SlideShare a Scribd company logo
1 of 108
Take Home Portion
Directions: Problems (150pts). This part, i.e. the Problems
section is ‘open internet’ meaning that you can use any online
source, physical book, slides from class, or personal notes on
the take home portion. Doing so will result in a significant
grade penalty (an F for the course). I will be employing a
number of cheat detection mechanisms to ensure compliance.
You must submit all of your answers as typed text in-line in this
test document. Graphs may be hand-drawn and scanned in or
created digitally.Problems [___/150]
1. (90pts) you are a security analyst that is consulting with an
industrial factory called Fictional Terrible Company (FTC) that
makes a special type of chemical called
“Destructoviralbacteriumuraniumpostapocalypseicus” (DVBUP
for short). FTC is regulated by the Environmental Protection
Agency (EPA) and has to pay for each violation they have:
The EPA provides the following information about penalties:
Laws and regulations
Fine for each violation (per day per incident)
Hazardous Waste Disposal
$71,264
Clean Air Act
$95,284
Clean Water Act
$52,414
Safe Drinking Water Act
$54,789
FTC provides the following information about their industrial
plant
Below is the table of the past problems we have had.
Problem
Incidents
Avg. Number of Days to mitigate
Sabotage Pipe breakages that have led to DVBUP release
16 times in the past 8 years
2
Insecure SCADA system that has led to DVBUP release
2 times in the past 8 years
15
FTC also states the following:
· 8 of the pipe breakages caused a Clean Air Act violation, 4
caused a Clean Water Act violation, and 4 lead to violations of
both the Clean Air and Clean Water acts
· 1 of the times, when hackers breached our SCADA systems,
they released materials into the air, the other led to a release
that went into the water, but also got into the drinking water
table causing us to pay penalties for the both the clean water act
and the safe drinking water act
· We’ve not had any Hazardous Waste Disposal issues
a) (35pts) State the two security problems as threats
quantitatively using Expected Threat Impact (ETI) and Annual
Threat Loss Expectancy (ATLE). Show all of the work.
b) (35pts) Given your calculations in a) you confer with Bill
Mahoney of the UNO Cybersecurity department to come up with
a secure SCADA architecture. Bill says that, for $600,000 he
can assemble a team of students at UNO that can mitigate the
problem. Based on a look at the national numbers, you figure
the solution will reduce the number of exploitations to once in
10 years – but wont change how long it takes to mitigate. You
also confer with a physical security company to look into new
fencing that will reduce the sabotage incident frequency by 90%
(i.e. to once every 5 years). The fencing will cost $500,000.
Draw a single decision tree that shows ALL of the different
options available. Use the information above to estimate the
probabilities and costs at each step.
c) (20pts) Assuming a risk neutral approach, is it cheaper to
mitigate one SCADA, sabotage, both problems, or to do nothing
in a 1 year time interval? What about a 5 year time interval?
Use the decision tree to justify your answer
2. (60pts) You are given the following policy from the NIST SP
800-53.
IA-3: DEVICE IDENTIFICATION AND AUTHENTICATION
The information system uniquely identifies and authenticates
[Assignment: organization-defined specific and/or types of
devices] before establishing a [Selection (one or more): local;
remote; network] connection.
Assume the organization in question – FTC from question 1, has
instantiated the policy using the following selections as follows.
The information system uniquely identifies and authenticates
mobile phones and IoT devices before establishing a network
connection.
a) (35pts) Write a first order logic statement that represents the
control for FTC.
b) (15pts) Identify a non-compliant state (i.e. scenario that is
not acceptable by the policy) using first order logic.
c) (10pts) Given your answers, draw a Venn diagram that
visibly shows the “secure states” and “insecure states” for the
information system. Assume a closed-world assumption is in
place (i.e. only your logic statement defines what is secure and
insecure.
Anim. Behav., 1983, 31, 748-758
TRANSFER OF INFORMATION CONCERNING DISTANT
FOODS:
A LABORATORY INVESTIGATION OF THE
'INFORMATION-CENTRE' HYPOTHESIS
BY BENNEIT G. GALEF, JR & STEPHEN W. WIGMORE
Department of Psychology, McMaster University, Hamilton,
Ontario, L8S 4Kl Canada
Abstract. When choosing between two novel diets, an 'observer'
rat (Rattus norvegicus) prefers that diet
previously eaten by a 'demonstrator' conspecific with whom the
observer has interacted prior to making
its choice between diets. Demonstrator influence on observer
diet selection is maintained even if, during
the period of demonstrator-observer interaction, the
demonstrator is anaesthetized and a wire-mesh
barrier prevents the demonstrator from physically contacting the
observer. Demonstrator influence on
observer diet choice is blocked by either rendering the observer
anosmic or placing a transparent
Plexiglas barrier between demonstrator and observer during
their period of interaction. We conclude
that olfactory cues passing from demonstrator to observer
provide observers with information con-
cerning demonstrators' diets and that these olfactory cues are
sufficient to bias diet selection by ob-
servers. Further, observer rats that hav~ learned the locations at
which each of three diets are inter-
mittently available can use information provided by
demonstrators to increase foraging efficiency. We
discuss the implications of the results both for the 'information-
centre' hypothesis of the function of
mammalian aggregations and for previous studies of social
transmission of diet preference in rats.
Wild Norway rats (R. norvegicus) are social,
central-place foragers. Each rat lives as a
member of a colony inhabiting a fixed system
of burrows from which colony members emerge
to forage and to which they subsequently return
(Calhoun 1962; Telle 1966).
It has been suggested that in environments
characterized by an unpredictable and patchy
distribution of food, social birds or mammals
which forage from a central place could benefit
from exchange of information with conspecifics
at the central site concerning the availablility
of food in the larger environment (Ward &
Zahavi 1973; Erwin 1977; Bertram 1978 ;
Waltz 1982). This 'information-centre' hypothe-
sis concerning the function of vertebrate aggre-
gations carries the implication that members of
vertebrate social groups can acquire information
from others of their species concerning potential
feeding sites or foods remote in both time and
space from the locus of information trans-
mission (Ward & Zahavi 1973; De Groot 1980).
Although the hypothesis that aggregation
sites of birds and mammals may serve as
information-centres is an attractive one, evi-
dence sufficient to establish such a function of
sociality in vertebrates has proven difficult to
collect in field situations. Supporting evidence is
largely circumstantial or comparative rather
than direct (De Groot 1980). Further, there is
little evidence from laboratory study of either
mammals or birds of behavioural mechanisms
sufficient to support the transmission from one
individual to another of information concerning
distant food sources. In the literature, it is
usually assumed that unsuccessful vertebrate
foragers can use behavioural cues to identify
successful foragers at a central site such as a
burrow or roost, and will then follow a successful
individual to food (Ward 1965; Ward & Zahavi
1973; De Groot 1980; Waltz 1982).
It is, of course, possible that information about
distant foods can be transferred via behavioural
mechanisms other than following to a food
source (von Frisch 1967). For example, results
of previous studies in our laboratory indicate
that flavour cues contained in the milk of a
lactating rat reflect the flavour of her diet. These
flavours in mother's milk are sufficient to en-
able pups nursing from a dam to recognize her
diet at weaning and to cause pups to seek their
mother's diet when initiating feeding outside
the nest (Galef & Clark 1972; Galef & Henderson
1972; Galef & Sherry 1973). Thus the nest-site
serves as an information-centre for weanling
rat pups where they acquire information about
foods distant in both time and space. Such an
information-centre is a restricted one in that
the medium for information transfer (mother's
milk) is available only to lactating females and
the information contained in the medium can
only be received by suckling young.
There is also evidence suggesting that adult
rats might be able to exchange information
748
IIbl
2 Ooys 1 Doy 15 Min TEST
[E!NIA.-- ---.. ~0, ~' lot.
STEP 1 STEP 2 STEP 3 STEP 4 STEP 5
GALEF & WIGMORE: INFORMATION TRANSFER IN RATS
concerning distant foods. Strupp (1982) has
shown that an observer rat, choosing between
two diets available to a demonstrator on the
opposite side of a wire-mesh barrier from the
observer, will exhibit a strong preference for
whichever diet the demonstrator is eating
(see also Posadas-Andrews & Roper 1983).
While Strupp's findings indicate that adult rats
can exchange information concerning their
diets, her paradigm allowed demonstrator and
observer to interact in the presence of the foods
about which information was being communi-
cated. Extensive modifications in Strupp's
procedure were, therefore, necessary to explore
the possibility that rats can exchange informa-
tion concerning foods distant from an ingestion
site.
In the series of laboratory experiments des-
cribed below, we first demonstrate that one
Norway rat can communicate to another infor-
mation concerning food it has eaten at a distant
site. Second, we determine the mode of trans-
mission of this information, and finally we
demonstrate that the transmitted information
is adequate to facilitate foraging by its recipient.
Experiment 1
The first experiment was undertaken to deter-
mine whether one adult rat (a 'demonstrator')
cim communicate information to a second (an
'observer') concerning the food the demon-
strator has eaten prior to interaction with the
observer. Although we will refer to the rats in
our experiments as demonstrators and obser-
vers to conform to current usuage (Levitsky &
Strupp 1981; Strupp 1982), we do not intend to
imply that the behavioural process supporting
information transfer is learning by observation.
In fact, our experiments will demonstrate that
observational learning is not involved.
Methods
Subjects. Subjects were 32 Long-Evans rats
born in the McMaster colony to breeding pairs
descended from animals purchased from Blue
Spruce Farms (Altamont, N.Y.). Prior to
initiation of the experiment all subjects were
maintained ad libitum on Purina Laboratory
chow and water in same-sex groups of 4 to 5
littermates.
Sixteen subjects, 42 days of age at the time
of initiation of the experiment, served as
observers. Sixteen 90-120-day-old subjects
served as demonstrators. At the start of the
749
experiment, each observer was randomly paired
with a demonstrator of the same sex.
Apparatus. During the experiment, subjects
were housed and tested as demonstrator-
observer pairs in 42.5 x 24 x 27.5-cm wire-
mesh hanging cages (Wahmann Co., Balitmore,
Md). Each hanging cage was divided in two
equal parts by a 1.25-cm (t-inch) wire-mesh
partition (24 x 27.5 em) attached at the mid-
points of each cage's 42.5-cm sides. The same
apparatus was also used in experiments 2-5.
Procedure. Our procedure was designed to
mimic a situation in which a foraging rat ingests
a food at some distance from the burrow, re-
turns to the burrow, and interacts with a fami-
liar burrow-mate. We were interested to know
whether, as a result of such interaction, the
burrow-mate could acquire information concern-
ing the food the forager had eaten and would
use this information in selecting a diet.
Treatment of subjects during the ~xperiment
was as follows (Fig. la): Step 1: demonstrator
and observer were first maintained together
with ad libitum access to Purina Laboratory
Chow pellets for a 2-day familiarization period.
Step 2: the demonstrator was moved to the oppo-
site side of the wire-mesh partition from the ob-
server and food-deprived for 24 h to ensure
that the demonstrator fed when given the op-
portunity to do so. Step 3: chow was then
removed from the observer's side of the cage
(in preparation for testing) and the demonstrator
was moved to an enclosure in a separate room
and allowed to feed for 30 min on either pow-
dered Purina Laboratory chow adulterated
with 2 % by weight Hershey's cocoa (cocoa-
n.,
2 Doy. 1 Doy
[1)-
Fig. 1. Schematic diagram of the procedures of experi.
ments 1 and 2 (Fig. 1a) and experiments 3, 4 and 5 (Fig.
1b).O = observer;D = demonstrator; hatching indicates
that pellets of Purina Laboratory chow were present in
the cage.
Mingwen
Pencil
Mingwen
Pencil
Mingwen
Pencil
750 ANIMAL BEHA VIOUR, 31, 3
flavoured diet) or powdered Purina Laboratory
chow adulterated with 1% by weight Mc-
Cormick's pure ground cinnamon (cinnamon-
flavoured diet). Step 4: the demonstrator was
returned to the observer's cage and demon-
strator and observer were allowed to interact
for 15 min. Step 5: the demonstrator was re-
moved from the experiment and the observer
was offered, for 60 h, two weighed food-cups,
one containing cinnamon-flavoured diet and
one containing cocoa-flavoured diet. The food
bowl containing the diet which a given ob-
server's demonstrator had eaten was placed in
the front of the cage of half the observers and
in the back of the cage of the remainder of
observers.
The experimenter weighed the two food cups
once every 12 h throughout the 6O-h test period.
Results and Discussion
The results of experiment 1 are presented in
Fig. 2, which shows the mean amount of cocoa-
flavoured diet, as a percentage of total amount
eaten, ingested by observers whose demonstrators
had eaten cocoa-flavoured or cinnamon-
flavoured diet during step 4 of the experiment
(see Fig. la). Those observers whose demon-
strators ate cocoa-flavoured diet ~te a greater
0-12h 48-60h
80
~
~ 70
Q::
W
~
o 60
>-m
z 50
W
tc
w 40
o
U
~
30
Q::
~ 20
~
W 10
::IE
o
8
co CtN
8
co
DEMONSTRATORS' DIET
Fig. 2. Mean amount of cocoa-flavoured diet ingested, as
a percentage of total amount eaten, by observers whose
demonstrators had eaten either cocoa- or cinnamon-
flavoured diet. eo = cocoa-flavoured diet; eIN =
cinnamon-flavoured diet. Bars indicate::!: ISE.
percentage of cocoa-flavoured diet than did
those observers whose demonstrators ate
cinnamon-flavoured diet (0-12-h test, Mann-
Whitney U = 0, P < 0.(01). The effects of the
diet eaten by the demonstrator on the diet
preference of the observer were still observable
48-60 h after the interaction of observer and
demonstrator in step 4 of Fig. la (Mann-
Whitney U = 2, P < 0.001).
The results of experiment I clearly demon-
strate that rats are able to communicate infor-
mation concerning a diet they have eaten at a
time and place distant from the locus of their
interaction. The data also show that this
information is sufficient to bias a recipient's
subsequent choice of diet.
Experiment 2
In experiment 1, an observer and demonstrator
interacted immediately after the demonstrator
had eaten a novel diet and the observer had the
opportunity to choose between novel diets
immediately following receipt of information
from the demonstrator. Free-living rats must
expend time in returning from a feeding site to
their home burrow, and foragers departing from
their home burrow must expend further time in
reaching a feeding site. If the capacity of rats
to transmit information concerning a food they
have eaten at a distance from the nest site is to
function as the basis for information transfer at
the home-site in natural settings, transmission
must occur even if there is a delay between a
successful forager's ingestion of a food and
its return to the burrow and a further delay bet-
ween the interaction of a successful forager with
other rats and the latters' arrival at a potential
feeding site. The present experiment was under-
taken to discover whether the information
transfer demonstrated in experiment 1 could
tolerate such delays.
Methods
Subjects. Forty-eight 42-day-old experi-
mentally-naive Long-Evans rats born in the
McMaster colony served as observers, and 48
120-day-old Long-Evans rats as demonstrators.
Observers and demonstrators were randomly
assigned to one of three conditions: Control,
30-min-delay and 60-min-delay (described
below).
Apparatus. The apparatus was that used in
experiment 1.
Procedure. The eight demonstrator and eight
observer rats assigned to the Control condition
Mingwen
Pencil
Mingwen
Pencil
GALEF & WIGMORE: INFORMATION TRANSFER IN RATS
were treated identically to observers and demon-
strators in experiment I (see Fig. la). Demon-
strators and observers assigned to 30-min-delay
and 60-min-delay conditions were treated
identically to those in the Control condition
except in the following two respects. (I) After
each demonstrator had eaten either cinnamon-
flavoured or cocoa-flavoured diet for 30 min
(step 3 of Fig. la), it was food-deprived for
either 30 min (30-min-delay condition) or 60
min (60-min-delay condition) before being
placed in the cage with its observer (step 4 in
Fig. I a). (2) After the observer and demon-
strator had interacted for 15 min (step 4 in
Fig. la), the observer was food-deprived for
either 30 min (30-min-delay condition) or 60
min (60-min-delay condition) before being
offered a choice of cinnamon- and cocoa-
flavoured diets. A third departure from the
procedure of experiment I was that the food
cups offered to observers in the test phase
(step 5) were weighed only once, 12 h after the
initiation of testing.
Results and Discussion
The main results of experiment 2 are pre-
sented in Fig. 3, which shows the mean
percentage of cocoa-flavoured diet ingested by
observers whose demonstrators had eaten either
cinnamon- or cocoa-flavoured diet. Observers
in both Control and 30-min-delay conditions
exhibited marked effects of interaction with
demonstrators on their food choice (Mann-
3O-DELAY-30 6O-OEI.AY-60
80
! 10
~80
~
zSO
~
:i 40
8
ffi
30-
U
~ 20
~ 10
8 8
co a.Io
DEMONSTRATORS' DIET
Fig. 3. Mean amount of cocoa-flavoured diet ingested, as
a percentage of total amount eaten by observers in
Control, 30-min-delay, and 6O-min-delay groups when
their demonstrators had eaten either cinnamon- or cocoa-
flavoured diet. Bars indicate :f: 1 SE.
1S1
Whitney U; Control condition, U = 4, P =
0.002, 30-min-delay condition, U = 3, P =
0.002). There was a marginal effect of demon-
strator diet on the diet selection of observers in
the 6O-min-delay condition, but this failed to
reach significance (Mann-Whitney U = 14,
P < 0.064).
The results of the present experiment repli-
cate the finding in experiment 1 of a transfer of
information between adult rats concerning a
diet distant in time and space. The results also
indicate that this transfer is not disrupted by
30-min delays between a demonstrator's feed-
ing and its interaction with an observer and an
observer's interaction with a demonstrator and
that observer's initiation of feeding. The data
further suggest that similar I-h delays interfere
with the influence of demonstrators on the
observers' diet choice. The present results do
not provide sufficient information to determine
whether this decrement is due to the first I-h
delay causing a deterioration of the signal pass-
ing from demonstrator to observer, or the
second I-h delay reducing the eft'ect of the signal
on the observer's behaviour. We shall address
this issue elsewhere.
Experiment 3
Experiments 3, 4, and 5 were undertaken to
determine the medium of transmission of
information from demonstrator to observer.
Methods
Subjects. Thirty-two experimentally-naive
42-day-old Long-Evans rats born in the Mc-
Master colony served as observers and 32
90-11O-day-old Long-Evans rats served as
demonstrators. Observers and demonstrators
were randomly assigned to the two experimental
groups described below.
Apparatus. The apparatus was the same as
used in experiment I.
.
Procedure. The procedure was similar to
that described in Methods of experiment 1,
except that during the period of interaction bet-
ween demonstrator and observer in step 4 of
Fig. la, demonstrator and observer were sepa-
rated by a partition. For subjects in the 'Screen
condition' this partition was constructed of
1.3-cm (t-inch) wire-mesh. For subjects in the
'Plexiglas condition' this partition was con-
structed of clear, 0.65-cm (i-inch) Plexiglas.
Figure 1b presents a schematic diagram of the
procedure of the present experiment. Minor
differences between the procedure of experiment
100
SCREEN
90
!Q
80W
>Q:
W
f(/) 70m I0
~I
60
Z
w
tc 50W
8
!z
40
W
U
Q: 30
~Z
< 20w
~10
8 8
0 CO CIN
751 ANIMAL BEIiA VI OUR, 31, 3
I and that of the present experiment may be
noted in comparison of Fig. la and lb. These
differences are of no particular moment. They
were introduced to ascertain whether the
phenomenon under examination was sufficiently
robust to withstand small modifications in
procedure.
Results and Discussion
The main results of experiment 3 are pre-
sented in Fig. 4, which shows the mean amount
of cocoa-flavoured diet, as a percentage of
total amount eaten, ingested by observers in
Screen and Plexiglas conditions whose demon-
strators had eaten either cocoa- or cinnamon-
flavoured diet. Observers in the Screen condition
exhibited an influence of their respective
demonstrators on their diet choice, while
subjects in the Plexiglas condition did not
(Mann-Whitney U; Screen condition, U = 0,
P = 0.001; Plexiglas condition, U = 30, P>
0.30).
The results of the present experiment indi-
cate that transfer of information from demon-
PLEXIGLAS
8 8
CO CIN
DEMONSTRATORS' DIET
Fig. 4. Mean amount of cocoa-flavoured diet ingested, as
a percentage of total amount eaten, by observers in
Screen and Plexiglas conditions when their demonstrators
had eaten either cinnamon- or cocoa-flavoured diet. Bars
indicate :J: 1 SE.
strator to observer can occur in the absence of
free interaction between pair members. The
data further indicate both that visual contact
between observer and demonstrator is not
sufficient and that some other form of contact
(olfactory, gustatory, or tactile) is necessary for
information transfer.
Experiment 4
In this experiment we examined whether ol-
factory cues were sufficient to effect information
transfer from demonstrator to observer.
Methods
Subjects. Forty Long-Evans rats, 42 days of
age at the initiation of the experiment, served
as observers, and 40 lOO-l20-day old rats of
the same strain served as demonstrators. Half
of the observers and half of the demonstrators
were randomly assigned to Control and Anaes-
thetized conditions.
Apparatus. The apparatus was the same as
used in experiment 1.
Procedure. Subjects in the Control group of
the present experiment were treated identically
to those in the 'Screen' condition of experiment
2 (see Methods of experiment 2 and Fig. Ib).
Subjects in the Anaesthetized condition were
treated like those in the Control group except
that immediately after each demonstrator had
eaten cinnamon- or cocoa-flavoured diet in
step 3, it was anaesthetized by intraperitoneal
injection of sodium pentobarbitol, draped over
an inverted Pyrex petri dish (100 em diam x
50 em), fastened to it with a piece of masking
tape, and placed in the apparatus facing its
observer with the end of its nose 2.5 em from the
wire-mesh partition separating demonstrator
and observer. (Pilot studies had revealed that
an anaesthetized demonstrator 2.5 em from a
hardware cloth partition could not be physically
contacted by an observer on the far side of the
barrier.) Each anaesthetized demonstrator was
left on its side of the cage for 15 min (cf. step
4, Fig. Ib) and then removed. Each observer
was then offered a choice of cinnamon- and
cocoa-flavoured diets for 12 h.
Results
The main results of experiment 4 are pre-
sented in Fig. 5, which shows the mean per-
centage of cocoa-flavoured diet ingested by
observers in Anaesthetized and Control groups
when their demonstrators had eaten either
cinnamon- or cocoa-flavoured diet. Observers
GALEF & WIGMORE: INFORMATION TRANSFER IN RATS
CONTROL ANAESnETlZED
T
10
o
10 10
CO CIN
10
CO
DEMONSTRATORS' DIET
Fig. 5. Mean amount of cocoa-flavoured diet ingested, as
a percentage of total amount eaten, by observers in
Anaesthetized and Control groups when their demonstra-
tors had eaten either cinnamon- or cocoa-flavoured diet.
Bars indicate :f: 1 SE.
in both Anaesthetized and Control groups
exhibited marked effects of interaction with
demonstrators on their food choice (Mann-
Whitney U; Anaesthetized group, U = 8, P <
0.002; Control group, U = 13, P < 0.02).
'
Discussion .
Because transfer of information between
demonstrator and observer occurred in the
absence of physical contact between them, it
can be concluded that gustatory cues passing
from demonstrator to receiver (for example the
taste of the demonstrator's saliva or of food
particles clinging to the demonstrator's coat)
are not necessary for information transfer to
occur. The finding that demonstrators kept
2.5 em distant from their observers can transmit
the necessary signal suggests that olfactory cues
passing from demonstrator to observer are
sufficient to support the transmission process.
Because anaesthetized demonstrators are as
effective in transmitting information as intact
demonstrators, it would appear that signal emis-
sion by the demonstrator is a passive rather
753
than active process, and that the necessary
signal is emitted by the demonstrator rather
than elicited by the observer.
Experiment 5
Given that physical contact . between demon-
strator and observer is not necessary for informa-
tion transfer from the former to the latter, it
seems probable that olfactory cues sent by the
demonstrator and received by the observer are
sufficient for communication to occur. If so,
anosmic observers should not exhibit an influ-
ence of interaction with an observer on their diet
choice.
~ethods .
Subjects. Thirty-six 42-day-old Long-Evans
rats served as observers and 36 90-day-old
rats of the same strain served as demonstrators.
Half the observers were randomly assigned to an
Anosmic group and half to a Control group.
Apparatus. The apparatus was the same as
used in experiment I.
Procedure. The procedure was that of experi-
ment 2 (see Fig. Ib) except in two respects.
(1) Twenty-four hours following initiation of the
experiment, at the start of the last day of
familiariZJltion, observers assigned to the Anos-
mic group were rendered anosmic by intranasal
lavage with zinc sulphate solution (Alberts &
Galef 1971). Observers in the Control group
were treated with isotonic saline solution at
the time subjects in the Anosmic group were
treated with Zinc Sulphate. (2) For 30 min during
step 3 of Fig. Ib, demonstrator rats were fed
either unadulterated powdered Purina Labora-
tory Chow (Pu Diet) or Normal Protein Test
Diet (NPT Diet) (Teklad Test Diets, Madison,
Wise.). NPT Diet is a powdered mixture of the
following ingredients (values in g{kg): vitamin-
free test casein, 260.06; corn starch, 598.24,
hydrogenerated vegetable oil, 80.00; cod-liver
oil, 20.00, USP XIV Mineral mix 40.00; vita-
mins, 1.70. (3) Observers were offered a choice
between Pu Diet and NPT Diet for 12 h during
the test period (step 5 of Fig. 1b).
The change in diets offered to demonstrators
and observers in the present experiment was
necessitated by pilot studies showing that
anosmic rats could not discriminate cinnamon-
from cocoa-flavoured
.
diet. (To test for the
ability of anosmic subjects to discriminate bet-
ween any pair of diets we compared the ability
of anosmic 'and saline-Iavaged subjects to
exhibit a toxicosis-induced aversion toone of
CONTROL ANOSMIC
9 9 9 9
100
90
II)
Q:
au 80
~au
II)
m 70
0
~z
au
~au -50
t-
o.
Z
40
!z
au
0Q:
~Z
20
-<
~10
0
754 ANIMAL BEHAVIOUR, 31, 3
those two diets when subsequently offered both
simultaneously. The results of such tests were
clear-cut. Anosmic subjects poisoned after they
had eaten cocoa-flavoured diet ate as much
cocoa- as cinnamon-flavoured diet in a 6-h test
conducted 24 h after poisoning; control subjects
poisoned after they had eaten cocoa-flavoured
diet ate no cocoa-flavoured diet during the test.
Both anosmic and control subjects poisoned
after ingesting NPT Diet ate no NPT Diet when,
24 h after poisoning, they were offered a choice
of NPT and Pu Diets for 6 h.) If anosmic obser-
vers were unable to discriminate between the
two diets offered during testing (step 5 of Fig.
1b), then even if we were to observe a breakdown
in demonstrator influence on diet selection in
anosmic observers, we could not attribute this
deficit to an anosmia-induced interference with
communication between demonstrator and
observer.
Results and Discussion
The main results of experiment 5 are presented
in Fig. 6, which shows the mean amount of
60
30
NPT PU
'
NPT PU
"DEMONSTRATORS' DIET
Fig. 6. Mean amount of NPT Diet, as a percentage of
total amount eaten, ingested by observers in Anosmic and
Control 'groups when their demonstrators had eaten
either NPT Diet or Pu Diet. Pu = powdered Purina
Laboratory Chow, NPT = Normal Protein Test Diet.
Bars indicate :l: 1 SE.
NPT Diet, as a percentage of total amount
eaten, ingested by observers in Anosmic and
Control groups whose demonstrators had eaten
either NPT Diet or Pu Diet. Observers in the
Control group whose demonstrators ate NPT
Diet ate significantly more NPT Diet during
testing than did observers in the Control group
whose demonstrators ate Pu Diet (Mann-
Whitney U = 4, P < 0.002). By contrast, the
demonstrators' diet did not affect diet choice
by anosmic observers during testing (Mann-
Whitney U = 35, P > 0.30).
Given that NPT and Pu Diets are discrimin-
able by anosmic subjects and that demonstrators
can successfully transfer information concerning
NPT and Pu Diets to intact observers, the
failure of anosmic observers to use the informa-
tion transmitted by their observers suggests
that anosmia interfered with receipt of the
necessary olfactory information. The results of
both the present experiment and experiment 4
thus indicate that the cue passing from demon-
strator to observer is olfactory in nature.
We have independent evidence of the pre-
sence of an olfactory cue carried on the breath
of rats and reflecting the flavour of their diets,
which could serve as the means of information
transfer from demonstrator to observer. A
human subject (C.M.), by sniffing rat breaths,
successfully distinguished six rats fed cinnamon-
flavoured diet from six rats fed cocoa-
flavoured diet with better than 85 % accuracy
during two 18-trial sessions (binomial test,
Session 1, x = 3, P < 0.004, Session 2, x =
2, P < 0.00 I). While this does not prove that
observer rats use information carried 011 the
breath of demonstrators in selecting diets in
our experiments, it does indicate that such
information would be sufficient for communi-
cation from demonstrator to observer.
Experiment 6
The results of the experiments described above
demonstrate that rats can communicate informa-
tion to their fellows about diets they have
ingested far from the locus of information
transfer. The data also indicate that such infor-
mation can effect food preferences in a receiver,
and that the signal passing from demonstrator
to observer is olfactory in nature. They do not
however provide evidence that such information
can be used by a recipient to facilitate foraging
in environments characterized by an unpredic-
table and patchy distribution of food.
I FOOO CUP
~ONE-WAY DOOO
I REMOVABlE DOOO
HARDWARE ClOTH
GALEF & WIGMORE: INFORMATION TRANSFER IN RATS
In the present study we simulated an environ-
ment in which each of a variety of foods became
available in fixed locations on an unpredictable
schedule. Once our subjects had demonstrated
that they knew the location in which each food
was to be found, we provided each subject with
the opportunity to interact with a conspecific
which had eaten one of the foods. We then
determined whether our subjects would use the
information provided by the fed individual to
direct their subsequent foraging.
Methods
Subjects. Eight Long-Evans rats 42 days of
age at the start of experimentation served as
subjects and eight additional rats of the same
age served as demonstrators. One subject and
one demonstrator were excluded from the
experiment when the subject learned to open
the one-way doors described below from both
sides.
Apparatus. The apparatus is illustrated in
plan view in Fig. 7. It consisted of a three-arm
maze attached to a cage holding four subject-
