SlideShare a Scribd company logo
1 of 133
(1) Post a responsive review to someone else's essay paper from
Week #14 (last week) according to the directions that are posted
there
IN THE FOLLOWING ABBREVIATED (!!) WEEK: Each
student will post one review of one of the essay papers that
another student has posted. No late reviews will be accepted
for any reason. Reviews should be substantively interactive,
constructively critical, and at least 35 sentences long. Reviews
should be "spread around" so that everyone's essay paper is
reviewed by at least one other student. Also, reviews
should utilize the FIVE STAR RATING SCHEME feature. The
peer rating feature has to do with the 5 stars as shown here (see
below). Reviews will be figured into the grade for this "Essay
Paper" assignment.
ORIGINAL ARTICLE
Gender Representation on Gender-Targeted Television
Channels:
A Comparison of Female- and Male-Targeted TV
Channels in the Netherlands
Serena Daalmans1 & Mariska Kleemans1 & Anne Sadza1
Published online: 5 January 2017
# The Author(s) 2017. This article is published with open access
at Springerlink.com
Abstract The current study investigated the differences in the
representation of gender on male- and female-targeted channels
with regard to recognition (i.e., the actual presence of men and
women) and respect (i.e., the nature of that representation or
portrayal). To this end, the presence of men and women on two
female- and two male-targeted Dutch channels (N = 115 pro-
grams, N = 1091 persons) were compared via content analysis.
The expectation that men’s channels would portray a less equal
and more traditional image of gender than women’s channels
was generally supported by the results. Regardless of genre as
well as country of origin of the program, women were under-
represented on men’s channels, while gender distribution on
women’s channels was more equal. The representation of wom-
en in terms of age and occupation was more stereotypical on
men’s channels than on women’s channels, whereas men were
represented in more contra-stereotypical ways (e.g., performing
household tasks) on women’s channels. Since television view -
ing contributes to the learning and maintenance of stereotyped
perceptions, the results imply that it is important to strengthen
viewers’ defenses against the effects of gender stereotyping
when watching gendered television channels, for instance
through media literacy programs in schools.
Keywords Gender-targeted channels . Gender stereotyping .
Gender representation . Content analysis . Television
Over the past decades, research has made it abundantly clear
that women are underrepresented in the media and that, when
they are present, they are more often than not represented in
stereotypical roles (Collins 2011; Emons et al. 2010; Furnham
and Paltzer 2010; Lauzen et al. 2008; Signorielli and Bacue
1999). Because the roles of women in society have expanded
tremendously as a result of the ongoing process of emancipa-
tion, these consistent findings are often seen as remarkable
(Collins 2011; Emons et al. 2010; Lauzen et al. 2008;
Signorielli and Bacue 1999). However, recent developments
in the television landscape may provide new insights on the
issue. There are indications that specific gender-targeted
genres (e.g., soaps and teen scene) might actually showcase
both a more equal distribution of men and women as well as
less stereotyping in its gender portrayals (Gerding and
Signorielli 2014; Lauzen et al. 2006). Following on from this
speculation, the emergence of channels that specifically define
men or women as their target group and thus predominantly
broadcast gender-targeted genres (also called narrowcasting,
Kuipers 2012; Smith-Shomade 2004) might be a promising
development with regard to a more representative portrayal of
both men and women. However, gender portrayal on such
gendered channels is rather unexplored, and is therefore cen-
tral to the current study.
Narrowcasting, or organizing user groups into specific au-
dience markets, started from the 1980s onward in the United
States (Kuipers 2012; Lotz 2006). It meant that specific, often
gendered, audience groups were targeted via specific pro-
gramming and advertising content (Sheperd 2014).
Following this pattern, we term narrowcasting on channels
that explicitly target either a female or a male audience
Bgender-targeted channels^ here. Gender-targeted television
channels employing narrowcasting currently form an increas-
ingly large portion of the television landscape in a multitude of
countries (Kuipers 2012; Smith-Shomade 2004; Van Bauwel
* Serena Daalmans
[email protected]
1 Behavioural Science Institute, Communication Science,
Radboud
University, Postbus 9104, 6500, HE Nijmegen, The Netherlands
Sex Roles (2017) 77:366–378
DOI 10.1007/s11199-016-0727-6
http://crossmark.crossref.org/dialog/?doi=10.1007/s11199-016-
0727-6&domain=pdf
2016). Previous research into narrowcasting has, on the one
hand, focused on the emergence of female-targeted channels,
such as Lifetime and Oxygen, and its programming strategies,
content choices, and rhetoric used to win female audiences
and advertisers of female products (Byars and Meehan 1994;
Hundley 2002; Lotz 2006; Meehan and Byars 2000; Tankel
and Banks 1997). On the other hand, researchers studied
whether specific gender-targeted programs such as Sex and
The City and Ally McBeal can be considered (post-) feminist
texts (Akass and McCabe 2004; Dubrofsky 2002). What re-
mains unclear based on previous research is how the gendered
focus of such channels and of programs suitable to broadcast
on gender-targeted channels affect the representation of men
and women in terms of presence and stereotyping.
Investigation of the portrayal of men and women in pro-
grams broadcast at gender-targeted channels becomes urgent
when considering statements by television scholars who label
gender-targeting or narrowcasting as a hegemonic practice
(Meehan 1990; Sheperd 2014; Smith-Shomade 2004).
Moreover, studying gender representation remains of the ut-
most importance because watching television still is the most
time-consuming pastime (Collins 2011; Signorielli 2012). As
a result, television is seen as one of the main institutions as -
sociated with disseminating stereotyped views of the world
and its gender roles. From the theoretical vantage point of
cultivation theory as well as social learning theory, television
is confirmed as one of the main agents of socialization
(Bandura 1977; Gerbner 1979; Signorielli 2012). Research
has shown that television viewing contributes to the mainte-
nance as well as the learning (molding) of gender stereotyped
perceptions among children, adolescents, and adults (Larson
2001; McGhee and Frueh 1980; Signorielli 1989; Welch et al.
1979). Furthermore, it is generally accepted that television
impacts gender socialization in people’s self-image as well
as their image of others (Signorielli 2012). Assuming that
television has the potential to shape attitudes, self-perception,
and behavior—on top of the idea that stereotyping plays a
crucial role in the maintenance of power inequalities within
wider social, cultural, political, and economic structures
(Cottle 2000; Dyer 1993, 1997; Morgan 2007)—it becomes
important to analyze and understand the nature of gender-role
portrayals on gender-targeted channels.
Previous research on gender stereotyping in media has gen-
erally focused on two levels of gender stereotyping. The first
level of gender stereotyping includes the actual presence of
men and women in television programs (whether they ap-
pear), termed recognition. The second level focuses on the
nature of that representation or portrayal (how they appear),
termed respect (Collins 2011; Signorielli and Bacue 1999).
Research into narrowcasting remains inconclusive about
whether the representation of men and women on gender-
targeted channels differs from the overall established patterns
of underrepresentation of women (i.e., recognition) and
stereotyping portrayal of both men and women (i.e, respect).
Moreover, known previous research into narrowcasting has
only studied female-targeted channels (Byars and Meehan
1994; Hundley 2002; Lotz 2006; Meehan and Byars 2000;
Tankel and Banks 1997), but a comparison between the gen-
der representation on male-targeted and female-targeted chan-
nels is lacking. This comparison is important because, if these
gender-targeted channels differ in how they represent gender,
this might lead to differing conceptions of gender roles and
gender aspirations between their male and female target audi -
ences (Gerding and Signorielli 2014).
In all, the question that is central to the current study is: What
are the differences in the representation of gender on male- and
female-targeted Dutch channels with regard to the characteriza-
tion concepts of recognition and respect? This question will be
investigated by analyzing gender-targeted television channels in
the Netherlands, a country in which 40% of the television land-
scape currently explicitly targets a gendered target audience
(Stichting Kijkonderzoek [SKO] 2014).
In their longitudinal analysis of gender on television,
Signorielli and Bacue (1999) use presence and gender-role
stereotyping as indicators of recognition and respect respec -
tively. These concepts stem from a study of televised racial
minorities by Clark (1972), who posited that positive changes
in the treatment of lower status groups (minorities including
women) can be seen as a process that follows two stages:
recognition and respect. In gender research, recognition can
be seen as the presence of women on the television screen
(Lauzen and Dozier 2005; Signorielli and Bacue 1999).
Respect is measured by the extent toward which women are
not portrayed in stereotypical roles, but rather represented in a
diverse manner because this diversity is necessary to be rep-
resented fairly and positively (Signorielli and Bacue 1999).
Recognition: Presence of Men and Women
on Television
Recognition is defined as men and women being represented on
television proportional to their presence in society (Signorielli
and Bacue 1999). Research into gender representation has over -
whelmingly found that women are underrepresented on televi-
sion compared to their presence in society (Collins 2011; Emons
et al. 2010; Glascock 2001; Greenberg and Atkin 1980;
Koeman et al. 2007; Signorielli and Bacue 1999; Tedesco
1974). The last decades have shown a trend towards a more
equal distribution of male and female characters (Elasmar
et al. 1999; Emons et al. 2010; Glascock 2001; Greenberg and
Collette 1997; Lauzen and Dozier 2005; Vande Berg and
Streckfuss 1992), but increases are often small and women re-
main underrepresented (Koeman et al. 2007; Segijn et al. 2014).
There are some indications that levels of over- or under-
representation are connected to the gender of the target
Sex Roles (2017) 77:366–378 367
audience. Gunter (1986) found that soap operas in 1970s that
were specifically geared toward a female audience had an
equal distribution of male and female characters. Gerding
and Signorielli (2014) similarly found that whereas females
were underrepresented in ‘tween programs geared towards
male adolescents, the programs geared towards female ado-
lescents mirrored the U.S. population. Based on previous re-
sults we posit that women will be underrepresented compared
to their presence in society on men’s channels, but not on
women’s channels (Hypothesis 1).
Previous studies have revealed that television genres can
differ in the ways they represent gender. It has been shown that
gender stereotyping is surely still present, but has generally re -
vealed a trend of decreasing stereotyping in genres such as tel e-
vision fiction (Emons et al. 2010; Greenwood and Lippman
2010; Gunter 1986; Signorielli and Bacue 1999), teen scene
programming (Gerding and Signorielli 2014), and advertising
(Wolin 2003). Moreover, the portrayal of women in these genres
has become more representative of the lives and status of con-
temporary women. In contrast, other studies—mostly on televi-
sion advertising, but also on fictional programs—conclude that
women are still underrepresented and portrayed in a stereotyp-
ical way and that the degree of stereotyping is even worsening
(Allen and Coltrane 1996; Bretl and Cantor 1988; Ganahl et al.
2003; Harwood and Anderson 2002; Koeman et al. 2007;
Milner and Higgs 2004). Based on the latter results, some have
argued that, due to its continued gender-stereotyped nature,
tele-
vision forms a lagging social indicator, which reflects Bhow the
economy or society was rather than how it is or how it will be^
(Estes 2003, p.4; see also Emons 2011; Kim and Lowry 2005).
Taken together these results reveal that there is inconsisten-
cy in the literature about the relation between genre and gen-
der-stereotyping. These possible genre differences become
relevant and important when combined with the increased
attention from a cultivation perspective that has been given
to the possibility of genre-specific cultivation effects
(Bilandzic and Busselle 2008; Bilandzic and Rössler 2004;
Cohen and Weimann 2000; Grabe and Drew 2007; Morgan
and Shanahan 2010). Following the idea put forth by Hawkins
and Pingree (1981) that different TV genres may cultivate
different views of the world, research has revealed large dif-
ferences between genres (Gomes and Williams 1990; Koeman
et al. 2007; Pennekamp 2011). Because men’s and women’s
channels cater to different expected audiences most likely
with a selection of different gendered genres, this might influ-
ence the gender representation on these channels as a whole.
This then leads to the following research question: Do genre-
differences play a role in the presence of men and women on
gender-targeted television channels? (Research Question 1).
The country of origin of the selected programming might
also play a role in gender representational differences between
channel types. Previous research has revealed that country of
origin plays a significant role in the degree of stereotyping
present in the representation of gender (Emons et al. 2010;
Furnham and Paltzer 2010; Wiles et al. 1995). For example,
Emons et al. (2010) found that U.S. programs on Dutch tele-
vision represented more male adults, more women involved in
childcare, more men involved in a job, and fewer males in-
volved in other activities compared to Dutch programs on the
same channels. Based on this comparison, they conclude that
American programs on Dutch television are more gender-
stereotyped than programs of Dutch origin. Their research
indicates that gender portrayals on television can be artifacts
of the culture of the society they were created and thereby
potentially reflective of the degree of gender equality in the
culture of origin. Because the Dutch television landscape
hosts a large degree of foreign (especially American) pro-
gramming, it becomes interesting to see how this affects gen-
der representation on Dutch gendered channels (Kuipers
2008, 2011). We therefore pose the following research ques-
tion: Do differences in country of origin of the program play a
role in the presence of men and women on gender-targeted
television channels? (Research Question 2).
Respect: Stereotyping in Gender Representation
Analyzing the presence of women in the television world is only
a relatively small aspect the representation of women on TV.
BA
more complete understanding of how women are portrayed on
television comes from examining the type and depth of the roles
in which they are cast – what Clark (1972) referred to as
respect^ (Signorielli and Bacue 1999, p. 530). We therefore also
investigate how women appear in television programs.
The first indicator of respect analyzed in the present study
is age. Studies have found that inequalities in the age of tele-
vised men and women persist because women are continually
represented as younger than their male counterparts are
(Emons et al. 2010; Glascock 2001; Signorielli and Bacue
1999). The fact that men on television are predominantly rep-
resented as older and therewith are perceived to be wiser than
their female counterparts can be interpreted as women being
given less respect (Signorielli and Bacue 1999).
Furthermore, previous research on gender-targeted children’s
programming has revealed that programs geared toward a boy
audience often showcased rather traditional gender-biased por-
trayals, whereas programs geared toward a girl audience less
often featured gender-stereotypical roles and sometimes even
showcased counter-stereotypical roles (Banet-Weiser 2004;
Gerding and Signorielli 2014; Leaper et al. 2002; Northup and
Liebler 2010; Thompson and Zerbinos 1995). Taken together
with previous research on age as an indicator of respect (Emons
et al. 2010; Glascock 2001; Signorielli and Bacue 1999), we
expect a more traditional image on men’s channels than on
women’s channels. This leads to the expectations that a larger
percentage of women will be young adults on men’s channels
368 Sex Roles (2017) 77:366–378
than on women’s channels (Hypothesis 2a) and a larger percent-
age of men will be adults on men’s channels than on women’s
channels (Hypothesis 2b).
The second aspect of respect that we will analyze focuses on
the social roles in which men and women are cast (Emons et al.
2010; Gerbner 1995; Greenberg and Atkin 1980; Gunter 1986;
Signorielli and Bacue 1999). Studies showed that televised men
are more likely to be cast in occupational roles, whereas tele -
vised women are more likely to be cast in nurturing or marital
roles (Gunter 1986; Lauzen et al. 2008; Signorielli and Bacue
1999; Tedesco 1974). Televised women are traditionally repre -
sented as housewives who perform housekeeping chores and
are preoccupied with family life (Emons et al. 2010; Gunter
1986; Koeman et al. 2007; Signorielli and Bacue 1999). The
results on gendered representation of social roles, combined
with the previously outlined results regarding the predominance
of traditionally gender-stereotyped portrayals in programming
geared towards boys and a more gender-balanced representation
in programming geared towards girls (Banet-Weiser 2004;
Gerding and Signorielli 2014; Leaper et al. 2002; Northup and
Liebler 2010; Thompson and Zerbinos 1995), leads us to expect
that women on men’s channels will be represented performing
household/caregiving tasks more often than women on
women’s channels (Hypothesis 3a) and men on men’s channels
will be represented performing household/caregiving tasks less
often than men on women’s channels (Hypothesis 3b).
In line with this reasoning, studies have found women to be
underrepresented with regard to being professionally employed
(Elasmar et al. 1999; Gunter 1986; Coltrane and Adams 1997;
McNeil 1975; Signorielli 1989; Signorielli and Bacue 1999),
which is the next indicator for respect that we will analyze.
Over time, there appears to be an increase of women
represented
as having a job and a decrease of men represented as have a job
(Signorielli and Bacue 1999). Again, however, this trend is not
straightforward because the occupational status of men and
women on television tends to fluctuate (Emons et al. 2010).
Nevertheless, due to the expectedly more gender-traditional rep-
resentation on men’s channels (Banet-Weiser 2004; Gerding
and Signorielli 2014; Leaper et al. 2002; Northup and Liebler
2010; Thompson and Zerbinos 1995), we predict that women
will be granted less respect on men’s channels in terms of oc-
cupational status. Thus we hypothesize that women on men’s
channels will be represented as being professionally employed
less often than women on women’s channels (Hypothesis 4a)
and that men on men’s channels will be represented as being
professionally employed more often than men on women’s
channels (Hypothesis 4b).
The suggestion that family life and parenthood are of greater
significance to women than to men is also implicit in the fact
that parental status is more often made explicit for women than
for men (Davis 1990; Emons et al. 2010; Glascock 2001;
Gunter 1986; McNeil 1975). The final indicator of respect that
we will analyze, therefore, is parental status. Again there is a
trend toward a more egalitarian distribution of known parental
status for characters over time. The percentages of known pa-
rental status decreased from 81% for women and 54% for men
in McNeil’s (1975) study to 56% for women and 42% for men
in Glascock’s (2001) study. Again, a more traditional represen-
tation is expected to be more prevalent on men’s channels than
on women’s channels (cf. Banet-Weiser 2004; Gerding and
Signorielli 2014; Leaper et al. 2002; Northup and Liebler
2010; Thompson and Zerbinos 1995). We therefore expect
women on men’s channels to be represented as mothers more
often than women on women’s channels will be (Hypothesis 5a)
and men on men’s channels to be represented as fathers less
often than men on women’s channels will be (Hypothesis 5b).
Combined, these hypotheses test differences in the levels of
recognition and respect given to men and women on men’s
and women’s television channels by testing differences in
terms of stereotypical or counter-stereotypical role portrayals
between the two channel types, comparable to what Gerding
and Signorielli (2014) did in their study on ‘tween programs.
As we argued, these differences may impact the ideas that men
and women have about what are acceptable gender-role pat-
terns and what to expect from oneself and others.
Method
To test our hypotheses, we conducted a quantitative content
analysis of gender-role portrayals of 1091 characters in tele-
vision 115 programs aired during primetime on four gender-
specific television channels in the Netherlands in 2014. All
four gendered cable channels on the Dutch television were
analyzed: RTL7 and Veronica as men’s channels and RTL8
and Net5 as women’s channels. This distinction was made
clear in the explicit statements on their marketing pages as
well as their slogans broadcasted during their televised com-
mercial breaks (e.g., BEverything women love,^ BWhat wom-
en want,^ BRTL7 knows what men want,^ and BMore for
men^). Examples of programs on women’s channels are 15
Kids and Counting and Sex and The City; men’s channels
broadcast, for instance, Top Gear and programs about soccer.
All included channels are commercial broadcasters because
Dutch public service broadcasters do not identify specific tar-
get audience but are (by law) aimed at informing and enter-
taining the general population (Koeman et al. 2007).
A check of the validity of the distinction in gendered chan-
nels was conducted by verifying the audience profiles, based
on gendered viewer ratings, for each channel (Stichting
Kijkonderzoek [SKO] 2014, p. 39). The ratings revealed that
the audience profile of the male channels RTL 7 and Veronica
is indeed male-dominated, with respectively 64.0% and
55.1% of the viewers in 2014 being male. The women’s chan-
nels RTL8 and Net5 were shown to have a more female audi -
ence. Women represented 64.2% of the audience watching
Sex Roles (2017) 77:366–378 369
RTL8, and 63.5% of the audience watching Net5. In terms of
market share, the male channels RTL7 (5.0%) and Veronica
(4.4%) had a combined market share of just below 10%,
whereas the women’s channels RTL8 (2.4%) and Net5
(3.5%) had a combined market share of just below 6%
(Stichting Kijkonderzoek [SKO] 2014).
Sample
For each of the four channels, five primetime evenings
(6 PM–midnight) of broadcasting were coded and analyzed.
The evenings were recorded as part of a larger clustered sam-
ple of several constructed weeks and the channels were there-
fore recorded on consecutive days over the course of several
weeks (29 March 2014–12 May 2014). This type of sampling
leads to more representative results than recording an actual
week (Riffe et al. 2005) because television channels some-
times have thematic weeks that may not be representative of
year-long programming. All programs were analyzed but only
if the complete episode was aired within the timeframe of the
sample. The context unit of our study was one episode, there-
fore only information that was shown in the specific episode
was used for coding.
A total number of 56 programs (48.3%) were broadcasted
on a women’s channel: NET5 (26, 22.6%) and RTL8 (30,
26.1%), and 59 programs (51.7%) were broadcasted on a
men’s channel: RTL7 (24, 20.9%) and Veronica (35,
30.4%). From the total of 1091 characters that were present
in the programs, 597 (54.7%) were presented on men’s chan-
nels; 494 (45.3%), on women’s channels.
Recording Units and Coding Procedure
The two channel types constitute the units of analysis for our
study. Recording units were storylines (for fictional programs)
and items (for entertainment and reality programs) within pro-
grams and the main characters or persons who were featured in
them. Entertainment and reality as a genre contained programs,
such as Masterchef and Top Gear, whereas fiction as a genre
contained both comedic fiction, such as Two and a Half Men
and Mike & Molly, and dramatic fiction, such as Criminal
Minds
and The Bold and the Beautiful. The women’s channels sample
consisted of 56 programs, of which 34 (60.7%) were fictional
and 22 (39.3%) were entertainment and reality. The men’s chan-
nels sample consisted of 59 programs, of which 30 (50.9%)
were fictional and 29 (49.1%) were entertainment and reality.
Coding initially differentiated between fictional and non-
fictional programming, of which the latter included the genres
News and Information and Entertainment and Reality.
However, the results revealed that there were actually no pro-
grams in the sample which belonged to the News and
Information genre.
Based on Emons et al. (2010) up to ten items or storylines,
each with up to eight persons, were coded. For fiction these
eight
characters were selected by coding only main characters in the
episode. A main character was defined as a character who plays
a
leading role in the narrative and whose choices and behavior
were essential for the development of the plot (Egri 1960;
Lauzen and Dozier 2005; Weijers 2014). In entertainment and
reality programs, the eight persons who had the most speaki ng
and/or screen time per item were coded. Show hosts and re-
porters were coded but excluded from the analyses because they
differed from the rest of the population as a result of their func -
tion rather than their gender or the channel type on which they
were portrayed (i.e., every show host always has an occupation;
marital and parental status are very rarely made explicit).
Moreover, only a minority of the initial sample consisted of
these
types of characters (n = 50). In addition, animated persons from
three program broadcast on men’s channels were also excluded.
The coding instrument used to analyze the persons and
characters in the sample (see Table 1 for coding definitions,
categories, and frequencies) was developed using prior studies
of prime-time television (Davis 1990; Elasmar et al. 1999;
Emons et al. 2010; Glascock 2001; Greenberg and Atkin
1980; Koeman et al. 2007; Signorielli and Bacue 1999).
Two coders were involved in the coding process. Coders re-
ceived coder training and independent practice on programs
that were not part of the sample. The coders independently
coded practice materials and then compared and discussed the
results. The coding instrument was edited after these discus-
sions to fix potential problems prior to coding and analysis.
After the final revisions in the coding instrument, a little over
10% of the program sample (n = 14; 12.1%) was randomly
selected to be double coded.
Intercoder reliabilities were calculated in SPSS using the
macro by Hayes for Krippendorff’s alpha (Hayes and
Krippendorff 2007). All variables were analyzed as nominal
variables except for age, which was analyzed as an ordinal
variable. The cut-off points were defined based on Lombard
et al. (2002) who suggest that Krippendorff’s alpha coeffi -
cients of .90 or higher are always acceptable and .80 or higher
are acceptable in most situations. As reported in Table 1,
intercoder reliabilities are acceptable for all variables
(Kalphas > .87).
Results
Presence as an Indicator of Recognition
The first hypothesis stated that women would be underrepre-
sented on men’s channels, but not on women’s channels, com-
pared to their presence in (Dutch) society (Hypothesis 1). In
2014, 49.5% of the Dutch population (N = 16,829,289) was
male whereas 50.5% of the population was female (Centraal
370 Sex Roles (2017) 77:366–378
Bureau voor de Statistiek 2014). The gender distribution on
men’s and women’s channels was examined with a Chi square
goodness of fit test whereby the societal percentage was used
as the expected value. The results showed that women were
significantly underrepresented on men’s channels compared
to their presence in society. Women made up only 22% of
the cast, whereas men (78%) were much more present com-
pared to the gender distribution in society, χ2(1,
n = 597) = 192.48, p < .001, Cramer’s V = .57. In contrast,
women (and men) on women’s channels were not underrep-
resented. Their presence did not differ significantly from so-
ciety in terms of gender. Women composed 48.6% of the
population whereas men composed 51.4%, χ2(1,
n = 494) = .73, p = .394, Cramer’s V = .04. In all, the results
provide support for Hypothesis 1 because underrepresentation
of women was only found on men’s channels.
The first research question asked if the genre of the pro-
grams presented on men’s and women’s channels would lead
to a differing presence of men and women on these channels.
The results revealed that, based on the adjusted residuals on
men’s channels, the percentage of men significantly exceeded
expected frequencies within the entertainment and reality
genre (87.9%, adjusted residuals =8.1), whereas the percent-
ages for women (although still underrepresented) significantly
exceeded expected frequencies within the fictional genre on
men’s channels (40.1%, adjusted residuals =8.1), χ2(1,
n = 597) = 65.20, p < .001, Cramer’s V = .33. In contrast,
on women’s channels the percentage of women significantly
exceeded expected frequencies within the entertainment and
reality genre (54.8%, adjusted residuals =2.4), whereas the
percentage of men (55.9%, adjusted residuals = 2.4) signifi -
cantly exceeded expected frequencies in fictional genre, χ2(1,
n = 494) = 5.57, p = .018, Cramer’s V = .11. In all, there is a
more pronounced difference in the representation of gender on
men’s channels in different genres than on women’s channels,
where gender is more evenly divided.
The second research question asked if country of origin of
the programs presented on men’s and women’s channels
would lead to a differing presence of men and women on these
channels. Overall, results showed that women were underrep-
resented in programming from all countries. However, on
men’s channels in programs created in the United States, the
percentage of women significantly exceeded expected fre-
quencies (34%, adjusted residuals =8.0) with the percentage
of men (66.0%) falling below expected rates, χ2(2,
n = 597) = 64.85, p < .001, Cramer’s V = .33.
Comparatively, men are starkly overrepresented in program-
ming from both the Netherlands (95.2%, adjusted residuals
= 5.8) and Other countries (91.1%, adjusted residuals =3.7),
with the percentage of women falling below expected rates
(NL: 4.8%, Other: 8.9%). In contrast, on women’s channels
there were no significant differences between countries of
origin in the presence of men and women, χ2(2,
n = 494) = 4.35, p = .114, Cramer’s V = .10.
These results prompted a closer inspection of the particular
programming originating in the Netherlands and Other coun-
tries in the sample. Programs on men’s channels that were
created in The Netherlands were predominantly sports talk
shows (n = 4) and reality crime shows (n = 5). We need to
note here that Dutch programs constitute only 17.9% of the
Table 1 Overview of coded variables
Variable Definition Categories Kalpha Frequencies
n (%)
Genre Genre to which the program belongs
(N = 115)
Entertainment and Reality Fiction 1.00 52 (45%)
63 (55%)
Country of origina Country of origin of the program
(N = 115)
The Netherlands
United States,
Great-Britain / Other
1.00 22 (20%)
85 (73%)
4 (3.5%) / 4 (3.5%)
Gender Person or character’s gender
(N = 1091)
Male
Female
.98 720 (66%)
371 (34%)
Age Person or character’s age in terms of the life cycle
(N = 1091)
Child (0–12)
Teenager (13–18)
Young adult (19–34)
Adult (35–49)
Middle-aged (50–64)
Senior (65+)
.87 21 (1.9%)
45 (4.1%)
408 (37.4%)
455 (41.7%)
116 (10.6%)
46 (4.2%)
Household and caregiving tasks Whether the person or character
engages in household
or caregiving tasks (for example: cleaning, doing
the laundry, or taking children to school)
(N = 1091)
Yes
No
.93 71 (6.5%)
1020 (93.5%)
Employment Whether the person or character is portrayed as
being
professionally employed or explicitly mentions
having a job
(N = 1091)
Yes
No
Unknown
.96 681 (62.4%)
20 (1.8%)
390 (35.7%)
Parental status Whether the person or character is portrayed as a
parent
or explicitly mentions being a parent
(N = 1091)
Yes
No
Unknown
.96 147 (13.7%)
19 (1.8%)
905 (84.5%).
a In the analyses, the categories of BGreat-Britain^ and BOther^
were combined due to low cell frequencies
Sex Roles (2017) 77:366–378 371
programming on men’s channels. All other programs were
originally from United States (71.4%) or from Great Britain
and other countries (10.7%). Programs on men’s channels
from other countries were all male-oriented shows such as
Top Gear, the famous BBC car talk show. Comparatively,
the programs aired on men’s channels originating in the
United States are a little more mixed, consisting of for exam-
ple the reality crime show Cops as well as comedy shows such
as Mike & Molly and drama such as Criminal Minds.
Age as Indicator of Respect
The second set of hypotheses stated that women would be
represented as young adults on men’s channels more often
than on women’s channels (Hypothesis 2a), whereas men
would be portrayed as adults on men’s channels more often
than on women’s channels (Hypothesis 2b). The distribution
of women along age categories differed significantly between
channel types, χ2 (5, n = 372) = 13.70, p = .018, Cramer’s
V = .19 (see Table 2a). In accordance with hypothesis 2a, the
residual analysis revealed that on men’s channels over half of
