SlideShare a Scribd company logo
1 of 7
Download to read offline
Psychological Science
1­–7
© The Author(s) 2015
Reprints and permissions:
sagepub.com/journalsPermissions.nav
DOI: 10.1177/0956797615586403
pss.sagepub.com
Research Report
Evidence indicates that men perceive women dressed in
red as more sexually attractive (Elliot & Niesta, 2008;
Elliot, Tracy, Pazda, & Beall, 2013; Niesta Kayser, Elliot, &
Feltman, 2010; Pazda, Elliot, & Greitemeyer, 2012;
Schwarz & Singer, 2013) and that women use red cloth-
ing as a courtship tactic, insofar as they are more likely to
choose red clothes when they anticipate meeting poten-
tial mates (Elliot, Greitemeyer, & Pazda, 2013; Elliot &
Pazda, 2012; Prokop & Hromada, 2013). Beall and Tracy
(2013) argued further that red clothing may be an observ-
able cue of women’s fecundity; this assertion was based
on their finding that women were more likely to wear a
red or pink top on high-fertility days of the menstrual
cycle. Subsequent studies, however, provided mixed
(Tracy & Beall, 2014) and null (Prokop & Hromada, 2013)
support for the cycle-phase effect.
The studies that examined cycle-phase effects on red-
clothing choices were limited by use of between-subjects
comparisons and error-prone counting methods to deter-
mine menstrual phase. Some researchers have argued
that the use of counting methods by Beall and Tracy
(2013) may have allowed flexibility in the definition of
fertile cycle days that could have rendered their finding a
false positive (Gelman, 2013; cf. Tracy & Beall, 2013). In
the study reported here, we tested the cycle-phase shift
in red-clothing choices using a within-participants design
in which we used frequent hormone sampling to identify
the timing of ovulation.
We also directly tested hormonal predictors of red-
clothing choices. In nonhuman primates, the red sex-skin
swellings that advertise fecundity and correlate with sex-
ual receptivity are promoted by estradiol and inhibited
by progesterone (Emery Thompson, 2009). Women’s
libido likewise appears to fluctuate positively with shifts
in estradiol but negatively with shifts in progesterone
586403PSSXXX10.1177/0956797615586403Eisenbruch et al.Hormonal Predictors of Clothing Choices
research-article2015
Corresponding Author:
James R. Roney, Department of Psychological and Brain Sciences,
University of California, Santa Barbara, CA 93106-9660
E-mail: roney@psych.ucsb.edu
Lady in Red: Hormonal Predictors of
Women’s Clothing Choices
Adar B. Eisenbruch1
, Zachary L. Simmons2
, and
James R. Roney1
1
Department of Psychological and Brain Sciences, University of California, Santa Barbara, and
2
Department of Psychological Sciences, University of Portland
Abstract
Recent evidence supports the idea that women use red clothing as a courtship tactic, and results from one study further
suggested that women were more likely to wear red on days of high fertility in their menstrual cycles. Subsequent
studies provided mixed support for the cycle-phase effect, although all such studies relied on counting methods of
cycle-phase estimation and used between-subjects designs. By comparison, in the study reported here, we employed
frequent hormone sampling to more accurately assess ovulatory timing and used a within-subjects design. We found
that women were more likely to wear red during the fertile window than on other cycle days. Furthermore, within-
subjects fluctuations in the ratio of estradiol to progesterone statistically mediated the within-subjects shifts in red-
clothing choices. Our results appear to represent the first direct demonstration of specific hormone measurements
predicting observable changes in women’s courtship-related behaviors. We also demonstrate the advantages of
hormonal determination of ovulatory timing for tests of cycle-phase shifts in psychology or behavior.
Keywords
estradiol, progesterone, fertile window, cycle phase, red, mating psychology, open data
Received 1/17/15; Revision accepted 4/21/15
Psychological Science OnlineFirst, published on July 8, 2015 as doi:10.1177/0956797615586403
at UNIVERSIDAD DEL DESARROLLO on July 15, 2015pss.sagepub.comDownloaded from
2	 Eisenbruch et al.
(Roney & Simmons, 2013). Therefore, reasoning that red
clothing may reflect enhanced mating motivation, we
predicted that the odds of wearing red would be posi-
tively associated with within-subjects shifts in estradiol
and negatively associated with within-subjects shifts in
progesterone. Although our a priori hypotheses per-
tained to the individual hormones on the basis of previ-
ous findings for libido, we also tested the effects of the
estradiol-to-progesterone ratio, given our interest in
cycle-phase shifts, because this ratio better predicts fer-
tile-window timing (i.e., cycle days when conception is
possible) than does either hormone in isolation (Baird,
Weinberg, Wilcox, McConnaughey, & Musey, 1991).
Method
Participants and procedure
We took photos of a sample of naturally cycling women
who participated in a broader study on the relationship
between ovarian hormones and mating behavior (see
Grillot, Simmons, Lukaszewski, & Roney, 2014; Roney &
Simmons, 2013). A total of 52 women participated in the
overall study, but only data from 46 women who con-
sented to use of their photos were analyzed here (mean
age = 18.80 years, SD = 1.22). Twenty-five of these
women identified themselves as White, 10 as Asian, 7 as
Hispanic, and 4 as “other.” Further details regarding this
sample can be found in Roney and Simmons (2013).
Participants provided written informed consent for their
participation, and all procedures were approved by the
institutional review board at the University of California,
Santa Barbara.
Participants attended four afternoon laboratory ses-
sions during either one or two menstrual cycles. The ses-
sions were spaced approximately 1 week apart, so each
woman participated in either four or eight laboratory ses-
sions. During each session, women provided saliva sam-
ples via passive drool and were photographed under
standard lighting conditions in the clothing that they had
worn to the lab. Thirty-six women provided photos
across two menstrual cycles (separated by 1–2 months)
and 10 women provided photos in one cycle.
As part of the broader study, the participants also col-
lected daily saliva samples each morning. Hormones were
assayed from a subset of these to estimate day of ovula-
tion for the fertile-window analyses (see Estimation of
Fertile-Window Timing). Only samples collected from the
afternoon laboratory sessions were used to test hormonal
predictors of red-clothing choices because hormones
were assayed from every laboratory-session saliva sample
but from only a subset of the daily morning samples.
All data and stimuli from the participants were col-
lected years before the authors considered examining
red-clothing choices. Therefore, sample size was deter-
mined by the original data collection. The original study
was designed to examine hormonal predictors of wom-
en’s sexual psychology and behavior, as well as their
physical attractiveness. Sample size was determined as
the largest number of participants for whom available
funding would allow daily hormone assays across two
menstrual cycles.
Measures
Hormone measures. Saliva samples were shipped to
the Endocrine Core Laboratory of the California Regional
Primate Research Center (Davis, CA), where they were
assayed for concentrations of estradiol, testosterone, and
progesterone. Samples were centrifuged at 3,000 rpm for
20 min to separate the aqueous component from other
particles. Estradiol concentrations were estimated in
duplicate using the high-sensitivity salivary 17β-estradiol
enzyme immunoassay kit (Salimetrics LLC, State College,
PA). The estradiol assay has a least detectable dose of 0.1
pg/ml; intra-assay and interassay coefficients of variation
(CVs) were 4.52% and 8.15%, respectively. Concentra-
tions of progesterone were estimated in duplicate using
commercial radioimmunoassay kits (Siemens Health
Diagnostics, Inc., Los Angeles, CA). The procedures were
modified as follows to accommodate overall lower levels
of progesterone in human saliva relative to plasma: (a)
Standards were diluted to concentrations ranging from
0.05 to 4.0 ng/ml, and (b) sample volume was increased
to 200 µl. The progesterone assay has a least detectable
dose of 0.00914 ng/ml; intra-assay and interassay CVs
were 4.57% and 7.36%, respectively. Concentrations of
testosterone were estimated in duplicate using commer-
cial double-antibody radioimmunoassay kits (Beckman
Coulter Inc., Webster, TX). The testosterone assay has a
least detectable dose of 1.3697 pg/ml; intra-assay and
interassay CVs were 5.20% and 9.83%, respectively.
All saliva samples from the laboratory sessions were
assayed. Mean concentrations were 2.96 pg/ml (SD =
1.23) for estradiol, 0.05 ng/ml (SD = 0.04) for progester-
one, and 26.15 pg/ml (SD = 14.98) for testosterone. For
the daily samples, all samples in a 9-day window sur-
rounding the initial estimated day of ovulation (com-
puted as 15 days before the end of each cycle) were sent
for assay, as were samples from alternating days outside
of this window. The estradiol-to-progesterone ratio was
computed by first converting the unit of measure for each
hormone to picomoles per liter and then dividing the
value for estradiol by the value for progesterone.
Red-clothing measures. Photos from the participants’
lab sessions were coded independently by two female
research assistants who were blind to the participants’
at UNIVERSIDAD DEL DESARROLLO on July 15, 2015pss.sagepub.comDownloaded from
Hormonal Predictors of Clothing Choices	3
hormonal status, using criteria from Beall and Tracy
(2013). Research assistants were instructed to code the
color (black, blue, gray, green, pink, red, white, yellow, or
other) of each visible item in seven categories of clothing
(dress, shirt/blouse, jacket or sweatshirt, pants, shorts,
skirt, and accessories). If an item was multicolored, they
were instructed to pick the color that was most prevalent.
Because the lower body was not fully visible in all photo-
graphs, and because past research on red clothing has
focused on the color of shirts (Beall & Tracy, 2013), the
dependent variable for our data analyses was whether the
