SlideShare a Scribd company logo
1 of 284
The Journal of Modern Craft Volume 1—Issue 1—March 2008,
pp. 13–34
Fiber Art and the
Hierarchy of Art
and Craft, 1960–80
Elissa Auther
Elissa Auther is Assistant Professor of Contemporary Art
at the University of Colorado at Colorado Springs. Her
essay for the Journal of Modern Craft is derived from her
book manuscript about the innovative use of fi ber across
the art world in the 1960s and 1970s. Recent publications
about the position of craft under modernism include “The
Decorative, Abstraction and the Hierarchy of Art and Craft
in the Art Criticism of Clement Greenberg,” Oxford Art Journal
27(3) (December 2004) and “Andy Warhol, Wallpaper and
Contemporary Installation Art,” for the forthcoming edited
collection Extra/Ordinary: Craft Culture and Contemporary Art.
Abstract
“Fiber Art and the Hierarchy of Art and Craft, 1960–
1980” both explores the artistic, historical, institutional
and extra-aesthetic forces affecting the formation of the
fi ber movement and evaluates the curatorial strategies of
Mildred Constantine and Jack Lenor Larsen to negotiate
the hierarchy of art and craft in order to elevate fi ber
as a medium of “high art.” The analysis considers the
emergence of the category of fi ber art in the 1960s and
1970s, the cultural contexts in which fi ber or textiles
were utilized in the period, strategies of transcending
the hierarchy of art and craft, and other relations
of dominance and subordination that defi ned fi ber’s
marginality in the hierarchy of the arts and shaped the
responses of artists, critics and curators to aesthetic
boundaries.
Keywords: fi ber, craft, feminism, decorative, hierarchy,
modernism, Mildred Constantine, weaving.
The Journal of
Modern Craft
Volume I—Issue I
March 2008
pp. 13–34
Reprints available directly
from the publishers
Photocopying permitted by
licence only
© Berg 2008
14 Fiber Art and the Hierarchy of Art and Craft, 1960–1980
Elissa Auther
The Journal of Modern Craft Volume 1—Issue 1—March 2008,
pp. 13–34
In 1972, Mildred Constantine—a former
curator of architecture and design at the
Museum of Modern Art (MoMA), New
York—reproduced Adams’s Construction
in Beyond Craft, the fi rst in-depth study of
the emerging fi ber art movement.1 This
important text, co-authored with textile
designer Jack Lenor Larsen, chronicled the
movement’s evolution, defi ned its aesthetic
priorities and defended work made of fi ber
as “fi ne art.” In 1963, Adams’s unorthodox
woven works had been included in New
York’s Museum of Contemporary Crafts’
exhibition Woven Forms (Figure 3), a show
that Constantine and Larsen’s study singled
out as groundbreaking.2
Signifi cantly, critic Lucy Lippard also
exhibited Adams’s Construction in her
eclectic, “post-minimalist” show Eccentric
Abstraction, at the Fischbach Gallery in New
York in 1966 (Figure 4). Unlike Beyond
Craft, Eccentric Abstraction situated Adams’s
Construction among avant-garde work by
such emerging artists as Eva Hesse, Bruce
Nauman and Keith Sonnier. The show’s
works utilized a variety of non-traditional,
fl exible media, including wire mesh, vinyl,
cloth and rope.
Saret exhibited Untitled in 1970 in New
York’s Sidney Janis Gallery’s January group
show String and Rope (Figure 5), alongside
works by Christo, Fred Sandback, Bruce
Nauman and Robert Morris. Arts Magazine,
one of the era’s major fi ne-art periodicals,
reproduced Saret’s piece and a critic writing
for the Christian Science Monitor noted the
use of fi ber as an autonomous abstract
element. The “artists included,” he wrote,
“use string or rope (or thread), not as line,
but as falling, tangling, stretching, or coiling
matter” (Andreae 1970: 58).3
Fig 1 Alan Saret, Untitled, 1968, rope and wire,
variable dimensions.
Alan Saret’s Untitled (1968) (Figure 1), a
work of rope and wire and Alice Adams’s
Construction (1966) (Figure 2), of rope
and steel cable, share signifi cant formal
similarities. Both works are fl oor based,
of similar size and shape, and both utilize
materials associated with “craft,” hand labor
or industry. Indeed, one could conclude
that the same artist made both works. But
this is not the case, and the two artists
were associated with very different artistic
circles in the 1960s: Saret was an anti-form
sculptor, whereas Adams was associated
with what came to be known as the fi ber
art movement. Moreover, the works were
exhibited and received very differently.
Comparing the varied reception of these
two similar objects reveals not only fi ber’s
arrival as a new medium of “high art” but
also how this elevation of fi ber issued from
multiple sites or positions, each with a distinct
location within the complex network of
power relations governed by the application
of the term “craft” in the United States in the
1960s and 1970s.
Elissa Auther Fiber Art and the Hierarchy of Art and Craft,
1960–1980 15
The Journal of Modern Craft Volume 1—Issue 1—March 2008,
pp. 13–34
Studies like Beyond Craft (1972),
exhibitions such as Woven Forms (1963),
Eccentric Abstraction (1966) and String and
Rope (1969), and works in fi ber like Saret’s
and Adams’s are representative of a number
of projects in the 1960s and 1970s that
signaled fi ber’s potential as a fi ne-art medium
and illustrate different relations to the art
world’s center of power. In each case, fi ber,
that is, craft—typically dismissed or even
invisible as a force shaping the art world in
this period—was in actuality central to its
constitution. Beyond Craft and Woven Forms
attempted to elevate fi ber from the realm of
“craft” to that of “art” and were undertaken
by individuals and institutions dedicated to
legitimating new work in materials traditional
to craft. The goal of legitimating so-called
fi ber art as “Art” sets these projects apart
from exhibitions such as Eccentric Abstraction
and String and Rope, which attempted to
capture sculpture’s latest vanguard. Here craft
functioned as a conceptual limit, essential
to the elevation of art—in the words of
Fig 2 Alice Adams, Construction,
1966, rope and steel cable.
Robert Mates photographer.
Courtesy of the artist.
16 Fiber Art and the Hierarchy of Art and Craft, 1960–1980
Elissa Auther
The Journal of Modern Craft Volume 1—Issue 1—March 2008,
pp. 13–34
Fig 3 Installation view of
work by Lenore Tawney in
Woven Forms, Museum of
Contemporary Craft, New
York, 1963. Ferdinand Boesch
photographer. Courtesy
of the American Craft
Council Library/Museum of
Contemporary Craft Archives.
Fig 4 Installation view of Lucy Lippard’s Eccentric
Abstraction at the Fischbach Gallery, New York,
1966. Rudy Burckhardt photographer. Courtesy
of the Fischbach Gallery records, 1954–1978.
Archives of American Art, Smithsonian Institution.
Glenn Adamson, as “a border that can never
be reached, but is nonetheless intrinsic to
any sense of position” (Adamson 2007: 2).
Eccentric Abstraction and String and Rope
were both produced by members of the art
world whose authority in itself legitimated
the work in question, a privilege not fully
extended to Constantine and Larsen. Rather
than focusing on whether Saret’s Untitled was
a work of art, these exhibitions theorized the
artistic use of fi ber and fi ber-like materials
in relation to previous examples of non-
traditional media in sculpture and to other
Fig 5 String and Rope, Installation view, Sidney
Janis Gallery, New York, 1969. Photographer
Geoffrey Clements.
Elissa Auther Fiber Art and the Hierarchy of Art and Craft,
1960–1980 17
The Journal of Modern Craft Volume 1—Issue 1—March 2008,
pp. 13–34
conditions, such as craft, that oppose the
defi nition of art.
Despite these differences in the projects’
orientations and goals, they all probed fi ber’s
symbolic power, paradoxically generated
by its subordination in the history of art
through deep-rooted associations with
utility or craft, a phenomenon Saret’s and
Adams’s works also illuminate. In challenging
the subordination of fi ber as a medium of
craft or of primarily utilitarian value, Adams’s
and Saret’s works additionally illustrate the
role fi ber played in testing the art world’s
aesthetic boundaries in the 1960s and
1970s. Yet, with the exception of Adams’s
Construction, which circulated widely, the
status of “high art” remained elusive for the
objects that Constantine and Larsen featured
in Beyond Craft. In fact, fi ber art in this period
was typically viewed as neither art nor craft,
but as between the two categories, thwarting
the works’ potential to undermine the
hierarchy of media responsible for fi ber’s low
aesthetic status.
This article examines the efforts of
Mildred Constantine—the movement’s
principal architect and supporter—and her
collaborator Jack Lenor Larsen to eliminate
this ambiguity, securing fi ber’s identity as
a medium of high art. Their strategy was
defi ned by the goal of assimilating fi ber-based
work to the fi ne arts and did not address
the fact that the boundary separating art
from craft is constructed rather than natural.
Although this strategy led to an unfortunate
collusion with the very hierarchy of media
they sought to transform and in hindsight
might seem obviously fl awed, my analysis
suggests that the options available to them
as defenders of fi ber art were tremendously
constrained by the period’s artistic discourse
defi ning the work of art, particularly its
autonomy from social contexts and practices
outside the art world with which fi ber
was intimately connected. The reasons
surrounding the art world’s resistance to
fi ber art were complex and varied, involving
the cultural connotations of fi ber, popular
trends in fi ber crafts and gender bias deriving
from fi ber’s association with women and the
domestic realm; with such factors working
against their project, perhaps no curatorial
strategy Constantine and Larsen could have
adopted at the time would have fared better.
The Aesthetic Status of Fiber in the
1960s and 1970s
In 1961, with Lenore Tawney’s solo exhibition
at the Staten Island Museum, fi ber art as it
would be recognized in the following two
decades made its public debut in a fi ne-art
context. The show consisted of forty works
produced between 1955 and 1961 in a
technique now referred to as open-warp
weave: a structure in which large parts of
the warp are left unwoven. The reception
of this and subsequent work demonstrates
that from the start fi ber artists experienced
considerable resistance from the realms
of both craft and fi ne art for the way their
work violated conventions of both practices.
Tawney recalled that her early “open-warp
weaves . . . caused quite a controversy
[amongst weavers]. No one had done this
kind of weaving ... It’s against the rules and
those people who go by the rules were
against it” (Tawney 1978: n.p.).
However, posing a challenge to the
defi nition of weaving in the 1960s did not
automatically confer the status of “art” on
Tawney’s work; in the “high” art world too,
her work’s identity was unstable. For instance,
18 Fiber Art and the Hierarchy of Art and Craft, 1960–1980
Elissa Auther
The Journal of Modern Craft Volume 1—Issue 1—March 2008,
pp. 13–34
the small catalog for Tawney’s 1961 exhibition
reveals that on the one hand the show was
sponsored by the “Section of Handcrafts” but
elsewhere attempts to position Tawney as
a “fi ne artist.” In particular, the appreciation
written by Tawney’s close friend, painter
Agnes Martin, defends her artistic identity,
praising the work’s “originality” and its
relation to larger art-world trends toward
exploring new media.
Before Tawney’s Staten Island Museum
show was even mounted, the artist had
started using her open-warp method to
combine different weave structures in a
single work by dividing and redividing the
warp as the weave was in progress. The
resulting works, which she called “woven
forms” (see Figure 3), were monumental in
scale, departed from the rectilinear shape
of loom-woven fabrics and were not even
remotely utilitarian. Given their size, which
ranged from 11 to 27 feet (3.3 to 8.1 m)
in height, their abstraction (like some of
her earlier open-warp weaves, the “woven
forms” have no pictorial content) and
Tawney’s method of hanging them from the
ceiling, her work sharply departed from the
conventions of the “decorative wall hanging,”
the category in which weaves without
everyday utility were normally classifi ed in
the 1960s.
Yet this work too fell between accepted
divisions between art and craft. For instance,
Tawney’s 1962 solo exhibition at the Art
Institute of Chicago was installed in the
Textiles Department and Woven Forms,
the 1963 group show at the Museum of
Contemporary Crafts that included her
work, also institutionally situated her weaving
as craft. Even so, a reviewer for the New
York Times remarked that Tawney “is more
than just a weaver—she is also an artist.
Unfortunately, craft work has for many
years implied to the general public work
done by amateurs . . . with little merit . . . Miss
Tawney’s craft is in marked contrast to this
mistaken concept and her woven forms are
considered by experts to be works of fi ne
art” (O’Brien 1963).
This reviewer’s willingness to embrace
Tawney’s work as “fi ne art” against prevailing
sentiment that connected craft media to the
amateur or hobbyist, however, was more the
exception than the rule. Subsequent reviews
continued to question her work’s artistic
status. As late as 1990, on the occasion of
Tawney’s retrospective at the American Craft
Museum, Roberta Smith drew a negative
conclusion in her review for the New York
Times, leaving the impression that the
intervening years had done little to resolve
questions about the works’ status and
identity within the art world: “Mrs. Tawney’s
work exists in a limbo that is endemic to
much contemporary craft: it has departed
from craft and function without quite
arriving at art . . . Handsome and impressive
as her best efforts often are . . . [m]ost of
them sustain comparison neither to such
achievements in weaving as Navajo blankets,
nor to contemporary painting and sculpture”
(Smith 1990).
Smith’s evaluation presents another facet
of the picture that often consigned Tawney’s
work to an art world “limbo,” the artist’s
refusal of the utilitarian associations of craft.
Instead of exploring this aspect of her work,
however, Smith uses Tawney’s departure from
utility or function to reassert the differences
between craft (Navajo weaving) and art
(painting and sculpture) that the woven
forms actually complicated.
Elissa Auther Fiber Art and the Hierarchy of Art and Craft,
1960–1980 19
The Journal of Modern Craft Volume 1—Issue 1—March 2008,
pp. 13–34
These equivocations over the value
of Tawney’s woven work indicate the
institutional obstacles artists, critics and
curators of fi ber art faced in trying to
legitimate their work or the genre of fi ber
art. Examining Constantine and Larsen’s
efforts to consecrate fi ber art provides an
opportunity to look more systematically
at the questions of artistic legitimacy that
Tawney’s work provoked.
Wall Hangings
The fi rst major American exhibition of the
new genre of fi ber art was MoMA’s Wall
Hangings (Figures 6 and 7), co-curated by
Constantine and, at her invitation, Larsen.
With this exhibition, the two established
themselves as the fi ber movement’s
leading experts and also set the canon of
vanguard fi ber art (or “Art Fabric,” as they
called it). Wall Hangings presented to the
American museum-going public the fi rst
international survey of primarily large-scale,
abstract woven and off-loom work in fi ber.
Planned by Constantine since 1966, the
exhibition toured eleven cities in 1968,
returning in early 1969 to MoMA, where it
was installed, at Constantine’s insistence, in
the museum’s fi rst-fl oor special exhibition
galleries, rather than in the Department of
Architecture and Design. To the American
fi ber world’s leading triumvirate of Lenore
Tawney, Sheila Hicks (Figure 8) and Claire
Zeisler, Constantine and Larsen added Kay
Sekimachi (Figure 9), Walter Nottingham and
Ed Rossbach, among others. Some of the
exhibition’s most advanced work came from
abroad, including that of the Swiss Françoise
Grossen, Yugoslavian (Croatian) Jagoda Buic,
and Poles Magdalena Abakanowicz and
Wojcieh Sadley, whose woven forms radically
departed from the conventions of tapestry
as practiced in Europe at the time.
Regrettably, given the exhibition’s
importance in the history of the American
fi ber movement, the only national art-world
press Wall Hangings received was a review
that Craft Horizons commissioned from
Fig 6 Wall Hangings, Installation
view, Museum of Modern Art,
New York, 1969.
20 Fiber Art and the Hierarchy of Art and Craft, 1960–1980
Elissa Auther
The Journal of Modern Craft Volume 1—Issue 1—March 2008,
pp. 13–34
Fig 7 Wall Hangings, Installation
view, Museum of Modern Art,
New York, 1969.
Fig 8 Sheila Hicks, The Principal Wife Goes On,
1977, silk, linen, and wool elements ca. 180 in. long,
Smithsonian American Art Museum, Gift of
S.C. Johnson & Son, Inc.
sculptor Louise Bourgeois.4 Bourgeois’s
response to the show contrasts strikingly
with the curatorial statement written by
Constantine and Larsen for the exhibition
catalog. Whereas they asserted: “During the
last ten years, developments in weaving have
caused us to revise our concepts of this craft
and to view the work within the context of
twentieth-century art” (1969: n.p.), Bourgeois
concluded:
The pieces in the show rarely liberate
themselves from decoration . . . A painting
or a sculpture makes great demand on
the onlooker at the same time that it
is independent of him. These weaves,
delightful as they are, seem more engaging
and less demanding. If they must be
classifi ed, they would fall somewhere
between fi ne and applied art. (Bourgeois
1969: 33f)
These opposing evaluations speak to the
period’s symbolic boundaries between
Elissa Auther Fiber Art and the Hierarchy of Art and Craft,
1960–1980 21
The Journal of Modern Craft Volume 1—Issue 1—March 2008,
pp. 13–34
art and non-art and such boundaries’
centrality to maintaining a hierarchy of
media separating fi ne art from craft. They
also point to a set of real and conceptual
obstacles heavily borne by fi ber artists and
their supporters, who sought to transcend
the aesthetic boundaries structuring the
art world in the 1960s and 1970s. It is
clear that although Bourgeois disagreed
with Constantine and Larsen about the
works’ value, the question of their identity
preoccupied them all. This preoccupation
continued to burden Constantine and Larsen
and subsequent scholars of fi ber art, who
took up issues of category over the nature
of the work itself. Bourgeois also exhibits
concern about categories of art, which
she strategically upheld in this review by
using fi ber art to explain what painting and
sculpture were not.
Bourgeois’s evaluation of the works
in Wall Hangings as “decoration” traded
upon the distinction between art and
craft, reasserting the aesthetic boundaries
fi ber artists and their supporters sought
to overcome.5 In her words, the objects
were “delightful” and “engaging.” In the
writing of modernist critics such as Clement
Greenberg, to be either was to succumb
to the decorative by making “immediately
pleasing” what true artists achieved through
rigorous intellectual struggle and risk-taking
(Greenberg 1945; 1986: 41). Bourgeois
reasserts the high-art status of painting and
sculpture by claiming that these genres place
a “demand” upon the viewer absent in fi ber
art. The opposition she reinforces—that
between the merely attractive object and
that which requires sustained attention—is
central to the hierarchy of art and craft,
which associates art with the cognitive realm,
craft with mere surface effect.
In an interview, Constantine related
her reaction to Bourgeois’s review of Wall
Hangings: “I was furious,” she reported. “It
represented exactly the attitude we were
trying to work against” (conversation with
the author 1999). Despite their awareness of
the negative art-world attitudes concerning
Fig 9 Kay Sekimachi, Nagare VII, 1970, woven
monofi lament, 80 × 9 × 9 in. Smithsonian
American Art Museum.
22 Fiber Art and the Hierarchy of Art and Craft, 1960–1980
Elissa Auther
The Journal of Modern Craft Volume 1—Issue 1—March 2008,
pp. 13–34
media traditionally associated with craft,
Constantine and her co-curator attempted
to assert fi ber’s art status by introducing
the new genre into the fi ne art world on
the terms set by that world. This strategy
required that they adhere to the dominant
philosophy of art voiced by Bourgeois and
that they collude in maintaining the hierarchy
of media responsible for fi ber’s general
exclusion from “high art.”
Two factors in particular made this
strategy meaningful to Constantine and
Larsen, factors that were themselves effects
of the hierarchy of art and craft and indicate
its self-perpetuating nature. The most
important such factor was that contesting
the hierarchy of art and craft rather than
assimilating select objects (those without
utility, for instance) into the category of
“high art” would have been to destabilize
the very artistic status they claimed for fi ber
artists. The identity of fi ber artists, because
they worked in a medium traditional to
craft, required authorization different from
that bestowed upon artists working securely
within the “high art” world. The necessity of
establishing one’s art as art was a handicap
that belonged only to the artist working in
craft media. An excellent example of the
nearly automatic security afforded the artist
with a position within the “high art” world is
demonstrated by the critic Hilton Kramer’s
review of Lucy Lippard’s 1966 exhibition
Eccentric Abstraction for the New York Times.
About the show, which featured work that
radically fl outed sculptural conventions of
the time, Kramer wrote: “[although the] work
is neither painting nor exactly sculpture . . .
art—of some sort—it is” (1966). The lack of
legitimacy afforded an exhibition such as Wall
Hangings illustrates that this basic security
was not extended to fi ber artists in the
1960s and 1970s.
A second factor informing Constantine
and Larsen’s approach was the fact that
overtly challenging the hierarchy of art and
craft might also undermine their authority
as curators, with negative consequences
for fi ber art’s legitimation. Constantine, like
others of her generation at MoMA (she
joined the staff in 1948), was committed
to the museum’s mission of identifying and
collecting modern works of the highest
achievement in art and design, a goal that
included examples of applied art or craft
to the extent such objects conformed
to modernist norms of innovation and
abstraction. Anni Albers’s solo exhibition in
1949, MoMA’s fi rst exhibition of weaving,
exemplifi es this approach to craft and design.
Albers’s association with the Bauhaus, her
commitment to experimentation and her
adaptation of non-objective form to weaving
all refl ected the museum’s vision of the
modern in art.
In the 1960s, exemplary objects of
craft and design were still subject to this
form of evaluation, which helped to shape
Constantine’s curatorial vision for Wall
Hangings. An anecdote of Constantine’s
about her experience as a curator while
MoMA was directed by René d’Harnoncourt
is revealing in this regard. Sometime in the
mid-1960s a hand-thrown vessel by the
celebrated Japanese potter Kitaoji Rosanjin
was under consideration for acquisition. In
Constantine’s account, d’Harnoncourt put
this rhetorical question to his curators at
the acquisitions meeting: “Do you know
when a pot is no longer a pot but a work
of art?” (in conversation with the author).6
To Constantine, such a question affi rmed
Elissa Auther Fiber Art and the Hierarchy of Art and Craft,
1960–1980 23
The Journal of Modern Craft Volume 1—Issue 1—March 2008,
pp. 13–34
that the distinction to be found between art
and craft resided in the object rather than
being culturally projected onto it. Such an
assumption underscores Constantine and
Larsen’s confi dence regarding the art status
they asserted for the objects exhibited in
Wall Hangings.
The story also foregrounds the authority
that rested in the curator (in the capacity
of his or her “eye”) to discern quality in
objects of cultural signifi cance—that is, to
distinguish between good and bad, “high”
and “low,” “fi ne art” and mere “craft.”7 A
theory of boundary maintenance in the arts
that pointed to the role of extra-aesthetic
factors such as gender, different contexts of
production, or cultural presumptions about
craft in the production of distinctions of
value had the potential to undermine this
curatorial and ultimately institutional form of
power in the 1960s. To the extent that the
authorization of an object as art is only as
good (that is, as convincing) as the authority
of the authorizer, undercutting the myth of
the “good eye,” could result in the forfeiture
of institutional validation required by a new
genre or movement.
A straightforward way of approaching
the conception of value that Constantine
and Larsen used—a conception of value
as intrinsic to an object—is to consider
it alongside the recontextualization of
non-Western objects from ethnographic
specimens to works of art, a taxonomic
shift to which Constantine and Larsen’s
conception is related. Recent anthropological
and art historical considerations of shifts in
classifying objects previously recognized for
their ethnographic value demonstrate that
museum strategies of acquisition and display
that elide issues of context, technique and
utility in favor of disinterested contemplation
of an object according to a modernist theory
of art are premised upon art’s autonomy
from the social realm.8 As Mary Anne
Staniszewski has shown in her study of the
history of exhibitions at MoMA, the type of
installation now considered standard in fi ne-
art museums—in which works are spotlit,
arranged at eye level on a neutral-colored
wall and widely spaced—emerged in the
US in the 1930s.9 This type of installation,
which “facilitated appreciation of the
singular artwork,” was also applied to the
exhibition of ethnic “artifacts” in museums
in the 1930s and 1940s, refl ecting a growing
aesthetic appreciation of non-Western
objects. In general, the aestheticized display
of ethnographic objects suppressed issues
of utility and highlighted form, creating an
atemporal, formalist viewing experience
unencumbered by the social formations
that gave rise to the work. As scholars of
exhibition history have pointed out, this
reconceptualization confi rms the aesthetic
categories and assumptions of the West’s
“high art,” leaving intact the hierarchy of
the arts and stripping those aspects of
non-Western objects in confl ict with that
hierarchy’s values.
Constantine, who assisted d’Harnoncourt
in a major exhibition that employed
