SlideShare a Scribd company logo
1 of 9
Download to read offline
Full Terms & Conditions of access and use can be found at
http://www.tandfonline.com/action/journalInformation?journalCode=haaw20
Download by: [University of Roehampton], [Stuart Semple] Date: 23 November 2015, At: 06:52
Journal of Applied Animal Welfare Science
ISSN: 1088-8705 (Print) 1532-7604 (Online) Journal homepage: http://www.tandfonline.com/loi/haaw20
Investigating Potential Effects of the Contraceptive
Implanon on the Behavior of Free-Ranging Adult
Female Barbary Macaques
Amanda M. Maijer & Stuart Semple
To cite this article: Amanda M. Maijer & Stuart Semple (2015): Investigating Potential Effects of
the Contraceptive Implanon on the Behavior of Free-Ranging Adult Female Barbary Macaques,
Journal of Applied Animal Welfare Science, DOI: 10.1080/10888705.2015.1083432
To link to this article: http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/10888705.2015.1083432
Published online: 14 Oct 2015.
Submit your article to this journal
Article views: 8
View related articles
View Crossmark data
Investigating Potential Effects of the Contraceptive Implanon on the
Behavior of Free-Ranging Adult Female Barbary Macaques
Amanda M. Maijer and Stuart Semple
Centre for Research in Evolutionary and Environmental Anthropology, University of Roehampton, London,
United Kingdom
ABSTRACT
In recent years, the use of hormonal contraception in captive, free-ranging,
and wild mammal populations has increased, but the effects on these
nonhuman animalsā€™ behavior and the associated welfare impacts remain
poorly understood. This study of free-ranging adult female Barbary macaques
(Macaca sylvanus) at Trentham Monkey Forest compared females implanted
with the progestin-only contraceptive Implanon to those not receiving
contraception. Females with contraceptive implants had higher rates of self-
scratching and spent more time self-grooming (2 behavioral indexes of
anxiety) than those without implants. They also directed more aggression at
others, spent more time receiving grooming and traveling, and spent less
time giving grooming and resting. No signiļ¬cant differences between the 2
groups of females were seen for time spent foraging. These results suggest
that Implanon had a number of effects on Barbary macaques, although these
need to be considered in light of the signiļ¬cant beneļ¬ts afforded by the
use of this contraceptive. The ļ¬ndings of this study provide important
information to those evaluating the relative welfare costs and beneļ¬ts of
alternative methods of population control in this and other species.
KEYWORDS
Behavior; welfare; contra-
ception; anxiety; stress
For many mammals in captivity, excellent husbandry conditions lead to high reproductive rates and
populations rapidly outgrowing the available space (Asa, 1997). Moreover, many species of mammals in
the wild increasingly share space with human populations and ļ¬‚ourish in such environments, leading
to rapid population increases and calls to restrict their numbers (Asa & Porton, 2005). Initially, lethal
control and surgical sterilization were the predominant methods used to control such nonhuman
animal populations, but these techniques are costly and logistically challenging and can have a range
of undesirable welfare consequences (Gray & Cameron, 2010; Kirkpatrick, 2007). More recently,
hormonal contraception has become a popular method to control animal population size (Asa, 2005;
Gray & Cameron, 2010). This approach has been primarily adopted in captive settings (Asa, Porton, &
Calle, 2005; Porton & Dematteo, 2005), but it is also used in free-ranging and wild populations
(Kirkpatrick & Frank, 2005).
The use of hormonal contraceptives in captive and noncaptive mammals gives rise to a number of
concerns, however. When capture is necessary to implant a contraceptive device, it may result in stress
to the individuals concerned and disruption to the social structure of the group (Munson, Moresco, &
Calle, 2005). Even when capture is not needed, or after this event has taken place when it is required, the
biological action of the contraception may have negative welfare impacts (Asa et al., 2005; Munson
et al., 2005). In particular, changes in levels of sex steroids brought about by many hormonal
contraceptives could potentially negatively affect emotional statesā€”and therefore psychological well
beingā€”of the animals in question and/or may modify their behaviors in ways that detrimentally affect
q 2015 Taylor & Francis
CONTACT Stuart Semple s.semple@roehampton.ac.uk Centre for Research in Evolutionary and Environmental Anthropology,
University of Roehampton, Holybourne Avenue, London SW15 4JD, United Kingdom.
JOURNAL OF APPLIED ANIMAL WELFARE SCIENCE
http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/10888705.2015.1083432
Downloadedby[UniversityofRoehampton],[StuartSemple]at06:5223November2015
their welfare. Ultimately, the decision to implement hormonal contraception in mammals must be
based on an analysis of the costs and beneļ¬ts of this method of population control and of the alternative
options (Kirkpatrick, 2007). Detailed information on the effects of hormonal contraception is key to
this decision process but is so far very limited.
Sex steroids can have potent effects on emotional states in humans, with female sex hormones being
linked in particular to increased anxiety (Toufexis, Myers, & Davis, 2006). It has been known for more
than 40 years that the use of hormonal contraceptives can cause negative changes in womenā€™s moods
(Cullberg, 1972; Poromaa & Segebladh, 2012), but the impact of contraception on emotional states in
nonhuman animals remains a neglected area of research (Gray & Cameron, 2010). To our knowledge,
only one study to date has attempted to explore this issue. Plowman, Jordan, Anderson, Condon,
and Fraser (2004) tested zoo-housed hamadryas baboons (Papio hamadryas) for potential negative
psychological effects of Norplant, an implanted contraceptive with an active ingredient with
progesterone-like activity. To do so, they measured the rate of self-directed behaviors, which provides a
useful index of anxiety in humans and nonhuman primates (Maestripieri, Schino, Aureli, & Troisi,
1992; Schino, Perretta, Taglioni, Monaco, & Troisi, 1996). No evidence was found for a change in self-
directed behavior rates linked to contraception use, but as the authors acknowledged, changes in group
size during their study might have masked any such effects.
Evidence that hormonal contraception increases levels of aggressive behavior has been found in
captive long-tailed macaques (Henderson & Shively, 2004) and pigtail macaques (Pazol, Wilson, &
Wallen, 2004). Such effects are of clear welfare concern, as elevated rates of aggression increase the risk
for physical injury and disrupt normal patterns of social organization (Penfold, Patton, & Jochle, 2005).
There is also evidence from captive populations of a range of mammal speciesā€”such as white-tailed
deer (Bertrand, DeNicola, Beissinger, & Swihart, 1996), long-tailed macaques (Shively, Manuck,
Kaplan, & Koritnik, 1990), and golden-headed lion tamarins (De Vleeschouwer, Leus, & Van Elsacker,
2000)ā€”that hormonal contraceptives decrease rates of afļ¬liative behavior. These effects are also
important when assessing welfare impacts, as there is evidence that sociopositive behaviors such as
grooming can reduce anxiety and physiological stress levels (Crockford et al., 2008; Shutt, MacLarnon,
Heistermann, & Semple, 2007; Yates & Aureli, 2010).
In this study, we explored potential impacts of the progestin-only contraceptive Implanon in free-
ranging, adult female Barbary macaques. We tested whether there were differences between females
with and without Implanon implants with respect to behavioral indexes of anxiety (self-directed
behaviors) and measures of aggression and afļ¬liation. To allow for comparison with the broadest range
of previous studies of contraception in animals, in which impacts on general activity patterns were
frequently assessed (Gray & Cameron, 2010), we also tested for potential effects on three measures of
activity: traveling, foraging, and resting. Hormonal contraception is used in both free-ranging (Wallner,
Mƶstl, & Dittami, 2007) and wild populations (Cortes & Shaw, 2006) of our study species and in a
range of other mammal species in captive, free-ranging, and wild settings (Gray & Cameron, 2010).
Our ļ¬ndings will therefore provide valuable information to those evaluating the relative costs and
beneļ¬ts of alternative methods of population control in Barbary macaques and other species.
Materials and methods
Study site and animals
We studied the behavior of 20 adult, female Barbary macaques (age range Ā¼ 5ā€“17 years) living in one
group of the free-ranging population at Trentham Monkey Forest in Stoke-on-Trent, United Kingdom.
The total troop size was 73 individuals: 24 subadult or adult males, 28 subadult or adult females, 16
juveniles, and 5 infants. Barbary macaques live in multimale/multifemale groups characterized by
female philopatry and male dispersal, with close bonds between matrilineal kin (Paul & Kuester, 1987;
Silk, 1987). Of the focal females, 13 were on the progestin-only contraceptive implant Implanon,
which had been administered 1 year to 4 years prior to the start of this study; this implant has been
2 MAIJER AND SEMPLE
Downloadedby[UniversityofRoehampton],[StuartSemple]at06:5223November2015
found to be effective at preventing pregnancy for at least 4 years in this population (S. Wiper, personal
communication, March 14, 2013). Implanon releases the progestin etonogestrel (Bennink, 2000;
Croxatto, 2002). It functions by partially or completely inhibiting ovulation via suppression of midcycle
luteinizing hormone peaks by causing thickening of the cervical mucus, which prevents sperm from
meeting the egg, and by thinning of the endometrium, which prevents implantation (Croxatto &
Makarainen, 1998; Varma & Mascarenhas, 2001).
The researchers did not know the contraception status of each focal female at the start of the
observation period, and their status remained unknown throughout the study except for the ļ¬ve
females who did not have contraceptive implants and gave birth. The selection of females for
contraception was based on a range of factors including their reproductive histories, the sizes of their
matrilines, and how many closely related females were on contraception. Although this was not a truly
random selection process, we did not think it would lead to biases that would affect our study.
