SlideShare a Scribd company logo
1 of 40
Running head: DETOXIFICATION FROM ALCOHOL AND
OPIATES 1
5
DETOXIFICATION FROM ALCOHOL AND OPIATES
Research Paper Outline: Detoxification from Alcohol and
Opiates
Grand Canyon University: PCN-527-0500
December 06, 2017
I. BRIEF HISTORY OF THE DRUG
A. History of Alcohol
B. History of Opiates
II. PSYCHOPHARMACOLOGY OF THE DRUG
A. Psychopharmacology of Alcohol
1. Effects of alcohol on mood
2. Effects of alcohol on thinking
3. Effects of alcohol on behavior
4. Effects of alcohol on sensation
B. Psychopharmacology of Opiates
1. Effects of alcohol on mood
2. Effects of alcohol on thinking
3. Effects of alcohol on behavior
4. Effects of alcohol on sensation
III. CURRENT TRENDS OF ISSUES
A. Current trends of issues of alcohol
1. Alcohol addiction
2. Binge drinking
3. Addiction
4. Physical symptom of withdrawal
5. Treatment
B. Current trends or issues of opiates
1. Types of opiates
2. Addiction
3. Physical symptoms of withdrawal
4. Treatment
IV. SOCIETAL CONCERNS OR ISSUES RELATED TO THE
TOPIC
A. Stigma of being an alcoholic
1. Physical symptoms
2. Disruption in functioning
3. Other’s perceptions of addiction
B. Stigma of being addicted to opiates
1. Physical Symptoms
2. Disruption in functioning
3. Other’s perception of addiction
V. SIGNIFICANCE OR RELEVANCE ON THE COUNSELING
PROFESSION
A. Relevance of alcohol addiction and treatment on the
counseling profession
1. Different option for treatment
2. Treatment after detoxification
B. Relevance of opiate addiction and treatment on the
counseling profession
1. Different option for treatment
2. Treatment after detoxification
VI. ANY FUTURE IMPLICATIONS
A. Future implications of alcohol detoxification
B. Future implications of opiate detoxification
References
Carlebach, S., Wake, D., & Hamilton, S. (2011). Experiences of
home detoxification for alcohol
dependency. Nursing Standard, 26(10), 41-47.
Green, L., & Gossop, M. (1988). Effects of information on the
opiate withdrawal syndrome. British Journal Of Addiction,
83(3), 305-309. doi:10.1111/j.1360-0443.1988.tb00472.x
Ison, J., Day, E., Fisher, K., Pratt, M., Hull, M., & Copello, A.
(2006). Self-detoxification from
opioid drugs. Journal Of Substance Use, 11(2), 81-88.
doi:10.1080/14659890500143697
Masson, C. L., Barnett, P. G., Sees, K. L., Delucchi, K. L.,
Rosen, A., Wong, W., & Hall, S. M. (2004). Cost and cost-
effectiveness of standard methadone maintenance treatment
compared to enriched 180-day methadone detoxification.
Addiction, 99(6), 718-726. doi:10.1111/j.1360-
0443.2004.00728.x
Perdomo Gutiérrez, R. E. (2011). Clinical case of rapid opiate
detoxification under anesthesia. Anestesia Pediatrica E
Neonatale, 9(1), 1-10.
Van den Berg, J. F., Van den Brink, W., Kist, N., Hermes, J. J.,
& Kok, R. M. (2015). Social
factors and readmission after inpatient detoxification in older
alcohol-dependent patients.
The American Journal On Addictions, (7), 661.
doi:10.1111/ajad.12287
Wulffson, R. M. (2014). Detoxification and substance abuse.
Salem Press Encyclopedia
Of Health.
Cognitive Development (Piaget) and Intelligence
The topic for this week is cognitive development and
intelligence from the perspective of Piaget and Vygotsky.
Additionally, we will learn definitions of intelligence, the
predictive value of intelligence tests, variations in IQ, the role
of early intervention in intellectual development, and the
development of creativity.
Topics to be covered include:
· Cognitive Development: Piagetian, Core Knowledge, and
Vygotskian Perspectives
· Role of Intelligence Testing in the Development of
Educational Programs
· Case Studies Related to Intelligence
Cognitive Development
We will begin to examine cognitive development, or how the
intellectual capabilities of infants transform into those of the
child, adolescent, and adult. First, let us define cognition.
Cognition refers to the inner processes and products of the mind
that lead to “knowing.” In other words, how do we acquire,
comprehend, and apply knowledge? What transformations must
occur for individuals to develop increasingly sophisticated
mental capacities?
JEAN PIAGET
You have likely heard the name of Swiss cognitive theorist,
Jean Piaget. According to Piaget, people are not cognitive
beings at birth; instead, they discover, or construct, all
knowledge of the world through their own experiences. As they
begin to construct knowledge, they refine and organize the
information in order to effectively adapt to their environments.
This theory of active construction of knowledge is known as the
constructivist approach to cognitive development. This
approach follows children through four invariant (fixed order)
and universal (assumed to characterize all children) stages:
sensorimotor, preoperational, concrete operational, and formal
operational. Throughout these stages, infants’ investigative
behaviors gradually transform into the abstract, rational
intelligence of more mature individuals.
PIAGET'S STAGES OF COGNITIVE DEVELOPMENT
SENSORIMOTOR
PREOPERATIONAL
CONCRETE OPERATIONAL
FORMAL OPERATIONAL
Piaget identified specific psychological structures called
schemes (organized ways of making sense of experiences) that
change with age. Initially, schemes are patterns of action
involving the senses and motor functions. For example, a baby
may simply grab and release an object. As the baby gets older,
this scheme becomes more deliberate, and she may begin to
throw the object down the stairs, up in the air, or against walls.
In other words, she is thinking before she acts. When there
evidence of this, Piaget says the child has moved from a
sensorimotor approach to a cognitive approach, which uses
mental representations (images and concepts) to develop
efficient thinking habits. This advancement in thinking relies on
adaptation and organization. Adaptation involves building
schemes through direct interaction with the environment, while
organization is an internal process that connects schemes to
create a powerfully interrelated cognitive system.
Piaget’s Impact
Although follow-up research has changed the way we look at
some aspects of Piaget’s theory, it is important to note the
major impact his theory had on educational practices. Teacher
training and learning procedures have reflected three
educational principles derived from his view: discovery learning
(providing exploration experiences to guide learning rather than
verbally offering facts), sensitivity to children’s readiness to
learn (introducing new activities when children are ready, not to
speed up development), and acceptance of individual
differences (using knowledge of each child’s specific rate of
development to plan small group activities).
CHALLENGES AND EXTENSIONS TO PIAGET’S THEORIES
At present, most experts agree that cognition is less stage-like
than Piaget suggested. They instead embrace the understanding
that children are continuously modifying their thinking and
obtaining new skills. Moreover, researchers typically disagree
on whether cognitive development is general or specific. These
challenges have led researchers to extend or modify Piaget’s
work. Those who believe differently than Piaget regarding the
limited cognitive capabilities of infants have proposed the core
knowledge perspective.
CORE KNOWLEDGE PERSPECTIVE
The core knowledge perspective is a second set of theories
related to cognitive development. Unlike Piaget, who believed
infants come into the world only with sensorimotor reflexes,
those who embrace this perspective believe that infants are
innately equipped with core domains of thought that support
rapid cognitive development. In other words, infants are
prewired to make sense of certain stimuli. Each core domain is
essential for survival and develops independently, resulting in
uneven, domain-specific changes.
PHYSICAL KNOWLEDGE AND NUMERICAL KNOWLEDGE
Two core domains have been studied at length in infancy:
physical knowledge and numerical knowledge. Physical
knowledge is the understanding of objects and their effects on
one another. Numerical knowledge is the capacity to keep track
of multiple objects and to add and subtract small quantities.
Observation of infants has shown understanding in these areas
occurring quite early, supporting the idea that some knowledge
must be innate. Children gradually build on that knowledge and
it becomes more intricate as through exploration, play, and
social interaction. They are viewed as naïve theorists, who
create explanations of events based on innate knowledge. Their
explanations, or theories, are tested with experience and revised
if needed. These revisions lead to increased reasoning about
cause and effect situations. While this is an intriguing idea
about how cognitive skills are able to emerge early and rapidly
develop, this theory has not offered clarity on how children
make the necessary revisions that prompt cognitive change.
Vygotsky’s Sociocultural Theory
The sociocultural theory is the third set of theories related to
cognitive development and is founded in the work of Lev
Vygotsky. Vygotsky believed that infants are born with
elementary perceptual, attention, and memory capacities, which
develop in the first two years through interaction with others.
Vygotsky did not view cognitive development as individualistic,
but placed a significant emphasis on culture or community.
Unlike Piaget, who favored independent discovery leading to
construction of knowledge, Vygotsky asserted that acquisition
of knowledge is a consequence of social interactions.
Specifically, learning takes place within the zone of proximal
development, which is a range of tasks too difficult for the child
to do alone but possible with the help of adults and more skilled
peers. When these more knowledgeable individuals question,
prompt, and suggest strategies for mastering a specific task
within the zone of proximal development, the child is drawn
into more mature thinking processes. Support during learning
can gradually be adjusted, based on the child’s needs, a concept
known as scaffolding. Also, due to his emphasis on social
experience and language, Vygotsky saw make-believe play as a
major zone of proximal development for preschoolers.
VYGOTSKY’S IMPACT
When applied in the classroom, Vygotsky’s theory teaches us to
highlight collaboration. While we again see that children should
be active participants in learning, we now go beyond individual
discovery (Piaget) to discovery through teacher guidance and
peer partnerships. In preschool, there should be many
opportunities for make-believe play. In all grades, there should
be opportunities for talk, as this dialogue prompts reflection on
thought processes, which, in turn leads to increased cognitive
awareness.
Knowledge Check
1
Question 1
Which theory on cognitive development places a large emphasis
on language and social interaction as the foundation for
learning?
Vygotsky’s Sociocultural Theory
Piaget’s Theory of Cognitive Development
Core Knowledge Perspective
I don't know
One attempt
Submit answer
You answered 0 out of 0 correctly. Asking up to 1.
Case Study
‹ 1/2 ›
· STUDY 1
To highlight how children’s drawings can help identify
adjustment difficulties at school.
As noted in the text, a variety of factors—the realization that
pictures can serve as symbols, improved planning and spatial
understanding, and the emphasis the child’s culture places on
artistic expression—influence the development of children’s
artful representations.
According to Harrison, Clarke, and Ungerer (2007), drawings
can also provide insight into relationships with adults, as well
as children’s overall adjustment. In one study, researchers
recruited 125 six-year-olds and collected the following
information:
· Participants completed a 30-minute interview that addressed
general perceptions of themselves, their school, and their
teachers.
· Participants completed the School Liking and Avoidance
Scale. Using a three-point scale (yes=3, no=2, sometimes=1),
children were asked such questions as, “Is school fun?” “Do you
enjoy school?” “Do you ask your parents to let you stay home
from school?” “Does school make you feel like crying?” To
assess participants’ feelings about their teacher, they were
asked: “Do you like to see your teacher when you get to
school?” “Is your teacher nice to you?” “Does your teacher
smile at you?” “Does your teacher play or read with you?”
· Participants were asked to draw a picture of themselves and
their teacher at school. Drawing materials were provided but no
further instructions were given. Researchers recorded any
spontaneous comments, and once participants completed their
drawings, they were asked to identify the people and objects in
the picture. Pictures were scored on the following dimensions,
which are commonly used to assess child/family drawings:
· Creativity—going beyond the instructions and adding lively,
colorful, or imaginative features.
· Pride/happiness—showing an emotional connectedness to the
teacher, such as holding hands or doing something fun with the
teacher.
· Vulnerability—using overwhelming, exaggerated, distorted, or
displaced body images.
· Emotional distance/isolation—using expressions of anger,
negative affect, physical distance from the teacher, or physical
barriers between the child and teacher.
· Tension/anger—showing rigid and constricted features or
scribbling out the teacher’s face.
· Role reversal—representing the child as larger, more
powerful, or more potent than the teacher.
· Bizarreness/dissociation—including unusual signs or symbols,
angry facial features, or morbid fantasy themes.
· Teachers rated their relationship quality with each child on
five dimensions: conflict/anger, warmth/positive emotions, open
communication, dependency, and troubled feelings.
· Teachers rated participants’ overall school adjustment by
identifying the prevalence of problem behaviors (for example,
acting out, aggression, learning problems) and
strengths/competencies (for example, leadership, frustration
tolerance, social skills).
Results
Findings revealed significant relationships among children’s
reports, children’s drawings, teacher-rated relationship quality,
and teacher-rated school adjustment. That is, children who
reported negative feelings about their teacher also included
negative themes in their drawings, such as scribbling out the
teacher’s face. Their teachers, in turn, were likely to report
strained teacher–child relationships and problematic classroom
behavior. Taken together, these findings suggest that children’s
artistic representations of relationships with teachers are an
important tool for identifying adjustment difficulties at school.
Definitions of Intelligence
How do you view intelligence? When you consider a highly
intelligent person, what behaviors stand out to you? It is
probably not surprising to learn that experts disagree on the
attributes that make up intelligence and that the definition of
intelligence has evolved over time. You may recall that the first
intelligence test was created by Alfred Binet and his colleague,
Theodore Simon, in 1905 in response to educators’ needs to
identify students who were unlikely to benefit from standard
classroom instruction. Binet was asked to develop an impartial
means of assigning students to classes, one which focused on
mental aptitude and not disruptive behavior. Factors such as
memory and reasoning skills were considered and compared to
children of the same ages.
PSYCHOMETRIC APPROACH
FACTOR ANALYSIS
OTHER APPROACHES
Louis Thurstone, in contrast, viewed intelligence as less of a
single, general ability and more of a set of distinct abilities. His
factor analysis on more than 50 intelligence tests suggested that
independent, individual factors of intelligence exist. He called
these factors primary mental abilities, and categorized these
abilities as follows: verbal comprehension, reasoning,
perceptual speed, numerical ability, word fluency, associative
memory, and spatial visualization.
Eventually, both Spearman and Thurstone recognized each
other’s perspectives, and current theorists combine both
approaches when designing tests. Subtest scores are used to
determine an individual’s specific strengths and weaknesses
(Thurstone’s original theory), but can also be combined into an
overall general intelligence score (Spearman’s original theory).
However, many researchers believe that merely combining these
two perspectives is not enough, as factors on intelligence tests
have only partial value if we cannot recognize the cognitive
processes accountable for those factors. If we are able to do
that, we will better understand what skills must be reinforced to
improve a particular child’s performance. To facilitate this,
psychometric and information-processing approaches were
combined and componential analyses conducted, looking for
relationships involving components of information processing
and children’s intelligence test performance. While this helps to
detect cognitive skills that contribute to intelligence, this
approach does not include cultural and situational factors that
may affect intelligence.
STERNBERG
GARDNER
Measuring Intelligence
Although we have discussed some limitations to intelligence
testing, they are often helpful in identifying highly gifted
children and diagnosing learning problems. Standardized
intelligence tests utilize the scores of a large, representative
sample of individuals as a standard for interpreting individual
scores. Scores produce an intelligence quotient, or IQ, which
indicates the extent to which the number of items passed (raw
score) deviates from the average performance of individuals of
the same age. Group-administered standardized tests are useful
for instructional planning, while individually administered tests
consider both the child’s answers and behaviors, which paints a
more accurate picture of the child’s abilities. Two of these
individual tests – the Stanford-Binet Intelligence Scales, Fifth
Edition, and the Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children–IV
(WISC–IV) – are used most often.
APTITUDE AND ACHIEVEMENT TESTS
Aptitude and achievement tests are similar to intelligence tests.
Aptitude tests assess a child’s propensity for excelling in a
specific type of activity, such as music or language. On the
other hand, achievement tests measure the attainment of
particular knowledge and skills, such as reading comprehension
or concepts covered within a particular class. Achievement test
are often given by teachers to assess their students’
understanding of a particular subject area. There are even tests
to measure the intelligence of infants, although these present
unique challenges, as babies are unable to answer questions or
follow directions. Tests performed with infants typically
measure perceptual and motor responses; however, more recent
tests also attempt to determine early language, cognition, and
social behavior.
Ethnic and Socioeconomic Impact on IQ scores
We know that many factors affect child development and that
both heredity and environment shape a child’s unique qualities.
What are the roles of nature versus nurture in regards to
determining IQ? You will not be surprised to learn that there
has been much debate regarding this question, as researchers are
concerned with locating the cause of IQ disparities between and
among certain ethnicities. Either heredity or environmental
factors must be responsible for the variations in test
performance.
HEREDITY
Heritability estimates, or measures which researchers use to
correlate the IQs of family members who share genes, reveal
that heredity does play a role in IQ test performance. Adoption
studies have also supported these findings, as children in the
study eventually became more similar in IQ to their biological
mothers and less similar to their adoptive parents. However,
within the same studies, it was noted that the IQ of children
were adopted into privileged homes benefited from a rise in IQ
in comparison with non-adopted children who remained in
disadvantaged homes.
ENVIRONMENTAL FACTORS
This reminds us that environmental factors also contribute to
individual differences in intelligence, as do family beliefs about
intellectual success and expectations for children’s educational
attainment. Other factors, such as communication styles, lack of
familiarity with test content, reactions to testing conditions, and
fear of being judged on the basis of negative stereotypes can
lead test scores to underestimate minority children’s
intelligence. An approach to testing, known as dynamic
assessment, seems to discover what a child can attain with
social support, thus reducing bias in testing.
USE OF TESTING‹ 1/2 ›
· Intervention Programs for Lower Achieving Students
Because research shows a correlation between socioeconomic
status and performance in intelligence tests, intervention
programs for economically disadvantaged students have been
implemented. The majority of these programs begin as early as
preschool, with the hopes of offsetting learning challenges
before formal schooling begins. You may have heard of Project
Head Start, which is one of the largest federal programs,
offering academic support, as well as nutritional and health
service and promoting parental involvement. Research on early
intervention programs indicates that, although instantaneous
gains in IQ tend to rapidly dissipate, enduring benefits are
noted in school adjustment, attitudes, and motivation. A
program in which high-quality intervention starts early, is
intensive, emphasizes parent involvement, and focuses on the
whole child is likely to have a more far-reaching impact in all
areas, including IQ.
Knowledge Check
1
Question 1
