SlideShare a Scribd company logo
1 of 21
Download to read offline
Running head: SOCIAL SUPPORT AND ADOLESCENT STRESS !1
!
!
!
!
!
!
!
The Role of Family and Peer Social Support in Reported Adolescent Stress
Tabitha Smith
Western Washington University
!
!
!
!
!
!
!
!
!
!
!
SOCIAL SUPPORT AND ADOLESCENT STRESS !2
The Role of Family and Peer Social Support in Reported Adolescent Stress
Stress. We all have experience with it. This is not necessarily a bad thing. Stress can help
us to focus and complete projects on time, help us to make the game winning basket, or help us
to survive in a life threatening situation. Experiencing stress can also be a bad thing, like being
excluded by a peer group, panicking over a test, or fighting with those closest to us. Stress is
defined as an individual appraising an event as a threat to the psychological or physiological self
(McEwen, 2000). When we experience a stressor, our body copes by producing specific
biological markers such as cortisol. These biological markers are protective in the short term but
damaging to our biological processes in the long term (McEwen, 2000). If a stressor happens
repeatedly, such as daily exclusion from a peer group, it is called chronic stress. One does not
have to experience the same stressor on a regular basis to have chronic stress, just repeated
activation of the stress response.
Chronic stress can have serious health implications, such as greater risk of cardiovascular
disease and lower immune functioning (Black & Garbutt, 2002). The effects of stress can have
an additive effect on a person, a principal called allostatic load. Allostatic load is the
physiological burden imposed by repeated stress activation (Geronimus, Hicken, Keene, &
Bound, 2006). This physiological burden has been shown to accelerate the disease process and
increase disease risk (e.g. cardiovascular disease). Early childhood experiences with multiple risk
factors, such as abuse, neglect, low socioeconomic status, contributes to allostatic load
(McEwen, 2000). However, the level of social support a person receives moderates the effects of
stress activation on health outcomes. A high level of social support equates with better health
outcomes, while a low level of social support is linked to higher mortality and lower immune
SOCIAL SUPPORT AND ADOLESCENT STRESS !3
functioning (Uchino, 2006). We will first discuss how risk factors in adolescence lead to greater
stress, then briefly discuss stress activation pathways, followed by discussing the role social
support from families and friends can play in moderating the effect of stress for adolescents.
“Development is a person-context interaction (Jessor, 1993, p. 119),” suggesting that how an
adolescent develops depends not only on their genetics, but also on the interactions that person
has with her environment (e.g. family, school, peers). Social environment and family risk factors
are related with adolescent risk behaviors and health compromising outcomes. Family, school,
and neighborhood are the three most important life contexts that affect an adolescent. These
contexts have the most influence on an adolescent’s development. These three settings also affect
each other and are influenced by a larger social-structural and cultural environment (e.g.
economic, political). These contexts influence the adolescent over time, with time also
interacting with the above contexts. These contexts influence adolescent development both
directly (i.e. family, school, neighborhood) as well as indirectly (e.g. economic, political)
(Bronfenbrenner, 1986 ; Jessor, 1993).
An adolescent may experience risk associated within one or more of these contexts, for
example being of low socioeconomic status (SES). Low-SES influences all three of the
important life contexts (family, school, neighborhood) the adolescent is in, this influence has the
possibility of increasing the risk factors an adolescent experiences (Miller & Chen, 2013).
Looking at the family context, low-SES can include exposure to unstable family dynamics,
caregivers who are unresponsive to an adolescents needs, lack of parental monitoring, household
crowding, or inadequate nutrition (Fergus & Zimmerman, 2005; Evans, 2004; Repetti, Taylor, &
Seeman, 2002). Looking at the neighborhood context, low-SES can include exposure to
SOCIAL SUPPORT AND ADOLESCENT STRESS !4
infectious microorganisms, industrial pollutants, or limited community resources (Fergus &
Zimmerman, 2005; Evans, 2004). These risk factors lead to a number of negative outcomes, such
as poor academic achievement, violent behavior, and a number of adult health problems (e.g.
cardiovascular disease) (Arnold & Doctorff, 2003). Low parental SES is known to increase child
cortisol levels and chronic stress. Chronic stress experienced by children damage the biological
and psychological regulatory processes (Evans & Kim, 2013). Low-SES children are exposed to
many more multiple stressors than their more economically advantaged peers. There are few
low-SES children who are exposed to only one or fewer stressors (Evans & English, 2002).
For stress experiences to increase health and behavior problems in adolescence, exposure
to multiple risk factors is what matters most. The cumulative risk research shows that co-
occurring risk factors have a deleterious effect on health and behavior outcomes (Appleyard,
Egeland, van Dulmen, & Sroufe, 2005; Sameroff, Seifer, Zax, & Barocas, 1987). The cumulative
risk hypothesis asserts that with increased risk factors come increased clinical problems
(Sameroff, 2000). The number of risk factors experienced is associated with adolescent mental
health, problem behaviors, and academic problems (Yates, Dodds, Sroufe, & Egeland, 2003).
Additionally, it is now clear that life course experiences contribute to a number of common
medical problems (e.g. heart disease, stroke; Miller & Chen, 2013). Several other risk factors
associated with adolescent mental health, problem behavior, and academic problems are:
unskilled parental occupation status, low maternal education status, disadvantaged minority
status, single parenthood, and stressful life events (Sameroff, 2000). However, socially
supportive relationships have positive effects on the health outcomes and the mortality rates
(Fergus & Zimmerman, 2005; Heinrichs, Baumgartner, Kirschbaum, & Ehlert, 2003).
SOCIAL SUPPORT AND ADOLESCENT STRESS !5
I will briefly discuss what systems are activated from a stress response, for a detailed review of
this process see other reviews (Gunnar & Quevedo, 2007; Ulrich-Lai & Herman, 2009). When
we appraise a situation as threatening, a stress response is activated within our body. The two
interrelated but distinct systems activated by appraising a situation as threatening are the
sympathetic nervous symptom (SNS) and the hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenalcortical (HPA) axis
(Gunnar & Quevedo, 2007). The SNS is responsible for releasing catecholamines such as
epinephrine (adrenaline), from the adrenal gland into the body. Epinephrine is a hormone
responsible for the fight or flight response. It is responsible for increasing the heart rate and
blood pressure, thus ensuring the body has enough oxygenated blood to send to the muscles so
we can run away or stay and fight. The HPA axis is responsible for producing glucocorticoids,
such as cortisol. Cortisol helps to mobilize the energy resources needed within the body during
stressful situations (McEwen, 1998). Glucocorticoids take about 25 minutes to be proceed to
peak levels within the body. Thus, the effects are slower to develop and last longer than the
hormones released by the SNS (Gunnar & Quevedo, 2007).
The actions of the SNS results in the activation of the HPA axis. The eventual aim of the
HPA axis is the activation of the peripheral nervous system (PNS), which is responsible for
turning off the stress response cascade initiated by the SNS and HPA systems. Usually, the
activation of these stress systems help to keep the body in allostasis (McEwen & Seeman, 1999).
However, frequent activation dysregulates these systems which inhibits the ability to turn off the
stress cascade. As such, the physiological systems impacted stay elevated, with cortisol not being
able to decrease or having habitually raised blood pressure and heart rate (Gunnar & Quevedo,
2007). When cortisol levels are elevated the immune system is suppressed, leading to greater
SOCIAL SUPPORT AND ADOLESCENT STRESS !6
vulnerability to illness (Ader, 2001). This system dysregulation increases the risk of both
physical and mental health problems (McEwen & Seeman, 1999). However, social support
reduces the effect physiological stress reactivity has on the body (Uchino, Cacioppo, & Kiecolt-
Glaser, 1996).
Social support may moderate stress reactivity at two points during the appraisal of a
stressful event. Social support may alter the appraisal itself, preventing a threatening event from
being perceived as stressful, which prevents the stress response activation. Alternatively, social
support may alter the period after appraisal, but before a prolonged physiological stress response
is activated. Perceived social support may help people to have better regulation or coping skills
for a stressful experience which reduces the physiological stress response (Cohen & Wills,
1985).
Social support attenuates the relationship between risk factors and the physiological
stress response activated by appraising an event as threatening (Eisenberger, Taylor, Gable,
Hillmert, & Lieberman, 2007; Heinrichs, Baumgartner, Kirschbaum, Ehlert, 2003; Uchino,
2006). In a laboratory study, participants completed the Trier Social Stress Test (TSST) to induce
the stress response. The TSST consists of verbal mental arithmetic and public speaking
performed in front of a crowd. To test if social support does lower the stress response, half of the
participants were told to bring a friend. This friend was with them during their preparation for the
TSST tasks. Participants with a friend present experienced lower levels of cortisol activation
during the TSST tasks (Heinrichs, Baumgartner, Kirschbaum, Ehlert, 2003).
However, less social support increases an individual’s negative health risks, such as
earlier mortality rates, increased cardiovascular disease (Brummet et al., 2001), and depression
SOCIAL SUPPORT AND ADOLESCENT STRESS !7
(Sipal & Sayin, 2013). Social support is defined as social relationship processes that have the
ability to promote health and well-being (Cohen, Gottlieb, & Underwood, 2000) and refers to the
psychological and material resources received from belonging to a social group that can help an
individual to cope with stress (Cohen, 2000). Social support can come from a variety of sources,
including a partner, friends, or family. Social support may include distinct transactions where a
person receives benefits from someone else, or a person may feel they have access to help or
support from someone (Taylor, 2007). Social support is thought to buffer the effects of stress by
reducing the effects from a stressful experience through promoting the appropriate coping
mechanisms and reappraising the event as less threatening (Cohen, 2004).
During adolescence social support groups change, giving adolescents the ability to
receive support from multiple groups. More time is spent with friends in addition to a
considerable drop in the amount of time spent with parents. However, family influence over an
adolescent is still high, but the amount of influence changes depending on the cohesiveness of
the family and the closeness of friends (Steinberg & Morris, 2001). In a less cohesive family, the
influence a family has on an adolescent is low. For an adolescent with no close friends, family
influence is high (Gauze, Bukowski, Aquan-Assee, & Sippola, 1996). These different influences
can effect perceived support. We will be looking at the effect both family and friend social
support has on perceived stress experiences for adolescents.