this strategy—1954’s Ancient Art of the
Andes—was familiar with such a curatorial
strategy and it undoubtedly infl uenced the
conception of Wall Hangings and subsequent
fi ber-art projects. As installation photographs
of the show suggest (see Figures 6 and
7), Constantine and Larsen hung the
work according to a method of displaying
painting and sculpture calculated to enhance
the objects’ visual impact and autonomy.
24 Fiber Art and the Hierarchy of Art and Craft, 1960–1980
Elissa Auther
The Journal of Modern Craft Volume 1—Issue 1—March 2008,
pp. 13–34
Although such installation was by then
typical at MoMA for painting and sculpture,
its adoption in Wall Hangings can be seen
as strategic given fi ber’s lack of autonomy—
that is, its extensive use in cultural and
practical contexts outside the art world.
The exhibition displayed works in relative
isolation within the austere, white-walled
special exhibition galleries. This strategy was
carried over to the catalog, which lacked
detailed information about technique and
enhanced the works’ formal qualities by
using photographs in which each object was
suspended in space against a dark ground, a
classic mid-century method of photographing
ethnic objects that privileged form over
implied utility.10
Constantine and Larsen’s approach
to the consecration of fi ber art has a
logic rooted in a belief in formalism as a
democratic, legitimating discourse capable of
transforming any work into an object of pure,
aesthetic contemplation. As the negative
reception of Wall Hangings suggests, however,
applying a formalist exhibition strategy did
not resolve the hierarchy of art and craft
Constantine and Larsen sought to transcend.
An important factor at play here is the
way fi ber’s “low” aesthetic status derived in
part from its extensive use in cultural and
practical contexts outside the art world.
Briefl y examining these contexts illuminates
the larger challenge that Constantine and
Larsen faced in attempting to elevate fi ber as
a medium of “high art.”
Utility and Amateurism
Outside the “high” art world, fi ber gained
a new visibility in the United States in
the 1960s and 1970s with revivals of the
traditional crafts of hand weaving, quilting,
embroidery, dyeing, knotting and basketry.
The social and artistic contexts and practices
surrounding these revivals included the
back-to-the-land and hippie movements,
the renewed interest in folk art around
the American Bicentennial, trends in the
personalization of clothing like the adoption
of African dress by African Americans, the
feminist recuperation of women’s history,
the revival of traditional arts of minority
communities in the South and Southwest,
and the popular craze of macramé. Increased
funding from the National Endowment
for the Arts to support regional arts and
a burgeoning commercial market for craft
and folk art bolstered interest in such
work.11 These contexts and practices
demonstrate both the richness of what
ought to be defi ned as a major craft revival
in the United States in the 1960s and
1970s and the diffi culty, for fi ber artists, of
distinguishing their work from this nebulous
conglomeration.
The frequent connection made between
fi ber art and macramé—an association
encouraged by fi ber artists’ extensive use of
off-loom techniques such as knotting, looping,
linking and plaiting—shows why fi ber artists
strove to distinguish their work from popular
craft. The term macramé, which denotes a
form of lateral knotting probably Arabic in
origin, referred in the late 1960s and 1970s
to a genre of useful objects (jewelry, belts,
handbags, lampshades, plant holders and
hammocks, for instance) produced using
“decorative” knotting techniques. Macramé’s
enormous popularity is attested to by the
fact that fi ber artist and macramé specialist
Mary Walker Phillip’s best-selling book Step-
by-Step Macramé (1970) had sold more than
a million copies by 1976. In 1971, macramé’s
Elissa Auther Fiber Art and the Hierarchy of Art and Craft,
1960–1980 25
The Journal of Modern Craft Volume 1—Issue 1—March 2008,
pp. 13–34
mass cultural appeal even led the Museum of
American Folk Art in New York to organize
a major exhibition on the subject as part of
its NEA-funded series, Rediscovery of Grass
Roots America.12 Unusually, this exhibition
placed contemporary macramé within the
larger history of knotted-fabric construction,
bringing together, among other examples,
seventeenth- and eighteenth-century
European lace, knotted fabrics and jewelry
of North American Indians, the decorative
knotted work of sailors from the nineteenth
and twentieth centuries, and the large-
scale knotted work of fi ber artist Françoise
Grossen, which emphasized the fi ber art-
macramé connection.
The macramé hobby craze was
problematic for the fi ber movement because
it reinforced assumptions about fi ber as
a woman’s medium or of “low” art status.
In her 1979 interview for Arts Magazine,
Claire Zeisler (Figure 10) took advantage of
a question about technique to distinguish
her knotting (often in macramé) from the
“decorative” knots used by “macramé artists,”
suggesting the sensitivity of the topic for
artists working in fi ber.
J. PATRICE MARANDEL: In your own mind,
do techniques such as knotting or cutting
. . . set you apart from other fi ber artists?
CLAIRE ZEISLER: When I fi rst started
knotting, it was not a trend . . . You
certainly have heard the word macramé.
Some people referred to my work as
Claire Zeisler’s macramé. That’s when
I hit the ceiling . . . I do mind the word
macramé because macramé today
means a decorative knot and I use my
knotting technique as structure . . . The
knot becomes the base for the piece, like
the canvas is the base for a painting. (Arts
Magazine 1979: 151f)
Zeisler’s insistence on distinguishing between
her work and popular craft underscores the
degree to which macramé had become a
cultural phenomenon that impinged upon
the fi ber movement’s bid for status as art. It
is likely that Zeisler’s discussion of technique
in the pages of Arts Magazine—then a
leading periodical of contemporary art that
rarely dedicated copy to work in traditional
craft media—was a deliberate attempt to
persuade readers that work in fi ber shared
important features with art rather than the
popular hobby of macramé. Her reference to
knotting as integral to the “structure” of her
pieces emphasizes her art’s formalist nature,
as does her explicit parallel between her
work and painting. The latter comparison
places her work fi rmly in the category
of “high art” by invoking its pre-eminent
medium to highlight not craft-oriented
technique but art-oriented practice. Zeisler’s
rhetorical strategy was similar to that
adopted by numerous fi ber artists and their
supporters who were work ing to change the
place of fi ber in the art world’s traditional
hierarchy, dismissing its con nections to
activities classifi ed as “non-art.”
This effort was actually initiated some
years earlier by Anni Albers, who in 1940
entered into a debate over the function and
value of hand weaving with Mary Atwater,
America’s leading spokesperson for the
non-professional weaver, in the pages of The
Weaver, a nationally distributed quarterly
for the American hand-weaving community.
Atwater and Albers’s confl ict over the
meaning and role of textiles not only helps
to demonstrate how deep the association of
26 Fiber Art and the Hierarchy of Art and Craft, 1960–1980
Elissa Auther
The Journal of Modern Craft Volume 1—Issue 1—March 2008,
pp. 13–34
Fig 10 Claire Zeisler, Coil Series III—A Celebration, 1978,
natural hemp and wool, 66½ × 34 in.
Smithsonian American Art Museum, Museum Purchase.
Elissa Auther Fiber Art and the Hierarchy of Art and Craft,
1960–1980 27
The Journal of Modern Craft Volume 1—Issue 1—March 2008,
pp. 13–34
amateurism with fi ber runs, but also points to
another serious hurdle fi ber artists (working
on or off the loom) faced in defi ning their
work as art in the 1960s and 1970s—fi ber’s
gendered associations.
Atwater, perhaps best known as the
founder of the Shuttle Craft Guild and
its correspondence course in weaving in
1920, advocated a view of weaving as a
leisure-time or therapeutic activity. She also
researched nineteenth-century weaving
traditions, publishing her fi ndings alone or in
publications for hand weavers. Her research
was instrumental to the survival of these
historical and regional practices, but her
practical how-to approach ran counter to
the idea of weaving as an art form. As Ed
Rossbach put it, “She told Americans what to
weave, how to weave, [and] what to do with
their weavings” (Rossbach 1983: 22).13
Unlike Atwater, Albers regarded herself
as an artist and was particularly outspoken
regarding hand weaving’s potential to move
beyond a leisure pursuit with utilitarian
imperatives. She encapsulated her views in
a statement from 1959 that implored, “let
threads be articulate ... and fi nd a form for
themselves to no other end than their own
orchestration, not to be sat on, walked on,
only to be looked at” (Albers 1959: 5).
Albers’s article for The Weaver
commented upon the state of contemporary
hand weaving in the United States and
illustrated textiles produced by her students
at Black Mountain College in North
Carolina.14 The publication coincided with
the height of the Appalachian craft revival,
in which weaving played a major role,
providing the immediate context for her
sharp critique of “recipes” and “traditional
formulas, which once proved successful”
(Albers 1940: 3). Albers argued that “such
work is often no more than a romantic
attempt to recall a temps perdu” and that
it refl ected the state of “isolation” and
“degeneration” of US hand weaving in
the period (Albers 1940: 3). Albers called
for a return to “fundamentals” and free
experimentation on and off the loom to
foster innovation and she suggested that the
resulting development of new forms in fi ber
could be art.
Atwater, who had no interest in the
imperatives of industry, modern design, or
art, responded to Albers with the predictably
titled piece “It’s Pretty—But is it Art?” which
she published in her advice column for The
Weaver.15 Not surprisingly, Atwater took
offense at Albers’s criticisms of faithfulness
to tradition and reproduction of historical
patterns (or “recipes,” as Atwater called
them). Atwater found the idea that a textile
might lack utility or be considered art
preposterous and she asserted that fi ber and
weaving were essentially useful and of value
primarily as an “escape from the distresses
or the hum-drum detail of our daily lives”
(Atwater 1941: 13). Atwater responded to
Albers’s emphasis upon experimentation,
imagination and the creation of new forms
(key elements of modernism in the arts)
with similar disdain, defending the right of her
readers (mostly women isolated at home
with little access to artistic training) to draw
aesthetic pleasure from weaving regardless
of their level of expertise or whether they
relied upon a pattern. Atwater’s acceptance,
even promotion, of her audience’s
amateurism raises the issue of fi ber’s
connections to femininity and domesticity,
associations that plagued fi ber artists in the
1960s and 1970s.
28 Fiber Art and the Hierarchy of Art and Craft, 1960–1980
Elissa Auther
The Journal of Modern Craft Volume 1—Issue 1—March 2008,
pp. 13–34
Femininity and Domesticity
As Rozsika Parker and Griselda Pollock
asserted over twenty-fi ve years ago in
their groundbreaking study Old Mistresses:
Women, Art and Ideology, “the sex of the artist
matters. It conditions the way art is seen
and discussed” (Parker and Pollock 1981: 50).
This is nowhere more evident in the late
twentieth century than in the history of the
fi ber movement, whose status and reception
was affected by weaving’s near-universal
association with women and the domestic
realm.
At its worst, the effort to challenge the
gendering of fi ber as “women’s work” actually
reinforced its association with femininity and
its low place in the art-world hierarchy. In
the late 1960s, when off-loom techniques
rose to prominence, the work of fi ber artists
adopting these techniques was interpreted
as free from the craft tradition’s conventional
values by virtue of their rejection of the
loom. Critics such as Rose Slivka posited
an antithesis between loom and off-loom
construction techniques, praising the latter’s
superior artistic quality. In the provocatively
titled piece “Hard String,” her review of the
1972 group exhibition Sculpture in Fiber for
Craft Horizons, Slivka characterized the loom
as an impotent instrument, a gendering of
technology that had the unintended effect
(as did the title of the show) of associating
off-loom fi ber art with the perceived
masculine virility of modern art.16
Not surprisingly, given this context,
the housewife is a key fi gure in critical
considerations of fi ber art, where she
signifi es amateurism and lack of creativity.
In criticism about fi ber art, this set of
associations often had to be overcome
before the writer could consider a work
of fi ber art worthy of discursive attention.
Fiber’s problematic association with
the domestic accounts for the distinctly
confessional tone or hedging found in fi ber-
art criticism that invokes the fi gure of the
housewife. Katherine Kuh’s article about the
work of Lenore Tawney and Claire Zeisler
for Saturday Review in 1968, “Sculpture:
Woven and Knotted,” is typical. Despite her
eventual enthusiasm, Kuh opens the article
by admitting her own negative assumptions
about weaving, shared, presumably, by her
readers: “Until recently, I always considered
weaving a ladylike pursuit for frustrated
housewives, but I am drastically changing
my mind. The best weavers are, to be sure,
still women, but some of them are also
fi rst-rate artists” (Kuh 1968: 36). The use of
the qualifi cation “also,” as in “also fi rst-rate
artists,” echoes George O’Brien’s language
in his review of Tawney’s work in the 1963
exhibition Woven Forms cited earlier. In that
piece, Tawney is “more than a weaver—she
is also an artist,” a qualifi cation that O’Brien
used later in the review to question craft’s
amateur associations. In Kuh’s assessment,
the amateur associations of craft are now
infl ected by gender, yet another barrier to
participation in the art world in this period
faced by fi ber artists—a problem shared
by female artists generally, in fact. Critic
Gregory Battcock’s review of Claire Zeisler’s
show at the Richard Feigen gallery in 1969
provides another example of how the image
of the housewife was used to great effect in
backhanded compliments during the 1960s:
Mops, fl oppiness and house-wifey
dumpiness might distract the viewer,
but only momentarily. The colors are
certainly more arresting than the Sheriff
Elissa Auther Fiber Art and the Hierarchy of Art and Craft,
1960–1980 29
The Journal of Modern Craft Volume 1—Issue 1—March 2008,
pp. 13–34
in Darien Conn. and the general tone
is more elegant than The Hilton Inn.
Zeisler’s sculptures emphasize texture at
the expense of form and since texture is
emphasized in just the right way, it’s O.K.
(Battcock 1969: 65)
For Kuh the housewife is capable but
frustrated, while for Battcock she is dumpy—
differences that might have to do with the
differing gender of the two critics or the
audience for whom they were writing. In
any case, Battcock’s idea of the housewife
and the comparison of Zeisler’s work to
mops remove fi ber from even the sphere of
craft, downgrading it to drudgery. Ultimately,
Kuh’s and Battcock’s language permits notice
of Zeisler’s work only to the extent that
their own critical authority regarding the
evaluation of art is assured.
This brief examination of fi ber’s extra-
aesthetic associations sheds additional light
on Constantine and Larsen’s approach to
elevating fi ber in a manner that completely
elided its history as a craft and its presence
in numerous contemporary non-art
contexts. Alternate approaches proved no
more successful and further demonstrate
the severity of the constraints they faced
in championing a material historically
categorized and marginalized as craft.
For instance, hybrid categories that could
bridge the divide between “art” and “craft,”
presumably better accommodating fi ber art,
also faced serious resistance in the art world.
Two such categories were “soft art” and “soft
sculpture.” Both functioned as organizing
rubrics for several shows in the late 1960s
and 1970s such as the New Jersey State
Museum’s Soft Art (1969), curated by Ralph
Pomeroy; Lucy Lippard’s traveling exhibition
for the American Federation of the Arts, Soft
and Apparently Soft Sculpture (1968–9); and
the New York Cultural Center’s traveling
exhibition, Softness as Art (1973). Soft Art
included work by Tawney as well as Richard
Tuttle, Robert Morris, Eva Hesse and Claes
Oldenburg. Softness as Art included one
work by Françoise Grossen, amongst that of
Jackie Ferrara, Harmony Hammond, Richard
Serra, Robert Morris and Hannah Wilke.
Fiber artists recognized the potential of the
categories “soft art” or “soft sculpture” to
bridge the divide separating art from craft. In
a 1970 article for Craft Horizons, “When Will
Weaving Be an Art Form?” Virginia Hoffman
observed that “soft sculpture”
could logically include any three-
dimensional form made by fl exible joinings,
fi brous materials, modules with no fi xed
beginning or end, soft materials made hard
and vice versa . . . One thinks of . . . works
by Eva Hesse, Alan Saret, Robert Morris,
[and] Alice Adams. (Hoffman 1970: 18)17
However, art critics were not as
enthusiastic about the soft art phenomenon.
Some went to great lengths to reassert
boundaries between genres and materials
that the rubric blurred. In his Artforum review
of the New York Cultural Center’s exhibition,
Soft as Art, James Collins asserted, “one of the
things artists shouldn’t do today is to make
art with anything soft” (Collins 1973: 89).
Most problematic for him was the category
itself, which “denies criticism the luxury of a
single critical framework” (Collins 1973: 89).
His response to this dilemma was to divide
the work in the show into four categories:
“process,” “revamped painting,” “craft/fetish,”
and “novelty art.” In his review these
categories redrew boundaries distinguishing
30 Fiber Art and the Hierarchy of Art and Craft, 1960–1980
Elissa Auther
The Journal of Modern Craft Volume 1—Issue 1—March 2008,
pp. 13–34
“art” from “craft” through a separation of
works in fi ber into either the “process”
category or the “craft/fetish” category. In the
“process” category, Collins singled out a felt
work by Robert Morris (along with a work
by Richard Serra) as worthy of attention for
its “theoretical underpinnings” (Collins 1973:
89). The rest of the work in the exhibition
was “neither experientially nor theoretically
interesting” (Collins 1973: 90). In the “craft/
fetish” category, Collins placed the work of
Françoise Grossen, Jackie Ferrara and Brenda
Miller, all of whom exhibited work in fi ber.
Anticipating readers’ objections to his
application of the term craft to the work
of three female artists working in rope and
other forms of fi ber, Collins asserted that
“labeling a work as craft orientated isn’t an
attack on women” (Collins 1973: 90). He
continued:
[it] is only to say they give the impression
of manual over mental dexterity and
people who are manually dextrous aren’t
necessarily interesting artists . . . Both Jackie
Ferrara with her Four Balls II made of
cotton bailing, rope and chains and Brenda
Miller’s Abscissa, consisting of a numbering
system dictating the structure of a twine
wall piece just by the use and association
of their materials suggest a “grass skirt”
reference—a gender neutral one. (Collins
1973: 90)
That the term “grass skirt” in this context
is not gender neutral hardly requires
elucidation. By rejecting the hybrid category
“soft art” and reinforcing fi ber’s association
to femininity and primitiveness, Collins neatly
maintains the boundary between art and
craft. Given the degree to which fi ber’s
associations to craft overwhelm Collins’s
response to Ferrara and Miller’s work, one
can imagine how easily the hybrid character
of work by fi ber artists such as Grossen,
Sheila Hicks, or Ed Rossbach could be
dismissed as illegitimate.
Thanks to such challenging associations,
Constantine and Larsen’s goal of elevating
fi ber art, in Wall Hangings and subsequent
projects, was diffi cult if not impossible to
accomplish in the 1960s and 1970s. In
addition, their acceptance of the division
between “high” and “low” art—and, one
might argue, their refusal to engage with the
very non-art connotations of fi ber that were
so problematic for their goals—left art-world
prejudices intact. Despite Constantine and
Larsen’s efforts, the work of Alan Saret—a
male artist who had worked previously in
other media and was part of the “high” art
world—was critically received as art, while
Alice Adams’s remarkably similar work—
emerging from her previous practice as a
weaver—was viewed with skepticism.
Despite these setbacks, however, evidence
suggests that Constantine and Larsen’s work
did make an impact, albeit one that did
not come to fruition until fairly recently. In
1993 the well-known independent curator
Mary Jane Jacob wrote about the infl uence
of Constantine upon her own innovative,
unorthodox, curatorial mission, which has
been instrumental in promoting an open use
of non-traditional materials without regard to
the art world’s hierarchical distinctions:
Unbeknownst to me, I began following
[Constantine’s] work as a young visitor to
The Museum of Modern Art on frequent
occasions from 1965 to 1969. Reading
and just looking at the images in Beyond
Craft sent my own curatorial work in
Elissa Auther Fiber Art and the Hierarchy of Art and Craft,
1960–1980 31
The Journal of Modern Craft Volume 1—Issue 1—March 2008,
pp. 13–34
another direction. Most of all, her avant-
garde philosophy of inclusionism remains
compelling and her career—battling
to bring into the mainstream art and
artists from the outside—is a model of
independent vision and courage. (Jacob
1993: 9)
Dramatic change in the use and reception
of fi ber in the art world has occurred since
the 1970s, thanks in part to curators such
as Jacob, as well as artists of the fi rst-wave
feminist art movement, who set as one of
their goals the analysis of the hierarchy of art
and craft and women’s low position within
it through the incorporation of fi ber into
their work. For feminists, fi ber craft played
a role in the construction of an alternate
history of art making. A shared marginality
between the female traditional artist and
the contemporary feminist artist helped
the latter negotiate the paradoxical goal of
seeking recognition in the mainstream art
world, while at the same time attempting to
critique it. In this context, the once negative
associations of fi ber or craft with femininity
and the home were recast as distinctive
and culturally valuable features of an artistic
heritage specifi c to women.
While I’m not convinced that the use
of fi ber by artists today demonstrates the
complete effacement of the hierarchy of art
and craft, the medium’s ubiquitous use in
contemporary art no doubt represents an
important stage in a decades-long process
of art-world assimilation of the medium.
The evolution of Louise Bourgeois’s attitude
toward fi ber—from one of dismissal in the
1960s to full embrace with her latest soft
sculptures made from her personal collection
of linens and fabric remnants—is only one
Fig 11 Louise Bourgeois, Untitled, 2002, tapestry
and aluminum. 17 × 12 × 12 in.; 43.1 × 30.4 ×
30.4 cm. Private Collection, courtesy of Cheim &
Reid, New York. Christopher Burke photographer.
example of the medium’s new currency. The
limitations of their strategy aside, the work
of Constantine and Larsen in the 1960s and
1970s to legitimate fi ber as a medium of art
represents a historically important moment
in this process.
Notes
1 Constantine, Mildred and Larsen, Jack Lenor.
1972. Beyond Craft: The Art Fabric. New York: Van
Nostrand. For an assessment of Constantine’s
career see Sorkin, Jenni. 2003. Way Beyond
Craft: Thinking Through the Work of Mildred
Constantine. Textile 1(1): 30–47.
32 Fiber Art and the Hierarchy of Art and Craft, 1960–1980
Elissa Auther
The Journal of Modern Craft Volume 1—Issue 1—March 2008,
pp. 13–34
2 Woven Forms. 1963. New York: Museum of
Contemporary Craft.
3 See also Atirnomus, “String and Rope at Janis.”
Arts Magazine. February 1970: 58.
4 Bourgeois, Louis. 1969. “The Fabric of
Construction.” Craft Horizons 29 (March): 31–35.
5 On the subject, see my 2004 essay “The
Decorative, Abstraction and the Hierarchy of
Art and Craft in the Art Criticism of Clement
Greenberg,” in Oxford Art Journal 27(3): 339–64.
6 Constantine also recounts this experience in The
Art Fabric: Mainstream (New York: Van Nostrand
Reinhold, 1981, p. 8).
7 Thanks to Judith Bettelheim, Constantine’s
daughter, for helping me clarify this point.
8 On the subject, see Marcus, George E. and
Myers, Fred R. (eds). 1995. Traffi c in Culture:
Refi guring Art and Anthropology. Berkeley:
University of California Press.
9 Staniszewski, Mary Anne. 1998. The Power of
Display: A History of Exhibition Installations at the
Museum of Modern Art. Cambridge, MA: MIT
Press, chapter two, passim.
10 See Price, Sally. 1989. Primitive Art in Civilized
Places. Chicago: University of Chicago Press; and
Staniszewski (1998).
11 See National Endowment for the Arts. 1977.
To Survey American Crafts: A Planning Study; and
McLean, John. (ed.). 1981. National Crafts Planning
Project. National Endowment for the Arts.
12 See Macramé. 1971. New York: Museum of
American Folk Art. See also Mary Walker
Phillips’s review of the exhibition for Craft
Horizons (December 1971): 62.
13 Rossbach also reported that “[Atwater’s]
writings were consulted (often a bit sheepishly)
by many who deplored her approach to textiles,
who did not believe that weavers should be
provided with ‘recipes,’ as she called them, for
works that ought to have been creative and
individual. I remember the small regard I once
felt for Atwater and her coverlet weaves, even
though my fi rst weaving experience consisted of
following a pattern carefully selected from her
book” (1983: 22).
14 Albers, Anni. 1940. Hand Weaving Today: Textile
Work at Black Mountain College. The Weaver
6(1): 3–7.
15 Atwater, Mary. 1941. It’s Pretty, But is it Art? The
Weaver 6(3): 13–14 and 26.
16 Slivka, Rose. 1972. Hard String. Craft Horizons
(April): 16–17.
17 See also Meilach, Dona Z. 1974. Soft Sculpture
and Other Soft Art Forms with Stuffed Fabrics,
Fibers and Plastics. New York: Crown Publishers.
References
Adamson, Glenn. 2007. Thinking Through Craft. Oxford
and New York: Berg.
Albers, Anni. 1940. Hand Weaving Today: Textile Work
