SlideShare a Scribd company logo
1 of 121
This article was downloaded by: [134.88.77.64] On: 30 April
2018, At: 07:05
Publisher: Institute for Operations Research and the
Management Sciences (INFORMS)
INFORMS is located in Maryland, USA
Organization Science
Publication details, including instructions for authors and
subscription information:
http://pubsonline.informs.org
The Effects of Communication Networks and Turnover on
Transactive Memory and Group Performance
Linda Argote, Brandy L. Aven, Jonathan Kush
To cite this article:
Linda Argote, Brandy L. Aven, Jonathan Kush (2018) The
Effects of Communication Networks and Turnover on
Transactive
Memory and Group Performance. Organization Science
29(2):191-206. https://doi.org/10.1287/orsc.2017.1176
Full terms and conditions of use:
http://pubsonline.informs.org/page/terms-and-conditions
This article may be used only for the purposes of research,
teaching, and/or private study. Commercial use
or systematic downloading (by robots or other automatic
processes) is prohibited without explicit Publisher
approval, unless otherwise noted. For more information, contact
[email protected]
The Publisher does not warrant or guarantee the article’s
accuracy, completeness, merchantability, fitness
for a particular purpose, or non-infringement. Descriptions of,
or references to, products or publications, or
inclusion of an advertisement in this article, neither constitutes
nor implies a guarantee, endorsement, or
support of claims made of that product, publication, or service.
Copyright © 2018, INFORMS
Please scroll down for article—it is on subsequent pages
INFORMS is the largest professional society in the world for
professionals in the fields of operations research, management
science, and analytics.
For more information on INFORMS, its publications,
membership, or meetings visit http://www.informs.org
http://pubsonline.informs.org
https://doi.org/10.1287/orsc.2017.1176
http://pubsonline.informs.org/page/terms-and-conditions
http://www.informs.org
ORGANIZATION SCIENCE
Vol. 29, No. 2, March–April 2018, pp. 191–206
http://pubsonline.informs.org/journal/orsc/ ISSN 1047-7039
(print), ISSN 1526-5455 (online)
The Effects of Communication Networks and Turnover on
Transactive Memory and Group Performance
Linda Argote,a Brandy L. Aven,a Jonathan Kushb
a Tepper School of Business, Carnegie Mellon University,
Pittsburgh, Pennsylvania 15213; b Charlton College of
Business,
University of Massachusetts Dartmouth, North Dartmouth,
Massachusetts 02747
Contact: [email protected], http://orcid.org/0000-0002-4149-
6968 (LA); [email protected], http://orcid.org/0000-0001-7086-
5876 (BLA);
[email protected], http://orcid.org/0000-0003-4872-8650 (JK)
Received: May 28, 2015
Revised: March 8, 2016; December 11, 2016;
June 25, 2017; August 17, 2017
Accepted: September 4, 2017
Published Online in Articles in Advance:
March 30, 2018
https://doi.org/10.1287/orsc.2017.1176
Copyright: © 2018 INFORMS
Abstract. We theorize that the effect of membership turnover on
group processes and
performance depends on a group’s communication network. We
describe two mecha-
nisms through which communication networks affect group
performance: (1) the number
of direct communication paths and (2) the clarity of the
coordination logic. These mech-
anisms map onto two network dimensions: density, which
affects a group’s behavior
through the number of available communication paths, and
centralization, which affects a
group’s behavior through the clarity of the coordination logic.
We empirically analyze the
effects of turnover on the performance of fully connected all-
channel networks and hub-
and-spoke or wheel networks in an experiment of 109 four-
person groups performing two
collaborative problem-solving tasks. The greater number of
direct communication paths
enabled fully connected groups with stable membership to
develop stronger transactive
memory systems (TMSs) and perform better than fully
connected groups that experienced
turnover. By contrast, the clear coordination logic of perfectly
centralized groups that expe-
rienced turnover facilitated more frequent dyadic
communication, which enabled them to
strengthen their TMSs, incorporate the contributions of new
members, and improve their
performance. Thus, our results indicate that communication
networks condition the effect
of membership turnover on group processes and performance.
Funding: The authors gratefully acknowledge the support of the
National Science Foundation [Grants
1111750 and 1459963] and the Center for Organizational
Learning, Innovation and Knowledge at
the Tepper School of Business at Carnegie Mellon University.
Supplemental Material: The online appendix is available at
https://doi.org/10.1287/orsc.2017.1176.
Keywords: social networks • turnover • transactive memory •
group performance
Introduction
Organizational activity comprises the coordination of
individuals to solve complex problems. Group mem-
bers must identify the expertise of others, access infor-
mation held by different members, and coordinate
that information to accomplish shared goals. Mem-
ber turnover, the exit of an incumbent member and
introduction of a new member, in groups can com-
plicate the identification and coordination of expertise
and information (Arrow and McGrath 1995). Groups
with stable membership are able to learn each other’s
skills and expertise, allocate tasks to the most qualified
members, and coordinate the interdependent activi-
ties of their members. When turnover occurs, however,
incumbent members know little about the expertise
and skills of the new member, and coordination can
become challenging (Lewis et al. 2007). Yet new mem-
bers can nonetheless be a source of new ideas and per-
spectives that improve group performance (Choi and
Thompson 2005).
We theorize that the effect of turnover on group
performance depends on the group’s communication
network. From a network perspective, groups can be
categorized by their structural features—in particular,
their centralization (Katz et al. 2004, Leavitt 1951, Rulke
and Galaskiewicz 2000) and their density (Freeman
1979, Friedkin 1981, Balkundi and Harrison 2006). Cen-
tralization increases for a group as the inequality or
variance in the number of connections group members
have to others increases. A group’s density increases as
the ratio of actual to potential connections increases.
We identify two mechanisms through which a
group’s communication network affects performance:
(1) the number of direct communication paths avail-
able to group members and (2) the clarity of the
coordination logic. The first mechanism maps onto
network density, which affects a group’s behavior
through the number of available communication paths.
High levels of density in communication networks
allow team members to communicate directly with
one another. The second mechanism maps onto net-
work centralization, which affects a group’s behavior
through the clarity of the coordination logic. Central-
ized communication networks direct how information
is shared and inform how members coordinate (Blau
1974, Bunderson and Boumgarden 2010).
191
D
ow
nl
oa
de
d
fr
om
i
nf
or
m
s.
or
g
by
[
13
4.
88
.7
7.
64
]
on
3
0
A
pr
il
2
01
8,
a
t
07
:0
5
. F
or
p
er
so
na
l
us
e
on
ly
, a
ll
r
ig
ht
s
re
se
rv
ed
.
http://pubsonline.informs.org/journal/orsc/
mailto:[email protected]
http://orcid.org/0000-0002-4149-6968
mailto:[email protected]
http://orcid.org/0000-0001-7086-5876
mailto:[email protected]
http://orcid.org/0000-0003-4872-8650
https://doi.org/10.1287/orsc.2017.1176
Argote, Aven, and Kush: The Effects of Turnover on Group
Performance
192 Organization Science, 2018, vol. 29, no. 2, pp. 191–206, ©
2018 INFORMS
Figure 1. Fully Connected and Perfectly Centralized
Communication Networks
(a) (b)
A
C
DB
A
C
B D
Notes. Panel (a) represents the fully connected communication
net-
work. Panel (b) represents the perfectly centralized
communication
network. C is the central member, whereas A, B, and D are
peripheral
members.
We focus on the two network structures that max-
imize one structural dimension while minimizing the
other: a perfectly centralized or “hub-and-spoke” net-
work that has the highest degree of centralization and
minimizes density (see Figure 1), and a fully con-
nected, “all-channel” network that maximizes den-
sity and minimizes centralization. These two network
structures not only allow the investigation of cen-
tralization and density in tandem but are also struc-
tures common in organizational settings. For exam-
ple, programming groups’ communication networks
have often been found to be fully connected, where
members can communicate directly with any other
member in an open-source setting (Tsay et al. 2014).
By contrast, particularly in proprietary software, pro-
gramming groups generally have a perfectly central-
ized communication network where one central mem-
ber acts as the “software architect,” and other members
can communicate directly only with this central mem-
ber (Kruchten 2008, Bosch and Bosch-Sijtsema 2010). In
addition, even though project groups in organizations
commonly communicate in a fully connected manner,
groups engaged in covert projects typically communi-
cate in a centralized fashion (Aven 2015).
In the case of stable group membership, high cen-
tralization, such as in our perfectly centralized groups,
limits direct communication among members through
their restricted communication network, which hin-
ders members from learning about others’ expertise.
Centralized communication networks also force mem-
bers to coordinate in a particular manner, irrespective
of members’ preferences and abilities. Rather than pro-
vide members with the opportunity to customize their
coordination logic, centralized group members must
channel information to the central member(s), who
then orchestrate the group’s activities. Hence, the coor-
dination logic in centralized groups is independent of
the particular members and their attributes.
When turnover occurs, the communication network
of perfectly centralized groups improves the group’s
ability to integrate a new member. The coordina-
tion logic of perfectly centralized networks is read-
ily discernible by both incumbent and new members,
which enhances their ability to contribute to the group
(Bunderson and Boumgarden 2010, Morrison 2002).
Because members’ roles in perfectly centralized groups
are not customized to individual members, it is more
likely that the new member can adequately perform the
activities of the departing member in centralized than
in decentralized groups. Finally, the restricted com-
munication pathways in centralized networks require
reliance on the few existing pathways to coordinate,
which reduces the likelihood that any communica-
tion pathway and its respective member are neglected.
These factors enable a perfectly centralized group to
incorporate the contributions of a new member and
improve group outcomes for tasks involving problem
solving and creativity (e.g., Wells and Pelz 1966, Choi
and Thompson 2005).
When membership is stable, high density in com-
munication networks, such as in the fully connected
groups, enables members to establish a strong trans-
active memory system (TMS), a collective system for
encoding, storing, and retrieving information (Wegner
1987, Lewis and Herndon 2011). Because of fully
connected group members’ greater ability to communi-
cate directly with other members, group members are
able to learn about each other’s expertise and, in turn,
develop a shared cognitive map of expertise. In addi-
tion, fully connected networks allow members to tailor
the group’s coordination logic to account for each mem-
ber’s preferences and abilities.
Although fully connected communication networks
facilitate group performance by encouraging the devel-
opment of a strong TMS, fully connected networks
can also hinder the integration of new group members
when turnover occurs. New members often do not have
the same attributes and knowledge as departing mem-
bers, which makes the substitution of a new member
challenging for these groups because their coordina-
tion logic is tailored to the original members’ unique
abilities and characteristics. Furthermore, the group’s
coordination logic cannot be readily observed by new
members, which impairs their ability to contribute to
the group.
Communication Networks and Transactive
Memory Systems
A group’s communication network serves to channel
information among group members and coordinate
their activities (Leavitt 1951, Faucheux and Macken-
zie 1966). In a dense, fully connected network (see
Figure 1(a)), all of the members share the same number
of communication pathways as all of the other mem-
bers. By contrast, in the perfectly centralized group
(see Figure 1(b)), only member C has communication
D
ow
nl
oa
de
d
fr
om
i
nf
or
m
s.
or
g
by
[
13
4.
88
.7
7.
64
]
on
3
0
A
pr
il
2
01
8,
a
t
07
:0
5
. F
or
p
er
so
na
l
us
e
on
ly
, a
ll
r
ig
ht
s
re
se
rv
ed
.
Argote, Aven, and Kush: The Effects of Turnover on Group
Performance
Organization Science, 2018, vol. 29, no. 2, pp. 191–206, © 2018
INFORMS 193
pathways to the other members (A, B, and D), and
members A, B, and D have only one pathway each to
member C. To both communicate and coordinate activ-
ity in the centralized group, the peripheral members
(A, B, and D) must rely on the central member C. Thus,
the perfectly centralized communication network both
minimizes the number of available communication
pathways and maximizes the clarity of the coordina-
tion logic for the group: all peripheral members com-
municate to the central member, and the central mem-
ber dispatches information to peripheral members. By
contrast, the fully connected network maximizes the
number of communication paths but reduces the clar-
ity of the coordination logic.
The greater number of communication pathways in
fully connected networks promotes information shar-
ing and fosters an understanding of member expertise.
When group members learn information about other
members and their expertise, the group’s transactive
memory system develops (Moreland and Myaskovsky
2000). The transactive memory systems of fully con-
nected groups become tailored to the different skills,
knowledge, and preferences of group members.
Within perfectly centralized groups, the communi-
cation network prevents group members from directly
interacting with one another and developing a coordi-
nation logic that is well suited to the particular mem-
bers of the group. Instead, the communication network
of perfectly centralized groups requires that certain
members relay information to others and thus imposes
a particular coordination logic on the group (Gouldner
1954, Weber 1947). The limited number of communi-
cation pathways also hinders members from directly
learning about each other’s areas of expertise, which in
turn inhibits the development of a strong transactive
memory system.
Transactive memory systems have been found to
improve group performance (see Ren and Argote 2011
for a review) on outcomes such as time to complete
tasks (Faraj and Sproull 2000), errors (Liang et al. 1995),
and creativity (Gino et al. 2010). Transactive memory
systems can be distinguished from related constructs
such as shared mental models, because the knowledge
held by group members with a strong TMS is differ-
entiated through specialization (Lewis and Herndon
2011). In groups with a strong TMS, members have
different information about how to complete their sub-
tasks, whereas in groups with shared mental models,
members have the same information (DeChurch and
Mesmer-Magnus 2010).
Transactive memory systems develop from expe-
rience working together. As individuals work with
the same group members, they discern each other’s
skills and abilities, which allows them to special-
ize their expertise, fine-tune their division of labor,
and coordinate effectively (Wegner 1987). The strength
of a group’s TMS is affected by its communications
(Hollingshead 1998, Yuan et al. 2010). We predict that
differences in communication networks influence the
strength of a group’s TMS. Furthermore, we predict
that the effect of a group’s communication network
depends on whether turnover occurs.
Turnover
Turnover, or membership change, is a common occur-
rence within work groups (Kush et al. 2012). In some
instances, turnover can benefit the group by invigorat-
ing it with information and perspectives from the new
member, which improves its performance (Choi and
Levine 2004, Choi and Thompson 2005, Hancock et al.
2013). The positive effect of turnover on group perfor-
mance is most likely to occur on tasks involving cre-
ativity and innovation (Choi and Thompson 2005). For
example, Wells and Pelz (1966) found that turnover in
groups of scientists improved group performance. The
introduction of a new member, even a novice, into an
existing group can enhance the group’s performance
(Ferriani et al. 2009, Uzzi and Spiro 2005). For new
members to benefit a group, however, they must be
integrated into the group by existing members.
Membership change can hinder group outcomes
because the new member might not have the same
skills as the departing member, understand the coor-
dination logic of the group, or be integrated into
the group (Hausknecht and Holwerda 2013). Lewis
et al. (2007) found that when only one group mem-
ber was replaced in a group, those groups performed
worse than either groups whose membership was sta-
ble or groups whose membership changed totally. The
advantages conferred on fully connected groups by
their strong TMSs when membership is stable are
undermined when turnover occurs. Because TMS is a
cognitive division of labor, the group’s implicit coor-
dination logic is not easily observed by the new mem-
ber (Wegner 1987). Hence, for a fully connected group,
a new member would have difficulty contributing
because he or she would not understand how the
group communicates or organizes its activities. When
a group has developed a strong TMS, the incumbent
members of the group tended to ignore the contribu-
tions of a new member and expect him or her to fill the
role of the departing member (Lewis et al. 2007). Thus,
we anticipate that turnover will harm the performance
of fully connected groups.
The features that undermine TMS formation in per-
fectly centralized groups with stable membership—
limited communication pathways and mandated coor-
dination logic—aid in the integration of new members
when turnover occurs. New members in perfectly cen-
tralized groups can easily discern the simple organiz-
ing principle: all members contribute their information
and views to the central member(s), who then integrate
D
ow
nl
oa
de
d
fr
om
i
nf
or
m
s.
or
g
by
[
13
4.
88
.7
7.
64
]
on
3
0
A
pr
il
2
01
8,
a
t
07
:0
5
. F
or
p
er
so
na
l
us
e
on
ly
, a
ll
r
ig
ht
s
re
se
rv
ed
.
Argote, Aven, and Kush: The Effects of Turnover on Group
Performance
194 Organization Science, 2018, vol. 29, no. 2, pp. 191–206, ©
2018 INFORMS
the information for the group (Guetzkow and Simon
1955). In turn, understanding the group’s coordina-
tion logic helps new members to work effectively with
other members, which is critical to group performance
(Chao et al. 1994, Morrison 2002). Moreover, the lim-
ited connectivity of perfectly centralized groups also
requires greater reliance on the extant pathways for
members to communicate. Frequent communication
improves the new member’s knowledge of how to per-
form his or her tasks and provides clarity in terms of
the responsibilities associated with the position, which
expedites the new member’s ability to contribute to the
group (Morrison 2002). When the group incorporates
the new member’s ideas and perspectives, the group is
able to perform its task more effectively (Levine et al.
2003). In addition to increasing the creative problem
solving of incumbent members (Choi and Thompson
2005), new members can also introduce new insights
that improve group performance.
We theorize two mechanisms through which net-
work structure affects performance: (1) the number of
direct communication paths available to group mem-
bers and (2) the clarity of the coordination logic. We
have argued that the first mechanism is more impor-
tant when membership is stable and that the second
mechanism is more important when turnover occurs.
Thus, we expect that fully connected groups have
stronger TMSs and better performance under stable
membership than when turnover occurs, because of
their greater number of communication paths. The
greater number of paths, however, undermines their
ability to integrate a new member when turnover
occurs. We anticipate that the clarity of the commu-
nication logic of perfectly centralized groups enables
them to integrate the new member, benefit from his or
her new ideas and perspectives, and thereby improve
their performance when turnover occurs. Thus, we pre-
dict the following.
Hypothesis 1. Communication networks and turnover in-
teract to affect group performance: fully connected groups
perform better when group membership is stable than when
turnover occurs, whereas perfectly centralized groups per-
form better when turnover occurs than when group member-
ship is stable.
Hypothesis 2. Communication networks and turnover in-
teract to affect transactive memory systems: fully connected
networks have stronger transactive memory systems when
group membership is stable than when turnover occurs,
whereas perfectly centralized networks have stronger trans-
active memory systems when turnover occurs than when
membership is stable.
Dyadic Communication Frequency
Dyadic communication frequency represents the total
communication that occurs in a group relative to
the number of available communication paths in the
group. The greater number of communication path-
ways in fully connected relative to perfectly central-
ized groups increases the demands on members’ time
and attention and reduces the frequency of commu-
nication along any one pathway. As opposed to fully
connected groups, perfectly centralized groups must
rely on fewer pathways to communicate, increasing
group members’ dependence on each individual path-
way, potentially strengthening one-on-one relation-
ships. Thus, the communication constraints of per-
fectly centralized groups increase members’ reliance
on the available pathways and result in greater dyadic
communication frequency than in less restricted, fully
connected groups.
Dyadic communication along each pathway is espe-
cially important in integrating a new member. The
fewer paths available to perfectly centralized groups
make each connection more salient to group mem-
bers. The greater amount of dyadic communication in
perfectly centralized groups conveys the group’s coor-
dination logic, which is critical in integrating a new
member, and allows members to update the group’s
knowledge of who knows what and to perform bet-
ter when turnover occurs. By contrast, when turnover
occurs in fully connected groups with strong TMSs,
incumbent members neglect to communicate with the
new member, which impairs their performance. For-
mally, we predict the following.
Hypothesis 3. Dyadic communication frequency mediates
the interaction of communication network and turnover on
transactive memory systems, and the mediation is stronger
when turnover occurs than when group membership is
stable.
Scope Conditions
Given that our primary interest is in the interactive
effect of communication networks and turnover on the
processes and performance of small groups, we focus
on perfectly centralized groups and fully connected
groups of four members. Small groups are a predom-
inant means by which tasks are accomplished within
organizations as well as an important unit of analy-
sis for organizational research (Katz et al. 2004, Leavitt
1996, Uzzi and Spiro 2005). In addition, because we
are interested in communication networks, we selected
interdependent tasks that require coordination, so that
we could observe how information was shared in the
group. We focus on nonroutine, complex tasks; there-
fore, our theory applies to complex problem-solving
tasks that require coordination and creativity. Exam-
ples of such tasks include product design, prototype
development, engineering design processes, and soft-
ware engineering. Last, because our tasks involve cre-
ative problem solving, we expect that the contribu-
tions of the new member have the potential to improve
D
ow
nl
oa
de
d
fr
om
i
nf
or
m
s.
or
g
by
[
13
4.
88
.7
7.
64
]
on
3
0
A
pr
il
2
01
8,
a
t
07
:0
5
. F
or
p
er
so
na
l
us
e
on
ly
, a
ll
r
ig
ht
s
re
se
rv
ed
.
Argote, Aven, and Kush: The Effects of Turnover on Group
Performance
Organization Science, 2018, vol. 29, no. 2, pp. 191–206, © 2018
INFORMS 195
group performance (e.g., see Choi and Thompson
2005). We have hypothesized that whether the group
benefits from the new member depends on the group’s
communication network.
Methods
Participants
One hundred and nine four-person groups composed
of 503 individuals (49% female) were recruited from
a mid-Atlantic American university participant pool.1
Participants received $20 or course credit for their par-
ticipation, and an additional reward of $20 per person
was given to the members of the best performing group
in each condition. The ages of participants ranged from
18 to 37, with an average age of 22.0 years. Forty-nine
percent of the participants were Asian or Indian, 37%
were Caucasian, and 14% were other ethnicities.
Tasks
Because our focus is interdependent problem-solving
tasks, we used two tasks that fell in the conceptual
cooperative quadrant of McGrath’s (1984) circumplex
model. Both tasks required creative problem solving.
The first task was a programming task with the pos-
sibility of errors; the second was an idea-generating
task. Both tasks were based on an online graphical
programming interface. This interface allowed partici-
pants to design programs—called pipes—that collect,
manipulate, organize, and filter information from the
Internet to create a desired output. Although similar-
ities exist between this interface and other program-
ming languages, knowledge of other programming
languages would not provide direct benefits because
of the unique graphical programming interface of our
tasks. The preponderance of participants were unfa-
miliar with this interface: 94.2% rated themselves as
unfamiliar or very unfamiliar with the interface in a
postexperiment survey.
The programming task required each group to work
together to create a pipe that (1) sorted selected news
items by publication date, (2) removed any repeated
articles, and (3) allowed a user to specify a search key-
word. The task required the use of five specific mod-
ules. Although working on separate computers, par-
ticipants had access to a virtual work environment,
allowing them to contribute and to see the contribu-
tions of their group mates simultaneously. All mem-
bers could access and make changes to the pipe. In the
idea-generating task, participants were asked to gen-
erate collectively as many new features or functional
improvements as possible to enhance the pipe that they
had created in the programming task. The new features
had to be both novel and feasible to implement, and
therefore required the module expertise of all of the
members. For both tasks, group members never inter-
acted face-to-face and communicated only through an
instant messaging client.
Manipulations
We manipulated two variables in the experiment: com-
munication network and turnover. We manipulated
the communication network by controlling who could
communicate with whom through an instant messag-
ing client. In the perfectly centralized condition, three
peripheral members could contact only one central
member who could communicate with all the periph-
eral members. In the fully connected condition, all
members could communicate with all other members
(see Figure 1). In both conditions, group members
could send only one message to one member at a time.
Throughout both tasks, the communication network
remained unchanged.
We manipulated turnover by replacing a randomly
selected member from each group after the practice
task with another participant who acquired the net-
work position of the departing member. Participants
were not warned that their group might experience
turnover. The new member received the same train-
ing and training materials as the departing member
but had not previously worked with the graphical pro-
gramming tool or with a group. In fully connected
groups that experienced turnover, a randomly chosen
member was selected to be replaced. In the perfectly
centralized groups that experienced turnover, either a
peripheral member or a central member was replaced.
For example, in the centralized (see Figure 1(b)) periph-
eral turnover condition, a peripheral member (A, B,
or D) was randomly selected and replaced. In the cen-
tralized central turnover condition, the central mem-
ber (C) was replaced. We did not predict that turnover
of the central member would have a significantly dif-
ferent effect than turnover of peripheral members, but
we allow for the possibility in our design by including
both types of centralized turnover.
Procedures
After arriving at the laboratory, participants were ran-
domly assigned to one of four isolated rooms, each
associated with a member ID and a position within the
network. We allowed members to communicate using
only an instant text messenger and limited their oppor-
tunities to see one another. The participants attended a
training phase where they received information about
the programming task as well as instructions for the
experiment. The materials explained how to commu-
nicate with other members through the instant mes-
senger, directed participants to a short video demon-
strating the creation of a sample pipe, and provided
detailed information about the programming mod-
ules available. Additionally, we provided each group
member with different, randomly assigned, special-
ized information about one of the modules necessary
for completing the task to ensure that group members
were interdependent. We did not tell members that
D
ow
nl
oa
de
d
fr
om
i
nf
or
m
s.
or
g
by
[
13
4.
88
.7
7.
64
]
on
3
0
A
pr
il
2
01
8,
a
t
07
:0
5
. F
or
p
er
so
na
l
us
e
on
ly
, a
ll
r
ig
ht
s
re
se
rv
ed
.
Argote, Aven, and Kush: The Effects of Turnover on Group
Performance
196 Organization Science, 2018, vol. 29, no. 2, pp. 191–206, ©
2018 INFORMS
each had specialized information. For groups experi-
encing turnover, the new member received the same
specialized information as the member whom he or
she replaced.
Once all the participants had read the training mate-
rials, they were given 30 minutes to practice the task
together. After they finished the practice task, group
members completed the first survey. At this point,
for groups in the turnover condition, an announce-
ment was made: “One new member will be joining
your group, and another will be randomly chosen to
leave.” A randomly chosen group member was then
removed from the communication network and dis-
missed from the experiment, and a new member was
introduced to the group. The new member occupied
the same position in the communication network as
the departing member and received the same special-
ized information during training. Groups were then
given instructions for the programming task, which
they had 30 minutes to complete. The programming
task was followed by the 5-minute feature-generating
task.2 Group members then completed the second sur-
vey. Finally, we debriefed and thanked participants.
Measures
Several variables in this study were behavioral mea-
sures, including our two dependent measures: the
number of errors in the programming task and the
number of functional improvements in the feature-
generating task. The dyadic communication between
group members was also a behavioral measure based
on the messages sent on the instant messaging client.
The remaining measures were collected from a sur-
vey that measured the group’s TMS, perceptions of the
communication network, and group member demo-
graphics and experience, such as their familiarity with
the programming interface.
Errors. We calculated the number of errors in the pipe
that each group submitted. Errors included both cases