subject was coded as wearing a top garment (i.e., shirt,
sweatshirt, dress, or jacket) that was red or pink (hereafter
referred to as red). Coders discussed each case in which
they disagreed on the presence of a red top (n = 2) and
then reached a joint decision regarding the final coding.
Data analysis
Estimation of fertile-window timing. To classify
individual photo sessions as being inside or outside of
the fertile window, we used the daily hormone values to
estimate the day of ovulation in each cycle. We first
judged whether cycles were ovulatory by following the
procedures in Ellison, Lager, and Calfee (1987), in which
cycles that did not achieve a maximum progesterone
value of at least 300 pmol/L were judged anovulatory; 53
of 82 total cycles were judged ovulatory. (Note that in the
previous articles in which cycle-phase effects on red-
clothing choices were assessed, ovulatory status was not
confirmed, which was an additional source of error.)
Next, for each cycle identified as ovulatory, we deter-
mined the day of peak estradiol. Following the method
of Lipson and Ellison (1996), we then found the two con-
secutive days (including or after the estradiol peak)
between which there was the greatest decrease in estra-
diol. The second of those two days was considered the
day of ovulation (for further details, see Grillot et  al.,
2014). The fertile window was then defined as the day of
ovulation and the 5 days preceding it (see Wilcox, Wein-
berg, & Baird, 1995).
To compare findings across different methods of esti-
mating ovulatory timing, we also computed high-fertility
days using the method of Beall and Tracy (2013). They
specified Days 6 to 14 after the start of menses as high-
fertility days and Days 0 to 5 and 15 to 28 after menses
onset as low-fertility days. We counted forward from the
participants’ self-reported last onset of menses to com-
pute these two time windows.
Statistical models.  We constructed binary mixed logis-
tic regression models in IBM SPSS Statistics (Version 22)
to model the probability of wearing a red top as a dichot-
omous outcome. Although our data were technically on
three levels (laboratory sessions nested within cycles
nested within women), we took the advice of statistical
consultants and constructed two-level models (sessions
nested within women), given the known convergence
problems in logistic mixed regression models with few
higher level units (in this case, two cycles). The two-level
structure meant that the probability of wearing red was
modeled across four to eight sessions within each woman,
depending on whether she contributed one or two cycles
of data. All models included a random subject-level inter-
cept and used robust estimation of standard errors.
To place all variables on the same metric, we first stan-
dardized values for hormone predictor variables relative
to their respective grand means, but we then group-mean
centered the standardized values within cycles. Thus, for
women with two cycles of data, each hormone value was
centered relative to the mean of the cycle from which it
was drawn rather than relative to the woman’s overall
mean. This centering decision means that regression mod-
els assessed the effects of within-cycle shifts in hormones.
Centered hormone values that were more than 3 standard
deviations higher or lower than their respective means
were excluded to avoid undue influence of outliers (there
was one such outlier each for testosterone, progesterone,
and the estradiol-to-progesterone ratio), though results
were very similar when outliers were included. Fertile-
window timing was entered as a dummy variable. Models
including the fertile-window variable were computed
only for ovulatory cycles, whereas models containing
only hormonal predictors were estimated for all cycles.
Tests for mediation of fertile-window effects by hormonal
predictors were conducted using the Monte Carlo method
for assessing mediation as described in Selig and Preacher
(2008), with adjustment of coefficients to account for the
dichotomous outcome variable (see Herr, 2014).
Results
Fertile-window timing
Across all ovulatory cycles, the participants wore red 17%
(8 of 48 days) of the time during the fertile window but
only 8% (13 of 160 days) of the time on other cycle days.
A mixed regression model confirmed that, within subjects,
the odds of wearing red were higher during the estimated
fertile window than on other cycle days, b = 0.93, p = .040,
odds ratio (OR) = 2.53, 95% confidence interval (CI) =
[1.04, 6.14]. The 2.53 odds ratio indicates that the odds of
wearing a red top were about 2.5 times higher inside the
fertile window, but there was a wide confidence interval.
Using Beall and Tracy’s (2013) counting method of
estimating high fertility (see Method), we found that red
was worn on 11% (8 of 76) of high-fertility days and on
a virtually identical 10% (13 of 129) of low-fertility days.
at UNIVERSIDAD DEL DESARROLLO on July 15, 2015pss.sagepub.comDownloaded from
4	 Eisenbruch et al.
A mixed regression model confirmed the absence of a
within-subjects fertility effect when using this method,
b = 0.09, p > .250, OR = 1.09, 95% CI = [0.47, 2.55]. These
results pertain only to ovulatory cycles, but application of
this method to all sessions still produced a null result
(i.e., a 10% rate of wearing red on both high- and low-
fertility days).
Hormonal predictors
Table 1 presents results of separate mixed logistic regres-
sion models to test the zero-order effects of each mea-
sured hormone and the estradiol-to-progesterone ratio
on the odds of wearing red (we had no predictions for
testosterone, but we included it given the common
belief that testosterone affects women’s libido; however,
see Roney & Simmons, 2013). Only the estradiol-to-­
progesterone ratio had a confidence interval for the odds
ratio that excluded 1. For each 1-SD increase in the estra-
diol-to-progesterone ratio above a cycle mean, the odds
of wearing red nearly doubled. Null results for the three
individual hormones persisted when all were entered
simultaneously into the same regression model (without
the estradiol-to-progesterone ratio), whereas the effect
size for the estradiol-to-progesterone ratio was even
larger when added to a model that included the three
individual hormones, b = 0.84, p = .009, OR = 2.33, 95%
CI = [1.24, 4.36].
Next, for the subset of ovulatory cycles, we simultane-
ously entered fertile-window timing and the estradiol-to-
progesterone ratio into a logistic mixed regression model.
In this model, the confidence interval for the odds ratio
associated with fertile window timing no longer excluded
1, b = 0.47, p > .250, OR = 1.60, 95% CI = [0.63, 4.03],
whereas the interval for the estradiol-to-progesterone
ratio did exclude 1, b = 0.65, p = .041, OR = 1.92, 95%
CI = [1.03, 3.58]. In a Monte Carlo simulation with 20,000
random draws, the estradiol-to-progesterone ratio did in
fact mediate the effect of fertile-window timing on the
probability of wearing red, indirect effect = 0.042, 95%
CI = [0.002, 0.09].
Figure 1 presents the mean estradiol-to-progesterone
ratio and mean probability of wearing red aggregated
across all ovulatory cycles and plotted against cycle days.
The fertile-window effect is visible as the increased prob-
ability of red-clothing choices in the late follicular phase
relative to most other regions of the cycle. The corre-
spondence between the estradiol-to-progesterone ratio
and frequency of red clothing is also clearly visible; both
variables show sharp declines from their follicular-phase
peaks after the onset of the nonfecund luteal phase.
Discussion
The present findings support the idea that women more
frequently choose to wear red clothing during the fertile
window than on other days of their menstrual cycles.
Previous studies had provided mixed evidence for this
effect (Beall & Tracy, 2013; Prokop & Hromada, 2013;
Tracy & Beall, 2014) but were limited by between-subject
analyses, error-prone methods of estimating ovulatory
timing, and an inability to exclude anovulatory cycles.
Here, we used hormonal confirmation of ovulatory tim-
ing to demonstrate this effect within subjects. These
results suggest that women are more likely to exploit
men’s preference for women wearing red (see the intro-
duction) on days when conception is possible than on
other days in the cycle. This complements other findings
of women’s increased motivation to enhance their attrac-
tiveness during the fertile window (e.g., Durante, Li, &
Haselton, 2008).
It is not entirely clear why red in particular is used to
enhance attractiveness. Women’s facial redness is posi-
tively related to shifts in estradiol (Jones et al., 2015), and
red flushing has been associated with sexual arousal
(Katchadourian, 1984). Men may therefore prefer red as a
marker of hormone production or sexual interest, which
women may exploit when their sexual motivation is
higher. Further research on ultimate causes is necessary.
Our results highlight limitations of counting methods
of cycle-phase estimation (see Gildersleeve, Haselton, &
Fales, 2014). Our classification of fertile-window timing
via frequent hormone sampling (see Baird et al., 1991;
Lipson & Ellison, 1996) agreed with the counting method
used by Beall and Tracy (2013) in only 64% of total cases,
and 46% of hormone-identified high-fertility days were
classified as low fertility by the counting method. Beall
and Tracy’s classification of Days 6 to 14 as high-fertility
days may be especially problematic given evidence that
later days are often fertile (Wilcox, Dunson, Weinberg,
Trussell, & Baird, 2001). However, other counting meth-
ods could be more accurate. Our data nonetheless dem-
onstrate errors associated with common counting
methods and thus lead us to recommend hormonal con-
firmation of ovulatory timing.
Table 1.  Zero-Order Effects of the Hormone Variables on the
Odds of Wearing Red
Hormone variable b p Odds ratio
Estradiol −0.03 > .250 0.97 [0.59, 1.60]
Progesterone −0.27 > .250 0.76 [0.36, 1.63]
Testosterone −0.24 > .250 0.79 [0.31, 1.98]
E:P ratio 0.63 .010 1.88 [1.17, 3.03]
Note: The values in square brackets are 95% confidence intervals.
Each row represents a separate binary mixed logistic regression
model predicting red clothing as a dichotomous outcome. E:P ratio =
