 1987) and conducting a
pilot study of workers who reported how frequently they expressed
each of 10 emotions at work, we selected three positive (happiness,
enthusiasm, and optimism) and three negative (anxiety, anger, and
irritation) work-relevant emotions. We excluded some basic emo-
tions (e.g., fear and love) and low-activation affect terms (e.g.,
contentment and calm), deeming them of low relevance to our
study. Participants reported the degree to which they felt the six
selected emotions at each signal.
Because Glomb and Tews (2004) illustrated the importance of
distinguishing between felt emotions and the act of emotional
regulation, we also asked participants about faking (i.e., expressing
an emotion they did not feel) the three positive emotions (happi-
ness, enthusiasm, and optimism) and hiding (i.e., feeling an emo-
tion they were not expressing) the three negative emotions (anxi-
ety, irritation, and anger). Responses for these items were
evaluated on a 7-point scale (1 ϭ none at all, 7 ϭ an intense
amount) for feeling, hiding, and faking.
Stress. We assessed momentary stress by asking participants
to respond to the statement “At this very moment, I am experi-
encing stress.” This measure was intended to capture variability in
stress throughout the day. We also obtained an overall, global
measure of stress, using Motowidlo, Packard, and Manning’s
(1986) four-item measure. Responses for both momentary and
overall stress items were on the same 5-point scale used for job
satisfaction.
Data Analyses
Prior to conducting our analyses of interest, we examined sev-
eral properties of our data. We excluded data from 3 participants
who had responded to fewer than three signals over the course of
the 2 weeks (1 because of job termination, 1 because of PDA
failure, and 1 for an unknown reason). Therefore, the total possible
number of responses was approximately 2,160 (4 responses per
day ϫ 10 workdays ϫ 54 participants ϭ 2,160); however, because
of scheduling the drop-off and pick-up of PDAs, a few participants
responded to surveys on Days 11 and 12, raising the total possible
to 2,266). Participants responded to 1,983 signals (approximately
88% of the total possible), with an average of 37 responses per
participant (ranging from 11 to 42). We excluded data from re-
sponses when participants were not at work (n ϭ 287 episodes)
and for interactions with family or friends (n ϭ 52 episodes),
resulting in 1,653 work-related responses. Our goal was to exam-
ine emotional regulation in a social context. However, in many
cases participants were alone when the signal sounded (n ϭ 764;
48% of work-related responses).4
Thus, our final data set consisted
of 889 responses (52% of total at-work responses; average of 15
per participant) from employees who were at work and involved in
a work-related interaction at the time of the signal.
To form a single leadership score for the supervisor of each
participant, we aggregated the leadership survey responses of all
employees who completed a survey for the target supervisor (av-
erage n ϭ 5 reports per supervisor). Aggregating leadership reports
across followers was deemed justifiable in these data by a signif-
icant ICC-1 value of .29 (p Ͻ .01) and an ICC-2 value of .72. The
ICC-2 value can be interpreted as the reliability of the aggregated
measure of leadership. An average rwg ϭ .84 across groups
(James, Demaree, & Wolf, 1984; assuming a slight negative skew
in the data [Bono & Judge, 2003]) further supports aggregation.
We also created several composite emotion variables. The three
positive (enthusiasm, happiness, and optimism) and three negative
(anger, irritation, and anxiety) emotions were highly correlated
within individuals for both felt emotions and emotional regulation
(average r ϭ .87, .77, .93, and .82, for positive experienced,
negative experienced, positive faked, and negative hidden, respec-
tively). Therefore, we computed four composite emotion variables:
positive emotions experienced, negative emotions experienced,
positive emotions faked, and negative emotions hidden. Principal-
components analysis provided support for creating these compos-
ites, as we found that a single factor explained most of the variance
(ranging from 70%–82%) in each set of related emotions. We also
found that the two types of emotional regulation were highly
related (i.e., faking positive emotions and hiding negative emo-
tions tended to co-occur; r ϭ .88, p Ͻ .01). Because we were
interested in possible differential effects of the two types of emo-
tional regulation, we included faking positive and hiding negative
in our analyses. However, we also formed a total emotional reg-
ulation score by averaging the two types of emotional regulation.
Our hypotheses address two levels of analysis. Within-subject
analyses must be used to link emotional regulation within an
individual with momentary variation in stress and job satisfaction
(Hypothesis 3). In these analyses, data from each individual are
centered around that individual’s mean score, effectively control-
ling for mean differences between individuals in emotional regu-
lation, job satisfaction, and stress. Cross-level (between- and
within-subjects) analyses are used to test our leadership hypothe-
ses (Hypotheses 1, 2, and 4), which posit associations between
supervisory behavior and employees’ emotional experiences.
Therefore, we analyzed our data using multilevel modeling tech-
niques (i.e., HLM 5; Raudenbush & Bryk, 2002).
4
A comparison of affective experiences when interacting with others
versus alone revealed no significant differences in experienced positive and
negative emotions or in emotional regulation.
1361LEADERSHIP AND EMPLOYEE EMOTIONS
Results
Means, standard deviations, and correlations are presented in
Table 2, revealing that participants generally experienced more
positive emotions than negative emotions at work and tended to
hide negative emotions only slightly more than they faked positive
emotions (mean hide negative ϭ 1.67, and mean fake positive ϭ
1.50). Mean levels of job satisfaction and stress were similar in the
momentary, PDA data (Level 1) and in the global, overall measure
(Level 2). We note a high correlation between negative mood and
emotional regulation (r ϭ .78, .92, and .88, all ps Ͻ .01, for faking
positive, hiding negative, and total emotional regulation, respec-
tively). These high correlations are consistent with the notion that
positive emotions are valued (and negative emotions devalued) in
organizations, leading employees to suppress negative emotions.
Leaders and Employee Emotions
Hypotheses 1 and 2 address the role of supervisors in employ-
ees’ emotional experiences. We first examined the link between
employees’ affective experiences and interaction partner (e.g.,
supervisor vs. customer–client). Analysis of variance results (see
Table 3) revealed significant differences in affective experiences
based on interaction partner. Specifically, participants reported the
most positive emotions when interacting with their coworkers
(mean positive emotions experienced ϭ 3.75) and fewest positive
emotions when interacting with their supervisors (mean positive
emotions experienced ϭ 3.05). This was true for each of the
individual positive emotions, which we do not report in Table 3,
and the positive emotion composite, F(3, 886) ϭ 5.74, p Ͻ .01.
Thus, Hypothesis 1 was supported for positive emotions; however,
interaction partner was significantly related neither to the experi-
ence of negative emotions nor to employees’ emotional regulation.
Next, we examined the direct effects of supervisors’ transfor-
mational leadership behaviors on employees’ momentary emo-
tional experiences, testing the theory that transformational leaders
influence employee emotions. In this analysis we linked the su-
pervisor’s typical leadership behaviors to employees’ emotions
across all interactions, including those with customers and cowork-
ers. Table 4 presents coefficients linking supervisors’ leadership
behaviors to employees’ experienced emotions and emotional reg-
ulation. As hypothesized (Hypothesis 2), employees who work for
supervisors rated high on transformational leadership reported
experiencing more positive emotions throughout the course of
their workday (␥ ϭ .54, p Ͻ .01). This held true for each of the
individual positive emotions (results not reported in Table 4), as
well as the positive emotions composite. Leadership behaviors
were not linked to participants’ experience of negative emotions or
to reports of emotional regulation.
Leaders and Employee Emotional Regulation
Emotional regulation, job satisfaction, and stress. In addition
to main effects of supervisors’ leadership behaviors on employees’
affective experiences, we also hypothesized a moderating effect
for transformational leadership. We examined the data to deter-
mine whether emotional regulation covaries with stress and job
satisfaction within individuals and over time. Results in Table 5
(upper portion: momentary data) addressed the link between mo-
mentary emotional regulation, job satisfaction, and stress. Results
revealed a significant, negative association between both types of
emotional regulation and job satisfaction (␥ ϭ –.18 and –.18, p Ͻ
.01, for hiding negative and faking positive, respectively) and a
significant positive link between both types of emotional regula-
tion and stress (␥ ϭ .40 and .35, p Ͻ .01, for hiding negative and
faking positive, respectively). These results suggest that regardless
of how often certain individuals regulate their emotions, individual
episodes of emotional regulation are associated with increased
stress (above the mean for a given individual) and decreased job
satisfaction (below the mean for a given individual). This was true
for both faking positive and hiding negative emotions, supporting
Hypotheses 3a and 3b.
A unique advantage of experience sampling research is that we
can examine the link between emotional regulation at one point in
time (t) and stress or job satisfaction for that individual at the next
random signal (t ϩ 1), allowing us to draw stronger causal infer-
ences and to determine how long these effects last. Results in
Table 5 (lower portion: lagged momentary data) showed that the
association between emotional regulation and stress extended over
time. Although weaker than the concurrent effects, results indi-
cated a significant positive association between emotional regula-
tion at one time point and stress approximately 2 hr later. No
Table 2
Means, Standard Deviations, and Intercorrelations Between Level 2 Variables and Aggregate of Level 1 Variables
Scale M SD 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 12
1. Positive affect 3.78 .52 .82
2. Negative affect 1.66 .61 Ϫ.13 .91
3. Transformational leadership 3.24 .55 .24 Ϫ.19 .93
4. ESM job satisfaction 3.73 .59 .28*
Ϫ.17 .19 —
5. ESM job stress 2.22 .67 .05 .31*
.12 Ϫ.19 —
6. Feel positive emotions 3.65 .98 .31*
Ϫ.22 .31*
.56**
Ϫ.24 .95
7. Feel negative emotions 1.90 .81 .15 .38**
.03 Ϫ.38**
.66**
Ϫ.26 .90
8. Fake positive emotions 1.50 .67 .09 .24 .07 Ϫ.28*
.51**
Ϫ.06 .78**
.97
9. Hide negative emotions 1.67 .81 .15 .24 .12 Ϫ.37**
.58**
Ϫ.20 .92**
.88**
.92
10. Total emotional regulation 1.59 .71 .13 .25 .10 Ϫ.34**
.57**
Ϫ.14 .88**
.96**
.98**
—
11. General job satisfaction 3.74 .59 .39**
Ϫ.24 .29*
.74**
Ϫ.14 .43**
Ϫ.27**
Ϫ.21 Ϫ.21 Ϫ.22 .85 —
12. General stress 2.84 .82 Ϫ.20 .20 Ϫ.25 Ϫ.35**
.36**
Ϫ.28*
.31*
.26 .28*
.28*
Ϫ.38**
.79
Note. N ϭ 54. Coefficient alphas are presented on the main diagonal. ESM ϭ Experience sampling method.
*
p Ͻ .05. **
p Ͻ .01.
1362 BONO, FOLDES, VINSON, AND MUROS
significant effects for emotional regulation (t) on stress were found
after 4 hr (t ϩ 2), and sample sizes were too small to examine t ϩ
3 data (i.e., average 6-hr time lag). Although our data are corre-
lational and preclude firm causal conclusions, these results lend
support to our proposed causal ordering of the variables (e.g.,
emotional regulation 3 stress), as we also tested the reverse
ordering (e.g., stress [t] 3 emotional regulation [t ϩ 1]) and found
no significant effects. The effects of emotional regulation on job
satisfaction appear to be more fleeting than the effects of stress, as
emotional regulation did not predict job satisfaction after 2 hr.
Leadership as buffer. We suggested that leadership behaviors
would buffer employees from the negative effects of emotional
regulation. Results in Table 6 support Hypothesis 4a, revealing a
significant moderating effect for transformational leadership (␥ ϭ
.21, p Ͻ .01) on the association between emotional regulation and
job satisfaction. Figure 2 shows that when transformational lead-
ership behaviors were low (1 standard deviation below the mean),
episodes of emotional regulation were associated with decreased
job satisfaction. However, when transformational leadership be-
haviors were high (1 standard deviation above the mean), there
was little or no association between emotional regulation and job
satisfaction. A different picture emerged when we examined stress
(Hypothesis 4b). Contrary to our expectations, supervisors’ trans-
formational leadership behaviors did not protect employees from
the stress associated with emotional regulation (see Table 6).5
Discussion
According to Brief and Weiss (2002), “the organizational liter-
ature is populated with many more ideas about the leader’s role in
the production of moods and emotions than it is with relevant data”
(p. 289). In this study we focused on the role of managers in
producing employee emotions and in buffering them from the
effects of emotional regulation. With respect to the former, our
results revealed that employees experience less optimism, happi-
ness, and enthusiasm when they interact with supervisors than
when they interact with customers, clients, and coworkers. We also
found that employees who report to supervisors who engage in
transformational leadership behaviors, compared with those who
do not, experience more optimism, happiness, and enthusiasm
throughout the day, including during their interactions with cus-
tomers and coworkers. With respect to the latter, our results
indicate that when employees regulate their emotions at work, they
experience increased stress and decreased job satisfaction. Further-
5
Several participants in our sample reported to the same supervisor,
creating dependencies in the leadership data that were not addressed in our
hierarchical linear modeling analyses and may have potentially produced
biased tests of significance. Thus, we repeated our moderator analyses
involving leadership (see Table 6) using only 1 participant per leader. This
reduced our sample size (from 54 to 27) but eliminated the dependencies.
All significant effects reported in Table 6 remained significant in this
subsample.
Table 3
Analysis of Variance for Felt and Regulated Emotions by Interaction Partner
Emotion
Interaction
partner M SD F
% total variance explained
by interaction partner
Feel positive Supervisor 3.05 1.44 5.74**
36
Coworker 3.75 1.25
Customer-client 3.53 1.58
Other 3.54 1.47
Feel negative Supervisor 2.08 1.35 1.01 4
Coworker 1.83 1.13
Customer-client 1.90 1.14
Other 1.84 1.19
Fake positive Supervisor 1.38 0.91 0.30 6
Coworker 1.47 0.88
Customer-client 1.46 0.85
Other 1.42 0.86
Hide negative Supervisor 1.50 1.01 0.61 3
Coworker 1.62 1.04
Customer-client 1.67 1.05
Other 1.56 0.99
Note. N ϭ 889. dfs ϭ 3, 886. The frequency of interactions was as follows: coworker, 55%; customer-client,
23%; supervisor, 8%; other (which included groups of individuals), 14%.
**
p Ͻ .01.
Table 4
Hierarchical Linear Modeling Estimates of the Associations
Between Supervisors’ Leadership Behaviors and Participants’
Affective Experiences
Affective experiences
Transformational leadership behavior
(␥01)
Coefficient SE t
Feeling positive .54**
.18 2.94
Feeling negative .03 .22 0.15
Faking positive .11 .16 0.73
Hiding negative .18 .20 0.94
Total emotional regulation .14 .17 0.85
Note. N ϭ 54 participants; N ϭ 889 observations. Coefficients for feeling
positive, feeling negative, faking positive, hiding negative, and total emo-
tional regulation were estimated independently. Approximate df ϭ 51.
**
p Ͻ .01.
1363LEADERSHIP AND EMPLOYEE EMOTIONS
more, although the effects of emotional regulation on job satisfac-
tion appear to be short-lived, the effects of emotional regulation on
stress are more long lasting (e.g., 2 hr). Managers who use trans-
formational leadership behaviors buffer employees who regulate
their emotions from decreased job satisfaction, but not from in-
creased stress. Given these results, two important questions re-
main: (a) How do these results contribute to our understanding of
“the leader’s role in the production of moods and emotions” (Brief
& Weiss, 2002, p. 289)?, and (b) How do these results contribute
to our understanding of the role of managers in workplace emo-
tional regulation?
With respect to leadership and emotions, results of this study
demonstrate the powerful role that managers and supervisors have
on employee emotions. Transformational leadership theory sug-
gests emotional links between leaders and followers but offers
little explanation of how such emotional links contribute to lead-
ership effectiveness. Based on theories of primitive emotional
contagion, lab studies (e.g., Barsade, 2002; Sy et al., 2005) have
documented the fleeting effects of a leader’s positive mood on the
mood of workers. Given evidence in this study that managers’
leadership behaviors have an ongoing influence on employee
optimism and enthusiasm, it may be time to look beyond primitive
emotional contagion as the primary process by which leaders
influence follower emotions and organizational outcomes. Our
results suggest that managers’ transformational leadership behav-
iors may have broad, deep, and long-lasting effects on individual
employees and the organization as a whole. Beyond their imme-
diate effects on employee mood, the positive emotions elicited by
transformational leaders have the potential to influence the overall
work climate and customer satisfaction. Employees who are opti-
mistic are more likely to invest the time to help others (Lee &
Allen, 2002) and can be expected to persist in work tasks – even
in the face of difficulty (Seligman & Schulman, 1986). In the same
vein, research by Pugh (2001) linked positive emotional displays
by employees to customer evaluations of service quality.
Our study also contributes to theory and existing research on
emotional regulation. What we know from the existing literature is
that individuals who tend to regulate their emotions are also less
happy with their jobs and more stressed, although the link with job
satisfaction varies across studies. Our results show that discrete
episodes of emotional regulation are also associated with de-
creased job satisfaction and increased stress, even for individuals
who do not frequently regulate their emotions. This is an important
finding given that emotional regulation theory is focused on the
negative effects of sustained and frequent emotional regulation.
However, it is also important to note that we did not find any
time-lagged effects of emotional regulation on job satisfaction.
Thus, the effects of discrete episodes of emotional regulation on
Table 5