when the group used incorrect modules and cases
when the group omitted modules required for the pro-
gram to function properly. To calculate the errors, we
categorized errors into four comprehensive categories:
missing modules, incorrect settings, incorrect modules,
or unconnected modules. The number of errors for a
module was based on the number of settings in that
module. For example, if a group did not include a par-
ticular module in their program and that module had
three required settings, three errors would be recorded
for the group. If a module was included but one of
the settings was wrong, the group was coded as hav-
ing one error. These errors were summed into a single
measure for each group. A portion of the groups had
their errors assessed by two coders (n � 70). A Cohen’s
kappa of 0.72 (p < 0.001) indicated good agreement
between coders (Cohen 1960). A single coder then
coded errors for the remaining groups.
New Features. During the feature-generating task,
participants responded to the question, “Think of any
ways that you think the pipe you just built could be
improved by creating new features. What other ways
could this pipe be more helpful to an end user, be
more simply designed, etc.?” Two coders coded a sub-
set of the groups (n � 40), assessing each new fea-
ture on whether it introduced novel functionality and
was feasible to implement. Two coders attained very
good reliability, as indicated by a Cohen’s kappa of 0.88
(p < 0.001). Any disagreements between coders were
addressed and resolved. A single coder then coded the
number of new features for the rest of the groups.
Transactive Memory Systems. Lewis’s (2003) 15-item
survey measure was used to measure the groups’
transactive memory systems. The survey instrument
was administered at the end of the study, after groups
had completed both tasks. The overall reliability was
acceptable (Cronbach’s alpha�0.82). The average inter-
group reliability (rw g) was 0.95, indicating that it is
appropriate to aggregate the individual-level measures
to the group level. The intraclass correlation coefficient
(ICC(2)) value was 0.61, indicating acceptable reliabil-
ity of the measure. These reliability statistics provide
evidence for general agreement among group mem-
bers and the appropriateness of aggregating to the
group level (LeBreton and Senter 2008). The ICC(1)
value, which indicates the extent to which the variabil-
ity in individual responses can be predicted by group
membership, was 0.28. Values of 0.25 or higher are con-
sidered to be large effects (Murphy and Myors 1998).
Dyadic Communication Frequency. We calculated the
dyadic communication frequency based on group
members’ instant text messages. As our theory is about
the integration of new members, we chose to use a
measure that would capture the strength of the com-
munication relationships between individuals in the
group. Dyadic communication frequency was calcu-
lated as the sum of all group messages exchanged
between members during the task performance period,
divided by the total available dyadic communication
paths in the group (three for perfectly centralized and
six for fully connected). This value was then divided
by the time to task completion to account for varia-
tion in the groups’ completion times. We calculated
this variable separately for the programming task and
the feature-generating task. Analyses predicting errors
used the dyadic communication frequency during the
programming task, and analyses predicting new fea-
tures used the dyadic communication frequency dur-
ing the feature-generation task. For Table 2, we used
cumulative dyadic communication frequency, the com-
bination of dyadic communication frequency across
both task periods, to predict the effect of the network
on communication and on TMS (Models 6 and 7),
D
ow
nl
oa
de
d
fr
om
i
nf
or
m
s.
or
g
by
[
13
4.
88
.7
7.
64
]
on
3
0
A
pr
il
2
01
8,
a
t
07
:0
5
. F
or
p
er
so
na
l
us
e
on
ly
, a
ll
r
ig
ht
s
re
se
rv
ed
.
Argote, Aven, and Kush: The Effects of Turnover on Group
Performance
Organization Science, 2018, vol. 29, no. 2, pp. 191–206, © 2018
INFORMS 197
because TMS development should be affected by all
prior communication in the group, not just communi-
cation relevant to a particular task. We present analyses
for communication to and from the new member for
groups in the turnover conditions in the online sup-
plement. Because these measures of communication to
and from the new member do not apply for groups that
did not experience turnover, we use dyadic communi-
cation frequency in analyses involving both turnover
and no-turnover groups. Dyadic communication fre-
quency includes communication to and from the new
member for groups that experienced turnover.
Results
We begin this section by discussing the effectiveness
of the communication network manipulation. We then
present results for errors and new features. Next,
we provide mediation analyses indicating that dyadic
communication frequency explains the effects of the
communication network and turnover on TMSs. Last,
we provide a test of the overall model implied by
Hypotheses 1–3. The section concludes with a presen-
tation of robustness checks.
Communication Network Manipulation Checks
First, we determined whether members correctly as-
sessed their group’s communication network. Each
group member was asked which communication path-
ways existed among members of the group. Group
members correctly identified the group’s communica-
tion pathways 86% of the time. Fully connected groups
were slightly more accurate (88%) than perfectly cen-
tralized groups (84%) in identifying communication
pathways (p < 0.1). These results suggest that group
members were aware of their available communication
pathways in both the centralized and fully connected
conditions.
Second, although it was not possible for the perfectly
centralized groups to send messages in a decentral-
ized pattern, it was possible for the fully connected
groups to send messages in a centralized pattern. To
explore this possibility, we calculated Freeman’s (1979)
degree centralization measure based on the commu-
nication messages shared within each group. Degree
centralization is a group-level measure of the disper-
sion of group members’ degree centrality scores. Given
that degree centralization can be calculated only for
dichotomous networks, we included a communication
pathway only if two members communicated over a
certain threshold. The threshold for inclusion was one
standard deviation below the group’s mean level of
messages.3 Using a threshold for tie inclusion is com-
mon in network research, and the threshold we applied
provides a reasonable distribution of degree centraliza-
tion values within the sample (Borgatti et al. 2013). The
degree centralization measure was highly correlated
with the manipulation of the communication network
(r � 0.81, p < 0.001), which suggests that our commu-
nication manipulation influenced the pattern of mes-
sages exchanged among group members, as expected.4
Performance
We predicted that the communication network and
turnover would interact to affect group performance
(Hypothesis 1) such that fully connected groups would
perform better when there was no turnover than when
there was, and perfectly centralized groups would per-
form better when turnover occurred than when mem-
bership was stable. Next, we hypothesized that the
communication network and turnover would inter-
act to predict transactive memory systems such that
fully connected groups would have stronger TMSs
when membership was stable than when turnover
occurred, and perfectly centralized networks would
have stronger TMSs when turnover occurred than
when membership was stable (Hypothesis 2). Finally,
we proposed that dyadic communication would medi-
ate the interaction between the communication net-
work and turnover in predicting the TMS and that the
mediation would be stronger when turnover occurred
than when it did not (Hypothesis 3). A stronger medi-
ation would mean that more of the effect of the net-
work on the TMS was due to the network’s effect
on dyadic communication frequency when the group
experienced turnover than when it did not.
We present descriptive statistics and correlations in
Table 1. Table 2 presents the ordinary least squares
estimates predicting errors based on the communi-
cation network, turnover, and their interaction.5 In
Model 1 of Table 2, the interaction of communication
network and turnover was negative and significant
(B � −2.31, p < 0.05), as predicted by Hypothesis 1.
Additionally, we found a significant positive interac-
tion between communication network and turnover
on the number of new features (B � 2.64, p < 0.05) in
Model 3 of Table 2. These interactions suggest that, for
both errors and new features, fully connected groups
performed better when group membership was sta-
ble than when turnover occurred, whereas perfectly
centralized groups performed better when turnover
occurred than when membership was stable. This pat-
tern can be seen in Figure 2, which depicts the mean
numbers of errors (left side) and new features (right
side) as functions of communication networks and
turnover. This pattern of results supports Hypothesis 1.
We turn now to a test of Hypothesis 2. As can
be seen from Model 5 of Table 2, the interaction of
the network and turnover was positive and signifi-
cant in predicting TMS (B � 4.71, p < 0.05), consis-
tent with Hypothesis 2. Fully connected groups devel-
oped stronger TMSs when membership was stable
than when turnover occurred (55.1 versus 52.4), and
D
ow
nl
oa
de
d
fr
om
i
nf
or
m
s.
or
g
by
[
13
4.
88
.7
7.
64
]
on
3
0
A
pr
il
2
01
8,
a
t
07
:0
5
. F
or
p
er
so
na
l
us
e
on
ly
, a
ll
r
ig
ht
s
re
se
rv
ed
.
Argote, Aven, and Kush: The Effects of Turnover on Group
Performance
198 Organization Science, 2018, vol. 29, no. 2, pp. 191–206, ©
2018 INFORMS
Table 1. Means and Correlations
Mean SD 1 2 3 4 5 6
1. Errors 2.04 2.87
2. New Features 4.39 2.63 −0.33∗ ∗ ∗
3. TMS 52.84 5.17 −0.34∗ ∗ ∗ 0.19†
4. Dyadic Communication Frequency During Programming Task
1.53 0.68 −0.24∗ 0.15 0.26∗ ∗
5. Dyadic Communication Frequency During Feature Generation
Task 0.87 0.85 −0.05 −0.01 0.26∗ ∗ 0.63∗ ∗ ∗
6. Dyadic Communication to New Member During Prog. Task
0.83 0.51 −0.30∗ 0.21† 0.36∗ ∗ 0.85∗ ∗ ∗ 0.51∗ ∗ ∗
7. Dyadic Communication from New Member During Prog. Task
0.69 0.43 −0.27∗ −0.01 0.35∗ ∗ 0.76∗ ∗ ∗ 0.53∗ ∗ ∗ 0.67∗ ∗ ∗
†p < 0.1; ∗ p < 0.05; ∗ ∗ p < 0.01; ∗ ∗ ∗ p < 0.001.
perfectly centralized groups developed stronger TMSs
when turnover occurred than when membership was
stable (50.9 versus 52.9).
Although not explicitly hypothesized, we examined
whether transactive memory systems mediated the
effect of communication networks on performance. As
can be seen from Table 2, when TMS was included
as a predictor of errors in Model 2, its coefficient was
negative and significant. Furthermore, the compari-
son of results between Model 1 and Model 2 reveals
that the interaction of communication network and
turnover was no longer significant when TMS was
included, suggesting that TMS accounted for the effect
of the interaction on errors. Similarly, when TMS was
included as a predictor of new features (see Model 4), it
was positive and marginally significant and the inter-
action of network structure and turnover became less
informative (p < 0.05 to p < 0.1).
Next, we investigated the role of dyadic communi-
cation frequency in explaining TMS. As can be seen
Table 2. Models of Errors, New Features, Dyadic
Communication Frequency, and Transactive Memory Systems
Dyadic
New New Communication
Errors Errors Features Features TMS Frequency TMS
Variable Model 1 Model 2 Model 3 Model 4 Model 5 Model 6
Model 7
Centralized Network 0.67 −0.14 −1.76∗ −1.28 −4.24∗ ∗ 0.33†
−5.26∗ ∗ ∗
(0.87) (0.85) (0.80) (0.83) (1.56) (0.18) (1.49)
Turnover 0.72 0.21 −1.28 −0.98 −2.68† −0.17 −2.15
(0.86) (0.82) (0.79) (0.79) (1.55) (0.18) (1.46)
Centralized×Turnover −2.31∗ −1.42 2.64∗ 2.09† 4.71∗ 0.48†
3.28†
(1.13) (1.10) (1.04) (1.07) (2.05) (0.23) (1.96)
TMS −0.19∗ ∗ ∗ 0.10†
(0.05) (0.05)
Dyadic Communication 3.11∗ ∗ ∗
Frequency (0.80)
R2 0.06 0.17 0.08 0.11 0.07 0.24 0.18
N 109 109 109 109 109 109 109
Notes. Values in parentheses are standard errors. Models 3 and
4 include a control for groups that inadvertently received
additional time on
the task; 1 � received 10 minutes, and 0 � received 5 minutes.
The results are consistent if the groups who received 10 minutes
are removed
from the analysis. Cumulative dyadic communication frequency,
which is used in Models 6 and 7, is the dyadic communication
frequency for
the combination of the programming task and the feature-
generating task.
†p < 0.1; ∗ p < 0.05; ∗ ∗ p < 0.01; ∗ ∗ ∗ p < 0.001.
from Model 6 of Table 2, the interaction of the commu-
nication network with turnover in predicting dyadic
communication was marginally significant and pos-
itive (B � 0.48, p < 0.1). The mean levels of dyadic
communication as a function of communication net-
work and turnover can be seen in Figure 3. Fully con-
nected groups had somewhat more dyadic commu-
nication when they did not experience turnover than
when they did, whereas perfectly centralized groups
had somewhat more dyadic communication when they
experienced turnover than when they did not. These
results are in line with Hypothesis 3.
Subsequently, we examined how communication
networks, turnover, and dyadic communication fre-
quency affected TMS. Model 7 in Table 2 adds dyadic
communication frequency as a predictor of TMS and
shows that dyadic communication frequency had a
significant and positive effect on TMS. Also, relative
to Model 5, the interaction between network struc-
ture and turnover became marginally significant when
D
ow
nl
oa
de
d
fr
om
i
nf
or
m
s.
or
g
by
[
13
4.
88
.7
7.
64
]
on
3
0
A
pr
il
2
01
8,
a
t
07
:0
5
. F
or
p
er
so
na
l
us
e
on
ly
, a
ll
r
ig
ht
s
re
se
rv
ed
.
Argote, Aven, and Kush: The Effects of Turnover on Group
Performance
Organization Science, 2018, vol. 29, no. 2, pp. 191–206, © 2018
INFORMS 199
Figure 2. Mean Errors and New Features as Functions of
Communication Network and Turnover
0
1
2
3
4
5
Fully
connected
Perfectly
centralized
E
rr
or
s
0
1
2
3
4
5
N
ew
f
ea
tu
re
s
6
7
Fully
connected
†
Perfectly
centralized
*
†
*
*
No turnover
Turnover
Note. Error bars are 95% confidence intervals.
† p < 0.1; ∗ p < 0.05.
dyadic communication frequency was included in
Model 7. These results indicate that dyadic communi-
cation partially explained the effects of the interaction
of network structure and turnover on the development
of TMS, in support of Hypothesis 3.
Figure 3 also shows the amount of communication
that was directed to the new member (see the darker
bands in the turnover condition bars) compared with
the other three members. As can be seen from the
Figure 3. Dyadic Communication Frequency by Network
and Turnover During the Programming Task
0
0.5
1.0
1.5
D
ya
di
c
co
m
m
un
ic
at
io
n
fr
eq
ue
nc
y
2.0
2.5
No turnover Turnover No turnover Turnover
Fully connected Perfectly centralized
†
†
Notes. Within each group that experienced turnover, the darker
sub-
section is the portion of the communication within the group
that
went to the new member. The rest of the bar represents the com-
munication to the other three members. Error bars are 95% con-
fidence intervals on the group’s dyadic communication
frequency.
Planned contrasts are on dyadic communication frequency.
Planned
contrasts were also performed comparing the proportion of
dyadic
communication that went to the new member in perfectly
central-
ized versus fully connected groups. Perfectly centralized groups
had
significantly more communication to their new members than
fully
connected groups (p < 0.001).
† p < 0.1.
figure, the proportion of communication directed to
the new member was much higher in perfectly cen-
tralized than in fully connected groups. The difference
in the amount of communication to the new mem-
ber was significant using planned contrasts (p < 0.001),
indicating that perfectly centralized groups communi-
cated more to their new members than fully connected
groups. We also replaced the communication directed
to the new member with the communication from the
new member and found a similar pattern: the new
member communicated more in perfectly centralized
than fully connected groups.
In addition, we analyzed communication to or from
the new member as a predictor of TMS (see the online
supplement for more details). Dyadic communication
(B � 3.59, p < 0.001), communication to the new mem-
ber (B � 4.52, p < 0.001), and communication from
the new member (B � 4.55, p < 0.01) were all associ-
ated with increases in the strength of a group’s TMS.
When both communication to and from the new mem-
ber were included, only communication to the new
member was significant (B � 3.20, p < 0.05). Thus, the
effect of incumbent members communicating to the
new member is a more robust predictor of TMS than
communication from the new member for groups that
experienced turnover.
As a supplement to our quantitative results, we pro-
vide examples from groups’ transcripts to illustrate the
differences in how fully connected and perfectly cen-
tralized groups integrated the new member. The first
quote is from a fully connected group that experienced
turnover. Incumbent 1 wrote, “Are you working on it?
Just checking.” Incumbent 2 responded, “I think [New
Member] is doing random shit” (Group 55). This quote
illustrates that the incumbent group members in the
fully connected groups did not directly communicate
with the new member, whom they seemed to ignore
and assumed would not contribute.
D
ow
nl
oa
de
d
fr
om
i
nf
or
m
s.
or
g
by
[
13
4.
88
.7
7.
64
]
on
3
0
A
pr
il
2
01
8,
a
t
07
:0
5
. F
or
p
er
so
na
l
us
e
on
ly
, a
ll
r
ig
ht
s
re
se
rv
ed
.
Argote, Aven, and Kush: The Effects of Turnover on Group
Performance
200 Organization Science, 2018, vol. 29, no. 2, pp. 191–206, ©
2018 INFORMS
In a perfectly centralized group with peripheral
member turnover, we see encouragement between
incumbents and the new member. Incumbent 1 wrote,
“We figured out up to part c, [but] we can’t figure
that out.” After the new member works on the shared
screen, the new member responded, “[That] should be
[the solution to part c].” Incumbent 1 answered with
“mmm . . . let me ask the others . . .you are amazin [sic]”
(Group 70).
In a perfectly centralized group with central member
turnover, incumbent members informed the new mem-
ber about the organizing logic in the group. Incumb-
ent 1 wrote, “[C]ould you speak with A and B???” The
new member responded, “[Y]eah [I] think so. I can talk
to B and B said he/she can only talk to me.” Incum-
bent 1 answered with “you [perform] as a communica-
tor for the group” (Group 70). These quotes reinforce
the quantitative results indicating that perfectly cen-
tralized groups communicated more with their new
members and incorporated their contributions better
than fully connected groups did.
Moderated Mediation Analyses
We tested the moderated mediation proposed in Hy-
pothesis 3 using PROCESS (Hayes 2013). Figure 4 pro-
vides a theoretical model of the moderated mediation
we predict. The use of bootstrap sampling in PROCESS
allows for tests of mediation and moderation that have
more statistical power and violate fewer assumptions
than other tests of mediation (Hayes 2013). For all of the
Figure 4. Theoretical Framework and Statistical Models
Predicting Errors by Turnover Condition
Full moderated serial mediation
(a)
(b) Groups that did not experience turnover (c) Groups that
experienced turnover
+
+_
+
+
+
Centralized
network
Performance
Dyadic
communication
frequency
Transactive
memory systems
Turnover
+
Hypothesis 1
Hypothesis 2
Hypothesis 3
_
No turnover serial mediation
– 0.20*
– 5.09**
2.30
0.37*
Centralized
network
Errors
Dyadic
communication
frequency
Transactive
memory systems
– 0.38
Just turnover serial mediation
– 2.82†
3.59***
– 0.15*0.92***
Centralized
network
Errors
Dyadic
communication
frequency
Transactive
memory systems
– 0.91
Notes. Panel (a) shows the full statistical framework. Panel (b)
shows coefficients for groups that did not experience turnover
(n � 42), and
panel (c) shows coefficients for groups that experienced
turnover (n � 67) for errors.
†p < 0.1; ∗ p < 0.05; ∗ ∗ p < 0.01; ∗ ∗ ∗ p < 0.001.
analyses, we used 50,000 bias-corrected bootstrap sam-
ples. In the overall model, we used dyadic communica-
tion frequency because it occurs for both turnover and
no-turnover groups. The results of the PROCESS anal-
ysis indicated that dyadic communication frequency
mediated the relationship between the communication
network and transactive memory systems, and that
the relationship was stronger when turnover occurred
(B � 2.45; 95% confidence intervals (CIs): 1.14, 4.28)
than when it did not (B � 1.02; 95% CIs: 0.01, 2.69).
The index of moderated mediation test (Hayes 2015)
was significant (95% CIs: 0.20, 3.28), indicating that the
strength of the mediation was significantly stronger for
the turnover than no-turnover groups, providing addi-
tional support for Hypothesis 3.
We used the partially standardized indirect effect to
determinetheeffectsizeofthenetworkvariableonTMS
through dyadic communication frequency (Preacher
and Kelley 2011). The partially standardized indirect
effect can be interpreted the same as a Cohen’s d—
the number of standard deviations of change in TMS
that occur as a result of the effect of the network on
dyadic communication frequency. For groups that did
not experience turnover, perfectly centralized groups
had TMSs that were 0.22 standard deviations stronger
than fully connected groups (95% CIs: 0.01, 0.56),
due solely to centralized groups communicating more
than fully connected groups. Because perfectly cen-
tralized groups communicated even more when they
experienced turnover, centralized groups had TMSs
that were 0.57 standard deviations stronger than fully
D
ow
nl
oa
de
d
fr
om
i
nf
or
m
s.
or
g
by
[
13
4.
88
.7
7.
64
]
on
3
0
A
pr
il
2
01
8,
a
t
07
:0
5
. F
or
p
er
so
na
l
us
e
on
ly
, a
ll
r
ig
ht
s
re
se
rv
ed
.
Argote, Aven, and Kush: The Effects of Turnover on Group
Performance
Organization Science, 2018, vol. 29, no. 2, pp. 191–206, © 2018
INFORMS 201
connected groups when turnover occurred (95% CIs:
0.27, 0.91), because of their communication. Cohen
(1960) described 0.22 as a small effect and 0.57 as a
medium effect. These calculations demonstrate that the
importance of dyadic communication in explaining the
network’s effect on TMS increases substantially when
groups experienced turnover.
We now turn to a test of a full “moderated serial
mediation” model as seen in Figure 4. The analysis
for errors indicated that for groups that did not ex-
perience turnover, the effect of communication net-
work on errors was partially mediated by dyadic
communication frequency and TMS (B � −0.21; 95%
CIs: −0.66, −0.03); however, the majority of the effect
was accounted for by TMS alone (B � 0.98; 95% CIs:
0.41, 1.92). By contrast, for groups that experienced
turnover, the strength of the effect of communication
network through dyadic communication frequency
and TMS was larger in magnitude (B �−0.52; 95% CIs:
−1.08,−0.21) than that of the effect through TMS alone
(B � 0.44; 95% CIs: 0.0002, 1.1386). The index for mod-
erated mediation (Hayes 2015) indicated that the effect
of having a perfectly centralized network on errors
through dyadic communication frequency and TMS
was stronger in turnover versus no-turnover groups
(95% CIs: −0.68, −0.05). The effect of having a perfectly
centralized network on errors just through TMS did
not differ in magnitude based on turnover (95% CIs:
−1.35, 0.16).6
For groups that did not experience turnover, TMS
explained the majority of the effect of the communi-
cation network on errors: perfectly centralized groups
had weaker TMSs than fully connected groups, which
led to more errors (see Figure 4(b)). There was a
countervailing effect of the communication network
through dyadic communication frequency—perfectly
centralized groups communicated more than fully
connected groups, which improved their TMSs and
thereby reduced errors. But this second effect was
smaller than the first when group membership was sta-
ble. Hence, when there was no turnover, the net effect
of having a perfectly centralized network increased
errors (−0.21 + 0.98 � 0.77). When groups experi-
enced turnover, however, the magnitudes of the effects
changed (see Figure 4(c)). For groups that experi-
enced turnover, the effect of having a perfectly central-
ized network on dyadic communication frequency was
much stronger than for groups that did not experience
turnover, and the effect of dyadic communication fre-
quency on TMS was also stronger. When groups expe-
rienced turnover, the effect of perfectly centralized net-
works reducing errors through their effect on dyadic
communication frequency and TMS exceeded the per-
fectly centralized network’s direct effect on increasing
errors through TMS alone (−0.52 + 0.44 �−0.08).
To provide a sense of the size of these effects, we
present the effect sizes for the moderated serial medi-
ation. For groups that did not experience turnover, the
partially standardized indirect effect of the network on
errors through dyadic communication frequency and
TMS was −0.07 (95% CIs: −0.23, −0.01), and there was
also a significant mediation just through TMS (0.35;
95% CIs: 0.14, 0.68). The interpretation of these signif-
icant effect sizes is that for groups that did not expe-
rience turnover, there was a 0.28 (−0.07 + 0.35) SD
increase in the number of errors for perfectly central-
ized versus fully connected groups. This effect reversed
for those that experienced turnover, with a 0.18 SD
(95% CIs:−0.37,−0.07) decrease in errors due to dyadic
communication and TMS.
We tested the same moderated serial mediation with
new features as the dependent variable. The serial
pathway of the centralized network effect on new fea-
tures through dyadic communication frequency and
TMS was not significant for groups with stable group
membership (B � 0.02; 95% CIs: −0.04, 0.16) but was
positive and significant for groups that experienced
turnover (B � 0.09; 95% CIs: 0.01, 0.28). TMS by itself
acted as a significant mediator of the effect of hav-
ing a centralized network on new features for groups
with stable membership (B � −0.55; 95% CIs: −1.34,
−0.09) but not for groups that experienced turnover
(B � −0.03; 95% CIs: −0.33, 0.26).7 These results are
similar to the results for errors; however, neither of
the mediations was moderated by turnover. In these
analyses, we found general support for our hypotheses
and for the overall model in Figure 4(a). Results were
stronger for errors than for new features.
Robustness Checks
Because the central and peripheral turnover conditions
did not differ in their errors (1.22 and 1.23, respectively;
p � 0.99), new features (4.52 versus 5.00, respectively;
p � 0.54), or the strength of their transactive mem-
ory systems (52.2 versus 53.6, respectively; p � 0.37),
these two centralized turnover conditions were com-
bined into a single centralized network turnover con-
dition for the analyses shown in Table 2. We repeated
these analyses but included only one form of central-
ized turnover at a time. When either of the central-
ized turnover conditions (central member or periph-
eral member turnover) was removed from Model 1
in Table 2, the interaction of network structure and
turnover on errors was in the same direction and
marginally significant (p < 0.1). This reduction in sig-
nificance is likely due to a reduction in power. No dif-
ferences were detected when Model 3 in Table 2, pre-
dicting new features, was run with only one of the
perfectly centralized turnover conditions included in
the analysis at a time. Second, we reran all moderated
mediation analyses with data from only one of the two
D
ow
nl
oa
de
d
fr
om
i
nf
or
m
s.
or
g
by
[
13
4.
88
.7
7.
64
]
on
3
0
A
pr
il
2
01
8,
a
t
07
:0
5
. F
or
p
er
so
na
l
us
e
on
ly
, a
ll
r
ig
ht
s
re
se
rv
ed
.
Argote, Aven, and Kush: The Effects of Turnover on Group
Performance
202 Organization Science, 2018, vol. 29, no. 2, pp. 191–206, ©
2018 INFORMS
perfectly centralized turnover conditions included at
a time and did not find any significant differences for
turnover based on whether the peripheral or central
member left the group.
We created two variables that measured the imbal-
ance of messages sent and received among the group
members, using the Herfindahl–Hirschman index of
communication corrected for the number of pathways
as an alternative explanation of our findings. These
indices measure imbalances, and they increase to 1 as
more communication is either sent or received by a
single individual and decrease to 0 if there is perfect
equality in the number of messages sent or received.
Although the imbalance of sent messages (B �−14.89,
p < 0.001) and the imbalance of received messages
(B � −18.73, p < 0.1) were both negatively related
to TMS, neither measure was statistically significant
when dyadic communication frequency was added to
the model, and dyadic communication frequency was
significant and positive (p < 0.01) in both cases. These
results suggest that dyadic communication frequency
is a more robust predictor of TMS than imbalanced
communication.
Discussion and Conclusion
Our results indicate that perfectly centralized com-
munication networks can reverse the negative effect
of turnover in groups. When turnover occurred in
fully connected groups, incumbents tended to ignore
new members who could not readily understand the
group’s implicit coordination logic or their roles within
the group. By contrast, the explicit coordination logic
of perfectly centralized groups enabled them to make
use of the contributions of new members. In central-
ized groups, the means of coordination, which were
largely determined by the communication structure,
were simple and explicit. The high dyadic communi-
cation frequency in the perfectly centralized groups
reduced the likelihood that the incumbent members
ignored the new member. The active integration of
the new member not only fostered the formation of
a strong TMS but also improved the creativity and
problem-solving capabilities of centralized groups.
The experimental manipulation of communication
networks we use here provides two distinct method-
ological advantages. First, identifying the direct causal
effects of networks has proven challenging in field
settings, particularly because of endogeneity issues
(Ferriani et al. 2009, Lee et al. 2014, Uzzi and Spiro
2005). Our study design provides insight into commu-
nication networks and group processes and is unham-
pered by endogeneity concerns (see Croson et al.
2007 for a discussion of the advantages of experimen-
tal research). The experimental design and random
assignment of participants to communication networks
and to positions in the networks also enabled us to
attribute effects to the communication network rather
than to conditions that led to the formation of the net-
works or to qualities of individuals who might gravi-
tate to certain positions in the networks (Sasovova et al.
2010). Thus, directly manipulating the communication
network allows us to make causal claims about the
effects of communication networks on group perfor-
mance (Manski 1993). Second, where possible, we used
behavioral measures, such as the amount of commu-
nication, which are more objective than self-reported
variables (Spector 1994). In particular, self-reports of
communication and social networks have been found
to be heavily biased by social factors, such as individ-
ual status (Bernard et al. 1984).
In addition to providing insights difficult to obtain
by other methods, the design of our study maps well to
organizational phenomena. The two tasks we used—
the programming task and the idea-generating task—
parallel many tasks found in organizational settings.
Within organizations, groups often work on complex
tasks, which involve both problem solving and creativ-
ity (Devine et al. 1999). Thus, our realistic tasks provide
external validity. In addition, we trained individuals on
different information to increase task interdependence
among the group members, which captures the chal-
