SlideShare a Scribd company logo
1 of 147
The Sweet Taste Test: Relationships with Anhedonia Subtypes,
Personality Traits, and Menstrual Cycle Phases
Jeffrey S. Bedwell1 & Christopher C. Spencer1 & Cristina A.
Chirino1 & John P. O’Donnell1
Published online: 15 January 2019
# Springer Science+Business Media, LLC, part of Springer
Nature 2019
Abstract
A better understanding of the etiology and pathology related to
distinct subtypes of anhedonia can lead to more efficacious
personalized treatments. The current study advances knowledge
on consummatory anhedonia - represented in the Research
Domain Criteria (RDoC) subconstruct of Initial Response to
Reward (IRR). The Sweet Taste Test (STT) has promise as a
behavioral paradigm for IRR, as it is sensitive to manipulation
of μ-opioid receptors. However, there is a lack of existing
knowledge of how the STT relates to subtypes of anhedonia,
personality traits, and phases of the menstrual cycle. To address
these questions, we administered the STT to 72 nonpsychiatric
adults (76% women; mean age: 19.11). As predicted, the
hedonic
slope reflecting increasing Blike^ ratings over increasing
concentrations of five sucrose solutions (ranging from 0.05 M
to
0.86 M), was lower in individuals reporting higher
consummatory anhedonia (measured with Temporal Experience
of
Pleasure Scales - Consummatory Subscale) and in women in the
mid-to-late luteal menstrual phase (days 20 to 28). Both effects
were driven by lower hedonic ratings to the sweetest
concentration. The hedonic slope was larger in individuals
scoring higher on
the Flight-Freeze-Avoidance System personality factor from the
Reinforcement Sensitivity Theory Personality Questionnaire -
driven by lower hedonic ratings for the least sweet
concentration. No factor or aspect from the Big Five Aspects
Scale related to
hedonic ratings on the STT. The STT may be a valid and
specific standardized behavioral paradigm to add to IRR,
particularly if
validated in a large transdiagnostic psychiatric sample.
Keywords Sucrose .Reward .Consummatoryanhedonia
.Initialresponsivenesstorewardattainment
.Researchdomaincriteria .
Consummatory pleasure
Introduction
Anhedonia is a transdiagnostic symptom which is particularly
resistant to existing treatments relative to most co-occurring
symptoms (McCabe et al. 2010; Vittengl et al. 2015). While
the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders
(DSM-5) defines anhedonia as a unitary construct (e.g.,
Bdecreased interest and pleasure in most activities most of
the day;^ American Psychiatric Association 2013), recent ad-
vances in affective neuroscience suggest three distinct sub-
types (Treadway and Zald 2011): motivational (i.e., wanting),
consummatory (i.e., liking), and decisional (i.e., reward learn-
ing). The National Institute of Mental Health’s (NIMH)
Research Domain Criteria (RDoC) Matrix includes
corresponding constructs under the Positive Valence
Systems (PVS) domain (Insel et al. 2010). A better under-
standing of the etiology and pathology related to distinct sub-
types of anhedonia can lead to more efficacious personalized
interventions for this treatment-resistant symptom (Insel and
Cuthbert 2015; Strauss and Cohen 2017).
Consummatory anhedonia is partially represented in the
PVS construct Reward Responsiveness under the
subconstruct of Initial Response to Reward (IRR; as of 10/2/
18), defined by NIMH as: BProcesses evoked by the initial
presentation of a positive reinforcer as reflected by indices
of neuronal activity and verbal or behavioral responses.^
This is reflected by the inclusion of the Consummatory sub-
scale of the Temporal Experience of Pleasure Scales (TEPS-C;
Gard et al. 2007) under Self-Reports in IRR. While IRR cur-
rently names a single paradigm of Bsimple guessing task,^
application of such tasks require physiological measures of
brain response (e.g., Tsypes et al. 2018). Alternatively, the
Sweet Taste Test (STT; Kampov-Polevoy et al. 1997;
Dichter et al. 2010) has promise as a behavioral IRR paradigm
* Jeffrey S. Bedwell
[email protected]
1 Department of Psychology, University of Central Florida,
4111 Pictor
Lane, Orlando, FL 32816-1390, USA
Journal of Psychopathology and Behavioral Assessment (2019)
41:235–248
https://doi.org/10.1007/s10862-019-09717-2
http://crossmark.crossref.org/dialog/?doi=10.1007/s10862-019-
09717-2&domain=pdf
http://orcid.org/0000-0002-1205-5792
mailto:[email protected]
that could be used in clinical settings, as it is does not require
physiological equipment and related expertise. While the STT
is not specifically named under Paradigms for IRR, Btaste
reactivity^ is listed under Behavior. In the STT, the participant
tastes a random order of five trials for each of five concentra-
tions of sucrose, ranging from minimal sweetness to two so-
lutions that are sweeter than beverages such as Coca-Cola®,
and collects subjective ratings of sweetness and liking after
each trial (Kampov-Polevoy et al. 1997).
Individuals whose hedonic Blike^ ratings are highest
for the strongest sucrose concentration have been termed
Bsweet likers,^ a category associated with alcohol use dis-
order (Kampov-Polevoy et al. 2001, 2003; Wronski et al.
2007), cocaine use disorder (Janowsky et al. 2003), binge-
eating disorder (Goodman et al. 2017), impulsive choice
on a delay discounting task (Weafer et al. 2014), better
response to naltrexone treatment for alcohol use disorder
(Garbutt et al. 2009, 2016), and greater amphetamine-
induced euphoria in nonpsychiatric women but not men
(Weafer et al. 2017). Consistent with inclusion of μ-
opioid receptors under IRR Molecules, research has
shown that stimulation of μ-opioid Bhedonic hotspots^
in the nucleus accumbens shell, ventral pallidum, anterior
orbitofrontal cortex, and posterior insula increase sucrose
Bliking^ behavior in rats (Castro and Berridge 2017).
Similarly, a study of nonpsychiatric men found a decrease
in sucrose liking following administration of the μ-opioid
antagonist naltrexone, and increased liking following the
μ-opioid agonist morphine (Eikemo et al. 2016). Both
effects were observed for only dimensional hedonic rat-
ings for the sweetest concentration (0.65 M in that study)
and did not change a slope-based categorical sweet liker
status. This finding is consistent with studies showing that
naltrexone reduced average STT hedonic ratings in non-
psychiatric women (Arbisi et al. 1999) and opioid depen-
dent participants (Langleben et al. 2012). Overall, it ap-
pears that the STT partially reflects endogenous opioid
functioning, particularly the dimensional hedonic rating
to the sweetest solution.
Considering this emerging literature, we were interested in
how STT performance relates to subtypes of anhedonia and a
range of personality traits. This will inform investigators and
clinicians regarding which anhedonia measure or subscale
may best assess IRR/consummatory anhedonia, and general
personality research by highlighting particular traits which
may be sensitive to individual differences in the endogenous
opioid system. Finally, there is increasing evidence that the
luteal menstrual cycle phase, via an increase in progesterone,
dampens hedonic responsivity to cocaine in female rhesus
macaques (Carroll et al. 2016) and humans (Evans and
Foltin 2006; Evans et al. 2002). As both progesterone and
estradiol interact with opioid receptors (Schroeder et al.
2003; Lee and Ho 2013), it is possible that estradiol, which
is higher during the follicular menstrual phase, may also effect
reward sensitivity. However, studies in rhesus monkeys found
that administering exogenous estradiol did not alter cocaine
self-administration, while administering progesterone reduced
the cocaine use (Mello et al. 2008, 2011). A recent study
found an amplitude reduction in the event-related potential
of reward positivity in response to monetary gains in women
during the luteal as opposed to follicular phase of the menstru-
al cycle, but this influence was only significant in women with
greater severity of depression (Mulligan et al. 2018).
Examining the relationship between menstrual phases and
STT performance in a nonpsychiatric sample will account
for important individual differences present in menstruating
women and add to our understanding of how these phases
may induce temporary changes in IRR.
It appears that only one existing study examined relation-
ships between a behavioral assessment of sweet taste liking
and anhedonia severity. This study found that, across a
transdiagnostic sample of individuals with major depressive
disorder, schizophrenia, and nonpsychiatric controls, higher
scores on the self-report Chapman Physical, but not Social,
Anhedonia scale was associated with a lower maximum
hedonic rating across all sucrose concentrations on the
STT (Berlin et al. 1998). There appear to be few published
studies on the relationship of a behavioral measure of sweet
liking with personality traits. One reported that individuals
who preferred the sweeter of two blinded samples of the
same white wine scored lower on self-reported openness
and higher on impulsiveness, a facet of neuroticism
(Saliba et al. 2009). A study using five blinded levels of
sweetness of the same red wine found that participants
who preferred sweeter concentrations were higher in neu-
roticism (Sena-Esteves et al. 2018). A study using the STT
reported that categorical sweet likers did not differ from
other participants on self-reported novelty seeking (Lange
et al. 2010). Studies that did not include a behavioral mea-
sure found that higher self-reported generalized liking of
sweet foods and drinks was associated with higher self-
reported agreeableness (Sagioglou and Greitemeyer 2016;
Meier et al. 2012; Ashton et al. 2014), neuroticism (Kikuchi
and Watanabe 2000; Elfhag and Erlanson-Albertsson
2006), and/or openness/intellect (Ashton et al. 2014) scores.
Finally, while some research has found that women con-
sume more sweet foods during the luteal phase (Bowen
and Grunberg 1990; Tucci et al. 2010), one study examined
hedonic rating to varying concentrations of sucrose solu-
tions reported that ratings were lowest in the luteal phase,
236 J Psychopathol Behav Assess (2019) 41:235–248
particularly for the sweetest concentration (Elliott et al.
2015). However, this study did not use the traditional STT,
was limited by a single trial of four sucrose solutions, and
did not include men in the comparison group.
Based on existing findings using behavioral measures of
hedonic response to sweet tastes, and current information in
the IRR domain, we hypothesized that hedonic ratings to the
sweetest STT concentration would be lower in participants
reporting increased consummatory anhedonia, and higher in
participants reporting more neuroticism and/or impulsiveness.
In addition, we predicted that women in the luteal phase of the
menstrual cycle would report a reduced hedonic response to
the sweetest STT concentration as compared to other women
and men.
Methods
Participants
Participants were undergraduate students enrolled in a
Psychology Department course that offered credit in ex-
change for research participation at a large southeastern
public university in the United States. Participants were
screened online (N = 2135) and excluded for: completing
questionnaire either too quickly (< 10th %ile of duration
from sample; N = 122; 5.7%) or slowly (> 90%ile; N =
146; 6.8%), scoring >2 SD above mean on the
Abbreviated Marlow-Crowne Social Desirability Scale
(Reynolds 1982; N = 75; 3.5%), using non-prescribed stim-
ulant or narcotic medication (N = 23; 1.1%), excessive
chronic alcohol use (N = 16; 1.0%), hypothyroidism in self
(N = 30; 1.4%) or first-degree family member (N = 91;
4.3%), significant past head injury or chronic neurological
disorder (N = 50; 2.3%), failure to endorse willingness to
abstain from recreational drugs for 48 h prior to the testing
session (N = 369; 17.3%) or alcohol for 24 h prior to testing
(N = 11; 0.5%), significant uncorrected vision impairment
(N = 51; 2.4%), physical impairment in arms/hands (N = 4;
0.2%), or endorsing more than two items incorrectly on an
8-item Infrequency Scale (Jackson 1984; N = 16; 1.0%).
The remaining 1131 participants received an invitation to
participate in the lab-based portion of the task. However,
only a minority of those participants completed the lab ses-
sion (N = 79), as the online questionnaires were designed
for a larger study examining relationships between those
scales (currently unpublished). On the day of laboratory
testing, all participants denied recent nicotine use, alcohol
in past 24 h, and other recreational drug use in past 48 h.
However, this was based on self-report as we did not have
laboratory tests to confirm.
Of the 79 participants, 77 had valid data for the STT, as two
were excluded for sweetness ratings that did not approximate
a linear increase with increasing concentrations and were sta-
tistical outliers for that slope value (Z < −3.33 for each). An
additional 5 participants were excluded for missing all anhe-
donia measures and bio-demographic data (e.g., menstrual
phase) due to experimenter error (both a priori reasons for
excluding participants). This resulted in 72 participants used
in final analyses (76% women; mean age: 19.11; SD = 2.07;
range 18 to 28). For race, 75.0% identified as BWhite,^ 8.3%
as BBlack,^ 8.3% as BAsian, 4.2% as BOther,^ 2.8% as
BMixed,^ and one participant declined to answer.
Independent of race, 34.7% identified as BHispanic/
Latino(a).^ Two participants reported selective-serotonin re-
uptake inhibitor (SSRI) medication use at the time of testing.
The remaining participants denied current psychotropic and
narcotic medication use.
Measures
Self-Report Scales
The Reinforcement Sensitivity Theory of Personality
Questionnaire (RST-PQ; Corr and Cooper 2016) was admin-
istered during the online phase, which consists of 65 state-
ments with a response key asking the participant to rate how
much the statement describes them in general - ranging from
B1 - Not At All^ to B4 - Highly.^ The RST-PQ produces six
scales - four behavioral activation system subscales: Reward
Interest, Goal-Drive Persistence, Reward Reactivity, and
Impulsivity, along with scales for the Behavioral Inhibition
System (BIS) and Flight-Freeze- Avoidance System (FFAS).
The reliability and validity of the RST-PQ have received ad-
ditional recent support in chronic pain samples (Amiri et al.
2017), and through relationships found with event-related po-
tentials from electroencephalogram (Kaye et al. 2018; De
Pascalis et al. 2017). The Big Five Aspect Scales (BFAS;
DeYoung et al. 2007) was also administered online, and con-
sists of 100 statements with a response key asking the partic-
ipant to rate how accurately the statement reflects how they
generally are - ranging from B1 - Very Inaccurate^ to B5 - Very
Accurate.^ The BFAS results in the traditional big five factors
along with two aspects for each factor: Neuroticism
(Volatility, Withdrawal), Agreeableness (Compassion,
Politeness), Conscientiousness (Industriousness,
Orderliness), Extraversion (Enthusiasm, Assertiveness), and
Openness/Intellect (Openness, Intellect). The validity of
BFAS aspect scales have been supported with specific
J Psychopathol Behav Assess (2019) 41:235–248 237
relationships to psychopathology (Allen et al. 2018; Quilty
et al. 2014) and self-reported personality disorder symptoms
(DeYoung et al. 2016).
During the day of STTassessment, we administered three
anhedonia self-report scales - the 18-item Temporal
Experience of Pleasure Scales, which includes subscales
for anticipatory (TEPS-A) and consummatory (TEPS-C)
anhedonia (Gard et al. 2007), the 17-item Anticipatory and
Consummatory Interpersonal Pleasure Scale total score
(ACIPS; Gooding and Pflum 2014), and the 14-item
Snaith-Hamilton Pleasure Scale total score (SHAPS;
Snaith et al. 1995). Unlike the TEPS, factor analysis has
not supported separate factors for anticipatory and consum-
matory anhedonia on the ACIPS (Gooding and Pflum
2014). Increased anhedonia is indicated by lower scores
on the TEPS and ACIPS and higher scores on the SHAPS.
The Cronbach alphas for the anhedonia scales in our sample
were: TEPS-A (.76), TEPS-C (.75), ACIPS (.91), and
SHAPS (.59).
Sweet Taste Test (STT)
The STT is a standardized assessment of hedonic response to
sweet tastes in human participants (Kampov-Polevoy et al.
1997). Powdered sugar and distilled water were used to create
five concentrations of sucrose solutions (approximately
0.05 M, 0.10 M, 0.19 M, 0.42 M, and 0.86 M). As a reference,
Coca-Cola® is a 0.33 M solution. Participants received five
trials of each of the five solutions in a blinded random order.
During each trial, participants drank from a small plastic
opaque cup containing 2 mL of solution, swished the solution
in their mouth for about 5 s, and then expectorated into a
container. Participants were asked to mark an BX^ on a
201 mm analog scale line to indicate perceived sweetness,
from BNot sweet at all^ (left anchor) to BExtremely sweet^
(right anchor), and, on a separate scale, how much they liked it
from BDislike it very much^ (left anchor) to BLike it very
much^ (right anchor). Following the ratings, participants
rinsed with distilled water and proceeded to the next trial.
Ratings were then scored based on distance (in mm) of the
BX^ from the left side of the respective scale line, and then
averaged within the five concentrations.
We used the slope of hedonic ratings by increasing
sucrose concentration (i.e., hedonic slope) as the primary
measure of dimensional hedonic ratings and any statisti-
cally significant relationship with this dependent variable
was then further explored across the separate hedonic rat-
ings for the five concentrations. We also examined rela-
tionships with the categorical Bsweet liker^ status, defined
as having the highest hedonic rating for the sweetest
concentration (Kampov-Polevoy et al. 2001, 2003;
Wronski et al. 2007). Finally, we included the sweet sen-
sitivity slope to clarify specificity of any findings with the
hedonic variables. The hedonic and sweet sensitivity
slopes and the five individual concentration hedonic rat-
ing values all showed relatively normal distributions with
skewness and kurtosis <1.05.
Menstrual Cycle Stage Identification
We asked all female participants to identify the current
day in their menstrual cycle using day one defined by
the start of last menstruation. We also asked women if
they thought they may be pregnant, which all denied.
We created a Bluteal menstrual phase status^ variable
which classified women reporting being in the mid-to-
late luteal menstrual stage during the day of testing, de-
fined as days 20 to 28 from start of last mentruation, with
all remaining women and men combined in the second of
the two categories. Although reward processing studies
with rhesus macaques typically chose a late luteal window
around days 24 to 27 to optimally isolate the rise in pro-
gesterone (Carroll et al. 2016), we chose a wider mid-to-
late luteal phase window to allow for inherent error in
estimating the current day of cycle based on self-report/
memory, and to allow for inclusion of a sufficient number
of females in this subgroup for statistical analyses.
Similarly, we created a Bfollicular menstrual phase status^
variable which classified women being in the mid-to-late
follicular phase during the day of testing, which we de-
fined as days 5 to 12 following the onset of menstruation.
This window is also somewhat wider than the follicular
window typically used in the rhesus monkey studies (7 to
10 days following menstruation; Carroll et al. 2016),
based on the same rationale.
Procedures
This study was approved by the Institutional Review Board of
the authors’ university. Research followed ethical principles
described in the Declaration of Helsinki. Participants provided
informed consent at the beginning of both online and lab-
based sessions. During the lab-based session, participants
completed demographic information, including questions
about current medication and supplement use, the anhedonia
questionnaires, and the STT, as part of a larger study. No data
resulting from the larger study have been published at this
time.
238 J Psychopathol Behav Assess (2019) 41:235–248
Statistical Analyses
For self-report measures, one participant was missing the
TEPS and ACIPS. Across measures, if either one or two
items for a subscale/factor were skipped by a particular
participant, the respective score was then based on the
average of the remaining items for that factor. IBM
SPSS Statistics (Version 25) was used for all analyses.
Initial regressions examined relationships of the STT he-
donic and sweet sensitivity slopes and sweet liker catego-
ry with simultaneous entry of: age, sex, luteal menstrual
phase status, follicular menstrual phase status, birth con-
trol medication use, race, ethnicity, and time of day during
testing (variance inflation factor [VIF] < 1.60). Time of
day during testing was rounded to nearest hour (mode =
4:00 p.m.; range = 9:00 a.m. to 4:00 p.m.). This was ex-
amined as an approximation of fluctuations in the circa-
dian system (e.g., hormone levels), which can influence
behavioral responses to rewards (DePoy et al. 2017).
Additional regressions were used to examine sets of
predictors on each of the three STT variables, the: 1) four
anhedonia scales: TEPS-A, TEPS-C, ACIPS, and SHAPS,
2) six factor scores from the RST-PQ, 3) five BFAS fac-
tors, and 4) ten BFAS aspects. Simultaneous entry was
used for all predictor sets (VIF < 3.10 for all sets). If a
predictor showed a significant relationship with the he-
donic slope, this was followed by an exploratory analysis
of the five concentration hedonic ratings that comprised
the slope, using stepwise entry, regressed on the respec-
tive predictor. All regressions involving the sweet liker
