SlideShare a Scribd company logo
1 of 35
Download to read offline
See discussions, stats, and author profiles for this publication at: https://www.researchgate.net/publication/14192210
Effects of maternal mental retardation and poverty on intellectual, academic,
and behavioral status of school-age children
Article  in  American journal of mental retardation: AJMR · February 1997
Source: PubMed
CITATIONS
88
READS
350
2 authors, including:
Some of the authors of this publication are also working on these related projects:
Group Function-Based Cognitive Behavior Therapy for Children with Autism and Obsessive Compulsive Behavior View project
Province-Wide Survey of the Effects of Quality Assurance Measures on Services for Adults with Intellectual/Developmental Disabilities and Challenging Behaviours View
project
Maurice A Feldman
Brock University
104 PUBLICATIONS   2,462 CITATIONS   
SEE PROFILE
All content following this page was uploaded by Maurice A Feldman on 29 January 2017.
The user has requested enhancement of the downloaded file.
Maternal Mental Retardation and Poverty:
Intellectual, Academic, and Behavioral Status of School-Age Children
Maurice A. Feldman
Queen's University and Ogwanada Hospital
Kingston, Ontario, Canada
and
Nicole Walton-Allen
Chedoke-McMaster Hospitals
Hamilton, Ontario, Canada
Address: Maurice Feldman, Ph.D., Dept. of Psychology, Queen's University, Kingston, Ontario
Canada K7L 3N6.
Key Terms: CHILD DEVELOPMENT, MATERNAL MENTAL RETARDATION, POVERTY
Running Head: MATERNAL MENTAL RETARDATION
Maternal Mental Retardation 2
Maternal Mental Retardation and Poverty:
Intellectual, Academic, and Behavioral Status of School-Age Children
ABSTRACT
This study examined the impact of low maternal IQ on children living in poverty by comparing
WISC-R, WRAT-R, and Ontario Child Behavior Checklist scores of 27 school-age children of
mothers with mild mental retardation to 25 similarly impoverished children whose mothers did
not have mental retardation. The children of mothers with mental retardation had lower IQ and
academic achievement, and more behavior problems than the low income children of parents
without mental retardation. Not one child with a mother with mental retardation was free of
problems and boys were affected more severely than girls. The quality of the home environment
and maternal social supports were also lower in the group with maternal mental retardation; both
of these measures were negatively correlated with child behavior disorders in this group. This
study shows that being raised by mothers with mental retardation can have detrimental effects on
child development and behavior that cannot be attributed to poverty alone.
Maternal Mental Retardation 3
Maternal Mental Retardation and Poverty:
Intellectual, Academic, and Behavioral Status of School-Age Children
The relationship between maternal and child IQ is well-established (Reed & Reed, 1965)
and low parental IQ is related to child intellectual and language delays (Feldman, Case, Towns,
& Betel, 1985; Feldman, Sparks, & Case, 1993; Gillberg & Geijer-Karlsson, 1983; Ramey &
Ramey, 1992). Poverty is also implicated as a risk factor in child development and academic
achievement (Campbell & Ramey, 1994; Parker, Greer, & Zuckerman, 1988; Zigler, 1967). Most
parents with mild mental retardation also have low income (Fotheringham, 1971; Garber, 1988).
Given the paucity of studies that directly compare development of children raised by low income
parents with versus without mental retardation, it is not clear to what extent child problems are a
function of variables related to low maternal IQ or poverty.
In addition to the possible genetic transmission of low intelligence, parents with low IQ
often exhibit deficits in basic child-care, nourishment, and positive interactions which may
jeopardize their children's well being and development (Feldman, Case, & Sparks, 1992;
Feldman et al., 1993; Keltner, 1992). Child maltreatment studies have found a disproportionate
number of parents with mental retardation to be incompetent or abusive (Schilling, Schinke,
Blythe, & Barth, 1982; Seagull & Scheurer, 1986; Taylor et al., 1991). This over-representation
in child custody cases may to some extent reflect society's bias against these parents, but their
interactions with their children nonetheless resemble that of known neglectful parents who did
not have mental retardation (Crittenden & Bonvillian, 1984).
Maternal Mental Retardation 4
There is likely to be an increasing number of parents with low IQ. Courts are banning
involuntary sterilization of persons with disabilities and are upholding parenting as a basic right
of all adult citizens (Hayman, 1990; Vogel, 1987). With virtually all persons with mild mental
retardation now raised and socialized in the community, more of them may exercise their
parenting rights.
Despite concerns about the parenting abilities of persons with mental retardation and the
demands that these families are placing on the social service system (Tymchuk & Feldman,
1991), relatively little is known about the development of their children. To complicate matters,
studies of these parents have used varying definitions of mental retardation; some researchers
(e.g., Reed & Reed, 1965) included only those with IQs less than 70, some used a cut-off of 75
(Garber, 1988; Keltner, 1994), while others (Feldman et al., 1985; Tymchuk, Andron, &
Tymchuk, 1990), used a social system perspective (Mercer, 1973) and accepted parents with IQs
between 70-80. Nevertheless, most studies have reported that children of parents labelled as
having mental retardation have lower mean IQ scores and more of them have scores in the range
of intellectual retardation than would be expected from a random, but not necessarily an
economically disadvantaged, sample of the general population (Bass, l963; Feldman et al., 1985;
Garber, 1988; Gillberg & Geijer-Karlsson, 1983; Mickelson, l947; Reed & Reed, l965; Scally,
l973). Other studies reported significantly less vocalizations and speech in young children of
mothers with mental retardation as compared to peers of low and middle SES whose parents did
not have mental retardation (Feldman, Case, Rincover, Towns, & Betel, 1989; Feldman et al.,
1986, 1993).
Maternal Mental Retardation 5
Most studies of children raised by parents with mental retardation have focused on
intellectual and language deficits (e.g., Feldman et al., 1985; Reed & Reed, 1965). In one study
which assessed behavioral adjustment, O'Neill (1985), using interviews and projective tests,
found behavior problems (e.g., oppositional behaviors, pseudo-retardation) in approximately
50% of 19 "normal" or "bright" children of parents with mental retardation. Likewise, a
retrospective study of 41 offspring of 15 mothers with mental retardation in Sweden found that
58% of the children had required psychiatric services (Gillberg & Geijer-Karlsson, 1983).
Few studies of parents with mental retardation have examined factors that may predict
child development such as the quality of the home environment and familial variables. Feldman
et al. (1985) found a significant positive correlation between total Caldwell HOME Inventory
(Caldwell & Bradley, 1984) scores and Mental Development Index scores on the Bayley Scales
of Infant Development (Bayley, 1969) in 12, 2 year old children of parents with mental
retardation. Keltner (1994) reported that mothers with IQs less than 75 had significantly lower
HOME scores than a low income comparison group of parents with IQs > 85. According to the
results of Feldman et al. (1985), Keltner's findings suggested a greater risk of developmental
delay for the infants of the parents with low IQs, but she did not provide developmental test data.
Maternal social isolation/support is another predictor variable that has not been adequately
studied in parents with intellectual disabilities. Social support has been shown to be related to
child outcomes in families from low SES backgrounds (Bee, Hammond, Eyres, Barnard, &
Snyder, 1986) and in families of parents without mental retardation raising children with
disabilities (Dunst, Trivette, & Cross, 1986).
Maternal Mental Retardation 6
We are aware of no study that has simultaneously evaluated the intellectual, academic,
and behavioral status of school-age children raised by parents with mental retardation. Few
studies of these children examined predictor variables and incorporated a low income
comparison group of parents who do not have mental retardation to control for the impact of
poverty per se on child development. To fill significant gaps in knowledge, the present study
investigated the effects of low maternal intelligence and poverty on several crucial areas of child
development by comparing the performance of children raised in poverty by mothers with and
without mild mental retardation on a battery of standardized measures of intelligence, academic
performance, and behavioral adjustment. We also explored the relationship of child outcome to
the quality of the home environment and maternal social isolation/support.
METHOD
Subjects
Two groups of families with children between the ages of 6 and 12 years participated.
Although there were no restrictions regarding ethnicity of the families, all of the 34 families
referred to this study were Caucasian. Referrals came from 10 community agencies providing
advocacy and support services to adults with mental retardation in Southern and Eastern Ontario
(where Caucasians are the substantial majority). We avoided referrals from child welfare
agencies so as to not over-represent the sample with known maltreating parents with mental
retardation (however, we did not exclude parents whom we subsequently found to be involved
with a child protection agency). Agency workers initially contacted their clients who met the
eligiblity requirements of this study (see below) and referred interested parties to us. Although
Maternal Mental Retardation 7
we do not know the exact number of parents who said they were not interested in participating,
the workers told us that most of the eligible parents agreed to allow us to contact them; no parent
we contacted subsequently refused to participate or dropped out. To address the possibility that
the parents with mild mental retardation may have had difficulty giving informed consent, their
workers accompanied us on the first visit, witnessed our explanation to the parents, and had the
opportunity to express any reservations about the parents' ability to give informed consent (this
was never an issue with any of the participants).
Criteria for inclusion in the mental retardation group required a current maternal full-
scale WAIS-R IQ < 70 (the accepted cut-off at the time of the study - Grossman, 1983) and
previous independent diagnosis of mental retardation with no known biological conditions
associated with cognitive deficits (e.g., Down syndrome, brain damage). Total family income had
to be below the Statistics Canada urban poverty level (Ross & Shillington, 1989). Seven families
were excluded because maternal IQ was greater than 70 (despite a diagnosis of mental
retardation), leaving a total of 27 in the group with maternal mental retardation.
The second group was recruited by placing flyers in community resource and drop-in
centers located in low income neighborhoods in the same Ontario communities in which the
target parents with mental retardation resided. Resource and drop-in centers are used quite
frequently in these communities as they offer a variety of free recreational and educational
programs, advice, and support for children, families, and adults. The first 25 mothers who
responded to the flyers and did not have mental retardation or a history of special education
placement participated and none dropped out.
Maternal Mental Retardation 8
Table 1 provides a comparison of group demographics. For the most part, mothers in both
groups were between 30-35 years old, more than 50% were married (or in a conjugal
relationship), total family incomes were below the poverty level, a majority were receiving
welfare, and few were employed. Eighteen comparison mothers agreed to take an IQ test and all
had WAIS-R IQ > 80. The seven low income mothers who refused to take an IQ test were
included in the study as comparison subjects because their backgrounds suggested at least
average intellectual development (highschool and community college diplomas; previously or
currently employed in the secretarial or accounting fields). There were no significant differences
between the seven who refused to take an IQ test and the remaining 18 comparison families on
any of the measures reported in Tables 1 or 2.
None of the children in either group had known disabilities which could affect their
development. Both sets of families were eligible for services for economically disadvantaged
families such as regular home visits by a public health nurse and access to community resource
centers. Mothers with intellectual disabilities were also eligible for advocacy services. When the
children were younger, early intervention programs for children living in poverty (or for children
of parents with intellectual disabilities) were not as readily available as they are today. In fact,
only one mother (who was in the group with maternal mental retardation) received specialized
early intervention services for the target child in this study (another mother with low IQ received
similar services for a subsequent child not included in this study).
Maternal Mental Retardation 9
--------------------------
INSERT TABLE 1 ABOUT HERE
--------------------------
Dependent measures
Intelligence. The Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children - Revised (WISC-R:
Wechsler, l974) was used. The WISC-III was not as yet available when this study was
conducted.
Academic achievement. Reading, spelling, and arithmetic achievement were assessed
using the Wide Range Achievement Test-Revised (WRAT-R: Jastak & Jastak, l984). The
educational placements of the children were also noted.
Behavior disorders. Behavioral adjustment was measured using the Ontario version of
the Child Behavior Checklist (CBCL: Statistics Canada, 1987) which was validated and normed
on 3294 children as part of a well-documented Ontario Child Health Study (Offord et al., 1987).
It is an augmented version of the original CBCL (Achenbach & Edelbrook, 1981) and is divided
into four behavior disorders: conduct, hyperactivity, emotional, and somatization (the latter scale
was not used as there were no subjects over the required age of 12 years). The mothers were
asked to judge her child's behavior over the last 6 months. To determine whether the mothers
with mental retardation were consistent in reporting on their children's behavior problems, we
also asked them to complete the Parent Attitude Test (PAT: Cowen, Huser, Beach, & Rappaport,
1970). The PAT asks similar questions to the CBCL about the child's behavior. Significant
Spearman Rank correlation coefficients were found between the PAT total score and the three
Maternal Mental Retardation 10
CBCL subscales (conduct: r = .59, p < .001; hyperactivity: r = .40, p < .05; emotional: r = .33, p
= .05).
Quality of the home environment. The elementary school-age version of the HOME
Inventory (Caldwell & Bradley, 1984) was used.
Maternal social isolation/support. We devised a measure of the mother's social
isolation/support by adding the raw scores on the Social Isolation and Marital Satisfaction
subscales of the Parenting Stress Index (PSI: Abidin, 1990). Items in these scales measure the
degree of the mother's social and spousal support (e.g., "I have a lot of people to whom I can talk,
get help or advice." "My spouse has not given me as much help and support as I expected."). The
mother's perception of support on the PSI subscales was partially corroborated by the listing of
all services received by the family on the demographics form. As per the directions of the PSI,
mothers who were not married were asked to respond based on their closest friend.
Design and procedure
A nonequivalent between-group design was used to compare the children of low income
mothers with mental retardation to similar aged children of low income mothers without mental
retardation. The tests were given in the family home by one of two experienced testers, who were
not naive to the study purpose and design, or group classification of the family. The child
received the WISC-R and the WRAT-R in a quiet room free from distractions with only the
tester present (the same conditions applied when the WAIS-R was administered to the mothers).
As many of the mothers with mental retardation had poor reading skills, the questionnaires (e.g.,
CBCL, Social Isolation/Support) were administered orally to all of these mothers; none of the
Maternal Mental Retardation 11
mothers had any problems understanding and responding to the verbal questions and their
responses were consistent with other similar measures (e.g., PAT, demographic questionnaire).
RESULTS
The scores of all the primary dependent measures in the group with maternal mental
retardation failed the test of normalacy even when subjected to several logarithmic
transformations (i.e., base 10, 1/2 log, and arcsine). Thus, we were obliged to use nonparametric
statistical tests such as the two-tailed Mann-Whitney U (z scores were used for n > 20, see
Daniel, 1978), Wilcoxon Signed-Rank, and Chi-Square tests for between group comparisons and
the Spearman Sign-Rank Coefficient for correlational analyses. Table 2 presents the total group,
boy's, and girl's means and standard deviations of the nine dependent measures for both groups.
--------------------------
INSERT TABLE 2 ABOUT HERE
--------------------------
Demographics
As can be seen in Table 1, in addition to differences due to subject selection criteria (the
first four variables), the two groups differed significantly on the percentage of families: (a)
involved with child protection agencies and (b) where the mother reported the father as having
mental retardation or special education experience.
Home Environment
Table 2 shows the Caldwell HOME Inventory mean total scores. The group with maternal
mental retardation was significantly lower than that of the comparison group (z = 6.94; p <
Maternal Mental Retardation 12
.001). Note, however, that the mean total scores of both groups were within one standard
deviation of the school-age HOME Inventory normative group reported in Bradley, Rock,
Caldwell, and Brisby (1989). As seen in Table 2, in the group with maternal mental retardation
(but not in the comparison group), the boys' HOME total scores were significantly lower than
those of the girls (z = 2.74, p < .01); this finding should be interpreted cautiously due to the
disproportionate number of boys to girls in this group.
Social Isolation
The mean maternal social isolation score (see Table 2) of the mothers with mental
retardation was significantly higher than the comparison mothers without mental retardation (z =
10.4, p < .001). In the group with maternal mental retardation (but not in the comparison group),
there was significantly more social isolation amongst the mothers of the boys than the mothers of
the girls (z = 10.63, p < .001); again, these gender differences should be interpreted
conservatively.
Child IQ
Table 2 presents the group WISC-R IQ means and standard deviations and Figure 1
compares the IQ distributions of the children of mothers with and without mental retardation. A
Mann-Whitney U test revealed that the children's IQ scores in the group with maternal mental
retardation were significantly lower than in the low income comparison group (z = 4.27, p <
.001). The group difference upheld for both boys (z = 3.33, p < .001) and girls (z = 2.78, p < .01).
Note that the children in both groups had higher mean IQ scores than their mothers (see Table 1),
perhaps reflecting regression to the mean. A Wilcoxon Signed-Rank test revealed that these
Maternal Mental Retardation 13
mother-child IQ differences were significant in both the group with maternal mental retardation
(z = -3.81, p < .001) and the low income group (z = -1.99, p < .05).
---------------------------------
INSERT FIGURES 1 AND 2 ABOUT HERE
---------------------------------
Academic Achievement
Table 2 presents the group WRAT-R means and standard deviations and Figure 2
illustrates the distributions of scores of the children of mothers with and without mental
retardation. As expected, the academic achievement of the comparison group children was lower
than the norm, but to a lesser degree than the children whose mothers had mental retardation.
Two-tailed Mann-Whitney U tests revealed that the children of mothers with mental retardation
scored significantly lower than the children of mothers without mental retardation on reading (z
= 3.58, p < .001), spelling (z = 3.04, p < .003), and math (z = 3.96, p < .001). Between-group
differences were significant for boys (reading - z = 2.79. p < .006; spelling - z = 3.06, p < .003;
math - z = 2.95, p < .004); the girls showed marginally significant differences in reading (z =
1.78, p < .08) and math (z = 1.87, p < .07) and no statistically significant difference in spelling (z
= .48, p > .6) (because the relatively small number of girls increases the risk of Type II error,
their marginally significant results are reported for the WRAT-R above, and the CBCL, below).
Using the local school board's diagnostic criteria for learning disabilities (i.e., having a
normal IQ with a 15 point split between performance and verbal WISC-R scores, and at least one
year behind in either reading, spelling, or math achievement test scores), we found that more
Maternal Mental Retardation 14
(36.4%) of the children of mothers with mental retardation than the children of parents without
mental retardation (4.5%) met the criteria (X2
= 5.8, p < .02). Also, 59.3% of the children of
mothers with mental retardation were receiving various special education services for children
identified as having "mental retardation," "learning disabilities," or "behavioral maladjustment"
(e.g., full- or part-day self-contained classes, specialized curriculum and instruction, availability
of an Education Aide in the regular classroom, tutoring) compared to 12% of the children of
parents without mental retardation; this difference was also statistically significant (X2
= 11.5, p
< .001).
Behavior Disorders
Figure 3 shows that the group with maternal mental retardation had a higher percentage of
children scoring above the CBCL clinical thresholds for behavioral disorders than the low
income comparison and the Ontario normative groups. Table 2 shows that the children of
mothers with mental retardation had significantly higher scores than the low income comparison
children in conduct disorders (z = 2.80, p < .005), hyperactivity (z = 3.30, p < .001), and
emotional disorders (z = 2.20, p < .03). Between-group CBCL differences for boys were
significant on all three scales (conduct and emotional - both zs = 2.47, ps < .02; hyperactivity - z
= 2.70, p < .007); the girls showed marginally significant differences in hyperactivity (z = 1.76, p
< .08), but differences on the other two scales did not approach significance for the girls (conduct
- z = 0.48; emotional - z = 0.66, ps > .5).
We tested the hypothesis (O'Neill, 1985) that more competent children of slow parents
would have more social maladjustment by examining the percentage of children with clinically
Maternal Mental Retardation 15
significant behavior problems (as determined by CBCL cut-off scores) with each group
subdivided into child IQ below 85 or equal/above 85. In the group with maternal mental
retardation, a greater percentage of children with IQs> 85 (n = 11) had at least one behavior
problem (63.6% vs. 43.8%). Although this numerical difference was not statistically significant,
an examination of children with multiple behavior problems revealed that while no children with
low IQ scored above the clinical threshold on all three scales of the CBCL, 27.3% of the children
with IQ > 85 did; this difference was significant (z = 2.22, p< .02), but did not hold for the low
income comparison children as none of these children had all three behavior problems. Thus, the
children who met criteria for multiple behavior problems had IQs > 85 and mothers with mental
retardation (IQs < 70).
--------------------------------------
INSERT FIGURE 3 AND TABLE 3 ABOUT HERE
--------------------------------------
Correlations
As the above results indicated that children of mothers with mental retardation apparently