Hierarchical Linear Modeling Analysis Linking Emotional Regulation to Stress and Job Satisfaction
Variable
Job satisfaction Stress
Coefficient SE t Coefficient SE t
Momentary dataa
Intercept (␥00) 3.73**
0.08 46.47 2.21**
0.09 24.89
Hiding negative emotions (␥10) Ϫ.18**
.04 Ϫ4.94 .40**
.07 5.92
Faking positive emotions (␥10) Ϫ.18**
.05 Ϫ3.60 .35**
.06 5.78
Total emotional regulation (␥10) Ϫ.23**
.05 Ϫ4.74 .49**
.07 6.88
Lagged momentary datab
Intercept (␥00) 3.76**
0.10 39.08 2.25**
0.12 19.33
Hiding negative emotions (␥10) Ϫ.05 .04 Ϫ1.24 .20**
.08 2.63
Faking positive emotions (␥10) Ϫ.00 .05 Ϫ0.29 .19 .10 1.85
Total emotional regulation (␥10) Ϫ.04 .05 Ϫ0.95 .25*
.10 2.41
Note. Coefficients for hiding negative, faking positive, and total emotional regulation were estimated independently. For lagged data, we used emotional
regulation at t and stress and job satisfaction at t ϩ 1, where t represents a time point and t ϩ 1 represents the time of the next random signal (2 hr later
on average).
a
n ϭ 821; approximate df ϭ 819. b
n ϭ 284; approximate df ϭ 281.
*
p Ͻ .05. **
p Ͻ .01.
Table 6
Hierarchical Linear Modeling Estimates of the Moderating Effect of Transformational Leadership
Variable
Hiding negative emotion Faking positive emotion Total emotion regulation
Coefficient SE t Coefficient SE t Coefficient SE t
Moderator effects—job satisfaction
Transformational leadership (␥13) .12**
.06 2.01 .24**
.07 3.21 .21**
.07 2.87
Moderator effects—stress
Transformational leadership (␥13) .01**
.10 0.14 .02**
.11 0.14 .01**
.10 0.09
Note. N ϭ 54 participants; N ϭ 889 observations. Coefficients for hiding negative, faking positive, and total emotional regulation were estimated
independently. Approximate df ϭ 51.
**
p Ͻ .01.
1364 BONO, FOLDES, VINSON, AND MUROS
job satisfaction appear to be very short-lived (less than 2 hr). This
is in contrast to the effects of emotional regulation on stress, which
remained significant 2 hr later. Hochschild’s (1983) seminal re-
search suggested that individuals who must frequently regulate
their emotions on the job will experience depersonalization and
stress. Although our results do not speak to cumulative effects over
time, they do suggest that even for individuals who rarely regulate
their emotions, doing so on a single occasion is associated with
increased stress, which may last for several hours. An important
topic for future research is to examine the long-term, sustained
effects of emotional regulation on employee health.
In organizational research on emotional regulation, it is common
to do as we did and consider both job satisfaction and stress as
important outcomes of emotional regulation; however, our results
suggest that different processes may underlie the effects of emo-
tional regulation on these two outcomes. We found that emotional
regulation was associated with stress 2 hr later, but the effects of
emotional regulation on job satisfaction were more fleeting (i.e.,
we found no time-lagged effects). We also found that managerial
behavior buffered employees from the effects of emotional regu-
lation on job satisfaction but not on stress, suggesting that the
processes linking emotional regulation with job satisfaction and
stress may differ. The link between emotional regulation and job
satisfaction may be cognitive in nature. One way that managers
provide support for employees is by enhancing internalized self-
regulation and drawing attention to outcomes associated with
emotional regulation (e.g., greater service for customers). There-
fore, when employees regulate their emotions, they may con-
sciously decide that it is worth doing because it helps them to
perform better in a job they care about and identify with. Thus,
during emotional regulation they may feel momentarily unhappy
with their job because it demands inauthentic emotional expres-
sions, but these effects are fleeting (and do not occur for employ-
ees who report to transformational managers) because employees
understand how emotional regulation contributes to their job.
In contrast, the effects of emotional regulation on stress may be,
at least in part, physiological. Empirical evidence shows that
emotional regulation leads to cardiac arousal (see Gross & Lev-
enson, 1993), which is linked to perceptions of stress (see Blas-
covich & Tomaka, 1996). It may be that the association between
emotional regulation and stress is stronger, lasts longer, and is less
amenable to reduction through managerial support because it has a
physiological basis. Clearly, we can only speculate about the
processes by which emotional regulation affects stress and job
satisfaction, but this is an important area for future research. If our
theoretical propositions are correct, managers may be able to help
employees more by reducing or eliminating the need for emotional
regulation (perhaps by encouraging and supporting authenticity)
rather than attempting to buffer them from the negative effects of
emotional regulation after it occurs.
Our study highlights the need for follow-up research. First, it is
important to establish causality by manipulating both emotional
regulation and leadership behavior in the lab. Some work has been
done in this regard by Bono and Vey (2007), who linked emotional
regulation to increased heart rate and stress. Controlled laboratory
settings would also be useful in assessing the longevity of the
effects of emotional regulation episodes and sustained emotional
regulation. Second, it is important to determine the direct cause of
emotional regulation. Although our theoretical model suggests that
emotional regulation causes stress, our methods did not allow us to
establish causality. Indeed, it is possible that certain events in the
workplace may cause both emotional regulation and stress. For
example, in cases in which workers are asked to be pleasant and
enthusiastic with customers, a rude customer may be the source of
emotional regulation, which then leads to stress. Alternatively, a
rude customer may be the source of both emotional regulation and
stress, creating the illusion of a causal association between them.
It is also plausible that an unpleasant interaction with a patient or
customer would have both direct effects on stress and indirect
effects, through emotional regulation. One possible next step
Figure 2. The moderating effect of transformational leadership on the relationship between hiding negative
emotions and faking positive emotions and job satisfaction. Job satisfaction scores are based on responses to the
question “At this very moment, I am fairly satisfied with my job,” rated on a 5-point scale (1 ϭ strongly
disagree, 5 ϭ strongly agree).
1365LEADERSHIP AND EMPLOYEE EMOTIONS
would be to use experience sampling methods to catalogue events
that lead to emotional regulation in the natural work environment
and determine how these events affect both emotions and stress.
Our study is unique in investigating leadership as an important
factor in employees’ experienced emotions and in the emotional
regulation process; it has some methodological strengths and lim-
itations worth noting. Concerning the former, we used a longitu-
dinal design in conjunction with an experience sampling method-
ology. In doing so, we were able to collect daily emotional
regulation data across multiple locations and jobs as emotional
regulation occurred. Thus, we avoided retrospective and recall
biases that occur in reporting emotional experiences. Our within-
person design also allowed us to confidently link specific events of
emotional regulation to stress and job satisfaction, free from the
biasing effects of individual differences. Furthermore, our mea-
sures of leadership behavior and employee emotional experiences
were relatively independent of each other. Our study also ad-
dressed the call for more research to be conducted in health care
settings (Ehler, Major, & Fletcher, 2003).
Despite these strengths, our study also has some limitations.
First, although our participants held a variety of jobs, we did not
have the sample size to examine subgroups of employees in
service versus nonservice support jobs. Second, our sample was
drawn from a single organization. Thus, it is possible that charac-
teristics of the work climate, shared by all participants, influenced
the experience of both positive and negative emotions and overall
levels of emotional regulation. If the entire work climate was
particularly supportive or abusive, our results may under- or over-
estimate the moderating effects of leadership behaviors. Third, our
sample—though representative of the larger organization and the
health care industry—was largely female, and we cannot be certain
that the associations we found would hold for men. Fourth, it is
possible that the PDA signals may have annoyed participants,
causing increased irritation and stress. If this is true, the effects
were small because mean stress from PDA data was not higher
than global reports of stress.
Considered as a whole, our results highlight the importance of
supervisors in employees’ emotional experiences. Our results are
provocative in that they suggest the possibility of different theo-
retical explanations (and therefore the need for different manage-
rial interventions) for the effects of emotional regulation on stress
and job satisfaction. Our study also extends existing theory on
leader–follower emotional links by looking beyond the primitive
emotional contagion process. We hope this study will motivate
future longitudinal research in naturalistic settings along with
experimental research in the laboratory, including both psycholog-
ical and physiological measures of employee moods, emotional
regulation, and stress over time.
References
Abraham, R. (1998). Emotional dissonance in organizations: Antecedents,
consequences, and moderators. Genetic, Social, and General Psychology
Monographs, 124, 229–246.
Ashforth, B. E., & Humphrey, R. H. (1993). Emotional labor in service
roles: The influence of identity. Academy of Management Review, 18,
88–115.
Avolio, B. J., Bass, B. M., & Jung, D. I. (1995). Multifactor Leadership
Questionnaire technical report. Redwood City, CA: Mind Garden.
Barsade, S. G. (2002). The ripple effects: Emotional contagion and its
influence on group behavior. Administrative Science Quarterly, 47,
644–675.
Bass, B. M. (1985). Leadership and performance beyond expectations.
New York: Free Press.
Blascovich, J., & Tomaka, J. (1996). The biopsychosocial model of arousal
regulation. Advances in Experimental Social Psychology, 28, 1–51.
Bono, J. E., & Ilies, R. (2006). Charisma, positive emotions, and mood
contagion. Leadership Quarterly, 17, 317–334.
Bono, J. E., & Judge, T. A. (2003). Self concordance at work: Toward
understanding the motivational effects of transformational leaders.
Academy of Management Journal, 46, 554–571.
Bono, J. E., & Vey, M. A. (2007). Personality and emotional performance:
Extraversion, neuroticism, and self-monitoring. Journal of Occupational
Health Psychology, 12, 177–192.
Brayfield, A. H., & Rothe, H. F. (1951). An index of job satisfaction.
Journal of Applied Psychology, 35, 307–311.
Brief, A. P., & Weiss, H. M. (2002). Organizational behavior: Affect in the
workplace. Annual Review of Psychology, 53, 279–307.
Conger, J. A., & Kanungo, R. N. (1998). Charismatic leadership in
organizations. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.
Danna, K., & Griffin, R. W. (1999). Health and well-being in the work-
place: A review and synthesis of the literature. Journal of Management,
25, 357–384.
Diefendorff, J. M., & Richard, E. M. (2003). Antecedents and conse-
quences of emotional display rule perceptions. Journal of Applied Psy-
chology, 88, 284–294.
Diener, E., & Emmons, R. (1984). The independence of positive and
negative affect. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 47,
1105–1117.
Ehler, M. L., Major, D. A., & Fletcher, T. D. (2003). Applying I-O to
medicine: Making the case that it can be done and that it should be done.
The Industrial–Organizational Psychologist, 41, 50–54.
Erez, A. I., & Isen, A. M. (2002). The influence of positive affect on the
components of expectancy motivation. Journal of Applied Psychology,
87, 1055–1067.
Fitness, J. (2000). Anger in the workplace: An emotion script approach to
anger episodes between workers and their superiors, coworkers, and
subordinates. Journal of Organizational Behavior, 21, 147–162.
George, J. M., & Zhou, J. (2001). When Openness to Experience and
Conscientiousness are related to creative behavior: An interactional
approach. Journal of Applied Psychology, 86, 513–524.
George, J. M., & Zhou, J. (2002). Understanding when bad moods foster
creativity and good ones don’t: The role of context and clarity of
feelings. Journal of Applied Psychology, 87, 687–697.
Glaso, L., & Einarsen, S. (2006). Experienced affects in leader–subordinate
relationships. Scandinavian Journal of Management, 22, 49–73.
Glomb, T. M., & Tews, M. J. (2004). Emotional labor: A conceptualization
and scale development. Journal of Vocational Behavior, 64, 1–23.
Gross, J. J. (1998). Antecedent- and response-focused emotion regulation:
Divergent consequences for experience, expression, and physiology.
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 74, 224–237.
Gross, J. J., & Levenson, R. W. (1993). Emotional suppression: Physiol-
ogy, self-report, and expressive behavior. Journal of Personality and
Social Psychology, 64, 970–986.
Gross, J. J., & Levenson, R. W. (1997). Hiding feelings: The acute effects
of inhibiting negative and positive emotion. Journal of Abnormal Psy-
chology, 106, 95–103.
Gross, J. J., Richards, J. M., & John, O. P. (2006). Emotion regulation in
everyday life. In D. K. Snyder, J. A. Simpson, & J. N. Hughes (Eds.),
Emotion regulation in couples and families: Pathways to dysfunction
and health (pp. 13–39). Washington, DC: American Psychological As-
sociation.
Hochschild, A. R. (1979). Emotion work, feeling rules, and social struc-
ture. American Journal of Sociology, 85, 551–575.
1366 BONO, FOLDES, VINSON, AND MUROS
Hochschild, A. R. (1983). The managed heart. Berkeley: University of
California Press.
House, R. J. (1977). A 1976 theory of charismatic leadership. In J. G. Hunt
& L. L. Larsen (Eds.), Leadership: The cutting edge. Carbondale:
Southern Illinois University Press.
Ilies, R., & Judge, T. A. (2002). Understanding the dynamic relationships
among personality, mood, and job satisfaction: A field experience sam-
pling study. Organizational Behavior & Human Decision Processes, 89,
1119–1139.
James, L. R., Demaree, R. G., & Wolf, G. (1984). Estimating within-group
reliability with and without response bias. Journal of Applied Psychol-
ogy, 69, 85–98.
John, O. P., & Gross, J. J. (2004). Healthy and unhealthy emotional
regulation: Personality processes, individual differences and life span
development. Journal of Personality, 72, 1301–1333.
Judge, T. A., & Ilies, R. (2004). Affect and job satisfaction: A study of their
relationship at work and at home. Journal of Applied Psychology, 89,
661–673.
Law, K. S., Wong, C., & Song, L. (2004). The construct and criterion
validity of emotional intelligence and its potential utility for manage-
ment studies. Journal of Applied Psychology, 89, 483–496.
Lee, K., & Allen, N. J. (2002). Organizational citizenship behavior and
workplace deviance: The role of affect and cognitions. Journal of
Applied Psychology, 87, 131–142.
Lim, B. C., & Ployhart, R. E. (2004). Transformational leadership: Rela-
tions to the five-factor model and team performance in typical and
maximum contexts. Journal of Applied Psychology, 89, 610–621.
Little, B. R. (2000). Free traits and personal contexts: Expanding a social
ecological model of well-being. In W. B. Walsh & K. H. Craik (Eds.),
Person–environment psychology: New directions and perspectives (2nd
ed., pp. 87–116). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
McColl-Kennedy, J. R., & Anderson, R. D. (2002). Impact of leadership
style and emotions on subordinate performance. Leadership Quarterly,
13, 545–559.
Miner, A., Glomb, T. M., & Hulin, C. (2005). Experience sampling mood
and its correlates at work. Journal of Occupational and Organizational
Psychology, 78, 171–193.
Motowidlo, S. J., Packard, J. S., & Manning, M. R. (1986). Occupational
stress: Its causes and consequences for job performance. Journal of
Applied Psychology, 71, 618–629.
Ortony, A., & Turner, T. (1990). What’s basic about basic emotions?
Psychological Review, 97, 313–331.
Pugh, S. D. (2001). Service with a smile: Emotional contagion in the
service encounter. Academy of Management Journal, 44, 1018–1027.
Pugliesi, K. (1999). The consequences of emotional labor: Effects on work
stress, job satisfaction, and well-being. Motivation and Emotion, 23,
125–154.
Raudenbush, S. W., & Bryk, A. S. (2002). Hierarchical linear models:
Applications and data analysis methods. London: Sage.
Russell, J. A., & Feldman Barrett, L. (1999). Core affect, prototypical
emotional episodes, and other things called emotion: Dissecting the
elephant. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 76, 805–819.
Ryan, R. M., & Deci, E. L. (2000). Self-determination theory and the
facilitation of intrinsic motivation, social development, and well-being.
American Psychologist, 55, 68–78.
Schaubroeck, J., & Jones, J. R. (2000). Antecedents of workplace emo-
tional labor dimensions and moderators of their effects on physical
symptoms. Journal of Organizational Behavior, 21, 163–183.
Seligman, M. E., & Schulman, P. (1986). Explanatory style as a predictor
of productivity and quitting among life insurance sales agents. Journal
of Personality and Social Psychology, 50, 832–838.
Shamir, B., House, R. J., & Arthur, M. B. (1993). The motivational effects
of charismatic leadership: A self-concept based theory. Organization
Science, 4, 577–594.
Shaver, P., Schwartz, J., Kirson, D., & O’Connor, C. (1987). Emotion
knowledge: Further exploration of a prototype approach. Journal of
Personality and Social Psychology, 52, 1061–1086.
Sheldon, K. M., & Elliott, A. J. (1999). Goal striving, need satisfaction and
longitudinal well-being: The self-concordance model. Journal of Per-
sonality and Social Psychology, 76, 482–497.
Sy, T., Coˆte´, S., & Saavedra, R. (2005). The contagious leader: Impact of
the leader’s mood on the mood of group members, group affective tone,
and group processes. Journal of Applied Psychology, 90, 205–305.
Tepper, B. J. (2000). Consequences of abusive supervision. Academy of
Management Journal, 43, 178–190.
Watson, D., & Clark, L. A. (1997). Measurement and mismeasurement of
mood: Recurrent and emergent issues. Journal of Personality Assess-
ment, 68, 267–296.
Weiss, H. M., & Cropanzano, R. (1996). Affective events theory: A
theoretical discussion of the structure, causes and consequences of
affective experiences at work. In B. M. Staw & L. L. Cummings (Eds.),
Research in organizational behavior: An annual series of analytical
essays and critical reviews (pp. 1–74). Greenwich, CT: JAI Press.
Zapf, D. (2002). Emotion work and psychological well-being: A review of
the literature and some conceptual considerations. Human Resource
Management Review, 12, 237–268.
Received February 9, 2005
Revision received December 15, 2006
Accepted December 18, 2006 Ⅲ
1367LEADERSHIP AND EMPLOYEE EMOTIONS