category used binary logistic regressions, while the re-
maining analyses on dimensional STT variables used
linear regression. Across all regressions, there were no
statistical outliers, as defined by a Studentized residual
>3.0.
Results
See Fig. 1 for a depiction of the hedonic slope by sweet liker
status. Descriptive statistics and zero-correlations for STT var-
iables with each of the predictor sets can be found in Tables 1
(anhedonia scales), 2 (RST-PQ scales), 3 (BFAS factors), and
4 (BFAS aspects). Thirty participants (42%) were classified as
Bsweet likers.^ Excluding the two participants taking SSRI
medications did not change the pattern of significant findings
across all analyses. They were therefore retained in all results
presented below.
STT Relationships with Menstrual Phase Status,
Demographic Variables, and Time of Day
during Testing
We examined the three STT variables using regressions with
simultaneous entry of sex, luteal menstrual phase status (N =
13), follicular menstrual phase status (N = 13), birth control
medication use (N = 19), age, race, ethnicity, and time of day
during testing. As shown in Table 5, women in the mid-to-late
luteal menstrual phase had a smaller hedonic slope, driven by
lower hedonic ratings to the sweetest concentration (see
Fig. 2), and were less likely to be categorical sweet likers
(15%), as compared to the combination of the remaining
women and men (47.5%). Results also revealed that women
had a larger sweet sensitivity slope than men (see Table 5).
There were no other significant relationships across these re-
gressions. Therefore, we included covariates of luteal men-
strual phase status and sex in the first block of all remaining
regressions.
STT Relationships with Anhedonia
See Table 5 for summary of all findings, including unstan-
dardized beta values with standard error and level of statis-
tical significance. For anhedonia, the hedonic slope was
predicted by only the TEPS-C subscale, which was driven
by a positive relationship between the TEPS-C scores and
hedonic rating scores to the sweetest concentration (see
Fig. 3). Thus, individuals self-reporting more consummato-
ry anhedonia (i.e., a lower TEPS-C score) had a smaller
hedonic slope and rated the sweetest concentration as less
pleasant. The SST sweet sensitivity slope and sweet liker
status did not relate to anhedonia scales. Removal of the
Fig. 1 Hedonic slope lines from hedonic rating scores by
sucrose
concentration for sweet likers and sweet dislikers. Error bars
represent
the 95% confidence interval
J Psychopathol Behav Assess (2019) 41:235–248 239
SHAPS from the regressions, which had poor internal reli-
ability (α = .59), did not change the pattern of significance.
To examine specificity of consummatory vs. anticipa-
tory anxiety, we conducted regressions in which sex, lu-
teal phase status, and TEPS-A were entered in the first
block, followed by TEPS-C in the second block. TEPS-
C remained statistically significant for both hedonic slope,
unstandardized B = 36.81, SE = 18.15, p = .047, and the
hedonic rating to the sweetest concentration, unstandard-
ized B = 25.25, SE = 8.80, p = .006. As all four measures
of anhedonia were not significantly related to these STT
variables in zero-order correlations (see Table 1), we also
explored specificity of TEPS-C by entering each anhedo-
nia scale in individual regressions, after entering sex and
luteal phase status in block 1 – thus removing the effect of
multicollinearity among the anhedonia measures. These
analyses showed that of the four anhedonia scales, only
the TEPS-C related to the hedonic slope or hedonic rating
to the sweetest concentration.
STT Relationships with Personality Factors
For the RST-PQ, the hedonic slope was predicted by the
Flight-Freeze-Avoidance System (FFAS) score, which was
driven by a negative relationship between the FFAS score
and the hedonic rating to the least sweet concentration (see
Fig. 4). In addition, the sweet sensitivity slope was predict-
ed by RST-PQ Goal-Drive Persistence and BFAS
Extraversion and Conscientiousness factors (all in the pos-
itive direction). The BFAS aspect scales did not relate to any
STT variable when entered simultaneously in the
regressions.
Examination of Change in Results with Retention
of Excluded Participants
When repeating all analyses with inclusion of the seven
participants who were excluded (N = 79), the pattern of
significance across analyses did not change with two mi-
nor exceptions: For the RST-PQ FFFS, the hedonic slope
relationship changed to a trend level (p = .065), although
the FFFS relationship with the hedonic rating to the least
sweet concentration remained significant (p = .003). For
the BFAS factor relationships with the sweet sensitivity
slope, Conscientiousness remained significant (p = .04),
but Extraversion was no longer significant (p = .31). In
these analyses, all seven of the previously excluded par-
ticipants had RST-PQ and BFAS data, but five were miss-
ing all anhedonia measures and bio-demographic data
(e.g., menstrual phase), and two had sweet sensitivity
slopes >3.00 SD below the sample mean.Ta
b
le
1
D
es
cr
ip
ti
v
e
st
at
is
ti
cs
an
d
ze
ro
-o
rd
er
co
rr
el
at
io
n
s
am
o
n
g
an
h
ed
o
n
ia
sc
al
es
an
d
sw
ee
t
ta
st
e
te
st
v
ar
ia
b
le
s
1
2
3
4
5
6
7
8
9
1.
T
E
P
S
-C
on
su
m
m
at
or
y
S
ca
le
4.
66
(0
.7
5)
2.
T
E
P
S
-A
nt
ic
ip
at
or
y
S
ca
le
.3
6*
*
4.
80
(0
.6
1)
3.
A
C
IP
S
T
ot
al
S
co
re
.4
5*
*
*
.7
7*
*
*
5
.1
2
(0
.6
6)
4.
S
H
A
P
S
T
ot
al
S
co
re
−.
4
0*
*
*
−.
3
6*
*
−.
45
*
**
0
.9
4
(1
.3
4
)
5.
S
S
T
H
ed
on
ic
S
lo
pe
.1
5
.0
3
.0
6
−.
01
24
.3
8
(1
11
.6
8)
6.
S
S
T
C
at
eg
or
ic
al
S
w
ee
t
L
ik
er
^
.1
1
.1
3
.0
7
−.
11
.6
9
**
*
42
%
7.
S
S
T
H
ed
on
ic
R
at
in
g
to
S
w
ee
te
st
.2
0
−.
0
7
<
.0
01
−.
00
1
.9
0
**
*
.6
0*
*
*
10
4
.7
5
(5
5
.1
2
)
8.
S
S
T
H
ed
on
ic
R
at
in
g
to
L
ea
st
S
w
ee
t
−.
1
2
−.
1
9
−.
13
.1
0
−.
84
**
*
−.
6
1*
*
*
−.
56
**
*
89
.6
2
(5
1
.5
1
)
9.
S
S
T
S
w
ee
t
S
en
si
ti
vi
ty
S
lo
pe
.0
5
.1
9
.1
2
−.
21
−.
01
.0
2
.0
9
.1
2
18
6
.4
2
(4
0.
77
)
D
es
cr
ip
ti
v
e
st
at
is
ti
cs
o
n
th
e
o
u
te
r
d
ia
g
o
n
al
in
fo
rm
at
:
m
ea
n
(s
ta
nd
ar
d
d
ev
ia
ti
o
n
),
w
it
h
th
e
ex
ce
pt
io
n
o
f
a
p
er
ce
n
ta
g
e
fo
r
S
T
T
C
at
eg
o
ri
ca
l
S
w
ee
t
L
ik
er
;
re
m
ai
n
in
g
v
al
u
es
ar
e
P
ea
rs
o
n
’s
o
r
p
o
in
t-
b
is
er
ia
l
r
v
al
u
es
T
E
P
S
T
em
p
o
ra
l
E
x
p
er
ie
n
ce
o
f
P
le
as
u
re
S
ca
le
s,
A
C
IP
S
A
n
ti
ci
p
at
o
ry
an
d
C
on
su
m
m
at
o
ry
In
te
rp
er
so
n
al
P
le
as
u
re
S
ca
le
,
SH
A
P
S
S
n
ai
th
-H
am
il
to
n
P
le
as
u
re
S
ca
le
,
ST
T
S
w
ee
t
T
as
te
T
es
t
*
p
<
.0
5
,
*
*
p
<
.0
1
,
*
*
*
p
<
.0
0
1
^
D
ef
in
ed
b
y
h
ig
h
es
t
h
ed
on
ic
ra
ti
n
g
fo
r
sw
ee
te
st
co
n
ce
n
tr
at
io
n
240 J Psychopathol Behav Assess (2019) 41:235–248
Ta
b
le
2
D
es
cr
ip
ti
v
e
st
at
is
ti
cs
an
d
ze
ro
-o
rd
er
co
rr
el
at
io
n
s
am
o
n
g
R
ei
n
fo
rc
em
en
t
S
en
si
ti
v
it
y
P
er
so
n
al
it
y
Q
u
es
ti
o
n
n
ai
re
S
ca
le
sc
o
re
s
an
d
sw
ee
t
ta
st
e
te
st
v
ar
ia
b
le
s
1
2
3
4
5
6
1
.
R
S
T
-P
Q
R
ew
ar
d
In
te
re
st
2
.8
3
(0
.6
6
)
2
.
R
S
T
-P
Q
R
ew
ar
d
R
ea
ct
iv
it
y
.5
6
*
**
2
.9
7
(0
.4
8
)
3
.
R
S
T
-P
Q
G
o
al
-D
ri
v
e
P
er
si
st
en
ce
.5
1
*
**
.2
6
*
3
.4
8
(0
.4
6
)
4
.
R
S
T
-P
Q
Im
p
u
ls
iv
it
y
.4
9
*
**
.5
8
*
*
*
.1
8
2
.2
2
(0
.6
0
)
5
.
R
S
T
-P
Q
B
eh
av
io
ra
l
In
h
ib
it
io
n
S
y
st
em
−.
0
9
.2
5
*
−.
0
7
.2
3
2
.5
0
(0
.6
8
)
6
.
R
S
T
-P
Q
F
li
g
h
t-
F
re
ez
e-
A
v
o
id
an
ce
S
y
st
em
−.
0
7
.1
1
.1
3
.1
0
.5
4
*
*
*
2
.5
2
(0
.6
6
)
7
.
S
S
T
H
ed
o
n
ic
S
lo
p
e
.0
1
.1
8
.1
1
.0
8
−.
0
3
.1
6
8
.
S
S
T
C
at
eg
o
ri
ca
l
S
w
ee
t
L
ik
er
−.
0
4
.1
2
.0
2
.1
1
.1
4
.2
5
*
9
.
S
S
T
H
ed
o
n
ic
R
at
in
g
to
S
w
ee
te
st
.0
3
.1
4
.1
7
.0
7
−.
0
9
.0
2
1
0
.
S
S
T
H
ed
o
n
ic
R
at
in
g
to
L
ea
st
S
w
ee
t
−.
0
2
−.
1
7
−.
0
3
−.
06
−.
02
−.
2
7
*
11
.
S
S
T
S
w
ee
t
S
en
si
ti
v
it
y
S
lo
p
e
.1
4
.0
7
.4
3
*
*
*
.0
0
4
.0
1
−.
0
0
3
D
es
cr
ip
ti
v
e
st
at
is
ti
cs
o
n
th
e
o
u
te
r
d
ia
g
o
n
al
in
fo
rm
at
:
m
ea
n
(s
ta
n
d
ar
d
de
v
ia
ti
o
n
);
re
m
ai
n
in
g
v
al
u
es
ar
e
P
ea
rs
o
n’
s
o
r
po
in
t-
b
is
er
ia
l
r
v
al
u
es
D
es
cr
ip
ti
v
e
st
at
is
ti
cs
fo
r
th
e
R
S
T
-P
Q
ar
e
fo
r
th
e
av
er
ag
e
it
em
-l
ev
el
sc
o
re
in
a
g
iv
en
fa
ct
or
(r
an
g
in
g
fr
o
m
1
to
4
)
In
te
rc
o
rr
el
at
io
n
s
am
o
n
g
S
S
T
v
ar
ia
b
le
s
d
ep
ic
te
d
in
T
ab
le
1
R
ST
-P
Q
R
ei
n
fo
rc
em
en
t
S
en
si
ti
v
it
y
T
h
eo
ry
P
er
so
n
al
it
y
Q
u
es
ti
o
n
n
ai
re
,
ST
T
S
w
ee
t
T
as
te
T
es
t
*
p
<
.0
5
,
*
*
p
<
.0
1
,
*
*
*
p
<
.0
0
1
^
D
ef
in
ed
b
y
h
ig
h
es
t
h
ed
on
ic
ra
ti
n
g
fo
r
sw
ee
te
st
co
n
ce
n
tr
at
io
n
J Psychopathol Behav Assess (2019) 41:235–248 241
Discussion
As hypothesized, participants who self-reported more con-
summatory anhedonia (i.e., lower TEPS-C scores) had a re-
duced hedonic slope that was driven by a reduction in the
hedonic rating for the sweetest solution (see Fig. 3). This find-
ing was specific to consummatory anhedonia as the TEPS-C
continued to show these significant relationships after entering
TEPS-A in the first block of the regression. In addition, indi-
vidual exploratory regressions with each of the other anhedo-
nia measures were not significant. This finding with the TEPS-
C is broadly consistent with a previous study which found
reduced hedonic ratings to the STT in a transdiagnostic psy-
chiatric sample related to increased anhedonia from the
Chapman Physical Anhedonia Scale, although that study fo-
cused on the maximum hedonic rating across all concentra-
tions (Berlin et al. 1998). Physical anhedonia is lack of plea-
sure from physical sensations, which partially overlaps the
TEPS as it assesses lack of pleasure across a wider variety of
stimuli. The current finding extends this earlier work by find-
ing the relationship in a nonpsychiatric sample, and supports
the current inclusion of the TEPS-C and Btaste reactivity^ in
the RDoC IRR domain. As both the TEPS-A and the ACIPS
contain items regarding anticipatory anhedonia, the specificity
of our finding to the TEPS-C suggests that questions regarding
consummatory pleasure more precisely reflect IRR. Although
the SHAPS questions also reflect consummatory pleasure, the
directions to the SHAPS asks participants to answer based on
the Blast few days,^ while the TEPS-C asks participants to
answer how they feel Bin general.^ Therefore, it may be the
more trait-like aspects of consummatory pleasure that correlate
with individual differences in IRR.
Also consistent with our hypotheses, women who report-
ed being in the mid-to-late luteal menstrual cycle phase
(days 20 to 28), had a lower hedonic slope, driven by lower
hedonic ratings for the sweetest concentration (see Fig. 2),
and were less likely to be categorical sweet likers, as com-
pared to the remaining women and men (see Table 5). This
finding is similar to the one other study that examined this
relationship (Elliott et al. 2015), but extends their limited
methodology of using one sample of four sucrose solutions
and no male participants, to the use of the standardized and
validated STT and a sample containing both women and
men. These findings provide further support for the STT
representing IRR, as earlier work has found decreased he-
donic ratings following smoked cocaine in women during
the luteal, compared with follicular, phase (Evans et al.
2002; Sofuoglu et al. 1999). We did not find any STT rela-
tionships with the follicular menstrual phase (days 5–12),
consistent with a report that exogenously-administered es-
tradiol, which is naturally higher in the follicular phase, did
not affect self-administration of cocaine in either intact or
ovariectomized rhesus monkeys (Mello et al. 2008).
Our hypotheses regarding the relationship between STT
hedonic ratings and personality traits of neuroticism and im-
pulsivity were not supported, as there were no statistically
significant relationships between STT hedonic ratings and
BFAS Neuroticism and RST-PQ Impulsivity factor scores.
While this is somewhat inconsistent with the existing research
on sweet tastes and personality traits, none of the previous
Table 3 Descriptive statistics and
zero-order correlations among
Big Five Aspect Scale factor
scores and sweet taste test
variables
1 2 3 4 5
1. BFAS Neuroticism Factor 2.78 (0.64)
2. BFAS Agreeableness Factor .01 4.07 (0.47)
3. BFAS Conscientiousness Factor −.37** .23 3.61 (0.52)
4. BFAS Extraversion Factor −.28* .29* .30* 3.47 (0.62)
5. BFAS Openness/Intellect Factor −.25* .27* .32** .56***
3.75 (0.48)
6. SST Hedonic Slope −.05 .07 .08 .12 .21
7. SST Categorical Sweet Liker^ .16 .21 −.03 .01 .11
8. SST Hedonic Rating to Sweetest −.05 −.01 .07 .15 .25*
9. SST Hedonic Rating to Least Sweet .06 −.17 −.09 −.09 −.13
10. SST Sweet Sensitivity Slope .10 .13 .32** .30* .21
Descriptive statistics on the outer diagonal in format: mean
(standard deviation); remaining values are Pearson’s or
point-biserial r values
Descriptive statistics for the BFAS are for the average item-
level score in a given factor (ranging from 1 to 5)
Intercorrelations among SST variables depicted in Table 1
BFAS Big Five Aspects Scale, STT Sweet Taste Test
* p < .05, ** p < .01, *** p < .001
^ Defined by highest hedonic rating for sweetest concentration
242 J Psychopathol Behav Assess (2019) 41:235–248
Ta
b
le
4
D
es
cr
ip
ti
v
e
st
at
is
ti
cs
an
d
ze
ro
-o
rd
er
co
rr
el
at
io
ns
am
on
g
B
ig
F
iv
e
A
sp
ec
t
S
ca
le
as
pe
ct
sc
or
es
an
d
sw
ee
t
ta
st
e
te
st
v
ar
ia
bl
es
1
2
3
4
5
6
7
8
9
1
0
1
.
B
FA
S
V
o
la
ti
li
ty
A
sp
ec
t
2
.6
4
(0
.7
0
)
2
.
B
FA
S
W
it
h
d
ra
w
al
A
sp
ec
t
.6
2
*
*
*
2
.9
4
(0
.7
1
)
3
.
B
FA
S
C
o
m
p
as
si
o
n
A
sp
ec
t
−.
01
.0
1
4
.1
1
(0
.5
9
)
4
.
B
FA
S
P
o
li
te
ne
ss
A
sp
ec
t
−.
20
.1
2
.5
5
*
*
*
4
.0
3
(0
.4
6
)
5
.
B
FA
S
In
du
st
ri
o
u
sn
es
s
A
sp
ec
t
−.
45
*
*
*
−.
5
1
*
*
*
.1
2
.1
8
3
.4
6
(0
.6
6
)
6
.
B
FA
S
O
rd
er
li
n
es
s
A
sp
ec
t
−.
02
−.
0
7
.2
0
.2
2
.4
7
*
*
*
3
.7
7
(0
.5
4
)
7
.
B
FA
S
E
n
th
u
si
as
m
A
sp
ec
t
−.
02
−.
2
5
*
.5
0
*
*
*
.2
2
.2
2
.0
9
3
.6
6
(0
.7
0
)
8
.
B
FA
S
A
ss
er
ti
v
en
es
s
A
sp
ec
t
−.
01
−.
3
4
*
*
.3
1
*
*
−.
2
6*
.3
5
*
*
.0
9
.5
0
*
*
*
3
.3
1
(0
.7
1
)
9
.
B
FA
S
O
p
en
n
es
s
A
sp
ec
t
−.
10
−.
0
0
3
.3
5
*
*
.0
9
.0
5
.1
6
.2
4
*
.1
7
3
.8
1
(0
.6
1
)
1
0
.
B
FA
S
In
te
ll
ec
t
A
sp
ec
t
−.
18
−.
4
1
*
*
*
.2
9
*
−.
1
3
.5
0
*
*
*
.1
4
.4
5
*
*
*
.6
6
*
*
*
.1
9
3
.6
7
(0
.6
2
)
11
.
S
S
T
H
ed
on
ic
S
lo
p
e
−.
00
1
−.
0
9
.0
5
.0
8
.0
7
.0
6
.1
2
.1
2
.2
0
.1
1
1
2
.
S
S
T
C
at
eg
o
ri
ca
l
S
w
ee
t
L
ik
er
.1
3
.1
4
.1
8
.1
9
−.
0
8
.0
3
.0
3
.0
4
.1
2
.0
2
1
3
.
S
S
T
H
ed
o
n
ic
R
at
in
g
to
S
w
ee
te
st
.0
0
4
−.
0
6
.0
3
−.
0
6
.0
6
.0
5
.0
9
.2
0
.1
6
.2
0
1
4
.
S
S
T
H
ed
o
n
ic
R
at
in
g
to
L
ea
st
S
w
ee
t
.0
4
.1
4
−.
0
5
−.
2
8*
−.
1
0
−.
0
5
−.
14
−.
02
−.
2
1
.0
0
3
1
5
.
S
S
T
S
w
ee
t
S
en
si
ti
v
it
y
S
lo
p
e
.1
3
.0
7
.1
8
.0
4
.2
5
*
.3
0
*
.2
3
.3
1
*
*
.2
0
.1
3
D
es
cr
ip
ti
v
e
st
at
is
ti
cs
o
n
th
e
o
u
te
r
d
ia
g
o
n
al
in
fo
rm
at
:
m
ea
n
(s
ta
n
d
ar
d
de
v
ia
ti
o
n
);
re
m
ai
n
in
g
v
al
u
es
ar
e
P
ea
rs
o
n’
s
o
r
po
in
t-
b
is
er
ia
l
r
v
al
u
es
D
es
cr
ip
ti
v
e
st
at
is
ti
cs
fo
r
th
e
B
FA
S
ar
e
fo
r
th
e
av
er
ag
e
it
em
-l
ev
el
sc
o
re
in
a
g
iv
en
as
p
ec
t
(r
an
g
in
g
fr
o
m
1
to
5
)
In
te
rc
o
rr
el
at
io
n
s
am
o
n
g
S
S
T
v
ar
ia
b
le
s
d
ep
ic
te
d
in
T
ab
le
1
B
FA
S
B
ig
F
iv
e
A
sp
ec
ts
S
ca
le
,
ST
T
S
w
ee
t
T
as
te
T
es
t
*
p
<
.0
5
,
*
*
p
<
.0
1
,
*
*
*
p
<
.0
0
1
^
D
ef
in
ed
b
y
h
ig
h
es
t
h
ed
on
ic
ra
ti
n
g
fo
r
sw
ee
te
st
co
n
ce
n
tr
at
io
n
J Psychopathol Behav Assess (2019) 41:235–248 243
Ta
b
le
5
R
es
u
lt
s
of
li
n
ea
r
an
d
lo
g
is
ti
c
re
g
re
ss
io
n
an
al
ys
es
S
T
T
D
ep
en
de
nt
V
ar
ia
b
le
s
S
ex
an
d
L
ut
ea
l
M
en
st
ru
al
P
h
as
e
S
ta
tu
s
P
re
d
ic
to
rs
#
A
n
h
ed
o
ni
a
P
re
d
ic
to
rs
#
#
R
S
T
-P
Q
P
re
d
ic
to
rs
#
#
B
FA
S
F
iv
e
F
ac
to
r
P
re
d
ic
to
rs
#
#
H
ed
o
n
ic
S
lo
p
e
L
u
te
al
(l
o
w
er
th
an
o
th
er
s)
T
E
P
S
-C
F
FA
S
N
o
n
e
B
=
−
8
0
.3
2
,
S
E
=
3
6
.5
0
*
B
=
4
4
.1
1
,
S
E
=
1
9
.9
1
*
B
=
4
7
.7
4
,
S
E
=
2
3
.1
5
*
S
u
cr
o
se
C
o
n
ce
n
tr
at
io
n
P
o
st
-H
o
c
T
es
ts
^
0
.8
6
M
0.
8
6
M
0
.0
5
M
N
A
B
=
−
0
.0
0
2
,
S
E
=
0
.0
0
1
*
B
=
0
.0
0
4
,
S
E
=
0
.0
0
2
*
*
B
=
−0
.0
0
4
,
S
E
=
0
.0
0
1
*
*
S
w
ee
t
L
ik
er
C
at
eg
o
ry
^^
L
u
te
al
(l
es
s
li
k
el
y
th
an
o
th
er
s)
N
o
n
e
N
o
n
e
N
o
n
e
B
=
−2
.0
6
,
S
E
=
0
.9
2
*
S
w
ee
t
S
en
si
ti
v
it
y
S
lo
p
e
S
ex
(w
o
m
en
>
m
en
)
N
o
n
e
G
D
P
E
x
tr
av
er
si
on
B
=
3
9
.5
9
,
S
E
=
1
2
.4
9
*
*
B
=
3
8
.1
8
,
S
E
=
11
.0
8
*
*
*
B
=
1
8
.8
3
,
S
E
=
8
.6
8
*
C
o
ns
ci
en
ti
ou
sn
es
s
B
=
2
2
.0
3
,
S
E
=
9
.8
0
*
F
o
r
co
n
si
st
en
cy
,
al
l
b
et
a
v
al
ue
s
ar
e
u
n
st
an
d
ar
d
iz
ed
(B
)
d
u
e
to
in
cl
u
si
o
n
o
f
b
o
th
ca
te
g
o
ri
ca
l
an
d
d
im
en
si
o
n
al
pr
ed
ic
to
rs
an
d
d
ep
en
d
en
t
va
ri
ab
le
s
N
o
st
at
is
ti
ca
ll
y
si
g
n
if
ic
an
t
re
la
ti
o
n
sh
ip
s
w
er
e
fo
u
n
d
w
h
en
si
m
u
lt
an
eo
u
s
en
te
ri
ng
th
e
te
n
B
FA
S
as
p
ec
t
sc
al
es
w
it
h
an
y
S
T
T
d
ep
en
d
en
t
v
ar
ia
b
le
ST
T
S
w
ee
tt
as
te
te
st
,M
M
o
la
ri
ty
o
f
co
n
ce
nt
ra
ti
o
n
,T
E
P
S-
C
T
em
p
o
ra
lE
x
p
er
ie
n
ce
o
f
P
le
as
u
re
S
ca
le
s
–
C
o
n
su
m
m
at
o
ry
,R
ST
-P
Q
R
ei
n
fo
rc
em
en
tS
en
si
ti
v
it
y
T
h
eo
ry
P
er
so
n
al
it
y
Q
u
es
ti
o
n
n
ai
re
,F
FA
S
F
li
g
h
t-
F
re
ez
e-
A
v
o
id
an
ce
S
y
st
em
,
G
D
P
G
oa
l-
D
ri
ve
P
er
si
st
en
ce
,
B
FA
S
B
ig
F
iv
e
A
sp
ec
ts
S
ca
le
*
p
<
.0
5
,
*
*
p
<
.0
1
,
*
*
*
p
<
.0
0
1
#
S
im
u
lt
an
eo
u
sl
y
en
te
re
d
w
it
h
ag
e,
u
se
o
f
b
ir
th
co
n
tr
ol
m
ed
ic
at
io
n
,
ra
ce
,
et
h
n
ic
it
y,
an
d
ti
m
e
o
f
d
ay
d
u
ri
n
g
te
st
in
g
#
#
C
o
v
ar
ie
d
fo
r
se
x
an
d
lu
te
al
p
h
as
e
st
at
u
s,
al
l
p
re
d
ic
to
rs
en
te
re
d
si
m
ul
ta
n
eo
u
sl
y
^
If
a
v
ar
ia
b
le
w
as
si
g
n
if
ic
an
tl
y
re
la
te
d
to
h
ed
o
n
ic
sl
op
e,
th
e
fi
v
e
h
ed
o
n
ic
ra
ti
n
g
s
o
f
th
e
in
di
v
id
u
al
co
n
ce
n
tr
at
io
n
s
w
er
e
en
te
re
d
in
a
st
ep
w
is
e
m
an
n
er
o
n
th
e
v
ar
ia
b
le
re
la
te
d
to
h
ed
o
n
ic
sl
o
p
e,
co
v
ar
y
in
g
fo
r
se
x
an
d
lu
te
al
p
h
as
e
st
at
us
^^
D
ef
in
ed
b
y
h
ig
h
es
t
h
ed
o
n
ic
ra
ti
n
g
fo
r
sw
ee
te
st
co
n
ce
n
tr
at
io
n
;
u
se
d
bi
n
o
m
ia
l
lo
g
is
ti
c
re
g
re
ss
io
n
s,
al
l
p
re
d
ic
to
rs
en
te
re
d
si
m
u
lt
an
eo
u
sl
y
244 J Psychopathol Behav Assess (2019) 41:235–248
studies used the STT or controlled for sex/luteal menstrual
phase, but instead used behavioral tests involving varying
sweetness levels of wine (Saliba et al. 2009; Sena-Esteves
et al. 2018) or general self-reported tendencies to like sweet
food and beverages (Kikuchi and Watanabe 2000; Elfhag and
Erlanson-Albertsson 2006).
We found one unexpected personality trait predicted STT
hedonic ratings, a positive relationship between the hedonic
slope with RST-PQ FFAS factor scores, driven by a negative
relationship between the hedonic rating to the least sweet
concentration and the FFAS score (see Fig. 4). The Flight-
Freeze-Avoidance System (FFAS) factor from the RST-PQ
is intended to represent a punishment sensitivity system
which controls fear-related behaviors to avoidable aversive
stimuli, such as fleeing, active avoidance, and freezing
(Corr and Cooper 2016). While speculative, our finding is
similar to a report that individuals with a family history of
alcoholism had greater amygdala fMRI activation after tast-
ing a sucrose solution than those without a family history,
which was significant for the low (0.10 M), but not high
(0.83 M), concentration (Eiler et al. 2017). As the amygdala
is a primary region for processing fear (Davis and Reijmers
2018), and fear is reflected by the FFAS, this specificity of
amygdala activation to the least sweet solution is broadly
similar to our finding that individuals with greater FFAS had
lower ratings to the least sweet solution.
We found exploratory personality traits that predicted the
sweet sensitivity slope, which is thought to reflect gustatory
sensory processes rather than IRR (Eikemo et al. 2016). These
included positive relationships with RST-PQ Goal-Drive
Persistence and BFAS Extraversion and Conscientiousness
factor scores. Interestingly, the BFAS aspect scales constitut-
ing the Extraversion and Conscientiousness factors did not
relate to the sweet sensitivity slope. This may be due to the
two aspects of a given factor showing similar relationships
and the resulting shared variance being removed due to use
of simultaneous entry in the regressions. From inspection of
the zero-order correlations on Table 4, both aspects of
Conscientiousness (i.e., Industriousness and Orderliness)
showed significant positive relationships with the sweet sen-
sitivity slope. For aspects of Extraversion, Assertiveness was
statistically significant (p = .007), while Enthusiasm was a
similar effect size but at a statistical trend level (p = .055). It
is possible that individuals higher in conscientiousness and/or
goal drive persistence were more thoughtful and reliable in
recording the sweetness level which then resulted in a stronger
slope value. Similarly, it is possible that individual differences
Fig. 2 Hedonic slope lines from hedonic rating scores by
sucrose
concentration for women in the mid-to-late luteal menstrual
phase and
the combination of the remaining women and men. Error bars
represent
the 95% confidence interval
Fig. 3 Scatterplot of the
consummatory subscale score
from the temporal experience of
pleasure scales (unstandardized
residuals after covarying for sex
and luteal phase) by the hedonic
rating scores for the sweet
concentration. Shaded region
represents 95% confidence
interval
J Psychopathol Behav Assess (2019) 41:235–248 245
in brain mechanisms related to extraversion also confer in-
creased gustatory discrimination of sweetness concentrations.
As these were exploratory findings, replication is needed be-
fore such speculation can be translated into detailed theory.
The study is limited by a moderate sample size, use of
