SlideShare a Scribd company logo
1 of 201
Classroom Ordering and the
Situational Imperatives of
Routine and Ritual
David Diehl1 and Daniel A. McFarland2
Abstract
This article contends that the problem of classroom order rests
less in the roles and compositions of class-
rooms than in the multidimensional nature of their social
situations. Classroom order arises from the
dynamic relationship between distinct situational requirements:
the coordination of interaction into institu-
tionalized patterns (routine) and the validation of participants’
identities (ritual). Utilizing a unique data set of
more than 800,000 turns of talk from 601 high school
classrooms, the authors develop metrics for measur-
ing the longitudinal accomplishment of routine (interactional
stability) and ritual (interactional concord) and
present two sets of analyses. The first analyses identify the
antecedents to stability and concord, and the
second examine how stability and concord shape the
experiences and attitudes of classroom participants.
Results indicate that activities and discourse combine to fulfill
the requirements of ritual and routine in dif-
ferent ways, often meeting one at the expense of the other, and
that the accomplishment of stability and
concord has positive returns to classroom experiences, but in
different ways for teachers and students.
Keywords
social networks, classrooms, interaction, communication,
academic activities
Creating and maintaining order in the classroom is
a central concern for educators (Waller 1932; Metz
1978; Boocock 1978; Bossert 1979; Mehan 1979;
Doyle 1986; McNeil 1986; Stodolsky 1988;
Cazden 1988; Nystrand 1997; Gamoran and Kelly
2003). Despite the best efforts of teachers, how-
ever, research from diverse perspectives has shown
that disruption and defiance are ubiquitous and
seemingly unavoidable (MacBeth 1990:192). It is
unclear why some classes are more orderly than
others and why disorder can arise and recede
even within individual class periods. Fundamental
aspects of classroom teaching and learning remain
obscure without a better understanding of how
teachers and students construct, and are molded
by, the social context they jointly enact through
moment-by-moment social transactions (Wells
1993).
We address this lacuna through two theoretical
and empirical elaborations. The first is to regard
interactional order as a multidimensional phenom-
enon. Following Goffman, we relate order as meet-
ing two requirements, namely, those of routine and
ritual (cf. system and normative requirements,
Goffman 1981:14–15; 1983). For an encounter to
be comprehensible, participants must coordinate
according to some collective routine. In class-
rooms, this entails the mobilization of participants
into and across stable interactional configurations
associated with particular academic activities
(Goffman 1981:137). For an encounter to be
embraced and valued, participants must conduct
1Duke Network Analysis Center, Durham, NC, USA
2
Stanford University, Stanford, CA, USA
Corresponding Author:
Daniel A. McFarland, Stanford University, 520 Galvez Mall,
Stanford, CA 94305, USA
Email: [email protected]
Sociology of Education
85(4) 326–349
� American Sociological Association 2012
DOI: 10.1177/0038040712452093
http://soe.sagepub.com
at WESTERN OREGON UNIVERSITY on February 10,
2015soe.sagepub.comDownloaded from
http://soe.sagepub.com/
interpersonal rituals demonstrating concord and
a sense of collective goodwill. In classrooms, this
entails the ratification of valued identities
(Goffman 1981:266). While situational anchors of
routine and ritual can reinforce one another, in
crowded classroom settings there arises a tension
between efforts to coordinate the group and to rat-
ify identities.
Our second elaboration is to regard interactional
order as a situated phenomenon. By this we mean
that the problem of classroom order changes from
moment to moment. As teachers move their classes
through different academic activities, the structures
and expectations for interaction shift, as do the
means by which routine and ritual requirements
of interaction are accomplished. Most practitioners
negotiate these dynamics and tensions via ongoing
talk and communication. However, discursive
moves seldom have the same effect on every situa-
tion. As most educators can attest, declarative state-
ments and jokes have different effects on the
enactment of lectures and group work activities.
To study this situated nature of classroom order,
we identify proxies for interactional stability (rou-
tine) and concord (ritual) within the overall net-
work of moment-to-moment discourse. By
treating individual acts of communication within
institutionalized activities as the building blocks
of classroom order (Goffman 1981; Wells 1999;
Butts 2008), we are able to offer a novel compari-
son of the dynamic patterns of interactions (and
their effects) across a multitude of diverse class-
rooms, class periods, and class lessons.
For our empirical analysis, we use an unparalleled
data set of more than 800,000 turns of discourse col-
lected from 601 high school class periods that enables
us to view classroom order longitudinally. In the first
set of analyses, we model the antecedents of stability
and concord. We demonstrate the general validity of
our approach by showing the timing of stable and
concordant interaction is consistent with existing
qualitative understandings of classrooms. We then
go beyond extant work and find that the accomplish-
ment of stability and concord depends on situational
levers such as the selection of activities and the appro-
priate use of discursive moves suited to each task. Not
only do we demonstrate that forms of talk have differ-
ent structuring effects depending on the academic
activity in which they take place; we also show that
efforts to achieve stability may have a negative effect
on concord, and vice versa. We can thus see the class-
room as a tenuous balance between stability and con-
cord, the nature of which is constantly shifting as the
teacher moves students through different tasks. In the
second set of analyses, we model the effects of stabil-
ity and routine on task survival and participants’
schooling experiences. Overall, we show that varia-
tion in classroom order is situational and multidimen-
sional, the product of a constantly shifting
interrelation between discourse and activity—and
not simply the result of static transsituational roles,
motives, and classroom compositions. In addition,
we show that the accomplishment of interactional sta-
bility and concord matters and has differential returns
to the quality of schooling.
Routine and Ritual: Dual
Requirements for Classroom Ordering
Our work can be located within an ongoing effort to
understand variation in classroom order at increas-
ingly fine-grained levels of analysis. Pioneering
work on the social order of schooling was primarily
concerned with providing a generalizable descrip-
tion of the interaction dynamics between teachers
and students rather than explaining variation within
classrooms. The result was a view of classroom
order shaped by a ubiquitous tension between the
different interests and motivations of teachers and
students as well as the incompatible demands of
the teaching profession itself (Waller 1932;
Bidwell 1965; Lortie 1975).
Building off of this work, midcentury sociology of
education began to offer explanations for variations in
the teacher-student tension between classrooms. This
work identified numerous sources of interclassroom
differences that affected interaction, including
teacher styles and dispositions (Brookover 1945;
Cogan 1956) as well as the culture (Gordon 1957;
Coleman 1961) and composition (Dreeben and Barr
1988) of the student body. In this work variation
arises from differences in how teachers and students
consistently enact their institutional roles. Following
the general trend of post-Parsonian sociology in the
1960s, however, sociolinguists and interactionists
shifted focus inward toward language, micro-pro-
cesses, and variations within classrooms.
Sociolinguists, for their part, paid close atten-
tion to conversational dynamics and began to doc-
ument routinized (generally dyadic) turn-taking
structures in activities such as recitation and group
work (Mehan 1979; Erickson 1982; Stodolsky
1988). Interactionists, in contrast, drew on ethnog-
raphies to illuminate the ongoing negotiation of
control and defiance in the classroom (Jackson
Diehl and McFarland 327
at WESTERN OREGON UNIVERSITY on February 10,
2015soe.sagepub.comDownloaded from
http://soe.sagepub.com/
1968; Metz 1978; Pace 2006). Both streams of
research focused on moment-to-moment dynamics
of interaction, but each offered a fundamentally
different conception of the nature of classroom
order and how to research it.
The study of these two aspects of social order—
the formal structure of talk and the ongoing negotia-
tion of identities—has remained largely disconnected
in the sociological literature. One exception is in
Goffman’s work on what he called the ‘‘interaction
order’’ (Goffman 1967, 1981, 1983).
1
Goffman
argued that interaction is multidimensional and that
the individual has multiple obligations to and interests
in maintaining order (Goffman 1983:5). To that end,
the actor uses various strategies and techniques to
ensure his or her own proper involvement as well as
the proper involvement of others (Goffman
1967:115). Doing so entails meeting both routine
(system) and ritual (normative) requirements of inter-
action (Goffman 1981:14-15). Problematically, how-
ever, Goffman did not adequately delineate between
these two central features of situated order, nor did
he provide means for studying them (Schegloff
1988). Part of our aim in this work is to address
both of these shortcomings.
First, the requirement of routine concerns render-
ing interactional exchanges into recursive and pre-
dictable configurations based on a working
agreement about ‘‘what is going on here’’ (Goffman
1974; 1981). The most important organizing conven-
tions in classrooms are academic activities and the
typified sets of relations, orientations, and discourses
they entail (Erickson 1982; Doyle and Carter 1984;
Stodolsky 1988). Activities provide routinized fea-
tures such as the opening and closing of communica-
tion channels, the distribution of turns, and
communication rules for bystanders (Goffman
1981). In a classroom discussion, for example, stu-
dents follow teacher-defined and enforced rules for
legitimate talk, generally taking the floor and speak-
ing only once given permission to do so.
From this perspective, classrooms can be con-
ceptualized as networks of discourse and routine
as the mobilization of those networks into configu-
rations related to particular academic activities.
Stability is the term we use for when these networks
become recursive, or patterned in a way that repeats
through time. Instability, in contrast, occurs when
these configurations are loosened through gaffes,
interruptions, mistakes, or the occurrence of other
digressions and side events. Stability is associated
with positive learning environments because pre-
dictable behavior allows teachers and students to
expend their cognitive energy on the academic con-
tent of tasks rather than interactional coordination
(Leinhardt and Greeno 1986). For these reasons,
effective teachers rely on an array of repeatedly
used activities (Doyle 1980:13).
The requirement of ritual, in contrast, refers to
the feature of interaction related to the ratification
of valued identities (Goffman 1967:19). In typical
social encounters, actors coordinate actions not
only around recognized activities but also around
identities they recognize and value in their commu-
nication. For example, in everyday conversation,
Goffman (1981:16) repeatedly showed how per-
sons show deference to the demeanor of their inter-
action partners, treating selves as something of
social worth. These ritual elements are present in
all successfully coordinated interactions since
identities of speakers, listeners, and the social rela-
tionships between the two are often normatively
valued (Goffman 1981:269). However, Goffman
failed to account for the fact that the nature of the
institutional context shapes the manner in which
selves are expressed and validated (Strong 1988).
We address this shortcoming by studying how the
ritual requirements of selves are variably met
within the structures of different activities.
In classrooms, the successful accomplishment
of ritual interaction creates a shared sense of
respect, or goodwill, between teachers and students
(Dreeben 1970; Bidwell 2005). Interpersonal good-
will often is marked by subjective and experiential
factors that are difficult to observe. Therefore,
teachers and students mostly infer the presence of
goodwill from overt acts of compliance and defi-
ance (Kelman 1961). The absence of defiance in
social encounters can be referred to as the behav-
ioral expression of concord. To be clear, concord
is not synonymous with the subjective experience
of classroom goodwill. In the first part of our
empirical analysis, we address concord as an
observable feature of interaction patterns, and in
the second part, we look at goodwill as a feature
of subjective experience reported on surveys.
As with routine, meeting ritual requirements of
interaction has a positive effect on teaching and
learning. Students, for their part, defy authority
when they feel their character has been imputed
in a demeaning way (Lawrence et al. 1984:122).
Teachers, in turn, feel student defiance threatens
their institutional identity and devalues the invest-
ment of time and energy they put into an activity
(Lawrence et al. 1984:121; McFarland 2001,
2004). When there is classroom goodwill,
328 Sociology of Education 85(4)
at WESTERN OREGON UNIVERSITY on February 10,
2015soe.sagepub.comDownloaded from
http://soe.sagepub.com/
interactions are characterized by warmth and
respect, and with those come lower levels of disrup-
tive behaviors (Battistich et al. 1997), higher levels
of on-task behavior (Marzano, Marzano, and
Pickering 2003), and greater intellectual risk taking
(Murray and Greenberg 2000).
The Variable Relationship between
Stability and Concord. Classrooms are
crowded settings where a multiplicity of doings
and interests come into and fall out of focus
over the course of a lesson (Jackson 1968;
McFarland 2005). In the flux of affairs, situational
requirements of coordination and identity are var-
iably catered to and met. This means that there are
two distinct ways that disorder can arise: through
either problems related to instability in activity
structures or challenges to concord. How any par-
ticular communicative act relates to these interac-
tional anchors depends, however, on the nature of
the activity within which it takes place.
This conceptualization emphasizes the nature of
participation, or the way that individuals align or
orient themselves toward the activity and its rules
and expectations for speaking, listening, and turn
taking (Erickson 2004). Sociolinguists have long
described the pattern of these alignments and orien-
tations as the participation framework of an activity
(Goffman 1981).
2
These frameworks define inter-
personal expectations and obligations such that
everyone is called on to adopt particular interaction
roles or participation statuses (e.g., speaker or
hearer). These participation statuses are then ar-
ranged in such a way as to form a larger pattern.
When recursively enacted, it becomes a recognized,
guiding framework to which everyone adheres
(Gee 1999). This is true for both academic activi-
ties prescribed by the teacher and informal activi-
ties initiated by students (e.g., gossip in Eder
and Enke 1991). The successful cueing of an activ-
ity’s participation framework, the mobilization
of classroom participants into the activity, and its
stable reenactment over time are quite an
accomplishment.
From this perspective, the basic empirical units
out of which classroom order is generated are dis-
crete communicative events (i.e., utterances) pro-
duced by one actor and directed toward one or
more targets (Butts 2008:158). Adapting the lan-
guage of Goffman (1981), we refer to these as dis-
cursive ‘‘moves.’’ Actors use discursive moves to
present particular orientations toward each other
and toward shared interaction (see ‘‘footings’’ in
Goffman 1981). These moves can commence inter-
actional alignments, which in turn lead to larger
changes in the structural configuration of group
communication (Goodwin 1998; Gibson 2003).
Teachers use discourse to manage the timing and
sequencing of interactions to strike a balance
between coercion and engendering voluntary partic-
ipation that allows them to mobilize students in and
out of the participation framework of academic
activities (Kounin 1970; McFarland 2004).
Students, for their part, also use discourse both to
align with academic activities and to resist them,
all the while asserting and expressing valued identi-
ties (Alpert 1991; McFarland 2001). The overall pic-
ture is one of an ongoing struggle about positions in
the classroom network of talk (Grenfell 1998) as dis-
tinct activity frameworks are cued via discourse and
ensuing patterns of association form and fall (see
Figure 1 for a schematic and Supplemental Online
Materials for a network movie demonstrating pat-
tern switches at http://soe.sagepub.com/content/85/
4/326/suppl/DC1).
Two features of activity frameworks are espe-
cially important for understanding the situated
nature of routine and ritual imperatives: prescribed
interactivity and the centralization of attention
(Stinchcombe 1968). Prescribed interactivity re-
fers to the spread of responsibility or range of par-
ticipants necessary to enact a given activity
structure. It also refers to the degree to which stu-
dents are afforded active or nonactive participation
statuses (e.g., speaker or hearer). This degree of in-
teractivity ranges from no participation; to some
participation, but only as an animator of other’s
views (e.g., in recitation where the student recalls
a fact); to more participation and authoring of
views; to full participation and responsibility. For
example, group work requires frequent interaction
between many persons assuming the participation
status of cospeakers, and student presentations
call on students to take turns presenting their own
ideas to the entire class. In contrast, lectures and
tests call on students to be hearers or noninteractive
individual workers. The centralization of attention,
in contrast, refers to the degree to which an activity
focuses on a single speaker and ignores others in
the audience. This shapes the extent to which the
possibilities for identity expression and validation
are evenly or unevenly distributed across partici-
pants. For example, recitation is highly centralized,
with all eyes on the speaker, whereas seatwork is
highly decentralized, with each student focused
Diehl and McFarland 329
at WESTERN OREGON UNIVERSITY on February 10,
2015soe.sagepub.comDownloaded from
http://soe.sagepub.com/
on his or her own work. Common classroom activ-
ities can be broadly characterized by the interrela-
tion of their amount of interactivity and attention
structure, as summarized in Table 1.
As activities and their features of interactivity and
attention change, so too does the relationship between
routine and ritual aspects of interaction. Activities
that call on students to assume passive participation
statuses are often easier to coordinate and stabilize
(e.g., lecture), but they undermine goodwill because
they centralize identity valuation on only a few peo-
ple, leaving others unacknowledged. By contrast,
activities that call on students to assume active partic-
ipation statuses are harder to coordinate (more au-
thors defining the situation), but they ratify and
value a greater number of identities, thereby encour-
aging goodwill. As such, activities activate different
situational concerns. Solving problems of routine
may create problems of ritual, and solving problems
of ritual may create problems of routine.
In sum, this article proposes a significant recon-
ceptualization of social order in classrooms. We
argue that the most fundamental problems of class-
room order do not stem from factors that hold
across classroom settings, such as classroom com-
positions, role tensions, or even poor-fitting
instructional activities. Rather, the core problems
of social order are multidimensional and situa-
tional. Classroom order is multidimensional in
that it pertains to the dynamic interrelation of two
distinct requirements for successful activities: the
coordination of interaction into institutionalized pat-
terns (routine) and the validation of the identities of
participants (ritual). And classroom order is situational
in that finding balance between these interrelated as-
pects of interaction is an ongoing practical
Table 1. Dimensions of Activity Frameworks
Focus of attention (ritual)
Centralized and focused Distributed and differentiated
Prescribed Interactivity (Routine) Active Presentation,
discussion Group work, free time
Nonactive Recitation, lecture Seatwork, test, audiovisual
Channel
Undefined
Structure
1
Lecture
Structure
2
Group
Work
Undefined:
Available
Stories, Props
Discourses,
Etc.., Transition
Release
Figure 1. Shifting participation structures in class periods
330 Sociology of Education 85(4)
at WESTERN OREGON UNIVERSITY on February 10,
2015soe.sagepub.comDownloaded from
http://soe.sagepub.com/
achievement accomplished jointly by teachers and
students within the dynamic network of moment-to-
moment classroom communication. As such, class-
room participants find themselves in a context with
a multiplicity of problems that vary with the situation.
In what follows, we turn to the empirical study by
which we establish this argument.
SETTINGS AND DATA
While researchers have recently begun to introduce
increasingly systematic methods for classroom
observation (e.g., Cameron, Connor, and
Morrison 2005; Good, Mulryan, and McCaslin
2006; Pianta and Hamre 2009), this work typically
focuses on either sequences of dyadic interactions
(usually between teacher and single students),
thereby unwittingly bracketing all other concurrent
interaction, or summary counts of behaviors,
thereby obscuring interactional dynamics. We
argue that the goal of a theory of classroom social
order, however, is to elaborate global patterns of
interaction not evident in individual episodes or se-
quences of talk (Gibson 2008; Wyatt et al. 2008).
To accomplish this, we drew on an unparalleled
data set of streaming interaction recorded in two
high schools during the 1996-1997 school year
(McFarland 1999, 2001, 2004). It consists of obser-
vations in 650 separate class periods across 165
classrooms. Of those, 601 class periods across
153 classrooms had observation records suitable
for quantitative analyses. This yielded an analyz-
able sample of more than 800,000 sequential turns
of interaction. In what follows, we describe these
settings and the manner in which observations
were recorded and formatted for analysis.
Settings. The first high school, Magnet High, is
a racially integrated elite magnet school located in
an inner-city neighborhood of a large midwestern
metropolitan area. The second high school, Rural
High, is located more than 500 miles away in
a town of 17,000 residents. It is a traditional rural
school that is racially homogeneous and serves pre-
dominantly lower- to middle-income students.
Magnet and Rural represent distinct learning envi-
ronments, but they nonetheless have surprisingly
similar student and teacher behaviors. More specific
details about Magnet and Rural High Schools and
the students who attend them can be found in
McFarland (2004).
Data. Classroom observations focused on
10th- and 12th-grade core subjects in math, sci-
