SlideShare a Scribd company logo
1 of 26
Psychological Science
2014, Vol. 25(3) 817 –823
© The Author(s) 2014
Reprints and permissions:
sagepub.com/journalsPermissions.nav
DOI: 10.1177/0956797613515681
pss.sagepub.com
Research Report
Diagnosis of ailments based on tasting and smelling
bodily fluids goes back to ancient history. More recently,
several diseases have been reported to yield characteris-
tic odors, such as Scrofula (which smells like stale beer),
Typhoid fever (which smells like baked bread) and
Yellow fever (which smells like a butcher’s shop; Penn &
Potts, 1998). Today, the idea of medical diagnosis of
infectious diseases and other disorders through analysis
of volatile organic compounds from skin, breath, feces,
or urine by aid of electronic noses has refocused atten-
tion on these observations, and the results hold promise
for disease-specific volatile biomarkers to be of wide-
spread clinical use in the future (Shirasu & Touhara,
2011).
In rodents, a wide array of infections, ranging from
gastrointestinal nematodes to viruses, are known to alter
body odor. This alteration results in a lowered preference
for the infected individual during initial investigation
(Ehman & Scott, 2001, 2002; Kiesecker, Skelly, Beard, &
Preisser, 1999). Chemosensory-mediated avoidance of
sick conspecifics in animals is now well established
(Arakawa, Cruz, & Deak, 2011; Kavaliers & Colwell,
1995a, 1995b). To investigate whether the first line of
defense to microbes entails detectable chemosensory
sickness cues, researchers in animal studies have used
lipopolysaccharide (LPS) to activate the innate immune
515681PSSXXX10.1177/0956797613515681Olsson et al.Scent
of Disease
research-article2014
Corresponding Author:
Mats J. Olsson, Department of Clinical Neuroscience, Division
of
Psychology, Karolinska Institutet, Nobels väg 9, SE-17177
Stockholm,
Sweden
E-mail: [email protected]
The Scent of Disease: Human Body Odor
Contains an Early Chemosensory Cue of
Sickness
Mats J. Olsson1, Johan N. Lundström1,2,3, Bruce A.
Kimball2,4,
Amy R. Gordon1,2, Bianka Karshikoff1, Nishteman Hosseini1,
Kimmo Sorjonen1, Caroline Olgart Höglund1, Carmen Solares1,
Anne Soop1, John Axelsson1, and Mats Lekander1,5
1Department of Clinical Neuroscience, Karolinska Institutet;
2Monell Chemical Senses Center, Philadelphia,
Pennsylvania; 3Department of Psychology, University of
Pennsylvania; 4U.S. Department of Agriculture,
Animal and Plant Health Inspection Service, Wildlife Services,
National Wildlife Research Center,
Philadelphia, Pennsylvania; and 5Stress Research Institute,
Stockholm University
Abstract
Observational studies have suggested that with time, some
diseases result in a characteristic odor emanating from
different sources on the body of a sick individual.
Evolutionarily, however, it would be more advantageous if the
innate immune response were detectable by healthy individuals
as a first line of defense against infection by various
pathogens, to optimize avoidance of contagion. We activated the
innate immune system in healthy individuals by
injecting them with endotoxin (lipopolysaccharide). Within just
a few hours, endotoxin-exposed individuals had a
more aversive body odor relative to when they were exposed to
a placebo. Moreover, this effect was statistically
mediated by the individuals’ level of immune activation. This
chemosensory detection of the early innate immune
response in humans represents the first experimental evidence
that disease smells and supports the notion of a
“behavioral immune response” that protects healthy individuals
from sick ones by altering patterns of interpersonal
contact.
Keywords
olfactory perception, human body, health
Received 6/9/13; Revision accepted 10/27/13
at ARKANSAS STATE UNIV on January 29,
2015pss.sagepub.comDownloaded from
http://pss.sagepub.com/
818 Olsson et al.
system and an inflammatory response (Beutler, 2009;
Suffredini, Fantuzzi, Badolato, Oppenheim, & O’Grady,
1999). Indeed, in the rat, it has been shown that LPS
injection influences the body odor of an individual such
that other rats avoid contact (Arakawa, Blandino, & Deak,
2009; Dantzer, 2009). Therefore, the innate immune
response is also relevant to the investigation of olfactory
markers in humans.
With this background, we set out to—for the first
time—experimentally test the idea of a chemosensory
sickness cue in humans. We hypothesized that humans
are able to perceptually dissociate between healthy and
sick individuals’ body odors. Moreover, under the
assumption that this capacity has evolved to reduce con-
tamination risks, we hypothesized that it should be pres-
ent at an early stage of the sickness response. We tested
this hypothesis by comparing body-odor samples from
individuals following LPS treatment to samples from the
same individuals following saline treatment. Because
inflammatory cytokines are involved in sickness behavior
(Avitsur, Cohen, & Yirmiya, 1997) and are also implicated
in the expression of aversive odor cues in response to a
number of pathogens in animals (Dantzer, 2004), we
measured proinflammatory cytokines as key mediators of
these odor cues.
Method
Sampling of body odors
Eight healthy volunteers (7 men, 1 woman; mean age =
24 years, SD = 3.71) were recruited for donation of body
odor during two sessions. To be included in the study,
participants had to be between 18 and 45 years old, right-
handed, and nonsmoking and to neither be taking medi-
cation (including nonbarrier contraceptives for female
participants) nor have a history of drug abuse, chronic
pain, or psychiatric disorders. The eight donors took part
in two sessions, both conducted at 1 p.m. and separated
by 28 days, in which they received either LPS or saline
injections. Participants wore tight T-shirts to allow for
body-odor sampling. The sampling took place in a hos-
pital environment and lasted for a total of 4 hr. A physi-
cian supervised the donors (for details, see Body-Odor