all women (56.8%, adjusted residuals =2.7) were portrayed as
young adults which was significantly more than the 42.1% on
women’s channels. As predicted in Hypothesis 2b, 49.2% of
men (adjusted residuals =2.9) were represented as adults on
men’s channels, and this was also significantly more than the
37.8% on women’s channels, χ2(5, n = 719) = 19.04, p = .002,
Cramer’s V = .16. Although not hypothesized, these results
also revealed that adult women (40.0%, adjusted residuals
=2.8) were significantly more present on women’s channels
than on men’s channels (25.8%).
As the percentages above already indicate, most of main
characters in the sample were either young adults or adults.
The other age categories that we discerned (child, teenager,
middle-aged adults, and seniors) appeared less frequent (12.3–
14.6% for middle aged men, all other percentages <7.5%, see
Table 2a). No notable differences in presence of men and
women of these age categories on either men’s or women’s
channels were found, except for significantly more male chil -
dren on women’s channels (3.9%, adjusted residuals = 3.5)
than on men’s channels (.4%).
Tasks as Indicators of Respect
We expected that women on men’s channels would be over-
represented performing household or caregiving tasks com-
pared to women on women’s channels (H3a). We did not find
support for this hypothesis, since 12.1% of women on men’s
channels and 11.7% of women on women’s channels per-
formed these tasks and these numbers did not differ signifi -
cantly, χ2(1, n = 372) = .017, Cramer’s V = .007, p = .897 (see
Table 2b).
It was further hypothesized that men on men’s channels
would be significantly underrepresented performing house-
hold or caregiving tasks compared to men on women’s chan-
nels (Hypothesis 3b). The results supported this hypothesis as
Table 2 Indicators of respect for
women and men on gender-
targeted channels
Represented women Represented men
Indicators Men’s channels Women’s channels Men’s channels
Women’s channels
(a) Age
Child 3.8% 1.7% .4% 3.9%*
Teenager 4.5% 4.2% 3.7% 4.7%
Young adult 56.8%* 42.1% 31.0% 34.6%
Adult 25.8% 40.0%* 49.2%* 37.8%
Middle aged 3.0% 7.5% 12.3% 14.6%
Senior 6.1% 4.6% 3.4% 4.3%
n 132 240 465 254
(b) Household and caregiving tasks
Yes 12.1% 11.7% 1.5% 7.9%*
No 87.9% 88.3% 98.5%* 92.1%
n 132 240 465 254
(c) Employment
Yes 43.2% 57.1%* 64.9% 72.8%*
No 3.0% 4.6% .4% 1.2%
Unknown 53.8%* 38.3% 34.3%* 26.0%
n 132 240 465 254
(d) Parental status
Yes 19.7% 23.8% 5.4% 15.4%*
No .8% 5.4%* .2% 1.6%*
Unknown 79.5% 70.8% 94.4%* 83.1%
n 132 240 465 254
* Frequency significantly exceeded expectations by adjusted
standardized residuals
372 Sex Roles (2017) 77:366–378
7.9% of men on women’s channels performed household or
caregiving tasks (adjusted residuals =4.3) compared to only
1.5% of men on men’s channels, χ2(1, n = 719) = 18.44,
p < .001, Cramer’s V = .16 (see Table 2b).
Employment as Indicator of Respect
Women on men’s channels were expected to be represented as
having an occupation less often than women on women’s
channels (Hypothesis 4a), whereas men on men’s channels
were expected to be represented as having an occupation more
often than men on women’s channels (Hypothesis 4b). A sig-
nificantly larger percentage of women (57.1%, adjusted resid-
uals = 2.6) was indeed portrayed as having an occupation on
women’s channels than on men’s channels (43.2%), χ2 (2,
n = 372) = 8.31, p = .016, Cramer’s V = .15 (see Table 2c).
This finding confirms Hypothesis 4a. Contrary to Hypothesis
4b, men on men’s channels were portrayed as having an oc-
cupation significantly less often than men on women’s chan-
nels were (see Table 2c). On men’s channels, 64.9% (adj usted
residuals = − 2.6) of the men were portrayed as having an
occupation compared to 72.8% of men on women’s channels,
χ2(2, n = 719) = 6.75, p = .035, Cramer’s V = .10.
Furthermore, the results also revealed a significant overrepre-
sentation within the unknown category of occupation for both
women (53.8%, adjusted residuals = 2.9) and men (34.6%,
adjusted residuals = 2.4) on men’s channels.
Parental Status as Indicator of Respect
The final set of hypotheses predicted that women on men’s
channels would be represented as mothers more often than
women on women’s channels (Hypothesis 5a) and, converse-
ly, that men on men’s channels would be represented as fathers
less often than men on women’s channels (Hypothesis 5b).
The results revealed that women’s parental state significantly
differed between the channel types, χ2(2, n = 372) = 6.41,
p = .040, Cramer’s V = .13 (see Table 2d). However, the
residual analysis shows that this difference is due to the
knowledge of explicit childlessness of women between chan-
nel types, where women were significantly more often explic-
itly childless on women’s channels (5.4%, adjusted residuals
= 2.3) than on men’s channels (.8%). There was no significant
difference between the representation of women as mother
between men’s channels (19.7%) and women’s channels
(23.8%). We thus need to reject Hypothesis 5a. The results
also indicated that men were, as expected in Hypothesis 5b,
significantly more often represented as fathers (15.4%, adjust-
ed residuals = 4.5) on women’s channels than on men’s chan-
nels (5.4%), χ2(2, n = 719) = 25.08, p < .001, Cramer’s V = .19
(see Table 2d). Hypothesis 5b is thus supported.
Discussion
The primary goal of our study was to provide insight into the
way
that gender was represented on television channels targeting ei-
ther men or women. The overarching expectation that, due to
the
target audience, men’s channels would portray a less equal and
more traditional image of gender than women’s channels would
in terms of both recognition and respect was generally
supported
by our results. In line with the results of Gerding and
Signorielli
(2014), our study revealed that whereas women were grossly
underrepresented on men’s channels, gender distribution on
women’s channels mirrored the Dutch population. This phenom-
enon cannot be explained by the idea that audiences would
prefer
watching members of their own gender because women should
then have been overrepresented on women’s channels just like
men are on men’s channels. Because this was not the case, we
can conclude that in contrast to women’s channels, men’s chan-
nels show a lack of recognition for women. Moreover, an explo-
ration of the level of recognition per genre and per country of
origin of the programs on the gendered channels also revealed
that women’s channels, regardless of genre as well as the
country
of origin of the program, showcased a more equal presence of
men and women than men’s channels did. In all, this means that
only women’s channels fulfill the first of the two stages towards
positive and fair treatment of women: recognition (Clark 1972;
Lauzen and Dozier 2005; Signorielli and Bacue 1999).
In addition, our results revealed that men’s channels also
lagged behind in terms of the second stage of positive and
equal portrayal (Clark 1972): respect. First, the stereotypical
value of youth for women (as an indicator of a lack of respect,
see also Davis 1990; Emons et al. 2010; Lauzen and Dozier
2005; Signorielli and Bacue 1999) is much more pronounced
on men’s channels than it is on women’s channels. Women on
women’s channels were not only represented as older than
they were on men’s channels, but they were also distributed
more evenly over the age categories of young adult and adult.
Second, in terms of occupational status, women on women’s
channels were more likely to be portrayed as being profession-
ally employed than women on men’s channels were.
However, the results for the other indicators of respect re-
vealed a less straightforward conclusion. As for the percentage
of women performing household or caregiving tasks and the
overrepresentation of women as parents, there were no signifi -
cant differences between men’s and women’s channels.
However, it should be noted that women’s channels were more
likely than men’s channels were to represent men as performing
household and caregiving tasks and being parents. Household
and caregiving tasks especially were divided rather equally be -
tween both genders—in contrast to the results by Emons et al.
(2010). Therefore we conclude that at least the degree of gender
stereotyping (at least in terms of these indicators) is lower on
women’s channels and that they thereby still grant more respect
to women and men than men’s channels do.
Sex Roles (2017) 77:366–378 373
The most striking result found in our study is that it is the
representation of men (rather than women) in which the true
differences between men’s and women’s channels are found.
Men are represented in counter-stereotypical ways on women’s
channels and become connected to the home and family almost
as much as women. This focus on home and family life on
women’s channels could be interpreted as a post-feminist resur-
gence of the focus on gender differences and stereotypical fem-
inine values on women’s channels (Gill 2007; McRobbie 2009).
Another, more positive, interpretation might, however, be that
the private domain is being re-evaluated. Women’s channels
seem to present an image in which home and family life are
important—for both men and women. This might be interpreted
as the private domain itself increasingly gaining respect on
these
channels. Also, besides their connection to family life, high
percentages of both men and women are represented as being
professionally employed on these channels, resulting in a more
diverse depiction of both genders.
Limitations and Directions for Future Research
Although the findings here are of clear importance in adding
to our understandings of the differences in gender representa-
tion on gender-targeted channels, there are some limitations to
our study that should be addressed. First, the sample consisted
of a constructed week recorded on consecutive days.
Therewith, we did not control for seasonal differences in pro-
gramming. Recording several weeks, one in each season may
contribute to the representativeness of the results.
Second, in terms of variables we decided to code as many
of the aspects that were associated with the central concepts of
recognition and respect as discerned by Clark (1972) and
Signorielli and Bacue (1999). To this end, we closely followed
previous codebooks (Davis 1990; Elasmar et al. 1999; Emons
et al. 2010; Glascock 2001; Greenberg and Atkin 1980;
Koeman et al. 2007; Signorielli and Bacue 1999) in the
operationalization of our variables. As a consequence, we
were not able to explore every one of the aspects in an in-
depth manner. For example, household and caregiving tasks
were coded as either being performed or not performed.
Because of this, if one character was portrayed performing
household tasks for an entire episode and another performed
only one such task, both were coded as performing these tasks
in the same way. It is recommended that future research would
measure the number of tasks performed by each character so
as to gain more insight into gendered task distribution.
Furthermore, with regard to the variable of occupation, the
coding based on previous work relatively simplistically
reflected
if a person had a job or not (or if it was unknown). Even though
the differentiation between having a job or not captures implicit
messages of gendered worth in the public sphere, future
research
should add a dimension of gender-role stereotyping to this vari-
able in building on previous work (Coltrane and Adams 1997;
Glascock 2001; Signorielli 1989; Signorielli and Bacue 1999;
Signorielli and Kahlenberg 2001). Future research could
establish
for gendered channels if there are differences between men and
women with regard to the type of occupation, the prestige asso-
ciated with that occupation, as well as the authority they have
over other workers in that occupation in order to determine how
stereotypical the representation of work for men and women is
on
these channels. This is important because media portrayals pro-
vide one of the sources of information against which people in
modern societies give meaning to in their work and family lives
(Coltrane and Adams 1997; Signorielli and Kahlenberg 2001).
Seen in this light, the media’s previously documented tendency
of linking men with jobs in which they have authority and wom-
en with jobs with less prestige and less authority over other
workers (Coltrane and Adams 1997; Glascock 2001; Signorielli
1989), potentially aids in the perpetuation of gender stereotypes
and the maintenance of the gendered status quo.
Third, we decided to exclude show hosts and reporters in
entertainment and reality programs because there was almost
no variation with regard to the dependent variables. Although
their presence was low compared to the other characters who
were coded, their representation is important against the back-
ground of the current study. For instance, talk shows are fre-
quently watched, and the position of host carries a certain
amount of authority and expertise with it, implying that the
gender representation of show hosts may also contribute to
viewers’ stereotyped views of the world and its gender roles.
Previous research has already outlined that men form the ma-
jority of show hosts in programs devoted to Bhard^ content
(e.g.,
politics and economy), whereas women are dominant as show
hosts for Bsoft^ topics (e.g., family and romance) (Gerbner
1995; Koeman et al. 2007; Segijn et al. 2014). Therefore, future
research should include an in-depth analysis of these roles on
gendered channels to assess potential differences.
In addition to including show hosts and reporters in future
analyses, it might be interesting to explore the relationship of
the gender of individuals behind the scenes of the gender -
targeted television channels (e.g., managers, writers, directors,
advertisers) and the on-screen portrayal of men and women.
This is particularly interesting because it could explore the
assumption which some media analysts and scholars have
put forward that if more women had positions of authority
behind the scenes in the entertainment world, the media would
offer less gender-stereotyped portrayals of men and women
(Benét 1978; Lauzen and Dozier 1999; Mills 1988). Some of
these dynamics have been explored for prime-time program-
ming by other researchers (e.g., Glascock 2001; Lauzen and
Dozier 1999; Lauzen et al. 2006; Lauzen et al. 2008), but
never systematically for gendered television channels.
A final recommendation for future research would be to
complete a cultivation analysis to measure how the audiences’
conceptions of gender are affected by the programming on
these gendered channels (Gerbner et al. 1978; Morgan 2007;
374 Sex Roles (2017) 77:366–378
Signorielli 1989; Signorielli and Bacue 1999). This directive
for future cultivation research is further strengthene d by re-
search based in social role theory (Eagly and Wood 2012)
which proposed that gender stereotypes might change when
gender distribution changes (Diekman and Eagly 2000), and it
is encountered in daily life or through mediated exposure
(Eagly and Steffen 1984). Future research could then assess
if more traditional or progressive role-pattern expectancies are
indeed cultivated in the audiences of men’s and women’s
channels respectively. This would necessitate surveying
viewers of both types of channels with varying levels of ex-
posure to television about their conceptions of gender roles.
As outlined previously, some of the findings presented in our
study are concerning; however, until a cultivation analysis is
conducted, the actual influence of these programs on actual
perceptions of gender and gender roles remain unknown.
Practice Implications
Television broadcasters work fervently to entertain and keep
their audiences watching and loyal, all in order to turn a profit
by selling advertising space to makers of products and services
aimed at specific (gendered) niche audience groups (Kuipers
2012; Turow 1998). However, through their politics of narrow -
casting, viewers—and particularly viewers of men’s channels
our study points out—are at risk for developing too narrow
conceptions about gender roles that may prove to be limiting
in real life. Because research has shown that television viewing
contributes to the maintenance, as well as the learning, of
gender
stereotyped perceptions (Gerbner et al. 1978; Larson 2001;
McGhee and Frueh 1980; Welch et al. 1979), we would there-
fore argue that the everyday television viewer should be made
aware of the presence of stereotyping on television. A possible
way to do this is to pay attention to this issue in media literacy
programs, which are part of school curricula in an increasing
number of schools (Koltay 2011; Tuominen and Kotilainen
2012). In these lessons, educators at all levels of education
should sensitize their students about gender-role depictions in
television programming and the (possible) effects they have on
men and women. In this way, they would ideally be providing a
continuous strengthening of children’s, adolescents, and young
adults’ defenses against the effects of gender stereotyping they
may encounter when watching various (gendered) television
channels throughout their life.
Conclusion
To conclude, some context for the rise of men’s and women’s
channels might be provided by the idea that we now live in a
post-feminist era in which there has been a resurgence of the
belief in sexual difference (Gill 2007; McRobbie 2009). It has
also been suggested that the increased status of women in
society has put pressure on the concept of masculinity
(Beynon 2002), which could explain the more traditional gen-
der portrayal on channels aimed at male audiences. However,
whatever the cause of the matter may be, our study makes
clear that rather than mirroring emancipatory changes in soci -
ety via a trend towards a more equal representation of gender
on television, the Dutch television landscape has become di -
vided through some large differences in the way gender is
represented on men’s versus women’s channels.
Taken together, we can conclude that, in the
Netherlands, it seems that a more equal image of gender
might be cultivated particularly for female audiences,
while the messages cultivated for male audiences remain
highly gender-stereotyped. These findings highlight a wor-
risome phenomenon when combined with several com-
plexities in gender-related practices and attitudes in the
Netherlands. The Netherlands is generally regarded as
one of the more gender-equal countries in the world
(United Nations Development Program [UNDP], 2015),
due for example to the continuously growing working
force participation of women, the increasing percentage
of women with a college degree, the increase of women
with a seat in parliament, a growing participation of men
in household chores as well as childcare in the last de-
cades, and a continuous increase in the share of individ-
uals who do not favor gender stereotypes in upbringing,
education, and the workplace (Arends-Tóth and van de
Vijver 2007; Collier et al. 2013; Emons 2011;
Gesthuizen et al. 2002; UNDP 2015). However, the
Netherlands also recently dropped three places in the
Gender Gap Index (World Economic Forum 2016) due
to the fact that in the Netherlands women’s workforce
participation rate is still lower than men’s. Women gener-
ally have part-time jobs, earn considerably less than men
do, and are severely underrepresented in senior executive
positions. Considering these complex and conflicting real -
ities in gender-related practices and attitudes in the
Netherlands, we feel that the results of our study seen in
the light of cultivation theory (Gerbner et al. 1978) and
social learning theory (Bandura 1977), combined with the
persistent finding that men in general tend to hold more
traditional gender views than women do (Bolzendahl and
Myers 2004; Brewster and Padavic 2000; Cameron and
Lalonde 2001), highlight issues that are important and
relevant to consider when examining the potential effects
of these gendered representations. As such, the gendered
representation on particularly men’s TV channels might
form a roadblock that stands in the way of true emanci-
pation and ideas of gender equality being reinforced in the
minds of not only women but particularly men.
Compliance with Ethical Standards The authors declare that
they have
no conflict of interest and that they have complied with the
APA ethical
standards.
Sex Roles (2017) 77:366–378 375
Open Access This article is distributed under the terms of the
Creative
Commons Attribution 4.0 International License (http://
creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits
unrestricted use,
distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided you
give appro-
priate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a
link to the
Creative Commons license, and indicate if changes were made.
References
Akass, K., & McCabe, J. (2004). Reading sex and the City.
London: I. B.
Tauris.
Allen, K., & Coltrane, S. (1996). Gender displaying television
commer-
cials: A comparative study of television commercials in the
1950s
and 1980s. Sex Roles, 35(3/4), 185–203.
doi:10.1007/BF01433106.
Arends-Tóth, J., & van de Vijver, F. J. R. (2007). Cultural and
gender
differences in gender-role beliefs, sharing household task and
child-
care responsibilities, and well-being among immigrants and
major-
ity members in the Netherlands. Sex Roles, 57(11/12), 813–824.
doi:10.1007/s11199-007-9316-z.
Bandura, A. (1977). Social learning theory. London: Prentice-
Hall.
Banet-Weiser, S. (2004). Girls rule!: Gender, feminism, and
nickelodeon.
Critical Studies in Media. Communication, 21, 119–139.
doi:10.1080/07393180410001688038.
Benét, J. W. (1978). Will media treatment of women improve?
In G.
Tuchman, A. K. Daniels, & J. W. Benét (Eds.), Hearth and
home:
Images of women in the mass media (pp. 266–271). New York:
Oxford University Press.
Beynon, J. (2002). Masculinities and the notion of 'crisis'. In J.
Beynon
(Ed.), Masculinities and culture (pp. 75–97). Buckingham: Open
University.
Bretl, D. J., & Cantor, J. (1988). The portrayal of men and
women in U.S.
television commercials: A recent content analysis and trends
over
15 years. Sex Roles, 18(9/10), 595–609.
doi:10.1007/BF00287963.
Brewster, K. L., & Padavic, I. (2000). Change in gender-
ideology, 1977–
1996: The contributions of intracohort change and population
turn-
over. Journal of Marriage and Family, 62(2), 477–487.
doi:10.1111
/j.1741-3737.2000.00477.x.
Bilandzic, H., & Busselle, R. W. (2008). Transportation and
transport-
ability in the cultivation of genre-consistent attitudes and
estimates.
Journal of Communication, 58(3), 508–529. doi:10.1111/j.1460-
2466.2008.00397.x.
Bilandzic, H., & Rössler, P. (2004). Life according to
television.
Implications of genre-specific cultivation effects: The
gratification/
cultivation model. Communications, 29, 295–326. doi:10.1515
/comm.2004.020.
Bolzendahl, C. I., & Myers, D. J. (2004). Feminist attitudes and
support
for gender equality: Opinion change in women and men, 1974–
1998. Social Forces, 83(2), 759–789.
doi:10.1353/sof.2005.0005.
Byars, J., & Meehan, E. R. (1994). Once in a lifetime:
Constructing Bthe
working woman^ through cable narrowcasting. Camera Obscura,
11, 12–41. doi:10.1215/02705346-11-12-3-1_33-34-12.
Cameron, J. E., & Lalonde, R. N. (2001). Social identification
and
gender-related ideology in women and men. British Journal of
Social Psychology, 40(1), 59–77.
doi:10.1348/014466601164696.
Centraal Bureau voor de Statistiek (2014). Statline. Retrieved
from
http://statline.cbs.nl.
Clark, C. (1972). Race, identification, and television violence.
In G. A.
Comstock, E. A. Rubinstein, & J. P. Murray (Eds.), Television
and
social behavior. Vol. 5.Television's effects: Further
explorations (pp.
120–184). Washington, DC: U.S. Government Printing Office.
Cohen, J., & Weimann, G. (2000). Cultivation revisited: Some
genres
have some effects on some viewers. Communication Reports,
13(2), 99–114. doi:10.1080/08934210009367728.
Collier, K. L., Bos, H. M. W., Merry, M. S., & Sandfort, T. G.
M. (2013).
Gender, ethnicity, religiosity, and same-sex sexual attraction
and the
acceptance of same-sex sexuality and gender non-conformity.
Sex
Roles, 68, 724–737. doi:10.1007/s11199-012-0135-5.
Collins, R. L. (2011). Content analysis of gender roles in media:
Where
are we now and where should we go? Sex Roles, 64(3–4), 290–
298.
doi:10.1007/s11199-010-9929-5.
Coltrane, S., & Adams, M. (1997). Work–family imagery and
gender
stereotypes: Television and the reproduction of difference.
Journal
of Vocational Behavior, 50(2), 323–347. doi:10.1006
/jvbe.1996.1575.
Cottle, S. (Ed.) (2000). Ethnic minorities and the media:
Changing cul-
tural boundaries. Buckingham: Open University Press.
Davis, D. M. (1990). Portrayals of women in prime-time
network televi-
sion: Some demographic characteristics. Sex Roles, 23(5–6),
325–
332. doi:10.1007/BF00290052.
Diekman, A. B., & Eagly, A. H. (2000). Stereotypes as dynamic
con-
structs: Women and men of the past, present, and future.
Personality
and Social Psychology Bulletin, 26, 1171–1188. doi:10.1177
/0146167200262001.
Dubrofsky, R. (2002). Ally McBeal as postfeminist icon: The
aestheticiz-
ing and fetishizing of the independent working woman. The
Communication Review, 5 , 265–284. doi:10.1080
/10714420214690.
Dyer, R. (1993). The matter of images: Essays on
representations.
London. New York: Routledge.
Dyer, R. (1997). White. London. New York: Routledge.
Eagly, A. H., & Steffen, V. J. (1984). Gender stereotypes stem
from the
distribution of women and men into social roles. Journal of
Personality and Social Psychology, 46(4), 735–754. doi:10.1037
/0022-3514.46.4.735.
Eagly, A. H., & Wood, W. (2012). Social role theory. A
biosocial analysis
of sex differences and similarities. In P. A. M. Van Lange, A.
W.
Kruglanski, & E. T. Higgins (Eds.), Handbook of theories in
social
psychology (pp. 458–476). London: Sage.
Egri, L. (1960). The art of dramatic writing. New York: Simon
&
Schuster.
Elasmar, M., Hasegawa, K., & Brain, M. (1999). The portrayal
of women
in U.S. prime time television. Journal of Broadcasting &
Electronic
Media, 43(1), 20–34. doi:10.1080/08838159909364472.
Emons, P. A. A. (2011). Social-cultural changes in Dutch
society and
their representations in television fiction, 1980–2005.
Nijmegen:
Knust.
Emons, P. A. A., Wester, F., & Scheepers, P. (2010). BHe
works outside
the home; she drinkscoffee and does the dishes^: Gender roles
in
fiction programs on Dutch television. Journal of Broadcasting &
Electronic Media, 51(1), 40–53.
doi:10.1080/08838150903550386.
Estes, R. J. (2003). Global change and indicators of social
development.
Retrieved from http://www.sp2.upenn.edu/restes/Estes%20
Papers/Global%20Change%20Indicators_2003.pdf.
Furnham, A., & Paltzer, S. (2010). The portrayal of men and
women in
television advertisements: An updated review of 30 studies pub-
lished since 2000. Scandinavian Journal of Psychology, 51(3),
216–236. doi:10.1111/j.1467-9450.2009.00772.x.
Ganahl, D. J., Prinsen, T. J., & Netzley, S. B. (2003). A content
analysis of
prime time commercials: A contextual framework of gender
repre-
sentation. Sex Roles, 49(9/10), 545–551. doi:10.1023
/A:1025893025658.
Gerbner, G. (1979). Television’s influence on values and
behaviour.
Massacommunicatie, 7, 215–222.
Gerbner, G. (1995). Casting and fate: Women and minorities on
television
drama, gameshows and news. In E. Hollander, P. Rutten, & L.
Van
der Linden (Eds.), Communication, culture & community: Liber
amicorum James Stappers (pp. 125–137). Houten: Bohn Stafleu
van Loghum.
376 Sex Roles (2017) 77:366–378
http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/BF01433106
http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s11199-007-9316-z
http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/07393180410001688038
http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/BF00287963
http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1741-3737.2000.00477.x
http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1741-3737.2000.00477.x
http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1460-2466.2008.00397.x
http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1460-2466.2008.00397.x
http://dx.doi.org/10.1515/comm.2004.020
http://dx.doi.org/10.1515/comm.2004.020
http://dx.doi.org/10.1353/sof.2005.0005
http://dx.doi.org/10.1215/02705346-11-12-3-1_33-34-12
http://dx.doi.org/10.1348/014466601164696
http://dx.doi.org/http://statline.cbs.nl
http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/08934210009367728
http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s11199-012-0135-5
http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s11199-010-9929-5
http://dx.doi.org/10.1006/jvbe.1996.1575
http://dx.doi.org/10.1006/jvbe.1996.1575
http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/BF00290052
http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/0146167200262001
http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/0146167200262001
http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/10714420214690
http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/10714420214690
http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.46.4.735
http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.46.4.735
http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/08838159909364472
http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/08838150903550386
http://www.sp2.upenn.edu/restes/Estes%20Papers/Global%20Ch
ange%20Indicators_2003.pdf
http://www.sp2.upenn.edu/restes/Estes%20Papers/Global%20Ch
ange%20Indicators_2003.pdf
http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9450.2009.00772.x
http://dx.doi.org/10.1023/A:1025893025658
http://dx.doi.org/10.1023/A:1025893025658
Gerbner, G., Gross, L., Jackson-Beeck, M., Jeffries-Fox, S., &
Signorielli,
N. (1978). Cultural indicators: Violence profile no. 9. Journal
of
Communication, 28(3), 176–207. doi:10.1111/j.1460-2466.1978.
tb01646.x.
Gerding, A., & Signorielli, N. (2014). Gender roles in tween
television
programming: A content analysis of two genres. Sex Roles,
70(1),
43–56. doi:10.1007/s11199-013-0330-z.
Gesthuizen, M., Scheepers, P., & Verloo, M. (2002). Support
for the
discrimination of women on the labour market in the
Netherlands:
Individual and contextual characteristics. Netherlands Journal
of
Social Sciences, 38(1), 48–64.
Gill, R. (2007). Postfeminist media culture: Elements of a
sensibility.
European Journal of Cultural Studies, 10(2), 147–166.
doi:10.1177/1367549407075898.
Glascock, J. (2001). Gender roles on prime-time network
television:
Demographics and behaviors. Journal of Broadcasting &
Electronic Media, 45(4), 656–669. doi:10.1207/s15506878
jobem4504_7.
Gomes, R. C., & Williams, L. F. (1990). Race and crime: The
role of the
media in perpetuating racism and classism in America. Urban
League Review, 14(1), 57–69.
Grabe, M. E., & Drew, D. G. (2007). Crime cultivation:
Comparisons
across media genres and channels. Journal of Broadcasting &
E l e c t ron i c M e di a , 5 1( 1) , 1 4 7– 1 7 1. do i : 1 0 . 1 08
0
/08838150701308143.
Greenberg, B. S., & Atkin, C. K. (1980). Life on television:
Content
analyses of U.S. TV drama. Norwood, NJ: Ablex Pub. Corp.
Greenberg, B. S., & Collette, L. (1997). The changing faces on
TV: A
demographic analysis of network television’s new seasons,
1966-
1992. Journal of Broadcasting and Electronic Media, 41(1), 1–
13.
doi:10.1080/08838159709364386.
Greenwood, D. N., & Lippman, J. R. (2010). Gender and media:
Content,
uses, and impact. In J. C. Chrisler & D. R. McCreary (Eds.),
Handbook of gender research in psychology (pp. 643–669). New
York: Springer.
Gunter, B. (1986). Television and sex role stereotyping.
London: Libbey.
Harwood, J., & Anderson, K. (2002). The presence and
portrayal of social
groups on prime-time television. Communication Reports, 15,
81–
97. doi:10.1080/08934210209367756.
Hawkins, R. P., & Pingree, S. (1981). Uniform messages and
habitual
viewing: Unnecessary assumptions in social reality effects.
Human
Communication Research, 7, 291–301.
Hayes, A. F., & Krippendorff, K. (2007). Answering the call for
a stan-
dard reliability measure for coding data. Communication
Methods
and Measures, 1, 77–89. doi:10.1080/19312450709336664.
Hundley, H. (2002). The evolution of gendercasting: The
lifetime televi-
sion network-Btelevision for women^. Journal of Popular Film
and
Television, 29(4), 174–181. doi:10.1080/01956050209601023.
Kim, K., & Lowry, D. T. (2005). Television commercials as a
lagging
social indicator: Gender role stereotypes in Korean television
adver-
tising. Sex Roles, 53(11/12), 901–910. doi:10.1007/s11199-005-
8307-1.
Koeman, J., Peeters, A. L., & d'Haenens, L. S. J. (2007).
Diversity mon-
itor 2005: Diversity as a quality aspect of television in the
Netherlands. Communications, 32(1), 97–121. doi:10.1515
/commun.2007.005.
Koltay, T. (2011). The media and the literacies: Media literacy,
informa-
tion literacy, digital literacy. Media, Culture & Society, 33(2),
211–
221. doi:10.1177/0163443710393382.
Kuipers, G. (2008). De VS in Europese huiskamers [the Unitest
states in
European living rooms]. Sociologie, 4(2/3), 174–194.
Kuipers, G. (2011). Cultural globalization as the emergence of a
transna-
tional cultural field: Transnational television and national media
landscapes in four European countries. American Behavioral
Scientist, 55(5), 541–557.
Kuipers, G. (2012). South Park boys and sex and the City
women:
Television trade, narrowcasting and the export of gender
categories.
Interactions: Studies in Communication & Culture, 2(3), 179–
196.
Larson, M. S. (2001). Interactions, activities and gender in
children’s
television commercials: A content analysis. Journal of
Broadcasting & Electronic Media, 45(1), 41–56. doi:10.1207
/s15506878jobem4501_4.
Lauzen, M. M., & Dozier, D. M. (1999). Making a difference in
prime
time: Women on screen and behind the scenes in the 1995–1996
television season. Journal of Broadcasting and Electronic
Media,
43, 1–43. doi:10.1080/08838159909364471.
Lauzen, M. M., & Dozier, D. M. (2005). Recognition and
respect
revisited: Portrayals of age and gender in prime-time television.
Mass Communication & Society, 8(3), 241–256. doi:10.1207
/s15327825mcs0803_4.
Lauzen, M. M., Dozier, D. M., & Cleveland, E. (2006). Genre
matters:
An examination of women working behind the scenes and on-
screen
portrayals in reality and scripted prime-time programming. Sex
Roles, 55, 445–455. doi:10.1007/s11199-006-9100-5.
Lauzen, M. M., Dozier, D. M., & Horan, N. (2008).
Constructing gender
stereotypes through social roles in prime-time television.
Journal of
Broadcasting & Electronic Media, 52(2), 200–214. doi:10.1080
/08838150801991971.
Leaper, C., Breed, L., Hoffman, L., & Perlman, C. (2002).
Variations in
the gender-stereotyped content of children’s television cartoons
across genres. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 32, 1653–
1662. doi:10.1111/j.1559-1816.2002.tb02767.x.
Lombard, M., Snyder-Duch, J., & Bracken, C. C. (2002).
Content anal-
ysis in mass communication. Human Communication Research,
28(4), 587–604. doi:10.1111/j.1468-2958.2002.tb00826.x.
Lotz, A. D. (2006). Redesigning women: Television after the
network era.
Urbana: University of Illinois Press.
McGhee, P., & Frueh, T. (1980). Television viewing and the
learning of
sex-role stereotypes. Sex Roles, 6(2), 179–188. doi:10.1007
/BF00287341.
McNeil, J. C. (1975). Feminism, femininity, and the television
series: A
content analysis. Journal of Broadcasting, 19(3), 259–271.
doi:10.1080/08838157509363786.
McRobbie, A. (2009). The aftermath of feminism: Gender,
culture and
social change. London: Sage.
Meehan, E. (1990). Why we don't count: The commodity
audience. In P.
Mellenkamp (Ed.), Logics of television: Essays in cultural
criticism
(pp. 117–137). Bloomington, IN: University Press.
Meehan, E. R., & Byars, J. (2000). Telefeminism: How lifetime
got its
groove, 1984-1997. Television & New Media, 1(1), 33–51.
doi:10.1177/152747640000100103.
Mills, K. (1988). A place in the news: From the women's pages
to the
front page. New York: Dodd, Mead.
Milner, L. M., & Higgs, B. (2004). Gender sex-role portrayals
in interna-
tional television advertising over time: The Australian
experience.
Journal of Current Issues and Research in Advertising, 26, 81–
95.
doi:10.1080/10641734.2004.10505166.
Morgan, M. (2007). What do young people learn about the
world from
watching television? In S. R. Mazzarella (Ed.), 20 questions
about
youth & the media(pp. 153–166). New York: Peter Lang
Publishing,
Inc.
Morgan, M., & Shanahan, J. (2010). The state of cultivation.
Journal of
Broadcasting & Electronic Media, 54(2), 337–355. doi:10.1080
/08838151003735018.
Northup, T., & Liebler, C. M. (2010). The good, the bad, and
the beau-
tiful: Beauty ideals on the Disney and nickelodeon channels.
Journal of Children and Media, 4(3), 265–282. doi:10.1080
/17482798.2010.496917.
Pennekamp, S. (2011). Monitor representatie 2010 [monitor
representa-
tion 2010]. Den Haag: NPO.
Sex Roles (2017) 77:366–378 377
http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1460-2466.1978.tb01646.x
http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1460-2466.1978.tb01646.x
http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s11199-013-0330-z
http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/1367549407075898
http://dx.doi.org/10.1207/s15506878jobem4504_7
http://dx.doi.org/10.1207/s15506878jobem4504_7
http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/08838150701308143
http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/08838150701308143
http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/08838159709364386
http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/08934210209367756
http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/19312450709336664
http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/01956050209601023
http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s11199-005-8307-1
http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s11199-005-8307-1
http://dx.doi.org/10.1515/commun.2007.005
http://dx.doi.org/10.1515/commun.2007.005
http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/0163443710393382
http://dx.doi.org/10.1207/s15506878jobem4501_4
http://dx.doi.org/10.1207/s15506878jobem4501_4
http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/08838159909364471
http://dx.doi.org/10.1207/s15327825mcs0803_4
http://dx.doi.org/10.1207/s15327825mcs0803_4
http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s11199-006-9100-5
http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/08838150801991971
http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/08838150801991971
http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1559-1816.2002.tb02767.x
http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1468-2958.2002.tb00826.x
http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/BF00287341
http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/BF00287341
http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/08838157509363786
http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/152747640000100103
http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/10641734.2004.10505166
http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/08838151003735018
http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/08838151003735018
http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/17482798.2010.496917
http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/17482798.2010.496917
Riffe, D., Lacy, S., & Fico, F. (2005). Analyzing media
messages: Using
quantitative content analysis in research. Mahwah, NJ:
Lawrence
Erlbaum.
Segijn, C., Bartholomé, G., Pennekamp, S., & Timmers, M.
(2014). De
afbeelding van statusverschillen in sekse en etniciteit in
Nederlandse
non-fictieprogramma’s [the portrayal of status difference
associated
with gender and ethnicity in Dutch non-fictional programs].
Tijdschrift voor Communicatiewetenschap, 42(3), 305–320.
doi:10.5553/TCW/138469302014042003008.
Sheperd, T. (2014). Gendering the commodity audience in
social media.
In C. Carter, L. Steiner, & L. McLaughlin (Eds.), The Routledge
companion to media & gender (pp. 157–167). London:
Routledge.
Signorielli, N. (1989). Television and conceptions about sex
roles:
Maintaining conventionality and the status quo. Sex Roles,
21(5–
6), 341–360. doi:10.1007/BF00289596.
Signorielli, N. (2012). Gender-role socialization in the twenty-
first cen-
tury. The International Encyclopedia of Media. Studies, 5(2), 1–
23.
doi:10.1002/9781444361506.wbiems116.
Signorielli, N., & Bacue, A. (1999). Recognition and respect: A
content
analysis of prime-time television characters across three
decades.
Sex Roles, 40(7–8), 527–544. doi:10.1023/a:1018883912900.
Signorielli, N., & Kahlenberg, S. (2001). Television’s world of
work in
the nineties. Journal of Broadcasting & Electronic Media, 45,
4–22.
doi:10.1207/s15506878jobem4501_2.
Stichting Kijkonderzoek. (2014). SKO Year Report 2014.
Retrieved from
https://kijkonderzoek.nl/images/SKO_Jaarrapport/SKO_
jaarrapport_2014.pdf.
Smith-Shomade, B. (2004). Narrowcasting in the new world
information
order. A space for the audience? Television & New Media, 5(1),
69–
81. doi:10.1177/1527476403259746.
Tankel, J. D., & Banks, J. (1997). Lifetime television and
women:
Narrowcasting as electronic space. In S. J. Drucker & G.
Gumpert
(Eds.), Voices in the street: Explorations in gender, media, and
pub-
lic space (pp. 255–270). Cresskill, NJ: Hampton Press.
Tedesco, N. S. (1974). Patterns in prime time. Journal of
Communication,
24(2), 119–124. doi:10.1111/j.1460-2466.1974.tb00376.x.
Thompson, T., & Zerbinos, E. (1995). Gender roles in animated
cartoons:
Has the picture changed in 20 years? Sex Roles, 32, 651–673.
doi:10.1007/BF01544217.
Tuominen, S., & Kotilainen, S. (2012). Pedagogies of media and
infor-
mation literacies. Retrieved from
http://iite.unesco.org/pics/publica-
tions/en/files/3214705.pdf.
Turow, J. (1998). Breaking up America: Advertisers and the
new media
world. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.
United Nations Development Program. (2015). Human
Development
Report. Retrieved from
http://hdr.undp.org/sites/default/files/2015_
human_development_report.pdf.
Van Bauwel, S. (2016). Gendered television genres, the same
old song: A
case study of gendered television genres in the north of
Belgium. In
K. Ross & C. Padovani (Eds.), Gender equality and the media:
A
challenge for Europe (pp. 60–71). New York: Routledge.
Vande Berg, L. R., & Streckfuss, D. (1992). Prime-time
television’s por-
trayal of women and the world of work: A demographic profile.
Journal of Broadcasting & Electronic Media, 36(2), 195–208.
doi:10.1080/08838159209364167.
Weijers, A. (2014). The craft of screenwriting. Den Haag: Boom
Lemma.
Welch, R. L., Huston-Stein, A., Wright, J. C., & Plehal, R.
(1979). Subtle
sex-role cues in children's commercials. Journal of
Communication,
29, 202–209. doi:10.1111/j.1460-2466.1979.tb01733.x.
Wiles, J. A., Wiles, C. R., & Tjernlund, A. (1995). A
comparison of gender
role portrayal in magazine advertising: The Netherlands,
Sweden and
the USA. European Journal of Marketing, 29(11), 35–49.
Wolin, L. D. (2003). Gender issues in advertising – an oversight
synthesis
of research – 1970-2002. Journal of Advertising Research,
43(1),
111–129.
World Economic Forum. (2016). The Global Gender Gap Report
2016.
Retrieved from https://www.weforum.org/reports/ the-global-
gender-gap-report-2016/.
378 Sex Roles (2017) 77:366–378
http://dx.doi.org/10.5553/TCW/138469302014042003008
http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/BF00289596
http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/9781444361506.wbiems116
http://dx.doi.org/10.1023/a:1018883912900
http://dx.doi.org/10.1207/s15506878jobem4501_2
https://kijkonderzoek.nl/images/SKO_Jaarrapport/SKO_jaarrapp
ort_2014.pdf
https://kijkonderzoek.nl/images/SKO_Jaarrapport/SKO_jaarrapp
ort_2014.pdf
http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/1527476403259746
http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1460-2466.1974.tb00376.x
http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/BF01544217
http://iite.unesco.org/pics/publica-tions/en/files/3214705.pdf
http://iite.unesco.org/pics/publica-tions/en/files/3214705.pdf
http://hdr.undp.org/sites/default/files/2015_human_development
_report.pdf
http://hdr.undp.org/sites/default/files/2015_human_development
_report.pdf
http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/08838159209364167
http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1460-2466.1979.tb01733.x
https://www.weforum.org/reports/the-global-gender-gap-report-
2016/
https://www.weforum.org/reports/the-global-gender-gap-report-
2016/
Sex Roles is a copyright of Springer, 2017. All Rights
Reserved.
Gender...AbstractRecognition: Presence of Men and Women on
TelevisionRespect: Stereotyping in Gender
RepresentationMethodSampleRecording Units and Coding
ProcedureResultsPresence as an Indicator of RecognitionAge as
Indicator of RespectTasks as Indicators of RespectEmployment
as Indicator of RespectParental Status as Indicator of
RespectDiscussionLimitations and Directions for Future
ResearchPractice ImplicationsConclusionReferences
https://doi.org/10.1177/1350508416689095
Organization
2017, Vol. 24(3) 397 –417
© The Author(s) 2017
Reprints and permissions:
sagepub.co.uk/journalsPermissions.nav
DOI: 10.1177/1350508416689095
journals.sagepub.com/home/org
‘It’s a man’s man’s man’s world’:
Music groupies and the othering of
women in the world of rock
Gretchen Larsen
Durham University, UK
Abstract
Groupies are understood as a particular type of fan that are most
commonly associated with
rock music. The ‘groupie’ identity is almost exclusively applied
to female fans but sometimes also
to female music producers and is largely used in a derogatory
manner both by the popular media
and by fans themselves. This article argues that the ‘groupie’
identity is used to ‘other’ and exclude
women from creative production in rock music. This study
draws on a rhetorical analysis of five
published biographical accounts of groupies to examine how the
labeling of certain people as ‘groupies’
works as an othering practice that serves to support and
maintain the gendered norms of rock and
identifies three underlying discursive processes. First, popular
and music media played a significant
role in stereotyping groupie as female right from the emergence
of the label. Second, the notions of
‘credibility’ and ‘authenticity’, which are central to serious
music journalism, are constructed in such
a way as to stigmatize and therefore exclude women from rock,
primarily by reframing ‘groupies’ as
inauthentic consumers rather than proper fans. Finally, the
intertwining of femininity with fandom, as
occurs in groupiedom, serves to magnify cultural assumptions
about women as sex objects and as
passive consumers of mass culture. In elucidating both the
gender and marketplace role politics at play
in the ‘groupie’ identity and the mechanisms involved in
othering women, space is opened in which
alternative possibilities for understanding and enacting the role
of women in rock can be imagined.
Keywords
Consumer, creative industries, fans, gender, groupies, music,
patriarchy
Introduction
The figure of the groupie looms large in the discourses and
social imaginary surrounding rock
music, playing an integral role in the mythology of ‘sex, drugs
and rock n’ roll’. Groupies can be
Corresponding author:
Gretchen Larsen, Department of Management and Marketing,
Durham University Business School, Durham University,
Queen’s Campus, University Boulevard, Thornaby, Stockton-
on-Tees TS17 6BH, UK.
Email: [email protected]
Article
689095ORG0010.1177/1350508416689095OrganizationLarsen
research-article2017
https://uk.sagepub.com/en-gb/journals-permissions
https://journals.sagepub.com/home/org
mailto:[email protected]
http://crossmark.crossref.org/dialog/?doi=10.1177%2F13505084
16689095&domain=pdf&date_stamp=2017-05-10
398 Organization 24(3)
found across a range of culture, leisure, and sports activities
(Forsyth and Thompson, 2007;
Gauthier and Forsyth, 2000; Gmelch and San Antonio, 1998),
but it is with rock music that they are
most closely associated. The phenomenon of the groupie gained
recognition and took shape as a
social identity within the counter-culture of 1960s’ rock music
and continues to hold significant
cultural currency and power. For example, films such as
‘Almost Famous’ (2000) and ‘The Banger
Sisters’ (2002) employ and as such reinforce the media
representation of the groupie, and Hill
(2013) shows how women feel the need to negotiate that same
representation when expressing
their fandom of metal music. There is no agreed definition as to
who or what a groupie is, but a
dominant representation exists in popular media/culture and
academic literature of a more extreme
type of female fan who seeks intimate emotional and/or sexual
relations with musicians (e.g. Cline,
1992; Des Barres, 2005 [1987]; Fonarow, 2006; Forrest,
2010).The consolidation of the groupie
identity can be found in a cover article in Rolling Stone
magazine in 1969 entitled ‘The Groupies
and Other Girls’, by John Burks, Jerry Hopkins, and Paul
Nelson. However, as Rhodes (2005)
explains, this was not the first mention or use of the term
‘groupie’. As the groupie subculture
emerged, several articles about women who could be
categorized as groupies were published, such
as Tom Wolfe’s (1965) ‘The Girl of the Year’ essay on Baby
Jane Holzer. But in the wake of the
Rolling Stone article, ‘alternate visions of what a groupie was
(or could be) were discarded in favor
of that offered by Rolling Stone and its highly sexualized and
misogynistic approach to the groupie
and rock culture’ (Rhodes, 2005: 137). As Rhodes (2005) then
carefully evidences, the Rolling
Stone article, which it is important to note is written entirely by
males, carried such power that any
further negotiation of the groupie identity ceased.
Consequently, it has provided what has been for
a long time, the definitive statement of what groupies are.
Warwick (2007) summarizes this view
of the groupie as
a kind of female fan assumed to be more interested in sex with
rock stars than in their music. Groupies are
understood to be ‘easy’ [i.e. sexually promiscuous], with low
self-esteem, and too stupid about music to be
proper fans, but also—paradoxically—predatory and
exploitative of the hapless musicians whose artistry
they cruelly ignore in their lust for celebrity sex. (p. 170)
which she argues is an unmistakably derisive and pejorative
description focusing almost entirely
on the sexual motivations of groupies. One of the greatest
concerns is that the label ‘groupie’ is
almost exclusively applied to females and has become a term
used to describe all female fans,
wives, and girlfriends and even those females who work in rock
music (Davies, 2001). This labe-
ling reduces the experiences of all women in rock to a singular
one driven by sex and effectively
excludes them from productive participation. It is to this crucial
issue that the article turns, as it
examines how the labeling of certain people as ‘groupies’ works
as an othering practice that
serves to support and maintain the gendered norms of rock and
thus exclude women from creative
production.
Beyond a surface-level recognition, we know little about
groupies other than that they are
reduced to some kind of caricature used in a derogatory manner
both by the popular media (Davies,
2001) and by fans (Hill, 2013) and are ‘treated like a punch line
to a never ending joke that only
the boys are in on’ (Forrest, 2010: 135). However, as we prise
beneath the surface, we begin to see
that the groupie identity sits at the intersection between the
social identities of gender and market-
place role. Gender has functioned as a primary site for the
analysis and interpretation of diversity
in the creative industries (e.g. Dean, 2008; Nixon and Crewe,
2004; Proctor-Thomson, 2013; Sang
et al., 2014). This is in large part because creativity, creative
work, and creative identities are con-
structed in such a way that women are marginalized or even
excluded. In the music industry,
women are underrepresented at all levels and in all roles
(Leonard, 2015); discounted in the music
Larsen 399
press and media (Davies, 2001), and even where they have been
acknowledged, their participation
is represented in gendered and often highly objectified ways
(Hatton and Trautner, 2011). While,
as Schippers (2000) notes, rock music is a setting in which
gender norms have sometimes been
challenged, particularly those related to appearance such as hair
length and the use of make-up, this
genre of music has long been synonymous with hegemonic
masculinity (e.g. Hill, 2014), heter-
onormativity (e.g. Frith and McRobbie, 1990 [1978]), and
homosociality (e.g. Davies, 2001) which
act as pillars upholding the patriarchy of rock.In the immortal
words of James Brown and (his
lesser known female co-writer and one-time girlfriend) Betty
Jean Newsom, ‘it’s a man’s, man’s,
man’s world’.
In working to exclude women from creative production, the
‘groupie’ identity draws not only
on gender identity but also on the dichotomy between work and
non-work. The key, relevant
social identity is what is called here ‘marketplace role’, which
categorizes people according to the
producer–consumer dualism. In the creative industries,
marketplace roles manifest in dualisms
such as artist–audience and musician–fan (e.g. Beauregard,
2012). Like any other social identity,
these categories imply subjectivities which define positions in
relationships between socio-
political actors and carry with them various assumptions and
statuses that serve to structure and
shape experience and engagement with the creative (e.g.
Bradshaw, 2010; Bradshaw et al., 2005).
An important assumption is that because a market requires both
production and consumption to
work, producers and consumers are co-dependent and therefore
hold equal status. This assump-
tion is underpinned by notions of consumer sovereignty
(Rothenberg, 1962), consumer subjectiv-
ity (Firat and Dholakia, 2016), and the logic of co-creation
(Vargo and Lusch, 2004; Venkatesh
and Meamber, 2006). However, not only have producers and
consumers largely been approached
in academia as separate, independent, and somewhat unrelated
entities but also consumers have
been historically viewed, particularly in the cultural and
creative sectors, as secondary, subordi-
nate figures (e.g. Beauregard, 2012; Huyssen, 1986). As noted
in the Call for Papers for this
Special Issue, much remains to be understood about how gender
intersects with other identities in
constructing experiences of creativity and creative wor k. There
has been little exploration of how
gender and marketplace roles intersect to frame who and what
type of work is considered to be
‘creative’ or productive and what the practices of inclusion and
exclusion are. This article puts
forth the argument that groupies are othered in both
categories—as women and as consumers, and
that in fact it is the intertwining of the two identities that has
underpinned and reinforced the
groupie identity, and thus helped construct and maintain the
patriarchy of rock music.
Following a deeper examination of the social identities of
gender and marketplace roles in the
context of rock music, this article draws on a rhetorical analysis
of five published biographical
accounts of groupies and rock wives in order to examine how
the labeling of certain people as
‘groupies’ works as an othering practice that maintains the
gendered norms of rock. Examining the
cultural phenomenon of the groupie retrospectively allows the
processes behind the construction
and maintenance of the identity and its consequences to be
explored. Three important discursive
processes emerge. First, popular and music media played a
significant role in stereotyping groupies
right from the emergence of the term. Second, the notions of
‘credibility’ and ‘authenticity’, which
are central to serious music journalism, are constructed in such
a way as to stigmatize and therefore
exclude, discredit, and invalidate the role of women in rock,
primarily by reframing ‘groupies’ as
inauthentic consumers rather than proper fans. Third, the
intertwining of femininity with fandom,
as occurs in the construction of the ‘groupie’, serves to magnify
cultural assumptions about women
as sex objects and as passive consumers of mass culture and
thus reinforces the groupie identity
and their exclusion from creative work in the world of rock.
This article contributes in important ways to a growing body of
literature that considers how
intersectional social identities are constructed and articulated in
rock music (Elafros, 2010) and the
400 Organization 24(3)
creative industries. It provides a historically and culturally
embedded account of how the ‘groupie’
label works as an othering practice to exclude women from
creative work. It resonates with and
builds upon other accounts in both the music and creative
industries (e.g. on processes of forgetting
identified by Strong (2011)) that have written women out of the
history of popular culture. It expands
our understanding of the role of gender in diversifying the
creative by locating the groupie identity
as the nexus of gender and marketplace role. Through the
processes identified, and contrary to the
transgressive and liberatory perspective taken by many of the
original groupies, the groupie identity
effectively reproduces and reinforces gendered hierarchies
within the creative industries. Finally, in
elucidating both the gender and marketplace politics at play in
the ‘groupie’ label and the discursive
processes involved in othering women, space is opened up
through which alternative possibilities
for understanding and enacting the role of women in rock can be
imagined.
Gender and rock music
The idea that notions of creativity and creative work are
gendered is one that is well established in
the literature (see, for example, Dean, 2008; Grugulis and
Stoyanova, 2012; Huyssen, 1986;
Leonard, 2015; Sang et al., 2014). Sustained academic interest
in gender and diversity in the crea-
tive industries is relatively recent, but the complex and thorny
relationship between gender and the
creative has long underscored important debates surrounding
culture and art. For example, Huyssen
(1986) elucidates and then interrogates the modernist notion of
mass culture as woman, and real,
authentic culture as man. He ultimately argues that while this
particular claim has lost its persua-
sive power as a result of a combination of feminist activism
increasing the presence of women in
art, and the postmodern project of the blurring of boundaries
between high art and mass culture,
gendering of the creative is still pervasive:
certain forms of mass culture, with their obsession with
gendered violence are more of a threat to women
than to men. After all, it has always been men rather than
women who have had real control over the
production of mass culture. (Huyssen, 1986: 62)
This is echoed in more recent research which shows how women
remain marginalized and excluded
from work and identities in such diverse sectors of the creative
industries as film and television
(e.g. Bielby and Bielby, 1996; Grugulis and Stoyanova, 2012),
acting and performance (e.g. Dean,
2008; Dean and Jones, 2003), and music (e.g. Davies, 2001;
Leonard, 2015; Maus, 2011). Even the
creative and cultural products that are produced and/or
consumed by females are valued less and
placed further down the cultural hierarchy than those of their
male counterparts, such as in the case
of the ‘inferior’ romance novels preferred by Flaubert’s
Madame Bovary (Huyssen, 1986); the
immensely successful Twilight series (Strong, 2009); and ‘girl
groups’, ‘boy bands’, and other
types of pop music (Railton, 2001). Strong (2009: p.1) argues
that the construction and naturaliza-
tion of females as occupants of the lower levels of the cultural
hierarchy is a form of ‘symbolic
violence that helps reproduce power relations between men and
women’. The key issue here is the
struggle over the control of cultural production, or who has the
power to define what creative work
is, who gets to do it, and under what conditions.
The rock industry has long been acknowledged and criticized
for being gendered male in many
facets and norms. First, the style and form of rock music is
commonly understood to be both
masculine and an expression of male sexuality (Frith and
McRobbie, 1990 [1978]). For example,
in rock performances, musicians are aggressive, dominating,
boastful, and in control; the music is
loud and rhythmically insistent; and the lyrics are assertive and
arrogant. August (2009) elabo-
rates that much, although not all, of the lyrical content of the
songs written by the rock band, the
Larsen 401
Rolling Stones, is misogynistic and promotes the subordination
and objectification of women.
‘Musically, such rock takes off from the sexual frankness of
rhythm and blues but adds a cruder
male physicality (hardness, control, virtuosity)’ (Frith and
McRobbie, 1990 [1978]: 374). Second,
in terms of the nature and means of production, the rock music
business is run predominantly by
males. Musicians, writers, technicians, engineers, producers,
and road crew are largely male
(Frith and McRobbie, 1990 [1978]). Women remain
underrepresented (Leonard, 2015) with roles
limited to those that fit with male notions of female ability, for
example, singers and publicity
agents. Cohen (1997) illustrates how the rock scene is actively
produced as male through various
institutions and social practices. For example, social interaction
inside rock music venues is mas-
culine—referring to each other by nicknames; using technical
and in-house jargon; and sharing
the jokes, myths, and hype that surround the bands on the scene.
Third, mainstream rock music
press is gendered male in associations, assumptions, and
representations of the music and the
musicians (Elafros, 2010). The contributions of female
musicians are often excluded (Davies,
2001) or represented through the male gaze, as little more than
a body. Davies (2001) argues that
the British music press uses its own very particular idiom,
which is very similar to that found in
more explicitly ‘laddish’ magazines such as Loaded: ‘this
distinctive idiom is internalized by
aspiring music journalists, who realise that they must write in
the correct style to be successful.
Such compliance and conservatism mean that the sexism of the
music press is self-perpetuating’
(p. 316). Finally, even practices of fandom in rock music are
gendered male, particularly those
that occur publicly and are characterized by communal practices
of engagement (O’Reilly et al.,
2013). Arguing for a new, more gender-inclusive framework for
studying fans of hard rock and
heavy metal as a group, Hill (2014) explains that the
underlying gendered epistemology of fandom has resulted in a
dismissal of women fans or, at best, a
systematic reduction of their experiences as fans (such as
private engagements with the music, the
representation of women fans as groupies, and fannish activities
such as reading magazines and
participating in online fora). (p. 174)
This patriarchy of rock music appears to be held in place by
three distinct but interrelated forces
and their attendant institutions and practices: hegemonic
masculinity, heteronormativity, and
homosociality. Hegemonic masculinity, in its original and most
prevalent formulation, refers to
‘the pattern of practice […] that allowed men’s dominance over
women to continue’ (Connell and
Messerschmidt, 2005: 832). As introduced by Connell (1995),
hegemonic masculinity embodies
the most honored, culturally valued, and therefore ideal way of
being a man. While only a minority
of men might enact hegemonic masculinity, it is normative and
at its most powerful when under-
pinned by complicit masculinities (embodied by men who do
not conform to all characteristics of
hegemonic masculinity but because they do not resist it still
benefit from the patriarchy (Connell,
1995)) and compliance among heterosexual women. In effect, it
ideologically legitimates the sub-
ordination of women and non-hegemonic masculinities, to men.
Much of the existing literature on
(1) Post a responsive review to someone elses essay paper from We
(1) Post a responsive review to someone elses essay paper from We
(1) Post a responsive review to someone elses essay paper from We
(1) Post a responsive review to someone elses essay paper from We
(1) Post a responsive review to someone elses essay paper from We
(1) Post a responsive review to someone elses essay paper from We
(1) Post a responsive review to someone elses essay paper from We
(1) Post a responsive review to someone elses essay paper from We
(1) Post a responsive review to someone elses essay paper from We
(1) Post a responsive review to someone elses essay paper from We
(1) Post a responsive review to someone elses essay paper from We
(1) Post a responsive review to someone elses essay paper from We
(1) Post a responsive review to someone elses essay paper from We
(1) Post a responsive review to someone elses essay paper from We
(1) Post a responsive review to someone elses essay paper from We
(1) Post a responsive review to someone elses essay paper from We
(1) Post a responsive review to someone elses essay paper from We
(1) Post a responsive review to someone elses essay paper from We
(1) Post a responsive review to someone elses essay paper from We
(1) Post a responsive review to someone elses essay paper from We
(1) Post a responsive review to someone elses essay paper from We
(1) Post a responsive review to someone elses essay paper from We
(1) Post a responsive review to someone elses essay paper from We
(1) Post a responsive review to someone elses essay paper from We
(1) Post a responsive review to someone elses essay paper from We
(1) Post a responsive review to someone elses essay paper from We
(1) Post a responsive review to someone elses essay paper from We
(1) Post a responsive review to someone elses essay paper from We
(1) Post a responsive review to someone elses essay paper from We
(1) Post a responsive review to someone elses essay paper from We
(1) Post a responsive review to someone elses essay paper from We
(1) Post a responsive review to someone elses essay paper from We
(1) Post a responsive review to someone elses essay paper from We
(1) Post a responsive review to someone elses essay paper from We
(1) Post a responsive review to someone elses essay paper from We
(1) Post a responsive review to someone elses essay paper from We
(1) Post a responsive review to someone elses essay paper from We
(1) Post a responsive review to someone elses essay paper from We
(1) Post a responsive review to someone elses essay paper from We
(1) Post a responsive review to someone elses essay paper from We
(1) Post a responsive review to someone elses essay paper from We
(1) Post a responsive review to someone elses essay paper from We
(1) Post a responsive review to someone elses essay paper from We
(1) Post a responsive review to someone elses essay paper from We
(1) Post a responsive review to someone elses essay paper from We
(1) Post a responsive review to someone elses essay paper from We
(1) Post a responsive review to someone elses essay paper from We
(1) Post a responsive review to someone elses essay paper from We
(1) Post a responsive review to someone elses essay paper from We
(1) Post a responsive review to someone elses essay paper from We
(1) Post a responsive review to someone elses essay paper from We
(1) Post a responsive review to someone elses essay paper from We
(1) Post a responsive review to someone elses essay paper from We
(1) Post a responsive review to someone elses essay paper from We
(1) Post a responsive review to someone elses essay paper from We
(1) Post a responsive review to someone elses essay paper from We
(1) Post a responsive review to someone elses essay paper from We
(1) Post a responsive review to someone elses essay paper from We
(1) Post a responsive review to someone elses essay paper from We
(1) Post a responsive review to someone elses essay paper from We
(1) Post a responsive review to someone elses essay paper from We
(1) Post a responsive review to someone elses essay paper from We
(1) Post a responsive review to someone elses essay paper from We
(1) Post a responsive review to someone elses essay paper from We
(1) Post a responsive review to someone elses essay paper from We
(1) Post a responsive review to someone elses essay paper from We
(1) Post a responsive review to someone elses essay paper from We