ence, English, and history, but some effort was
made to observe foreign language and arts classes.
A sum of observed class periods by school and
subject matter is presented in Table 2.
3
Core sub-
jects were selected because they allowed for more
comparability across schools than elective sub-
jects could have. Within core subjects, 10th- and
12th-grade classes were selected because multiple
observations of the same individuals were desir-
able and the observer was unable to observe the
entire array of courses offered.
The coding scheme used was developed from
prior work (Mehan 1979; Stodolsky 1988;
McFarland 1999) and piloted two times before
this study. The resulting data set has been used
for statistical modeling in several previous articles
(McFarland 2001; Moody, McFarland, and
Bender-deMoll 2005; Bender-deMoll and
McFarland 2006). An assessment of coding reli-
ability was performed using classroom videos,
and it is presented in the Supplemental Online
Materials. Due to the nature of data collection,
coder reliability noticeably decreased in activity
segments wherein simultaneous talk occurred
(e.g., free time and group work). This resulted in
underreported frequencies in the more fluid and de-
centralized task structures. However, the sheer vol-
ume and breadth of total observations decreases the
amount of error that affects results.
PART A. ANTECEDENTS OF
INTERACTIONAL CONCORD
AND STABILITY
In the first two sets of analyses, below, we examine
the discursive antecedents of interactional stability
and concord as well as their relationship with dif-
ferent academic activities.
Table 2. Class Periods Observed by Subject and
School
School
Subject Magnet Rural Row totals
Mathematics 75 93 168
Science 58 117 175
Social science 51 37 88
English 79 60 139
Foreign language 15 14 29
Visual arts 2 2
Columns totals 278 323 601
Diehl and McFarland 331
at WESTERN OREGON UNIVERSITY on February 10,
2015soe.sagepub.comDownloaded from
http://soe.sagepub.com/
Dependent Variables: Operationalizing
Interactional Stability and Concord. Since
our interest is in the dynamic, and hence temporal,
nature of talk, our first step is to aggregate com-
munications into slices of class time (2½ mi-
nutes)
4
and to render them into network matrices
reflecting two distinct types of transaction
(McFarland 2005): one for task-related communi-
cation and another for social-related communica-
tion. Because we are interested in higher-order
structures of interaction, we follow one of the lon-
gest traditions in network analysis and examine
the composition of interactional triads (Taylor
1970; Chase 1974; Hallinan 1976; Kalish and
Robins 2006). We look at triads because they
reflect forms of positioning in interaction that
extend across multiple dyads, and as such they
identify various interaction roles or local partici-
pation structures that particular individuals move
in and out of during a class period. In this way,
the study of triads provides a better means for
measuring the global structures of network
arrangement as they capture the pattern of depen-
dencies that extends beyond simple pairings of ac-
tors (Butts 2008).
5
Analysis of triads is relatively straightforward as
networks can be broken down into a finite set of 16
triad types under which all observed token triads are
subsumed (Wasserman and Faust 1994).
Conceptually, each triad type can be seen as reflect-
ing a particular interaction role. Hence, a high count
for triad number 4 (021D: ) indicates many of the
triads have central actors who speak asymmetrically
to others (i.e., talk is not reciprocated by the hearer),
much like lectures and student presentations have.
6
Recitation will include a number of direct teacher-
student interactions (i.e., question and answer), so
there are a large number of number 12 triads
(120D: ). A high count for triad number 3 (102)
and number 16 (300) reveals a great deal of cluster-
ing in interaction, a characteristic of group work. The
pattern of triads thus reflects the interaction roles
being used in a particular activity and thereby the
kind of coordinated behavior that induces discursive
stability (see Burt 1990 for a similar method).
Using this framework, we can look at the corre-
lation of triad frequencies to assess how similar or
dissimilar the pattern of triads (and thereby interac-
tion roles) is within a classroom from one moment
of class time to the next. Here, we use a very simple
measure of similarity—correlation of triadic arrays
(see the Supplemental Online Materials for
details). We do not want the size of the network
to shape the measure, nor do we want zero cells
to eclipse the estimation of variance. Since the triad
census in sparse networks always results in high
counts that would drive correlation values, we
omit null triads (triad number 1, 001) before gener-
ating correlations across successive time slices.
The resultant measure of interaction order corre-
lates the census for 30 types of triads (15 for task
and 15 for social interaction, omitting nulls) at
one time point with the one succeeding it. The mea-
sure is highly skewed (mean = .66, standard devia-
tion = .39, and median = .89), suggesting much of
schooling entails either stable or unstable patterns
of interaction roles. The plotting of this variable
over class time corresponds well with observer
field notes and recollections (see Figure A in the
Supplemental Online Materials). To utilize it in
predictive models, we binarize the measure at the
median (.89) such that values above are considered
stabilizing moments and values below are consid-
ered destabilizing moments. In doing so, we lose
some of the variance in the correlation measure,
but in return we gain reliable standard errors and
tests of significance.
Interactional concord, in contrast, is defined as
time slices that lack conflict, or what we call here
discord. Student and teacher behavior is coded as
being discordant when it entails the airing of a neg-
ative sanction. From the perspective of the student,
this can be conceptualized as an everyday form of
resistance that aims to undermine task or teacher
through the expression of discontent, challenge,
or refusal (Metz 1978; McFarland 2001). Such
acts may be active rebellions against teacher
authority, or they may be subtler in the form of
jokes or sarcastic comments. Here, a degree of
interpretation is required. If a student’s joke about
a teacher or the task stops class, it is recorded as
an instance of discord. Thus, discordant acts are
observed as intentional, publicly displayed forms
of student-initiated disruptions of classroom order,
whether they are in the form of refusal, challenge,
insult, personal ridicule, or loud complaint. Time
slices lacking any such acts, then, are coded as in-
stances of concord.
Since our measures of stability and concord are
time dependent, it is likely serial autocorrelation is
present. For research on classroom order, it is a not
a nuisance to be eliminated but rather a central fea-
ture of what we are interested in. For this reason we
incorporate autocorrelation into all our models by
332 Sociology of Education 85(4)
at WESTERN OREGON UNIVERSITY on February 10,
2015soe.sagepub.comDownloaded from
http://soe.sagepub.com/
including a lagged variable for stability and con-
cord at the previous time slice (Beck 2001).
Independent Variables: Timing, Activity,
and Discourse. A variety of independent vari-
ables is used for our different models (including,
as mentioned above, stability and concord them-
selves). In what follows, we briefly describe pre-
dictors and their hypothesized effects, but more
details about each measure are included in the
Supplemental Online Materials (see Table A).
Briefly, the three categories of independent varia-
bles we include are temporality, prescribed activ-
ity, and discursive moves.
Timing. The first set of variables captures the
effects of timing on stability. Current work sug-
gests that the opening and closing of classes are
relatively informal and disorganized phases of
interaction (Mehan 1979; Cazden 1988).
Research has also found that student engagement
starts low, grows higher as the class continues,
and then lowers again at the end of the class
(Doyle 1980:12). We normalize this intraperiod
timing variable by dividing each observation of
class time by the maximum number of minutes
the specific class period was observed. Thus, our
measure of class time varies from 0 to 1, with
0 being the start and 1 being the end. We also
include a measure for class time squared to plot
a function for nonlinear effects of class time on
stability. Other literature suggests that classes set-
tle into a routine after the first several months of
the school year, so we also include an interperiod
timing variable (Woods 1983; Doyle 1986). For
this we use coarser units of time, months into
the school year (0 = August, 1 = September,
etc.). Finally, a third timing variable concerns
transitions or slices of class time that span distinct
activity segments. Transitions are liminal mo-
ments that lack the well-defined behavioral ex-
pectations associated with participation structures.
Activity. The second set of independent varia-
bles concern teacher-prescribed activities
(Bossert 1979; Doyle 1986; Stodolsky 1988;
Stein, Grover, and Henningsen 1996). We test
the effects of activities on classroom stability by
creating nine dummy variables: lecture, recitation,
discussion, student presentations, group work,
undefined/free time, seatwork, test/quiz, and
audio/film. It is an empirical question whether
activities involving high or low levels of interac-
tivity and centralized or decentralized attention
structures are associated with greater stability
and interpersonal concord.
Discourse. The third set of variables involves
discursive moves. Within activity segments, in-
stances of talk between pairs of senders and re-
ceivers were recorded based on the identification
numbers listed on classroom seating charts. Even
though talk varied in duration, no attempt was
made to capture that information. When long turns
did arise, the observer recorded them as if the
same interaction were repeated each minute of
class time. Each interaction was coded for who
spoke to whom, the content of interactions in
terms of topic (i.e., task or sociable), and the
tone in which they were stated (i.e., none, techni-
cal, or moral) as well as the form of speech used in
the communication (see Supplemental Online
Materials, Table A). Summarizing all these fea-
tures of interaction may seem like a complex
task, but a simple shorthand captures the
information.
Take the following sequence of interaction
coded in shorthand: T-A, T-QA, A-TG, T-10Q,
10-TG, T-10 (1t), T-12Q, 12-13 S, 13-12 S, T-12
(–m), T-13 (–m), T-A. It shows the teacher speak-
ing to everyone (broadcast), asking everyone
a question, and everyone giving the teacher a choral
response. After that, the teacher asks student num-
ber 10 a question, to which he gives an answer, and
the teacher replies that student 10 is correct (‘‘1t’’ =
positive technical evaluation). The teacher then
asks student 12 a question, and student 12 turns to
socialize with student 13 (note ‘‘S’’ = social). At
this point, the teacher sanctions students 12 and
13 for their social behavior (‘‘–m’’ = negative moral
evaluation) and then turns to broadcast to the class
again.
Empirical Models: Fixed-effects Hazard
Models. Again, our first two sets of models
look at the interactional correlates of stability
and concord. First, to model the effects of activi-
ties and discourse on stability, we use conditional
logit models that render invariant characteristics
of classrooms and schools as fixed effects (see
Allison 1999:188-97). We use this model because
it corrects for standard error biases (presence of
clustering), spuriousness (i.e., correlation between
individual and cluster-level variables), and hetero-
geneity shrinkage (i.e., attenuation toward 0 in
presence of heterogeneity; see Allison 1999:212-
13). Moreover, these models present the most
Diehl and McFarland 333
at WESTERN OREGON UNIVERSITY on February 10,
2015soe.sagepub.comDownloaded from
http://soe.sagepub.com/
conservative test for the effects of micro-
mechanisms endogenous to classrooms.
The conditional logit with fixed effects can be
specified as follows:
Logðpit=ð1 � pitÞÞ¼ ai1bxit: ð1Þ
Let pit be the probability that a class is stable at
time t. Let xit be a vector of variables for classroom
i at time t. These vectors can contain variables that
differ over time (rates for forms of speech) or
remain constant (teacher’s race). The ai term repre-
sents all the differences among classrooms that are
stable over time. For all time points, ai is the same
for a given classroom, and a positive correlation
arises among observed outcomes (Allison
1999:189). When equation 1 is applied to panel
data, each classroom adds an additional parameter
(a dummy variable for each classroom) to the
model and can create biases in parameter estimates.
To solve this, conditional likelihood estimations
are constructed that condition the number of 1s and
0s observed in each classroom. If a class is stable at
time 1 and time 2 but not at time 3, one calculates
the probability that stability arose at times 1 and
2 but not at time 3. This probability is then written
as a function of the explanatory variables and the
b parameters, and the probabilities are multiplied
for all classes to get the overall likelihood. When
done, the ai parameters cancel from the likelihood
function (Allison 1999:190), and the biases in
parameter estimates are removed.
7
As a precaution,
we also ran mixed logit models and found nearly
the exact results. We rely on the results from condi-
tional models with fixed effects because they are
more conservative and robust and focus our analy-
ses within classrooms.
The eventual model estimates moments of sta-
bility and concord within classrooms and uses
lagged independent variables:
Yðt:t11ÞðstabilityÞ¼ a1b1Yðt:t�1ÞðstabilityÞ
1b2XtðtimeÞ1b3Xtðtime
2Þ
1b4XtðmonthÞ1b5XtðtransitionÞ
1b6Xtðtask structureÞ:::
1b7Xtðforms of speechÞ:::
1mtðclassroomÞ1e;
YtðconcordÞ¼ a
1b1Yt�1ðconcordÞ
1b2XtðtimeÞ
1b3Xtðtime
2Þ1b4XtðmonthÞ
1b5XtðtransitionÞ
1b6Xtðtask structureÞ:::
1b7Xtðforms of speechÞ:::
1mtðclassroomÞ1e; ð2Þ
where m is the fixed effect for each classroom.
Results: General Effects of Timing,
Activity, and Discourse. We begin by present-
ing results for unconditional mixed models of stabil-
ity and concord in Table 3. These models decompose
variance at three levels: the variance occurring
between classrooms, between periods, and within
individual class periods. The results indicate that
Table 3. Decomposition of Variance for Interactional Stability
and Concord
Level Variance component Standard error Percentage variance
Stability
Between classes 0.22 0.06*** 13
Between periods 0.53 0.06*** 31
Between segments (residual) 0.94 0.01*** 56
Concord
Between classes 0.96 0.161*** 40
Between periods 0.59 0.072*** 25
Between segments (residual) 0.82 0.012*** 35
Source: McFarland (1999) classroom interaction study.
***p  .001.
334 Sociology of Education 85(4)
at WESTERN OREGON UNIVERSITY on February 10,
2015soe.sagepub.comDownloaded from
http://soe.sagepub.com/
87 percent of the variance of stability and 60 percent
of conflict are within classrooms and class periods
(in contrast to between classrooms). This is strong
evidence that the primary levers of classroom order
reside within classroom situations and their mo-
ments, so we put our focus there.
We look first at the central tendencies of factors
we think generally influence interactional stability
and concord. These results are generated with
conditional fixed-effects models and can be found
in Table 4. For ease of interpretation, results are pre-
sented as odds ratios. Here the ratios represent the pre-
dicted odds of network pattern stability (from t to
t 11) and the presence of concord (at t) given
a one-standard-deviation change in each explanatory
variable. Odds ratios greater than 1 mean the variable
adds to pattern stability and concord, whereas being
less than 1 means it increases instability and conflict.
Table 4. Longitudinal Fixed-effects Logit Models, Predicting
Classroom Stability and Moments of Concord
(Odds)
Stability Concord
Model 1 Model 2 Model 3 Model 3 Model 4 Model 5
Timing variables
Preceding stability 1.68*** 1.66*** 1.66*** — — —
Preceding concord — — — 1.24*** 1.24*** 1.24***
Class time 1.40*** 1.29** 1.21 1.97*** 1.88*** 1.80***
Class time 3 2 0.69*** 0.81* 0.87 0.53*** 0.56*** 0.59***
Months into school year 0.92** 0.94* 0.93* 0.80*** 0.79***
0.81***
Transition segment 0.87*** 0.87*** 0.88*** 0.88*** 0.89***
0.88***
Activities
Lecture 1.58*** 1.16*** 1.00 1.01
Recitation RC RC RC RC
Discussion 0.85 0.99 0.93* 0.97
Presentation 1.09 1.06* 0.97 1.02
Group work 0.61*** 0.94* 1.11** 1.12**
Test/quiz 0.58*** 0.91** 1.18*** 1.33***
Audiovisual 0.46*** 0.88*** 1.21*** 1.34***
Seatwork 0.40*** 0.79*** 0.96 1.07
Undefined 0.46*** 0.89*** 0.96 1.03
Forms of speech
Teacher broadcast 1.11* 1.24***
Teacher question 0.91* 0.86**
Teacher technical 1.05 1.03
Teacher joke 0.93* 0.96
Student broadcast 1.00 0.96
Student question 0.97 0.91*
Student technical 1.01 1.07
Student joke 0.96 0.86***
Teacher-student task 0.92* 0.70***
Student-student task 0.89** 1.07
Teacher-student social 0.80*** 0.84***
Student-student social 0.86** 0.88
Model statistics
Wald chi-square 530 747 840 169 228 425
Source: McFarland (1999) classroom interaction study.
Note: RC = reference category. N = 8,946. Stable instances = 50
percent of cases. Conflict instances = 20 percent of
cases. Invariant characteristics of classrooms are fixed in these
models. Presented values = Exponential (bx 3 stddev x),
unless dichotomous, then the coefficient is raw. Dashes indicate
variables are not included in particular models.
*p  .05. **p  .01. ***p  .001.
Diehl and McFarland 335
at WESTERN OREGON UNIVERSITY on February 10,
2015soe.sagepub.comDownloaded from
http://soe.sagepub.com/
The first models look only at timing variables.
We start in model 1 for stability and model 4 for
concord and identify a variety of expected results.
First, stability in the preceding time slice signifi-
cantly increases the probability of future stability.
The same relationship holds for past and future
concord. Second, class time increases the odds of
both stability and concord while class time squared
decreases it, suggesting an inverse curvilinear rela-
tionship in which periods open with chaotic pat-
terns of interaction, settle into stable and
concordant instructional routines, and then grow
chaotic and conflictual again as periods draw to
a close. This intraperiod pattern is embedded in
the larger interperiod trajectory in which instability
and conflict increase during the year. We also see
that transitional periods between activities are
more likely to be unstable and discordant.
Overall, we see that stability and concord are boot-
strapped from moment to moment, have a cyclic
pattern in every class period, decrease during tran-
sitions, and slowly diminish during the year. These
results correspond with standard qualitative de-
scriptions of phases in classroom interaction (e.g.,
Doyle 1980), lending credence to the validity of
our measures as a proxy for classroom order.
These similar results suggest that there may be
parallels in how stability and concord are con-
structed and sustained during interaction. As a result
of introducing teacher-prescribed activities in mod-
els 2 and 5, however, important differences begin to
emerge. We use recitation as the baseline in our
models because not only was it the most common
activity in our data, but it is generally found to be
the most common teacher-prescribed activity in
classrooms (Mehan 1979; Cazden 1988). We find
that centralized and student-isolated instructional
formats such as lectures and seatwork channel atten-
tion and suppress interactivity and thereby forge
interactional stability and at least the appearance
of concord (i.e., compliance and a lack of conflict).
In contrast, student-centered tasks such as group
work encourage interactivity and decentralize the
focus of attention, thereby providing opportunities
for identity validation at the expense of coordination
and stability. Last, formats such as discussion
require the centralization of attention and greater in-
teractivity, thereby rendering situations more rou-
tine but at the same time increasing the possibility
of conflict by offering only limited turns of talk,
about which students may fight.
Finally, in models 3 and 6, we move to an even
more fine-grained level of analysis and include
different forms of talk. In terms of similarity across
the models, we find that teacher broadcasts are asso-
ciated with an increase in stability and concord
whereas teacher questions are associated with
a decrease in both. In the case of broadcasts, teachers
are using their talk for control by stopping other inter-
actions and centralizing attention. With questions,
however, teachers are providing students turns of
talk, which present opportunities for digression and
defiance.
The differences are perhaps more interesting,
however. While teacher jokes are associated with
a decrease in stability, they have no association
with concord. In contrast, student jokes (as well
as questions) are associated with a decrease in con-
cord but have no association with stability. We
posit that this is because teacher jokes represent di-
gressions from routine whereas student jokes and
questions may be challenges to teacher control of
attention or interactivity. Finally, neither social
nor task-related dyadic talk between students is
associated with concord, supporting the idea that
how teachers manage attention and interaction is
the key to shaping order. Teacher-student talk of
either kind, in contrast, has a significant negative
association with concord. This is, again, because
dyadic communication of all kinds between teach-
ers and students opens up the interactive space nec-
essary for conflict and defiance to occur.
Overall, the consistency of the results in Table 4
with our existing qualitative understanding of the
classroom provides a strong warrant for our
approach. In addition, they support our claim that
classroom order is multidimensional. Conflict, as
a manifestation of struggles about positions in the
classroom system, is interrelated but distinct from
the recursive patterns of interaction that generate
stability. Nonetheless, what we have presented
thus does not go far enough into the situated nature
of classroom order. These results reflect central
tendencies, but still missing is a discussion of
how problems of order are managed differently
across activities as the same discursive move can
have differential effects depending on the partici-
pation framework within which it takes place
(Tannen 1994:19). This interrelation between
activities and forms of talk is where we turn next.
Results: Interaction Terms and the
Structuring of Different Classroom
Activities. We now identify the discursive moves
utilized to overcome problems of order within
336 Sociology of Education 85(4)
at WESTERN OREGON UNIVERSITY on February 10,
2015soe.sagepub.comDownloaded from
http://soe.sagepub.com/
each activity. The full analysis can be found in the
Supplemental Online Materials (see Table B), but
for the sake of brevity here, we focus on four
activities that represent variability in interactivity
and attention: lecture, student presentations, group
work, and seatwork. As discussed earlier, control
of interactivity and attention shapes classroom
order by varying the diffusion of responsibility
for coordinating routine and the spread of identi-
ties needing to be ritually verified. Instead of
using comparison values for this part of the anal-
ysis, we focus directly on the conditional effects
of mechanisms within each type of prescribed