CH DIET
CO DIET
CIN DET CHOICE POINT
Fig. 7. Plan of apparatus used in experiment 6. CH =
cheese-flavoured; CIN = cinnamon-flavoured; CO =
cocoa-flavoured; S = Subject; D = Demonstrator.
755
demonstrator pairs. Each I x O.09-m arm of
the maze (constructed of plywood with a
transparent Plexiglas cover) led via a removable
guillotine door to a 23 x 15-cm goal box con-
taining a food cup. At the choice point of the
maze the subject was faced with a choice bet-
ween three ont:-way doors.
Each subject was maintained in a 30 x 30 x
l5-cm plywood cage opening into an alley via a
removable guillotine door. Each subject was
housed adjacent to its demonstrator and sepa-
rated from the latter by a 30 x 15-cm wire-
mesh partition.
Procedure. To begin the experiment, subjects
and demonstrators were tail-marked for indivi-
dual recognition, introduced individually into
their respective compartments, and placed on a
23 h/day food-deprivation schedule.
Habituation. Three days following initiation
of the experiment, the guillotine doors were
removed from the subjects' cages and from the
goal boxes, the one-way doors were taped open,
and subjects were allowed to explore around the
apparatus for 2 h/day. Three different diets,
cinnamon-flavoured powdered Purina Labora-
tory chow (Diet CIN), cocoa:-flavoured pow-
dered Purina Laboratory chow (Diet CO) and
cheese-flavoured powdered Purina Laboratory
chow (Diet CH, 2 % by weight Kraft's romano
cheese) were placed in the goal-boxes in the
positions indicated in Fig. 7. These apparatus
habituation sessions were continued for several
days, with the experimenter gradually lowering
the flaps on the one-way doors from one day
to the next, until the one-way doors were
completely closed and all subjects were passing
through them without hesitation.
Training. On training days, each subject
received four trials per day. To begin a series
of trials the experimenter consulted a random
number table to determine which of the three
goal-boxes would be open to the first subject.
That goal-box door was opened, the door to the
first subject's living cage removed, and the
subject was given 5 min to pass through a
one-way door. If the subject chose the one-way
door leading to food it was allowed to feed for
2 min and then returned- to its living cage. If
the subject chose one of the other two one-way
doors, it was left in the arm it had chosen for
2.5 min and then returned to its living cage. If
the subject failed to pass through a one-way
door within 5 min, it was returned to its living
cage.
"f
. SUBJECT 1. SLeJECT 2
"
SUBJECT3
I .. SUBJECT4
III 80 . SlBJECT5
-':!! SUBJECT6
Ct:
SUBJECT7I- 0
~70
Z
0
W
U
(5
600
~0:
0: 50
8
!Z
~0: 40W
11.
756 ANIMAL BEHAVIOUR, 31, 3
Any subject choosing either of the incorrect
doors was left in its living cage for 2 min and
then allowed to choose again. The first trial was
repeated until the subject chose the correct one-
way door.
After the first subject's first trial of a day was
completed, the second, third and fourth subjects'
first trials of that day were run in an identical
fashion to the first trial of the first subject, ex-
cept that the goal-box door that was opened for
each subject was different from that opened
for the preceding subject.
Second, third and fourth trials on each day
for each subject were run under conditions
identical to those prevailing on that subject's
first trial of the day. Because the location of
the three diets remained fixed and the same goal-
box was open on trials 1--4of each day for each
subject, perfect performance was possible on
trials 2--4 (but not trial 1) of each day. After
all subjects had completed their four trials, each
was offered a food bowl containing unadult-
erated powdered Purina Laboratory chow for
1 h.
Testing. Once subjects were responding cor-
rectly on an average of two or more of the
last three trials of each day for six consecutive
days, that subject entered into the test phase of
the experiment. During the test phase, each
subject was run 4 trials/day, exactly as during
training. However, during testing, 45 min prior
to each subject's first trial of each day, that
subject's demonstrator was removed from its
living cage, and treated in the following fashion:
(1) Each demonstrator was first placed in a
cage in a separate room and allowed to feed
for 30 min on the diet which was located in the
arm of the maze to which that demonstrator's
subject was to have access on that day. (2)
The demonstrator was then returned to its
living cage to interact through the wire-mesh
partition with its subject for IS min, and was
then removed from its living cage until that
day's ; testing of all subjects was completed.
Each subject was tested (four trials/day) for 12
consecutive days.
Results and Discussion
. The main results of .experiment 6 are pre-
sented in Fig. 8, which indicates the percentage
correct responses made by each of the seven
subjects on its daily first trials of the last 18
days of training and on its daily first trials
during the 12 days of testing. Four of the seven
subjects did significantly better than chance
during the testing phase of the experiment
(Binomial test, Ho that p = !; see Fig. 8 for
significance levels). It is somewhat disappoint-
ing that not all subjects made use of the informa-
tion provided by their respective demonstrators
in selecting an arm of the maze for initial
exploration. However, the fact that four subjects
did use the information provided by their
demonstrators indicates that the mode of
information transfer examined in experiments
1-5 is adequate to facilitate foraging in
circumstances in which a food is available
intermittently at a fixed location.
General Discussion
The results of the present experiments demon-
strate that adult rats can exchange information
concerning ingested foods at a time and place
distant from the ingestion site. The results also
demonstrate that the reception of olfactory
information from conspecifics can profoundly
bias diet choice in the recipient, and under
appropriate circumstances, can facilitate the
recipient's location of available nutrients. The
present data thus provide indirect support for
30
"
01 I
LAST 18 DAYS
OF TRAINING
I
12 DAYS
OF TESTING
Fig. 8. Percentage of correct choi~ on first trials by
individual subjects on the last 18 days of training anQ the
12 days of testing in experiment 6.
758 ANIMAL BEHAVIOUR, 31, 3
Galef, B. G., Jr. & Clark, M. M. 1971b. Parent-offspring
interactions determine time and place of first
ingestion of solid food by wild rat pups. Psychon.
Sci., 25, 15-16.
Galef, B. G., Jr. & Clark, M. M. 1972. Mother's milk and
adult presence: two factors determining initial
dietary selection by weanling rats. J. compo physiol.
Psychol., 78, 220-225.
Galef, B. G., Jr. & Heiber, L. 1976. The role of residual
olfactory cues in the determination of feeding site
selection and exploration patterns of domestic rats.
J. compo physiol. Psychol., 90, 727-739.
Galef, B. G., Jr. & Henderson, P. W. 1972. Mother's milk:
A determinant of the feeding preferences of wean-
ing rat pups. J. compo physiol. Psychol., 78,
213-219.
Galef, B. G., Jr. & Sherry, D. F. 1973. Mother's milk: A
medium for the transmission of cues reflecting the
flavor of mother's diet. J. compo physiol. Psychol.,
83, 374-378.
Levitsky, D. A. & Strupp, B. J. 1981. Malnutrition and
tests of brain function. In: Nutrition and Behavior
(Ed. by A. A. Miller), Philadelphia, Pa: Franklin
Institute Press.
Posadas-Andrews, A. & Roper, T. J. 1983. Social trans-
. mission of food-preferences in adult rats. Anim.
Behav., 31, 265-271.
Strupp, B. J. 1982. Malnutrition and animal models of
cognitive development. Ph.D. thesis, Cornell
University.
Telle, H. J. .1966. Beitrag zur Kenntnis der Verhaltensweise
von Ratten, vergleichend dargestellt bei Rattus
norvegicus und Rattus rattus. Z. Angew. Zool., 53,
129-196. '
von Frisch, K. 1967. The Dance Language and Orientatioll
of Bees. Cambridge, Mass.: Belknap Press.
Waltz, E. C. 1982. Resource characteristics and the evo-
lution of information centers. Am. Nat., 119, 73-90.
Ward, P. 1965. Feeding ecology of the Black-faced Dioch
(Quelea quelea) in Nigeria. Ibis, 107, 173-214.
Ward, P. & Zahavi, A. 1973. The importance of certain
assemblages of birds as "information-centres" for
food-finding. Ibis, 115, 517-534.
(Received 14 September 1982; revised 28 October 1982;
MS. number: A3031)
.Journal of Comparative Psychology
198:3, Vol. 97, No.4, :358-:36:3
Copyright 198:1 hy the
American Psychological Association, Inc.
A Failure to Find Socially Mediated Taste Aversion
Learning in Norway Rats (R. norvegicus)
Bennett G. Galef, Jr., Stephen W. Wigmore, and Deborah J.
Kennett
McMaster University, Hamilton, Ontario, Canada
Observer rats interacted with conspecific demonstrators
immediately after
demonstrators ate a novel diet and were made ill by LiCI
injection. Following
their interaction with demonstrators, observers were tested for
aversion to
their ill demonstrator's diet. Previous research has shown that
(a) an observer
can extract information from a demonstrator sufficient to permit
identification
of the demonstrator's diet (Galef & Wigmore, 1983) and (b) a
rat ill from LiCI
toxicosis is an adequate unconditioned stimulus in a taste
aversion learning
paradigm (Lavin, Freise, & Coombes, 1980). Further, two of the
present
experiments demonstrated that cues emitted by a rat, reflecting
the particular
diet it has eaten, are an adequate conditional stimulus in a
toxicosis-induced
aversion learning situation. Observer avoidance of a diet
previously ingested
by an ill demonstrator was, however, not demonstrated. The
implications of
the failure to find socially mediated aversion learning are
discussed.
The results of a number of recent studies
demonstrate that signals emitted by rats
suffering LiCI toxicosis can serve as uncon-
ditioned stimuli in a taste aversion learning
paradigm. A rat ingesting some unfamiliar
diet prior to exposure to a LiCI-injected
conspecific subsequently exhibits reluct-
ance to ingest that diet (Bond, 1982;
Coombes, Revusky, & Lett, 1980; Lavin,
Freise, & Coombes, 1980; Stierhoff &
Lavin, 1982).
Although exposure to an ill conspecific
can result in a specific food avoidance in
rats in laboratory settings, it is not obvious
how such socially mediated taste aversion
learning might be used by free-living ani-
mals to enhance avoidance of toxic foods.
If an unfamiliar food eaten by an individual
prior to interaction with an ill conspecific
were safe, then subsequent avoidance of
that food would be counterproductive. If,
to the contrary, an unfamiliar food ingested
prior to interaction with an ill conspecific
were toxic, then information received from
This research was supported by Natural Sciences
and Engineering Research Council of Canada Grant
A0307 and McMaster University Research Board
grants to B. G. Galef, Jr. We thank Harvey Weingar-
ten and Mertice Clark for their thoughtful commen-
tary on earlier drafts of the manuscript.
Requests for reprints should be sent to Bennett G.
Galef, Jr., Department of Psychology, McMaster Uni-
versity, Hamilton, Ontario, Canada L8S 4Kl.
the ill individual would be redundant. Even
in the absence of social learning, a rat in-
gesting an unfamiliar toxic food would sub-
sequently avoid that food. The functional
significance of the potential of ill rats to
act as unconditioned aversive stimuli in an
avoidance learning situation is not obvious.
Stierhoff and Lavin (1982) suggested
that an ill rat may deposit aversion-produc-
ing residual odors in the vicinity of a nox-
ious food and that such odors might inhibit
ingestion of that food by others of their
colony (see also Steiniger, 1950). Unfortu-
nately, Stierhoff and Lavin did not provide
evidence either that aversion-producing
substances emitted by ill rats directly in-
hibit ingestion or that rats preferentially
deposit such substances in the vicinity of
toxic foods.
The results of recent studies both in our
laboratory and elsewhere demonstrate that
a rat briefly exposed to an unpoisoned con-
specific that has eaten some food subse-
quently exhibits an enhanced preference
for that food. One rat can extract infor-
mation from another concerning the diet
the latter individual has recently eaten
(Galef & Wigmore, 1983; Posadas-Andrews
& Roper, 1983; Strupp & Levitsky, in press-
a, in press-b). This finding, taken together
with the observation that an ill rat can
serve as an unconditioned stimulus for
taste aversion learning, suggests that a rat
358
100
..... 90
w
C 80
CIJ!
wa:
70a:w
0>
>a:
ctW 60
....IIJ!
...CD
.(0 50
o >-
o CD
40o z
Ow
;j.!C 30
z w
ct 20
w
::;;
10
0
360 B. GALEF, JR.,S. WIGMORE, AND D. KENNETT
30-MININTERACTION 2-HRINTERACTION
Step 5 Step5
DEMONSTRATOR'S DIET:
oCocoa
p<.Q02 0 Cinnamon
p<.003
p<.008 p<.001
8 8
SALINE LiCI SALINE LiCI
TREATMENT CONDITION
Figure 2. Mean amount of cocoa-flavoreddiet in-
gested by observers as a percentage of total amount
eaten. (Bars indicate :t 1 SE.)
Results and Discussion
The main results of Experiment 1 are
presented in Figure 2, which indicates the
mean percentage of cocoa-flavored diet ea-
ten during testing by observers whose dem-
onstrators had eaten either cinnamon-fla-
vored or cocoa-flavored diet. As is evident
from inspection of the figure and as statis-
tical tests confirm (Mann-Whitney U tests,
see Figure 2 for p values), subjects in both
experimental and both control groups ex-
hibited a marked preference for the diet
that their respective demonstrators had ea-
ten. Poisoned demonstrators were as effec-
tive in promoting intake of the diet they
had eaten as were unpoisoned demonstra-
tors.
The failure to find an effect of poisoning
demonstrators on their capacity to transfer
a preference for the diet they have eaten is
open to a variety of intepretations. First, it
is possible that although demonstrators
emit signals specifying both the food they
have eaten and that they have eaten some-
thing toxic, observers fail to associate the
two messages. Difficulties in forming aver-
sions to demonstrator-produced cues, prob-
lems in the temporal patterning of receipt
of the two signals, or problems with the
relative strength of the preference induced
by one signal and the aversion induced by
the other are possible causes of the ob-
served failure of transfer of aversion.
Alternatively, as is the case with all null
outcomes, the failure to demonstrate a
transfer of aversion from poisoned demon-
strators to observers may have been due to
our selecting an inappropriate set of exper-
imental conditions. We were, however,
careful to select parameters of toxicosis
induction in demonstrators and of interac-
tion between demonstrator and observer
previously shown to cause observers to
learn aversions to unfamiliar foods ingested
prior to interaction with a poisoned dem-
onstrator (Bond, 1982; Lavin et aI., 1980).
Further, the procedures we used were
clearly adequate to allow observers to ex-
tract information from demonstrators con-
cerning the diets demonstrators had eaten.
Conditions were thus appropriate for aver-
sion transfer from observer to demonstra-
tor, yet the anticipated outcome was not
observed.
We could continue seeking a set of pa-
rameters that would permit socially me-
diated transfer of aversion, but it is not
obvious what conditions to select. We de-
cided instead to ask whether the informa-
tion extracted by an observer from a dem-
onstrator could serve as the conditional
stimulus in a standard aversion learning
paradigm. It seemed to us that if an ob-
server could not form an aversion to the
food a demonstrator had eaten when that
observer was poisoned directly after inter-
acting with a demonstrator fed a novel food,
then it was unlikely that under any condi-
tions observers would avoid a food eaten by
a demonstrator exhibiting symptoms of
toxicosis.
Experiment 2
In this experiment, observers were first
allowed to interact with demonstrators fed
one of two diets. Each observer was then
poisoned by ip injection of LiCI and sub-
sequently offered a choice between the two
diets fed to demonstrators.
Method
Subjects. Thirty-two 42-day-old experimentally
naive Long-Evans rats from the McMaster colony
ABSENCE OF SOCIALLY MEDIATED TASTE AVERSION
359
made ill following ingestion of a novel food
might provide two potentially useful signals
to a conspecific: (a) a signal containing
information sufficient to permit identifi-
cation of the food that the signal-emitter
has recently eaten and (b) a signal capable
of inducing a learned aversion. Exposure to
these two signals in temporal contiguity
might suffice to produce in their recipient
avoidance of the specific diet recently in-
gested by an ill conspecific.
Experiment 1
Our procedure was designed to mimic a
situation in which one rat (a demonstrator)
departs from its burrow, ingests a novel
toxic food, returns to its burrow, and while
suffering toxicosis, interacts with a burrow-
mate (an observer) which subsequently has
the opportunity to ingest the novel food
previously eaten by the demonstrator. Our
goal was to determine whether such a series
of events would result in avoidance by the
observer of the food the demonstrator had
eaten prior to the onset of toxicosis.
Method
Subjects. Sixty-four experimentally naive Long-
Evans rats born in the McMaster colony to breeding
stock acquired from Blue Spruce Farms (Altamont,
New York) served as observers in the procedure de-
scribed below. Each observer was 42 days of age at the
time of initiation of the experiment. Sixty-four addi-
tionaI92-day-old Long-Evans rats from the McMaster
colony served as demcinstrators.
Apparatus. Subjects were housed and tested in
same-sex demonstrator-observer pairs in 42.5 x 24 x
27.5 cm wire mesh hanging cages (Wahmann Co.,
Baltimore, Maryland). Each cage was divided into two
equal parts by a 1.25-cm ('/2-in.) hardware-cloth screen
attached to the midpoint of each 42.5-cm side.
Procedure. Treatment of subjects during the ex-
periment was as follows (see Figure 1):
Step 1: In order to permit familiarization with
both apparatus and partner, demonstrator and ob-
server were maintained together with ad lib access to
Purina Laboratory Rodent Chow pellets (their normal
maintenance diet) and left undisturbed for 2 days.
Step 2: In order to ensure that the demonstrator
ate when given the opportunity to do so, each dem-
onstrator was moved to the opposite side of the screen
partition from its observer and food deprived for 24
hr.
Step 3: In preparation for testing of each observer,
chow was removed from each observer's side of the
apparatus. Each demonstrator was then moved to an
individual enclosure in a room separate from that
housing the apparatus and allowed to feed for 30 min
on either powdered Purina Laboratory Rodent Chow
adulterated 2% by weight with sifted Hershey's Pure
Cocoa (cocoa-flavored diet) or powdered Purina Lab-
oratory Rodent Chow adulterated 1% by weight with
McCormick's Fancy Ground Cinnamon (cinammon-
flavored diet).
Step 4: Immediately following termination of Step
3, each demonstrator was randomly assigned to one of
two experimental groups or one of two control groups.
Demonstrators assigned to experimental groups each
received ip injection of 1% of body weight of 2% (w/
v) LiCI solution. Members of control groups were
injected with an equivalent volume of isotonic saline
solution.
Step 5: Immediately following injection, each
demonstrator was returned to its respective observer's
cage, and demonstrator and observer were allowed to
interact for either 30 min or 2 hr, depending on the
condition to which a given demonstrator-observer pair
had been assigned.
Step 6: Each demonstrator was removed from the
experiment, and each observer was offered, for 18 hr,
two weighed food cups, one containing cinnamon-
flavored diet and one containing cocoa-flavored diet.
30 Min
2 Days 1 Day 30 Min or 2 Hr TEST
.--0;0' coc
~-~-G -~~I-[E[
~ ~
O;o'CIN
o;o,c;:-~
1
or
Diet coe
LiCI (Experimental)
or
Saline (Control)
STEP 1 STEP 2 STEP 3
INJECTION
STEP 4 STEP 5 STEP 6
Figure J. Schematic diagram of the procedure of Experiment 1.
(0 = observer; D = demonstrator;
Diet CIN = cinnamon-flavored diet; Diet COC = cocoa-flavored
diet. Hatching indicates pellets of
Purina Laboratory Rodent Chow present in cage.)
Mingwen
Pencil
Mingwen
Pencil
I/) 100 Sellne LithiumChloride
a:
w
90> pc.OO3 pc.OOOIa:
WI/) 80III
0
> 70III
Z 60w....
< 50w
u
0 40
U
.... 30w
is
~20
z 10<w
:I;
0
ABSENCE OF SOCIALLY MEDIATED TASTE AVERSION
361
2 Days 1 Day 30 Min 15 Min INJECTION TEST
Diet COC
~-~--[G--rn-o-lli
~ /
1
'DleICIN
Diet CIN 8or ---+- DDiet CQe
liCI (Experimental)
or
Saline (Control)
Figure 3. Schematic diagram of the procedure of Experiment 2.
(See Figure 1 for abbreviations.)
served as observers, and an additional 32 rats 60-90
days of age served as demonstrators.
Procedure. The procedure (see Figure 3) was sim-
ilar to that described in Method of Experiment 1 (see
Figure 1) except that instead of injecting each dem-
onstrator immediately before it interacted with an
observer, we injected each observer immediately after
it interacted with a demonstrator. Observers in the
experimental group received 1% of body weight of 2%
(wjv) LiCI solution; observers in the control group
received an equivalent volume of isotonic saline solu-
tion. Fifteen minutes following injection, each ob-
server was offered, for 18 hr, a choice between weighed
samples of cocoa- and cinnamon-flavored diets.
Results and Discussion
The main results of Experiment 2 are
presented in Figure 4 which indicates the
percentage of cocoa-flavored diet eaten by
observers whose demonstrators had in-
gested either cinnamon-flavored or cocoa-
flavored diet prior to their interaction with
observers. During testing, observers in the
control group exhibited a preference for the
diet their respective demonstrators ate,
while those in the experimental group ex-
hibited an aversion to the diet of their
respective demonstrators (Mann-Whitney
U tests, see Figure 4 for p values).
The results of the present experiment
indicate that cues received by observer rats
from demonstrators are adequate condi-
tional stimuli for the learning of an aver-
sion. It is, of course, possible that the avoid-
ance of demonstrators' diet exhibited by
subjects in the experimental group was not
the result of a learned aversion but rather
of an unconditioned response to toxicosis.
Experiment 3 was undertaken to directly
examine the unconditioned effects of toxi-
cosis on observers' preference for demon-
strators' diets.
Experiment 3
If the avoidance of demonstrators' diets
exhibited by observers were the result of an
unconditioned response to toxicosis, one
would expect observers poisoned prior to
interaction with demonstrators to exhibit
an aversion to demonstrators' diets.
Method
Subjects. Twenty-four experimentally naive 42-
day-old Long-Evans rats from the McMaster colony
served as observers. An additional 24 rats from the
same source, 2-3 wk older than observers, served as
demonstrators. Half of the observers and half of the
demonstrators were assigned to an experimental
group; the remainder, to a control group.
Procedure. Treatment of both experimental and
control groups is illustrated in Figure 5. In brief, on
the third day of the experiment, observers received ip
injection of 1% of body weight of a solution. The
OBSERVERS INJECTED
COC CIN COC CIN
DEMONSTRATORS'DIET
Figure 4. Mean amount of cocoa-flavored (COC) diet
ingested by observers as a percentage of total amount
eaten. (CIN = cinnamon-flavored diet. Bars indicate
:t 1 SE.)
362 B. GALEF, JR., S. WIGMORE, AND D. KENNETT
2 Days 1 Day INJECTION
~__~-O
r
LiCI (Experimental)
or
Saline (Control)
4 Hr 30 Min 15 Min TEST
I
~ I!"::l nDl
__Diet COC
· D ~--Lb--~--G
/
"'--Diet CIN
Diet CIN /
or D
Diet COC
Figure 5. Schematic diagram of the procedure of Experiment 3.
(See Figure 1 for abbreviations.)
observers in the control group were injected with
isotonic saline, and those in the experimental group
with 2% (w/v) LiCl solution. All observers were then
left undisturbed for 4 hr while subjects in the experi-
mental group recovered from acute effects of toxicosis
induction. Each observer then interacted for 15 min
with a demonstrator fed either cinnamon- or cocoa-
flavored diet, and each observer was subsequently
tested for its preference between cinnamon- and co-
coa -flavored diets.
Results and Discussion
The main results of Experiment 3 are
presented in Figure 6 which indicates the
percentage of cocoa-flavored diet eaten by
observers whose demonstrators had in-
gested either cocoa- or cinnamon-flavored
diet. During testing, subjects in both con-
trol and experimental groups exhibited
marked preference for the diet their respec-
tive demonstrators had eaten (Mann- Whit-
OBSERVERS INJECTED
<II 100
a:
w
~ 90
w
~ 80
o
> 70aI
ffi 60
I-
~ 50
u
o 40
u
tu 30
is
;f'. 20
:i 10
w
::E
Saline Lithium Chloride
o
COC CIN COC CIN
DEMONSTRATORS'DIET
Figure 6. Mean amount of cocoa-flavored (COC) diet
ingested by observers as a percentage of total amount
eaten. (CIN = cinnamon-flavored diet. Bars indicate
:!: 1 SE.)
ney U tests, see Figure 6 for p values).
Experience of toxicosis does not in itself
result in avoidance by observers of the diet
eaten by demonstrators.
General Discussion
The results of the present series of ex-
periments indicate both (a) that cues emit-
ted by one rat, reflecting the identity of the
diet that rat has recently eaten, form an
adequate conditional stimulus for toxicosis-
based aversion learning (Experiments 2
and 3) and (b) that naive rats experiencing
the cues emitted by an ill conspecific pre-
viously fed a diet do not develop an aversion
to that diet (Experiment 1).
Given that the purpose of undertaking
this series of studies was to determine
whether a rat would avoid ingesting a diet
as the result of interacting with an ill con-
specific that had ingested that diet, the
failure to find such a phenomenon consti-
tutes a null outcome. Like all null outcomes
the present finding is difficult to interpret.
Taken together, the finding of Lavin et al.
(1980), that an ill rat is an adequate uncon-
ditioned stimulus for aversion learning, and
the finding in Experiment 2 above, that
cues emitted by a fed rat are an adequate
conditional stimulus for aversion learning,
suggest that under the proper set of exper-
imental parameters, one would find socially
mediated aversion learning.
Our reason for reporting the present re-
sults, rather than searching the relevant
parameter space until a situation in which
socially mediated aversion learning is ob-
tained, is to make clear that even if the
desired result is eventually found, there
must be serious question as to its relevance
to toxin-avoidance behavior of rats in nat-
ABSENCE OF SOCIALLY MEDIATED TASTE AVERSION
363
ural environments. The more restricted the
parameter space in which socially mediated
learned aversions occur, the less likely they
are to play a role in the diet selection of
free-living organisms.
In more than 20 experiments on the so-
cially mediated transfer of diet preference,
we have invariably seen preference for dem-
onstrators' diet by observers (Galef, 1983;
Galef & Wigmore, 1983). Similarly Strupp
(in press-a, in press-b, Note 1) and Posa-
das-Andrews (1983; Note 2), using quite
different experimental paradigms, have re-
peatedly found social transfer of diet pref-
erence. This consistency of outcome across
a broad range of conditions suggests that
the social transmission of preference for a
diet is likely to playa role in natural envi-
ronments. The difficulty that both our lab-
oratory and that of Posadas-Andrews (Note
2) have experienced in demonstrating so-
cially mediated aversion learning suggests
that it is unlikely to be an important aspect
of the poison-avoidance behavior of free-
living animals. At the very least, any future
demonstration of social transmission by ill
rats of an aversion to a specific diet will
have to be critically examined to determine
whether the parameters allowing such
transmission to occur are likely to be found
in natural situations.
Reference Notes
1. Strupp, B. J. Personal communications, 1982,
1983.
2. Posadas-Andrews, A. Personal communication,
1982.
References
Bond, N. W. Transferred odor aversions in adult rats.
Behavioral and Neural Biology, 1982,35, 417-421.
Coombes, S., Revusky, S. H., & Lett, B. T. Long-delay
taste-aversion learning in an unpoisoned rat: Ex-
posure to a poisoned rat as the unconditioned stim-
ulus. Learning and Motivation, 1980, 11, 256-266.
Galef, B. G., Jr. Utilization by Norway rats (R. nor-
vegicWl) of multiple messages concerning distant
foods. Journal of Comparative Psychology, 1983, 97,
364-371.
Galef, B. G., Jr., & Wigmore, S. W. Transfer of infor-
mation concerning distant foods: A laboratory in-
vestigation of the "information-centre" hypothesis.
Animal Behaviour, 1983,31, 748-758.
Lavin, M. J., Freise, B., & Coombes, S. Transferred
flavor aversions in adult rats. Behavioral and Neural
Biology, 1980, 28, 15-33.
Posadas-Andrews, A., & Roper, T. J. Social transmis-
sion of food preferences in adult rats. Animal Be-
haviour, 1983,31,265-271.
Steiniger, F. von. Beitrage zur Soziologie und sonsti-
gen Biologie der Wanderratte. Zeitschrift fur Tier-
psychologie, 1950, 7,356-379.
Stierhoff, K. A., & Lavin, M. The influence of render-
ing rats anosmic on the poisoned-partner effect.
Behavioral and Neural Biology, 1982, 34, 180-189.
Strupp, B. J., & Levitsky, D. A. Early brain insult and
cognition: A comparison of malnutrition and hypo-
thyroidism. Developmental Psychobiology, in press-
a.
Strupp, B. J., & Levitsky, D. A. PKU, learning, and
models of mental retardation. Developmental Psy-
chobiology, in press-b.
Received April 2, 1983.
Revision received June 16, 1983 .
Social Interaction with an
Intoxicated Sibling Can Result
in Increased Intake of Ethanol
by Periadolescent Rats
Pamela S. Hunt
Jennifer L. Holloway
Elka M. Scordalakes
Department of Psychology
College of William & Mary
Williamsburg, VA 23187-8795
Received 13 December 1999; accepted 10 October 2000
ABSTRACT: A novel procedure for enhancing voluntary intake
of ethanol in periadolescent rats is
described. The procedure is a modi®cation of Galef et al.'s
(e.g., Galet, Kennett, & Stein, 1985; Anim
Learn Behave 13:25 ± 30) demonstrator± observer procedure.
Subjects were Sprague-Dawley rats,
28 ± 35 days of age. The experimental subject (observer)
interacted with a same-sex conspeci®c
(demonstrator) previously administered (a) 1.5 g/kg ethanol, (b)
an equal volume of water, or (c)
2.1% Sanka coffee intragastrically. Observers were tested with
24-hour access to ethanol and coffee
solutions. Observers that had interacted with demonstrators
administered ethanol ingested
signi®cantly more ethanol during the test than observers in the
other two groups. In Experiment 2
demonstrators were administered one of several doses of ethanol
(0.0, 1.0, 1.5, or 3.0 g/kg) and
observers' ethanol intakes were assessed. Only those observers
that interacted with 1.5 g/kg
demonstrators increased their ingestion of ethanol, relative to
water controls. The lower (1.0 g/kg)
and higher (3.0 g/kg) dose groups did not show altered ethanol
ingestion. These results are discussed
with respect to threshold levels of respired ethanol cues and the
ability of observers to detect these
cues from demonstrators. The demonstrator± observer procedure
appears to be effective for the
social transmission of preferences for ethanol in periadolescent
rats. ß 2001 John Wiley & Sons,
Inc. Dev Psychobiol 38: 101 ± 109, 2001
Keywords: ethanol; periadolescent rats; alcohol ingestion;
social learning
Alcohol abuse continues to be one of the most health-
costly issues in today's society, and the incidence of
alcohol abuse is ever-increasing. Particularly disturb-
ing statistics have recently been reported that the age
at which adolescents and young adults begin to
consume alcohol in rather large quantities is dropping
appreciably. A recent University of Michigan health
survey noted that 51% of 12th graders reported
drinking within the previous month and 26% of 8th
graders reported doing so. Binge drinking, de®ned as
having ®ve or more drinks on occasion, is also on the
rise, with 30% of all 12th graders and 16% of all 8th
graders reporting binge drinking at least once in the 2
weeks prior to the survey (National Institute on
Alcohol Abuse and Alcoholism, 1997).
Given the abuse potential for alcohol, and the
decreasing age at which alcohol is being consumed, it
is imperative that information be gained about the
causative factors involved in the initiation of drinking,
from both basic and applied research domains. To
Correspondence to: P. S. Hunt ([email protected])
Contract grant sponsor: National Institutes of Alcohol Abuse
and Alcoholism
Contract grant numbers: AA12135, AA12466
