SlideShare a Scribd company logo
1 of 9
Download to read offline
Full Terms & Conditions of access and use can be found at
http://www.tandfonline.com/action/journalInformation?journalCode=htip20
Download by: [The UC San Diego Library] Date: 03 May 2017, At: 00:04
Theory Into Practice
ISSN: 0040-5841 (Print) 1543-0421 (Online) Journal homepage: http://www.tandfonline.com/loi/htip20
Addressing Academic Dishonesty Among the
Highest Achievers
Angela D. Miller, Tamera B. Murdock & Morgan M. Grotewiel
To cite this article: Angela D. Miller, Tamera B. Murdock & Morgan M. Grotewiel (2017)
Addressing Academic Dishonesty Among the Highest Achievers, Theory Into Practice, 56:2,
121-128, DOI: 10.1080/00405841.2017.1283574
To link to this article: http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/00405841.2017.1283574
Published online: 08 Mar 2017.
Submit your article to this journal
Article views: 34
View related articles
View Crossmark data
Angela D. Miller
Tamera B. Murdock
Morgan M. Grotewiel
Addressing Academic Dishonesty
Among the Highest Achievers
Although research shows that higher-achieving
students report engaging in cheating behaviors
less frequently than lower-achieving students, the
cheating rates among this population are still star-
tling. Certain aspects of the context of being a
high-achieving student support academic dishon-
esty. We investigate integrity among the highest
achievers using a motivational framework, ļ¬rst
examining why these students feel the need to
cheat. We discuss personal standards of perfor-
mance, social comparison and competition, pres-
sure to succeed, and these studentsā€™ ability to
rationalize cheating behaviors. Finally, we suggest
what can be done to combat cheating among high
achieving students, including thinking about
approaches to pedagogy and assessment, provid-
ing clarity and consequences for cheating, and
considering the culture of high achievers.
Academic dishonesty is any deceitful or unfair
act intended to produce a more desirable
outcome on an exam, paper, homework assign-
ment, or other assessment of learning. The perva-
siveness of cheating from middle school through
graduate education has stimulated many discus-
sions of the factors that support academic dishon-
esty (for reviews, see Murdock & Anderman,
2006; Whitley, 1998). Although cheating is
sometimes viewed as a victimless crime, it has
numerous consequences for others. Cheating
interferes with teachersā€™ ability to accurately
assess learning and noncheating students suffer
from the potential elevation of the normative
achievement bar. Being compared to others with
inļ¬‚ated grades might affect studentsā€™ self-
conļ¬dence and create a true competitive disad-
vantage for grade-based awards (i.e., admission
to college, receipt of an award). Finally, students
who witness unchecked cheating in a given class
may develop the belief that cheating is normative
and thereby be more inclined to cheat themselves
(Oā€™Rourke et al., 2010). In sum, there are numer-
ous reasons for educators to care about academic
dishonesty.
Angela D. Miller, Ph.D., is at George Mason University.
Tamera B. Murdock, Ph.D., is at the University of
Missouri-Kansas City. Morgan M. Grotewiel, M.A., is
at the University of Missouri-Kansas City.
Correspondence should be addressed to Angela D.
Miller, Ph.D., George Mason University, West
Building, Room 2105, 4400 University Drive MS
6D2, Fairfax, VA 22030. E-mail: amille35@gmu.edu
Theory Into Practice, 56:121ā€“128, 2017
Copyright Ā© The College of Education and Human Ecology, The Ohio State University
ISSN: 0040-5841 print/1543-0421 online
DOI: 10.1080/00405841.2017.1283574
121
Conceptual Framework
We discuss cheating among high-achieving
students using a motivational framework offered
by Murdock and Anderman (2006). They pro-
posed that cheating in any speciļ¬c context can
be predicted by studentsā€™ answers to three
questions:
ā— What is my purpose here?
ā— Can I do this? and
ā— What are the costs of cheating?
Studentsā€™ motivation or purpose when they
approach their academic work is often viewed
as some combination of their learning goals, or
a desire to learn, understand, and master the
material and their performance goals, or their
desire to win, outperform others, demonstrate
aptitude, or earn a high grade (Dweck &
Leggett, 1988). Students with strong learning
goals are less likely to cheat, as compared to
students who see less relevance to what they are
learning and are simply aiming to get a high
grade (Anderman, Griesinger & Westerļ¬eld,
1998).
Beyond purpose, studentsā€™ evaluation of their
own capabilities drives the decision to cheat.
These capabilities are most often assessed as
studentsā€™ self-efļ¬cacy or success expectations
(Bandura, 1997). They are inļ¬‚uenced by their
own academic performance history, the social
comparisons they make, the pedagogical skills
of the teacher, the workload students carry, and
a teacherā€™s emphasis on individual growth versus
social comparisons. Students cheat more fre-
quently when their expectancies for success or
academic self-beliefs are low (Anderman, et al.,
1998).
Finally, students estimate the perceived costs
of cheating using some form of risk-beneļ¬t
analysis (Rettinger, 2007). If the perceived
costs of dishonesty outweigh the perceived
gain, cheating is less likely to be a desirable
strategy thus students become less likely to
cheat as the perceived risk of detection and
punishment rise. Cheating also requires stu-
dents to reconcile the costs to their self-
concept by violating the norms of what a
good person does. This ability to neutralize,
justify, or explain away cheating by blaming
others for it (e.g., I have a bad teacher), fram-
ing the behavior as typical (e.g., everyone else
cheats), and/or minimizing the behavior (e.g., it
hurts no one) reduces the threat to oneā€™s self-
concept and increases the likelihood of dishon-
esty (Murdock & Stephens, 2007).
Why Care About Cheating Among
High-Achieving Students?
Research consistently ļ¬nds that higher-
achieving students report fewer cheating beha-
viors compared to their lower achieving peers,
which may explain the relative lack of empirical
attention to dishonesty in this speciļ¬c population.
However, high-achieving middle and high school
students are by no means immune from cheating.
Eighty percent of students attending Stuyvesant
High School in New York City, a highly compe-
titive magnet school for the best and brightest,
admitted to cheating according to an article in the
New York Times (Yess, 2012). A survey of high
school students (age 14 to 18) from Advanced
Placement (AP) or Honors Math and/or Science
classes revealed high rates of cheating behaviors
on exams (approximately 75%) and even higher
rates of cheating on homework (Geddes, 2011).
Finally, a 2005 review of the literature on medi-
cal school students reports high rates of academic
cheating (25%) and even higher rates of witnes-
sing cheating (66%; Dyrbye, Thomas, &
Shanafelt, 2005).
High-achieving students have the human capi-
tal to ascend to positions in society where they
have inļ¬‚uence over others both directly (e.g., as
supervisors, leaders, etc.) and indirectly (e.g.,
conducting and sharing research, contributing to
policy, donating money to causes and candi-
dates). Evidence suggests that people who are
dishonest in their academic careers are more apt
to continue a pattern of dishonesty in their work
lives (Nonis & Smith, 2001), making the integrity
of these students a concern for us all.
Theoretical Approaches to Understanding and Promoting Academic Integrity
122
Why Do High Achievers Cheat?
Personal Standard of Performance
Although students in academically competi-
tive programs may have higher academic self-
efļ¬cacy than students in less competitive pro-
grams, they also report more stress than their
peers (Suldo, Shaunessy, & Hardesty, 2008).
Worry has been linked to increased cheating
(Anderman et al., 1998) and high-achieving
high school students identify pressure for grades
as one of their top reasons for cheating (Geddes,
2011).
The pressure created from studentsā€™ perfor-
mance standards might be augmented by their
understanding of intelligence. Dweckā€™s (2006)
widely cited Mindset theory delineates between
students who see intelligence as something that
improves with learning, termed growth mindsets,
and those who believe that the intellect they are
born with cannot be modiļ¬ed, termed a ā€œļ¬xedā€
mindset. A ļ¬xed mindset is associated with nega-
tive emotions such as shame after feedback and
maladaptive behaviors including academic cheat-
ing (Dweck & Master, 2009). A substantial num-
ber of gifted high school students hold entity or
ļ¬xed views of intelligence and even more of
these same students see giftedness as something
that is not malleable (Makel, Snyder, Chandler,
Malone, & Puttallaz, 2015). Similarly, students at
a highly competitive university were more apt to
hold entity views of intelligence if they were
identiļ¬ed as gifted during high school, and the
higher their own aptitude, the more likely they
were to hold these beliefs (Snyder, Barger,
Wormington, Schwartz-Bloom, & Linnenbrink-
Garcia, 2013). Given the centrality of academic
accomplishment to the self-concept of high
achieving students, they might be particularly
reactive when threatened with performance out-
comes that donā€™t meet their high standards. In
other words, the risk of getting low grades may
pose such a grave threat to these studentsā€™ being
seen as smart or gifted that cheating may be seen
as relatively less threatening to their self-concept,
especially if it is the norm within their academic
culture.
Ever-Increasing Social Comparisons and
Competition
As high achievers progress through the educa-
tional pipeline, they tend to be concentrated into
increasingly homogenous classes in terms of their
ability and accomplishments, with ever-
increasing demands and higher levels of social
comparison. Through the lens of our theoretical
framework, these contexts might create an
increased focus on performance, and increase
the perceived normativeness of dishonesty,
thereby making cheating seem more acceptable.
One study explicitly looked at the role of
achievement context versus individual achieve-
ment as a contributor to cheating by comparing
cheating at public high schools that were in the
top, second, third, and fourth quartiles in terms of
school-level performance as measured by state-
wide assessments (Brandes, 1986). A students at
the highest achieving schools cheated less often
than A students at the lowest-achieving schools;
however, students with GPAs of B+ or below
were more likely to cheat if they attended a
higher-achieving school, suggesting the inļ¬‚uence
of local achievement norms on cheating patterns.