activity.
8
The results are presented in Table 5
and Figure 2. We will explain these results by
focusing on the pattern of odds ratios within
each activity.
The first column in Table 5 and the first set of bar
plots in Figure 2 relate results for lectures in terms of
both stability and concord. During a lecture, only the
teacher has a legitimate speaking turn, so it is not
surprising to see that teacher broadcasts are associ-
ated with an increase in both stability and concord
and that student broadcasts have the opposite effect.
Student talk is slightly related to both stability and
concord in the form of technical comments, which
can be seen as adding appropriate coauthorship to
the lecture. What is perhaps more interesting is
that social talk, either between students or with the
teacher, is associated with a decrease in stability
but not in concord. This suggests that social talk
leads to digressions from the routinized form of
the activity but not in a way that threatens the ritual
enactment of institutional roles. In contrast, student
Table 5. Conditional Effects of Speech Forms on Stability and
Concord, by Task
Lecture Presentation Group work Seatwork Main effect
Stability
Teacher broadcast 1.29*** 1.11** 0.89*** 1.00 1.11*
Teacher question 1.06y 1.05 0.96 1.01 0.91*
Teacher technical 1.02 1.00 1.00 1.00 1.05
Teacher joke 0.95 1.02 0.99 0.97 0.93*
Student broadcast 0.80*** 1.18*** 0.84* 0.92* 1.00
Student question 0.97 0.97 0.97 1.04 0.98
Student technical 1.06y 0.96 0.97 1.03 1.01
Student joke 0.97y 0.98 1.01 1.02 0.97
Teacher-student task 0.92** 0.98 1.00 1.00 0.93*
Student-student task 1.01 1.10** 1.08y 0.92** 0.89**
Teacher-student social 0.90*** 0.97* 0.95*** 0.95* 0.81***
Student-student social 0.91* 0.90** 0.99 1.03 0.86**
Concord
Teacher broadcast 1.55*** 1.21 0.55* 1.22 1.24***
Teacher question 1.07 1.04 0.99 0.97 0.86**
Teacher technical 1.10 0.90 1.32 0.97 1.03
Teacher joke 0.96 0.90 1.00 0.83* 0.96
Student broadcast 0.58* 1.21** 0.29* 0.74y 0.96
Student question 0.91 0.86 0.78 0.96 0.91*
Student technical 1.33y 1.20 0.66 1.28 1.07
Student joke 0.85* 0.94 0.80 0.89y 0.86***
Teacher-student task 0.58*** 0.69** 0.57*** 0.70*** 0.70***
Student-student task 1.00 1.31y 1.35*** 1.25* 1.07
Teacher-student social 0.95 1.07 0.99 0.95 0.84***
Student-student social 0.94 1.01 0.95 1.09 0.88
Source: McFarland (1999) classroom interaction study.
Note: N = 8,946. Stable = 50 percent of cases. Conflict = 20
percent of cases. Invariant characteristics of classrooms are
fixed. Main effects lists values from models 3 and 5 from Table
4 and reflects the norm. Presented values = Exponential
(bx 3 stddev x).
*p  .05. **p  .01. ***p  .001. yp  .10.
Diehl and McFarland 337
at WESTERN OREGON UNIVERSITY on February 10,
2015soe.sagepub.comDownloaded from
http://soe.sagepub.com/
Figure 2. Effects of discursive moves on stability and concord
by type of activity (odds)
Note: Effect = Odds of Y given 11 stddev in X (form of
speech).
*p .05, p .10. Black = Y (stability), Grey = Y (nondefiance).
338 Sociology of Education 85(4)
at WESTERN OREGON UNIVERSITY on February 10,
2015soe.sagepub.comDownloaded from
http://soe.sagepub.com/
jokes are slightly related to instability but more
strongly to conflict. This may be because teachers
see student jokes as either a threat to their control
of the activity or as an undermining of their author-
itative role.
Student presentations operate much like lectures,
with a central speaker and a largely passive audi-
ence, but students take turns at the center of the dis-
cursive network as the focus of collective attention
(although the teacher never fully relinquishes his
or her role as the central coordinator of talk). For
this reason, and in contrast to lectures, student broad-
casts (and to a lesser degree student-student task
talk) correspond with both stability and concord.
Like during a lecture, however, social talk with the
teacher or between students leads to destabilization
through digressions but not to conflict. Turning
speeches into conversations undermines the gather-
ing that is being forged around individual presenters’
identities. Finally, in student presentations, task-
related talk between teacher and student is associ-
ated with conflict because the nature of the activity
is to give students more authorship over shared
talk, and teacher-student task talk may represent at-
tempts by teachers to reassert their control over dis-
course or their ritualized role as arbiter of correct and
appropriate knowledge.
Group work is essentially the inverse of the cen-
tralized formats discussed so far in that it is a highly
interactive and decentralized task that relies on the
generation and maintenance of reciprocity within
numerous simultaneous conversations. For that rea-
son, it is stabilized by direct task communications
between students and made problematic by asymmet-
ric talk of any kind. As such, teacher involvement,
especially in the form of a broadcast, shifts interaction
into asymmetric and centralized forms that represent
a disruption in the enactment of group work roles.
Another important difference with centralized for-
mats is that group work can sustain reciprocal social
talk between peers without resulting in destabiliza-
tion. Social talk need not lead to disorder by usurping
limited turns of talk or foci of attention; rather, social
talk becomes less important as conversations and
identity verifications become diffused throughout
the classroom.
Finally, in seatwork, the task is private and sta-
bilized by nonaction between individuals, which
also means there are few opportunities for either
validation or contestation of identities. Looking at
the results, we see that seatwork is similar to group
work in that student broadcasts and teacher social-
ization, both of which shift interaction away from
activity-prescribed forms, are destabilizing. It dif-
fers from group work, however, in that task-related
talk between students is also destabilizing, which
makes sense given that a primary difference
between the two activities is the degree to which
work is independent. Seatwork is also often not
a cognitively demanding activity, and this is per-
haps part of the reason we see that reciprocal social
talk between students is associated with neither sta-
bility nor concord. Students can essentially multi-
task while working alone on routine academic
tasks and at the same time privately socialize so
long as the combination does not lead to disruption.
Nonsocial teacher talk has no effect on either stabil-
ity or concord, which suggests that most seatwork
does not require teacher assistance, but when it
does occur it has little influence on the activity.
Overall, we see that each activity has a unique
participation structure that influences how particular
forms of talk relate to meeting routine and ritual im-
peratives. The more centralized attention is and the
less interactivity is sanctioned, the easier it is for
the teacher to coordinate students and to generate
stable and concordant social order in the classroom
by controlling talk. As activities provide more possi-
bilities for interaction, the ability to display, vali-
date, and contest valued identities also increases.
When, however, interactivity increases but attention
remains centralized, conflict becomes more likely as
more is at stake for each turn of talk. In a student pre-
sentation, for example, teachers must coordinate stu-
dent talk but be on guard to prevent students from
using their turn as a platform for ritual work.
When interactivity increases and attention becomes
decentralized, as in group work, we see less conflict
as discursive opportunities for ritual verification
abound and, with most interaction taking place out-
side the view of the teacher, less ability to keep it on
task. What we see, in other words, is a series of trade-
offs between activities concerning how well they are
able to meet the different interactional imperatives
of classroom order. Remiss in these analyses, how-
ever, is some sense of the participants’ subjective ex-
periences in the classroom.
PART B. THE EFFECT OF
INTERACTIONAL STABILITY AND
CONCORD
Now that we have looked at how the shifting inter-
relation of talk and activity influences stability and
concord in the classroom, we next look at whether
Diehl and McFarland 339
at WESTERN OREGON UNIVERSITY on February 10,
2015soe.sagepub.comDownloaded from
http://soe.sagepub.com/
meeting routine and ritual imperatives has a distinct
effect on the duration of classroom activities and
the experiences and attitudes of teachers and stu-
dents. Important to note, by looking at teacher
and student attitudes, we are able to study the ef-
fects of ritual features of interaction in terms of
not only the visible manifestations of compliance
discussed above but also the subjective experiences
of students and teachers associated with role
embracement and collective goodwill.
Task Duration. Our first outcome of interest
concerns the duration of classroom tasks. Prior
research has identified a connection between
maintaining activities and learning (Stallings
1980), and we attempt to build on that connection
here. We believe that the degree of interactional
stability and concord will be positively associated
with longer tasks and that the degree of interac-
tional instability and discord will be associated
with shorter tasks that fail to last. We also suspect
that distinct types of tasks will be more sensitive
to issues of stability than concord and vice versa.
To study the effects of stability and concord on
task duration, our unit of analysis shifts to the individ-
ual task segments. In the McFarland Classroom
Study, there are 1,501 separate task segments (token
tasks). We measure the duration in minutes of class
time of each segment, s. This is our dependent variable
reflective of task duration (Y[Task durationsj] = time
of segment s in class j). Next, we want to develop pre-
dictors for interaction stability and concord so as to
identify their effect on task duration. We measure
the stability of each task as the percentage of time
the token segment was stable (%Stablesj), and we
measure the concord in each task as the percentage
of time the segment lacked conflict (%Concordsj).
Task duration is a skewed metric, so it is log
transformed and predicted using ordinary least
squares regression. However, we are interested in
identifying the main effect of our predictors (b1)
as well as their conditional effects within each
type of activity (b2). As such, we include an inter-
action term by activity type (b3).
YðTask durationsjÞ¼ asj1b1
ð%StablesjÞ1b2ðActivity TypesjÞ
1b3ð%Stablesj*Activity TypesjÞ: ð3Þ
As a more conservative test, we add fixed ef-
fects for classrooms (mj) onto equation 3, thereby
removing exogenous factors also omitted in prior
models.
Results: The Impact of Instability and
Conflict on Activity Length. Turning now to
Table 6, we examine whether the presence of
instability or conflict significantly influences
task duration. Overall, we see that longer activities
are generally more stable and concordant whereas
shorter activities are more unstable and conflic-
tual. Even in the fixed-effects models, these gen-
eral results hold. Notably, interactional stability
is more associated with task duration than con-
cord. Results for each type of activity suggest
some variability, however. For example, activities
with set clocks are least influenced by unstable
interaction (e.g., tests and videos have a prescribed
length from which it is hard to deviate). In con-
trast, concord appears to have greater influence
on centralized activities. This is likely because
defiant acts cannot be as easily ignored in central-
ized compared to decentralized activities. This
means teachers may be less likely to perceive mo-
ments of conflict as a threat to classroom order if
they take place outside of shared attention.
Student and Teacher Attitudes. Our second
outcome of interest concerns student and teacher
attitudes. We hypothesize that interactional stabil-
ity will be associated with positive teacher atti-
tudes and interactional concord will be
associated with positive student attitudes. This is
because teachers are mostly concerned with the
accomplishment of routines whereas student expe-
rience is more closely tied to feelings associated
with the violations of self that often occur in the
service of accomplishing routines.
To test these ideas, we shift to individual-level
outcomes and use stability and concord as class-
room-level predictors. Where possible, we focus
on longitudinal outcomes spanning the observation
period of first and second semester. For student at-
titudes, we use students’ semester and yearly re-
ports on the quality of their classes and course
difficulty levels. Sample sizes differ depending
on whether we use the larger cross-sectional sam-
ple (1,021 to 1,095 reports on our 152 classrooms)
or the smaller longitudinal survey sample (443 per-
sons in 24 focal classrooms). We will briefly relate
the construction of variables below and direct the
reader’s attention to Table C in the Supplemental
Online Materials for details.
340 Sociology of Education 85(4)
at WESTERN OREGON UNIVERSITY on February 10,
2015soe.sagepub.comDownloaded from
http://soe.sagepub.com/
We construct two variables that reflect students’
perceptions of a good class. The first uses longitu-
dinal information that is ‘‘scoped’’ to the classroom
setting. The measure is a sum of how much students
liked the class, teacher, teaching style, and subject
and the grades they expected to get. All these items
were highly correlated and formed a simple addi-
tive composite with a Cronbach’s alpha of .81
(Good class v1ij). The second measure is drawn
from a larger, cross-sectional sample; items con-
cern how much students liked the class, the amount
they learned, the quality of teaching, their interest
in the subject, and the inverse-ranked liking of
the class. These variables also were highly corre-
lated and formed a simple additive composite
with a Cronbach’s alpha of .76 (Good class v2ij).
As a contrast to students’ perceptions of good clas-
ses, we looked at student reports about how diffi-
cult each class was (Class difficultyij). The reason
for including class difficulty is that a stable class
may be considered difficult or boring.
To capture teacher attitudes, we sought measures
reflecting their views of students and their content-
ment with their teaching role. This sample corre-
sponds with the observed 150 classrooms minus
cases with missing data. The first dependent variable
measures the perceived quality of teachers’ students.
It is a sum of four highly correlated items concerning
how often the teacher believed each student was on-
task or socializing, their academic ability, and their
expected grade (scales are listed in Table 6). These
variables form another simple additive component
that has a Cronbach’s alpha of .84 (Good studentij).
The second dependent variable reflects teachers’
contentment with their role (Role-Contentmentij).
It is derived from a cross-sectional teacher survey
wherein 1 = the teacher’s report of not wanting to
teach a different subject, or at a different school, or
to be in a different profession, and 0 is coded for
any expression of role discontent. Of the 151 teach-
ers surveyed, only 99 had no desire to change their
subject, school, or career, whereas 52 did.
9
The samples for these models are relatively small
in comparison with the models of interaction
dynamics. Hence, we are limited in the number of
predictors we can include. As a proxy for task struc-
tures, we use the average percentage of students who
spoke in all observed class slices (%Class Talkingij).
As this variable gets larger, it reflects classes that use
a more open, student-centered task structure, and as
it gets smaller, it reflects classes that constrain
access to the floor (lecture, exams). Our other pre-
dictors reflect our focal theoretical variables of inter-
action stability and concord (Class Stabilityij and
Class Concordij). As with task segments in the prior
section, we use the percentage of observed class
Table 6. Conditional Effects of Stability and Concord on Task
Duration (by Type of Task)
Percentage of
segment that
is stable
Percentage of
segment that
has concord Task length
Estimated
coefficient Fixed
Estimated
coefficient Fixed Mean
Standard
deviation n
Main effect .14*** .14*** .04y .07** 13.38 11.48 1,501
Lecture .31*** .39*** .25*** .29*** 9.32 9.24 203
Recitation .17*** .18*** .22*** .21*** 15.68 11.67 526
Discussion .29*** .32*** .26*** .26** 11.60 10.23 78
Student
presentation
.03 .17 .13 .10 17.32 12.93 70
Group work .32*** .26** .05 .03 12.09 8.71 116
Test/quiz .14y .14 .35*** .35*** 20.58 13.25 73
Audiovisual .20* .11 .18* .16y 16.25 13.80 62
Seatwork .20*** .19* –.03 .03 13.81 11.14 229
Undefined .31*** .27*** .25*** .26*** 5.23 6.70 144
Source: McFarland (1999) classroom interaction study.
Note: N = 1,501 task segments (models fix classroom traits).
All models are standardized (Y) and exponentiated (X 3
task type). Stability: mean = .48, standard deviation = .33.
Nondefiance: mean = .78, standard deviation = .28.
*p  .05. **p  .01. ***p  .001. yp  .10.
Diehl and McFarland 341
at WESTERN OREGON UNIVERSITY on February 10,
2015soe.sagepub.comDownloaded from
http://soe.sagepub.com/
time (as opposed to segment time) that is stable or
concordant as our measure.
We model student and teacher attitudes using
multilevel linear regression models that include
random intercepts for classrooms (equation 4). At
level 1, we have the attitude variable Y for individ-
ual i in classroom j. The attitude Y is equal to the
average outcome in the classroom plus a variety
of student-level covariates (above variables) and
a classroom-level error term. At the individual
level, we interact our key features of stability and
concord with task variability (here %Class
Talkingij 3 Class Concordij). These interactions
illustrate whether the effects of concord and stabil-
ity vary by task structure as hypothesized.
YðattitudeÞij ¼ b0j1b1jð%Class TalkingijÞ
1b2jðClass StableijÞ1b3jð%Class Talkingij
3Class StableijÞ
1b4jðClass ConcordijÞ1b5jð%Class Talkingij
3Class ConcordijÞ ð4Þ
At level 2, we treat classrooms as random
effects.
b0j ¼ g001y0j
Here, g00 is the average outcome of the population,
and y0j is the classroom-specific effect. The multi-
level framework allows us to take into account
response clustering within classrooms.
Results: Teacher and Student Attitudes
and Preferences. The findings in Table 7 indi-
cate that classroom stability and concord differen-
tially shape the attitudes and experiences of
teachers and students, lending credence to our
claim that they have different interests and mo-
tives that may work at cross-purposes. Students
are more likely to report liking classes with higher
levels of instability. Teachers, in contrast, are
more likely to report being content in their jobs
when they teach stable classes. This suggests stu-
dents like nonroutine classes wherein control is
loose and interaction variable, whereas teachers
prefer classes in which they are able to manage
student behavior in a routinized manner.
When looking at interactional concord, we see
the opposite pattern. Students report liking classes
marked by concord, which our theory would sug-
gest is associated with respect for identity and
more opportunities for discursive authorship. This
view is given extra credence by the significant
interaction between classroom concord and the per-
centage of the class talking. In other words, stu-
dents like classes in which there are many
opportunities for talk and those opportunities do
not lead to conflict over identity. For teachers, the
relationship is more complicated. Their perception
of what constitutes good students is related to a lack
of classroom conflict but not instability. Yet at the
same time, the more teachers report having classes
marked by interactional concord but little talk (or
compliance), the less likely they are to report con-
tentment in their role. To a teacher, then, a class of
nondefiant, passive students may be perceived as
being good but not interesting. The significant in-
teractions between the percentage of the class talk-
ing and both perception of good students and role
contentment suggests that teachers prefer engaged
students who do not challenge their authority.
We can see general consistency in the results on
task survival and teacher and student attitudes.
First, teachers and students have different feelings
about stable interaction. Teachers prefer stability,
partly because it is associated with activity length,
which can be seen as a proxy for fulfilling their
institutional role. Students, in contrast, find routine
tasks to be boring and stable classes to be difficult
(student perceptions about which activities they
find boring can be seen in Figure 3). Moreover, stu-
dents prefer interaction that lacks conflict, which is
generally found in classes in which they do not vie
with teachers about the control of talk. Teachers in
turn prefer engaged but nondefiant students.
From a ritual perspective, then, it is not the case
that teachers want to suppress student identity but
rather that the importance of completing activities
sometimes necessitates it. Overall, these results sug-
gest that behaviors and attitudes in the classroom are
significantly shaped by the nature of moment-to-
moment interaction. Rather than placing the burden
of explaining classroom experience on exogenous fac-
tors of the school and setting, we can begin to see how
it is dynamically shaped by the nature of the micro-
situations inhabited by teachers and students.
DISCUSSION
In summary, we present multiple findings about
social order in classrooms. Our analyses in part A
342 Sociology of Education 85(4)
at WESTERN OREGON UNIVERSITY on February 10,
2015soe.sagepub.comDownloaded from
http://soe.sagepub.com/
T
a
b
le
7
.
T
h
e
R
e
la
ti
o
n
o
f
C
la
ss
ro
o
m
S
ta
b
ili
ty
an
d
N
o
n
d
e
fi
an
ce
to
S
tu
d
e
n
t
an
d
T
e
ac
h
e
r
A
tt
it
u
d
e
s
S
tu
d
e
n
t
at
ti
tu
d
e
s
T
e
ac
h
e
r
at
ti
tu
d
e
s
G
o
o
d
cl
as
s
v1
G
o
o
d
cl
as
s
v2
C
la
ss
d
if
fi
cu
lt
y
G
o
o
d
st
u
d
e
n
ts
R
o
le
co
n
te
n
tm
e
n
t
E
x
p
la
n
at
o
ry
va
ri
ab
le
E
st
im
at
e
d
co
e
ff
ic
ie
n
t
S
ta
n
d
ar
d
e
rr
o
r
E
st
im
at
e
d
co
e
ff
ic
ie
n
t
S
ta
n
d
ar
d
e
rr
o
r
E
st
im
at
e
d
co
e
ff
ic
ie
n
t
S
ta
n
d
ar
d
e
rr
o
r
E
st
im
at
e
d
co
e
ff
ic
ie
n
t
S
ta
n
d
ar
d
e
rr
o
r
E
st
im
at
e
d
co
e
ff
ic
ie
n
t
S
ta
n
d
ar
d
e
rr
o
r
C
la
ss
ro
o
m
le
ve
l
(c
la
ss
av
e
ra
ge
)
In
te
rc
e
p
t
.0
5
.0
7
–
.0
9
.0
6
.2
8
.0
5
*
*
*
–
.2
5
.0
7
*
*
.9
2
.2
2
*
*
*
P
e
rc
e
n
ta
ge
o
f
cl
as
s
ta
lk
in
g
.1
1
.1
7
–
.0
3
.0
6
.1
4
.0
6
*
–
.2
3
.1
5
.5
7
.2
9
y
S
ta
b
ili
ty
–
.3
0
.1
2
*
–
.0
2
.0
7
.1
0
.0
6
y
–
.0
3
.0
9
.6
3
.2
4
*
*
S
ta
b
ili
ty
3
P
e
rc
e
n
ta
ge
o
f
C
la
ss
T
al
k
in
g
–
.0
4
.3
1
–
.0
9
.0
5
*
*
.1
0
.0
5
y
–
.2
3
.2
6
.0
2
.2
4
C
o
n
co
rd
.3
0
.1
6
y
.2
4
.0
6
*
*
*
–
.0
2
.0
6
.3
5
.1
2
*
–
.4
8
.2
5
*
C
o
n
co
rd
3
P
e
rc
e
n
ta
ge
o
f
C
la
ss
T
al
k
in
g
.2
5
.3
6
.1
3
.0
5
.0
0
.0
5
.6
0
.2
9
y
.8
0
.3
1
*
In
d
iv
id
u
al
le
ve
l
P
ri
o
r
se
m
e
st
e
r
at
ti
tu
d
e
s
.6
3
.0
4
*
*
*
—
—
.8
0
.0
4
*
*
*
—
V
ar
ia
n
ce
e
x
p
la
in
e
d
B
e
tw
e
e
n
cl
as
sr
o
o
m
s
(m
u
lt
ile
ve
l)
.8
8
.2
6
.0
6
.7
9
—
W
it
h
in
cl
as
sr
o
o
m
s
(m
u
lt
ile
ve
l)
.3
6
.0
0
.0
0
.5
4
—
T
o
ta
l
va
ri
an
ce
(m
u
lt
ile
ve
l)
.5
5
.0
9
.0
2
.5
7
—
M
ax
-r
e
sc
al
e
d
R
-s
q
u
ar
e
(l
o
gi
t)
—
—
—
—
.2
2
N
u
m
b
e
r
o
f
o
b
se
rv
at
io
n
s
/
cl
as
sr
o
o
m
s
4
4
3
/2
4
1
0
2
1
/1
5
2
1
0
9
5
/1
5
2
4
0
4
/1
4
5
1
5
1
/1
5
1
S
o
u
rc
e
:
M
cF
ar
la
n
d
(1
9
9
9
)
cl
as
sr
o
o
m
in
te
ra
ct
io
n
st
u
d
y.
N
o
te
:
A
ll
m
o
d
e
ls
re
p
o
rt
st
an
d
ar
d
iz
e
d
re
su
lt
s.
D
as
h
e
s
in
d
ic
at
e
va
ri
ab
le
s
ar
e
n
o
t
in
cl
u
d
e
d
in
p
ar
ti
cu
la
r
m
o
d
e
ls
.
*
p