estradiol-to-progesterone ratio.
at UNIVERSIDAD DEL DESARROLLO on July 15, 2015pss.sagepub.comDownloaded from
Hormonal Predictors of Clothing Choices	5
We also found that the estradiol-to-progesterone ratio
positively predicted the odds of wearing red clothing, and
that this ratio mediated the fertile-window effect. This is
the first direct demonstration of statistical mediation of a
fertile-window effect by hormone variables, but it should
be interpreted with some caution. Contrary to our hypoth-
eses, we did not observe main effects of estradiol and
progesterone but rather a nonadditive effect produced by
their ratio. Previous work examining daily fluctuations in
women’s libido demonstrated only additive effects of
these hormones (Roney & Simmons, 2013), whereas stud-
ies on the motivational salience of sexually dimorphic
faces also found effects of the estradiol-to-progesterone
ratio (Wang, Hahn, Fisher, DeBruine, & Jones, 2014).
Variables that are tightly coupled to the fertile window
may be especially sensitive to this ratio, given how closely
it tracks fluctuations in fecundity (Baird et al., 1991), but
future research will be necessary to test this possibility
and to systematically map the additive and nonadditive
psychological effects of ovarian hormones.
In conclusion, we found positive effects of the fertile
window and the estradiol-to-progesterone ratio on wom-
en’s odds of wearing red. The finding of a fertile-window
effect replicates a contested effect (Beall & Tracy, 2013;
Gelman, 2013) and complements other studies in sug-
gesting that the motivational salience of mate attraction
increases on fecund cycle days (e.g., Durante et al., 2008).
The findings regarding hormones complement recent
studies that have provided evidence for hormonal predic-
tors of cycle-phase shifts in mate preferences (e.g.,
Pisanski et  al., 2014; Roney & Simmons, 2008; Roney,
Simmons, & Gray, 2011) and extends this literature to
include hormonal prediction of women’s observable
courtship behaviors.
Author Contributions
A. B. Eisenbruch and J. R. Roney designed the study. Z. L.
Simmons and J. R. Roney collected the original data set. A. B.
Eisenbruch and J. R. Roney conducted the data analysis and
drafted the manuscript, and Z. L. Simmons provided critical
revisions. All authors approved the final version of the manu-
script for submission.
Acknowledgments
We thank Shelly Gable for statistical advice, Ellise Fallon and
Samantha Wilcoxon for data assistance, and Rachel Grillot for
comments on the manuscript. Other analyses from this data set
have been published in Roney and Simmons (2013) and Grillot,
Simmons, Lukaszewski, and Roney (2014).
Declaration of Conflicting Interests
The authors declared that they had no conflicts of interest with
respect to their authorship or the publication of this article.
.00
.05
.10
.15
.20
.25
–1.50
–1.00
–0.50
0.00
0.50
1.00
< −11 −11 to −9 −8 to −6 −5 to −3 −2 to 0 1 to 3 4 to 6 7 to 9 10 to 13
MeanProbabilityofWearingRed
Cycle Days Relative to Ovulation
Estradiol-to-Progesterone Ratio
Mean Probability of Wearing Red
Estradiol-to-ProgesteroneRatio(Meanz-Score)
Fig. 1. Estradiol-to-progesterone ratio (left y-axis) and probability of wearing red (right y-axis) plotted against cycle day
relative to ovulation. Day 0 is the estimated day of ovulation. Days in the follicular phase (i.e., days before ovulation) are
numbered negatively, and days in the luteal phase (i.e., days after ovulation) are numbered positively. Error bars represent
95% confidence intervals.
at UNIVERSIDAD DEL DESARROLLO on July 15, 2015pss.sagepub.comDownloaded from
6	 Eisenbruch et al.
Funding
This research was funded by a Hellman Family Faculty
Fellowship; by a University of California, Santa Barbara (UCSB),
Academic Senate grant (both to J. R. Roney); and by a grant
from the Global Centers of Excellence Program of Hokkaido
University to the Center for Evolutionary Psychology at UCSB.
Open Practices
All data have been made publicly available via Merritt and can
be accessed at http://n2t.net/ark:/13030/m5bc629d. The com-
plete Open Practices Disclosure for this article can be found
at http://pss.sagepub.com/content/by/supplemental-data. This
article has received the badge for Open Data. More informa-
tion about the Open Practices badges can be found at https://
osf.io/tvyxz/wiki/1.%20View%20the%20Badges/ and http://
pss.sagepub.com/content/25/1/3.full.
References
Baird, D. D., Weinberg, C. R., Wilcox, A. J., McConnaughey,
D.  R., & Musey, P. I. (1991). Using the ratio of urinary
oestrogen and progesterone metabolites to estimate day of
ovulation. Statistics in Medicine, 10, 255–266.
Beall, A. T., & Tracy, J. L. (2013). Women are more likely to
wear red or pink at peak fertility. Psychological Science,
24, 1837–1841.
Durante, K. M., Li, N. P., & Haselton, M. G. (2008). Changes
in women’s choice of dress across the ovulatory cycle:
Naturalistic and laboratory task-based evidence. Personality
and Social Psychology Bulletin, 34, 1451–1460.
Elliot, A. J., Greitemeyer, T., & Pazda, A. D. (2013). Women’s use
of red clothing as a sexual signal in intersexual interaction.
Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 49, 599–602.
Elliot, A. J., & Niesta, D. (2008). Romantic red: Red enhances
men’s attraction to women. Journal of Personality and
Social Psychology, 95, 1150–1164.
Elliot, A. J., & Pazda, A. D. (2012). Dressed for sex: Red as a
female sexual signal in humans. PLoS ONE, 7(4), Article
e34607. Retrieved from http://journals.plos.org/plosone/
article?id=10.1371/journal.pone.0034607
Elliot, A. J., Tracy, J. L., Pazda, A. D., & Beall, A. T. (2013). Red
enhances women’s attractiveness to men: First evidence
suggesting universality. Journal of Experimental Social
Psychology, 49, 165–168.
Ellison, P. T., Lager, C., & Calfee, J. (1987). Low profiles of sali-
vary progesterone among college undergraduate women.
Journal of Adolescent Health Care, 8, 204–207.
Emery Thompson, M. (2009). The endocrinology of intersex-
ual relationships in the apes. In P. T. Ellison & P. B. Gray
(Eds.), Endocrinology of social relationships (pp. 196–222).
Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
Gelman, A. (2013, July 24). Too good to be true. Slate. Retrieved
from http://www.slate.com
Gildersleeve, K., Haselton, M. G., & Fales, M. R. (2014). Do
women’s mate preferences change across the ovulatory
cycle? A meta-analytic review. Psychological Bulletin, 140,
1205–1259.
Grillot, R. L., Simmons, Z. L., Lukaszewski, A. W., & Roney, J. R.
(2014). Hormonal and morphological predictors of wom-
en’s body attractiveness. Evolution & Human Behavior, 35,
176–183.
Herr, N. (2014). Mediation with dichotomous outcomes
[Computer software]. Retrieved from http://www.nrhpsych
.com/mediation/logmed.html
Jones, B. C., Hahn, A. C., Fisher, C. I., Wincenciak, J.,
Kandrik, M., Roberts, S. C., . . . DeBruine, L. M. (2015).
Facial coloration tracks changes in women’s estradiol.
Psychoneuroendocrinology, 56, 29–34.
Katchadourian, H. A. (1984). Fundamentals of human sexuality
(4th ed.). New York, NY: Holt, Rinehart & Winston.
Lipson, S. F., & Ellison, P. T. (1996). Comparison of salivary
steroid profiles in naturally occurring conception and non-
conception cycles. Human Reproduction, 11, 2090–2096.
Niesta Kayser, D., Elliot, A. J., & Feltman, R. (2010). Red and
romantic behavior in men viewing women. European
Journal of Social Psychology, 40, 901–908.
Pazda, A. D., Elliot, A. J., & Greitemeyer, T. (2012). Sexy red:
Perceived sexual receptivity mediates the red-attraction
relation in men viewing woman. Journal of Experimental
Social Psychology, 48, 787–790.
Pisanski, K., Hahn, A. C., Fisher, C. I., DeBruine, L. M., Feinberg,
D. R., & Jones, B. C. (2014). Changes in salivary estradiol
predict changes in women’s preferences for vocal mascu-
linity. Hormones and Behavior, 66, 493–497.
Prokop, P., & Hromada, M. (2013). Women use red in order to
attract mates. Ethology, 119, 605–613.
Roney, J. R., & Simmons, Z. L. (2008). Women’s estradiol
predicts preference for facial cues of men’s testosterone.
Hormones and Behavior, 53, 14–19.
Roney, J. R., & Simmons, Z. L. (2013). Hormonal predictors of
sexual motivation in natural menstrual cycles. Hormones
and Behavior, 63, 636–645.
Roney, J. R., Simmons, Z. L., & Gray, P. B. (2011). Changes in
estradiol predict within-women shifts in attraction to facial
cues of men’s testosterone. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 36,
742–749.
Schwarz, S., & Singer, M. (2013). Romantic red revisited: Red
enhances men’s attraction to young, but not menopausal
women. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 49,
161–164.
Selig, J. P., & Preacher, K. J. (2008). Monte Carlo method for assess-
ing mediation: An interactive tool for creating confidence
intervals for indirect effects [Computer software]. Available
from http://www.quantpsy.org/medmc/medmc.htm
Tracy, J. L., & Beall, A. T. (2013, July 30). Too good does not
always mean not true [Web log post]. Retrieved from http://
ubc-emotionlab.ca/2013/07/too-good-does-not-always-
mean-not-true/
Tracy, J. L., & Beall, A. T. (2014). The impact of weather on
women’s tendency to wear red or pink when at high risk
for conception. PLoS ONE, 9(2), Article e88852. Retrieved
from http://journals.plos.org/plosone/article?id=10.1371/
journal.pone.0088852
at UNIVERSIDAD DEL DESARROLLO on July 15, 2015pss.sagepub.comDownloaded from
Hormonal Predictors of Clothing Choices	7
Wang, H., Hahn, A. C., Fisher, C. I., DeBruine, L. M., & Jones,
B. C. (2014). Women’s hormone levels modulate the moti-
vational salience of facial attractiveness and sexual dimor-
phism. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 50, 246–251.
Wilcox, A. J., Dunson, D. B., Weinberg, C. R., Trussell, J.,
& Baird, D. D. (2001). Likelihood of conception with a
single act of intercourse: Providing benchmark rates for
assessment of post-coital contraceptives. Contraception,
63, 211–215.
Wilcox, A. J., Weinberg, C. R., & Baird, D. D. (1995). Timing of
sexual intercourse in relation to ovulation: Effects on the
probability of conception, survival of the pregnancy, and
sex of the baby. New England Journal of Medicine, 333,
1517–1521.
at UNIVERSIDAD DEL DESARROLLO on July 15, 2015pss.sagepub.comDownloaded from