Which type of standardized test provides us with knowledge
about a child’s potential proficiency with a particular talent?
Aptitude tests
Achievement tests
Intelligence tests
I don't know
One attempt
Submit answer
You answered 0 out of 0 correctly. Asking up to 1.
Case Study
Research studies provide examples various types of intelligence
testing and the use of testing results. The first case study
examines the relationship of emotional intelligence and
successful intelligence to leadership skills in gifted students.
The second case study inspects the relationship among low birth
weight, social disadvantage, and children’s IQ scores at ages
six, 11, and 17.
‹ 1/2 ›
· STUDY 1
Emotional Intelligence, Successful Intelligence, and Leadership
Skills
Emotional intelligence is positively associated with self-esteem,
empathy, prosocial behavior, cooperation, leadership skills, and
life satisfaction. In addition, Sternberg’s theory underscores the
importance of analytical, creative, and practical skills—skills
that are often overlooked on intelligence tests—for life success.
To examine the relationship among emotional intelligence,
successful intelligence, and leadership skills, Chan (2007)
recruited 498 gifted students in grades 4 through 12 and
collected the following information:
(1) Participants completed a leadership rating scale, which
measures leadership self-efficacy, leadership flexibility, and
goal orientation (related to leadership and achievement). (2)
Participants completed an emotional intelligence scale that
assesses social skills, empathy, management of emotions, and
utilization of emotions. (3) Participants completed Sternberg’s
Successful Intelligence Questionnaire, which measures
analytical, creative, and practical abilities.
Results
Results indicated that both emotional intelligence and
successful intelligence predicted leadership skills. That is,
participants who scored high in emotional and successful
intelligence scored higher in overall leadership skills than
participants who scored low in these areas. When looking at
specific aspects of emotional and successful intelligence,
practical abilities and management of emotions were especially
strong predictors of leadership skills. These findings suggest
that the abilities to apply intellectual skills in everyday
situations and to manage and regulate emotions are important
leadership qualities. No significant age or gender differences
were found.
Overview
During the first two decades of life, the human body
continuously and dramatically changes, a process regulated and
controlled by a number of biological and environmental factors.
Heredity, nutrition, infectious disease, and parental affection all
contribute to physical growth and overall health. Stimulation of
the brain is vital during periods in which it is growing most
rapidly in order to enhance cognitive development. Both Piaget
and Vygotsky have created theories centered around cognitive
development that have impacted our view of the way children
acquire, comprehend, and apply knowledge. As children grow
older, puberty causes their bodies to drastically change, which
is also accompanied by psychological changes, as teenagers
strive to adjust to what is happening in their bodies.
Information-processing research seeks to understand how
children develop the attention, memory, and self-management
skills to succeed with complex tasks. Those who study this
approach compare the human mind to a computer, or an
intricate, symbol-manipulating system through which
information flows. Attention to task is essential to thinking
because it helps an individual determine which information
needs to be considered. Development of attentional strategies
occurs in phases and, over time, children gain an increased
capacity for planning. As the ability to sustain attention grows,
memory also improves, and the implementation of memory
strategies increases a child’s likelihood of transferring
information from the working memory to the long-term memory.
Children also develop metacognition, which is another form of
knowledge that influences how well children remember and
solve problems. Fundamental discoveries about information
processing have been applied to children’s mastery of academic
skills, particularly in the areas of reading and mathematics.
Identifying differences in cognitive skills between weak and
strong learners can lead to strategies and interventions to
increase performance. In addition, intelligence tests are helpful
in identifying highly gifted children and diagnosing learning
problems. The use of various types of intelligence testing has
led to specific educational programs for diverse groups of
students.
Key Terms
ADAPTATION
COGNITION
COGNITIVE-DEVELOPMENTAL THEORY
CONSTRUCTIVIST APPROACH
CORE KNOWLEDGE PERSPECTIVE
DEFERRED IMITATION
EGOCENTRISM
MENTAL REPRESENTATION
INTENTIONAL (GOAL DIRECTED) BEHAVIOR
OPERATIONS
ORGANIZATION
SCAFFOLDING
SCHEMES
ZONE OF PROXIMAL DEVELOPMENT
ACHIEVEMENT TEST
APTITUDE TEST
COMPONENTIAL ANALYSIS
CREATIVITY
DYNAMIC ASSESSMENT
FACTOR ANALYSIS
GIFTED
INTELLIGENCE QUOTIENT (IQ)
THEORY OF MULTIPLE INTELLIGENCES
Sources
Brainerd, C.J., Forrest, T.J., Karibian, D., & Reyna, V. F.
(2006). Development of the false-memory illusion.
Developmental Psychology, 42, 962–979.
Breslau, N., Dickens, W. T., Flynn, J. R., Peterson, E. L., &
Lucia, V.C. (2006). Low birth weight and social disadvantage:
Tracking their relationship with children’s IQ during the period
of school attendance. Intelligence, 34, 351–362.
Chan, D. W. (2007). Leadership and intelligence. Roeper
Review, 29, 183–189.
Friedman, N. P., Haberstick, B. C., Willcutt, E. G., Miyake, A.,
Young, S. E., Corley, R. P., & Hewitt, J. K. (2007). Greater
attention problems during childhood predict poorer executive
functioning in late adolescence. Psychological Science, 18,
893–900.
All graphics are public domain images or stock images from
123RF.
Play
from Cambridge Encyclopedia of Child Development
Introduction
This entry reviews the main different types of play, and the
kinds of developmental benefits they may bring to children. The
ubiquity of play in childhood (and in most species of mammals
when young) strongly suggests its benefits for development, but
what these benefits are, and how important or essential they are,
are still debated. Classic perspectives on the development and
function of play can be found in the writings of Piaget and
Vygotsky. Let us begin by tackling the issue of what constitutes
play, and then turn to how it undergoes age-related changes.
Defining play
Play is often defined as activity that is both done for its own
sake, and characterized by ‘ means rather than ends’ (i.e., the
process of the play is more important than any end point or
goal). These criteria contrast play with, for example,
exploration (which may lead into play as a child gets more
familiar with a new toy or environment), with work (which has
a definite goal), and fighting (different from play fighting as
discussed later). Additional characteristics of play are
flexibility (objects being put in new combinations, roles acted
out in new ways), positive affect (children often smile and
laugh in play, and say they enjoy it), and pretence (use of
objects and actions in non-literal ways).
Main types of play
Although classifications differ, the following main types of play
are well recognized: object play, pretend play and sociodramatic
play, and physical activity play (exercise play; rough-and-
tumble play). Of these, object play and physical activity play
are seen widely in other species of mammals. Pretend and
sociodramatic play are only seen in humans, apart from some
possibly very elementary forms of pretence in great apes.
Besides play, there is the related concept of games. Games with
rules are more organized forms of play in which there is some
goal (e.g., winning the game) and are not reviewed further.
Object play
This starts in infancy and may help children develop creative
problem-solving skills. Researchers such as Jerome Bruner and
Kathy Sylva have reported experiments with children in which
they are given a chance to play with objects, then solve a task.
Those with the play experience solved the task better. However,
subsequent research has suggested that instruction can often be
equally effective (Johnson, Christie, & Yawkey, 1999). The
benefits of play need to be balanced against those of instruction,
bearing in mind the ages of the children, the nature of the task,
and the specificity of the learning expected - whether for
specific skills or a more generally inquisitive and creative
attitude.
Pretend play
This develops from about 15 months, with simple actions such
as ‘ pretending to sleep’ or ‘ putting dolly to bed,’ developing
into longer story sequences and role play (Fig. 1). Much early
pretend play can be with parents, and older siblings. In Western
societies especially, it is common for parents to model or ‘
scaffold’ early pretend play actions. By 3 to 4 years, pretend
play becomes common with same-age peers.
Pretend play among children is seen very widely in different
societies. It is often imitative of adult roles (e.g., in rural
societies, children may play at ‘ herding cattle’ with stones and
at ‘ pounding maize’ with sticks and pebbles). Such play might
be considered as ‘ practice’ for the adult activities concerned.
However, rather more ambitious developmental benefits for
pretend play have been put forward.
Leslie (1987) argued that pretend play is an early indicator of
theory of mind abilities. In simple object substitution pretence,
the knowledge or representation that ‘ this is a banana’ becomes
‘ this banana is a telephone.’ Correspondingly, in theory of
mind, the representation that ‘ this is a banana’ is related to the
representation that ‘ X believes that the banana is a telephone.’
Leslie argued that this similarity suggested that pretence might
be very important in theory of mind acquisition. However, this
early pretend play before 3 years is often very imitative, and it
is not clear whether a young child who talks into a banana is
actually having the cognitive representations that Leslie
describes, or is simply imitating what older children or adults
do. The nature of any relationship between pretend play and
theory of mind is still disputed.
Figure 1. Pretend play. Photograph by John Walmsley.
Sociodramatic play
Defined in terms of social play with others, sustained role
taking, and a narrative line, this is something that children from
about 3 years of age engage in a lot. Such play can be quite
complex, involving an understanding of others’ intent and role,
sophisticated language constructions, and the development of
sometimes novel (sometimes less novel!) story lines. Smilansky
(1968) suggested that sociodramatic play assists language
development, cognitive development, creativity, and role
taking. She also claimed that pretend and sociodramatic play
were less frequent and less complex in disadvantaged children.
This led her and others to develop play tutoring (intervention by
an adult) to raise levels of these kinds of play; adults would
provide suitable props, visits, etc. and encourage the
sociodramatic play of children in nurseries and kindergartens,
such that subsequently they became more able to sustain this
play themselves.
Smilansky's ideas about the value of sociodramatic play were
tested by a number of experimental studies, including play-
tutoring studies. In these, a group or class of children that
received play tutoring were compared with those who did not.
Generally, the play-tutored children improved more on measures
of cognition, language, and creativity, apparently supporting
Smilansky's views.
A number of critiques were made of these studies. Many of
them pointed to flaws due to selective interpretation of results,
effects of experimental bias, and the use of inappropriate
control groups. For example, in the traditional play-tutoring
study, the play-tutored children received more stimulation and
adult contact generally, so one cannot really conclude that it is
the extra play that brought about the developmental benefits.
Further studies took account of these criticisms. This step
included balancing play-tutoring with skills tutoring (e.g.,
coloring, picture dominoes) and assessing outcomes blind to the
child's treatment condition. Doing so failed to reveal many
differences (P. K. Smith, 1988), which suggests that benefits of
socio drama tic play need not be essential for development.
Nevertheless, play-tutoring does work out as equal to skills
tutoring in many domains, and it is generally enjoyable and
sociable for children in the preschool years, so there are sound
reasons to encourage it in the nursery curriculum.
Table 1. Some criteria distiguishing play fighting and real
fighting.
Criterion
Play fighting
Real fighting
Facial expression
Smiling, laughing
Frowning, tearful
Restraint
Kicks and blows are not hard or do not make contact
Kicks and blows are hard or make contact
Role-reversal
Voluntarily take it in turns to be ‘ on top’ or be ‘ chased'
Aims to be ‘ on top’ or to chase the other
How encounter starts and finishes
Starting by invitation and ending with continued play or activity
together
Starting with challenge and ending in separation
One kind of pretend play, often not encouraged in nurseries, is
war play, which is pretend play with toy guns or weapons, or
military action figures. Many educators believe that this play
encourages real aggression, though others emphasize its pretend
nature and feel that no real harm results from it.
Physical activity play
This refers to playful activity involving large body activity,
particularly exercise play that includes running, climbing, and
other large body or large muscle activity, as well as rough-and-
tumble play, that covers play fighting and play chasing. These
forms of play have been reviewed by Pellegrini & Smith (1998).
Exercise play
This increases in frequency from toddlers to preschool children,
peaks at early primary school ages, and then declines. Young
children seem to need opportunities for physical exercise more
than older children, and are more likely to get restless after long
sedentary periods and to run around when released from them.
Boys do more of this kind of play than girls. It is often
hypothesized to support physical training of muscles, for
strength and endurance, and skill and economy of movement.
Another hypothesis is that exercise play encourages younger
children to take breaks from being overloaded on cognitive
tasks (the cognitive immaturity hypothesis). The argument here
is that younger children have less mature cognitive capacities,
so benefits of concentrating on a cognitively demanding task
decrease after a shorter time than for older children. The ‘ need’
to exercise thus helps children ‘ space out’ these cognitive
demands.
Figure 2. Rough-and-tumble play. Photograph by John
Walmsley.
Rough-and-tumble play
This seems to increase from toddlers through preschool and
primary school children, to peak at late primary age, and then
decline in frequency. It takes up some 10 percent of playground
time, though varying by the nature of the surface, physical
conditions, etc. Boys do more than girls, especially play
fighting. Rough-and-tumble play looks like real fighting, but
can be distinguished from it by several criteria (Table 1; Fig.
2).
Most children can distinguish playful from real fighting, and
from 8 years give similar cues to those described in Table 1. In
one study, English and Italian children were found to be
accurate in judging videotapes of play fighting and real
fighting, irrespective of which nationality they were watching.
During the primary school years, only about 1% of rough-and-
tumble episodes usually turn into real fighting, although many
teachers and lunchtime supervisors think it is as much as about
30%. However, ‘ rejected’ children (those disliked by many
peers and seldom liked much) more often respond to rough-and-
tumble aggressively (around 25% of episodes). So, it is possible
that teachers or lunchtime supervisors are making general
judgments about children, based on these ‘ rejected’ children
who may be taking up a lot of their supervisory time.
Rough-and-tumble is often between friends. By early
adolescence, however, there appears to be some change, with
dominance/status becoming important in choosing play partners,
as well as friendship, with a greater risk of play fights turning
into real fights. It is hypothesized that rough-and-tumble play in
younger children may (in addition to benefits of exercise play)
provide practice in fighting/hunting skills, at least in earlier
human societies. By adolescence, however, it may involve
dominance relationships (e.g., using rough-and-tumble play to
establish or maintain dominance in the peer group).
What do children learn from play?
Evolutionary arguments suggest that the propensity to play has
been selected for, so we can expect there to be benefits to
playing, and that these may vary by species, and by types of
play. There can be a lot of incidental benefits to play such that
it keeps children active and provides them with opportunities to
encounter new situations. With human children, and with object,
pretend, and sociodramatic play, there may be a balance to draw
between benefits of playing and of instruction. Instruction can
be more focused on a precise goal, but play is often more
enjoyable for young children and, even if less efficient for a
precise goal, may foster a more generally inquisitive and
creative approach to problem-solving.
Conclusions
Among the theoretical issues in play research remaining
unresolved, two are currently especially noteworthy. The first
relates to rough-and-tumble play. We know that this is primarily
friendly and non-exploitative in preadolescents, but how does
this change as children move into adolescence? Does the
function of this form of play then change and, in particular, is it
used for purposes related to dominance, especially for boys?
The second issue relates to pretend play. An earlier phase of
research queried the findings from play tutoring studies, but,
more recently, pretend play has been proposed as an important
component of developing a theory of mind. Greater conceptual
clarity and empirical evidence are called for here, together with
a willingness to learn from the problems encountered in the
earlier studies (e.g., experimenter bias).
Amongst practical issues, the issue of war play continues to be
debated in early education. There have been moves to ban war
play in many nursery schools; however, there is also a
recognition that such play may be generally harmless in itself
and a rather natural play format, especially for boys (Holland,
2003). Regarding educational practice through the school years,
there has been a general movement toward shortening or
eliminating playground breaks. However, leaving aside social
benefits of playtime, the benefits for physical activity and for
providing breaks between instruction (cf., the cognitive
immaturity hypothesis), argue for retaining playground breaks.
More systematic study is still needed in these areas.
Questions
1. What value does physical activity play have in providing
spacing for concentration on school-based tasks?
2. How does rough-and-tumble play change in form and
function from the primary to secondary school ages?
3. Does young children's war play have any connection with
later aggressiveness?
4. Does pretend play have any important role in helping young
children develop a theory of mind?
See also
· Cross-cultural comparisons
· Cross-species comparisons
· Theories of the child's mind
· Social development
· Aggressive and prosocial behavior
· Peers and siblings
· Sex differences
· Anthropology
· Jeromes s. Bruner
· Jean Piaget
· Lev S. Vygotsky
· Donald Winnicott.
Further reading
· Lancy, D. F. 1996 Playingon the Mother-Ground: Cultural
Routines for Children's Development
New York: Guilford Press
.
· Pellegrini, A. D. 2002 Rough-and-tumble play. P.K. Smith,
C. Hart, Handbook of Social Development
Oxford: Blackwell,
pp. 438-453 .
· Power, T. 2000 Play and Exploration in Children and Animals
NJ: Erlbaum
.
PETER K. SMITH
© Cambridge University Press 2005
· APA
· Chicago
· Harvard
· MLA
Smith, P. K., & SMITH, P. K. (2005). Play. In B. Hopkins
(Ed.), Cambridge encyclopedia of child development.
Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press. Retrieved from
http://ezproxy.apus.edu/login?url=http://search.credoreference.c
om/content/entry/cupchilddev/play/0?institutionId=8703
Top of Form
Search
Bottom of Form
Browse Book
Previous ArticleNext Article
Cambridge Encyclopedia of Child Development
An authoritative, accessible and up-to-date account of all
aspects of child development. Covers everything from prenatal
development to education, pediatrics, neuroscience, theories and
research methods to physical development, social development,
cognitive development, psychopathology and parenting. It also
looks at cultural issues, sex differences and the history of child
development.
Editor(s): Brian Hopkins
Edition: 1st
Articles: 113
Images: 277
People: 21
Search other sites
· Academic Search Premier
· Business Source Complete
· Ebrary
· EBSCOhost
· Gale Virtual Reference Library
· IEEE
· JSTOR
· Lexis Nexis Academic
· Proquest Databases
· Safari
· Summon
Terms of usePrivacy policyContactAbout Credo
ZONE OF PROXIMAL DEVELOPMENT and: CULTURAL
TOOLS SCAFFOLDING GUIDED PARTICIPATION
in SOCIAL COGNITION
from Key Concepts in Developmental Psychology
MEANING
Although Vygotsky devoted only limited space to the zone of
proximal (i.e. next) development in his description of socio-