Sampling in the Supplemental Material available online).
The study was approved by the regional ethical review
board in Stockholm. Written informed consent was
obtained from all participants.
Measures of sickness response
During the sampling period, tympanic temperature was
measured (using a Thermoscan PRO-1; Braun, Inc., San
Diego, CA) before injection and once every hour following
injection. Four hours after injection with LPS, participants’
body temperature had increased by about 1 °C (Fig. 1d).
Plasma samples were provided 0 hr (baseline), 1 hr, 1.5 hr,
2 hr, 3 hr, and 4 hr after injection; they were frozen at −70
°C and were later thawed for analysis with Millipore’s
MILLIPLEX MAP high-sensitivity human-cytokine kit
(Millipore, Billerica, MA) using Luminex xMAP methodol-
ogy (Luminex, Austin, TX). In response to LPS, clear rises
in levels of tumor necrosis factor-alpha (TNF-a), interleu-
kin-6 (IL-6), and IL-8, peaking between 1.5 and 2 hr after
injection, were observed, confirming an inflammatory
response to LPS (see Figs. 1a–1c).
Chemical assays
We conducted chemical assays of body-odor samples to
assess the relative abundance of potentially odorous (i.e.,
volatile) compounds with gas chromatography–mass
spectrometry (GC-MS; see GC-MS Analysis in the
Supplemental Material for details).
Participants
Forty participants (28 women, 12 men; mean age = 26.2
years, SD = 6.3) were recruited from the Karolinska
Institutet university campus to take part in the body-
odor-assessment portion of the study. To be included,
participants had to be nonsmokers and to have self-
reported good health and functional sense of smell.
Procedure
Using a double-blind, within-group experimental design,
we tested participants separately with 18 unique odor
stimuli (8 LPS body-odor samples, 8 placebo body-odor
samples, and 2 samples from unworn T shirts, which
served as controls) in squeeze bottles.
For each participant, the odor stimuli were first pre-
sented one at a time in a uniquely randomized order with
an intertrial interval of 30 s. After a short break of 1.5
min, the odor stimuli were presented a second time,
again in a uniquely randomized order. On each trial, the
participant could squeeze the bottle and smell the head-
space a maximum of two times to prevent sensory adap-
tation. After smelling the sample, they rated its perceived
intensity (using a scale from 0 to 7), pleasantness (using
a scale from −7 to 7), and health (using a scale from −4
to 4). Unique scales were used to avoid the potential
confound that participants would convert ratings from
one scale to another. The extremes of the scales were
referred to as “maximal experiences.” The value of 0 on
the pleasantness and health scales was referred to as
“neither pleasant nor unpleasant” and “neither healthy
nor sick,” respectively. We used a measure of perceived
intensity to assess whether there was a quantitative rather
than qualitative difference between sick and healthy
at ARKANSAS STATE UNIV on January 29,
2015pss.sagepub.comDownloaded from
http://pss.sagepub.com/
Scent of Disease 819
body odors, and we used a measure of pleasantness
because it is the primary dimension of the olfactory per-
ceptual space and is therefore at the base of olfactory
functioning (Khan et al., 2007).
Results and Discussion
Participants’ ratings of the perceived intensity, pleasant-
ness, and health of odor samples from the three experi-
mental conditions (LPS, placebo, and control; see Fig. 2)
were submitted to analyses. Control odors (unworn
T-shirts) were rated as smelling significantly less intense,
more pleasant, and healthier than the LPS and placebo
odors (worn T-shirts; see Additional Analyses of Ratings of
Control Shirts and Table S1 in the Supplemental Material),
which indicates that the body-odor-sampling technique
was adequate. Further analyses, therefore, were focused
on the difference between LPS and placebo body odors.
Linear mixed model analyses, using both donors and rat-
ers as statistical units (see Choice of Statistical Analysis of
Body-Odor Ratings in the Supplemental Material for
details) with Bonferroni-Holm correction for multiple test-
ing (Holm, 1979), showed that the LPS body odors smelled
significantly more unpleasant, d = 0.259, t(592) = 4.487,
a
0
25
50
75
100
0
25
50
75
100
TN
F-
α
L
ev
el
(p
g/
m
l)
Before 1 1.5 2 3 4
Time After Injection (hours)
b
0
200
400
600
Before 1 1.5 2 3 4
Time After Injection (hours)
c
Before 1 1.5 2 3 4
Time After Injection (hours)
d
36
37
38
Ty
m
pa
ni
c
B
od
y
Te
m
pe
ra
tu
re
(°
C
)
Before 1 2 3 4
Time After Injection (hours)
Placebo LPS
IL
-8
L
ev
el
(p
g/
m
l)
IL
-6
L
ev
el
(p
g/
m
l)
Fig. 1. Mean levels of proinflammatory cytokines (a) tumor
necrosis factor-alpha (TNF-a), (b) interleukin-6 (IL-6), and (c)
IL-8 and
(d) tympanic body temperature as a function of time after
injection and treatment (lipopolysaccharide, LPS, vs. placebo).
Error bars represent
standard errors.
at ARKANSAS STATE UNIV on January 29,
2015pss.sagepub.comDownloaded from
http://pss.sagepub.com/
820 Olsson et al.
p < .001, more intense, d = 0.212, t(592) = 4.423, p < .001,
and more unhealthy, d = 0.133, t(592) = 2.025, p = .043.
These results indicate that humans can indeed dissoci-
ate between the odors of sick and healthy individuals
within 4 hr of innate immune-system activation; there are
at least two possible reasons for this effect. The body
odors of sick and healthy people may differ in perceived
intensity, reflecting that the sick body emits more of the
same types of volatile and odorous substances as the
healthy body. The higher perceived intensity of the sick
body-odor samples supports such a “more-of-the-same”
model. The other possible, and more intriguing, explana-
tion for the LPS-induced changes in body odor is that the
pattern of substance concentrations emitted from the
body has changed and forms a cue of sickness, reflecting
the activation of the innate immune system. Such a quali-