were not all affected in the same way -- despite being raised by mothers with similar IQs -- we
ran a series of simple correlations using the Spearman Sign-Rank coefficient, appropriate for
non-normal distributions. We correlated the child outcome measures with maternal WAIS-R,
Social Isolation/Support, and Caldwell HOME Inventory scores. These variables have been
shown in the literature to be related to child development in low income families (Parker et al.,
Maternal Mental Retardation 16
1988), and they were significantly different between the groups in this study. The coefficients are
presented in Table 3.
In the group of children of mothers with mental retardation, maternal IQ was not
significantly correlated with any child measure. Maternal social isolation was significantly
positively correlated, and HOME total scores were significantly negatively correlated with child
conduct and hyperactivity disorders; HOME total score was also significantly correlated with
WRAT-R math scores.
With respect to the low income mothers without mental retardation, maternal IQ was
significantly correlated with WRAT-R math scores. Maternal social isolation was significantly
related to child hyperactivity and emotional disorders; the HOME total score was significantly
correlated with WISC-R and WRAT-R reading and math scores. Thus, both groups share in
common significant correlations between maternal social isolation and child behavior problems,
as well as HOME scores and math achievement.
DISCUSSION
This study reveals the risk status of school-age children of mothers with mental
retardation across intellectual, academic, behavioral, and family environment domains. There
was no child in the group with maternal mental retardation who was completely free of problems.
Close to 60% had IQs below 85 and even those children who had normal intelligence met the
criteria for either a behavior disorder or a learning disability. These problems cannot be attributed
solely to being raised in poverty because the children of mothers with mental retardation had
significantly more deficits than similar aged children of parents without mental retardation from
Maternal Mental Retardation 17
comparably impoverished families in the same communities. In addition to the child outcomes,
the mothers with mental retardation were providing less stimulating home environments and
were more socially isolated than the comparison mothers without mental retardation.
Maternal mental retardation did not affect boys and girls in the same manner. While both
the boys and girls of mothers with mental retardation had significantly lower IQs than their low
income counterparts raised by mothers without mental retardation, only the boys had
significantly lower academic achievement and significantly higher behavior problem scores.
These gender differences may be partly related to the boys' significantly lower quality of the
home environment and the significantly greater social isolation of their mothers; no other
variables (listed in Table 1) were significantly different between the boys and girls. Further
research is needed with a larger sample to explore possible gender differences in this population.
Given the finding that the boys in the group with maternal mental retardation were
generally more negatively affected than the girls, it is conceivable that the between-group
differences on the dependent measures may have been influenced by the group disparities in the
proportion of boys to girls. To ascertain this potential confound, we reformed the two groups so
that they had the same number of boys (n = 12) and girls (n = 9). This was accomplished by
randomly selecting 12 boys from the original pool of 18 in the group with maternal mental
retardation and then adding them to the original 9 girls in this group. In the low income
comparison group, we randomly chose 9 girls from the pool of 13 and added them to the original
12 boys in this group. Between-group Mann-Whitney U tests on all the dependent measures
were still highly significant (zs <.001). Nevertheless, between-gender differences in this study
Maternal Mental Retardation 18
should be viewed conservatively because of the relatively small number of girls in the group with
maternal mental retardation.
The results of this study suggest that there may be two distinct types of school-age
Caucasian children raised in poverty who can be differentiated by their mothers' intelligence.
While these findings await replication with more subjects and naive testers, the differences in the
children found here suggest that research examining the impact of poverty on child development
should more closely examine parental IQ as a contributing factor in the variable and often
adverse child outcomes seen in economically disadvantaged families (Garber, 1988; Garner,
Carson Jones, & Miner, 1994; Ramey & Ramey, 1992).
The children's intellectual problems were also reflected in their poor academic
achievement, especially for the boys. Besides having significantly lower WRAT-R scores than
the comparison children, the children of mothers with mental retardation were eight times more
likely to meet the local school board's criterion for a diagnosis of learning disabilities, and five
times more likely to receive special education services. The special education placements
provided some independent corroboration of our test scores.
The boys of mothers with mental retardation were also at considerable risk for behavior
problems as measured by the Ontario CBCL. More data are needed with a larger sample to
determine if child behavior problems may be related to inadequate knowledge and skills in basic
child management strategies such as positive reinforcement, supervision, limit-setting, and
consistent discipline seen in parents with mental retardation (Fantuzzo, Wray, Hall, Goins, &
Azar, 1986; Tymchuk et al., 1990).
Maternal Mental Retardation 19
In the group with maternal mental retardation, the children with average intelligence were
more likely to have multiple behavior problems than the children whose IQs were below 85. A
substantiated explanation of this result is presently lacking. One can speculate that conflicts may
arise when the parents fail to set reasonable limits and comprehend their (average IQ) children's
more abstract communications. These children may quickly learn to take advantage of, and rebel
against less competent parents. Early anti-social acts directed at their parents (e.g., disobedience,
stealing) may subsequently generalize to other authority figures (O'Neill, 1985). Further research
with more subjects is needed to determine why and how the combination of having normal
intelligence and parents with mental retardation places children at-risk for behavioral
maladjustment.
Not all the children of mothers with mental retardation were adversely affected in the
same way. On the WISC-R, 37% of the children had IQs <70 (i.e., mental retardation range),
while 40.7% had IQs > 85. Moreover, 26% of the children of mothers with mental retardation
were at or close to expected grade level in reading, spelling, and arithmetic, and about 36% did
not exhibit clinically significant behavior problems.
Correlational analyses of key variables likely related to differential outcomes revealed
that for the group with maternal mental retardation, maternal social isolation/support and the
quality of the home environment were related to child behavioral, but not intellectual outcomes.
It may be the case that factors other than the ones we examined in this study may also be
implicated in the development of school-age children of parents with mental retardation. For
example, maternal depression has been shown to be related to negative child outcomes
Maternal Mental Retardation 20
(Hammen, Burge, & Stansbury, 1990) and the prevalence of depression is relatively high in
adults with mild mental retardation (Eaton & Menolascino, 1982). Perhaps, a measure of
depression in the mothers with mental retardation in this study would also have been
significantly related to child developmental and behavioral outcomes.
A transactional approach (Sameroff & Chandler, 1975) which describes the interaction of
organismic and environmental variables on child outcome could be utilized to help identify
variables affecting the vulnerability and resiliency of these children (Crittenden, 1985). Future
research should try to differentiate children of parents with mental retardation who are at greater
risk of subsequent problems. Following the transactional model, it is important to examine the
accumulating impact of adverse preconceptual genetic influences (e.g., Fragile X), prenatal (e.g.,
maternal nutrition, smoking, stress) and perinatal factors (e.g., low birthweight, congenital
cytomegalovirus infection) in conjunction with subsequent environmental, nutritional,
personality, and mother-child interactional variables (Bee et al., 1982; Breitmayer & Ramey,
1986; Parker et al., 1988).
Given the results of this study in the context of an increasing societal trend to both protect
parenting rights and provide supports and services to try to keep the natural family intact, further
attention should be paid to the development and evaluation of specialized services for these
families (Feldman, 1994; Walton-Allen & Feldman, 1991). Several studies have shown that
child language and cognitive delays can be reduced either by teaching mothers with mental
retardation to interact in a more sensitive, reinforcing, and responsive manner with their children
(Feldman et al., 1993; Slater, 1986) or enrolling the child in a specialized preschool (Garber,
Maternal Mental Retardation 21
1988; Ramey & Ramey, 1992). In the present study, the one child in the group with maternal
mental retardation whose mother received interaction training when he was an infant had an IQ
of 97. Future early intervention efforts should focus on the prevention of not only intellectual
and academic deficits but also behavioral and psychiatric disorders in these children.
Considerable work is still needed in first identifying, and then preventing, eliminating or
compensating for factors which promote the intergenerational recurrence of child developmental
and other problems in these families (Ramey, 1992; Zigler, 1967).
Maternal Mental Retardation 22
REFERENCES
Abidin, R.R. (1990). Parenting stress index - manual. Charlottesville, VA: Pediatric Psychology
Press.
Achenbach, T.M., & Edelbrook, C.S. (1981). Behavioral problems and competencies reported by
parents of normal and disturbed children aged four through sixteen. Monograph on Social Research in
Child Development, 46: (1, Serial No. 188).
Bass, M.S. (1963). Marriage, parenthood, and prevention of pregnancy. American Journal of Mental
Deficiency, 68, 320-335.
Bayley, N. (1969). Bayley scales of infant development: Birth to two years. New York:
Psychological Corporation.
Bee, H.L., Barnard, K.E., Eyres, S.J., Gray, C.L., Hammond, M.A., Spietz, A.L., Snyder, C., &
Clark, B. (1982). Predictions of IQ and language skill from perinatal status, child performance, family
characteristics, and mother-infant interaction. Child Development, 53, 1134-1156.
Bee, H.L., Hammond, M.A., Eyres, S.J., Barnard, K.E., & Snyder, C. (1986). The impact of
parental life change on the early development of children. Research in Nursing and Health, 9, 65-74.
Bradley, R.L., Rock, S.L., Caldwell, B.M., & Brisby, J. (1989). Uses of the HOME Inventory for
families with handicapped children. American Journal on Mental Retardation, 94, 313-330.
Breitmayer, B.J., & Ramey, C.T. (1986). Biological nonoptimality and quality of postnatal
environment as codeterminants of intellectual development. Child Development, 57, 1151-1165.
Caldwell, B., & Bradley, R. (1984). Home Observation for Measurement of the environment.
Unpublished manuscript. Little Rock: University of Arkansas.
Maternal Mental Retardation 23
Campbell, F.A., & Ramey, C.T. (1994). Effects of early intervention on intellectual and academic
achievement: A follow-up study of children from low income families. Child Development, 65, 684-
698.
Cowen, E.L., Huser, J., Beach, D.R., & Rappaport, J. (1970). Parent perceptions of young children
and their relation to indexes of development. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 34, 97-
103.
Crittenden, P.M. (1985). Maltreated infants: Vulnerability and resilience. Journal of Child
Psychology and Psychiatry, 26, 85-96.
Crittenden, P.M., & Bonvillian, J.D. (1984). The relationship between maternal risk status and
maternal sensitivity. American Journal of Orthopsychiatry, 54, 250-262.
Daniel, W.W. (1978). Applied nonparametric statistics. Boston: Houghton-Mifflin.
Dunst, C.J., Trivette, C., & Cross, A. (1986). Mediating influences of social support: Personal,
family, and child outcomes. American Journal of Mental Deficiency, 90, 403-417.
Eaton, L., & Menolascino, F.J. (1982). Psychiatric disorders in the mentally retarded: Types,
problems, and challenges. American Journal of Psychiatry, 139, 1297-1303.
Fantuzzo, J.W., Wray, L., Hall, R., Goins, C., & Azar, S. (1986). Parent and social-skills training
for mentally retarded mothers identified as child maltreaters. American Journal of Mental Deficiency,
91, 135-140.
Feldman, M.A. (1994). Parenting education for parents with intellectual disabilities: A review of
outcome studies. Research in Developmental Disabilities, 15, 299-332.
Maternal Mental Retardation 24
Feldman, M.A., Case, L., Rincover, A., Towns, F., & Betel, J. (1989). Parent education project III:
Increasing affection and responsivity in developmentally handicapped mothers: Component analysis,
generalization, and effects on child language. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 22, 211-222.
Feldman, M.A., Case, L., & Sparks, B. (1992). Effectiveness of a child-care training program for
parents at-risk for child neglect. Canadian Journal of Behavioral Science, 24, 14-28.
Feldman, M.A., Case, L., Towns, F., & Betel, J. (1985). Parent Education Project I: The
development and nurturance of children of mentally retarded parents. American Journal of Mental
Deficiency, 90, 253-258.
Feldman, M.A., Sparks, B., & Case, L. (1993). Effectiveness of home-based early intervention on
the language development of children of mothers with mental retardation. Research in Developmental
Disabilities, 14, 387-408.
Feldman, M.A., Towns, F., Betel, J., Case, L., Rincover, A., & Rubino, C.. (1986). Parent
Education Project II: Increasing stimulating interactions of developmentally handicapped mothers.
Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 19, 23-37.
Fotheringham, J.B. (1971). The concept of social competence as applied to marriage and child care
in those classified as mentally retarded. Canadian Medical Association Journal, 104, 813-816.
Garber, H.L. (1988). The Milwaukee project. Preventing mental retardation in children at risk.
Washington, DC: American Association on Mental Retardation.
Garner, P.W., Carlson Jones, D., & Miner, J.L. (1994). Social competence among low-income
preschoolers: Emotion socialization practices and social cognitive correlates. Child Development, 65,
622-637.
Maternal Mental Retardation 25
Gillberg, C., & Geijer-Karlsson, M. (1983). Children born to mentally retarded women: A 1-21 year
follow-up study of 41 cases. Psychological Medicine, 13, 891-894.
Grossman, H.J. (Ed.). (1983). Classification in mental retardation. Washington, DC: American
Association on Mental Deficiency.
Hammen, C., Burge, D., & Stansbury, K. (1990). Relationship of mother and child variables to
child outcomes in a high-risk sample. Developmental Psychology, 26, 24-30.
Hayman, R.L. (1990). Presumptions of justice: Law, politics, and the mentally retarded parent.
Harvard Law Review, 103, 1201-1271.
Jastak, J., & Jastak, S. (1984). Wide Range Achievement Test-Revised. Wilmington, DE: Jastak
Associates.
Keltner, B.R. (1992). Caregiving by mothers with mental retardation. Family and Community
Health, 15, 10-18.
Keltner, B.R. (1994). Home environments of mothers with mental retardation. Mental Retardation,
32, 123-127.
Mercer, J.R. (1973). Labelling the mentally retarded: Clinical and social system perspectives on
mental retardation. Berkeley, CA: University of California Press.
Mickelson, P. (1947). The feebleminded parent: A study of 90 family cases. American Journal of
Mental Deficiency, 51, 644-653.
Offord, D.R., Boyle, M.H., Szatmari, P., Rae-Grant, N.I., Links, P.S., Cadman, D.T., Byles, J.A.,
Crawford, J.W., Blum, H.M., Byrne, C., Thomas, H., & Woodward, C.A. (1987). Ontario child health
Maternal Mental Retardation 26
study: II. Six month prevalence of disorder and rates of service utilization. Archives of General
Psychiatry, 44, 832-836.
O'Neill, A.M. (1985). Normal and bright children of mentally retarded parents: The Huck Finn
syndrome. Child Psychiatry and Human Development 15, 255-268.
Parker, S., Greer, S., & Zuckerman, B. (1988). Double jeopardy: The impact of poverty on early
child development. The Pediatric Clinics of North America, 35, 1227-1239.
Ramey, C.T. (1992). High-risk children and IQ: Altering intergenerational patterns. Intelligence, 16,
239-256.
Ramey, C.T., & Ramey, S.L. (1992). Effective early intervention. Mental Retardation, 30, 337-345.
Reed, E.W., & Reed, S.C. (1965). Mental retardation: A family study. Philadelphia: Saunders.
Ross, D.P., & Shillington, E.R. (1989). The Canadian fact book on poverty - 1989. Ottawa:
Canadian Council on Social Development.
Sameroff, A.J., & Chandler, M. (1975). Reproductive risk and the continuum of caretaker causality.
In F. Horowitz (Ed.), Review of child development research (vol. 4) (pp. 157-243). Chicago:
University of Chicago Press.
Scally, B.G. (1973). Marriage and mental handicap: Some observations in Northern Ireland. In F.F.
de la Cruz and G.D. La Veck (Eds.) Human sexuality and the mentally retarded (pp. 186-194). New
York: Brunner/Mazel.
Schilling, R.F., Schinke, S.P, Blythe, B.J., & Barth, R.P. (1982). Child maltreatment and mentally
retarded parents: Is there a relationship? Mental Retardation, 20, 201-209.
Maternal Mental Retardation 27
Seagull, E.A., & Scheurer, S.L. (1986). Neglected and abused children of mentally retarded parents.
Child Abuse and Neglect, 10, 493-500.
Slater, M.A. (1986). Modification of mother-child interaction processes in families with children at-
risk for mental retardation. American Journal of Mental Deficiency, 91, 257-267.
Statistics Canada (1987). Ontario Child Health Study Child Behaviour Checklist. Ottawa: Author.
Taylor, C.G., Norman, D.K., Murphy, J.M., Jellinek, M., Quinn, D., Poitrast, F.G., & Goshko, M.
(1991). Diagnosed intellectual and emotional impairment among parents who seriously mistreat their
children: Prevalence, type, and outcome in a court sample. Child Abuse and Neglect, 15, 389-401.
Tymchuk, A.J., Andron, L., & Tymchuk, M. (1990). Training mothers with mental handicaps to
understand developmental and behavioural principles. Mental Handicap Research, 3, 51-59.
Tymchuk, A.J., & Feldman, M.A. (1991). Parents with mental retardation and their children: A
review of research relevant to professional practice. Canadian Psychology/Psychologie Canadienne, 32,
486-494.
Vogel, P. (1987). The right to parent. Entourage, 2, 33-39.
Walton-Allen, N., & Feldman, M.A. (1991). Perceptions of service needs by parents who are
mentally retarded and their workers. Comprehensive Mental Health Care, 1, 57-67.
Wechsler, D. (1974). Manual for the Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children - Revised.
Zigler, E.F. (1967). Familial mental retardation: A continuing dilemma. Science, 155, 292-298.
Maternal Mental Retardation 28
Table 1
Demographic information
Family Variables Parents with Parents without
Mental Retardation Mental Retardation
(n = 27) (n = 25)
Subject-Selection Variables
Mean Maternal IQ (WAIS-R) 63.6 (5.5) 93.8 (11.2)**
Percent of Mothers Who Received Special Ed. Services 48.1% 0**
Mean No. of Social Service Agencies Involved
Per Family 2.27 (1.44) 0
Median Range of Annual Family Incomea
$7K - 10.5K $7K - 10.5K
Other variables
Mean Maternal Age (years) 35.1 (4.7) 33.7 (6.1)
Percent Single Mothers 44.4% 44.0%
Percent of Mothers Institutionalized 3.7% 0
Percent Mothers Receiving Welfare 88.9% 68.0%
Percent Mothers Employed 11.1% 24.0%
Percent Living in Subsidized Housing 63.0% 48.0%
Mean Crowding Ratiob
1.62 (0.6) 1.49 (0.4)
Percent with Father Reported to
be MR or Received Special Ed. Services 66.7% 8%**
Maternal Mental Retardation 29
Table 1 (Con't)
Demographic information
Family Variables Parents with Parents without
Mental Retardation Mental Retardation
(n = 27) (n = 25)
Percent with Target
Child's Sibling Removed
14.8%
0
Percent with Child Welfare Supervision 22.2% 0*
Mean Child Age 9.7 (2.2) 10.2 (2.3)
Mean Birth Order of Target Child 1.1 (0.4) 1.3 (0.5)
Proportion of Boys to Girls 19/8 12/13
*
p< .05; **
p< .01; the numbers in brackets are standard deviations.
a
Exact incomes are not available as parents were asked to indicate income within C$5K intervals from 0-
C$5K to > C$25K (converted here to US funds).
b
Crowding Ratio = total number of persons living at home divided by the total number of rooms.
Maternal Mental Retardation 30
Table 2
Means and standard deviations of dependent measures
Parents with Mental Retardation Parents without Mental Retardation
MEASURES TOTAL BOYS GIRLS TOTAL BOYS GIRLS
HOME 36.52
(8.07)
34.11
(7.71)
41.33
(6.80)
44.64
(6.34)
46.18
(6.14)
43.09
(6.43)
SOCIAL
ISOLATIONa
39.85
(6.00)
41.33
(6.15)
36.89
(4.65)
33.75
(7.16)
33.33
(6.73)
34.17
(7.84)
WISC-R 80.54
(14.32)
80.11
(15.13)
81.50
(13.22)
102.88
(14.25)
103.58
(16.66)
102.23
(12.28)
WRAT-R
READING
73.20
(19.10)
71.05
(19.49)
77.75
(18.65)
94.25
(16.80)
97.18
(23.34)
91.77
(8.50)
WRAT-R
SPELLING
71.56
(21.38)
66.88
(21.42)
81.50
(18.77)
89.54
(17.50)
93.09
(20.34)
86.54
(14.86)
WRAT-R
MATH
69.68
(14.68)
68.12
(14.06)
73.00
(16.38)
89.21
(13.12)
89.27
(16.80)
89.15
(9.75)
CBCL
CONDUCT
6.30
(5.43)
7.28
(5.62)
4.33
(4.69)
2.36
(2.06)
2.75
(2.14)
2.00
(2.00)
CBCL
HYPERACT.
5.44
(2.71)
6.17
(2.71)
4.00
(2.18)
2.76
(2.50)
3.17
(2.33)
2.38
(2.69)
CBCL
EMOTIONAL
5.93
(2.56)
6.06
(2.26)
5.67
(3.20)
4.44
(3.50)
3.50
(2.61)
5.31
(4.07)
a
Social Isolation: higher scores indicate more maternal social isolation and less social support
Maternal Mental Retardation 31
Table 3
Spearman sign-rank correlation coefficents of parental IQ, social isolation, and home environment scores
with child IQ, academic achievement, and behavior problem scores
Parents with Mental Retardation Parents without Mental Retardation
WAIS-R SOC. ISOL. HOME WAIS-R SOC. ISOL. HOME
WISC-R -.30 .20 .22 .10 .13 .51**
WRAT-READ .17 .09 .25 -.04 .04 .47*
WRAT-SPELL -.20 -.09 .30 -.27 .09 .27
WRAT-MATH -.14 .08 .45*
.42*
.07 .60**
CBCL-CON. -.30 .48*
-.43*
.04 .18 -.04
CBCL-HYP. -.08 .47*
-.39*
.29 .45*
-.21
CBCL-EMOT. .10 .30 .14 .09 .37*
-.11
*
p< .05; **
p< 01
Maternal Mental Retardation 32
Figure Captions
Figure 1. Distribution of WISC-R full scale IQ scores for children of parents with mental
retardation and low income comparison children of parents without mental retardation.
Figure 2. Distribution of WRAT-R Reading, Spelling, and Arithmetic standard scores for
children of parents with mental retardation and low income comparison children of parents
without mental retardation.
Figure 3. Percentage of children of parents with mental retardation, low income comparison
children of parents without mental retardation, and Ontario norms for CBCL scores above the
threshold for clinically significant behavior disorders.
MATERNAL MENTAL RETARDATION 33
Authors' Notes
This research was sponsored by the Ontario Mental Health Foundation and the Ontario Ministry
of Community and Social Services Research Grants Program (administered by the Research and
Program Evaluation Unit). We thank: J. Carnwell, L. Case, M. Garrick, W. MacIntyre-Grande,
and R. Malik for their assistance in collecting the data; Drs. J. Berg, and D. Yu for their feedback
on earlier versions of the manuscript; and Dr. L. Atkinson for his statistical advice and insightful
commentary. Requests for reprints should be sent to Maurice Feldman, Ph.D., Dept. of
Psychology, Queen's University, Kingston, Ontario Canada K7L 3N6; email:
feldman@pavlov.psyc.queensu.ca.
Maternal Mental Retardation and Poverty:
Intellectual, Academic, and Behavioral Status of School-Age Children
Key Terms: CHILD DEVELOPMENT, MATERNAL MENTAL RETARDATION, POVERTY
Running Head: MATERNAL MENTAL RETARDATION
View publication statsView publication stats

