SlideShare a Scribd company logo
2/4/2019 Assignment due Week 5 - Mainstream media and
HIV/AIDS
https://oregonstate.instructure.com/courses/1708319/assignment
s/7429085?module_item_id=18423339 1/3
Assignment due Week 5 - Mainstream media and HIV/AIDS
Due Thursday by 11:59pm Points 30 Submitting a text entry
box or a file upload
Assignment Rubric
Submit Assignment
Purpose
Critical analysis is a vital part of learning. The goal of this
assignment is to help you practice
your skills in reading mainstream media articles and being able
to critically evaluate it as a
source of information.
Instruc ons
Your analysis of the article should exhibit careful thought,
logical reasoning and provide evidence for your
answers. Each post should be at least two well-developed
paragraph (approximately 4-6 sentences each
minimum). Use correct spelling, punctuation, and grammar.
Identify an article released by the mainstream media to the
general public that discusses HIV/AIDS.
Example of an article is Botswana HIV Aids Deaths Decline
(https://allafrica.com/stories/201810030288.html) .
1. Identify which levels of the SEM are being addressed
2. Identify which levels of the SEM not addressed
3. In what ways might this article lead to stigma and
discrimination that places a person or group at risk of
contracting HIV/AIDS?
4. Provide a written opinion on the how this article positively,
negatively, or even excludes information
that contributes to the stemming of the HIV/AIDS epidemic.
Submission Details
Due: Thursday by 11:59 pm
Grading Criteria
Opinion based in logical reasoning and evidence: 4 points
Identified SEM levels (both addressed and not addressed): 2
points
Length of post approximately 4-6 sentences or more: 2 points
Spelling and grammar: 2 points
2/4/2019 Assignment due Week 5 - Mainstream media and
HIV/AIDS
https://oregonstate.instructure.com/courses/1708319/assignment
s/7429085?module_item_id=18423339 2/3
Criteria Ratings Pts
8.0 pts
8.0 pts
4.0 pts
3.0 pts
Organization
8.0 pts
Full Marks
Writing shows high
degree of attention
to logic and
reasoning of points.
Unity clearly leads
the reader to the
conclusion and stirs
thought regarding
the topic.
6.0 pts
Above Average
Writing is coherent
and logically
organized with
transitions used
between ideas and
paragraphs to
create coherence.
Overall unity of
ideas is present.
4.0 pts
Adequate
Writing is coherent
and logically
organized. Some
points remain
misplaced and
stray from the topic.
Transitions evident
but not used
throughout essay.
2.0 pts
Inadequate
Writing lacks
logical
organization.
It shows
some
coherence
but ideas
lack unity.
Serious
errors.
0.0 pts
No
Marks
Level of
Content
8.0 pts
Full Marks
Content indicates
synthesis of ideas,
in-depth analysis
and evidences
original thought and
support for the topic.
6.0 pts
Above Average
Content
indicates
original thinking
and develops
ideas with
sufficient and
firm evidence.
4.0 pts
Adequate
Content
indicates
thinking and
reasoning
applied with
original thought
on a few ideas.
2.0 pts
Inadequate
Shows some
thinking and
reasoning but
most ideas are
underdeveloped
and unoriginal.
0.0 pts
No
Marks
Development
4.0 pts
Full Marks
Main points well
developed with
high quality and
quantity support.
Reveals high
degree of critical
thinking.
3.0 pts
Above Average
Main points well
developed with
quality supporting
details and
quantity. Critical
thinking is weaved
into points
2.0 pts
Adequate
Main points are
present with
limited detail and
development.
Some critical
thinking is
present.
1.0 pts
Inadequate
Main points lack
detailed
development.
Ideas are vague
with little
evidence of
critical thinking.
0.0 pts
No
Marks
Grammar
and
Mechanics
3.0 pts
Full Marks
Free of
distracting
spelling,
punctuation,
and
grammatical
errors; absent of
fragments,
comma splices,
and run-ons.
2.0 pts
Above Average
Essay has few
spelling,
punctuation, and
grammatical
errors allowing
reader to follow
ideas clearly. Very
few fragments or
run-ons.
1.0 pts
Adequate
Most spelling,
punctuation,
and grammar
correct allowing
reader to
progress
though essay.
Some errors
remain.
0.0 pts
Inadequate
Spelling, punctuation,
and grammatical
errors create
distraction, making
reading difficult;
fragments, comma
splices, run-ons
evident. Errors are
frequent.
0.0 pts
No
Marks
2/4/2019 Assignment due Week 5 - Mainstream media and
HIV/AIDS
https://oregonstate.instructure.com/courses/1708319/assignment
s/7429085?module_item_id=18423339 3/3
Total Points: 30.0
Criteria Ratings Pts
4.0 pts
3.0 pts
Style
4.0 pts
Full Marks
Shows outstanding
style going beyond
usual college level;
rhetorical devices and
tone used effectively;
creative use of
sentence structure
and coordination
3.0 pts
Above Average
Attains college
level style; tone is
appropriate and
rhetorical devices
used to enhance
content; sentence
variety used
effectively.
2.0 pts
Adequate
Approaches
college level
usage of some
variety in
sentence
patterns,
diction, and
rhetorical
devices.
1.0 pts
Inadequate
Mostly in
elementary form
with little or no
variety in
sentence
structure,
diction,
rhetorical
devices or
emphasis.
0.0 pts
No
Marks
Format
3.0 pts
Full Marks
Meets all formal and
assignment
requirements and
evidences attention to
detail; all margins,
spacing and
indentations are
correct; essay is neat
and correctly
assembled with
professional look.
2.0 pts
Above
Average
Meets format
and assignment
requirements;
margins,
spacing, and
indentations are
correct; essay
is neat and
correctly
assembled.
1.0 pts
Adequate
Meets format and
assignment
requirements;
generally correct
margins, spacing,
and indentations;
essay is neat but
may have some
assembly errors.
0.0 pts
Inadequate
Fails to follow
format and
assignment
requirements;
incorrect
margins,
spacing and
indentation;
neatness of
essay needs
attention.
0.0 pts
No
Marks
Exemplary
Proficient
Progressing
Emerging
Element (1): Responsiveness: Did the student respond to the
main question of the week?
9 points (28%)
Posts exceed requirements of the Discussion instructions (e.g.,
respond to the question being asked; go beyond what is required
[i.e., incorporates additional readings outside of the assigned
Learning Resources, and/or shares relevant professional
experiences]; are substantive, reflective, and refers to Learning
Resources demonstrating that the student has considered the
information in Learning Resources and colleague postings).
9 points
Posts are responsive to and meet the requirements of the
Discussion instructions. Posts respond to the question being
asked in a substantive, reflective way and refer to Learning
Resources demonstrating that the student has read, viewed, and
considered the Learning Resources and colleague postings.
7–8 points
Posts are somewhat responsive to the requirements of the
Discussion instructions. Posts are not substantive and rely more
on anecdotal evidence (i.e., largely comprised of student
opinion); and/or does not adequately demonstrate that the
student has read, viewed, and considered Learning Resources
and colleague postings.
4–6 points
Posts are unresponsive to the requirements of the Discussion
instructions; miss the point of the question by providing
responses that are not substantive and/or solely anecdotal (i.e.,
comprised of only student opinion); and do not demonstrate that
the student has read, viewed, and considered Learning
Resources and colleague postings.
0–3 points
Element (2): Critical Thinking, Analysis, and Synthesis: Is the
student able to make meaning of the information?
9 points (28%)
Posts demonstrate the student’s ability to apply, reflect, AND
synthesize concepts and issues presented in the weekly Learning
Objectives. Student has integrated and mastered the general
principles, ideas, and skills presented. Reflections include clear
and direct correlation to authentic examples or are drawn from
professional experience; insights demonstrate significant
changes in awareness, self-understanding, and knowledge.
9 points
Posts demonstrate the student’s ability to apply, reflect OR
synthesize concepts and issues presented in the weekly Learning
Objectives. The student has integrated many of the general
principles, ideas, and skills presented. Reflections include clear
and direct correlation to authentic examples or are drawn from
professional experience, share insights that demonstrate a
change in awareness, self- understanding, and knowledge.
7–8 points
Posts demonstrate minimal ability to apply, reflect, or
synthesize concepts and issues presented in the weekly Learning
Objectives. The student has not fully integrated the general
principles, ideas, and skills presented. There are little to no
salient reflections, examples, or insights/experiences provided.
4–6 points
Posts demonstrate a lack of ability to apply, reflect, or
synthesize concepts and issues presented in the weekly Learning
Objectives. The student has not integrated the general
principles, ideas, and skills presented. There are no reflections,
examples, or insights/experiences provided.
0–3 points
Element (3): Professionalism of Writing: Does the student meet
graduate level writing expectations?
5 points (16%)
Posts meet graduate-level writing expectations (e.g., are clear,
concise, and use appropriate language; make few errors in
spelling, grammar, and syntax; provide information about
sources when paraphrasing or referring to it; use a
preponderance of original language and directly quote only
when necessary or appropriate). Postings are courteous and
respectful when offering suggestions, constructive feedback, or
opposing viewpoints.
5 points
Posts meet most graduate-level writing expectations (e.g., are
clear; make only a few errors in spelling, grammar, and syntax;
provide adequate information about a source when paraphrasing
or referring to it; use original language wherever possible and
directly quote only when necessary and/or appropriate).
Postings are courteous and respectful when offering
suggestions, constructive feedback, or opposing viewpoints.
4 points
Posts partially meet graduate-level writing expectation (e.g.,
use language that is unclear/inappropriate; make more than
occasional errors in spelling, grammar, and syntax; provide
inadequate information about a source when paraphrasing or
referring to it; under-use original language and over-use direct
quotes). Postings are at times less than courteous and respectful
when offering suggestions, feedback, or opposing viewpoints.
2–3 points
Posts do not meet graduate-level writing expectations (e.g., use
unclear/inappropriate language; make many errors in spelling,
grammar, and syntax; do not provide information about a source
when paraphrasing or referring to it; directly quote from
original source materials or consistently paraphrase rather than
use original language; or are discourteous and disrespectful
when offering suggestions, feedback, or opposing viewpoints).
0–1 points
Element (4):
Responses to Peers: Did the student respond to peer posts and
contribute professionally?
9 points (28%)
Responds to two or more peers in a manner that significantly
contributes to the Discussion.
9 points
Responds to one or more peers in a manner that significantly
contributes to the Discussion.
7–8 points
Responds to one or more peers in a manner that minimally
contributes to the Discussion.
4–6 points
Does not respond to any peer posts.
0–3 points
32 points
100%
25–28 points
78–88%
14–21 points
44–66%
0–10 points
0–31%
© 2015 Laureate Education, Inc.
Page 2 of 3
ORIGINAL PAPER
Students with Exceptionalities and the Peer Group Context
of Bullying and Victimization in Late Elementary School
David B. Estell Æ Thomas W. Farmer Æ Matthew J. Irvin Æ
Amity Crowther Æ
Patrick Akos Æ Daniel J. Boudah
Published online: 5 June 2008
� Springer Science+Business Media, LLC 2008
Abstract We examined bullying and victimization in 5th
grade classrooms in relation to students’ education status
and peer group membership. The sample consisted of 484
participants (258 girls, 226 boys), including 369 general
education students, 74 academically gifted students, and 41
students with mild disabilities. Students with mild dis-
abilities were more likely to be perceived as being bullies
by both teachers and peers. Teachers also rated students
with mild disabilities significantly higher for being bullied
by peers. Academically gifted students were rated by
teachers as the lowest for both bullying and being bullied.
Associating with aggressive or perceived-popular peers
increased the likelihood of being perceived as a bully.
Social isolates were more likely to be bullied than students
who did not associate with perceived-popular peers who, in
turn, were more likely to be bullied than students who
associated with perceived-popular peers. Students with
mild disabilities who had aggressive and perceived-popular
associates had more peer nominations for bullying than
all others. In contrast, students in general education with
neither aggressive nor perceived-popular associates had the
fewest peer nominations for bullying. We discuss impli-
cations for research and intervention.
Keywords Bullying � Victimization � Mild disabilities �
Giftedness � Peer groups
Introduction
In recent years, several distinct lines of inquiry have helped
to inform current perspectives on bullying and aggression
in schools. This includes research on the social and
behavioral characteristics of youth who are at risk for
bullying and victimization (Hodges and Perry 1999;
Pellegrini et al. 1999; Schwartz 2000), developmental
patterns of victimization (Hanish and Guerra 2002;
Pellegrini and Bartini 2000; Smith et al. 2004; Troop-
Gordon and Ladd 2005), broader classroom social
dynamics and aggression in the peer group (Farmer et al.
2002; O’Connell et al. 1999; Salmivalli et al. 1997) and
linkages between bullying, victimization, and other school
adjustment factors (Glew et al. 2005; Schwartz et al.
2005). Collectively, these studies indicate that students
involved in bullying and victimization are more likely to
have academic and social adjustment problems, that social
roles and peer group processes support bullying, and that
the late elementary school years are a time when classroom
social dynamics may be particularly important to bullying
and victimization.
D. B. Estell (&)
Department of Counseling and Educational Psychology, Indiana
University, 201 N. Rose Avenue, Bloomington, IN 47401, USA
e-mail: [email protected]
T. W. Farmer
Department of Educational Psychology, School Psychology, and
Special Education, Pennsylvania State University, University
Park, PA, USA
M. J. Irvin � A. Crowther
Center for Developmental Science, University of North Carolina
at Chapel Hill, Chapel Hill, NC, USA
P. Akos
Human Development and Psychological Studies Area, School of
Education, University of North Carolina at Chapel Hill, Chapel
Hill, NC, USA
D. J. Boudah
Department of Curriculum and Instruction, Special Education,
East Carolina University, Greenville, NC, USA
123
J Child Fam Stud (2009) 18:136–150
DOI 10.1007/s10826-008-9214-1
Most research on bullying and victimization focuses on
general school populations. Little work explicitly examines
the involvement of students with exceptionalities. In the
current climate of inclusion, students who receive excep-
tional children’s services tend to make up 10–20% or more
of the public school population and are integrated at least
part of the school day with their general education peers.
While it is likely that many studies on bullying include
students who receive exceptional services within their
samples, these children are typically not identified and it is
not possible to determine the degree to which they impact
results. This is potentially an important oversight because
students at the extremes of school functioning (students
with pronounced learning problems, and students achieving
at advanced levels) may have differential social experi-
ences that impact the degree to which they are involved
in bullying relative to their general education peers
(Kaukiainen et al. 2002; Peterson and Ray 2006). Peer
group affiliations are associated with students’ level of
involvement as bullies and victims (Estell et al. 2007;
Salmivalli et al. 1997), and students who are at the
extremes of school functioning tend to have peer affiliation
patterns that are distinct from those of general education
students (Farmer and Hollowell 1994; Pearl et al. 1998). It
is possible that exceptional students’ involvement in bul-
lying and victimization is related to their peer group
affiliations. Information along these lines could help clarify
how individual and peer group level factors come together
to impact bullying and victimization in the classroom.
Peer dynamics are important part of development. In
late elementary school, children form hierarchical social
structures in which some individuals and peer groups are
more popular and central than others (Adler and Adler
1996; Farmer and Rodkin 1996). Taunting, teasing, direct
confrontation, and physical attacks are forms of aggression
that children (particularly boys) use to demonstrate their
prowess and to establish and protect their positions in the
social structure (Adler and Adler 1996; Farmer 2000;
Pellegrini 1998; Rodkin and Hodges 2003). While physical
aggression is almost universally related to being disliked
(i.e., having low social preference; Asher and Coie 1990),
aggressive behavior has a close association with perceived
popularity (Rose et al. 2004), and social dominance—
while eventually adapting a more affiliative nature—often
begins with aggressive behavior (Pellegrini and Bartini
2000). Not only is aggression a common part of the daily
interpersonal dynamics in school, distinct subtypes of
aggressive youth can be differentiated in terms of popu-
larity (Estell et al. 2002; Estell et al. 2003; Rodkin et al.
2000). Popular aggressive youth are more likely to asso-
ciate with other popular and aggressive peers while
unpopular aggressive youth are more likely to associate
with non-aggressive and unpopular peers (Bagwell et al.
2000; Farmer et al. 2002). In short, while aggression and
dominance are often related to being disliked, they can lead
to high social status and perceived popularity (Estell et al.
2003).
Social dominance and influence in the social structure
also come into play in bullying. Although some bullies are
themselves aggressive victims, many others have high
social positions and are able to engage peers to support
their behavior (Atlas and Pepler 1998; Salmivalli et al.
1997). In fact, bullying episodes in elementary school tend
to involve several peers as onlookers, helpers, and
encouragers (O’Connell et al. 1999). Although some chil-
dren, usually girls, come to the aid of the victimized child,
many youth appear to respond in ways that are aimed at
protecting their status in the social structure including
currying favor with dominant bullies (Adler and Adler
1995; Hawkins and Pepler 2001).
A recent study of the social dynamics of bullying in two
middle schools comprised almost exclusively of rural
African American youth found that both bullies and vic-
tims tended to have rejected sociometric status. However,
while bullies were well integrated into their peer groups,
victims tended to be withdrawn and marginal in the social
structure (Estell et al. 2007). Bullies were heteroge-
neously dispersed in both popular and unpopular groups
and aggressive and non-aggressive groups and they were
highly likely to be leaders of their peer group. Of particular
interest to us, students who were bullies and/or victims
tended to have behaviors (i.e., higher teacher ratings of
attention problems and hyperactivity) and social skills (i.e.,
lower rates of peer nominations of prosocial behavior) that
are typically associated with students with mild
disabilities.
Exceptional students include such children with mild
disabilities as well as students who are identified as aca-
demically gifted, though it is important to note that in our
study both groups spend the majority of their school days
in general education classrooms. For our purposes, the term
students with mild disabilities refers to children with high
incidence disabilities (e.g., learning disabilities, mild
mental retardation, and mild emotional and behavioral
disorders), academically gifted refers to high achieving
children who have been identified for academically gifted
education programs, and general education students refers
to children who are not receiving special services to sup-
port their learning needs. Students with exceptionalities
tend to have patterns of social behavior, peer acceptance,
and peer affiliations that are distinct from their general
education peers. While both academically gifted students
and students with mild disabilities differ from general
education peers in terms of social functioning and social
adjustment, they are also markedly different from each
other.
J Child Fam Stud (2009) 18:136–150 137
123
Prior to diagnosis, students later identified as having
high incidence disabilities display social skill deficits and
have low social status (Vaughn et al. 1990; Walker et al.
1998). In preschool settings, students with mild develop-
mental delays are likely to have significant social
interaction problems (Guralnick and Groom 1987) and
these problems are likely to be sustained from the pre-
school to the early elementary school years (Guralnick
et al. 2006). In late childhood and early adolescence, stu-
dents with mild disabilities tend to have problem social
behaviors, social skills difficulties, and are often not well
accepted by their peers (Estell et al. 2008; Farmer et al.
1999; Frederickson and Furnham 2004; Gresham and
MacMillan 1997; Sale and Carey 1995). In turn, they are
also more likely to be socially isolated (Kavale and Forness
1996; Pearl et al. 1998) and to report less social acceptance
(Al-Yagon and Mikulincer 2004).
Although they are more likely to have social difficulties,
most students with mild disabilities are members of peer
groups (Farmer and Farmer 1996; Pearl et al. 1998).
However, the group membership of students with mild
disabilities may support social problems as they are more
likely to affiliate with classmates with problematic inter-
personal characteristics and less likely to associate with
peers with prosocial characteristics (Farmer and Hollowell
1994). Students with mild disabilities are also more likely
to develop social roles that support aggression or that
makes them the target of the aggressive behavior of others
(Evans and Eder 1993; Farmer and Farmer 1996; Farmer
and Rodkin 1996). In addition, students with mild dis-
abilities who associate with antisocial peers tend to hold
these peers in high esteem (Rodkin et al. 2006).
These social functioning challenges may place students
with mild disabilities at greater risk for involvement with
bullying as both targets and aggressors. Problems with
social functioning, including social information processing
difficulties, social skills deficits, and social isolation are all
risk factors for being victimized by peers (Fox and Boulton
2006). Likewise, elementary students who are chronically
victimized are more likely to experience academic prob-
lems and internalizing difficulties that may place them at-
risk of later learning and behavior problems (Hodges et al.
1999; Schwartz et al. 2005). In contrast, affiliations with
aggressive and deviant peers tend to be associated with a
greater likelihood of bullying others (Estell et al. 2007).
Collectively, these findings suggest that children with mild
disabilities may be more likely to be involved in bullying
as aggressors and/or targets than their typically achieving
peers. Consistent with this view, extant data suggest that
youth with mild disabilities are more likely to be victims
than are their typically-achieving counterparts and they
may be at increased risk of being bullies and bully-victims
(Kaukiainen et al. 2002; Nazuboka and Smith 1993).
Further, data from the National Longitudinal Study of
Adolescent Health, which included 1,301 adolescents with
learning disabilities from a sample of 20,780, indicate that
youth with learning disabilities were significantly more
likely than others to report involvement in violent behav-
iors (31% vs. 25% for boys; 20% vs. 11% for girls) and
were more likely to have witnessed or have been a victim
of a violent act (Svetaz et al. 2000).
In contrast to students with mild disabilities, children
who are identified as academically gifted tend to have
higher levels of social functioning than their general edu-
cation peers. Preuss and Dubow (2004) found that
academically gifted students were more likely to employ
active problem-solving approaches to social stressors, and
that being academically gifted moderated the impact of
stressors on adjustment. Academically gifted students are
rated by teachers as having highly positive levels of social
skills (Janke and Lee 1991) and during the elementary
school years they are likely to experience high levels of
social acceptance and low levels of peer rejection (Austin
and Draper 1981; Luftig and Nichols 1990; Schneider et al.
1989). Academically gifted students are also more likely to
have prosocial or popular friends than their non-gifted
peers and less likely to be socially isolated (Farmer and
Rodkin 1996; Pearl et al. 1998; Schneider and Daniels
1992). On the whole, while they may face considerable
stress due to their own high expectations (Moon 2004),
gifted children are largely socially skilled, use effective
social coping mechanisms, are socially integrated with
prosocial peers, and are well adjusted. These factors tend to
be negatively related to victimization, and may also make
involvement with deviant peers and a subsequent increase
in bullying behavior less likely.
In summary, research on peer group dynamics and the
social relations of exceptional students comes together to
suggest that students with mild disabilities may be more
likely to be bullies and victims and they may have social
characteristics and peer affiliation patterns that exacerbate
their risk for involvement in bullying. In contrast, aca-
demically gifted students may have social characteristics
and affiliation patterns that protect against bullying and
victimization. Little work, however, has examined the
relationship between educational status (i.e., academically
gifted, general education, mild disabilities) and involve-
ment in bullying, and less has looked at how this may
interact with associations with aggressive and/or popular
peers. Information along these lines may help clarify peer
group dynamics that contribute to bullying and victimiza-
tion in the classroom. Toward this end, we sought to
examine differences among academically gifted students,
students with mild disabilities, and general education stu-
dents on ratings of victimization and bullying in relation to
their peer associations.
138 J Child Fam Stud (2009) 18:136–150
123
Four research questions guided our work. Our first
research question focused on the relationship between peer
nominated social characteristics and involvement in bul-
lying. Are teachers’ and peers’ perceptions of bullying and
victimization related to peer nominations on key social
factors (i.e., aggression, positive behavior, internalizing,
social prominence, social preference)? We hypothesized
that peer nominations of aggression would be positively
related, and social preference negatively related to both
teacher ratings and peer nominations of bullying. We fur-
ther hypothesized that victimization would be negatively
related to peer nominated popularity and social preference,
and positively related to internalizing. Our second research
question examined whether peers perceived classmates
differently as a function of their education status. Do
academically gifted students, general education students,
and students with mild disabilities differ in the nominations
they receive from peers? We hypothesized that gifted stu-
dents would be viewed as popular, well-liked, having
positive behavior, and neither aggressive nor high on
internalizing, while students with special needs would be
viewed by peers as aggressive, high on internalizing, and
low on social preference, popularity, and positive behavior.
Our third research question investigated the relationship
between education status and associations with teacher-
rated aggressive and popular peers. Is education status
differentially linked to peer group membership? We
hypothesized that gifted students would be least likely and
students with special needs the most likely to have
aggressive friends. We also hypothesized that gifted stu-
dents would be most likely and students with special needs
least likely to have popular friends. Our fourth research
question explored the combined effect of education status
and group types on involvement in bullying. Is bullying
and victimization for students in different education status
categories moderated by membership in aggressive and
popular peer groups? We hypothesized that students with
special needs who have aggressive but not popular asso-
ciates would have the highest levels of bullying, and gifted
students with popular but not aggressive associates would
have the lowest levels of bullying. We further hypothesized
that students with special needs and neither aggressive nor
popular associates would have the highest victimization
ratings while gifted students with popular and non-
aggressive associates would have the lowest levels of
victimization.
Method
We employed a multi-source survey design that included
teacher- and peer-report measures as well as school record
information. This included peer reports to identify groups
and to examine how bullying and education status relate to
a variety of other behaviors. We relied on teacher reports to
classify the participants’ associates, and used both peer and
teacher reports to examine involvement in bullying.
Finally, we used school record information to determine
the education status of the participants.
Participants
Participants came from eleven elementary schools in two
school districts. Both districts were located in a state in the
Southeastern United States. One district served a small
metropolitan area and the other served a rural county. We
obtained parent consent and student assent for 69% of the
701 students from 35 classrooms across the schools. Class
size ranged from 18 to 27, with an average of 20 students
per classroom. An average of nearly 14 of these 20 students
participated in the present study. The sample consisted of
484 fifth graders (258 girls, 226 boys), including 369
(76.2%; 196 girls and 173 boys) general education stu-
dents, 74 (15.3%; 50 girls and 24 boys) academically gifted
students, and 41 (8.5%; 12 girls and 29 boys) students with
high incidence disabilities (e.g., learning disabilities, mild
mental retardation, emotional and behavioral disorders; see
Table 1). While 10–20% of students typically qualify as
having special needs, we examined only those students
with special needs who spent the majority of their day in
Table 1 Sample characteristics
by education classification and
gender
Girls Boys Total
Not in special education 196 (53.1%) 173 (46.9%) 369 (76.2%)
Academic gift 51 (68.0%) 24 (32.0%) 75 (15.5%)
Learning disability 3 (20.0%) 12 (80.0%) 15 (3.1%)
Emotional handicap/emotional behavior disability 1 (20.0%) 4
(80.0%) 5 (1.0%)
Mild mental retardation 2 (33.3%) 4 (66.7%) 6 (1.2%)
Speech impairment 2 (33.3%) 4 (66.7%) 6 (1.2%)
Other health impairment 0 (0.0%) 4 (100.0%) 4 (0.8%)
Unspecified special need 1 (100.0%) 0 (0.0%) 1 (0.2%)
504 plan 2 (66.7%) 1 (33.3%) 3 (0.6%)
J Child Fam Stud (2009) 18:136–150 139
123
general education classrooms. This reduced the number of
children with special needs in the sample to the previously
mentioned 8.5%. These percentages reflect the distribution
of these populations within the school districts where this
research was conducted and were also consistent with state
level rates of having mild special needs and giftedness.
All students who were in the top 15% of their class in
achievement were considered academically gifted by the
schools. Students in the mild disability sample were iden-
tified by local school assessment procedures that reflected
federal definitions and guidelines for learning disabled,
behavioral disordered, health disability, noncategorical
disability, mild mental retardation, and speech disability.
The different definitions are not provided because all stu-
dents with disabilities were collapsed into a single category
(mild disabilities). We did this for three reasons. First,
preliminary analyses indicated a high level of similarity
between the different special education classifications for
teacher-, peer-, and self-assessed characteristics. However,