In support of this, we found that females with and without contraceptive implants did not differ
signiļ¬cantly in ageā€”analysis of variance (ANOVA), F(1, 18) Ā¼ 3.045, p Ā¼ .098ā€”or in dominance
rankā€”ANOVA, F(1, 18) Ā¼ 1.154, p Ā¼ .297ā€”as assessed from a matrix of the outcomes of dyadic
agonistic interactions.
Data collection
The protocol for this project was approved by the Roehampton University Ethics Committee and by
Trentham Monkey Forest. Behavioral data (220 hr total, 11 hr per female) were collected from April 18,
2011, to June 8, 2011, during 15-min continuous focal observations using a Teklogix Psion handheld
computer loaded with Noldus Observer 5 software. The order of focal samples was randomized to
ensure that they were distributed evenly across the day for each female. The data collection period was
after the end of the breeding season, which ends in late January.
We recorded the duration of time spent giving and receiving grooming, self-grooming, traveling,
foraging, and resting. In addition, we recorded the frequency of occurrence of self-scratching (with
separate bouts of self-scratching deļ¬ned by a break of 3 s or more) and the frequency of occurrence of
aggression (threats, chasing, contact, and biting). For any observations where the focal animal was out
of sight for more than 2 min, the observation was discontinued and discarded and another female was
followed; the original female was followed again at the next available opportunity.
Statistical analysis
The time that the focal animal was out of sight in a focal observation was subtracted from the total
observation time to determine the duration that the animal was in view for that observation. These
values were used to calculate for each focal observation the rates of self-scratching and of aggression
per hour and percentage time engaged in foraging, resting, traveling, giving and receiving grooming,
and self-grooming. For each female, a mean was then calculated across all focal watches for each
behavior. Initial analyses indicated no signiļ¬cant differences in behavior between females not on
contraception who did and did not give birth (Mann Whitney tests, all p . .095), so these females
were grouped together for comparison with the group of females with contraceptive implants.
Data were found to be normally distributed, so ANOVAs were used to compare rates or durations
of behavior between females on contraception and those not on contraception. As both pregnancy
and the birth of an infant may affect femalesā€™ behaviors, we then repeated all analyses using, for the
ļ¬ve females who gave birth, (a) only data from before birth of the infant and (b) only data after the
birth of the infant. Only four results differed with respect to signiļ¬cance between the analyses using
the complete data set and those using data from only before, or only after, birth. For brevity, we
report here only the results for the complete data set but highlight the instances where the signiļ¬cance
of the result changed when using one of the restricted data sets. Analyses were carried out in the
EFFECTS OF IMPLANON ON MACAQUE BEHAVIOR 3
Downloadedby[UniversityofRoehampton],[StuartSemple]at06:5223November2015
Statistical Package for the Social Sciences Version 17.0. All statistical tests were two-tailed,
with a Ā¼ .05.
Results
The rates and time spent in each behavior for females with and without Implanon contraceptive
implants are shown inTable 1. Compared with females not on contraception, females with implants had
signiļ¬cantly higher rates of self-scratchingā€”ANOVA, F(1, 18) Ā¼ 20.878, p , .001ā€”and giving
aggressionā€”ANOVA, F(1, 18) Ā¼ 7.490, p Ā¼ .014ā€”and spent more time self-groomingā€”ANOVA, F(1,
18) Ā¼ 12.365, p Ā¼ .002ā€”receiving groomingā€”ANOVA, F(1, 18) Ā¼ 13.365, p Ā¼ .002ā€”and travelingā€”
ANOVA, F(1, 18) Ā¼ 21.158, p , .001. Females on contraception also spent less time giving groomingā€”
ANOVA, F(1, 18) Ā¼ 4.502, p Ā¼ .048ā€”and less time restingā€”ANOVA, F(1, 18) Ā¼ 6.765, p Ā¼ .018ā€”than
did females not on contraception. There were no signiļ¬cant differences between the two groups of
females with respect to time spent foragingā€”ANOVA, F(1, 18) Ā¼ 1.043, p Ā¼ .321.
The difference in resting time between the two groups of females was no longer signiļ¬cant when, for
those females who gave birth, only the behavioral data after the birth of infants were includedā€”
ANOVA, F(1, 18) Ā¼ 2.146, p Ā¼ .160. Similarly, the difference in the amount of grooming received
was no longer signiļ¬cant when only postbirth data were analyzed for these femalesā€”ANOVA, F(1,
18) Ā¼ 2.416, p Ā¼ .138. Finally, the difference in the amount of grooming given was not signiļ¬cant when
only data beforeā€”ANOVA, F(1, 17) Ā¼ 0.130, p Ā¼ .723ā€”or afterā€”ANOVA, F(1, 18) Ā¼ 0.542, p Ā¼ .471
ā€”birth were considered for those females who gave birth.
Discussion
In this study, we compared the behavior of free-ranging, adult female Barbary macaques implanted
with the progestin-only contraceptive Implanon with others in the same group who were not on
contraception. Females with the contraceptive implant showed higher rates of two behavioral indexes
of anxietyā€”self-scratching and self-groomingā€”and were also more frequently aggressive toward
conspeciļ¬cs. There was evidence that afļ¬liative behavior was also affected by the implant: Females on
contraception gave less and received more grooming than did females not on contraception.
In addition, some aspects of general activity appeared to be altered by Implanon: Being on this
contraception was linked to increased traveling and reduced resting times, but foraging time was not
affected.
Both the rate of self-scratching and the proportion of time spent self-grooming were markedly
higher among females with Implanon implants, indicating that females on contraception may have
higher average anxiety levels than those not on contraception. This ļ¬nding is in line with studies in the
human literature indicating that hormonal contraception can have negative effects on mood (Poromaa
& Segebladh, 2012). However, in such studies, the prevalence of such effects tends to be low, with
Table 1. Rates or time spent in each behavior for females with and without Implanon implants.
Behavior
Females with Implanon
implant (n Ā¼ 13)
Females without Implanon
implant (n Ā¼ 7)
Difference between
groups
Self-scratching (bouts/hr) 14.76 ^ 2.85 (11.09 2 20.36) 8.56 ^ 2.98 (3.60 2 10.78) ***
Self-grooming (% time) 1.93 ^ 0.80 (0.77 2 3.27) 0.73 ^ 0.55 (0.11 2 1.67) **
Giving grooming (% time) 4.71 ^ 2.66 (1.35 2 8.91) 7.66 ^ 3.52 (2.01 2 12.69) *
Being groomed (% time) 13.47 ^ 3.56 (6.54 2 20.08) 7.08 ^ 4.03 (2.27 2 12.96) **
Aggression given (bouts/hr) 2.12 ^ 1.11 (0.59 2 4.28) 0.84 ^ 0.67 (0.36 2 2.32) *
Travel (% time) 16.69 ^ 2.91 (10.84 2 21.30) 9.89 ^ 3.58 (6.87 2 17.07) ***
Forage (% time) 26.40 ^ 3.70 (20.0 2 32.16) 29.13 ^ 8.23 (19.13 2 42.86) ns
Rest (% time) 34.46 ^ 4.75 (24.59 2 42.83) 44.45^12.51 (23.68 2 57.30) *
Note. Numbers indicate mean ^1 SD, with the range given in parentheses. Differences between the two groups of females are
indicated in the ļ¬nal column, where * p , .05, ** p , .01, *** p , .001, and ns Ā¼ not signiļ¬cant.
4 MAIJER AND SEMPLE
Downloadedby[UniversityofRoehampton],[StuartSemple]at06:5223November2015
between 4% and 10% of individuals reporting effects. A similar low frequency of effect in the present
study would almost certainly have precluded any detection of signiļ¬cant differences; the statistically
highly signiļ¬cant link between contraception and increased indexes of anxiety in our study suggests
that effects on emotional state may be more common in Barbary macaques than in humans.
However, the apparently negative impacts of Implanon on anxiety in the current study need to be
interpreted with caution. First, as is the case for all of the results presented in this study, the nature of
our analyses precludes robust inference of causality. Second, evidence from rodents (Berridge, Mitton,
Clark, & Roth, 1999; Hennessy & Foy, 1987; Hori, Yuyama, & Tamura, 2004) and humans
(Mohiyeddini & Semple, 2012; Pico-Alfonso et al., 2007) has suggested that self-directed behaviors
represent effective strategies for coping with short-term, negative shifts in emotional states; thus,
elevated rates of such behavior may not translate into detrimental, long-term, or physiological impacts
if this coping function is effective. Third, it is possible that contraception increases skin sensitivity and
irritation in monkeys, as happens in humans (Jelinek, 1970); this may lead to an increase in self-
scratching and self-grooming that is unrelated to anxiety, thus making these self-directed behaviors less
reliable as indicators of how anxious an animal is.
Females with Implanon implants more frequently directed aggression at other group members than
did those without implants. This may be due to the underlying changes in reproductive hormones that
Implanon produces. Progestins are antagonistic to androgens, and this may lead to important behavioral
changes in animals ļ¬tted with contraceptive implants such as Implanon. Pazol et al. (2004) found
evidence that the steroidal progestin medroxyprogesterone acetate (MPA) antagonizes the effects of
estradiol in female pigtail macaques. In their study, ovariectomized females given estradiol showed
a trend toward lower levels of aggression compared with control females; by contrast, females given
estradiol and MPA showed markedly higher rates of aggression compared with controls. Thus,
progestin-based contraception may interfere with the sociopositive effects of female reproductive
hormones (Pazol et al., 2004). Future investigations of the effects of contraception in captive or wild
animals will beneļ¬t from exploring the interaction between androgen levels and patterns of aggressive
and other types of behaviors; unfortunately, endocrinological analyses of this kind were beyond the scope
of the present study.