.0
5
.
*
*
p

.0
1
.
*
*
*
p

.0
0
1
.
y
p

.1
0
.
343
at WESTERN OREGON UNIVERSITY on February 10,
2015soe.sagepub.comDownloaded from
http://soe.sagepub.com/
reveal the multidimensional and situated nature of
classroom order. First, we find a strong relation
between temporality and classroom order that is
consistent with prior qualitative work and reinfor-
ces the validity of our models. Second, we find
that most of the variance in stability and concord
occurs within classroom contexts and their mo-
ments. As such, the primary issue of classroom
order is often situational. Third, we find that activ-
ities often dictate the overall levels of stability and
concord in participant behaviors and that they often
work at cross-purposes. Activities that require
greater interactivity and distribute attention evenly
are harder to stabilize but less conflictual. By con-
trast, activities that are less interactive and focus
attention unevenly are easier to stabilize and
acquire compliance (but not goodwill). Fourth,
we find that the stability and concord within activ-
ities entail situated usage of discursive moves. The
use of a joke in lectures and seatwork, for example,
has different effects on the stability and concord in
those particular situations.
Our analyses in part B concern the effects of sta-
bility and concord on instruction and the attitudes
of classroom participants. Our analyses of task
duration find that in general, stable and concordant
activities last longer. The relationship is weaker,
however, for activities such as tests and videos
that have more rigid starting and ending times.
When the degree of interactivity is high and/or
attention is centralized on the teacher, activities
are more sensitive to issues of stability. The
influence of concord, in contrast, is weaker in activ-
ities wherein students have the opportunity to
engage in decentralized conversations. Next, we
find that teacher attitudes respond to issues of sta-
bility and student attitudes to issues of concord.
Such biases reflect a central tendency across les-
sons. Teachers adopt more transmittal formats of
instruction that diminish interactivity and central-
ize attention on themselves so that collective mobi-
lization and compliance occurs. This places
students in a consistently nonactive role and deval-
ues identity. What we learn is that our analyses of
concord in part A may often reflect compliance
more than goodwill and that we need to take into
account participants’ attitudes. Once we do, we
see that teachers want more than mere compliance;
they want stability with student role embracement.
CONCLUSION
In this article, we have attempted to reconceptual-
ize the problem of classroom social order. Prior
work in the sociology of education explains class-
room order in terms of factors that are static across
classroom moments, such as the composition of
students (Dreeben and Barr 1988; McFarland
2001) and characteristics of teachers (Pianta and
Hamre 2009), the mismatch between adolescent
and academic cultures (Willis 1977; Woods
1983), and the organizational features of the class-
room setting (McFarland 2005). In contrast, we
have located important interactional dynamics
between teachers and students within the discur-
sively constructed structures of classroom situa-
tions themselves. In doing so we build off of
Goffman’s insight that ‘‘the proper study of interac-
tion is not the individual and his psychology, but
rather the syntactical relations among acts of differ-
ent persons mutually present to one another . . . not,
then, men and their moments. Rather, moments and
their men’’ (Goffman 1967:3).
In the classroom, this means seeing the motiva-
tions and interests of students and teachers not as
essential features of their characters or roles but
rather as the result of their shifting positions in
changing activity structures and discourse.
In this view, problems and perturbations in
classroom order are largely a function of the inter-
relation of discourse and task in particular social
situations. Each activity and form of talk cues dif-
ferent participation statuses, and with those come
different experiences and obligations for stability
and concord. By altering the task and the discursive
0%
10%
20%
30%
40%
50%
60%
70%
80%
90%
%
o
f A
ct
iv
iti
es
C
on
si
de
re
d
"B
or
in
g"
Figure 3. Percentage of activities students con-
sider boring
344 Sociology of Education 85(4)
at WESTERN OREGON UNIVERSITY on February 10,
2015soe.sagepub.comDownloaded from
http://soe.sagepub.com/
cues, the situation changes, and another arrange-
ment of participation statuses arises along with dif-
ferent potential affronts to activity and self (i.e.,
routine and ritual). It is only in the consistent selec-
tion of activities such as recitation, lecture, and
seatwork that classroom actors take up orientations
toward one another and a more abstract sense of the
institutional roles of teacher and student forms.
One might infer from our results that the best
course for educators interested in improving experi-
ence for students is to shift focus away from stability
and routine and toward building concord and ritual
in the classroom. Similar prescriptions were made
with student-centered instruction and youth empow-
erment (Swidler 1979; Cohen and Lotan 1997).
Opposite prescriptions are made by traditionalists
arguing centralized activities have greater returns
to achievement (Adams and Engelmann 1996).
The findings of this article, however, suggest that
any either-or prescription is problematic.
Successful situations must be ‘‘worked,’’ and inter-
actional requirements must be balanced so as best
to accomplish collective mobilization and rapport.
This suggests part of the reason reform efforts argu-
ing for one direction over another tend not to suc-
ceed. Rather than pitting the efficacy of traditional
and progressive pedagogies against each other,
then, researchers should be investigating how differ-
ent patterns in the usage of types of academic activ-
ities create variable classroom experiences. Instead
of promoting single instructional formats, the
research presented here suggests educators may
want to consider the alternation of divergent forms.
Using decentralized and centralized tasks in cycles
may meet situational requirements of routine and rit-
ual in an oscillating manner.
The work presented here is both theoretically
and empirically derived; however, it is not without
its shortcomings. Future work can improve on what
was presented here by collecting better data. In par-
ticular, the coordination of routine is more than
a network mobilization effort; it is also a topical
corralling effort. As such, future work would do
well to collect greater detail on the topic of talk,
especially as it pertains to subject matter
(Stodolsky 1988). In contrast, the normative or rit-
ual requirements of situations are exemplified by
more than the lack of discord but often are observed
in nonverbal communication such as body posi-
tioning and facial gestures seen in video recordings
(Bremme and Erickson 1977; Goodwin 1998).
Likewise, research that makes use of audio
recordings can help us understand the role of pro-
sodic, emotive features of talk such as pitch, fre-
quency, and synchronicity (Jurafsky, Ranganath,
and McFarland 2009).
Our data do not contain information about the
duration of talk and therefore lack the truly continu-
ous notion of time and interaction that would allow
for real integration between network and conversa-
tion analysis (Moody et al. 2005; Gibson 2008). In
addition, our distinction between dyadic and broad-
cast forms of speech is too coarse. Private interac-
tions reflect dyad-specific interactions, but public
interactions involve bystanders (i.e., those who
hear talk but are not its intended recipients). As
such, it is a local broadcast and therefore distinct
not only from private chains of concealed talk but
from global broadcasts of indirect speech. By ren-
dering interaction into these distinct network arenas,
we may fundamentally alter the results. Such render-
ing requires data on physical layouts, something
already being integrated into this type of research
(Eagle, Pentland, and Lazer 2009).
In spite of the aforementioned data limitations,
the data used here remain unparalleled in education
research and offer a novel glimpse into the interac-
tional construction of classroom social order.
Future research would do well to look at how
planned changes in classroom practices reshape
the interrelation of routine and ritual features of
interaction, often in unintentional ways. Studying
the dynamics laid out in this article during instances
of reform can offer a new way of studying the fail-
ure or success of change efforts. Future work
should look at the same activities across a wide
range of school settings and grades. Doing so
would provide new and important perspectives on
some of the most central questions in the sociology
of education including the processes through which
students are socialized into school practices and the
micro-interactional sources of inequality and dif-
ferences in educational outcomes.
ACKNOWLEDGMENTS
Author names are presented in alphabetical order. The
work here has benefitted tremendously from a series of
anonymous reviewers, as well as participants in
Stanford’s Workshop on Networks, History and Theories
of Action, Duke’s Workshop on Social Structure, and the
helpful advice of Charles Bidwell, David Gibson and
James Moody. We also thank Rachel Lotan for providing
classroom video data and David Suarez for coding
assistance.
Diehl and McFarland 345
at WESTERN OREGON UNIVERSITY on February 10,
2015soe.sagepub.comDownloaded from
http://soe.sagepub.com/
FUNDING
The authors disclosed receipt of the following financial
support for the research, authorship, and/or publication
of this article: The research presented here is supported
by NSF award 0624134, the Spencer Foundation, and
Stanford University’s Office of Technology and
Licensing (grant# 2-CDZ-108).
NOTES
1. Notably, both sociolinguists and interactionists draw
extensively on Goffman in their work (e.g., Erickson
1982; Pace 2006), but each research stream tends to
addresses only part of his overall framework.
2. We will use activity framework and participation
framework interchangeably. For ease of understand-
ing, the reader may regard frameworks as reflecting
schemas and expectations and structures as enacted
forms that come about (Diehl and McFarland 2010).
These are not always consistent due to competing
activity frameworks.
3. More classes were observed at Rural because it was
a larger school where more classes had to be observed
so as to get multiple observations of individuals. In
addition, more math and science classes were
observed because they were yearlong courses with
more stable populations than courses in other subjects.
There was an effort to observe an equal numbers of
courses across tracks, but some were not offered for
basic students (and no basic courses existed at
Magnet), so that track is slightly underrepresented.
Around 60 percent of the classes observed were in
the regular track, 36 percent were in the accelerated
track, and only 4 percent were from the basic track
at Rural High. In spite of these biases, this is a sizeable
sample of classes within which we can study the pro-
cess of interaction ordering.
4. Our goal is to model interaction dynamics and identify
situational levers patterning them. To this end, a slice
of class time is sought that is longer than a single turn
and shorter than an activity segment. If too narrow, we
lose the patterning of association, and if too wide, we
confound the influence of multiple segments. We tried
windows varying from one to four minutes and found
two- to three-minute windows best fit our research
focus. That said, results for one to four minutes are
qualitatively consistent and do not change the article’s
story.
5. An elucidation of the difference between the standard
study of dyads and our approach of studying triads can
be found in looking at the participation structure of
student presentations. When teachers call on students
to present their work, the students take turns making
broadcast statements to the entire class, and the
teacher acts as master of ceremonies and discussant.
As each student speaks, he or she commences a revolu-
tion in who talks to whom (dyads) even though partic-
ipants consistently enact the same interaction roles
and smoothly maintain the activity. Measures that
focus only on patterns of dyadic talk do not capture
the stable enactment of this type of activity because
they fail to capture whether classroom participants
are enacting stable interaction patterns, or interaction
roles. By contrast, conceptualizing the participation
structure in terms of triadic forms allows us to identify
consistent interactional roles underlying the activity
despite the contingent movements of particular actors
through those roles.
6. Triad labels follow the MAN acronym. M = number of
mutual ties, A = number of asymmetric ties, and N =
number of null ties. Hence, the label 020 refers to tri-
ads with two asymmetric ties. The suffixes of triad
types refer to directions of up (U), down (D), cyclic
(C), and transitive (T). Transitivity is a simple law:
if a �! b and b �! c, then a �! c. As such, a triad
is transitive when a friend of my friend is also my
friend (a general network property).
7. SAS syntax for these models is as follows: PROC
PHREG DATA=datasetname NOSUMMARY;
MODEL stability = x1 x2 x3 . . . xn /
TIES=DISCRETE; STRATA classrooms; RUN;
8. We add all interaction terms to models 5 (for dis-
course) and 6 (for dyadic formats) in Table 2. We
relied on Jaccard’s description of modeling interaction
terms using qualitative and quantitative variables
(Jaccard 2001:30-34). When exploring the effect of
a variable such as task reciprocity within various
activity frames, it is helpful to report the conditional
effect task reciprocity has within each activity. This
is the effect of task reciprocity when the moderator
variables value is 0. Hence, we report the effect of
task reciprocity on recitation by determining task rec-
iprocity’s coefficient net of interactions with all other
types of activity structures.
9. The role-contentment variable is assessed by a logit
model that is run at the classroom level, Log (pi /
(1–pi)), where pi is the probability of role contentment
for teacher i.
REFERENCES
Adams, Gary and Siegfried Engelmann. 1996. Research
on Direct Instruction: 25 Years beyond DISTAR.
Seattle, WA: Educational Achievement Systems.
Allison, Paul D. 1999. Logistic Regression Using the SAS
System: Theory and Application. Cary, NC: SAS.
Alpert, Bracha. 1991. ‘‘Students’ Resistance in the
Classroom.’’ Anthropology and Education Quarterly
22(4):350-66.
Battistich, Victor, Daniel Solomon, Marilyn Watson, and
Eric Schaps. 1997. ‘‘Caring School Communities.’’
Educational Psychologist 32(3):137-51.
346 Sociology of Education 85(4)
at WESTERN OREGON UNIVERSITY on February 10,
2015soe.sagepub.comDownloaded from
http://soe.sagepub.com/
Beck, Nathaniel. 2001. ‘‘Time-series-cross-section Data:
What Have We Learned in the Last Few Years?’’
Annual Review of Political Science 4:271-93.
Bender-deMoll, Skye and Daniel A. McFarland. 2006.
‘‘The Art and Science of Dynamic Network
Visualization.’’ Journal of Social Structure 7(2).
Retrieved.
Bidwell, Charles E. 1965. ‘‘The School as a Formal
Organization.’’ Pp. 972-1022 in Handbook of
Organizations, edited by James P. March. Chicago:
Rand McNally.
Bidwell, Charles E. 2005. ‘‘A Sociological Agenda for
Research on Education.’’ Pp. 15-36 in The Social
Organization of Schooling, edited by Larry Hedges
and Barbara Schneider. New York: Russell Sage.
Boocock, Sarane. 1978. ‘‘The Social Organization of the
Classroom.’’ Annual Review of Sociology 4:1-28.
Bossert, Steven T. 1979. Tasks and Social Relationships
in Classrooms. New York: Cambridge University
Press.
Bremme, Donald W. and Frederick Erickson. 1977.
‘‘Relationships among Verbal and Nonverbal
Classroom Behaviors.’’ Theory into Practice 16(3):
153-61.
Brookover, Wilbur. 1945. ‘‘The Relation of Social
Factors to Teaching Ability.’’ Journal of
Experimental Education 13(4):191-205.
Burt, Ronald S. 1990. ‘‘Detecting Role Equivalence.’’
Social Networks 12(1):83-97.
Butts, Carter T. 2008. ‘‘A Relational Event Framework
for Social Action.’’ Sociological Methodology 38(1):
155-200.
Cameron, Claire, Carol Connor, and Frederick Morrison.
2005. ‘‘Effects of Variation in Teacher Organization
on Classroom Functioning.’’ Journal of School
Psychology 43(1):61-85.
Cazden, Courtney B. 1988. Classroom Discourse.
Portsmouth, NH: Heinemann.
Chase, Ivan D. 1974. ‘‘Models of Hierarchy Formation in
Animal Societies.’’ Behavioral Science 19(6):374-82.
Cogan, Morris L. 1956. ‘‘Theory and Design of a Study of
Teacher-Pupil Interaction.’’ Harvard Educational
Review 26(4):315-42.
Cohen, Elizabeth G. and Rachel A. Lotan. 1997. Working
for Equity in Heterogeneous Classrooms:
Sociological Theory in Practice. Sociology of
Education Series. New York: Teachers College Press.
Coleman, James. 1961. The Adolescent Society: The
Social Life of the Teenager and Its Impact on
Education. New York: Free Press of Glencoe.
Diehl, David and Daniel McFarland. 2010. ‘‘Toward
a Historical Sociology of Situations.’’ American
Journal of Sociology 115(6):1713-52.
Doyle, Walter. 1980. Classroom Management. West
Lafayette, IN: Kappa Delta Pi.
Doyle, Walter. 1986. ‘‘Classroom Organization and
Management.’’ Pp. 392-431 in Handbook of
Research on Teaching, edited by M. C. Wittrock.
New York: Macmillan.
Doyle, Walter and Kathy Carter. 1984. ‘‘Academic Tasks
in Classrooms.’’ Curriculum Inquiry 14(2):129-49.
Dreeben, Robert. 1970. The Nature of Teaching.
Glenview, IL: Scott Foresman.
Dreeben, Robert and Rebecca Barr. 1988. ‘‘Classroom
Composition and the Design of Instruction.’’
Sociology of Education 61(3):129-42.
Eagle, Nathan, Alex (Sandy) Pentland, and David Lazer.
2009. ‘‘Inferring Friendship Network Structure by
Using Mobile Phone Data.’’ Proceedings of the
National Academy of Sciences 106(36):15274.
Eder, Donna and Janet Enke. 1991. ‘‘The Structure of
Gossip: Opportunities and Constraints on Collective
Expression amongst Adolescents.’’ American
Sociological Review 56(4):494-508.
Erickson, Frederick. 1982. ‘‘Classroom Discourse as
Improvisation: Relationships between Academic
Task Structure and Social Participation Structure in
Lessons.’’ Pp. 153-81 in Communicating in the
Classroom, edited by L. C. Wilkinson. New York:
Academic.
Erickson, Frederick. 2004. Talk and Social Theory:
Ecologies of Speaking and Listening in Everyday
Life. Malden, MA: Polity Press.
Fine, Gary Alan and Philip Manning. 2000. ‘‘Erving
Goffman: Shifting Impressions and Tight Frames.’’
Pp. 457-85 in Blackwell Companion to Major Social
Theorists, edited by George Ritzer. Oxford,
England: Blackwell.
Gamoran, Adam and Sean Kelly. 2003. ‘‘On What Is
Learned in School to How Schools Work: Learning
and Teaching in Secondary School English
Classrooms.’’ Pp. 109-26 in Stability and Change in
American Education: Structure, Processes and
Outcomes, edited by Maureen T. Hallinan, Adam
Gamoran, Warren Kubitschek, and Tom Loveless.
Clinton Corners, NY: Eliot Werner.
Gee, John Paul. 1999. An Introduction to Discourse
Analysis: Theory and Method. New York: Routledge.
Gibson, David R. 2003. ‘‘Participation Shifts: Order and
Differentiation in Group Conversation.’’ Social
Forces 81(4):1335-80.
Gibson, David R. 2008. ‘‘How the Outside Gets In:
Modeling Conversational Permeation.’’ Annual
Review of Sociology 34:359-84.
Goffman, Erving. 1967. Interaction Ritual: Essays on
Face-to-face Behavior. New York: Pantheon.
Goffman, Erving. 1974. Frame Analysis: An Essay on the
Organization of Experience. New York: Harper and
Row.
Goffman, Erving. 1981. Forms of Talk. Philadelphia:
University of Pennsylvania Press.
Goffman, Erving. 1983. ‘‘Presidential Address: The
Interaction Order.’’ American Sociological Review
48(1):1-17.
Diehl and McFarland 347
at WESTERN OREGON UNIVERSITY on February 10,
2015soe.sagepub.comDownloaded from
http://soe.sagepub.com/
Good, Thomas L., Catherine Mulryan, and Mary
McCaslin. 2006. ‘‘Grouping for Instruction in
Mathematics: A Call for Programmatic Research
on Small-group Processes.’’ Pp. 165-96 in
Handbook of Research on Mathematics Teaching
and Learning, edited by D. Grouws. New York:
Macmillan.
Goodwin, Marjorie H. 1998. ‘‘Games of Stance: Conflict
and Footing in Hopscotch.’’ Pp. 23-46 in Kid’s Talk:
Strategic Language Use in Later Childhood, edited
by Susan Hoyle and Carolyn Temple Adger. New
York: Oxford University Press.
Gordon, Calvin Wayne. 1957. The Social System of the
High School: A Study in the Sociology of
Adolescence. Glencoe, IL: Free Press.
Grenfell, Michael. 1998. ‘‘Language in the Classroom.’’
Pp. 71-88 in Bourdieu and Education, edited by
Michael Grenfell and David James. Bristol, PA:
Falmer Press.
Hallinan, Maureen T. 1976. ‘‘Friendship Patterns in Open
and Traditional Classrooms.’’ Sociology of Education
49(4):254-65.
Jaccard, James. 2001. Interaction Effects in Logistic
Regression. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.
Jackson, Philip W. 1968. In Life in Classrooms. Oxford,
England: Routledge.
Jurafsky, Dan, Rajesh Ranganath, and Daniel A.
McFarland. 2009. ‘‘Extracting Social Meaning:
Identifying Interactional Style in Spoken
Conversation.’’ Proceedings of NAACL HLT 2009.
Retrieved (http://dl.acm.org/citation.cfm?id=1620
847).
Kalish, Yuval and Gary Robins. 2006. ‘‘Psychological
Predispositions and Network Structure: The
Relationship between Individual Predispositions,
Structural Holes and Network Closure.’’ Social
Networks 28(1):56-84.
Kelman, Herbert C. 1961. ‘‘Processes of Opinion Change.’’
Public Opinion Quarterly 25(1):57-78.
Kounin, Jacob S. 1970. Discipline and Group
Management in Classrooms. New York: Holt,
Rinehart and Winston.
Lawrence, Jean, David Steed, and Pamela Young. 1984.
Disruptive Children, Disruptive Schools? New
York: Croom Helm.
Leinhardt, Gaea and James G. Greeno. 1986. ‘‘The
Cognitive Skill of Teaching.’’ Journal of
Educational Psychology 78(2):75-95.
Lortie, Dan. 1975. Schoolteacher: A Sociological Study.
Chicago: University of Chicago Press.
MacBeth, Douglas H. 1990. ‘‘Classroom Order as
Practical Action: The Making and Un-making of
a Quiet Reproach.’’ British Journal of Sociology of
Education 11(2):189-214.
Marzano, Robert J., Jana Marzano, and Deborah
Pickering. 2003. Classroom Management That
Works: Research-based Strategies for Every
Teacher. Alexandria, VA: ASCD.
McFarland, Daniel A. 1999. ‘‘Organized Behavior in
Social Systems: A Study of Student Engagement
and Resistance in High Schools.’’ PhD dissertation,
Department of Sociology, University of Chicago.
McFarland, Daniel A. 2001. ‘‘Student Resistance: How the
Formal and Informal Organization of Classrooms
Facilitate Everyday Forms of Student Defiance.’’
American Journal of Sociology 107(3):612-78.
McFarland, Daniel A. 2004. ‘‘Resistance as a Social
Drama: A Study of Change-oriented Encounters.’’
American Journal of Sociology 109(6):1249-1318.
McFarland, Daniel A. 2005. ‘‘Why Work When You Can
Play?’’ Pp. 147-74 in The Social Organization of
Schooling, edited by Larry V. Hedges and Barbara
L. Schneider. New York: Russell Sage.
McNeil, Linda M. 1986. Contradictions of Control:
School Structure and School Knowledge. New York:
Routledge.
Mehan, Hugh. 1979. Learning Lessons: Social
Organization in the Classroom. Cambridge, MA:
Harvard University Press.
Metz, Mary. 1978. Classrooms and Corridors: A Study of
Authority in American Secondary Schools. Berkeley:
University of California Press.
Moody, James, Daniel A. McFarland, and Skye
Bender-deMoll. 2005. ‘‘Dynamic Network
Visualization.’’ American Journal of Sociology
110(4):1206-41.
Murray, Christopher and Mark Greenberg. 2000.
‘‘Children’s Relationship with Teachers and Bonds
with School: An Investigation of Patterns and
Correlates in Middle Childhood.’’ Journal of School
Psychology 38(5):423-45.
Nystrand, Martin. 1997. Opening Dialogue:
Understanding the Dynamics of Language and
Learning in the English Classroom. With Adam
Gamoran, Robert Kachur, and Catherine
Prendergast. New York: Teachers College Press.
Pace, Judith L. 2006. ‘‘Saving (and Losing) Face, Race,
and Authority in a 9th Grade English Class.’’ Pp.
87-112 in Classroom Authority: Theory, Research,
and Practice, edited by Judith L. Pace and A.
Classroom Ordering and theSituational Imperatives ofRoutin.docx
Classroom Ordering and theSituational Imperatives ofRoutin.docx
Classroom Ordering and theSituational Imperatives ofRoutin.docx
Classroom Ordering and theSituational Imperatives ofRoutin.docx
Classroom Ordering and theSituational Imperatives ofRoutin.docx
Classroom Ordering and theSituational Imperatives ofRoutin.docx
Classroom Ordering and theSituational Imperatives ofRoutin.docx
Classroom Ordering and theSituational Imperatives ofRoutin.docx
Classroom Ordering and theSituational Imperatives ofRoutin.docx
Classroom Ordering and theSituational Imperatives ofRoutin.docx
Classroom Ordering and theSituational Imperatives ofRoutin.docx
Classroom Ordering and theSituational Imperatives ofRoutin.docx
Classroom Ordering and theSituational Imperatives ofRoutin.docx
Classroom Ordering and theSituational Imperatives ofRoutin.docx
Classroom Ordering and theSituational Imperatives ofRoutin.docx
Classroom Ordering and theSituational Imperatives ofRoutin.docx
Classroom Ordering and theSituational Imperatives ofRoutin.docx
Classroom Ordering and theSituational Imperatives ofRoutin.docx
Classroom Ordering and theSituational Imperatives ofRoutin.docx
Classroom Ordering and theSituational Imperatives ofRoutin.docx
Classroom Ordering and theSituational Imperatives ofRoutin.docx
Classroom Ordering and theSituational Imperatives ofRoutin.docx
Classroom Ordering and theSituational Imperatives ofRoutin.docx
Classroom Ordering and theSituational Imperatives ofRoutin.docx
Classroom Ordering and theSituational Imperatives ofRoutin.docx
Classroom Ordering and theSituational Imperatives ofRoutin.docx
Classroom Ordering and theSituational Imperatives ofRoutin.docx
Classroom Ordering and theSituational Imperatives ofRoutin.docx
Classroom Ordering and theSituational Imperatives ofRoutin.docx
Classroom Ordering and theSituational Imperatives ofRoutin.docx
Classroom Ordering and theSituational Imperatives ofRoutin.docx
Classroom Ordering and theSituational Imperatives ofRoutin.docx
Classroom Ordering and theSituational Imperatives ofRoutin.docx
Classroom Ordering and theSituational Imperatives ofRoutin.docx
Classroom Ordering and theSituational Imperatives ofRoutin.docx
Classroom Ordering and theSituational Imperatives ofRoutin.docx
Classroom Ordering and theSituational Imperatives ofRoutin.docx
Classroom Ordering and theSituational Imperatives ofRoutin.docx
Classroom Ordering and theSituational Imperatives ofRoutin.docx
Classroom Ordering and theSituational Imperatives ofRoutin.docx
Classroom Ordering and theSituational Imperatives ofRoutin.docx
Classroom Ordering and theSituational Imperatives ofRoutin.docx
Classroom Ordering and theSituational Imperatives ofRoutin.docx
Classroom Ordering and theSituational Imperatives ofRoutin.docx
Classroom Ordering and theSituational Imperatives ofRoutin.docx
Classroom Ordering and theSituational Imperatives ofRoutin.docx
Classroom Ordering and theSituational Imperatives ofRoutin.docx
Classroom Ordering and theSituational Imperatives ofRoutin.docx
Classroom Ordering and theSituational Imperatives ofRoutin.docx
Classroom Ordering and theSituational Imperatives ofRoutin.docx
Classroom Ordering and theSituational Imperatives ofRoutin.docx
Classroom Ordering and theSituational Imperatives ofRoutin.docx
Classroom Ordering and theSituational Imperatives ofRoutin.docx
Classroom Ordering and theSituational Imperatives ofRoutin.docx
Classroom Ordering and theSituational Imperatives ofRoutin.docx
Classroom Ordering and theSituational Imperatives ofRoutin.docx
Classroom Ordering and theSituational Imperatives ofRoutin.docx
Classroom Ordering and theSituational Imperatives ofRoutin.docx

More Related Content

Similar to Classroom Ordering and theSituational Imperatives ofRoutin.docx

About The Differences Of Teachers Self-Perceptions To The Statements Of Soci...
About The Differences Of Teachers  Self-Perceptions To The Statements Of Soci...About The Differences Of Teachers  Self-Perceptions To The Statements Of Soci...
About The Differences Of Teachers Self-Perceptions To The Statements Of Soci...Joshua Gorinson
 
Examine the Relevance of Processes in How Individuals and Organiza.docx
Examine the Relevance of Processes in How Individuals and Organiza.docxExamine the Relevance of Processes in How Individuals and Organiza.docx
Examine the Relevance of Processes in How Individuals and Organiza.docxSANSKAR20
 
Paper11_PenuelRielSabelli
Paper11_PenuelRielSabelliPaper11_PenuelRielSabelli
Paper11_PenuelRielSabelliwebuploader
 
Combined spring 10
Combined spring 10Combined spring 10
Combined spring 10dabneyluang
 
online assignment
online assignmentonline assignment
online assignmentaneesh a
 
Case study in decision making- Akram Jabar Najim
Case study in decision making- Akram Jabar NajimCase study in decision making- Akram Jabar Najim
Case study in decision making- Akram Jabar NajimAkramEnglish
 
1 s2.0-s0883035503000788-main
1 s2.0-s0883035503000788-main1 s2.0-s0883035503000788-main
1 s2.0-s0883035503000788-mainmohammad yusuf
 
www.nationalforum.com - Dr. William Allan Kritsonis, Editor-in-Chief
www.nationalforum.com - Dr. William Allan Kritsonis, Editor-in-Chiefwww.nationalforum.com - Dr. William Allan Kritsonis, Editor-in-Chief
www.nationalforum.com - Dr. William Allan Kritsonis, Editor-in-ChiefWilliam Kritsonis
 
Glaeser, susan a colorful field of learners visualizing nftej v22 n2 2012[1]
Glaeser, susan a colorful field of learners   visualizing nftej v22 n2 2012[1]Glaeser, susan a colorful field of learners   visualizing nftej v22 n2 2012[1]
Glaeser, susan a colorful field of learners visualizing nftej v22 n2 2012[1]William Kritsonis
 
Managing a primary_school_class_an_empirical_study-1
Managing a primary_school_class_an_empirical_study-1Managing a primary_school_class_an_empirical_study-1
Managing a primary_school_class_an_empirical_study-1Catina Feresin
 
Restorative practices Laura Mirsky
Restorative practices Laura MirskyRestorative practices Laura Mirsky
Restorative practices Laura Mirskygathyus
 
A qualitative study of primary teachers classroom feedback rationales.pdf
A qualitative study of primary teachers  classroom feedback rationales.pdfA qualitative study of primary teachers  classroom feedback rationales.pdf
A qualitative study of primary teachers classroom feedback rationales.pdfMd. Shahriar Shafiq
 
A Multilevel Study Of Self-Beliefs And Student Behaviors In A Group Problem-S...
A Multilevel Study Of Self-Beliefs And Student Behaviors In A Group Problem-S...A Multilevel Study Of Self-Beliefs And Student Behaviors In A Group Problem-S...
A Multilevel Study Of Self-Beliefs And Student Behaviors In A Group Problem-S...Jeff Nelson
 
Week 10 recasts
Week 10 recastsWeek 10 recasts
Week 10 recastsAsniem CA
 
Action Research In Second Language Teacher Education
Action Research In Second Language Teacher EducationAction Research In Second Language Teacher Education
Action Research In Second Language Teacher EducationCynthia King
 
Towards a framework of teaching effectiveness in public
Towards a framework of teaching effectiveness in publicTowards a framework of teaching effectiveness in public
Towards a framework of teaching effectiveness in publicjaderex
 

Similar to Classroom Ordering and theSituational Imperatives ofRoutin.docx (20)

About The Differences Of Teachers Self-Perceptions To The Statements Of Soci...
About The Differences Of Teachers  Self-Perceptions To The Statements Of Soci...About The Differences Of Teachers  Self-Perceptions To The Statements Of Soci...
About The Differences Of Teachers Self-Perceptions To The Statements Of Soci...
 
02
0202
02
 
Examine the Relevance of Processes in How Individuals and Organiza.docx
Examine the Relevance of Processes in How Individuals and Organiza.docxExamine the Relevance of Processes in How Individuals and Organiza.docx
Examine the Relevance of Processes in How Individuals and Organiza.docx
 
Paper11_PenuelRielSabelli
Paper11_PenuelRielSabelliPaper11_PenuelRielSabelli
Paper11_PenuelRielSabelli
 
Bullying and teacher practices
Bullying and teacher practicesBullying and teacher practices
Bullying and teacher practices
 
Combined spring 10
Combined spring 10Combined spring 10
Combined spring 10
 
online assignment
online assignmentonline assignment
online assignment
 
Systems 550 ppt_art
Systems 550 ppt_artSystems 550 ppt_art
Systems 550 ppt_art
 
Case study in decision making- Akram Jabar Najim
Case study in decision making- Akram Jabar NajimCase study in decision making- Akram Jabar Najim
Case study in decision making- Akram Jabar Najim
 
1 s2.0-s0883035503000788-main
1 s2.0-s0883035503000788-main1 s2.0-s0883035503000788-main
1 s2.0-s0883035503000788-main
 
www.nationalforum.com - Dr. William Allan Kritsonis, Editor-in-Chief
www.nationalforum.com - Dr. William Allan Kritsonis, Editor-in-Chiefwww.nationalforum.com - Dr. William Allan Kritsonis, Editor-in-Chief
www.nationalforum.com - Dr. William Allan Kritsonis, Editor-in-Chief
 
Glaeser, susan a colorful field of learners visualizing nftej v22 n2 2012[1]
Glaeser, susan a colorful field of learners   visualizing nftej v22 n2 2012[1]Glaeser, susan a colorful field of learners   visualizing nftej v22 n2 2012[1]
Glaeser, susan a colorful field of learners visualizing nftej v22 n2 2012[1]
 
Managing a primary_school_class_an_empirical_study-1
Managing a primary_school_class_an_empirical_study-1Managing a primary_school_class_an_empirical_study-1
Managing a primary_school_class_an_empirical_study-1
 
Restorative practices Laura Mirsky
Restorative practices Laura MirskyRestorative practices Laura Mirsky
Restorative practices Laura Mirsky
 
A qualitative study of primary teachers classroom feedback rationales.pdf
A qualitative study of primary teachers  classroom feedback rationales.pdfA qualitative study of primary teachers  classroom feedback rationales.pdf
A qualitative study of primary teachers classroom feedback rationales.pdf
 
Hamman stevens1
Hamman stevens1Hamman stevens1
Hamman stevens1
 
A Multilevel Study Of Self-Beliefs And Student Behaviors In A Group Problem-S...
A Multilevel Study Of Self-Beliefs And Student Behaviors In A Group Problem-S...A Multilevel Study Of Self-Beliefs And Student Behaviors In A Group Problem-S...
A Multilevel Study Of Self-Beliefs And Student Behaviors In A Group Problem-S...
 