ß 2001 John Wiley & Sons, Inc.
adequately achieve these goals, there is also a need for
adequate animal models to examine relevant social,
environmental and genetic contributions to alcohol
consumption, especially early in development. Cur-
rent research using selectively bred rodent strains
continues to afford valuable insights into the role of
genetic predispositions toward alcohol consumption
and physical dependence (e.g., Brown, Tate, Vik, Haas,
& Aarons, 1999; Grahame, Li, & Lumeng, 1999;
Lumeng, Waller, McBride, & Li, 1982). However, it is
also recognized that genetic factors alone cannot
account for the widespread incidence of alcohol
abuse, either during adolescence or later in life (e.g.,
McKinzie et al., 1998). Experience also contributes
enormously.
As a means of examining experiential factors,
several paradigms have been developed to induce
ethanol intake in rats, a species valuable to addiction
and teratological research, but one that typically
avoids ethanol solutions. Many of the procedures that
result in increased intake of ethanol add confounding
stressor variables (e.g., Adams, 1995; Hansen, Fahlke,
Soederpalm, & Hard, 1995; Le et al., 1998; Roske,
Baeger, Frenzel, & Oehme, 1994), making the study
of mechanistic issues underlying the initiation of
voluntary ethanol intake dif®cult, if not impossible.
Moreover, these and other methods can involve rather
long training histories or complex motor require-
ments, making them unsuitable for use with young
rats because of their rapid rate of development and
relatively immature sensorimotor capabilities. A few
procedures have been speci®cally designed to exam-
ine the consequences of direct exposure to ethanol
cues during the preweanling period on subsequent
alcohol intake patterns (e.g., Bannoura, Kraebel,
Spear, & Spear, 1998; Hunt, Kraebel, Rabin, Spear,
& Spear, 1993; Hunt, Lant, & Carroll, 2000). To date,
however, the effects of direct social contact with an
intoxicated sibling on periadolescent animal's sub-
sequent willingness to ingest ethanol has not been
systematically examined.
It is known that rats detect and learn about foods
through olfactory cues present on the breath or peri-
oral region of a conspeci®c that has recently ingested
a particular food (e.g., Galef, Kennett, & Stein, 1985;
Galef & Stein, 1985; Posadas-Andrews & Roper,
1983; Strupp & Levitsky, 1984). In the experimental
preparation used to examine this social transmission
of dietary information, Galef and colleagues term the
animal that is fed a diet initially the `̀ demonstrator.''
The experimental subject, the one that is tested for
intake of that diet, is termed the `̀ observer.'' The
demonstrator is ®rst fed a diet with a particularly
salient olfactory component, such as chocolate, coffee,
or cinnamon. After the demonstrator has eaten the
food, it is introduced into a cage housing the observer
animal and the two are allowed to interact for a brief
period of time (e.g., 5 ± 30 min). The observer is then
given access to two diets simultaneouslyÐthe one
previously eaten by the demonstrator and another,
novel diet. In several reports, the observer animal was
found to preferentially ingest the diet eaten by its
demonstrator, even if the alternative is normally the
more preferred of the two. The effect is not one of
mere familiarity or simple exposure to the diet (Galef
et al., 1985), but requires a speci®c type of social
context to be established; that is, exposure to food
cues surrounding the peri-oral region of the demon-
strator (Galef & Stein, 1985; Galef & Wigmore, 1983).
Given the pervasive role of social behaviors in the
transmission of dietary information in rats (e.g., Galef,
Kennett, & Wigmore, 1984), it was hypothesized
that the same or similar processes might also act
when the food of interest is a drug of abuse, that is,
alcohol. Because ethanol is eliminated in unmeta-
bolized form through routes such as respiration and
salivation (Hollstedt & Rydberg, 1985) these cues
would be present on the breath of an organism follow-
ing ingestion of alcohol.
We have recently shown that this type of socially
mediated learning can be applied to alcohol intake in
preweanling animals (Hunt et al., 2000). In that study,
the observers were pups ranging in age from 8 to 16
days. After a brief period of social deprivation these
subjects were allowed to interact with a sibling that
had been intragastrically administered a 1.5 g/kg dose
of ethanol or water. Following this interaction, pups
were tested for voluntary intake of ethanol by infusing
ethanol into the mouth via an intra-oral cannula (Hall
& Rosenblatt, 1977). Observers that had interacted
with demonstrators administered ethanol ingested
signi®cantly more ethanol during the test than those
observers that had interacted with demonstrators
administered water.
The purpose of the present experiments was to test
the role of this type of social context in modifying
voluntary ethanol intake in periadolescent Sprague-
Dawley rats. The subjects ranged in age from 28 to 35
days at the beginning of the experiment, considered to
encompass the periadolescent period (Spear & Brake,
1983). In the experimental preparation, the subject of
interest (the observer) interacted with an animal (the
demonstrator) that had previously been force-fed
ethanol. Following this interaction, the observer was
tested for intake of ethanol versus coffee in a two-
bottle test. In two experiments the effectiveness of
this preparation for increasing observers' voluntary
ethanol intake was shown.
102 Hunt, Holloway, and Scordalakes
EXPERIMENT 1
The purpose of Experiment 1 was to determine whether
the demonstrator± observer preparation (Galef et al.,
1985) would be an effective means by which to
promote voluntary intake of ethanol in periadolescent
rats (cf., Hunt et al., 2000). Our choice of periadoles-
cent rats for subjects was in¯uenced by our interest in
adolescent alcohol intake and the social factors that
may contribute to the initiation of drinking. Addi-
tionally, this age allowed for the use of a two-bottle
test, potentially providing more information than that
available when testing preweanlings about alcohol
choice behavior.
Methods
Subjects. The subjects were 30 pairs of same-sex
sibling Sprague-Dawley-derived rats (16 female, 14
male), representing seven different litters. No more
than one male and one female from each litter served
as observers in each treatment group. The subjects
ranged from 30 to 35 days of age at the beginning of
the procedure. All animals were born and reared in the
psychology department vivarium at the College of
William & Mary. Breeders were housed together in
50.8�40.6�21.6 cm3 clear polycarbonate cages
with pine chip bedding and free access to food
(ProLab, St. Louis, MO) and water. Cages were
checked daily for births. Two days after birth (day of
birth�Postnatal Day [PD] 0) litters were culled to
8 ± 10 pups. Animals were maintained on a 12:12 hr
light:dark cycle with light onset at 0700 hr in the
temperature-controlled vivarium. On PD21 pups were
weaned and group-housed with same-sex littermates
in 50.8�40.6�21.6 cm3 clear polycarbonate cages
with ad lib access to food and water until the ®rst day
of the experiment. All procedures were approved by
the College of William & Mary Research on Animal
Subjects Committee and followed guidelines estab-
lished by the International Society for Developmental
Psychobiology.
Apparatus. All training and testing occurred in indi-
vidually sized stainless steel hanging cages located in
a room adjacent to the main vivarium, and maintained
on the same temperature and light control systems.
Demonstrators were intragastrically administered
ethanol, water, or coffee solutions using 12-cm lengths
of polyethylene tubing (PE-50; Clay Adams) attached
to 5-cc disposable syringes. Observers were tested for
intake of ethanol and coffee using 50-ml graduated
borosilicate drinking tubes with curved sipper spouts
that were attached to the outside of the hanging cages.
Procedure. The experiment was conducted over 5
days and is outlined in Table 1. On days 1 and 2 of
the procedure, same-sex siblings were pair-housed in
metal hanging cages and handled for approximately
2 min each day. At this time animals within each pair
were assigned to be the demonstrator or the observer.
On Day 3, the demonstrator was removed from the
home cage and placed alone in an adjacent hanging
cage. The demonstrator was given free access to both
food and water, while at this time the observer was
water-deprived but continued to have free access to
food.
On Day 4, 23 hr following separation and the
initiation of water deprivation, demonstrators were
weighed to the nearest 0.1 g. Next, the demonstrator
was intragastrically administered a 1.5 g/kg dose of
12.0% v/v ethanol solution (EtOH, n�10), an equal
volume of 2.1% w/v Sanka decaffeinated coffee (COF,
n�9), or an equal volume of the tap water vehicle
(WATER, n�10). One pair of male COF subjects was
eliminated from the experiment because of improper
intubation. Following intubation, demonstrators were
returned to their cages for 30 min. The 30-minute
period was selected because it has been shown to be
suf®cient for attaining approximately peak blood ±
alcohol levels and respiratory/salivary elimination
of alcohol cues (Molina & Chotro, 1989a; Molina,
Chotro, & Spear, 1989; Pohorecky & Brick, 1982).
The demonstrator was then placed into the observer's
cage and the two animals were left undisturbed for 30
min. After the interaction phase the demonstrator was
once again removed from the observer's cage.
Observers (34 ± 39 days at test) were then offered
two graduated drinking tubes; one containing a 5.6%
v/v ethanol solution and the second containing a 2.1%
Table 1. Procedure Used for Experiments 1 and 2
Day/time Procedure
1/10:00 am Pair-house demonstrators and
observers
Handle subjects 2 min
2/10:00 am Handle subjects 2 min
3/10:00 am Remove demonstrator and individually
house animals
Water deprive observers
4/9:00 am Weigh demonstrator
i.g. administration of EtOH, WATER,
or COF
4/9:30 am Demonstrators placed into observers'
cages
4/10:00 am Demonstrators removed
Two-bottle test begins (observer)
5/10:00 am End of two-bottle test
Periadolescent Ethanol Intake 103
w/v Sanka decaffeinated coffee solution. Both test
solutions were prepared with tap water and the
concentrations of each were chosen on the basis of
preliminary data (Scordalakes & Hunt, unpublished
data) indicating an equal preference for these solu-
tions in naõÈve animals. Observers were allowed free
access to both solutions for 24 hr. The left ± right
placement of the drinking bottles containing the two
solutions was counterbalanced across subjects in all
conditions to avoid a side-preference bias in the data.
At the end of the testing interval, the volume of each
solution ingested (ml) was recorded. The data were
also expressed in terms of the dose of ethanol ingested
during the 24-hour test period (g/kg/day).
Results
The data from this experiment are shown in Figure 1
and Table 2. It was predicted that EtOH observers,
those that had interacted with demonstrators adminis-
tered ethanol, would ingest more ethanol during the
test than WATER controls. Furthermore, observers
interacting with demonstrators administered coffee
(COF) were expected to ingest more coffee than
WATER controls (cf. Galef & Stein, 1985). The ®rst
prediction was realized. Observers that had interacted
with EtOH demonstrators exhibited increased intake
of the ethanol solution relative to coffee. Second, and
in contrast to what was predicted, observers that had
interacted with COF demonstrators did not exhibit
altered intake of coffee relative to that observed in the
WATER controls. When the data were expressed as a
daily dose of ethanol ingested (g/kg/day), again the
observers that had interacted with demonstrators
administered ethanol ingested a higher dose of ethanol
than controls.
The ethanol and coffee intake data were analyzed
using a 3 (demonstrator solution)�2 (test solution:
ethanol or coffee)�2 (gender) mixed-design analysis
of variance (ANOVA). This analysis yielded a signi-
®cant main effect of test solution [F (1,23)�6.23,
p < :05]. The demonstrator solution � test solution
[F (2,23)�4.85, p < :05] and test solution�gender
[F (2,23)�5.93, p < :05] interactions were also sig-
ni®cant. Post hoc comparisons conducted with Neu-
man ± Keuls tests revealed that the EtOH observers
ingested more ethanol and less coffee than either
the WATER or COF observers. Additionally, male
observers ingested more of the ethanol solution than
females, regardless of prior experience. Finally, total
¯uid intake during the test did not differ as a function
of group assignment. A 3 (demonstrator solution)�2
(gender) ANOVA conducted on the dose (g/kg/day)
data revealed signi®cant main effects of demon-
strator solution [F (2,23)�5.37, p < :05] and gender
[F (1,23)�7.87, p < :01]. Observers interacting with
ethanol-administered demonstrators ingested a higher
dose of ethanol during the test, and males ingested a
higher dose of ethanol than females, regardless of group.
Discussion
Results of the ®rst experiment indicate that an ob-
server animal will increase its intake of a 5.6% v/v
FIGURE 1 Mean (�SEM) intakes of ethanol and coffee
(ml) by observers in Experiment 1 during a 24-hour two-
bottle choice test. Observers had previously interacted for
30 min with demonstrators that had been intragastrically
administered a 1.5 g/kg dose of ethanol (EtOH), an equal
volume of tap water (WATER) or an equal volume of 2.1%
Sanka coffee (COF).
Table 2. Mean (�SEM) Dose of Ethanol (g/kg/day)
Ingested During the 24-Hour Two-Bottle Ingestion Test
for Experiments 1 and 2
Experiment 1
Observer group
EtOH 9.45 (1.13)
H2O 5.80 (0.98)
COF 5.97 (0.99)
Experiment 2
Demonstrator dose
0.0 5.69 (0.77)
1.0 6.45 (1.10)
1.5 8.16 (0.67)
3.0 5.96 (0.75)
104 Hunt, Holloway, and Scordalakes
ethanol solution following a brief (30 min) interaction
with an intoxicated sibling. Presumably, the adminis-
tration of ethanol to the demonstrator resulted in the
elimination of unmetabolized ethanol that was dete-
cted by the observer. By experiencing these ethanol
cues in the context of social interactions, the observer
acquired suf®cient information about ethanol to guide
later voluntary choice of this drug. In contrast to the
®nding of increased ethanol ingestion in EtOH
observers, animals that had interacted with demon-
strators administered a coffee solution showed no
socially mediated change in coffee preference (but see
Galef & Stein, 1985). It is possible that the time
between administration of coffee to the demonstrator
and the interaction phase, or characteristics of the
coffee solution intubated, may have affected the
observers detecting and learning about the coffee cue
from the demonstrators.
The work of Galef and colleagues (Galef & Stein,
1985; Galef & Wigmore, 1983) has repeatedly indi-
cated that physical contact per se is not a necessary
component of this type of learning. Observers have
been found to acquire dietary information from de-
monstrators even when the animals are separated by a
wire mesh screen or when demonstrators are anesthe-
tized. There is, however, a lack of data on preweanling
and periadolescent animals. The availability for social
interactions may in fact enhance the strength of diet
acquisition in younger animals more so than in adults.
Several laboratories have shown that social interac-
tions and the opportunity for play are highly rein-
forcing to adolescent age rats. Most of the research in
this area has utilized the conditioned place preference
paradigm, in which one arm of a T-maze contains a
play partner while the alternative arm contains no
stimulus, a surrogate animal, or an animal that does
not engage in play behavior. Using variants of this
paradigm, adolescent rats have consistently been
shown to prefer the arm previously associated with
a play partner (e.g., Calcagnetti & Schechter, 1992;
Crowder & Hutto, 1992; Humphreys & Einon, 1981;
Normansell & Panksepp, 1990). In the present proce-
dure the demonstrators and observers were allowed to
physically interact following a period of social isola-
tion. It is unclear at present whether or not this
physical contact had the effect of promoting acquisi-
tion of appetitive responses toward ethanol.
EXPERIMENT 2
In Experiment 1 the EtOH demonstrators were ad-
ministered a 1.5 g/kg dose of ethanol. This dose was
chosen on the basis of a series of reports by Molina
and colleagues (Molina et al., 1989; Molina & Chotro,
1989b) indicating that 1.5 g/kg ethanol, but not lower
doses, resulted in detectable levels of ethanol orosen-
sory cues suf®cient for learning about those cues to
occur. In the Molina studies, the animal that was
administered the ethanol intragastrically was assessed
for its own learning about alcohol in terms of olfactory
preferences and alcohol intake. The results of the
present Experiment 1 indicate that another animal can
detect and learn about these cues as well. It is possible
that with the demonstrator± observer procedure doses
lower than 1.5 g/kg would be effective for producing
suf®cient elimination of ethanol to affect the alcohol
intake of an observer. The purpose of Experiment 2
was to conduct a dose ± response study to determine
(a) whether demonstrators administered a dose of
ethanol lower than 1.5 g/kg would provide suf®-
cient orosensory stimuli to promote ethanol intake
in observers and (b) whether demonstrators adminis-
tered a dose higher than 1.5 g/kg would establish
even stronger preferences for the ethanol solution in
observers. In this experiment demonstrators were
intragastrically administered 0, 1.0, 1.5, or 3.0 g/kg
ethanol.
Methods
Subjects. The subjects in this experiment were 48
pairs of same-sex sibling Sprague-Dawley-derived
rats (24 male, 24 female), representing 14 different
litters. Subjects ranged in age from 28 to 33 days on
the ®rst day of the procedure. Animals were born and
reared in the psychology department vivarium at the
College of William & Mary under conditions identical
to those described in Experiment 1. Pairs of animals
were randomly assigned to one of four conditions,
designated according to the dose of ethanol adminis-
tered to the demonstrator: 0 (n�12), 1.0 (n�12), 1.5
(n�12), or 3.0 g/kg (n�11). One pair of female
subjects assigned to the 3.0 g/kg group was eliminated
from the study due to the improper intubation of the
demonstrator.
Procedure. The experimental procedure was similar
to that of Experiment 1, except for a change in coffee
concentration and the dose of ethanol administered to
the demonstrators. Following 2 days of pair housing
in hanging cages, demonstrators were removed and
placed in an adjacent cage. Observers were then
water-deprived. Demonstrators were intragastrically
administered one of three doses of a 12.0% v/v ethanol
solution prepared in a tap water vehicle: 1.0, 1.5, or
3.0 g/kg. A water-intubated control group (0 g/kg) was
Periadolescent Ethanol Intake 105
administered a volume of tap water equal to the
volume administered to the 1.5 g/kg EtOH group.
Thirty minutes following intubation, the demonstrator
was placed into the observer's cage and the two
animals were allowed to interact for 30 min. The
demonstrator was again removed and the observer
was given a 24-hour test of intake of 5.6% v/v ethanol
vs. 1.8% w/v Sanka coffee. The concentration of the
coffee solution was decreased in order to reduce
baseline ethanol preference. This allowed us to more
clearly address the question of whether the preference
for ethanol established through interactions with a 3.0
g/kg demonstrator would be greater than that with a
1.5 g/kg demonstrator. The volume of each solution
ingested by the observers was recorded, and the data
were also expressed in terms of the dose of ethanol
ingested (g/kg/day).
Results
The data from this experiment are presented in Figure
2 and Table 2. As can be seen in the ®gure, observers
that had interacted with demonstrators administered
1.5 g/kg ethanol exhibited an increased intake of
ethanol vs. coffee, and therefore a higher daily dose of
ethanol consumed, replicating the results of Experi-
ment 1. However, observers that had interacted with
demonstrators administered either a lower (1.0 g/kg)
or a higher (3.0 g/kg) dose of ethanol failed to exhi-
bit a reliable change in ethanol intake. Finally, the
water observers (0 g/kg) showed approximately equal
intake of both solutions, similar to that observed in
Experiment 1.
A 4 (demonstrator dose)�2 (test solution)�2
(gender) ANOVA conducted on the intake data
revealed only a signi®cant main effect of gender [F
(1,39)�4.18, p < :05]. Generally, males ingested
more ¯uid during the test than females, although there
were no differences in terms of the proportions of
ethanol and coffee. Follow-up ANOVAs [2 (demon-
strator dose)�2 (test solution)] were used to compare
intakes of the 0.0 g/kg group with each of the other
groups. The only reliable demonstrator dose� test
solution interaction occurred for the 1.5 g/kg obser-
vers that exhibited increased intakes of ethanol, and
reduced intakes of coffee, relative to the water (0.0 g/
kg) controls [F (1,22)�5.77, p < :05]. The analyses
comparing the 0.0 with the 1.0 g/kg and 3.0 g/kg
groups, respectively, revealed no signi®cant diffe-
rences. The analyses of the ethanol dose ingested
yielded comparable results (see Table 2). The 4
(demonstrator dose) �2 (gender) ANOVA revealed
no signi®cant effects or interactions. Planned t-tests,
however, indicated that the dose of ethanol ingested
by the 1.5 g/kg observer group was signi®cantly
higher than that ingested by the water control group
[t (22)�2.36, p < :05]. No other differences were
statistically reliable.
Discussion
The results of Experiment 2 once again indicated that
observers that had brie¯y interacted with an intoxi-
cated sibling exhibited increased intake of ethanol
during a two-bottle choice test. Furthermore, there
was an inverted-U dose ± response relationship. Obser-
vers interacting with siblings given a 1.5 g/kg dose of
ethanol did increase their intake of ethanol; however,
observers interacting with siblings administered either
a lower (1.0 g/kg) or a higher (3.0 g/kg) dose failed to
exhibit altered ethanol ingestion.
The failure to obtain increased ethanol intake in
the low (1.0 g/kg) dose condition is likely due to
insuf®cient olfactory cues present on the breath of the
demonstrator animals. Decreased salience of these
cues would correspondingly reduce the ability of
observers to detect and/or learn about alcohol from the
demonstrators (cf. Molina & Chotro, 1989b). This
social modulation of ethanol intake can easily be
viewed within a Pavlovian conditioning perspective,
for which the ethanol olfactory cues detected on a
conspeci®c's breath serve as the conditioned stimulus
(CS) and some aspect of the social interaction serves
FIGURE 2 Mean (�SEM) intakes of ethanol and coffee
(ml) by observers in Experiment 2 during a 24-hour two-
bottle choice test. Observers had previously interacted for
30 min with demonstrators that had been intragastrically
administered 0, 1.0, 1.5, or 3.0 g/kg ethanol.
106 Hunt, Holloway, and Scordalakes
as the unconditioned stimulus (US). It is well known
that characteristics of the CS, as well as the US, can
in¯uence the rate and asymptotic level of conditioning
(e.g., Pearce & Hall, 1980; Rescorla & Wagner, 1972).
If the amount of respired ethanol cues is not of
suf®cient intensity, then minimal conditioning would
be expected. Therefore, little or no conditioned res-
ponse (ethanol intake above 50%) would be expected
to occur. This is precisely what was observed in
Experiment 2.
A different explanation, however, is required to
describe the failure of the 3.0 g/kg observers to exhibit
an ethanol preference. First, it is possible that the
severely intoxicated demonstrator was capable through
some means of communicating an aversion for ethanol
to the observer (but see Galef, Wigmore, & Kennett,
1983). The post-ingestional consequences of a dose of
3.0 g/kg ethanol have been found to condition taste
aversion to a concurrently presented sucrose solution,
at least in younger animals (Hunt, Spear, & Spear,
1991). Furthermore, preweanling rats administered a
3.0 g/kg dose will subsequently avoid both ethanol
odor and taste (e.g., Molina et al., 1989). It is therefore
possible that a severely intoxicated demonstrator
could transmit cues that serve to communicate even
a weak ethanol avoidance to an observer, one that was
capable of counteracting an established preference.
A second explanation for the failure of 3.0 g/kg
observers to acquire a preference for ethanol centers
on the ability of the observers to detect the ethanol
cues from the demonstrators. Clearly if the salience of
the CS in part determines the strength of conditioning,
then the intensity of the cues resulting from this high
dose of ethanol should be even greater than those from
the 1.5 g/kg condition (Hollstedt & Rydberg, 1985;
Pohorecky & Brick, 1982). Although the concentra-
tion of these cues might in fact be greater, the question
remains as to whether the observers were actually able
to detect these cues from the demonstrator. Casual
observation of the 3.0 g/kg demonstrators indicated
an almost complete lack of movement during the
30-minute interaction period; demonstrators tended to
lie in a prone posture with their mouths directed
toward the bottom of the cage. Although quantitative
measurements of peri-oral contact between demon-
strators and observers were not made, it is possible
that the observers were unable to detect the ethanol
cues emanating from the peri-oral region of the
3.0 g/kg demonstrators. In the absence of adequate
detection of ethanol orosensory cues, the transmission
of ethanol information from demonstrator to observer
would not occur. By this account then, the failure of
these observers to exhibit modi®ed ethanol ingestion
resulted from an inability to learn about ethanol.
GENERAL DISCUSSION
Among the ways that human infants, young children,
and adolescents can be exposed to alcohol in the
context of the home and other social environments is
through the detection of ethanol in the milk of a
breast-feeding mother, long-term exposure to the odor
of ethanol, direct topical application (e.g., for relief of
teething pain), and/or the detection of respired ethanol
cues (e.g., Fossey, 1993; Mennella & Beauchamp,
1991, 1993; Noll, Zucker, & Greenberg, 1990). It has
been suggested that young children acquire informa-
tion about alcohol via such types of exposure. Noll
et al. (1990) reported that children whose parents were
heavy drinkers were more likely to correctly identify
the odor of alcohol than children whose parents drank
only moderately or not at all. Another study reported
similar results from a cohort of Scottish and British
children (Fossey, 1993). In this study, the children
were capable of correctly identifying alcohol by smell
on over 80% of the occasions presented. Whether
exposure to ethanol cues in these situations would be
suf®cient to modify a human's later choice and
acceptance of this drug is not yet known.
Such experiential factors may indeed contribute
substantially to the initiation of alcohol drinking in
children and adolescents. It is known that children of
alcoholics tend to drink more than children of non-
alcoholics (U.S. Department of Health and Human
Services, 1993). Although genetics no doubt play an
important role here, the home environment is likely to
also be extremely important. Children of alcoholics
would spend more time in contact with alcohol-
related cues, potentially affecting these children's
learning of positive responses toward this drug.
In animal studies designed to model such situations,
young rats have consistently been shown to increase
their acceptance of ethanol following each type of
exposure. These results are highly suggestive that early
experiences with alcohol cues can impact alcohol inges-
tion patterns. For example, Hunt et al. (1993) investi-
gated alcohol intake in preweanling rats following
exposure to ethanol during a bout of nursing. Pups
12 ± 16 days of age increased their intake of ethanol
after experiencing alcohol-contaminated milk in con-
junction with the opportunity to suckle. Pepino,
Kraebel, Lopez, Spear, and Molina (1998) have addi-
tionally reported that exposure to alcohol-contami-
nated milk can result in altered motor and cardiac
responsivity toward ethanol cues in preweanlings.
Bannoura et al. (1998) raised rat pups from birth to
weaning in cages that were continuously scented with
ethanol odor. These pups exhibited a clear preference
for ethanol when tested for intake after weaning,
Periadolescent Ethanol Intake 107
compared with controls not exposed to ethanol cues.
These ®ndings could re¯ect a conditioned preference
for ethanol odor experienced in the context of social
interactions with the dam and littermates, or the effect
of familiarity from long-term repeated exposure
(Bronstein & Crockett, 1976; Leon, Galef, & Behse,
1977).
Once ingested, a proportion of the ethanol dose is
distributed throughout the body in unmetabolized
form and is expired via its accumulation in the lungs.
Additional routes of nonhepatic elimination involve
salivation, urination, and perspiration (Hollstedt &
Rydberg, 1985). Regardless of the actual route of
elimination, animals are known to be capable of
detecting these cues and learning about them in a
variety of experimental preparations in which social
contact with other animals is a critical feature (e.g.,
Bannoura et al., 1998; Chotro, Kraebel, McKinzie,
Molina, & Spear, 1996; Hunt et al., 2000). Hunt et al.
reported that social interactions with an intoxicated
sibling result in enhanced intake of ethanol by pre-
weanling animals, ranging in age from 8 to 16 days.
These results, along with those of previous research
(e.g., Chotro et al., 1996) indicate that exposure to
ethanol in social contexts can play a role in modifying
a preweanling animal's responsiveness toward etha-
nol. The results of the present experiments indicate that
a periadolescent rat will also acquire information
about alcohol cues from an intoxicated sibling, and as
a result will increase its subsequent intake of an etha-
nol solution. It is, however, unclear at this time, whether
or not exposure to ethanol-derived cues in these
situations would have a relatively long-term impact on
later alcohol choice (but see Galef, 1989; Hunt et al.,
2000, Experiment 3; Strupp & Levitsky, 1984).
Clearly, research is beginning to identify ways in
which early experiences involving direct exposure to
ethanol cues in the context of the home environment
can affect young organisms' later acceptance of this
drug. Continued investigation into the nature of the
alcohol cues detected, and the mechanisms through
which various types of learning take place, may help
to further elucidate the consequences of social learn-
ing for alcohol abuse in late childhood and adoles-
cence. Moreover, the present experimental procedure
may afford a relatively simple and robust experimental
situation in which to further study the role of social
experiences with alcohol in the initiation, and possibly
also the maintenance, of excessive ethanol intake.
NOTES
This research was supported by grants AA12135 and
AA12466 from the National Institutes of Alcohol
Abuse and Alcoholism to P.S.H.. Experiment 1 was
conducted as part of the requirements for a B.S. with
honors in biological psychology by E.M.S., and
Experiment 2 was conducted as part of the require-
ments for a master's degree in psychology by J.L.H..
We are grateful to Elena Cuticelli for her assistance
with data collection.
REFERENCES
Adams, N. (1995). Sex differences and the effects of tail
pinch on ethanol drinking in Maudsley rats. Alcohol, 12,
463 ± 468.
Bannoura, M. D., Kraebel, K. S., Spear, L. P., and Spear,
N. E. (1998). Effects of preweanling ethanol odor expo-
sure on ethanol preference. Alcohol, 15, 213 ± 217.
Bronstein, P. M., & Crockett, D. P. (1976). Exposure to the
odor of food determines the eating preferences of rat
Take Home PortionDirections Problems (150pts). This part, i.e. th.docx
Take Home PortionDirections Problems (150pts). This part, i.e. th.docx
Take Home PortionDirections Problems (150pts). This part, i.e. th.docx
Take Home PortionDirections Problems (150pts). This part, i.e. th.docx
Take Home PortionDirections Problems (150pts). This part, i.e. th.docx
Take Home PortionDirections Problems (150pts). This part, i.e. th.docx
Take Home PortionDirections Problems (150pts). This part, i.e. th.docx
Take Home PortionDirections Problems (150pts). This part, i.e. th.docx
Take Home PortionDirections Problems (150pts). This part, i.e. th.docx