lenge of integration and coordination among group
members with specialized members. Group members
also communicated through computers located in
separate rooms and thus operated as a distributed
group. This arrangement is analogous to contempo-
rary project-based work conducted in organizations
via email (Kleinbaum et al. 2013, Aven 2015). This fea-
ture of our experiment not only represents a condition
under which many groups operate in organizations
today but also allows us to control the communication
network and to capture all communication that occurs
among group members. Although the features of our
experimental design were chosen to reflect character-
istics of real organizations, these design features also
present boundary conditions to which our findings are
most likely to generalize. Therefore, we anticipate that
our findings will generalize to small groups in which
members perform complex interdependent tasks and
communicate in a distributed manner.
Another boundary condition of our findings is the
experience of the new member. The new member in
our study received the same individual training as the
departing member but did not work with a group pre-
viously. We chose this study design because we felt that
new members to groups typically would have received
some training but would have limited group experi-
ence. Understanding the effect of the prior experience
of the new member on group processes and perfor-
mance is an interesting issue for future research.
Although we did not hypothesize differences in the
effects of turnover in central and peripheral mem-
bers in perfectly centralized groups, we included both
D
ow
nl
oa
de
d
fr
om
i
nf
or
m
s.
or
g
by
[
13
4.
88
.7
7.
64
]
on
3
0
A
pr
il
2
01
8,
a
t
07
:0
5
. F
or
p
er
so
na
l
us
e
on
ly
, a
ll
r
ig
ht
s
re
se
rv
ed
.
Argote, Aven, and Kush: The Effects of Turnover on Group
Performance
Organization Science, 2018, vol. 29, no. 2, pp. 191–206, © 2018
INFORMS 203
types of turnover in our design to be able to ascer-
tain whether any differences observed were due to
the structure, as we hypothesized, or to the position.
To accomplish this, we randomly assigned individu-
als to networks and to positions rather than allow-
ing individuals to gravitate to the networks or posi-
tions that they preferred or for which they believed
they were especially qualified. Furthermore, the new
member received the same training as other members
and the same specialized information as the depart-
ing member. These features of the design enabled us
to investigate the causal effects of the communication
network, a major goal of the study. We did not find dif-
ferences between turnover in the central or peripheral
positions in dyadic communication, TMS, or perfor-
mance. It is important to note that turnover of central
and peripheral members might have different effects
in groups where members’ positions are correlated
with other characteristics, such as experience or skills,
which is often the case in organizations. By using ran-
dom assignment, our study enabled us to determine
the effect of network structure, independent of these
individual factors, on group outcomes.
One limitation of our study was that the feature-
generating task always followed the programming
task. We thought that this was the most realistic
sequence: it would be more natural for groups to
identify “ways that the pipe you just built could be
improved by creating new features” after rather than
before they had built the pipe. The correlation between
errors in the programming task and new features in
the feature-generating task was −0.33, which indicates
a moderate relationship between the two tasks: as
errors on the programming task decreased, the number
of new features typically increased. Thus, performing
well on the first task did not constrain members from
being able to identify ways the pipe could be improved
on the second task. Nonetheless, it would be useful
in future work to develop an idea-generating task that
could occur before or after the programming task and
thus allow the order of tasks to be counterbalanced.
This paper contributes to several literatures as well
as highlights their intersections. In a review of the
transactive memory literature, Ren and Argote (2011)
recommended greater research on how social networks
affect the development of transactive memory systems.
Our research shows that the communication network
is an important predictor of transactive memory sys-
tems. In addition, our findings demonstrate that com-
munication networks condition the effect of turnover
on TMS development. Previous work has shown that
membership change disables a group’s TMS, which
impairs its performance (Lewis et al. 2007, Moreland
et al. 1996). Our study demonstrates that perfectly cen-
tralized communication networks can reverse the neg-
ative effect of turnover of one member. Relative to fully
connected groups, incumbent members of centralized
groups communicate more with the new member,
which enables them to update their TMS and improve
their performance. Our results suggest that perfectly
centralized groups are more receptive to newcomers
and more likely to incorporate the ideas of new mem-
bers than fully connected groups.
Our findings also offer insights into the effects of
the communication network structure on group-level
outcomes, such as group performance (Casciaro et al.
2015). Although there is a robust literature on the
effects of communication networks on individual out-
comes (Ahuja et al. 2003, Borgatti and Cross 2003),
our work provides new insights into the effects of net-
work configurations on group-level outcomes. In this
study, we empirically established that, under stable
membership, a fully connected communication net-
work enables the development of a TMS, which in turn
leads to positive performance. When turnover occurs,
a perfectly centralized network enables the develop-
ment of a TMS, which in turn improves performance.
In addition, our findings begin to unpack how commu-
nication networks interact with membership change to
affect group processes.
We described two mechanisms through which cen-
tralization affects performance: (1) the number of direct
communication paths available to team members and
(2) the clarity of the coordination logic. The first mech-
anism maps onto network density, which affects a
team’s behavior through the number of available com-
munication paths. The second mechanism maps onto
network centralization, which affects a team’s behav-
ior through the clarity of the coordination logic. We
focused on the two network structures that maximize
one structural dimension and minimize the other: (1) a
fully connected, all-channel network that maximizes
density and minimizes centralization and (2) a hub-
and-spoke or perfectly centralized network that max-
imizes centralization and minimizes density. Network
types, however, vary across both density and central-
ization. We suggest that an important issue for future
research is to disentangle the effects of network den-
sity and centralization. Although it is not possible to
make the two dimensions of density and centraliza-
tion orthogonal,8 one could design studies where the
two dimensions were not highly correlated and thereby
investigate their separate effects. On the basis of our
theory and evidence, we would predict that density
is a more important predictor of group performance
when membership is stable and that the clarity of the
coordination logic is more important when turnover
occurs on complex interdependent tasks such as the
ones used here. This theory leads to a variety of pre-
dictions about the effects of particular network typolo-
gies on group processes and performance under con-
ditions of member stability or turnover. For example,
D
ow
nl
oa
de
d
fr
om
i
nf
or
m
s.
or
g
by
[
13
4.
88
.7
7.
64
]
on
3
0
A
pr
il
2
01
8,
a
t
07
:0
5
. F
or
p
er
so
na
l
us
e
on
ly
, a
ll
r
ig
ht
s
re
se
rv
ed
.
Argote, Aven, and Kush: The Effects of Turnover on Group
Performance
204 Organization Science, 2018, vol. 29, no. 2, pp. 191–206, ©
2018 INFORMS
one prediction would be that a ring or circle network
would not perform as well as a hub-and-spoke net-
work when turnover occurred, because of the lower
centralization and correspondingly less clear coordi-
nation logic of the circle network. Another research
approach would be to identify networks, such as cer-
tain lattices, that do not maximize one network dimen-
sion and minimize another but that are, rather, inter-
mediate on both dimensions. Although not optimal for
either stable or turnover conditions, such a network
might perform better when there is uncertainty about
whether turnover will occur. Testing these predictions
is an interesting avenue for future research.
Our results suggest that environments where mem-
ber turnover commonly occurs might benefit from en-
couraging centralized communication networks. The
results of our mediation analyses provide further in-
sights into how groups might address membership
turnover, such as promoting communication, specif-
ically to new members. Just as assigning a facili-
tator in brainstorming sessions improves creativity
and efficiency (Sutton and Hargadon 1996), assign-
ing an incumbent group member to communicate
with new members and encourage them to con-
tribute might facilitate the integration of new mem-
bers even in decentralized communication settings.
Alternatively, structured processes or formal proce-
dures that facilitate contributions from all members
could also be effective in offsetting the negative effects
of membership turnover. Future research should exam-
ine additional mechanisms for onboarding new mem-
bers effectively and enabling them to contribute to
group performance.
Our results have implications for organizational
design and contradict the classic advice in the litera-
ture (e.g., see Burns and Stalker 1961) that organiza-
tions in dynamic environments should be organized in
an organic rather than hierarchical manner. Instead, our
study indicates that centralized or hierarchical groups
perform better in dynamic rather than in stable envi-
ronments but fully connected groups do not when the
dynamism is caused by membership change. Thus,
our results suggest that taking a fine-grained approach
to understanding the source of the dynamism will
advance organization design research as well as con-
tribute to a growing body of literature on the benefits
of certain forms of bureaucracy (Bunderson and Boum-
garden 2010).
In their seminal work, Burns and Stalker (1961)
argued that groups can cultivate either efficiency
through constrained, formal, or mandated structures or
creativity by permitting more autonomy and emergent
coordination. Centralized networks are rarely viewed
to be useful for innovation, as their limited pathways
reduce the interconnectedness and, thus, interactions
of unique idea holders, undermining innovation. How-
ever, limited structures may help provide the necessary
communication arrangement to share ideas when the
group’s membership is not stable. Our findings suggest
that turnover provides a means to introduce innovation
and creativity into mandated structures or centralized
networks. Thus, organizations may be able to gain the
benefits of efficiency while also increasing the innova-
tion capabilities of their teams by adopting centralized
structures coupled with the frequent rotation of team
members.
Acknowledgments
This research was presented at the 2015 Academy of Man-
agement meeting in Vancouver, Carnegie Mellon University,
Johns Hopkins University, Massachusetts Institute of Tech-
nology, the University of California at Santa Barbara, the
University of Pennsylvania, the University of Virginia, and
Washington University in St. Louis. The authors wish to
thank participants in these forums, especially Taya Cohen
and Anita Woolley, for their helpful feedback, and members
of the SCALE (Social Coordination Across Large Environ-
ments) project, especially Jim Herbsleb and Anita Sarma, for
their helpful advice. They would also like to thank senior edi-
tor Tiziana Casciaro and the reviewers for their very thought-
ful comments.
Endnotes
1Data from seven groups were dropped from the analysis. Two
groups failed to follow the experiment’s instructions, and five
groups contained members who had previously participated in
the
experiment.
2Eleven groups were inadvertently given 10 minutes instead of
5.
There was no main effect of this on new features, but a control
vari-
able was included in all predictions of new features (see the
notes to
Table 2).
3We used the R (version 3.1.3) package tnet (Opsahl 2012, ver-
sion 3.011) to convert the networks, and we used the R package
igraph (Csárdi and Nepusz 2006, version 0.7.1) to calculate
Freeman’s
(1979) centralization.
4Betweenness centralization, variance in how often a member
resides
on the shortest communication path between all possible pairs,
was
also used as a robustness check. Similar to degree
centralization,
betweenness centralization had a positive correlation with
central-
ization (0.79, p < 0.001).
5To aid in interpretation, we report coefficients for ordinary
least
squares models; however, all analyses involving errors and new
fea-
tures were repeated with Poisson regressions, a version of the
gen-
eralized linear model appropriate for a dependent variable that
is
a count. These Poisson regressions produced results very similar
to
those presented here.
6As a robustness check, we repeated these analyses using just
dyadic
communication frequency that occurred within the first six
minutes
of the programming task. Six minutes was the minimum time to
completion among all the groups on the programming task.
Focus-
ing on the first six minutes allowed us to explore variations in
dyadic
communication frequency without having to account for time to
complete the task. Results from these analyses were identical to
those using dyadic communication frequency across the entire
time
groups worked. The serial pathway in the moderated serial
media-
tion predicting errors was smaller (no turnover, B �−0.13; 95%
CIs:
D
ow
nl
oa
de
d
fr
om
i
nf
or
m
s.
or
g
by
[
13
4.
88
.7
7.
64
]
on
3
0
A
pr
il
2
01
8,
a
t
07
:0
5
. F
or
p
er
so
na
l
us
e
on
ly
, a
ll
r
ig
ht
s
re
se
rv
ed
.
Argote, Aven, and Kush: The Effects of Turnover on Group
Performance
Organization Science, 2018, vol. 29, no. 2, pp. 191–206, © 2018
INFORMS 205
−0.48, 0.05; turnover, B � −0.41; 95% CIs: −0.89, −0.16)
though still
significantly moderated by turnover (95% CIs: −0.71, −0.01).
7These analyses were repeated using communications from only
the
first five minutes of the task. Five minutes was the minimum
time
to completion for the feature-generating task. Results from
these
analyses are similar in pattern and direction to those reported
here,
indicating that the results are robust.
8The lowest possible network density in any fully connected
network
is always greater than the density for a perfectly centralized
network.
The formula for the density of a fully connected network with
the
lowest possible density would be N/(N · (N −1)/2), where N is
the
number of members of the group. The formula for the density of
a
perfectly centralized network is similar, but the numerator is N
− 1
instead of N. Thus, all fully connected groups have higher
density
than their perfectly centralized counterparts.
References
Ahuja MK, Galletta DF, Carley KM (2003) Individual centrality
and
performance in virtual R&D groups: An empirical study. Man-
agement Sci. 49(1):21–38.
Arrow H, McGrath JE (1995) Membership dynamics in groups
at
work: A theoretical framework. Res. Organ. Behav. 17:373–373.
Aven BL (2015) The paradox of corrupt networks: An analysis
of
organizational crime at Enron. Organ. Sci. 26(4):980–996.
Balkundi P, Harrison DA (2006) Ties, leaders, and time in
teams:
Strong inference about network structure’s effects on team via-
bility and performance. Acad. Management J. 49(1):49–68.
Bernard HR, Killworth P, Kronenfeld D, Sailer L (1984) The
problem
of informant accuracy: The validity of retrospective data.
Annual
Rev. Anthropol. 13:495–517.
Blau PM (1974) Presidential address: Parameters of social
structure.
Amer. Sociol. Rev. 39(5):615–635.
Borgatti SP, Cross R (2003) A relational view of information
seeking
and learning in social networks. Management Sci. 49(4):432–
445.
Borgatti SP, Everett MG, Johnson JC (2013) Analyzing Social
Networks
(Sage, London).
Bosch J, Bosch-Sijtsema P (2010) From integration to
composition: On
the impact of software product lines, global development and
ecosystems. J. Systems Software 83(1):67–76.
Bunderson JS, Boumgarden P (2010) Structure and learning in
self-
managed teams: Why “bureaucratic” teams can be better learn-
ers. Organ. Sci. 21(3):609–624.
Burns T, Stalker G (1961) The Management of Innovation
(Tavistock
Press, London).
Casciaro T, Barsade SG, Edmondson AC, Gibson CB,
Krackhardt D,
Labianca G (2015) The integration of psychological and net-
work perspectives in organizational scholarship. Organ. Sci.
26(4):1162–1176.
Chao GT, O’Leary-Kelly AM, Wolf S, Klein HJ, Gardner PD
(1994)
Organizational socialization: Its content and consequences.
J. Appl. Psych. 79(5):730–743.
Choi HS, Levine JM (2004) Minority influence in work teams:
The
impact of newcomers. J. Experiment. Soc. Psych. 40(2):273–
280.
Choi HS, Thompson L (2005) Old wine in a new bottle: Impact
of
membership change on group creativity. Organ. Behav. Human
Decision Processes 98(2):121–132.
Cohen J (1960) A coefficient of agreement for nominal scales.
Educa-
tional Psych. Measurement 20(1):37–46.
Croson R, Anand J, Agarwal R (2007) Using experiments in
corporate
strategy research. Eur. Management Rev. 4(3):173–181.
Csárdi G, Nepusz T (2006) The igraph software package for
complex network research. Interjournal, Complex Systems:
Manuscript 1695.
DeChurch LA, Mesmer-Magnus JR (2010) The cognitive
underpin-
nings of effective teamwork: A meta-analysis. J. Appl. Psych.
95(1):32–53.
Devine DJ, Clayton LD, Philips JL, Dunford BB, Melner SB
(1999)
Teams in organizations prevalence, characteristics, and
effective-
ness. Small Group Res. 30(6):678–711.
Faraj S, Sproull L (2000) Coordinating expertise in software
develop-
ment teams. Management Sci. 46(12):1554–1568.
Faucheux C, Mackenzie KD (1966) Task dependency of
organiza-
tional centrality: Its behavioral consequences. J. Experiment.
Soc.
Psych. 2(4):361–375.
Ferriani S, Cattani G, Baden-Fuller C (2009) The relational
antecedents of project-entrepreneurship: Network centrality,
team composition and project performance. Res. Policy 38(10):
1545–1558.
Freeman L (1979) Centrality in social networks conceptual
clarifica-
tion. Soc. Networks 1(3):215–239.
Friedkin NE (1981) The development of structure in random
net-
works: An analysis of the effects of increasing network density
on five measures of structure. Soc. Networks 3(1):41–52.
Gino F, Argote L, Miron-Spektor E, Todorova G (2010) First,
get your
feet wet: The effects of learning from direct and indirect experi-
ence on team creativity. Organ. Behav. Human Decision
Processes
111(2):102–115.
Gouldner AW (1954) Patterns of Industrial Bureaucracy (Free
Press,
New York).
Guetzkow H, Simon H (1955) The impact of certain
communica-
tion nets upon organization and performance in task-oriented
groups. Management Sci. 1(3):233–250.
Hancock JI, Allen DG, Bosco FA, McDaniel KR, Pierce CA
(2013)
Meta-analytic review of employee turnover as a predictor of
firm
performance. J. Management 39(3):573–603.
Hausknecht JP, Holwerda JA (2013) When does employee
turnover
matter? Dynamic member configurations, productive capacity,
and collective performance. Organ. Sci. 24(1):210–225.
Hayes AF (2013) Introduction to Mediation, Moderation, and
Conditional
Process Analysis: A Regression-Based Approach (Guilford
Press,
New York).
Hayes AF (2015) An index and test of linear moderated
mediation.
Multivariate Behavioral Res. 50(1):1–22.
Hollingshead AB (1998) Retrieval processes in transactive
memory
systems. J. Personality Soc. Psych. 74(3):659–671.
Katz N, Lazer D, Arrow H, Contractor N (2004) Network theory
and
small groups. Small Group Res. 35(3):307–332.
Kleinbaum AM, Stuart TE, Tushman ML (2013) Discretion
within
constraint: Homophily and structure in a formal organization.
Organ. Sci. 24(5):1316–1357.
Kruchten P (2008) What do software architects do? J. Systems
Software
81(12):2413–2416.
Kush J, Williamson C, Argote L (2012) Challenges and
opportuni-
ties for group learning and group learning researchers. Neale
MA, Mannix EA, eds. Looking Back, Moving Forward: A
Review of
Group and Team-Based Research, Research on Managing
Groups
and Teams, Vol. 15 (Emerald Group Publishing, Bingley, UK),
209–243.
Leavitt HJ (1951) Some effects of certain communication
patterns on
group performance. J. Abnormal Soc. Psych. 46(1):38–50.
Leavitt HJ (1996) The old days, hot groups and managers’ lib.
Admin.
Sci. Quart. 41(2):288–300.
LeBreton JM, Senter JL (2008) Answers to 20 questions about
inter-
rater reliability and interrater agreement. Organ. Res. Methods
11(4):815–852.
Lee J, Bachrach DG, Lewis K (2014) Social network ties,
transactive
memory, and performance in groups. Organ. Sci. 25(3):951–
967.
Levine JM, Choi HS, Moreland RL (2003) Newcomer
innovation in
work teams. Paulus P, Nijstad B, eds. Group Creativity: Innova-
tion Through Collaboration (Oxford University Press, New
York),
202–224.
Lewis K (2003) Measuring transactive memory systems in the
field:
Scale development and validation. J. Appl. Psych. 88(4):587–
604.
Lewis K, Herndon B (2011) Transactive memory systems: Cur-
rent issues and future research directions. Organ. Sci. 22(5):
1254–1265.
Lewis K, Belliveau M, Herndon B, Keller J (2007) Group
cognition,
membership change, and performance: Investigating the bene-
fits and detriments of collective knowledge. Organ. Behav.
Human
Decision Processes 103(2):159–178.
D
ow
nl
oa
de
d
fr
om
i
nf
or
m
s.
or
g
by
[
13
4.
88
.7
7.
64
]
on
3
0
A
pr
il
2
01
8,
a
t
07
:0
5
. F
or
p
er
so
na
l
us
e
on
ly
, a
ll
r
ig
ht
s
re
se
rv
ed
.
Argote, Aven, and Kush: The Effects of Turnover on Group
Performance
206 Organization Science, 2018, vol. 29, no. 2, pp. 191–206, ©
2018 INFORMS
Liang DW, Moreland RL, Argote L (1995) Group versus
individual
training and group performance: The mediating role of transac-
tive memory. Personality Soc. Psych. Bull. 21(4):384–393.
Manski CF (1993) Identification of endogenous social effects:
The
reflection problem. Rev. Econom. Stud. 60(3):531–542.
McGrath JE (1984) Groups: Interaction and Performance
(Prentice-Hall,
Englewood Cliffs, NJ).
Moreland RL, Myaskovsky L (2000) Exploring the performance
bene-
fits of group training: Transactive memory or improved commu-
nication? Organ. Behav. Human Decision Processes 82(1):117–
133.
Moreland RL, Argote L, Krishnan R (1996) Socially shared
cognition
at work: Transactive memory and group performance. Nye JL,
Brower AM, eds. What’s Social About Social Cognition?
Research on
Socially Shared Cognition in Small Groups (Sage, Thousand
Oaks,
CA), 57–84.
Morrison EW (2002) Newcomers’ relationships: The role of
social
network ties during socialization. Acad. Management J. 45(6):
1149–1160.
Murphy KR, Myors B (1998) Statistical Power Analysis: A
Simple
and General Model for Traditional and Modern Hypothesis
Tests
(Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Mahwah, NJ).
Opsahl T (2012) tnet: Software for analysis of weighted, two-
mode,
and longitudinal networks. Accessed July 1, 2013, https://cran.r
-project.org/package�tnet.
Preacher KJ, Kelley K (2011) Effect size measures for
mediation mod-
els: Quantitative strategies for communicating indirect effects.
Psych. Methods 16(2):93–115.
Ren Y, Argote L (2011) Transactive memory systems 1985–
2010: An
integrative framework of key dimensions, antecedents, and con-
sequences. Acad. Management Ann. 5(1):189–229.
Rulke DL, Galaskiewicz J (2000) Distribution of knowledge,
group
network structure, and group performance. Management Sci.
46(5):612–625.
Sasovova Z, Mehra A, Borgatti SP, Schippers MC (2010)
Network
churn: The effects of self-monitoring personality on brokerage
dynamics. Admin. Sci. Quart. 55(4):639–670.
Spector PE (1994) Using self-report questionnaires in OB
research:
A comment on the use of a controversial method. J. Organ.
Behav.
15(5):385–392.
Sutton RI, Hargadon A (1996) Brainstorming groups in context:
Effectiveness in a product design firm. Admin. Sci. Quart.
41(4):
685–718.
Tsay J, Dabbish L, Herbsleb J (2014) Let’s talk about it:
Evaluating
contributions through discussion in GitHub. Proc. 22nd ACM
SIGSOFT Internat. Sympos. Foundations Software Engrg.
(ACM,
New York), 144–154.
Uzzi B, Spiro J (2005) Collaboration and creativity: The small
world
problem. Amer. J. Sociol. 111(2):447–504.
Weber M (1947) Henderson AM, Parsons T, eds. The Theory of
Economic and Social Organization (Oxford University Press,
New York).
Wegner DM (1987) Transactive memory: A contemporary
analysis of
the group mind. Mullen B, Goethals GR, eds. Theories of Group
Behavior (Springer-Verlag, New York), 185–208.
Wells WP, Pelz DC (1966) Scientists in Organizations (John
Wiley &
Sons, New York).
Yuan YC, Fulk J, Monge PR, Contractor N (2010) Expertise
direc-
tory development, shared task interdependence, and strength of
communication network ties as multilevel predictors of exper-
tise exchange in transactive memory work groups. Comm. Res.
37(1):20–47.
Linda Argote is the David M. and Barbara A. Kirr Pro-
fessor of Organizational Behavior and Theory at the Tep-
per School of Business at Carnegie Mellon University, where
she directs the Center for Organizational Learning, Innova-
tion and Knowledge. Her research focuses on organizational
learning, organizational memory, knowledge transfer, social
networks, and group processes and performance. She served
as editor-in-chief of Organization Science from 2004 to 2010.
Brandy L. Aven is an associate professor of organiza-
tional behavior and theory at the Tepper School of Busi-
ness, Carnegie Mellon University. She received her Ph.D. in
sociology from Stanford University. Her research interests
include market and organizational fraud, entrepreneurship,
and social networks.
Jonathan Kush is an assistant professor of management
at the Charlton College of Business, University of Mas-
sachusetts Dartmouth. He received his Ph.D. from the Tepper
School of Business, Carnegie Mellon University. His research
interests include group collaboration, collective cognition,
social networks, and their effects on group performance.
D
ow
nl
oa
de
d
fr
om
i
nf
or
m
s.
or
g
by
[
13
4.
88
.7
7.
64
]
on
3
0
A
pr
il
2
01
8,
a
t
07
:0
5
. F
or
p
er
so
na
l
us
e
on
ly
, a
ll
r
ig
ht
s
re
se
rv
ed
.
https://cran.r-project.org/package=tnet
https://cran.r-project.org/package=tnet
Two Types of Privilege
According to McIntosh, the first type of privilege is based on
“unearned entitlements,” which are things of value that all
people should have, such as feeling safe or working in a place
where they are valued and accepted for what they have to offer.
When an unearned entitlement is restricted to certain groups,
however, it becomesPage 22 an “unearned advantage,” a form of
privilege.
Sometimes it’s possible to do away with unearned advantages
without anyone losing out. When a workplace changes so that
everyone is valued, for example, the unearned entitlement is
available to all and, as such, is no longer a form of privilege. In
other cases, however, unearned advantage gives dominant
groups a competitive edge they are reluctant to acknowledge,
much less give up. This can be especially true of men and
whites who lack the advantages of social class and know all too
well how hard it is to improve their lives and hang on to what
they’ve managed to achieve. This can blind them, however, to
the fact that the cultural valuing of whiteness and maleness over
color and femaleness loads the odds in their favor in most
situations that involve evaluations of credibility or competence.
To lose that advantage would greatly increase the amount of
competition for white men, who are a shrinking minority of the
U.S. population.
The second form of privilege, “conferred dominance,” goes a
step further by giving one group power over another. The
common pattern of men controlling conversations with women,
for example, is grounded in a cultural assumption that men are
superior and supposed to dominate women. An adolescent boy
who appears too willing to defer to his mother risks being called
a “mama’s boy,” in the same way that a husband who appears in
any way controlled by his wife can be labeled “henpecked” (or
worse). The counterpart for girls carries no such stigma:
“Daddy’s girl” is not considered an insult in this culture, and
there are no insulting terms for a wife who is under the control
of her husband.
Conferred dominance also manifests itself in white privilege. In
his book The Rage of a Privileged Class, for example, Ellis
Cose tells the story of an African American lawyer, a partner in
a large firm, who goes to the office one Saturday morning and is
confronted by a recently hired young white attorney, who has
never met the partner.
“Can I help you?” says the white man pointedly.
The partner shakes his head and tries to pass, but the white man
steps in his way and repeats what is now a challenge to the
man’s presence in the building: “Can I help you?” Only then
does the partner reveal his identity to the young man, who in
turn steps aside to let him pass. The white man has no reason to
assume the right to control the older man standing before him,
except the reason provided by the cultural assumption of white
dominance, which can overridePage 23 any class advantage a
person of color might have.19
The milder forms of unearned advantage are usually the first to
change because they are the easiest for privileged groups to
give up. In the decades leading up to the election of Barack
Obama as president, for example, national surveys showed a
steady decline in the percentage of whites holding racist
attitudes toward blacks. This trend is reflected in diversity
programs that focus on appreciating or tolerating differences—
in other words, extending unearned entitlements to everyone
instead of the dominant group alone.
It is much harder, however, to do something about power and
the unequal distribution of wealth, income, and other resources
and rewards. This is why issues of conferred dominance and the
stronger forms of unearned advantage get much less attention,
and why, when these issues are raised, they can provoke hostile
defensiveness and denial. Perhaps more than any other factor,
the reluctance to recognize the more serious and entrenched
forms of privilege is why most diversity programs serve as little
more than a distraction and produce limited and short-lived
results.20
Privilege as Paradox
Aparadox of privilege is that even though it is received by and
benefits individuals, access to it has nothing to do with who
they are as people. Male privilege, for example, is more
about male people than it is about male people, making me
eligible when people assign me to that category, which they can
do without knowing a single other thing about me.
This means that actually being male is not required in order to
receive male privilege. The film, Shakespeare in Love, for
example, is set in Elizabethan England, where acting on the
stage was reserved for men. The character of Viola wants to act
This article was downloaded by [134.88.77.64] On 30 April 20.docx
This article was downloaded by [134.88.77.64] On 30 April 20.docx
This article was downloaded by [134.88.77.64] On 30 April 20.docx
This article was downloaded by [134.88.77.64] On 30 April 20.docx
This article was downloaded by [134.88.77.64] On 30 April 20.docx
This article was downloaded by [134.88.77.64] On 30 April 20.docx
This article was downloaded by [134.88.77.64] On 30 April 20.docx
This article was downloaded by [134.88.77.64] On 30 April 20.docx
This article was downloaded by [134.88.77.64] On 30 April 20.docx
This article was downloaded by [134.88.77.64] On 30 April 20.docx
This article was downloaded by [134.88.77.64] On 30 April 20.docx
This article was downloaded by [134.88.77.64] On 30 April 20.docx
This article was downloaded by [134.88.77.64] On 30 April 20.docx