variability in the sample sizes for different categories,
together with the very small sample size of some catego-
ries, could have led us to misinterpret the results.
Collapsing students into a single mild disabilities category
protected against inaccurate comparisons across disability
areas.
Second, during the current climate of serving and
identifying special education students based on service
needs (e.g., mild and moderate need, high incidence ser-
vice needs) rather than categorical approaches, there is
considerable variability across states in terms of how
categorical classifications are operationalized. By focusing
on students with mild disabilities who were included in
general education classrooms for most of the school day,
we avoid the issue of variability in categorical classifi-
cations. Third, our aim was not to identify characteristics
associated with specific disabilities. Rather, our goal was
to explore the relationship between bullying involvement
and students with disabilities in general. Further, this
approach has been used in other studies of the peer
relations of students with disabilities in general education
classrooms (e.g., Farmer et al. 1999; Sale and Carey
1995; Rodkin et al. 2006).
Because we focused on the social relations of students
with disabilities in inclusive settings, we recruited only
those students who spent more than 50% of the school day
in general education classrooms to participate. Therefore,
all participants, regardless of education status, were
included in general education classrooms for all or most of
the school day, and all classroom assessments refer to
inclusive rooms. Fifty-five percent (267/483) of our par-
ticipants were Caucasian, 41% (198/483) were African
American, 1% (3/483) were Latino(a), and 3% (15/483)
were of another ethnicity.
Ethnicity and special education status were related
(v(6,n=483)
2 = 17.74, p  .01). Among Caucasians, 70.4%
(188/267) were in general education, 8.2% (22/267) had
special needs, and 21.3% (57/267) were academically gif-
ted. Eighty-three percent (165/198) African Americans
were in general education, 8.6% (17/198) had special
needs, and 8.1% (16/198) were academically gifted. All
three Latino/a students were in general education. Finally,
80.0% (12/15) of individuals from other ethnicities were in
general education, 13.3% (2/15) had special needs, and
6.7% (1/15) were academically gifted.
Procedures
We used group administration procedures when collecting
the survey data. Before the administration of the survey, we
assured participants their answers would be kept confi-
dential, asked them to protect the confidentiality of their
responses, and told them that they could stop participating
at any time. We consulted teachers prior to administration
about typical accommodations needed by students for
exams, and made parallel accommodations where needed.
During the survey, one administrator read the instructions
and questions aloud, while additional administrators pro-
vided mobile monitoring and assistance as needed.
Teachers also completed rating forms on each participant
during the group administration. Non-participants were
told by their teachers to work on class assignments during
the data collection.
For all peer nomination measures, the probe focused on
the classroom level (i.e., participants were told that they
could only nominate peers in their classroom). We did this
because the sample was in elementary school and there was
minimal interaction among students in different classes. All
peer nominations were made from free recall (i.e., we did
not provide class rosters).
Measures
Social Cognitive Maps (SCM)
For this measure, participants were asked ‘‘Are there some
kids in your class who hang around together a lot? Who are
they?’’ Following the procedures developed by Cairns and
colleagues (e.g., Cairns et al. 1985), participants were
instructed to list, from free recall, as many groups as they
could think of in their class. In addition, they were asked to
circle the individual or individuals who were the leaders of
each group, if the group had a leader.
To identify distinct groups within the classroom social
network, we analyzed the SCM data following the proce-
dures outlined by Cairns and colleagues (Cairns and Cairns
1994; Cairns et al. 1995a). SCM procedures have been
140 J Child Fam Stud (2009) 18:136–150
123
used extensively in research on school social networks in a
variety of populations including a mixed-race urban and
rural sample (Cairns et al. 1988), a mixed-race suburban
sample (Rodkin et al. 2000), inner-city African Americans
(Estell et al. 2002; Xie et al. 1999), Chinese students
(Leung 1996), and students with special needs (Farmer and
Hollowell, 1994). Three week test-retest reliability coeffi-
cients indicate high short-term stability of children’s peer
groups (i.e., 90% of groups maintain a majority of their
members over this period; Cairns et al. 1995a). Further, the
results of SCM analyses match observed affiliations
(Cairns and Cairns 1994; Cairns et al. 1985).
We identified a total of 45 primarily-male groups with a
range of 2–9 members, and 29 male isolates. The mean
group size for boys was 6.26. A total of 61 female groups
were identified with a range of 2–9 members, and there
were 14 female isolates. The mean group size for girls was
5.23. These means and ranges are consistent with those of
other studies of late elementary and middle school peer
groups (Bagwell et al. 2000; Cairns et al. 1988; Farmer
and Hollowell 1994; Xie et al. 1999).
Teacher Ratings of Social Adaptation
Teachers rated participants on a number of items relating to
social adaptation. These items were displayed in Likert-
type seven-point formats. The present study utilized three
of these items: ‘‘bullied by peers,’’ ‘‘bullies peers,’’
‘‘manipulates friendships.’’ The two items relating to bul-
lying (as aggressor or target) were used as dependent
variables, and the ‘‘manipulates friendships’’ was included
in the ICS-T factor analysis (described below) as a measure
of relational aggression.
ICS-T
Teachers rated participants on the Interpersonal Compe-
tence Scale—Teacher (ICS-T; Cairns et al. 1995b), an 18-
item instrument which asks teachers to rate children on a
variety of characteristics relating to aggressiveness, popu-
larity, and academic competence. The items are displayed
in Likert-type, seven-point scales. These scales are
anchored by frequency (i.e., often vs. never fights, argues,
etc.) or degree (very popular vs. not popular; has lots of
friends vs. has very few friends). These 18 items load onto
six sub-scales, though for the present study, only an
aggression factor (‘‘argues,’’ ‘‘gets in trouble,’’ and
‘‘fights’’ from the ICS-T, with ‘‘manipulates friendships’’
from the teacher ratings described above added; a = .84)
and the popularity factor (‘‘popular with boys,’’ ‘‘popular
with girls,’’ ‘‘has lots of friends,’’ a = .81) were examined.
Past research has shown that 3-week test-retest reliability
coefficients are moderately high (i.e., .80–.92), median
test-retest r across the factors are .81 for girls and .87 for
boys, and 1-year coefficients are moderately strong (i.e.,
.40–.50; Cairns et al. 1995b). ICS-T has been shown to
have convergent validity with direct observations, grades,
school discipline reports, and peer nominations, and pre-
dictive validity for early school dropout, teen parenthood,
and criminal arrest in early adulthood, (Cairns and Cairns
1994; Cairns et al. 1995b). This measure has also been
used extensively in past work on issues pertaining to
popularity and aggression (Cairns et al. 1988; Estell et al.
2002, 2007; Farmer and Rodkin 1996; Rodkin et al. 2000).
Peer Social Preference
Participants were asked to nominate up to three classmates
from a list of participants they liked the most and up to
three they liked the least, and social preference was cal-
culated following the criteria described in Asher and Coie
(1990). Specifically, each participant’s number of nomi-
nations for being most liked and least liked were
standardized within class, and social preference score was
calculated by subtracting their liked-least z-score from
their liked-most z-score.
Peer Interpersonal Assessments
We used peer interpersonal assessments to determine
classmates’ perceptions of peers’ social and behavioral
characteristics. Students were asked to nominate, from free
recall, up to three classmates who best fit descriptors for
several items. They were told during the testing procedures
that they could nominate the same person for more than
one item, they did not need to fill all three blanks if they
did not know three people who fit an item, they could
bypass an item if they felt that they did not know anyone at
all who fit that particular item, and that they could nomi-
nate themselves. For analyses using these items, however,
all self-nominations were removed from consideration,
making these peer-nominations exclusively.
Past studies using these measures have indicated 3-week
test-retest reliability with individual items ranged from .72
to .93. These items are identical with or similar to peer
assessments used by other investigators (e.g., Coie et al.
1982; Masten et al. 1985), and past work has found strong
evidence of reliability and validity in diverse samples (Coie
et al. 1982; Masten et al.1985). We divided the total num-
ber of nominations participants received for each peer
assessment item by the total number of possible nominators
(i.e., all participants in the class). Because the denominator
was the total number of participants in each class, the
resulting proportions were in many cases quite small. In
order to make mean differences clearer, we linearly trans-
formed these proportions by multiplying them by 1,000.
J Child Fam Stud (2009) 18:136–150 141
123
We ran a principal components analysis with a Varimax
rotation on 12 items (‘‘bully’’ and ‘‘picked on’’ were
excluded from the factor analysis and instead used as
dependent variables). Four factors had eigenvalues in excess
of 1.0, and a scree plot indicated that a four-factor solution
provided the best fit to the data, as there was a major change
in slope between the fourth and fifth factors. This four-
component solution accounted for 75.6% of the variance in
the items. As seen in the rotated loading matrix in Table 2,
all items loaded on their corresponding component in excess
of .74, and no item cross-loaded on another component
greater than .31. The resulting factors were aggression
(Cronbach’s a = .87; consists of ‘‘disruptive,’’ ‘‘starts
fights,’’ ‘‘gets in trouble,’’ and ‘‘starts rumors’’), positive
behavior (a = .85; consists of ‘‘cooperative,’’ ‘‘good stu-
dent,’’ and ‘‘friendly’’), social promience (a = .76; consists
of ‘‘athletic,’’ ‘‘cool,’’ and ‘‘popular’’), and internalizing
(a = .66; consists of ‘‘acts shy’’ and ‘‘sad’’).
Peer-group Types
We classified the peer groups identified by the SCM
analysis according to the aggression and popularity level of
their constituent members. Consistent with past work
examining peer-group characteristics (Farmer et al. 2002),
we calculated within-sex and -class z-scores for the ICS-T
aggression and popularity factors. We classified a partici-
pant as popular or aggressive if their z-score was greater
than or equal to ?.50. This resulted in 32.2% (146/454) of
participants being identified as popular and 31.0% (140/
452) as aggressive.
Peer-group types were a function of the proportion and
number of aggressive and popular members in the group.
For the example of aggression, we classified a participant
as having aggressive associates if at least 50% of their
SCM-derived peer group members were classified as
aggressive or, in larger groups, if they had at least three
aggressive associates. We used identical criteria to classify
groups by the proportion and/or number of popular
members.
Results
We present out results in four major sections, corre-
sponding to our four research questions. The first section
uses correlation analyses to examine how peer-nominated
interpersonal characteristics relate to bullying and victim-
ization. The second section uses ANOVAs to examine the
relationship between educational status and peer-nomi-
nated characteristics. The third section uses v2 analyses to
investigate the relationship between education status and
aggressive and popular groups. The fourth and final section
employs MANOVAs with follow-up ANOVAs and Tu-
key’s tests to explore the combined effect of education
status and group types on bullying and victimization.
Peer Behavioral Nominations and Bullying and
Victimization
We calculated correlations within classrooms and then
combined them for the following analyses. Due to the
number of correlations run, we only considered those sig-
nificant at less than the .0025 level (.05/20) as significant.
Teacher ratings of ‘‘bullies’’ were positively related to peer
nominated aggression (r(454) = .50, p  .001) and promi-
nence (r(454) = .21, p  .001) factors, negatively related to
the peer nominated positive behavior factor (r(454) = -.17,
p  .001) and social preference (r(451) = -.28, p  .001),
and unrelated to the peer nominated internalizing factor
(r(454) = -.01, p = .86). Peer nominations for ‘‘bully’’
were positively related to the peer aggression factor
(r(480) = .89, p  .001) and the prominence factor
(r(480) = .40, p  .001), negatively related to social pref-
erence (r(477) = -.24, p  .001), and unrelated to the peer
positive behavior (r(480) = -.08, p = .08) and internaliz-
ing (r(480) = .08, p = .10) factors.
Teacher ratings of ‘‘bullied’’ were negatively related to
social preference (r(452) = -.17, p  .001), but unrelated
to aggression (r(455) = .06, p = .19), internalizing
(r(455) = .28, p = .05), positive behavior (r(455) = -.13,
p = .01), or prominence (r(455) = -.11, p = .05). Peer
nominations of ‘‘picked on’’ were positively related to
aggression (r(480) = .29, p  .001) and internalizing
Table 2 Peer nomination loadings for varimax-rotated
component
matrix
Component
Aggression Positive
behav.
Social
prom.
Internalizing
Disruptive .90 -.04 -.02 -.02
Starts fights .86 -.06 .24 .06
Gets in
trouble
.82 -.11 .14 .02
Starts rumors .76 .07 .25 -.04
Cooperative -.06 .88 .25 .12
Good student .01 .85 .12 -.01
Friendly -.09 .86 .11 .17
Athletic .26 -.00 .74 .06
Cool .25 .24 .83 -.10
Popular .02 .31 .78 -.06
Acts shy -.07 .21 .02 .84
Sad .08 .01 -.07 .86
Note: all loadings in excess of .4 are bolded
142 J Child Fam Stud (2009) 18:136–150
123
(r(480) = .81, p  .001), negatively related to social pref-
erence (r(477) = -.37, p  .001), and unrelated to positive
behavior (r(480) = .04, p = .44) or prominence (r(480) =
-.03, p = .55).
These results indicate that both teachers and students
viewed bullying as related to aggression and social prom-
inence, but also associated with being disliked (i.e., low
social preference). Teachers also saw bullying as related to
low levels of prosocial behaviors. Both students and
teachers viewed being the target of bullying as related to
being disliked, and students further saw it as related to
being aggressive and high in internalizing behavior.
Education Status and Peer Behavioral Nominations
Table 3 shows the means and standard deviations of the
peer-nomination factors and social preference by educa-
tional classification. Education status was not related to
either the social prominence (F(2,477) = 1.04, p = .35;
gp
2 = .004) or aggression (F(2,477) = 0.79, p = .45;
gp
2 = .003) factors. It was related to peer-nominated pro-
social behavior (F(2,477) = 7.13, p  .001; gp
2 = .029),
internalizing (F(2,477) = 8.43, p  .001; gp
2 = .034), and
social preference (F(2,474) = 4.61, p  .01; gp
2 = .019).
Post-hoc Tukey’s tests indicated that academically gifted
students had more positive behavior nominations than
general education students or students with mild disabili-
ties. Students with mild disabilities had higher internalizing
nominations than general education or academically gifted
students. Finally, academically gifted students had signifi-
cantly higher social preference scores than students with
mild disabilities.
Education Status, Behavioral Classification, and Peer
Associates
Education status was not significantly related to the tea-
cher-rated aggressive classification (v(2,n=452)
2 = 4.51,
p = .11). For students in general education, 32.7% (111/
339) were rated by teachers as aggressive. This compared
to 35.0% (14/40) of students with mild disabilities and
20.5% (15/73) of academically gifted students. Education
status was also not related to teacher-rated popularity status
(v(2,n=454)
2 = 4.00, p = .14). Among children in general
education, 30.6% (104/340) were classified as popular, as
were 27.5% (11/40) of those with mild disabilities and
41.8% (31/74) of academically gifted participants.
Our analyses for peer associations suggested that there
were differences in peer group affiliations as a function of
education status. The effect for aggressive associates was
not significant (v(2,n=465)
2 = 5.30, p = .07). The differences
for education status were pronounced for popular associ-
ates (v(2,n=465)
2 = 39.37, p  .001). A lower proportion (57/
350, or 16.3%) of participants in general education had
popular associates than expected by chance (Fisher’s Exact
Probability  .001), and a higher proportion (36/74, or
48.6%) of academically gifted students had popular asso-
ciates (Fisher’s Exact Probability  .001). The proportion
of students with mild disabilities (6/41, or 14.6%) was not
significantly different from chance (Fisher’s Exact
Probability = .19).
These results show that students in general education,
those who are gifted, and those who have mild disabilities
were all equally likely to be considered aggressive or
popular, and are equally likely to have aggressive associ-
ates. Gifted students were, however, more likely to have
popular associates.
Bullying and Victimization by Education Status and
Group Types
Bullying by Education Status and Group Types
To examine the potential interaction of groups and edu-
cation classifications on bullying, we ran a MANOVA with
education classification, popular associates, and aggressive
associates as independent variables and peer-nominations
and teacher-ratings of being a bully as the dependent
variables. The MANOVA indicated that there were main
effects of education status (Wilks’ K = 0.93,
Table 3 Peer-nominated characteristics by education status
Variable Education status
General education Mild disabilities Academically gifted
M SD M SD M SD
Social prominence 82.06 94.43 89.45 143.21 100.00 92.77
Aggression 56.73 102.65 76.34 137.69 51.27 85.08
Positive behavior 92.75A 107.29 70.39A 96.28 141.07B 136.23
Internalizing 52.64A 73.32 96.87B 121.03 34.89A 62.22
Social preference 0.11AB 1.45 -0.31A 1.61 0.56B 1.69
Different superscripts indicate significant differences (p  .01)
J Child Fam Stud (2009) 18:136–150 143
123
F(4,884) = 8.55, p  .001; gp
2 = .053), aggressive-associ-
ates (Wilks’ K = 0.93, F(2,442) = 16.93, p  .001;
gp
2 = .071), popular-associates (Wilks’ K = 0.96,
F(2,442) = 9.57, p  .001; gp
2 = .041), education status by
aggressive associates (Wilks’ K = 0.95, F(4,884) = 5.57,
p  .001; gp
2 = .048), education status by popular associ-
ates (Wilks’ K = 0.94, F(4,884) = 6.42, p  .001;
gp
2 = .043), and the three-way interaction of education
status by aggressive associates by popular associates
(Wilks’ K = 0.94, F(2,442) = 14.59, p  .001; gp
2 = .062).
To probe these multivariate effects, we ran follow-up
ANOVAs for each dependent. For teacher ratings of
‘‘bullies’’ we found main effects for education status
(F(2,443) = 4.79, p  .01; gp
2 = .021), and aggressive
associates (F(1,443) = 9.05, p  .01; gp
2 = .020), but no
main effect of popular associates (F(1,443) = 1.49, p = .22;
gp
2 = .003), and no two-way interactive effects of educa-
tion status by either aggressive associates (F(2,443) = 0.12,
p = .88; gp
2 = .001) or popular associates (F(2,443) = 3.01,
p = .05; gp
2 = .013), and no three-way interaction of edu-
cation status by aggressive group by popularity group
(F(1,443) = 0.35, p = .55; gp
2 = .001).
Post-hoc tests indicated that the main effect of education
status was due to students with mild disabilities (M = 4.16,
SE = 0.54) having significantly higher teacher ratings of
being bullies than those in general education (M = 3.15,
SE = 0.17) who in turn had higher ratings of being bullies
than academically gifted students (M = 2.14, SE = 0.26).
The main effect of aggressive associates was due to those
with aggressive associates (M = 4.21, SE = 0.43) having
higher ratings for being bullies than those without
aggressive associates (M = 2.43, SE = 0.17).
In summary, teacher ratings of bullying were highest
among students with mild disabilities and lowest among
gifted students. Further, all students with aggressive asso-
ciates had higher teacher ratings of bullying.
For peer-nominations of ‘‘bully’’ there were main effects
for education status (F(1,443) = 11.59, p  .001; gp
2 = .050),
aggressive associates (F(1,443) = 33.53, p  .001; gp
2 =.
070), and popular associates (F(1,443) = 18.74, p  .001;
gp
2 = .041). There were also two-way interactions of edu-
cation status by aggressive associates (F(2,443) = 8.96,
p  .001; gp
2 = .039) and popular associates (F(2,443) =
9.48, p  .001; gp
2 = .041). Finally, the three-way interac-
tion of education status by aggressive group by popularity
group was significant (F(1,443) = 21.42, p  .001; gp
2 =
.046).
Post-hoc tests indicated that the main effect of education
status was due to individuals with mild disabilities
(M = 200.97, SE = 29.51) having more peer nominations
for bullying than students in general education
(M = 56.64, SE = 9.25) or academically gifted students
(M = 50.89, SE = 14.17). The main effect of aggressive
associates was due to those with aggressive associates
(M = 190.19, SE = 23.65) having more nominations for
bullying than those without aggressive associates
(M = 38.69, SE = 9.33). Similarly, the main effect of
popular associates was due to those with popular associates
(M = 162.74, SE = 22.89) having more nominations for
bullying than those without popular associates (M = 61.57,
SE = 10.56).
Figures 1 and 2 show the two-way interactions of edu-
cation status by aggressive associates and education status
by popular associates, respectively. Post-hoc tests indicate
that students with mild disabilities and aggressive associ-
ates had more peer nominations of being bullies than all
other cells. Individuals in general education and aggressive
associates had significantly more peer nominations for
being bullies than all those without aggressive associates.
Finally, academically gifted students with aggressive
associates had more peer nominations for bullying than
academically gifted students and general education stu-
dents without aggressive associates.
Post-hoc tests for the education status by popular asso-
ciates interaction indicated that students with mild
disabilities and popular associates had more peer nomina-
tions for being bullies than all others. Students with mild
disabilities without popular associates had significantly
0
100
200
300
400
500
600
700
Agg. Assoc. Not Agg. Assoc.
P
ee
r
"B
ul
ly
"
N
om
in
at
io
ns General Education Students
Students With Mild Disabilities
Academically Gifted StudentsD
C BC
ABA A
Fig. 1 Peer nominations of
‘‘bully’’ by education status and
aggressive affiliations
144 J Child Fam Stud (2009) 18:136–150
123
more peer nominations for being bullies than those in
general education with popular associates and academi-
cally gifted students with popular associates.
Figure 3 shows the three-way interaction of education
status by aggressive associates by popular associates. Post-
hoc tests indicated that individuals with mild disabilities
who had aggressive and popular associates had more peer
nominations for bullying than all others. On the other end
of the spectrum, individuals in general education with
neither aggressive nor popular associates had the lowest
number of nominations. These individuals significantly
differed from all participants with aggressive associates as
well as students with mild disabilities with neither
aggressive nor popular associates.
In summary, peer nominations for bullying were highest
among students with mild disabilities, students with
aggressive associates, and students with popular associates.
But these relationships were more complicated. While
having aggressive associates was related to higher nomi-
nations for bullying for all groups, the effect was especially
evident among students with mild disabilities, and much
less marked in gifted students. A more pronounced dif-
ference occured with popular associates. While having
popular associates was related to higher nominations for
bullying in students with mild disabilities, popular
associations related to fewer nominations for bullying in
gifted and general education students. Finally, these vary-
ing types of associations exacerbated each others’ effects:
students with mild disabilities who belonged to groups of
both aggressive and popular peers had extremely high
numbers of peer nominations for bullying.
Victimization by Education Status and Group Types
We ran parallel analyses to those for bullying for victim-
ization, with the inclusion of isolates as a third group type.
We ran a MANOVA with education classification, popular
associates, and aggressive associates as independent vari-
ables and peer-nominations of being picked on and teacher-
ratings of being bullied as the dependent variables. This
MANOVA indicated that there was a main effect of edu-
cation status (Wilks’ K = 0.95, F(4,886) = 5.92, p  .001;
gp
2 = .037), aggressive associates (Wilks’ K = 0.97,
F(2,443) = 4.66, p  .01; gp
2 = .020) and popular associates
(Wilks’ K = 0.96, F(2,443) = 4.68, p  .01; gp
2 = .021).
There were no interactive effects for education status by
either aggressive associates (Wilks’ K = 1.00, F(4,880) =
0.59, p = .67; gp
2 = .005) or popular associates (Wilks’
K = 0.98, F(4,880) = 0.31, p = .87; gp
2 = .002), and no
significant three-way interaction of education status by
0
Pop. Assoc.
P
ee
r
"B
ul
ly
"
N
om
in
at
io
ns
100
200
300
400
500
600
700
Not Pop. Assoc.
General Education Students
Students With Mild Disabilities
Academically Gifted Students
AB
C
A AB B AB
Fig. 2 Peer nominations of
‘‘bully’’ by education status and
popular affiliations
Not Agg. & Pop Assoc. Not Agg. & Not Pop Assoc.
General Education Students
Students With Mild Disabilities
Academically Gifted Students
0
100
200
300
400
500
600
700
Agg. & Pop. Assoc. Agg. & Not Pop. Assoc.
P
ee
r
"B
ul
ly
"
N
om
in
at
io
ns
ABCBCDAABABAB
CD
EE
F
DE
Fig. 3 Peer-nominations of
‘‘bully’’ by education status,
aggressive and popular
affiliations
J Child Fam Stud (2009) 18:136–150 145
123
aggressive associates by popular associates (Wilks’
K = 1.00, F(2,440) = 0.89, p = .41; gp
2 = .004).
We probed these effects with follow-up ANOVAs for
each dependent variable. For teacher ratings of ‘‘bullied’’
there was a main effect of education status (F(2,441) = 7.62,
p  .001; gp
2 = .033), aggressive associates (F(1,441) =
4.09, p  .05; gp
2 = .019), and popular associates
(F(1,441) = 5.55, p  .01; gp
2 = .022). There were no
interactive effects for education status by either aggressive
associates (F(2,441) = 1.04, p = .35; gp
2 = .005) or popular
associates (F(2,441) = 0.45, p = .64; gp
2 = .002), and no
significant three-way interaction of education status by
aggressive associates by popular associates (F(1,441) =
1.78, p = .18; gp
2 = .004). Post-hoc tests indicated that the
main effect of education status was due to significant dif-
ferences among all three groups of students: those with
mild disabilities (M = 3.10, SE = 0.29) had the highest
ratings for being bullied, followed by those in general
education (M = 2.44 SE = 0.08), while academically gif-
ted participants were the lowest (M = 1.34, SE = 0.07).
The main effect of aggressive associates was due to isolates
(M = 3.37, SE = 0.32) having higher ratings of being
bullied than participants with (M = 2.47, SE = 0.14) or
without (M = 2.15, SE = 0.08) aggressive associates.
Finally, the main effect of popular associates was due to
isolates (M = 3.37, SE = 0.32) having higher ratings of
being bullied than those without popular associates
(M = 2.35, SE = 0.08) and those with popular associates
(M = 1.77, SE = 0.12). Students without popular associ-
ates had significantly higher ratings of being bullied than
those with popular associates.
In summary, teachers rated students with mild disabili-
ties as highest in being bullied, and gifted students lowest.
Further, while isolates had high ratings for being bullied,
those with popular associates had very low ratings for
being bullied.
For peer nominations of ‘‘picked on’’ there were sig-
nificant main effects of aggressive associates (F(1,441) =
5.33, p  .01; gp
2 = .023), and popular associates
(F(1,441) = 5.47, p  .01; gp
2 = .022). The main effects of
education status (F(2,441) = 2.17, p = .12; gp
2 = .010) was
non-significant. The interactions of education status by
aggressive associates (F(2,441 = 0.22, p = .80; gp
2 = .001),
education status by popular associates (F(2,441 = 0.17,
p = .84; gp
2 = .001), and education status by aggressive
associates by popular associates (F(1,441 = 0.04, p = .84;
gp
2 = .000) were also all non-significant. Post-hoc tests
indicated that the main effect of aggressive associates was
due to isolates (M = 181.04, SE = 41.59) having higher
numbers of nominations for being picked on than those
with (M = 75.92, SE = 12.91) or without (M = 59.68,
SE = 6.87) aggressive associates. Similarly, the main
effect of popular associates was due to isolates
(M = 181.04, SE = 41.59) having higher numbers of
nominations for being picked on than those with
(M = 47.41, SE = 9.67) or without (M = 67.99,
SE = 7.34) popular associates. In short, isolates had high
numbers of peer nominations for being bullied.
Discussion
The social dynamics of bullying and victimization involves
the interplay between the interpersonal characteristics of
individual students and the characteristics of the peer
groups in which they are embedded (Rodkin and Hodges
2003; Salmivalli et al. 1997). Consistent with prior
research with late elementary students (e.g., Adler and
Adler 1995; Hodges and Perry 1999; Pellegrini et al.
1999), our results indicate that bullying was positively
associated with peer nominated aggression and—more
modestly—social prominence, and negatively related to
peer social preference and peer nominations of positive
behavior. Victimization was positively related to peer
nominations for internalizing and modestly related to
aggressive behavior (positively) and social preference
(negatively). Further, while the effect sizes were small,
students who associated with aggressive peers and students
who associated with popular peers were more likely to be
identified as bullies. Students who were isolated were more
likely to be bullied than students were in non-popular
groups, who, in turn, were more likely to be bullied than
peers who were in popular groups.
Our findings help extend current viewpoints by showing
that academically gifted students, general education stu-
dents, and students with mild disabilities are differentially
involved in bullying. Further, such involvement is moder-
ated in part by their peer group membership. In our sample
of 5th graders, students with mild disabilities were more
likely to be viewed by peers as being bullies than
were academically gifted and general education students.
Teachers also perceived students with mild disabilities as
being more likely to be bullies and victims of bullying than
were general education students. General education stu-
dents were, in turn, more likely to be rated by teachers as
bullies and as being bullied than were academically gifted
students. In addition, students with mild disabilities who
associated with aggressive and popular peers were more
likely to be bullies than all other students. In contrast,
general education students who were not in popular or
aggressive groups were least likely to be nominated by
peers as bullies.
The importance of our findings comes into focus when
considered in light of ethnographic research on social
dynamics and survey research on social networks and
bullying participant roles. Ethnographic studies (e.g., Adler
146 J Child Fam Stud (2009) 18:136–150
123
and Adler 1995; Evans and Eder 1993) describe early
adolescent social dynamics as being a process where stu-
dents are constantly vying for social positions and for
social identities that protect against being victimized and