More Related Content

Similar to (1) Post a responsive review to someone elses essay paper from We

10 Diversity and Assessment Joyce P. Chu, Brian A. Maruy
10 Diversity and Assessment Joyce P. Chu, Brian A. Maruy10 Diversity and Assessment Joyce P. Chu, Brian A. Maruy
10 Diversity and Assessment Joyce P. Chu, Brian A. MaruyBenitoSumpter862
 
10 Diversity and Assessment Joyce P. Chu, Brian A. Maruy
10 Diversity and Assessment Joyce P. Chu, Brian A. Maruy10 Diversity and Assessment Joyce P. Chu, Brian A. Maruy
10 Diversity and Assessment Joyce P. Chu, Brian A. MaruySantosConleyha
 
An Examination of Gender Roles and Homophobia in the TV program, "Friends"
An Examination of Gender Roles and Homophobia in the TV program, "Friends"An Examination of Gender Roles and Homophobia in the TV program, "Friends"
An Examination of Gender Roles and Homophobia in the TV program, "Friends"Zoe_Cumler
 
final thesis revision
final thesis revisionfinal thesis revision
final thesis revisionKat Wortham
 
concept engagement.pdf
concept engagement.pdfconcept engagement.pdf
concept engagement.pdfstudywriters
 
Beyond Soap Opera for Social Change: An Analysis of Kenya's The Team
Beyond Soap Opera for Social Change: An Analysis of Kenya's The TeamBeyond Soap Opera for Social Change: An Analysis of Kenya's The Team
Beyond Soap Opera for Social Change: An Analysis of Kenya's The TeamMelissa Tully
 
Bushnell, S Research Paper
Bushnell, S Research PaperBushnell, S Research Paper
Bushnell, S Research PaperSarah Bushnell
 
CARDReport FINAL 2.22.16
CARDReport FINAL 2.22.16CARDReport FINAL 2.22.16
CARDReport FINAL 2.22.16Sofia Elias
 
CARDReport FINAL 2.22.16
CARDReport FINAL 2.22.16CARDReport FINAL 2.22.16
CARDReport FINAL 2.22.16Breanne Flores
 
Write a one to two (1-2) page paper in which you1.    Use the p.docx
Write a one to two (1-2) page paper in which you1.    Use the p.docxWrite a one to two (1-2) page paper in which you1.    Use the p.docx
Write a one to two (1-2) page paper in which you1.    Use the p.docxericbrooks84875
 
Masculinity and femenity
Masculinity and femenityMasculinity and femenity
Masculinity and femenityKatalia1
 
Sample Student Paper1 Abstract The purpose of this.docx
Sample Student Paper1  Abstract  The purpose of this.docxSample Student Paper1  Abstract  The purpose of this.docx
Sample Student Paper1 Abstract The purpose of this.docxtodd331
 
Sample Student Paper1 Abstract The purpose of this.docx
Sample Student Paper1  Abstract  The purpose of this.docxSample Student Paper1  Abstract  The purpose of this.docx
Sample Student Paper1 Abstract The purpose of this.docxjeffsrosalyn
 
The Muses Project (2.0)
The Muses Project (2.0)The Muses Project (2.0)
The Muses Project (2.0)Arielle Neal
 
HT500-FINAL-BenKeeler
HT500-FINAL-BenKeelerHT500-FINAL-BenKeeler
HT500-FINAL-BenKeelerBen Keeler
 
Sex Roles, Vol. 53, Nos. 1112, December 2005 ( C© 2005)DOI.docx
Sex Roles, Vol. 53, Nos. 1112, December 2005 ( C© 2005)DOI.docxSex Roles, Vol. 53, Nos. 1112, December 2005 ( C© 2005)DOI.docx
Sex Roles, Vol. 53, Nos. 1112, December 2005 ( C© 2005)DOI.docxlesleyryder69361
 
Audience theory research
Audience theory researchAudience theory research
Audience theory researchfarah2512
 
Dissertation FINAL (Autosaved)FINAINFINAL
Dissertation FINAL (Autosaved)FINAINFINALDissertation FINAL (Autosaved)FINAINFINAL
Dissertation FINAL (Autosaved)FINAINFINALRyan Cockrell
 

Similar to (1) Post a responsive review to someone elses essay paper from We (20)

EURAM 2015
EURAM 2015EURAM 2015
EURAM 2015
 
10 Diversity and Assessment Joyce P. Chu, Brian A. Maruy
10 Diversity and Assessment Joyce P. Chu, Brian A. Maruy10 Diversity and Assessment Joyce P. Chu, Brian A. Maruy
10 Diversity and Assessment Joyce P. Chu, Brian A. Maruy
 
10 Diversity and Assessment Joyce P. Chu, Brian A. Maruy
10 Diversity and Assessment Joyce P. Chu, Brian A. Maruy10 Diversity and Assessment Joyce P. Chu, Brian A. Maruy
10 Diversity and Assessment Joyce P. Chu, Brian A. Maruy
 
An Examination of Gender Roles and Homophobia in the TV program, "Friends"
An Examination of Gender Roles and Homophobia in the TV program, "Friends"An Examination of Gender Roles and Homophobia in the TV program, "Friends"
An Examination of Gender Roles and Homophobia in the TV program, "Friends"
 
final thesis revision
final thesis revisionfinal thesis revision
final thesis revision
 
concept engagement.pdf
concept engagement.pdfconcept engagement.pdf
concept engagement.pdf
 
Beyond Soap Opera for Social Change: An Analysis of Kenya's The Team
Beyond Soap Opera for Social Change: An Analysis of Kenya's The TeamBeyond Soap Opera for Social Change: An Analysis of Kenya's The Team
Beyond Soap Opera for Social Change: An Analysis of Kenya's The Team
 
Rhiana
RhianaRhiana
Rhiana
 
Bushnell, S Research Paper
Bushnell, S Research PaperBushnell, S Research Paper
Bushnell, S Research Paper
 
CARDReport FINAL 2.22.16
CARDReport FINAL 2.22.16CARDReport FINAL 2.22.16
CARDReport FINAL 2.22.16
 
CARDReport FINAL 2.22.16
CARDReport FINAL 2.22.16CARDReport FINAL 2.22.16
CARDReport FINAL 2.22.16
 
Write a one to two (1-2) page paper in which you1.    Use the p.docx
Write a one to two (1-2) page paper in which you1.    Use the p.docxWrite a one to two (1-2) page paper in which you1.    Use the p.docx
Write a one to two (1-2) page paper in which you1.    Use the p.docx
 
Masculinity and femenity
Masculinity and femenityMasculinity and femenity
Masculinity and femenity
 
Sample Student Paper1 Abstract The purpose of this.docx
Sample Student Paper1  Abstract  The purpose of this.docxSample Student Paper1  Abstract  The purpose of this.docx
Sample Student Paper1 Abstract The purpose of this.docx
 
Sample Student Paper1 Abstract The purpose of this.docx
Sample Student Paper1  Abstract  The purpose of this.docxSample Student Paper1  Abstract  The purpose of this.docx
Sample Student Paper1 Abstract The purpose of this.docx
 
The Muses Project (2.0)
The Muses Project (2.0)The Muses Project (2.0)
The Muses Project (2.0)
 
HT500-FINAL-BenKeeler
HT500-FINAL-BenKeelerHT500-FINAL-BenKeeler
HT500-FINAL-BenKeeler
 
Sex Roles, Vol. 53, Nos. 1112, December 2005 ( C© 2005)DOI.docx
Sex Roles, Vol. 53, Nos. 1112, December 2005 ( C© 2005)DOI.docxSex Roles, Vol. 53, Nos. 1112, December 2005 ( C© 2005)DOI.docx
Sex Roles, Vol. 53, Nos. 1112, December 2005 ( C© 2005)DOI.docx
 
Audience theory research
Audience theory researchAudience theory research
Audience theory research
 
Dissertation FINAL (Autosaved)FINAINFINAL
Dissertation FINAL (Autosaved)FINAINFINALDissertation FINAL (Autosaved)FINAINFINAL
Dissertation FINAL (Autosaved)FINAINFINAL
 

More from SilvaGraf83

1 Evidence-Based Practices to Guide Clinica
1  Evidence-Based Practices to Guide Clinica1  Evidence-Based Practices to Guide Clinica
1 Evidence-Based Practices to Guide ClinicaSilvaGraf83
 
1 Green Book Film Analysis Sugiarto Mulj
1  Green Book Film Analysis  Sugiarto Mulj1  Green Book Film Analysis  Sugiarto Mulj
1 Green Book Film Analysis Sugiarto MuljSilvaGraf83
 
1 Film Essay 1 Film from 1940-1970
1  Film Essay 1 Film from 1940-1970 1  Film Essay 1 Film from 1940-1970
1 Film Essay 1 Film from 1940-1970 SilvaGraf83
 
1 Department of Health and Human Performance, College of Ch
1  Department of Health and Human Performance, College of Ch1  Department of Health and Human Performance, College of Ch
1 Department of Health and Human Performance, College of ChSilvaGraf83
 
1 FIN 2063 INSURANCE FINANCIAL PLANNING Case As
1  FIN 2063 INSURANCE FINANCIAL PLANNING Case As1  FIN 2063 INSURANCE FINANCIAL PLANNING Case As
1 FIN 2063 INSURANCE FINANCIAL PLANNING Case AsSilvaGraf83
 
1 Faculty of Science, Engineering and Computi
1  Faculty of Science, Engineering and Computi1  Faculty of Science, Engineering and Computi
1 Faculty of Science, Engineering and ComputiSilvaGraf83
 
1 Case Grading Procedure Your grade from each case
1  Case Grading Procedure Your grade from each case 1  Case Grading Procedure Your grade from each case
1 Case Grading Procedure Your grade from each case SilvaGraf83
 
1 Kilimanjaro is a snow-covered mountain 19,710 feet hi
1  Kilimanjaro is a snow-covered mountain 19,710 feet hi1  Kilimanjaro is a snow-covered mountain 19,710 feet hi
1 Kilimanjaro is a snow-covered mountain 19,710 feet hiSilvaGraf83
 
1 Assignment 2 Winter 2022Problem 1 Assume yo
1  Assignment 2 Winter 2022Problem 1 Assume yo1  Assignment 2 Winter 2022Problem 1 Assume yo
1 Assignment 2 Winter 2022Problem 1 Assume yoSilvaGraf83
 
