SlideShare a Scribd company logo
1 of 109
311
AFFECTIV TEMPERAMENTS AND PERSONALIY TRAITS
IN
ADDICT SUBJECTS
Alexander KELEMEN 1
Maria PUSCHITA
Delia PODEA
ABSTRACT
Drug abuse is a phenomenon that can be found in almost all
studied societies from
antiquity to nowadays. Much of the designed literature has
described personality disorder
constructs for addict subjects (Vincent, 2009).
Building on previous work, and after reviewing the theory of
affective temperaments of
Akiskal and Mallya, we examined the relevance of affective
temperament and personality
measures in patients with alcohol and heroin addictions.
The patients were compared, regarding affective temperaments
and personality lines,
according to the Akiskal formulation and EPQ questionnaire, in
which 50 were heroin addicts
and 50 were alcohol addict people, both of them sharing similar
backgrounds.
As a result no differences were observed between heroin
addicts and alcohol users on
either cyclothymic or hyperthymic scales. Significant
discrepancies were noted in depressive
and irritability scales, on which heroin addicts scored higher.
In a multivariate discriminant analysis, mainly depressive and
irritable traits show a
distinction between heroin addicts and alcohol users. Our data
suggest a new hypothesis,
stating that some of hyperthymic and irritable traits, could
represent the temperamental profile
of heroin addicts. Personality traits are also associated with
heroin and alcohol use and
extroversion and psycho-emotional instability are common
features; the motivation for testing is
higher in heroin users and alcohol abusers tend to dissimulate
more frequently.
KEY WORDS: alcohol abusers, heroin addicts, affective
temperaments and
personality
JEL: I10
INTRODUCTION
The temperament and personality characteristics of alcoholics
and drug addicts
have been a major issue in the field of substance abuse research,
but the results reported
are seldom comparable, on account of the differences in the
means of assessment and
the conceptualizations used (Basiaux et al, (2001) ).
Difficult temperament, antisocial personality disorder and
borderline, or
affectively unstable profiles have been associated with alcohol
abuse (De Jong et al,
(1993) ).
Alcohol abuse has also been linked to axis I mood instability
while the role of
axis II mood instability remained unclear and defined along
different constructs.
More recently, some authors have been building on the concept
of an affective
spectrum, meaning by tha, that a group of syndromes varying in
severity and symptom
quality, but all representing degrees, stages, or variants of the
same basic biological
1 PhD. Faculty of Medicine, Vasile Goldis Western University
of Arad, Romania, e-mail:
[email protected]
312
substrate (Perugi et al, (2002) ). The study of temperaments can
play a crucial role in
identifying predictors of affective instability in healthy
individuals. Temperamental
traits also appear to be important as putative prognostic and
therapeutic variables in
patients with full-blown affective disorders (Cassano et al,
(1992) ).
The International Consortium of Psychiatric Epidemiology has
confirmed the
high comorbidity in community-drawn samples between
substance use disorders and
affective temperaments lines. In the same way, associations
between substance use and
specific personality traits (such as novelty seeking, harm
avoidance or
antisocial personality) have also been extensively documented
(Chakroun, 2004). Much
of the designed literature has described personality disorder
constructs for addict
subjects (Vincent, (2009) ).
The present paper is design to be a psychological study, which
consists in
examine the relevance of affective temperament and personality
measures in alcohol
and heroin use subjects.
Temperament is defined as a biological disposition,
corresponding to a
constitutional substrate. Temperament is expressed through a
series of signs and
features usually manifested by a certain stability of mood,
attitudes towards the
environment, sensitivity to external stimulus and characteristic
modes of reaction
(Vincent, (2013) ).
Most clinicians agree that early-age experiences account for a
wide variability of
adult individual personality traits, so the development of
personality traits is usually
considered to be primarily through crucial environmental
challenges and experiences.
Nevertheless, since the '80s several authors have maintained
that some early and stable
personality traits exist, which can be described as features of
premorbid phases of major
Psychiatric Illnesses (Maremmani et al, (2009) ).
Consistent with the concept of an affective spectrum, the study
of temperaments
is crucial, in the context of identify early predictors of future
affective instability even in
healthy individuals. Temperamental traits of personality, also
prove important as
putative prognostic and therapeutic variables in patients with
alcohol and heroin
addiction.
OBJECTIVES:
Our study first objective is the evaluation of the affective
temperamental traits
and the personality traits in alcohol and heroin addicted
patients; the second objective is
the evaluation of differences between alcohol addicted patients
and heroin users.
METHODS:
The sample is composed of 100 adult patients (50 with a alcohol
and 50 heroin
addiction diagnosis), enrolled at the Department of Psychiatry
of the Municipal Clinic
Hospital, Cluj.
Patients have fill out the TEMPS-A (Temperament Evaluation
of Memphis,
Pisa, Paris and San Diego - autoquestionnaire version) and the
EPQ questionnaire, so
we compared, regarding affective temperaments and personality
lines, according to the
Akiskal formulation and Eysenck personality questionnaire
formulation.
All patients included in the study signed informed consent.
Both the consent
form and the experimental procedures were approved by the
competent ethics
committees in accordance with internationally accepted criteria
for ethical research. The
research was conducted in accordance with the ethical standards
of the Declaration of
Helsinki (1964). The study was conducted with the approval of
the Ethics and Research
313
Committee of the Vasile Goldis Western University of Arad and
Cluj Municipal
Hospital.
All patients were evaluated during the stabilization phase to
avoid possible
interferences due to the acute phase of their illness in the
temperament and personality
questionnaires (TEMPS-A and EPQ).
To achieve the research objectives, subjects were evaluated
once during the 12
months. Each subject was interviewed and completed all scales
above, as paper and
pencil, so were assessed personality traits in terms of the
psychometrics scale,
respectively affective temperament types by Akiskal scale.
Data Analysis:
Analyzes based on scores E, N, L and P (EPQ questionnaire's
dimensions)
indicates the fact that the majority of respondents can be
classified as extroverted
persons, highly unstable psycho-emotional and almost the lack
of some schizoid
tendencies from the sample.
Out of the 100 individuals, more then half (64 %) have
obtained an E score of 9
points, denoting that they can be characterized as persons with
more accelerate
processes of excitement and inhibitions of the SNC, who tend to
adopt easier risky and
scandalous behavior, with a lower control of the emotional
reactions and a lower
tolerance to frustration, which they associate as their lifestyle.
The following table consist of the exacts distribution of the 100
respondents
involved in the study and highlights that the majority obtain a 9
points score, meanwhile
only 16 subjects obtained higher scores.
Table 1.
Distribution of individuals according to the score E
Frequency Percent Valid Percent
Cumulative
Percent
Valid 3 10 10.0 10.0 10.0
5 1 1.0 1.0 11.0
6 9 9.0 9.0 20.0
9 64 64.0 64.0 84.0
11 1 1.0 1.0 85.0
12 2 2.0 2.0 87.0
13 7 7.0 7.0 94.0
14 1 1.0 1.0 95.0
15 2 2.0 2.0 97.0
16 2 2.0 2.0 99.0
17 1 1.0 1.0 100.0
Total 100 100.0 100.0
Source: Carried out by the Author, Own calculation, 2013
Histogram corresponding variable gives us an image regarding
the respondents
distribution according to the score obtained for E category,
being easy to observe that
the majority of the respondents s
Source: Carried out by the Author, Own calculation, 2013
Regarding this research, is noticeable that the most common
value for dimension
N was 12 points, 25 % of the re
We find that the distribution of one variable is abnormal, 25 %
of the subjects
are placed in the middle range (at the value of 23 points), but
more than half of all
(52%) stand near the maximum value of the range (getting sc
also noted a tremendous difference between the range’s limits,
the minimum value
being 4 points and the maximum 23 points.
The existing data reveal that most subjects are supra
react strongly to stimulus
various emotional experiences.
314
Histogram corresponding variable gives us an image regarding
the respondents
distribution according to the score obtained for E category,
being easy to observe that
the majority of the respondents stand around the value of 9
points.
Respondents distribution for E scale
Source: Carried out by the Author, Own calculation, 2013
Regarding this research, is noticeable that the most common
value for dimension
N was 12 points, 25 % of the respondents getting that value.
We find that the distribution of one variable is abnormal, 25 %
of the subjects
are placed in the middle range (at the value of 23 points), but
more than half of all
(52%) stand near the maximum value of the range (getting
scores of over 20 points). Is
also noted a tremendous difference between the range’s limits,
the minimum value
being 4 points and the maximum 23 points.
The existing data reveal that most subjects are supra-emotional
persons, who
and who find it hard to come back to their balance after
various emotional experiences.
Histogram corresponding variable gives us an image regarding
the respondents
distribution according to the score obtained for E category,
being easy to observe that
Figure 1.
Respondents distribution for E scale
Source: Carried out by the Author, Own calculation, 2013
Regarding this research, is noticeable that the most common
value for dimension
We find that the distribution of one variable is abnormal, 25 %
of the subjects
are placed in the middle range (at the value of 23 points), but
more than half of all
ores of over 20 points). Is
also noted a tremendous difference between the range’s limits,
the minimum value
emotional persons, who
and who find it hard to come back to their balance after
315
Table 2.
Distribution of individuals according to the score N
Frequency Percent Valid Percent Cumulative Percent
Valid 4 2 2.0 2.0 2.0
6 1 1.0 1.0 3.0
7 10 10.0 10.0 13.0
9 1 1.0 1.0 14.0
10 2 2.0 2.0 16.0
12 25 25.0 25.0 41.0
14 2 2.0 2.0 43.0
16 5 5.0 5.0 48.0
21 20 20.0 20.0 68.0
22 22 22.0 22.0 90.0
23 10 10.0 10.0 100.0
Total
100 100.0 100.0
Source: Carried out by the Author, Own calculation, 2013
Histogram corresponding variable provides us an image
regarding the
distribution of respondents according the score obtained for N
category.
Figure 2.
Respondents distribution for N scale
Source: Carried out by the Author, Own calcula
In a multivariate discriminant analysis, mainly depressive and
irritable traits
show a distinction between heroin addicts and alcohol users .
respondents 78 % have offered positive answers to questions
which give a profile with
saddest mood, ruminative and willing to apprehension. Out of
these, 55% are part of
the alcohol consumers category and 45% are part of the heroin
consumers category. We
can state that alcoholic respondents tend to be in a sad mood.
In terms of frequenc
irritable-choleric disposition, measured trough the confession of
the respondents
regarding on how often they get irritable, choleric, we notice
that 84% of the subjects
have given us answers positive rated as irritable
than half belong to the alcohol consumers category, therefore
we conclude that alcohol
consumers category tend to be more often choleric than the
heroin consumers category.
Conclusions:
Our data suggest a new hypothesis:
• some of hyperthymic
profile of heroin addicts.
• some of depressive and irritable traits, could represent the
temperamental
profile of heroin addicts.
• personality traits are associated with heroin and alcohol use.
• extroversion and psycho
• the motivation for testing is higher in heroin users, alcohol
abusers tend to
dissimulate more frequently.
Considerations:
The scientific community has recently examined whether
correlations
between affective temperaments and substance abuse disorders.
We try to summarized some of these correlations, comparing
two groups of
addict subjects (heroin vs alcohol).
316
Source: Carried out by the Author, Own calculation, 2013
In a multivariate discriminant analysis, mainly depressive and
irritable traits
show a distinction between heroin addicts and alcohol users .
Out of the 100
respondents 78 % have offered positive answers to questions
which give a profile with
saddest mood, ruminative and willing to apprehension. Out of
these, 55% are part of
the alcohol consumers category and 45% are part of the heroin
consumers category. We
can state that alcoholic respondents tend to be in a sad mood.
In terms of frequenc
choleric disposition, measured trough the confession of the
respondents
regarding on how often they get irritable, choleric, we notice
that 84% of the subjects
have given us answers positive rated as irritable-choleric
disposition. Out of
than half belong to the alcohol consumers category, therefore
we conclude that alcohol
consumers category tend to be more often choleric than the
heroin consumers category.
Our data suggest a new hypothesis:
some of hyperthymic and irritable traits, could represent the
temperamental
profile of heroin addicts.
some of depressive and irritable traits, could represent the
temperamental
profile of heroin addicts.
personality traits are associated with heroin and alcohol use.
roversion and psycho-emotional instability are common features
the motivation for testing is higher in heroin users, alcohol
abusers tend to
dissimulate more frequently.
The scientific community has recently examined whether
correlations
between affective temperaments and substance abuse disorders.
We try to summarized some of these correlations, comparing
two groups of
addict subjects (heroin vs alcohol).
tion, 2013
In a multivariate discriminant analysis, mainly depressive and
irritable traits
Out of the 100
respondents 78 % have offered positive answers to questions
which give a profile with a
saddest mood, ruminative and willing to apprehension. Out of
these, 55% are part of
the alcohol consumers category and 45% are part of the heroin
consumers category. We
can state that alcoholic respondents tend to be in a sad mood.
In terms of frequency of
choleric disposition, measured trough the confession of the
respondents
regarding on how often they get irritable, choleric, we notice
that 84% of the subjects
choleric disposition. Out of these more
than half belong to the alcohol consumers category, therefore
we conclude that alcohol
consumers category tend to be more often choleric than the
heroin consumers category.
and irritable traits, could represent the temperamental
some of depressive and irritable traits, could represent the
temperamental
emotional instability are common features
the motivation for testing is higher in heroin users, alcohol
abusers tend to
The scientific community has recently examined whether
correlations exist
We try to summarized some of these correlations, comparing
two groups of
317
The correlation between affective temperaments line,
personality traits and
substance abuse disorders is generally accepted. Some of these
can be considered
expressions of the heroin and alcohol use.
In heroin addicts and alcoholics the role of “these traited” can
be extended from
the area of full-blown affective disorders to that of affective
temperaments
(Maremmani, 2005). What emerges is the central role of
cyclothymia, with irritable
traits discovered in a group of heroin addicts, and depressive
traits in alcoholics. While
the cyclothymic temperament qualifies as a real temperamental
risk factor for the
development of these diseases, irritable and depressive traits,
which differentiated these
two groups of subjects, may be considered not so much a
premorbid condition, but,
rather, as a simple association (Vincent, 2013).
After reviewing the theory of affective temperaments of
Akiskal and Mallya, we
will discuss affective temperaments in heroin addicts and
alcoholics, with the aim of
providing an “at-risk temperamental profile” for the
development of substance abuse
disorders. A working hypothesis is then formulated to help
explain how temperamental
profile may promote the initiation of substance use and
contribute to the development of
addiction.
REFERENCES
Akiskal H.S., Placidi G.F., Signoretta S., Liguori A., Gervasi
R., Maremmani I., Mallya
G., Puzantian V.R. (1998). TEMPS-I: -delineating the most
discriminant traits of the
cyclothymic, depressive, hyperthymic and irritable
temperaments in a nonpatient population. J.
Affect. Disorders. 51, 7–19.
Akiskal H.S., Akiskal K.K., Haykal R.F., Manning J.S., Connor
P. (2005). TEMPS-A:
progress towards validation of a self-rated clinical version of
the Temperament Evaluation of
the Memphis, Pisa, Paris, and San Diego Autoquestionnaire. J.
Affect. Disorders. 85, 3-16.
Hagop S. Akiskal MD; Robert M. A. Hirschfeld MD; Boghos I.
Yerevanian MD.
(2005) "The Relationship of Personality to Affective
Disorders." N.p., n.d. Web.
Kaplan H., Sadock B.J. (2000) “Substance Related Disorders”,
Comprehensive
Textbook of Psychiatry, Seventh Edition, Vol. I, Ed. Lippincott
Williams & Wilkins,
Philadelphia, USA, 11: 924-1095.
Maremmani, Angelo Giovanni Icro, Luca Rovai, Fabio Rugani,
Matteo Pacini,
Francesco Lamanna, Silvia Bacciardi, Giulio Perugi, Joseph
Deltito, Liliana Dell'Osso, and Icro
Maremmani, (2012), “National Center for Biotechnology
Information”. U.S. National Library
of Medicine.
Maremmani I, Pacini M, Popovic D, Romano A, Maremmani
AG, Perugi G, Deltito J,
Akiskal K, Akiskal H. (2009) "Affective Temperaments in
Heroin Addiction." Journal of
Affective Disorders 117:3.
Pacini M., Maremmani I., Vitali M., Santini P., Romeo M.,
Ceccanti M. (2009).
Affective temperaments in alcoholic patients. Alcohol 43, 397–
404.
Perez-Rios M., Santiago-Perez M.I., Alsonso B., Malvar A.,
Hervada X., deLeon, J.
(2009). Fagerstrom Test for Nicotine Dependence vs heavy
smoking index in a general
population survey. BMC Public Health 9, 493.
Perugi G., Toni C., Travierso M.C., Akiskal H.S. (2003). The
role of cyclothymia in
atypical depression: toward a data-based reconceptualization of
the borderline-bipolar II
connection. Journal of Affective Disorders 73, 87–98.
Umee, "Affective Temperaments in Alcoholic Patients." Scribd.
N.p., n.d. Web. 17
Sept. 2013.
Http://lib.bioinfo.pl/pmid:20414155. N.p., n.d. Web. 17 Sept.
2013.
Copyright of Annals of Eftimie Murgu University Resita,
Fascicle II, Economic Studies is the
property of Annals of Eftimie Murgu University Resita, Fascicle
II, Economic Studies and its
content may not be copied or emailed to multiple sites or posted
to a listserv without the
copyright holder's express written permission. However, users
may print, download, or email
articles for individual use.
Copyright of Analele Universitatii 'Eftimie Murgu' Resita.
Fascicola II. Studii Economice is
the property of Annals of Eftimie Murgu University Resita,
Fascicle II, Economic Studies and
its content may not be copied or emailed to multiple sites or
posted to a listserv without the
copyright holder's express written permission. However, users
may print, download, or email
articles for individual use.
1)The text lists five psychological factors that can influence
purchase decisions. List and give an example of each.
Motives coupled with MASLOW’s hierarchy of needs: love and
belonging, safety, physiological, esteem, and self-actualization.
An example would be Nutrament, which was a fitness drink
targeted to customers for additional energy, suggestive to
esteem or even love and belonging in the MASLOW’s hierarchy
of needs. The product wasn’t selling well, it was discovered
that sales mainly consisted of inner city convenience stores,
neighborhoods associated with high drug use. Instead of the
product being used by fitness minded individuals, it was mainly
being used by drug users as a means of supplemental diet.
Attitudes can be tied to a product in either a positive or
negative manner. An example that comes to mind is when
Honda first mass introduction to the American motorcycle
market by stating “You meet the nicest people on a Honda
motorcycle”. This was to combat the previous negative
stereotype of individuals who own a motorcycle.
Perceptions could be deceiving at times. A prime example
comes to mind with Florida. Most individuals think of Florida
as a state that produces all sorts of citrus. Yes, Florida’s
license plate has “the sunshine state” and a picture of two
oranges, but Florida is a big producer of corn and sugar cane –
something that tends to be either unknown or ignored.
Learning is often thought in an educational setting. However,
one could argue that the simple act of shopping for a car falls
within this category. Individuals usually read reviews on their
desired car, talk with friends, visit dealerships etc. – all in an
effort to learn what is the best car that suits their needs in terms
of practicality, price, and safety etc.
Lifestyle could consist of eating right for a healthy lifestyle,
which suggests by eating correctly one will achieve a healthy
and reduced chronic disease.
2)
Growing in California, In N Out Burgers was just starting up in
the fast food industry. Suddenly, In N Out Burgers becomes the
place to get the best and freshest burgers in the area. This chain
began to grow rapidly due to their simple philosophy, which
also became their mission statement. With their philosophy, the
fast food restaurant has been able to maintain their core values
in their burgers. Due to their core values, every new restaurant
provides the freshest and high-quality ingredients while their
associates deliver friendly customer service and spotless
restaurant. So the philosophy carries this regionally fast food
chain to give their customers the freshest and made-to-order
foods prepared quickly.
Due their simplicity, the chain has not changed their
menu options, which has aided in keeping up a fast pace
service. Just having hamburgers, cheeseburgers, and double
double, the chain has focused its energy on ensuring high
quality and fresh ingredients to their customers. Furthermore,
the company kept itself within certain parts of the United States
such as the west coast. This decision keeps to the company’s
mentality about simplicity.
Within their philosophy, the company has been able to
further add value to their products by exploiting the needs of
their customers. Besides their menu options, the chain has found
a unique selling point which is their special menu. Their secret
menu is open to the public via their webpage or word of mouth.
Some options in the secret menu are Animal Styles Fries or 4X4
or 3X3. Aside from their secret menu, In N Out Burgers has
special options in turn means tailored orders. One example is
burger mustard grilled which the burger has mustard seeping
into it when being cooked. Or something simpler like a grilled
cheese, which has two buns with cheese in the middle. These
options are exclusive at In N Out Burgers so far. Providing their
customers with these specials, the customers can customize
their order however they like.
From their philosophy, the corporation has been able to
maintain its value via simplistic advertisement. In N Out
Burgers is known for creating bumper stickers that has only the
logo of the chain. Furthermore, the restaurant has made t-shirts
and hats that have only the In N Out logo. Combine these
marketing skills with just word of mouth has kept the company
going for many years. Word of mouth may be the oldest trick in
marketing but it has worked quite well for this business. In
other to keep this system, In N Out has a unique customer
service during rush hours for lunch and dinner. During these
times, four workers will come out to take orders for drive thru
customers. This process aids in keeping up a speedy pace, which
keeps the customer happy. Having live testimonies and simple
marketing truly sticks to the value of their product: simple,
fresh, and fast.
Even though In N Out Burgers’ value proposition is its
philosophy, this technique has been able to uphold constituency
of quality in food and customer service in all of its locations.
Simplicity has led to concentrate their efforts to deliver good
meat and ingredients to every customer. Plus, they have been
able to provide their customers with options to modify their
burgers according to individual liking without changing its
actual menu. In N Out Burgers will continue to be a competitor
in the fast food industry due to their high quality yet simple
burgers.
References:
Daniels, W. (2010). In-N-out burger. Nation's Restaurant News,
44(2), 32.
Duerr, C. (2015). In-N-out burger. Nation's Restaurant News,
49(5), 12.
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology
2000, Vol. 78, No. 1, 173-186
In the public domain
DOI: 1O.1037//O022-3514.7S.1.173
Nature Over Nurture: Temperament, Personality, and
Life Span Development
Robert R. McCrae and Paul T. Costa, Jr.
National Institute on Aging
Fritz Ostendorf and Alois Angleitner
Universitat Bielefeld
Martina Hrebickova
Academy of Sciences of the Czech Republic
Maria D. Avia, Jesiis Sanz, and
Maria L. Sanchez-Bernardos
Universidad Complutense de Madrid
M. Ersin Kusdil, Ruth Woodfield, Peter R. Saunders, and Peter
B. Smith
University of Sussex
Temperaments arc often regarded as biologically based
psychological tendencies with intrinsic paths of
development. It is argued that this definition applies to the
personality traits of the five-factor model.
Evidence for the endogenous nature of traits is summarized
from studies of behavior genetics, parent-
child relations, personality structure, animal personality, and
the longitudinal stability of individual
differences. New evidence for intrinsic maturation is offered
from analyses of NEO Five-Factor
Inventory scores for men and women age 14 and over in
German, British, Spanish, Czech, and Turkish
samples (N = 5,085). These data support strong conceptual links
to child temperament despite modest
empirical associations. The intrinsic maturation of personality
is complemented by the culturally
conditioned development of characteristic adaptations that
express personality; interventions in human
development are best addressed to these.
There are both empirical and conceptual links between child
temperaments and adult personality traits. The empirical
associa-
tions are modest, but the conceptual relations are profound. Ex-
plaining how this is so requires a complicated chain of
arguments
and evidence. For example, we report cross-sectional data
showing
(among other things) that adolescents are lower in
Conscientious-
ness than are middle-aged and older adults in Germany, the
United
Kingdom, Spain, the Czech Republic, and Turkey. The
relevance
of such data may not be immediately obvious, but in fact they
speak to the transcontextual nature of personality traits and thus
to
the fundamental issue of nature versus nurture.
Robert R. McCrae and Paul T. Costa, Jr., Personality, Stress,
and
Coping Section, Gerontology Research Center, National
Institute on Ag-
ing, National Institutes of Health; Fritz Ostendorf and Alois
Angleitner,
Department of Psychology, Universitat Bielefeld, Bielefeld,
Germany;
Martina HrebfEkova", Institute of Psychology, Academy of
Sciences of the
Czech Republic, Brno, Czech Republic; Maria D. Avia, Jesus
Sanz, and
Maria L. Sanchez-Bernardos, Department of Psychology,
Universidad
Complutense de Madrid, Madrid, Spain; M. Ersin Kusdil, Ruth
Woodfield,
Peter R. Saunders, and Peter B. Smith, School of Social
Sciences, Univer-
sity of Sussex, Sussex, England.
Portions of this article were presented at the 106th Annual
Convention
of the American Psychological Association, San Francisco,
August 1998.
Czech data collection was supported by Grant 406/99/1155 from
the Grant
Agency of the Czech Republic.
Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to
Robert R.
McCrae, Box 3, National Institute on Aging Gerontology
Research Center,
5600 Nathan Shock Drive, Baltimore, Maryland 21224-6825.
Electronic
mail may be sent to [email protected]
The gist of our argument is easily stated: Personality traits, like
temperaments, are endogenous dispositions that follow intrinsic
paths of development essentially independent of environmental
influences. That idea is simple, but it is so foreign to the
thinking
of most psychologists that it requires a detailed exposition and
defense. Once grasped, however, it offers a new and fruitful
perspective on personality and its development.
A Theoretical Perspective on Temperament
There is no hard and fast distinction between temperament and
personality. The American Heritage Dictionary of the English
Language defines temperament as "the manner of thinking, be-
having, or reacting characteristic of a specific individual"
(Morris,
1976, p. 1324), a definition which might serve equally well for
personality trait. One of the first omnibus personality
inventories,
measuring such traits as ascendance, emotional stability, and
thoughtfulness, was designated by J. P. Guilford and his
colleagues
(Guilford, Zimmerman, & Guilford, 1976) as a "temperament
survey." In some respects, then, there is a long tradition of
equat-
ing these two sets of individual differences variables.
There is also a long tradition of distinguishing them. Tempera-
ment is frequently regarded as a constitutional predisposition,
observable in preverbal infants and animals, and tied, at least
theoretically, to basic psychological processes. Personality
traits,
in contrast, are often assumed to be acquired patterns of thought
and behavior that might be found only in organisms with sophis-
ticated cognitive systems. Constructs like authoritarianism, self-
173
174 McCRAE ET AL.
monitoring, and narcissism do not appear to be directly
applicable
to chimpanzees or human infants.
Some theorists divide personality traits into two categories,
corresponding to innate and acquired characteristics. For
example,
Cloninger and his colleagues (Cloninger, Przybeck, Svrakic, &
Wetzel, 1994) classified Novelty Seeking, Harm Avoidance, Re-
ward Dependence, and Persistence as temperaments, and Self-
Directedness, Cooperativeness, and Self-Transcendence as
aspects
of character. Other theorists assume that temperament provides
the
starting place for personality development, a tabula that is not
quite
rasa. All those personality theorists who nod to "constitutional
factors" (e.g., Kluckhohn & Murray, 1953) adopt some such
position. An appealing version of this constitutional perspective
would distinguish between broad factors, like Extraversion, that
might correspond to basic temperamental influences, and
specific
traits, like sociability or dominance, that might be interpreted as
acquired personality traits.
There is, however, a completely different way to conceptualize
these important distinctions. McAdams (1996) has offered a for-
mulation of the personality system as a whole in terms of three
levels. Personality traits are assigned to Level 1 in McAdams's
scheme, whereas "constructs that are contextualized in time,
place
or role" (p. 301), such as coping strategies, skills, and values,
occupy Level 2. (Level 3 includes life narratives that give unity
and purpose to the self.) A related system has been proposed by
McCrae and Costa (1996, 1999) in a five-factor theory (FFT) of
personality. As shown schematically in Figure 1, the FFT high-
lights the distinction between biologically based basic
tendencies
and culturally conditioned characteristic adaptations (which in-
clude the important subcategory of self-concepts). Basic tenden-
cies comprise abstract potentials and dispositions (including the
traits in McAdams's Level 1), whereas characteristic adaptations
include acquired skills, habits, beliefs, roles, and relationships
(constructs from McAdams's Level 2).
In the terminology of FFT, Cloninger and colleagues (Cloninger
et a l , 1994) would presumably place Novelty Seeking and
Harm
Avoidance in the category of basic tendencies, and Self-
Directedness and Cooperativeness in the category of
characteristic
adaptations. The alternative, constitutional view would perhaps
hold that the temperamental basis of personality—including the
five factors listed in Figure 1—is a part of basic tendencies,
whereas personality traits like sociability and dominance are
char-
acteristic adaptations.
According to FFT, however, both broad personality factors and
the specific traits that define them are best understood not as
characteristic adaptations, but rather as endogenous basic
tenden-
cies. FFT has returned, as it were, to Guilford's (Guilford et al.,
1976) view that the attributes measured by personality question-
naires can be identified as temperaments (Costa & McCrae,
in press).