More Related Content

Similar to Take Home PortionDirections Problems (150pts). This part, i.e. th.docx

Vanderwall cheminformatics Drexel Part 1
Vanderwall cheminformatics Drexel Part 1Vanderwall cheminformatics Drexel Part 1
Vanderwall cheminformatics Drexel Part 1Jean-Claude Bradley
 
Plant Leaf Diseases Identification in Deep Learning
Plant Leaf Diseases Identification in Deep LearningPlant Leaf Diseases Identification in Deep Learning
Plant Leaf Diseases Identification in Deep LearningCSEIJJournal
 
Using the Semantic Web to Support Ecoinformatics
Using the Semantic Web to Support EcoinformaticsUsing the Semantic Web to Support Ecoinformatics
Using the Semantic Web to Support Ecoinformaticsebiquity
 
Retrospective and Prospective Studies of Gastro-Intestinal Helminths of Human...
Retrospective and Prospective Studies of Gastro-Intestinal Helminths of Human...Retrospective and Prospective Studies of Gastro-Intestinal Helminths of Human...
Retrospective and Prospective Studies of Gastro-Intestinal Helminths of Human...theijes
 
Essay On Topic United We Stand. Online assignment writing service.
Essay On Topic United We Stand. Online assignment writing service.Essay On Topic United We Stand. Online assignment writing service.
Essay On Topic United We Stand. Online assignment writing service.Andrea Briggs
 
ICPSR - Complex Systems Models in the Social Sciences - Lab Session 5 - Profe...
ICPSR - Complex Systems Models in the Social Sciences - Lab Session 5 - Profe...ICPSR - Complex Systems Models in the Social Sciences - Lab Session 5 - Profe...
ICPSR - Complex Systems Models in the Social Sciences - Lab Session 5 - Profe...Daniel Katz
 
GenomeTrakr: Perspectives on linking internationally - Canada and IRIDA.ca
GenomeTrakr: Perspectives on linking internationally - Canada and IRIDA.caGenomeTrakr: Perspectives on linking internationally - Canada and IRIDA.ca
GenomeTrakr: Perspectives on linking internationally - Canada and IRIDA.cafionabrinkman
 
Lessons From The Core: Longitudinal Assessment vs. Point Sampling of Behavior...
Lessons From The Core: Longitudinal Assessment vs. Point Sampling of Behavior...Lessons From The Core: Longitudinal Assessment vs. Point Sampling of Behavior...
Lessons From The Core: Longitudinal Assessment vs. Point Sampling of Behavior...InsideScientific
 
Test ch 2 2012
Test ch 2 2012Test ch 2 2012
Test ch 2 2012fearonc
 
How long should Offspring Lifespan be in order to obtain a proper exploration?
How long should Offspring Lifespan be in order to obtain a proper exploration?How long should Offspring Lifespan be in order to obtain a proper exploration?
How long should Offspring Lifespan be in order to obtain a proper exploration?Mario Pavone
 
Scott Edmunds: GigaScience - a journal or a database? Lessons learned from th...
Scott Edmunds: GigaScience - a journal or a database? Lessons learned from th...Scott Edmunds: GigaScience - a journal or a database? Lessons learned from th...
Scott Edmunds: GigaScience - a journal or a database? Lessons learned from th...GigaScience, BGI Hong Kong
 
Exploiting NLP for Digital Disease Informatics
Exploiting NLP for Digital Disease InformaticsExploiting NLP for Digital Disease Informatics
Exploiting NLP for Digital Disease InformaticsNigel Collier
 
Bed Bug Biology and Research Central Ohio Bed Bug Task Force
Bed Bug Biology and Research Central Ohio Bed Bug Task ForceBed Bug Biology and Research Central Ohio Bed Bug Task Force
Bed Bug Biology and Research Central Ohio Bed Bug Task ForceCentral Ohio Bed Bug Task Force
 
Thrive:Timely Health Indicators Using Remote Sensing & innovation for the Vit...
Thrive:Timely Health Indicators Using Remote Sensing & innovation for the Vit...Thrive:Timely Health Indicators Using Remote Sensing & innovation for the Vit...
Thrive:Timely Health Indicators Using Remote Sensing & innovation for the Vit...US-Ignite
 
2010StanfordE25 Michele dragoescu e25 project
2010StanfordE25 Michele dragoescu e25 project2010StanfordE25 Michele dragoescu e25 project
2010StanfordE25 Michele dragoescu e25 projectmdragoescu
 

Similar to Take Home PortionDirections Problems (150pts). This part, i.e. th.docx (20)

Vanderwall cheminformatics Drexel Part 1
Vanderwall cheminformatics Drexel Part 1Vanderwall cheminformatics Drexel Part 1
Vanderwall cheminformatics Drexel Part 1
 
Biodiversity Management
Biodiversity ManagementBiodiversity Management
Biodiversity Management
 
Plant Leaf Diseases Identification in Deep Learning
Plant Leaf Diseases Identification in Deep LearningPlant Leaf Diseases Identification in Deep Learning
Plant Leaf Diseases Identification in Deep Learning
 
MedEx'10
MedEx'10MedEx'10
MedEx'10
 
Using the Semantic Web to Support Ecoinformatics
Using the Semantic Web to Support EcoinformaticsUsing the Semantic Web to Support Ecoinformatics
Using the Semantic Web to Support Ecoinformatics
 
Retrospective and Prospective Studies of Gastro-Intestinal Helminths of Human...
Retrospective and Prospective Studies of Gastro-Intestinal Helminths of Human...Retrospective and Prospective Studies of Gastro-Intestinal Helminths of Human...
Retrospective and Prospective Studies of Gastro-Intestinal Helminths of Human...
 
Essay On Topic United We Stand. Online assignment writing service.
Essay On Topic United We Stand. Online assignment writing service.Essay On Topic United We Stand. Online assignment writing service.
Essay On Topic United We Stand. Online assignment writing service.
 
ICPSR - Complex Systems Models in the Social Sciences - Lab Session 5 - Profe...
ICPSR - Complex Systems Models in the Social Sciences - Lab Session 5 - Profe...ICPSR - Complex Systems Models in the Social Sciences - Lab Session 5 - Profe...
ICPSR - Complex Systems Models in the Social Sciences - Lab Session 5 - Profe...
 
GenomeTrakr: Perspectives on linking internationally - Canada and IRIDA.ca
GenomeTrakr: Perspectives on linking internationally - Canada and IRIDA.caGenomeTrakr: Perspectives on linking internationally - Canada and IRIDA.ca
GenomeTrakr: Perspectives on linking internationally - Canada and IRIDA.ca
 
Ijetr042156
Ijetr042156Ijetr042156
Ijetr042156
 
Lessons From The Core: Longitudinal Assessment vs. Point Sampling of Behavior...
Lessons From The Core: Longitudinal Assessment vs. Point Sampling of Behavior...Lessons From The Core: Longitudinal Assessment vs. Point Sampling of Behavior...
Lessons From The Core: Longitudinal Assessment vs. Point Sampling of Behavior...
 
Test ch 2 2012
Test ch 2 2012Test ch 2 2012
Test ch 2 2012
 
Biosurveillance 2.0
Biosurveillance 2.0Biosurveillance 2.0
Biosurveillance 2.0
 
research publication
research publicationresearch publication
research publication
 
How long should Offspring Lifespan be in order to obtain a proper exploration?
How long should Offspring Lifespan be in order to obtain a proper exploration?How long should Offspring Lifespan be in order to obtain a proper exploration?
How long should Offspring Lifespan be in order to obtain a proper exploration?
 
Scott Edmunds: GigaScience - a journal or a database? Lessons learned from th...
Scott Edmunds: GigaScience - a journal or a database? Lessons learned from th...Scott Edmunds: GigaScience - a journal or a database? Lessons learned from th...
Scott Edmunds: GigaScience - a journal or a database? Lessons learned from th...
 
Exploiting NLP for Digital Disease Informatics
Exploiting NLP for Digital Disease InformaticsExploiting NLP for Digital Disease Informatics
Exploiting NLP for Digital Disease Informatics
 
Bed Bug Biology and Research Central Ohio Bed Bug Task Force
Bed Bug Biology and Research Central Ohio Bed Bug Task ForceBed Bug Biology and Research Central Ohio Bed Bug Task Force
Bed Bug Biology and Research Central Ohio Bed Bug Task Force
 
Thrive:Timely Health Indicators Using Remote Sensing & innovation for the Vit...
Thrive:Timely Health Indicators Using Remote Sensing & innovation for the Vit...Thrive:Timely Health Indicators Using Remote Sensing & innovation for the Vit...
Thrive:Timely Health Indicators Using Remote Sensing & innovation for the Vit...
 