1 COU 680 Adult Psychosocial Assessment Sabrina Da
1  COU 680 Adult Psychosocial Assessment Sabrina  Da1  COU 680 Adult Psychosocial Assessment Sabrina  Da
1 COU 680 Adult Psychosocial Assessment Sabrina DaSilvaGraf83
 
1 Literature Review on How Biofilm Affect the
1  Literature Review on How Biofilm Affect the1  Literature Review on How Biofilm Affect the
1 Literature Review on How Biofilm Affect theSilvaGraf83
 
1 Canterbury Tales (c. 12th century)
1  Canterbury Tales        (c. 12th century)  1  Canterbury Tales        (c. 12th century)
1 Canterbury Tales (c. 12th century) SilvaGraf83
 
1 Math 140 Exam 2 COC Spring 2022 150 Points
1  Math 140 Exam 2 COC Spring 2022 150 Points  1  Math 140 Exam 2 COC Spring 2022 150 Points
1 Math 140 Exam 2 COC Spring 2022 150 Points SilvaGraf83
 
1 Lessons from the past How the deadly second wave
1  Lessons from the past How the deadly second wave1  Lessons from the past How the deadly second wave
1 Lessons from the past How the deadly second waveSilvaGraf83
 
1 Lockheed Martin Corporation Abdussamet Akca
1  Lockheed Martin Corporation Abdussamet Akca  1  Lockheed Martin Corporation Abdussamet Akca
1 Lockheed Martin Corporation Abdussamet Akca SilvaGraf83
 
1 Lab 9 Comparison of Two Field Methods in a Scien
1  Lab 9 Comparison of Two Field Methods in a Scien1  Lab 9 Comparison of Two Field Methods in a Scien
1 Lab 9 Comparison of Two Field Methods in a ScienSilvaGraf83
 
1 LAB MODULE 5 GLOBAL TEMPERATURE PATTERNS Note P
1  LAB MODULE 5 GLOBAL TEMPERATURE PATTERNS Note P1  LAB MODULE 5 GLOBAL TEMPERATURE PATTERNS Note P
1 LAB MODULE 5 GLOBAL TEMPERATURE PATTERNS Note PSilvaGraf83
 
1 Instructions for Coming of Age in Mississippi
1  Instructions for Coming of  Age in Mississippi 1  Instructions for Coming of  Age in Mississippi
1 Instructions for Coming of Age in Mississippi SilvaGraf83
 
1 Institutional Assessment Report 2012-13
1  Institutional Assessment Report 2012-13  1  Institutional Assessment Report 2012-13
1 Institutional Assessment Report 2012-13 SilvaGraf83
 

More from SilvaGraf83 (20)

1 Evidence-Based Practices to Guide Clinica
1  Evidence-Based Practices to Guide Clinica1  Evidence-Based Practices to Guide Clinica
1 Evidence-Based Practices to Guide Clinica
 
1 Green Book Film Analysis Sugiarto Mulj
1  Green Book Film Analysis  Sugiarto Mulj1  Green Book Film Analysis  Sugiarto Mulj
1 Green Book Film Analysis Sugiarto Mulj
 
1 Film Essay 1 Film from 1940-1970
1  Film Essay 1 Film from 1940-1970 1  Film Essay 1 Film from 1940-1970
1 Film Essay 1 Film from 1940-1970
 
1 Department of Health and Human Performance, College of Ch
1  Department of Health and Human Performance, College of Ch1  Department of Health and Human Performance, College of Ch
1 Department of Health and Human Performance, College of Ch
 
1 FIN 2063 INSURANCE FINANCIAL PLANNING Case As
1  FIN 2063 INSURANCE FINANCIAL PLANNING Case As1  FIN 2063 INSURANCE FINANCIAL PLANNING Case As
1 FIN 2063 INSURANCE FINANCIAL PLANNING Case As
 
1 Faculty of Science, Engineering and Computi
1  Faculty of Science, Engineering and Computi1  Faculty of Science, Engineering and Computi
1 Faculty of Science, Engineering and Computi
 
1 EARLY C
1  EARLY C1  EARLY C
1 EARLY C
 
1 Case Grading Procedure Your grade from each case
1  Case Grading Procedure Your grade from each case 1  Case Grading Procedure Your grade from each case
1 Case Grading Procedure Your grade from each case
 
1 Kilimanjaro is a snow-covered mountain 19,710 feet hi
1  Kilimanjaro is a snow-covered mountain 19,710 feet hi1  Kilimanjaro is a snow-covered mountain 19,710 feet hi
1 Kilimanjaro is a snow-covered mountain 19,710 feet hi
 
1 Assignment 2 Winter 2022Problem 1 Assume yo
1  Assignment 2 Winter 2022Problem 1 Assume yo1  Assignment 2 Winter 2022Problem 1 Assume yo
1 Assignment 2 Winter 2022Problem 1 Assume yo
 
1 COU 680 Adult Psychosocial Assessment Sabrina Da
1  COU 680 Adult Psychosocial Assessment Sabrina  Da1  COU 680 Adult Psychosocial Assessment Sabrina  Da
1 COU 680 Adult Psychosocial Assessment Sabrina Da
 
1 Literature Review on How Biofilm Affect the
1  Literature Review on How Biofilm Affect the1  Literature Review on How Biofilm Affect the
1 Literature Review on How Biofilm Affect the
 
1 Canterbury Tales (c. 12th century)
1  Canterbury Tales        (c. 12th century)  1  Canterbury Tales        (c. 12th century)
1 Canterbury Tales (c. 12th century)
 
1 Math 140 Exam 2 COC Spring 2022 150 Points
1  Math 140 Exam 2 COC Spring 2022 150 Points  1  Math 140 Exam 2 COC Spring 2022 150 Points
1 Math 140 Exam 2 COC Spring 2022 150 Points
 
1 Lessons from the past How the deadly second wave
1  Lessons from the past How the deadly second wave1  Lessons from the past How the deadly second wave
1 Lessons from the past How the deadly second wave
 
1 Lockheed Martin Corporation Abdussamet Akca
1  Lockheed Martin Corporation Abdussamet Akca  1  Lockheed Martin Corporation Abdussamet Akca
1 Lockheed Martin Corporation Abdussamet Akca
 
1 Lab 9 Comparison of Two Field Methods in a Scien
1  Lab 9 Comparison of Two Field Methods in a Scien1  Lab 9 Comparison of Two Field Methods in a Scien
1 Lab 9 Comparison of Two Field Methods in a Scien
 
1 LAB MODULE 5 GLOBAL TEMPERATURE PATTERNS Note P
1  LAB MODULE 5 GLOBAL TEMPERATURE PATTERNS Note P1  LAB MODULE 5 GLOBAL TEMPERATURE PATTERNS Note P
1 LAB MODULE 5 GLOBAL TEMPERATURE PATTERNS Note P
 
1 Instructions for Coming of Age in Mississippi
1  Instructions for Coming of  Age in Mississippi 1  Instructions for Coming of  Age in Mississippi
1 Instructions for Coming of Age in Mississippi
 
1 Institutional Assessment Report 2012-13
1  Institutional Assessment Report 2012-13  1  Institutional Assessment Report 2012-13
1 Institutional Assessment Report 2012-13
 

Recently uploaded

Presentation by Andreas Schleicher Tackling the School Absenteeism Crisis 30 ...
Presentation by Andreas Schleicher Tackling the School Absenteeism Crisis 30 ...Presentation by Andreas Schleicher Tackling the School Absenteeism Crisis 30 ...
Presentation by Andreas Schleicher Tackling the School Absenteeism Crisis 30 ...EduSkills OECD
 
Q4-W6-Restating Informational Text Grade 3
Q4-W6-Restating Informational Text Grade 3Q4-W6-Restating Informational Text Grade 3
Q4-W6-Restating Informational Text Grade 3JemimahLaneBuaron
 
Call Girls in Dwarka Mor Delhi Contact Us 9654467111
Call Girls in Dwarka Mor Delhi Contact Us 9654467111Call Girls in Dwarka Mor Delhi Contact Us 9654467111
Call Girls in Dwarka Mor Delhi Contact Us 9654467111Sapana Sha
 
Concept of Vouching. B.Com(Hons) /B.Compdf
Concept of Vouching. B.Com(Hons) /B.CompdfConcept of Vouching. B.Com(Hons) /B.Compdf
Concept of Vouching. B.Com(Hons) /B.CompdfUmakantAnnand
 
Introduction to AI in Higher Education_draft.pptx
Introduction to AI in Higher Education_draft.pptxIntroduction to AI in Higher Education_draft.pptx
Introduction to AI in Higher Education_draft.pptxpboyjonauth
 
APM Welcome, APM North West Network Conference, Synergies Across Sectors
APM Welcome, APM North West Network Conference, Synergies Across SectorsAPM Welcome, APM North West Network Conference, Synergies Across Sectors
APM Welcome, APM North West Network Conference, Synergies Across SectorsAssociation for Project Management
 
Crayon Activity Handout For the Crayon A
Crayon Activity Handout For the Crayon ACrayon Activity Handout For the Crayon A
Crayon Activity Handout For the Crayon AUnboundStockton
 
MENTAL STATUS EXAMINATION format.docx
MENTAL     STATUS EXAMINATION format.docxMENTAL     STATUS EXAMINATION format.docx
MENTAL STATUS EXAMINATION format.docxPoojaSen20
 
CARE OF CHILD IN INCUBATOR..........pptx
CARE OF CHILD IN INCUBATOR..........pptxCARE OF CHILD IN INCUBATOR..........pptx
CARE OF CHILD IN INCUBATOR..........pptxGaneshChakor2
 
POINT- BIOCHEMISTRY SEM 2 ENZYMES UNIT 5.pptx
POINT- BIOCHEMISTRY SEM 2 ENZYMES UNIT 5.pptxPOINT- BIOCHEMISTRY SEM 2 ENZYMES UNIT 5.pptx
POINT- BIOCHEMISTRY SEM 2 ENZYMES UNIT 5.pptxSayali Powar
 
mini mental status format.docx
mini    mental       status     format.docxmini    mental       status     format.docx
mini mental status format.docxPoojaSen20
 
_Math 4-Q4 Week 5.pptx Steps in Collecting Data
_Math 4-Q4 Week 5.pptx Steps in Collecting Data_Math 4-Q4 Week 5.pptx Steps in Collecting Data
_Math 4-Q4 Week 5.pptx Steps in Collecting DataJhengPantaleon
 
Presiding Officer Training module 2024 lok sabha elections
Presiding Officer Training module 2024 lok sabha electionsPresiding Officer Training module 2024 lok sabha elections
Presiding Officer Training module 2024 lok sabha electionsanshu789521
 
Hybridoma Technology ( Production , Purification , and Application )
Hybridoma Technology  ( Production , Purification , and Application  ) Hybridoma Technology  ( Production , Purification , and Application  )
Hybridoma Technology ( Production , Purification , and Application ) Sakshi Ghasle
 
18-04-UA_REPORT_MEDIALITERAСY_INDEX-DM_23-1-final-eng.pdf
18-04-UA_REPORT_MEDIALITERAСY_INDEX-DM_23-1-final-eng.pdf18-04-UA_REPORT_MEDIALITERAСY_INDEX-DM_23-1-final-eng.pdf
18-04-UA_REPORT_MEDIALITERAСY_INDEX-DM_23-1-final-eng.pdfssuser54595a
 
Organic Name Reactions for the students and aspirants of Chemistry12th.pptx
Organic Name Reactions  for the students and aspirants of Chemistry12th.pptxOrganic Name Reactions  for the students and aspirants of Chemistry12th.pptx
Organic Name Reactions for the students and aspirants of Chemistry12th.pptxVS Mahajan Coaching Centre
 
Kisan Call Centre - To harness potential of ICT in Agriculture by answer farm...
Kisan Call Centre - To harness potential of ICT in Agriculture by answer farm...Kisan Call Centre - To harness potential of ICT in Agriculture by answer farm...
Kisan Call Centre - To harness potential of ICT in Agriculture by answer farm...Krashi Coaching
 
A Critique of the Proposed National Education Policy Reform
A Critique of the Proposed National Education Policy ReformA Critique of the Proposed National Education Policy Reform
A Critique of the Proposed National Education Policy ReformChameera Dedduwage
 

Recently uploaded (20)

Presentation by Andreas Schleicher Tackling the School Absenteeism Crisis 30 ...
Presentation by Andreas Schleicher Tackling the School Absenteeism Crisis 30 ...Presentation by Andreas Schleicher Tackling the School Absenteeism Crisis 30 ...
Presentation by Andreas Schleicher Tackling the School Absenteeism Crisis 30 ...
 
Q4-W6-Restating Informational Text Grade 3
Q4-W6-Restating Informational Text Grade 3Q4-W6-Restating Informational Text Grade 3
Q4-W6-Restating Informational Text Grade 3
 
Call Girls in Dwarka Mor Delhi Contact Us 9654467111
Call Girls in Dwarka Mor Delhi Contact Us 9654467111Call Girls in Dwarka Mor Delhi Contact Us 9654467111
Call Girls in Dwarka Mor Delhi Contact Us 9654467111
 
Concept of Vouching. B.Com(Hons) /B.Compdf
Concept of Vouching. B.Com(Hons) /B.CompdfConcept of Vouching. B.Com(Hons) /B.Compdf
Concept of Vouching. B.Com(Hons) /B.Compdf
 
Introduction to AI in Higher Education_draft.pptx
Introduction to AI in Higher Education_draft.pptxIntroduction to AI in Higher Education_draft.pptx
Introduction to AI in Higher Education_draft.pptx
 
APM Welcome, APM North West Network Conference, Synergies Across Sectors
APM Welcome, APM North West Network Conference, Synergies Across SectorsAPM Welcome, APM North West Network Conference, Synergies Across Sectors
APM Welcome, APM North West Network Conference, Synergies Across Sectors
 
Crayon Activity Handout For the Crayon A
Crayon Activity Handout For the Crayon ACrayon Activity Handout For the Crayon A
Crayon Activity Handout For the Crayon A
 
Código Creativo y Arte de Software | Unidad 1
Código Creativo y Arte de Software | Unidad 1Código Creativo y Arte de Software | Unidad 1
Código Creativo y Arte de Software | Unidad 1
 
MENTAL STATUS EXAMINATION format.docx
MENTAL     STATUS EXAMINATION format.docxMENTAL     STATUS EXAMINATION format.docx
MENTAL STATUS EXAMINATION format.docx
 
CARE OF CHILD IN INCUBATOR..........pptx
CARE OF CHILD IN INCUBATOR..........pptxCARE OF CHILD IN INCUBATOR..........pptx
CARE OF CHILD IN INCUBATOR..........pptx
 
TataKelola dan KamSiber Kecerdasan Buatan v022.pdf
TataKelola dan KamSiber Kecerdasan Buatan v022.pdfTataKelola dan KamSiber Kecerdasan Buatan v022.pdf
TataKelola dan KamSiber Kecerdasan Buatan v022.pdf
 
POINT- BIOCHEMISTRY SEM 2 ENZYMES UNIT 5.pptx
POINT- BIOCHEMISTRY SEM 2 ENZYMES UNIT 5.pptxPOINT- BIOCHEMISTRY SEM 2 ENZYMES UNIT 5.pptx
POINT- BIOCHEMISTRY SEM 2 ENZYMES UNIT 5.pptx
 
mini mental status format.docx
mini    mental       status     format.docxmini    mental       status     format.docx
mini mental status format.docx
 
_Math 4-Q4 Week 5.pptx Steps in Collecting Data
_Math 4-Q4 Week 5.pptx Steps in Collecting Data_Math 4-Q4 Week 5.pptx Steps in Collecting Data
_Math 4-Q4 Week 5.pptx Steps in Collecting Data
 
Presiding Officer Training module 2024 lok sabha elections
Presiding Officer Training module 2024 lok sabha electionsPresiding Officer Training module 2024 lok sabha elections
Presiding Officer Training module 2024 lok sabha elections
 
Hybridoma Technology ( Production , Purification , and Application )
Hybridoma Technology  ( Production , Purification , and Application  ) Hybridoma Technology  ( Production , Purification , and Application  )
Hybridoma Technology ( Production , Purification , and Application )
 
18-04-UA_REPORT_MEDIALITERAСY_INDEX-DM_23-1-final-eng.pdf
18-04-UA_REPORT_MEDIALITERAСY_INDEX-DM_23-1-final-eng.pdf18-04-UA_REPORT_MEDIALITERAСY_INDEX-DM_23-1-final-eng.pdf
18-04-UA_REPORT_MEDIALITERAСY_INDEX-DM_23-1-final-eng.pdf
 
Organic Name Reactions for the students and aspirants of Chemistry12th.pptx
Organic Name Reactions  for the students and aspirants of Chemistry12th.pptxOrganic Name Reactions  for the students and aspirants of Chemistry12th.pptx
Organic Name Reactions for the students and aspirants of Chemistry12th.pptx
 
Kisan Call Centre - To harness potential of ICT in Agriculture by answer farm...
Kisan Call Centre - To harness potential of ICT in Agriculture by answer farm...Kisan Call Centre - To harness potential of ICT in Agriculture by answer farm...
Kisan Call Centre - To harness potential of ICT in Agriculture by answer farm...
 
A Critique of the Proposed National Education Policy Reform
A Critique of the Proposed National Education Policy ReformA Critique of the Proposed National Education Policy Reform
A Critique of the Proposed National Education Policy Reform
 