Some readers will be surprised by the claim that the whole
range
of personality traits can be subsumed by temperament. In
support
of that claim, most of the findings summarized in this article are
taken from research on the five-factor model of personality,
which
is intended to provide a comprehensive taxomomy of traits
(Gold-
berg, 1993). It should be noted, however, that the basic ideas
are
Basic
Tendencies
Neurottcism,
Extraversfon,
Openness,
Agreeabteness,
Conscientiousness
Dynamic
Processes
Objective
Biography
External
Influences
Emotional reactions,
Mid-career shifts:
Behavior
Guttural norms
Ufe events:
Situation
Characteristic
Adaptations
Culturally-conditioned
phenomena:
Personal strivings,
Attitudes
Processes
Self-Concept
Seff-schemas,
Personal myths
Figure 1. A model of the personality system according to five-
factor theory, with examples of specific content
in each category and arrows indicating paths of causal
influence. Adapted from "A Five-Factor Theory of
Personality," by R. R. McCrae and P. T. Costa, Jr., 1999, in
Handbook of Personality (2nd ed., p. 142), edited
by L. Pervin and O. P. John, New York: Guilford Press.
SPECIAL SECTION: PERSONALITY DEVELOPMENT 175
likely to be applicable to many alternative models as well. For
example, there is evidence of cross-cultural invariance for
three-
and seven-factor models (Benet-Martfnez & Waller, 1997; S. B.
G.
Eysenck, 1983), and the pattern of adult age differences
reported
here can also be seen in California Psychological Inventory
scales
(Gough, 1987; Labouvie-Vief, Diehl, Tarnowski, & Shen, in
press;
Yang, McCrae, & Costa, 1998).
Most readers will probably be startled by the conspicuous ab-
sence in Figure 1 of an arrow from external influences to basic
tendencies. This is not an oversight; FFT deliberately asserts
that
personality traits are endogenous dispositions, influenced not at
all
by the environment. That assertion is, of course, an
oversimplifi-
cation, but we believe it is a heuristically valuable one and a
useful
corrective to what Asendorpf and Wilpers (1998) recently called
"the naive environmentalism that has for a long time dominated
the literature on personality development" (p. 1543). In this
arti-
cle we hope to show that FFT provides a useful framework
for understanding child temperament and adult personality
development.
The Roles of the Environment
First, however, we must reassure the reader that environmental
influences play crucial roles in the functioning of the
personality
system in several different respects: They define the conditions
under which human personality evolved; they shape a vast array
of
skills, values, attitudes, and identities; they provide the
concrete
forms in which personality traits are expressed; and they supply
the trait indicators from which personality traits are inferred
and
trait levels are assessed.
At one level, all psychological characteristics must be under-
stood as end results of evolutionary processes by which
organisms
have adapted to their environment (D. M. Buss, 1991).
Evolution-
ary principles are most easily applied to explain characteristics
that
distinguish different species, and their application to the
explana-
tion of individual differences within species is controversial (D.
M.
Buss & Greiling, 1999). Indeed, Tooby and Cosmides (1990)
argued that differences among human beings in personality
traits
are best regarded as noise of no evolutionary significance. At a
minimum, however, that implies that personality variations are
compatible with the usual human environment: We know from
their continued presence among us that both introverts and
extra-
verts can survive in the human world.
The environment also operates at a much more direct level. A
recent book on the limited influences of parenting (Harris,
1998)
was greeted with alarm by many psychologists, who interpreted
it
to imply that the way parents treat their children does not matter
(Begley, 1998). In contrast, FFT explicitly recognizes that
The influence of parents on their children is surely incalculable:
they
nourish and protect them, teach them to walk and talk, instill
habits,
aversions, and values, and provide some of the earliest models
for
social interaction and emotional regulation (McCrae & Costa,
1994,
p. 107).
In short, parenting has important long-term consequences for
the
development of characteristic adaptations, including, of course,
the
lifelong relationship between parent and child. Many other
aspects
of the environment are also significant influences on
characteristic
adaptations, including peers (Harris, 1998), the media,
educational
systems, and so on. Vocational interests, religious beliefs, food
preferences, tactics of interpersonal manipulation, and group
loy-
alties are some of the products of these influences, and it is
possible to view and study psychological development as the
creation and integration of these characteristic adaptations. This
approach may be particularly appealing in collectivistic
cultures, in
which the individual's evolving place in social networks is of
more
concern than are autonomous features of the individual
(Kagitci-
ba§i, 1996). But important as this form of development may be,
FFT asserts that it is not what personality psychologists get at
when they administer personality questionnaires to assess such
characteristics as assertiveness, curiosity, or shyness.
However, the environment also has a direct relation to person-
ality traits, because characteristic adaptations are always
involved
in their expression. To take a simple example, interpersonal
traits
are most often inferred from communication with others, and
that
normally requires a common acquired language such as English,
Shona, or Hindi. At what is perhaps a more psychologically
meaningful level, trait manifestations must fit within a cultural
context. An expression of sympathy for the deceased could be
insulting in a culture in which the dead are never mentioned by
name; thus, an agreeable person must learn how to be polite in
terms of the culture's rules of etiquette. Even apparently direct
manifestations of personality, such as the chronic anxiety of an
individual high in Neuroticism, are usually contextualized:
Anx-
ious Americans worry about computer viruses and the future of
Social Security; anxious Navahos—at least when they were
stud-
ied by Clyde Kluckhohn (1944)—worried about ghosts and
witches (cf. Kitayama & Markus, 1994).
According to FFT, traits cannot be directly observed, but rather
must be inferred from patterns of behavior and experience that
are
known to be valid trait indicators (Tellegen, 1988). Personality
scales rely on these indicators and need to be sensitive to
variations
introduced by culture, age, and other contexts. But although
they
may ask respondents about their values, habits, or concerns,
per-
sonality inventories are designed to allow the inference of
deeper
psychological constructs.
Personality Traits as Endogenous Basic Tendencies
If the environment has such obvious and pervasive effects on
characteristic adaptations and the expression of personality
traits,
why not presume that it also affects traits themselves?
According
to FFT, personality is biologically based, but it is well
established
that perceptual and learning experiences can reshape the
develop-
ing brain (Kolb & Whishaw, 1998), and recent studies suggest
that
traumatic stress may contribute to atrophy in the hippocampus
(Bremner, 1998). Thus, life experience might affect personality
through its effects on the brain (Nelson, 1999). There is cross-
sectional evidence that the experience of acculturation can
change
personality profiles (McCrae, Yik, Trapnell, Bond, & Paulhus,
1998), and some longitudinal research has shown that
personality
change is associated with life events (Agrbnick & Duncan,
1998).
All of these findings are useful reminders that the theoretical
generalizations represented in Figure 1 certainly have
exceptions.
However, the generalization that personality traits are more or
less
immune to environmental influences is supported by multiple,
converging lines of empirical evidence that significant
variations
in life experience have little or no effect on measured
personality
176 McCRAE ET AL.
traits. Any one of these lines of evidence is subject to many
alternative interpretations, but taken together, they make a
strong
case for regarding personality traits as fundamentally
temperament-
like. That assumption makes sense of many findings that would
remain puzzling from the perspective of naive
environmentalism.
In the following section, we review some research consistent
with
this premise of FFT.
Heritability of personality. The study of behavior genetics has
flourished in the past 20 years, and the results of many twin and
adoption studies have shown remarkable unanimity (Loehlin,
1992): Personality traits have a substantial genetic component,
little or no component that can be attributed to shared environ-
mental effects (e.g., attending the same school or having the
same
parents), and a residual component about which little is yet
known.
Heritability is virtually a sine qua non of biologically based the-
ories of personality, so it is crucial to note that it is not limited
to
Neuroticism and Extraversion, which are often conceded to be
temperamental traits (H. J. Eysenck, 1990). All five factors are
heritable; in fact, some estimates find the strongest evidence of
heritability for Openness to Experience (Loehlin, 1992).
Further, people inherit more than the global dispositions sum-
marized by the five major personality factors; specific traits
such
as self-consciousness, gregariousness, and openness to ideas are
also specifically heritable (Jang, McCrae, Angleitner, Riemann,
&
Livesley, 1998), and in this regard can better be considered
basic
tendencies than characteristic adaptations.
But behavior-genetic studies also speak to the importance of
environmental effects, although what they say is subject to
differ-
ent interpretations. The sheer weight of evidence has by now
convinced most psychologists familiar with that literature that
environmental influences shared by children in the same family
have little or no effect on adult personality (Plomin & Daniels,
1987). If the environment is to have any effect, it must be
through
what is typically labeled the nonshared environment, the set of
experiences unique to different children in the same family
(e.g.,
having different first-grade teachers or being a parent's
favorite).
However, this term is not measured directly, but rather it is
calculated as a residual, and as such it includes far more than
experience; in particular, it includes both random error of mea-
surement and systematic method bias. When Riemann,
Angleitner,
and Strelau (1997) reduced method variance by combining self-
reports and observer ratings from two peers, their heritability
estimates for the five factors, ranging from .66 to .79, were
considerably higher than the .50 usually cited. The remaining
21%
to 34% of the variance might include nonshared influences from
the psychological environment, such as peer groups, but it
might
instead reflect wholly biological sources, such as the prenatal
hormonal environment (Resnick, Gottesman, & McGue, 1993),
minor brain damage or infection, or simply the imperfect
operation
of genetic mechanisms. Behavior-genetic studies still allow for
the possibility of some kinds of environmental influences on
traits,
but they do not as yet offer a compelling reason to modify
Figure 1.
Studies of parental influences. Behavior-genetic designs infer
effects indirectly from the phenotypic similarity of people with
different kinds and degrees of relatedness; they do not directly
measure any putative cause of personality traits. There are,
how-
ever, studies that have linked child-rearing behaviors or parent-
child relations to adult personality traits (e.g., Rapee, 1997).
Most
of these studies were retrospective, and many found some
associ-
ation. McCrae and Costa (1988), for example, previously
reported
that men and women who recalled their parents as being
especially
loving described themselves as being better adjusted and more
agreeable. Although this appears to provide direct support for
parental influences on personality, there are many alternative
in-
terpretations. Perhaps parents had been more loving because
these
adjusted and agreeable children had been more lovable. Perhaps
the same genes that made the parents loving made the children
adjusted. Perhaps retrospective bias made kind children recall
their
childhood with exaggerated fondness. Despite the possible
opera-
tion of all these artifacts, the observed correlations were only in
the
range from .10 to .30, accounting for at most 10% of the
variance
in adult personality traits (cf. Rapee, 1997).
It is possible that the effects of parenting are more focused,
affecting specific personality traits rather than broad factors.
But
when the 30 facet scales of the Revised NEO Personality
Inventory
(NEO-PI-R; Costa & McCrae, 1992a) were correlated with Lov-
ing/Rejecting, Casual/Demanding, and Attention scales for
father
and for mother, none of the 180 correlations reached .30 (Mdn
r
= .08; McCrae & Costa, 1994).
The results of the rare prospective-longitudinal studies are more
informative. In one of the first and best of these, Kagan and
Moss
(1962) examined maternal characteristics during three age
periods
from infancy to age 10 and assessed the child's personality at
ages
19-29. Of 552 relevant correlations, only 35 (6%) reached
statis-
tical significance at thep < .05 level. If parenting has an effect
on
personality, it is subtle indeed (Harris, 1998).
All these findings are consistent with the results of adoption
studies (e.g., Plomin, Corley, Caspi, Fulker, & DeFries, 1998),
which showed that children bear little resemblance to either
their
adoptive parents or their adoptive siblings. Neither parental role
modeling nor the parenting practices that would affect all
children
in a family seem to have much influence on personality traits.
Cross-cultural studies of personality structure. It is possible
that environmental influences relevant to personality
development
lie outside the family, in the broader institutions that are collec-
tively called culture. As a biologically based phenomenon com-
mon to the human species, the fundamental structure of infant
and
child temperament ought to transcend culture, and there is some
evidence that it does (Ahadi, Rothbart, & Ye, 1993). But over
time,
many psychologists would find it reasonable to argue that the
pervasive forces of culture can arbitrarily redefine the
parameters
of personality—indeed, that was a central premise of the school
of
culture and personality that flourished in the first half of this
century (Singer, 1961). Some contemporary social scientists
still
find this a plausible argument. Juni (1996) challenged the idea
that
the five-factor model would apply cross-culturally: "Different
cul-
tures and different languages should give rise to other models
that
have little chance of being five in number nor of having any of
the
factors resemble those . . . of middle-class Americans" (p. 864).
However, studies using the Personality Research Form
(Paunonen, Jackson, Trzebinski, & Forsterling, 1992; Stumpf,
1993) and the NEO-PI-R (e.g., Martin et al., 1997; McCrae &
Costa, 1997; McCrae, Costa, del Pilar, Rolland, & Parker, 1998)
have reported clear and detailed replication of the five-factor
model in cultures ranging from Malaysia to Estonia. The traits
that
define the five factors in American samples define the same
factors
around the world. In this respect, the structure of individual dif-
SPECIAL SECTION: PERSONALITY DEVELOPMENT 177
ferences appears to be a universal feature of human groups,
rela-
tively impervious to cultural variation.
Some authors have argued that there are additional personality
factors, such as Chinese Tradition (Cheung et al., 1996) and
(Filipino) Temperamentalness (Church, Katigbak, & Reyes,
1998)
that are indigenous to specific cultures. Such culture-based
factors
would constitute evidence against a purely endogenous theory
of
the origins of personality. As yet, however, we know too little
about indigenous factors to understand how to evaluate this evi-
dence. Perhaps they are measurement artifacts or social
attitudes
that should be distinguished from personality traits per se;
perhaps
they really are universal factors that have so far gone unnoticed
in
other cultures. Because of their importance in the nature-nurture
controversy, such proposed factors merit intensive longitudinal,
cross-observer, and behavior-genetic research.
Comparative studies. The five-factor model may be found in
every culture because it is a product of human biology; recent
research on animals suggests that at least some of the five
factors
may also be shared by nonhuman species. Gosling and John
(1998)
asked cat and dog owners to describe their pets, with terms
taken
from the five-factor model or from a list intended to describe
temperament in animals. In both instruments and in both
species,
they found four factors: three corresponding to Neuroticism,
Ex-
traversion, and Agreeableness, and the fourth combining
features
of Openness to Experience and Conscientiousness in a kind of
animal Intellect factor. King and Figueredo (1997) analyzed
zookeeper ratings of chimpanzees and found six factors, which
corresponded to the five-factor model plus a large dominance
factor.
It has been known for many years that the five-factor structure
of personality can be approximated even in ratings of strangers
(Passini & Norman, 1966), so it might be suspected that these
ratings of animals were merely projections of implicit
personality
theory. But Gosling and John (1998) could not replicate a five-
factor structure of personality in cats or dogs, even when they
used
Procrustes rotation, suggesting that something other than sheer
implicit personality theory was at work. King and Figueredo
(1997) demonstrated substantial agreement between observers
on
chimpanzee personality ratings—the same kind of evidence that
Norman and Goldberg (1966) had used to rebut the claim that
personality ratings of humans were mere cognitive fictions.
The use of personality ratings in the description of nonhuman
species may seem odd—is it meaningful to assess a dog's effi-
ciency, harshness, or creativity?—but there is by now
substantial
scientific literature on the topic (A. H. Buss, 1997; Gosling,
1998).
It seems much less odd to speak about temperament in animals;
if
traits are temperaments, then the literature on individual differ-
ences in animals can be more easily understood.
Temporal stability of adult personality. Beginning in the
1970s, several independent longitudinal studies (e.g., Block,
1981;
Siegler, George, & Okun, 1979) began to address Ihe stability
of
individual differences in personality traits. Results, with
research-
ers using a variety of samples, instruments, and methods of
mea-
surement, showed a consistent pattern of stability. Retest
correla-
tions over 6, 12, or 20 years were not much smaller than short-
term
retest reliabilities; personality in 70-year-olds could be
predicted
with remarkable accuracy from assessments made 30 years
earlier
(Costa & McCrae, 1992b; Finn, 1986).
On the one hand, these findings pointed to the existence of
something in the individual that endured over long periods of
time—a key piece of evidence for the reality of personality
traits.
On the other hand, it cast into doubt the influence of
intervening
events. Over the course of a 30-year study, many participants
would have had major life changes in occupation, marital status,
family stage, physical health, and place of residence. They
would
have shared their cohort's experience of assassinations, wars,
and
recessions; read dozens of books; watched thousands of hours of
television. But the cumulative force of all these external
influences
on personality test scores is barely detectable.
Again, it is possible that life events and experiences affect some
specific traits even if they do not have a major impact on broad
factors. However, in a study of 2,274 men and women traced
from
about age 40 to age 50, retest correlations for the 30 eight-item
NEO-PI-R facet scales were uniformly high, ranging from .64
for
Vulnerability to .80 for Assertiveness and Openness to
Aesthetics
(Siegler & Costa, 1999).
The Intrinsic Maturation of Personality
Studies of heritability, limited parental influence, structural in-
variance across cultures and species, and temporal stability all
point to the notion that personality traits are more expressions
of
human biology than products of life experience. Another more
recent line of evidence concerns maturation and personality
change. Here we present the latest findings from a series of
studies
that have examined age differences in the mean levels of
person-
ality traits across cultures. The basic argument is
straightforward:
If personality development reflects environmental influences,
then
groups whose histories have led them through different environ-
ments should show different developmental outcomes. Con-
versely, if personality development proceeds independently of
life
experiences, then similar trends should be seen everywhere.
The data reviewed above on the temporal stability of personality
traits were retest correlations that reflect the consistency of
rank
order across two occasions. High stability of individual
differences
does not mean that personality trait scores are unchanging, only
that people retain their relative standing across any changes that
occur. If the trait score of every individual in a sample
increased
by exactly the same amount over an interval, the retest
correlation
would be 1.0, no matter how large or small the increase. The
personality changes of interest here must be examined by
compar-
ing mean levels.
Initial work in studies of adults conducted in the United States
found very modest mean level effects after age 30. For example,
in
a large and representative sample of men and women between
ages 35 and 84, the correlations of age with Neuroticism, Extra-
version, and Openness to Experience were - . 1 2 , —.16, and —
.19,
respectively (Costa et al., 1986). Later comparisons of college
students with older adults showed larger effects, albeit in the
same
direction: Students scored about one-half standard deviation
higher
than adults on Neuroticism, Extraversion, and Openness to
Expe-
rience (Costa & McCrae, 1994). They also scored consistently
lower than adults on Agreeableness and Conscientiousness.
In themselves, these data are powerfully ambiguous. Perhaps
they represent the effects of intrinsic maturation, but there are
many other possibilities as well. This pattern of maturation may
be
purely American, a response to an educational and economic
178 McCRAE ET AL.
system that encourages an extended adolescence. Or it may
reflect
cohort differences, the effects of coming of age at specific times
in
history. Perhaps present-day adolescents are Less conscientious
than their grandparents are because they have grown up in an
era
of affluence, or of easily available drugs, or of rock music.
The usual suggestion for a research design to help untangle such
confounds is the longitudinal study. Because comparisons are
made between the same individuals tested on two (or more)
occasions, birth cohort effects are controlled in longitudinal de-
signs. If increases in Conscientiousness were documented in a
group of college students as they grew into middle adulthood,
that
would provide more direct evidence of a true maturational
effect.
In fact, some studies have reported just such longitudinal
changes
in variables related to Conscientiousness (lessor, 1983; McGue,
Bacon, & Lykken, 1993).
Longitudinal studies take time to conduct, however, and longi-
tudinal studies of Americans tell us nothing directly about age
changes in different cultural and historical contexts. Cross-
sectional studies of age differences conducted in other cultures,
however, provide a simple way to circumvent some limitations
of
both cohort and culture, because different cultures have usually
had differing recent histories.
Consider Turkey and the Czech Republic. Turkey is an Islamic
country, and its citizens speak an Altaic language. Following
the
disintegration of the Ottoman Empire at the end of World War L
a new and radically secular society was established, modeled on
the West. Institutions from the alphabet to style of dress were
reformed; most significantly, women were given unprecedented
opportunities for education and occupations outside the home.
Turkey was not directly involved in World War II and has pro-
gressed slowly toward multiparty democracy. Throughout the
cen-
tury it has grown in prosperity and urbanization, with a concom-
itant decline in strong kinship systems.
The Czech Republic, a traditionally Christian nation whose
citizens speak a language from the Slavic branch of the Indo-
European family, began the century as part of the Austro-
Hungarian Empire. Between world wars it functioned as a
democ-
racy with a highly industrialized economy. In 1938, Germany
began an occupation of Czechoslovakia that was ended by
Soviet
troops in 1945; Soviet dominance continued thereafter, with na-
tionalization of industry and collectivization of agriculture. At-
tempted reform in 1968 led to a military response from the War-
saw Pact, and political repression continued until the collapse
of
Communist control in 1989.
The life experiences of Turks and Czechs have thus been radi-
cally different in this century, and both have differed from those
of
Americans. If experiences shape personality, then cohorts born
at
the same time in these three countries would presumably differ
in
mean levels. Czech adolescents, for example, who have spent
much of their lives in a democratic society, might be better
adjusted than their politically traumatized parents and grandpar-
ents. In contrast, American adolescents are known to be higher
in
Neuroticism than their parents' generation (Costa & McCrae,
1994).
Two previous studies have compared age differences on NEO-
PI-R scale scores across cultures (Costa et al., in press; McCrae
et
al., 1999). In each, data were standardized within culture (to
eliminate translation effects) and means were calculated for the
age groups of 18-21, 22-29, 30-49, and 50+. Data were avail-
able for secondary analysis from Germany, Italy, Portugal,
Croatia, South Korea, Russia, Estonia, and Japan. In four of the
cultures (Italy, Croatia, Russia, and Estonia), there were no sig-
nificant age effects for Neuroticism. In the other four cultures,
Neuroticism was higher in younger respondents—just as it had
been in American studies. Results for the remaining factors are
easily summarized; In every culture, the American pattern was
replicated. Extraversion and Openness to Experience declined
and
Agreeableness and Conscientiousness increased with age in
Ger-
many, Italy, Portugal, Croatia, South Korea, Russia, Estonia,
and
Japan.
Xiu, Wu, Wu, and Shui (1996) examined age differences on a
Chinese version of the short form of the NEO-PI-R, the NEO
Five-Factor Inventory (NEO-FFI; Costa & McCrae, 1992a). In a
sample of 593 men and women between ages 20 and 84, small
but
significant age effects were found for Neuroticism and
Openness
to Experience, which declined with age, and Agreeableness,
which
increased with age. Thus, this study offers a partial replication
of
American effects (see also Yang, McCrae, & Costa, 1998).
New Data From Five Cultures
In this article we report analyses of the NEO-FFI administered
in Germany, the United Kingdom, Spain, the Czech Republic,
and
Turkey. The American version was adapted for use in the U.K.,
and translations were made into the other languages and
checked
by review of a back-translation. Internal consistency for the five
12-item scales ranged from .48 (for Agreeableness in the
Turkish
sample) to .85, with a median of .76; in every sample, internal
consistency was lowest for the Agreeableness and Openness to
Experience scales, suggesting that results with these two scales
should be viewed with some caution.
Previous cross-cultural studies using the NEO-PI-R have exam-
ined only adult development, in part because American
normative
data have been published only for college-age and older adults.
The present article includes data from adolescents between ages
14
and 17 from four of the samples. The NEO-FFI has
demonstrated
validity when used in samples of intellectually gifted American
sixth graders (Parker & Stumpf, 1998); internal consistencies in
the four adolescent subsamples studied here ranged from .57 to
.86, with a median of .75, values which are comparable to those
seen in adults.
Data were originally collected for a variety of purposes, and as
Table 1 shows, the distribution by age group is not optimal in
several instances. Nevertheless, there appear to be sufficient
cases
in most age groups to make secondary analyses worthwhile. The
German sample consists of mono- and dizygotic twins, on whom
both self-reports and mean peer ratings of personality are
available
(Riemann et al., 1997). These respondents are part of a large
German sample whose full NEO-PI-R scale scores were previ-
ously analyzed (McCrae et al., 1999). They are included here
not
as an independent replication, but rather as a check on the
consis-
tency of results from the long and short versions of the NEO-PI-
R.
Data from the U.K. were obtained in three studies that involved
adolescent school children, their parents, and university
students.
An effort was made to include respondents from all
occupational
groups; most respondents were from the southern part of the
U.K.
The Turkish sample consisted of adolescents from many regions
in
Turkey that attended a summer camp, and families in the city of
SPECIAL SECTION: PERSONALITY DEVELOPMENT 179
Table 1
Composition of the Samples by Age Group and Gender
Sample
German
British
Spanish
Czech
Turkish
M
42
41
147
157
14-17
W
149
39
263
112
18-21
M
85
135
49
117
16
W
252
135
74
116
7
Age group (in
22-29
M
215
28
145
26
years)
W
515
29
116
25
M
182
40
117
78
84
30-49
W
615
72
143
76
108
M
73
12
67
40
21
50+
W
230
9
53
24
6
Note. None of the Spanish respondents was under 18 years old:
none of the Turkish students or their parents
was between ages 22 and 29. M = men; W = women.
Bursa, a major industrial center. The Spanish and Czech
samples
were both recruited by undergraduate psychology students who
invited friends, relatives, and partners to join the study. None of
these samples is either random or nationally representative, but
it
seems unlikely that they share any systematic sampling bias that
might explain common age trends.
As in previous studies, T scores were computed within each
culture using means and standard deviations from the adults
over
age 21 (following the American convention). The only
meaningful
comparisons are thus among age groups within each culture.
Analyses of variance (ANOVAs) with age group and gender as
classifying variables showed generally similar patterns in men
and
women: Of the 25 ANOVAs, only 5 showed significant
interaction
terms, with no pattern replicated across cultures. Four of the
interactions were quite small, accounting for less than 2% of the
variance. A somewhat larger effect was seen for Openness to
Experience in the Turkish sample, in which age differences
were
found only in women.
Results for the total sample are summarized in Figures 2 - 6 .
The
ANOVAs confirm that there are significant cross-sectional de-
clines in Neuroticism and Extraversion and increases in Consci-
entiousness in all five samples. There are significant increases
in
Agreeableness in the German, Czech, and Turkish samples, but
these trends do not reach significance in the British and Spanish
samples. The hypothesized decline in Openness to Experience is
seen clearly in the Spanish sample, and is significant in the
Czech
and Turkish samples. In contrast, German and British samples
show significantly lower levels of Openness to Experience in
the
6 5 -
£oo
CO
1-
c
ro
CD
ig
h
X
a)
ro
i_
CD
<
6 0 -
5 5 -
5 0 -
4 5 -
O 40H
35
] 14-17 | | 18-21 f ^ | 22-29
•M 30-49 ^ 5 0 +
German British Spanish Czech Turkish
Figure 2. Mean levels of Neuroticism in five cultures. T scores
are based on the mean and standard deviation
of all respondents over age 21 within each culture. Error bars
represent standard errors of the means.
180 McCRAE ET AL
s
o
o
K JS
c
re
35
German British Spanish Czech Turkish
Figure 3. Mean levels of Extraversion in five cultures. T scores
are based on the mean and standard deviation
of all respondents over age 21 within each culture. Error bars
represent standard errors of the means.
65 -|
2!
o
o
w
c
CO
Q 14-17 | | 18-21 ^ 22-29
fe:1 30-49 ^ 5 0 +
German British Spanish Czech Turkish
Figure 4. Mean levels of Openness to Experience in five
cultures. T scores are based on the mean and standard
deviation of all respondents over age 21 within each culture.
Error bars represent standard errors of the means.
SPECIAL SECTION: PERSONALITY DEVELOPMENT 181
o
o
C/)
c
ro
German British Spanish Czech Turkish
Figure 5. Mean levels of Agreeableness in five cultures. r scores
are based on the mean and standard deviation
of all respondents over age 21 within each culture. Age groups