More Related Content

What's hot

Santrock 16e ch03_accessible
Santrock 16e ch03_accessibleSantrock 16e ch03_accessible
Santrock 16e ch03_accessiblelightdark13
 
Santrock 16e ch10_accessible
Santrock 16e ch10_accessibleSantrock 16e ch10_accessible
Santrock 16e ch10_accessiblelightdark13
 
Case Management and Outcomes for Neglected Children: A 5 year follow up study
Case Management and Outcomes for Neglected Children: A 5 year follow up studyCase Management and Outcomes for Neglected Children: A 5 year follow up study
Case Management and Outcomes for Neglected Children: A 5 year follow up studyBASPCAN
 
Santrock 16e ch11_accessible
Santrock 16e ch11_accessibleSantrock 16e ch11_accessible
Santrock 16e ch11_accessiblelightdark13
 
AR Well Child Reads/Reach Out and Read Hunger Conference
AR Well Child Reads/Reach Out and Read Hunger ConferenceAR Well Child Reads/Reach Out and Read Hunger Conference
AR Well Child Reads/Reach Out and Read Hunger ConferenceEd_doc_Peggy
 
Corboy_ASDsiblings2016
Corboy_ASDsiblings2016Corboy_ASDsiblings2016
Corboy_ASDsiblings2016Daire Corboy
 
Santrock 16e ch02_accessible
Santrock 16e ch02_accessibleSantrock 16e ch02_accessible
Santrock 16e ch02_accessiblelightdark13
 
Au Psy492 W7 A2 Pp Golub R
Au Psy492 W7 A2 Pp Golub RAu Psy492 W7 A2 Pp Golub R
Au Psy492 W7 A2 Pp Golub RRachel Golub
 
Santrock 16e ch16_accessible
Santrock 16e ch16_accessibleSantrock 16e ch16_accessible
Santrock 16e ch16_accessiblelightdark13
 
concept of child development
concept of child developmentconcept of child development
concept of child developmentSalina Sultana
 
Wekerle CIHR Team - Anne Niec - Understanding Child Maltreatment
Wekerle CIHR Team - Anne Niec - Understanding Child MaltreatmentWekerle CIHR Team - Anne Niec - Understanding Child Maltreatment
Wekerle CIHR Team - Anne Niec - Understanding Child MaltreatmentChristine Wekerle
 
Age of Learning Research | Kindergarten Readiness Standards Among U.S. States
Age of Learning Research | Kindergarten Readiness Standards Among U.S. StatesAge of Learning Research | Kindergarten Readiness Standards Among U.S. States
Age of Learning Research | Kindergarten Readiness Standards Among U.S. StatesAge of Learning
 
Age of Learning Research | What It Means to Be Kindergarten Ready in the U.S.
Age of Learning Research | What It Means to Be Kindergarten Ready in the U.S.Age of Learning Research | What It Means to Be Kindergarten Ready in the U.S.
Age of Learning Research | What It Means to Be Kindergarten Ready in the U.S.Doug Dohring
 
Santrock 16e ch09_accessible
Santrock 16e ch09_accessibleSantrock 16e ch09_accessible
Santrock 16e ch09_accessiblelightdark13
 
Coley031408
Coley031408Coley031408
Coley031408Jainna
 
The voice of children in family law: The child centred continuum approach
The voice of children in family law: The child centred continuum approachThe voice of children in family law: The child centred continuum approach
The voice of children in family law: The child centred continuum approachResolution Institute
 
Santrock 16e ch17_accessible
Santrock 16e ch17_accessibleSantrock 16e ch17_accessible
Santrock 16e ch17_accessiblelightdark13
 
Santrock 16e ch07_accessible
Santrock 16e ch07_accessibleSantrock 16e ch07_accessible
Santrock 16e ch07_accessiblelightdark13
 
