SlideShare a Scribd company logo
MAKALAH REVIEW
A PRAGMATIC RESEARCH PHILOSOPHY FOR APPLIED SPORT PSYCHOLOGY
Dosen Pengampu :
Dr. Made Pramono, S.S. M.Hum
Disusun oleh :
Tiffanny Tessantya Utami
20060484039
2020B
UNIVERSITAS NEGERI SURABAYA
FAKULTAS ILMU OLAHRAGA
JURUSAN PENDIDIKAN KESEHATAN DAN REKREASI
TAHUN 2021
i
KATA PENGANTAR
Puji syukur kehadirat Tuhan Yang MahaKuasa karena telah memberikan kesempatan pada
penulis untuk menyelesaikan makalah ini. Atas rahmat dan hidayah-nya lah penulis dapat
menyelesaikan makalah yang berjudul A Pragmatic Research Philosophy for Applied Sport
Psychology tepat waktu.
Makalah berjudul A Pragmatic Research Philosophy for Applied Sport Psychology disusun
guna memenuhi tugas dosen Dr. Made Pramono, S.S. M.Hum pada mata kuliah Filsafat dan Sejarah
Olahraga di Universitas Negeri Surabaya. Selain itu, penulis juga berharap agar makalah ini dapat
menambah wawasan bagi pembaca tentang revie jurnal berjudul A Pragmatic Research Philosophy
for Applied Sport Psychology.
Penulis mengucapkan terimakasih sebesar-besarnya kepada Dr. Made Pramono, S. S. M.
Hum selaku dosen mata kuliah Filsafat dan Sejarah Olahraga. Tugas yang telah diberikan ini dapat
menambah wawasan dan pengetahuan terkait bidang yang ditekuni penulis. Penulis juga
mengucapkan terimakasih kepada semua pihak yang telah membantu proses penyusunan makalah
ini.
Penulis menyadari bahwa makalah ini masih jauh dari kata sempurna. Oleh karena itu,
penulis membutuhkan kritik dan saran yang membangun akan penulis terima demi kesempurnaan
makalah ini.
Gresik, 16 Maret 2021
Penulis
ii
DAFTAR ISI
Kata Pengantar......................................................................................................................i
Daftar Isi...............................................................................................................................ii
BAB I Jurnal.........................................................................................................................1
BAB II Review Jurnal...........................................................................................................34
BAB III Kesimpulan dan Saran...........................................................................................36
LINK SLIDE SHARE..........................................................................................................37
Daftar Pustaka......................................................................................................................38
1
BAB I
JURNAL
The College at Brockport: State University of New York
Digital Commons @Brockport
Kinesiology, Sport Studies and Physical Education
Faculty Publications Kinesiology, Sport Studies and Physical Education
3-2005
APragmatic Research Philosophy for Applied Sport Psychology
Peter R. Giacobbi Jr.
University of Florida
Artur Poczwardowski
Barry University
Peter F. Hager
The College at Brockport, phager@brockport.edu
2
Follow this and additional works at: https://digitalcommons.brockport.edu/pes_facpub
Part of the Kinesiology Commons
Repository Citation
Giacobbi, Peter R. Jr.; Poczwardowski, Artur; and Hager, Peter F., "A Pragmatic Research
Philosophy for Applied Sport Psychology" (2005). Kinesiology, Sport Studies and Physical
Education Faculty Publications. 80. https://digitalcommons.brockport.edu/pes_facpub/80
Citation/Publisher Attribution:
Sport Psychologist Mar2005, Vol. 19 Issue 1, p18-31
This Article is brought to you for free and open access by the Kinesiology, Sport Studies and
Physical Education at Digital Commons @Brockport. It has been accepted for inclusion in
Kinesiology, Sport Studies and Physical Education Faculty Publications by an authorized
administrator of Digital Commons @Brockport. For more information, please contact
kmyers@brockport.edu.
3
The Sport Psychologist, 2005, 19, 18-31
© 2005 Human Kinetics Publishers, Inc.
A Pragmatic Research Philosophy for Applied Sport
Psychology
Peter R. Giacobbi, Jr.
University of Florida
Artur Poczwardowski
Barry University
Peter Hager
State University of New York,
College at Brockport
A pragmatic research philosophy is introduced that
embraces mixed-method approaches to applied research
questions. With its origins in the work of Peirce (1984),
James (1907), Dewey (1931), and contemporary support
from Rorty (1982,1990,1991),pragmatism emphasizes the
practical problems experienced by people, the research
questions posited, and the consequences of inquiry. As a
way to highlight applications of pragmatism in sport
psychology, pragmatism is compared to constructivism
and positivism in terms of philosophical underpinnings
and methodological applications. The pragmatic
researcher is sensitive to the social, historical, and
political context from which inquiry begins and considers
morality, ethics, and issues of social justice to be important
throughout the research process. Pragmatists often use
4
pluralistic methods during multiphase research projects.
Exemplar design types are discussed that logically cohere
to a pragmatic research philosophy.
Debate and discussion concerned with knowledge construction
have occurred throughout the history of science (Wilson, 1998). In
the sport and exercise psychology literature, discussions have
focused on divisions between
Peter Giacobbi, Jr. is with the Department of Applied
Physiology and Kinesiology at the University of Florida,
Gainesville, FL 32611-8205. E-mail: Pgiacobbi@hhp.ufl.edu. Artur
Poczwardowski is with the Department of Sport and Exercise
Sciences at Barry University in Miami Shores, FL 33161; Peter
Hager is with the Department of Physical Education and Sport at the
State University of New York, College at Brockport. E-mail:
phager@Brockport.edu.
18
5
the academic discipline and professional activities of applied
practitioners, the appropriate sources of knowledge (e.g., interviews,
observations, experiments), data analytic strategies (e.g., quantitative
and/or qualitative), the underlying philosophies of various research
methodologies, and the development ofalternative paradigms
(Brustad, 2002; Dewar & Horn, 1992; Dzewaltowski, 1997; Hardy,
Jones, & Gould, 1996; Krane, 1994; Krane, Andersen, & Strean,
1997; Martens, 1979, 1987). While these discussions have influenced
the field of sport psychology, there is still a need to develop and
accept alternative ways of examining human behavior within sport
and physical activity contexts (Brustad, 2002; Dewar & Horn,
1992). Hence, this paper will characterize a pragmatic viewpoint
about the nature of paradigmatic controversies and present an
alternative position that is guided by the practical concerns of
applied sport psychologists and their clients (i.e., coaches and
athletes). Pragmatism, with origins in the work of Peirce (1984)
and James (1907), is a philosophy ofknowledge construction that
emphasizes practical solutions toapplied research questions and the
consequences of inquiry. The purpose of this paper is to offer
researchers and practitioners in sport psychology integrated
approaches to knowledge construction that logically cohere to a
pragmatic research philosophy.
The primary motivation for the present discussion stems from the
longstanding
gap between academic and applied sport psychology practice.
Martens (1979) was perhaps the first to express concern about
ecological validity of laboratory-based sport psychology research.
He noted the gap between field-based observations and orthodox
approaches to knowledge construction when he claimed,
“experiential knowledge and common sense have been more
appealing, and usually more beneficial, than knowledge from sport
psychology research” (p. 95). Martens (1979, 1987) recognized that
reliance on orthodox scientific methods and a failure to pay attention
to the unique sport and physical activity context lead to two
divergent aspects of sport psychology, academic and applied. His
solution was a heuristic approach that emphasized knowledge
gained through field studies, idiographic, and introspective methods.
Brustad (2002) and others (Biddle, Markland, Gilbourne,
Chatzisarantis, & Sparkes,2001;Dewar & Horn, 1992)have called for
the use of multiple methods and acceptance of alternative paradigms
in the research process. Brustad (2002) noted “it is possible to use
multi-method approaches to address any research question” (p. 34)
while Hardy et al. (1996) offered the following: “At times it is best
6
to use a qualitative methods, and at other times a quantitative
approach. Because both methods have strengths and limitations,
sometimes it may also be advisable to combine the two approaches”
(p. 259). Consistent with these sentiments, we feel it is important to
continue paradigmatic and epistemological discussion to advance the
use of mixed-method forms of knowledge construction in sport
psychology.
Another complimentary rationale for the present manuscript
stems from findings reported by Culver, Gilbert, and Trudel (2003).
They reviewed 485 published studies in three major sport
psychology journals (e.g., The Sport Psychologist, The Journal of
Applied Sport Psychology, and the Journal of Sport and Exercise
Psychology) to determine the number of studies that used qualitative
(e.g., interviews, life histories, participant observation) versus
quantitative methods (e.g., emphasis on numerical data and/or
inferential statistics). Of particular interest here was that only 32
studies over a period of ten years used a combination of qualitative
and quantitative techniques. This finding raises some interesting
questions similar to those raised by Culver et al. (2003). First, are
we to presume that many of the
7
important research questions in sport psychology can only be
answered by one technique alone? And second, is it possible that
multiple methods of data collection and analysis, used
simultaneously, could answer important research questions more
adequately? We argue the answer to question two is aresounding yes.
In an attempt to back up this claim, we present an alternative
philosophical and methodological paradigm for researchers and
practitioners in applied sport psychology: pragmatism. What follows
is a review of the historical roots of pragmatism. This review will
provide the philosophical underpinnings of a pragmatic paradigm of
knowledge construction for applied researchers in sport psychology.
Then, specific design strategies and examples of published studies
will be discussed followed by a review of pragmatic conceptions of
validity in scientific inquiry.
Before giving a brief account of pragmatism, two caveats are
in order. First, there are different forms of pragmatism and the reader
is encouraged to see Murphy (1990), Diggins (1994), and Putnam
(1995) to better understand the diverse representations of
pragmatism. Our intent here is to present one form of pragmatism
with the specific goal of offering applied researchers in sport
psychology a philosophical and methodological framework for
future research. Second, this work is intended to add to responsible
and productive discussion surrounding the underlying assumptions
of the research process (i.e., methodology) and the implications of
adopting one paradigmatic approach over another. The arguments
presented in this manuscript are consistent with the views of
scholars who have encouraged acceptance and tolerance of diverse
research epistemologies and methodologies in sport and exercise
psychology (Biddle et al. 2001; Brustad, 2002; Culver et al., 2003;
Hardy et al., 1996; Krane, 1994; Krane et al., 1997;
Strean & Roberts, 1992; Whaley, 2001).
An Introduction to Pragmatism
The word pragmatism comes from the Greek word, π
which means action from which the English words “practice” and
“practical” were derived. The term was popularized by William
James in his publication Pragmatism: A New Name for Some Old
Waysof Thinking.For James, pragmatism was not aprogram for what
he called “solving names” such as God or answering questions about
an absolute truth. Rather, pragmatism was an attempt to provide
practical solutions to contemporary problems experienced by people
and society. In this light, pragmatic thought was a “method only”
intended to change existing realities through concerted effort and
8
continued work (James, 1907, p. 51). Especially important to James
(1907) were the “respective practical consequences” of work, a
concern that has implications for the present manuscript (p. 45).
In more recent years, Rorty (1982, 1990, 1991) has emerged as
a proponent of pragmatism. Rorty argued that the concept of
knowledge as an accurate representation of truth needed to be
discarded. From this perspective, the methods and theories of
empirical science or any other discipline (e.g., theology) are not
capable of describing truth once and for all. Pragmatists deny there is
single reality and see no way for scientists or others to determine
whether their theories are closer to the truth than are their colleagues.
For these reasons, pragmatists have abandoned discussions that
concern the correspondence of theory and reality in favor of
dialogues where the value of different types of knowledge are
viewed as tools for helping us cope with and thrive within our
environment (Rorty, 1990).
9
Put simply, pragmatists opt for methods and theories that are more
useful to us within specific contexts (e.g., answers to practical
problems), not those that reveal underlying truths about the nature of
reality. As Rorty noted, pragmatists are set apart from positivists
and/or scientific realists who believe there is an objective truth or
“God’s-eye view” of the world unobstructed by the influence of
socio- cultural conditions and subjective biases (Rorty, 1990, p. 2).
Pragmatists view this purported truth as being “irrelevant to our
needs and our practices” (Rorty, 1990,
p. 2) because such a view would not account for the peculiarities of
our world (Williams, 1985).
Pragmatists further recognize that scientific inquiry is
contextual in nature and that the past and current social, historica l,
and political conditions strongly influence the scientific process.
Dewey (1931) noted “They [general ideas] are the basis for
organizing future observations and experience” (pp. 32-33) while
Rorty (1982) claimed that all inquiry begins with and is guided by the
previous discourse that researchers inherit from their predecessors.
In summary, pragmatists evaluate research findings based upon
their practical, social, and moral consequences as well as their
bearing on the human condition. They consider the problems under
study and the specific research questions as more important than the
underlying philosophical assumptions of the method. Pragmatists
typically use one or more methods deemed appropriate to the specific
research question being asked while simultaneously considering the
consequences of such inquiry (Cherryholmes, 1992; Howe, 1988;
Rorty, 1982).
Pragmatism and the Major Paradigms
In order to demonstrate the strengths and flexibility afforded by the
pragmatic research tradition, a more specific discussion about
philosophical issues in research is warranted. Rather than
abandoning discussions of this kind, as Rorty (1991) suggested, we
have chosen to take part in them in order to facilitate dialogue with
proponents of other research paradigms. As a way to highlight the
philosophical underpinnings of pragmatic thought, the discussion
that follows will review important terms related to the research
process. Also included in this discussion are examples of how two
relatively dichotomous paradigmatic philosophies, positivism and
constructivism, are unique from one another. Because of space
limitations, discussions about the broad array of post-positivistic
and non-positivistic viewpoints (e.g., cultural studies, feminist,
10
critical theory) are beyond the scope of this manuscript; Lincoln and
Guba (2000) provided an excellent review of these philosophies.
Rather than present all of these viewpoints, our intent is to contrast
the major philosophical assumptions of positivism and
constructivism in order to shed light on a pragmatic philosophy.
Epistemology refers to theories of knowledge, the nature and
source of legitimate knowledge, and the ability of research
subjects/participants to possess knowledge (Childers & Hentzi,
1995). Ontology generally refers to questions about the nature of
reality, that which is real, and whether an “objective” reality exists
“independent of the researcher” (Creswell, 1994, p. 4). Positivists
adopt objective epistemological and ontological views that assume
the existence of a real or true reality that can be measured and
understood with the application of scientific methods devoid of any
human contamination or subjective bias. The positivist also believes
that scientific findings can be generalized across time and
11
in different contexts and their findings represent so called truths
about the world devoid of the social, cultural, or historical context
from which these findings were made (Lincoln & Guba, 2000).
In contrast, field based researchers and constructivists often
embrace a subjective view of knowledge that is individualized and
context specific (Lincoln & Guba, 2000). Generally speaking, a
constructivistic philosophy focuses on the social, historical, and
value driven process of knowledge claims and denies the existence
of permanent (foundational) objective truths about reality. Thus for
the constructivist, the epistemological and ontological positions
embrace subjective views about reality constructed through
transactions within the socially situated activities of people and
communities (Lincoln & Guba, 2000).
The epistemological and ontological extremes represented by
positivists and constructivists have lead to contentious debates
within the social and behavioral sciences including sport
psychology (Brustad 2002; Dewar & Horn, 1992; Dzewaltowski
1997; Lincoln & Guba, 2000; Martens 1979, 1987). While
positivists and constructivists appear to have dichotomous
epistemological views (Lincoln & Guba, 2000), the pragmatist
argues that a continuum exists between objective and subjective
viewpoints the choice of which depends on the nature of the research
question being asked and the particular point in the research process
(Creswell, 2003; Tashakkori & Teddlie, 1998, 2003). Because
knowledge construction is contextual in nature and influenced by
the cultural, political, and historical conditions of the day, the
pragmatist equates objectivity with solidarity and agreement that
occurs through discourse and transaction within and between
communities of people (James, 1907). In short, pragmatists test the
veracity of facts through dialogue, the usefulness and consequences