tative shift, independent of overall odorant concentra-
tion, would be a more ecologically viable candidate to
modulate behavioral adaptations. The observation that a
sick individual’s body odor smells more unpleasant and
unhealthy supports such a “sickness-cue” model.
However, it is well established that odor pleasantness—
in addition to odor intensity—changes as a function of
odorant concentration (Doty, 1975). Whereas intensity
increases with odor concentration, pleasantness follows
a more complicated model: Pleasant odors tend to have
an optimum along the stimulus-concentration range, and
unpleasant odors simply get more unpleasant as the con-
centration increases (Lawless, 1977). Given that body
odors are on the unpleasant end of the olfactory contin-
uum (Fig. 2), we expect that an increase in body-odor
intensity would be accompanied by an increase in unpleas-
antness. Hence, a more-of-the-same model could also pre-
dict a shift in odor pleasantness.
To answer the question of whether or not there was a
treatment-related shift in body-odor pleasantness inde-
pendent of the changes in odor intensity, we performed
an analysis similar to that used to assess the effect of
body-odor type (LPS, placebo) on pleasantness ratings,
but we added odor intensity as a covariate. The results
showed a significant and separate effect of LPS treatment
on body-odor pleasantness, d = −0.118, t(597) = 2.424,
p = .016, and this effect could not be explained by differ-
ences in intensity. The same test of perceived health
using intensity as a covariate did not reveal a significant
separate effect of treatment, d = −0.046, t(598) = −0.730,
p = .465.
Moreover, to directly test the more-of-the-same model,
we analyzed the body-odor samples (using GC-MS) to
assess the concentration of odorous compounds. Because
the samples had been used in the behavioral test and
were therefore potentially depleted and contaminated,
the analysis was restricted to determining the overall
abundance of volatile compounds in the LPS and placebo
samples. The results indicated that the concentrations of
the LPS samples were, on average, lower than those in
placebo samples across all compounds identified, but
insignificantly so, d = 0.134, t(16) = 0.715, n.s. In other
words, LPS-treated participants did not seem to sweat
more, but rather less, than those who were placebo
treated. Thus, these results fail to support a more-of-the-
same model as the explanation of LPS-induced changes
in body odor and are consistent with the notion that dur-
ing a generalized sickness response, humans emit a
chemical cue. Dedicated studies should target these
chemicals in the future.
In order to link the change in body-odor composition
not only to the LPS treatment but also to the actual
Placebo LPS
0
1
2
3
4
In
te
ns
ity
a
***
Placebo LPS
–2.5
–2.0
–1.5
–1.0
–0.5
0.0
Pl
ea
sa
nt
ne
ss
b
Placebo LPS
–0.45
–0.60
–0.30
–0.15
0.00
H
ea
lth
c
***
*
Fig. 2. Mean ratings of the perceived (a) intensity, (b)
pleasantness, and (c) health of odors of T-shirts worn by
individuals exposed to lipo-
polysaccharide (LPS) and a placebo. Error bars show standard
errors. Asterisks indicate significant differences between
conditions (*p < .05;
***p < .001).
at ARKANSAS STATE UNIV on January 29,
2015pss.sagepub.comDownloaded from
http://pss.sagepub.com/
Scent of Disease 821
treatment-induced inflammatory response, we performed
mediation analyses of cytokines. Cytokines IL-6 and
TNF-a, but not IL-8, significantly mediated the effect
of treatment on body-odor pleasantness and intensity
(Table 1). Taken together, these results strongly support
that humans emit a chemical cue during a generalized
sickness response that can be perceived by others.
According to participants’ verbal reports, rating body-
odor intensity and pleasantness was an easier task
than rating the perceived health of body-odor samples.
Moreover, the effect size of LPS treatment on health rat-
ings, albeit significant, was relatively small. In fact, there
was no significant effect of treatment on health ratings
when either pleasantness or intensity was controlled for;
thus, these results do not support a direct perception of
health status. Instead, health ratings may have been
based on inferences from the other aspects of the odor,
such as its pleasantness. It has also been suggested that
the emotion of disgust has evolved as a disease-avoid-
ance mechanism (Oaten, Stevenson, & Case, 2009). The
current results may be relevant to this hypothesis, in that
a disgust-driven, negative response promotes withdrawal
from and avoidance of a sick individual by healthy ones.
In concert with the social withdrawal exhibited by
infected individuals, such mechanisms are modeled
to be highly effective in containing an epidemic—
particularly if instigated soon after infection (Cole, 2006).
In this study, when participants rated body odors of
“sick” individuals, they found them significantly more
unpleasant than “healthy” body odors; future studies
should focus on the role of disgust in such responses to
“sick” body odor.
The exact nature of the cue or signal has yet to be
determined. For instance, the volatile substances in the
skin mediating the effect from an inflammatory response
to body odor need careful investigation. Moreover, with
regard to theory, there are two assumptions one can
make about a sickness odor: It could be viewed as either
a cue or a signal. As a cue, it would be inadvertent on
behalf of the sender and beneficial to the receiver. In line
with that notion, most animal studies have revealed
avoidance behavior in response to a sick body odor.
However, the avoidance behavior is typically seen in
response to body odors of unfamiliar conspecifics; in
contrast, increased maternal licking of LPS-treated rat
pups has also been reported (Breivik et al., 2002). This
pattern of results supports the idea that the sickness odor
can also be a signal beneficial to the sender. Some evi-