14. ENVIRONMENT & INTELLIGENCE
14. ENVIRONMENT & INTELLIGENCE14. ENVIRONMENT & INTELLIGENCE
14. ENVIRONMENT & INTELLIGENCEDr. Bhuvan Nagpal
 

What's hot (20)

Santrock 16e ch03_accessible
Santrock 16e ch03_accessibleSantrock 16e ch03_accessible
Santrock 16e ch03_accessible
 
Santrock 16e ch10_accessible
Santrock 16e ch10_accessibleSantrock 16e ch10_accessible
Santrock 16e ch10_accessible
 
Case Management and Outcomes for Neglected Children: A 5 year follow up study
Case Management and Outcomes for Neglected Children: A 5 year follow up studyCase Management and Outcomes for Neglected Children: A 5 year follow up study
Case Management and Outcomes for Neglected Children: A 5 year follow up study
 
A Perspective on Shared Care from the US
A Perspective on Shared Care from the USA Perspective on Shared Care from the US
A Perspective on Shared Care from the US
 
Santrock 16e ch11_accessible
Santrock 16e ch11_accessibleSantrock 16e ch11_accessible
Santrock 16e ch11_accessible
 
AR Well Child Reads/Reach Out and Read Hunger Conference
AR Well Child Reads/Reach Out and Read Hunger ConferenceAR Well Child Reads/Reach Out and Read Hunger Conference
AR Well Child Reads/Reach Out and Read Hunger Conference
 
Corboy_ASDsiblings2016
Corboy_ASDsiblings2016Corboy_ASDsiblings2016
Corboy_ASDsiblings2016
 
Santrock 16e ch02_accessible
Santrock 16e ch02_accessibleSantrock 16e ch02_accessible
Santrock 16e ch02_accessible
 
Au Psy492 W7 A2 Pp Golub R
Au Psy492 W7 A2 Pp Golub RAu Psy492 W7 A2 Pp Golub R
Au Psy492 W7 A2 Pp Golub R
 
Santrock 16e ch16_accessible
Santrock 16e ch16_accessibleSantrock 16e ch16_accessible
Santrock 16e ch16_accessible
 
concept of child development
concept of child developmentconcept of child development
concept of child development
 
Wekerle CIHR Team - Anne Niec - Understanding Child Maltreatment
Wekerle CIHR Team - Anne Niec - Understanding Child MaltreatmentWekerle CIHR Team - Anne Niec - Understanding Child Maltreatment
Wekerle CIHR Team - Anne Niec - Understanding Child Maltreatment
 
Age of Learning Research | Kindergarten Readiness Standards Among U.S. States
Age of Learning Research | Kindergarten Readiness Standards Among U.S. StatesAge of Learning Research | Kindergarten Readiness Standards Among U.S. States
Age of Learning Research | Kindergarten Readiness Standards Among U.S. States
 
Age of Learning Research | What It Means to Be Kindergarten Ready in the U.S.
Age of Learning Research | What It Means to Be Kindergarten Ready in the U.S.Age of Learning Research | What It Means to Be Kindergarten Ready in the U.S.
Age of Learning Research | What It Means to Be Kindergarten Ready in the U.S.
 
Santrock 16e ch09_accessible
Santrock 16e ch09_accessibleSantrock 16e ch09_accessible
Santrock 16e ch09_accessible
 
Coley031408
Coley031408Coley031408
Coley031408
 
The voice of children in family law: The child centred continuum approach
The voice of children in family law: The child centred continuum approachThe voice of children in family law: The child centred continuum approach
The voice of children in family law: The child centred continuum approach
 
Santrock 16e ch17_accessible
Santrock 16e ch17_accessibleSantrock 16e ch17_accessible
Santrock 16e ch17_accessible
 
Santrock 16e ch07_accessible
Santrock 16e ch07_accessibleSantrock 16e ch07_accessible
Santrock 16e ch07_accessible
 
14. ENVIRONMENT & INTELLIGENCE
14. ENVIRONMENT & INTELLIGENCE14. ENVIRONMENT & INTELLIGENCE
14. ENVIRONMENT & INTELLIGENCE
 

Similar to Effects of maternal mental retardation and poverty on intellectual, academic, and behavioral status of school-age children

The Role of Mental Health Professionals in Adoption
The Role of Mental Health Professionals in Adoption The Role of Mental Health Professionals in Adoption
The Role of Mental Health Professionals in Adoption Ashutosh Ratnam
 
Literature
LiteratureLiterature
LiteratureMentor
 
Ethel teichberg ref 970313
Ethel teichberg ref 970313Ethel teichberg ref 970313
Ethel teichberg ref 970313beitissienew
 
6 Interventions In Specific Learning Disabilities Through Families
6 Interventions In Specific Learning Disabilities Through Families6 Interventions In Specific Learning Disabilities Through Families
6 Interventions In Specific Learning Disabilities Through FamiliesSean Flores
 
Evaluating PICCOLO Scores Against the Crowell Is the PICCOLO Valid with Pare...
Evaluating PICCOLO Scores Against the Crowell  Is the PICCOLO Valid with Pare...Evaluating PICCOLO Scores Against the Crowell  Is the PICCOLO Valid with Pare...
Evaluating PICCOLO Scores Against the Crowell Is the PICCOLO Valid with Pare...Felicia Nicole Ghrist
 
Autism &amp; Poverty
Autism &amp; PovertyAutism &amp; Poverty
Autism &amp; Povertydrdanmarston
 
Another sample paperRelating Adults and ChildrenA S.docx
Another sample paperRelating Adults and ChildrenA S.docxAnother sample paperRelating Adults and ChildrenA S.docx
Another sample paperRelating Adults and ChildrenA S.docxrossskuddershamus
 
Lack of Parental Supervision and Psychosocial Development of Children of Scho...
Lack of Parental Supervision and Psychosocial Development of Children of Scho...Lack of Parental Supervision and Psychosocial Development of Children of Scho...
Lack of Parental Supervision and Psychosocial Development of Children of Scho...ijtsrd
 
International Journal of Humanities and Social Science Invention (IJHSSI)
International Journal of Humanities and Social Science Invention (IJHSSI)International Journal of Humanities and Social Science Invention (IJHSSI)
International Journal of Humanities and Social Science Invention (IJHSSI)inventionjournals
 
Journal of Attention Disorders2017, Vol. 21(4) 316 –322© T
Journal of Attention Disorders2017, Vol. 21(4) 316 –322© TJournal of Attention Disorders2017, Vol. 21(4) 316 –322© T
Journal of Attention Disorders2017, Vol. 21(4) 316 –322© TTatianaMajor22
 
Running head CHILDREN OF THE SUBSTANCE ABUSE WARS 6.docx
Running head CHILDREN OF THE SUBSTANCE ABUSE WARS 6.docxRunning head CHILDREN OF THE SUBSTANCE ABUSE WARS 6.docx
Running head CHILDREN OF THE SUBSTANCE ABUSE WARS 6.docxsusanschei
 
Running head CHILDREN OF THE SUBSTANCE ABUSE WARS 9.docx
Running head CHILDREN OF THE SUBSTANCE ABUSE WARS 9.docxRunning head CHILDREN OF THE SUBSTANCE ABUSE WARS 9.docx
Running head CHILDREN OF THE SUBSTANCE ABUSE WARS 9.docxsusanschei
 
Talking to your children and young people about Familial Alzheimer's/Frontote...
Talking to your children and young people about Familial Alzheimer's/Frontote...Talking to your children and young people about Familial Alzheimer's/Frontote...
Talking to your children and young people about Familial Alzheimer's/Frontote...RareDementiaSupport
 
Talking to your children and young people about Familial Alzheimer's/Frontote...
Talking to your children and young people about Familial Alzheimer's/Frontote...Talking to your children and young people about Familial Alzheimer's/Frontote...
Talking to your children and young people about Familial Alzheimer's/Frontote...Jessica Collins
 
Fathers’ Influence on Children’s Cognitive and Behavioural Fun.docx
Fathers’ Influence on Children’s Cognitive and Behavioural Fun.docxFathers’ Influence on Children’s Cognitive and Behavioural Fun.docx
Fathers’ Influence on Children’s Cognitive and Behavioural Fun.docxgreg1eden90113
 
Information-Seeking Behaviors of Parents of Children with ADHD: Experiences, ...
Information-Seeking Behaviors of Parents of Children with ADHD: Experiences, ...Information-Seeking Behaviors of Parents of Children with ADHD: Experiences, ...
Information-Seeking Behaviors of Parents of Children with ADHD: Experiences, ...Laura Levy
 
COVID-19 Impact on Child Development
COVID-19 Impact on Child DevelopmentCOVID-19 Impact on Child Development
COVID-19 Impact on Child DevelopmentNicoleNadler2
 
Critique the following research article.pdf
Critique the following research article.pdfCritique the following research article.pdf
Critique the following research article.pdfsdfghj21
 

Similar to Effects of maternal mental retardation and poverty on intellectual, academic, and behavioral status of school-age children (20)

Poverty slides
Poverty slidesPoverty slides
Poverty slides
 
The Role of Mental Health Professionals in Adoption
The Role of Mental Health Professionals in Adoption The Role of Mental Health Professionals in Adoption
The Role of Mental Health Professionals in Adoption
 
Literature
LiteratureLiterature
Literature
 
Ethel teichberg ref 970313
Ethel teichberg ref 970313Ethel teichberg ref 970313
Ethel teichberg ref 970313
 
6 Interventions In Specific Learning Disabilities Through Families
6 Interventions In Specific Learning Disabilities Through Families6 Interventions In Specific Learning Disabilities Through Families
6 Interventions In Specific Learning Disabilities Through Families
 
Evaluating PICCOLO Scores Against the Crowell Is the PICCOLO Valid with Pare...
Evaluating PICCOLO Scores Against the Crowell  Is the PICCOLO Valid with Pare...Evaluating PICCOLO Scores Against the Crowell  Is the PICCOLO Valid with Pare...
Evaluating PICCOLO Scores Against the Crowell Is the PICCOLO Valid with Pare...
 
Autism &amp; Poverty
Autism &amp; PovertyAutism &amp; Poverty
Autism &amp; Poverty
 
Another sample paperRelating Adults and ChildrenA S.docx
Another sample paperRelating Adults and ChildrenA S.docxAnother sample paperRelating Adults and ChildrenA S.docx
Another sample paperRelating Adults and ChildrenA S.docx
 
Lack of Parental Supervision and Psychosocial Development of Children of Scho...
Lack of Parental Supervision and Psychosocial Development of Children of Scho...Lack of Parental Supervision and Psychosocial Development of Children of Scho...
Lack of Parental Supervision and Psychosocial Development of Children of Scho...
 
International Journal of Humanities and Social Science Invention (IJHSSI)
International Journal of Humanities and Social Science Invention (IJHSSI)International Journal of Humanities and Social Science Invention (IJHSSI)
International Journal of Humanities and Social Science Invention (IJHSSI)
 
Ob m
Ob mOb m
Ob m
 
Journal of Attention Disorders2017, Vol. 21(4) 316 –322© T
Journal of Attention Disorders2017, Vol. 21(4) 316 –322© TJournal of Attention Disorders2017, Vol. 21(4) 316 –322© T
Journal of Attention Disorders2017, Vol. 21(4) 316 –322© T
 
Running head CHILDREN OF THE SUBSTANCE ABUSE WARS 6.docx
Running head CHILDREN OF THE SUBSTANCE ABUSE WARS 6.docxRunning head CHILDREN OF THE SUBSTANCE ABUSE WARS 6.docx
Running head CHILDREN OF THE SUBSTANCE ABUSE WARS 6.docx
 
Running head CHILDREN OF THE SUBSTANCE ABUSE WARS 9.docx
Running head CHILDREN OF THE SUBSTANCE ABUSE WARS 9.docxRunning head CHILDREN OF THE SUBSTANCE ABUSE WARS 9.docx
Running head CHILDREN OF THE SUBSTANCE ABUSE WARS 9.docx
 
Talking to your children and young people about Familial Alzheimer's/Frontote...
Talking to your children and young people about Familial Alzheimer's/Frontote...Talking to your children and young people about Familial Alzheimer's/Frontote...
Talking to your children and young people about Familial Alzheimer's/Frontote...
 
Talking to your children and young people about Familial Alzheimer's/Frontote...
Talking to your children and young people about Familial Alzheimer's/Frontote...Talking to your children and young people about Familial Alzheimer's/Frontote...
Talking to your children and young people about Familial Alzheimer's/Frontote...
 
Fathers’ Influence on Children’s Cognitive and Behavioural Fun.docx
Fathers’ Influence on Children’s Cognitive and Behavioural Fun.docxFathers’ Influence on Children’s Cognitive and Behavioural Fun.docx
Fathers’ Influence on Children’s Cognitive and Behavioural Fun.docx
 
Information-Seeking Behaviors of Parents of Children with ADHD: Experiences, ...
Information-Seeking Behaviors of Parents of Children with ADHD: Experiences, ...Information-Seeking Behaviors of Parents of Children with ADHD: Experiences, ...
Information-Seeking Behaviors of Parents of Children with ADHD: Experiences, ...
 
COVID-19 Impact on Child Development
COVID-19 Impact on Child DevelopmentCOVID-19 Impact on Child Development
COVID-19 Impact on Child Development
 
Critique the following research article.pdf
Critique the following research article.pdfCritique the following research article.pdf
Critique the following research article.pdf
 

More from eraser Juan José Calderón

Evaluación de t-MOOC universitario sobre competencias digitales docentes medi...
Evaluación de t-MOOC universitario sobre competencias digitales docentes medi...Evaluación de t-MOOC universitario sobre competencias digitales docentes medi...
Evaluación de t-MOOC universitario sobre competencias digitales docentes medi...eraser Juan José Calderón
 
Editorial of the JBBA Vol 4, Issue 1, May 2021. Naseem Naqvi,
Editorial of the JBBA Vol 4, Issue 1, May 2021. Naseem Naqvi, Editorial of the JBBA Vol 4, Issue 1, May 2021. Naseem Naqvi,
Editorial of the JBBA Vol 4, Issue 1, May 2021. Naseem Naqvi, eraser Juan José Calderón
 
REGULATION OF THE EUROPEAN PARLIAMENT AND OF THE COUNCIL LAYING DOWN HARMONIS...
REGULATION OF THE EUROPEAN PARLIAMENT AND OF THE COUNCIL LAYING DOWN HARMONIS...REGULATION OF THE EUROPEAN PARLIAMENT AND OF THE COUNCIL LAYING DOWN HARMONIS...
REGULATION OF THE EUROPEAN PARLIAMENT AND OF THE COUNCIL LAYING DOWN HARMONIS...eraser Juan José Calderón
 
Predicting Big Data Adoption in Companies With an Explanatory and Predictive ...
Predicting Big Data Adoption in Companies With an Explanatory and Predictive ...Predicting Big Data Adoption in Companies With an Explanatory and Predictive ...
Predicting Big Data Adoption in Companies With an Explanatory and Predictive ...eraser Juan José Calderón
 
Innovar con blockchain en las ciudades: Ideas para lograrlo, casos de uso y a...
Innovar con blockchain en las ciudades: Ideas para lograrlo, casos de uso y a...Innovar con blockchain en las ciudades: Ideas para lograrlo, casos de uso y a...
Innovar con blockchain en las ciudades: Ideas para lograrlo, casos de uso y a...eraser Juan José Calderón
 
Innovar con blockchain en las ciudades: Ideas para lograrlo, casos de uso y a...
Innovar con blockchain en las ciudades: Ideas para lograrlo, casos de uso y a...Innovar con blockchain en las ciudades: Ideas para lograrlo, casos de uso y a...
Innovar con blockchain en las ciudades: Ideas para lograrlo, casos de uso y a...eraser Juan José Calderón
 
Ética y Revolución Digital . revista Diecisiete nº 4. 2021
Ética y Revolución Digital . revista Diecisiete nº 4. 2021Ética y Revolución Digital . revista Diecisiete nº 4. 2021
Ética y Revolución Digital . revista Diecisiete nº 4. 2021eraser Juan José Calderón
 