More Related Content

What's hot

Maijer and Semple 2015
Maijer and Semple 2015Maijer and Semple 2015
Maijer and Semple 2015Amanda Maijer
 
Lebidine_FullDraft_041715_kws (1)
Lebidine_FullDraft_041715_kws (1)Lebidine_FullDraft_041715_kws (1)
Lebidine_FullDraft_041715_kws (1)Kathryn Lebidine
 
Do Women With Polycystic Morphology Without Any Other Features of PCOS Benefi...
Do Women With Polycystic Morphology Without Any Other Features of PCOS Benefi...Do Women With Polycystic Morphology Without Any Other Features of PCOS Benefi...
Do Women With Polycystic Morphology Without Any Other Features of PCOS Benefi...Alex Swanton
 
A Study Of Age At Menarche And Body Composition In School Girls Of Metro cities
A Study Of Age At Menarche And Body Composition In School Girls Of Metro citiesA Study Of Age At Menarche And Body Composition In School Girls Of Metro cities
A Study Of Age At Menarche And Body Composition In School Girls Of Metro citiesijcite
 
Fatores de risco iu pós prostatectomia
Fatores de risco iu pós prostatectomiaFatores de risco iu pós prostatectomia
Fatores de risco iu pós prostatectomiaclaudiahacad
 
Paper - analysis of primary literature
Paper - analysis of primary literaturePaper - analysis of primary literature
Paper - analysis of primary literatureLaura Patriarca
 
Effectiveness of the atkins diet as a treatment of weight reduction
Effectiveness of the atkins diet as a treatment of weight reductionEffectiveness of the atkins diet as a treatment of weight reduction
Effectiveness of the atkins diet as a treatment of weight reductionAlexander Decker
 
Nutritional Status And Somatotype Study on Sabar males of Purulia
Nutritional Status And Somatotype Study on Sabar males of PuruliaNutritional Status And Somatotype Study on Sabar males of Purulia
Nutritional Status And Somatotype Study on Sabar males of Puruliapratanubanerjee3
 
Prevalence of poor semen parameters in men with metabolic syndrome
Prevalence of poor semen parameters in men with metabolic syndromePrevalence of poor semen parameters in men with metabolic syndrome
Prevalence of poor semen parameters in men with metabolic syndromeDrSathyaBalasubramanyam
 
Obesity Nature Vs Nuture[1]
Obesity  Nature Vs Nuture[1]Obesity  Nature Vs Nuture[1]
Obesity Nature Vs Nuture[1]AndrewRosenthal
 
Integrated Cancer Prevention
Integrated Cancer Prevention Integrated Cancer Prevention
Integrated Cancer Prevention Lisa Nelson
 

What's hot (20)

Cmwh 7-2014-045
Cmwh 7-2014-045Cmwh 7-2014-045
Cmwh 7-2014-045
 
Maijer and Semple 2015
Maijer and Semple 2015Maijer and Semple 2015
Maijer and Semple 2015
 
Group o
Group oGroup o
Group o
 
CORRELATIONS OF WEIGHT FOR HEIGHT % WITH SERUM TRIGLYCERIDE AND TOTAL CHOLEST...
CORRELATIONS OF WEIGHT FOR HEIGHT % WITH SERUM TRIGLYCERIDE AND TOTAL CHOLEST...CORRELATIONS OF WEIGHT FOR HEIGHT % WITH SERUM TRIGLYCERIDE AND TOTAL CHOLEST...
CORRELATIONS OF WEIGHT FOR HEIGHT % WITH SERUM TRIGLYCERIDE AND TOTAL CHOLEST...
 
International Journal of Reproductive Medicine & Gynecology
International Journal of Reproductive Medicine & GynecologyInternational Journal of Reproductive Medicine & Gynecology
International Journal of Reproductive Medicine & Gynecology
 
Lebidine_FullDraft_041715_kws (1)
Lebidine_FullDraft_041715_kws (1)Lebidine_FullDraft_041715_kws (1)
Lebidine_FullDraft_041715_kws (1)
 
Do Women With Polycystic Morphology Without Any Other Features of PCOS Benefi...
Do Women With Polycystic Morphology Without Any Other Features of PCOS Benefi...Do Women With Polycystic Morphology Without Any Other Features of PCOS Benefi...
Do Women With Polycystic Morphology Without Any Other Features of PCOS Benefi...
 
A Study Of Age At Menarche And Body Composition In School Girls Of Metro cities
A Study Of Age At Menarche And Body Composition In School Girls Of Metro citiesA Study Of Age At Menarche And Body Composition In School Girls Of Metro cities
A Study Of Age At Menarche And Body Composition In School Girls Of Metro cities
 
Fatores de risco iu pós prostatectomia
Fatores de risco iu pós prostatectomiaFatores de risco iu pós prostatectomia
Fatores de risco iu pós prostatectomia
 
Paper - analysis of primary literature
Paper - analysis of primary literaturePaper - analysis of primary literature
Paper - analysis of primary literature
 
THE EFFECTS OF CIGARETTE SMOKING ON SEMEN QUALITY OF INFERTILE AND FERTILE ME...
THE EFFECTS OF CIGARETTE SMOKING ON SEMEN QUALITY OF INFERTILE AND FERTILE ME...THE EFFECTS OF CIGARETTE SMOKING ON SEMEN QUALITY OF INFERTILE AND FERTILE ME...
THE EFFECTS OF CIGARETTE SMOKING ON SEMEN QUALITY OF INFERTILE AND FERTILE ME...
 
Effectiveness of the atkins diet as a treatment of weight reduction
Effectiveness of the atkins diet as a treatment of weight reductionEffectiveness of the atkins diet as a treatment of weight reduction
Effectiveness of the atkins diet as a treatment of weight reduction
 
Nutritional Status And Somatotype Study on Sabar males of Purulia
Nutritional Status And Somatotype Study on Sabar males of PuruliaNutritional Status And Somatotype Study on Sabar males of Purulia
Nutritional Status And Somatotype Study on Sabar males of Purulia
 
Smell Study On Weight Loss Effects
Smell Study On Weight Loss EffectsSmell Study On Weight Loss Effects
Smell Study On Weight Loss Effects
 
Prevalence of poor semen parameters in men with metabolic syndrome
Prevalence of poor semen parameters in men with metabolic syndromePrevalence of poor semen parameters in men with metabolic syndrome
Prevalence of poor semen parameters in men with metabolic syndrome
 
TEG_sTBI_AAST_FINAL
TEG_sTBI_AAST_FINALTEG_sTBI_AAST_FINAL
TEG_sTBI_AAST_FINAL
 
Okkkkkk2
Okkkkkk2Okkkkkk2
Okkkkkk2
 
Lab Report 3
Lab Report 3Lab Report 3
Lab Report 3
 
Obesity Nature Vs Nuture[1]
Obesity  Nature Vs Nuture[1]Obesity  Nature Vs Nuture[1]
Obesity Nature Vs Nuture[1]
 
Integrated Cancer Prevention
Integrated Cancer Prevention Integrated Cancer Prevention
Integrated Cancer Prevention
 

Viewers also liked

Recruiting and Retaining Millennials
Recruiting and Retaining MillennialsRecruiting and Retaining Millennials
Recruiting and Retaining Millennialswmiller824
 
Graff Innovation Best Practices v1
Graff Innovation Best Practices v1Graff Innovation Best Practices v1
Graff Innovation Best Practices v1Chris Graff
 
Creative Use of Widgets for Content Management
Creative Use of Widgets for Content ManagementCreative Use of Widgets for Content Management
Creative Use of Widgets for Content ManagementJessica C. Gardner
 
Increasing Student Interest and Response Rates
Increasing Student Interest and Response RatesIncreasing Student Interest and Response Rates
Increasing Student Interest and Response Rateswmiller824
 
Игрушка на ёлку
Игрушка на ёлкуИгрушка на ёлку
Игрушка на ёлкуOlga Grigoriuk
 
MY LAST VACATIONP
MY LAST VACATIONPMY LAST VACATIONP
MY LAST VACATIONPyonalid
 
KRAZZA CV WORLD FORMAT
KRAZZA CV WORLD FORMATKRAZZA CV WORLD FORMAT
KRAZZA CV WORLD FORMATkrazza Layachi
 
Millennial Students
Millennial StudentsMillennial Students
Millennial Studentswmiller824
 
Counter-Pressures and Assessment
Counter-Pressures and AssessmentCounter-Pressures and Assessment
Counter-Pressures and Assessmentwmiller824
 
Make Poverty History
Make Poverty HistoryMake Poverty History
Make Poverty HistoryAaron Lum
 
Vetting Plugins : WordCamp Columbus 2015
Vetting Plugins : WordCamp Columbus 2015Vetting Plugins : WordCamp Columbus 2015
Vetting Plugins : WordCamp Columbus 2015Jessica C. Gardner
 