There seem to be gender differences in the effect social support plays in moderating the
relationship between risk factors and stress (Rueger, Malecki, & Demaray, 2010). While both
boys and girls report similar levels of family social support (Rueger, Malecki, & Demaray,
2010), girls report higher levels of peer social support than boys (Cheng & Chan, 2004; Furman
SOCIAL SUPPORT AND ADOLESCENT STRESS !8
& Buhrmester, 1992). Girls also report receiving more social support from peers than from their
parents, while boys report more social support from their parents (Frey & Röthlisberger, 1996).
High levels of family social support have been consistently associated with lower depression,
higher self-esteem, and better academic adjustment. In contrast, low levels of family social
support is associated with psychological distress and emotional problems (Rueger, Malecki, &
Demaray, 2010). There is evidence for both low levels of family social support not being
compensated by high levels of peer support (van Beest & Baerveldt, 1999), and evidence that
high peer social support can compensate for of family social support (Barrera & Garrison-Jones,
1992).
Family social support as perceived by an adolescent is expected to moderate the link
between risk factors and experienced stress (Barrera & Garrison-Jones, 1992). In a study using
an inpatient population of depressed adolescents, adolescents who perceived more social support
from families had fewer depressive symptoms than adolescents who perceived low levels of
social support from their families (Barrera & Garrison-Jones, 1992). Low friend social support
can increase an adolescent’s stress response when being excluded by their peers, while
perceiving high friend social support can decrease the stress response when excluded by peers
(Rigby, 2000; Peters, Riksen-Walraven, Cillessen, & de Weerth, 2011). In an inpatient population
of depressed adolescents, those who perceived high friend social support experienced fewer
depressive symptoms, but only when perceived family social support was low (Barrera &
Garrison-Jones, 1992).
We designed the following hypothesis to determine if the number of risk factors moderate
type of support (peer vs family) effect on stress. Overall, adolescents with more risk factors will
SOCIAL SUPPORT AND ADOLESCENT STRESS !9
report higher levels of stress. Adolescents with more social support will report lower levels of
stress. This relationship is expected to be present for both family and peer social support. The
effect of family vs friend support on stress is expected to vary depending on the amount of risk
the adolescent experiences. For adolescents with more risk factors, family social support will
result in lower levels of reported stress. Low levels of family social support will result in higher
levels of reported stress. Adolescents with more risk factors and high levels of peer social
support will report lower levels of stress, while low peer social support will result in higher
reported levels of stress.
Method
Participants
This sample consisted of 276 parents and adolescents. The number of participants varied
slightly, ranging from 244 to 276, depending on the analysis that was examined. We used all data
available for each analysis. The primary responder (parent) in each sample was predominately
white (85.5%), 7.2% black, 2.9% Hispanic, and the remaining identified as multi-racial or Asian
Pacific Islander, with 1.4% not specifying. Most target parents were in monogamous
relationships (80.5%), 15.8% were divorced or separated, 3.7% were never married. Adolescents
were 53.9% female and 45.4% male, with .7% not indicating sex. The average age of the
adolescents was 15.4 years (1.59). Most participants were middle-upper class in socioeconomic
status, with 20.4% indicating that they earned more than $100,000 per year. This sample was
taken from a larger sample of five hundred families, who were recruited from seven different
sites throughout the U.S., located in the Midwest, Southeast, Northeast, and West Coast.
Measures
SOCIAL SUPPORT AND ADOLESCENT STRESS !10
Stress. Adolescent’s reports of stress were obtained from the experience sampling portion of this
study. For each experience sampling reading, adolescents responded to the question: As you were
beeped were you feeling… “nervous”, “strained”, or “stressed”. Adolescents chose a response on
a scale of zero (not at all) to three (very much). Responses to these three items were positively
associated, Cronbach’s alpha = .793. A mean was taken for each of the three measures as they
were reported for each experience sampling moment. Next, all of the momentary stress scores for
a particular individual were combined to form an average level of stress for each person. This
score summarizes the adolescents’ average level of stress as it was reported over the course of
the experience sampling portion of the study. This stress measure ranged from zero to 2.06, M = .
50, SD = .39. The distribution was positively skewed suggesting that adolescents reported very
little stress overall.
Social Support. Peer support was measured through teen responses to 5-items. Teens responded
to questions such as “my friends care about how I am” and “I can count on my friends” using
response options from 1 (never true) to 5 (always true). The peer social support measure was
reliable with a cronbachs alpha = .82 (m = 4.00, sd = 0.74). Responses ranged from 1 through 5,
and the overall distribution was negatively skewed. Six items were used to capture social support
from mothers while six items captured social support from fathers. These 12 items were
combined to form a single measure of social support from parents. If measures of support were
only available from one parent, that parent’s supportive information was used. Questions
included “I can depend on my [mother/father] for help with problems” and “My mom/dad helps
me talk about feelings”. Response options ranged from 1 (never true) to 5 (always true), with
higher values indicating more family support. The measure of parent support was reliable
SOCIAL SUPPORT AND ADOLESCENT STRESS !11
(cronbachs alpha = .86, m = 3.11, sd = .69). Responses were normally distributed with a low
score of 1.25 and a high score of 5.
Cumulative Risk. The measure of cumulative risk used in this study was formed by combining 13
dichotomous indicators of risk. For each of the 13 factors, a score of one was used to indicate
high risk and a score of zero was used to indicate low risk. When two family members both
reported on risk items, the average of both reports was used to indicate the level of household
risk. Mothers and fathers level of education were used as separate indicators of risk. Those who
had high school education or less were coded as high risk for the purposes of this study. Total
household income in the last year was counted as a risk factor if a household earned less than
$20,000 in the previous year. A fourth risk factor indicated that someone in the family was
unemployed and in need of work. One risk factor was given to those who were employed in
temporary or seasonal work and single parent families were given a score of one. Parent age at
the time of the birth of the oldest child was used to calculate a dichotomous risk factor that was
given a score of one if a parent was under age 22 at the time of the birth of the first child. Young
mothers and fathers both counted as separate risk factors.
The 500 Families Study used standardized metrics of job prestige and of SES. Prestige
scores were coded according to Nakao and Treas (1994), while SES were coded according to
Duncan SEI scores. Level of risk for prestige and SEI score were determined, for this study, by
choosing the lowest quintile of the scores in this sample, consistent with previous research
(Sameroff, 2005). Mothers’ and fathers’ job prestige and SEI were considered separately,
yielding four additional employment risk factors. For father’s prestige score a score of one was
given to all men with a prestige score of lower than 34, while the cutoff from mother’s was 32.
SOCIAL SUPPORT AND ADOLESCENT STRESS !12
SEI scores were cut off at a score of 380 or lower indicating risk for men, and a score of 374 or
lower indicating risk for women. The final risk factor was formed from a measure of the number
of months the primary respondent had been employed in their current position. Those who had
been employed for less than one year were assigned one risk factor. The sum of all 13 risk factors
was taken to form a measure of cumulative risk. The cumulative risk variable was exceptionally
skewed, M = 1.24, SD = 1.24. The variable ranged from zero to a maximum of seven and well
over half of the sample had a score of one or less on cumulative risk.
Procedure
This data was collected between 1999-2000. Researchers met individually within each
family’s home. The adolescents in this study took part in an experience-sampling project (ESM)
for seven consecutive days. Thus, each participant was signaled a total of 56 times. Adolescents
wore wristwatches that signaled them randomly eight times per day between 7:30 AM and 10:30
PM. At the time they were signaled, adolescents reported their current activities and emotions,
including level of stress and perceptions of control. Participants, on average, provided 30.06 (SD
=15.05) ESM reports.
Results
There was a negative bivariate association between stress and risk, indicating that those
families with relatively more risk reported less stress overall (r = -.144, p = .017) however, there
were no associations between level of risk and adolescents report of either parental or peer
support, r = -.08, p = .201, r = -.00, p = .950, respectively. In addition, neither adolescent reports
of support received from parents nor support received from peers was associated with their
SOCIAL SUPPORT AND ADOLESCENT STRESS !13
reports of stress, r = -.01, p = .87, r = -.04, p = .51, respectively. Adolescents who reported more
support from their peers also tended to report more support from parents (r = .18, p = .006).
Two multiple regression analyses were conducted to examine the role of peer support and
parent support as they interact with multiple risk in predicting adolescent stress. The first
analysis examined adolescent reports of parent support and cumulative risk status as well as the
interaction between those two factors in predicting stress. No unique effect of risk was found on
stress, no unique effect of parent support was found on stress, and there was no evidence of an
interaction between cumulative risk and parent support in predicting teen stress. The second
analysis tested the role of peer support in cumulative risk in predicting stress. For this analysis,
there was a statistically significant effect of cumulative risk on stress, suggesting that those with
higher levels of risk reported less stress. However, there was no effect of peer support on stress,
nor any interaction between cumulative risk and peer support.
Discussion
Our results indicate that none of our hypotheses were supported by the data. Overall, teen
reports of both peer support and parent support did not predict stress. We also did not find an
interaction in either the peer or family support models, thus the association between risk and
stress did not differ for people high or low in either peer or parent support. However, we did find
that parent support was positively related to peer support, meaning that more parental support
was related to more peer support. We also had a main effect of multiple risk on stress in the peer
support model. This suggests that adolescents with higher levels of risk reported less stress.
Overall, our adolescent population reported very low levels of stress, lending credence to those
researchers who are going away from the storm-and-stress model of adolescence.
SOCIAL SUPPORT AND ADOLESCENT STRESS !14
The storm-and-stress model of adolescence is the idea that adolescence is the period of
life that is the most difficult to go through for both the adolescent and others around them
