SlideShare a Scribd company logo
1 of 196
Some fun facts
Number of chromosomes an organism has does not equate to
level of intelligence
It takes about 8 hours for your cells to completely copy your
DNA
If the human genome were a book, it would be equivalent to 800
dictionaries
We share 98% of our genes with chimpanzees, 90%with mice,
21% with worms, and 7% with e.coli bacteria
Genetics for the non medical professional
We have 23 pairs of chromosomes, 22 are autosomes, 1 pair is
the sex chromosomes. Males are XY, females XX. Why is this
important?
Chromosomes are arranged from large to small
The Basics
Alteration in genetic material
Alteration is present at birth
May or may not be other members of the family affected
Sometimes genetic errors occur spontaneously in dominant or
recessive patterns of inheritance
Causes
Dysmorphic features
Organ abnormalities
Medical issues
Developmental disabilites
Characteristics of genetic disorders
Brain
Congenital heart disease
Kidney problems
Endocrine disorders
Eye/ear abnormalities
GI problems
Organ abnormalities
Respiratory issues
Growth issues
Seizure disorders
Feeding dysfunction
aspiration
Medical
Forehead
Eyes
Nose
Philtrum
Lips and mouth
chin
Dysmorphic features
Has to be ordered
Karotype
Genetic micro array
FISH test
Genetic counseling
Genetic testing
Intellectual disability
ASD
Behavior issues
Hypotonia
Vision and/or hearing impairments
Developmental Disabilities
When fertilization occurs
Cell reproduction
In one, mitosis, cells duplicate
In another process, called meiosis cells reduce (occurs on the
sex chromosome)
Most genetic errors occur during the cell reduction phase
Mitosis and Meiosis
Meiosis
Karotype/Genotype
Nondisjunction
Translocation
Mosaic
Deletions
Micro-deletions
Types of genetic errors
Trisomy
Translocation
Down syndrome
Fragile X syndrome
Prader Willi syndrome
Angelman syndrome
William syndrome
Sotos syndrome
Rett syndrome
CHARGE syndrome
Fetal alcohol syndrome
Common genetic syndromes
Sex chromosome errors
Sex chromosome errors
Sex chromosome errors
Dominant
recessive
Dominance patterns
It is not due to infections, trauma, emotional distress, lifestyle
habits, income level or what a mother did or did not do during
her pregnancy
It is estimated that all of us have anywhere between 6-8 genetic
defects that we carry
We do not have any control over how the genes separate and
divide at conception
Still- families feel guilty
What are not causes?
Functional outcomes are important
Although a child may need therapy, often does not need weekly
therapy, in fact, research shows that weekly therapy is not
beneficial for some of these children
Working on functional outcomes, functional goals in the
community setting is important
Intervention
Complicated process
Genetic abnormalities are classified by the chromosome number
they occur on
Children with a trisomy abnormality have more problems than a
child with a mosaic or deletion
A child with a genetic abnormality on a higher number
chromosome (21, 22) will have less difficulty than a child with
a 13 or 18 defect
Most fetuses are spontaneously miscarried that carry a genetic
defect.
Summary
Chromosome are like the individual pages of an encyclopedia,
genes are the words are on the pages, and DNA is the individual
letters of the words. When all these things are in the right
order, we consider that a normal person. And most of the time
things go right in such a complicated process
Summary
Behavioural phenotypes and special educational needs:
is aetiology important in the classroom?jir_1542 929..946
C. Reilly
School of Education, University College Dublin, Belfield,
Dublin 4, Ireland
Abstract
Background A number of genetic conditions with
associated intellectual disability and/or special edu-
cational needs have increasingly well-defined behav-
ioural phenotypes. Thus, the concept of ‘behavioural
phenotype’ and aetiology of intellectual disability
may be important with regard to school-based
interventions.
Method The evidence for distinctive cognitive and
behavioural aspects of five of the most common
genetic syndromes (Down syndrome, fragile X syn-
drome, Williams syndrome, Prader–Willi syndrome
and velo-cardio-facial syndrome) associated with
special educational needs is reviewed with respect
to key studies and findings. The possible utility of
aetiology-related interventions in education is dis-
cussed with reference to arguments for and against
such approaches with respect to published guide-
lines and published research.
Results Behavioural phenotypes are probabilistic
and many children with a specific genetic syndrome
will share commonalities with other children with
other genetic syndromes and within syndrome vari-
ability is not uncommon. There is evidence that
teachers and parents have limited knowledge of
aspects of the proposed cognitive and behaviour
profiles associated with the reviewed syndromes.
While there are published guidelines in the area of
learning and behaviour for each of the five reviewed
syndromes there is a limited amount of evidence of
the efficacy of such approaches in school settings.
Conclusion It is likely that knowing the aetiology of
a child’s special educational needs will be helpful
for staff who work in school settings in relation to
cognitive and behavioural implications. However,
how such knowledge might inform teaching practice
or behavioural interventions has not been studied. A
model is proposed that might help inform educators
about the possible role of aetiology in the
classroom.
Keywords behavioural phenotypes, Down
syndrome, fragile X, Prader–Willi syndrome, VCFS,
Williams syndrome
Introduction – aetiology and
behavioural phenotypes
There is increasing evidence which suggests that
there are distinctive cognitive and behavioural pro-
files in a number of genetic syndromes with associ-
ated intellectual disability (ID) (Dykens et al. 2000;
Rosner et al. 2004). While there is likely to be a
core set of deficits in neuropsychological function-
ing that is shared by all people with ID regardless
of cause, there are more specific patterns of abilities
Correspondence: Mr Colin Reilly, School of Education,
University
College Dublin, Belfield, Dublin 4, Ireland (e-mail:
[email protected]
ucdconnect.ie).
Journal of Intellectual Disability Research doi: 10.1111/j.1365-
2788.2012.01542.x
VOLUME 56 PART 10 pp 929–946 OCTOBER 2012
929
bs_bs_banner
© 2012 The Author. Journal of Intellectual Disability Research
© 2012 Blackwell Publishing Ltd
and deficits associated with known genetic syn-
dromes (Garner et al. 1999). Important in any dis-
cussion on the role of aetiology is the concept of a
‘behavioural phenotype’. While some definitions of
a ‘behavioural phenotype’ have emphasised ‘distinct
behavioural features occurring in almost every case
of the condition and rarely in other conditions’
(Flynt & Yule 1994, p. 666), others have emphasised
a heightened probability that a behaviour or cluster
of behaviours will be associated with a syndrome.
Dykens (1995) has defined a ‘behavioural pheno-
type’ as involving the ‘heightened probability or
likelihood that people with a given syndrome will
exhibit certain behavioural and developmental
sequelae relative to those without the syndrome’ (p.
523). This definition highlights the probable nature
of behavioural phenotypes in that not all individuals
with a specific genetic disorder will display particu-
lar aetiology-related behaviours (Fidler et al. 2002).
For the purposes of the article the concept of a
behavioural phenotype is understood in terms of
Dykens’s definition with the focus primarily
on cognitive and behavioural profiles likely to be
relevant for school aged children. Five genetic
syndromes associated with ID and/or special
educational needs are examined in this the article to
consider if aetiology of ID matters when consider-
ing educational interventions and approaches. The
five were chosen as they all have relatively well-
defined cognitive and behavioural profiles and all
have had guidelines published for intervention in
teaching and behavioural domains.
Work on behavioural phenotypes aims to link
together genes, brain and behaviour as well as to
refine treatment and intervention (Dykens 2000).
When considering treatment and intervention it
is thought that comparisons of children with one
genetic syndrome with children with another
genetic syndrome, or with children with ID without
a known aetiology, or with typically developing chil-
dren matched for mental or chronological age may
reveal specific aetiology-related strengths and diffi-
culties associated with each syndrome. It has been
proposed that such understanding is vital in select-
ing appropriate educational intervention strategies
for children with genetic syndromes (e.g. Campbell
et al. 2009). Children without genetic aetiologies for
their ID and associated special educational needs
may not show the same pattern of significant areas
of strengths or weaknesses in terms of development,
as children with a known genetic aetiology for their
ID across cognitive, behavioural and linguistic
domains (Dykens & Hodapp 2001).
However, adopting an approach whereby one
treats a syndrome group as largely homogeneous
with regard to treatment and intervention may have
significant drawbacks and may not concur with evi-
dence suggesting that within syndrome variation is
common. It has been noted that in some neurode-
velopmental disorders, within syndrome variability
is as great as or greater than between syndrome
variability (Levy & Ebstein 2009). Porter & Colth-
eart’s (2005) study of the performance of individu-
als with Williams syndrome on the subtests of the
Woodcock-Johnson Tests of Cognitive Ability –
Revised ( Woodcock & Johnson 1990), suggests that
as many as six different non-normative cognitive
profiles exist in Williams syndrome. A danger then
may exist if syndromes with known genetic causes
are treated as well-controlled homogenous study
groups that do not differ across cognitive and
linguistic domains. As well as within syndrome
variability there may also be cross-syndrome
similarities, and co-morbidities between a syndrome
with a known genetic cause and a syndrome
without a single known genetic cause. For example,
a child with fragile X syndrome may also meet the
diagnostic criteria for autism spectrum disorder
(ASD) (although diagnosis of ASD in genetic syn-
dromes associated with ID is not without chal-
lenges, see Moss & Howlin 2009). In this case, the
child is likely to share cognitive and behavioural
features with other children with fragile X syn-
drome and other children with ASD.
In the light of this discussion then it is worth
noting that there appears to be a need to recognise
that an individual with a syndrome with a known
aetiology may be likely to conform to the behav-
ioural phenotype proposed for that syndrome in
some domains, but also may differ from that pro-
posed behavioural phenotype in other domains. At
times the child may share more in common with a
child with another genetic syndrome or a child who
does not have a genetic aetiology for their difficul-
ties than they do with other children with the same
syndrome. In addition, there may be some children
who seem to closely ‘fit’ the proposed behavioural
phenotype associated with the syndrome while
930
Journal of Intellectual Disability Research VOLUME 56 PART
10 OCTOBER 2012
C. Reilly • Aetiology in the classroom
© 2012 The Author. Journal of Intellectual Disability Research
© 2012 Blackwell Publishing Ltd
other children appear not to ‘fit’ as well with the
proposed phenotype. A child with Down syndrome
who meets the diagnostic criteria for ASD could be
seen as not fitting with the proposed phenotype
associated with Down syndrome which usually
involves assumptions of comparatively low levels of
behavioural difficulties and a sociable personality. In
the case of some aspects of a behavioural phenotype
associated with a genetic condition there appears to
be a very high probability that a child with the con-
dition will conform in some domains. For example,
almost all school aged children with Prader–Willi
syndrome will display an excessive interest in food.
In such a case where a particular behaviour is asso-
ciated with one syndrome and not usually with
others, the term ‘total specificity’ has been proposed
(Hodapp 1997). Where two or more syndromes
share a propensity for a particular behaviour the
behaviour shows ‘partial specificity’ (Hodapp 1997).
For example, on the Kaufman Assessment Battery
for Children (Kaufman & Kaufman 1983) boys
with fragile X syndrome show a relative strength in
simultaneous processing (Hodapp et al. 1992). This
relative strength may also be shared by children
with Prader–Willi syndrome (Dykens et al. 1992),
but not by the majority of children with Down syn-
drome (Pueschel et al. 1987), or children with ID of
unknown aetiology (Hodapp et al. 1992). Therefore,
when studying genetic syndromes and possible
aetiology-specific approaches there is a need to
understand that behavioural phenotypes associated
with those syndromes are probabilistic, and not
every individual with the syndrome will display the
pattern of behaviours associated with the syndrome.
Evidence for behavioural phenotypes
In this section, evidence for specific behavioural
phenotypes associated with five well-described
genetic syndromes, Down Syndrome, fragile X syn-
drome, Williams syndrome, Prader–Willi syndrome
and velo-cardio-facial syndrome (VCFS; also known
as 22q11.2 syndrome) will be summarised across a
number of domains which are relevant in classroom
settings. It is not possible to comprehensively review
all research findings across all domains for each
syndrome but the main findings for each syndrome
will be summarised with a particular emphasis on
findings that have been used with respect to pro-
posed educational interventions.
Examining evidence for distinctive cognitive and
behavioural profiles associated with the syndromes
is important because evidence of this nature is often
used when guidelines for intervention are devel-
oped. Those who advocate an aetiological approach
argue that it is the distinctive profiles of relative
cognitive and behavioural strengths and weaknesses
associated with syndromes which make aetiology
important with regard to programme planning.
Table 1 summarises characteristics associated with
the syndromes in global and specific aspects of cog-
nition, adaptive behaviour, symptoms of ASD and
attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD),
and maladaptive/problematic behaviour. Early
research on the syndromes tended to focus on mea-
sures of broad aspects of cognition such as global
scores on IQ tests and strengths and weakness on
subtests of commonly used IQ tests where more
recently there has been a trend towards more
detailed neuropsychological testing, which has
examined areas such as memory, attention and
social cognition. With the exception of a UK
population-based study focussing on Prader–Willi
syndrome (Holland et al. 2003) the studies carried
out on cognitive and behavioural aspects of the syn-
dromes have not been population based and as a
result findings may not be representative of the
entire population affected by the syndrome,
although an increasing number of studies have
included large samples and multisite studies. The
lack of population-based data may be particularly
problematic in syndromes such as VCFS where
those with a milder presentation may not have been
identified and thus not included in clinic-based
studies.
In relation to ASD it is difficult to compare rates
of ASD across syndromes because of differences in
how rates were ascertained and the instruments
used in studies. In Table 1 rates of ASD symptoms
across the syndromes are compared based on scores
on the Social Communication Questionnaire
(Rutter et al. 2003), or the earlier equivalent Autism
Screening Questionnaire (Berument et al. 1999).
The prevalence rates indicate the percentage of
individuals who meet the cut-off for ASD on the
Social Communication Questionnaire/Autism
Screening Questionnaire and do not indicate the
931
Journal of Intellectual Disability Research VOLUME 56 PART
10 OCTOBER 2012
C. Reilly • Aetiology in the classroom
© 2012 The Author. Journal of Intellectual Disability Research
© 2012 Blackwell Publishing Ltd
T
ab
le
1
G
lo
b
al
co
gn
it
iv
e
fu
n
ct
io
n
in
g,
co
gn
it
iv
e
st
re
n
gt
h
s
an
d
w
ea
kn
es
se
s,
an
d
ad
ap
ti
ve
b
eh
av
io
u
r
p
ro
fi
le
s
o
f
se
le
ct
ed
sy
n
d
ro
m
es
E
st
im
at
ed
p
re
va
le
n
ce
D
o
w
n
sy
n
d
ro
m
e
F
ra
gi
le
X
sy
n
d
ro
m
e
(m
al
es
)
F
ra
gi
le
X
sy
n
d
ro
m
e
(f
em
al
es
)
W
ill
ia
m
s
sy
n
d
ro
m
e
P
ra
d
er
–W
ill
i
sy
n
d
ro
m
e
V
el
o
-c
ar
d
io
-f
ac
ia
l
sy
n
d
ro
m
e
1
in
80
01
1
in
40
00
2
1
in
60
00
–8
00
03
1
in
75
00
–2
0
00
04
1
in
80
00
–2
0
00
05
1
in
16
00
–4
00
06
G
lo
ba
l
co
gn
it
iv
e
fu
nc
ti
o
ni
ng
•
M
ea
n
IQ
is
m
o
de
ra
te
ID
(r
an
ge
ty
pi
ca
lly
fr
o
m
m
ild
to
se
ve
re
ID
)7
•
M
ea
n
IQ
is
m
o
de
ra
te
ID
(r
an
ge
ty
pi
ca
lly
fr
o
m
m
ild
to
se
ve
re
ID
)8
•
M
ea
n
IQ
is
bo
rd
er
lin
e
fu
nc
ti
o
ni
ng
(r
an
ge
ty
pi
ca
lly
fr
o
m
lo
w
av
er
ag
e
fu
nc
ti
o
ni
ng
to
M
o
de
ra
te
ID
)9
•
M
ea
n
IQ
is
m
ild
ID
(r
an
ge
ty
pi
ca
lly
fr
o
m
bo
rd
er
lin
e
ID
to
m
o
de
ra
te
ID
)1
0
•
M
ea
n
IQ
is
m
ild
ID
(r
an
ge
fr
o
m
bo
rd
er
lin
e
ID
to
m
o
de
ra
te
ID
)1
1
•
M
ea
n
IQ
is
in
bo
rd
er
lin
e
(r
an
ge
fr
o
m
lo
w
av
er
ag
e
fu
nc
ti
o
ni
ng
to
m
ild
ID
)1
2
•
IQ
sc
o
re
s
de
cl
in
e
w
it
h
ag
e
in
ch
ild
ho
o
d7
•
IQ
sc
o
re
s
de
cl
in
e
w
it
h
ag
e
in
ch
ild
ho
o
d1
3
•
IQ
sc
o
re
s
do
no
t
ap
pe
ar
to
de
cl
in
e
w
it
h
ag
e
o
r
at
le
as
t
no
t
to
sa
m
e
ex
te
nt
as
m
al
es
14
•
N
o
ev
id
en
ce
o
f
de
cl
in
es
in
IQ
sc
o
re
s
in
ch
ild
ho
o
d1
5
•
N
o
re
po
rt
ed
de
cl
in
e
in
IQ
sc
o
re
s
in
ch
ild
ho
o
d1
3
•
Po
ss
ib
le
de
cl
in
e
in
ve
rb
al
IQ
sc
o
re
s
w
it
h
ag
e1
6
R
el
at
iv
e
co
gn
it
iv
e
st
re
ng
th
s
an
d
w
ea
kn
es
se
s
•
B
et
te
r
o
n
vi
su
al
-s
pa
ti
al
ta
sk
s
th
an
o
n
ve
rb
al
o
r
au
di
to
ry
ta
sk
s1
7
•
Si
m
ul
ta
ne
o
us
co
gn
it
iv
e
pr
o
ce
ss
in
g
be
tt
er
th
an
se
qu
en
ti
al
pr
o
ce
ss
in
g1
9
•
R
el
at
iv
e
w
ea
kn
es
s
o
n
ta
sk
s
in
vo
lv
in
g
vi
su
o
sp
at
ia
l
ab
ili
ti
es
an
d
w
o
rk
in
g
m
em
o
ry
bu
t
be
tt
er
pe
rf
o
rm
an
ce
o
n
ta
sk
s
w
it
h
m
ea
ni
ng
fu
l
in
fo
rm
at
io
n2
2
•
V
IQ
>
PI
Q
23
•
Si
m
ul
ta
ne
o
us
pr
o
ce
ss
in
g
be
tt
er
se
qu
en
ti
al
pr
o
ce
ss
in
g2
7
•
V
IQ
>
PI
Q
fo
r
m
aj
o
ri
ty
bu
t
di
ffe
re
nc
e
is
re
la
ti
ve
ly
sm
al
l3
0
•
R
el
at
iv
e
w
ea
kn
es
s
in
ex
pr
es
si
ve
la
ng
ua
ge
in
co
m
pa
ri
so
n
w
it
h
re
ce
pt
iv
e
la
ng
ua
ge
18
•
R
el
at
iv
e
w
ea
kn
es
s
in
sh
o
rt
te
rm
m
em
o
ry
,
vi
su
al
sp
at
ia
l
pr
o
ce
ss
in
g,
an
d
vi
su
al
m
o
to
r
ta
sk
s,
bu
t
do
be
tt
er
if
ta
sk
s
in
vo
lv
e
m
ea
ni
ng
fu
l/
co
nt
ex
tu
al
in
fo
rm
at
io
n2
0
•
R
el
at
iv
e
st
re
ng
th
in
au
di
to
ry
ro
te
m
em
o
ry
24
•
R
el
at
iv
e
st
re
ng
th
in
vi
su
al
pr
o
ce
ss
in
g
in
co
m
pa
ri
so
n
w
it
h
au
di
to
ry
pr
o
ce
ss
in
g2
8
•
R
el
at
iv
e
st
re
ng
th
in
ro
te
au
di
to
ry
m
em
o
ry
–
m
em
o
ry
fo
r
lis
t
an
d
o
bj
ec
ts
31
•
R
el
at
iv
e
st
re
ng
th
s
w
it
h
ve
rb
al
la
be
lli
ng
an
d
lo
ng
-t
er
m
m
em
o
ry
21
•
R
el
at
iv
e
w
ea
kn
es
s
o
n
sp
at
ia
l
ta
sk
s2
4
•
T
he
o
ry
o
f
m
in
d
de
fe
ci
ts
29
•
W
ea
kn
es
s
in
vi
su
al
-s
pa
ti
al
m
em
o
ry
ta
sk
s
an
d
m
o
re
co
m
pl
ex
au
di
to
ry
ta
sk
s
in
cl
ud
in
g
po
ss
ib
le
w
o
rk
in
g
m
em
o
ry
de
fic
it
32
•
R
el
at
iv
e
st
re
ng
th
in
fa
ci
al
re
co
gn
it
io
n2
5
•
W
ea
kn
es
s
at
la
be
lli
ng
em
o
ti
o
ns
33
•
R
el
at
iv
e
st
re
ng
th
s
in
ex
pr
es
si
ve
la
ng
ua
ge
26
932
Journal of Intellectual Disability Research VOLUME 56 PART
10 OCTOBER 2012
C. Reilly • Aetiology in the classroom
© 2012 The Author. Journal of Intellectual Disability Research
© 2012 Blackwell Publishing Ltd
A
da
pt
iv
e
be
ha
vi
o
ur
•
C
o
m
m
un
ic
at
io
n
m
ay
be
a
re
la
ti
ve
w
ea
kn
es
s
co
m
pa
re
d
w
it
h
da
ily
liv
in
g
sk
ill
s
an
d
so
ci
al
is
at
io
n3
4
•
R
el
at
iv
e
st
re
ng
th
in
da
ily
liv
in
g
sk
ill
s
do
m
ai
n3
5
•
R
el
at
iv
e
w
ea
kn
es
se
s
in
so
ci
al
is
at
io
n
sk
ill
s,
es
pe
ci
al
ly
in
in
te
rp
er
so
na
l
sk
ill
s3
6
•
St
re
ng
th
in
co
m
m
un
ic
at
io
n
an
d
so
ci
al
is
at
io
n
in
co
m
pa
ri
so
n
w
it
h
da
ily
liv
in
g
sk
ill
s3
7
•
R
el
at
iv
e
st
re
ng
th
in
da
ily
liv
in
g
sk
ill
s
an
d
w
ea
kn
es
s
in
so
ci
al
is
at
io
n1
1
•
R
el
at
iv
e
w
ea
kn
es
s
o
n
da
ily
liv
in
g
sk
ill
s3
8
Sy
m
pt
o
m
s
o
f
SC
Q
/A
SQ
•
15
.6
%
39
•
69
.7
%
40
•
22
.6
%
40
•
N
o
re
se
ar
ch
us
in
g
SC
Q
/
A
SQ
•
36
.1
%
41
•
19
.2
%
42
Sy
m
pt
o
m
s
o
f
A
D
H
D
•
2.
4%
to
44
%
43
•
U
p
to
70
%
re
po
rt
ed
in
yo
un
g
m
al
es
bu
t
m
ay
no
t
be
an
y
hi
gh
er
th
an
IQ
m
at
ch
es
co
nt
ro
ls
44
•
A
pp
ro
xi
m
at
el
y
30
%
an
d
pr
im
ar
ily
di
ffi
cu
lt
ie
s
w
it
h
in
at
te
nt
io
n4
5
•
64
.7
%
w
it
h
di
ffi
cu
lt
ie
s
w
it
h
in
at
te
nt
io
n
be
in
g
m
o
re
co
m
m
o
n
th
an
di
ffi
cu
lt
ie
s
w
it
h
hy
pe
ra
ct
iv
it
y/
im
pu
ls
iv
it
y4
6
•
19
–2
6%
sc
o
re
d
ab
ov
e
at
ri
sk
cu
t-
o
ffs
o
n
A
D
H
D
ra
ti
ng
sc
al
e4
7
•
30
–5
5%
w
it
h
di
ffi
cu
lt
ie
s
w
it
h
in
at
te
nt
io
n
pr
ed
o
m
in
at
in
g4
8
Pr
o
bl
em
at
ic
be
ha
vi
o
ur
•
St
ub
bo
rn
ne
ss
an
d
no
n-
co
m
pl
ia
nc
e
bu
t
le
ss
pr
o
bl
em
s
th
at
o
th
er
s
w
it
h
in
te
lle
ct
ua
l
di
sa
bi
lit
y4
9
•
H
an
d
bi
ti
ng
,s
o
ci
al
an
xi
et
y,
sp
ec
ifi
c
ph
o
bi
as
an
d
ga
ze
av
er
si
o
n
ar
e
ve
ry
co
m
m
o
n.
R
ep
et
it
iv
e
ve
rb
al
an
d
no
nv
er
ba
l
be
ha
vi
o
ur
s5
0
•
So
ci
al
an
xi
et
y,
sp
ec
ifi
c
ph
o
bi
as
,a
nd
ga
ze
av
er
si
o
n5
1
•
A
nx
ie
ty
pa
rt
ic
ul
ar
ly
fe
ar
s
an
d
ph
o
bi
as
ar
e
co
m
m
o
n5
2
•
Ex
ce
ss
iv
e
in
te
re
st
in
fo
o
d1
1
•
H
ig
he
r
ri
sk
fo
r
de
pr
es
si
o
n
an
d
an
xi
et
y
th
an
sa
m
e
ag
e
pe
er
s5
5
•
D
efi
ci
ts
in
at
te
nt
io
n
sw
it
ch
in
g5
9
•
D
efi
ci
ts
in
at
te
nt
io
n
sw
it
ch
in
g5
7
•
H
yp
er
so
ci
ab
ili
ty
53
•
Sk
in
pi
ck
in
g1
3
•
In
cr
ea
se
d
ri
sk
fo
r
sc
hi
zo
ph
re
ni
a
ri
sk
ps
yc
ho
ti
c
di
so
rd
er
in
la
te
ad
o
le
sc
en
ce
an
d
ea
rl
y
ad
ul
th
o
o
d5
6
•
H
yp
er
ac
us
is
54
•
O
bs
es
si
o
ns
an
d
co
m
pu
ls
io
ns
13
•
St
ub
bo
rn
13
•
Te
m
pe
r
ta
nt
ru
m
s
58
•
D
efi
ci
ts
in
at
te
nt
io
n
sw
it
ch
in
g6
0
1
S
h
er
m
an
et
al
.
(2
0
0
7
),
2
,3
S
h
er
m
an
(2
0
0
2
),
4
W
an
g
et
al
.
(1
9
9
7
)
an
d
S
tr
o
m
m
e
et
al
.
(2
0
0
2
),
5
B
u
tl
er
(1
9
9
0
)
an
d
C
as
si
d
y
(1
9
9
7
),
6
G
o
o
d
sh
ip
et
al
.
(1
9
9
8
)
an
d
S
h
p
ri
n
tz
en
(2
0
0
8
),
7
H
o
d
ap
p
et
al
.
(1
9
9
9
),
8
H
al
l
(2
0
0
9
),
9
d
e
V
ri
es
et
al
.
(1
9
9
6
)
an
d
C
ro
n
is
te
r
et
al
.
(1
9
9
1
),
1
0
M
ar
te
n
s
et
al
.
(2
0
0
8
),
1
1
W
h
it
ti
n
gt
o
n
&
H
o
ll
an
d
(2
0
0
4
),
1
2
G
re
en
et
al
.
(2
0
0
9
),
1
3
D
yk
en
s
et
al
.
(2
0
0
0
),
1
4
B
ru
n
et
al
.
(1
9
9
5
),
1
5
H
ow
li
n
et
al
.
(1
9
9
8
)
an
d
S
ea
rc
y
et
al
.
(2
0
0
4
),
1
6
G
o
ld
in
g-
K
u
sh
n
er
et
al
.
(1
9
8
5
),
1
7
C
o
u
ze
n
s
et
al
.
(2
0
1
1
)
an
d
H
ax
b
y
(1
9
8
9
),
1
8
M
il
le
r
(1
9
9
2
),
1
9
H
o
d
ap
p
et
al
.
(1
9
9
2
),
2
0
C
o
rn
is
h
et
al
.
(1
9
9
9
)
an
d
V
an
d
er
M
o
le
n
et
al
.
(2
0
1
0
),
2
1
F
re
u
n
d
&
R
ei
ss
(1
9
9
1
),
2
2
M
az
zo
cc
o
et
al
.
(1
9
9
3
)
an
d
C
o
rn
is
h
et
al
.
(1
9
9
8
),
2
3
M
er
vi
s
&
K
le
in
-T
as
m
an
(2
0
0
0
),
2
4
M
er
vi
s
&
M
o
rr
is
(2
0
0
7
),
2
5
D
er
u
el
le
et
al
.
(1
9
9
9
),
2
6
M
er
vi
s
&
K
le
in
-
T
as
m
an
(2
0
0
0
),
2
7
D
yk
en
s
et
al
.
(1
9
9
2
),
2
8
S
ta
u
d
er
et
al
.
(2
0
0
2
),
2
9
K
o
en
ig
et
al
.
(2
0
0
5
),
3
0
C
am
p
b
el
l
&
S
w
il
le
n
(2
0
0
5
),
3
1
A
n
ts
h
el
et
al
.
(2
0
0
8
),
3
2
S
w
il
le
n
et
al
.
(1
9
9
7
),
3
3
C
am
p
b
el
l
et
al
.
(2
0
1
0
),
3
4
D
yk
en
s
et
al
.
(1
9
9
4
),
3
5
D
yk
en
s
et
al
.
(1
9
9
3
),
3
6
F
re
u
n
d
et
al
.
(1
9
9
3
),
3
7
M
er
vi
s
&
Jo
h
n
(2
0
1
0
),
3
8
K
at
es
et
al
.
(2
0
0
7
),
3
9
L
ow
en
th
al
et
al
.
(2
0
0
7
),
4
0
C
li
ff
o
rd
et
al
.
(2
0
0
7
),
4
1
V
el
tm
an
et
al
.
(2
0
0
4
),
4
2
F
in
e
et
al
.
(2
0
0
5
),
4
3
G
at
h
&
G
u
m
le
y
(1
9
8
6
)
an
d
E
ks
te
in
et
al
.
(2
0
1
1
),
4
4
H
ag
er
m
an
(1
9
8
7
),
B
ac
ke
s
et
al
.
(2
0
0
0
)
an
d
K
au
et
al
.
(2
0
0
0
),
4
5
F
re
u
n
d
et
al
.
(1
9
9
3
),
4
6
L
ey
fe
r
et
al
.
(2
0
0
6
),
4
7
W
ig
re
n
&
H
an
se
n
(2
0
0
5
),
4
8
N
ik
la
ss
o
n
et
al
.
(2
0
0
2
),
G
o
th
el
f
et
al
.
(2
0
0
4
),
N
ik
la
ss
o
n
et
al
.
(2
0
0
5
),
A
n
ts
h
el
et
al
.
(2
0
0
6
)
an
d
N
ik
la
ss
o
n
et
al
.
(2
0
0
9
),
4
9
E
sb
en
se
n
&
S
el
tz
er
(2
0
1
1
),
5
0
C
o
h
en
(1
9
9
5
)
an
d
B
el
se
r
&
S
u
d
h
al
te
r
(2
0
0
1
),
5
1
C
o
rd
ei
ro
et
al
.
(2
0
1
1
),
5
2
L
ey
fe
r
et
al
.
(2
0
0
6
),
5
3
Jo
n
es
et
al
.
(2
0
0
0
),
5
4
B
lo
m
b
er
g
et
al
.
(2
0
0
6
),
5
5
F
ei
n
st
ei
n
et
al
.
(2
0
0
2
),
5
6
M
u
rp
h
y
et
al
.
(1
9
9
9
),
5
7
C
o
rn
is
h
et
al
.
(2
0
0
4
),
5
8
H
o
ll
an
d
et
al
.
(2
0
0
3
),
5
9
W
o
o
d
co
ck
et
al
.
(2
0
0
9
),
6
0
W
o
o
d
co
ck
et
al
.
(2
0
1
0
).
A
D
H
D
,
at
te
n
ti
o
n
d
efi
ci
t
h
yp
er
ac
ti
vi
ty
d
is
o
rd
er
;
A
D
A
S
Q
,
A
u
ti
sm
S
cr
ee
n
in
g
Q
u
es
ti
o
n
n
ai
re
(B
er
u
m
en
t
et
al
.
1
9
9
9
);
ID
,
in
te
ll
ec
tu
al
d
is
ab
il
it
y;
S
C
Q
,
S
o
ci
al
C
o
m
m
u
n
ic
at
io
n
Q
u
es
ti
o
n
n
ai
re
(R
u
tt
er
et
al
.
2
0
0
3
).
933
Journal of Intellectual Disability Research VOLUME 56 PART
10 OCTOBER 2012
C. Reilly • Aetiology in the classroom
© 2012 The Author. Journal of Intellectual Disability Research
© 2012 Blackwell Publishing Ltd
percentage of individuals who actually received a
clinical diagnosis of ASD. There have also been a
number of studies reporting the presence of signifi-
cant ADHD symptoms in each of the syndromes
and the main findings are summarised in Table 1. It
has been posited that the utility of an ADHD diag-
nosis in children with complex behavioural pheno-
types needs further evaluation (Zwaigenbaum et al.
2006), and the higher rate of the primarily inatten-
tive subtype in this population could point to both
a different manifestation of symptoms and underly-
ing pathophysiology in individuals with genetic
syndromes associated with ID.
Cognitive and behavioural interventions
Proposed classroom interventions
Having summarised some of the research evidence
for the existence of ‘behavioural phenotypes’ or
aetiology-specific profiles in five syndromes associ-
ated with ID it is now worth examining proposed
guidelines for intervention based on the outlined
behavioural phenotypes. Most of these guidelines
reflect early research focussing on analysis of cogni-
tive profiles via patterns of strengths and weak-
nesses on subtests of tests of cognitive functioning
and parental report of patterns of maladaptive or
problematic behaviours, as opposed to more recent
detailed observational studies of behaviour and
assessment of specific aspects of cognition. For all
of the syndromes there are now guidelines and in
some cases specific resources aimed at targeting
classroom learning and proposing particular teach-
ing approaches (e.g. Oelwein 1995; Braden 2002;
Dew-Hughes 2004; Horsteimer 2004; Chedd et al.
2006; Cutler-Landsman 2007; Udwin et al. 2007).
In relation to behaviour management there are also
guidelines that reflect the behavioural profiles asso-
ciated with syndromes (e.g. Hills-Epstein et al.
2002; Semel & Rosner 2003; Whitman & Jackson
2006). These guidelines are typically based on rec-
ommendations derived from the cognitive and
behavioural profiles of the syndromes and extensive
experience of working with children affected by the
syndromes and there are few empirical studies to
verify the validity of the recommendations. Some of
the guidelines are syndrome specific (e.g. in fragile
X syndrome sitting side-on as opposed to face-on
due to hyperarousal and resultant gaze aversion).
Other guidelines for practice are shared across two
or more genetic syndromes and might also be com-
monly used with children with conditions such as
ASD and ADHD (e.g. use of low distraction work
areas such as individual workstations for children
with fragile X syndrome, Williams syndrome, ASD
and ADHD). In this way the guidelines reflect
Hodapp’s (1997) notion of both ‘total specificity’
and ‘partial specificity’.
Some examples of the suggested recommenda-
tions are now outlined. The relative strengths in
visual-spatial domain and early gesturing have led
to suggestions that children with Down syndrome
may be particularly suitable for interventions such
as sign language (Hodapp et al. 1992) and visual
approaches to reading (Buckley 1995). Dykens &
Hodapp (1997) have suggested that children with
Williams syndrome may benefit from team or
buddy systems in school- and people-oriented jobs
based on their relative strength in language and
sociability. Teachers of children with fragile X syn-
drome and Prader–Willi syndrome could rely on
visual perceptual cues including pictures and