cognitive theory, this concept has become the best-known part
of his account. The ZPD (as it is always referred to) expresses
Vygotsky's belief that children's cognitive development occurs
essentially as a result of interacting with more knowledgeable
and competent others, who are willing to provide guidance and
support in problem solving situations and will sensitively adjust
their help in such a way that the child is challenged to
participate in activities just beyond his or her current level of
understanding. The ZPD is thus –
the region between what children already know and what they
are capable of learning under guidance.
It is in this region that children are most receptive to new
learning; it is there that any new intellectual skill is first of all
performed jointly with a competent adult before it is in due
course taken over by the child and internalized. It is the region
where children are not quite capable of managing on their own
but where the adult can stretch their abilities by suitably pacing
demands so that the child can gradually assume responsibility
for performing the task in a solo capacity.
The ZPD is an expression of the basic proposition that Vygotsky
set out to convey, namely that cognitive development is not a
process that occurs spontaneously nor can it merely be
explained by the child's interaction with the physical
environment. It occurs because children are embedded in a
social context, surrounded by people of greater expertise willing
to share their knowledge with the child. Cognitive development
can thus be seen as a progression
from intermental to intramental, from joint regulation to self-
regulation. The adult acts as tutor, the child as apprentice, but
the interaction of the two is of a dynamic, mutually adjustive
nature, for the child is no mere passive participant but an active
partner in the learning process, albeit a junior one. Cognition is
socially created, and the ZPD is a means of bringing this about.
According to Vygotsky, the ZPD has one further use, namely in
the assessment of intelligence. Vygotsky was convinced that
children's potential is best demonstrated when working with a
more competent person than when working on their own. Such
an assertion goes, of course, directly counter to the generally
accepted view, as seen in psychometric and other assessment
procedures, that children's true capacities can only be revealed
by tests administered to them in isolation. However, Vygotsky
argued that children's ability to profit from help can tell us
more about their eventual capacities than their efforts at
unsupported problem solving. As he put it, it is in the ZPD that
the 'buds of development' are to be found rather than the 'fruits',
and it is the former that he considered to be of greater
diagnostic value with respect to an individual's future progress.
ORIGINS
Although Vygotsky became aware of Piaget's writings, he did
not formulate his ideas in direct opposition to them – as an
effort, that is, to correct the latter's view of children as lone
learners. Rather, Vygotsky (1896–1934) was very much a child
of his time and place – a Russian steeped in Marxian theory who
saw human nature as a socio-cultural product and childhood as
the time when the accumulated wisdom of previous generations
is handed on to the new generation.
The ZPD was conceived by him as the primary setting in which
such handing on is accomplished; the significance of these
encounters, therefore, extends beyond the cognitive benefits
derived by individual children: they also play an essential role
in bringing the child in contact with the culture, the continuity
of which is thereby ensured. To explain this process Vygotsky
used the concept of CULTURAL TOOLS, these being –
the psychological and technological devices perfected in the
course of each society's history for the purpose of supporting
and extending our understanding of the world.
Psychological tools include language, writing, counting systems
and scientific theories; among technological tools are books,
clocks, calendars, calculators and computers. All these serve to
channel our thinking in particular culturally valued directions:
clocks and calendars, for example, ensure that we organize our
ideas in a time dimension, the importance of which we set out to
convey to children from an early age.
However, by far the most important cultural tool is language,
for it is the preeminent means of passing on society's
experience. Children first encounter language as an integral part
of their social exchanges with their caretakers, who sensitively
(though usually quite unconsciously) adapt both the complexity
and the content of their speech to the child's ability to
comprehend, thus facilitating the eventual acquisition of
language in children themselves (see motherese). Language thus
begins in a social setting and, according to Vygotsky, is also
initially used solely for social purposes, that is, to influence the
behaviour of others. Eventually its function becomes extended:
private speech (or what Piaget called egocentric speech)
appears, whereby children talk to themselves in order to
regulate their own behaviour, to be replaced subsequently by
internal speech which forms the beginning of thought. Thus,
what was in the first place behaviour that took place between
child and others is transformed in the course of development to
an internalized activity: the intermental becomes the
intramental; an essentially social function evolves into the
principal tool for cognitive functioning.
Vygotsky did not produce any fully fledged theory, nor did he
leave behind a coherent body of research, and it was not until
many years after his early death that his two major
books, Thought and language (1962) and Mind in
society (1978), were translated into English and began to attract
international attention. Even in his own country his work was
neglected when his writings were suppressed during the
Stalinist purges – an ironic fate to overcome a convinced
Marxist who firmly believed that human behaviour is moulded
by social organization and that the historical forces shaping our
society need to be taken into account if we are to understand
how children's development takes place. Indeed his aim was not
merely to throw light on the nature of individual development;
he was also convinced that by understanding the processes
responsible for children becoming adults one can help to create
a better socialist society. His vision was thus much wider than
that of any other child psychologist: it extended beyond
psychology to history, sociology, politics, economics, education
and linguistics, all of which he regarded as relevant to the study
of individual development. No wonder his theory is variously
characterized as socio-cultural, socio-historical and socio-
cognitive.
CURRENT STATUS
In the past few decades Vygotsky's writings have attracted
enormous attention, and most of all serious consideration has
been given to his belief that we need to think of the child-in-
context as the basic unit rather than the child-in-isolation
(see context). He is also responsible for stimulating a
considerable amount of research on topics such as the analysis
of parent–child joint problem solving (e.g. Wood, Bruner &
Ross, 1976), peer tutoring (e.g. Foot & Howe, 1998), the role of
private (egocentric) speech (e.g. Bivens & Berk, 1990) and
cross-cultural comparisons of socialization and training
practices (e.g. Rogoff, Mistry, Goncu & Mosier, 1993). The
notion of the ZPD in particular has been the starting point for a
lot of further work.
However, Vygotsky's conception of the ZPD has also been
seriously criticized. Much of this has been directed at the
following features:
· Vagueness regarding processes. The ZPD has been condemned
for using a unidimensional concept to represent a multifaceted
phenomenon (Paris & Cross, 1988). It does not specify the many
and diverse processes that socially supported learning involves,
nor does it provide a guide to the variations in zones that one
can expect across different domains, settings and teachers.
· The failure to consider developmental aspects. Vygotsky
conceived of a prototype child, who functions in the ZPD in the
same way at the age of 2 as at the age of 12. The roles of adult
and child remain fixed; the appearance of new motives, needs
and abilities are neglected, as are children's changing
definitions of what constitutes their social context.
· The disregard of children's individuality. Although Vygotsky
stressed the active part played by the child in the ZPD, he paid
more attention to the adult's contribution. At any one age
children differ in what they bring to a social interchange, but
Vygotsky had nothing to say about individual differences in
such aspects as learning style, motivation and emotional
regulation, nor about the various relationship qualities that
distinguish different adult–child dyads.
· Vagueness concerning the precise means whereby learning is
produced. Just what are the processes taking place between
child and partner that result in children taking over
responsibility for more and more parts of the task and
inernalizing them as skills of their own? Vygotsky did not go
into detail; it has been left to others to pursue this line of
enquiry.
One attempt to provide an answer to this last question is built
around the concept of SCAFFOLDING, first put forward
by Wood, Bruner and Ross (1976). Scaffolding is –
the process whereby a more expert partner offers help to a child
in problem solving by adjusting both the amount and kind of
help to the child's level of performance.
In order to determine precisely what adults actually do in a joint
task in order to help children become independent problem
solvers, Wood and his colleagues observed the teaching
techniques adopted by mothers when their 3- to 4-year-old
children were confronted by a construction task that they could
not initially cope with on their own. They observed a great
range of actions employed by the mothers to keep the children
on task and simplify the problem to an appropriate degree, but
their use appeared to follow two rules: first, when a child is
struggling the tutor should immediately offer more help;
second, when the child is succeeding the tutor should give less
help and fade into the background. By offering support that is
always contingent on what the child is achieving, the child is
given considerable autonomy and yet also has the opportunity at
every step of relying on assistance, this diminishing as the child
increasingly takes over responsibility for completing the task. A
scaffold is thus put in place, but used in a far more flexible
manner than this metaphor might suggest, in that the two
contingency rules mean that the adult's behaviour is constantly
modified in the light of what the child is achieving, thus leading
the child step by step to eventual success.
Such a notion of scaffolding certainly expresses the essence of
what Vygotsky had in mind for the ZPD, and has been used in a
large number of studies investigating a range of problem
solving tasks. Yet this concept too is not without its critics (e.g.
Stone, 1993), mainly because it pays insufficient attention to
the communicative processes taking place in the adult–child
interaction; also because it does not allow for the fact that the
effectiveness of adults' actions is dependent on the particular
relationship they have with the child. Another approach, that
based on the concept of GUIDED PARTICIPATION, has
attempted to correct these deficiencies.
Guided participation is a term proposed by Barbara Rogoff
(1990, 2003) as one of the mechanisms employed to advance a
socio-cultural view of human development. Children, that is,
become immersed in the practices of their particular culture
from the moment of birth onwards; everything they experience
transmits to them the accumulated beliefs and values of their
society, and the main setting in which this occurs is in the
child's exchanges with its caretakers, teachers and other
concerned adults. Thus, as Rogoff (1998) put it, 'Cognitive
development occurs as new generations collaborate with older
generations in varying forms of interpersonal engagement and
institutional practices.' Guided participation may therefore be
defined as –
the process by which children develop through their
involvement in the practices of their community.
By its use Rogoff wanted to emphasize that adult and child play
essentially complementary roles in the latter's development,
namely guidance by the adult and participation by the child. On
the one hand the adult acts as guide to the culturally valued
practices the child is expected to adopt – a role that can be
played in many ways, some didactic as in structured teaching
situations (which Vygotsky mainly concentrated on), others
informal as when the child is given the opportunity to observe
and join an adult in performing some activity. On the other hand
the child acts as apprentice – not merely as passive bystander,
that is, but as an active contributor to the activity that is the
joint focus of the partner's attention. Thus guided participation
is not some particular method of support for learning, nor does
it depend on some conscious, previously formulated goal. A lot
of joint teaching-learning is of an informal nature, and Rogoff
uses a wealth of observations from different cultures to
illustrate its role in enabling children to participate in the
activities of their society and to advance from their present to a
more advanced level of understanding.
The general theme conveyed by Vygotsky, Rogoff and other
socio-cultural theorists, as signalled by the concepts they
employ such as the ZPD and guided participation, is that
cognitive growth can only be understood if we acknowledge the
social origins of mental processes and recognize that cognitive
functions, especially in the early years, extend beyond the skin.
This is in marked contrast to the customary individualistic
assumption of psychologists – an assumption difficult to
abandon despite the lipservice now paid to the importance of
context, but well illustrated by the complete failure to take up
Vygotsky's suggestion that intellectual assessment should take
into account children's performance in joint sessions. Vygotsky
was convinced that thinking is not just something that goes on
inside an individual's head but is an activity that can be shared
– indeed of necessity has to be shared in the early stages of
development. As Rowe and Wertsch (2002) have put it, 'Study
of the "I" is thus abandoned in favour of study of the social,
cultural and historically situated ways by which "we" create
"I's".'
Further reading
· Faulkner, D., Littleton, K., & Woodhead, M. (Eds.)
(1998). Learning relationships in the classroom.
London: Routledge. Written mainly from an educational point
of view, this collection contains a lot of material relevant to the
concepts described above.
· Fernyhough, C. (1997). Vygotsky's sociocultural approach:
theoretical issues and implications for current research. In Hala,
S. (Ed.), The development of social cognition.
Hove: Psychology Press. A concise account of the main themes
in Vygotsky's theory.
· Rogoff, B. (2003). The cultural nature of human development.
Oxford: Oxford University Press. Gives a very detailed
description not only of Rogoff's theoretical position but also of
the research and thinking by other socio-cultural writers.
See also constructivism; context; social constructivism
© H. Rudolph Schaffer 2006
· APA
· Chicago
· Harvard
· MLA
ZONE OF PROXIMAL DEVELOPMENT and: CULTURAL
TOOLS SCAFFOLDING GUIDED PARTICIPATION. (2006). In
R. H. Schaffer, Key concepts in developmental psychology.
London, UK: Sage UK. Retrieved from
http://ezproxy.apus.edu/login?url=http://search.credoreference.c
om/content/entry/sageukdp/zone_of_proximal_development_and
_cultural_tools_scaffolding_guided_participation/0?institutionI
d=8703
Top of Form
Search
Bottom of Form
Browse Book
Previous ArticleNext Article
Key Concepts in Developmental Psychology
This book serves as a guide to the principal concepts currently
in use in Developmental Psychology and is perfect for courses
in child development or developmental psychology.
Author(s): Rudolph H. Schaffer
Edition: 1st
Articles: 66
Images: 1
People: 49
Related Credo Articles
Full text Article
Vygotsky, Lev
in Science in the Early Twentieth Century: An Encyclopedia
(b. Orsche, Russia 1896; d. USSR, 1934) Russian psychologist
Lev Vygotsky helped to establish the science of child
development. His work on...
566 words from ABC-CLIO
Full text Article
Introduction
in Curriculum Connections Psychology: Cognitive Development
Psychology forms part of the Curriculum Connections series.
Each of the six volumes of the set covers a particular aspect of
psychology: History of
416 words from Brown Bear Books Ltd
Full text Article
Reflections
in Key Thinkers in Psychology
While both Lev Vygotsky and Jean Piaget were concerned with
the development of cognitive capacities, their approaches
differed in...
264 words from Sage UK
Full text Article
Lev S., Vygotsky (1896–1934)
in Cambridge Encyclopedia of Child Development
Vygotsky was born in 1896 in Orsha (Belarus) to a middle-class
Jewish family, the second of eight children. He grew up in
Gomel...
719 words from Cambridge University Press
Full text Article
Lev Semionovich Vygotsky (1896-1934)
in Key Thinkers in Psychology
Suppressed on the orders of Stalin, the work of Vygotsky was
almost unknown in the West until the 1960s. Its first appearance
was in the now...
4011 words from Sage UK
Search other sites
· Academic Search Premier
· Business Source Complete
· Ebrary
· EBSCOhost
· Gale Virtual Reference Library
· IEEE
· JSTOR
· Lexis Nexis Academic
· Proquest Databases
· Safari
· Summon
Cognitive Development (Piaget) and
Intelligence
The topic for this week is cognitive development and
intelligence from the perspective of Piaget
and Vygotsky. Additionally, we will learn definitions of
intelligence, the predictive value of
intelligence tes
ts, variations in IQ, the role of early intervention in intellectual
development, and
the development of creativity.
Topics to be covered include:
·
Cognitive Development: Piagetian, Core Knowledge, and
Vygotskian
Perspectives
·
Role of Intelligence Testing in
the Development of Educational
Programs
·
Case Studies Related to Intelligence
Cognitive Development
We will begin to examine cognitive development, or how the
intellectual capabilities of infants
transform into those of the child, adolescent, and adult. Fi
rst, let us define cognition. Cognition
refers to the inner processes and products of the mind that lead
to “knowing.” In other words,
how do we acquire, comprehend, and apply knowledge? What
transformations must occur for
individuals to develop increasing
ly sophisticated mental capacities?
JEAN PIAGET
You have likely heard the name of Swiss cognitive theorist,
Jean Piaget. According to Piaget,
people are not cognitive beings at birth; instead, they discover,
or construct, all knowledge of the
world through
their own experiences. As they begin to construct knowledge,
they refine and
organize the information in order to effectively adapt to their
environments. This theory of active
construction of knowledge is known as the constructivist
approach to cognitive
development.
This approach follows children through four invariant (fixed
order) and universal (assumed to
characterize all children) stages: sensorimotor, preoperational,
concrete operational, and formal
operational. Throughout these stages, infants’ inv
estigative behaviors gradually transform into
the abstract, rational intelligence of more mature individuals.
PIAGET'S STAGES OF C
OGNITIVE DEVELOPMENT
SENSORIMOTOR
PREOPERATIONAL
CONCRETE OPERATIONAL
FORMAL OPERATIONAL
Cognitive Development (Piaget) and
Intelligence
The topic for this week is cognitive development and
intelligence from the perspective of Piaget
and Vygotsky. Additionally, we will learn definitions of
intelligence, the predictive value of
intelligence tests, variations in IQ, the role of early intervention
in intellectual development, and
the development of creativity.
Topics to be covered include:
Vygotskian
Perspectives
Educational
Programs
se Studies Related to Intelligence
Cognitive Development
We will begin to examine cognitive development, or how the
intellectual capabilities of infants
transform into those of the child, adolescent, and adult. First,
let us define cognition. Cognition
refers to the inner processes and products of the mind that lead
to “knowing.” In other words,
how do we acquire, comprehend, and apply knowledge? What
transformations must occur for
individuals to develop increasingly sophisticated mental
capacities?
JEAN PIAGET
You have likely heard the name of Swiss cognitive theorist,
Jean Piaget. According to Piaget,
people are not cognitive beings at birth; instead, they discover,
or construct, all knowledge of the
world through their own experiences. As they begin to construct
knowledge, they refine and
organize the information in order to effectively adapt to their
environments. This theory of active
construction of knowledge is known as the constructivist
approach to cognitive development.
This approach follows children through four invariant (fixed
order) and universal (assumed to
characterize all children) stages: sensorimotor, preoperational,
concrete operational, and formal
operational. Throughout these stages, infants’ investigative
behaviors gradually transform into
the abstract, rational intelligence of more mature individuals.
PIAGET'S STAGES OF COGNITIVE DEVELOPMENT
SENSORIMOTOR
PREOPERATIONAL
CONCRETE OPERATIONAL
FORMAL OPERATIONAL

