SlideShare a Scribd company logo
1 of 9
How to Prevent Autism, FABA, 2006                                                           1


                   HOW TO PREVENT AUTISM BY TEACHING

                  AT-RISK INFANTS AND TODDLERS TO TALK 1

                           Philip W. Drash, Ph.D., BCBA
             Autism Early Intervention & Prevention Center, Tampa, FL
                              Roger M. Tudor, Ph. D.
                      Westfield State College, Westfield, MA

     In our recent publications (Drash & Tudor, 2004, 2004a) we presented an analysis of
autism as a contingency shaped disorder of verbal behavior. This analysis explained how
some cases of autism might begin to be shaped in at-risk children by environmental
contingencies of reinforcement occurring during the first year to two years of a child’s
life.
     Our analysis of autism further indicated that one of the key factors in preventing
many cases of autism might be to shape age-appropriate verbal and social behavior
during the first year to two years of an at-risk child’s life before autistic behaviors can be
firmly established and thus prevent the acquisition of an autistic behavior repertoire
(Drash, 2004).
      Last year we (Drash & Tudor, 2005) presented data showing that this prevention
approach resulted in the acquisition of normal or near normal language and behavioral
repertoires in five young children at risk for autism ranging in initial age from 14 months
to 2 years, 10 months. All five children are now functioning in the normal range and
none is any longer at risk for autism.
     The present paper extends these findings to children as young as 8 months and
demonstrates the effectiveness of using ABA verbal behavior procedures to prevent
autistic behavior repertoires by teaching verbal and social behavior to infants and toddlers
at high-risk for serious language delay and/or autism.
     This paper will present and discuss verbal behavior therapy techniques and
procedures for teaching nonverbal and at-risk infants and toddlers to talk. Illustrative
cases ranging in age from eight months to sixteen months will be presented and
discussed.

    The purpose of this paper is to answer two critical and longstanding questions in
the treatment of autism. These are:

    Can autism be prevented?

    If autism can be prevented, how can this be done?

Subjects
       Subject data is shown in Table 1. The subjects in this study were three young
children at risk for autism, two females and one male, ranging in age at admission from
eight months to sixteen months. All children had risk factors to be presented below that

1
 Presented at: Florida Association for Behavior Analysis,
26th Annual Convention, Daytona Beach, FL, September, 2006
How to Prevent Autism, FABA, 2006                                                         2


placed them at-risk for substantial language delay and possible autism. All children were
private outpatients in our applied behavior analysis clinic that specialized in teaching
functional verbal behavior to children with autism/ PDD or other serious language delay.
        One of the children, Subject 2, received two hours of in-office therapy per week
plus an additional 5 to 8 hours of in-home therapy weekly, one child, Subject 3, received
one hour of individual verbal behavior therapy per week, while the third was seen on an
intermittent basis. The duration of therapy varied from 3 months to 10 months.

I. The Risk Factors for Each Child Are Presented Below
        Subject 1 (F). This child was admitted to our clinical treatment program at 8
months of age. She was the younger sister of an older non-verbal child diagnosed with
autism. Schriebman, (2005, p. 91) stated that, “Siblings of children with autism have a
2 to 7 percent probability of also being autistic. This represents a 50- to 100-fold
increase in risk over that expected in the general population.”
        This child’s mother became concerned at eight months because her child was
babbling very little and her eye contact was poor. When evaluated at 9 months on the
Cattell Infant Intelligence Scale, she obtained a developmental age of 9 months, which
was average for age. However, her verbal skills were below age level. At the 8 month
level she did not imitate, and at the 9 month level, she said no words and did not adjust to
words.
        Subject 2 (M). This child was admitted for treatment at 14 months. When
admitted he had no speech and did not understand his mother’s instructions, was not
walking, and did not eat solid foods. His affect showed a very limited range of emotional
expression, and his vocal rate was initially quite low. His eye contact was inconsistent
and he constantly clung to his mother.
        He had a number of medical problems immediately following birth that placed
him at very high-risk for serious language delay, developmental delay, and possible
autism. He was not breathing at birth and had to remain in the neonatal intensive care
unit for a week. During this period he was on a ventilator and the medical staff had
difficulty keeping his oxygen level in the normal range. During the first year of life he
was delayed in sitting, standing, crawling, and walking, according to developmental
norms. At 14 months he continued to refuse to eat any textured foods.
        Subject 3 (F). This child was admitted at 16 months of age. She was the
 non-identical twin sibling of a brother who was advanced in language development and
was speaking approximately 100 words when his sister was admitted. This child’s
mother had been concerned about her child’s language development since the child was
one year because the child’s language was considerably behind that of her brother.
Although she could say about 7 to 10 words, she seldom spoke. She did not imitate
words, but could imitate some sounds. When given the opportunity, she preferred to be
picked up and cuddled while she placed her thumb in her mouth and went to sleep. Since
this behavior was mutually reinforcing to the child and to family members, it occurred
frequently. From a functional standpoint, this behavior reduced the probability that the
child would engage in vocal interactions with her siblings or family members.
How to Prevent Autism, FABA, 2006                                                            3


II. The Intervention Program

Nature of the Intervention Program
        The intervention program was primarily an expressive verbal behavior program.
It was based on the ABA verbal behavior intervention programs and procedures that we
and others have previously described (Drash, 2001; Drash, High & Tudor, 1999; Drash &
Tudor, 1990; Drash & Tudor, 1993; Sundberg & Partington, 1998; Sundberg & Michael,
2001).
        The program incorporated a modified discrete-trial procedure in which the child
was seated in a high chair directly in front of the therapist or parent, depending on who
was working with the child at the time. The mothers of two of the children attended each
therapy session and were taught how to use discrete trials to teach their child at home.
        The importance of teaching interactive social behavior to the child regularly was
stressed to the parents, but it was not specifically taught.

Goals of Intervention
       The intervention program had the following major goals.
1. To prevent the occurrence of autism or serious language delay in the children in the
       program by teaching age-appropriate expressive verbal behavior.
2. To have the children obtain normal or near normal language and social behavior for
       chronological age.
3. To eliminate and or prevent the development of negative or task avoidant behaviors
       that might prevent or interfere with the development of language and social
       behaviors.
4.To teach the mother the basic skills of teaching expressive verbal behavior to her child
       so that she could continue to teach her child at home.