dence even suggests that, rather than being aversive, the
odor of infected males simply loses its attractiveness,
which suggests a reduced signal of health rather than an
increased signal of sickness (Kavaliers & Colwell, 1995a;
Penn, Schneider, White, Slev, & Potts, 1998). It is not yet
clear whether these changes are best characterized as a
cue of illness beneficial to recipients or simply as a
reduced signal of health from a sender whose resources
necessary for health-signal maintenance have been real-
located (Penn & Potts, 1998). It should also be noted that
the specificity of the type of olfactory cue indicated here
and in animal models remains to be determined.
Among the physiological adaptations that occur after
immune challenge is an activation of the hypothalamic-
pituitary-adrenal (HPA) axis (Maier, Watkins, & Nance,
2001), and consequently, increased cortisol can be seen
after LPS administration in humans (Grigoleit et al., 2011).
This overlap between sickness and the fight-flight
response is expected, given that mobilization and redi-
rection of energy is central to handling threats from both
within and without (Segerstrom & Miller, 2004). Future
studies should therefore investigate the extent to which
internal and external challenges result in similar olfactory
changes.
Table 1. Crude and Mediated Effects on Ratings of Body-Odor
Pleasantness,
Intensity, and Health
Effect and mediator Pleasantness Intensity Health
Crude effect
LPS –0.259 (0.058)*** 0.212 (0.048)*** –0.133 (0.066)*
Temperature –0.120 (0.034)*** 0.091 (0.029)** –0.041
(0.039)
IL-6 –0.181 (0.034)*** 0.145 (0.029)*** –0.080 (0.039)*
IL-8 –0.144 (0.033)*** 0.107 (0.027)*** –0.059 (0.037)
TNF-a –0.153 (0.031)*** 0.126 (0.026)*** –0.075 (0.035)*
Mediator of effects of LPS
IL-6 –0.149 (0.051)** 0.113 (0.043)* –0.049 (0.057)
IL-8 –0.070 (0.061) 0.017 (0.051) 0.013 (0.068)
TNF-a –0.154 (0.077)*a 0.127 (0.064)*,a –0.058 (0.085)
Note: Tests of the significance of mediation effects were
conducted using Sobel tests and were
based on MacKinnon and Fritz (2007). Standard deviations are
shown in parentheses. LPS =
lipopolysaccharide; IL-6 = interleukin-6; IL-8 = interleukin-8;
TNF-a = tumor necrosis factor-
alpha.
aSignificance does not withstand a Bonferroni-Holm correction
for multiple testing.
*p < .05. **p < .01. ***p < .001.
at ARKANSAS STATE UNIV on January 29,
2015pss.sagepub.comDownloaded from
http://pss.sagepub.com/
822 Olsson et al.
Altogether, the results of this experimental study sug-
gest that, akin to rodents, humans are able to detect a
social cue of sickness from body odor alone, which can be
used for avoidance of infected conspecifics. Moreover, this
social information can be triggered by the innate immune
response, which is observable just a few hours after innate
immune-system activation and is a general response to a
variety of pathogens. Human olfaction may thus prove to
be a signaling route to a “behavioral immune response”
(Breivik et al., 2002) that protects healthy individuals by
altering patterns of interpersonal contact and, possibly, by
heightening the immune-system response to infection in
the receiver, as has been shown for other disease-related
stimuli (Schaller & Park, 2011; Stevenson et al., 2012).
Author Contributions
M. J. Olsson, J. N. Lundström, B. Karshikoff, N. Hosseini, C.
Olgart Höglund, A. Soop, J. Axelsson, and M. Lekander
designed
the method for data collection; data collection was carried out
by A. R. Gordon, B. Karshikoff, C. Solares, N. Hosseini, and
B. A. Kimball under the supervision of M. J. Olsson. A. Soop
administered lipopolysaccharide. M. J. Olsson and K. Sorjonen
wrote the statistical-analysis plan and carried out the statistical
analyses. Data analyses were conducted by A. R. Gordon, C.
Solares, and B. A. Kimball, and J. Axelsson conducted GC-MS
analyses. M. J. Olsson, C. Olgart Höglund, and M. Lekander
obtained funding with help from J. Axelsson. The manuscript
was drafted by M. J. Olsson, N. Hosseini, and K. Sorjonen and
revised by J. N. Lundström, A. R. Gordon, B. Karshikoff, C.
Solares, N. Hosseini, K. Sorjonen, B. A. Kimball, J. Axelsson,
and M. Lekander. M. J. Olsson served as the study’s guarantor.
All authors approved the final version of the manuscript for
submission.
Acknowledgments
Thanks are due to Malin Schütze for technical assistance and to
Andreas Olsson for commenting on a previous version of the
manuscript.
Declaration of Conflicting Interests
The authors declared that they had no conflicts of interest with
respect to their authorship or the publication of this article.
Funding
This study was supported by the Swedish Research Council
(Grant 421-2012-1125) and the Bank of Sweden Tercentenary
Foundation (Grant P12-1017; M. J. Olsson); by the Knut and
Alice Wallenberg Foundation (Grant KAW 2012.0141; J. N.
Lundström); by the Osher Center for Integrative Medicine
(J. Axelsson); and by the Swedish Heart-Lung Foundation, the
Center for Allergy Research and the Petrus and Augusta
Hedlund’s Foundation, the Swedish Asthma and Allergy
Foundation, the Stockholm Stress Center, and the Swedish
Council for Working Life and Social Research (M. Lekander
and
C. Olgart Höglund).
Supplemental Material
Additional supporting information may be found at http://pss
.sagepub.com/content/by/supplemental-data
References
Arakawa, H., Blandino, P., Jr., & Deak, T. (2009). Central
infusion of interleukin-1 receptor antagonist blocks the
reduction in social behavior produced by prior stressor
exposure. Physiology & Behavior, 98, 139–146. doi:10.1016/j
.physbeh.2009.04.024
Arakawa, H., Cruz, S., & Deak, T. (2011). From models to
mech-
anisms: Odorant communication as a key determinant of
social behavior in rodents during illness-associated states.
Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 35, 1916–1928.
doi:10.1016/j.neubiorev.2011.03.007
Avitsur, R., Cohen, E., & Yirmiya, R. (1997). Effects of inter-