More Related Content

Similar to Running head DETOXIFICATION FROM ALCOHOL AND OPIATES 1.docx

Similar to Running head DETOXIFICATION FROM ALCOHOL AND OPIATES 1.docx (8)

Piaget's Cognative Development
Piaget's Cognative DevelopmentPiaget's Cognative Development
Piaget's Cognative Development
 
Cognitive and social development are key areas of development
Cognitive and social development are key areas of development Cognitive and social development are key areas of development
Cognitive and social development are key areas of development
 
big dreams will help you a lot.
big dreams will help you a lot.big dreams will help you a lot.
big dreams will help you a lot.
 
PED 221 DEVELOPMENTAL PSYCHOLOGY.pdf
PED 221 DEVELOPMENTAL PSYCHOLOGY.pdfPED 221 DEVELOPMENTAL PSYCHOLOGY.pdf
PED 221 DEVELOPMENTAL PSYCHOLOGY.pdf
 
125956903 (2).pdf
125956903 (2).pdf125956903 (2).pdf
125956903 (2).pdf
 
Jean piaget by sheena bernal
Jean piaget by sheena bernalJean piaget by sheena bernal
Jean piaget by sheena bernal
 
Essay On Psychology
Essay On PsychologyEssay On Psychology
Essay On Psychology
 
Theories of development
Theories of developmentTheories of development
Theories of development
 

More from susanschei

Src TemplateStandard Recipe CardName of dishSpanish Vegie Tray Ba.docx
Src TemplateStandard Recipe CardName of dishSpanish Vegie Tray Ba.docxSrc TemplateStandard Recipe CardName of dishSpanish Vegie Tray Ba.docx
Src TemplateStandard Recipe CardName of dishSpanish Vegie Tray Ba.docxsusanschei
 
SPT 208 Final Project Guidelines and Rubric Overview .docx
SPT 208 Final Project Guidelines and Rubric  Overview .docxSPT 208 Final Project Guidelines and Rubric  Overview .docx
SPT 208 Final Project Guidelines and Rubric Overview .docxsusanschei
 
Ssalinas_ThreeMountainsRegionalHospitalCodeofEthics73119.docxR.docx
Ssalinas_ThreeMountainsRegionalHospitalCodeofEthics73119.docxR.docxSsalinas_ThreeMountainsRegionalHospitalCodeofEthics73119.docxR.docx
Ssalinas_ThreeMountainsRegionalHospitalCodeofEthics73119.docxR.docxsusanschei
 
Spring 2020Professor Tim SmithE mail [email protected]Teach.docx
Spring 2020Professor Tim SmithE mail [email protected]Teach.docxSpring 2020Professor Tim SmithE mail [email protected]Teach.docx
Spring 2020Professor Tim SmithE mail [email protected]Teach.docxsusanschei
 
Spring 2020 – Business Continuity & Disaster R.docx
Spring 2020 – Business Continuity & Disaster R.docxSpring 2020 – Business Continuity & Disaster R.docx
Spring 2020 – Business Continuity & Disaster R.docxsusanschei
 
Sports Business Landscape Graphic OrganizerContent.docx
Sports Business Landscape Graphic OrganizerContent.docxSports Business Landscape Graphic OrganizerContent.docx
Sports Business Landscape Graphic OrganizerContent.docxsusanschei
 
Spring 2020Carlow University Department of Psychology & Co.docx
Spring 2020Carlow University Department of Psychology & Co.docxSpring 2020Carlow University Department of Psychology & Co.docx
Spring 2020Carlow University Department of Psychology & Co.docxsusanschei
 
SPOTLIGHT ON STRATEGY FOR TURBULENT TIMESSpotlight ARTWORK.docx
SPOTLIGHT ON STRATEGY FOR TURBULENT TIMESSpotlight ARTWORK.docxSPOTLIGHT ON STRATEGY FOR TURBULENT TIMESSpotlight ARTWORK.docx
SPOTLIGHT ON STRATEGY FOR TURBULENT TIMESSpotlight ARTWORK.docxsusanschei
 
Sport Ticket sales staff trainingChapter 4Sales .docx
Sport Ticket sales staff trainingChapter 4Sales .docxSport Ticket sales staff trainingChapter 4Sales .docx
Sport Ticket sales staff trainingChapter 4Sales .docxsusanschei
 
SPOTLIGHT ARTWORK Do Ho Suh, Floor, 1997–2000, PVC figures, gl.docx
SPOTLIGHT ARTWORK Do Ho Suh, Floor, 1997–2000, PVC figures, gl.docxSPOTLIGHT ARTWORK Do Ho Suh, Floor, 1997–2000, PVC figures, gl.docx
SPOTLIGHT ARTWORK Do Ho Suh, Floor, 1997–2000, PVC figures, gl.docxsusanschei
 
Sponsorship Works 2018 8PROJECT DETAILSSponsorship tit.docx
Sponsorship Works 2018 8PROJECT DETAILSSponsorship tit.docxSponsorship Works 2018 8PROJECT DETAILSSponsorship tit.docx
Sponsorship Works 2018 8PROJECT DETAILSSponsorship tit.docxsusanschei
 
SPM 4723 Annotated Bibliography You second major proje.docx
SPM 4723 Annotated Bibliography You second major proje.docxSPM 4723 Annotated Bibliography You second major proje.docx
SPM 4723 Annotated Bibliography You second major proje.docxsusanschei
 
Speech Environment and Recording Requirements• You must have a.docx
Speech Environment and Recording Requirements• You must have a.docxSpeech Environment and Recording Requirements• You must have a.docx
Speech Environment and Recording Requirements• You must have a.docxsusanschei
 
Sped4 Interview 2.10.17 Audio.m4aJodee [000008] And we are .docx
Sped4 Interview 2.10.17 Audio.m4aJodee [000008] And we are .docxSped4 Interview 2.10.17 Audio.m4aJodee [000008] And we are .docx
Sped4 Interview 2.10.17 Audio.m4aJodee [000008] And we are .docxsusanschei
 
Speech Recognition in the Electronic Health Record (2013 u.docx
Speech Recognition in the Electronic Health Record (2013 u.docxSpeech Recognition in the Electronic Health Record (2013 u.docx
Speech Recognition in the Electronic Health Record (2013 u.docxsusanschei
 
Sped Focus Group.m4aJodee [000001] This is a focus group wi.docx
Sped Focus Group.m4aJodee [000001] This is a focus group wi.docxSped Focus Group.m4aJodee [000001] This is a focus group wi.docx
Sped Focus Group.m4aJodee [000001] This is a focus group wi.docxsusanschei
 
Specialized Terms 20.0 Definitions and examples of specialized.docx
Specialized Terms 20.0 Definitions and examples of specialized.docxSpecialized Terms 20.0 Definitions and examples of specialized.docx
Specialized Terms 20.0 Definitions and examples of specialized.docxsusanschei
 
Special notes Media and the media are plural and take plural verb.docx
Special notes Media and the media are plural and take plural verb.docxSpecial notes Media and the media are plural and take plural verb.docx
Special notes Media and the media are plural and take plural verb.docxsusanschei
 
SPECIAL ISSUE ON POLITICAL VIOLENCEResearch on Social Move.docx
SPECIAL ISSUE ON POLITICAL VIOLENCEResearch on Social Move.docxSPECIAL ISSUE ON POLITICAL VIOLENCEResearch on Social Move.docx
SPECIAL ISSUE ON POLITICAL VIOLENCEResearch on Social Move.docxsusanschei
 
SPECIAL ISSUE CRITICAL REALISM IN IS RESEARCHCRITICAL RE.docx
SPECIAL ISSUE  CRITICAL REALISM IN IS RESEARCHCRITICAL RE.docxSPECIAL ISSUE  CRITICAL REALISM IN IS RESEARCHCRITICAL RE.docx
SPECIAL ISSUE CRITICAL REALISM IN IS RESEARCHCRITICAL RE.docxsusanschei
 

More from susanschei (20)

Src TemplateStandard Recipe CardName of dishSpanish Vegie Tray Ba.docx
Src TemplateStandard Recipe CardName of dishSpanish Vegie Tray Ba.docxSrc TemplateStandard Recipe CardName of dishSpanish Vegie Tray Ba.docx
Src TemplateStandard Recipe CardName of dishSpanish Vegie Tray Ba.docx
 
SPT 208 Final Project Guidelines and Rubric Overview .docx
SPT 208 Final Project Guidelines and Rubric  Overview .docxSPT 208 Final Project Guidelines and Rubric  Overview .docx
SPT 208 Final Project Guidelines and Rubric Overview .docx
 
Ssalinas_ThreeMountainsRegionalHospitalCodeofEthics73119.docxR.docx
Ssalinas_ThreeMountainsRegionalHospitalCodeofEthics73119.docxR.docxSsalinas_ThreeMountainsRegionalHospitalCodeofEthics73119.docxR.docx
Ssalinas_ThreeMountainsRegionalHospitalCodeofEthics73119.docxR.docx
 
Spring 2020Professor Tim SmithE mail [email protected]Teach.docx
Spring 2020Professor Tim SmithE mail [email protected]Teach.docxSpring 2020Professor Tim SmithE mail [email protected]Teach.docx
Spring 2020Professor Tim SmithE mail [email protected]Teach.docx
 
Spring 2020 – Business Continuity & Disaster R.docx
Spring 2020 – Business Continuity & Disaster R.docxSpring 2020 – Business Continuity & Disaster R.docx
Spring 2020 – Business Continuity & Disaster R.docx
 
Sports Business Landscape Graphic OrganizerContent.docx
Sports Business Landscape Graphic OrganizerContent.docxSports Business Landscape Graphic OrganizerContent.docx
Sports Business Landscape Graphic OrganizerContent.docx
 
Spring 2020Carlow University Department of Psychology & Co.docx
Spring 2020Carlow University Department of Psychology & Co.docxSpring 2020Carlow University Department of Psychology & Co.docx
Spring 2020Carlow University Department of Psychology & Co.docx
 
SPOTLIGHT ON STRATEGY FOR TURBULENT TIMESSpotlight ARTWORK.docx
SPOTLIGHT ON STRATEGY FOR TURBULENT TIMESSpotlight ARTWORK.docxSPOTLIGHT ON STRATEGY FOR TURBULENT TIMESSpotlight ARTWORK.docx
SPOTLIGHT ON STRATEGY FOR TURBULENT TIMESSpotlight ARTWORK.docx
 
Sport Ticket sales staff trainingChapter 4Sales .docx
Sport Ticket sales staff trainingChapter 4Sales .docxSport Ticket sales staff trainingChapter 4Sales .docx
Sport Ticket sales staff trainingChapter 4Sales .docx
 