excluded by others. In such a context, students who are
social isolates are particularly vulnerable to being targeted
by bullies. Students who are themselves socially vulnerable
(e.g., students with mild disabilities) are likely to seek out
the approval of popular peers and to bully others as a way
to promote their own social positions. Further, by targeting
peers who are vulnerable, bullies appear to use aggressive
strategies as a way to consolidate the support of others in
their peer group (Salmivalli et al. 1997). In contrast, stu-
dents in middle-level peer groups (e.g., groups that are not
prominent or peripheral in the social structure) are more
focused on school activities than their social prominence
and are less likely to be involved in bullying (Adler and
Adler 1996).
When examined from this backdrop, our results have
important implications for understanding bullying and
victimization in late elementary classrooms. As students
jockey for position in their classroom social network, they
may seek to reduce their own social vulnerability by bul-
lying others and by associating with popular and aggressive
peers. Because they are socially vulnerable (Frederickson
and Furnham 2004; Gresham and MacMillan 1997; Sale
and Carey 1995), students with mild disabilities may be
susceptible to being bullied and may compensate by bul-
lying others and by affiliating with peers who support this
behavior. On the other hand, academically gifted students
tend to be fairly well accepted in late elementary school
(Austin and Draper 1981; Schneider et al. 1989) and may
focus more on academics than social prominence. There-
fore, while they tend to be socially prominent and associate
with peers who teachers view as being popular, academi-
cally gifted students do not appear to be vulnerable to being
bullied and seem to be relatively less likely to be involved
in bullying. Nonetheless, our findings suggest that general
education students who are not in popular or aggressive
groups—i.e., those who are not supported in aggression by
aggressive peers and are not fighting to maintain a prom-
inent position in the social hierarchy—may be the least
likely to be involved in bullying.
Our study has important implications for the develop-
ment of bullying prevention programs and for the inclusion
of students with exceptionalities in general education
classrooms. First, there is a need for teachers to be aware
that membership in both aggressive peer groups and pop-
ular peer groups may place students at risk for bullying
others. Teachers may then be able to utilize seating charts
and group activities to encourage such students to form
associations with prosocial peers. Second, there is a need
for anti-bullying efforts that focus specifically on the social
risks of students with mild disabilities. Such students are
susceptible to both bullying and being victimized and may
also develop peer affiliations that further support their risk
for involvement in bullying. Current efforts in these areas
may need to be modified to account for individual needs.
Third, there is a need to develop inclusion strategies that
are responsive to the differential social needs and risks of
students with exceptionalities and that focus on creating
classroom social contexts that reduce bullying by enhanc-
ing the social opportunities and social positions of all
students.
While our findings are compelling, our study has several
limitations that must be considered. First, it should be
noted that despite their significance, the effect sizes for all
the relationships discussed above are small—ranging from
.02 to .07. These effect sizes may be due in part to the
complexity of the models being fitted—inclusion of three
main effects in addition to two-two-way and one-three-way
interactions greatly reduces the variability that any one
component of the model can explain. Beyond that, how-
ever, this speaks to the complexity of the processes leading
to bullying and victimization: while exceptionalities and
peer relations are important, many other factors are at
work. This leads directly to the second major limitation.
There are a number of variables that could be confounds
that we could not include in the study. Family structure and
socio-economic status, for example, tend to be related to
the need for special education services and academic
giftedness, and also have demonstrated relationships with
social outcomes. These variables could be driving many of
the relationships we found and may be contributing to the
variance unexplained by the our model.
A third key limitation was that the sample only included
fifth grade students. While this was consistent with our goal
of focusing on classroom social dynamics during late ele-
mentary school and the emerging transition to early
adolescence, it is not appropriate to generalize our findings
to early elementary school or to middle school. Additional
research is needed that addresses our aims in samples of
both younger and older students. Fourth, the sample of
students with disabilities was small and students with dif-
ferent types of mild disabilities were aggregated together.
Although our sample was small in statistical terms, it is
fairly large relative to many studies that focus on the social
relations of students with mild disabilities. Because of
issues of critical mass (i.e., only a few students with mild
disabilities per classroom), it was necessary to have very
large samples to study this population. This made it nec-
essary to collapse students with different high incidence
disabilities into a single category. While this is not an
appropriate practice when the aim is to generate knowledge
about a particular disability category, it does demonstrate
the need to include a focus on students with disabilities in
J Child Fam Stud (2009) 18:136–150 147
123
bullying and victimization research (which was one of our
key aims). There is clearly a significant need for large scale
studies that have sufficiently large samples of students with
different disabilities.
Finally, our investigation did not examine gender dif-
ferences. It is likely that boys and girls are involved in
bullying and victimization in different ways. Girls are more
likely to be involved with relational forms of bullying and
aggression while boys tend toward physical forms of
harassment (Crick et al. 1996). These forms of aggression
have differential relationships with both social skills
(Farmer 2000) and social functioning with peers (Cillessen
and Mayeux 2004).This is especially important in light of
the higher incidence of mild disabilities among boys and
the higher rate of giftedness among girls. The relationships
between exceptionalities, bullying, and popularity may be
in many ways driven by these gender differences. Unfor-
tunately, it was not possible to examine gender differences
among students with exceptionalities because of our small
sample sizes. As a result, our measures also collapsed
across forms of aggression (i.e., included both physical and
relational aggression). While the resulting measures of
internal consistency were adequate, there may be differ-
ential relationships between exceptionalities, social
functioning, and different forms of aggression. Research
with larger samples is needed to explore gender differences
across different special education categories as well as
within general education populations. Such work with
larger samples may be able to disentangle the effects of
exceptionalities on bullying and social functioning behav-
ior from those associated with gender and forms of
aggression.
In conclusion, our study indicates that students with
mild disabilities, academically gifted students, and general
education students are differentially at-risk for involvement
in bullying and victimization. Further, membership in both
aggressive and popular peer groups supports bullying,
while membership in popular groups appears to protect
against victimization. While our work has several limita-
tions, it does provide clear evidence to suggest a need for
research that focuses more specifically on exceptional
populations in bullying research. In addition, the finding
that membership in popular peer groups may support bul-
lying but protect against victimization is highly
provocative and warrants more intensive examination.
Finally, our results suggest there is a need to carefully
consider and explore the social vulnerability and bullying
involvement of students with exceptionalities in the
development of anti-bullying interventions. While there is
currently a strong focus on inclusion and on the use of peer
support strategies (i.e., cooperative learning, peer tutoring)
to address the instructional needs of students with mild
disabilities in general education classrooms, our findings
suggest that some pairings of students may be advanta-
geous while others may be highly detrimental. There is a
critical need to scientifically generate new information in
this area that can ultimately be used to help guide peer
support practices and anti-bullying efforts.
Acknowledgments This research was supported by grants
H325C020106 and H324C040230 from the Office of Special
Edu-
cation Programs of the Department of Education to Thomas W.
Farmer (Principal Investigator). The views expressed in this
article
are ours and do not represent the granting agency.
References
Adler, P. A., & Adler, P. (1995). Dynamics of inclusion and
exclusion
in preadolescent cliques. Social Psychology Quarterly, 58, 145–
162.
Adler, P. A., & Adler, P. (1996). Preadolescent clique
stratification
and the hierarchy of identity. Sociological Inquiry, 66, 111–
142.
Al-Yagon, M., & Mikulincer, M. (2004). Patterns of close
relation-
ships and socioemotional and academic adjustment among
school-age children with learning disabilities. Learning Disabil-
ities Research and Practice, 19, 12–19.
Asher, S. R., & Coie, J. (1990). Peer rejection in childhood.
New
York: Cambridge University Press.
Atlas, R. S., & Pepler, D. J. (1998). Observations of bullying in
the
classroom. Journal of Educational Research, 92, 86–99.
Austin, A. B., & Draper, D. C. (1981). Peer relationships of
the academically gifted: A review. Gifted Child Quarterly, 25,
129–133.
Bagwell, C. L., Coie, J. D., Terry, R. A., & Lochman, J. E.
(2000).
Peer clique participation and social status in preadolescence.
Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 46, 280–305.
Cairns, R. B., & Cairns, B. D. (1994). Lifelines and risks:
Pathways of
youth in our time. Cambridge, England: Cambridge University
Press.
Cairns, R. B., Cairns, B. D., Neckerman, H. J., Gest, S. J., &
Gariépy,
J.-L. (1988). Social networks and aggressive behavior: Peer
support or peer rejection? Developmental Psychology, 24,
815–823.
Cairns, R. B., Leung, M.-C., Buchanan, L., & Cairns, B. D.
(1995a).
Friendships and social networks in childhood and adolescence:
Fluidity, reliability, and interrelations. Child Development, 66,
1330–1345.
Cairns, R. B., Leung, M.-C., Gest, S. D., & Cairns, B. D.
(1995b). A
brief method for assessing social development: Structure,
reliability, stability, and developmental validity of the Interper-
sonal Competence Scale. Behavioral Research and Therapy, 33,
725–736.
Cairns, R. B., Perrin, J. E., & Cairns, B. D. (1985). Social
structure ad
social cognition in early adolescence: Affiliative patterns.
Journal of Early Adolescence, 5, 339–355.
Cillessen, A. N. H., & Mayeux, L. (2004). From Censure to
reinforcement: Developmental changes in the association
between aggression and social status. Aggression & Violent
Behavior, 75, 147–163.
Coie, J. D., Dodge, K. A., & Coppotelli, H. (1982). Dimensions
and
types of social status: A cross-age perspective. Developmental
Psychology, 18, 557–570.
Crick, N. R., Bigbee, M. A., & Howes, C. (1996). Gender
differences
in children’s normative beliefs about aggression: How do I hurt
thee? Let me count the ways. Child Development, 67, 1003–
1014.
148 J Child Fam Stud (2009) 18:136–150
123
Estell, D. B., Cairns, R. B., Farmer, T. W., & Cairns, B. D.
(2002).
Aggression in inner-city early elementary classrooms:
Individual
and peer group configurations. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 48,
52–76.
Estell, D. B., Farmer, T. W., & Cairns, B. D. (2007). Bullies
and
victims in rural African American youth: Individual character-
istics and social network placement. Aggressive Behavior, 33,
145–159.
Estell, D. B., Farmer, T. W., Pearl, R., Van Acker, R., &
Rodkin, P. C.
(2003). Heterogeneity in the relationship between popularity
and
aggression: Individual, group, and classroom influences. In W.
Damon (Series Ed.), S. C. Peck, & R. W. Roeser (Vol. Eds.),
New directions for child and adolescent development: Vol. 101.
Person-centered approaches to studying human development in
context (pp. 75–85). San Francisco: Jossey-Bass.
Estell, D. B., Jones, M. H., Pearl, R., Van Acker, R., Farmer, T.
W., &
Rodkin, P. C. (2008). Peer groups, popularity, and social
preference: Trajectories of social functioning among students
with and without learning disabilities. Journal of Learning
Disabilities, 41, 5–14.
Evans, C., & Eder, D. (1993). ‘No exit’: Processes of social
isolation
in the middle school. Journal of Contemporary Ethnography, 22,
139–170.
Farmer, T. W. (2000). The social dynamics of aggressive and
disruptive behavior in school: Implications for behavioral
consultation. Journal of Education and Behavioral Consultation,
21, 194–208.
Farmer, T. W., & Farmer, E. M. Z. (1996). Social relationships
of
students with exceptionalities in mainstream classrooms: Social
networks and homophily. Exceptional Children, 62, 431–450.
Farmer, T. W., & Hollowell, J. H. (1994). Social networks in
mainstream classrooms: Social affiliations and behavioral char-
acteristics of students with EBD. Journal of Emotional and
Behavioral Disorders, 2, 143–155.
Farmer, T. W., Leung, M.-C., Pearl, R., Rodkin, P. C.,
Cadwallader,
T. W., & Van Acker, R. (2002). Deviant or diverse groups? The
peer affiliations of aggressive elementary students. Journal of
Educational Psychology, 94, 611–620.
Farmer, T. W., & Rodkin, P. C. (1996). Antisocial and prosocial
correlates of classroom social positions: The social network
centrality perspective. Social Development, 5, 174–188.
Farmer, T. W., Rodkin, P. C., Pearl, R., & Van Acker, R. M.
(1999).
Teacher-assessed behavioral configurations, peer assessments,
and self-concepts of elementary students with mild disabilities.
Journal of Special Education, 33, 66–80.
Fox, C. L., & Boulton, M. J. (2006). Friendship as a moderator
of the
relationship between social skills problems and peer victimiza-
tion. Aggressive Behavior, 32, 110–121.
Frederickson, N. L., & Furnham, A. F. (2004). Peer-assessed
behavioural characteristics and sociometric rejection: Differ-
ences between pupils who have moderate learning difficulties
and their mainstream peers. British Journal of Educational
Psychology, 74, 391–410.
Glew, G. M., Fan, M. Y., Katon, W., Rivara, F. P., & Kernic,
M. A.
(2005). Bullying, psychosocial adjustment, and academic per-
formance in elementary school. Archives of Pediatrics &
Adolescent Medicine, 159, 1026–1031.
Gresham, F. M., & MacMillan, D. L. (1997). Social competence
and
affective characteristics of students with mild disabilities.
Review of Educational Research, 67, 377–415.
Guralnick, M. J., & Groom, J. M. (1987). The peer relations of
mildly
delayed and nonhandicapped preschool children in mainstrea-
med playgroups. Child Development, 58, 1556–1572.
Guralnick, M. J., Hammond, M. A., Connor, R. T., & Neville,
B.
(2006). Stability, change, and correlates of the peer
relationships
of young children with mild developmental delays. Child
Development, 77, 312–324.
Hanish, L. D., & Guerra, N. G. (2002). A longitudinal analysis
of
patterns of adjustment following peer victimization. Develop-
ment and Psychopathology, 14, 69–89.
Hawkins, D. L., & Pepler, D. J. (2001). Naturalistic
observations
of peer interventions in bullying. Social Development, 10,
512–527.
Hodges, E. V. E., Boivin, M., Vitaro, F., & Bukowski, W. M.
(1999).
The power of friendship: Protection against an escalating cycle
of peer victimization. Developmental Psychology, 35, 94–101.
Hodges, E. V. E., & Perry, D. G. (1999). Personal and
interpersonal
antecedents and consequences of victimization by peers. Journal
of Personality and Social Psychology, 76, 677–685.
Janke, R. W., & Lee, K. (1991). Social skills ratings of
exceptional
students. Journal of Psychoeducational Assessment, 9, 54–66.
Kaukiainen, A., Salmivalli, C., Lagerspetz, K., Tamminen, M.,
Vauras, M., Maki, H., et al. (2002). Learning difficulties, social
intelligence, and self-concept: Connections to bully-victim
problems. Scandinavian Journal of Psychology, 43, 269–278.
Kavale, K. A., & Forness, S. R. (1996). Social skill deficits and
learning disabilities: A meta-analysis. Journal of Learning
Disabilities, 29, 226–237.
Leung, M.-C. (1996). Social networks and self enhancement in
Chinese children: A comparison of self reports and peer reports
of group membership. Social Development, 2, 146–157.
Luftig, R. L., & Nichols, M. L. (1990). Assessing the social
status of
gifted students by their age peers. Gifted Child Quarterly, 34,
111–115.
Masten, A. S., Morison, P., & Pellegrini, D. S. (1985). A
revised class
play method of peer assessment. Developmental Psychology, 21,
523–533.
Moon, S. M. (Ed.). (2004). Social/emotional issues,
underachieve-
ment, and counseling of gifted and talented students. Thousand
Oaks, CA: Corwin Press.
Nazuboka, D., & Smith, P. K. (1993). Sociometric status and
social
behavior of children with and without learning difficulties.
Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry and Allied Disci-
plines, 34, 1435–1448.
O’Connell, P., Pepler, D., & Craig, W. (1999). Peer
involvement in
bullying: Insights and challenges for intervention. Journal of
Adolescence, 22, 437–452.
Pearl, R., Farmer, T. W., Van Acker, R., Rodkin, P., Bost, K.
K., Coe,
M., et al. (1998). The social integration of students with mild
disabilities in general education classrooms: Peer group mem-
bership and peer-assessed social behavior. The Elementary
School Journal, 99, 167–185.
Pellegrini, A. D. (1998). Bullies and victims in school: A
review of
and call for research. Journal of Applied Developmental
Psychology, 19, 165–176.
Pellegrini, A. D., & Bartini, M. (2000). A longitudinal study of
bullying, victimization, and peer affiliation during the transition
from primary school to middle school. American Educational
Research Journal, 37, 699–725.
Pellegrini, A. D., Bartini, M., & Brooks, F. (1999). School
bullies,
victims, and aggressive victims: Factors relating to group
affiliation and victimization in early adolescence. Journal of
Educational Psychology, 91, 216–224.
Peterson, J. S., & Ray, K. E. (2006). Bullying and the gifted:
Victims,
perpetrators, prevalences and effects. Gifted Child Quarterly,
50,
148–168.
Preuss, L. J., & Dubow, E. F. (2004). A comparison between
intellectually gifted and typical children in their coping
responses to a school and a peer stressor. Roeper Review, 26,
105–111.
J Child Fam Stud (2009) 18:136–150 149
123
Rodkin, P. C., Farmer, T. W., Van Acker, R., Pearl, R.,
Thompson, J.
H., & Fedora, P. (2006). Who do students with mild disabilities
nominate as cool in inclusive general education classrooms?
Journal of School Psychology, 44, 67–84.
Rodkin, P. C., Farmer, T. W., Pearl, R., & Van Acker, R. M.
(2000).
Heterogeneity of popular boys: Antisocial and prosocial config-
urations. Developmental Psychology, 36, 14–24.
Rodkin, P. C., & Hodges, E. V. E. (2003). Bullies and victims
in the
peer ecology: Four questions for psychologists and school
professionals. School Psychology Review, 32, 384–400.
Rose, A. J., Swenson, L. P., & Waller, E. M. (2004). Overt and
relational aggression and perceived popularity: Developmental
differences in concurrent and prospective relations. Develop-
mental Psychology, 40, 378–387.
Sale, P., & Carey, D. M. (1995). The sociometric status of
students
with disabilities in a full-inclusion school. Exceptional
Children,
62, 6–19.
Salmivalli, C., Huttunen, A., & Lagerspetz, K. M. J. (1997).
Peer
networks and bullying in schools. Scandinavian Journal of
Psychology, 38, 305–312.
Schneider, B., & Daniels, T. (1992). Peer acceptance and social
play
of gifted kindergarten children. Exceptionality, 3, 17–29.
Schneider, B. H., Clegg, M. R., Byrne, B. M., Ledingham, J. E.,
&
Crombie, G. (1989). Social relations of gifted children as a
function of age and school program. Journal of Educational
Psychology, 81, 48–56.
Schwartz, D. (2000). Subtypes of victims and aggressors in
children’s
peer groups. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 28, 181–
192.
Schwartz, D., Gorman, A. H., Nakamoto, J., & Toblin, R. L.
(2005).
Victimization in the peer group and children’s academic
functioning. Journal of Educational Psychology, 97, 425–435.
Smith, P. K., Talamelli, L., Cowie, H., Naylor, P., & Chauhan,
P.
(2004). Profiles of non-victims, escaped victims, continuing
victims and new victims of school bullying. British Journal of
Educational Psychology, 74, 565–581.
Svetaz, M. V., Ireland, M., & Blum, R. (2000). Adolescents
with
learning disabilities: Risk and protective factors associated with
emotional well-being: Findings from the National Longitudinal
Study of Adolescent Health. Journal of Adolescent Health, 27,
340–348.
Troop-Gordon, W., & Ladd, G. W. (2005). Trajectories of peer
victimization and perceptions of the self and schoolmates:
Precursors to internalizing and externalizing problems. Child
Development, 76, 1072–1091.
Vaughn, S., Hogan, A., Kouzekanani, K., & Shapiro, S. (1990).
Peer
acceptance, self-perceptions, and social skills of learning
disabled students prior to identification. Journal of Educational
Psychology, 82, 101–106.
Walker, H. M., Kavanagh, K., Stiller, B., Golly, A., Severson,
H. H.,
& Feil, E. G. (1998). First step to success: An early intervention
approach for preventing school antisocial behavior. Journal of
Emotional and Behavioral Disorders, 6, 66–80.
Xie, H., Cairns, R. B., & Cairns, B. D. (1999). Social networks
and
configurations in inner-city schools: Aggression, popularity,
and
implications for students with EBD. Journal of Emotional and
Behavioral Disorders, 7, 147–155.
150 J Child Fam Stud (2009) 18:136–150
123
1
Exceptionalties
“Exceptionalities” is a general, nonclinical term to describe
conditions and behaviors that lie
outside the norm. These may include physical, emotional, or
psychological differences. For the
purposes of this DSM-5 update, the focus will be on the
exceptionalities that align with the
DSM-5 classification of neurodevelopmental disorders.
One major shift in the DSM-5 was to abandon the classification
of disorders usually first
diagnosed in infancy, childhood, or adolescence in favor of
more clinically appropriate
classifications. That is, the diagnoses formerly found in this
group were moved to classifications
more specifically related to symptomology. One of the new
classifications—neurodevelopmental
disorders—includes diagnoses generally made early in
childhood. A summary of changes in
diagnostic criteria of these disorders can be found below.
Neurodevelopmental Disorders
Intellectual Disability (Intellectual Developmental Disorder)
This group of diagnoses most closely resembles the formerly
described mental retardation
diagnoses. The phrase “mental retardation” is no longer used in
clinical or educational settings.
In addition to a change in the diagnostic label, there has been a
change to criteria as well. Mental
retardation diagnoses were previously identified as five separate
diagnoses reflecting severity of
impairment based on IQ score. The new intellectual disability
diagnoses include only two
diagnoses, with specifiers reflecting severity of impairment. It
is important to note that severity is
determined by adaptive functioning and cognitive capacity (IQ),
rather than solely by the latter,
as was the case in the DSM-IV.
Communication Disorders
This group of disorders includes deficits in language, speech,
and communication. Changes from
the DSM-IV to the DSM-5 for this group include new diagnostic
labels of speech sound disorder
(formerly phonological disorder), childhood-onset fluency
disorder (formerly stuttering), and
language disorder (a combination of expressive and mixed
receptive-expressive language
disorders). Also included is an entirely new diagnosis of social
(pragmatic) communication
disorder, which is characterized by persistent deficits in verbal
and nonverbal communication. It
is important to note that because social communication deficits
are a diagnostic criteria of autism
spectrum disorders, these diagnoses cannot be comorbid.
Autism Spectrum Disorder
The diagnostic grouping formerly known as pervasive
developmental disorders has been
streamlined and renamed autism spectrum disorder. This is a
single diagnostic label that includes
the formerly identified autistic disorder, Asperger’s disorder,
childhood disintegrative disorder,
and pervasive developmental disorder not otherwise specified.
The autism spectrum disorder is
characterized by two key criterion: a) deficits in social
communication and social interaction and
b) restricted, repetitive behaviors. The specifiers used with this
diagnosis include intellectual
impairment, language impairment, and associations with other
conditions, factors, or comorbid
diagnoses. It is important to note that the DSM-IV diagnosis of
Rett disorder has been entirely
removed, as it is considered the manifestation of the biological
condition Rett syndrome.
2
Characteristics of Rett syndrome often appear to be very similar
to autism spectrum disorder at
onset; however, symptoms often improve substantially after 5–6
years of age, making differential
diagnosis more clear.
Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder (ADHD)
Though the basic diagnostic criteria for ADHD is very similar
in the DSM-5, there are a number
of key differences from the DSM-IV, including stage-related
examples to aid in diagnosis in
childhood, adolescence, and adulthood. One of the key changes
has been to raise the
identification of symptomology from before age 7 to before age
12 and to use a single diagnosis
with specifiers rather than several related diagnoses in a group.
Specifiers replace prior subtypes,
identifying the predominant presenting symptomology.
Specifiers are also now used to reflect
severity of impairment of functioning.
Specific Learning Disorder
This is a new diagnosis in the DSM-5 and includes the entire
group of disorders from the DSM-
IV group learning disorders. These disorders included reading
disorder, mathematics disorder,
disorder of written expression, and learning disorder not
otherwise specified. Specifiers are used
to identify the areas of impairment (reading, written expression,
or mathematics).
Motor Disorders
This diagnostic group has replaced the motor skills disorder
group from the DSM-IV, which
previously included only developmental coordination disorder.
In recognition of the neurological
foundations of several disorders relating to control of movement
and coordination, this new
DSM-5 diagnostic group includes developmental coordination
disorder, stereotypic movement
disorder, and tic disorders. More clear language has also been
included in stereotypic movement
disorder criteria to better facilitate distinction from similar
behaviors associated with obsessive-
compulsive disorder.
Reference:
• American Psychiatric Association. (2013). Highlights of
changes from DSM-IV-TR to
DSM-5. Retrieved from:
http://www.dsm5.org/Documents/changes%20from%20dsm-iv-
tr%20to%20dsm-5.pdf
© 2013 Laureate Education, Inc.
Investigation of raising burden of children with autism, physical
disability and mental disability in China
Nina Xiong *, Li Yang, Yang Yu, Jiaxun Hou, Jia Li, Yuanyuan
Li, Hairong Liu,
Ying Zhang, Zhengang Jiao
Beijing Disabled Persons’ Rehabilitation Service and Guidance
Center, Beijing 100070, China
1. The investigation of raising burden of children with autism,
physical disability and mental disability in China
The economic burden of raising children with disabilities is a
problem in many countries. Study in South India
(Kandamuthan & Kandamuthan, 2004) indicated that, the mean
expenditure of the families with a severely disabled child
was $254 per year, which is significantly higher than the
corresponding expenditure of $181 per year of families with
normal
child. Of the disabled children, 80% were not getting any social
security payments and 90% had no special concessions for
medical and other educational purposes. Of the mothers of the
disabled children 21% were unemployed as against 12% in the
case of normal children. Parents of disabled children demanded
an average additional amount of $203 per year as social
security payments from the Government to meet the essential
necessities of their disabled children. There are extra costs for
disabled children for travel, domestic help, medical care, and
health care expenditures (hospital care, physician services,
dentistry, drugs and others) for disabled individuals; financial
impact of disablement in a child on the family is significant;
medical expenditure was a significant variable that
differentiated the disabled and normal child. Findings
(Honeycutt et al.,
Research in Developmental Disabilities 32 (2011) 306–311
A R T I C L E I N F O
Article history:
Received 21 September 2010
Received in revised form 29 September 2010
Accepted 7 October 2010
Keywords:
Autism
Physical disability
Mental disability
Raising burden
A B S T R A C T
The family economic burden of raising autistic children,
physical disabled children and
mental disabled children were evaluated in China. 227 parents
of children with autism,
children with physical disability, children with mental disability
and normal children were
interviewed for children’s costs, family income and economic
assistance, etc. The medical
cost and caring cost of disabled children were significantly
more than those of normal
children, and the education cost, clothes cost and amusement
cost of disabled children
were significantly less than those of normal children. Family
income was only predicted by
parents’ education level. Families of disabled children received
more economic assistance
than families of normal children except families of autistic
children. More children the
family had, less economic assistance the family acquired.
Compared with normal children,
the raising burden of children with disabilities were as follows:
children with autism
(19582.4 RMB per year), children with physical disability
(16410.1 RMB per year), children
with mental disability (6391.0 RMB per year). Families of
autistic children, physical
disabled children and mental disabled children have heavier
raising burden than families
of normal children, they need more help from many aspects.
� 2010 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.
* Corresponding author. Tel.: +86 10 63781942 8071.
E-mail addresses: [email protected] (N. Xiong),
[email protected] (L. Yang), [email protected] (Y. Yu),
[email protected] (J. Hou),
[email protected] (J. Li), [email protected] (Y. Li),
[email protected] (H. Liu), [email protected] (Y. Zhang),
[email protected]
(Z. Jiao).
Contents lists available at ScienceDirect
Research in Developmental Disabilities
0891-4222/$ – see front matter � 2010 Elsevier Ltd. All rights
reserved.
doi:10.1016/j.ridd.2010.10.003
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.ridd.2010.10.003
242019 Assignment due Week 5 - Mainstream media and HIVAIDS.docx
242019 Assignment due Week 5 - Mainstream media and HIVAIDS.docx
242019 Assignment due Week 5 - Mainstream media and HIVAIDS.docx
242019 Assignment due Week 5 - Mainstream media and HIVAIDS.docx
242019 Assignment due Week 5 - Mainstream media and HIVAIDS.docx
242019 Assignment due Week 5 - Mainstream media and HIVAIDS.docx
242019 Assignment due Week 5 - Mainstream media and HIVAIDS.docx
242019 Assignment due Week 5 - Mainstream media and HIVAIDS.docx
242019 Assignment due Week 5 - Mainstream media and HIVAIDS.docx
242019 Assignment due Week 5 - Mainstream media and HIVAIDS.docx
242019 Assignment due Week 5 - Mainstream media and HIVAIDS.docx
242019 Assignment due Week 5 - Mainstream media and HIVAIDS.docx
242019 Assignment due Week 5 - Mainstream media and HIVAIDS.docx
242019 Assignment due Week 5 - Mainstream media and HIVAIDS.docx
242019 Assignment due Week 5 - Mainstream media and HIVAIDS.docx
242019 Assignment due Week 5 - Mainstream media and HIVAIDS.docx
242019 Assignment due Week 5 - Mainstream media and HIVAIDS.docx
242019 Assignment due Week 5 - Mainstream media and HIVAIDS.docx