III. What Are the Initial Steps in Teaching Non-Verbal or At-Risk Infants and
                Toddlers to Talk?
        The initial steps in teaching these nonverbal or at-risk infants or toddlers to talk
are best illustrated by Subject 2. This child was initially the most delayed of the three
children. He also had the greatest number of at-risk factors as discussed above. The
following is a condensed version of the steps presented in Drash (2001).
        Step 1. Establish a Manding (Requesting) Repertoire. The first step was to
establish a manding repertoire so that the child could begin to use his voice to request
items he wanted in the environment. When the program began he spoke no words and
imitated no sounds. To create a manding repertoire it was necessary to identify one or
more powerful reinforcers. The number of items that functioned as reinforcers for the
child was quite limited. Since he did not like to eat, food was not an effective reinforcer.
Fortunately, having the room light turned on and off functioned as a powerful reinforcer.
Because he was not able to turn on the light switch, his mother held the child where he
could touch the light switch, and prompted him with, “Do you want the light off? Say,
“light.” As soon as the child produced any acceptable vocalization, the light was turned
off. Then he was asked, “Do you want the light on?” As soon as the child vocalized, the
light was immediately turned on. This procedure was repeated periodically during the
session, and by the mother at home. The two treatment goals of this procedure were, first
How to Prevent Autism, FABA, 2006                                                         4


to begin to establish a manding repertoire, and second to increase the frequency and
variety of the child’s vocalizations.
        Step 2. Establishing an Echoic Repertoire. After the child was vocalizing
consistently to the light technique, a variety of different reinforcers were introduced. The
goal at this level was first to strengthen the manding repertoire, and second to use
manding to facilitate establishing an echoic repertoire. The echoic repertoire was then
used to produce new mand-tact sounds, words, or phrases. (See Drash, High, & Tudor,
1999 for more detail on this procedure.)
         The following is an example of a typical procedure using the manding strategy to
develop new echoic sounds or words. The child’s mother had a toy drum that was highly
reinforcing to him. The procedure was to provide him with the drum and let him play
with it briefly. It was then moved just out of his reach, and he was asked, “Do you want
the drum? Say, “Drr,” (for drum), or “Mmm” (for more), or some other sound or word
approximation that he had produced previously. As soon as he vocalized an
approximation to the requested sound, he was given the drum to play with, and the
process was repeated several times using other sounds or words. This procedure was
repeated with a variety of toy objects, such as a cat, a dog, a bell, a ball, and a book.
        Step 3. Establishing Mand-Tacts and shaping new words. As the child began
to vocalize more sounds and word approximations, the sounds were paired with various
objects and toys, and the child was required to say the first sound of the object or toy in
order to play with it. For example, to play with the ball he would have to request with a
“Buh.” These various sounds were then combined to create full words.
        Step 4. Shaping an Initial Intraverbal Repertoire. After the child began to
develop a large single word vocabulary of mand-tacts, he was then required to produce
short 2 to 3 word phrases or sentences to indicate preference for desired objects. For
example, some of his early phrases were “I hold it,” “I want it,” “I see fish,” “More
apples.”
        Step 5. Developing a Receptive and a Tacting Vocabulary. After the child
began to develop a taste for small bites of dried bananas, apples, and raisins, these were
used as reinforcers to teach pointing activities in books and picture displays. The child
was asked, “Do you want more bananas? Show me the dog.” This procedure was used to
develop a large receptive vocabulary. In addition to the in-session activities, his mother
often read books to him at night. She would ask him, “Where is the dog, etc.?” She
reported that in most cases he pointed to the item, and at the same time named the object.
He also spontaneously asked his mother for items he wanted at home, such as, “More
apples,” or “More bananas.” He also began randomly naming (tacting) a variety of items
at home.
        Step 6. Expanding sentence length by requiring manding statements. After
the child had begun to use a number of two-word request phrases, his sentence length was
expanded by requiring that he produce 4 and 5 word phrases to obtain desired items. For
example, “What do you want?” “I want to hold crayon,” “I want more bananas,” “I
want more apples.” After the child was taught 4 and 5 word manding phrases, he was
taught to tact action pictures with two and three word phrases. For example, “What is the
boy doing?” “Kicking the ball,” “Washing his hands,” “Ringing the bell.”
How to Prevent Autism, FABA, 2006                                                        5


IV. The Overall Strategy of the Language Development Program:
Using manding (requesting) as the foundation for the entire verbal behavior
program for developing speech in nonverbal or verbally delayed infants and
toddlers.
        As shown in the above examples, manding is used as the foundation for
developing all the other basic verbal operants including echoics, tacts, and intraverbals.
One of the major advantages of using manding to produce other verbal repertoires is that
the reinforcer for the mand is of direct and immediate benefit to the child. The child is,
therefore, much more willing to participate in the training program and to tolerate
shaping activities than he would be in the typical discrete-trial training format in which
the reinforcers may not be those that are most highly desired or most immediately
reinforcing to the child.

V. Results of Intervention
        As shown in Table 2, all children made good progress during the treatment
program, and all children obtained an initial repertoire of basic verbal operants including
mands, echoics, tacts, and intraverbals.
        Subject 1. This child is now 18 months of age and has been in the program for 10
months. When evaluated on the Cattell Infant Scale at 18 months she obtained a
developmental age of 16 months. She has now acquired at least 10 words, and has also
acquired at least one two-word manding phrase, “More cookie.” She is socially very
responsive, has excellent eye contact, and emotional affect. Although it is too early to
determine if her language development will be totally normal for age, she no longer
displays behaviors that would put her at risk for autism.
        Subject 2. This child was originally the most delayed of the three children. He
entered our program at 14 months of age. He is now 23 months of age and has been in
treatment for 8 months. When evaluated at age 22 months on the Stanford Binet he
obtained a Mental Age of 2 years, 6 months, and an IQ of 112. At 23 months he obtained
a vocabulary age of 2 years, 9 months on both the EOW and the ROW. He is now using
two and three word manding phrases to request what he wants and is spontaneously
naming action pictures with two and three word phrases, such as, “Kick the ball,” “Read
the book,” “Open the door.” His articulation is very good, and he is able to imitate most
words his therapist or his mother asks him to say. While he continues to need therapy for
generalization training, he no longer engages in behaviors that would place him at high-
risk for autism or general language delay.
        Subject 3. This child entered the program at 16 months of age and remained in
treatment for only 3 months. Despite the short period of treatment, she made excellent
progress. When evaluated with the Cattell Infant Scale at 19 months of age just before
discharge she obtained a Developmental Age of 20 months and a Developmental
Quotient of 107. She is now 2 years of age and is speaking in three and four word
phrases and sentences, is highly social and has good eye contact. She plays actively with
her brother, her older sister, and the members of her extended family. She readily asks for
items she wants. Overall her development appears to be typical for age level, and she no
longer displays behaviors that would place her at risk for language delay, developmental
delay or autism.
How to Prevent Autism, FABA, 2006                                                        6


        Essentially all three children have now developed language and behavioral
repertoires that place them in the normal range, and none any longer displays behavior
that would place them at risk for autism.

                                     Discussion

       The purpose of this paper is to provide preliminary answers to two of the most
important and long standing questions in the field of autism.

First, can autism be prevented?

Second, if autism can be prevented, how can this be done?

