SlideShare a Scribd company logo
1 of 41
Company Highlights
● The organization currently maintains 2,000 Microsoft
Windows desktop computers
for the workforce. As you recall, these desktop computers were
about to be
refreshed. The organization is currently considering the Dell
Optiplex series.
Specifically, each computer would have a Intel Core i5
processor, run Microsoft
Windows 10, contain 8GB DDR4 RAM, and have a 256GB solid
state drive. The
rough estimate for each PC is $1,250, which equates to a
$2,500,000 upgrade cost
to the organization.
● Microsoft Office runs on each of these computers. The license
fee for each
computer is $150, which equates to a $300,000 outlay. The
organization typically
upgrades all PCs with the latest version of Microsoft Office
when it is released.
● The organization used multiples instances of Oracle Database.
These databases
are distributed across 10 Dell PowerEdge servers. These servers
cost a minimum of
$2,159 each, for a total cost of $21,580.
● The organization maintains an additional 20 Dell PowerEdge
servers for a variety of
reasons to include email, file backup, remote access, etc. The
total cost to refresh
each of these servers is $3,000 for a total cost of $60,000.
● The organization maintains various networking devices. The
total cost of this
equipment is $3,000,000 and yearly refresh costs are $750,000.
● The organization maintains a suite of network security
hardware and software
solutions. They include a DMZ configuration, multiple
firewalls, VPN hardware,
antivirus solutions, intrusion detection and protection devices,
and log management
solutions. The total yearly hardware and software costs for the
organization
cybersecurity efforts are $2,000,000.
● The organization also allows users to access the network via
their company-issued
Android phones, or iPads. Employees receive a mobile device
with VPN software
installed so that they may remotely access company systems
when they are at
home, or traveling.
● Because this is a notional example and assignment, you may
make further
assumptions about the organization IT environment. There is no
need to ask your
instructor for permission. Simply make explicit your
assumptions in the white paper.
For example, you are free to assume the number of IT
professionals, their
specialties, and more importantly the cost to run the IT
department.
RESEARCH ARTICLE
The public’s belief in climate change and its
human cause are increasing over time
Taciano L. Milfont
1*, Marc S. Wilson1, Chris G. Sibley2
1 School of Psychology, Victoria University of Wellington,
Wellington, New Zealand, 2 School of Psychology,
University of Auckland, Auckland, New Zealand
* [email protected]
Abstract
Polls examining public opinion on the subject of climate change
are now commonplace, and
one-off public opinion polls provide a snapshot of citizen’s
opinions that can inform policy
and communication strategies. However, cross-sectional polls
do not track opinions over
time, thus making it impossible to ascertain whether key climate
change beliefs held by the
same group of individuals are changing or not. Here we examine
the extent to which indivi-
dual’s level of agreement with two key beliefs ("climate change
is real" and "climate change
is caused by humans") remain stable or increase/decrease over a
six-year period in New
Zealand using latent growth curve modelling (n = 10,436). Data
were drawn from the New
Zealand Attitudes and Values Study, a probabilistic national
panel study, and indicated that
levels of agreement to both beliefs have steadily increased over
the 2009–2015 period.
Given that climate change beliefs and concerns are key
predictors of climate change action,
our findings suggest that a combination of targeted endeavors,
as well as serendipitous
events, may successfully convey the emergency of the issue.
Introduction
In 2015, Pope Francis encouraged Catholics worldwide to be
attentive to global climate change
in his encyclical Laudato si’. More recently, Leonardo DiCaprio
was allowed a longer-than-
average Oscar acceptance speech to similarly raise worldwide
attention to climate change,
while leaders and representatives from over 150 countries
gathered to sign the global climate
change agreement reached in Paris in December, 2015. One
question that arises from these
international examples of public figures trying to promote
awareness and action on climate
change is whether they reflect and/or influence a popular
upswing in climate change beliefs
and concerns over time.
Although perceptions of climate change threat and beliefs vary
internationally [1], opinion
polls indicate that an overall global majority believe in the
reality and human causation of cli-
mate change [2–4]. In particular, short-term longitudinal panel
studies that have examined the
effect of factors such as popular media interventions or extreme
weather events on climate
change concern suggest that individuals’ attitudes may change
after exposure to such events.
For example, a field experiment investigating impacts of the
provision of climate information
through TV meteorologists (presented on air and online)
indicated that viewers were both
PLOS ONE | https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0174246
March 20, 2017 1 / 9
a1111111111
a1111111111
a1111111111
a1111111111
a1111111111
OPEN ACCESS
Citation: Milfont TL, Wilson MS, Sibley CG (2017)
The public’s belief in climate change and its human
cause are increasing over time. PLoS ONE 12(3):
e0174246. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.
pone.0174246
Editor: Stefan Lötters, Universitat Trier, GERMANY
Received: May 25, 2016
Accepted: March 6, 2017
Published: March 20, 2017
Copyright: © 2017 Milfont et al. This is an open
access article distributed under the terms of the
Creative Commons Attribution License, which
permits unrestricted use, distribution, and
reproduction in any medium, provided the original
author and source are credited.
Data Availability Statement: Data cannot be made
available due to ethical restrictions imposed by
University of Auckland Human Participants Ethics
Committee. A de-identified dataset is available
upon request from the corresponding author for all
appropriately qualified researchers. A de-identified
dataset is also available from any member of the
advisory board of the New Zealand Attitudes and
Values Study (www.nzvalues.org). The University
of Auckland Human Participants Ethics Committee
can be contacted at [email protected]
Funding: This research was supported by a
Templeton World Charity Foundation grant (http://
https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0174246
http://crossmark.crossref.org/dialog/?doi=10.1371/journal.pone.
0174246&domain=pdf&date_stamp=2017-03-20
http://crossmark.crossref.org/dialog/?doi=10.1371/journal.pone.
0174246&domain=pdf&date_stamp=2017-03-20
http://crossmark.crossref.org/dialog/?doi=10.1371/journal.pone.
0174246&domain=pdf&date_stamp=2017-03-20
http://crossmark.crossref.org/dialog/?doi=10.1371/journal.pone.
0174246&domain=pdf&date_stamp=2017-03-20
http://crossmark.crossref.org/dialog/?doi=10.1371/journal.pone.
0174246&domain=pdf&date_stamp=2017-03-20
http://crossmark.crossref.org/dialog/?doi=10.1371/journal.pone.
0174246&domain=pdf&date_stamp=2017-03-20
https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0174246
https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0174246
http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/
http://www.nzvalues.org
mailto:[email protected]
http://www.templetonworldcharity.org/
more concerned and more likely to perceive scientific consensus
than others [5]. Another
study found that climate change concern was greater among UK
respondents who have had
first-hand experience with floods ([6]; see also [7]). In contrast,
little change in climate change
attitudes were observed pre- and post-2012 Midwestern US
drought [8], and UK viewers of
the climate change documentary The Age of Stupid initially
reported increased concern, action
intention and efficacy but these effects deteriorated over time
[9].
Although inconclusive, this emerging literature suggests that
researchers and policy makers
would benefit by knowing whether citizens are becoming more
or less credulous of climate
change today than previously, and also the rate of change in
citizens’ belief in climate change.
Public opinion polling on the subject of climate change concern
and beliefs is now common-
place, and results from such one-off surveys are useful in
providing a snapshot of the level of
agreement and/or support to climate-related questions and
issues from a given population.
However, such cohort-based findings are limited in the extent to
which researchers and practi-
tioners can draw inferences about the nature, and causes, of
changes in attitude or belief. To
illustrate, while results suggest that climate change concern
among Americans increased sig-
nificantly during the 1980s, more recent polls suggest that
fewer American citizens consider
climate change to be a threat than only a few years prior [2].
Since these studies base their con-
clusions on distinct population cohorts, we do not have a
particularly robust understanding of
whether, and how much, beliefs about global climate change are
indeed changing over time.
Thanks to the ubiquity of public opinion polls, we also know
that perception of climate change
threat varies internationally [1] and fluctuates over time [2, 10].
Studies assessing climate-relevant questions longitudinally are
better equipped than cross-
sectional public opinion polls to provide information regarding
temporal change in climate
change attitudes and beliefs. However, there are relatively few
studies assessing climate-rele-
vant attitudes in the same panel longitudinally, and very few
indeed that do so over more than
a year, or with a representative sample.
One study assessed a national sample of 269 New Zealanders’
responses on climate-related
questions three times over a one-year period [11], reporting that
concern about global warm-
ing and climate change did not increase over this duration—
perhaps due to the limited time
frame—but that higher levels of climate-related knowledge
increased climate change concern,
which in turn translated into greater subjective environmental
responsibility. Another study
assessed a nationally representative sample of US citizens about
climate belief and personal
experience of climate and weather, assessed in 2008 and 2011
[12]. Results showed that per-
sonal experience of climate change-affected weather increased
climate change belief and cer-
tainty, but also that high certainty also predicted subjective
experience—strong climate change
skeptics and believers tended to interpret subjective weather
experience consistent with their
belief.
Another large scale study with a longitudinal component
reported that a majority of Aus-
tralians believe climate change is occurring, that climate change
is of lesser priority than other
personal concerns, and that Australians were poor predictors of
others’ attitudes; but longitu-
dinal analyses are not reported excepting in reference to
absolute levels of attitude change over
time [13]. Another longitudinal study conducted in Australian
provides a qualitative analysis
of perceptions of climate and climate change, emphasising that
for many people the notion of
“natural cycles” was an important common sense idea that
served to provide an appreciation
of an understandable dynamic relationship between humans and
their environment [14].
Despite the increase in the number of studies examining change
in climate change-related
variables across time considering the same population, none of
the available studies are both
representative or of a sufficiently large scale to draw and report
robust inferences of population
change. In response to these challenges, we present the first
longitudinal analysis of key climate
Changes in climate change beliefs over time
PLOS ONE | https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0174246
March 20, 2017 2 / 9
www.templetonworldcharity.org/; ID: 0077) to CGS
and a Marsden Fast-Start grant (E1908) from The
Royal Society of New Zealand (http://www.
royalsociety.org.nz/programmes/funds/marsden/)
to TLM. The funders had no role in study design,
data collection and analysis, decision to publish, or
preparation of the manuscript.
Competing interests: The authors have declared
that no competing interests exist.
https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0174246
http://www.templetonworldcharity.org/
http://www.royalsociety.org.nz/programmes/funds/marsden/
http://www.royalsociety.org.nz/programmes/funds/marsden/
change beliefs with a large nationally representative sample
assessed over a six-year period
(from 2009 to 2015). The aim of this research is to present an
initial flavour of the extent and
rate of change of climate change beliefs. We analyse the first
six waves of longitudinal data
from the New Zealand Attitudes and Values Study comprising
10,436 respondents who com-
pleted at least three of the waves of annual data collection.
Participants indicated their level of
agreement to one question examining the belief in the reality of
climate change and another
question examining belief in anthropogenic climate change.
Methods
Sampling procedure
This study analysed data from the New Zealand Attitudes and
Values Study (NZAVS), which
is is an annual, longitudinal national probability study that has
been assessing people’s socio-
political attitudes annually since 2009. Invitations to participate
in a mail-based survey were
sent to people randomly sampled from the New Zealand
Electoral Roll. Participants were
posted a copy of the questionnaire, with a second postal follow-
up two months later. Partici-
pants who provided an email address in a previous wave were
also emailed and invited to com-
plete an online version if they preferred.
The NZAVS is reviewed every three years by the University of
Auckland Human Partici-
pants Ethics Committee. The first phases of the longitudinal