More Related Content

Similar to This article was downloaded by [134.88.77.64] On 30 April 20.docx

AN EXTENDED HYBRID RECOMMENDER SYSTEM BASED ON ASSOCIATION RULES MINING IN DI...
AN EXTENDED HYBRID RECOMMENDER SYSTEM BASED ON ASSOCIATION RULES MINING IN DI...AN EXTENDED HYBRID RECOMMENDER SYSTEM BASED ON ASSOCIATION RULES MINING IN DI...
AN EXTENDED HYBRID RECOMMENDER SYSTEM BASED ON ASSOCIATION RULES MINING IN DI...csandit
 
An Sna-Bi Based System for Evaluating Virtual Teams: A Software Development P...
An Sna-Bi Based System for Evaluating Virtual Teams: A Software Development P...An Sna-Bi Based System for Evaluating Virtual Teams: A Software Development P...
An Sna-Bi Based System for Evaluating Virtual Teams: A Software Development P...ijcsit
 
Least Cost Influence by Mapping Online Social Networks
Least Cost Influence by Mapping Online Social Networks Least Cost Influence by Mapping Online Social Networks
Least Cost Influence by Mapping Online Social Networks paperpublications3
 
9 A Preliminary Theory of Interorganizational Network Effectivenes.docx
9 A Preliminary Theory of Interorganizational Network Effectivenes.docx9 A Preliminary Theory of Interorganizational Network Effectivenes.docx
9 A Preliminary Theory of Interorganizational Network Effectivenes.docxransayo
 
Finding The Voice of A Virtual Community of Practice
Finding The Voice of A Virtual Community of PracticeFinding The Voice of A Virtual Community of Practice
Finding The Voice of A Virtual Community of PracticeConnie White
 
Avoiding Anonymous Users in Multiple Social Media Networks (SMN)
Avoiding Anonymous Users in Multiple Social Media Networks (SMN)Avoiding Anonymous Users in Multiple Social Media Networks (SMN)
Avoiding Anonymous Users in Multiple Social Media Networks (SMN)paperpublications3
 
IRJET- Social Network Mental Disorders Detection Via Online Social Media Mining
IRJET- Social Network Mental Disorders Detection Via Online Social Media MiningIRJET- Social Network Mental Disorders Detection Via Online Social Media Mining
IRJET- Social Network Mental Disorders Detection Via Online Social Media MiningIRJET Journal
 
Church Choir Online Communication and Music Recording and Streaming System
Church Choir Online Communication and Music Recording and Streaming SystemChurch Choir Online Communication and Music Recording and Streaming System
Church Choir Online Communication and Music Recording and Streaming SystemEditor IJCATR
 
INTRA- AND INTER- ‎ORGANIZATIONAL ‎SYSTEMS
INTRA- AND INTER- ‎ORGANIZATIONAL ‎SYSTEMSINTRA- AND INTER- ‎ORGANIZATIONAL ‎SYSTEMS
INTRA- AND INTER- ‎ORGANIZATIONAL ‎SYSTEMSLibcorpio
 
Presentation November Trier
Presentation November  TrierPresentation November  Trier
Presentation November TrierHeks1956
 
Using SNA for organisational and personal improvement
Using SNA for organisational and personal improvementUsing SNA for organisational and personal improvement
Using SNA for organisational and personal improvementactkm
 
INTELLIGENT SOCIAL NETWORKS MODEL BASED ON SEMANTIC TAG RANKING
INTELLIGENT SOCIAL NETWORKS MODEL BASED ON SEMANTIC TAG RANKINGINTELLIGENT SOCIAL NETWORKS MODEL BASED ON SEMANTIC TAG RANKING
INTELLIGENT SOCIAL NETWORKS MODEL BASED ON SEMANTIC TAG RANKINGdannyijwest
 
INTELLIGENT SOCIAL NETWORKS MODEL BASED ON SEMANTIC TAG RANKING
INTELLIGENT SOCIAL NETWORKS MODEL BASED  ON SEMANTIC TAG RANKINGINTELLIGENT SOCIAL NETWORKS MODEL BASED  ON SEMANTIC TAG RANKING
INTELLIGENT SOCIAL NETWORKS MODEL BASED ON SEMANTIC TAG RANKINGdannyijwest
 
INTELLIGENT SOCIAL NETWORKS MODEL BASED ON SEMANTIC TAG RANKING
INTELLIGENT SOCIAL NETWORKS MODEL BASED ON SEMANTIC TAG RANKINGINTELLIGENT SOCIAL NETWORKS MODEL BASED ON SEMANTIC TAG RANKING
INTELLIGENT SOCIAL NETWORKS MODEL BASED ON SEMANTIC TAG RANKINGIJwest
 
Current trends of opinion mining and sentiment analysis in social networks
Current trends of opinion mining and sentiment analysis in social networksCurrent trends of opinion mining and sentiment analysis in social networks
Current trends of opinion mining and sentiment analysis in social networkseSAT Publishing House
 
Organizational Overlap on Social Networks and its Applications
Organizational Overlap on Social Networks and its ApplicationsOrganizational Overlap on Social Networks and its Applications
Organizational Overlap on Social Networks and its ApplicationsSam Shah
 
Ona For Community Roundtable
Ona For Community RoundtableOna For Community Roundtable
Ona For Community RoundtablePatti Anklam
 
Asq Worcester Network Power Public
Asq Worcester Network Power PublicAsq Worcester Network Power Public
Asq Worcester Network Power PublicMaya Townsend
 
Online Data Preprocessing: A Case Study Approach
Online Data Preprocessing: A Case Study ApproachOnline Data Preprocessing: A Case Study Approach
Online Data Preprocessing: A Case Study ApproachIJECEIAES
 
Human Due Diligence Methodology
Human Due Diligence MethodologyHuman Due Diligence Methodology
Human Due Diligence MethodologyManofthetaste
 

Similar to This article was downloaded by [134.88.77.64] On 30 April 20.docx (20)

AN EXTENDED HYBRID RECOMMENDER SYSTEM BASED ON ASSOCIATION RULES MINING IN DI...
AN EXTENDED HYBRID RECOMMENDER SYSTEM BASED ON ASSOCIATION RULES MINING IN DI...AN EXTENDED HYBRID RECOMMENDER SYSTEM BASED ON ASSOCIATION RULES MINING IN DI...
AN EXTENDED HYBRID RECOMMENDER SYSTEM BASED ON ASSOCIATION RULES MINING IN DI...
 
An Sna-Bi Based System for Evaluating Virtual Teams: A Software Development P...
An Sna-Bi Based System for Evaluating Virtual Teams: A Software Development P...An Sna-Bi Based System for Evaluating Virtual Teams: A Software Development P...
An Sna-Bi Based System for Evaluating Virtual Teams: A Software Development P...
 
Least Cost Influence by Mapping Online Social Networks
Least Cost Influence by Mapping Online Social Networks Least Cost Influence by Mapping Online Social Networks
Least Cost Influence by Mapping Online Social Networks
 
9 A Preliminary Theory of Interorganizational Network Effectivenes.docx
9 A Preliminary Theory of Interorganizational Network Effectivenes.docx9 A Preliminary Theory of Interorganizational Network Effectivenes.docx
9 A Preliminary Theory of Interorganizational Network Effectivenes.docx
 
Finding The Voice of A Virtual Community of Practice
Finding The Voice of A Virtual Community of PracticeFinding The Voice of A Virtual Community of Practice
Finding The Voice of A Virtual Community of Practice
 
Avoiding Anonymous Users in Multiple Social Media Networks (SMN)
Avoiding Anonymous Users in Multiple Social Media Networks (SMN)Avoiding Anonymous Users in Multiple Social Media Networks (SMN)
Avoiding Anonymous Users in Multiple Social Media Networks (SMN)
 
IRJET- Social Network Mental Disorders Detection Via Online Social Media Mining
IRJET- Social Network Mental Disorders Detection Via Online Social Media MiningIRJET- Social Network Mental Disorders Detection Via Online Social Media Mining
IRJET- Social Network Mental Disorders Detection Via Online Social Media Mining
 
Church Choir Online Communication and Music Recording and Streaming System
Church Choir Online Communication and Music Recording and Streaming SystemChurch Choir Online Communication and Music Recording and Streaming System
Church Choir Online Communication and Music Recording and Streaming System
 
INTRA- AND INTER- ‎ORGANIZATIONAL ‎SYSTEMS
INTRA- AND INTER- ‎ORGANIZATIONAL ‎SYSTEMSINTRA- AND INTER- ‎ORGANIZATIONAL ‎SYSTEMS
INTRA- AND INTER- ‎ORGANIZATIONAL ‎SYSTEMS
 
Presentation November Trier
Presentation November  TrierPresentation November  Trier
Presentation November Trier
 
Using SNA for organisational and personal improvement
Using SNA for organisational and personal improvementUsing SNA for organisational and personal improvement
Using SNA for organisational and personal improvement
 
INTELLIGENT SOCIAL NETWORKS MODEL BASED ON SEMANTIC TAG RANKING
INTELLIGENT SOCIAL NETWORKS MODEL BASED ON SEMANTIC TAG RANKINGINTELLIGENT SOCIAL NETWORKS MODEL BASED ON SEMANTIC TAG RANKING
INTELLIGENT SOCIAL NETWORKS MODEL BASED ON SEMANTIC TAG RANKING
 
INTELLIGENT SOCIAL NETWORKS MODEL BASED ON SEMANTIC TAG RANKING
INTELLIGENT SOCIAL NETWORKS MODEL BASED  ON SEMANTIC TAG RANKINGINTELLIGENT SOCIAL NETWORKS MODEL BASED  ON SEMANTIC TAG RANKING
INTELLIGENT SOCIAL NETWORKS MODEL BASED ON SEMANTIC TAG RANKING
 
INTELLIGENT SOCIAL NETWORKS MODEL BASED ON SEMANTIC TAG RANKING
INTELLIGENT SOCIAL NETWORKS MODEL BASED ON SEMANTIC TAG RANKINGINTELLIGENT SOCIAL NETWORKS MODEL BASED ON SEMANTIC TAG RANKING
INTELLIGENT SOCIAL NETWORKS MODEL BASED ON SEMANTIC TAG RANKING
 
Current trends of opinion mining and sentiment analysis in social networks
Current trends of opinion mining and sentiment analysis in social networksCurrent trends of opinion mining and sentiment analysis in social networks
Current trends of opinion mining and sentiment analysis in social networks
 
Organizational Overlap on Social Networks and its Applications
Organizational Overlap on Social Networks and its ApplicationsOrganizational Overlap on Social Networks and its Applications
Organizational Overlap on Social Networks and its Applications
 
Ona For Community Roundtable
Ona For Community RoundtableOna For Community Roundtable
Ona For Community Roundtable
 
Asq Worcester Network Power Public
Asq Worcester Network Power PublicAsq Worcester Network Power Public
Asq Worcester Network Power Public
 
Online Data Preprocessing: A Case Study Approach
Online Data Preprocessing: A Case Study ApproachOnline Data Preprocessing: A Case Study Approach
Online Data Preprocessing: A Case Study Approach
 
Human Due Diligence Methodology
Human Due Diligence MethodologyHuman Due Diligence Methodology
Human Due Diligence Methodology
 