        We believe that the data presented on the three cases in this paper in combination
with the data on the five cases presented at ABA last year (Drash and Tudor, 2005)
clearly demonstrate that it is possible to prevent the development of autistic behavior
repertoires in some young children who are at high-risk for autism.
        Moreover, as both of these papers demonstrate, it appears that teaching functional
language and associated social skills to very young children at risk for autism is a highly
effective method for preventing the development of those behaviors that may later result
in the diagnosis of autism.
        For prevention programming to become accepted as a standard component in the
ABA treatment of autism, it is important to understand the difference between the direct
treatment model and the prevention model. In direct treatment the diagnosis of autism is
usually established before treatment begins. In the prevention model it is only necessary
to establish that a child is in a high risk category before beginning treatment.
        How is it possible to know when a young child is at risk for autism? That is, how
do we identify which children should be treated? In prevention programming there are
two generally accepted methods to determine whether a child is at risk for a physical
disease or a behavioral disorder. The first method is by statistical risk factors that have
been established by epidemiological research. If a child falls into a statistical high risk
category he or she is automatically considered to be at risk. Subject #1 in the present
study, whose older brother was diagnosed with autism, provides a typical example of
such a risk factor. Schriebman (2005) stated, “Siblings of children with autism have a
2 to 7 percent probability of also being autistic. This represents a 50- to 100-fold
increase in risk over that expected in the general population.”
        The second method of establishing risk is to determine whether a child exhibits
behaviors that have been shown by research to be highly correlated with a later diagnosis
of autism. Subjects # 2 and #3 both present examples of such behavioral risk factors.
Their primary risk factors were substantial language delay, and in the case of Subject 2,
poor eye contact, social isolation and overall developmental delay. Thus determining
when a child is at risk for autism is a relatively straightforward and objective task.
          However, after successful intervention, one of the most frequently raised
objections to the prevention approach is that the children might not have become autistic
even without intervention. This objection represents a misunderstanding of the
prevention model. In the prevention model there is no assumption that any one specific
How to Prevent Autism, FABA, 2006                                                        7


child will become autistic without intervention. Instead, it is a population based,
probability model in which a certain percentage of the at-risk group will likely be
diagnosed as autistic unless intervention occurs (NIMH, 1980).
       By beginning intervention as soon as the at-risk factors are identified, the
probability of a child’s developing an autistic behavior repertoire is greatly reduced
(Drash, 1992).

                                     Conclusion

        Although our data are clearly preliminary in nature, the data on the three cases in
this study in combination with the data on the five cases presented last year (Drash &
Tudor, 2005) indicate that prevention of autism in some young children at high-risk for
autism may be possible by using the prevention model discussed in this paper.
        In the words of our keynote speaker, Dennis Embry (2002, 2004), our results
suggest that there may be a

       Behavioral Vaccine for the Prevention of Autism and that is,

       Teaching At-Risk Infants and Toddlers to Talk.

       Based on these findings, we recommend that ABA researchers and clinicians
begin to aggressively test this prevention model with very young children at risk for
autism. If our results are confirmed and extended by further research, then prevention
programming for young children at risk for autism may become one of the most
important and essential components in the behavioral prevention and treatment of autism.
How to Prevent Autism, FABA, 2006                                                      8



                                     References

Drash, P. W. (1992). The failure of prevention, or our failure to implement prevention
       knowledge? Mental Retardation, 30, 93-96.
Drash, P. W. (2001, May). A systematic programming sequence to insure acquisition of
       initial functional verbal behavior by young nonverbal children with autism. Paper
       presented at the 27th Annual meeting, Association for Behavior Analysis, New
       Orleans, LA.
Drash, P. W. (2004, May). Preventing autism now: A possible next step for Behavior
       Analysis. Paper presented at the 30th Annual meeting, Association for Behavior
       Analysis, Boston, MA.
Drash, P. W., High, R. L., & Tudor, R. M. (1999). Using mand training to establish an
       echoic repertoire in young children with autism. The Analysis of Verbal
       Behavior, 16, 29-44.
Drash, P. W., & Tudor, R. M. (1990). Language and cognitive development: A
       systematic behavioral program and technology for increasing the language and
       cognitive skills of developmentally disabled and at-risk preschool children. In M.
       Hersen, R. M. Eisler, & P. M. Miller (Eds.), Progress in Behavior Modification,
       26, 173-220.
Drash, P. W., & Tudor, R. M. (1993). A functional analysis of verbal delay in preschool
       children: Implications for prevention and total recovery. The Analysis of Verbal
       Behavior, 11, 19-29.
Drash, P. W., & Tudor, R. M. (2004). An analysis of autism as a contingency-
       shaped disorder of verbal behavior. The Analysis of Verbal Behavior, 20, 5-23.
Drash, P. W., & Tudor, R. M. (2004a). Is autism a preventable disorder of verbal
       behavior? A response to five commentaries. The Analysis of Verbal Behavior,
       20, 55-62.
Drash, P. W., & Tudor, R. M. (2005, May). Exploratory studies in the prevention of
       autism: An analysis of five successful cases. Paper presented at the 31st Annual
       meeting of the Association for Behavior Analysis. Chicago, IL.
Embry, D. D. (2002). The good behavior game: A best practice candidate as a universal
       behavioral vaccine. Clinical Child and Family Psychology Review, 5, 273-297.
Embry, D. D. (2004). Community-based prevention using simple, low-cost, evidence-
       based kernels and behavioral vaccines. Journal of Community Psychology, 32,
       575-591.
NIMH (1980). Primary prevention: An idea whose time has come. DHHS Publication
       No. (ADM)80-447
Schriebman, L. (2005). The science and fiction of autism. Cambridge: Harvard
       University Press.
Sundberg, M. L., & Partington, J. W. (1998). Teaching language to children with
       autism or other developmental disabilities. Danville, CA: Behavior Analysts, Inc.
Sundberg, M. L., & Michael, J. (2001). The Benefits of Skinner’s Verbal Behavior for
       Children with Autism. Behavior Modification, 25, 698-724.
How to Prevent Autism, FABA, 2006                                            9



                         Table 1. Individual Subject Data

Subject Admission Age Wds at Adm.            Interim Eval.   Durat. Treat.

 1. (F)         8 Mo.             None         18 Mo.           10 Mo.

 2. (M)         14 Mo.            None          23 Mo.           8 Mo.

 3. (F)         16 Mo.            7 to 10       19 Mo.           3 Mo.

**********************************************************
Table 2. Results of Intervention: Gains in Language Development

               Subj 1 (F)     Subj 2 (M)        Subj 3 (F)

CAA            8 Mo.           14 Mo.           16 Mo.

Mos. Treat. 10 Mo.             8 Mo.             3 Mo.

CA Interim
 Eval.     18 Mo.              23 Mo.           19 Mo.

MA/DA          16 Mo.         2 Yr., 6 Mo.       20 Mo.