We found evidence linking contraception to patterns of grooming: Females with the implants spent
less time grooming and more time being groomed compared with females not on the contraception.
There is evidence that receiving grooming lowers heart rate (Aureli, Preston, & de Waal, 1999; Boccia,
Laudenslager, & Reite, 1989) and leads to endorphin release (Keverne, Martensz, & Tuite, 1989), and
thus, it may have an important calming effect. If animals on contraception are more anxious, they may
therefore solicit grooming from others to regulate their emotional states.
Additionally, such animals may be less inclined to approach other animals to groom them due to
their anxiety. In support of this idea, Shutt et al. (2007) found a negative relationship between
physiological stress levels and both the amount of grooming given away and the number of females to
whom it was given among female Barbary macaques, potentially indicating that more stressed animals
are less likely to groom others. In this and other primate species, grooming is a key behavior in social
bonding (Dunbar, 1988) and is also ā€œtradedā€ for other services such as coalitionary support (Carne,
Wiper, & Semple, 2011); the apparent impact of contraception on the balance of giving and receiving
grooming may therefore have important implications for the social relationships within the group.
For females on contraception, time spent traveling was signiļ¬cantly higher and time spent resting
was lower. A similar increase in time spent moving was seen in a study of the effects of oral
contraceptive administration in captive long-tailed macaques (Macaca fascicularis; Henderson &
Shively, 2004). By contrast, there was no difference in foraging time between females with and without
the Implanon implant, although as the study population was provisioned, foraging time was greatly
reduced compared with the wild populations, and energy demands could be met in a shorter time
period. Any potential differences in foraging time among females with and without contraceptionā€”for
example, due to increased energetic expenditure as a result of increased traveling and reduced resting
timeā€”may be hard to detect.
EFFECTS OF IMPLANON ON MACAQUE BEHAVIOR 5
Downloadedby[UniversityofRoehampton],[StuartSemple]at06:5223November2015
Some of the differences in behavior we found linked to contraception were no longer signiļ¬cant
when, for the females who gave birth, only data from before birth of the infant or after the birth of the
infant were analyzed. Unfortunately, the small number of such femalesā€”ļ¬ve in totalā€”meant we were
unable to explore whether this was a biologically meaningful effect or rather the result of reduced
statistical power.
Conclusion
Overall, our ļ¬ndings provide evidence that the contraceptive Implanon may have a number of effects
on the behavior of free-ranging, adult, female Barbary macaques; such effects may also be seen when
other forms of chemical contraception manipulating reproductive hormone levels (e.g., oral
contraceptives) are used. It is crucial that the changes in behavior that we found, while potentially
indicative of undesirable impacts, be considered carefully in the light of two key factors. First, it is
unclear whether the magnitude of the behavioral changes found is biologically meaningful in terms of
affecting welfare; studies incorporating cognitive and physiological measures would be needed to
determine whether changes in grooming, aggression, or self-directed behaviors may translate into
psychologically or physically deleterious impacts. Second, it is important to consider the behavioral
impacts of contraception not in isolation, but rather within the framework of all the other potential
means of effective population control (Kirkpatrick, 2007). This holistic evaluation of the relative costs
and beneļ¬ts of all available options should be underpinned by rigorous data. The ļ¬ndings of the current
study contribute to the growing database on the effects of contraception in wildlife that will be crucial in
these types of decision making.
Acknowledgments
We sincerely thank Sue Wiper and Guillaume and Sophie de Turckheim for allowing us to work at Trentham Monkey
Forest. Frimpong Twum provided expert technical support. We thank also Charlotte Carne and Ann MacLarnon for their
thoughtful comments on an earlier draft of this manuscript.
References
Asa, C. S. (1997). The development of contraceptive methods for captive wildlife. In T. J. Kreeger (Ed.), Contraception in
wildlife management (Technical Bulletin 1853, pp. 235ā€“240). Washington, DC: U.S. Department of Agriculture,
Animal and Plant Health Inspection Service.
Asa, C. S. (2005). Types of contraception: The choices. In C. S. Asa & I. J. Porton (Eds.), Wildlife contraception: Issues,
methods, and applications (pp. 29ā€“52). Baltimore, MD: Johns Hopkins University Press.
Asa, C. S., & Porton, I. J. (2005). Introduction: The need for wildlife contraception. In C. S. Asa & I. J. Porton (Eds.),
Wildlife contraception: Issues, methods, and applications (pp. xxvā€“xxxii). Baltimore, MD: Johns Hopkins University
Press.
Asa, C. S., Porton, I. J., & Calle, P. P. (2005). Choosing the most appropriate contraceptive. In C. S. Asa & I. J. Porton
(Eds.), Wildlife contraception: Issues, methods, and applications (pp. 83ā€“95). Baltimore, MD: Johns Hopkins University
Press.
Aureli, F., Preston, S. D., & de Waal, F. B. M. (1999). Heart rate responses to social interactions in free-moving rhesus
macaques (Macaca mulatta): A pilot study. Journal of Comparative Psychology, 113, 59ā€“65.
Bennink, H. J. (2000). The pharmacokinetics and pharmacodynamics of Implanon, a single-rod etonogestrel
contraceptive implant. European Journal of Contraception & Reproductive Health Care, 5(2), 12ā€“20.
Berridge, C. W., Mitton, E., Clark, W., & Roth, R. H. (1999). Engagement in a non-escape (displacement) behavior elicits a
selective and lateralized suppression of frontal cortical dopaminergic utilization in stress. Synapse, 32, 187ā€“197.
Bertrand, M. R., DeNicola, A. J., Beissinger, S. R., & Swihart, R. K. (1996). Effects of parturition on home ranges and social
afļ¬liations of female white-tailed deer. Journal of Wildlife Management, 60, 899ā€“909.
Boccia, M. L., Laudenslager, M., & Reite, M. (1989). On the physiology of grooming in a pigtail macaque. Physiology and
Behavior, 45, 667ā€“670.
Carne, C., Wiper, S., & Semple, S. (2011). Reciprocation and interchange of grooming, agonistic support, feeding
tolerance, and aggression in semi-free-ranging Barbary macaques. American Journal of Primatology, 73, 1127ā€“1133.
6 MAIJER AND SEMPLE
Downloadedby[UniversityofRoehampton],[StuartSemple]at06:5223November2015
Cortes, J., & Shaw, E. (2006). The Gibraltar macaques: Management and future. In J. K. Hodges & J. Cortes (Eds.), The
Barbary macaque: Biology, managements, and conservation (pp. 199ā€“210). Nottingham, UK: Nottingham University
Press.
Crockford, C., Wittig, R. M., Lehmann, J., Whitten, P. L., Seyfarth, R. M., & Cheney, D. (2008). Focused grooming
networks and stress alleviation in wild female baboons. Hormones and Behavior, 54, 170ā€“177.
Croxatto, H. B. (2002). Mechanisms that explain the contraceptive action of progestin implants for women.
Contraception, 65, 21ā€“27.
Croxatto, H. B., & Makarainen, L. (1998). The pharmacodynamics and efļ¬cacy of Implanon: An overview of the data.
Contraception, 58(Suppl. 1), 91ā€“97.
Cullberg, J. (1972). Mood changes and menstrual symptoms with different gestagen/estrogen combinations. Acta
Psychiatrica Scandinavica, 236, 1ā€“86.
De Vleeschouwer, K., Leus, K., & Van Elsacker, L. (2000). An evaluation of the suitability of contraceptive methods in
golden-headed lion tamarins (Leontopithicus chrysomelas), with emphasis on melengestrol acetate (MGA) implants:
(II) Endocrinological and behavioral effects. Animal Welfare, 9, 385ā€“401.
Dunbar, R. I. M. (1988). Primate social systems. Ithaca, NY: Cornell University Press.
Gray, M. E., & Cameron, E. Z. (2010). Does contraceptive treatment in wildlife result in side effects? A review of
quantitative and anecdotal evidence. Reproduction, 139, 45ā€“55.
Henderson, J. A., & Shively, C. A. (2004). Triphasic oral contraceptive treatment alters the behavior and neurobiology of
female cynomolgus monkeys. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 29, 21ā€“34.
Hennessy, M. B., & Foy, T. (1987). Nonedible material elicits chewing and reduces the plasma corticosterone response
during novelty exposure in mice. Behavior and Neuroscience, 101, 237ā€“245.
Hori, N., Yuyama, N., & Tamura, K. (2004). Biting suppresses stress-induced expression of corticotropin-releasing factor
(CRF) in the rat hypothalamus. Journal of Dental Research, 83, 124ā€“128.
Jelinek, J. E. (1970). Cutaneous side effect of oral contraceptives. Archives of Dermatology, 101, 181ā€“186.
Keverne, E. B., Martensz, N. D., & Tuite, B. (1989). Beta-endorphin concentrations in cerebrospinal ļ¬‚uid of monkeys are
inļ¬‚uenced by grooming relationships. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 14, 155ā€“161.
Kirkpatrick, J. F. (2007). Measuring the effects of wildlife contraception: The argument for comparing apples with
oranges. Reproduction, Fertility, and Development, 19, 548ā€“552.