More Related Content

Similar to 242019 Assignment due Week 5 - Mainstream media and HIVAIDS.docx

1 of 2 Fall 2010 Management Program PowerPoint P.docx
1 of 2  Fall 2010 Management Program  PowerPoint P.docx1 of 2  Fall 2010 Management Program  PowerPoint P.docx
1 of 2 Fall 2010 Management Program PowerPoint P.docx
jeremylockett77
 
Directions essay 3 Write a post-session summary based on the com.docx
Directions essay 3 Write a post-session summary based on the com.docxDirections essay 3 Write a post-session summary based on the com.docx
Directions essay 3 Write a post-session summary based on the com.docx
mariona83
 
ExemplaryProficientProgressingEmergingElement (1) Respo.docx
ExemplaryProficientProgressingEmergingElement (1) Respo.docxExemplaryProficientProgressingEmergingElement (1) Respo.docx
ExemplaryProficientProgressingEmergingElement (1) Respo.docx
SANSKAR20
 
Aaa assessment 2Assessment description.pdfCritical Thinki.docx
Aaa assessment 2Assessment description.pdfCritical Thinki.docxAaa assessment 2Assessment description.pdfCritical Thinki.docx
Aaa assessment 2Assessment description.pdfCritical Thinki.docx
bartholomeocoombs
 
1 BUSS215 – Management Principles Portfolio .docx
1     BUSS215 – Management Principles Portfolio .docx1     BUSS215 – Management Principles Portfolio .docx
1 BUSS215 – Management Principles Portfolio .docx
honey725342
 
APA style with 3 or more referencesResearch Theory in Homeland S.docx
APA style with 3 or more referencesResearch Theory in Homeland S.docxAPA style with 3 or more referencesResearch Theory in Homeland S.docx
APA style with 3 or more referencesResearch Theory in Homeland S.docx
armitageclaire49
 
Examine current practice guidelines related to suicide screeni.docx
Examine current practice guidelines related to suicide screeni.docxExamine current practice guidelines related to suicide screeni.docx
Examine current practice guidelines related to suicide screeni.docx
cravennichole326
 
75645 Topic documenting the project life cycleNumber of Pages.docx
75645 Topic documenting the project life cycleNumber of Pages.docx75645 Topic documenting the project life cycleNumber of Pages.docx
75645 Topic documenting the project life cycleNumber of Pages.docx
sleeperharwell
 
CJUS 310Research Paper – Outline InstructionsYou must provide .docx
CJUS 310Research Paper – Outline InstructionsYou must provide .docxCJUS 310Research Paper – Outline InstructionsYou must provide .docx
CJUS 310Research Paper – Outline InstructionsYou must provide .docx
clarebernice
 
Assignment ApplicationUsing the DataInformationKnowledgeWisdo.docx
Assignment ApplicationUsing the DataInformationKnowledgeWisdo.docxAssignment ApplicationUsing the DataInformationKnowledgeWisdo.docx
Assignment ApplicationUsing the DataInformationKnowledgeWisdo.docx
rosemariebrayshaw
 
Evaluation Criteria for Applications and Formal Papers Level.docx
Evaluation Criteria for Applications and Formal Papers Level.docxEvaluation Criteria for Applications and Formal Papers Level.docx
Evaluation Criteria for Applications and Formal Papers Level.docx
SANSKAR20
 
MBA Instructional Methodology
MBA Instructional MethodologyMBA Instructional Methodology
MBA Instructional MethodologyLaura Sandera
 
MBA Instructional Methodology
MBA Instructional MethodologyMBA Instructional Methodology
MBA Instructional MethodologyLaura Sandera
 
Mba Instructional Methodology
Mba Instructional MethodologyMba Instructional Methodology
Mba Instructional MethodologyLaura Sandera
 
MBA Instructional Methodology
MBA Instructional MethodologyMBA Instructional Methodology
MBA Instructional MethodologyLaura Sandera
 
MBA Instructional Methodology
MBA Instructional MethodologyMBA Instructional Methodology
MBA Instructional MethodologyLaura Sandera
 
College of administration and finance sciences assignment (
College of administration and finance sciences assignment (College of administration and finance sciences assignment (
College of administration and finance sciences assignment (
RAJU852744
 
Scoring Full Points on Discussion Questions1. Use headings to se.docx
Scoring Full Points on Discussion Questions1. Use headings to se.docxScoring Full Points on Discussion Questions1. Use headings to se.docx
Scoring Full Points on Discussion Questions1. Use headings to se.docx
bagotjesusa
 
Assessment Task 3 What are the functions of management (Individu.docx
Assessment Task 3 What are the functions of management (Individu.docxAssessment Task 3 What are the functions of management (Individu.docx
Assessment Task 3 What are the functions of management (Individu.docx
galerussel59292
 
Topic   Explain why obesity is undesirable in the elderly and what
Topic    Explain why obesity is undesirable in the elderly and whatTopic    Explain why obesity is undesirable in the elderly and what
Topic   Explain why obesity is undesirable in the elderly and what
maryettamckinnel
 

Similar to 242019 Assignment due Week 5 - Mainstream media and HIVAIDS.docx (20)

1 of 2 Fall 2010 Management Program PowerPoint P.docx
1 of 2  Fall 2010 Management Program  PowerPoint P.docx1 of 2  Fall 2010 Management Program  PowerPoint P.docx
1 of 2 Fall 2010 Management Program PowerPoint P.docx
 
Directions essay 3 Write a post-session summary based on the com.docx
Directions essay 3 Write a post-session summary based on the com.docxDirections essay 3 Write a post-session summary based on the com.docx
Directions essay 3 Write a post-session summary based on the com.docx
 
ExemplaryProficientProgressingEmergingElement (1) Respo.docx
ExemplaryProficientProgressingEmergingElement (1) Respo.docxExemplaryProficientProgressingEmergingElement (1) Respo.docx
ExemplaryProficientProgressingEmergingElement (1) Respo.docx
 
Aaa assessment 2Assessment description.pdfCritical Thinki.docx
Aaa assessment 2Assessment description.pdfCritical Thinki.docxAaa assessment 2Assessment description.pdfCritical Thinki.docx
Aaa assessment 2Assessment description.pdfCritical Thinki.docx
 
1 BUSS215 – Management Principles Portfolio .docx
1     BUSS215 – Management Principles Portfolio .docx1     BUSS215 – Management Principles Portfolio .docx
1 BUSS215 – Management Principles Portfolio .docx
 
APA style with 3 or more referencesResearch Theory in Homeland S.docx
APA style with 3 or more referencesResearch Theory in Homeland S.docxAPA style with 3 or more referencesResearch Theory in Homeland S.docx
APA style with 3 or more referencesResearch Theory in Homeland S.docx
 
Examine current practice guidelines related to suicide screeni.docx
Examine current practice guidelines related to suicide screeni.docxExamine current practice guidelines related to suicide screeni.docx
Examine current practice guidelines related to suicide screeni.docx
 
75645 Topic documenting the project life cycleNumber of Pages.docx
75645 Topic documenting the project life cycleNumber of Pages.docx75645 Topic documenting the project life cycleNumber of Pages.docx
75645 Topic documenting the project life cycleNumber of Pages.docx
 
CJUS 310Research Paper – Outline InstructionsYou must provide .docx
CJUS 310Research Paper – Outline InstructionsYou must provide .docxCJUS 310Research Paper – Outline InstructionsYou must provide .docx
CJUS 310Research Paper – Outline InstructionsYou must provide .docx
 
Assignment ApplicationUsing the DataInformationKnowledgeWisdo.docx
Assignment ApplicationUsing the DataInformationKnowledgeWisdo.docxAssignment ApplicationUsing the DataInformationKnowledgeWisdo.docx
Assignment ApplicationUsing the DataInformationKnowledgeWisdo.docx
 
Evaluation Criteria for Applications and Formal Papers Level.docx
Evaluation Criteria for Applications and Formal Papers Level.docxEvaluation Criteria for Applications and Formal Papers Level.docx
Evaluation Criteria for Applications and Formal Papers Level.docx
 
MBA Instructional Methodology
MBA Instructional MethodologyMBA Instructional Methodology
MBA Instructional Methodology
 
MBA Instructional Methodology
MBA Instructional MethodologyMBA Instructional Methodology
MBA Instructional Methodology
 
Mba Instructional Methodology
Mba Instructional MethodologyMba Instructional Methodology
Mba Instructional Methodology
 
MBA Instructional Methodology
MBA Instructional MethodologyMBA Instructional Methodology
MBA Instructional Methodology
 
MBA Instructional Methodology
MBA Instructional MethodologyMBA Instructional Methodology
MBA Instructional Methodology
 
College of administration and finance sciences assignment (
College of administration and finance sciences assignment (College of administration and finance sciences assignment (
College of administration and finance sciences assignment (
 
Scoring Full Points on Discussion Questions1. Use headings to se.docx
Scoring Full Points on Discussion Questions1. Use headings to se.docxScoring Full Points on Discussion Questions1. Use headings to se.docx
Scoring Full Points on Discussion Questions1. Use headings to se.docx
 
Assessment Task 3 What are the functions of management (Individu.docx
Assessment Task 3 What are the functions of management (Individu.docxAssessment Task 3 What are the functions of management (Individu.docx
Assessment Task 3 What are the functions of management (Individu.docx
 
Topic   Explain why obesity is undesirable in the elderly and what
Topic    Explain why obesity is undesirable in the elderly and whatTopic    Explain why obesity is undesirable in the elderly and what
Topic   Explain why obesity is undesirable in the elderly and what
 

More from vickeryr87

COLLEGEPHYSICS LAB REPORTSTUDENTS NAME.docx
COLLEGEPHYSICS LAB REPORTSTUDENTS NAME.docxCOLLEGEPHYSICS LAB REPORTSTUDENTS NAME.docx
COLLEGEPHYSICS LAB REPORTSTUDENTS NAME.docx
vickeryr87
 
Collins did not understand the events that led to the reasoning .docx
Collins did not understand the events that led to the reasoning .docxCollins did not understand the events that led to the reasoning .docx
Collins did not understand the events that led to the reasoning .docx
vickeryr87
 
Define discrimination, victimization and affirmative actions; .docx
Define discrimination, victimization and affirmative actions; .docxDefine discrimination, victimization and affirmative actions; .docx
Define discrimination, victimization and affirmative actions; .docx
vickeryr87
 
Define data mining. Why are there many names and definitions for d.docx
Define data mining. Why are there many names and definitions for d.docxDefine data mining. Why are there many names and definitions for d.docx
Define data mining. Why are there many names and definitions for d.docx
vickeryr87
 
Define culture. How can culture be conceptionalizedDiscuss at l.docx
Define culture. How can culture be conceptionalizedDiscuss at l.docxDefine culture. How can culture be conceptionalizedDiscuss at l.docx
Define culture. How can culture be conceptionalizedDiscuss at l.docx
vickeryr87
 
Define cultural relativism and how it is used by anthropologis.docx
Define cultural relativism and how it is used by anthropologis.docxDefine cultural relativism and how it is used by anthropologis.docx
Define cultural relativism and how it is used by anthropologis.docx
vickeryr87
 
Define cost control and provide several examples of how it affec.docx
Define cost control and provide several examples of how it affec.docxDefine cost control and provide several examples of how it affec.docx
Define cost control and provide several examples of how it affec.docx
vickeryr87
 
Define corporate governance.Discuss the events that led up.docx
Define corporate governance.Discuss the events that led up.docxDefine corporate governance.Discuss the events that led up.docx
Define corporate governance.Discuss the events that led up.docx
vickeryr87
 
Define communication in your own words. Identify and distinguish amo.docx
Define communication in your own words. Identify and distinguish amo.docxDefine communication in your own words. Identify and distinguish amo.docx
Define communication in your own words. Identify and distinguish amo.docx
vickeryr87
 
Define Civil Liberties. List 5 Civil Liberties. How do they differ.docx
Define Civil Liberties. List 5 Civil Liberties. How do they differ.docxDefine Civil Liberties. List 5 Civil Liberties. How do they differ.docx
Define Civil Liberties. List 5 Civil Liberties. How do they differ.docx
vickeryr87
 
Define civilization. Do we really need it in order to survive and pr.docx
Define civilization. Do we really need it in order to survive and pr.docxDefine civilization. Do we really need it in order to survive and pr.docx
Define civilization. Do we really need it in order to survive and pr.docx
vickeryr87
 
Define case management and care management and compare the dif.docx
Define case management and care management and compare the dif.docxDefine case management and care management and compare the dif.docx
Define case management and care management and compare the dif.docx
vickeryr87
 