Adm. Wds.       None           None              7 to 10 Wds.

Wds. At Eval. 10               > 50               > 25

Manding?         Yes            Yes               Yes

Echoing?        Yes             Yes               Yes

Tacting?         Yes            Yes               Yes

Intraverbal?     Yes            Yes                Yes

EOW                           2 Yr., 9 Mo.

ROW                           2 Yr., 9 Mo.

More Related Content

What's hot

Speech and Language Disorders
Speech and Language DisordersSpeech and Language Disorders
Speech and Language Disordersanagd
 
Mary and Sharon
Mary and SharonMary and Sharon
Mary and SharonSreeve
 
Reading List 2016; "Why do some children find language so hard to learn?"
Reading List 2016; "Why do some children find language so hard to learn?"Reading List 2016; "Why do some children find language so hard to learn?"
Reading List 2016; "Why do some children find language so hard to learn?"Dorothy Bishop
 
How is specific language impairment identified
How is specific language impairment identifiedHow is specific language impairment identified
How is specific language impairment identifiedDorothy Bishop
 
Speech Therapy Internship PowerPoint
Speech Therapy Internship PowerPointSpeech Therapy Internship PowerPoint
Speech Therapy Internship PowerPointerinm94
 
Speaking to babies in long sentences boosts language development
Speaking to babies in long sentences boosts language developmentSpeaking to babies in long sentences boosts language development
Speaking to babies in long sentences boosts language development5onlinelearning
 
Speech language pathology
Speech language pathologySpeech language pathology
Speech language pathologyPsiyke
 
Natural history of language development
Natural history of language developmentNatural history of language development
Natural history of language developmentgimari Oh
 
Language impairment Presentation
Language impairment PresentationLanguage impairment Presentation
Language impairment PresentationAlyssa Heba
 
Students with Speech and Language Disorder 2
Students with Speech and Language Disorder 2Students with Speech and Language Disorder 2
Students with Speech and Language Disorder 2Jilliene Samantha Napil
 
Speech and language difficulties (2002)
Speech and language difficulties (2002)Speech and language difficulties (2002)
Speech and language difficulties (2002)Dorothy Bishop
 
Development of phonological processes in typically
Development of phonological processes in typicallyDevelopment of phonological processes in typically
Development of phonological processes in typicallyAlexander Decker
 
Language Acquisition Theories
Language Acquisition TheoriesLanguage Acquisition Theories
Language Acquisition TheoriesS Naila Ahsan
 
Chapter 7 language development
Chapter 7   language developmentChapter 7   language development
Chapter 7 language developmentRellien Basco
 
Students with Physical Disabilities, hearing Impairments and Severe Disabilit...
Students with Physical Disabilities, hearing Impairments and Severe Disabilit...Students with Physical Disabilities, hearing Impairments and Severe Disabilit...
Students with Physical Disabilities, hearing Impairments and Severe Disabilit...Jilliene Samantha Napil
 
Presentation on communication disorders
Presentation on communication disordersPresentation on communication disorders
Presentation on communication disorderstaycharles
 
Getting rid of speech difficulties and swallowing disorders
Getting rid of speech difficulties and swallowing disordersGetting rid of speech difficulties and swallowing disorders
Getting rid of speech difficulties and swallowing disordersBrandon Ridley
 

What's hot (20)

Speech and Language Disorders
Speech and Language DisordersSpeech and Language Disorders
Speech and Language Disorders
 
Down syndrome
Down syndromeDown syndrome
Down syndrome
 
Mary and Sharon
Mary and SharonMary and Sharon
Mary and Sharon
 
Speech delay and beyond
Speech delay and beyondSpeech delay and beyond
Speech delay and beyond
 
Reading List 2016; "Why do some children find language so hard to learn?"
Reading List 2016; "Why do some children find language so hard to learn?"Reading List 2016; "Why do some children find language so hard to learn?"
Reading List 2016; "Why do some children find language so hard to learn?"
 
How is specific language impairment identified
How is specific language impairment identifiedHow is specific language impairment identified
How is specific language impairment identified
 
Speech Therapy Internship PowerPoint
Speech Therapy Internship PowerPointSpeech Therapy Internship PowerPoint
Speech Therapy Internship PowerPoint
 
Speaking to babies in long sentences boosts language development
Speaking to babies in long sentences boosts language developmentSpeaking to babies in long sentences boosts language development
Speaking to babies in long sentences boosts language development
 
Speech language pathology
Speech language pathologySpeech language pathology
Speech language pathology
 
Natural history of language development
Natural history of language developmentNatural history of language development
Natural history of language development
 
Language impairment Presentation
Language impairment PresentationLanguage impairment Presentation
Language impairment Presentation
 
Nyborg causes2
Nyborg causes2Nyborg causes2
Nyborg causes2
 
Students with Speech and Language Disorder 2
Students with Speech and Language Disorder 2Students with Speech and Language Disorder 2
Students with Speech and Language Disorder 2
 
Speech and language difficulties (2002)
Speech and language difficulties (2002)Speech and language difficulties (2002)
Speech and language difficulties (2002)
 
Development of phonological processes in typically
Development of phonological processes in typicallyDevelopment of phonological processes in typically
Development of phonological processes in typically
 
Language Acquisition Theories
Language Acquisition TheoriesLanguage Acquisition Theories
Language Acquisition Theories
 
Chapter 7 language development
Chapter 7   language developmentChapter 7   language development
Chapter 7 language development
 
Students with Physical Disabilities, hearing Impairments and Severe Disabilit...
Students with Physical Disabilities, hearing Impairments and Severe Disabilit...Students with Physical Disabilities, hearing Impairments and Severe Disabilit...
Students with Physical Disabilities, hearing Impairments and Severe Disabilit...
 
Presentation on communication disorders
Presentation on communication disordersPresentation on communication disorders
Presentation on communication disorders
 
Getting rid of speech difficulties and swallowing disorders
Getting rid of speech difficulties and swallowing disordersGetting rid of speech difficulties and swallowing disorders
Getting rid of speech difficulties and swallowing disorders
 

Similar to How to prevent autism by teaching at risk infants and toddlers to talk

Mastering Scholarly WritingRemove or Replace Header Is Not Do
Mastering Scholarly WritingRemove or Replace Header Is Not DoMastering Scholarly WritingRemove or Replace Header Is Not Do
Mastering Scholarly WritingRemove or Replace Header Is Not DoAbramMartino96
 
Mastering Scholarly WritingRemove or Replace Header Is Not Do
Mastering Scholarly WritingRemove or Replace Header Is Not DoMastering Scholarly WritingRemove or Replace Header Is Not Do
Mastering Scholarly WritingRemove or Replace Header Is Not DoAbramMartino96
 
Autism: Defined Disability
Autism: Defined DisabilityAutism: Defined Disability
Autism: Defined Disabilitylblippert
 