leukin-1 on sexual attractivity in a model of sickness
behavior. Physiology & Behavior, 63, 25–30. doi:10.1016/
S0031-9384(97)00381-8
Beutler, B. A. (2009). TLRs and innate immunity. Blood, 113,
1399–1407. doi:10.1182/blood-2008-07-019307
Breivik, T., Stephan, M., Brabant, G. E., Straub, R. H., Pabst,
R., & von Hörsten, S. (2002). Postnatal lipopolysaccha-
ride-induced illness predisposes to periodontal disease in
adulthood. Brain, Behavior, and Immunity, 16, 421–438.
doi:10.1006/brbi.2001.0642
Cole, S. W. (2006). The complexity of dynamic host networks.
In T. S. Deisboeck & J. Y. Kresh (Eds.), Complex systems sci-
ence in biomedicine (pp. 605–629). New York, NY: Kluwer
Academic. doi:10.1007/978-0-387-33532-2_26
Dantzer, R. (2004). Cytokine-induced sickness behaviour: A
neuroimmune response to activation of innate immu-
nity. European Journal of Pharmacology, 500, 399–411.
doi:10.1016/j.ejphar.2004.07.040
Dantzer, R. (2009). Cytokine, sickness behavior, and depres-
sion. Immunology and Allergy Clinics of North America, 29,
247–264. doi:10.1016/j.iac.2009.02.002
Doty, R. L. (1975). An examination of relationships between
the pleasantness, intensity, and concentration of 10 odor-
ous stimuli. Perception & Psychophysics, 17, 492–496.
doi:10.3758/BF03203300
Ehman, K. D., & Scott, M. E. (2001). Urinary odour preferences
of
MHC congenic female mice, Mus domesticus: Implications for
kin recognition and detection of parasitized males. Animal
Behaviour, 62, 781–789. doi:10.1006/anbe.2001.1805
Ehman, K. D., & Scott, M. E. (2002). Female mice mate pref-
erentially with non-parasitized males. Parasitology, 125,
461–466. doi:10.1017/S003118200200224X
Grigoleit, J. S., Kullmann, J. S., Wolf, O. T., Hammes, F.,
Wegner, A., Jablonowski, S., . . . Schedlowski, M. (2011).
Dose-dependent effects of endotoxin on neurobehavioral
functions in humans. PLoS ONE, 6, e28330. Retrieved from
http://www.plosone.org/article/info%3Adoi%2F10.1371
%2Fjournal.pone.0028330
Holm, S. (1979). A simple sequentially rejective multiple test
procedure. Scandinavian Journal of Statistics, 6, 65–70.
Retrieved from http://www.jstor.org/discover/10.2307/461
at ARKANSAS STATE UNIV on January 29,
2015pss.sagepub.comDownloaded from
http://www.plosone.org/article/info%3Adoi%2F10.1371%2Fjour
nal.pone.0028330
http://www.jstor.org/discover/10.2307/4615733?uid=3738984&;
uid=2134&uid=2129&uid=2&uid=70&uid=4&sid=21102598283
337
http://pss.sagepub.com/content/by/supplemental-data
http://pss.sagepub.com/
Scent of Disease 823
5733?uid=3738984&;uid=2134&uid=2129&uid=2&uid=70&u
id=4&sid=21102598283337
Kavaliers, M., & Colwell, D. D. (1995a). Discrimination
by female mice between the odours of parasitized
and non-parasitized males. Proceedings of the Royal
Society B: Biological Sciences, 261, 37–41. doi:10.1098/
rspb.1995.0113
Kavaliers, M., & Colwell, D. D. (1995b). Odours of parasit-
ized males induce aversive responses in female mice.
Animal Behaviour, 50, 1161–1169. doi:10.1016/0003-
3472(95)80032-8
Khan, R. M., Luk, C. H., Flinker, A., Aggarwal, A., Lapid, H.,
Haddad, R., & Sobel, N. (2007). Predicting odor pleasant-
ness from odorant structure: Pleasantness as a reflection
of the physical world. Journal of Neuroscience, 27, 10015–
10023. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1158-07.2007
Kiesecker, J. M., Skelly, D. K., Beard, K. H., & Preisser, E.
(1999). Behavioral reduction of infection risk. Proceedings
of the National Academy of Sciences, USA, 96, 9165–9168.
doi:10.1073/pnas.96.16.9165
Lawless, H. (1977). The pleasantness of mixtures in taste and
olfaction. Sensory Processes, 1, 227–237.
MacKinnon, D. P., Fritz, M. S., Williams, J., & Lockwood,
C. M. (2007). Distribution of the product confidence lim-
its for the indirect effect: Program PRODCLIN. Behavior
Research Methods, 39, 384–389. doi:10.3758/BF03193007
Maier, S. F., Watkins, L. R., & Nance, D. M. (2001). Multiple
routes
of action of Interleukin-1 on the nervous system. In R. Ader,
D. L. Felten, & L. Cohen (Eds.), Psychoneuroimmunology
(3rd ed., pp. 563–583). San Diego, CA: Academic Press.
Oaten, M., Stevenson, R. J., & Case, T. I. (2009). Disgust as a
disease-avoidance mechanism. Psychological Bulletin, 135,
303–321. doi:10.1037/a0014823
Penn, D., & Potts, W. K. (1998). Chemical signals and parasite-
mediated sexual selection. Trends in Ecology & Evolution,
13, 391–396. doi:10.1016/S0169-5347(98)01473-6
Penn, D., Schneider, G., White, K., Slev, P., & Potts, W.
(1998). Influenza infection neutralizes the attractiveness of
male odour to female mice (Mus musculus). Ethology, 104,
685–694. doi:10.1111/j.1439-0310.1998.tb00102.x
Schaller, M., & Park, J. H. (2011). The behavioral immune
system
(and why it matters). Current Directions in Psychological
Science, 20, 99–103. doi:10.1177/0963721411402596
Segerstrom, S. C., & Miller, G. E. (2004). Psychological stress
and the human immune system: A meta-analytic study of
30 years of inquiry. Psychological Bulletin, 130, 601–630.
doi:10.1037/0033-2909.130.4.601
Shirasu, M., & Touhara, K. (2011). The scent of disease:
Volatile
organic compounds of the human body related to dis-
ease and disorder. Journal of Biochemistry, 150, 257–266.
doi:10.1093/jb/mvr090
Stevenson, R. J., Hodgson, D., Oaten, M. J., Moussavi, M.,
Langberg, R., Case, T. I., & Barouei, J. (2012). Disgust
elevates core body temperature and up-regulates certain
oral immune markers. Brain, Behavior, and Immunity, 26,
1160–1168. doi:10.1016/j.bbi.2012.07.010
Suffredini, A. F., Fantuzzi, G., Badolato, R., Oppenheim, J. J.,
&
O’Grady, N. P. (1999). New insights into the biology of the
acute phase response. Journal of Clinical Immunology, 19,
203–214. doi:10.1023/A:1020563913045
at ARKANSAS STATE UNIV on January 29,
2015pss.sagepub.comDownloaded from
http://www.jstor.org/discover/10.2307/4615733?uid=3738984&;
uid=2134&uid=2129&uid=2&uid=70&uid=4&sid=21102598283
337
http://pss.sagepub.com/
Psychological Science2014, Vol. 25(3) 817 –823© The Author.docx