Week 10 recasts
Week 10 recastsWeek 10 recasts
Week 10 recasts
 
Action Research In Second Language Teacher Education
Action Research In Second Language Teacher EducationAction Research In Second Language Teacher Education
Action Research In Second Language Teacher Education
 
Towards a framework of teaching effectiveness in public
Towards a framework of teaching effectiveness in publicTowards a framework of teaching effectiveness in public
Towards a framework of teaching effectiveness in public
 

More from monicafrancis71118

1. Discuss Blockchains potential application in compensation system.docx
1. Discuss Blockchains potential application in compensation system.docx1. Discuss Blockchains potential application in compensation system.docx
1. Discuss Blockchains potential application in compensation system.docxmonicafrancis71118
 
1. Describe the characteristics of the aging process. Explain how so.docx
1. Describe the characteristics of the aging process. Explain how so.docx1. Describe the characteristics of the aging process. Explain how so.docx
1. Describe the characteristics of the aging process. Explain how so.docxmonicafrancis71118
 
1. Dis. 7Should we continue to collect data on race and .docx
1. Dis. 7Should we continue to collect data on race and .docx1. Dis. 7Should we continue to collect data on race and .docx
1. Dis. 7Should we continue to collect data on race and .docxmonicafrancis71118
 
1. Differentiate crisis intervention from other counseling therapeut.docx
1. Differentiate crisis intervention from other counseling therapeut.docx1. Differentiate crisis intervention from other counseling therapeut.docx
1. Differentiate crisis intervention from other counseling therapeut.docxmonicafrancis71118
 
1. Despite our rational nature, our ability to reason well is ofte.docx
1. Despite our rational nature, our ability to reason well is ofte.docx1. Despite our rational nature, our ability to reason well is ofte.docx
1. Despite our rational nature, our ability to reason well is ofte.docxmonicafrancis71118
 
1. Describe the ethical challenges faced by organizations operating .docx
1. Describe the ethical challenges faced by organizations operating .docx1. Describe the ethical challenges faced by organizations operating .docx
1. Describe the ethical challenges faced by organizations operating .docxmonicafrancis71118
 
1. Describe in your own words the anatomy of a muscle.  This sho.docx
1. Describe in your own words the anatomy of a muscle.  This sho.docx1. Describe in your own words the anatomy of a muscle.  This sho.docx
1. Describe in your own words the anatomy of a muscle.  This sho.docxmonicafrancis71118
 
1. Describe how your attitude of including aspects of health literac.docx
1. Describe how your attitude of including aspects of health literac.docx1. Describe how your attitude of including aspects of health literac.docx
1. Describe how your attitude of including aspects of health literac.docxmonicafrancis71118
 
1. Choose a behavior (such as overeating, shopping, Internet use.docx
1. Choose a behavior (such as overeating, shopping, Internet use.docx1. Choose a behavior (such as overeating, shopping, Internet use.docx
1. Choose a behavior (such as overeating, shopping, Internet use.docxmonicafrancis71118
 
1. Case 3-4 Franklin Industries’ Whistleblowing (a GVV Case)Natali.docx
1. Case 3-4 Franklin Industries’ Whistleblowing (a GVV Case)Natali.docx1. Case 3-4 Franklin Industries’ Whistleblowing (a GVV Case)Natali.docx
1. Case 3-4 Franklin Industries’ Whistleblowing (a GVV Case)Natali.docxmonicafrancis71118
 
1. Cryptography is used to protect confidential data in many areas. .docx
1. Cryptography is used to protect confidential data in many areas. .docx1. Cryptography is used to protect confidential data in many areas. .docx
1. Cryptography is used to protect confidential data in many areas. .docxmonicafrancis71118
 
1. Compare and contrast steganography and cryptography.2. Why st.docx
1. Compare and contrast steganography and cryptography.2. Why st.docx1. Compare and contrast steganography and cryptography.2. Why st.docx
1. Compare and contrast steganography and cryptography.2. Why st.docxmonicafrancis71118
 
1. Date September 13, 2017 – September 15, 2017 2. Curr.docx
1. Date September 13, 2017 – September 15, 2017 2. Curr.docx1. Date September 13, 2017 – September 15, 2017 2. Curr.docx
1. Date September 13, 2017 – September 15, 2017 2. Curr.docxmonicafrancis71118
 
1. compare and contrast predictive analytics with prescriptive and d.docx
1. compare and contrast predictive analytics with prescriptive and d.docx1. compare and contrast predictive analytics with prescriptive and d.docx
1. compare and contrast predictive analytics with prescriptive and d.docxmonicafrancis71118
 
1. Creating and maintaining relationships between home and schoo.docx
1. Creating and maintaining relationships between home and schoo.docx1. Creating and maintaining relationships between home and schoo.docx
1. Creating and maintaining relationships between home and schoo.docxmonicafrancis71118
 
1. Compare and contrast Strategic and Tactical Analysis and its .docx
1. Compare and contrast Strategic and Tactical Analysis and its .docx1. Compare and contrast Strategic and Tactical Analysis and its .docx
1. Compare and contrast Strategic and Tactical Analysis and its .docxmonicafrancis71118
 
1. Coalition ProposalVaccination Policy for Infectious Disease P.docx
1. Coalition ProposalVaccination Policy for Infectious Disease P.docx1. Coalition ProposalVaccination Policy for Infectious Disease P.docx
1. Coalition ProposalVaccination Policy for Infectious Disease P.docxmonicafrancis71118
 
1. Company Description and Backgrounda. Weight Watchers was cr.docx
1. Company Description and Backgrounda. Weight Watchers was cr.docx1. Company Description and Backgrounda. Weight Watchers was cr.docx
1. Company Description and Backgrounda. Weight Watchers was cr.docxmonicafrancis71118
 
1. Come up with TWO movie ideas -- as in for TWO screenplays that .docx
1. Come up with TWO movie ideas -- as in for TWO screenplays that .docx1. Come up with TWO movie ideas -- as in for TWO screenplays that .docx
1. Come up with TWO movie ideas -- as in for TWO screenplays that .docxmonicafrancis71118
 
1. Choose a case for the paper that interests you. Most choose a .docx
1. Choose a case for the paper that interests you.  Most choose a .docx1. Choose a case for the paper that interests you.  Most choose a .docx
1. Choose a case for the paper that interests you. Most choose a .docxmonicafrancis71118
 

More from monicafrancis71118 (20)

1. Discuss Blockchains potential application in compensation system.docx
1. Discuss Blockchains potential application in compensation system.docx1. Discuss Blockchains potential application in compensation system.docx
1. Discuss Blockchains potential application in compensation system.docx
 
1. Describe the characteristics of the aging process. Explain how so.docx
1. Describe the characteristics of the aging process. Explain how so.docx1. Describe the characteristics of the aging process. Explain how so.docx
1. Describe the characteristics of the aging process. Explain how so.docx
 
1. Dis. 7Should we continue to collect data on race and .docx
1. Dis. 7Should we continue to collect data on race and .docx1. Dis. 7Should we continue to collect data on race and .docx
1. Dis. 7Should we continue to collect data on race and .docx
 
1. Differentiate crisis intervention from other counseling therapeut.docx
1. Differentiate crisis intervention from other counseling therapeut.docx1. Differentiate crisis intervention from other counseling therapeut.docx
1. Differentiate crisis intervention from other counseling therapeut.docx
 
1. Despite our rational nature, our ability to reason well is ofte.docx
1. Despite our rational nature, our ability to reason well is ofte.docx1. Despite our rational nature, our ability to reason well is ofte.docx
1. Despite our rational nature, our ability to reason well is ofte.docx
 
1. Describe the ethical challenges faced by organizations operating .docx
1. Describe the ethical challenges faced by organizations operating .docx1. Describe the ethical challenges faced by organizations operating .docx
1. Describe the ethical challenges faced by organizations operating .docx
 
1. Describe in your own words the anatomy of a muscle.  This sho.docx
1. Describe in your own words the anatomy of a muscle.  This sho.docx1. Describe in your own words the anatomy of a muscle.  This sho.docx
1. Describe in your own words the anatomy of a muscle.  This sho.docx
 
1. Describe how your attitude of including aspects of health literac.docx
1. Describe how your attitude of including aspects of health literac.docx1. Describe how your attitude of including aspects of health literac.docx
1. Describe how your attitude of including aspects of health literac.docx
 
1. Choose a behavior (such as overeating, shopping, Internet use.docx
1. Choose a behavior (such as overeating, shopping, Internet use.docx1. Choose a behavior (such as overeating, shopping, Internet use.docx
1. Choose a behavior (such as overeating, shopping, Internet use.docx
 
1. Case 3-4 Franklin Industries’ Whistleblowing (a GVV Case)Natali.docx
1. Case 3-4 Franklin Industries’ Whistleblowing (a GVV Case)Natali.docx1. Case 3-4 Franklin Industries’ Whistleblowing (a GVV Case)Natali.docx
1. Case 3-4 Franklin Industries’ Whistleblowing (a GVV Case)Natali.docx
 
1. Cryptography is used to protect confidential data in many areas. .docx
1. Cryptography is used to protect confidential data in many areas. .docx1. Cryptography is used to protect confidential data in many areas. .docx
1. Cryptography is used to protect confidential data in many areas. .docx
 
1. Compare and contrast steganography and cryptography.2. Why st.docx
1. Compare and contrast steganography and cryptography.2. Why st.docx1. Compare and contrast steganography and cryptography.2. Why st.docx
1. Compare and contrast steganography and cryptography.2. Why st.docx
 
1. Date September 13, 2017 – September 15, 2017 2. Curr.docx
1. Date September 13, 2017 – September 15, 2017 2. Curr.docx1. Date September 13, 2017 – September 15, 2017 2. Curr.docx
1. Date September 13, 2017 – September 15, 2017 2. Curr.docx
 
1. compare and contrast predictive analytics with prescriptive and d.docx
1. compare and contrast predictive analytics with prescriptive and d.docx1. compare and contrast predictive analytics with prescriptive and d.docx
1. compare and contrast predictive analytics with prescriptive and d.docx
 
1. Creating and maintaining relationships between home and schoo.docx
1. Creating and maintaining relationships between home and schoo.docx1. Creating and maintaining relationships between home and schoo.docx
1. Creating and maintaining relationships between home and schoo.docx
 
1. Compare and contrast Strategic and Tactical Analysis and its .docx
1. Compare and contrast Strategic and Tactical Analysis and its .docx1. Compare and contrast Strategic and Tactical Analysis and its .docx
1. Compare and contrast Strategic and Tactical Analysis and its .docx
 
1. Coalition ProposalVaccination Policy for Infectious Disease P.docx
1. Coalition ProposalVaccination Policy for Infectious Disease P.docx1. Coalition ProposalVaccination Policy for Infectious Disease P.docx
1. Coalition ProposalVaccination Policy for Infectious Disease P.docx
 
1. Company Description and Backgrounda. Weight Watchers was cr.docx
1. Company Description and Backgrounda. Weight Watchers was cr.docx1. Company Description and Backgrounda. Weight Watchers was cr.docx
1. Company Description and Backgrounda. Weight Watchers was cr.docx
 
1. Come up with TWO movie ideas -- as in for TWO screenplays that .docx
1. Come up with TWO movie ideas -- as in for TWO screenplays that .docx1. Come up with TWO movie ideas -- as in for TWO screenplays that .docx
1. Come up with TWO movie ideas -- as in for TWO screenplays that .docx
 
1. Choose a case for the paper that interests you. Most choose a .docx
1. Choose a case for the paper that interests you.  Most choose a .docx1. Choose a case for the paper that interests you.  Most choose a .docx
1. Choose a case for the paper that interests you. Most choose a .docx
 

Recently uploaded

Graduate Outcomes Presentation Slides - English (v3).pptx
Graduate Outcomes Presentation Slides - English (v3).pptxGraduate Outcomes Presentation Slides - English (v3).pptx
Graduate Outcomes Presentation Slides - English (v3).pptxneillewis46
 
UChicago CMSC 23320 - The Best Commit Messages of 2024
UChicago CMSC 23320 - The Best Commit Messages of 2024UChicago CMSC 23320 - The Best Commit Messages of 2024
UChicago CMSC 23320 - The Best Commit Messages of 2024Borja Sotomayor
 
How To Create Editable Tree View in Odoo 17
How To Create Editable Tree View in Odoo 17How To Create Editable Tree View in Odoo 17
How To Create Editable Tree View in Odoo 17Celine George
 
diagnosting testing bsc 2nd sem.pptx....
diagnosting testing bsc 2nd sem.pptx....diagnosting testing bsc 2nd sem.pptx....
diagnosting testing bsc 2nd sem.pptx....Ritu480198
 
8 Tips for Effective Working Capital Management
8 Tips for Effective Working Capital Management8 Tips for Effective Working Capital Management
8 Tips for Effective Working Capital ManagementMBA Assignment Experts
 
The Story of Village Palampur Class 9 Free Study Material PDF
The Story of Village Palampur Class 9 Free Study Material PDFThe Story of Village Palampur Class 9 Free Study Material PDF
The Story of Village Palampur Class 9 Free Study Material PDFVivekanand Anglo Vedic Academy
 
Observing-Correct-Grammar-in-Making-Definitions.pptx
Observing-Correct-Grammar-in-Making-Definitions.pptxObserving-Correct-Grammar-in-Making-Definitions.pptx
Observing-Correct-Grammar-in-Making-Definitions.pptxAdelaideRefugio
 
SURVEY I created for uni project research
SURVEY I created for uni project researchSURVEY I created for uni project research
SURVEY I created for uni project researchCaitlinCummins3
 
Transparency, Recognition and the role of eSealing - Ildiko Mazar and Koen No...
Transparency, Recognition and the role of eSealing - Ildiko Mazar and Koen No...Transparency, Recognition and the role of eSealing - Ildiko Mazar and Koen No...
Transparency, Recognition and the role of eSealing - Ildiko Mazar and Koen No...EADTU
 
Book Review of Run For Your Life Powerpoint
Book Review of Run For Your Life PowerpointBook Review of Run For Your Life Powerpoint
Book Review of Run For Your Life Powerpoint23600690
 
Trauma-Informed Leadership - Five Practical Principles
Trauma-Informed Leadership - Five Practical PrinciplesTrauma-Informed Leadership - Five Practical Principles
Trauma-Informed Leadership - Five Practical PrinciplesPooky Knightsmith
 
Basic Civil Engineering notes on Transportation Engineering & Modes of Transport
Basic Civil Engineering notes on Transportation Engineering & Modes of TransportBasic Civil Engineering notes on Transportation Engineering & Modes of Transport
Basic Civil Engineering notes on Transportation Engineering & Modes of TransportDenish Jangid
 
Spellings Wk 4 and Wk 5 for Grade 4 at CAPS
Spellings Wk 4 and Wk 5 for Grade 4 at CAPSSpellings Wk 4 and Wk 5 for Grade 4 at CAPS
Spellings Wk 4 and Wk 5 for Grade 4 at CAPSAnaAcapella
 
Spring gala 2024 photo slideshow - Celebrating School-Community Partnerships
Spring gala 2024 photo slideshow - Celebrating School-Community PartnershipsSpring gala 2024 photo slideshow - Celebrating School-Community Partnerships
Spring gala 2024 photo slideshow - Celebrating School-Community Partnershipsexpandedwebsite
 
會考英聽會考英聽會考英聽會考英聽會考英聽會考英聽會考英聽會考英聽會考英聽會考英聽
會考英聽會考英聽會考英聽會考英聽會考英聽會考英聽會考英聽會考英聽會考英聽會考英聽會考英聽會考英聽會考英聽會考英聽會考英聽會考英聽會考英聽會考英聽會考英聽會考英聽
會考英聽會考英聽會考英聽會考英聽會考英聽會考英聽會考英聽會考英聽會考英聽會考英聽中 央社
 
Analyzing and resolving a communication crisis in Dhaka textiles LTD.pptx
Analyzing and resolving a communication crisis in Dhaka textiles LTD.pptxAnalyzing and resolving a communication crisis in Dhaka textiles LTD.pptx
Analyzing and resolving a communication crisis in Dhaka textiles LTD.pptxLimon Prince
 
TỔNG HỢP HƠN 100 ĐỀ THI THỬ TỐT NGHIỆP THPT TOÁN 2024 - TỪ CÁC TRƯỜNG, TRƯỜNG...
TỔNG HỢP HƠN 100 ĐỀ THI THỬ TỐT NGHIỆP THPT TOÁN 2024 - TỪ CÁC TRƯỜNG, TRƯỜNG...TỔNG HỢP HƠN 100 ĐỀ THI THỬ TỐT NGHIỆP THPT TOÁN 2024 - TỪ CÁC TRƯỜNG, TRƯỜNG...
TỔNG HỢP HƠN 100 ĐỀ THI THỬ TỐT NGHIỆP THPT TOÁN 2024 - TỪ CÁC TRƯỜNG, TRƯỜNG...Nguyen Thanh Tu Collection
 
TỔNG HỢP HƠN 100 ĐỀ THI THỬ TỐT NGHIỆP THPT TOÁN 2024 - TỪ CÁC TRƯỜNG, TRƯỜNG...
TỔNG HỢP HƠN 100 ĐỀ THI THỬ TỐT NGHIỆP THPT TOÁN 2024 - TỪ CÁC TRƯỜNG, TRƯỜNG...TỔNG HỢP HƠN 100 ĐỀ THI THỬ TỐT NGHIỆP THPT TOÁN 2024 - TỪ CÁC TRƯỜNG, TRƯỜNG...
TỔNG HỢP HƠN 100 ĐỀ THI THỬ TỐT NGHIỆP THPT TOÁN 2024 - TỪ CÁC TRƯỜNG, TRƯỜNG...Nguyen Thanh Tu Collection
 

Recently uploaded (20)

Graduate Outcomes Presentation Slides - English (v3).pptx
Graduate Outcomes Presentation Slides - English (v3).pptxGraduate Outcomes Presentation Slides - English (v3).pptx
Graduate Outcomes Presentation Slides - English (v3).pptx
 
UChicago CMSC 23320 - The Best Commit Messages of 2024
UChicago CMSC 23320 - The Best Commit Messages of 2024UChicago CMSC 23320 - The Best Commit Messages of 2024
UChicago CMSC 23320 - The Best Commit Messages of 2024
 
How To Create Editable Tree View in Odoo 17
How To Create Editable Tree View in Odoo 17How To Create Editable Tree View in Odoo 17
How To Create Editable Tree View in Odoo 17
 
diagnosting testing bsc 2nd sem.pptx....
diagnosting testing bsc 2nd sem.pptx....diagnosting testing bsc 2nd sem.pptx....
diagnosting testing bsc 2nd sem.pptx....
 
8 Tips for Effective Working Capital Management
8 Tips for Effective Working Capital Management8 Tips for Effective Working Capital Management
8 Tips for Effective Working Capital Management
 
The Story of Village Palampur Class 9 Free Study Material PDF
The Story of Village Palampur Class 9 Free Study Material PDFThe Story of Village Palampur Class 9 Free Study Material PDF
The Story of Village Palampur Class 9 Free Study Material PDF
 
Mattingly "AI and Prompt Design: LLMs with NER"
Mattingly "AI and Prompt Design: LLMs with NER"Mattingly "AI and Prompt Design: LLMs with NER"
Mattingly "AI and Prompt Design: LLMs with NER"
 
Observing-Correct-Grammar-in-Making-Definitions.pptx
Observing-Correct-Grammar-in-Making-Definitions.pptxObserving-Correct-Grammar-in-Making-Definitions.pptx
Observing-Correct-Grammar-in-Making-Definitions.pptx
 
SURVEY I created for uni project research
SURVEY I created for uni project researchSURVEY I created for uni project research
SURVEY I created for uni project research
 
ESSENTIAL of (CS/IT/IS) class 07 (Networks)
ESSENTIAL of (CS/IT/IS) class 07 (Networks)ESSENTIAL of (CS/IT/IS) class 07 (Networks)
ESSENTIAL of (CS/IT/IS) class 07 (Networks)
 
Transparency, Recognition and the role of eSealing - Ildiko Mazar and Koen No...
Transparency, Recognition and the role of eSealing - Ildiko Mazar and Koen No...Transparency, Recognition and the role of eSealing - Ildiko Mazar and Koen No...
Transparency, Recognition and the role of eSealing - Ildiko Mazar and Koen No...
 
Book Review of Run For Your Life Powerpoint
Book Review of Run For Your Life PowerpointBook Review of Run For Your Life Powerpoint
Book Review of Run For Your Life Powerpoint
 
Trauma-Informed Leadership - Five Practical Principles
Trauma-Informed Leadership - Five Practical PrinciplesTrauma-Informed Leadership - Five Practical Principles
Trauma-Informed Leadership - Five Practical Principles
 
Basic Civil Engineering notes on Transportation Engineering & Modes of Transport
Basic Civil Engineering notes on Transportation Engineering & Modes of TransportBasic Civil Engineering notes on Transportation Engineering & Modes of Transport
Basic Civil Engineering notes on Transportation Engineering & Modes of Transport
 
Spellings Wk 4 and Wk 5 for Grade 4 at CAPS
Spellings Wk 4 and Wk 5 for Grade 4 at CAPSSpellings Wk 4 and Wk 5 for Grade 4 at CAPS
Spellings Wk 4 and Wk 5 for Grade 4 at CAPS
 
Spring gala 2024 photo slideshow - Celebrating School-Community Partnerships
Spring gala 2024 photo slideshow - Celebrating School-Community PartnershipsSpring gala 2024 photo slideshow - Celebrating School-Community Partnerships
Spring gala 2024 photo slideshow - Celebrating School-Community Partnerships
 
會考英聽會考英聽會考英聽會考英聽會考英聽會考英聽會考英聽會考英聽會考英聽會考英聽
會考英聽會考英聽會考英聽會考英聽會考英聽會考英聽會考英聽會考英聽會考英聽會考英聽會考英聽會考英聽會考英聽會考英聽會考英聽會考英聽會考英聽會考英聽會考英聽會考英聽
會考英聽會考英聽會考英聽會考英聽會考英聽會考英聽會考英聽會考英聽會考英聽會考英聽
 
Analyzing and resolving a communication crisis in Dhaka textiles LTD.pptx
Analyzing and resolving a communication crisis in Dhaka textiles LTD.pptxAnalyzing and resolving a communication crisis in Dhaka textiles LTD.pptx
Analyzing and resolving a communication crisis in Dhaka textiles LTD.pptx
 
TỔNG HỢP HƠN 100 ĐỀ THI THỬ TỐT NGHIỆP THPT TOÁN 2024 - TỪ CÁC TRƯỜNG, TRƯỜNG...
TỔNG HỢP HƠN 100 ĐỀ THI THỬ TỐT NGHIỆP THPT TOÁN 2024 - TỪ CÁC TRƯỜNG, TRƯỜNG...TỔNG HỢP HƠN 100 ĐỀ THI THỬ TỐT NGHIỆP THPT TOÁN 2024 - TỪ CÁC TRƯỜNG, TRƯỜNG...
TỔNG HỢP HƠN 100 ĐỀ THI THỬ TỐT NGHIỆP THPT TOÁN 2024 - TỪ CÁC TRƯỜNG, TRƯỜNG...
 
TỔNG HỢP HƠN 100 ĐỀ THI THỬ TỐT NGHIỆP THPT TOÁN 2024 - TỪ CÁC TRƯỜNG, TRƯỜNG...
TỔNG HỢP HƠN 100 ĐỀ THI THỬ TỐT NGHIỆP THPT TOÁN 2024 - TỪ CÁC TRƯỜNG, TRƯỜNG...TỔNG HỢP HƠN 100 ĐỀ THI THỬ TỐT NGHIỆP THPT TOÁN 2024 - TỪ CÁC TRƯỜNG, TRƯỜNG...
TỔNG HỢP HƠN 100 ĐỀ THI THỬ TỐT NGHIỆP THPT TOÁN 2024 - TỪ CÁC TRƯỜNG, TRƯỜNG...
 