do not differ significantly in the British and
Spanish samples. Error bars represent standard errors of the
means.
youngest group than in the group of 18- to 21-year-olds. (The
same
pattern was seen when mean peer ratings were examined in the
German sample.) It is not clear whether this reflects a true de-
velopmental trend, a sampling bias, or some culture-specific
phenomenon.
Although the pattern of results across these samples conforms
very closely to hypotheses, it is important to recall that most of
the
effects are quite small in magnitude. Across cultures, the
median
correlations of age with Neuroticism, Extraversion, Openness to
Experience, Agreeableness, and Conscientiousness scales are
- . 1 7 , - . 2 1 , - . 0 8 , .09, and .23, respectively. Thus,
previous
reviews of the literature that concluded that mean levels of per-
sonality traits are generally stable in adulthood (McCrae &
Costa,
1990) are only modestly qualified by the present findings.
To date, most information on adult age differences in personal-
ity has been based on analyses of self-reports. Comparison of
peer
ratings of college-age men (Costa, McCrae, & Dembroski,
1989)
with older adult men (see Costa & McCrae, 1989) on the
original
NEO Personality Inventory showed significant effects in the ex-
pected direction for all five domains, which were substantial in
magnitude (greater than one-half standard deviation) for
Neuroti-
cism and Conscientiousness. However, in the German sample
examined here, mean peer ratings showed significant
correlations
with age only for Neuroticism (-.05), Agreeableness (.06), and
Conscientiousness (.21). Research using the full NEO-PI-R in
other cultures would be helpful in clarifying the nature and
extent
of age differences and changes in observer-rated personality
traits.
The NEO-FFI used in the present study does not assess specific
facets of the five factors. Earlier research, however, has shown
that
individual facet scales of the NEO-PI-R show distinctive age
trends across cultures. For example, the Excitement-Seeking
facet
of Extroversion declined markedly in nine out of nine cultures,
whereas the Assertiveness facet showed significant (and small)
declines in only four of them. Additional analyses on the
specific
variance in facet scales (net of the five factors) also showed
generalizable, albeit very small, effects (Costa et ah, in press).
Intrinsic Maturation and Adult Temperament
The data in Figures 2-6 are largely consistent with earlier
observations that the same pattern of age differences in
personality
traits can be seen across different cultures with different recent
histories. There appear to be three possible explanations for this
phenomenon. The first is that age differences are cohort effects,
reflecting the influence of historical forces common to all these
cultures, such as the rise of the mass media or the near-
universal
improvement in health care. Although this possibility cannot be
excluded, it would seem to be a remarkable coincidence that
common historical forces affect all five factors, whereas
historical
experiences unique to each culture affect none of the factors
enough to reverse the usual pattern.
One way to test this hypothesis would be to assess the effect
within cultures of variables that might plausibly account for
com-
mon cohort differences. For example, higher levels of Openness
to
Experience in younger cohorts might be due to increasing levels
of
formal education over the course of this century in most
cultures.
If so, covarying years of education would reduce or eliminate
age
differences in Openness to Experience. We tested that
hypothesis
182 McCRAE ET AL.
65-i
a>
ou
c
(0
German British Spanish Czech Turkish
Figure 6. Mean levels of Conscientiousness in five cultures. T
scores are based on the mean and standard
deviation of all respondents over age 21 within each culture.
Error bars represent standard errors of the means.
in the Spanish, German, and Turkish samples, in which data on
education were available, but found that significant age
differences
in Openness to Experience remained.
A second possibility is that societies everywhere (or perhaps
modern industrial societies everywhere) spontaneously develop
parallel institutions that encourage the same trends in
personality
development. Adult responsibilities may make adults more
respon-
sible; caring for children may make them more caring. This pos-
sibility cannot be easily dismissed, but it is not yet proven.
Even if
there is an association between age-role demands and
personality
traits, it is possible that the causal order is reversed, and that
social
norms have been crafted to accommodate intrinsic maturational
trends in personality. This is quite clear in the case of laws
defining
a minimum age for driving, voting, and drinking.
A third possibility is that there are natural progressions of
personality development that occur without regard to cultural
and
historical context. Just as children leara to talk, count, and
reason
in a fixed order and time course, so too may adults become
more
agreeable and less extraverted as a natural consequence of
aging.
This notion of intrinsic maturation is consistent with the other
lines
of evidence—heritability, stability, and cross-cultural univer-
sality—that point to the interpretation of traits as endogenous
basic
tendencies.
It is also supported more directly by behavior-genetic and
comparative evidence on age changes in personality. Changes in
personality traits between adolescence and young adulthood
have
been shown to be modestly to moderately heritable (McGue et a
l ,
1993), and developmental trends in chimpanzees (King, Landau,
& Guggenheim, 1998) and rhesus monkeys (Suomi, Novak, &
Well, 1996) have shown some intriguing parallels to adult
human
development.
Whether age grading in the social structure shapes personality
development or vice-versa—or whether both processes are at
work—cannot be determined from available data. Future
research
might test these alternative hypotheses in third-world nations
where adult responsibilities are assumed at an earlier age or
among
people with different relevant life experiences, such as
.parenting.
But viewing personality as temperament at least has the virtue
of
making intrinsic maturation a plausible hypothesis that merits
testing.
Linking Child Temperament and Adult Personality
The intent of the whole preceding argument was to demonstrate
that if by temperament we mean biologically based
psychological
tendencies with intrinsic paths of development, then standard
personality inventories assess temperament, and traits such as
aesthetic sensitivity, achievement striving, and modesty are as
much temperaments as are activity level and behavioral
inhibition.
From this perspective it is perhaps not surprising that when
Ang-
leitner and Ostendorf (1994) factored adult temperament
measures
(A. H. Buss & Plomin, 1975; Strelau, Angleitner, Bantelmann,
&
Ruth, 1990) along with other markers they found the familiar
structure of the five-factor model.
SPECIAL SECTION: PERSONALITY DEVELOPMENT 183
But if the individual differences identified by temperament
researchers and personality trait psychologists are much the
same,
the goals and methods of these two research traditions are not.
Researchers within the temperament tradition often emphasize
basic processes and mechanisms. Ahadi and Rothbart (1994),
for
example, have examined psychological systems such as
Approach
and Effortful Control, and Strelau and colleagues (Strelau et al.,
1990) have developed a set of constructs based on hypothesized
Pavlovian properties of the central nervous system. In contrast,
trait psychologists more often focus on outcomes and other
corre-
lates of traits. For example, Barrick and Mount (1991) showed
that
Conscientiousness is associated with superior job performance.
By
identifying personality traits with temperaments, researchers
may
begin to integrate these different emphases on causes and
effects
and come to a better understanding of both the origins and the
expressions of basic tendencies (Costa & McCrae, in press).
The Structure and Stability of Individual Differences
It cannot be assumed that the adult structure of temperament
will appear in analyses of temperament variables in children,
but
there is evidence that something similar to the five factors can
be
found in adult ratings of school children (Digman & Shmelyov,
1996; Kohnstamm, Halverson, Mervielde, & Havill, 1998) and
in
self-reports from children as young as 5 years old (Measelle &
John, 1997). Ahadi and Rothbart (1994) have offered conceptual
analyses that link child temperament constructs to adult
personal-
ity factors: Approach to Extraversion, Anxiety to Neuroticism,
and
Effortful Control to Conscientiousness and Agreeableness.
Classic
efforts at understanding infant temperament (Thomas, Chess, &
Birch, 1968) were not informed by the five-factor model; if
inves-
tigators looked for these factors, they might find them even in
neonates, just as they have been found in nonhuman animals
(King
& Figueredo, 1997).
Even if identical factors were found in infants and adults, it
would not imply that infant temperament is a good predictor of
adult personality. Reviews of the longitudinal literature have re-
ported that temperament variables in fact show limited stability
across relatively short intervals, especially among infants (e.g.,
Lemery, Goldsmith, Klinnert, & Mrazek, 1999), and very
modest
prediction of adult traits (Wachs, 1994). Block (1993), for
exam-
ple, examined retest correlations for ego undercontrol and ego
resiliency at age 3 and age 23 in boys and girls; only one of
these
four correlations reached significance (although all were
positive).
In a recent review of the longitudinal attachment literature,
Fraley
(1998) reported an average correlation of .19 between
attachment
at age 1 and age 19. Kagan and Zentner (1996) found only
modest
associations between characteristics of early childhood and
adult
psy chopathology.
Even modest associations can be meaningful if the outcomes are
socially significant. Caspi and colleagues (Caspi, Elder, & Her-
bener, 1990) have shown that childhood personality traits
(includ-
ing shyness and ill-temperedness) can predict important life
out-
comes such as delayed marriage and downward mobility.
Undercontrol at age 3 predicts health-risk behaviors in young
adults through the mediation of personality traits in adolescence
(Caspi et al., 1997).
With shorter intervals and older children, stronger associations
are found. For example, ego control showed a retest correlation
of
.70 between age 3 and age 4, and .67 between age 14 and age 23
(Block, 1993). Siegler and colleagues (Siegler et al., 1990) esti-
mated that half of the variance in personality dimensions is
stable
from late adolescence to middle adulthood, and Helson and
Moane
(1987) reported greater stability between age 27 and age 43 (a
16-year interval) than between age 21 and age 27 (a 6-year
interval). When adults initially over age 30 are studied, uncor-
rected retest coefficients near .70 are not uncommon over 30-
year
periods (Costa & McCrae, 1992b).
One very general principle of life span personality development
thus appears to be that the stability of individual differences
over
a fixed time interval increases steadily from infancy up to at
least
age 30. Environmentalists might assume that this phenomenon
is
attributable to the accumulation of life experiences: Any single
new experience should affect more change when it occurs in the
context of the limited experience of early life than when it com-
petes with a lifetime of other experiences.
In contrast, FFT suggests another answer: Endogenous disposi-
tions develop over time in ways that redistribute rank orderings.
The functioning of genes, after all, is not fixed at birth; they
switch
on and off across the life span and contribute to individual
patterns
of aging. The brain itself continues to grow and develop until at
least the mid-20s (Pujol, Vendrell, Junque", Marti-Vilalta, &
Cap-
devila, 1993), so it is hardly surprising that personality traits
would
also change in this period.
Developmental Trends for Five Factors
At the aggregate level, it is possible to describe general devel-
opmental trends for the five factors (and the specific traits that
define them; see McCrae et al., 1999). From age 18 to age 30
there
are declines in Neuroticism, Extraversion, and Openness to
Expe-
rience, and increases in Agreeableness and Conscientiousness;
after age 30 the same trends are found, although the rate of
change
seems to decrease.
In this article we presented some of the first data tracing the
five
factors backward from age 18, with German, British, Czech, and
Turkish samples. For the most part, high-school-age boys and
girls
appeared to continue the same trends: They were even higher in
Neuroticism and Extraversion and lower in Agreeableness and
Conscientiousness than were college-age students. No clear
trend
could be discerned for Openness to Experience, as lower instead
of
higher scores were found in the German and British samples.
The present data do support the use of instruments like the
NEO-FFI in younger adolescents, and it would be a relatively
simple matter to conduct cross-sectional studies on
representative
samples of this age group. Research with even younger samples
is
possible, but would require new instruments. Measelle and John
(1997), for example, used a puppet interview to assess
personality
in young children and reported increases in Conscientiousness
between ages 5 and 7. Calibrating puppet interviews and NEO-
FFIs would be difficult, so it is likely that developmental trends
will have to be pieced together from studies of overlapping seg-
ments of childhood.
What could account for these developmental trends? Evolution-
ary arguments might be offered. High levels of Extraversion and
Openness to Experience might be useful in finding a mate,
whereas
higher Agreeableness and Conscientiousness might be more im-
portant for raising a family. Comparative studies of personality
184 McCRAE ET AL.
development in other primates (King et al., 1998) with different
patterns of mating and child rearing might be used to test such
evolutionary hypotheses.
The Development of Characteristic Adaptations
Finally, it is worth recalling that FFT postulates developments
on two separate tracks: Basic tendencies follow a pattern of in-
trinsic maturation, whereas characteristic adaptations respond to
the opportunities and incentives of the social environment. To
the
extent that the theory is correct, psychologists, educators, and
parents will have relatively little impact on the long-term devel-
opment of personality traits, but they can have an influence on
characteristic adaptations (cf. Harkness & Lilienfeld, 1997).
Traits
can be channeled even if they cannot be changed. What kinds of
habits, skills, beliefs, and social networks are optimal for shy or
ill-tempered children? These are likely to be the most
productive
questions for those concerned about shaping human
development.
References
Agronick, G. S., & Duncan, L. E. (1998). Personality and social
change:
Individual differences, life path, and importance attributed to
the wom-
en's movement. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,
74,
1545-1555.
Ahadi, S. A., & Rothbart, M. K. (1994). Temperament,
development, and
the Big Five. In C. F. Halverson, Jr., G. A. Kohnstamm, & R. P.
Martin
(Eds,), The developing structure of temperament and personality
from
infancy to adulthood (pp. 189-207). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Ahadi, S. A., Rothbart, M. K., & Ye, R. (1993). Children's
temperament in
the US and China: Similarities and differences. European
Journal of
Personality, 7, 359-377.
Angleitner, A., & Ostendorf, F. (1994). Temperament and the
Big Five
factors of personality, hi C. F. Halverson, G. A. Kohnstamm, &
R. P.
Martin (Eds.), The developing structure of temperament and
personality
from infancy to adulthood (pp. 69-90). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Asendorpf, J. B., & Wilpers, S. (1998). Personality effects on
social
relationships. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 74,
1531-
1544.
Barrick, M. R., & Mount M. K. (1991). The Big Five
personality dimen-
sions and job performance: A meta-analysis. Personnel
Psychology, 44,
1-26.
Begley, S. (1998, September 7). The parent trap. Newsweek, 52-
59.
Benet-Martinez, V., &. Waller, N. G. (1997). Further evidence
for the
cross-cultural generality of the Big Seven Factor model:
Indigenous and
imported Spanish personality constructs. Journal of Personality,
65,
567-598.
Block, J. (1981). Some enduring and consequential structures of
person-
ality. In A. I. Rabin, J. Aronoff, A. M. Barclay, & R. A. Zucker
(Eds.),
Further explorations in personality (pp. 27-43). New York:
Wiley-
Interscience.
Block, J. (1993). Studying personality the long way. In D. C.
Funder, R. D.
Parke, C. Tomlinson-Keasey, & K. Widaman (Eds.), Studying
lives
through time: Personality and development (pp. 9—41).
Washington,
DC: American Psychological Association.
Bremner, J. D. (1998). Neuroimaging of posttraumatic stress
disorder.
Psychiatric Annals, 28, 445-450.
Buss, A. H. (1997). Evolutionary perspectives on personality
traits. In R.
Hogan, J. Johnson, & S. R. Briggs (Eds.), Handbook of
personality
psychology (pp. 345-366). San Diego, CA: Academic Press.
Buss, A. H., & Plomin, R. (1975). A temperament theory of
personality
development. New York: Wiley.
Buss, D. M. (1991). Evolutionary personality psychology.
Annual Review
of Psychology, 42, 459-491.
Buss, D. M., & Greiling, H. (1999). Adaptive individual
differences.
Journal of Personality, 67, 209-243.
Caspi, A., Begg, D., Dickson, N., Harrington, H. L., Langley,
J., Moffitt,
T. E., & Silva, P. A. (1997). Personality differences predict
health-risk
behaviors in young adulthood: Evidence from a longitudinal
study.
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 73, 1052-1063.
Caspi, A., Elder, G. H., Jr., & Herbener, E. S. (1990).
Childhood person-
ality and the prediction of life-course patterns. In L. N. Robins
& M.
Rutter (Eds.), Straight and devious pathways from childhood to
adult-
hood (pp. 13-35). New York: Cambridge University Press.
Cheung, F. M., Leung, K., Fan, R. M., Song, W. Z., Zhang, J.
X., & Zhang,
J. P. (1996). Development of the Chinese Personality
Assessment In-
ventory. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 27, 181-199.
Church, A. T., Katigbak, M. S., & Reyes, J. A. S. (1998).
Further explo-
ration of Filipino personality structure using the lexical
approach: Do the
Big-Five or Big-Seven dimensions emerge? European Journal of
Per-
sonality, 12, 249-270.
Cloninger, C. R., Przybeck, T. R., Svrakic, D. M., & Wetzel, R.
D. (1994).
The Temperament and Character Inventory (TCI): A guide to Us
devel-
opment and use. St. Louis, MO: Washington University, Center
for
Psychobiology of Personality.
Costa, P. T., Jr., & McCrae, R. R. (1989). The NEO-P1/NEO-
FFI manual
supplement. Odessa, FL: Psychological Assessment Resources.
Costa, P. T., Jr., & McCrae, R. R. (1992a). Revised NEO
Personality
Inventory (NEO-PI-R) and NEO Five-Factor Inventory
(NEOFFI) pro-
fessional manual. Odessa, FL: Psychological Assessment
Resources.
Costa, P. T., Jr., & McCrae, R. R. (1992b). Trait psychology
comes of age.
In T. B. Sonderegger (Ed.), Nebraska Symposium on
Motivation: Psy-
chology and aging (pp. 169-204). Lincoln: University of
Nebraska
Press.
Costa, P. T., Jr., & McCrae, R. R. (1994). Stability and change
in person-
ality from adolescence through adulthood. In C. F. Halverson,
G. A.
Kohnstamm, & R. P. Martin (Eds.), The developing structure of
tem-
perament and personality from infancy to adulthood (pp. 139-
150).
Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Costa, P. T., Jr., & McCrae, R. R. (in press). A theoretical
context for adult
temperament. In T. D. Wachs & G. A. Kohnstamm (Eds.),
Temperament
in context. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Costa, P. T., Jr., McCrae, R. R., & Dembroski, T. M. (1989).
Agreeable-
ness vs. antagonism: Explication of a potential risk factor for
CHD. In A.
Siegman & T. M. Dembroski (Eds.), In search of coronary-
prone
behavior: Beyond Type A (pp. 41-63). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Costa, P. T., Jr., McCrae, R. R., Martin, T. A., Oryol, V. E.,
Senin, I. G.,
Rukavishnikov, A. A., Shimonaka, Y., Nakazato, K., Gondo, Y.,
Takayama, M., Allik, J., Kallasmaa, T., & Realo, A. (in press).
Person-
ality development from adolescence through adulthood: Further
cross-
cultural comparisons of age differences. In V. J. Molfese & D.
Molfese
(Eds.), Temperament and personality development across the
life span.
Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Costa, P. T., Jr., McCrae, R. R., Zonderman, A. B., Barbano, H.
E.,
Lebowitz, B., & Larson, D. M. (1986). Cross-sectional studies
of per-
sonality in a national sample: 2. Stability in neuroticism,
extraversion,
and openness. Psychology and Aging, I, 144-149.
Digman, J. M., & Shraelyov, A. G. (1996). The structure of
temperament
and personality in Russian children. Journal of Personality and
Social
Psychology, 71, 341-351.
Eysenck, H. J. (1990). Genetic and environmental contributions
to indi-
vidual differences: The three major dimensions of personality.
Journal
of Personality, 58, 245-262.
Eysenck, S. B. G. (1983). One approach to cross-cultural
studies of
personality. Australian Journal of Psychology, 35, 383-390.
Finn, S. E. (1986). Stability of personality self-ratings over 30
years:
SPECIAL SECnON: PERSONALITY DEVELOPMENT 185
Evidence for an age/cohort interaction. Journal of Personality
and
Social Psychology, 50, 813-818.
Fraley, R. C. (1998). Attachment continuity from infancy to
adulthood:
Meta-analysis and dynamic modeling of developmental
mechanisms.
Unpublished manuscript. University of California, Davis.
Goldberg, L. R. (1993J. The structure of phenotypic personality
traits.
American Psychologist, 48, 26-34,
Gosling, S. D. (1998). From mice to men: What can animal
research tell
us about human personality? Unpublished manuscript,
University of
California, Berkeley.
Gosling, S. D., & John, O. P. (1998, May). Personality
dimensions in dogs,
cats, and hyenas. In S. D. Gosling & S. Suomi (Chairs), From
mice to
men: Bridging the gap between personality and animal
researchers.
Symposium conducted at the 10th Annual Convention of the
American
Psychological Society, Washington, DC.
Gough, H. G. (1987), California Psychological Inventory
administrator's
guide. Palo Alto, CA: Consulting Psychologists Press.
Guilford, J. S., Zimmerman, W. S., & Guilford, J. P. (1976).
The Guilford-
Zimmerman Temperament Survey Handbook: Twenty-five years
of re-
search and application. San Diego, CA: EdTTS.
Harkness, A. R., & Lilienfeld, S. O. (1997). Individual
differences science
for treatment planning: Personality traits. Psychological
Assessment, 9,
349-360.
Harris, J. R. (1998). The nurture assumption: Why children turn
out the
way they do. New York: Free Press.
Helson, R., & Moane, G. (1987). Personality change in women
from
college to midlife. Journal of Personality and Social
Psychology, 53,
176-186.
Jang, K. L., McCrae, R. R., Angleitner, A., Riemann, R., &
Livesley, W. J.
(1998). Heritability of facet-level traits in a cross-cultural twin
study:
Support for a hierarchical model of personality. Journal of
Personality
and Social Psychology, 74, 1556-1565.
Jessor, R. (1983). The stability of change: Psychosocial
development from
adolescence to young adulthood. In D. Magnusson & V. L.
Allen (Eds.),
Human development: An interactional perspective (pp. 321—
341). New
York: Academic Press.
Juni, S. (1996). Review of the revised NEO Personality
Inventory. In J. C.
Conoley & J. C. Impara (Eds.), 12th Mental Measurements
Yearbook
(pp. 863-868). Lincoln: University of Nebraska Press.
Kagan, J., & Moss, H. A. (1962). From birth to maturity. New
York:
Wiley.
Kagan, J., & Zentner, M. (1996). Early childhood predictors of
adult
psychopathology. Harvard Review of Psychiatry, 3, 341-350.
Kagitcibasj, C. (1996). Family and human development across
cultures: A
view from the other side. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
King, J. E., & Figueredo, A. J. (1997). The five-factor model
plus domi-
nance in chimpanzee personality. Journal of Research in
Personal-
ity, 31, 257-271.
King, J. E.( Landau. V. I., & Guggenheim, C. B. (1998, May).
Age-related
personality changes in chimpanzees. In S. D. Gosling & S.
Suomi
(Chairs), From mice to men: Bridging the gap between
personality and
animal researchers. Symposium conducted at the 10th Annual
Conven-
tion of die American Psychological Society, Washington, DC.
Kitayama, S-, & Markus, H. R. (Eds.). (1994). Emotion and
culture:
Empirical studies of mutual influences. Washington, DC:
American
Psychological Association.
Kluckhohn, C. (1944). Navaho witchcraft. Papers of the
Peabody Museum
of American Archaeology and Ethnology (Vol. 22, No. 3).
Cambridge,
MA: Harvard University.
Kluckhohn, C , & Murray, H. A. (1953). Personality formation:
The
determinants. In C. Kluckhohn, H. A. Murray, & D. M.
Schneider
(Eds.), Personality in nature, society, and culture (2nd ed., pp.
53-67).
New York: Knopf.
Kohnstamm, G. A., Havlerson, C. F., Jr., Mervielde, I., &
Havill, V. L.
(Eds.). (1998). Parental descriptions of child personality:
Developmen-
tal antecedents of the Big Five? Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Kolb, B., & Whishaw, I. Q. (1998). Brain plasticity and
behavior. Annual
Review of Psychology, 49, 43-64.
Labouvie-Vief, G., Diehl, M., Tarnowski, A., & Shen, J. (in
press). Age
differences in adult personality: Findings from the United States
and
China. Journal of Gerontology: Psychological Sciences.
Lemery, K. S., Goldsmith, H. H., Klinnert, M. D., & Mrazek, D.
A. (1999).
Developmental models of infant and childhood temperament.
Develop-
mental Psychology, 35, 189-204.
Loehlin, J. C. (1992). Genes and environment in personality
development.
Newbury Park, CA: Sage.
Martin, T. A., Draguns, J. G., Oryol, V. E., Senin, I. G.,
Rukavishnikov,
A. A., & Klotz, M. L. (1997, August). Development of a
Russian-
language NEO-PI-R. Paper presented at the 105th Annual
Convention of
the American Psychological Association, Chicago, IL.
McAdams, D. P. (1996). Personality, modernity, and the storied
self: A
contemporary framework for studying persons. Psychological
Inquiry, 7,
295-321.
McCrae, R. R., & Costa, P. T , Jr. (1988). Recalled parent-child
relations
and adult personality. Journal of Personality, 56, 417-434.
McCrae, R. R., & Costa, P. T., Jr. (1990). Personality in
adulthood. New
York: Guilford Press.
McCrae, R. R., & Costa, P. T., Jr. (1994). The paradox of
parental
influence: Understanding retrospective studies of parent-child
relations
and adult personality. In C. Perris, W. A. Arrindell, & M.
Eisemann
(Eds.), Parenting and psychopathology (pp. 107-125). New
York:
Wiley.
McCrae, R. R., & Costa, P. T., Jr. (1996). Toward a new
generation of
personality theories: Theoretical contexts for the five-factor
model. In
J. S. Wiggins (Ed.), The five-factor model of personality:
Theoretical
perspectives (pp. 51-87). New York: Guilford Press.
McCrae, R. R., & Costa, P. T., Jr. (1997). Personality trait
structure as a
human universal. American Psychologist, 52, 509-516.
McCrae, R. R., & Costa, P. T., Jr. (1999). A five-factor theory
of person-
ality. In L. Pervin & O. P. John (Eds.), Handbook of personality
(2nd
ed., pp. 139-153). New York: Guilford Press.
McCrae, R. R., Costa, P. T., Jr., del Pilar, G. H., Rolland, J. P.,
& Parker,
W. D. (1998). Cross-cultural assessment of the five-factor
model: The
Revised NEO Personality Inventory. Journal of Cross-Cultural
Psychol-
ogy, 29, 171-188.
McCrae, R. R., Costa, P. T., Jr., Lima, M. P., Simoes, A.,
Ostendorf, F.,
Angleitner, A., Marusic I., Bratko, D., Caprara, G. V.,
Barbaranelli, C ,
Chae, J. H., & Piedmont, R. L. (1999). Age differences in
personality
across the adult life span: Parallels in five cultures.
Developmental
Psychology, 35, 466-477.
McCrae, R. R., Yik, M. S. M., Trapnell, P. D., Bond, M. H., &
Paulhus,
D. L. (1998). Interpreting personality profiles across cultures:
Bilingual,
acculturation, and peer rating studies of Chinese
undergraduates. Jour-
nal of Personality and Social Psychology, 74, 1041-1055.
McGue, M., Bacon, S., & Lykken, D. T. (1993). Personality
stability and
change in early adulthood: A behavioral genetic analysis.
Developmental
Psychology, 29, 96-109. .
Measelle, J., & John, O. P. (1997, May). Young children's self-
perceptions
on the Big Five: Consistency, stability, and school adaptation
from age 5
to age 7. Paper presented at the Biennial Meeting of die Society
for
Research in Child Development, Washington, DC.
Morris, W. (1976). The American heritage dictionary of the
English
language. Boston: Houghton Mifflin.
Nelson, C. A. (1999). Neural plasticity and human development.
Current
Directions in Psychological Science, 8f 42-45.
Norman, W. T., & Goldberg, L. R. (1966). Raters, ratees, and
randomness
in personality structure. Journal of Personality and Social
Psychol-
ogy, 4, 681-691.
186 McCRAE ET AL.
Parker, W. D., & Stumpf, H. (1998). A validation of the five-
factor model
of personality in academically-talented youth across observers
and in-
struments. Personality and Individual Differences, 25, 1005-
1025.
Passini, F. T., & Norman, W. T. (1966). A universal conception
of
personality structure? Journal of Personality and Social
Psychology, 4,
4 4 - 4 9 .
Paunonen, S. V., Jackson, D. N., Trzebinski, J., & Forsteriing,
F. (1992).
Personality structure across cultures: A multimethod evaluation.
Journal
of Personality and Social Psychology, 62, 447-456.
Plomin, R., Corley, R., Caspi, A., Fulker, D. W., & DeFries, J.
(1998).
Adoption results for self-reported personality: Evidence for
nonadditive
genetic effects? Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,
75,
211-218.
Plomin, R., & Daniels, D. (1987). Why are children in the same
family so
different from one another? Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 10,
1-16.
Pujol, J., Vendrell, P., Junque", C , Martf-Vilalta, J. L., &
Capdevila, A.
(1993). When does human brain development end? Evidence of
corpus
callosum growth up to adulthood. Annals of Neurology, 34, 71-
75.
Rapee, R. M. (1997). Potential role of childrearing practices in
the devel-
opment of anxiety and depression. Clinical Psychology Review,
17,
47-67.
Resnick, S. M., Gottesman, I. L, & McGue, M. (1993).
Sensation seeking
in opposite-sex twins: An effect of prenatal hormones? Behavior
Ge-
netics, 23, 323-329.
Riemann, R., Angleitner, A., & Strelau, J. (1997). Genetic and
environ-
mental influences on personality: A study of twins reared
together using
the self- and peer-report NEO-FFI scales. Journal of
Personality, 65,
449-475-
Siegler, I. C , & Costa, P. T., Jr. (1999, August). Personality
continuity and
change in midlife men and women. Symposium presented at the
107th
Annual Convention of the American Psychological Association,
Boston,
MA.
Siegler, I. C , George, L. K., & Okun, M. A. (1979). Cross-
sequential
analysis of adult personality. Developmental Psychology, 15,
350-351.
Siegler, I. C , Zonderman, A. B., Barefoot, J. C , Williams, R.
B., Jr., Costa,
P. T., Jr., & McCrae, R. R. (1990). Predicting personality in
adulthood
from college MMP1 scores: Implications for follow-up studies
in psy-
chosomatic medicine. Psychosomatic Medicine, 52, 644-652.
Singer, M. (1961). A survey of culture and personality theory
and research.
In B. Kaplan (Ed.), Studying personality cross-culturally (pp. 9-
90).
Evanston, IL: Row, Peterson.
Strelau, J., Angleitner, A., Bantelmann, J., & Ruch, W. (1990).
The Strelau
Temperament Inventory—Revised (STT-R): Theoretical
considerations
and scale development. European Journal of Personality, 4, 209-
311 AFFECTIV TEMPERAMENTS AND PERSONALIY TRAITS IN A.docx
311 AFFECTIV TEMPERAMENTS AND PERSONALIY TRAITS IN A.docx
311 AFFECTIV TEMPERAMENTS AND PERSONALIY TRAITS IN A.docx
311 AFFECTIV TEMPERAMENTS AND PERSONALIY TRAITS IN A.docx
311 AFFECTIV TEMPERAMENTS AND PERSONALIY TRAITS IN A.docx
311 AFFECTIV TEMPERAMENTS AND PERSONALIY TRAITS IN A.docx
311 AFFECTIV TEMPERAMENTS AND PERSONALIY TRAITS IN A.docx
311 AFFECTIV TEMPERAMENTS AND PERSONALIY TRAITS IN A.docx
311 AFFECTIV TEMPERAMENTS AND PERSONALIY TRAITS IN A.docx
311 AFFECTIV TEMPERAMENTS AND PERSONALIY TRAITS IN A.docx
311 AFFECTIV TEMPERAMENTS AND PERSONALIY TRAITS IN A.docx
311 AFFECTIV TEMPERAMENTS AND PERSONALIY TRAITS IN A.docx
311 AFFECTIV TEMPERAMENTS AND PERSONALIY TRAITS IN A.docx
311 AFFECTIV TEMPERAMENTS AND PERSONALIY TRAITS IN A.docx
311 AFFECTIV TEMPERAMENTS AND PERSONALIY TRAITS IN A.docx
311 AFFECTIV TEMPERAMENTS AND PERSONALIY TRAITS IN A.docx
311 AFFECTIV TEMPERAMENTS AND PERSONALIY TRAITS IN A.docx
311 AFFECTIV TEMPERAMENTS AND PERSONALIY TRAITS IN A.docx
311 AFFECTIV TEMPERAMENTS AND PERSONALIY TRAITS IN A.docx
311 AFFECTIV TEMPERAMENTS AND PERSONALIY TRAITS IN A.docx
311 AFFECTIV TEMPERAMENTS AND PERSONALIY TRAITS IN A.docx
311 AFFECTIV TEMPERAMENTS AND PERSONALIY TRAITS IN A.docx
311 AFFECTIV TEMPERAMENTS AND PERSONALIY TRAITS IN A.docx
311 AFFECTIV TEMPERAMENTS AND PERSONALIY TRAITS IN A.docx