Kirkpatrick, J. F., & Frank, K. M. (2005). Contraception in free-ranging wildlife. In C. S. Asa & I. J. Porton (Eds.), Wildlife
contraception: Issues, methods, and applications (pp. 195ā€“221). Baltimore, MD: Johns Hopkins University Press.
Maestripieri, D., Schino, G., Aureli, F., & Troisi, A. (1992). A modest proposal: Displacement activities as an indicator of
emotions in primates. Animal Behaviour, 44, 967ā€“979.
Mohiyeddini, C., & Semple, S. (2012). Displacement behaviour regulates the experience of stress in men. Stress, 16,
163ā€“171.
Munson, L., Moresco, A., & Calle, P. P. (2005). Adverse effects of contraceptives. In C. S. Asa & I. J. Porton (Eds.), Wildlife
contraception: Issues, methods, and applications (pp. 66ā€“82). Baltimore, MD: Johns Hopkins University Press.
Paul, A., & Kuester, J. (1987). Dominance, kinship, and reproductive value in female Barbary macaques (Macaca sylvanus)
at Affenberg Salem. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology, 21, 1885ā€“1891.
Pazol, K., Wilson, M. E., & Wallen, K. (2004). Medroxyprogesterone acetate antagonizes the effects of estrogen treatment
on social and sexual behavior in female macaques. Journal of Clinical Endocrinology & Metabolism, 89, 2998ā€“3006.
Penfold, L. M., Patton, M. L., & Jochle, W. (2005). Contraception agents in aggression control. In C. S. Asa & I. J. Porton
(Eds.), Wildlife contraception: Issues, methods, and applications Zoo and Aquarium Biology and Conservation Series
(pp. 184ā€“195). Washington, DC: Smithsonian Institution.
Pico-Alfonso, M. A., Mastorci, F., Ceresini, G., Ceda, G. P., Manghi, M., Pino, O. . . . Sgoifo, A. (2007). Acute
psychosocial challenge and cardiac autonomic response in women: The role of estrogens, corticosteroids, and
behavioral coping styles. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 32, 451ā€“463.
Plowman, A. B., Jordan, N. R., Anderson, N., Condon, E., & Fraser, O. (2004). Welfare implications of captive primate
population management: Behavioral and psycho-social effects of female-based contraception, oestrus and male removal
in hamadryas baboons (Papio hamadryas). Applied Animal Behaviour Science, 90, 155ā€“165.
Poromaa, I. S., & Segebladh, B. (2012). Adverse mood symptoms with oral contraceptives. Acta Obstetricia et Gynecologica
Scandinavica, 91, 420ā€“427.
Porton, I. J., & Dematteo, K. E. (2005). Contraception in nonhuman primates. In C. S. Asa & I. J. Porton (Eds.), Wildlife
contraception: Issues, methods, and applications Zoo and Aquarium Biology and Conservation Series (pp. 119ā€“148).
Washington, DC: Smithsonian Institution.
Schino, G., Perretta, G., Taglioni, A. M., Monaco, V., & Troisi, A. (1996). Primate displacement activities as an
ethopharmacological model of anxiety. Anxiety, 2, 186ā€“191.
Shively, C. A., Manuck, S. B., Kaplan, J. R., & Koritnik, D. R. (1990). Oral contraceptive administration, interfemale
relationships, and sexual behavior in Macaca fascicularis. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 19, 101ā€“117.
Shutt, K., MacLarnon, A., Heistermann, M., & Semple, S. (2007). Grooming in Barbary macaques: Better to give than
receive? Biology Letters, 3, 231ā€“233.
EFFECTS OF IMPLANON ON MACAQUE BEHAVIOR 7
Downloadedby[UniversityofRoehampton],[StuartSemple]at06:5223November2015
Silk, J. B. (1987). Activities and feeding behavior of free-ranging pregnant baboons. International Journal of Primatology,
8, 593ā€“613.
Toufexis, D. J., Myers, K. M., & Davis, M. (2006). The effect of gonadal hormones and gender on anxiety and emotional
learning. Hormones and Behavior, 50, 539ā€“549.
Varma, R., & Mascarenhas, L. (2001). Endometrial effects of etonogestrel (Implanon) contraceptive implant. Current
Opinion in Obstetrics & Gynecology, 13, 335ā€“341.
Wallner, B., Mƶstl, E., & Dittami, J. (2007). Chronic levonorgestrel treatment in Macaca sylvanus: Effects on perineal
swelling size and fecal sex steroid excretion. Neuroendocrinology Letters, 28, 326ā€“332.
Yates, F., & Aureli, K. (2010). Distress prevention by grooming others in crested black macaques. Biology Letters, 6, 27ā€“29.
8 MAIJER AND SEMPLE
Downloadedby[UniversityofRoehampton],[StuartSemple]at06:5223November2015

More Related Content

What's hot

Antenatal Peer Support Workers And Initiation Of Breast Feeding Cluster Rando...
Antenatal Peer Support Workers And Initiation Of Breast Feeding Cluster Rando...Antenatal Peer Support Workers And Initiation Of Breast Feeding Cluster Rando...
Antenatal Peer Support Workers And Initiation Of Breast Feeding Cluster Rando...
Biblioteca Virtual
Ā 
Haad addiction and evolutionary process common aspetcs in physiopathologic pa...
Haad addiction and evolutionary process common aspetcs in physiopathologic pa...Haad addiction and evolutionary process common aspetcs in physiopathologic pa...
Haad addiction and evolutionary process common aspetcs in physiopathologic pa...
M. Luisetto Pharm.D.Spec. Pharmacology
Ā 
Effectiveness of Early Ambulation on Post Operative Recovery among the Women ...
Effectiveness of Early Ambulation on Post Operative Recovery among the Women ...Effectiveness of Early Ambulation on Post Operative Recovery among the Women ...
Effectiveness of Early Ambulation on Post Operative Recovery among the Women ...
ijtsrd
Ā 
Animal research
Animal researchAnimal research
Animal research
Subhi Sharma
Ā 

What's hot (20)

Animal Experimentation- Contemporary Issue
Animal Experimentation- Contemporary IssueAnimal Experimentation- Contemporary Issue
Animal Experimentation- Contemporary Issue
Ā 
Expert reaction to new study on triclosan and tumours in mice
Expert reaction to new study on triclosan and tumours in miceExpert reaction to new study on triclosan and tumours in mice
Expert reaction to new study on triclosan and tumours in mice
Ā 
Antenatal Peer Support Workers And Initiation Of Breast Feeding Cluster Rando...
Antenatal Peer Support Workers And Initiation Of Breast Feeding Cluster Rando...Antenatal Peer Support Workers And Initiation Of Breast Feeding Cluster Rando...
Antenatal Peer Support Workers And Initiation Of Breast Feeding Cluster Rando...
Ā 
Searching for predictors of male fecundity
Searching for predictors of male fecunditySearching for predictors of male fecundity
Searching for predictors of male fecundity
Ā 
Bioethics presentation
Bioethics presentationBioethics presentation
Bioethics presentation
Ā 
Animal experimentation
Animal experimentationAnimal experimentation
Animal experimentation
Ā 
2016 CV
2016 CV2016 CV
2016 CV
Ā 
An Approach to Calculating Childhood Body Burdens
An Approach to Calculating Childhood Body Burdens An Approach to Calculating Childhood Body Burdens
An Approach to Calculating Childhood Body Burdens
Ā 
Haad addiction and evolutionary process common aspetcs in physiopathologic pa...
Haad addiction and evolutionary process common aspetcs in physiopathologic pa...Haad addiction and evolutionary process common aspetcs in physiopathologic pa...
Haad addiction and evolutionary process common aspetcs in physiopathologic pa...
Ā 
Animal ethics: a social controversy
Animal ethics: a social controversyAnimal ethics: a social controversy
Animal ethics: a social controversy
Ā 
"Research with Animals" lecture
"Research with Animals" lecture"Research with Animals" lecture
"Research with Animals" lecture
Ā 
Reduction in use of Animals in Scientific Research
Reduction in use of Animals in Scientific ResearchReduction in use of Animals in Scientific Research
Reduction in use of Animals in Scientific Research
Ā 
Research Methods: Ethics II (Animal Research)
Research Methods: Ethics II (Animal Research)Research Methods: Ethics II (Animal Research)
Research Methods: Ethics II (Animal Research)
Ā 
Animal testing
Animal testing Animal testing
Animal testing
Ā 
Animal welfare
Animal welfareAnimal welfare
Animal welfare
Ā 
ANIMAL STUDIES
ANIMAL STUDIESANIMAL STUDIES
ANIMAL STUDIES
Ā 
Animal experiments
Animal experimentsAnimal experiments
Animal experiments
Ā 
Effectiveness of Early Ambulation on Post Operative Recovery among the Women ...
Effectiveness of Early Ambulation on Post Operative Recovery among the Women ...Effectiveness of Early Ambulation on Post Operative Recovery among the Women ...
Effectiveness of Early Ambulation on Post Operative Recovery among the Women ...
Ā 
Animal research
Animal researchAnimal research
Animal research
Ā 
Animal experimentation
Animal experimentationAnimal experimentation
Animal experimentation
Ā 

Similar to Maijer and Semple 2015

Zebrafish as an alternative method for determining the embryo toxicity of pla...
Zebrafish as an alternative method for determining the embryo toxicity of pla...Zebrafish as an alternative method for determining the embryo toxicity of pla...
Zebrafish as an alternative method for determining the embryo toxicity of pla...