It is likely that lower achieving students in high
performing schools feel the most pressure to suc-
ceed because there is the most discrepancy
between their performance and the schoolā€™s over-
all level of achievement, ultimately making them
the most likely to cheat.
Other evidence for the potentially deleterious
inļ¬‚uence of competition or other-based compar-
isons comes from the literature on perfectionism.
Perfectionism, or critically evaluating oneself
against high standards, has been found to be
quite common among high-ability students
(Neumeister, 2004). However, while some gifted
students have more self-oriented perfectionism,
setting high standards for themselves, socially-
oriented perfectionists strive to meet the per-
ceived standards of others. Self-oriented perfec-
tionists tend to be driven by a desire for mastery
and approach goals; they compete with their own
standards, display adaptive behaviors such as
effort and persistence and have generally positive
affect in achievement settings. Those with
Miller, Murdock, Grotewiel Academic Dishonesty Among High Achievers
123
socially oriented perfectionism are more prone to
worry and anxiety, compete with their peers, give
up easily, and, are more apt to believe that cheat-
ing is justiļ¬able (Bong, Hwang, Noh, & Kim,
2014; Neumeister, 2004). Classrooms that stu-
dents perceive as emphasizing social compari-
sons and competition also are ones in which
rates of academic cheating are higher
(Anderman & Midgely, 2004).
Pressures to ā€œDo It Allā€
High-achieving high school students cite
heavy workloads (68%) and multiple tests being
scheduled on the same day (60%) as two of the
top reasons that they cheat (Geddes, 2011). On
top of this, high achieving students at all levels
talk about the demands to carry difļ¬cult course
loads while engaging in activities that distinguish
them from other high achievers when they go to
compete at the ā€œnext levelā€ (Geddes, 2011).
Although students may have the efļ¬cacy (and
skills) for any of these tasks in isolation, the
cumulative burden creates enormous stress and
encourages students to seek shortcuts to accom-
plish all of their goals. This overload has been
explicitly examined among medical students,
where high rates of burnout are common:
Although burnout was not linked to academic
cheating per se, it did predict unprofessional con-
duct in the context of clinical work (Dyrbye
et al., 2010). Other reasons for cheating listed
by high-achieving students in Geddesā€™ (2011)
study include ā€œunrealisticā€ workloads, helping a
friend, and ā€œloyalty to a group.ā€ Similar views
were echoed by the Stuyvesant students who
described the environment as one in which stu-
dents worked together at cheating to beat what
they perceived to be the demands being placed on
them (Yess, 2012).
Increased Rationalization
High-achieving studentsā€™ well-developed cog-
nitive abilities might translate into an increased
ability to rationalize or justify cheating; these atti-
tudes are consistently one of the strongest predic-
tors of dishonesty (Murdock & Anderman, 2006;
Whitley, 1998). Cheating is regularly rationalized
by externalizing the blame onto the teacher or
situation, and these externalizations increase
when students see the pedagogy as poor, the tea-
cher as uncaring, or the classroom as focused on
performance versus mastery goals (Murdock,
Miller, & Kohlhardt, 2004; Murdock &
Stephens, 2007). The enormity of the demands
that many high-achieving students feel might pro-
vide an easy justiļ¬cation for dishonest behavior.
For example, Geddes (2011) argued that many
high achieving students adopt an us versus them
mentality that pits the students against the teacher.
This mentality may help students justify cheating.
Cultural Pressure
High-achieving students have a status to main-
tain. They want to get the best grades, get into the
best colleges and universities, and live up to
parental, teacher, and peer expectations.
Galloway (2012) interviewed students in 10 high-
achieving, advantaged high schools where there
are many honors and AP courses and a majority
of the graduating students attend 4-year colleges.
She found that these students feel forced to cheat
because of the pressure, workload, and culture of
their school communities. Students described
their school experience as being corrupted by
their environment. These students viewed cheat-
ing as a compromise for a good reason: They saw
their situation as ā€œcheat or be cheatedā€ (p. 393).
Cheating for students in these contexts is a way
to sustain their status in a community that cele-
brates their success.
What Must Change?
There are many obvious steps to curbing
cheating by increasing the likelihood of detec-
tion, including changing seating arrangements,
creating multiple versions of exams, limiting
access to technology during testing, and using
antiplagiarism software to ļ¬ght the proliferation
of the electronic dimension to cheating.
Ultimately, students learn increasingly sophisti-
cated ways to cheat and the myriad of factors
Theoretical Approaches to Understanding and Promoting Academic Integrity
124
that create a culture of cheating are not
addressed. Attitudes must change as well, such
that students see intrinsic value in completing
honest work. These changes require creating a
culture where demands are reasonable, learning
is valued over performance, and cheating is
treated as unacceptable. In the following we
look at several concrete steps that can help
promote these values.
Reasonable Demands
High-achieving students are often taking sev-
eral challenging classes at the same time, taxing
studentsā€™ limits to get all of their work done. At
the school and classroom level, this might be
addressed in several ways. First, school adminis-
ters can encourage collaboration among teachers
of advanced classes so that large assignments and
exams are spaced across days. Being explicit
about this spacing and the reasons for this spa-
cing with students will also communicate to them
an empathy for the workload they are carrying
and a commitment to good pedagogy. Thus, these
practices make the work more doable; the like-
lihood of cheating should be lessened because of
the more favorable way that teachers will be
viewed.
In a similar vein, teachers might strive to limit
out-of-classroom work (e.g., homework) to a rea-
sonable amount across classes. Students from 10
high-achieving high schools reported completing
an average over 3 hr of homework each day
(Cooper, Robinson, & Patall, 2006). Although
students doing more homework were rated as
more engaged in school, they were also more
stressed and reported having a harder time balan-
cing the rest of their lives. Homework require-
ments should promote learning: Lots of busy
work creates a focus on completion, rather than
understanding (for more detail on best homework
practices see, Cooper, 2007).
Finally, it is never too early to explicitly teach
students self-regulation and time management
strategies so that they can more effectively man-
age their academic lives, including learning to
use a planner to map out shorter and longer
periods of time, breaking tasks down into
manageable parts and learning how to trouble-
shoot a difļ¬cult problem or assignment by using
available resources and/or reaching out for help.
Teaching parents how to offer scaffolding versus
providing answers will also increase studentsā€™
ability to complete tasks on their own, rather
than be tempted to take shortcuts (see Cooper
et al., 2006).
Promoting Learning Versus Performance
Self-reported cheating has been correlated
with a fear of failure (Schab, 1991), test anxiety
(Malinowski & Smith, 1985), and worry over
performance (Anderman, et al., 1998) all of
which increase in classrooms where there is an
emphasis on normative performance. Moreover,
in an era of high-stakes testing, it is easy to be
consumed by the importance and implications of
test performance instead of teaching for the pur-
poses of conceptual understanding. Students fol-
low their teachersā€™ lead and if the focus of each
lesson is to learn the content for the next big test,
then that impending test becomes the goal, rather
than learning the course content. Results become
most highly valued. When middle and high
school students read scenarios about classrooms
where the teacher was focused on grades and test
scores rather than mastery of concepts, they rated
these classrooms as those where cheating is the
most likely to occur (Murdock et al., 2004).
These same students also noted that cheating
was less likely to occur in settings where it was
evident that the teacher cared about student pro-
gress and learning, and emphasized effort as a
means to better understanding.
Although research suggests that all students
fair better in classrooms that are mastery
oriented, achieving a mastery focus may be the
more difļ¬cult with highest achieving students,
who often have multiple sources of achievement
pressure. Providing formative assessment oppor-
tunities is one suggestion for achieving this goal.
Nonpunitive miniassessments, practice exams
that are reviewed in class, homework assign-
ments that are reviewed for accuracy and can be
corrected until the concepts are mastered and
clear alignment of formative and summative
Miller, Murdock, Grotewiel Academic Dishonesty Among High Achievers
125
assessments are some concrete ways to achieve
this. Increasingly, teachers at all levels can rely
on technology to aid them with these practices:
many textbooks publishers provide online home-
work systems that provide students with the
opportunity to redo homework multiple times
and provide them with explanations to help
them achieve mastery. Course shells such as
Blackboard allow posting of timed assessments
and immediate feedback for students.
Clarity and Consequences
Academic dishonesty is commonly
addressed administratively with honor code
systems at either the classroom or institutional
level. Typically, these codes clearly delineate
what is considered honest and dishonest,
require students to formally attest that they
will behave in an honorable manner, and,
many times, require students to agree to create
a culture of honesty in their school by reporting
any incident of peer transgression. Although
some research has suggested that these honor
codes might improve student academic integrity
(Konheim-Kalkstein, 2006; McCabe & Pavela,
2000), a recent analysis at the college level
suggest that the effects of these codes are
declining, because students value grades more
than an abstract moral standard, and they see it
as the job of the school/teacher to detect and
deal with cheating (Vandehey, Diekhoff, &
LaBeff, 2007).
At a basic level, schools must provide informa-
tion on academic integrity and speciļ¬c deļ¬nitions
of what is considered cheating as students often do
not understand what constitutes academic dishon-
esty (Galloway, 2012). In addition, there also
needs to be a culture of integrity (Schwartz,
Tatum, & Hageman, 2013) and clear conse-
quences for cheating (Galloway, 2012).
Gallowayā€™s interviews with high school students
and staff members found that many teachers donā€™t
follow the honor code, and that some staff mem-
bers even condone cheating as a high school norm.
In a school lacking academic integrity as a cultural
value, students can more easily justify cheating
and develop neutralizing attitudes toward cheating
(Rettinger & Kramer, 2009). There must be dis-
cussion, and schools must model integrity and
open dialog with students and their parents.