2010StanfordE25 Michele dragoescu e25 project
2010StanfordE25 Michele dragoescu e25 project2010StanfordE25 Michele dragoescu e25 project
2010StanfordE25 Michele dragoescu e25 project
 

More from ssuserf9c51d

Muslims in the Golden Age is the theme for the research project. You.docx
Muslims in the Golden Age is the theme for the research project. You.docxMuslims in the Golden Age is the theme for the research project. You.docx
Muslims in the Golden Age is the theme for the research project. You.docxssuserf9c51d
 
Multiple Sources of MediaExamine the impact of multiple sour.docx
Multiple Sources of MediaExamine the impact of multiple sour.docxMultiple Sources of MediaExamine the impact of multiple sour.docx
Multiple Sources of MediaExamine the impact of multiple sour.docxssuserf9c51d
 
Multicultural Event WrittenPlease choose and research a cult.docx
Multicultural Event WrittenPlease choose and research a cult.docxMulticultural Event WrittenPlease choose and research a cult.docx
Multicultural Event WrittenPlease choose and research a cult.docxssuserf9c51d
 
Multi-Party NegotiationFor this Essay, you will explore the co.docx
Multi-Party NegotiationFor this Essay, you will explore the co.docxMulti-Party NegotiationFor this Essay, you will explore the co.docx
Multi-Party NegotiationFor this Essay, you will explore the co.docxssuserf9c51d
 
Music has long been used by movements seeking social change.  In the.docx
Music has long been used by movements seeking social change.  In the.docxMusic has long been used by movements seeking social change.  In the.docx
Music has long been used by movements seeking social change.  In the.docxssuserf9c51d
 
MSW Advanced Clinical Concentration -Student Learning AgreementW.docx
MSW Advanced Clinical Concentration -Student Learning AgreementW.docxMSW Advanced Clinical Concentration -Student Learning AgreementW.docx
MSW Advanced Clinical Concentration -Student Learning AgreementW.docxssuserf9c51d
 
Multimedia Instructional MaterialsStaying current on technolog.docx
Multimedia Instructional MaterialsStaying current on technolog.docxMultimedia Instructional MaterialsStaying current on technolog.docx
Multimedia Instructional MaterialsStaying current on technolog.docxssuserf9c51d
 
Murray Bowen is one of the most respected family theorists in th.docx
Murray Bowen is one of the most respected family theorists in th.docxMurray Bowen is one of the most respected family theorists in th.docx
Murray Bowen is one of the most respected family theorists in th.docxssuserf9c51d
 
Mrs. Thomas is a 54, year old African American widow, mother and gra.docx
Mrs. Thomas is a 54, year old African American widow, mother and gra.docxMrs. Thomas is a 54, year old African American widow, mother and gra.docx
Mrs. Thomas is a 54, year old African American widow, mother and gra.docxssuserf9c51d
 
Multiple Source Essay, Speculating about CausesProposing a Solution.docx
Multiple Source Essay, Speculating about CausesProposing a Solution.docxMultiple Source Essay, Speculating about CausesProposing a Solution.docx
Multiple Source Essay, Speculating about CausesProposing a Solution.docxssuserf9c51d
 
Multiyear Plans Please respond to the followingDo you.docx
Multiyear Plans Please respond to the followingDo you.docxMultiyear Plans Please respond to the followingDo you.docx
Multiyear Plans Please respond to the followingDo you.docxssuserf9c51d
 
Multinational Financial ManagementDetermine key reasons wh.docx
Multinational Financial ManagementDetermine key reasons wh.docxMultinational Financial ManagementDetermine key reasons wh.docx
Multinational Financial ManagementDetermine key reasons wh.docxssuserf9c51d
 
Murder CasePreambleAn organization system administrator .docx
Murder CasePreambleAn organization system administrator .docxMurder CasePreambleAn organization system administrator .docx
Murder CasePreambleAn organization system administrator .docxssuserf9c51d
 
Multimodal Personal Narrative – Develop a multimodal document to bot.docx
Multimodal Personal Narrative – Develop a multimodal document to bot.docxMultimodal Personal Narrative – Develop a multimodal document to bot.docx
Multimodal Personal Narrative – Develop a multimodal document to bot.docxssuserf9c51d
 
Multigenre ProjectEN101O Fall 2019 Dr. WalterA Multigenre Pr.docx
Multigenre ProjectEN101O Fall 2019 Dr. WalterA Multigenre Pr.docxMultigenre ProjectEN101O Fall 2019 Dr. WalterA Multigenre Pr.docx
Multigenre ProjectEN101O Fall 2019 Dr. WalterA Multigenre Pr.docxssuserf9c51d
 
Multimedia activity Business OrganizationVisit the Choose Your .docx
Multimedia activity Business OrganizationVisit the Choose Your .docxMultimedia activity Business OrganizationVisit the Choose Your .docx
Multimedia activity Business OrganizationVisit the Choose Your .docxssuserf9c51d
 
Multicultural PerspectiveToday’s classrooms are diverse and .docx
Multicultural PerspectiveToday’s classrooms are diverse and .docxMulticultural PerspectiveToday’s classrooms are diverse and .docx
Multicultural PerspectiveToday’s classrooms are diverse and .docxssuserf9c51d
 
Muhammad Ali, how did his refusal to go into the army affect his.docx
Muhammad Ali, how did his refusal to go into the army affect his.docxMuhammad Ali, how did his refusal to go into the army affect his.docx
Muhammad Ali, how did his refusal to go into the army affect his.docxssuserf9c51d
 
MS 113 Some key concepts that you need to know to navigate th.docx
MS 113 Some key concepts that you need to know to navigate th.docxMS 113 Some key concepts that you need to know to navigate th.docx
MS 113 Some key concepts that you need to know to navigate th.docxssuserf9c51d
 
Much has been made of the new Web 2.0 phenomenon, including social n.docx
Much has been made of the new Web 2.0 phenomenon, including social n.docxMuch has been made of the new Web 2.0 phenomenon, including social n.docx
Much has been made of the new Web 2.0 phenomenon, including social n.docxssuserf9c51d
 

More from ssuserf9c51d (20)

Muslims in the Golden Age is the theme for the research project. You.docx
Muslims in the Golden Age is the theme for the research project. You.docxMuslims in the Golden Age is the theme for the research project. You.docx
Muslims in the Golden Age is the theme for the research project. You.docx
 
Multiple Sources of MediaExamine the impact of multiple sour.docx
Multiple Sources of MediaExamine the impact of multiple sour.docxMultiple Sources of MediaExamine the impact of multiple sour.docx
Multiple Sources of MediaExamine the impact of multiple sour.docx
 
Multicultural Event WrittenPlease choose and research a cult.docx
Multicultural Event WrittenPlease choose and research a cult.docxMulticultural Event WrittenPlease choose and research a cult.docx
Multicultural Event WrittenPlease choose and research a cult.docx
 
Multi-Party NegotiationFor this Essay, you will explore the co.docx
Multi-Party NegotiationFor this Essay, you will explore the co.docxMulti-Party NegotiationFor this Essay, you will explore the co.docx
Multi-Party NegotiationFor this Essay, you will explore the co.docx
 
Music has long been used by movements seeking social change.  In the.docx
Music has long been used by movements seeking social change.  In the.docxMusic has long been used by movements seeking social change.  In the.docx
Music has long been used by movements seeking social change.  In the.docx
 
MSW Advanced Clinical Concentration -Student Learning AgreementW.docx
MSW Advanced Clinical Concentration -Student Learning AgreementW.docxMSW Advanced Clinical Concentration -Student Learning AgreementW.docx
MSW Advanced Clinical Concentration -Student Learning AgreementW.docx
 
Multimedia Instructional MaterialsStaying current on technolog.docx
Multimedia Instructional MaterialsStaying current on technolog.docxMultimedia Instructional MaterialsStaying current on technolog.docx
Multimedia Instructional MaterialsStaying current on technolog.docx
 
Murray Bowen is one of the most respected family theorists in th.docx
Murray Bowen is one of the most respected family theorists in th.docxMurray Bowen is one of the most respected family theorists in th.docx
Murray Bowen is one of the most respected family theorists in th.docx
 
Mrs. Thomas is a 54, year old African American widow, mother and gra.docx
Mrs. Thomas is a 54, year old African American widow, mother and gra.docxMrs. Thomas is a 54, year old African American widow, mother and gra.docx
Mrs. Thomas is a 54, year old African American widow, mother and gra.docx
 
Multiple Source Essay, Speculating about CausesProposing a Solution.docx
Multiple Source Essay, Speculating about CausesProposing a Solution.docxMultiple Source Essay, Speculating about CausesProposing a Solution.docx
Multiple Source Essay, Speculating about CausesProposing a Solution.docx
 
Multiyear Plans Please respond to the followingDo you.docx
Multiyear Plans Please respond to the followingDo you.docxMultiyear Plans Please respond to the followingDo you.docx
Multiyear Plans Please respond to the followingDo you.docx
 
Multinational Financial ManagementDetermine key reasons wh.docx
Multinational Financial ManagementDetermine key reasons wh.docxMultinational Financial ManagementDetermine key reasons wh.docx
Multinational Financial ManagementDetermine key reasons wh.docx
 
Murder CasePreambleAn organization system administrator .docx
Murder CasePreambleAn organization system administrator .docxMurder CasePreambleAn organization system administrator .docx
Murder CasePreambleAn organization system administrator .docx
 
Multimodal Personal Narrative – Develop a multimodal document to bot.docx
Multimodal Personal Narrative – Develop a multimodal document to bot.docxMultimodal Personal Narrative – Develop a multimodal document to bot.docx
Multimodal Personal Narrative – Develop a multimodal document to bot.docx
 
Multigenre ProjectEN101O Fall 2019 Dr. WalterA Multigenre Pr.docx
Multigenre ProjectEN101O Fall 2019 Dr. WalterA Multigenre Pr.docxMultigenre ProjectEN101O Fall 2019 Dr. WalterA Multigenre Pr.docx
Multigenre ProjectEN101O Fall 2019 Dr. WalterA Multigenre Pr.docx
 
Multimedia activity Business OrganizationVisit the Choose Your .docx
Multimedia activity Business OrganizationVisit the Choose Your .docxMultimedia activity Business OrganizationVisit the Choose Your .docx
Multimedia activity Business OrganizationVisit the Choose Your .docx
 
Multicultural PerspectiveToday’s classrooms are diverse and .docx
Multicultural PerspectiveToday’s classrooms are diverse and .docxMulticultural PerspectiveToday’s classrooms are diverse and .docx
Multicultural PerspectiveToday’s classrooms are diverse and .docx
 
Muhammad Ali, how did his refusal to go into the army affect his.docx
Muhammad Ali, how did his refusal to go into the army affect his.docxMuhammad Ali, how did his refusal to go into the army affect his.docx
Muhammad Ali, how did his refusal to go into the army affect his.docx
 
MS 113 Some key concepts that you need to know to navigate th.docx
MS 113 Some key concepts that you need to know to navigate th.docxMS 113 Some key concepts that you need to know to navigate th.docx
MS 113 Some key concepts that you need to know to navigate th.docx
 
Much has been made of the new Web 2.0 phenomenon, including social n.docx
Much has been made of the new Web 2.0 phenomenon, including social n.docxMuch has been made of the new Web 2.0 phenomenon, including social n.docx
Much has been made of the new Web 2.0 phenomenon, including social n.docx
 

Recently uploaded

Russian Escort Service in Delhi 11k Hotel Foreigner Russian Call Girls in Delhi
Russian Escort Service in Delhi 11k Hotel Foreigner Russian Call Girls in DelhiRussian Escort Service in Delhi 11k Hotel Foreigner Russian Call Girls in Delhi
Russian Escort Service in Delhi 11k Hotel Foreigner Russian Call Girls in Delhikauryashika82
 
Web & Social Media Analytics Previous Year Question Paper.pdf
Web & Social Media Analytics Previous Year Question Paper.pdfWeb & Social Media Analytics Previous Year Question Paper.pdf
Web & Social Media Analytics Previous Year Question Paper.pdfJayanti Pande
 
Z Score,T Score, Percential Rank and Box Plot Graph
Z Score,T Score, Percential Rank and Box Plot GraphZ Score,T Score, Percential Rank and Box Plot Graph
Z Score,T Score, Percential Rank and Box Plot GraphThiyagu K
 
Explore beautiful and ugly buildings. Mathematics helps us create beautiful d...
Explore beautiful and ugly buildings. Mathematics helps us create beautiful d...Explore beautiful and ugly buildings. Mathematics helps us create beautiful d...
Explore beautiful and ugly buildings. Mathematics helps us create beautiful d...christianmathematics
 
Class 11th Physics NEET formula sheet pdf
Class 11th Physics NEET formula sheet pdfClass 11th Physics NEET formula sheet pdf
Class 11th Physics NEET formula sheet pdfAyushMahapatra5
 
Accessible design: Minimum effort, maximum impact
Accessible design: Minimum effort, maximum impactAccessible design: Minimum effort, maximum impact
Accessible design: Minimum effort, maximum impactdawncurless
 
Ecosystem Interactions Class Discussion Presentation in Blue Green Lined Styl...
Ecosystem Interactions Class Discussion Presentation in Blue Green Lined Styl...Ecosystem Interactions Class Discussion Presentation in Blue Green Lined Styl...
Ecosystem Interactions Class Discussion Presentation in Blue Green Lined Styl...fonyou31
 
Advanced Views - Calendar View in Odoo 17
Advanced Views - Calendar View in Odoo 17Advanced Views - Calendar View in Odoo 17
Advanced Views - Calendar View in Odoo 17Celine George
 
Paris 2024 Olympic Geographies - an activity
Paris 2024 Olympic Geographies - an activityParis 2024 Olympic Geographies - an activity
Paris 2024 Olympic Geographies - an activityGeoBlogs
 
Measures of Central Tendency: Mean, Median and Mode
Measures of Central Tendency: Mean, Median and ModeMeasures of Central Tendency: Mean, Median and Mode
Measures of Central Tendency: Mean, Median and ModeThiyagu K
 
microwave assisted reaction. General introduction
microwave assisted reaction. General introductionmicrowave assisted reaction. General introduction
microwave assisted reaction. General introductionMaksud Ahmed
 
Arihant handbook biology for class 11 .pdf
Arihant handbook biology for class 11 .pdfArihant handbook biology for class 11 .pdf
Arihant handbook biology for class 11 .pdfchloefrazer622
 
Q4-W6-Restating Informational Text Grade 3
Q4-W6-Restating Informational Text Grade 3Q4-W6-Restating Informational Text Grade 3
Q4-W6-Restating Informational Text Grade 3JemimahLaneBuaron
 
Sanyam Choudhary Chemistry practical.pdf
Sanyam Choudhary Chemistry practical.pdfSanyam Choudhary Chemistry practical.pdf
Sanyam Choudhary Chemistry practical.pdfsanyamsingh5019
 
BASLIQ CURRENT LOOKBOOK LOOKBOOK(1) (1).pdf
BASLIQ CURRENT LOOKBOOK  LOOKBOOK(1) (1).pdfBASLIQ CURRENT LOOKBOOK  LOOKBOOK(1) (1).pdf
BASLIQ CURRENT LOOKBOOK LOOKBOOK(1) (1).pdfSoniaTolstoy
 
General AI for Medical Educators April 2024
General AI for Medical Educators April 2024General AI for Medical Educators April 2024
General AI for Medical Educators April 2024Janet Corral
 

Recently uploaded (20)

Russian Escort Service in Delhi 11k Hotel Foreigner Russian Call Girls in Delhi
Russian Escort Service in Delhi 11k Hotel Foreigner Russian Call Girls in DelhiRussian Escort Service in Delhi 11k Hotel Foreigner Russian Call Girls in Delhi
Russian Escort Service in Delhi 11k Hotel Foreigner Russian Call Girls in Delhi
 
Web & Social Media Analytics Previous Year Question Paper.pdf
Web & Social Media Analytics Previous Year Question Paper.pdfWeb & Social Media Analytics Previous Year Question Paper.pdf
Web & Social Media Analytics Previous Year Question Paper.pdf
 
Z Score,T Score, Percential Rank and Box Plot Graph
Z Score,T Score, Percential Rank and Box Plot GraphZ Score,T Score, Percential Rank and Box Plot Graph
Z Score,T Score, Percential Rank and Box Plot Graph
 
Explore beautiful and ugly buildings. Mathematics helps us create beautiful d...
Explore beautiful and ugly buildings. Mathematics helps us create beautiful d...Explore beautiful and ugly buildings. Mathematics helps us create beautiful d...
Explore beautiful and ugly buildings. Mathematics helps us create beautiful d...
 
Class 11th Physics NEET formula sheet pdf
Class 11th Physics NEET formula sheet pdfClass 11th Physics NEET formula sheet pdf
Class 11th Physics NEET formula sheet pdf
 
Accessible design: Minimum effort, maximum impact
Accessible design: Minimum effort, maximum impactAccessible design: Minimum effort, maximum impact
Accessible design: Minimum effort, maximum impact
 
Ecosystem Interactions Class Discussion Presentation in Blue Green Lined Styl...
Ecosystem Interactions Class Discussion Presentation in Blue Green Lined Styl...Ecosystem Interactions Class Discussion Presentation in Blue Green Lined Styl...
Ecosystem Interactions Class Discussion Presentation in Blue Green Lined Styl...
 
Mattingly "AI & Prompt Design: The Basics of Prompt Design"
Mattingly "AI & Prompt Design: The Basics of Prompt Design"Mattingly "AI & Prompt Design: The Basics of Prompt Design"
Mattingly "AI & Prompt Design: The Basics of Prompt Design"
 
Advanced Views - Calendar View in Odoo 17
Advanced Views - Calendar View in Odoo 17Advanced Views - Calendar View in Odoo 17
Advanced Views - Calendar View in Odoo 17
 
Advance Mobile Application Development class 07
Advance Mobile Application Development class 07Advance Mobile Application Development class 07
Advance Mobile Application Development class 07
 
Paris 2024 Olympic Geographies - an activity
Paris 2024 Olympic Geographies - an activityParis 2024 Olympic Geographies - an activity
Paris 2024 Olympic Geographies - an activity
 
Measures of Central Tendency: Mean, Median and Mode
Measures of Central Tendency: Mean, Median and ModeMeasures of Central Tendency: Mean, Median and Mode
Measures of Central Tendency: Mean, Median and Mode
 
microwave assisted reaction. General introduction
microwave assisted reaction. General introductionmicrowave assisted reaction. General introduction
microwave assisted reaction. General introduction
 
Arihant handbook biology for class 11 .pdf
Arihant handbook biology for class 11 .pdfArihant handbook biology for class 11 .pdf
Arihant handbook biology for class 11 .pdf
 
Q4-W6-Restating Informational Text Grade 3
Q4-W6-Restating Informational Text Grade 3Q4-W6-Restating Informational Text Grade 3
Q4-W6-Restating Informational Text Grade 3
 
INDIA QUIZ 2024 RLAC DELHI UNIVERSITY.pptx
INDIA QUIZ 2024 RLAC DELHI UNIVERSITY.pptxINDIA QUIZ 2024 RLAC DELHI UNIVERSITY.pptx
INDIA QUIZ 2024 RLAC DELHI UNIVERSITY.pptx
 
Mattingly "AI & Prompt Design: Structured Data, Assistants, & RAG"
Mattingly "AI & Prompt Design: Structured Data, Assistants, & RAG"Mattingly "AI & Prompt Design: Structured Data, Assistants, & RAG"
Mattingly "AI & Prompt Design: Structured Data, Assistants, & RAG"
 
Sanyam Choudhary Chemistry practical.pdf
Sanyam Choudhary Chemistry practical.pdfSanyam Choudhary Chemistry practical.pdf
Sanyam Choudhary Chemistry practical.pdf
 
BASLIQ CURRENT LOOKBOOK LOOKBOOK(1) (1).pdf
BASLIQ CURRENT LOOKBOOK  LOOKBOOK(1) (1).pdfBASLIQ CURRENT LOOKBOOK  LOOKBOOK(1) (1).pdf
BASLIQ CURRENT LOOKBOOK LOOKBOOK(1) (1).pdf
 
General AI for Medical Educators April 2024
General AI for Medical Educators April 2024General AI for Medical Educators April 2024
General AI for Medical Educators April 2024
 