SPOTLIGHT ARTWORK Do Ho Suh, Floor, 1997–2000, PVC figures, gl.docx
SPOTLIGHT ARTWORK Do Ho Suh, Floor, 1997–2000, PVC figures, gl.docxSPOTLIGHT ARTWORK Do Ho Suh, Floor, 1997–2000, PVC figures, gl.docx
SPOTLIGHT ARTWORK Do Ho Suh, Floor, 1997–2000, PVC figures, gl.docx
 
Sponsorship Works 2018 8PROJECT DETAILSSponsorship tit.docx
Sponsorship Works 2018 8PROJECT DETAILSSponsorship tit.docxSponsorship Works 2018 8PROJECT DETAILSSponsorship tit.docx
Sponsorship Works 2018 8PROJECT DETAILSSponsorship tit.docx
 
SPM 4723 Annotated Bibliography You second major proje.docx
SPM 4723 Annotated Bibliography You second major proje.docxSPM 4723 Annotated Bibliography You second major proje.docx
SPM 4723 Annotated Bibliography You second major proje.docx
 
Speech Environment and Recording Requirements• You must have a.docx
Speech Environment and Recording Requirements• You must have a.docxSpeech Environment and Recording Requirements• You must have a.docx
Speech Environment and Recording Requirements• You must have a.docx
 
Sped4 Interview 2.10.17 Audio.m4aJodee [000008] And we are .docx
Sped4 Interview 2.10.17 Audio.m4aJodee [000008] And we are .docxSped4 Interview 2.10.17 Audio.m4aJodee [000008] And we are .docx
Sped4 Interview 2.10.17 Audio.m4aJodee [000008] And we are .docx
 
Speech Recognition in the Electronic Health Record (2013 u.docx
Speech Recognition in the Electronic Health Record (2013 u.docxSpeech Recognition in the Electronic Health Record (2013 u.docx
Speech Recognition in the Electronic Health Record (2013 u.docx
 
Sped Focus Group.m4aJodee [000001] This is a focus group wi.docx
Sped Focus Group.m4aJodee [000001] This is a focus group wi.docxSped Focus Group.m4aJodee [000001] This is a focus group wi.docx
Sped Focus Group.m4aJodee [000001] This is a focus group wi.docx
 
Specialized Terms 20.0 Definitions and examples of specialized.docx
Specialized Terms 20.0 Definitions and examples of specialized.docxSpecialized Terms 20.0 Definitions and examples of specialized.docx
Specialized Terms 20.0 Definitions and examples of specialized.docx
 
Special notes Media and the media are plural and take plural verb.docx
Special notes Media and the media are plural and take plural verb.docxSpecial notes Media and the media are plural and take plural verb.docx
Special notes Media and the media are plural and take plural verb.docx
 
SPECIAL ISSUE ON POLITICAL VIOLENCEResearch on Social Move.docx
SPECIAL ISSUE ON POLITICAL VIOLENCEResearch on Social Move.docxSPECIAL ISSUE ON POLITICAL VIOLENCEResearch on Social Move.docx
SPECIAL ISSUE ON POLITICAL VIOLENCEResearch on Social Move.docx
 
SPECIAL ISSUE CRITICAL REALISM IN IS RESEARCHCRITICAL RE.docx
SPECIAL ISSUE  CRITICAL REALISM IN IS RESEARCHCRITICAL RE.docxSPECIAL ISSUE  CRITICAL REALISM IN IS RESEARCHCRITICAL RE.docx
SPECIAL ISSUE CRITICAL REALISM IN IS RESEARCHCRITICAL RE.docx
 

Recently uploaded

Introduction to AI in Higher Education_draft.pptx
Introduction to AI in Higher Education_draft.pptxIntroduction to AI in Higher Education_draft.pptx
Introduction to AI in Higher Education_draft.pptxpboyjonauth
 
Measures of Central Tendency: Mean, Median and Mode
Measures of Central Tendency: Mean, Median and ModeMeasures of Central Tendency: Mean, Median and Mode
Measures of Central Tendency: Mean, Median and ModeThiyagu K
 
Employee wellbeing at the workplace.pptx
Employee wellbeing at the workplace.pptxEmployee wellbeing at the workplace.pptx
Employee wellbeing at the workplace.pptxNirmalaLoungPoorunde1
 
microwave assisted reaction. General introduction
microwave assisted reaction. General introductionmicrowave assisted reaction. General introduction
microwave assisted reaction. General introductionMaksud Ahmed
 
Separation of Lanthanides/ Lanthanides and Actinides
Separation of Lanthanides/ Lanthanides and ActinidesSeparation of Lanthanides/ Lanthanides and Actinides
Separation of Lanthanides/ Lanthanides and ActinidesFatimaKhan178732
 
Solving Puzzles Benefits Everyone (English).pptx
Solving Puzzles Benefits Everyone (English).pptxSolving Puzzles Benefits Everyone (English).pptx
Solving Puzzles Benefits Everyone (English).pptxOH TEIK BIN
 
_Math 4-Q4 Week 5.pptx Steps in Collecting Data
_Math 4-Q4 Week 5.pptx Steps in Collecting Data_Math 4-Q4 Week 5.pptx Steps in Collecting Data
_Math 4-Q4 Week 5.pptx Steps in Collecting DataJhengPantaleon
 
BASLIQ CURRENT LOOKBOOK LOOKBOOK(1) (1).pdf
BASLIQ CURRENT LOOKBOOK  LOOKBOOK(1) (1).pdfBASLIQ CURRENT LOOKBOOK  LOOKBOOK(1) (1).pdf
BASLIQ CURRENT LOOKBOOK LOOKBOOK(1) (1).pdfSoniaTolstoy
 
Call Girls in Dwarka Mor Delhi Contact Us 9654467111
Call Girls in Dwarka Mor Delhi Contact Us 9654467111Call Girls in Dwarka Mor Delhi Contact Us 9654467111
Call Girls in Dwarka Mor Delhi Contact Us 9654467111Sapana Sha
 
URLs and Routing in the Odoo 17 Website App
URLs and Routing in the Odoo 17 Website AppURLs and Routing in the Odoo 17 Website App
URLs and Routing in the Odoo 17 Website AppCeline George
 
Incoming and Outgoing Shipments in 1 STEP Using Odoo 17
Incoming and Outgoing Shipments in 1 STEP Using Odoo 17Incoming and Outgoing Shipments in 1 STEP Using Odoo 17
Incoming and Outgoing Shipments in 1 STEP Using Odoo 17Celine George
 
Hybridoma Technology ( Production , Purification , and Application )
Hybridoma Technology  ( Production , Purification , and Application  ) Hybridoma Technology  ( Production , Purification , and Application  )
Hybridoma Technology ( Production , Purification , and Application ) Sakshi Ghasle
 
A Critique of the Proposed National Education Policy Reform
A Critique of the Proposed National Education Policy ReformA Critique of the Proposed National Education Policy Reform
A Critique of the Proposed National Education Policy ReformChameera Dedduwage
 
Software Engineering Methodologies (overview)
Software Engineering Methodologies (overview)Software Engineering Methodologies (overview)
Software Engineering Methodologies (overview)eniolaolutunde
 
Crayon Activity Handout For the Crayon A
Crayon Activity Handout For the Crayon ACrayon Activity Handout For the Crayon A
Crayon Activity Handout For the Crayon AUnboundStockton
 
Arihant handbook biology for class 11 .pdf
Arihant handbook biology for class 11 .pdfArihant handbook biology for class 11 .pdf
Arihant handbook biology for class 11 .pdfchloefrazer622
 
The basics of sentences session 2pptx copy.pptx
The basics of sentences session 2pptx copy.pptxThe basics of sentences session 2pptx copy.pptx
The basics of sentences session 2pptx copy.pptxheathfieldcps1
 
Concept of Vouching. B.Com(Hons) /B.Compdf
Concept of Vouching. B.Com(Hons) /B.CompdfConcept of Vouching. B.Com(Hons) /B.Compdf
Concept of Vouching. B.Com(Hons) /B.CompdfUmakantAnnand
 
Science 7 - LAND and SEA BREEZE and its Characteristics
Science 7 - LAND and SEA BREEZE and its CharacteristicsScience 7 - LAND and SEA BREEZE and its Characteristics
Science 7 - LAND and SEA BREEZE and its CharacteristicsKarinaGenton
 

Recently uploaded (20)

Introduction to AI in Higher Education_draft.pptx
Introduction to AI in Higher Education_draft.pptxIntroduction to AI in Higher Education_draft.pptx
Introduction to AI in Higher Education_draft.pptx
 
Measures of Central Tendency: Mean, Median and Mode
Measures of Central Tendency: Mean, Median and ModeMeasures of Central Tendency: Mean, Median and Mode
Measures of Central Tendency: Mean, Median and Mode
 
Employee wellbeing at the workplace.pptx
Employee wellbeing at the workplace.pptxEmployee wellbeing at the workplace.pptx
Employee wellbeing at the workplace.pptx
 
microwave assisted reaction. General introduction
microwave assisted reaction. General introductionmicrowave assisted reaction. General introduction
microwave assisted reaction. General introduction
 
Separation of Lanthanides/ Lanthanides and Actinides
Separation of Lanthanides/ Lanthanides and ActinidesSeparation of Lanthanides/ Lanthanides and Actinides
Separation of Lanthanides/ Lanthanides and Actinides
 
Solving Puzzles Benefits Everyone (English).pptx
Solving Puzzles Benefits Everyone (English).pptxSolving Puzzles Benefits Everyone (English).pptx
Solving Puzzles Benefits Everyone (English).pptx
 
_Math 4-Q4 Week 5.pptx Steps in Collecting Data
_Math 4-Q4 Week 5.pptx Steps in Collecting Data_Math 4-Q4 Week 5.pptx Steps in Collecting Data
_Math 4-Q4 Week 5.pptx Steps in Collecting Data
 
BASLIQ CURRENT LOOKBOOK LOOKBOOK(1) (1).pdf
BASLIQ CURRENT LOOKBOOK  LOOKBOOK(1) (1).pdfBASLIQ CURRENT LOOKBOOK  LOOKBOOK(1) (1).pdf
BASLIQ CURRENT LOOKBOOK LOOKBOOK(1) (1).pdf
 
Call Girls in Dwarka Mor Delhi Contact Us 9654467111
Call Girls in Dwarka Mor Delhi Contact Us 9654467111Call Girls in Dwarka Mor Delhi Contact Us 9654467111
Call Girls in Dwarka Mor Delhi Contact Us 9654467111
 
URLs and Routing in the Odoo 17 Website App
URLs and Routing in the Odoo 17 Website AppURLs and Routing in the Odoo 17 Website App
URLs and Routing in the Odoo 17 Website App
 
Incoming and Outgoing Shipments in 1 STEP Using Odoo 17
Incoming and Outgoing Shipments in 1 STEP Using Odoo 17Incoming and Outgoing Shipments in 1 STEP Using Odoo 17
Incoming and Outgoing Shipments in 1 STEP Using Odoo 17
 
Hybridoma Technology ( Production , Purification , and Application )
Hybridoma Technology  ( Production , Purification , and Application  ) Hybridoma Technology  ( Production , Purification , and Application  )
Hybridoma Technology ( Production , Purification , and Application )
 
A Critique of the Proposed National Education Policy Reform
A Critique of the Proposed National Education Policy ReformA Critique of the Proposed National Education Policy Reform
A Critique of the Proposed National Education Policy Reform
 
Software Engineering Methodologies (overview)
Software Engineering Methodologies (overview)Software Engineering Methodologies (overview)
Software Engineering Methodologies (overview)
 
Model Call Girl in Tilak Nagar Delhi reach out to us at 🔝9953056974🔝
Model Call Girl in Tilak Nagar Delhi reach out to us at 🔝9953056974🔝Model Call Girl in Tilak Nagar Delhi reach out to us at 🔝9953056974🔝
Model Call Girl in Tilak Nagar Delhi reach out to us at 🔝9953056974🔝
 
Crayon Activity Handout For the Crayon A
Crayon Activity Handout For the Crayon ACrayon Activity Handout For the Crayon A
Crayon Activity Handout For the Crayon A
 
Arihant handbook biology for class 11 .pdf
Arihant handbook biology for class 11 .pdfArihant handbook biology for class 11 .pdf
Arihant handbook biology for class 11 .pdf
 
The basics of sentences session 2pptx copy.pptx
The basics of sentences session 2pptx copy.pptxThe basics of sentences session 2pptx copy.pptx
The basics of sentences session 2pptx copy.pptx
 
Concept of Vouching. B.Com(Hons) /B.Compdf
Concept of Vouching. B.Com(Hons) /B.CompdfConcept of Vouching. B.Com(Hons) /B.Compdf
Concept of Vouching. B.Com(Hons) /B.Compdf
 
Science 7 - LAND and SEA BREEZE and its Characteristics
Science 7 - LAND and SEA BREEZE and its CharacteristicsScience 7 - LAND and SEA BREEZE and its Characteristics
Science 7 - LAND and SEA BREEZE and its Characteristics
 