Noor Zada
Ā 
Human Clinical Relevance of Developmental and Reproductive Toxicology and Non...
Human Clinical Relevance of Developmental and Reproductive Toxicology and Non...Human Clinical Relevance of Developmental and Reproductive Toxicology and Non...
Human Clinical Relevance of Developmental and Reproductive Toxicology and Non...
Joseph Holson
Ā 
Experimental animal models ( lab animals )
Experimental animal models ( lab animals )Experimental animal models ( lab animals )
Experimental animal models ( lab animals )
ebrahem elalfy
Ā 
Physiological and ethological effects of antidepressants: a study using ants ...
Physiological and ethological effects of antidepressants: a study using ants ...Physiological and ethological effects of antidepressants: a study using ants ...
Physiological and ethological effects of antidepressants: a study using ants ...
inventionjournals
Ā 
cloning. Second, it is sensitive. Activities canbe detected
cloning. Second, it is sensitive. Activities canbe detected cloning. Second, it is sensitive. Activities canbe detected
cloning. Second, it is sensitive. Activities canbe detected
WilheminaRossi174
Ā 
Animals testing should be banned
Animals testing should be bannedAnimals testing should be banned
Animals testing should be banned
IqraAfaq
Ā 

Similar to Maijer and Semple 2015 (18)

Zebrafish as an alternative method for determining the embryo toxicity of pla...
Zebrafish as an alternative method for determining the embryo toxicity of pla...Zebrafish as an alternative method for determining the embryo toxicity of pla...
Zebrafish as an alternative method for determining the embryo toxicity of pla...
Ā 
Cytogenetic an Experimental Monitoring Test for Plant Extracts
Cytogenetic an Experimental Monitoring Test for Plant ExtractsCytogenetic an Experimental Monitoring Test for Plant Extracts
Cytogenetic an Experimental Monitoring Test for Plant Extracts
Ā 
Ethics of Using animals in Research
Ethics of Using animals in ResearchEthics of Using animals in Research
Ethics of Using animals in Research
Ā 
Descriptive Comparative Anatomohistological Study of the Main Dissected Organ...
Descriptive Comparative Anatomohistological Study of the Main Dissected Organ...Descriptive Comparative Anatomohistological Study of the Main Dissected Organ...
Descriptive Comparative Anatomohistological Study of the Main Dissected Organ...
Ā 
Review Sperm Comparative Morphology and Function Related to Altered Reproductive
Review Sperm Comparative Morphology and Function Related to Altered ReproductiveReview Sperm Comparative Morphology and Function Related to Altered Reproductive
Review Sperm Comparative Morphology and Function Related to Altered Reproductive
Ā 
Kangaroo Mother Care: systematic review. Metodologia MĆ£e Canguru: uma revisĆ£o...
Kangaroo Mother Care: systematic review. Metodologia MĆ£e Canguru: uma revisĆ£o...Kangaroo Mother Care: systematic review. Metodologia MĆ£e Canguru: uma revisĆ£o...
Kangaroo Mother Care: systematic review. Metodologia MĆ£e Canguru: uma revisĆ£o...
Ā 
Human Clinical Relevance of Developmental and Reproductive Toxicology and Non...
Human Clinical Relevance of Developmental and Reproductive Toxicology and Non...Human Clinical Relevance of Developmental and Reproductive Toxicology and Non...
Human Clinical Relevance of Developmental and Reproductive Toxicology and Non...
Ā 
Experimental animal models ( lab animals )
Experimental animal models ( lab animals )Experimental animal models ( lab animals )
Experimental animal models ( lab animals )
Ā 
Experimental animal models ( lab animals )
Experimental animal models ( lab animals )Experimental animal models ( lab animals )
Experimental animal models ( lab animals )
Ā 
EDCS.pptx
EDCS.pptxEDCS.pptx
EDCS.pptx
Ā 
An Inventory Of Peer-Reviewed Articles On Killer Whales (Orcinus Orca) With A...
An Inventory Of Peer-Reviewed Articles On Killer Whales (Orcinus Orca) With A...An Inventory Of Peer-Reviewed Articles On Killer Whales (Orcinus Orca) With A...
An Inventory Of Peer-Reviewed Articles On Killer Whales (Orcinus Orca) With A...
Ā 
EE
EEEE
EE
Ā 
Animal toxicity studies
Animal toxicity studiesAnimal toxicity studies
Animal toxicity studies
Ā 
Physiological and ethological effects of antidepressants: a study using ants ...
Physiological and ethological effects of antidepressants: a study using ants ...Physiological and ethological effects of antidepressants: a study using ants ...
Physiological and ethological effects of antidepressants: a study using ants ...
Ā 
cloning. Second, it is sensitive. Activities canbe detected
cloning. Second, it is sensitive. Activities canbe detected cloning. Second, it is sensitive. Activities canbe detected
cloning. Second, it is sensitive. Activities canbe detected
Ā 
Animals testing should be banned
Animals testing should be bannedAnimals testing should be banned
Animals testing should be banned
Ā 
Wagner College Forum for Undergraduate Research, Vol. 16 No. 2
Wagner College Forum for Undergraduate Research, Vol. 16 No. 2Wagner College Forum for Undergraduate Research, Vol. 16 No. 2
Wagner College Forum for Undergraduate Research, Vol. 16 No. 2
Ā 
Shorter anogenital distance predicts poorer semen quality in young men in NY ...
Shorter anogenital distance predicts poorer semen quality in young men in NY ...Shorter anogenital distance predicts poorer semen quality in young men in NY ...
Shorter anogenital distance predicts poorer semen quality in young men in NY ...
Ā 

Maijer and Semple 2015

  • 1. Full Terms & Conditions of access and use can be found at http://www.tandfonline.com/action/journalInformation?journalCode=haaw20 Download by: [University of Roehampton], [Stuart Semple] Date: 23 November 2015, At: 06:52 Journal of Applied Animal Welfare Science ISSN: 1088-8705 (Print) 1532-7604 (Online) Journal homepage: http://www.tandfonline.com/loi/haaw20 Investigating Potential Effects of the Contraceptive Implanon on the Behavior of Free-Ranging Adult Female Barbary Macaques Amanda M. Maijer & Stuart Semple To cite this article: Amanda M. Maijer & Stuart Semple (2015): Investigating Potential Effects of the Contraceptive Implanon on the Behavior of Free-Ranging Adult Female Barbary Macaques, Journal of Applied Animal Welfare Science, DOI: 10.1080/10888705.2015.1083432 To link to this article: http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/10888705.2015.1083432 Published online: 14 Oct 2015. Submit your article to this journal Article views: 8 View related articles View Crossmark data
  • 2. Investigating Potential Effects of the Contraceptive Implanon on the Behavior of Free-Ranging Adult Female Barbary Macaques Amanda M. Maijer and Stuart Semple Centre for Research in Evolutionary and Environmental Anthropology, University of Roehampton, London, United Kingdom ABSTRACT In recent years, the use of hormonal contraception in captive, free-ranging, and wild mammal populations has increased, but the effects on these nonhuman animalsā€™ behavior and the associated welfare impacts remain poorly understood. This study of free-ranging adult female Barbary macaques (Macaca sylvanus) at Trentham Monkey Forest compared females implanted with the progestin-only contraceptive Implanon to those not receiving contraception. Females with contraceptive implants had higher rates of self- scratching and spent more time self-grooming (2 behavioral indexes of anxiety) than those without implants. They also directed more aggression at others, spent more time receiving grooming and traveling, and spent less time giving grooming and resting. No signiļ¬cant differences between the 2 groups of females were seen for time spent foraging. These results suggest that Implanon had a number of effects on Barbary macaques, although these need to be considered in light of the signiļ¬cant beneļ¬ts afforded by the use of this contraceptive. The ļ¬ndings of this study provide important information to those evaluating the relative welfare costs and beneļ¬ts of alternative methods of population control in this and other species. KEYWORDS Behavior; welfare; contra- ception; anxiety; stress For many mammals in captivity, excellent husbandry conditions lead to high reproductive rates and populations rapidly outgrowing the available space (Asa, 1997). Moreover, many species of mammals in the wild increasingly share space with human populations and ļ¬‚ourish in such environments, leading to rapid population increases and calls to restrict their numbers (Asa & Porton, 2005). Initially, lethal control and surgical sterilization were the predominant methods used to control such nonhuman animal populations, but these techniques are costly and logistically challenging and can have a range of undesirable welfare consequences (Gray & Cameron, 2010; Kirkpatrick, 2007). More recently, hormonal contraception has become a popular method to control animal population size (Asa, 2005; Gray & Cameron, 2010). This approach has been primarily adopted in captive settings (Asa, Porton, & Calle, 2005; Porton & Dematteo, 2005), but it is also used in free-ranging and wild populations (Kirkpatrick & Frank, 2005). The use of hormonal contraceptives in captive and noncaptive mammals gives rise to a number of concerns, however. When capture is necessary to implant a contraceptive device, it may result in stress to the individuals concerned and disruption to the social structure of the group (Munson, Moresco, & Calle, 2005). Even when capture is not needed, or after this event has taken place when it is required, the biological action of the contraception may have negative welfare impacts (Asa et al., 2005; Munson et al., 2005). In particular, changes in levels of sex steroids brought about by many hormonal contraceptives could potentially negatively affect emotional statesā€”and therefore psychological well beingā€”of the animals in question and/or may modify their behaviors in ways that detrimentally affect q 2015 Taylor & Francis CONTACT Stuart Semple s.semple@roehampton.ac.uk Centre for Research in Evolutionary and Environmental Anthropology, University of Roehampton, Holybourne Avenue, London SW15 4JD, United Kingdom. JOURNAL OF APPLIED ANIMAL WELFARE SCIENCE http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/10888705.2015.1083432 Downloadedby[UniversityofRoehampton],[StuartSemple]at06:5223November2015