Define Bureaucracy.  Government at all levels has grown enormously, .docx
Define Bureaucracy.  Government at all levels has grown enormously, .docxDefine Bureaucracy.  Government at all levels has grown enormously, .docx
Define Bureaucracy.  Government at all levels has grown enormously, .docx
vickeryr87
 
Define and explain how the Twitter search function works to search f.docx
Define and explain how the Twitter search function works to search f.docxDefine and explain how the Twitter search function works to search f.docx
Define and explain how the Twitter search function works to search f.docx
vickeryr87
 
Define and relate these different terminologies and Information Gove.docx
Define and relate these different terminologies and Information Gove.docxDefine and relate these different terminologies and Information Gove.docx
Define and relate these different terminologies and Information Gove.docx
vickeryr87
 
Define and provide examples of-Basic probability- Bayes the.docx
Define and provide examples of-Basic probability- Bayes the.docxDefine and provide examples of-Basic probability- Bayes the.docx
Define and provide examples of-Basic probability- Bayes the.docx
vickeryr87
 
Define and discuss the phrase Manifest Destiny. Explain how this b.docx
Define and discuss the phrase Manifest Destiny. Explain how this b.docxDefine and discuss the phrase Manifest Destiny. Explain how this b.docx
Define and discuss the phrase Manifest Destiny. Explain how this b.docx
vickeryr87
 
Define and discuss the differences between vision and mission stat.docx
Define and discuss the differences between vision and mission stat.docxDefine and discuss the differences between vision and mission stat.docx
Define and discuss the differences between vision and mission stat.docx
vickeryr87
 
Define and discuss the four types of innovation. How might these.docx
Define and discuss the four types of innovation. How might these.docxDefine and discuss the four types of innovation. How might these.docx
Define and discuss the four types of innovation. How might these.docx
vickeryr87
 
Define and discuss the data wiping process.Discuss how a cloud.docx
Define and discuss the data wiping process.Discuss how a cloud.docxDefine and discuss the data wiping process.Discuss how a cloud.docx
Define and discuss the data wiping process.Discuss how a cloud.docx
vickeryr87
 

More from vickeryr87 (20)

COLLEGEPHYSICS LAB REPORTSTUDENTS NAME.docx
COLLEGEPHYSICS LAB REPORTSTUDENTS NAME.docxCOLLEGEPHYSICS LAB REPORTSTUDENTS NAME.docx
COLLEGEPHYSICS LAB REPORTSTUDENTS NAME.docx
 
Collins did not understand the events that led to the reasoning .docx
Collins did not understand the events that led to the reasoning .docxCollins did not understand the events that led to the reasoning .docx
Collins did not understand the events that led to the reasoning .docx
 
Define discrimination, victimization and affirmative actions; .docx
Define discrimination, victimization and affirmative actions; .docxDefine discrimination, victimization and affirmative actions; .docx
Define discrimination, victimization and affirmative actions; .docx
 
Define data mining. Why are there many names and definitions for d.docx
Define data mining. Why are there many names and definitions for d.docxDefine data mining. Why are there many names and definitions for d.docx
Define data mining. Why are there many names and definitions for d.docx
 
Define culture. How can culture be conceptionalizedDiscuss at l.docx
Define culture. How can culture be conceptionalizedDiscuss at l.docxDefine culture. How can culture be conceptionalizedDiscuss at l.docx
Define culture. How can culture be conceptionalizedDiscuss at l.docx
 
Define cultural relativism and how it is used by anthropologis.docx
Define cultural relativism and how it is used by anthropologis.docxDefine cultural relativism and how it is used by anthropologis.docx
Define cultural relativism and how it is used by anthropologis.docx
 
Define cost control and provide several examples of how it affec.docx
Define cost control and provide several examples of how it affec.docxDefine cost control and provide several examples of how it affec.docx
Define cost control and provide several examples of how it affec.docx
 
Define corporate governance.Discuss the events that led up.docx
Define corporate governance.Discuss the events that led up.docxDefine corporate governance.Discuss the events that led up.docx
Define corporate governance.Discuss the events that led up.docx
 
Define communication in your own words. Identify and distinguish amo.docx
Define communication in your own words. Identify and distinguish amo.docxDefine communication in your own words. Identify and distinguish amo.docx
Define communication in your own words. Identify and distinguish amo.docx
 
Define Civil Liberties. List 5 Civil Liberties. How do they differ.docx
Define Civil Liberties. List 5 Civil Liberties. How do they differ.docxDefine Civil Liberties. List 5 Civil Liberties. How do they differ.docx
Define Civil Liberties. List 5 Civil Liberties. How do they differ.docx
 
Define civilization. Do we really need it in order to survive and pr.docx
Define civilization. Do we really need it in order to survive and pr.docxDefine civilization. Do we really need it in order to survive and pr.docx
Define civilization. Do we really need it in order to survive and pr.docx
 
Define case management and care management and compare the dif.docx
Define case management and care management and compare the dif.docxDefine case management and care management and compare the dif.docx
Define case management and care management and compare the dif.docx
 
Define Bureaucracy.  Government at all levels has grown enormously, .docx
Define Bureaucracy.  Government at all levels has grown enormously, .docxDefine Bureaucracy.  Government at all levels has grown enormously, .docx
Define Bureaucracy.  Government at all levels has grown enormously, .docx
 
Define and explain how the Twitter search function works to search f.docx
Define and explain how the Twitter search function works to search f.docxDefine and explain how the Twitter search function works to search f.docx
Define and explain how the Twitter search function works to search f.docx
 
Define and relate these different terminologies and Information Gove.docx
Define and relate these different terminologies and Information Gove.docxDefine and relate these different terminologies and Information Gove.docx
Define and relate these different terminologies and Information Gove.docx
 
Define and provide examples of-Basic probability- Bayes the.docx
Define and provide examples of-Basic probability- Bayes the.docxDefine and provide examples of-Basic probability- Bayes the.docx
Define and provide examples of-Basic probability- Bayes the.docx
 
Define and discuss the phrase Manifest Destiny. Explain how this b.docx
Define and discuss the phrase Manifest Destiny. Explain how this b.docxDefine and discuss the phrase Manifest Destiny. Explain how this b.docx
Define and discuss the phrase Manifest Destiny. Explain how this b.docx
 
Define and discuss the differences between vision and mission stat.docx
Define and discuss the differences between vision and mission stat.docxDefine and discuss the differences between vision and mission stat.docx
Define and discuss the differences between vision and mission stat.docx
 
Define and discuss the four types of innovation. How might these.docx
Define and discuss the four types of innovation. How might these.docxDefine and discuss the four types of innovation. How might these.docx
Define and discuss the four types of innovation. How might these.docx
 
Define and discuss the data wiping process.Discuss how a cloud.docx
Define and discuss the data wiping process.Discuss how a cloud.docxDefine and discuss the data wiping process.Discuss how a cloud.docx
Define and discuss the data wiping process.Discuss how a cloud.docx
 

Recently uploaded

ESC Beyond Borders _From EU to You_ InfoPack general.pdf
ESC Beyond Borders _From EU to You_ InfoPack general.pdfESC Beyond Borders _From EU to You_ InfoPack general.pdf
ESC Beyond Borders _From EU to You_ InfoPack general.pdf
Fundacja Rozwoju Społeczeństwa Przedsiębiorczego
 
Polish students' mobility in the Czech Republic
Polish students' mobility in the Czech RepublicPolish students' mobility in the Czech Republic
Polish students' mobility in the Czech Republic
Anna Sz.
 
Mule 4.6 & Java 17 Upgrade | MuleSoft Mysore Meetup #46
Mule 4.6 & Java 17 Upgrade | MuleSoft Mysore Meetup #46Mule 4.6 & Java 17 Upgrade | MuleSoft Mysore Meetup #46
Mule 4.6 & Java 17 Upgrade | MuleSoft Mysore Meetup #46
MysoreMuleSoftMeetup
 
2024.06.01 Introducing a competency framework for languag learning materials ...
2024.06.01 Introducing a competency framework for languag learning materials ...2024.06.01 Introducing a competency framework for languag learning materials ...
2024.06.01 Introducing a competency framework for languag learning materials ...
Sandy Millin
 
GIÁO ÁN DẠY THÊM (KẾ HOẠCH BÀI BUỔI 2) - TIẾNG ANH 8 GLOBAL SUCCESS (2 CỘT) N...
GIÁO ÁN DẠY THÊM (KẾ HOẠCH BÀI BUỔI 2) - TIẾNG ANH 8 GLOBAL SUCCESS (2 CỘT) N...GIÁO ÁN DẠY THÊM (KẾ HOẠCH BÀI BUỔI 2) - TIẾNG ANH 8 GLOBAL SUCCESS (2 CỘT) N...
GIÁO ÁN DẠY THÊM (KẾ HOẠCH BÀI BUỔI 2) - TIẾNG ANH 8 GLOBAL SUCCESS (2 CỘT) N...
Nguyen Thanh Tu Collection
 
How libraries can support authors with open access requirements for UKRI fund...
How libraries can support authors with open access requirements for UKRI fund...How libraries can support authors with open access requirements for UKRI fund...
How libraries can support authors with open access requirements for UKRI fund...
Jisc
 
Unit 8 - Information and Communication Technology (Paper I).pdf
Unit 8 - Information and Communication Technology (Paper I).pdfUnit 8 - Information and Communication Technology (Paper I).pdf
Unit 8 - Information and Communication Technology (Paper I).pdf
Thiyagu K
 
Fish and Chips - have they had their chips
Fish and Chips - have they had their chipsFish and Chips - have they had their chips
Fish and Chips - have they had their chips
GeoBlogs
 
Sha'Carri Richardson Presentation 202345
Sha'Carri Richardson Presentation 202345Sha'Carri Richardson Presentation 202345
Sha'Carri Richardson Presentation 202345
beazzy04
 
Thesis Statement for students diagnonsed withADHD.ppt
Thesis Statement for students diagnonsed withADHD.pptThesis Statement for students diagnonsed withADHD.ppt
Thesis Statement for students diagnonsed withADHD.ppt
EverAndrsGuerraGuerr
 
Unit 2- Research Aptitude (UGC NET Paper I).pdf
Unit 2- Research Aptitude (UGC NET Paper I).pdfUnit 2- Research Aptitude (UGC NET Paper I).pdf
Unit 2- Research Aptitude (UGC NET Paper I).pdf
Thiyagu K
 
Language Across the Curriculm LAC B.Ed.
Language Across the  Curriculm LAC B.Ed.Language Across the  Curriculm LAC B.Ed.
Language Across the Curriculm LAC B.Ed.
Atul Kumar Singh
 
Operation Blue Star - Saka Neela Tara
Operation Blue Star   -  Saka Neela TaraOperation Blue Star   -  Saka Neela Tara
Operation Blue Star - Saka Neela Tara
Balvir Singh
 
Welcome to TechSoup New Member Orientation and Q&A (May 2024).pdf
Welcome to TechSoup   New Member Orientation and Q&A (May 2024).pdfWelcome to TechSoup   New Member Orientation and Q&A (May 2024).pdf
Welcome to TechSoup New Member Orientation and Q&A (May 2024).pdf
TechSoup
 
Ethnobotany and Ethnopharmacology ......
Ethnobotany and Ethnopharmacology ......Ethnobotany and Ethnopharmacology ......
Ethnobotany and Ethnopharmacology ......
Ashokrao Mane college of Pharmacy Peth-Vadgaon
 
The Roman Empire A Historical Colossus.pdf
The Roman Empire A Historical Colossus.pdfThe Roman Empire A Historical Colossus.pdf
The Roman Empire A Historical Colossus.pdf
kaushalkr1407
 
aaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaa
aaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaa
aaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaa
siemaillard
 
Additional Benefits for Employee Website.pdf
Additional Benefits for Employee Website.pdfAdditional Benefits for Employee Website.pdf
Additional Benefits for Employee Website.pdf
joachimlavalley1
 
Model Attribute Check Company Auto Property
Model Attribute  Check Company Auto PropertyModel Attribute  Check Company Auto Property
Model Attribute Check Company Auto Property
Celine George
 
The Art Pastor's Guide to Sabbath | Steve Thomason
The Art Pastor's Guide to Sabbath | Steve ThomasonThe Art Pastor's Guide to Sabbath | Steve Thomason
The Art Pastor's Guide to Sabbath | Steve Thomason
Steve Thomason
 

Recently uploaded (20)

ESC Beyond Borders _From EU to You_ InfoPack general.pdf
ESC Beyond Borders _From EU to You_ InfoPack general.pdfESC Beyond Borders _From EU to You_ InfoPack general.pdf
ESC Beyond Borders _From EU to You_ InfoPack general.pdf
 
Polish students' mobility in the Czech Republic
Polish students' mobility in the Czech RepublicPolish students' mobility in the Czech Republic
Polish students' mobility in the Czech Republic
 
Mule 4.6 & Java 17 Upgrade | MuleSoft Mysore Meetup #46
Mule 4.6 & Java 17 Upgrade | MuleSoft Mysore Meetup #46Mule 4.6 & Java 17 Upgrade | MuleSoft Mysore Meetup #46
Mule 4.6 & Java 17 Upgrade | MuleSoft Mysore Meetup #46
 
2024.06.01 Introducing a competency framework for languag learning materials ...
2024.06.01 Introducing a competency framework for languag learning materials ...2024.06.01 Introducing a competency framework for languag learning materials ...
2024.06.01 Introducing a competency framework for languag learning materials ...
 
GIÁO ÁN DẠY THÊM (KẾ HOẠCH BÀI BUỔI 2) - TIẾNG ANH 8 GLOBAL SUCCESS (2 CỘT) N...
GIÁO ÁN DẠY THÊM (KẾ HOẠCH BÀI BUỔI 2) - TIẾNG ANH 8 GLOBAL SUCCESS (2 CỘT) N...GIÁO ÁN DẠY THÊM (KẾ HOẠCH BÀI BUỔI 2) - TIẾNG ANH 8 GLOBAL SUCCESS (2 CỘT) N...
GIÁO ÁN DẠY THÊM (KẾ HOẠCH BÀI BUỔI 2) - TIẾNG ANH 8 GLOBAL SUCCESS (2 CỘT) N...
 
How libraries can support authors with open access requirements for UKRI fund...
How libraries can support authors with open access requirements for UKRI fund...How libraries can support authors with open access requirements for UKRI fund...
How libraries can support authors with open access requirements for UKRI fund...
 
Unit 8 - Information and Communication Technology (Paper I).pdf
Unit 8 - Information and Communication Technology (Paper I).pdfUnit 8 - Information and Communication Technology (Paper I).pdf
Unit 8 - Information and Communication Technology (Paper I).pdf
 
Fish and Chips - have they had their chips
Fish and Chips - have they had their chipsFish and Chips - have they had their chips
Fish and Chips - have they had their chips
 
Sha'Carri Richardson Presentation 202345
Sha'Carri Richardson Presentation 202345Sha'Carri Richardson Presentation 202345
Sha'Carri Richardson Presentation 202345
 
Thesis Statement for students diagnonsed withADHD.ppt
Thesis Statement for students diagnonsed withADHD.pptThesis Statement for students diagnonsed withADHD.ppt
Thesis Statement for students diagnonsed withADHD.ppt
 
Unit 2- Research Aptitude (UGC NET Paper I).pdf
Unit 2- Research Aptitude (UGC NET Paper I).pdfUnit 2- Research Aptitude (UGC NET Paper I).pdf
Unit 2- Research Aptitude (UGC NET Paper I).pdf
 
Language Across the Curriculm LAC B.Ed.
Language Across the  Curriculm LAC B.Ed.Language Across the  Curriculm LAC B.Ed.
Language Across the Curriculm LAC B.Ed.
 
Operation Blue Star - Saka Neela Tara
Operation Blue Star   -  Saka Neela TaraOperation Blue Star   -  Saka Neela Tara
Operation Blue Star - Saka Neela Tara
 
Welcome to TechSoup New Member Orientation and Q&A (May 2024).pdf
Welcome to TechSoup   New Member Orientation and Q&A (May 2024).pdfWelcome to TechSoup   New Member Orientation and Q&A (May 2024).pdf
Welcome to TechSoup New Member Orientation and Q&A (May 2024).pdf
 
Ethnobotany and Ethnopharmacology ......
Ethnobotany and Ethnopharmacology ......Ethnobotany and Ethnopharmacology ......
Ethnobotany and Ethnopharmacology ......
 
The Roman Empire A Historical Colossus.pdf
The Roman Empire A Historical Colossus.pdfThe Roman Empire A Historical Colossus.pdf
The Roman Empire A Historical Colossus.pdf
 
aaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaa
aaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaa
aaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaaa
 
Additional Benefits for Employee Website.pdf
Additional Benefits for Employee Website.pdfAdditional Benefits for Employee Website.pdf
Additional Benefits for Employee Website.pdf
 
Model Attribute Check Company Auto Property
Model Attribute  Check Company Auto PropertyModel Attribute  Check Company Auto Property
Model Attribute Check Company Auto Property
 
The Art Pastor's Guide to Sabbath | Steve Thomason
The Art Pastor's Guide to Sabbath | Steve ThomasonThe Art Pastor's Guide to Sabbath | Steve Thomason
The Art Pastor's Guide to Sabbath | Steve Thomason
 