undergraduate students as participants, and a larger number
of women than men. We also did not ask participants about
self-reported hunger or time since last meal, which can affect
hedonic responses to sweet stimuli (Cabanac and Lafrance
1990). Other limitations are inherent in the pre-existing stan-
dardized STT methods, such as the lack of a control solution
(e.g., water with no sucrose) and use of analog scales that
introduce confounds instead of a scale such as the generalized
Labeled Magnitude Scale (Bartoshuk et al. 2004).
Additionally, it would be optimal to measure the same women
longitudinally across their menstrual phases to avoid potential
confounds. However, a strength is assessing and accounting
for female menstrual phase status in the analyses. The study
provides what appears to be the first evidence that the STT,
particularly hedonic ratings of the sweetest concentration, are
reduced in individuals with elevated consummatory anhedo-
nia and in women who are in the mid-to-late luteal phase of
their menstrual cycle. If replicated, the current results and the
recent finding regarding μ-opioid specificity (Eikemo et al.
2016), suggest that Btaste reactivity,^ currently listed under
the RDoC IRR BBehaviors,^ be replaced with a more specific
inclusion of the hedonic rating to the sweetest concentration
from the STT under BParadigms.^ Our finding also supports
the validity of the TEPS-C as an index of individual differ-
ences in consummatory anhedonia/IRR, which, given that it is
a brief self-report scale, has considerable translational value
for clinical and research applications. Similarly, our findings
suggest that future research on IRR/consummatory anhedonia
account for the potential influence of menstrual cycle phase in
female participants.
Acknowledgments The authors would like to thank the
following research
assistants for their help on this project: Ashley Lee Lum,
Pamela Askins,
Doniya Milani, Jasmine Montero, Nicholas Picanso, and Brooke
Fenton.
Compliance with Ethical Standards
Conflict of Interest Jeffrey S. Bedwell, Christopher C. Spencer,
Cristina
A. Chirino, and John P. O’Donnell report no conflicts of
interest related to
this manuscript and study.
Experiment Participants Research followed ethical principles
described
in the Declaration of Helsinki.
Ethical Approval This study was approved by the Institutional
Review
Board of the University of Central Florida (IRB #: SBE-16-
12,678).
Informed Consent All participicipants provided informed
consent for
both the online and laboratory portions of this study.
Publisher’s Note Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to
jurisdic-
tional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
References
Allen, T. A., Carey, B. E., McBride, C., Bagby, R. M.,
DeYoung, C. G., &
Quilty, L. C. (2018). Big Five aspects of personality interact to
predict depression. Journal of Personality, 86, 714–725. https://
doi.org/10.1111/jopy.12352.
Fig. 4 Scatterplot of the flight-
freeze-avoidance system factor
score from the reinforcement
sensitivity theory personality
questionnaire (unstandardized re-
siduals after covarying for sex and
luteal phase) by the hedonic rating
scores for the least sweet concen-
tration. Shaded region represents
95% confidence interval
246 J Psychopathol Behav Assess (2019) 41:235–248
https://doi.org/10.1111/jopy.12352
https://doi.org/10.1111/jopy.12352
American Psychiatric Association. (2013). Diagnostic and
statistical
manual of mental disorders; DSM-5. Washington, D.C.:
American
Psychiatric Association.
Amiri, S., Behnezhad, S., & Azad-Marzabadi, E. (2017).
Psychometric
properties revised reinforcement sensitivity theory (r-RST)
scale in
chronic pain patients. Journal of Pain Research, 10, 1879–1885.
https://doi.org/10.2147/jpr.s132133.
Arbisi, P. A., Billington, C. J., & Levine, A. S. (1999). The
effect of
naltrexone on taste detection and recognition threshold.
Appetite,
32(2), 241–249. https://doi.org/10.1006/appe.1998.0217.
Ashton, M. C., Pilkington, A. C., & Lee, K. (2014). Do
prosocial people
prefer sweet-tasting foods? An attempted replication of Meier,
Moeller, Riemer-Peltz, and Robinson (2012). Journal of
Research
in Personality, 52, 42–46.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jrp.2014.06.006.
Bartoshuk, L. M., Duffy, V. B., Green, B. G., Hoffman, H. J.,
Ko, C. W.,
Lucchina, L. A., Marks, L. E., Snyder, D. J., & Weiffenbach, J.
M.
(2004). Valid across-group comparisons with labeled scales:
The
gLMS versus magnitude matching. Physiology & Behavior,
82(1),
109–114.
Berlin, I., Givry-Steiner, L., Lecrubier, Y., & Puech, A. J.
(1998).
Measures of anhedonia and hedonic responses to sucrose in
depres-
sive and schizophrenic patients in comparison with healthy
subjects.
European Psychiatry, 13(6), 303–309. https://doi.org/10.1016/
s0924-9338(98)80048-5.
Bowen, D. J., & Grunberg, N. E. (1990). Variations in food
preference
and consumption across the menstrual cycle. Physiology &
Behavior, 47(2), 287–291. https://doi.org/10.1016/0031-
9384(90)
90144-S.
Cabanac, M., & Lafrance, L. (1990). Postingestive alliesthesia:
The rat
tells the same story. Physiology & Behavior, 47(3), 539–543.
Carroll, M. E., Collins, M., Kohl, E. A., Johnson, S., & Dougen,
B.
(2016). Sex and menstrual cycle effects on chronic oral cocaine
self-administration in rhesus monkeys: Effects of a nondrug
alterna-
tive reward (journal article). Psychopharmacology, 233(15),
2973–
2984. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00213-016-4343-5.
Castro, D. C., & Berridge, K. C. (2017). Opioid and orexin
hedonic
hotspots in rat orbitofrontal cortex and insula. Proceedings of
the
National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America,
114(43), E9125–e9134.
https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1705753114.
Corr, P. J., & Cooper, A. J. (2016). The reinforcement
sensitivity theory of
personality questionnaire (RST-PQ): Development and
validation.
Psychological Assessment, 28(11), 1427–1440.
https://doi.org/10.
1037/pas0000273.
Davis, P., & Reijmers, L. G. (2018). The dynamic nature of fear
engrams
in the basolateral amygdala. Brain Research Bulletin, 141, 44–
49.
De Pascalis, V., Fracasso, F., & Corr, P. J. (2017). The
behavioral ap-
proach system and augmenting/reducing in auditory event-
related
potentials during emotional visual stimulation. Biological
Psychology, 123, 310–323. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsycho.
2016.10.015.
DePoy, L. M., McClung, C. A., & Logan, R. W. (2017). Neural
mecha-
nisms of circadian regulation of natural and drug reward. Neural
Plasticity, 2017, 5720842.
https://doi.org/10.1155/2017/5720842.
DeYoung, C. G., Quilty, L. C., & Peterson, J. B. (2007).
Between facets
and domains: 10 aspects of the big five. Journal of Personality
and
Social Psychology, 93(5), 880–896.
https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-
3514.93.5.880.
DeYoung, C. G., Carey, B. E., Krueger, R. F., & Ross, S. R.
(2016). Ten
aspects of the big five in the personality inventory for DSM-5.
Personal Disorder, 7(2), 113–123. https://doi.org/10.1037/
per0000170.
Dichter, G. S., Smoski, M. J., Kampov-Polevoy, A. B., Gallop,
R., &
Garbutt, J. C. (2010). Unipolar depression does not moderate re-
sponses to the sweet taste test. Depression and Anxiety, 27(9),
859–863. https://doi.org/10.1002/da.20690.
Eikemo, M., Loseth, G. E., Johnstone, T., Gjerstad, J., Willoch,
F., &
Leknes, S. (2016). Sweet taste pleasantness is modulated by
mor-
phine and naltrexone. Psychopharmacology, 233(21–22), 3711–
3723. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00213-016-4403-x.
Eiler, W. J. A., Dzemidzic, M., Soeurt, C. M., Carron, C. R.,
Oberline, B.
G., Considine, R. V., Harezlak, J., & Kareken, D. A. (2017).
Family
history of alcoholism and the human brain response to oral
sucrose.
Neuroimage Clinical, 17, 1036–1046.
Elfhag, K., & Erlanson-Albertsson, C. (2006). Sweet and fat
taste prefer-
ence in obesity have different associations with personality and
eat-
ing behavior. Physiology & Behavior, 88(1), 61–66.
https://doi.org/
10.1016/j.physbeh.2006.03.006.
Elliott, S. A., Ng, J., Leow, M. K., & Henry, C. J. (2015). The
influence of
the menstrual cycle on energy balance and taste preference in
Asian
Chinese women. European Journal of Nutrition, 54(8), 1323–
1332.
https://doi.org/10.1007/s00394-014-0812-y.
Evans, S. M., & Foltin, R. W. (2006). Exogenous progesterone
attenuates
the subjective effects of smoked cocaine in women, but not in
men.
Neuropsychopharmacology, 31(3), 659–674. https://doi.org/10.
1038/sj.npp.1300887.
Evans, S. M., Haney, M., & Foltin, R. W. (2002). The effects of
smoked
cocaine during the follicular and luteal phases of the menstrual
cycle
in women. Psychopharmacology, 159(4), 397–406.
https://doi.org/
10.1007/s00213-001-0944-7.
Garbutt, J. C., Osborne, M., Gallop, R., Barkenbus, J., Grace,
K., Cody,
M., Flannery, B., & Kampov-Polevoy, A. B. (2009). Sweet
liking
phenotype, alcohol craving and response to naltrexone treatment
in
alcohol dependence. Alcohol and Alcoholism, 44(3), 293–300.
https://doi.org/10.1093/alcalc/agn122.
Garbutt, J. C., Kampov-Polevoy, A. B., Kalka-Juhl, L. S., &
Gallop, R. J.
(2016). Association of the Sweet-Liking Phenotype and Craving
for
alcohol with the response to naltrexone treatment in alcohol
depen-
dence: A randomized clinical trial. JAMA Psychiatry, 73(10),
1056–
1063. https://doi.org/10.1001/jamapsychiatry.2016.2157.
Gard, D. E., Kring, A. M., Gard, M. G., Horan, W. P., & Green,
M. F.
(2007). Anhedonia in schizophrenia: Distinctions between
anticipa-
tory and consummatory pleasure. Schizophrenia Research,
93(1–3),
253–260. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.schres.2007.03.008.
Gooding, D. C., & Pflum, M. J. (2014). The assessment of
interpersonal
pleasure: Introduction of the anticipatory and Consummatory
inter-
personal pleasure scale (ACIPS) and preliminary findings.
Psychiatry Research, 215(1), 237–243. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.
psychres.2013.10.012.
Goodman, E. L., Breithaupt, L., Watson, H. J., Peat, C. M.,
Baker, J. H.,
Bulik, C. M., & Brownley, K. A. (2017). Sweet taste preference
in
binge-eating disorder: A preliminary investigation. Eating
Behaviors, 28, 8–15.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.eatbeh.2017.11.005.
Insel, T. R., & Cuthbert, B. N. (2015). Medicine. Brain
disorders?
Precisely. Science, 348, 499–500.
Insel, T., Cuthbert, B., Garvey, M., Heinssen, R., Pine, D. S.,
Quinn, K.,
Sanislow, C., & Wang, P. (2010). Research domain criteria
(RDoC):
Toward a new classification framework for research on mental
dis-
orders. The American Journal of Psychiatry, 167(7), 748–751.
https://doi.org/10.1176/appi.ajp.2010.09091379.
Jackson, D. N. (1984). Personality Research Form Manual. Port
Heron:
Research Psychologists Press.
J Psychopathol Behav Assess (2019) 41:235–248 247
https://doi.org/10.2147/jpr.s132133
https://doi.org/10.1006/appe.1998.0217
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jrp.2014.06.006
https://doi.org/10.1016/s0924-9338(98)80048-5
https://doi.org/10.1016/s0924-9338(98)80048-5
https://doi.org/10.1016/0031-9384(90)90144-S
https://doi.org/10.1016/0031-9384(90)90144-S
https://doi.org/10.1007/s00213-016-4343-5.
https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1705753114
https://doi.org/10.1037/pas0000273
https://doi.org/10.1037/pas0000273
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsycho.2016.10.015
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsycho.2016.10.015
https://doi.org/10.1155/2017/5720842
https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.93.5.880
https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.93.5.880
https://doi.org/10.1037/per0000170
https://doi.org/10.1037/per0000170
https://doi.org/10.1002/da.20690
https://doi.org/10.1007/s00213-016-4403-x
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.physbeh.2006.03.006
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.physbeh.2006.03.006
https://doi.org/10.1007/s00394-014-0812-y
https://doi.org/10.1038/sj.npp.1300887
https://doi.org/10.1038/sj.npp.1300887
https://doi.org/10.1007/s00213-001-0944-7
https://doi.org/10.1007/s00213-001-0944-7
https://doi.org/10.1093/alcalc/agn122
https://doi.org/10.1001/jamapsychiatry.2016.2157
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.schres.2007.03.008
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psychres.2013.10.012
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psychres.2013.10.012
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.eatbeh.2017.11.005.
https://doi.org/10.1176/appi.ajp.2010.09091379
Janowsky, D. S., Pucilowski, O., & Buyinza, M. (2003).
Preference for
higher sucrose concentrations in cocaine abusing-dependent pa-
tients. Journal of Psychiatric Research, 37(1), 35–41.
Kampov-Polevoy, A., Garbutt, J. C., & Janowsky, D. (1997).
Evidence of
preference for a high-concentration sucrose solution in
alcoholic
men. The American Journal of Psychiatry, 154(2), 269–270.
https://doi.org/10.1176/ajp.154.2.269.
Kampov-Polevoy, A. B., Tsoi, M. V., Zvartau, E. E., Neznanov,
N. G., &
Khalitov, E. (2001). Sweet liking and family history of
alcoholism in
hospitalized alcoholic and non-alcoholic patients. Alcohol and
Alcoholism, 36(2), 165–170.
Kampov-Polevoy, A. B., Ziedonis, D., Steinberg, M. L., Pinsky,
I.,
Krejci, J., Eick, C., Boland, G., Khalitov, E., & Crews, F. T.
(2003). Association between sweet preference and paternal
history
of alcoholism in psychiatric and substance abuse patients.
Alcoholism, Clinical and Experimental Research, 27(12), 1929–
1936. https://doi.org/10.1097/01.alc.0000099265.60216.23.
Kaye, S. A., White, M. J., & Lewis, I. (2018). Young females'
attention
toward road safety images: An ERP study of the revised
reinforce-
ment sensitivity theory. Traffic Injury Prevention, 19(2), 201–
206.
https://doi.org/10.1080/15389588.2017.1369531.
Kikuchi, Y., & Watanabe, S. (2000). Personality and dietary
habits.
Journal of Epidemiology, 10(3), 191–198.
Lange, L. A., Kampov-Polevoy, A. B., & Garbutt, J. C. (2010).
Sweet
liking and high novelty seeking: Independent phenotypes
associated
with alcohol-related problems† (article). Alcohol and
Alcoholism,
45(5), 431–436. https://doi.org/10.1093/alcalc/agq040.
Langleben, D. D., Busch, E. L., O'Brien, C. P., & Elman, I.
(2012). Depot
naltrexone decreases rewarding properties of sugar in patients
with
opioid dependence. Psychopharmacology, 220(3), 559–564.
https://
doi.org/10.1007/s00213-011-2503-1.
Lee, C. W., & Ho, I. K. (2013). Sex differences in opioid
analgesia and
addiction: Interactions among opioid receptors and estrogen
recep-
tors. Molecular Pain, 9, 45.
McCabe, C., Mishor, Z., Cowen, P. J., & Harmer, C. J. (2010).
Diminished neural processing of aversive and rewarding stimuli
during selective serotonin reuptake inhibitor treatment.
Biological
Psychiatry, 67(5), 439–445. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsych.
2009.11.001.
Meier, B. P., Moeller, S. K., Riemer-Peltz, M., & Robinson, M.
D. (2012).
Sweet taste preferences and experiences predict prosocial infer-
ences, personalities, and behaviors. Journal of Personality and
Social Psychology, 102(1), 163–174. https://doi.org/10.1037/
a0025253.
Mello, N. K., Negus, S. S., Knudson, I. M., Kelly, M., &
Mendelson, J. H.
(2008). Effects of estradiol on cocaine self-administration and
co-
caine discrimination by female rhesus monkeys.
Neuropsychopharmacology, 33, 783–795.
Mello, N. K., Knudson, I. M., Kelly, M., Fivel, P. A., &
Medelson, J. H.
(2011). Effects of progesterone and testosterone on cocaine
self-
administration and cocaine discrimination by female rhesus
mon-
keys. Neuropsychopharmacology, 36, 2187–2199.
Mulligan, E. M., Nelson, B. D., Infantolino, Z. P., Luking, K.
R., Sharma,
R., & Hajcak, G. (2018). Effects of menstrual cycle phase on
electrocortical response to reward and depressive symptoms in
women. Psychophysiology, 55, e13268. https://doi.org/10.1111/
psyp.13268.
Quilty, L. C., DeYoung, C. G., Oakman, J. M., & Bagby, R. M.
(2014).
Extraversion and behavioral activation: Integrating the
components
of approach (Article). Journal of Personality Assessment, 96(1),
87–
94. https://doi.org/10.1080/00223891.2013.834440.
Reynolds, W. (1982). Development of reliable and valid short
forms of
the Marlowe-Crowne social desirability scale. Journal of
Clinical
Psychology, 38(1), 119–125.
Sagioglou, C., & Greitemeyer, T. (2016). Individual differences
in bitter
taste preferences are associated with antisocial personality
traits.
Appetite, 96, 299–308.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.appet.2015.09.031.
Saliba, A. J., Wragg, K., & Richardson, P. (2009). Sweet taste
preference
and personality traits using a white wine (Article). Food Quality
and
Preference, 20(8), 572–575. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.foodqual.
2009.05.009.
Schroeder, J. A., Niculescu, M., & Unterwald, E. M. (2003).
Cocaine
alters mu but not delta or kappa opioid receptor-stimulated in
situ
[35S]GTPgammaS binding in rat brain. Synapse, 47(1), 26–32.
https://doi.org/10.1002/syn.10148.
Sena-Esteves, M. M., Mota, M., & Malfeito-Ferreira, M. (2018).
Patterns
of sweetness preference in red wine according to consumer
charac-
terisation (Article). Food Research International, 106, 38–44.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.foodres.2017.12.043.
Snaith, R. P., Hamilton, M., Morley, S., Humayan, A.,
Hargreaves, D., &
Trigwell, P. (1995). A scale for the assessment of hedonic tone
the
Snaith-Hamilton pleasure scale. The British Journal of
Psychiatry,
167(1), 99–103.
Strauss, G. P., & Cohen, A. S. (2017). A transdiagnostic review
of neg-
ative symptom phenomenology and etiology. Schizophrenia
Bulletin, 43, 712–719.
Sofuoglu, M., Dudish-Poulsen, S., Nelson, D., Pentel, P. R., &
Hatsukami, D. K. (1999). Sex and menstrual cycle differences
in
the subjective effects from smoked cocaine in humans.
Experimental and Clinical Psychopharmacology, 7(3), 274–283.
Treadway, M. T., & Zald, D. H. (2011). Reconsidering
anhedonia in
depression: Lessons from translational neuroscience.
Neuroscience
and Biobehavioral Reviews, 35(3), 537–555. https://doi.org/10.
1016/j.neubiorev.2010.06.006.
Tsypes, A., Owens, M., Hajcak, G., & Gibb, B. E. (2018).
Neural reward
responsiveness in children who engage in nonsuicidal self-
injury: an
ERP study. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 59,
1289–
1297. https://doi.org/10.1111/jcpp.12919.
Tucci, S. A., Murphy, L. E., Boyland, E. J., Dye, L., & Halford,
J. C.
(2010). Oral contraceptive effects on food choice during the
follic-
ular and luteal phases of the menstrual cycle. A laboratory
based
study. Appetite, 55(3), 388–392. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.appet.
2010.06.005.
Vittengl, J. R., Clark, L. A., Thase, M. E., & Jarrett, R. B.
(2015).
Predictors of longitudinal outcomes after unstable response to
acute-phase cognitive therapy for major depressive disorder.
Psychotherapy (Chicago, Ill.), 52(2), 268–277.
https://doi.org/10.
1037/pst0000021.
Weafer, J., Burkhardt, A., & de Wit, H. (2014). Sweet taste
liking is
associated with impulsive behaviors in humans. Frontiers in
Behavioral Neuroscience, 8, 228. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnbeh.
2014.00228.
Weafer, J., Lyon, N., Hedeker, D., & de Wit, H. (2017). Sweet
taste liking
is associated with subjective response to amphetamine in
women but
not men. Psychopharmacology, 234(21), 3185–3194. https://doi.
org/10.1007/s00213-017-4702-x.
Wronski, M., Skrok-Wolska, D., Samochowiec, J., Ziolkowski,
M.,
Swiecicki, L., Bienkowski, P., et al. (2007). Perceived intensity
and pleasantness of sucrose taste in male alcoholics. Alcohol
and
Alcoholism, 42(2), 75–79.
https://doi.org/10.1093/alcalc/agl097.
248 J Psychopathol Behav Assess (2019) 41:235–248
https://doi.org/10.1176/ajp.154.2.269
https://doi.org/10.1097/01.alc.0000099265.60216.23
https://doi.org/10.1080/15389588.2017.1369531
https://doi.org/10.1093/alcalc/agq040.
https://doi.org/10.1007/s00213-011-2503-1
https://doi.org/10.1007/s00213-011-2503-1
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsych.2009.11.001
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsych.2009.11.001
https://doi.org/10.1037/a0025253
https://doi.org/10.1037/a0025253
https://doi.org/10.1111/psyp.13268
https://doi.org/10.1111/psyp.13268
https://doi.org/10.1080/00223891.2013.834440.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.appet.2015.09.031
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.foodqual.2009.05.009
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.foodqual.2009.05.009
https://doi.org/10.1002/syn.10148
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.foodres.2017.12.043.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2010.06.006
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2010.06.006
https://doi.org/10.1111/jcpp.12919
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.appet.2010.06.005
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.appet.2010.06.005
https://doi.org/10.1037/pst0000021
https://doi.org/10.1037/pst0000021
https://doi.org/10.3389/fnbeh.2014.00228
https://doi.org/10.3389/fnbeh.2014.00228
https://doi.org/10.1007/s00213-017-4702-x
https://doi.org/10.1007/s00213-017-4702-x
https://doi.org/10.1093/alcalc/agl097.The Sweet Taste Test:
Relationships with Anhedonia Subtypes, Personality Traits, and
Menstrual Cycle
PhasesAbstractIntroductionMethodsParticipantsMeasuresSelf-
Report ScalesSweet Taste Test (STT)Menstrual Cycle Stage
IdentificationProceduresStatistical AnalysesResultsSTT
Relationships with Menstrual Phase Status, Demographic
Variables, and Time of Day during TestingSTT Relationships
with AnhedoniaSTT Relationships with Personality
FactorsExamination of Change in Results with Retention of
Excluded ParticipantsDiscussionReferences
The Sweet Taste Test Relationships with Anhedonia Subtypes,.docx