Classroom Ordering and theSituational Imperatives ofRoutin.docx

  • 1. Classroom Ordering and the Situational Imperatives of Routine and Ritual David Diehl1 and Daniel A. McFarland2 Abstract This article contends that the problem of classroom order rests less in the roles and compositions of class- rooms than in the multidimensional nature of their social situations. Classroom order arises from the dynamic relationship between distinct situational requirements: the coordination of interaction into institu- tionalized patterns (routine) and the validation of participants’ identities (ritual). Utilizing a unique data set of more than 800,000 turns of talk from 601 high school classrooms, the authors develop metrics for measur- ing the longitudinal accomplishment of routine (interactional stability) and ritual (interactional concord) and present two sets of analyses. The first analyses identify the antecedents to stability and concord, and the second examine how stability and concord shape the experiences and attitudes of classroom participants. Results indicate that activities and discourse combine to fulfill the requirements of ritual and routine in dif- ferent ways, often meeting one at the expense of the other, and that the accomplishment of stability and concord has positive returns to classroom experiences, but in different ways for teachers and students. Keywords
  • 2. social networks, classrooms, interaction, communication, academic activities Creating and maintaining order in the classroom is a central concern for educators (Waller 1932; Metz 1978; Boocock 1978; Bossert 1979; Mehan 1979; Doyle 1986; McNeil 1986; Stodolsky 1988; Cazden 1988; Nystrand 1997; Gamoran and Kelly 2003). Despite the best efforts of teachers, how- ever, research from diverse perspectives has shown that disruption and defiance are ubiquitous and seemingly unavoidable (MacBeth 1990:192). It is unclear why some classes are more orderly than others and why disorder can arise and recede even within individual class periods. Fundamental aspects of classroom teaching and learning remain obscure without a better understanding of how teachers and students construct, and are molded by, the social context they jointly enact through
  • 3. moment-by-moment social transactions (Wells 1993). We address this lacuna through two theoretical and empirical elaborations. The first is to regard interactional order as a multidimensional phenom- enon. Following Goffman, we relate order as meet- ing two requirements, namely, those of routine and ritual (cf. system and normative requirements, Goffman 1981:14–15; 1983). For an encounter to be comprehensible, participants must coordinate according to some collective routine. In class- rooms, this entails the mobilization of participants into and across stable interactional configurations associated with particular academic activities (Goffman 1981:137). For an encounter to be embraced and valued, participants must conduct 1Duke Network Analysis Center, Durham, NC, USA 2 Stanford University, Stanford, CA, USA
  • 4. Corresponding Author: Daniel A. McFarland, Stanford University, 520 Galvez Mall, Stanford, CA 94305, USA Email: [email protected] Sociology of Education 85(4) 326–349 � American Sociological Association 2012 DOI: 10.1177/0038040712452093 http://soe.sagepub.com at WESTERN OREGON UNIVERSITY on February 10, 2015soe.sagepub.comDownloaded from http://soe.sagepub.com/ interpersonal rituals demonstrating concord and a sense of collective goodwill. In classrooms, this entails the ratification of valued identities (Goffman 1981:266). While situational anchors of routine and ritual can reinforce one another, in crowded classroom settings there arises a tension between efforts to coordinate the group and to rat- ify identities.
  • 5. Our second elaboration is to regard interactional order as a situated phenomenon. By this we mean that the problem of classroom order changes from moment to moment. As teachers move their classes through different academic activities, the structures and expectations for interaction shift, as do the means by which routine and ritual requirements of interaction are accomplished. Most practitioners negotiate these dynamics and tensions via ongoing talk and communication. However, discursive moves seldom have the same effect on every situa- tion. As most educators can attest, declarative state- ments and jokes have different effects on the enactment of lectures and group work activities. To study this situated nature of classroom order, we identify proxies for interactional stability (rou- tine) and concord (ritual) within the overall net- work of moment-to-moment discourse. By
  • 6. treating individual acts of communication within institutionalized activities as the building blocks of classroom order (Goffman 1981; Wells 1999; Butts 2008), we are able to offer a novel compari- son of the dynamic patterns of interactions (and their effects) across a multitude of diverse class- rooms, class periods, and class lessons. For our empirical analysis, we use an unparalleled data set of more than 800,000 turns of discourse col- lected from 601 high school class periods that enables us to view classroom order longitudinally. In the first set of analyses, we model the antecedents of stability and concord. We demonstrate the general validity of our approach by showing the timing of stable and concordant interaction is consistent with existing qualitative understandings of classrooms. We then go beyond extant work and find that the accomplish- ment of stability and concord depends on situational
  • 7. levers such as the selection of activities and the appro- priate use of discursive moves suited to each task. Not only do we demonstrate that forms of talk have differ- ent structuring effects depending on the academic activity in which they take place; we also show that efforts to achieve stability may have a negative effect on concord, and vice versa. We can thus see the class- room as a tenuous balance between stability and con- cord, the nature of which is constantly shifting as the teacher moves students through different tasks. In the second set of analyses, we model the effects of stabil- ity and routine on task survival and participants’ schooling experiences. Overall, we show that varia- tion in classroom order is situational and multidimen- sional, the product of a constantly shifting interrelation between discourse and activity—and not simply the result of static transsituational roles, motives, and classroom compositions. In addition,
  • 8. we show that the accomplishment of interactional sta- bility and concord matters and has differential returns to the quality of schooling. Routine and Ritual: Dual Requirements for Classroom Ordering Our work can be located within an ongoing effort to understand variation in classroom order at increas- ingly fine-grained levels of analysis. Pioneering work on the social order of schooling was primarily concerned with providing a generalizable descrip- tion of the interaction dynamics between teachers and students rather than explaining variation within classrooms. The result was a view of classroom order shaped by a ubiquitous tension between the different interests and motivations of teachers and students as well as the incompatible demands of the teaching profession itself (Waller 1932; Bidwell 1965; Lortie 1975).
  • 9. Building off of this work, midcentury sociology of education began to offer explanations for variations in the teacher-student tension between classrooms. This work identified numerous sources of interclassroom differences that affected interaction, including teacher styles and dispositions (Brookover 1945; Cogan 1956) as well as the culture (Gordon 1957; Coleman 1961) and composition (Dreeben and Barr 1988) of the student body. In this work variation arises from differences in how teachers and students consistently enact their institutional roles. Following the general trend of post-Parsonian sociology in the 1960s, however, sociolinguists and interactionists shifted focus inward toward language, micro-pro- cesses, and variations within classrooms. Sociolinguists, for their part, paid close atten- tion to conversational dynamics and began to doc- ument routinized (generally dyadic) turn-taking
  • 10. structures in activities such as recitation and group work (Mehan 1979; Erickson 1982; Stodolsky 1988). Interactionists, in contrast, drew on ethnog- raphies to illuminate the ongoing negotiation of control and defiance in the classroom (Jackson Diehl and McFarland 327 at WESTERN OREGON UNIVERSITY on February 10, 2015soe.sagepub.comDownloaded from http://soe.sagepub.com/ 1968; Metz 1978; Pace 2006). Both streams of research focused on moment-to-moment dynamics of interaction, but each offered a fundamentally different conception of the nature of classroom order and how to research it. The study of these two aspects of social order— the formal structure of talk and the ongoing negotia- tion of identities—has remained largely disconnected in the sociological literature. One exception is in
  • 11. Goffman’s work on what he called the ‘‘interaction order’’ (Goffman 1967, 1981, 1983). 1 Goffman argued that interaction is multidimensional and that the individual has multiple obligations to and interests in maintaining order (Goffman 1983:5). To that end, the actor uses various strategies and techniques to ensure his or her own proper involvement as well as the proper involvement of others (Goffman 1967:115). Doing so entails meeting both routine (system) and ritual (normative) requirements of inter- action (Goffman 1981:14-15). Problematically, how- ever, Goffman did not adequately delineate between these two central features of situated order, nor did he provide means for studying them (Schegloff 1988). Part of our aim in this work is to address both of these shortcomings. First, the requirement of routine concerns render-
  • 12. ing interactional exchanges into recursive and pre- dictable configurations based on a working agreement about ‘‘what is going on here’’ (Goffman 1974; 1981). The most important organizing conven- tions in classrooms are academic activities and the typified sets of relations, orientations, and discourses they entail (Erickson 1982; Doyle and Carter 1984; Stodolsky 1988). Activities provide routinized fea- tures such as the opening and closing of communica- tion channels, the distribution of turns, and communication rules for bystanders (Goffman 1981). In a classroom discussion, for example, stu- dents follow teacher-defined and enforced rules for legitimate talk, generally taking the floor and speak- ing only once given permission to do so. From this perspective, classrooms can be con- ceptualized as networks of discourse and routine as the mobilization of those networks into configu-
  • 13. rations related to particular academic activities. Stability is the term we use for when these networks become recursive, or patterned in a way that repeats through time. Instability, in contrast, occurs when these configurations are loosened through gaffes, interruptions, mistakes, or the occurrence of other digressions and side events. Stability is associated with positive learning environments because pre- dictable behavior allows teachers and students to expend their cognitive energy on the academic con- tent of tasks rather than interactional coordination (Leinhardt and Greeno 1986). For these reasons, effective teachers rely on an array of repeatedly used activities (Doyle 1980:13). The requirement of ritual, in contrast, refers to the feature of interaction related to the ratification of valued identities (Goffman 1967:19). In typical social encounters, actors coordinate actions not
  • 14. only around recognized activities but also around identities they recognize and value in their commu- nication. For example, in everyday conversation, Goffman (1981:16) repeatedly showed how per- sons show deference to the demeanor of their inter- action partners, treating selves as something of social worth. These ritual elements are present in all successfully coordinated interactions since identities of speakers, listeners, and the social rela- tionships between the two are often normatively valued (Goffman 1981:269). However, Goffman failed to account for the fact that the nature of the institutional context shapes the manner in which selves are expressed and validated (Strong 1988). We address this shortcoming by studying how the ritual requirements of selves are variably met within the structures of different activities. In classrooms, the successful accomplishment
  • 15. of ritual interaction creates a shared sense of respect, or goodwill, between teachers and students (Dreeben 1970; Bidwell 2005). Interpersonal good- will often is marked by subjective and experiential factors that are difficult to observe. Therefore, teachers and students mostly infer the presence of goodwill from overt acts of compliance and defi- ance (Kelman 1961). The absence of defiance in social encounters can be referred to as the behav- ioral expression of concord. To be clear, concord is not synonymous with the subjective experience of classroom goodwill. In the first part of our empirical analysis, we address concord as an observable feature of interaction patterns, and in the second part, we look at goodwill as a feature of subjective experience reported on surveys. As with routine, meeting ritual requirements of interaction has a positive effect on teaching and
  • 16. learning. Students, for their part, defy authority when they feel their character has been imputed in a demeaning way (Lawrence et al. 1984:122). Teachers, in turn, feel student defiance threatens their institutional identity and devalues the invest- ment of time and energy they put into an activity (Lawrence et al. 1984:121; McFarland 2001, 2004). When there is classroom goodwill, 328 Sociology of Education 85(4) at WESTERN OREGON UNIVERSITY on February 10, 2015soe.sagepub.comDownloaded from http://soe.sagepub.com/ interactions are characterized by warmth and respect, and with those come lower levels of disrup- tive behaviors (Battistich et al. 1997), higher levels of on-task behavior (Marzano, Marzano, and Pickering 2003), and greater intellectual risk taking (Murray and Greenberg 2000).
  • 17. The Variable Relationship between Stability and Concord. Classrooms are crowded settings where a multiplicity of doings and interests come into and fall out of focus over the course of a lesson (Jackson 1968; McFarland 2005). In the flux of affairs, situational requirements of coordination and identity are var- iably catered to and met. This means that there are two distinct ways that disorder can arise: through either problems related to instability in activity structures or challenges to concord. How any par- ticular communicative act relates to these interac- tional anchors depends, however, on the nature of the activity within which it takes place. This conceptualization emphasizes the nature of participation, or the way that individuals align or orient themselves toward the activity and its rules and expectations for speaking, listening, and turn taking (Erickson 2004). Sociolinguists have long
  • 18. described the pattern of these alignments and orien- tations as the participation framework of an activity (Goffman 1981). 2 These frameworks define inter- personal expectations and obligations such that everyone is called on to adopt particular interaction roles or participation statuses (e.g., speaker or hearer). These participation statuses are then ar- ranged in such a way as to form a larger pattern. When recursively enacted, it becomes a recognized, guiding framework to which everyone adheres (Gee 1999). This is true for both academic activi- ties prescribed by the teacher and informal activi- ties initiated by students (e.g., gossip in Eder and Enke 1991). The successful cueing of an activ- ity’s participation framework, the mobilization of classroom participants into the activity, and its
  • 19. stable reenactment over time are quite an accomplishment. From this perspective, the basic empirical units out of which classroom order is generated are dis- crete communicative events (i.e., utterances) pro- duced by one actor and directed toward one or more targets (Butts 2008:158). Adapting the lan- guage of Goffman (1981), we refer to these as dis- cursive ‘‘moves.’’ Actors use discursive moves to present particular orientations toward each other and toward shared interaction (see ‘‘footings’’ in Goffman 1981). These moves can commence inter- actional alignments, which in turn lead to larger changes in the structural configuration of group communication (Goodwin 1998; Gibson 2003). Teachers use discourse to manage the timing and sequencing of interactions to strike a balance between coercion and engendering voluntary partic-
  • 20. ipation that allows them to mobilize students in and out of the participation framework of academic activities (Kounin 1970; McFarland 2004). Students, for their part, also use discourse both to align with academic activities and to resist them, all the while asserting and expressing valued identi- ties (Alpert 1991; McFarland 2001). The overall pic- ture is one of an ongoing struggle about positions in the classroom network of talk (Grenfell 1998) as dis- tinct activity frameworks are cued via discourse and ensuing patterns of association form and fall (see Figure 1 for a schematic and Supplemental Online Materials for a network movie demonstrating pat- tern switches at http://soe.sagepub.com/content/85/ 4/326/suppl/DC1). Two features of activity frameworks are espe- cially important for understanding the situated nature of routine and ritual imperatives: prescribed
  • 21. interactivity and the centralization of attention (Stinchcombe 1968). Prescribed interactivity re- fers to the spread of responsibility or range of par- ticipants necessary to enact a given activity structure. It also refers to the degree to which stu- dents are afforded active or nonactive participation statuses (e.g., speaker or hearer). This degree of in- teractivity ranges from no participation; to some participation, but only as an animator of other’s views (e.g., in recitation where the student recalls a fact); to more participation and authoring of views; to full participation and responsibility. For example, group work requires frequent interaction between many persons assuming the participation status of cospeakers, and student presentations call on students to take turns presenting their own ideas to the entire class. In contrast, lectures and tests call on students to be hearers or noninteractive
  • 22. individual workers. The centralization of attention, in contrast, refers to the degree to which an activity focuses on a single speaker and ignores others in the audience. This shapes the extent to which the possibilities for identity expression and validation are evenly or unevenly distributed across partici- pants. For example, recitation is highly centralized, with all eyes on the speaker, whereas seatwork is highly decentralized, with each student focused Diehl and McFarland 329 at WESTERN OREGON UNIVERSITY on February 10, 2015soe.sagepub.comDownloaded from http://soe.sagepub.com/ on his or her own work. Common classroom activ- ities can be broadly characterized by the interrela- tion of their amount of interactivity and attention structure, as summarized in Table 1. As activities and their features of interactivity and
  • 23. attention change, so too does the relationship between routine and ritual aspects of interaction. Activities that call on students to assume passive participation statuses are often easier to coordinate and stabilize (e.g., lecture), but they undermine goodwill because they centralize identity valuation on only a few peo- ple, leaving others unacknowledged. By contrast, activities that call on students to assume active partic- ipation statuses are harder to coordinate (more au- thors defining the situation), but they ratify and value a greater number of identities, thereby encour- aging goodwill. As such, activities activate different situational concerns. Solving problems of routine may create problems of ritual, and solving problems of ritual may create problems of routine. In sum, this article proposes a significant recon- ceptualization of social order in classrooms. We argue that the most fundamental problems of class-
  • 24. room order do not stem from factors that hold across classroom settings, such as classroom com- positions, role tensions, or even poor-fitting instructional activities. Rather, the core problems of social order are multidimensional and situa- tional. Classroom order is multidimensional in that it pertains to the dynamic interrelation of two distinct requirements for successful activities: the coordination of interaction into institutionalized pat- terns (routine) and the validation of the identities of participants (ritual). And classroom order is situational in that finding balance between these interrelated as- pects of interaction is an ongoing practical Table 1. Dimensions of Activity Frameworks Focus of attention (ritual) Centralized and focused Distributed and differentiated Prescribed Interactivity (Routine) Active Presentation, discussion Group work, free time Nonactive Recitation, lecture Seatwork, test, audiovisual
  • 25. Channel Undefined Structure 1 Lecture Structure 2 Group Work Undefined: Available Stories, Props Discourses, Etc.., Transition Release Figure 1. Shifting participation structures in class periods 330 Sociology of Education 85(4) at WESTERN OREGON UNIVERSITY on February 10, 2015soe.sagepub.comDownloaded from http://soe.sagepub.com/ achievement accomplished jointly by teachers and students within the dynamic network of moment-to-
  • 26. moment classroom communication. As such, class- room participants find themselves in a context with a multiplicity of problems that vary with the situation. In what follows, we turn to the empirical study by which we establish this argument. SETTINGS AND DATA While researchers have recently begun to introduce increasingly systematic methods for classroom observation (e.g., Cameron, Connor, and Morrison 2005; Good, Mulryan, and McCaslin 2006; Pianta and Hamre 2009), this work typically focuses on either sequences of dyadic interactions (usually between teacher and single students), thereby unwittingly bracketing all other concurrent interaction, or summary counts of behaviors, thereby obscuring interactional dynamics. We argue that the goal of a theory of classroom social order, however, is to elaborate global patterns of
  • 27. interaction not evident in individual episodes or se- quences of talk (Gibson 2008; Wyatt et al. 2008). To accomplish this, we drew on an unparalleled data set of streaming interaction recorded in two high schools during the 1996-1997 school year (McFarland 1999, 2001, 2004). It consists of obser- vations in 650 separate class periods across 165 classrooms. Of those, 601 class periods across 153 classrooms had observation records suitable for quantitative analyses. This yielded an analyz- able sample of more than 800,000 sequential turns of interaction. In what follows, we describe these settings and the manner in which observations were recorded and formatted for analysis. Settings. The first high school, Magnet High, is a racially integrated elite magnet school located in an inner-city neighborhood of a large midwestern metropolitan area. The second high school, Rural High, is located more than 500 miles away in
  • 28. a town of 17,000 residents. It is a traditional rural school that is racially homogeneous and serves pre- dominantly lower- to middle-income students. Magnet and Rural represent distinct learning envi- ronments, but they nonetheless have surprisingly similar student and teacher behaviors. More specific details about Magnet and Rural High Schools and the students who attend them can be found in McFarland (2004). Data. Classroom observations focused on 10th- and 12th-grade core subjects in math, sci- ence, English, and history, but some effort was made to observe foreign language and arts classes. A sum of observed class periods by school and subject matter is presented in Table 2. 3 Core sub- jects were selected because they allowed for more comparability across schools than elective sub-
  • 29. jects could have. Within core subjects, 10th- and 12th-grade classes were selected because multiple observations of the same individuals were desir- able and the observer was unable to observe the entire array of courses offered. The coding scheme used was developed from prior work (Mehan 1979; Stodolsky 1988; McFarland 1999) and piloted two times before this study. The resulting data set has been used for statistical modeling in several previous articles (McFarland 2001; Moody, McFarland, and Bender-deMoll 2005; Bender-deMoll and McFarland 2006). An assessment of coding reli- ability was performed using classroom videos, and it is presented in the Supplemental Online Materials. Due to the nature of data collection, coder reliability noticeably decreased in activity segments wherein simultaneous talk occurred
  • 30. (e.g., free time and group work). This resulted in underreported frequencies in the more fluid and de- centralized task structures. However, the sheer vol- ume and breadth of total observations decreases the amount of error that affects results. PART A. ANTECEDENTS OF INTERACTIONAL CONCORD AND STABILITY In the first two sets of analyses, below, we examine the discursive antecedents of interactional stability and concord as well as their relationship with dif- ferent academic activities. Table 2. Class Periods Observed by Subject and School School Subject Magnet Rural Row totals Mathematics 75 93 168 Science 58 117 175 Social science 51 37 88 English 79 60 139 Foreign language 15 14 29 Visual arts 2 2
  • 31. Columns totals 278 323 601 Diehl and McFarland 331 at WESTERN OREGON UNIVERSITY on February 10, 2015soe.sagepub.comDownloaded from http://soe.sagepub.com/ Dependent Variables: Operationalizing Interactional Stability and Concord. Since our interest is in the dynamic, and hence temporal, nature of talk, our first step is to aggregate com- munications into slices of class time (2½ mi- nutes) 4 and to render them into network matrices reflecting two distinct types of transaction (McFarland 2005): one for task-related communi- cation and another for social-related communica- tion. Because we are interested in higher-order structures of interaction, we follow one of the lon- gest traditions in network analysis and examine the composition of interactional triads (Taylor
  • 32. 1970; Chase 1974; Hallinan 1976; Kalish and Robins 2006). We look at triads because they reflect forms of positioning in interaction that extend across multiple dyads, and as such they identify various interaction roles or local partici- pation structures that particular individuals move in and out of during a class period. In this way, the study of triads provides a better means for measuring the global structures of network arrangement as they capture the pattern of depen- dencies that extends beyond simple pairings of ac- tors (Butts 2008). 5 Analysis of triads is relatively straightforward as networks can be broken down into a finite set of 16 triad types under which all observed token triads are subsumed (Wasserman and Faust 1994). Conceptually, each triad type can be seen as reflect-
  • 33. ing a particular interaction role. Hence, a high count for triad number 4 (021D: ) indicates many of the triads have central actors who speak asymmetrically to others (i.e., talk is not reciprocated by the hearer), much like lectures and student presentations have. 6 Recitation will include a number of direct teacher- student interactions (i.e., question and answer), so there are a large number of number 12 triads (120D: ). A high count for triad number 3 (102) and number 16 (300) reveals a great deal of cluster- ing in interaction, a characteristic of group work. The pattern of triads thus reflects the interaction roles being used in a particular activity and thereby the kind of coordinated behavior that induces discursive stability (see Burt 1990 for a similar method). Using this framework, we can look at the corre- lation of triad frequencies to assess how similar or dissimilar the pattern of triads (and thereby interac-
  • 34. tion roles) is within a classroom from one moment of class time to the next. Here, we use a very simple measure of similarity—correlation of triadic arrays (see the Supplemental Online Materials for details). We do not want the size of the network to shape the measure, nor do we want zero cells to eclipse the estimation of variance. Since the triad census in sparse networks always results in high counts that would drive correlation values, we omit null triads (triad number 1, 001) before gener- ating correlations across successive time slices. The resultant measure of interaction order corre- lates the census for 30 types of triads (15 for task and 15 for social interaction, omitting nulls) at one time point with the one succeeding it. The mea- sure is highly skewed (mean = .66, standard devia- tion = .39, and median = .89), suggesting much of schooling entails either stable or unstable patterns
  • 35. of interaction roles. The plotting of this variable over class time corresponds well with observer field notes and recollections (see Figure A in the Supplemental Online Materials). To utilize it in predictive models, we binarize the measure at the median (.89) such that values above are considered stabilizing moments and values below are consid- ered destabilizing moments. In doing so, we lose some of the variance in the correlation measure, but in return we gain reliable standard errors and tests of significance. Interactional concord, in contrast, is defined as time slices that lack conflict, or what we call here discord. Student and teacher behavior is coded as being discordant when it entails the airing of a neg- ative sanction. From the perspective of the student, this can be conceptualized as an everyday form of resistance that aims to undermine task or teacher
  • 36. through the expression of discontent, challenge, or refusal (Metz 1978; McFarland 2001). Such acts may be active rebellions against teacher authority, or they may be subtler in the form of jokes or sarcastic comments. Here, a degree of interpretation is required. If a student’s joke about a teacher or the task stops class, it is recorded as an instance of discord. Thus, discordant acts are observed as intentional, publicly displayed forms of student-initiated disruptions of classroom order, whether they are in the form of refusal, challenge, insult, personal ridicule, or loud complaint. Time slices lacking any such acts, then, are coded as in- stances of concord. Since our measures of stability and concord are time dependent, it is likely serial autocorrelation is present. For research on classroom order, it is a not a nuisance to be eliminated but rather a central fea-
  • 37. ture of what we are interested in. For this reason we incorporate autocorrelation into all our models by 332 Sociology of Education 85(4) at WESTERN OREGON UNIVERSITY on February 10, 2015soe.sagepub.comDownloaded from http://soe.sagepub.com/ including a lagged variable for stability and con- cord at the previous time slice (Beck 2001). Independent Variables: Timing, Activity, and Discourse. A variety of independent vari- ables is used for our different models (including, as mentioned above, stability and concord them- selves). In what follows, we briefly describe pre- dictors and their hypothesized effects, but more details about each measure are included in the Supplemental Online Materials (see Table A). Briefly, the three categories of independent varia- bles we include are temporality, prescribed activ- ity, and discursive moves.