Running head DETOXIFICATION FROM ALCOHOL AND OPIATES 1.docx

  • 1. Running head: DETOXIFICATION FROM ALCOHOL AND OPIATES 1 5 DETOXIFICATION FROM ALCOHOL AND OPIATES Research Paper Outline: Detoxification from Alcohol and Opiates Grand Canyon University: PCN-527-0500 December 06, 2017 I. BRIEF HISTORY OF THE DRUG A. History of Alcohol B. History of Opiates II. PSYCHOPHARMACOLOGY OF THE DRUG A. Psychopharmacology of Alcohol 1. Effects of alcohol on mood 2. Effects of alcohol on thinking 3. Effects of alcohol on behavior 4. Effects of alcohol on sensation B. Psychopharmacology of Opiates 1. Effects of alcohol on mood 2. Effects of alcohol on thinking 3. Effects of alcohol on behavior
  • 2. 4. Effects of alcohol on sensation III. CURRENT TRENDS OF ISSUES A. Current trends of issues of alcohol 1. Alcohol addiction 2. Binge drinking 3. Addiction 4. Physical symptom of withdrawal 5. Treatment B. Current trends or issues of opiates 1. Types of opiates 2. Addiction 3. Physical symptoms of withdrawal 4. Treatment IV. SOCIETAL CONCERNS OR ISSUES RELATED TO THE TOPIC A. Stigma of being an alcoholic 1. Physical symptoms 2. Disruption in functioning 3. Other’s perceptions of addiction B. Stigma of being addicted to opiates 1. Physical Symptoms 2. Disruption in functioning 3. Other’s perception of addiction V. SIGNIFICANCE OR RELEVANCE ON THE COUNSELING PROFESSION A. Relevance of alcohol addiction and treatment on the counseling profession 1. Different option for treatment 2. Treatment after detoxification B. Relevance of opiate addiction and treatment on the counseling profession 1. Different option for treatment 2. Treatment after detoxification VI. ANY FUTURE IMPLICATIONS A. Future implications of alcohol detoxification
  • 3. B. Future implications of opiate detoxification References Carlebach, S., Wake, D., & Hamilton, S. (2011). Experiences of home detoxification for alcohol dependency. Nursing Standard, 26(10), 41-47. Green, L., & Gossop, M. (1988). Effects of information on the opiate withdrawal syndrome. British Journal Of Addiction, 83(3), 305-309. doi:10.1111/j.1360-0443.1988.tb00472.x Ison, J., Day, E., Fisher, K., Pratt, M., Hull, M., & Copello, A. (2006). Self-detoxification from opioid drugs. Journal Of Substance Use, 11(2), 81-88. doi:10.1080/14659890500143697 Masson, C. L., Barnett, P. G., Sees, K. L., Delucchi, K. L., Rosen, A., Wong, W., & Hall, S. M. (2004). Cost and cost- effectiveness of standard methadone maintenance treatment compared to enriched 180-day methadone detoxification. Addiction, 99(6), 718-726. doi:10.1111/j.1360- 0443.2004.00728.x Perdomo Gutiérrez, R. E. (2011). Clinical case of rapid opiate detoxification under anesthesia. Anestesia Pediatrica E Neonatale, 9(1), 1-10. Van den Berg, J. F., Van den Brink, W., Kist, N., Hermes, J. J., & Kok, R. M. (2015). Social factors and readmission after inpatient detoxification in older alcohol-dependent patients. The American Journal On Addictions, (7), 661. doi:10.1111/ajad.12287 Wulffson, R. M. (2014). Detoxification and substance abuse. Salem Press Encyclopedia Of Health.
  • 4. Cognitive Development (Piaget) and Intelligence The topic for this week is cognitive development and intelligence from the perspective of Piaget and Vygotsky. Additionally, we will learn definitions of intelligence, the predictive value of intelligence tests, variations in IQ, the role of early intervention in intellectual development, and the development of creativity. Topics to be covered include: · Cognitive Development: Piagetian, Core Knowledge, and Vygotskian Perspectives · Role of Intelligence Testing in the Development of Educational Programs · Case Studies Related to Intelligence Cognitive Development We will begin to examine cognitive development, or how the intellectual capabilities of infants transform into those of the child, adolescent, and adult. First, let us define cognition. Cognition refers to the inner processes and products of the mind that lead to “knowing.” In other words, how do we acquire, comprehend, and apply knowledge? What transformations must occur for individuals to develop increasingly sophisticated mental capacities? JEAN PIAGET You have likely heard the name of Swiss cognitive theorist, Jean Piaget. According to Piaget, people are not cognitive beings at birth; instead, they discover, or construct, all knowledge of the world through their own experiences. As they begin to construct knowledge, they refine and organize the information in order to effectively adapt to their environments. This theory of active construction of knowledge is known as the constructivist approach to cognitive development. This approach follows children through four invariant (fixed order) and universal (assumed to characterize all children) stages: sensorimotor, preoperational, concrete operational, and formal operational. Throughout these stages, infants’ investigative
  • 5. behaviors gradually transform into the abstract, rational intelligence of more mature individuals. PIAGET'S STAGES OF COGNITIVE DEVELOPMENT SENSORIMOTOR PREOPERATIONAL CONCRETE OPERATIONAL FORMAL OPERATIONAL Piaget identified specific psychological structures called schemes (organized ways of making sense of experiences) that change with age. Initially, schemes are patterns of action involving the senses and motor functions. For example, a baby may simply grab and release an object. As the baby gets older, this scheme becomes more deliberate, and she may begin to throw the object down the stairs, up in the air, or against walls. In other words, she is thinking before she acts. When there evidence of this, Piaget says the child has moved from a sensorimotor approach to a cognitive approach, which uses mental representations (images and concepts) to develop efficient thinking habits. This advancement in thinking relies on adaptation and organization. Adaptation involves building schemes through direct interaction with the environment, while organization is an internal process that connects schemes to create a powerfully interrelated cognitive system. Piaget’s Impact Although follow-up research has changed the way we look at some aspects of Piaget’s theory, it is important to note the major impact his theory had on educational practices. Teacher training and learning procedures have reflected three educational principles derived from his view: discovery learning (providing exploration experiences to guide learning rather than verbally offering facts), sensitivity to children’s readiness to learn (introducing new activities when children are ready, not to speed up development), and acceptance of individual differences (using knowledge of each child’s specific rate of
  • 6. development to plan small group activities). CHALLENGES AND EXTENSIONS TO PIAGET’S THEORIES At present, most experts agree that cognition is less stage-like than Piaget suggested. They instead embrace the understanding that children are continuously modifying their thinking and obtaining new skills. Moreover, researchers typically disagree on whether cognitive development is general or specific. These challenges have led researchers to extend or modify Piaget’s work. Those who believe differently than Piaget regarding the limited cognitive capabilities of infants have proposed the core knowledge perspective. CORE KNOWLEDGE PERSPECTIVE The core knowledge perspective is a second set of theories related to cognitive development. Unlike Piaget, who believed infants come into the world only with sensorimotor reflexes, those who embrace this perspective believe that infants are innately equipped with core domains of thought that support rapid cognitive development. In other words, infants are prewired to make sense of certain stimuli. Each core domain is essential for survival and develops independently, resulting in uneven, domain-specific changes. PHYSICAL KNOWLEDGE AND NUMERICAL KNOWLEDGE Two core domains have been studied at length in infancy: physical knowledge and numerical knowledge. Physical knowledge is the understanding of objects and their effects on one another. Numerical knowledge is the capacity to keep track of multiple objects and to add and subtract small quantities. Observation of infants has shown understanding in these areas occurring quite early, supporting the idea that some knowledge must be innate. Children gradually build on that knowledge and it becomes more intricate as through exploration, play, and social interaction. They are viewed as naïve theorists, who create explanations of events based on innate knowledge. Their explanations, or theories, are tested with experience and revised if needed. These revisions lead to increased reasoning about cause and effect situations. While this is an intriguing idea
  • 7. about how cognitive skills are able to emerge early and rapidly develop, this theory has not offered clarity on how children make the necessary revisions that prompt cognitive change. Vygotsky’s Sociocultural Theory The sociocultural theory is the third set of theories related to cognitive development and is founded in the work of Lev Vygotsky. Vygotsky believed that infants are born with elementary perceptual, attention, and memory capacities, which develop in the first two years through interaction with others. Vygotsky did not view cognitive development as individualistic, but placed a significant emphasis on culture or community. Unlike Piaget, who favored independent discovery leading to construction of knowledge, Vygotsky asserted that acquisition of knowledge is a consequence of social interactions. Specifically, learning takes place within the zone of proximal development, which is a range of tasks too difficult for the child to do alone but possible with the help of adults and more skilled peers. When these more knowledgeable individuals question, prompt, and suggest strategies for mastering a specific task within the zone of proximal development, the child is drawn into more mature thinking processes. Support during learning can gradually be adjusted, based on the child’s needs, a concept known as scaffolding. Also, due to his emphasis on social experience and language, Vygotsky saw make-believe play as a major zone of proximal development for preschoolers. VYGOTSKY’S IMPACT When applied in the classroom, Vygotsky’s theory teaches us to highlight collaboration. While we again see that children should be active participants in learning, we now go beyond individual discovery (Piaget) to discovery through teacher guidance and peer partnerships. In preschool, there should be many opportunities for make-believe play. In all grades, there should be opportunities for talk, as this dialogue prompts reflection on thought processes, which, in turn leads to increased cognitive awareness.
  • 8. Knowledge Check 1 Question 1 Which theory on cognitive development places a large emphasis on language and social interaction as the foundation for learning? Vygotsky’s Sociocultural Theory Piaget’s Theory of Cognitive Development Core Knowledge Perspective I don't know One attempt Submit answer You answered 0 out of 0 correctly. Asking up to 1. Case Study ‹ 1/2 › · STUDY 1 To highlight how children’s drawings can help identify adjustment difficulties at school. As noted in the text, a variety of factors—the realization that pictures can serve as symbols, improved planning and spatial understanding, and the emphasis the child’s culture places on artistic expression—influence the development of children’s artful representations. According to Harrison, Clarke, and Ungerer (2007), drawings can also provide insight into relationships with adults, as well as children’s overall adjustment. In one study, researchers recruited 125 six-year-olds and collected the following information: · Participants completed a 30-minute interview that addressed general perceptions of themselves, their school, and their teachers. · Participants completed the School Liking and Avoidance Scale. Using a three-point scale (yes=3, no=2, sometimes=1), children were asked such questions as, “Is school fun?” “Do you
  • 9. enjoy school?” “Do you ask your parents to let you stay home from school?” “Does school make you feel like crying?” To assess participants’ feelings about their teacher, they were asked: “Do you like to see your teacher when you get to school?” “Is your teacher nice to you?” “Does your teacher smile at you?” “Does your teacher play or read with you?” · Participants were asked to draw a picture of themselves and their teacher at school. Drawing materials were provided but no further instructions were given. Researchers recorded any spontaneous comments, and once participants completed their drawings, they were asked to identify the people and objects in the picture. Pictures were scored on the following dimensions, which are commonly used to assess child/family drawings: · Creativity—going beyond the instructions and adding lively, colorful, or imaginative features. · Pride/happiness—showing an emotional connectedness to the teacher, such as holding hands or doing something fun with the teacher. · Vulnerability—using overwhelming, exaggerated, distorted, or displaced body images. · Emotional distance/isolation—using expressions of anger, negative affect, physical distance from the teacher, or physical barriers between the child and teacher. · Tension/anger—showing rigid and constricted features or scribbling out the teacher’s face. · Role reversal—representing the child as larger, more powerful, or more potent than the teacher. · Bizarreness/dissociation—including unusual signs or symbols, angry facial features, or morbid fantasy themes. · Teachers rated their relationship quality with each child on five dimensions: conflict/anger, warmth/positive emotions, open communication, dependency, and troubled feelings. · Teachers rated participants’ overall school adjustment by identifying the prevalence of problem behaviors (for example, acting out, aggression, learning problems) and strengths/competencies (for example, leadership, frustration
  • 10. tolerance, social skills). Results Findings revealed significant relationships among children’s reports, children’s drawings, teacher-rated relationship quality, and teacher-rated school adjustment. That is, children who reported negative feelings about their teacher also included negative themes in their drawings, such as scribbling out the teacher’s face. Their teachers, in turn, were likely to report strained teacher–child relationships and problematic classroom behavior. Taken together, these findings suggest that children’s artistic representations of relationships with teachers are an important tool for identifying adjustment difficulties at school. Definitions of Intelligence How do you view intelligence? When you consider a highly intelligent person, what behaviors stand out to you? It is probably not surprising to learn that experts disagree on the attributes that make up intelligence and that the definition of intelligence has evolved over time. You may recall that the first intelligence test was created by Alfred Binet and his colleague, Theodore Simon, in 1905 in response to educators’ needs to identify students who were unlikely to benefit from standard classroom instruction. Binet was asked to develop an impartial means of assigning students to classes, one which focused on mental aptitude and not disruptive behavior. Factors such as memory and reasoning skills were considered and compared to children of the same ages. PSYCHOMETRIC APPROACH FACTOR ANALYSIS OTHER APPROACHES Louis Thurstone, in contrast, viewed intelligence as less of a single, general ability and more of a set of distinct abilities. His factor analysis on more than 50 intelligence tests suggested that independent, individual factors of intelligence exist. He called these factors primary mental abilities, and categorized these abilities as follows: verbal comprehension, reasoning, perceptual speed, numerical ability, word fluency, associative
  • 11. memory, and spatial visualization. Eventually, both Spearman and Thurstone recognized each other’s perspectives, and current theorists combine both approaches when designing tests. Subtest scores are used to determine an individual’s specific strengths and weaknesses (Thurstone’s original theory), but can also be combined into an overall general intelligence score (Spearman’s original theory). However, many researchers believe that merely combining these two perspectives is not enough, as factors on intelligence tests have only partial value if we cannot recognize the cognitive processes accountable for those factors. If we are able to do that, we will better understand what skills must be reinforced to improve a particular child’s performance. To facilitate this, psychometric and information-processing approaches were combined and componential analyses conducted, looking for relationships involving components of information processing and children’s intelligence test performance. While this helps to detect cognitive skills that contribute to intelligence, this approach does not include cultural and situational factors that may affect intelligence. STERNBERG GARDNER Measuring Intelligence Although we have discussed some limitations to intelligence testing, they are often helpful in identifying highly gifted children and diagnosing learning problems. Standardized intelligence tests utilize the scores of a large, representative sample of individuals as a standard for interpreting individual scores. Scores produce an intelligence quotient, or IQ, which indicates the extent to which the number of items passed (raw score) deviates from the average performance of individuals of the same age. Group-administered standardized tests are useful for instructional planning, while individually administered tests consider both the child’s answers and behaviors, which paints a more accurate picture of the child’s abilities. Two of these individual tests – the Stanford-Binet Intelligence Scales, Fifth
  • 12. Edition, and the Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children–IV (WISC–IV) – are used most often. APTITUDE AND ACHIEVEMENT TESTS Aptitude and achievement tests are similar to intelligence tests. Aptitude tests assess a child’s propensity for excelling in a specific type of activity, such as music or language. On the other hand, achievement tests measure the attainment of particular knowledge and skills, such as reading comprehension or concepts covered within a particular class. Achievement test are often given by teachers to assess their students’ understanding of a particular subject area. There are even tests to measure the intelligence of infants, although these present unique challenges, as babies are unable to answer questions or follow directions. Tests performed with infants typically measure perceptual and motor responses; however, more recent tests also attempt to determine early language, cognition, and social behavior. Ethnic and Socioeconomic Impact on IQ scores We know that many factors affect child development and that both heredity and environment shape a child’s unique qualities. What are the roles of nature versus nurture in regards to determining IQ? You will not be surprised to learn that there has been much debate regarding this question, as researchers are concerned with locating the cause of IQ disparities between and among certain ethnicities. Either heredity or environmental factors must be responsible for the variations in test performance. HEREDITY Heritability estimates, or measures which researchers use to correlate the IQs of family members who share genes, reveal that heredity does play a role in IQ test performance. Adoption studies have also supported these findings, as children in the study eventually became more similar in IQ to their biological mothers and less similar to their adoptive parents. However, within the same studies, it was noted that the IQ of children were adopted into privileged homes benefited from a rise in IQ
  • 13. in comparison with non-adopted children who remained in disadvantaged homes. ENVIRONMENTAL FACTORS This reminds us that environmental factors also contribute to individual differences in intelligence, as do family beliefs about intellectual success and expectations for children’s educational attainment. Other factors, such as communication styles, lack of familiarity with test content, reactions to testing conditions, and fear of being judged on the basis of negative stereotypes can lead test scores to underestimate minority children’s intelligence. An approach to testing, known as dynamic assessment, seems to discover what a child can attain with social support, thus reducing bias in testing. USE OF TESTING‹ 1/2 › · Intervention Programs for Lower Achieving Students Because research shows a correlation between socioeconomic status and performance in intelligence tests, intervention programs for economically disadvantaged students have been implemented. The majority of these programs begin as early as preschool, with the hopes of offsetting learning challenges before formal schooling begins. You may have heard of Project Head Start, which is one of the largest federal programs, offering academic support, as well as nutritional and health service and promoting parental involvement. Research on early intervention programs indicates that, although instantaneous gains in IQ tend to rapidly dissipate, enduring benefits are noted in school adjustment, attitudes, and motivation. A program in which high-quality intervention starts early, is intensive, emphasizes parent involvement, and focuses on the whole child is likely to have a more far-reaching impact in all areas, including IQ. Knowledge Check 1 Question 1 Which type of standardized test provides us with knowledge