Given the repeated ļ¬nding that students cheat
less when they feel that teachers treat them with
fairness and respect (Murdock et al., 2004), dia-
logs with students that are explicit about expecta-
tions and followed up with clear, consistent action
are likely to mitigate cheating.
In short, high-achieving students are experien-
cing an early entrance into the highly competi-
tive, winner takes all society that Callahan
described in his book The Cheating Culture
(2004). The growing gap separating those at the
top means increases in the pressures that parents
and their children feel to be successful and not be
left behind. Although there is no magical solution
for addressing dishonesty in this climate of win-
ner takes all, particularly for youth who are on
the fast track to be the winners, cheating among
this group might be reduced if teachers and
school administrators work to make assessment
clear, fair, and consistent; model the value of
learning; minimize comparisons among students;
teach students prioritization and regulation skills;
and communicate their empathy for high
demands and worries associated with the stu-
dentsā€™ pressured lives.
References
Anderman, E. M., Griesinger, T., & Westerļ¬eld, G.
(1998). Motivation and cheating during early
adolescence. Journal of Educational Psychology,
90, 84ā€“93. doi:10.1037/0022-0663.90.1.84
Anderman, E. M., & Midgley, C. (2004). Changes in
self-reported academic cheating across the transi-
tion from middle school to high school.
Contemporary Educational Psychology, 29,
499ā€“517. doi:10.1016/j.cedpsych.2004.02.002
Bandura, A. (1997). Self-efļ¬cacy: The exercise of con-
trol. New York, NY: Freeman.
Bong, M. (2008). Effects of parentā€“child relationships
and classroom goal structures on motivation, help-
seeking avoidance, and cheating. Journal of
Experimental Education, 76, 191ā€“217.
doi:10.3200/JEXE.76.2.191-217
Theoretical Approaches to Understanding and Promoting Academic Integrity
126
Bong, M., Hwang, A., Noh, A., & Kim, S. I. (2014).
Perfectionism and motivation of adolescents in aca-
demic contexts. Journal of Educational
Psychology, 106, 711ā€“729. doi:10.1037/a0035836
Brandes, B. (1986). Academic honesty: A special study
of California students. Sacramento, CA: California
State Department of Education, Bureau of
Publications.
Callahan, D. (2004). The cheating culture: Why more
Americans are doing wrong to get ahead. Orlando,
FL: Harcourt.
Cooper, H., Robinson, J. C., & Patall, E. A. (2006).
Does homework improve academic achievement?
A synthesis of research, 1987ā€“2003. Review of
Educational Research, 76, 1ā€“62. doi: 10.3102/
00346543076001001
Cooper, H. M. (2007). The battle over homework:
Common ground for administrators, teachers, and
parents (3rd ed.). Thousand Oaks, CA: Corwin
Press.
Dweck, C. (2006). Mindset: The new psychology of
success. New York, NY: Random House.
Dweck, C. S., & Leggett, E. L. (1988). A
social-cognitive approach to motivation and
personality. Psychological Review, 95, 256ā€“270.
doi:10.1037/0033-295X.95.2.256
Dweck, C. S., & Master, A. (2009). Self-theories and
motivation: Studentsā€™ beliefs about intelligence. In
K. R. Wentzel & A. Wigļ¬eld (Eds.), Handbook of
motivation at school (pp. 123ā€“140). New York,
NY: Routledge.
Dyrbye, L. N., Thomas, M. R., & Shanafelt, T. D.
(2005). Medical student distress: causes, conse-
quences, and proposed solutions. Mayo Clinic
Proceedings, 80, 1613ā€“1622. doi:10.4065/
80.12.1613
Galloway, M. K. (2012). Cheating in advantaged high
schools: Prevalence, justiļ¬cations, and possibilities
for change. Ethics & Behavior, 22, 378ā€“399.
doi:10.1080/10508422.2012.679143
Geddes, K. A. (2011). Academic dishonesty among gifted
and high-achieving students. Gifted Child Today, 34,
50ā€“56. doi:10.1177/107621751103400214
Konheim-Kalkstein, Y. L. (2006). Use of a classroom
honor code in higher education. Journal of
Credibility Assessment and Witness Psychology, 7,
169ā€“179.
Makel, M. C., Snyder, K.E., Chandler, C., Malone,
P.S., & Puttallaz, M. (2015). Gifted studentsā€™ impli-
cit beliefs about intelligence and giftedness. Gifted
Child Quarterly, 59, 203ā€“212. doi:10.1177/
0016986215599057
Malinowski, C. I., & Smith, C. P. (1985). Moral rea-
soning and moral conduct: An investigation
prompted by Kohlbergā€™s theory. Journal of
Personality and Social Psychology, 49,
1016ā€“1027. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.49.4.1016
McCabe, D. L., & Pavela, G. (2000). Some good news
about academic integrity. Change, 33, 32ā€“38.
doi:10.1080/00091380009605738
Murdock, T. B., & Anderman, E. M. (2006).
Motivational perspectives on student cheating:
Toward an integrated model of academic
dishonesty. Educational Psychologist, 41,
129ā€“145. doi:10.1207/s15326985ep4103_1
Murdock, T. B., Miller, A., & Kohlhardt, J. (2004).
Effects of classroom context variables on high
school studentsā€™ judgments of the acceptability
and likelihood of cheating. Journal of Educational
Psychology, 96, 765ā€“777. doi:http:10.1037/0022-
0663.96.4.765
Murdock, T. B., & Stephens, J. B. (2007). Is cheating
wrong? Studentsā€™ reasoning about academic dis-
honesty. In E. A. Anderman & T. B. Murdock
(Eds.), The psychology of academic cheating (pp.
229ā€“251). San Diego, CA: Elsevier.
Neumeister, K. L. S. (2004). Understanding the relation-
ship between perfectionism and achievement motiva-
tion in gifted college students. Gifted Child Quarterly,
48, 219ā€“231. doi:10.1177/001698620404800306
Nonis, S., & Swift, C. O. (2001). An examination of
the relationship between academic dishonesty and
workplace dishonesty: A multicampus
investigation. Journal of Education for Business,
77(2), 69ā€“77. doi:10.1080/08832320109599052
Oā€™Rourke, J., Barnes, J., Deaton, A., Fulks, K., Ryan,
K., & Rettinger, D. A. (2010). Imitation is the
sincerest form of cheating: The inļ¬‚uence of direct
knowledge and attitudes on academic dishonesty.
Ethics & Behavior, 20, 47ā€“64. doi:10.1080/
10508420903482616
Rettinger, D. 2007. Applying decision theory to aca-
demic integrity decisions. In E. Anderman & T.
Murdock (Eds.), Psychology of academic cheating
(pp. 141ā€“167). San Diego, CA: Elsevier.
Rettinger, D. A., & Kramer, Y. (2009). Situational and
personal causes of student cheating. Research in
Higher Education, 50, 293ā€“313. doi:10.1007/
s11162-008-9116-5
Schab, F. (1991). Schooling without learning: Thirty years
of cheating in high school. Adolescence, 26, 839ā€“847.
Schwartz, B. M., Tatum, H. E., & Hageman, M. C.
(2013). College studentsā€™ perceptions of and
responses to cheating at traditional, modiļ¬ed, and
Miller, Murdock, Grotewiel Academic Dishonesty Among High Achievers
127
non-honor system institutions. Ethics & Behavior,
23, 463ā€“476. doi:10.1080/10508422.2013.814538
Snyder, K. E., Barger, M., Wormington, S.V.,
Schwartz-Bloom, R., & Linnenbrink-Garcia, L.
(2013). Identiļ¬cation as gifted and implicit beliefs
about intelligence: An examination of potential
moderators. Journal of Advanced Academics, 24,
242ā€“258. doi:10.1177/1932202X13507971.
Suldo, S. M., Shaunessy, E., & Hardesty, R. (2008).
Relationships among stress, coping, and mental
health in highā€achieving high school students.
Psychology in the Schools, 45, 273ā€“290.
doi:10.1002/pits.20300
Vandehey, M. A. & Diekhoff, G. & LaBeff, E. (2007).
College cheating: A twenty-year follow-up and the
addition of an honor code. Journal of College
Student Development, 48, 468ā€“480 doi:10.1353/
csd.2007.0043.
Whitley, B. E. (1998). Factors associated with cheating
among college students: A review. Research in
Higher Education, 29, 235ā€“274. doi:10.1023/
a:1018724900565
Yess, V. (2012, September 25). Stuyvesant students
describe the how and why of cheating. New York
Times. Retrieved from. http://www.nytimes.com/
2012/09/26/education/stuyvesant-high-school-
students-describe-rationale-for-cheating.html
Additional Resources
1. Anderman, E. A., & Murdock, T. B. (Eds.).
(2006). Psychology of academic cheating.
San Diego, CA: Elsevier.
This book is a research-based examination
of cheating: Who cheats and why? How do
they cheat? What are the consequences?
What are the ways of stopping it before it
starts? Individual personality variables are
considered, as well as contextual correlates
to cheating.
2. Cooper, H. M. (2007). Battle over home-
work: Common ground for administrators,
teachers, and parents (3rd ed.) Thousand
Oaks, CA: Corwin.
A research-based book written for practi-
tioners and parents. Many practical tools
for creating homework policies, as well as
information on the purpose and types of
homework.
3. Demerath, P. (2009). Producing success:
The culture of personal advancement in
an American high school. Chicago, IL:
The University of Chicago Press.
In this book, the author spends 4 years in a
Midwestern high school examining the cul-
ture of success and need for advancement.
The author discusses work ethics, negotiat-
ing tactics of students to get ahead and
cheating as a means to the desired
outcome.