More Related Content

Similar to The Sweet Taste Test Relationships with Anhedonia Subtypes,.docx

Alcohol And The Adolescent Brain Human Studies
Alcohol And The Adolescent Brain Human StudiesAlcohol And The Adolescent Brain Human Studies
Alcohol And The Adolescent Brain Human StudiesGina Rizzo
 
Intrauterine alcohol exposure and offspring mental health: A systematic review
Intrauterine alcohol exposure and offspring mental health: A systematic reviewIntrauterine alcohol exposure and offspring mental health: A systematic review
Intrauterine alcohol exposure and offspring mental health: A systematic reviewBARRY STANLEY 2 fasd
 
ANTH336_The Proximate Causes of ADHD
ANTH336_The Proximate Causes of ADHDANTH336_The Proximate Causes of ADHD
ANTH336_The Proximate Causes of ADHDAustin Pittsley
 
Running Head Critique 1Critique2CritiqueAma.docx
Running Head Critique  1Critique2CritiqueAma.docxRunning Head Critique  1Critique2CritiqueAma.docx
Running Head Critique 1Critique2CritiqueAma.docxjoellemurphey
 
The Impact of Ethnicity on Antidepressant Therapy.docx
The Impact of Ethnicity on Antidepressant Therapy.docxThe Impact of Ethnicity on Antidepressant Therapy.docx
The Impact of Ethnicity on Antidepressant Therapy.docxwrite5
 
The utility of psychotropic drugs on patients with fetal alcohol spectrum dis...
The utility of psychotropic drugs on patients with fetal alcohol spectrum dis...The utility of psychotropic drugs on patients with fetal alcohol spectrum dis...
The utility of psychotropic drugs on patients with fetal alcohol spectrum dis...BARRY STANLEY 2 fasd
 
Comparison of Executive Functions in Addicted Young People who Referred to Ad...
Comparison of Executive Functions in Addicted Young People who Referred to Ad...Comparison of Executive Functions in Addicted Young People who Referred to Ad...
Comparison of Executive Functions in Addicted Young People who Referred to Ad...INFOGAIN PUBLICATION
 
11. Identifying the Elements of the Limitations & ImplicationsGo t
11. Identifying the Elements of the Limitations & ImplicationsGo t11. Identifying the Elements of the Limitations & ImplicationsGo t
11. Identifying the Elements of the Limitations & ImplicationsGo tBenitoSumpter862
 
11. Identifying the Elements of the Limitations & ImplicationsGo t
11. Identifying the Elements of the Limitations & ImplicationsGo t11. Identifying the Elements of the Limitations & ImplicationsGo t
11. Identifying the Elements of the Limitations & ImplicationsGo tSantosConleyha
 
TO REPLY 1 COMMENT TO EACH POST WITH CITATION AND TWO REFERENCE EACH.docx
TO REPLY 1 COMMENT TO EACH POST WITH CITATION AND TWO REFERENCE EACH.docxTO REPLY 1 COMMENT TO EACH POST WITH CITATION AND TWO REFERENCE EACH.docx
TO REPLY 1 COMMENT TO EACH POST WITH CITATION AND TWO REFERENCE EACH.docxrowthechang
 
Case # 29- The depressed man who thought he was out of options. .docx
Case # 29- The depressed man who thought he was out of options. .docxCase # 29- The depressed man who thought he was out of options. .docx
Case # 29- The depressed man who thought he was out of options. .docxannandleola
 
Moderate alcohol consumption as risk factor for adverse brain outcomes and co...
Moderate alcohol consumption as risk factor for adverse brain outcomes and co...Moderate alcohol consumption as risk factor for adverse brain outcomes and co...
Moderate alcohol consumption as risk factor for adverse brain outcomes and co...BARRY STANLEY 2 fasd
 
Impact of substance abuse on.pdf
Impact of substance abuse on.pdfImpact of substance abuse on.pdf
Impact of substance abuse on.pdfstudywriters
 
1 Clinical Problem Social Anxiety is described by .docx
1  Clinical Problem  Social Anxiety is described by .docx1  Clinical Problem  Social Anxiety is described by .docx
1 Clinical Problem Social Anxiety is described by .docxcroftsshanon
 
1 Clinical Problem Social Anxiety is described by .docx
1  Clinical Problem  Social Anxiety is described by .docx1  Clinical Problem  Social Anxiety is described by .docx
1 Clinical Problem Social Anxiety is described by .docxjeremylockett77
 
3 understanding and applying epidemiology hb lead-conf
3  understanding and applying epidemiology hb lead-conf3  understanding and applying epidemiology hb lead-conf
3 understanding and applying epidemiology hb lead-confHB Litigation Conferences
 
Sensory processing disorder_dsm-5_proposal
Sensory processing disorder_dsm-5_proposalSensory processing disorder_dsm-5_proposal
Sensory processing disorder_dsm-5_proposalJennifer Jo Brout
 

Similar to The Sweet Taste Test Relationships with Anhedonia Subtypes,.docx (20)

Alcohol And The Adolescent Brain Human Studies
Alcohol And The Adolescent Brain Human StudiesAlcohol And The Adolescent Brain Human Studies
Alcohol And The Adolescent Brain Human Studies
 
Intrauterine alcohol exposure and offspring mental health: A systematic review
Intrauterine alcohol exposure and offspring mental health: A systematic reviewIntrauterine alcohol exposure and offspring mental health: A systematic review
Intrauterine alcohol exposure and offspring mental health: A systematic review
 
ANTH336_The Proximate Causes of ADHD
ANTH336_The Proximate Causes of ADHDANTH336_The Proximate Causes of ADHD
ANTH336_The Proximate Causes of ADHD
 
Running Head Critique 1Critique2CritiqueAma.docx
Running Head Critique  1Critique2CritiqueAma.docxRunning Head Critique  1Critique2CritiqueAma.docx
Running Head Critique 1Critique2CritiqueAma.docx
 
The Impact of Ethnicity on Antidepressant Therapy.docx
The Impact of Ethnicity on Antidepressant Therapy.docxThe Impact of Ethnicity on Antidepressant Therapy.docx
The Impact of Ethnicity on Antidepressant Therapy.docx
 
The utility of psychotropic drugs on patients with fetal alcohol spectrum dis...
The utility of psychotropic drugs on patients with fetal alcohol spectrum dis...The utility of psychotropic drugs on patients with fetal alcohol spectrum dis...
The utility of psychotropic drugs on patients with fetal alcohol spectrum dis...
 