  • 14. about a child’s potential proficiency with a particular talent? Aptitude tests Achievement tests Intelligence tests I don't know One attempt Submit answer You answered 0 out of 0 correctly. Asking up to 1. Case Study Research studies provide examples various types of intelligence testing and the use of testing results. The first case study examines the relationship of emotional intelligence and successful intelligence to leadership skills in gifted students. The second case study inspects the relationship among low birth weight, social disadvantage, and children’s IQ scores at ages six, 11, and 17. ‹ 1/2 › · STUDY 1 Emotional Intelligence, Successful Intelligence, and Leadership Skills Emotional intelligence is positively associated with self-esteem, empathy, prosocial behavior, cooperation, leadership skills, and life satisfaction. In addition, Sternberg’s theory underscores the importance of analytical, creative, and practical skills—skills that are often overlooked on intelligence tests—for life success. To examine the relationship among emotional intelligence, successful intelligence, and leadership skills, Chan (2007) recruited 498 gifted students in grades 4 through 12 and collected the following information: (1) Participants completed a leadership rating scale, which measures leadership self-efficacy, leadership flexibility, and goal orientation (related to leadership and achievement). (2) Participants completed an emotional intelligence scale that assesses social skills, empathy, management of emotions, and utilization of emotions. (3) Participants completed Sternberg’s
  • 15. Successful Intelligence Questionnaire, which measures analytical, creative, and practical abilities. Results Results indicated that both emotional intelligence and successful intelligence predicted leadership skills. That is, participants who scored high in emotional and successful intelligence scored higher in overall leadership skills than participants who scored low in these areas. When looking at specific aspects of emotional and successful intelligence, practical abilities and management of emotions were especially strong predictors of leadership skills. These findings suggest that the abilities to apply intellectual skills in everyday situations and to manage and regulate emotions are important leadership qualities. No significant age or gender differences were found. Overview During the first two decades of life, the human body continuously and dramatically changes, a process regulated and controlled by a number of biological and environmental factors. Heredity, nutrition, infectious disease, and parental affection all contribute to physical growth and overall health. Stimulation of the brain is vital during periods in which it is growing most rapidly in order to enhance cognitive development. Both Piaget and Vygotsky have created theories centered around cognitive development that have impacted our view of the way children acquire, comprehend, and apply knowledge. As children grow older, puberty causes their bodies to drastically change, which is also accompanied by psychological changes, as teenagers strive to adjust to what is happening in their bodies. Information-processing research seeks to understand how children develop the attention, memory, and self-management skills to succeed with complex tasks. Those who study this approach compare the human mind to a computer, or an intricate, symbol-manipulating system through which information flows. Attention to task is essential to thinking because it helps an individual determine which information
  • 16. needs to be considered. Development of attentional strategies occurs in phases and, over time, children gain an increased capacity for planning. As the ability to sustain attention grows, memory also improves, and the implementation of memory strategies increases a child’s likelihood of transferring information from the working memory to the long-term memory. Children also develop metacognition, which is another form of knowledge that influences how well children remember and solve problems. Fundamental discoveries about information processing have been applied to children’s mastery of academic skills, particularly in the areas of reading and mathematics. Identifying differences in cognitive skills between weak and strong learners can lead to strategies and interventions to increase performance. In addition, intelligence tests are helpful in identifying highly gifted children and diagnosing learning problems. The use of various types of intelligence testing has led to specific educational programs for diverse groups of students. Key Terms ADAPTATION COGNITION COGNITIVE-DEVELOPMENTAL THEORY CONSTRUCTIVIST APPROACH CORE KNOWLEDGE PERSPECTIVE DEFERRED IMITATION EGOCENTRISM MENTAL REPRESENTATION INTENTIONAL (GOAL DIRECTED) BEHAVIOR OPERATIONS ORGANIZATION SCAFFOLDING SCHEMES ZONE OF PROXIMAL DEVELOPMENT ACHIEVEMENT TEST APTITUDE TEST COMPONENTIAL ANALYSIS
  • 17. CREATIVITY DYNAMIC ASSESSMENT FACTOR ANALYSIS GIFTED INTELLIGENCE QUOTIENT (IQ) THEORY OF MULTIPLE INTELLIGENCES Sources Brainerd, C.J., Forrest, T.J., Karibian, D., & Reyna, V. F. (2006). Development of the false-memory illusion. Developmental Psychology, 42, 962–979. Breslau, N., Dickens, W. T., Flynn, J. R., Peterson, E. L., & Lucia, V.C. (2006). Low birth weight and social disadvantage: Tracking their relationship with children’s IQ during the period of school attendance. Intelligence, 34, 351–362. Chan, D. W. (2007). Leadership and intelligence. Roeper Review, 29, 183–189. Friedman, N. P., Haberstick, B. C., Willcutt, E. G., Miyake, A., Young, S. E., Corley, R. P., & Hewitt, J. K. (2007). Greater attention problems during childhood predict poorer executive functioning in late adolescence. Psychological Science, 18, 893–900. All graphics are public domain images or stock images from 123RF. Play from Cambridge Encyclopedia of Child Development Introduction This entry reviews the main different types of play, and the kinds of developmental benefits they may bring to children. The ubiquity of play in childhood (and in most species of mammals when young) strongly suggests its benefits for development, but what these benefits are, and how important or essential they are, are still debated. Classic perspectives on the development and function of play can be found in the writings of Piaget and Vygotsky. Let us begin by tackling the issue of what constitutes play, and then turn to how it undergoes age-related changes. Defining play
  • 18. Play is often defined as activity that is both done for its own sake, and characterized by ‘ means rather than ends’ (i.e., the process of the play is more important than any end point or goal). These criteria contrast play with, for example, exploration (which may lead into play as a child gets more familiar with a new toy or environment), with work (which has a definite goal), and fighting (different from play fighting as discussed later). Additional characteristics of play are flexibility (objects being put in new combinations, roles acted out in new ways), positive affect (children often smile and laugh in play, and say they enjoy it), and pretence (use of objects and actions in non-literal ways). Main types of play Although classifications differ, the following main types of play are well recognized: object play, pretend play and sociodramatic play, and physical activity play (exercise play; rough-and- tumble play). Of these, object play and physical activity play are seen widely in other species of mammals. Pretend and sociodramatic play are only seen in humans, apart from some possibly very elementary forms of pretence in great apes. Besides play, there is the related concept of games. Games with rules are more organized forms of play in which there is some goal (e.g., winning the game) and are not reviewed further. Object play This starts in infancy and may help children develop creative problem-solving skills. Researchers such as Jerome Bruner and Kathy Sylva have reported experiments with children in which they are given a chance to play with objects, then solve a task. Those with the play experience solved the task better. However, subsequent research has suggested that instruction can often be equally effective (Johnson, Christie, & Yawkey, 1999). The benefits of play need to be balanced against those of instruction, bearing in mind the ages of the children, the nature of the task, and the specificity of the learning expected - whether for specific skills or a more generally inquisitive and creative attitude.
  • 19. Pretend play This develops from about 15 months, with simple actions such as ‘ pretending to sleep’ or ‘ putting dolly to bed,’ developing into longer story sequences and role play (Fig. 1). Much early pretend play can be with parents, and older siblings. In Western societies especially, it is common for parents to model or ‘ scaffold’ early pretend play actions. By 3 to 4 years, pretend play becomes common with same-age peers. Pretend play among children is seen very widely in different societies. It is often imitative of adult roles (e.g., in rural societies, children may play at ‘ herding cattle’ with stones and at ‘ pounding maize’ with sticks and pebbles). Such play might be considered as ‘ practice’ for the adult activities concerned. However, rather more ambitious developmental benefits for pretend play have been put forward. Leslie (1987) argued that pretend play is an early indicator of theory of mind abilities. In simple object substitution pretence, the knowledge or representation that ‘ this is a banana’ becomes ‘ this banana is a telephone.’ Correspondingly, in theory of mind, the representation that ‘ this is a banana’ is related to the representation that ‘ X believes that the banana is a telephone.’ Leslie argued that this similarity suggested that pretence might be very important in theory of mind acquisition. However, this early pretend play before 3 years is often very imitative, and it is not clear whether a young child who talks into a banana is actually having the cognitive representations that Leslie describes, or is simply imitating what older children or adults do. The nature of any relationship between pretend play and theory of mind is still disputed. Figure 1. Pretend play. Photograph by John Walmsley. Sociodramatic play Defined in terms of social play with others, sustained role taking, and a narrative line, this is something that children from about 3 years of age engage in a lot. Such play can be quite complex, involving an understanding of others’ intent and role,
  • 20. sophisticated language constructions, and the development of sometimes novel (sometimes less novel!) story lines. Smilansky (1968) suggested that sociodramatic play assists language development, cognitive development, creativity, and role taking. She also claimed that pretend and sociodramatic play were less frequent and less complex in disadvantaged children. This led her and others to develop play tutoring (intervention by an adult) to raise levels of these kinds of play; adults would provide suitable props, visits, etc. and encourage the sociodramatic play of children in nurseries and kindergartens, such that subsequently they became more able to sustain this play themselves. Smilansky's ideas about the value of sociodramatic play were tested by a number of experimental studies, including play- tutoring studies. In these, a group or class of children that received play tutoring were compared with those who did not. Generally, the play-tutored children improved more on measures of cognition, language, and creativity, apparently supporting Smilansky's views. A number of critiques were made of these studies. Many of them pointed to flaws due to selective interpretation of results, effects of experimental bias, and the use of inappropriate control groups. For example, in the traditional play-tutoring study, the play-tutored children received more stimulation and adult contact generally, so one cannot really conclude that it is the extra play that brought about the developmental benefits. Further studies took account of these criticisms. This step included balancing play-tutoring with skills tutoring (e.g., coloring, picture dominoes) and assessing outcomes blind to the child's treatment condition. Doing so failed to reveal many differences (P. K. Smith, 1988), which suggests that benefits of socio drama tic play need not be essential for development. Nevertheless, play-tutoring does work out as equal to skills tutoring in many domains, and it is generally enjoyable and sociable for children in the preschool years, so there are sound reasons to encourage it in the nursery curriculum.
  • 21. Table 1. Some criteria distiguishing play fighting and real fighting. Criterion Play fighting Real fighting Facial expression Smiling, laughing Frowning, tearful Restraint Kicks and blows are not hard or do not make contact Kicks and blows are hard or make contact Role-reversal Voluntarily take it in turns to be ‘ on top’ or be ‘ chased' Aims to be ‘ on top’ or to chase the other How encounter starts and finishes Starting by invitation and ending with continued play or activity together Starting with challenge and ending in separation One kind of pretend play, often not encouraged in nurseries, is war play, which is pretend play with toy guns or weapons, or military action figures. Many educators believe that this play encourages real aggression, though others emphasize its pretend nature and feel that no real harm results from it. Physical activity play This refers to playful activity involving large body activity, particularly exercise play that includes running, climbing, and other large body or large muscle activity, as well as rough-and- tumble play, that covers play fighting and play chasing. These forms of play have been reviewed by Pellegrini & Smith (1998). Exercise play This increases in frequency from toddlers to preschool children, peaks at early primary school ages, and then declines. Young children seem to need opportunities for physical exercise more than older children, and are more likely to get restless after long sedentary periods and to run around when released from them. Boys do more of this kind of play than girls. It is often
  • 22. hypothesized to support physical training of muscles, for strength and endurance, and skill and economy of movement. Another hypothesis is that exercise play encourages younger children to take breaks from being overloaded on cognitive tasks (the cognitive immaturity hypothesis). The argument here is that younger children have less mature cognitive capacities, so benefits of concentrating on a cognitively demanding task decrease after a shorter time than for older children. The ‘ need’ to exercise thus helps children ‘ space out’ these cognitive demands. Figure 2. Rough-and-tumble play. Photograph by John Walmsley. Rough-and-tumble play This seems to increase from toddlers through preschool and primary school children, to peak at late primary age, and then decline in frequency. It takes up some 10 percent of playground time, though varying by the nature of the surface, physical conditions, etc. Boys do more than girls, especially play fighting. Rough-and-tumble play looks like real fighting, but can be distinguished from it by several criteria (Table 1; Fig. 2). Most children can distinguish playful from real fighting, and from 8 years give similar cues to those described in Table 1. In one study, English and Italian children were found to be accurate in judging videotapes of play fighting and real fighting, irrespective of which nationality they were watching. During the primary school years, only about 1% of rough-and- tumble episodes usually turn into real fighting, although many teachers and lunchtime supervisors think it is as much as about 30%. However, ‘ rejected’ children (those disliked by many peers and seldom liked much) more often respond to rough-and- tumble aggressively (around 25% of episodes). So, it is possible that teachers or lunchtime supervisors are making general judgments about children, based on these ‘ rejected’ children who may be taking up a lot of their supervisory time.
  • 23. Rough-and-tumble is often between friends. By early adolescence, however, there appears to be some change, with dominance/status becoming important in choosing play partners, as well as friendship, with a greater risk of play fights turning into real fights. It is hypothesized that rough-and-tumble play in younger children may (in addition to benefits of exercise play) provide practice in fighting/hunting skills, at least in earlier human societies. By adolescence, however, it may involve dominance relationships (e.g., using rough-and-tumble play to establish or maintain dominance in the peer group). What do children learn from play? Evolutionary arguments suggest that the propensity to play has been selected for, so we can expect there to be benefits to playing, and that these may vary by species, and by types of play. There can be a lot of incidental benefits to play such that it keeps children active and provides them with opportunities to encounter new situations. With human children, and with object, pretend, and sociodramatic play, there may be a balance to draw between benefits of playing and of instruction. Instruction can be more focused on a precise goal, but play is often more enjoyable for young children and, even if less efficient for a precise goal, may foster a more generally inquisitive and creative approach to problem-solving. Conclusions Among the theoretical issues in play research remaining unresolved, two are currently especially noteworthy. The first relates to rough-and-tumble play. We know that this is primarily friendly and non-exploitative in preadolescents, but how does this change as children move into adolescence? Does the function of this form of play then change and, in particular, is it used for purposes related to dominance, especially for boys? The second issue relates to pretend play. An earlier phase of research queried the findings from play tutoring studies, but, more recently, pretend play has been proposed as an important component of developing a theory of mind. Greater conceptual clarity and empirical evidence are called for here, together with
  • 24. a willingness to learn from the problems encountered in the earlier studies (e.g., experimenter bias). Amongst practical issues, the issue of war play continues to be debated in early education. There have been moves to ban war play in many nursery schools; however, there is also a recognition that such play may be generally harmless in itself and a rather natural play format, especially for boys (Holland, 2003). Regarding educational practice through the school years, there has been a general movement toward shortening or eliminating playground breaks. However, leaving aside social benefits of playtime, the benefits for physical activity and for providing breaks between instruction (cf., the cognitive immaturity hypothesis), argue for retaining playground breaks. More systematic study is still needed in these areas. Questions 1. What value does physical activity play have in providing spacing for concentration on school-based tasks? 2. How does rough-and-tumble play change in form and function from the primary to secondary school ages? 3. Does young children's war play have any connection with later aggressiveness? 4. Does pretend play have any important role in helping young children develop a theory of mind? See also · Cross-cultural comparisons · Cross-species comparisons · Theories of the child's mind · Social development · Aggressive and prosocial behavior · Peers and siblings · Sex differences · Anthropology · Jeromes s. Bruner · Jean Piaget · Lev S. Vygotsky · Donald Winnicott.
  • 25. Further reading · Lancy, D. F. 1996 Playingon the Mother-Ground: Cultural Routines for Children's Development New York: Guilford Press . · Pellegrini, A. D. 2002 Rough-and-tumble play. P.K. Smith, C. Hart, Handbook of Social Development Oxford: Blackwell, pp. 438-453 . · Power, T. 2000 Play and Exploration in Children and Animals NJ: Erlbaum . PETER K. SMITH © Cambridge University Press 2005 · APA · Chicago · Harvard · MLA Smith, P. K., & SMITH, P. K. (2005). Play. In B. Hopkins (Ed.), Cambridge encyclopedia of child development. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press. Retrieved from http://ezproxy.apus.edu/login?url=http://search.credoreference.c om/content/entry/cupchilddev/play/0?institutionId=8703 Top of Form Search Bottom of Form Browse Book Previous ArticleNext Article Cambridge Encyclopedia of Child Development An authoritative, accessible and up-to-date account of all aspects of child development. Covers everything from prenatal development to education, pediatrics, neuroscience, theories and research methods to physical development, social development, cognitive development, psychopathology and parenting. It also
  • 26. looks at cultural issues, sex differences and the history of child development. Editor(s): Brian Hopkins Edition: 1st Articles: 113 Images: 277 People: 21 Search other sites · Academic Search Premier · Business Source Complete · Ebrary · EBSCOhost · Gale Virtual Reference Library · IEEE · JSTOR · Lexis Nexis Academic · Proquest Databases · Safari · Summon Terms of usePrivacy policyContactAbout Credo ZONE OF PROXIMAL DEVELOPMENT and: CULTURAL TOOLS SCAFFOLDING GUIDED PARTICIPATION in SOCIAL COGNITION from Key Concepts in Developmental Psychology MEANING Although Vygotsky devoted only limited space to the zone of proximal (i.e. next) development in his description of socio- cognitive theory, this concept has become the best-known part of his account. The ZPD (as it is always referred to) expresses Vygotsky's belief that children's cognitive development occurs essentially as a result of interacting with more knowledgeable and competent others, who are willing to provide guidance and support in problem solving situations and will sensitively adjust their help in such a way that the child is challenged to participate in activities just beyond his or her current level of understanding. The ZPD is thus –