Take Home PortionDirections Problems (150pts). This part, i.e. th.docx

  • 1. Take Home Portion Directions: Problems (150pts). This part, i.e. the Problems section is ‘open internet’ meaning that you can use any online source, physical book, slides from class, or personal notes on the take home portion. Doing so will result in a significant grade penalty (an F for the course). I will be employing a number of cheat detection mechanisms to ensure compliance. You must submit all of your answers as typed text in-line in this test document. Graphs may be hand-drawn and scanned in or created digitally.Problems [___/150] 1. (90pts) you are a security analyst that is consulting with an industrial factory called Fictional Terrible Company (FTC) that makes a special type of chemical called “Destructoviralbacteriumuraniumpostapocalypseicus” (DVBUP for short). FTC is regulated by the Environmental Protection Agency (EPA) and has to pay for each violation they have: The EPA provides the following information about penalties: Laws and regulations Fine for each violation (per day per incident) Hazardous Waste Disposal $71,264 Clean Air Act $95,284 Clean Water Act $52,414 Safe Drinking Water Act $54,789 FTC provides the following information about their industrial plant Below is the table of the past problems we have had. Problem Incidents
  • 2. Avg. Number of Days to mitigate Sabotage Pipe breakages that have led to DVBUP release 16 times in the past 8 years 2 Insecure SCADA system that has led to DVBUP release 2 times in the past 8 years 15 FTC also states the following: · 8 of the pipe breakages caused a Clean Air Act violation, 4 caused a Clean Water Act violation, and 4 lead to violations of both the Clean Air and Clean Water acts · 1 of the times, when hackers breached our SCADA systems, they released materials into the air, the other led to a release that went into the water, but also got into the drinking water table causing us to pay penalties for the both the clean water act and the safe drinking water act · We’ve not had any Hazardous Waste Disposal issues a) (35pts) State the two security problems as threats quantitatively using Expected Threat Impact (ETI) and Annual Threat Loss Expectancy (ATLE). Show all of the work. b) (35pts) Given your calculations in a) you confer with Bill Mahoney of the UNO Cybersecurity department to come up with a secure SCADA architecture. Bill says that, for $600,000 he can assemble a team of students at UNO that can mitigate the
  • 3. problem. Based on a look at the national numbers, you figure the solution will reduce the number of exploitations to once in 10 years – but wont change how long it takes to mitigate. You also confer with a physical security company to look into new fencing that will reduce the sabotage incident frequency by 90% (i.e. to once every 5 years). The fencing will cost $500,000. Draw a single decision tree that shows ALL of the different options available. Use the information above to estimate the probabilities and costs at each step. c) (20pts) Assuming a risk neutral approach, is it cheaper to mitigate one SCADA, sabotage, both problems, or to do nothing in a 1 year time interval? What about a 5 year time interval? Use the decision tree to justify your answer 2. (60pts) You are given the following policy from the NIST SP 800-53. IA-3: DEVICE IDENTIFICATION AND AUTHENTICATION The information system uniquely identifies and authenticates
  • 4. [Assignment: organization-defined specific and/or types of devices] before establishing a [Selection (one or more): local; remote; network] connection. Assume the organization in question – FTC from question 1, has instantiated the policy using the following selections as follows. The information system uniquely identifies and authenticates mobile phones and IoT devices before establishing a network connection. a) (35pts) Write a first order logic statement that represents the control for FTC. b) (15pts) Identify a non-compliant state (i.e. scenario that is not acceptable by the policy) using first order logic. c) (10pts) Given your answers, draw a Venn diagram that visibly shows the “secure states” and “insecure states” for the information system. Assume a closed-world assumption is in place (i.e. only your logic statement defines what is secure and insecure.
  • 5. Anim. Behav., 1983, 31, 748-758 TRANSFER OF INFORMATION CONCERNING DISTANT FOODS: A LABORATORY INVESTIGATION OF THE 'INFORMATION-CENTRE' HYPOTHESIS BY BENNEIT G. GALEF, JR & STEPHEN W. WIGMORE Department of Psychology, McMaster University, Hamilton, Ontario, L8S 4Kl Canada Abstract. When choosing between two novel diets, an 'observer' rat (Rattus norvegicus) prefers that diet previously eaten by a 'demonstrator' conspecific with whom the observer has interacted prior to making its choice between diets. Demonstrator influence on observer diet selection is maintained even if, during the period of demonstrator-observer interaction, the demonstrator is anaesthetized and a wire-mesh barrier prevents the demonstrator from physically contacting the observer. Demonstrator influence on observer diet choice is blocked by either rendering the observer anosmic or placing a transparent Plexiglas barrier between demonstrator and observer during their period of interaction. We conclude
  • 6. that olfactory cues passing from demonstrator to observer provide observers with information con- cerning demonstrators' diets and that these olfactory cues are sufficient to bias diet selection by ob- servers. Further, observer rats that hav~ learned the locations at which each of three diets are inter- mittently available can use information provided by demonstrators to increase foraging efficiency. We discuss the implications of the results both for the 'information- centre' hypothesis of the function of mammalian aggregations and for previous studies of social transmission of diet preference in rats. Wild Norway rats (R. norvegicus) are social, central-place foragers. Each rat lives as a member of a colony inhabiting a fixed system of burrows from which colony members emerge to forage and to which they subsequently return (Calhoun 1962; Telle 1966). It has been suggested that in environments characterized by an unpredictable and patchy distribution of food, social birds or mammals which forage from a central place could benefit from exchange of information with conspecifics at the central site concerning the availablility of food in the larger environment (Ward & Zahavi 1973; Erwin 1977; Bertram 1978 ; Waltz 1982). This 'information-centre' hypothe- sis concerning the function of vertebrate aggre- gations carries the implication that members of vertebrate social groups can acquire information from others of their species concerning potential feeding sites or foods remote in both time and space from the locus of information trans- mission (Ward & Zahavi 1973; De Groot 1980).
  • 7. Although the hypothesis that aggregation sites of birds and mammals may serve as information-centres is an attractive one, evi- dence sufficient to establish such a function of sociality in vertebrates has proven difficult to collect in field situations. Supporting evidence is largely circumstantial or comparative rather than direct (De Groot 1980). Further, there is little evidence from laboratory study of either mammals or birds of behavioural mechanisms sufficient to support the transmission from one individual to another of information concerning distant food sources. In the literature, it is usually assumed that unsuccessful vertebrate foragers can use behavioural cues to identify successful foragers at a central site such as a burrow or roost, and will then follow a successful individual to food (Ward 1965; Ward & Zahavi 1973; De Groot 1980; Waltz 1982). It is, of course, possible that information about distant foods can be transferred via behavioural mechanisms other than following to a food source (von Frisch 1967). For example, results of previous studies in our laboratory indicate that flavour cues contained in the milk of a lactating rat reflect the flavour of her diet. These flavours in mother's milk are sufficient to en- able pups nursing from a dam to recognize her diet at weaning and to cause pups to seek their mother's diet when initiating feeding outside the nest (Galef & Clark 1972; Galef & Henderson 1972; Galef & Sherry 1973). Thus the nest-site serves as an information-centre for weanling
  • 8. rat pups where they acquire information about foods distant in both time and space. Such an information-centre is a restricted one in that the medium for information transfer (mother's milk) is available only to lactating females and the information contained in the medium can only be received by suckling young. There is also evidence suggesting that adult rats might be able to exchange information 748 IIbl 2 Ooys 1 Doy 15 Min TEST [E!NIA.-- ---.. ~0, ~' lot. STEP 1 STEP 2 STEP 3 STEP 4 STEP 5 GALEF & WIGMORE: INFORMATION TRANSFER IN RATS concerning distant foods. Strupp (1982) has shown that an observer rat, choosing between two diets available to a demonstrator on the opposite side of a wire-mesh barrier from the observer, will exhibit a strong preference for whichever diet the demonstrator is eating (see also Posadas-Andrews & Roper 1983). While Strupp's findings indicate that adult rats can exchange information concerning their diets, her paradigm allowed demonstrator and observer to interact in the presence of the foods about which information was being communi-
  • 9. cated. Extensive modifications in Strupp's procedure were, therefore, necessary to explore the possibility that rats can exchange informa- tion concerning foods distant from an ingestion site. In the series of laboratory experiments des- cribed below, we first demonstrate that one Norway rat can communicate to another infor- mation concerning food it has eaten at a distant site. Second, we determine the mode of trans- mission of this information, and finally we demonstrate that the transmitted information is adequate to facilitate foraging by its recipient. Experiment 1 The first experiment was undertaken to deter- mine whether one adult rat (a 'demonstrator') cim communicate information to a second (an 'observer') concerning the food the demon- strator has eaten prior to interaction with the observer. Although we will refer to the rats in our experiments as demonstrators and obser- vers to conform to current usuage (Levitsky & Strupp 1981; Strupp 1982), we do not intend to imply that the behavioural process supporting information transfer is learning by observation. In fact, our experiments will demonstrate that observational learning is not involved. Methods Subjects. Subjects were 32 Long-Evans rats born in the McMaster colony to breeding pairs descended from animals purchased from Blue Spruce Farms (Altamont, N.Y.). Prior to
  • 10. initiation of the experiment all subjects were maintained ad libitum on Purina Laboratory chow and water in same-sex groups of 4 to 5 littermates. Sixteen subjects, 42 days of age at the time of initiation of the experiment, served as observers. Sixteen 90-120-day-old subjects served as demonstrators. At the start of the 749 experiment, each observer was randomly paired with a demonstrator of the same sex. Apparatus. During the experiment, subjects were housed and tested as demonstrator- observer pairs in 42.5 x 24 x 27.5-cm wire- mesh hanging cages (Wahmann Co., Balitmore, Md). Each hanging cage was divided in two equal parts by a 1.25-cm (t-inch) wire-mesh partition (24 x 27.5 em) attached at the mid- points of each cage's 42.5-cm sides. The same apparatus was also used in experiments 2-5. Procedure. Our procedure was designed to mimic a situation in which a foraging rat ingests a food at some distance from the burrow, re- turns to the burrow, and interacts with a fami- liar burrow-mate. We were interested to know whether, as a result of such interaction, the burrow-mate could acquire information concern- ing the food the forager had eaten and would use this information in selecting a diet. Treatment of subjects during the ~xperiment
  • 11. was as follows (Fig. la): Step 1: demonstrator and observer were first maintained together with ad libitum access to Purina Laboratory Chow pellets for a 2-day familiarization period. Step 2: the demonstrator was moved to the oppo- site side of the wire-mesh partition from the ob- server and food-deprived for 24 h to ensure that the demonstrator fed when given the op- portunity to do so. Step 3: chow was then removed from the observer's side of the cage (in preparation for testing) and the demonstrator was moved to an enclosure in a separate room and allowed to feed for 30 min on either pow- dered Purina Laboratory chow adulterated with 2 % by weight Hershey's cocoa (cocoa- n., 2 Doy. 1 Doy [1)- Fig. 1. Schematic diagram of the procedures of experi. ments 1 and 2 (Fig. 1a) and experiments 3, 4 and 5 (Fig. 1b).O = observer;D = demonstrator; hatching indicates that pellets of Purina Laboratory chow were present in the cage. Mingwen Pencil Mingwen Pencil Mingwen Pencil
  • 12. 750 ANIMAL BEHA VIOUR, 31, 3 flavoured diet) or powdered Purina Laboratory chow adulterated with 1% by weight Mc- Cormick's pure ground cinnamon (cinnamon- flavoured diet). Step 4: the demonstrator was returned to the observer's cage and demon- strator and observer were allowed to interact for 15 min. Step 5: the demonstrator was re- moved from the experiment and the observer was offered, for 60 h, two weighed food-cups, one containing cinnamon-flavoured diet and one containing cocoa-flavoured diet. The food bowl containing the diet which a given ob- server's demonstrator had eaten was placed in the front of the cage of half the observers and in the back of the cage of the remainder of observers. The experimenter weighed the two food cups once every 12 h throughout the 6O-h test period. Results and Discussion The results of experiment 1 are presented in Fig. 2, which shows the mean amount of cocoa- flavoured diet, as a percentage of total amount eaten, ingested by observers whose demonstrators had eaten cocoa-flavoured or cinnamon- flavoured diet during step 4 of the experiment (see Fig. la). Those observers whose demon- strators ate cocoa-flavoured diet ~te a greater 0-12h 48-60h
  • 13. 80 ~ ~ 70 Q:: W ~ o 60 >-m z 50 W tc w 40 o U ~ 30 Q:: ~ 20 ~ W 10 ::IE o 8 co CtN 8 co DEMONSTRATORS' DIET
  • 14. Fig. 2. Mean amount of cocoa-flavoured diet ingested, as a percentage of total amount eaten, by observers whose demonstrators had eaten either cocoa- or cinnamon- flavoured diet. eo = cocoa-flavoured diet; eIN = cinnamon-flavoured diet. Bars indicate::!: ISE. percentage of cocoa-flavoured diet than did those observers whose demonstrators ate cinnamon-flavoured diet (0-12-h test, Mann- Whitney U = 0, P < 0.(01). The effects of the diet eaten by the demonstrator on the diet preference of the observer were still observable 48-60 h after the interaction of observer and demonstrator in step 4 of Fig. la (Mann- Whitney U = 2, P < 0.001). The results of experiment I clearly demon- strate that rats are able to communicate infor- mation concerning a diet they have eaten at a time and place distant from the locus of their interaction. The data also show that this information is sufficient to bias a recipient's subsequent choice of diet. Experiment 2 In experiment 1, an observer and demonstrator interacted immediately after the demonstrator had eaten a novel diet and the observer had the opportunity to choose between novel diets immediately following receipt of information from the demonstrator. Free-living rats must expend time in returning from a feeding site to their home burrow, and foragers departing from their home burrow must expend further time in reaching a feeding site. If the capacity of rats
  • 15. to transmit information concerning a food they have eaten at a distance from the nest site is to function as the basis for information transfer at the home-site in natural settings, transmission must occur even if there is a delay between a successful forager's ingestion of a food and its return to the burrow and a further delay bet- ween the interaction of a successful forager with other rats and the latters' arrival at a potential feeding site. The present experiment was under- taken to discover whether the information transfer demonstrated in experiment 1 could tolerate such delays. Methods Subjects. Forty-eight 42-day-old experi- mentally-naive Long-Evans rats born in the McMaster colony served as observers, and 48 120-day-old Long-Evans rats as demonstrators. Observers and demonstrators were randomly assigned to one of three conditions: Control, 30-min-delay and 60-min-delay (described below). Apparatus. The apparatus was that used in experiment 1. Procedure. The eight demonstrator and eight observer rats assigned to the Control condition Mingwen Pencil Mingwen
  • 16. Pencil GALEF & WIGMORE: INFORMATION TRANSFER IN RATS were treated identically to observers and demon- strators in experiment I (see Fig. la). Demon- strators and observers assigned to 30-min-delay and 60-min-delay conditions were treated identically to those in the Control condition except in the following two respects. (I) After each demonstrator had eaten either cinnamon- flavoured or cocoa-flavoured diet for 30 min (step 3 of Fig. la), it was food-deprived for either 30 min (30-min-delay condition) or 60 min (60-min-delay condition) before being placed in the cage with its observer (step 4 in Fig. I a). (2) After the observer and demon- strator had interacted for 15 min (step 4 in Fig. la), the observer was food-deprived for either 30 min (30-min-delay condition) or 60 min (60-min-delay condition) before being offered a choice of cinnamon- and cocoa- flavoured diets. A third departure from the procedure of experiment I was that the food cups offered to observers in the test phase (step 5) were weighed only once, 12 h after the initiation of testing. Results and Discussion The main results of experiment 2 are pre- sented in Fig. 3, which shows the mean percentage of cocoa-flavoured diet ingested by observers whose demonstrators had eaten either cinnamon- or cocoa-flavoured diet. Observers
  • 17. in both Control and 30-min-delay conditions exhibited marked effects of interaction with demonstrators on their food choice (Mann- 3O-DELAY-30 6O-OEI.AY-60 80 ! 10 ~80 ~ zSO ~ :i 40 8 ffi 30- U ~ 20 ~ 10 8 8 co a.Io DEMONSTRATORS' DIET Fig. 3. Mean amount of cocoa-flavoured diet ingested, as a percentage of total amount eaten by observers in Control, 30-min-delay, and 6O-min-delay groups when their demonstrators had eaten either cinnamon- or cocoa- flavoured diet. Bars indicate :f: 1 SE.
  • 18. 1S1 Whitney U; Control condition, U = 4, P = 0.002, 30-min-delay condition, U = 3, P = 0.002). There was a marginal effect of demon- strator diet on the diet selection of observers in the 6O-min-delay condition, but this failed to reach significance (Mann-Whitney U = 14, P < 0.064). The results of the present experiment repli- cate the finding in experiment 1 of a transfer of information between adult rats concerning a diet distant in time and space. The results also indicate that this transfer is not disrupted by 30-min delays between a demonstrator's feed- ing and its interaction with an observer and an observer's interaction with a demonstrator and that observer's initiation of feeding. The data further suggest that similar I-h delays interfere with the influence of demonstrators on the observers' diet choice. The present results do not provide sufficient information to determine whether this decrement is due to the first I-h delay causing a deterioration of the signal pass- ing from demonstrator to observer, or the second I-h delay reducing the eft'ect of the signal on the observer's behaviour. We shall address this issue elsewhere. Experiment 3 Experiments 3, 4, and 5 were undertaken to determine the medium of transmission of information from demonstrator to observer.
  • 19. Methods Subjects. Thirty-two experimentally-naive 42-day-old Long-Evans rats born in the Mc- Master colony served as observers and 32 90-11O-day-old Long-Evans rats served as demonstrators. Observers and demonstrators were randomly assigned to the two experimental groups described below. Apparatus. The apparatus was the same as used in experiment I. . Procedure. The procedure was similar to that described in Methods of experiment 1, except that during the period of interaction bet- ween demonstrator and observer in step 4 of Fig. la, demonstrator and observer were sepa- rated by a partition. For subjects in the 'Screen condition' this partition was constructed of 1.3-cm (t-inch) wire-mesh. For subjects in the 'Plexiglas condition' this partition was con- structed of clear, 0.65-cm (i-inch) Plexiglas. Figure 1b presents a schematic diagram of the procedure of the present experiment. Minor differences between the procedure of experiment 100 SCREEN 90
  • 20. !Q 80W >Q: W f(/) 70m I0 ~I 60 Z w tc 50W 8 !z 40 W U Q: 30 ~Z < 20w ~10 8 8 0 CO CIN 751 ANIMAL BEIiA VI OUR, 31, 3 I and that of the present experiment may be noted in comparison of Fig. la and lb. These
  • 21. differences are of no particular moment. They were introduced to ascertain whether the phenomenon under examination was sufficiently robust to withstand small modifications in procedure. Results and Discussion The main results of experiment 3 are pre- sented in Fig. 4, which shows the mean amount of cocoa-flavoured diet, as a percentage of total amount eaten, ingested by observers in Screen and Plexiglas conditions whose demon- strators had eaten either cocoa- or cinnamon- flavoured diet. Observers in the Screen condition exhibited an influence of their respective demonstrators on their diet choice, while subjects in the Plexiglas condition did not (Mann-Whitney U; Screen condition, U = 0, P = 0.001; Plexiglas condition, U = 30, P> 0.30). The results of the present experiment indi- cate that transfer of information from demon- PLEXIGLAS 8 8 CO CIN DEMONSTRATORS' DIET Fig. 4. Mean amount of cocoa-flavoured diet ingested, as a percentage of total amount eaten, by observers in Screen and Plexiglas conditions when their demonstrators
  • 22. had eaten either cinnamon- or cocoa-flavoured diet. Bars indicate :J: 1 SE. strator to observer can occur in the absence of free interaction between pair members. The data further indicate both that visual contact between observer and demonstrator is not sufficient and that some other form of contact (olfactory, gustatory, or tactile) is necessary for information transfer. Experiment 4 In this experiment we examined whether ol- factory cues were sufficient to effect information transfer from demonstrator to observer. Methods Subjects. Forty Long-Evans rats, 42 days of age at the initiation of the experiment, served as observers, and 40 lOO-l20-day old rats of the same strain served as demonstrators. Half of the observers and half of the demonstrators were randomly assigned to Control and Anaes- thetized conditions. Apparatus. The apparatus was the same as used in experiment 1. Procedure. Subjects in the Control group of the present experiment were treated identically to those in the 'Screen' condition of experiment 2 (see Methods of experiment 2 and Fig. Ib). Subjects in the Anaesthetized condition were treated like those in the Control group except that immediately after each demonstrator had
  • 23. eaten cinnamon- or cocoa-flavoured diet in step 3, it was anaesthetized by intraperitoneal injection of sodium pentobarbitol, draped over an inverted Pyrex petri dish (100 em diam x 50 em), fastened to it with a piece of masking tape, and placed in the apparatus facing its observer with the end of its nose 2.5 em from the wire-mesh partition separating demonstrator and observer. (Pilot studies had revealed that an anaesthetized demonstrator 2.5 em from a hardware cloth partition could not be physically contacted by an observer on the far side of the barrier.) Each anaesthetized demonstrator was left on its side of the cage for 15 min (cf. step 4, Fig. Ib) and then removed. Each observer was then offered a choice of cinnamon- and cocoa-flavoured diets for 12 h. Results The main results of experiment 4 are pre- sented in Fig. 5, which shows the mean per- centage of cocoa-flavoured diet ingested by observers in Anaesthetized and Control groups when their demonstrators had eaten either cinnamon- or cocoa-flavoured diet. Observers GALEF & WIGMORE: INFORMATION TRANSFER IN RATS CONTROL ANAESnETlZED T 10
  • 24. o 10 10 CO CIN 10 CO DEMONSTRATORS' DIET Fig. 5. Mean amount of cocoa-flavoured diet ingested, as a percentage of total amount eaten, by observers in Anaesthetized and Control groups when their demonstra- tors had eaten either cinnamon- or cocoa-flavoured diet. Bars indicate :f: 1 SE. in both Anaesthetized and Control groups exhibited marked effects of interaction with demonstrators on their food choice (Mann- Whitney U; Anaesthetized group, U = 8, P < 0.002; Control group, U = 13, P < 0.02). ' Discussion . Because transfer of information between demonstrator and observer occurred in the absence of physical contact between them, it can be concluded that gustatory cues passing from demonstrator to receiver (for example the taste of the demonstrator's saliva or of food particles clinging to the demonstrator's coat) are not necessary for information transfer to
  • 25. occur. The finding that demonstrators kept 2.5 em distant from their observers can transmit the necessary signal suggests that olfactory cues passing from demonstrator to observer are sufficient to support the transmission process. Because anaesthetized demonstrators are as effective in transmitting information as intact demonstrators, it would appear that signal emis- sion by the demonstrator is a passive rather 753 than active process, and that the necessary signal is emitted by the demonstrator rather than elicited by the observer. Experiment 5 Given that physical contact . between demon- strator and observer is not necessary for informa- tion transfer from the former to the latter, it seems probable that olfactory cues sent by the demonstrator and received by the observer are sufficient for communication to occur. If so, anosmic observers should not exhibit an influ- ence of interaction with an observer on their diet choice. ~ethods . Subjects. Thirty-six 42-day-old Long-Evans rats served as observers and 36 90-day-old rats of the same strain served as demonstrators. Half the observers were randomly assigned to an Anosmic group and half to a Control group.
  • 26. Apparatus. The apparatus was the same as used in experiment I. Procedure. The procedure was that of experi- ment 2 (see Fig. Ib) except in two respects. (1) Twenty-four hours following initiation of the experiment, at the start of the last day of familiariZJltion, observers assigned to the Anos- mic group were rendered anosmic by intranasal lavage with zinc sulphate solution (Alberts & Galef 1971). Observers in the Control group were treated with isotonic saline solution at the time subjects in the Anosmic group were treated with Zinc Sulphate. (2) For 30 min during step 3 of Fig. Ib, demonstrator rats were fed either unadulterated powdered Purina Labora- tory Chow (Pu Diet) or Normal Protein Test Diet (NPT Diet) (Teklad Test Diets, Madison, Wise.). NPT Diet is a powdered mixture of the following ingredients (values in g{kg): vitamin- free test casein, 260.06; corn starch, 598.24, hydrogenerated vegetable oil, 80.00; cod-liver oil, 20.00, USP XIV Mineral mix 40.00; vita- mins, 1.70. (3) Observers were offered a choice between Pu Diet and NPT Diet for 12 h during the test period (step 5 of Fig. 1b). The change in diets offered to demonstrators and observers in the present experiment was necessitated by pilot studies showing that anosmic rats could not discriminate cinnamon- from cocoa-flavoured . diet. (To test for the
  • 27. ability of anosmic subjects to discriminate bet- ween any pair of diets we compared the ability of anosmic 'and saline-Iavaged subjects to exhibit a toxicosis-induced aversion toone of CONTROL ANOSMIC 9 9 9 9 100 90 II) Q: au 80 ~au II) m 70 0 ~z au ~au -50 t- o. Z 40 !z au 0Q:
  • 28. ~Z 20 -< ~10 0 754 ANIMAL BEHAVIOUR, 31, 3 those two diets when subsequently offered both simultaneously. The results of such tests were clear-cut. Anosmic subjects poisoned after they had eaten cocoa-flavoured diet ate as much cocoa- as cinnamon-flavoured diet in a 6-h test conducted 24 h after poisoning; control subjects poisoned after they had eaten cocoa-flavoured diet ate no cocoa-flavoured diet during the test. Both anosmic and control subjects poisoned after ingesting NPT Diet ate no NPT Diet when, 24 h after poisoning, they were offered a choice of NPT and Pu Diets for 6 h.) If anosmic obser- vers were unable to discriminate between the two diets offered during testing (step 5 of Fig. 1b), then even if we were to observe a breakdown in demonstrator influence on diet selection in anosmic observers, we could not attribute this deficit to an anosmia-induced interference with communication between demonstrator and observer. Results and Discussion The main results of experiment 5 are presented
  • 29. in Fig. 6, which shows the mean amount of 60 30 NPT PU ' NPT PU "DEMONSTRATORS' DIET Fig. 6. Mean amount of NPT Diet, as a percentage of total amount eaten, ingested by observers in Anosmic and Control 'groups when their demonstrators had eaten either NPT Diet or Pu Diet. Pu = powdered Purina Laboratory Chow, NPT = Normal Protein Test Diet. Bars indicate :l: 1 SE. NPT Diet, as a percentage of total amount eaten, ingested by observers in Anosmic and Control groups whose demonstrators had eaten either NPT Diet or Pu Diet. Observers in the Control group whose demonstrators ate NPT Diet ate significantly more NPT Diet during testing than did observers in the Control group whose demonstrators ate Pu Diet (Mann- Whitney U = 4, P < 0.002). By contrast, the demonstrators' diet did not affect diet choice by anosmic observers during testing (Mann- Whitney U = 35, P > 0.30). Given that NPT and Pu Diets are discrimin- able by anosmic subjects and that demonstrators can successfully transfer information concerning
  • 30. NPT and Pu Diets to intact observers, the failure of anosmic observers to use the informa- tion transmitted by their observers suggests that anosmia interfered with receipt of the necessary olfactory information. The results of both the present experiment and experiment 4 thus indicate that the cue passing from demon- strator to observer is olfactory in nature. We have independent evidence of the pre- sence of an olfactory cue carried on the breath of rats and reflecting the flavour of their diets, which could serve as the means of information transfer from demonstrator to observer. A human subject (C.M.), by sniffing rat breaths, successfully distinguished six rats fed cinnamon- flavoured diet from six rats fed cocoa- flavoured diet with better than 85 % accuracy during two 18-trial sessions (binomial test, Session 1, x = 3, P < 0.004, Session 2, x = 2, P < 0.00 I). While this does not prove that observer rats use information carried 011 the breath of demonstrators in selecting diets in our experiments, it does indicate that such information would be sufficient for communi- cation from demonstrator to observer. Experiment 6 The results of the experiments described above demonstrate that rats can communicate informa- tion to their fellows about diets they have ingested far from the locus of information transfer. The data also indicate that such infor- mation can effect food preferences in a receiver, and that the signal passing from demonstrator to observer is olfactory in nature. They do not
  • 31. however provide evidence that such information can be used by a recipient to facilitate foraging in environments characterized by an unpredic- table and patchy distribution of food. I FOOO CUP ~ONE-WAY DOOO I REMOVABlE DOOO HARDWARE ClOTH GALEF & WIGMORE: INFORMATION TRANSFER IN RATS In the present study we simulated an environ- ment in which each of a variety of foods became available in fixed locations on an unpredictable schedule. Once our subjects had demonstrated that they knew the location in which each food was to be found, we provided each subject with the opportunity to interact with a conspecific which had eaten one of the foods. We then determined whether our subjects would use the information provided by the fed individual to direct their subsequent foraging. Methods Subjects. Eight Long-Evans rats 42 days of age at the start of experimentation served as subjects and eight additional rats of the same age served as demonstrators. One subject and one demonstrator were excluded from the experiment when the subject learned to open