models whereas teachers of children with Williams
syndrome may not benefit as much from visual
approaches (Hodapp & Fidler 1999). It has been
posited that boys with fragile X syndrome will
struggle with phonics-based approaches to reading
(Braden 2002). However, young children with
VCFS have been noted to have a good ability to
decode words, and thus are more likely to benefit
from early phonics-based instruction (Cutler-
Landsman 2007), as are children with Williams
syndrome (Udwin et al. 2007). With regard to
therapeutic intervention it has been suggested that
‘group therapies’ may work well for persons with
Williams syndrome due to sociability and ‘people
orientation’ but not as well for children with fragile
X syndrome due to often displayed anxiety in social
environments (Hodapp & Dykens 2001). However,
children with fragile X syndrome may benefit from
indirect instruction in a group where peers are used
as teaching models and the child with fragile X is
not the focus of direct questioning which may be
anxiety provoking (Braden 2002). In terms of
behaviour management, Einfeld (2005) has argued
that factors contributing to behavioural issues may
arise from four causes and one of the four is the
934
Journal of Intellectual Disability Research VOLUME 56 PART
10 OCTOBER 2012
C. Reilly • Aetiology in the classroom
© 2012 The Author. Journal of Intellectual Disability Research
© 2012 Blackwell Publishing Ltd
specific aetiology of the child’s condition. In the
case of Prader–Willi syndrome, Einfeld (2005)
argues that traditional contingent reinforcement
strategies are likely to be less successful due to
hypothalamic dysfunction associated with the syn-
drome and that environmental or antecedent-based
approaches may be more successful. In a similar
vein in relation to Williams syndrome, Einfeld
(2005) argues that traditional behavioural
approaches may not be helpful when dealing with a
child’s avoidance or reluctance, but that a knowl-
edge of the high prevalence of fears and anxieties
associated with the syndrome might prompt a
medical referral and possible use of anti-anxiety
medication.
In relation to interventions for children with
genetic syndromes it has been proposed that where
possible proposed interventions should focus on a
child’s strengths (Hodapp & Dykens 2001; Hodapp
et al. 2003), as opposed to attempting to remediate
the child’s relative weaknesses. The majority of
syndrome-specific recommendations tend to focus
on a child’s strengths or compensate for their weak-
nesses as opposed to attempting to remediate or
ameliorate weaknesses. However, the view that the
‘playing to strengths’ is the most efficacious way to
approach educational instruction is not shared by
all in the wider educational community. Alferink &
Farmer-Dougan (2010) argue that teaching to a
specific strength in the absence of teaching to a
weakness may be a disservice to students and the
larger the child’s inventory of learning strategies,
the more likely the child is to learn across environ-
mental settings. In relation to fragile X syndrome
the commonly observed gaze aversion in boys has
led to recommendations that teachers and school
staff should not force eye contact but should
instead sit side-on as opposed to face on
(Scharfenaker et al. 2002). This would be an
example of playing to a child’s strengths or at least
not focussing on a child’s weakness. However, it has
been argued that with the use of behavioural tech-
niques, eye contact duration can be improved in
children with fragile X syndrome (Hall et al. 2009),
so in this case the intervention is focussing on ame-
liorating a syndrome-specific weakness. There is
evidence that aetiology-related strengths and weak-
nesses become more pronounced with age (Hodapp
2004). Whether this is related to the fact that chil-
dren gravitate towards activities that reinforce their
strengths or parents and teachers focus on this
strengths is not clear. If profiles become more pro-
nounced with age it may that early intensive inter-
vention is vital in order to address relative
weaknesses and that with older children, focussing
on strengths may be more appropriate.
Intervention studies
Despite the number of studies which have focussed
on describing the cognitive and behavioural aspects
of the syndromes under review, there have been few
intervention studies assessing the validity of specific
approaches in teaching skills to children affected by
the syndromes. The small number of studies that
have been carried out have focussed mainly on
reading (mostly in Down syndrome) and specific
behavioural difficulties and have been single-
participant designs or contained a relatively small
number of participants. A number of these studies
are now described.
Relative weaknesses in auditory short-term
memory have led to the view that children with
Down syndrome may struggle with phonics-based
approaches to reading instruction and that starting
with sight words may be more profitable (Oelwein
1995). Cologon et al. (2011) described how seven
children with Down syndrome benefited from
phonological awareness training over 10 weekly
sessions and post intervention showed improved
reading skills and phonological awareness. Goetz
et al. (2008) described how 15 children with
Down syndrome benefitted from a phonics-based
approach to reading over 8 weeks and improved
significantly compared to a waiting group of eight
children with Down syndrome. Neither of these
two studies included a comparison with sight
word approaches. However, Cupples & Iacono
(2002) described a study where seven children with
Down syndrome received training in either phono-
logical awareness (four children) or sight word
approaches to reading (three children). Reading of
non-training generalisation words improved signifi-
cantly in only three children, all of whom had
received phonics-based training (Cupples & Iacono
2002).
In fragile X syndrome a number of small-scale
intervention studies have been carried and are in
935
Journal of Intellectual Disability Research VOLUME 56 PART
10 OCTOBER 2012
C. Reilly • Aetiology in the classroom
© 2012 The Author. Journal of Intellectual Disability Research
© 2012 Blackwell Publishing Ltd
informative with regard to the possible usefulness
of approaches based on the syndrome’s proposed
behavioural phenotype. Hall et al. (2009) employed
a percentile reinforcement schedule with and
without overcorrection to shape eye-contact dura-
tion of six boys with the syndrome. Results showed
that eye contact, often thought to be aversive in
fragile X syndrome improved in four boys using
percentile schedules either alone or in combination
with overcorrection technique without concomitant
increases in social anxiety or behavioural problems.
Based on evidence that some children with fragile
X may benefit from teaching strategies that adopt a
simultaneous stimulus presentation, as opposed to a
sequential stimulus presentation Hall et al. (2006)
used a computerised matching to sample procedure
to teach mathematics and geography skills. Five
adolescents (four boys, one girl) with fragile X syn-
drome aged 12–19 years were taught these skills
using a self-paced computer program implemented
intensively with the children over 2 days. Results
indicated that four of the five children successfully
learned the mathematics trained relations and three
of the five children successfully learned the geogra-
phy trained relations. The authors concluded that
that computerised matching-to-sample procedures,
even when conducted in time-limited sessions, may
help individuals with fragile X syndrome learn new
skills although again no controls were used and
alternative teaching approaches were not assessed.
Based on a relative strength in auditory short-
term memory, it has been recommended that chil-
dren with Williams syndrome be taught to read
using phonics-based approaches (e.g. Levy &
Antebi 2004). Becerra et al. (2008) examined types
of reading instruction and performance in 44 indi-
viduals with Williams syndrome. Twenty-four indi-
viduals received instruction via phonics-based
approaches and 20 via sight word approaches. The
children’s reading standard scores were then com-
pared to those predicted based on their overall level
of cognitive functioning. Results indicated that most
children in the phonics group read at or above the
level expected for their level of cognitive function-
ing. In contrast, most children in the sight word
group read below the level expected for their level
of cognitive functioning. In Prader–Willi syndrome,
two case studies have been published of interven-
tions for reducing the occurrence of skin-picking a
common problematic behaviour in the syndrome.
Radstaake et al. (2011) reported on the successful
use of functional behavioural assessment and treat-
ment of skin picking in a 16-year-old female with
the syndrome. Stokes & Luiselli (2009) describe a
study where the skin picking of a 26-year-old male
with Prader–Willi was reduced by restricting the
individual’s access to the setting in which skin
picking most often occurred (i.e. bathroom), teach-
ing the individual novel forms of communication
(i.e. functional communication training), and pro-
viding reinforcement for behaviours other than skin
picking.
Behavioural phenotypes in the classroom
Is genetic aetiology important in the classroom?
Arguments exist for and against the importance
aetiology with regard to classroom interventions.
For those who argue that aetiology is important
(e.g. Hodapp & Fidler 1999; Hodapp & Dykens
2001; Hodapp et al. 2003), it is posited that knowl-
edge of aetiology may aid early identification of
syndrome-specific cognitive and behavioural profiles
thus enabling parents and professionals to target
strengths and needs with appropriate therapeutic
interventions and teaching strategies. By utilising
these syndrome-specific behavioural profiles, it is
proposed teachers can develop more targeted, ‘fine-
grained, and effective interventions’ (Dykens 1999;
Hodapp & Fidler 1999). Although genetic aetiology
may provide guidelines that will work in some but
not every case (Hodapp & Fidler 1999), an under-
standing of behavioural phenotypes can help special
educators foresee and avoid certain difficulties and
deal more effectively with others (Hodapp & Fidler
1999). It is also argued that in order to take full
advantage of genetic research, educators and clini-
cians require access to up-to-date and relevant
research findings on syndromes associated with ID
(Lee et al. 2005).
Others have expressed the view that aetiology is
not important in educational programming. Halla-
han & Kauffman (2000) argue that the focus of
educational programmes should vary according to
the degree of the student’s ID, and Blackhurst &
Berndine (1993) have expressed the view systems of
classification of based on aetiology are not valuable
936
Journal of Intellectual Disability Research VOLUME 56 PART
10 OCTOBER 2012
C. Reilly • Aetiology in the classroom
© 2012 The Author. Journal of Intellectual Disability Research
© 2012 Blackwell Publishing Ltd
in education. Abbeduto & Keller-Bell (2002)
described calls for curricula for individual syn-
dromes as reflecting ‘insularity’ and suggested the
importance of recognising that common problems
exist across syndromes. As well as questions about
the relevance of aetiology one reason for this
concern about aetiology-specific approaches may be
worries about the ‘balkanisation’ of special educa-
tion (Forness & Kavale 1994). Balkanisation refers
to the idea that all children with specific disorders
would need separate educational categories with
eligibility criteria and separate educational pro-
grammes and resources.
Despite increasing evidence of syndrome-specific
profiles and the prospect of aetiology-specific inter-
ventions there is as of yet, limited research evidence
of the efficacy of such aetiology-related approaches
in the classroom and few of the recommendations
have been systematically evaluated. It is not clear
how much knowledge about aetiology-related pro-
files is available in classrooms and if so how it is
used in terms of educational practices (Fidler et al.
2002). It has also been asserted that special educa-
tors have largely ignored calls to tailor interventions
to cognitive or behavioural profiles associated with
specific syndromes (Hodapp & Dykens 2009).
Making inferences from the clinic to the classroom
or other settings may not always be warranted. In
the case of classroom behaviours a study by Saun-
ders (1999) suggests that behaviour in the class-
room does reflect aspects of the behavioural
phenotype associated with a particular syndrome.
Teachers of children with fragile X syndrome
reported that boys with the syndrome did not like
activities involving large groups including dinner-
time and playtime and group activities such as story
time, circle time or class discussion (Saunders
1999), which could be seen to reflect ‘hyperarousal’
and related behaviours associated with the syn-
drome. In terms of favoured activities teachers
mentioned ‘play, cookery, computer, trips out and
stories’ which it could be argued link in to the rela-
tive strengths in daily living as identified on mea-
sures of adaptive functioning. Symons et al. (2001)
reported on the classroom engagement of 26 boys
with fragile X syndrome with a mean age of 7.5
years. Using classroom observations they reported
that the boys were engaged at a similar level to
peers although they indicated that their measure-
ment system may have been limited with regard to
measuring subtle aspects of classroom engagement.
They reported that the engagement of the boys was
significantly related to how the teacher had struc-
tured the classroom suggesting that ‘classroom
ecology was more important than biology’ (Symons
et al. 2001). These two pieces of research suggest
that while aetiology may indicate a child’s prefer-
ence for types of activities, getting the classroom
environment right for all children with ID regard-
less of aetiology should still be the key starting
point.
Incorporating aetiological knowledge in
the classroom
Because of the limited research evidence on the
manifestation of behavioural phenotypes in the
classroom it could be argued that there is not yet
enough evidence to advocate aetiology as an impor-
tant consideration in the classroom, and there is no
consensus regarding the role that this aetiology-
specific information should have in shaping inter-
vention and service delivery (Fidler et al. 2007). In
the absence of such evidence, how then should aeti-
ology be considered with respect to the teaching of
children with genetic syndromes associated with
ID? On the one hand, it would be difficult for most
teachers to have an in-depth knowledge of cognitive
and behavioural aspects of the syndromes discussed
in this review as well as the many other genetic syn-
dromes associated with ID. The fact that many
teachers will only teach a small number of children
with genetic syndromes throughout their career
would make comprehensive training an inefficient
allocation of resources. On the other hand, there
would appear to be key aspects of the cognitive
and behavioural profiles of some of the syndromes
described here that teachers need to know.
Knowing that hyperphagia is associated with
Prader–Willi syndrome, hyperarousal with fragile X
syndrome, and fears/phobias with Williams syn-
drome would appear vital in understanding an
affected child’s behaviour. In terms of cognitive
functioning, being aware of relative strengths in
simultaneous processing among children with
fragile X syndrome and Prader–Willi syndrome and
relative strengths in auditory processing among chil-
937
Journal of Intellectual Disability Research VOLUME 56 PART
10 OCTOBER 2012
C. Reilly • Aetiology in the classroom
© 2012 The Author. Journal of Intellectual Disability Research
© 2012 Blackwell Publishing Ltd
dren with Williams syndrome and VCFS would also
appear important in understanding the learning
strengths of these children. However, even if educa-
tors have a good awareness of a proposed behav-
ioural phenotype, there still exists a dilemma with
regards to whether an approach should focus on
teaching via strengths or should resources be used
to intensively targets weaknesses.
One difficulty with applying findings from
research on behavioural phenotypes in educational
settings is the lack of knowledge among health and
educational professionals of syndromes and associ-
ated cognitive and behavioural profiles. In a survey
by York et al. (1999), significantly fewer school staff
had knowledge of fragile X syndrome and the asso-
ciated cognitive and behavioural profile in compari-
son with Down syndrome. Even among staff who
had taught a child with fragile X syndrome specific
knowledge about recommended interventions and
approaches was limited (York et al. 1999). Lee et al.
(2005) looked at the awareness of special educators
and paediatricians of Down syndrome, fragile X
syndrome and VCFS. With regard to cognitive fea-
tures the highest knowledge among teachers was for
Down syndrome with the least knowledge for VCFS
and both teachers and paediatricians demonstrated
difficulties in identifying cognitive aspects of fragile
X and VCFS. In relation to behavioural aspects the
highest level of knowledge was for fragile X syn-
drome followed by Down syndrome and then
VCFS. In this study, the number of children that
the teachers had taught with each syndrome corre-
lated positively with their level of knowledge of the
cognitive and behavioural phenotype. In the case of
Prader–Willi syndrome and Williams syndrome,
parents tend not to know about cognitive aspects
associated with their child’s syndrome but may
know more about behavioural aspects (Fidler et al.
2002). Given that parents are often the sole provid-
ers about information about their child’s syndrome
(Fidler et al. 2002) it would appear that teachers
will have limited knowledge of aetiology-specific
profiles or interventions especially in the cognitive
domain. Teachers are likely to learn about syn-
dromes on a case-by-case basis through experiences
with individual children (Lee et al. 2005). There-
fore, an issue would appear to be, should prospec-
tive teacher education be offered and if so how best
to offer it? Forness & Kavale (1994) have suggested
that a middle ground may be possible where school
staff become aware of relevant features of a syn-
drome or disorder while still adhering to traditional
programmes. In this scenario firstly knowing that a
child’s syndrome may have an impact on their
learning and behavioural profile should be the start-
ing point. With regard to understanding how this
information might be imparted the pyramid in
Fig. 1 adapted from Braden & Riley (2006) and
Brown-Chidsey & Steege (2005) might be useful as
an aid to understanding how interventions might be
conceptualised.
The approach in Fig. 1 recognises that in many
cases the needs of children with special educational
needs can be met with interventions and
approaches that work with the majority of children.
However, there will be times that such universal
approaches do not best meet the needs of all stu-
dents and there may be groups of children with
similar needs. Some of these children may have
genetic syndromes, some may have well-defined
conditions without an identified genetic aetiology
(e.g. ASD, ADHD), and some may have an educa-
tional need but not have a diagnosed syndrome or
condition. For example, a number of children may
have significant ADHD type difficulties and will
need distraction-free work areas or use of individual
workstations. Such environmental accommodations
could be made for children with ADHD, ASD,
fragile X syndrome, and Williams syndrome and
may be made within the classroom or in a more
specialised setting. In terms of shared cognitive
profile the promotion of word recognition for chil-
dren with Prader–Willi and fragile X syndrome may
benefit significantly from approaches emphasising
simultaneous visual approaches. In some cases chil-
dren with specific syndromes may need very specific
guidelines based on aspects of their syndrome. Chil-
dren with Prader–Willi may need their educational
programme to contain clear guidelines around
access to food and need for regular physical exer-
cise. The final aspect of educational planning for
children with special educational needs and/or ID
should be a focus on the child’s own particular
interests and learning strengths and weaknesses,
and other relevant individual factors. These aspects
of the child’s profile may or may not be related to
the child’s hypothesised behavioural phenotype but
will be garnered by careful observations by school
938
Journal of Intellectual Disability Research VOLUME 56 PART
10 OCTOBER 2012
C. Reilly • Aetiology in the classroom
© 2012 The Author. Journal of Intellectual Disability Research
© 2012 Blackwell Publishing Ltd
staff and consultation with the child’s parents and
teachers.
In considering aetiology it is important that edu-
cators understand that within syndrome variability
is common and when considering specific aetiology-
related guidelines, these should be evaluated in the
same manner as other interventions. An overly rigid
application of syndrome-specific information could
lead to poor intervention planning with a risk that
intervention resources are wasted on difficulties that
a child does not actually demonstrate, despite a
child’s higher probability of demonstrating certain
difficulties as a function of their diagnosis (Fidler
et al. 2007). The approach outlined here recognises
that some educational needs are shared across syn-
dromes whereas some seem relatively unique and
while many teaching techniques are useful regard-
less of diagnosis this does not negate the need for
aetiology-specific information (Starr et al. 2006). A
consideration of genetic aetiology should not be at
the expense of neglecting environmental influences.
The influence of the child’s environment at home
and school including family, teacher and peer influ-
ences, is also likely to significantly impact on the
development of a behavioural phenotype. The
display of challenging behaviour may be influenced
by challenging environments including environ-
ments where there is a lack of stimulation, atten-
Universal strategies/interventions for children with special
educational needs
(e.g. curriculum adaptations for children with intellectual
disability, use of classroom
wide behaviour modification strategies )
Interventions for children with specific behaviour
associated with a syndrome (e.g. environment
that restricts access to food for children with
Prader−Willi syndrome)
Interventions for individual
children
(e.g. Individual
Behaviour/Education Plan
reflecting child’s interests)
Interventions for conditions with similar cognitive profiles (e.g.
use of visual
materials focussing on simultaneous strengths in fragile X
syndrome and
Prader−Willi syndrome )
Environmental accommodations for children with ADHD type
difficulties (e.g.
distraction free areas in classroom, individual workstations)
Individualised
Behaviour/Education
Plan
Shared
plan/resources for
children with
common difficulties
Class/Group
Plan
School adapts to
accommodate
child’s specific and
shared special
education needs
Syndrome-specific
approach
Figure 1 Planning school-based interventions for children with
intellectual disability and/or special educational needs (adapted
from
Braden & Riley 2006; Brown-Chidsey & Steege 2005).
939
Journal of Intellectual Disability Research VOLUME 56 PART
10 OCTOBER 2012
C. Reilly • Aetiology in the classroom
© 2012 The Author. Journal of Intellectual Disability Research
© 2012 Blackwell Publishing Ltd
tion, access to preferred objects/activities, or the
presence aversive stimuli (Langthorne et al. 2007).
In a study examining the function of challenging
behaviour Langthorne et al. (2011) reported that
children with fragile X are more likely to have an
escape-maintained function than attention-
maintained function for problem behaviour possibly
due to the hyperarousal and associated behaviours
common in the syndrome. Therefore, the impor-
tance of identifying the function of challenging
behaviour in the context of the child’s environment
and genetic aetiology is likely to make a significant
contribution to understanding what might help in
the classroom. Educators are likely to benefit from
a good knowledge of behavioural principles and the
use of functional behavioural assessment is likely to
be particularly important with regard to under-
standing the role of the environment and the role of
reinforcement in maintaining a problematic behav-
iour whether or not that behaviour is common in a
particular syndrome or not. Therefore, knowledge of
a behavioural phenotype should contribute to an
understanding of a problematic behaviour and
should complement knowledge of sound behav-
ioural principles and inform functional assessment
and subsequent behavioural intervention.
Summary and conclusion
There are now a good number of studies on the
cognitive and behavioural aspects of various genetic
syndromes associated with ID and/or special educa-
tion needs, and studies are becoming more sophisti-
cated with regard to design and more specific with
regard to aspects of functioning being assessed.
These studies suggest that there are distinctive
‘behavioural phenotypes’ associated with some syn-
dromes. However, the notion of a behavioural phe-
notype should not be taken to imply homogeneity
of profiles within syndromes and within-syndrome
variability is common. It is likely that many children
will ‘fit’ the proposed ‘behavioural phenotype’ for
the syndrome across a number of domains but on
occasions the they will not ‘fit’ the phenotype and
each child will have an individual phenotype which
may or may not conform to the group phenotype.
There is as of yet, limited evidence on aetiology-
related interventions or the usefulness of adopting
an ‘aetiological approach’ in the classroom, and the
issues surrounding the potential for developing
phenotype-specific intervention practices are
complex (Fidler et al. 2007). While it may be diffi-
cult to carry out studies on specific interventions
for children with specific syndromes, research on
the manifestation of proposed cognitive and behav-
ioural profiles in the classroom is important. It is
not clear how teachers who are well informed about
proposed cognitive and behavioural profiles of a
particular syndrome apply educational practices in
the classroom. It is therefore, important that
research is carried out with those who work in edu-
cational settings with children with genetic syn-
dromes, to ascertain their views on the most
efficacious practices. As well as research on how
elucidated cognitive and behavioural profiles are
manifest in school settings, there is a need for work
on how issues such as co-morbidity with conditions
such as ASD, affect educational practice for chil-
dren with genetic syndromes.
There is a particular need to examine whether
commonly used instructional methods (e.g.
phonics-based reading instruction) work in particu-
lar genetic syndromes and how such approaches
might compare with other instructional methods. In
this way, it may be possible to select approaches
that lead to faster rates of skill acquisition and help
decide whether to focus on capitalising on pheno-
typic strengths or whether to intensively target phe-
notypic weaknesses. Although intervention studies
in genetic syndromes are difficult to carry out, a
multiple baseline individual and small group studies
approach can start to answer questions about phe-
notypic approaches with relatively small number of
participants. In the absence of evidence-based
interventions school-based practitioners and educa-
tors will continue to base their intervention
approaches on prior clinical experience and profes-
sional judgement rather than on evidence-based
research. Much of the research in syndromes is
driven by those working in clinical- or university-
based research settings but there is also a need for
classroom-based research and this will require
strong links with those who work in educational
settings. Research outlining current practices and
teacher and parent views of their efficacy will be
useful and not as resource intensive as intervention
studies.
940
Journal of Intellectual Disability Research VOLUME 56 PART
10 OCTOBER 2012
C. Reilly • Aetiology in the classroom
© 2012 The Author. Journal of Intellectual Disability Research
© 2012 Blackwell Publishing Ltd
With regard to current classroom practice and
prospective training for educators the approach pre-