Comparison of Executive Functions in Addicted Young People who Referred to Ad...
Comparison of Executive Functions in Addicted Young People who Referred to Ad...Comparison of Executive Functions in Addicted Young People who Referred to Ad...
Comparison of Executive Functions in Addicted Young People who Referred to Ad...
 
John Read, Placebo
John Read, PlaceboJohn Read, Placebo
John Read, Placebo
 
11. Identifying the Elements of the Limitations & ImplicationsGo t
11. Identifying the Elements of the Limitations & ImplicationsGo t11. Identifying the Elements of the Limitations & ImplicationsGo t
11. Identifying the Elements of the Limitations & ImplicationsGo t
 
11. Identifying the Elements of the Limitations & ImplicationsGo t
11. Identifying the Elements of the Limitations & ImplicationsGo t11. Identifying the Elements of the Limitations & ImplicationsGo t
11. Identifying the Elements of the Limitations & ImplicationsGo t
 
TO REPLY 1 COMMENT TO EACH POST WITH CITATION AND TWO REFERENCE EACH.docx
TO REPLY 1 COMMENT TO EACH POST WITH CITATION AND TWO REFERENCE EACH.docxTO REPLY 1 COMMENT TO EACH POST WITH CITATION AND TWO REFERENCE EACH.docx
TO REPLY 1 COMMENT TO EACH POST WITH CITATION AND TWO REFERENCE EACH.docx
 
Case # 29- The depressed man who thought he was out of options. .docx
Case # 29- The depressed man who thought he was out of options. .docxCase # 29- The depressed man who thought he was out of options. .docx
Case # 29- The depressed man who thought he was out of options. .docx
 
Moderate alcohol consumption as risk factor for adverse brain outcomes and co...
Moderate alcohol consumption as risk factor for adverse brain outcomes and co...Moderate alcohol consumption as risk factor for adverse brain outcomes and co...
Moderate alcohol consumption as risk factor for adverse brain outcomes and co...
 
Final Paper
Final PaperFinal Paper
Final Paper
 
Impact of substance abuse on.pdf
Impact of substance abuse on.pdfImpact of substance abuse on.pdf
Impact of substance abuse on.pdf
 
1 Clinical Problem Social Anxiety is described by .docx
1  Clinical Problem  Social Anxiety is described by .docx1  Clinical Problem  Social Anxiety is described by .docx
1 Clinical Problem Social Anxiety is described by .docx
 
1 Clinical Problem Social Anxiety is described by .docx
1  Clinical Problem  Social Anxiety is described by .docx1  Clinical Problem  Social Anxiety is described by .docx
1 Clinical Problem Social Anxiety is described by .docx
 
3 understanding and applying epidemiology hb lead-conf
3  understanding and applying epidemiology hb lead-conf3  understanding and applying epidemiology hb lead-conf
3 understanding and applying epidemiology hb lead-conf
 
Research_Proposal
Research_ProposalResearch_Proposal
Research_Proposal
 
Sensory processing disorder_dsm-5_proposal
Sensory processing disorder_dsm-5_proposalSensory processing disorder_dsm-5_proposal
Sensory processing disorder_dsm-5_proposal
 

More from arnoldmeredith47041

Write a scholarly paper in which you apply the concepts of epide.docx
Write a scholarly paper in which you apply the concepts of epide.docxWrite a scholarly paper in which you apply the concepts of epide.docx
Write a scholarly paper in which you apply the concepts of epide.docxarnoldmeredith47041
 
Write a S.M.A.R.T. goal to improve the Habit 5 Seek First to .docx
Write a S.M.A.R.T. goal to improve the Habit 5 Seek First to .docxWrite a S.M.A.R.T. goal to improve the Habit 5 Seek First to .docx
Write a S.M.A.R.T. goal to improve the Habit 5 Seek First to .docxarnoldmeredith47041
 
Write a Risk Management Plan for a School FacilityInclude th.docx
Write a Risk Management Plan for a School FacilityInclude th.docxWrite a Risk Management Plan for a School FacilityInclude th.docx
Write a Risk Management Plan for a School FacilityInclude th.docxarnoldmeredith47041
 
Write a review that 750 - 1000 words in length about one chapter in .docx
Write a review that 750 - 1000 words in length about one chapter in .docxWrite a review that 750 - 1000 words in length about one chapter in .docx
Write a review that 750 - 1000 words in length about one chapter in .docxarnoldmeredith47041
 
write a resume using the example belowCONTACT INFOFirs.docx
write a resume using the example belowCONTACT INFOFirs.docxwrite a resume using the example belowCONTACT INFOFirs.docx
write a resume using the example belowCONTACT INFOFirs.docxarnoldmeredith47041
 
Write a resume and cover letter for the following positionOnline.docx
Write a resume and cover letter for the following positionOnline.docxWrite a resume and cover letter for the following positionOnline.docx
Write a resume and cover letter for the following positionOnline.docxarnoldmeredith47041
 
Write a response to the peers post based on the readings. Origi.docx
Write a response to the peers post based on the readings. Origi.docxWrite a response to the peers post based on the readings. Origi.docx
Write a response to the peers post based on the readings. Origi.docxarnoldmeredith47041
 
Write a response to the following prompt.Analyze the characteriz.docx
Write a response to the following prompt.Analyze the characteriz.docxWrite a response to the following prompt.Analyze the characteriz.docx
Write a response to the following prompt.Analyze the characteriz.docxarnoldmeredith47041
 
Write a response to a peers post that adds or extends to the discus.docx
Write a response to a peers post that adds or extends to the discus.docxWrite a response to a peers post that adds or extends to the discus.docx
Write a response to a peers post that adds or extends to the discus.docxarnoldmeredith47041
 
Write a response mini-essay of at least 150 to 300 words on  the dis.docx
Write a response mini-essay of at least 150 to 300 words on  the dis.docxWrite a response mini-essay of at least 150 to 300 words on  the dis.docx
Write a response mini-essay of at least 150 to 300 words on  the dis.docxarnoldmeredith47041
 
Write a response for each document.Instructions Your post sho.docx
Write a response for each document.Instructions Your post sho.docxWrite a response for each document.Instructions Your post sho.docx
Write a response for each document.Instructions Your post sho.docxarnoldmeredith47041
 
write a resonse paper mla styleHAIRHair deeply affects people,.docx
write a resonse paper mla styleHAIRHair deeply affects people,.docxwrite a resonse paper mla styleHAIRHair deeply affects people,.docx
write a resonse paper mla styleHAIRHair deeply affects people,.docxarnoldmeredith47041
 
Write a response about the topic in the reading (see attached) and m.docx
Write a response about the topic in the reading (see attached) and m.docxWrite a response about the topic in the reading (see attached) and m.docx
Write a response about the topic in the reading (see attached) and m.docxarnoldmeredith47041
 
Write a research report based on a hypothetical research study.  Con.docx
Write a research report based on a hypothetical research study.  Con.docxWrite a research report based on a hypothetical research study.  Con.docx
Write a research report based on a hypothetical research study.  Con.docxarnoldmeredith47041
 
Write a Research Paper with the topic Pregnancy in the adolesce.docx
Write a Research Paper with the topic Pregnancy in the adolesce.docxWrite a Research Paper with the topic Pregnancy in the adolesce.docx
Write a Research Paper with the topic Pregnancy in the adolesce.docxarnoldmeredith47041
 
Write a Research Paper with the topic Autism a major problem. T.docx
Write a Research Paper with the topic Autism a major problem. T.docxWrite a Research Paper with the topic Autism a major problem. T.docx
Write a Research Paper with the topic Autism a major problem. T.docxarnoldmeredith47041
 
Write a research paper that explains how Information Technology (IT).docx
Write a research paper that explains how Information Technology (IT).docxWrite a research paper that explains how Information Technology (IT).docx
Write a research paper that explains how Information Technology (IT).docxarnoldmeredith47041
 
Write a research paper outlining possible career paths in the field .docx
Write a research paper outlining possible career paths in the field .docxWrite a research paper outlining possible career paths in the field .docx
Write a research paper outlining possible career paths in the field .docxarnoldmeredith47041
 
Write a Research paper on the Legal issues associated with pentestin.docx
Write a Research paper on the Legal issues associated with pentestin.docxWrite a Research paper on the Legal issues associated with pentestin.docx
Write a Research paper on the Legal issues associated with pentestin.docxarnoldmeredith47041
 
Write a research paper on one of the following topics .docx
Write a research paper on one of the following topics .docxWrite a research paper on one of the following topics .docx
Write a research paper on one of the following topics .docxarnoldmeredith47041
 

More from arnoldmeredith47041 (20)

Write a scholarly paper in which you apply the concepts of epide.docx
Write a scholarly paper in which you apply the concepts of epide.docxWrite a scholarly paper in which you apply the concepts of epide.docx
Write a scholarly paper in which you apply the concepts of epide.docx
 
Write a S.M.A.R.T. goal to improve the Habit 5 Seek First to .docx
Write a S.M.A.R.T. goal to improve the Habit 5 Seek First to .docxWrite a S.M.A.R.T. goal to improve the Habit 5 Seek First to .docx
Write a S.M.A.R.T. goal to improve the Habit 5 Seek First to .docx
 
Write a Risk Management Plan for a School FacilityInclude th.docx
Write a Risk Management Plan for a School FacilityInclude th.docxWrite a Risk Management Plan for a School FacilityInclude th.docx
Write a Risk Management Plan for a School FacilityInclude th.docx
 
Write a review that 750 - 1000 words in length about one chapter in .docx
Write a review that 750 - 1000 words in length about one chapter in .docxWrite a review that 750 - 1000 words in length about one chapter in .docx
Write a review that 750 - 1000 words in length about one chapter in .docx
 
write a resume using the example belowCONTACT INFOFirs.docx
write a resume using the example belowCONTACT INFOFirs.docxwrite a resume using the example belowCONTACT INFOFirs.docx
write a resume using the example belowCONTACT INFOFirs.docx
 
Write a resume and cover letter for the following positionOnline.docx
Write a resume and cover letter for the following positionOnline.docxWrite a resume and cover letter for the following positionOnline.docx
Write a resume and cover letter for the following positionOnline.docx
 
Write a response to the peers post based on the readings. Origi.docx
Write a response to the peers post based on the readings. Origi.docxWrite a response to the peers post based on the readings. Origi.docx
Write a response to the peers post based on the readings. Origi.docx
 
Write a response to the following prompt.Analyze the characteriz.docx
Write a response to the following prompt.Analyze the characteriz.docxWrite a response to the following prompt.Analyze the characteriz.docx
Write a response to the following prompt.Analyze the characteriz.docx
 
Write a response to a peers post that adds or extends to the discus.docx
Write a response to a peers post that adds or extends to the discus.docxWrite a response to a peers post that adds or extends to the discus.docx
Write a response to a peers post that adds or extends to the discus.docx
 
Write a response mini-essay of at least 150 to 300 words on  the dis.docx
Write a response mini-essay of at least 150 to 300 words on  the dis.docxWrite a response mini-essay of at least 150 to 300 words on  the dis.docx
Write a response mini-essay of at least 150 to 300 words on  the dis.docx
 
Write a response for each document.Instructions Your post sho.docx
Write a response for each document.Instructions Your post sho.docxWrite a response for each document.Instructions Your post sho.docx
Write a response for each document.Instructions Your post sho.docx
 
write a resonse paper mla styleHAIRHair deeply affects people,.docx
write a resonse paper mla styleHAIRHair deeply affects people,.docxwrite a resonse paper mla styleHAIRHair deeply affects people,.docx
write a resonse paper mla styleHAIRHair deeply affects people,.docx
 
Write a response about the topic in the reading (see attached) and m.docx
Write a response about the topic in the reading (see attached) and m.docxWrite a response about the topic in the reading (see attached) and m.docx
Write a response about the topic in the reading (see attached) and m.docx
 
Write a research report based on a hypothetical research study.  Con.docx
Write a research report based on a hypothetical research study.  Con.docxWrite a research report based on a hypothetical research study.  Con.docx
Write a research report based on a hypothetical research study.  Con.docx
 
Write a Research Paper with the topic Pregnancy in the adolesce.docx
Write a Research Paper with the topic Pregnancy in the adolesce.docxWrite a Research Paper with the topic Pregnancy in the adolesce.docx
Write a Research Paper with the topic Pregnancy in the adolesce.docx
 
Write a Research Paper with the topic Autism a major problem. T.docx
Write a Research Paper with the topic Autism a major problem. T.docxWrite a Research Paper with the topic Autism a major problem. T.docx
Write a Research Paper with the topic Autism a major problem. T.docx
 
Write a research paper that explains how Information Technology (IT).docx
Write a research paper that explains how Information Technology (IT).docxWrite a research paper that explains how Information Technology (IT).docx
Write a research paper that explains how Information Technology (IT).docx
 
Write a research paper outlining possible career paths in the field .docx
Write a research paper outlining possible career paths in the field .docxWrite a research paper outlining possible career paths in the field .docx
Write a research paper outlining possible career paths in the field .docx
 
Write a Research paper on the Legal issues associated with pentestin.docx
Write a Research paper on the Legal issues associated with pentestin.docxWrite a Research paper on the Legal issues associated with pentestin.docx
Write a Research paper on the Legal issues associated with pentestin.docx
 
Write a research paper on one of the following topics .docx
Write a research paper on one of the following topics .docxWrite a research paper on one of the following topics .docx
Write a research paper on one of the following topics .docx
 

Recently uploaded

How to Configure Email Server in Odoo 17
How to Configure Email Server in Odoo 17How to Configure Email Server in Odoo 17
How to Configure Email Server in Odoo 17Celine George
 
Introduction to ArtificiaI Intelligence in Higher Education
Introduction to ArtificiaI Intelligence in Higher EducationIntroduction to ArtificiaI Intelligence in Higher Education
Introduction to ArtificiaI Intelligence in Higher Educationpboyjonauth
 
Enzyme, Pharmaceutical Aids, Miscellaneous Last Part of Chapter no 5th.pdf
Enzyme, Pharmaceutical Aids, Miscellaneous Last Part of Chapter no 5th.pdfEnzyme, Pharmaceutical Aids, Miscellaneous Last Part of Chapter no 5th.pdf
Enzyme, Pharmaceutical Aids, Miscellaneous Last Part of Chapter no 5th.pdfSumit Tiwari
 
Computed Fields and api Depends in the Odoo 17
Computed Fields and api Depends in the Odoo 17Computed Fields and api Depends in the Odoo 17
Computed Fields and api Depends in the Odoo 17Celine George
 
History Class XII Ch. 3 Kinship, Caste and Class (1).pptx
History Class XII Ch. 3 Kinship, Caste and Class (1).pptxHistory Class XII Ch. 3 Kinship, Caste and Class (1).pptx
History Class XII Ch. 3 Kinship, Caste and Class (1).pptxsocialsciencegdgrohi
 
Organic Name Reactions for the students and aspirants of Chemistry12th.pptx
Organic Name Reactions  for the students and aspirants of Chemistry12th.pptxOrganic Name Reactions  for the students and aspirants of Chemistry12th.pptx
Organic Name Reactions for the students and aspirants of Chemistry12th.pptxVS Mahajan Coaching Centre
 
Call Girls in Dwarka Mor Delhi Contact Us 9654467111
Call Girls in Dwarka Mor Delhi Contact Us 9654467111Call Girls in Dwarka Mor Delhi Contact Us 9654467111
Call Girls in Dwarka Mor Delhi Contact Us 9654467111Sapana Sha
 
SOCIAL AND HISTORICAL CONTEXT - LFTVD.pptx
SOCIAL AND HISTORICAL CONTEXT - LFTVD.pptxSOCIAL AND HISTORICAL CONTEXT - LFTVD.pptx
SOCIAL AND HISTORICAL CONTEXT - LFTVD.pptxiammrhaywood
 
Mastering the Unannounced Regulatory Inspection
Mastering the Unannounced Regulatory InspectionMastering the Unannounced Regulatory Inspection
Mastering the Unannounced Regulatory InspectionSafetyChain Software
 
Incoming and Outgoing Shipments in 1 STEP Using Odoo 17
Incoming and Outgoing Shipments in 1 STEP Using Odoo 17Incoming and Outgoing Shipments in 1 STEP Using Odoo 17
Incoming and Outgoing Shipments in 1 STEP Using Odoo 17Celine George
 
Kisan Call Centre - To harness potential of ICT in Agriculture by answer farm...
Kisan Call Centre - To harness potential of ICT in Agriculture by answer farm...Kisan Call Centre - To harness potential of ICT in Agriculture by answer farm...
Kisan Call Centre - To harness potential of ICT in Agriculture by answer farm...Krashi Coaching
 
Solving Puzzles Benefits Everyone (English).pptx
Solving Puzzles Benefits Everyone (English).pptxSolving Puzzles Benefits Everyone (English).pptx
Solving Puzzles Benefits Everyone (English).pptxOH TEIK BIN
 
18-04-UA_REPORT_MEDIALITERAСY_INDEX-DM_23-1-final-eng.pdf
18-04-UA_REPORT_MEDIALITERAСY_INDEX-DM_23-1-final-eng.pdf18-04-UA_REPORT_MEDIALITERAСY_INDEX-DM_23-1-final-eng.pdf
18-04-UA_REPORT_MEDIALITERAСY_INDEX-DM_23-1-final-eng.pdfssuser54595a
 
The Most Excellent Way | 1 Corinthians 13
The Most Excellent Way | 1 Corinthians 13The Most Excellent Way | 1 Corinthians 13
The Most Excellent Way | 1 Corinthians 13Steve Thomason
 
Class 11 Legal Studies Ch-1 Concept of State .pdf
Class 11 Legal Studies Ch-1 Concept of State .pdfClass 11 Legal Studies Ch-1 Concept of State .pdf
Class 11 Legal Studies Ch-1 Concept of State .pdfakmcokerachita
 
POINT- BIOCHEMISTRY SEM 2 ENZYMES UNIT 5.pptx
POINT- BIOCHEMISTRY SEM 2 ENZYMES UNIT 5.pptxPOINT- BIOCHEMISTRY SEM 2 ENZYMES UNIT 5.pptx
POINT- BIOCHEMISTRY SEM 2 ENZYMES UNIT 5.pptxSayali Powar
 
Pharmacognosy Flower 3. Compositae 2023.pdf
Pharmacognosy Flower 3. Compositae 2023.pdfPharmacognosy Flower 3. Compositae 2023.pdf
Pharmacognosy Flower 3. Compositae 2023.pdfMahmoud M. Sallam
 
Paris 2024 Olympic Geographies - an activity
Paris 2024 Olympic Geographies - an activityParis 2024 Olympic Geographies - an activity
Paris 2024 Olympic Geographies - an activityGeoBlogs
 