More Related Content

Similar to Psychological Science2014, Vol. 25(3) 817 –823© The Author.docx

Introduction to Epidemiology
Introduction to EpidemiologyIntroduction to Epidemiology
Introduction to EpidemiologyAya Mohamed
 
Occurrence of COPD in Patients with Respiratory Allergy: A Clinico-Spirometri...
Occurrence of COPD in Patients with Respiratory Allergy: A Clinico-Spirometri...Occurrence of COPD in Patients with Respiratory Allergy: A Clinico-Spirometri...
Occurrence of COPD in Patients with Respiratory Allergy: A Clinico-Spirometri...DR. SUJOY MUKHERJEE
 
PPT_1696715234394_6264250763664275160.pdf
PPT_1696715234394_6264250763664275160.pdfPPT_1696715234394_6264250763664275160.pdf
PPT_1696715234394_6264250763664275160.pdfAbdoELsaid2
 
Chapter 1Toxicology -1.ppt
Chapter 1Toxicology -1.pptChapter 1Toxicology -1.ppt
Chapter 1Toxicology -1.pptFerhanKadir
 
Neuroscience Research
Neuroscience ResearchNeuroscience Research
Neuroscience ResearchMia C. Harris
 
Experimental Design: The Smell of Fear
Experimental Design: The Smell of FearExperimental Design: The Smell of Fear
Experimental Design: The Smell of FearChris Willmott
 
HLTH 104 Chapter 03
HLTH 104 Chapter 03HLTH 104 Chapter 03
HLTH 104 Chapter 03misteraugie
 
A_Biopac_Analysis_of_Physiological_Responses_to_Disgusting_Stimuli
A_Biopac_Analysis_of_Physiological_Responses_to_Disgusting_StimuliA_Biopac_Analysis_of_Physiological_Responses_to_Disgusting_Stimuli
A_Biopac_Analysis_of_Physiological_Responses_to_Disgusting_StimuliAmber Anger
 
Experimental epidemiology
 Experimental epidemiology Experimental epidemiology
Experimental epidemiologyimprovemed
 
EXPERIMENTAL EPIDEMIOLOGY
EXPERIMENTAL EPIDEMIOLOGYEXPERIMENTAL EPIDEMIOLOGY
EXPERIMENTAL EPIDEMIOLOGYimprovemed
 
Forensic Toxicology-1.pptx
Forensic Toxicology-1.pptxForensic Toxicology-1.pptx
Forensic Toxicology-1.pptxMansoor883391
 
Animal Assisted Therapy Program Poster for MLA 2015 (4) April 24
Animal Assisted Therapy Program Poster for MLA 2015 (4) April 24Animal Assisted Therapy Program Poster for MLA 2015 (4) April 24
Animal Assisted Therapy Program Poster for MLA 2015 (4) April 24kzeblis
 
Epidemiology and preventive veterinary medicine.docx1
Epidemiology and preventive veterinary medicine.docx1Epidemiology and preventive veterinary medicine.docx1
Epidemiology and preventive veterinary medicine.docx1Arjun Chapagain
 
Honey with Coffee Calms Persistent Coughs
Honey with Coffee Calms Persistent CoughsHoney with Coffee Calms Persistent Coughs
Honey with Coffee Calms Persistent CoughsBee Healthy Farms
 
Epidemiology and Community Health
Epidemiology and Community HealthEpidemiology and Community Health
Epidemiology and Community HealthPushpaLatha186
 
Short-Term Effects of Repeated Olfactory Administration of Homeopathic Sulphu...
Short-Term Effects of Repeated Olfactory Administration of Homeopathic Sulphu...Short-Term Effects of Repeated Olfactory Administration of Homeopathic Sulphu...
Short-Term Effects of Repeated Olfactory Administration of Homeopathic Sulphu...Francisco Navarro
 

Similar to Psychological Science2014, Vol. 25(3) 817 –823© The Author.docx (20)

Introduction to Epidemiology
Introduction to EpidemiologyIntroduction to Epidemiology
Introduction to Epidemiology
 
Occurrence of COPD in Patients with Respiratory Allergy: A Clinico-Spirometri...
Occurrence of COPD in Patients with Respiratory Allergy: A Clinico-Spirometri...Occurrence of COPD in Patients with Respiratory Allergy: A Clinico-Spirometri...
Occurrence of COPD in Patients with Respiratory Allergy: A Clinico-Spirometri...
 