More from howardh5

This assessment is designed to evaluate your proficiency in the foll.docx
This assessment is designed to evaluate your proficiency in the foll.docxThis assessment is designed to evaluate your proficiency in the foll.docx
This assessment is designed to evaluate your proficiency in the foll.docxhowardh5
 
This assessment has three-parts.  Click each of the items below to.docx
This assessment has three-parts.  Click each of the items below to.docxThis assessment has three-parts.  Click each of the items below to.docx
This assessment has three-parts.  Click each of the items below to.docxhowardh5
 
This Assessment constitutes a Performance Task in which you are as.docx
This Assessment constitutes a Performance Task in which you are as.docxThis Assessment constitutes a Performance Task in which you are as.docx
This Assessment constitutes a Performance Task in which you are as.docxhowardh5
 
This assessment challenges students to collect, compare and contrast.docx
This assessment challenges students to collect, compare and contrast.docxThis assessment challenges students to collect, compare and contrast.docx
This assessment challenges students to collect, compare and contrast.docxhowardh5
 
This Assessment constitutes a Performance Task in which you are aske.docx
This Assessment constitutes a Performance Task in which you are aske.docxThis Assessment constitutes a Performance Task in which you are aske.docx
This Assessment constitutes a Performance Task in which you are aske.docxhowardh5
 
This Asignment has audio (Dnt knw how to post AUDIO but if u have .docx
This Asignment has audio (Dnt knw how to post AUDIO but if u have .docxThis Asignment has audio (Dnt knw how to post AUDIO but if u have .docx
This Asignment has audio (Dnt knw how to post AUDIO but if u have .docxhowardh5
 
This article was downloaded by[Florida International Universi.docx
This article was downloaded by[Florida International Universi.docxThis article was downloaded by[Florida International Universi.docx
This article was downloaded by[Florida International Universi.docxhowardh5
 
This article was downloaded by [Carnegie Mellon University].docx
This article was downloaded by [Carnegie Mellon University].docxThis article was downloaded by [Carnegie Mellon University].docx
This article was downloaded by [Carnegie Mellon University].docxhowardh5
 
This article was downloaded by [University of California, Ber.docx
This article was downloaded by [University of California, Ber.docxThis article was downloaded by [University of California, Ber.docx
This article was downloaded by [University of California, Ber.docxhowardh5
 
This article was downloaded by [71.212.37.56]On 18 August .docx
This article was downloaded by [71.212.37.56]On 18 August .docxThis article was downloaded by [71.212.37.56]On 18 August .docx
This article was downloaded by [71.212.37.56]On 18 August .docxhowardh5
 
This article states that people of color have a high death rate in t.docx
This article states that people of color have a high death rate in t.docxThis article states that people of color have a high death rate in t.docx
This article states that people of color have a high death rate in t.docxhowardh5
 
This article needs to combine four articles to write my own views on.docx
This article needs to combine four articles to write my own views on.docxThis article needs to combine four articles to write my own views on.docx
This article needs to combine four articles to write my own views on.docxhowardh5
 
This article deals basically with the dynamic environment of todays.docx
This article deals basically with the dynamic environment of todays.docxThis article deals basically with the dynamic environment of todays.docx
This article deals basically with the dynamic environment of todays.docxhowardh5
 
This article appeared in a journal published by Elsevier. The .docx
This article appeared in a journal published by Elsevier. The .docxThis article appeared in a journal published by Elsevier. The .docx
This article appeared in a journal published by Elsevier. The .docxhowardh5
 
This are two separate questions with 100 words each.1)Currentl.docx
This are two separate questions with 100 words each.1)Currentl.docxThis are two separate questions with 100 words each.1)Currentl.docx
This are two separate questions with 100 words each.1)Currentl.docxhowardh5
 
This age of improvement” held both a collective (public) component .docx
This age of improvement” held both a collective (public) component .docxThis age of improvement” held both a collective (public) component .docx
This age of improvement” held both a collective (public) component .docxhowardh5
 
This assignment has three partsKnowing that global busine.docx
This assignment has three partsKnowing that global busine.docxThis assignment has three partsKnowing that global busine.docx
This assignment has three partsKnowing that global busine.docxhowardh5
 
This assignment has many benefits. First, it requires a search o.docx
This assignment has many benefits. First, it requires a search o.docxThis assignment has many benefits. First, it requires a search o.docx
This assignment has many benefits. First, it requires a search o.docxhowardh5
 
This assignment has 5 parts.Collaboration in a business enviro.docx
This assignment has 5 parts.Collaboration in a business enviro.docxThis assignment has 5 parts.Collaboration in a business enviro.docx
This assignment has 5 parts.Collaboration in a business enviro.docxhowardh5
 
This assignment has one of two options. Please open and view the ass.docx
This assignment has one of two options. Please open and view the ass.docxThis assignment has one of two options. Please open and view the ass.docx
This assignment has one of two options. Please open and view the ass.docxhowardh5
 

More from howardh5 (20)

This assessment is designed to evaluate your proficiency in the foll.docx
This assessment is designed to evaluate your proficiency in the foll.docxThis assessment is designed to evaluate your proficiency in the foll.docx
This assessment is designed to evaluate your proficiency in the foll.docx
 
This assessment has three-parts.  Click each of the items below to.docx
This assessment has three-parts.  Click each of the items below to.docxThis assessment has three-parts.  Click each of the items below to.docx
This assessment has three-parts.  Click each of the items below to.docx
 
This Assessment constitutes a Performance Task in which you are as.docx
This Assessment constitutes a Performance Task in which you are as.docxThis Assessment constitutes a Performance Task in which you are as.docx
This Assessment constitutes a Performance Task in which you are as.docx
 
This assessment challenges students to collect, compare and contrast.docx
This assessment challenges students to collect, compare and contrast.docxThis assessment challenges students to collect, compare and contrast.docx
This assessment challenges students to collect, compare and contrast.docx
 
This Assessment constitutes a Performance Task in which you are aske.docx
This Assessment constitutes a Performance Task in which you are aske.docxThis Assessment constitutes a Performance Task in which you are aske.docx
This Assessment constitutes a Performance Task in which you are aske.docx
 
This Asignment has audio (Dnt knw how to post AUDIO but if u have .docx
This Asignment has audio (Dnt knw how to post AUDIO but if u have .docxThis Asignment has audio (Dnt knw how to post AUDIO but if u have .docx
This Asignment has audio (Dnt knw how to post AUDIO but if u have .docx
 
This article was downloaded by[Florida International Universi.docx
This article was downloaded by[Florida International Universi.docxThis article was downloaded by[Florida International Universi.docx
This article was downloaded by[Florida International Universi.docx
 
This article was downloaded by [Carnegie Mellon University].docx
This article was downloaded by [Carnegie Mellon University].docxThis article was downloaded by [Carnegie Mellon University].docx
This article was downloaded by [Carnegie Mellon University].docx
 
This article was downloaded by [University of California, Ber.docx
This article was downloaded by [University of California, Ber.docxThis article was downloaded by [University of California, Ber.docx
This article was downloaded by [University of California, Ber.docx
 
This article was downloaded by [71.212.37.56]On 18 August .docx
This article was downloaded by [71.212.37.56]On 18 August .docxThis article was downloaded by [71.212.37.56]On 18 August .docx
This article was downloaded by [71.212.37.56]On 18 August .docx
 
This article states that people of color have a high death rate in t.docx
This article states that people of color have a high death rate in t.docxThis article states that people of color have a high death rate in t.docx
This article states that people of color have a high death rate in t.docx
 
This article needs to combine four articles to write my own views on.docx
This article needs to combine four articles to write my own views on.docxThis article needs to combine four articles to write my own views on.docx
This article needs to combine four articles to write my own views on.docx
 
This article deals basically with the dynamic environment of todays.docx
This article deals basically with the dynamic environment of todays.docxThis article deals basically with the dynamic environment of todays.docx
This article deals basically with the dynamic environment of todays.docx
 
This article appeared in a journal published by Elsevier. The .docx
This article appeared in a journal published by Elsevier. The .docxThis article appeared in a journal published by Elsevier. The .docx
This article appeared in a journal published by Elsevier. The .docx
 
This are two separate questions with 100 words each.1)Currentl.docx
This are two separate questions with 100 words each.1)Currentl.docxThis are two separate questions with 100 words each.1)Currentl.docx
This are two separate questions with 100 words each.1)Currentl.docx
 
This age of improvement” held both a collective (public) component .docx
This age of improvement” held both a collective (public) component .docxThis age of improvement” held both a collective (public) component .docx
This age of improvement” held both a collective (public) component .docx
 
This assignment has three partsKnowing that global busine.docx
This assignment has three partsKnowing that global busine.docxThis assignment has three partsKnowing that global busine.docx
This assignment has three partsKnowing that global busine.docx
 
This assignment has many benefits. First, it requires a search o.docx
This assignment has many benefits. First, it requires a search o.docxThis assignment has many benefits. First, it requires a search o.docx
This assignment has many benefits. First, it requires a search o.docx
 
This assignment has 5 parts.Collaboration in a business enviro.docx
This assignment has 5 parts.Collaboration in a business enviro.docxThis assignment has 5 parts.Collaboration in a business enviro.docx
This assignment has 5 parts.Collaboration in a business enviro.docx
 
This assignment has one of two options. Please open and view the ass.docx
This assignment has one of two options. Please open and view the ass.docxThis assignment has one of two options. Please open and view the ass.docx
This assignment has one of two options. Please open and view the ass.docx
 

Recently uploaded

Application orientated numerical on hev.ppt
Application orientated numerical on hev.pptApplication orientated numerical on hev.ppt
Application orientated numerical on hev.pptRamjanShidvankar
 
Basic Civil Engineering first year Notes- Chapter 4 Building.pptx
Basic Civil Engineering first year Notes- Chapter 4 Building.pptxBasic Civil Engineering first year Notes- Chapter 4 Building.pptx
Basic Civil Engineering first year Notes- Chapter 4 Building.pptxDenish Jangid
 
The basics of sentences session 3pptx.pptx
The basics of sentences session 3pptx.pptxThe basics of sentences session 3pptx.pptx
The basics of sentences session 3pptx.pptxheathfieldcps1
 
UGC NET Paper 1 Mathematical Reasoning & Aptitude.pdf
UGC NET Paper 1 Mathematical Reasoning & Aptitude.pdfUGC NET Paper 1 Mathematical Reasoning & Aptitude.pdf
UGC NET Paper 1 Mathematical Reasoning & Aptitude.pdfNirmal Dwivedi
 
Single or Multiple melodic lines structure
Single or Multiple melodic lines structureSingle or Multiple melodic lines structure
Single or Multiple melodic lines structuredhanjurrannsibayan2
 
Food safety_Challenges food safety laboratories_.pdf
Food safety_Challenges food safety laboratories_.pdfFood safety_Challenges food safety laboratories_.pdf
Food safety_Challenges food safety laboratories_.pdfSherif Taha
 
Key note speaker Neum_Admir Softic_ENG.pdf
Key note speaker Neum_Admir Softic_ENG.pdfKey note speaker Neum_Admir Softic_ENG.pdf
Key note speaker Neum_Admir Softic_ENG.pdfAdmir Softic
 
Wellbeing inclusion and digital dystopias.pptx
Wellbeing inclusion and digital dystopias.pptxWellbeing inclusion and digital dystopias.pptx
Wellbeing inclusion and digital dystopias.pptxJisc
 
2024-NATIONAL-LEARNING-CAMP-AND-OTHER.pptx
2024-NATIONAL-LEARNING-CAMP-AND-OTHER.pptx2024-NATIONAL-LEARNING-CAMP-AND-OTHER.pptx
2024-NATIONAL-LEARNING-CAMP-AND-OTHER.pptxMaritesTamaniVerdade
 
How to Give a Domain for a Field in Odoo 17
How to Give a Domain for a Field in Odoo 17How to Give a Domain for a Field in Odoo 17
How to Give a Domain for a Field in Odoo 17Celine George
 
FSB Advising Checklist - Orientation 2024
FSB Advising Checklist - Orientation 2024FSB Advising Checklist - Orientation 2024
FSB Advising Checklist - Orientation 2024Elizabeth Walsh
 
HMCS Vancouver Pre-Deployment Brief - May 2024 (Web Version).pptx
HMCS Vancouver Pre-Deployment Brief - May 2024 (Web Version).pptxHMCS Vancouver Pre-Deployment Brief - May 2024 (Web Version).pptx
HMCS Vancouver Pre-Deployment Brief - May 2024 (Web Version).pptxmarlenawright1
 
COMMUNICATING NEGATIVE NEWS - APPROACHES .pptx
COMMUNICATING NEGATIVE NEWS - APPROACHES .pptxCOMMUNICATING NEGATIVE NEWS - APPROACHES .pptx
COMMUNICATING NEGATIVE NEWS - APPROACHES .pptxannathomasp01
 
Graduate Outcomes Presentation Slides - English
Graduate Outcomes Presentation Slides - EnglishGraduate Outcomes Presentation Slides - English
Graduate Outcomes Presentation Slides - Englishneillewis46
 
Holdier Curriculum Vitae (April 2024).pdf
Holdier Curriculum Vitae (April 2024).pdfHoldier Curriculum Vitae (April 2024).pdf
Holdier Curriculum Vitae (April 2024).pdfagholdier
 
Micro-Scholarship, What it is, How can it help me.pdf
Micro-Scholarship, What it is, How can it help me.pdfMicro-Scholarship, What it is, How can it help me.pdf
Micro-Scholarship, What it is, How can it help me.pdfPoh-Sun Goh
 
Accessible Digital Futures project (20/03/2024)
Accessible Digital Futures project (20/03/2024)Accessible Digital Futures project (20/03/2024)
Accessible Digital Futures project (20/03/2024)Jisc
 
HMCS Max Bernays Pre-Deployment Brief (May 2024).pptx
HMCS Max Bernays Pre-Deployment Brief (May 2024).pptxHMCS Max Bernays Pre-Deployment Brief (May 2024).pptx
HMCS Max Bernays Pre-Deployment Brief (May 2024).pptxEsquimalt MFRC
 
NO1 Top Black Magic Specialist In Lahore Black magic In Pakistan Kala Ilam Ex...
NO1 Top Black Magic Specialist In Lahore Black magic In Pakistan Kala Ilam Ex...NO1 Top Black Magic Specialist In Lahore Black magic In Pakistan Kala Ilam Ex...
NO1 Top Black Magic Specialist In Lahore Black magic In Pakistan Kala Ilam Ex...Amil baba
 
Beyond_Borders_Understanding_Anime_and_Manga_Fandom_A_Comprehensive_Audience_...
Beyond_Borders_Understanding_Anime_and_Manga_Fandom_A_Comprehensive_Audience_...Beyond_Borders_Understanding_Anime_and_Manga_Fandom_A_Comprehensive_Audience_...
Beyond_Borders_Understanding_Anime_and_Manga_Fandom_A_Comprehensive_Audience_...Pooja Bhuva
 

Recently uploaded (20)

Application orientated numerical on hev.ppt
Application orientated numerical on hev.pptApplication orientated numerical on hev.ppt
Application orientated numerical on hev.ppt
 
Basic Civil Engineering first year Notes- Chapter 4 Building.pptx
Basic Civil Engineering first year Notes- Chapter 4 Building.pptxBasic Civil Engineering first year Notes- Chapter 4 Building.pptx
Basic Civil Engineering first year Notes- Chapter 4 Building.pptx
 
The basics of sentences session 3pptx.pptx
The basics of sentences session 3pptx.pptxThe basics of sentences session 3pptx.pptx
The basics of sentences session 3pptx.pptx
 
UGC NET Paper 1 Mathematical Reasoning & Aptitude.pdf
UGC NET Paper 1 Mathematical Reasoning & Aptitude.pdfUGC NET Paper 1 Mathematical Reasoning & Aptitude.pdf
UGC NET Paper 1 Mathematical Reasoning & Aptitude.pdf
 
Single or Multiple melodic lines structure
Single or Multiple melodic lines structureSingle or Multiple melodic lines structure
Single or Multiple melodic lines structure
 
Food safety_Challenges food safety laboratories_.pdf
Food safety_Challenges food safety laboratories_.pdfFood safety_Challenges food safety laboratories_.pdf
Food safety_Challenges food safety laboratories_.pdf
 
Key note speaker Neum_Admir Softic_ENG.pdf
Key note speaker Neum_Admir Softic_ENG.pdfKey note speaker Neum_Admir Softic_ENG.pdf
Key note speaker Neum_Admir Softic_ENG.pdf
 
Wellbeing inclusion and digital dystopias.pptx
Wellbeing inclusion and digital dystopias.pptxWellbeing inclusion and digital dystopias.pptx
Wellbeing inclusion and digital dystopias.pptx
 
2024-NATIONAL-LEARNING-CAMP-AND-OTHER.pptx
2024-NATIONAL-LEARNING-CAMP-AND-OTHER.pptx2024-NATIONAL-LEARNING-CAMP-AND-OTHER.pptx
2024-NATIONAL-LEARNING-CAMP-AND-OTHER.pptx
 
How to Give a Domain for a Field in Odoo 17
How to Give a Domain for a Field in Odoo 17How to Give a Domain for a Field in Odoo 17
How to Give a Domain for a Field in Odoo 17
 
FSB Advising Checklist - Orientation 2024
FSB Advising Checklist - Orientation 2024FSB Advising Checklist - Orientation 2024
FSB Advising Checklist - Orientation 2024
 
HMCS Vancouver Pre-Deployment Brief - May 2024 (Web Version).pptx
HMCS Vancouver Pre-Deployment Brief - May 2024 (Web Version).pptxHMCS Vancouver Pre-Deployment Brief - May 2024 (Web Version).pptx
HMCS Vancouver Pre-Deployment Brief - May 2024 (Web Version).pptx
 
COMMUNICATING NEGATIVE NEWS - APPROACHES .pptx
COMMUNICATING NEGATIVE NEWS - APPROACHES .pptxCOMMUNICATING NEGATIVE NEWS - APPROACHES .pptx
COMMUNICATING NEGATIVE NEWS - APPROACHES .pptx
 
Graduate Outcomes Presentation Slides - English
Graduate Outcomes Presentation Slides - EnglishGraduate Outcomes Presentation Slides - English
Graduate Outcomes Presentation Slides - English
 
Holdier Curriculum Vitae (April 2024).pdf
Holdier Curriculum Vitae (April 2024).pdfHoldier Curriculum Vitae (April 2024).pdf
Holdier Curriculum Vitae (April 2024).pdf
 
Micro-Scholarship, What it is, How can it help me.pdf
Micro-Scholarship, What it is, How can it help me.pdfMicro-Scholarship, What it is, How can it help me.pdf
Micro-Scholarship, What it is, How can it help me.pdf
 
Accessible Digital Futures project (20/03/2024)
Accessible Digital Futures project (20/03/2024)Accessible Digital Futures project (20/03/2024)
Accessible Digital Futures project (20/03/2024)
 
HMCS Max Bernays Pre-Deployment Brief (May 2024).pptx
HMCS Max Bernays Pre-Deployment Brief (May 2024).pptxHMCS Max Bernays Pre-Deployment Brief (May 2024).pptx
HMCS Max Bernays Pre-Deployment Brief (May 2024).pptx
 
NO1 Top Black Magic Specialist In Lahore Black magic In Pakistan Kala Ilam Ex...
NO1 Top Black Magic Specialist In Lahore Black magic In Pakistan Kala Ilam Ex...NO1 Top Black Magic Specialist In Lahore Black magic In Pakistan Kala Ilam Ex...
NO1 Top Black Magic Specialist In Lahore Black magic In Pakistan Kala Ilam Ex...
 
Beyond_Borders_Understanding_Anime_and_Manga_Fandom_A_Comprehensive_Audience_...
Beyond_Borders_Understanding_Anime_and_Manga_Fandom_A_Comprehensive_Audience_...Beyond_Borders_Understanding_Anime_and_Manga_Fandom_A_Comprehensive_Audience_...
Beyond_Borders_Understanding_Anime_and_Manga_Fandom_A_Comprehensive_Audience_...
 