Lisa power point presentation
Lisa power point presentationLisa power point presentation
Lisa power point presentationSineth Pathirana
 
Early Language Acquisition
Early Language AcquisitionEarly Language Acquisition
Early Language AcquisitionAsma Almashad
 
Nonverbal Communication and Autism April Priest
Nonverbal Communication and Autism April PriestNonverbal Communication and Autism April Priest
Nonverbal Communication and Autism April PriestApril Priest
 
Educational Audiology: Auditory-Verbal Therapy and Cued Speech
Educational Audiology: Auditory-Verbal Therapy and Cued SpeechEducational Audiology: Auditory-Verbal Therapy and Cued Speech
Educational Audiology: Auditory-Verbal Therapy and Cued Speechalexandracostlow
 
Autism spectrum disorder
Autism spectrum disorderAutism spectrum disorder
Autism spectrum disorderisisblue
 
Autism spectrum disorder
Autism spectrum disorderAutism spectrum disorder
Autism spectrum disorderjtevlin
 
Autism spectrum disorder
Autism spectrum disorderAutism spectrum disorder
Autism spectrum disorderisisblue
 
About autism for int'l conference
About autism for int'l conferenceAbout autism for int'l conference
About autism for int'l conferenceSelf-employed
 
Autism Spectrum
Autism SpectrumAutism Spectrum
Autism Spectrumers331
 
Communication problems and intervention for children with autism
Communication problems and intervention for children with autismCommunication problems and intervention for children with autism
Communication problems and intervention for children with autismfouzia saleemi
 
Case Conference Spring 08
Case Conference Spring 08Case Conference Spring 08
Case Conference Spring 08Xiola
 
Pervasive developmental disorder not otherwise specified
Pervasive developmental disorder not otherwise specifiedPervasive developmental disorder not otherwise specified
Pervasive developmental disorder not otherwise specifiedAlexander Decker
 

Similar to How to prevent autism by teaching at risk infants and toddlers to talk (20)

Exploratory
ExploratoryExploratory
Exploratory
 
Mastering Scholarly WritingRemove or Replace Header Is Not Do
Mastering Scholarly WritingRemove or Replace Header Is Not DoMastering Scholarly WritingRemove or Replace Header Is Not Do
Mastering Scholarly WritingRemove or Replace Header Is Not Do
 
Mastering Scholarly WritingRemove or Replace Header Is Not Do
Mastering Scholarly WritingRemove or Replace Header Is Not DoMastering Scholarly WritingRemove or Replace Header Is Not Do
Mastering Scholarly WritingRemove or Replace Header Is Not Do
 
Autism: Defined Disability
Autism: Defined DisabilityAutism: Defined Disability
Autism: Defined Disability
 
Lisa power point presentation
Lisa power point presentationLisa power point presentation
Lisa power point presentation
 
Autism
AutismAutism
Autism
 
Early Language Acquisition
Early Language AcquisitionEarly Language Acquisition
Early Language Acquisition
 
Nonverbal Communication and Autism April Priest
Nonverbal Communication and Autism April PriestNonverbal Communication and Autism April Priest
Nonverbal Communication and Autism April Priest
 
CHILDREN WITH STUTTERING AT SCHOOL
CHILDREN WITH STUTTERING AT SCHOOLCHILDREN WITH STUTTERING AT SCHOOL
CHILDREN WITH STUTTERING AT SCHOOL
 
Educational Audiology: Auditory-Verbal Therapy and Cued Speech
Educational Audiology: Auditory-Verbal Therapy and Cued SpeechEducational Audiology: Auditory-Verbal Therapy and Cued Speech
Educational Audiology: Auditory-Verbal Therapy and Cued Speech
 
Autism spectrum disorder
Autism spectrum disorderAutism spectrum disorder
Autism spectrum disorder
 
Autism spectrum disorder
Autism spectrum disorderAutism spectrum disorder
Autism spectrum disorder
 
Autism spectrum disorder
Autism spectrum disorderAutism spectrum disorder
Autism spectrum disorder
 
About autism for int'l conference
About autism for int'l conferenceAbout autism for int'l conference
About autism for int'l conference
 
Autism Spectrum
Autism SpectrumAutism Spectrum
Autism Spectrum
 
Communication problems and intervention for children with autism
Communication problems and intervention for children with autismCommunication problems and intervention for children with autism
Communication problems and intervention for children with autism
 
Adhd, autism
Adhd, autismAdhd, autism
Adhd, autism
 
Autism
AutismAutism
Autism
 
Case Conference Spring 08
Case Conference Spring 08Case Conference Spring 08
Case Conference Spring 08
 
Pervasive developmental disorder not otherwise specified
Pervasive developmental disorder not otherwise specifiedPervasive developmental disorder not otherwise specified
Pervasive developmental disorder not otherwise specified
 

More from Behavior Analysis Training System, WMU

More from Behavior Analysis Training System, WMU (20)

Final fiesta
Final fiesta Final fiesta
Final fiesta
 
Techy Tots - Final Fiesta
Techy Tots - Final FiestaTechy Tots - Final Fiesta
Techy Tots - Final Fiesta
 
Final fiesta
Final fiestaFinal fiesta
Final fiesta
 
Stella's Surrender
Stella's SurrenderStella's Surrender
Stella's Surrender
 
Stranger Things: A Behavioral Analysis
Stranger Things: A Behavioral AnalysisStranger Things: A Behavioral Analysis
Stranger Things: A Behavioral Analysis
 
Contingencies of Kingdom Hearts
Contingencies of Kingdom HeartsContingencies of Kingdom Hearts
Contingencies of Kingdom Hearts
 
Squidward's Contingencies
Squidward's ContingenciesSquidward's Contingencies
Squidward's Contingencies
 
Behavior Analysis with Lily the Beagle
Behavior Analysis with Lily the BeagleBehavior Analysis with Lily the Beagle
Behavior Analysis with Lily the Beagle
 
How To Get Your Boyfriend To Clean
How To Get Your Boyfriend To CleanHow To Get Your Boyfriend To Clean
How To Get Your Boyfriend To Clean
 
Online Professionalism and Monitoring ppt
Online Professionalism and Monitoring pptOnline Professionalism and Monitoring ppt
Online Professionalism and Monitoring ppt
 
Dead Man Test Blues
Dead Man Test BluesDead Man Test Blues
Dead Man Test Blues
 
Structured Play
Structured PlayStructured Play
Structured Play
 
Discrimination
DiscriminationDiscrimination
Discrimination
 
Icon Exchange Assignment #2: Distance and Persistence
Icon Exchange Assignment #2: Distance and PersistenceIcon Exchange Assignment #2: Distance and Persistence
Icon Exchange Assignment #2: Distance and Persistence
 