242019 Assignment due Week 5 - Mainstream media and HIVAIDS.docx

  • 1. 2/4/2019 Assignment due Week 5 - Mainstream media and HIV/AIDS https://oregonstate.instructure.com/courses/1708319/assignment s/7429085?module_item_id=18423339 1/3 Assignment due Week 5 - Mainstream media and HIV/AIDS Due Thursday by 11:59pm Points 30 Submitting a text entry box or a file upload Assignment Rubric Submit Assignment Purpose Critical analysis is a vital part of learning. The goal of this assignment is to help you practice your skills in reading mainstream media articles and being able to critically evaluate it as a source of information. Instruc ons Your analysis of the article should exhibit careful thought, logical reasoning and provide evidence for your answers. Each post should be at least two well-developed paragraph (approximately 4-6 sentences each minimum). Use correct spelling, punctuation, and grammar. Identify an article released by the mainstream media to the
  • 2. general public that discusses HIV/AIDS. Example of an article is Botswana HIV Aids Deaths Decline (https://allafrica.com/stories/201810030288.html) . 1. Identify which levels of the SEM are being addressed 2. Identify which levels of the SEM not addressed 3. In what ways might this article lead to stigma and discrimination that places a person or group at risk of contracting HIV/AIDS? 4. Provide a written opinion on the how this article positively, negatively, or even excludes information that contributes to the stemming of the HIV/AIDS epidemic. Submission Details Due: Thursday by 11:59 pm Grading Criteria Opinion based in logical reasoning and evidence: 4 points Identified SEM levels (both addressed and not addressed): 2 points Length of post approximately 4-6 sentences or more: 2 points Spelling and grammar: 2 points 2/4/2019 Assignment due Week 5 - Mainstream media and HIV/AIDS https://oregonstate.instructure.com/courses/1708319/assignment s/7429085?module_item_id=18423339 2/3 Criteria Ratings Pts
  • 3. 8.0 pts 8.0 pts 4.0 pts 3.0 pts Organization 8.0 pts Full Marks Writing shows high degree of attention to logic and reasoning of points. Unity clearly leads the reader to the conclusion and stirs thought regarding the topic. 6.0 pts Above Average Writing is coherent and logically organized with transitions used between ideas and paragraphs to create coherence. Overall unity of
  • 4. ideas is present. 4.0 pts Adequate Writing is coherent and logically organized. Some points remain misplaced and stray from the topic. Transitions evident but not used throughout essay. 2.0 pts Inadequate Writing lacks logical organization. It shows some coherence but ideas lack unity. Serious errors. 0.0 pts No Marks
  • 5. Level of Content 8.0 pts Full Marks Content indicates synthesis of ideas, in-depth analysis and evidences original thought and support for the topic. 6.0 pts Above Average Content indicates original thinking and develops ideas with sufficient and firm evidence. 4.0 pts Adequate Content indicates thinking and reasoning applied with
  • 6. original thought on a few ideas. 2.0 pts Inadequate Shows some thinking and reasoning but most ideas are underdeveloped and unoriginal. 0.0 pts No Marks Development 4.0 pts Full Marks Main points well developed with high quality and quantity support. Reveals high degree of critical thinking. 3.0 pts Above Average
  • 7. Main points well developed with quality supporting details and quantity. Critical thinking is weaved into points 2.0 pts Adequate Main points are present with limited detail and development. Some critical thinking is present. 1.0 pts Inadequate Main points lack detailed development. Ideas are vague with little evidence of critical thinking. 0.0 pts No
  • 8. Marks Grammar and Mechanics 3.0 pts Full Marks Free of distracting spelling, punctuation, and grammatical errors; absent of fragments, comma splices, and run-ons. 2.0 pts Above Average Essay has few spelling, punctuation, and grammatical errors allowing reader to follow ideas clearly. Very few fragments or run-ons.
  • 9. 1.0 pts Adequate Most spelling, punctuation, and grammar correct allowing reader to progress though essay. Some errors remain. 0.0 pts Inadequate Spelling, punctuation, and grammatical errors create distraction, making reading difficult; fragments, comma splices, run-ons evident. Errors are frequent. 0.0 pts No Marks
  • 10. 2/4/2019 Assignment due Week 5 - Mainstream media and HIV/AIDS https://oregonstate.instructure.com/courses/1708319/assignment s/7429085?module_item_id=18423339 3/3 Total Points: 30.0 Criteria Ratings Pts 4.0 pts 3.0 pts Style 4.0 pts Full Marks Shows outstanding style going beyond usual college level; rhetorical devices and tone used effectively; creative use of sentence structure and coordination 3.0 pts Above Average Attains college level style; tone is appropriate and rhetorical devices
  • 11. used to enhance content; sentence variety used effectively. 2.0 pts Adequate Approaches college level usage of some variety in sentence patterns, diction, and rhetorical devices. 1.0 pts Inadequate Mostly in elementary form with little or no variety in sentence structure, diction, rhetorical devices or emphasis. 0.0 pts
  • 12. No Marks Format 3.0 pts Full Marks Meets all formal and assignment requirements and evidences attention to detail; all margins, spacing and indentations are correct; essay is neat and correctly assembled with professional look. 2.0 pts Above Average Meets format and assignment requirements; margins, spacing, and indentations are correct; essay is neat and correctly
  • 13. assembled. 1.0 pts Adequate Meets format and assignment requirements; generally correct margins, spacing, and indentations; essay is neat but may have some assembly errors. 0.0 pts Inadequate Fails to follow format and assignment requirements; incorrect margins, spacing and indentation; neatness of essay needs attention. 0.0 pts No
  • 14. Marks Exemplary Proficient Progressing Emerging Element (1): Responsiveness: Did the student respond to the main question of the week? 9 points (28%) Posts exceed requirements of the Discussion instructions (e.g., respond to the question being asked; go beyond what is required [i.e., incorporates additional readings outside of the assigned Learning Resources, and/or shares relevant professional experiences]; are substantive, reflective, and refers to Learning Resources demonstrating that the student has considered the information in Learning Resources and colleague postings). 9 points Posts are responsive to and meet the requirements of the Discussion instructions. Posts respond to the question being asked in a substantive, reflective way and refer to Learning Resources demonstrating that the student has read, viewed, and considered the Learning Resources and colleague postings. 7–8 points Posts are somewhat responsive to the requirements of the Discussion instructions. Posts are not substantive and rely more on anecdotal evidence (i.e., largely comprised of student opinion); and/or does not adequately demonstrate that the student has read, viewed, and considered Learning Resources and colleague postings. 4–6 points
  • 15. Posts are unresponsive to the requirements of the Discussion instructions; miss the point of the question by providing responses that are not substantive and/or solely anecdotal (i.e., comprised of only student opinion); and do not demonstrate that the student has read, viewed, and considered Learning Resources and colleague postings. 0–3 points Element (2): Critical Thinking, Analysis, and Synthesis: Is the student able to make meaning of the information? 9 points (28%) Posts demonstrate the student’s ability to apply, reflect, AND synthesize concepts and issues presented in the weekly Learning Objectives. Student has integrated and mastered the general principles, ideas, and skills presented. Reflections include clear and direct correlation to authentic examples or are drawn from professional experience; insights demonstrate significant changes in awareness, self-understanding, and knowledge. 9 points Posts demonstrate the student’s ability to apply, reflect OR synthesize concepts and issues presented in the weekly Learning Objectives. The student has integrated many of the general principles, ideas, and skills presented. Reflections include clear and direct correlation to authentic examples or are drawn from professional experience, share insights that demonstrate a change in awareness, self- understanding, and knowledge. 7–8 points Posts demonstrate minimal ability to apply, reflect, or synthesize concepts and issues presented in the weekly Learning Objectives. The student has not fully integrated the general principles, ideas, and skills presented. There are little to no salient reflections, examples, or insights/experiences provided. 4–6 points
  • 16. Posts demonstrate a lack of ability to apply, reflect, or synthesize concepts and issues presented in the weekly Learning Objectives. The student has not integrated the general principles, ideas, and skills presented. There are no reflections, examples, or insights/experiences provided. 0–3 points Element (3): Professionalism of Writing: Does the student meet graduate level writing expectations? 5 points (16%) Posts meet graduate-level writing expectations (e.g., are clear, concise, and use appropriate language; make few errors in spelling, grammar, and syntax; provide information about sources when paraphrasing or referring to it; use a preponderance of original language and directly quote only when necessary or appropriate). Postings are courteous and respectful when offering suggestions, constructive feedback, or opposing viewpoints. 5 points Posts meet most graduate-level writing expectations (e.g., are clear; make only a few errors in spelling, grammar, and syntax; provide adequate information about a source when paraphrasing or referring to it; use original language wherever possible and directly quote only when necessary and/or appropriate). Postings are courteous and respectful when offering suggestions, constructive feedback, or opposing viewpoints. 4 points Posts partially meet graduate-level writing expectation (e.g., use language that is unclear/inappropriate; make more than occasional errors in spelling, grammar, and syntax; provide inadequate information about a source when paraphrasing or referring to it; under-use original language and over-use direct quotes). Postings are at times less than courteous and respectful
  • 17. when offering suggestions, feedback, or opposing viewpoints. 2–3 points Posts do not meet graduate-level writing expectations (e.g., use unclear/inappropriate language; make many errors in spelling, grammar, and syntax; do not provide information about a source when paraphrasing or referring to it; directly quote from original source materials or consistently paraphrase rather than use original language; or are discourteous and disrespectful when offering suggestions, feedback, or opposing viewpoints). 0–1 points Element (4): Responses to Peers: Did the student respond to peer posts and contribute professionally? 9 points (28%) Responds to two or more peers in a manner that significantly contributes to the Discussion. 9 points Responds to one or more peers in a manner that significantly contributes to the Discussion. 7–8 points Responds to one or more peers in a manner that minimally contributes to the Discussion. 4–6 points Does not respond to any peer posts. 0–3 points 32 points 100% 25–28 points 78–88% 14–21 points
  • 18. 44–66% 0–10 points 0–31% © 2015 Laureate Education, Inc. Page 2 of 3 ORIGINAL PAPER Students with Exceptionalities and the Peer Group Context of Bullying and Victimization in Late Elementary School David B. Estell Æ Thomas W. Farmer Æ Matthew J. Irvin Æ Amity Crowther Æ Patrick Akos Æ Daniel J. Boudah Published online: 5 June 2008 � Springer Science+Business Media, LLC 2008 Abstract We examined bullying and victimization in 5th grade classrooms in relation to students’ education status and peer group membership. The sample consisted of 484 participants (258 girls, 226 boys), including 369 general
  • 19. education students, 74 academically gifted students, and 41 students with mild disabilities. Students with mild dis- abilities were more likely to be perceived as being bullies by both teachers and peers. Teachers also rated students with mild disabilities significantly higher for being bullied by peers. Academically gifted students were rated by teachers as the lowest for both bullying and being bullied. Associating with aggressive or perceived-popular peers increased the likelihood of being perceived as a bully. Social isolates were more likely to be bullied than students who did not associate with perceived-popular peers who, in turn, were more likely to be bullied than students who associated with perceived-popular peers. Students with mild disabilities who had aggressive and perceived-popular associates had more peer nominations for bullying than all others. In contrast, students in general education with neither aggressive nor perceived-popular associates had the fewest peer nominations for bullying. We discuss impli-
  • 20. cations for research and intervention. Keywords Bullying � Victimization � Mild disabilities � Giftedness � Peer groups Introduction In recent years, several distinct lines of inquiry have helped to inform current perspectives on bullying and aggression in schools. This includes research on the social and behavioral characteristics of youth who are at risk for bullying and victimization (Hodges and Perry 1999; Pellegrini et al. 1999; Schwartz 2000), developmental patterns of victimization (Hanish and Guerra 2002; Pellegrini and Bartini 2000; Smith et al. 2004; Troop- Gordon and Ladd 2005), broader classroom social dynamics and aggression in the peer group (Farmer et al. 2002; O’Connell et al. 1999; Salmivalli et al. 1997) and linkages between bullying, victimization, and other school adjustment factors (Glew et al. 2005; Schwartz et al. 2005). Collectively, these studies indicate that students
  • 21. involved in bullying and victimization are more likely to have academic and social adjustment problems, that social roles and peer group processes support bullying, and that the late elementary school years are a time when classroom social dynamics may be particularly important to bullying and victimization. D. B. Estell (&) Department of Counseling and Educational Psychology, Indiana University, 201 N. Rose Avenue, Bloomington, IN 47401, USA e-mail: [email protected] T. W. Farmer Department of Educational Psychology, School Psychology, and Special Education, Pennsylvania State University, University Park, PA, USA M. J. Irvin � A. Crowther Center for Developmental Science, University of North Carolina at Chapel Hill, Chapel Hill, NC, USA P. Akos Human Development and Psychological Studies Area, School of Education, University of North Carolina at Chapel Hill, Chapel
  • 22. Hill, NC, USA D. J. Boudah Department of Curriculum and Instruction, Special Education, East Carolina University, Greenville, NC, USA 123 J Child Fam Stud (2009) 18:136–150 DOI 10.1007/s10826-008-9214-1 Most research on bullying and victimization focuses on general school populations. Little work explicitly examines the involvement of students with exceptionalities. In the current climate of inclusion, students who receive excep- tional children’s services tend to make up 10–20% or more of the public school population and are integrated at least part of the school day with their general education peers. While it is likely that many studies on bullying include students who receive exceptional services within their samples, these children are typically not identified and it is
  • 23. not possible to determine the degree to which they impact results. This is potentially an important oversight because students at the extremes of school functioning (students with pronounced learning problems, and students achieving at advanced levels) may have differential social experi- ences that impact the degree to which they are involved in bullying relative to their general education peers (Kaukiainen et al. 2002; Peterson and Ray 2006). Peer group affiliations are associated with students’ level of involvement as bullies and victims (Estell et al. 2007; Salmivalli et al. 1997), and students who are at the extremes of school functioning tend to have peer affiliation patterns that are distinct from those of general education students (Farmer and Hollowell 1994; Pearl et al. 1998). It is possible that exceptional students’ involvement in bul- lying and victimization is related to their peer group affiliations. Information along these lines could help clarify how individual and peer group level factors come together
  • 24. to impact bullying and victimization in the classroom. Peer dynamics are important part of development. In late elementary school, children form hierarchical social structures in which some individuals and peer groups are more popular and central than others (Adler and Adler 1996; Farmer and Rodkin 1996). Taunting, teasing, direct confrontation, and physical attacks are forms of aggression that children (particularly boys) use to demonstrate their prowess and to establish and protect their positions in the social structure (Adler and Adler 1996; Farmer 2000; Pellegrini 1998; Rodkin and Hodges 2003). While physical aggression is almost universally related to being disliked (i.e., having low social preference; Asher and Coie 1990), aggressive behavior has a close association with perceived popularity (Rose et al. 2004), and social dominance— while eventually adapting a more affiliative nature—often begins with aggressive behavior (Pellegrini and Bartini 2000). Not only is aggression a common part of the daily
  • 25. interpersonal dynamics in school, distinct subtypes of aggressive youth can be differentiated in terms of popu- larity (Estell et al. 2002; Estell et al. 2003; Rodkin et al. 2000). Popular aggressive youth are more likely to asso- ciate with other popular and aggressive peers while unpopular aggressive youth are more likely to associate with non-aggressive and unpopular peers (Bagwell et al. 2000; Farmer et al. 2002). In short, while aggression and dominance are often related to being disliked, they can lead to high social status and perceived popularity (Estell et al. 2003). Social dominance and influence in the social structure also come into play in bullying. Although some bullies are themselves aggressive victims, many others have high social positions and are able to engage peers to support their behavior (Atlas and Pepler 1998; Salmivalli et al. 1997). In fact, bullying episodes in elementary school tend to involve several peers as onlookers, helpers, and
  • 26. encouragers (O’Connell et al. 1999). Although some chil- dren, usually girls, come to the aid of the victimized child, many youth appear to respond in ways that are aimed at protecting their status in the social structure including currying favor with dominant bullies (Adler and Adler 1995; Hawkins and Pepler 2001). A recent study of the social dynamics of bullying in two middle schools comprised almost exclusively of rural African American youth found that both bullies and vic- tims tended to have rejected sociometric status. However, while bullies were well integrated into their peer groups, victims tended to be withdrawn and marginal in the social structure (Estell et al. 2007). Bullies were heteroge- neously dispersed in both popular and unpopular groups and aggressive and non-aggressive groups and they were highly likely to be leaders of their peer group. Of particular interest to us, students who were bullies and/or victims tended to have behaviors (i.e., higher teacher ratings of
  • 27. attention problems and hyperactivity) and social skills (i.e., lower rates of peer nominations of prosocial behavior) that are typically associated with students with mild disabilities. Exceptional students include such children with mild disabilities as well as students who are identified as aca- demically gifted, though it is important to note that in our study both groups spend the majority of their school days in general education classrooms. For our purposes, the term students with mild disabilities refers to children with high incidence disabilities (e.g., learning disabilities, mild mental retardation, and mild emotional and behavioral disorders), academically gifted refers to high achieving children who have been identified for academically gifted education programs, and general education students refers to children who are not receiving special services to sup- port their learning needs. Students with exceptionalities tend to have patterns of social behavior, peer acceptance,
  • 28. and peer affiliations that are distinct from their general education peers. While both academically gifted students and students with mild disabilities differ from general education peers in terms of social functioning and social adjustment, they are also markedly different from each other. J Child Fam Stud (2009) 18:136–150 137 123 Prior to diagnosis, students later identified as having high incidence disabilities display social skill deficits and have low social status (Vaughn et al. 1990; Walker et al. 1998). In preschool settings, students with mild develop- mental delays are likely to have significant social interaction problems (Guralnick and Groom 1987) and these problems are likely to be sustained from the pre- school to the early elementary school years (Guralnick et al. 2006). In late childhood and early adolescence, stu-
  • 29. dents with mild disabilities tend to have problem social behaviors, social skills difficulties, and are often not well accepted by their peers (Estell et al. 2008; Farmer et al. 1999; Frederickson and Furnham 2004; Gresham and MacMillan 1997; Sale and Carey 1995). In turn, they are also more likely to be socially isolated (Kavale and Forness 1996; Pearl et al. 1998) and to report less social acceptance (Al-Yagon and Mikulincer 2004). Although they are more likely to have social difficulties, most students with mild disabilities are members of peer groups (Farmer and Farmer 1996; Pearl et al. 1998). However, the group membership of students with mild disabilities may support social problems as they are more likely to affiliate with classmates with problematic inter- personal characteristics and less likely to associate with peers with prosocial characteristics (Farmer and Hollowell 1994). Students with mild disabilities are also more likely to develop social roles that support aggression or that
  • 30. makes them the target of the aggressive behavior of others (Evans and Eder 1993; Farmer and Farmer 1996; Farmer and Rodkin 1996). In addition, students with mild dis- abilities who associate with antisocial peers tend to hold these peers in high esteem (Rodkin et al. 2006). These social functioning challenges may place students with mild disabilities at greater risk for involvement with bullying as both targets and aggressors. Problems with social functioning, including social information processing difficulties, social skills deficits, and social isolation are all risk factors for being victimized by peers (Fox and Boulton 2006). Likewise, elementary students who are chronically victimized are more likely to experience academic prob- lems and internalizing difficulties that may place them at- risk of later learning and behavior problems (Hodges et al. 1999; Schwartz et al. 2005). In contrast, affiliations with aggressive and deviant peers tend to be associated with a greater likelihood of bullying others (Estell et al. 2007).
  • 31. Collectively, these findings suggest that children with mild disabilities may be more likely to be involved in bullying as aggressors and/or targets than their typically achieving peers. Consistent with this view, extant data suggest that youth with mild disabilities are more likely to be victims than are their typically-achieving counterparts and they may be at increased risk of being bullies and bully-victims (Kaukiainen et al. 2002; Nazuboka and Smith 1993). Further, data from the National Longitudinal Study of Adolescent Health, which included 1,301 adolescents with learning disabilities from a sample of 20,780, indicate that youth with learning disabilities were significantly more likely than others to report involvement in violent behav- iors (31% vs. 25% for boys; 20% vs. 11% for girls) and were more likely to have witnessed or have been a victim of a violent act (Svetaz et al. 2000). In contrast to students with mild disabilities, children who are identified as academically gifted tend to have
  • 32. higher levels of social functioning than their general edu- cation peers. Preuss and Dubow (2004) found that academically gifted students were more likely to employ active problem-solving approaches to social stressors, and that being academically gifted moderated the impact of stressors on adjustment. Academically gifted students are rated by teachers as having highly positive levels of social skills (Janke and Lee 1991) and during the elementary school years they are likely to experience high levels of social acceptance and low levels of peer rejection (Austin and Draper 1981; Luftig and Nichols 1990; Schneider et al. 1989). Academically gifted students are also more likely to have prosocial or popular friends than their non-gifted peers and less likely to be socially isolated (Farmer and Rodkin 1996; Pearl et al. 1998; Schneider and Daniels 1992). On the whole, while they may face considerable stress due to their own high expectations (Moon 2004), gifted children are largely socially skilled, use effective
  • 33. social coping mechanisms, are socially integrated with prosocial peers, and are well adjusted. These factors tend to be negatively related to victimization, and may also make involvement with deviant peers and a subsequent increase in bullying behavior less likely. In summary, research on peer group dynamics and the social relations of exceptional students comes together to suggest that students with mild disabilities may be more likely to be bullies and victims and they may have social characteristics and peer affiliation patterns that exacerbate their risk for involvement in bullying. In contrast, aca- demically gifted students may have social characteristics and affiliation patterns that protect against bullying and victimization. Little work, however, has examined the relationship between educational status (i.e., academically gifted, general education, mild disabilities) and involve- ment in bullying, and less has looked at how this may interact with associations with aggressive and/or popular
  • 34. peers. Information along these lines may help clarify peer group dynamics that contribute to bullying and victimiza- tion in the classroom. Toward this end, we sought to examine differences among academically gifted students, students with mild disabilities, and general education stu- dents on ratings of victimization and bullying in relation to their peer associations. 138 J Child Fam Stud (2009) 18:136–150 123 Four research questions guided our work. Our first research question focused on the relationship between peer nominated social characteristics and involvement in bul- lying. Are teachers’ and peers’ perceptions of bullying and victimization related to peer nominations on key social factors (i.e., aggression, positive behavior, internalizing, social prominence, social preference)? We hypothesized that peer nominations of aggression would be positively
  • 35. related, and social preference negatively related to both teacher ratings and peer nominations of bullying. We fur- ther hypothesized that victimization would be negatively related to peer nominated popularity and social preference, and positively related to internalizing. Our second research question examined whether peers perceived classmates differently as a function of their education status. Do academically gifted students, general education students, and students with mild disabilities differ in the nominations they receive from peers? We hypothesized that gifted stu- dents would be viewed as popular, well-liked, having positive behavior, and neither aggressive nor high on internalizing, while students with special needs would be viewed by peers as aggressive, high on internalizing, and low on social preference, popularity, and positive behavior. Our third research question investigated the relationship between education status and associations with teacher- rated aggressive and popular peers. Is education status
  • 36. differentially linked to peer group membership? We hypothesized that gifted students would be least likely and students with special needs the most likely to have aggressive friends. We also hypothesized that gifted stu- dents would be most likely and students with special needs least likely to have popular friends. Our fourth research question explored the combined effect of education status and group types on involvement in bullying. Is bullying and victimization for students in different education status categories moderated by membership in aggressive and popular peer groups? We hypothesized that students with special needs who have aggressive but not popular asso- ciates would have the highest levels of bullying, and gifted students with popular but not aggressive associates would have the lowest levels of bullying. We further hypothesized that students with special needs and neither aggressive nor popular associates would have the highest victimization ratings while gifted students with popular and non-
  • 37. aggressive associates would have the lowest levels of victimization. Method We employed a multi-source survey design that included teacher- and peer-report measures as well as school record information. This included peer reports to identify groups and to examine how bullying and education status relate to a variety of other behaviors. We relied on teacher reports to classify the participants’ associates, and used both peer and teacher reports to examine involvement in bullying. Finally, we used school record information to determine the education status of the participants. Participants Participants came from eleven elementary schools in two school districts. Both districts were located in a state in the Southeastern United States. One district served a small metropolitan area and the other served a rural county. We obtained parent consent and student assent for 69% of the
  • 38. 701 students from 35 classrooms across the schools. Class size ranged from 18 to 27, with an average of 20 students per classroom. An average of nearly 14 of these 20 students participated in the present study. The sample consisted of 484 fifth graders (258 girls, 226 boys), including 369 (76.2%; 196 girls and 173 boys) general education stu- dents, 74 (15.3%; 50 girls and 24 boys) academically gifted students, and 41 (8.5%; 12 girls and 29 boys) students with high incidence disabilities (e.g., learning disabilities, mild mental retardation, emotional and behavioral disorders; see Table 1). While 10–20% of students typically qualify as having special needs, we examined only those students with special needs who spent the majority of their day in Table 1 Sample characteristics by education classification and gender Girls Boys Total Not in special education 196 (53.1%) 173 (46.9%) 369 (76.2%)
  • 39. Academic gift 51 (68.0%) 24 (32.0%) 75 (15.5%) Learning disability 3 (20.0%) 12 (80.0%) 15 (3.1%) Emotional handicap/emotional behavior disability 1 (20.0%) 4 (80.0%) 5 (1.0%) Mild mental retardation 2 (33.3%) 4 (66.7%) 6 (1.2%) Speech impairment 2 (33.3%) 4 (66.7%) 6 (1.2%) Other health impairment 0 (0.0%) 4 (100.0%) 4 (0.8%) Unspecified special need 1 (100.0%) 0 (0.0%) 1 (0.2%) 504 plan 2 (66.7%) 1 (33.3%) 3 (0.6%) J Child Fam Stud (2009) 18:136–150 139 123 general education classrooms. This reduced the number of children with special needs in the sample to the previously mentioned 8.5%. These percentages reflect the distribution of these populations within the school districts where this research was conducted and were also consistent with state level rates of having mild special needs and giftedness. All students who were in the top 15% of their class in