  • 27. the region between what children already know and what they are capable of learning under guidance. It is in this region that children are most receptive to new learning; it is there that any new intellectual skill is first of all performed jointly with a competent adult before it is in due course taken over by the child and internalized. It is the region where children are not quite capable of managing on their own but where the adult can stretch their abilities by suitably pacing demands so that the child can gradually assume responsibility for performing the task in a solo capacity. The ZPD is an expression of the basic proposition that Vygotsky set out to convey, namely that cognitive development is not a process that occurs spontaneously nor can it merely be explained by the child's interaction with the physical environment. It occurs because children are embedded in a social context, surrounded by people of greater expertise willing to share their knowledge with the child. Cognitive development can thus be seen as a progression from intermental to intramental, from joint regulation to self- regulation. The adult acts as tutor, the child as apprentice, but the interaction of the two is of a dynamic, mutually adjustive nature, for the child is no mere passive participant but an active partner in the learning process, albeit a junior one. Cognition is socially created, and the ZPD is a means of bringing this about. According to Vygotsky, the ZPD has one further use, namely in the assessment of intelligence. Vygotsky was convinced that children's potential is best demonstrated when working with a more competent person than when working on their own. Such an assertion goes, of course, directly counter to the generally accepted view, as seen in psychometric and other assessment procedures, that children's true capacities can only be revealed by tests administered to them in isolation. However, Vygotsky argued that children's ability to profit from help can tell us more about their eventual capacities than their efforts at unsupported problem solving. As he put it, it is in the ZPD that the 'buds of development' are to be found rather than the 'fruits',
  • 28. and it is the former that he considered to be of greater diagnostic value with respect to an individual's future progress. ORIGINS Although Vygotsky became aware of Piaget's writings, he did not formulate his ideas in direct opposition to them – as an effort, that is, to correct the latter's view of children as lone learners. Rather, Vygotsky (1896–1934) was very much a child of his time and place – a Russian steeped in Marxian theory who saw human nature as a socio-cultural product and childhood as the time when the accumulated wisdom of previous generations is handed on to the new generation. The ZPD was conceived by him as the primary setting in which such handing on is accomplished; the significance of these encounters, therefore, extends beyond the cognitive benefits derived by individual children: they also play an essential role in bringing the child in contact with the culture, the continuity of which is thereby ensured. To explain this process Vygotsky used the concept of CULTURAL TOOLS, these being – the psychological and technological devices perfected in the course of each society's history for the purpose of supporting and extending our understanding of the world. Psychological tools include language, writing, counting systems and scientific theories; among technological tools are books, clocks, calendars, calculators and computers. All these serve to channel our thinking in particular culturally valued directions: clocks and calendars, for example, ensure that we organize our ideas in a time dimension, the importance of which we set out to convey to children from an early age. However, by far the most important cultural tool is language, for it is the preeminent means of passing on society's experience. Children first encounter language as an integral part of their social exchanges with their caretakers, who sensitively (though usually quite unconsciously) adapt both the complexity and the content of their speech to the child's ability to comprehend, thus facilitating the eventual acquisition of language in children themselves (see motherese). Language thus
  • 29. begins in a social setting and, according to Vygotsky, is also initially used solely for social purposes, that is, to influence the behaviour of others. Eventually its function becomes extended: private speech (or what Piaget called egocentric speech) appears, whereby children talk to themselves in order to regulate their own behaviour, to be replaced subsequently by internal speech which forms the beginning of thought. Thus, what was in the first place behaviour that took place between child and others is transformed in the course of development to an internalized activity: the intermental becomes the intramental; an essentially social function evolves into the principal tool for cognitive functioning. Vygotsky did not produce any fully fledged theory, nor did he leave behind a coherent body of research, and it was not until many years after his early death that his two major books, Thought and language (1962) and Mind in society (1978), were translated into English and began to attract international attention. Even in his own country his work was neglected when his writings were suppressed during the Stalinist purges – an ironic fate to overcome a convinced Marxist who firmly believed that human behaviour is moulded by social organization and that the historical forces shaping our society need to be taken into account if we are to understand how children's development takes place. Indeed his aim was not merely to throw light on the nature of individual development; he was also convinced that by understanding the processes responsible for children becoming adults one can help to create a better socialist society. His vision was thus much wider than that of any other child psychologist: it extended beyond psychology to history, sociology, politics, economics, education and linguistics, all of which he regarded as relevant to the study of individual development. No wonder his theory is variously characterized as socio-cultural, socio-historical and socio- cognitive. CURRENT STATUS In the past few decades Vygotsky's writings have attracted
  • 30. enormous attention, and most of all serious consideration has been given to his belief that we need to think of the child-in- context as the basic unit rather than the child-in-isolation (see context). He is also responsible for stimulating a considerable amount of research on topics such as the analysis of parent–child joint problem solving (e.g. Wood, Bruner & Ross, 1976), peer tutoring (e.g. Foot & Howe, 1998), the role of private (egocentric) speech (e.g. Bivens & Berk, 1990) and cross-cultural comparisons of socialization and training practices (e.g. Rogoff, Mistry, Goncu & Mosier, 1993). The notion of the ZPD in particular has been the starting point for a lot of further work. However, Vygotsky's conception of the ZPD has also been seriously criticized. Much of this has been directed at the following features: · Vagueness regarding processes. The ZPD has been condemned for using a unidimensional concept to represent a multifaceted phenomenon (Paris & Cross, 1988). It does not specify the many and diverse processes that socially supported learning involves, nor does it provide a guide to the variations in zones that one can expect across different domains, settings and teachers. · The failure to consider developmental aspects. Vygotsky conceived of a prototype child, who functions in the ZPD in the same way at the age of 2 as at the age of 12. The roles of adult and child remain fixed; the appearance of new motives, needs and abilities are neglected, as are children's changing definitions of what constitutes their social context. · The disregard of children's individuality. Although Vygotsky stressed the active part played by the child in the ZPD, he paid more attention to the adult's contribution. At any one age children differ in what they bring to a social interchange, but Vygotsky had nothing to say about individual differences in such aspects as learning style, motivation and emotional regulation, nor about the various relationship qualities that distinguish different adult–child dyads. · Vagueness concerning the precise means whereby learning is
  • 31. produced. Just what are the processes taking place between child and partner that result in children taking over responsibility for more and more parts of the task and inernalizing them as skills of their own? Vygotsky did not go into detail; it has been left to others to pursue this line of enquiry. One attempt to provide an answer to this last question is built around the concept of SCAFFOLDING, first put forward by Wood, Bruner and Ross (1976). Scaffolding is – the process whereby a more expert partner offers help to a child in problem solving by adjusting both the amount and kind of help to the child's level of performance. In order to determine precisely what adults actually do in a joint task in order to help children become independent problem solvers, Wood and his colleagues observed the teaching techniques adopted by mothers when their 3- to 4-year-old children were confronted by a construction task that they could not initially cope with on their own. They observed a great range of actions employed by the mothers to keep the children on task and simplify the problem to an appropriate degree, but their use appeared to follow two rules: first, when a child is struggling the tutor should immediately offer more help; second, when the child is succeeding the tutor should give less help and fade into the background. By offering support that is always contingent on what the child is achieving, the child is given considerable autonomy and yet also has the opportunity at every step of relying on assistance, this diminishing as the child increasingly takes over responsibility for completing the task. A scaffold is thus put in place, but used in a far more flexible manner than this metaphor might suggest, in that the two contingency rules mean that the adult's behaviour is constantly modified in the light of what the child is achieving, thus leading the child step by step to eventual success. Such a notion of scaffolding certainly expresses the essence of what Vygotsky had in mind for the ZPD, and has been used in a large number of studies investigating a range of problem
  • 32. solving tasks. Yet this concept too is not without its critics (e.g. Stone, 1993), mainly because it pays insufficient attention to the communicative processes taking place in the adult–child interaction; also because it does not allow for the fact that the effectiveness of adults' actions is dependent on the particular relationship they have with the child. Another approach, that based on the concept of GUIDED PARTICIPATION, has attempted to correct these deficiencies. Guided participation is a term proposed by Barbara Rogoff (1990, 2003) as one of the mechanisms employed to advance a socio-cultural view of human development. Children, that is, become immersed in the practices of their particular culture from the moment of birth onwards; everything they experience transmits to them the accumulated beliefs and values of their society, and the main setting in which this occurs is in the child's exchanges with its caretakers, teachers and other concerned adults. Thus, as Rogoff (1998) put it, 'Cognitive development occurs as new generations collaborate with older generations in varying forms of interpersonal engagement and institutional practices.' Guided participation may therefore be defined as – the process by which children develop through their involvement in the practices of their community. By its use Rogoff wanted to emphasize that adult and child play essentially complementary roles in the latter's development, namely guidance by the adult and participation by the child. On the one hand the adult acts as guide to the culturally valued practices the child is expected to adopt – a role that can be played in many ways, some didactic as in structured teaching situations (which Vygotsky mainly concentrated on), others informal as when the child is given the opportunity to observe and join an adult in performing some activity. On the other hand the child acts as apprentice – not merely as passive bystander, that is, but as an active contributor to the activity that is the joint focus of the partner's attention. Thus guided participation is not some particular method of support for learning, nor does
  • 33. it depend on some conscious, previously formulated goal. A lot of joint teaching-learning is of an informal nature, and Rogoff uses a wealth of observations from different cultures to illustrate its role in enabling children to participate in the activities of their society and to advance from their present to a more advanced level of understanding. The general theme conveyed by Vygotsky, Rogoff and other socio-cultural theorists, as signalled by the concepts they employ such as the ZPD and guided participation, is that cognitive growth can only be understood if we acknowledge the social origins of mental processes and recognize that cognitive functions, especially in the early years, extend beyond the skin. This is in marked contrast to the customary individualistic assumption of psychologists – an assumption difficult to abandon despite the lipservice now paid to the importance of context, but well illustrated by the complete failure to take up Vygotsky's suggestion that intellectual assessment should take into account children's performance in joint sessions. Vygotsky was convinced that thinking is not just something that goes on inside an individual's head but is an activity that can be shared – indeed of necessity has to be shared in the early stages of development. As Rowe and Wertsch (2002) have put it, 'Study of the "I" is thus abandoned in favour of study of the social, cultural and historically situated ways by which "we" create "I's".' Further reading · Faulkner, D., Littleton, K., & Woodhead, M. (Eds.) (1998). Learning relationships in the classroom. London: Routledge. Written mainly from an educational point of view, this collection contains a lot of material relevant to the concepts described above. · Fernyhough, C. (1997). Vygotsky's sociocultural approach: theoretical issues and implications for current research. In Hala, S. (Ed.), The development of social cognition. Hove: Psychology Press. A concise account of the main themes in Vygotsky's theory.
  • 34. · Rogoff, B. (2003). The cultural nature of human development. Oxford: Oxford University Press. Gives a very detailed description not only of Rogoff's theoretical position but also of the research and thinking by other socio-cultural writers. See also constructivism; context; social constructivism © H. Rudolph Schaffer 2006 · APA · Chicago · Harvard · MLA ZONE OF PROXIMAL DEVELOPMENT and: CULTURAL TOOLS SCAFFOLDING GUIDED PARTICIPATION. (2006). In R. H. Schaffer, Key concepts in developmental psychology. London, UK: Sage UK. Retrieved from http://ezproxy.apus.edu/login?url=http://search.credoreference.c om/content/entry/sageukdp/zone_of_proximal_development_and _cultural_tools_scaffolding_guided_participation/0?institutionI d=8703 Top of Form Search Bottom of Form Browse Book Previous ArticleNext Article Key Concepts in Developmental Psychology This book serves as a guide to the principal concepts currently in use in Developmental Psychology and is perfect for courses in child development or developmental psychology. Author(s): Rudolph H. Schaffer Edition: 1st Articles: 66 Images: 1 People: 49 Related Credo Articles Full text Article
  • 35. Vygotsky, Lev in Science in the Early Twentieth Century: An Encyclopedia (b. Orsche, Russia 1896; d. USSR, 1934) Russian psychologist Lev Vygotsky helped to establish the science of child development. His work on... 566 words from ABC-CLIO Full text Article Introduction in Curriculum Connections Psychology: Cognitive Development Psychology forms part of the Curriculum Connections series. Each of the six volumes of the set covers a particular aspect of psychology: History of 416 words from Brown Bear Books Ltd Full text Article Reflections in Key Thinkers in Psychology While both Lev Vygotsky and Jean Piaget were concerned with the development of cognitive capacities, their approaches differed in... 264 words from Sage UK Full text Article Lev S., Vygotsky (1896–1934) in Cambridge Encyclopedia of Child Development Vygotsky was born in 1896 in Orsha (Belarus) to a middle-class Jewish family, the second of eight children. He grew up in Gomel... 719 words from Cambridge University Press Full text Article
  • 36. Lev Semionovich Vygotsky (1896-1934) in Key Thinkers in Psychology Suppressed on the orders of Stalin, the work of Vygotsky was almost unknown in the West until the 1960s. Its first appearance was in the now... 4011 words from Sage UK Search other sites · Academic Search Premier · Business Source Complete · Ebrary · EBSCOhost · Gale Virtual Reference Library · IEEE · JSTOR · Lexis Nexis Academic · Proquest Databases · Safari · Summon Cognitive Development (Piaget) and Intelligence The topic for this week is cognitive development and intelligence from the perspective of Piaget and Vygotsky. Additionally, we will learn definitions of intelligence, the predictive value of intelligence tes ts, variations in IQ, the role of early intervention in intellectual development, and the development of creativity. Topics to be covered include:
  • 37. · Cognitive Development: Piagetian, Core Knowledge, and Vygotskian Perspectives · Role of Intelligence Testing in the Development of Educational Programs · Case Studies Related to Intelligence Cognitive Development We will begin to examine cognitive development, or how the intellectual capabilities of infants transform into those of the child, adolescent, and adult. Fi rst, let us define cognition. Cognition refers to the inner processes and products of the mind that lead to “knowing.” In other words, how do we acquire, comprehend, and apply knowledge? What transformations must occur for individuals to develop increasing ly sophisticated mental capacities? JEAN PIAGET You have likely heard the name of Swiss cognitive theorist, Jean Piaget. According to Piaget, people are not cognitive beings at birth; instead, they discover, or construct, all knowledge of the
  • 38. world through their own experiences. As they begin to construct knowledge, they refine and organize the information in order to effectively adapt to their environments. This theory of active construction of knowledge is known as the constructivist approach to cognitive development. This approach follows children through four invariant (fixed order) and universal (assumed to characterize all children) stages: sensorimotor, preoperational, concrete operational, and formal operational. Throughout these stages, infants’ inv estigative behaviors gradually transform into the abstract, rational intelligence of more mature individuals. PIAGET'S STAGES OF C OGNITIVE DEVELOPMENT SENSORIMOTOR PREOPERATIONAL CONCRETE OPERATIONAL FORMAL OPERATIONAL Cognitive Development (Piaget) and Intelligence The topic for this week is cognitive development and intelligence from the perspective of Piaget and Vygotsky. Additionally, we will learn definitions of intelligence, the predictive value of intelligence tests, variations in IQ, the role of early intervention
  • 39. in intellectual development, and the development of creativity. Topics to be covered include: Vygotskian Perspectives Educational Programs se Studies Related to Intelligence Cognitive Development We will begin to examine cognitive development, or how the intellectual capabilities of infants transform into those of the child, adolescent, and adult. First, let us define cognition. Cognition refers to the inner processes and products of the mind that lead to “knowing.” In other words, how do we acquire, comprehend, and apply knowledge? What transformations must occur for individuals to develop increasingly sophisticated mental capacities? JEAN PIAGET You have likely heard the name of Swiss cognitive theorist, Jean Piaget. According to Piaget, people are not cognitive beings at birth; instead, they discover, or construct, all knowledge of the world through their own experiences. As they begin to construct knowledge, they refine and organize the information in order to effectively adapt to their environments. This theory of active construction of knowledge is known as the constructivist approach to cognitive development. This approach follows children through four invariant (fixed order) and universal (assumed to characterize all children) stages: sensorimotor, preoperational, concrete operational, and formal
  • 40. operational. Throughout these stages, infants’ investigative behaviors gradually transform into the abstract, rational intelligence of more mature individuals. PIAGET'S STAGES OF COGNITIVE DEVELOPMENT SENSORIMOTOR PREOPERATIONAL CONCRETE OPERATIONAL FORMAL OPERATIONAL