More Related Content

What's hot

ORganisational Relation note chapter two
ORganisational Relation note chapter twoORganisational Relation note chapter two
ORganisational Relation note chapter two
Classic Tech
 
Expectancy theory
Expectancy theoryExpectancy theory
Expectancy theory
Pramodh Sherla
 
Job satisfaction and morale
Job satisfaction and moraleJob satisfaction and morale
Job satisfaction and morale
Aanchal
 
Psych 642 Poster David Dysart
Psych 642 Poster David DysartPsych 642 Poster David Dysart
Psych 642 Poster David Dysart
David Dysart
 
Gagne´ Forest 2008 The study of compensation systems through the lens of self...
Gagne´ Forest 2008 The study of compensation systems through the lens of self...Gagne´ Forest 2008 The study of compensation systems through the lens of self...
Gagne´ Forest 2008 The study of compensation systems through the lens of self...
Labour Market Reform Commission
 
On Attitudes and Job Satisfaction
On Attitudes and Job SatisfactionOn Attitudes and Job Satisfaction
On Attitudes and Job Satisfaction
Gregar Donaven Valdehueza
 
Mathematical modeling to monitor workplace humor style and subordinate worked...
Mathematical modeling to monitor workplace humor style and subordinate worked...Mathematical modeling to monitor workplace humor style and subordinate worked...
Mathematical modeling to monitor workplace humor style and subordinate worked...
Triple A Research Journal
 
Effects of Occupational Stress on Employees: Gender Reaction and Coping Techn...
Effects of Occupational Stress on Employees: Gender Reaction and Coping Techn...Effects of Occupational Stress on Employees: Gender Reaction and Coping Techn...
Effects of Occupational Stress on Employees: Gender Reaction and Coping Techn...
DenisseDiaz30
 
Job satisfaction arihant duraplast , no c 19 sipcot industrial park kanchee...
Job satisfaction arihant duraplast , no c   19 sipcot industrial park kanchee...Job satisfaction arihant duraplast , no c   19 sipcot industrial park kanchee...
Job satisfaction arihant duraplast , no c 19 sipcot industrial park kanchee...
Vignesh Machi
 
1Motivation
1Motivation1Motivation
Well Being, Fairness, and Supervisor’s Ability and Support
Well Being, Fairness, and Supervisor’s Ability and Support Well Being, Fairness, and Supervisor’s Ability and Support
Well Being, Fairness, and Supervisor’s Ability and Support
IJSRP Journal
 
Job satisfaction in teaching profession
Job satisfaction in teaching professionJob satisfaction in teaching profession
Job satisfaction in teaching profession
buket77
 
Job Satisfaction Of Teachers
Job Satisfaction Of TeachersJob Satisfaction Of Teachers
Job Satisfaction Of Teachers
guest5e0c7e
 
Sess 7
Sess 7Sess 7
A Study of Organisational Role Stress on Merchandisers Working in Export Orie...
A Study of Organisational Role Stress on Merchandisers Working in Export Orie...A Study of Organisational Role Stress on Merchandisers Working in Export Orie...
A Study of Organisational Role Stress on Merchandisers Working in Export Orie...
Dr. Krishnanand Tripathi
 
Reinforcement theory of Motivation
Reinforcement theory of  MotivationReinforcement theory of  Motivation
Reinforcement theory of Motivation
CHITRAKSH KAPIL
 
Cognitive evaluation theory
Cognitive evaluation theoryCognitive evaluation theory
Cognitive evaluation theory
Jitin Kollamkudy
 
Impact of Abusive Supervision on Employee Turnover Intention: The Moderating ...
Impact of Abusive Supervision on Employee Turnover Intention: The Moderating ...Impact of Abusive Supervision on Employee Turnover Intention: The Moderating ...
Impact of Abusive Supervision on Employee Turnover Intention: The Moderating ...
ijtsrd
 
Effect of Job Stress and Job Satisfaction on Performance: An Empirical Study
Effect of Job Stress and Job Satisfaction on Performance: An Empirical StudyEffect of Job Stress and Job Satisfaction on Performance: An Empirical Study
Effect of Job Stress and Job Satisfaction on Performance: An Empirical Study
ridhsi
 
Motivation
MotivationMotivation

What's hot (20)

ORganisational Relation note chapter two
ORganisational Relation note chapter twoORganisational Relation note chapter two
ORganisational Relation note chapter two
 
Expectancy theory
Expectancy theoryExpectancy theory
Expectancy theory
 
Job satisfaction and morale
Job satisfaction and moraleJob satisfaction and morale
Job satisfaction and morale
 
Psych 642 Poster David Dysart
Psych 642 Poster David DysartPsych 642 Poster David Dysart
Psych 642 Poster David Dysart
 
Gagne´ Forest 2008 The study of compensation systems through the lens of self...
Gagne´ Forest 2008 The study of compensation systems through the lens of self...Gagne´ Forest 2008 The study of compensation systems through the lens of self...
Gagne´ Forest 2008 The study of compensation systems through the lens of self...
 
On Attitudes and Job Satisfaction
On Attitudes and Job SatisfactionOn Attitudes and Job Satisfaction
On Attitudes and Job Satisfaction
 
Mathematical modeling to monitor workplace humor style and subordinate worked...
Mathematical modeling to monitor workplace humor style and subordinate worked...Mathematical modeling to monitor workplace humor style and subordinate worked...
Mathematical modeling to monitor workplace humor style and subordinate worked...
 
Effects of Occupational Stress on Employees: Gender Reaction and Coping Techn...
Effects of Occupational Stress on Employees: Gender Reaction and Coping Techn...Effects of Occupational Stress on Employees: Gender Reaction and Coping Techn...
Effects of Occupational Stress on Employees: Gender Reaction and Coping Techn...
 