(Buchanen et al., 1990). However, current psychologists have rejected the notion that the storm-
and-stress model is actually universal and inevitable (Arnett, 1999). The storm-and-stress model
has less relevance with an upper middle class population as they are not subjected to the same
level of stressors as adolescents who belong to a lower socioeconomic status (Elkin & Westley,
1955). Additionally, adolescents who belong to a high SES group experience significantly lower
levels of negative life change overall than their low SES counterparts (Gad & Johnson, 1980).
Our results are in line with stress experiences in upper middle class adolescents. Since upper
middle class adolescents experience low levels of stress on average, our results make sense. We
wouldn’t expect to find an adolescent population like ours to experience high levels of stress or
even moderate levels of stress or risk factors (Evens & English, 2002). It also makes sense that
peer or family social support would not be found to influence stress levels in a population with
near non-existent stress levels.
Exposure to stress during adolescence is a large factor in determining vulnerability to
later psychopathologies (Grant et al., 2003). Due to the negative health risks associated with
stress, it is reassuring that there are populations of adolescents who are basically unstressed. As
our population was predominantly upper middle class, it would be beneficial to look into why
this population experiences low stress and low risk as compared to other socioeconomic groups.
While it is possible that the deciding factor for stress and risk levels is lower socioeconomic
status, it is also likely that other factors (e.g. low maternal nurturing, minimal access to
healthcare) are contributing to increased stress and risk experiences in other socioeconomic
SOCIAL SUPPORT AND ADOLESCENT STRESS !15
levels. If high maternal nurturing or increased access to healthcare is contributing to lower stress
and risk experiences for upper middle class adolescents, than increasing maternal nurturing and
healthcare access for other socioeconomic groups could potentially lower stress and risk
experiences for those adolescents.
!
!
!
!
!
!
!
!
!
!
!
!
!
!
!
!
!
!
SOCIAL SUPPORT AND ADOLESCENT STRESS !16
References
!
Ader, R. (2001). Psychoneuroimmunology. Current Directions In Psychological Science, 10,
94-98. doi:10.1111/1467-8721.00124
Appleyard, K., Egeland, B., van Dulmen, M. M., & Sroufe, L. (2005). When more is not better:
The role of cumulative risk in child behavior outcomes. Journal Of Child Psychology
And Psychiatry, 46, 235-245. doi:10.1111/j.1469-7610.2004.00351.x
Arnett, J. (1999). Adolescent storm and stress, reconsidered. American Psychologist, 54,
317-326. doi:10.1037/0003-066X.54.5.317
Arnold, D. H., & Doctoroff, G. L. (2003). The early education of socioeconomically
disadvantaged children. Annual Review Of Psychology, 54, 517-545. doi:10.1146/
annurev.psych.54.111301.145442
Barrera, M., & Garrison-Jones, C. (1992). Family and peer social support as specific correlates
of adolescent depressive symptoms. Journal Of Abnormal Child Psychology, 20, 1-16.
doi:10.1007/BF00927113
Black, P. H., & Garbutt, L. D. (2002). Stress, inflammation and cardiovascular disease. Journal
Of Psychosomatic Research, 52, 1-23. doi:10.1016/S0022-3999(01)00302-6
Bronfenbrenner, U. (1986). Ecology of the family as a context for human development: Research
perspectives. Developmental Psychology, 22, 723-742
Brummett, B. H., Barefoot, J. C., Siegler, I. C., Clapp-Channing, N. E., Lytle, B. L., Bosworth,
H. B., & ... Mark, D. B. (2001). Characteristics of socially isolated patients with coronary
artery disease who are at elevated risk for mortality. Psychosomatic Medicine, 63,
267-272.
SOCIAL SUPPORT AND ADOLESCENT STRESS !17
Buchanan, C., Eccles, J., Flanagan, C., Midgley, C., Feldlaufer, H., & Harold, R. (1990). Parents'
and teachers' beliefs about adolescents: Effects of sex and experience. Journal of Youth
and Adolescence, 19, 363-394. doi:10.1007/BF01537078
Cheng, S., & Chan, A. M. (2004). The multidimensional scale of perceived social support:
dimensionality and age and gender differences in adolescents. Personality & Individual
Differences, 37, 1359-1369. doi:10.1016/j.paid.2004.01.006
Cohen, S., & Wills, T. A. (1985). Stress, social support, and the buffering hypothesis.
Psychological Bulletin, 98, 310-357. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.98.2.310
Cohen, S., Gottlieb, B. H., & Underwood, L. G. (2000). Social relationships and health. In S.
Cohen, L. G. Underwood, B. H. Gottlieb (Eds.) , Social support measurement and
intervention: A guide for health and social scientists (pp. 3-25). New York, NY US:
Oxford University Press.
Cohen, S. (2004). Social Relationships and Health. American Psychologist, 59, 676-684. doi:
10.1037/0003-066X.59.8.676
Elkin, F., & Westley, W. A. (1955). The myth of adolescent culture. American
Sociological Review, 20, 680-684.
Eisenberger, N., Taylor, S., Gable, S., Hilmert, C., & Lieberman, M. (2007). Neural pathways
link social support to attenuated neuroendocrine stress responses. Neuroimage, 35,
1601-1612. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2007.01.038
Evans, G. W., & English, K. (2002). The environment of poverty: Multiple stressor exposure,
psychophysiological stress, and socioemotional adjustment. Child Development, 73,
1238-1248
SOCIAL SUPPORT AND ADOLESCENT STRESS !18
Evans, G. W. (2004). The Environment of Childhood Poverty. American Psychologist, 59,
77-92. doi:10.1037/0003-066X.59.2.77
Evens, G. W, & Kim, P. (2013). Childhood poverty, chronic stress, self-regulation, and coping.
Child Development Perspectives, 7, 43-48. doi:101111/cdep.12013
Fergus, S., & Zimmerman, M. A. (2005). Adolescent resilience: A framework for understanding
healthy development in the face of risk. Annual Review Of Public Health, 26, 399-419.
doi:10.1146/annurev.publhealth.26.021304.144357
Frey, C. U., & Röthlisberger, C. (1996). Social support in healthy adolescents. Journal Of Youth
And Adolescence, 25, 17-31. doi:10.1007/BF01537378
Furman, W., & Buhrmester, D. (1992). Age and Sex Differences in Perceptions of Networks of
Personal Relationships. Child Development, 63, 103-15.
Gad, M., & Johnson, J. H. (1980). Correlates of adolescent life stress as related to race, SES,
and levels of perceived social support. Journal of Clinical Child Psychology, 9, 13.
Gauze, C., Bukowski, W. M., Aquan-Assee, J., & Sippola, L. K. (1996). Interactions between
family environment and friendship and associations with self-perceived well-being
during adolescence. Child Development, 67, 2201-2216. doi:10.2307/1131618
Geronimus, A. T., Hicken, M., Keene, D., & Bound, J. (2006). 'Weathering' and Age Patterns of
Allostatic Load Scores Among Blacks and Whites in the United States. American Journal
Of Public Health, 96, 826-833. doi:10.2105/AJPH.2004.060749
Grant, K. E., Compas, B. E., Stuhlmacher, A. F., Thurm, A. E., McMahon, S. D., & Halpert, J. A.
(2003). Stressors and child and adolescent psychopathology: Moving from markers to
mechanisms of risk. Psychological Bulletin, 129, 447-466. doi:
SOCIAL SUPPORT AND ADOLESCENT STRESS !19
10.1037/0033-2909.129.3.447
Gunnar, M., & Quevedo, K. (2007). The Neurobiology of Stress and Development. Annual
Review Of Psychology, 58, 145-173. doi:10.1146/annurev.psych.58.110405.085605
Heinrichs, M., Baumgartner, T., Kirschbaum, C., & Ehlert, U. (2003). Social support and
oxytocin interact to suppress cortisol and subjective responses to psychosocial stress.
Biological Psychiatry, 54, 1389-1398. doi:10.1016/S0006-3223(03)00465-7
Jessor, R. (1993). Successful adolescent development among youth in high-risk settings.
American Psychologist, 48, 117-126. doi:10.1037/0003-066X.48.2.117
McEwen, B. S. (1998). Stress, adaptation, and disease: Allostasis and allostatic load. In S. M.
McCann, J. M. Lipton, E. M. Sternberg, G. P. Chrousos, P. W. Gold, C. C. Smith (Eds.) ,
Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, Vol. 840: Neuroimmunomodulation:
Molecular aspects, integrative systems, and clinical advances (pp. 33-44). New York, NY
US: New York Academy of Sciences.
McEwen, B. S., & Seeman, T. (1999). Protective and damaging effects of mediators of stress:
Elaborating and testing the concepts of allostasis and allostatic load. In N. E. Adler, M.
Marmot, B. S. McEwen, J. Stewart (Eds.) , Socioeconomic status and health in industrial
nations: Social, psychological, and biological pathways (pp. 30-47). New York, NY US:
New York Academy of Sciences.
McEwen, B. S. (2000). Allostasis and allostatic load: Implications for
neuropsychopharmacology. Neuropsychopharmacology, 22, 108-124. doi:10.1016/
S0893-133X(99)00129-3
Miller, G. E., & Chen, E. (2013). The biological residue of childhood poverty. Child
SOCIAL SUPPORT AND ADOLESCENT STRESS !20
Development Perspectives, 7, 67-73. doi:10.1111/cdep.12021
Peters, E., Riksen-Walraven, J., Cillessen, A. N., & de Weerth, C. (2011). Peer rejection and
HPA activity in middle childhood: Friendship makes a difference. Child Development, 82,
1906-1920. doi:10.1111/j.1467-8624.2011.01647.x
Repetti, R. L., Taylor, S. E., & Seeman, T. E. (2002). Risky families: Family social environments
and the mental and physical health of offspring. Psychological Bulletin, 128, 330-366.
doi:10.1037/0033-2909.128.2.330
Rigby, K. (2000). Effects of peer victimization in schools and perceived social support on
adolescent well-being. Journal of Adolescence, 23, 57-68. doi:10.1006/jado.1999.0289
Rueger, S., Malecki, C., & Demaray, M. (2010). Relationship between Multiple Sources of
Perceived Social Support and Psychological and Academic Adjustment in Early
Adolescence: Comparisons across Gender. Journal Of Youth And Adolescence, 39, 47-61.
doi:10.1007/s10964-008-9368-6
Sameroff, A., Seifer, R., Zax, M., & Barocas, R. (1987). Early Indicators of Developmental Risk:
Rochester Longitudinal Study. Schizophrenia Bulletin, 13, 383-394.
Sameroff, A. J. (2000). Dialectical processes in developmental psychopathology. In A. J.
Sameroff, M. Lewis, S. M. Miller (Eds.) , Handbook of developmental psychopathology
(2nd ed.) (pp. 23-40). Dordrecht Netherlands: Kluwer Academic Publishers. doi:
10.1007/978-1-4615-4163-9_2
Sipal, R., & Sayin, U. (2013). Impact of perceived social support and depression on the parental
attitudes of mothers of children who are deaf. Journal Of Child And Family Studies, 22,
1103-1111. doi:10.1007/s10826-012-9672-3
SOCIAL SUPPORT AND ADOLESCENT STRESS !21
Steinberg, L., & Morris, A. (2001). Adolescent development. Annual Review Of Psychology, 52,
83-110. doi:10.1146/annurev.psych.52.1.83
Taylor, S. E. (2007). Social Support. In H. S. Friedman, R. Silver (Eds.), Foundations of health
psychology (pp. 145-171). New York, NY US: Oxford University Press.
Uchino, B. N., Cacioppo, J. T., & Kiecolt-Glaser, J. K. (1996). The relationship between social
support and physiological processes: A review with emphasis on underlying mechanisms
and implications for health. Psychological Bulletin, 119, 488-531. doi:
10.1037/0033-2909.119.3.488
Uchino, B. N. (2006). Social Support and Health: A Review of Physiological Processes
Potentially Underlying Links to Disease Outcomes. Journal Of Behavioral Medicine,
29, 377-387. doi:10.1007/s10865-006-9056-5
Ulrich-Lai, Y. M., & Herman, J. P. (2009). Neural regulation of endocrine and autonomic stress
responses. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 10, 397-409. doi:10.1038/nrn2647
van Beest, M., & Baerveldt, C. (1999). The Relationship Between Adolescents' Social Support
From Parents and From Peers. Adolescence, 34, 193-201
Yates, T. M., Dodds, M. F., Sroufe, L., & Egeland, B. (2003). Exposure to partner violence and
child behavior problems: A prospective study controlling for child physical abuse and
neglect, child cognitive ability, socioeconomic status, and life stress. Development And
Psychopathology, 15, 199-218. doi:10.1017/S0954579403000117
!