study were approved on 09-Sep-
tember-2009 for 3 years (reference number: 2009/336). Ethics
approval for the study was re-
approved by the University of Auckland Human Participants
Ethics Committee on 17-Febru-
ary-2012 until 09-September-2015 (reference number: 6171),
and then on 03-June-2015 until
03-June-2018 (reference number: 014889). All participants
granted informed written consent.
Contact details are removed when the questionnaires are
received, and all data were de-identi-
fied before analyses were conducted. NZAVS data is hosted at
the University of Auckland, and
the de-identified data is available to appropriately qualified
researchers upon request for the
purposes of re-analysis. We describe in more detail the
sampling procedure for each of the
waves used in this study in S1 Supporting Information (see also
[15]).
Participants
Table 1 presents sample size and characteristics for each of the
survey waves (see also S1 Sup-
porting Information). The present analysis included only
participants who responded to three
or more waves. Missing data among those completing at least
three waves was estimated using
Full Information Maximum Likelihood and assuming that data
were missing at random. This
assumption seems reasonable given that analysis of panel
attrition indicates that demographic
factors are only weakly predictive of dropout in the NZAVS
[15]; however, people for whom
English is not a first language are more likely to drop out of the
study relative to other groups.
This method of missing data estimation weighted each
individual-level trajectory based on its
reliability, which is in turn a function that takes the number of
observations into account.
Those who responded to all six-time points contributed the most
information to the estima-
tion of the mean-level trajectory, while those who responded to
three contributed the least.
Syntax for our Mplus models are available on the NZAVS
website: www.psych.auckland.ac.nz/
uoa/NZAVS
Measures
The two NZAVS questions analysed in the present article (i.e.,
“Climate change is real” and
“Climate change is caused by humans”) were embedded in a
large battery of Likert-type
Changes in climate change beliefs over time
PLOS ONE | https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0174246
March 20, 2017 3 / 9
http://www.psych.auckland.ac.nz/uoa/NZAVS
http://www.psych.auckland.ac.nz/uoa/NZAVS
https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0174246
questions. Across the first six waves of the NZAVS respondents
expressed their levels of dis-
agreement-agreement to these questions on a 7-point answer
scale with end-labels only: 1
(strongly disagree) and 7 (strongly agree). Table 1 presents the
mean and standard deviation for
the agreement levels of these questions, as well as the frequency
distribution of the answers.
Analysis
We simultaneously examined respondents’ level of agreement to
both climate change beliefs
across time using a parallel process Latent Growth Model. This
technique has many advan-
tages when compared to other longitudinal data-analytic
techniques [16]. Some of the advan-
tages include the ability to estimate reference levels (or initial
amount; intercept) and their
developmental trajectories to and from those levels (slope); the
ability to estimate change in
latent variables as the technique is a special case of structural
equation modeling; the ability to
estimate means, variances and covariances of individuals
differences in both intercepts and
slopes; and the technique does not assume that all individuals in
a particular longitudinal proj-
ect change over time at the same rate.
We estimated the rate of change in levels of disagreement-
agreement with the two climate
change belief items over the October 2009 –October 2015
period. We modelled the intercepts,
and linear and quadratic growth factors for these two aspects of
climate belief as parallel pro-
cesses, and allowed the intercepts and slopes to correlate with
one another.
To reflect the variation in response time to each wave between
individuals, response time at
each wave was then converted into yearly units and modelled as
individually time-varying
effects, with time = 0 being the 30th June, 2009. This accounted
for the fact that different indi-
viduals completed the surveys at different times throughout the
year, and with different dura-
tions between each assessment, rather than fixing all responses
in 2009 to 1, all responses in
2010 to 2, and so forth. Our models therefore provide a latent
intercept representing estimated
mean climate change belief at 30th June, 2009. Residual
variances, or disturbances, in the man-
ifest ratings of both Likert items were constrained to equality
across waves. This therefore
Table 1. Distribution of responses to the questions “Climate
change is real” (reality) and “Climate change is caused by
humans” (cause) in the
first six waves of the New Zealand Attitudes and Values Study,
with descriptive statistics for each question at each wave.
Response options
Year Question 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 M SD N
strongly disagree strongly agree
2009 Reality 4% 4% 6% 14% 15% 24% 33% 5.34 1.69 4630
Cause 6 7 9 19 19 22 17 4.74 1.74 4591
2010 Reality 4 5 6 15 17 26 28 5.26 1.65 4022
Cause 7 7 9 20 20 21 17 4.70 1.74 3993
2011 Reality 3 4 5 15 18 24 31 5.37 1.62 6177
Cause 6 7 8 19 20 22 17 4.75 1.74 6127
2012 Reality 3 3 5 13 16 26 34 5.45 1.61 8638
Cause 5 6 9 18 21 22 19 4.84 1.70 8592
2013 Reality 3 3 4 12 15 24 37 5.56 1.58 9188
Cause 5 6 7 17 19 24 23 5.02 1.69 9172
2014 Reality 2 2 4 11 17 24 40 5.72 1.44 8928
Cause 3 4 6 17 20 23 26 5.20 1.61 8889
Note: Distributions are based on valid percent, and sample sizes
at each time point refer to those who completed at least three of
the first six waves of the
New Zealand Attitudes and Values Study.
https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0174246.t001
Changes in climate change beliefs over time
PLOS ONE | https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0174246
March 20, 2017 4 / 9
https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0174246.t001
https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0174246
assumes that the amount of variation that is unexplained by the
intercept and rate of change in
our outcome measure is the same across waves [17].
Our model estimated the fixed and random effect for the
intercept, linear and quadratic
growth components spanning Waves 1–6 of data collection. The
intercept represents the mean
score on the outcome (disagreement-agreement levels for the
climate item of interest) as of
June 2009. Belief in climate change reality and human causation
were then estimated every
three months from October 2009 to October 2015.
We integrated the parameters for the fixed effects in our models
to generate the model-
implied rate of change in climate beliefs over the October
2009–October 2015 period. The fol-
lowing regression equation was used to estimate this model-
implied rate of change:
yt ¼ c þ st þ s
2t2
In this regression equation, yt is the predicted level of climate
belief in the given question at
time t, c is the intercept, or model-predicted mean level of
belief when t = 0 (i.e., 30th June,
2009), s is the fixed effect for the linear growth component of
the model, and s2 is the fixed
effect for the quadratic growth component. Values of t are
distributed in this model so that 1.0
unit represents a change of one year. We used the parameters
reported in Table 2 to derive
model-implied levels of disagreement-agreement that “climate
change is real” and “climate
change is caused by humans” for values of t ranging from .25
(October 2009) to 6.25 (October
2015) in quarterly increments.
Table 2. Fixed and random effects for parallel process Latent
Growth Model predicting change in disagreement to agreement
(on a scale from 1 to
7) in response to key climate change questions) for New
Zealand residents in the first six waves of the New Zealand
Attitudes and Values Study
over the October 2009 –October 2015 period.
Model “Climate change is real” “Climate change is caused
by humans”
Linear
Fixed effects (means)
Intercept 5.244 (.019)** 4.612 (.020)**
Linear growth parameter .072 (.003)** .090 (.004)**
Random effects (variances)
Intercept 2.246 (.059)** 2.409 (.053)**
Linear growth parameter .031 (.003)** .030 (.002)**
Quadratic
Fixed effects (means)
Intercept 5.403 (0.023)** 4.813 (0.024)**
Linear growth parameter -0.061 (0.012)** -0.080 (0.013)**
Quadratic growth parameter 0.020 (0.002)** 0.026 (0.002)**
Random effects (variances)
Intercept 2.209 (0.070)** 2.396 (0.062)**
Linear growth parameter 0.262 (0.033)** 0.306 (0.031)**
Quadratic growth parameter 0.005 (0.001)** 0.006 (0.001)**
Note: N = 10,436 participants who completed at least three of
the first six waves of the New Zealand Attitudes and Values
Study. Participants who
completed less than three of the six waves were excluded from
the model. Missing data among participants who completed
three or more waves were
estimated using Full Information Maximum Likelihood and
assuming data were missing at random. Models estimated using
Maximum Likelihood with robust
estimation of standard errors. Standard errors reported in
parentheses. Disturbances of the manifest indicators were
constrained to equality over time. Fit
indices: Linear model: loglikelihood = -130590.18, AIC =
261212.36, BIC = 261328.41. loglikelihood = -130055.73, AIC
= 260169.46, BIC = 260379.79.
*p < .05.
**p < .01.
https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0174246.t002
Changes in climate change beliefs over time
PLOS ONE | https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0174246
March 20, 2017 5 / 9
https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0174246.t002
https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0174246
Results
Fig 1 presents the estimated rates of change in disagreement to
agreement in response to the
climate change belief items over the six-year period. Our results
show that agreement with cli-
mate change reality was higher at all time points than agreement
with anthropogenic climate
change.
What about change in the climate change beliefs over time?
Table 2 presents the results
from the parallel process Latent Growth Model predicting
change in disagreement to agree-
ment in response to the climate change belief items. We first
tested a linear growth model, and
then extended this to include a second-order polynomial
(quadratic) growth component. The
quadratic growth component in the quadratic model was
significant, and thus we focus on this
model as it allowed for potential change occurring in a simple
accelerating (or decelerating)
fashion.
Our analyses reveal a significant curvilinear increase in the
average level of agreement with
both climate change beliefs over the 2009–2015 period. This
curvilinear rate of growth
occurred because climate change beliefs were generally
increasing over time, but in an acceler-
ating fashion with the most rapid increased occurring from
about 2013 onwards (see Fig 1).
Fig 1. Model-implied rate of change in the level of agreement
with climate change beliefs. Rate of change over the 2009–2015
period for 10,436
participants who completed at least three of the first six waves
of the New Zealand Attitudes and Values Study. Ratings ranged
from 1 (strongly disagree) to 7
(strongly agree), and error bars represent the 95% CI of the
model-implied estimate.
https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0174246.g001
Changes in climate change beliefs over time
PLOS ONE | https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0174246
March 20, 2017 6 / 9
https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0174246.g001
https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0174246
Moreover, although the observed increase was relatively small,
the across-time increase was
statistically significant for both belief in climate change reality
and anthropogenic climate
change. Our results show that from October 2009 to October
2015 respondents’ level of agree-
ment about the reality of climate change increased, on average,
from 5.39 to 5.82 on an agree-
ment scale ranging from 1–7. Similarly, respondents’ level of
agreement for anthropogenic
climate change increased on average from 4.79 to 5.34 for the
same six-year period.
Finally, we examined whether the observed increase in average
levels of agreement in cli-
mate change reality correlated with the observed increase in
average levels of agreement in
anthropogenic climate change, and vice-versa. In the linear
(baseline) model, the linear growth
functions for climate change reality and anthropogenic climate
change were positively associ-
ated (b = .027, se = .002, z = 15.773, p < .001). The linear
growth factors for reality and anthro-
pogenic beliefs were also correlated in the quadratic model (b =
.232, se = .020, z = 11.51, p <
.001), as were the quadratic growth factors (b = .005, se = .001,
z = 9.49, p < .001). These results
indicate that people who tended to increase their level of
agreement in one climate change
belief also tended to increase their agreement level in the other
belief.
Discussion
We provide the first comprehensive population-level
examination of change in climate change
beliefs. First, we showed that the level of agreement to the
reality of climate change is greater
than agreement to its human causation. This indicates a clear
differentiation of the two issues
among respondents regarding their belief levels: Respondents in
our sample are more likely to
believe in the reality of climate change than whether or not
climate change is caused by
humans. Perhaps believe in the reality of climate change is a
precondition for believing in its
human causation. This is consequential since the belief in
climate change reality has been
shown to have stronger effects on a number of outcome
variables compared to the belief in
anthropogenic climate change; moreover, these beliefs interact
so that belief in climate change