Job satisfaction arihant duraplast , no c 19 sipcot industrial park kanchee...
Job satisfaction arihant duraplast , no c   19 sipcot industrial park kanchee...Job satisfaction arihant duraplast , no c   19 sipcot industrial park kanchee...
Job satisfaction arihant duraplast , no c 19 sipcot industrial park kanchee...
 
1Motivation
1Motivation1Motivation
1Motivation
 
Well Being, Fairness, and Supervisor’s Ability and Support
Well Being, Fairness, and Supervisor’s Ability and Support Well Being, Fairness, and Supervisor’s Ability and Support
Well Being, Fairness, and Supervisor’s Ability and Support
 
Job satisfaction in teaching profession
Job satisfaction in teaching professionJob satisfaction in teaching profession
Job satisfaction in teaching profession
 
Job Satisfaction Of Teachers
Job Satisfaction Of TeachersJob Satisfaction Of Teachers
Job Satisfaction Of Teachers
 
Sess 7
Sess 7Sess 7
Sess 7
 
A Study of Organisational Role Stress on Merchandisers Working in Export Orie...
A Study of Organisational Role Stress on Merchandisers Working in Export Orie...A Study of Organisational Role Stress on Merchandisers Working in Export Orie...
A Study of Organisational Role Stress on Merchandisers Working in Export Orie...
 
Reinforcement theory of Motivation
Reinforcement theory of  MotivationReinforcement theory of  Motivation
Reinforcement theory of Motivation
 
Cognitive evaluation theory
Cognitive evaluation theoryCognitive evaluation theory
Cognitive evaluation theory
 
Impact of Abusive Supervision on Employee Turnover Intention: The Moderating ...
Impact of Abusive Supervision on Employee Turnover Intention: The Moderating ...Impact of Abusive Supervision on Employee Turnover Intention: The Moderating ...
Impact of Abusive Supervision on Employee Turnover Intention: The Moderating ...
 
Effect of Job Stress and Job Satisfaction on Performance: An Empirical Study
Effect of Job Stress and Job Satisfaction on Performance: An Empirical StudyEffect of Job Stress and Job Satisfaction on Performance: An Empirical Study
Effect of Job Stress and Job Satisfaction on Performance: An Empirical Study
 
Motivation
MotivationMotivation
Motivation
 

Viewers also liked

Brandon bays-libertatea-este
Brandon bays-libertatea-esteBrandon bays-libertatea-este
Brandon bays-libertatea-este
Anca Ioana Vlad
 
Teorii.emotii
Teorii.emotiiTeorii.emotii
Teorii.emotii
Dorina Croitori
 
Manifestarile procesului emotional
Manifestarile procesului emotionalManifestarile procesului emotional
Manifestarile procesului emotional
Cabinet Psihologic
 
Inventarul de personalitate eysenck
Inventarul de personalitate eysenckInventarul de personalitate eysenck
Inventarul de personalitate eysenck
Cabinet Psihologic
 
Autoaparare psihica
Autoaparare psihicaAutoaparare psihica
Autoaparare psihicaNicu Barbi
 
How to score in exams with little prepration
How to score in exams with little preprationHow to score in exams with little prepration
How to score in exams with little prepration
Trending Us
 
Groovy Grammar! Interesting ways to learn grammar!
Groovy Grammar! Interesting ways to learn grammar!Groovy Grammar! Interesting ways to learn grammar!
Groovy Grammar! Interesting ways to learn grammar!
Shelly Sanchez Terrell
 
9 Learning Strategies from Knowledge to Know-How
9 Learning Strategies from Knowledge to Know-How9 Learning Strategies from Knowledge to Know-How
9 Learning Strategies from Knowledge to Know-How
LinkedIn Learning Solutions
 
Succession “Losers”: What Happens to Executives Passed Over for the CEO Job?
Succession “Losers”: What Happens to Executives Passed Over for the CEO Job? Succession “Losers”: What Happens to Executives Passed Over for the CEO Job?
Succession “Losers”: What Happens to Executives Passed Over for the CEO Job?
Stanford GSB Corporate Governance Research Initiative
 

Viewers also liked (9)

Brandon bays-libertatea-este
Brandon bays-libertatea-esteBrandon bays-libertatea-este
Brandon bays-libertatea-este
 
Teorii.emotii
Teorii.emotiiTeorii.emotii
Teorii.emotii
 
Manifestarile procesului emotional
Manifestarile procesului emotionalManifestarile procesului emotional
Manifestarile procesului emotional
 
Inventarul de personalitate eysenck
Inventarul de personalitate eysenckInventarul de personalitate eysenck
Inventarul de personalitate eysenck
 
Autoaparare psihica
Autoaparare psihicaAutoaparare psihica
Autoaparare psihica
 
How to score in exams with little prepration
How to score in exams with little preprationHow to score in exams with little prepration
How to score in exams with little prepration
 
Groovy Grammar! Interesting ways to learn grammar!
Groovy Grammar! Interesting ways to learn grammar!Groovy Grammar! Interesting ways to learn grammar!
Groovy Grammar! Interesting ways to learn grammar!
 
9 Learning Strategies from Knowledge to Know-How
9 Learning Strategies from Knowledge to Know-How9 Learning Strategies from Knowledge to Know-How
9 Learning Strategies from Knowledge to Know-How
 
Succession “Losers”: What Happens to Executives Passed Over for the CEO Job?
Succession “Losers”: What Happens to Executives Passed Over for the CEO Job? Succession “Losers”: What Happens to Executives Passed Over for the CEO Job?
Succession “Losers”: What Happens to Executives Passed Over for the CEO Job?
 

Similar to Apl 92 5_1357.pdf emotii

16Personal Consequences of Employee CommitmentUniversity o.docx
16Personal Consequences of Employee CommitmentUniversity o.docx16Personal Consequences of Employee CommitmentUniversity o.docx
16Personal Consequences of Employee CommitmentUniversity o.docx
drennanmicah
 
The Relationship between Emotional Intelligence (EQ) and Adversity Quotient (AQ)
The Relationship between Emotional Intelligence (EQ) and Adversity Quotient (AQ)The Relationship between Emotional Intelligence (EQ) and Adversity Quotient (AQ)
The Relationship between Emotional Intelligence (EQ) and Adversity Quotient (AQ)
IOSRJBM
 
Running head EMOTIONAL INTELLEGENCE 1 Re.docx
Running head EMOTIONAL INTELLEGENCE  1 Re.docxRunning head EMOTIONAL INTELLEGENCE  1 Re.docx
Running head EMOTIONAL INTELLEGENCE 1 Re.docx
susanschei
 
Managerial Psychology By Dr.Mahboob Khan Phd
Managerial Psychology By Dr.Mahboob Khan PhdManagerial Psychology By Dr.Mahboob Khan Phd
Managerial Psychology By Dr.Mahboob Khan Phd
Healthcare consultant
 
Running head RESEARCH ANALYSIS .docx
Running head RESEARCH ANALYSIS                               .docxRunning head RESEARCH ANALYSIS                               .docx
Running head RESEARCH ANALYSIS .docx
todd521
 
20131211378&r1995 v1-n3-str252-261
20131211378&r1995 v1-n3-str252-26120131211378&r1995 v1-n3-str252-261
20131211378&r1995 v1-n3-str252-261
Dr ASMA Imran
 
HR Research paper Employee Morale final
HR Research paper Employee Morale finalHR Research paper Employee Morale final
HR Research paper Employee Morale final
Gwen Knight
 
Each answer 250 words minimumQuestion 1What assumptions and.docx
Each answer 250 words minimumQuestion 1What assumptions and.docxEach answer 250 words minimumQuestion 1What assumptions and.docx
Each answer 250 words minimumQuestion 1What assumptions and.docx
jacksnathalie
 
The Effect of Job Satisfaction on Organizational Commitment of Healthcare Per...
The Effect of Job Satisfaction on Organizational Commitment of Healthcare Per...The Effect of Job Satisfaction on Organizational Commitment of Healthcare Per...
The Effect of Job Satisfaction on Organizational Commitment of Healthcare Per...
IOSRJBM
 
Job Insecurity and Emotional Stability of Professionals at Their Work Place
Job Insecurity and Emotional Stability of Professionals at Their Work PlaceJob Insecurity and Emotional Stability of Professionals at Their Work Place
Job Insecurity and Emotional Stability of Professionals at Their Work Place
inventionjournals
 
Running head EMPLOYEE WORK BEHAVIOR 1EMPLOYEE WORK BEHAVIOR.docx
Running head EMPLOYEE WORK BEHAVIOR  1EMPLOYEE WORK BEHAVIOR.docxRunning head EMPLOYEE WORK BEHAVIOR  1EMPLOYEE WORK BEHAVIOR.docx
Running head EMPLOYEE WORK BEHAVIOR 1EMPLOYEE WORK BEHAVIOR.docx
todd271
 
I4105665.pdf
I4105665.pdfI4105665.pdf
I4105665.pdf
aijbm
 
Mediating Effect of Primary Coping Strategies in the relationship between Big...
Mediating Effect of Primary Coping Strategies in the relationship between Big...Mediating Effect of Primary Coping Strategies in the relationship between Big...
Mediating Effect of Primary Coping Strategies in the relationship between Big...
Jayamini D Samarathunge
 
a project report on Job satisfaction
 a project report on Job satisfaction a project report on Job satisfaction
a project report on Job satisfaction
Babasab Patil
 
Human Resource Practices as an Amplifier for Managerial Emotional Intelligenc...
Human Resource Practices as an Amplifier for Managerial Emotional Intelligenc...Human Resource Practices as an Amplifier for Managerial Emotional Intelligenc...
Human Resource Practices as an Amplifier for Managerial Emotional Intelligenc...
Dr. Nasir Mustafa
 
Ej1094678
Ej1094678Ej1094678
Ej1094678
chotika homhoon
 
Research Proposal
Research ProposalResearch Proposal
Research Proposal
Habib Rehman
 
106 International Journal of Business and Public A.docx
106             International Journal of Business and Public A.docx106             International Journal of Business and Public A.docx
106 International Journal of Business and Public A.docx
hyacinthshackley2629
 
Emotional Labor
Emotional Labor Emotional Labor
Emotional Labor
Bahadir Beadin
 
The Influence of Work Culture, Work Stress to the Job Satisfaction and Employ...
The Influence of Work Culture, Work Stress to the Job Satisfaction and Employ...The Influence of Work Culture, Work Stress to the Job Satisfaction and Employ...
The Influence of Work Culture, Work Stress to the Job Satisfaction and Employ...
IOSRJBM
 

Similar to Apl 92 5_1357.pdf emotii (20)

16Personal Consequences of Employee CommitmentUniversity o.docx
16Personal Consequences of Employee CommitmentUniversity o.docx16Personal Consequences of Employee CommitmentUniversity o.docx
16Personal Consequences of Employee CommitmentUniversity o.docx
 
The Relationship between Emotional Intelligence (EQ) and Adversity Quotient (AQ)
The Relationship between Emotional Intelligence (EQ) and Adversity Quotient (AQ)The Relationship between Emotional Intelligence (EQ) and Adversity Quotient (AQ)
The Relationship between Emotional Intelligence (EQ) and Adversity Quotient (AQ)
 
Running head EMOTIONAL INTELLEGENCE 1 Re.docx
Running head EMOTIONAL INTELLEGENCE  1 Re.docxRunning head EMOTIONAL INTELLEGENCE  1 Re.docx
Running head EMOTIONAL INTELLEGENCE 1 Re.docx
 
Managerial Psychology By Dr.Mahboob Khan Phd
Managerial Psychology By Dr.Mahboob Khan PhdManagerial Psychology By Dr.Mahboob Khan Phd
Managerial Psychology By Dr.Mahboob Khan Phd
 
Running head RESEARCH ANALYSIS .docx
Running head RESEARCH ANALYSIS                               .docxRunning head RESEARCH ANALYSIS                               .docx
Running head RESEARCH ANALYSIS .docx
 
20131211378&r1995 v1-n3-str252-261
20131211378&r1995 v1-n3-str252-26120131211378&r1995 v1-n3-str252-261
20131211378&r1995 v1-n3-str252-261
 
HR Research paper Employee Morale final
HR Research paper Employee Morale finalHR Research paper Employee Morale final
HR Research paper Employee Morale final
 
Each answer 250 words minimumQuestion 1What assumptions and.docx
Each answer 250 words minimumQuestion 1What assumptions and.docxEach answer 250 words minimumQuestion 1What assumptions and.docx
Each answer 250 words minimumQuestion 1What assumptions and.docx
 
The Effect of Job Satisfaction on Organizational Commitment of Healthcare Per...
The Effect of Job Satisfaction on Organizational Commitment of Healthcare Per...The Effect of Job Satisfaction on Organizational Commitment of Healthcare Per...
The Effect of Job Satisfaction on Organizational Commitment of Healthcare Per...
 
Job Insecurity and Emotional Stability of Professionals at Their Work Place
Job Insecurity and Emotional Stability of Professionals at Their Work PlaceJob Insecurity and Emotional Stability of Professionals at Their Work Place
Job Insecurity and Emotional Stability of Professionals at Their Work Place
 
Running head EMPLOYEE WORK BEHAVIOR 1EMPLOYEE WORK BEHAVIOR.docx
Running head EMPLOYEE WORK BEHAVIOR  1EMPLOYEE WORK BEHAVIOR.docxRunning head EMPLOYEE WORK BEHAVIOR  1EMPLOYEE WORK BEHAVIOR.docx
Running head EMPLOYEE WORK BEHAVIOR 1EMPLOYEE WORK BEHAVIOR.docx
 
I4105665.pdf
I4105665.pdfI4105665.pdf
I4105665.pdf
 
Mediating Effect of Primary Coping Strategies in the relationship between Big...
Mediating Effect of Primary Coping Strategies in the relationship between Big...Mediating Effect of Primary Coping Strategies in the relationship between Big...
Mediating Effect of Primary Coping Strategies in the relationship between Big...
 
a project report on Job satisfaction
 a project report on Job satisfaction a project report on Job satisfaction
a project report on Job satisfaction
 
Human Resource Practices as an Amplifier for Managerial Emotional Intelligenc...
Human Resource Practices as an Amplifier for Managerial Emotional Intelligenc...Human Resource Practices as an Amplifier for Managerial Emotional Intelligenc...
Human Resource Practices as an Amplifier for Managerial Emotional Intelligenc...
 