  • 3. their welfare. Ultimately, the decision to implement hormonal contraception in mammals must be based on an analysis of the costs and beneļ¬ts of this method of population control and of the alternative options (Kirkpatrick, 2007). Detailed information on the effects of hormonal contraception is key to this decision process but is so far very limited. Sex steroids can have potent effects on emotional states in humans, with female sex hormones being linked in particular to increased anxiety (Toufexis, Myers, & Davis, 2006). It has been known for more than 40 years that the use of hormonal contraceptives can cause negative changes in womenā€™s moods (Cullberg, 1972; Poromaa & Segebladh, 2012), but the impact of contraception on emotional states in nonhuman animals remains a neglected area of research (Gray & Cameron, 2010). To our knowledge, only one study to date has attempted to explore this issue. Plowman, Jordan, Anderson, Condon, and Fraser (2004) tested zoo-housed hamadryas baboons (Papio hamadryas) for potential negative psychological effects of Norplant, an implanted contraceptive with an active ingredient with progesterone-like activity. To do so, they measured the rate of self-directed behaviors, which provides a useful index of anxiety in humans and nonhuman primates (Maestripieri, Schino, Aureli, & Troisi, 1992; Schino, Perretta, Taglioni, Monaco, & Troisi, 1996). No evidence was found for a change in self- directed behavior rates linked to contraception use, but as the authors acknowledged, changes in group size during their study might have masked any such effects. Evidence that hormonal contraception increases levels of aggressive behavior has been found in captive long-tailed macaques (Henderson & Shively, 2004) and pigtail macaques (Pazol, Wilson, & Wallen, 2004). Such effects are of clear welfare concern, as elevated rates of aggression increase the risk for physical injury and disrupt normal patterns of social organization (Penfold, Patton, & Jochle, 2005). There is also evidence from captive populations of a range of mammal speciesā€”such as white-tailed deer (Bertrand, DeNicola, Beissinger, & Swihart, 1996), long-tailed macaques (Shively, Manuck, Kaplan, & Koritnik, 1990), and golden-headed lion tamarins (De Vleeschouwer, Leus, & Van Elsacker, 2000)ā€”that hormonal contraceptives decrease rates of afļ¬liative behavior. These effects are also important when assessing welfare impacts, as there is evidence that sociopositive behaviors such as grooming can reduce anxiety and physiological stress levels (Crockford et al., 2008; Shutt, MacLarnon, Heistermann, & Semple, 2007; Yates & Aureli, 2010). In this study, we explored potential impacts of the progestin-only contraceptive Implanon in free- ranging, adult female Barbary macaques. We tested whether there were differences between females with and without Implanon implants with respect to behavioral indexes of anxiety (self-directed behaviors) and measures of aggression and afļ¬liation. To allow for comparison with the broadest range of previous studies of contraception in animals, in which impacts on general activity patterns were frequently assessed (Gray & Cameron, 2010), we also tested for potential effects on three measures of activity: traveling, foraging, and resting. Hormonal contraception is used in both free-ranging (Wallner, Mƶstl, & Dittami, 2007) and wild populations (Cortes & Shaw, 2006) of our study species and in a range of other mammal species in captive, free-ranging, and wild settings (Gray & Cameron, 2010). Our ļ¬ndings will therefore provide valuable information to those evaluating the relative costs and beneļ¬ts of alternative methods of population control in Barbary macaques and other species. Materials and methods Study site and animals We studied the behavior of 20 adult, female Barbary macaques (age range Ā¼ 5ā€“17 years) living in one group of the free-ranging population at Trentham Monkey Forest in Stoke-on-Trent, United Kingdom. The total troop size was 73 individuals: 24 subadult or adult males, 28 subadult or adult females, 16 juveniles, and 5 infants. Barbary macaques live in multimale/multifemale groups characterized by female philopatry and male dispersal, with close bonds between matrilineal kin (Paul & Kuester, 1987; Silk, 1987). Of the focal females, 13 were on the progestin-only contraceptive implant Implanon, which had been administered 1 year to 4 years prior to the start of this study; this implant has been 2 MAIJER AND SEMPLE Downloadedby[UniversityofRoehampton],[StuartSemple]at06:5223November2015
  • 4. found to be effective at preventing pregnancy for at least 4 years in this population (S. Wiper, personal communication, March 14, 2013). Implanon releases the progestin etonogestrel (Bennink, 2000; Croxatto, 2002). It functions by partially or completely inhibiting ovulation via suppression of midcycle luteinizing hormone peaks by causing thickening of the cervical mucus, which prevents sperm from meeting the egg, and by thinning of the endometrium, which prevents implantation (Croxatto & Makarainen, 1998; Varma & Mascarenhas, 2001). The researchers did not know the contraception status of each focal female at the start of the observation period, and their status remained unknown throughout the study except for the ļ¬ve females who did not have contraceptive implants and gave birth. The selection of females for contraception was based on a range of factors including their reproductive histories, the sizes of their matrilines, and how many closely related females were on contraception. Although this was not a truly random selection process, we did not think it would lead to biases that would affect our study. In support of this, we found that females with and without contraceptive implants did not differ signiļ¬cantly in ageā€”analysis of variance (ANOVA), F(1, 18) Ā¼ 3.045, p Ā¼ .098ā€”or in dominance rankā€”ANOVA, F(1, 18) Ā¼ 1.154, p Ā¼ .297ā€”as assessed from a matrix of the outcomes of dyadic agonistic interactions. Data collection The protocol for this project was approved by the Roehampton University Ethics Committee and by Trentham Monkey Forest. Behavioral data (220 hr total, 11 hr per female) were collected from April 18, 2011, to June 8, 2011, during 15-min continuous focal observations using a Teklogix Psion handheld computer loaded with Noldus Observer 5 software. The order of focal samples was randomized to ensure that they were distributed evenly across the day for each female. The data collection period was after the end of the breeding season, which ends in late January. We recorded the duration of time spent giving and receiving grooming, self-grooming, traveling, foraging, and resting. In addition, we recorded the frequency of occurrence of self-scratching (with separate bouts of self-scratching deļ¬ned by a break of 3 s or more) and the frequency of occurrence of aggression (threats, chasing, contact, and biting). For any observations where the focal animal was out of sight for more than 2 min, the observation was discontinued and discarded and another female was followed; the original female was followed again at the next available opportunity. Statistical analysis The time that the focal animal was out of sight in a focal observation was subtracted from the total observation time to determine the duration that the animal was in view for that observation. These values were used to calculate for each focal observation the rates of self-scratching and of aggression per hour and percentage time engaged in foraging, resting, traveling, giving and receiving grooming, and self-grooming. For each female, a mean was then calculated across all focal watches for each behavior. Initial analyses indicated no signiļ¬cant differences in behavior between females not on contraception who did and did not give birth (Mann Whitney tests, all p . .095), so these females were grouped together for comparison with the group of females with contraceptive implants. Data were found to be normally distributed, so ANOVAs were used to compare rates or durations of behavior between females on contraception and those not on contraception. As both pregnancy and the birth of an infant may affect femalesā€™ behaviors, we then repeated all analyses using, for the ļ¬ve females who gave birth, (a) only data from before birth of the infant and (b) only data after the birth of the infant. Only four results differed with respect to signiļ¬cance between the analyses using the complete data set and those using data from only before, or only after, birth. For brevity, we report here only the results for the complete data set but highlight the instances where the signiļ¬cance of the result changed when using one of the restricted data sets. Analyses were carried out in the EFFECTS OF IMPLANON ON MACAQUE BEHAVIOR 3 Downloadedby[UniversityofRoehampton],[StuartSemple]at06:5223November2015