reality is more predictive for individuals who strongly believe
in human cause of climate
change [18].
We then showed that agreement to the reality of climate change
and agreement to its
human causation showed comparable (and coupled) increase
over the six-year period. The
observed steady increase in climate change belief over a six-
year period is promising and sug-
gests that people (at least those in New Zealand) are becoming
increasingly aware of the reality
of climate change and its human causation. We examine change
spanning the 2009–2015
period, and showed that this increase in belief has been most
pronounced in more recent
years, from about 2013 onwards. It should also be noted that a
simple linear model also fit the
data fairly well, indicating that the core pattern in our data is a
general and gradual increase in
climate belief, with the caveat that such change may be slightly
more pronounced in recent
years. Moreover, that across-time increase in climate change
beliefs are related is consequential
given that these beliefs predict a number of important outcomes
relevant for mitigation and
adaptation actions [18, 19].
Past research has examined the many predictors of climate
change concern and belief [20–
24], with a recent meta-analysis demonstrating that political
affiliation and political ideology
are the main predictors of climate change belief [19]. Consistent
with this finding, previous
research in New Zealand identifies a latent profile of
respondents (“climate skeptics”) with low
agreement levels in both climate change reality and its human
causation that had the highest
levels of self-reported political conservatism [25]. Since
distrust in the reality of climate change
and its human cause is associated with a more conservative
political orientation, it is likely that
change in climate change belief will differ for distinct groups in
a given society. That is to say,
Changes in climate change beliefs over time
PLOS ONE | https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0174246
March 20, 2017 7 / 9
https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0174246
we predict that the observed increase in climate change beliefs
might be greatest among politi-
cally liberal individuals, while small or null for their politically
conservative counterparts. Due
to power considerations we cannot test this prediction at present
[26], but the New Zealand
Attitudes and Values Study is ongoing and this is something we
aim to address in the years to
come. Similar to public opinion polls showing that perception
of climate change threat fluctu-
ates over time [2] and [10], we also expect that climate change
beliefs would fluctuate over
time. The ongoing nature of this longitudinal project means we
will be able to pinpoint
whether fluctuations on climate change beliefs is a direct result
of particular socio-economic
circumstances.
To conclude, our findings suggest that communicating the
reality and urgency of climate
change—perhaps combined with media attention to the issue
[27] and with communicating
individual experience of climate change-related extreme
weather events [5]—is successfully
influencing the levels of climate change belief in New Zealand.
Similar studies will be able to
demonstrate whether this is also happening in other societies.
Supporting information
S1 Supporting Information.
(DOCX)
Author Contributions
Conceptualization: TLM CGS.
Data curation: CGS.
Formal analysis: CGS.
Funding acquisition: TLM CGS.
Investigation: CGS.
Methodology: CGS.
Writing – original draft: MSW TLM.
Writing – review & editing: TLM MSW CGS.
References
1. Lee TM, Markowitz EM, Howe PD, Ko C-Y, Leiserowitz AA.
Predictors of public climate change aware-
ness and risk perception around the world. Nature Climate
Change. 2015; 5: 1014–1020.
2. Capstick SB, Whitmarsh LE, Poortinga W, Pidgeon NF,
Upham P. International trends in public percep-
tions of climate change over the past quarter century. WIREs
Climate Change. 2015; 6: 35–61.
3. Pugliese A, Ray J. Awareness of climate change and threat
vary by region. 2009. Available: http://www.
gallup.com/poll/124652/Awareness-Climate-Change-Threat-
Vary-Region.aspx.
4. Wike R. What the world thinks about climate change in 7
charts. 2016. Available: http://www.
pewresearch.org/fact-tank/2016/04/18/what-the-world-thinks-
about-climate-change-in-7-charts/.
5. Zhao X, Maibach E, Gandy J, Witte J, Cullen H, Klinger BA,
Rowan KE, Witte J, Pyle A. Climate change
education through TV weathercasts: Results of a field
experiment. Bulletin of the American Meteorologi-
cal Society. 2014; 95: 117–130.
6. Spence A, Poortinga W, Pidgeon N. The psychological
distance of climate change. Risk Analysis.
2012; 32: 957–972. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1539-
6924.2011.01695.x PMID: 21992607
7. Milfont TL, Evans L, Sibley CG, Ries J, Cunningham A.
Proximity to coast is linked to climate change
belief. PLoS ONE. 2014; 9: e103180.
https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0103180 PMID: 25047568
Changes in climate change beliefs over time
PLOS ONE | https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0174246
March 20, 2017 8 / 9
http://www.plosone.org/article/fetchSingleRepresentation.action
?uri=info:doi/10.1371/journal.pone.0174246.s001
http://www.gallup.com/poll/124652/Awareness-Climate-
Change-Threat-Vary-Region.aspx
http://www.gallup.com/poll/124652/Awareness-Climate-
Change-Threat-Vary-Region.aspx
http://www.pewresearch.org/fact-tank/2016/04/18/what-the-
world-thinks-about-climate-change-in-7-charts/
http://www.pewresearch.org/fact-tank/2016/04/18/what-the-
world-thinks-about-climate-change-in-7-charts/
https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1539-6924.2011.01695.x
http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/21992607
https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0103180
http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/25047568
https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0174246
8. Carlton JS, Mase AS, Knutson CL, Lemos MC, Haigh T,
Todey DP, Prokopy LS. The effects of extreme
drought on climate change beliefs, risk perceptions, and
adaptation attitudes. Climate Change. 2016;
135: 211–226.
9. Howell RA. Lights, camera. . . action? Altered attitudes and
behaviour in response to the climate change
film The Age of Stupid. Global Environmental Change. 2011;
21: 177–187.
10. Scruggs L, Benegal S. Declining public concern about
climate change: Can we blame the great reces-
sion? Global Environmental Change. 2012; 22: 505–515.
11. Milfont TL. The interplay between knowledge, perceived
efficacy and concern about global warming
and climate change: A one-year longitudinal study. Risk
Analysis. 2012; 32: 1003–1020. https://doi.org/
10.1111/j.1539-6924.2012.01800.x PMID: 22489642
12. Myers T, Maibach E, Roser-Renouf C, Akerlof K,
Leiserowitz A. The relationship between personal
experience and belief in the reality of global warming. Nature
Climate Change. 2012; 3: 343–347.
13. Leviston Z, Price J, Malkin S, McCrea R. Fourth annual
survey of Australian attitudes to climate change:
Interim report. 2014. Available:
http://images.smh.com.au/file/2014/02/07/5139061/CSIROCC4.
pdf
14. Connor L, Higginbotham N. “Natural cycles” in lay
understandings of climate change. Global Environ-
mental Change. 2013; 23: 1852–1861.
15. Satherley N, Milojev P, Greaves LM, Huang Y, Osborne D,
Bulbulia J, et al. Demographic and psycho-
logical predictors of panel attrition: evidence from the New
Zealand Attitudes and Values Study. PLoS
ONE. 2015; 10(3): e0121950.
https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0121950 PMID: 25793746
16. Hancock GR, Lawrence FR. Using latent growth models to
evaluate longitudinal change. In: Hancock
GR, Mueller RO, editors. Structural Equation Modeling: A
Second Course. Greenwood, CT: Informa-
tion Age Publishing, Inc; 2006. pp. 171–196.
17. Grimm KJ, Widaman KF. Residual structures in latent
growth curve modeling. Structural Equation
Modeling. 2010; 17: 424–442.
18. Sibley CG, Kurz T. A model of climate belief profiles: How
much does it matter if people question human
causation? Analyses of Social Issues and Public Policy. 2013;
13: 245–261.
19. Hornsey MJ, Harris EA, Bain PG, Fielding KS. Meta-
analyses of the determinants and outcomes of
belief in climate change. Nature Climate Change. 2016; 6: 622–
626.
20. Dunlap RE, McCright AM. Cool dudes: The denial of
climate change among conservative white males
in the United States. Global Environmental Change. 2011; 21:
1163–1172.
21. Guy S, Kashima Y, Walker I, O’Neill S. Investigating the
effects of knowledge and ideology on climate
change beliefs. European Journal of Social Psychology. 2014;
44: 421–429.
22. Hart PS, Nisbet EC, Myers TA. Public attention to science
and political news and support for climate
change mitigation. Nature Climate Change. 2015; 5: 541–545.
23. Kellstedt PM, Zahran S, Vedlitz A. Personal efficacy, the
information environment, and attitudes toward
global warming and climate change in the United States. Risk
Analysis. 2008; 28: 113–126. https://doi.
org/10.1111/j.1539-6924.2008.01010.x PMID: 18304110
24. Milfont TL, Richter I, Sibley CG, Wilson MS, Fischer R.
Environmental consequences of the desire to
dominate and be superior. Personality and Social Psychology
Bulletin. 2013; 39: 1127–1138. https://
doi.org/10.1177/0146167213490805 PMID: 23798371
25. Milfont TL, Milojev P, Greaves L, Sibley CG. Socio-
structural and psychological foundations of climate
change beliefs. New Zealand Journal of Psychology. 2015; 44:
17–30. Available: http://www.
psychology.org.nz/wp-content/uploads/Article-21.pdf.
26. Sibley CG, Milojev P. Power estimation of slope growth
factors in the NZAVS using Monte Carlo simula-
tion. NZAVS Technical Documents, e19. 2014. Available:
www.psych.auckland.ac.nz/uoa/NZAVS
27. Schmidt A, Ivanova A, Schäfer MS. Media attention for
climate change around the world: A comparative
analysis of newspaper coverage in 27 countries. Global
Environmental Change. 2013; 23: 1233–1248.
Changes in climate change beliefs over time
PLOS ONE | https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0174246
March 20, 2017 9 / 9
https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1539-6924.2012.01800.x
https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1539-6924.2012.01800.x
http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/22489642
http://images.smh.com.au/file/2014/02/07/5139061/CSIROCC4.
pdf
https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0121950
http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/25793746
https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1539-6924.2008.01010.x
https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1539-6924.2008.01010.x
http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/18304110
https://doi.org/10.1177/0146167213490805
https://doi.org/10.1177/0146167213490805
http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/23798371
http://www.psychology.org.nz/wp-content/uploads/Article-
21.pdf
http://www.psychology.org.nz/wp-content/uploads/Article-
21.pdf
http://www.psych.auckland.ac.nz/uoa/NZAVS
https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0174246
Copyright of PLoS ONE is the property of Public Library of
Science and its content may not
be copied or emailed to multiple sites or posted to a listserv
without the copyright holder's
express written permission. However, users may print,
download, or email articles for
individual use.
CURRENCY The timeliness of the information
When was the information published or posted?
Has the information been revised or updated?
Does your topic require current information, or will older
sources work as well?
Are the links functional? *
RELEVANCE The importance of the information for your
needs
Does the information relate to your topic or answer your
question?
Who is the intended audience?
Is the information at an appropriate level (i.e. not too
elementary or advanced for your needs)?
Have you looked at a variety of sources before determining
this is one you will use?
Would you be comfortable citing this source in your research
paper?
AUTHORITY The source of the information
Who is the author/publisher/source/sponsor?
What are the author’s credentials or organizational
affiliations?
Is the author qualified to write on the topic?
Is there contact information, such as a publisher or email
address?
Does the URL reveal anything about the author or source? *
examples .com .edu .gov .org .net
ACCURACY The reliability, truthfulness and correctness of the
content
Where does the information come from?
Is the information supported by evidence?
Has the information been reviewed or refereed?
Can you verify any of the information in another source or
from personal knowledge?
Does the language or tone seem unbiased and free of emotion?
Are there spelling, grammar or typographical errors?
PURPOSE The reason the information exists.
What is the purpose of the information? Is it to inform, teach,
sell, entertain or persuade?
Do the authors/sponsors make their intentions or purpose
clear?
Is the information fact, opinion or propaganda?
Does the point of view appear objective and impartial?
Are there political, ideological, cultural, religious,
institutional or personal biases?
* indicates criteria is for web
The CRAAP Test was developed by librarians at California
State University, Chico
EVALUATING INFORMATION
Applying the CRAAP Test
When you search for information, you’re going to find lots of
it… but is it good information?
You will have to determine that for yourself, and the CRAAP
Test can help. The CRAAP Test
is a list of questions to help you evaluate the information you
find. Different criteria will be
more or less important depending on your situation or need.
http://www.csuchico.edu/lins/handouts/eval_websites.pdf
SDN & IBN White Paper
CMIT 495: Current Trends and Projects in Computer Networks
and Security
[PROFESSOR NAME: Andrew Humes]
By:
[Student Name: Shreeji Patel]
Company IT Refresh and Security Costs Analyzed