More Related Content

Similar to 311 AFFECTIV TEMPERAMENTS AND PERSONALIY TRAITS IN A.docx

Obsessive compulsive disorder in adults assignment to turn in for grade
Obsessive compulsive disorder in adults assignment to turn in for gradeObsessive compulsive disorder in adults assignment to turn in for grade
Obsessive compulsive disorder in adults assignment to turn in for gradeCASCHU3937
 
Cognitive conflicts in major depression: Between desired change and personal ...
Cognitive conflicts in major depression: Between desired change and personal ...Cognitive conflicts in major depression: Between desired change and personal ...
Cognitive conflicts in major depression: Between desired change and personal ...Guillem Feixas
 
Research Methods in PsychologyThe Effectiveness of Psychodyn.docx
Research Methods in PsychologyThe Effectiveness of Psychodyn.docxResearch Methods in PsychologyThe Effectiveness of Psychodyn.docx
Research Methods in PsychologyThe Effectiveness of Psychodyn.docxronak56
 
An Epidemiological Investigation of Age-Related Determinants of Anxiety and M...
An Epidemiological Investigation of Age-Related Determinants of Anxiety and M...An Epidemiological Investigation of Age-Related Determinants of Anxiety and M...
An Epidemiological Investigation of Age-Related Determinants of Anxiety and M...Wally Wah Lap Cheung
 
Running Head ARTICLE EVALUATION1ARTICLE EVALUATION2.docx
Running Head ARTICLE EVALUATION1ARTICLE EVALUATION2.docxRunning Head ARTICLE EVALUATION1ARTICLE EVALUATION2.docx
Running Head ARTICLE EVALUATION1ARTICLE EVALUATION2.docxSUBHI7
 
Assessing anxiety and depression with HADS Scale in a spanish cancer populati...
Assessing anxiety and depression with HADS Scale in a spanish cancer populati...Assessing anxiety and depression with HADS Scale in a spanish cancer populati...
Assessing anxiety and depression with HADS Scale in a spanish cancer populati...José Ignacio Leyda Menéndez
 
Birmaher et al 2009; coby study
Birmaher et al 2009; coby studyBirmaher et al 2009; coby study
Birmaher et al 2009; coby studyMaria Karagkouni
 
2014 cognitive conflicts and symptom severity in dysthymia i'd rather be goo...
2014 cognitive conflicts and symptom severity in dysthymia  i'd rather be goo...2014 cognitive conflicts and symptom severity in dysthymia  i'd rather be goo...
2014 cognitive conflicts and symptom severity in dysthymia i'd rather be goo...Guillem Feixas
 
1.Freeman, S. (2011). Improving cognitive treatments for delus.docx
1.Freeman, S. (2011). Improving cognitive treatments for delus.docx1.Freeman, S. (2011). Improving cognitive treatments for delus.docx
1.Freeman, S. (2011). Improving cognitive treatments for delus.docxjeremylockett77
 
Running Head Critique 1Critique2CritiqueAma.docx
Running Head Critique  1Critique2CritiqueAma.docxRunning Head Critique  1Critique2CritiqueAma.docx
Running Head Critique 1Critique2CritiqueAma.docxjoellemurphey
 
Mapping the Structure of the At-Risk Mental State
Mapping the Structure of the At-Risk Mental StateMapping the Structure of the At-Risk Mental State
Mapping the Structure of the At-Risk Mental StateSylvester de Koning
 
prevalence and correlates bipolar spectrum disorder in the world mental healt...
prevalence and correlates bipolar spectrum disorder in the world mental healt...prevalence and correlates bipolar spectrum disorder in the world mental healt...
prevalence and correlates bipolar spectrum disorder in the world mental healt...Priscila Navarro
 
Anil Malhotra - Dimensional Symptom Severity
Anil Malhotra - Dimensional Symptom SeverityAnil Malhotra - Dimensional Symptom Severity
Anil Malhotra - Dimensional Symptom SeverityAnil Malhotra
 
Attenuated psychosis syndrome, at risk mental state and ultra high risk
Attenuated psychosis syndrome, at risk mental state and ultra high riskAttenuated psychosis syndrome, at risk mental state and ultra high risk
Attenuated psychosis syndrome, at risk mental state and ultra high riskDr. Sriram Raghavendran
 
Unit 12 epidemiology substance abuse
Unit 12 epidemiology substance abuseUnit 12 epidemiology substance abuse
Unit 12 epidemiology substance abuseUniversity of Miami
 
The DSM-5 Clinical Cases e-book has provided multiple case-scena.docx
The DSM-5 Clinical Cases e-book has provided multiple case-scena.docxThe DSM-5 Clinical Cases e-book has provided multiple case-scena.docx
The DSM-5 Clinical Cases e-book has provided multiple case-scena.docxkarisariddell
 
ANGELO.Psychosis characteristics (thesis)May25th2015 (final version) (1)
ANGELO.Psychosis characteristics (thesis)May25th2015 (final version) (1)ANGELO.Psychosis characteristics (thesis)May25th2015 (final version) (1)
ANGELO.Psychosis characteristics (thesis)May25th2015 (final version) (1)Angelo Laine
 
Agresivity, narcissism, self destruct
Agresivity, narcissism, self destructAgresivity, narcissism, self destruct
Agresivity, narcissism, self destructCarmen Elena
 

Similar to 311 AFFECTIV TEMPERAMENTS AND PERSONALIY TRAITS IN A.docx (20)

Obsessive compulsive disorder in adults assignment to turn in for grade
Obsessive compulsive disorder in adults assignment to turn in for gradeObsessive compulsive disorder in adults assignment to turn in for grade
Obsessive compulsive disorder in adults assignment to turn in for grade
 
Cognitive conflicts in major depression: Between desired change and personal ...
Cognitive conflicts in major depression: Between desired change and personal ...Cognitive conflicts in major depression: Between desired change and personal ...
Cognitive conflicts in major depression: Between desired change and personal ...
 
Research Methods in PsychologyThe Effectiveness of Psychodyn.docx
Research Methods in PsychologyThe Effectiveness of Psychodyn.docxResearch Methods in PsychologyThe Effectiveness of Psychodyn.docx
Research Methods in PsychologyThe Effectiveness of Psychodyn.docx
 
An Epidemiological Investigation of Age-Related Determinants of Anxiety and M...
An Epidemiological Investigation of Age-Related Determinants of Anxiety and M...An Epidemiological Investigation of Age-Related Determinants of Anxiety and M...
An Epidemiological Investigation of Age-Related Determinants of Anxiety and M...
 
Running Head ARTICLE EVALUATION1ARTICLE EVALUATION2.docx
Running Head ARTICLE EVALUATION1ARTICLE EVALUATION2.docxRunning Head ARTICLE EVALUATION1ARTICLE EVALUATION2.docx
Running Head ARTICLE EVALUATION1ARTICLE EVALUATION2.docx
 
Assessing anxiety and depression with HADS Scale in a spanish cancer populati...
Assessing anxiety and depression with HADS Scale in a spanish cancer populati...Assessing anxiety and depression with HADS Scale in a spanish cancer populati...
Assessing anxiety and depression with HADS Scale in a spanish cancer populati...
 
Birmaher et al 2009; coby study
Birmaher et al 2009; coby studyBirmaher et al 2009; coby study
Birmaher et al 2009; coby study
 
2014 cognitive conflicts and symptom severity in dysthymia i'd rather be goo...
2014 cognitive conflicts and symptom severity in dysthymia  i'd rather be goo...2014 cognitive conflicts and symptom severity in dysthymia  i'd rather be goo...
2014 cognitive conflicts and symptom severity in dysthymia i'd rather be goo...
 
1.Freeman, S. (2011). Improving cognitive treatments for delus.docx
1.Freeman, S. (2011). Improving cognitive treatments for delus.docx1.Freeman, S. (2011). Improving cognitive treatments for delus.docx
1.Freeman, S. (2011). Improving cognitive treatments for delus.docx
 
Running Head Critique 1Critique2CritiqueAma.docx
Running Head Critique  1Critique2CritiqueAma.docxRunning Head Critique  1Critique2CritiqueAma.docx
Running Head Critique 1Critique2CritiqueAma.docx
 
Journal club.pptx
Journal club.pptxJournal club.pptx
Journal club.pptx
 
FioreFinalProject
FioreFinalProjectFioreFinalProject
FioreFinalProject
 
Mapping the Structure of the At-Risk Mental State
Mapping the Structure of the At-Risk Mental StateMapping the Structure of the At-Risk Mental State
Mapping the Structure of the At-Risk Mental State
 
prevalence and correlates bipolar spectrum disorder in the world mental healt...
prevalence and correlates bipolar spectrum disorder in the world mental healt...prevalence and correlates bipolar spectrum disorder in the world mental healt...
prevalence and correlates bipolar spectrum disorder in the world mental healt...
 