#StopBigTechGoverningBigTech . More than 170 Civil Society Groups Worldwide O...
#StopBigTechGoverningBigTech . More than 170 Civil Society Groups Worldwide O...#StopBigTechGoverningBigTech . More than 170 Civil Society Groups Worldwide O...
#StopBigTechGoverningBigTech . More than 170 Civil Society Groups Worldwide O...eraser Juan José Calderón
 
PACTO POR LA CIENCIA Y LA INNOVACIÓN 8 de febrero de 2021
PACTO POR LA CIENCIA Y LA INNOVACIÓN 8 de febrero de 2021PACTO POR LA CIENCIA Y LA INNOVACIÓN 8 de febrero de 2021
PACTO POR LA CIENCIA Y LA INNOVACIÓN 8 de febrero de 2021eraser Juan José Calderón
 
Expert Panel of the European Blockchain Observatory and Forum
Expert Panel of the European Blockchain Observatory and ForumExpert Panel of the European Blockchain Observatory and Forum
Expert Panel of the European Blockchain Observatory and Forumeraser Juan José Calderón
 
Desigualdades educativas derivadas del COVID-19 desde una perspectiva feminis...
Desigualdades educativas derivadas del COVID-19 desde una perspectiva feminis...Desigualdades educativas derivadas del COVID-19 desde una perspectiva feminis...
Desigualdades educativas derivadas del COVID-19 desde una perspectiva feminis...eraser Juan José Calderón
 
"Experiencias booktuber: Más allá del libro y de la pantalla"
"Experiencias booktuber: Más allá del libro y de la pantalla""Experiencias booktuber: Más allá del libro y de la pantalla"
"Experiencias booktuber: Más allá del libro y de la pantalla"eraser Juan José Calderón
 
The impact of digital influencers on adolescent identity building.
The impact of digital influencers on adolescent identity building.The impact of digital influencers on adolescent identity building.
The impact of digital influencers on adolescent identity building.eraser Juan José Calderón
 
Open educational resources (OER) in the Spanish universities
Open educational resources (OER) in the Spanish universitiesOpen educational resources (OER) in the Spanish universities
Open educational resources (OER) in the Spanish universitieseraser Juan José Calderón
 
El modelo flipped classroom: un reto para una enseñanza centrada en el alumno
El modelo flipped classroom: un reto para una enseñanza centrada en el alumnoEl modelo flipped classroom: un reto para una enseñanza centrada en el alumno
El modelo flipped classroom: un reto para una enseñanza centrada en el alumnoeraser Juan José Calderón
 
Pensamiento propio e integración transdisciplinaria en la epistémica social. ...
Pensamiento propio e integración transdisciplinaria en la epistémica social. ...Pensamiento propio e integración transdisciplinaria en la epistémica social. ...
Pensamiento propio e integración transdisciplinaria en la epistémica social. ...eraser Juan José Calderón
 
Escuela de Robótica de Misiones. Un modelo de educación disruptiva.
Escuela de Robótica de Misiones. Un modelo de educación disruptiva.Escuela de Robótica de Misiones. Un modelo de educación disruptiva.
Escuela de Robótica de Misiones. Un modelo de educación disruptiva.eraser Juan José Calderón
 
La Universidad española Frente a la pandemia. Actuaciones de Crue Universidad...
La Universidad española Frente a la pandemia. Actuaciones de Crue Universidad...La Universidad española Frente a la pandemia. Actuaciones de Crue Universidad...
La Universidad española Frente a la pandemia. Actuaciones de Crue Universidad...eraser Juan José Calderón
 
Covid-19 and IoT: Some Perspectives on the Use of IoT Technologies in Prevent...
Covid-19 and IoT: Some Perspectives on the Use of IoT Technologies in Prevent...Covid-19 and IoT: Some Perspectives on the Use of IoT Technologies in Prevent...
Covid-19 and IoT: Some Perspectives on the Use of IoT Technologies in Prevent...eraser Juan José Calderón
 

More from eraser Juan José Calderón (20)

Evaluación de t-MOOC universitario sobre competencias digitales docentes medi...
Evaluación de t-MOOC universitario sobre competencias digitales docentes medi...Evaluación de t-MOOC universitario sobre competencias digitales docentes medi...
Evaluación de t-MOOC universitario sobre competencias digitales docentes medi...
 
Call for paper 71. Revista Comunicar
Call for paper 71. Revista ComunicarCall for paper 71. Revista Comunicar
Call for paper 71. Revista Comunicar
 
Editorial of the JBBA Vol 4, Issue 1, May 2021. Naseem Naqvi,
Editorial of the JBBA Vol 4, Issue 1, May 2021. Naseem Naqvi, Editorial of the JBBA Vol 4, Issue 1, May 2021. Naseem Naqvi,
Editorial of the JBBA Vol 4, Issue 1, May 2021. Naseem Naqvi,
 
REGULATION OF THE EUROPEAN PARLIAMENT AND OF THE COUNCIL LAYING DOWN HARMONIS...
REGULATION OF THE EUROPEAN PARLIAMENT AND OF THE COUNCIL LAYING DOWN HARMONIS...REGULATION OF THE EUROPEAN PARLIAMENT AND OF THE COUNCIL LAYING DOWN HARMONIS...
REGULATION OF THE EUROPEAN PARLIAMENT AND OF THE COUNCIL LAYING DOWN HARMONIS...
 
Predicting Big Data Adoption in Companies With an Explanatory and Predictive ...
Predicting Big Data Adoption in Companies With an Explanatory and Predictive ...Predicting Big Data Adoption in Companies With an Explanatory and Predictive ...
Predicting Big Data Adoption in Companies With an Explanatory and Predictive ...
 
Innovar con blockchain en las ciudades: Ideas para lograrlo, casos de uso y a...
Innovar con blockchain en las ciudades: Ideas para lograrlo, casos de uso y a...Innovar con blockchain en las ciudades: Ideas para lograrlo, casos de uso y a...
Innovar con blockchain en las ciudades: Ideas para lograrlo, casos de uso y a...
 
Innovar con blockchain en las ciudades: Ideas para lograrlo, casos de uso y a...
Innovar con blockchain en las ciudades: Ideas para lograrlo, casos de uso y a...Innovar con blockchain en las ciudades: Ideas para lograrlo, casos de uso y a...
Innovar con blockchain en las ciudades: Ideas para lograrlo, casos de uso y a...
 
Ética y Revolución Digital . revista Diecisiete nº 4. 2021
Ética y Revolución Digital . revista Diecisiete nº 4. 2021Ética y Revolución Digital . revista Diecisiete nº 4. 2021
Ética y Revolución Digital . revista Diecisiete nº 4. 2021
 
#StopBigTechGoverningBigTech . More than 170 Civil Society Groups Worldwide O...
#StopBigTechGoverningBigTech . More than 170 Civil Society Groups Worldwide O...#StopBigTechGoverningBigTech . More than 170 Civil Society Groups Worldwide O...
#StopBigTechGoverningBigTech . More than 170 Civil Society Groups Worldwide O...
 
PACTO POR LA CIENCIA Y LA INNOVACIÓN 8 de febrero de 2021
PACTO POR LA CIENCIA Y LA INNOVACIÓN 8 de febrero de 2021PACTO POR LA CIENCIA Y LA INNOVACIÓN 8 de febrero de 2021
PACTO POR LA CIENCIA Y LA INNOVACIÓN 8 de febrero de 2021
 
Expert Panel of the European Blockchain Observatory and Forum
Expert Panel of the European Blockchain Observatory and ForumExpert Panel of the European Blockchain Observatory and Forum
Expert Panel of the European Blockchain Observatory and Forum
 
Desigualdades educativas derivadas del COVID-19 desde una perspectiva feminis...
Desigualdades educativas derivadas del COVID-19 desde una perspectiva feminis...Desigualdades educativas derivadas del COVID-19 desde una perspectiva feminis...
Desigualdades educativas derivadas del COVID-19 desde una perspectiva feminis...
 
"Experiencias booktuber: Más allá del libro y de la pantalla"
"Experiencias booktuber: Más allá del libro y de la pantalla""Experiencias booktuber: Más allá del libro y de la pantalla"
"Experiencias booktuber: Más allá del libro y de la pantalla"
 
The impact of digital influencers on adolescent identity building.
The impact of digital influencers on adolescent identity building.The impact of digital influencers on adolescent identity building.
The impact of digital influencers on adolescent identity building.
 
Open educational resources (OER) in the Spanish universities
Open educational resources (OER) in the Spanish universitiesOpen educational resources (OER) in the Spanish universities
Open educational resources (OER) in the Spanish universities
 
El modelo flipped classroom: un reto para una enseñanza centrada en el alumno
El modelo flipped classroom: un reto para una enseñanza centrada en el alumnoEl modelo flipped classroom: un reto para una enseñanza centrada en el alumno
El modelo flipped classroom: un reto para una enseñanza centrada en el alumno
 
Pensamiento propio e integración transdisciplinaria en la epistémica social. ...
Pensamiento propio e integración transdisciplinaria en la epistémica social. ...Pensamiento propio e integración transdisciplinaria en la epistémica social. ...
Pensamiento propio e integración transdisciplinaria en la epistémica social. ...
 
Escuela de Robótica de Misiones. Un modelo de educación disruptiva.
Escuela de Robótica de Misiones. Un modelo de educación disruptiva.Escuela de Robótica de Misiones. Un modelo de educación disruptiva.
Escuela de Robótica de Misiones. Un modelo de educación disruptiva.
 
La Universidad española Frente a la pandemia. Actuaciones de Crue Universidad...
La Universidad española Frente a la pandemia. Actuaciones de Crue Universidad...La Universidad española Frente a la pandemia. Actuaciones de Crue Universidad...
La Universidad española Frente a la pandemia. Actuaciones de Crue Universidad...
 
Covid-19 and IoT: Some Perspectives on the Use of IoT Technologies in Prevent...
Covid-19 and IoT: Some Perspectives on the Use of IoT Technologies in Prevent...Covid-19 and IoT: Some Perspectives on the Use of IoT Technologies in Prevent...
Covid-19 and IoT: Some Perspectives on the Use of IoT Technologies in Prevent...
 

Recently uploaded

call girls in Kamla Market (DELHI) 🔝 >༒9953330565🔝 genuine Escort Service 🔝✔️✔️
call girls in Kamla Market (DELHI) 🔝 >༒9953330565🔝 genuine Escort Service 🔝✔️✔️call girls in Kamla Market (DELHI) 🔝 >༒9953330565🔝 genuine Escort Service 🔝✔️✔️
call girls in Kamla Market (DELHI) 🔝 >༒9953330565🔝 genuine Escort Service 🔝✔️✔️9953056974 Low Rate Call Girls In Saket, Delhi NCR
 
Organic Name Reactions for the students and aspirants of Chemistry12th.pptx
Organic Name Reactions  for the students and aspirants of Chemistry12th.pptxOrganic Name Reactions  for the students and aspirants of Chemistry12th.pptx
Organic Name Reactions for the students and aspirants of Chemistry12th.pptxVS Mahajan Coaching Centre
 
Procuring digital preservation CAN be quick and painless with our new dynamic...
Procuring digital preservation CAN be quick and painless with our new dynamic...Procuring digital preservation CAN be quick and painless with our new dynamic...
Procuring digital preservation CAN be quick and painless with our new dynamic...Jisc
 
What is Model Inheritance in Odoo 17 ERP
What is Model Inheritance in Odoo 17 ERPWhat is Model Inheritance in Odoo 17 ERP
What is Model Inheritance in Odoo 17 ERPCeline George
 
Employee wellbeing at the workplace.pptx
Employee wellbeing at the workplace.pptxEmployee wellbeing at the workplace.pptx
Employee wellbeing at the workplace.pptxNirmalaLoungPoorunde1
 
Like-prefer-love -hate+verb+ing & silent letters & citizenship text.pdf
Like-prefer-love -hate+verb+ing & silent letters & citizenship text.pdfLike-prefer-love -hate+verb+ing & silent letters & citizenship text.pdf
Like-prefer-love -hate+verb+ing & silent letters & citizenship text.pdfMr Bounab Samir
 
Pharmacognosy Flower 3. Compositae 2023.pdf
Pharmacognosy Flower 3. Compositae 2023.pdfPharmacognosy Flower 3. Compositae 2023.pdf
Pharmacognosy Flower 3. Compositae 2023.pdfMahmoud M. Sallam
 
ENGLISH 7_Q4_LESSON 2_ Employing a Variety of Strategies for Effective Interp...
ENGLISH 7_Q4_LESSON 2_ Employing a Variety of Strategies for Effective Interp...ENGLISH 7_Q4_LESSON 2_ Employing a Variety of Strategies for Effective Interp...
ENGLISH 7_Q4_LESSON 2_ Employing a Variety of Strategies for Effective Interp...JhezDiaz1
 
ECONOMIC CONTEXT - LONG FORM TV DRAMA - PPT
ECONOMIC CONTEXT - LONG FORM TV DRAMA - PPTECONOMIC CONTEXT - LONG FORM TV DRAMA - PPT
ECONOMIC CONTEXT - LONG FORM TV DRAMA - PPTiammrhaywood
 
Earth Day Presentation wow hello nice great
Earth Day Presentation wow hello nice greatEarth Day Presentation wow hello nice great
Earth Day Presentation wow hello nice greatYousafMalik24
 
Framing an Appropriate Research Question 6b9b26d93da94caf993c038d9efcdedb.pdf
Framing an Appropriate Research Question 6b9b26d93da94caf993c038d9efcdedb.pdfFraming an Appropriate Research Question 6b9b26d93da94caf993c038d9efcdedb.pdf
Framing an Appropriate Research Question 6b9b26d93da94caf993c038d9efcdedb.pdfUjwalaBharambe
 
Meghan Sutherland In Media Res Media Component
Meghan Sutherland In Media Res Media ComponentMeghan Sutherland In Media Res Media Component
Meghan Sutherland In Media Res Media ComponentInMediaRes1
 
CELL CYCLE Division Science 8 quarter IV.pptx
CELL CYCLE Division Science 8 quarter IV.pptxCELL CYCLE Division Science 8 quarter IV.pptx
CELL CYCLE Division Science 8 quarter IV.pptxJiesonDelaCerna
 
Solving Puzzles Benefits Everyone (English).pptx
Solving Puzzles Benefits Everyone (English).pptxSolving Puzzles Benefits Everyone (English).pptx
Solving Puzzles Benefits Everyone (English).pptxOH TEIK BIN
 
Presiding Officer Training module 2024 lok sabha elections
Presiding Officer Training module 2024 lok sabha electionsPresiding Officer Training module 2024 lok sabha elections
Presiding Officer Training module 2024 lok sabha electionsanshu789521
 
Historical philosophical, theoretical, and legal foundations of special and i...
Historical philosophical, theoretical, and legal foundations of special and i...Historical philosophical, theoretical, and legal foundations of special and i...
Historical philosophical, theoretical, and legal foundations of special and i...jaredbarbolino94
 

Recently uploaded (20)

call girls in Kamla Market (DELHI) 🔝 >༒9953330565🔝 genuine Escort Service 🔝✔️✔️
call girls in Kamla Market (DELHI) 🔝 >༒9953330565🔝 genuine Escort Service 🔝✔️✔️call girls in Kamla Market (DELHI) 🔝 >༒9953330565🔝 genuine Escort Service 🔝✔️✔️
call girls in Kamla Market (DELHI) 🔝 >༒9953330565🔝 genuine Escort Service 🔝✔️✔️
 
TataKelola dan KamSiber Kecerdasan Buatan v022.pdf
TataKelola dan KamSiber Kecerdasan Buatan v022.pdfTataKelola dan KamSiber Kecerdasan Buatan v022.pdf
TataKelola dan KamSiber Kecerdasan Buatan v022.pdf
 
OS-operating systems- ch04 (Threads) ...
OS-operating systems- ch04 (Threads) ...OS-operating systems- ch04 (Threads) ...
OS-operating systems- ch04 (Threads) ...
 