More Related Content

Similar to Company IT Refresh and Security Costs Analyzed

Informing the policymaking landscape: From research to action in the fight ag...
Informing the policymaking landscape: From research to action in the fight ag...Informing the policymaking landscape: From research to action in the fight ag...
Informing the policymaking landscape: From research to action in the fight ag...CIFOR-ICRAF
 
Horizontal communication and the evolution of journalism
Horizontal communication and the evolution of journalismHorizontal communication and the evolution of journalism
Horizontal communication and the evolution of journalismDonica Mensing
 
Learning for Change_Booklet_July2015
Learning for Change_Booklet_July2015Learning for Change_Booklet_July2015
Learning for Change_Booklet_July2015Rachel Phillips
 
Essay On Human Rights Essay Writing English Ess
Essay On Human Rights Essay Writing English EssEssay On Human Rights Essay Writing English Ess
Essay On Human Rights Essay Writing English EssJessica Rinehart
 
Plain2013 PL and Knowledge Mobilization P Levesque
Plain2013 PL and Knowledge Mobilization P Levesque Plain2013 PL and Knowledge Mobilization P Levesque
Plain2013 PL and Knowledge Mobilization P Levesque 2013PLAINConference
 
Climate Catalyst Report 01
Climate Catalyst Report 01Climate Catalyst Report 01
Climate Catalyst Report 01EddieKowalski
 
Sport Essay Questions In 2021 Essay Writing, Essa
Sport Essay Questions In 2021 Essay Writing, EssaSport Essay Questions In 2021 Essay Writing, Essa
Sport Essay Questions In 2021 Essay Writing, EssaMaggie Cavanaugh
 
Data For Policy Influence: How to Manage, Distribute, and Present Your Data
Data For Policy Influence: How to Manage, Distribute, and Present Your DataData For Policy Influence: How to Manage, Distribute, and Present Your Data
Data For Policy Influence: How to Manage, Distribute, and Present Your DataForum One
 
Research Evaluation in an Open Science context
Research Evaluation in an Open Science contextResearch Evaluation in an Open Science context
Research Evaluation in an Open Science contextHilda Muchando
 
FSCI Data management and data sharing
FSCI Data management and data sharingFSCI Data management and data sharing
FSCI Data management and data sharingARDC
 
Open Data in a Global Ecosystem
Open Data in a Global EcosystemOpen Data in a Global Ecosystem
Open Data in a Global EcosystemPhilip Bourne
 
Green Warriors Change project proposal.pptx
Green Warriors Change project proposal.pptxGreen Warriors Change project proposal.pptx
Green Warriors Change project proposal.pptxpeaceadejumobi
 
Week 6.docxby blanca luz benavidez submission date 22 fe
Week 6.docxby blanca luz benavidez submission date 22 feWeek 6.docxby blanca luz benavidez submission date 22 fe
Week 6.docxby blanca luz benavidez submission date 22 fessuser774ad41
 
The World in 2025 - Future Agenda (2016)
The World in 2025 - Future Agenda (2016)The World in 2025 - Future Agenda (2016)
The World in 2025 - Future Agenda (2016)Tim Jones
 

Similar to Company IT Refresh and Security Costs Analyzed (20)

Informing the policymaking landscape: From research to action in the fight ag...
Informing the policymaking landscape: From research to action in the fight ag...Informing the policymaking landscape: From research to action in the fight ag...
Informing the policymaking landscape: From research to action in the fight ag...
 
Informing the policymaking landscape: From research to action in the fight ag...
Informing the policymaking landscape: From research to action in the fight ag...Informing the policymaking landscape: From research to action in the fight ag...
Informing the policymaking landscape: From research to action in the fight ag...
 
Horizontal communication and the evolution of journalism
Horizontal communication and the evolution of journalismHorizontal communication and the evolution of journalism
Horizontal communication and the evolution of journalism
 
Learning for Change_Booklet_July2015
Learning for Change_Booklet_July2015Learning for Change_Booklet_July2015
Learning for Change_Booklet_July2015
 
Essay On Human Rights Essay Writing English Ess
Essay On Human Rights Essay Writing English EssEssay On Human Rights Essay Writing English Ess
Essay On Human Rights Essay Writing English Ess
 
Plain2013 peter levesque ikmb
Plain2013 peter levesque ikmbPlain2013 peter levesque ikmb
Plain2013 peter levesque ikmb
 
Plain2013 PL and Knowledge Mobilization P Levesque
Plain2013 PL and Knowledge Mobilization P Levesque Plain2013 PL and Knowledge Mobilization P Levesque
Plain2013 PL and Knowledge Mobilization P Levesque
 
Climate Catalyst Report 01
Climate Catalyst Report 01Climate Catalyst Report 01
Climate Catalyst Report 01
 
Sport Essay Questions In 2021 Essay Writing, Essa
Sport Essay Questions In 2021 Essay Writing, EssaSport Essay Questions In 2021 Essay Writing, Essa
Sport Essay Questions In 2021 Essay Writing, Essa
 
[Final] Act Globally
[Final] Act Globally[Final] Act Globally
[Final] Act Globally
 
Data For Policy Influence: How to Manage, Distribute, and Present Your Data
Data For Policy Influence: How to Manage, Distribute, and Present Your DataData For Policy Influence: How to Manage, Distribute, and Present Your Data
Data For Policy Influence: How to Manage, Distribute, and Present Your Data
 
Research Evaluation in an Open Science context
Research Evaluation in an Open Science contextResearch Evaluation in an Open Science context
Research Evaluation in an Open Science context
 
FSCI Data management and data sharing
FSCI Data management and data sharingFSCI Data management and data sharing
FSCI Data management and data sharing
 
Open Data in a Global Ecosystem
Open Data in a Global EcosystemOpen Data in a Global Ecosystem
Open Data in a Global Ecosystem
 
Navigating Continual Disruption . richard Adler
Navigating Continual Disruption . richard AdlerNavigating Continual Disruption . richard Adler
Navigating Continual Disruption . richard Adler
 
Green Warriors Change project proposal.pptx
Green Warriors Change project proposal.pptxGreen Warriors Change project proposal.pptx
Green Warriors Change project proposal.pptx
 
Week 6.docxby blanca luz benavidez submission date 22 fe
Week 6.docxby blanca luz benavidez submission date 22 feWeek 6.docxby blanca luz benavidez submission date 22 fe
Week 6.docxby blanca luz benavidez submission date 22 fe
 
Professor Bryan Edwards - Data for Security and Resilience
Professor Bryan Edwards - Data for Security and ResilienceProfessor Bryan Edwards - Data for Security and Resilience
Professor Bryan Edwards - Data for Security and Resilience
 
WE_scenarios_study_online.pdf
WE_scenarios_study_online.pdfWE_scenarios_study_online.pdf
WE_scenarios_study_online.pdf
 
The World in 2025 - Future Agenda (2016)
The World in 2025 - Future Agenda (2016)The World in 2025 - Future Agenda (2016)
The World in 2025 - Future Agenda (2016)
 

More from mccormicknadine86

Option #2Researching a Leader Complete preliminary rese.docx
Option #2Researching a Leader Complete preliminary rese.docxOption #2Researching a Leader Complete preliminary rese.docx
Option #2Researching a Leader Complete preliminary rese.docxmccormicknadine86
 
Option 1 ImperialismThe exploitation of  colonial resources.docx
Option 1 ImperialismThe exploitation of  colonial resources.docxOption 1 ImperialismThe exploitation of  colonial resources.docx
Option 1 ImperialismThe exploitation of  colonial resources.docxmccormicknadine86
 
Option Wireless LTD v. OpenPeak, Inc.Be sure to save an elec.docx
Option Wireless LTD v. OpenPeak, Inc.Be sure to save an elec.docxOption Wireless LTD v. OpenPeak, Inc.Be sure to save an elec.docx
Option Wireless LTD v. OpenPeak, Inc.Be sure to save an elec.docxmccormicknadine86
 
Option A Land SharkWhen is a shark just a shark Consider the.docx
Option A Land SharkWhen is a shark just a shark Consider the.docxOption A Land SharkWhen is a shark just a shark Consider the.docx
Option A Land SharkWhen is a shark just a shark Consider the.docxmccormicknadine86
 
Option 3 Discuss your thoughts on drugs and deviance. Do you think .docx
Option 3 Discuss your thoughts on drugs and deviance. Do you think .docxOption 3 Discuss your thoughts on drugs and deviance. Do you think .docx
Option 3 Discuss your thoughts on drugs and deviance. Do you think .docxmccormicknadine86
 
OPTION 2 Can we make the changes we need to make After the pandemi.docx
OPTION 2 Can we make the changes we need to make After the pandemi.docxOPTION 2 Can we make the changes we need to make After the pandemi.docx
OPTION 2 Can we make the changes we need to make After the pandemi.docxmccormicknadine86
 
Option 1 You will create a PowerPoint (or equivalent) of your p.docx
Option 1 You will create a PowerPoint (or equivalent) of your p.docxOption 1 You will create a PowerPoint (or equivalent) of your p.docx
Option 1 You will create a PowerPoint (or equivalent) of your p.docxmccormicknadine86
 
Option A Description of Dance StylesSelect two styles of danc.docx
Option A Description of Dance StylesSelect two styles of danc.docxOption A Description of Dance StylesSelect two styles of danc.docx
Option A Description of Dance StylesSelect two styles of danc.docxmccormicknadine86
 
Option #2Provide several slides that explain the key section.docx
Option #2Provide several slides that explain the key section.docxOption #2Provide several slides that explain the key section.docx
Option #2Provide several slides that explain the key section.docxmccormicknadine86
 
Option 2 Slavery vs. Indentured ServitudeExplain how and wh.docx
Option 2 Slavery vs. Indentured ServitudeExplain how and wh.docxOption 2 Slavery vs. Indentured ServitudeExplain how and wh.docx
Option 2 Slavery vs. Indentured ServitudeExplain how and wh.docxmccormicknadine86
 
Option 2 ArtSelect any 2 of works of art about the Holocaus.docx
Option 2 ArtSelect any 2 of works of art about the Holocaus.docxOption 2 ArtSelect any 2 of works of art about the Holocaus.docx
Option 2 ArtSelect any 2 of works of art about the Holocaus.docxmccormicknadine86
 
Option #1 Stanford University Prison Experiment Causality, C.docx
Option #1 Stanford University Prison Experiment Causality, C.docxOption #1 Stanford University Prison Experiment Causality, C.docx
Option #1 Stanford University Prison Experiment Causality, C.docxmccormicknadine86
 
Option A  Gender CrimesCriminal acts occur against individu.docx
Option A  Gender CrimesCriminal acts occur against individu.docxOption A  Gender CrimesCriminal acts occur against individu.docx
Option A  Gender CrimesCriminal acts occur against individu.docxmccormicknadine86
 
opic 4 Discussion Question 1 May students express religious bel.docx
opic 4 Discussion Question 1 May students express religious bel.docxopic 4 Discussion Question 1 May students express religious bel.docx
opic 4 Discussion Question 1 May students express religious bel.docxmccormicknadine86
 
Option 1Choose a philosopher who interests you. Research that p.docx
Option 1Choose a philosopher who interests you. Research that p.docxOption 1Choose a philosopher who interests you. Research that p.docx
Option 1Choose a philosopher who interests you. Research that p.docxmccormicknadine86
 
Option #1The Stanford University Prison Experiment Structu.docx
Option #1The Stanford University Prison Experiment Structu.docxOption #1The Stanford University Prison Experiment Structu.docx
Option #1The Stanford University Prison Experiment Structu.docxmccormicknadine86
 
Operationaland Organizational SecurityChapter 3Princ.docx
Operationaland Organizational SecurityChapter 3Princ.docxOperationaland Organizational SecurityChapter 3Princ.docx
Operationaland Organizational SecurityChapter 3Princ.docxmccormicknadine86
 
Open the file (Undergrad Reqt_Individual In-Depth Case Study) for in.docx
Open the file (Undergrad Reqt_Individual In-Depth Case Study) for in.docxOpen the file (Undergrad Reqt_Individual In-Depth Case Study) for in.docx
Open the file (Undergrad Reqt_Individual In-Depth Case Study) for in.docxmccormicknadine86
 
onsider whether you think means-tested programs, such as the Tem.docx
onsider whether you think means-tested programs, such as the Tem.docxonsider whether you think means-tested programs, such as the Tem.docx
onsider whether you think means-tested programs, such as the Tem.docxmccormicknadine86
 
Operations security - PPT should cover below questions (chapter 1 to.docx
Operations security - PPT should cover below questions (chapter 1 to.docxOperations security - PPT should cover below questions (chapter 1 to.docx
Operations security - PPT should cover below questions (chapter 1 to.docxmccormicknadine86
 

More from mccormicknadine86 (20)

Option #2Researching a Leader Complete preliminary rese.docx
Option #2Researching a Leader Complete preliminary rese.docxOption #2Researching a Leader Complete preliminary rese.docx
Option #2Researching a Leader Complete preliminary rese.docx
 
Option 1 ImperialismThe exploitation of  colonial resources.docx
Option 1 ImperialismThe exploitation of  colonial resources.docxOption 1 ImperialismThe exploitation of  colonial resources.docx
Option 1 ImperialismThe exploitation of  colonial resources.docx
 