of knowledge, and negotiations within communities, all of which
require ongoing dialog.
Both pragmatists and constructivists agree that the consequences
of scientific inquiry require reflection and analysis. Both approaches
consider the practical, moral, and ethical consequences of knowledge
construction as important considerations in research (Dewey, 1931;
James, 1907). Like constructivists, pragmatists would encourage
scholars and practitioners to engage in moral and ethical discussions
before, during, and after the implementation of scientific findings
(Dewey, 1931; Whaley, 2001). However, unlike more radical forms
of constructivism (Lincoln & Guba, 2000 review the varying forms
of constructivism), pragmatists consider agreement within a
scientific community as a way to approach objectivity. For the
12
pragmatist, total agreement, complete objectivity, and the search for
some ultimate truth are not considered useful endeavors (James,
1907). Rather, agreement within communities may allow a practical
level of truth to exist, but such practical truths are those findings that
prove useful within specific contexts (James, 1907). In other words,
the pragmatist prefers to avoid debate about whether constructivitic
or positivistic conceptions of truth are more accurate. The
pragmatist considers the practical concerns with human existence,
the research questions being asked, and the consequences of inquiry,
to be more important than which version of the truth is better than
another (James, 1907). As James (1907) asked “What difference
would it practically make to any one if this notion rather than that
notion were true? If no practical difference whatever can be traced,
then the alternatives mean practically the same thing” (p. 45). With
continued work and an examination of consequences, knowledge
claims may be supported by the weight of evidence available and
the logical arguments used to apply those claims. For instance,
13
sport psychology researchers and/or practitioners may test
psychological interventions to improve sport performance. If
agreement within a community were established that a specific
intervention improved sport performance, then pragmatists would
consider such agreement about research findings to approach an
objective position.
Another epistemological debate concerns the issue of time and
context free generalizations. The pragmatist argues that scientists
must be aware of the context within which research applications take
place. Because time and context free generalizations tend todiscount
or eliminate the role of social-historical-contextual factors on human
experience (Lincoln & Guba, 2000), a pragmatist would not be
inclined to make such claims. Science, knowledge construction, and
the practical concerns of people require ongoing deliberations about
the use and benefits of knowledge at a particular time or place.
Again, the iterative nature of knowledge construction requires
scientists to continuously reexamine the application of research
findings in many different contexts. This does not necessarily mean
that scientific findings must be replicated continuously. Rather, the
application and consequences of science must be reflected upon to
ensure practical utility, social value, and fairness to anyone who
might be or could be impacted by research findings.
Research Methods
Research methods are procedures, tools, or strategies of doing
research while methodology refers to larger issues in the research
process such as why particular methods are chosen (Smith, 1989).In
other words, methods are equated with specific design elements and
strategies ofinquiry such as how torecruit participants, the type of data
collected (i.e., numeric versus interview based); methodology is more
closely linked to philosophical issues within the research process. For
example, positivists believe only objective, tightly controlled,
directly observable empirical data provide legitimate knowledge;
therefore, experimental designs within contrived settings and
quantitative representations of data are typically chosen. A positivist
might approach the study of psychological stress by creating a
contrived laboratory task that mimics a real world competition
scenario. This approach may yield meaningful data about particular
research questions, but the context with which stress and
competition occur in actual athletic settings would not be readily
apparent from the data. In contrast, constructivists disavow
objective claims about reality and embrace time and context
dependent explanations about knowledge that are explored through
14
life histories, interviews, or participant observation. Elaborating on
the example above, a constructivist would interview an athlete
immediately following several athletic contests to understand an
athlete’s perceptions of competition stress. While mixing methods
from different paradigms is possible, desirable, and often productive,
the underlying assumptions of various paradigms (i.e., constructivists
versus positivism) may contradict one another (Krane & Baird, in
press; Lincoln & Guba, 2000). In other words, a constructivist may
use quantitative data but will adopt a subjective epistemology, while
a positivist who uses a post-experiment interview will do so under
an objective epistemology.
Pragmatists are trained in, and “use pluralistic approaches
to derive
knowledge about the problem” under study (Creswell, 2003, p. 12).
Because of the epistemological continuum noted above, the
pragmatist will often use multiple and/or mixed method designs
within single investigations and in an iterative
15
programmatic manner over several investigations. What follows is
a review of some mixed-method design strategies that cohere to a
pragmatic philosophy.
Mixed-Method Research
Mixed-method research involves a combination of procedures where
two or more data collection techniques and forms of analyses are
used and both contribute to the final results (Tashakkori & Teddlie,
1998). As discussed by Creswell (2003) and Tashakkori and Teddlie
(1998), mixed-method research involves both data collection
techniques and analyses because the data collected dictates the
analyses performed. In the following sections, we will review and
adapt the mixed-method designs described by Tashakkori and
Teddlie (1998) to the work of applied researchers in sport and
exercise psychology. Specifically, we will discuss equivalent status
designs, dominant-less-dominant designs, and designs with
multilevel uses. Within this discussion, examples of published
research studies that resemble the design types being reviewed here
will be highlighted.
Equivalent Status Designs
Equivalent status designs can involve both qualitative and quantitative
approaches in either temporal order. Consistent with a pragmatic
epistemology, researchers and/or practitioners who are trained in
multiple methods often favor the equivalent status design. With this
design, both qualitative and quantitative approaches contribute
equally to the final results, the results of phase one somehow inform
phase two, and neither approach is viewed asmore important than the
other (Tashakkori & Teddlie, 1998). For instance, Gould, Udrey,
Tuffey, and Loehr (1996a) and Gould Tuffey, Udrey, and Loehr
(1996b), in two stages, assessed the levels of burnout experienced by
competitive junior tennis players. In the first stage, a battery of
measures was administered to 62 junior tennis players and
discriminant function analysis helped identify the defining
psychological characteristics of burned out players. Then, 10
participants who scored highest on measures of burnout and
perfectionism were interviewed to shed further light on the
psychological characteristics of burned out junior tennis players.
Although the two studies independently contributed to our
knowledge of burned out junior tennis players, the value of this two-
stage approach was made evident by the authors’ conclusions
regarding the salience placed on social psychological factors by the
16
10 interview participants, and the support offered to Smith’s (1986)
cognitive-affective stress model of burnout. In this case, both stages
of data collection provided useful information to help scientists and
practitioners identify the characteristics and experiences of burned
out junior tennis players.
Alternatively, Strean (1998) discussed another potential
application of
qualitative methods as researchers can use interview data as a basis
for designing survey measures. This approach is consistent with an
equivalent status design as long as both studies contribute
independently to new knowledge and study one somehow informs
study two. For instance, Conroy, Poczwardowski, and Henschen,
(2001) used interviews to enhance understanding of individuals’
emotional responses to failure in stage one. Their interviews, along
with extensive theorizing about the fear of failure construct, formed
the theoretical rationale toward the development of items for the
Fear Appraisal Inventory designed and psychometrically tested
during stage two (Conroy, Willow, & Metzler, 2002). The findings
from both studies provided researchers and practitioners a practical
means to assess fear of
17
failure within the context of sport, and both studies independently
contributed new knowledge to the literature.
Equivalent status designs could also involve both qualitative and
quantitative data collection techniques used in a parallel or
simultaneous manner. As with the study designs described above,
this form of data collection involves dual and equal contributions to
the final results by both qualitative and quantitative forms of data
collection and analysis. Chase, Lirgg, and Feltz (1997) presented an
excellent example of an equivalent status design. They
simultaneously tested aspects of Bandura’s (1977, 1986) self-
efficacy theory while inductively building a new theory concerned
with the information used by coaches to form efficacy judgments.
Specifically, Bandura’s previous theoretical writings influenced the
authors’ theoretical rationale, the study design, and creation of items
to measure coaches’ efficacy. The authors also used an inductive
open-ended approach and created a conceptual framework of
important sources of efficacy information used by coaches. In this
case, the conceptual framework developed from the inductive
analysis of coaches’ open-ended responses were usefully combined
with the quantitative analyses and both forms of data were used to
support the authors’ conclusions that coaches’ efficacy expectations
can influence the performance of athletes.
Dominant-Less Dominant Design
Another mixed-method approach involves the use of one dominant
method while a relatively smaller part of the study uses an alternative
method. The dominant-less dominant design may appeal to
researchers or practitioners who strongly favor one research
approach, but collaboration with researchers who have different
training and/or epistemological viewpoints is needed. For instance,
Chaffin and Imreh (2002) assessed the practice of a concert pianist as
she learned Presto, of J. S. Bach’s Italian Concerto. In this study, the
researchers were interested in the performance cues (e.g., features of
music attended to during performance) identified by the learner for
the Presto to determine whether there were specific places during
the piece more difficult to remember than others. The main unit of
analysis was the number of starting or stopping places identified
through audio recordings during an extended learning period.
Secondary data collection involved a post experiment interview
concerned with when the learner was thinking about the identified
performance cues during memory runs and a recorded performance.
In this case, the researchers used the interview data in aless dominant
manner as a manipulation check of their quantitative findings.
18
A second approach to the dominant-less dominant design would
involve the
simultaneous or parallel collection of both qualitative interview and
quantitative survey data. For instance, Gould, Dieffenbach, and
Moffett (2002) interviewed 32 Olympic champions, their coaches,
and a parent or guardian to examine the psychological
characteristics of the athletes. At the same time, the authors
administered a battery of survey measures to compliment their
interview data. Although the authors contended that qualitative
interviews were the primary methods used, their results were
presented and discussed in a complimentary manner thus fitting the
description of a parallel-simultaneous dominant-less dominant
design. The value of this design was that both forms of data
collection, with a unique and difficult-to-attain participant
population, allowed the researchers tobring multiple epistemological
views and interpretations to the research process.
19
The simultaneous/parallel dominant-less dominant design type is
ideally suited to the work of applied sport psychologists who might
have unique opportunities to collect diverse forms of data with small
numbers of athletes or significant others within the athletic context.
Multilevel Approaches
While the preceding sections of this manuscript have focused on
design issues, recent developments in statistics offer potential new
design formulations in applied sport psychology. Specifically, the
advent of multilevel modeling and specifically hierarchical linear
modeling (HLM: Raudenbush & Bryk, 2002) can help researchers
address pervasive design challenges in the social and behavioral
sciences. Researchers in sport psychology (Pasevich, Estabrooks,
Brawley, & Carron, 2001) and other disciplines (Arnold, 1992;
Goldstein, 1995; Morris & Normand, 1992; Rosenberg, 1973) have
noted the challenges associated with hierarchical or nested structures
within the social and behavioral sciences. Pasevich et al. (2001)
claimed that athletic teams can be conceptualized as groups nested
within larger organizational units, while factors at each group level
may differentially impact performance. If repeated observations are
collected with a group of athletes from different teams, athletes on
the same team would likely exhibit more similarities to one another
than athletes on different teams. The observation that individual,
group, and organizational factors are unique entities leads to
violations of statistical independence and any between group
differences would be unaccounted error variance using traditional
statistics (Raudenbush & Bryk, 2002). To analyze multilevel data of
this nature, HLM analyses allow researchers to assess individual
behavior while controlling for the unique variability associated with
moderator variables such as group membership, level of athletic
competition (Raudenbush & Bryk, 2002), and personality
characteristics (David, Green, Martin, & Suls, 1997). While
applications of HLM remain scant in sport and exercise psychology
(see Giacobbi, Hausenblas, & Frye, 2005, for an exception), Pasevich
et al. (2001) noted the vast potential of HLM to address many
important research questions in sport and exercise psychology.
Multilevel research can also be used to collect quantitative data
on one level
and qualitative data on another. For instance, a sport psychologist
working or doing research with collegiate athletes could administer
the Group Environment Questionnaire (Brawley, Carron, &
Widmeyer, 1987) to athletes from various schools. At the same time,
the researcher could interview coaches and athletic directors to
20
better understand how group level variables such as institutional,
academic, and athletic priorities are related to interactions within
teams. The coach/athletic director data could be used to make the
athlete level data more meaningful and understandable, and vice
versa. Multilevel approaches can be used simultaneously or
sequentially.
In summary, equivalent status designs involve two stages of
data collection with either form of inquiry preceded by the other.
Both elements of the design involve dual contributions to the
literature and data collected in stage one somehow informs data
collected in stage two. The dominant-less dominant design also
involves either form of inquiry followed by the other with one
method forming the dominant unit of analysis. Another variant of the
dominant-less dominant design would comprise two forms of data
collected in a parallel or simultaneous manner. Similar to equivalent
status designs, dominant-less-dominant designs allow the
21
use of objective and subjective viewpoints, diverse forms of data
collection, and the possibility of multiple ways to interpret the data.
As long as epistemological differences can be put aside and
researchers adopt the pragmatic maxims, these design strategies
could provide a useful way for researchers and practitioners to
engage in collaborative projects to address applied research
questions. Finally, multilevel approaches offer pragmatic
researchers new and innovative ways to advance applied research.
We would argue that familiarity with multilevel approaches could
lead to creative new strategies for mixed-method researchers. The
focus now turns to pragmatic conceptions of validity.
A Pragmatic Critique of Validity in Science
A positivist embraces rigid criteria and the application of accepted
methods to make validity claims. This position considers the
application of rigorous or foundational criteria as a means to gain
scientific truths unimpeded by human bias (Lincoln & Guba, 2000).
In contrast, constructivists, like pragmatists, reject permanent or
universal truths and instead argue that valid truth arises out of
relationships, negotiations, or dialog between members of
communities (Lincoln & Guba, 2000; Sparkes, 1998). In this light,
pragmatic conceptions of validity call for extended discussions
about the practical utility, role, and impact of science within specific
contexts (Dewey, 1931; James, 1907). Classical pragmatists, like
Peirce (1984) and Dewey (1931), explicitly discussed the
consequences and outcomes of inquiry (i.e., practical, ethical, and
moral) because research outcomes are directly linked to claims about
validity in the scientific process. As such, these outcomes should be
viewed as guiding principles that require ongoing discussions about
the application of research in sport psychology.
From a pragmatic perspective, research findings, outcomes, and
consequences require dialog and discussion within the scientific
community. Since morality and ethics are part of the scientific
process (Bernard, 2000), the pragmatic moral/ ethical rule is to
continuously reflect on the many possible consequences of scientific
inquiry. Dewey considered two ethical perspectives for the scientist