Organic Name Reactions for the students and aspirants of Chemistry12th.pptx
Organic Name Reactions  for the students and aspirants of Chemistry12th.pptxOrganic Name Reactions  for the students and aspirants of Chemistry12th.pptx
Organic Name Reactions for the students and aspirants of Chemistry12th.pptx
 
Procuring digital preservation CAN be quick and painless with our new dynamic...
Procuring digital preservation CAN be quick and painless with our new dynamic...Procuring digital preservation CAN be quick and painless with our new dynamic...
Procuring digital preservation CAN be quick and painless with our new dynamic...
 
What is Model Inheritance in Odoo 17 ERP
What is Model Inheritance in Odoo 17 ERPWhat is Model Inheritance in Odoo 17 ERP
What is Model Inheritance in Odoo 17 ERP
 
Employee wellbeing at the workplace.pptx
Employee wellbeing at the workplace.pptxEmployee wellbeing at the workplace.pptx
Employee wellbeing at the workplace.pptx
 
9953330565 Low Rate Call Girls In Rohini Delhi NCR
9953330565 Low Rate Call Girls In Rohini  Delhi NCR9953330565 Low Rate Call Girls In Rohini  Delhi NCR
9953330565 Low Rate Call Girls In Rohini Delhi NCR
 
Like-prefer-love -hate+verb+ing & silent letters & citizenship text.pdf
Like-prefer-love -hate+verb+ing & silent letters & citizenship text.pdfLike-prefer-love -hate+verb+ing & silent letters & citizenship text.pdf
Like-prefer-love -hate+verb+ing & silent letters & citizenship text.pdf
 
Pharmacognosy Flower 3. Compositae 2023.pdf
Pharmacognosy Flower 3. Compositae 2023.pdfPharmacognosy Flower 3. Compositae 2023.pdf
Pharmacognosy Flower 3. Compositae 2023.pdf
 
ENGLISH 7_Q4_LESSON 2_ Employing a Variety of Strategies for Effective Interp...
ENGLISH 7_Q4_LESSON 2_ Employing a Variety of Strategies for Effective Interp...ENGLISH 7_Q4_LESSON 2_ Employing a Variety of Strategies for Effective Interp...
ENGLISH 7_Q4_LESSON 2_ Employing a Variety of Strategies for Effective Interp...
 
ECONOMIC CONTEXT - LONG FORM TV DRAMA - PPT
ECONOMIC CONTEXT - LONG FORM TV DRAMA - PPTECONOMIC CONTEXT - LONG FORM TV DRAMA - PPT
ECONOMIC CONTEXT - LONG FORM TV DRAMA - PPT
 
Earth Day Presentation wow hello nice great
Earth Day Presentation wow hello nice greatEarth Day Presentation wow hello nice great
Earth Day Presentation wow hello nice great
 
Framing an Appropriate Research Question 6b9b26d93da94caf993c038d9efcdedb.pdf
Framing an Appropriate Research Question 6b9b26d93da94caf993c038d9efcdedb.pdfFraming an Appropriate Research Question 6b9b26d93da94caf993c038d9efcdedb.pdf
Framing an Appropriate Research Question 6b9b26d93da94caf993c038d9efcdedb.pdf
 
Meghan Sutherland In Media Res Media Component
Meghan Sutherland In Media Res Media ComponentMeghan Sutherland In Media Res Media Component
Meghan Sutherland In Media Res Media Component
 
CELL CYCLE Division Science 8 quarter IV.pptx
CELL CYCLE Division Science 8 quarter IV.pptxCELL CYCLE Division Science 8 quarter IV.pptx
CELL CYCLE Division Science 8 quarter IV.pptx
 
Model Call Girl in Tilak Nagar Delhi reach out to us at 🔝9953056974🔝
Model Call Girl in Tilak Nagar Delhi reach out to us at 🔝9953056974🔝Model Call Girl in Tilak Nagar Delhi reach out to us at 🔝9953056974🔝
Model Call Girl in Tilak Nagar Delhi reach out to us at 🔝9953056974🔝
 
Solving Puzzles Benefits Everyone (English).pptx
Solving Puzzles Benefits Everyone (English).pptxSolving Puzzles Benefits Everyone (English).pptx
Solving Puzzles Benefits Everyone (English).pptx
 
Presiding Officer Training module 2024 lok sabha elections
Presiding Officer Training module 2024 lok sabha electionsPresiding Officer Training module 2024 lok sabha elections
Presiding Officer Training module 2024 lok sabha elections
 
Historical philosophical, theoretical, and legal foundations of special and i...
Historical philosophical, theoretical, and legal foundations of special and i...Historical philosophical, theoretical, and legal foundations of special and i...
Historical philosophical, theoretical, and legal foundations of special and i...
 

Effects of maternal mental retardation and poverty on intellectual, academic, and behavioral status of school-age children