Ej1094678
Ej1094678Ej1094678
Ej1094678
 
Research Proposal
Research ProposalResearch Proposal
Research Proposal
 
106 International Journal of Business and Public A.docx
106             International Journal of Business and Public A.docx106             International Journal of Business and Public A.docx
106 International Journal of Business and Public A.docx
 
Emotional Labor
Emotional Labor Emotional Labor
Emotional Labor
 
The Influence of Work Culture, Work Stress to the Job Satisfaction and Employ...
The Influence of Work Culture, Work Stress to the Job Satisfaction and Employ...The Influence of Work Culture, Work Stress to the Job Satisfaction and Employ...
The Influence of Work Culture, Work Stress to the Job Satisfaction and Employ...
 

Apl 92 5_1357.pdf emotii

  • 1. Workplace Emotions: The Role of Supervision and Leadership Joyce E. Bono, Hannah Jackson Foldes, Gregory Vinson, and John P. Muros University of Minnesota In this experience sampling study, the authors examined the role of organizational leaders in employees’ emotional experiences. Data were collected from health care workers 4 times a day for 2 weeks. Results indicate supervisors were associated with employee emotions in 3 ways: (a) Employees experienced fewer positive emotions when interacting with their supervisors as compared with interactions with coworkers and customers; (b) employees with supervisors high on transformational leadership experi- enced more positive emotions throughout the workday, including interactions with coworkers and customers; and (c) employees who regulated their emotions experienced decreased job satisfaction and increased stress, but those with supervisors high on transformational leadership were less likely to experience decreased job satisfaction. The results also suggest that the effects of emotional regulation on stress are long lasting (up to 2 hr) and not easily reduced by leadership behaviors. Keywords: emotional regulation, mood, stress, job satisfaction, leadership Over the past 2 decades, there has been a growing interest in affective and emotional experiences at work (Brief & Weiss, 2002), including interest in the role of mood and emotions in employee motivation (Erez & Isen, 2002), job performance (Law, Wong, & Song, 2004), creativity (George & Zhou, 2002), and job attitudes (Weiss & Cropanzano, 1996). Emotional regulation at work has also received attention, especially given evidence that regulating emotions is associated with cardiovascular system ac- tivation (Gross & Levenson, 1993, 1997), stress, emotional ex- haustion (Pugliesi, 1999), and physical symptoms such as head- aches (Schaubroeck & Jones, 2000). There has also been interest in emotions in the leadership domain. Most influential theories of transformational and charis- matic leadership (e.g., Bass, 1985; Conger & Kanungo, 1998; House, 1977; Shamir, House, & Arthur, 1993) posit emotional links between leaders and followers, yet there is little empirical research linking managers and their leadership behaviors to em- ployees’ emotions. A few studies have been conducted, but these studies have not fully elucidated emotional links between leaders and followers because they either manipulated leader emotions (Sy, Coˆte´, & Saavedra, 2005); used simulated leaders and follow- ers (Bono & Ilies, 2006); or used a single-time, single-source survey to assess follower emotions and leader behavior (McColl- Kennedy & Anderson, 2002). Therefore, the primary purpose of our study was to examine the effects of supervisors and managers on employees’ emotions in a natural work setting. First, we ex- amined the direct effects of supervisors and managers and their leadership behaviors on employees’ emotional experiences (i.e., experienced emotions, expressed emotions, and emotional regula- tion). Second, we examined the extent to which managers’ lead- ership behaviors can buffer employees from the negative conse- quences of emotional regulation. We used an experience sampling methodology and within-person analyses, which allowed us to focus on within-person covariations and the effects of leaders’ behaviors on these covariations. Figure 1 displays the associations of interest in this research. Leaders and Employee Emotions Despite widespread beliefs that supervisors are a key source of bad moods at work, there is little empirical research documenting these effects. There is an extensive literature on workplace factors associated with employee well-being and stress (see Danna & Griffin, 1999, for a review), and a key assumption of this research is that supervisors affect employees’ emotional experiences. From a theoretical standpoint, there are at least two reasons why em- ployees may feel increased anxiety during supervisory interac- tions. First, supervisors are the individuals who directly evaluate performance, and thus interactions with supervisors may increase anxiety about performance, which can be directly evaluated by the supervisor during interactions. Second, Ryan and Deci (2000) provided compelling evidence that individuals have a need for autonomy, which tends to be limited in the workplace by super- visors. Interactions with supervisors allows for closer observations of employee behavior, which may leave employees feeling mon- itored and controlled (George & Zhou, 2001), leading to feelings of irritation. Furthermore, research by Diefendorff and Richard (2003) suggests that supervisors’ expectations may lead employees to constrain their emotional expressions, which also leads to neg- ative affect. Empirical research is also consistent with the notion that super- visors may negatively influence employee emotions. Glaso and Einarsen (2006) found four affective factors relevant to the Joyce E. Bono, Hannah Jackson Foldes, Gregory Vinson, and John P. Muros, Department of Psychology, University of Minnesota. Hannah Jackson Foldes is now employed at Personnel Decisions Inter- national, and Gregory Vinson is now employed at The Center for Victims of Torture. This article was supported in part by a Grant-in-Aid of Research and Artistry, University of Minnesota. Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to Joyce E. Bono, Department of Psychology, University of Minnesota, 75 East River Road, Minneapolis, MN 55455. E-mail: jbono@umn.edu Journal of Applied Psychology Copyright 2007 by the American Psychological Association 2007, Vol. 92, No. 5, 1357–1367 0021-9010/07/$12.00 DOI: 10.1037/0021-9010.92.5.1357 1357
  • 2. supervisor–subordinate relationship, three of which were negative (i.e., frustration, violation, and uncertainty). Fitness (2000) inter- viewed employees about their experiences of anger and found that unfair treatment by supervisors, which tended to remain unre- solved, was a key source of employee anger. A recent experience sampling study directly examined the link between mood and supervisory interactions (Miner, Glomb, & Hulin, 2005). This research revealed that employees rated 80% of their interactions with their supervisors as positive and only 20% as negative; however, the effects of negative interactions on employee mood were, in general, 5 times stronger than the effects of positive interactions. These findings suggest that even though most super- visory interactions are positive, the overall net effect of interac- tions with supervisors may be slightly negative because of the stronger effects of negative interactions on employee mood.1 Hypothesis 1: Employees experience more negative and fewer positive emotions when interacting with supervisors than when interacting with coworkers or clients and custom- ers. In addition to the general mood effects of interactions with supervisors, we also expected supervisors’ typical behavioral pat- terns (i.e., leadership behaviors) to influence employees’ emo- tional experiences. Abusive supervisors (Tepper, 2000) might be expected to elicit frustration, anxiety, and anger. In contrast, su- pervisors who use transformational leadership behaviors may elicit feelings of happiness and enthusiasm in employees. Transforma- tional and charismatic leadership theories (Bass, 1985; Conger & Kanungo, 1998; House, 1977) are unique in the emphasis they place on the emotional aspects of leadership. Shamir et al. (1993) focused on emotional attachment to the leader and the emotional arousal of followers, and Bass (1985) suggested that transforma- tional leadership “has an intense emotional component” (p. 36). Testing these notions, Bono and Ilies (2006) recently linked ratings of transformational leadership to managers’ use of positive affect words (e.g., good, happy) in their written and verbal communica- tions. Lab studies also suggest that positive emotions experienced and expressed by leaders may be transferred to employees through the process of emotional contagion. Sy et al. (2005) reported that leader’s positive mood, which was induced in their study, influ- enced group affect and performance. Bono and Ilies extended this work by linking leaders’ expressions of enthusiasm to followers’ positive mood states. Whereas these results are promising, there has been no research examining the effects of leadership behaviors on employee mood throughout the day in a natural work setting. Hypothesis 2: There is a positive association between super- visors’ transformational leadership behaviors and employees’ experiences of positive emotions throughout the workday. Leaders and Employee Emotional Regulation The preceding arguments imply direct effects of supervisors and their leadership behaviors on employees’ experienced emotions. A second potential influence of leader behaviors involves leaders as a coping resource for employees who regulate their emotions at work. Emotional regulation refers to processes by which individ- uals choose which emotions they express, relative to those they experience, in either a controlled or an automatic way (Gross, 1998). People habitually regulate their emotions and emotional displays to conform with norms and expectations of the workplace, as well as job role demands. Initial theoretical work by Hochschild (1979) and the more recent free trait theory (Little, 2000) both view emotional regulation as harmful to employees because it involves acting without authenticity. Empirical evidence supports this notion, as researchers have found that suppressing emotions has both physiological and cognitive costs, including cardiovascu- lar activation and decreased memory for social information (see Gross, Richards, & John, 2006, for a review), and is also associ- ated with psychological strain (e.g., stress, emotional exhaustion, and burnout) and physical complaints (Schaubroeck & Jones, 2000). Therefore, we expected the following: Hypothesis 3a: Emotional regulation (both hiding negative emotions and faking positive emotions) is negatively associ- ated with job satisfaction within individuals. Hypothesis 3b: Emotional regulation (both hiding negative emotions and faking positive emotions) is positively associ- ated with experienced stress within individuals. Whereas these hypotheses are not unique in linking emotional regulation to stress and job satisfaction, existing research has examined only the association between employees’ general ten- 1 Although there is considerable evidence that positive and negative mood represent independent dimensions of affect experienced by individ- uals in general (Watson & Clark, 1997), when participants are asked to report their current mood, a single dimension (i.e., happiness vs. unhappi- ness) best represents mood data (Diener & Emmons, 1984). However, because we wished to examine the potential differential effects of regulat- ing positive and negative emotions, we considered positive and negative mood separately, expecting them to be negatively correlated. Figure 1. Model linking supervision to employees’ experienced emotions, emotional regulation, stress, and job satisfaction. H ϭ Hypothesis. 1358 BONO, FOLDES, VINSON, AND MUROS
  • 3. dencies to regulate emotions at work and their general tendencies to experience stress and decreased job satisfaction. In contrast, we hypothesized and tested direct links between individual episodes of emotional regulation, job stress, and job satisfaction and their covariation within individuals. Using a repeated measures, within- subjects design, we sought to determine whether individuals’ lev- els of stress and job satisfaction fluctuate throughout the course of the workday in conjunction with emotional regulation. By exam- ining variability around each individual’s mean level, we con- trolled for differences between individuals in the tendency to experience, remember, and report emotional experiences and stress. This design also allowed us to examine the association between emotional regulation and stress over time. In linking supervisors’ and managers’ leadership behaviors to emotional regulation, we drew from two theoretical perspectives. Self-determination theory (Ryan & Deci, 2000) and the goal self- concordance model (Sheldon & Elliott, 1999) both posit links between authentic self-expression and individual well-being. Hochschild (1983) suggested that when employees regulate their emotions at work, they experience feelings of depersonalization and separation from self; however, it appears that this is not always the case, as certain conditions may weaken or eliminate the neg- ative effects of emotional regulation. In a review of the literature, John and Gross (2004) reported that healthier patterns of social functioning and greater well-being were linked to emotional reg- ulation for individuals who changed the way they thought about an emotional event as compared with individuals who changed only their emotional expressions. In addition, Ashforth and Humphrey (1993) suggested that employees who identify with their work are more likely to feel authentic even when conforming to role expec- tations, such as demands for emotional regulation. This is where leadership can play a role. In both lab and field studies, Bono and Judge (2003) demonstrated that managers’ transformational lead- ership behaviors can influence employees’ identification with their work. They also found that transformational leadership was a positive predictor of the extent to which employees felt that their work activities were self-congruent and consistent with their own interests and values. This suggests that individuals who report to transformational leaders, compared with those who do not, will be more likely to identify with their work and to change the way they think about role-required emotional regulation, thereby leading them to experience less stress and greater job satisfaction. Managers who engage in transformational leadership behaviors may also provide greater social support for their employees. Social support has been recognized as important to the experience of emotional management at work because providers of social sup- port (e.g., family, coworkers, and supervisors) are thought to enable employees to cope better with job stressors and increase their sense of control (Abraham, 1998; Zapf, 2002). Abraham reported that when employees experienced high social support, no association between emotional regulation and job satisfaction was found; however, under conditions of low social support, emotional labor negatively affected satisfaction. Transformational leaders are typically characterized as empathetic, and the behavioral dimen- sion of individualized consideration explicitly describes leaders as attending to and supporting the individual needs of followers. Such leaders may be able to help employees cope with emotional regulation in more effective and less psychologically draining ways by helping employees to understand why and how positive emotional expressions contribute to work goals (e.g., Shamir et al., 1993). Because they engender trust, transformational leaders are also uniquely positioned to assist employees in coping with emo- tional labor. Therefore, we expected the following: Hypothesis 4a: Supervisors’ transformational leadership be- haviors moderate the link between emotional regulation and job satisfaction, such that