4. Zito, N., & McQuillan, P. (2010). ā€œItā€™s not
my faultā€: Using neutralization theory to
understand cheating by middle school stu-
dents. Current Issues in Education, 13.
http://cie.asu.edu/.
This article includes interviews with
faculty and students in a high achieving
competitive middle school and explains
how students ā€˜neutralizeā€™ and justify
cheating behaviors. The author concludes
with suggestions for school faculty on
how to promote value in course work
and eliminate the competitive focus on
grades.
Theoretical Approaches to Understanding and Promoting Academic Integrity
128

More Related Content

Similar to Addressing Academic Dishonesty Among The Highest Achievers

Quantitative Research Proposal College.pdf
Quantitative Research Proposal College.pdfQuantitative Research Proposal College.pdf
Quantitative Research Proposal College.pdf
sdfghj21
Ā 
Michael, There are two major flaws here, the first being that your
Michael, There are two major flaws here, the first being that yourMichael, There are two major flaws here, the first being that your
Michael, There are two major flaws here, the first being that your
DioneWang844
Ā 
Running head RESEARCH PROPOSAL 1RESEARCH PROPOSAL4.docx
Running head RESEARCH PROPOSAL 1RESEARCH PROPOSAL4.docxRunning head RESEARCH PROPOSAL 1RESEARCH PROPOSAL4.docx
Running head RESEARCH PROPOSAL 1RESEARCH PROPOSAL4.docx
toltonkendal
Ā 
Effects of School of Life Foundation Intervention on Grade Advancement, Drop...
 Effects of School of Life Foundation Intervention on Grade Advancement, Drop... Effects of School of Life Foundation Intervention on Grade Advancement, Drop...
Effects of School of Life Foundation Intervention on Grade Advancement, Drop...
Research Journal of Education
Ā 
GLOBAL ETHICS IN HIGHER EDUCATIONGlobal Grads 2
GLOBAL ETHICS IN HIGHER EDUCATIONGlobal Grads 2GLOBAL ETHICS IN HIGHER EDUCATIONGlobal Grads 2
GLOBAL ETHICS IN HIGHER EDUCATIONGlobal Grads 2
MatthewTennant613
Ā 
Institutional Undermatching
Institutional UndermatchingInstitutional Undermatching
Institutional Undermatching
Tyler Guenette
Ā 
Project Individual Reflection Paper And Project Group
Project Individual Reflection Paper And Project GroupProject Individual Reflection Paper And Project Group
Project Individual Reflection Paper And Project Group
Kate Loge
Ā 
University Students Perceptions of PlagiarismAuthor(s).docx
 University Students Perceptions of PlagiarismAuthor(s).docx University Students Perceptions of PlagiarismAuthor(s).docx
University Students Perceptions of PlagiarismAuthor(s).docx
aryan532920
Ā 
Building student trust in online learning environmentsYe D.docx
Building student trust in online learning environmentsYe D.docxBuilding student trust in online learning environmentsYe D.docx
Building student trust in online learning environmentsYe D.docx
hartrobert670
Ā 
AcademicIntegrity-WhatItIsandWhyItMattersfinal.pptx
AcademicIntegrity-WhatItIsandWhyItMattersfinal.pptxAcademicIntegrity-WhatItIsandWhyItMattersfinal.pptx
AcademicIntegrity-WhatItIsandWhyItMattersfinal.pptx
ShotaOzzy
Ā 
Critical Thinking Processby Shawnesty MaysSubmission dat.docx
Critical Thinking Processby Shawnesty MaysSubmission dat.docxCritical Thinking Processby Shawnesty MaysSubmission dat.docx
Critical Thinking Processby Shawnesty MaysSubmission dat.docx
mydrynan
Ā 

Similar to Addressing Academic Dishonesty Among The Highest Achievers (20)

Quantitative Research Proposal College.pdf
Quantitative Research Proposal College.pdfQuantitative Research Proposal College.pdf
Quantitative Research Proposal College.pdf
Ā 
Final ppt presentation
Final ppt presentationFinal ppt presentation
Final ppt presentation
Ā 
Michael, There are two major flaws here, the first being that your
Michael, There are two major flaws here, the first being that yourMichael, There are two major flaws here, the first being that your
Michael, There are two major flaws here, the first being that your
Ā 
Academic Honesty and Online Courses.pdf
Academic Honesty and Online Courses.pdfAcademic Honesty and Online Courses.pdf
Academic Honesty and Online Courses.pdf
Ā 
Running head RESEARCH PROPOSAL 1RESEARCH PROPOSAL4.docx
Running head RESEARCH PROPOSAL 1RESEARCH PROPOSAL4.docxRunning head RESEARCH PROPOSAL 1RESEARCH PROPOSAL4.docx
Running head RESEARCH PROPOSAL 1RESEARCH PROPOSAL4.docx
Ā 
Advancing Cognitive Moral Development A Field Observation Of College Students
Advancing Cognitive Moral Development  A Field Observation Of College StudentsAdvancing Cognitive Moral Development  A Field Observation Of College Students
Advancing Cognitive Moral Development A Field Observation Of College Students
Ā 
Effects of School of Life Foundation Intervention on Grade Advancement, Drop...
 Effects of School of Life Foundation Intervention on Grade Advancement, Drop... Effects of School of Life Foundation Intervention on Grade Advancement, Drop...
Effects of School of Life Foundation Intervention on Grade Advancement, Drop...
Ā 
Lai 615 McBride lit review presentation
Lai 615 McBride lit review presentationLai 615 McBride lit review presentation
Lai 615 McBride lit review presentation
Ā 
GLOBAL ETHICS IN HIGHER EDUCATIONGlobal Grads 2
GLOBAL ETHICS IN HIGHER EDUCATIONGlobal Grads 2GLOBAL ETHICS IN HIGHER EDUCATIONGlobal Grads 2
GLOBAL ETHICS IN HIGHER EDUCATIONGlobal Grads 2
Ā 
Institutional Undermatching
Institutional UndermatchingInstitutional Undermatching
Institutional Undermatching
Ā 
D3160 done
D3160 doneD3160 done
D3160 done
Ā 
Project Individual Reflection Paper And Project Group
Project Individual Reflection Paper And Project GroupProject Individual Reflection Paper And Project Group
Project Individual Reflection Paper And Project Group
Ā 
University Students Perceptions of PlagiarismAuthor(s).docx
 University Students Perceptions of PlagiarismAuthor(s).docx University Students Perceptions of PlagiarismAuthor(s).docx
University Students Perceptions of PlagiarismAuthor(s).docx
Ā 
Academic Cheating Among Youths: A Causal Pathway Model
Academic Cheating Among Youths: A Causal Pathway Model Academic Cheating Among Youths: A Causal Pathway Model
Academic Cheating Among Youths: A Causal Pathway Model
Ā 
Building student trust in online learning environmentsYe D.docx
Building student trust in online learning environmentsYe D.docxBuilding student trust in online learning environmentsYe D.docx
Building student trust in online learning environmentsYe D.docx
Ā 
Academic Self-Concept And Critical Thinking Dispositions Devising A Predicti...
Academic Self-Concept And Critical Thinking Dispositions  Devising A Predicti...Academic Self-Concept And Critical Thinking Dispositions  Devising A Predicti...
Academic Self-Concept And Critical Thinking Dispositions Devising A Predicti...
Ā 
Literature Review
Literature ReviewLiterature Review
Literature Review
Ā 
AcademicIntegrity-WhatItIsandWhyItMattersfinal.pptx
AcademicIntegrity-WhatItIsandWhyItMattersfinal.pptxAcademicIntegrity-WhatItIsandWhyItMattersfinal.pptx
AcademicIntegrity-WhatItIsandWhyItMattersfinal.pptx
Ā 
Wilson jones, linda graduate females focus v6 n1 2011
Wilson jones, linda graduate females focus v6 n1 2011Wilson jones, linda graduate females focus v6 n1 2011
Wilson jones, linda graduate females focus v6 n1 2011
Ā 
Critical Thinking Processby Shawnesty MaysSubmission dat.docx
Critical Thinking Processby Shawnesty MaysSubmission dat.docxCritical Thinking Processby Shawnesty MaysSubmission dat.docx
Critical Thinking Processby Shawnesty MaysSubmission dat.docx
Ā 

More from Zaara Jensen

More from Zaara Jensen (20)

009 High School Vs College Essay Com
009 High School Vs College Essay Com009 High School Vs College Essay Com
009 High School Vs College Essay Com
Ā 
College Essay Quote Essays
College Essay Quote EssaysCollege Essay Quote Essays
College Essay Quote Essays
Ā 
Write My Academic Essay. Why Is It Important To Write
Write My Academic Essay. Why Is It Important To WriteWrite My Academic Essay. Why Is It Important To Write
Write My Academic Essay. Why Is It Important To Write
Ā 
Bol.Com A Manual For Writers Of Term Papers, Th
Bol.Com A Manual For Writers Of Term Papers, ThBol.Com A Manual For Writers Of Term Papers, Th
Bol.Com A Manual For Writers Of Term Papers, Th
Ā 
Classification Essays Mr Hall S
Classification Essays Mr Hall SClassification Essays Mr Hall S
Classification Essays Mr Hall S
Ā 
Fabulous How To Write A Good Conclusion For An Analysis Ess
Fabulous How To Write A Good Conclusion For An Analysis EssFabulous How To Write A Good Conclusion For An Analysis Ess
Fabulous How To Write A Good Conclusion For An Analysis Ess
Ā 
Can Money Buy Happiness Essay Essay On Can Mo
Can Money Buy Happiness Essay Essay On Can MoCan Money Buy Happiness Essay Essay On Can Mo
Can Money Buy Happiness Essay Essay On Can Mo
Ā 
Structure Body Paragraphs, Essay, T
Structure Body Paragraphs, Essay, TStructure Body Paragraphs, Essay, T
Structure Body Paragraphs, Essay, T
Ā 
10 Proven Steps How To Write A Reflection On An Ar
10 Proven Steps How To Write A Reflection On An Ar10 Proven Steps How To Write A Reflection On An Ar
10 Proven Steps How To Write A Reflection On An Ar
Ā 
Psychology Research Paper Sample Apa Format.