More Related Content

What's hot (20)

stress adaptation process
stress adaptation processstress adaptation process
stress adaptation process
 
2 Theories of stress narrated
2 Theories of stress narrated2 Theories of stress narrated
2 Theories of stress narrated
 
Personal Mastery
Personal MasteryPersonal Mastery
Personal Mastery
 
Stressandadaptatiton
Stressandadaptatiton Stressandadaptatiton
Stressandadaptatiton
 
Novacroft_Thought_Paper_SELF_ESTEEM
Novacroft_Thought_Paper_SELF_ESTEEMNovacroft_Thought_Paper_SELF_ESTEEM
Novacroft_Thought_Paper_SELF_ESTEEM
 
éMotion
 éMotion éMotion
éMotion
 
Theories of Stress
Theories of StressTheories of Stress
Theories of Stress
 
F0134249
F0134249F0134249
F0134249
 
Resilience
ResilienceResilience
Resilience
 
Stress
StressStress
Stress
 
The Mental and Physical Health Outcomes of Green Exercise
The Mental and Physical Health Outcomes of Green ExerciseThe Mental and Physical Health Outcomes of Green Exercise
The Mental and Physical Health Outcomes of Green Exercise
 
Stress, Stress Adaptation Model & Crisis intervention
Stress, Stress Adaptation Model & Crisis interventionStress, Stress Adaptation Model & Crisis intervention
Stress, Stress Adaptation Model & Crisis intervention
 
presentation on psychological theories about disasters and impacts
  presentation on psychological theories about disasters and impacts  presentation on psychological theories about disasters and impacts
presentation on psychological theories about disasters and impacts
 
STRESS THEORY
STRESS THEORYSTRESS THEORY
STRESS THEORY
 
Affect / Weight Management
Affect / Weight ManagementAffect / Weight Management
Affect / Weight Management
 
Lifetime trauma-prayer-and-psychological-distress-in-late-life
Lifetime trauma-prayer-and-psychological-distress-in-late-lifeLifetime trauma-prayer-and-psychological-distress-in-late-life
Lifetime trauma-prayer-and-psychological-distress-in-late-life
 
C14
C14C14
C14
 
ATT_1436082840939_Prenatal stress
ATT_1436082840939_Prenatal stressATT_1436082840939_Prenatal stress
ATT_1436082840939_Prenatal stress
 
16 a. anbazhagan new
16 a. anbazhagan new16 a. anbazhagan new
16 a. anbazhagan new
 
Chapter 3
Chapter 3Chapter 3
Chapter 3
 

Viewers also liked

hughes_marcus_sasp14
hughes_marcus_sasp14hughes_marcus_sasp14
hughes_marcus_sasp14Marcus Hughes
 
Online and In the Aisle in Australia, 3 Word of Mouth Strategies for Global R...
Online and In the Aisle in Australia, 3 Word of Mouth Strategies for Global R...Online and In the Aisle in Australia, 3 Word of Mouth Strategies for Global R...
Online and In the Aisle in Australia, 3 Word of Mouth Strategies for Global R...Dalia Seidner
 
mhughes_sasp14_narrow_cplus (1)
mhughes_sasp14_narrow_cplus (1)mhughes_sasp14_narrow_cplus (1)
mhughes_sasp14_narrow_cplus (1)Marcus Hughes
 
KNAC_Lewis_Banales_Hughes_final
KNAC_Lewis_Banales_Hughes_finalKNAC_Lewis_Banales_Hughes_final
KNAC_Lewis_Banales_Hughes_finalMarcus Hughes
 
The Future of High-Tech In China
The Future of High-Tech In ChinaThe Future of High-Tech In China
The Future of High-Tech In ChinaDaxue Consulting
 
Two speed gear box mini project
Two speed gear box mini projectTwo speed gear box mini project
Two speed gear box mini projectAravind Ganesh
 
Models of teaching and learning
Models of teaching  and learningModels of teaching  and learning
Models of teaching and learningchicken52
 
كتاب الخيال العلمى
كتاب الخيال العلمىكتاب الخيال العلمى
كتاب الخيال العلمىhagersaid
 
Πίεση Α
Πίεση ΑΠίεση Α
Πίεση Αsarmeni
 
Webquest milexa
Webquest milexaWebquest milexa
Webquest milexaaftnsdc
 
Samuelnorlen
SamuelnorlenSamuelnorlen
Samuelnorlenfia_j
 

Viewers also liked (20)

hughes_marcus_sasp14
hughes_marcus_sasp14hughes_marcus_sasp14
hughes_marcus_sasp14
 
Online and In the Aisle in Australia, 3 Word of Mouth Strategies for Global R...
Online and In the Aisle in Australia, 3 Word of Mouth Strategies for Global R...Online and In the Aisle in Australia, 3 Word of Mouth Strategies for Global R...
Online and In the Aisle in Australia, 3 Word of Mouth Strategies for Global R...
 
psy 303 presentation
psy 303 presentationpsy 303 presentation
psy 303 presentation
 
Protopiper
Protopiper Protopiper
Protopiper
 
Classroom processbook
Classroom processbookClassroom processbook
Classroom processbook
 
mhughes_sasp14_narrow_cplus (1)
mhughes_sasp14_narrow_cplus (1)mhughes_sasp14_narrow_cplus (1)
mhughes_sasp14_narrow_cplus (1)
 
KNAC_Lewis_Banales_Hughes_final
KNAC_Lewis_Banales_Hughes_finalKNAC_Lewis_Banales_Hughes_final
KNAC_Lewis_Banales_Hughes_final
 
How Will China Age?
How Will China Age? How Will China Age?
How Will China Age?
 
shredder design
shredder designshredder design
shredder design
 
Smart buildings
Smart buildings Smart buildings
Smart buildings
 
Chinese Students Abroad
Chinese Students AbroadChinese Students Abroad
Chinese Students Abroad
 
The Future of High-Tech In China
The Future of High-Tech In ChinaThe Future of High-Tech In China
The Future of High-Tech In China
 
Two speed gear box mini project
Two speed gear box mini projectTwo speed gear box mini project
Two speed gear box mini project
 
Models of teaching and learning
Models of teaching  and learningModels of teaching  and learning
Models of teaching and learning
 
كتاب الخيال العلمى
كتاب الخيال العلمىكتاب الخيال العلمى
كتاب الخيال العلمى
 
Vacations
VacationsVacations
Vacations
 
Friesland tips voor_de_jaarwisseilng
Friesland tips voor_de_jaarwisseilngFriesland tips voor_de_jaarwisseilng
Friesland tips voor_de_jaarwisseilng
 