Icon Exchange Assignment #1: Enticing, Prompting, and Motivation
Icon Exchange Assignment #1: Enticing, Prompting, and MotivationIcon Exchange Assignment #1: Enticing, Prompting, and Motivation
Icon Exchange Assignment #1: Enticing, Prompting, and Motivation
 
Motivating Operations
Motivating OperationsMotivating Operations
Motivating Operations
 
Fall 2014 syllabus
Fall 2014 syllabusFall 2014 syllabus
Fall 2014 syllabus
 
Icon Exchange The Basics Phase 2-5
Icon Exchange The Basics Phase 2-5Icon Exchange The Basics Phase 2-5
Icon Exchange The Basics Phase 2-5
 
Consequences
ConsequencesConsequences
Consequences
 
ELOs
ELOsELOs
ELOs
 

How to prevent autism by teaching at risk infants and toddlers to talk

  • 1. How to Prevent Autism, FABA, 2006 1 HOW TO PREVENT AUTISM BY TEACHING AT-RISK INFANTS AND TODDLERS TO TALK 1 Philip W. Drash, Ph.D., BCBA Autism Early Intervention & Prevention Center, Tampa, FL Roger M. Tudor, Ph. D. Westfield State College, Westfield, MA In our recent publications (Drash & Tudor, 2004, 2004a) we presented an analysis of autism as a contingency shaped disorder of verbal behavior. This analysis explained how some cases of autism might begin to be shaped in at-risk children by environmental contingencies of reinforcement occurring during the first year to two years of a child’s life. Our analysis of autism further indicated that one of the key factors in preventing many cases of autism might be to shape age-appropriate verbal and social behavior during the first year to two years of an at-risk child’s life before autistic behaviors can be firmly established and thus prevent the acquisition of an autistic behavior repertoire (Drash, 2004). Last year we (Drash & Tudor, 2005) presented data showing that this prevention approach resulted in the acquisition of normal or near normal language and behavioral repertoires in five young children at risk for autism ranging in initial age from 14 months to 2 years, 10 months. All five children are now functioning in the normal range and none is any longer at risk for autism. The present paper extends these findings to children as young as 8 months and demonstrates the effectiveness of using ABA verbal behavior procedures to prevent autistic behavior repertoires by teaching verbal and social behavior to infants and toddlers at high-risk for serious language delay and/or autism. This paper will present and discuss verbal behavior therapy techniques and procedures for teaching nonverbal and at-risk infants and toddlers to talk. Illustrative cases ranging in age from eight months to sixteen months will be presented and discussed. The purpose of this paper is to answer two critical and longstanding questions in the treatment of autism. These are: Can autism be prevented? If autism can be prevented, how can this be done? Subjects Subject data is shown in Table 1. The subjects in this study were three young children at risk for autism, two females and one male, ranging in age at admission from eight months to sixteen months. All children had risk factors to be presented below that 1 Presented at: Florida Association for Behavior Analysis, 26th Annual Convention, Daytona Beach, FL, September, 2006
  • 2. How to Prevent Autism, FABA, 2006 2 placed them at-risk for substantial language delay and possible autism. All children were private outpatients in our applied behavior analysis clinic that specialized in teaching functional verbal behavior to children with autism/ PDD or other serious language delay. One of the children, Subject 2, received two hours of in-office therapy per week plus an additional 5 to 8 hours of in-home therapy weekly, one child, Subject 3, received one hour of individual verbal behavior therapy per week, while the third was seen on an intermittent basis. The duration of therapy varied from 3 months to 10 months. I. The Risk Factors for Each Child Are Presented Below Subject 1 (F). This child was admitted to our clinical treatment program at 8 months of age. She was the younger sister of an older non-verbal child diagnosed with autism. Schriebman, (2005, p. 91) stated that, “Siblings of children with autism have a 2 to 7 percent probability of also being autistic. This represents a 50- to 100-fold increase in risk over that expected in the general population.” This child’s mother became concerned at eight months because her child was babbling very little and her eye contact was poor. When evaluated at 9 months on the Cattell Infant Intelligence Scale, she obtained a developmental age of 9 months, which was average for age. However, her verbal skills were below age level. At the 8 month level she did not imitate, and at the 9 month level, she said no words and did not adjust to words. Subject 2 (M). This child was admitted for treatment at 14 months. When admitted he had no speech and did not understand his mother’s instructions, was not walking, and did not eat solid foods. His affect showed a very limited range of emotional expression, and his vocal rate was initially quite low. His eye contact was inconsistent and he constantly clung to his mother. He had a number of medical problems immediately following birth that placed him at very high-risk for serious language delay, developmental delay, and possible autism. He was not breathing at birth and had to remain in the neonatal intensive care unit for a week. During this period he was on a ventilator and the medical staff had difficulty keeping his oxygen level in the normal range. During the first year of life he was delayed in sitting, standing, crawling, and walking, according to developmental norms. At 14 months he continued to refuse to eat any textured foods. Subject 3 (F). This child was admitted at 16 months of age. She was the non-identical twin sibling of a brother who was advanced in language development and was speaking approximately 100 words when his sister was admitted. This child’s mother had been concerned about her child’s language development since the child was one year because the child’s language was considerably behind that of her brother. Although she could say about 7 to 10 words, she seldom spoke. She did not imitate words, but could imitate some sounds. When given the opportunity, she preferred to be picked up and cuddled while she placed her thumb in her mouth and went to sleep. Since this behavior was mutually reinforcing to the child and to family members, it occurred frequently. From a functional standpoint, this behavior reduced the probability that the child would engage in vocal interactions with her siblings or family members.
  • 3. How to Prevent Autism, FABA, 2006 3 II. The Intervention Program Nature of the Intervention Program The intervention program was primarily an expressive verbal behavior program. It was based on the ABA verbal behavior intervention programs and procedures that we and others have previously described (Drash, 2001; Drash, High & Tudor, 1999; Drash & Tudor, 1990; Drash & Tudor, 1993; Sundberg & Partington, 1998; Sundberg & Michael, 2001). The program incorporated a modified discrete-trial procedure in which the child was seated in a high chair directly in front of the therapist or parent, depending on who was working with the child at the time. The mothers of two of the children attended each therapy session and were taught how to use discrete trials to teach their child at home. The importance of teaching interactive social behavior to the child regularly was stressed to the parents, but it was not specifically taught. Goals of Intervention The intervention program had the following major goals. 1. To prevent the occurrence of autism or serious language delay in the children in the program by teaching age-appropriate expressive verbal behavior. 2. To have the children obtain normal or near normal language and social behavior for chronological age. 3. To eliminate and or prevent the development of negative or task avoidant behaviors that might prevent or interfere with the development of language and social behaviors. 