sented here may be helpful with regard to support-
ing educators understanding of when, and if,
aetiology might impact on a child’s learning or
behaviour. As Einfeld (2005) suggests educators
cannot be expected to be aware of the behavioural
phenotypes of all syndromes but being aware that
different aetiologies can impact learning and behav-
iour may allow teachers to seek out further advice
and support. The concept of behavioural pheno-
types implies probability, and a knowledge of inter-
ventions that are likely to work for the majority of
children with a syndrome should be considered
along with other sources of data (e.g. individual
assessment data), when formulating recommenda-
tions for educational and behavioural interventions.
In this way a ‘label’ can become ‘an enabler’
(O’Brien 2002), signposting educational profession-
als to interventions that are more likely to lead to
positive outcomes for the affected children. Knowl-
edge of a genetic syndrome can help anticipate
potential developmental vulnerabilities and resilien-
cies, and in this way phenotypic research may make
it possible to ‘know where to look’ for potential vul-
nerabilities in early development (Fidler et al. 2007)
and potential difficulties during the school years.
However, even for children with distinct genetic
aetiologies, aetiology is just one aspect of under-
standing a child’s needs which will reflect a range of
bio-psycho-social factors.
References
Abbeduto L. & Keller-Bell Y. (2002) Review of genetics
and mental retardation syndromes: a new look at behav-
ior and interventions. American Journal of Mental Retar-
dation 107, 412–4.
Alferink L. A. & Farmer-Dougan V. (2010) Brain-(not)
based education: dangers of misunderstanding and mis-
application of neuroscience research. Exceptionality 18,
42–52.
Antshel K. M., Fremont W., Roizen N. J., Sphrintzen R.,
Higgins A. M., Dhamoon B. A. et al. (2006) ADHD,
major depressive disorder, and simple phobias are
prevalent psychiatric conditions in youth with velocar-
diofacial syndrome. Journal of the American Academy of
Child and Adolescent Psychiatry 45, 596–603.
Antshel K. M., Freemont W. & Kates W. R. (2008) The
neurocognitive phenotype in Velo-cardio-facial syn-
drome: a developmental perspective. Developmental Dis-
abilities Research Reviews 14, 43–51.
Backes M., Genç B., Schrek J., Dorfler W., Lehmkuhl G.
& Von Gotard A. (2000) Cognitive and behavioral
profile of fragile X boys: correlations to molecular data.
American Journal of Medical Genetics 95, 150–6.
Becerra A. M., John A. E., Peregrine E. & Mervis C. B.
(2008) Reading Abilities of 9–17-Year-Olds with Williams
Syndrome: Impact of Reading Method. Symposium on
Research in Child Language Disorders, Madison, WI.
Belser R. C. & Sudhalter V. (2001) Conversational charac-
teristics of children with fragile X syndrome: repetitive
speech. American Journal of Mental Retardation 106, 128–
38.
Berument S. K., Rutter M., Lord C., Pickles A. & Bailey
A. (1999) Autism screening questionnaire: diagnostic
validity. The British Journal of Psychiatry 175, 444–51.
Blackhurst A. E. & Berndine W. H. (1993) An Introduction
to Special Education. HarperCollins, New York.
Blomberg S., Rosander M. & Andersson G. (2006) Fears,
hyperacusis and musicality in Williams syndrome.
Research in Developmental Disabilities 27, 668–80.
Braden M. (2002) Academic Interventions. In: Fragile X
Syndrome: Diagnosis, Treatment and Research, 3rd edn.
(eds R. J. Hagerman & P. J. Hagerman), pp. 428–64.
Johns Hopkins University Press, Baltimore, MD.
Braden M. L. & Riley K. (2006) Educational strategies.
The UK Fragile X Society Family Conference, Belfast, 11
November 2006.
Brown-Chidsey R. & Steege M. W. (2005) Response to
Intervention: Principles and Strategies for Effective Practice.
Guildford Press, New York.
Brun C., Obiols J. E., Cheema A., O’Connor R., Riddle
J., DiMaria M. et al. (1995) Longitudinal IQ changes in
fragile X females. Developmental Brain Dysfunction 8,
230–41.
Buckley S. (1995) Teaching children with Down syndrome
to read and write. In: Down Syndrome: Living Learning
in the Community (eds L. Nadel & D. Rosenthal), pp.
158–69. Wiley-Liss, New York.
Butler M. G. (1990) Prader-Willi syndrome: current
understanding of cause and diagnosis. American Journal
of Medical Genetics 35, 319–32.
Campbell L. & Swillen A. (2005) The cognitive spectrum
in velo-cardio-facial syndrome. In: Velo-Cardio-Facial
Syndrome – A Model for Understanding Microdeletion Dis-
orders (eds K. C. Murphy & P. J. Scambler), pp. 147–64.
Cambridge University press, Cambridge.
Campbell L., Daly E., Toal F., Stevens A., Azuma R.,
Karmiloff-Smith A. et al. (2009) Brain structural differ-
ences associated with the behavioural research in Will-
iams syndrome. Brain Research 1258, 96–107.
Campbell L., McCabe K., Leadbetter K., Schall U.,
Loughland C. & Rih D. (2010) Visual scanning of faces
941
Journal of Intellectual Disability Research VOLUME 56 PART
10 OCTOBER 2012
C. Reilly • Aetiology in the classroom
© 2012 The Author. Journal of Intellectual Disability Research
© 2012 Blackwell Publishing Ltd
in 22q11.2 deletion syndrome: attention to the mouth
or eyes? Psychiatry Research 177, 211–5.
Cassidy S. B. (1997) Prader-Willi syndrome. Journal of
Medical Genetics 34, 917–23.
Chedd N., Levine K. & Wharton R. H. (2006) Educa-
tional considerations for children with Prader-Willi Syn-
drome. In: Management of Prader-Willi Syndrome, 3rd
edn. (eds M. G. Butler, P. D. K. Lee & B. Y. Whitman),
pp. 302–16. Springer, New York.
Clifford S., Dissanayke C., Bui Q. M., Huggins R., Tyalor
A. K., Loesch D. Z. et al. (2007) Autism spectrum phe-
notype in males and females with fragile X full mutation
and permutation. Journal of Autism and Developmental
Disorders 37, 738–47.
Cohen I. L. (1995) A theoretical analysis of the role of
hyperarousal in the learning and behaviour of fragile X
males. Mental Retardation and Developmental Disabilities
Research Reviews 1, 286–91.
Cologon K., Cupples L. & Wyver S. (2011) Effects of tar-
geted reading instruction on phonological awareness,
phonic decoding and reading comprehension in children
with Down syndrome. American Journal on Intellectual
and Developmental Disabilities 116, 111–29.
Cordeiro L., Ballinger E., Hagerman R. & Hessl D. (2011)
Clinical assessment of DSM-IV anxiety disorders in
fragile X syndrome: prevalence and characterisation.
Journal of Neurodevelopmental Disorders 3, 57–67.
Cornish K., Swainson R., Cunnington R., Wilding J.,
Morris P. & Jackson G. (2004) Do women with fragile
X syndrome have problems in switching attention: pre-
liminary findings from ERP and fMRI. Brain and Cogni-
tion 54, 235–9.
Cornish K. M., Munir F. & Cross G. (1998) The nature
of the spatial deficit in young females with fragile X
syndrome: a neuropsychological and molecular perspec-
tive. Neuropsychologia 36, 1239–46.
Cornish K. M., Munir F. & Cross G. (1999) Spatial cog-
nition in males with Fragile X syndrome: evidence for a
neuropsychological phenotype. Cortex 35, 263–71.
Couzens D., Cuskelly M. & Haynes M. (2011) Cognitive
development and Down syndrome: age-related change
on the Stanford-Binet test (fourth edition). American
Journal on Intellectual and Developmental Disabilities 116,
181–204.
Cronister A., Hagerman R. J., Wittenberger M. & Amiri
K. (1991) Mental impairment in cytogenetically positive
fragile X females. American Journal of Medical Genetics
38, 503–4.
Cupples L. & Iacono T. (2002) The efficacy of ‘whole
word’ versus ‘analytic’ reading instruction for children
with Down syndrome. Reading and Writing: An Interdisci-
plinary Journal 15, 549–74.
Cutler-Landsman D. (2007) Educating Children with Velo-
Cardio-Facial Syndrome. Plural Publishing, San Diego,
CA.
Deruelle C., Mancini J., Livet M. O., Casse-Perot C. & de
Schonene S. (1999) Configural and local processing of
faces in children with Williams syndrome. Brain and
Cognition 41, 278–98.
Dew-Hughes D. (2004) Educating Children with Fragile X
Syndrome. RoutledgeFalmer, London.
Dykens E. M. (1995) Measuring behavioral phenotypes:
provocations from the ‘new genetics. American Journal of
Mental Retardation 99, 522–32.
Dykens E. M. (1999) Direct effects of mental retardation
syndromes: maladaptive behavior and psychopathology.
International Review of Research in Mental Retardation 22,
1–23.
Dykens E. M. (2000) Annotation: psychopathology in
children with intellectual disability. Journal of Child Psy-
chology and Psychiatry 41, 407–17.
Dykens E. M. & Hodapp R. M. (1997) Treatment issues
in genetic mental retardation syndromes. Professional
Psychology: Research and Practice 28, 263–70.
Dykens E. M. & Hodapp R. M. (2001) Research in
mental retardation: toward an etiologic approach.
Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry 42, 49–71.
Dykens E. M., Hodapp R. M., Walsh K. & Nash L. J.
(1992) Profile, correlates, and trajectories of intelligence
in Prader-Willi syndrome. Journal of the American
Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry 31, 1125–30.
Dykens E. M., Hodapp R. M., Ort S. I. & Leckman J. F.
(1993) Trajectory of adaptive behaviour in males with
fragile X syndrome. Journal of Autism and Developmental
Disorders 23, 135–45.
Dykens E. M., Hodapp R. M. & Evans D. W. (1994) Pro-
files and development of adaptive behaviour in children
with Down syndrome. American Journal of Mental Retar-
dation 98, 580–7.
Dykens E. M., Hodapp R. M. & Finucane B. (2000)
Genetics and Mental Retardation Syndromes: A New Look
at Behaviour and Interventions. Brookes, Baltimore, MD.
Einfeld S. (2005) Behaviour problems in children with
genetic disorders causing intellectual disabilities. Educa-
tional Psychology 25, 341–6.
Ekstein S., Weill B., Glick M., Kay B. & Berger I. (2011)
Down syndrome and attention-deficit/hyperactivity dis-
order (ADHD). Journal of Child Neurology 26, 1290–5.
Esbensen A. J. & Seltzer M. M. (2011) Accounting for the
‘Down Syndrome Advantage’. American Journal on Intel-
lectual and Developmental Disabilities 116, 3–15.
Feinstein C., Eliez S., Blasey C. & Reiss A. L. (2002) Psy-
chiatric disorders and behavioral problems in children
with velocardiofacial syndrome: usefulness as pheno-
typic indicators of schizophrenia risk. Biological Psychia-
try 51, 312–8.
Fidler D. J., Hodapp R. M. & Dykens E. M. (2002)
Behavioral phenotypes and special education: parent
report of educational issues for children with Down syn-
942
Journal of Intellectual Disability Research VOLUME 56 PART
10 OCTOBER 2012
C. Reilly • Aetiology in the classroom
© 2012 The Author. Journal of Intellectual Disability Research
© 2012 Blackwell Publishing Ltd
drome, Prader-Willi syndrome, and Williams syndrome.
The Journal of Special Education 36, 80–8.
Fidler D. J., Philofsky A. & Hepburn S. L. (2007) Lan-
guage phenotypes and intervention planning: bridging
research and practice. Mental Retardation and Develop-
mental Disabilities Research Reviews 13, 47–57.
Fine S. E., Weissman A., Gerdes M., Pinto-Martin J.,
Zackai E. H., McDonald-McGinn D. M. et al. (2005)
Autism spectrum disorders and symptoms in children
with molecularly confirmed 22q11.2 deletion syndrome.
Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders 35, 461–
70.
Flynt J. & Yule W. (1994) Behavioural phenotyes. In: Child
and Adolescent Psychiatry: Modern Approaches, 3rd edn.
(eds M. Rutter, E. Taylor & L. Hersov), pp. 666–87.
Blackwell Scientific, London.
Forness S. & Kavale K. (1994) The Balkanization of
special education: proliferation of categories for ‘new’
behavioural disorders. Education and Treatment of Chil-
dren 17, 215–27.
Freund L. S. & Reiss A. L. (1991) Cognitive profiles asso-
ciated with the fra(X) syndrome in males and females.
American Journal of Medical Genetics 38, 542–7.
Freund L. S., Reiss A. L. & Abrams M. (1993) Psychiatric
disorders associated with fragile X in young females.
Pediatrics 91, 321–9.
Garner C., Callias M. & Turk J. (1999) Executive function
and theory of mind performance of boys with fragile X
syndrome. Journal of Intellectual Disability Research 43,
466–74.
Gath A. & Gumley D. (1986) Behaviour problems in
retarded children with special reference to Down’s syn-
drome. The British Journal of Psychiatry 149, 156–61.
Goetz K., Hulme C., Brigstocke S., Carroll J. M., Nasir
L. & Snowling M. (2008) Training reading and
phoneme awareness skills in children with Down syn-
drome. Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal
21, 395–412.
Golding-Kushner K. J., Weller G. & Sphrintzen R. J.
(1985) Velo-cardio-facial syndrome: language and psy-
chological profiles. Journal of Craniofacial Genetics and
Developmental Biology 5, 259–66.
Goodship J., Cross I., Li Ling J. & Wren C. (1998) A
population study of chromosome 22q11 deletions in
infancy. Archives of Disease in Childhood 79, 348–51.
Gothelf D., Presburger G., Levy D., Nahmani A., Burg
M., Berant M. et al. (2004) Genetic, developmental,
and physical factors associated with attention deficit
hyperactivity disorder in patients, with velocardiofacial
syndrome. American Journal of Medical Genetics. Part B
126B, 116–21.
Green T., Gothelf D., Glaser B., Debbané M., Frisch A.,
Kotler M. et al. (2009) Psychiatric disorders and intel-
lectual functioning throughout development in velo-
cardio-facial syndrome (22q11.2 deletion) syndrome.
Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent
Psychiatry 48, 1060–8.
Hagerman R. J. (1987) Fragile X syndrome. Current Prob-
lems in Pediatrics 17, 627–74.
Hall S. S. (2009) Treatments for fragile X syndrome: a
closer look at the data. Developmental Disabilities
Research Reviews 15, 353–60.
Hall S. S., Debernardis G. M. & Reiss A. L. (2006) The
acquisition of stimulus equivalence in individuals with
fragile X syndrome. Journal of Intellectual Disability
Research 50, 643–51.
Hall S. S., Maynes N. P. & Reiss A. L. (2009) Using per-
centile schedules to increase eye contact duration in
children with fragile X syndrome. Journal of Applied
Behavior Analysis 42, 171–6.
Hallahan D. P. & Kauffman J. M. (2000) Exceptional Chil-
dren: Introduction to Special Education, 8th edn. Allyn and
Bacon, Needham Heights, MA.
Haxby J. V. (1989) Neuropsychological evaluations of
adults with Down’s syndrome: patterns of selective
impairment in non-demented old adults. Journal of
Mental Deficiency Research 88, 193–210.
Hills-Epstein J. L., Riley K. & Sobesky W. E. (2002) The
treatment of emotional and behavioural problems. In:
Fragile X Syndrome: Diagnosis, Treatment and Research,
3rd edn. (eds R. J. Hagerman & P. J. Hagerman), pp.
339–62. Johns Hopkins University Press, Baltimore,
MD.
Hodapp R. M. (1997) Direct and indirect behavioral
effects of different genetic disorders of mental retarda-
tion. American Journal of Mental Retardation 102, 67–79.
Hodapp R. M. (2004) Behavioural phenotypes: going
beyond the two group approach. International Review of
Research in Mental Retardation 29, 1–30.
Hodapp R. M. & Dykens E. M. (2001) Strengthening
behavioral research on genetic mental retardation syn-
dromes. American Journal of Mental Retardation 106,
4–15.
Hodapp R. M. & Dykens E. M. (2009) Intellectual dis-
abilities and child psychiatry: looking to the future.
Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry 50, 99–107.
Hodapp R. M. & Fidler D. J. (1999) Special education
and genetics: connections for the 21st century. The
Journal of Special Education 33, 130–7.
Hodapp R. M., Leckman J. F., Dykens S., Sparrow D.,
Zelinsky D. & Ort S. I. (1992) K-ABC profiles in chil-
dren with fragile X syndrome, Down syndrome, and
non-specific mental retardation. American Journal of
Mental Retardation 97, 39–46.
Hodapp R. M., Evans D. & Gray F. L. (1999) Intellectual
development in children with Down syndrome. In:
Down Syndrome: A Review of Current Knowledge (eds
J. A. Rodal, J. Perera & L. Nadel), pp. 124–32. Whurr
Publishers, London.
943
Journal of Intellectual Disability Research VOLUME 56 PART
10 OCTOBER 2012
C. Reilly • Aetiology in the classroom
© 2012 The Author. Journal of Intellectual Disability Research
© 2012 Blackwell Publishing Ltd
Hodapp R. M., Des Jardin J. L. & Ricci L. A. (2003)
Genetic syndromes of Mental Retardation: should they
matter for the early interventionist? Infants and Young
Children 16, 152–60.
Holland A. J., Whittington J. E., Butler J., Webb T., Boer
H. & Clarke D. (2003) Behavioural phenotypes associ-
ated with specific genetic disorders: evidence from a
population-based study of people with Prader-Willi syn-
drome. Psychological Medicine 33, 141–53.
Horsteimer D. (2004) Teaching Math to People with Down
Syndrome and Other Hands on Learners. Woodbine
House, Bethesda, MA.
Howlin P., Davies M. & Udwin O. (1998) Cognitive func-
tioning in adults with Williams syndrome. Journal of
Child Psychology and Psychiatry 39, 163–89.
Jones W., Bellugi U., Lai Z., Chiles M., Reilly J., Lincoln
A. et al. (2000) Hypersociability: the social and affective
phenotype of Williams syndrome. Journal of Cognitive
Neuroscience 12, 30–46.
Kates W., Antshel J. M., Fremont W. P., Sphrintzen R. J.,
Strunge L. A., Burnette C. P. et al. (2007) Comparing
phenotypes in patients with idiopathic autism to
patients with velocardiofacial syndrome (22q11 DS)
with and without Autism. American Journal of Medical
Genetics. Part A 143A, 2642–50.
Kau A. S. M., Reider E. E., Payne L., Meyer W. A. &
Freund L. (2000) Early behaviour signs of psychiatric
phenotypes in fragile X syndrome. American Journal of
Mental Retardation 105, 266–9.
Kaufman A. S. & Kaufman N. L. (1983) Kaufman Assess-
ment Battery for Children (K-ABC). American Guidance
Service, Circle Pines, MN.
Koenig K., Klin A. & Schultz R. (2005) Deficits in social
attribution ability in Prader–Willi syndrome. Journal of
Autism and Developmental Disorders 34, 573–82.
Langthorne P., McGill P. & O’Reilly M. (2007) Incorpo-
rating ‘motivation’ into the functional analysis of chal-
lenging behaviour. On the interactive and integrative
potential of the motivating operation. Behavior Modifica-
tion 31, 466–87.
Langthorne P., McGill P., O’Reilly M., Lang R., Machal-
icek W., Chan J. M. et al. (2011) Examining the func-
tion of problem behaviour in fragile X syndrome:
preliminary experimental analysis. American Journal on
Intellectual and Developmental Disabilities 116, 65–80.
Lee T. H., Blasey C. M., Dyer-Friedman J., Glaser B.,
Reiss A. & Eliez S. (2005) From research to practice:
teacher and Paediatrician awareness of phenotypic traits
in neurogenetic syndromes. American Journal of Mental
Retardation 110, 100–6.
Levy Y. & Antebi V. (2004) Word reading and reading-
related skills in Hebrew-speaking adolescents with Will-
iams syndrome. Neurocase 10, 444–51.
Levy Y. & Ebstein R. P. (2009) Research review: crossing
syndrome boundaries in the search for endophenotypes.
Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry 50, 657–68.
Leyfer O. T., Woodruff-Borden J., Klein-Tasman B. P.,
Fricke J. S. & Mervis C. B. (2006) Prevalence of psychi-
atric disorders in 4 to 16-year-olds with Williams syn-
drome. American Journal of Medical Genetics. Part B
141B, 615–22.
Lowenthal R., Paula C. S., Schwartzman J. S., Brunioni
D. & Mercadante M. T. (2007) Prevalence of pervasive
developmental disorders in Down’s syndrome. Journal of
Autism and Developmental Disorders 37, 1394–5.
Martens M. A., Wilson S. J. & Reutens D. C. (2008)
Research review: Williams syndrome: a critical review of
the cognitive, behavioral, and neuroanatomical pheno-
type. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry 49, 576–
608.
Mazzocco M. M. M., Pennington B. F. & Hagerman R. J.
(1993) The neurocognitive phenotype of female carriers
of Fragile X: additional evidence for specificity. Journal
of Developmental and Behavioral Pediatrics 14, 328–35.
Mervis C. B. & John A. E. (2010) Cognitive and behav-
ioral characteristics of children with Williams syndrome:
implications for intervention approaches. American
Journal of Medical Genetics. Part C 154C, 229–48.
Mervis C. B. & Klein-Tasman B. P. (2000) Williams syn-
drome: cognition, personality, and adaptive behavior.
Mental Retardation and Developmental Disabilities
Research Reviews 6, 148–58.
Mervis C. M. & Morris C. A. (2007) Williams syndrome.
In: Neurogenetic Developmental Disorders:Variation of
Manifestation in Childhood (eds M. M. M. Mazzocco &
J. L. Ross), pp. 199–262. MIT Press, Cambridge, MA.
Miller J. F. (1992) Development of speech and language
in children with Down syndrome. In: Down Syndrome:
Advances in Medical Care (eds I. T. Lott & E. E.
McCoy), pp. 39–50. Wiley, Chichester.
Moss J. & Howlin P. (2009) Autism spectrum disorders in
genetic syndromes: implications for diagnosis, interven-
tion and understanding the wider autism spectrum
disorder population. Journal of Intellectual Disability
Research 53, 852–73.
Murphy K. C., Jones L. A. & Owen M. J. (1999) High
rates of schizophrenia in adults with velo-cardio-facial
syndrome. Archives of General Psychiatry 56, 949–5.
Niklasson L., Rasmussen P., Óskarsdóttir S. & Gillberg C.
(2002) Chromosome 22q.11 deletion syndrome
(CATCH22): neuropsychiatric and neuropsychological
aspects. Developmental Medicine and Child Neurology 44,
44–50.
Niklasson L., Rasmussen P., Óskarsdóttir S. & Gillberg C.
(2005) Attention defecits in children with 22q.11 dele-
tion syndrome. Developmental Medicine and Child Neurol-
ogy 47, 803–7.
944
Journal of Intellectual Disability Research VOLUME 56 PART
10 OCTOBER 2012
C. Reilly • Aetiology in the classroom
© 2012 The Author. Journal of Intellectual Disability Research
© 2012 Blackwell Publishing Ltd
Niklasson L., Rasmussen P., Óskarsdóttir S. & Gillberg C.
(2009) Autism, ADHD, mental retardation, and behav-
ior problems in 100 individuals with 22q11 deletion syn-
drome. Research in Developmental Disabilities 30, 763–73.
O’Brien G. (2002) The clinical relevance of behavioural phe-
notypes. In: Behavioural Phenotypes in Clinical Practice
(ed. G. O’Brien), pp. 1–12. Mac Keith Press, London.
Oelwein P. L. (1995) Teaching Reading to Children with
Down Syndrome. A Guide for Parents and Teachers. Wood-
bine House, Bethesda, MA.
Porter M. A. & Coltheart M. (2005) Cognitive heteroge-
neity in Williams syndrome. Developmental Neuropsychol-
ogy 27, 275–306.
Pueschel S. M., Gallagher P. L., Zartler A. S. & Pezzullo
J. C. (1987) Cognitive and learning processes in chil-
dren with Down syndrome. Research in Developmental
Disabilities 8, 21–37.
Radstaake M., Didden R., Bolio M., Lang R., Lancioni
G. E. & Curfs L. M. G. (2011) Functional assessment
and behavioral treatment of skin picking in a teenage
girl with Prader-Willi syndrome. Clinical Case Studies 10,
67–78.
Rosner B. A., Hodapp R. M., Fidler D. J., Sagun J. N. &
Dykens E. M. (2004) Social competence in persons
with Prader-Willi, Williams and Down syndromes.
Journal of Applied Research in Intellectual Disabilities 17,
209–17.
Rutter M., Bailey A. & Lord C. (2003) The Social Commu-
nication Questionnaire. Western Psychological Services,
Los Angeles, CA.
Saunders S. (1999) Teaching children with fragile X syn-
drome. British Journal of Special Education 26, 6–79.
Scharfenaker S., O’Connor R., Stackhouse T., Braden
M. L. & Gray K. (2002) An integrated approach to
intervention. In: Fragile X Syndrome: Diagnosis, Treatment
and Research, 3rd edn. (eds R. J. Hagerman & P. J.
Hagerman), pp. 363 –427. Johns Hopkins University
Press, Baltimore, MD.
Searcy Y. M., Lincoln A., Rose F., Klima E., Bavar N. &
Korenberg J. R. (2004) The relationship between age
and IQ in adults with Williams syndrome. American
Journal of Mental Retardation 109, 231–6.
Semel E. & Rosner S. R. (2003) Understanding Williams
Syndrome: Behavioral Patterns and Interventions.
Lawrence Erlbaum, Mahwah, NJ.
Sherman S. (2002) Epidemiology. In: Fragile X Syndrome:
Diagnosis, Treatment and Research, 3rd edn. (eds R. J.
Hagerman & P. J. Hagerman), pp. 136–68. Johns
Hopkins University Press, Baltimore, MD.
Sherman S. L., Allen E. G., Bean L. H. & Freeman S. B.
(2007) Epidemiology of Down syndrome. Mental Retar-
dation and Developmental Disabilities Research Reviews 13,
221–7.
Shprintzen R. J. (2008) Velo-cardio-facial syndrome: 30
years of study. Developmental Disabilities Research Reviews
14, 3–10.
Starr E. M., Foy J. B., Cramer K. M. & Singh H. (2006)
How are schools doing? Parental perceptions of children
with autism spectrum disorder, Down syndrome and
learning disabilities. Education and Training in Develop-
mental Disabilities 41, 315–32.
Stauder J. E., Brinkman M. J. & Curfs L. M. G. (2002)
Multi-modal P3 deflation of event related brain activity
in Prader-Willi syndrome. Neuroscience Letters 327,
99–102.
Stokes J. V. & Luiselli J. K. (2009) Applied behavior analy-
sis assessment and intervention for health threatening
self-injury in an adult with Prader–Willisyndrome. Clini-
cal Case Studies 8, 38–47.
Stromme P., Bjornstad M. D. & Ramstad M. D. (2002)
Prevalence estimation of Williams syndrome. Journal of
Child Neurology 17, 269–71.
Swillen A., Vanputte L., Cracco J., Maes B., Ghesquiere
P., Devriendt K. et al. (1997) Neuropsychological, learn-
ing and psychosocial profile of primary school aged
children with the velo-cardio-facial syndrome (22q11
deletion): evidence for a nonverbal learning disability?
Child Neuropsychology 5, 230–41.
Symons F. J., Clark R. D., Roberts J. P. & Bailey D. B.
(2001) Classroom behaviour of elementary school-age
boys with fragile X syndrome. The Journal of Special
Education 34, 194–202.
Udwin O., Yule W. & Howlin P. (2007) Williams Syn-
drome. Guidelines for Teachers. The Williams Syndrome
Foundation, Tonbridge.
Van der Molen M. J. W., Huizanga M., Huizenga H. M.,
Ridderinkhof K. R., Van der Molen M. W., Hamel
B. J. C. et al. (2010) Profiling fragile X syndrome in
males: strengths and weaknesses in cognitive abilities.
Research in Developmental Disabilities 31, 426–39.
Veltman M. W. M., Thompson R. J., Roberts S. E.,
Thomas N. S., Whittington J. & Bolton P. (2004)
Prader-Willi syndrome: a study comparing deletion and
uniparental disomy cases with reference to autism spec-
trum disorders. European Child and Adolescent Psychiatry
13, 42–50.
de Vries B. B. A., Wiegers A. M., Smits A. P. T., Mohkam-
sing S., Duivenvoorden H. J., Fryns J. P. et al. (1996)
Mental status of females with the FMR1 gene full muta-
tion. American Journal of Medical Genetics 58, 1025–32.
Wang Y. K., Samos C. H., Peoples R., Perez-Jurado L. A.,
Nusse R. & Francke U. (1997) A novel human homo-
logue of the Drosophilia frizzled wnt receptor gene
binds wingless protein and is in the Williams syndrome
deletion at 7q11.23. Human Molecular Genetics 6, 465–
72.
Whitman B. Y. & Jackson K. (2006) Tools for Psychologi-
cal and Behavioural Management. In: Management of
945
Journal of Intellectual Disability Research VOLUME 56 PART
10 OCTOBER 2012
C. Reilly • Aetiology in the classroom
© 2012 The Author. Journal of Intellectual Disability Research
© 2012 Blackwell Publishing Ltd
Prader-Willi Syndrome, 3rd edn. (eds M. G. Butler,
P. D. K. Lee & B. Y. Whitman), pp. 317–43. Springer,
New York.
Whittington J. & Holland T. (2004) Prader-Willi Syndrome.
Development and Manifestations. University Press,
Cambridge.
Wigren M. & Hansen S. (2005) ADHD symptoms and
insistence on sameness in Prader-Willi syndrome.
Journal of Intellectual Disability Research 49, 449–56.
Woodcock K. A., Oliver C. & Humphreys G. W. (2009)
Task switching deficits and repetitive behaviour in
genetic neurodevelopmental disorders: data from chil-
dren with Prader-Willi syndrome chromosome
15q11-q13 deletion and boys with fragile X syndrome.
Cognitive Neuropsychology 26, 172–94.
Woodcock K. A., Humphreys G. W., Oliver C. & Hansen
P. C. (2010) Neural correlates of task switching in
paternal 15q11-q13 deletion Prader-Willi syndrome.
Brain Research 1363, 128–42.
Woodcock R. W. & Johnson M. B. (1990) Woodcock-
Johnson Psycho-Educational Battery-Revised. Riverside,
Itasca, IL.
York A., Von Fraunhofer N., Turk J. & Sedgwick P.
(1999) Fragile X syndrome, Down’s syndrome and
autism: awareness and knowledge amongst special
educators. Journal of Intellectual Disability Research 43,
314–24.
Zwaigenbaum L., Dick P., Handley-Derry M., Malone M.
& Jacobson S. (2006) ‘N of 1’ trails of methylphenidate
in two children with Williams syndrome and attention
deficit hyperactivity disorder. Journal of Developmental
and Physical Disabilities 18, 45–58.
Accepted 31 January 2012
946
Journal of Intellectual Disability Research VOLUME 56 PART
10 OCTOBER 2012
C. Reilly • Aetiology in the classroom
© 2012 The Author. Journal of Intellectual Disability Research
© 2012 Blackwell Publishing Ltd
This document is a scanned copy of a printed document. No
warranty is given about the accuracy of the copy.
Users should refer to the original published version of the
material.