Πίεση Α
Πίεση ΑΠίεση Α
Πίεση Α
 
Webquest milexa
Webquest milexaWebquest milexa
Webquest milexa
 
Samuelnorlen
SamuelnorlenSamuelnorlen
Samuelnorlen
 

Similar to psy 430 research paper (final)

STRESSMANAGEMENT1321452674686970789-.ppt
STRESSMANAGEMENT1321452674686970789-.pptSTRESSMANAGEMENT1321452674686970789-.ppt
STRESSMANAGEMENT1321452674686970789-.pptCreativeBrainInstitu
 
When Emotional Pain Becomes Physical
When Emotional Pain Becomes PhysicalWhen Emotional Pain Becomes Physical
When Emotional Pain Becomes PhysicalPaul Coelho, MD
 
Over the past several decades
Over the past several decadesOver the past several decades
Over the past several decadeschirag patel
 
Social psychology Entering:Health Psychology
Social psychology Entering:Health PsychologySocial psychology Entering:Health Psychology
Social psychology Entering:Health PsychologyWaseemSharif4
 
ALCohoL ReSeARCh C u r r e n t R e v i e w s506 Alcohol .docx
ALCohoL ReSeARCh C u r r e n t  R e v i e w s506 Alcohol .docxALCohoL ReSeARCh C u r r e n t  R e v i e w s506 Alcohol .docx
ALCohoL ReSeARCh C u r r e n t R e v i e w s506 Alcohol .docxADDY50
 
ALCohoL ReSeARCh C u r r e n t R e v i e w s506 Alcohol .docx
ALCohoL ReSeARCh C u r r e n t  R e v i e w s506 Alcohol .docxALCohoL ReSeARCh C u r r e n t  R e v i e w s506 Alcohol .docx
ALCohoL ReSeARCh C u r r e n t R e v i e w s506 Alcohol .docxSHIVA101531
 
GERO 508 Spring 2021Week 2The Mind & Body Connecti
GERO 508 Spring 2021Week 2The Mind & Body ConnectiGERO 508 Spring 2021Week 2The Mind & Body Connecti
GERO 508 Spring 2021Week 2The Mind & Body ConnectiMatthewTennant613
 
Promoting occupational stress management for a small office (final)
Promoting occupational stress management for a small office (final)Promoting occupational stress management for a small office (final)
Promoting occupational stress management for a small office (final)Katrina Brown
 
PSY 150 403 Chapter 11 SLIDES
PSY 150 403 Chapter 11 SLIDESPSY 150 403 Chapter 11 SLIDES
PSY 150 403 Chapter 11 SLIDESkimappel
 
Abnormal psychology Stress and mental health
 Abnormal psychology Stress and mental health Abnormal psychology Stress and mental health
Abnormal psychology Stress and mental healthKadine Duncan
 
Understanding trauma to promote healing in child welfare.By co-invest.org
Understanding trauma to promote healing in child welfare.By co-invest.orgUnderstanding trauma to promote healing in child welfare.By co-invest.org
Understanding trauma to promote healing in child welfare.By co-invest.orgCassondra Turner McArthur
 
Health Psychology Psychological Adjustment to the Disease, Disability and Loss
Health Psychology Psychological Adjustment to the Disease, Disability and LossHealth Psychology Psychological Adjustment to the Disease, Disability and Loss
Health Psychology Psychological Adjustment to the Disease, Disability and Lossijtsrd
 
How does stress affect mental health
How does stress affect mental healthHow does stress affect mental health
How does stress affect mental healthDrNirajyadav1
 

Similar to psy 430 research paper (final) (19)

STRESSMANAGEMENT1321452674686970789-.ppt
STRESSMANAGEMENT1321452674686970789-.pptSTRESSMANAGEMENT1321452674686970789-.ppt
STRESSMANAGEMENT1321452674686970789-.ppt
 
When Emotional Pain Becomes Physical
When Emotional Pain Becomes PhysicalWhen Emotional Pain Becomes Physical
When Emotional Pain Becomes Physical
 
Thesis_40093281
Thesis_40093281Thesis_40093281
Thesis_40093281
 
Over the past several decades
Over the past several decadesOver the past several decades
Over the past several decades
 
Social psychology Entering:Health Psychology
Social psychology Entering:Health PsychologySocial psychology Entering:Health Psychology
Social psychology Entering:Health Psychology
 
Psychological Trauma Essay
Psychological Trauma EssayPsychological Trauma Essay
Psychological Trauma Essay
 
ALCohoL ReSeARCh C u r r e n t R e v i e w s506 Alcohol .docx
ALCohoL ReSeARCh C u r r e n t  R e v i e w s506 Alcohol .docxALCohoL ReSeARCh C u r r e n t  R e v i e w s506 Alcohol .docx
ALCohoL ReSeARCh C u r r e n t R e v i e w s506 Alcohol .docx
 
ALCohoL ReSeARCh C u r r e n t R e v i e w s506 Alcohol .docx
ALCohoL ReSeARCh C u r r e n t  R e v i e w s506 Alcohol .docxALCohoL ReSeARCh C u r r e n t  R e v i e w s506 Alcohol .docx
ALCohoL ReSeARCh C u r r e n t R e v i e w s506 Alcohol .docx
 
GERO 508 Spring 2021Week 2The Mind & Body Connecti
GERO 508 Spring 2021Week 2The Mind & Body ConnectiGERO 508 Spring 2021Week 2The Mind & Body Connecti
GERO 508 Spring 2021Week 2The Mind & Body Connecti
 
Essay On Stress
Essay On StressEssay On Stress
Essay On Stress
 
Promoting occupational stress management for a small office (final)
Promoting occupational stress management for a small office (final)Promoting occupational stress management for a small office (final)
Promoting occupational stress management for a small office (final)
 
PSY 150 403 Chapter 11 SLIDES
PSY 150 403 Chapter 11 SLIDESPSY 150 403 Chapter 11 SLIDES
PSY 150 403 Chapter 11 SLIDES
 
stress
stressstress
stress
 
Abnormal psychology Stress and mental health
 Abnormal psychology Stress and mental health Abnormal psychology Stress and mental health
Abnormal psychology Stress and mental health
 
STRESS AND COPING STYLE OF URBAN AND RURAL ADOLESCENTS
STRESS AND COPING STYLE OF URBAN AND RURAL ADOLESCENTSSTRESS AND COPING STYLE OF URBAN AND RURAL ADOLESCENTS
STRESS AND COPING STYLE OF URBAN AND RURAL ADOLESCENTS
 
STRESS AND COPING STYLE OF URBAN AND RURAL ADOLESCENTS
STRESS AND COPING STYLE OF URBAN AND RURAL ADOLESCENTSSTRESS AND COPING STYLE OF URBAN AND RURAL ADOLESCENTS
STRESS AND COPING STYLE OF URBAN AND RURAL ADOLESCENTS
 
Understanding trauma to promote healing in child welfare.By co-invest.org
Understanding trauma to promote healing in child welfare.By co-invest.orgUnderstanding trauma to promote healing in child welfare.By co-invest.org
Understanding trauma to promote healing in child welfare.By co-invest.org
 
Health Psychology Psychological Adjustment to the Disease, Disability and Loss
Health Psychology Psychological Adjustment to the Disease, Disability and LossHealth Psychology Psychological Adjustment to the Disease, Disability and Loss
Health Psychology Psychological Adjustment to the Disease, Disability and Loss
 
How does stress affect mental health
How does stress affect mental healthHow does stress affect mental health
How does stress affect mental health
 

psy 430 research paper (final)