  • 5. Statistical Package for the Social Sciences Version 17.0. All statistical tests were two-tailed, with a Ā¼ .05. Results The rates and time spent in each behavior for females with and without Implanon contraceptive implants are shown inTable 1. Compared with females not on contraception, females with implants had signiļ¬cantly higher rates of self-scratchingā€”ANOVA, F(1, 18) Ā¼ 20.878, p , .001ā€”and giving aggressionā€”ANOVA, F(1, 18) Ā¼ 7.490, p Ā¼ .014ā€”and spent more time self-groomingā€”ANOVA, F(1, 18) Ā¼ 12.365, p Ā¼ .002ā€”receiving groomingā€”ANOVA, F(1, 18) Ā¼ 13.365, p Ā¼ .002ā€”and travelingā€” ANOVA, F(1, 18) Ā¼ 21.158, p , .001. Females on contraception also spent less time giving groomingā€” ANOVA, F(1, 18) Ā¼ 4.502, p Ā¼ .048ā€”and less time restingā€”ANOVA, F(1, 18) Ā¼ 6.765, p Ā¼ .018ā€”than did females not on contraception. There were no signiļ¬cant differences between the two groups of females with respect to time spent foragingā€”ANOVA, F(1, 18) Ā¼ 1.043, p Ā¼ .321. The difference in resting time between the two groups of females was no longer signiļ¬cant when, for those females who gave birth, only the behavioral data after the birth of infants were includedā€” ANOVA, F(1, 18) Ā¼ 2.146, p Ā¼ .160. Similarly, the difference in the amount of grooming received was no longer signiļ¬cant when only postbirth data were analyzed for these femalesā€”ANOVA, F(1, 18) Ā¼ 2.416, p Ā¼ .138. Finally, the difference in the amount of grooming given was not signiļ¬cant when only data beforeā€”ANOVA, F(1, 17) Ā¼ 0.130, p Ā¼ .723ā€”or afterā€”ANOVA, F(1, 18) Ā¼ 0.542, p Ā¼ .471 ā€”birth were considered for those females who gave birth. Discussion In this study, we compared the behavior of free-ranging, adult female Barbary macaques implanted with the progestin-only contraceptive Implanon with others in the same group who were not on contraception. Females with the contraceptive implant showed higher rates of two behavioral indexes of anxietyā€”self-scratching and self-groomingā€”and were also more frequently aggressive toward conspeciļ¬cs. There was evidence that afļ¬liative behavior was also affected by the implant: Females on contraception gave less and received more grooming than did females not on contraception. In addition, some aspects of general activity appeared to be altered by Implanon: Being on this contraception was linked to increased traveling and reduced resting times, but foraging time was not affected. Both the rate of self-scratching and the proportion of time spent self-grooming were markedly higher among females with Implanon implants, indicating that females on contraception may have higher average anxiety levels than those not on contraception. This ļ¬nding is in line with studies in the human literature indicating that hormonal contraception can have negative effects on mood (Poromaa & Segebladh, 2012). However, in such studies, the prevalence of such effects tends to be low, with Table 1. Rates or time spent in each behavior for females with and without Implanon implants. Behavior Females with Implanon implant (n Ā¼ 13) Females without Implanon implant (n Ā¼ 7) Difference between groups Self-scratching (bouts/hr) 14.76 ^ 2.85 (11.09 2 20.36) 8.56 ^ 2.98 (3.60 2 10.78) *** Self-grooming (% time) 1.93 ^ 0.80 (0.77 2 3.27) 0.73 ^ 0.55 (0.11 2 1.67) ** Giving grooming (% time) 4.71 ^ 2.66 (1.35 2 8.91) 7.66 ^ 3.52 (2.01 2 12.69) * Being groomed (% time) 13.47 ^ 3.56 (6.54 2 20.08) 7.08 ^ 4.03 (2.27 2 12.96) ** Aggression given (bouts/hr) 2.12 ^ 1.11 (0.59 2 4.28) 0.84 ^ 0.67 (0.36 2 2.32) * Travel (% time) 16.69 ^ 2.91 (10.84 2 21.30) 9.89 ^ 3.58 (6.87 2 17.07) *** Forage (% time) 26.40 ^ 3.70 (20.0 2 32.16) 29.13 ^ 8.23 (19.13 2 42.86) ns Rest (% time) 34.46 ^ 4.75 (24.59 2 42.83) 44.45^12.51 (23.68 2 57.30) * Note. Numbers indicate mean ^1 SD, with the range given in parentheses. Differences between the two groups of females are indicated in the ļ¬nal column, where * p , .05, ** p , .01, *** p , .001, and ns Ā¼ not signiļ¬cant. 4 MAIJER AND SEMPLE Downloadedby[UniversityofRoehampton],[StuartSemple]at06:5223November2015
  • 6. between 4% and 10% of individuals reporting effects. A similar low frequency of effect in the present study would almost certainly have precluded any detection of signiļ¬cant differences; the statistically highly signiļ¬cant link between contraception and increased indexes of anxiety in our study suggests that effects on emotional state may be more common in Barbary macaques than in humans. However, the apparently negative impacts of Implanon on anxiety in the current study need to be interpreted with caution. First, as is the case for all of the results presented in this study, the nature of our analyses precludes robust inference of causality. Second, evidence from rodents (Berridge, Mitton, Clark, & Roth, 1999; Hennessy & Foy, 1987; Hori, Yuyama, & Tamura, 2004) and humans (Mohiyeddini & Semple, 2012; Pico-Alfonso et al., 2007) has suggested that self-directed behaviors represent effective strategies for coping with short-term, negative shifts in emotional states; thus, elevated rates of such behavior may not translate into detrimental, long-term, or physiological impacts if this coping function is effective. Third, it is possible that contraception increases skin sensitivity and irritation in monkeys, as happens in humans (Jelinek, 1970); this may lead to an increase in self- scratching and self-grooming that is unrelated to anxiety, thus making these self-directed behaviors less reliable as indicators of how anxious an animal is. Females with Implanon implants more frequently directed aggression at other group members than did those without implants. This may be due to the underlying changes in reproductive hormones that Implanon produces. Progestins are antagonistic to androgens, and this may lead to important behavioral changes in animals ļ¬tted with contraceptive implants such as Implanon. Pazol et al. (2004) found evidence that the steroidal progestin medroxyprogesterone acetate (MPA) antagonizes the effects of estradiol in female pigtail macaques. In their study, ovariectomized females given estradiol showed a trend toward lower levels of aggression compared with control females; by contrast, females given estradiol and MPA showed markedly higher rates of aggression compared with controls. Thus, progestin-based contraception may interfere with the sociopositive effects of female reproductive hormones (Pazol et al., 2004). Future investigations of the effects of contraception in captive or wild animals will beneļ¬t from exploring the interaction between androgen levels and patterns of aggressive and other types of behaviors; unfortunately, endocrinological analyses of this kind were beyond the scope of the present study. We found evidence linking contraception to patterns of grooming: Females with the implants spent less time grooming and more time being groomed compared with females not on the contraception. There is evidence that receiving grooming lowers heart rate (Aureli, Preston, & de Waal, 1999; Boccia, Laudenslager, & Reite, 1989) and leads to endorphin release (Keverne, Martensz, & Tuite, 1989), and thus, it may have an important calming effect. If animals on contraception are more anxious, they may therefore solicit grooming from others to regulate their emotional states. Additionally, such animals may be less inclined to approach other animals to groom them due to their anxiety. In support of this idea, Shutt et al. (2007) found a negative relationship between physiological stress levels and both the amount of grooming given away and the number of females to whom it was given among female Barbary macaques, potentially indicating that more stressed animals are less likely to groom others. In this and other primate species, grooming is a key behavior in social bonding (Dunbar, 1988) and is also ā€œtradedā€ for other services such as coalitionary support (Carne, Wiper, & Semple, 2011); the apparent impact of contraception on the balance of giving and receiving grooming may therefore have important implications for the social relationships within the group. For females on contraception, time spent traveling was signiļ¬cantly higher and time spent resting was lower. A similar increase in time spent moving was seen in a study of the effects of oral contraceptive administration in captive long-tailed macaques (Macaca fascicularis; Henderson & Shively, 2004). By contrast, there was no difference in foraging time between females with and without the Implanon implant, although as the study population was provisioned, foraging time was greatly reduced compared with the wild populations, and energy demands could be met in a shorter time period. Any potential differences in foraging time among females with and without contraceptionā€”for example, due to increased energetic expenditure as a result of increased traveling and reduced resting timeā€”may be hard to detect. EFFECTS OF IMPLANON ON MACAQUE BEHAVIOR 5 Downloadedby[UniversityofRoehampton],[StuartSemple]at06:5223November2015