Anil Malhotra - Dimensional Symptom Severity
Anil Malhotra - Dimensional Symptom SeverityAnil Malhotra - Dimensional Symptom Severity
Anil Malhotra - Dimensional Symptom Severity
 
Attenuated psychosis syndrome, at risk mental state and ultra high risk
Attenuated psychosis syndrome, at risk mental state and ultra high riskAttenuated psychosis syndrome, at risk mental state and ultra high risk
Attenuated psychosis syndrome, at risk mental state and ultra high risk
 
Unit 12 epidemiology substance abuse
Unit 12 epidemiology substance abuseUnit 12 epidemiology substance abuse
Unit 12 epidemiology substance abuse
 
The DSM-5 Clinical Cases e-book has provided multiple case-scena.docx
The DSM-5 Clinical Cases e-book has provided multiple case-scena.docxThe DSM-5 Clinical Cases e-book has provided multiple case-scena.docx
The DSM-5 Clinical Cases e-book has provided multiple case-scena.docx
 
ANGELO.Psychosis characteristics (thesis)May25th2015 (final version) (1)
ANGELO.Psychosis characteristics (thesis)May25th2015 (final version) (1)ANGELO.Psychosis characteristics (thesis)May25th2015 (final version) (1)
ANGELO.Psychosis characteristics (thesis)May25th2015 (final version) (1)
 
Agresivity, narcissism, self destruct
Agresivity, narcissism, self destructAgresivity, narcissism, self destruct
Agresivity, narcissism, self destruct
 

More from tamicawaysmith

(No Plagiarism) Explain the statement Although many leading organi.docx
(No Plagiarism) Explain the statement Although many leading organi.docx(No Plagiarism) Explain the statement Although many leading organi.docx
(No Plagiarism) Explain the statement Although many leading organi.docxtamicawaysmith
 
 What made you choose this career path What advice do you hav.docx
 What made you choose this career path What advice do you hav.docx What made you choose this career path What advice do you hav.docx
 What made you choose this career path What advice do you hav.docxtamicawaysmith
 
 Patient Population The student will describe the patient populati.docx
 Patient Population The student will describe the patient populati.docx Patient Population The student will describe the patient populati.docx
 Patient Population The student will describe the patient populati.docxtamicawaysmith
 
 Dr. Paul Murray  Bessie Coleman  Jean-Bapiste Bell.docx
 Dr. Paul Murray  Bessie Coleman  Jean-Bapiste Bell.docx Dr. Paul Murray  Bessie Coleman  Jean-Bapiste Bell.docx
 Dr. Paul Murray  Bessie Coleman  Jean-Bapiste Bell.docxtamicawaysmith
 
 In depth analysis of your physical fitness progress  Term p.docx
 In depth analysis of your physical fitness progress  Term p.docx In depth analysis of your physical fitness progress  Term p.docx
 In depth analysis of your physical fitness progress  Term p.docxtamicawaysmith
 
 Information systems infrastructure evolution and trends  Str.docx
 Information systems infrastructure evolution and trends  Str.docx Information systems infrastructure evolution and trends  Str.docx
 Information systems infrastructure evolution and trends  Str.docxtamicawaysmith
 
⦁One to two paragraph brief summary of the book. ⦁Who is the.docx
⦁One to two paragraph brief summary of the book. ⦁Who is the.docx⦁One to two paragraph brief summary of the book. ⦁Who is the.docx
⦁One to two paragraph brief summary of the book. ⦁Who is the.docxtamicawaysmith
 
101018, 6(27 PMPage 1 of 65httpsjigsaw.vitalsource.co.docx
101018, 6(27 PMPage 1 of 65httpsjigsaw.vitalsource.co.docx101018, 6(27 PMPage 1 of 65httpsjigsaw.vitalsource.co.docx
101018, 6(27 PMPage 1 of 65httpsjigsaw.vitalsource.co.docxtamicawaysmith
 
100.0 Criteria10.0 Part 1 PLAAFP The PLAAFP thoroughly an.docx
100.0 Criteria10.0 Part 1 PLAAFP The PLAAFP thoroughly an.docx100.0 Criteria10.0 Part 1 PLAAFP The PLAAFP thoroughly an.docx
100.0 Criteria10.0 Part 1 PLAAFP The PLAAFP thoroughly an.docxtamicawaysmith
 
100635307FLORIDABUILDINGCODE Sixth Edition(2017).docx
100635307FLORIDABUILDINGCODE Sixth Edition(2017).docx100635307FLORIDABUILDINGCODE Sixth Edition(2017).docx
100635307FLORIDABUILDINGCODE Sixth Edition(2017).docxtamicawaysmith
 
1003Violence Against WomenVolume 12 Number 11Novembe.docx
1003Violence Against WomenVolume 12 Number 11Novembe.docx1003Violence Against WomenVolume 12 Number 11Novembe.docx
1003Violence Against WomenVolume 12 Number 11Novembe.docxtamicawaysmith
 
102120151De-Myth-tifying Grading in Sp.docx
102120151De-Myth-tifying Grading             in Sp.docx102120151De-Myth-tifying Grading             in Sp.docx
102120151De-Myth-tifying Grading in Sp.docxtamicawaysmith
 
100.0 Criteria30.0 Flowchart ContentThe flowchart skillful.docx
100.0 Criteria30.0 Flowchart ContentThe flowchart skillful.docx100.0 Criteria30.0 Flowchart ContentThe flowchart skillful.docx
100.0 Criteria30.0 Flowchart ContentThe flowchart skillful.docxtamicawaysmith
 
100 words agree or disagree to eac questions Q 1.As her .docx
100 words agree or disagree to eac questions Q 1.As her .docx100 words agree or disagree to eac questions Q 1.As her .docx
100 words agree or disagree to eac questions Q 1.As her .docxtamicawaysmith
 
101118, 4(36 PMCollection – MSA 603 Strategic Planning for t.docx
101118, 4(36 PMCollection – MSA 603 Strategic Planning for t.docx101118, 4(36 PMCollection – MSA 603 Strategic Planning for t.docx
101118, 4(36 PMCollection – MSA 603 Strategic Planning for t.docxtamicawaysmith
 
100 words per question, no references needed or quotations. Only a g.docx
100 words per question, no references needed or quotations. Only a g.docx100 words per question, no references needed or quotations. Only a g.docx
100 words per question, no references needed or quotations. Only a g.docxtamicawaysmith
 
100A 22 4 451A 1034 51B 1000 101C 1100 11D 112.docx
100A 22 4 451A 1034  51B 1000 101C 1100  11D 112.docx100A 22 4 451A 1034  51B 1000 101C 1100  11D 112.docx
100A 22 4 451A 1034 51B 1000 101C 1100 11D 112.docxtamicawaysmith
 
10122018Week 5 Required Reading and Supplementary Materials - .docx
10122018Week 5 Required Reading and Supplementary Materials - .docx10122018Week 5 Required Reading and Supplementary Materials - .docx
10122018Week 5 Required Reading and Supplementary Materials - .docxtamicawaysmith
 
101416 526 PMAfter September 11 Our State of Exception by .docx
101416 526 PMAfter September 11 Our State of Exception by .docx101416 526 PMAfter September 11 Our State of Exception by .docx
101416 526 PMAfter September 11 Our State of Exception by .docxtamicawaysmith
 
100 words per question, no references needed or quotations. Only.docx
100 words per question, no references needed or quotations. Only.docx100 words per question, no references needed or quotations. Only.docx
100 words per question, no references needed or quotations. Only.docxtamicawaysmith
 

More from tamicawaysmith (20)

(No Plagiarism) Explain the statement Although many leading organi.docx
(No Plagiarism) Explain the statement Although many leading organi.docx(No Plagiarism) Explain the statement Although many leading organi.docx
(No Plagiarism) Explain the statement Although many leading organi.docx
 
 What made you choose this career path What advice do you hav.docx
 What made you choose this career path What advice do you hav.docx What made you choose this career path What advice do you hav.docx
 What made you choose this career path What advice do you hav.docx
 
 Patient Population The student will describe the patient populati.docx
 Patient Population The student will describe the patient populati.docx Patient Population The student will describe the patient populati.docx
 Patient Population The student will describe the patient populati.docx
 
 Dr. Paul Murray  Bessie Coleman  Jean-Bapiste Bell.docx
 Dr. Paul Murray  Bessie Coleman  Jean-Bapiste Bell.docx Dr. Paul Murray  Bessie Coleman  Jean-Bapiste Bell.docx
 Dr. Paul Murray  Bessie Coleman  Jean-Bapiste Bell.docx
 
 In depth analysis of your physical fitness progress  Term p.docx
 In depth analysis of your physical fitness progress  Term p.docx In depth analysis of your physical fitness progress  Term p.docx
 In depth analysis of your physical fitness progress  Term p.docx
 
 Information systems infrastructure evolution and trends  Str.docx
 Information systems infrastructure evolution and trends  Str.docx Information systems infrastructure evolution and trends  Str.docx
 Information systems infrastructure evolution and trends  Str.docx
 
⦁One to two paragraph brief summary of the book. ⦁Who is the.docx
⦁One to two paragraph brief summary of the book. ⦁Who is the.docx⦁One to two paragraph brief summary of the book. ⦁Who is the.docx
⦁One to two paragraph brief summary of the book. ⦁Who is the.docx
 
101018, 6(27 PMPage 1 of 65httpsjigsaw.vitalsource.co.docx
101018, 6(27 PMPage 1 of 65httpsjigsaw.vitalsource.co.docx101018, 6(27 PMPage 1 of 65httpsjigsaw.vitalsource.co.docx
101018, 6(27 PMPage 1 of 65httpsjigsaw.vitalsource.co.docx
 
100.0 Criteria10.0 Part 1 PLAAFP The PLAAFP thoroughly an.docx
100.0 Criteria10.0 Part 1 PLAAFP The PLAAFP thoroughly an.docx100.0 Criteria10.0 Part 1 PLAAFP The PLAAFP thoroughly an.docx
100.0 Criteria10.0 Part 1 PLAAFP The PLAAFP thoroughly an.docx
 
100635307FLORIDABUILDINGCODE Sixth Edition(2017).docx
100635307FLORIDABUILDINGCODE Sixth Edition(2017).docx100635307FLORIDABUILDINGCODE Sixth Edition(2017).docx
100635307FLORIDABUILDINGCODE Sixth Edition(2017).docx
 
1003Violence Against WomenVolume 12 Number 11Novembe.docx
1003Violence Against WomenVolume 12 Number 11Novembe.docx1003Violence Against WomenVolume 12 Number 11Novembe.docx
1003Violence Against WomenVolume 12 Number 11Novembe.docx
 
102120151De-Myth-tifying Grading in Sp.docx
102120151De-Myth-tifying Grading             in Sp.docx102120151De-Myth-tifying Grading             in Sp.docx
102120151De-Myth-tifying Grading in Sp.docx
 
100.0 Criteria30.0 Flowchart ContentThe flowchart skillful.docx
100.0 Criteria30.0 Flowchart ContentThe flowchart skillful.docx100.0 Criteria30.0 Flowchart ContentThe flowchart skillful.docx
100.0 Criteria30.0 Flowchart ContentThe flowchart skillful.docx
 
100 words agree or disagree to eac questions Q 1.As her .docx
100 words agree or disagree to eac questions Q 1.As her .docx100 words agree or disagree to eac questions Q 1.As her .docx
100 words agree or disagree to eac questions Q 1.As her .docx
 
101118, 4(36 PMCollection – MSA 603 Strategic Planning for t.docx
101118, 4(36 PMCollection – MSA 603 Strategic Planning for t.docx101118, 4(36 PMCollection – MSA 603 Strategic Planning for t.docx
101118, 4(36 PMCollection – MSA 603 Strategic Planning for t.docx
 
100 words per question, no references needed or quotations. Only a g.docx
100 words per question, no references needed or quotations. Only a g.docx100 words per question, no references needed or quotations. Only a g.docx
100 words per question, no references needed or quotations. Only a g.docx
 
100A 22 4 451A 1034 51B 1000 101C 1100 11D 112.docx
100A 22 4 451A 1034  51B 1000 101C 1100  11D 112.docx100A 22 4 451A 1034  51B 1000 101C 1100  11D 112.docx
100A 22 4 451A 1034 51B 1000 101C 1100 11D 112.docx
 
10122018Week 5 Required Reading and Supplementary Materials - .docx
10122018Week 5 Required Reading and Supplementary Materials - .docx10122018Week 5 Required Reading and Supplementary Materials - .docx
10122018Week 5 Required Reading and Supplementary Materials - .docx
 
101416 526 PMAfter September 11 Our State of Exception by .docx
101416 526 PMAfter September 11 Our State of Exception by .docx101416 526 PMAfter September 11 Our State of Exception by .docx
101416 526 PMAfter September 11 Our State of Exception by .docx
 
100 words per question, no references needed or quotations. Only.docx
100 words per question, no references needed or quotations. Only.docx100 words per question, no references needed or quotations. Only.docx
100 words per question, no references needed or quotations. Only.docx
 

Recently uploaded

How to Configure Email Server in Odoo 17
How to Configure Email Server in Odoo 17How to Configure Email Server in Odoo 17
How to Configure Email Server in Odoo 17Celine George
 
Class 11 Legal Studies Ch-1 Concept of State .pdf
Class 11 Legal Studies Ch-1 Concept of State .pdfClass 11 Legal Studies Ch-1 Concept of State .pdf
Class 11 Legal Studies Ch-1 Concept of State .pdfakmcokerachita
 
The basics of sentences session 2pptx copy.pptx
The basics of sentences session 2pptx copy.pptxThe basics of sentences session 2pptx copy.pptx
The basics of sentences session 2pptx copy.pptxheathfieldcps1
 
Science 7 - LAND and SEA BREEZE and its Characteristics
Science 7 - LAND and SEA BREEZE and its CharacteristicsScience 7 - LAND and SEA BREEZE and its Characteristics
Science 7 - LAND and SEA BREEZE and its CharacteristicsKarinaGenton
 
Hybridoma Technology ( Production , Purification , and Application )
Hybridoma Technology  ( Production , Purification , and Application  ) Hybridoma Technology  ( Production , Purification , and Application  )
Hybridoma Technology ( Production , Purification , and Application ) Sakshi Ghasle
 
A Critique of the Proposed National Education Policy Reform
A Critique of the Proposed National Education Policy ReformA Critique of the Proposed National Education Policy Reform
A Critique of the Proposed National Education Policy ReformChameera Dedduwage
 
call girls in Kamla Market (DELHI) 🔝 >༒9953330565🔝 genuine Escort Service 🔝✔️✔️
call girls in Kamla Market (DELHI) 🔝 >༒9953330565🔝 genuine Escort Service 🔝✔️✔️call girls in Kamla Market (DELHI) 🔝 >༒9953330565🔝 genuine Escort Service 🔝✔️✔️
call girls in Kamla Market (DELHI) 🔝 >༒9953330565🔝 genuine Escort Service 🔝✔️✔️9953056974 Low Rate Call Girls In Saket, Delhi NCR
 
Concept of Vouching. B.Com(Hons) /B.Compdf
Concept of Vouching. B.Com(Hons) /B.CompdfConcept of Vouching. B.Com(Hons) /B.Compdf
Concept of Vouching. B.Com(Hons) /B.CompdfUmakantAnnand
 
Presentation by Andreas Schleicher Tackling the School Absenteeism Crisis 30 ...
Presentation by Andreas Schleicher Tackling the School Absenteeism Crisis 30 ...Presentation by Andreas Schleicher Tackling the School Absenteeism Crisis 30 ...
Presentation by Andreas Schleicher Tackling the School Absenteeism Crisis 30 ...EduSkills OECD
 
Interactive Powerpoint_How to Master effective communication
Interactive Powerpoint_How to Master effective communicationInteractive Powerpoint_How to Master effective communication
Interactive Powerpoint_How to Master effective communicationnomboosow
 
BASLIQ CURRENT LOOKBOOK LOOKBOOK(1) (1).pdf
BASLIQ CURRENT LOOKBOOK  LOOKBOOK(1) (1).pdfBASLIQ CURRENT LOOKBOOK  LOOKBOOK(1) (1).pdf
BASLIQ CURRENT LOOKBOOK LOOKBOOK(1) (1).pdfSoniaTolstoy
 
Accessible design: Minimum effort, maximum impact
Accessible design: Minimum effort, maximum impactAccessible design: Minimum effort, maximum impact
Accessible design: Minimum effort, maximum impactdawncurless
 
Software Engineering Methodologies (overview)
Software Engineering Methodologies (overview)Software Engineering Methodologies (overview)
Software Engineering Methodologies (overview)eniolaolutunde
 
KSHARA STURA .pptx---KSHARA KARMA THERAPY (CAUSTIC THERAPY)————IMP.OF KSHARA ...
KSHARA STURA .pptx---KSHARA KARMA THERAPY (CAUSTIC THERAPY)————IMP.OF KSHARA ...KSHARA STURA .pptx---KSHARA KARMA THERAPY (CAUSTIC THERAPY)————IMP.OF KSHARA ...
KSHARA STURA .pptx---KSHARA KARMA THERAPY (CAUSTIC THERAPY)————IMP.OF KSHARA ...M56BOOKSTORE PRODUCT/SERVICE
 
Mastering the Unannounced Regulatory Inspection
Mastering the Unannounced Regulatory InspectionMastering the Unannounced Regulatory Inspection
Mastering the Unannounced Regulatory InspectionSafetyChain Software
 
microwave assisted reaction. General introduction
microwave assisted reaction. General introductionmicrowave assisted reaction. General introduction
microwave assisted reaction. General introductionMaksud Ahmed
 
CARE OF CHILD IN INCUBATOR..........pptx
CARE OF CHILD IN INCUBATOR..........pptxCARE OF CHILD IN INCUBATOR..........pptx
CARE OF CHILD IN INCUBATOR..........pptxGaneshChakor2
 

Recently uploaded (20)

How to Configure Email Server in Odoo 17
How to Configure Email Server in Odoo 17How to Configure Email Server in Odoo 17
How to Configure Email Server in Odoo 17
 
Class 11 Legal Studies Ch-1 Concept of State .pdf
Class 11 Legal Studies Ch-1 Concept of State .pdfClass 11 Legal Studies Ch-1 Concept of State .pdf
Class 11 Legal Studies Ch-1 Concept of State .pdf
 
The basics of sentences session 2pptx copy.pptx
The basics of sentences session 2pptx copy.pptxThe basics of sentences session 2pptx copy.pptx
The basics of sentences session 2pptx copy.pptx
 
Science 7 - LAND and SEA BREEZE and its Characteristics
Science 7 - LAND and SEA BREEZE and its CharacteristicsScience 7 - LAND and SEA BREEZE and its Characteristics
Science 7 - LAND and SEA BREEZE and its Characteristics
 
Hybridoma Technology ( Production , Purification , and Application )
Hybridoma Technology  ( Production , Purification , and Application  ) Hybridoma Technology  ( Production , Purification , and Application  )
Hybridoma Technology ( Production , Purification , and Application )
 
A Critique of the Proposed National Education Policy Reform
A Critique of the Proposed National Education Policy ReformA Critique of the Proposed National Education Policy Reform
A Critique of the Proposed National Education Policy Reform
 
call girls in Kamla Market (DELHI) 🔝 >༒9953330565🔝 genuine Escort Service 🔝✔️✔️
call girls in Kamla Market (DELHI) 🔝 >༒9953330565🔝 genuine Escort Service 🔝✔️✔️call girls in Kamla Market (DELHI) 🔝 >༒9953330565🔝 genuine Escort Service 🔝✔️✔️
call girls in Kamla Market (DELHI) 🔝 >༒9953330565🔝 genuine Escort Service 🔝✔️✔️
 
Concept of Vouching. B.Com(Hons) /B.Compdf
Concept of Vouching. B.Com(Hons) /B.CompdfConcept of Vouching. B.Com(Hons) /B.Compdf
Concept of Vouching. B.Com(Hons) /B.Compdf
 
Presentation by Andreas Schleicher Tackling the School Absenteeism Crisis 30 ...
Presentation by Andreas Schleicher Tackling the School Absenteeism Crisis 30 ...Presentation by Andreas Schleicher Tackling the School Absenteeism Crisis 30 ...
Presentation by Andreas Schleicher Tackling the School Absenteeism Crisis 30 ...
 
Interactive Powerpoint_How to Master effective communication
Interactive Powerpoint_How to Master effective communicationInteractive Powerpoint_How to Master effective communication
Interactive Powerpoint_How to Master effective communication
 
Model Call Girl in Bikash Puri Delhi reach out to us at 🔝9953056974🔝
Model Call Girl in Bikash Puri  Delhi reach out to us at 🔝9953056974🔝Model Call Girl in Bikash Puri  Delhi reach out to us at 🔝9953056974🔝
Model Call Girl in Bikash Puri Delhi reach out to us at 🔝9953056974🔝
 
BASLIQ CURRENT LOOKBOOK LOOKBOOK(1) (1).pdf
BASLIQ CURRENT LOOKBOOK  LOOKBOOK(1) (1).pdfBASLIQ CURRENT LOOKBOOK  LOOKBOOK(1) (1).pdf
BASLIQ CURRENT LOOKBOOK LOOKBOOK(1) (1).pdf
 
9953330565 Low Rate Call Girls In Rohini Delhi NCR
9953330565 Low Rate Call Girls In Rohini  Delhi NCR9953330565 Low Rate Call Girls In Rohini  Delhi NCR
9953330565 Low Rate Call Girls In Rohini Delhi NCR
 
Accessible design: Minimum effort, maximum impact
Accessible design: Minimum effort, maximum impactAccessible design: Minimum effort, maximum impact
Accessible design: Minimum effort, maximum impact
 
Software Engineering Methodologies (overview)
Software Engineering Methodologies (overview)Software Engineering Methodologies (overview)
Software Engineering Methodologies (overview)
 
Código Creativo y Arte de Software | Unidad 1
Código Creativo y Arte de Software | Unidad 1Código Creativo y Arte de Software | Unidad 1
Código Creativo y Arte de Software | Unidad 1
 
KSHARA STURA .pptx---KSHARA KARMA THERAPY (CAUSTIC THERAPY)————IMP.OF KSHARA ...
KSHARA STURA .pptx---KSHARA KARMA THERAPY (CAUSTIC THERAPY)————IMP.OF KSHARA ...KSHARA STURA .pptx---KSHARA KARMA THERAPY (CAUSTIC THERAPY)————IMP.OF KSHARA ...
KSHARA STURA .pptx---KSHARA KARMA THERAPY (CAUSTIC THERAPY)————IMP.OF KSHARA ...
 
Mastering the Unannounced Regulatory Inspection
Mastering the Unannounced Regulatory InspectionMastering the Unannounced Regulatory Inspection
Mastering the Unannounced Regulatory Inspection
 
microwave assisted reaction. General introduction
microwave assisted reaction. General introductionmicrowave assisted reaction. General introduction
microwave assisted reaction. General introduction
 
CARE OF CHILD IN INCUBATOR..........pptx
CARE OF CHILD IN INCUBATOR..........pptxCARE OF CHILD IN INCUBATOR..........pptx
CARE OF CHILD IN INCUBATOR..........pptx
 