to consider. The first perspective occurs at the front end of inquiry
and is concerned with the overarching ideals, values, and purposes
that guide inquiry. The second perspective is the test of consequences
that occurs when research results are applied or disseminated in some
practical manner. Along these lines, we consider the following
questions, similar to those discussed by Whaley (2001), to be
22
important for researchers to consider: (a) What questions are being
addressed and are these questions relevant to the individuals being
examined and/or society as a whole?
(b) Who are the participants? Is the research sample diverse in terms
of race, age, ethnicity, socioeconomic status, and sexual orientation?
(c) What implications do the study findings have for diverse groups
of individuals? If the findings do not generalize to diverse groups,
do the study authors address these limitations? (d) Finally, are
attempts made to disseminate the study findings to people who
might benefit from this knowledge (e.g., coaches, athletes,
psychologists)? We agree with Whaley’s (2001) proposal that
researchers should consider reflections about the research questions
being asked, who should be sampled, why the sample was chosen,
and how researchers choose to practice science as integral to
scientific advancement and the promotion of social justice. Such
questions are important for applied researchers throughout the
research process and are consistent with
23
the pragmatic moral and ethical views described above.
The contemporary writings of Schwandt (1996) and Messick
(1995) also evoked pragmatic conversations about validity.
Schwandt (1996) argued for the development of a “practical
philosophy” where inquiry is judged by how well research reports
stimulate judgment, “practical wisdom,” and dialog between
“critical parties to the research encounter” (p. 69). Schwandt noted
the biggest challenge for the “practical philosopher” involves the
application of general theories or principles to particular cases (i.e.,
deduction). Schwandt (1996) claimed “The aim of such inquiry is not
to replace practitioners’ common sense knowledge of their
respective joint practices with allegedly more sophisticated,
theoretical, scientific knowledge but to encourage practitioners to
critically reflect on and reappraise their commonsense knowledge”
(p. 64). The practical application of scientific findings is especially
important for the work of applied sport psychologists and/or the
scientist-practitioner where knowledge dissemination about the
ethical and valid delivery of psychological services requires
continuous reflection, dialog, and public discourse.
Messicks’s notion of “consequential validity” is consistent with
Schwandt’s (1996) call for ongoing dialog and discussion between
those involved with, or impacted by, the research process. While
Messick’s critique is specific to the validation of psychological tests
and measures, there are close parallels to lessons learned in the sport
psychology literature. For instance, the valid and ethical use of
personality inventories in sport is an important issue (Vealey, 2002).
When practitioners use and market personality inventories as
screening tools or make predictive claims about sport performance,
issues of consequential validity are raised. Without extensive testing
and evidence to form the basis of such claims, personality tests
should not be used or marketed in such a manner (Vealey, 2002). In
short, any use or application of psychological tests or principles
would raise pragmatic issues of validity and dialog between
communities of people would be encouraged.
Conclusion
The pragmatic philosophy presented in this manuscript offers a
variety of potential applications in sport psychology. We discussed
examples of published studies that used mixed-methods of analysis
in the applied sport psychology literature. As a concluding point,
we feel it is important to note that we do not encourage the
“atheoretical” practice of science. Rather, we would argue the use of
24
mixed methods within a pragmatic philosophy would help address
applied research questions from a theoretical perspective. James
(1907) offered the following perspective about how pragmatism
could influence the way theories are used in science:
Theories thus become instruments, not answers to
enigmas in which we can rest. We don’t lie back
upon them, we move forward, and, on occasion,
make nature over again by their aid. Pragmatism
unstiffens all our theories, limbers them up and sets
each one at work. (p. 52)
By embracing an eclectic research approach, and by
considering the consequences of inquiry, future collaborations
between academic and applied sport psychologists might be
enhanced. It is our hope that the pragmatic research
25
philosophy presented here will open doors to collaborative
opportunities for individuals interested in exploring applied research
questions from multiple perspectives.
References
Arnold, C.L. (1992). An introduction to hierarchical linear models.
Measurement and Evaluation in Counseling and Development,
25, 58-90.
Bandura, A. (1977). Self-efficacy: Toward a unifying theory of
behavioral change.
Psychological Review, 84, 191-215.
Bandura, A. (1986). Social Foundations of thought and action: A
social cognitive theory.
Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall.
Bernard, H.R. (2000). Social research methods: Qualitative and
quantitative approaches.
Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.
Biddle, S.J.H., Markland, D., Gilbourne, D., Chatzisarantis, N.L.D.,
& Sparkes, A.C. (2001). Research methods in sport and
exercise psychology: Quantitative and qualitative issues.
Journal of Sport Sciences, 19, 777-809.
Brawley, L.R., Carron, A.V., & Widmeyer, W.N. (1987).Assessing
the cohesion of teams: Validity of the group environment
questionnaire. Journal of Sport Psychology, 9, 275-294.
Brustad, R. (2002). A critical analysis of knowledge construction in
sport psychology. In
T. Horn (Ed.), Advances in sport psychology (2nd ed.).
Champaign, IL: Human Kinetics.
Chaffin, R., & Imreh, G. (2002). Practicing perfection: Piano
performance asexpert memory.
26
Psychological Science, 13, 342-349.
Chase, M.A., Lirgg, C.D., & Feltz, D.L. (1997). Do coaches’ efficacy
expectations for their team predict team performance? The
Sport Psychologist, 11, 8-23.
Cherryholmes, C.C. (1992). Notes on pragmatism and scientific
realism. Educational Researcher, 21, 13-17.
Childers, J., & Hentzi, G. (1995). The Columbia dictionary of
modern literary and cultural criticism. New York: Columbia
University Press.
Conroy, D.E., Poczwardowski, A., & Henschen, K.P. (2001).
Evaluative criteria associated with failure and success for elite
athletes and performing artists. Journal of Applied Sport
Psychology, 13, 300-322.
Conroy, D.E., Willow, J.P., & Metzler, J.N. (2002).
Multidimensional fear of failure measurement: The
Performance Failure Appraisal Inventory. Journal of Applied
Sport Psychology, 14, 76-90.
Creswell, J.W.(1994). Research design: Qualitativeand quantitative
approaches. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.
Creswell, J.W. (2003). Research design: Qualitative, quantitative,
and mixed methodsapproaches (2nd ed.). Thousand Oaks, CA:
Sage.
Culver, D.M., Gilbert, W.D., & Trudel, P. (2003). A decade of
qualitative research in sport psychology journals: 1990-1999.
The Sport Psychologist, 17, 1-15.
David, J.P., Green, P.J., Martin, R., & Suls, J. (1997). Differential
roles of neuroticism, extraversion, and event desirability for
mood in daily life: An integrative model of top-down and
bottom-up influences. Journal of Personality and Social
Psychology, 73, 149-159.
Dewar, A., & Horn, T.S. (1992). A critical analysis of knowledge
construction in sport psychology. In T.S. Horn (Ed.), Advances
27
in sport psychology (pp. 13-22). Champaign, IL: Human
Kinetics.
28
Dewey, J. (1931). The development of pragmatism. In H.S. Thayer
(Ed.), Pragmatism:
The classic writings (pp. 23-40). Indianapolis, IN: Hackett.
Diggins, J.P. (1994). The promise of pragmatism. Chicago: The
University of Chicago Press.
Dzewaltowski, D.A. (1997). The ecology of physical activity and
sport: Merging science and practice. Journal of Applied Sport
Psychology, 9, 254-276.
Giacobbi, P.R., Jr., Hausenblas, H.A., & Frye, N. (2005). A
naturalistic assessment of the relationship between personality,
daily life event, leisure-time physical activity, and mood.
Psychology of Sport & Exercise, 6, 67-81.
Goldstein, H. (1995). Multilevel statistical models (2nd ed.). New
York: John Wiley. Gould, D., Tuffey, S., Udry, E., & Loehr, J.
(1996a). Burnout in junior elite tennis players
II: Qualitative analysis. The Sport Psychologist, 10, 341-366.
Gould, D., Udry, E., Tuffey, S., & Loehr, J. (1996b). Burnout in
junior elite tennis players: A quantitative psychological
assessment. The Sport Psychologist, 10, 322-340.
Gould, D., Dieffenbach, K., & Moffett, A. (2002). Psychological
characteristics and their development in Olympic Champions.
Journal of Applied Sport Psychology, 14, 172-204.
Hardy, L., Jones, G., & Gould, D. (1996). Understanding
psychological preparation for sport: Theory and practice of
elite performers. Chichester, UK: Wiley.
Howe, K.R. (1988). Against the quantitative-qualitative
incompatibility thesis or dogmas die hard. Educational
Researcher, 17, 10-16.
James, W. (1907). Pragmatism: A new name for some old ways of
thinking. New York: Longmans, Green, and Company.
29
Krane, V. (1994). A feminist perspective on contemporary sport
psychology research. The Sport Psychologist, 8, 393-410.
Krane, V., Andersen, M.B., & Strean, W.B. (1997). Issues of
qualitative research methods and presentation. Journal of Sport
and Exercise Psychology, 19, 213-218.
Krane, V., & Baird, S.M. (in press). Using ethnography in applied
sport psychology. Journal of Applied Sport Psychology.
Lincoln, Y.S., & Guba, E.G. (2000). Paradigmatic controversies,
contradictions, and emerging confluences. In N.K. Denzin &
Y.S.Lincoln (Eds.), Handbook of qualitativeresearch (2nd ed.,
pp. 163-188). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.
Martens, R. (1979). About smocks and jocks. Journal of Sport
Psychology, 1, 94-99. Martens. R. (1987). Science, philosophy, and
sport psychology. The Sport Psychologist,
1, 29-55.
Messick, S. (1995). Validity of psychological assessment:
Validation of inferences from persons’ responses and
performances as scientific inquiry into score meaning.
American Psychologist, 50, 741-749.
Morris, C., & Normand, S. (1992). Hierarchical models for
combining information and for meta analysis. Bayesian
Statistics, 4, 321-344.
Murphy, J.P. (1990). Pragmatism: From Peirce to Davidson.
Boulder, CO: Westview.
Paskevich, D.M., Estabrooks, P.A., Brawley, L.R., & Carron, A.V.
(2001). Group cohesion in sport and exercise. In R.N. Singer,
H.A. Hausenblas, & C.M. Janelle (Eds.), Handbook of sport
psychology (2nd ed., pg. 472-494). New York: Wiley.
Peirce, C.S. (1984). Review of Nichols’ A treatise on cosmology. In
H.S. Thayer (Ed.), Meaning and action: A critical history of
pragmatism (pp. 493-495). Indianapolis: IN: Hackett.
30
Putnam, H. (1995). Pragmatism: An open question. Oxford:
Blackwell.
31
Raudenbush, S.W., & Bryk, A.A., (2002). Hierarchical linear
models: Applications and data analysis methods (2nd ed.).
Newbury Park, CA: Sage.
Rorty, R. (1982). Consequences of pragmatism (Essays: 1972-
1980). Minneapolis, MN: University of Minnesota Press.
Rorty, R. (1990). Introduction: Pragmatism as anti-
representationalism. In J.P. Murphy,
Pragmatism: From Peirce to Davidson. Boulder, CO:
Westview Press.
Rorty, R. (1991). Objectivity,relativismandtruth:Philosophical
paper,volumeI.Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Rosenberg, B. (1973). Linear regression with randomly dispersed
parameters. Biometrika,
60, 61-75.
Schwandt, T.A. (1996). Farewell to criteriology. Qualitative Inquiry,
2, 58-72.
Smith, R. (1986). Toward a cognitive-affective model of athletic
burnout. Journal of Sport Psychology, 8, 36-50.
Smith, J. K. (1989). The nature of social and educational inquiry.
Empiricism versus interpretation. Norwood, NJ: Ablex.
Sparkes, A.C. (1998). Validity in qualitative inquiry and the problem
of criteria: Implications for sport psychology. The Sport
Psychologist, 12, 363-386.
Strean, W.B. (1998). Possibilities for qualitative research in sport
psychology. The Sport Psychologist, 12, 333-345.
Strean, W.B. & Roberts, G.C. (1992). Future directions in applied
sport psychology research.
The Sport Psychologist, 6, 55-65.
32
Tashakkori, A., & Teddlie, C. (1998). Mixed methodology:
Combining qualitative and quantitative approaches. Thousand
Oaks, CA: Sage.
Tashakkori, A. & Teddlie, C. (2003). Handbook of mixed-methods
research in the social and behavioral sciences. Thousand
Oaks, CA: Sage.
Vealey, R.S. (2002). Personality and sport behavior. In T. Horn
(Ed.), Advances in sport psychology (2nd ed., pp. 39-82).
Champaign, IL: Human Kinetics.
Whaley, D.E. (2001). Feminist methods and methodologies in sport
and exercise and sport psychology: Issues of identity and
differences. The Sport Psychologist, 15, 419- 430.
Williams, B. (1985). Ethics and the limits of philosophy.
Cambridge: Harvard University Press.
Wilson, E.O. (1998). Consilience: The unity of knowledge. New
York: Vintage Books.
Author Note
The authors would like to thank Vikki Krane for her insightful
comments during the review process.
Manuscript submitted: December 16, 2003
Revision received: May 28, 2004
33
Copyright of Sport Psychologist is the property of Human Kinetics Publishers, Inc. and its content
may not be copied or emailed to multiple sites or posted to a listserv without the copyright
holder's express written permission. However, users may print, download, or email articles for
individual use.
34
BAB II
REVIEW JURNAL
Judul A Pragmatic Research Philosophy for Applied Sport Psychology
Pengarang Peter R. Giacobbi, Jr.
Nama Jurnal A Pragmatic Research Philosophy
Volume, issue Vol. 19 Issue 1, p18-31
Tahun, Halaman Maret 2005, 16
Tujuan Penelitian Untuk menawarkan para peneliti dan praktisi dalam pendekatan psikologi
olahraga terintegrasi untuk konstruksi pengetahuan yang secara logis
sesuai dengan filosofi penelitian pragmatis
Kutipan Repositori Giacobbi, Peter R. Jr .; Poczwardowski, Artur; dan Hager, Peter F., "Filsafat
Penelitian Pragmatis untuk Psikologi Olahraga Terapan" (2005).
Filsafat Penelitian Pragmatis untuk Psikologi Olahraga Terapan Peter R. Giacobbi, Jr.
Universitas Florida Artur Poczwardowski Barry University Peter Hager State University of New
York, College di Brockport Sebuah filosofi penelitian pragmatis diperkenalkan yang mencakup
pendekatan metode campuran untuk pertanyaan penelitian terapan.
Sebagai cara untuk menyoroti aplikasi pragmatisme dalam psikologi olahraga , pragmatisme
dibandingkan dengan konstruktivisme dan positivisme dalam hal dasar filosofis dan aplikasi
metodologis.
Jenis desain contoh dibahas yang secara logis sesuai dengan filosofi penelitian pragmatis.
Dalam literatur psikologi olahraga dan latihan , diskusi telah difokuskan pada pembagian antara
Filsafat Penelitian Pragmatis: 19 disiplin akademik dan kegiatan profesional praktisi terapan,
sumber pengetahuan yang sesuai (misalnya, wawancara, ob porsi, eksperimen), strategi analitik
data (misalnya, kuantitatif dan / atau kualitatif), filosofi yang mendasari berbagai metodologi
penelitian, dan pengembangan paradigma alternatif (Brustad, 2002; Dewar & Horn, 1992;
35
Dzewaltowski, 1997; Hardy, Jones, & Gould, 1996; Krane, 1994; Krane, Andersen, & Strean,
1997; Martens, 1979, 1987).
Tujuan dari makalah ini adalah untuk menawarkan para peneliti dan praktisi dalam pendekatan
psikologi olahraga terintegrasi untuk konstruksi pengetahuan yang secara logis sesuai dengan
filosofi penelitian pragmatis.
Motivasi utama karena diskusi ini berasal dari kesenjangan yang sudah berlangsung lama antara
praktik psikologi olahraga akademis dan terapan.
Dia mencatat kesenjangan antara pengamatan berbasis lapangan dan pendekatan ortodoks untuk
konstruksi pengetahuan ketika dia mengklaim, "pengetahuan pengalaman dan akal sehat telah
lebih menarik, dan biasanya lebih bermanfaat, daripada pengetahuan dari penelitian psikologi
olahraga ".
Mereka meninjau 485 studi yang diterbitkan dalam tiga jurnal psikologi olahraga utama (misalnya,
The Sport Psychologist, The Journal of Applied Sport Psychology, dan Jurnal Psikologi Olahraga
dan Latihan) untuk menentukan jumlah studi yang menggunakan kualitatif (misalnya, wawancara,
riwayat hidup, observasi partisipan) versus metode kuantitatif (misalnya, penekanan pada n data
umerical dan / atau statistik inferensial).
Dalam upaya untuk mendukung klaim ini, kami menyajikan paradigma filosofis dan metodologis
alternatif bagi para peneliti dan praktisi dalam psikologi olahraga terapan: pragmatism.
Ini review akan memberikan dasar filosofis dari paradigma pragmatis konstruksi pengetahuan
untuk peneliti terapan dalam psikologi olahraga.
Maksud kami di sini adalah untuk menyajikan satu bentuk pragmatisme dengan tujuan khusus
menawarkan peneliti terapan dalam psikologi olahraga filosofis dan kerangka metodologi untuk
penelitian masa depan.
Argumen yang disajikan dalam naskah ini konsisten dengan pandangan para sarjana yang telah
mendorong penerimaan dan toleransi dari berbagai epistemologi penelitian dan metodologi dalam
psikologi olahraga dan Latihan.
36
BAB III
KESIMPULAN DAN SARAN
A. Kesimpulan
jurnal diatas membahas tentang literatur psikologi olahraga dan latihan , diskusi telah
difokuskan pada pembagian antara Filsafat Penelitian Pragmatis: 19 disiplin akademik dan
kegiatan profesional praktisi terapan, sumber pengetahuan yang sesuai (misalnya,
wawancara, ob porsi, eksperimen), strategi analitik data (misalnya, kuantitatif dan / atau
kualitatif), filosofi yang mendasari berbagai metodologi penelitian, dan pengembangan
paradigma alternatif (Brustad, 2002; Dewar & Horn, 1992; Dzewaltowski, 1997; Hardy,
Jones, & Gould, 1996; Krane, 1994; Krane, Andersen, & Strean, 1997; Martens,
1979, 1987).
Tujuan dari makalah ini adalah untuk menawarkan para peneliti dan praktisi dalam
pendekatan psikologi olahraga terintegrasi untuk konstruksi pengetahuan yang secara logis
sesuai dengan filosofi penelitian pragmatis.
B. Saran
Sebagai penulis saya menyadari bahwa masih banyak kekurangan dalam makalah ini.
Untuk kedepannya penulis akan menjelaskan secara detail dari sumber yang lebih banyak.
37
LINK SLIDE SHARE
38
DAFTAR PUSTAKA
Peter R. Giacobbi Jr. University of Florida Artur Poczwardowski Barry University Peter
F. Hager The College at Brockport, phager@brockport.edu