at Black Mountain College. The Weaver 6(1): 3–7.
Albers, Anni. 1959. Pictorial Weaves. Cambridge, MA:
MIT Press.
Andreae, Christopher. 1970. String and Rope. The
Christian Science Monitor, January 23.
Atwater, Mary. 1941. It’s Pretty, But is it Art? The
Weaver 6 (3): 13–14, 26.
Battcock, Gregory. 1969. Claire Zeisler. Arts Magazine,
43(6): 65.
Bourgeois, Louise. 1969. The Fabric of Construction.
Craft Horizons 29: 31–5.
Collins, James. 1973. Review. Artforum 11(10): 89–93.
Constantine, Mildred. 1999. Personal interview with
the author, February 23.
Constantine, Mildred and Larsen, Jack Lenor. 1969.
Wall Hangings. New York: Museum of Modern Art.
Constantine, Mildred and Larsen, Jack Lenor. 1972.
Beyond Craft: The Art Fabric. New York: Van Nostrand.
Greenberg, Clement. “Review of Exhibitions . . .”
17 November 1945. The Nation. Reprinted in John
O’Brian, ed. 1986. Clement Greenberg, The Collected
Elissa Auther Fiber Art and the Hierarchy of Art and Craft,
1960–1980 33
The Journal of Modern Craft Volume 1—Issue 1—March 2008,
pp. 13–34
Essays and Criticism. Vol. 2, Arrogant Purpose,
1945–1949: 39–42.
Hoffman, Virginia. 1970. When Will Weaving Be an
Art Form? Craft Horizons 30: 18.
Jacob, Mary Jane. 1993. Beyond Craft: Curating for
Change. Small Works in Fiber: The Mildred Constantine
Collection. Cleveland, OH: The Cleveland Museum of
Art, pp. 1–9.
Kramer, Hilton. 1966. And Now “Eccentric
Abstraction”: It’s Art But Does It Matter? New York
Times September 25: 27.
Kuh, Katherine. 1968. Sculpture: Woven and Knotted.
Saturday Review. July 27: 36–7.
Lenore Tawney: A Personal World. 1978. Brookfi eld,
CT: Brookfi eld Craft Center.
Lippard, Lucy. 1966. Eccentric Abstraction. New York:
Fischbach Gallery. Reprinted in The New Sculpture
1965-1975: Between Geometry and Gesture. 1990.
New York: Whitney Museum of American Art, pp.
54–8.
Marandel, J. Patrice. 1979. An Interview with Claire
Zeisler. Arts Magazine 54(1): 150–152.
O’Brien, George. 1963. Many Materials Used in
Unusual Technique. New York Times. April 29
Parker, Rozsika and Pollock, Griselda. 1981. Old
Mistresses: Women, Art and Ideology. New York:
Pantheon Books.
Phillips, Mary Walker. 1970. Step-by-Step Macramé: A
Complete Introduction to the Craft of Creative Knotting.
New York: Golden Press.
Rossbach, Ed. 1983. Mary Atwater and the Revival of
American Traditional Weaving. American Craft 43(2):
22–26.
Smith, Paul. 1963. Woven Forms. New York: Museum
of Contemporary Craft.
Smith, Roberta. 1990. Lenore Tawney’s Work in Fiber
and Beyond. New York Times. May 18.
String and Rope. December 1969/January 1970. New
York: Sidney Janis Gallery.
Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further
reproduction prohibited without permission.
Belbin's team role theory: For non-managers also?
Fisher, S G;Hunter, T A;Macrosson, W D K
Journal of Managerial Psychology; 2002; 17, 1/2; ABI/INFORM
Collection
pg. 14
Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further
reproduction prohibited without permission.
Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further
reproduction prohibited without permission.
Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further
reproduction prohibited without permission.
Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further
reproduction prohibited without permission.
Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further
reproduction prohibited without permission.
Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further
reproduction prohibited without permission.
Exploring the Effects of Value Diversity on Team Effectiveness
David J. Woehr • Luis M. Arciniega •
Taylor L. Poling
Published online: 2 June 2012
� Springer Science+Business Media, LLC 2012
Abstract
Purpose The goal of the present study was to explore the
potential impact of within-team value diversity with
respect to both team processes and task performance.
Design/Methodology/Approach We explored value
diversity within a comprehensive framework such that all
components of basic human values were examined. A
sample of 306 participants randomly assigned to 60 teams,
performed a complex hands-on task, demanding high
interdependence among team members, and completed
different measures of values and team processes.
Findings Results indicated that value diversity among
team members had no significant impact on task perfor-
mance. However, diversity with respect to several value
dimensions had a significant unique effect on team process
criteria. Results were consistent with respect to the nature
of the impact of value diversity on team process outcomes.
Specifically, the impact of team value diversity was such
that less diversity was positively related to process out-
comes (i.e., more similarity resulted in more team cohesion
and efficacy and less conflict).
Implications The results indicated that disparity among
teammates in many of these values may have important
implications on subsequent team-level phenomena. We
suggest team leaders and facilitators of teambuilding
efforts could consider adding to their agendas a session
with team members to analyze and discuss the combined
value profiles of their team.
Originality/Value This is the first study to highlight the
unique impact of many unexamined, specific components
of team diversity with respect to values on team effec-
tiveness criteria.
Keywords Value diversity � Team diversity �
Team processes � Team effectiveness � Team cohesion �
Team conflict
Work force diversity has become an increasingly promi-
nent concern in recent years. Many organizations have
moved to incorporate diversity into their business structure
and strategy, hoping for both societal approval and positive
performance dividends (Horwitz 2005). The focus on
workforce diversity taken together with the increased
emphasis on team-based work groups (Applebaum and Batt
1994; Ilgen 1999) has resulted in a surge of interest in the
relationship between team member diversity and overall
team effectiveness (e.g., Bell 2007; Horwitz 2005; Klein
et al. 2011; Mannix and Neale 2005; Mohammed and
Angell 2004; Webber and Donahue 2001; Harrison et al.
1998; Jackson and Rudermann 1997; Milliken and Martins
1996). Although the majority of past research has focused
on demographic, surface-level diversity (e.g., age, race,
gender) (Harrison et al. 1998), the impact of diversity with
respect to deeper, psychological variables (e.g., values,
personality, attitudes, etc.) is likely of greater concern. As
stated by Hollenbeck et al. (2004), ‘‘demographic diversity
is actually less important to team performance than
D. J. Woehr
Management Department, University of North Carolina,
Charlotte, NC, USA
L. M. Arciniega (&)
Management Department, Instituto Tecnológico Autónomo de
México (ITAM) School of Business, Rı́o Hondo 1, Mexico,
D.F. 01080, Mexico
e-mail: [email protected]
T. L. Poling
Joint Advertising Market Research and Studies (JAMRS),
Alexandria, VA, USA
123
J Bus Psychol (2013) 28:107–121
DOI 10.1007/s10869-012-9267-4
psychological diversity, especially over time’’ (p. 357).
Thus, the goal of the present study is to explore the
potential impact of within team diversity with respect to a
specific set of psychological characteristics, namely values,
on both team processes and task outcomes. It is important
to highlight the lack of studies in this specific line of
research, and more over, the inexistence of a study utilizing
a comprehensive taxonomy of basic values to examine
these relations. This study focuses on filling this gap.
Defining Diversity
Mannix and Neale (2005) note that diversity is a complex
and multifaceted term. It encompasses a variety of differ-
ences among people (e.g., demographic variables, job-
related characteristics, attitudes, values, personality traits,
etc.), and the positive or negative effects of diversity may
be contingent on the variables under investigation as well
as the performance criteria. In a review of the team com-
position literature, Bell (2007) contends that discussing
‘‘diversity’’ without reference to a particular attribute or
variable is meaningless; thus, the specific variable or
family of variables on which team members differ must be
identified before the relationship with team effectiveness
can be examined. In order to provide some conceptual
organization to this multifaceted phenomenon, recent the-
oretical discussions have classified dimensions of diversity
into two general categories: attributes that are demographic
and non-psychological versus attributes that are non-visi-
ble, underlying, and psychological in nature.
Attributes that are non-psychological and easily detect-
able fall under Harrison et al.’s (1998) surface-level
categorization. Typical attributes associated with this cat-
egorization include age, gender, race, and physical dis-
abilities (Mannix and Neale 2005). In contrast, non-
apparent, psychological differences (e.g., cognitive ability,
personality traits, values, beliefs, and attitudes) fall under
the deep-level diversity category (Harrison et al. 1998).
Information on deep-level attributes is acquired via
observation of verbal and non-verbal behavioral patterns
(Harrison et al. 1998). The deep-level label is analogous to
similar descriptors such as ‘‘underlying’’ (Milliken and
Martins 1996), ‘‘less observable’’ (Jackson et al. 1995), and
‘‘psychological’’ (Jackson and Rudermann 1997) used by
other researchers. General values are prominent examples
of Harrison’s deep-level diversity category.
Team diversity in surface-level characteristics has been
the focus of ample research, which taken as a whole has
yielded equivocal results (e.g., Webber and Donahue
2001). However, deep-level diversity may operate dif-
ferently than surface-level diversity. In fact, research
has demonstrated that surface-level diversity does not
necessarily relate to deep-level diversity (Harrison et al.
2002; Horwitz and Horwitz 2007). These findings highlight
the need for the direct investigation of deep-level diversity.
Yet, the research on deep-level diversity to date is under-
developed. Specifically, Bell (2007) demonstrated that of
studies examining deep-level team composition, those
focusing on diversity are few, fragmented, and inconsis-
tent. Bell (2007) also highlighted the dearth of studies on
team member value composition despite the role of values
as behavioral and attitudinal determinants. Tentative find-
ings for only two specific value dimensions (i.e., collec-
tivism and preference for teamwork) were reported, which
were based on data from three and two studies, respec-
tively. Bell (2007) commented that further research is
necessary to assess the impact of other value dimensions on
team performance. We propose that these other dimensions
should include basic human values such as those related to
independent thought and action, stability of self and rela-
tionships, and the welfare of others with whom one is in
constant interaction. Basic values such as these have sig-
nificant implications for interpersonal behavior.
Theoretical Underpinnings of Potential Diversity
Effects
Research on deep-level team diversity in general and value
diversity in particular, has only recently begun to emerge
and is relatively limited (Harrison et al. 2002). However, the
theoretical foundations of this research are longstanding.
There are two primary classes of theories underpinning this
area of study. On the surface, these theories appear to be in
direct opposition, as one advocates a ‘‘pessimistic’’ view of
diversity and the other an ‘‘optimistic’’ view (Mannix and
Neale 2005). In general, the pessimistic view focuses on the
affective and interactional problems caused by diversity;
whereas, the optimistic view posits enhanced creativity,
quality, and innovative task performance that results from
increased access to a variety of perspectives and resources
(Kravitz 2005). Respectively, these views are grounded in
the similarity-attraction/social identity paradigms, and the
information processing/cognitive resource paradigms.
The Pessimistic Perspective
In general, negative expectations regarding the impact of
diversity in team composition stem from the similarity-
attraction paradigm (Byrne 1971; Tziner 1985) and social
identity theory (Tajfel 1978). Social attraction theory posits
that similarity in values, beliefs, and attitudes increases
interpersonal attraction, and when individuals like each
other, their values, beliefs, and attitudes become more
aligned. Together, attraction and similarity reciprocally
108 J Bus Psychol (2013) 28:107–121
123
build on one another, facilitating a pull toward symmetry
and an avoidance of the strain produced by dissimilarity
(Rosenbaum 1986; Mannix and Neale 2005). Furthermore,
people tend to categorize themselves relative to similar
others and in an effort to maintain their social identities,
they will demonstrate a bias toward those whom they
believe share similar characteristics (Tajfel and Turner
1986; Turner and Haslam 2001). Byrne’s (1971) early
attraction-similarity research supports the perspective that
individuals are drawn toward others who they think share
similar attitudes to themselves and report that these indi-
viduals are smarter and more well-adjusted than others.
These propositions also underlie Schneider’s (1987) well-
known attraction, selection, attrition (ASA) theory, which
supports the notion that this similarity-attraction process
naturally produces increasingly homogenous work envi-
ronments (Giberson et al. 2005).
Overall, these theories posit that teams with members
who have homogeneous values will more readily identify
with each other. As a result, they will be more cohesive,
facilitating both interaction and subsequent performance.
Teams with heterogeneous values are predicted to be less
cohesive and thus less productive because of the conflicts
and stress that result from the differences in members’
beliefs.
The Optimistic Perspective
Alternately, cognitive resource theory posits the ‘‘value in
diversity’’ hypothesis, which advocates the benefits of the
unique resources that members with diverse attributes
bring to the team (Cox and Blake 1991; Easley 2001). The
underlying assumption here is that diversity in members’
attributes will result in more informed decisions by pro-
moting creativity, innovation, and alternative problem
solving. This information processing perspective suggests
that differences create an opportunity for team members to
share different perspectives and thus examine issues at a
deeper level of analysis (Mannix and Neale 2005). More-
over, different perspectives are predicted to interact syn-
ergistically to create a ‘‘process gain’’ for the group to the
extent that they are able to overcome potential social-
integration problems that may result from their differing
perspectives (Steiner 1972).
Overall, cognitive resource theory posits that diverse
values among teammates will contribute to better team
performance. Members will share information from a
greater variety of perspectives, a practice that leads to
higher quality analysis of tasks, which in turn fosters higher
quality results. Although these two perspectives are often
pitted against one another, they are not necessarily mutu-
ally exclusive. For example, a team with a diverse set of
perspectives may produce high quality task results (e.g.,
more creative solutions), but in the process experience
interpersonal conflict and low cohesion. Moreover, even
supporters of the ‘‘value in diversity’’ hypothesis have
noted that diversity may offer benefits for team outcomes
even as it creates barriers for team interaction processes
(Mannix and Neale 2005). In sum, while these theoretical
perspectives posit different expectations with respect to
value diversity, it is feasible for both perspectives to
operate simultaneously contingent on the value and type of
criterion in question. Even when this approach is theoret-
ically feasible, a recent meta-analysis highlights the lack of
studies combining both perspectives at the same time (Van
Knippenberg and Schippers 2007).
As noted above, the primary goal of the present study is
to explore team member value diversity with respect to an
encompassing taxonomy of basic human values. Our
objective is to dig into the impact of team value diversity
on task performance, and on the following aspects of team
effectiveness: relationship and task conflict, cohesion, and
team efficacy. Below, we begin by presenting a brief
review of previous findings pertaining to value diversity.
Next, we discuss the concept of basic values and present
Schwartz comprehensive model of values. We then briefly
highlight the key team process variables that reflect team
effectiveness and that are likely to be influenced by team
member value diversity. Finally, we consider several
research questions addressed in this study.
Previous Findings on Value Diversity
As noted by Harrison et al. (2002), and more recently by
Bell (2007), relatively little research has examined the
impact of diversity among team members in terms of basic
values on effectiveness (c.f., Fisher et al. 1996; Jehn et al.
1997; Jehn and Mannix 2001; Klein et al. 2011). Among
the few relevant existing studies, there is substantial vari-
ability with respect to both the conceptual and operational
definitions of values as well as the outcomes examined.
One approach has been to measure basic values at the
individual-level using the classic Rokeach Value Survey
(RVS; Rokeach 1979), and then either aggregate members
responses to represent the team level (Rodriguez 1998) or
compute their concordance (Fisher et al. 1996). Regarding
this measure, Rokeach never suggested any a priori struc-
ture for the RVS, and further studies suggest that it does not
cover the full domain of the values construct (e.g., Sch-
wartz 1992). Nonetheless, Fisher and collaborators indi-
cated that teams with more agreement in their ratings
across the set of nine personal value scales, demonstrated
better task performance than those with less concordance.
Though the study presented promising results, it had a
serious limitation: after collecting data from 22 teams of
J Bus Psychol (2013) 28:107–121 109
123
undergraduate students, 12 teams were dropped due to
inadequate levels of concordance between the members’
values ratings. As a result, the analyses were based on only
ten teams. The other study employing this approach
(Rodriguez 1998), also used a very small sample - 11
teams.
Recently, Klein et al. (2011) demonstrated that team
leader style moderates the relationship between team val-
ues diversity and team conflict. They propose that leaders
who are task-focused, create a strong team setting, with
clear rules and roles, constraining the influence of team
members’ values. Even when the results are relevant, they
analyzed value diversity from a simple framework of two
values: Protestant work ethic (individual tendency to work
hard even in the absence of material rewards) and tradi-
tionalism (commitment and acceptance of the customs and
ideas of traditional cultures or religions). This framework
does not cover relevant aspects of the values domain such
as: power, achievement, altruism, etc.
Another approach used previously has been to measure
organizational values preferences at the individual-level,
and then compare the profiles of team members to assess
value congruence (Jehn 1994). Using this approximation,
Jehn and Mannix (2001) found that team member simi-
larity with respect to work-related values (i.e., similarity
across a set of 54 items related to innovativeness, stability,
detail orientation, outcome orientation, aggressiveness,
supportiveness, reward orientation, team orientation, and
decisiveness) was positively related to task performance.
However, Jehn and collaborators (1997) examined team
member value congruence (defined in terms of a median
split of team consistency scores derived from ratings of the
same work-related values discussed previously) and found
that value congruence was related to perceived perfor-
mance via a negative correlation with relationship conflict;
however, value congruence did not relate to subjective
expert ratings of task performance. Both studies employed
an adapted version of the Organizational Culture Profile
(OCP; O’Reilly et al. 1991). The OCP was developed to
obtain profiles of the cultures of organizations, and to
assess individual preferences for organizational cultures
(O’Reilly et al. 1991, p. 496), but not to measure individual
values.
Finally, there are various studies using cultural-level
values measured at the individual-level (e.g., Eby and
Dobbins 1997; Vodosek 2007). These studies rest on the
assumption that research on cultural diversity can be con-
ducted using individual-level representations of cultural-
level constructs (e.g., Maznevski et al. 2002), since some
aspects of culture are internalized by individuals. For
instance, Kirkman and Shapiro (2001) reported on the
impact of collectivism, power distance, doing orientation,
and determinism on the performance of self-managed work
teams, utilizing an instrument that operationalized two
different models of cultural values (i.e., Hofstede 1991;
Kluckhohn 1951). According to Smith and Schwartz
(1997), this approach is inappropriate. These cross-cultural
scholars argue that the relations between values at the
cultural level reflect the dynamic conflicts of the societies
that exist as a result of the way their institutions pursue
their goals, adding that these relations are not necessarily
the same at the individual-level.
Schwartz Model of Values
Values have generally been referred to as needs, beliefs, or
norms. Values can be best understood as cognitive repre-
sentations of universal needs (Rokeach 1979; Schwartz
1992). These needs are expressed through over-arching
goals that direct behavior across situations, and are ordered
by importance as guiding principles in life (Schwartz et al.
2001). Schwartz (1992) posits that the essence of a value is
the motivational goal it expresses. From this premise,
Schwartz derived ten value types that form a circumplex
structure (see Fig. 1) such that value types that share a
similar general motivational goal are arranged closer
together. Alternately, types representing divergent goals
are arranged more distantly around the circumference of
the model. For instance, power (PO) and achievement (AC)
are two compatible value types sharing the general
SD
UN
BE
CO
TR
SE
PO
AC
HE
ST
Fig. 1 Example value profiles of the members of a team. SD
self-
direction, UN universalism, BE benevolence, CO conformity,
TR tradition, SE security, PO power, AC achievement, HE
hedonism,
ST stimulation
110 J Bus Psychol (2013) 28:107–121
123
motivational goal of enhancing personal status, even at the
expenses of others. Conversely, power (PO) and univer-
salism (UN) are two conflicting values; the motivational
goal of power is enhancing personal interests, whereas the
motivational goal of universalism is promoting the welfare
of others. The pattern of compatibilities and incompati-
bilities between value types is based on the premise that
actions taken in the pursuit of each type have both psy-
chological and behavioral consequences, which may be
compatible or in conflict with the goals derived from other
values. The Appendix provides a brief description of the
ten value types (for a full description of these values see
Schwartz 1992). It is important to highlight that a recent
meta-analysis has validated this circumplex structure in
more than seventy countries (Steinmetz et al. 2012)
Team member diversity in values is represented by the
disparity in the importance assigned by each team member
to each value type. The more the variability in the impor-
tance assigned by the members of the team to each value
type, the greater the level of diversity across team mem-
bers. Figure 1 shows this idea schematically. Each radius
represents one of ten value types, and each irregular
decagon, the profile of a team member. The lower the
importance a team member assigns to a specific value, the
closer the point associated with that individual’s profile
will be to the center on the respective value radius. The
degree of spread among the points on a given value radius
represents the degree of team diversity on that value. For
instance, in the case of the radius of benevolence (BE), the
variance among the four value profiles is low compared to
the variance on the radius for achievement (AC). Given the
contradictory nature across the motivational goals under-
lying opposing values, it is inappropriate to compute an
overall ‘‘values’’ score. Moreover, the configuration
approach (Moynihan and Peterson 2001) argues that whe-
ther homogeneity or heterogeneity regarding deep-level
attributes is preferable depends on the specific value or trait
in question. Consistent with these propositions, diversity
should be examined separately with respect to each of the
ten values. Given the predominant role of values with
respect to attitudes and subsequent behavior, team member
value diversity is likely to be a key factor for both process
and outcome measures of team effectiveness.
Team Effectiveness
The evaluation of teams encompasses a variety of com-
ponents. Many theories have addressed the multifaceted
nature of team effectiveness (e.g., Shea and Guzzo 1987;
Gladstein 1984; Hackman 1987). According to Hackman
(1987), group effectiveness can be defined in terms of three
criteria. First, the final outputs produced by the team must
meet or exceed the standards set by key constituents within
the organization. Second, the internal social processes
operating as the team interacts should enhance, or at least
maintain, the group’s ability to work together in the future.
Finally, the experience of working in the team environment
should act to satisfy rather than aggravate the personal
needs of team members. In order to address these criteria,
team effectiveness evaluation should include both a mea-
sure of the teams’ final task performance as well as criteria
with which to assess intragroup process. The current paper
explores task performance and prominent team process
criteria. The three major intragroup process constructs
examined are: intra-group conflict, team cohesion, and
team-efficacy.
Intra-group conflict has emerged as an integral team
process variable. Previous research has differentiated two
components of intra-group conflict: relationship conflict and
task conflict. Jehn (1994) describes relationship conflict as
interpersonal incompatibilities between team members such
as annoyance and animosity. Task conflict occurs when
members convey divergent ideas and opinions about specific
aspects related to task accomplishment (Jehn 1994).