This article was downloaded by [134.88.77.64] On 30 April 20.docx

  • 1. This article was downloaded by: [134.88.77.64] On: 30 April 2018, At: 07:05 Publisher: Institute for Operations Research and the Management Sciences (INFORMS) INFORMS is located in Maryland, USA Organization Science Publication details, including instructions for authors and subscription information: http://pubsonline.informs.org The Effects of Communication Networks and Turnover on Transactive Memory and Group Performance Linda Argote, Brandy L. Aven, Jonathan Kush To cite this article: Linda Argote, Brandy L. Aven, Jonathan Kush (2018) The Effects of Communication Networks and Turnover on Transactive Memory and Group Performance. Organization Science 29(2):191-206. https://doi.org/10.1287/orsc.2017.1176 Full terms and conditions of use: http://pubsonline.informs.org/page/terms-and-conditions This article may be used only for the purposes of research, teaching, and/or private study. Commercial use or systematic downloading (by robots or other automatic processes) is prohibited without explicit Publisher approval, unless otherwise noted. For more information, contact [email protected]
  • 2. The Publisher does not warrant or guarantee the article’s accuracy, completeness, merchantability, fitness for a particular purpose, or non-infringement. Descriptions of, or references to, products or publications, or inclusion of an advertisement in this article, neither constitutes nor implies a guarantee, endorsement, or support of claims made of that product, publication, or service. Copyright © 2018, INFORMS Please scroll down for article—it is on subsequent pages INFORMS is the largest professional society in the world for professionals in the fields of operations research, management science, and analytics. For more information on INFORMS, its publications, membership, or meetings visit http://www.informs.org http://pubsonline.informs.org https://doi.org/10.1287/orsc.2017.1176 http://pubsonline.informs.org/page/terms-and-conditions http://www.informs.org ORGANIZATION SCIENCE Vol. 29, No. 2, March–April 2018, pp. 191–206 http://pubsonline.informs.org/journal/orsc/ ISSN 1047-7039 (print), ISSN 1526-5455 (online) The Effects of Communication Networks and Turnover on Transactive Memory and Group Performance Linda Argote,a Brandy L. Aven,a Jonathan Kushb a Tepper School of Business, Carnegie Mellon University, Pittsburgh, Pennsylvania 15213; b Charlton College of Business,
  • 3. University of Massachusetts Dartmouth, North Dartmouth, Massachusetts 02747 Contact: [email protected], http://orcid.org/0000-0002-4149- 6968 (LA); [email protected], http://orcid.org/0000-0001-7086- 5876 (BLA); [email protected], http://orcid.org/0000-0003-4872-8650 (JK) Received: May 28, 2015 Revised: March 8, 2016; December 11, 2016; June 25, 2017; August 17, 2017 Accepted: September 4, 2017 Published Online in Articles in Advance: March 30, 2018 https://doi.org/10.1287/orsc.2017.1176 Copyright: © 2018 INFORMS Abstract. We theorize that the effect of membership turnover on group processes and performance depends on a group’s communication network. We describe two mecha- nisms through which communication networks affect group performance: (1) the number of direct communication paths and (2) the clarity of the coordination logic. These mech- anisms map onto two network dimensions: density, which affects a group’s behavior through the number of available communication paths, and centralization, which affects a group’s behavior through the clarity of the coordination logic. We empirically analyze the effects of turnover on the performance of fully connected all- channel networks and hub- and-spoke or wheel networks in an experiment of 109 four- person groups performing two
  • 4. collaborative problem-solving tasks. The greater number of direct communication paths enabled fully connected groups with stable membership to develop stronger transactive memory systems (TMSs) and perform better than fully connected groups that experienced turnover. By contrast, the clear coordination logic of perfectly centralized groups that expe- rienced turnover facilitated more frequent dyadic communication, which enabled them to strengthen their TMSs, incorporate the contributions of new members, and improve their performance. Thus, our results indicate that communication networks condition the effect of membership turnover on group processes and performance. Funding: The authors gratefully acknowledge the support of the National Science Foundation [Grants 1111750 and 1459963] and the Center for Organizational Learning, Innovation and Knowledge at the Tepper School of Business at Carnegie Mellon University. Supplemental Material: The online appendix is available at https://doi.org/10.1287/orsc.2017.1176. Keywords: social networks • turnover • transactive memory • group performance Introduction Organizational activity comprises the coordination of individuals to solve complex problems. Group mem- bers must identify the expertise of others, access infor- mation held by different members, and coordinate that information to accomplish shared goals. Mem- ber turnover, the exit of an incumbent member and introduction of a new member, in groups can com-
  • 5. plicate the identification and coordination of expertise and information (Arrow and McGrath 1995). Groups with stable membership are able to learn each other’s skills and expertise, allocate tasks to the most qualified members, and coordinate the interdependent activi- ties of their members. When turnover occurs, however, incumbent members know little about the expertise and skills of the new member, and coordination can become challenging (Lewis et al. 2007). Yet new mem- bers can nonetheless be a source of new ideas and per- spectives that improve group performance (Choi and Thompson 2005). We theorize that the effect of turnover on group performance depends on the group’s communication network. From a network perspective, groups can be categorized by their structural features—in particular, their centralization (Katz et al. 2004, Leavitt 1951, Rulke and Galaskiewicz 2000) and their density (Freeman 1979, Friedkin 1981, Balkundi and Harrison 2006). Cen- tralization increases for a group as the inequality or variance in the number of connections group members have to others increases. A group’s density increases as the ratio of actual to potential connections increases. We identify two mechanisms through which a group’s communication network affects performance: (1) the number of direct communication paths avail- able to group members and (2) the clarity of the coordination logic. The first mechanism maps onto network density, which affects a group’s behavior through the number of available communication paths. High levels of density in communication networks allow team members to communicate directly with one another. The second mechanism maps onto net-
  • 6. work centralization, which affects a group’s behavior through the clarity of the coordination logic. Central- ized communication networks direct how information is shared and inform how members coordinate (Blau 1974, Bunderson and Boumgarden 2010). 191 D ow nl oa de d fr om i nf or m s. or g by [ 13 4.
  • 8. na l us e on ly , a ll r ig ht s re se rv ed . http://pubsonline.informs.org/journal/orsc/ mailto:[email protected] http://orcid.org/0000-0002-4149-6968 mailto:[email protected] http://orcid.org/0000-0001-7086-5876 mailto:[email protected] http://orcid.org/0000-0003-4872-8650 https://doi.org/10.1287/orsc.2017.1176
  • 9. Argote, Aven, and Kush: The Effects of Turnover on Group Performance 192 Organization Science, 2018, vol. 29, no. 2, pp. 191–206, © 2018 INFORMS Figure 1. Fully Connected and Perfectly Centralized Communication Networks (a) (b) A C DB A C B D Notes. Panel (a) represents the fully connected communication net- work. Panel (b) represents the perfectly centralized communication network. C is the central member, whereas A, B, and D are peripheral members. We focus on the two network structures that max- imize one structural dimension while minimizing the other: a perfectly centralized or “hub-and-spoke” net- work that has the highest degree of centralization and minimizes density (see Figure 1), and a fully con- nected, “all-channel” network that maximizes den-
  • 10. sity and minimizes centralization. These two network structures not only allow the investigation of cen- tralization and density in tandem but are also struc- tures common in organizational settings. For exam- ple, programming groups’ communication networks have often been found to be fully connected, where members can communicate directly with any other member in an open-source setting (Tsay et al. 2014). By contrast, particularly in proprietary software, pro- gramming groups generally have a perfectly central- ized communication network where one central mem- ber acts as the “software architect,” and other members can communicate directly only with this central mem- ber (Kruchten 2008, Bosch and Bosch-Sijtsema 2010). In addition, even though project groups in organizations commonly communicate in a fully connected manner, groups engaged in covert projects typically communi- cate in a centralized fashion (Aven 2015). In the case of stable group membership, high cen- tralization, such as in our perfectly centralized groups, limits direct communication among members through their restricted communication network, which hin- ders members from learning about others’ expertise. Centralized communication networks also force mem- bers to coordinate in a particular manner, irrespective of members’ preferences and abilities. Rather than pro- vide members with the opportunity to customize their coordination logic, centralized group members must channel information to the central member(s), who then orchestrate the group’s activities. Hence, the coor- dination logic in centralized groups is independent of the particular members and their attributes. When turnover occurs, the communication network of perfectly centralized groups improves the group’s
  • 11. ability to integrate a new member. The coordina- tion logic of perfectly centralized networks is read- ily discernible by both incumbent and new members, which enhances their ability to contribute to the group (Bunderson and Boumgarden 2010, Morrison 2002). Because members’ roles in perfectly centralized groups are not customized to individual members, it is more likely that the new member can adequately perform the activities of the departing member in centralized than in decentralized groups. Finally, the restricted com- munication pathways in centralized networks require reliance on the few existing pathways to coordinate, which reduces the likelihood that any communica- tion pathway and its respective member are neglected. These factors enable a perfectly centralized group to incorporate the contributions of a new member and improve group outcomes for tasks involving problem solving and creativity (e.g., Wells and Pelz 1966, Choi and Thompson 2005). When membership is stable, high density in com- munication networks, such as in the fully connected groups, enables members to establish a strong trans- active memory system (TMS), a collective system for encoding, storing, and retrieving information (Wegner 1987, Lewis and Herndon 2011). Because of fully connected group members’ greater ability to communi- cate directly with other members, group members are able to learn about each other’s expertise and, in turn, develop a shared cognitive map of expertise. In addi- tion, fully connected networks allow members to tailor the group’s coordination logic to account for each mem- ber’s preferences and abilities. Although fully connected communication networks
  • 12. facilitate group performance by encouraging the devel- opment of a strong TMS, fully connected networks can also hinder the integration of new group members when turnover occurs. New members often do not have the same attributes and knowledge as departing mem- bers, which makes the substitution of a new member challenging for these groups because their coordina- tion logic is tailored to the original members’ unique abilities and characteristics. Furthermore, the group’s coordination logic cannot be readily observed by new members, which impairs their ability to contribute to the group. Communication Networks and Transactive Memory Systems A group’s communication network serves to channel information among group members and coordinate their activities (Leavitt 1951, Faucheux and Macken- zie 1966). In a dense, fully connected network (see Figure 1(a)), all of the members share the same number of communication pathways as all of the other mem- bers. By contrast, in the perfectly centralized group (see Figure 1(b)), only member C has communication D ow nl oa de d fr om
  • 15. se rv ed . Argote, Aven, and Kush: The Effects of Turnover on Group Performance Organization Science, 2018, vol. 29, no. 2, pp. 191–206, © 2018 INFORMS 193 pathways to the other members (A, B, and D), and members A, B, and D have only one pathway each to member C. To both communicate and coordinate activ- ity in the centralized group, the peripheral members (A, B, and D) must rely on the central member C. Thus, the perfectly centralized communication network both minimizes the number of available communication pathways and maximizes the clarity of the coordina- tion logic for the group: all peripheral members com- municate to the central member, and the central mem- ber dispatches information to peripheral members. By contrast, the fully connected network maximizes the number of communication paths but reduces the clar- ity of the coordination logic. The greater number of communication pathways in fully connected networks promotes information shar- ing and fosters an understanding of member expertise. When group members learn information about other members and their expertise, the group’s transactive memory system develops (Moreland and Myaskovsky 2000). The transactive memory systems of fully con- nected groups become tailored to the different skills,
  • 16. knowledge, and preferences of group members. Within perfectly centralized groups, the communi- cation network prevents group members from directly interacting with one another and developing a coordi- nation logic that is well suited to the particular mem- bers of the group. Instead, the communication network of perfectly centralized groups requires that certain members relay information to others and thus imposes a particular coordination logic on the group (Gouldner 1954, Weber 1947). The limited number of communi- cation pathways also hinders members from directly learning about each other’s areas of expertise, which in turn inhibits the development of a strong transactive memory system. Transactive memory systems have been found to improve group performance (see Ren and Argote 2011 for a review) on outcomes such as time to complete tasks (Faraj and Sproull 2000), errors (Liang et al. 1995), and creativity (Gino et al. 2010). Transactive memory systems can be distinguished from related constructs such as shared mental models, because the knowledge held by group members with a strong TMS is differ- entiated through specialization (Lewis and Herndon 2011). In groups with a strong TMS, members have different information about how to complete their sub- tasks, whereas in groups with shared mental models, members have the same information (DeChurch and Mesmer-Magnus 2010). Transactive memory systems develop from expe- rience working together. As individuals work with the same group members, they discern each other’s skills and abilities, which allows them to special- ize their expertise, fine-tune their division of labor,
  • 17. and coordinate effectively (Wegner 1987). The strength of a group’s TMS is affected by its communications (Hollingshead 1998, Yuan et al. 2010). We predict that differences in communication networks influence the strength of a group’s TMS. Furthermore, we predict that the effect of a group’s communication network depends on whether turnover occurs. Turnover Turnover, or membership change, is a common occur- rence within work groups (Kush et al. 2012). In some instances, turnover can benefit the group by invigorat- ing it with information and perspectives from the new member, which improves its performance (Choi and Levine 2004, Choi and Thompson 2005, Hancock et al. 2013). The positive effect of turnover on group perfor- mance is most likely to occur on tasks involving cre- ativity and innovation (Choi and Thompson 2005). For example, Wells and Pelz (1966) found that turnover in groups of scientists improved group performance. The introduction of a new member, even a novice, into an existing group can enhance the group’s performance (Ferriani et al. 2009, Uzzi and Spiro 2005). For new members to benefit a group, however, they must be integrated into the group by existing members. Membership change can hinder group outcomes because the new member might not have the same skills as the departing member, understand the coor- dination logic of the group, or be integrated into the group (Hausknecht and Holwerda 2013). Lewis et al. (2007) found that when only one group mem- ber was replaced in a group, those groups performed worse than either groups whose membership was sta- ble or groups whose membership changed totally. The
  • 18. advantages conferred on fully connected groups by their strong TMSs when membership is stable are undermined when turnover occurs. Because TMS is a cognitive division of labor, the group’s implicit coor- dination logic is not easily observed by the new mem- ber (Wegner 1987). Hence, for a fully connected group, a new member would have difficulty contributing because he or she would not understand how the group communicates or organizes its activities. When a group has developed a strong TMS, the incumbent members of the group tended to ignore the contribu- tions of a new member and expect him or her to fill the role of the departing member (Lewis et al. 2007). Thus, we anticipate that turnover will harm the performance of fully connected groups. The features that undermine TMS formation in per- fectly centralized groups with stable membership— limited communication pathways and mandated coor- dination logic—aid in the integration of new members when turnover occurs. New members in perfectly cen- tralized groups can easily discern the simple organiz- ing principle: all members contribute their information and views to the central member(s), who then integrate D ow nl oa de d fr om
  • 21. se rv ed . Argote, Aven, and Kush: The Effects of Turnover on Group Performance 194 Organization Science, 2018, vol. 29, no. 2, pp. 191–206, © 2018 INFORMS the information for the group (Guetzkow and Simon 1955). In turn, understanding the group’s coordina- tion logic helps new members to work effectively with other members, which is critical to group performance (Chao et al. 1994, Morrison 2002). Moreover, the lim- ited connectivity of perfectly centralized groups also requires greater reliance on the extant pathways for members to communicate. Frequent communication improves the new member’s knowledge of how to per- form his or her tasks and provides clarity in terms of the responsibilities associated with the position, which expedites the new member’s ability to contribute to the group (Morrison 2002). When the group incorporates the new member’s ideas and perspectives, the group is able to perform its task more effectively (Levine et al. 2003). In addition to increasing the creative problem solving of incumbent members (Choi and Thompson 2005), new members can also introduce new insights that improve group performance. We theorize two mechanisms through which net- work structure affects performance: (1) the number of
  • 22. direct communication paths available to group mem- bers and (2) the clarity of the coordination logic. We have argued that the first mechanism is more impor- tant when membership is stable and that the second mechanism is more important when turnover occurs. Thus, we expect that fully connected groups have stronger TMSs and better performance under stable membership than when turnover occurs, because of their greater number of communication paths. The greater number of paths, however, undermines their ability to integrate a new member when turnover occurs. We anticipate that the clarity of the commu- nication logic of perfectly centralized groups enables them to integrate the new member, benefit from his or her new ideas and perspectives, and thereby improve their performance when turnover occurs. Thus, we pre- dict the following. Hypothesis 1. Communication networks and turnover in- teract to affect group performance: fully connected groups perform better when group membership is stable than when turnover occurs, whereas perfectly centralized groups per- form better when turnover occurs than when group member- ship is stable. Hypothesis 2. Communication networks and turnover in- teract to affect transactive memory systems: fully connected networks have stronger transactive memory systems when group membership is stable than when turnover occurs, whereas perfectly centralized networks have stronger trans- active memory systems when turnover occurs than when membership is stable. Dyadic Communication Frequency Dyadic communication frequency represents the total communication that occurs in a group relative to
  • 23. the number of available communication paths in the group. The greater number of communication path- ways in fully connected relative to perfectly central- ized groups increases the demands on members’ time and attention and reduces the frequency of commu- nication along any one pathway. As opposed to fully connected groups, perfectly centralized groups must rely on fewer pathways to communicate, increasing group members’ dependence on each individual path- way, potentially strengthening one-on-one relation- ships. Thus, the communication constraints of per- fectly centralized groups increase members’ reliance on the available pathways and result in greater dyadic communication frequency than in less restricted, fully connected groups. Dyadic communication along each pathway is espe- cially important in integrating a new member. The fewer paths available to perfectly centralized groups make each connection more salient to group mem- bers. The greater amount of dyadic communication in perfectly centralized groups conveys the group’s coor- dination logic, which is critical in integrating a new member, and allows members to update the group’s knowledge of who knows what and to perform bet- ter when turnover occurs. By contrast, when turnover occurs in fully connected groups with strong TMSs, incumbent members neglect to communicate with the new member, which impairs their performance. For- mally, we predict the following. Hypothesis 3. Dyadic communication frequency mediates the interaction of communication network and turnover on transactive memory systems, and the mediation is stronger when turnover occurs than when group membership is
  • 24. stable. Scope Conditions Given that our primary interest is in the interactive effect of communication networks and turnover on the processes and performance of small groups, we focus on perfectly centralized groups and fully connected groups of four members. Small groups are a predom- inant means by which tasks are accomplished within organizations as well as an important unit of analy- sis for organizational research (Katz et al. 2004, Leavitt 1996, Uzzi and Spiro 2005). In addition, because we are interested in communication networks, we selected interdependent tasks that require coordination, so that we could observe how information was shared in the group. We focus on nonroutine, complex tasks; there- fore, our theory applies to complex problem-solving tasks that require coordination and creativity. Exam- ples of such tasks include product design, prototype development, engineering design processes, and soft- ware engineering. Last, because our tasks involve cre- ative problem solving, we expect that the contribu- tions of the new member have the potential to improve D ow nl oa de d fr om
  • 27. se rv ed . Argote, Aven, and Kush: The Effects of Turnover on Group Performance Organization Science, 2018, vol. 29, no. 2, pp. 191–206, © 2018 INFORMS 195 group performance (e.g., see Choi and Thompson 2005). We have hypothesized that whether the group benefits from the new member depends on the group’s communication network. Methods Participants One hundred and nine four-person groups composed of 503 individuals (49% female) were recruited from a mid-Atlantic American university participant pool.1 Participants received $20 or course credit for their par- ticipation, and an additional reward of $20 per person was given to the members of the best performing group in each condition. The ages of participants ranged from 18 to 37, with an average age of 22.0 years. Forty-nine percent of the participants were Asian or Indian, 37% were Caucasian, and 14% were other ethnicities. Tasks Because our focus is interdependent problem-solving tasks, we used two tasks that fell in the conceptual cooperative quadrant of McGrath’s (1984) circumplex model. Both tasks required creative problem solving.
  • 28. The first task was a programming task with the pos- sibility of errors; the second was an idea-generating task. Both tasks were based on an online graphical programming interface. This interface allowed partici- pants to design programs—called pipes—that collect, manipulate, organize, and filter information from the Internet to create a desired output. Although similar- ities exist between this interface and other program- ming languages, knowledge of other programming languages would not provide direct benefits because of the unique graphical programming interface of our tasks. The preponderance of participants were unfa- miliar with this interface: 94.2% rated themselves as unfamiliar or very unfamiliar with the interface in a postexperiment survey. The programming task required each group to work together to create a pipe that (1) sorted selected news items by publication date, (2) removed any repeated articles, and (3) allowed a user to specify a search key- word. The task required the use of five specific mod- ules. Although working on separate computers, par- ticipants had access to a virtual work environment, allowing them to contribute and to see the contribu- tions of their group mates simultaneously. All mem- bers could access and make changes to the pipe. In the idea-generating task, participants were asked to gen- erate collectively as many new features or functional improvements as possible to enhance the pipe that they had created in the programming task. The new features had to be both novel and feasible to implement, and therefore required the module expertise of all of the members. For both tasks, group members never inter- acted face-to-face and communicated only through an instant messaging client.
  • 29. Manipulations We manipulated two variables in the experiment: com- munication network and turnover. We manipulated the communication network by controlling who could communicate with whom through an instant messag- ing client. In the perfectly centralized condition, three peripheral members could contact only one central member who could communicate with all the periph- eral members. In the fully connected condition, all members could communicate with all other members (see Figure 1). In both conditions, group members could send only one message to one member at a time. Throughout both tasks, the communication network remained unchanged. We manipulated turnover by replacing a randomly selected member from each group after the practice task with another participant who acquired the net- work position of the departing member. Participants were not warned that their group might experience turnover. The new member received the same train- ing and training materials as the departing member but had not previously worked with the graphical pro- gramming tool or with a group. In fully connected groups that experienced turnover, a randomly chosen member was selected to be replaced. In the perfectly centralized groups that experienced turnover, either a peripheral member or a central member was replaced. For example, in the centralized (see Figure 1(b)) periph- eral turnover condition, a peripheral member (A, B, or D) was randomly selected and replaced. In the cen- tralized central turnover condition, the central mem- ber (C) was replaced. We did not predict that turnover of the central member would have a significantly dif- ferent effect than turnover of peripheral members, but we allow for the possibility in our design by including
  • 30. both types of centralized turnover. Procedures After arriving at the laboratory, participants were ran- domly assigned to one of four isolated rooms, each associated with a member ID and a position within the network. We allowed members to communicate using only an instant text messenger and limited their oppor- tunities to see one another. The participants attended a training phase where they received information about the programming task as well as instructions for the experiment. The materials explained how to commu- nicate with other members through the instant mes- senger, directed participants to a short video demon- strating the creation of a sample pipe, and provided detailed information about the programming mod- ules available. Additionally, we provided each group member with different, randomly assigned, special- ized information about one of the modules necessary for completing the task to ensure that group members were interdependent. We did not tell members that D ow nl oa de d fr om i nf
  • 33. ed . Argote, Aven, and Kush: The Effects of Turnover on Group Performance 196 Organization Science, 2018, vol. 29, no. 2, pp. 191–206, © 2018 INFORMS each had specialized information. For groups experi- encing turnover, the new member received the same specialized information as the member whom he or she replaced. Once all the participants had read the training mate- rials, they were given 30 minutes to practice the task together. After they finished the practice task, group members completed the first survey. At this point, for groups in the turnover condition, an announce- ment was made: “One new member will be joining your group, and another will be randomly chosen to leave.” A randomly chosen group member was then removed from the communication network and dis- missed from the experiment, and a new member was introduced to the group. The new member occupied the same position in the communication network as the departing member and received the same special- ized information during training. Groups were then given instructions for the programming task, which they had 30 minutes to complete. The programming task was followed by the 5-minute feature-generating task.2 Group members then completed the second sur- vey. Finally, we debriefed and thanked participants.