Resume of David A. Lytle 2015
Resume of David A. Lytle 2015Resume of David A. Lytle 2015
Resume of David A. Lytle 2015David Lytle
 
jolon_amst402_thesis_workdoc
jolon_amst402_thesis_workdocjolon_amst402_thesis_workdoc
jolon_amst402_thesis_workdocLawrence Jolon
 
Integrating Assessment and Faculty Development
Integrating Assessment and Faculty DevelopmentIntegrating Assessment and Faculty Development
Integrating Assessment and Faculty Developmentwmiller824
 

Viewers also liked (20)

Recruiting and Retaining Millennials
Recruiting and Retaining MillennialsRecruiting and Retaining Millennials
Recruiting and Retaining Millennials
 
Graff Innovation Best Practices v1
Graff Innovation Best Practices v1Graff Innovation Best Practices v1
Graff Innovation Best Practices v1
 
Creative Use of Widgets for Content Management
Creative Use of Widgets for Content ManagementCreative Use of Widgets for Content Management
Creative Use of Widgets for Content Management
 
Increasing Student Interest and Response Rates
Increasing Student Interest and Response RatesIncreasing Student Interest and Response Rates
Increasing Student Interest and Response Rates
 
Игрушка на ёлку
Игрушка на ёлкуИгрушка на ёлку
Игрушка на ёлку
 
MY LAST VACATIONP
MY LAST VACATIONPMY LAST VACATIONP
MY LAST VACATIONP
 
โปรแกรมย่อยและฟังก์ชันมาตรฐาน 1
โปรแกรมย่อยและฟังก์ชันมาตรฐาน 1โปรแกรมย่อยและฟังก์ชันมาตรฐาน 1
โปรแกรมย่อยและฟังก์ชันมาตรฐาน 1
 
KRAZZA CV WORLD FORMAT
KRAZZA CV WORLD FORMATKRAZZA CV WORLD FORMAT
KRAZZA CV WORLD FORMAT
 
Millennial Students
Millennial StudentsMillennial Students
Millennial Students
 
vijay resume
vijay resumevijay resume
vijay resume
 
Counter-Pressures and Assessment
Counter-Pressures and AssessmentCounter-Pressures and Assessment
Counter-Pressures and Assessment
 
CPF.pdf
CPF.pdfCPF.pdf
CPF.pdf
 
Madhumita Nag -CV 1
Madhumita Nag -CV 1Madhumita Nag -CV 1
Madhumita Nag -CV 1
 
TriVista - BlogPost - Production Bottlenecks
TriVista - BlogPost - Production BottlenecksTriVista - BlogPost - Production Bottlenecks
TriVista - BlogPost - Production Bottlenecks
 
Make Poverty History
Make Poverty HistoryMake Poverty History
Make Poverty History
 
Vetting Plugins : WordCamp Columbus 2015
Vetting Plugins : WordCamp Columbus 2015Vetting Plugins : WordCamp Columbus 2015
Vetting Plugins : WordCamp Columbus 2015
 
Resume of David A. Lytle 2015
Resume of David A. Lytle 2015Resume of David A. Lytle 2015
Resume of David A. Lytle 2015
 
jolon_amst402_thesis_workdoc
jolon_amst402_thesis_workdocjolon_amst402_thesis_workdoc
jolon_amst402_thesis_workdoc
 
Integrating Assessment and Faculty Development
Integrating Assessment and Faculty DevelopmentIntegrating Assessment and Faculty Development
Integrating Assessment and Faculty Development
 
Gettinger_CV_2015
Gettinger_CV_2015Gettinger_CV_2015
Gettinger_CV_2015
 

Similar to Eisenbruch et al_Lady in Red

Prospective study of infertility in humans due to life style changes
Prospective study of infertility in humans due to life style changesProspective study of infertility in humans due to life style changes
Prospective study of infertility in humans due to life style changesAsma Afreen
 
IOSR Journal of Pharmacy (IOSRPHR)
IOSR Journal of Pharmacy (IOSRPHR)IOSR Journal of Pharmacy (IOSRPHR)
IOSR Journal of Pharmacy (IOSRPHR)iosrphr_editor
 
poster helsinki vespiras - kleur - final
poster helsinki vespiras - kleur - finalposter helsinki vespiras - kleur - final
poster helsinki vespiras - kleur - finalDirk Janssens
 
Icsi cycle with a sperm from tese versus from ejaculate in oligospermic men
Icsi cycle with a sperm from tese versus from ejaculate in oligospermic menIcsi cycle with a sperm from tese versus from ejaculate in oligospermic men
Icsi cycle with a sperm from tese versus from ejaculate in oligospermic menAlexander Decker
 
Arteria Uterina Primer Trimestre Y Rciu
Arteria Uterina Primer Trimestre Y RciuArteria Uterina Primer Trimestre Y Rciu
Arteria Uterina Primer Trimestre Y RciuEliana Cordero
 
Xx determination of self efficacy, body image and sexual
Xx determination of self efficacy, body image and sexualXx determination of self efficacy, body image and sexual
Xx determination of self efficacy, body image and sexualYelmi Reni Putri SY
 
Journal of the Formosan Medical Association (2011) 110, 695e70.docx
Journal of the Formosan Medical Association (2011) 110, 695e70.docxJournal of the Formosan Medical Association (2011) 110, 695e70.docx
Journal of the Formosan Medical Association (2011) 110, 695e70.docxcroysierkathey
 
What makes a girl unattractive
What makes a girl unattractiveWhat makes a girl unattractive
What makes a girl unattractiveNoorUllah Jan
 
Olive palpation sonography_and_barium_study_in_the
Olive palpation sonography_and_barium_study_in_theOlive palpation sonography_and_barium_study_in_the
Olive palpation sonography_and_barium_study_in_theangelicaRAMIREZALTAM
 
Cat clitoral phimosis effects on female sexual function and surgical final
Cat clitoral phimosis effects on female sexual function and surgical finalCat clitoral phimosis effects on female sexual function and surgical final
Cat clitoral phimosis effects on female sexual function and surgical finaladi bachtiar
 
Jenney et al 2016 Early avoidance
Jenney et al 2016 Early avoidanceJenney et al 2016 Early avoidance
Jenney et al 2016 Early avoidanceTeddy Uzamere
 
Poster on clinical significance of sperm morphology assessment a lekshmi
Poster on clinical significance of sperm morphology assessment a lekshmiPoster on clinical significance of sperm morphology assessment a lekshmi
Poster on clinical significance of sperm morphology assessment a lekshmiAiswarya Lekshmi
 
qpcr 1 s2.0-s0015028213005499-main (1)
qpcr           1 s2.0-s0015028213005499-main (1)qpcr           1 s2.0-s0015028213005499-main (1)
qpcr 1 s2.0-s0015028213005499-main (1)鋒博 蔡
 

Similar to Eisenbruch et al_Lady in Red (20)

Prospective study of infertility in humans due to life style changes
Prospective study of infertility in humans due to life style changesProspective study of infertility in humans due to life style changes
Prospective study of infertility in humans due to life style changes
 
International Journal of Reproductive Medicine & Gynecology
International Journal of Reproductive Medicine & GynecologyInternational Journal of Reproductive Medicine & Gynecology
International Journal of Reproductive Medicine & Gynecology
 
IOSR Journal of Pharmacy (IOSRPHR)
IOSR Journal of Pharmacy (IOSRPHR)IOSR Journal of Pharmacy (IOSRPHR)
IOSR Journal of Pharmacy (IOSRPHR)
 
Testosterone Concentrations in Women
Testosterone Concentrations in Women Testosterone Concentrations in Women
Testosterone Concentrations in Women
 
poster helsinki vespiras - kleur - final
poster helsinki vespiras - kleur - finalposter helsinki vespiras - kleur - final
poster helsinki vespiras - kleur - final
 
Icsi cycle with a sperm from tese versus from ejaculate in oligospermic men
Icsi cycle with a sperm from tese versus from ejaculate in oligospermic menIcsi cycle with a sperm from tese versus from ejaculate in oligospermic men
Icsi cycle with a sperm from tese versus from ejaculate in oligospermic men
 
health
healthhealth
health
 
Arteria Uterina Primer Trimestre Y Rciu
Arteria Uterina Primer Trimestre Y RciuArteria Uterina Primer Trimestre Y Rciu
Arteria Uterina Primer Trimestre Y Rciu
 
Xx determination of self efficacy, body image and sexual
Xx determination of self efficacy, body image and sexualXx determination of self efficacy, body image and sexual
Xx determination of self efficacy, body image and sexual
 
AMRS_Project_1_Report
AMRS_Project_1_ReportAMRS_Project_1_Report
AMRS_Project_1_Report
 
Journal of the Formosan Medical Association (2011) 110, 695e70.docx
Journal of the Formosan Medical Association (2011) 110, 695e70.docxJournal of the Formosan Medical Association (2011) 110, 695e70.docx
Journal of the Formosan Medical Association (2011) 110, 695e70.docx
 
What makes a girl unattractive
What makes a girl unattractiveWhat makes a girl unattractive
What makes a girl unattractive
 
Olive palpation sonography_and_barium_study_in_the
Olive palpation sonography_and_barium_study_in_theOlive palpation sonography_and_barium_study_in_the
Olive palpation sonography_and_barium_study_in_the
 
20120331 internet medical journal
20120331 internet medical journal20120331 internet medical journal
20120331 internet medical journal
 
Cat clitoral phimosis effects on female sexual function and surgical final
Cat clitoral phimosis effects on female sexual function and surgical finalCat clitoral phimosis effects on female sexual function and surgical final
Cat clitoral phimosis effects on female sexual function and surgical final
 
Bott et al 2006
Bott et al 2006Bott et al 2006
Bott et al 2006
 
Jenney et al 2016 Early avoidance
Jenney et al 2016 Early avoidanceJenney et al 2016 Early avoidance
Jenney et al 2016 Early avoidance
 