Psychology Research Paper Sample Apa Format.Psychology Research Paper Sample Apa Format.
Psychology Research Paper Sample Apa Format.
Ā 
Write In The Rain Paper - Cool Product Reviews, Deals, And Buying
Write In The Rain Paper - Cool Product Reviews, Deals, And BuyingWrite In The Rain Paper - Cool Product Reviews, Deals, And Buying
Write In The Rain Paper - Cool Product Reviews, Deals, And Buying
Ā 
Caspa Letter Of Recommendation Exampl
Caspa Letter Of Recommendation ExamplCaspa Letter Of Recommendation Exampl
Caspa Letter Of Recommendation Exampl
Ā 
Best Paper Writing Website, Any Good
Best Paper Writing Website, Any GoodBest Paper Writing Website, Any Good
Best Paper Writing Website, Any Good
Ā 
024 Essay Example Inspirational Essays Popjp
024 Essay Example Inspirational Essays Popjp024 Essay Example Inspirational Essays Popjp
024 Essay Example Inspirational Essays Popjp
Ā 
Specialty Paper Card Stock, Vellums Invitation Paper
Specialty Paper Card Stock, Vellums Invitation PaperSpecialty Paper Card Stock, Vellums Invitation Paper
Specialty Paper Card Stock, Vellums Invitation Paper
Ā 
White Papers - White Paper Writing Service Cartoozo
White Papers - White Paper Writing Service CartoozoWhite Papers - White Paper Writing Service Cartoozo
White Papers - White Paper Writing Service Cartoozo
Ā 
How To Write An Observation Essay Writing Steps, Sam
How To Write An Observation Essay Writing Steps, SamHow To Write An Observation Essay Writing Steps, Sam
How To Write An Observation Essay Writing Steps, Sam
Ā 
How To WriteTheRationaleEssay
How To WriteTheRationaleEssayHow To WriteTheRationaleEssay
How To WriteTheRationaleEssay
Ā 
Fall Maple And Oak Leaf Lined Writing Paper Zazzle
Fall Maple And Oak Leaf Lined Writing Paper ZazzleFall Maple And Oak Leaf Lined Writing Paper Zazzle
Fall Maple And Oak Leaf Lined Writing Paper Zazzle
Ā 
Essay On My Favourite Author My Favourite
Essay On My Favourite Author My FavouriteEssay On My Favourite Author My Favourite
Essay On My Favourite Author My Favourite
Ā 

Recently uploaded

Making and Justifying Mathematical Decisions.pdf
Making and Justifying Mathematical Decisions.pdfMaking and Justifying Mathematical Decisions.pdf
Making and Justifying Mathematical Decisions.pdf
Chris Hunter
Ā 
The basics of sentences session 3pptx.pptx
The basics of sentences session 3pptx.pptxThe basics of sentences session 3pptx.pptx
The basics of sentences session 3pptx.pptx
heathfieldcps1
Ā 
Beyond the EU: DORA and NIS 2 Directive's Global Impact
Beyond the EU: DORA and NIS 2 Directive's Global ImpactBeyond the EU: DORA and NIS 2 Directive's Global Impact
Beyond the EU: DORA and NIS 2 Directive's Global Impact
PECB
Ā 
1029-Danh muc Sach Giao Khoa khoi 6.pdf
1029-Danh muc Sach Giao Khoa khoi  6.pdf1029-Danh muc Sach Giao Khoa khoi  6.pdf
1029-Danh muc Sach Giao Khoa khoi 6.pdf
QucHHunhnh
Ā 
1029 - Danh muc Sach Giao Khoa 10 . pdf
1029 -  Danh muc Sach Giao Khoa 10 . pdf1029 -  Danh muc Sach Giao Khoa 10 . pdf
1029 - Danh muc Sach Giao Khoa 10 . pdf
QucHHunhnh
Ā 
Activity 01 - Artificial Culture (1).pdf
Activity 01 - Artificial Culture (1).pdfActivity 01 - Artificial Culture (1).pdf
Activity 01 - Artificial Culture (1).pdf
ciinovamais
Ā 
The basics of sentences session 2pptx copy.pptx
The basics of sentences session 2pptx copy.pptxThe basics of sentences session 2pptx copy.pptx
The basics of sentences session 2pptx copy.pptx
heathfieldcps1
Ā 

Recently uploaded (20)

Making and Justifying Mathematical Decisions.pdf
Making and Justifying Mathematical Decisions.pdfMaking and Justifying Mathematical Decisions.pdf
Making and Justifying Mathematical Decisions.pdf
Ā 
The basics of sentences session 3pptx.pptx
The basics of sentences session 3pptx.pptxThe basics of sentences session 3pptx.pptx
The basics of sentences session 3pptx.pptx
Ā 
Beyond the EU: DORA and NIS 2 Directive's Global Impact
Beyond the EU: DORA and NIS 2 Directive's Global ImpactBeyond the EU: DORA and NIS 2 Directive's Global Impact
Beyond the EU: DORA and NIS 2 Directive's Global Impact
Ā 
Ecological Succession. ( ECOSYSTEM, B. Pharmacy, 1st Year, Sem-II, Environmen...
Ecological Succession. ( ECOSYSTEM, B. Pharmacy, 1st Year, Sem-II, Environmen...Ecological Succession. ( ECOSYSTEM, B. Pharmacy, 1st Year, Sem-II, Environmen...
Ecological Succession. ( ECOSYSTEM, B. Pharmacy, 1st Year, Sem-II, Environmen...
Ā 
ComPTIA Overview | Comptia Security+ Book SY0-701
ComPTIA Overview | Comptia Security+ Book SY0-701ComPTIA Overview | Comptia Security+ Book SY0-701
ComPTIA Overview | Comptia Security+ Book SY0-701
Ā 
microwave assisted reaction. General introduction
microwave assisted reaction. General introductionmicrowave assisted reaction. General introduction
microwave assisted reaction. General introduction
Ā 
1029-Danh muc Sach Giao Khoa khoi 6.pdf
1029-Danh muc Sach Giao Khoa khoi  6.pdf1029-Danh muc Sach Giao Khoa khoi  6.pdf
1029-Danh muc Sach Giao Khoa khoi 6.pdf
Ā 
ICT role in 21st century education and it's challenges.
ICT role in 21st century education and it's challenges.ICT role in 21st century education and it's challenges.
ICT role in 21st century education and it's challenges.
Ā 
Measures of Central Tendency: Mean, Median and Mode
Measures of Central Tendency: Mean, Median and ModeMeasures of Central Tendency: Mean, Median and Mode
Measures of Central Tendency: Mean, Median and Mode
Ā 
Asian American Pacific Islander Month DDSD 2024.pptx
Asian American Pacific Islander Month DDSD 2024.pptxAsian American Pacific Islander Month DDSD 2024.pptx
Asian American Pacific Islander Month DDSD 2024.pptx
Ā 
Energy Resources. ( B. Pharmacy, 1st Year, Sem-II) Natural Resources
Energy Resources. ( B. Pharmacy, 1st Year, Sem-II) Natural ResourcesEnergy Resources. ( B. Pharmacy, 1st Year, Sem-II) Natural Resources
Energy Resources. ( B. Pharmacy, 1st Year, Sem-II) Natural Resources
Ā 
1029 - Danh muc Sach Giao Khoa 10 . pdf
1029 -  Danh muc Sach Giao Khoa 10 . pdf1029 -  Danh muc Sach Giao Khoa 10 . pdf
1029 - Danh muc Sach Giao Khoa 10 . pdf
Ā 
Class 11th Physics NEET formula sheet pdf
Class 11th Physics NEET formula sheet pdfClass 11th Physics NEET formula sheet pdf
Class 11th Physics NEET formula sheet pdf
Ā 
Tį»”NG ƔN Tįŗ¬P THI VƀO Lį»šP 10 MƔN TIįŗ¾NG ANH NĂM Hį»ŒC 2023 - 2024 CƓ ĐƁP ƁN (NGį»® Ƃ...
Tį»”NG ƔN Tįŗ¬P THI VƀO Lį»šP 10 MƔN TIįŗ¾NG ANH NĂM Hį»ŒC 2023 - 2024 CƓ ĐƁP ƁN (NGį»® Ƃ...Tį»”NG ƔN Tįŗ¬P THI VƀO Lį»šP 10 MƔN TIįŗ¾NG ANH NĂM Hį»ŒC 2023 - 2024 CƓ ĐƁP ƁN (NGį»® Ƃ...
Tį»”NG ƔN Tįŗ¬P THI VƀO Lį»šP 10 MƔN TIįŗ¾NG ANH NĂM Hį»ŒC 2023 - 2024 CƓ ĐƁP ƁN (NGį»® Ƃ...