  • 34. Measures Several variables in this study were behavioral mea- sures, including our two dependent measures: the number of errors in the programming task and the number of functional improvements in the feature- generating task. The dyadic communication between group members was also a behavioral measure based on the messages sent on the instant messaging client. The remaining measures were collected from a sur- vey that measured the group’s TMS, perceptions of the communication network, and group member demo- graphics and experience, such as their familiarity with the programming interface. Errors. We calculated the number of errors in the pipe that each group submitted. Errors included both cases when the group used incorrect modules and cases when the group omitted modules required for the pro- gram to function properly. To calculate the errors, we categorized errors into four comprehensive categories: missing modules, incorrect settings, incorrect modules, or unconnected modules. The number of errors for a module was based on the number of settings in that module. For example, if a group did not include a par- ticular module in their program and that module had three required settings, three errors would be recorded for the group. If a module was included but one of the settings was wrong, the group was coded as hav- ing one error. These errors were summed into a single measure for each group. A portion of the groups had their errors assessed by two coders (n � 70). A Cohen’s kappa of 0.72 (p < 0.001) indicated good agreement between coders (Cohen 1960). A single coder then coded errors for the remaining groups. New Features. During the feature-generating task, participants responded to the question, “Think of any
  • 35. ways that you think the pipe you just built could be improved by creating new features. What other ways could this pipe be more helpful to an end user, be more simply designed, etc.?” Two coders coded a sub- set of the groups (n � 40), assessing each new fea- ture on whether it introduced novel functionality and was feasible to implement. Two coders attained very good reliability, as indicated by a Cohen’s kappa of 0.88 (p < 0.001). Any disagreements between coders were addressed and resolved. A single coder then coded the number of new features for the rest of the groups. Transactive Memory Systems. Lewis’s (2003) 15-item survey measure was used to measure the groups’ transactive memory systems. The survey instrument was administered at the end of the study, after groups had completed both tasks. The overall reliability was acceptable (Cronbach’s alpha�0.82). The average inter- group reliability (rw g) was 0.95, indicating that it is appropriate to aggregate the individual-level measures to the group level. The intraclass correlation coefficient (ICC(2)) value was 0.61, indicating acceptable reliabil- ity of the measure. These reliability statistics provide evidence for general agreement among group mem- bers and the appropriateness of aggregating to the group level (LeBreton and Senter 2008). The ICC(1) value, which indicates the extent to which the variabil- ity in individual responses can be predicted by group membership, was 0.28. Values of 0.25 or higher are con- sidered to be large effects (Murphy and Myors 1998). Dyadic Communication Frequency. We calculated the dyadic communication frequency based on group members’ instant text messages. As our theory is about the integration of new members, we chose to use a measure that would capture the strength of the com-
  • 36. munication relationships between individuals in the group. Dyadic communication frequency was calcu- lated as the sum of all group messages exchanged between members during the task performance period, divided by the total available dyadic communication paths in the group (three for perfectly centralized and six for fully connected). This value was then divided by the time to task completion to account for varia- tion in the groups’ completion times. We calculated this variable separately for the programming task and the feature-generating task. Analyses predicting errors used the dyadic communication frequency during the programming task, and analyses predicting new fea- tures used the dyadic communication frequency dur- ing the feature-generation task. For Table 2, we used cumulative dyadic communication frequency, the com- bination of dyadic communication frequency across both task periods, to predict the effect of the network on communication and on TMS (Models 6 and 7), D ow nl oa de d fr om i nf or
  • 39. . Argote, Aven, and Kush: The Effects of Turnover on Group Performance Organization Science, 2018, vol. 29, no. 2, pp. 191–206, © 2018 INFORMS 197 because TMS development should be affected by all prior communication in the group, not just communi- cation relevant to a particular task. We present analyses for communication to and from the new member for groups in the turnover conditions in the online sup- plement. Because these measures of communication to and from the new member do not apply for groups that did not experience turnover, we use dyadic communi- cation frequency in analyses involving both turnover and no-turnover groups. Dyadic communication fre- quency includes communication to and from the new member for groups that experienced turnover. Results We begin this section by discussing the effectiveness of the communication network manipulation. We then present results for errors and new features. Next, we provide mediation analyses indicating that dyadic communication frequency explains the effects of the communication network and turnover on TMSs. Last, we provide a test of the overall model implied by Hypotheses 1–3. The section concludes with a presen- tation of robustness checks. Communication Network Manipulation Checks First, we determined whether members correctly as- sessed their group’s communication network. Each
  • 40. group member was asked which communication path- ways existed among members of the group. Group members correctly identified the group’s communica- tion pathways 86% of the time. Fully connected groups were slightly more accurate (88%) than perfectly cen- tralized groups (84%) in identifying communication pathways (p < 0.1). These results suggest that group members were aware of their available communication pathways in both the centralized and fully connected conditions. Second, although it was not possible for the perfectly centralized groups to send messages in a decentral- ized pattern, it was possible for the fully connected groups to send messages in a centralized pattern. To explore this possibility, we calculated Freeman’s (1979) degree centralization measure based on the commu- nication messages shared within each group. Degree centralization is a group-level measure of the disper- sion of group members’ degree centrality scores. Given that degree centralization can be calculated only for dichotomous networks, we included a communication pathway only if two members communicated over a certain threshold. The threshold for inclusion was one standard deviation below the group’s mean level of messages.3 Using a threshold for tie inclusion is com- mon in network research, and the threshold we applied provides a reasonable distribution of degree centraliza- tion values within the sample (Borgatti et al. 2013). The degree centralization measure was highly correlated with the manipulation of the communication network (r � 0.81, p < 0.001), which suggests that our commu- nication manipulation influenced the pattern of mes- sages exchanged among group members, as expected.4
  • 41. Performance We predicted that the communication network and turnover would interact to affect group performance (Hypothesis 1) such that fully connected groups would perform better when there was no turnover than when there was, and perfectly centralized groups would per- form better when turnover occurred than when mem- bership was stable. Next, we hypothesized that the communication network and turnover would inter- act to predict transactive memory systems such that fully connected groups would have stronger TMSs when membership was stable than when turnover occurred, and perfectly centralized networks would have stronger TMSs when turnover occurred than when membership was stable (Hypothesis 2). Finally, we proposed that dyadic communication would medi- ate the interaction between the communication net- work and turnover in predicting the TMS and that the mediation would be stronger when turnover occurred than when it did not (Hypothesis 3). A stronger medi- ation would mean that more of the effect of the net- work on the TMS was due to the network’s effect on dyadic communication frequency when the group experienced turnover than when it did not. We present descriptive statistics and correlations in Table 1. Table 2 presents the ordinary least squares estimates predicting errors based on the communi- cation network, turnover, and their interaction.5 In Model 1 of Table 2, the interaction of communication network and turnover was negative and significant (B � −2.31, p < 0.05), as predicted by Hypothesis 1. Additionally, we found a significant positive interac- tion between communication network and turnover on the number of new features (B � 2.64, p < 0.05) in Model 3 of Table 2. These interactions suggest that, for
  • 42. both errors and new features, fully connected groups performed better when group membership was sta- ble than when turnover occurred, whereas perfectly centralized groups performed better when turnover occurred than when membership was stable. This pat- tern can be seen in Figure 2, which depicts the mean numbers of errors (left side) and new features (right side) as functions of communication networks and turnover. This pattern of results supports Hypothesis 1. We turn now to a test of Hypothesis 2. As can be seen from Model 5 of Table 2, the interaction of the network and turnover was positive and signifi- cant in predicting TMS (B � 4.71, p < 0.05), consis- tent with Hypothesis 2. Fully connected groups devel- oped stronger TMSs when membership was stable than when turnover occurred (55.1 versus 52.4), and D ow nl oa de d fr om i nf or m
  • 45. Argote, Aven, and Kush: The Effects of Turnover on Group Performance 198 Organization Science, 2018, vol. 29, no. 2, pp. 191–206, © 2018 INFORMS Table 1. Means and Correlations Mean SD 1 2 3 4 5 6 1. Errors 2.04 2.87 2. New Features 4.39 2.63 −0.33∗ ∗ ∗ 3. TMS 52.84 5.17 −0.34∗ ∗ ∗ 0.19† 4. Dyadic Communication Frequency During Programming Task 1.53 0.68 −0.24∗ 0.15 0.26∗ ∗ 5. Dyadic Communication Frequency During Feature Generation Task 0.87 0.85 −0.05 −0.01 0.26∗ ∗ 0.63∗ ∗ ∗ 6. Dyadic Communication to New Member During Prog. Task 0.83 0.51 −0.30∗ 0.21† 0.36∗ ∗ 0.85∗ ∗ ∗ 0.51∗ ∗ ∗ 7. Dyadic Communication from New Member During Prog. Task 0.69 0.43 −0.27∗ −0.01 0.35∗ ∗ 0.76∗ ∗ ∗ 0.53∗ ∗ ∗ 0.67∗ ∗ ∗ †p < 0.1; ∗ p < 0.05; ∗ ∗ p < 0.01; ∗ ∗ ∗ p < 0.001. perfectly centralized groups developed stronger TMSs when turnover occurred than when membership was stable (50.9 versus 52.9). Although not explicitly hypothesized, we examined whether transactive memory systems mediated the effect of communication networks on performance. As can be seen from Table 2, when TMS was included as a predictor of errors in Model 2, its coefficient was negative and significant. Furthermore, the compari- son of results between Model 1 and Model 2 reveals
  • 46. that the interaction of communication network and turnover was no longer significant when TMS was included, suggesting that TMS accounted for the effect of the interaction on errors. Similarly, when TMS was included as a predictor of new features (see Model 4), it was positive and marginally significant and the inter- action of network structure and turnover became less informative (p < 0.05 to p < 0.1). Next, we investigated the role of dyadic communi- cation frequency in explaining TMS. As can be seen Table 2. Models of Errors, New Features, Dyadic Communication Frequency, and Transactive Memory Systems Dyadic New New Communication Errors Errors Features Features TMS Frequency TMS Variable Model 1 Model 2 Model 3 Model 4 Model 5 Model 6 Model 7 Centralized Network 0.67 −0.14 −1.76∗ −1.28 −4.24∗ ∗ 0.33† −5.26∗ ∗ ∗ (0.87) (0.85) (0.80) (0.83) (1.56) (0.18) (1.49) Turnover 0.72 0.21 −1.28 −0.98 −2.68† −0.17 −2.15 (0.86) (0.82) (0.79) (0.79) (1.55) (0.18) (1.46) Centralized×Turnover −2.31∗ −1.42 2.64∗ 2.09† 4.71∗ 0.48† 3.28† (1.13) (1.10) (1.04) (1.07) (2.05) (0.23) (1.96) TMS −0.19∗ ∗ ∗ 0.10† (0.05) (0.05)
  • 47. Dyadic Communication 3.11∗ ∗ ∗ Frequency (0.80) R2 0.06 0.17 0.08 0.11 0.07 0.24 0.18 N 109 109 109 109 109 109 109 Notes. Values in parentheses are standard errors. Models 3 and 4 include a control for groups that inadvertently received additional time on the task; 1 � received 10 minutes, and 0 � received 5 minutes. The results are consistent if the groups who received 10 minutes are removed from the analysis. Cumulative dyadic communication frequency, which is used in Models 6 and 7, is the dyadic communication frequency for the combination of the programming task and the feature- generating task. †p < 0.1; ∗ p < 0.05; ∗ ∗ p < 0.01; ∗ ∗ ∗ p < 0.001. from Model 6 of Table 2, the interaction of the commu- nication network with turnover in predicting dyadic communication was marginally significant and pos- itive (B � 0.48, p < 0.1). The mean levels of dyadic communication as a function of communication net- work and turnover can be seen in Figure 3. Fully con- nected groups had somewhat more dyadic commu- nication when they did not experience turnover than when they did, whereas perfectly centralized groups had somewhat more dyadic communication when they experienced turnover than when they did not. These results are in line with Hypothesis 3. Subsequently, we examined how communication networks, turnover, and dyadic communication fre- quency affected TMS. Model 7 in Table 2 adds dyadic
  • 48. communication frequency as a predictor of TMS and shows that dyadic communication frequency had a significant and positive effect on TMS. Also, relative to Model 5, the interaction between network struc- ture and turnover became marginally significant when D ow nl oa de d fr om i nf or m s. or g by [ 13 4. 88
  • 50. l us e on ly , a ll r ig ht s re se rv ed . Argote, Aven, and Kush: The Effects of Turnover on Group Performance Organization Science, 2018, vol. 29, no. 2, pp. 191–206, © 2018 INFORMS 199 Figure 2. Mean Errors and New Features as Functions of Communication Network and Turnover 0
  • 52. f ea tu re s 6 7 Fully connected † Perfectly centralized * † * * No turnover Turnover Note. Error bars are 95% confidence intervals. † p < 0.1; ∗ p < 0.05. dyadic communication frequency was included in
  • 53. Model 7. These results indicate that dyadic communi- cation partially explained the effects of the interaction of network structure and turnover on the development of TMS, in support of Hypothesis 3. Figure 3 also shows the amount of communication that was directed to the new member (see the darker bands in the turnover condition bars) compared with the other three members. As can be seen from the Figure 3. Dyadic Communication Frequency by Network and Turnover During the Programming Task 0 0.5 1.0 1.5 D ya di c co m m un ic at
  • 54. io n fr eq ue nc y 2.0 2.5 No turnover Turnover No turnover Turnover Fully connected Perfectly centralized † † Notes. Within each group that experienced turnover, the darker sub- section is the portion of the communication within the group that went to the new member. The rest of the bar represents the com- munication to the other three members. Error bars are 95% con- fidence intervals on the group’s dyadic communication frequency. Planned contrasts are on dyadic communication frequency. Planned contrasts were also performed comparing the proportion of dyadic communication that went to the new member in perfectly
  • 55. central- ized versus fully connected groups. Perfectly centralized groups had significantly more communication to their new members than fully connected groups (p < 0.001). † p < 0.1. figure, the proportion of communication directed to the new member was much higher in perfectly cen- tralized than in fully connected groups. The difference in the amount of communication to the new mem- ber was significant using planned contrasts (p < 0.001), indicating that perfectly centralized groups communi- cated more to their new members than fully connected groups. We also replaced the communication directed to the new member with the communication from the new member and found a similar pattern: the new member communicated more in perfectly centralized than fully connected groups. In addition, we analyzed communication to or from the new member as a predictor of TMS (see the online supplement for more details). Dyadic communication (B � 3.59, p < 0.001), communication to the new mem- ber (B � 4.52, p < 0.001), and communication from the new member (B � 4.55, p < 0.01) were all associ- ated with increases in the strength of a group’s TMS. When both communication to and from the new mem- ber were included, only communication to the new member was significant (B � 3.20, p < 0.05). Thus, the effect of incumbent members communicating to the new member is a more robust predictor of TMS than communication from the new member for groups that experienced turnover.
  • 56. As a supplement to our quantitative results, we pro- vide examples from groups’ transcripts to illustrate the differences in how fully connected and perfectly cen- tralized groups integrated the new member. The first quote is from a fully connected group that experienced turnover. Incumbent 1 wrote, “Are you working on it? Just checking.” Incumbent 2 responded, “I think [New Member] is doing random shit” (Group 55). This quote illustrates that the incumbent group members in the fully connected groups did not directly communicate with the new member, whom they seemed to ignore and assumed would not contribute. D ow nl oa de d fr om i nf or m s. or g by
  • 58. p er so na l us e on ly , a ll r ig ht s re se rv ed . Argote, Aven, and Kush: The Effects of Turnover on Group Performance 200 Organization Science, 2018, vol. 29, no. 2, pp. 191–206, ©
  • 59. 2018 INFORMS In a perfectly centralized group with peripheral member turnover, we see encouragement between incumbents and the new member. Incumbent 1 wrote, “We figured out up to part c, [but] we can’t figure that out.” After the new member works on the shared screen, the new member responded, “[That] should be [the solution to part c].” Incumbent 1 answered with “mmm . . . let me ask the others . . .you are amazin [sic]” (Group 70). In a perfectly centralized group with central member turnover, incumbent members informed the new mem- ber about the organizing logic in the group. Incumb- ent 1 wrote, “[C]ould you speak with A and B???” The new member responded, “[Y]eah [I] think so. I can talk to B and B said he/she can only talk to me.” Incum- bent 1 answered with “you [perform] as a communica- tor for the group” (Group 70). These quotes reinforce the quantitative results indicating that perfectly cen- tralized groups communicated more with their new members and incorporated their contributions better than fully connected groups did. Moderated Mediation Analyses We tested the moderated mediation proposed in Hy- pothesis 3 using PROCESS (Hayes 2013). Figure 4 pro- vides a theoretical model of the moderated mediation we predict. The use of bootstrap sampling in PROCESS allows for tests of mediation and moderation that have more statistical power and violate fewer assumptions than other tests of mediation (Hayes 2013). For all of the Figure 4. Theoretical Framework and Statistical Models Predicting Errors by Turnover Condition
  • 60. Full moderated serial mediation (a) (b) Groups that did not experience turnover (c) Groups that experienced turnover + +_ + + + Centralized network Performance Dyadic communication frequency Transactive memory systems Turnover + Hypothesis 1 Hypothesis 2
  • 61. Hypothesis 3 _ No turnover serial mediation – 0.20* – 5.09** 2.30 0.37* Centralized network Errors Dyadic communication frequency Transactive memory systems – 0.38 Just turnover serial mediation – 2.82† 3.59*** – 0.15*0.92***
  • 62. Centralized network Errors Dyadic communication frequency Transactive memory systems – 0.91 Notes. Panel (a) shows the full statistical framework. Panel (b) shows coefficients for groups that did not experience turnover (n � 42), and panel (c) shows coefficients for groups that experienced turnover (n � 67) for errors. †p < 0.1; ∗ p < 0.05; ∗ ∗ p < 0.01; ∗ ∗ ∗ p < 0.001. analyses, we used 50,000 bias-corrected bootstrap sam- ples. In the overall model, we used dyadic communica- tion frequency because it occurs for both turnover and no-turnover groups. The results of the PROCESS anal- ysis indicated that dyadic communication frequency mediated the relationship between the communication network and transactive memory systems, and that the relationship was stronger when turnover occurred (B � 2.45; 95% confidence intervals (CIs): 1.14, 4.28) than when it did not (B � 1.02; 95% CIs: 0.01, 2.69). The index of moderated mediation test (Hayes 2015) was significant (95% CIs: 0.20, 3.28), indicating that the strength of the mediation was significantly stronger for
  • 63. the turnover than no-turnover groups, providing addi- tional support for Hypothesis 3. We used the partially standardized indirect effect to determinetheeffectsizeofthenetworkvariableonTMS through dyadic communication frequency (Preacher and Kelley 2011). The partially standardized indirect effect can be interpreted the same as a Cohen’s d— the number of standard deviations of change in TMS that occur as a result of the effect of the network on dyadic communication frequency. For groups that did not experience turnover, perfectly centralized groups had TMSs that were 0.22 standard deviations stronger than fully connected groups (95% CIs: 0.01, 0.56), due solely to centralized groups communicating more than fully connected groups. Because perfectly cen- tralized groups communicated even more when they experienced turnover, centralized groups had TMSs that were 0.57 standard deviations stronger than fully D ow nl oa de d fr om i nf or
  • 66. . Argote, Aven, and Kush: The Effects of Turnover on Group Performance Organization Science, 2018, vol. 29, no. 2, pp. 191–206, © 2018 INFORMS 201 connected groups when turnover occurred (95% CIs: 0.27, 0.91), because of their communication. Cohen (1960) described 0.22 as a small effect and 0.57 as a medium effect. These calculations demonstrate that the importance of dyadic communication in explaining the network’s effect on TMS increases substantially when groups experienced turnover. We now turn to a test of a full “moderated serial mediation” model as seen in Figure 4. The analysis for errors indicated that for groups that did not ex- perience turnover, the effect of communication net- work on errors was partially mediated by dyadic communication frequency and TMS (B � −0.21; 95% CIs: −0.66, −0.03); however, the majority of the effect was accounted for by TMS alone (B � 0.98; 95% CIs: 0.41, 1.92). By contrast, for groups that experienced turnover, the strength of the effect of communication network through dyadic communication frequency and TMS was larger in magnitude (B �−0.52; 95% CIs: −1.08,−0.21) than that of the effect through TMS alone (B � 0.44; 95% CIs: 0.0002, 1.1386). The index for mod- erated mediation (Hayes 2015) indicated that the effect of having a perfectly centralized network on errors through dyadic communication frequency and TMS was stronger in turnover versus no-turnover groups (95% CIs: −0.68, −0.05). The effect of having a perfectly
  • 67. centralized network on errors just through TMS did not differ in magnitude based on turnover (95% CIs: −1.35, 0.16).6 For groups that did not experience turnover, TMS explained the majority of the effect of the communi- cation network on errors: perfectly centralized groups had weaker TMSs than fully connected groups, which led to more errors (see Figure 4(b)). There was a countervailing effect of the communication network through dyadic communication frequency—perfectly centralized groups communicated more than fully connected groups, which improved their TMSs and thereby reduced errors. But this second effect was smaller than the first when group membership was sta- ble. Hence, when there was no turnover, the net effect of having a perfectly centralized network increased errors (−0.21 + 0.98 � 0.77). When groups experi- enced turnover, however, the magnitudes of the effects changed (see Figure 4(c)). For groups that experi- enced turnover, the effect of having a perfectly central- ized network on dyadic communication frequency was much stronger than for groups that did not experience turnover, and the effect of dyadic communication fre- quency on TMS was also stronger. When groups expe- rienced turnover, the effect of perfectly centralized net- works reducing errors through their effect on dyadic communication frequency and TMS exceeded the per- fectly centralized network’s direct effect on increasing errors through TMS alone (−0.52 + 0.44 �−0.08). To provide a sense of the size of these effects, we present the effect sizes for the moderated serial medi- ation. For groups that did not experience turnover, the partially standardized indirect effect of the network on errors through dyadic communication frequency and
  • 68. TMS was −0.07 (95% CIs: −0.23, −0.01), and there was also a significant mediation just through TMS (0.35; 95% CIs: 0.14, 0.68). The interpretation of these signif- icant effect sizes is that for groups that did not expe- rience turnover, there was a 0.28 (−0.07 + 0.35) SD increase in the number of errors for perfectly central- ized versus fully connected groups. This effect reversed for those that experienced turnover, with a 0.18 SD (95% CIs:−0.37,−0.07) decrease in errors due to dyadic communication and TMS. We tested the same moderated serial mediation with new features as the dependent variable. The serial pathway of the centralized network effect on new fea- tures through dyadic communication frequency and TMS was not significant for groups with stable group membership (B � 0.02; 95% CIs: −0.04, 0.16) but was positive and significant for groups that experienced turnover (B � 0.09; 95% CIs: 0.01, 0.28). TMS by itself acted as a significant mediator of the effect of hav- ing a centralized network on new features for groups with stable membership (B � −0.55; 95% CIs: −1.34, −0.09) but not for groups that experienced turnover (B � −0.03; 95% CIs: −0.33, 0.26).7 These results are similar to the results for errors; however, neither of the mediations was moderated by turnover. In these analyses, we found general support for our hypotheses and for the overall model in Figure 4(a). Results were stronger for errors than for new features. Robustness Checks Because the central and peripheral turnover conditions did not differ in their errors (1.22 and 1.23, respectively; p � 0.99), new features (4.52 versus 5.00, respectively; p � 0.54), or the strength of their transactive mem- ory systems (52.2 versus 53.6, respectively; p � 0.37),
  • 69. these two centralized turnover conditions were com- bined into a single centralized network turnover con- dition for the analyses shown in Table 2. We repeated these analyses but included only one form of central- ized turnover at a time. When either of the central- ized turnover conditions (central member or periph- eral member turnover) was removed from Model 1 in Table 2, the interaction of network structure and turnover on errors was in the same direction and marginally significant (p < 0.1). This reduction in sig- nificance is likely due to a reduction in power. No dif- ferences were detected when Model 3 in Table 2, pre- dicting new features, was run with only one of the perfectly centralized turnover conditions included in the analysis at a time. Second, we reran all moderated mediation analyses with data from only one of the two D ow nl oa de d fr om i nf or m s.
  • 72. Argote, Aven, and Kush: The Effects of Turnover on Group Performance 202 Organization Science, 2018, vol. 29, no. 2, pp. 191–206, © 2018 INFORMS perfectly centralized turnover conditions included at a time and did not find any significant differences for turnover based on whether the peripheral or central member left the group. We created two variables that measured the imbal- ance of messages sent and received among the group members, using the Herfindahl–Hirschman index of communication corrected for the number of pathways as an alternative explanation of our findings. These indices measure imbalances, and they increase to 1 as more communication is either sent or received by a single individual and decrease to 0 if there is perfect equality in the number of messages sent or received. Although the imbalance of sent messages (B �−14.89, p < 0.001) and the imbalance of received messages (B � −18.73, p < 0.1) were both negatively related to TMS, neither measure was statistically significant when dyadic communication frequency was added to the model, and dyadic communication frequency was significant and positive (p < 0.01) in both cases. These results suggest that dyadic communication frequency is a more robust predictor of TMS than imbalanced communication. Discussion and Conclusion Our results indicate that perfectly centralized com- munication networks can reverse the negative effect of turnover in groups. When turnover occurred in fully connected groups, incumbents tended to ignore
  • 73. new members who could not readily understand the group’s implicit coordination logic or their roles within the group. By contrast, the explicit coordination logic of perfectly centralized groups enabled them to make use of the contributions of new members. In central- ized groups, the means of coordination, which were largely determined by the communication structure, were simple and explicit. The high dyadic communi- cation frequency in the perfectly centralized groups reduced the likelihood that the incumbent members ignored the new member. The active integration of the new member not only fostered the formation of a strong TMS but also improved the creativity and problem-solving capabilities of centralized groups. The experimental manipulation of communication networks we use here provides two distinct method- ological advantages. First, identifying the direct causal effects of networks has proven challenging in field settings, particularly because of endogeneity issues (Ferriani et al. 2009, Lee et al. 2014, Uzzi and Spiro 2005). Our study design provides insight into commu- nication networks and group processes and is unham- pered by endogeneity concerns (see Croson et al. 2007 for a discussion of the advantages of experimen- tal research). The experimental design and random assignment of participants to communication networks and to positions in the networks also enabled us to attribute effects to the communication network rather than to conditions that led to the formation of the net- works or to qualities of individuals who might gravi- tate to certain positions in the networks (Sasovova et al. 2010). Thus, directly manipulating the communication network allows us to make causal claims about the effects of communication networks on group perfor-