  • 1. Running head: SOCIAL SUPPORT AND ADOLESCENT STRESS !1 ! ! ! ! ! ! ! The Role of Family and Peer Social Support in Reported Adolescent Stress Tabitha Smith Western Washington University ! ! ! ! ! ! ! ! ! ! !
  • 2. SOCIAL SUPPORT AND ADOLESCENT STRESS !2 The Role of Family and Peer Social Support in Reported Adolescent Stress Stress. We all have experience with it. This is not necessarily a bad thing. Stress can help us to focus and complete projects on time, help us to make the game winning basket, or help us to survive in a life threatening situation. Experiencing stress can also be a bad thing, like being excluded by a peer group, panicking over a test, or fighting with those closest to us. Stress is defined as an individual appraising an event as a threat to the psychological or physiological self (McEwen, 2000). When we experience a stressor, our body copes by producing specific biological markers such as cortisol. These biological markers are protective in the short term but damaging to our biological processes in the long term (McEwen, 2000). If a stressor happens repeatedly, such as daily exclusion from a peer group, it is called chronic stress. One does not have to experience the same stressor on a regular basis to have chronic stress, just repeated activation of the stress response. Chronic stress can have serious health implications, such as greater risk of cardiovascular disease and lower immune functioning (Black & Garbutt, 2002). The effects of stress can have an additive effect on a person, a principal called allostatic load. Allostatic load is the physiological burden imposed by repeated stress activation (Geronimus, Hicken, Keene, & Bound, 2006). This physiological burden has been shown to accelerate the disease process and increase disease risk (e.g. cardiovascular disease). Early childhood experiences with multiple risk factors, such as abuse, neglect, low socioeconomic status, contributes to allostatic load (McEwen, 2000). However, the level of social support a person receives moderates the effects of stress activation on health outcomes. A high level of social support equates with better health outcomes, while a low level of social support is linked to higher mortality and lower immune
  • 3. SOCIAL SUPPORT AND ADOLESCENT STRESS !3 functioning (Uchino, 2006). We will first discuss how risk factors in adolescence lead to greater stress, then briefly discuss stress activation pathways, followed by discussing the role social support from families and friends can play in moderating the effect of stress for adolescents. “Development is a person-context interaction (Jessor, 1993, p. 119),” suggesting that how an adolescent develops depends not only on their genetics, but also on the interactions that person has with her environment (e.g. family, school, peers). Social environment and family risk factors are related with adolescent risk behaviors and health compromising outcomes. Family, school, and neighborhood are the three most important life contexts that affect an adolescent. These contexts have the most influence on an adolescent’s development. These three settings also affect each other and are influenced by a larger social-structural and cultural environment (e.g. economic, political). These contexts influence the adolescent over time, with time also interacting with the above contexts. These contexts influence adolescent development both directly (i.e. family, school, neighborhood) as well as indirectly (e.g. economic, political) (Bronfenbrenner, 1986 ; Jessor, 1993). An adolescent may experience risk associated within one or more of these contexts, for example being of low socioeconomic status (SES). Low-SES influences all three of the important life contexts (family, school, neighborhood) the adolescent is in, this influence has the possibility of increasing the risk factors an adolescent experiences (Miller & Chen, 2013). Looking at the family context, low-SES can include exposure to unstable family dynamics, caregivers who are unresponsive to an adolescents needs, lack of parental monitoring, household crowding, or inadequate nutrition (Fergus & Zimmerman, 2005; Evans, 2004; Repetti, Taylor, & Seeman, 2002). Looking at the neighborhood context, low-SES can include exposure to
  • 4. SOCIAL SUPPORT AND ADOLESCENT STRESS !4 infectious microorganisms, industrial pollutants, or limited community resources (Fergus & Zimmerman, 2005; Evans, 2004). These risk factors lead to a number of negative outcomes, such as poor academic achievement, violent behavior, and a number of adult health problems (e.g. cardiovascular disease) (Arnold & Doctorff, 2003). Low parental SES is known to increase child cortisol levels and chronic stress. Chronic stress experienced by children damage the biological and psychological regulatory processes (Evans & Kim, 2013). Low-SES children are exposed to many more multiple stressors than their more economically advantaged peers. There are few low-SES children who are exposed to only one or fewer stressors (Evans & English, 2002). For stress experiences to increase health and behavior problems in adolescence, exposure to multiple risk factors is what matters most. The cumulative risk research shows that co- occurring risk factors have a deleterious effect on health and behavior outcomes (Appleyard, Egeland, van Dulmen, & Sroufe, 2005; Sameroff, Seifer, Zax, & Barocas, 1987). The cumulative risk hypothesis asserts that with increased risk factors come increased clinical problems (Sameroff, 2000). The number of risk factors experienced is associated with adolescent mental health, problem behaviors, and academic problems (Yates, Dodds, Sroufe, & Egeland, 2003). Additionally, it is now clear that life course experiences contribute to a number of common medical problems (e.g. heart disease, stroke; Miller & Chen, 2013). Several other risk factors associated with adolescent mental health, problem behavior, and academic problems are: unskilled parental occupation status, low maternal education status, disadvantaged minority status, single parenthood, and stressful life events (Sameroff, 2000). However, socially supportive relationships have positive effects on the health outcomes and the mortality rates (Fergus & Zimmerman, 2005; Heinrichs, Baumgartner, Kirschbaum, & Ehlert, 2003).
  • 5. SOCIAL SUPPORT AND ADOLESCENT STRESS !5 I will briefly discuss what systems are activated from a stress response, for a detailed review of this process see other reviews (Gunnar & Quevedo, 2007; Ulrich-Lai & Herman, 2009). When we appraise a situation as threatening, a stress response is activated within our body. The two interrelated but distinct systems activated by appraising a situation as threatening are the sympathetic nervous symptom (SNS) and the hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenalcortical (HPA) axis (Gunnar & Quevedo, 2007). The SNS is responsible for releasing catecholamines such as epinephrine (adrenaline), from the adrenal gland into the body. Epinephrine is a hormone responsible for the fight or flight response. It is responsible for increasing the heart rate and blood pressure, thus ensuring the body has enough oxygenated blood to send to the muscles so we can run away or stay and fight. The HPA axis is responsible for producing glucocorticoids, such as cortisol. Cortisol helps to mobilize the energy resources needed within the body during stressful situations (McEwen, 1998). Glucocorticoids take about 25 minutes to be proceed to peak levels within the body. Thus, the effects are slower to develop and last longer than the hormones released by the SNS (Gunnar & Quevedo, 2007). The actions of the SNS results in the activation of the HPA axis. The eventual aim of the HPA axis is the activation of the peripheral nervous system (PNS), which is responsible for turning off the stress response cascade initiated by the SNS and HPA systems. Usually, the activation of these stress systems help to keep the body in allostasis (McEwen & Seeman, 1999). However, frequent activation dysregulates these systems which inhibits the ability to turn off the stress cascade. As such, the physiological systems impacted stay elevated, with cortisol not being able to decrease or having habitually raised blood pressure and heart rate (Gunnar & Quevedo, 2007). When cortisol levels are elevated the immune system is suppressed, leading to greater
  • 6. SOCIAL SUPPORT AND ADOLESCENT STRESS !6 vulnerability to illness (Ader, 2001). This system dysregulation increases the risk of both physical and mental health problems (McEwen & Seeman, 1999). However, social support reduces the effect physiological stress reactivity has on the body (Uchino, Cacioppo, & Kiecolt- Glaser, 1996). Social support may moderate stress reactivity at two points during the appraisal of a stressful event. Social support may alter the appraisal itself, preventing a threatening event from being perceived as stressful, which prevents the stress response activation. Alternatively, social support may alter the period after appraisal, but before a prolonged physiological stress response is activated. Perceived social support may help people to have better regulation or coping skills for a stressful experience which reduces the physiological stress response (Cohen & Wills, 1985). Social support attenuates the relationship between risk factors and the physiological stress response activated by appraising an event as threatening (Eisenberger, Taylor, Gable, Hillmert, & Lieberman, 2007; Heinrichs, Baumgartner, Kirschbaum, Ehlert, 2003; Uchino, 2006). In a laboratory study, participants completed the Trier Social Stress Test (TSST) to induce the stress response. The TSST consists of verbal mental arithmetic and public speaking performed in front of a crowd. To test if social support does lower the stress response, half of the participants were told to bring a friend. This friend was with them during their preparation for the TSST tasks. Participants with a friend present experienced lower levels of cortisol activation during the TSST tasks (Heinrichs, Baumgartner, Kirschbaum, Ehlert, 2003). However, less social support increases an individual’s negative health risks, such as earlier mortality rates, increased cardiovascular disease (Brummet et al., 2001), and depression
  • 7. SOCIAL SUPPORT AND ADOLESCENT STRESS !7 (Sipal & Sayin, 2013). Social support is defined as social relationship processes that have the ability to promote health and well-being (Cohen, Gottlieb, & Underwood, 2000) and refers to the psychological and material resources received from belonging to a social group that can help an individual to cope with stress (Cohen, 2000). Social support can come from a variety of sources, including a partner, friends, or family. Social support may include distinct transactions where a person receives benefits from someone else, or a person may feel they have access to help or support from someone (Taylor, 2007). Social support is thought to buffer the effects of stress by reducing the effects from a stressful experience through promoting the appropriate coping mechanisms and reappraising the event as less threatening (Cohen, 2004). During adolescence social support groups change, giving adolescents the ability to receive support from multiple groups. More time is spent with friends in addition to a considerable drop in the amount of time spent with parents. However, family influence over an adolescent is still high, but the amount of influence changes depending on the cohesiveness of the family and the closeness of friends (Steinberg & Morris, 2001). In a less cohesive family, the influence a family has on an adolescent is low. For an adolescent with no close friends, family influence is high (Gauze, Bukowski, Aquan-Assee, & Sippola, 1996). These different influences can effect perceived support. We will be looking at the effect both family and friend social support has on perceived stress experiences for adolescents. There seem to be gender differences in the effect social support plays in moderating the relationship between risk factors and stress (Rueger, Malecki, & Demaray, 2010). While both boys and girls report similar levels of family social support (Rueger, Malecki, & Demaray, 2010), girls report higher levels of peer social support than boys (Cheng & Chan, 2004; Furman