Ā 
Holdier Curriculum Vitae (April 2024).pdf
Holdier Curriculum Vitae (April 2024).pdfHoldier Curriculum Vitae (April 2024).pdf
Holdier Curriculum Vitae (April 2024).pdf
Ā 
Activity 01 - Artificial Culture (1).pdf
Activity 01 - Artificial Culture (1).pdfActivity 01 - Artificial Culture (1).pdf
Activity 01 - Artificial Culture (1).pdf
Ā 
Nutritional Needs Presentation - HLTH 104
Nutritional Needs Presentation - HLTH 104Nutritional Needs Presentation - HLTH 104
Nutritional Needs Presentation - HLTH 104
Ā 
The basics of sentences session 2pptx copy.pptx
The basics of sentences session 2pptx copy.pptxThe basics of sentences session 2pptx copy.pptx
The basics of sentences session 2pptx copy.pptx
Ā 
Web & Social Media Analytics Previous Year Question Paper.pdf
Web & Social Media Analytics Previous Year Question Paper.pdfWeb & Social Media Analytics Previous Year Question Paper.pdf
Web & Social Media Analytics Previous Year Question Paper.pdf
Ā 
Key note speaker Neum_Admir Softic_ENG.pdf
Key note speaker Neum_Admir Softic_ENG.pdfKey note speaker Neum_Admir Softic_ENG.pdf
Key note speaker Neum_Admir Softic_ENG.pdf
Ā 

Addressing Academic Dishonesty Among The Highest Achievers

  • 1. Full Terms & Conditions of access and use can be found at http://www.tandfonline.com/action/journalInformation?journalCode=htip20 Download by: [The UC San Diego Library] Date: 03 May 2017, At: 00:04 Theory Into Practice ISSN: 0040-5841 (Print) 1543-0421 (Online) Journal homepage: http://www.tandfonline.com/loi/htip20 Addressing Academic Dishonesty Among the Highest Achievers Angela D. Miller, Tamera B. Murdock & Morgan M. Grotewiel To cite this article: Angela D. Miller, Tamera B. Murdock & Morgan M. Grotewiel (2017) Addressing Academic Dishonesty Among the Highest Achievers, Theory Into Practice, 56:2, 121-128, DOI: 10.1080/00405841.2017.1283574 To link to this article: http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/00405841.2017.1283574 Published online: 08 Mar 2017. Submit your article to this journal Article views: 34 View related articles View Crossmark data
  • 2. Angela D. Miller Tamera B. Murdock Morgan M. Grotewiel Addressing Academic Dishonesty Among the Highest Achievers Although research shows that higher-achieving students report engaging in cheating behaviors less frequently than lower-achieving students, the cheating rates among this population are still star- tling. Certain aspects of the context of being a high-achieving student support academic dishon- esty. We investigate integrity among the highest achievers using a motivational framework, ļ¬rst examining why these students feel the need to cheat. We discuss personal standards of perfor- mance, social comparison and competition, pres- sure to succeed, and these studentsā€™ ability to rationalize cheating behaviors. Finally, we suggest what can be done to combat cheating among high achieving students, including thinking about approaches to pedagogy and assessment, provid- ing clarity and consequences for cheating, and considering the culture of high achievers. Academic dishonesty is any deceitful or unfair act intended to produce a more desirable outcome on an exam, paper, homework assign- ment, or other assessment of learning. The perva- siveness of cheating from middle school through graduate education has stimulated many discus- sions of the factors that support academic dishon- esty (for reviews, see Murdock & Anderman, 2006; Whitley, 1998). Although cheating is sometimes viewed as a victimless crime, it has numerous consequences for others. Cheating interferes with teachersā€™ ability to accurately assess learning and noncheating students suffer from the potential elevation of the normative achievement bar. Being compared to others with inļ¬‚ated grades might affect studentsā€™ self- conļ¬dence and create a true competitive disad- vantage for grade-based awards (i.e., admission to college, receipt of an award). Finally, students who witness unchecked cheating in a given class may develop the belief that cheating is normative and thereby be more inclined to cheat themselves (Oā€™Rourke et al., 2010). In sum, there are numer- ous reasons for educators to care about academic dishonesty. Angela D. Miller, Ph.D., is at George Mason University. Tamera B. Murdock, Ph.D., is at the University of Missouri-Kansas City. Morgan M. Grotewiel, M.A., is at the University of Missouri-Kansas City. Correspondence should be addressed to Angela D. Miller, Ph.D., George Mason University, West Building, Room 2105, 4400 University Drive MS 6D2, Fairfax, VA 22030. E-mail: amille35@gmu.edu Theory Into Practice, 56:121ā€“128, 2017 Copyright Ā© The College of Education and Human Ecology, The Ohio State University ISSN: 0040-5841 print/1543-0421 online DOI: 10.1080/00405841.2017.1283574 121
  • 3. Conceptual Framework We discuss cheating among high-achieving students using a motivational framework offered by Murdock and Anderman (2006). They pro- posed that cheating in any speciļ¬c context can be predicted by studentsā€™ answers to three questions: ā— What is my purpose here? ā— Can I do this? and ā— What are the costs of cheating? Studentsā€™ motivation or purpose when they approach their academic work is often viewed as some combination of their learning goals, or a desire to learn, understand, and master the material and their performance goals, or their desire to win, outperform others, demonstrate aptitude, or earn a high grade (Dweck & Leggett, 1988). Students with strong learning goals are less likely to cheat, as compared to students who see less relevance to what they are learning and are simply aiming to get a high grade (Anderman, Griesinger & Westerļ¬eld, 1998). Beyond purpose, studentsā€™ evaluation of their own capabilities drives the decision to cheat. These capabilities are most often assessed as studentsā€™ self-efļ¬cacy or success expectations (Bandura, 1997). They are inļ¬‚uenced by their own academic performance history, the social comparisons they make, the pedagogical skills of the teacher, the workload students carry, and a teacherā€™s emphasis on individual growth versus social comparisons. Students cheat more fre- quently when their expectancies for success or academic self-beliefs are low (Anderman, et al., 1998). Finally, students estimate the perceived costs of cheating using some form of risk-beneļ¬t analysis (Rettinger, 2007). If the perceived costs of dishonesty outweigh the perceived gain, cheating is less likely to be a desirable strategy thus students become less likely to cheat as the perceived risk of detection and punishment rise. Cheating also requires stu- dents to reconcile the costs to their self- concept by violating the norms of what a good person does. This ability to neutralize, justify, or explain away cheating by blaming others for it (e.g., I have a bad teacher), fram- ing the behavior as typical (e.g., everyone else cheats), and/or minimizing the behavior (e.g., it hurts no one) reduces the threat to oneā€™s self- concept and increases the likelihood of dishon- esty (Murdock & Stephens, 2007). Why Care About Cheating Among High-Achieving Students? Research consistently ļ¬nds that higher- achieving students report fewer cheating beha- viors compared to their lower achieving peers, which may explain the relative lack of empirical attention to dishonesty in this speciļ¬c population. However, high-achieving middle and high school students are by no means immune from cheating. Eighty percent of students attending Stuyvesant High School in New York City, a highly compe- titive magnet school for the best and brightest, admitted to cheating according to an article in the New York Times (Yess, 2012). A survey of high school students (age 14 to 18) from Advanced Placement (AP) or Honors Math and/or Science classes revealed high rates of cheating behaviors on exams (approximately 75%) and even higher rates of cheating on homework (Geddes, 2011). Finally, a 2005 review of the literature on medi- cal school students reports high rates of academic cheating (25%) and even higher rates of witnes- sing cheating (66%; Dyrbye, Thomas, & Shanafelt, 2005). High-achieving students have the human capi- tal to ascend to positions in society where they have inļ¬‚uence over others both directly (e.g., as supervisors, leaders, etc.) and indirectly (e.g., conducting and sharing research, contributing to policy, donating money to causes and candi- dates). Evidence suggests that people who are dishonest in their academic careers are more apt to continue a pattern of dishonesty in their work lives (Nonis & Smith, 2001), making the integrity of these students a concern for us all. Theoretical Approaches to Understanding and Promoting Academic Integrity 122
  • 4. Why Do High Achievers Cheat? Personal Standard of Performance Although students in academically competi- tive programs may have higher academic self- efļ¬cacy than students in less competitive pro- grams, they also report more stress than their peers (Suldo, Shaunessy, & Hardesty, 2008). Worry has been linked to increased cheating (Anderman et al., 1998) and high-achieving high school students identify pressure for grades as one of their top reasons for cheating (Geddes, 2011). The pressure created from studentsā€™ perfor- mance standards might be augmented by their understanding of intelligence. Dweckā€™s (2006) widely cited Mindset theory delineates between students who see intelligence as something that improves with learning, termed growth mindsets, and those who believe that the intellect they are born with cannot be modiļ¬ed, termed a ā€œļ¬xedā€ mindset. A ļ¬xed mindset is associated with nega- tive emotions such as shame after feedback and maladaptive behaviors including academic cheat- ing (Dweck & Master, 2009). A substantial num- ber of gifted high school students hold entity or ļ¬xed views of intelligence and even more of these same students see giftedness as something that is not malleable (Makel, Snyder, Chandler, Malone, & Puttallaz, 2015). Similarly, students at a highly competitive university were more apt to hold entity views of intelligence if they were identiļ¬ed as gifted during high school, and the higher their own aptitude, the more likely they were to hold these beliefs (Snyder, Barger, Wormington, Schwartz-Bloom, & Linnenbrink- Garcia, 2013). Given the centrality of academic accomplishment to the self-concept of high achieving students, they might be particularly reactive when threatened with performance out- comes that donā€™t meet their high standards. In other words, the risk of getting low grades may pose such a grave threat to these studentsā€™ being seen as smart or gifted that cheating may be seen as relatively less threatening to their self-concept, especially if it is the norm within their academic culture. Ever-Increasing Social Comparisons and Competition As high achievers progress through the educa- tional pipeline, they tend to be concentrated into increasingly homogenous classes in terms of their ability and