More Related Content

Similar to Genetic disorders: Understanding behavioural phenotypes to inform classroom interventions

Running Head MANAGEMENT OF DOWN SYNDROME LITERATURE REVIEW .docx
Running Head MANAGEMENT OF DOWN SYNDROME LITERATURE REVIEW  .docxRunning Head MANAGEMENT OF DOWN SYNDROME LITERATURE REVIEW  .docx
Running Head MANAGEMENT OF DOWN SYNDROME LITERATURE REVIEW .docxcowinhelen
 
Autism Spectrum Disorder A case study of Mikey.pdf
Autism Spectrum Disorder  A case study of Mikey.pdfAutism Spectrum Disorder  A case study of Mikey.pdf
Autism Spectrum Disorder A case study of Mikey.pdfKathryn Patel
 
Au Psy492 W7 A2 Pp Golub R
Au Psy492 W7 A2 Pp Golub RAu Psy492 W7 A2 Pp Golub R
Au Psy492 W7 A2 Pp Golub RRachel Golub
 
Child with Special Needs Part 1
Child with Special Needs Part 1Child with Special Needs Part 1
Child with Special Needs Part 1Dominick Maino
 
Child with Special Needs Part 1
Child with Special Needs   Part 1Child with Special Needs   Part 1
Child with Special Needs Part 1Dominick Maino
 
Riverpoint writer autism
Riverpoint writer autismRiverpoint writer autism
Riverpoint writer autismJody Marvin
 
Child Development - 2022 - Savopoulos - Intimate partner violence during infa...
Child Development - 2022 - Savopoulos - Intimate partner violence during infa...Child Development - 2022 - Savopoulos - Intimate partner violence during infa...
Child Development - 2022 - Savopoulos - Intimate partner violence during infa...ConstanzaMeneses7
 
Scientific Method- ED's
Scientific Method- ED'sScientific Method- ED's
Scientific Method- ED'sTaylor Hartman
 
What is Developmental Language Disorder
What is Developmental Language DisorderWhat is Developmental Language Disorder
What is Developmental Language DisorderDorothy Bishop
 
ADHD and Chiropractic
ADHD and  ChiropracticADHD and  Chiropractic
ADHD and Chiropracticcsmart4
 
Genetic and Environmental FoundationsChapter 2.docx
Genetic and Environmental FoundationsChapter 2.docxGenetic and Environmental FoundationsChapter 2.docx
Genetic and Environmental FoundationsChapter 2.docxbudbarber38650
 
EMOTIONAL REGULATION AND COGNITIVE FLEXIBILITY AS PREDICTORS OF MATERNAL ACCE...
EMOTIONAL REGULATION AND COGNITIVE FLEXIBILITY AS PREDICTORS OF MATERNAL ACCE...EMOTIONAL REGULATION AND COGNITIVE FLEXIBILITY AS PREDICTORS OF MATERNAL ACCE...
EMOTIONAL REGULATION AND COGNITIVE FLEXIBILITY AS PREDICTORS OF MATERNAL ACCE...indexPub
 
Returning genetic research results in neurodevelopmental disorders: report an...
Returning genetic research results in neurodevelopmental disorders: report an...Returning genetic research results in neurodevelopmental disorders: report an...
Returning genetic research results in neurodevelopmental disorders: report an...KBHN KT
 
Disabilities Slide Presentation
Disabilities Slide PresentationDisabilities Slide Presentation
Disabilities Slide PresentationCarolLWebster
 
Why do neurodevelopmental disorders co-occur?
Why do neurodevelopmental disorders co-occur?Why do neurodevelopmental disorders co-occur?
Why do neurodevelopmental disorders co-occur?Dorothy Bishop
 
Disability & special education
Disability & special educationDisability & special education
Disability & special educationDr Saim Ali soomro
 
Disability & special education
Disability & special educationDisability & special education
Disability & special educationDr Saim Ali soomro
 
Biological Foundations
Biological FoundationsBiological Foundations
Biological Foundationspuggirl
 

Similar to Genetic disorders: Understanding behavioural phenotypes to inform classroom interventions (20)

Running Head MANAGEMENT OF DOWN SYNDROME LITERATURE REVIEW .docx
Running Head MANAGEMENT OF DOWN SYNDROME LITERATURE REVIEW  .docxRunning Head MANAGEMENT OF DOWN SYNDROME LITERATURE REVIEW  .docx
Running Head MANAGEMENT OF DOWN SYNDROME LITERATURE REVIEW .docx
 
Autism Spectrum Disorder A case study of Mikey.pdf
Autism Spectrum Disorder  A case study of Mikey.pdfAutism Spectrum Disorder  A case study of Mikey.pdf
Autism Spectrum Disorder A case study of Mikey.pdf
 
Au Psy492 W7 A2 Pp Golub R
Au Psy492 W7 A2 Pp Golub RAu Psy492 W7 A2 Pp Golub R
Au Psy492 W7 A2 Pp Golub R
 
Child with Special Needs Part 1
Child with Special Needs Part 1Child with Special Needs Part 1
Child with Special Needs Part 1
 
Child with Special Needs Part 1
Child with Special Needs   Part 1Child with Special Needs   Part 1
Child with Special Needs Part 1
 
Riverpoint writer autism
Riverpoint writer autismRiverpoint writer autism
Riverpoint writer autism
 
Child Development - 2022 - Savopoulos - Intimate partner violence during infa...
Child Development - 2022 - Savopoulos - Intimate partner violence during infa...Child Development - 2022 - Savopoulos - Intimate partner violence during infa...
Child Development - 2022 - Savopoulos - Intimate partner violence during infa...
 