311 AFFECTIV TEMPERAMENTS AND PERSONALIY TRAITS IN A.docx

  • 1. 311 AFFECTIV TEMPERAMENTS AND PERSONALIY TRAITS IN ADDICT SUBJECTS Alexander KELEMEN 1 Maria PUSCHITA Delia PODEA ABSTRACT Drug abuse is a phenomenon that can be found in almost all studied societies from antiquity to nowadays. Much of the designed literature has described personality disorder constructs for addict subjects (Vincent, 2009). Building on previous work, and after reviewing the theory of affective temperaments of Akiskal and Mallya, we examined the relevance of affective temperament and personality measures in patients with alcohol and heroin addictions. The patients were compared, regarding affective temperaments and personality lines, according to the Akiskal formulation and EPQ questionnaire, in
  • 2. which 50 were heroin addicts and 50 were alcohol addict people, both of them sharing similar backgrounds. As a result no differences were observed between heroin addicts and alcohol users on either cyclothymic or hyperthymic scales. Significant discrepancies were noted in depressive and irritability scales, on which heroin addicts scored higher. In a multivariate discriminant analysis, mainly depressive and irritable traits show a distinction between heroin addicts and alcohol users. Our data suggest a new hypothesis, stating that some of hyperthymic and irritable traits, could represent the temperamental profile of heroin addicts. Personality traits are also associated with heroin and alcohol use and extroversion and psycho-emotional instability are common features; the motivation for testing is higher in heroin users and alcohol abusers tend to dissimulate more frequently. KEY WORDS: alcohol abusers, heroin addicts, affective temperaments and personality JEL: I10 INTRODUCTION The temperament and personality characteristics of alcoholics and drug addicts have been a major issue in the field of substance abuse research, but the results reported
  • 3. are seldom comparable, on account of the differences in the means of assessment and the conceptualizations used (Basiaux et al, (2001) ). Difficult temperament, antisocial personality disorder and borderline, or affectively unstable profiles have been associated with alcohol abuse (De Jong et al, (1993) ). Alcohol abuse has also been linked to axis I mood instability while the role of axis II mood instability remained unclear and defined along different constructs. More recently, some authors have been building on the concept of an affective spectrum, meaning by tha, that a group of syndromes varying in severity and symptom quality, but all representing degrees, stages, or variants of the same basic biological 1 PhD. Faculty of Medicine, Vasile Goldis Western University of Arad, Romania, e-mail: [email protected] 312 substrate (Perugi et al, (2002) ). The study of temperaments can play a crucial role in identifying predictors of affective instability in healthy individuals. Temperamental traits also appear to be important as putative prognostic and therapeutic variables in
  • 4. patients with full-blown affective disorders (Cassano et al, (1992) ). The International Consortium of Psychiatric Epidemiology has confirmed the high comorbidity in community-drawn samples between substance use disorders and affective temperaments lines. In the same way, associations between substance use and specific personality traits (such as novelty seeking, harm avoidance or antisocial personality) have also been extensively documented (Chakroun, 2004). Much of the designed literature has described personality disorder constructs for addict subjects (Vincent, (2009) ). The present paper is design to be a psychological study, which consists in examine the relevance of affective temperament and personality measures in alcohol and heroin use subjects. Temperament is defined as a biological disposition, corresponding to a constitutional substrate. Temperament is expressed through a series of signs and features usually manifested by a certain stability of mood, attitudes towards the environment, sensitivity to external stimulus and characteristic modes of reaction (Vincent, (2013) ). Most clinicians agree that early-age experiences account for a wide variability of adult individual personality traits, so the development of personality traits is usually considered to be primarily through crucial environmental challenges and experiences. Nevertheless, since the '80s several authors have maintained
  • 5. that some early and stable personality traits exist, which can be described as features of premorbid phases of major Psychiatric Illnesses (Maremmani et al, (2009) ). Consistent with the concept of an affective spectrum, the study of temperaments is crucial, in the context of identify early predictors of future affective instability even in healthy individuals. Temperamental traits of personality, also prove important as putative prognostic and therapeutic variables in patients with alcohol and heroin addiction. OBJECTIVES: Our study first objective is the evaluation of the affective temperamental traits and the personality traits in alcohol and heroin addicted patients; the second objective is the evaluation of differences between alcohol addicted patients and heroin users. METHODS: The sample is composed of 100 adult patients (50 with a alcohol and 50 heroin addiction diagnosis), enrolled at the Department of Psychiatry of the Municipal Clinic Hospital, Cluj. Patients have fill out the TEMPS-A (Temperament Evaluation of Memphis, Pisa, Paris and San Diego - autoquestionnaire version) and the EPQ questionnaire, so we compared, regarding affective temperaments and personality lines, according to the Akiskal formulation and Eysenck personality questionnaire formulation. All patients included in the study signed informed consent.
  • 6. Both the consent form and the experimental procedures were approved by the competent ethics committees in accordance with internationally accepted criteria for ethical research. The research was conducted in accordance with the ethical standards of the Declaration of Helsinki (1964). The study was conducted with the approval of the Ethics and Research 313 Committee of the Vasile Goldis Western University of Arad and Cluj Municipal Hospital. All patients were evaluated during the stabilization phase to avoid possible interferences due to the acute phase of their illness in the temperament and personality questionnaires (TEMPS-A and EPQ). To achieve the research objectives, subjects were evaluated once during the 12 months. Each subject was interviewed and completed all scales above, as paper and pencil, so were assessed personality traits in terms of the psychometrics scale, respectively affective temperament types by Akiskal scale. Data Analysis: Analyzes based on scores E, N, L and P (EPQ questionnaire's dimensions) indicates the fact that the majority of respondents can be
  • 7. classified as extroverted persons, highly unstable psycho-emotional and almost the lack of some schizoid tendencies from the sample. Out of the 100 individuals, more then half (64 %) have obtained an E score of 9 points, denoting that they can be characterized as persons with more accelerate processes of excitement and inhibitions of the SNC, who tend to adopt easier risky and scandalous behavior, with a lower control of the emotional reactions and a lower tolerance to frustration, which they associate as their lifestyle. The following table consist of the exacts distribution of the 100 respondents involved in the study and highlights that the majority obtain a 9 points score, meanwhile only 16 subjects obtained higher scores. Table 1. Distribution of individuals according to the score E Frequency Percent Valid Percent Cumulative Percent Valid 3 10 10.0 10.0 10.0 5 1 1.0 1.0 11.0 6 9 9.0 9.0 20.0 9 64 64.0 64.0 84.0
  • 8. 11 1 1.0 1.0 85.0 12 2 2.0 2.0 87.0 13 7 7.0 7.0 94.0 14 1 1.0 1.0 95.0 15 2 2.0 2.0 97.0 16 2 2.0 2.0 99.0 17 1 1.0 1.0 100.0 Total 100 100.0 100.0 Source: Carried out by the Author, Own calculation, 2013 Histogram corresponding variable gives us an image regarding the respondents distribution according to the score obtained for E category, being easy to observe that the majority of the respondents s
  • 9. Source: Carried out by the Author, Own calculation, 2013 Regarding this research, is noticeable that the most common value for dimension N was 12 points, 25 % of the re We find that the distribution of one variable is abnormal, 25 % of the subjects are placed in the middle range (at the value of 23 points), but more than half of all (52%) stand near the maximum value of the range (getting sc also noted a tremendous difference between the range’s limits, the minimum value being 4 points and the maximum 23 points. The existing data reveal that most subjects are supra react strongly to stimulus various emotional experiences. 314 Histogram corresponding variable gives us an image regarding the respondents distribution according to the score obtained for E category, being easy to observe that the majority of the respondents stand around the value of 9 points. Respondents distribution for E scale Source: Carried out by the Author, Own calculation, 2013 Regarding this research, is noticeable that the most common value for dimension
  • 10. N was 12 points, 25 % of the respondents getting that value. We find that the distribution of one variable is abnormal, 25 % of the subjects are placed in the middle range (at the value of 23 points), but more than half of all (52%) stand near the maximum value of the range (getting scores of over 20 points). Is also noted a tremendous difference between the range’s limits, the minimum value being 4 points and the maximum 23 points. The existing data reveal that most subjects are supra-emotional persons, who and who find it hard to come back to their balance after various emotional experiences. Histogram corresponding variable gives us an image regarding the respondents distribution according to the score obtained for E category, being easy to observe that Figure 1. Respondents distribution for E scale Source: Carried out by the Author, Own calculation, 2013 Regarding this research, is noticeable that the most common value for dimension We find that the distribution of one variable is abnormal, 25 % of the subjects are placed in the middle range (at the value of 23 points), but more than half of all
  • 11. ores of over 20 points). Is also noted a tremendous difference between the range’s limits, the minimum value emotional persons, who and who find it hard to come back to their balance after 315 Table 2. Distribution of individuals according to the score N Frequency Percent Valid Percent Cumulative Percent Valid 4 2 2.0 2.0 2.0 6 1 1.0 1.0 3.0 7 10 10.0 10.0 13.0 9 1 1.0 1.0 14.0 10 2 2.0 2.0 16.0 12 25 25.0 25.0 41.0 14 2 2.0 2.0 43.0
  • 12. 16 5 5.0 5.0 48.0 21 20 20.0 20.0 68.0 22 22 22.0 22.0 90.0 23 10 10.0 10.0 100.0 Total 100 100.0 100.0 Source: Carried out by the Author, Own calculation, 2013 Histogram corresponding variable provides us an image regarding the distribution of respondents according the score obtained for N category. Figure 2. Respondents distribution for N scale Source: Carried out by the Author, Own calcula In a multivariate discriminant analysis, mainly depressive and irritable traits show a distinction between heroin addicts and alcohol users . respondents 78 % have offered positive answers to questions
  • 13. which give a profile with saddest mood, ruminative and willing to apprehension. Out of these, 55% are part of the alcohol consumers category and 45% are part of the heroin consumers category. We can state that alcoholic respondents tend to be in a sad mood. In terms of frequenc irritable-choleric disposition, measured trough the confession of the respondents regarding on how often they get irritable, choleric, we notice that 84% of the subjects have given us answers positive rated as irritable than half belong to the alcohol consumers category, therefore we conclude that alcohol consumers category tend to be more often choleric than the heroin consumers category. Conclusions: Our data suggest a new hypothesis: • some of hyperthymic profile of heroin addicts. • some of depressive and irritable traits, could represent the temperamental profile of heroin addicts. • personality traits are associated with heroin and alcohol use. • extroversion and psycho • the motivation for testing is higher in heroin users, alcohol abusers tend to dissimulate more frequently. Considerations:
  • 14. The scientific community has recently examined whether correlations between affective temperaments and substance abuse disorders. We try to summarized some of these correlations, comparing two groups of addict subjects (heroin vs alcohol). 316 Source: Carried out by the Author, Own calculation, 2013 In a multivariate discriminant analysis, mainly depressive and irritable traits show a distinction between heroin addicts and alcohol users . Out of the 100 respondents 78 % have offered positive answers to questions which give a profile with saddest mood, ruminative and willing to apprehension. Out of these, 55% are part of the alcohol consumers category and 45% are part of the heroin consumers category. We can state that alcoholic respondents tend to be in a sad mood. In terms of frequenc choleric disposition, measured trough the confession of the respondents regarding on how often they get irritable, choleric, we notice that 84% of the subjects have given us answers positive rated as irritable-choleric disposition. Out of than half belong to the alcohol consumers category, therefore we conclude that alcohol consumers category tend to be more often choleric than the heroin consumers category. Our data suggest a new hypothesis:
  • 15. some of hyperthymic and irritable traits, could represent the temperamental profile of heroin addicts. some of depressive and irritable traits, could represent the temperamental profile of heroin addicts. personality traits are associated with heroin and alcohol use. roversion and psycho-emotional instability are common features the motivation for testing is higher in heroin users, alcohol abusers tend to dissimulate more frequently. The scientific community has recently examined whether correlations between affective temperaments and substance abuse disorders. We try to summarized some of these correlations, comparing two groups of addict subjects (heroin vs alcohol). tion, 2013 In a multivariate discriminant analysis, mainly depressive and irritable traits Out of the 100 respondents 78 % have offered positive answers to questions which give a profile with a saddest mood, ruminative and willing to apprehension. Out of these, 55% are part of the alcohol consumers category and 45% are part of the heroin consumers category. We can state that alcoholic respondents tend to be in a sad mood. In terms of frequency of
  • 16. choleric disposition, measured trough the confession of the respondents regarding on how often they get irritable, choleric, we notice that 84% of the subjects choleric disposition. Out of these more than half belong to the alcohol consumers category, therefore we conclude that alcohol consumers category tend to be more often choleric than the heroin consumers category. and irritable traits, could represent the temperamental some of depressive and irritable traits, could represent the temperamental emotional instability are common features the motivation for testing is higher in heroin users, alcohol abusers tend to The scientific community has recently examined whether correlations exist We try to summarized some of these correlations, comparing two groups of 317 The correlation between affective temperaments line, personality traits and substance abuse disorders is generally accepted. Some of these
  • 17. can be considered expressions of the heroin and alcohol use. In heroin addicts and alcoholics the role of “these traited” can be extended from the area of full-blown affective disorders to that of affective temperaments (Maremmani, 2005). What emerges is the central role of cyclothymia, with irritable traits discovered in a group of heroin addicts, and depressive traits in alcoholics. While the cyclothymic temperament qualifies as a real temperamental risk factor for the development of these diseases, irritable and depressive traits, which differentiated these two groups of subjects, may be considered not so much a premorbid condition, but, rather, as a simple association (Vincent, 2013). After reviewing the theory of affective temperaments of Akiskal and Mallya, we will discuss affective temperaments in heroin addicts and alcoholics, with the aim of providing an “at-risk temperamental profile” for the development of substance abuse disorders. A working hypothesis is then formulated to help explain how temperamental profile may promote the initiation of substance use and contribute to the development of addiction. REFERENCES Akiskal H.S., Placidi G.F., Signoretta S., Liguori A., Gervasi R., Maremmani I., Mallya
  • 18. G., Puzantian V.R. (1998). TEMPS-I: -delineating the most discriminant traits of the cyclothymic, depressive, hyperthymic and irritable temperaments in a nonpatient population. J. Affect. Disorders. 51, 7–19. Akiskal H.S., Akiskal K.K., Haykal R.F., Manning J.S., Connor P. (2005). TEMPS-A: progress towards validation of a self-rated clinical version of the Temperament Evaluation of the Memphis, Pisa, Paris, and San Diego Autoquestionnaire. J. Affect. Disorders. 85, 3-16. Hagop S. Akiskal MD; Robert M. A. Hirschfeld MD; Boghos I. Yerevanian MD. (2005) "The Relationship of Personality to Affective Disorders." N.p., n.d. Web. Kaplan H., Sadock B.J. (2000) “Substance Related Disorders”, Comprehensive Textbook of Psychiatry, Seventh Edition, Vol. I, Ed. Lippincott Williams & Wilkins, Philadelphia, USA, 11: 924-1095. Maremmani, Angelo Giovanni Icro, Luca Rovai, Fabio Rugani, Matteo Pacini, Francesco Lamanna, Silvia Bacciardi, Giulio Perugi, Joseph Deltito, Liliana Dell'Osso, and Icro Maremmani, (2012), “National Center for Biotechnology Information”. U.S. National Library of Medicine. Maremmani I, Pacini M, Popovic D, Romano A, Maremmani AG, Perugi G, Deltito J, Akiskal K, Akiskal H. (2009) "Affective Temperaments in Heroin Addiction." Journal of
  • 19. Affective Disorders 117:3. Pacini M., Maremmani I., Vitali M., Santini P., Romeo M., Ceccanti M. (2009). Affective temperaments in alcoholic patients. Alcohol 43, 397– 404. Perez-Rios M., Santiago-Perez M.I., Alsonso B., Malvar A., Hervada X., deLeon, J. (2009). Fagerstrom Test for Nicotine Dependence vs heavy smoking index in a general population survey. BMC Public Health 9, 493. Perugi G., Toni C., Travierso M.C., Akiskal H.S. (2003). The role of cyclothymia in atypical depression: toward a data-based reconceptualization of the borderline-bipolar II connection. Journal of Affective Disorders 73, 87–98. Umee, "Affective Temperaments in Alcoholic Patients." Scribd. N.p., n.d. Web. 17 Sept. 2013. Http://lib.bioinfo.pl/pmid:20414155. N.p., n.d. Web. 17 Sept. 2013. Copyright of Annals of Eftimie Murgu University Resita, Fascicle II, Economic Studies is the property of Annals of Eftimie Murgu University Resita, Fascicle II, Economic Studies and its content may not be copied or emailed to multiple sites or posted to a listserv without the copyright holder's express written permission. However, users may print, download, or email
  • 20. articles for individual use. Copyright of Analele Universitatii 'Eftimie Murgu' Resita. Fascicola II. Studii Economice is the property of Annals of Eftimie Murgu University Resita, Fascicle II, Economic Studies and its content may not be copied or emailed to multiple sites or posted to a listserv without the copyright holder's express written permission. However, users may print, download, or email articles for individual use. 1)The text lists five psychological factors that can influence purchase decisions. List and give an example of each. Motives coupled with MASLOW’s hierarchy of needs: love and belonging, safety, physiological, esteem, and self-actualization. An example would be Nutrament, which was a fitness drink targeted to customers for additional energy, suggestive to esteem or even love and belonging in the MASLOW’s hierarchy of needs. The product wasn’t selling well, it was discovered that sales mainly consisted of inner city convenience stores, neighborhoods associated with high drug use. Instead of the product being used by fitness minded individuals, it was mainly being used by drug users as a means of supplemental diet. Attitudes can be tied to a product in either a positive or negative manner. An example that comes to mind is when Honda first mass introduction to the American motorcycle market by stating “You meet the nicest people on a Honda motorcycle”. This was to combat the previous negative stereotype of individuals who own a motorcycle.
  • 21. Perceptions could be deceiving at times. A prime example comes to mind with Florida. Most individuals think of Florida as a state that produces all sorts of citrus. Yes, Florida’s license plate has “the sunshine state” and a picture of two oranges, but Florida is a big producer of corn and sugar cane – something that tends to be either unknown or ignored. Learning is often thought in an educational setting. However, one could argue that the simple act of shopping for a car falls within this category. Individuals usually read reviews on their desired car, talk with friends, visit dealerships etc. – all in an effort to learn what is the best car that suits their needs in terms of practicality, price, and safety etc. Lifestyle could consist of eating right for a healthy lifestyle, which suggests by eating correctly one will achieve a healthy and reduced chronic disease. 2) Growing in California, In N Out Burgers was just starting up in the fast food industry. Suddenly, In N Out Burgers becomes the place to get the best and freshest burgers in the area. This chain began to grow rapidly due to their simple philosophy, which also became their mission statement. With their philosophy, the fast food restaurant has been able to maintain their core values in their burgers. Due to their core values, every new restaurant provides the freshest and high-quality ingredients while their associates deliver friendly customer service and spotless restaurant. So the philosophy carries this regionally fast food chain to give their customers the freshest and made-to-order foods prepared quickly. Due their simplicity, the chain has not changed their menu options, which has aided in keeping up a fast pace
  • 22. service. Just having hamburgers, cheeseburgers, and double double, the chain has focused its energy on ensuring high quality and fresh ingredients to their customers. Furthermore, the company kept itself within certain parts of the United States such as the west coast. This decision keeps to the company’s mentality about simplicity. Within their philosophy, the company has been able to further add value to their products by exploiting the needs of their customers. Besides their menu options, the chain has found a unique selling point which is their special menu. Their secret menu is open to the public via their webpage or word of mouth. Some options in the secret menu are Animal Styles Fries or 4X4 or 3X3. Aside from their secret menu, In N Out Burgers has special options in turn means tailored orders. One example is burger mustard grilled which the burger has mustard seeping into it when being cooked. Or something simpler like a grilled cheese, which has two buns with cheese in the middle. These options are exclusive at In N Out Burgers so far. Providing their customers with these specials, the customers can customize their order however they like. From their philosophy, the corporation has been able to maintain its value via simplistic advertisement. In N Out Burgers is known for creating bumper stickers that has only the logo of the chain. Furthermore, the restaurant has made t-shirts and hats that have only the In N Out logo. Combine these marketing skills with just word of mouth has kept the company going for many years. Word of mouth may be the oldest trick in marketing but it has worked quite well for this business. In other to keep this system, In N Out has a unique customer service during rush hours for lunch and dinner. During these times, four workers will come out to take orders for drive thru customers. This process aids in keeping up a speedy pace, which keeps the customer happy. Having live testimonies and simple marketing truly sticks to the value of their product: simple,
  • 23. fresh, and fast. Even though In N Out Burgers’ value proposition is its philosophy, this technique has been able to uphold constituency of quality in food and customer service in all of its locations. Simplicity has led to concentrate their efforts to deliver good meat and ingredients to every customer. Plus, they have been able to provide their customers with options to modify their burgers according to individual liking without changing its actual menu. In N Out Burgers will continue to be a competitor in the fast food industry due to their high quality yet simple burgers. References: Daniels, W. (2010). In-N-out burger. Nation's Restaurant News, 44(2), 32. Duerr, C. (2015). In-N-out burger. Nation's Restaurant News, 49(5), 12. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 2000, Vol. 78, No. 1, 173-186 In the public domain DOI: 1O.1037//O022-3514.7S.1.173 Nature Over Nurture: Temperament, Personality, and Life Span Development Robert R. McCrae and Paul T. Costa, Jr. National Institute on Aging
  • 24. Fritz Ostendorf and Alois Angleitner Universitat Bielefeld Martina Hrebickova Academy of Sciences of the Czech Republic Maria D. Avia, Jesiis Sanz, and Maria L. Sanchez-Bernardos Universidad Complutense de Madrid M. Ersin Kusdil, Ruth Woodfield, Peter R. Saunders, and Peter B. Smith University of Sussex Temperaments arc often regarded as biologically based psychological tendencies with intrinsic paths of development. It is argued that this definition applies to the personality traits of the five-factor model. Evidence for the endogenous nature of traits is summarized from studies of behavior genetics, parent- child relations, personality structure, animal personality, and the longitudinal stability of individual differences. New evidence for intrinsic maturation is offered from analyses of NEO Five-Factor Inventory scores for men and women age 14 and over in German, British, Spanish, Czech, and Turkish samples (N = 5,085). These data support strong conceptual links to child temperament despite modest empirical associations. The intrinsic maturation of personality is complemented by the culturally conditioned development of characteristic adaptations that express personality; interventions in human development are best addressed to these.
  • 25. There are both empirical and conceptual links between child temperaments and adult personality traits. The empirical associa- tions are modest, but the conceptual relations are profound. Ex- plaining how this is so requires a complicated chain of arguments and evidence. For example, we report cross-sectional data showing (among other things) that adolescents are lower in Conscientious- ness than are middle-aged and older adults in Germany, the United Kingdom, Spain, the Czech Republic, and Turkey. The relevance of such data may not be immediately obvious, but in fact they speak to the transcontextual nature of personality traits and thus to the fundamental issue of nature versus nurture. Robert R. McCrae and Paul T. Costa, Jr., Personality, Stress, and Coping Section, Gerontology Research Center, National Institute on Ag- ing, National Institutes of Health; Fritz Ostendorf and Alois Angleitner, Department of Psychology, Universitat Bielefeld, Bielefeld, Germany; Martina HrebfEkova", Institute of Psychology, Academy of Sciences of the Czech Republic, Brno, Czech Republic; Maria D. Avia, Jesus Sanz, and Maria L. Sanchez-Bernardos, Department of Psychology, Universidad Complutense de Madrid, Madrid, Spain; M. Ersin Kusdil, Ruth Woodfield, Peter R. Saunders, and Peter B. Smith, School of Social
  • 26. Sciences, Univer- sity of Sussex, Sussex, England. Portions of this article were presented at the 106th Annual Convention of the American Psychological Association, San Francisco, August 1998. Czech data collection was supported by Grant 406/99/1155 from the Grant Agency of the Czech Republic. Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to Robert R. McCrae, Box 3, National Institute on Aging Gerontology Research Center, 5600 Nathan Shock Drive, Baltimore, Maryland 21224-6825. Electronic mail may be sent to [email protected] The gist of our argument is easily stated: Personality traits, like temperaments, are endogenous dispositions that follow intrinsic paths of development essentially independent of environmental influences. That idea is simple, but it is so foreign to the thinking of most psychologists that it requires a detailed exposition and defense. Once grasped, however, it offers a new and fruitful perspective on personality and its development. A Theoretical Perspective on Temperament There is no hard and fast distinction between temperament and personality. The American Heritage Dictionary of the English Language defines temperament as "the manner of thinking, be- having, or reacting characteristic of a specific individual" (Morris, 1976, p. 1324), a definition which might serve equally well for personality trait. One of the first omnibus personality
  • 27. inventories, measuring such traits as ascendance, emotional stability, and thoughtfulness, was designated by J. P. Guilford and his colleagues (Guilford, Zimmerman, & Guilford, 1976) as a "temperament survey." In some respects, then, there is a long tradition of equat- ing these two sets of individual differences variables. There is also a long tradition of distinguishing them. Tempera- ment is frequently regarded as a constitutional predisposition, observable in preverbal infants and animals, and tied, at least theoretically, to basic psychological processes. Personality traits, in contrast, are often assumed to be acquired patterns of thought and behavior that might be found only in organisms with sophis- ticated cognitive systems. Constructs like authoritarianism, self- 173 174 McCRAE ET AL. monitoring, and narcissism do not appear to be directly applicable to chimpanzees or human infants. Some theorists divide personality traits into two categories, corresponding to innate and acquired characteristics. For example, Cloninger and his colleagues (Cloninger, Przybeck, Svrakic, & Wetzel, 1994) classified Novelty Seeking, Harm Avoidance, Re- ward Dependence, and Persistence as temperaments, and Self- Directedness, Cooperativeness, and Self-Transcendence as aspects
  • 28. of character. Other theorists assume that temperament provides the starting place for personality development, a tabula that is not quite rasa. All those personality theorists who nod to "constitutional factors" (e.g., Kluckhohn & Murray, 1953) adopt some such position. An appealing version of this constitutional perspective would distinguish between broad factors, like Extraversion, that might correspond to basic temperamental influences, and specific traits, like sociability or dominance, that might be interpreted as acquired personality traits. There is, however, a completely different way to conceptualize these important distinctions. McAdams (1996) has offered a for- mulation of the personality system as a whole in terms of three levels. Personality traits are assigned to Level 1 in McAdams's scheme, whereas "constructs that are contextualized in time, place or role" (p. 301), such as coping strategies, skills, and values, occupy Level 2. (Level 3 includes life narratives that give unity and purpose to the self.) A related system has been proposed by McCrae and Costa (1996, 1999) in a five-factor theory (FFT) of personality. As shown schematically in Figure 1, the FFT high- lights the distinction between biologically based basic tendencies and culturally conditioned characteristic adaptations (which in- clude the important subcategory of self-concepts). Basic tenden- cies comprise abstract potentials and dispositions (including the traits in McAdams's Level 1), whereas characteristic adaptations include acquired skills, habits, beliefs, roles, and relationships (constructs from McAdams's Level 2). In the terminology of FFT, Cloninger and colleagues (Cloninger et a l , 1994) would presumably place Novelty Seeking and