accomplishments, with ever- increasing demands and higher levels of social comparison. Through the lens of our theoretical framework, these contexts might create an increased focus on performance, and increase the perceived normativeness of dishonesty, thereby making cheating seem more acceptable. One study explicitly looked at the role of achievement context versus individual achieve- ment as a contributor to cheating by comparing cheating at public high schools that were in the top, second, third, and fourth quartiles in terms of school-level performance as measured by state- wide assessments (Brandes, 1986). A students at the highest achieving schools cheated less often than A students at the lowest-achieving schools; however, students with GPAs of B+ or below were more likely to cheat if they attended a higher-achieving school, suggesting the inļ¬‚uence of local achievement norms on cheating patterns. It is likely that lower achieving students in high performing schools feel the most pressure to suc- ceed because there is the most discrepancy between their performance and the schoolā€™s over- all level of achievement, ultimately making them the most likely to cheat. Other evidence for the potentially deleterious inļ¬‚uence of competition or other-based compar- isons comes from the literature on perfectionism. Perfectionism, or critically evaluating oneself against high standards, has been found to be quite common among high-ability students (Neumeister, 2004). However, while some gifted students have more self-oriented perfectionism, setting high standards for themselves, socially- oriented perfectionists strive to meet the per- ceived standards of others. Self-oriented perfec- tionists tend to be driven by a desire for mastery and approach goals; they compete with their own standards, display adaptive behaviors such as effort and persistence and have generally positive affect in achievement settings. Those with Miller, Murdock, Grotewiel Academic Dishonesty Among High Achievers 123
  • 5. socially oriented perfectionism are more prone to worry and anxiety, compete with their peers, give up easily, and, are more apt to believe that cheat- ing is justiļ¬able (Bong, Hwang, Noh, & Kim, 2014; Neumeister, 2004). Classrooms that stu- dents perceive as emphasizing social compari- sons and competition also are ones in which rates of academic cheating are higher (Anderman & Midgely, 2004). Pressures to ā€œDo It Allā€ High-achieving high school students cite heavy workloads (68%) and multiple tests being scheduled on the same day (60%) as two of the top reasons that they cheat (Geddes, 2011). On top of this, high achieving students at all levels talk about the demands to carry difļ¬cult course loads while engaging in activities that distinguish them from other high achievers when they go to compete at the ā€œnext levelā€ (Geddes, 2011). Although students may have the efļ¬cacy (and skills) for any of these tasks in isolation, the cumulative burden creates enormous stress and encourages students to seek shortcuts to accom- plish all of their goals. This overload has been explicitly examined among medical students, where high rates of burnout are common: Although burnout was not linked to academic cheating per se, it did predict unprofessional con- duct in the context of clinical work (Dyrbye et al., 2010). Other reasons for cheating listed by high-achieving students in Geddesā€™ (2011) study include ā€œunrealisticā€ workloads, helping a friend, and ā€œloyalty to a group.ā€ Similar views were echoed by the Stuyvesant students who described the environment as one in which stu- dents worked together at cheating to beat what they perceived to be the demands being placed on them (Yess, 2012). Increased Rationalization High-achieving studentsā€™ well-developed cog- nitive abilities might translate into an increased ability to rationalize or justify cheating; these atti- tudes are consistently one of the strongest predic- tors of dishonesty (Murdock & Anderman, 2006; Whitley, 1998). Cheating is regularly rationalized by externalizing the blame onto the teacher or situation, and these externalizations increase when students see the pedagogy as poor, the tea- cher as uncaring, or the classroom as focused on performance versus mastery goals (Murdock, Miller, & Kohlhardt, 2004; Murdock & Stephens, 2007). The enormity of the demands that many high-achieving students feel might pro- vide an easy justiļ¬cation for dishonest behavior. For example, Geddes (2011) argued that many high achieving students adopt an us versus them mentality that pits the students against the teacher. This mentality may help students justify cheating. Cultural Pressure High-achieving students have a status to main- tain. They want to get the best grades, get into the best colleges and universities, and live up to parental, teacher, and peer expectations. Galloway (2012) interviewed students in 10 high- achieving, advantaged high schools where there are many honors and AP courses and a majority of the graduating students attend 4-year colleges. She found that these students feel forced to cheat because of the pressure, workload, and culture of their school communities. Students described their school experience as being corrupted by their environment. These students viewed cheat- ing as a compromise for a good reason: They saw their situation as ā€œcheat or be cheatedā€ (p. 393). Cheating for students in these contexts is a way to sustain their status in a community that cele- brates their success. What Must Change? There are many obvious steps to curbing cheating by increasing the likelihood of detec- tion, including changing seating arrangements, creating multiple versions of exams, limiting access to technology during testing, and using antiplagiarism software to ļ¬ght the proliferation of the electronic dimension to cheating. Ultimately, students learn increasingly sophisti- cated ways to cheat and the myriad of factors Theoretical Approaches to Understanding and Promoting Academic Integrity 124
  • 6. that create a culture of cheating are not addressed. Attitudes must change as well, such that students see intrinsic value in completing honest work. These changes require creating a culture where demands are reasonable, learning is valued over performance, and cheating is treated as unacceptable. In the following we look at several concrete steps that can help promote these values. Reasonable Demands High-achieving students are often taking sev- eral challenging classes at the same time, taxing studentsā€™ limits to get all of their work done. At the school and classroom level, this might be addressed in several ways. First, school adminis- ters can encourage collaboration among teachers of advanced classes so that large assignments and exams are spaced across days. Being explicit about this spacing and the reasons for this spa- cing with students will also communicate to them an empathy for the workload they are carrying and a commitment to good pedagogy. Thus, these practices make the work more doable; the like- lihood of cheating should be lessened because of the more favorable way that teachers will be viewed. In a similar vein, teachers might strive to limit out-of-classroom work (e.g., homework) to a rea- sonable amount across classes. Students from 10 high-achieving high schools reported completing an average over 3 hr of homework each day (Cooper, Robinson, & Patall, 2006). Although students doing more homework were rated as more engaged in school, they were also more stressed and reported having a harder time balan- cing the rest of their lives. Homework require- ments should promote learning: Lots of busy work creates a focus on completion, rather than understanding (for more detail on best homework practices see, Cooper, 2007). Finally, it is never too early to explicitly teach students self-regulation and time management strategies so that they can more effectively man- age their academic lives, including learning to use a planner to map out shorter and longer periods of time, breaking tasks down into manageable parts and learning how to trouble- shoot a difļ¬cult problem or assignment by using available resources and/or reaching out for help. Teaching parents how to offer scaffolding versus providing answers will also increase studentsā€™ ability to complete tasks on their own, rather than be tempted to take shortcuts (see Cooper et al., 2006). Promoting Learning Versus Performance Self-reported cheating has been correlated with a fear of failure (Schab, 1991), test anxiety (Malinowski & Smith, 1985), and worry over performance (Anderman, et al., 1998) all of which increase in classrooms where there is an emphasis on normative performance. Moreover, in an era of high-stakes testing, it is easy to be consumed by the importance and implications of test performance instead of teaching for the pur- poses of conceptual understanding. Students fol- low their teachersā€™ lead and if the focus of each lesson is to learn the content for the next big test, then that impending test becomes the goal, rather than learning the course content. Results become most highly valued. When middle and high school students read scenarios about classrooms where the teacher was focused on grades and test scores rather than mastery of concepts, they rated these classrooms as those where cheating is the most likely to occur (Murdock et al., 2004). These same students also noted that cheating was less likely to occur in settings where it was evident that the teacher cared about student pro- gress and learning, and emphasized effort as a means to better understanding. Although research suggests that all students fair better in classrooms that are mastery oriented, achieving a mastery focus may be the more difļ¬cult with highest achieving students, who often have multiple sources of achievement pressure. Providing formative assessment oppor- tunities is one suggestion for achieving this goal. Nonpunitive miniassessments, practice exams that are reviewed in class, homework assign- ments that are reviewed for accuracy and can be corrected until the concepts are mastered and clear alignment of formative and summative Miller, Murdock, Grotewiel Academic Dishonesty Among High Achievers 125