(1) Post a responsive review to someone elses essay paper from We

  • 1. (1) Post a responsive review to someone else's essay paper from Week #14 (last week) according to the directions that are posted there IN THE FOLLOWING ABBREVIATED (!!) WEEK: Each student will post one review of one of the essay papers that another student has posted. No late reviews will be accepted for any reason. Reviews should be substantively interactive, constructively critical, and at least 35 sentences long. Reviews should be "spread around" so that everyone's essay paper is reviewed by at least one other student. Also, reviews should utilize the FIVE STAR RATING SCHEME feature. The peer rating feature has to do with the 5 stars as shown here (see below). Reviews will be figured into the grade for this "Essay Paper" assignment. ORIGINAL ARTICLE Gender Representation on Gender-Targeted Television Channels: A Comparison of Female- and Male-Targeted TV Channels in the Netherlands Serena Daalmans1 & Mariska Kleemans1 & Anne Sadza1 Published online: 5 January 2017 # The Author(s) 2017. This article is published with open access at Springerlink.com Abstract The current study investigated the differences in the
  • 2. representation of gender on male- and female-targeted channels with regard to recognition (i.e., the actual presence of men and women) and respect (i.e., the nature of that representation or portrayal). To this end, the presence of men and women on two female- and two male-targeted Dutch channels (N = 115 pro- grams, N = 1091 persons) were compared via content analysis. The expectation that men’s channels would portray a less equal and more traditional image of gender than women’s channels was generally supported by the results. Regardless of genre as well as country of origin of the program, women were under- represented on men’s channels, while gender distribution on women’s channels was more equal. The representation of wom- en in terms of age and occupation was more stereotypical on men’s channels than on women’s channels, whereas men were represented in more contra-stereotypical ways (e.g., performing household tasks) on women’s channels. Since television view - ing contributes to the learning and maintenance of stereotyped perceptions, the results imply that it is important to strengthen viewers’ defenses against the effects of gender stereotyping when watching gendered television channels, for instance through media literacy programs in schools. Keywords Gender-targeted channels . Gender stereotyping . Gender representation . Content analysis . Television Over the past decades, research has made it abundantly clear that women are underrepresented in the media and that, when they are present, they are more often than not represented in stereotypical roles (Collins 2011; Emons et al. 2010; Furnham and Paltzer 2010; Lauzen et al. 2008; Signorielli and Bacue 1999). Because the roles of women in society have expanded tremendously as a result of the ongoing process of emancipa- tion, these consistent findings are often seen as remarkable (Collins 2011; Emons et al. 2010; Lauzen et al. 2008; Signorielli and Bacue 1999). However, recent developments
  • 3. in the television landscape may provide new insights on the issue. There are indications that specific gender-targeted genres (e.g., soaps and teen scene) might actually showcase both a more equal distribution of men and women as well as less stereotyping in its gender portrayals (Gerding and Signorielli 2014; Lauzen et al. 2006). Following on from this speculation, the emergence of channels that specifically define men or women as their target group and thus predominantly broadcast gender-targeted genres (also called narrowcasting, Kuipers 2012; Smith-Shomade 2004) might be a promising development with regard to a more representative portrayal of both men and women. However, gender portrayal on such gendered channels is rather unexplored, and is therefore cen- tral to the current study. Narrowcasting, or organizing user groups into specific au- dience markets, started from the 1980s onward in the United States (Kuipers 2012; Lotz 2006). It meant that specific, often gendered, audience groups were targeted via specific pro- gramming and advertising content (Sheperd 2014). Following this pattern, we term narrowcasting on channels that explicitly target either a female or a male audience Bgender-targeted channels^ here. Gender-targeted television channels employing narrowcasting currently form an increas- ingly large portion of the television landscape in a multitude of countries (Kuipers 2012; Smith-Shomade 2004; Van Bauwel * Serena Daalmans [email protected] 1 Behavioural Science Institute, Communication Science, Radboud University, Postbus 9104, 6500, HE Nijmegen, The Netherlands Sex Roles (2017) 77:366–378 DOI 10.1007/s11199-016-0727-6
  • 4. http://crossmark.crossref.org/dialog/?doi=10.1007/s11199-016- 0727-6&domain=pdf 2016). Previous research into narrowcasting has, on the one hand, focused on the emergence of female-targeted channels, such as Lifetime and Oxygen, and its programming strategies, content choices, and rhetoric used to win female audiences and advertisers of female products (Byars and Meehan 1994; Hundley 2002; Lotz 2006; Meehan and Byars 2000; Tankel and Banks 1997). On the other hand, researchers studied whether specific gender-targeted programs such as Sex and The City and Ally McBeal can be considered (post-) feminist texts (Akass and McCabe 2004; Dubrofsky 2002). What re- mains unclear based on previous research is how the gendered focus of such channels and of programs suitable to broadcast on gender-targeted channels affect the representation of men and women in terms of presence and stereotyping. Investigation of the portrayal of men and women in pro- grams broadcast at gender-targeted channels becomes urgent when considering statements by television scholars who label gender-targeting or narrowcasting as a hegemonic practice (Meehan 1990; Sheperd 2014; Smith-Shomade 2004). Moreover, studying gender representation remains of the ut- most importance because watching television still is the most time-consuming pastime (Collins 2011; Signorielli 2012). As a result, television is seen as one of the main institutions as - sociated with disseminating stereotyped views of the world and its gender roles. From the theoretical vantage point of cultivation theory as well as social learning theory, television is confirmed as one of the main agents of socialization (Bandura 1977; Gerbner 1979; Signorielli 2012). Research has shown that television viewing contributes to the mainte- nance as well as the learning (molding) of gender stereotyped perceptions among children, adolescents, and adults (Larson
  • 5. 2001; McGhee and Frueh 1980; Signorielli 1989; Welch et al. 1979). Furthermore, it is generally accepted that television impacts gender socialization in people’s self-image as well as their image of others (Signorielli 2012). Assuming that television has the potential to shape attitudes, self-perception, and behavior—on top of the idea that stereotyping plays a crucial role in the maintenance of power inequalities within wider social, cultural, political, and economic structures (Cottle 2000; Dyer 1993, 1997; Morgan 2007)—it becomes important to analyze and understand the nature of gender-role portrayals on gender-targeted channels. Previous research on gender stereotyping in media has gen- erally focused on two levels of gender stereotyping. The first level of gender stereotyping includes the actual presence of men and women in television programs (whether they ap- pear), termed recognition. The second level focuses on the nature of that representation or portrayal (how they appear), termed respect (Collins 2011; Signorielli and Bacue 1999). Research into narrowcasting remains inconclusive about whether the representation of men and women on gender- targeted channels differs from the overall established patterns of underrepresentation of women (i.e., recognition) and stereotyping portrayal of both men and women (i.e, respect). Moreover, known previous research into narrowcasting has only studied female-targeted channels (Byars and Meehan 1994; Hundley 2002; Lotz 2006; Meehan and Byars 2000; Tankel and Banks 1997), but a comparison between the gen- der representation on male-targeted and female-targeted chan- nels is lacking. This comparison is important because, if these gender-targeted channels differ in how they represent gender, this might lead to differing conceptions of gender roles and gender aspirations between their male and female target audi - ences (Gerding and Signorielli 2014).
  • 6. In all, the question that is central to the current study is: What are the differences in the representation of gender on male- and female-targeted Dutch channels with regard to the characteriza- tion concepts of recognition and respect? This question will be investigated by analyzing gender-targeted television channels in the Netherlands, a country in which 40% of the television land- scape currently explicitly targets a gendered target audience (Stichting Kijkonderzoek [SKO] 2014). In their longitudinal analysis of gender on television, Signorielli and Bacue (1999) use presence and gender-role stereotyping as indicators of recognition and respect respec - tively. These concepts stem from a study of televised racial minorities by Clark (1972), who posited that positive changes in the treatment of lower status groups (minorities including women) can be seen as a process that follows two stages: recognition and respect. In gender research, recognition can be seen as the presence of women on the television screen (Lauzen and Dozier 2005; Signorielli and Bacue 1999). Respect is measured by the extent toward which women are not portrayed in stereotypical roles, but rather represented in a diverse manner because this diversity is necessary to be rep- resented fairly and positively (Signorielli and Bacue 1999). Recognition: Presence of Men and Women on Television Recognition is defined as men and women being represented on television proportional to their presence in society (Signorielli and Bacue 1999). Research into gender representation has over - whelmingly found that women are underrepresented on televi- sion compared to their presence in society (Collins 2011; Emons et al. 2010; Glascock 2001; Greenberg and Atkin 1980; Koeman et al. 2007; Signorielli and Bacue 1999; Tedesco 1974). The last decades have shown a trend towards a more equal distribution of male and female characters (Elasmar
  • 7. et al. 1999; Emons et al. 2010; Glascock 2001; Greenberg and Collette 1997; Lauzen and Dozier 2005; Vande Berg and Streckfuss 1992), but increases are often small and women re- main underrepresented (Koeman et al. 2007; Segijn et al. 2014). There are some indications that levels of over- or under- representation are connected to the gender of the target Sex Roles (2017) 77:366–378 367 audience. Gunter (1986) found that soap operas in 1970s that were specifically geared toward a female audience had an equal distribution of male and female characters. Gerding and Signorielli (2014) similarly found that whereas females were underrepresented in ‘tween programs geared towards male adolescents, the programs geared towards female ado- lescents mirrored the U.S. population. Based on previous re- sults we posit that women will be underrepresented compared to their presence in society on men’s channels, but not on women’s channels (Hypothesis 1). Previous studies have revealed that television genres can differ in the ways they represent gender. It has been shown that gender stereotyping is surely still present, but has generally re - vealed a trend of decreasing stereotyping in genres such as tel e- vision fiction (Emons et al. 2010; Greenwood and Lippman 2010; Gunter 1986; Signorielli and Bacue 1999), teen scene programming (Gerding and Signorielli 2014), and advertising (Wolin 2003). Moreover, the portrayal of women in these genres has become more representative of the lives and status of con- temporary women. In contrast, other studies—mostly on televi- sion advertising, but also on fictional programs—conclude that women are still underrepresented and portrayed in a stereotyp- ical way and that the degree of stereotyping is even worsening
  • 8. (Allen and Coltrane 1996; Bretl and Cantor 1988; Ganahl et al. 2003; Harwood and Anderson 2002; Koeman et al. 2007; Milner and Higgs 2004). Based on the latter results, some have argued that, due to its continued gender-stereotyped nature, tele- vision forms a lagging social indicator, which reflects Bhow the economy or society was rather than how it is or how it will be^ (Estes 2003, p.4; see also Emons 2011; Kim and Lowry 2005). Taken together these results reveal that there is inconsisten- cy in the literature about the relation between genre and gen- der-stereotyping. These possible genre differences become relevant and important when combined with the increased attention from a cultivation perspective that has been given to the possibility of genre-specific cultivation effects (Bilandzic and Busselle 2008; Bilandzic and Rössler 2004; Cohen and Weimann 2000; Grabe and Drew 2007; Morgan and Shanahan 2010). Following the idea put forth by Hawkins and Pingree (1981) that different TV genres may cultivate different views of the world, research has revealed large dif- ferences between genres (Gomes and Williams 1990; Koeman et al. 2007; Pennekamp 2011). Because men’s and women’s channels cater to different expected audiences most likely with a selection of different gendered genres, this might influ- ence the gender representation on these channels as a whole. This then leads to the following research question: Do genre- differences play a role in the presence of men and women on gender-targeted television channels? (Research Question 1). The country of origin of the selected programming might also play a role in gender representational differences between channel types. Previous research has revealed that country of origin plays a significant role in the degree of stereotyping present in the representation of gender (Emons et al. 2010; Furnham and Paltzer 2010; Wiles et al. 1995). For example,
  • 9. Emons et al. (2010) found that U.S. programs on Dutch tele- vision represented more male adults, more women involved in childcare, more men involved in a job, and fewer males in- volved in other activities compared to Dutch programs on the same channels. Based on this comparison, they conclude that American programs on Dutch television are more gender- stereotyped than programs of Dutch origin. Their research indicates that gender portrayals on television can be artifacts of the culture of the society they were created and thereby potentially reflective of the degree of gender equality in the culture of origin. Because the Dutch television landscape hosts a large degree of foreign (especially American) pro- gramming, it becomes interesting to see how this affects gen- der representation on Dutch gendered channels (Kuipers 2008, 2011). We therefore pose the following research ques- tion: Do differences in country of origin of the program play a role in the presence of men and women on gender-targeted television channels? (Research Question 2). Respect: Stereotyping in Gender Representation Analyzing the presence of women in the television world is only a relatively small aspect the representation of women on TV. BA more complete understanding of how women are portrayed on television comes from examining the type and depth of the roles in which they are cast – what Clark (1972) referred to as respect^ (Signorielli and Bacue 1999, p. 530). We therefore also investigate how women appear in television programs. The first indicator of respect analyzed in the present study is age. Studies have found that inequalities in the age of tele- vised men and women persist because women are continually represented as younger than their male counterparts are (Emons et al. 2010; Glascock 2001; Signorielli and Bacue 1999). The fact that men on television are predominantly rep-
  • 10. resented as older and therewith are perceived to be wiser than their female counterparts can be interpreted as women being given less respect (Signorielli and Bacue 1999). Furthermore, previous research on gender-targeted children’s programming has revealed that programs geared toward a boy audience often showcased rather traditional gender-biased por- trayals, whereas programs geared toward a girl audience less often featured gender-stereotypical roles and sometimes even showcased counter-stereotypical roles (Banet-Weiser 2004; Gerding and Signorielli 2014; Leaper et al. 2002; Northup and Liebler 2010; Thompson and Zerbinos 1995). Taken together with previous research on age as an indicator of respect (Emons et al. 2010; Glascock 2001; Signorielli and Bacue 1999), we expect a more traditional image on men’s channels than on women’s channels. This leads to the expectations that a larger percentage of women will be young adults on men’s channels 368 Sex Roles (2017) 77:366–378 than on women’s channels (Hypothesis 2a) and a larger percent- age of men will be adults on men’s channels than on women’s channels (Hypothesis 2b). The second aspect of respect that we will analyze focuses on the social roles in which men and women are cast (Emons et al. 2010; Gerbner 1995; Greenberg and Atkin 1980; Gunter 1986; Signorielli and Bacue 1999). Studies showed that televised men are more likely to be cast in occupational roles, whereas tele - vised women are more likely to be cast in nurturing or marital roles (Gunter 1986; Lauzen et al. 2008; Signorielli and Bacue 1999; Tedesco 1974). Televised women are traditionally repre - sented as housewives who perform housekeeping chores and are preoccupied with family life (Emons et al. 2010; Gunter
  • 11. 1986; Koeman et al. 2007; Signorielli and Bacue 1999). The results on gendered representation of social roles, combined with the previously outlined results regarding the predominance of traditionally gender-stereotyped portrayals in programming geared towards boys and a more gender-balanced representation in programming geared towards girls (Banet-Weiser 2004; Gerding and Signorielli 2014; Leaper et al. 2002; Northup and Liebler 2010; Thompson and Zerbinos 1995), leads us to expect that women on men’s channels will be represented performing household/caregiving tasks more often than women on women’s channels (Hypothesis 3a) and men on men’s channels will be represented performing household/caregiving tasks less often than men on women’s channels (Hypothesis 3b). In line with this reasoning, studies have found women to be underrepresented with regard to being professionally employed (Elasmar et al. 1999; Gunter 1986; Coltrane and Adams 1997; McNeil 1975; Signorielli 1989; Signorielli and Bacue 1999), which is the next indicator for respect that we will analyze. Over time, there appears to be an increase of women represented as having a job and a decrease of men represented as have a job (Signorielli and Bacue 1999). Again, however, this trend is not straightforward because the occupational status of men and women on television tends to fluctuate (Emons et al. 2010). Nevertheless, due to the expectedly more gender-traditional rep- resentation on men’s channels (Banet-Weiser 2004; Gerding and Signorielli 2014; Leaper et al. 2002; Northup and Liebler 2010; Thompson and Zerbinos 1995), we predict that women will be granted less respect on men’s channels in terms of oc- cupational status. Thus we hypothesize that women on men’s channels will be represented as being professionally employed less often than women on women’s channels (Hypothesis 4a) and that men on men’s channels will be represented as being professionally employed more often than men on women’s channels (Hypothesis 4b).
  • 12. The suggestion that family life and parenthood are of greater significance to women than to men is also implicit in the fact that parental status is more often made explicit for women than for men (Davis 1990; Emons et al. 2010; Glascock 2001; Gunter 1986; McNeil 1975). The final indicator of respect that we will analyze, therefore, is parental status. Again there is a trend toward a more egalitarian distribution of known parental status for characters over time. The percentages of known pa- rental status decreased from 81% for women and 54% for men in McNeil’s (1975) study to 56% for women and 42% for men in Glascock’s (2001) study. Again, a more traditional represen- tation is expected to be more prevalent on men’s channels than on women’s channels (cf. Banet-Weiser 2004; Gerding and Signorielli 2014; Leaper et al. 2002; Northup and Liebler 2010; Thompson and Zerbinos 1995). We therefore expect women on men’s channels to be represented as mothers more often than women on women’s channels will be (Hypothesis 5a) and men on men’s channels to be represented as fathers less often than men on women’s channels will be (Hypothesis 5b). Combined, these hypotheses test differences in the levels of recognition and respect given to men and women on men’s and women’s television channels by testing differences in terms of stereotypical or counter-stereotypical role portrayals between the two channel types, comparable to what Gerding and Signorielli (2014) did in their study on ‘tween programs. As we argued, these differences may impact the ideas that men and women have about what are acceptable gender-role pat- terns and what to expect from oneself and others. Method To test our hypotheses, we conducted a quantitative content analysis of gender-role portrayals of 1091 characters in tele-
  • 13. vision 115 programs aired during primetime on four gender- specific television channels in the Netherlands in 2014. All four gendered cable channels on the Dutch television were analyzed: RTL7 and Veronica as men’s channels and RTL8 and Net5 as women’s channels. This distinction was made clear in the explicit statements on their marketing pages as well as their slogans broadcasted during their televised com- mercial breaks (e.g., BEverything women love,^ BWhat wom- en want,^ BRTL7 knows what men want,^ and BMore for men^). Examples of programs on women’s channels are 15 Kids and Counting and Sex and The City; men’s channels broadcast, for instance, Top Gear and programs about soccer. All included channels are commercial broadcasters because Dutch public service broadcasters do not identify specific tar- get audience but are (by law) aimed at informing and enter- taining the general population (Koeman et al. 2007). A check of the validity of the distinction in gendered chan- nels was conducted by verifying the audience profiles, based on gendered viewer ratings, for each channel (Stichting Kijkonderzoek [SKO] 2014, p. 39). The ratings revealed that the audience profile of the male channels RTL 7 and Veronica is indeed male-dominated, with respectively 64.0% and 55.1% of the viewers in 2014 being male. The women’s chan- nels RTL8 and Net5 were shown to have a more female audi - ence. Women represented 64.2% of the audience watching Sex Roles (2017) 77:366–378 369 RTL8, and 63.5% of the audience watching Net5. In terms of market share, the male channels RTL7 (5.0%) and Veronica (4.4%) had a combined market share of just below 10%, whereas the women’s channels RTL8 (2.4%) and Net5 (3.5%) had a combined market share of just below 6%
  • 14. (Stichting Kijkonderzoek [SKO] 2014). Sample For each of the four channels, five primetime evenings (6 PM–midnight) of broadcasting were coded and analyzed. The evenings were recorded as part of a larger clustered sam- ple of several constructed weeks and the channels were there- fore recorded on consecutive days over the course of several weeks (29 March 2014–12 May 2014). This type of sampling leads to more representative results than recording an actual week (Riffe et al. 2005) because television channels some- times have thematic weeks that may not be representative of year-long programming. All programs were analyzed but only if the complete episode was aired within the timeframe of the sample. The context unit of our study was one episode, there- fore only information that was shown in the specific episode was used for coding. A total number of 56 programs (48.3%) were broadcasted on a women’s channel: NET5 (26, 22.6%) and RTL8 (30, 26.1%), and 59 programs (51.7%) were broadcasted on a men’s channel: RTL7 (24, 20.9%) and Veronica (35, 30.4%). From the total of 1091 characters that were present in the programs, 597 (54.7%) were presented on men’s chan- nels; 494 (45.3%), on women’s channels. Recording Units and Coding Procedure The two channel types constitute the units of analysis for our study. Recording units were storylines (for fictional programs) and items (for entertainment and reality programs) within pro- grams and the main characters or persons who were featured in them. Entertainment and reality as a genre contained programs, such as Masterchef and Top Gear, whereas fiction as a genre contained both comedic fiction, such as Two and a Half Men
  • 15. and Mike & Molly, and dramatic fiction, such as Criminal Minds and The Bold and the Beautiful. The women’s channels sample consisted of 56 programs, of which 34 (60.7%) were fictional and 22 (39.3%) were entertainment and reality. The men’s chan- nels sample consisted of 59 programs, of which 30 (50.9%) were fictional and 29 (49.1%) were entertainment and reality. Coding initially differentiated between fictional and non- fictional programming, of which the latter included the genres News and Information and Entertainment and Reality. However, the results revealed that there were actually no pro- grams in the sample which belonged to the News and Information genre. Based on Emons et al. (2010) up to ten items or storylines, each with up to eight persons, were coded. For fiction these eight characters were selected by coding only main characters in the episode. A main character was defined as a character who plays a leading role in the narrative and whose choices and behavior were essential for the development of the plot (Egri 1960; Lauzen and Dozier 2005; Weijers 2014). In entertainment and reality programs, the eight persons who had the most speaki ng and/or screen time per item were coded. Show hosts and re- porters were coded but excluded from the analyses because they differed from the rest of the population as a result of their func - tion rather than their gender or the channel type on which they were portrayed (i.e., every show host always has an occupation; marital and parental status are very rarely made explicit). Moreover, only a minority of the initial sample consisted of these types of characters (n = 50). In addition, animated persons from three program broadcast on men’s channels were also excluded.
  • 16. The coding instrument used to analyze the persons and characters in the sample (see Table 1 for coding definitions, categories, and frequencies) was developed using prior studies of prime-time television (Davis 1990; Elasmar et al. 1999; Emons et al. 2010; Glascock 2001; Greenberg and Atkin 1980; Koeman et al. 2007; Signorielli and Bacue 1999). Two coders were involved in the coding process. Coders re- ceived coder training and independent practice on programs that were not part of the sample. The coders independently coded practice materials and then compared and discussed the results. The coding instrument was edited after these discus- sions to fix potential problems prior to coding and analysis. After the final revisions in the coding instrument, a little over 10% of the program sample (n = 14; 12.1%) was randomly selected to be double coded. Intercoder reliabilities were calculated in SPSS using the macro by Hayes for Krippendorff’s alpha (Hayes and Krippendorff 2007). All variables were analyzed as nominal variables except for age, which was analyzed as an ordinal variable. The cut-off points were defined based on Lombard et al. (2002) who suggest that Krippendorff’s alpha coeffi - cients of .90 or higher are always acceptable and .80 or higher are acceptable in most situations. As reported in Table 1, intercoder reliabilities are acceptable for all variables (Kalphas > .87). Results Presence as an Indicator of Recognition The first hypothesis stated that women would be underrepre- sented on men’s channels, but not on women’s channels, com- pared to their presence in (Dutch) society (Hypothesis 1). In 2014, 49.5% of the Dutch population (N = 16,829,289) was male whereas 50.5% of the population was female (Centraal
  • 17. 370 Sex Roles (2017) 77:366–378 Bureau voor de Statistiek 2014). The gender distribution on men’s and women’s channels was examined with a Chi square goodness of fit test whereby the societal percentage was used as the expected value. The results showed that women were significantly underrepresented on men’s channels compared to their presence in society. Women made up only 22% of the cast, whereas men (78%) were much more present com- pared to the gender distribution in society, χ2(1, n = 597) = 192.48, p < .001, Cramer’s V = .57. In contrast, women (and men) on women’s channels were not underrep- resented. Their presence did not differ significantly from so- ciety in terms of gender. Women composed 48.6% of the population whereas men composed 51.4%, χ2(1, n = 494) = .73, p = .394, Cramer’s V = .04. In all, the results provide support for Hypothesis 1 because underrepresentation of women was only found on men’s channels. The first research question asked if the genre of the pro- grams presented on men’s and women’s channels would lead to a differing presence of men and women on these channels. The results revealed that, based on the adjusted residuals on men’s channels, the percentage of men significantly exceeded expected frequencies within the entertainment and reality genre (87.9%, adjusted residuals =8.1), whereas the percent- ages for women (although still underrepresented) significantly exceeded expected frequencies within the fictional genre on men’s channels (40.1%, adjusted residuals =8.1), χ2(1, n = 597) = 65.20, p < .001, Cramer’s V = .33. In contrast, on women’s channels the percentage of women significantly exceeded expected frequencies within the entertainment and reality genre (54.8%, adjusted residuals =2.4), whereas the
  • 18. percentage of men (55.9%, adjusted residuals = 2.4) signifi - cantly exceeded expected frequencies in fictional genre, χ2(1, n = 494) = 5.57, p = .018, Cramer’s V = .11. In all, there is a more pronounced difference in the representation of gender on men’s channels in different genres than on women’s channels, where gender is more evenly divided. The second research question asked if country of origin of the programs presented on men’s and women’s channels would lead to a differing presence of men and women on these channels. Overall, results showed that women were underrep- resented in programming from all countries. However, on men’s channels in programs created in the United States, the percentage of women significantly exceeded expected fre- quencies (34%, adjusted residuals =8.0) with the percentage of men (66.0%) falling below expected rates, χ2(2, n = 597) = 64.85, p < .001, Cramer’s V = .33. Comparatively, men are starkly overrepresented in program- ming from both the Netherlands (95.2%, adjusted residuals = 5.8) and Other countries (91.1%, adjusted residuals =3.7), with the percentage of women falling below expected rates (NL: 4.8%, Other: 8.9%). In contrast, on women’s channels there were no significant differences between countries of origin in the presence of men and women, χ2(2, n = 494) = 4.35, p = .114, Cramer’s V = .10. These results prompted a closer inspection of the particular programming originating in the Netherlands and Other coun- tries in the sample. Programs on men’s channels that were created in The Netherlands were predominantly sports talk shows (n = 4) and reality crime shows (n = 5). We need to note here that Dutch programs constitute only 17.9% of the Table 1 Overview of coded variables
  • 19. Variable Definition Categories Kalpha Frequencies n (%) Genre Genre to which the program belongs (N = 115) Entertainment and Reality Fiction 1.00 52 (45%) 63 (55%) Country of origina Country of origin of the program (N = 115) The Netherlands United States, Great-Britain / Other 1.00 22 (20%) 85 (73%) 4 (3.5%) / 4 (3.5%) Gender Person or character’s gender (N = 1091) Male Female .98 720 (66%) 371 (34%) Age Person or character’s age in terms of the life cycle (N = 1091) Child (0–12) Teenager (13–18) Young adult (19–34) Adult (35–49)
  • 20. Middle-aged (50–64) Senior (65+) .87 21 (1.9%) 45 (4.1%) 408 (37.4%) 455 (41.7%) 116 (10.6%) 46 (4.2%) Household and caregiving tasks Whether the person or character engages in household or caregiving tasks (for example: cleaning, doing the laundry, or taking children to school) (N = 1091) Yes No .93 71 (6.5%) 1020 (93.5%) Employment Whether the person or character is portrayed as being professionally employed or explicitly mentions having a job (N = 1091) Yes No Unknown .96 681 (62.4%) 20 (1.8%)