when supervisors engage in more transformational leadership behaviors, the negative associa- tion between emotional regulation and job satisfaction is weaker than when supervisors engage in less transformational leadership behaviors. Hypothesis 4b: Supervisors’ transformational leadership be- haviors moderate the link between emotional regulation and stress, such that when supervisors engage in more transfor- mational leadership behaviors, the positive association be- tween emotional regulation and stress is weaker than when supervisors engage in less transformational leadership behav- iors. Method Participants Participants were 57 employees of an ambulatory health care organization, randomly selected from nonmanagement employees participating in an organization-wide survey. As is common in health care organizations, participants were predominantly women (94%). They were, on average, 41 years old (SD ϭ 10 years). Eighty-six percent were Caucasian, 9% were African American, and 5% were of Asian origin. Participants’ average job tenure was 5.24 years. Participants worked in family practice clinics (n ϭ 14), administrative offices (n ϭ 10), and a billing office (n ϭ 33), and held a broad variety of jobs ranging from nurse, medical assistant, and lab technician in the family practice clinics; patient services support, case analyst, and account follow-up specialist in the billing office; and accountant, case manager, and human resource specialist in the administrative office. Procedure Participants provided two types of data: survey data and expe- rience sampling data. Paper surveys were used to gather general job satisfaction and stress data. Experience sampling data were obtained via a handheld computer that participants carried for 2 weeks and included momentary stress, job satisfaction, and affec- tive experiences. A third type of data—leadership behaviors of participants’ supervisors—was obtained from an organization- wide survey. As part of an organization-wide survey that we administered, all employees (N ϭ 365; 309 nonmanagement and 56 manage- ment) reported on the leadership behaviors of their direct su- pervisor. Surveys, completed in small group sessions by 253 (73%) nonmanagement and 56 (100%) management employees, were anonymous (employees recorded only the name of their supervisor) and were returned to us in unmarked, sealed enve- lopes. Only aggregate data were provided to organization ex- ecutives. One month following the organizational survey, we invited 73 employees to participate in the study. Participants 1359LEADERSHIP AND EMPLOYEE EMOTIONS
  • 4. were quasi-randomly selected, proportionately from each loca- tion, by entering the names of those who participated in the organization-wide survey into a spreadsheet, sorting by first name, and then choosing every other person on the list until the desired number of participants was achieved. Fifty-eight of the 73 employees contacted (79%) affirmed they would attend an information session, 6 (8%) were interested but could not make it to a scheduled information session, and 9 (12%) indicated they were not interested in participating in this research. At the orientation session, participants were informed that the organization’s management had endorsed their participation in the study and that they were allowed to respond to surveys during normal working hours. They were also informed that they would receive $25 as payment for participation. After obtaining this information, 57 of the 58 employees agreed to participate. After agreeing to enroll in the study, participants completed a survey, after which they were trained in the use of a personal digital assistant (PDA). For the experience sampling surveys, participants were signaled four times each day, Monday through Friday, for 2 weeks. The signal intervals were modified to accom- modate participants’ regular working hours (e.g., 7 a.m. to 3 p.m. or 8 a.m. to 4 p.m.). Participants were signaled once during each 2-hr segment of their 8-hr workday. Participants were given 10 min to respond to each signal, after which the survey was no longer available. They were told to respond to the surveys based on the emotions and attitudes they were experiencing “immediately be- fore the beep went off.” In addition, they were instructed to respond to as many signals as possible during the workday without compromising their job performance or service to patients or customers. Because we were not able to program the PDAs to stop signaling on weekends (i.e., Saturday and Sunday), we asked participants to leave the PDA either at work, in their car, or at home in a location where the signals would not be annoying. Data were collected for 10 working days.2 At each signal, participants were asked to report whether they were at work. As we were attempting to collect data based spe- cifically on emotional regulation in a social context, we next asked whether they were interacting with others when the signal sounded. Only surveys with yes responses to this item were in- cluded in this research. Next, they were asked to report on their level of stress and job satisfaction at that moment. Finally, they were asked to report on their affective experiences at that moment, including the extent to which they were feeling six emotions (happiness, enthusiasm, optimism, anxiety, irritation, and anger) and the extent to which they were faking the three positive emo- tions (happiness, enthusiasm, and optimism) and hiding the three negative emotions (anxiety, irritation, and anger). Hereinafter, we refer to experienced emotions as emotions experienced or felt emotions and to faking and hiding emotions as emotional regula- tion. The 12 affective items (i.e., 6 experienced emotion items and 6 emotional regulation items) appeared in random order for each survey. At the end of 2 weeks, participants attended a debriefing session, completed a second paper survey regarding general stress at work, returned their handheld computers, and were paid $25. At this time, they were also asked the name of their supervisor so that we could obtain reports on their supervisor’s leadership behaviors from the organizational survey. Table 1 displays the source and format of the data, along with the data collection timeline. Measures Supervisors’ leadership behaviors. The leadership behaviors of participants’ supervisors were measured using the 20-item Mul- tifactor Leadership Questionnaire (MLQ – Form 5x; Avolio, Bass, & Jung, 1995).3 The MLQ is the most frequently used measure of transformational leadership and has exhibited high reliability and convergent and discriminant validity (Avolio et al., 1995). Re- sponses were evaluated on a 5-point scale ranging from 1 (not at all) to 5 (frequently, if not always). Consistent with other research- ers whose interest is the general construct of transformational leadership behaviors, we combined items to form a single score for each supervisor (e.g., Lim & Ployhart, 2004). Job satisfaction. We assessed momentary job satisfaction by asking participants to rate their agreement with the statement “At this very moment, I am fairly satisfied with my job” on a 5-point scale (1 ϭ strongly disagree, 5 ϭ strongly agree). Ilies and Judge 2 The PDAs were programmed to signal twice during the evening hours as well. The optional evening surveys were used to collect pilot data for another project. Thirty-five individuals responded to evening surveys, which were not included in this study. 3 The MLQ, Form 5x (Copyright 1995 by Bernard Bass and Bruce Avolio) is used with permission of Mind Garden, 1690 Woodside Road, Suite 202, Redwood City, CA 94061. Table 1 Overview of Data Collection Variable Source Format Timeline Supervisors’ leadership behaviors Aggregate of employee responses to organizational survey Paper survey T1: One month prior to ESM General job satisfaction Participant Paper survey T2: Immediately prior to ESM Work status and interactions Participant Handheld computer T3: During ESM Momentary job satisfaction Participant Handheld computer T3: During ESM Momentary stress Participant Handheld computer T3: During ESM Affective experiences Participant Handheld computer T3: During ESM Overall stress Participant Paper survey T4: Immediately following ESM Note. The Experience sampling method (ESM) period consisted of a 2-week time frame during which participants responded to multiple surveys administered daily via a handheld computer. T1–T4 refer to the four distinct episodes of data collection. 1360 BONO, FOLDES, VINSON, AND MUROS
  • 5. (2002; Judge & Ilies, 2004) have found considerable variation in job satisfaction over the course of the workday, which our mo- mentary measure was designed to capture. We also obtained a measure of general job satisfaction, using five Brayfield–Rothe items (e.g., “Most days I am enthusiastic about my work”; see Bono & Judge, 2003; Brayfield & Rothe, 1951). Responses for these five items were on the same 5-point scale used for momen- tary job satisfaction. Work status and interactions. Immediately after being sig- naled by the PDA, participants were asked to report whether they were currently at work (yes or no). Next, they responded to the following question: “When the beep went off, were you interacting with any of the following?” 1 (supervisor), 2 (coworker), 3 (cus- tomer/client), 4 (family/friends), 5 (no one), 6 (other). Affective experiences. Although many taxonomies of affect and emotion exist in the literature, we were constrained by our method (i.e., surveys of only 1–2 min in length) to select a small number of affect terms in attempting to cover the full range of work-relevant affect and emotion. After reviewing the literature (Ortony & Turner, 1990; Russell & Feldman Barrett, 1999; Shaver, Schwartz, Kirson, & O’Connor, 1987) and conducting a pilot study of workers who reported how frequently they expressed each of 10 emotions at work, we selected three positive (happiness, enthusiasm, and optimism) and three negative (anxiety, anger, and irritation) work-relevant emotions. We excluded some basic emo- tions (e.g., fear and love) and low-activation affect terms (e.g., contentment and calm), deeming them of low relevance to our study. Participants reported the degree to which they felt the six selected emotions at each signal. Because Glomb and Tews (2004) illustrated the importance of distinguishing between felt emotions and the act of emotional regulation, we also asked participants about faking (i.e., expressing an emotion they did not feel) the three positive emotions (happi- ness, enthusiasm, and optimism) and hiding (i.e., feeling an emo- tion they were not expressing) the three negative emotions (anxi- ety, irritation, and anger). Responses for these items were evaluated on a 7-point scale (1 ϭ none at all, 7 ϭ an intense amount) for feeling, hiding, and faking. Stress. We assessed momentary stress by asking participants to respond to the statement “At this very moment, I am experi- encing stress.” This measure was intended to capture variability in stress throughout the day. We also obtained an overall, global measure of stress, using Motowidlo, Packard, and Manning’s (1986) four-item measure. Responses for both momentary and overall stress items were on the same 5-point scale used for job satisfaction. Data Analyses Prior to conducting our analyses of interest, we examined sev- eral properties of our data. We excluded data from 3 participants who had responded to fewer than three signals over the course of the 2 weeks (1 because of job termination, 1 because of PDA failure, and 1 for an unknown reason). Therefore, the total possible number of responses was approximately 2,160 (4 responses per day ϫ 10 workdays ϫ 54 participants ϭ 2,160); however, because of scheduling the drop-off and pick-up of PDAs, a few participants responded to surveys on Days 11 and 12, raising the total possible to 2,266). Participants responded to 1,983 signals (approximately 88% of the total possible), with an average of 37 responses per participant (ranging from 11 to 42). We excluded data from re- sponses when participants were not at work (n ϭ 287 episodes) and for interactions with family or friends (n ϭ 52 episodes), resulting in 1,653 work-related responses. Our goal was to exam- ine emotional regulation in a social context. However, in many cases participants were alone when the signal sounded (n ϭ 764; 48% of work-related responses).4 Thus, our final data set consisted of 889 responses (52% of total at-work responses; average of 15 per participant) from employees who were at work and involved in a work-related interaction at the time of the signal. To form a single leadership score for the supervisor of each participant, we aggregated the leadership survey responses of all employees who completed a survey for the target supervisor (av- erage n ϭ 5 reports per supervisor). Aggregating leadership reports across followers was deemed justifiable in these data by a signif- icant ICC-1 value of .29 (p Ͻ .01) and an ICC-2 value of .72. The ICC-2 value can be interpreted as the reliability of the aggregated measure of leadership. An average rwg ϭ .84 across groups (James, Demaree, & Wolf, 1984; assuming a slight negative skew in the data [Bono & Judge, 2003]) further supports aggregation. We also created several composite emotion variables. The three positive (enthusiasm, happiness, and optimism) and three negative (anger, irritation, and anxiety) emotions were highly correlated within individuals for both felt emotions and emotional regulation (average r ϭ .87, .77, .93, and .82, for positive experienced, negative experienced, positive faked, and negative hidden, respec- tively). Therefore, we computed four composite emotion variables: positive emotions experienced, negative emotions experienced, positive emotions faked, and negative emotions hidden. Principal- components analysis provided support for creating these compos- ites, as we found that a single factor explained most of the variance (ranging from 70%–82%) in each set of related emotions. We also found that the two types of emotional regulation were highly related (i.e., faking positive emotions and hiding negative emo- tions tended to co-occur; r ϭ .88, p Ͻ .01). Because we were interested in possible differential effects of the two types of emo- tional regulation, we included faking positive and hiding negative in our analyses. However, we also formed a total emotional reg- ulation score by averaging the two types of emotional regulation. Our hypotheses address two levels of analysis. Within-subject analyses must be used to link emotional regulation within an individual with momentary variation in stress and job satisfaction (Hypothesis 3). In these analyses, data from each individual are centered around that individual’s mean score, effectively control- ling for mean differences between individuals in emotional regu- lation, job satisfaction, and stress. Cross-level (between- and within-subjects) analyses are used to test our leadership hypothe- ses (Hypotheses 1, 2, and 4), which posit associations between supervisory behavior and employees’ emotional experiences. Therefore, we analyzed our data using multilevel modeling tech- niques (i.e., HLM 5; Raudenbush & Bryk, 2002). 4 A comparison of affective experiences when interacting with others versus alone revealed no significant differences in experienced positive and negative emotions or in emotional regulation. 1361LEADERSHIP AND EMPLOYEE EMOTIONS