Science lesson Moon for 4th quarter lesson
Science lesson Moon for 4th quarter lessonScience lesson Moon for 4th quarter lesson
Science lesson Moon for 4th quarter lessonJericReyAuditor
 

Recently uploaded (20)

How to Configure Email Server in Odoo 17
How to Configure Email Server in Odoo 17How to Configure Email Server in Odoo 17
How to Configure Email Server in Odoo 17
 
Introduction to ArtificiaI Intelligence in Higher Education
Introduction to ArtificiaI Intelligence in Higher EducationIntroduction to ArtificiaI Intelligence in Higher Education
Introduction to ArtificiaI Intelligence in Higher Education
 
Enzyme, Pharmaceutical Aids, Miscellaneous Last Part of Chapter no 5th.pdf
Enzyme, Pharmaceutical Aids, Miscellaneous Last Part of Chapter no 5th.pdfEnzyme, Pharmaceutical Aids, Miscellaneous Last Part of Chapter no 5th.pdf
Enzyme, Pharmaceutical Aids, Miscellaneous Last Part of Chapter no 5th.pdf
 
Computed Fields and api Depends in the Odoo 17
Computed Fields and api Depends in the Odoo 17Computed Fields and api Depends in the Odoo 17
Computed Fields and api Depends in the Odoo 17
 
History Class XII Ch. 3 Kinship, Caste and Class (1).pptx
History Class XII Ch. 3 Kinship, Caste and Class (1).pptxHistory Class XII Ch. 3 Kinship, Caste and Class (1).pptx
History Class XII Ch. 3 Kinship, Caste and Class (1).pptx
 
Organic Name Reactions for the students and aspirants of Chemistry12th.pptx
Organic Name Reactions  for the students and aspirants of Chemistry12th.pptxOrganic Name Reactions  for the students and aspirants of Chemistry12th.pptx
Organic Name Reactions for the students and aspirants of Chemistry12th.pptx
 
Call Girls in Dwarka Mor Delhi Contact Us 9654467111
Call Girls in Dwarka Mor Delhi Contact Us 9654467111Call Girls in Dwarka Mor Delhi Contact Us 9654467111
Call Girls in Dwarka Mor Delhi Contact Us 9654467111
 
SOCIAL AND HISTORICAL CONTEXT - LFTVD.pptx
SOCIAL AND HISTORICAL CONTEXT - LFTVD.pptxSOCIAL AND HISTORICAL CONTEXT - LFTVD.pptx
SOCIAL AND HISTORICAL CONTEXT - LFTVD.pptx
 
Mastering the Unannounced Regulatory Inspection
Mastering the Unannounced Regulatory InspectionMastering the Unannounced Regulatory Inspection
Mastering the Unannounced Regulatory Inspection
 
Incoming and Outgoing Shipments in 1 STEP Using Odoo 17
Incoming and Outgoing Shipments in 1 STEP Using Odoo 17Incoming and Outgoing Shipments in 1 STEP Using Odoo 17
Incoming and Outgoing Shipments in 1 STEP Using Odoo 17
 
Kisan Call Centre - To harness potential of ICT in Agriculture by answer farm...
Kisan Call Centre - To harness potential of ICT in Agriculture by answer farm...Kisan Call Centre - To harness potential of ICT in Agriculture by answer farm...
Kisan Call Centre - To harness potential of ICT in Agriculture by answer farm...
 
Solving Puzzles Benefits Everyone (English).pptx
Solving Puzzles Benefits Everyone (English).pptxSolving Puzzles Benefits Everyone (English).pptx
Solving Puzzles Benefits Everyone (English).pptx
 
18-04-UA_REPORT_MEDIALITERAСY_INDEX-DM_23-1-final-eng.pdf
18-04-UA_REPORT_MEDIALITERAСY_INDEX-DM_23-1-final-eng.pdf18-04-UA_REPORT_MEDIALITERAСY_INDEX-DM_23-1-final-eng.pdf
18-04-UA_REPORT_MEDIALITERAСY_INDEX-DM_23-1-final-eng.pdf
 
The Most Excellent Way | 1 Corinthians 13
The Most Excellent Way | 1 Corinthians 13The Most Excellent Way | 1 Corinthians 13
The Most Excellent Way | 1 Corinthians 13
 
Class 11 Legal Studies Ch-1 Concept of State .pdf
Class 11 Legal Studies Ch-1 Concept of State .pdfClass 11 Legal Studies Ch-1 Concept of State .pdf
Class 11 Legal Studies Ch-1 Concept of State .pdf
 
Staff of Color (SOC) Retention Efforts DDSD
Staff of Color (SOC) Retention Efforts DDSDStaff of Color (SOC) Retention Efforts DDSD
Staff of Color (SOC) Retention Efforts DDSD
 
POINT- BIOCHEMISTRY SEM 2 ENZYMES UNIT 5.pptx
POINT- BIOCHEMISTRY SEM 2 ENZYMES UNIT 5.pptxPOINT- BIOCHEMISTRY SEM 2 ENZYMES UNIT 5.pptx
POINT- BIOCHEMISTRY SEM 2 ENZYMES UNIT 5.pptx
 
Pharmacognosy Flower 3. Compositae 2023.pdf
Pharmacognosy Flower 3. Compositae 2023.pdfPharmacognosy Flower 3. Compositae 2023.pdf
Pharmacognosy Flower 3. Compositae 2023.pdf
 
Paris 2024 Olympic Geographies - an activity
Paris 2024 Olympic Geographies - an activityParis 2024 Olympic Geographies - an activity
Paris 2024 Olympic Geographies - an activity
 
Science lesson Moon for 4th quarter lesson
Science lesson Moon for 4th quarter lessonScience lesson Moon for 4th quarter lesson
Science lesson Moon for 4th quarter lesson
 