Research to date indicates that relationship conflict is largely
detrimental to team performance (e.g., Evan 1965; Baron
1991; van Woerkom and van Engen 2009; Vodosek 2007).
The impact of task conflict is less clear. While it has been
argued that task conflict facilitates enhanced performance
via thorough task analysis (e.g., Amason and Schweiger
1994), empirical evidence is equivocal. Some research has
found positive relationships between task conflict and novel
idea generation and strategic planning (Baron 1991; Amason
1996), but others have shown task conflict may hinder goal
accomplishment and implementation (Amason 1996;
Vodosek 2007). In a direct investigation of task conflict,
Jehn (1994) found only a small amount of task conflict was
beneficial, after which team performance began to deterio-
rate, and a meta-analysis demonstrated a negative relation-
ship between task conflict and team satisfaction and
performance (De Dreu and Weingart 2003). Nonetheless,
both types of conflict have proven to be significant correlates
of a variety of team effectiveness criteria.
Team cohesion is another important process variable
(Chiocchio and Essiembre 2009; Swezey and Salas 1992).
While a multitude of definitions and measures have been
offered (Mullen and Copper 1994), basically, cohesion is
viewed as ‘‘a general indicator of synergistic group
interaction—or process’’ (Barrick et al. 1998, p. 382).
Meta-analyses have revealed significant team cohesion-
performance effects (Beal et al. 2003; Mullen and Copper
1994). Furthermore, cohesion has been linked to greater
coordination during team-tasks (Morgan and Lassiter 1992)
as well as improved satisfaction, productivity, and group
interactions (Bettenhausen 1991).
J Bus Psychol (2013) 28:107–121 111
123
Finally, team efficacy is another important team process
construct. Team efficacy refers to team members’ percep-
tions of task-specific team competence (Gibson 1999). This
construct is thought to create a sense of confidence within
the team that enables the group to persevere when faced
with hardship (Gully et al. 2002). Several researchers have
related team efficacy to aspects of team effectiveness (e.g.,
Campion et al. 1993; Gibson 1999; Gibson et al. 2000),
and a meta-analysis demonstrated that the relationship
between team efficacy and performance was greater than
that between cohesion and performance (Gully et al. 2002).
Present Study
Our review of both the extant research as well as the
theoretical underpinnings highlights several limitations in
the literature to date. Specifically, the few studies exam-
ining diversity with respect to team member values has
largely focused on congruence across a set of context
specific values as opposed to a comprehensive set of more
broadly defined values such as that posited by Schwartz.
To date, no study has examined team member diversity
with respect to the full domain of values on both process
and performance criteria. Moreover, the literature offers
little guidance as to whether value diversity will lead to
positive or negative effects with respect to team processes
and outcomes, or to what extent these effects will be
contingent on the specific value dimensions examined.
Finally, while the literature examining the impact of sur-
face-level diversity has capitalized on the fact that these
characteristics are readily observable and therefore likely
to have immediate effects on team outcomes. It has been
argued, however, that diversity with respect to deep-level
characteristics such as values may take longer to manifest
itself with respect to team outcomes (e.g., Harrison et al.
1998). Therefore, it is not at all clear as to whether there
will be effects of value diversity in short-term novel
teams.
Thus, our goal in the present study is to explore the
relationship between values diversity and team effective-
ness. We provide a preliminary investigation of the impact
of basic values, on both process and task performance
aspects of team effectiveness. Toward this end, we seek to
address two research questions. Specifically:
(1) To what extent does diversity across team members
with respect to basic values impact team process and
outcome variables in a short-term team task with
novel teams?
(2) Will the impact of team member value diversity be
consistent with the optimistic or pessimistic views of
team diversity?
Method
Participants
Three hundred and six undergraduate college students at a
large U.S. southeastern university participated in the
present study. Participants were randomly assigned to
mixed gender teams of 4–6 (M = 4.6; mode = 5) resulting
in 60 teams. Participants were 43 % (134) male, and 78 %
(237) white. The mean age of participants was 22 years,
and the ages ranged from 19 to 38 years (SD = 2.32).
Task
The task was a complex team-based exercise called the
Chinese Bridge (Arciniega and Castañón 2002). The task is
a relatively difficult one requiring both the design and
building of a complex structure. Specifically, the task
requires each team to design and build a replica of a real
bridge, using 33 plastic pipes of three different sizes and 20
rubber bands (with instructions that all of the materials
must be used in the bridge). The task is designed such that,
given the material available, there is one optimal solution.
In addition, the task is designed so that even if a team were
given specific plans for the bridge, multiple people working
together are required to actually build the structure (e.g.,
one must hold pieces while another connects them, etc.).
Thus, successful completion requires team members to
work interdependently. This type of simulation could be
categorized as a high interdependence task, since team
members collectively work together to complete the task
while sharing information and resources (Horwitz and
Horwitz 2007). Based on a recent review of the literature
(Joshi and Roh 2008), these contextual factors combining
task interdependence and complexity, promote a scenario
where deep-level team diversity variables really emerge.
The simulation consists of four phases: (a) a multimedia
presentation describing the task and presenting a picture of
the real bridge; (b) a 20-min period for team members to
familiarize themselves with the materials; (c) a 30-min
period to sketch a proposed design of the bridge; and
(d) the building phase lasting approximately 60 min.
Measures
Values
Values were assessed using the Portrait Values Question-
naire (PVQ; Schwartz et al. 2001). The 40-item PVQ
measures the ten value types proposed by Schwartz (1992).
These values as measured by the PVQ are defined in the
Appendix. The scale includes short verbal portraits of
hypothetical individuals (e.g., Thinking up new ideas and
112 J Bus Psychol (2013) 28:107–121
123
being creative is important to him. He likes to do things in his
own original way). Respondents are asked to rate the extent
to which they agreed with each item on a scale from 1 (not
like me at all) to 6 (very much like me). The PVQ has been
used in several studies, in more than 60 countries, and has
been shown to be a reliable and valid measure of personal
values (Schwartz 1992; Schwartz et al. 2001). The average
alpha across these ten dimensions was .68 (SD = .08).
Here, it is important to note that previous studies utilizing
the PVQ during the last decade have tended to report rel-
atively low reliability estimates (particularly estimates of
internal consistency) for many of the value dimensions
(e.g., Aitken-Schermer et al. 2008; Fotopoulos et al. 2011;
Liem et al. 2011; Schwartz et al. 2001). Schwartz justifies
the low reliability of some of the value scales based on the
fact that many of these, have conceptually broad defini-
tions, assessing multiple components, rather than narrowly
defined constructs (Schwartz et al. 2001, pp. 531–532).
Consistent with this perspective, previous studies have
found clear effects between the value types and a plethora
of psychological variables under study despite relatively
low reliability estimates. Nonetheless, the tradition scale
demonstrated a particularly low alpha (.54) in the present
study; thus, it was not included in further analyses.
Task and Relationship Conflict
Conflict was measured using Jehn’s Intragroup Conflict
Scale (ICS; 1994). The scale contains four items related to
the task conflict dimension (e.g., There are differences of
opinion regarding the task in my work group), and four
items related to the relationship conflict dimension (e.g.,
There are personality clashes present in my work group).
Participants are asked to rate the extent to which they
agreed with each item on a scale from 1 (strongly disagree)
to 7 (strongly agree). Alphas for these scales were .85 (task
conflict) and .87 (relationship conflict).
Cohesion
Cohesion was measured using two items from Podsakoff and
MacKenzie’s (1994) Substitutes for Leadership Scale and
four items from Zaccaro (1991). Each of the 6 items consists
of a short statement regarding the cohesion of the individ-
ual’s team (e.g., I generally get along well with my fellow
group members). Respondents are asked to rate the extent to
which they agreed with each item on a scale from 1 (strongly
disagree) to 7 (strongly agree). Alpha for the scale was .75.
Team Efficacy
Team-efficacy was assessed with a 2-item scale con-
structed specifically for this study (e.g., My team works as
an effective unit and My team has an effective plan for
completing the bridge task). Respondents rated the extent
to which they agreed with each item using a 7-point scale
from 1 (strongly agree) to 7 (strongly disagree). Alpha for
this scale was .73.
Team Task Performance
Task performance was measured as the extent to which a
team was able to complete the task and the quality of the
finished product (i.e., the bridge replica). Following com-
pletion of the task, a photograph was taken of each team’s
bridge. Next, each photograph was evaluated by a group of
5 raters familiar with the task. Ratings were made on a
5-point scale from 1 (non-standing structure) to 5 (arched
bridge with 5 cross pieces and perfect joints). After each
member of the research group provided their initial rating,
the entire group came to consensus on a single rating for
the bridge (mean level of agreement in the initial ratings
across raters was 88 %). The consensus rating was used as
the measure of team performance.
Procedure
All participants completed the values measures during the
week preceding their participation in the bridge task. On
the day of the simulation, all participants first viewed the
task overview presentation as a group and then were bro-
ken up into their teams and assigned to separate team
rooms to complete the task. Teams completed each phase
of the bridge task, and then each participant individually
completed the team process measures.
Results
All analyses were conducted at the team-level. To assess
diversity, the variance across team members for each of the
ten value scales was computed (e.g., Barrick et al. 1998;
Mohammed and Angell 2003; Neuman et al. 1999).
Descriptive statistics and intercorrelations for the compo-
sition of team value types are shown in Table 1. Statistics
are presented for the team-level for mean composition of
values (control variables) and the average degree of within-
team variance in each value (diversity variables). Alpha
values for each value scale are also presented in Table 1.
Team process was assessed as the aggregate (mean) score
across team members on each of the process measures (task
and relationship conflict, cohesion, and team efficacy).
Descriptive statistics and intercorrelations for the team
process criterion variables are presented in Table 2.
Here, it is important to note that the use of these
aggregated variables as indicators of team level processes
J Bus Psychol (2013) 28:107–121 113
123
T
a
b
le
1
D
e
sc
ri
p
ti
v
e
st
a
ti
st
ic
s
&
in
te
rc
o
rr
e
la
ti
o
n
s
fo
r
te
a
m
le
v
e
l
m
e
a
n
s
&
te
a
m
le
v
e
l
v
a
ri
a
n
c
e
s
in
v
a
lu
e
v
a
ri
a
b
le
s
M
S
D
1
2
3
4
5
6
7
8
9
1
0
1
1
1
2
1
3
1
4
1
5
1
6
1
7
1
8
T
e
a
m
m
e
a
n
1
.
B
e
n
e
v
o
le
n
c
e
4
.6
7
.3
3
(.
6
6
)
2
.
U
n
iv
e
rs
a
li
sm
4
.1
6
.4
3
.5
0
(.
7
9
)
3
.
S
e
lf
-d
ir
e
c
ti
o
n
4
.5
3
.3
7
.2
6
.1
9
(.
6
4
)
4
.
S
ti
m
u
la
ti
o
n
4
.2
0
.4
8
.3
7
.3
3
.6
1
(.
6
9
)
5
.
H
e
d
o
n
is
m
4
.5
9
.4
8
.5
1
.1
6
.5
6
.6
5
(.
7
4
)
6
.
A
c
h
ie
v
e
m
e
n
t
4
.6
1
.4
5
.2
8
.0
6
.5
5
.6
6
.6
5
(.
8
0
)
7
.
P
o
w
e
r
3
.6
1
.5
1
.0
4
-
.1
1
.4
5
.4
0
.4
7
.5
9
(.
6
6
)
8
.
S
e
c
u
ri
ty
4
.3
5
.4
2
.3
9
.2
9
.4
3
.3
5
.4
7
.4
1
.2
1
(.
6
1
)
9
.
C
o
n
fo
rm
it
y
4
.3
1
.4
1
.3
8
.3
0
.1
0
.2
0
.2
7
.3
0
.1
1
.4
8
(.
6
5
)
T
e
a
m
v
a
ri
a
n
c
e
1
0
.
B
e
n
e
v
o
le
n
c
e
.5
0
.5
9
-
.2
9
-
.2
6
-
.2
0
-
.2
2
-
.3
3
-
.0
6
-
.0
3
-
.3
6
-
.3
5
1
1
.
U
n
iv
e
rs
a
li
sm
.6
5
.6
6
-
.0
4
-
.0
9
.0
0
-
.0
3
-
.0
4
.1
5
.1
0
-
.2
0
-
.2
8
.6
2
1
2
.
S
e
lf
-d
ir
e
c
ti
o
n
.5
2
.5
7
-
.2
6
-
.1
6
-
.3
1
-
.2
4
-
.3
8
-
.0
6
-
.0
5
-
.3
1
-
.1
5
.6
0
.6
1
1
3
.
S
ti
m
u
la
ti
o
n
.8
7
.5
2
.1
0
-
.0
7
-
.0
4
-
.1
4
.0
4
.0
3
-
.1
2
.1
2
-
.2
1
.2
6
.3
4
.4
0
1
4
.
H
e
d
o
n
is
m
.6
9
.7
6
-
.2
0
-
.0
8
-
.0
1
-
.1
0
-
.3
3
-
.0
5
-
.0
1
-
.1
6
-
.3
8
.7
2
.6
2
.5
9
.4
7
1
5
.
A
c
h
ie
v
e
m
e
n
t
.8
5
.9
3
-
.0
1
.0
7
-
.3
0
-
.3
8
-
.4
2
-
.4
1
-
.2
4
-
.2
1
-
.3
1
.5
0
.3
7
.4
1
.2
5
.6
3
1
6
.
P
o
w
e
r
.9
6
.8
1
.0
0
.0
4
-
.0
1
-
.1
3
-
.0
2
.0
0
.1
3
.1
5
.0
4
.2
5
.2
1
.2
4
.1
4
.3
0
.4
1
1
7
.
S
e
c
u
ri
ty
.5
3
.5
3
-
.2
7
-
.0
6
-
.1
3
-
.1
0
-
.2
9
-
.1
3
.0
4
-
.5
1
-
.2
9
.4
8
.3
7
.4
0
.0
7
.4
6
.3
8
.2
0
1
8
.
C
o
n
fo
rm
it
y
.7
1
.8
4
-
.1
0
.0
5
-
.1
5
-
.1
8
-
.2
8
-
.1
8
-
.2
9
-
.3
3
-
.4
5
.5
9
.4
7
.5
2
.5
1
.5
9
.5
3
.1
7
.4
8
N
o
te
.
‘T
e
a
m
M
e
a
n
’
st
a
ti
st
ic
s
re
p
re
se
n
ts
th
e
a
v
e
ra
g
e
v
a
ri
a
b
le
sc
o
re
a
m
o
n
g
te
a
m
m
e
m
b
e
rs
a
c
ro
ss
te
a
m
s
fo
r
e
a
c
h
v
a
lu
e
.
‘T
e
a
m
V
a
ri
a
n
c
e
’
st
a
ti
st
ic
s
re
p
re
se
n
t
th
e
a
v
e
ra
g
e
a
m
o
u
n
t
o
f
v
a
ri
a
n
c
e
in
e
a
c
h
v
a
ri
a
b
le
a
m
o
n
g
te
a
m
m
e
m
b
e
rs
a
c
ro
ss
a
ll
te
a
m
s.
N
=
6
0
.
S
c
a
le
le
v
e
l
a
lp
h
a
s
a
re
li
st
e
d
in
th
e
p
a
re
n
th
e
se
s
114 J Bus Psychol (2013) 28:107–121
123
requires sufficient agreement across team members to
warrant aggregation (James 1982; James et al. 1984). Thus,
before aggregating, we examined the level of agreement
across team members. To assess interrater reliability, we
calculated intraclass correlations coefficients (ICCs) where
ICC = (MSbetween - MSwithin)/MSbetween (Shrout and Fle-
iss 1979). The ICC estimates across all teammates were .62
for team efficacy, .77 for team cohesion, .78 for role con-
flict, and .85 for task conflict. Landis and Koch (1977)
suggest that interrater reliabilities above .61 should be
considered substantial level of agreement. Nonetheless,
recent research indicates that ICC’s tend to underestimate
levels of agreement (LeBretton et al. 2003). Thus, we also
calculated rwg(j) for each of these variables for each team
1
.
Results (included in Table 2) indicate adequate agreement
to justify aggregation, (i.e., overall mean rwg(j) was
approximately .85).
Next, we examined the zero-order correlations between
diversity in each of the value types and the five criterion
variables. These correlations (presented in Table 3) indi-
cate that team diversity with respect to seven of the values
is significantly related to one of the outcome variables, and
six of these are related to at least two of the four team
process variables. Task performance was not related to
diversity in any of the values. Previous research has indi-
cated that the team’s mean (across team members) level of
deep-level attributes is often a significant predictor of team
performance criteria (e.g., Barrick et al. 1998), and thus the
mean level may confound the amount of variance in these
attributes (Bedeian and Mossholder 2000). Consequently, it
is important to control for teams’ mean level (i.e., ‘‘team-
level’’) before interpreting the impact of variability. As
stated by Steiner (1972, p. 667), ‘‘a completely satisfactory
The Journal of Modern Craft Volume 1—Issue 1—March 2008, pp. 1.docx
The Journal of Modern Craft Volume 1—Issue 1—March 2008, pp. 1.docx
The Journal of Modern Craft Volume 1—Issue 1—March 2008, pp. 1.docx
The Journal of Modern Craft Volume 1—Issue 1—March 2008, pp. 1.docx
The Journal of Modern Craft Volume 1—Issue 1—March 2008, pp. 1.docx
The Journal of Modern Craft Volume 1—Issue 1—March 2008, pp. 1.docx
The Journal of Modern Craft Volume 1—Issue 1—March 2008, pp. 1.docx
The Journal of Modern Craft Volume 1—Issue 1—March 2008, pp. 1.docx
The Journal of Modern Craft Volume 1—Issue 1—March 2008, pp. 1.docx
The Journal of Modern Craft Volume 1—Issue 1—March 2008, pp. 1.docx
The Journal of Modern Craft Volume 1—Issue 1—March 2008, pp. 1.docx
The Journal of Modern Craft Volume 1—Issue 1—March 2008, pp. 1.docx
The Journal of Modern Craft Volume 1—Issue 1—March 2008, pp. 1.docx
The Journal of Modern Craft Volume 1—Issue 1—March 2008, pp. 1.docx
The Journal of Modern Craft Volume 1—Issue 1—March 2008, pp. 1.docx
The Journal of Modern Craft Volume 1—Issue 1—March 2008, pp. 1.docx
The Journal of Modern Craft Volume 1—Issue 1—March 2008, pp. 1.docx
The Journal of Modern Craft Volume 1—Issue 1—March 2008, pp. 1.docx
The Journal of Modern Craft Volume 1—Issue 1—March 2008, pp. 1.docx
The Journal of Modern Craft Volume 1—Issue 1—March 2008, pp. 1.docx
The Journal of Modern Craft Volume 1—Issue 1—March 2008, pp. 1.docx
The Journal of Modern Craft Volume 1—Issue 1—March 2008, pp. 1.docx
The Journal of Modern Craft Volume 1—Issue 1—March 2008, pp. 1.docx
The Journal of Modern Craft Volume 1—Issue 1—March 2008, pp. 1.docx
The Journal of Modern Craft Volume 1—Issue 1—March 2008, pp. 1.docx
The Journal of Modern Craft Volume 1—Issue 1—March 2008, pp. 1.docx
The Journal of Modern Craft Volume 1—Issue 1—March 2008, pp. 1.docx
The Journal of Modern Craft Volume 1—Issue 1—March 2008, pp. 1.docx
The Journal of Modern Craft Volume 1—Issue 1—March 2008, pp. 1.docx
The Journal of Modern Craft Volume 1—Issue 1—March 2008, pp. 1.docx
The Journal of Modern Craft Volume 1—Issue 1—March 2008, pp. 1.docx
The Journal of Modern Craft Volume 1—Issue 1—March 2008, pp. 1.docx
The Journal of Modern Craft Volume 1—Issue 1—March 2008, pp. 1.docx
The Journal of Modern Craft Volume 1—Issue 1—March 2008, pp. 1.docx
The Journal of Modern Craft Volume 1—Issue 1—March 2008, pp. 1.docx
The Journal of Modern Craft Volume 1—Issue 1—March 2008, pp. 1.docx
The Journal of Modern Craft Volume 1—Issue 1—March 2008, pp. 1.docx
The Journal of Modern Craft Volume 1—Issue 1—March 2008, pp. 1.docx
The Journal of Modern Craft Volume 1—Issue 1—March 2008, pp. 1.docx
The Journal of Modern Craft Volume 1—Issue 1—March 2008, pp. 1.docx
The Journal of Modern Craft Volume 1—Issue 1—March 2008, pp. 1.docx
The Journal of Modern Craft Volume 1—Issue 1—March 2008, pp. 1.docx
The Journal of Modern Craft Volume 1—Issue 1—March 2008, pp. 1.docx
The Journal of Modern Craft Volume 1—Issue 1—March 2008, pp. 1.docx
The Journal of Modern Craft Volume 1—Issue 1—March 2008, pp. 1.docx
The Journal of Modern Craft Volume 1—Issue 1—March 2008, pp. 1.docx
The Journal of Modern Craft Volume 1—Issue 1—March 2008, pp. 1.docx
The Journal of Modern Craft Volume 1—Issue 1—March 2008, pp. 1.docx
The Journal of Modern Craft Volume 1—Issue 1—March 2008, pp. 1.docx
The Journal of Modern Craft Volume 1—Issue 1—March 2008, pp. 1.docx
The Journal of Modern Craft Volume 1—Issue 1—March 2008, pp. 1.docx
The Journal of Modern Craft Volume 1—Issue 1—March 2008, pp. 1.docx
The Journal of Modern Craft Volume 1—Issue 1—March 2008, pp. 1.docx
The Journal of Modern Craft Volume 1—Issue 1—March 2008, pp. 1.docx
The Journal of Modern Craft Volume 1—Issue 1—March 2008, pp. 1.docx
The Journal of Modern Craft Volume 1—Issue 1—March 2008, pp. 1.docx
The Journal of Modern Craft Volume 1—Issue 1—March 2008, pp. 1.docx
The Journal of Modern Craft Volume 1—Issue 1—March 2008, pp. 1.docx
The Journal of Modern Craft Volume 1—Issue 1—March 2008, pp. 1.docx
The Journal of Modern Craft Volume 1—Issue 1—March 2008, pp. 1.docx
The Journal of Modern Craft Volume 1—Issue 1—March 2008, pp. 1.docx
The Journal of Modern Craft Volume 1—Issue 1—March 2008, pp. 1.docx
The Journal of Modern Craft Volume 1—Issue 1—March 2008, pp. 1.docx
The Journal of Modern Craft Volume 1—Issue 1—March 2008, pp. 1.docx
The Journal of Modern Craft Volume 1—Issue 1—March 2008, pp. 1.docx
The Journal of Modern Craft Volume 1—Issue 1—March 2008, pp. 1.docx
The Journal of Modern Craft Volume 1—Issue 1—March 2008, pp. 1.docx
The Journal of Modern Craft Volume 1—Issue 1—March 2008, pp. 1.docx
The Journal of Modern Craft Volume 1—Issue 1—March 2008, pp. 1.docx
The Journal of Modern Craft Volume 1—Issue 1—March 2008, pp. 1.docx
The Journal of Modern Craft Volume 1—Issue 1—March 2008, pp. 1.docx
The Journal of Modern Craft Volume 1—Issue 1—March 2008, pp. 1.docx
The Journal of Modern Craft Volume 1—Issue 1—March 2008, pp. 1.docx
The Journal of Modern Craft Volume 1—Issue 1—March 2008, pp. 1.docx
The Journal of Modern Craft Volume 1—Issue 1—March 2008, pp. 1.docx
The Journal of Modern Craft Volume 1—Issue 1—March 2008, pp. 1.docx
The Journal of Modern Craft Volume 1—Issue 1—March 2008, pp. 1.docx
The Journal of Modern Craft Volume 1—Issue 1—March 2008, pp. 1.docx
The Journal of Modern Craft Volume 1—Issue 1—March 2008, pp. 1.docx
The Journal of Modern Craft Volume 1—Issue 1—March 2008, pp. 1.docx
The Journal of Modern Craft Volume 1—Issue 1—March 2008, pp. 1.docx
The Journal of Modern Craft Volume 1—Issue 1—March 2008, pp. 1.docx
The Journal of Modern Craft Volume 1—Issue 1—March 2008, pp. 1.docx
The Journal of Modern Craft Volume 1—Issue 1—March 2008, pp. 1.docx
The Journal of Modern Craft Volume 1—Issue 1—March 2008, pp. 1.docx
The Journal of Modern Craft Volume 1—Issue 1—March 2008, pp. 1.docx
The Journal of Modern Craft Volume 1—Issue 1—March 2008, pp. 1.docx
The Journal of Modern Craft Volume 1—Issue 1—March 2008, pp. 1.docx
The Journal of Modern Craft Volume 1—Issue 1—March 2008, pp. 1.docx
The Journal of Modern Craft Volume 1—Issue 1—March 2008, pp. 1.docx
The Journal of Modern Craft Volume 1—Issue 1—March 2008, pp. 1.docx
The Journal of Modern Craft Volume 1—Issue 1—March 2008, pp. 1.docx
The Journal of Modern Craft Volume 1—Issue 1—March 2008, pp. 1.docx
The Journal of Modern Craft Volume 1—Issue 1—March 2008, pp. 1.docx
The Journal of Modern Craft Volume 1—Issue 1—March 2008, pp. 1.docx
The Journal of Modern Craft Volume 1—Issue 1—March 2008, pp. 1.docx
The Journal of Modern Craft Volume 1—Issue 1—March 2008, pp. 1.docx
The Journal of Modern Craft Volume 1—Issue 1—March 2008, pp. 1.docx
The Journal of Modern Craft Volume 1—Issue 1—March 2008, pp. 1.docx
The Journal of Modern Craft Volume 1—Issue 1—March 2008, pp. 1.docx
The Journal of Modern Craft Volume 1—Issue 1—March 2008, pp. 1.docx
The Journal of Modern Craft Volume 1—Issue 1—March 2008, pp. 1.docx
The Journal of Modern Craft Volume 1—Issue 1—March 2008, pp. 1.docx
The Journal of Modern Craft Volume 1—Issue 1—March 2008, pp. 1.docx
The Journal of Modern Craft Volume 1—Issue 1—March 2008, pp. 1.docx
The Journal of Modern Craft Volume 1—Issue 1—March 2008, pp. 1.docx
The Journal of Modern Craft Volume 1—Issue 1—March 2008, pp. 1.docx
The Journal of Modern Craft Volume 1—Issue 1—March 2008, pp. 1.docx
The Journal of Modern Craft Volume 1—Issue 1—March 2008, pp. 1.docx
The Journal of Modern Craft Volume 1—Issue 1—March 2008, pp. 1.docx
The Journal of Modern Craft Volume 1—Issue 1—March 2008, pp. 1.docx
The Journal of Modern Craft Volume 1—Issue 1—March 2008, pp. 1.docx
The Journal of Modern Craft Volume 1—Issue 1—March 2008, pp. 1.docx
The Journal of Modern Craft Volume 1—Issue 1—March 2008, pp. 1.docx
The Journal of Modern Craft Volume 1—Issue 1—March 2008, pp. 1.docx
The Journal of Modern Craft Volume 1—Issue 1—March 2008, pp. 1.docx
The Journal of Modern Craft Volume 1—Issue 1—March 2008, pp. 1.docx
The Journal of Modern Craft Volume 1—Issue 1—March 2008, pp. 1.docx
The Journal of Modern Craft Volume 1—Issue 1—March 2008, pp. 1.docx
The Journal of Modern Craft Volume 1—Issue 1—March 2008, pp. 1.docx
The Journal of Modern Craft Volume 1—Issue 1—March 2008, pp. 1.docx
The Journal of Modern Craft Volume 1—Issue 1—March 2008, pp. 1.docx
The Journal of Modern Craft Volume 1—Issue 1—March 2008, pp. 1.docx
The Journal of Modern Craft Volume 1—Issue 1—March 2008, pp. 1.docx
The Journal of Modern Craft Volume 1—Issue 1—March 2008, pp. 1.docx
The Journal of Modern Craft Volume 1—Issue 1—March 2008, pp. 1.docx
The Journal of Modern Craft Volume 1—Issue 1—March 2008, pp. 1.docx
The Journal of Modern Craft Volume 1—Issue 1—March 2008, pp. 1.docx
The Journal of Modern Craft Volume 1—Issue 1—March 2008, pp. 1.docx
The Journal of Modern Craft Volume 1—Issue 1—March 2008, pp. 1.docx
The Journal of Modern Craft Volume 1—Issue 1—March 2008, pp. 1.docx
The Journal of Modern Craft Volume 1—Issue 1—March 2008, pp. 1.docx
The Journal of Modern Craft Volume 1—Issue 1—March 2008, pp. 1.docx
The Journal of Modern Craft Volume 1—Issue 1—March 2008, pp. 1.docx
The Journal of Modern Craft Volume 1—Issue 1—March 2008, pp. 1.docx
The Journal of Modern Craft Volume 1—Issue 1—March 2008, pp. 1.docx
The Journal of Modern Craft Volume 1—Issue 1—March 2008, pp. 1.docx
The Journal of Modern Craft Volume 1—Issue 1—March 2008, pp. 1.docx
The Journal of Modern Craft Volume 1—Issue 1—March 2008, pp. 1.docx
The Journal of Modern Craft Volume 1—Issue 1—March 2008, pp. 1.docx
The Journal of Modern Craft Volume 1—Issue 1—March 2008, pp. 1.docx
The Journal of Modern Craft Volume 1—Issue 1—March 2008, pp. 1.docx
The Journal of Modern Craft Volume 1—Issue 1—March 2008, pp. 1.docx
The Journal of Modern Craft Volume 1—Issue 1—March 2008, pp. 1.docx
The Journal of Modern Craft Volume 1—Issue 1—March 2008, pp. 1.docx
The Journal of Modern Craft Volume 1—Issue 1—March 2008, pp. 1.docx
The Journal of Modern Craft Volume 1—Issue 1—March 2008, pp. 1.docx
The Journal of Modern Craft Volume 1—Issue 1—March 2008, pp. 1.docx
The Journal of Modern Craft Volume 1—Issue 1—March 2008, pp. 1.docx
The Journal of Modern Craft Volume 1—Issue 1—March 2008, pp. 1.docx
The Journal of Modern Craft Volume 1—Issue 1—March 2008, pp. 1.docx
The Journal of Modern Craft Volume 1—Issue 1—March 2008, pp. 1.docx
The Journal of Modern Craft Volume 1—Issue 1—March 2008, pp. 1.docx
The Journal of Modern Craft Volume 1—Issue 1—March 2008, pp. 1.docx
The Journal of Modern Craft Volume 1—Issue 1—March 2008, pp. 1.docx
The Journal of Modern Craft Volume 1—Issue 1—March 2008, pp. 1.docx
The Journal of Modern Craft Volume 1—Issue 1—March 2008, pp. 1.docx
The Journal of Modern Craft Volume 1—Issue 1—March 2008, pp. 1.docx