  • 32. the one-way doors described below from both sides. Apparatus. The apparatus is illustrated in plan view in Fig. 7. It consisted of a three-arm maze attached to a cage holding four subject- CH DIET CO DIET CIN DET CHOICE POINT Fig. 7. Plan of apparatus used in experiment 6. CH = cheese-flavoured; CIN = cinnamon-flavoured; CO = cocoa-flavoured; S = Subject; D = Demonstrator. 755 demonstrator pairs. Each I x O.09-m arm of the maze (constructed of plywood with a transparent Plexiglas cover) led via a removable guillotine door to a 23 x 15-cm goal box con- taining a food cup. At the choice point of the maze the subject was faced with a choice bet- ween three ont:-way doors. Each subject was maintained in a 30 x 30 x l5-cm plywood cage opening into an alley via a removable guillotine door. Each subject was housed adjacent to its demonstrator and sepa- rated from the latter by a 30 x 15-cm wire- mesh partition. Procedure. To begin the experiment, subjects and demonstrators were tail-marked for indivi-
  • 33. dual recognition, introduced individually into their respective compartments, and placed on a 23 h/day food-deprivation schedule. Habituation. Three days following initiation of the experiment, the guillotine doors were removed from the subjects' cages and from the goal boxes, the one-way doors were taped open, and subjects were allowed to explore around the apparatus for 2 h/day. Three different diets, cinnamon-flavoured powdered Purina Labora- tory chow (Diet CIN), cocoa:-flavoured pow- dered Purina Laboratory chow (Diet CO) and cheese-flavoured powdered Purina Laboratory chow (Diet CH, 2 % by weight Kraft's romano cheese) were placed in the goal-boxes in the positions indicated in Fig. 7. These apparatus habituation sessions were continued for several days, with the experimenter gradually lowering the flaps on the one-way doors from one day to the next, until the one-way doors were completely closed and all subjects were passing through them without hesitation. Training. On training days, each subject received four trials per day. To begin a series of trials the experimenter consulted a random number table to determine which of the three goal-boxes would be open to the first subject. That goal-box door was opened, the door to the first subject's living cage removed, and the subject was given 5 min to pass through a one-way door. If the subject chose the one-way door leading to food it was allowed to feed for 2 min and then returned- to its living cage. If the subject chose one of the other two one-way
  • 34. doors, it was left in the arm it had chosen for 2.5 min and then returned to its living cage. If the subject failed to pass through a one-way door within 5 min, it was returned to its living cage. "f . SUBJECT 1. SLeJECT 2 " SUBJECT3 I .. SUBJECT4 III 80 . SlBJECT5 -':!! SUBJECT6 Ct: SUBJECT7I- 0 ~70 Z 0 W U (5 600 ~0: 0: 50 8 !Z ~0: 40W 11.
  • 35. 756 ANIMAL BEHAVIOUR, 31, 3 Any subject choosing either of the incorrect doors was left in its living cage for 2 min and then allowed to choose again. The first trial was repeated until the subject chose the correct one- way door. After the first subject's first trial of a day was completed, the second, third and fourth subjects' first trials of that day were run in an identical fashion to the first trial of the first subject, ex- cept that the goal-box door that was opened for each subject was different from that opened for the preceding subject. Second, third and fourth trials on each day for each subject were run under conditions identical to those prevailing on that subject's first trial of the day. Because the location of the three diets remained fixed and the same goal- box was open on trials 1--4of each day for each subject, perfect performance was possible on trials 2--4 (but not trial 1) of each day. After all subjects had completed their four trials, each was offered a food bowl containing unadult- erated powdered Purina Laboratory chow for 1 h. Testing. Once subjects were responding cor- rectly on an average of two or more of the last three trials of each day for six consecutive days, that subject entered into the test phase of the experiment. During the test phase, each subject was run 4 trials/day, exactly as during
  • 36. training. However, during testing, 45 min prior to each subject's first trial of each day, that subject's demonstrator was removed from its living cage, and treated in the following fashion: (1) Each demonstrator was first placed in a cage in a separate room and allowed to feed for 30 min on the diet which was located in the arm of the maze to which that demonstrator's subject was to have access on that day. (2) The demonstrator was then returned to its living cage to interact through the wire-mesh partition with its subject for IS min, and was then removed from its living cage until that day's ; testing of all subjects was completed. Each subject was tested (four trials/day) for 12 consecutive days. Results and Discussion . The main results of .experiment 6 are pre- sented in Fig. 8, which indicates the percentage correct responses made by each of the seven subjects on its daily first trials of the last 18 days of training and on its daily first trials during the 12 days of testing. Four of the seven subjects did significantly better than chance during the testing phase of the experiment (Binomial test, Ho that p = !; see Fig. 8 for significance levels). It is somewhat disappoint- ing that not all subjects made use of the informa- tion provided by their respective demonstrators in selecting an arm of the maze for initial exploration. However, the fact that four subjects did use the information provided by their demonstrators indicates that the mode of
  • 37. information transfer examined in experiments 1-5 is adequate to facilitate foraging in circumstances in which a food is available intermittently at a fixed location. General Discussion The results of the present experiments demon- strate that adult rats can exchange information concerning ingested foods at a time and place distant from the ingestion site. The results also demonstrate that the reception of olfactory information from conspecifics can profoundly bias diet choice in the recipient, and under appropriate circumstances, can facilitate the recipient's location of available nutrients. The present data thus provide indirect support for 30 " 01 I LAST 18 DAYS OF TRAINING I 12 DAYS OF TESTING Fig. 8. Percentage of correct choi~ on first trials by individual subjects on the last 18 days of training anQ the 12 days of testing in experiment 6.
  • 38. 758 ANIMAL BEHAVIOUR, 31, 3 Galef, B. G., Jr. & Clark, M. M. 1971b. Parent-offspring interactions determine time and place of first ingestion of solid food by wild rat pups. Psychon. Sci., 25, 15-16. Galef, B. G., Jr. & Clark, M. M. 1972. Mother's milk and adult presence: two factors determining initial dietary selection by weanling rats. J. compo physiol. Psychol., 78, 220-225. Galef, B. G., Jr. & Heiber, L. 1976. The role of residual olfactory cues in the determination of feeding site selection and exploration patterns of domestic rats. J. compo physiol. Psychol., 90, 727-739. Galef, B. G., Jr. & Henderson, P. W. 1972. Mother's milk: A determinant of the feeding preferences of wean- ing rat pups. J. compo physiol. Psychol., 78, 213-219. Galef, B. G., Jr. & Sherry, D. F. 1973. Mother's milk: A medium for the transmission of cues reflecting the flavor of mother's diet. J. compo physiol. Psychol., 83, 374-378. Levitsky, D. A. & Strupp, B. J. 1981. Malnutrition and tests of brain function. In: Nutrition and Behavior (Ed. by A. A. Miller), Philadelphia, Pa: Franklin Institute Press. Posadas-Andrews, A. & Roper, T. J. 1983. Social trans- . mission of food-preferences in adult rats. Anim. Behav., 31, 265-271.
  • 39. Strupp, B. J. 1982. Malnutrition and animal models of cognitive development. Ph.D. thesis, Cornell University. Telle, H. J. .1966. Beitrag zur Kenntnis der Verhaltensweise von Ratten, vergleichend dargestellt bei Rattus norvegicus und Rattus rattus. Z. Angew. Zool., 53, 129-196. ' von Frisch, K. 1967. The Dance Language and Orientatioll of Bees. Cambridge, Mass.: Belknap Press. Waltz, E. C. 1982. Resource characteristics and the evo- lution of information centers. Am. Nat., 119, 73-90. Ward, P. 1965. Feeding ecology of the Black-faced Dioch (Quelea quelea) in Nigeria. Ibis, 107, 173-214. Ward, P. & Zahavi, A. 1973. The importance of certain assemblages of birds as "information-centres" for food-finding. Ibis, 115, 517-534. (Received 14 September 1982; revised 28 October 1982; MS. number: A3031) .Journal of Comparative Psychology 198:3, Vol. 97, No.4, :358-:36:3 Copyright 198:1 hy the American Psychological Association, Inc.
  • 40. A Failure to Find Socially Mediated Taste Aversion Learning in Norway Rats (R. norvegicus) Bennett G. Galef, Jr., Stephen W. Wigmore, and Deborah J. Kennett McMaster University, Hamilton, Ontario, Canada Observer rats interacted with conspecific demonstrators immediately after demonstrators ate a novel diet and were made ill by LiCI injection. Following their interaction with demonstrators, observers were tested for aversion to their ill demonstrator's diet. Previous research has shown that (a) an observer can extract information from a demonstrator sufficient to permit identification of the demonstrator's diet (Galef & Wigmore, 1983) and (b) a rat ill from LiCI toxicosis is an adequate unconditioned stimulus in a taste aversion learning paradigm (Lavin, Freise, & Coombes, 1980). Further, two of the present experiments demonstrated that cues emitted by a rat, reflecting the particular diet it has eaten, are an adequate conditional stimulus in a toxicosis-induced aversion learning situation. Observer avoidance of a diet previously ingested by an ill demonstrator was, however, not demonstrated. The implications of the failure to find socially mediated aversion learning are discussed. The results of a number of recent studies demonstrate that signals emitted by rats
  • 41. suffering LiCI toxicosis can serve as uncon- ditioned stimuli in a taste aversion learning paradigm. A rat ingesting some unfamiliar diet prior to exposure to a LiCI-injected conspecific subsequently exhibits reluct- ance to ingest that diet (Bond, 1982; Coombes, Revusky, & Lett, 1980; Lavin, Freise, & Coombes, 1980; Stierhoff & Lavin, 1982). Although exposure to an ill conspecific can result in a specific food avoidance in rats in laboratory settings, it is not obvious how such socially mediated taste aversion learning might be used by free-living ani- mals to enhance avoidance of toxic foods. If an unfamiliar food eaten by an individual prior to interaction with an ill conspecific were safe, then subsequent avoidance of that food would be counterproductive. If, to the contrary, an unfamiliar food ingested prior to interaction with an ill conspecific were toxic, then information received from This research was supported by Natural Sciences and Engineering Research Council of Canada Grant A0307 and McMaster University Research Board grants to B. G. Galef, Jr. We thank Harvey Weingar- ten and Mertice Clark for their thoughtful commen- tary on earlier drafts of the manuscript. Requests for reprints should be sent to Bennett G. Galef, Jr., Department of Psychology, McMaster Uni- versity, Hamilton, Ontario, Canada L8S 4Kl. the ill individual would be redundant. Even
  • 42. in the absence of social learning, a rat in- gesting an unfamiliar toxic food would sub- sequently avoid that food. The functional significance of the potential of ill rats to act as unconditioned aversive stimuli in an avoidance learning situation is not obvious. Stierhoff and Lavin (1982) suggested that an ill rat may deposit aversion-produc- ing residual odors in the vicinity of a nox- ious food and that such odors might inhibit ingestion of that food by others of their colony (see also Steiniger, 1950). Unfortu- nately, Stierhoff and Lavin did not provide evidence either that aversion-producing substances emitted by ill rats directly in- hibit ingestion or that rats preferentially deposit such substances in the vicinity of toxic foods. The results of recent studies both in our laboratory and elsewhere demonstrate that a rat briefly exposed to an unpoisoned con- specific that has eaten some food subse- quently exhibits an enhanced preference for that food. One rat can extract infor- mation from another concerning the diet the latter individual has recently eaten (Galef & Wigmore, 1983; Posadas-Andrews & Roper, 1983; Strupp & Levitsky, in press- a, in press-b). This finding, taken together with the observation that an ill rat can serve as an unconditioned stimulus for taste aversion learning, suggests that a rat 358
  • 43. 100 ..... 90 w C 80 CIJ! wa: 70a:w 0> >a: ctW 60 ....IIJ! ...CD .(0 50 o >- o CD 40o z Ow ;j.!C 30 z w ct 20 w ::;; 10 0 360 B. GALEF, JR.,S. WIGMORE, AND D. KENNETT
  • 44. 30-MININTERACTION 2-HRINTERACTION Step 5 Step5 DEMONSTRATOR'S DIET: oCocoa p<.Q02 0 Cinnamon p<.003 p<.008 p<.001 8 8 SALINE LiCI SALINE LiCI TREATMENT CONDITION Figure 2. Mean amount of cocoa-flavoreddiet in- gested by observers as a percentage of total amount eaten. (Bars indicate :t 1 SE.) Results and Discussion The main results of Experiment 1 are presented in Figure 2, which indicates the mean percentage of cocoa-flavored diet ea- ten during testing by observers whose dem- onstrators had eaten either cinnamon-fla- vored or cocoa-flavored diet. As is evident from inspection of the figure and as statis- tical tests confirm (Mann-Whitney U tests, see Figure 2 for p values), subjects in both experimental and both control groups ex-
  • 45. hibited a marked preference for the diet that their respective demonstrators had ea- ten. Poisoned demonstrators were as effec- tive in promoting intake of the diet they had eaten as were unpoisoned demonstra- tors. The failure to find an effect of poisoning demonstrators on their capacity to transfer a preference for the diet they have eaten is open to a variety of intepretations. First, it is possible that although demonstrators emit signals specifying both the food they have eaten and that they have eaten some- thing toxic, observers fail to associate the two messages. Difficulties in forming aver- sions to demonstrator-produced cues, prob- lems in the temporal patterning of receipt of the two signals, or problems with the relative strength of the preference induced by one signal and the aversion induced by the other are possible causes of the ob- served failure of transfer of aversion. Alternatively, as is the case with all null outcomes, the failure to demonstrate a transfer of aversion from poisoned demon- strators to observers may have been due to our selecting an inappropriate set of exper- imental conditions. We were, however, careful to select parameters of toxicosis induction in demonstrators and of interac- tion between demonstrator and observer previously shown to cause observers to learn aversions to unfamiliar foods ingested
  • 46. prior to interaction with a poisoned dem- onstrator (Bond, 1982; Lavin et aI., 1980). Further, the procedures we used were clearly adequate to allow observers to ex- tract information from demonstrators con- cerning the diets demonstrators had eaten. Conditions were thus appropriate for aver- sion transfer from observer to demonstra- tor, yet the anticipated outcome was not observed. We could continue seeking a set of pa- rameters that would permit socially me- diated transfer of aversion, but it is not obvious what conditions to select. We de- cided instead to ask whether the informa- tion extracted by an observer from a dem- onstrator could serve as the conditional stimulus in a standard aversion learning paradigm. It seemed to us that if an ob- server could not form an aversion to the food a demonstrator had eaten when that observer was poisoned directly after inter- acting with a demonstrator fed a novel food, then it was unlikely that under any condi- tions observers would avoid a food eaten by a demonstrator exhibiting symptoms of toxicosis. Experiment 2 In this experiment, observers were first allowed to interact with demonstrators fed one of two diets. Each observer was then poisoned by ip injection of LiCI and sub- sequently offered a choice between the two
  • 47. diets fed to demonstrators. Method Subjects. Thirty-two 42-day-old experimentally naive Long-Evans rats from the McMaster colony ABSENCE OF SOCIALLY MEDIATED TASTE AVERSION 359 made ill following ingestion of a novel food might provide two potentially useful signals to a conspecific: (a) a signal containing information sufficient to permit identifi- cation of the food that the signal-emitter has recently eaten and (b) a signal capable of inducing a learned aversion. Exposure to these two signals in temporal contiguity might suffice to produce in their recipient avoidance of the specific diet recently in- gested by an ill conspecific. Experiment 1 Our procedure was designed to mimic a situation in which one rat (a demonstrator) departs from its burrow, ingests a novel toxic food, returns to its burrow, and while suffering toxicosis, interacts with a burrow- mate (an observer) which subsequently has the opportunity to ingest the novel food previously eaten by the demonstrator. Our goal was to determine whether such a series of events would result in avoidance by the
  • 48. observer of the food the demonstrator had eaten prior to the onset of toxicosis. Method Subjects. Sixty-four experimentally naive Long- Evans rats born in the McMaster colony to breeding stock acquired from Blue Spruce Farms (Altamont, New York) served as observers in the procedure de- scribed below. Each observer was 42 days of age at the time of initiation of the experiment. Sixty-four addi- tionaI92-day-old Long-Evans rats from the McMaster colony served as demcinstrators. Apparatus. Subjects were housed and tested in same-sex demonstrator-observer pairs in 42.5 x 24 x 27.5 cm wire mesh hanging cages (Wahmann Co., Baltimore, Maryland). Each cage was divided into two equal parts by a 1.25-cm ('/2-in.) hardware-cloth screen attached to the midpoint of each 42.5-cm side. Procedure. Treatment of subjects during the ex- periment was as follows (see Figure 1): Step 1: In order to permit familiarization with both apparatus and partner, demonstrator and ob- server were maintained together with ad lib access to Purina Laboratory Rodent Chow pellets (their normal maintenance diet) and left undisturbed for 2 days. Step 2: In order to ensure that the demonstrator ate when given the opportunity to do so, each dem- onstrator was moved to the opposite side of the screen partition from its observer and food deprived for 24 hr.
  • 49. Step 3: In preparation for testing of each observer, chow was removed from each observer's side of the apparatus. Each demonstrator was then moved to an individual enclosure in a room separate from that housing the apparatus and allowed to feed for 30 min on either powdered Purina Laboratory Rodent Chow adulterated 2% by weight with sifted Hershey's Pure Cocoa (cocoa-flavored diet) or powdered Purina Lab- oratory Rodent Chow adulterated 1% by weight with McCormick's Fancy Ground Cinnamon (cinammon- flavored diet). Step 4: Immediately following termination of Step 3, each demonstrator was randomly assigned to one of two experimental groups or one of two control groups. Demonstrators assigned to experimental groups each received ip injection of 1% of body weight of 2% (w/ v) LiCI solution. Members of control groups were injected with an equivalent volume of isotonic saline solution. Step 5: Immediately following injection, each demonstrator was returned to its respective observer's cage, and demonstrator and observer were allowed to interact for either 30 min or 2 hr, depending on the condition to which a given demonstrator-observer pair had been assigned. Step 6: Each demonstrator was removed from the experiment, and each observer was offered, for 18 hr, two weighed food cups, one containing cinnamon- flavored diet and one containing cocoa-flavored diet. 30 Min 2 Days 1 Day 30 Min or 2 Hr TEST
  • 50. .--0;0' coc ~-~-G -~~I-[E[ ~ ~ O;o'CIN o;o,c;:-~ 1 or Diet coe LiCI (Experimental) or Saline (Control) STEP 1 STEP 2 STEP 3 INJECTION STEP 4 STEP 5 STEP 6 Figure J. Schematic diagram of the procedure of Experiment 1. (0 = observer; D = demonstrator; Diet CIN = cinnamon-flavored diet; Diet COC = cocoa-flavored diet. Hatching indicates pellets of Purina Laboratory Rodent Chow present in cage.) Mingwen Pencil Mingwen
  • 51. Pencil I/) 100 Sellne LithiumChloride a: w 90> pc.OO3 pc.OOOIa: WI/) 80III 0 > 70III Z 60w.... < 50w u 0 40 U .... 30w is ~20 z 10<w :I; 0 ABSENCE OF SOCIALLY MEDIATED TASTE AVERSION 361 2 Days 1 Day 30 Min 15 Min INJECTION TEST Diet COC ~-~--[G--rn-o-lli ~ / 1
  • 52. 'DleICIN Diet CIN 8or ---+- DDiet CQe liCI (Experimental) or Saline (Control) Figure 3. Schematic diagram of the procedure of Experiment 2. (See Figure 1 for abbreviations.) served as observers, and an additional 32 rats 60-90 days of age served as demonstrators. Procedure. The procedure (see Figure 3) was sim- ilar to that described in Method of Experiment 1 (see Figure 1) except that instead of injecting each dem- onstrator immediately before it interacted with an observer, we injected each observer immediately after it interacted with a demonstrator. Observers in the experimental group received 1% of body weight of 2% (wjv) LiCI solution; observers in the control group received an equivalent volume of isotonic saline solu- tion. Fifteen minutes following injection, each ob- server was offered, for 18 hr, a choice between weighed samples of cocoa- and cinnamon-flavored diets. Results and Discussion The main results of Experiment 2 are presented in Figure 4 which indicates the percentage of cocoa-flavored diet eaten by observers whose demonstrators had in- gested either cinnamon-flavored or cocoa- flavored diet prior to their interaction with observers. During testing, observers in the
  • 53. control group exhibited a preference for the diet their respective demonstrators ate, while those in the experimental group ex- hibited an aversion to the diet of their respective demonstrators (Mann-Whitney U tests, see Figure 4 for p values). The results of the present experiment indicate that cues received by observer rats from demonstrators are adequate condi- tional stimuli for the learning of an aver- sion. It is, of course, possible that the avoid- ance of demonstrators' diet exhibited by subjects in the experimental group was not the result of a learned aversion but rather of an unconditioned response to toxicosis. Experiment 3 was undertaken to directly examine the unconditioned effects of toxi- cosis on observers' preference for demon- strators' diets. Experiment 3 If the avoidance of demonstrators' diets exhibited by observers were the result of an unconditioned response to toxicosis, one would expect observers poisoned prior to interaction with demonstrators to exhibit an aversion to demonstrators' diets. Method Subjects. Twenty-four experimentally naive 42- day-old Long-Evans rats from the McMaster colony served as observers. An additional 24 rats from the same source, 2-3 wk older than observers, served as
  • 54. demonstrators. Half of the observers and half of the demonstrators were assigned to an experimental group; the remainder, to a control group. Procedure. Treatment of both experimental and control groups is illustrated in Figure 5. In brief, on the third day of the experiment, observers received ip injection of 1% of body weight of a solution. The OBSERVERS INJECTED COC CIN COC CIN DEMONSTRATORS'DIET Figure 4. Mean amount of cocoa-flavored (COC) diet ingested by observers as a percentage of total amount eaten. (CIN = cinnamon-flavored diet. Bars indicate :t 1 SE.) 362 B. GALEF, JR., S. WIGMORE, AND D. KENNETT 2 Days 1 Day INJECTION ~__~-O r LiCI (Experimental) or Saline (Control) 4 Hr 30 Min 15 Min TEST
  • 55. I ~ I!"::l nDl __Diet COC · D ~--Lb--~--G / "'--Diet CIN Diet CIN / or D Diet COC Figure 5. Schematic diagram of the procedure of Experiment 3. (See Figure 1 for abbreviations.) observers in the control group were injected with isotonic saline, and those in the experimental group with 2% (w/v) LiCl solution. All observers were then left undisturbed for 4 hr while subjects in the experi- mental group recovered from acute effects of toxicosis induction. Each observer then interacted for 15 min with a demonstrator fed either cinnamon- or cocoa- flavored diet, and each observer was subsequently tested for its preference between cinnamon- and co- coa -flavored diets. Results and Discussion The main results of Experiment 3 are presented in Figure 6 which indicates the percentage of cocoa-flavored diet eaten by observers whose demonstrators had in- gested either cocoa- or cinnamon-flavored
  • 56. diet. During testing, subjects in both con- trol and experimental groups exhibited marked preference for the diet their respec- tive demonstrators had eaten (Mann- Whit- OBSERVERS INJECTED <II 100 a: w ~ 90 w ~ 80 o > 70aI ffi 60 I- ~ 50 u o 40 u tu 30 is ;f'. 20 :i 10 w ::E Saline Lithium Chloride o COC CIN COC CIN DEMONSTRATORS'DIET
  • 57. Figure 6. Mean amount of cocoa-flavored (COC) diet ingested by observers as a percentage of total amount eaten. (CIN = cinnamon-flavored diet. Bars indicate :!: 1 SE.) ney U tests, see Figure 6 for p values). Experience of toxicosis does not in itself result in avoidance by observers of the diet eaten by demonstrators. General Discussion The results of the present series of ex- periments indicate both (a) that cues emit- ted by one rat, reflecting the identity of the diet that rat has recently eaten, form an adequate conditional stimulus for toxicosis- based aversion learning (Experiments 2 and 3) and (b) that naive rats experiencing the cues emitted by an ill conspecific pre- viously fed a diet do not develop an aversion to that diet (Experiment 1). Given that the purpose of undertaking this series of studies was to determine whether a rat would avoid ingesting a diet as the result of interacting with an ill con- specific that had ingested that diet, the failure to find such a phenomenon consti- tutes a null outcome. Like all null outcomes the present finding is difficult to interpret. Taken together, the finding of Lavin et al. (1980), that an ill rat is an adequate uncon- ditioned stimulus for aversion learning, and the finding in Experiment 2 above, that
  • 58. cues emitted by a fed rat are an adequate conditional stimulus for aversion learning, suggest that under the proper set of exper- imental parameters, one would find socially mediated aversion learning. Our reason for reporting the present re- sults, rather than searching the relevant parameter space until a situation in which socially mediated aversion learning is ob- tained, is to make clear that even if the desired result is eventually found, there must be serious question as to its relevance to toxin-avoidance behavior of rats in nat- ABSENCE OF SOCIALLY MEDIATED TASTE AVERSION 363 ural environments. The more restricted the parameter space in which socially mediated learned aversions occur, the less likely they are to play a role in the diet selection of free-living organisms. In more than 20 experiments on the so- cially mediated transfer of diet preference, we have invariably seen preference for dem- onstrators' diet by observers (Galef, 1983; Galef & Wigmore, 1983). Similarly Strupp (in press-a, in press-b, Note 1) and Posa- das-Andrews (1983; Note 2), using quite different experimental paradigms, have re- peatedly found social transfer of diet pref- erence. This consistency of outcome across
  • 59. a broad range of conditions suggests that the social transmission of preference for a diet is likely to playa role in natural envi- ronments. The difficulty that both our lab- oratory and that of Posadas-Andrews (Note 2) have experienced in demonstrating so- cially mediated aversion learning suggests that it is unlikely to be an important aspect of the poison-avoidance behavior of free- living animals. At the very least, any future demonstration of social transmission by ill rats of an aversion to a specific diet will have to be critically examined to determine whether the parameters allowing such transmission to occur are likely to be found in natural situations. Reference Notes 1. Strupp, B. J. Personal communications, 1982, 1983. 2. Posadas-Andrews, A. Personal communication, 1982. References Bond, N. W. Transferred odor aversions in adult rats. Behavioral and Neural Biology, 1982,35, 417-421. Coombes, S., Revusky, S. H., & Lett, B. T. Long-delay taste-aversion learning in an unpoisoned rat: Ex- posure to a poisoned rat as the unconditioned stim- ulus. Learning and Motivation, 1980, 11, 256-266. Galef, B. G., Jr. Utilization by Norway rats (R. nor-
  • 60. vegicWl) of multiple messages concerning distant foods. Journal of Comparative Psychology, 1983, 97, 364-371. Galef, B. G., Jr., & Wigmore, S. W. Transfer of infor- mation concerning distant foods: A laboratory in- vestigation of the "information-centre" hypothesis. Animal Behaviour, 1983,31, 748-758. Lavin, M. J., Freise, B., & Coombes, S. Transferred flavor aversions in adult rats. Behavioral and Neural Biology, 1980, 28, 15-33. Posadas-Andrews, A., & Roper, T. J. Social transmis- sion of food preferences in adult rats. Animal Be- haviour, 1983,31,265-271. Steiniger, F. von. Beitrage zur Soziologie und sonsti- gen Biologie der Wanderratte. Zeitschrift fur Tier- psychologie, 1950, 7,356-379. Stierhoff, K. A., & Lavin, M. The influence of render- ing rats anosmic on the poisoned-partner effect. Behavioral and Neural Biology, 1982, 34, 180-189. Strupp, B. J., & Levitsky, D. A. Early brain insult and cognition: A comparison of malnutrition and hypo- thyroidism. Developmental Psychobiology, in press- a. Strupp, B. J., & Levitsky, D. A. PKU, learning, and models of mental retardation. Developmental Psy- chobiology, in press-b. Received April 2, 1983. Revision received June 16, 1983 .
  • 61. Social Interaction with an Intoxicated Sibling Can Result in Increased Intake of Ethanol by Periadolescent Rats Pamela S. Hunt Jennifer L. Holloway Elka M. Scordalakes Department of Psychology College of William & Mary Williamsburg, VA 23187-8795 Received 13 December 1999; accepted 10 October 2000 ABSTRACT: A novel procedure for enhancing voluntary intake of ethanol in periadolescent rats is described. The procedure is a modi®cation of Galef et al.'s (e.g., Galet, Kennett, & Stein, 1985; Anim Learn Behave 13:25 ± 30) demonstrator± observer procedure. Subjects were Sprague-Dawley rats, 28 ± 35 days of age. The experimental subject (observer) interacted with a same-sex conspeci®c (demonstrator) previously administered (a) 1.5 g/kg ethanol, (b) an equal volume of water, or (c) 2.1% Sanka coffee intragastrically. Observers were tested with
  • 62. 24-hour access to ethanol and coffee solutions. Observers that had interacted with demonstrators administered ethanol ingested signi®cantly more ethanol during the test than observers in the other two groups. In Experiment 2 demonstrators were administered one of several doses of ethanol (0.0, 1.0, 1.5, or 3.0 g/kg) and observers' ethanol intakes were assessed. Only those observers that interacted with 1.5 g/kg demonstrators increased their ingestion of ethanol, relative to water controls. The lower (1.0 g/kg) and higher (3.0 g/kg) dose groups did not show altered ethanol ingestion. These results are discussed with respect to threshold levels of respired ethanol cues and the ability of observers to detect these cues from demonstrators. The demonstrator± observer procedure appears to be effective for the social transmission of preferences for ethanol in periadolescent rats. ß 2001 John Wiley & Sons, Inc. Dev Psychobiol 38: 101 ± 109, 2001 Keywords: ethanol; periadolescent rats; alcohol ingestion; social learning Alcohol abuse continues to be one of the most health- costly issues in today's society, and the incidence of alcohol abuse is ever-increasing. Particularly disturb- ing statistics have recently been reported that the age at which adolescents and young adults begin to consume alcohol in rather large quantities is dropping
  • 63. appreciably. A recent University of Michigan health survey noted that 51% of 12th graders reported drinking within the previous month and 26% of 8th graders reported doing so. Binge drinking, de®ned as having ®ve or more drinks on occasion, is also on the rise, with 30% of all 12th graders and 16% of all 8th graders reporting binge drinking at least once in the 2 weeks prior to the survey (National Institute on Alcohol Abuse and Alcoholism, 1997). Given the abuse potential for alcohol, and the decreasing age at which alcohol is being consumed, it is imperative that information be gained about the causative factors involved in the initiation of drinking, from both basic and applied research domains. To Correspondence to: P. S. Hunt ([email protected]) Contract grant sponsor: National Institutes of Alcohol Abuse and Alcoholism Contract grant numbers: AA12135, AA12466 ß 2001 John Wiley & Sons, Inc.