Pre .ppt 2
Pre .ppt 2Pre .ppt 2
Pre .ppt 2
 
Scientific Method- ED's
Scientific Method- ED'sScientific Method- ED's
Scientific Method- ED's
 
What is Developmental Language Disorder
What is Developmental Language DisorderWhat is Developmental Language Disorder
What is Developmental Language Disorder
 
ADHD in children.pptx
ADHD in children.pptxADHD in children.pptx
ADHD in children.pptx
 
ADHD and Chiropractic
ADHD and  ChiropracticADHD and  Chiropractic
ADHD and Chiropractic
 
Genetic and Environmental FoundationsChapter 2.docx
Genetic and Environmental FoundationsChapter 2.docxGenetic and Environmental FoundationsChapter 2.docx
Genetic and Environmental FoundationsChapter 2.docx
 
EMOTIONAL REGULATION AND COGNITIVE FLEXIBILITY AS PREDICTORS OF MATERNAL ACCE...
EMOTIONAL REGULATION AND COGNITIVE FLEXIBILITY AS PREDICTORS OF MATERNAL ACCE...EMOTIONAL REGULATION AND COGNITIVE FLEXIBILITY AS PREDICTORS OF MATERNAL ACCE...
EMOTIONAL REGULATION AND COGNITIVE FLEXIBILITY AS PREDICTORS OF MATERNAL ACCE...
 
Returning genetic research results in neurodevelopmental disorders: report an...
Returning genetic research results in neurodevelopmental disorders: report an...Returning genetic research results in neurodevelopmental disorders: report an...
Returning genetic research results in neurodevelopmental disorders: report an...
 
Disabilities Slide Presentation
Disabilities Slide PresentationDisabilities Slide Presentation
Disabilities Slide Presentation
 
Why do neurodevelopmental disorders co-occur?
Why do neurodevelopmental disorders co-occur?Why do neurodevelopmental disorders co-occur?
Why do neurodevelopmental disorders co-occur?
 
Disability & special education
Disability & special educationDisability & special education
Disability & special education
 
Disability & special education
Disability & special educationDisability & special education
Disability & special education
 
Biological Foundations
Biological FoundationsBiological Foundations
Biological Foundations
 

More from whitneyleman54422

In this unit, you will experience the powerful impact communication .docx
In this unit, you will experience the powerful impact communication .docxIn this unit, you will experience the powerful impact communication .docx
In this unit, you will experience the powerful impact communication .docxwhitneyleman54422
 
In this task, you will write an analysis (suggested length of 3–5 .docx
In this task, you will write an analysis (suggested length of 3–5 .docxIn this task, you will write an analysis (suggested length of 3–5 .docx
In this task, you will write an analysis (suggested length of 3–5 .docxwhitneyleman54422
 
In this SLP you will identify where the major transportation modes a.docx
In this SLP you will identify where the major transportation modes a.docxIn this SLP you will identify where the major transportation modes a.docx
In this SLP you will identify where the major transportation modes a.docxwhitneyleman54422
 
In this module the student will present writing which focuses attent.docx
In this module the student will present writing which focuses attent.docxIn this module the student will present writing which focuses attent.docx
In this module the student will present writing which focuses attent.docxwhitneyleman54422
 
In this module, we looked at a variety of styles in the Renaissa.docx
In this module, we looked at a variety of styles in the Renaissa.docxIn this module, we looked at a variety of styles in the Renaissa.docx
In this module, we looked at a variety of styles in the Renaissa.docxwhitneyleman54422
 
In this experiential learning experience, you will evaluate a health.docx
In this experiential learning experience, you will evaluate a health.docxIn this experiential learning experience, you will evaluate a health.docx
In this experiential learning experience, you will evaluate a health.docxwhitneyleman54422
 
In this essay you should combine your practice responding and analyz.docx
In this essay you should combine your practice responding and analyz.docxIn this essay you should combine your practice responding and analyz.docx
In this essay you should combine your practice responding and analyz.docxwhitneyleman54422
 
In this Discussion, pick one film to write about and answer ques.docx
In this Discussion, pick one film to write about and answer ques.docxIn this Discussion, pick one film to write about and answer ques.docx
In this Discussion, pick one film to write about and answer ques.docxwhitneyleman54422
 
In this assignment, you will identify and interview a family who.docx
In this assignment, you will identify and interview a family who.docxIn this assignment, you will identify and interview a family who.docx
In this assignment, you will identify and interview a family who.docxwhitneyleman54422
 
In this assignment, you will assess the impact of health legisla.docx
In this assignment, you will assess the impact of health legisla.docxIn this assignment, you will assess the impact of health legisla.docx
In this assignment, you will assess the impact of health legisla.docxwhitneyleman54422
 
In this assignment, you will create a presentation. Select a topic o.docx
In this assignment, you will create a presentation. Select a topic o.docxIn this assignment, you will create a presentation. Select a topic o.docx
In this assignment, you will create a presentation. Select a topic o.docxwhitneyleman54422
 
In this assignment, the student will understand the growth and devel.docx
In this assignment, the student will understand the growth and devel.docxIn this assignment, the student will understand the growth and devel.docx
In this assignment, the student will understand the growth and devel.docxwhitneyleman54422
 
In this assignment, I want you to locate two pieces of news detailin.docx
In this assignment, I want you to locate two pieces of news detailin.docxIn this assignment, I want you to locate two pieces of news detailin.docx
In this assignment, I want you to locate two pieces of news detailin.docxwhitneyleman54422
 
In this assignment worth 150 points, you will consider the present-d.docx
In this assignment worth 150 points, you will consider the present-d.docxIn this assignment worth 150 points, you will consider the present-d.docx
In this assignment worth 150 points, you will consider the present-d.docxwhitneyleman54422
 
In the readings thus far, the text identified many early American in.docx
In the readings thus far, the text identified many early American in.docxIn the readings thus far, the text identified many early American in.docx
In the readings thus far, the text identified many early American in.docxwhitneyleman54422
 
In the Roman Colony, leaders, or members of the court, were to be.docx
In the Roman Colony, leaders, or members of the court, were to be.docxIn the Roman Colony, leaders, or members of the court, were to be.docx
In the Roman Colony, leaders, or members of the court, were to be.docxwhitneyleman54422
 
In the provided scenario there are a few different crimes being .docx
In the provided scenario there are a few different crimes being .docxIn the provided scenario there are a few different crimes being .docx
In the provided scenario there are a few different crimes being .docxwhitneyleman54422
 
STOP THE MEETING MADNESS HOW TO FREE UP TIME FOR ME.docx
STOP  THE MEETING MADNESS HOW TO FREE UP TIME FOR ME.docxSTOP  THE MEETING MADNESS HOW TO FREE UP TIME FOR ME.docx
STOP THE MEETING MADNESS HOW TO FREE UP TIME FOR ME.docxwhitneyleman54422
 
Stoichiometry Lab – The Chemistry Behind Carbonates reacting with .docx
Stoichiometry Lab – The Chemistry Behind Carbonates reacting with .docxStoichiometry Lab – The Chemistry Behind Carbonates reacting with .docx
Stoichiometry Lab – The Chemistry Behind Carbonates reacting with .docxwhitneyleman54422
 
Stock-Trak Portfolio Report Write-Up GuidelinesYou may want to.docx
Stock-Trak Portfolio Report Write-Up GuidelinesYou may want to.docxStock-Trak Portfolio Report Write-Up GuidelinesYou may want to.docx
Stock-Trak Portfolio Report Write-Up GuidelinesYou may want to.docxwhitneyleman54422
 

More from whitneyleman54422 (20)

In this unit, you will experience the powerful impact communication .docx
In this unit, you will experience the powerful impact communication .docxIn this unit, you will experience the powerful impact communication .docx
In this unit, you will experience the powerful impact communication .docx
 
In this task, you will write an analysis (suggested length of 3–5 .docx
In this task, you will write an analysis (suggested length of 3–5 .docxIn this task, you will write an analysis (suggested length of 3–5 .docx
In this task, you will write an analysis (suggested length of 3–5 .docx
 
In this SLP you will identify where the major transportation modes a.docx
In this SLP you will identify where the major transportation modes a.docxIn this SLP you will identify where the major transportation modes a.docx
In this SLP you will identify where the major transportation modes a.docx
 
In this module the student will present writing which focuses attent.docx
In this module the student will present writing which focuses attent.docxIn this module the student will present writing which focuses attent.docx
In this module the student will present writing which focuses attent.docx
 
In this module, we looked at a variety of styles in the Renaissa.docx
In this module, we looked at a variety of styles in the Renaissa.docxIn this module, we looked at a variety of styles in the Renaissa.docx
In this module, we looked at a variety of styles in the Renaissa.docx
 
In this experiential learning experience, you will evaluate a health.docx
In this experiential learning experience, you will evaluate a health.docxIn this experiential learning experience, you will evaluate a health.docx
In this experiential learning experience, you will evaluate a health.docx
 
In this essay you should combine your practice responding and analyz.docx
In this essay you should combine your practice responding and analyz.docxIn this essay you should combine your practice responding and analyz.docx
In this essay you should combine your practice responding and analyz.docx
 
In this Discussion, pick one film to write about and answer ques.docx
In this Discussion, pick one film to write about and answer ques.docxIn this Discussion, pick one film to write about and answer ques.docx
In this Discussion, pick one film to write about and answer ques.docx
 
In this assignment, you will identify and interview a family who.docx
In this assignment, you will identify and interview a family who.docxIn this assignment, you will identify and interview a family who.docx
In this assignment, you will identify and interview a family who.docx
 
In this assignment, you will assess the impact of health legisla.docx
In this assignment, you will assess the impact of health legisla.docxIn this assignment, you will assess the impact of health legisla.docx
In this assignment, you will assess the impact of health legisla.docx
 
In this assignment, you will create a presentation. Select a topic o.docx
In this assignment, you will create a presentation. Select a topic o.docxIn this assignment, you will create a presentation. Select a topic o.docx
In this assignment, you will create a presentation. Select a topic o.docx
 
In this assignment, the student will understand the growth and devel.docx
In this assignment, the student will understand the growth and devel.docxIn this assignment, the student will understand the growth and devel.docx
In this assignment, the student will understand the growth and devel.docx
 
In this assignment, I want you to locate two pieces of news detailin.docx
In this assignment, I want you to locate two pieces of news detailin.docxIn this assignment, I want you to locate two pieces of news detailin.docx
In this assignment, I want you to locate two pieces of news detailin.docx
 
In this assignment worth 150 points, you will consider the present-d.docx
In this assignment worth 150 points, you will consider the present-d.docxIn this assignment worth 150 points, you will consider the present-d.docx
In this assignment worth 150 points, you will consider the present-d.docx
 
In the readings thus far, the text identified many early American in.docx
In the readings thus far, the text identified many early American in.docxIn the readings thus far, the text identified many early American in.docx
In the readings thus far, the text identified many early American in.docx
 
In the Roman Colony, leaders, or members of the court, were to be.docx
In the Roman Colony, leaders, or members of the court, were to be.docxIn the Roman Colony, leaders, or members of the court, were to be.docx
In the Roman Colony, leaders, or members of the court, were to be.docx
 
In the provided scenario there are a few different crimes being .docx
In the provided scenario there are a few different crimes being .docxIn the provided scenario there are a few different crimes being .docx
In the provided scenario there are a few different crimes being .docx
 
STOP THE MEETING MADNESS HOW TO FREE UP TIME FOR ME.docx
STOP  THE MEETING MADNESS HOW TO FREE UP TIME FOR ME.docxSTOP  THE MEETING MADNESS HOW TO FREE UP TIME FOR ME.docx
STOP THE MEETING MADNESS HOW TO FREE UP TIME FOR ME.docx
 
Stoichiometry Lab – The Chemistry Behind Carbonates reacting with .docx
Stoichiometry Lab – The Chemistry Behind Carbonates reacting with .docxStoichiometry Lab – The Chemistry Behind Carbonates reacting with .docx
Stoichiometry Lab – The Chemistry Behind Carbonates reacting with .docx
 
Stock-Trak Portfolio Report Write-Up GuidelinesYou may want to.docx
Stock-Trak Portfolio Report Write-Up GuidelinesYou may want to.docxStock-Trak Portfolio Report Write-Up GuidelinesYou may want to.docx
Stock-Trak Portfolio Report Write-Up GuidelinesYou may want to.docx
 

Recently uploaded

Earth Day Presentation wow hello nice great
Earth Day Presentation wow hello nice greatEarth Day Presentation wow hello nice great
Earth Day Presentation wow hello nice greatYousafMalik24
 
Incoming and Outgoing Shipments in 1 STEP Using Odoo 17
Incoming and Outgoing Shipments in 1 STEP Using Odoo 17Incoming and Outgoing Shipments in 1 STEP Using Odoo 17
Incoming and Outgoing Shipments in 1 STEP Using Odoo 17Celine George
 
KSHARA STURA .pptx---KSHARA KARMA THERAPY (CAUSTIC THERAPY)————IMP.OF KSHARA ...
KSHARA STURA .pptx---KSHARA KARMA THERAPY (CAUSTIC THERAPY)————IMP.OF KSHARA ...KSHARA STURA .pptx---KSHARA KARMA THERAPY (CAUSTIC THERAPY)————IMP.OF KSHARA ...
KSHARA STURA .pptx---KSHARA KARMA THERAPY (CAUSTIC THERAPY)————IMP.OF KSHARA ...M56BOOKSTORE PRODUCT/SERVICE
 
ECONOMIC CONTEXT - PAPER 1 Q3: NEWSPAPERS.pptx
ECONOMIC CONTEXT - PAPER 1 Q3: NEWSPAPERS.pptxECONOMIC CONTEXT - PAPER 1 Q3: NEWSPAPERS.pptx
ECONOMIC CONTEXT - PAPER 1 Q3: NEWSPAPERS.pptxiammrhaywood
 
भारत-रोम व्यापार.pptx, Indo-Roman Trade,
भारत-रोम व्यापार.pptx, Indo-Roman Trade,भारत-रोम व्यापार.pptx, Indo-Roman Trade,
भारत-रोम व्यापार.pptx, Indo-Roman Trade,Virag Sontakke
 
CELL CYCLE Division Science 8 quarter IV.pptx
CELL CYCLE Division Science 8 quarter IV.pptxCELL CYCLE Division Science 8 quarter IV.pptx
CELL CYCLE Division Science 8 quarter IV.pptxJiesonDelaCerna
 
Crayon Activity Handout For the Crayon A
Crayon Activity Handout For the Crayon ACrayon Activity Handout For the Crayon A
Crayon Activity Handout For the Crayon AUnboundStockton
 
Employee wellbeing at the workplace.pptx
Employee wellbeing at the workplace.pptxEmployee wellbeing at the workplace.pptx
Employee wellbeing at the workplace.pptxNirmalaLoungPoorunde1
 
How to Make a Pirate ship Primary Education.pptx
How to Make a Pirate ship Primary Education.pptxHow to Make a Pirate ship Primary Education.pptx
How to Make a Pirate ship Primary Education.pptxmanuelaromero2013
 
Full Stack Web Development Course for Beginners
Full Stack Web Development Course  for BeginnersFull Stack Web Development Course  for Beginners
Full Stack Web Development Course for BeginnersSabitha Banu
 
Final demo Grade 9 for demo Plan dessert.pptx
Final demo Grade 9 for demo Plan dessert.pptxFinal demo Grade 9 for demo Plan dessert.pptx
Final demo Grade 9 for demo Plan dessert.pptxAvyJaneVismanos
 
Alper Gobel In Media Res Media Component
Alper Gobel In Media Res Media ComponentAlper Gobel In Media Res Media Component
Alper Gobel In Media Res Media ComponentInMediaRes1
 
POINT- BIOCHEMISTRY SEM 2 ENZYMES UNIT 5.pptx
POINT- BIOCHEMISTRY SEM 2 ENZYMES UNIT 5.pptxPOINT- BIOCHEMISTRY SEM 2 ENZYMES UNIT 5.pptx
POINT- BIOCHEMISTRY SEM 2 ENZYMES UNIT 5.pptxSayali Powar
 
Organic Name Reactions for the students and aspirants of Chemistry12th.pptx
Organic Name Reactions  for the students and aspirants of Chemistry12th.pptxOrganic Name Reactions  for the students and aspirants of Chemistry12th.pptx
Organic Name Reactions for the students and aspirants of Chemistry12th.pptxVS Mahajan Coaching Centre
 
“Oh GOSH! Reflecting on Hackteria's Collaborative Practices in a Global Do-It...
“Oh GOSH! Reflecting on Hackteria's Collaborative Practices in a Global Do-It...“Oh GOSH! Reflecting on Hackteria's Collaborative Practices in a Global Do-It...
“Oh GOSH! Reflecting on Hackteria's Collaborative Practices in a Global Do-It...Marc Dusseiller Dusjagr
 
How to Configure Email Server in Odoo 17
How to Configure Email Server in Odoo 17How to Configure Email Server in Odoo 17
How to Configure Email Server in Odoo 17Celine George
 
Enzyme, Pharmaceutical Aids, Miscellaneous Last Part of Chapter no 5th.pdf
Enzyme, Pharmaceutical Aids, Miscellaneous Last Part of Chapter no 5th.pdfEnzyme, Pharmaceutical Aids, Miscellaneous Last Part of Chapter no 5th.pdf
Enzyme, Pharmaceutical Aids, Miscellaneous Last Part of Chapter no 5th.pdfSumit Tiwari
 

Recently uploaded (20)

Earth Day Presentation wow hello nice great
Earth Day Presentation wow hello nice greatEarth Day Presentation wow hello nice great
Earth Day Presentation wow hello nice great
 
Incoming and Outgoing Shipments in 1 STEP Using Odoo 17
Incoming and Outgoing Shipments in 1 STEP Using Odoo 17Incoming and Outgoing Shipments in 1 STEP Using Odoo 17
Incoming and Outgoing Shipments in 1 STEP Using Odoo 17
 
KSHARA STURA .pptx---KSHARA KARMA THERAPY (CAUSTIC THERAPY)————IMP.OF KSHARA ...
KSHARA STURA .pptx---KSHARA KARMA THERAPY (CAUSTIC THERAPY)————IMP.OF KSHARA ...KSHARA STURA .pptx---KSHARA KARMA THERAPY (CAUSTIC THERAPY)————IMP.OF KSHARA ...
KSHARA STURA .pptx---KSHARA KARMA THERAPY (CAUSTIC THERAPY)————IMP.OF KSHARA ...
 
ECONOMIC CONTEXT - PAPER 1 Q3: NEWSPAPERS.pptx
ECONOMIC CONTEXT - PAPER 1 Q3: NEWSPAPERS.pptxECONOMIC CONTEXT - PAPER 1 Q3: NEWSPAPERS.pptx
ECONOMIC CONTEXT - PAPER 1 Q3: NEWSPAPERS.pptx
 
OS-operating systems- ch04 (Threads) ...
OS-operating systems- ch04 (Threads) ...OS-operating systems- ch04 (Threads) ...
OS-operating systems- ch04 (Threads) ...
 
भारत-रोम व्यापार.pptx, Indo-Roman Trade,
भारत-रोम व्यापार.pptx, Indo-Roman Trade,भारत-रोम व्यापार.pptx, Indo-Roman Trade,
भारत-रोम व्यापार.pptx, Indo-Roman Trade,
 
TataKelola dan KamSiber Kecerdasan Buatan v022.pdf
TataKelola dan KamSiber Kecerdasan Buatan v022.pdfTataKelola dan KamSiber Kecerdasan Buatan v022.pdf
TataKelola dan KamSiber Kecerdasan Buatan v022.pdf
 
CELL CYCLE Division Science 8 quarter IV.pptx
CELL CYCLE Division Science 8 quarter IV.pptxCELL CYCLE Division Science 8 quarter IV.pptx
CELL CYCLE Division Science 8 quarter IV.pptx
 
Model Call Girl in Tilak Nagar Delhi reach out to us at 🔝9953056974🔝
Model Call Girl in Tilak Nagar Delhi reach out to us at 🔝9953056974🔝Model Call Girl in Tilak Nagar Delhi reach out to us at 🔝9953056974🔝
Model Call Girl in Tilak Nagar Delhi reach out to us at 🔝9953056974🔝
 
Crayon Activity Handout For the Crayon A
Crayon Activity Handout For the Crayon ACrayon Activity Handout For the Crayon A
Crayon Activity Handout For the Crayon A
 
Employee wellbeing at the workplace.pptx
Employee wellbeing at the workplace.pptxEmployee wellbeing at the workplace.pptx
Employee wellbeing at the workplace.pptx
 
How to Make a Pirate ship Primary Education.pptx
How to Make a Pirate ship Primary Education.pptxHow to Make a Pirate ship Primary Education.pptx
How to Make a Pirate ship Primary Education.pptx
 
Full Stack Web Development Course for Beginners
Full Stack Web Development Course  for BeginnersFull Stack Web Development Course  for Beginners
Full Stack Web Development Course for Beginners
 
Final demo Grade 9 for demo Plan dessert.pptx
Final demo Grade 9 for demo Plan dessert.pptxFinal demo Grade 9 for demo Plan dessert.pptx
Final demo Grade 9 for demo Plan dessert.pptx
 
Alper Gobel In Media Res Media Component
Alper Gobel In Media Res Media ComponentAlper Gobel In Media Res Media Component
Alper Gobel In Media Res Media Component
 
POINT- BIOCHEMISTRY SEM 2 ENZYMES UNIT 5.pptx
POINT- BIOCHEMISTRY SEM 2 ENZYMES UNIT 5.pptxPOINT- BIOCHEMISTRY SEM 2 ENZYMES UNIT 5.pptx
POINT- BIOCHEMISTRY SEM 2 ENZYMES UNIT 5.pptx
 
Organic Name Reactions for the students and aspirants of Chemistry12th.pptx
Organic Name Reactions  for the students and aspirants of Chemistry12th.pptxOrganic Name Reactions  for the students and aspirants of Chemistry12th.pptx
Organic Name Reactions for the students and aspirants of Chemistry12th.pptx
 
“Oh GOSH! Reflecting on Hackteria's Collaborative Practices in a Global Do-It...
“Oh GOSH! Reflecting on Hackteria's Collaborative Practices in a Global Do-It...“Oh GOSH! Reflecting on Hackteria's Collaborative Practices in a Global Do-It...
“Oh GOSH! Reflecting on Hackteria's Collaborative Practices in a Global Do-It...
 