More Related Content

Similar to The Journal of Modern Craft Volume 1—Issue 1—March 2008, pp. 1.docx

The Trouble with (The Term) ArtAuthor(s) Carolyn DeanSour.docx
The Trouble with (The Term) ArtAuthor(s) Carolyn DeanSour.docxThe Trouble with (The Term) ArtAuthor(s) Carolyn DeanSour.docx
The Trouble with (The Term) ArtAuthor(s) Carolyn DeanSour.docxwsusan1
 
Constructivism architetural
Constructivism architeturalConstructivism architetural
Constructivism architeturalMayur Waghulde
 
How Art Works: Week 1 The ‘unruly discipline’
How Art Works: Week 1 The ‘unruly discipline’ How Art Works: Week 1 The ‘unruly discipline’
How Art Works: Week 1 The ‘unruly discipline’ DeborahJ
 
Krauss guarantee of the medium 2009
Krauss guarantee of the medium 2009Krauss guarantee of the medium 2009
Krauss guarantee of the medium 2009Heikki Nuutinen
 
Characteristics Of Lowbrow Art
Characteristics Of Lowbrow ArtCharacteristics Of Lowbrow Art
Characteristics Of Lowbrow ArtMonica Rivera
 
Avant-Garde Sculpture (I)
Avant-Garde Sculpture (I)Avant-Garde Sculpture (I)
Avant-Garde Sculpture (I)mfresnillo
 
Painting under glass technique and plastic dimensions
Painting under glass  technique and plastic dimensionsPainting under glass  technique and plastic dimensions
Painting under glass technique and plastic dimensionsAlexander Decker
 
Feminisms, Sexualities, And Queer Theory.docx
Feminisms, Sexualities, And Queer Theory.docxFeminisms, Sexualities, And Queer Theory.docx
Feminisms, Sexualities, And Queer Theory.docxjacobmwenda529
 
SHGC The Women’s Art Movement (Realism) Part 1
SHGC The Women’s Art Movement (Realism)   Part 1SHGC The Women’s Art Movement (Realism)   Part 1
SHGC The Women’s Art Movement (Realism) Part 1rachaelwhare
 
Rauschenberg, Robert (Milton Ernest)(b Port Arthur, TX, 22 Oct 1.docx
Rauschenberg, Robert (Milton Ernest)(b Port Arthur, TX, 22 Oct 1.docxRauschenberg, Robert (Milton Ernest)(b Port Arthur, TX, 22 Oct 1.docx
Rauschenberg, Robert (Milton Ernest)(b Port Arthur, TX, 22 Oct 1.docxcatheryncouper
 
Chapter 23 post-minimalism earth art and new imagists x
Chapter 23   post-minimalism earth art and new imagists xChapter 23   post-minimalism earth art and new imagists x
Chapter 23 post-minimalism earth art and new imagists xPetrutaLipan
 
ART HISTORY AS A THEORY OF ART
ART HISTORY AS A THEORY OF ARTART HISTORY AS A THEORY OF ART
ART HISTORY AS A THEORY OF ARTMaria Perkins
 
Chapter 15 american art before world war ii
Chapter 15   american art before world war iiChapter 15   american art before world war ii
Chapter 15 american art before world war iiPetrutaLipan
 

Similar to The Journal of Modern Craft Volume 1—Issue 1—March 2008, pp. 1.docx (17)

The Trouble with (The Term) ArtAuthor(s) Carolyn DeanSour.docx
The Trouble with (The Term) ArtAuthor(s) Carolyn DeanSour.docxThe Trouble with (The Term) ArtAuthor(s) Carolyn DeanSour.docx
The Trouble with (The Term) ArtAuthor(s) Carolyn DeanSour.docx
 
Contemporary Art Essay
Contemporary Art EssayContemporary Art Essay
Contemporary Art Essay
 
Constructivism architetural
Constructivism architeturalConstructivism architetural
Constructivism architetural
 
How Art Works: Week 1 The ‘unruly discipline’
How Art Works: Week 1 The ‘unruly discipline’ How Art Works: Week 1 The ‘unruly discipline’
How Art Works: Week 1 The ‘unruly discipline’
 
Form
FormForm
Form
 
Krauss guarantee of the medium 2009
Krauss guarantee of the medium 2009Krauss guarantee of the medium 2009
Krauss guarantee of the medium 2009
 
Characteristics Of Lowbrow Art
Characteristics Of Lowbrow ArtCharacteristics Of Lowbrow Art
Characteristics Of Lowbrow Art
 
Avant-Garde Sculpture (I)
Avant-Garde Sculpture (I)Avant-Garde Sculpture (I)
Avant-Garde Sculpture (I)
 
Painting under glass technique and plastic dimensions
Painting under glass  technique and plastic dimensionsPainting under glass  technique and plastic dimensions
Painting under glass technique and plastic dimensions
 
Feminisms, Sexualities, And Queer Theory.docx
Feminisms, Sexualities, And Queer Theory.docxFeminisms, Sexualities, And Queer Theory.docx
Feminisms, Sexualities, And Queer Theory.docx
 
Moma 1991 0098_71
Moma 1991 0098_71Moma 1991 0098_71
Moma 1991 0098_71
 
SHGC The Women’s Art Movement (Realism) Part 1
SHGC The Women’s Art Movement (Realism)   Part 1SHGC The Women’s Art Movement (Realism)   Part 1
SHGC The Women’s Art Movement (Realism) Part 1
 
Rauschenberg, Robert (Milton Ernest)(b Port Arthur, TX, 22 Oct 1.docx
Rauschenberg, Robert (Milton Ernest)(b Port Arthur, TX, 22 Oct 1.docxRauschenberg, Robert (Milton Ernest)(b Port Arthur, TX, 22 Oct 1.docx
Rauschenberg, Robert (Milton Ernest)(b Port Arthur, TX, 22 Oct 1.docx
 
Chapter 23 post-minimalism earth art and new imagists x
Chapter 23   post-minimalism earth art and new imagists xChapter 23   post-minimalism earth art and new imagists x
Chapter 23 post-minimalism earth art and new imagists x
 
ART HISTORY AS A THEORY OF ART
ART HISTORY AS A THEORY OF ARTART HISTORY AS A THEORY OF ART
ART HISTORY AS A THEORY OF ART
 
Art noveau
Art noveauArt noveau
Art noveau
 
Chapter 15 american art before world war ii
Chapter 15   american art before world war iiChapter 15   american art before world war ii
Chapter 15 american art before world war ii
 

More from oreo10

All scientific theories must be able to make testable predictions. S.docx
All scientific theories must be able to make testable predictions. S.docxAll scientific theories must be able to make testable predictions. S.docx
All scientific theories must be able to make testable predictions. S.docxoreo10
 
All I wnat is to write a reflection paper on my project which is hac.docx
All I wnat is to write a reflection paper on my project which is hac.docxAll I wnat is to write a reflection paper on my project which is hac.docx
All I wnat is to write a reflection paper on my project which is hac.docxoreo10
 
Alice,Betty, and Carol are playing a game with 48 marbles in a circl.docx
Alice,Betty, and Carol are playing a game with 48 marbles in a circl.docxAlice,Betty, and Carol are playing a game with 48 marbles in a circl.docx
Alice,Betty, and Carol are playing a game with 48 marbles in a circl.docxoreo10
 
All healthcare organizations must convert to an Electronic Health Re.docx
All healthcare organizations must convert to an Electronic Health Re.docxAll healthcare organizations must convert to an Electronic Health Re.docx
All healthcare organizations must convert to an Electronic Health Re.docxoreo10
 
All round writer onlyThis is an individual Mediation assignment..docx
All round writer onlyThis is an individual Mediation assignment..docxAll round writer onlyThis is an individual Mediation assignment..docx
All round writer onlyThis is an individual Mediation assignment..docxoreo10
 
Alice was wondering whether it was a good idea to invest her money i.docx
Alice was wondering whether it was a good idea to invest her money i.docxAlice was wondering whether it was a good idea to invest her money i.docx
Alice was wondering whether it was a good idea to invest her money i.docxoreo10
 
All organisms have DNA, which differs only in the number and order o.docx
All organisms have DNA, which differs only in the number and order o.docxAll organisms have DNA, which differs only in the number and order o.docx
All organisms have DNA, which differs only in the number and order o.docxoreo10
 
All literature involves some kind of performance which is intended f.docx
All literature involves some kind of performance which is intended f.docxAll literature involves some kind of performance which is intended f.docx
All literature involves some kind of performance which is intended f.docxoreo10
 
All key elements of the assignment are covered in a substantiv.docx
All key elements of the assignment are covered in a substantiv.docxAll key elements of the assignment are covered in a substantiv.docx
All key elements of the assignment are covered in a substantiv.docxoreo10
 
Alice, Betty and Carol are playing a game with 48 marbles in a circl.docx
Alice, Betty and Carol are playing a game with 48 marbles in a circl.docxAlice, Betty and Carol are playing a game with 48 marbles in a circl.docx
Alice, Betty and Carol are playing a game with 48 marbles in a circl.docxoreo10
 
Alice Jones was employed as a clerk-typist by a company. She request.docx
Alice Jones was employed as a clerk-typist by a company. She request.docxAlice Jones was employed as a clerk-typist by a company. She request.docx
Alice Jones was employed as a clerk-typist by a company. She request.docxoreo10
 
Air and Water Pollution PaperAir and water pollutants exist in m.docx
Air and Water Pollution PaperAir and water pollutants exist in m.docxAir and Water Pollution PaperAir and water pollutants exist in m.docx
Air and Water Pollution PaperAir and water pollutants exist in m.docxoreo10
 
Air pollution is an environmental health problem in many cities thro.docx
Air pollution is an environmental health problem in many cities thro.docxAir pollution is an environmental health problem in many cities thro.docx
Air pollution is an environmental health problem in many cities thro.docxoreo10
 
After your topic has been approved, the next step is to research.docx
After your topic has been approved, the next step is to research.docxAfter your topic has been approved, the next step is to research.docx
After your topic has been approved, the next step is to research.docxoreo10
 
After watching three of the five movie clips listed in the Multime.docx
After watching three of the five movie clips listed in the Multime.docxAfter watching three of the five movie clips listed in the Multime.docx
After watching three of the five movie clips listed in the Multime.docxoreo10
 
Aging and Disability WorksheetPart IIdentify 2 or .docx
Aging and Disability WorksheetPart IIdentify 2 or .docxAging and Disability WorksheetPart IIdentify 2 or .docx
Aging and Disability WorksheetPart IIdentify 2 or .docxoreo10
 
After watching the video and reading the Web Resource, CDC Autism .docx
After watching the video and reading the Web Resource, CDC Autism .docxAfter watching the video and reading the Web Resource, CDC Autism .docx
After watching the video and reading the Web Resource, CDC Autism .docxoreo10
 
AI Artificial Intelligence1Reading responsePeter .docx
AI Artificial Intelligence1Reading responsePeter .docxAI Artificial Intelligence1Reading responsePeter .docx
AI Artificial Intelligence1Reading responsePeter .docxoreo10
 
Agree or disagree with, and discuss the following statement Corp.docx
Agree or disagree with, and discuss the following statement Corp.docxAgree or disagree with, and discuss the following statement Corp.docx
Agree or disagree with, and discuss the following statement Corp.docxoreo10
 
After watching Reactions to an Impending Death Sentence and Ti.docx
After watching Reactions to an Impending Death Sentence and Ti.docxAfter watching Reactions to an Impending Death Sentence and Ti.docx
After watching Reactions to an Impending Death Sentence and Ti.docxoreo10
 

More from oreo10 (20)

All scientific theories must be able to make testable predictions. S.docx
All scientific theories must be able to make testable predictions. S.docxAll scientific theories must be able to make testable predictions. S.docx
All scientific theories must be able to make testable predictions. S.docx
 
All I wnat is to write a reflection paper on my project which is hac.docx
All I wnat is to write a reflection paper on my project which is hac.docxAll I wnat is to write a reflection paper on my project which is hac.docx
All I wnat is to write a reflection paper on my project which is hac.docx
 
Alice,Betty, and Carol are playing a game with 48 marbles in a circl.docx
Alice,Betty, and Carol are playing a game with 48 marbles in a circl.docxAlice,Betty, and Carol are playing a game with 48 marbles in a circl.docx
Alice,Betty, and Carol are playing a game with 48 marbles in a circl.docx
 
All healthcare organizations must convert to an Electronic Health Re.docx
All healthcare organizations must convert to an Electronic Health Re.docxAll healthcare organizations must convert to an Electronic Health Re.docx
All healthcare organizations must convert to an Electronic Health Re.docx
 
All round writer onlyThis is an individual Mediation assignment..docx
All round writer onlyThis is an individual Mediation assignment..docxAll round writer onlyThis is an individual Mediation assignment..docx
All round writer onlyThis is an individual Mediation assignment..docx
 
Alice was wondering whether it was a good idea to invest her money i.docx
Alice was wondering whether it was a good idea to invest her money i.docxAlice was wondering whether it was a good idea to invest her money i.docx
Alice was wondering whether it was a good idea to invest her money i.docx
 
All organisms have DNA, which differs only in the number and order o.docx
All organisms have DNA, which differs only in the number and order o.docxAll organisms have DNA, which differs only in the number and order o.docx
All organisms have DNA, which differs only in the number and order o.docx
 
All literature involves some kind of performance which is intended f.docx
All literature involves some kind of performance which is intended f.docxAll literature involves some kind of performance which is intended f.docx
All literature involves some kind of performance which is intended f.docx
 
All key elements of the assignment are covered in a substantiv.docx
All key elements of the assignment are covered in a substantiv.docxAll key elements of the assignment are covered in a substantiv.docx
All key elements of the assignment are covered in a substantiv.docx
 
Alice, Betty and Carol are playing a game with 48 marbles in a circl.docx
Alice, Betty and Carol are playing a game with 48 marbles in a circl.docxAlice, Betty and Carol are playing a game with 48 marbles in a circl.docx
Alice, Betty and Carol are playing a game with 48 marbles in a circl.docx
 
Alice Jones was employed as a clerk-typist by a company. She request.docx
Alice Jones was employed as a clerk-typist by a company. She request.docxAlice Jones was employed as a clerk-typist by a company. She request.docx
Alice Jones was employed as a clerk-typist by a company. She request.docx
 
Air and Water Pollution PaperAir and water pollutants exist in m.docx
Air and Water Pollution PaperAir and water pollutants exist in m.docxAir and Water Pollution PaperAir and water pollutants exist in m.docx
Air and Water Pollution PaperAir and water pollutants exist in m.docx
 
Air pollution is an environmental health problem in many cities thro.docx
Air pollution is an environmental health problem in many cities thro.docxAir pollution is an environmental health problem in many cities thro.docx
Air pollution is an environmental health problem in many cities thro.docx
 
After your topic has been approved, the next step is to research.docx
After your topic has been approved, the next step is to research.docxAfter your topic has been approved, the next step is to research.docx
After your topic has been approved, the next step is to research.docx
 
After watching three of the five movie clips listed in the Multime.docx
After watching three of the five movie clips listed in the Multime.docxAfter watching three of the five movie clips listed in the Multime.docx
After watching three of the five movie clips listed in the Multime.docx
 
Aging and Disability WorksheetPart IIdentify 2 or .docx
Aging and Disability WorksheetPart IIdentify 2 or .docxAging and Disability WorksheetPart IIdentify 2 or .docx
Aging and Disability WorksheetPart IIdentify 2 or .docx
 
After watching the video and reading the Web Resource, CDC Autism .docx
After watching the video and reading the Web Resource, CDC Autism .docxAfter watching the video and reading the Web Resource, CDC Autism .docx
After watching the video and reading the Web Resource, CDC Autism .docx
 
AI Artificial Intelligence1Reading responsePeter .docx
AI Artificial Intelligence1Reading responsePeter .docxAI Artificial Intelligence1Reading responsePeter .docx
AI Artificial Intelligence1Reading responsePeter .docx
 
Agree or disagree with, and discuss the following statement Corp.docx
Agree or disagree with, and discuss the following statement Corp.docxAgree or disagree with, and discuss the following statement Corp.docx
Agree or disagree with, and discuss the following statement Corp.docx
 
After watching Reactions to an Impending Death Sentence and Ti.docx
After watching Reactions to an Impending Death Sentence and Ti.docxAfter watching Reactions to an Impending Death Sentence and Ti.docx
After watching Reactions to an Impending Death Sentence and Ti.docx
 

Recently uploaded

Beyond the EU: DORA and NIS 2 Directive's Global Impact
Beyond the EU: DORA and NIS 2 Directive's Global ImpactBeyond the EU: DORA and NIS 2 Directive's Global Impact
Beyond the EU: DORA and NIS 2 Directive's Global ImpactPECB
 
Student login on Anyboli platform.helpin
Student login on Anyboli platform.helpinStudent login on Anyboli platform.helpin
Student login on Anyboli platform.helpinRaunakKeshri1
 
General AI for Medical Educators April 2024
General AI for Medical Educators April 2024General AI for Medical Educators April 2024
General AI for Medical Educators April 2024Janet Corral
 
IGNOU MSCCFT and PGDCFT Exam Question Pattern: MCFT003 Counselling and Family...
IGNOU MSCCFT and PGDCFT Exam Question Pattern: MCFT003 Counselling and Family...IGNOU MSCCFT and PGDCFT Exam Question Pattern: MCFT003 Counselling and Family...
IGNOU MSCCFT and PGDCFT Exam Question Pattern: MCFT003 Counselling and Family...PsychoTech Services
 
Software Engineering Methodologies (overview)
Software Engineering Methodologies (overview)Software Engineering Methodologies (overview)
Software Engineering Methodologies (overview)eniolaolutunde
 
Measures of Central Tendency: Mean, Median and Mode
Measures of Central Tendency: Mean, Median and ModeMeasures of Central Tendency: Mean, Median and Mode
Measures of Central Tendency: Mean, Median and ModeThiyagu K
 
9548086042 for call girls in Indira Nagar with room service
9548086042  for call girls in Indira Nagar  with room service9548086042  for call girls in Indira Nagar  with room service
9548086042 for call girls in Indira Nagar with room servicediscovermytutordmt
 
Q4-W6-Restating Informational Text Grade 3
Q4-W6-Restating Informational Text Grade 3Q4-W6-Restating Informational Text Grade 3
Q4-W6-Restating Informational Text Grade 3JemimahLaneBuaron
 
1029-Danh muc Sach Giao Khoa khoi 6.pdf
1029-Danh muc Sach Giao Khoa khoi  6.pdf1029-Danh muc Sach Giao Khoa khoi  6.pdf
1029-Danh muc Sach Giao Khoa khoi 6.pdfQucHHunhnh
 
Interactive Powerpoint_How to Master effective communication
Interactive Powerpoint_How to Master effective communicationInteractive Powerpoint_How to Master effective communication
Interactive Powerpoint_How to Master effective communicationnomboosow
 
BAG TECHNIQUE Bag technique-a tool making use of public health bag through wh...
BAG TECHNIQUE Bag technique-a tool making use of public health bag through wh...BAG TECHNIQUE Bag technique-a tool making use of public health bag through wh...
BAG TECHNIQUE Bag technique-a tool making use of public health bag through wh...Sapna Thakur
 
Holdier Curriculum Vitae (April 2024).pdf
Holdier Curriculum Vitae (April 2024).pdfHoldier Curriculum Vitae (April 2024).pdf
Holdier Curriculum Vitae (April 2024).pdfagholdier
 
BASLIQ CURRENT LOOKBOOK LOOKBOOK(1) (1).pdf
BASLIQ CURRENT LOOKBOOK  LOOKBOOK(1) (1).pdfBASLIQ CURRENT LOOKBOOK  LOOKBOOK(1) (1).pdf
BASLIQ CURRENT LOOKBOOK LOOKBOOK(1) (1).pdfSoniaTolstoy
 
Russian Escort Service in Delhi 11k Hotel Foreigner Russian Call Girls in Delhi
Russian Escort Service in Delhi 11k Hotel Foreigner Russian Call Girls in DelhiRussian Escort Service in Delhi 11k Hotel Foreigner Russian Call Girls in Delhi
Russian Escort Service in Delhi 11k Hotel Foreigner Russian Call Girls in Delhikauryashika82
 
Advanced Views - Calendar View in Odoo 17
Advanced Views - Calendar View in Odoo 17Advanced Views - Calendar View in Odoo 17
Advanced Views - Calendar View in Odoo 17Celine George
 
fourth grading exam for kindergarten in writing
fourth grading exam for kindergarten in writingfourth grading exam for kindergarten in writing
fourth grading exam for kindergarten in writingTeacherCyreneCayanan
 
Sports & Fitness Value Added Course FY..
Sports & Fitness Value Added Course FY..Sports & Fitness Value Added Course FY..
Sports & Fitness Value Added Course FY..Disha Kariya
 
The Most Excellent Way | 1 Corinthians 13
The Most Excellent Way | 1 Corinthians 13The Most Excellent Way | 1 Corinthians 13
The Most Excellent Way | 1 Corinthians 13Steve Thomason
 

Recently uploaded (20)

Beyond the EU: DORA and NIS 2 Directive's Global Impact
Beyond the EU: DORA and NIS 2 Directive's Global ImpactBeyond the EU: DORA and NIS 2 Directive's Global Impact
Beyond the EU: DORA and NIS 2 Directive's Global Impact
 
Student login on Anyboli platform.helpin
Student login on Anyboli platform.helpinStudent login on Anyboli platform.helpin
Student login on Anyboli platform.helpin
 
General AI for Medical Educators April 2024
General AI for Medical Educators April 2024General AI for Medical Educators April 2024
General AI for Medical Educators April 2024
 
IGNOU MSCCFT and PGDCFT Exam Question Pattern: MCFT003 Counselling and Family...
IGNOU MSCCFT and PGDCFT Exam Question Pattern: MCFT003 Counselling and Family...IGNOU MSCCFT and PGDCFT Exam Question Pattern: MCFT003 Counselling and Family...
IGNOU MSCCFT and PGDCFT Exam Question Pattern: MCFT003 Counselling and Family...
 
Software Engineering Methodologies (overview)
Software Engineering Methodologies (overview)Software Engineering Methodologies (overview)
Software Engineering Methodologies (overview)
 
Measures of Central Tendency: Mean, Median and Mode
Measures of Central Tendency: Mean, Median and ModeMeasures of Central Tendency: Mean, Median and Mode
Measures of Central Tendency: Mean, Median and Mode
 
9548086042 for call girls in Indira Nagar with room service
9548086042  for call girls in Indira Nagar  with room service9548086042  for call girls in Indira Nagar  with room service
9548086042 for call girls in Indira Nagar with room service
 
Mattingly "AI & Prompt Design: Structured Data, Assistants, & RAG"
Mattingly "AI & Prompt Design: Structured Data, Assistants, & RAG"Mattingly "AI & Prompt Design: Structured Data, Assistants, & RAG"
Mattingly "AI & Prompt Design: Structured Data, Assistants, & RAG"
 
Q4-W6-Restating Informational Text Grade 3
Q4-W6-Restating Informational Text Grade 3Q4-W6-Restating Informational Text Grade 3
Q4-W6-Restating Informational Text Grade 3
 
1029-Danh muc Sach Giao Khoa khoi 6.pdf
1029-Danh muc Sach Giao Khoa khoi  6.pdf1029-Danh muc Sach Giao Khoa khoi  6.pdf
1029-Danh muc Sach Giao Khoa khoi 6.pdf
 
Interactive Powerpoint_How to Master effective communication
Interactive Powerpoint_How to Master effective communicationInteractive Powerpoint_How to Master effective communication
Interactive Powerpoint_How to Master effective communication
 
BAG TECHNIQUE Bag technique-a tool making use of public health bag through wh...
BAG TECHNIQUE Bag technique-a tool making use of public health bag through wh...BAG TECHNIQUE Bag technique-a tool making use of public health bag through wh...
BAG TECHNIQUE Bag technique-a tool making use of public health bag through wh...
 
Holdier Curriculum Vitae (April 2024).pdf
Holdier Curriculum Vitae (April 2024).pdfHoldier Curriculum Vitae (April 2024).pdf
Holdier Curriculum Vitae (April 2024).pdf
 
BASLIQ CURRENT LOOKBOOK LOOKBOOK(1) (1).pdf
BASLIQ CURRENT LOOKBOOK  LOOKBOOK(1) (1).pdfBASLIQ CURRENT LOOKBOOK  LOOKBOOK(1) (1).pdf
BASLIQ CURRENT LOOKBOOK LOOKBOOK(1) (1).pdf
 
Russian Escort Service in Delhi 11k Hotel Foreigner Russian Call Girls in Delhi
Russian Escort Service in Delhi 11k Hotel Foreigner Russian Call Girls in DelhiRussian Escort Service in Delhi 11k Hotel Foreigner Russian Call Girls in Delhi
Russian Escort Service in Delhi 11k Hotel Foreigner Russian Call Girls in Delhi
 
Advanced Views - Calendar View in Odoo 17
Advanced Views - Calendar View in Odoo 17Advanced Views - Calendar View in Odoo 17
Advanced Views - Calendar View in Odoo 17
 
fourth grading exam for kindergarten in writing
fourth grading exam for kindergarten in writingfourth grading exam for kindergarten in writing
fourth grading exam for kindergarten in writing
 
Sports & Fitness Value Added Course FY..
Sports & Fitness Value Added Course FY..Sports & Fitness Value Added Course FY..
Sports & Fitness Value Added Course FY..
 