  • 7. Some of the differences in behavior we found linked to contraception were no longer signiļ¬cant when, for the females who gave birth, only data from before birth of the infant or after the birth of the infant were analyzed. Unfortunately, the small number of such femalesā€”ļ¬ve in totalā€”meant we were unable to explore whether this was a biologically meaningful effect or rather the result of reduced statistical power. Conclusion Overall, our ļ¬ndings provide evidence that the contraceptive Implanon may have a number of effects on the behavior of free-ranging, adult, female Barbary macaques; such effects may also be seen when other forms of chemical contraception manipulating reproductive hormone levels (e.g., oral contraceptives) are used. It is crucial that the changes in behavior that we found, while potentially indicative of undesirable impacts, be considered carefully in the light of two key factors. First, it is unclear whether the magnitude of the behavioral changes found is biologically meaningful in terms of affecting welfare; studies incorporating cognitive and physiological measures would be needed to determine whether changes in grooming, aggression, or self-directed behaviors may translate into psychologically or physically deleterious impacts. Second, it is important to consider the behavioral impacts of contraception not in isolation, but rather within the framework of all the other potential means of effective population control (Kirkpatrick, 2007). This holistic evaluation of the relative costs and beneļ¬ts of all available options should be underpinned by rigorous data. The ļ¬ndings of the current study contribute to the growing database on the effects of contraception in wildlife that will be crucial in these types of decision making. Acknowledgments We sincerely thank Sue Wiper and Guillaume and Sophie de Turckheim for allowing us to work at Trentham Monkey Forest. Frimpong Twum provided expert technical support. We thank also Charlotte Carne and Ann MacLarnon for their thoughtful comments on an earlier draft of this manuscript. References Asa, C. S. (1997). The development of contraceptive methods for captive wildlife. In T. J. Kreeger (Ed.), Contraception in wildlife management (Technical Bulletin 1853, pp. 235ā€“240). Washington, DC: U.S. Department of Agriculture, Animal and Plant Health Inspection Service. Asa, C. S. (2005). Types of contraception: The choices. In C. S. Asa & I. J. Porton (Eds.), Wildlife contraception: Issues, methods, and applications (pp. 29ā€“52). Baltimore, MD: Johns Hopkins University Press. Asa, C. S., & Porton, I. J. (2005). Introduction: The need for wildlife contraception. In C. S. Asa & I. J. Porton (Eds.), Wildlife contraception: Issues, methods, and applications (pp. xxvā€“xxxii). Baltimore, MD: Johns Hopkins University Press. Asa, C. S., Porton, I. J., & Calle, P. P. (2005). Choosing the most appropriate contraceptive. In C. S. Asa & I. J. Porton (Eds.), Wildlife contraception: Issues, methods, and applications (pp. 83ā€“95). Baltimore, MD: Johns Hopkins University Press. Aureli, F., Preston, S. D., & de Waal, F. B. M. (1999). Heart rate responses to social interactions in free-moving rhesus macaques (Macaca mulatta): A pilot study. Journal of Comparative Psychology, 113, 59ā€“65. Bennink, H. J. (2000). The pharmacokinetics and pharmacodynamics of Implanon, a single-rod etonogestrel contraceptive implant. European Journal of Contraception & Reproductive Health Care, 5(2), 12ā€“20. Berridge, C. W., Mitton, E., Clark, W., & Roth, R. H. (1999). Engagement in a non-escape (displacement) behavior elicits a selective and lateralized suppression of frontal cortical dopaminergic utilization in stress. Synapse, 32, 187ā€“197. Bertrand, M. R., DeNicola, A. J., Beissinger, S. R., & Swihart, R. K. (1996). Effects of parturition on home ranges and social afļ¬liations of female white-tailed deer. Journal of Wildlife Management, 60, 899ā€“909. Boccia, M. L., Laudenslager, M., & Reite, M. (1989). On the physiology of grooming in a pigtail macaque. Physiology and Behavior, 45, 667ā€“670. Carne, C., Wiper, S., & Semple, S. (2011). Reciprocation and interchange of grooming, agonistic support, feeding tolerance, and aggression in semi-free-ranging Barbary macaques. American Journal of Primatology, 73, 1127ā€“1133. 6 MAIJER AND SEMPLE Downloadedby[UniversityofRoehampton],[StuartSemple]at06:5223November2015
  • 8. Cortes, J., & Shaw, E. (2006). The Gibraltar macaques: Management and future. In J. K. Hodges & J. Cortes (Eds.), The Barbary macaque: Biology, managements, and conservation (pp. 199ā€“210). Nottingham, UK: Nottingham University Press. Crockford, C., Wittig, R. M., Lehmann, J., Whitten, P. L., Seyfarth, R. M., & Cheney, D. (2008). Focused grooming networks and stress alleviation in wild female baboons. Hormones and Behavior, 54, 170ā€“177. Croxatto, H. B. (2002). Mechanisms that explain the contraceptive action of progestin implants for women. Contraception, 65, 21ā€“27. Croxatto, H. B., & Makarainen, L. (1998). The pharmacodynamics and efļ¬cacy of Implanon: An overview of the data. Contraception, 58(Suppl. 1), 91ā€“97. Cullberg, J. (1972). Mood changes and menstrual symptoms with different gestagen/estrogen combinations. Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica, 236, 1ā€“86. De Vleeschouwer, K., Leus, K., & Van Elsacker, L. (2000). An evaluation of the suitability of contraceptive methods in golden-headed lion tamarins (Leontopithicus chrysomelas), with emphasis on melengestrol acetate (MGA) implants: (II) Endocrinological and behavioral effects. Animal Welfare, 9, 385ā€“401. Dunbar, R. I. M. (1988). Primate social systems. Ithaca, NY: Cornell University Press. Gray, M. E., & Cameron, E. Z. (2010). Does contraceptive treatment in wildlife result in side effects? A review of quantitative and anecdotal evidence. Reproduction, 139, 45ā€“55. Henderson, J. A., & Shively, C. A. (2004). Triphasic oral contraceptive treatment alters the behavior and neurobiology of female cynomolgus monkeys. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 29, 21ā€“34. Hennessy, M. B., & Foy, T. (1987). Nonedible material elicits chewing and reduces the plasma corticosterone response during novelty exposure in mice. Behavior and Neuroscience, 101, 237ā€“245. Hori, N., Yuyama, N., & Tamura, K. (2004). Biting suppresses stress-induced expression of corticotropin-releasing factor (CRF) in the rat hypothalamus. Journal of Dental Research, 83, 124ā€“128. Jelinek, J. E. (1970). Cutaneous side effect of oral contraceptives. Archives of Dermatology, 101, 181ā€“186. Keverne, E. B., Martensz, N. D., & Tuite, B. (1989). Beta-endorphin concentrations in cerebrospinal ļ¬‚uid of monkeys are inļ¬‚uenced by grooming relationships. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 14, 155ā€“161. Kirkpatrick, J. F. (2007). Measuring the effects of wildlife contraception: The argument for comparing apples with oranges. Reproduction, Fertility, and Development, 19, 548ā€“552. Kirkpatrick, J. F., & Frank, K. M. (2005). Contraception in free-ranging wildlife. In C. S. Asa & I. J. Porton (Eds.), Wildlife contraception: Issues, methods, and applications (pp. 195ā€“221). Baltimore, MD: Johns Hopkins University Press. Maestripieri, D., Schino, G., Aureli, F., & Troisi, A. (1992). A modest proposal: Displacement activities as an indicator of emotions in primates. Animal Behaviour, 44, 967ā€“979. Mohiyeddini, C., & Semple, S. (2012). Displacement behaviour regulates the experience of stress in men. Stress, 16, 163ā€“171. Munson, L., Moresco, A., & Calle, P. P. (2005). Adverse effects of contraceptives. In C. S. Asa & I. J. Porton (Eds.), Wildlife contraception: Issues, methods, and applications (pp. 66ā€“82). Baltimore, MD: Johns Hopkins University Press. Paul, A., & Kuester, J. (1987). Dominance, kinship, and reproductive value in female Barbary macaques (Macaca sylvanus) at Affenberg Salem. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology, 21, 1885ā€“1891. Pazol, K., Wilson, M. E., & Wallen, K. (2004). Medroxyprogesterone acetate antagonizes the effects of estrogen treatment on social and sexual behavior in female macaques. Journal of Clinical Endocrinology & Metabolism, 89, 2998ā€“3006. Penfold, L. M., Patton, M. L., & Jochle, W. (2005). Contraception agents in aggression control. In C. S. Asa & I. J. Porton (Eds.), Wildlife contraception: Issues, methods, and applications Zoo and Aquarium Biology and Conservation Series (pp. 184ā€“195). Washington, DC: Smithsonian Institution. Pico-Alfonso, M. A., Mastorci, F., Ceresini, G., Ceda, G. P., Manghi, M., Pino, O. . . . Sgoifo, A. (2007). Acute psychosocial challenge and cardiac autonomic response in women: The role of estrogens, corticosteroids, and behavioral coping styles. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 32, 451ā€“463. Plowman, A. B., Jordan, N. R., Anderson, N., Condon, E., & Fraser, O. (2004). Welfare implications of captive primate population management: Behavioral and psycho-social effects of female-based contraception, oestrus and male removal in hamadryas baboons (Papio hamadryas). Applied Animal Behaviour Science, 90, 155ā€“165. Poromaa, I. S., & Segebladh, B. (2012). Adverse mood symptoms with oral contraceptives. Acta Obstetricia et Gynecologica Scandinavica, 91, 420ā€“427. Porton, I. J., & Dematteo, K. E. (2005). Contraception in nonhuman primates. In C. S. Asa & I. J. Porton (Eds.), Wildlife contraception: Issues, methods, and applications Zoo and Aquarium Biology and Conservation Series (pp. 119ā€“148). Washington, DC: Smithsonian Institution. Schino, G., Perretta, G., Taglioni, A. M., Monaco, V., & Troisi, A. (1996). Primate displacement activities as an ethopharmacological model of anxiety. Anxiety, 2, 186ā€“191. Shively, C. A., Manuck, S. B., Kaplan, J. R., & Koritnik, D. R. (1990). Oral contraceptive administration, interfemale relationships, and sexual behavior in Macaca fascicularis. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 19, 101ā€“117. Shutt, K., MacLarnon, A., Heistermann, M., & Semple, S. (2007). Grooming in Barbary macaques: Better to give than receive? Biology Letters, 3, 231ā€“233. EFFECTS OF IMPLANON ON MACAQUE BEHAVIOR 7 Downloadedby[UniversityofRoehampton],[StuartSemple]at06:5223November2015
  • 9. Silk, J. B. (1987). Activities and feeding behavior of free-ranging pregnant baboons. International Journal of Primatology, 8, 593ā€“613. Toufexis, D. J., Myers, K. M., & Davis, M. (2006). The effect of gonadal hormones and gender on anxiety and emotional learning. Hormones and Behavior, 50, 539ā€“549. Varma, R., & Mascarenhas, L. (2001). Endometrial effects of etonogestrel (Implanon) contraceptive implant. Current Opinion in Obstetrics & Gynecology, 13, 335ā€“341. Wallner, B., Mƶstl, E., & Dittami, J. (2007). Chronic levonorgestrel treatment in Macaca sylvanus: Effects on perineal swelling size and fecal sex steroid excretion. Neuroendocrinology Letters, 28, 326ā€“332. Yates, F., & Aureli, K. (2010). Distress prevention by grooming others in crested black macaques. Biology Letters, 6, 27ā€“29. 8 MAIJER AND SEMPLE Downloadedby[UniversityofRoehampton],[StuartSemple]at06:5223November2015