More Related Content

What's hot

PSY 326 Exceptional Education - snaptutorial.com
PSY 326  Exceptional Education - snaptutorial.comPSY 326  Exceptional Education - snaptutorial.com
PSY 326 Exceptional Education - snaptutorial.com
DavisMurphyB45
 
Psy 326 Enhance teaching / snaptutorial.com
Psy 326  Enhance teaching / snaptutorial.comPsy 326  Enhance teaching / snaptutorial.com
Psy 326 Enhance teaching / snaptutorial.com
HarrisGeorg64
 
Writing a literature review
Writing a literature reviewWriting a literature review
Writing a literature review
Vevi WulanSari
 
PSY 326 Extraordinary Life/newtonhelp.com 
PSY 326 Extraordinary Life/newtonhelp.com PSY 326 Extraordinary Life/newtonhelp.com 
PSY 326 Extraordinary Life/newtonhelp.com 
myblue47
 
Psy 326 Effective Communication - tutorialrank.com
Psy 326 Effective Communication - tutorialrank.comPsy 326 Effective Communication - tutorialrank.com
Psy 326 Effective Communication - tutorialrank.com
Bartholomew83
 
PSY 326 Education Specialist / snaptutorial.com
PSY 326 Education Specialist / snaptutorial.comPSY 326 Education Specialist / snaptutorial.com
PSY 326 Education Specialist / snaptutorial.com
McdonaldRyan164
 
Review of literature
Review of literatureReview of literature
Review of literature
Meghana Sudhir
 
Review of literature and thesis writing
Review of literature and thesis writingReview of literature and thesis writing
Review of literature and thesis writing
Ramani Kalidasan
 
Challenges Confronting Scientific Research A Systematic Review
Challenges Confronting Scientific Research A Systematic ReviewChallenges Confronting Scientific Research A Systematic Review
Challenges Confronting Scientific Research A Systematic Review
ijtsrd
 
Research Methodology-01: Review of Research Literature
Research Methodology-01: Review of Research LiteratureResearch Methodology-01: Review of Research Literature
Research Methodology-01: Review of Research Literature
Bhasker Vijaykumar Bhatt
 
Sources of Review Of Related Literature
Sources of Review Of Related LiteratureSources of Review Of Related Literature
Sources of Review Of Related Literature
KAVITHACO
 
Mmc doctors as researchers
Mmc doctors as researchersMmc doctors as researchers
Mmc doctors as researchers
RajasekaranNeelakantan
 
Literature Review and Research Related Problems
Literature Review and Research Related ProblemsLiterature Review and Research Related Problems
Literature Review and Research Related Problems
Chris Okiki
 
Systems thinking in Ayurveda
Systems thinking in AyurvedaSystems thinking in Ayurveda
Systems thinking in Ayurveda
Kishor Patwardhan
 
Dr. Obumneke Amadi-Onuoha Scripts- 9
Dr. Obumneke Amadi-Onuoha  Scripts- 9Dr. Obumneke Amadi-Onuoha  Scripts- 9
Dr. Obumneke Amadi-Onuoha Scripts- 9
Discover Health Global Initiative
 
The Literature and Study Review and Ethical Concern
The Literature and Study  Review and Ethical ConcernThe Literature and Study  Review and Ethical Concern
The Literature and Study Review and Ethical Concern
Jo Balucanag - Bitonio
 
Chapters 1 And 2
Chapters 1 And 2Chapters 1 And 2
Chapters 1 And 2
EDUCAUSE
 
Literature review and referencing
Literature review and referencingLiterature review and referencing
Literature review and referencing
Dr Lipilekha Patnaik
 
Research process
Research processResearch process
Research process
StudsPlanet.com
 
What are the perceived coaching practices that have contributed to participat...
What are the perceived coaching practices that have contributed to participat...What are the perceived coaching practices that have contributed to participat...
What are the perceived coaching practices that have contributed to participat...
iosrjce
 

What's hot (20)

PSY 326 Exceptional Education - snaptutorial.com
PSY 326  Exceptional Education - snaptutorial.comPSY 326  Exceptional Education - snaptutorial.com
PSY 326 Exceptional Education - snaptutorial.com
 
Psy 326 Enhance teaching / snaptutorial.com
Psy 326  Enhance teaching / snaptutorial.comPsy 326  Enhance teaching / snaptutorial.com
Psy 326 Enhance teaching / snaptutorial.com
 
Writing a literature review
Writing a literature reviewWriting a literature review
Writing a literature review
 
PSY 326 Extraordinary Life/newtonhelp.com 
PSY 326 Extraordinary Life/newtonhelp.com PSY 326 Extraordinary Life/newtonhelp.com 
PSY 326 Extraordinary Life/newtonhelp.com 
 
Psy 326 Effective Communication - tutorialrank.com
Psy 326 Effective Communication - tutorialrank.comPsy 326 Effective Communication - tutorialrank.com
Psy 326 Effective Communication - tutorialrank.com
 
PSY 326 Education Specialist / snaptutorial.com
PSY 326 Education Specialist / snaptutorial.comPSY 326 Education Specialist / snaptutorial.com
PSY 326 Education Specialist / snaptutorial.com
 
Review of literature
Review of literatureReview of literature
Review of literature
 
Review of literature and thesis writing
Review of literature and thesis writingReview of literature and thesis writing
Review of literature and thesis writing
 
Challenges Confronting Scientific Research A Systematic Review
Challenges Confronting Scientific Research A Systematic ReviewChallenges Confronting Scientific Research A Systematic Review
Challenges Confronting Scientific Research A Systematic Review
 
Research Methodology-01: Review of Research Literature
Research Methodology-01: Review of Research LiteratureResearch Methodology-01: Review of Research Literature
Research Methodology-01: Review of Research Literature
 
Sources of Review Of Related Literature
Sources of Review Of Related LiteratureSources of Review Of Related Literature
Sources of Review Of Related Literature
 
Mmc doctors as researchers
Mmc doctors as researchersMmc doctors as researchers
Mmc doctors as researchers
 
Literature Review and Research Related Problems
Literature Review and Research Related ProblemsLiterature Review and Research Related Problems
Literature Review and Research Related Problems
 
Systems thinking in Ayurveda
Systems thinking in AyurvedaSystems thinking in Ayurveda
Systems thinking in Ayurveda
 
Dr. Obumneke Amadi-Onuoha Scripts- 9
Dr. Obumneke Amadi-Onuoha  Scripts- 9Dr. Obumneke Amadi-Onuoha  Scripts- 9
Dr. Obumneke Amadi-Onuoha Scripts- 9
 
The Literature and Study Review and Ethical Concern
The Literature and Study  Review and Ethical ConcernThe Literature and Study  Review and Ethical Concern
The Literature and Study Review and Ethical Concern
 
Chapters 1 And 2
Chapters 1 And 2Chapters 1 And 2
Chapters 1 And 2
 
Literature review and referencing
Literature review and referencingLiterature review and referencing
Literature review and referencing
 
Research process
Research processResearch process
Research process
 
What are the perceived coaching practices that have contributed to participat...
What are the perceived coaching practices that have contributed to participat...What are the perceived coaching practices that have contributed to participat...
What are the perceived coaching practices that have contributed to participat...
 

Similar to Tiffanny tessantya utami 039 2020_b_review jurnal 2

Rindang muhammad husain 2020 b_review jurnal 3
Rindang muhammad husain 2020 b_review jurnal 3Rindang muhammad husain 2020 b_review jurnal 3
Rindang muhammad husain 2020 b_review jurnal 3
rindanghusain
 
Rindang muhammad husain 2020 b_review jurnal 1
Rindang muhammad husain 2020 b_review jurnal 1Rindang muhammad husain 2020 b_review jurnal 1
Rindang muhammad husain 2020 b_review jurnal 1
rindanghusain
 
Lailatul rohmah 2020 b_review jurnal 3
Lailatul rohmah 2020 b_review jurnal 3Lailatul rohmah 2020 b_review jurnal 3
Lailatul rohmah 2020 b_review jurnal 3
LailatulRohmah8
 
Rindang muhammad husain 2020 b_review jurnal 2
Rindang muhammad husain 2020 b_review jurnal 2Rindang muhammad husain 2020 b_review jurnal 2
Rindang muhammad husain 2020 b_review jurnal 2
rindanghusain
 
Rindang muhammad husain 2020 b_review jurnal 4
Rindang muhammad husain 2020 b_review jurnal 4Rindang muhammad husain 2020 b_review jurnal 4
Rindang muhammad husain 2020 b_review jurnal 4
rindanghusain
 
Ethical issues in sports1.  Post three peer-reviewed articles on.docx
Ethical issues in sports1.  Post three peer-reviewed articles on.docxEthical issues in sports1.  Post three peer-reviewed articles on.docx
Ethical issues in sports1.  Post three peer-reviewed articles on.docx
elbanglis
 
Lailatul rohmah 2020 b_review jurnal 2
Lailatul rohmah 2020 b_review jurnal 2Lailatul rohmah 2020 b_review jurnal 2
Lailatul rohmah 2020 b_review jurnal 2
LailatulRohmah8
 
Makalah Review Jurnal Internasional
Makalah Review Jurnal InternasionalMakalah Review Jurnal Internasional
Makalah Review Jurnal Internasional
JuliaPrasanti
 
Ika Puji Rahayu _2020 B_Review Jurnal 2 Olahraga
Ika Puji Rahayu _2020 B_Review Jurnal 2 OlahragaIka Puji Rahayu _2020 B_Review Jurnal 2 Olahraga
Ika Puji Rahayu _2020 B_Review Jurnal 2 Olahraga
IKAPUJIRAHAYU
 
Arum suryaningsih k 062 2020_b_jurnal4
Arum suryaningsih k 062 2020_b_jurnal4Arum suryaningsih k 062 2020_b_jurnal4
Arum suryaningsih k 062 2020_b_jurnal4
ArumKusmawati
 
Ethics in Youth Sport and Exercise Training Assignment Instr.docx
Ethics in Youth Sport and Exercise Training Assignment Instr.docxEthics in Youth Sport and Exercise Training Assignment Instr.docx
Ethics in Youth Sport and Exercise Training Assignment Instr.docx
elbanglis
 
Research Question
Research QuestionResearch Question
Research Question
Theresa Singh
 
ORGANISATIONAL PSYCHOLOGY: SCIENTIFIC DISCIPLINE, MANAGERIAL TOOL OR NEITHER?...
ORGANISATIONAL PSYCHOLOGY: SCIENTIFIC DISCIPLINE, MANAGERIAL TOOL OR NEITHER?...ORGANISATIONAL PSYCHOLOGY: SCIENTIFIC DISCIPLINE, MANAGERIAL TOOL OR NEITHER?...
ORGANISATIONAL PSYCHOLOGY: SCIENTIFIC DISCIPLINE, MANAGERIAL TOOL OR NEITHER?...
IAEME Publication
 
The Psychology of Sport & Exercise
The Psychology of Sport & Exercise The Psychology of Sport & Exercise
The Psychology of Sport & Exercise
PsychFutures
 
Makalah Review Jurnal Internasional
Makalah Review Jurnal Internasional Makalah Review Jurnal Internasional
Makalah Review Jurnal Internasional
JuliaPrasanti
 
Review jurnal 5
Review jurnal 5Review jurnal 5
Review jurnal 5
AzrulAzwar3
 
Ika Puji Rahayu_2020 B_review jurnal 3 Olahraga
Ika Puji Rahayu_2020 B_review jurnal 3 OlahragaIka Puji Rahayu_2020 B_review jurnal 3 Olahraga
Ika Puji Rahayu_2020 B_review jurnal 3 Olahraga
IKAPUJIRAHAYU
 
A Systematic Review Of Professional Reasoning Literature In Occupational Therapy
A Systematic Review Of Professional Reasoning Literature In Occupational TherapyA Systematic Review Of Professional Reasoning Literature In Occupational Therapy
A Systematic Review Of Professional Reasoning Literature In Occupational Therapy
Deja Lewis
 
Running head SPORTS SCIENCE1SPORTS SCIENCE6.docx
Running head SPORTS SCIENCE1SPORTS SCIENCE6.docxRunning head SPORTS SCIENCE1SPORTS SCIENCE6.docx
Running head SPORTS SCIENCE1SPORTS SCIENCE6.docx
todd521
 
Journal article summary.pdf
Journal article summary.pdfJournal article summary.pdf
Journal article summary.pdf
sdfghj21
 

Similar to Tiffanny tessantya utami 039 2020_b_review jurnal 2 (20)

Rindang muhammad husain 2020 b_review jurnal 3
Rindang muhammad husain 2020 b_review jurnal 3Rindang muhammad husain 2020 b_review jurnal 3
Rindang muhammad husain 2020 b_review jurnal 3
 
Rindang muhammad husain 2020 b_review jurnal 1
Rindang muhammad husain 2020 b_review jurnal 1Rindang muhammad husain 2020 b_review jurnal 1
Rindang muhammad husain 2020 b_review jurnal 1
 
Lailatul rohmah 2020 b_review jurnal 3
Lailatul rohmah 2020 b_review jurnal 3Lailatul rohmah 2020 b_review jurnal 3
Lailatul rohmah 2020 b_review jurnal 3
 