  • 74. mance (Manski 1993). Second, where possible, we used behavioral measures, such as the amount of commu- nication, which are more objective than self-reported variables (Spector 1994). In particular, self-reports of communication and social networks have been found to be heavily biased by social factors, such as individ- ual status (Bernard et al. 1984). In addition to providing insights difficult to obtain by other methods, the design of our study maps well to organizational phenomena. The two tasks we used— the programming task and the idea-generating task— parallel many tasks found in organizational settings. Within organizations, groups often work on complex tasks, which involve both problem solving and creativ- ity (Devine et al. 1999). Thus, our realistic tasks provide external validity. In addition, we trained individuals on different information to increase task interdependence among the group members, which captures the chal- lenge of integration and coordination among group members with specialized members. Group members also communicated through computers located in separate rooms and thus operated as a distributed group. This arrangement is analogous to contempo- rary project-based work conducted in organizations via email (Kleinbaum et al. 2013, Aven 2015). This fea- ture of our experiment not only represents a condition under which many groups operate in organizations today but also allows us to control the communication network and to capture all communication that occurs among group members. Although the features of our experimental design were chosen to reflect character- istics of real organizations, these design features also present boundary conditions to which our findings are most likely to generalize. Therefore, we anticipate that our findings will generalize to small groups in which
  • 75. members perform complex interdependent tasks and communicate in a distributed manner. Another boundary condition of our findings is the experience of the new member. The new member in our study received the same individual training as the departing member but did not work with a group pre- viously. We chose this study design because we felt that new members to groups typically would have received some training but would have limited group experi- ence. Understanding the effect of the prior experience of the new member on group processes and perfor- mance is an interesting issue for future research. Although we did not hypothesize differences in the effects of turnover in central and peripheral mem- bers in perfectly centralized groups, we included both D ow nl oa de d fr om i nf or m
  • 78. Argote, Aven, and Kush: The Effects of Turnover on Group Performance Organization Science, 2018, vol. 29, no. 2, pp. 191–206, © 2018 INFORMS 203 types of turnover in our design to be able to ascer- tain whether any differences observed were due to the structure, as we hypothesized, or to the position. To accomplish this, we randomly assigned individu- als to networks and to positions rather than allow- ing individuals to gravitate to the networks or posi- tions that they preferred or for which they believed they were especially qualified. Furthermore, the new member received the same training as other members and the same specialized information as the depart- ing member. These features of the design enabled us to investigate the causal effects of the communication network, a major goal of the study. We did not find dif- ferences between turnover in the central or peripheral positions in dyadic communication, TMS, or perfor- mance. It is important to note that turnover of central and peripheral members might have different effects in groups where members’ positions are correlated with other characteristics, such as experience or skills, which is often the case in organizations. By using ran- dom assignment, our study enabled us to determine the effect of network structure, independent of these individual factors, on group outcomes. One limitation of our study was that the feature- generating task always followed the programming task. We thought that this was the most realistic sequence: it would be more natural for groups to identify “ways that the pipe you just built could be
  • 79. improved by creating new features” after rather than before they had built the pipe. The correlation between errors in the programming task and new features in the feature-generating task was −0.33, which indicates a moderate relationship between the two tasks: as errors on the programming task decreased, the number of new features typically increased. Thus, performing well on the first task did not constrain members from being able to identify ways the pipe could be improved on the second task. Nonetheless, it would be useful in future work to develop an idea-generating task that could occur before or after the programming task and thus allow the order of tasks to be counterbalanced. This paper contributes to several literatures as well as highlights their intersections. In a review of the transactive memory literature, Ren and Argote (2011) recommended greater research on how social networks affect the development of transactive memory systems. Our research shows that the communication network is an important predictor of transactive memory sys- tems. In addition, our findings demonstrate that com- munication networks condition the effect of turnover on TMS development. Previous work has shown that membership change disables a group’s TMS, which impairs its performance (Lewis et al. 2007, Moreland et al. 1996). Our study demonstrates that perfectly cen- tralized communication networks can reverse the neg- ative effect of turnover of one member. Relative to fully connected groups, incumbent members of centralized groups communicate more with the new member, which enables them to update their TMS and improve their performance. Our results suggest that perfectly centralized groups are more receptive to newcomers and more likely to incorporate the ideas of new mem-
  • 80. bers than fully connected groups. Our findings also offer insights into the effects of the communication network structure on group-level outcomes, such as group performance (Casciaro et al. 2015). Although there is a robust literature on the effects of communication networks on individual out- comes (Ahuja et al. 2003, Borgatti and Cross 2003), our work provides new insights into the effects of net- work configurations on group-level outcomes. In this study, we empirically established that, under stable membership, a fully connected communication net- work enables the development of a TMS, which in turn leads to positive performance. When turnover occurs, a perfectly centralized network enables the develop- ment of a TMS, which in turn improves performance. In addition, our findings begin to unpack how commu- nication networks interact with membership change to affect group processes. We described two mechanisms through which cen- tralization affects performance: (1) the number of direct communication paths available to team members and (2) the clarity of the coordination logic. The first mech- anism maps onto network density, which affects a team’s behavior through the number of available com- munication paths. The second mechanism maps onto network centralization, which affects a team’s behav- ior through the clarity of the coordination logic. We focused on the two network structures that maximize one structural dimension and minimize the other: (1) a fully connected, all-channel network that maximizes density and minimizes centralization and (2) a hub- and-spoke or perfectly centralized network that max- imizes centralization and minimizes density. Network types, however, vary across both density and central-
  • 81. ization. We suggest that an important issue for future research is to disentangle the effects of network den- sity and centralization. Although it is not possible to make the two dimensions of density and centraliza- tion orthogonal,8 one could design studies where the two dimensions were not highly correlated and thereby investigate their separate effects. On the basis of our theory and evidence, we would predict that density is a more important predictor of group performance when membership is stable and that the clarity of the coordination logic is more important when turnover occurs on complex interdependent tasks such as the ones used here. This theory leads to a variety of pre- dictions about the effects of particular network typolo- gies on group processes and performance under con- ditions of member stability or turnover. For example, D ow nl oa de d fr om i nf or m s.
  • 84. Argote, Aven, and Kush: The Effects of Turnover on Group Performance 204 Organization Science, 2018, vol. 29, no. 2, pp. 191–206, © 2018 INFORMS one prediction would be that a ring or circle network would not perform as well as a hub-and-spoke net- work when turnover occurred, because of the lower centralization and correspondingly less clear coordi- nation logic of the circle network. Another research approach would be to identify networks, such as cer- tain lattices, that do not maximize one network dimen- sion and minimize another but that are, rather, inter- mediate on both dimensions. Although not optimal for either stable or turnover conditions, such a network might perform better when there is uncertainty about whether turnover will occur. Testing these predictions is an interesting avenue for future research. Our results suggest that environments where mem- ber turnover commonly occurs might benefit from en- couraging centralized communication networks. The results of our mediation analyses provide further in- sights into how groups might address membership turnover, such as promoting communication, specif- ically to new members. Just as assigning a facili- tator in brainstorming sessions improves creativity and efficiency (Sutton and Hargadon 1996), assign- ing an incumbent group member to communicate with new members and encourage them to con- tribute might facilitate the integration of new mem- bers even in decentralized communication settings. Alternatively, structured processes or formal proce- dures that facilitate contributions from all members could also be effective in offsetting the negative effects
  • 85. of membership turnover. Future research should exam- ine additional mechanisms for onboarding new mem- bers effectively and enabling them to contribute to group performance. Our results have implications for organizational design and contradict the classic advice in the litera- ture (e.g., see Burns and Stalker 1961) that organiza- tions in dynamic environments should be organized in an organic rather than hierarchical manner. Instead, our study indicates that centralized or hierarchical groups perform better in dynamic rather than in stable envi- ronments but fully connected groups do not when the dynamism is caused by membership change. Thus, our results suggest that taking a fine-grained approach to understanding the source of the dynamism will advance organization design research as well as con- tribute to a growing body of literature on the benefits of certain forms of bureaucracy (Bunderson and Boum- garden 2010). In their seminal work, Burns and Stalker (1961) argued that groups can cultivate either efficiency through constrained, formal, or mandated structures or creativity by permitting more autonomy and emergent coordination. Centralized networks are rarely viewed to be useful for innovation, as their limited pathways reduce the interconnectedness and, thus, interactions of unique idea holders, undermining innovation. How- ever, limited structures may help provide the necessary communication arrangement to share ideas when the group’s membership is not stable. Our findings suggest that turnover provides a means to introduce innovation and creativity into mandated structures or centralized networks. Thus, organizations may be able to gain the
  • 86. benefits of efficiency while also increasing the innova- tion capabilities of their teams by adopting centralized structures coupled with the frequent rotation of team members. Acknowledgments This research was presented at the 2015 Academy of Man- agement meeting in Vancouver, Carnegie Mellon University, Johns Hopkins University, Massachusetts Institute of Tech- nology, the University of California at Santa Barbara, the University of Pennsylvania, the University of Virginia, and Washington University in St. Louis. The authors wish to thank participants in these forums, especially Taya Cohen and Anita Woolley, for their helpful feedback, and members of the SCALE (Social Coordination Across Large Environ- ments) project, especially Jim Herbsleb and Anita Sarma, for their helpful advice. They would also like to thank senior edi- tor Tiziana Casciaro and the reviewers for their very thought- ful comments. Endnotes 1Data from seven groups were dropped from the analysis. Two groups failed to follow the experiment’s instructions, and five groups contained members who had previously participated in the experiment. 2Eleven groups were inadvertently given 10 minutes instead of 5. There was no main effect of this on new features, but a control vari- able was included in all predictions of new features (see the notes to Table 2). 3We used the R (version 3.1.3) package tnet (Opsahl 2012, ver- sion 3.011) to convert the networks, and we used the R package igraph (Csárdi and Nepusz 2006, version 0.7.1) to calculate
  • 87. Freeman’s (1979) centralization. 4Betweenness centralization, variance in how often a member resides on the shortest communication path between all possible pairs, was also used as a robustness check. Similar to degree centralization, betweenness centralization had a positive correlation with central- ization (0.79, p < 0.001). 5To aid in interpretation, we report coefficients for ordinary least squares models; however, all analyses involving errors and new fea- tures were repeated with Poisson regressions, a version of the gen- eralized linear model appropriate for a dependent variable that is a count. These Poisson regressions produced results very similar to those presented here. 6As a robustness check, we repeated these analyses using just dyadic communication frequency that occurred within the first six minutes of the programming task. Six minutes was the minimum time to completion among all the groups on the programming task. Focus- ing on the first six minutes allowed us to explore variations in dyadic communication frequency without having to account for time to complete the task. Results from these analyses were identical to those using dyadic communication frequency across the entire time groups worked. The serial pathway in the moderated serial
  • 88. media- tion predicting errors was smaller (no turnover, B �−0.13; 95% CIs: D ow nl oa de d fr om i nf or m s. or g by [ 13 4. 88 .7 7.
  • 90. e on ly , a ll r ig ht s re se rv ed . Argote, Aven, and Kush: The Effects of Turnover on Group Performance Organization Science, 2018, vol. 29, no. 2, pp. 191–206, © 2018 INFORMS 205 −0.48, 0.05; turnover, B � −0.41; 95% CIs: −0.89, −0.16) though still significantly moderated by turnover (95% CIs: −0.71, −0.01). 7These analyses were repeated using communications from only the first five minutes of the task. Five minutes was the minimum time
  • 91. to completion for the feature-generating task. Results from these analyses are similar in pattern and direction to those reported here, indicating that the results are robust. 8The lowest possible network density in any fully connected network is always greater than the density for a perfectly centralized network. The formula for the density of a fully connected network with the lowest possible density would be N/(N · (N −1)/2), where N is the number of members of the group. The formula for the density of a perfectly centralized network is similar, but the numerator is N − 1 instead of N. Thus, all fully connected groups have higher density than their perfectly centralized counterparts. References Ahuja MK, Galletta DF, Carley KM (2003) Individual centrality and performance in virtual R&D groups: An empirical study. Man- agement Sci. 49(1):21–38. Arrow H, McGrath JE (1995) Membership dynamics in groups at work: A theoretical framework. Res. Organ. Behav. 17:373–373. Aven BL (2015) The paradox of corrupt networks: An analysis of organizational crime at Enron. Organ. Sci. 26(4):980–996.
  • 92. Balkundi P, Harrison DA (2006) Ties, leaders, and time in teams: Strong inference about network structure’s effects on team via- bility and performance. Acad. Management J. 49(1):49–68. Bernard HR, Killworth P, Kronenfeld D, Sailer L (1984) The problem of informant accuracy: The validity of retrospective data. Annual Rev. Anthropol. 13:495–517. Blau PM (1974) Presidential address: Parameters of social structure. Amer. Sociol. Rev. 39(5):615–635. Borgatti SP, Cross R (2003) A relational view of information seeking and learning in social networks. Management Sci. 49(4):432– 445. Borgatti SP, Everett MG, Johnson JC (2013) Analyzing Social Networks (Sage, London). Bosch J, Bosch-Sijtsema P (2010) From integration to composition: On the impact of software product lines, global development and ecosystems. J. Systems Software 83(1):67–76. Bunderson JS, Boumgarden P (2010) Structure and learning in self- managed teams: Why “bureaucratic” teams can be better learn- ers. Organ. Sci. 21(3):609–624. Burns T, Stalker G (1961) The Management of Innovation (Tavistock
  • 93. Press, London). Casciaro T, Barsade SG, Edmondson AC, Gibson CB, Krackhardt D, Labianca G (2015) The integration of psychological and net- work perspectives in organizational scholarship. Organ. Sci. 26(4):1162–1176. Chao GT, O’Leary-Kelly AM, Wolf S, Klein HJ, Gardner PD (1994) Organizational socialization: Its content and consequences. J. Appl. Psych. 79(5):730–743. Choi HS, Levine JM (2004) Minority influence in work teams: The impact of newcomers. J. Experiment. Soc. Psych. 40(2):273– 280. Choi HS, Thompson L (2005) Old wine in a new bottle: Impact of membership change on group creativity. Organ. Behav. Human Decision Processes 98(2):121–132. Cohen J (1960) A coefficient of agreement for nominal scales. Educa- tional Psych. Measurement 20(1):37–46. Croson R, Anand J, Agarwal R (2007) Using experiments in corporate strategy research. Eur. Management Rev. 4(3):173–181. Csárdi G, Nepusz T (2006) The igraph software package for complex network research. Interjournal, Complex Systems: Manuscript 1695. DeChurch LA, Mesmer-Magnus JR (2010) The cognitive
  • 94. underpin- nings of effective teamwork: A meta-analysis. J. Appl. Psych. 95(1):32–53. Devine DJ, Clayton LD, Philips JL, Dunford BB, Melner SB (1999) Teams in organizations prevalence, characteristics, and effective- ness. Small Group Res. 30(6):678–711. Faraj S, Sproull L (2000) Coordinating expertise in software develop- ment teams. Management Sci. 46(12):1554–1568. Faucheux C, Mackenzie KD (1966) Task dependency of organiza- tional centrality: Its behavioral consequences. J. Experiment. Soc. Psych. 2(4):361–375. Ferriani S, Cattani G, Baden-Fuller C (2009) The relational antecedents of project-entrepreneurship: Network centrality, team composition and project performance. Res. Policy 38(10): 1545–1558. Freeman L (1979) Centrality in social networks conceptual clarifica- tion. Soc. Networks 1(3):215–239. Friedkin NE (1981) The development of structure in random net- works: An analysis of the effects of increasing network density on five measures of structure. Soc. Networks 3(1):41–52. Gino F, Argote L, Miron-Spektor E, Todorova G (2010) First, get your
  • 95. feet wet: The effects of learning from direct and indirect experi- ence on team creativity. Organ. Behav. Human Decision Processes 111(2):102–115. Gouldner AW (1954) Patterns of Industrial Bureaucracy (Free Press, New York). Guetzkow H, Simon H (1955) The impact of certain communica- tion nets upon organization and performance in task-oriented groups. Management Sci. 1(3):233–250. Hancock JI, Allen DG, Bosco FA, McDaniel KR, Pierce CA (2013) Meta-analytic review of employee turnover as a predictor of firm performance. J. Management 39(3):573–603. Hausknecht JP, Holwerda JA (2013) When does employee turnover matter? Dynamic member configurations, productive capacity, and collective performance. Organ. Sci. 24(1):210–225. Hayes AF (2013) Introduction to Mediation, Moderation, and Conditional Process Analysis: A Regression-Based Approach (Guilford Press, New York). Hayes AF (2015) An index and test of linear moderated mediation. Multivariate Behavioral Res. 50(1):1–22. Hollingshead AB (1998) Retrieval processes in transactive
  • 96. memory systems. J. Personality Soc. Psych. 74(3):659–671. Katz N, Lazer D, Arrow H, Contractor N (2004) Network theory and small groups. Small Group Res. 35(3):307–332. Kleinbaum AM, Stuart TE, Tushman ML (2013) Discretion within constraint: Homophily and structure in a formal organization. Organ. Sci. 24(5):1316–1357. Kruchten P (2008) What do software architects do? J. Systems Software 81(12):2413–2416. Kush J, Williamson C, Argote L (2012) Challenges and opportuni- ties for group learning and group learning researchers. Neale MA, Mannix EA, eds. Looking Back, Moving Forward: A Review of Group and Team-Based Research, Research on Managing Groups and Teams, Vol. 15 (Emerald Group Publishing, Bingley, UK), 209–243. Leavitt HJ (1951) Some effects of certain communication patterns on group performance. J. Abnormal Soc. Psych. 46(1):38–50. Leavitt HJ (1996) The old days, hot groups and managers’ lib. Admin. Sci. Quart. 41(2):288–300. LeBreton JM, Senter JL (2008) Answers to 20 questions about inter-
  • 97. rater reliability and interrater agreement. Organ. Res. Methods 11(4):815–852. Lee J, Bachrach DG, Lewis K (2014) Social network ties, transactive memory, and performance in groups. Organ. Sci. 25(3):951– 967. Levine JM, Choi HS, Moreland RL (2003) Newcomer innovation in work teams. Paulus P, Nijstad B, eds. Group Creativity: Innova- tion Through Collaboration (Oxford University Press, New York), 202–224. Lewis K (2003) Measuring transactive memory systems in the field: Scale development and validation. J. Appl. Psych. 88(4):587– 604. Lewis K, Herndon B (2011) Transactive memory systems: Cur- rent issues and future research directions. Organ. Sci. 22(5): 1254–1265. Lewis K, Belliveau M, Herndon B, Keller J (2007) Group cognition, membership change, and performance: Investigating the bene- fits and detriments of collective knowledge. Organ. Behav. Human Decision Processes 103(2):159–178. D ow nl oa
  • 100. ig ht s re se rv ed . Argote, Aven, and Kush: The Effects of Turnover on Group Performance 206 Organization Science, 2018, vol. 29, no. 2, pp. 191–206, © 2018 INFORMS Liang DW, Moreland RL, Argote L (1995) Group versus individual training and group performance: The mediating role of transac- tive memory. Personality Soc. Psych. Bull. 21(4):384–393. Manski CF (1993) Identification of endogenous social effects: The reflection problem. Rev. Econom. Stud. 60(3):531–542. McGrath JE (1984) Groups: Interaction and Performance (Prentice-Hall, Englewood Cliffs, NJ). Moreland RL, Myaskovsky L (2000) Exploring the performance bene- fits of group training: Transactive memory or improved commu-
  • 101. nication? Organ. Behav. Human Decision Processes 82(1):117– 133. Moreland RL, Argote L, Krishnan R (1996) Socially shared cognition at work: Transactive memory and group performance. Nye JL, Brower AM, eds. What’s Social About Social Cognition? Research on Socially Shared Cognition in Small Groups (Sage, Thousand Oaks, CA), 57–84. Morrison EW (2002) Newcomers’ relationships: The role of social network ties during socialization. Acad. Management J. 45(6): 1149–1160. Murphy KR, Myors B (1998) Statistical Power Analysis: A Simple and General Model for Traditional and Modern Hypothesis Tests (Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Mahwah, NJ). Opsahl T (2012) tnet: Software for analysis of weighted, two- mode, and longitudinal networks. Accessed July 1, 2013, https://cran.r -project.org/package�tnet. Preacher KJ, Kelley K (2011) Effect size measures for mediation mod- els: Quantitative strategies for communicating indirect effects. Psych. Methods 16(2):93–115. Ren Y, Argote L (2011) Transactive memory systems 1985– 2010: An integrative framework of key dimensions, antecedents, and con-
  • 102. sequences. Acad. Management Ann. 5(1):189–229. Rulke DL, Galaskiewicz J (2000) Distribution of knowledge, group network structure, and group performance. Management Sci. 46(5):612–625. Sasovova Z, Mehra A, Borgatti SP, Schippers MC (2010) Network churn: The effects of self-monitoring personality on brokerage dynamics. Admin. Sci. Quart. 55(4):639–670. Spector PE (1994) Using self-report questionnaires in OB research: A comment on the use of a controversial method. J. Organ. Behav. 15(5):385–392. Sutton RI, Hargadon A (1996) Brainstorming groups in context: Effectiveness in a product design firm. Admin. Sci. Quart. 41(4): 685–718. Tsay J, Dabbish L, Herbsleb J (2014) Let’s talk about it: Evaluating contributions through discussion in GitHub. Proc. 22nd ACM SIGSOFT Internat. Sympos. Foundations Software Engrg. (ACM, New York), 144–154. Uzzi B, Spiro J (2005) Collaboration and creativity: The small world problem. Amer. J. Sociol. 111(2):447–504. Weber M (1947) Henderson AM, Parsons T, eds. The Theory of Economic and Social Organization (Oxford University Press,
  • 103. New York). Wegner DM (1987) Transactive memory: A contemporary analysis of the group mind. Mullen B, Goethals GR, eds. Theories of Group Behavior (Springer-Verlag, New York), 185–208. Wells WP, Pelz DC (1966) Scientists in Organizations (John Wiley & Sons, New York). Yuan YC, Fulk J, Monge PR, Contractor N (2010) Expertise direc- tory development, shared task interdependence, and strength of communication network ties as multilevel predictors of exper- tise exchange in transactive memory work groups. Comm. Res. 37(1):20–47. Linda Argote is the David M. and Barbara A. Kirr Pro- fessor of Organizational Behavior and Theory at the Tep- per School of Business at Carnegie Mellon University, where she directs the Center for Organizational Learning, Innova- tion and Knowledge. Her research focuses on organizational learning, organizational memory, knowledge transfer, social networks, and group processes and performance. She served as editor-in-chief of Organization Science from 2004 to 2010. Brandy L. Aven is an associate professor of organiza- tional behavior and theory at the Tepper School of Busi- ness, Carnegie Mellon University. She received her Ph.D. in sociology from Stanford University. Her research interests include market and organizational fraud, entrepreneurship, and social networks. Jonathan Kush is an assistant professor of management at the Charlton College of Business, University of Mas-
  • 104. sachusetts Dartmouth. He received his Ph.D. from the Tepper School of Business, Carnegie Mellon University. His research interests include group collaboration, collective cognition, social networks, and their effects on group performance. D ow nl oa de d fr om i nf or m s. or g by [ 13 4. 88 .7
  • 106. us e on ly , a ll r ig ht s re se rv ed . https://cran.r-project.org/package=tnet https://cran.r-project.org/package=tnet Two Types of Privilege According to McIntosh, the first type of privilege is based on “unearned entitlements,” which are things of value that all people should have, such as feeling safe or working in a place where they are valued and accepted for what they have to offer. When an unearned entitlement is restricted to certain groups, however, it becomesPage 22 an “unearned advantage,” a form of privilege. Sometimes it’s possible to do away with unearned advantages without anyone losing out. When a workplace changes so that
  • 107. everyone is valued, for example, the unearned entitlement is available to all and, as such, is no longer a form of privilege. In other cases, however, unearned advantage gives dominant groups a competitive edge they are reluctant to acknowledge, much less give up. This can be especially true of men and whites who lack the advantages of social class and know all too well how hard it is to improve their lives and hang on to what they’ve managed to achieve. This can blind them, however, to the fact that the cultural valuing of whiteness and maleness over color and femaleness loads the odds in their favor in most situations that involve evaluations of credibility or competence. To lose that advantage would greatly increase the amount of competition for white men, who are a shrinking minority of the U.S. population. The second form of privilege, “conferred dominance,” goes a step further by giving one group power over another. The common pattern of men controlling conversations with women, for example, is grounded in a cultural assumption that men are superior and supposed to dominate women. An adolescent boy who appears too willing to defer to his mother risks being called a “mama’s boy,” in the same way that a husband who appears in any way controlled by his wife can be labeled “henpecked” (or worse). The counterpart for girls carries no such stigma: “Daddy’s girl” is not considered an insult in this culture, and there are no insulting terms for a wife who is under the control of her husband. Conferred dominance also manifests itself in white privilege. In his book The Rage of a Privileged Class, for example, Ellis Cose tells the story of an African American lawyer, a partner in a large firm, who goes to the office one Saturday morning and is confronted by a recently hired young white attorney, who has never met the partner. “Can I help you?” says the white man pointedly. The partner shakes his head and tries to pass, but the white man steps in his way and repeats what is now a challenge to the man’s presence in the building: “Can I help you?” Only then
  • 108. does the partner reveal his identity to the young man, who in turn steps aside to let him pass. The white man has no reason to assume the right to control the older man standing before him, except the reason provided by the cultural assumption of white dominance, which can overridePage 23 any class advantage a person of color might have.19 The milder forms of unearned advantage are usually the first to change because they are the easiest for privileged groups to give up. In the decades leading up to the election of Barack Obama as president, for example, national surveys showed a steady decline in the percentage of whites holding racist attitudes toward blacks. This trend is reflected in diversity programs that focus on appreciating or tolerating differences— in other words, extending unearned entitlements to everyone instead of the dominant group alone. It is much harder, however, to do something about power and the unequal distribution of wealth, income, and other resources and rewards. This is why issues of conferred dominance and the stronger forms of unearned advantage get much less attention, and why, when these issues are raised, they can provoke hostile defensiveness and denial. Perhaps more than any other factor, the reluctance to recognize the more serious and entrenched forms of privilege is why most diversity programs serve as little more than a distraction and produce limited and short-lived results.20 Privilege as Paradox Aparadox of privilege is that even though it is received by and benefits individuals, access to it has nothing to do with who they are as people. Male privilege, for example, is more about male people than it is about male people, making me eligible when people assign me to that category, which they can do without knowing a single other thing about me. This means that actually being male is not required in order to receive male privilege. The film, Shakespeare in Love, for example, is set in Elizabethan England, where acting on the stage was reserved for men. The character of Viola wants to act