  • 64. adequately achieve these goals, there is also a need for adequate animal models to examine relevant social, environmental and genetic contributions to alcohol consumption, especially early in development. Cur- rent research using selectively bred rodent strains continues to afford valuable insights into the role of genetic predispositions toward alcohol consumption and physical dependence (e.g., Brown, Tate, Vik, Haas, & Aarons, 1999; Grahame, Li, & Lumeng, 1999; Lumeng, Waller, McBride, & Li, 1982). However, it is also recognized that genetic factors alone cannot account for the widespread incidence of alcohol abuse, either during adolescence or later in life (e.g., McKinzie et al., 1998). Experience also contributes enormously. As a means of examining experiential factors, several paradigms have been developed to induce
  • 65. ethanol intake in rats, a species valuable to addiction and teratological research, but one that typically avoids ethanol solutions. Many of the procedures that result in increased intake of ethanol add confounding stressor variables (e.g., Adams, 1995; Hansen, Fahlke, Soederpalm, & Hard, 1995; Le et al., 1998; Roske, Baeger, Frenzel, & Oehme, 1994), making the study of mechanistic issues underlying the initiation of voluntary ethanol intake dif®cult, if not impossible. Moreover, these and other methods can involve rather long training histories or complex motor require- ments, making them unsuitable for use with young rats because of their rapid rate of development and relatively immature sensorimotor capabilities. A few procedures have been speci®cally designed to exam- ine the consequences of direct exposure to ethanol cues during the preweanling period on subsequent alcohol intake patterns (e.g., Bannoura, Kraebel,
  • 66. Spear, & Spear, 1998; Hunt, Kraebel, Rabin, Spear, & Spear, 1993; Hunt, Lant, & Carroll, 2000). To date, however, the effects of direct social contact with an intoxicated sibling on periadolescent animal's sub- sequent willingness to ingest ethanol has not been systematically examined. It is known that rats detect and learn about foods through olfactory cues present on the breath or peri- oral region of a conspeci®c that has recently ingested a particular food (e.g., Galef, Kennett, & Stein, 1985; Galef & Stein, 1985; Posadas-Andrews & Roper, 1983; Strupp & Levitsky, 1984). In the experimental preparation used to examine this social transmission of dietary information, Galef and colleagues term the animal that is fed a diet initially the `̀ demonstrator.'' The experimental subject, the one that is tested for intake of that diet, is termed the `̀ observer.'' The demonstrator is ®rst fed a diet with a particularly
  • 67. salient olfactory component, such as chocolate, coffee, or cinnamon. After the demonstrator has eaten the food, it is introduced into a cage housing the observer animal and the two are allowed to interact for a brief period of time (e.g., 5 ± 30 min). The observer is then given access to two diets simultaneouslyÐthe one previously eaten by the demonstrator and another, novel diet. In several reports, the observer animal was found to preferentially ingest the diet eaten by its demonstrator, even if the alternative is normally the more preferred of the two. The effect is not one of mere familiarity or simple exposure to the diet (Galef et al., 1985), but requires a speci®c type of social context to be established; that is, exposure to food cues surrounding the peri-oral region of the demon- strator (Galef & Stein, 1985; Galef & Wigmore, 1983). Given the pervasive role of social behaviors in the transmission of dietary information in rats (e.g., Galef,
  • 68. Kennett, & Wigmore, 1984), it was hypothesized that the same or similar processes might also act when the food of interest is a drug of abuse, that is, alcohol. Because ethanol is eliminated in unmeta- bolized form through routes such as respiration and salivation (Hollstedt & Rydberg, 1985) these cues would be present on the breath of an organism follow- ing ingestion of alcohol. We have recently shown that this type of socially mediated learning can be applied to alcohol intake in preweanling animals (Hunt et al., 2000). In that study, the observers were pups ranging in age from 8 to 16 days. After a brief period of social deprivation these subjects were allowed to interact with a sibling that had been intragastrically administered a 1.5 g/kg dose of ethanol or water. Following this interaction, pups were tested for voluntary intake of ethanol by infusing ethanol into the mouth via an intra-oral cannula (Hall
  • 69. & Rosenblatt, 1977). Observers that had interacted with demonstrators administered ethanol ingested signi®cantly more ethanol during the test than those observers that had interacted with demonstrators administered water. The purpose of the present experiments was to test the role of this type of social context in modifying voluntary ethanol intake in periadolescent Sprague- Dawley rats. The subjects ranged in age from 28 to 35 days at the beginning of the experiment, considered to encompass the periadolescent period (Spear & Brake, 1983). In the experimental preparation, the subject of interest (the observer) interacted with an animal (the demonstrator) that had previously been force-fed ethanol. Following this interaction, the observer was tested for intake of ethanol versus coffee in a two- bottle test. In two experiments the effectiveness of this preparation for increasing observers' voluntary
  • 70. ethanol intake was shown. 102 Hunt, Holloway, and Scordalakes EXPERIMENT 1 The purpose of Experiment 1 was to determine whether the demonstrator± observer preparation (Galef et al., 1985) would be an effective means by which to promote voluntary intake of ethanol in periadolescent rats (cf., Hunt et al., 2000). Our choice of periadoles- cent rats for subjects was in¯uenced by our interest in adolescent alcohol intake and the social factors that may contribute to the initiation of drinking. Addi- tionally, this age allowed for the use of a two-bottle test, potentially providing more information than that available when testing preweanlings about alcohol choice behavior. Methods Subjects. The subjects were 30 pairs of same-sex sibling Sprague-Dawley-derived rats (16 female, 14
  • 71. male), representing seven different litters. No more than one male and one female from each litter served as observers in each treatment group. The subjects ranged from 30 to 35 days of age at the beginning of the procedure. All animals were born and reared in the psychology department vivarium at the College of William & Mary. Breeders were housed together in 50.8�40.6�21.6 cm3 clear polycarbonate cages with pine chip bedding and free access to food (ProLab, St. Louis, MO) and water. Cages were checked daily for births. Two days after birth (day of birth�Postnatal Day [PD] 0) litters were culled to 8 ± 10 pups. Animals were maintained on a 12:12 hr light:dark cycle with light onset at 0700 hr in the temperature-controlled vivarium. On PD21 pups were weaned and group-housed with same-sex littermates in 50.8�40.6�21.6 cm3 clear polycarbonate cages with ad lib access to food and water until the ®rst day of the experiment. All procedures were approved by
  • 72. the College of William & Mary Research on Animal Subjects Committee and followed guidelines estab- lished by the International Society for Developmental Psychobiology. Apparatus. All training and testing occurred in indi- vidually sized stainless steel hanging cages located in a room adjacent to the main vivarium, and maintained on the same temperature and light control systems. Demonstrators were intragastrically administered ethanol, water, or coffee solutions using 12-cm lengths of polyethylene tubing (PE-50; Clay Adams) attached to 5-cc disposable syringes. Observers were tested for intake of ethanol and coffee using 50-ml graduated borosilicate drinking tubes with curved sipper spouts that were attached to the outside of the hanging cages. Procedure. The experiment was conducted over 5 days and is outlined in Table 1. On days 1 and 2 of the procedure, same-sex siblings were pair-housed in metal hanging cages and handled for approximately
  • 73. 2 min each day. At this time animals within each pair were assigned to be the demonstrator or the observer. On Day 3, the demonstrator was removed from the home cage and placed alone in an adjacent hanging cage. The demonstrator was given free access to both food and water, while at this time the observer was water-deprived but continued to have free access to food. On Day 4, 23 hr following separation and the initiation of water deprivation, demonstrators were weighed to the nearest 0.1 g. Next, the demonstrator was intragastrically administered a 1.5 g/kg dose of 12.0% v/v ethanol solution (EtOH, n�10), an equal volume of 2.1% w/v Sanka decaffeinated coffee (COF, n�9), or an equal volume of the tap water vehicle (WATER, n�10). One pair of male COF subjects was eliminated from the experiment because of improper intubation. Following intubation, demonstrators were returned to their cages for 30 min. The 30-minute period was selected because it has been shown to be
  • 74. suf®cient for attaining approximately peak blood ± alcohol levels and respiratory/salivary elimination of alcohol cues (Molina & Chotro, 1989a; Molina, Chotro, & Spear, 1989; Pohorecky & Brick, 1982). The demonstrator was then placed into the observer's cage and the two animals were left undisturbed for 30 min. After the interaction phase the demonstrator was once again removed from the observer's cage. Observers (34 ± 39 days at test) were then offered two graduated drinking tubes; one containing a 5.6% v/v ethanol solution and the second containing a 2.1% Table 1. Procedure Used for Experiments 1 and 2 Day/time Procedure 1/10:00 am Pair-house demonstrators and observers Handle subjects 2 min 2/10:00 am Handle subjects 2 min 3/10:00 am Remove demonstrator and individually
  • 75. house animals Water deprive observers 4/9:00 am Weigh demonstrator i.g. administration of EtOH, WATER, or COF 4/9:30 am Demonstrators placed into observers' cages 4/10:00 am Demonstrators removed Two-bottle test begins (observer) 5/10:00 am End of two-bottle test Periadolescent Ethanol Intake 103 w/v Sanka decaffeinated coffee solution. Both test solutions were prepared with tap water and the concentrations of each were chosen on the basis of preliminary data (Scordalakes & Hunt, unpublished data) indicating an equal preference for these solu- tions in naõÈve animals. Observers were allowed free
  • 76. access to both solutions for 24 hr. The left ± right placement of the drinking bottles containing the two solutions was counterbalanced across subjects in all conditions to avoid a side-preference bias in the data. At the end of the testing interval, the volume of each solution ingested (ml) was recorded. The data were also expressed in terms of the dose of ethanol ingested during the 24-hour test period (g/kg/day). Results The data from this experiment are shown in Figure 1 and Table 2. It was predicted that EtOH observers, those that had interacted with demonstrators adminis- tered ethanol, would ingest more ethanol during the test than WATER controls. Furthermore, observers interacting with demonstrators administered coffee (COF) were expected to ingest more coffee than WATER controls (cf. Galef & Stein, 1985). The ®rst prediction was realized. Observers that had interacted
  • 77. with EtOH demonstrators exhibited increased intake of the ethanol solution relative to coffee. Second, and in contrast to what was predicted, observers that had interacted with COF demonstrators did not exhibit altered intake of coffee relative to that observed in the WATER controls. When the data were expressed as a daily dose of ethanol ingested (g/kg/day), again the observers that had interacted with demonstrators administered ethanol ingested a higher dose of ethanol than controls. The ethanol and coffee intake data were analyzed using a 3 (demonstrator solution)�2 (test solution: ethanol or coffee)�2 (gender) mixed-design analysis of variance (ANOVA). This analysis yielded a signi- ®cant main effect of test solution [F (1,23)�6.23, p < :05]. The demonstrator solution � test solution [F (2,23)�4.85, p < :05] and test solution�gender [F (2,23)�5.93, p < :05] interactions were also sig- ni®cant. Post hoc comparisons conducted with Neu- man ± Keuls tests revealed that the EtOH observers ingested more ethanol and less coffee than either
  • 78. the WATER or COF observers. Additionally, male observers ingested more of the ethanol solution than females, regardless of prior experience. Finally, total ¯uid intake during the test did not differ as a function of group assignment. A 3 (demonstrator solution)�2 (gender) ANOVA conducted on the dose (g/kg/day) data revealed signi®cant main effects of demon- strator solution [F (2,23)�5.37, p < :05] and gender [F (1,23)�7.87, p < :01]. Observers interacting with ethanol-administered demonstrators ingested a higher dose of ethanol during the test, and males ingested a higher dose of ethanol than females, regardless of group. Discussion Results of the ®rst experiment indicate that an ob- server animal will increase its intake of a 5.6% v/v FIGURE 1 Mean (�SEM) intakes of ethanol and coffee (ml) by observers in Experiment 1 during a 24-hour two- bottle choice test. Observers had previously interacted for 30 min with demonstrators that had been intragastrically administered a 1.5 g/kg dose of ethanol (EtOH), an equal
  • 79. volume of tap water (WATER) or an equal volume of 2.1% Sanka coffee (COF). Table 2. Mean (�SEM) Dose of Ethanol (g/kg/day) Ingested During the 24-Hour Two-Bottle Ingestion Test for Experiments 1 and 2 Experiment 1 Observer group EtOH 9.45 (1.13) H2O 5.80 (0.98) COF 5.97 (0.99) Experiment 2 Demonstrator dose 0.0 5.69 (0.77) 1.0 6.45 (1.10) 1.5 8.16 (0.67) 3.0 5.96 (0.75) 104 Hunt, Holloway, and Scordalakes
  • 80. ethanol solution following a brief (30 min) interaction with an intoxicated sibling. Presumably, the adminis- tration of ethanol to the demonstrator resulted in the elimination of unmetabolized ethanol that was dete- cted by the observer. By experiencing these ethanol cues in the context of social interactions, the observer acquired suf®cient information about ethanol to guide later voluntary choice of this drug. In contrast to the ®nding of increased ethanol ingestion in EtOH observers, animals that had interacted with demon- strators administered a coffee solution showed no socially mediated change in coffee preference (but see Galef & Stein, 1985). It is possible that the time between administration of coffee to the demonstrator and the interaction phase, or characteristics of the coffee solution intubated, may have affected the observers detecting and learning about the coffee cue from the demonstrators.
  • 81. The work of Galef and colleagues (Galef & Stein, 1985; Galef & Wigmore, 1983) has repeatedly indi- cated that physical contact per se is not a necessary component of this type of learning. Observers have been found to acquire dietary information from de- monstrators even when the animals are separated by a wire mesh screen or when demonstrators are anesthe- tized. There is, however, a lack of data on preweanling and periadolescent animals. The availability for social interactions may in fact enhance the strength of diet acquisition in younger animals more so than in adults. Several laboratories have shown that social interac- tions and the opportunity for play are highly rein- forcing to adolescent age rats. Most of the research in this area has utilized the conditioned place preference paradigm, in which one arm of a T-maze contains a play partner while the alternative arm contains no stimulus, a surrogate animal, or an animal that does
  • 82. not engage in play behavior. Using variants of this paradigm, adolescent rats have consistently been shown to prefer the arm previously associated with a play partner (e.g., Calcagnetti & Schechter, 1992; Crowder & Hutto, 1992; Humphreys & Einon, 1981; Normansell & Panksepp, 1990). In the present proce- dure the demonstrators and observers were allowed to physically interact following a period of social isola- tion. It is unclear at present whether or not this physical contact had the effect of promoting acquisi- tion of appetitive responses toward ethanol. EXPERIMENT 2 In Experiment 1 the EtOH demonstrators were ad- ministered a 1.5 g/kg dose of ethanol. This dose was chosen on the basis of a series of reports by Molina and colleagues (Molina et al., 1989; Molina & Chotro, 1989b) indicating that 1.5 g/kg ethanol, but not lower doses, resulted in detectable levels of ethanol orosen-
  • 83. sory cues suf®cient for learning about those cues to occur. In the Molina studies, the animal that was administered the ethanol intragastrically was assessed for its own learning about alcohol in terms of olfactory preferences and alcohol intake. The results of the present Experiment 1 indicate that another animal can detect and learn about these cues as well. It is possible that with the demonstrator± observer procedure doses lower than 1.5 g/kg would be effective for producing suf®cient elimination of ethanol to affect the alcohol intake of an observer. The purpose of Experiment 2 was to conduct a dose ± response study to determine (a) whether demonstrators administered a dose of ethanol lower than 1.5 g/kg would provide suf®- cient orosensory stimuli to promote ethanol intake in observers and (b) whether demonstrators adminis- tered a dose higher than 1.5 g/kg would establish even stronger preferences for the ethanol solution in
  • 84. observers. In this experiment demonstrators were intragastrically administered 0, 1.0, 1.5, or 3.0 g/kg ethanol. Methods Subjects. The subjects in this experiment were 48 pairs of same-sex sibling Sprague-Dawley-derived rats (24 male, 24 female), representing 14 different litters. Subjects ranged in age from 28 to 33 days on the ®rst day of the procedure. Animals were born and reared in the psychology department vivarium at the College of William & Mary under conditions identical to those described in Experiment 1. Pairs of animals were randomly assigned to one of four conditions, designated according to the dose of ethanol adminis- tered to the demonstrator: 0 (n�12), 1.0 (n�12), 1.5 (n�12), or 3.0 g/kg (n�11). One pair of female subjects assigned to the 3.0 g/kg group was eliminated from the study due to the improper intubation of the demonstrator. Procedure. The experimental procedure was similar
  • 85. to that of Experiment 1, except for a change in coffee concentration and the dose of ethanol administered to the demonstrators. Following 2 days of pair housing in hanging cages, demonstrators were removed and placed in an adjacent cage. Observers were then water-deprived. Demonstrators were intragastrically administered one of three doses of a 12.0% v/v ethanol solution prepared in a tap water vehicle: 1.0, 1.5, or 3.0 g/kg. A water-intubated control group (0 g/kg) was Periadolescent Ethanol Intake 105 administered a volume of tap water equal to the volume administered to the 1.5 g/kg EtOH group. Thirty minutes following intubation, the demonstrator was placed into the observer's cage and the two animals were allowed to interact for 30 min. The demonstrator was again removed and the observer was given a 24-hour test of intake of 5.6% v/v ethanol
  • 86. vs. 1.8% w/v Sanka coffee. The concentration of the coffee solution was decreased in order to reduce baseline ethanol preference. This allowed us to more clearly address the question of whether the preference for ethanol established through interactions with a 3.0 g/kg demonstrator would be greater than that with a 1.5 g/kg demonstrator. The volume of each solution ingested by the observers was recorded, and the data were also expressed in terms of the dose of ethanol ingested (g/kg/day). Results The data from this experiment are presented in Figure 2 and Table 2. As can be seen in the ®gure, observers that had interacted with demonstrators administered 1.5 g/kg ethanol exhibited an increased intake of ethanol vs. coffee, and therefore a higher daily dose of ethanol consumed, replicating the results of Experi- ment 1. However, observers that had interacted with
  • 87. demonstrators administered either a lower (1.0 g/kg) or a higher (3.0 g/kg) dose of ethanol failed to exhi- bit a reliable change in ethanol intake. Finally, the water observers (0 g/kg) showed approximately equal intake of both solutions, similar to that observed in Experiment 1. A 4 (demonstrator dose)�2 (test solution)�2 (gender) ANOVA conducted on the intake data revealed only a signi®cant main effect of gender [F (1,39)�4.18, p < :05]. Generally, males ingested more ¯uid during the test than females, although there were no differences in terms of the proportions of ethanol and coffee. Follow-up ANOVAs [2 (demon- strator dose)�2 (test solution)] were used to compare intakes of the 0.0 g/kg group with each of the other groups. The only reliable demonstrator dose� test solution interaction occurred for the 1.5 g/kg obser- vers that exhibited increased intakes of ethanol, and reduced intakes of coffee, relative to the water (0.0 g/ kg) controls [F (1,22)�5.77, p < :05]. The analyses comparing the 0.0 with the 1.0 g/kg and 3.0 g/kg
  • 88. groups, respectively, revealed no signi®cant diffe- rences. The analyses of the ethanol dose ingested yielded comparable results (see Table 2). The 4 (demonstrator dose) �2 (gender) ANOVA revealed no signi®cant effects or interactions. Planned t-tests, however, indicated that the dose of ethanol ingested by the 1.5 g/kg observer group was signi®cantly higher than that ingested by the water control group [t (22)�2.36, p < :05]. No other differences were statistically reliable. Discussion The results of Experiment 2 once again indicated that observers that had brie¯y interacted with an intoxi- cated sibling exhibited increased intake of ethanol during a two-bottle choice test. Furthermore, there was an inverted-U dose ± response relationship. Obser- vers interacting with siblings given a 1.5 g/kg dose of ethanol did increase their intake of ethanol; however, observers interacting with siblings administered either
  • 89. a lower (1.0 g/kg) or a higher (3.0 g/kg) dose failed to exhibit altered ethanol ingestion. The failure to obtain increased ethanol intake in the low (1.0 g/kg) dose condition is likely due to insuf®cient olfactory cues present on the breath of the demonstrator animals. Decreased salience of these cues would correspondingly reduce the ability of observers to detect and/or learn about alcohol from the demonstrators (cf. Molina & Chotro, 1989b). This social modulation of ethanol intake can easily be viewed within a Pavlovian conditioning perspective, for which the ethanol olfactory cues detected on a conspeci®c's breath serve as the conditioned stimulus (CS) and some aspect of the social interaction serves FIGURE 2 Mean (�SEM) intakes of ethanol and coffee (ml) by observers in Experiment 2 during a 24-hour two- bottle choice test. Observers had previously interacted for 30 min with demonstrators that had been intragastrically
  • 90. administered 0, 1.0, 1.5, or 3.0 g/kg ethanol. 106 Hunt, Holloway, and Scordalakes as the unconditioned stimulus (US). It is well known that characteristics of the CS, as well as the US, can in¯uence the rate and asymptotic level of conditioning (e.g., Pearce & Hall, 1980; Rescorla & Wagner, 1972). If the amount of respired ethanol cues is not of suf®cient intensity, then minimal conditioning would be expected. Therefore, little or no conditioned res- ponse (ethanol intake above 50%) would be expected to occur. This is precisely what was observed in Experiment 2. A different explanation, however, is required to describe the failure of the 3.0 g/kg observers to exhibit an ethanol preference. First, it is possible that the severely intoxicated demonstrator was capable through some means of communicating an aversion for ethanol
  • 91. to the observer (but see Galef, Wigmore, & Kennett, 1983). The post-ingestional consequences of a dose of 3.0 g/kg ethanol have been found to condition taste aversion to a concurrently presented sucrose solution, at least in younger animals (Hunt, Spear, & Spear, 1991). Furthermore, preweanling rats administered a 3.0 g/kg dose will subsequently avoid both ethanol odor and taste (e.g., Molina et al., 1989). It is therefore possible that a severely intoxicated demonstrator could transmit cues that serve to communicate even a weak ethanol avoidance to an observer, one that was capable of counteracting an established preference. A second explanation for the failure of 3.0 g/kg observers to acquire a preference for ethanol centers on the ability of the observers to detect the ethanol cues from the demonstrators. Clearly if the salience of the CS in part determines the strength of conditioning, then the intensity of the cues resulting from this high
  • 92. dose of ethanol should be even greater than those from the 1.5 g/kg condition (Hollstedt & Rydberg, 1985; Pohorecky & Brick, 1982). Although the concentra- tion of these cues might in fact be greater, the question remains as to whether the observers were actually able to detect these cues from the demonstrator. Casual observation of the 3.0 g/kg demonstrators indicated an almost complete lack of movement during the 30-minute interaction period; demonstrators tended to lie in a prone posture with their mouths directed toward the bottom of the cage. Although quantitative measurements of peri-oral contact between demon- strators and observers were not made, it is possible that the observers were unable to detect the ethanol cues emanating from the peri-oral region of the 3.0 g/kg demonstrators. In the absence of adequate detection of ethanol orosensory cues, the transmission of ethanol information from demonstrator to observer
  • 93. would not occur. By this account then, the failure of these observers to exhibit modi®ed ethanol ingestion resulted from an inability to learn about ethanol. GENERAL DISCUSSION Among the ways that human infants, young children, and adolescents can be exposed to alcohol in the context of the home and other social environments is through the detection of ethanol in the milk of a breast-feeding mother, long-term exposure to the odor of ethanol, direct topical application (e.g., for relief of teething pain), and/or the detection of respired ethanol cues (e.g., Fossey, 1993; Mennella & Beauchamp, 1991, 1993; Noll, Zucker, & Greenberg, 1990). It has been suggested that young children acquire informa- tion about alcohol via such types of exposure. Noll et al. (1990) reported that children whose parents were heavy drinkers were more likely to correctly identify the odor of alcohol than children whose parents drank
  • 94. only moderately or not at all. Another study reported similar results from a cohort of Scottish and British children (Fossey, 1993). In this study, the children were capable of correctly identifying alcohol by smell on over 80% of the occasions presented. Whether exposure to ethanol cues in these situations would be suf®cient to modify a human's later choice and acceptance of this drug is not yet known. Such experiential factors may indeed contribute substantially to the initiation of alcohol drinking in children and adolescents. It is known that children of alcoholics tend to drink more than children of non- alcoholics (U.S. Department of Health and Human Services, 1993). Although genetics no doubt play an important role here, the home environment is likely to also be extremely important. Children of alcoholics would spend more time in contact with alcohol- related cues, potentially affecting these children's
  • 95. learning of positive responses toward this drug. In animal studies designed to model such situations, young rats have consistently been shown to increase their acceptance of ethanol following each type of exposure. These results are highly suggestive that early experiences with alcohol cues can impact alcohol inges- tion patterns. For example, Hunt et al. (1993) investi- gated alcohol intake in preweanling rats following exposure to ethanol during a bout of nursing. Pups 12 ± 16 days of age increased their intake of ethanol after experiencing alcohol-contaminated milk in con- junction with the opportunity to suckle. Pepino, Kraebel, Lopez, Spear, and Molina (1998) have addi- tionally reported that exposure to alcohol-contami- nated milk can result in altered motor and cardiac responsivity toward ethanol cues in preweanlings. Bannoura et al. (1998) raised rat pups from birth to weaning in cages that were continuously scented with
  • 96. ethanol odor. These pups exhibited a clear preference for ethanol when tested for intake after weaning, Periadolescent Ethanol Intake 107 compared with controls not exposed to ethanol cues. These ®ndings could re¯ect a conditioned preference for ethanol odor experienced in the context of social interactions with the dam and littermates, or the effect of familiarity from long-term repeated exposure (Bronstein & Crockett, 1976; Leon, Galef, & Behse, 1977). Once ingested, a proportion of the ethanol dose is distributed throughout the body in unmetabolized form and is expired via its accumulation in the lungs. Additional routes of nonhepatic elimination involve salivation, urination, and perspiration (Hollstedt & Rydberg, 1985). Regardless of the actual route of elimination, animals are known to be capable of
  • 97. detecting these cues and learning about them in a variety of experimental preparations in which social contact with other animals is a critical feature (e.g., Bannoura et al., 1998; Chotro, Kraebel, McKinzie, Molina, & Spear, 1996; Hunt et al., 2000). Hunt et al. reported that social interactions with an intoxicated sibling result in enhanced intake of ethanol by pre- weanling animals, ranging in age from 8 to 16 days. These results, along with those of previous research (e.g., Chotro et al., 1996) indicate that exposure to ethanol in social contexts can play a role in modifying a preweanling animal's responsiveness toward etha- nol. The results of the present experiments indicate that a periadolescent rat will also acquire information about alcohol cues from an intoxicated sibling, and as a result will increase its subsequent intake of an etha- nol solution. It is, however, unclear at this time, whether or not exposure to ethanol-derived cues in these
  • 98. situations would have a relatively long-term impact on later alcohol choice (but see Galef, 1989; Hunt et al., 2000, Experiment 3; Strupp & Levitsky, 1984). Clearly, research is beginning to identify ways in which early experiences involving direct exposure to ethanol cues in the context of the home environment can affect young organisms' later acceptance of this drug. Continued investigation into the nature of the alcohol cues detected, and the mechanisms through which various types of learning take place, may help to further elucidate the consequences of social learn- ing for alcohol abuse in late childhood and adoles- cence. Moreover, the present experimental procedure may afford a relatively simple and robust experimental situation in which to further study the role of social experiences with alcohol in the initiation, and possibly also the maintenance, of excessive ethanol intake. NOTES
  • 99. This research was supported by grants AA12135 and AA12466 from the National Institutes of Alcohol Abuse and Alcoholism to P.S.H.. Experiment 1 was conducted as part of the requirements for a B.S. with honors in biological psychology by E.M.S., and Experiment 2 was conducted as part of the require- ments for a master's degree in psychology by J.L.H.. We are grateful to Elena Cuticelli for her assistance with data collection. REFERENCES Adams, N. (1995). Sex differences and the effects of tail pinch on ethanol drinking in Maudsley rats. Alcohol, 12, 463 ± 468. Bannoura, M. D., Kraebel, K. S., Spear, L. P., and Spear, N. E. (1998). Effects of preweanling ethanol odor expo- sure on ethanol preference. Alcohol, 15, 213 ± 217. Bronstein, P. M., & Crockett, D. P. (1976). Exposure to the odor of food determines the eating preferences of rat