  • 21. 390 (35.7%) Parental status Whether the person or character is portrayed as a parent or explicitly mentions being a parent (N = 1091) Yes No Unknown .96 147 (13.7%) 19 (1.8%) 905 (84.5%). a In the analyses, the categories of BGreat-Britain^ and BOther^ were combined due to low cell frequencies Sex Roles (2017) 77:366–378 371 programming on men’s channels. All other programs were originally from United States (71.4%) or from Great Britain and other countries (10.7%). Programs on men’s channels from other countries were all male-oriented shows such as Top Gear, the famous BBC car talk show. Comparatively, the programs aired on men’s channels originating in the United States are a little more mixed, consisting of for exam- ple the reality crime show Cops as well as comedy shows such as Mike & Molly and drama such as Criminal Minds. Age as Indicator of Respect The second set of hypotheses stated that women would be
  • 22. represented as young adults on men’s channels more often than on women’s channels (Hypothesis 2a), whereas men would be portrayed as adults on men’s channels more often than on women’s channels (Hypothesis 2b). The distribution of women along age categories differed significantly between channel types, χ2 (5, n = 372) = 13.70, p = .018, Cramer’s V = .19 (see Table 2a). In accordance with hypothesis 2a, the residual analysis revealed that on men’s channels over half of all women (56.8%, adjusted residuals =2.7) were portrayed as young adults which was significantly more than the 42.1% on women’s channels. As predicted in Hypothesis 2b, 49.2% of men (adjusted residuals =2.9) were represented as adults on men’s channels, and this was also significantly more than the 37.8% on women’s channels, χ2(5, n = 719) = 19.04, p = .002, Cramer’s V = .16. Although not hypothesized, these results also revealed that adult women (40.0%, adjusted residuals =2.8) were significantly more present on women’s channels than on men’s channels (25.8%). As the percentages above already indicate, most of main characters in the sample were either young adults or adults. The other age categories that we discerned (child, teenager, middle-aged adults, and seniors) appeared less frequent (12.3– 14.6% for middle aged men, all other percentages <7.5%, see Table 2a). No notable differences in presence of men and women of these age categories on either men’s or women’s channels were found, except for significantly more male chil - dren on women’s channels (3.9%, adjusted residuals = 3.5) than on men’s channels (.4%). Tasks as Indicators of Respect We expected that women on men’s channels would be over- represented performing household or caregiving tasks com- pared to women on women’s channels (H3a). We did not find
  • 23. support for this hypothesis, since 12.1% of women on men’s channels and 11.7% of women on women’s channels per- formed these tasks and these numbers did not differ signifi - cantly, χ2(1, n = 372) = .017, Cramer’s V = .007, p = .897 (see Table 2b). It was further hypothesized that men on men’s channels would be significantly underrepresented performing house- hold or caregiving tasks compared to men on women’s chan- nels (Hypothesis 3b). The results supported this hypothesis as Table 2 Indicators of respect for women and men on gender- targeted channels Represented women Represented men Indicators Men’s channels Women’s channels Men’s channels Women’s channels (a) Age Child 3.8% 1.7% .4% 3.9%* Teenager 4.5% 4.2% 3.7% 4.7% Young adult 56.8%* 42.1% 31.0% 34.6% Adult 25.8% 40.0%* 49.2%* 37.8% Middle aged 3.0% 7.5% 12.3% 14.6% Senior 6.1% 4.6% 3.4% 4.3% n 132 240 465 254 (b) Household and caregiving tasks Yes 12.1% 11.7% 1.5% 7.9%* No 87.9% 88.3% 98.5%* 92.1% n 132 240 465 254
  • 24. (c) Employment Yes 43.2% 57.1%* 64.9% 72.8%* No 3.0% 4.6% .4% 1.2% Unknown 53.8%* 38.3% 34.3%* 26.0% n 132 240 465 254 (d) Parental status Yes 19.7% 23.8% 5.4% 15.4%* No .8% 5.4%* .2% 1.6%* Unknown 79.5% 70.8% 94.4%* 83.1% n 132 240 465 254 * Frequency significantly exceeded expectations by adjusted standardized residuals 372 Sex Roles (2017) 77:366–378 7.9% of men on women’s channels performed household or caregiving tasks (adjusted residuals =4.3) compared to only 1.5% of men on men’s channels, χ2(1, n = 719) = 18.44, p < .001, Cramer’s V = .16 (see Table 2b). Employment as Indicator of Respect Women on men’s channels were expected to be represented as having an occupation less often than women on women’s channels (Hypothesis 4a), whereas men on men’s channels were expected to be represented as having an occupation more often than men on women’s channels (Hypothesis 4b). A sig- nificantly larger percentage of women (57.1%, adjusted resid- uals = 2.6) was indeed portrayed as having an occupation on
  • 25. women’s channels than on men’s channels (43.2%), χ2 (2, n = 372) = 8.31, p = .016, Cramer’s V = .15 (see Table 2c). This finding confirms Hypothesis 4a. Contrary to Hypothesis 4b, men on men’s channels were portrayed as having an oc- cupation significantly less often than men on women’s chan- nels were (see Table 2c). On men’s channels, 64.9% (adj usted residuals = − 2.6) of the men were portrayed as having an occupation compared to 72.8% of men on women’s channels, χ2(2, n = 719) = 6.75, p = .035, Cramer’s V = .10. Furthermore, the results also revealed a significant overrepre- sentation within the unknown category of occupation for both women (53.8%, adjusted residuals = 2.9) and men (34.6%, adjusted residuals = 2.4) on men’s channels. Parental Status as Indicator of Respect The final set of hypotheses predicted that women on men’s channels would be represented as mothers more often than women on women’s channels (Hypothesis 5a) and, converse- ly, that men on men’s channels would be represented as fathers less often than men on women’s channels (Hypothesis 5b). The results revealed that women’s parental state significantly differed between the channel types, χ2(2, n = 372) = 6.41, p = .040, Cramer’s V = .13 (see Table 2d). However, the residual analysis shows that this difference is due to the knowledge of explicit childlessness of women between chan- nel types, where women were significantly more often explic- itly childless on women’s channels (5.4%, adjusted residuals = 2.3) than on men’s channels (.8%). There was no significant difference between the representation of women as mother between men’s channels (19.7%) and women’s channels (23.8%). We thus need to reject Hypothesis 5a. The results also indicated that men were, as expected in Hypothesis 5b, significantly more often represented as fathers (15.4%, adjust- ed residuals = 4.5) on women’s channels than on men’s chan- nels (5.4%), χ2(2, n = 719) = 25.08, p < .001, Cramer’s V = .19
  • 26. (see Table 2d). Hypothesis 5b is thus supported. Discussion The primary goal of our study was to provide insight into the way that gender was represented on television channels targeting ei- ther men or women. The overarching expectation that, due to the target audience, men’s channels would portray a less equal and more traditional image of gender than women’s channels would in terms of both recognition and respect was generally supported by our results. In line with the results of Gerding and Signorielli (2014), our study revealed that whereas women were grossly underrepresented on men’s channels, gender distribution on women’s channels mirrored the Dutch population. This phenom- enon cannot be explained by the idea that audiences would prefer watching members of their own gender because women should then have been overrepresented on women’s channels just like men are on men’s channels. Because this was not the case, we can conclude that in contrast to women’s channels, men’s chan- nels show a lack of recognition for women. Moreover, an explo- ration of the level of recognition per genre and per country of origin of the programs on the gendered channels also revealed that women’s channels, regardless of genre as well as the country of origin of the program, showcased a more equal presence of men and women than men’s channels did. In all, this means that only women’s channels fulfill the first of the two stages towards positive and fair treatment of women: recognition (Clark 1972; Lauzen and Dozier 2005; Signorielli and Bacue 1999). In addition, our results revealed that men’s channels also
  • 27. lagged behind in terms of the second stage of positive and equal portrayal (Clark 1972): respect. First, the stereotypical value of youth for women (as an indicator of a lack of respect, see also Davis 1990; Emons et al. 2010; Lauzen and Dozier 2005; Signorielli and Bacue 1999) is much more pronounced on men’s channels than it is on women’s channels. Women on women’s channels were not only represented as older than they were on men’s channels, but they were also distributed more evenly over the age categories of young adult and adult. Second, in terms of occupational status, women on women’s channels were more likely to be portrayed as being profession- ally employed than women on men’s channels were. However, the results for the other indicators of respect re- vealed a less straightforward conclusion. As for the percentage of women performing household or caregiving tasks and the overrepresentation of women as parents, there were no signifi - cant differences between men’s and women’s channels. However, it should be noted that women’s channels were more likely than men’s channels were to represent men as performing household and caregiving tasks and being parents. Household and caregiving tasks especially were divided rather equally be - tween both genders—in contrast to the results by Emons et al. (2010). Therefore we conclude that at least the degree of gender stereotyping (at least in terms of these indicators) is lower on women’s channels and that they thereby still grant more respect to women and men than men’s channels do. Sex Roles (2017) 77:366–378 373 The most striking result found in our study is that it is the representation of men (rather than women) in which the true differences between men’s and women’s channels are found. Men are represented in counter-stereotypical ways on women’s
  • 28. channels and become connected to the home and family almost as much as women. This focus on home and family life on women’s channels could be interpreted as a post-feminist resur- gence of the focus on gender differences and stereotypical fem- inine values on women’s channels (Gill 2007; McRobbie 2009). Another, more positive, interpretation might, however, be that the private domain is being re-evaluated. Women’s channels seem to present an image in which home and family life are important—for both men and women. This might be interpreted as the private domain itself increasingly gaining respect on these channels. Also, besides their connection to family life, high percentages of both men and women are represented as being professionally employed on these channels, resulting in a more diverse depiction of both genders. Limitations and Directions for Future Research Although the findings here are of clear importance in adding to our understandings of the differences in gender representa- tion on gender-targeted channels, there are some limitations to our study that should be addressed. First, the sample consisted of a constructed week recorded on consecutive days. Therewith, we did not control for seasonal differences in pro- gramming. Recording several weeks, one in each season may contribute to the representativeness of the results. Second, in terms of variables we decided to code as many of the aspects that were associated with the central concepts of recognition and respect as discerned by Clark (1972) and Signorielli and Bacue (1999). To this end, we closely followed previous codebooks (Davis 1990; Elasmar et al. 1999; Emons et al. 2010; Glascock 2001; Greenberg and Atkin 1980; Koeman et al. 2007; Signorielli and Bacue 1999) in the operationalization of our variables. As a consequence, we were not able to explore every one of the aspects in an in-
  • 29. depth manner. For example, household and caregiving tasks were coded as either being performed or not performed. Because of this, if one character was portrayed performing household tasks for an entire episode and another performed only one such task, both were coded as performing these tasks in the same way. It is recommended that future research would measure the number of tasks performed by each character so as to gain more insight into gendered task distribution. Furthermore, with regard to the variable of occupation, the coding based on previous work relatively simplistically reflected if a person had a job or not (or if it was unknown). Even though the differentiation between having a job or not captures implicit messages of gendered worth in the public sphere, future research should add a dimension of gender-role stereotyping to this vari- able in building on previous work (Coltrane and Adams 1997; Glascock 2001; Signorielli 1989; Signorielli and Bacue 1999; Signorielli and Kahlenberg 2001). Future research could establish for gendered channels if there are differences between men and women with regard to the type of occupation, the prestige asso- ciated with that occupation, as well as the authority they have over other workers in that occupation in order to determine how stereotypical the representation of work for men and women is on these channels. This is important because media portrayals pro- vide one of the sources of information against which people in modern societies give meaning to in their work and family lives (Coltrane and Adams 1997; Signorielli and Kahlenberg 2001). Seen in this light, the media’s previously documented tendency of linking men with jobs in which they have authority and wom- en with jobs with less prestige and less authority over other workers (Coltrane and Adams 1997; Glascock 2001; Signorielli
  • 30. 1989), potentially aids in the perpetuation of gender stereotypes and the maintenance of the gendered status quo. Third, we decided to exclude show hosts and reporters in entertainment and reality programs because there was almost no variation with regard to the dependent variables. Although their presence was low compared to the other characters who were coded, their representation is important against the back- ground of the current study. For instance, talk shows are fre- quently watched, and the position of host carries a certain amount of authority and expertise with it, implying that the gender representation of show hosts may also contribute to viewers’ stereotyped views of the world and its gender roles. Previous research has already outlined that men form the ma- jority of show hosts in programs devoted to Bhard^ content (e.g., politics and economy), whereas women are dominant as show hosts for Bsoft^ topics (e.g., family and romance) (Gerbner 1995; Koeman et al. 2007; Segijn et al. 2014). Therefore, future research should include an in-depth analysis of these roles on gendered channels to assess potential differences. In addition to including show hosts and reporters in future analyses, it might be interesting to explore the relationship of the gender of individuals behind the scenes of the gender - targeted television channels (e.g., managers, writers, directors, advertisers) and the on-screen portrayal of men and women. This is particularly interesting because it could explore the assumption which some media analysts and scholars have put forward that if more women had positions of authority behind the scenes in the entertainment world, the media would offer less gender-stereotyped portrayals of men and women (Benét 1978; Lauzen and Dozier 1999; Mills 1988). Some of these dynamics have been explored for prime-time program- ming by other researchers (e.g., Glascock 2001; Lauzen and Dozier 1999; Lauzen et al. 2006; Lauzen et al. 2008), but
  • 31. never systematically for gendered television channels. A final recommendation for future research would be to complete a cultivation analysis to measure how the audiences’ conceptions of gender are affected by the programming on these gendered channels (Gerbner et al. 1978; Morgan 2007; 374 Sex Roles (2017) 77:366–378 Signorielli 1989; Signorielli and Bacue 1999). This directive for future cultivation research is further strengthene d by re- search based in social role theory (Eagly and Wood 2012) which proposed that gender stereotypes might change when gender distribution changes (Diekman and Eagly 2000), and it is encountered in daily life or through mediated exposure (Eagly and Steffen 1984). Future research could then assess if more traditional or progressive role-pattern expectancies are indeed cultivated in the audiences of men’s and women’s channels respectively. This would necessitate surveying viewers of both types of channels with varying levels of ex- posure to television about their conceptions of gender roles. As outlined previously, some of the findings presented in our study are concerning; however, until a cultivation analysis is conducted, the actual influence of these programs on actual perceptions of gender and gender roles remain unknown. Practice Implications Television broadcasters work fervently to entertain and keep their audiences watching and loyal, all in order to turn a profit by selling advertising space to makers of products and services aimed at specific (gendered) niche audience groups (Kuipers 2012; Turow 1998). However, through their politics of narrow - casting, viewers—and particularly viewers of men’s channels
  • 32. our study points out—are at risk for developing too narrow conceptions about gender roles that may prove to be limiting in real life. Because research has shown that television viewing contributes to the maintenance, as well as the learning, of gender stereotyped perceptions (Gerbner et al. 1978; Larson 2001; McGhee and Frueh 1980; Welch et al. 1979), we would there- fore argue that the everyday television viewer should be made aware of the presence of stereotyping on television. A possible way to do this is to pay attention to this issue in media literacy programs, which are part of school curricula in an increasing number of schools (Koltay 2011; Tuominen and Kotilainen 2012). In these lessons, educators at all levels of education should sensitize their students about gender-role depictions in television programming and the (possible) effects they have on men and women. In this way, they would ideally be providing a continuous strengthening of children’s, adolescents, and young adults’ defenses against the effects of gender stereotyping they may encounter when watching various (gendered) television channels throughout their life. Conclusion To conclude, some context for the rise of men’s and women’s channels might be provided by the idea that we now live in a post-feminist era in which there has been a resurgence of the belief in sexual difference (Gill 2007; McRobbie 2009). It has also been suggested that the increased status of women in society has put pressure on the concept of masculinity (Beynon 2002), which could explain the more traditional gen- der portrayal on channels aimed at male audiences. However, whatever the cause of the matter may be, our study makes clear that rather than mirroring emancipatory changes in soci - ety via a trend towards a more equal representation of gender on television, the Dutch television landscape has become di -
  • 33. vided through some large differences in the way gender is represented on men’s versus women’s channels. Taken together, we can conclude that, in the Netherlands, it seems that a more equal image of gender might be cultivated particularly for female audiences, while the messages cultivated for male audiences remain highly gender-stereotyped. These findings highlight a wor- risome phenomenon when combined with several com- plexities in gender-related practices and attitudes in the Netherlands. The Netherlands is generally regarded as one of the more gender-equal countries in the world (United Nations Development Program [UNDP], 2015), due for example to the continuously growing working force participation of women, the increasing percentage of women with a college degree, the increase of women with a seat in parliament, a growing participation of men in household chores as well as childcare in the last de- cades, and a continuous increase in the share of individ- uals who do not favor gender stereotypes in upbringing, education, and the workplace (Arends-Tóth and van de Vijver 2007; Collier et al. 2013; Emons 2011; Gesthuizen et al. 2002; UNDP 2015). However, the Netherlands also recently dropped three places in the Gender Gap Index (World Economic Forum 2016) due to the fact that in the Netherlands women’s workforce participation rate is still lower than men’s. Women gener- ally have part-time jobs, earn considerably less than men do, and are severely underrepresented in senior executive positions. Considering these complex and conflicting real - ities in gender-related practices and attitudes in the Netherlands, we feel that the results of our study seen in the light of cultivation theory (Gerbner et al. 1978) and social learning theory (Bandura 1977), combined with the persistent finding that men in general tend to hold more traditional gender views than women do (Bolzendahl and
  • 34. Myers 2004; Brewster and Padavic 2000; Cameron and Lalonde 2001), highlight issues that are important and relevant to consider when examining the potential effects of these gendered representations. As such, the gendered representation on particularly men’s TV channels might form a roadblock that stands in the way of true emanci- pation and ideas of gender equality being reinforced in the minds of not only women but particularly men. Compliance with Ethical Standards The authors declare that they have no conflict of interest and that they have complied with the APA ethical standards. Sex Roles (2017) 77:366–378 375 Open Access This article is distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License (http:// creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided you give appro- priate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons license, and indicate if changes were made. References Akass, K., & McCabe, J. (2004). Reading sex and the City. London: I. B. Tauris.
  • 35. Allen, K., & Coltrane, S. (1996). Gender displaying television commer- cials: A comparative study of television commercials in the 1950s and 1980s. Sex Roles, 35(3/4), 185–203. doi:10.1007/BF01433106. Arends-Tóth, J., & van de Vijver, F. J. R. (2007). Cultural and gender differences in gender-role beliefs, sharing household task and child- care responsibilities, and well-being among immigrants and major- ity members in the Netherlands. Sex Roles, 57(11/12), 813–824. doi:10.1007/s11199-007-9316-z. Bandura, A. (1977). Social learning theory. London: Prentice- Hall. Banet-Weiser, S. (2004). Girls rule!: Gender, feminism, and nickelodeon. Critical Studies in Media. Communication, 21, 119–139. doi:10.1080/07393180410001688038. Benét, J. W. (1978). Will media treatment of women improve? In G. Tuchman, A. K. Daniels, & J. W. Benét (Eds.), Hearth and home: Images of women in the mass media (pp. 266–271). New York: Oxford University Press. Beynon, J. (2002). Masculinities and the notion of 'crisis'. In J. Beynon (Ed.), Masculinities and culture (pp. 75–97). Buckingham: Open University.
  • 36. Bretl, D. J., & Cantor, J. (1988). The portrayal of men and women in U.S. television commercials: A recent content analysis and trends over 15 years. Sex Roles, 18(9/10), 595–609. doi:10.1007/BF00287963. Brewster, K. L., & Padavic, I. (2000). Change in gender- ideology, 1977– 1996: The contributions of intracohort change and population turn- over. Journal of Marriage and Family, 62(2), 477–487. doi:10.1111 /j.1741-3737.2000.00477.x. Bilandzic, H., & Busselle, R. W. (2008). Transportation and transport- ability in the cultivation of genre-consistent attitudes and estimates. Journal of Communication, 58(3), 508–529. doi:10.1111/j.1460- 2466.2008.00397.x. Bilandzic, H., & Rössler, P. (2004). Life according to television. Implications of genre-specific cultivation effects: The gratification/ cultivation model. Communications, 29, 295–326. doi:10.1515 /comm.2004.020. Bolzendahl, C. I., & Myers, D. J. (2004). Feminist attitudes and support for gender equality: Opinion change in women and men, 1974– 1998. Social Forces, 83(2), 759–789. doi:10.1353/sof.2005.0005. Byars, J., & Meehan, E. R. (1994). Once in a lifetime:
  • 37. Constructing Bthe working woman^ through cable narrowcasting. Camera Obscura, 11, 12–41. doi:10.1215/02705346-11-12-3-1_33-34-12. Cameron, J. E., & Lalonde, R. N. (2001). Social identification and gender-related ideology in women and men. British Journal of Social Psychology, 40(1), 59–77. doi:10.1348/014466601164696. Centraal Bureau voor de Statistiek (2014). Statline. Retrieved from http://statline.cbs.nl. Clark, C. (1972). Race, identification, and television violence. In G. A. Comstock, E. A. Rubinstein, & J. P. Murray (Eds.), Television and social behavior. Vol. 5.Television's effects: Further explorations (pp. 120–184). Washington, DC: U.S. Government Printing Office. Cohen, J., & Weimann, G. (2000). Cultivation revisited: Some genres have some effects on some viewers. Communication Reports, 13(2), 99–114. doi:10.1080/08934210009367728. Collier, K. L., Bos, H. M. W., Merry, M. S., & Sandfort, T. G. M. (2013). Gender, ethnicity, religiosity, and same-sex sexual attraction and the acceptance of same-sex sexuality and gender non-conformity. Sex Roles, 68, 724–737. doi:10.1007/s11199-012-0135-5. Collins, R. L. (2011). Content analysis of gender roles in media:
  • 38. Where are we now and where should we go? Sex Roles, 64(3–4), 290– 298. doi:10.1007/s11199-010-9929-5. Coltrane, S., & Adams, M. (1997). Work–family imagery and gender stereotypes: Television and the reproduction of difference. Journal of Vocational Behavior, 50(2), 323–347. doi:10.1006 /jvbe.1996.1575. Cottle, S. (Ed.) (2000). Ethnic minorities and the media: Changing cul- tural boundaries. Buckingham: Open University Press. Davis, D. M. (1990). Portrayals of women in prime-time network televi- sion: Some demographic characteristics. Sex Roles, 23(5–6), 325– 332. doi:10.1007/BF00290052. Diekman, A. B., & Eagly, A. H. (2000). Stereotypes as dynamic con- structs: Women and men of the past, present, and future. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 26, 1171–1188. doi:10.1177 /0146167200262001. Dubrofsky, R. (2002). Ally McBeal as postfeminist icon: The aestheticiz- ing and fetishizing of the independent working woman. The Communication Review, 5 , 265–284. doi:10.1080 /10714420214690. Dyer, R. (1993). The matter of images: Essays on
  • 39. representations. London. New York: Routledge. Dyer, R. (1997). White. London. New York: Routledge. Eagly, A. H., & Steffen, V. J. (1984). Gender stereotypes stem from the distribution of women and men into social roles. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 46(4), 735–754. doi:10.1037 /0022-3514.46.4.735. Eagly, A. H., & Wood, W. (2012). Social role theory. A biosocial analysis of sex differences and similarities. In P. A. M. Van Lange, A. W. Kruglanski, & E. T. Higgins (Eds.), Handbook of theories in social psychology (pp. 458–476). London: Sage. Egri, L. (1960). The art of dramatic writing. New York: Simon & Schuster. Elasmar, M., Hasegawa, K., & Brain, M. (1999). The portrayal of women in U.S. prime time television. Journal of Broadcasting & Electronic Media, 43(1), 20–34. doi:10.1080/08838159909364472. Emons, P. A. A. (2011). Social-cultural changes in Dutch society and their representations in television fiction, 1980–2005. Nijmegen: Knust. Emons, P. A. A., Wester, F., & Scheepers, P. (2010). BHe
  • 40. works outside the home; she drinkscoffee and does the dishes^: Gender roles in fiction programs on Dutch television. Journal of Broadcasting & Electronic Media, 51(1), 40–53. doi:10.1080/08838150903550386. Estes, R. J. (2003). Global change and indicators of social development. Retrieved from http://www.sp2.upenn.edu/restes/Estes%20 Papers/Global%20Change%20Indicators_2003.pdf. Furnham, A., & Paltzer, S. (2010). The portrayal of men and women in television advertisements: An updated review of 30 studies pub- lished since 2000. Scandinavian Journal of Psychology, 51(3), 216–236. doi:10.1111/j.1467-9450.2009.00772.x. Ganahl, D. J., Prinsen, T. J., & Netzley, S. B. (2003). A content analysis of prime time commercials: A contextual framework of gender repre- sentation. Sex Roles, 49(9/10), 545–551. doi:10.1023 /A:1025893025658. Gerbner, G. (1979). Television’s influence on values and behaviour. Massacommunicatie, 7, 215–222. Gerbner, G. (1995). Casting and fate: Women and minorities on television drama, gameshows and news. In E. Hollander, P. Rutten, & L. Van der Linden (Eds.), Communication, culture & community: Liber amicorum James Stappers (pp. 125–137). Houten: Bohn Stafleu van Loghum.
  • 41. 376 Sex Roles (2017) 77:366–378 http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/BF01433106 http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s11199-007-9316-z http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/07393180410001688038 http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/BF00287963 http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1741-3737.2000.00477.x http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1741-3737.2000.00477.x http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1460-2466.2008.00397.x http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1460-2466.2008.00397.x http://dx.doi.org/10.1515/comm.2004.020 http://dx.doi.org/10.1515/comm.2004.020 http://dx.doi.org/10.1353/sof.2005.0005 http://dx.doi.org/10.1215/02705346-11-12-3-1_33-34-12 http://dx.doi.org/10.1348/014466601164696 http://dx.doi.org/http://statline.cbs.nl http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/08934210009367728 http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s11199-012-0135-5 http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s11199-010-9929-5 http://dx.doi.org/10.1006/jvbe.1996.1575 http://dx.doi.org/10.1006/jvbe.1996.1575 http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/BF00290052 http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/0146167200262001 http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/0146167200262001 http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/10714420214690 http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/10714420214690 http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.46.4.735 http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.46.4.735 http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/08838159909364472 http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/08838150903550386 http://www.sp2.upenn.edu/restes/Estes%20Papers/Global%20Ch ange%20Indicators_2003.pdf http://www.sp2.upenn.edu/restes/Estes%20Papers/Global%20Ch ange%20Indicators_2003.pdf http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9450.2009.00772.x
  • 42. http://dx.doi.org/10.1023/A:1025893025658 http://dx.doi.org/10.1023/A:1025893025658 Gerbner, G., Gross, L., Jackson-Beeck, M., Jeffries-Fox, S., & Signorielli, N. (1978). Cultural indicators: Violence profile no. 9. Journal of Communication, 28(3), 176–207. doi:10.1111/j.1460-2466.1978. tb01646.x. Gerding, A., & Signorielli, N. (2014). Gender roles in tween television programming: A content analysis of two genres. Sex Roles, 70(1), 43–56. doi:10.1007/s11199-013-0330-z. Gesthuizen, M., Scheepers, P., & Verloo, M. (2002). Support for the discrimination of women on the labour market in the Netherlands: Individual and contextual characteristics. Netherlands Journal of Social Sciences, 38(1), 48–64. Gill, R. (2007). Postfeminist media culture: Elements of a sensibility. European Journal of Cultural Studies, 10(2), 147–166. doi:10.1177/1367549407075898. Glascock, J. (2001). Gender roles on prime-time network television: Demographics and behaviors. Journal of Broadcasting & Electronic Media, 45(4), 656–669. doi:10.1207/s15506878 jobem4504_7.
  • 43. Gomes, R. C., & Williams, L. F. (1990). Race and crime: The role of the media in perpetuating racism and classism in America. Urban League Review, 14(1), 57–69. Grabe, M. E., & Drew, D. G. (2007). Crime cultivation: Comparisons across media genres and channels. Journal of Broadcasting & E l e c t ron i c M e di a , 5 1( 1) , 1 4 7– 1 7 1. do i : 1 0 . 1 08 0 /08838150701308143. Greenberg, B. S., & Atkin, C. K. (1980). Life on television: Content analyses of U.S. TV drama. Norwood, NJ: Ablex Pub. Corp. Greenberg, B. S., & Collette, L. (1997). The changing faces on TV: A demographic analysis of network television’s new seasons, 1966- 1992. Journal of Broadcasting and Electronic Media, 41(1), 1– 13. doi:10.1080/08838159709364386. Greenwood, D. N., & Lippman, J. R. (2010). Gender and media: Content, uses, and impact. In J. C. Chrisler & D. R. McCreary (Eds.), Handbook of gender research in psychology (pp. 643–669). New York: Springer. Gunter, B. (1986). Television and sex role stereotyping. London: Libbey. Harwood, J., & Anderson, K. (2002). The presence and portrayal of social groups on prime-time television. Communication Reports, 15,