DePauwThesis
DePauwThesisDePauwThesis
DePauwThesis
 
Poster on clinical significance of sperm morphology assessment a lekshmi
Poster on clinical significance of sperm morphology assessment a lekshmiPoster on clinical significance of sperm morphology assessment a lekshmi
Poster on clinical significance of sperm morphology assessment a lekshmi
 
qpcr 1 s2.0-s0015028213005499-main (1)
qpcr           1 s2.0-s0015028213005499-main (1)qpcr           1 s2.0-s0015028213005499-main (1)
qpcr 1 s2.0-s0015028213005499-main (1)
 

Eisenbruch et al_Lady in Red

  • 1. Psychological Science 1­–7 © The Author(s) 2015 Reprints and permissions: sagepub.com/journalsPermissions.nav DOI: 10.1177/0956797615586403 pss.sagepub.com Research Report Evidence indicates that men perceive women dressed in red as more sexually attractive (Elliot & Niesta, 2008; Elliot, Tracy, Pazda, & Beall, 2013; Niesta Kayser, Elliot, & Feltman, 2010; Pazda, Elliot, & Greitemeyer, 2012; Schwarz & Singer, 2013) and that women use red cloth- ing as a courtship tactic, insofar as they are more likely to choose red clothes when they anticipate meeting poten- tial mates (Elliot, Greitemeyer, & Pazda, 2013; Elliot & Pazda, 2012; Prokop & Hromada, 2013). Beall and Tracy (2013) argued further that red clothing may be an observ- able cue of women’s fecundity; this assertion was based on their finding that women were more likely to wear a red or pink top on high-fertility days of the menstrual cycle. Subsequent studies, however, provided mixed (Tracy & Beall, 2014) and null (Prokop & Hromada, 2013) support for the cycle-phase effect. The studies that examined cycle-phase effects on red- clothing choices were limited by use of between-subjects comparisons and error-prone counting methods to deter- mine menstrual phase. Some researchers have argued that the use of counting methods by Beall and Tracy (2013) may have allowed flexibility in the definition of fertile cycle days that could have rendered their finding a false positive (Gelman, 2013; cf. Tracy & Beall, 2013). In the study reported here, we tested the cycle-phase shift in red-clothing choices using a within-participants design in which we used frequent hormone sampling to identify the timing of ovulation. We also directly tested hormonal predictors of red- clothing choices. In nonhuman primates, the red sex-skin swellings that advertise fecundity and correlate with sex- ual receptivity are promoted by estradiol and inhibited by progesterone (Emery Thompson, 2009). Women’s libido likewise appears to fluctuate positively with shifts in estradiol but negatively with shifts in progesterone 586403PSSXXX10.1177/0956797615586403Eisenbruch et al.Hormonal Predictors of Clothing Choices research-article2015 Corresponding Author: James R. Roney, Department of Psychological and Brain Sciences, University of California, Santa Barbara, CA 93106-9660 E-mail: roney@psych.ucsb.edu Lady in Red: Hormonal Predictors of Women’s Clothing Choices Adar B. Eisenbruch1 , Zachary L. Simmons2 , and James R. Roney1 1 Department of Psychological and Brain Sciences, University of California, Santa Barbara, and 2 Department of Psychological Sciences, University of Portland Abstract Recent evidence supports the idea that women use red clothing as a courtship tactic, and results from one study further suggested that women were more likely to wear red on days of high fertility in their menstrual cycles. Subsequent studies provided mixed support for the cycle-phase effect, although all such studies relied on counting methods of cycle-phase estimation and used between-subjects designs. By comparison, in the study reported here, we employed frequent hormone sampling to more accurately assess ovulatory timing and used a within-subjects design. We found that women were more likely to wear red during the fertile window than on other cycle days. Furthermore, within- subjects fluctuations in the ratio of estradiol to progesterone statistically mediated the within-subjects shifts in red- clothing choices. Our results appear to represent the first direct demonstration of specific hormone measurements predicting observable changes in women’s courtship-related behaviors. We also demonstrate the advantages of hormonal determination of ovulatory timing for tests of cycle-phase shifts in psychology or behavior. Keywords estradiol, progesterone, fertile window, cycle phase, red, mating psychology, open data Received 1/17/15; Revision accepted 4/21/15 Psychological Science OnlineFirst, published on July 8, 2015 as doi:10.1177/0956797615586403 at UNIVERSIDAD DEL DESARROLLO on July 15, 2015pss.sagepub.comDownloaded from
  • 2. 2 Eisenbruch et al. (Roney & Simmons, 2013). Therefore, reasoning that red clothing may reflect enhanced mating motivation, we predicted that the odds of wearing red would be posi- tively associated with within-subjects shifts in estradiol and negatively associated with within-subjects shifts in progesterone. Although our a priori hypotheses per- tained to the individual hormones on the basis of previ- ous findings for libido, we also tested the effects of the estradiol-to-progesterone ratio, given our interest in cycle-phase shifts, because this ratio better predicts fer- tile-window timing (i.e., cycle days when conception is possible) than does either hormone in isolation (Baird, Weinberg, Wilcox, McConnaughey, & Musey, 1991). Method Participants and procedure We took photos of a sample of naturally cycling women who participated in a broader study on the relationship between ovarian hormones and mating behavior (see Grillot, Simmons, Lukaszewski, & Roney, 2014; Roney & Simmons, 2013). A total of 52 women participated in the overall study, but only data from 46 women who con- sented to use of their photos were analyzed here (mean age = 18.80 years, SD = 1.22). Twenty-five of these women identified themselves as White, 10 as Asian, 7 as Hispanic, and 4 as “other.” Further details regarding this sample can be found in Roney and Simmons (2013). Participants provided written informed consent for their participation, and all procedures were approved by the institutional review board at the University of California, Santa Barbara. Participants attended four afternoon laboratory ses- sions during either one or two menstrual cycles. The ses- sions were spaced approximately 1 week apart, so each woman participated in either four or eight laboratory ses- sions. During each session, women provided saliva sam- ples via passive drool and were photographed under standard lighting conditions in the clothing that they had worn to the lab. Thirty-six women provided photos across two menstrual cycles (separated by 1–2 months) and 10 women provided photos in one cycle. As part of the broader study, the participants also col- lected daily saliva samples each morning. Hormones were assayed from a subset of these to estimate day of ovula- tion for the fertile-window analyses (see Estimation of Fertile-Window Timing). Only samples collected from the afternoon laboratory sessions were used to test hormonal predictors of red-clothing choices because hormones were assayed from every laboratory-session saliva sample but from only a subset of the daily morning samples. All data and stimuli from the participants were col- lected years before the authors considered examining red-clothing choices. Therefore, sample size was deter- mined by the original data collection. The original study was designed to examine hormonal predictors of wom- en’s sexual psychology and behavior, as well as their physical attractiveness. Sample size was determined as the largest number of participants for whom available funding would allow daily hormone assays across two menstrual cycles. Measures Hormone measures. Saliva samples were shipped to the Endocrine Core Laboratory of the California Regional Primate Research Center (Davis, CA), where they were assayed for concentrations of estradiol, testosterone, and progesterone. Samples were centrifuged at 3,000 rpm for 20 min to separate the aqueous component from other particles. Estradiol concentrations were estimated in duplicate using the high-sensitivity salivary 17β-estradiol enzyme immunoassay kit (Salimetrics LLC, State College, PA). The estradiol assay has a least detectable dose of 0.1 pg/ml; intra-assay and interassay coefficients of variation (CVs) were 4.52% and 8.15%, respectively. Concentra- tions of progesterone were estimated in duplicate using commercial radioimmunoassay kits (Siemens Health Diagnostics, Inc., Los Angeles, CA). The procedures were modified as follows to accommodate overall lower levels of progesterone in human saliva relative to plasma: (a) Standards were diluted to concentrations ranging from 0.05 to 4.0 ng/ml, and (b) sample volume was increased to 200 µl. The progesterone assay has a least detectable dose of 0.00914 ng/ml; intra-assay and interassay CVs were 4.57% and 7.36%, respectively. Concentrations of testosterone were estimated in duplicate using commer- cial double-antibody radioimmunoassay kits (Beckman Coulter Inc., Webster, TX). The testosterone assay has a least detectable dose of 1.3697 pg/ml; intra-assay and interassay CVs were 5.20% and 9.83%, respectively. All saliva samples from the laboratory sessions were assayed. Mean concentrations were 2.96 pg/ml (SD = 1.23) for estradiol, 0.05 ng/ml (SD = 0.04) for progester- one, and 26.15 pg/ml (SD = 14.98) for testosterone. For the daily samples, all samples in a 9-day window sur- rounding the initial estimated day of ovulation (com- puted as 15 days before the end of each cycle) were sent for assay, as were samples from alternating days outside of this window. The estradiol-to-progesterone ratio was computed by first converting the unit of measure for each hormone to picomoles per liter and then dividing the value for estradiol by the value for progesterone. Red-clothing measures. Photos from the participants’ lab sessions were coded independently by two female research assistants who were blind to the participants’ at UNIVERSIDAD DEL DESARROLLO on July 15, 2015pss.sagepub.comDownloaded from