How to Configure Email Server in Odoo 17
How to Configure Email Server in Odoo 17How to Configure Email Server in Odoo 17
How to Configure Email Server in Odoo 17
 
Enzyme, Pharmaceutical Aids, Miscellaneous Last Part of Chapter no 5th.pdf
Enzyme, Pharmaceutical Aids, Miscellaneous Last Part of Chapter no 5th.pdfEnzyme, Pharmaceutical Aids, Miscellaneous Last Part of Chapter no 5th.pdf
Enzyme, Pharmaceutical Aids, Miscellaneous Last Part of Chapter no 5th.pdf
 

Genetic disorders: Understanding behavioural phenotypes to inform classroom interventions

  • 1. Some fun facts Number of chromosomes an organism has does not equate to level of intelligence It takes about 8 hours for your cells to completely copy your DNA If the human genome were a book, it would be equivalent to 800 dictionaries We share 98% of our genes with chimpanzees, 90%with mice, 21% with worms, and 7% with e.coli bacteria Genetics for the non medical professional We have 23 pairs of chromosomes, 22 are autosomes, 1 pair is the sex chromosomes. Males are XY, females XX. Why is this important? Chromosomes are arranged from large to small The Basics Alteration in genetic material Alteration is present at birth May or may not be other members of the family affected Sometimes genetic errors occur spontaneously in dominant or recessive patterns of inheritance Causes
  • 2. Dysmorphic features Organ abnormalities Medical issues Developmental disabilites Characteristics of genetic disorders Brain Congenital heart disease Kidney problems Endocrine disorders Eye/ear abnormalities GI problems Organ abnormalities Respiratory issues Growth issues Seizure disorders Feeding dysfunction aspiration Medical
  • 3. Forehead Eyes Nose Philtrum Lips and mouth chin Dysmorphic features Has to be ordered Karotype Genetic micro array FISH test Genetic counseling Genetic testing Intellectual disability ASD Behavior issues Hypotonia Vision and/or hearing impairments Developmental Disabilities When fertilization occurs
  • 4. Cell reproduction In one, mitosis, cells duplicate In another process, called meiosis cells reduce (occurs on the sex chromosome) Most genetic errors occur during the cell reduction phase Mitosis and Meiosis Meiosis Karotype/Genotype
  • 5. Nondisjunction Translocation Mosaic Deletions Micro-deletions Types of genetic errors Trisomy Translocation Down syndrome Fragile X syndrome Prader Willi syndrome Angelman syndrome William syndrome Sotos syndrome Rett syndrome CHARGE syndrome Fetal alcohol syndrome Common genetic syndromes
  • 6. Sex chromosome errors Sex chromosome errors Sex chromosome errors Dominant recessive Dominance patterns It is not due to infections, trauma, emotional distress, lifestyle
  • 7. habits, income level or what a mother did or did not do during her pregnancy It is estimated that all of us have anywhere between 6-8 genetic defects that we carry We do not have any control over how the genes separate and divide at conception Still- families feel guilty What are not causes? Functional outcomes are important Although a child may need therapy, often does not need weekly therapy, in fact, research shows that weekly therapy is not beneficial for some of these children Working on functional outcomes, functional goals in the community setting is important Intervention Complicated process Genetic abnormalities are classified by the chromosome number they occur on Children with a trisomy abnormality have more problems than a child with a mosaic or deletion A child with a genetic abnormality on a higher number chromosome (21, 22) will have less difficulty than a child with a 13 or 18 defect Most fetuses are spontaneously miscarried that carry a genetic defect. Summary
  • 8. Chromosome are like the individual pages of an encyclopedia, genes are the words are on the pages, and DNA is the individual letters of the words. When all these things are in the right order, we consider that a normal person. And most of the time things go right in such a complicated process Summary Behavioural phenotypes and special educational needs: is aetiology important in the classroom?jir_1542 929..946 C. Reilly School of Education, University College Dublin, Belfield, Dublin 4, Ireland Abstract Background A number of genetic conditions with associated intellectual disability and/or special edu- cational needs have increasingly well-defined behav- ioural phenotypes. Thus, the concept of ‘behavioural phenotype’ and aetiology of intellectual disability may be important with regard to school-based interventions.
  • 9. Method The evidence for distinctive cognitive and behavioural aspects of five of the most common genetic syndromes (Down syndrome, fragile X syn- drome, Williams syndrome, Prader–Willi syndrome and velo-cardio-facial syndrome) associated with special educational needs is reviewed with respect to key studies and findings. The possible utility of aetiology-related interventions in education is dis- cussed with reference to arguments for and against such approaches with respect to published guide- lines and published research. Results Behavioural phenotypes are probabilistic and many children with a specific genetic syndrome will share commonalities with other children with other genetic syndromes and within syndrome vari- ability is not uncommon. There is evidence that teachers and parents have limited knowledge of aspects of the proposed cognitive and behaviour profiles associated with the reviewed syndromes. While there are published guidelines in the area of learning and behaviour for each of the five reviewed syndromes there is a limited amount of evidence of the efficacy of such approaches in school settings. Conclusion It is likely that knowing the aetiology of a child’s special educational needs will be helpful for staff who work in school settings in relation to cognitive and behavioural implications. However, how such knowledge might inform teaching practice or behavioural interventions has not been studied. A model is proposed that might help inform educators about the possible role of aetiology in the classroom. Keywords behavioural phenotypes, Down syndrome, fragile X, Prader–Willi syndrome, VCFS,
  • 10. Williams syndrome Introduction – aetiology and behavioural phenotypes There is increasing evidence which suggests that there are distinctive cognitive and behavioural pro- files in a number of genetic syndromes with associ- ated intellectual disability (ID) (Dykens et al. 2000; Rosner et al. 2004). While there is likely to be a core set of deficits in neuropsychological function- ing that is shared by all people with ID regardless of cause, there are more specific patterns of abilities Correspondence: Mr Colin Reilly, School of Education, University College Dublin, Belfield, Dublin 4, Ireland (e-mail: [email protected] ucdconnect.ie). Journal of Intellectual Disability Research doi: 10.1111/j.1365- 2788.2012.01542.x VOLUME 56 PART 10 pp 929–946 OCTOBER 2012 929 bs_bs_banner © 2012 The Author. Journal of Intellectual Disability Research © 2012 Blackwell Publishing Ltd and deficits associated with known genetic syn- dromes (Garner et al. 1999). Important in any dis- cussion on the role of aetiology is the concept of a
  • 11. ‘behavioural phenotype’. While some definitions of a ‘behavioural phenotype’ have emphasised ‘distinct behavioural features occurring in almost every case of the condition and rarely in other conditions’ (Flynt & Yule 1994, p. 666), others have emphasised a heightened probability that a behaviour or cluster of behaviours will be associated with a syndrome. Dykens (1995) has defined a ‘behavioural pheno- type’ as involving the ‘heightened probability or likelihood that people with a given syndrome will exhibit certain behavioural and developmental sequelae relative to those without the syndrome’ (p. 523). This definition highlights the probable nature of behavioural phenotypes in that not all individuals with a specific genetic disorder will display particu- lar aetiology-related behaviours (Fidler et al. 2002). For the purposes of the article the concept of a behavioural phenotype is understood in terms of Dykens’s definition with the focus primarily on cognitive and behavioural profiles likely to be relevant for school aged children. Five genetic syndromes associated with ID and/or special educational needs are examined in this the article to consider if aetiology of ID matters when consider- ing educational interventions and approaches. The five were chosen as they all have relatively well- defined cognitive and behavioural profiles and all have had guidelines published for intervention in teaching and behavioural domains. Work on behavioural phenotypes aims to link together genes, brain and behaviour as well as to refine treatment and intervention (Dykens 2000). When considering treatment and intervention it is thought that comparisons of children with one genetic syndrome with children with another
  • 12. genetic syndrome, or with children with ID without a known aetiology, or with typically developing chil- dren matched for mental or chronological age may reveal specific aetiology-related strengths and diffi- culties associated with each syndrome. It has been proposed that such understanding is vital in select- ing appropriate educational intervention strategies for children with genetic syndromes (e.g. Campbell et al. 2009). Children without genetic aetiologies for their ID and associated special educational needs may not show the same pattern of significant areas of strengths or weaknesses in terms of development, as children with a known genetic aetiology for their ID across cognitive, behavioural and linguistic domains (Dykens & Hodapp 2001). However, adopting an approach whereby one treats a syndrome group as largely homogeneous with regard to treatment and intervention may have significant drawbacks and may not concur with evi- dence suggesting that within syndrome variation is common. It has been noted that in some neurode- velopmental disorders, within syndrome variability is as great as or greater than between syndrome variability (Levy & Ebstein 2009). Porter & Colth- eart’s (2005) study of the performance of individu- als with Williams syndrome on the subtests of the Woodcock-Johnson Tests of Cognitive Ability – Revised ( Woodcock & Johnson 1990), suggests that as many as six different non-normative cognitive profiles exist in Williams syndrome. A danger then may exist if syndromes with known genetic causes are treated as well-controlled homogenous study groups that do not differ across cognitive and linguistic domains. As well as within syndrome
  • 13. variability there may also be cross-syndrome similarities, and co-morbidities between a syndrome with a known genetic cause and a syndrome without a single known genetic cause. For example, a child with fragile X syndrome may also meet the diagnostic criteria for autism spectrum disorder (ASD) (although diagnosis of ASD in genetic syn- dromes associated with ID is not without chal- lenges, see Moss & Howlin 2009). In this case, the child is likely to share cognitive and behavioural features with other children with fragile X syn- drome and other children with ASD. In the light of this discussion then it is worth noting that there appears to be a need to recognise that an individual with a syndrome with a known aetiology may be likely to conform to the behav- ioural phenotype proposed for that syndrome in some domains, but also may differ from that pro- posed behavioural phenotype in other domains. At times the child may share more in common with a child with another genetic syndrome or a child who does not have a genetic aetiology for their difficul- ties than they do with other children with the same syndrome. In addition, there may be some children who seem to closely ‘fit’ the proposed behavioural phenotype associated with the syndrome while 930 Journal of Intellectual Disability Research VOLUME 56 PART 10 OCTOBER 2012 C. Reilly • Aetiology in the classroom © 2012 The Author. Journal of Intellectual Disability Research © 2012 Blackwell Publishing Ltd
  • 14. other children appear not to ‘fit’ as well with the proposed phenotype. A child with Down syndrome who meets the diagnostic criteria for ASD could be seen as not fitting with the proposed phenotype associated with Down syndrome which usually involves assumptions of comparatively low levels of behavioural difficulties and a sociable personality. In the case of some aspects of a behavioural phenotype associated with a genetic condition there appears to be a very high probability that a child with the con- dition will conform in some domains. For example, almost all school aged children with Prader–Willi syndrome will display an excessive interest in food. In such a case where a particular behaviour is asso- ciated with one syndrome and not usually with others, the term ‘total specificity’ has been proposed (Hodapp 1997). Where two or more syndromes share a propensity for a particular behaviour the behaviour shows ‘partial specificity’ (Hodapp 1997). For example, on the Kaufman Assessment Battery for Children (Kaufman & Kaufman 1983) boys with fragile X syndrome show a relative strength in simultaneous processing (Hodapp et al. 1992). This relative strength may also be shared by children with Prader–Willi syndrome (Dykens et al. 1992), but not by the majority of children with Down syn- drome (Pueschel et al. 1987), or children with ID of unknown aetiology (Hodapp et al. 1992). Therefore, when studying genetic syndromes and possible aetiology-specific approaches there is a need to understand that behavioural phenotypes associated with those syndromes are probabilistic, and not every individual with the syndrome will display the
  • 15. pattern of behaviours associated with the syndrome. Evidence for behavioural phenotypes In this section, evidence for specific behavioural phenotypes associated with five well-described genetic syndromes, Down Syndrome, fragile X syn- drome, Williams syndrome, Prader–Willi syndrome and velo-cardio-facial syndrome (VCFS; also known as 22q11.2 syndrome) will be summarised across a number of domains which are relevant in classroom settings. It is not possible to comprehensively review all research findings across all domains for each syndrome but the main findings for each syndrome will be summarised with a particular emphasis on findings that have been used with respect to pro- posed educational interventions. Examining evidence for distinctive cognitive and behavioural profiles associated with the syndromes is important because evidence of this nature is often used when guidelines for intervention are devel- oped. Those who advocate an aetiological approach argue that it is the distinctive profiles of relative cognitive and behavioural strengths and weaknesses associated with syndromes which make aetiology important with regard to programme planning. Table 1 summarises characteristics associated with the syndromes in global and specific aspects of cog- nition, adaptive behaviour, symptoms of ASD and attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD), and maladaptive/problematic behaviour. Early research on the syndromes tended to focus on mea- sures of broad aspects of cognition such as global scores on IQ tests and strengths and weakness on
  • 16. subtests of commonly used IQ tests where more recently there has been a trend towards more detailed neuropsychological testing, which has examined areas such as memory, attention and social cognition. With the exception of a UK population-based study focussing on Prader–Willi syndrome (Holland et al. 2003) the studies carried out on cognitive and behavioural aspects of the syn- dromes have not been population based and as a result findings may not be representative of the entire population affected by the syndrome, although an increasing number of studies have included large samples and multisite studies. The lack of population-based data may be particularly problematic in syndromes such as VCFS where those with a milder presentation may not have been identified and thus not included in clinic-based studies. In relation to ASD it is difficult to compare rates of ASD across syndromes because of differences in how rates were ascertained and the instruments used in studies. In Table 1 rates of ASD symptoms across the syndromes are compared based on scores on the Social Communication Questionnaire (Rutter et al. 2003), or the earlier equivalent Autism Screening Questionnaire (Berument et al. 1999). The prevalence rates indicate the percentage of individuals who meet the cut-off for ASD on the Social Communication Questionnaire/Autism Screening Questionnaire and do not indicate the 931 Journal of Intellectual Disability Research VOLUME 56 PART 10 OCTOBER 2012
  • 17. C. Reilly • Aetiology in the classroom © 2012 The Author. Journal of Intellectual Disability Research © 2012 Blackwell Publishing Ltd T ab le 1 G lo b al co gn it iv e fu n ct io n in g,
  • 67. ng th s in ex pr es si ve la ng ua ge 26 932 Journal of Intellectual Disability Research VOLUME 56 PART 10 OCTOBER 2012 C. Reilly • Aetiology in the classroom © 2012 The Author. Journal of Intellectual Disability Research © 2012 Blackwell Publishing Ltd A da pt
  • 153. 3 ). 933 Journal of Intellectual Disability Research VOLUME 56 PART 10 OCTOBER 2012 C. Reilly • Aetiology in the classroom © 2012 The Author. Journal of Intellectual Disability Research © 2012 Blackwell Publishing Ltd percentage of individuals who actually received a clinical diagnosis of ASD. There have also been a number of studies reporting the presence of signifi- cant ADHD symptoms in each of the syndromes and the main findings are summarised in Table 1. It has been posited that the utility of an ADHD diag- nosis in children with complex behavioural pheno- types needs further evaluation (Zwaigenbaum et al. 2006), and the higher rate of the primarily inatten- tive subtype in this population could point to both a different manifestation of symptoms and underly- ing pathophysiology in individuals with genetic syndromes associated with ID. Cognitive and behavioural interventions Proposed classroom interventions Having summarised some of the research evidence for the existence of ‘behavioural phenotypes’ or aetiology-specific profiles in five syndromes associ- ated with ID it is now worth examining proposed
  • 154. guidelines for intervention based on the outlined behavioural phenotypes. Most of these guidelines reflect early research focussing on analysis of cogni- tive profiles via patterns of strengths and weak- nesses on subtests of tests of cognitive functioning and parental report of patterns of maladaptive or problematic behaviours, as opposed to more recent detailed observational studies of behaviour and assessment of specific aspects of cognition. For all of the syndromes there are now guidelines and in some cases specific resources aimed at targeting classroom learning and proposing particular teach- ing approaches (e.g. Oelwein 1995; Braden 2002; Dew-Hughes 2004; Horsteimer 2004; Chedd et al. 2006; Cutler-Landsman 2007; Udwin et al. 2007). In relation to behaviour management there are also guidelines that reflect the behavioural profiles asso- ciated with syndromes (e.g. Hills-Epstein et al. 2002; Semel & Rosner 2003; Whitman & Jackson 2006). These guidelines are typically based on rec- ommendations derived from the cognitive and behavioural profiles of the syndromes and extensive experience of working with children affected by the syndromes and there are few empirical studies to verify the validity of the recommendations. Some of the guidelines are syndrome specific (e.g. in fragile X syndrome sitting side-on as opposed to face-on due to hyperarousal and resultant gaze aversion). Other guidelines for practice are shared across two or more genetic syndromes and might also be com- monly used with children with conditions such as ASD and ADHD (e.g. use of low distraction work areas such as individual workstations for children with fragile X syndrome, Williams syndrome, ASD and ADHD). In this way the guidelines reflect
  • 155. Hodapp’s (1997) notion of both ‘total specificity’ and ‘partial specificity’. Some examples of the suggested recommenda- tions are now outlined. The relative strengths in visual-spatial domain and early gesturing have led to suggestions that children with Down syndrome may be particularly suitable for interventions such as sign language (Hodapp et al. 1992) and visual approaches to reading (Buckley 1995). Dykens & Hodapp (1997) have suggested that children with Williams syndrome may benefit from team or buddy systems in school- and people-oriented jobs based on their relative strength in language and sociability. Teachers of children with fragile X syn- drome and Prader–Willi syndrome could rely on visual perceptual cues including pictures and models whereas teachers of children with Williams syndrome may not benefit as much from visual approaches (Hodapp & Fidler 1999). It has been posited that boys with fragile X syndrome will struggle with phonics-based approaches to reading (Braden 2002). However, young children with VCFS have been noted to have a good ability to decode words, and thus are more likely to benefit from early phonics-based instruction (Cutler- Landsman 2007), as are children with Williams syndrome (Udwin et al. 2007). With regard to therapeutic intervention it has been suggested that ‘group therapies’ may work well for persons with Williams syndrome due to sociability and ‘people orientation’ but not as well for children with fragile X syndrome due to often displayed anxiety in social environments (Hodapp & Dykens 2001). However, children with fragile X syndrome may benefit from indirect instruction in a group where peers are used
  • 156. as teaching models and the child with fragile X is not the focus of direct questioning which may be anxiety provoking (Braden 2002). In terms of behaviour management, Einfeld (2005) has argued that factors contributing to behavioural issues may arise from four causes and one of the four is the 934 Journal of Intellectual Disability Research VOLUME 56 PART 10 OCTOBER 2012 C. Reilly • Aetiology in the classroom © 2012 The Author. Journal of Intellectual Disability Research © 2012 Blackwell Publishing Ltd specific aetiology of the child’s condition. In the case of Prader–Willi syndrome, Einfeld (2005) argues that traditional contingent reinforcement strategies are likely to be less successful due to hypothalamic dysfunction associated with the syn- drome and that environmental or antecedent-based approaches may be more successful. In a similar vein in relation to Williams syndrome, Einfeld (2005) argues that traditional behavioural approaches may not be helpful when dealing with a child’s avoidance or reluctance, but that a knowl- edge of the high prevalence of fears and anxieties associated with the syndrome might prompt a medical referral and possible use of anti-anxiety medication. In relation to interventions for children with genetic syndromes it has been proposed that where
  • 157. possible proposed interventions should focus on a child’s strengths (Hodapp & Dykens 2001; Hodapp et al. 2003), as opposed to attempting to remediate the child’s relative weaknesses. The majority of syndrome-specific recommendations tend to focus on a child’s strengths or compensate for their weak- nesses as opposed to attempting to remediate or ameliorate weaknesses. However, the view that the ‘playing to strengths’ is the most efficacious way to approach educational instruction is not shared by all in the wider educational community. Alferink & Farmer-Dougan (2010) argue that teaching to a specific strength in the absence of teaching to a weakness may be a disservice to students and the larger the child’s inventory of learning strategies, the more likely the child is to learn across environ- mental settings. In relation to fragile X syndrome the commonly observed gaze aversion in boys has led to recommendations that teachers and school staff should not force eye contact but should instead sit side-on as opposed to face on (Scharfenaker et al. 2002). This would be an example of playing to a child’s strengths or at least not focussing on a child’s weakness. However, it has been argued that with the use of behavioural tech- niques, eye contact duration can be improved in children with fragile X syndrome (Hall et al. 2009), so in this case the intervention is focussing on ame- liorating a syndrome-specific weakness. There is evidence that aetiology-related strengths and weak- nesses become more pronounced with age (Hodapp 2004). Whether this is related to the fact that chil- dren gravitate towards activities that reinforce their strengths or parents and teachers focus on this strengths is not clear. If profiles become more pro-
  • 158. nounced with age it may that early intensive inter- vention is vital in order to address relative weaknesses and that with older children, focussing on strengths may be more appropriate. Intervention studies Despite the number of studies which have focussed on describing the cognitive and behavioural aspects of the syndromes under review, there have been few intervention studies assessing the validity of specific approaches in teaching skills to children affected by the syndromes. The small number of studies that have been carried out have focussed mainly on reading (mostly in Down syndrome) and specific behavioural difficulties and have been single- participant designs or contained a relatively small number of participants. A number of these studies are now described. Relative weaknesses in auditory short-term memory have led to the view that children with Down syndrome may struggle with phonics-based approaches to reading instruction and that starting with sight words may be more profitable (Oelwein 1995). Cologon et al. (2011) described how seven children with Down syndrome benefited from phonological awareness training over 10 weekly sessions and post intervention showed improved reading skills and phonological awareness. Goetz et al. (2008) described how 15 children with Down syndrome benefitted from a phonics-based approach to reading over 8 weeks and improved significantly compared to a waiting group of eight children with Down syndrome. Neither of these two studies included a comparison with sight