Rindang muhammad husain 2020 b_review jurnal 2
Rindang muhammad husain 2020 b_review jurnal 2Rindang muhammad husain 2020 b_review jurnal 2
Rindang muhammad husain 2020 b_review jurnal 2
 
Rindang muhammad husain 2020 b_review jurnal 4
Rindang muhammad husain 2020 b_review jurnal 4Rindang muhammad husain 2020 b_review jurnal 4
Rindang muhammad husain 2020 b_review jurnal 4
 
Ethical issues in sports1.  Post three peer-reviewed articles on.docx
Ethical issues in sports1.  Post three peer-reviewed articles on.docxEthical issues in sports1.  Post three peer-reviewed articles on.docx
Ethical issues in sports1.  Post three peer-reviewed articles on.docx
 
Lailatul rohmah 2020 b_review jurnal 2
Lailatul rohmah 2020 b_review jurnal 2Lailatul rohmah 2020 b_review jurnal 2
Lailatul rohmah 2020 b_review jurnal 2
 
Makalah Review Jurnal Internasional
Makalah Review Jurnal InternasionalMakalah Review Jurnal Internasional
Makalah Review Jurnal Internasional
 
Ika Puji Rahayu _2020 B_Review Jurnal 2 Olahraga
Ika Puji Rahayu _2020 B_Review Jurnal 2 OlahragaIka Puji Rahayu _2020 B_Review Jurnal 2 Olahraga
Ika Puji Rahayu _2020 B_Review Jurnal 2 Olahraga
 
Arum suryaningsih k 062 2020_b_jurnal4
Arum suryaningsih k 062 2020_b_jurnal4Arum suryaningsih k 062 2020_b_jurnal4
Arum suryaningsih k 062 2020_b_jurnal4
 
Ethics in Youth Sport and Exercise Training Assignment Instr.docx
Ethics in Youth Sport and Exercise Training Assignment Instr.docxEthics in Youth Sport and Exercise Training Assignment Instr.docx
Ethics in Youth Sport and Exercise Training Assignment Instr.docx
 
Research Question
Research QuestionResearch Question
Research Question
 
ORGANISATIONAL PSYCHOLOGY: SCIENTIFIC DISCIPLINE, MANAGERIAL TOOL OR NEITHER?...
ORGANISATIONAL PSYCHOLOGY: SCIENTIFIC DISCIPLINE, MANAGERIAL TOOL OR NEITHER?...ORGANISATIONAL PSYCHOLOGY: SCIENTIFIC DISCIPLINE, MANAGERIAL TOOL OR NEITHER?...
ORGANISATIONAL PSYCHOLOGY: SCIENTIFIC DISCIPLINE, MANAGERIAL TOOL OR NEITHER?...
 
The Psychology of Sport & Exercise
The Psychology of Sport & Exercise The Psychology of Sport & Exercise
The Psychology of Sport & Exercise
 
Makalah Review Jurnal Internasional
Makalah Review Jurnal Internasional Makalah Review Jurnal Internasional
Makalah Review Jurnal Internasional
 
Review jurnal 5
Review jurnal 5Review jurnal 5
Review jurnal 5
 
Ika Puji Rahayu_2020 B_review jurnal 3 Olahraga
Ika Puji Rahayu_2020 B_review jurnal 3 OlahragaIka Puji Rahayu_2020 B_review jurnal 3 Olahraga
Ika Puji Rahayu_2020 B_review jurnal 3 Olahraga
 
A Systematic Review Of Professional Reasoning Literature In Occupational Therapy
A Systematic Review Of Professional Reasoning Literature In Occupational TherapyA Systematic Review Of Professional Reasoning Literature In Occupational Therapy
A Systematic Review Of Professional Reasoning Literature In Occupational Therapy
 
Running head SPORTS SCIENCE1SPORTS SCIENCE6.docx
Running head SPORTS SCIENCE1SPORTS SCIENCE6.docxRunning head SPORTS SCIENCE1SPORTS SCIENCE6.docx
Running head SPORTS SCIENCE1SPORTS SCIENCE6.docx
 
Journal article summary.pdf
Journal article summary.pdfJournal article summary.pdf
Journal article summary.pdf
 

Recently uploaded

Deep Software Variability and Frictionless Reproducibility
Deep Software Variability and Frictionless ReproducibilityDeep Software Variability and Frictionless Reproducibility
Deep Software Variability and Frictionless Reproducibility
University of Rennes, INSA Rennes, Inria/IRISA, CNRS
 
Eukaryotic Transcription Presentation.pptx
Eukaryotic Transcription Presentation.pptxEukaryotic Transcription Presentation.pptx
Eukaryotic Transcription Presentation.pptx
RitabrataSarkar3
 
Describing and Interpreting an Immersive Learning Case with the Immersion Cub...
Describing and Interpreting an Immersive Learning Case with the Immersion Cub...Describing and Interpreting an Immersive Learning Case with the Immersion Cub...
Describing and Interpreting an Immersive Learning Case with the Immersion Cub...
Leonel Morgado
 
20240520 Planning a Circuit Simulator in JavaScript.pptx
20240520 Planning a Circuit Simulator in JavaScript.pptx20240520 Planning a Circuit Simulator in JavaScript.pptx
20240520 Planning a Circuit Simulator in JavaScript.pptx
Sharon Liu
 
Applied Science: Thermodynamics, Laws & Methodology.pdf
Applied Science: Thermodynamics, Laws & Methodology.pdfApplied Science: Thermodynamics, Laws & Methodology.pdf
Applied Science: Thermodynamics, Laws & Methodology.pdf
University of Hertfordshire
 
bordetella pertussis.................................ppt
bordetella pertussis.................................pptbordetella pertussis.................................ppt
bordetella pertussis.................................ppt
kejapriya1
 
8.Isolation of pure cultures and preservation of cultures.pdf
8.Isolation of pure cultures and preservation of cultures.pdf8.Isolation of pure cultures and preservation of cultures.pdf
8.Isolation of pure cultures and preservation of cultures.pdf
by6843629
 
Medical Orthopedic PowerPoint Templates.pptx
Medical Orthopedic PowerPoint Templates.pptxMedical Orthopedic PowerPoint Templates.pptx
Medical Orthopedic PowerPoint Templates.pptx
terusbelajar5
 
The binding of cosmological structures by massless topological defects
The binding of cosmological structures by massless topological defectsThe binding of cosmological structures by massless topological defects
The binding of cosmological structures by massless topological defects
Sérgio Sacani
 
Cytokines and their role in immune regulation.pptx
Cytokines and their role in immune regulation.pptxCytokines and their role in immune regulation.pptx
Cytokines and their role in immune regulation.pptx
Hitesh Sikarwar
 
The debris of the ‘last major merger’ is dynamically young
The debris of the ‘last major merger’ is dynamically youngThe debris of the ‘last major merger’ is dynamically young
The debris of the ‘last major merger’ is dynamically young
Sérgio Sacani
 
Shallowest Oil Discovery of Turkiye.pptx
Shallowest Oil Discovery of Turkiye.pptxShallowest Oil Discovery of Turkiye.pptx
Shallowest Oil Discovery of Turkiye.pptx
Gokturk Mehmet Dilci
 
SAR of Medicinal Chemistry 1st by dk.pdf
SAR of Medicinal Chemistry 1st by dk.pdfSAR of Medicinal Chemistry 1st by dk.pdf
SAR of Medicinal Chemistry 1st by dk.pdf
KrushnaDarade1
 
EWOCS-I: The catalog of X-ray sources in Westerlund 1 from the Extended Weste...
EWOCS-I: The catalog of X-ray sources in Westerlund 1 from the Extended Weste...EWOCS-I: The catalog of X-ray sources in Westerlund 1 from the Extended Weste...
EWOCS-I: The catalog of X-ray sources in Westerlund 1 from the Extended Weste...
Sérgio Sacani
 
ESR spectroscopy in liquid food and beverages.pptx
ESR spectroscopy in liquid food and beverages.pptxESR spectroscopy in liquid food and beverages.pptx
ESR spectroscopy in liquid food and beverages.pptx
PRIYANKA PATEL
 
Basics of crystallography, crystal systems, classes and different forms
Basics of crystallography, crystal systems, classes and different formsBasics of crystallography, crystal systems, classes and different forms
Basics of crystallography, crystal systems, classes and different forms
MaheshaNanjegowda
 
THEMATIC APPERCEPTION TEST(TAT) cognitive abilities, creativity, and critic...
THEMATIC  APPERCEPTION  TEST(TAT) cognitive abilities, creativity, and critic...THEMATIC  APPERCEPTION  TEST(TAT) cognitive abilities, creativity, and critic...
THEMATIC APPERCEPTION TEST(TAT) cognitive abilities, creativity, and critic...
Abdul Wali Khan University Mardan,kP,Pakistan
 
Travis Hills' Endeavors in Minnesota: Fostering Environmental and Economic Pr...
Travis Hills' Endeavors in Minnesota: Fostering Environmental and Economic Pr...Travis Hills' Endeavors in Minnesota: Fostering Environmental and Economic Pr...
Travis Hills' Endeavors in Minnesota: Fostering Environmental and Economic Pr...
Travis Hills MN
 
Sharlene Leurig - Enabling Onsite Water Use with Net Zero Water
Sharlene Leurig - Enabling Onsite Water Use with Net Zero WaterSharlene Leurig - Enabling Onsite Water Use with Net Zero Water
Sharlene Leurig - Enabling Onsite Water Use with Net Zero Water
Texas Alliance of Groundwater Districts
 
molar-distalization in orthodontics-seminar.pptx
molar-distalization in orthodontics-seminar.pptxmolar-distalization in orthodontics-seminar.pptx
molar-distalization in orthodontics-seminar.pptx
Anagha Prasad
 

Recently uploaded (20)

Deep Software Variability and Frictionless Reproducibility
Deep Software Variability and Frictionless ReproducibilityDeep Software Variability and Frictionless Reproducibility
Deep Software Variability and Frictionless Reproducibility
 
Eukaryotic Transcription Presentation.pptx
Eukaryotic Transcription Presentation.pptxEukaryotic Transcription Presentation.pptx
Eukaryotic Transcription Presentation.pptx
 
Describing and Interpreting an Immersive Learning Case with the Immersion Cub...
Describing and Interpreting an Immersive Learning Case with the Immersion Cub...Describing and Interpreting an Immersive Learning Case with the Immersion Cub...
Describing and Interpreting an Immersive Learning Case with the Immersion Cub...
 
20240520 Planning a Circuit Simulator in JavaScript.pptx
20240520 Planning a Circuit Simulator in JavaScript.pptx20240520 Planning a Circuit Simulator in JavaScript.pptx
20240520 Planning a Circuit Simulator in JavaScript.pptx
 
Applied Science: Thermodynamics, Laws & Methodology.pdf
Applied Science: Thermodynamics, Laws & Methodology.pdfApplied Science: Thermodynamics, Laws & Methodology.pdf
Applied Science: Thermodynamics, Laws & Methodology.pdf
 
bordetella pertussis.................................ppt
bordetella pertussis.................................pptbordetella pertussis.................................ppt
bordetella pertussis.................................ppt
 
8.Isolation of pure cultures and preservation of cultures.pdf
8.Isolation of pure cultures and preservation of cultures.pdf8.Isolation of pure cultures and preservation of cultures.pdf
8.Isolation of pure cultures and preservation of cultures.pdf
 
Medical Orthopedic PowerPoint Templates.pptx
Medical Orthopedic PowerPoint Templates.pptxMedical Orthopedic PowerPoint Templates.pptx
Medical Orthopedic PowerPoint Templates.pptx
 
The binding of cosmological structures by massless topological defects
The binding of cosmological structures by massless topological defectsThe binding of cosmological structures by massless topological defects
The binding of cosmological structures by massless topological defects
 
Cytokines and their role in immune regulation.pptx
Cytokines and their role in immune regulation.pptxCytokines and their role in immune regulation.pptx
Cytokines and their role in immune regulation.pptx
 
The debris of the ‘last major merger’ is dynamically young
The debris of the ‘last major merger’ is dynamically youngThe debris of the ‘last major merger’ is dynamically young
The debris of the ‘last major merger’ is dynamically young
 
Shallowest Oil Discovery of Turkiye.pptx
Shallowest Oil Discovery of Turkiye.pptxShallowest Oil Discovery of Turkiye.pptx
Shallowest Oil Discovery of Turkiye.pptx
 
SAR of Medicinal Chemistry 1st by dk.pdf
SAR of Medicinal Chemistry 1st by dk.pdfSAR of Medicinal Chemistry 1st by dk.pdf
SAR of Medicinal Chemistry 1st by dk.pdf
 
EWOCS-I: The catalog of X-ray sources in Westerlund 1 from the Extended Weste...
EWOCS-I: The catalog of X-ray sources in Westerlund 1 from the Extended Weste...EWOCS-I: The catalog of X-ray sources in Westerlund 1 from the Extended Weste...
EWOCS-I: The catalog of X-ray sources in Westerlund 1 from the Extended Weste...
 
ESR spectroscopy in liquid food and beverages.pptx
ESR spectroscopy in liquid food and beverages.pptxESR spectroscopy in liquid food and beverages.pptx
ESR spectroscopy in liquid food and beverages.pptx
 
Basics of crystallography, crystal systems, classes and different forms
Basics of crystallography, crystal systems, classes and different formsBasics of crystallography, crystal systems, classes and different forms
Basics of crystallography, crystal systems, classes and different forms
 
THEMATIC APPERCEPTION TEST(TAT) cognitive abilities, creativity, and critic...
THEMATIC  APPERCEPTION  TEST(TAT) cognitive abilities, creativity, and critic...THEMATIC  APPERCEPTION  TEST(TAT) cognitive abilities, creativity, and critic...
THEMATIC APPERCEPTION TEST(TAT) cognitive abilities, creativity, and critic...
 
Travis Hills' Endeavors in Minnesota: Fostering Environmental and Economic Pr...
Travis Hills' Endeavors in Minnesota: Fostering Environmental and Economic Pr...Travis Hills' Endeavors in Minnesota: Fostering Environmental and Economic Pr...
Travis Hills' Endeavors in Minnesota: Fostering Environmental and Economic Pr...
 