4.To teach the mother the basic skills of teaching expressive verbal behavior to her child so that she could continue to teach her child at home. III. What Are the Initial Steps in Teaching Non-Verbal or At-Risk Infants and Toddlers to Talk? The initial steps in teaching these nonverbal or at-risk infants or toddlers to talk are best illustrated by Subject 2. This child was initially the most delayed of the three children. He also had the greatest number of at-risk factors as discussed above. The following is a condensed version of the steps presented in Drash (2001). Step 1. Establish a Manding (Requesting) Repertoire. The first step was to establish a manding repertoire so that the child could begin to use his voice to request items he wanted in the environment. When the program began he spoke no words and imitated no sounds. To create a manding repertoire it was necessary to identify one or more powerful reinforcers. The number of items that functioned as reinforcers for the child was quite limited. Since he did not like to eat, food was not an effective reinforcer. Fortunately, having the room light turned on and off functioned as a powerful reinforcer. Because he was not able to turn on the light switch, his mother held the child where he could touch the light switch, and prompted him with, “Do you want the light off? Say, “light.” As soon as the child produced any acceptable vocalization, the light was turned off. Then he was asked, “Do you want the light on?” As soon as the child vocalized, the light was immediately turned on. This procedure was repeated periodically during the session, and by the mother at home. The two treatment goals of this procedure were, first
  • 4. How to Prevent Autism, FABA, 2006 4 to begin to establish a manding repertoire, and second to increase the frequency and variety of the child’s vocalizations. Step 2. Establishing an Echoic Repertoire. After the child was vocalizing consistently to the light technique, a variety of different reinforcers were introduced. The goal at this level was first to strengthen the manding repertoire, and second to use manding to facilitate establishing an echoic repertoire. The echoic repertoire was then used to produce new mand-tact sounds, words, or phrases. (See Drash, High, & Tudor, 1999 for more detail on this procedure.) The following is an example of a typical procedure using the manding strategy to develop new echoic sounds or words. The child’s mother had a toy drum that was highly reinforcing to him. The procedure was to provide him with the drum and let him play with it briefly. It was then moved just out of his reach, and he was asked, “Do you want the drum? Say, “Drr,” (for drum), or “Mmm” (for more), or some other sound or word approximation that he had produced previously. As soon as he vocalized an approximation to the requested sound, he was given the drum to play with, and the process was repeated several times using other sounds or words. This procedure was repeated with a variety of toy objects, such as a cat, a dog, a bell, a ball, and a book. Step 3. Establishing Mand-Tacts and shaping new words. As the child began to vocalize more sounds and word approximations, the sounds were paired with various objects and toys, and the child was required to say the first sound of the object or toy in order to play with it. For example, to play with the ball he would have to request with a “Buh.” These various sounds were then combined to create full words. Step 4. Shaping an Initial Intraverbal Repertoire. After the child began to develop a large single word vocabulary of mand-tacts, he was then required to produce short 2 to 3 word phrases or sentences to indicate preference for desired objects. For example, some of his early phrases were “I hold it,” “I want it,” “I see fish,” “More apples.” Step 5. Developing a Receptive and a Tacting Vocabulary. After the child began to develop a taste for small bites of dried bananas, apples, and raisins, these were used as reinforcers to teach pointing activities in books and picture displays. The child was asked, “Do you want more bananas? Show me the dog.” This procedure was used to develop a large receptive vocabulary. In addition to the in-session activities, his mother often read books to him at night. She would ask him, “Where is the dog, etc.?” She reported that in most cases he pointed to the item, and at the same time named the object. He also spontaneously asked his mother for items he wanted at home, such as, “More apples,” or “More bananas.” He also began randomly naming (tacting) a variety of items at home. Step 6. Expanding sentence length by requiring manding statements. After the child had begun to use a number of two-word request phrases, his sentence length was expanded by requiring that he produce 4 and 5 word phrases to obtain desired items. For example, “What do you want?” “I want to hold crayon,” “I want more bananas,” “I want more apples.” After the child was taught 4 and 5 word manding phrases, he was taught to tact action pictures with two and three word phrases. For example, “What is the boy doing?” “Kicking the ball,” “Washing his hands,” “Ringing the bell.”
  • 5. How to Prevent Autism, FABA, 2006 5 IV. The Overall Strategy of the Language Development Program: Using manding (requesting) as the foundation for the entire verbal behavior program for developing speech in nonverbal or verbally delayed infants and toddlers. As shown in the above examples, manding is used as the foundation for developing all the other basic verbal operants including echoics, tacts, and intraverbals. One of the major advantages of using manding to produce other verbal repertoires is that the reinforcer for the mand is of direct and immediate benefit to the child. The child is, therefore, much more willing to participate in the training program and to tolerate shaping activities than he would be in the typical discrete-trial training format in which the reinforcers may not be those that are most highly desired or most immediately reinforcing to the child. V. Results of Intervention As shown in Table 2, all children made good progress during the treatment program, and all children obtained an initial repertoire of basic verbal operants including mands, echoics, tacts, and intraverbals. Subject 1. This child is now 18 months of age and has been in the program for 10 months. When evaluated on the Cattell Infant Scale at 18 months she obtained a developmental age of 16 months. She has now acquired at least 10 words, and has also acquired at least one two-word manding phrase, “More cookie.” She is socially very responsive, has excellent eye contact, and emotional affect. Although it is too early to determine if her language development will be totally normal for age, she no longer displays behaviors that would put her at risk for autism. Subject 2. This child was originally the most delayed of the three children. He entered our program at 14 months of age. He is now 23 months of age and has been in treatment for 8 months. When evaluated at age 22 months on the Stanford Binet he obtained a Mental Age of 2 years, 6 months, and an IQ of 112. At 23 months he obtained a vocabulary age of 2 years, 9 months on both the EOW and the ROW. He is now using two and three word manding phrases to request what he wants and is spontaneously naming action pictures with two and three word phrases, such as, “Kick the ball,” “Read the book,” “Open the door.” His articulation is very good, and he is able to imitate most words his therapist or his mother asks him to say. While he continues to need therapy for generalization training, he no longer engages in behaviors that would place him at high- risk for autism or general language delay. Subject 3. This child entered the program at 16 months of age and remained in treatment for only 3 months. Despite the short period of treatment, she made excellent progress. When evaluated with the Cattell Infant Scale at 19 months of age just before discharge she obtained a Developmental Age of 20 months and a Developmental Quotient of 107. She is now 2 years of age and is speaking in three and four word phrases and sentences, is highly social and has good eye contact. She plays actively with her brother, her older sister, and the members of her extended family. She readily asks for items she wants. Overall her development appears to be typical for age level, and she no longer displays behaviors that would place her at risk for language delay, developmental delay or autism.