  • 40. achievement were considered academically gifted by the schools. Students in the mild disability sample were iden- tified by local school assessment procedures that reflected federal definitions and guidelines for learning disabled, behavioral disordered, health disability, noncategorical disability, mild mental retardation, and speech disability. The different definitions are not provided because all stu- dents with disabilities were collapsed into a single category (mild disabilities). We did this for three reasons. First, preliminary analyses indicated a high level of similarity between the different special education classifications for teacher-, peer-, and self-assessed characteristics. However, variability in the sample sizes for different categories, together with the very small sample size of some catego- ries, could have led us to misinterpret the results. Collapsing students into a single mild disabilities category protected against inaccurate comparisons across disability areas.
  • 41. Second, during the current climate of serving and identifying special education students based on service needs (e.g., mild and moderate need, high incidence ser- vice needs) rather than categorical approaches, there is considerable variability across states in terms of how categorical classifications are operationalized. By focusing on students with mild disabilities who were included in general education classrooms for most of the school day, we avoid the issue of variability in categorical classifi- cations. Third, our aim was not to identify characteristics associated with specific disabilities. Rather, our goal was to explore the relationship between bullying involvement and students with disabilities in general. Further, this approach has been used in other studies of the peer relations of students with disabilities in general education classrooms (e.g., Farmer et al. 1999; Sale and Carey 1995; Rodkin et al. 2006). Because we focused on the social relations of students
  • 42. with disabilities in inclusive settings, we recruited only those students who spent more than 50% of the school day in general education classrooms to participate. Therefore, all participants, regardless of education status, were included in general education classrooms for all or most of the school day, and all classroom assessments refer to inclusive rooms. Fifty-five percent (267/483) of our par- ticipants were Caucasian, 41% (198/483) were African American, 1% (3/483) were Latino(a), and 3% (15/483) were of another ethnicity. Ethnicity and special education status were related (v(6,n=483) 2 = 17.74, p .01). Among Caucasians, 70.4% (188/267) were in general education, 8.2% (22/267) had special needs, and 21.3% (57/267) were academically gif- ted. Eighty-three percent (165/198) African Americans were in general education, 8.6% (17/198) had special needs, and 8.1% (16/198) were academically gifted. All
  • 43. three Latino/a students were in general education. Finally, 80.0% (12/15) of individuals from other ethnicities were in general education, 13.3% (2/15) had special needs, and 6.7% (1/15) were academically gifted. Procedures We used group administration procedures when collecting the survey data. Before the administration of the survey, we assured participants their answers would be kept confi- dential, asked them to protect the confidentiality of their responses, and told them that they could stop participating at any time. We consulted teachers prior to administration about typical accommodations needed by students for exams, and made parallel accommodations where needed. During the survey, one administrator read the instructions and questions aloud, while additional administrators pro- vided mobile monitoring and assistance as needed. Teachers also completed rating forms on each participant during the group administration. Non-participants were
  • 44. told by their teachers to work on class assignments during the data collection. For all peer nomination measures, the probe focused on the classroom level (i.e., participants were told that they could only nominate peers in their classroom). We did this because the sample was in elementary school and there was minimal interaction among students in different classes. All peer nominations were made from free recall (i.e., we did not provide class rosters). Measures Social Cognitive Maps (SCM) For this measure, participants were asked ‘‘Are there some kids in your class who hang around together a lot? Who are they?’’ Following the procedures developed by Cairns and colleagues (e.g., Cairns et al. 1985), participants were instructed to list, from free recall, as many groups as they could think of in their class. In addition, they were asked to circle the individual or individuals who were the leaders of
  • 45. each group, if the group had a leader. To identify distinct groups within the classroom social network, we analyzed the SCM data following the proce- dures outlined by Cairns and colleagues (Cairns and Cairns 1994; Cairns et al. 1995a). SCM procedures have been 140 J Child Fam Stud (2009) 18:136–150 123 used extensively in research on school social networks in a variety of populations including a mixed-race urban and rural sample (Cairns et al. 1988), a mixed-race suburban sample (Rodkin et al. 2000), inner-city African Americans (Estell et al. 2002; Xie et al. 1999), Chinese students (Leung 1996), and students with special needs (Farmer and Hollowell, 1994). Three week test-retest reliability coeffi- cients indicate high short-term stability of children’s peer groups (i.e., 90% of groups maintain a majority of their members over this period; Cairns et al. 1995a). Further, the
  • 46. results of SCM analyses match observed affiliations (Cairns and Cairns 1994; Cairns et al. 1985). We identified a total of 45 primarily-male groups with a range of 2–9 members, and 29 male isolates. The mean group size for boys was 6.26. A total of 61 female groups were identified with a range of 2–9 members, and there were 14 female isolates. The mean group size for girls was 5.23. These means and ranges are consistent with those of other studies of late elementary and middle school peer groups (Bagwell et al. 2000; Cairns et al. 1988; Farmer and Hollowell 1994; Xie et al. 1999). Teacher Ratings of Social Adaptation Teachers rated participants on a number of items relating to social adaptation. These items were displayed in Likert- type seven-point formats. The present study utilized three of these items: ‘‘bullied by peers,’’ ‘‘bullies peers,’’ ‘‘manipulates friendships.’’ The two items relating to bul- lying (as aggressor or target) were used as dependent
  • 47. variables, and the ‘‘manipulates friendships’’ was included in the ICS-T factor analysis (described below) as a measure of relational aggression. ICS-T Teachers rated participants on the Interpersonal Compe- tence Scale—Teacher (ICS-T; Cairns et al. 1995b), an 18- item instrument which asks teachers to rate children on a variety of characteristics relating to aggressiveness, popu- larity, and academic competence. The items are displayed in Likert-type, seven-point scales. These scales are anchored by frequency (i.e., often vs. never fights, argues, etc.) or degree (very popular vs. not popular; has lots of friends vs. has very few friends). These 18 items load onto six sub-scales, though for the present study, only an aggression factor (‘‘argues,’’ ‘‘gets in trouble,’’ and ‘‘fights’’ from the ICS-T, with ‘‘manipulates friendships’’ from the teacher ratings described above added; a = .84) and the popularity factor (‘‘popular with boys,’’ ‘‘popular with girls,’’ ‘‘has lots of friends,’’ a = .81) were examined.
  • 48. Past research has shown that 3-week test-retest reliability coefficients are moderately high (i.e., .80–.92), median test-retest r across the factors are .81 for girls and .87 for boys, and 1-year coefficients are moderately strong (i.e., .40–.50; Cairns et al. 1995b). ICS-T has been shown to have convergent validity with direct observations, grades, school discipline reports, and peer nominations, and pre- dictive validity for early school dropout, teen parenthood, and criminal arrest in early adulthood, (Cairns and Cairns 1994; Cairns et al. 1995b). This measure has also been used extensively in past work on issues pertaining to popularity and aggression (Cairns et al. 1988; Estell et al. 2002, 2007; Farmer and Rodkin 1996; Rodkin et al. 2000). Peer Social Preference Participants were asked to nominate up to three classmates from a list of participants they liked the most and up to three they liked the least, and social preference was cal- culated following the criteria described in Asher and Coie
  • 49. (1990). Specifically, each participant’s number of nomi- nations for being most liked and least liked were standardized within class, and social preference score was calculated by subtracting their liked-least z-score from their liked-most z-score. Peer Interpersonal Assessments We used peer interpersonal assessments to determine classmates’ perceptions of peers’ social and behavioral characteristics. Students were asked to nominate, from free recall, up to three classmates who best fit descriptors for several items. They were told during the testing procedures that they could nominate the same person for more than one item, they did not need to fill all three blanks if they did not know three people who fit an item, they could bypass an item if they felt that they did not know anyone at all who fit that particular item, and that they could nomi- nate themselves. For analyses using these items, however, all self-nominations were removed from consideration,
  • 50. making these peer-nominations exclusively. Past studies using these measures have indicated 3-week test-retest reliability with individual items ranged from .72 to .93. These items are identical with or similar to peer assessments used by other investigators (e.g., Coie et al. 1982; Masten et al. 1985), and past work has found strong evidence of reliability and validity in diverse samples (Coie et al. 1982; Masten et al.1985). We divided the total num- ber of nominations participants received for each peer assessment item by the total number of possible nominators (i.e., all participants in the class). Because the denominator was the total number of participants in each class, the resulting proportions were in many cases quite small. In order to make mean differences clearer, we linearly trans- formed these proportions by multiplying them by 1,000. J Child Fam Stud (2009) 18:136–150 141 123
  • 51. We ran a principal components analysis with a Varimax rotation on 12 items (‘‘bully’’ and ‘‘picked on’’ were excluded from the factor analysis and instead used as dependent variables). Four factors had eigenvalues in excess of 1.0, and a scree plot indicated that a four-factor solution provided the best fit to the data, as there was a major change in slope between the fourth and fifth factors. This four- component solution accounted for 75.6% of the variance in the items. As seen in the rotated loading matrix in Table 2, all items loaded on their corresponding component in excess of .74, and no item cross-loaded on another component greater than .31. The resulting factors were aggression (Cronbach’s a = .87; consists of ‘‘disruptive,’’ ‘‘starts fights,’’ ‘‘gets in trouble,’’ and ‘‘starts rumors’’), positive behavior (a = .85; consists of ‘‘cooperative,’’ ‘‘good stu- dent,’’ and ‘‘friendly’’), social promience (a = .76; consists of ‘‘athletic,’’ ‘‘cool,’’ and ‘‘popular’’), and internalizing (a = .66; consists of ‘‘acts shy’’ and ‘‘sad’’). Peer-group Types We classified the peer groups identified by the SCM
  • 52. analysis according to the aggression and popularity level of their constituent members. Consistent with past work examining peer-group characteristics (Farmer et al. 2002), we calculated within-sex and -class z-scores for the ICS-T aggression and popularity factors. We classified a partici- pant as popular or aggressive if their z-score was greater than or equal to ?.50. This resulted in 32.2% (146/454) of participants being identified as popular and 31.0% (140/ 452) as aggressive. Peer-group types were a function of the proportion and number of aggressive and popular members in the group. For the example of aggression, we classified a participant as having aggressive associates if at least 50% of their SCM-derived peer group members were classified as aggressive or, in larger groups, if they had at least three aggressive associates. We used identical criteria to classify groups by the proportion and/or number of popular members.
  • 53. Results We present out results in four major sections, corre- sponding to our four research questions. The first section uses correlation analyses to examine how peer-nominated interpersonal characteristics relate to bullying and victim- ization. The second section uses ANOVAs to examine the relationship between educational status and peer-nomi- nated characteristics. The third section uses v2 analyses to investigate the relationship between education status and aggressive and popular groups. The fourth and final section employs MANOVAs with follow-up ANOVAs and Tu- key’s tests to explore the combined effect of education status and group types on bullying and victimization. Peer Behavioral Nominations and Bullying and Victimization We calculated correlations within classrooms and then combined them for the following analyses. Due to the number of correlations run, we only considered those sig-
  • 54. nificant at less than the .0025 level (.05/20) as significant. Teacher ratings of ‘‘bullies’’ were positively related to peer nominated aggression (r(454) = .50, p .001) and promi- nence (r(454) = .21, p .001) factors, negatively related to the peer nominated positive behavior factor (r(454) = -.17, p .001) and social preference (r(451) = -.28, p .001), and unrelated to the peer nominated internalizing factor (r(454) = -.01, p = .86). Peer nominations for ‘‘bully’’ were positively related to the peer aggression factor (r(480) = .89, p .001) and the prominence factor (r(480) = .40, p .001), negatively related to social pref- erence (r(477) = -.24, p .001), and unrelated to the peer positive behavior (r(480) = -.08, p = .08) and internaliz- ing (r(480) = .08, p = .10) factors. Teacher ratings of ‘‘bullied’’ were negatively related to social preference (r(452) = -.17, p .001), but unrelated to aggression (r(455) = .06, p = .19), internalizing (r(455) = .28, p = .05), positive behavior (r(455) = -.13, p = .01), or prominence (r(455) = -.11, p = .05). Peer nominations of ‘‘picked on’’ were positively related to aggression (r(480) = .29, p .001) and internalizing Table 2 Peer nomination loadings for varimax-rotated
  • 55. component matrix Component Aggression Positive behav. Social prom. Internalizing Disruptive .90 -.04 -.02 -.02 Starts fights .86 -.06 .24 .06 Gets in trouble .82 -.11 .14 .02 Starts rumors .76 .07 .25 -.04 Cooperative -.06 .88 .25 .12 Good student .01 .85 .12 -.01 Friendly -.09 .86 .11 .17 Athletic .26 -.00 .74 .06 Cool .25 .24 .83 -.10
  • 56. Popular .02 .31 .78 -.06 Acts shy -.07 .21 .02 .84 Sad .08 .01 -.07 .86 Note: all loadings in excess of .4 are bolded 142 J Child Fam Stud (2009) 18:136–150 123 (r(480) = .81, p .001), negatively related to social pref- erence (r(477) = -.37, p .001), and unrelated to positive behavior (r(480) = .04, p = .44) or prominence (r(480) = -.03, p = .55). These results indicate that both teachers and students viewed bullying as related to aggression and social prom- inence, but also associated with being disliked (i.e., low social preference). Teachers also saw bullying as related to low levels of prosocial behaviors. Both students and teachers viewed being the target of bullying as related to being disliked, and students further saw it as related to being aggressive and high in internalizing behavior.
  • 57. Education Status and Peer Behavioral Nominations Table 3 shows the means and standard deviations of the peer-nomination factors and social preference by educa- tional classification. Education status was not related to either the social prominence (F(2,477) = 1.04, p = .35; gp 2 = .004) or aggression (F(2,477) = 0.79, p = .45; gp 2 = .003) factors. It was related to peer-nominated pro- social behavior (F(2,477) = 7.13, p .001; gp 2 = .029), internalizing (F(2,477) = 8.43, p .001; gp 2 = .034), and social preference (F(2,474) = 4.61, p .01; gp 2 = .019). Post-hoc Tukey’s tests indicated that academically gifted students had more positive behavior nominations than general education students or students with mild disabili- ties. Students with mild disabilities had higher internalizing nominations than general education or academically gifted
  • 58. students. Finally, academically gifted students had signifi- cantly higher social preference scores than students with mild disabilities. Education Status, Behavioral Classification, and Peer Associates Education status was not significantly related to the tea- cher-rated aggressive classification (v(2,n=452) 2 = 4.51, p = .11). For students in general education, 32.7% (111/ 339) were rated by teachers as aggressive. This compared to 35.0% (14/40) of students with mild disabilities and 20.5% (15/73) of academically gifted students. Education status was also not related to teacher-rated popularity status (v(2,n=454) 2 = 4.00, p = .14). Among children in general education, 30.6% (104/340) were classified as popular, as were 27.5% (11/40) of those with mild disabilities and 41.8% (31/74) of academically gifted participants. Our analyses for peer associations suggested that there
  • 59. were differences in peer group affiliations as a function of education status. The effect for aggressive associates was not significant (v(2,n=465) 2 = 5.30, p = .07). The differences for education status were pronounced for popular associ- ates (v(2,n=465) 2 = 39.37, p .001). A lower proportion (57/ 350, or 16.3%) of participants in general education had popular associates than expected by chance (Fisher’s Exact Probability .001), and a higher proportion (36/74, or 48.6%) of academically gifted students had popular asso- ciates (Fisher’s Exact Probability .001). The proportion of students with mild disabilities (6/41, or 14.6%) was not significantly different from chance (Fisher’s Exact Probability = .19). These results show that students in general education, those who are gifted, and those who have mild disabilities were all equally likely to be considered aggressive or popular, and are equally likely to have aggressive associ- ates. Gifted students were, however, more likely to have
  • 60. popular associates. Bullying and Victimization by Education Status and Group Types Bullying by Education Status and Group Types To examine the potential interaction of groups and edu- cation classifications on bullying, we ran a MANOVA with education classification, popular associates, and aggressive associates as independent variables and peer-nominations and teacher-ratings of being a bully as the dependent variables. The MANOVA indicated that there were main effects of education status (Wilks’ K = 0.93, Table 3 Peer-nominated characteristics by education status Variable Education status General education Mild disabilities Academically gifted M SD M SD M SD Social prominence 82.06 94.43 89.45 143.21 100.00 92.77 Aggression 56.73 102.65 76.34 137.69 51.27 85.08 Positive behavior 92.75A 107.29 70.39A 96.28 141.07B 136.23
  • 61. Internalizing 52.64A 73.32 96.87B 121.03 34.89A 62.22 Social preference 0.11AB 1.45 -0.31A 1.61 0.56B 1.69 Different superscripts indicate significant differences (p .01) J Child Fam Stud (2009) 18:136–150 143 123 F(4,884) = 8.55, p .001; gp 2 = .053), aggressive-associ- ates (Wilks’ K = 0.93, F(2,442) = 16.93, p .001; gp 2 = .071), popular-associates (Wilks’ K = 0.96, F(2,442) = 9.57, p .001; gp 2 = .041), education status by aggressive associates (Wilks’ K = 0.95, F(4,884) = 5.57, p .001; gp 2 = .048), education status by popular associ- ates (Wilks’ K = 0.94, F(4,884) = 6.42, p .001; gp 2 = .043), and the three-way interaction of education status by aggressive associates by popular associates (Wilks’ K = 0.94, F(2,442) = 14.59, p .001; gp
  • 62. 2 = .062). To probe these multivariate effects, we ran follow-up ANOVAs for each dependent. For teacher ratings of ‘‘bullies’’ we found main effects for education status (F(2,443) = 4.79, p .01; gp 2 = .021), and aggressive associates (F(1,443) = 9.05, p .01; gp 2 = .020), but no main effect of popular associates (F(1,443) = 1.49, p = .22; gp 2 = .003), and no two-way interactive effects of educa- tion status by either aggressive associates (F(2,443) = 0.12, p = .88; gp 2 = .001) or popular associates (F(2,443) = 3.01, p = .05; gp 2 = .013), and no three-way interaction of edu- cation status by aggressive group by popularity group (F(1,443) = 0.35, p = .55; gp 2 = .001). Post-hoc tests indicated that the main effect of education status was due to students with mild disabilities (M = 4.16,
  • 63. SE = 0.54) having significantly higher teacher ratings of being bullies than those in general education (M = 3.15, SE = 0.17) who in turn had higher ratings of being bullies than academically gifted students (M = 2.14, SE = 0.26). The main effect of aggressive associates was due to those with aggressive associates (M = 4.21, SE = 0.43) having higher ratings for being bullies than those without aggressive associates (M = 2.43, SE = 0.17). In summary, teacher ratings of bullying were highest among students with mild disabilities and lowest among gifted students. Further, all students with aggressive asso- ciates had higher teacher ratings of bullying. For peer-nominations of ‘‘bully’’ there were main effects for education status (F(1,443) = 11.59, p .001; gp 2 = .050), aggressive associates (F(1,443) = 33.53, p .001; gp 2 =. 070), and popular associates (F(1,443) = 18.74, p .001; gp 2 = .041). There were also two-way interactions of edu-
  • 64. cation status by aggressive associates (F(2,443) = 8.96, p .001; gp 2 = .039) and popular associates (F(2,443) = 9.48, p .001; gp 2 = .041). Finally, the three-way interac- tion of education status by aggressive group by popularity group was significant (F(1,443) = 21.42, p .001; gp 2 = .046). Post-hoc tests indicated that the main effect of education status was due to individuals with mild disabilities (M = 200.97, SE = 29.51) having more peer nominations for bullying than students in general education (M = 56.64, SE = 9.25) or academically gifted students (M = 50.89, SE = 14.17). The main effect of aggressive associates was due to those with aggressive associates (M = 190.19, SE = 23.65) having more nominations for bullying than those without aggressive associates (M = 38.69, SE = 9.33). Similarly, the main effect of
  • 65. popular associates was due to those with popular associates (M = 162.74, SE = 22.89) having more nominations for bullying than those without popular associates (M = 61.57, SE = 10.56). Figures 1 and 2 show the two-way interactions of edu- cation status by aggressive associates and education status by popular associates, respectively. Post-hoc tests indicate that students with mild disabilities and aggressive associ- ates had more peer nominations of being bullies than all other cells. Individuals in general education and aggressive associates had significantly more peer nominations for being bullies than all those without aggressive associates. Finally, academically gifted students with aggressive associates had more peer nominations for bullying than academically gifted students and general education stu- dents without aggressive associates. Post-hoc tests for the education status by popular asso- ciates interaction indicated that students with mild
  • 66. disabilities and popular associates had more peer nomina- tions for being bullies than all others. Students with mild disabilities without popular associates had significantly 0 100 200 300 400 500 600 700 Agg. Assoc. Not Agg. Assoc. P ee r "B ul ly " N
  • 67. om in at io ns General Education Students Students With Mild Disabilities Academically Gifted StudentsD C BC ABA A Fig. 1 Peer nominations of ‘‘bully’’ by education status and aggressive affiliations 144 J Child Fam Stud (2009) 18:136–150 123 more peer nominations for being bullies than those in general education with popular associates and academi- cally gifted students with popular associates. Figure 3 shows the three-way interaction of education status by aggressive associates by popular associates. Post-
  • 68. hoc tests indicated that individuals with mild disabilities who had aggressive and popular associates had more peer nominations for bullying than all others. On the other end of the spectrum, individuals in general education with neither aggressive nor popular associates had the lowest number of nominations. These individuals significantly differed from all participants with aggressive associates as well as students with mild disabilities with neither aggressive nor popular associates. In summary, peer nominations for bullying were highest among students with mild disabilities, students with aggressive associates, and students with popular associates. But these relationships were more complicated. While having aggressive associates was related to higher nomi- nations for bullying for all groups, the effect was especially evident among students with mild disabilities, and much less marked in gifted students. A more pronounced dif- ference occured with popular associates. While having
  • 69. popular associates was related to higher nominations for bullying in students with mild disabilities, popular associations related to fewer nominations for bullying in gifted and general education students. Finally, these vary- ing types of associations exacerbated each others’ effects: students with mild disabilities who belonged to groups of both aggressive and popular peers had extremely high numbers of peer nominations for bullying. Victimization by Education Status and Group Types We ran parallel analyses to those for bullying for victim- ization, with the inclusion of isolates as a third group type. We ran a MANOVA with education classification, popular associates, and aggressive associates as independent vari- ables and peer-nominations of being picked on and teacher- ratings of being bullied as the dependent variables. This MANOVA indicated that there was a main effect of edu- cation status (Wilks’ K = 0.95, F(4,886) = 5.92, p .001; gp 2 = .037), aggressive associates (Wilks’ K = 0.97,
  • 70. F(2,443) = 4.66, p .01; gp 2 = .020) and popular associates (Wilks’ K = 0.96, F(2,443) = 4.68, p .01; gp 2 = .021). There were no interactive effects for education status by either aggressive associates (Wilks’ K = 1.00, F(4,880) = 0.59, p = .67; gp 2 = .005) or popular associates (Wilks’ K = 0.98, F(4,880) = 0.31, p = .87; gp 2 = .002), and no significant three-way interaction of education status by 0 Pop. Assoc. P ee r "B ul ly " N om in
  • 71. at io ns 100 200 300 400 500 600 700 Not Pop. Assoc. General Education Students Students With Mild Disabilities Academically Gifted Students AB C A AB B AB Fig. 2 Peer nominations of ‘‘bully’’ by education status and
  • 72. popular affiliations Not Agg. & Pop Assoc. Not Agg. & Not Pop Assoc. General Education Students Students With Mild Disabilities Academically Gifted Students 0 100 200 300 400 500 600 700 Agg. & Pop. Assoc. Agg. & Not Pop. Assoc. P ee r "B ul
  • 73. ly " N om in at io ns ABCBCDAABABAB CD EE F DE Fig. 3 Peer-nominations of ‘‘bully’’ by education status, aggressive and popular affiliations J Child Fam Stud (2009) 18:136–150 145 123 aggressive associates by popular associates (Wilks’
  • 74. K = 1.00, F(2,440) = 0.89, p = .41; gp 2 = .004). We probed these effects with follow-up ANOVAs for each dependent variable. For teacher ratings of ‘‘bullied’’ there was a main effect of education status (F(2,441) = 7.62, p .001; gp 2 = .033), aggressive associates (F(1,441) = 4.09, p .05; gp 2 = .019), and popular associates (F(1,441) = 5.55, p .01; gp 2 = .022). There were no interactive effects for education status by either aggressive associates (F(2,441) = 1.04, p = .35; gp 2 = .005) or popular associates (F(2,441) = 0.45, p = .64; gp 2 = .002), and no significant three-way interaction of education status by aggressive associates by popular associates (F(1,441) = 1.78, p = .18; gp 2 = .004). Post-hoc tests indicated that the main effect of education status was due to significant dif-
  • 75. ferences among all three groups of students: those with mild disabilities (M = 3.10, SE = 0.29) had the highest ratings for being bullied, followed by those in general education (M = 2.44 SE = 0.08), while academically gif- ted participants were the lowest (M = 1.34, SE = 0.07). The main effect of aggressive associates was due to isolates (M = 3.37, SE = 0.32) having higher ratings of being bullied than participants with (M = 2.47, SE = 0.14) or without (M = 2.15, SE = 0.08) aggressive associates. Finally, the main effect of popular associates was due to isolates (M = 3.37, SE = 0.32) having higher ratings of being bullied than those without popular associates (M = 2.35, SE = 0.08) and those with popular associates (M = 1.77, SE = 0.12). Students without popular associ- ates had significantly higher ratings of being bullied than those with popular associates. In summary, teachers rated students with mild disabili- ties as highest in being bullied, and gifted students lowest.
  • 76. Further, while isolates had high ratings for being bullied, those with popular associates had very low ratings for being bullied. For peer nominations of ‘‘picked on’’ there were sig- nificant main effects of aggressive associates (F(1,441) = 5.33, p .01; gp 2 = .023), and popular associates (F(1,441) = 5.47, p .01; gp 2 = .022). The main effects of education status (F(2,441) = 2.17, p = .12; gp 2 = .010) was non-significant. The interactions of education status by aggressive associates (F(2,441 = 0.22, p = .80; gp 2 = .001), education status by popular associates (F(2,441 = 0.17, p = .84; gp 2 = .001), and education status by aggressive associates by popular associates (F(1,441 = 0.04, p = .84; gp 2 = .000) were also all non-significant. Post-hoc tests indicated that the main effect of aggressive associates was
  • 77. due to isolates (M = 181.04, SE = 41.59) having higher numbers of nominations for being picked on than those with (M = 75.92, SE = 12.91) or without (M = 59.68, SE = 6.87) aggressive associates. Similarly, the main effect of popular associates was due to isolates (M = 181.04, SE = 41.59) having higher numbers of nominations for being picked on than those with (M = 47.41, SE = 9.67) or without (M = 67.99, SE = 7.34) popular associates. In short, isolates had high numbers of peer nominations for being bullied. Discussion The social dynamics of bullying and victimization involves the interplay between the interpersonal characteristics of individual students and the characteristics of the peer groups in which they are embedded (Rodkin and Hodges 2003; Salmivalli et al. 1997). Consistent with prior research with late elementary students (e.g., Adler and Adler 1995; Hodges and Perry 1999; Pellegrini et al.
  • 78. 1999), our results indicate that bullying was positively associated with peer nominated aggression and—more modestly—social prominence, and negatively related to peer social preference and peer nominations of positive behavior. Victimization was positively related to peer nominations for internalizing and modestly related to aggressive behavior (positively) and social preference (negatively). Further, while the effect sizes were small, students who associated with aggressive peers and students who associated with popular peers were more likely to be identified as bullies. Students who were isolated were more likely to be bullied than students were in non-popular groups, who, in turn, were more likely to be bullied than peers who were in popular groups. Our findings help extend current viewpoints by showing that academically gifted students, general education stu- dents, and students with mild disabilities are differentially involved in bullying. Further, such involvement is moder-
  • 79. ated in part by their peer group membership. In our sample of 5th graders, students with mild disabilities were more likely to be viewed by peers as being bullies than were academically gifted and general education students. Teachers also perceived students with mild disabilities as being more likely to be bullies and victims of bullying than were general education students. General education stu- dents were, in turn, more likely to be rated by teachers as bullies and as being bullied than were academically gifted students. In addition, students with mild disabilities who associated with aggressive and popular peers were more likely to be bullies than all other students. In contrast, general education students who were not in popular or aggressive groups were least likely to be nominated by peers as bullies. The importance of our findings comes into focus when considered in light of ethnographic research on social dynamics and survey research on social networks and