Sharlene Leurig - Enabling Onsite Water Use with Net Zero Water
Sharlene Leurig - Enabling Onsite Water Use with Net Zero WaterSharlene Leurig - Enabling Onsite Water Use with Net Zero Water
Sharlene Leurig - Enabling Onsite Water Use with Net Zero Water
 
molar-distalization in orthodontics-seminar.pptx
molar-distalization in orthodontics-seminar.pptxmolar-distalization in orthodontics-seminar.pptx
molar-distalization in orthodontics-seminar.pptx
 

Tiffanny tessantya utami 039 2020_b_review jurnal 2

  • 1. MAKALAH REVIEW A PRAGMATIC RESEARCH PHILOSOPHY FOR APPLIED SPORT PSYCHOLOGY Dosen Pengampu : Dr. Made Pramono, S.S. M.Hum Disusun oleh : Tiffanny Tessantya Utami 20060484039 2020B UNIVERSITAS NEGERI SURABAYA FAKULTAS ILMU OLAHRAGA JURUSAN PENDIDIKAN KESEHATAN DAN REKREASI TAHUN 2021
  • 2. i KATA PENGANTAR Puji syukur kehadirat Tuhan Yang MahaKuasa karena telah memberikan kesempatan pada penulis untuk menyelesaikan makalah ini. Atas rahmat dan hidayah-nya lah penulis dapat menyelesaikan makalah yang berjudul A Pragmatic Research Philosophy for Applied Sport Psychology tepat waktu. Makalah berjudul A Pragmatic Research Philosophy for Applied Sport Psychology disusun guna memenuhi tugas dosen Dr. Made Pramono, S.S. M.Hum pada mata kuliah Filsafat dan Sejarah Olahraga di Universitas Negeri Surabaya. Selain itu, penulis juga berharap agar makalah ini dapat menambah wawasan bagi pembaca tentang revie jurnal berjudul A Pragmatic Research Philosophy for Applied Sport Psychology. Penulis mengucapkan terimakasih sebesar-besarnya kepada Dr. Made Pramono, S. S. M. Hum selaku dosen mata kuliah Filsafat dan Sejarah Olahraga. Tugas yang telah diberikan ini dapat menambah wawasan dan pengetahuan terkait bidang yang ditekuni penulis. Penulis juga mengucapkan terimakasih kepada semua pihak yang telah membantu proses penyusunan makalah ini. Penulis menyadari bahwa makalah ini masih jauh dari kata sempurna. Oleh karena itu, penulis membutuhkan kritik dan saran yang membangun akan penulis terima demi kesempurnaan makalah ini. Gresik, 16 Maret 2021 Penulis
  • 3. ii DAFTAR ISI Kata Pengantar......................................................................................................................i Daftar Isi...............................................................................................................................ii BAB I Jurnal.........................................................................................................................1 BAB II Review Jurnal...........................................................................................................34 BAB III Kesimpulan dan Saran...........................................................................................36 LINK SLIDE SHARE..........................................................................................................37 Daftar Pustaka......................................................................................................................38
  • 4.
  • 5. 1 BAB I JURNAL The College at Brockport: State University of New York Digital Commons @Brockport Kinesiology, Sport Studies and Physical Education Faculty Publications Kinesiology, Sport Studies and Physical Education 3-2005 APragmatic Research Philosophy for Applied Sport Psychology Peter R. Giacobbi Jr. University of Florida Artur Poczwardowski Barry University Peter F. Hager The College at Brockport, phager@brockport.edu
  • 6. 2 Follow this and additional works at: https://digitalcommons.brockport.edu/pes_facpub Part of the Kinesiology Commons Repository Citation Giacobbi, Peter R. Jr.; Poczwardowski, Artur; and Hager, Peter F., "A Pragmatic Research Philosophy for Applied Sport Psychology" (2005). Kinesiology, Sport Studies and Physical Education Faculty Publications. 80. https://digitalcommons.brockport.edu/pes_facpub/80 Citation/Publisher Attribution: Sport Psychologist Mar2005, Vol. 19 Issue 1, p18-31 This Article is brought to you for free and open access by the Kinesiology, Sport Studies and Physical Education at Digital Commons @Brockport. It has been accepted for inclusion in Kinesiology, Sport Studies and Physical Education Faculty Publications by an authorized administrator of Digital Commons @Brockport. For more information, please contact kmyers@brockport.edu.
  • 7. 3 The Sport Psychologist, 2005, 19, 18-31 © 2005 Human Kinetics Publishers, Inc. A Pragmatic Research Philosophy for Applied Sport Psychology Peter R. Giacobbi, Jr. University of Florida Artur Poczwardowski Barry University Peter Hager State University of New York, College at Brockport A pragmatic research philosophy is introduced that embraces mixed-method approaches to applied research questions. With its origins in the work of Peirce (1984), James (1907), Dewey (1931), and contemporary support from Rorty (1982,1990,1991),pragmatism emphasizes the practical problems experienced by people, the research questions posited, and the consequences of inquiry. As a way to highlight applications of pragmatism in sport psychology, pragmatism is compared to constructivism and positivism in terms of philosophical underpinnings and methodological applications. The pragmatic researcher is sensitive to the social, historical, and political context from which inquiry begins and considers morality, ethics, and issues of social justice to be important throughout the research process. Pragmatists often use
  • 8. 4 pluralistic methods during multiphase research projects. Exemplar design types are discussed that logically cohere to a pragmatic research philosophy. Debate and discussion concerned with knowledge construction have occurred throughout the history of science (Wilson, 1998). In the sport and exercise psychology literature, discussions have focused on divisions between Peter Giacobbi, Jr. is with the Department of Applied Physiology and Kinesiology at the University of Florida, Gainesville, FL 32611-8205. E-mail: Pgiacobbi@hhp.ufl.edu. Artur Poczwardowski is with the Department of Sport and Exercise Sciences at Barry University in Miami Shores, FL 33161; Peter Hager is with the Department of Physical Education and Sport at the State University of New York, College at Brockport. E-mail: phager@Brockport.edu. 18
  • 9. 5 the academic discipline and professional activities of applied practitioners, the appropriate sources of knowledge (e.g., interviews, observations, experiments), data analytic strategies (e.g., quantitative and/or qualitative), the underlying philosophies of various research methodologies, and the development ofalternative paradigms (Brustad, 2002; Dewar & Horn, 1992; Dzewaltowski, 1997; Hardy, Jones, & Gould, 1996; Krane, 1994; Krane, Andersen, & Strean, 1997; Martens, 1979, 1987). While these discussions have influenced the field of sport psychology, there is still a need to develop and accept alternative ways of examining human behavior within sport and physical activity contexts (Brustad, 2002; Dewar & Horn, 1992). Hence, this paper will characterize a pragmatic viewpoint about the nature of paradigmatic controversies and present an alternative position that is guided by the practical concerns of applied sport psychologists and their clients (i.e., coaches and athletes). Pragmatism, with origins in the work of Peirce (1984) and James (1907), is a philosophy ofknowledge construction that emphasizes practical solutions toapplied research questions and the consequences of inquiry. The purpose of this paper is to offer researchers and practitioners in sport psychology integrated approaches to knowledge construction that logically cohere to a pragmatic research philosophy. The primary motivation for the present discussion stems from the longstanding gap between academic and applied sport psychology practice. Martens (1979) was perhaps the first to express concern about ecological validity of laboratory-based sport psychology research. He noted the gap between field-based observations and orthodox approaches to knowledge construction when he claimed, “experiential knowledge and common sense have been more appealing, and usually more beneficial, than knowledge from sport psychology research” (p. 95). Martens (1979, 1987) recognized that reliance on orthodox scientific methods and a failure to pay attention to the unique sport and physical activity context lead to two divergent aspects of sport psychology, academic and applied. His solution was a heuristic approach that emphasized knowledge gained through field studies, idiographic, and introspective methods. Brustad (2002) and others (Biddle, Markland, Gilbourne, Chatzisarantis, & Sparkes,2001;Dewar & Horn, 1992)have called for the use of multiple methods and acceptance of alternative paradigms in the research process. Brustad (2002) noted “it is possible to use multi-method approaches to address any research question” (p. 34) while Hardy et al. (1996) offered the following: “At times it is best
  • 10. 6 to use a qualitative methods, and at other times a quantitative approach. Because both methods have strengths and limitations, sometimes it may also be advisable to combine the two approaches” (p. 259). Consistent with these sentiments, we feel it is important to continue paradigmatic and epistemological discussion to advance the use of mixed-method forms of knowledge construction in sport psychology. Another complimentary rationale for the present manuscript stems from findings reported by Culver, Gilbert, and Trudel (2003). They reviewed 485 published studies in three major sport psychology journals (e.g., The Sport Psychologist, The Journal of Applied Sport Psychology, and the Journal of Sport and Exercise Psychology) to determine the number of studies that used qualitative (e.g., interviews, life histories, participant observation) versus quantitative methods (e.g., emphasis on numerical data and/or inferential statistics). Of particular interest here was that only 32 studies over a period of ten years used a combination of qualitative and quantitative techniques. This finding raises some interesting questions similar to those raised by Culver et al. (2003). First, are we to presume that many of the
  • 11. 7 important research questions in sport psychology can only be answered by one technique alone? And second, is it possible that multiple methods of data collection and analysis, used simultaneously, could answer important research questions more adequately? We argue the answer to question two is aresounding yes. In an attempt to back up this claim, we present an alternative philosophical and methodological paradigm for researchers and practitioners in applied sport psychology: pragmatism. What follows is a review of the historical roots of pragmatism. This review will provide the philosophical underpinnings of a pragmatic paradigm of knowledge construction for applied researchers in sport psychology. Then, specific design strategies and examples of published studies will be discussed followed by a review of pragmatic conceptions of validity in scientific inquiry. Before giving a brief account of pragmatism, two caveats are in order. First, there are different forms of pragmatism and the reader is encouraged to see Murphy (1990), Diggins (1994), and Putnam (1995) to better understand the diverse representations of pragmatism. Our intent here is to present one form of pragmatism with the specific goal of offering applied researchers in sport psychology a philosophical and methodological framework for future research. Second, this work is intended to add to responsible and productive discussion surrounding the underlying assumptions of the research process (i.e., methodology) and the implications of adopting one paradigmatic approach over another. The arguments presented in this manuscript are consistent with the views of scholars who have encouraged acceptance and tolerance of diverse research epistemologies and methodologies in sport and exercise psychology (Biddle et al. 2001; Brustad, 2002; Culver et al., 2003; Hardy et al., 1996; Krane, 1994; Krane et al., 1997; Strean & Roberts, 1992; Whaley, 2001). An Introduction to Pragmatism The word pragmatism comes from the Greek word, π which means action from which the English words “practice” and “practical” were derived. The term was popularized by William James in his publication Pragmatism: A New Name for Some Old Waysof Thinking.For James, pragmatism was not aprogram for what he called “solving names” such as God or answering questions about an absolute truth. Rather, pragmatism was an attempt to provide practical solutions to contemporary problems experienced by people and society. In this light, pragmatic thought was a “method only” intended to change existing realities through concerted effort and
  • 12. 8 continued work (James, 1907, p. 51). Especially important to James (1907) were the “respective practical consequences” of work, a concern that has implications for the present manuscript (p. 45). In more recent years, Rorty (1982, 1990, 1991) has emerged as a proponent of pragmatism. Rorty argued that the concept of knowledge as an accurate representation of truth needed to be discarded. From this perspective, the methods and theories of empirical science or any other discipline (e.g., theology) are not capable of describing truth once and for all. Pragmatists deny there is single reality and see no way for scientists or others to determine whether their theories are closer to the truth than are their colleagues. For these reasons, pragmatists have abandoned discussions that concern the correspondence of theory and reality in favor of dialogues where the value of different types of knowledge are viewed as tools for helping us cope with and thrive within our environment (Rorty, 1990).
  • 13. 9 Put simply, pragmatists opt for methods and theories that are more useful to us within specific contexts (e.g., answers to practical problems), not those that reveal underlying truths about the nature of reality. As Rorty noted, pragmatists are set apart from positivists and/or scientific realists who believe there is an objective truth or “God’s-eye view” of the world unobstructed by the influence of socio- cultural conditions and subjective biases (Rorty, 1990, p. 2). Pragmatists view this purported truth as being “irrelevant to our needs and our practices” (Rorty, 1990, p. 2) because such a view would not account for the peculiarities of our world (Williams, 1985). Pragmatists further recognize that scientific inquiry is contextual in nature and that the past and current social, historica l, and political conditions strongly influence the scientific process. Dewey (1931) noted “They [general ideas] are the basis for organizing future observations and experience” (pp. 32-33) while Rorty (1982) claimed that all inquiry begins with and is guided by the previous discourse that researchers inherit from their predecessors. In summary, pragmatists evaluate research findings based upon their practical, social, and moral consequences as well as their bearing on the human condition. They consider the problems under study and the specific research questions as more important than the underlying philosophical assumptions of the method. Pragmatists typically use one or more methods deemed appropriate to the specific research question being asked while simultaneously considering the consequences of such inquiry (Cherryholmes, 1992; Howe, 1988; Rorty, 1982). Pragmatism and the Major Paradigms In order to demonstrate the strengths and flexibility afforded by the pragmatic research tradition, a more specific discussion about philosophical issues in research is warranted. Rather than abandoning discussions of this kind, as Rorty (1991) suggested, we have chosen to take part in them in order to facilitate dialogue with proponents of other research paradigms. As a way to highlight the philosophical underpinnings of pragmatic thought, the discussion that follows will review important terms related to the research process. Also included in this discussion are examples of how two relatively dichotomous paradigmatic philosophies, positivism and constructivism, are unique from one another. Because of space limitations, discussions about the broad array of post-positivistic and non-positivistic viewpoints (e.g., cultural studies, feminist,
  • 14. 10 critical theory) are beyond the scope of this manuscript; Lincoln and Guba (2000) provided an excellent review of these philosophies. Rather than present all of these viewpoints, our intent is to contrast the major philosophical assumptions of positivism and constructivism in order to shed light on a pragmatic philosophy. Epistemology refers to theories of knowledge, the nature and source of legitimate knowledge, and the ability of research subjects/participants to possess knowledge (Childers & Hentzi, 1995). Ontology generally refers to questions about the nature of reality, that which is real, and whether an “objective” reality exists “independent of the researcher” (Creswell, 1994, p. 4). Positivists adopt objective epistemological and ontological views that assume the existence of a real or true reality that can be measured and understood with the application of scientific methods devoid of any human contamination or subjective bias. The positivist also believes that scientific findings can be generalized across time and
  • 15. 11 in different contexts and their findings represent so called truths about the world devoid of the social, cultural, or historical context from which these findings were made (Lincoln & Guba, 2000). In contrast, field based researchers and constructivists often embrace a subjective view of knowledge that is individualized and context specific (Lincoln & Guba, 2000). Generally speaking, a constructivistic philosophy focuses on the social, historical, and value driven process of knowledge claims and denies the existence of permanent (foundational) objective truths about reality. Thus for the constructivist, the epistemological and ontological positions embrace subjective views about reality constructed through transactions within the socially situated activities of people and communities (Lincoln & Guba, 2000). The epistemological and ontological extremes represented by positivists and constructivists have lead to contentious debates within the social and behavioral sciences including sport psychology (Brustad 2002; Dewar & Horn, 1992; Dzewaltowski 1997; Lincoln & Guba, 2000; Martens 1979, 1987). While positivists and constructivists appear to have dichotomous epistemological views (Lincoln & Guba, 2000), the pragmatist argues that a continuum exists between objective and subjective viewpoints the choice of which depends on the nature of the research question being asked and the particular point in the research process (Creswell, 2003; Tashakkori & Teddlie, 1998, 2003). Because knowledge construction is contextual in nature and influenced by the cultural, political, and historical conditions of the day, the pragmatist equates objectivity with solidarity and agreement that occurs through discourse and transaction within and between communities of people (James, 1907). In short, pragmatists test the veracity of facts through dialogue, the usefulness and consequences of knowledge, and negotiations within communities, all of which require ongoing dialog. Both pragmatists and constructivists agree that the consequences of scientific inquiry require reflection and analysis. Both approaches consider the practical, moral, and ethical consequences of knowledge construction as important considerations in research (Dewey, 1931; James, 1907). Like constructivists, pragmatists would encourage scholars and practitioners to engage in moral and ethical discussions before, during, and after the implementation of scientific findings (Dewey, 1931; Whaley, 2001). However, unlike more radical forms of constructivism (Lincoln & Guba, 2000 review the varying forms of constructivism), pragmatists consider agreement within a scientific community as a way to approach objectivity. For the