Código Creativo y Arte de Software | Unidad 1
Código Creativo y Arte de Software | Unidad 1Código Creativo y Arte de Software | Unidad 1
Código Creativo y Arte de Software | Unidad 1
 
The Most Excellent Way | 1 Corinthians 13
The Most Excellent Way | 1 Corinthians 13The Most Excellent Way | 1 Corinthians 13
The Most Excellent Way | 1 Corinthians 13
 

The Journal of Modern Craft Volume 1—Issue 1—March 2008, pp. 1.docx

  • 1. The Journal of Modern Craft Volume 1—Issue 1—March 2008, pp. 13–34 Fiber Art and the Hierarchy of Art and Craft, 1960–80 Elissa Auther Elissa Auther is Assistant Professor of Contemporary Art at the University of Colorado at Colorado Springs. Her essay for the Journal of Modern Craft is derived from her book manuscript about the innovative use of fi ber across the art world in the 1960s and 1970s. Recent publications about the position of craft under modernism include “The Decorative, Abstraction and the Hierarchy of Art and Craft in the Art Criticism of Clement Greenberg,” Oxford Art Journal 27(3) (December 2004) and “Andy Warhol, Wallpaper and Contemporary Installation Art,” for the forthcoming edited collection Extra/Ordinary: Craft Culture and Contemporary Art. Abstract “Fiber Art and the Hierarchy of Art and Craft, 1960– 1980” both explores the artistic, historical, institutional and extra-aesthetic forces affecting the formation of the fi ber movement and evaluates the curatorial strategies of Mildred Constantine and Jack Lenor Larsen to negotiate the hierarchy of art and craft in order to elevate fi ber as a medium of “high art.” The analysis considers the emergence of the category of fi ber art in the 1960s and 1970s, the cultural contexts in which fi ber or textiles were utilized in the period, strategies of transcending the hierarchy of art and craft, and other relations
  • 2. of dominance and subordination that defi ned fi ber’s marginality in the hierarchy of the arts and shaped the responses of artists, critics and curators to aesthetic boundaries. Keywords: fi ber, craft, feminism, decorative, hierarchy, modernism, Mildred Constantine, weaving. The Journal of Modern Craft Volume I—Issue I March 2008 pp. 13–34 Reprints available directly from the publishers Photocopying permitted by licence only © Berg 2008 14 Fiber Art and the Hierarchy of Art and Craft, 1960–1980 Elissa Auther The Journal of Modern Craft Volume 1—Issue 1—March 2008, pp. 13–34 In 1972, Mildred Constantine—a former curator of architecture and design at the Museum of Modern Art (MoMA), New York—reproduced Adams’s Construction in Beyond Craft, the fi rst in-depth study of
  • 3. the emerging fi ber art movement.1 This important text, co-authored with textile designer Jack Lenor Larsen, chronicled the movement’s evolution, defi ned its aesthetic priorities and defended work made of fi ber as “fi ne art.” In 1963, Adams’s unorthodox woven works had been included in New York’s Museum of Contemporary Crafts’ exhibition Woven Forms (Figure 3), a show that Constantine and Larsen’s study singled out as groundbreaking.2 Signifi cantly, critic Lucy Lippard also exhibited Adams’s Construction in her eclectic, “post-minimalist” show Eccentric Abstraction, at the Fischbach Gallery in New York in 1966 (Figure 4). Unlike Beyond Craft, Eccentric Abstraction situated Adams’s Construction among avant-garde work by such emerging artists as Eva Hesse, Bruce Nauman and Keith Sonnier. The show’s works utilized a variety of non-traditional, fl exible media, including wire mesh, vinyl, cloth and rope. Saret exhibited Untitled in 1970 in New York’s Sidney Janis Gallery’s January group show String and Rope (Figure 5), alongside works by Christo, Fred Sandback, Bruce Nauman and Robert Morris. Arts Magazine, one of the era’s major fi ne-art periodicals, reproduced Saret’s piece and a critic writing for the Christian Science Monitor noted the use of fi ber as an autonomous abstract element. The “artists included,” he wrote, “use string or rope (or thread), not as line,
  • 4. but as falling, tangling, stretching, or coiling matter” (Andreae 1970: 58).3 Fig 1 Alan Saret, Untitled, 1968, rope and wire, variable dimensions. Alan Saret’s Untitled (1968) (Figure 1), a work of rope and wire and Alice Adams’s Construction (1966) (Figure 2), of rope and steel cable, share signifi cant formal similarities. Both works are fl oor based, of similar size and shape, and both utilize materials associated with “craft,” hand labor or industry. Indeed, one could conclude that the same artist made both works. But this is not the case, and the two artists were associated with very different artistic circles in the 1960s: Saret was an anti-form sculptor, whereas Adams was associated with what came to be known as the fi ber art movement. Moreover, the works were exhibited and received very differently. Comparing the varied reception of these two similar objects reveals not only fi ber’s arrival as a new medium of “high art” but also how this elevation of fi ber issued from multiple sites or positions, each with a distinct location within the complex network of power relations governed by the application of the term “craft” in the United States in the 1960s and 1970s. Elissa Auther Fiber Art and the Hierarchy of Art and Craft, 1960–1980 15
  • 5. The Journal of Modern Craft Volume 1—Issue 1—March 2008, pp. 13–34 Studies like Beyond Craft (1972), exhibitions such as Woven Forms (1963), Eccentric Abstraction (1966) and String and Rope (1969), and works in fi ber like Saret’s and Adams’s are representative of a number of projects in the 1960s and 1970s that signaled fi ber’s potential as a fi ne-art medium and illustrate different relations to the art world’s center of power. In each case, fi ber, that is, craft—typically dismissed or even invisible as a force shaping the art world in this period—was in actuality central to its constitution. Beyond Craft and Woven Forms attempted to elevate fi ber from the realm of “craft” to that of “art” and were undertaken by individuals and institutions dedicated to legitimating new work in materials traditional to craft. The goal of legitimating so-called fi ber art as “Art” sets these projects apart from exhibitions such as Eccentric Abstraction and String and Rope, which attempted to capture sculpture’s latest vanguard. Here craft functioned as a conceptual limit, essential to the elevation of art—in the words of Fig 2 Alice Adams, Construction, 1966, rope and steel cable. Robert Mates photographer. Courtesy of the artist.
  • 6. 16 Fiber Art and the Hierarchy of Art and Craft, 1960–1980 Elissa Auther The Journal of Modern Craft Volume 1—Issue 1—March 2008, pp. 13–34 Fig 3 Installation view of work by Lenore Tawney in Woven Forms, Museum of Contemporary Craft, New York, 1963. Ferdinand Boesch photographer. Courtesy of the American Craft Council Library/Museum of Contemporary Craft Archives. Fig 4 Installation view of Lucy Lippard’s Eccentric Abstraction at the Fischbach Gallery, New York, 1966. Rudy Burckhardt photographer. Courtesy of the Fischbach Gallery records, 1954–1978. Archives of American Art, Smithsonian Institution. Glenn Adamson, as “a border that can never be reached, but is nonetheless intrinsic to any sense of position” (Adamson 2007: 2). Eccentric Abstraction and String and Rope were both produced by members of the art world whose authority in itself legitimated the work in question, a privilege not fully extended to Constantine and Larsen. Rather than focusing on whether Saret’s Untitled was a work of art, these exhibitions theorized the artistic use of fi ber and fi ber-like materials in relation to previous examples of non-
  • 7. traditional media in sculpture and to other Fig 5 String and Rope, Installation view, Sidney Janis Gallery, New York, 1969. Photographer Geoffrey Clements. Elissa Auther Fiber Art and the Hierarchy of Art and Craft, 1960–1980 17 The Journal of Modern Craft Volume 1—Issue 1—March 2008, pp. 13–34 conditions, such as craft, that oppose the defi nition of art. Despite these differences in the projects’ orientations and goals, they all probed fi ber’s symbolic power, paradoxically generated by its subordination in the history of art through deep-rooted associations with utility or craft, a phenomenon Saret’s and Adams’s works also illuminate. In challenging the subordination of fi ber as a medium of craft or of primarily utilitarian value, Adams’s and Saret’s works additionally illustrate the role fi ber played in testing the art world’s aesthetic boundaries in the 1960s and 1970s. Yet, with the exception of Adams’s Construction, which circulated widely, the status of “high art” remained elusive for the objects that Constantine and Larsen featured in Beyond Craft. In fact, fi ber art in this period was typically viewed as neither art nor craft, but as between the two categories, thwarting
  • 8. the works’ potential to undermine the hierarchy of media responsible for fi ber’s low aesthetic status. This article examines the efforts of Mildred Constantine—the movement’s principal architect and supporter—and her collaborator Jack Lenor Larsen to eliminate this ambiguity, securing fi ber’s identity as a medium of high art. Their strategy was defi ned by the goal of assimilating fi ber-based work to the fi ne arts and did not address the fact that the boundary separating art from craft is constructed rather than natural. Although this strategy led to an unfortunate collusion with the very hierarchy of media they sought to transform and in hindsight might seem obviously fl awed, my analysis suggests that the options available to them as defenders of fi ber art were tremendously constrained by the period’s artistic discourse defi ning the work of art, particularly its autonomy from social contexts and practices outside the art world with which fi ber was intimately connected. The reasons surrounding the art world’s resistance to fi ber art were complex and varied, involving the cultural connotations of fi ber, popular trends in fi ber crafts and gender bias deriving from fi ber’s association with women and the domestic realm; with such factors working against their project, perhaps no curatorial strategy Constantine and Larsen could have adopted at the time would have fared better.
  • 9. The Aesthetic Status of Fiber in the 1960s and 1970s In 1961, with Lenore Tawney’s solo exhibition at the Staten Island Museum, fi ber art as it would be recognized in the following two decades made its public debut in a fi ne-art context. The show consisted of forty works produced between 1955 and 1961 in a technique now referred to as open-warp weave: a structure in which large parts of the warp are left unwoven. The reception of this and subsequent work demonstrates that from the start fi ber artists experienced considerable resistance from the realms of both craft and fi ne art for the way their work violated conventions of both practices. Tawney recalled that her early “open-warp weaves . . . caused quite a controversy [amongst weavers]. No one had done this kind of weaving ... It’s against the rules and those people who go by the rules were against it” (Tawney 1978: n.p.). However, posing a challenge to the defi nition of weaving in the 1960s did not automatically confer the status of “art” on Tawney’s work; in the “high” art world too, her work’s identity was unstable. For instance, 18 Fiber Art and the Hierarchy of Art and Craft, 1960–1980 Elissa Auther The Journal of Modern Craft Volume 1—Issue 1—March 2008, pp. 13–34
  • 10. the small catalog for Tawney’s 1961 exhibition reveals that on the one hand the show was sponsored by the “Section of Handcrafts” but elsewhere attempts to position Tawney as a “fi ne artist.” In particular, the appreciation written by Tawney’s close friend, painter Agnes Martin, defends her artistic identity, praising the work’s “originality” and its relation to larger art-world trends toward exploring new media. Before Tawney’s Staten Island Museum show was even mounted, the artist had started using her open-warp method to combine different weave structures in a single work by dividing and redividing the warp as the weave was in progress. The resulting works, which she called “woven forms” (see Figure 3), were monumental in scale, departed from the rectilinear shape of loom-woven fabrics and were not even remotely utilitarian. Given their size, which ranged from 11 to 27 feet (3.3 to 8.1 m) in height, their abstraction (like some of her earlier open-warp weaves, the “woven forms” have no pictorial content) and Tawney’s method of hanging them from the ceiling, her work sharply departed from the conventions of the “decorative wall hanging,” the category in which weaves without everyday utility were normally classifi ed in the 1960s. Yet this work too fell between accepted divisions between art and craft. For instance,
  • 11. Tawney’s 1962 solo exhibition at the Art Institute of Chicago was installed in the Textiles Department and Woven Forms, the 1963 group show at the Museum of Contemporary Crafts that included her work, also institutionally situated her weaving as craft. Even so, a reviewer for the New York Times remarked that Tawney “is more than just a weaver—she is also an artist. Unfortunately, craft work has for many years implied to the general public work done by amateurs . . . with little merit . . . Miss Tawney’s craft is in marked contrast to this mistaken concept and her woven forms are considered by experts to be works of fi ne art” (O’Brien 1963). This reviewer’s willingness to embrace Tawney’s work as “fi ne art” against prevailing sentiment that connected craft media to the amateur or hobbyist, however, was more the exception than the rule. Subsequent reviews continued to question her work’s artistic status. As late as 1990, on the occasion of Tawney’s retrospective at the American Craft Museum, Roberta Smith drew a negative conclusion in her review for the New York Times, leaving the impression that the intervening years had done little to resolve questions about the works’ status and identity within the art world: “Mrs. Tawney’s work exists in a limbo that is endemic to much contemporary craft: it has departed from craft and function without quite arriving at art . . . Handsome and impressive
  • 12. as her best efforts often are . . . [m]ost of them sustain comparison neither to such achievements in weaving as Navajo blankets, nor to contemporary painting and sculpture” (Smith 1990). Smith’s evaluation presents another facet of the picture that often consigned Tawney’s work to an art world “limbo,” the artist’s refusal of the utilitarian associations of craft. Instead of exploring this aspect of her work, however, Smith uses Tawney’s departure from utility or function to reassert the differences between craft (Navajo weaving) and art (painting and sculpture) that the woven forms actually complicated. Elissa Auther Fiber Art and the Hierarchy of Art and Craft, 1960–1980 19 The Journal of Modern Craft Volume 1—Issue 1—March 2008, pp. 13–34 These equivocations over the value of Tawney’s woven work indicate the institutional obstacles artists, critics and curators of fi ber art faced in trying to legitimate their work or the genre of fi ber art. Examining Constantine and Larsen’s efforts to consecrate fi ber art provides an opportunity to look more systematically at the questions of artistic legitimacy that Tawney’s work provoked.
  • 13. Wall Hangings The fi rst major American exhibition of the new genre of fi ber art was MoMA’s Wall Hangings (Figures 6 and 7), co-curated by Constantine and, at her invitation, Larsen. With this exhibition, the two established themselves as the fi ber movement’s leading experts and also set the canon of vanguard fi ber art (or “Art Fabric,” as they called it). Wall Hangings presented to the American museum-going public the fi rst international survey of primarily large-scale, abstract woven and off-loom work in fi ber. Planned by Constantine since 1966, the exhibition toured eleven cities in 1968, returning in early 1969 to MoMA, where it was installed, at Constantine’s insistence, in the museum’s fi rst-fl oor special exhibition galleries, rather than in the Department of Architecture and Design. To the American fi ber world’s leading triumvirate of Lenore Tawney, Sheila Hicks (Figure 8) and Claire Zeisler, Constantine and Larsen added Kay Sekimachi (Figure 9), Walter Nottingham and Ed Rossbach, among others. Some of the exhibition’s most advanced work came from abroad, including that of the Swiss Françoise Grossen, Yugoslavian (Croatian) Jagoda Buic, and Poles Magdalena Abakanowicz and Wojcieh Sadley, whose woven forms radically departed from the conventions of tapestry as practiced in Europe at the time. Regrettably, given the exhibition’s importance in the history of the American
  • 14. fi ber movement, the only national art-world press Wall Hangings received was a review that Craft Horizons commissioned from Fig 6 Wall Hangings, Installation view, Museum of Modern Art, New York, 1969. 20 Fiber Art and the Hierarchy of Art and Craft, 1960–1980 Elissa Auther The Journal of Modern Craft Volume 1—Issue 1—March 2008, pp. 13–34 Fig 7 Wall Hangings, Installation view, Museum of Modern Art, New York, 1969. Fig 8 Sheila Hicks, The Principal Wife Goes On, 1977, silk, linen, and wool elements ca. 180 in. long, Smithsonian American Art Museum, Gift of S.C. Johnson & Son, Inc. sculptor Louise Bourgeois.4 Bourgeois’s response to the show contrasts strikingly with the curatorial statement written by Constantine and Larsen for the exhibition catalog. Whereas they asserted: “During the last ten years, developments in weaving have caused us to revise our concepts of this craft and to view the work within the context of twentieth-century art” (1969: n.p.), Bourgeois concluded:
  • 15. The pieces in the show rarely liberate themselves from decoration . . . A painting or a sculpture makes great demand on the onlooker at the same time that it is independent of him. These weaves, delightful as they are, seem more engaging and less demanding. If they must be classifi ed, they would fall somewhere between fi ne and applied art. (Bourgeois 1969: 33f) These opposing evaluations speak to the period’s symbolic boundaries between Elissa Auther Fiber Art and the Hierarchy of Art and Craft, 1960–1980 21 The Journal of Modern Craft Volume 1—Issue 1—March 2008, pp. 13–34 art and non-art and such boundaries’ centrality to maintaining a hierarchy of media separating fi ne art from craft. They also point to a set of real and conceptual obstacles heavily borne by fi ber artists and their supporters, who sought to transcend the aesthetic boundaries structuring the art world in the 1960s and 1970s. It is clear that although Bourgeois disagreed with Constantine and Larsen about the works’ value, the question of their identity preoccupied them all. This preoccupation continued to burden Constantine and Larsen
  • 16. and subsequent scholars of fi ber art, who took up issues of category over the nature of the work itself. Bourgeois also exhibits concern about categories of art, which she strategically upheld in this review by using fi ber art to explain what painting and sculpture were not. Bourgeois’s evaluation of the works in Wall Hangings as “decoration” traded upon the distinction between art and craft, reasserting the aesthetic boundaries fi ber artists and their supporters sought to overcome.5 In her words, the objects were “delightful” and “engaging.” In the writing of modernist critics such as Clement Greenberg, to be either was to succumb to the decorative by making “immediately pleasing” what true artists achieved through rigorous intellectual struggle and risk-taking (Greenberg 1945; 1986: 41). Bourgeois reasserts the high-art status of painting and sculpture by claiming that these genres place a “demand” upon the viewer absent in fi ber art. The opposition she reinforces—that between the merely attractive object and that which requires sustained attention—is central to the hierarchy of art and craft, which associates art with the cognitive realm, craft with mere surface effect. In an interview, Constantine related her reaction to Bourgeois’s review of Wall Hangings: “I was furious,” she reported. “It represented exactly the attitude we were trying to work against” (conversation with
  • 17. the author 1999). Despite their awareness of the negative art-world attitudes concerning Fig 9 Kay Sekimachi, Nagare VII, 1970, woven monofi lament, 80 × 9 × 9 in. Smithsonian American Art Museum. 22 Fiber Art and the Hierarchy of Art and Craft, 1960–1980 Elissa Auther The Journal of Modern Craft Volume 1—Issue 1—March 2008, pp. 13–34 media traditionally associated with craft, Constantine and her co-curator attempted to assert fi ber’s art status by introducing the new genre into the fi ne art world on the terms set by that world. This strategy required that they adhere to the dominant philosophy of art voiced by Bourgeois and that they collude in maintaining the hierarchy of media responsible for fi ber’s general exclusion from “high art.” Two factors in particular made this strategy meaningful to Constantine and Larsen, factors that were themselves effects of the hierarchy of art and craft and indicate its self-perpetuating nature. The most important such factor was that contesting the hierarchy of art and craft rather than assimilating select objects (those without utility, for instance) into the category of “high art” would have been to destabilize
  • 18. the very artistic status they claimed for fi ber artists. The identity of fi ber artists, because they worked in a medium traditional to craft, required authorization different from that bestowed upon artists working securely within the “high art” world. The necessity of establishing one’s art as art was a handicap that belonged only to the artist working in craft media. An excellent example of the nearly automatic security afforded the artist with a position within the “high art” world is demonstrated by the critic Hilton Kramer’s review of Lucy Lippard’s 1966 exhibition Eccentric Abstraction for the New York Times. About the show, which featured work that radically fl outed sculptural conventions of the time, Kramer wrote: “[although the] work is neither painting nor exactly sculpture . . . art—of some sort—it is” (1966). The lack of legitimacy afforded an exhibition such as Wall Hangings illustrates that this basic security was not extended to fi ber artists in the 1960s and 1970s. A second factor informing Constantine and Larsen’s approach was the fact that overtly challenging the hierarchy of art and craft might also undermine their authority as curators, with negative consequences for fi ber art’s legitimation. Constantine, like others of her generation at MoMA (she joined the staff in 1948), was committed to the museum’s mission of identifying and collecting modern works of the highest achievement in art and design, a goal that
  • 19. included examples of applied art or craft to the extent such objects conformed to modernist norms of innovation and abstraction. Anni Albers’s solo exhibition in 1949, MoMA’s fi rst exhibition of weaving, exemplifi es this approach to craft and design. Albers’s association with the Bauhaus, her commitment to experimentation and her adaptation of non-objective form to weaving all refl ected the museum’s vision of the modern in art. In the 1960s, exemplary objects of craft and design were still subject to this form of evaluation, which helped to shape Constantine’s curatorial vision for Wall Hangings. An anecdote of Constantine’s about her experience as a curator while MoMA was directed by René d’Harnoncourt is revealing in this regard. Sometime in the mid-1960s a hand-thrown vessel by the celebrated Japanese potter Kitaoji Rosanjin was under consideration for acquisition. In Constantine’s account, d’Harnoncourt put this rhetorical question to his curators at the acquisitions meeting: “Do you know when a pot is no longer a pot but a work of art?” (in conversation with the author).6 To Constantine, such a question affi rmed Elissa Auther Fiber Art and the Hierarchy of Art and Craft, 1960–1980 23 The Journal of Modern Craft Volume 1—Issue 1—March 2008,
  • 20. pp. 13–34 that the distinction to be found between art and craft resided in the object rather than being culturally projected onto it. Such an assumption underscores Constantine and Larsen’s confi dence regarding the art status they asserted for the objects exhibited in Wall Hangings. The story also foregrounds the authority that rested in the curator (in the capacity of his or her “eye”) to discern quality in objects of cultural signifi cance—that is, to distinguish between good and bad, “high” and “low,” “fi ne art” and mere “craft.”7 A theory of boundary maintenance in the arts that pointed to the role of extra-aesthetic factors such as gender, different contexts of production, or cultural presumptions about craft in the production of distinctions of value had the potential to undermine this curatorial and ultimately institutional form of power in the 1960s. To the extent that the authorization of an object as art is only as good (that is, as convincing) as the authority of the authorizer, undercutting the myth of the “good eye,” could result in the forfeiture of institutional validation required by a new genre or movement. A straightforward way of approaching the conception of value that Constantine and Larsen used—a conception of value as intrinsic to an object—is to consider it alongside the recontextualization of
  • 21. non-Western objects from ethnographic specimens to works of art, a taxonomic shift to which Constantine and Larsen’s conception is related. Recent anthropological and art historical considerations of shifts in classifying objects previously recognized for their ethnographic value demonstrate that museum strategies of acquisition and display that elide issues of context, technique and utility in favor of disinterested contemplation of an object according to a modernist theory of art are premised upon art’s autonomy from the social realm.8 As Mary Anne Staniszewski has shown in her study of the history of exhibitions at MoMA, the type of installation now considered standard in fi ne- art museums—in which works are spotlit, arranged at eye level on a neutral-colored wall and widely spaced—emerged in the US in the 1930s.9 This type of installation, which “facilitated appreciation of the singular artwork,” was also applied to the exhibition of ethnic “artifacts” in museums in the 1930s and 1940s, refl ecting a growing aesthetic appreciation of non-Western objects. In general, the aestheticized display of ethnographic objects suppressed issues of utility and highlighted form, creating an atemporal, formalist viewing experience unencumbered by the social formations that gave rise to the work. As scholars of exhibition history have pointed out, this reconceptualization confi rms the aesthetic categories and assumptions of the West’s “high art,” leaving intact the hierarchy of
  • 22. the arts and stripping those aspects of non-Western objects in confl ict with that hierarchy’s values. Constantine, who assisted d’Harnoncourt in a major exhibition that employed this strategy—1954’s Ancient Art of the Andes—was familiar with such a curatorial strategy and it undoubtedly infl uenced the conception of Wall Hangings and subsequent fi ber-art projects. As installation photographs of the show suggest (see Figures 6 and 7), Constantine and Larsen hung the work according to a method of displaying painting and sculpture calculated to enhance the objects’ visual impact and autonomy. 24 Fiber Art and the Hierarchy of Art and Craft, 1960–1980 Elissa Auther The Journal of Modern Craft Volume 1—Issue 1—March 2008, pp. 13–34 Although such installation was by then typical at MoMA for painting and sculpture, its adoption in Wall Hangings can be seen as strategic given fi ber’s lack of autonomy— that is, its extensive use in cultural and practical contexts outside the art world. The exhibition displayed works in relative isolation within the austere, white-walled special exhibition galleries. This strategy was carried over to the catalog, which lacked detailed information about technique and
  • 23. enhanced the works’ formal qualities by using photographs in which each object was suspended in space against a dark ground, a classic mid-century method of photographing ethnic objects that privileged form over implied utility.10 Constantine and Larsen’s approach to the consecration of fi ber art has a logic rooted in a belief in formalism as a democratic, legitimating discourse capable of transforming any work into an object of pure, aesthetic contemplation. As the negative reception of Wall Hangings suggests, however, applying a formalist exhibition strategy did not resolve the hierarchy of art and craft Constantine and Larsen sought to transcend. An important factor at play here is the way fi ber’s “low” aesthetic status derived in part from its extensive use in cultural and practical contexts outside the art world. Briefl y examining these contexts illuminates the larger challenge that Constantine and Larsen faced in attempting to elevate fi ber as a medium of “high art.” Utility and Amateurism Outside the “high” art world, fi ber gained a new visibility in the United States in the 1960s and 1970s with revivals of the traditional crafts of hand weaving, quilting, embroidery, dyeing, knotting and basketry. The social and artistic contexts and practices surrounding these revivals included the back-to-the-land and hippie movements,
  • 24. the renewed interest in folk art around the American Bicentennial, trends in the personalization of clothing like the adoption of African dress by African Americans, the feminist recuperation of women’s history, the revival of traditional arts of minority communities in the South and Southwest, and the popular craze of macramé. Increased funding from the National Endowment for the Arts to support regional arts and a burgeoning commercial market for craft and folk art bolstered interest in such work.11 These contexts and practices demonstrate both the richness of what ought to be defi ned as a major craft revival in the United States in the 1960s and 1970s and the diffi culty, for fi ber artists, of distinguishing their work from this nebulous conglomeration. The frequent connection made between fi ber art and macramé—an association encouraged by fi ber artists’ extensive use of off-loom techniques such as knotting, looping, linking and plaiting—shows why fi ber artists strove to distinguish their work from popular craft. The term macramé, which denotes a form of lateral knotting probably Arabic in origin, referred in the late 1960s and 1970s to a genre of useful objects (jewelry, belts, handbags, lampshades, plant holders and hammocks, for instance) produced using “decorative” knotting techniques. Macramé’s enormous popularity is attested to by the fact that fi ber artist and macramé specialist Mary Walker Phillip’s best-selling book Step-