Option Wireless LTD v. OpenPeak, Inc.Be sure to save an elec.docx
Option Wireless LTD v. OpenPeak, Inc.Be sure to save an elec.docxOption Wireless LTD v. OpenPeak, Inc.Be sure to save an elec.docx
Option Wireless LTD v. OpenPeak, Inc.Be sure to save an elec.docx
 
Option A Land SharkWhen is a shark just a shark Consider the.docx
Option A Land SharkWhen is a shark just a shark Consider the.docxOption A Land SharkWhen is a shark just a shark Consider the.docx
Option A Land SharkWhen is a shark just a shark Consider the.docx
 
Option 3 Discuss your thoughts on drugs and deviance. Do you think .docx
Option 3 Discuss your thoughts on drugs and deviance. Do you think .docxOption 3 Discuss your thoughts on drugs and deviance. Do you think .docx
Option 3 Discuss your thoughts on drugs and deviance. Do you think .docx
 
OPTION 2 Can we make the changes we need to make After the pandemi.docx
OPTION 2 Can we make the changes we need to make After the pandemi.docxOPTION 2 Can we make the changes we need to make After the pandemi.docx
OPTION 2 Can we make the changes we need to make After the pandemi.docx
 
Option 1 You will create a PowerPoint (or equivalent) of your p.docx
Option 1 You will create a PowerPoint (or equivalent) of your p.docxOption 1 You will create a PowerPoint (or equivalent) of your p.docx
Option 1 You will create a PowerPoint (or equivalent) of your p.docx
 
Option A Description of Dance StylesSelect two styles of danc.docx
Option A Description of Dance StylesSelect two styles of danc.docxOption A Description of Dance StylesSelect two styles of danc.docx
Option A Description of Dance StylesSelect two styles of danc.docx
 
Option #2Provide several slides that explain the key section.docx
Option #2Provide several slides that explain the key section.docxOption #2Provide several slides that explain the key section.docx
Option #2Provide several slides that explain the key section.docx
 
Option 2 Slavery vs. Indentured ServitudeExplain how and wh.docx
Option 2 Slavery vs. Indentured ServitudeExplain how and wh.docxOption 2 Slavery vs. Indentured ServitudeExplain how and wh.docx
Option 2 Slavery vs. Indentured ServitudeExplain how and wh.docx
 
Option 2 ArtSelect any 2 of works of art about the Holocaus.docx
Option 2 ArtSelect any 2 of works of art about the Holocaus.docxOption 2 ArtSelect any 2 of works of art about the Holocaus.docx
Option 2 ArtSelect any 2 of works of art about the Holocaus.docx
 
Option #1 Stanford University Prison Experiment Causality, C.docx
Option #1 Stanford University Prison Experiment Causality, C.docxOption #1 Stanford University Prison Experiment Causality, C.docx
Option #1 Stanford University Prison Experiment Causality, C.docx
 
Option A  Gender CrimesCriminal acts occur against individu.docx
Option A  Gender CrimesCriminal acts occur against individu.docxOption A  Gender CrimesCriminal acts occur against individu.docx
Option A  Gender CrimesCriminal acts occur against individu.docx
 
opic 4 Discussion Question 1 May students express religious bel.docx
opic 4 Discussion Question 1 May students express religious bel.docxopic 4 Discussion Question 1 May students express religious bel.docx
opic 4 Discussion Question 1 May students express religious bel.docx
 
Option 1Choose a philosopher who interests you. Research that p.docx
Option 1Choose a philosopher who interests you. Research that p.docxOption 1Choose a philosopher who interests you. Research that p.docx
Option 1Choose a philosopher who interests you. Research that p.docx
 
Option #1The Stanford University Prison Experiment Structu.docx
Option #1The Stanford University Prison Experiment Structu.docxOption #1The Stanford University Prison Experiment Structu.docx
Option #1The Stanford University Prison Experiment Structu.docx
 
Operationaland Organizational SecurityChapter 3Princ.docx
Operationaland Organizational SecurityChapter 3Princ.docxOperationaland Organizational SecurityChapter 3Princ.docx
Operationaland Organizational SecurityChapter 3Princ.docx
 
Open the file (Undergrad Reqt_Individual In-Depth Case Study) for in.docx
Open the file (Undergrad Reqt_Individual In-Depth Case Study) for in.docxOpen the file (Undergrad Reqt_Individual In-Depth Case Study) for in.docx
Open the file (Undergrad Reqt_Individual In-Depth Case Study) for in.docx
 
onsider whether you think means-tested programs, such as the Tem.docx
onsider whether you think means-tested programs, such as the Tem.docxonsider whether you think means-tested programs, such as the Tem.docx
onsider whether you think means-tested programs, such as the Tem.docx
 
Operations security - PPT should cover below questions (chapter 1 to.docx
Operations security - PPT should cover below questions (chapter 1 to.docxOperations security - PPT should cover below questions (chapter 1 to.docx
Operations security - PPT should cover below questions (chapter 1 to.docx
 

Recently uploaded

Beyond the EU: DORA and NIS 2 Directive's Global Impact
Beyond the EU: DORA and NIS 2 Directive's Global ImpactBeyond the EU: DORA and NIS 2 Directive's Global Impact
Beyond the EU: DORA and NIS 2 Directive's Global ImpactPECB
 
mini mental status format.docx
mini    mental       status     format.docxmini    mental       status     format.docx
mini mental status format.docxPoojaSen20
 
18-04-UA_REPORT_MEDIALITERAСY_INDEX-DM_23-1-final-eng.pdf
18-04-UA_REPORT_MEDIALITERAСY_INDEX-DM_23-1-final-eng.pdf18-04-UA_REPORT_MEDIALITERAСY_INDEX-DM_23-1-final-eng.pdf
18-04-UA_REPORT_MEDIALITERAСY_INDEX-DM_23-1-final-eng.pdfssuser54595a
 
Industrial Policy - 1948, 1956, 1973, 1977, 1980, 1991
Industrial Policy - 1948, 1956, 1973, 1977, 1980, 1991Industrial Policy - 1948, 1956, 1973, 1977, 1980, 1991
Industrial Policy - 1948, 1956, 1973, 1977, 1980, 1991RKavithamani
 
BASLIQ CURRENT LOOKBOOK LOOKBOOK(1) (1).pdf
BASLIQ CURRENT LOOKBOOK  LOOKBOOK(1) (1).pdfBASLIQ CURRENT LOOKBOOK  LOOKBOOK(1) (1).pdf
BASLIQ CURRENT LOOKBOOK LOOKBOOK(1) (1).pdfSoniaTolstoy
 
Interactive Powerpoint_How to Master effective communication
Interactive Powerpoint_How to Master effective communicationInteractive Powerpoint_How to Master effective communication
Interactive Powerpoint_How to Master effective communicationnomboosow
 
Measures of Central Tendency: Mean, Median and Mode
Measures of Central Tendency: Mean, Median and ModeMeasures of Central Tendency: Mean, Median and Mode
Measures of Central Tendency: Mean, Median and ModeThiyagu K
 
SOCIAL AND HISTORICAL CONTEXT - LFTVD.pptx
SOCIAL AND HISTORICAL CONTEXT - LFTVD.pptxSOCIAL AND HISTORICAL CONTEXT - LFTVD.pptx
SOCIAL AND HISTORICAL CONTEXT - LFTVD.pptxiammrhaywood
 
The Most Excellent Way | 1 Corinthians 13
The Most Excellent Way | 1 Corinthians 13The Most Excellent Way | 1 Corinthians 13
The Most Excellent Way | 1 Corinthians 13Steve Thomason
 
Introduction to ArtificiaI Intelligence in Higher Education
Introduction to ArtificiaI Intelligence in Higher EducationIntroduction to ArtificiaI Intelligence in Higher Education
Introduction to ArtificiaI Intelligence in Higher Educationpboyjonauth
 
URLs and Routing in the Odoo 17 Website App
URLs and Routing in the Odoo 17 Website AppURLs and Routing in the Odoo 17 Website App
URLs and Routing in the Odoo 17 Website AppCeline George
 
Kisan Call Centre - To harness potential of ICT in Agriculture by answer farm...
Kisan Call Centre - To harness potential of ICT in Agriculture by answer farm...Kisan Call Centre - To harness potential of ICT in Agriculture by answer farm...
Kisan Call Centre - To harness potential of ICT in Agriculture by answer farm...Krashi Coaching
 
Paris 2024 Olympic Geographies - an activity
Paris 2024 Olympic Geographies - an activityParis 2024 Olympic Geographies - an activity
Paris 2024 Olympic Geographies - an activityGeoBlogs
 
Accessible design: Minimum effort, maximum impact
Accessible design: Minimum effort, maximum impactAccessible design: Minimum effort, maximum impact
Accessible design: Minimum effort, maximum impactdawncurless
 
Introduction to AI in Higher Education_draft.pptx
Introduction to AI in Higher Education_draft.pptxIntroduction to AI in Higher Education_draft.pptx
Introduction to AI in Higher Education_draft.pptxpboyjonauth
 
Mastering the Unannounced Regulatory Inspection
Mastering the Unannounced Regulatory InspectionMastering the Unannounced Regulatory Inspection
Mastering the Unannounced Regulatory InspectionSafetyChain Software
 
Arihant handbook biology for class 11 .pdf
Arihant handbook biology for class 11 .pdfArihant handbook biology for class 11 .pdf
Arihant handbook biology for class 11 .pdfchloefrazer622
 
CARE OF CHILD IN INCUBATOR..........pptx
CARE OF CHILD IN INCUBATOR..........pptxCARE OF CHILD IN INCUBATOR..........pptx
CARE OF CHILD IN INCUBATOR..........pptxGaneshChakor2
 

Recently uploaded (20)

Beyond the EU: DORA and NIS 2 Directive's Global Impact
Beyond the EU: DORA and NIS 2 Directive's Global ImpactBeyond the EU: DORA and NIS 2 Directive's Global Impact
Beyond the EU: DORA and NIS 2 Directive's Global Impact
 
mini mental status format.docx
mini    mental       status     format.docxmini    mental       status     format.docx
mini mental status format.docx
 
18-04-UA_REPORT_MEDIALITERAСY_INDEX-DM_23-1-final-eng.pdf
18-04-UA_REPORT_MEDIALITERAСY_INDEX-DM_23-1-final-eng.pdf18-04-UA_REPORT_MEDIALITERAСY_INDEX-DM_23-1-final-eng.pdf
18-04-UA_REPORT_MEDIALITERAСY_INDEX-DM_23-1-final-eng.pdf
 
Industrial Policy - 1948, 1956, 1973, 1977, 1980, 1991
Industrial Policy - 1948, 1956, 1973, 1977, 1980, 1991Industrial Policy - 1948, 1956, 1973, 1977, 1980, 1991
Industrial Policy - 1948, 1956, 1973, 1977, 1980, 1991
 
BASLIQ CURRENT LOOKBOOK LOOKBOOK(1) (1).pdf
BASLIQ CURRENT LOOKBOOK  LOOKBOOK(1) (1).pdfBASLIQ CURRENT LOOKBOOK  LOOKBOOK(1) (1).pdf
BASLIQ CURRENT LOOKBOOK LOOKBOOK(1) (1).pdf
 
Interactive Powerpoint_How to Master effective communication
Interactive Powerpoint_How to Master effective communicationInteractive Powerpoint_How to Master effective communication
Interactive Powerpoint_How to Master effective communication
 
Measures of Central Tendency: Mean, Median and Mode
Measures of Central Tendency: Mean, Median and ModeMeasures of Central Tendency: Mean, Median and Mode
Measures of Central Tendency: Mean, Median and Mode
 
SOCIAL AND HISTORICAL CONTEXT - LFTVD.pptx
SOCIAL AND HISTORICAL CONTEXT - LFTVD.pptxSOCIAL AND HISTORICAL CONTEXT - LFTVD.pptx
SOCIAL AND HISTORICAL CONTEXT - LFTVD.pptx
 
The Most Excellent Way | 1 Corinthians 13
The Most Excellent Way | 1 Corinthians 13The Most Excellent Way | 1 Corinthians 13
The Most Excellent Way | 1 Corinthians 13
 
Introduction to ArtificiaI Intelligence in Higher Education
Introduction to ArtificiaI Intelligence in Higher EducationIntroduction to ArtificiaI Intelligence in Higher Education
Introduction to ArtificiaI Intelligence in Higher Education
 
URLs and Routing in the Odoo 17 Website App
URLs and Routing in the Odoo 17 Website AppURLs and Routing in the Odoo 17 Website App
URLs and Routing in the Odoo 17 Website App
 
Kisan Call Centre - To harness potential of ICT in Agriculture by answer farm...
Kisan Call Centre - To harness potential of ICT in Agriculture by answer farm...Kisan Call Centre - To harness potential of ICT in Agriculture by answer farm...
Kisan Call Centre - To harness potential of ICT in Agriculture by answer farm...
 