The Sweet Taste Test Relationships with Anhedonia Subtypes,.docx

  • 1. The Sweet Taste Test: Relationships with Anhedonia Subtypes, Personality Traits, and Menstrual Cycle Phases Jeffrey S. Bedwell1 & Christopher C. Spencer1 & Cristina A. Chirino1 & John P. O’Donnell1 Published online: 15 January 2019 # Springer Science+Business Media, LLC, part of Springer Nature 2019 Abstract A better understanding of the etiology and pathology related to distinct subtypes of anhedonia can lead to more efficacious personalized treatments. The current study advances knowledge on consummatory anhedonia - represented in the Research Domain Criteria (RDoC) subconstruct of Initial Response to Reward (IRR). The Sweet Taste Test (STT) has promise as a behavioral paradigm for IRR, as it is sensitive to manipulation of μ-opioid receptors. However, there is a lack of existing knowledge of how the STT relates to subtypes of anhedonia, personality traits, and phases of the menstrual cycle. To address these questions, we administered the STT to 72 nonpsychiatric adults (76% women; mean age: 19.11). As predicted, the hedonic slope reflecting increasing Blike^ ratings over increasing concentrations of five sucrose solutions (ranging from 0.05 M to 0.86 M), was lower in individuals reporting higher consummatory anhedonia (measured with Temporal Experience of Pleasure Scales - Consummatory Subscale) and in women in the mid-to-late luteal menstrual phase (days 20 to 28). Both effects
  • 2. were driven by lower hedonic ratings to the sweetest concentration. The hedonic slope was larger in individuals scoring higher on the Flight-Freeze-Avoidance System personality factor from the Reinforcement Sensitivity Theory Personality Questionnaire - driven by lower hedonic ratings for the least sweet concentration. No factor or aspect from the Big Five Aspects Scale related to hedonic ratings on the STT. The STT may be a valid and specific standardized behavioral paradigm to add to IRR, particularly if validated in a large transdiagnostic psychiatric sample. Keywords Sucrose .Reward .Consummatoryanhedonia .Initialresponsivenesstorewardattainment .Researchdomaincriteria . Consummatory pleasure Introduction Anhedonia is a transdiagnostic symptom which is particularly resistant to existing treatments relative to most co-occurring symptoms (McCabe et al. 2010; Vittengl et al. 2015). While the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (DSM-5) defines anhedonia as a unitary construct (e.g., Bdecreased interest and pleasure in most activities most of the day;^ American Psychiatric Association 2013), recent ad- vances in affective neuroscience suggest three distinct sub- types (Treadway and Zald 2011): motivational (i.e., wanting), consummatory (i.e., liking), and decisional (i.e., reward learn- ing). The National Institute of Mental Health’s (NIMH) Research Domain Criteria (RDoC) Matrix includes corresponding constructs under the Positive Valence Systems (PVS) domain (Insel et al. 2010). A better under-
  • 3. standing of the etiology and pathology related to distinct sub- types of anhedonia can lead to more efficacious personalized interventions for this treatment-resistant symptom (Insel and Cuthbert 2015; Strauss and Cohen 2017). Consummatory anhedonia is partially represented in the PVS construct Reward Responsiveness under the subconstruct of Initial Response to Reward (IRR; as of 10/2/ 18), defined by NIMH as: BProcesses evoked by the initial presentation of a positive reinforcer as reflected by indices of neuronal activity and verbal or behavioral responses.^ This is reflected by the inclusion of the Consummatory sub- scale of the Temporal Experience of Pleasure Scales (TEPS-C; Gard et al. 2007) under Self-Reports in IRR. While IRR cur- rently names a single paradigm of Bsimple guessing task,^ application of such tasks require physiological measures of brain response (e.g., Tsypes et al. 2018). Alternatively, the Sweet Taste Test (STT; Kampov-Polevoy et al. 1997; Dichter et al. 2010) has promise as a behavioral IRR paradigm * Jeffrey S. Bedwell [email protected] 1 Department of Psychology, University of Central Florida, 4111 Pictor Lane, Orlando, FL 32816-1390, USA Journal of Psychopathology and Behavioral Assessment (2019) 41:235–248 https://doi.org/10.1007/s10862-019-09717-2 http://crossmark.crossref.org/dialog/?doi=10.1007/s10862-019- 09717-2&domain=pdf http://orcid.org/0000-0002-1205-5792 mailto:[email protected]
  • 4. that could be used in clinical settings, as it is does not require physiological equipment and related expertise. While the STT is not specifically named under Paradigms for IRR, Btaste reactivity^ is listed under Behavior. In the STT, the participant tastes a random order of five trials for each of five concentra- tions of sucrose, ranging from minimal sweetness to two so- lutions that are sweeter than beverages such as Coca-Cola®, and collects subjective ratings of sweetness and liking after each trial (Kampov-Polevoy et al. 1997). Individuals whose hedonic Blike^ ratings are highest for the strongest sucrose concentration have been termed Bsweet likers,^ a category associated with alcohol use dis- order (Kampov-Polevoy et al. 2001, 2003; Wronski et al. 2007), cocaine use disorder (Janowsky et al. 2003), binge- eating disorder (Goodman et al. 2017), impulsive choice on a delay discounting task (Weafer et al. 2014), better response to naltrexone treatment for alcohol use disorder (Garbutt et al. 2009, 2016), and greater amphetamine- induced euphoria in nonpsychiatric women but not men (Weafer et al. 2017). Consistent with inclusion of μ- opioid receptors under IRR Molecules, research has shown that stimulation of μ-opioid Bhedonic hotspots^ in the nucleus accumbens shell, ventral pallidum, anterior orbitofrontal cortex, and posterior insula increase sucrose Bliking^ behavior in rats (Castro and Berridge 2017). Similarly, a study of nonpsychiatric men found a decrease in sucrose liking following administration of the μ-opioid antagonist naltrexone, and increased liking following the μ-opioid agonist morphine (Eikemo et al. 2016). Both effects were observed for only dimensional hedonic rat- ings for the sweetest concentration (0.65 M in that study) and did not change a slope-based categorical sweet liker status. This finding is consistent with studies showing that naltrexone reduced average STT hedonic ratings in non- psychiatric women (Arbisi et al. 1999) and opioid depen-
  • 5. dent participants (Langleben et al. 2012). Overall, it ap- pears that the STT partially reflects endogenous opioid functioning, particularly the dimensional hedonic rating to the sweetest solution. Considering this emerging literature, we were interested in how STT performance relates to subtypes of anhedonia and a range of personality traits. This will inform investigators and clinicians regarding which anhedonia measure or subscale may best assess IRR/consummatory anhedonia, and general personality research by highlighting particular traits which may be sensitive to individual differences in the endogenous opioid system. Finally, there is increasing evidence that the luteal menstrual cycle phase, via an increase in progesterone, dampens hedonic responsivity to cocaine in female rhesus macaques (Carroll et al. 2016) and humans (Evans and Foltin 2006; Evans et al. 2002). As both progesterone and estradiol interact with opioid receptors (Schroeder et al. 2003; Lee and Ho 2013), it is possible that estradiol, which is higher during the follicular menstrual phase, may also effect reward sensitivity. However, studies in rhesus monkeys found that administering exogenous estradiol did not alter cocaine self-administration, while administering progesterone reduced the cocaine use (Mello et al. 2008, 2011). A recent study found an amplitude reduction in the event-related potential of reward positivity in response to monetary gains in women during the luteal as opposed to follicular phase of the menstru- al cycle, but this influence was only significant in women with greater severity of depression (Mulligan et al. 2018). Examining the relationship between menstrual phases and STT performance in a nonpsychiatric sample will account for important individual differences present in menstruating women and add to our understanding of how these phases may induce temporary changes in IRR.
  • 6. It appears that only one existing study examined relation- ships between a behavioral assessment of sweet taste liking and anhedonia severity. This study found that, across a transdiagnostic sample of individuals with major depressive disorder, schizophrenia, and nonpsychiatric controls, higher scores on the self-report Chapman Physical, but not Social, Anhedonia scale was associated with a lower maximum hedonic rating across all sucrose concentrations on the STT (Berlin et al. 1998). There appear to be few published studies on the relationship of a behavioral measure of sweet liking with personality traits. One reported that individuals who preferred the sweeter of two blinded samples of the same white wine scored lower on self-reported openness and higher on impulsiveness, a facet of neuroticism (Saliba et al. 2009). A study using five blinded levels of sweetness of the same red wine found that participants who preferred sweeter concentrations were higher in neu- roticism (Sena-Esteves et al. 2018). A study using the STT reported that categorical sweet likers did not differ from other participants on self-reported novelty seeking (Lange et al. 2010). Studies that did not include a behavioral mea- sure found that higher self-reported generalized liking of sweet foods and drinks was associated with higher self- reported agreeableness (Sagioglou and Greitemeyer 2016; Meier et al. 2012; Ashton et al. 2014), neuroticism (Kikuchi and Watanabe 2000; Elfhag and Erlanson-Albertsson 2006), and/or openness/intellect (Ashton et al. 2014) scores. Finally, while some research has found that women con- sume more sweet foods during the luteal phase (Bowen and Grunberg 1990; Tucci et al. 2010), one study examined hedonic rating to varying concentrations of sucrose solu- tions reported that ratings were lowest in the luteal phase, 236 J Psychopathol Behav Assess (2019) 41:235–248
  • 7. particularly for the sweetest concentration (Elliott et al. 2015). However, this study did not use the traditional STT, was limited by a single trial of four sucrose solutions, and did not include men in the comparison group. Based on existing findings using behavioral measures of hedonic response to sweet tastes, and current information in the IRR domain, we hypothesized that hedonic ratings to the sweetest STT concentration would be lower in participants reporting increased consummatory anhedonia, and higher in participants reporting more neuroticism and/or impulsiveness. In addition, we predicted that women in the luteal phase of the menstrual cycle would report a reduced hedonic response to the sweetest STT concentration as compared to other women and men. Methods Participants Participants were undergraduate students enrolled in a Psychology Department course that offered credit in ex- change for research participation at a large southeastern public university in the United States. Participants were screened online (N = 2135) and excluded for: completing questionnaire either too quickly (< 10th %ile of duration from sample; N = 122; 5.7%) or slowly (> 90%ile; N = 146; 6.8%), scoring >2 SD above mean on the Abbreviated Marlow-Crowne Social Desirability Scale (Reynolds 1982; N = 75; 3.5%), using non-prescribed stim- ulant or narcotic medication (N = 23; 1.1%), excessive chronic alcohol use (N = 16; 1.0%), hypothyroidism in self (N = 30; 1.4%) or first-degree family member (N = 91; 4.3%), significant past head injury or chronic neurological disorder (N = 50; 2.3%), failure to endorse willingness to
  • 8. abstain from recreational drugs for 48 h prior to the testing session (N = 369; 17.3%) or alcohol for 24 h prior to testing (N = 11; 0.5%), significant uncorrected vision impairment (N = 51; 2.4%), physical impairment in arms/hands (N = 4; 0.2%), or endorsing more than two items incorrectly on an 8-item Infrequency Scale (Jackson 1984; N = 16; 1.0%). The remaining 1131 participants received an invitation to participate in the lab-based portion of the task. However, only a minority of those participants completed the lab ses- sion (N = 79), as the online questionnaires were designed for a larger study examining relationships between those scales (currently unpublished). On the day of laboratory testing, all participants denied recent nicotine use, alcohol in past 24 h, and other recreational drug use in past 48 h. However, this was based on self-report as we did not have laboratory tests to confirm. Of the 79 participants, 77 had valid data for the STT, as two were excluded for sweetness ratings that did not approximate a linear increase with increasing concentrations and were sta- tistical outliers for that slope value (Z < −3.33 for each). An additional 5 participants were excluded for missing all anhe- donia measures and bio-demographic data (e.g., menstrual phase) due to experimenter error (both a priori reasons for excluding participants). This resulted in 72 participants used in final analyses (76% women; mean age: 19.11; SD = 2.07; range 18 to 28). For race, 75.0% identified as BWhite,^ 8.3% as BBlack,^ 8.3% as BAsian, 4.2% as BOther,^ 2.8% as BMixed,^ and one participant declined to answer. Independent of race, 34.7% identified as BHispanic/ Latino(a).^ Two participants reported selective-serotonin re- uptake inhibitor (SSRI) medication use at the time of testing. The remaining participants denied current psychotropic and narcotic medication use.
  • 9. Measures Self-Report Scales The Reinforcement Sensitivity Theory of Personality Questionnaire (RST-PQ; Corr and Cooper 2016) was admin- istered during the online phase, which consists of 65 state- ments with a response key asking the participant to rate how much the statement describes them in general - ranging from B1 - Not At All^ to B4 - Highly.^ The RST-PQ produces six scales - four behavioral activation system subscales: Reward Interest, Goal-Drive Persistence, Reward Reactivity, and Impulsivity, along with scales for the Behavioral Inhibition System (BIS) and Flight-Freeze- Avoidance System (FFAS). The reliability and validity of the RST-PQ have received ad- ditional recent support in chronic pain samples (Amiri et al. 2017), and through relationships found with event-related po- tentials from electroencephalogram (Kaye et al. 2018; De Pascalis et al. 2017). The Big Five Aspect Scales (BFAS; DeYoung et al. 2007) was also administered online, and con- sists of 100 statements with a response key asking the partic- ipant to rate how accurately the statement reflects how they generally are - ranging from B1 - Very Inaccurate^ to B5 - Very Accurate.^ The BFAS results in the traditional big five factors along with two aspects for each factor: Neuroticism (Volatility, Withdrawal), Agreeableness (Compassion, Politeness), Conscientiousness (Industriousness, Orderliness), Extraversion (Enthusiasm, Assertiveness), and Openness/Intellect (Openness, Intellect). The validity of BFAS aspect scales have been supported with specific J Psychopathol Behav Assess (2019) 41:235–248 237 relationships to psychopathology (Allen et al. 2018; Quilty
  • 10. et al. 2014) and self-reported personality disorder symptoms (DeYoung et al. 2016). During the day of STTassessment, we administered three anhedonia self-report scales - the 18-item Temporal Experience of Pleasure Scales, which includes subscales for anticipatory (TEPS-A) and consummatory (TEPS-C) anhedonia (Gard et al. 2007), the 17-item Anticipatory and Consummatory Interpersonal Pleasure Scale total score (ACIPS; Gooding and Pflum 2014), and the 14-item Snaith-Hamilton Pleasure Scale total score (SHAPS; Snaith et al. 1995). Unlike the TEPS, factor analysis has not supported separate factors for anticipatory and consum- matory anhedonia on the ACIPS (Gooding and Pflum 2014). Increased anhedonia is indicated by lower scores on the TEPS and ACIPS and higher scores on the SHAPS. The Cronbach alphas for the anhedonia scales in our sample were: TEPS-A (.76), TEPS-C (.75), ACIPS (.91), and SHAPS (.59). Sweet Taste Test (STT) The STT is a standardized assessment of hedonic response to sweet tastes in human participants (Kampov-Polevoy et al. 1997). Powdered sugar and distilled water were used to create five concentrations of sucrose solutions (approximately 0.05 M, 0.10 M, 0.19 M, 0.42 M, and 0.86 M). As a reference, Coca-Cola® is a 0.33 M solution. Participants received five trials of each of the five solutions in a blinded random order. During each trial, participants drank from a small plastic opaque cup containing 2 mL of solution, swished the solution in their mouth for about 5 s, and then expectorated into a container. Participants were asked to mark an BX^ on a 201 mm analog scale line to indicate perceived sweetness, from BNot sweet at all^ (left anchor) to BExtremely sweet^ (right anchor), and, on a separate scale, how much they liked it
  • 11. from BDislike it very much^ (left anchor) to BLike it very much^ (right anchor). Following the ratings, participants rinsed with distilled water and proceeded to the next trial. Ratings were then scored based on distance (in mm) of the BX^ from the left side of the respective scale line, and then averaged within the five concentrations. We used the slope of hedonic ratings by increasing sucrose concentration (i.e., hedonic slope) as the primary measure of dimensional hedonic ratings and any statisti- cally significant relationship with this dependent variable was then further explored across the separate hedonic rat- ings for the five concentrations. We also examined rela- tionships with the categorical Bsweet liker^ status, defined as having the highest hedonic rating for the sweetest concentration (Kampov-Polevoy et al. 2001, 2003; Wronski et al. 2007). Finally, we included the sweet sen- sitivity slope to clarify specificity of any findings with the hedonic variables. The hedonic and sweet sensitivity slopes and the five individual concentration hedonic rat- ing values all showed relatively normal distributions with skewness and kurtosis <1.05. Menstrual Cycle Stage Identification We asked all female participants to identify the current day in their menstrual cycle using day one defined by the start of last menstruation. We also asked women if they thought they may be pregnant, which all denied. We created a Bluteal menstrual phase status^ variable which classified women reporting being in the mid-to- late luteal menstrual stage during the day of testing, de- fined as days 20 to 28 from start of last mentruation, with all remaining women and men combined in the second of the two categories. Although reward processing studies
  • 12. with rhesus macaques typically chose a late luteal window around days 24 to 27 to optimally isolate the rise in pro- gesterone (Carroll et al. 2016), we chose a wider mid-to- late luteal phase window to allow for inherent error in estimating the current day of cycle based on self-report/ memory, and to allow for inclusion of a sufficient number of females in this subgroup for statistical analyses. Similarly, we created a Bfollicular menstrual phase status^ variable which classified women being in the mid-to-late follicular phase during the day of testing, which we de- fined as days 5 to 12 following the onset of menstruation. This window is also somewhat wider than the follicular window typically used in the rhesus monkey studies (7 to 10 days following menstruation; Carroll et al. 2016), based on the same rationale. Procedures This study was approved by the Institutional Review Board of the authors’ university. Research followed ethical principles described in the Declaration of Helsinki. Participants provided informed consent at the beginning of both online and lab- based sessions. During the lab-based session, participants completed demographic information, including questions about current medication and supplement use, the anhedonia questionnaires, and the STT, as part of a larger study. No data resulting from the larger study have been published at this time. 238 J Psychopathol Behav Assess (2019) 41:235–248 Statistical Analyses For self-report measures, one participant was missing the
  • 13. TEPS and ACIPS. Across measures, if either one or two items for a subscale/factor were skipped by a particular participant, the respective score was then based on the average of the remaining items for that factor. IBM SPSS Statistics (Version 25) was used for all analyses. Initial regressions examined relationships of the STT he- donic and sweet sensitivity slopes and sweet liker catego- ry with simultaneous entry of: age, sex, luteal menstrual phase status, follicular menstrual phase status, birth con- trol medication use, race, ethnicity, and time of day during testing (variance inflation factor [VIF] < 1.60). Time of day during testing was rounded to nearest hour (mode = 4:00 p.m.; range = 9:00 a.m. to 4:00 p.m.). This was ex- amined as an approximation of fluctuations in the circa- dian system (e.g., hormone levels), which can influence behavioral responses to rewards (DePoy et al. 2017). Additional regressions were used to examine sets of predictors on each of the three STT variables, the: 1) four anhedonia scales: TEPS-A, TEPS-C, ACIPS, and SHAPS, 2) six factor scores from the RST-PQ, 3) five BFAS fac- tors, and 4) ten BFAS aspects. Simultaneous entry was used for all predictor sets (VIF < 3.10 for all sets). If a predictor showed a significant relationship with the he- donic slope, this was followed by an exploratory analysis of the five concentration hedonic ratings that comprised the slope, using stepwise entry, regressed on the respec- tive predictor. All regressions involving the sweet liker category used binary logistic regressions, while the re- maining analyses on dimensional STT variables used linear regression. Across all regressions, there were no statistical outliers, as defined by a Studentized residual >3.0. Results
  • 14. See Fig. 1 for a depiction of the hedonic slope by sweet liker status. Descriptive statistics and zero-correlations for STT var- iables with each of the predictor sets can be found in Tables 1 (anhedonia scales), 2 (RST-PQ scales), 3 (BFAS factors), and 4 (BFAS aspects). Thirty participants (42%) were classified as Bsweet likers.^ Excluding the two participants taking SSRI medications did not change the pattern of significant findings across all analyses. They were therefore retained in all results presented below. STT Relationships with Menstrual Phase Status, Demographic Variables, and Time of Day during Testing We examined the three STT variables using regressions with simultaneous entry of sex, luteal menstrual phase status (N = 13), follicular menstrual phase status (N = 13), birth control medication use (N = 19), age, race, ethnicity, and time of day during testing. As shown in Table 5, women in the mid-to-late luteal menstrual phase had a smaller hedonic slope, driven by lower hedonic ratings to the sweetest concentration (see Fig. 2), and were less likely to be categorical sweet likers (15%), as compared to the combination of the remaining women and men (47.5%). Results also revealed that women had a larger sweet sensitivity slope than men (see Table 5). There were no other significant relationships across these re- gressions. Therefore, we included covariates of luteal men- strual phase status and sex in the first block of all remaining regressions. STT Relationships with Anhedonia See Table 5 for summary of all findings, including unstan- dardized beta values with standard error and level of statis- tical significance. For anhedonia, the hedonic slope was
  • 15. predicted by only the TEPS-C subscale, which was driven by a positive relationship between the TEPS-C scores and hedonic rating scores to the sweetest concentration (see Fig. 3). Thus, individuals self-reporting more consummato- ry anhedonia (i.e., a lower TEPS-C score) had a smaller hedonic slope and rated the sweetest concentration as less pleasant. The SST sweet sensitivity slope and sweet liker status did not relate to anhedonia scales. Removal of the Fig. 1 Hedonic slope lines from hedonic rating scores by sucrose concentration for sweet likers and sweet dislikers. Error bars represent the 95% confidence interval J Psychopathol Behav Assess (2019) 41:235–248 239 SHAPS from the regressions, which had poor internal reli- ability (α = .59), did not change the pattern of significance. To examine specificity of consummatory vs. anticipa- tory anxiety, we conducted regressions in which sex, lu- teal phase status, and TEPS-A were entered in the first block, followed by TEPS-C in the second block. TEPS- C remained statistically significant for both hedonic slope, unstandardized B = 36.81, SE = 18.15, p = .047, and the hedonic rating to the sweetest concentration, unstandard- ized B = 25.25, SE = 8.80, p = .006. As all four measures of anhedonia were not significantly related to these STT variables in zero-order correlations (see Table 1), we also explored specificity of TEPS-C by entering each anhedo- nia scale in individual regressions, after entering sex and luteal phase status in block 1 – thus removing the effect of multicollinearity among the anhedonia measures. These
  • 16. analyses showed that of the four anhedonia scales, only the TEPS-C related to the hedonic slope or hedonic rating to the sweetest concentration. STT Relationships with Personality Factors For the RST-PQ, the hedonic slope was predicted by the Flight-Freeze-Avoidance System (FFAS) score, which was driven by a negative relationship between the FFAS score and the hedonic rating to the least sweet concentration (see Fig. 4). In addition, the sweet sensitivity slope was predict- ed by RST-PQ Goal-Drive Persistence and BFAS Extraversion and Conscientiousness factors (all in the pos- itive direction). The BFAS aspect scales did not relate to any STT variable when entered simultaneously in the regressions. Examination of Change in Results with Retention of Excluded Participants When repeating all analyses with inclusion of the seven participants who were excluded (N = 79), the pattern of significance across analyses did not change with two mi- nor exceptions: For the RST-PQ FFFS, the hedonic slope relationship changed to a trend level (p = .065), although the FFFS relationship with the hedonic rating to the least sweet concentration remained significant (p = .003). For the BFAS factor relationships with the sweet sensitivity slope, Conscientiousness remained significant (p = .04), but Extraversion was no longer significant (p = .31). In these analyses, all seven of the previously excluded par- ticipants had RST-PQ and BFAS data, but five were miss- ing all anhedonia measures and bio-demographic data (e.g., menstrual phase), and two had sweet sensitivity slopes >3.00 SD below the sample mean.Ta
  • 37. ee te st co n ce n tr at io n 240 J Psychopathol Behav Assess (2019) 41:235–248 Ta b le 2 D es cr ip ti v e st at is ti cs an d
  • 61. at io n J Psychopathol Behav Assess (2019) 41:235–248 241 Discussion As hypothesized, participants who self-reported more con- summatory anhedonia (i.e., lower TEPS-C scores) had a re- duced hedonic slope that was driven by a reduction in the hedonic rating for the sweetest solution (see Fig. 3). This find- ing was specific to consummatory anhedonia as the TEPS-C continued to show these significant relationships after entering TEPS-A in the first block of the regression. In addition, indi- vidual exploratory regressions with each of the other anhedo- nia measures were not significant. This finding with the TEPS- C is broadly consistent with a previous study which found reduced hedonic ratings to the STT in a transdiagnostic psy- chiatric sample related to increased anhedonia from the Chapman Physical Anhedonia Scale, although that study fo- cused on the maximum hedonic rating across all concentra- tions (Berlin et al. 1998). Physical anhedonia is lack of plea- sure from physical sensations, which partially overlaps the TEPS as it assesses lack of pleasure across a wider variety of stimuli. The current finding extends this earlier work by find- ing the relationship in a nonpsychiatric sample, and supports the current inclusion of the TEPS-C and Btaste reactivity^ in the RDoC IRR domain. As both the TEPS-A and the ACIPS contain items regarding anticipatory anhedonia, the specificity of our finding to the TEPS-C suggests that questions regarding consummatory pleasure more precisely reflect IRR. Although the SHAPS questions also reflect consummatory pleasure, the directions to the SHAPS asks participants to answer based on