  • 8. SOCIAL SUPPORT AND ADOLESCENT STRESS !8 & Buhrmester, 1992). Girls also report receiving more social support from peers than from their parents, while boys report more social support from their parents (Frey & Röthlisberger, 1996). High levels of family social support have been consistently associated with lower depression, higher self-esteem, and better academic adjustment. In contrast, low levels of family social support is associated with psychological distress and emotional problems (Rueger, Malecki, & Demaray, 2010). There is evidence for both low levels of family social support not being compensated by high levels of peer support (van Beest & Baerveldt, 1999), and evidence that high peer social support can compensate for of family social support (Barrera & Garrison-Jones, 1992). Family social support as perceived by an adolescent is expected to moderate the link between risk factors and experienced stress (Barrera & Garrison-Jones, 1992). In a study using an inpatient population of depressed adolescents, adolescents who perceived more social support from families had fewer depressive symptoms than adolescents who perceived low levels of social support from their families (Barrera & Garrison-Jones, 1992). Low friend social support can increase an adolescent’s stress response when being excluded by their peers, while perceiving high friend social support can decrease the stress response when excluded by peers (Rigby, 2000; Peters, Riksen-Walraven, Cillessen, & de Weerth, 2011). In an inpatient population of depressed adolescents, those who perceived high friend social support experienced fewer depressive symptoms, but only when perceived family social support was low (Barrera & Garrison-Jones, 1992). We designed the following hypothesis to determine if the number of risk factors moderate type of support (peer vs family) effect on stress. Overall, adolescents with more risk factors will
  • 9. SOCIAL SUPPORT AND ADOLESCENT STRESS !9 report higher levels of stress. Adolescents with more social support will report lower levels of stress. This relationship is expected to be present for both family and peer social support. The effect of family vs friend support on stress is expected to vary depending on the amount of risk the adolescent experiences. For adolescents with more risk factors, family social support will result in lower levels of reported stress. Low levels of family social support will result in higher levels of reported stress. Adolescents with more risk factors and high levels of peer social support will report lower levels of stress, while low peer social support will result in higher reported levels of stress. Method Participants This sample consisted of 276 parents and adolescents. The number of participants varied slightly, ranging from 244 to 276, depending on the analysis that was examined. We used all data available for each analysis. The primary responder (parent) in each sample was predominately white (85.5%), 7.2% black, 2.9% Hispanic, and the remaining identified as multi-racial or Asian Pacific Islander, with 1.4% not specifying. Most target parents were in monogamous relationships (80.5%), 15.8% were divorced or separated, 3.7% were never married. Adolescents were 53.9% female and 45.4% male, with .7% not indicating sex. The average age of the adolescents was 15.4 years (1.59). Most participants were middle-upper class in socioeconomic status, with 20.4% indicating that they earned more than $100,000 per year. This sample was taken from a larger sample of five hundred families, who were recruited from seven different sites throughout the U.S., located in the Midwest, Southeast, Northeast, and West Coast. Measures
  • 10. SOCIAL SUPPORT AND ADOLESCENT STRESS !10 Stress. Adolescent’s reports of stress were obtained from the experience sampling portion of this study. For each experience sampling reading, adolescents responded to the question: As you were beeped were you feeling… “nervous”, “strained”, or “stressed”. Adolescents chose a response on a scale of zero (not at all) to three (very much). Responses to these three items were positively associated, Cronbach’s alpha = .793. A mean was taken for each of the three measures as they were reported for each experience sampling moment. Next, all of the momentary stress scores for a particular individual were combined to form an average level of stress for each person. This score summarizes the adolescents’ average level of stress as it was reported over the course of the experience sampling portion of the study. This stress measure ranged from zero to 2.06, M = . 50, SD = .39. The distribution was positively skewed suggesting that adolescents reported very little stress overall. Social Support. Peer support was measured through teen responses to 5-items. Teens responded to questions such as “my friends care about how I am” and “I can count on my friends” using response options from 1 (never true) to 5 (always true). The peer social support measure was reliable with a cronbachs alpha = .82 (m = 4.00, sd = 0.74). Responses ranged from 1 through 5, and the overall distribution was negatively skewed. Six items were used to capture social support from mothers while six items captured social support from fathers. These 12 items were combined to form a single measure of social support from parents. If measures of support were only available from one parent, that parent’s supportive information was used. Questions included “I can depend on my [mother/father] for help with problems” and “My mom/dad helps me talk about feelings”. Response options ranged from 1 (never true) to 5 (always true), with higher values indicating more family support. The measure of parent support was reliable
  • 11. SOCIAL SUPPORT AND ADOLESCENT STRESS !11 (cronbachs alpha = .86, m = 3.11, sd = .69). Responses were normally distributed with a low score of 1.25 and a high score of 5. Cumulative Risk. The measure of cumulative risk used in this study was formed by combining 13 dichotomous indicators of risk. For each of the 13 factors, a score of one was used to indicate high risk and a score of zero was used to indicate low risk. When two family members both reported on risk items, the average of both reports was used to indicate the level of household risk. Mothers and fathers level of education were used as separate indicators of risk. Those who had high school education or less were coded as high risk for the purposes of this study. Total household income in the last year was counted as a risk factor if a household earned less than $20,000 in the previous year. A fourth risk factor indicated that someone in the family was unemployed and in need of work. One risk factor was given to those who were employed in temporary or seasonal work and single parent families were given a score of one. Parent age at the time of the birth of the oldest child was used to calculate a dichotomous risk factor that was given a score of one if a parent was under age 22 at the time of the birth of the first child. Young mothers and fathers both counted as separate risk factors. The 500 Families Study used standardized metrics of job prestige and of SES. Prestige scores were coded according to Nakao and Treas (1994), while SES were coded according to Duncan SEI scores. Level of risk for prestige and SEI score were determined, for this study, by choosing the lowest quintile of the scores in this sample, consistent with previous research (Sameroff, 2005). Mothers’ and fathers’ job prestige and SEI were considered separately, yielding four additional employment risk factors. For father’s prestige score a score of one was given to all men with a prestige score of lower than 34, while the cutoff from mother’s was 32.
  • 12. SOCIAL SUPPORT AND ADOLESCENT STRESS !12 SEI scores were cut off at a score of 380 or lower indicating risk for men, and a score of 374 or lower indicating risk for women. The final risk factor was formed from a measure of the number of months the primary respondent had been employed in their current position. Those who had been employed for less than one year were assigned one risk factor. The sum of all 13 risk factors was taken to form a measure of cumulative risk. The cumulative risk variable was exceptionally skewed, M = 1.24, SD = 1.24. The variable ranged from zero to a maximum of seven and well over half of the sample had a score of one or less on cumulative risk. Procedure This data was collected between 1999-2000. Researchers met individually within each family’s home. The adolescents in this study took part in an experience-sampling project (ESM) for seven consecutive days. Thus, each participant was signaled a total of 56 times. Adolescents wore wristwatches that signaled them randomly eight times per day between 7:30 AM and 10:30 PM. At the time they were signaled, adolescents reported their current activities and emotions, including level of stress and perceptions of control. Participants, on average, provided 30.06 (SD =15.05) ESM reports. Results There was a negative bivariate association between stress and risk, indicating that those families with relatively more risk reported less stress overall (r = -.144, p = .017) however, there were no associations between level of risk and adolescents report of either parental or peer support, r = -.08, p = .201, r = -.00, p = .950, respectively. In addition, neither adolescent reports of support received from parents nor support received from peers was associated with their
  • 13. SOCIAL SUPPORT AND ADOLESCENT STRESS !13 reports of stress, r = -.01, p = .87, r = -.04, p = .51, respectively. Adolescents who reported more support from their peers also tended to report more support from parents (r = .18, p = .006). Two multiple regression analyses were conducted to examine the role of peer support and parent support as they interact with multiple risk in predicting adolescent stress. The first analysis examined adolescent reports of parent support and cumulative risk status as well as the interaction between those two factors in predicting stress. No unique effect of risk was found on stress, no unique effect of parent support was found on stress, and there was no evidence of an interaction between cumulative risk and parent support in predicting teen stress. The second analysis tested the role of peer support in cumulative risk in predicting stress. For this analysis, there was a statistically significant effect of cumulative risk on stress, suggesting that those with higher levels of risk reported less stress. However, there was no effect of peer support on stress, nor any interaction between cumulative risk and peer support. Discussion Our results indicate that none of our hypotheses were supported by the data. Overall, teen reports of both peer support and parent support did not predict stress. We also did not find an interaction in either the peer or family support models, thus the association between risk and stress did not differ for people high or low in either peer or parent support. However, we did find that parent support was positively related to peer support, meaning that more parental support was related to more peer support. We also had a main effect of multiple risk on stress in the peer support model. This suggests that adolescents with higher levels of risk reported less stress. Overall, our adolescent population reported very low levels of stress, lending credence to those researchers who are going away from the storm-and-stress model of adolescence.