  • 1. See discussions, stats, and author profiles for this publication at: https://www.researchgate.net/publication/14192210 Effects of maternal mental retardation and poverty on intellectual, academic, and behavioral status of school-age children Article  in  American journal of mental retardation: AJMR · February 1997 Source: PubMed CITATIONS 88 READS 350 2 authors, including: Some of the authors of this publication are also working on these related projects: Group Function-Based Cognitive Behavior Therapy for Children with Autism and Obsessive Compulsive Behavior View project Province-Wide Survey of the Effects of Quality Assurance Measures on Services for Adults with Intellectual/Developmental Disabilities and Challenging Behaviours View project Maurice A Feldman Brock University 104 PUBLICATIONS   2,462 CITATIONS    SEE PROFILE All content following this page was uploaded by Maurice A Feldman on 29 January 2017. The user has requested enhancement of the downloaded file.
  • 2. Maternal Mental Retardation and Poverty: Intellectual, Academic, and Behavioral Status of School-Age Children Maurice A. Feldman Queen's University and Ogwanada Hospital Kingston, Ontario, Canada and Nicole Walton-Allen Chedoke-McMaster Hospitals Hamilton, Ontario, Canada Address: Maurice Feldman, Ph.D., Dept. of Psychology, Queen's University, Kingston, Ontario Canada K7L 3N6. Key Terms: CHILD DEVELOPMENT, MATERNAL MENTAL RETARDATION, POVERTY Running Head: MATERNAL MENTAL RETARDATION
  • 3. Maternal Mental Retardation 2 Maternal Mental Retardation and Poverty: Intellectual, Academic, and Behavioral Status of School-Age Children ABSTRACT This study examined the impact of low maternal IQ on children living in poverty by comparing WISC-R, WRAT-R, and Ontario Child Behavior Checklist scores of 27 school-age children of mothers with mild mental retardation to 25 similarly impoverished children whose mothers did not have mental retardation. The children of mothers with mental retardation had lower IQ and academic achievement, and more behavior problems than the low income children of parents without mental retardation. Not one child with a mother with mental retardation was free of problems and boys were affected more severely than girls. The quality of the home environment and maternal social supports were also lower in the group with maternal mental retardation; both of these measures were negatively correlated with child behavior disorders in this group. This study shows that being raised by mothers with mental retardation can have detrimental effects on child development and behavior that cannot be attributed to poverty alone.
  • 4. Maternal Mental Retardation 3 Maternal Mental Retardation and Poverty: Intellectual, Academic, and Behavioral Status of School-Age Children The relationship between maternal and child IQ is well-established (Reed & Reed, 1965) and low parental IQ is related to child intellectual and language delays (Feldman, Case, Towns, & Betel, 1985; Feldman, Sparks, & Case, 1993; Gillberg & Geijer-Karlsson, 1983; Ramey & Ramey, 1992). Poverty is also implicated as a risk factor in child development and academic achievement (Campbell & Ramey, 1994; Parker, Greer, & Zuckerman, 1988; Zigler, 1967). Most parents with mild mental retardation also have low income (Fotheringham, 1971; Garber, 1988). Given the paucity of studies that directly compare development of children raised by low income parents with versus without mental retardation, it is not clear to what extent child problems are a function of variables related to low maternal IQ or poverty. In addition to the possible genetic transmission of low intelligence, parents with low IQ often exhibit deficits in basic child-care, nourishment, and positive interactions which may jeopardize their children's well being and development (Feldman, Case, & Sparks, 1992; Feldman et al., 1993; Keltner, 1992). Child maltreatment studies have found a disproportionate number of parents with mental retardation to be incompetent or abusive (Schilling, Schinke, Blythe, & Barth, 1982; Seagull & Scheurer, 1986; Taylor et al., 1991). This over-representation in child custody cases may to some extent reflect society's bias against these parents, but their interactions with their children nonetheless resemble that of known neglectful parents who did not have mental retardation (Crittenden & Bonvillian, 1984).
  • 5. Maternal Mental Retardation 4 There is likely to be an increasing number of parents with low IQ. Courts are banning involuntary sterilization of persons with disabilities and are upholding parenting as a basic right of all adult citizens (Hayman, 1990; Vogel, 1987). With virtually all persons with mild mental retardation now raised and socialized in the community, more of them may exercise their parenting rights. Despite concerns about the parenting abilities of persons with mental retardation and the demands that these families are placing on the social service system (Tymchuk & Feldman, 1991), relatively little is known about the development of their children. To complicate matters, studies of these parents have used varying definitions of mental retardation; some researchers (e.g., Reed & Reed, 1965) included only those with IQs less than 70, some used a cut-off of 75 (Garber, 1988; Keltner, 1994), while others (Feldman et al., 1985; Tymchuk, Andron, & Tymchuk, 1990), used a social system perspective (Mercer, 1973) and accepted parents with IQs between 70-80. Nevertheless, most studies have reported that children of parents labelled as having mental retardation have lower mean IQ scores and more of them have scores in the range of intellectual retardation than would be expected from a random, but not necessarily an economically disadvantaged, sample of the general population (Bass, l963; Feldman et al., 1985; Garber, 1988; Gillberg & Geijer-Karlsson, 1983; Mickelson, l947; Reed & Reed, l965; Scally, l973). Other studies reported significantly less vocalizations and speech in young children of mothers with mental retardation as compared to peers of low and middle SES whose parents did not have mental retardation (Feldman, Case, Rincover, Towns, & Betel, 1989; Feldman et al., 1986, 1993).
  • 6. Maternal Mental Retardation 5 Most studies of children raised by parents with mental retardation have focused on intellectual and language deficits (e.g., Feldman et al., 1985; Reed & Reed, 1965). In one study which assessed behavioral adjustment, O'Neill (1985), using interviews and projective tests, found behavior problems (e.g., oppositional behaviors, pseudo-retardation) in approximately 50% of 19 "normal" or "bright" children of parents with mental retardation. Likewise, a retrospective study of 41 offspring of 15 mothers with mental retardation in Sweden found that 58% of the children had required psychiatric services (Gillberg & Geijer-Karlsson, 1983). Few studies of parents with mental retardation have examined factors that may predict child development such as the quality of the home environment and familial variables. Feldman et al. (1985) found a significant positive correlation between total Caldwell HOME Inventory (Caldwell & Bradley, 1984) scores and Mental Development Index scores on the Bayley Scales of Infant Development (Bayley, 1969) in 12, 2 year old children of parents with mental retardation. Keltner (1994) reported that mothers with IQs less than 75 had significantly lower HOME scores than a low income comparison group of parents with IQs > 85. According to the results of Feldman et al. (1985), Keltner's findings suggested a greater risk of developmental delay for the infants of the parents with low IQs, but she did not provide developmental test data. Maternal social isolation/support is another predictor variable that has not been adequately studied in parents with intellectual disabilities. Social support has been shown to be related to child outcomes in families from low SES backgrounds (Bee, Hammond, Eyres, Barnard, & Snyder, 1986) and in families of parents without mental retardation raising children with disabilities (Dunst, Trivette, & Cross, 1986).
  • 7. Maternal Mental Retardation 6 We are aware of no study that has simultaneously evaluated the intellectual, academic, and behavioral status of school-age children raised by parents with mental retardation. Few studies of these children examined predictor variables and incorporated a low income comparison group of parents who do not have mental retardation to control for the impact of poverty per se on child development. To fill significant gaps in knowledge, the present study investigated the effects of low maternal intelligence and poverty on several crucial areas of child development by comparing the performance of children raised in poverty by mothers with and without mild mental retardation on a battery of standardized measures of intelligence, academic performance, and behavioral adjustment. We also explored the relationship of child outcome to the quality of the home environment and maternal social isolation/support. METHOD Subjects Two groups of families with children between the ages of 6 and 12 years participated. Although there were no restrictions regarding ethnicity of the families, all of the 34 families referred to this study were Caucasian. Referrals came from 10 community agencies providing advocacy and support services to adults with mental retardation in Southern and Eastern Ontario (where Caucasians are the substantial majority). We avoided referrals from child welfare agencies so as to not over-represent the sample with known maltreating parents with mental retardation (however, we did not exclude parents whom we subsequently found to be involved with a child protection agency). Agency workers initially contacted their clients who met the eligiblity requirements of this study (see below) and referred interested parties to us. Although
  • 8. Maternal Mental Retardation 7 we do not know the exact number of parents who said they were not interested in participating, the workers told us that most of the eligible parents agreed to allow us to contact them; no parent we contacted subsequently refused to participate or dropped out. To address the possibility that the parents with mild mental retardation may have had difficulty giving informed consent, their workers accompanied us on the first visit, witnessed our explanation to the parents, and had the opportunity to express any reservations about the parents' ability to give informed consent (this was never an issue with any of the participants). Criteria for inclusion in the mental retardation group required a current maternal full- scale WAIS-R IQ < 70 (the accepted cut-off at the time of the study - Grossman, 1983) and previous independent diagnosis of mental retardation with no known biological conditions associated with cognitive deficits (e.g., Down syndrome, brain damage). Total family income had to be below the Statistics Canada urban poverty level (Ross & Shillington, 1989). Seven families were excluded because maternal IQ was greater than 70 (despite a diagnosis of mental retardation), leaving a total of 27 in the group with maternal mental retardation. The second group was recruited by placing flyers in community resource and drop-in centers located in low income neighborhoods in the same Ontario communities in which the target parents with mental retardation resided. Resource and drop-in centers are used quite frequently in these communities as they offer a variety of free recreational and educational programs, advice, and support for children, families, and adults. The first 25 mothers who responded to the flyers and did not have mental retardation or a history of special education placement participated and none dropped out.
  • 9. Maternal Mental Retardation 8 Table 1 provides a comparison of group demographics. For the most part, mothers in both groups were between 30-35 years old, more than 50% were married (or in a conjugal relationship), total family incomes were below the poverty level, a majority were receiving welfare, and few were employed. Eighteen comparison mothers agreed to take an IQ test and all had WAIS-R IQ > 80. The seven low income mothers who refused to take an IQ test were included in the study as comparison subjects because their backgrounds suggested at least average intellectual development (highschool and community college diplomas; previously or currently employed in the secretarial or accounting fields). There were no significant differences between the seven who refused to take an IQ test and the remaining 18 comparison families on any of the measures reported in Tables 1 or 2. None of the children in either group had known disabilities which could affect their development. Both sets of families were eligible for services for economically disadvantaged families such as regular home visits by a public health nurse and access to community resource centers. Mothers with intellectual disabilities were also eligible for advocacy services. When the children were younger, early intervention programs for children living in poverty (or for children of parents with intellectual disabilities) were not as readily available as they are today. In fact, only one mother (who was in the group with maternal mental retardation) received specialized early intervention services for the target child in this study (another mother with low IQ received similar services for a subsequent child not included in this study).
  • 10. Maternal Mental Retardation 9 -------------------------- INSERT TABLE 1 ABOUT HERE -------------------------- Dependent measures Intelligence. The Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children - Revised (WISC-R: Wechsler, l974) was used. The WISC-III was not as yet available when this study was conducted. Academic achievement. Reading, spelling, and arithmetic achievement were assessed using the Wide Range Achievement Test-Revised (WRAT-R: Jastak & Jastak, l984). The educational placements of the children were also noted. Behavior disorders. Behavioral adjustment was measured using the Ontario version of the Child Behavior Checklist (CBCL: Statistics Canada, 1987) which was validated and normed on 3294 children as part of a well-documented Ontario Child Health Study (Offord et al., 1987). It is an augmented version of the original CBCL (Achenbach & Edelbrook, 1981) and is divided into four behavior disorders: conduct, hyperactivity, emotional, and somatization (the latter scale was not used as there were no subjects over the required age of 12 years). The mothers were asked to judge her child's behavior over the last 6 months. To determine whether the mothers with mental retardation were consistent in reporting on their children's behavior problems, we also asked them to complete the Parent Attitude Test (PAT: Cowen, Huser, Beach, & Rappaport, 1970). The PAT asks similar questions to the CBCL about the child's behavior. Significant Spearman Rank correlation coefficients were found between the PAT total score and the three
  • 11. Maternal Mental Retardation 10 CBCL subscales (conduct: r = .59, p < .001; hyperactivity: r = .40, p < .05; emotional: r = .33, p = .05). Quality of the home environment. The elementary school-age version of the HOME Inventory (Caldwell & Bradley, 1984) was used. Maternal social isolation/support. We devised a measure of the mother's social isolation/support by adding the raw scores on the Social Isolation and Marital Satisfaction subscales of the Parenting Stress Index (PSI: Abidin, 1990). Items in these scales measure the degree of the mother's social and spousal support (e.g., "I have a lot of people to whom I can talk, get help or advice." "My spouse has not given me as much help and support as I expected."). The mother's perception of support on the PSI subscales was partially corroborated by the listing of all services received by the family on the demographics form. As per the directions of the PSI, mothers who were not married were asked to respond based on their closest friend. Design and procedure A nonequivalent between-group design was used to compare the children of low income mothers with mental retardation to similar aged children of low income mothers without mental retardation. The tests were given in the family home by one of two experienced testers, who were not naive to the study purpose and design, or group classification of the family. The child received the WISC-R and the WRAT-R in a quiet room free from distractions with only the tester present (the same conditions applied when the WAIS-R was administered to the mothers). As many of the mothers with mental retardation had poor reading skills, the questionnaires (e.g., CBCL, Social Isolation/Support) were administered orally to all of these mothers; none of the
  • 12. Maternal Mental Retardation 11 mothers had any problems understanding and responding to the verbal questions and their responses were consistent with other similar measures (e.g., PAT, demographic questionnaire). RESULTS The scores of all the primary dependent measures in the group with maternal mental retardation failed the test of normalacy even when subjected to several logarithmic transformations (i.e., base 10, 1/2 log, and arcsine). Thus, we were obliged to use nonparametric statistical tests such as the two-tailed Mann-Whitney U (z scores were used for n > 20, see Daniel, 1978), Wilcoxon Signed-Rank, and Chi-Square tests for between group comparisons and the Spearman Sign-Rank Coefficient for correlational analyses. Table 2 presents the total group, boy's, and girl's means and standard deviations of the nine dependent measures for both groups. -------------------------- INSERT TABLE 2 ABOUT HERE -------------------------- Demographics As can be seen in Table 1, in addition to differences due to subject selection criteria (the first four variables), the two groups differed significantly on the percentage of families: (a) involved with child protection agencies and (b) where the mother reported the father as having mental retardation or special education experience. Home Environment Table 2 shows the Caldwell HOME Inventory mean total scores. The group with maternal mental retardation was significantly lower than that of the comparison group (z = 6.94; p <
  • 13. Maternal Mental Retardation 12 .001). Note, however, that the mean total scores of both groups were within one standard deviation of the school-age HOME Inventory normative group reported in Bradley, Rock, Caldwell, and Brisby (1989). As seen in Table 2, in the group with maternal mental retardation (but not in the comparison group), the boys' HOME total scores were significantly lower than those of the girls (z = 2.74, p < .01); this finding should be interpreted cautiously due to the disproportionate number of boys to girls in this group. Social Isolation The mean maternal social isolation score (see Table 2) of the mothers with mental retardation was significantly higher than the comparison mothers without mental retardation (z = 10.4, p < .001). In the group with maternal mental retardation (but not in the comparison group), there was significantly more social isolation amongst the mothers of the boys than the mothers of the girls (z = 10.63, p < .001); again, these gender differences should be interpreted conservatively. Child IQ Table 2 presents the group WISC-R IQ means and standard deviations and Figure 1 compares the IQ distributions of the children of mothers with and without mental retardation. A Mann-Whitney U test revealed that the children's IQ scores in the group with maternal mental retardation were significantly lower than in the low income comparison group (z = 4.27, p < .001). The group difference upheld for both boys (z = 3.33, p < .001) and girls (z = 2.78, p < .01). Note that the children in both groups had higher mean IQ scores than their mothers (see Table 1), perhaps reflecting regression to the mean. A Wilcoxon Signed-Rank test revealed that these
  • 14. Maternal Mental Retardation 13 mother-child IQ differences were significant in both the group with maternal mental retardation (z = -3.81, p < .001) and the low income group (z = -1.99, p < .05). --------------------------------- INSERT FIGURES 1 AND 2 ABOUT HERE --------------------------------- Academic Achievement Table 2 presents the group WRAT-R means and standard deviations and Figure 2 illustrates the distributions of scores of the children of mothers with and without mental retardation. As expected, the academic achievement of the comparison group children was lower than the norm, but to a lesser degree than the children whose mothers had mental retardation. Two-tailed Mann-Whitney U tests revealed that the children of mothers with mental retardation scored significantly lower than the children of mothers without mental retardation on reading (z = 3.58, p < .001), spelling (z = 3.04, p < .003), and math (z = 3.96, p < .001). Between-group differences were significant for boys (reading - z = 2.79. p < .006; spelling - z = 3.06, p < .003; math - z = 2.95, p < .004); the girls showed marginally significant differences in reading (z = 1.78, p < .08) and math (z = 1.87, p < .07) and no statistically significant difference in spelling (z = .48, p > .6) (because the relatively small number of girls increases the risk of Type II error, their marginally significant results are reported for the WRAT-R above, and the CBCL, below). Using the local school board's diagnostic criteria for learning disabilities (i.e., having a normal IQ with a 15 point split between performance and verbal WISC-R scores, and at least one year behind in either reading, spelling, or math achievement test scores), we found that more
  • 15. Maternal Mental Retardation 14 (36.4%) of the children of mothers with mental retardation than the children of parents without mental retardation (4.5%) met the criteria (X2 = 5.8, p < .02). Also, 59.3% of the children of mothers with mental retardation were receiving various special education services for children identified as having "mental retardation," "learning disabilities," or "behavioral maladjustment" (e.g., full- or part-day self-contained classes, specialized curriculum and instruction, availability of an Education Aide in the regular classroom, tutoring) compared to 12% of the children of parents without mental retardation; this difference was also statistically significant (X2 = 11.5, p < .001). Behavior Disorders Figure 3 shows that the group with maternal mental retardation had a higher percentage of children scoring above the CBCL clinical thresholds for behavioral disorders than the low income comparison and the Ontario normative groups. Table 2 shows that the children of mothers with mental retardation had significantly higher scores than the low income comparison children in conduct disorders (z = 2.80, p < .005), hyperactivity (z = 3.30, p < .001), and emotional disorders (z = 2.20, p < .03). Between-group CBCL differences for boys were significant on all three scales (conduct and emotional - both zs = 2.47, ps < .02; hyperactivity - z = 2.70, p < .007); the girls showed marginally significant differences in hyperactivity (z = 1.76, p < .08), but differences on the other two scales did not approach significance for the girls (conduct - z = 0.48; emotional - z = 0.66, ps > .5). We tested the hypothesis (O'Neill, 1985) that more competent children of slow parents would have more social maladjustment by examining the percentage of children with clinically
  • 16. Maternal Mental Retardation 15 significant behavior problems (as determined by CBCL cut-off scores) with each group subdivided into child IQ below 85 or equal/above 85. In the group with maternal mental retardation, a greater percentage of children with IQs> 85 (n = 11) had at least one behavior problem (63.6% vs. 43.8%). Although this numerical difference was not statistically significant, an examination of children with multiple behavior problems revealed that while no children with low IQ scored above the clinical threshold on all three scales of the CBCL, 27.3% of the children with IQ > 85 did; this difference was significant (z = 2.22, p< .02), but did not hold for the low income comparison children as none of these children had all three behavior problems. Thus, the children who met criteria for multiple behavior problems had IQs > 85 and mothers with mental retardation (IQs < 70). -------------------------------------- INSERT FIGURE 3 AND TABLE 3 ABOUT HERE -------------------------------------- Correlations As the above results indicated that children of mothers with mental retardation apparently were not all affected in the same way -- despite being raised by mothers with similar IQs -- we ran a series of simple correlations using the Spearman Sign-Rank coefficient, appropriate for non-normal distributions. We correlated the child outcome measures with maternal WAIS-R, Social Isolation/Support, and Caldwell HOME Inventory scores. These variables have been shown in the literature to be related to child development in low income families (Parker et al.,