Paris 2024 Olympic Geographies - an activity
Paris 2024 Olympic Geographies - an activityParis 2024 Olympic Geographies - an activity
Paris 2024 Olympic Geographies - an activity
 
Accessible design: Minimum effort, maximum impact
Accessible design: Minimum effort, maximum impactAccessible design: Minimum effort, maximum impact
Accessible design: Minimum effort, maximum impact
 
Introduction to AI in Higher Education_draft.pptx
Introduction to AI in Higher Education_draft.pptxIntroduction to AI in Higher Education_draft.pptx
Introduction to AI in Higher Education_draft.pptx
 
Mastering the Unannounced Regulatory Inspection
Mastering the Unannounced Regulatory InspectionMastering the Unannounced Regulatory Inspection
Mastering the Unannounced Regulatory Inspection
 
Mattingly "AI & Prompt Design: Structured Data, Assistants, & RAG"
Mattingly "AI & Prompt Design: Structured Data, Assistants, & RAG"Mattingly "AI & Prompt Design: Structured Data, Assistants, & RAG"
Mattingly "AI & Prompt Design: Structured Data, Assistants, & RAG"
 
Arihant handbook biology for class 11 .pdf
Arihant handbook biology for class 11 .pdfArihant handbook biology for class 11 .pdf
Arihant handbook biology for class 11 .pdf
 
Código Creativo y Arte de Software | Unidad 1
Código Creativo y Arte de Software | Unidad 1Código Creativo y Arte de Software | Unidad 1
Código Creativo y Arte de Software | Unidad 1
 
CARE OF CHILD IN INCUBATOR..........pptx
CARE OF CHILD IN INCUBATOR..........pptxCARE OF CHILD IN INCUBATOR..........pptx
CARE OF CHILD IN INCUBATOR..........pptx
 