PPT_1696715234394_6264250763664275160.pdf
PPT_1696715234394_6264250763664275160.pdfPPT_1696715234394_6264250763664275160.pdf
PPT_1696715234394_6264250763664275160.pdf
 
Chapter 1Toxicology -1.ppt
Chapter 1Toxicology -1.pptChapter 1Toxicology -1.ppt
Chapter 1Toxicology -1.ppt
 
Neuroscience Research
Neuroscience ResearchNeuroscience Research
Neuroscience Research
 
Experimental Design: The Smell of Fear
Experimental Design: The Smell of FearExperimental Design: The Smell of Fear
Experimental Design: The Smell of Fear
 
HLTH 104 Chapter 03
HLTH 104 Chapter 03HLTH 104 Chapter 03
HLTH 104 Chapter 03
 
Animal research
Animal researchAnimal research
Animal research
 
A_Biopac_Analysis_of_Physiological_Responses_to_Disgusting_Stimuli
A_Biopac_Analysis_of_Physiological_Responses_to_Disgusting_StimuliA_Biopac_Analysis_of_Physiological_Responses_to_Disgusting_Stimuli
A_Biopac_Analysis_of_Physiological_Responses_to_Disgusting_Stimuli
 
Experimental epidemiology
 Experimental epidemiology Experimental epidemiology
Experimental epidemiology
 
EXPERIMENTAL EPIDEMIOLOGY
EXPERIMENTAL EPIDEMIOLOGYEXPERIMENTAL EPIDEMIOLOGY
EXPERIMENTAL EPIDEMIOLOGY
 
Forensic Toxicology-1.pptx
Forensic Toxicology-1.pptxForensic Toxicology-1.pptx
Forensic Toxicology-1.pptx
 
Animal Assisted Therapy Program Poster for MLA 2015 (4) April 24
Animal Assisted Therapy Program Poster for MLA 2015 (4) April 24Animal Assisted Therapy Program Poster for MLA 2015 (4) April 24
Animal Assisted Therapy Program Poster for MLA 2015 (4) April 24
 
Epidemiology Depuk sir_ 1,2,3 chapter,OK
Epidemiology Depuk sir_ 1,2,3 chapter,OKEpidemiology Depuk sir_ 1,2,3 chapter,OK
Epidemiology Depuk sir_ 1,2,3 chapter,OK
 
Epidemiology and preventive veterinary medicine.docx1
Epidemiology and preventive veterinary medicine.docx1Epidemiology and preventive veterinary medicine.docx1
Epidemiology and preventive veterinary medicine.docx1
 
Honey with Coffee Calms Persistent Coughs
Honey with Coffee Calms Persistent CoughsHoney with Coffee Calms Persistent Coughs
Honey with Coffee Calms Persistent Coughs
 
Epidemiology and Community Health
Epidemiology and Community HealthEpidemiology and Community Health
Epidemiology and Community Health
 
Casecontrolstudy
CasecontrolstudyCasecontrolstudy
Casecontrolstudy
 
Short-Term Effects of Repeated Olfactory Administration of Homeopathic Sulphu...
Short-Term Effects of Repeated Olfactory Administration of Homeopathic Sulphu...Short-Term Effects of Repeated Olfactory Administration of Homeopathic Sulphu...
Short-Term Effects of Repeated Olfactory Administration of Homeopathic Sulphu...
 
Animal toxicity studies
Animal toxicity studiesAnimal toxicity studies
Animal toxicity studies
 

More from potmanandrea

Select a foreign country and analyze its monetary system. Formatted .docx
Select a foreign country and analyze its monetary system. Formatted .docxSelect a foreign country and analyze its monetary system. Formatted .docx
Select a foreign country and analyze its monetary system. Formatted .docxpotmanandrea
 
Select a company of your choice. Study the company’ CEO and determ.docx
Select a company of your choice. Study the company’ CEO and determ.docxSelect a company of your choice. Study the company’ CEO and determ.docx
Select a company of your choice. Study the company’ CEO and determ.docxpotmanandrea
 
Seeing” Social MovementsAn important component of studying social.docx
Seeing” Social MovementsAn important component of studying social.docxSeeing” Social MovementsAn important component of studying social.docx
Seeing” Social MovementsAn important component of studying social.docxpotmanandrea
 
Seeking someone that is great at writing papers!  Must be familar .docx
Seeking someone that is great at writing papers!  Must be familar .docxSeeking someone that is great at writing papers!  Must be familar .docx
Seeking someone that is great at writing papers!  Must be familar .docxpotmanandrea
 
See comments at the right of the data set..docx
See comments at the right of the data set..docxSee comments at the right of the data set..docx
See comments at the right of the data set..docxpotmanandrea
 
Select a company you are familiar with. Imagine that this company is.docx
Select a company you are familiar with. Imagine that this company is.docxSelect a company you are familiar with. Imagine that this company is.docx
Select a company you are familiar with. Imagine that this company is.docxpotmanandrea
 
Select a contemporary issue that is of interest to you.Create .docx
Select a contemporary issue that is of interest to you.Create .docxSelect a contemporary issue that is of interest to you.Create .docx
Select a contemporary issue that is of interest to you.Create .docxpotmanandrea
 
see the attached filesThe QuestionIs management in the publ.docx
see the attached filesThe QuestionIs management in the publ.docxsee the attached filesThe QuestionIs management in the publ.docx
see the attached filesThe QuestionIs management in the publ.docxpotmanandrea
 