  • 3. Hormonal Predictors of Clothing Choices 3 hormonal status, using criteria from Beall and Tracy (2013). Research assistants were instructed to code the color (black, blue, gray, green, pink, red, white, yellow, or other) of each visible item in seven categories of clothing (dress, shirt/blouse, jacket or sweatshirt, pants, shorts, skirt, and accessories). If an item was multicolored, they were instructed to pick the color that was most prevalent. Because the lower body was not fully visible in all photo- graphs, and because past research on red clothing has focused on the color of shirts (Beall & Tracy, 2013), the dependent variable for our data analyses was whether the subject was coded as wearing a top garment (i.e., shirt, sweatshirt, dress, or jacket) that was red or pink (hereafter referred to as red). Coders discussed each case in which they disagreed on the presence of a red top (n = 2) and then reached a joint decision regarding the final coding. Data analysis Estimation of fertile-window timing. To classify individual photo sessions as being inside or outside of the fertile window, we used the daily hormone values to estimate the day of ovulation in each cycle. We first judged whether cycles were ovulatory by following the procedures in Ellison, Lager, and Calfee (1987), in which cycles that did not achieve a maximum progesterone value of at least 300 pmol/L were judged anovulatory; 53 of 82 total cycles were judged ovulatory. (Note that in the previous articles in which cycle-phase effects on red- clothing choices were assessed, ovulatory status was not confirmed, which was an additional source of error.) Next, for each cycle identified as ovulatory, we deter- mined the day of peak estradiol. Following the method of Lipson and Ellison (1996), we then found the two con- secutive days (including or after the estradiol peak) between which there was the greatest decrease in estra- diol. The second of those two days was considered the day of ovulation (for further details, see Grillot et  al., 2014). The fertile window was then defined as the day of ovulation and the 5 days preceding it (see Wilcox, Wein- berg, & Baird, 1995). To compare findings across different methods of esti- mating ovulatory timing, we also computed high-fertility days using the method of Beall and Tracy (2013). They specified Days 6 to 14 after the start of menses as high- fertility days and Days 0 to 5 and 15 to 28 after menses onset as low-fertility days. We counted forward from the participants’ self-reported last onset of menses to com- pute these two time windows. Statistical models.  We constructed binary mixed logis- tic regression models in IBM SPSS Statistics (Version 22) to model the probability of wearing a red top as a dichot- omous outcome. Although our data were technically on three levels (laboratory sessions nested within cycles nested within women), we took the advice of statistical consultants and constructed two-level models (sessions nested within women), given the known convergence problems in logistic mixed regression models with few higher level units (in this case, two cycles). The two-level structure meant that the probability of wearing red was modeled across four to eight sessions within each woman, depending on whether she contributed one or two cycles of data. All models included a random subject-level inter- cept and used robust estimation of standard errors. To place all variables on the same metric, we first stan- dardized values for hormone predictor variables relative to their respective grand means, but we then group-mean centered the standardized values within cycles. Thus, for women with two cycles of data, each hormone value was centered relative to the mean of the cycle from which it was drawn rather than relative to the woman’s overall mean. This centering decision means that regression mod- els assessed the effects of within-cycle shifts in hormones. Centered hormone values that were more than 3 standard deviations higher or lower than their respective means were excluded to avoid undue influence of outliers (there was one such outlier each for testosterone, progesterone, and the estradiol-to-progesterone ratio), though results were very similar when outliers were included. Fertile- window timing was entered as a dummy variable. Models including the fertile-window variable were computed only for ovulatory cycles, whereas models containing only hormonal predictors were estimated for all cycles. Tests for mediation of fertile-window effects by hormonal predictors were conducted using the Monte Carlo method for assessing mediation as described in Selig and Preacher (2008), with adjustment of coefficients to account for the dichotomous outcome variable (see Herr, 2014). Results Fertile-window timing Across all ovulatory cycles, the participants wore red 17% (8 of 48 days) of the time during the fertile window but only 8% (13 of 160 days) of the time on other cycle days. A mixed regression model confirmed that, within subjects, the odds of wearing red were higher during the estimated fertile window than on other cycle days, b = 0.93, p = .040, odds ratio (OR) = 2.53, 95% confidence interval (CI) = [1.04, 6.14]. The 2.53 odds ratio indicates that the odds of wearing a red top were about 2.5 times higher inside the fertile window, but there was a wide confidence interval. Using Beall and Tracy’s (2013) counting method of estimating high fertility (see Method), we found that red was worn on 11% (8 of 76) of high-fertility days and on a virtually identical 10% (13 of 129) of low-fertility days. at UNIVERSIDAD DEL DESARROLLO on July 15, 2015pss.sagepub.comDownloaded from
  • 4. 4 Eisenbruch et al. A mixed regression model confirmed the absence of a within-subjects fertility effect when using this method, b = 0.09, p > .250, OR = 1.09, 95% CI = [0.47, 2.55]. These results pertain only to ovulatory cycles, but application of this method to all sessions still produced a null result (i.e., a 10% rate of wearing red on both high- and low- fertility days). Hormonal predictors Table 1 presents results of separate mixed logistic regres- sion models to test the zero-order effects of each mea- sured hormone and the estradiol-to-progesterone ratio on the odds of wearing red (we had no predictions for testosterone, but we included it given the common belief that testosterone affects women’s libido; however, see Roney & Simmons, 2013). Only the estradiol-to-­ progesterone ratio had a confidence interval for the odds ratio that excluded 1. For each 1-SD increase in the estra- diol-to-progesterone ratio above a cycle mean, the odds of wearing red nearly doubled. Null results for the three individual hormones persisted when all were entered simultaneously into the same regression model (without the estradiol-to-progesterone ratio), whereas the effect size for the estradiol-to-progesterone ratio was even larger when added to a model that included the three individual hormones, b = 0.84, p = .009, OR = 2.33, 95% CI = [1.24, 4.36]. Next, for the subset of ovulatory cycles, we simultane- ously entered fertile-window timing and the estradiol-to- progesterone ratio into a logistic mixed regression model. In this model, the confidence interval for the odds ratio associated with fertile window timing no longer excluded 1, b = 0.47, p > .250, OR = 1.60, 95% CI = [0.63, 4.03], whereas the interval for the estradiol-to-progesterone ratio did exclude 1, b = 0.65, p = .041, OR = 1.92, 95% CI = [1.03, 3.58]. In a Monte Carlo simulation with 20,000 random draws, the estradiol-to-progesterone ratio did in fact mediate the effect of fertile-window timing on the probability of wearing red, indirect effect = 0.042, 95% CI = [0.002, 0.09]. Figure 1 presents the mean estradiol-to-progesterone ratio and mean probability of wearing red aggregated across all ovulatory cycles and plotted against cycle days. The fertile-window effect is visible as the increased prob- ability of red-clothing choices in the late follicular phase relative to most other regions of the cycle. The corre- spondence between the estradiol-to-progesterone ratio and frequency of red clothing is also clearly visible; both variables show sharp declines from their follicular-phase peaks after the onset of the nonfecund luteal phase. Discussion The present findings support the idea that women more frequently choose to wear red clothing during the fertile window than on other days of their menstrual cycles. Previous studies had provided mixed evidence for this effect (Beall & Tracy, 2013; Prokop & Hromada, 2013; Tracy & Beall, 2014) but were limited by between-subject analyses, error-prone methods of estimating ovulatory timing, and an inability to exclude anovulatory cycles. Here, we used hormonal confirmation of ovulatory tim- ing to demonstrate this effect within subjects. These results suggest that women are more likely to exploit men’s preference for women wearing red (see the intro- duction) on days when conception is possible than on other days in the cycle. This complements other findings of women’s increased motivation to enhance their attrac- tiveness during the fertile window (e.g., Durante, Li, & Haselton, 2008). It is not entirely clear why red in particular is used to enhance attractiveness. Women’s facial redness is posi- tively related to shifts in estradiol (Jones et al., 2015), and red flushing has been associated with sexual arousal (Katchadourian, 1984). Men may therefore prefer red as a marker of hormone production or sexual interest, which women may exploit when their sexual motivation is higher. Further research on ultimate causes is necessary. Our results highlight limitations of counting methods of cycle-phase estimation (see Gildersleeve, Haselton, & Fales, 2014). Our classification of fertile-window timing via frequent hormone sampling (see Baird et al., 1991; Lipson & Ellison, 1996) agreed with the counting method used by Beall and Tracy (2013) in only 64% of total cases, and 46% of hormone-identified high-fertility days were classified as low fertility by the counting method. Beall and Tracy’s classification of Days 6 to 14 as high-fertility days may be especially problematic given evidence that later days are often fertile (Wilcox, Dunson, Weinberg, Trussell, & Baird, 2001). However, other counting meth- ods could be more accurate. Our data nonetheless dem- onstrate errors associated with common counting methods and thus lead us to recommend hormonal con- firmation of ovulatory timing. Table 1.  Zero-Order Effects of the Hormone Variables on the Odds of Wearing Red Hormone variable b p Odds ratio Estradiol −0.03 > .250 0.97 [0.59, 1.60] Progesterone −0.27 > .250 0.76 [0.36, 1.63] Testosterone −0.24 > .250 0.79 [0.31, 1.98] E:P ratio 0.63 .010 1.88 [1.17, 3.03] Note: The values in square brackets are 95% confidence intervals. Each row represents a separate binary mixed logistic regression model predicting red clothing as a dichotomous outcome. E:P ratio = estradiol-to-progesterone ratio. at UNIVERSIDAD DEL DESARROLLO on July 15, 2015pss.sagepub.comDownloaded from