  • 7. assessments are some concrete ways to achieve this. Increasingly, teachers at all levels can rely on technology to aid them with these practices: many textbooks publishers provide online home- work systems that provide students with the opportunity to redo homework multiple times and provide them with explanations to help them achieve mastery. Course shells such as Blackboard allow posting of timed assessments and immediate feedback for students. Clarity and Consequences Academic dishonesty is commonly addressed administratively with honor code systems at either the classroom or institutional level. Typically, these codes clearly delineate what is considered honest and dishonest, require students to formally attest that they will behave in an honorable manner, and, many times, require students to agree to create a culture of honesty in their school by reporting any incident of peer transgression. Although some research has suggested that these honor codes might improve student academic integrity (Konheim-Kalkstein, 2006; McCabe & Pavela, 2000), a recent analysis at the college level suggest that the effects of these codes are declining, because students value grades more than an abstract moral standard, and they see it as the job of the school/teacher to detect and deal with cheating (Vandehey, Diekhoff, & LaBeff, 2007). At a basic level, schools must provide informa- tion on academic integrity and speciļ¬c deļ¬nitions of what is considered cheating as students often do not understand what constitutes academic dishon- esty (Galloway, 2012). In addition, there also needs to be a culture of integrity (Schwartz, Tatum, & Hageman, 2013) and clear conse- quences for cheating (Galloway, 2012). Gallowayā€™s interviews with high school students and staff members found that many teachers donā€™t follow the honor code, and that some staff mem- bers even condone cheating as a high school norm. In a school lacking academic integrity as a cultural value, students can more easily justify cheating and develop neutralizing attitudes toward cheating (Rettinger & Kramer, 2009). There must be dis- cussion, and schools must model integrity and open dialog with students and their parents. Given the repeated ļ¬nding that students cheat less when they feel that teachers treat them with fairness and respect (Murdock et al., 2004), dia- logs with students that are explicit about expecta- tions and followed up with clear, consistent action are likely to mitigate cheating. In short, high-achieving students are experien- cing an early entrance into the highly competi- tive, winner takes all society that Callahan described in his book The Cheating Culture (2004). The growing gap separating those at the top means increases in the pressures that parents and their children feel to be successful and not be left behind. Although there is no magical solution for addressing dishonesty in this climate of win- ner takes all, particularly for youth who are on the fast track to be the winners, cheating among this group might be reduced if teachers and school administrators work to make assessment clear, fair, and consistent; model the value of learning; minimize comparisons among students; teach students prioritization and regulation skills; and communicate their empathy for high demands and worries associated with the stu- dentsā€™ pressured lives. References Anderman, E. M., Griesinger, T., & Westerļ¬eld, G. (1998). Motivation and cheating during early adolescence. Journal of Educational Psychology, 90, 84ā€“93. doi:10.1037/0022-0663.90.1.84 Anderman, E. M., & Midgley, C. (2004). Changes in self-reported academic cheating across the transi- tion from middle school to high school. Contemporary Educational Psychology, 29, 499ā€“517. doi:10.1016/j.cedpsych.2004.02.002 Bandura, A. (1997). Self-efļ¬cacy: The exercise of con- trol. New York, NY: Freeman. Bong, M. (2008). Effects of parentā€“child relationships and classroom goal structures on motivation, help- seeking avoidance, and cheating. Journal of Experimental Education, 76, 191ā€“217. doi:10.3200/JEXE.76.2.191-217 Theoretical Approaches to Understanding and Promoting Academic Integrity 126
  • 8. Bong, M., Hwang, A., Noh, A., & Kim, S. I. (2014). Perfectionism and motivation of adolescents in aca- demic contexts. Journal of Educational Psychology, 106, 711ā€“729. doi:10.1037/a0035836 Brandes, B. (1986). Academic honesty: A special study of California students. Sacramento, CA: California State Department of Education, Bureau of Publications. Callahan, D. (2004). The cheating culture: Why more Americans are doing wrong to get ahead. Orlando, FL: Harcourt. Cooper, H., Robinson, J. C., & Patall, E. A. (2006). Does homework improve academic achievement? A synthesis of research, 1987ā€“2003. Review of Educational Research, 76, 1ā€“62. doi: 10.3102/ 00346543076001001 Cooper, H. M. (2007). The battle over homework: Common ground for administrators, teachers, and parents (3rd ed.). Thousand Oaks, CA: Corwin Press. Dweck, C. (2006). Mindset: The new psychology of success. New York, NY: Random House. Dweck, C. S., & Leggett, E. L. (1988). A social-cognitive approach to motivation and personality. Psychological Review, 95, 256ā€“270. doi:10.1037/0033-295X.95.2.256 Dweck, C. S., & Master, A. (2009). Self-theories and motivation: Studentsā€™ beliefs about intelligence. In K. R. Wentzel & A. Wigļ¬eld (Eds.), Handbook of motivation at school (pp. 123ā€“140). New York, NY: Routledge. Dyrbye, L. N., Thomas, M. R., & Shanafelt, T. D. (2005). Medical student distress: causes, conse- quences, and proposed solutions. Mayo Clinic Proceedings, 80, 1613ā€“1622. doi:10.4065/ 80.12.1613 Galloway, M. K. (2012). Cheating in advantaged high schools: Prevalence, justiļ¬cations, and possibilities for change. Ethics & Behavior, 22, 378ā€“399. doi:10.1080/10508422.2012.679143 Geddes, K. A. (2011). Academic dishonesty among gifted and high-achieving students. Gifted Child Today, 34, 50ā€“56. doi:10.1177/107621751103400214 Konheim-Kalkstein, Y. L. (2006). Use of a classroom honor code in higher education. Journal of Credibility Assessment and Witness Psychology, 7, 169ā€“179. Makel, M. C., Snyder, K.E., Chandler, C., Malone, P.S., & Puttallaz, M. (2015). Gifted studentsā€™ impli- cit beliefs about intelligence and giftedness. Gifted Child Quarterly, 59, 203ā€“212. doi:10.1177/ 0016986215599057 Malinowski, C. I., & Smith, C. P. (1985). Moral rea- soning and moral conduct: An investigation prompted by Kohlbergā€™s theory. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 49, 1016ā€“1027. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.49.4.1016 McCabe, D. L., & Pavela, G. (2000). Some good news about academic integrity. Change, 33, 32ā€“38. doi:10.1080/00091380009605738 Murdock, T. B., & Anderman, E. M. (2006). Motivational perspectives on student cheating: Toward an integrated model of academic dishonesty. Educational Psychologist, 41, 129ā€“145. doi:10.1207/s15326985ep4103_1 Murdock, T. B., Miller, A., & Kohlhardt, J. (2004). Effects of classroom context variables on high school studentsā€™ judgments of the acceptability and likelihood of cheating. Journal of Educational Psychology, 96, 765ā€“777. doi:http:10.1037/0022- 0663.96.4.765 Murdock, T. B., & Stephens, J. B. (2007). Is cheating wrong? Studentsā€™ reasoning about academic dis- honesty. In E. A. Anderman & T. B. Murdock (Eds.), The psychology of academic cheating (pp. 229ā€“251). San Diego, CA: Elsevier. Neumeister, K. L. S. (2004). Understanding the relation- ship between perfectionism and achievement motiva- tion in gifted college students. Gifted Child Quarterly, 48, 219ā€“231. doi:10.1177/001698620404800306 Nonis, S., & Swift, C. O. (2001). An examination of the relationship between academic dishonesty and workplace dishonesty: A multicampus investigation. Journal of Education for Business, 77(2), 69ā€“77. doi:10.1080/08832320109599052 Oā€™Rourke, J., Barnes, J., Deaton, A., Fulks, K., Ryan, K., & Rettinger, D. A. (2010). Imitation is the sincerest form of cheating: The inļ¬‚uence of direct knowledge and attitudes on academic dishonesty. Ethics & Behavior, 20, 47ā€“64. doi:10.1080/ 10508420903482616 Rettinger, D. 2007. Applying decision theory to aca- demic integrity decisions. In E. Anderman & T. Murdock (Eds.), Psychology of academic cheating (pp. 141ā€“167). San Diego, CA: Elsevier. Rettinger, D. A., & Kramer, Y. (2009). Situational and personal causes of student cheating. Research in Higher Education, 50, 293ā€“313. doi:10.1007/ s11162-008-9116-5 Schab, F. (1991). Schooling without learning: Thirty years of cheating in high school. Adolescence, 26, 839ā€“847. Schwartz, B. M., Tatum, H. E., & Hageman, M. C. (2013). College studentsā€™ perceptions of and responses to cheating at traditional, modiļ¬ed, and Miller, Murdock, Grotewiel Academic Dishonesty Among High Achievers 127
  • 9. non-honor system institutions. Ethics & Behavior, 23, 463ā€“476. doi:10.1080/10508422.2013.814538 Snyder, K. E., Barger, M., Wormington, S.V., Schwartz-Bloom, R., & Linnenbrink-Garcia, L. (2013). Identiļ¬cation as gifted and implicit beliefs about intelligence: An examination of potential moderators. Journal of Advanced Academics, 24, 242ā€“258. doi:10.1177/1932202X13507971. Suldo, S. M., Shaunessy, E., & Hardesty, R. (2008). Relationships among stress, coping, and mental health in highā€achieving high school students. Psychology in the Schools, 45, 273ā€“290. doi:10.1002/pits.20300 Vandehey, M. A. & Diekhoff, G. & LaBeff, E. (2007). College cheating: A twenty-year follow-up and the addition of an honor code. Journal of College Student Development, 48, 468ā€“480 doi:10.1353/ csd.2007.0043. Whitley, B. E. (1998). Factors associated with cheating among college students: A review. Research in Higher Education, 29, 235ā€“274. doi:10.1023/ a:1018724900565 Yess, V. (2012, September 25). Stuyvesant students describe the how and why of cheating. New York Times. Retrieved from. http://www.nytimes.com/ 2012/09/26/education/stuyvesant-high-school- students-describe-rationale-for-cheating.html Additional Resources 1. Anderman, E. A., & Murdock, T. B. (Eds.). (2006). Psychology of academic cheating. San Diego, CA: Elsevier. This book is a research-based examination of cheating: Who cheats and why? How do they cheat? What are the consequences? What are the ways of stopping it before it starts? Individual personality variables are considered, as well as contextual correlates to cheating. 2. Cooper, H. M. (2007). Battle over home- work: Common ground for administrators, teachers, and parents (3rd ed.) Thousand Oaks, CA: Corwin. A research-based book written for practi- tioners and parents. Many practical tools for creating homework policies, as well as information on the purpose and types of homework. 3. Demerath, P. (2009). Producing success: The culture of personal advancement in an American high school. Chicago, IL: The University of Chicago Press. In this book, the author spends 4 years in a Midwestern high school examining the cul- ture of success and need for advancement. The author discusses work ethics, negotiat- ing tactics of students to get ahead and cheating as a means to the desired outcome. 4. Zito, N., & McQuillan, P. (2010). ā€œItā€™s not my faultā€: Using neutralization theory to understand cheating by middle school stu- dents. Current Issues in Education, 13. http://cie.asu.edu/. This article includes interviews with faculty and students in a high achieving competitive middle school and explains how students ā€˜neutralizeā€™ and justify cheating behaviors. The author concludes with suggestions for school faculty on how to promote value in course work and eliminate the competitive focus on grades. Theoretical Approaches to Understanding and Promoting Academic Integrity 128