Company IT Refresh and Security Costs Analyzed

  • 1. Company Highlights ● The organization currently maintains 2,000 Microsoft Windows desktop computers for the workforce. As you recall, these desktop computers were about to be refreshed. The organization is currently considering the Dell Optiplex series. Specifically, each computer would have a Intel Core i5 processor, run Microsoft Windows 10, contain 8GB DDR4 RAM, and have a 256GB solid state drive. The rough estimate for each PC is $1,250, which equates to a $2,500,000 upgrade cost to the organization. ● Microsoft Office runs on each of these computers. The license fee for each computer is $150, which equates to a $300,000 outlay. The organization typically upgrades all PCs with the latest version of Microsoft Office when it is released. ● The organization used multiples instances of Oracle Database. These databases are distributed across 10 Dell PowerEdge servers. These servers cost a minimum of $2,159 each, for a total cost of $21,580. ● The organization maintains an additional 20 Dell PowerEdge servers for a variety of reasons to include email, file backup, remote access, etc. The
  • 2. total cost to refresh each of these servers is $3,000 for a total cost of $60,000. ● The organization maintains various networking devices. The total cost of this equipment is $3,000,000 and yearly refresh costs are $750,000. ● The organization maintains a suite of network security hardware and software solutions. They include a DMZ configuration, multiple firewalls, VPN hardware, antivirus solutions, intrusion detection and protection devices, and log management solutions. The total yearly hardware and software costs for the organization cybersecurity efforts are $2,000,000. ● The organization also allows users to access the network via their company-issued Android phones, or iPads. Employees receive a mobile device with VPN software installed so that they may remotely access company systems when they are at home, or traveling. ● Because this is a notional example and assignment, you may make further assumptions about the organization IT environment. There is no need to ask your instructor for permission. Simply make explicit your assumptions in the white paper. For example, you are free to assume the number of IT professionals, their specialties, and more importantly the cost to run the IT department.
  • 3. RESEARCH ARTICLE The public’s belief in climate change and its human cause are increasing over time Taciano L. Milfont 1*, Marc S. Wilson1, Chris G. Sibley2 1 School of Psychology, Victoria University of Wellington, Wellington, New Zealand, 2 School of Psychology, University of Auckland, Auckland, New Zealand * [email protected] Abstract Polls examining public opinion on the subject of climate change are now commonplace, and one-off public opinion polls provide a snapshot of citizen’s opinions that can inform policy and communication strategies. However, cross-sectional polls do not track opinions over time, thus making it impossible to ascertain whether key climate change beliefs held by the same group of individuals are changing or not. Here we examine the extent to which indivi- dual’s level of agreement with two key beliefs ("climate change
  • 4. is real" and "climate change is caused by humans") remain stable or increase/decrease over a six-year period in New Zealand using latent growth curve modelling (n = 10,436). Data were drawn from the New Zealand Attitudes and Values Study, a probabilistic national panel study, and indicated that levels of agreement to both beliefs have steadily increased over the 2009–2015 period. Given that climate change beliefs and concerns are key predictors of climate change action, our findings suggest that a combination of targeted endeavors, as well as serendipitous events, may successfully convey the emergency of the issue. Introduction In 2015, Pope Francis encouraged Catholics worldwide to be attentive to global climate change in his encyclical Laudato si’. More recently, Leonardo DiCaprio was allowed a longer-than- average Oscar acceptance speech to similarly raise worldwide attention to climate change, while leaders and representatives from over 150 countries gathered to sign the global climate change agreement reached in Paris in December, 2015. One
  • 5. question that arises from these international examples of public figures trying to promote awareness and action on climate change is whether they reflect and/or influence a popular upswing in climate change beliefs and concerns over time. Although perceptions of climate change threat and beliefs vary internationally [1], opinion polls indicate that an overall global majority believe in the reality and human causation of cli- mate change [2–4]. In particular, short-term longitudinal panel studies that have examined the effect of factors such as popular media interventions or extreme weather events on climate change concern suggest that individuals’ attitudes may change after exposure to such events. For example, a field experiment investigating impacts of the provision of climate information through TV meteorologists (presented on air and online) indicated that viewers were both PLOS ONE | https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0174246 March 20, 2017 1 / 9 a1111111111
  • 6. a1111111111 a1111111111 a1111111111 a1111111111 OPEN ACCESS Citation: Milfont TL, Wilson MS, Sibley CG (2017) The public’s belief in climate change and its human cause are increasing over time. PLoS ONE 12(3): e0174246. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal. pone.0174246 Editor: Stefan Lötters, Universitat Trier, GERMANY Received: May 25, 2016 Accepted: March 6, 2017 Published: March 20, 2017 Copyright: © 2017 Milfont et al. This is an open access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License, which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original
  • 7. author and source are credited. Data Availability Statement: Data cannot be made available due to ethical restrictions imposed by University of Auckland Human Participants Ethics Committee. A de-identified dataset is available upon request from the corresponding author for all appropriately qualified researchers. A de-identified dataset is also available from any member of the advisory board of the New Zealand Attitudes and Values Study (www.nzvalues.org). The University of Auckland Human Participants Ethics Committee can be contacted at [email protected] Funding: This research was supported by a Templeton World Charity Foundation grant (http:// https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0174246 http://crossmark.crossref.org/dialog/?doi=10.1371/journal.pone. 0174246&domain=pdf&date_stamp=2017-03-20 http://crossmark.crossref.org/dialog/?doi=10.1371/journal.pone. 0174246&domain=pdf&date_stamp=2017-03-20 http://crossmark.crossref.org/dialog/?doi=10.1371/journal.pone. 0174246&domain=pdf&date_stamp=2017-03-20 http://crossmark.crossref.org/dialog/?doi=10.1371/journal.pone.
  • 8. 0174246&domain=pdf&date_stamp=2017-03-20 http://crossmark.crossref.org/dialog/?doi=10.1371/journal.pone. 0174246&domain=pdf&date_stamp=2017-03-20 http://crossmark.crossref.org/dialog/?doi=10.1371/journal.pone. 0174246&domain=pdf&date_stamp=2017-03-20 https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0174246 https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0174246 http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/ http://www.nzvalues.org mailto:[email protected] http://www.templetonworldcharity.org/ more concerned and more likely to perceive scientific consensus than others [5]. Another study found that climate change concern was greater among UK respondents who have had first-hand experience with floods ([6]; see also [7]). In contrast, little change in climate change attitudes were observed pre- and post-2012 Midwestern US drought [8], and UK viewers of the climate change documentary The Age of Stupid initially reported increased concern, action intention and efficacy but these effects deteriorated over time [9]. Although inconclusive, this emerging literature suggests that researchers and policy makers would benefit by knowing whether citizens are becoming more or less credulous of climate
  • 9. change today than previously, and also the rate of change in citizens’ belief in climate change. Public opinion polling on the subject of climate change concern and beliefs is now common- place, and results from such one-off surveys are useful in providing a snapshot of the level of agreement and/or support to climate-related questions and issues from a given population. However, such cohort-based findings are limited in the extent to which researchers and practi- tioners can draw inferences about the nature, and causes, of changes in attitude or belief. To illustrate, while results suggest that climate change concern among Americans increased sig- nificantly during the 1980s, more recent polls suggest that fewer American citizens consider climate change to be a threat than only a few years prior [2]. Since these studies base their con- clusions on distinct population cohorts, we do not have a particularly robust understanding of whether, and how much, beliefs about global climate change are indeed changing over time. Thanks to the ubiquity of public opinion polls, we also know that perception of climate change
  • 10. threat varies internationally [1] and fluctuates over time [2, 10]. Studies assessing climate-relevant questions longitudinally are better equipped than cross- sectional public opinion polls to provide information regarding temporal change in climate change attitudes and beliefs. However, there are relatively few studies assessing climate-rele- vant attitudes in the same panel longitudinally, and very few indeed that do so over more than a year, or with a representative sample. One study assessed a national sample of 269 New Zealanders’ responses on climate-related questions three times over a one-year period [11], reporting that concern about global warm- ing and climate change did not increase over this duration— perhaps due to the limited time frame—but that higher levels of climate-related knowledge increased climate change concern, which in turn translated into greater subjective environmental responsibility. Another study assessed a nationally representative sample of US citizens about climate belief and personal experience of climate and weather, assessed in 2008 and 2011 [12]. Results showed that per-
  • 11. sonal experience of climate change-affected weather increased climate change belief and cer- tainty, but also that high certainty also predicted subjective experience—strong climate change skeptics and believers tended to interpret subjective weather experience consistent with their belief. Another large scale study with a longitudinal component reported that a majority of Aus- tralians believe climate change is occurring, that climate change is of lesser priority than other personal concerns, and that Australians were poor predictors of others’ attitudes; but longitu- dinal analyses are not reported excepting in reference to absolute levels of attitude change over time [13]. Another longitudinal study conducted in Australian provides a qualitative analysis of perceptions of climate and climate change, emphasising that for many people the notion of “natural cycles” was an important common sense idea that served to provide an appreciation of an understandable dynamic relationship between humans and their environment [14].
  • 12. Despite the increase in the number of studies examining change in climate change-related variables across time considering the same population, none of the available studies are both representative or of a sufficiently large scale to draw and report robust inferences of population change. In response to these challenges, we present the first longitudinal analysis of key climate Changes in climate change beliefs over time PLOS ONE | https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0174246 March 20, 2017 2 / 9 www.templetonworldcharity.org/; ID: 0077) to CGS and a Marsden Fast-Start grant (E1908) from The Royal Society of New Zealand (http://www. royalsociety.org.nz/programmes/funds/marsden/) to TLM. The funders had no role in study design, data collection and analysis, decision to publish, or preparation of the manuscript. Competing interests: The authors have declared that no competing interests exist. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0174246
  • 13. http://www.templetonworldcharity.org/ http://www.royalsociety.org.nz/programmes/funds/marsden/ http://www.royalsociety.org.nz/programmes/funds/marsden/ change beliefs with a large nationally representative sample assessed over a six-year period (from 2009 to 2015). The aim of this research is to present an initial flavour of the extent and rate of change of climate change beliefs. We analyse the first six waves of longitudinal data from the New Zealand Attitudes and Values Study comprising 10,436 respondents who com- pleted at least three of the waves of annual data collection. Participants indicated their level of agreement to one question examining the belief in the reality of climate change and another question examining belief in anthropogenic climate change. Methods Sampling procedure This study analysed data from the New Zealand Attitudes and Values Study (NZAVS), which is is an annual, longitudinal national probability study that has been assessing people’s socio- political attitudes annually since 2009. Invitations to participate
  • 14. in a mail-based survey were sent to people randomly sampled from the New Zealand Electoral Roll. Participants were posted a copy of the questionnaire, with a second postal follow- up two months later. Partici- pants who provided an email address in a previous wave were also emailed and invited to com- plete an online version if they preferred. The NZAVS is reviewed every three years by the University of Auckland Human Partici- pants Ethics Committee. The first phases of the longitudinal study were approved on 09-Sep- tember-2009 for 3 years (reference number: 2009/336). Ethics approval for the study was re- approved by the University of Auckland Human Participants Ethics Committee on 17-Febru- ary-2012 until 09-September-2015 (reference number: 6171), and then on 03-June-2015 until 03-June-2018 (reference number: 014889). All participants granted informed written consent. Contact details are removed when the questionnaires are received, and all data were de-identi- fied before analyses were conducted. NZAVS data is hosted at the University of Auckland, and
  • 15. the de-identified data is available to appropriately qualified researchers upon request for the purposes of re-analysis. We describe in more detail the sampling procedure for each of the waves used in this study in S1 Supporting Information (see also [15]). Participants Table 1 presents sample size and characteristics for each of the survey waves (see also S1 Sup- porting Information). The present analysis included only participants who responded to three or more waves. Missing data among those completing at least three waves was estimated using Full Information Maximum Likelihood and assuming that data were missing at random. This assumption seems reasonable given that analysis of panel attrition indicates that demographic factors are only weakly predictive of dropout in the NZAVS [15]; however, people for whom English is not a first language are more likely to drop out of the study relative to other groups. This method of missing data estimation weighted each individual-level trajectory based on its
  • 16. reliability, which is in turn a function that takes the number of observations into account. Those who responded to all six-time points contributed the most information to the estima- tion of the mean-level trajectory, while those who responded to three contributed the least. Syntax for our Mplus models are available on the NZAVS website: www.psych.auckland.ac.nz/ uoa/NZAVS Measures The two NZAVS questions analysed in the present article (i.e., “Climate change is real” and “Climate change is caused by humans”) were embedded in a large battery of Likert-type Changes in climate change beliefs over time PLOS ONE | https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0174246 March 20, 2017 3 / 9 http://www.psych.auckland.ac.nz/uoa/NZAVS http://www.psych.auckland.ac.nz/uoa/NZAVS https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0174246 questions. Across the first six waves of the NZAVS respondents expressed their levels of dis- agreement-agreement to these questions on a 7-point answer
  • 17. scale with end-labels only: 1 (strongly disagree) and 7 (strongly agree). Table 1 presents the mean and standard deviation for the agreement levels of these questions, as well as the frequency distribution of the answers. Analysis We simultaneously examined respondents’ level of agreement to both climate change beliefs across time using a parallel process Latent Growth Model. This technique has many advan- tages when compared to other longitudinal data-analytic techniques [16]. Some of the advan- tages include the ability to estimate reference levels (or initial amount; intercept) and their developmental trajectories to and from those levels (slope); the ability to estimate change in latent variables as the technique is a special case of structural equation modeling; the ability to estimate means, variances and covariances of individuals differences in both intercepts and slopes; and the technique does not assume that all individuals in a particular longitudinal proj- ect change over time at the same rate. We estimated the rate of change in levels of disagreement- agreement with the two climate
  • 18. change belief items over the October 2009 –October 2015 period. We modelled the intercepts, and linear and quadratic growth factors for these two aspects of climate belief as parallel pro- cesses, and allowed the intercepts and slopes to correlate with one another. To reflect the variation in response time to each wave between individuals, response time at each wave was then converted into yearly units and modelled as individually time-varying effects, with time = 0 being the 30th June, 2009. This accounted for the fact that different indi- viduals completed the surveys at different times throughout the year, and with different dura- tions between each assessment, rather than fixing all responses in 2009 to 1, all responses in 2010 to 2, and so forth. Our models therefore provide a latent intercept representing estimated mean climate change belief at 30th June, 2009. Residual variances, or disturbances, in the man- ifest ratings of both Likert items were constrained to equality across waves. This therefore Table 1. Distribution of responses to the questions “Climate change is real” (reality) and “Climate change is caused by humans” (cause) in the
  • 19. first six waves of the New Zealand Attitudes and Values Study, with descriptive statistics for each question at each wave. Response options Year Question 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 M SD N strongly disagree strongly agree 2009 Reality 4% 4% 6% 14% 15% 24% 33% 5.34 1.69 4630 Cause 6 7 9 19 19 22 17 4.74 1.74 4591 2010 Reality 4 5 6 15 17 26 28 5.26 1.65 4022 Cause 7 7 9 20 20 21 17 4.70 1.74 3993 2011 Reality 3 4 5 15 18 24 31 5.37 1.62 6177 Cause 6 7 8 19 20 22 17 4.75 1.74 6127 2012 Reality 3 3 5 13 16 26 34 5.45 1.61 8638 Cause 5 6 9 18 21 22 19 4.84 1.70 8592 2013 Reality 3 3 4 12 15 24 37 5.56 1.58 9188 Cause 5 6 7 17 19 24 23 5.02 1.69 9172 2014 Reality 2 2 4 11 17 24 40 5.72 1.44 8928 Cause 3 4 6 17 20 23 26 5.20 1.61 8889 Note: Distributions are based on valid percent, and sample sizes at each time point refer to those who completed at least three of
  • 20. the first six waves of the New Zealand Attitudes and Values Study. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0174246.t001 Changes in climate change beliefs over time PLOS ONE | https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0174246 March 20, 2017 4 / 9 https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0174246.t001 https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0174246 assumes that the amount of variation that is unexplained by the intercept and rate of change in our outcome measure is the same across waves [17]. Our model estimated the fixed and random effect for the intercept, linear and quadratic growth components spanning Waves 1–6 of data collection. The intercept represents the mean score on the outcome (disagreement-agreement levels for the climate item of interest) as of June 2009. Belief in climate change reality and human causation were then estimated every three months from October 2009 to October 2015. We integrated the parameters for the fixed effects in our models to generate the model-