  • 44. 81– 97. doi:10.1080/08934210209367756. Hawkins, R. P., & Pingree, S. (1981). Uniform messages and habitual viewing: Unnecessary assumptions in social reality effects. Human Communication Research, 7, 291–301. Hayes, A. F., & Krippendorff, K. (2007). Answering the call for a stan- dard reliability measure for coding data. Communication Methods and Measures, 1, 77–89. doi:10.1080/19312450709336664. Hundley, H. (2002). The evolution of gendercasting: The lifetime televi- sion network-Btelevision for women^. Journal of Popular Film and Television, 29(4), 174–181. doi:10.1080/01956050209601023. Kim, K., & Lowry, D. T. (2005). Television commercials as a lagging social indicator: Gender role stereotypes in Korean television adver- tising. Sex Roles, 53(11/12), 901–910. doi:10.1007/s11199-005- 8307-1. Koeman, J., Peeters, A. L., & d'Haenens, L. S. J. (2007). Diversity mon- itor 2005: Diversity as a quality aspect of television in the Netherlands. Communications, 32(1), 97–121. doi:10.1515 /commun.2007.005. Koltay, T. (2011). The media and the literacies: Media literacy, informa-
  • 45. tion literacy, digital literacy. Media, Culture & Society, 33(2), 211– 221. doi:10.1177/0163443710393382. Kuipers, G. (2008). De VS in Europese huiskamers [the Unitest states in European living rooms]. Sociologie, 4(2/3), 174–194. Kuipers, G. (2011). Cultural globalization as the emergence of a transna- tional cultural field: Transnational television and national media landscapes in four European countries. American Behavioral Scientist, 55(5), 541–557. Kuipers, G. (2012). South Park boys and sex and the City women: Television trade, narrowcasting and the export of gender categories. Interactions: Studies in Communication & Culture, 2(3), 179– 196. Larson, M. S. (2001). Interactions, activities and gender in children’s television commercials: A content analysis. Journal of Broadcasting & Electronic Media, 45(1), 41–56. doi:10.1207 /s15506878jobem4501_4. Lauzen, M. M., & Dozier, D. M. (1999). Making a difference in prime time: Women on screen and behind the scenes in the 1995–1996 television season. Journal of Broadcasting and Electronic Media, 43, 1–43. doi:10.1080/08838159909364471. Lauzen, M. M., & Dozier, D. M. (2005). Recognition and respect
  • 46. revisited: Portrayals of age and gender in prime-time television. Mass Communication & Society, 8(3), 241–256. doi:10.1207 /s15327825mcs0803_4. Lauzen, M. M., Dozier, D. M., & Cleveland, E. (2006). Genre matters: An examination of women working behind the scenes and on- screen portrayals in reality and scripted prime-time programming. Sex Roles, 55, 445–455. doi:10.1007/s11199-006-9100-5. Lauzen, M. M., Dozier, D. M., & Horan, N. (2008). Constructing gender stereotypes through social roles in prime-time television. Journal of Broadcasting & Electronic Media, 52(2), 200–214. doi:10.1080 /08838150801991971. Leaper, C., Breed, L., Hoffman, L., & Perlman, C. (2002). Variations in the gender-stereotyped content of children’s television cartoons across genres. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 32, 1653– 1662. doi:10.1111/j.1559-1816.2002.tb02767.x. Lombard, M., Snyder-Duch, J., & Bracken, C. C. (2002). Content anal- ysis in mass communication. Human Communication Research, 28(4), 587–604. doi:10.1111/j.1468-2958.2002.tb00826.x. Lotz, A. D. (2006). Redesigning women: Television after the network era. Urbana: University of Illinois Press. McGhee, P., & Frueh, T. (1980). Television viewing and the learning of sex-role stereotypes. Sex Roles, 6(2), 179–188. doi:10.1007
  • 47. /BF00287341. McNeil, J. C. (1975). Feminism, femininity, and the television series: A content analysis. Journal of Broadcasting, 19(3), 259–271. doi:10.1080/08838157509363786. McRobbie, A. (2009). The aftermath of feminism: Gender, culture and social change. London: Sage. Meehan, E. (1990). Why we don't count: The commodity audience. In P. Mellenkamp (Ed.), Logics of television: Essays in cultural criticism (pp. 117–137). Bloomington, IN: University Press. Meehan, E. R., & Byars, J. (2000). Telefeminism: How lifetime got its groove, 1984-1997. Television & New Media, 1(1), 33–51. doi:10.1177/152747640000100103. Mills, K. (1988). A place in the news: From the women's pages to the front page. New York: Dodd, Mead. Milner, L. M., & Higgs, B. (2004). Gender sex-role portrayals in interna- tional television advertising over time: The Australian experience. Journal of Current Issues and Research in Advertising, 26, 81– 95. doi:10.1080/10641734.2004.10505166. Morgan, M. (2007). What do young people learn about the world from
  • 48. watching television? In S. R. Mazzarella (Ed.), 20 questions about youth & the media(pp. 153–166). New York: Peter Lang Publishing, Inc. Morgan, M., & Shanahan, J. (2010). The state of cultivation. Journal of Broadcasting & Electronic Media, 54(2), 337–355. doi:10.1080 /08838151003735018. Northup, T., & Liebler, C. M. (2010). The good, the bad, and the beau- tiful: Beauty ideals on the Disney and nickelodeon channels. Journal of Children and Media, 4(3), 265–282. doi:10.1080 /17482798.2010.496917. Pennekamp, S. (2011). Monitor representatie 2010 [monitor representa- tion 2010]. Den Haag: NPO. Sex Roles (2017) 77:366–378 377 http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1460-2466.1978.tb01646.x http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1460-2466.1978.tb01646.x http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s11199-013-0330-z http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/1367549407075898 http://dx.doi.org/10.1207/s15506878jobem4504_7 http://dx.doi.org/10.1207/s15506878jobem4504_7 http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/08838150701308143 http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/08838150701308143 http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/08838159709364386 http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/08934210209367756 http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/19312450709336664 http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/01956050209601023 http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s11199-005-8307-1
  • 49. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s11199-005-8307-1 http://dx.doi.org/10.1515/commun.2007.005 http://dx.doi.org/10.1515/commun.2007.005 http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/0163443710393382 http://dx.doi.org/10.1207/s15506878jobem4501_4 http://dx.doi.org/10.1207/s15506878jobem4501_4 http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/08838159909364471 http://dx.doi.org/10.1207/s15327825mcs0803_4 http://dx.doi.org/10.1207/s15327825mcs0803_4 http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s11199-006-9100-5 http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/08838150801991971 http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/08838150801991971 http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1559-1816.2002.tb02767.x http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1468-2958.2002.tb00826.x http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/BF00287341 http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/BF00287341 http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/08838157509363786 http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/152747640000100103 http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/10641734.2004.10505166 http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/08838151003735018 http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/08838151003735018 http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/17482798.2010.496917 http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/17482798.2010.496917 Riffe, D., Lacy, S., & Fico, F. (2005). Analyzing media messages: Using quantitative content analysis in research. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum. Segijn, C., Bartholomé, G., Pennekamp, S., & Timmers, M. (2014). De afbeelding van statusverschillen in sekse en etniciteit in Nederlandse non-fictieprogramma’s [the portrayal of status difference
  • 50. associated with gender and ethnicity in Dutch non-fictional programs]. Tijdschrift voor Communicatiewetenschap, 42(3), 305–320. doi:10.5553/TCW/138469302014042003008. Sheperd, T. (2014). Gendering the commodity audience in social media. In C. Carter, L. Steiner, & L. McLaughlin (Eds.), The Routledge companion to media & gender (pp. 157–167). London: Routledge. Signorielli, N. (1989). Television and conceptions about sex roles: Maintaining conventionality and the status quo. Sex Roles, 21(5– 6), 341–360. doi:10.1007/BF00289596. Signorielli, N. (2012). Gender-role socialization in the twenty- first cen- tury. The International Encyclopedia of Media. Studies, 5(2), 1– 23. doi:10.1002/9781444361506.wbiems116. Signorielli, N., & Bacue, A. (1999). Recognition and respect: A content analysis of prime-time television characters across three decades. Sex Roles, 40(7–8), 527–544. doi:10.1023/a:1018883912900. Signorielli, N., & Kahlenberg, S. (2001). Television’s world of work in the nineties. Journal of Broadcasting & Electronic Media, 45, 4–22. doi:10.1207/s15506878jobem4501_2. Stichting Kijkonderzoek. (2014). SKO Year Report 2014.
  • 51. Retrieved from https://kijkonderzoek.nl/images/SKO_Jaarrapport/SKO_ jaarrapport_2014.pdf. Smith-Shomade, B. (2004). Narrowcasting in the new world information order. A space for the audience? Television & New Media, 5(1), 69– 81. doi:10.1177/1527476403259746. Tankel, J. D., & Banks, J. (1997). Lifetime television and women: Narrowcasting as electronic space. In S. J. Drucker & G. Gumpert (Eds.), Voices in the street: Explorations in gender, media, and pub- lic space (pp. 255–270). Cresskill, NJ: Hampton Press. Tedesco, N. S. (1974). Patterns in prime time. Journal of Communication, 24(2), 119–124. doi:10.1111/j.1460-2466.1974.tb00376.x. Thompson, T., & Zerbinos, E. (1995). Gender roles in animated cartoons: Has the picture changed in 20 years? Sex Roles, 32, 651–673. doi:10.1007/BF01544217. Tuominen, S., & Kotilainen, S. (2012). Pedagogies of media and infor- mation literacies. Retrieved from http://iite.unesco.org/pics/publica- tions/en/files/3214705.pdf. Turow, J. (1998). Breaking up America: Advertisers and the new media world. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.
  • 52. United Nations Development Program. (2015). Human Development Report. Retrieved from http://hdr.undp.org/sites/default/files/2015_ human_development_report.pdf. Van Bauwel, S. (2016). Gendered television genres, the same old song: A case study of gendered television genres in the north of Belgium. In K. Ross & C. Padovani (Eds.), Gender equality and the media: A challenge for Europe (pp. 60–71). New York: Routledge. Vande Berg, L. R., & Streckfuss, D. (1992). Prime-time television’s por- trayal of women and the world of work: A demographic profile. Journal of Broadcasting & Electronic Media, 36(2), 195–208. doi:10.1080/08838159209364167. Weijers, A. (2014). The craft of screenwriting. Den Haag: Boom Lemma. Welch, R. L., Huston-Stein, A., Wright, J. C., & Plehal, R. (1979). Subtle sex-role cues in children's commercials. Journal of Communication, 29, 202–209. doi:10.1111/j.1460-2466.1979.tb01733.x. Wiles, J. A., Wiles, C. R., & Tjernlund, A. (1995). A comparison of gender role portrayal in magazine advertising: The Netherlands, Sweden and the USA. European Journal of Marketing, 29(11), 35–49.
  • 53. Wolin, L. D. (2003). Gender issues in advertising – an oversight synthesis of research – 1970-2002. Journal of Advertising Research, 43(1), 111–129. World Economic Forum. (2016). The Global Gender Gap Report 2016. Retrieved from https://www.weforum.org/reports/ the-global- gender-gap-report-2016/. 378 Sex Roles (2017) 77:366–378 http://dx.doi.org/10.5553/TCW/138469302014042003008 http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/BF00289596 http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/9781444361506.wbiems116 http://dx.doi.org/10.1023/a:1018883912900 http://dx.doi.org/10.1207/s15506878jobem4501_2 https://kijkonderzoek.nl/images/SKO_Jaarrapport/SKO_jaarrapp ort_2014.pdf https://kijkonderzoek.nl/images/SKO_Jaarrapport/SKO_jaarrapp ort_2014.pdf http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/1527476403259746 http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1460-2466.1974.tb00376.x http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/BF01544217 http://iite.unesco.org/pics/publica-tions/en/files/3214705.pdf http://iite.unesco.org/pics/publica-tions/en/files/3214705.pdf http://hdr.undp.org/sites/default/files/2015_human_development _report.pdf http://hdr.undp.org/sites/default/files/2015_human_development _report.pdf http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/08838159209364167 http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1460-2466.1979.tb01733.x https://www.weforum.org/reports/the-global-gender-gap-report- 2016/ https://www.weforum.org/reports/the-global-gender-gap-report-
  • 54. 2016/ Sex Roles is a copyright of Springer, 2017. All Rights Reserved. Gender...AbstractRecognition: Presence of Men and Women on TelevisionRespect: Stereotyping in Gender RepresentationMethodSampleRecording Units and Coding ProcedureResultsPresence as an Indicator of RecognitionAge as Indicator of RespectTasks as Indicators of RespectEmployment as Indicator of RespectParental Status as Indicator of RespectDiscussionLimitations and Directions for Future ResearchPractice ImplicationsConclusionReferences https://doi.org/10.1177/1350508416689095 Organization 2017, Vol. 24(3) 397 –417 © The Author(s) 2017 Reprints and permissions: sagepub.co.uk/journalsPermissions.nav DOI: 10.1177/1350508416689095 journals.sagepub.com/home/org ‘It’s a man’s man’s man’s world’: Music groupies and the othering of women in the world of rock Gretchen Larsen Durham University, UK
  • 55. Abstract Groupies are understood as a particular type of fan that are most commonly associated with rock music. The ‘groupie’ identity is almost exclusively applied to female fans but sometimes also to female music producers and is largely used in a derogatory manner both by the popular media and by fans themselves. This article argues that the ‘groupie’ identity is used to ‘other’ and exclude women from creative production in rock music. This study draws on a rhetorical analysis of five published biographical accounts of groupies to examine how the labeling of certain people as ‘groupies’ works as an othering practice that serves to support and maintain the gendered norms of rock and identifies three underlying discursive processes. First, popular and music media played a significant role in stereotyping groupie as female right from the emergence of the label. Second, the notions of ‘credibility’ and ‘authenticity’, which are central to serious music journalism, are constructed in such a way as to stigmatize and therefore exclude women from rock, primarily by reframing ‘groupies’ as inauthentic consumers rather than proper fans. Finally, the intertwining of femininity with fandom, as occurs in groupiedom, serves to magnify cultural assumptions about women as sex objects and as passive consumers of mass culture. In elucidating both the gender and marketplace role politics at play in the ‘groupie’ identity and the mechanisms involved in othering women, space is opened in which alternative possibilities for understanding and enacting the role of women in rock can be imagined. Keywords Consumer, creative industries, fans, gender, groupies, music,
  • 56. patriarchy Introduction The figure of the groupie looms large in the discourses and social imaginary surrounding rock music, playing an integral role in the mythology of ‘sex, drugs and rock n’ roll’. Groupies can be Corresponding author: Gretchen Larsen, Department of Management and Marketing, Durham University Business School, Durham University, Queen’s Campus, University Boulevard, Thornaby, Stockton- on-Tees TS17 6BH, UK. Email: [email protected] Article 689095ORG0010.1177/1350508416689095OrganizationLarsen research-article2017 https://uk.sagepub.com/en-gb/journals-permissions https://journals.sagepub.com/home/org mailto:[email protected] http://crossmark.crossref.org/dialog/?doi=10.1177%2F13505084 16689095&domain=pdf&date_stamp=2017-05-10 398 Organization 24(3) found across a range of culture, leisure, and sports activities (Forsyth and Thompson, 2007; Gauthier and Forsyth, 2000; Gmelch and San Antonio, 1998), but it is with rock music that they are most closely associated. The phenomenon of the groupie gained recognition and took shape as a social identity within the counter-culture of 1960s’ rock music
  • 57. and continues to hold significant cultural currency and power. For example, films such as ‘Almost Famous’ (2000) and ‘The Banger Sisters’ (2002) employ and as such reinforce the media representation of the groupie, and Hill (2013) shows how women feel the need to negotiate that same representation when expressing their fandom of metal music. There is no agreed definition as to who or what a groupie is, but a dominant representation exists in popular media/culture and academic literature of a more extreme type of female fan who seeks intimate emotional and/or sexual relations with musicians (e.g. Cline, 1992; Des Barres, 2005 [1987]; Fonarow, 2006; Forrest, 2010).The consolidation of the groupie identity can be found in a cover article in Rolling Stone magazine in 1969 entitled ‘The Groupies and Other Girls’, by John Burks, Jerry Hopkins, and Paul Nelson. However, as Rhodes (2005) explains, this was not the first mention or use of the term ‘groupie’. As the groupie subculture emerged, several articles about women who could be categorized as groupies were published, such as Tom Wolfe’s (1965) ‘The Girl of the Year’ essay on Baby Jane Holzer. But in the wake of the Rolling Stone article, ‘alternate visions of what a groupie was (or could be) were discarded in favor of that offered by Rolling Stone and its highly sexualized and misogynistic approach to the groupie and rock culture’ (Rhodes, 2005: 137). As Rhodes (2005) then carefully evidences, the Rolling Stone article, which it is important to note is written entirely by males, carried such power that any further negotiation of the groupie identity ceased. Consequently, it has provided what has been for a long time, the definitive statement of what groupies are.
  • 58. Warwick (2007) summarizes this view of the groupie as a kind of female fan assumed to be more interested in sex with rock stars than in their music. Groupies are understood to be ‘easy’ [i.e. sexually promiscuous], with low self-esteem, and too stupid about music to be proper fans, but also—paradoxically—predatory and exploitative of the hapless musicians whose artistry they cruelly ignore in their lust for celebrity sex. (p. 170) which she argues is an unmistakably derisive and pejorative description focusing almost entirely on the sexual motivations of groupies. One of the greatest concerns is that the label ‘groupie’ is almost exclusively applied to females and has become a term used to describe all female fans, wives, and girlfriends and even those females who work in rock music (Davies, 2001). This labe- ling reduces the experiences of all women in rock to a singular one driven by sex and effectively excludes them from productive participation. It is to this crucial issue that the article turns, as it examines how the labeling of certain people as ‘groupies’ works as an othering practice that serves to support and maintain the gendered norms of rock and thus exclude women from creative production. Beyond a surface-level recognition, we know little about groupies other than that they are reduced to some kind of caricature used in a derogatory manner both by the popular media (Davies, 2001) and by fans (Hill, 2013) and are ‘treated like a punch line to a never ending joke that only the boys are in on’ (Forrest, 2010: 135). However, as we prise
  • 59. beneath the surface, we begin to see that the groupie identity sits at the intersection between the social identities of gender and market- place role. Gender has functioned as a primary site for the analysis and interpretation of diversity in the creative industries (e.g. Dean, 2008; Nixon and Crewe, 2004; Proctor-Thomson, 2013; Sang et al., 2014). This is in large part because creativity, creative work, and creative identities are con- structed in such a way that women are marginalized or even excluded. In the music industry, women are underrepresented at all levels and in all roles (Leonard, 2015); discounted in the music Larsen 399 press and media (Davies, 2001), and even where they have been acknowledged, their participation is represented in gendered and often highly objectified ways (Hatton and Trautner, 2011). While, as Schippers (2000) notes, rock music is a setting in which gender norms have sometimes been challenged, particularly those related to appearance such as hair length and the use of make-up, this genre of music has long been synonymous with hegemonic masculinity (e.g. Hill, 2014), heter- onormativity (e.g. Frith and McRobbie, 1990 [1978]), and homosociality (e.g. Davies, 2001) which act as pillars upholding the patriarchy of rock.In the immortal words of James Brown and (his lesser known female co-writer and one-time girlfriend) Betty Jean Newsom, ‘it’s a man’s, man’s, man’s world’.
  • 60. In working to exclude women from creative production, the ‘groupie’ identity draws not only on gender identity but also on the dichotomy between work and non-work. The key, relevant social identity is what is called here ‘marketplace role’, which categorizes people according to the producer–consumer dualism. In the creative industries, marketplace roles manifest in dualisms such as artist–audience and musician–fan (e.g. Beauregard, 2012). Like any other social identity, these categories imply subjectivities which define positions in relationships between socio- political actors and carry with them various assumptions and statuses that serve to structure and shape experience and engagement with the creative (e.g. Bradshaw, 2010; Bradshaw et al., 2005). An important assumption is that because a market requires both production and consumption to work, producers and consumers are co-dependent and therefore hold equal status. This assump- tion is underpinned by notions of consumer sovereignty (Rothenberg, 1962), consumer subjectiv- ity (Firat and Dholakia, 2016), and the logic of co-creation (Vargo and Lusch, 2004; Venkatesh and Meamber, 2006). However, not only have producers and consumers largely been approached in academia as separate, independent, and somewhat unrelated entities but also consumers have been historically viewed, particularly in the cultural and creative sectors, as secondary, subordi- nate figures (e.g. Beauregard, 2012; Huyssen, 1986). As noted in the Call for Papers for this Special Issue, much remains to be understood about how gender intersects with other identities in constructing experiences of creativity and creative wor k. There has been little exploration of how
  • 61. gender and marketplace roles intersect to frame who and what type of work is considered to be ‘creative’ or productive and what the practices of inclusion and exclusion are. This article puts forth the argument that groupies are othered in both categories—as women and as consumers, and that in fact it is the intertwining of the two identities that has underpinned and reinforced the groupie identity, and thus helped construct and maintain the patriarchy of rock music. Following a deeper examination of the social identities of gender and marketplace roles in the context of rock music, this article draws on a rhetorical analysis of five published biographical accounts of groupies and rock wives in order to examine how the labeling of certain people as ‘groupies’ works as an othering practice that maintains the gendered norms of rock. Examining the cultural phenomenon of the groupie retrospectively allows the processes behind the construction and maintenance of the identity and its consequences to be explored. Three important discursive processes emerge. First, popular and music media played a significant role in stereotyping groupies right from the emergence of the term. Second, the notions of ‘credibility’ and ‘authenticity’, which are central to serious music journalism, are constructed in such a way as to stigmatize and therefore exclude, discredit, and invalidate the role of women in rock, primarily by reframing ‘groupies’ as inauthentic consumers rather than proper fans. Third, the intertwining of femininity with fandom, as occurs in the construction of the ‘groupie’, serves to magnify cultural assumptions about women as sex objects and as passive consumers of mass culture and
  • 62. thus reinforces the groupie identity and their exclusion from creative work in the world of rock. This article contributes in important ways to a growing body of literature that considers how intersectional social identities are constructed and articulated in rock music (Elafros, 2010) and the 400 Organization 24(3) creative industries. It provides a historically and culturally embedded account of how the ‘groupie’ label works as an othering practice to exclude women from creative work. It resonates with and builds upon other accounts in both the music and creative industries (e.g. on processes of forgetting identified by Strong (2011)) that have written women out of the history of popular culture. It expands our understanding of the role of gender in diversifying the creative by locating the groupie identity as the nexus of gender and marketplace role. Through the processes identified, and contrary to the transgressive and liberatory perspective taken by many of the original groupies, the groupie identity effectively reproduces and reinforces gendered hierarchies within the creative industries. Finally, in elucidating both the gender and marketplace politics at play in the ‘groupie’ label and the discursive processes involved in othering women, space is opened up through which alternative possibilities for understanding and enacting the role of women in rock can be imagined. Gender and rock music
  • 63. The idea that notions of creativity and creative work are gendered is one that is well established in the literature (see, for example, Dean, 2008; Grugulis and Stoyanova, 2012; Huyssen, 1986; Leonard, 2015; Sang et al., 2014). Sustained academic interest in gender and diversity in the crea- tive industries is relatively recent, but the complex and thorny relationship between gender and the creative has long underscored important debates surrounding culture and art. For example, Huyssen (1986) elucidates and then interrogates the modernist notion of mass culture as woman, and real, authentic culture as man. He ultimately argues that while this particular claim has lost its persua- sive power as a result of a combination of feminist activism increasing the presence of women in art, and the postmodern project of the blurring of boundaries between high art and mass culture, gendering of the creative is still pervasive: certain forms of mass culture, with their obsession with gendered violence are more of a threat to women than to men. After all, it has always been men rather than women who have had real control over the production of mass culture. (Huyssen, 1986: 62) This is echoed in more recent research which shows how women remain marginalized and excluded from work and identities in such diverse sectors of the creative industries as film and television (e.g. Bielby and Bielby, 1996; Grugulis and Stoyanova, 2012), acting and performance (e.g. Dean, 2008; Dean and Jones, 2003), and music (e.g. Davies, 2001; Leonard, 2015; Maus, 2011). Even the creative and cultural products that are produced and/or
  • 64. consumed by females are valued less and placed further down the cultural hierarchy than those of their male counterparts, such as in the case of the ‘inferior’ romance novels preferred by Flaubert’s Madame Bovary (Huyssen, 1986); the immensely successful Twilight series (Strong, 2009); and ‘girl groups’, ‘boy bands’, and other types of pop music (Railton, 2001). Strong (2009: p.1) argues that the construction and naturaliza- tion of females as occupants of the lower levels of the cultural hierarchy is a form of ‘symbolic violence that helps reproduce power relations between men and women’. The key issue here is the struggle over the control of cultural production, or who has the power to define what creative work is, who gets to do it, and under what conditions. The rock industry has long been acknowledged and criticized for being gendered male in many facets and norms. First, the style and form of rock music is commonly understood to be both masculine and an expression of male sexuality (Frith and McRobbie, 1990 [1978]). For example, in rock performances, musicians are aggressive, dominating, boastful, and in control; the music is loud and rhythmically insistent; and the lyrics are assertive and arrogant. August (2009) elabo- rates that much, although not all, of the lyrical content of the songs written by the rock band, the Larsen 401 Rolling Stones, is misogynistic and promotes the subordination and objectification of women.
  • 65. ‘Musically, such rock takes off from the sexual frankness of rhythm and blues but adds a cruder male physicality (hardness, control, virtuosity)’ (Frith and McRobbie, 1990 [1978]: 374). Second, in terms of the nature and means of production, the rock music business is run predominantly by males. Musicians, writers, technicians, engineers, producers, and road crew are largely male (Frith and McRobbie, 1990 [1978]). Women remain underrepresented (Leonard, 2015) with roles limited to those that fit with male notions of female ability, for example, singers and publicity agents. Cohen (1997) illustrates how the rock scene is actively produced as male through various institutions and social practices. For example, social interaction inside rock music venues is mas- culine—referring to each other by nicknames; using technical and in-house jargon; and sharing the jokes, myths, and hype that surround the bands on the scene. Third, mainstream rock music press is gendered male in associations, assumptions, and representations of the music and the musicians (Elafros, 2010). The contributions of female musicians are often excluded (Davies, 2001) or represented through the male gaze, as little more than a body. Davies (2001) argues that the British music press uses its own very particular idiom, which is very similar to that found in more explicitly ‘laddish’ magazines such as Loaded: ‘this distinctive idiom is internalized by aspiring music journalists, who realise that they must write in the correct style to be successful. Such compliance and conservatism mean that the sexism of the music press is self-perpetuating’ (p. 316). Finally, even practices of fandom in rock music are gendered male, particularly those
  • 66. that occur publicly and are characterized by communal practices of engagement (O’Reilly et al., 2013). Arguing for a new, more gender-inclusive framework for studying fans of hard rock and heavy metal as a group, Hill (2014) explains that the underlying gendered epistemology of fandom has resulted in a dismissal of women fans or, at best, a systematic reduction of their experiences as fans (such as private engagements with the music, the representation of women fans as groupies, and fannish activities such as reading magazines and participating in online fora). (p. 174) This patriarchy of rock music appears to be held in place by three distinct but interrelated forces and their attendant institutions and practices: hegemonic masculinity, heteronormativity, and homosociality. Hegemonic masculinity, in its original and most prevalent formulation, refers to ‘the pattern of practice […] that allowed men’s dominance over women to continue’ (Connell and Messerschmidt, 2005: 832). As introduced by Connell (1995), hegemonic masculinity embodies the most honored, culturally valued, and therefore ideal way of being a man. While only a minority of men might enact hegemonic masculinity, it is normative and at its most powerful when under- pinned by complicit masculinities (embodied by men who do not conform to all characteristics of hegemonic masculinity but because they do not resist it still benefit from the patriarchy (Connell, 1995)) and compliance among heterosexual women. In effect, it ideologically legitimates the sub- ordination of women and non-hegemonic masculinities, to men. Much of the existing literature on