  • 25. by-Step Macramé (1970) had sold more than a million copies by 1976. In 1971, macramé’s Elissa Auther Fiber Art and the Hierarchy of Art and Craft, 1960–1980 25 The Journal of Modern Craft Volume 1—Issue 1—March 2008, pp. 13–34 mass cultural appeal even led the Museum of American Folk Art in New York to organize a major exhibition on the subject as part of its NEA-funded series, Rediscovery of Grass Roots America.12 Unusually, this exhibition placed contemporary macramé within the larger history of knotted-fabric construction, bringing together, among other examples, seventeenth- and eighteenth-century European lace, knotted fabrics and jewelry of North American Indians, the decorative knotted work of sailors from the nineteenth and twentieth centuries, and the large- scale knotted work of fi ber artist Françoise Grossen, which emphasized the fi ber art- macramé connection. The macramé hobby craze was problematic for the fi ber movement because it reinforced assumptions about fi ber as a woman’s medium or of “low” art status. In her 1979 interview for Arts Magazine, Claire Zeisler (Figure 10) took advantage of a question about technique to distinguish her knotting (often in macramé) from the
  • 26. “decorative” knots used by “macramé artists,” suggesting the sensitivity of the topic for artists working in fi ber. J. PATRICE MARANDEL: In your own mind, do techniques such as knotting or cutting . . . set you apart from other fi ber artists? CLAIRE ZEISLER: When I fi rst started knotting, it was not a trend . . . You certainly have heard the word macramé. Some people referred to my work as Claire Zeisler’s macramé. That’s when I hit the ceiling . . . I do mind the word macramé because macramé today means a decorative knot and I use my knotting technique as structure . . . The knot becomes the base for the piece, like the canvas is the base for a painting. (Arts Magazine 1979: 151f) Zeisler’s insistence on distinguishing between her work and popular craft underscores the degree to which macramé had become a cultural phenomenon that impinged upon the fi ber movement’s bid for status as art. It is likely that Zeisler’s discussion of technique in the pages of Arts Magazine—then a leading periodical of contemporary art that rarely dedicated copy to work in traditional craft media—was a deliberate attempt to persuade readers that work in fi ber shared important features with art rather than the popular hobby of macramé. Her reference to knotting as integral to the “structure” of her
  • 27. pieces emphasizes her art’s formalist nature, as does her explicit parallel between her work and painting. The latter comparison places her work fi rmly in the category of “high art” by invoking its pre-eminent medium to highlight not craft-oriented technique but art-oriented practice. Zeisler’s rhetorical strategy was similar to that adopted by numerous fi ber artists and their supporters who were work ing to change the place of fi ber in the art world’s traditional hierarchy, dismissing its con nections to activities classifi ed as “non-art.” This effort was actually initiated some years earlier by Anni Albers, who in 1940 entered into a debate over the function and value of hand weaving with Mary Atwater, America’s leading spokesperson for the non-professional weaver, in the pages of The Weaver, a nationally distributed quarterly for the American hand-weaving community. Atwater and Albers’s confl ict over the meaning and role of textiles not only helps to demonstrate how deep the association of 26 Fiber Art and the Hierarchy of Art and Craft, 1960–1980 Elissa Auther The Journal of Modern Craft Volume 1—Issue 1—March 2008, pp. 13–34 Fig 10 Claire Zeisler, Coil Series III—A Celebration, 1978, natural hemp and wool, 66½ × 34 in.
  • 28. Smithsonian American Art Museum, Museum Purchase. Elissa Auther Fiber Art and the Hierarchy of Art and Craft, 1960–1980 27 The Journal of Modern Craft Volume 1—Issue 1—March 2008, pp. 13–34 amateurism with fi ber runs, but also points to another serious hurdle fi ber artists (working on or off the loom) faced in defi ning their work as art in the 1960s and 1970s—fi ber’s gendered associations. Atwater, perhaps best known as the founder of the Shuttle Craft Guild and its correspondence course in weaving in 1920, advocated a view of weaving as a leisure-time or therapeutic activity. She also researched nineteenth-century weaving traditions, publishing her fi ndings alone or in publications for hand weavers. Her research was instrumental to the survival of these historical and regional practices, but her practical how-to approach ran counter to the idea of weaving as an art form. As Ed Rossbach put it, “She told Americans what to weave, how to weave, [and] what to do with their weavings” (Rossbach 1983: 22).13 Unlike Atwater, Albers regarded herself as an artist and was particularly outspoken regarding hand weaving’s potential to move beyond a leisure pursuit with utilitarian
  • 29. imperatives. She encapsulated her views in a statement from 1959 that implored, “let threads be articulate ... and fi nd a form for themselves to no other end than their own orchestration, not to be sat on, walked on, only to be looked at” (Albers 1959: 5). Albers’s article for The Weaver commented upon the state of contemporary hand weaving in the United States and illustrated textiles produced by her students at Black Mountain College in North Carolina.14 The publication coincided with the height of the Appalachian craft revival, in which weaving played a major role, providing the immediate context for her sharp critique of “recipes” and “traditional formulas, which once proved successful” (Albers 1940: 3). Albers argued that “such work is often no more than a romantic attempt to recall a temps perdu” and that it refl ected the state of “isolation” and “degeneration” of US hand weaving in the period (Albers 1940: 3). Albers called for a return to “fundamentals” and free experimentation on and off the loom to foster innovation and she suggested that the resulting development of new forms in fi ber could be art. Atwater, who had no interest in the imperatives of industry, modern design, or art, responded to Albers with the predictably titled piece “It’s Pretty—But is it Art?” which she published in her advice column for The
  • 30. Weaver.15 Not surprisingly, Atwater took offense at Albers’s criticisms of faithfulness to tradition and reproduction of historical patterns (or “recipes,” as Atwater called them). Atwater found the idea that a textile might lack utility or be considered art preposterous and she asserted that fi ber and weaving were essentially useful and of value primarily as an “escape from the distresses or the hum-drum detail of our daily lives” (Atwater 1941: 13). Atwater responded to Albers’s emphasis upon experimentation, imagination and the creation of new forms (key elements of modernism in the arts) with similar disdain, defending the right of her readers (mostly women isolated at home with little access to artistic training) to draw aesthetic pleasure from weaving regardless of their level of expertise or whether they relied upon a pattern. Atwater’s acceptance, even promotion, of her audience’s amateurism raises the issue of fi ber’s connections to femininity and domesticity, associations that plagued fi ber artists in the 1960s and 1970s. 28 Fiber Art and the Hierarchy of Art and Craft, 1960–1980 Elissa Auther The Journal of Modern Craft Volume 1—Issue 1—March 2008, pp. 13–34 Femininity and Domesticity As Rozsika Parker and Griselda Pollock
  • 31. asserted over twenty-fi ve years ago in their groundbreaking study Old Mistresses: Women, Art and Ideology, “the sex of the artist matters. It conditions the way art is seen and discussed” (Parker and Pollock 1981: 50). This is nowhere more evident in the late twentieth century than in the history of the fi ber movement, whose status and reception was affected by weaving’s near-universal association with women and the domestic realm. At its worst, the effort to challenge the gendering of fi ber as “women’s work” actually reinforced its association with femininity and its low place in the art-world hierarchy. In the late 1960s, when off-loom techniques rose to prominence, the work of fi ber artists adopting these techniques was interpreted as free from the craft tradition’s conventional values by virtue of their rejection of the loom. Critics such as Rose Slivka posited an antithesis between loom and off-loom construction techniques, praising the latter’s superior artistic quality. In the provocatively titled piece “Hard String,” her review of the 1972 group exhibition Sculpture in Fiber for Craft Horizons, Slivka characterized the loom as an impotent instrument, a gendering of technology that had the unintended effect (as did the title of the show) of associating off-loom fi ber art with the perceived masculine virility of modern art.16 Not surprisingly, given this context, the housewife is a key fi gure in critical
  • 32. considerations of fi ber art, where she signifi es amateurism and lack of creativity. In criticism about fi ber art, this set of associations often had to be overcome before the writer could consider a work of fi ber art worthy of discursive attention. Fiber’s problematic association with the domestic accounts for the distinctly confessional tone or hedging found in fi ber- art criticism that invokes the fi gure of the housewife. Katherine Kuh’s article about the work of Lenore Tawney and Claire Zeisler for Saturday Review in 1968, “Sculpture: Woven and Knotted,” is typical. Despite her eventual enthusiasm, Kuh opens the article by admitting her own negative assumptions about weaving, shared, presumably, by her readers: “Until recently, I always considered weaving a ladylike pursuit for frustrated housewives, but I am drastically changing my mind. The best weavers are, to be sure, still women, but some of them are also fi rst-rate artists” (Kuh 1968: 36). The use of the qualifi cation “also,” as in “also fi rst-rate artists,” echoes George O’Brien’s language in his review of Tawney’s work in the 1963 exhibition Woven Forms cited earlier. In that piece, Tawney is “more than a weaver—she is also an artist,” a qualifi cation that O’Brien used later in the review to question craft’s amateur associations. In Kuh’s assessment, the amateur associations of craft are now infl ected by gender, yet another barrier to participation in the art world in this period faced by fi ber artists—a problem shared
  • 33. by female artists generally, in fact. Critic Gregory Battcock’s review of Claire Zeisler’s show at the Richard Feigen gallery in 1969 provides another example of how the image of the housewife was used to great effect in backhanded compliments during the 1960s: Mops, fl oppiness and house-wifey dumpiness might distract the viewer, but only momentarily. The colors are certainly more arresting than the Sheriff Elissa Auther Fiber Art and the Hierarchy of Art and Craft, 1960–1980 29 The Journal of Modern Craft Volume 1—Issue 1—March 2008, pp. 13–34 in Darien Conn. and the general tone is more elegant than The Hilton Inn. Zeisler’s sculptures emphasize texture at the expense of form and since texture is emphasized in just the right way, it’s O.K. (Battcock 1969: 65) For Kuh the housewife is capable but frustrated, while for Battcock she is dumpy— differences that might have to do with the differing gender of the two critics or the audience for whom they were writing. In any case, Battcock’s idea of the housewife and the comparison of Zeisler’s work to mops remove fi ber from even the sphere of craft, downgrading it to drudgery. Ultimately,
  • 34. Kuh’s and Battcock’s language permits notice of Zeisler’s work only to the extent that their own critical authority regarding the evaluation of art is assured. This brief examination of fi ber’s extra- aesthetic associations sheds additional light on Constantine and Larsen’s approach to elevating fi ber in a manner that completely elided its history as a craft and its presence in numerous contemporary non-art contexts. Alternate approaches proved no more successful and further demonstrate the severity of the constraints they faced in championing a material historically categorized and marginalized as craft. For instance, hybrid categories that could bridge the divide between “art” and “craft,” presumably better accommodating fi ber art, also faced serious resistance in the art world. Two such categories were “soft art” and “soft sculpture.” Both functioned as organizing rubrics for several shows in the late 1960s and 1970s such as the New Jersey State Museum’s Soft Art (1969), curated by Ralph Pomeroy; Lucy Lippard’s traveling exhibition for the American Federation of the Arts, Soft and Apparently Soft Sculpture (1968–9); and the New York Cultural Center’s traveling exhibition, Softness as Art (1973). Soft Art included work by Tawney as well as Richard Tuttle, Robert Morris, Eva Hesse and Claes Oldenburg. Softness as Art included one work by Françoise Grossen, amongst that of
  • 35. Jackie Ferrara, Harmony Hammond, Richard Serra, Robert Morris and Hannah Wilke. Fiber artists recognized the potential of the categories “soft art” or “soft sculpture” to bridge the divide separating art from craft. In a 1970 article for Craft Horizons, “When Will Weaving Be an Art Form?” Virginia Hoffman observed that “soft sculpture” could logically include any three- dimensional form made by fl exible joinings, fi brous materials, modules with no fi xed beginning or end, soft materials made hard and vice versa . . . One thinks of . . . works by Eva Hesse, Alan Saret, Robert Morris, [and] Alice Adams. (Hoffman 1970: 18)17 However, art critics were not as enthusiastic about the soft art phenomenon. Some went to great lengths to reassert boundaries between genres and materials that the rubric blurred. In his Artforum review of the New York Cultural Center’s exhibition, Soft as Art, James Collins asserted, “one of the things artists shouldn’t do today is to make art with anything soft” (Collins 1973: 89). Most problematic for him was the category itself, which “denies criticism the luxury of a single critical framework” (Collins 1973: 89). His response to this dilemma was to divide the work in the show into four categories: “process,” “revamped painting,” “craft/fetish,” and “novelty art.” In his review these categories redrew boundaries distinguishing
  • 36. 30 Fiber Art and the Hierarchy of Art and Craft, 1960–1980 Elissa Auther The Journal of Modern Craft Volume 1—Issue 1—March 2008, pp. 13–34 “art” from “craft” through a separation of works in fi ber into either the “process” category or the “craft/fetish” category. In the “process” category, Collins singled out a felt work by Robert Morris (along with a work by Richard Serra) as worthy of attention for its “theoretical underpinnings” (Collins 1973: 89). The rest of the work in the exhibition was “neither experientially nor theoretically interesting” (Collins 1973: 90). In the “craft/ fetish” category, Collins placed the work of Françoise Grossen, Jackie Ferrara and Brenda Miller, all of whom exhibited work in fi ber. Anticipating readers’ objections to his application of the term craft to the work of three female artists working in rope and other forms of fi ber, Collins asserted that “labeling a work as craft orientated isn’t an attack on women” (Collins 1973: 90). He continued: [it] is only to say they give the impression of manual over mental dexterity and people who are manually dextrous aren’t necessarily interesting artists . . . Both Jackie Ferrara with her Four Balls II made of cotton bailing, rope and chains and Brenda Miller’s Abscissa, consisting of a numbering
  • 37. system dictating the structure of a twine wall piece just by the use and association of their materials suggest a “grass skirt” reference—a gender neutral one. (Collins 1973: 90) That the term “grass skirt” in this context is not gender neutral hardly requires elucidation. By rejecting the hybrid category “soft art” and reinforcing fi ber’s association to femininity and primitiveness, Collins neatly maintains the boundary between art and craft. Given the degree to which fi ber’s associations to craft overwhelm Collins’s response to Ferrara and Miller’s work, one can imagine how easily the hybrid character of work by fi ber artists such as Grossen, Sheila Hicks, or Ed Rossbach could be dismissed as illegitimate. Thanks to such challenging associations, Constantine and Larsen’s goal of elevating fi ber art, in Wall Hangings and subsequent projects, was diffi cult if not impossible to accomplish in the 1960s and 1970s. In addition, their acceptance of the division between “high” and “low” art—and, one might argue, their refusal to engage with the very non-art connotations of fi ber that were so problematic for their goals—left art-world prejudices intact. Despite Constantine and Larsen’s efforts, the work of Alan Saret—a male artist who had worked previously in other media and was part of the “high” art world—was critically received as art, while
  • 38. Alice Adams’s remarkably similar work— emerging from her previous practice as a weaver—was viewed with skepticism. Despite these setbacks, however, evidence suggests that Constantine and Larsen’s work did make an impact, albeit one that did not come to fruition until fairly recently. In 1993 the well-known independent curator Mary Jane Jacob wrote about the infl uence of Constantine upon her own innovative, unorthodox, curatorial mission, which has been instrumental in promoting an open use of non-traditional materials without regard to the art world’s hierarchical distinctions: Unbeknownst to me, I began following [Constantine’s] work as a young visitor to The Museum of Modern Art on frequent occasions from 1965 to 1969. Reading and just looking at the images in Beyond Craft sent my own curatorial work in Elissa Auther Fiber Art and the Hierarchy of Art and Craft, 1960–1980 31 The Journal of Modern Craft Volume 1—Issue 1—March 2008, pp. 13–34 another direction. Most of all, her avant- garde philosophy of inclusionism remains compelling and her career—battling to bring into the mainstream art and artists from the outside—is a model of
  • 39. independent vision and courage. (Jacob 1993: 9) Dramatic change in the use and reception of fi ber in the art world has occurred since the 1970s, thanks in part to curators such as Jacob, as well as artists of the fi rst-wave feminist art movement, who set as one of their goals the analysis of the hierarchy of art and craft and women’s low position within it through the incorporation of fi ber into their work. For feminists, fi ber craft played a role in the construction of an alternate history of art making. A shared marginality between the female traditional artist and the contemporary feminist artist helped the latter negotiate the paradoxical goal of seeking recognition in the mainstream art world, while at the same time attempting to critique it. In this context, the once negative associations of fi ber or craft with femininity and the home were recast as distinctive and culturally valuable features of an artistic heritage specifi c to women. While I’m not convinced that the use of fi ber by artists today demonstrates the complete effacement of the hierarchy of art and craft, the medium’s ubiquitous use in contemporary art no doubt represents an important stage in a decades-long process of art-world assimilation of the medium. The evolution of Louise Bourgeois’s attitude toward fi ber—from one of dismissal in the 1960s to full embrace with her latest soft sculptures made from her personal collection
  • 40. of linens and fabric remnants—is only one Fig 11 Louise Bourgeois, Untitled, 2002, tapestry and aluminum. 17 × 12 × 12 in.; 43.1 × 30.4 × 30.4 cm. Private Collection, courtesy of Cheim & Reid, New York. Christopher Burke photographer. example of the medium’s new currency. The limitations of their strategy aside, the work of Constantine and Larsen in the 1960s and 1970s to legitimate fi ber as a medium of art represents a historically important moment in this process. Notes 1 Constantine, Mildred and Larsen, Jack Lenor. 1972. Beyond Craft: The Art Fabric. New York: Van Nostrand. For an assessment of Constantine’s career see Sorkin, Jenni. 2003. Way Beyond Craft: Thinking Through the Work of Mildred Constantine. Textile 1(1): 30–47. 32 Fiber Art and the Hierarchy of Art and Craft, 1960–1980 Elissa Auther The Journal of Modern Craft Volume 1—Issue 1—March 2008, pp. 13–34 2 Woven Forms. 1963. New York: Museum of Contemporary Craft. 3 See also Atirnomus, “String and Rope at Janis.” Arts Magazine. February 1970: 58.
  • 41. 4 Bourgeois, Louis. 1969. “The Fabric of Construction.” Craft Horizons 29 (March): 31–35. 5 On the subject, see my 2004 essay “The Decorative, Abstraction and the Hierarchy of Art and Craft in the Art Criticism of Clement Greenberg,” in Oxford Art Journal 27(3): 339–64. 6 Constantine also recounts this experience in The Art Fabric: Mainstream (New York: Van Nostrand Reinhold, 1981, p. 8). 7 Thanks to Judith Bettelheim, Constantine’s daughter, for helping me clarify this point. 8 On the subject, see Marcus, George E. and Myers, Fred R. (eds). 1995. Traffi c in Culture: Refi guring Art and Anthropology. Berkeley: University of California Press. 9 Staniszewski, Mary Anne. 1998. The Power of Display: A History of Exhibition Installations at the Museum of Modern Art. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press, chapter two, passim. 10 See Price, Sally. 1989. Primitive Art in Civilized Places. Chicago: University of Chicago Press; and Staniszewski (1998). 11 See National Endowment for the Arts. 1977. To Survey American Crafts: A Planning Study; and McLean, John. (ed.). 1981. National Crafts Planning Project. National Endowment for the Arts. 12 See Macramé. 1971. New York: Museum of
  • 42. American Folk Art. See also Mary Walker Phillips’s review of the exhibition for Craft Horizons (December 1971): 62. 13 Rossbach also reported that “[Atwater’s] writings were consulted (often a bit sheepishly) by many who deplored her approach to textiles, who did not believe that weavers should be provided with ‘recipes,’ as she called them, for works that ought to have been creative and individual. I remember the small regard I once felt for Atwater and her coverlet weaves, even though my fi rst weaving experience consisted of following a pattern carefully selected from her book” (1983: 22). 14 Albers, Anni. 1940. Hand Weaving Today: Textile Work at Black Mountain College. The Weaver 6(1): 3–7. 15 Atwater, Mary. 1941. It’s Pretty, But is it Art? The Weaver 6(3): 13–14 and 26. 16 Slivka, Rose. 1972. Hard String. Craft Horizons (April): 16–17. 17 See also Meilach, Dona Z. 1974. Soft Sculpture and Other Soft Art Forms with Stuffed Fabrics, Fibers and Plastics. New York: Crown Publishers. References Adamson, Glenn. 2007. Thinking Through Craft. Oxford and New York: Berg.
  • 43. Albers, Anni. 1940. Hand Weaving Today: Textile Work at Black Mountain College. The Weaver 6(1): 3–7. Albers, Anni. 1959. Pictorial Weaves. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. Andreae, Christopher. 1970. String and Rope. The Christian Science Monitor, January 23. Atwater, Mary. 1941. It’s Pretty, But is it Art? The Weaver 6 (3): 13–14, 26. Battcock, Gregory. 1969. Claire Zeisler. Arts Magazine, 43(6): 65. Bourgeois, Louise. 1969. The Fabric of Construction. Craft Horizons 29: 31–5. Collins, James. 1973. Review. Artforum 11(10): 89–93. Constantine, Mildred. 1999. Personal interview with the author, February 23. Constantine, Mildred and Larsen, Jack Lenor. 1969. Wall Hangings. New York: Museum of Modern Art. Constantine, Mildred and Larsen, Jack Lenor. 1972. Beyond Craft: The Art Fabric. New York: Van Nostrand. Greenberg, Clement. “Review of Exhibitions . . .” 17 November 1945. The Nation. Reprinted in John O’Brian, ed. 1986. Clement Greenberg, The Collected Elissa Auther Fiber Art and the Hierarchy of Art and Craft,
  • 44. 1960–1980 33 The Journal of Modern Craft Volume 1—Issue 1—March 2008, pp. 13–34 Essays and Criticism. Vol. 2, Arrogant Purpose, 1945–1949: 39–42. Hoffman, Virginia. 1970. When Will Weaving Be an Art Form? Craft Horizons 30: 18. Jacob, Mary Jane. 1993. Beyond Craft: Curating for Change. Small Works in Fiber: The Mildred Constantine Collection. Cleveland, OH: The Cleveland Museum of Art, pp. 1–9. Kramer, Hilton. 1966. And Now “Eccentric Abstraction”: It’s Art But Does It Matter? New York Times September 25: 27. Kuh, Katherine. 1968. Sculpture: Woven and Knotted. Saturday Review. July 27: 36–7. Lenore Tawney: A Personal World. 1978. Brookfi eld, CT: Brookfi eld Craft Center. Lippard, Lucy. 1966. Eccentric Abstraction. New York: Fischbach Gallery. Reprinted in The New Sculpture 1965-1975: Between Geometry and Gesture. 1990. New York: Whitney Museum of American Art, pp. 54–8. Marandel, J. Patrice. 1979. An Interview with Claire Zeisler. Arts Magazine 54(1): 150–152. O’Brien, George. 1963. Many Materials Used in
  • 45. Unusual Technique. New York Times. April 29 Parker, Rozsika and Pollock, Griselda. 1981. Old Mistresses: Women, Art and Ideology. New York: Pantheon Books. Phillips, Mary Walker. 1970. Step-by-Step Macramé: A Complete Introduction to the Craft of Creative Knotting. New York: Golden Press. Rossbach, Ed. 1983. Mary Atwater and the Revival of American Traditional Weaving. American Craft 43(2): 22–26. Smith, Paul. 1963. Woven Forms. New York: Museum of Contemporary Craft. Smith, Roberta. 1990. Lenore Tawney’s Work in Fiber and Beyond. New York Times. May 18. String and Rope. December 1969/January 1970. New York: Sidney Janis Gallery. Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. Belbin's team role theory: For non-managers also? Fisher, S G;Hunter, T A;Macrosson, W D K Journal of Managerial Psychology; 2002; 17, 1/2; ABI/INFORM Collection pg. 14
  • 46. Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission. Exploring the Effects of Value Diversity on Team Effectiveness
  • 47. David J. Woehr • Luis M. Arciniega • Taylor L. Poling Published online: 2 June 2012 � Springer Science+Business Media, LLC 2012 Abstract Purpose The goal of the present study was to explore the potential impact of within-team value diversity with respect to both team processes and task performance. Design/Methodology/Approach We explored value diversity within a comprehensive framework such that all components of basic human values were examined. A sample of 306 participants randomly assigned to 60 teams, performed a complex hands-on task, demanding high interdependence among team members, and completed different measures of values and team processes. Findings Results indicated that value diversity among team members had no significant impact on task perfor- mance. However, diversity with respect to several value
  • 48. dimensions had a significant unique effect on team process criteria. Results were consistent with respect to the nature of the impact of value diversity on team process outcomes. Specifically, the impact of team value diversity was such that less diversity was positively related to process out- comes (i.e., more similarity resulted in more team cohesion and efficacy and less conflict). Implications The results indicated that disparity among teammates in many of these values may have important implications on subsequent team-level phenomena. We suggest team leaders and facilitators of teambuilding efforts could consider adding to their agendas a session with team members to analyze and discuss the combined value profiles of their team. Originality/Value This is the first study to highlight the unique impact of many unexamined, specific components of team diversity with respect to values on team effec- tiveness criteria.
  • 49. Keywords Value diversity � Team diversity � Team processes � Team effectiveness � Team cohesion � Team conflict Work force diversity has become an increasingly promi- nent concern in recent years. Many organizations have moved to incorporate diversity into their business structure and strategy, hoping for both societal approval and positive performance dividends (Horwitz 2005). The focus on workforce diversity taken together with the increased emphasis on team-based work groups (Applebaum and Batt 1994; Ilgen 1999) has resulted in a surge of interest in the relationship between team member diversity and overall team effectiveness (e.g., Bell 2007; Horwitz 2005; Klein et al. 2011; Mannix and Neale 2005; Mohammed and Angell 2004; Webber and Donahue 2001; Harrison et al. 1998; Jackson and Rudermann 1997; Milliken and Martins 1996). Although the majority of past research has focused on demographic, surface-level diversity (e.g., age, race, gender) (Harrison et al. 1998), the impact of diversity with
  • 50. respect to deeper, psychological variables (e.g., values, personality, attitudes, etc.) is likely of greater concern. As stated by Hollenbeck et al. (2004), ‘‘demographic diversity is actually less important to team performance than D. J. Woehr Management Department, University of North Carolina, Charlotte, NC, USA L. M. Arciniega (&) Management Department, Instituto Tecnológico Autónomo de México (ITAM) School of Business, Rı́o Hondo 1, Mexico, D.F. 01080, Mexico e-mail: [email protected] T. L. Poling Joint Advertising Market Research and Studies (JAMRS), Alexandria, VA, USA 123 J Bus Psychol (2013) 28:107–121 DOI 10.1007/s10869-012-9267-4
  • 51. psychological diversity, especially over time’’ (p. 357). Thus, the goal of the present study is to explore the potential impact of within team diversity with respect to a specific set of psychological characteristics, namely values, on both team processes and task outcomes. It is important to highlight the lack of studies in this specific line of research, and more over, the inexistence of a study utilizing a comprehensive taxonomy of basic values to examine these relations. This study focuses on filling this gap. Defining Diversity Mannix and Neale (2005) note that diversity is a complex and multifaceted term. It encompasses a variety of differ- ences among people (e.g., demographic variables, job- related characteristics, attitudes, values, personality traits, etc.), and the positive or negative effects of diversity may be contingent on the variables under investigation as well as the performance criteria. In a review of the team com- position literature, Bell (2007) contends that discussing
  • 52. ‘‘diversity’’ without reference to a particular attribute or variable is meaningless; thus, the specific variable or family of variables on which team members differ must be identified before the relationship with team effectiveness can be examined. In order to provide some conceptual organization to this multifaceted phenomenon, recent the- oretical discussions have classified dimensions of diversity into two general categories: attributes that are demographic and non-psychological versus attributes that are non-visi- ble, underlying, and psychological in nature. Attributes that are non-psychological and easily detect- able fall under Harrison et al.’s (1998) surface-level categorization. Typical attributes associated with this cat- egorization include age, gender, race, and physical dis- abilities (Mannix and Neale 2005). In contrast, non- apparent, psychological differences (e.g., cognitive ability, personality traits, values, beliefs, and attitudes) fall under the deep-level diversity category (Harrison et al. 1998).