  • 17. Maternal Mental Retardation 16 1988), and they were significantly different between the groups in this study. The coefficients are presented in Table 3. In the group of children of mothers with mental retardation, maternal IQ was not significantly correlated with any child measure. Maternal social isolation was significantly positively correlated, and HOME total scores were significantly negatively correlated with child conduct and hyperactivity disorders; HOME total score was also significantly correlated with WRAT-R math scores. With respect to the low income mothers without mental retardation, maternal IQ was significantly correlated with WRAT-R math scores. Maternal social isolation was significantly related to child hyperactivity and emotional disorders; the HOME total score was significantly correlated with WISC-R and WRAT-R reading and math scores. Thus, both groups share in common significant correlations between maternal social isolation and child behavior problems, as well as HOME scores and math achievement. DISCUSSION This study reveals the risk status of school-age children of mothers with mental retardation across intellectual, academic, behavioral, and family environment domains. There was no child in the group with maternal mental retardation who was completely free of problems. Close to 60% had IQs below 85 and even those children who had normal intelligence met the criteria for either a behavior disorder or a learning disability. These problems cannot be attributed solely to being raised in poverty because the children of mothers with mental retardation had significantly more deficits than similar aged children of parents without mental retardation from
  • 18. Maternal Mental Retardation 17 comparably impoverished families in the same communities. In addition to the child outcomes, the mothers with mental retardation were providing less stimulating home environments and were more socially isolated than the comparison mothers without mental retardation. Maternal mental retardation did not affect boys and girls in the same manner. While both the boys and girls of mothers with mental retardation had significantly lower IQs than their low income counterparts raised by mothers without mental retardation, only the boys had significantly lower academic achievement and significantly higher behavior problem scores. These gender differences may be partly related to the boys' significantly lower quality of the home environment and the significantly greater social isolation of their mothers; no other variables (listed in Table 1) were significantly different between the boys and girls. Further research is needed with a larger sample to explore possible gender differences in this population. Given the finding that the boys in the group with maternal mental retardation were generally more negatively affected than the girls, it is conceivable that the between-group differences on the dependent measures may have been influenced by the group disparities in the proportion of boys to girls. To ascertain this potential confound, we reformed the two groups so that they had the same number of boys (n = 12) and girls (n = 9). This was accomplished by randomly selecting 12 boys from the original pool of 18 in the group with maternal mental retardation and then adding them to the original 9 girls in this group. In the low income comparison group, we randomly chose 9 girls from the pool of 13 and added them to the original 12 boys in this group. Between-group Mann-Whitney U tests on all the dependent measures were still highly significant (zs <.001). Nevertheless, between-gender differences in this study
  • 19. Maternal Mental Retardation 18 should be viewed conservatively because of the relatively small number of girls in the group with maternal mental retardation. The results of this study suggest that there may be two distinct types of school-age Caucasian children raised in poverty who can be differentiated by their mothers' intelligence. While these findings await replication with more subjects and naive testers, the differences in the children found here suggest that research examining the impact of poverty on child development should more closely examine parental IQ as a contributing factor in the variable and often adverse child outcomes seen in economically disadvantaged families (Garber, 1988; Garner, Carson Jones, & Miner, 1994; Ramey & Ramey, 1992). The children's intellectual problems were also reflected in their poor academic achievement, especially for the boys. Besides having significantly lower WRAT-R scores than the comparison children, the children of mothers with mental retardation were eight times more likely to meet the local school board's criterion for a diagnosis of learning disabilities, and five times more likely to receive special education services. The special education placements provided some independent corroboration of our test scores. The boys of mothers with mental retardation were also at considerable risk for behavior problems as measured by the Ontario CBCL. More data are needed with a larger sample to determine if child behavior problems may be related to inadequate knowledge and skills in basic child management strategies such as positive reinforcement, supervision, limit-setting, and consistent discipline seen in parents with mental retardation (Fantuzzo, Wray, Hall, Goins, & Azar, 1986; Tymchuk et al., 1990).
  • 20. Maternal Mental Retardation 19 In the group with maternal mental retardation, the children with average intelligence were more likely to have multiple behavior problems than the children whose IQs were below 85. A substantiated explanation of this result is presently lacking. One can speculate that conflicts may arise when the parents fail to set reasonable limits and comprehend their (average IQ) children's more abstract communications. These children may quickly learn to take advantage of, and rebel against less competent parents. Early anti-social acts directed at their parents (e.g., disobedience, stealing) may subsequently generalize to other authority figures (O'Neill, 1985). Further research with more subjects is needed to determine why and how the combination of having normal intelligence and parents with mental retardation places children at-risk for behavioral maladjustment. Not all the children of mothers with mental retardation were adversely affected in the same way. On the WISC-R, 37% of the children had IQs <70 (i.e., mental retardation range), while 40.7% had IQs > 85. Moreover, 26% of the children of mothers with mental retardation were at or close to expected grade level in reading, spelling, and arithmetic, and about 36% did not exhibit clinically significant behavior problems. Correlational analyses of key variables likely related to differential outcomes revealed that for the group with maternal mental retardation, maternal social isolation/support and the quality of the home environment were related to child behavioral, but not intellectual outcomes. It may be the case that factors other than the ones we examined in this study may also be implicated in the development of school-age children of parents with mental retardation. For example, maternal depression has been shown to be related to negative child outcomes
  • 21. Maternal Mental Retardation 20 (Hammen, Burge, & Stansbury, 1990) and the prevalence of depression is relatively high in adults with mild mental retardation (Eaton & Menolascino, 1982). Perhaps, a measure of depression in the mothers with mental retardation in this study would also have been significantly related to child developmental and behavioral outcomes. A transactional approach (Sameroff & Chandler, 1975) which describes the interaction of organismic and environmental variables on child outcome could be utilized to help identify variables affecting the vulnerability and resiliency of these children (Crittenden, 1985). Future research should try to differentiate children of parents with mental retardation who are at greater risk of subsequent problems. Following the transactional model, it is important to examine the accumulating impact of adverse preconceptual genetic influences (e.g., Fragile X), prenatal (e.g., maternal nutrition, smoking, stress) and perinatal factors (e.g., low birthweight, congenital cytomegalovirus infection) in conjunction with subsequent environmental, nutritional, personality, and mother-child interactional variables (Bee et al., 1982; Breitmayer & Ramey, 1986; Parker et al., 1988). Given the results of this study in the context of an increasing societal trend to both protect parenting rights and provide supports and services to try to keep the natural family intact, further attention should be paid to the development and evaluation of specialized services for these families (Feldman, 1994; Walton-Allen & Feldman, 1991). Several studies have shown that child language and cognitive delays can be reduced either by teaching mothers with mental retardation to interact in a more sensitive, reinforcing, and responsive manner with their children (Feldman et al., 1993; Slater, 1986) or enrolling the child in a specialized preschool (Garber,
  • 22. Maternal Mental Retardation 21 1988; Ramey & Ramey, 1992). In the present study, the one child in the group with maternal mental retardation whose mother received interaction training when he was an infant had an IQ of 97. Future early intervention efforts should focus on the prevention of not only intellectual and academic deficits but also behavioral and psychiatric disorders in these children. Considerable work is still needed in first identifying, and then preventing, eliminating or compensating for factors which promote the intergenerational recurrence of child developmental and other problems in these families (Ramey, 1992; Zigler, 1967).
  • 23. Maternal Mental Retardation 22 REFERENCES Abidin, R.R. (1990). Parenting stress index - manual. Charlottesville, VA: Pediatric Psychology Press. Achenbach, T.M., & Edelbrook, C.S. (1981). Behavioral problems and competencies reported by parents of normal and disturbed children aged four through sixteen. Monograph on Social Research in Child Development, 46: (1, Serial No. 188). Bass, M.S. (1963). Marriage, parenthood, and prevention of pregnancy. American Journal of Mental Deficiency, 68, 320-335. Bayley, N. (1969). Bayley scales of infant development: Birth to two years. New York: Psychological Corporation. Bee, H.L., Barnard, K.E., Eyres, S.J., Gray, C.L., Hammond, M.A., Spietz, A.L., Snyder, C., & Clark, B. (1982). Predictions of IQ and language skill from perinatal status, child performance, family characteristics, and mother-infant interaction. Child Development, 53, 1134-1156. Bee, H.L., Hammond, M.A., Eyres, S.J., Barnard, K.E., & Snyder, C. (1986). The impact of parental life change on the early development of children. Research in Nursing and Health, 9, 65-74. Bradley, R.L., Rock, S.L., Caldwell, B.M., & Brisby, J. (1989). Uses of the HOME Inventory for families with handicapped children. American Journal on Mental Retardation, 94, 313-330. Breitmayer, B.J., & Ramey, C.T. (1986). Biological nonoptimality and quality of postnatal environment as codeterminants of intellectual development. Child Development, 57, 1151-1165. Caldwell, B., & Bradley, R. (1984). Home Observation for Measurement of the environment. Unpublished manuscript. Little Rock: University of Arkansas.
  • 24. Maternal Mental Retardation 23 Campbell, F.A., & Ramey, C.T. (1994). Effects of early intervention on intellectual and academic achievement: A follow-up study of children from low income families. Child Development, 65, 684- 698. Cowen, E.L., Huser, J., Beach, D.R., & Rappaport, J. (1970). Parent perceptions of young children and their relation to indexes of development. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 34, 97- 103. Crittenden, P.M. (1985). Maltreated infants: Vulnerability and resilience. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 26, 85-96. Crittenden, P.M., & Bonvillian, J.D. (1984). The relationship between maternal risk status and maternal sensitivity. American Journal of Orthopsychiatry, 54, 250-262. Daniel, W.W. (1978). Applied nonparametric statistics. Boston: Houghton-Mifflin. Dunst, C.J., Trivette, C., & Cross, A. (1986). Mediating influences of social support: Personal, family, and child outcomes. American Journal of Mental Deficiency, 90, 403-417. Eaton, L., & Menolascino, F.J. (1982). Psychiatric disorders in the mentally retarded: Types, problems, and challenges. American Journal of Psychiatry, 139, 1297-1303. Fantuzzo, J.W., Wray, L., Hall, R., Goins, C., & Azar, S. (1986). Parent and social-skills training for mentally retarded mothers identified as child maltreaters. American Journal of Mental Deficiency, 91, 135-140. Feldman, M.A. (1994). Parenting education for parents with intellectual disabilities: A review of outcome studies. Research in Developmental Disabilities, 15, 299-332.
  • 25. Maternal Mental Retardation 24 Feldman, M.A., Case, L., Rincover, A., Towns, F., & Betel, J. (1989). Parent education project III: Increasing affection and responsivity in developmentally handicapped mothers: Component analysis, generalization, and effects on child language. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 22, 211-222. Feldman, M.A., Case, L., & Sparks, B. (1992). Effectiveness of a child-care training program for parents at-risk for child neglect. Canadian Journal of Behavioral Science, 24, 14-28. Feldman, M.A., Case, L., Towns, F., & Betel, J. (1985). Parent Education Project I: The development and nurturance of children of mentally retarded parents. American Journal of Mental Deficiency, 90, 253-258. Feldman, M.A., Sparks, B., & Case, L. (1993). Effectiveness of home-based early intervention on the language development of children of mothers with mental retardation. Research in Developmental Disabilities, 14, 387-408. Feldman, M.A., Towns, F., Betel, J., Case, L., Rincover, A., & Rubino, C.. (1986). Parent Education Project II: Increasing stimulating interactions of developmentally handicapped mothers. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 19, 23-37. Fotheringham, J.B. (1971). The concept of social competence as applied to marriage and child care in those classified as mentally retarded. Canadian Medical Association Journal, 104, 813-816. Garber, H.L. (1988). The Milwaukee project. Preventing mental retardation in children at risk. Washington, DC: American Association on Mental Retardation. Garner, P.W., Carlson Jones, D., & Miner, J.L. (1994). Social competence among low-income preschoolers: Emotion socialization practices and social cognitive correlates. Child Development, 65, 622-637.
  • 26. Maternal Mental Retardation 25 Gillberg, C., & Geijer-Karlsson, M. (1983). Children born to mentally retarded women: A 1-21 year follow-up study of 41 cases. Psychological Medicine, 13, 891-894. Grossman, H.J. (Ed.). (1983). Classification in mental retardation. Washington, DC: American Association on Mental Deficiency. Hammen, C., Burge, D., & Stansbury, K. (1990). Relationship of mother and child variables to child outcomes in a high-risk sample. Developmental Psychology, 26, 24-30. Hayman, R.L. (1990). Presumptions of justice: Law, politics, and the mentally retarded parent. Harvard Law Review, 103, 1201-1271. Jastak, J., & Jastak, S. (1984). Wide Range Achievement Test-Revised. Wilmington, DE: Jastak Associates. Keltner, B.R. (1992). Caregiving by mothers with mental retardation. Family and Community Health, 15, 10-18. Keltner, B.R. (1994). Home environments of mothers with mental retardation. Mental Retardation, 32, 123-127. Mercer, J.R. (1973). Labelling the mentally retarded: Clinical and social system perspectives on mental retardation. Berkeley, CA: University of California Press. Mickelson, P. (1947). The feebleminded parent: A study of 90 family cases. American Journal of Mental Deficiency, 51, 644-653. Offord, D.R., Boyle, M.H., Szatmari, P., Rae-Grant, N.I., Links, P.S., Cadman, D.T., Byles, J.A., Crawford, J.W., Blum, H.M., Byrne, C., Thomas, H., & Woodward, C.A. (1987). Ontario child health
  • 27. Maternal Mental Retardation 26 study: II. Six month prevalence of disorder and rates of service utilization. Archives of General Psychiatry, 44, 832-836. O'Neill, A.M. (1985). Normal and bright children of mentally retarded parents: The Huck Finn syndrome. Child Psychiatry and Human Development 15, 255-268. Parker, S., Greer, S., & Zuckerman, B. (1988). Double jeopardy: The impact of poverty on early child development. The Pediatric Clinics of North America, 35, 1227-1239. Ramey, C.T. (1992). High-risk children and IQ: Altering intergenerational patterns. Intelligence, 16, 239-256. Ramey, C.T., & Ramey, S.L. (1992). Effective early intervention. Mental Retardation, 30, 337-345. Reed, E.W., & Reed, S.C. (1965). Mental retardation: A family study. Philadelphia: Saunders. Ross, D.P., & Shillington, E.R. (1989). The Canadian fact book on poverty - 1989. Ottawa: Canadian Council on Social Development. Sameroff, A.J., & Chandler, M. (1975). Reproductive risk and the continuum of caretaker causality. In F. Horowitz (Ed.), Review of child development research (vol. 4) (pp. 157-243). Chicago: University of Chicago Press. Scally, B.G. (1973). Marriage and mental handicap: Some observations in Northern Ireland. In F.F. de la Cruz and G.D. La Veck (Eds.) Human sexuality and the mentally retarded (pp. 186-194). New York: Brunner/Mazel. Schilling, R.F., Schinke, S.P, Blythe, B.J., & Barth, R.P. (1982). Child maltreatment and mentally retarded parents: Is there a relationship? Mental Retardation, 20, 201-209.
  • 28. Maternal Mental Retardation 27 Seagull, E.A., & Scheurer, S.L. (1986). Neglected and abused children of mentally retarded parents. Child Abuse and Neglect, 10, 493-500. Slater, M.A. (1986). Modification of mother-child interaction processes in families with children at- risk for mental retardation. American Journal of Mental Deficiency, 91, 257-267. Statistics Canada (1987). Ontario Child Health Study Child Behaviour Checklist. Ottawa: Author. Taylor, C.G., Norman, D.K., Murphy, J.M., Jellinek, M., Quinn, D., Poitrast, F.G., & Goshko, M. (1991). Diagnosed intellectual and emotional impairment among parents who seriously mistreat their children: Prevalence, type, and outcome in a court sample. Child Abuse and Neglect, 15, 389-401. Tymchuk, A.J., Andron, L., & Tymchuk, M. (1990). Training mothers with mental handicaps to understand developmental and behavioural principles. Mental Handicap Research, 3, 51-59. Tymchuk, A.J., & Feldman, M.A. (1991). Parents with mental retardation and their children: A review of research relevant to professional practice. Canadian Psychology/Psychologie Canadienne, 32, 486-494. Vogel, P. (1987). The right to parent. Entourage, 2, 33-39. Walton-Allen, N., & Feldman, M.A. (1991). Perceptions of service needs by parents who are mentally retarded and their workers. Comprehensive Mental Health Care, 1, 57-67. Wechsler, D. (1974). Manual for the Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children - Revised. Zigler, E.F. (1967). Familial mental retardation: A continuing dilemma. Science, 155, 292-298.
  • 29. Maternal Mental Retardation 28 Table 1 Demographic information Family Variables Parents with Parents without Mental Retardation Mental Retardation (n = 27) (n = 25) Subject-Selection Variables Mean Maternal IQ (WAIS-R) 63.6 (5.5) 93.8 (11.2)** Percent of Mothers Who Received Special Ed. Services 48.1% 0** Mean No. of Social Service Agencies Involved Per Family 2.27 (1.44) 0 Median Range of Annual Family Incomea $7K - 10.5K $7K - 10.5K Other variables Mean Maternal Age (years) 35.1 (4.7) 33.7 (6.1) Percent Single Mothers 44.4% 44.0% Percent of Mothers Institutionalized 3.7% 0 Percent Mothers Receiving Welfare 88.9% 68.0% Percent Mothers Employed 11.1% 24.0% Percent Living in Subsidized Housing 63.0% 48.0% Mean Crowding Ratiob 1.62 (0.6) 1.49 (0.4) Percent with Father Reported to be MR or Received Special Ed. Services 66.7% 8%**
  • 30. Maternal Mental Retardation 29 Table 1 (Con't) Demographic information Family Variables Parents with Parents without Mental Retardation Mental Retardation (n = 27) (n = 25) Percent with Target Child's Sibling Removed 14.8% 0 Percent with Child Welfare Supervision 22.2% 0* Mean Child Age 9.7 (2.2) 10.2 (2.3) Mean Birth Order of Target Child 1.1 (0.4) 1.3 (0.5) Proportion of Boys to Girls 19/8 12/13 * p< .05; ** p< .01; the numbers in brackets are standard deviations. a Exact incomes are not available as parents were asked to indicate income within C$5K intervals from 0- C$5K to > C$25K (converted here to US funds). b Crowding Ratio = total number of persons living at home divided by the total number of rooms.
  • 31. Maternal Mental Retardation 30 Table 2 Means and standard deviations of dependent measures Parents with Mental Retardation Parents without Mental Retardation MEASURES TOTAL BOYS GIRLS TOTAL BOYS GIRLS HOME 36.52 (8.07) 34.11 (7.71) 41.33 (6.80) 44.64 (6.34) 46.18 (6.14) 43.09 (6.43) SOCIAL ISOLATIONa 39.85 (6.00) 41.33 (6.15) 36.89 (4.65) 33.75 (7.16) 33.33 (6.73) 34.17 (7.84) WISC-R 80.54 (14.32) 80.11 (15.13) 81.50 (13.22) 102.88 (14.25) 103.58 (16.66) 102.23 (12.28) WRAT-R READING 73.20 (19.10) 71.05 (19.49) 77.75 (18.65) 94.25 (16.80) 97.18 (23.34) 91.77 (8.50) WRAT-R SPELLING 71.56 (21.38) 66.88 (21.42) 81.50 (18.77) 89.54 (17.50) 93.09 (20.34) 86.54 (14.86) WRAT-R MATH 69.68 (14.68) 68.12 (14.06) 73.00 (16.38) 89.21 (13.12) 89.27 (16.80) 89.15 (9.75) CBCL CONDUCT 6.30 (5.43) 7.28 (5.62) 4.33 (4.69) 2.36 (2.06) 2.75 (2.14) 2.00 (2.00) CBCL HYPERACT. 5.44 (2.71) 6.17 (2.71) 4.00 (2.18) 2.76 (2.50) 3.17 (2.33) 2.38 (2.69) CBCL EMOTIONAL 5.93 (2.56) 6.06 (2.26) 5.67 (3.20) 4.44 (3.50) 3.50 (2.61) 5.31 (4.07) a Social Isolation: higher scores indicate more maternal social isolation and less social support
  • 32. Maternal Mental Retardation 31 Table 3 Spearman sign-rank correlation coefficents of parental IQ, social isolation, and home environment scores with child IQ, academic achievement, and behavior problem scores Parents with Mental Retardation Parents without Mental Retardation WAIS-R SOC. ISOL. HOME WAIS-R SOC. ISOL. HOME WISC-R -.30 .20 .22 .10 .13 .51** WRAT-READ .17 .09 .25 -.04 .04 .47* WRAT-SPELL -.20 -.09 .30 -.27 .09 .27 WRAT-MATH -.14 .08 .45* .42* .07 .60** CBCL-CON. -.30 .48* -.43* .04 .18 -.04 CBCL-HYP. -.08 .47* -.39* .29 .45* -.21 CBCL-EMOT. .10 .30 .14 .09 .37* -.11 * p< .05; ** p< 01
  • 33. Maternal Mental Retardation 32 Figure Captions Figure 1. Distribution of WISC-R full scale IQ scores for children of parents with mental retardation and low income comparison children of parents without mental retardation. Figure 2. Distribution of WRAT-R Reading, Spelling, and Arithmetic standard scores for children of parents with mental retardation and low income comparison children of parents without mental retardation. Figure 3. Percentage of children of parents with mental retardation, low income comparison children of parents without mental retardation, and Ontario norms for CBCL scores above the threshold for clinically significant behavior disorders.
  • 34. MATERNAL MENTAL RETARDATION 33 Authors' Notes This research was sponsored by the Ontario Mental Health Foundation and the Ontario Ministry of Community and Social Services Research Grants Program (administered by the Research and Program Evaluation Unit). We thank: J. Carnwell, L. Case, M. Garrick, W. MacIntyre-Grande, and R. Malik for their assistance in collecting the data; Drs. J. Berg, and D. Yu for their feedback on earlier versions of the manuscript; and Dr. L. Atkinson for his statistical advice and insightful commentary. Requests for reprints should be sent to Maurice Feldman, Ph.D., Dept. of Psychology, Queen's University, Kingston, Ontario Canada K7L 3N6; email: feldman@pavlov.psyc.queensu.ca.
  • 35. Maternal Mental Retardation and Poverty: Intellectual, Academic, and Behavioral Status of School-Age Children Key Terms: CHILD DEVELOPMENT, MATERNAL MENTAL RETARDATION, POVERTY Running Head: MATERNAL MENTAL RETARDATION View publication statsView publication stats