  • 29. Harm Avoidance in the category of basic tendencies, and Self- Directedness and Cooperativeness in the category of characteristic adaptations. The alternative, constitutional view would perhaps hold that the temperamental basis of personality—including the five factors listed in Figure 1—is a part of basic tendencies, whereas personality traits like sociability and dominance are char- acteristic adaptations. According to FFT, however, both broad personality factors and the specific traits that define them are best understood not as characteristic adaptations, but rather as endogenous basic tenden- cies. FFT has returned, as it were, to Guilford's (Guilford et al., 1976) view that the attributes measured by personality question- naires can be identified as temperaments (Costa & McCrae, in press). Some readers will be surprised by the claim that the whole range of personality traits can be subsumed by temperament. In support of that claim, most of the findings summarized in this article are taken from research on the five-factor model of personality, which is intended to provide a comprehensive taxomomy of traits (Gold- berg, 1993). It should be noted, however, that the basic ideas are Basic Tendencies Neurottcism,
  • 30. Extraversfon, Openness, Agreeabteness, Conscientiousness Dynamic Processes Objective Biography External Influences Emotional reactions, Mid-career shifts: Behavior Guttural norms Ufe events: Situation Characteristic Adaptations Culturally-conditioned phenomena: Personal strivings, Attitudes Processes Self-Concept Seff-schemas,
  • 31. Personal myths Figure 1. A model of the personality system according to five- factor theory, with examples of specific content in each category and arrows indicating paths of causal influence. Adapted from "A Five-Factor Theory of Personality," by R. R. McCrae and P. T. Costa, Jr., 1999, in Handbook of Personality (2nd ed., p. 142), edited by L. Pervin and O. P. John, New York: Guilford Press. SPECIAL SECTION: PERSONALITY DEVELOPMENT 175 likely to be applicable to many alternative models as well. For example, there is evidence of cross-cultural invariance for three- and seven-factor models (Benet-Martfnez & Waller, 1997; S. B. G. Eysenck, 1983), and the pattern of adult age differences reported here can also be seen in California Psychological Inventory scales (Gough, 1987; Labouvie-Vief, Diehl, Tarnowski, & Shen, in press; Yang, McCrae, & Costa, 1998). Most readers will probably be startled by the conspicuous ab- sence in Figure 1 of an arrow from external influences to basic tendencies. This is not an oversight; FFT deliberately asserts that personality traits are endogenous dispositions, influenced not at all by the environment. That assertion is, of course, an oversimplifi- cation, but we believe it is a heuristically valuable one and a
  • 32. useful corrective to what Asendorpf and Wilpers (1998) recently called "the naive environmentalism that has for a long time dominated the literature on personality development" (p. 1543). In this arti- cle we hope to show that FFT provides a useful framework for understanding child temperament and adult personality development. The Roles of the Environment First, however, we must reassure the reader that environmental influences play crucial roles in the functioning of the personality system in several different respects: They define the conditions under which human personality evolved; they shape a vast array of skills, values, attitudes, and identities; they provide the concrete forms in which personality traits are expressed; and they supply the trait indicators from which personality traits are inferred and trait levels are assessed. At one level, all psychological characteristics must be under- stood as end results of evolutionary processes by which organisms have adapted to their environment (D. M. Buss, 1991). Evolution- ary principles are most easily applied to explain characteristics that distinguish different species, and their application to the explana- tion of individual differences within species is controversial (D. M. Buss & Greiling, 1999). Indeed, Tooby and Cosmides (1990)
  • 33. argued that differences among human beings in personality traits are best regarded as noise of no evolutionary significance. At a minimum, however, that implies that personality variations are compatible with the usual human environment: We know from their continued presence among us that both introverts and extra- verts can survive in the human world. The environment also operates at a much more direct level. A recent book on the limited influences of parenting (Harris, 1998) was greeted with alarm by many psychologists, who interpreted it to imply that the way parents treat their children does not matter (Begley, 1998). In contrast, FFT explicitly recognizes that The influence of parents on their children is surely incalculable: they nourish and protect them, teach them to walk and talk, instill habits, aversions, and values, and provide some of the earliest models for social interaction and emotional regulation (McCrae & Costa, 1994, p. 107). In short, parenting has important long-term consequences for the development of characteristic adaptations, including, of course, the lifelong relationship between parent and child. Many other aspects of the environment are also significant influences on characteristic adaptations, including peers (Harris, 1998), the media,
  • 34. educational systems, and so on. Vocational interests, religious beliefs, food preferences, tactics of interpersonal manipulation, and group loy- alties are some of the products of these influences, and it is possible to view and study psychological development as the creation and integration of these characteristic adaptations. This approach may be particularly appealing in collectivistic cultures, in which the individual's evolving place in social networks is of more concern than are autonomous features of the individual (Kagitci- ba§i, 1996). But important as this form of development may be, FFT asserts that it is not what personality psychologists get at when they administer personality questionnaires to assess such characteristics as assertiveness, curiosity, or shyness. However, the environment also has a direct relation to person- ality traits, because characteristic adaptations are always involved in their expression. To take a simple example, interpersonal traits are most often inferred from communication with others, and that normally requires a common acquired language such as English, Shona, or Hindi. At what is perhaps a more psychologically meaningful level, trait manifestations must fit within a cultural context. An expression of sympathy for the deceased could be insulting in a culture in which the dead are never mentioned by name; thus, an agreeable person must learn how to be polite in terms of the culture's rules of etiquette. Even apparently direct manifestations of personality, such as the chronic anxiety of an individual high in Neuroticism, are usually contextualized: Anx-
  • 35. ious Americans worry about computer viruses and the future of Social Security; anxious Navahos—at least when they were stud- ied by Clyde Kluckhohn (1944)—worried about ghosts and witches (cf. Kitayama & Markus, 1994). According to FFT, traits cannot be directly observed, but rather must be inferred from patterns of behavior and experience that are known to be valid trait indicators (Tellegen, 1988). Personality scales rely on these indicators and need to be sensitive to variations introduced by culture, age, and other contexts. But although they may ask respondents about their values, habits, or concerns, per- sonality inventories are designed to allow the inference of deeper psychological constructs. Personality Traits as Endogenous Basic Tendencies If the environment has such obvious and pervasive effects on characteristic adaptations and the expression of personality traits, why not presume that it also affects traits themselves? According to FFT, personality is biologically based, but it is well established that perceptual and learning experiences can reshape the develop- ing brain (Kolb & Whishaw, 1998), and recent studies suggest that traumatic stress may contribute to atrophy in the hippocampus (Bremner, 1998). Thus, life experience might affect personality through its effects on the brain (Nelson, 1999). There is cross-
  • 36. sectional evidence that the experience of acculturation can change personality profiles (McCrae, Yik, Trapnell, Bond, & Paulhus, 1998), and some longitudinal research has shown that personality change is associated with life events (Agrbnick & Duncan, 1998). All of these findings are useful reminders that the theoretical generalizations represented in Figure 1 certainly have exceptions. However, the generalization that personality traits are more or less immune to environmental influences is supported by multiple, converging lines of empirical evidence that significant variations in life experience have little or no effect on measured personality 176 McCRAE ET AL. traits. Any one of these lines of evidence is subject to many alternative interpretations, but taken together, they make a strong case for regarding personality traits as fundamentally temperament- like. That assumption makes sense of many findings that would remain puzzling from the perspective of naive environmentalism. In the following section, we review some research consistent with this premise of FFT. Heritability of personality. The study of behavior genetics has
  • 37. flourished in the past 20 years, and the results of many twin and adoption studies have shown remarkable unanimity (Loehlin, 1992): Personality traits have a substantial genetic component, little or no component that can be attributed to shared environ- mental effects (e.g., attending the same school or having the same parents), and a residual component about which little is yet known. Heritability is virtually a sine qua non of biologically based the- ories of personality, so it is crucial to note that it is not limited to Neuroticism and Extraversion, which are often conceded to be temperamental traits (H. J. Eysenck, 1990). All five factors are heritable; in fact, some estimates find the strongest evidence of heritability for Openness to Experience (Loehlin, 1992). Further, people inherit more than the global dispositions sum- marized by the five major personality factors; specific traits such as self-consciousness, gregariousness, and openness to ideas are also specifically heritable (Jang, McCrae, Angleitner, Riemann, & Livesley, 1998), and in this regard can better be considered basic tendencies than characteristic adaptations. But behavior-genetic studies also speak to the importance of environmental effects, although what they say is subject to differ- ent interpretations. The sheer weight of evidence has by now convinced most psychologists familiar with that literature that environmental influences shared by children in the same family have little or no effect on adult personality (Plomin & Daniels, 1987). If the environment is to have any effect, it must be through what is typically labeled the nonshared environment, the set of
  • 38. experiences unique to different children in the same family (e.g., having different first-grade teachers or being a parent's favorite). However, this term is not measured directly, but rather it is calculated as a residual, and as such it includes far more than experience; in particular, it includes both random error of mea- surement and systematic method bias. When Riemann, Angleitner, and Strelau (1997) reduced method variance by combining self- reports and observer ratings from two peers, their heritability estimates for the five factors, ranging from .66 to .79, were considerably higher than the .50 usually cited. The remaining 21% to 34% of the variance might include nonshared influences from the psychological environment, such as peer groups, but it might instead reflect wholly biological sources, such as the prenatal hormonal environment (Resnick, Gottesman, & McGue, 1993), minor brain damage or infection, or simply the imperfect operation of genetic mechanisms. Behavior-genetic studies still allow for the possibility of some kinds of environmental influences on traits, but they do not as yet offer a compelling reason to modify Figure 1. Studies of parental influences. Behavior-genetic designs infer effects indirectly from the phenotypic similarity of people with different kinds and degrees of relatedness; they do not directly measure any putative cause of personality traits. There are, how- ever, studies that have linked child-rearing behaviors or parent- child relations to adult personality traits (e.g., Rapee, 1997). Most
  • 39. of these studies were retrospective, and many found some associ- ation. McCrae and Costa (1988), for example, previously reported that men and women who recalled their parents as being especially loving described themselves as being better adjusted and more agreeable. Although this appears to provide direct support for parental influences on personality, there are many alternative in- terpretations. Perhaps parents had been more loving because these adjusted and agreeable children had been more lovable. Perhaps the same genes that made the parents loving made the children adjusted. Perhaps retrospective bias made kind children recall their childhood with exaggerated fondness. Despite the possible opera- tion of all these artifacts, the observed correlations were only in the range from .10 to .30, accounting for at most 10% of the variance in adult personality traits (cf. Rapee, 1997). It is possible that the effects of parenting are more focused, affecting specific personality traits rather than broad factors. But when the 30 facet scales of the Revised NEO Personality Inventory (NEO-PI-R; Costa & McCrae, 1992a) were correlated with Lov- ing/Rejecting, Casual/Demanding, and Attention scales for father and for mother, none of the 180 correlations reached .30 (Mdn r = .08; McCrae & Costa, 1994).
  • 40. The results of the rare prospective-longitudinal studies are more informative. In one of the first and best of these, Kagan and Moss (1962) examined maternal characteristics during three age periods from infancy to age 10 and assessed the child's personality at ages 19-29. Of 552 relevant correlations, only 35 (6%) reached statis- tical significance at thep < .05 level. If parenting has an effect on personality, it is subtle indeed (Harris, 1998). All these findings are consistent with the results of adoption studies (e.g., Plomin, Corley, Caspi, Fulker, & DeFries, 1998), which showed that children bear little resemblance to either their adoptive parents or their adoptive siblings. Neither parental role modeling nor the parenting practices that would affect all children in a family seem to have much influence on personality traits. Cross-cultural studies of personality structure. It is possible that environmental influences relevant to personality development lie outside the family, in the broader institutions that are collec- tively called culture. As a biologically based phenomenon com- mon to the human species, the fundamental structure of infant and child temperament ought to transcend culture, and there is some evidence that it does (Ahadi, Rothbart, & Ye, 1993). But over time, many psychologists would find it reasonable to argue that the pervasive forces of culture can arbitrarily redefine the parameters of personality—indeed, that was a central premise of the school
  • 41. of culture and personality that flourished in the first half of this century (Singer, 1961). Some contemporary social scientists still find this a plausible argument. Juni (1996) challenged the idea that the five-factor model would apply cross-culturally: "Different cul- tures and different languages should give rise to other models that have little chance of being five in number nor of having any of the factors resemble those . . . of middle-class Americans" (p. 864). However, studies using the Personality Research Form (Paunonen, Jackson, Trzebinski, & Forsterling, 1992; Stumpf, 1993) and the NEO-PI-R (e.g., Martin et al., 1997; McCrae & Costa, 1997; McCrae, Costa, del Pilar, Rolland, & Parker, 1998) have reported clear and detailed replication of the five-factor model in cultures ranging from Malaysia to Estonia. The traits that define the five factors in American samples define the same factors around the world. In this respect, the structure of individual dif- SPECIAL SECTION: PERSONALITY DEVELOPMENT 177 ferences appears to be a universal feature of human groups, rela- tively impervious to cultural variation. Some authors have argued that there are additional personality factors, such as Chinese Tradition (Cheung et al., 1996) and (Filipino) Temperamentalness (Church, Katigbak, & Reyes,
  • 42. 1998) that are indigenous to specific cultures. Such culture-based factors would constitute evidence against a purely endogenous theory of the origins of personality. As yet, however, we know too little about indigenous factors to understand how to evaluate this evi- dence. Perhaps they are measurement artifacts or social attitudes that should be distinguished from personality traits per se; perhaps they really are universal factors that have so far gone unnoticed in other cultures. Because of their importance in the nature-nurture controversy, such proposed factors merit intensive longitudinal, cross-observer, and behavior-genetic research. Comparative studies. The five-factor model may be found in every culture because it is a product of human biology; recent research on animals suggests that at least some of the five factors may also be shared by nonhuman species. Gosling and John (1998) asked cat and dog owners to describe their pets, with terms taken from the five-factor model or from a list intended to describe temperament in animals. In both instruments and in both species, they found four factors: three corresponding to Neuroticism, Ex- traversion, and Agreeableness, and the fourth combining features of Openness to Experience and Conscientiousness in a kind of animal Intellect factor. King and Figueredo (1997) analyzed zookeeper ratings of chimpanzees and found six factors, which corresponded to the five-factor model plus a large dominance
  • 43. factor. It has been known for many years that the five-factor structure of personality can be approximated even in ratings of strangers (Passini & Norman, 1966), so it might be suspected that these ratings of animals were merely projections of implicit personality theory. But Gosling and John (1998) could not replicate a five- factor structure of personality in cats or dogs, even when they used Procrustes rotation, suggesting that something other than sheer implicit personality theory was at work. King and Figueredo (1997) demonstrated substantial agreement between observers on chimpanzee personality ratings—the same kind of evidence that Norman and Goldberg (1966) had used to rebut the claim that personality ratings of humans were mere cognitive fictions. The use of personality ratings in the description of nonhuman species may seem odd—is it meaningful to assess a dog's effi- ciency, harshness, or creativity?—but there is by now substantial scientific literature on the topic (A. H. Buss, 1997; Gosling, 1998). It seems much less odd to speak about temperament in animals; if traits are temperaments, then the literature on individual differ- ences in animals can be more easily understood. Temporal stability of adult personality. Beginning in the 1970s, several independent longitudinal studies (e.g., Block, 1981; Siegler, George, & Okun, 1979) began to address Ihe stability of individual differences in personality traits. Results, with research-
  • 44. ers using a variety of samples, instruments, and methods of mea- surement, showed a consistent pattern of stability. Retest correla- tions over 6, 12, or 20 years were not much smaller than short- term retest reliabilities; personality in 70-year-olds could be predicted with remarkable accuracy from assessments made 30 years earlier (Costa & McCrae, 1992b; Finn, 1986). On the one hand, these findings pointed to the existence of something in the individual that endured over long periods of time—a key piece of evidence for the reality of personality traits. On the other hand, it cast into doubt the influence of intervening events. Over the course of a 30-year study, many participants would have had major life changes in occupation, marital status, family stage, physical health, and place of residence. They would have shared their cohort's experience of assassinations, wars, and recessions; read dozens of books; watched thousands of hours of television. But the cumulative force of all these external influences on personality test scores is barely detectable. Again, it is possible that life events and experiences affect some specific traits even if they do not have a major impact on broad factors. However, in a study of 2,274 men and women traced from about age 40 to age 50, retest correlations for the 30 eight-item NEO-PI-R facet scales were uniformly high, ranging from .64 for
  • 45. Vulnerability to .80 for Assertiveness and Openness to Aesthetics (Siegler & Costa, 1999). The Intrinsic Maturation of Personality Studies of heritability, limited parental influence, structural in- variance across cultures and species, and temporal stability all point to the notion that personality traits are more expressions of human biology than products of life experience. Another more recent line of evidence concerns maturation and personality change. Here we present the latest findings from a series of studies that have examined age differences in the mean levels of person- ality traits across cultures. The basic argument is straightforward: If personality development reflects environmental influences, then groups whose histories have led them through different environ- ments should show different developmental outcomes. Con- versely, if personality development proceeds independently of life experiences, then similar trends should be seen everywhere. The data reviewed above on the temporal stability of personality traits were retest correlations that reflect the consistency of rank order across two occasions. High stability of individual differences does not mean that personality trait scores are unchanging, only that people retain their relative standing across any changes that occur. If the trait score of every individual in a sample increased by exactly the same amount over an interval, the retest
  • 46. correlation would be 1.0, no matter how large or small the increase. The personality changes of interest here must be examined by compar- ing mean levels. Initial work in studies of adults conducted in the United States found very modest mean level effects after age 30. For example, in a large and representative sample of men and women between ages 35 and 84, the correlations of age with Neuroticism, Extra- version, and Openness to Experience were - . 1 2 , —.16, and — .19, respectively (Costa et al., 1986). Later comparisons of college students with older adults showed larger effects, albeit in the same direction: Students scored about one-half standard deviation higher than adults on Neuroticism, Extraversion, and Openness to Expe- rience (Costa & McCrae, 1994). They also scored consistently lower than adults on Agreeableness and Conscientiousness. In themselves, these data are powerfully ambiguous. Perhaps they represent the effects of intrinsic maturation, but there are many other possibilities as well. This pattern of maturation may be purely American, a response to an educational and economic 178 McCRAE ET AL. system that encourages an extended adolescence. Or it may reflect cohort differences, the effects of coming of age at specific times
  • 47. in history. Perhaps present-day adolescents are Less conscientious than their grandparents are because they have grown up in an era of affluence, or of easily available drugs, or of rock music. The usual suggestion for a research design to help untangle such confounds is the longitudinal study. Because comparisons are made between the same individuals tested on two (or more) occasions, birth cohort effects are controlled in longitudinal de- signs. If increases in Conscientiousness were documented in a group of college students as they grew into middle adulthood, that would provide more direct evidence of a true maturational effect. In fact, some studies have reported just such longitudinal changes in variables related to Conscientiousness (lessor, 1983; McGue, Bacon, & Lykken, 1993). Longitudinal studies take time to conduct, however, and longi- tudinal studies of Americans tell us nothing directly about age changes in different cultural and historical contexts. Cross- sectional studies of age differences conducted in other cultures, however, provide a simple way to circumvent some limitations of both cohort and culture, because different cultures have usually had differing recent histories. Consider Turkey and the Czech Republic. Turkey is an Islamic country, and its citizens speak an Altaic language. Following the disintegration of the Ottoman Empire at the end of World War L a new and radically secular society was established, modeled on the West. Institutions from the alphabet to style of dress were reformed; most significantly, women were given unprecedented
  • 48. opportunities for education and occupations outside the home. Turkey was not directly involved in World War II and has pro- gressed slowly toward multiparty democracy. Throughout the cen- tury it has grown in prosperity and urbanization, with a concom- itant decline in strong kinship systems. The Czech Republic, a traditionally Christian nation whose citizens speak a language from the Slavic branch of the Indo- European family, began the century as part of the Austro- Hungarian Empire. Between world wars it functioned as a democ- racy with a highly industrialized economy. In 1938, Germany began an occupation of Czechoslovakia that was ended by Soviet troops in 1945; Soviet dominance continued thereafter, with na- tionalization of industry and collectivization of agriculture. At- tempted reform in 1968 led to a military response from the War- saw Pact, and political repression continued until the collapse of Communist control in 1989. The life experiences of Turks and Czechs have thus been radi- cally different in this century, and both have differed from those of Americans. If experiences shape personality, then cohorts born at the same time in these three countries would presumably differ in mean levels. Czech adolescents, for example, who have spent much of their lives in a democratic society, might be better adjusted than their politically traumatized parents and grandpar- ents. In contrast, American adolescents are known to be higher in Neuroticism than their parents' generation (Costa & McCrae, 1994).
  • 49. Two previous studies have compared age differences on NEO- PI-R scale scores across cultures (Costa et al., in press; McCrae et al., 1999). In each, data were standardized within culture (to eliminate translation effects) and means were calculated for the age groups of 18-21, 22-29, 30-49, and 50+. Data were avail- able for secondary analysis from Germany, Italy, Portugal, Croatia, South Korea, Russia, Estonia, and Japan. In four of the cultures (Italy, Croatia, Russia, and Estonia), there were no sig- nificant age effects for Neuroticism. In the other four cultures, Neuroticism was higher in younger respondents—just as it had been in American studies. Results for the remaining factors are easily summarized; In every culture, the American pattern was replicated. Extraversion and Openness to Experience declined and Agreeableness and Conscientiousness increased with age in Ger- many, Italy, Portugal, Croatia, South Korea, Russia, Estonia, and Japan. Xiu, Wu, Wu, and Shui (1996) examined age differences on a Chinese version of the short form of the NEO-PI-R, the NEO Five-Factor Inventory (NEO-FFI; Costa & McCrae, 1992a). In a sample of 593 men and women between ages 20 and 84, small but significant age effects were found for Neuroticism and Openness to Experience, which declined with age, and Agreeableness, which increased with age. Thus, this study offers a partial replication of American effects (see also Yang, McCrae, & Costa, 1998).
  • 50. New Data From Five Cultures In this article we report analyses of the NEO-FFI administered in Germany, the United Kingdom, Spain, the Czech Republic, and Turkey. The American version was adapted for use in the U.K., and translations were made into the other languages and checked by review of a back-translation. Internal consistency for the five 12-item scales ranged from .48 (for Agreeableness in the Turkish sample) to .85, with a median of .76; in every sample, internal consistency was lowest for the Agreeableness and Openness to Experience scales, suggesting that results with these two scales should be viewed with some caution. Previous cross-cultural studies using the NEO-PI-R have exam- ined only adult development, in part because American normative data have been published only for college-age and older adults. The present article includes data from adolescents between ages 14 and 17 from four of the samples. The NEO-FFI has demonstrated validity when used in samples of intellectually gifted American sixth graders (Parker & Stumpf, 1998); internal consistencies in the four adolescent subsamples studied here ranged from .57 to .86, with a median of .75, values which are comparable to those seen in adults. Data were originally collected for a variety of purposes, and as Table 1 shows, the distribution by age group is not optimal in several instances. Nevertheless, there appear to be sufficient cases in most age groups to make secondary analyses worthwhile. The German sample consists of mono- and dizygotic twins, on whom
  • 51. both self-reports and mean peer ratings of personality are available (Riemann et al., 1997). These respondents are part of a large German sample whose full NEO-PI-R scale scores were previ- ously analyzed (McCrae et al., 1999). They are included here not as an independent replication, but rather as a check on the consis- tency of results from the long and short versions of the NEO-PI- R. Data from the U.K. were obtained in three studies that involved adolescent school children, their parents, and university students. An effort was made to include respondents from all occupational groups; most respondents were from the southern part of the U.K. The Turkish sample consisted of adolescents from many regions in Turkey that attended a summer camp, and families in the city of SPECIAL SECTION: PERSONALITY DEVELOPMENT 179 Table 1 Composition of the Samples by Age Group and Gender Sample German British Spanish Czech Turkish
  • 54. W 615 72 143 76 108 M 73 12 67 40 21 50+ W 230 9 53 24 6 Note. None of the Spanish respondents was under 18 years old: none of the Turkish students or their parents was between ages 22 and 29. M = men; W = women. Bursa, a major industrial center. The Spanish and Czech samples
  • 55. were both recruited by undergraduate psychology students who invited friends, relatives, and partners to join the study. None of these samples is either random or nationally representative, but it seems unlikely that they share any systematic sampling bias that might explain common age trends. As in previous studies, T scores were computed within each culture using means and standard deviations from the adults over age 21 (following the American convention). The only meaningful comparisons are thus among age groups within each culture. Analyses of variance (ANOVAs) with age group and gender as classifying variables showed generally similar patterns in men and women: Of the 25 ANOVAs, only 5 showed significant interaction terms, with no pattern replicated across cultures. Four of the interactions were quite small, accounting for less than 2% of the variance. A somewhat larger effect was seen for Openness to Experience in the Turkish sample, in which age differences were found only in women. Results for the total sample are summarized in Figures 2 - 6 . The ANOVAs confirm that there are significant cross-sectional de- clines in Neuroticism and Extraversion and increases in Consci- entiousness in all five samples. There are significant increases in Agreeableness in the German, Czech, and Turkish samples, but these trends do not reach significance in the British and Spanish samples. The hypothesized decline in Openness to Experience is seen clearly in the Spanish sample, and is significant in the
  • 56. Czech and Turkish samples. In contrast, German and British samples show significantly lower levels of Openness to Experience in the 6 5 - £oo CO 1- c ro CD ig h X a) ro i_ CD < 6 0 - 5 5 - 5 0 - 4 5 - O 40H
  • 57. 35 ] 14-17 | | 18-21 f ^ | 22-29 •M 30-49 ^ 5 0 + German British Spanish Czech Turkish Figure 2. Mean levels of Neuroticism in five cultures. T scores are based on the mean and standard deviation of all respondents over age 21 within each culture. Error bars represent standard errors of the means. 180 McCRAE ET AL s o o K JS c re 35 German British Spanish Czech Turkish Figure 3. Mean levels of Extraversion in five cultures. T scores are based on the mean and standard deviation of all respondents over age 21 within each culture. Error bars represent standard errors of the means. 65 -|