  • 38. Timing. The first set of variables captures the effects of timing on stability. Current work sug- gests that the opening and closing of classes are relatively informal and disorganized phases of interaction (Mehan 1979; Cazden 1988). Research has also found that student engagement starts low, grows higher as the class continues, and then lowers again at the end of the class (Doyle 1980:12). We normalize this intraperiod timing variable by dividing each observation of class time by the maximum number of minutes the specific class period was observed. Thus, our measure of class time varies from 0 to 1, with 0 being the start and 1 being the end. We also include a measure for class time squared to plot a function for nonlinear effects of class time on stability. Other literature suggests that classes set- tle into a routine after the first several months of
  • 39. the school year, so we also include an interperiod timing variable (Woods 1983; Doyle 1986). For this we use coarser units of time, months into the school year (0 = August, 1 = September, etc.). Finally, a third timing variable concerns transitions or slices of class time that span distinct activity segments. Transitions are liminal mo- ments that lack the well-defined behavioral ex- pectations associated with participation structures. Activity. The second set of independent varia- bles concern teacher-prescribed activities (Bossert 1979; Doyle 1986; Stodolsky 1988; Stein, Grover, and Henningsen 1996). We test the effects of activities on classroom stability by creating nine dummy variables: lecture, recitation, discussion, student presentations, group work, undefined/free time, seatwork, test/quiz, and audio/film. It is an empirical question whether activities involving high or low levels of interac-
  • 40. tivity and centralized or decentralized attention structures are associated with greater stability and interpersonal concord. Discourse. The third set of variables involves discursive moves. Within activity segments, in- stances of talk between pairs of senders and re- ceivers were recorded based on the identification numbers listed on classroom seating charts. Even though talk varied in duration, no attempt was made to capture that information. When long turns did arise, the observer recorded them as if the same interaction were repeated each minute of class time. Each interaction was coded for who spoke to whom, the content of interactions in terms of topic (i.e., task or sociable), and the tone in which they were stated (i.e., none, techni- cal, or moral) as well as the form of speech used in the communication (see Supplemental Online
  • 41. Materials, Table A). Summarizing all these fea- tures of interaction may seem like a complex task, but a simple shorthand captures the information. Take the following sequence of interaction coded in shorthand: T-A, T-QA, A-TG, T-10Q, 10-TG, T-10 (1t), T-12Q, 12-13 S, 13-12 S, T-12 (–m), T-13 (–m), T-A. It shows the teacher speak- ing to everyone (broadcast), asking everyone a question, and everyone giving the teacher a choral response. After that, the teacher asks student num- ber 10 a question, to which he gives an answer, and the teacher replies that student 10 is correct (‘‘1t’’ = positive technical evaluation). The teacher then asks student 12 a question, and student 12 turns to socialize with student 13 (note ‘‘S’’ = social). At this point, the teacher sanctions students 12 and 13 for their social behavior (‘‘–m’’ = negative moral
  • 42. evaluation) and then turns to broadcast to the class again. Empirical Models: Fixed-effects Hazard Models. Again, our first two sets of models look at the interactional correlates of stability and concord. First, to model the effects of activi- ties and discourse on stability, we use conditional logit models that render invariant characteristics of classrooms and schools as fixed effects (see Allison 1999:188-97). We use this model because it corrects for standard error biases (presence of clustering), spuriousness (i.e., correlation between individual and cluster-level variables), and hetero- geneity shrinkage (i.e., attenuation toward 0 in presence of heterogeneity; see Allison 1999:212- 13). Moreover, these models present the most Diehl and McFarland 333 at WESTERN OREGON UNIVERSITY on February 10, 2015soe.sagepub.comDownloaded from http://soe.sagepub.com/
  • 43. conservative test for the effects of micro- mechanisms endogenous to classrooms. The conditional logit with fixed effects can be specified as follows: Logðpit=ð1 � pitÞÞ¼ ai1bxit: ð1Þ Let pit be the probability that a class is stable at time t. Let xit be a vector of variables for classroom i at time t. These vectors can contain variables that differ over time (rates for forms of speech) or remain constant (teacher’s race). The ai term repre- sents all the differences among classrooms that are stable over time. For all time points, ai is the same for a given classroom, and a positive correlation arises among observed outcomes (Allison 1999:189). When equation 1 is applied to panel data, each classroom adds an additional parameter (a dummy variable for each classroom) to the
  • 44. model and can create biases in parameter estimates. To solve this, conditional likelihood estimations are constructed that condition the number of 1s and 0s observed in each classroom. If a class is stable at time 1 and time 2 but not at time 3, one calculates the probability that stability arose at times 1 and 2 but not at time 3. This probability is then written as a function of the explanatory variables and the b parameters, and the probabilities are multiplied for all classes to get the overall likelihood. When done, the ai parameters cancel from the likelihood function (Allison 1999:190), and the biases in parameter estimates are removed. 7 As a precaution, we also ran mixed logit models and found nearly the exact results. We rely on the results from condi- tional models with fixed effects because they are more conservative and robust and focus our analy-
  • 45. ses within classrooms. The eventual model estimates moments of sta- bility and concord within classrooms and uses lagged independent variables: Yðt:t11ÞðstabilityÞ¼ a1b1Yðt:t�1ÞðstabilityÞ 1b2XtðtimeÞ1b3Xtðtime 2Þ 1b4XtðmonthÞ1b5XtðtransitionÞ 1b6Xtðtask structureÞ::: 1b7Xtðforms of speechÞ::: 1mtðclassroomÞ1e; YtðconcordÞ¼ a 1b1Yt�1ðconcordÞ 1b2XtðtimeÞ 1b3Xtðtime 2Þ1b4XtðmonthÞ 1b5XtðtransitionÞ 1b6Xtðtask structureÞ::: 1b7Xtðforms of speechÞ::: 1mtðclassroomÞ1e; ð2Þ where m is the fixed effect for each classroom.
  • 46. Results: General Effects of Timing, Activity, and Discourse. We begin by present- ing results for unconditional mixed models of stabil- ity and concord in Table 3. These models decompose variance at three levels: the variance occurring between classrooms, between periods, and within individual class periods. The results indicate that Table 3. Decomposition of Variance for Interactional Stability and Concord Level Variance component Standard error Percentage variance Stability Between classes 0.22 0.06*** 13 Between periods 0.53 0.06*** 31 Between segments (residual) 0.94 0.01*** 56 Concord Between classes 0.96 0.161*** 40 Between periods 0.59 0.072*** 25 Between segments (residual) 0.82 0.012*** 35 Source: McFarland (1999) classroom interaction study. ***p .001. 334 Sociology of Education 85(4) at WESTERN OREGON UNIVERSITY on February 10, 2015soe.sagepub.comDownloaded from http://soe.sagepub.com/
  • 47. 87 percent of the variance of stability and 60 percent of conflict are within classrooms and class periods (in contrast to between classrooms). This is strong evidence that the primary levers of classroom order reside within classroom situations and their mo- ments, so we put our focus there. We look first at the central tendencies of factors we think generally influence interactional stability and concord. These results are generated with conditional fixed-effects models and can be found in Table 4. For ease of interpretation, results are pre- sented as odds ratios. Here the ratios represent the pre- dicted odds of network pattern stability (from t to t 11) and the presence of concord (at t) given a one-standard-deviation change in each explanatory variable. Odds ratios greater than 1 mean the variable adds to pattern stability and concord, whereas being
  • 48. less than 1 means it increases instability and conflict. Table 4. Longitudinal Fixed-effects Logit Models, Predicting Classroom Stability and Moments of Concord (Odds) Stability Concord Model 1 Model 2 Model 3 Model 3 Model 4 Model 5 Timing variables Preceding stability 1.68*** 1.66*** 1.66*** — — — Preceding concord — — — 1.24*** 1.24*** 1.24*** Class time 1.40*** 1.29** 1.21 1.97*** 1.88*** 1.80*** Class time 3 2 0.69*** 0.81* 0.87 0.53*** 0.56*** 0.59*** Months into school year 0.92** 0.94* 0.93* 0.80*** 0.79*** 0.81*** Transition segment 0.87*** 0.87*** 0.88*** 0.88*** 0.89*** 0.88*** Activities Lecture 1.58*** 1.16*** 1.00 1.01 Recitation RC RC RC RC Discussion 0.85 0.99 0.93* 0.97 Presentation 1.09 1.06* 0.97 1.02 Group work 0.61*** 0.94* 1.11** 1.12** Test/quiz 0.58*** 0.91** 1.18*** 1.33*** Audiovisual 0.46*** 0.88*** 1.21*** 1.34*** Seatwork 0.40*** 0.79*** 0.96 1.07 Undefined 0.46*** 0.89*** 0.96 1.03 Forms of speech Teacher broadcast 1.11* 1.24*** Teacher question 0.91* 0.86** Teacher technical 1.05 1.03 Teacher joke 0.93* 0.96
  • 49. Student broadcast 1.00 0.96 Student question 0.97 0.91* Student technical 1.01 1.07 Student joke 0.96 0.86*** Teacher-student task 0.92* 0.70*** Student-student task 0.89** 1.07 Teacher-student social 0.80*** 0.84*** Student-student social 0.86** 0.88 Model statistics Wald chi-square 530 747 840 169 228 425 Source: McFarland (1999) classroom interaction study. Note: RC = reference category. N = 8,946. Stable instances = 50 percent of cases. Conflict instances = 20 percent of cases. Invariant characteristics of classrooms are fixed in these models. Presented values = Exponential (bx 3 stddev x), unless dichotomous, then the coefficient is raw. Dashes indicate variables are not included in particular models. *p .05. **p .01. ***p .001. Diehl and McFarland 335 at WESTERN OREGON UNIVERSITY on February 10, 2015soe.sagepub.comDownloaded from http://soe.sagepub.com/ The first models look only at timing variables. We start in model 1 for stability and model 4 for concord and identify a variety of expected results. First, stability in the preceding time slice signifi-
  • 50. cantly increases the probability of future stability. The same relationship holds for past and future concord. Second, class time increases the odds of both stability and concord while class time squared decreases it, suggesting an inverse curvilinear rela- tionship in which periods open with chaotic pat- terns of interaction, settle into stable and concordant instructional routines, and then grow chaotic and conflictual again as periods draw to a close. This intraperiod pattern is embedded in the larger interperiod trajectory in which instability and conflict increase during the year. We also see that transitional periods between activities are more likely to be unstable and discordant. Overall, we see that stability and concord are boot- strapped from moment to moment, have a cyclic pattern in every class period, decrease during tran- sitions, and slowly diminish during the year. These
  • 51. results correspond with standard qualitative de- scriptions of phases in classroom interaction (e.g., Doyle 1980), lending credence to the validity of our measures as a proxy for classroom order. These similar results suggest that there may be parallels in how stability and concord are con- structed and sustained during interaction. As a result of introducing teacher-prescribed activities in mod- els 2 and 5, however, important differences begin to emerge. We use recitation as the baseline in our models because not only was it the most common activity in our data, but it is generally found to be the most common teacher-prescribed activity in classrooms (Mehan 1979; Cazden 1988). We find that centralized and student-isolated instructional formats such as lectures and seatwork channel atten- tion and suppress interactivity and thereby forge interactional stability and at least the appearance
  • 52. of concord (i.e., compliance and a lack of conflict). In contrast, student-centered tasks such as group work encourage interactivity and decentralize the focus of attention, thereby providing opportunities for identity validation at the expense of coordination and stability. Last, formats such as discussion require the centralization of attention and greater in- teractivity, thereby rendering situations more rou- tine but at the same time increasing the possibility of conflict by offering only limited turns of talk, about which students may fight. Finally, in models 3 and 6, we move to an even more fine-grained level of analysis and include different forms of talk. In terms of similarity across the models, we find that teacher broadcasts are asso- ciated with an increase in stability and concord whereas teacher questions are associated with a decrease in both. In the case of broadcasts, teachers
  • 53. are using their talk for control by stopping other inter- actions and centralizing attention. With questions, however, teachers are providing students turns of talk, which present opportunities for digression and defiance. The differences are perhaps more interesting, however. While teacher jokes are associated with a decrease in stability, they have no association with concord. In contrast, student jokes (as well as questions) are associated with a decrease in con- cord but have no association with stability. We posit that this is because teacher jokes represent di- gressions from routine whereas student jokes and questions may be challenges to teacher control of attention or interactivity. Finally, neither social nor task-related dyadic talk between students is associated with concord, supporting the idea that how teachers manage attention and interaction is
  • 54. the key to shaping order. Teacher-student talk of either kind, in contrast, has a significant negative association with concord. This is, again, because dyadic communication of all kinds between teach- ers and students opens up the interactive space nec- essary for conflict and defiance to occur. Overall, the consistency of the results in Table 4 with our existing qualitative understanding of the classroom provides a strong warrant for our approach. In addition, they support our claim that classroom order is multidimensional. Conflict, as a manifestation of struggles about positions in the classroom system, is interrelated but distinct from the recursive patterns of interaction that generate stability. Nonetheless, what we have presented thus does not go far enough into the situated nature of classroom order. These results reflect central tendencies, but still missing is a discussion of
  • 55. how problems of order are managed differently across activities as the same discursive move can have differential effects depending on the partici- pation framework within which it takes place (Tannen 1994:19). This interrelation between activities and forms of talk is where we turn next. Results: Interaction Terms and the Structuring of Different Classroom Activities. We now identify the discursive moves utilized to overcome problems of order within 336 Sociology of Education 85(4) at WESTERN OREGON UNIVERSITY on February 10, 2015soe.sagepub.comDownloaded from http://soe.sagepub.com/ each activity. The full analysis can be found in the Supplemental Online Materials (see Table B), but for the sake of brevity here, we focus on four activities that represent variability in interactivity and attention: lecture, student presentations, group
  • 56. work, and seatwork. As discussed earlier, control of interactivity and attention shapes classroom order by varying the diffusion of responsibility for coordinating routine and the spread of identi- ties needing to be ritually verified. Instead of using comparison values for this part of the anal- ysis, we focus directly on the conditional effects of mechanisms within each type of prescribed activity. 8 The results are presented in Table 5 and Figure 2. We will explain these results by focusing on the pattern of odds ratios within each activity. The first column in Table 5 and the first set of bar plots in Figure 2 relate results for lectures in terms of both stability and concord. During a lecture, only the teacher has a legitimate speaking turn, so it is not surprising to see that teacher broadcasts are associ-
  • 57. ated with an increase in both stability and concord and that student broadcasts have the opposite effect. Student talk is slightly related to both stability and concord in the form of technical comments, which can be seen as adding appropriate coauthorship to the lecture. What is perhaps more interesting is that social talk, either between students or with the teacher, is associated with a decrease in stability but not in concord. This suggests that social talk leads to digressions from the routinized form of the activity but not in a way that threatens the ritual enactment of institutional roles. In contrast, student Table 5. Conditional Effects of Speech Forms on Stability and Concord, by Task Lecture Presentation Group work Seatwork Main effect Stability Teacher broadcast 1.29*** 1.11** 0.89*** 1.00 1.11* Teacher question 1.06y 1.05 0.96 1.01 0.91* Teacher technical 1.02 1.00 1.00 1.00 1.05 Teacher joke 0.95 1.02 0.99 0.97 0.93* Student broadcast 0.80*** 1.18*** 0.84* 0.92* 1.00
  • 58. Student question 0.97 0.97 0.97 1.04 0.98 Student technical 1.06y 0.96 0.97 1.03 1.01 Student joke 0.97y 0.98 1.01 1.02 0.97 Teacher-student task 0.92** 0.98 1.00 1.00 0.93* Student-student task 1.01 1.10** 1.08y 0.92** 0.89** Teacher-student social 0.90*** 0.97* 0.95*** 0.95* 0.81*** Student-student social 0.91* 0.90** 0.99 1.03 0.86** Concord Teacher broadcast 1.55*** 1.21 0.55* 1.22 1.24*** Teacher question 1.07 1.04 0.99 0.97 0.86** Teacher technical 1.10 0.90 1.32 0.97 1.03 Teacher joke 0.96 0.90 1.00 0.83* 0.96 Student broadcast 0.58* 1.21** 0.29* 0.74y 0.96 Student question 0.91 0.86 0.78 0.96 0.91* Student technical 1.33y 1.20 0.66 1.28 1.07 Student joke 0.85* 0.94 0.80 0.89y 0.86*** Teacher-student task 0.58*** 0.69** 0.57*** 0.70*** 0.70*** Student-student task 1.00 1.31y 1.35*** 1.25* 1.07 Teacher-student social 0.95 1.07 0.99 0.95 0.84*** Student-student social 0.94 1.01 0.95 1.09 0.88 Source: McFarland (1999) classroom interaction study. Note: N = 8,946. Stable = 50 percent of cases. Conflict = 20 percent of cases. Invariant characteristics of classrooms are fixed. Main effects lists values from models 3 and 5 from Table 4 and reflects the norm. Presented values = Exponential (bx 3 stddev x). *p .05. **p .01. ***p .001. yp .10. Diehl and McFarland 337 at WESTERN OREGON UNIVERSITY on February 10, 2015soe.sagepub.comDownloaded from http://soe.sagepub.com/
  • 59. Figure 2. Effects of discursive moves on stability and concord by type of activity (odds) Note: Effect = Odds of Y given 11 stddev in X (form of speech). *p .05, p .10. Black = Y (stability), Grey = Y (nondefiance). 338 Sociology of Education 85(4) at WESTERN OREGON UNIVERSITY on February 10, 2015soe.sagepub.comDownloaded from http://soe.sagepub.com/ jokes are slightly related to instability but more strongly to conflict. This may be because teachers see student jokes as either a threat to their control of the activity or as an undermining of their author- itative role. Student presentations operate much like lectures, with a central speaker and a largely passive audi- ence, but students take turns at the center of the dis- cursive network as the focus of collective attention (although the teacher never fully relinquishes his
  • 60. or her role as the central coordinator of talk). For this reason, and in contrast to lectures, student broad- casts (and to a lesser degree student-student task talk) correspond with both stability and concord. Like during a lecture, however, social talk with the teacher or between students leads to destabilization through digressions but not to conflict. Turning speeches into conversations undermines the gather- ing that is being forged around individual presenters’ identities. Finally, in student presentations, task- related talk between teacher and student is associ- ated with conflict because the nature of the activity is to give students more authorship over shared talk, and teacher-student task talk may represent at- tempts by teachers to reassert their control over dis- course or their ritualized role as arbiter of correct and appropriate knowledge. Group work is essentially the inverse of the cen-
  • 61. tralized formats discussed so far in that it is a highly interactive and decentralized task that relies on the generation and maintenance of reciprocity within numerous simultaneous conversations. For that rea- son, it is stabilized by direct task communications between students and made problematic by asymmet- ric talk of any kind. As such, teacher involvement, especially in the form of a broadcast, shifts interaction into asymmetric and centralized forms that represent a disruption in the enactment of group work roles. Another important difference with centralized for- mats is that group work can sustain reciprocal social talk between peers without resulting in destabiliza- tion. Social talk need not lead to disorder by usurping limited turns of talk or foci of attention; rather, social talk becomes less important as conversations and identity verifications become diffused throughout the classroom.
  • 62. Finally, in seatwork, the task is private and sta- bilized by nonaction between individuals, which also means there are few opportunities for either validation or contestation of identities. Looking at the results, we see that seatwork is similar to group work in that student broadcasts and teacher social- ization, both of which shift interaction away from activity-prescribed forms, are destabilizing. It dif- fers from group work, however, in that task-related talk between students is also destabilizing, which makes sense given that a primary difference between the two activities is the degree to which work is independent. Seatwork is also often not a cognitively demanding activity, and this is per- haps part of the reason we see that reciprocal social talk between students is associated with neither sta- bility nor concord. Students can essentially multi- task while working alone on routine academic
  • 63. tasks and at the same time privately socialize so long as the combination does not lead to disruption. Nonsocial teacher talk has no effect on either stabil- ity or concord, which suggests that most seatwork does not require teacher assistance, but when it does occur it has little influence on the activity. Overall, we see that each activity has a unique participation structure that influences how particular forms of talk relate to meeting routine and ritual im- peratives. The more centralized attention is and the less interactivity is sanctioned, the easier it is for the teacher to coordinate students and to generate stable and concordant social order in the classroom by controlling talk. As activities provide more possi- bilities for interaction, the ability to display, vali- date, and contest valued identities also increases. When, however, interactivity increases but attention remains centralized, conflict becomes more likely as
  • 64. more is at stake for each turn of talk. In a student pre- sentation, for example, teachers must coordinate stu- dent talk but be on guard to prevent students from using their turn as a platform for ritual work. When interactivity increases and attention becomes decentralized, as in group work, we see less conflict as discursive opportunities for ritual verification abound and, with most interaction taking place out- side the view of the teacher, less ability to keep it on task. What we see, in other words, is a series of trade- offs between activities concerning how well they are able to meet the different interactional imperatives of classroom order. Remiss in these analyses, how- ever, is some sense of the participants’ subjective ex- periences in the classroom. PART B. THE EFFECT OF INTERACTIONAL STABILITY AND CONCORD Now that we have looked at how the shifting inter-
  • 65. relation of talk and activity influences stability and concord in the classroom, we next look at whether Diehl and McFarland 339 at WESTERN OREGON UNIVERSITY on February 10, 2015soe.sagepub.comDownloaded from http://soe.sagepub.com/ meeting routine and ritual imperatives has a distinct effect on the duration of classroom activities and the experiences and attitudes of teachers and stu- dents. Important to note, by looking at teacher and student attitudes, we are able to study the ef- fects of ritual features of interaction in terms of not only the visible manifestations of compliance discussed above but also the subjective experiences of students and teachers associated with role embracement and collective goodwill. Task Duration. Our first outcome of interest concerns the duration of classroom tasks. Prior research has identified a connection between
  • 66. maintaining activities and learning (Stallings 1980), and we attempt to build on that connection here. We believe that the degree of interactional stability and concord will be positively associated with longer tasks and that the degree of interac- tional instability and discord will be associated with shorter tasks that fail to last. We also suspect that distinct types of tasks will be more sensitive to issues of stability than concord and vice versa. To study the effects of stability and concord on task duration, our unit of analysis shifts to the individ- ual task segments. In the McFarland Classroom Study, there are 1,501 separate task segments (token tasks). We measure the duration in minutes of class time of each segment, s. This is our dependent variable reflective of task duration (Y[Task durationsj] = time of segment s in class j). Next, we want to develop pre- dictors for interaction stability and concord so as to
  • 67. identify their effect on task duration. We measure the stability of each task as the percentage of time the token segment was stable (%Stablesj), and we measure the concord in each task as the percentage of time the segment lacked conflict (%Concordsj). Task duration is a skewed metric, so it is log transformed and predicted using ordinary least squares regression. However, we are interested in identifying the main effect of our predictors (b1) as well as their conditional effects within each type of activity (b2). As such, we include an inter- action term by activity type (b3). YðTask durationsjÞ¼ asj1b1 ð%StablesjÞ1b2ðActivity TypesjÞ 1b3ð%Stablesj*Activity TypesjÞ: ð3Þ As a more conservative test, we add fixed ef- fects for classrooms (mj) onto equation 3, thereby removing exogenous factors also omitted in prior
  • 68. models. Results: The Impact of Instability and Conflict on Activity Length. Turning now to Table 6, we examine whether the presence of instability or conflict significantly influences task duration. Overall, we see that longer activities are generally more stable and concordant whereas shorter activities are more unstable and conflic- tual. Even in the fixed-effects models, these gen- eral results hold. Notably, interactional stability is more associated with task duration than con- cord. Results for each type of activity suggest some variability, however. For example, activities with set clocks are least influenced by unstable interaction (e.g., tests and videos have a prescribed length from which it is hard to deviate). In con- trast, concord appears to have greater influence on centralized activities. This is likely because defiant acts cannot be as easily ignored in central-
  • 69. ized compared to decentralized activities. This means teachers may be less likely to perceive mo- ments of conflict as a threat to classroom order if they take place outside of shared attention. Student and Teacher Attitudes. Our second outcome of interest concerns student and teacher attitudes. We hypothesize that interactional stabil- ity will be associated with positive teacher atti- tudes and interactional concord will be associated with positive student attitudes. This is because teachers are mostly concerned with the accomplishment of routines whereas student expe- rience is more closely tied to feelings associated with the violations of self that often occur in the service of accomplishing routines. To test these ideas, we shift to individual-level outcomes and use stability and concord as class- room-level predictors. Where possible, we focus on longitudinal outcomes spanning the observation
  • 70. period of first and second semester. For student at- titudes, we use students’ semester and yearly re- ports on the quality of their classes and course difficulty levels. Sample sizes differ depending on whether we use the larger cross-sectional sam- ple (1,021 to 1,095 reports on our 152 classrooms) or the smaller longitudinal survey sample (443 per- sons in 24 focal classrooms). We will briefly relate the construction of variables below and direct the reader’s attention to Table C in the Supplemental Online Materials for details. 340 Sociology of Education 85(4) at WESTERN OREGON UNIVERSITY on February 10, 2015soe.sagepub.comDownloaded from http://soe.sagepub.com/ We construct two variables that reflect students’ perceptions of a good class. The first uses longitu- dinal information that is ‘‘scoped’’ to the classroom
  • 71. setting. The measure is a sum of how much students liked the class, teacher, teaching style, and subject and the grades they expected to get. All these items were highly correlated and formed a simple addi- tive composite with a Cronbach’s alpha of .81 (Good class v1ij). The second measure is drawn from a larger, cross-sectional sample; items con- cern how much students liked the class, the amount they learned, the quality of teaching, their interest in the subject, and the inverse-ranked liking of the class. These variables also were highly corre- lated and formed a simple additive composite with a Cronbach’s alpha of .76 (Good class v2ij). As a contrast to students’ perceptions of good clas- ses, we looked at student reports about how diffi- cult each class was (Class difficultyij). The reason for including class difficulty is that a stable class may be considered difficult or boring.
  • 72. To capture teacher attitudes, we sought measures reflecting their views of students and their content- ment with their teaching role. This sample corre- sponds with the observed 150 classrooms minus cases with missing data. The first dependent variable measures the perceived quality of teachers’ students. It is a sum of four highly correlated items concerning how often the teacher believed each student was on- task or socializing, their academic ability, and their expected grade (scales are listed in Table 6). These variables form another simple additive component that has a Cronbach’s alpha of .84 (Good studentij). The second dependent variable reflects teachers’ contentment with their role (Role-Contentmentij). It is derived from a cross-sectional teacher survey wherein 1 = the teacher’s report of not wanting to teach a different subject, or at a different school, or to be in a different profession, and 0 is coded for