  • 21. implied rate of change in climate beliefs over the October 2009–October 2015 period. The fol- lowing regression equation was used to estimate this model- implied rate of change: yt ¼ c þ st þ s 2t2 In this regression equation, yt is the predicted level of climate belief in the given question at time t, c is the intercept, or model-predicted mean level of belief when t = 0 (i.e., 30th June, 2009), s is the fixed effect for the linear growth component of the model, and s2 is the fixed effect for the quadratic growth component. Values of t are distributed in this model so that 1.0 unit represents a change of one year. We used the parameters reported in Table 2 to derive model-implied levels of disagreement-agreement that “climate change is real” and “climate change is caused by humans” for values of t ranging from .25 (October 2009) to 6.25 (October 2015) in quarterly increments. Table 2. Fixed and random effects for parallel process Latent Growth Model predicting change in disagreement to agreement (on a scale from 1 to 7) in response to key climate change questions) for New Zealand residents in the first six waves of the New Zealand Attitudes and Values Study
  • 22. over the October 2009 –October 2015 period. Model “Climate change is real” “Climate change is caused by humans” Linear Fixed effects (means) Intercept 5.244 (.019)** 4.612 (.020)** Linear growth parameter .072 (.003)** .090 (.004)** Random effects (variances) Intercept 2.246 (.059)** 2.409 (.053)** Linear growth parameter .031 (.003)** .030 (.002)** Quadratic Fixed effects (means) Intercept 5.403 (0.023)** 4.813 (0.024)** Linear growth parameter -0.061 (0.012)** -0.080 (0.013)** Quadratic growth parameter 0.020 (0.002)** 0.026 (0.002)** Random effects (variances) Intercept 2.209 (0.070)** 2.396 (0.062)** Linear growth parameter 0.262 (0.033)** 0.306 (0.031)**
  • 23. Quadratic growth parameter 0.005 (0.001)** 0.006 (0.001)** Note: N = 10,436 participants who completed at least three of the first six waves of the New Zealand Attitudes and Values Study. Participants who completed less than three of the six waves were excluded from the model. Missing data among participants who completed three or more waves were estimated using Full Information Maximum Likelihood and assuming data were missing at random. Models estimated using Maximum Likelihood with robust estimation of standard errors. Standard errors reported in parentheses. Disturbances of the manifest indicators were constrained to equality over time. Fit indices: Linear model: loglikelihood = -130590.18, AIC = 261212.36, BIC = 261328.41. loglikelihood = -130055.73, AIC = 260169.46, BIC = 260379.79. *p < .05. **p < .01. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0174246.t002 Changes in climate change beliefs over time PLOS ONE | https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0174246 March 20, 2017 5 / 9 https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0174246.t002 https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0174246
  • 24. Results Fig 1 presents the estimated rates of change in disagreement to agreement in response to the climate change belief items over the six-year period. Our results show that agreement with cli- mate change reality was higher at all time points than agreement with anthropogenic climate change. What about change in the climate change beliefs over time? Table 2 presents the results from the parallel process Latent Growth Model predicting change in disagreement to agree- ment in response to the climate change belief items. We first tested a linear growth model, and then extended this to include a second-order polynomial (quadratic) growth component. The quadratic growth component in the quadratic model was significant, and thus we focus on this model as it allowed for potential change occurring in a simple accelerating (or decelerating) fashion. Our analyses reveal a significant curvilinear increase in the average level of agreement with
  • 25. both climate change beliefs over the 2009–2015 period. This curvilinear rate of growth occurred because climate change beliefs were generally increasing over time, but in an acceler- ating fashion with the most rapid increased occurring from about 2013 onwards (see Fig 1). Fig 1. Model-implied rate of change in the level of agreement with climate change beliefs. Rate of change over the 2009–2015 period for 10,436 participants who completed at least three of the first six waves of the New Zealand Attitudes and Values Study. Ratings ranged from 1 (strongly disagree) to 7 (strongly agree), and error bars represent the 95% CI of the model-implied estimate. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0174246.g001 Changes in climate change beliefs over time PLOS ONE | https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0174246 March 20, 2017 6 / 9 https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0174246.g001 https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0174246 Moreover, although the observed increase was relatively small, the across-time increase was statistically significant for both belief in climate change reality and anthropogenic climate
  • 26. change. Our results show that from October 2009 to October 2015 respondents’ level of agree- ment about the reality of climate change increased, on average, from 5.39 to 5.82 on an agree- ment scale ranging from 1–7. Similarly, respondents’ level of agreement for anthropogenic climate change increased on average from 4.79 to 5.34 for the same six-year period. Finally, we examined whether the observed increase in average levels of agreement in cli- mate change reality correlated with the observed increase in average levels of agreement in anthropogenic climate change, and vice-versa. In the linear (baseline) model, the linear growth functions for climate change reality and anthropogenic climate change were positively associ- ated (b = .027, se = .002, z = 15.773, p < .001). The linear growth factors for reality and anthro- pogenic beliefs were also correlated in the quadratic model (b = .232, se = .020, z = 11.51, p < .001), as were the quadratic growth factors (b = .005, se = .001, z = 9.49, p < .001). These results indicate that people who tended to increase their level of agreement in one climate change
  • 27. belief also tended to increase their agreement level in the other belief. Discussion We provide the first comprehensive population-level examination of change in climate change beliefs. First, we showed that the level of agreement to the reality of climate change is greater than agreement to its human causation. This indicates a clear differentiation of the two issues among respondents regarding their belief levels: Respondents in our sample are more likely to believe in the reality of climate change than whether or not climate change is caused by humans. Perhaps believe in the reality of climate change is a precondition for believing in its human causation. This is consequential since the belief in climate change reality has been shown to have stronger effects on a number of outcome variables compared to the belief in anthropogenic climate change; moreover, these beliefs interact so that belief in climate change reality is more predictive for individuals who strongly believe in human cause of climate
  • 28. change [18]. We then showed that agreement to the reality of climate change and agreement to its human causation showed comparable (and coupled) increase over the six-year period. The observed steady increase in climate change belief over a six- year period is promising and sug- gests that people (at least those in New Zealand) are becoming increasingly aware of the reality of climate change and its human causation. We examine change spanning the 2009–2015 period, and showed that this increase in belief has been most pronounced in more recent years, from about 2013 onwards. It should also be noted that a simple linear model also fit the data fairly well, indicating that the core pattern in our data is a general and gradual increase in climate belief, with the caveat that such change may be slightly more pronounced in recent years. Moreover, that across-time increase in climate change beliefs are related is consequential given that these beliefs predict a number of important outcomes relevant for mitigation and adaptation actions [18, 19].
  • 29. Past research has examined the many predictors of climate change concern and belief [20– 24], with a recent meta-analysis demonstrating that political affiliation and political ideology are the main predictors of climate change belief [19]. Consistent with this finding, previous research in New Zealand identifies a latent profile of respondents (“climate skeptics”) with low agreement levels in both climate change reality and its human causation that had the highest levels of self-reported political conservatism [25]. Since distrust in the reality of climate change and its human cause is associated with a more conservative political orientation, it is likely that change in climate change belief will differ for distinct groups in a given society. That is to say, Changes in climate change beliefs over time PLOS ONE | https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0174246 March 20, 2017 7 / 9 https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0174246 we predict that the observed increase in climate change beliefs might be greatest among politi-
  • 30. cally liberal individuals, while small or null for their politically conservative counterparts. Due to power considerations we cannot test this prediction at present [26], but the New Zealand Attitudes and Values Study is ongoing and this is something we aim to address in the years to come. Similar to public opinion polls showing that perception of climate change threat fluctu- ates over time [2] and [10], we also expect that climate change beliefs would fluctuate over time. The ongoing nature of this longitudinal project means we will be able to pinpoint whether fluctuations on climate change beliefs is a direct result of particular socio-economic circumstances. To conclude, our findings suggest that communicating the reality and urgency of climate change—perhaps combined with media attention to the issue [27] and with communicating individual experience of climate change-related extreme weather events [5]—is successfully influencing the levels of climate change belief in New Zealand. Similar studies will be able to demonstrate whether this is also happening in other societies.
  • 31. Supporting information S1 Supporting Information. (DOCX) Author Contributions Conceptualization: TLM CGS. Data curation: CGS. Formal analysis: CGS. Funding acquisition: TLM CGS. Investigation: CGS. Methodology: CGS. Writing – original draft: MSW TLM. Writing – review & editing: TLM MSW CGS. References 1. Lee TM, Markowitz EM, Howe PD, Ko C-Y, Leiserowitz AA. Predictors of public climate change aware- ness and risk perception around the world. Nature Climate Change. 2015; 5: 1014–1020. 2. Capstick SB, Whitmarsh LE, Poortinga W, Pidgeon NF, Upham P. International trends in public percep-
  • 32. tions of climate change over the past quarter century. WIREs Climate Change. 2015; 6: 35–61. 3. Pugliese A, Ray J. Awareness of climate change and threat vary by region. 2009. Available: http://www. gallup.com/poll/124652/Awareness-Climate-Change-Threat- Vary-Region.aspx. 4. Wike R. What the world thinks about climate change in 7 charts. 2016. Available: http://www. pewresearch.org/fact-tank/2016/04/18/what-the-world-thinks- about-climate-change-in-7-charts/. 5. Zhao X, Maibach E, Gandy J, Witte J, Cullen H, Klinger BA, Rowan KE, Witte J, Pyle A. Climate change education through TV weathercasts: Results of a field experiment. Bulletin of the American Meteorologi- cal Society. 2014; 95: 117–130. 6. Spence A, Poortinga W, Pidgeon N. The psychological distance of climate change. Risk Analysis. 2012; 32: 957–972. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1539- 6924.2011.01695.x PMID: 21992607 7. Milfont TL, Evans L, Sibley CG, Ries J, Cunningham A. Proximity to coast is linked to climate change belief. PLoS ONE. 2014; 9: e103180. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0103180 PMID: 25047568 Changes in climate change beliefs over time
  • 33. PLOS ONE | https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0174246 March 20, 2017 8 / 9 http://www.plosone.org/article/fetchSingleRepresentation.action ?uri=info:doi/10.1371/journal.pone.0174246.s001 http://www.gallup.com/poll/124652/Awareness-Climate- Change-Threat-Vary-Region.aspx http://www.gallup.com/poll/124652/Awareness-Climate- Change-Threat-Vary-Region.aspx http://www.pewresearch.org/fact-tank/2016/04/18/what-the- world-thinks-about-climate-change-in-7-charts/ http://www.pewresearch.org/fact-tank/2016/04/18/what-the- world-thinks-about-climate-change-in-7-charts/ https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1539-6924.2011.01695.x http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/21992607 https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0103180 http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/25047568 https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0174246 8. Carlton JS, Mase AS, Knutson CL, Lemos MC, Haigh T, Todey DP, Prokopy LS. The effects of extreme drought on climate change beliefs, risk perceptions, and adaptation attitudes. Climate Change. 2016; 135: 211–226. 9. Howell RA. Lights, camera. . . action? Altered attitudes and behaviour in response to the climate change film The Age of Stupid. Global Environmental Change. 2011; 21: 177–187. 10. Scruggs L, Benegal S. Declining public concern about
  • 34. climate change: Can we blame the great reces- sion? Global Environmental Change. 2012; 22: 505–515. 11. Milfont TL. The interplay between knowledge, perceived efficacy and concern about global warming and climate change: A one-year longitudinal study. Risk Analysis. 2012; 32: 1003–1020. https://doi.org/ 10.1111/j.1539-6924.2012.01800.x PMID: 22489642 12. Myers T, Maibach E, Roser-Renouf C, Akerlof K, Leiserowitz A. The relationship between personal experience and belief in the reality of global warming. Nature Climate Change. 2012; 3: 343–347. 13. Leviston Z, Price J, Malkin S, McCrea R. Fourth annual survey of Australian attitudes to climate change: Interim report. 2014. Available: http://images.smh.com.au/file/2014/02/07/5139061/CSIROCC4. pdf 14. Connor L, Higginbotham N. “Natural cycles” in lay understandings of climate change. Global Environ- mental Change. 2013; 23: 1852–1861. 15. Satherley N, Milojev P, Greaves LM, Huang Y, Osborne D, Bulbulia J, et al. Demographic and psycho- logical predictors of panel attrition: evidence from the New Zealand Attitudes and Values Study. PLoS
  • 35. ONE. 2015; 10(3): e0121950. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0121950 PMID: 25793746 16. Hancock GR, Lawrence FR. Using latent growth models to evaluate longitudinal change. In: Hancock GR, Mueller RO, editors. Structural Equation Modeling: A Second Course. Greenwood, CT: Informa- tion Age Publishing, Inc; 2006. pp. 171–196. 17. Grimm KJ, Widaman KF. Residual structures in latent growth curve modeling. Structural Equation Modeling. 2010; 17: 424–442. 18. Sibley CG, Kurz T. A model of climate belief profiles: How much does it matter if people question human causation? Analyses of Social Issues and Public Policy. 2013; 13: 245–261. 19. Hornsey MJ, Harris EA, Bain PG, Fielding KS. Meta- analyses of the determinants and outcomes of belief in climate change. Nature Climate Change. 2016; 6: 622– 626. 20. Dunlap RE, McCright AM. Cool dudes: The denial of climate change among conservative white males in the United States. Global Environmental Change. 2011; 21: 1163–1172. 21. Guy S, Kashima Y, Walker I, O’Neill S. Investigating the effects of knowledge and ideology on climate
  • 36. change beliefs. European Journal of Social Psychology. 2014; 44: 421–429. 22. Hart PS, Nisbet EC, Myers TA. Public attention to science and political news and support for climate change mitigation. Nature Climate Change. 2015; 5: 541–545. 23. Kellstedt PM, Zahran S, Vedlitz A. Personal efficacy, the information environment, and attitudes toward global warming and climate change in the United States. Risk Analysis. 2008; 28: 113–126. https://doi. org/10.1111/j.1539-6924.2008.01010.x PMID: 18304110 24. Milfont TL, Richter I, Sibley CG, Wilson MS, Fischer R. Environmental consequences of the desire to dominate and be superior. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin. 2013; 39: 1127–1138. https:// doi.org/10.1177/0146167213490805 PMID: 23798371 25. Milfont TL, Milojev P, Greaves L, Sibley CG. Socio- structural and psychological foundations of climate change beliefs. New Zealand Journal of Psychology. 2015; 44: 17–30. Available: http://www. psychology.org.nz/wp-content/uploads/Article-21.pdf. 26. Sibley CG, Milojev P. Power estimation of slope growth factors in the NZAVS using Monte Carlo simula-
  • 37. tion. NZAVS Technical Documents, e19. 2014. Available: www.psych.auckland.ac.nz/uoa/NZAVS 27. Schmidt A, Ivanova A, Schäfer MS. Media attention for climate change around the world: A comparative analysis of newspaper coverage in 27 countries. Global Environmental Change. 2013; 23: 1233–1248. Changes in climate change beliefs over time PLOS ONE | https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0174246 March 20, 2017 9 / 9 https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1539-6924.2012.01800.x https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1539-6924.2012.01800.x http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/22489642 http://images.smh.com.au/file/2014/02/07/5139061/CSIROCC4. pdf https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0121950 http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/25793746 https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1539-6924.2008.01010.x https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1539-6924.2008.01010.x http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/18304110 https://doi.org/10.1177/0146167213490805 https://doi.org/10.1177/0146167213490805 http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/23798371 http://www.psychology.org.nz/wp-content/uploads/Article- 21.pdf http://www.psychology.org.nz/wp-content/uploads/Article- 21.pdf http://www.psych.auckland.ac.nz/uoa/NZAVS https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0174246 Copyright of PLoS ONE is the property of Public Library of
  • 38. Science and its content may not be copied or emailed to multiple sites or posted to a listserv without the copyright holder's express written permission. However, users may print, download, or email articles for individual use. CURRENCY The timeliness of the information When was the information published or posted? Has the information been revised or updated? Does your topic require current information, or will older sources work as well? Are the links functional? * RELEVANCE The importance of the information for your needs Does the information relate to your topic or answer your question? Who is the intended audience? Is the information at an appropriate level (i.e. not too elementary or advanced for your needs)? Have you looked at a variety of sources before determining this is one you will use? Would you be comfortable citing this source in your research paper? AUTHORITY The source of the information Who is the author/publisher/source/sponsor? What are the author’s credentials or organizational affiliations? Is the author qualified to write on the topic? Is there contact information, such as a publisher or email address?
  • 39. Does the URL reveal anything about the author or source? * examples .com .edu .gov .org .net ACCURACY The reliability, truthfulness and correctness of the content Where does the information come from? Is the information supported by evidence? Has the information been reviewed or refereed? Can you verify any of the information in another source or from personal knowledge? Does the language or tone seem unbiased and free of emotion? Are there spelling, grammar or typographical errors? PURPOSE The reason the information exists. What is the purpose of the information? Is it to inform, teach, sell, entertain or persuade? Do the authors/sponsors make their intentions or purpose clear? Is the information fact, opinion or propaganda? Does the point of view appear objective and impartial? Are there political, ideological, cultural, religious, institutional or personal biases? * indicates criteria is for web The CRAAP Test was developed by librarians at California State University, Chico EVALUATING INFORMATION Applying the CRAAP Test When you search for information, you’re going to find lots of it… but is it good information? You will have to determine that for yourself, and the CRAAP Test can help. The CRAAP Test is a list of questions to help you evaluate the information you find. Different criteria will be
  • 40. more or less important depending on your situation or need. http://www.csuchico.edu/lins/handouts/eval_websites.pdf SDN & IBN White Paper CMIT 495: Current Trends and Projects in Computer Networks and Security [PROFESSOR NAME: Andrew Humes] By: [Student Name: Shreeji Patel]