  • 58. 2! o o w c CO Q 14-17 | | 18-21 ^ 22-29 fe:1 30-49 ^ 5 0 + German British Spanish Czech Turkish Figure 4. Mean levels of Openness to Experience in five cultures. T scores are based on the mean and standard deviation of all respondents over age 21 within each culture. Error bars represent standard errors of the means. SPECIAL SECTION: PERSONALITY DEVELOPMENT 181 o o C/) c ro German British Spanish Czech Turkish Figure 5. Mean levels of Agreeableness in five cultures. r scores are based on the mean and standard deviation of all respondents over age 21 within each culture. Age groups do not differ significantly in the British and
  • 59. Spanish samples. Error bars represent standard errors of the means. youngest group than in the group of 18- to 21-year-olds. (The same pattern was seen when mean peer ratings were examined in the German sample.) It is not clear whether this reflects a true de- velopmental trend, a sampling bias, or some culture-specific phenomenon. Although the pattern of results across these samples conforms very closely to hypotheses, it is important to recall that most of the effects are quite small in magnitude. Across cultures, the median correlations of age with Neuroticism, Extraversion, Openness to Experience, Agreeableness, and Conscientiousness scales are - . 1 7 , - . 2 1 , - . 0 8 , .09, and .23, respectively. Thus, previous reviews of the literature that concluded that mean levels of per- sonality traits are generally stable in adulthood (McCrae & Costa, 1990) are only modestly qualified by the present findings. To date, most information on adult age differences in personal- ity has been based on analyses of self-reports. Comparison of peer ratings of college-age men (Costa, McCrae, & Dembroski, 1989) with older adult men (see Costa & McCrae, 1989) on the original NEO Personality Inventory showed significant effects in the ex- pected direction for all five domains, which were substantial in magnitude (greater than one-half standard deviation) for Neuroti- cism and Conscientiousness. However, in the German sample
  • 60. examined here, mean peer ratings showed significant correlations with age only for Neuroticism (-.05), Agreeableness (.06), and Conscientiousness (.21). Research using the full NEO-PI-R in other cultures would be helpful in clarifying the nature and extent of age differences and changes in observer-rated personality traits. The NEO-FFI used in the present study does not assess specific facets of the five factors. Earlier research, however, has shown that individual facet scales of the NEO-PI-R show distinctive age trends across cultures. For example, the Excitement-Seeking facet of Extroversion declined markedly in nine out of nine cultures, whereas the Assertiveness facet showed significant (and small) declines in only four of them. Additional analyses on the specific variance in facet scales (net of the five factors) also showed generalizable, albeit very small, effects (Costa et ah, in press). Intrinsic Maturation and Adult Temperament The data in Figures 2-6 are largely consistent with earlier observations that the same pattern of age differences in personality traits can be seen across different cultures with different recent histories. There appear to be three possible explanations for this phenomenon. The first is that age differences are cohort effects, reflecting the influence of historical forces common to all these cultures, such as the rise of the mass media or the near- universal improvement in health care. Although this possibility cannot be excluded, it would seem to be a remarkable coincidence that
  • 61. common historical forces affect all five factors, whereas historical experiences unique to each culture affect none of the factors enough to reverse the usual pattern. One way to test this hypothesis would be to assess the effect within cultures of variables that might plausibly account for com- mon cohort differences. For example, higher levels of Openness to Experience in younger cohorts might be due to increasing levels of formal education over the course of this century in most cultures. If so, covarying years of education would reduce or eliminate age differences in Openness to Experience. We tested that hypothesis 182 McCRAE ET AL. 65-i a> ou c (0 German British Spanish Czech Turkish Figure 6. Mean levels of Conscientiousness in five cultures. T scores are based on the mean and standard deviation of all respondents over age 21 within each culture. Error bars represent standard errors of the means.
  • 62. in the Spanish, German, and Turkish samples, in which data on education were available, but found that significant age differences in Openness to Experience remained. A second possibility is that societies everywhere (or perhaps modern industrial societies everywhere) spontaneously develop parallel institutions that encourage the same trends in personality development. Adult responsibilities may make adults more respon- sible; caring for children may make them more caring. This pos- sibility cannot be easily dismissed, but it is not yet proven. Even if there is an association between age-role demands and personality traits, it is possible that the causal order is reversed, and that social norms have been crafted to accommodate intrinsic maturational trends in personality. This is quite clear in the case of laws defining a minimum age for driving, voting, and drinking. A third possibility is that there are natural progressions of personality development that occur without regard to cultural and historical context. Just as children leara to talk, count, and reason in a fixed order and time course, so too may adults become more agreeable and less extraverted as a natural consequence of aging. This notion of intrinsic maturation is consistent with the other lines of evidence—heritability, stability, and cross-cultural univer-
  • 63. sality—that point to the interpretation of traits as endogenous basic tendencies. It is also supported more directly by behavior-genetic and comparative evidence on age changes in personality. Changes in personality traits between adolescence and young adulthood have been shown to be modestly to moderately heritable (McGue et a l , 1993), and developmental trends in chimpanzees (King, Landau, & Guggenheim, 1998) and rhesus monkeys (Suomi, Novak, & Well, 1996) have shown some intriguing parallels to adult human development. Whether age grading in the social structure shapes personality development or vice-versa—or whether both processes are at work—cannot be determined from available data. Future research might test these alternative hypotheses in third-world nations where adult responsibilities are assumed at an earlier age or among people with different relevant life experiences, such as .parenting. But viewing personality as temperament at least has the virtue of making intrinsic maturation a plausible hypothesis that merits testing. Linking Child Temperament and Adult Personality The intent of the whole preceding argument was to demonstrate that if by temperament we mean biologically based psychological
  • 64. tendencies with intrinsic paths of development, then standard personality inventories assess temperament, and traits such as aesthetic sensitivity, achievement striving, and modesty are as much temperaments as are activity level and behavioral inhibition. From this perspective it is perhaps not surprising that when Ang- leitner and Ostendorf (1994) factored adult temperament measures (A. H. Buss & Plomin, 1975; Strelau, Angleitner, Bantelmann, & Ruth, 1990) along with other markers they found the familiar structure of the five-factor model. SPECIAL SECTION: PERSONALITY DEVELOPMENT 183 But if the individual differences identified by temperament researchers and personality trait psychologists are much the same, the goals and methods of these two research traditions are not. Researchers within the temperament tradition often emphasize basic processes and mechanisms. Ahadi and Rothbart (1994), for example, have examined psychological systems such as Approach and Effortful Control, and Strelau and colleagues (Strelau et al., 1990) have developed a set of constructs based on hypothesized Pavlovian properties of the central nervous system. In contrast, trait psychologists more often focus on outcomes and other corre- lates of traits. For example, Barrick and Mount (1991) showed that Conscientiousness is associated with superior job performance. By
  • 65. identifying personality traits with temperaments, researchers may begin to integrate these different emphases on causes and effects and come to a better understanding of both the origins and the expressions of basic tendencies (Costa & McCrae, in press). The Structure and Stability of Individual Differences It cannot be assumed that the adult structure of temperament will appear in analyses of temperament variables in children, but there is evidence that something similar to the five factors can be found in adult ratings of school children (Digman & Shmelyov, 1996; Kohnstamm, Halverson, Mervielde, & Havill, 1998) and in self-reports from children as young as 5 years old (Measelle & John, 1997). Ahadi and Rothbart (1994) have offered conceptual analyses that link child temperament constructs to adult personal- ity factors: Approach to Extraversion, Anxiety to Neuroticism, and Effortful Control to Conscientiousness and Agreeableness. Classic efforts at understanding infant temperament (Thomas, Chess, & Birch, 1968) were not informed by the five-factor model; if inves- tigators looked for these factors, they might find them even in neonates, just as they have been found in nonhuman animals (King & Figueredo, 1997). Even if identical factors were found in infants and adults, it would not imply that infant temperament is a good predictor of adult personality. Reviews of the longitudinal literature have re-
  • 66. ported that temperament variables in fact show limited stability across relatively short intervals, especially among infants (e.g., Lemery, Goldsmith, Klinnert, & Mrazek, 1999), and very modest prediction of adult traits (Wachs, 1994). Block (1993), for exam- ple, examined retest correlations for ego undercontrol and ego resiliency at age 3 and age 23 in boys and girls; only one of these four correlations reached significance (although all were positive). In a recent review of the longitudinal attachment literature, Fraley (1998) reported an average correlation of .19 between attachment at age 1 and age 19. Kagan and Zentner (1996) found only modest associations between characteristics of early childhood and adult psy chopathology. Even modest associations can be meaningful if the outcomes are socially significant. Caspi and colleagues (Caspi, Elder, & Her- bener, 1990) have shown that childhood personality traits (includ- ing shyness and ill-temperedness) can predict important life out- comes such as delayed marriage and downward mobility. Undercontrol at age 3 predicts health-risk behaviors in young adults through the mediation of personality traits in adolescence (Caspi et al., 1997). With shorter intervals and older children, stronger associations are found. For example, ego control showed a retest correlation of
  • 67. .70 between age 3 and age 4, and .67 between age 14 and age 23 (Block, 1993). Siegler and colleagues (Siegler et al., 1990) esti- mated that half of the variance in personality dimensions is stable from late adolescence to middle adulthood, and Helson and Moane (1987) reported greater stability between age 27 and age 43 (a 16-year interval) than between age 21 and age 27 (a 6-year interval). When adults initially over age 30 are studied, uncor- rected retest coefficients near .70 are not uncommon over 30- year periods (Costa & McCrae, 1992b). One very general principle of life span personality development thus appears to be that the stability of individual differences over a fixed time interval increases steadily from infancy up to at least age 30. Environmentalists might assume that this phenomenon is attributable to the accumulation of life experiences: Any single new experience should affect more change when it occurs in the context of the limited experience of early life than when it com- petes with a lifetime of other experiences. In contrast, FFT suggests another answer: Endogenous disposi- tions develop over time in ways that redistribute rank orderings. The functioning of genes, after all, is not fixed at birth; they switch on and off across the life span and contribute to individual patterns of aging. The brain itself continues to grow and develop until at least the mid-20s (Pujol, Vendrell, Junque", Marti-Vilalta, & Cap- devila, 1993), so it is hardly surprising that personality traits would
  • 68. also change in this period. Developmental Trends for Five Factors At the aggregate level, it is possible to describe general devel- opmental trends for the five factors (and the specific traits that define them; see McCrae et al., 1999). From age 18 to age 30 there are declines in Neuroticism, Extraversion, and Openness to Expe- rience, and increases in Agreeableness and Conscientiousness; after age 30 the same trends are found, although the rate of change seems to decrease. In this article we presented some of the first data tracing the five factors backward from age 18, with German, British, Czech, and Turkish samples. For the most part, high-school-age boys and girls appeared to continue the same trends: They were even higher in Neuroticism and Extraversion and lower in Agreeableness and Conscientiousness than were college-age students. No clear trend could be discerned for Openness to Experience, as lower instead of higher scores were found in the German and British samples. The present data do support the use of instruments like the NEO-FFI in younger adolescents, and it would be a relatively simple matter to conduct cross-sectional studies on representative samples of this age group. Research with even younger samples is possible, but would require new instruments. Measelle and John (1997), for example, used a puppet interview to assess
  • 69. personality in young children and reported increases in Conscientiousness between ages 5 and 7. Calibrating puppet interviews and NEO- FFIs would be difficult, so it is likely that developmental trends will have to be pieced together from studies of overlapping seg- ments of childhood. What could account for these developmental trends? Evolution- ary arguments might be offered. High levels of Extraversion and Openness to Experience might be useful in finding a mate, whereas higher Agreeableness and Conscientiousness might be more im- portant for raising a family. Comparative studies of personality 184 McCRAE ET AL. development in other primates (King et al., 1998) with different patterns of mating and child rearing might be used to test such evolutionary hypotheses. The Development of Characteristic Adaptations Finally, it is worth recalling that FFT postulates developments on two separate tracks: Basic tendencies follow a pattern of in- trinsic maturation, whereas characteristic adaptations respond to the opportunities and incentives of the social environment. To the extent that the theory is correct, psychologists, educators, and parents will have relatively little impact on the long-term devel- opment of personality traits, but they can have an influence on characteristic adaptations (cf. Harkness & Lilienfeld, 1997). Traits can be channeled even if they cannot be changed. What kinds of habits, skills, beliefs, and social networks are optimal for shy or
  • 70. ill-tempered children? These are likely to be the most productive questions for those concerned about shaping human development. References Agronick, G. S., & Duncan, L. E. (1998). Personality and social change: Individual differences, life path, and importance attributed to the wom- en's movement. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 74, 1545-1555. Ahadi, S. A., & Rothbart, M. K. (1994). Temperament, development, and the Big Five. In C. F. Halverson, Jr., G. A. Kohnstamm, & R. P. Martin (Eds,), The developing structure of temperament and personality from infancy to adulthood (pp. 189-207). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. Ahadi, S. A., Rothbart, M. K., & Ye, R. (1993). Children's temperament in the US and China: Similarities and differences. European Journal of Personality, 7, 359-377. Angleitner, A., & Ostendorf, F. (1994). Temperament and the Big Five factors of personality, hi C. F. Halverson, G. A. Kohnstamm, & R. P. Martin (Eds.), The developing structure of temperament and personality from infancy to adulthood (pp. 69-90). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
  • 71. Asendorpf, J. B., & Wilpers, S. (1998). Personality effects on social relationships. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 74, 1531- 1544. Barrick, M. R., & Mount M. K. (1991). The Big Five personality dimen- sions and job performance: A meta-analysis. Personnel Psychology, 44, 1-26. Begley, S. (1998, September 7). The parent trap. Newsweek, 52- 59. Benet-Martinez, V., &. Waller, N. G. (1997). Further evidence for the cross-cultural generality of the Big Seven Factor model: Indigenous and imported Spanish personality constructs. Journal of Personality, 65, 567-598. Block, J. (1981). Some enduring and consequential structures of person- ality. In A. I. Rabin, J. Aronoff, A. M. Barclay, & R. A. Zucker (Eds.), Further explorations in personality (pp. 27-43). New York: Wiley- Interscience. Block, J. (1993). Studying personality the long way. In D. C. Funder, R. D. Parke, C. Tomlinson-Keasey, & K. Widaman (Eds.), Studying lives
  • 72. through time: Personality and development (pp. 9—41). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association. Bremner, J. D. (1998). Neuroimaging of posttraumatic stress disorder. Psychiatric Annals, 28, 445-450. Buss, A. H. (1997). Evolutionary perspectives on personality traits. In R. Hogan, J. Johnson, & S. R. Briggs (Eds.), Handbook of personality psychology (pp. 345-366). San Diego, CA: Academic Press. Buss, A. H., & Plomin, R. (1975). A temperament theory of personality development. New York: Wiley. Buss, D. M. (1991). Evolutionary personality psychology. Annual Review of Psychology, 42, 459-491. Buss, D. M., & Greiling, H. (1999). Adaptive individual differences. Journal of Personality, 67, 209-243. Caspi, A., Begg, D., Dickson, N., Harrington, H. L., Langley, J., Moffitt, T. E., & Silva, P. A. (1997). Personality differences predict health-risk behaviors in young adulthood: Evidence from a longitudinal study. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 73, 1052-1063. Caspi, A., Elder, G. H., Jr., & Herbener, E. S. (1990). Childhood person-
  • 73. ality and the prediction of life-course patterns. In L. N. Robins & M. Rutter (Eds.), Straight and devious pathways from childhood to adult- hood (pp. 13-35). New York: Cambridge University Press. Cheung, F. M., Leung, K., Fan, R. M., Song, W. Z., Zhang, J. X., & Zhang, J. P. (1996). Development of the Chinese Personality Assessment In- ventory. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 27, 181-199. Church, A. T., Katigbak, M. S., & Reyes, J. A. S. (1998). Further explo- ration of Filipino personality structure using the lexical approach: Do the Big-Five or Big-Seven dimensions emerge? European Journal of Per- sonality, 12, 249-270. Cloninger, C. R., Przybeck, T. R., Svrakic, D. M., & Wetzel, R. D. (1994). The Temperament and Character Inventory (TCI): A guide to Us devel- opment and use. St. Louis, MO: Washington University, Center for Psychobiology of Personality. Costa, P. T., Jr., & McCrae, R. R. (1989). The NEO-P1/NEO- FFI manual supplement. Odessa, FL: Psychological Assessment Resources. Costa, P. T., Jr., & McCrae, R. R. (1992a). Revised NEO Personality Inventory (NEO-PI-R) and NEO Five-Factor Inventory (NEOFFI) pro-
  • 74. fessional manual. Odessa, FL: Psychological Assessment Resources. Costa, P. T., Jr., & McCrae, R. R. (1992b). Trait psychology comes of age. In T. B. Sonderegger (Ed.), Nebraska Symposium on Motivation: Psy- chology and aging (pp. 169-204). Lincoln: University of Nebraska Press. Costa, P. T., Jr., & McCrae, R. R. (1994). Stability and change in person- ality from adolescence through adulthood. In C. F. Halverson, G. A. Kohnstamm, & R. P. Martin (Eds.), The developing structure of tem- perament and personality from infancy to adulthood (pp. 139- 150). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. Costa, P. T., Jr., & McCrae, R. R. (in press). A theoretical context for adult temperament. In T. D. Wachs & G. A. Kohnstamm (Eds.), Temperament in context. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. Costa, P. T., Jr., McCrae, R. R., & Dembroski, T. M. (1989). Agreeable- ness vs. antagonism: Explication of a potential risk factor for CHD. In A. Siegman & T. M. Dembroski (Eds.), In search of coronary- prone behavior: Beyond Type A (pp. 41-63). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. Costa, P. T., Jr., McCrae, R. R., Martin, T. A., Oryol, V. E.,
  • 75. Senin, I. G., Rukavishnikov, A. A., Shimonaka, Y., Nakazato, K., Gondo, Y., Takayama, M., Allik, J., Kallasmaa, T., & Realo, A. (in press). Person- ality development from adolescence through adulthood: Further cross- cultural comparisons of age differences. In V. J. Molfese & D. Molfese (Eds.), Temperament and personality development across the life span. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. Costa, P. T., Jr., McCrae, R. R., Zonderman, A. B., Barbano, H. E., Lebowitz, B., & Larson, D. M. (1986). Cross-sectional studies of per- sonality in a national sample: 2. Stability in neuroticism, extraversion, and openness. Psychology and Aging, I, 144-149. Digman, J. M., & Shraelyov, A. G. (1996). The structure of temperament and personality in Russian children. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 71, 341-351. Eysenck, H. J. (1990). Genetic and environmental contributions to indi- vidual differences: The three major dimensions of personality. Journal of Personality, 58, 245-262. Eysenck, S. B. G. (1983). One approach to cross-cultural studies of personality. Australian Journal of Psychology, 35, 383-390.
  • 76. Finn, S. E. (1986). Stability of personality self-ratings over 30 years: SPECIAL SECnON: PERSONALITY DEVELOPMENT 185 Evidence for an age/cohort interaction. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 50, 813-818. Fraley, R. C. (1998). Attachment continuity from infancy to adulthood: Meta-analysis and dynamic modeling of developmental mechanisms. Unpublished manuscript. University of California, Davis. Goldberg, L. R. (1993J. The structure of phenotypic personality traits. American Psychologist, 48, 26-34, Gosling, S. D. (1998). From mice to men: What can animal research tell us about human personality? Unpublished manuscript, University of California, Berkeley. Gosling, S. D., & John, O. P. (1998, May). Personality dimensions in dogs, cats, and hyenas. In S. D. Gosling & S. Suomi (Chairs), From mice to men: Bridging the gap between personality and animal researchers. Symposium conducted at the 10th Annual Convention of the American Psychological Society, Washington, DC.
  • 77. Gough, H. G. (1987), California Psychological Inventory administrator's guide. Palo Alto, CA: Consulting Psychologists Press. Guilford, J. S., Zimmerman, W. S., & Guilford, J. P. (1976). The Guilford- Zimmerman Temperament Survey Handbook: Twenty-five years of re- search and application. San Diego, CA: EdTTS. Harkness, A. R., & Lilienfeld, S. O. (1997). Individual differences science for treatment planning: Personality traits. Psychological Assessment, 9, 349-360. Harris, J. R. (1998). The nurture assumption: Why children turn out the way they do. New York: Free Press. Helson, R., & Moane, G. (1987). Personality change in women from college to midlife. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 53, 176-186. Jang, K. L., McCrae, R. R., Angleitner, A., Riemann, R., & Livesley, W. J. (1998). Heritability of facet-level traits in a cross-cultural twin study: Support for a hierarchical model of personality. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 74, 1556-1565. Jessor, R. (1983). The stability of change: Psychosocial
  • 78. development from adolescence to young adulthood. In D. Magnusson & V. L. Allen (Eds.), Human development: An interactional perspective (pp. 321— 341). New York: Academic Press. Juni, S. (1996). Review of the revised NEO Personality Inventory. In J. C. Conoley & J. C. Impara (Eds.), 12th Mental Measurements Yearbook (pp. 863-868). Lincoln: University of Nebraska Press. Kagan, J., & Moss, H. A. (1962). From birth to maturity. New York: Wiley. Kagan, J., & Zentner, M. (1996). Early childhood predictors of adult psychopathology. Harvard Review of Psychiatry, 3, 341-350. Kagitcibasj, C. (1996). Family and human development across cultures: A view from the other side. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. King, J. E., & Figueredo, A. J. (1997). The five-factor model plus domi- nance in chimpanzee personality. Journal of Research in Personal- ity, 31, 257-271. King, J. E.( Landau. V. I., & Guggenheim, C. B. (1998, May). Age-related personality changes in chimpanzees. In S. D. Gosling & S. Suomi (Chairs), From mice to men: Bridging the gap between
  • 79. personality and animal researchers. Symposium conducted at the 10th Annual Conven- tion of die American Psychological Society, Washington, DC. Kitayama, S-, & Markus, H. R. (Eds.). (1994). Emotion and culture: Empirical studies of mutual influences. Washington, DC: American Psychological Association. Kluckhohn, C. (1944). Navaho witchcraft. Papers of the Peabody Museum of American Archaeology and Ethnology (Vol. 22, No. 3). Cambridge, MA: Harvard University. Kluckhohn, C , & Murray, H. A. (1953). Personality formation: The determinants. In C. Kluckhohn, H. A. Murray, & D. M. Schneider (Eds.), Personality in nature, society, and culture (2nd ed., pp. 53-67). New York: Knopf. Kohnstamm, G. A., Havlerson, C. F., Jr., Mervielde, I., & Havill, V. L. (Eds.). (1998). Parental descriptions of child personality: Developmen- tal antecedents of the Big Five? Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. Kolb, B., & Whishaw, I. Q. (1998). Brain plasticity and behavior. Annual Review of Psychology, 49, 43-64.
  • 80. Labouvie-Vief, G., Diehl, M., Tarnowski, A., & Shen, J. (in press). Age differences in adult personality: Findings from the United States and China. Journal of Gerontology: Psychological Sciences. Lemery, K. S., Goldsmith, H. H., Klinnert, M. D., & Mrazek, D. A. (1999). Developmental models of infant and childhood temperament. Develop- mental Psychology, 35, 189-204. Loehlin, J. C. (1992). Genes and environment in personality development. Newbury Park, CA: Sage. Martin, T. A., Draguns, J. G., Oryol, V. E., Senin, I. G., Rukavishnikov, A. A., & Klotz, M. L. (1997, August). Development of a Russian- language NEO-PI-R. Paper presented at the 105th Annual Convention of the American Psychological Association, Chicago, IL. McAdams, D. P. (1996). Personality, modernity, and the storied self: A contemporary framework for studying persons. Psychological Inquiry, 7, 295-321. McCrae, R. R., & Costa, P. T , Jr. (1988). Recalled parent-child relations and adult personality. Journal of Personality, 56, 417-434. McCrae, R. R., & Costa, P. T., Jr. (1990). Personality in adulthood. New
  • 81. York: Guilford Press. McCrae, R. R., & Costa, P. T., Jr. (1994). The paradox of parental influence: Understanding retrospective studies of parent-child relations and adult personality. In C. Perris, W. A. Arrindell, & M. Eisemann (Eds.), Parenting and psychopathology (pp. 107-125). New York: Wiley. McCrae, R. R., & Costa, P. T., Jr. (1996). Toward a new generation of personality theories: Theoretical contexts for the five-factor model. In J. S. Wiggins (Ed.), The five-factor model of personality: Theoretical perspectives (pp. 51-87). New York: Guilford Press. McCrae, R. R., & Costa, P. T., Jr. (1997). Personality trait structure as a human universal. American Psychologist, 52, 509-516. McCrae, R. R., & Costa, P. T., Jr. (1999). A five-factor theory of person- ality. In L. Pervin & O. P. John (Eds.), Handbook of personality (2nd ed., pp. 139-153). New York: Guilford Press. McCrae, R. R., Costa, P. T., Jr., del Pilar, G. H., Rolland, J. P., & Parker, W. D. (1998). Cross-cultural assessment of the five-factor model: The Revised NEO Personality Inventory. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychol-
  • 82. ogy, 29, 171-188. McCrae, R. R., Costa, P. T., Jr., Lima, M. P., Simoes, A., Ostendorf, F., Angleitner, A., Marusic I., Bratko, D., Caprara, G. V., Barbaranelli, C , Chae, J. H., & Piedmont, R. L. (1999). Age differences in personality across the adult life span: Parallels in five cultures. Developmental Psychology, 35, 466-477. McCrae, R. R., Yik, M. S. M., Trapnell, P. D., Bond, M. H., & Paulhus, D. L. (1998). Interpreting personality profiles across cultures: Bilingual, acculturation, and peer rating studies of Chinese undergraduates. Jour- nal of Personality and Social Psychology, 74, 1041-1055. McGue, M., Bacon, S., & Lykken, D. T. (1993). Personality stability and change in early adulthood: A behavioral genetic analysis. Developmental Psychology, 29, 96-109. . Measelle, J., & John, O. P. (1997, May). Young children's self- perceptions on the Big Five: Consistency, stability, and school adaptation from age 5 to age 7. Paper presented at the Biennial Meeting of die Society for Research in Child Development, Washington, DC. Morris, W. (1976). The American heritage dictionary of the English
  • 83. language. Boston: Houghton Mifflin. Nelson, C. A. (1999). Neural plasticity and human development. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 8f 42-45. Norman, W. T., & Goldberg, L. R. (1966). Raters, ratees, and randomness in personality structure. Journal of Personality and Social Psychol- ogy, 4, 681-691. 186 McCRAE ET AL. Parker, W. D., & Stumpf, H. (1998). A validation of the five- factor model of personality in academically-talented youth across observers and in- struments. Personality and Individual Differences, 25, 1005- 1025. Passini, F. T., & Norman, W. T. (1966). A universal conception of personality structure? Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 4, 4 4 - 4 9 . Paunonen, S. V., Jackson, D. N., Trzebinski, J., & Forsteriing, F. (1992). Personality structure across cultures: A multimethod evaluation. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 62, 447-456. Plomin, R., Corley, R., Caspi, A., Fulker, D. W., & DeFries, J.
  • 84. (1998). Adoption results for self-reported personality: Evidence for nonadditive genetic effects? Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 75, 211-218. Plomin, R., & Daniels, D. (1987). Why are children in the same family so different from one another? Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 10, 1-16. Pujol, J., Vendrell, P., Junque", C , Martf-Vilalta, J. L., & Capdevila, A. (1993). When does human brain development end? Evidence of corpus callosum growth up to adulthood. Annals of Neurology, 34, 71- 75. Rapee, R. M. (1997). Potential role of childrearing practices in the devel- opment of anxiety and depression. Clinical Psychology Review, 17, 47-67. Resnick, S. M., Gottesman, I. L, & McGue, M. (1993). Sensation seeking in opposite-sex twins: An effect of prenatal hormones? Behavior Ge- netics, 23, 323-329. Riemann, R., Angleitner, A., & Strelau, J. (1997). Genetic and environ- mental influences on personality: A study of twins reared together using the self- and peer-report NEO-FFI scales. Journal of
  • 85. Personality, 65, 449-475- Siegler, I. C , & Costa, P. T., Jr. (1999, August). Personality continuity and change in midlife men and women. Symposium presented at the 107th Annual Convention of the American Psychological Association, Boston, MA. Siegler, I. C , George, L. K., & Okun, M. A. (1979). Cross- sequential analysis of adult personality. Developmental Psychology, 15, 350-351. Siegler, I. C , Zonderman, A. B., Barefoot, J. C , Williams, R. B., Jr., Costa, P. T., Jr., & McCrae, R. R. (1990). Predicting personality in adulthood from college MMP1 scores: Implications for follow-up studies in psy- chosomatic medicine. Psychosomatic Medicine, 52, 644-652. Singer, M. (1961). A survey of culture and personality theory and research. In B. Kaplan (Ed.), Studying personality cross-culturally (pp. 9- 90). Evanston, IL: Row, Peterson. Strelau, J., Angleitner, A., Bantelmann, J., & Ruch, W. (1990). The Strelau Temperament Inventory—Revised (STT-R): Theoretical considerations and scale development. European Journal of Personality, 4, 209-