  • 73. any expression of role discontent. Of the 151 teach- ers surveyed, only 99 had no desire to change their subject, school, or career, whereas 52 did. 9 The samples for these models are relatively small in comparison with the models of interaction dynamics. Hence, we are limited in the number of predictors we can include. As a proxy for task struc- tures, we use the average percentage of students who spoke in all observed class slices (%Class Talkingij). As this variable gets larger, it reflects classes that use a more open, student-centered task structure, and as it gets smaller, it reflects classes that constrain access to the floor (lecture, exams). Our other pre- dictors reflect our focal theoretical variables of inter- action stability and concord (Class Stabilityij and Class Concordij). As with task segments in the prior section, we use the percentage of observed class
  • 74. Table 6. Conditional Effects of Stability and Concord on Task Duration (by Type of Task) Percentage of segment that is stable Percentage of segment that has concord Task length Estimated coefficient Fixed Estimated coefficient Fixed Mean Standard deviation n Main effect .14*** .14*** .04y .07** 13.38 11.48 1,501 Lecture .31*** .39*** .25*** .29*** 9.32 9.24 203 Recitation .17*** .18*** .22*** .21*** 15.68 11.67 526 Discussion .29*** .32*** .26*** .26** 11.60 10.23 78 Student presentation .03 .17 .13 .10 17.32 12.93 70 Group work .32*** .26** .05 .03 12.09 8.71 116 Test/quiz .14y .14 .35*** .35*** 20.58 13.25 73 Audiovisual .20* .11 .18* .16y 16.25 13.80 62 Seatwork .20*** .19* –.03 .03 13.81 11.14 229 Undefined .31*** .27*** .25*** .26*** 5.23 6.70 144
  • 75. Source: McFarland (1999) classroom interaction study. Note: N = 1,501 task segments (models fix classroom traits). All models are standardized (Y) and exponentiated (X 3 task type). Stability: mean = .48, standard deviation = .33. Nondefiance: mean = .78, standard deviation = .28. *p .05. **p .01. ***p .001. yp .10. Diehl and McFarland 341 at WESTERN OREGON UNIVERSITY on February 10, 2015soe.sagepub.comDownloaded from http://soe.sagepub.com/ time (as opposed to segment time) that is stable or concordant as our measure. We model student and teacher attitudes using multilevel linear regression models that include random intercepts for classrooms (equation 4). At level 1, we have the attitude variable Y for individ- ual i in classroom j. The attitude Y is equal to the average outcome in the classroom plus a variety of student-level covariates (above variables) and a classroom-level error term. At the individual level, we interact our key features of stability and
  • 76. concord with task variability (here %Class Talkingij 3 Class Concordij). These interactions illustrate whether the effects of concord and stabil- ity vary by task structure as hypothesized. YðattitudeÞij ¼ b0j1b1jð%Class TalkingijÞ 1b2jðClass StableijÞ1b3jð%Class Talkingij 3Class StableijÞ 1b4jðClass ConcordijÞ1b5jð%Class Talkingij 3Class ConcordijÞ ð4Þ At level 2, we treat classrooms as random effects. b0j ¼ g001y0j Here, g00 is the average outcome of the population, and y0j is the classroom-specific effect. The multi- level framework allows us to take into account response clustering within classrooms. Results: Teacher and Student Attitudes and Preferences. The findings in Table 7 indi- cate that classroom stability and concord differen- tially shape the attitudes and experiences of
  • 77. teachers and students, lending credence to our claim that they have different interests and mo- tives that may work at cross-purposes. Students are more likely to report liking classes with higher levels of instability. Teachers, in contrast, are more likely to report being content in their jobs when they teach stable classes. This suggests stu- dents like nonroutine classes wherein control is loose and interaction variable, whereas teachers prefer classes in which they are able to manage student behavior in a routinized manner. When looking at interactional concord, we see the opposite pattern. Students report liking classes marked by concord, which our theory would sug- gest is associated with respect for identity and more opportunities for discursive authorship. This view is given extra credence by the significant interaction between classroom concord and the per-
  • 78. centage of the class talking. In other words, stu- dents like classes in which there are many opportunities for talk and those opportunities do not lead to conflict over identity. For teachers, the relationship is more complicated. Their perception of what constitutes good students is related to a lack of classroom conflict but not instability. Yet at the same time, the more teachers report having classes marked by interactional concord but little talk (or compliance), the less likely they are to report con- tentment in their role. To a teacher, then, a class of nondefiant, passive students may be perceived as being good but not interesting. The significant in- teractions between the percentage of the class talk- ing and both perception of good students and role contentment suggests that teachers prefer engaged students who do not challenge their authority. We can see general consistency in the results on
  • 79. task survival and teacher and student attitudes. First, teachers and students have different feelings about stable interaction. Teachers prefer stability, partly because it is associated with activity length, which can be seen as a proxy for fulfilling their institutional role. Students, in contrast, find routine tasks to be boring and stable classes to be difficult (student perceptions about which activities they find boring can be seen in Figure 3). Moreover, stu- dents prefer interaction that lacks conflict, which is generally found in classes in which they do not vie with teachers about the control of talk. Teachers in turn prefer engaged but nondefiant students. From a ritual perspective, then, it is not the case that teachers want to suppress student identity but rather that the importance of completing activities sometimes necessitates it. Overall, these results sug- gest that behaviors and attitudes in the classroom are
  • 80. significantly shaped by the nature of moment-to- moment interaction. Rather than placing the burden of explaining classroom experience on exogenous fac- tors of the school and setting, we can begin to see how it is dynamically shaped by the nature of the micro- situations inhabited by teachers and students. DISCUSSION In summary, we present multiple findings about social order in classrooms. Our analyses in part A 342 Sociology of Education 85(4) at WESTERN OREGON UNIVERSITY on February 10, 2015soe.sagepub.comDownloaded from http://soe.sagepub.com/ T a b le 7 . T
  • 113. 0 1 . y p .1 0 . 343 at WESTERN OREGON UNIVERSITY on February 10, 2015soe.sagepub.comDownloaded from http://soe.sagepub.com/ reveal the multidimensional and situated nature of classroom order. First, we find a strong relation between temporality and classroom order that is consistent with prior qualitative work and reinfor- ces the validity of our models. Second, we find that most of the variance in stability and concord occurs within classroom contexts and their mo-
  • 114. ments. As such, the primary issue of classroom order is often situational. Third, we find that activ- ities often dictate the overall levels of stability and concord in participant behaviors and that they often work at cross-purposes. Activities that require greater interactivity and distribute attention evenly are harder to stabilize but less conflictual. By con- trast, activities that are less interactive and focus attention unevenly are easier to stabilize and acquire compliance (but not goodwill). Fourth, we find that the stability and concord within activ- ities entail situated usage of discursive moves. The use of a joke in lectures and seatwork, for example, has different effects on the stability and concord in those particular situations. Our analyses in part B concern the effects of sta- bility and concord on instruction and the attitudes of classroom participants. Our analyses of task
  • 115. duration find that in general, stable and concordant activities last longer. The relationship is weaker, however, for activities such as tests and videos that have more rigid starting and ending times. When the degree of interactivity is high and/or attention is centralized on the teacher, activities are more sensitive to issues of stability. The influence of concord, in contrast, is weaker in activ- ities wherein students have the opportunity to engage in decentralized conversations. Next, we find that teacher attitudes respond to issues of sta- bility and student attitudes to issues of concord. Such biases reflect a central tendency across les- sons. Teachers adopt more transmittal formats of instruction that diminish interactivity and central- ize attention on themselves so that collective mobi- lization and compliance occurs. This places students in a consistently nonactive role and deval-
  • 116. ues identity. What we learn is that our analyses of concord in part A may often reflect compliance more than goodwill and that we need to take into account participants’ attitudes. Once we do, we see that teachers want more than mere compliance; they want stability with student role embracement. CONCLUSION In this article, we have attempted to reconceptual- ize the problem of classroom social order. Prior work in the sociology of education explains class- room order in terms of factors that are static across classroom moments, such as the composition of students (Dreeben and Barr 1988; McFarland 2001) and characteristics of teachers (Pianta and Hamre 2009), the mismatch between adolescent and academic cultures (Willis 1977; Woods 1983), and the organizational features of the class- room setting (McFarland 2005). In contrast, we
  • 117. have located important interactional dynamics between teachers and students within the discur- sively constructed structures of classroom situa- tions themselves. In doing so we build off of Goffman’s insight that ‘‘the proper study of interac- tion is not the individual and his psychology, but rather the syntactical relations among acts of differ- ent persons mutually present to one another . . . not, then, men and their moments. Rather, moments and their men’’ (Goffman 1967:3). In the classroom, this means seeing the motiva- tions and interests of students and teachers not as essential features of their characters or roles but rather as the result of their shifting positions in changing activity structures and discourse. In this view, problems and perturbations in classroom order are largely a function of the inter- relation of discourse and task in particular social
  • 118. situations. Each activity and form of talk cues dif- ferent participation statuses, and with those come different experiences and obligations for stability and concord. By altering the task and the discursive 0% 10% 20% 30% 40% 50% 60% 70% 80% 90% % o f A ct iv iti
  • 119. es C on si de re d "B or in g" Figure 3. Percentage of activities students con- sider boring 344 Sociology of Education 85(4) at WESTERN OREGON UNIVERSITY on February 10, 2015soe.sagepub.comDownloaded from http://soe.sagepub.com/ cues, the situation changes, and another arrange- ment of participation statuses arises along with dif- ferent potential affronts to activity and self (i.e., routine and ritual). It is only in the consistent selec-
  • 120. tion of activities such as recitation, lecture, and seatwork that classroom actors take up orientations toward one another and a more abstract sense of the institutional roles of teacher and student forms. One might infer from our results that the best course for educators interested in improving experi- ence for students is to shift focus away from stability and routine and toward building concord and ritual in the classroom. Similar prescriptions were made with student-centered instruction and youth empow- erment (Swidler 1979; Cohen and Lotan 1997). Opposite prescriptions are made by traditionalists arguing centralized activities have greater returns to achievement (Adams and Engelmann 1996). The findings of this article, however, suggest that any either-or prescription is problematic. Successful situations must be ‘‘worked,’’ and inter- actional requirements must be balanced so as best
  • 121. to accomplish collective mobilization and rapport. This suggests part of the reason reform efforts argu- ing for one direction over another tend not to suc- ceed. Rather than pitting the efficacy of traditional and progressive pedagogies against each other, then, researchers should be investigating how differ- ent patterns in the usage of types of academic activ- ities create variable classroom experiences. Instead of promoting single instructional formats, the research presented here suggests educators may want to consider the alternation of divergent forms. Using decentralized and centralized tasks in cycles may meet situational requirements of routine and rit- ual in an oscillating manner. The work presented here is both theoretically and empirically derived; however, it is not without its shortcomings. Future work can improve on what was presented here by collecting better data. In par-
  • 122. ticular, the coordination of routine is more than a network mobilization effort; it is also a topical corralling effort. As such, future work would do well to collect greater detail on the topic of talk, especially as it pertains to subject matter (Stodolsky 1988). In contrast, the normative or rit- ual requirements of situations are exemplified by more than the lack of discord but often are observed in nonverbal communication such as body posi- tioning and facial gestures seen in video recordings (Bremme and Erickson 1977; Goodwin 1998). Likewise, research that makes use of audio recordings can help us understand the role of pro- sodic, emotive features of talk such as pitch, fre- quency, and synchronicity (Jurafsky, Ranganath, and McFarland 2009). Our data do not contain information about the duration of talk and therefore lack the truly continu-
  • 123. ous notion of time and interaction that would allow for real integration between network and conversa- tion analysis (Moody et al. 2005; Gibson 2008). In addition, our distinction between dyadic and broad- cast forms of speech is too coarse. Private interac- tions reflect dyad-specific interactions, but public interactions involve bystanders (i.e., those who hear talk but are not its intended recipients). As such, it is a local broadcast and therefore distinct not only from private chains of concealed talk but from global broadcasts of indirect speech. By ren- dering interaction into these distinct network arenas, we may fundamentally alter the results. Such render- ing requires data on physical layouts, something already being integrated into this type of research (Eagle, Pentland, and Lazer 2009). In spite of the aforementioned data limitations, the data used here remain unparalleled in education
  • 124. research and offer a novel glimpse into the interac- tional construction of classroom social order. Future research would do well to look at how planned changes in classroom practices reshape the interrelation of routine and ritual features of interaction, often in unintentional ways. Studying the dynamics laid out in this article during instances of reform can offer a new way of studying the fail- ure or success of change efforts. Future work should look at the same activities across a wide range of school settings and grades. Doing so would provide new and important perspectives on some of the most central questions in the sociology of education including the processes through which students are socialized into school practices and the micro-interactional sources of inequality and dif- ferences in educational outcomes. ACKNOWLEDGMENTS
  • 125. Author names are presented in alphabetical order. The work here has benefitted tremendously from a series of anonymous reviewers, as well as participants in Stanford’s Workshop on Networks, History and Theories of Action, Duke’s Workshop on Social Structure, and the helpful advice of Charles Bidwell, David Gibson and James Moody. We also thank Rachel Lotan for providing classroom video data and David Suarez for coding assistance. Diehl and McFarland 345 at WESTERN OREGON UNIVERSITY on February 10, 2015soe.sagepub.comDownloaded from http://soe.sagepub.com/ FUNDING The authors disclosed receipt of the following financial support for the research, authorship, and/or publication of this article: The research presented here is supported by NSF award 0624134, the Spencer Foundation, and
  • 126. Stanford University’s Office of Technology and Licensing (grant# 2-CDZ-108). NOTES 1. Notably, both sociolinguists and interactionists draw extensively on Goffman in their work (e.g., Erickson 1982; Pace 2006), but each research stream tends to addresses only part of his overall framework. 2. We will use activity framework and participation framework interchangeably. For ease of understand- ing, the reader may regard frameworks as reflecting schemas and expectations and structures as enacted forms that come about (Diehl and McFarland 2010). These are not always consistent due to competing activity frameworks. 3. More classes were observed at Rural because it was a larger school where more classes had to be observed so as to get multiple observations of individuals. In addition, more math and science classes were
  • 127. observed because they were yearlong courses with more stable populations than courses in other subjects. There was an effort to observe an equal numbers of courses across tracks, but some were not offered for basic students (and no basic courses existed at Magnet), so that track is slightly underrepresented. Around 60 percent of the classes observed were in the regular track, 36 percent were in the accelerated track, and only 4 percent were from the basic track at Rural High. In spite of these biases, this is a sizeable sample of classes within which we can study the pro- cess of interaction ordering. 4. Our goal is to model interaction dynamics and identify situational levers patterning them. To this end, a slice of class time is sought that is longer than a single turn and shorter than an activity segment. If too narrow, we lose the patterning of association, and if too wide, we confound the influence of multiple segments. We tried
  • 128. windows varying from one to four minutes and found two- to three-minute windows best fit our research focus. That said, results for one to four minutes are qualitatively consistent and do not change the article’s story. 5. An elucidation of the difference between the standard study of dyads and our approach of studying triads can be found in looking at the participation structure of student presentations. When teachers call on students to present their work, the students take turns making broadcast statements to the entire class, and the teacher acts as master of ceremonies and discussant. As each student speaks, he or she commences a revolu- tion in who talks to whom (dyads) even though partic- ipants consistently enact the same interaction roles and smoothly maintain the activity. Measures that focus only on patterns of dyadic talk do not capture the stable enactment of this type of activity because
  • 129. they fail to capture whether classroom participants are enacting stable interaction patterns, or interaction roles. By contrast, conceptualizing the participation structure in terms of triadic forms allows us to identify consistent interactional roles underlying the activity despite the contingent movements of particular actors through those roles. 6. Triad labels follow the MAN acronym. M = number of mutual ties, A = number of asymmetric ties, and N = number of null ties. Hence, the label 020 refers to tri- ads with two asymmetric ties. The suffixes of triad types refer to directions of up (U), down (D), cyclic (C), and transitive (T). Transitivity is a simple law: if a �! b and b �! c, then a �! c. As such, a triad is transitive when a friend of my friend is also my friend (a general network property). 7. SAS syntax for these models is as follows: PROC PHREG DATA=datasetname NOSUMMARY; MODEL stability = x1 x2 x3 . . . xn /
  • 130. TIES=DISCRETE; STRATA classrooms; RUN; 8. We add all interaction terms to models 5 (for dis- course) and 6 (for dyadic formats) in Table 2. We relied on Jaccard’s description of modeling interaction terms using qualitative and quantitative variables (Jaccard 2001:30-34). When exploring the effect of a variable such as task reciprocity within various activity frames, it is helpful to report the conditional effect task reciprocity has within each activity. This is the effect of task reciprocity when the moderator variables value is 0. Hence, we report the effect of task reciprocity on recitation by determining task rec- iprocity’s coefficient net of interactions with all other types of activity structures. 9. The role-contentment variable is assessed by a logit model that is run at the classroom level, Log (pi / (1–pi)), where pi is the probability of role contentment for teacher i.
  • 131. REFERENCES Adams, Gary and Siegfried Engelmann. 1996. Research on Direct Instruction: 25 Years beyond DISTAR. Seattle, WA: Educational Achievement Systems. Allison, Paul D. 1999. Logistic Regression Using the SAS System: Theory and Application. Cary, NC: SAS. Alpert, Bracha. 1991. ‘‘Students’ Resistance in the Classroom.’’ Anthropology and Education Quarterly 22(4):350-66. Battistich, Victor, Daniel Solomon, Marilyn Watson, and Eric Schaps. 1997. ‘‘Caring School Communities.’’ Educational Psychologist 32(3):137-51. 346 Sociology of Education 85(4) at WESTERN OREGON UNIVERSITY on February 10, 2015soe.sagepub.comDownloaded from http://soe.sagepub.com/ Beck, Nathaniel. 2001. ‘‘Time-series-cross-section Data: What Have We Learned in the Last Few Years?’’
  • 132. Annual Review of Political Science 4:271-93. Bender-deMoll, Skye and Daniel A. McFarland. 2006. ‘‘The Art and Science of Dynamic Network Visualization.’’ Journal of Social Structure 7(2). Retrieved. Bidwell, Charles E. 1965. ‘‘The School as a Formal Organization.’’ Pp. 972-1022 in Handbook of Organizations, edited by James P. March. Chicago: Rand McNally. Bidwell, Charles E. 2005. ‘‘A Sociological Agenda for Research on Education.’’ Pp. 15-36 in The Social Organization of Schooling, edited by Larry Hedges and Barbara Schneider. New York: Russell Sage. Boocock, Sarane. 1978. ‘‘The Social Organization of the Classroom.’’ Annual Review of Sociology 4:1-28. Bossert, Steven T. 1979. Tasks and Social Relationships in Classrooms. New York: Cambridge University Press.
  • 133. Bremme, Donald W. and Frederick Erickson. 1977. ‘‘Relationships among Verbal and Nonverbal Classroom Behaviors.’’ Theory into Practice 16(3): 153-61. Brookover, Wilbur. 1945. ‘‘The Relation of Social Factors to Teaching Ability.’’ Journal of Experimental Education 13(4):191-205. Burt, Ronald S. 1990. ‘‘Detecting Role Equivalence.’’ Social Networks 12(1):83-97. Butts, Carter T. 2008. ‘‘A Relational Event Framework for Social Action.’’ Sociological Methodology 38(1): 155-200. Cameron, Claire, Carol Connor, and Frederick Morrison. 2005. ‘‘Effects of Variation in Teacher Organization on Classroom Functioning.’’ Journal of School Psychology 43(1):61-85. Cazden, Courtney B. 1988. Classroom Discourse. Portsmouth, NH: Heinemann.
  • 134. Chase, Ivan D. 1974. ‘‘Models of Hierarchy Formation in Animal Societies.’’ Behavioral Science 19(6):374-82. Cogan, Morris L. 1956. ‘‘Theory and Design of a Study of Teacher-Pupil Interaction.’’ Harvard Educational Review 26(4):315-42. Cohen, Elizabeth G. and Rachel A. Lotan. 1997. Working for Equity in Heterogeneous Classrooms: Sociological Theory in Practice. Sociology of Education Series. New York: Teachers College Press. Coleman, James. 1961. The Adolescent Society: The Social Life of the Teenager and Its Impact on Education. New York: Free Press of Glencoe. Diehl, David and Daniel McFarland. 2010. ‘‘Toward a Historical Sociology of Situations.’’ American Journal of Sociology 115(6):1713-52. Doyle, Walter. 1980. Classroom Management. West Lafayette, IN: Kappa Delta Pi. Doyle, Walter. 1986. ‘‘Classroom Organization and
  • 135. Management.’’ Pp. 392-431 in Handbook of Research on Teaching, edited by M. C. Wittrock. New York: Macmillan. Doyle, Walter and Kathy Carter. 1984. ‘‘Academic Tasks in Classrooms.’’ Curriculum Inquiry 14(2):129-49. Dreeben, Robert. 1970. The Nature of Teaching. Glenview, IL: Scott Foresman. Dreeben, Robert and Rebecca Barr. 1988. ‘‘Classroom Composition and the Design of Instruction.’’ Sociology of Education 61(3):129-42. Eagle, Nathan, Alex (Sandy) Pentland, and David Lazer. 2009. ‘‘Inferring Friendship Network Structure by Using Mobile Phone Data.’’ Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 106(36):15274. Eder, Donna and Janet Enke. 1991. ‘‘The Structure of Gossip: Opportunities and Constraints on Collective Expression amongst Adolescents.’’ American Sociological Review 56(4):494-508.
  • 136. Erickson, Frederick. 1982. ‘‘Classroom Discourse as Improvisation: Relationships between Academic Task Structure and Social Participation Structure in Lessons.’’ Pp. 153-81 in Communicating in the Classroom, edited by L. C. Wilkinson. New York: Academic. Erickson, Frederick. 2004. Talk and Social Theory: Ecologies of Speaking and Listening in Everyday Life. Malden, MA: Polity Press. Fine, Gary Alan and Philip Manning. 2000. ‘‘Erving Goffman: Shifting Impressions and Tight Frames.’’ Pp. 457-85 in Blackwell Companion to Major Social Theorists, edited by George Ritzer. Oxford, England: Blackwell. Gamoran, Adam and Sean Kelly. 2003. ‘‘On What Is Learned in School to How Schools Work: Learning and Teaching in Secondary School English Classrooms.’’ Pp. 109-26 in Stability and Change in
  • 137. American Education: Structure, Processes and Outcomes, edited by Maureen T. Hallinan, Adam Gamoran, Warren Kubitschek, and Tom Loveless. Clinton Corners, NY: Eliot Werner. Gee, John Paul. 1999. An Introduction to Discourse Analysis: Theory and Method. New York: Routledge. Gibson, David R. 2003. ‘‘Participation Shifts: Order and Differentiation in Group Conversation.’’ Social Forces 81(4):1335-80. Gibson, David R. 2008. ‘‘How the Outside Gets In: Modeling Conversational Permeation.’’ Annual Review of Sociology 34:359-84. Goffman, Erving. 1967. Interaction Ritual: Essays on Face-to-face Behavior. New York: Pantheon. Goffman, Erving. 1974. Frame Analysis: An Essay on the Organization of Experience. New York: Harper and Row. Goffman, Erving. 1981. Forms of Talk. Philadelphia:
  • 138. University of Pennsylvania Press. Goffman, Erving. 1983. ‘‘Presidential Address: The Interaction Order.’’ American Sociological Review 48(1):1-17. Diehl and McFarland 347 at WESTERN OREGON UNIVERSITY on February 10, 2015soe.sagepub.comDownloaded from http://soe.sagepub.com/ Good, Thomas L., Catherine Mulryan, and Mary McCaslin. 2006. ‘‘Grouping for Instruction in Mathematics: A Call for Programmatic Research on Small-group Processes.’’ Pp. 165-96 in Handbook of Research on Mathematics Teaching and Learning, edited by D. Grouws. New York: Macmillan. Goodwin, Marjorie H. 1998. ‘‘Games of Stance: Conflict and Footing in Hopscotch.’’ Pp. 23-46 in Kid’s Talk: Strategic Language Use in Later Childhood, edited
  • 139. by Susan Hoyle and Carolyn Temple Adger. New York: Oxford University Press. Gordon, Calvin Wayne. 1957. The Social System of the High School: A Study in the Sociology of Adolescence. Glencoe, IL: Free Press. Grenfell, Michael. 1998. ‘‘Language in the Classroom.’’ Pp. 71-88 in Bourdieu and Education, edited by Michael Grenfell and David James. Bristol, PA: Falmer Press. Hallinan, Maureen T. 1976. ‘‘Friendship Patterns in Open and Traditional Classrooms.’’ Sociology of Education 49(4):254-65. Jaccard, James. 2001. Interaction Effects in Logistic Regression. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage. Jackson, Philip W. 1968. In Life in Classrooms. Oxford, England: Routledge. Jurafsky, Dan, Rajesh Ranganath, and Daniel A. McFarland. 2009. ‘‘Extracting Social Meaning:
  • 140. Identifying Interactional Style in Spoken Conversation.’’ Proceedings of NAACL HLT 2009. Retrieved (http://dl.acm.org/citation.cfm?id=1620 847). Kalish, Yuval and Gary Robins. 2006. ‘‘Psychological Predispositions and Network Structure: The Relationship between Individual Predispositions, Structural Holes and Network Closure.’’ Social Networks 28(1):56-84. Kelman, Herbert C. 1961. ‘‘Processes of Opinion Change.’’ Public Opinion Quarterly 25(1):57-78. Kounin, Jacob S. 1970. Discipline and Group Management in Classrooms. New York: Holt, Rinehart and Winston. Lawrence, Jean, David Steed, and Pamela Young. 1984. Disruptive Children, Disruptive Schools? New York: Croom Helm. Leinhardt, Gaea and James G. Greeno. 1986. ‘‘The
  • 141. Cognitive Skill of Teaching.’’ Journal of Educational Psychology 78(2):75-95. Lortie, Dan. 1975. Schoolteacher: A Sociological Study. Chicago: University of Chicago Press. MacBeth, Douglas H. 1990. ‘‘Classroom Order as Practical Action: The Making and Un-making of a Quiet Reproach.’’ British Journal of Sociology of Education 11(2):189-214. Marzano, Robert J., Jana Marzano, and Deborah Pickering. 2003. Classroom Management That Works: Research-based Strategies for Every Teacher. Alexandria, VA: ASCD. McFarland, Daniel A. 1999. ‘‘Organized Behavior in Social Systems: A Study of Student Engagement and Resistance in High Schools.’’ PhD dissertation, Department of Sociology, University of Chicago. McFarland, Daniel A. 2001. ‘‘Student Resistance: How the Formal and Informal Organization of Classrooms
  • 142. Facilitate Everyday Forms of Student Defiance.’’ American Journal of Sociology 107(3):612-78. McFarland, Daniel A. 2004. ‘‘Resistance as a Social Drama: A Study of Change-oriented Encounters.’’ American Journal of Sociology 109(6):1249-1318. McFarland, Daniel A. 2005. ‘‘Why Work When You Can Play?’’ Pp. 147-74 in The Social Organization of Schooling, edited by Larry V. Hedges and Barbara L. Schneider. New York: Russell Sage. McNeil, Linda M. 1986. Contradictions of Control: School Structure and School Knowledge. New York: Routledge. Mehan, Hugh. 1979. Learning Lessons: Social Organization in the Classroom. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press. Metz, Mary. 1978. Classrooms and Corridors: A Study of Authority in American Secondary Schools. Berkeley: University of California Press.
  • 143. Moody, James, Daniel A. McFarland, and Skye Bender-deMoll. 2005. ‘‘Dynamic Network Visualization.’’ American Journal of Sociology 110(4):1206-41. Murray, Christopher and Mark Greenberg. 2000. ‘‘Children’s Relationship with Teachers and Bonds with School: An Investigation of Patterns and Correlates in Middle Childhood.’’ Journal of School Psychology 38(5):423-45. Nystrand, Martin. 1997. Opening Dialogue: Understanding the Dynamics of Language and Learning in the English Classroom. With Adam Gamoran, Robert Kachur, and Catherine Prendergast. New York: Teachers College Press. Pace, Judith L. 2006. ‘‘Saving (and Losing) Face, Race, and Authority in a 9th Grade English Class.’’ Pp. 87-112 in Classroom Authority: Theory, Research, and Practice, edited by Judith L. Pace and A.