  • 16. 12 pragmatist, total agreement, complete objectivity, and the search for some ultimate truth are not considered useful endeavors (James, 1907). Rather, agreement within communities may allow a practical level of truth to exist, but such practical truths are those findings that prove useful within specific contexts (James, 1907). In other words, the pragmatist prefers to avoid debate about whether constructivitic or positivistic conceptions of truth are more accurate. The pragmatist considers the practical concerns with human existence, the research questions being asked, and the consequences of inquiry, to be more important than which version of the truth is better than another (James, 1907). As James (1907) asked “What difference would it practically make to any one if this notion rather than that notion were true? If no practical difference whatever can be traced, then the alternatives mean practically the same thing” (p. 45). With continued work and an examination of consequences, knowledge claims may be supported by the weight of evidence available and the logical arguments used to apply those claims. For instance,
  • 17. 13 sport psychology researchers and/or practitioners may test psychological interventions to improve sport performance. If agreement within a community were established that a specific intervention improved sport performance, then pragmatists would consider such agreement about research findings to approach an objective position. Another epistemological debate concerns the issue of time and context free generalizations. The pragmatist argues that scientists must be aware of the context within which research applications take place. Because time and context free generalizations tend todiscount or eliminate the role of social-historical-contextual factors on human experience (Lincoln & Guba, 2000), a pragmatist would not be inclined to make such claims. Science, knowledge construction, and the practical concerns of people require ongoing deliberations about the use and benefits of knowledge at a particular time or place. Again, the iterative nature of knowledge construction requires scientists to continuously reexamine the application of research findings in many different contexts. This does not necessarily mean that scientific findings must be replicated continuously. Rather, the application and consequences of science must be reflected upon to ensure practical utility, social value, and fairness to anyone who might be or could be impacted by research findings. Research Methods Research methods are procedures, tools, or strategies of doing research while methodology refers to larger issues in the research process such as why particular methods are chosen (Smith, 1989).In other words, methods are equated with specific design elements and strategies ofinquiry such as how torecruit participants, the type of data collected (i.e., numeric versus interview based); methodology is more closely linked to philosophical issues within the research process. For example, positivists believe only objective, tightly controlled, directly observable empirical data provide legitimate knowledge; therefore, experimental designs within contrived settings and quantitative representations of data are typically chosen. A positivist might approach the study of psychological stress by creating a contrived laboratory task that mimics a real world competition scenario. This approach may yield meaningful data about particular research questions, but the context with which stress and competition occur in actual athletic settings would not be readily apparent from the data. In contrast, constructivists disavow objective claims about reality and embrace time and context dependent explanations about knowledge that are explored through
  • 18. 14 life histories, interviews, or participant observation. Elaborating on the example above, a constructivist would interview an athlete immediately following several athletic contests to understand an athlete’s perceptions of competition stress. While mixing methods from different paradigms is possible, desirable, and often productive, the underlying assumptions of various paradigms (i.e., constructivists versus positivism) may contradict one another (Krane & Baird, in press; Lincoln & Guba, 2000). In other words, a constructivist may use quantitative data but will adopt a subjective epistemology, while a positivist who uses a post-experiment interview will do so under an objective epistemology. Pragmatists are trained in, and “use pluralistic approaches to derive knowledge about the problem” under study (Creswell, 2003, p. 12). Because of the epistemological continuum noted above, the pragmatist will often use multiple and/or mixed method designs within single investigations and in an iterative
  • 19. 15 programmatic manner over several investigations. What follows is a review of some mixed-method design strategies that cohere to a pragmatic philosophy. Mixed-Method Research Mixed-method research involves a combination of procedures where two or more data collection techniques and forms of analyses are used and both contribute to the final results (Tashakkori & Teddlie, 1998). As discussed by Creswell (2003) and Tashakkori and Teddlie (1998), mixed-method research involves both data collection techniques and analyses because the data collected dictates the analyses performed. In the following sections, we will review and adapt the mixed-method designs described by Tashakkori and Teddlie (1998) to the work of applied researchers in sport and exercise psychology. Specifically, we will discuss equivalent status designs, dominant-less-dominant designs, and designs with multilevel uses. Within this discussion, examples of published research studies that resemble the design types being reviewed here will be highlighted. Equivalent Status Designs Equivalent status designs can involve both qualitative and quantitative approaches in either temporal order. Consistent with a pragmatic epistemology, researchers and/or practitioners who are trained in multiple methods often favor the equivalent status design. With this design, both qualitative and quantitative approaches contribute equally to the final results, the results of phase one somehow inform phase two, and neither approach is viewed asmore important than the other (Tashakkori & Teddlie, 1998). For instance, Gould, Udrey, Tuffey, and Loehr (1996a) and Gould Tuffey, Udrey, and Loehr (1996b), in two stages, assessed the levels of burnout experienced by competitive junior tennis players. In the first stage, a battery of measures was administered to 62 junior tennis players and discriminant function analysis helped identify the defining psychological characteristics of burned out players. Then, 10 participants who scored highest on measures of burnout and perfectionism were interviewed to shed further light on the psychological characteristics of burned out junior tennis players. Although the two studies independently contributed to our knowledge of burned out junior tennis players, the value of this two- stage approach was made evident by the authors’ conclusions regarding the salience placed on social psychological factors by the
  • 20. 16 10 interview participants, and the support offered to Smith’s (1986) cognitive-affective stress model of burnout. In this case, both stages of data collection provided useful information to help scientists and practitioners identify the characteristics and experiences of burned out junior tennis players. Alternatively, Strean (1998) discussed another potential application of qualitative methods as researchers can use interview data as a basis for designing survey measures. This approach is consistent with an equivalent status design as long as both studies contribute independently to new knowledge and study one somehow informs study two. For instance, Conroy, Poczwardowski, and Henschen, (2001) used interviews to enhance understanding of individuals’ emotional responses to failure in stage one. Their interviews, along with extensive theorizing about the fear of failure construct, formed the theoretical rationale toward the development of items for the Fear Appraisal Inventory designed and psychometrically tested during stage two (Conroy, Willow, & Metzler, 2002). The findings from both studies provided researchers and practitioners a practical means to assess fear of
  • 21. 17 failure within the context of sport, and both studies independently contributed new knowledge to the literature. Equivalent status designs could also involve both qualitative and quantitative data collection techniques used in a parallel or simultaneous manner. As with the study designs described above, this form of data collection involves dual and equal contributions to the final results by both qualitative and quantitative forms of data collection and analysis. Chase, Lirgg, and Feltz (1997) presented an excellent example of an equivalent status design. They simultaneously tested aspects of Bandura’s (1977, 1986) self- efficacy theory while inductively building a new theory concerned with the information used by coaches to form efficacy judgments. Specifically, Bandura’s previous theoretical writings influenced the authors’ theoretical rationale, the study design, and creation of items to measure coaches’ efficacy. The authors also used an inductive open-ended approach and created a conceptual framework of important sources of efficacy information used by coaches. In this case, the conceptual framework developed from the inductive analysis of coaches’ open-ended responses were usefully combined with the quantitative analyses and both forms of data were used to support the authors’ conclusions that coaches’ efficacy expectations can influence the performance of athletes. Dominant-Less Dominant Design Another mixed-method approach involves the use of one dominant method while a relatively smaller part of the study uses an alternative method. The dominant-less dominant design may appeal to researchers or practitioners who strongly favor one research approach, but collaboration with researchers who have different training and/or epistemological viewpoints is needed. For instance, Chaffin and Imreh (2002) assessed the practice of a concert pianist as she learned Presto, of J. S. Bach’s Italian Concerto. In this study, the researchers were interested in the performance cues (e.g., features of music attended to during performance) identified by the learner for the Presto to determine whether there were specific places during the piece more difficult to remember than others. The main unit of analysis was the number of starting or stopping places identified through audio recordings during an extended learning period. Secondary data collection involved a post experiment interview concerned with when the learner was thinking about the identified performance cues during memory runs and a recorded performance. In this case, the researchers used the interview data in aless dominant manner as a manipulation check of their quantitative findings.
  • 22. 18 A second approach to the dominant-less dominant design would involve the simultaneous or parallel collection of both qualitative interview and quantitative survey data. For instance, Gould, Dieffenbach, and Moffett (2002) interviewed 32 Olympic champions, their coaches, and a parent or guardian to examine the psychological characteristics of the athletes. At the same time, the authors administered a battery of survey measures to compliment their interview data. Although the authors contended that qualitative interviews were the primary methods used, their results were presented and discussed in a complimentary manner thus fitting the description of a parallel-simultaneous dominant-less dominant design. The value of this design was that both forms of data collection, with a unique and difficult-to-attain participant population, allowed the researchers tobring multiple epistemological views and interpretations to the research process.
  • 23. 19 The simultaneous/parallel dominant-less dominant design type is ideally suited to the work of applied sport psychologists who might have unique opportunities to collect diverse forms of data with small numbers of athletes or significant others within the athletic context. Multilevel Approaches While the preceding sections of this manuscript have focused on design issues, recent developments in statistics offer potential new design formulations in applied sport psychology. Specifically, the advent of multilevel modeling and specifically hierarchical linear modeling (HLM: Raudenbush & Bryk, 2002) can help researchers address pervasive design challenges in the social and behavioral sciences. Researchers in sport psychology (Pasevich, Estabrooks, Brawley, & Carron, 2001) and other disciplines (Arnold, 1992; Goldstein, 1995; Morris & Normand, 1992; Rosenberg, 1973) have noted the challenges associated with hierarchical or nested structures within the social and behavioral sciences. Pasevich et al. (2001) claimed that athletic teams can be conceptualized as groups nested within larger organizational units, while factors at each group level may differentially impact performance. If repeated observations are collected with a group of athletes from different teams, athletes on the same team would likely exhibit more similarities to one another than athletes on different teams. The observation that individual, group, and organizational factors are unique entities leads to violations of statistical independence and any between group differences would be unaccounted error variance using traditional statistics (Raudenbush & Bryk, 2002). To analyze multilevel data of this nature, HLM analyses allow researchers to assess individual behavior while controlling for the unique variability associated with moderator variables such as group membership, level of athletic competition (Raudenbush & Bryk, 2002), and personality characteristics (David, Green, Martin, & Suls, 1997). While applications of HLM remain scant in sport and exercise psychology (see Giacobbi, Hausenblas, & Frye, 2005, for an exception), Pasevich et al. (2001) noted the vast potential of HLM to address many important research questions in sport and exercise psychology. Multilevel research can also be used to collect quantitative data on one level and qualitative data on another. For instance, a sport psychologist working or doing research with collegiate athletes could administer the Group Environment Questionnaire (Brawley, Carron, & Widmeyer, 1987) to athletes from various schools. At the same time, the researcher could interview coaches and athletic directors to
  • 24. 20 better understand how group level variables such as institutional, academic, and athletic priorities are related to interactions within teams. The coach/athletic director data could be used to make the athlete level data more meaningful and understandable, and vice versa. Multilevel approaches can be used simultaneously or sequentially. In summary, equivalent status designs involve two stages of data collection with either form of inquiry preceded by the other. Both elements of the design involve dual contributions to the literature and data collected in stage one somehow informs data collected in stage two. The dominant-less dominant design also involves either form of inquiry followed by the other with one method forming the dominant unit of analysis. Another variant of the dominant-less dominant design would comprise two forms of data collected in a parallel or simultaneous manner. Similar to equivalent status designs, dominant-less-dominant designs allow the
  • 25. 21 use of objective and subjective viewpoints, diverse forms of data collection, and the possibility of multiple ways to interpret the data. As long as epistemological differences can be put aside and researchers adopt the pragmatic maxims, these design strategies could provide a useful way for researchers and practitioners to engage in collaborative projects to address applied research questions. Finally, multilevel approaches offer pragmatic researchers new and innovative ways to advance applied research. We would argue that familiarity with multilevel approaches could lead to creative new strategies for mixed-method researchers. The focus now turns to pragmatic conceptions of validity. A Pragmatic Critique of Validity in Science A positivist embraces rigid criteria and the application of accepted methods to make validity claims. This position considers the application of rigorous or foundational criteria as a means to gain scientific truths unimpeded by human bias (Lincoln & Guba, 2000). In contrast, constructivists, like pragmatists, reject permanent or universal truths and instead argue that valid truth arises out of relationships, negotiations, or dialog between members of communities (Lincoln & Guba, 2000; Sparkes, 1998). In this light, pragmatic conceptions of validity call for extended discussions about the practical utility, role, and impact of science within specific contexts (Dewey, 1931; James, 1907). Classical pragmatists, like Peirce (1984) and Dewey (1931), explicitly discussed the consequences and outcomes of inquiry (i.e., practical, ethical, and moral) because research outcomes are directly linked to claims about validity in the scientific process. As such, these outcomes should be viewed as guiding principles that require ongoing discussions about the application of research in sport psychology. From a pragmatic perspective, research findings, outcomes, and consequences require dialog and discussion within the scientific community. Since morality and ethics are part of the scientific process (Bernard, 2000), the pragmatic moral/ ethical rule is to continuously reflect on the many possible consequences of scientific inquiry. Dewey considered two ethical perspectives for the scientist to consider. The first perspective occurs at the front end of inquiry and is concerned with the overarching ideals, values, and purposes that guide inquiry. The second perspective is the test of consequences that occurs when research results are applied or disseminated in some practical manner. Along these lines, we consider the following questions, similar to those discussed by Whaley (2001), to be