  • 53. Information on deep-level attributes is acquired via observation of verbal and non-verbal behavioral patterns (Harrison et al. 1998). The deep-level label is analogous to similar descriptors such as ‘‘underlying’’ (Milliken and Martins 1996), ‘‘less observable’’ (Jackson et al. 1995), and ‘‘psychological’’ (Jackson and Rudermann 1997) used by other researchers. General values are prominent examples of Harrison’s deep-level diversity category. Team diversity in surface-level characteristics has been the focus of ample research, which taken as a whole has yielded equivocal results (e.g., Webber and Donahue 2001). However, deep-level diversity may operate dif- ferently than surface-level diversity. In fact, research has demonstrated that surface-level diversity does not necessarily relate to deep-level diversity (Harrison et al. 2002; Horwitz and Horwitz 2007). These findings highlight the need for the direct investigation of deep-level diversity. Yet, the research on deep-level diversity to date is under-
  • 54. developed. Specifically, Bell (2007) demonstrated that of studies examining deep-level team composition, those focusing on diversity are few, fragmented, and inconsis- tent. Bell (2007) also highlighted the dearth of studies on team member value composition despite the role of values as behavioral and attitudinal determinants. Tentative find- ings for only two specific value dimensions (i.e., collec- tivism and preference for teamwork) were reported, which were based on data from three and two studies, respec- tively. Bell (2007) commented that further research is necessary to assess the impact of other value dimensions on team performance. We propose that these other dimensions should include basic human values such as those related to independent thought and action, stability of self and rela- tionships, and the welfare of others with whom one is in constant interaction. Basic values such as these have sig- nificant implications for interpersonal behavior. Theoretical Underpinnings of Potential Diversity
  • 55. Effects Research on deep-level team diversity in general and value diversity in particular, has only recently begun to emerge and is relatively limited (Harrison et al. 2002). However, the theoretical foundations of this research are longstanding. There are two primary classes of theories underpinning this area of study. On the surface, these theories appear to be in direct opposition, as one advocates a ‘‘pessimistic’’ view of diversity and the other an ‘‘optimistic’’ view (Mannix and Neale 2005). In general, the pessimistic view focuses on the affective and interactional problems caused by diversity; whereas, the optimistic view posits enhanced creativity, quality, and innovative task performance that results from increased access to a variety of perspectives and resources (Kravitz 2005). Respectively, these views are grounded in the similarity-attraction/social identity paradigms, and the information processing/cognitive resource paradigms. The Pessimistic Perspective
  • 56. In general, negative expectations regarding the impact of diversity in team composition stem from the similarity- attraction paradigm (Byrne 1971; Tziner 1985) and social identity theory (Tajfel 1978). Social attraction theory posits that similarity in values, beliefs, and attitudes increases interpersonal attraction, and when individuals like each other, their values, beliefs, and attitudes become more aligned. Together, attraction and similarity reciprocally 108 J Bus Psychol (2013) 28:107–121 123 build on one another, facilitating a pull toward symmetry and an avoidance of the strain produced by dissimilarity (Rosenbaum 1986; Mannix and Neale 2005). Furthermore, people tend to categorize themselves relative to similar others and in an effort to maintain their social identities, they will demonstrate a bias toward those whom they believe share similar characteristics (Tajfel and Turner
  • 57. 1986; Turner and Haslam 2001). Byrne’s (1971) early attraction-similarity research supports the perspective that individuals are drawn toward others who they think share similar attitudes to themselves and report that these indi- viduals are smarter and more well-adjusted than others. These propositions also underlie Schneider’s (1987) well- known attraction, selection, attrition (ASA) theory, which supports the notion that this similarity-attraction process naturally produces increasingly homogenous work envi- ronments (Giberson et al. 2005). Overall, these theories posit that teams with members who have homogeneous values will more readily identify with each other. As a result, they will be more cohesive, facilitating both interaction and subsequent performance. Teams with heterogeneous values are predicted to be less cohesive and thus less productive because of the conflicts and stress that result from the differences in members’ beliefs.
  • 58. The Optimistic Perspective Alternately, cognitive resource theory posits the ‘‘value in diversity’’ hypothesis, which advocates the benefits of the unique resources that members with diverse attributes bring to the team (Cox and Blake 1991; Easley 2001). The underlying assumption here is that diversity in members’ attributes will result in more informed decisions by pro- moting creativity, innovation, and alternative problem solving. This information processing perspective suggests that differences create an opportunity for team members to share different perspectives and thus examine issues at a deeper level of analysis (Mannix and Neale 2005). More- over, different perspectives are predicted to interact syn- ergistically to create a ‘‘process gain’’ for the group to the extent that they are able to overcome potential social- integration problems that may result from their differing perspectives (Steiner 1972). Overall, cognitive resource theory posits that diverse
  • 59. values among teammates will contribute to better team performance. Members will share information from a greater variety of perspectives, a practice that leads to higher quality analysis of tasks, which in turn fosters higher quality results. Although these two perspectives are often pitted against one another, they are not necessarily mutu- ally exclusive. For example, a team with a diverse set of perspectives may produce high quality task results (e.g., more creative solutions), but in the process experience interpersonal conflict and low cohesion. Moreover, even supporters of the ‘‘value in diversity’’ hypothesis have noted that diversity may offer benefits for team outcomes even as it creates barriers for team interaction processes (Mannix and Neale 2005). In sum, while these theoretical perspectives posit different expectations with respect to value diversity, it is feasible for both perspectives to operate simultaneously contingent on the value and type of criterion in question. Even when this approach is theoret-
  • 60. ically feasible, a recent meta-analysis highlights the lack of studies combining both perspectives at the same time (Van Knippenberg and Schippers 2007). As noted above, the primary goal of the present study is to explore team member value diversity with respect to an encompassing taxonomy of basic human values. Our objective is to dig into the impact of team value diversity on task performance, and on the following aspects of team effectiveness: relationship and task conflict, cohesion, and team efficacy. Below, we begin by presenting a brief review of previous findings pertaining to value diversity. Next, we discuss the concept of basic values and present Schwartz comprehensive model of values. We then briefly highlight the key team process variables that reflect team effectiveness and that are likely to be influenced by team member value diversity. Finally, we consider several research questions addressed in this study. Previous Findings on Value Diversity
  • 61. As noted by Harrison et al. (2002), and more recently by Bell (2007), relatively little research has examined the impact of diversity among team members in terms of basic values on effectiveness (c.f., Fisher et al. 1996; Jehn et al. 1997; Jehn and Mannix 2001; Klein et al. 2011). Among the few relevant existing studies, there is substantial vari- ability with respect to both the conceptual and operational definitions of values as well as the outcomes examined. One approach has been to measure basic values at the individual-level using the classic Rokeach Value Survey (RVS; Rokeach 1979), and then either aggregate members responses to represent the team level (Rodriguez 1998) or compute their concordance (Fisher et al. 1996). Regarding this measure, Rokeach never suggested any a priori struc- ture for the RVS, and further studies suggest that it does not cover the full domain of the values construct (e.g., Sch- wartz 1992). Nonetheless, Fisher and collaborators indi- cated that teams with more agreement in their ratings
  • 62. across the set of nine personal value scales, demonstrated better task performance than those with less concordance. Though the study presented promising results, it had a serious limitation: after collecting data from 22 teams of J Bus Psychol (2013) 28:107–121 109 123 undergraduate students, 12 teams were dropped due to inadequate levels of concordance between the members’ values ratings. As a result, the analyses were based on only ten teams. The other study employing this approach (Rodriguez 1998), also used a very small sample - 11 teams. Recently, Klein et al. (2011) demonstrated that team leader style moderates the relationship between team val- ues diversity and team conflict. They propose that leaders who are task-focused, create a strong team setting, with clear rules and roles, constraining the influence of team
  • 63. members’ values. Even when the results are relevant, they analyzed value diversity from a simple framework of two values: Protestant work ethic (individual tendency to work hard even in the absence of material rewards) and tradi- tionalism (commitment and acceptance of the customs and ideas of traditional cultures or religions). This framework does not cover relevant aspects of the values domain such as: power, achievement, altruism, etc. Another approach used previously has been to measure organizational values preferences at the individual-level, and then compare the profiles of team members to assess value congruence (Jehn 1994). Using this approximation, Jehn and Mannix (2001) found that team member simi- larity with respect to work-related values (i.e., similarity across a set of 54 items related to innovativeness, stability, detail orientation, outcome orientation, aggressiveness, supportiveness, reward orientation, team orientation, and decisiveness) was positively related to task performance.
  • 64. However, Jehn and collaborators (1997) examined team member value congruence (defined in terms of a median split of team consistency scores derived from ratings of the same work-related values discussed previously) and found that value congruence was related to perceived perfor- mance via a negative correlation with relationship conflict; however, value congruence did not relate to subjective expert ratings of task performance. Both studies employed an adapted version of the Organizational Culture Profile (OCP; O’Reilly et al. 1991). The OCP was developed to obtain profiles of the cultures of organizations, and to assess individual preferences for organizational cultures (O’Reilly et al. 1991, p. 496), but not to measure individual values. Finally, there are various studies using cultural-level values measured at the individual-level (e.g., Eby and Dobbins 1997; Vodosek 2007). These studies rest on the assumption that research on cultural diversity can be con-
  • 65. ducted using individual-level representations of cultural- level constructs (e.g., Maznevski et al. 2002), since some aspects of culture are internalized by individuals. For instance, Kirkman and Shapiro (2001) reported on the impact of collectivism, power distance, doing orientation, and determinism on the performance of self-managed work teams, utilizing an instrument that operationalized two different models of cultural values (i.e., Hofstede 1991; Kluckhohn 1951). According to Smith and Schwartz (1997), this approach is inappropriate. These cross-cultural scholars argue that the relations between values at the cultural level reflect the dynamic conflicts of the societies that exist as a result of the way their institutions pursue their goals, adding that these relations are not necessarily the same at the individual-level. Schwartz Model of Values Values have generally been referred to as needs, beliefs, or norms. Values can be best understood as cognitive repre-
  • 66. sentations of universal needs (Rokeach 1979; Schwartz 1992). These needs are expressed through over-arching goals that direct behavior across situations, and are ordered by importance as guiding principles in life (Schwartz et al. 2001). Schwartz (1992) posits that the essence of a value is the motivational goal it expresses. From this premise, Schwartz derived ten value types that form a circumplex structure (see Fig. 1) such that value types that share a similar general motivational goal are arranged closer together. Alternately, types representing divergent goals are arranged more distantly around the circumference of the model. For instance, power (PO) and achievement (AC) are two compatible value types sharing the general SD UN BE CO TR
  • 67. SE PO AC HE ST Fig. 1 Example value profiles of the members of a team. SD self- direction, UN universalism, BE benevolence, CO conformity, TR tradition, SE security, PO power, AC achievement, HE hedonism, ST stimulation 110 J Bus Psychol (2013) 28:107–121 123 motivational goal of enhancing personal status, even at the expenses of others. Conversely, power (PO) and univer- salism (UN) are two conflicting values; the motivational goal of power is enhancing personal interests, whereas the motivational goal of universalism is promoting the welfare of others. The pattern of compatibilities and incompati- bilities between value types is based on the premise that
  • 68. actions taken in the pursuit of each type have both psy- chological and behavioral consequences, which may be compatible or in conflict with the goals derived from other values. The Appendix provides a brief description of the ten value types (for a full description of these values see Schwartz 1992). It is important to highlight that a recent meta-analysis has validated this circumplex structure in more than seventy countries (Steinmetz et al. 2012) Team member diversity in values is represented by the disparity in the importance assigned by each team member to each value type. The more the variability in the impor- tance assigned by the members of the team to each value type, the greater the level of diversity across team mem- bers. Figure 1 shows this idea schematically. Each radius represents one of ten value types, and each irregular decagon, the profile of a team member. The lower the importance a team member assigns to a specific value, the closer the point associated with that individual’s profile
  • 69. will be to the center on the respective value radius. The degree of spread among the points on a given value radius represents the degree of team diversity on that value. For instance, in the case of the radius of benevolence (BE), the variance among the four value profiles is low compared to the variance on the radius for achievement (AC). Given the contradictory nature across the motivational goals under- lying opposing values, it is inappropriate to compute an overall ‘‘values’’ score. Moreover, the configuration approach (Moynihan and Peterson 2001) argues that whe- ther homogeneity or heterogeneity regarding deep-level attributes is preferable depends on the specific value or trait in question. Consistent with these propositions, diversity should be examined separately with respect to each of the ten values. Given the predominant role of values with respect to attitudes and subsequent behavior, team member value diversity is likely to be a key factor for both process and outcome measures of team effectiveness.
  • 70. Team Effectiveness The evaluation of teams encompasses a variety of com- ponents. Many theories have addressed the multifaceted nature of team effectiveness (e.g., Shea and Guzzo 1987; Gladstein 1984; Hackman 1987). According to Hackman (1987), group effectiveness can be defined in terms of three criteria. First, the final outputs produced by the team must meet or exceed the standards set by key constituents within the organization. Second, the internal social processes operating as the team interacts should enhance, or at least maintain, the group’s ability to work together in the future. Finally, the experience of working in the team environment should act to satisfy rather than aggravate the personal needs of team members. In order to address these criteria, team effectiveness evaluation should include both a mea- sure of the teams’ final task performance as well as criteria with which to assess intragroup process. The current paper explores task performance and prominent team process
  • 71. criteria. The three major intragroup process constructs examined are: intra-group conflict, team cohesion, and team-efficacy. Intra-group conflict has emerged as an integral team process variable. Previous research has differentiated two components of intra-group conflict: relationship conflict and task conflict. Jehn (1994) describes relationship conflict as interpersonal incompatibilities between team members such as annoyance and animosity. Task conflict occurs when members convey divergent ideas and opinions about specific aspects related to task accomplishment (Jehn 1994). Research to date indicates that relationship conflict is largely detrimental to team performance (e.g., Evan 1965; Baron 1991; van Woerkom and van Engen 2009; Vodosek 2007). The impact of task conflict is less clear. While it has been argued that task conflict facilitates enhanced performance via thorough task analysis (e.g., Amason and Schweiger 1994), empirical evidence is equivocal. Some research has
  • 72. found positive relationships between task conflict and novel idea generation and strategic planning (Baron 1991; Amason 1996), but others have shown task conflict may hinder goal accomplishment and implementation (Amason 1996; Vodosek 2007). In a direct investigation of task conflict, Jehn (1994) found only a small amount of task conflict was beneficial, after which team performance began to deterio- rate, and a meta-analysis demonstrated a negative relation- ship between task conflict and team satisfaction and performance (De Dreu and Weingart 2003). Nonetheless, both types of conflict have proven to be significant correlates of a variety of team effectiveness criteria. Team cohesion is another important process variable (Chiocchio and Essiembre 2009; Swezey and Salas 1992). While a multitude of definitions and measures have been offered (Mullen and Copper 1994), basically, cohesion is viewed as ‘‘a general indicator of synergistic group interaction—or process’’ (Barrick et al. 1998, p. 382).
  • 73. Meta-analyses have revealed significant team cohesion- performance effects (Beal et al. 2003; Mullen and Copper 1994). Furthermore, cohesion has been linked to greater coordination during team-tasks (Morgan and Lassiter 1992) as well as improved satisfaction, productivity, and group interactions (Bettenhausen 1991). J Bus Psychol (2013) 28:107–121 111 123 Finally, team efficacy is another important team process construct. Team efficacy refers to team members’ percep- tions of task-specific team competence (Gibson 1999). This construct is thought to create a sense of confidence within the team that enables the group to persevere when faced with hardship (Gully et al. 2002). Several researchers have related team efficacy to aspects of team effectiveness (e.g., Campion et al. 1993; Gibson 1999; Gibson et al. 2000), and a meta-analysis demonstrated that the relationship
  • 74. between team efficacy and performance was greater than that between cohesion and performance (Gully et al. 2002). Present Study Our review of both the extant research as well as the theoretical underpinnings highlights several limitations in the literature to date. Specifically, the few studies exam- ining diversity with respect to team member values has largely focused on congruence across a set of context specific values as opposed to a comprehensive set of more broadly defined values such as that posited by Schwartz. To date, no study has examined team member diversity with respect to the full domain of values on both process and performance criteria. Moreover, the literature offers little guidance as to whether value diversity will lead to positive or negative effects with respect to team processes and outcomes, or to what extent these effects will be contingent on the specific value dimensions examined. Finally, while the literature examining the impact of sur-
  • 75. face-level diversity has capitalized on the fact that these characteristics are readily observable and therefore likely to have immediate effects on team outcomes. It has been argued, however, that diversity with respect to deep-level characteristics such as values may take longer to manifest itself with respect to team outcomes (e.g., Harrison et al. 1998). Therefore, it is not at all clear as to whether there will be effects of value diversity in short-term novel teams. Thus, our goal in the present study is to explore the relationship between values diversity and team effective- ness. We provide a preliminary investigation of the impact of basic values, on both process and task performance aspects of team effectiveness. Toward this end, we seek to address two research questions. Specifically: (1) To what extent does diversity across team members with respect to basic values impact team process and outcome variables in a short-term team task with
  • 76. novel teams? (2) Will the impact of team member value diversity be consistent with the optimistic or pessimistic views of team diversity? Method Participants Three hundred and six undergraduate college students at a large U.S. southeastern university participated in the present study. Participants were randomly assigned to mixed gender teams of 4–6 (M = 4.6; mode = 5) resulting in 60 teams. Participants were 43 % (134) male, and 78 % (237) white. The mean age of participants was 22 years, and the ages ranged from 19 to 38 years (SD = 2.32). Task The task was a complex team-based exercise called the Chinese Bridge (Arciniega and Castañón 2002). The task is a relatively difficult one requiring both the design and building of a complex structure. Specifically, the task
  • 77. requires each team to design and build a replica of a real bridge, using 33 plastic pipes of three different sizes and 20 rubber bands (with instructions that all of the materials must be used in the bridge). The task is designed such that, given the material available, there is one optimal solution. In addition, the task is designed so that even if a team were given specific plans for the bridge, multiple people working together are required to actually build the structure (e.g., one must hold pieces while another connects them, etc.). Thus, successful completion requires team members to work interdependently. This type of simulation could be categorized as a high interdependence task, since team members collectively work together to complete the task while sharing information and resources (Horwitz and Horwitz 2007). Based on a recent review of the literature (Joshi and Roh 2008), these contextual factors combining task interdependence and complexity, promote a scenario where deep-level team diversity variables really emerge.
  • 78. The simulation consists of four phases: (a) a multimedia presentation describing the task and presenting a picture of the real bridge; (b) a 20-min period for team members to familiarize themselves with the materials; (c) a 30-min period to sketch a proposed design of the bridge; and (d) the building phase lasting approximately 60 min. Measures Values Values were assessed using the Portrait Values Question- naire (PVQ; Schwartz et al. 2001). The 40-item PVQ measures the ten value types proposed by Schwartz (1992). These values as measured by the PVQ are defined in the Appendix. The scale includes short verbal portraits of hypothetical individuals (e.g., Thinking up new ideas and 112 J Bus Psychol (2013) 28:107–121 123 being creative is important to him. He likes to do things in his
  • 79. own original way). Respondents are asked to rate the extent to which they agreed with each item on a scale from 1 (not like me at all) to 6 (very much like me). The PVQ has been used in several studies, in more than 60 countries, and has been shown to be a reliable and valid measure of personal values (Schwartz 1992; Schwartz et al. 2001). The average alpha across these ten dimensions was .68 (SD = .08). Here, it is important to note that previous studies utilizing the PVQ during the last decade have tended to report rel- atively low reliability estimates (particularly estimates of internal consistency) for many of the value dimensions (e.g., Aitken-Schermer et al. 2008; Fotopoulos et al. 2011; Liem et al. 2011; Schwartz et al. 2001). Schwartz justifies the low reliability of some of the value scales based on the fact that many of these, have conceptually broad defini- tions, assessing multiple components, rather than narrowly defined constructs (Schwartz et al. 2001, pp. 531–532). Consistent with this perspective, previous studies have
  • 80. found clear effects between the value types and a plethora of psychological variables under study despite relatively low reliability estimates. Nonetheless, the tradition scale demonstrated a particularly low alpha (.54) in the present study; thus, it was not included in further analyses. Task and Relationship Conflict Conflict was measured using Jehn’s Intragroup Conflict Scale (ICS; 1994). The scale contains four items related to the task conflict dimension (e.g., There are differences of opinion regarding the task in my work group), and four items related to the relationship conflict dimension (e.g., There are personality clashes present in my work group). Participants are asked to rate the extent to which they agreed with each item on a scale from 1 (strongly disagree) to 7 (strongly agree). Alphas for these scales were .85 (task conflict) and .87 (relationship conflict). Cohesion Cohesion was measured using two items from Podsakoff and
  • 81. MacKenzie’s (1994) Substitutes for Leadership Scale and four items from Zaccaro (1991). Each of the 6 items consists of a short statement regarding the cohesion of the individ- ual’s team (e.g., I generally get along well with my fellow group members). Respondents are asked to rate the extent to which they agreed with each item on a scale from 1 (strongly disagree) to 7 (strongly agree). Alpha for the scale was .75. Team Efficacy Team-efficacy was assessed with a 2-item scale con- structed specifically for this study (e.g., My team works as an effective unit and My team has an effective plan for completing the bridge task). Respondents rated the extent to which they agreed with each item using a 7-point scale from 1 (strongly agree) to 7 (strongly disagree). Alpha for this scale was .73. Team Task Performance Task performance was measured as the extent to which a team was able to complete the task and the quality of the
  • 82. finished product (i.e., the bridge replica). Following com- pletion of the task, a photograph was taken of each team’s bridge. Next, each photograph was evaluated by a group of 5 raters familiar with the task. Ratings were made on a 5-point scale from 1 (non-standing structure) to 5 (arched bridge with 5 cross pieces and perfect joints). After each member of the research group provided their initial rating, the entire group came to consensus on a single rating for the bridge (mean level of agreement in the initial ratings across raters was 88 %). The consensus rating was used as the measure of team performance. Procedure All participants completed the values measures during the week preceding their participation in the bridge task. On the day of the simulation, all participants first viewed the task overview presentation as a group and then were bro- ken up into their teams and assigned to separate team rooms to complete the task. Teams completed each phase
  • 83. of the bridge task, and then each participant individually completed the team process measures. Results All analyses were conducted at the team-level. To assess diversity, the variance across team members for each of the ten value scales was computed (e.g., Barrick et al. 1998; Mohammed and Angell 2003; Neuman et al. 1999). Descriptive statistics and intercorrelations for the compo- sition of team value types are shown in Table 1. Statistics are presented for the team-level for mean composition of values (control variables) and the average degree of within- team variance in each value (diversity variables). Alpha values for each value scale are also presented in Table 1. Team process was assessed as the aggregate (mean) score across team members on each of the process measures (task and relationship conflict, cohesion, and team efficacy). Descriptive statistics and intercorrelations for the team process criterion variables are presented in Table 2.
  • 84. Here, it is important to note that the use of these aggregated variables as indicators of team level processes J Bus Psychol (2013) 28:107–121 113 123 T a b le 1 D e sc ri p ti v e st a ti st
  • 125. 123 requires sufficient agreement across team members to warrant aggregation (James 1982; James et al. 1984). Thus, before aggregating, we examined the level of agreement across team members. To assess interrater reliability, we calculated intraclass correlations coefficients (ICCs) where ICC = (MSbetween - MSwithin)/MSbetween (Shrout and Fle- iss 1979). The ICC estimates across all teammates were .62 for team efficacy, .77 for team cohesion, .78 for role con- flict, and .85 for task conflict. Landis and Koch (1977) suggest that interrater reliabilities above .61 should be considered substantial level of agreement. Nonetheless, recent research indicates that ICC’s tend to underestimate levels of agreement (LeBretton et al. 2003). Thus, we also calculated rwg(j) for each of these variables for each team 1 . Results (included in Table 2) indicate adequate agreement
  • 126. to justify aggregation, (i.e., overall mean rwg(j) was approximately .85). Next, we examined the zero-order correlations between diversity in each of the value types and the five criterion variables. These correlations (presented in Table 3) indi- cate that team diversity with respect to seven of the values is significantly related to one of the outcome variables, and six of these are related to at least two of the four team process variables. Task performance was not related to diversity in any of the values. Previous research has indi- cated that the team’s mean (across team members) level of deep-level attributes is often a significant predictor of team performance criteria (e.g., Barrick et al. 1998), and thus the mean level may confound the amount of variance in these attributes (Bedeian and Mossholder 2000). Consequently, it is important to control for teams’ mean level (i.e., ‘‘team- level’’) before interpreting the impact of variability. As stated by Steiner (1972, p. 667), ‘‘a completely satisfactory