  • 159. word approaches. However, Cupples & Iacono (2002) described a study where seven children with Down syndrome received training in either phono- logical awareness (four children) or sight word approaches to reading (three children). Reading of non-training generalisation words improved signifi- cantly in only three children, all of whom had received phonics-based training (Cupples & Iacono 2002). In fragile X syndrome a number of small-scale intervention studies have been carried and are in 935 Journal of Intellectual Disability Research VOLUME 56 PART 10 OCTOBER 2012 C. Reilly • Aetiology in the classroom © 2012 The Author. Journal of Intellectual Disability Research © 2012 Blackwell Publishing Ltd informative with regard to the possible usefulness of approaches based on the syndrome’s proposed behavioural phenotype. Hall et al. (2009) employed a percentile reinforcement schedule with and without overcorrection to shape eye-contact dura- tion of six boys with the syndrome. Results showed that eye contact, often thought to be aversive in fragile X syndrome improved in four boys using percentile schedules either alone or in combination with overcorrection technique without concomitant increases in social anxiety or behavioural problems. Based on evidence that some children with fragile
  • 160. X may benefit from teaching strategies that adopt a simultaneous stimulus presentation, as opposed to a sequential stimulus presentation Hall et al. (2006) used a computerised matching to sample procedure to teach mathematics and geography skills. Five adolescents (four boys, one girl) with fragile X syn- drome aged 12–19 years were taught these skills using a self-paced computer program implemented intensively with the children over 2 days. Results indicated that four of the five children successfully learned the mathematics trained relations and three of the five children successfully learned the geogra- phy trained relations. The authors concluded that that computerised matching-to-sample procedures, even when conducted in time-limited sessions, may help individuals with fragile X syndrome learn new skills although again no controls were used and alternative teaching approaches were not assessed. Based on a relative strength in auditory short- term memory, it has been recommended that chil- dren with Williams syndrome be taught to read using phonics-based approaches (e.g. Levy & Antebi 2004). Becerra et al. (2008) examined types of reading instruction and performance in 44 indi- viduals with Williams syndrome. Twenty-four indi- viduals received instruction via phonics-based approaches and 20 via sight word approaches. The children’s reading standard scores were then com- pared to those predicted based on their overall level of cognitive functioning. Results indicated that most children in the phonics group read at or above the level expected for their level of cognitive function- ing. In contrast, most children in the sight word group read below the level expected for their level of cognitive functioning. In Prader–Willi syndrome,
  • 161. two case studies have been published of interven- tions for reducing the occurrence of skin-picking a common problematic behaviour in the syndrome. Radstaake et al. (2011) reported on the successful use of functional behavioural assessment and treat- ment of skin picking in a 16-year-old female with the syndrome. Stokes & Luiselli (2009) describe a study where the skin picking of a 26-year-old male with Prader–Willi was reduced by restricting the individual’s access to the setting in which skin picking most often occurred (i.e. bathroom), teach- ing the individual novel forms of communication (i.e. functional communication training), and pro- viding reinforcement for behaviours other than skin picking. Behavioural phenotypes in the classroom Is genetic aetiology important in the classroom? Arguments exist for and against the importance aetiology with regard to classroom interventions. For those who argue that aetiology is important (e.g. Hodapp & Fidler 1999; Hodapp & Dykens 2001; Hodapp et al. 2003), it is posited that knowl- edge of aetiology may aid early identification of syndrome-specific cognitive and behavioural profiles thus enabling parents and professionals to target strengths and needs with appropriate therapeutic interventions and teaching strategies. By utilising these syndrome-specific behavioural profiles, it is proposed teachers can develop more targeted, ‘fine- grained, and effective interventions’ (Dykens 1999; Hodapp & Fidler 1999). Although genetic aetiology may provide guidelines that will work in some but
  • 162. not every case (Hodapp & Fidler 1999), an under- standing of behavioural phenotypes can help special educators foresee and avoid certain difficulties and deal more effectively with others (Hodapp & Fidler 1999). It is also argued that in order to take full advantage of genetic research, educators and clini- cians require access to up-to-date and relevant research findings on syndromes associated with ID (Lee et al. 2005). Others have expressed the view that aetiology is not important in educational programming. Halla- han & Kauffman (2000) argue that the focus of educational programmes should vary according to the degree of the student’s ID, and Blackhurst & Berndine (1993) have expressed the view systems of classification of based on aetiology are not valuable 936 Journal of Intellectual Disability Research VOLUME 56 PART 10 OCTOBER 2012 C. Reilly • Aetiology in the classroom © 2012 The Author. Journal of Intellectual Disability Research © 2012 Blackwell Publishing Ltd in education. Abbeduto & Keller-Bell (2002) described calls for curricula for individual syn- dromes as reflecting ‘insularity’ and suggested the importance of recognising that common problems exist across syndromes. As well as questions about the relevance of aetiology one reason for this concern about aetiology-specific approaches may be
  • 163. worries about the ‘balkanisation’ of special educa- tion (Forness & Kavale 1994). Balkanisation refers to the idea that all children with specific disorders would need separate educational categories with eligibility criteria and separate educational pro- grammes and resources. Despite increasing evidence of syndrome-specific profiles and the prospect of aetiology-specific inter- ventions there is as of yet, limited research evidence of the efficacy of such aetiology-related approaches in the classroom and few of the recommendations have been systematically evaluated. It is not clear how much knowledge about aetiology-related pro- files is available in classrooms and if so how it is used in terms of educational practices (Fidler et al. 2002). It has also been asserted that special educa- tors have largely ignored calls to tailor interventions to cognitive or behavioural profiles associated with specific syndromes (Hodapp & Dykens 2009). Making inferences from the clinic to the classroom or other settings may not always be warranted. In the case of classroom behaviours a study by Saun- ders (1999) suggests that behaviour in the class- room does reflect aspects of the behavioural phenotype associated with a particular syndrome. Teachers of children with fragile X syndrome reported that boys with the syndrome did not like activities involving large groups including dinner- time and playtime and group activities such as story time, circle time or class discussion (Saunders 1999), which could be seen to reflect ‘hyperarousal’ and related behaviours associated with the syn- drome. In terms of favoured activities teachers mentioned ‘play, cookery, computer, trips out and stories’ which it could be argued link in to the rela-
  • 164. tive strengths in daily living as identified on mea- sures of adaptive functioning. Symons et al. (2001) reported on the classroom engagement of 26 boys with fragile X syndrome with a mean age of 7.5 years. Using classroom observations they reported that the boys were engaged at a similar level to peers although they indicated that their measure- ment system may have been limited with regard to measuring subtle aspects of classroom engagement. They reported that the engagement of the boys was significantly related to how the teacher had struc- tured the classroom suggesting that ‘classroom ecology was more important than biology’ (Symons et al. 2001). These two pieces of research suggest that while aetiology may indicate a child’s prefer- ence for types of activities, getting the classroom environment right for all children with ID regard- less of aetiology should still be the key starting point. Incorporating aetiological knowledge in the classroom Because of the limited research evidence on the manifestation of behavioural phenotypes in the classroom it could be argued that there is not yet enough evidence to advocate aetiology as an impor- tant consideration in the classroom, and there is no consensus regarding the role that this aetiology- specific information should have in shaping inter- vention and service delivery (Fidler et al. 2007). In the absence of such evidence, how then should aeti- ology be considered with respect to the teaching of children with genetic syndromes associated with ID? On the one hand, it would be difficult for most
  • 165. teachers to have an in-depth knowledge of cognitive and behavioural aspects of the syndromes discussed in this review as well as the many other genetic syn- dromes associated with ID. The fact that many teachers will only teach a small number of children with genetic syndromes throughout their career would make comprehensive training an inefficient allocation of resources. On the other hand, there would appear to be key aspects of the cognitive and behavioural profiles of some of the syndromes described here that teachers need to know. Knowing that hyperphagia is associated with Prader–Willi syndrome, hyperarousal with fragile X syndrome, and fears/phobias with Williams syn- drome would appear vital in understanding an affected child’s behaviour. In terms of cognitive functioning, being aware of relative strengths in simultaneous processing among children with fragile X syndrome and Prader–Willi syndrome and relative strengths in auditory processing among chil- 937 Journal of Intellectual Disability Research VOLUME 56 PART 10 OCTOBER 2012 C. Reilly • Aetiology in the classroom © 2012 The Author. Journal of Intellectual Disability Research © 2012 Blackwell Publishing Ltd dren with Williams syndrome and VCFS would also appear important in understanding the learning strengths of these children. However, even if educa- tors have a good awareness of a proposed behav-
  • 166. ioural phenotype, there still exists a dilemma with regards to whether an approach should focus on teaching via strengths or should resources be used to intensively targets weaknesses. One difficulty with applying findings from research on behavioural phenotypes in educational settings is the lack of knowledge among health and educational professionals of syndromes and associ- ated cognitive and behavioural profiles. In a survey by York et al. (1999), significantly fewer school staff had knowledge of fragile X syndrome and the asso- ciated cognitive and behavioural profile in compari- son with Down syndrome. Even among staff who had taught a child with fragile X syndrome specific knowledge about recommended interventions and approaches was limited (York et al. 1999). Lee et al. (2005) looked at the awareness of special educators and paediatricians of Down syndrome, fragile X syndrome and VCFS. With regard to cognitive fea- tures the highest knowledge among teachers was for Down syndrome with the least knowledge for VCFS and both teachers and paediatricians demonstrated difficulties in identifying cognitive aspects of fragile X and VCFS. In relation to behavioural aspects the highest level of knowledge was for fragile X syn- drome followed by Down syndrome and then VCFS. In this study, the number of children that the teachers had taught with each syndrome corre- lated positively with their level of knowledge of the cognitive and behavioural phenotype. In the case of Prader–Willi syndrome and Williams syndrome, parents tend not to know about cognitive aspects associated with their child’s syndrome but may know more about behavioural aspects (Fidler et al. 2002). Given that parents are often the sole provid-
  • 167. ers about information about their child’s syndrome (Fidler et al. 2002) it would appear that teachers will have limited knowledge of aetiology-specific profiles or interventions especially in the cognitive domain. Teachers are likely to learn about syn- dromes on a case-by-case basis through experiences with individual children (Lee et al. 2005). There- fore, an issue would appear to be, should prospec- tive teacher education be offered and if so how best to offer it? Forness & Kavale (1994) have suggested that a middle ground may be possible where school staff become aware of relevant features of a syn- drome or disorder while still adhering to traditional programmes. In this scenario firstly knowing that a child’s syndrome may have an impact on their learning and behavioural profile should be the start- ing point. With regard to understanding how this information might be imparted the pyramid in Fig. 1 adapted from Braden & Riley (2006) and Brown-Chidsey & Steege (2005) might be useful as an aid to understanding how interventions might be conceptualised. The approach in Fig. 1 recognises that in many cases the needs of children with special educational needs can be met with interventions and approaches that work with the majority of children. However, there will be times that such universal approaches do not best meet the needs of all stu- dents and there may be groups of children with similar needs. Some of these children may have genetic syndromes, some may have well-defined conditions without an identified genetic aetiology (e.g. ASD, ADHD), and some may have an educa- tional need but not have a diagnosed syndrome or
  • 168. condition. For example, a number of children may have significant ADHD type difficulties and will need distraction-free work areas or use of individual workstations. Such environmental accommodations could be made for children with ADHD, ASD, fragile X syndrome, and Williams syndrome and may be made within the classroom or in a more specialised setting. In terms of shared cognitive profile the promotion of word recognition for chil- dren with Prader–Willi and fragile X syndrome may benefit significantly from approaches emphasising simultaneous visual approaches. In some cases chil- dren with specific syndromes may need very specific guidelines based on aspects of their syndrome. Chil- dren with Prader–Willi may need their educational programme to contain clear guidelines around access to food and need for regular physical exer- cise. The final aspect of educational planning for children with special educational needs and/or ID should be a focus on the child’s own particular interests and learning strengths and weaknesses, and other relevant individual factors. These aspects of the child’s profile may or may not be related to the child’s hypothesised behavioural phenotype but will be garnered by careful observations by school 938 Journal of Intellectual Disability Research VOLUME 56 PART 10 OCTOBER 2012 C. Reilly • Aetiology in the classroom © 2012 The Author. Journal of Intellectual Disability Research © 2012 Blackwell Publishing Ltd
  • 169. staff and consultation with the child’s parents and teachers. In considering aetiology it is important that edu- cators understand that within syndrome variability is common and when considering specific aetiology- related guidelines, these should be evaluated in the same manner as other interventions. An overly rigid application of syndrome-specific information could lead to poor intervention planning with a risk that intervention resources are wasted on difficulties that a child does not actually demonstrate, despite a child’s higher probability of demonstrating certain difficulties as a function of their diagnosis (Fidler et al. 2007). The approach outlined here recognises that some educational needs are shared across syn- dromes whereas some seem relatively unique and while many teaching techniques are useful regard- less of diagnosis this does not negate the need for aetiology-specific information (Starr et al. 2006). A consideration of genetic aetiology should not be at the expense of neglecting environmental influences. The influence of the child’s environment at home and school including family, teacher and peer influ- ences, is also likely to significantly impact on the development of a behavioural phenotype. The display of challenging behaviour may be influenced by challenging environments including environ- ments where there is a lack of stimulation, atten- Universal strategies/interventions for children with special educational needs (e.g. curriculum adaptations for children with intellectual disability, use of classroom
  • 170. wide behaviour modification strategies ) Interventions for children with specific behaviour associated with a syndrome (e.g. environment that restricts access to food for children with Prader−Willi syndrome) Interventions for individual children (e.g. Individual Behaviour/Education Plan reflecting child’s interests) Interventions for conditions with similar cognitive profiles (e.g. use of visual materials focussing on simultaneous strengths in fragile X syndrome and Prader−Willi syndrome ) Environmental accommodations for children with ADHD type difficulties (e.g. distraction free areas in classroom, individual workstations) Individualised Behaviour/Education Plan Shared plan/resources for children with common difficulties
  • 171. Class/Group Plan School adapts to accommodate child’s specific and shared special education needs Syndrome-specific approach Figure 1 Planning school-based interventions for children with intellectual disability and/or special educational needs (adapted from Braden & Riley 2006; Brown-Chidsey & Steege 2005). 939 Journal of Intellectual Disability Research VOLUME 56 PART 10 OCTOBER 2012 C. Reilly • Aetiology in the classroom © 2012 The Author. Journal of Intellectual Disability Research © 2012 Blackwell Publishing Ltd tion, access to preferred objects/activities, or the presence aversive stimuli (Langthorne et al. 2007). In a study examining the function of challenging behaviour Langthorne et al. (2011) reported that children with fragile X are more likely to have an escape-maintained function than attention- maintained function for problem behaviour possibly due to the hyperarousal and associated behaviours
  • 172. common in the syndrome. Therefore, the impor- tance of identifying the function of challenging behaviour in the context of the child’s environment and genetic aetiology is likely to make a significant contribution to understanding what might help in the classroom. Educators are likely to benefit from a good knowledge of behavioural principles and the use of functional behavioural assessment is likely to be particularly important with regard to under- standing the role of the environment and the role of reinforcement in maintaining a problematic behav- iour whether or not that behaviour is common in a particular syndrome or not. Therefore, knowledge of a behavioural phenotype should contribute to an understanding of a problematic behaviour and should complement knowledge of sound behav- ioural principles and inform functional assessment and subsequent behavioural intervention. Summary and conclusion There are now a good number of studies on the cognitive and behavioural aspects of various genetic syndromes associated with ID and/or special educa- tion needs, and studies are becoming more sophisti- cated with regard to design and more specific with regard to aspects of functioning being assessed. These studies suggest that there are distinctive ‘behavioural phenotypes’ associated with some syn- dromes. However, the notion of a behavioural phe- notype should not be taken to imply homogeneity of profiles within syndromes and within-syndrome variability is common. It is likely that many children will ‘fit’ the proposed ‘behavioural phenotype’ for the syndrome across a number of domains but on occasions the they will not ‘fit’ the phenotype and
  • 173. each child will have an individual phenotype which may or may not conform to the group phenotype. There is as of yet, limited evidence on aetiology- related interventions or the usefulness of adopting an ‘aetiological approach’ in the classroom, and the issues surrounding the potential for developing phenotype-specific intervention practices are complex (Fidler et al. 2007). While it may be diffi- cult to carry out studies on specific interventions for children with specific syndromes, research on the manifestation of proposed cognitive and behav- ioural profiles in the classroom is important. It is not clear how teachers who are well informed about proposed cognitive and behavioural profiles of a particular syndrome apply educational practices in the classroom. It is therefore, important that research is carried out with those who work in edu- cational settings with children with genetic syn- dromes, to ascertain their views on the most efficacious practices. As well as research on how elucidated cognitive and behavioural profiles are manifest in school settings, there is a need for work on how issues such as co-morbidity with conditions such as ASD, affect educational practice for chil- dren with genetic syndromes. There is a particular need to examine whether commonly used instructional methods (e.g. phonics-based reading instruction) work in particu- lar genetic syndromes and how such approaches might compare with other instructional methods. In this way, it may be possible to select approaches that lead to faster rates of skill acquisition and help decide whether to focus on capitalising on pheno- typic strengths or whether to intensively target phe-
  • 174. notypic weaknesses. Although intervention studies in genetic syndromes are difficult to carry out, a multiple baseline individual and small group studies approach can start to answer questions about phe- notypic approaches with relatively small number of participants. In the absence of evidence-based interventions school-based practitioners and educa- tors will continue to base their intervention approaches on prior clinical experience and profes- sional judgement rather than on evidence-based research. Much of the research in syndromes is driven by those working in clinical- or university- based research settings but there is also a need for classroom-based research and this will require strong links with those who work in educational settings. Research outlining current practices and teacher and parent views of their efficacy will be useful and not as resource intensive as intervention studies. 940 Journal of Intellectual Disability Research VOLUME 56 PART 10 OCTOBER 2012 C. Reilly • Aetiology in the classroom © 2012 The Author. Journal of Intellectual Disability Research © 2012 Blackwell Publishing Ltd With regard to current classroom practice and prospective training for educators the approach pre- sented here may be helpful with regard to support- ing educators understanding of when, and if, aetiology might impact on a child’s learning or
  • 175. behaviour. As Einfeld (2005) suggests educators cannot be expected to be aware of the behavioural phenotypes of all syndromes but being aware that different aetiologies can impact learning and behav- iour may allow teachers to seek out further advice and support. The concept of behavioural pheno- types implies probability, and a knowledge of inter- ventions that are likely to work for the majority of children with a syndrome should be considered along with other sources of data (e.g. individual assessment data), when formulating recommenda- tions for educational and behavioural interventions. In this way a ‘label’ can become ‘an enabler’ (O’Brien 2002), signposting educational profession- als to interventions that are more likely to lead to positive outcomes for the affected children. Knowl- edge of a genetic syndrome can help anticipate potential developmental vulnerabilities and resilien- cies, and in this way phenotypic research may make it possible to ‘know where to look’ for potential vul- nerabilities in early development (Fidler et al. 2007) and potential difficulties during the school years. However, even for children with distinct genetic aetiologies, aetiology is just one aspect of under- standing a child’s needs which will reflect a range of bio-psycho-social factors. References Abbeduto L. & Keller-Bell Y. (2002) Review of genetics and mental retardation syndromes: a new look at behav- ior and interventions. American Journal of Mental Retar- dation 107, 412–4. Alferink L. A. & Farmer-Dougan V. (2010) Brain-(not) based education: dangers of misunderstanding and mis-
  • 176. application of neuroscience research. Exceptionality 18, 42–52. Antshel K. M., Fremont W., Roizen N. J., Sphrintzen R., Higgins A. M., Dhamoon B. A. et al. (2006) ADHD, major depressive disorder, and simple phobias are prevalent psychiatric conditions in youth with velocar- diofacial syndrome. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry 45, 596–603. Antshel K. M., Freemont W. & Kates W. R. (2008) The neurocognitive phenotype in Velo-cardio-facial syn- drome: a developmental perspective. Developmental Dis- abilities Research Reviews 14, 43–51. Backes M., Genç B., Schrek J., Dorfler W., Lehmkuhl G. & Von Gotard A. (2000) Cognitive and behavioral profile of fragile X boys: correlations to molecular data. American Journal of Medical Genetics 95, 150–6. Becerra A. M., John A. E., Peregrine E. & Mervis C. B. (2008) Reading Abilities of 9–17-Year-Olds with Williams Syndrome: Impact of Reading Method. Symposium on Research in Child Language Disorders, Madison, WI. Belser R. C. & Sudhalter V. (2001) Conversational charac- teristics of children with fragile X syndrome: repetitive speech. American Journal of Mental Retardation 106, 128– 38. Berument S. K., Rutter M., Lord C., Pickles A. & Bailey A. (1999) Autism screening questionnaire: diagnostic validity. The British Journal of Psychiatry 175, 444–51. Blackhurst A. E. & Berndine W. H. (1993) An Introduction