  • 26. 22 important for researchers to consider: (a) What questions are being addressed and are these questions relevant to the individuals being examined and/or society as a whole? (b) Who are the participants? Is the research sample diverse in terms of race, age, ethnicity, socioeconomic status, and sexual orientation? (c) What implications do the study findings have for diverse groups of individuals? If the findings do not generalize to diverse groups, do the study authors address these limitations? (d) Finally, are attempts made to disseminate the study findings to people who might benefit from this knowledge (e.g., coaches, athletes, psychologists)? We agree with Whaley’s (2001) proposal that researchers should consider reflections about the research questions being asked, who should be sampled, why the sample was chosen, and how researchers choose to practice science as integral to scientific advancement and the promotion of social justice. Such questions are important for applied researchers throughout the research process and are consistent with
  • 27. 23 the pragmatic moral and ethical views described above. The contemporary writings of Schwandt (1996) and Messick (1995) also evoked pragmatic conversations about validity. Schwandt (1996) argued for the development of a “practical philosophy” where inquiry is judged by how well research reports stimulate judgment, “practical wisdom,” and dialog between “critical parties to the research encounter” (p. 69). Schwandt noted the biggest challenge for the “practical philosopher” involves the application of general theories or principles to particular cases (i.e., deduction). Schwandt (1996) claimed “The aim of such inquiry is not to replace practitioners’ common sense knowledge of their respective joint practices with allegedly more sophisticated, theoretical, scientific knowledge but to encourage practitioners to critically reflect on and reappraise their commonsense knowledge” (p. 64). The practical application of scientific findings is especially important for the work of applied sport psychologists and/or the scientist-practitioner where knowledge dissemination about the ethical and valid delivery of psychological services requires continuous reflection, dialog, and public discourse. Messicks’s notion of “consequential validity” is consistent with Schwandt’s (1996) call for ongoing dialog and discussion between those involved with, or impacted by, the research process. While Messick’s critique is specific to the validation of psychological tests and measures, there are close parallels to lessons learned in the sport psychology literature. For instance, the valid and ethical use of personality inventories in sport is an important issue (Vealey, 2002). When practitioners use and market personality inventories as screening tools or make predictive claims about sport performance, issues of consequential validity are raised. Without extensive testing and evidence to form the basis of such claims, personality tests should not be used or marketed in such a manner (Vealey, 2002). In short, any use or application of psychological tests or principles would raise pragmatic issues of validity and dialog between communities of people would be encouraged. Conclusion The pragmatic philosophy presented in this manuscript offers a variety of potential applications in sport psychology. We discussed examples of published studies that used mixed-methods of analysis in the applied sport psychology literature. As a concluding point, we feel it is important to note that we do not encourage the “atheoretical” practice of science. Rather, we would argue the use of
  • 28. 24 mixed methods within a pragmatic philosophy would help address applied research questions from a theoretical perspective. James (1907) offered the following perspective about how pragmatism could influence the way theories are used in science: Theories thus become instruments, not answers to enigmas in which we can rest. We don’t lie back upon them, we move forward, and, on occasion, make nature over again by their aid. Pragmatism unstiffens all our theories, limbers them up and sets each one at work. (p. 52) By embracing an eclectic research approach, and by considering the consequences of inquiry, future collaborations between academic and applied sport psychologists might be enhanced. It is our hope that the pragmatic research
  • 29. 25 philosophy presented here will open doors to collaborative opportunities for individuals interested in exploring applied research questions from multiple perspectives. References Arnold, C.L. (1992). An introduction to hierarchical linear models. Measurement and Evaluation in Counseling and Development, 25, 58-90. Bandura, A. (1977). Self-efficacy: Toward a unifying theory of behavioral change. Psychological Review, 84, 191-215. Bandura, A. (1986). Social Foundations of thought and action: A social cognitive theory. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall. Bernard, H.R. (2000). Social research methods: Qualitative and quantitative approaches. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage. Biddle, S.J.H., Markland, D., Gilbourne, D., Chatzisarantis, N.L.D., & Sparkes, A.C. (2001). Research methods in sport and exercise psychology: Quantitative and qualitative issues. Journal of Sport Sciences, 19, 777-809. Brawley, L.R., Carron, A.V., & Widmeyer, W.N. (1987).Assessing the cohesion of teams: Validity of the group environment questionnaire. Journal of Sport Psychology, 9, 275-294. Brustad, R. (2002). A critical analysis of knowledge construction in sport psychology. In T. Horn (Ed.), Advances in sport psychology (2nd ed.). Champaign, IL: Human Kinetics. Chaffin, R., & Imreh, G. (2002). Practicing perfection: Piano performance asexpert memory.
  • 30. 26 Psychological Science, 13, 342-349. Chase, M.A., Lirgg, C.D., & Feltz, D.L. (1997). Do coaches’ efficacy expectations for their team predict team performance? The Sport Psychologist, 11, 8-23. Cherryholmes, C.C. (1992). Notes on pragmatism and scientific realism. Educational Researcher, 21, 13-17. Childers, J., & Hentzi, G. (1995). The Columbia dictionary of modern literary and cultural criticism. New York: Columbia University Press. Conroy, D.E., Poczwardowski, A., & Henschen, K.P. (2001). Evaluative criteria associated with failure and success for elite athletes and performing artists. Journal of Applied Sport Psychology, 13, 300-322. Conroy, D.E., Willow, J.P., & Metzler, J.N. (2002). Multidimensional fear of failure measurement: The Performance Failure Appraisal Inventory. Journal of Applied Sport Psychology, 14, 76-90. Creswell, J.W.(1994). Research design: Qualitativeand quantitative approaches. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage. Creswell, J.W. (2003). Research design: Qualitative, quantitative, and mixed methodsapproaches (2nd ed.). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage. Culver, D.M., Gilbert, W.D., & Trudel, P. (2003). A decade of qualitative research in sport psychology journals: 1990-1999. The Sport Psychologist, 17, 1-15. David, J.P., Green, P.J., Martin, R., & Suls, J. (1997). Differential roles of neuroticism, extraversion, and event desirability for mood in daily life: An integrative model of top-down and bottom-up influences. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 73, 149-159. Dewar, A., & Horn, T.S. (1992). A critical analysis of knowledge construction in sport psychology. In T.S. Horn (Ed.), Advances
  • 31. 27 in sport psychology (pp. 13-22). Champaign, IL: Human Kinetics.
  • 32. 28 Dewey, J. (1931). The development of pragmatism. In H.S. Thayer (Ed.), Pragmatism: The classic writings (pp. 23-40). Indianapolis, IN: Hackett. Diggins, J.P. (1994). The promise of pragmatism. Chicago: The University of Chicago Press. Dzewaltowski, D.A. (1997). The ecology of physical activity and sport: Merging science and practice. Journal of Applied Sport Psychology, 9, 254-276. Giacobbi, P.R., Jr., Hausenblas, H.A., & Frye, N. (2005). A naturalistic assessment of the relationship between personality, daily life event, leisure-time physical activity, and mood. Psychology of Sport & Exercise, 6, 67-81. Goldstein, H. (1995). Multilevel statistical models (2nd ed.). New York: John Wiley. Gould, D., Tuffey, S., Udry, E., & Loehr, J. (1996a). Burnout in junior elite tennis players II: Qualitative analysis. The Sport Psychologist, 10, 341-366. Gould, D., Udry, E., Tuffey, S., & Loehr, J. (1996b). Burnout in junior elite tennis players: A quantitative psychological assessment. The Sport Psychologist, 10, 322-340. Gould, D., Dieffenbach, K., & Moffett, A. (2002). Psychological characteristics and their development in Olympic Champions. Journal of Applied Sport Psychology, 14, 172-204. Hardy, L., Jones, G., & Gould, D. (1996). Understanding psychological preparation for sport: Theory and practice of elite performers. Chichester, UK: Wiley. Howe, K.R. (1988). Against the quantitative-qualitative incompatibility thesis or dogmas die hard. Educational Researcher, 17, 10-16. James, W. (1907). Pragmatism: A new name for some old ways of thinking. New York: Longmans, Green, and Company.
  • 33. 29 Krane, V. (1994). A feminist perspective on contemporary sport psychology research. The Sport Psychologist, 8, 393-410. Krane, V., Andersen, M.B., & Strean, W.B. (1997). Issues of qualitative research methods and presentation. Journal of Sport and Exercise Psychology, 19, 213-218. Krane, V., & Baird, S.M. (in press). Using ethnography in applied sport psychology. Journal of Applied Sport Psychology. Lincoln, Y.S., & Guba, E.G. (2000). Paradigmatic controversies, contradictions, and emerging confluences. In N.K. Denzin & Y.S.Lincoln (Eds.), Handbook of qualitativeresearch (2nd ed., pp. 163-188). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage. Martens, R. (1979). About smocks and jocks. Journal of Sport Psychology, 1, 94-99. Martens. R. (1987). Science, philosophy, and sport psychology. The Sport Psychologist, 1, 29-55. Messick, S. (1995). Validity of psychological assessment: Validation of inferences from persons’ responses and performances as scientific inquiry into score meaning. American Psychologist, 50, 741-749. Morris, C., & Normand, S. (1992). Hierarchical models for combining information and for meta analysis. Bayesian Statistics, 4, 321-344. Murphy, J.P. (1990). Pragmatism: From Peirce to Davidson. Boulder, CO: Westview. Paskevich, D.M., Estabrooks, P.A., Brawley, L.R., & Carron, A.V. (2001). Group cohesion in sport and exercise. In R.N. Singer, H.A. Hausenblas, & C.M. Janelle (Eds.), Handbook of sport psychology (2nd ed., pg. 472-494). New York: Wiley. Peirce, C.S. (1984). Review of Nichols’ A treatise on cosmology. In H.S. Thayer (Ed.), Meaning and action: A critical history of pragmatism (pp. 493-495). Indianapolis: IN: Hackett.
  • 34. 30 Putnam, H. (1995). Pragmatism: An open question. Oxford: Blackwell.
  • 35. 31 Raudenbush, S.W., & Bryk, A.A., (2002). Hierarchical linear models: Applications and data analysis methods (2nd ed.). Newbury Park, CA: Sage. Rorty, R. (1982). Consequences of pragmatism (Essays: 1972- 1980). Minneapolis, MN: University of Minnesota Press. Rorty, R. (1990). Introduction: Pragmatism as anti- representationalism. In J.P. Murphy, Pragmatism: From Peirce to Davidson. Boulder, CO: Westview Press. Rorty, R. (1991). Objectivity,relativismandtruth:Philosophical paper,volumeI.Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. Rosenberg, B. (1973). Linear regression with randomly dispersed parameters. Biometrika, 60, 61-75. Schwandt, T.A. (1996). Farewell to criteriology. Qualitative Inquiry, 2, 58-72. Smith, R. (1986). Toward a cognitive-affective model of athletic burnout. Journal of Sport Psychology, 8, 36-50. Smith, J. K. (1989). The nature of social and educational inquiry. Empiricism versus interpretation. Norwood, NJ: Ablex. Sparkes, A.C. (1998). Validity in qualitative inquiry and the problem of criteria: Implications for sport psychology. The Sport Psychologist, 12, 363-386. Strean, W.B. (1998). Possibilities for qualitative research in sport psychology. The Sport Psychologist, 12, 333-345. Strean, W.B. & Roberts, G.C. (1992). Future directions in applied sport psychology research. The Sport Psychologist, 6, 55-65.
  • 36. 32 Tashakkori, A., & Teddlie, C. (1998). Mixed methodology: Combining qualitative and quantitative approaches. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage. Tashakkori, A. & Teddlie, C. (2003). Handbook of mixed-methods research in the social and behavioral sciences. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage. Vealey, R.S. (2002). Personality and sport behavior. In T. Horn (Ed.), Advances in sport psychology (2nd ed., pp. 39-82). Champaign, IL: Human Kinetics. Whaley, D.E. (2001). Feminist methods and methodologies in sport and exercise and sport psychology: Issues of identity and differences. The Sport Psychologist, 15, 419- 430. Williams, B. (1985). Ethics and the limits of philosophy. Cambridge: Harvard University Press. Wilson, E.O. (1998). Consilience: The unity of knowledge. New York: Vintage Books. Author Note The authors would like to thank Vikki Krane for her insightful comments during the review process. Manuscript submitted: December 16, 2003 Revision received: May 28, 2004
  • 37. 33 Copyright of Sport Psychologist is the property of Human Kinetics Publishers, Inc. and its content may not be copied or emailed to multiple sites or posted to a listserv without the copyright holder's express written permission. However, users may print, download, or email articles for individual use.
  • 38. 34 BAB II REVIEW JURNAL Judul A Pragmatic Research Philosophy for Applied Sport Psychology Pengarang Peter R. Giacobbi, Jr. Nama Jurnal A Pragmatic Research Philosophy Volume, issue Vol. 19 Issue 1, p18-31 Tahun, Halaman Maret 2005, 16 Tujuan Penelitian Untuk menawarkan para peneliti dan praktisi dalam pendekatan psikologi olahraga terintegrasi untuk konstruksi pengetahuan yang secara logis sesuai dengan filosofi penelitian pragmatis Kutipan Repositori Giacobbi, Peter R. Jr .; Poczwardowski, Artur; dan Hager, Peter F., "Filsafat Penelitian Pragmatis untuk Psikologi Olahraga Terapan" (2005). Filsafat Penelitian Pragmatis untuk Psikologi Olahraga Terapan Peter R. Giacobbi, Jr. Universitas Florida Artur Poczwardowski Barry University Peter Hager State University of New York, College di Brockport Sebuah filosofi penelitian pragmatis diperkenalkan yang mencakup pendekatan metode campuran untuk pertanyaan penelitian terapan. Sebagai cara untuk menyoroti aplikasi pragmatisme dalam psikologi olahraga , pragmatisme dibandingkan dengan konstruktivisme dan positivisme dalam hal dasar filosofis dan aplikasi metodologis. Jenis desain contoh dibahas yang secara logis sesuai dengan filosofi penelitian pragmatis. Dalam literatur psikologi olahraga dan latihan , diskusi telah difokuskan pada pembagian antara Filsafat Penelitian Pragmatis: 19 disiplin akademik dan kegiatan profesional praktisi terapan, sumber pengetahuan yang sesuai (misalnya, wawancara, ob porsi, eksperimen), strategi analitik data (misalnya, kuantitatif dan / atau kualitatif), filosofi yang mendasari berbagai metodologi penelitian, dan pengembangan paradigma alternatif (Brustad, 2002; Dewar & Horn, 1992;
  • 39. 35 Dzewaltowski, 1997; Hardy, Jones, & Gould, 1996; Krane, 1994; Krane, Andersen, & Strean, 1997; Martens, 1979, 1987). Tujuan dari makalah ini adalah untuk menawarkan para peneliti dan praktisi dalam pendekatan psikologi olahraga terintegrasi untuk konstruksi pengetahuan yang secara logis sesuai dengan filosofi penelitian pragmatis. Motivasi utama karena diskusi ini berasal dari kesenjangan yang sudah berlangsung lama antara praktik psikologi olahraga akademis dan terapan. Dia mencatat kesenjangan antara pengamatan berbasis lapangan dan pendekatan ortodoks untuk konstruksi pengetahuan ketika dia mengklaim, "pengetahuan pengalaman dan akal sehat telah lebih menarik, dan biasanya lebih bermanfaat, daripada pengetahuan dari penelitian psikologi olahraga ". Mereka meninjau 485 studi yang diterbitkan dalam tiga jurnal psikologi olahraga utama (misalnya, The Sport Psychologist, The Journal of Applied Sport Psychology, dan Jurnal Psikologi Olahraga dan Latihan) untuk menentukan jumlah studi yang menggunakan kualitatif (misalnya, wawancara, riwayat hidup, observasi partisipan) versus metode kuantitatif (misalnya, penekanan pada n data umerical dan / atau statistik inferensial). Dalam upaya untuk mendukung klaim ini, kami menyajikan paradigma filosofis dan metodologis alternatif bagi para peneliti dan praktisi dalam psikologi olahraga terapan: pragmatism. Ini review akan memberikan dasar filosofis dari paradigma pragmatis konstruksi pengetahuan untuk peneliti terapan dalam psikologi olahraga. Maksud kami di sini adalah untuk menyajikan satu bentuk pragmatisme dengan tujuan khusus menawarkan peneliti terapan dalam psikologi olahraga filosofis dan kerangka metodologi untuk penelitian masa depan. Argumen yang disajikan dalam naskah ini konsisten dengan pandangan para sarjana yang telah mendorong penerimaan dan toleransi dari berbagai epistemologi penelitian dan metodologi dalam psikologi olahraga dan Latihan.
  • 40. 36 BAB III KESIMPULAN DAN SARAN A. Kesimpulan jurnal diatas membahas tentang literatur psikologi olahraga dan latihan , diskusi telah difokuskan pada pembagian antara Filsafat Penelitian Pragmatis: 19 disiplin akademik dan kegiatan profesional praktisi terapan, sumber pengetahuan yang sesuai (misalnya, wawancara, ob porsi, eksperimen), strategi analitik data (misalnya, kuantitatif dan / atau kualitatif), filosofi yang mendasari berbagai metodologi penelitian, dan pengembangan paradigma alternatif (Brustad, 2002; Dewar & Horn, 1992; Dzewaltowski, 1997; Hardy, Jones, & Gould, 1996; Krane, 1994; Krane, Andersen, & Strean, 1997; Martens, 1979, 1987). Tujuan dari makalah ini adalah untuk menawarkan para peneliti dan praktisi dalam pendekatan psikologi olahraga terintegrasi untuk konstruksi pengetahuan yang secara logis sesuai dengan filosofi penelitian pragmatis. B. Saran Sebagai penulis saya menyadari bahwa masih banyak kekurangan dalam makalah ini. Untuk kedepannya penulis akan menjelaskan secara detail dari sumber yang lebih banyak.
  • 42. 38 DAFTAR PUSTAKA Peter R. Giacobbi Jr. University of Florida Artur Poczwardowski Barry University Peter F. Hager The College at Brockport, phager@brockport.edu