  • 6. Results Means, standard deviations, and correlations are presented in Table 2, revealing that participants generally experienced more positive emotions than negative emotions at work and tended to hide negative emotions only slightly more than they faked positive emotions (mean hide negative ϭ 1.67, and mean fake positive ϭ 1.50). Mean levels of job satisfaction and stress were similar in the momentary, PDA data (Level 1) and in the global, overall measure (Level 2). We note a high correlation between negative mood and emotional regulation (r ϭ .78, .92, and .88, all ps Ͻ .01, for faking positive, hiding negative, and total emotional regulation, respec- tively). These high correlations are consistent with the notion that positive emotions are valued (and negative emotions devalued) in organizations, leading employees to suppress negative emotions. Leaders and Employee Emotions Hypotheses 1 and 2 address the role of supervisors in employ- ees’ emotional experiences. We first examined the link between employees’ affective experiences and interaction partner (e.g., supervisor vs. customer–client). Analysis of variance results (see Table 3) revealed significant differences in affective experiences based on interaction partner. Specifically, participants reported the most positive emotions when interacting with their coworkers (mean positive emotions experienced ϭ 3.75) and fewest positive emotions when interacting with their supervisors (mean positive emotions experienced ϭ 3.05). This was true for each of the individual positive emotions, which we do not report in Table 3, and the positive emotion composite, F(3, 886) ϭ 5.74, p Ͻ .01. Thus, Hypothesis 1 was supported for positive emotions; however, interaction partner was significantly related neither to the experi- ence of negative emotions nor to employees’ emotional regulation. Next, we examined the direct effects of supervisors’ transfor- mational leadership behaviors on employees’ momentary emo- tional experiences, testing the theory that transformational leaders influence employee emotions. In this analysis we linked the su- pervisor’s typical leadership behaviors to employees’ emotions across all interactions, including those with customers and cowork- ers. Table 4 presents coefficients linking supervisors’ leadership behaviors to employees’ experienced emotions and emotional reg- ulation. As hypothesized (Hypothesis 2), employees who work for supervisors rated high on transformational leadership reported experiencing more positive emotions throughout the course of their workday (␥ ϭ .54, p Ͻ .01). This held true for each of the individual positive emotions (results not reported in Table 4), as well as the positive emotions composite. Leadership behaviors were not linked to participants’ experience of negative emotions or to reports of emotional regulation. Leaders and Employee Emotional Regulation Emotional regulation, job satisfaction, and stress. In addition to main effects of supervisors’ leadership behaviors on employees’ affective experiences, we also hypothesized a moderating effect for transformational leadership. We examined the data to deter- mine whether emotional regulation covaries with stress and job satisfaction within individuals and over time. Results in Table 5 (upper portion: momentary data) addressed the link between mo- mentary emotional regulation, job satisfaction, and stress. Results revealed a significant, negative association between both types of emotional regulation and job satisfaction (␥ ϭ –.18 and –.18, p Ͻ .01, for hiding negative and faking positive, respectively) and a significant positive link between both types of emotional regula- tion and stress (␥ ϭ .40 and .35, p Ͻ .01, for hiding negative and faking positive, respectively). These results suggest that regardless of how often certain individuals regulate their emotions, individual episodes of emotional regulation are associated with increased stress (above the mean for a given individual) and decreased job satisfaction (below the mean for a given individual). This was true for both faking positive and hiding negative emotions, supporting Hypotheses 3a and 3b. A unique advantage of experience sampling research is that we can examine the link between emotional regulation at one point in time (t) and stress or job satisfaction for that individual at the next random signal (t ϩ 1), allowing us to draw stronger causal infer- ences and to determine how long these effects last. Results in Table 5 (lower portion: lagged momentary data) showed that the association between emotional regulation and stress extended over time. Although weaker than the concurrent effects, results indi- cated a significant positive association between emotional regula- tion at one time point and stress approximately 2 hr later. No Table 2 Means, Standard Deviations, and Intercorrelations Between Level 2 Variables and Aggregate of Level 1 Variables Scale M SD 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 12 1. Positive affect 3.78 .52 .82 2. Negative affect 1.66 .61 Ϫ.13 .91 3. Transformational leadership 3.24 .55 .24 Ϫ.19 .93 4. ESM job satisfaction 3.73 .59 .28* Ϫ.17 .19 — 5. ESM job stress 2.22 .67 .05 .31* .12 Ϫ.19 — 6. Feel positive emotions 3.65 .98 .31* Ϫ.22 .31* .56** Ϫ.24 .95 7. Feel negative emotions 1.90 .81 .15 .38** .03 Ϫ.38** .66** Ϫ.26 .90 8. Fake positive emotions 1.50 .67 .09 .24 .07 Ϫ.28* .51** Ϫ.06 .78** .97 9. Hide negative emotions 1.67 .81 .15 .24 .12 Ϫ.37** .58** Ϫ.20 .92** .88** .92 10. Total emotional regulation 1.59 .71 .13 .25 .10 Ϫ.34** .57** Ϫ.14 .88** .96** .98** — 11. General job satisfaction 3.74 .59 .39** Ϫ.24 .29* .74** Ϫ.14 .43** Ϫ.27** Ϫ.21 Ϫ.21 Ϫ.22 .85 — 12. General stress 2.84 .82 Ϫ.20 .20 Ϫ.25 Ϫ.35** .36** Ϫ.28* .31* .26 .28* .28* Ϫ.38** .79 Note. N ϭ 54. Coefficient alphas are presented on the main diagonal. ESM ϭ Experience sampling method. * p Ͻ .05. ** p Ͻ .01. 1362 BONO, FOLDES, VINSON, AND MUROS
  • 7. significant effects for emotional regulation (t) on stress were found after 4 hr (t ϩ 2), and sample sizes were too small to examine t ϩ 3 data (i.e., average 6-hr time lag). Although our data are corre- lational and preclude firm causal conclusions, these results lend support to our proposed causal ordering of the variables (e.g., emotional regulation 3 stress), as we also tested the reverse ordering (e.g., stress [t] 3 emotional regulation [t ϩ 1]) and found no significant effects. The effects of emotional regulation on job satisfaction appear to be more fleeting than the effects of stress, as emotional regulation did not predict job satisfaction after 2 hr. Leadership as buffer. We suggested that leadership behaviors would buffer employees from the negative effects of emotional regulation. Results in Table 6 support Hypothesis 4a, revealing a significant moderating effect for transformational leadership (␥ ϭ .21, p Ͻ .01) on the association between emotional regulation and job satisfaction. Figure 2 shows that when transformational lead- ership behaviors were low (1 standard deviation below the mean), episodes of emotional regulation were associated with decreased job satisfaction. However, when transformational leadership be- haviors were high (1 standard deviation above the mean), there was little or no association between emotional regulation and job satisfaction. A different picture emerged when we examined stress (Hypothesis 4b). Contrary to our expectations, supervisors’ trans- formational leadership behaviors did not protect employees from the stress associated with emotional regulation (see Table 6).5 Discussion According to Brief and Weiss (2002), “the organizational liter- ature is populated with many more ideas about the leader’s role in the production of moods and emotions than it is with relevant data” (p. 289). In this study we focused on the role of managers in producing employee emotions and in buffering them from the effects of emotional regulation. With respect to the former, our results revealed that employees experience less optimism, happi- ness, and enthusiasm when they interact with supervisors than when they interact with customers, clients, and coworkers. We also found that employees who report to supervisors who engage in transformational leadership behaviors, compared with those who do not, experience more optimism, happiness, and enthusiasm throughout the day, including during their interactions with cus- tomers and coworkers. With respect to the latter, our results indicate that when employees regulate their emotions at work, they experience increased stress and decreased job satisfaction. Further- 5 Several participants in our sample reported to the same supervisor, creating dependencies in the leadership data that were not addressed in our hierarchical linear modeling analyses and may have potentially produced biased tests of significance. Thus, we repeated our moderator analyses involving leadership (see Table 6) using only 1 participant per leader. This reduced our sample size (from 54 to 27) but eliminated the dependencies. All significant effects reported in Table 6 remained significant in this subsample. Table 3 Analysis of Variance for Felt and Regulated Emotions by Interaction Partner Emotion Interaction partner M SD F % total variance explained by interaction partner Feel positive Supervisor 3.05 1.44 5.74** 36 Coworker 3.75 1.25 Customer-client 3.53 1.58 Other 3.54 1.47 Feel negative Supervisor 2.08 1.35 1.01 4 Coworker 1.83 1.13 Customer-client 1.90 1.14 Other 1.84 1.19 Fake positive Supervisor 1.38 0.91 0.30 6 Coworker 1.47 0.88 Customer-client 1.46 0.85 Other 1.42 0.86 Hide negative Supervisor 1.50 1.01 0.61 3 Coworker 1.62 1.04 Customer-client 1.67 1.05 Other 1.56 0.99 Note. N ϭ 889. dfs ϭ 3, 886. The frequency of interactions was as follows: coworker, 55%; customer-client, 23%; supervisor, 8%; other (which included groups of individuals), 14%. ** p Ͻ .01. Table 4 Hierarchical Linear Modeling Estimates of the Associations Between Supervisors’ Leadership Behaviors and Participants’ Affective Experiences Affective experiences Transformational leadership behavior (␥01) Coefficient SE t Feeling positive .54** .18 2.94 Feeling negative .03 .22 0.15 Faking positive .11 .16 0.73 Hiding negative .18 .20 0.94 Total emotional regulation .14 .17 0.85 Note. N ϭ 54 participants; N ϭ 889 observations. Coefficients for feeling positive, feeling negative, faking positive, hiding negative, and total emo- tional regulation were estimated independently. Approximate df ϭ 51. ** p Ͻ .01. 1363LEADERSHIP AND EMPLOYEE EMOTIONS
  • 8. more, although the effects of emotional regulation on job satisfac- tion appear to be short-lived, the effects of emotional regulation on stress are more long lasting (e.g., 2 hr). Managers who use trans- formational leadership behaviors buffer employees who regulate their emotions from decreased job satisfaction, but not from in- creased stress. Given these results, two important questions re- main: (a) How do these results contribute to our understanding of “the leader’s role in the production of moods and emotions” (Brief & Weiss, 2002, p. 289)?, and (b) How do these results contribute to our understanding of the role of managers in workplace emo- tional regulation? With respect to leadership and emotions, results of this study demonstrate the powerful role that managers and supervisors have on employee emotions. Transformational leadership theory sug- gests emotional links between leaders and followers but offers little explanation of how such emotional links contribute to lead- ership effectiveness. Based on theories of primitive emotional contagion, lab studies (e.g., Barsade, 2002; Sy et al., 2005) have documented the fleeting effects of a leader’s positive mood on the mood of workers. Given evidence in this study that managers’ leadership behaviors have an ongoing influence on employee optimism and enthusiasm, it may be time to look beyond primitive emotional contagion as the primary process by which leaders influence follower emotions and organizational outcomes. Our results suggest that managers’ transformational leadership behav- iors may have broad, deep, and long-lasting effects on individual employees and the organization as a whole. Beyond their imme- diate effects on employee mood, the positive emotions elicited by transformational leaders have the potential to influence the overall work climate and customer satisfaction. Employees who are opti- mistic are more likely to invest the time to help others (Lee & Allen, 2002) and can be expected to persist in work tasks – even in the face of difficulty (Seligman & Schulman, 1986). In the same vein, research by Pugh (2001) linked positive emotional displays by employees to customer evaluations of service quality. Our study also contributes to theory and existing research on emotional regulation. What we know from the existing literature is that individuals who tend to regulate their emotions are also less happy with their jobs and more stressed, although the link with job satisfaction varies across studies. Our results show that discrete episodes of emotional regulation are also associated with de- creased job satisfaction and increased stress, even for individuals who do not frequently regulate their emotions. This is an important finding given that emotional regulation theory is focused on the negative effects of sustained and frequent emotional regulation. However, it is also important to note that we did not find any time-lagged effects of emotional regulation on job satisfaction. Thus, the effects of discrete episodes of emotional regulation on Table 5 Hierarchical Linear Modeling Analysis Linking Emotional Regulation to Stress and Job Satisfaction Variable Job satisfaction Stress Coefficient SE t Coefficient SE t Momentary dataa Intercept (␥00) 3.73** 0.08 46.47 2.21** 0.09 24.89 Hiding negative emotions (␥10) Ϫ.18** .04 Ϫ4.94 .40** .07 5.92 Faking positive emotions (␥10) Ϫ.18** .05 Ϫ3.60 .35** .06 5.78 Total emotional regulation (␥10) Ϫ.23** .05 Ϫ4.74 .49** .07 6.88 Lagged momentary datab Intercept (␥00) 3.76** 0.10 39.08 2.25** 0.12 19.33 Hiding negative emotions (␥10) Ϫ.05 .04 Ϫ1.24 .20** .08 2.63 Faking positive emotions (␥10) Ϫ.00 .05 Ϫ0.29 .19 .10 1.85 Total emotional regulation (␥10) Ϫ.04 .05 Ϫ0.95 .25* .10 2.41 Note. Coefficients for hiding negative, faking positive, and total emotional regulation were estimated independently. For lagged data, we used emotional regulation at t and stress and job satisfaction at t ϩ 1, where t represents a time point and t ϩ 1 represents the time of the next random signal (2 hr later on average). a n ϭ 821; approximate df ϭ 819. b n ϭ 284; approximate df ϭ 281. * p Ͻ .05. ** p Ͻ .01. Table 6 Hierarchical Linear Modeling Estimates of the Moderating Effect of Transformational Leadership Variable Hiding negative emotion Faking positive emotion Total emotion regulation Coefficient SE t Coefficient SE t Coefficient SE t Moderator effects—job satisfaction Transformational leadership (␥13) .12** .06 2.01 .24** .07 3.21 .21** .07 2.87 Moderator effects—stress Transformational leadership (␥13) .01** .10 0.14 .02** .11 0.14 .01** .10 0.09 Note. N ϭ 54 participants; N ϭ 889 observations. Coefficients for hiding negative, faking positive, and total emotional regulation were estimated independently. Approximate df ϭ 51. ** p Ͻ .01. 1364 BONO, FOLDES, VINSON, AND MUROS