  • 62. the Blast few days,^ while the TEPS-C asks participants to answer how they feel Bin general.^ Therefore, it may be the more trait-like aspects of consummatory pleasure that correlate with individual differences in IRR. Also consistent with our hypotheses, women who report- ed being in the mid-to-late luteal menstrual cycle phase (days 20 to 28), had a lower hedonic slope, driven by lower hedonic ratings for the sweetest concentration (see Fig. 2), and were less likely to be categorical sweet likers, as com- pared to the remaining women and men (see Table 5). This finding is similar to the one other study that examined this relationship (Elliott et al. 2015), but extends their limited methodology of using one sample of four sucrose solutions and no male participants, to the use of the standardized and validated STT and a sample containing both women and men. These findings provide further support for the STT representing IRR, as earlier work has found decreased he- donic ratings following smoked cocaine in women during the luteal, compared with follicular, phase (Evans et al. 2002; Sofuoglu et al. 1999). We did not find any STT rela- tionships with the follicular menstrual phase (days 5–12), consistent with a report that exogenously-administered es- tradiol, which is naturally higher in the follicular phase, did not affect self-administration of cocaine in either intact or ovariectomized rhesus monkeys (Mello et al. 2008). Our hypotheses regarding the relationship between STT hedonic ratings and personality traits of neuroticism and im- pulsivity were not supported, as there were no statistically significant relationships between STT hedonic ratings and BFAS Neuroticism and RST-PQ Impulsivity factor scores. While this is somewhat inconsistent with the existing research on sweet tastes and personality traits, none of the previous
  • 63. Table 3 Descriptive statistics and zero-order correlations among Big Five Aspect Scale factor scores and sweet taste test variables 1 2 3 4 5 1. BFAS Neuroticism Factor 2.78 (0.64) 2. BFAS Agreeableness Factor .01 4.07 (0.47) 3. BFAS Conscientiousness Factor −.37** .23 3.61 (0.52) 4. BFAS Extraversion Factor −.28* .29* .30* 3.47 (0.62) 5. BFAS Openness/Intellect Factor −.25* .27* .32** .56*** 3.75 (0.48) 6. SST Hedonic Slope −.05 .07 .08 .12 .21 7. SST Categorical Sweet Liker^ .16 .21 −.03 .01 .11 8. SST Hedonic Rating to Sweetest −.05 −.01 .07 .15 .25* 9. SST Hedonic Rating to Least Sweet .06 −.17 −.09 −.09 −.13 10. SST Sweet Sensitivity Slope .10 .13 .32** .30* .21 Descriptive statistics on the outer diagonal in format: mean (standard deviation); remaining values are Pearson’s or point-biserial r values Descriptive statistics for the BFAS are for the average item- level score in a given factor (ranging from 1 to 5) Intercorrelations among SST variables depicted in Table 1 BFAS Big Five Aspects Scale, STT Sweet Taste Test * p < .05, ** p < .01, *** p < .001 ^ Defined by highest hedonic rating for sweetest concentration
  • 64. 242 J Psychopathol Behav Assess (2019) 41:235–248 Ta b le 4 D es cr ip ti v e st at is ti cs an d ze ro -o rd er co rr el at io
  • 94. sw ee te st co n ce n tr at io n J Psychopathol Behav Assess (2019) 41:235–248 243 Ta b le 5 R es u lt s of li n ea r an d
  • 127. ss io n s, al l p re d ic to rs en te re d si m u lt an eo u sl y 244 J Psychopathol Behav Assess (2019) 41:235–248 studies used the STT or controlled for sex/luteal menstrual phase, but instead used behavioral tests involving varying sweetness levels of wine (Saliba et al. 2009; Sena-Esteves et al. 2018) or general self-reported tendencies to like sweet food and beverages (Kikuchi and Watanabe 2000; Elfhag and Erlanson-Albertsson 2006).
  • 128. We found one unexpected personality trait predicted STT hedonic ratings, a positive relationship between the hedonic slope with RST-PQ FFAS factor scores, driven by a negative relationship between the hedonic rating to the least sweet concentration and the FFAS score (see Fig. 4). The Flight- Freeze-Avoidance System (FFAS) factor from the RST-PQ is intended to represent a punishment sensitivity system which controls fear-related behaviors to avoidable aversive stimuli, such as fleeing, active avoidance, and freezing (Corr and Cooper 2016). While speculative, our finding is similar to a report that individuals with a family history of alcoholism had greater amygdala fMRI activation after tast- ing a sucrose solution than those without a family history, which was significant for the low (0.10 M), but not high (0.83 M), concentration (Eiler et al. 2017). As the amygdala is a primary region for processing fear (Davis and Reijmers 2018), and fear is reflected by the FFAS, this specificity of amygdala activation to the least sweet solution is broadly similar to our finding that individuals with greater FFAS had lower ratings to the least sweet solution. We found exploratory personality traits that predicted the sweet sensitivity slope, which is thought to reflect gustatory sensory processes rather than IRR (Eikemo et al. 2016). These included positive relationships with RST-PQ Goal-Drive Persistence and BFAS Extraversion and Conscientiousness factor scores. Interestingly, the BFAS aspect scales constitut- ing the Extraversion and Conscientiousness factors did not relate to the sweet sensitivity slope. This may be due to the two aspects of a given factor showing similar relationships and the resulting shared variance being removed due to use of simultaneous entry in the regressions. From inspection of the zero-order correlations on Table 4, both aspects of Conscientiousness (i.e., Industriousness and Orderliness)
  • 129. showed significant positive relationships with the sweet sen- sitivity slope. For aspects of Extraversion, Assertiveness was statistically significant (p = .007), while Enthusiasm was a similar effect size but at a statistical trend level (p = .055). It is possible that individuals higher in conscientiousness and/or goal drive persistence were more thoughtful and reliable in recording the sweetness level which then resulted in a stronger slope value. Similarly, it is possible that individual differences Fig. 2 Hedonic slope lines from hedonic rating scores by sucrose concentration for women in the mid-to-late luteal menstrual phase and the combination of the remaining women and men. Error bars represent the 95% confidence interval Fig. 3 Scatterplot of the consummatory subscale score from the temporal experience of pleasure scales (unstandardized residuals after covarying for sex and luteal phase) by the hedonic rating scores for the sweet concentration. Shaded region represents 95% confidence interval J Psychopathol Behav Assess (2019) 41:235–248 245 in brain mechanisms related to extraversion also confer in- creased gustatory discrimination of sweetness concentrations. As these were exploratory findings, replication is needed be- fore such speculation can be translated into detailed theory.
  • 130. The study is limited by a moderate sample size, use of undergraduate students as participants, and a larger number of women than men. We also did not ask participants about self-reported hunger or time since last meal, which can affect hedonic responses to sweet stimuli (Cabanac and Lafrance 1990). Other limitations are inherent in the pre-existing stan- dardized STT methods, such as the lack of a control solution (e.g., water with no sucrose) and use of analog scales that introduce confounds instead of a scale such as the generalized Labeled Magnitude Scale (Bartoshuk et al. 2004). Additionally, it would be optimal to measure the same women longitudinally across their menstrual phases to avoid potential confounds. However, a strength is assessing and accounting for female menstrual phase status in the analyses. The study provides what appears to be the first evidence that the STT, particularly hedonic ratings of the sweetest concentration, are reduced in individuals with elevated consummatory anhedo- nia and in women who are in the mid-to-late luteal phase of their menstrual cycle. If replicated, the current results and the recent finding regarding μ-opioid specificity (Eikemo et al. 2016), suggest that Btaste reactivity,^ currently listed under the RDoC IRR BBehaviors,^ be replaced with a more specific inclusion of the hedonic rating to the sweetest concentration from the STT under BParadigms.^ Our finding also supports the validity of the TEPS-C as an index of individual differ- ences in consummatory anhedonia/IRR, which, given that it is a brief self-report scale, has considerable translational value for clinical and research applications. Similarly, our findings suggest that future research on IRR/consummatory anhedonia account for the potential influence of menstrual cycle phase in female participants. Acknowledgments The authors would like to thank the following research
  • 131. assistants for their help on this project: Ashley Lee Lum, Pamela Askins, Doniya Milani, Jasmine Montero, Nicholas Picanso, and Brooke Fenton. Compliance with Ethical Standards Conflict of Interest Jeffrey S. Bedwell, Christopher C. Spencer, Cristina A. Chirino, and John P. O’Donnell report no conflicts of interest related to this manuscript and study. Experiment Participants Research followed ethical principles described in the Declaration of Helsinki. Ethical Approval This study was approved by the Institutional Review Board of the University of Central Florida (IRB #: SBE-16- 12,678). Informed Consent All participicipants provided informed consent for both the online and laboratory portions of this study. Publisher’s Note Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdic- tional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations. References Allen, T. A., Carey, B. E., McBride, C., Bagby, R. M., DeYoung, C. G., & Quilty, L. C. (2018). Big Five aspects of personality interact to predict depression. Journal of Personality, 86, 714–725. https://
  • 132. doi.org/10.1111/jopy.12352. Fig. 4 Scatterplot of the flight- freeze-avoidance system factor score from the reinforcement sensitivity theory personality questionnaire (unstandardized re- siduals after covarying for sex and luteal phase) by the hedonic rating scores for the least sweet concen- tration. Shaded region represents 95% confidence interval 246 J Psychopathol Behav Assess (2019) 41:235–248 https://doi.org/10.1111/jopy.12352 https://doi.org/10.1111/jopy.12352 American Psychiatric Association. (2013). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders; DSM-5. Washington, D.C.: American Psychiatric Association. Amiri, S., Behnezhad, S., & Azad-Marzabadi, E. (2017). Psychometric properties revised reinforcement sensitivity theory (r-RST) scale in chronic pain patients. Journal of Pain Research, 10, 1879–1885. https://doi.org/10.2147/jpr.s132133. Arbisi, P. A., Billington, C. J., & Levine, A. S. (1999). The effect of naltrexone on taste detection and recognition threshold. Appetite,
  • 133. 32(2), 241–249. https://doi.org/10.1006/appe.1998.0217. Ashton, M. C., Pilkington, A. C., & Lee, K. (2014). Do prosocial people prefer sweet-tasting foods? An attempted replication of Meier, Moeller, Riemer-Peltz, and Robinson (2012). Journal of Research in Personality, 52, 42–46. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jrp.2014.06.006. Bartoshuk, L. M., Duffy, V. B., Green, B. G., Hoffman, H. J., Ko, C. W., Lucchina, L. A., Marks, L. E., Snyder, D. J., & Weiffenbach, J. M. (2004). Valid across-group comparisons with labeled scales: The gLMS versus magnitude matching. Physiology & Behavior, 82(1), 109–114. Berlin, I., Givry-Steiner, L., Lecrubier, Y., & Puech, A. J. (1998). Measures of anhedonia and hedonic responses to sucrose in depres- sive and schizophrenic patients in comparison with healthy subjects. European Psychiatry, 13(6), 303–309. https://doi.org/10.1016/ s0924-9338(98)80048-5. Bowen, D. J., & Grunberg, N. E. (1990). Variations in food preference and consumption across the menstrual cycle. Physiology & Behavior, 47(2), 287–291. https://doi.org/10.1016/0031- 9384(90) 90144-S.
  • 134. Cabanac, M., & Lafrance, L. (1990). Postingestive alliesthesia: The rat tells the same story. Physiology & Behavior, 47(3), 539–543. Carroll, M. E., Collins, M., Kohl, E. A., Johnson, S., & Dougen, B. (2016). Sex and menstrual cycle effects on chronic oral cocaine self-administration in rhesus monkeys: Effects of a nondrug alterna- tive reward (journal article). Psychopharmacology, 233(15), 2973– 2984. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00213-016-4343-5. Castro, D. C., & Berridge, K. C. (2017). Opioid and orexin hedonic hotspots in rat orbitofrontal cortex and insula. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 114(43), E9125–e9134. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1705753114. Corr, P. J., & Cooper, A. J. (2016). The reinforcement sensitivity theory of personality questionnaire (RST-PQ): Development and validation. Psychological Assessment, 28(11), 1427–1440. https://doi.org/10. 1037/pas0000273. Davis, P., & Reijmers, L. G. (2018). The dynamic nature of fear engrams in the basolateral amygdala. Brain Research Bulletin, 141, 44– 49. De Pascalis, V., Fracasso, F., & Corr, P. J. (2017). The behavioral ap-
  • 135. proach system and augmenting/reducing in auditory event- related potentials during emotional visual stimulation. Biological Psychology, 123, 310–323. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsycho. 2016.10.015. DePoy, L. M., McClung, C. A., & Logan, R. W. (2017). Neural mecha- nisms of circadian regulation of natural and drug reward. Neural Plasticity, 2017, 5720842. https://doi.org/10.1155/2017/5720842. DeYoung, C. G., Quilty, L. C., & Peterson, J. B. (2007). Between facets and domains: 10 aspects of the big five. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 93(5), 880–896. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022- 3514.93.5.880. DeYoung, C. G., Carey, B. E., Krueger, R. F., & Ross, S. R. (2016). Ten aspects of the big five in the personality inventory for DSM-5. Personal Disorder, 7(2), 113–123. https://doi.org/10.1037/ per0000170. Dichter, G. S., Smoski, M. J., Kampov-Polevoy, A. B., Gallop, R., & Garbutt, J. C. (2010). Unipolar depression does not moderate re- sponses to the sweet taste test. Depression and Anxiety, 27(9), 859–863. https://doi.org/10.1002/da.20690. Eikemo, M., Loseth, G. E., Johnstone, T., Gjerstad, J., Willoch, F., & Leknes, S. (2016). Sweet taste pleasantness is modulated by mor-
  • 136. phine and naltrexone. Psychopharmacology, 233(21–22), 3711– 3723. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00213-016-4403-x. Eiler, W. J. A., Dzemidzic, M., Soeurt, C. M., Carron, C. R., Oberline, B. G., Considine, R. V., Harezlak, J., & Kareken, D. A. (2017). Family history of alcoholism and the human brain response to oral sucrose. Neuroimage Clinical, 17, 1036–1046. Elfhag, K., & Erlanson-Albertsson, C. (2006). Sweet and fat taste prefer- ence in obesity have different associations with personality and eat- ing behavior. Physiology & Behavior, 88(1), 61–66. https://doi.org/ 10.1016/j.physbeh.2006.03.006. Elliott, S. A., Ng, J., Leow, M. K., & Henry, C. J. (2015). The influence of the menstrual cycle on energy balance and taste preference in Asian Chinese women. European Journal of Nutrition, 54(8), 1323– 1332. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00394-014-0812-y. Evans, S. M., & Foltin, R. W. (2006). Exogenous progesterone attenuates the subjective effects of smoked cocaine in women, but not in men. Neuropsychopharmacology, 31(3), 659–674. https://doi.org/10. 1038/sj.npp.1300887. Evans, S. M., Haney, M., & Foltin, R. W. (2002). The effects of smoked
  • 137. cocaine during the follicular and luteal phases of the menstrual cycle in women. Psychopharmacology, 159(4), 397–406. https://doi.org/ 10.1007/s00213-001-0944-7. Garbutt, J. C., Osborne, M., Gallop, R., Barkenbus, J., Grace, K., Cody, M., Flannery, B., & Kampov-Polevoy, A. B. (2009). Sweet liking phenotype, alcohol craving and response to naltrexone treatment in alcohol dependence. Alcohol and Alcoholism, 44(3), 293–300. https://doi.org/10.1093/alcalc/agn122. Garbutt, J. C., Kampov-Polevoy, A. B., Kalka-Juhl, L. S., & Gallop, R. J. (2016). Association of the Sweet-Liking Phenotype and Craving for alcohol with the response to naltrexone treatment in alcohol depen- dence: A randomized clinical trial. JAMA Psychiatry, 73(10), 1056– 1063. https://doi.org/10.1001/jamapsychiatry.2016.2157. Gard, D. E., Kring, A. M., Gard, M. G., Horan, W. P., & Green, M. F. (2007). Anhedonia in schizophrenia: Distinctions between anticipa- tory and consummatory pleasure. Schizophrenia Research, 93(1–3), 253–260. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.schres.2007.03.008. Gooding, D. C., & Pflum, M. J. (2014). The assessment of interpersonal pleasure: Introduction of the anticipatory and Consummatory
  • 138. inter- personal pleasure scale (ACIPS) and preliminary findings. Psychiatry Research, 215(1), 237–243. https://doi.org/10.1016/j. psychres.2013.10.012. Goodman, E. L., Breithaupt, L., Watson, H. J., Peat, C. M., Baker, J. H., Bulik, C. M., & Brownley, K. A. (2017). Sweet taste preference in binge-eating disorder: A preliminary investigation. Eating Behaviors, 28, 8–15. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.eatbeh.2017.11.005. Insel, T. R., & Cuthbert, B. N. (2015). Medicine. Brain disorders? Precisely. Science, 348, 499–500. Insel, T., Cuthbert, B., Garvey, M., Heinssen, R., Pine, D. S., Quinn, K., Sanislow, C., & Wang, P. (2010). Research domain criteria (RDoC): Toward a new classification framework for research on mental dis- orders. The American Journal of Psychiatry, 167(7), 748–751. https://doi.org/10.1176/appi.ajp.2010.09091379. Jackson, D. N. (1984). Personality Research Form Manual. Port Heron: Research Psychologists Press. J Psychopathol Behav Assess (2019) 41:235–248 247 https://doi.org/10.2147/jpr.s132133 https://doi.org/10.1006/appe.1998.0217 https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jrp.2014.06.006 https://doi.org/10.1016/s0924-9338(98)80048-5
  • 139. https://doi.org/10.1016/s0924-9338(98)80048-5 https://doi.org/10.1016/0031-9384(90)90144-S https://doi.org/10.1016/0031-9384(90)90144-S https://doi.org/10.1007/s00213-016-4343-5. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1705753114 https://doi.org/10.1037/pas0000273 https://doi.org/10.1037/pas0000273 https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsycho.2016.10.015 https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsycho.2016.10.015 https://doi.org/10.1155/2017/5720842 https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.93.5.880 https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.93.5.880 https://doi.org/10.1037/per0000170 https://doi.org/10.1037/per0000170 https://doi.org/10.1002/da.20690 https://doi.org/10.1007/s00213-016-4403-x https://doi.org/10.1016/j.physbeh.2006.03.006 https://doi.org/10.1016/j.physbeh.2006.03.006 https://doi.org/10.1007/s00394-014-0812-y https://doi.org/10.1038/sj.npp.1300887 https://doi.org/10.1038/sj.npp.1300887 https://doi.org/10.1007/s00213-001-0944-7 https://doi.org/10.1007/s00213-001-0944-7 https://doi.org/10.1093/alcalc/agn122 https://doi.org/10.1001/jamapsychiatry.2016.2157 https://doi.org/10.1016/j.schres.2007.03.008 https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psychres.2013.10.012 https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psychres.2013.10.012 https://doi.org/10.1016/j.eatbeh.2017.11.005. https://doi.org/10.1176/appi.ajp.2010.09091379 Janowsky, D. S., Pucilowski, O., & Buyinza, M. (2003). Preference for higher sucrose concentrations in cocaine abusing-dependent pa- tients. Journal of Psychiatric Research, 37(1), 35–41.
  • 140. Kampov-Polevoy, A., Garbutt, J. C., & Janowsky, D. (1997). Evidence of preference for a high-concentration sucrose solution in alcoholic men. The American Journal of Psychiatry, 154(2), 269–270. https://doi.org/10.1176/ajp.154.2.269. Kampov-Polevoy, A. B., Tsoi, M. V., Zvartau, E. E., Neznanov, N. G., & Khalitov, E. (2001). Sweet liking and family history of alcoholism in hospitalized alcoholic and non-alcoholic patients. Alcohol and Alcoholism, 36(2), 165–170. Kampov-Polevoy, A. B., Ziedonis, D., Steinberg, M. L., Pinsky, I., Krejci, J., Eick, C., Boland, G., Khalitov, E., & Crews, F. T. (2003). Association between sweet preference and paternal history of alcoholism in psychiatric and substance abuse patients. Alcoholism, Clinical and Experimental Research, 27(12), 1929– 1936. https://doi.org/10.1097/01.alc.0000099265.60216.23. Kaye, S. A., White, M. J., & Lewis, I. (2018). Young females' attention toward road safety images: An ERP study of the revised reinforce- ment sensitivity theory. Traffic Injury Prevention, 19(2), 201– 206. https://doi.org/10.1080/15389588.2017.1369531. Kikuchi, Y., & Watanabe, S. (2000). Personality and dietary habits. Journal of Epidemiology, 10(3), 191–198.
  • 141. Lange, L. A., Kampov-Polevoy, A. B., & Garbutt, J. C. (2010). Sweet liking and high novelty seeking: Independent phenotypes associated with alcohol-related problems† (article). Alcohol and Alcoholism, 45(5), 431–436. https://doi.org/10.1093/alcalc/agq040. Langleben, D. D., Busch, E. L., O'Brien, C. P., & Elman, I. (2012). Depot naltrexone decreases rewarding properties of sugar in patients with opioid dependence. Psychopharmacology, 220(3), 559–564. https:// doi.org/10.1007/s00213-011-2503-1. Lee, C. W., & Ho, I. K. (2013). Sex differences in opioid analgesia and addiction: Interactions among opioid receptors and estrogen recep- tors. Molecular Pain, 9, 45. McCabe, C., Mishor, Z., Cowen, P. J., & Harmer, C. J. (2010). Diminished neural processing of aversive and rewarding stimuli during selective serotonin reuptake inhibitor treatment. Biological Psychiatry, 67(5), 439–445. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsych. 2009.11.001. Meier, B. P., Moeller, S. K., Riemer-Peltz, M., & Robinson, M. D. (2012). Sweet taste preferences and experiences predict prosocial infer- ences, personalities, and behaviors. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 102(1), 163–174. https://doi.org/10.1037/ a0025253.
  • 142. Mello, N. K., Negus, S. S., Knudson, I. M., Kelly, M., & Mendelson, J. H. (2008). Effects of estradiol on cocaine self-administration and co- caine discrimination by female rhesus monkeys. Neuropsychopharmacology, 33, 783–795. Mello, N. K., Knudson, I. M., Kelly, M., Fivel, P. A., & Medelson, J. H. (2011). Effects of progesterone and testosterone on cocaine self- administration and cocaine discrimination by female rhesus mon- keys. Neuropsychopharmacology, 36, 2187–2199. Mulligan, E. M., Nelson, B. D., Infantolino, Z. P., Luking, K. R., Sharma, R., & Hajcak, G. (2018). Effects of menstrual cycle phase on electrocortical response to reward and depressive symptoms in women. Psychophysiology, 55, e13268. https://doi.org/10.1111/ psyp.13268. Quilty, L. C., DeYoung, C. G., Oakman, J. M., & Bagby, R. M. (2014). Extraversion and behavioral activation: Integrating the components of approach (Article). Journal of Personality Assessment, 96(1), 87– 94. https://doi.org/10.1080/00223891.2013.834440. Reynolds, W. (1982). Development of reliable and valid short forms of the Marlowe-Crowne social desirability scale. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 38(1), 119–125.
  • 143. Sagioglou, C., & Greitemeyer, T. (2016). Individual differences in bitter taste preferences are associated with antisocial personality traits. Appetite, 96, 299–308. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.appet.2015.09.031. Saliba, A. J., Wragg, K., & Richardson, P. (2009). Sweet taste preference and personality traits using a white wine (Article). Food Quality and Preference, 20(8), 572–575. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.foodqual. 2009.05.009. Schroeder, J. A., Niculescu, M., & Unterwald, E. M. (2003). Cocaine alters mu but not delta or kappa opioid receptor-stimulated in situ [35S]GTPgammaS binding in rat brain. Synapse, 47(1), 26–32. https://doi.org/10.1002/syn.10148. Sena-Esteves, M. M., Mota, M., & Malfeito-Ferreira, M. (2018). Patterns of sweetness preference in red wine according to consumer charac- terisation (Article). Food Research International, 106, 38–44. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.foodres.2017.12.043. Snaith, R. P., Hamilton, M., Morley, S., Humayan, A., Hargreaves, D., & Trigwell, P. (1995). A scale for the assessment of hedonic tone the Snaith-Hamilton pleasure scale. The British Journal of Psychiatry, 167(1), 99–103.
  • 144. Strauss, G. P., & Cohen, A. S. (2017). A transdiagnostic review of neg- ative symptom phenomenology and etiology. Schizophrenia Bulletin, 43, 712–719. Sofuoglu, M., Dudish-Poulsen, S., Nelson, D., Pentel, P. R., & Hatsukami, D. K. (1999). Sex and menstrual cycle differences in the subjective effects from smoked cocaine in humans. Experimental and Clinical Psychopharmacology, 7(3), 274–283. Treadway, M. T., & Zald, D. H. (2011). Reconsidering anhedonia in depression: Lessons from translational neuroscience. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 35(3), 537–555. https://doi.org/10. 1016/j.neubiorev.2010.06.006. Tsypes, A., Owens, M., Hajcak, G., & Gibb, B. E. (2018). Neural reward responsiveness in children who engage in nonsuicidal self- injury: an ERP study. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 59, 1289– 1297. https://doi.org/10.1111/jcpp.12919. Tucci, S. A., Murphy, L. E., Boyland, E. J., Dye, L., & Halford, J. C. (2010). Oral contraceptive effects on food choice during the follic- ular and luteal phases of the menstrual cycle. A laboratory based study. Appetite, 55(3), 388–392. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.appet. 2010.06.005. Vittengl, J. R., Clark, L. A., Thase, M. E., & Jarrett, R. B.
  • 145. (2015). Predictors of longitudinal outcomes after unstable response to acute-phase cognitive therapy for major depressive disorder. Psychotherapy (Chicago, Ill.), 52(2), 268–277. https://doi.org/10. 1037/pst0000021. Weafer, J., Burkhardt, A., & de Wit, H. (2014). Sweet taste liking is associated with impulsive behaviors in humans. Frontiers in Behavioral Neuroscience, 8, 228. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnbeh. 2014.00228. Weafer, J., Lyon, N., Hedeker, D., & de Wit, H. (2017). Sweet taste liking is associated with subjective response to amphetamine in women but not men. Psychopharmacology, 234(21), 3185–3194. https://doi. org/10.1007/s00213-017-4702-x. Wronski, M., Skrok-Wolska, D., Samochowiec, J., Ziolkowski, M., Swiecicki, L., Bienkowski, P., et al. (2007). Perceived intensity and pleasantness of sucrose taste in male alcoholics. Alcohol and Alcoholism, 42(2), 75–79. https://doi.org/10.1093/alcalc/agl097. 248 J Psychopathol Behav Assess (2019) 41:235–248 https://doi.org/10.1176/ajp.154.2.269 https://doi.org/10.1097/01.alc.0000099265.60216.23 https://doi.org/10.1080/15389588.2017.1369531 https://doi.org/10.1093/alcalc/agq040. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00213-011-2503-1 https://doi.org/10.1007/s00213-011-2503-1
  • 146. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsych.2009.11.001 https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsych.2009.11.001 https://doi.org/10.1037/a0025253 https://doi.org/10.1037/a0025253 https://doi.org/10.1111/psyp.13268 https://doi.org/10.1111/psyp.13268 https://doi.org/10.1080/00223891.2013.834440. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.appet.2015.09.031 https://doi.org/10.1016/j.foodqual.2009.05.009 https://doi.org/10.1016/j.foodqual.2009.05.009 https://doi.org/10.1002/syn.10148 https://doi.org/10.1016/j.foodres.2017.12.043. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2010.06.006 https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2010.06.006 https://doi.org/10.1111/jcpp.12919 https://doi.org/10.1016/j.appet.2010.06.005 https://doi.org/10.1016/j.appet.2010.06.005 https://doi.org/10.1037/pst0000021 https://doi.org/10.1037/pst0000021 https://doi.org/10.3389/fnbeh.2014.00228 https://doi.org/10.3389/fnbeh.2014.00228 https://doi.org/10.1007/s00213-017-4702-x https://doi.org/10.1007/s00213-017-4702-x https://doi.org/10.1093/alcalc/agl097.The Sweet Taste Test: Relationships with Anhedonia Subtypes, Personality Traits, and Menstrual Cycle PhasesAbstractIntroductionMethodsParticipantsMeasuresSelf- Report ScalesSweet Taste Test (STT)Menstrual Cycle Stage IdentificationProceduresStatistical AnalysesResultsSTT Relationships with Menstrual Phase Status, Demographic Variables, and Time of Day during TestingSTT Relationships with AnhedoniaSTT Relationships with Personality FactorsExamination of Change in Results with Retention of Excluded ParticipantsDiscussionReferences