  • 177. to Special Education. HarperCollins, New York. Blomberg S., Rosander M. & Andersson G. (2006) Fears, hyperacusis and musicality in Williams syndrome. Research in Developmental Disabilities 27, 668–80. Braden M. (2002) Academic Interventions. In: Fragile X Syndrome: Diagnosis, Treatment and Research, 3rd edn. (eds R. J. Hagerman & P. J. Hagerman), pp. 428–64. Johns Hopkins University Press, Baltimore, MD. Braden M. L. & Riley K. (2006) Educational strategies. The UK Fragile X Society Family Conference, Belfast, 11 November 2006. Brown-Chidsey R. & Steege M. W. (2005) Response to Intervention: Principles and Strategies for Effective Practice. Guildford Press, New York. Brun C., Obiols J. E., Cheema A., O’Connor R., Riddle J., DiMaria M. et al. (1995) Longitudinal IQ changes in fragile X females. Developmental Brain Dysfunction 8, 230–41. Buckley S. (1995) Teaching children with Down syndrome to read and write. In: Down Syndrome: Living Learning in the Community (eds L. Nadel & D. Rosenthal), pp. 158–69. Wiley-Liss, New York. Butler M. G. (1990) Prader-Willi syndrome: current understanding of cause and diagnosis. American Journal of Medical Genetics 35, 319–32. Campbell L. & Swillen A. (2005) The cognitive spectrum in velo-cardio-facial syndrome. In: Velo-Cardio-Facial Syndrome – A Model for Understanding Microdeletion Dis-
  • 178. orders (eds K. C. Murphy & P. J. Scambler), pp. 147–64. Cambridge University press, Cambridge. Campbell L., Daly E., Toal F., Stevens A., Azuma R., Karmiloff-Smith A. et al. (2009) Brain structural differ- ences associated with the behavioural research in Will- iams syndrome. Brain Research 1258, 96–107. Campbell L., McCabe K., Leadbetter K., Schall U., Loughland C. & Rih D. (2010) Visual scanning of faces 941 Journal of Intellectual Disability Research VOLUME 56 PART 10 OCTOBER 2012 C. Reilly • Aetiology in the classroom © 2012 The Author. Journal of Intellectual Disability Research © 2012 Blackwell Publishing Ltd in 22q11.2 deletion syndrome: attention to the mouth or eyes? Psychiatry Research 177, 211–5. Cassidy S. B. (1997) Prader-Willi syndrome. Journal of Medical Genetics 34, 917–23. Chedd N., Levine K. & Wharton R. H. (2006) Educa- tional considerations for children with Prader-Willi Syn- drome. In: Management of Prader-Willi Syndrome, 3rd edn. (eds M. G. Butler, P. D. K. Lee & B. Y. Whitman), pp. 302–16. Springer, New York. Clifford S., Dissanayke C., Bui Q. M., Huggins R., Tyalor A. K., Loesch D. Z. et al. (2007) Autism spectrum phe-
  • 179. notype in males and females with fragile X full mutation and permutation. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders 37, 738–47. Cohen I. L. (1995) A theoretical analysis of the role of hyperarousal in the learning and behaviour of fragile X males. Mental Retardation and Developmental Disabilities Research Reviews 1, 286–91. Cologon K., Cupples L. & Wyver S. (2011) Effects of tar- geted reading instruction on phonological awareness, phonic decoding and reading comprehension in children with Down syndrome. American Journal on Intellectual and Developmental Disabilities 116, 111–29. Cordeiro L., Ballinger E., Hagerman R. & Hessl D. (2011) Clinical assessment of DSM-IV anxiety disorders in fragile X syndrome: prevalence and characterisation. Journal of Neurodevelopmental Disorders 3, 57–67. Cornish K., Swainson R., Cunnington R., Wilding J., Morris P. & Jackson G. (2004) Do women with fragile X syndrome have problems in switching attention: pre- liminary findings from ERP and fMRI. Brain and Cogni- tion 54, 235–9. Cornish K. M., Munir F. & Cross G. (1998) The nature of the spatial deficit in young females with fragile X syndrome: a neuropsychological and molecular perspec- tive. Neuropsychologia 36, 1239–46. Cornish K. M., Munir F. & Cross G. (1999) Spatial cog- nition in males with Fragile X syndrome: evidence for a neuropsychological phenotype. Cortex 35, 263–71. Couzens D., Cuskelly M. & Haynes M. (2011) Cognitive
  • 180. development and Down syndrome: age-related change on the Stanford-Binet test (fourth edition). American Journal on Intellectual and Developmental Disabilities 116, 181–204. Cronister A., Hagerman R. J., Wittenberger M. & Amiri K. (1991) Mental impairment in cytogenetically positive fragile X females. American Journal of Medical Genetics 38, 503–4. Cupples L. & Iacono T. (2002) The efficacy of ‘whole word’ versus ‘analytic’ reading instruction for children with Down syndrome. Reading and Writing: An Interdisci- plinary Journal 15, 549–74. Cutler-Landsman D. (2007) Educating Children with Velo- Cardio-Facial Syndrome. Plural Publishing, San Diego, CA. Deruelle C., Mancini J., Livet M. O., Casse-Perot C. & de Schonene S. (1999) Configural and local processing of faces in children with Williams syndrome. Brain and Cognition 41, 278–98. Dew-Hughes D. (2004) Educating Children with Fragile X Syndrome. RoutledgeFalmer, London. Dykens E. M. (1995) Measuring behavioral phenotypes: provocations from the ‘new genetics. American Journal of Mental Retardation 99, 522–32. Dykens E. M. (1999) Direct effects of mental retardation syndromes: maladaptive behavior and psychopathology. International Review of Research in Mental Retardation 22, 1–23.
  • 181. Dykens E. M. (2000) Annotation: psychopathology in children with intellectual disability. Journal of Child Psy- chology and Psychiatry 41, 407–17. Dykens E. M. & Hodapp R. M. (1997) Treatment issues in genetic mental retardation syndromes. Professional Psychology: Research and Practice 28, 263–70. Dykens E. M. & Hodapp R. M. (2001) Research in mental retardation: toward an etiologic approach. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry 42, 49–71. Dykens E. M., Hodapp R. M., Walsh K. & Nash L. J. (1992) Profile, correlates, and trajectories of intelligence in Prader-Willi syndrome. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry 31, 1125–30. Dykens E. M., Hodapp R. M., Ort S. I. & Leckman J. F. (1993) Trajectory of adaptive behaviour in males with fragile X syndrome. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders 23, 135–45. Dykens E. M., Hodapp R. M. & Evans D. W. (1994) Pro- files and development of adaptive behaviour in children with Down syndrome. American Journal of Mental Retar- dation 98, 580–7. Dykens E. M., Hodapp R. M. & Finucane B. (2000) Genetics and Mental Retardation Syndromes: A New Look at Behaviour and Interventions. Brookes, Baltimore, MD. Einfeld S. (2005) Behaviour problems in children with genetic disorders causing intellectual disabilities. Educa- tional Psychology 25, 341–6. Ekstein S., Weill B., Glick M., Kay B. & Berger I. (2011)
  • 182. Down syndrome and attention-deficit/hyperactivity dis- order (ADHD). Journal of Child Neurology 26, 1290–5. Esbensen A. J. & Seltzer M. M. (2011) Accounting for the ‘Down Syndrome Advantage’. American Journal on Intel- lectual and Developmental Disabilities 116, 3–15. Feinstein C., Eliez S., Blasey C. & Reiss A. L. (2002) Psy- chiatric disorders and behavioral problems in children with velocardiofacial syndrome: usefulness as pheno- typic indicators of schizophrenia risk. Biological Psychia- try 51, 312–8. Fidler D. J., Hodapp R. M. & Dykens E. M. (2002) Behavioral phenotypes and special education: parent report of educational issues for children with Down syn- 942 Journal of Intellectual Disability Research VOLUME 56 PART 10 OCTOBER 2012 C. Reilly • Aetiology in the classroom © 2012 The Author. Journal of Intellectual Disability Research © 2012 Blackwell Publishing Ltd drome, Prader-Willi syndrome, and Williams syndrome. The Journal of Special Education 36, 80–8. Fidler D. J., Philofsky A. & Hepburn S. L. (2007) Lan- guage phenotypes and intervention planning: bridging research and practice. Mental Retardation and Develop- mental Disabilities Research Reviews 13, 47–57.
  • 183. Fine S. E., Weissman A., Gerdes M., Pinto-Martin J., Zackai E. H., McDonald-McGinn D. M. et al. (2005) Autism spectrum disorders and symptoms in children with molecularly confirmed 22q11.2 deletion syndrome. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders 35, 461– 70. Flynt J. & Yule W. (1994) Behavioural phenotyes. In: Child and Adolescent Psychiatry: Modern Approaches, 3rd edn. (eds M. Rutter, E. Taylor & L. Hersov), pp. 666–87. Blackwell Scientific, London. Forness S. & Kavale K. (1994) The Balkanization of special education: proliferation of categories for ‘new’ behavioural disorders. Education and Treatment of Chil- dren 17, 215–27. Freund L. S. & Reiss A. L. (1991) Cognitive profiles asso- ciated with the fra(X) syndrome in males and females. American Journal of Medical Genetics 38, 542–7. Freund L. S., Reiss A. L. & Abrams M. (1993) Psychiatric disorders associated with fragile X in young females. Pediatrics 91, 321–9. Garner C., Callias M. & Turk J. (1999) Executive function and theory of mind performance of boys with fragile X syndrome. Journal of Intellectual Disability Research 43, 466–74. Gath A. & Gumley D. (1986) Behaviour problems in retarded children with special reference to Down’s syn- drome. The British Journal of Psychiatry 149, 156–61. Goetz K., Hulme C., Brigstocke S., Carroll J. M., Nasir L. & Snowling M. (2008) Training reading and
  • 184. phoneme awareness skills in children with Down syn- drome. Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal 21, 395–412. Golding-Kushner K. J., Weller G. & Sphrintzen R. J. (1985) Velo-cardio-facial syndrome: language and psy- chological profiles. Journal of Craniofacial Genetics and Developmental Biology 5, 259–66. Goodship J., Cross I., Li Ling J. & Wren C. (1998) A population study of chromosome 22q11 deletions in infancy. Archives of Disease in Childhood 79, 348–51. Gothelf D., Presburger G., Levy D., Nahmani A., Burg M., Berant M. et al. (2004) Genetic, developmental, and physical factors associated with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder in patients, with velocardiofacial syndrome. American Journal of Medical Genetics. Part B 126B, 116–21. Green T., Gothelf D., Glaser B., Debbané M., Frisch A., Kotler M. et al. (2009) Psychiatric disorders and intel- lectual functioning throughout development in velo- cardio-facial syndrome (22q11.2 deletion) syndrome. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry 48, 1060–8. Hagerman R. J. (1987) Fragile X syndrome. Current Prob- lems in Pediatrics 17, 627–74. Hall S. S. (2009) Treatments for fragile X syndrome: a closer look at the data. Developmental Disabilities Research Reviews 15, 353–60. Hall S. S., Debernardis G. M. & Reiss A. L. (2006) The
  • 185. acquisition of stimulus equivalence in individuals with fragile X syndrome. Journal of Intellectual Disability Research 50, 643–51. Hall S. S., Maynes N. P. & Reiss A. L. (2009) Using per- centile schedules to increase eye contact duration in children with fragile X syndrome. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis 42, 171–6. Hallahan D. P. & Kauffman J. M. (2000) Exceptional Chil- dren: Introduction to Special Education, 8th edn. Allyn and Bacon, Needham Heights, MA. Haxby J. V. (1989) Neuropsychological evaluations of adults with Down’s syndrome: patterns of selective impairment in non-demented old adults. Journal of Mental Deficiency Research 88, 193–210. Hills-Epstein J. L., Riley K. & Sobesky W. E. (2002) The treatment of emotional and behavioural problems. In: Fragile X Syndrome: Diagnosis, Treatment and Research, 3rd edn. (eds R. J. Hagerman & P. J. Hagerman), pp. 339–62. Johns Hopkins University Press, Baltimore, MD. Hodapp R. M. (1997) Direct and indirect behavioral effects of different genetic disorders of mental retarda- tion. American Journal of Mental Retardation 102, 67–79. Hodapp R. M. (2004) Behavioural phenotypes: going beyond the two group approach. International Review of Research in Mental Retardation 29, 1–30. Hodapp R. M. & Dykens E. M. (2001) Strengthening behavioral research on genetic mental retardation syn- dromes. American Journal of Mental Retardation 106,
  • 186. 4–15. Hodapp R. M. & Dykens E. M. (2009) Intellectual dis- abilities and child psychiatry: looking to the future. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry 50, 99–107. Hodapp R. M. & Fidler D. J. (1999) Special education and genetics: connections for the 21st century. The Journal of Special Education 33, 130–7. Hodapp R. M., Leckman J. F., Dykens S., Sparrow D., Zelinsky D. & Ort S. I. (1992) K-ABC profiles in chil- dren with fragile X syndrome, Down syndrome, and non-specific mental retardation. American Journal of Mental Retardation 97, 39–46. Hodapp R. M., Evans D. & Gray F. L. (1999) Intellectual development in children with Down syndrome. In: Down Syndrome: A Review of Current Knowledge (eds J. A. Rodal, J. Perera & L. Nadel), pp. 124–32. Whurr Publishers, London. 943 Journal of Intellectual Disability Research VOLUME 56 PART 10 OCTOBER 2012 C. Reilly • Aetiology in the classroom © 2012 The Author. Journal of Intellectual Disability Research © 2012 Blackwell Publishing Ltd Hodapp R. M., Des Jardin J. L. & Ricci L. A. (2003) Genetic syndromes of Mental Retardation: should they matter for the early interventionist? Infants and Young
  • 187. Children 16, 152–60. Holland A. J., Whittington J. E., Butler J., Webb T., Boer H. & Clarke D. (2003) Behavioural phenotypes associ- ated with specific genetic disorders: evidence from a population-based study of people with Prader-Willi syn- drome. Psychological Medicine 33, 141–53. Horsteimer D. (2004) Teaching Math to People with Down Syndrome and Other Hands on Learners. Woodbine House, Bethesda, MA. Howlin P., Davies M. & Udwin O. (1998) Cognitive func- tioning in adults with Williams syndrome. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry 39, 163–89. Jones W., Bellugi U., Lai Z., Chiles M., Reilly J., Lincoln A. et al. (2000) Hypersociability: the social and affective phenotype of Williams syndrome. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 12, 30–46. Kates W., Antshel J. M., Fremont W. P., Sphrintzen R. J., Strunge L. A., Burnette C. P. et al. (2007) Comparing phenotypes in patients with idiopathic autism to patients with velocardiofacial syndrome (22q11 DS) with and without Autism. American Journal of Medical Genetics. Part A 143A, 2642–50. Kau A. S. M., Reider E. E., Payne L., Meyer W. A. & Freund L. (2000) Early behaviour signs of psychiatric phenotypes in fragile X syndrome. American Journal of Mental Retardation 105, 266–9. Kaufman A. S. & Kaufman N. L. (1983) Kaufman Assess- ment Battery for Children (K-ABC). American Guidance Service, Circle Pines, MN.
  • 188. Koenig K., Klin A. & Schultz R. (2005) Deficits in social attribution ability in Prader–Willi syndrome. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders 34, 573–82. Langthorne P., McGill P. & O’Reilly M. (2007) Incorpo- rating ‘motivation’ into the functional analysis of chal- lenging behaviour. On the interactive and integrative potential of the motivating operation. Behavior Modifica- tion 31, 466–87. Langthorne P., McGill P., O’Reilly M., Lang R., Machal- icek W., Chan J. M. et al. (2011) Examining the func- tion of problem behaviour in fragile X syndrome: preliminary experimental analysis. American Journal on Intellectual and Developmental Disabilities 116, 65–80. Lee T. H., Blasey C. M., Dyer-Friedman J., Glaser B., Reiss A. & Eliez S. (2005) From research to practice: teacher and Paediatrician awareness of phenotypic traits in neurogenetic syndromes. American Journal of Mental Retardation 110, 100–6. Levy Y. & Antebi V. (2004) Word reading and reading- related skills in Hebrew-speaking adolescents with Will- iams syndrome. Neurocase 10, 444–51. Levy Y. & Ebstein R. P. (2009) Research review: crossing syndrome boundaries in the search for endophenotypes. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry 50, 657–68. Leyfer O. T., Woodruff-Borden J., Klein-Tasman B. P., Fricke J. S. & Mervis C. B. (2006) Prevalence of psychi- atric disorders in 4 to 16-year-olds with Williams syn- drome. American Journal of Medical Genetics. Part B 141B, 615–22.
  • 189. Lowenthal R., Paula C. S., Schwartzman J. S., Brunioni D. & Mercadante M. T. (2007) Prevalence of pervasive developmental disorders in Down’s syndrome. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders 37, 1394–5. Martens M. A., Wilson S. J. & Reutens D. C. (2008) Research review: Williams syndrome: a critical review of the cognitive, behavioral, and neuroanatomical pheno- type. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry 49, 576– 608. Mazzocco M. M. M., Pennington B. F. & Hagerman R. J. (1993) The neurocognitive phenotype of female carriers of Fragile X: additional evidence for specificity. Journal of Developmental and Behavioral Pediatrics 14, 328–35. Mervis C. B. & John A. E. (2010) Cognitive and behav- ioral characteristics of children with Williams syndrome: implications for intervention approaches. American Journal of Medical Genetics. Part C 154C, 229–48. Mervis C. B. & Klein-Tasman B. P. (2000) Williams syn- drome: cognition, personality, and adaptive behavior. Mental Retardation and Developmental Disabilities Research Reviews 6, 148–58. Mervis C. M. & Morris C. A. (2007) Williams syndrome. In: Neurogenetic Developmental Disorders:Variation of Manifestation in Childhood (eds M. M. M. Mazzocco & J. L. Ross), pp. 199–262. MIT Press, Cambridge, MA. Miller J. F. (1992) Development of speech and language in children with Down syndrome. In: Down Syndrome: Advances in Medical Care (eds I. T. Lott & E. E. McCoy), pp. 39–50. Wiley, Chichester.
  • 190. Moss J. & Howlin P. (2009) Autism spectrum disorders in genetic syndromes: implications for diagnosis, interven- tion and understanding the wider autism spectrum disorder population. Journal of Intellectual Disability Research 53, 852–73. Murphy K. C., Jones L. A. & Owen M. J. (1999) High rates of schizophrenia in adults with velo-cardio-facial syndrome. Archives of General Psychiatry 56, 949–5. Niklasson L., Rasmussen P., Óskarsdóttir S. & Gillberg C. (2002) Chromosome 22q.11 deletion syndrome (CATCH22): neuropsychiatric and neuropsychological aspects. Developmental Medicine and Child Neurology 44, 44–50. Niklasson L., Rasmussen P., Óskarsdóttir S. & Gillberg C. (2005) Attention defecits in children with 22q.11 dele- tion syndrome. Developmental Medicine and Child Neurol- ogy 47, 803–7. 944 Journal of Intellectual Disability Research VOLUME 56 PART 10 OCTOBER 2012 C. Reilly • Aetiology in the classroom © 2012 The Author. Journal of Intellectual Disability Research © 2012 Blackwell Publishing Ltd Niklasson L., Rasmussen P., Óskarsdóttir S. & Gillberg C. (2009) Autism, ADHD, mental retardation, and behav- ior problems in 100 individuals with 22q11 deletion syn-
  • 191. drome. Research in Developmental Disabilities 30, 763–73. O’Brien G. (2002) The clinical relevance of behavioural phe- notypes. In: Behavioural Phenotypes in Clinical Practice (ed. G. O’Brien), pp. 1–12. Mac Keith Press, London. Oelwein P. L. (1995) Teaching Reading to Children with Down Syndrome. A Guide for Parents and Teachers. Wood- bine House, Bethesda, MA. Porter M. A. & Coltheart M. (2005) Cognitive heteroge- neity in Williams syndrome. Developmental Neuropsychol- ogy 27, 275–306. Pueschel S. M., Gallagher P. L., Zartler A. S. & Pezzullo J. C. (1987) Cognitive and learning processes in chil- dren with Down syndrome. Research in Developmental Disabilities 8, 21–37. Radstaake M., Didden R., Bolio M., Lang R., Lancioni G. E. & Curfs L. M. G. (2011) Functional assessment and behavioral treatment of skin picking in a teenage girl with Prader-Willi syndrome. Clinical Case Studies 10, 67–78. Rosner B. A., Hodapp R. M., Fidler D. J., Sagun J. N. & Dykens E. M. (2004) Social competence in persons with Prader-Willi, Williams and Down syndromes. Journal of Applied Research in Intellectual Disabilities 17, 209–17. Rutter M., Bailey A. & Lord C. (2003) The Social Commu- nication Questionnaire. Western Psychological Services, Los Angeles, CA. Saunders S. (1999) Teaching children with fragile X syn-
  • 192. drome. British Journal of Special Education 26, 6–79. Scharfenaker S., O’Connor R., Stackhouse T., Braden M. L. & Gray K. (2002) An integrated approach to intervention. In: Fragile X Syndrome: Diagnosis, Treatment and Research, 3rd edn. (eds R. J. Hagerman & P. J. Hagerman), pp. 363 –427. Johns Hopkins University Press, Baltimore, MD. Searcy Y. M., Lincoln A., Rose F., Klima E., Bavar N. & Korenberg J. R. (2004) The relationship between age and IQ in adults with Williams syndrome. American Journal of Mental Retardation 109, 231–6. Semel E. & Rosner S. R. (2003) Understanding Williams Syndrome: Behavioral Patterns and Interventions. Lawrence Erlbaum, Mahwah, NJ. Sherman S. (2002) Epidemiology. In: Fragile X Syndrome: Diagnosis, Treatment and Research, 3rd edn. (eds R. J. Hagerman & P. J. Hagerman), pp. 136–68. Johns Hopkins University Press, Baltimore, MD. Sherman S. L., Allen E. G., Bean L. H. & Freeman S. B. (2007) Epidemiology of Down syndrome. Mental Retar- dation and Developmental Disabilities Research Reviews 13, 221–7. Shprintzen R. J. (2008) Velo-cardio-facial syndrome: 30 years of study. Developmental Disabilities Research Reviews 14, 3–10. Starr E. M., Foy J. B., Cramer K. M. & Singh H. (2006) How are schools doing? Parental perceptions of children with autism spectrum disorder, Down syndrome and learning disabilities. Education and Training in Develop-
  • 193. mental Disabilities 41, 315–32. Stauder J. E., Brinkman M. J. & Curfs L. M. G. (2002) Multi-modal P3 deflation of event related brain activity in Prader-Willi syndrome. Neuroscience Letters 327, 99–102. Stokes J. V. & Luiselli J. K. (2009) Applied behavior analy- sis assessment and intervention for health threatening self-injury in an adult with Prader–Willisyndrome. Clini- cal Case Studies 8, 38–47. Stromme P., Bjornstad M. D. & Ramstad M. D. (2002) Prevalence estimation of Williams syndrome. Journal of Child Neurology 17, 269–71. Swillen A., Vanputte L., Cracco J., Maes B., Ghesquiere P., Devriendt K. et al. (1997) Neuropsychological, learn- ing and psychosocial profile of primary school aged children with the velo-cardio-facial syndrome (22q11 deletion): evidence for a nonverbal learning disability? Child Neuropsychology 5, 230–41. Symons F. J., Clark R. D., Roberts J. P. & Bailey D. B. (2001) Classroom behaviour of elementary school-age boys with fragile X syndrome. The Journal of Special Education 34, 194–202. Udwin O., Yule W. & Howlin P. (2007) Williams Syn- drome. Guidelines for Teachers. The Williams Syndrome Foundation, Tonbridge. Van der Molen M. J. W., Huizanga M., Huizenga H. M., Ridderinkhof K. R., Van der Molen M. W., Hamel B. J. C. et al. (2010) Profiling fragile X syndrome in males: strengths and weaknesses in cognitive abilities.
  • 194. Research in Developmental Disabilities 31, 426–39. Veltman M. W. M., Thompson R. J., Roberts S. E., Thomas N. S., Whittington J. & Bolton P. (2004) Prader-Willi syndrome: a study comparing deletion and uniparental disomy cases with reference to autism spec- trum disorders. European Child and Adolescent Psychiatry 13, 42–50. de Vries B. B. A., Wiegers A. M., Smits A. P. T., Mohkam- sing S., Duivenvoorden H. J., Fryns J. P. et al. (1996) Mental status of females with the FMR1 gene full muta- tion. American Journal of Medical Genetics 58, 1025–32. Wang Y. K., Samos C. H., Peoples R., Perez-Jurado L. A., Nusse R. & Francke U. (1997) A novel human homo- logue of the Drosophilia frizzled wnt receptor gene binds wingless protein and is in the Williams syndrome deletion at 7q11.23. Human Molecular Genetics 6, 465– 72. Whitman B. Y. & Jackson K. (2006) Tools for Psychologi- cal and Behavioural Management. In: Management of 945 Journal of Intellectual Disability Research VOLUME 56 PART 10 OCTOBER 2012 C. Reilly • Aetiology in the classroom © 2012 The Author. Journal of Intellectual Disability Research © 2012 Blackwell Publishing Ltd Prader-Willi Syndrome, 3rd edn. (eds M. G. Butler,
  • 195. P. D. K. Lee & B. Y. Whitman), pp. 317–43. Springer, New York. Whittington J. & Holland T. (2004) Prader-Willi Syndrome. Development and Manifestations. University Press, Cambridge. Wigren M. & Hansen S. (2005) ADHD symptoms and insistence on sameness in Prader-Willi syndrome. Journal of Intellectual Disability Research 49, 449–56. Woodcock K. A., Oliver C. & Humphreys G. W. (2009) Task switching deficits and repetitive behaviour in genetic neurodevelopmental disorders: data from chil- dren with Prader-Willi syndrome chromosome 15q11-q13 deletion and boys with fragile X syndrome. Cognitive Neuropsychology 26, 172–94. Woodcock K. A., Humphreys G. W., Oliver C. & Hansen P. C. (2010) Neural correlates of task switching in paternal 15q11-q13 deletion Prader-Willi syndrome. Brain Research 1363, 128–42. Woodcock R. W. & Johnson M. B. (1990) Woodcock- Johnson Psycho-Educational Battery-Revised. Riverside, Itasca, IL. York A., Von Fraunhofer N., Turk J. & Sedgwick P. (1999) Fragile X syndrome, Down’s syndrome and autism: awareness and knowledge amongst special educators. Journal of Intellectual Disability Research 43, 314–24. Zwaigenbaum L., Dick P., Handley-Derry M., Malone M. & Jacobson S. (2006) ‘N of 1’ trails of methylphenidate
  • 196. in two children with Williams syndrome and attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. Journal of Developmental and Physical Disabilities 18, 45–58. Accepted 31 January 2012 946 Journal of Intellectual Disability Research VOLUME 56 PART 10 OCTOBER 2012 C. Reilly • Aetiology in the classroom © 2012 The Author. Journal of Intellectual Disability Research © 2012 Blackwell Publishing Ltd This document is a scanned copy of a printed document. No warranty is given about the accuracy of the copy. Users should refer to the original published version of the material.