  • 6. How to Prevent Autism, FABA, 2006 6 Essentially all three children have now developed language and behavioral repertoires that place them in the normal range, and none any longer displays behavior that would place them at risk for autism. Discussion The purpose of this paper is to provide preliminary answers to two of the most important and long standing questions in the field of autism. First, can autism be prevented? Second, if autism can be prevented, how can this be done? We believe that the data presented on the three cases in this paper in combination with the data on the five cases presented at ABA last year (Drash and Tudor, 2005) clearly demonstrate that it is possible to prevent the development of autistic behavior repertoires in some young children who are at high-risk for autism. Moreover, as both of these papers demonstrate, it appears that teaching functional language and associated social skills to very young children at risk for autism is a highly effective method for preventing the development of those behaviors that may later result in the diagnosis of autism. For prevention programming to become accepted as a standard component in the ABA treatment of autism, it is important to understand the difference between the direct treatment model and the prevention model. In direct treatment the diagnosis of autism is usually established before treatment begins. In the prevention model it is only necessary to establish that a child is in a high risk category before beginning treatment. How is it possible to know when a young child is at risk for autism? That is, how do we identify which children should be treated? In prevention programming there are two generally accepted methods to determine whether a child is at risk for a physical disease or a behavioral disorder. The first method is by statistical risk factors that have been established by epidemiological research. If a child falls into a statistical high risk category he or she is automatically considered to be at risk. Subject #1 in the present study, whose older brother was diagnosed with autism, provides a typical example of such a risk factor. Schriebman (2005) stated, “Siblings of children with autism have a 2 to 7 percent probability of also being autistic. This represents a 50- to 100-fold increase in risk over that expected in the general population.” The second method of establishing risk is to determine whether a child exhibits behaviors that have been shown by research to be highly correlated with a later diagnosis of autism. Subjects # 2 and #3 both present examples of such behavioral risk factors. Their primary risk factors were substantial language delay, and in the case of Subject 2, poor eye contact, social isolation and overall developmental delay. Thus determining when a child is at risk for autism is a relatively straightforward and objective task. However, after successful intervention, one of the most frequently raised objections to the prevention approach is that the children might not have become autistic even without intervention. This objection represents a misunderstanding of the prevention model. In the prevention model there is no assumption that any one specific
  • 7. How to Prevent Autism, FABA, 2006 7 child will become autistic without intervention. Instead, it is a population based, probability model in which a certain percentage of the at-risk group will likely be diagnosed as autistic unless intervention occurs (NIMH, 1980). By beginning intervention as soon as the at-risk factors are identified, the probability of a child’s developing an autistic behavior repertoire is greatly reduced (Drash, 1992). Conclusion Although our data are clearly preliminary in nature, the data on the three cases in this study in combination with the data on the five cases presented last year (Drash & Tudor, 2005) indicate that prevention of autism in some young children at high-risk for autism may be possible by using the prevention model discussed in this paper. In the words of our keynote speaker, Dennis Embry (2002, 2004), our results suggest that there may be a Behavioral Vaccine for the Prevention of Autism and that is, Teaching At-Risk Infants and Toddlers to Talk. Based on these findings, we recommend that ABA researchers and clinicians begin to aggressively test this prevention model with very young children at risk for autism. If our results are confirmed and extended by further research, then prevention programming for young children at risk for autism may become one of the most important and essential components in the behavioral prevention and treatment of autism.
  • 8. How to Prevent Autism, FABA, 2006 8 References Drash, P. W. (1992). The failure of prevention, or our failure to implement prevention knowledge? Mental Retardation, 30, 93-96. Drash, P. W. (2001, May). A systematic programming sequence to insure acquisition of initial functional verbal behavior by young nonverbal children with autism. Paper presented at the 27th Annual meeting, Association for Behavior Analysis, New Orleans, LA. Drash, P. W. (2004, May). Preventing autism now: A possible next step for Behavior Analysis. Paper presented at the 30th Annual meeting, Association for Behavior Analysis, Boston, MA. Drash, P. W., High, R. L., & Tudor, R. M. (1999). Using mand training to establish an echoic repertoire in young children with autism. The Analysis of Verbal Behavior, 16, 29-44. Drash, P. W., & Tudor, R. M. (1990). Language and cognitive development: A systematic behavioral program and technology for increasing the language and cognitive skills of developmentally disabled and at-risk preschool children. In M. Hersen, R. M. Eisler, & P. M. Miller (Eds.), Progress in Behavior Modification, 26, 173-220. Drash, P. W., & Tudor, R. M. (1993). A functional analysis of verbal delay in preschool children: Implications for prevention and total recovery. The Analysis of Verbal Behavior, 11, 19-29. Drash, P. W., & Tudor, R. M. (2004). An analysis of autism as a contingency- shaped disorder of verbal behavior. The Analysis of Verbal Behavior, 20, 5-23. Drash, P. W., & Tudor, R. M. (2004a). Is autism a preventable disorder of verbal behavior? A response to five commentaries. The Analysis of Verbal Behavior, 20, 55-62. Drash, P. W., & Tudor, R. M. (2005, May). Exploratory studies in the prevention of autism: An analysis of five successful cases. Paper presented at the 31st Annual meeting of the Association for Behavior Analysis. Chicago, IL. Embry, D. D. (2002). The good behavior game: A best practice candidate as a universal behavioral vaccine. Clinical Child and Family Psychology Review, 5, 273-297. Embry, D. D. (2004). Community-based prevention using simple, low-cost, evidence- based kernels and behavioral vaccines. Journal of Community Psychology, 32, 575-591. NIMH (1980). Primary prevention: An idea whose time has come. DHHS Publication No. (ADM)80-447 Schriebman, L. (2005). The science and fiction of autism. Cambridge: Harvard University Press. Sundberg, M. L., & Partington, J. W. (1998). Teaching language to children with autism or other developmental disabilities. Danville, CA: Behavior Analysts, Inc. Sundberg, M. L., & Michael, J. (2001). The Benefits of Skinner’s Verbal Behavior for Children with Autism. Behavior Modification, 25, 698-724.
  • 9. How to Prevent Autism, FABA, 2006 9 Table 1. Individual Subject Data Subject Admission Age Wds at Adm. Interim Eval. Durat. Treat. 1. (F) 8 Mo. None 18 Mo. 10 Mo. 2. (M) 14 Mo. None 23 Mo. 8 Mo. 3. (F) 16 Mo. 7 to 10 19 Mo. 3 Mo. ********************************************************** Table 2. Results of Intervention: Gains in Language Development Subj 1 (F) Subj 2 (M) Subj 3 (F) CAA 8 Mo. 14 Mo. 16 Mo. Mos. Treat. 10 Mo. 8 Mo. 3 Mo. CA Interim Eval. 18 Mo. 23 Mo. 19 Mo. MA/DA 16 Mo. 2 Yr., 6 Mo. 20 Mo. Adm. Wds. None None 7 to 10 Wds. Wds. At Eval. 10 > 50 > 25 Manding? Yes Yes Yes Echoing? Yes Yes Yes Tacting? Yes Yes Yes Intraverbal? Yes Yes Yes EOW 2 Yr., 9 Mo. ROW 2 Yr., 9 Mo.