  • 80. bullying participant roles. Ethnographic studies (e.g., Adler 146 J Child Fam Stud (2009) 18:136–150 123 and Adler 1995; Evans and Eder 1993) describe early adolescent social dynamics as being a process where stu- dents are constantly vying for social positions and for social identities that protect against being victimized and excluded by others. In such a context, students who are social isolates are particularly vulnerable to being targeted by bullies. Students who are themselves socially vulnerable (e.g., students with mild disabilities) are likely to seek out the approval of popular peers and to bully others as a way to promote their own social positions. Further, by targeting peers who are vulnerable, bullies appear to use aggressive strategies as a way to consolidate the support of others in their peer group (Salmivalli et al. 1997). In contrast, stu- dents in middle-level peer groups (e.g., groups that are not
  • 81. prominent or peripheral in the social structure) are more focused on school activities than their social prominence and are less likely to be involved in bullying (Adler and Adler 1996). When examined from this backdrop, our results have important implications for understanding bullying and victimization in late elementary classrooms. As students jockey for position in their classroom social network, they may seek to reduce their own social vulnerability by bul- lying others and by associating with popular and aggressive peers. Because they are socially vulnerable (Frederickson and Furnham 2004; Gresham and MacMillan 1997; Sale and Carey 1995), students with mild disabilities may be susceptible to being bullied and may compensate by bul- lying others and by affiliating with peers who support this behavior. On the other hand, academically gifted students tend to be fairly well accepted in late elementary school (Austin and Draper 1981; Schneider et al. 1989) and may
  • 82. focus more on academics than social prominence. There- fore, while they tend to be socially prominent and associate with peers who teachers view as being popular, academi- cally gifted students do not appear to be vulnerable to being bullied and seem to be relatively less likely to be involved in bullying. Nonetheless, our findings suggest that general education students who are not in popular or aggressive groups—i.e., those who are not supported in aggression by aggressive peers and are not fighting to maintain a prom- inent position in the social hierarchy—may be the least likely to be involved in bullying. Our study has important implications for the develop- ment of bullying prevention programs and for the inclusion of students with exceptionalities in general education classrooms. First, there is a need for teachers to be aware that membership in both aggressive peer groups and pop- ular peer groups may place students at risk for bullying others. Teachers may then be able to utilize seating charts
  • 83. and group activities to encourage such students to form associations with prosocial peers. Second, there is a need for anti-bullying efforts that focus specifically on the social risks of students with mild disabilities. Such students are susceptible to both bullying and being victimized and may also develop peer affiliations that further support their risk for involvement in bullying. Current efforts in these areas may need to be modified to account for individual needs. Third, there is a need to develop inclusion strategies that are responsive to the differential social needs and risks of students with exceptionalities and that focus on creating classroom social contexts that reduce bullying by enhanc- ing the social opportunities and social positions of all students. While our findings are compelling, our study has several limitations that must be considered. First, it should be noted that despite their significance, the effect sizes for all the relationships discussed above are small—ranging from
  • 84. .02 to .07. These effect sizes may be due in part to the complexity of the models being fitted—inclusion of three main effects in addition to two-two-way and one-three-way interactions greatly reduces the variability that any one component of the model can explain. Beyond that, how- ever, this speaks to the complexity of the processes leading to bullying and victimization: while exceptionalities and peer relations are important, many other factors are at work. This leads directly to the second major limitation. There are a number of variables that could be confounds that we could not include in the study. Family structure and socio-economic status, for example, tend to be related to the need for special education services and academic giftedness, and also have demonstrated relationships with social outcomes. These variables could be driving many of the relationships we found and may be contributing to the variance unexplained by the our model. A third key limitation was that the sample only included
  • 85. fifth grade students. While this was consistent with our goal of focusing on classroom social dynamics during late ele- mentary school and the emerging transition to early adolescence, it is not appropriate to generalize our findings to early elementary school or to middle school. Additional research is needed that addresses our aims in samples of both younger and older students. Fourth, the sample of students with disabilities was small and students with dif- ferent types of mild disabilities were aggregated together. Although our sample was small in statistical terms, it is fairly large relative to many studies that focus on the social relations of students with mild disabilities. Because of issues of critical mass (i.e., only a few students with mild disabilities per classroom), it was necessary to have very large samples to study this population. This made it nec- essary to collapse students with different high incidence disabilities into a single category. While this is not an appropriate practice when the aim is to generate knowledge
  • 86. about a particular disability category, it does demonstrate the need to include a focus on students with disabilities in J Child Fam Stud (2009) 18:136–150 147 123 bullying and victimization research (which was one of our key aims). There is clearly a significant need for large scale studies that have sufficiently large samples of students with different disabilities. Finally, our investigation did not examine gender dif- ferences. It is likely that boys and girls are involved in bullying and victimization in different ways. Girls are more likely to be involved with relational forms of bullying and aggression while boys tend toward physical forms of harassment (Crick et al. 1996). These forms of aggression have differential relationships with both social skills (Farmer 2000) and social functioning with peers (Cillessen and Mayeux 2004).This is especially important in light of
  • 87. the higher incidence of mild disabilities among boys and the higher rate of giftedness among girls. The relationships between exceptionalities, bullying, and popularity may be in many ways driven by these gender differences. Unfor- tunately, it was not possible to examine gender differences among students with exceptionalities because of our small sample sizes. As a result, our measures also collapsed across forms of aggression (i.e., included both physical and relational aggression). While the resulting measures of internal consistency were adequate, there may be differ- ential relationships between exceptionalities, social functioning, and different forms of aggression. Research with larger samples is needed to explore gender differences across different special education categories as well as within general education populations. Such work with larger samples may be able to disentangle the effects of exceptionalities on bullying and social functioning behav- ior from those associated with gender and forms of
  • 88. aggression. In conclusion, our study indicates that students with mild disabilities, academically gifted students, and general education students are differentially at-risk for involvement in bullying and victimization. Further, membership in both aggressive and popular peer groups supports bullying, while membership in popular groups appears to protect against victimization. While our work has several limita- tions, it does provide clear evidence to suggest a need for research that focuses more specifically on exceptional populations in bullying research. In addition, the finding that membership in popular peer groups may support bul- lying but protect against victimization is highly provocative and warrants more intensive examination. Finally, our results suggest there is a need to carefully consider and explore the social vulnerability and bullying involvement of students with exceptionalities in the development of anti-bullying interventions. While there is
  • 89. currently a strong focus on inclusion and on the use of peer support strategies (i.e., cooperative learning, peer tutoring) to address the instructional needs of students with mild disabilities in general education classrooms, our findings suggest that some pairings of students may be advanta- geous while others may be highly detrimental. There is a critical need to scientifically generate new information in this area that can ultimately be used to help guide peer support practices and anti-bullying efforts. Acknowledgments This research was supported by grants H325C020106 and H324C040230 from the Office of Special Edu- cation Programs of the Department of Education to Thomas W. Farmer (Principal Investigator). The views expressed in this article are ours and do not represent the granting agency. References Adler, P. A., & Adler, P. (1995). Dynamics of inclusion and exclusion in preadolescent cliques. Social Psychology Quarterly, 58, 145– 162.
  • 90. Adler, P. A., & Adler, P. (1996). Preadolescent clique stratification and the hierarchy of identity. Sociological Inquiry, 66, 111– 142. Al-Yagon, M., & Mikulincer, M. (2004). Patterns of close relation- ships and socioemotional and academic adjustment among school-age children with learning disabilities. Learning Disabil- ities Research and Practice, 19, 12–19. Asher, S. R., & Coie, J. (1990). Peer rejection in childhood. New York: Cambridge University Press. Atlas, R. S., & Pepler, D. J. (1998). Observations of bullying in the classroom. Journal of Educational Research, 92, 86–99. Austin, A. B., & Draper, D. C. (1981). Peer relationships of the academically gifted: A review. Gifted Child Quarterly, 25, 129–133. Bagwell, C. L., Coie, J. D., Terry, R. A., & Lochman, J. E. (2000). Peer clique participation and social status in preadolescence. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 46, 280–305. Cairns, R. B., & Cairns, B. D. (1994). Lifelines and risks: Pathways of
  • 91. youth in our time. Cambridge, England: Cambridge University Press. Cairns, R. B., Cairns, B. D., Neckerman, H. J., Gest, S. J., & Gariépy, J.-L. (1988). Social networks and aggressive behavior: Peer support or peer rejection? Developmental Psychology, 24, 815–823. Cairns, R. B., Leung, M.-C., Buchanan, L., & Cairns, B. D. (1995a). Friendships and social networks in childhood and adolescence: Fluidity, reliability, and interrelations. Child Development, 66, 1330–1345. Cairns, R. B., Leung, M.-C., Gest, S. D., & Cairns, B. D. (1995b). A brief method for assessing social development: Structure, reliability, stability, and developmental validity of the Interper- sonal Competence Scale. Behavioral Research and Therapy, 33, 725–736. Cairns, R. B., Perrin, J. E., & Cairns, B. D. (1985). Social structure ad social cognition in early adolescence: Affiliative patterns. Journal of Early Adolescence, 5, 339–355. Cillessen, A. N. H., & Mayeux, L. (2004). From Censure to
  • 92. reinforcement: Developmental changes in the association between aggression and social status. Aggression & Violent Behavior, 75, 147–163. Coie, J. D., Dodge, K. A., & Coppotelli, H. (1982). Dimensions and types of social status: A cross-age perspective. Developmental Psychology, 18, 557–570. Crick, N. R., Bigbee, M. A., & Howes, C. (1996). Gender differences in children’s normative beliefs about aggression: How do I hurt thee? Let me count the ways. Child Development, 67, 1003– 1014. 148 J Child Fam Stud (2009) 18:136–150 123 Estell, D. B., Cairns, R. B., Farmer, T. W., & Cairns, B. D. (2002). Aggression in inner-city early elementary classrooms: Individual and peer group configurations. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 48, 52–76. Estell, D. B., Farmer, T. W., & Cairns, B. D. (2007). Bullies
  • 93. and victims in rural African American youth: Individual character- istics and social network placement. Aggressive Behavior, 33, 145–159. Estell, D. B., Farmer, T. W., Pearl, R., Van Acker, R., & Rodkin, P. C. (2003). Heterogeneity in the relationship between popularity and aggression: Individual, group, and classroom influences. In W. Damon (Series Ed.), S. C. Peck, & R. W. Roeser (Vol. Eds.), New directions for child and adolescent development: Vol. 101. Person-centered approaches to studying human development in context (pp. 75–85). San Francisco: Jossey-Bass. Estell, D. B., Jones, M. H., Pearl, R., Van Acker, R., Farmer, T. W., & Rodkin, P. C. (2008). Peer groups, popularity, and social preference: Trajectories of social functioning among students with and without learning disabilities. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 41, 5–14. Evans, C., & Eder, D. (1993). ‘No exit’: Processes of social isolation in the middle school. Journal of Contemporary Ethnography, 22, 139–170.
  • 94. Farmer, T. W. (2000). The social dynamics of aggressive and disruptive behavior in school: Implications for behavioral consultation. Journal of Education and Behavioral Consultation, 21, 194–208. Farmer, T. W., & Farmer, E. M. Z. (1996). Social relationships of students with exceptionalities in mainstream classrooms: Social networks and homophily. Exceptional Children, 62, 431–450. Farmer, T. W., & Hollowell, J. H. (1994). Social networks in mainstream classrooms: Social affiliations and behavioral char- acteristics of students with EBD. Journal of Emotional and Behavioral Disorders, 2, 143–155. Farmer, T. W., Leung, M.-C., Pearl, R., Rodkin, P. C., Cadwallader, T. W., & Van Acker, R. (2002). Deviant or diverse groups? The peer affiliations of aggressive elementary students. Journal of Educational Psychology, 94, 611–620. Farmer, T. W., & Rodkin, P. C. (1996). Antisocial and prosocial correlates of classroom social positions: The social network centrality perspective. Social Development, 5, 174–188. Farmer, T. W., Rodkin, P. C., Pearl, R., & Van Acker, R. M. (1999).
  • 95. Teacher-assessed behavioral configurations, peer assessments, and self-concepts of elementary students with mild disabilities. Journal of Special Education, 33, 66–80. Fox, C. L., & Boulton, M. J. (2006). Friendship as a moderator of the relationship between social skills problems and peer victimiza- tion. Aggressive Behavior, 32, 110–121. Frederickson, N. L., & Furnham, A. F. (2004). Peer-assessed behavioural characteristics and sociometric rejection: Differ- ences between pupils who have moderate learning difficulties and their mainstream peers. British Journal of Educational Psychology, 74, 391–410. Glew, G. M., Fan, M. Y., Katon, W., Rivara, F. P., & Kernic, M. A. (2005). Bullying, psychosocial adjustment, and academic per- formance in elementary school. Archives of Pediatrics & Adolescent Medicine, 159, 1026–1031. Gresham, F. M., & MacMillan, D. L. (1997). Social competence and affective characteristics of students with mild disabilities. Review of Educational Research, 67, 377–415. Guralnick, M. J., & Groom, J. M. (1987). The peer relations of
  • 96. mildly delayed and nonhandicapped preschool children in mainstrea- med playgroups. Child Development, 58, 1556–1572. Guralnick, M. J., Hammond, M. A., Connor, R. T., & Neville, B. (2006). Stability, change, and correlates of the peer relationships of young children with mild developmental delays. Child Development, 77, 312–324. Hanish, L. D., & Guerra, N. G. (2002). A longitudinal analysis of patterns of adjustment following peer victimization. Develop- ment and Psychopathology, 14, 69–89. Hawkins, D. L., & Pepler, D. J. (2001). Naturalistic observations of peer interventions in bullying. Social Development, 10, 512–527. Hodges, E. V. E., Boivin, M., Vitaro, F., & Bukowski, W. M. (1999). The power of friendship: Protection against an escalating cycle of peer victimization. Developmental Psychology, 35, 94–101. Hodges, E. V. E., & Perry, D. G. (1999). Personal and interpersonal antecedents and consequences of victimization by peers. Journal
  • 97. of Personality and Social Psychology, 76, 677–685. Janke, R. W., & Lee, K. (1991). Social skills ratings of exceptional students. Journal of Psychoeducational Assessment, 9, 54–66. Kaukiainen, A., Salmivalli, C., Lagerspetz, K., Tamminen, M., Vauras, M., Maki, H., et al. (2002). Learning difficulties, social intelligence, and self-concept: Connections to bully-victim problems. Scandinavian Journal of Psychology, 43, 269–278. Kavale, K. A., & Forness, S. R. (1996). Social skill deficits and learning disabilities: A meta-analysis. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 29, 226–237. Leung, M.-C. (1996). Social networks and self enhancement in Chinese children: A comparison of self reports and peer reports of group membership. Social Development, 2, 146–157. Luftig, R. L., & Nichols, M. L. (1990). Assessing the social status of gifted students by their age peers. Gifted Child Quarterly, 34, 111–115. Masten, A. S., Morison, P., & Pellegrini, D. S. (1985). A revised class play method of peer assessment. Developmental Psychology, 21, 523–533. Moon, S. M. (Ed.). (2004). Social/emotional issues,
  • 98. underachieve- ment, and counseling of gifted and talented students. Thousand Oaks, CA: Corwin Press. Nazuboka, D., & Smith, P. K. (1993). Sociometric status and social behavior of children with and without learning difficulties. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry and Allied Disci- plines, 34, 1435–1448. O’Connell, P., Pepler, D., & Craig, W. (1999). Peer involvement in bullying: Insights and challenges for intervention. Journal of Adolescence, 22, 437–452. Pearl, R., Farmer, T. W., Van Acker, R., Rodkin, P., Bost, K. K., Coe, M., et al. (1998). The social integration of students with mild disabilities in general education classrooms: Peer group mem- bership and peer-assessed social behavior. The Elementary School Journal, 99, 167–185. Pellegrini, A. D. (1998). Bullies and victims in school: A review of and call for research. Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology, 19, 165–176. Pellegrini, A. D., & Bartini, M. (2000). A longitudinal study of
  • 99. bullying, victimization, and peer affiliation during the transition from primary school to middle school. American Educational Research Journal, 37, 699–725. Pellegrini, A. D., Bartini, M., & Brooks, F. (1999). School bullies, victims, and aggressive victims: Factors relating to group affiliation and victimization in early adolescence. Journal of Educational Psychology, 91, 216–224. Peterson, J. S., & Ray, K. E. (2006). Bullying and the gifted: Victims, perpetrators, prevalences and effects. Gifted Child Quarterly, 50, 148–168. Preuss, L. J., & Dubow, E. F. (2004). A comparison between intellectually gifted and typical children in their coping responses to a school and a peer stressor. Roeper Review, 26, 105–111. J Child Fam Stud (2009) 18:136–150 149 123 Rodkin, P. C., Farmer, T. W., Van Acker, R., Pearl, R., Thompson, J.
  • 100. H., & Fedora, P. (2006). Who do students with mild disabilities nominate as cool in inclusive general education classrooms? Journal of School Psychology, 44, 67–84. Rodkin, P. C., Farmer, T. W., Pearl, R., & Van Acker, R. M. (2000). Heterogeneity of popular boys: Antisocial and prosocial config- urations. Developmental Psychology, 36, 14–24. Rodkin, P. C., & Hodges, E. V. E. (2003). Bullies and victims in the peer ecology: Four questions for psychologists and school professionals. School Psychology Review, 32, 384–400. Rose, A. J., Swenson, L. P., & Waller, E. M. (2004). Overt and relational aggression and perceived popularity: Developmental differences in concurrent and prospective relations. Develop- mental Psychology, 40, 378–387. Sale, P., & Carey, D. M. (1995). The sociometric status of students with disabilities in a full-inclusion school. Exceptional Children, 62, 6–19. Salmivalli, C., Huttunen, A., & Lagerspetz, K. M. J. (1997). Peer networks and bullying in schools. Scandinavian Journal of Psychology, 38, 305–312.
  • 101. Schneider, B., & Daniels, T. (1992). Peer acceptance and social play of gifted kindergarten children. Exceptionality, 3, 17–29. Schneider, B. H., Clegg, M. R., Byrne, B. M., Ledingham, J. E., & Crombie, G. (1989). Social relations of gifted children as a function of age and school program. Journal of Educational Psychology, 81, 48–56. Schwartz, D. (2000). Subtypes of victims and aggressors in children’s peer groups. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 28, 181– 192. Schwartz, D., Gorman, A. H., Nakamoto, J., & Toblin, R. L. (2005). Victimization in the peer group and children’s academic functioning. Journal of Educational Psychology, 97, 425–435. Smith, P. K., Talamelli, L., Cowie, H., Naylor, P., & Chauhan, P. (2004). Profiles of non-victims, escaped victims, continuing victims and new victims of school bullying. British Journal of Educational Psychology, 74, 565–581. Svetaz, M. V., Ireland, M., & Blum, R. (2000). Adolescents with
  • 102. learning disabilities: Risk and protective factors associated with emotional well-being: Findings from the National Longitudinal Study of Adolescent Health. Journal of Adolescent Health, 27, 340–348. Troop-Gordon, W., & Ladd, G. W. (2005). Trajectories of peer victimization and perceptions of the self and schoolmates: Precursors to internalizing and externalizing problems. Child Development, 76, 1072–1091. Vaughn, S., Hogan, A., Kouzekanani, K., & Shapiro, S. (1990). Peer acceptance, self-perceptions, and social skills of learning disabled students prior to identification. Journal of Educational Psychology, 82, 101–106. Walker, H. M., Kavanagh, K., Stiller, B., Golly, A., Severson, H. H., & Feil, E. G. (1998). First step to success: An early intervention approach for preventing school antisocial behavior. Journal of Emotional and Behavioral Disorders, 6, 66–80. Xie, H., Cairns, R. B., & Cairns, B. D. (1999). Social networks and configurations in inner-city schools: Aggression, popularity, and
  • 103. implications for students with EBD. Journal of Emotional and Behavioral Disorders, 7, 147–155. 150 J Child Fam Stud (2009) 18:136–150 123 1 Exceptionalties “Exceptionalities” is a general, nonclinical term to describe conditions and behaviors that lie outside the norm. These may include physical, emotional, or psychological differences. For the purposes of this DSM-5 update, the focus will be on the exceptionalities that align with the DSM-5 classification of neurodevelopmental disorders. One major shift in the DSM-5 was to abandon the classification of disorders usually first diagnosed in infancy, childhood, or adolescence in favor of more clinically appropriate classifications. That is, the diagnoses formerly found in this group were moved to classifications more specifically related to symptomology. One of the new classifications—neurodevelopmental disorders—includes diagnoses generally made early in childhood. A summary of changes in
  • 104. diagnostic criteria of these disorders can be found below. Neurodevelopmental Disorders Intellectual Disability (Intellectual Developmental Disorder) This group of diagnoses most closely resembles the formerly described mental retardation diagnoses. The phrase “mental retardation” is no longer used in clinical or educational settings. In addition to a change in the diagnostic label, there has been a change to criteria as well. Mental retardation diagnoses were previously identified as five separate diagnoses reflecting severity of impairment based on IQ score. The new intellectual disability diagnoses include only two diagnoses, with specifiers reflecting severity of impairment. It is important to note that severity is determined by adaptive functioning and cognitive capacity (IQ), rather than solely by the latter, as was the case in the DSM-IV. Communication Disorders This group of disorders includes deficits in language, speech, and communication. Changes from the DSM-IV to the DSM-5 for this group include new diagnostic labels of speech sound disorder (formerly phonological disorder), childhood-onset fluency disorder (formerly stuttering), and language disorder (a combination of expressive and mixed receptive-expressive language disorders). Also included is an entirely new diagnosis of social (pragmatic) communication disorder, which is characterized by persistent deficits in verbal and nonverbal communication. It is important to note that because social communication deficits
  • 105. are a diagnostic criteria of autism spectrum disorders, these diagnoses cannot be comorbid. Autism Spectrum Disorder The diagnostic grouping formerly known as pervasive developmental disorders has been streamlined and renamed autism spectrum disorder. This is a single diagnostic label that includes the formerly identified autistic disorder, Asperger’s disorder, childhood disintegrative disorder, and pervasive developmental disorder not otherwise specified. The autism spectrum disorder is characterized by two key criterion: a) deficits in social communication and social interaction and b) restricted, repetitive behaviors. The specifiers used with this diagnosis include intellectual impairment, language impairment, and associations with other conditions, factors, or comorbid diagnoses. It is important to note that the DSM-IV diagnosis of Rett disorder has been entirely removed, as it is considered the manifestation of the biological condition Rett syndrome. 2 Characteristics of Rett syndrome often appear to be very similar to autism spectrum disorder at onset; however, symptoms often improve substantially after 5–6 years of age, making differential diagnosis more clear. Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder (ADHD)
  • 106. Though the basic diagnostic criteria for ADHD is very similar in the DSM-5, there are a number of key differences from the DSM-IV, including stage-related examples to aid in diagnosis in childhood, adolescence, and adulthood. One of the key changes has been to raise the identification of symptomology from before age 7 to before age 12 and to use a single diagnosis with specifiers rather than several related diagnoses in a group. Specifiers replace prior subtypes, identifying the predominant presenting symptomology. Specifiers are also now used to reflect severity of impairment of functioning. Specific Learning Disorder This is a new diagnosis in the DSM-5 and includes the entire group of disorders from the DSM- IV group learning disorders. These disorders included reading disorder, mathematics disorder, disorder of written expression, and learning disorder not otherwise specified. Specifiers are used to identify the areas of impairment (reading, written expression, or mathematics). Motor Disorders This diagnostic group has replaced the motor skills disorder group from the DSM-IV, which previously included only developmental coordination disorder. In recognition of the neurological foundations of several disorders relating to control of movement and coordination, this new DSM-5 diagnostic group includes developmental coordination disorder, stereotypic movement disorder, and tic disorders. More clear language has also been included in stereotypic movement disorder criteria to better facilitate distinction from similar
  • 107. behaviors associated with obsessive- compulsive disorder. Reference: • American Psychiatric Association. (2013). Highlights of changes from DSM-IV-TR to DSM-5. Retrieved from: http://www.dsm5.org/Documents/changes%20from%20dsm-iv- tr%20to%20dsm-5.pdf © 2013 Laureate Education, Inc. Investigation of raising burden of children with autism, physical disability and mental disability in China Nina Xiong *, Li Yang, Yang Yu, Jiaxun Hou, Jia Li, Yuanyuan Li, Hairong Liu, Ying Zhang, Zhengang Jiao Beijing Disabled Persons’ Rehabilitation Service and Guidance Center, Beijing 100070, China 1. The investigation of raising burden of children with autism, physical disability and mental disability in China The economic burden of raising children with disabilities is a problem in many countries. Study in South India (Kandamuthan & Kandamuthan, 2004) indicated that, the mean expenditure of the families with a severely disabled child was $254 per year, which is significantly higher than the
  • 108. corresponding expenditure of $181 per year of families with normal child. Of the disabled children, 80% were not getting any social security payments and 90% had no special concessions for medical and other educational purposes. Of the mothers of the disabled children 21% were unemployed as against 12% in the case of normal children. Parents of disabled children demanded an average additional amount of $203 per year as social security payments from the Government to meet the essential necessities of their disabled children. There are extra costs for disabled children for travel, domestic help, medical care, and health care expenditures (hospital care, physician services, dentistry, drugs and others) for disabled individuals; financial impact of disablement in a child on the family is significant; medical expenditure was a significant variable that differentiated the disabled and normal child. Findings (Honeycutt et al., Research in Developmental Disabilities 32 (2011) 306–311 A R T I C L E I N F O Article history: Received 21 September 2010 Received in revised form 29 September 2010 Accepted 7 October 2010 Keywords: Autism Physical disability
  • 109. Mental disability Raising burden A B S T R A C T The family economic burden of raising autistic children, physical disabled children and mental disabled children were evaluated in China. 227 parents of children with autism, children with physical disability, children with mental disability and normal children were interviewed for children’s costs, family income and economic assistance, etc. The medical cost and caring cost of disabled children were significantly more than those of normal children, and the education cost, clothes cost and amusement cost of disabled children were significantly less than those of normal children. Family income was only predicted by parents’ education level. Families of disabled children received more economic assistance than families of normal children except families of autistic children. More children the family had, less economic assistance the family acquired. Compared with normal children,
  • 110. the raising burden of children with disabilities were as follows: children with autism (19582.4 RMB per year), children with physical disability (16410.1 RMB per year), children with mental disability (6391.0 RMB per year). Families of autistic children, physical disabled children and mental disabled children have heavier raising burden than families of normal children, they need more help from many aspects. � 2010 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved. * Corresponding author. Tel.: +86 10 63781942 8071. E-mail addresses: [email protected] (N. Xiong), [email protected] (L. Yang), [email protected] (Y. Yu), [email protected] (J. Hou), [email protected] (J. Li), [email protected] (Y. Li), [email protected] (H. Liu), [email protected] (Y. Zhang), [email protected] (Z. Jiao). Contents lists available at ScienceDirect Research in Developmental Disabilities 0891-4222/$ – see front matter � 2010 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved. doi:10.1016/j.ridd.2010.10.003 http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.ridd.2010.10.003