  • 5. Hormonal Predictors of Clothing Choices 5 We also found that the estradiol-to-progesterone ratio positively predicted the odds of wearing red clothing, and that this ratio mediated the fertile-window effect. This is the first direct demonstration of statistical mediation of a fertile-window effect by hormone variables, but it should be interpreted with some caution. Contrary to our hypoth- eses, we did not observe main effects of estradiol and progesterone but rather a nonadditive effect produced by their ratio. Previous work examining daily fluctuations in women’s libido demonstrated only additive effects of these hormones (Roney & Simmons, 2013), whereas stud- ies on the motivational salience of sexually dimorphic faces also found effects of the estradiol-to-progesterone ratio (Wang, Hahn, Fisher, DeBruine, & Jones, 2014). Variables that are tightly coupled to the fertile window may be especially sensitive to this ratio, given how closely it tracks fluctuations in fecundity (Baird et al., 1991), but future research will be necessary to test this possibility and to systematically map the additive and nonadditive psychological effects of ovarian hormones. In conclusion, we found positive effects of the fertile window and the estradiol-to-progesterone ratio on wom- en’s odds of wearing red. The finding of a fertile-window effect replicates a contested effect (Beall & Tracy, 2013; Gelman, 2013) and complements other studies in sug- gesting that the motivational salience of mate attraction increases on fecund cycle days (e.g., Durante et al., 2008). The findings regarding hormones complement recent studies that have provided evidence for hormonal predic- tors of cycle-phase shifts in mate preferences (e.g., Pisanski et  al., 2014; Roney & Simmons, 2008; Roney, Simmons, & Gray, 2011) and extends this literature to include hormonal prediction of women’s observable courtship behaviors. Author Contributions A. B. Eisenbruch and J. R. Roney designed the study. Z. L. Simmons and J. R. Roney collected the original data set. A. B. Eisenbruch and J. R. Roney conducted the data analysis and drafted the manuscript, and Z. L. Simmons provided critical revisions. All authors approved the final version of the manu- script for submission. Acknowledgments We thank Shelly Gable for statistical advice, Ellise Fallon and Samantha Wilcoxon for data assistance, and Rachel Grillot for comments on the manuscript. Other analyses from this data set have been published in Roney and Simmons (2013) and Grillot, Simmons, Lukaszewski, and Roney (2014). Declaration of Conflicting Interests The authors declared that they had no conflicts of interest with respect to their authorship or the publication of this article. .00 .05 .10 .15 .20 .25 –1.50 –1.00 –0.50 0.00 0.50 1.00 < −11 −11 to −9 −8 to −6 −5 to −3 −2 to 0 1 to 3 4 to 6 7 to 9 10 to 13 MeanProbabilityofWearingRed Cycle Days Relative to Ovulation Estradiol-to-Progesterone Ratio Mean Probability of Wearing Red Estradiol-to-ProgesteroneRatio(Meanz-Score) Fig. 1. Estradiol-to-progesterone ratio (left y-axis) and probability of wearing red (right y-axis) plotted against cycle day relative to ovulation. Day 0 is the estimated day of ovulation. Days in the follicular phase (i.e., days before ovulation) are numbered negatively, and days in the luteal phase (i.e., days after ovulation) are numbered positively. Error bars represent 95% confidence intervals. at UNIVERSIDAD DEL DESARROLLO on July 15, 2015pss.sagepub.comDownloaded from
  • 6. 6 Eisenbruch et al. Funding This research was funded by a Hellman Family Faculty Fellowship; by a University of California, Santa Barbara (UCSB), Academic Senate grant (both to J. R. Roney); and by a grant from the Global Centers of Excellence Program of Hokkaido University to the Center for Evolutionary Psychology at UCSB. Open Practices All data have been made publicly available via Merritt and can be accessed at http://n2t.net/ark:/13030/m5bc629d. The com- plete Open Practices Disclosure for this article can be found at http://pss.sagepub.com/content/by/supplemental-data. This article has received the badge for Open Data. More informa- tion about the Open Practices badges can be found at https:// osf.io/tvyxz/wiki/1.%20View%20the%20Badges/ and http:// pss.sagepub.com/content/25/1/3.full. References Baird, D. D., Weinberg, C. R., Wilcox, A. J., McConnaughey, D.  R., & Musey, P. I. (1991). Using the ratio of urinary oestrogen and progesterone metabolites to estimate day of ovulation. Statistics in Medicine, 10, 255–266. Beall, A. T., & Tracy, J. L. (2013). Women are more likely to wear red or pink at peak fertility. Psychological Science, 24, 1837–1841. Durante, K. M., Li, N. P., & Haselton, M. G. (2008). Changes in women’s choice of dress across the ovulatory cycle: Naturalistic and laboratory task-based evidence. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 34, 1451–1460. Elliot, A. J., Greitemeyer, T., & Pazda, A. D. (2013). Women’s use of red clothing as a sexual signal in intersexual interaction. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 49, 599–602. Elliot, A. J., & Niesta, D. (2008). Romantic red: Red enhances men’s attraction to women. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 95, 1150–1164. Elliot, A. J., & Pazda, A. D. (2012). Dressed for sex: Red as a female sexual signal in humans. PLoS ONE, 7(4), Article e34607. Retrieved from http://journals.plos.org/plosone/ article?id=10.1371/journal.pone.0034607 Elliot, A. J., Tracy, J. L., Pazda, A. D., & Beall, A. T. (2013). Red enhances women’s attractiveness to men: First evidence suggesting universality. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 49, 165–168. Ellison, P. T., Lager, C., & Calfee, J. (1987). Low profiles of sali- vary progesterone among college undergraduate women. Journal of Adolescent Health Care, 8, 204–207. Emery Thompson, M. (2009). The endocrinology of intersex- ual relationships in the apes. In P. T. Ellison & P. B. Gray (Eds.), Endocrinology of social relationships (pp. 196–222). Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press. Gelman, A. (2013, July 24). Too good to be true. Slate. Retrieved from http://www.slate.com Gildersleeve, K., Haselton, M. G., & Fales, M. R. (2014). Do women’s mate preferences change across the ovulatory cycle? A meta-analytic review. Psychological Bulletin, 140, 1205–1259. Grillot, R. L., Simmons, Z. L., Lukaszewski, A. W., & Roney, J. R. (2014). Hormonal and morphological predictors of wom- en’s body attractiveness. Evolution & Human Behavior, 35, 176–183. Herr, N. (2014). Mediation with dichotomous outcomes [Computer software]. Retrieved from http://www.nrhpsych .com/mediation/logmed.html Jones, B. C., Hahn, A. C., Fisher, C. I., Wincenciak, J., Kandrik, M., Roberts, S. C., . . . DeBruine, L. M. (2015). Facial coloration tracks changes in women’s estradiol. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 56, 29–34. Katchadourian, H. A. (1984). Fundamentals of human sexuality (4th ed.). New York, NY: Holt, Rinehart & Winston. Lipson, S. F., & Ellison, P. T. (1996). Comparison of salivary steroid profiles in naturally occurring conception and non- conception cycles. Human Reproduction, 11, 2090–2096. Niesta Kayser, D., Elliot, A. J., & Feltman, R. (2010). Red and romantic behavior in men viewing women. European Journal of Social Psychology, 40, 901–908. Pazda, A. D., Elliot, A. J., & Greitemeyer, T. (2012). Sexy red: Perceived sexual receptivity mediates the red-attraction relation in men viewing woman. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 48, 787–790. Pisanski, K., Hahn, A. C., Fisher, C. I., DeBruine, L. M., Feinberg, D. R., & Jones, B. C. (2014). Changes in salivary estradiol predict changes in women’s preferences for vocal mascu- linity. Hormones and Behavior, 66, 493–497. Prokop, P., & Hromada, M. (2013). Women use red in order to attract mates. Ethology, 119, 605–613. Roney, J. R., & Simmons, Z. L. (2008). Women’s estradiol predicts preference for facial cues of men’s testosterone. Hormones and Behavior, 53, 14–19. Roney, J. R., & Simmons, Z. L. (2013). Hormonal predictors of sexual motivation in natural menstrual cycles. Hormones and Behavior, 63, 636–645. Roney, J. R., Simmons, Z. L., & Gray, P. B. (2011). Changes in estradiol predict within-women shifts in attraction to facial cues of men’s testosterone. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 36, 742–749. Schwarz, S., & Singer, M. (2013). Romantic red revisited: Red enhances men’s attraction to young, but not menopausal women. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 49, 161–164. Selig, J. P., & Preacher, K. J. (2008). Monte Carlo method for assess- ing mediation: An interactive tool for creating confidence intervals for indirect effects [Computer software]. Available from http://www.quantpsy.org/medmc/medmc.htm Tracy, J. L., & Beall, A. T. (2013, July 30). Too good does not always mean not true [Web log post]. Retrieved from http:// ubc-emotionlab.ca/2013/07/too-good-does-not-always- mean-not-true/ Tracy, J. L., & Beall, A. T. (2014). The impact of weather on women’s tendency to wear red or pink when at high risk for conception. PLoS ONE, 9(2), Article e88852. Retrieved from http://journals.plos.org/plosone/article?id=10.1371/ journal.pone.0088852 at UNIVERSIDAD DEL DESARROLLO on July 15, 2015pss.sagepub.comDownloaded from
  • 7. Hormonal Predictors of Clothing Choices 7 Wang, H., Hahn, A. C., Fisher, C. I., DeBruine, L. M., & Jones, B. C. (2014). Women’s hormone levels modulate the moti- vational salience of facial attractiveness and sexual dimor- phism. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 50, 246–251. Wilcox, A. J., Dunson, D. B., Weinberg, C. R., Trussell, J., & Baird, D. D. (2001). Likelihood of conception with a single act of intercourse: Providing benchmark rates for assessment of post-coital contraceptives. Contraception, 63, 211–215. Wilcox, A. J., Weinberg, C. R., & Baird, D. D. (1995). Timing of sexual intercourse in relation to ovulation: Effects on the probability of conception, survival of the pregnancy, and sex of the baby. New England Journal of Medicine, 333, 1517–1521. at UNIVERSIDAD DEL DESARROLLO on July 15, 2015pss.sagepub.comDownloaded from