  • 14. SOCIAL SUPPORT AND ADOLESCENT STRESS !14 The storm-and-stress model of adolescence is the idea that adolescence is the period of life that is the most difficult to go through for both the adolescent and others around them (Buchanen et al., 1990). However, current psychologists have rejected the notion that the storm- and-stress model is actually universal and inevitable (Arnett, 1999). The storm-and-stress model has less relevance with an upper middle class population as they are not subjected to the same level of stressors as adolescents who belong to a lower socioeconomic status (Elkin & Westley, 1955). Additionally, adolescents who belong to a high SES group experience significantly lower levels of negative life change overall than their low SES counterparts (Gad & Johnson, 1980). Our results are in line with stress experiences in upper middle class adolescents. Since upper middle class adolescents experience low levels of stress on average, our results make sense. We wouldn’t expect to find an adolescent population like ours to experience high levels of stress or even moderate levels of stress or risk factors (Evens & English, 2002). It also makes sense that peer or family social support would not be found to influence stress levels in a population with near non-existent stress levels. Exposure to stress during adolescence is a large factor in determining vulnerability to later psychopathologies (Grant et al., 2003). Due to the negative health risks associated with stress, it is reassuring that there are populations of adolescents who are basically unstressed. As our population was predominantly upper middle class, it would be beneficial to look into why this population experiences low stress and low risk as compared to other socioeconomic groups. While it is possible that the deciding factor for stress and risk levels is lower socioeconomic status, it is also likely that other factors (e.g. low maternal nurturing, minimal access to healthcare) are contributing to increased stress and risk experiences in other socioeconomic
  • 15. SOCIAL SUPPORT AND ADOLESCENT STRESS !15 levels. If high maternal nurturing or increased access to healthcare is contributing to lower stress and risk experiences for upper middle class adolescents, than increasing maternal nurturing and healthcare access for other socioeconomic groups could potentially lower stress and risk experiences for those adolescents. ! ! ! ! ! ! ! ! ! ! ! ! ! ! ! ! ! !
  • 16. SOCIAL SUPPORT AND ADOLESCENT STRESS !16 References ! Ader, R. (2001). Psychoneuroimmunology. Current Directions In Psychological Science, 10, 94-98. doi:10.1111/1467-8721.00124 Appleyard, K., Egeland, B., van Dulmen, M. M., & Sroufe, L. (2005). When more is not better: The role of cumulative risk in child behavior outcomes. Journal Of Child Psychology And Psychiatry, 46, 235-245. doi:10.1111/j.1469-7610.2004.00351.x Arnett, J. (1999). Adolescent storm and stress, reconsidered. American Psychologist, 54, 317-326. doi:10.1037/0003-066X.54.5.317 Arnold, D. H., & Doctoroff, G. L. (2003). The early education of socioeconomically disadvantaged children. Annual Review Of Psychology, 54, 517-545. doi:10.1146/ annurev.psych.54.111301.145442 Barrera, M., & Garrison-Jones, C. (1992). Family and peer social support as specific correlates of adolescent depressive symptoms. Journal Of Abnormal Child Psychology, 20, 1-16. doi:10.1007/BF00927113 Black, P. H., & Garbutt, L. D. (2002). Stress, inflammation and cardiovascular disease. Journal Of Psychosomatic Research, 52, 1-23. doi:10.1016/S0022-3999(01)00302-6 Bronfenbrenner, U. (1986). Ecology of the family as a context for human development: Research perspectives. Developmental Psychology, 22, 723-742 Brummett, B. H., Barefoot, J. C., Siegler, I. C., Clapp-Channing, N. E., Lytle, B. L., Bosworth, H. B., & ... Mark, D. B. (2001). Characteristics of socially isolated patients with coronary artery disease who are at elevated risk for mortality. Psychosomatic Medicine, 63, 267-272.
  • 17. SOCIAL SUPPORT AND ADOLESCENT STRESS !17 Buchanan, C., Eccles, J., Flanagan, C., Midgley, C., Feldlaufer, H., & Harold, R. (1990). Parents' and teachers' beliefs about adolescents: Effects of sex and experience. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 19, 363-394. doi:10.1007/BF01537078 Cheng, S., & Chan, A. M. (2004). The multidimensional scale of perceived social support: dimensionality and age and gender differences in adolescents. Personality & Individual Differences, 37, 1359-1369. doi:10.1016/j.paid.2004.01.006 Cohen, S., & Wills, T. A. (1985). Stress, social support, and the buffering hypothesis. Psychological Bulletin, 98, 310-357. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.98.2.310 Cohen, S., Gottlieb, B. H., & Underwood, L. G. (2000). Social relationships and health. In S. Cohen, L. G. Underwood, B. H. Gottlieb (Eds.) , Social support measurement and intervention: A guide for health and social scientists (pp. 3-25). New York, NY US: Oxford University Press. Cohen, S. (2004). Social Relationships and Health. American Psychologist, 59, 676-684. doi: 10.1037/0003-066X.59.8.676 Elkin, F., & Westley, W. A. (1955). The myth of adolescent culture. American Sociological Review, 20, 680-684. Eisenberger, N., Taylor, S., Gable, S., Hilmert, C., & Lieberman, M. (2007). Neural pathways link social support to attenuated neuroendocrine stress responses. Neuroimage, 35, 1601-1612. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2007.01.038 Evans, G. W., & English, K. (2002). The environment of poverty: Multiple stressor exposure, psychophysiological stress, and socioemotional adjustment. Child Development, 73, 1238-1248
  • 18. SOCIAL SUPPORT AND ADOLESCENT STRESS !18 Evans, G. W. (2004). The Environment of Childhood Poverty. American Psychologist, 59, 77-92. doi:10.1037/0003-066X.59.2.77 Evens, G. W, & Kim, P. (2013). Childhood poverty, chronic stress, self-regulation, and coping. Child Development Perspectives, 7, 43-48. doi:101111/cdep.12013 Fergus, S., & Zimmerman, M. A. (2005). Adolescent resilience: A framework for understanding healthy development in the face of risk. Annual Review Of Public Health, 26, 399-419. doi:10.1146/annurev.publhealth.26.021304.144357 Frey, C. U., & Röthlisberger, C. (1996). Social support in healthy adolescents. Journal Of Youth And Adolescence, 25, 17-31. doi:10.1007/BF01537378 Furman, W., & Buhrmester, D. (1992). Age and Sex Differences in Perceptions of Networks of Personal Relationships. Child Development, 63, 103-15. Gad, M., & Johnson, J. H. (1980). Correlates of adolescent life stress as related to race, SES, and levels of perceived social support. Journal of Clinical Child Psychology, 9, 13. Gauze, C., Bukowski, W. M., Aquan-Assee, J., & Sippola, L. K. (1996). Interactions between family environment and friendship and associations with self-perceived well-being during adolescence. Child Development, 67, 2201-2216. doi:10.2307/1131618 Geronimus, A. T., Hicken, M., Keene, D., & Bound, J. (2006). 'Weathering' and Age Patterns of Allostatic Load Scores Among Blacks and Whites in the United States. American Journal Of Public Health, 96, 826-833. doi:10.2105/AJPH.2004.060749 Grant, K. E., Compas, B. E., Stuhlmacher, A. F., Thurm, A. E., McMahon, S. D., & Halpert, J. A. (2003). Stressors and child and adolescent psychopathology: Moving from markers to mechanisms of risk. Psychological Bulletin, 129, 447-466. doi:
  • 19. SOCIAL SUPPORT AND ADOLESCENT STRESS !19 10.1037/0033-2909.129.3.447 Gunnar, M., & Quevedo, K. (2007). The Neurobiology of Stress and Development. Annual Review Of Psychology, 58, 145-173. doi:10.1146/annurev.psych.58.110405.085605 Heinrichs, M., Baumgartner, T., Kirschbaum, C., & Ehlert, U. (2003). Social support and oxytocin interact to suppress cortisol and subjective responses to psychosocial stress. Biological Psychiatry, 54, 1389-1398. doi:10.1016/S0006-3223(03)00465-7 Jessor, R. (1993). Successful adolescent development among youth in high-risk settings. American Psychologist, 48, 117-126. doi:10.1037/0003-066X.48.2.117 McEwen, B. S. (1998). Stress, adaptation, and disease: Allostasis and allostatic load. In S. M. McCann, J. M. Lipton, E. M. Sternberg, G. P. Chrousos, P. W. Gold, C. C. Smith (Eds.) , Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, Vol. 840: Neuroimmunomodulation: Molecular aspects, integrative systems, and clinical advances (pp. 33-44). New York, NY US: New York Academy of Sciences. McEwen, B. S., & Seeman, T. (1999). Protective and damaging effects of mediators of stress: Elaborating and testing the concepts of allostasis and allostatic load. In N. E. Adler, M. Marmot, B. S. McEwen, J. Stewart (Eds.) , Socioeconomic status and health in industrial nations: Social, psychological, and biological pathways (pp. 30-47). New York, NY US: New York Academy of Sciences. McEwen, B. S. (2000). Allostasis and allostatic load: Implications for neuropsychopharmacology. Neuropsychopharmacology, 22, 108-124. doi:10.1016/ S0893-133X(99)00129-3 Miller, G. E., & Chen, E. (2013). The biological residue of childhood poverty. Child
  • 20. SOCIAL SUPPORT AND ADOLESCENT STRESS !20 Development Perspectives, 7, 67-73. doi:10.1111/cdep.12021 Peters, E., Riksen-Walraven, J., Cillessen, A. N., & de Weerth, C. (2011). Peer rejection and HPA activity in middle childhood: Friendship makes a difference. Child Development, 82, 1906-1920. doi:10.1111/j.1467-8624.2011.01647.x Repetti, R. L., Taylor, S. E., & Seeman, T. E. (2002). Risky families: Family social environments and the mental and physical health of offspring. Psychological Bulletin, 128, 330-366. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.128.2.330 Rigby, K. (2000). Effects of peer victimization in schools and perceived social support on adolescent well-being. Journal of Adolescence, 23, 57-68. doi:10.1006/jado.1999.0289 Rueger, S., Malecki, C., & Demaray, M. (2010). Relationship between Multiple Sources of Perceived Social Support and Psychological and Academic Adjustment in Early Adolescence: Comparisons across Gender. Journal Of Youth And Adolescence, 39, 47-61. doi:10.1007/s10964-008-9368-6 Sameroff, A., Seifer, R., Zax, M., & Barocas, R. (1987). Early Indicators of Developmental Risk: Rochester Longitudinal Study. Schizophrenia Bulletin, 13, 383-394. Sameroff, A. J. (2000). Dialectical processes in developmental psychopathology. In A. J. Sameroff, M. Lewis, S. M. Miller (Eds.) , Handbook of developmental psychopathology (2nd ed.) (pp. 23-40). Dordrecht Netherlands: Kluwer Academic Publishers. doi: 10.1007/978-1-4615-4163-9_2 Sipal, R., & Sayin, U. (2013). Impact of perceived social support and depression on the parental attitudes of mothers of children who are deaf. Journal Of Child And Family Studies, 22, 1103-1111. doi:10.1007/s10826-012-9672-3
  • 21. SOCIAL SUPPORT AND ADOLESCENT STRESS !21 Steinberg, L., & Morris, A. (2001). Adolescent development. Annual Review Of Psychology, 52, 83-110. doi:10.1146/annurev.psych.52.1.83 Taylor, S. E. (2007). Social Support. In H. S. Friedman, R. Silver (Eds.), Foundations of health psychology (pp. 145-171). New York, NY US: Oxford University Press. Uchino, B. N., Cacioppo, J. T., & Kiecolt-Glaser, J. K. (1996). The relationship between social support and physiological processes: A review with emphasis on underlying mechanisms and implications for health. Psychological Bulletin, 119, 488-531. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.119.3.488 Uchino, B. N. (2006). Social Support and Health: A Review of Physiological Processes Potentially Underlying Links to Disease Outcomes. Journal Of Behavioral Medicine, 29, 377-387. doi:10.1007/s10865-006-9056-5 Ulrich-Lai, Y. M., & Herman, J. P. (2009). Neural regulation of endocrine and autonomic stress responses. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 10, 397-409. doi:10.1038/nrn2647 van Beest, M., & Baerveldt, C. (1999). The Relationship Between Adolescents' Social Support From Parents and From Peers. Adolescence, 34, 193-201 Yates, T. M., Dodds, M. F., Sroufe, L., & Egeland, B. (2003). Exposure to partner violence and child behavior problems: A prospective study controlling for child physical abuse and neglect, child cognitive ability, socioeconomic status, and life stress. Development And Psychopathology, 15, 199-218. doi:10.1017/S0954579403000117 !