  • 9. job satisfaction appear to be very short-lived (less than 2 hr). This is in contrast to the effects of emotional regulation on stress, which remained significant 2 hr later. Hochschild’s (1983) seminal re- search suggested that individuals who must frequently regulate their emotions on the job will experience depersonalization and stress. Although our results do not speak to cumulative effects over time, they do suggest that even for individuals who rarely regulate their emotions, doing so on a single occasion is associated with increased stress, which may last for several hours. An important topic for future research is to examine the long-term, sustained effects of emotional regulation on employee health. In organizational research on emotional regulation, it is common to do as we did and consider both job satisfaction and stress as important outcomes of emotional regulation; however, our results suggest that different processes may underlie the effects of emo- tional regulation on these two outcomes. We found that emotional regulation was associated with stress 2 hr later, but the effects of emotional regulation on job satisfaction were more fleeting (i.e., we found no time-lagged effects). We also found that managerial behavior buffered employees from the effects of emotional regu- lation on job satisfaction but not on stress, suggesting that the processes linking emotional regulation with job satisfaction and stress may differ. The link between emotional regulation and job satisfaction may be cognitive in nature. One way that managers provide support for employees is by enhancing internalized self- regulation and drawing attention to outcomes associated with emotional regulation (e.g., greater service for customers). There- fore, when employees regulate their emotions, they may con- sciously decide that it is worth doing because it helps them to perform better in a job they care about and identify with. Thus, during emotional regulation they may feel momentarily unhappy with their job because it demands inauthentic emotional expres- sions, but these effects are fleeting (and do not occur for employ- ees who report to transformational managers) because employees understand how emotional regulation contributes to their job. In contrast, the effects of emotional regulation on stress may be, at least in part, physiological. Empirical evidence shows that emotional regulation leads to cardiac arousal (see Gross & Lev- enson, 1993), which is linked to perceptions of stress (see Blas- covich & Tomaka, 1996). It may be that the association between emotional regulation and stress is stronger, lasts longer, and is less amenable to reduction through managerial support because it has a physiological basis. Clearly, we can only speculate about the processes by which emotional regulation affects stress and job satisfaction, but this is an important area for future research. If our theoretical propositions are correct, managers may be able to help employees more by reducing or eliminating the need for emotional regulation (perhaps by encouraging and supporting authenticity) rather than attempting to buffer them from the negative effects of emotional regulation after it occurs. Our study highlights the need for follow-up research. First, it is important to establish causality by manipulating both emotional regulation and leadership behavior in the lab. Some work has been done in this regard by Bono and Vey (2007), who linked emotional regulation to increased heart rate and stress. Controlled laboratory settings would also be useful in assessing the longevity of the effects of emotional regulation episodes and sustained emotional regulation. Second, it is important to determine the direct cause of emotional regulation. Although our theoretical model suggests that emotional regulation causes stress, our methods did not allow us to establish causality. Indeed, it is possible that certain events in the workplace may cause both emotional regulation and stress. For example, in cases in which workers are asked to be pleasant and enthusiastic with customers, a rude customer may be the source of emotional regulation, which then leads to stress. Alternatively, a rude customer may be the source of both emotional regulation and stress, creating the illusion of a causal association between them. It is also plausible that an unpleasant interaction with a patient or customer would have both direct effects on stress and indirect effects, through emotional regulation. One possible next step Figure 2. The moderating effect of transformational leadership on the relationship between hiding negative emotions and faking positive emotions and job satisfaction. Job satisfaction scores are based on responses to the question “At this very moment, I am fairly satisfied with my job,” rated on a 5-point scale (1 ϭ strongly disagree, 5 ϭ strongly agree). 1365LEADERSHIP AND EMPLOYEE EMOTIONS
  • 10. would be to use experience sampling methods to catalogue events that lead to emotional regulation in the natural work environment and determine how these events affect both emotions and stress. Our study is unique in investigating leadership as an important factor in employees’ experienced emotions and in the emotional regulation process; it has some methodological strengths and lim- itations worth noting. Concerning the former, we used a longitu- dinal design in conjunction with an experience sampling method- ology. In doing so, we were able to collect daily emotional regulation data across multiple locations and jobs as emotional regulation occurred. Thus, we avoided retrospective and recall biases that occur in reporting emotional experiences. Our within- person design also allowed us to confidently link specific events of emotional regulation to stress and job satisfaction, free from the biasing effects of individual differences. Furthermore, our mea- sures of leadership behavior and employee emotional experiences were relatively independent of each other. Our study also ad- dressed the call for more research to be conducted in health care settings (Ehler, Major, & Fletcher, 2003). Despite these strengths, our study also has some limitations. First, although our participants held a variety of jobs, we did not have the sample size to examine subgroups of employees in service versus nonservice support jobs. Second, our sample was drawn from a single organization. Thus, it is possible that charac- teristics of the work climate, shared by all participants, influenced the experience of both positive and negative emotions and overall levels of emotional regulation. If the entire work climate was particularly supportive or abusive, our results may under- or over- estimate the moderating effects of leadership behaviors. Third, our sample—though representative of the larger organization and the health care industry—was largely female, and we cannot be certain that the associations we found would hold for men. Fourth, it is possible that the PDA signals may have annoyed participants, causing increased irritation and stress. If this is true, the effects were small because mean stress from PDA data was not higher than global reports of stress. Considered as a whole, our results highlight the importance of supervisors in employees’ emotional experiences. Our results are provocative in that they suggest the possibility of different theo- retical explanations (and therefore the need for different manage- rial interventions) for the effects of emotional regulation on stress and job satisfaction. Our study also extends existing theory on leader–follower emotional links by looking beyond the primitive emotional contagion process. We hope this study will motivate future longitudinal research in naturalistic settings along with experimental research in the laboratory, including both psycholog- ical and physiological measures of employee moods, emotional regulation, and stress over time. References Abraham, R. (1998). Emotional dissonance in organizations: Antecedents, consequences, and moderators. Genetic, Social, and General Psychology Monographs, 124, 229–246. Ashforth, B. E., & Humphrey, R. H. (1993). Emotional labor in service roles: The influence of identity. Academy of Management Review, 18, 88–115. Avolio, B. J., Bass, B. M., & Jung, D. I. (1995). Multifactor Leadership Questionnaire technical report. Redwood City, CA: Mind Garden. Barsade, S. G. (2002). The ripple effects: Emotional contagion and its influence on group behavior. Administrative Science Quarterly, 47, 644–675. Bass, B. M. (1985). Leadership and performance beyond expectations. New York: Free Press. Blascovich, J., & Tomaka, J. (1996). The biopsychosocial model of arousal regulation. Advances in Experimental Social Psychology, 28, 1–51. Bono, J. E., & Ilies, R. (2006). Charisma, positive emotions, and mood contagion. Leadership Quarterly, 17, 317–334. Bono, J. E., & Judge, T. A. (2003). Self concordance at work: Toward understanding the motivational effects of transformational leaders. Academy of Management Journal, 46, 554–571. Bono, J. E., & Vey, M. A. (2007). Personality and emotional performance: Extraversion, neuroticism, and self-monitoring. Journal of Occupational Health Psychology, 12, 177–192. Brayfield, A. H., & Rothe, H. F. (1951). An index of job satisfaction. Journal of Applied Psychology, 35, 307–311. Brief, A. P., & Weiss, H. M. (2002). Organizational behavior: Affect in the workplace. Annual Review of Psychology, 53, 279–307. Conger, J. A., & Kanungo, R. N. (1998). Charismatic leadership in organizations. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage. Danna, K., & Griffin, R. W. (1999). Health and well-being in the work- place: A review and synthesis of the literature. Journal of Management, 25, 357–384. Diefendorff, J. M., & Richard, E. M. (2003). Antecedents and conse- quences of emotional display rule perceptions. Journal of Applied Psy- chology, 88, 284–294. Diener, E., & Emmons, R. (1984). The independence of positive and negative affect. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 47, 1105–1117. Ehler, M. L., Major, D. A., & Fletcher, T. D. (2003). Applying I-O to medicine: Making the case that it can be done and that it should be done. The Industrial–Organizational Psychologist, 41, 50–54. Erez, A. I., & Isen, A. M. (2002). The influence of positive affect on the components of expectancy motivation. Journal of Applied Psychology, 87, 1055–1067. Fitness, J. (2000). Anger in the workplace: An emotion script approach to anger episodes between workers and their superiors, coworkers, and subordinates. Journal of Organizational Behavior, 21, 147–162. George, J. M., & Zhou, J. (2001). When Openness to Experience and Conscientiousness are related to creative behavior: An interactional approach. Journal of Applied Psychology, 86, 513–524. George, J. M., & Zhou, J. (2002). Understanding when bad moods foster creativity and good ones don’t: The role of context and clarity of feelings. Journal of Applied Psychology, 87, 687–697. Glaso, L., & Einarsen, S. (2006). Experienced affects in leader–subordinate relationships. Scandinavian Journal of Management, 22, 49–73. Glomb, T. M., & Tews, M. J. (2004). Emotional labor: A conceptualization and scale development. Journal of Vocational Behavior, 64, 1–23. Gross, J. J. (1998). Antecedent- and response-focused emotion regulation: Divergent consequences for experience, expression, and physiology. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 74, 224–237. Gross, J. J., & Levenson, R. W. (1993). Emotional suppression: Physiol- ogy, self-report, and expressive behavior. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 64, 970–986. Gross, J. J., & Levenson, R. W. (1997). Hiding feelings: The acute effects of inhibiting negative and positive emotion. Journal of Abnormal Psy- chology, 106, 95–103. Gross, J. J., Richards, J. M., & John, O. P. (2006). Emotion regulation in everyday life. In D. K. Snyder, J. A. Simpson, & J. N. Hughes (Eds.), Emotion regulation in couples and families: Pathways to dysfunction and health (pp. 13–39). Washington, DC: American Psychological As- sociation. Hochschild, A. R. (1979). Emotion work, feeling rules, and social struc- ture. American Journal of Sociology, 85, 551–575. 1366 BONO, FOLDES, VINSON, AND MUROS
  • 11. Hochschild, A. R. (1983). The managed heart. Berkeley: University of California Press. House, R. J. (1977). A 1976 theory of charismatic leadership. In J. G. Hunt & L. L. Larsen (Eds.), Leadership: The cutting edge. Carbondale: Southern Illinois University Press. Ilies, R., & Judge, T. A. (2002). Understanding the dynamic relationships among personality, mood, and job satisfaction: A field experience sam- pling study. Organizational Behavior & Human Decision Processes, 89, 1119–1139. James, L. R., Demaree, R. G., & Wolf, G. (1984). Estimating within-group reliability with and without response bias. Journal of Applied Psychol- ogy, 69, 85–98. John, O. P., & Gross, J. J. (2004). Healthy and unhealthy emotional regulation: Personality processes, individual differences and life span development. Journal of Personality, 72, 1301–1333. Judge, T. A., & Ilies, R. (2004). Affect and job satisfaction: A study of their relationship at work and at home. Journal of Applied Psychology, 89, 661–673. Law, K. S., Wong, C., & Song, L. (2004). The construct and criterion validity of emotional intelligence and its potential utility for manage- ment studies. Journal of Applied Psychology, 89, 483–496. Lee, K., & Allen, N. J. (2002). Organizational citizenship behavior and workplace deviance: The role of affect and cognitions. Journal of Applied Psychology, 87, 131–142. Lim, B. C., & Ployhart, R. E. (2004). Transformational leadership: Rela- tions to the five-factor model and team performance in typical and maximum contexts. Journal of Applied Psychology, 89, 610–621. Little, B. R. (2000). Free traits and personal contexts: Expanding a social ecological model of well-being. In W. B. Walsh & K. H. Craik (Eds.), Person–environment psychology: New directions and perspectives (2nd ed., pp. 87–116). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum. McColl-Kennedy, J. R., & Anderson, R. D. (2002). Impact of leadership style and emotions on subordinate performance. Leadership Quarterly, 13, 545–559. Miner, A., Glomb, T. M., & Hulin, C. (2005). Experience sampling mood and its correlates at work. Journal of Occupational and Organizational Psychology, 78, 171–193. Motowidlo, S. J., Packard, J. S., & Manning, M. R. (1986). Occupational stress: Its causes and consequences for job performance. Journal of Applied Psychology, 71, 618–629. Ortony, A., & Turner, T. (1990). What’s basic about basic emotions? Psychological Review, 97, 313–331. Pugh, S. D. (2001). Service with a smile: Emotional contagion in the service encounter. Academy of Management Journal, 44, 1018–1027. Pugliesi, K. (1999). The consequences of emotional labor: Effects on work stress, job satisfaction, and well-being. Motivation and Emotion, 23, 125–154. Raudenbush, S. W., & Bryk, A. S. (2002). Hierarchical linear models: Applications and data analysis methods. London: Sage. Russell, J. A., & Feldman Barrett, L. (1999). Core affect, prototypical emotional episodes, and other things called emotion: Dissecting the elephant. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 76, 805–819. Ryan, R. M., & Deci, E. L. (2000). Self-determination theory and the facilitation of intrinsic motivation, social development, and well-being. American Psychologist, 55, 68–78. Schaubroeck, J., & Jones, J. R. (2000). Antecedents of workplace emo- tional labor dimensions and moderators of their effects on physical symptoms. Journal of Organizational Behavior, 21, 163–183. Seligman, M. E., & Schulman, P. (1986). Explanatory style as a predictor of productivity and quitting among life insurance sales agents. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 50, 832–838. Shamir, B., House, R. J., & Arthur, M. B. (1993). The motivational effects of charismatic leadership: A self-concept based theory. Organization Science, 4, 577–594. Shaver, P., Schwartz, J., Kirson, D., & O’Connor, C. (1987). Emotion knowledge: Further exploration of a prototype approach. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 52, 1061–1086. Sheldon, K. M., & Elliott, A. J. (1999). Goal striving, need satisfaction and longitudinal well-being: The self-concordance model. Journal of Per- sonality and Social Psychology, 76, 482–497. Sy, T., Coˆte´, S., & Saavedra, R. (2005). The contagious leader: Impact of the leader’s mood on the mood of group members, group affective tone, and group processes. Journal of Applied Psychology, 90, 205–305. Tepper, B. J. (2000). Consequences of abusive supervision. Academy of Management Journal, 43, 178–190. Watson, D., & Clark, L. A. (1997). Measurement and mismeasurement of mood: Recurrent and emergent issues. Journal of Personality Assess- ment, 68, 267–296. Weiss, H. M., & Cropanzano, R. (1996). Affective events theory: A theoretical discussion of the structure, causes and consequences of affective experiences at work. In B. M. Staw & L. L. Cummings (Eds.), Research in organizational behavior: An annual series of analytical essays and critical reviews (pp. 1–74). Greenwich, CT: JAI Press. Zapf, D. (2002). Emotion work and psychological well-being: A review of the literature and some conceptual considerations. Human Resource Management Review, 12, 237–268. Received February 9, 2005 Revision received December 15, 2006 Accepted December 18, 2006 Ⅲ 1367LEADERSHIP AND EMPLOYEE EMOTIONS