See discussions, stats, and author profiles for this publicati.docx
See discussions, stats, and author profiles for this publicati.docxSee discussions, stats, and author profiles for this publicati.docx
See discussions, stats, and author profiles for this publicati.docxpotmanandrea
 
See details below with a minimum of 8 scholarly referencesKey .docx
See details below with a minimum of 8 scholarly referencesKey .docxSee details below with a minimum of 8 scholarly referencesKey .docx
See details below with a minimum of 8 scholarly referencesKey .docxpotmanandrea
 
See attatched for part 1Part 2Develop a 10- to 12-slide Micr.docx
See attatched for part 1Part 2Develop a 10- to 12-slide Micr.docxSee attatched for part 1Part 2Develop a 10- to 12-slide Micr.docx
See attatched for part 1Part 2Develop a 10- to 12-slide Micr.docxpotmanandrea
 
SEE ATTACHMENTRefer to the Intervention Series #38 Anthony” vid.docx
SEE ATTACHMENTRefer to the Intervention Series #38 Anthony” vid.docxSEE ATTACHMENTRefer to the Intervention Series #38 Anthony” vid.docx
SEE ATTACHMENTRefer to the Intervention Series #38 Anthony” vid.docxpotmanandrea
 
SEE Attachment for subject MatterMust be eight to ten double-spa.docx
SEE Attachment for subject MatterMust be eight to ten double-spa.docxSEE Attachment for subject MatterMust be eight to ten double-spa.docx
SEE Attachment for subject MatterMust be eight to ten double-spa.docxpotmanandrea
 
See attachment for my proposal and exampleUse the completed prio.docx
See attachment for my proposal and exampleUse the completed prio.docxSee attachment for my proposal and exampleUse the completed prio.docx
See attachment for my proposal and exampleUse the completed prio.docxpotmanandrea
 
Securities Fraud Please respond to the followingFrom the e-Acti.docx
Securities Fraud Please respond to the followingFrom the e-Acti.docxSecurities Fraud Please respond to the followingFrom the e-Acti.docx
Securities Fraud Please respond to the followingFrom the e-Acti.docxpotmanandrea
 
Security Standards  Please respond to the followingA number of .docx
Security Standards  Please respond to the followingA number of .docxSecurity Standards  Please respond to the followingA number of .docx
Security Standards  Please respond to the followingA number of .docxpotmanandrea
 
Section Four Answer all questions thoroughly and hand-write on loos.docx
Section Four Answer all questions thoroughly and hand-write on loos.docxSection Four Answer all questions thoroughly and hand-write on loos.docx
Section Four Answer all questions thoroughly and hand-write on loos.docxpotmanandrea
 
Security Support Responsibilities Please respond to the following.docx
Security Support Responsibilities Please respond to the following.docxSecurity Support Responsibilities Please respond to the following.docx
Security Support Responsibilities Please respond to the following.docxpotmanandrea
 
Section 1 (200 words)In cognition, one of the major areas of discu.docx
Section 1 (200 words)In cognition, one of the major areas of discu.docxSection 1 (200 words)In cognition, one of the major areas of discu.docx
Section 1 (200 words)In cognition, one of the major areas of discu.docxpotmanandrea
 
Second Wave Feminism, gained strength during the 1970s. For this .docx
Second Wave Feminism, gained strength during the 1970s. For this .docxSecond Wave Feminism, gained strength during the 1970s. For this .docx
Second Wave Feminism, gained strength during the 1970s. For this .docxpotmanandrea
 

More from potmanandrea (20)

Select a foreign country and analyze its monetary system. Formatted .docx
Select a foreign country and analyze its monetary system. Formatted .docxSelect a foreign country and analyze its monetary system. Formatted .docx
Select a foreign country and analyze its monetary system. Formatted .docx
 
Select a company of your choice. Study the company’ CEO and determ.docx
Select a company of your choice. Study the company’ CEO and determ.docxSelect a company of your choice. Study the company’ CEO and determ.docx
Select a company of your choice. Study the company’ CEO and determ.docx
 
Seeing” Social MovementsAn important component of studying social.docx
Seeing” Social MovementsAn important component of studying social.docxSeeing” Social MovementsAn important component of studying social.docx
Seeing” Social MovementsAn important component of studying social.docx
 
Seeking someone that is great at writing papers!  Must be familar .docx
Seeking someone that is great at writing papers!  Must be familar .docxSeeking someone that is great at writing papers!  Must be familar .docx
Seeking someone that is great at writing papers!  Must be familar .docx
 
See comments at the right of the data set..docx
See comments at the right of the data set..docxSee comments at the right of the data set..docx
See comments at the right of the data set..docx
 
Select a company you are familiar with. Imagine that this company is.docx
Select a company you are familiar with. Imagine that this company is.docxSelect a company you are familiar with. Imagine that this company is.docx
Select a company you are familiar with. Imagine that this company is.docx
 
Select a contemporary issue that is of interest to you.Create .docx
Select a contemporary issue that is of interest to you.Create .docxSelect a contemporary issue that is of interest to you.Create .docx
Select a contemporary issue that is of interest to you.Create .docx
 
see the attached filesThe QuestionIs management in the publ.docx
see the attached filesThe QuestionIs management in the publ.docxsee the attached filesThe QuestionIs management in the publ.docx
see the attached filesThe QuestionIs management in the publ.docx
 
See discussions, stats, and author profiles for this publicati.docx
See discussions, stats, and author profiles for this publicati.docxSee discussions, stats, and author profiles for this publicati.docx
See discussions, stats, and author profiles for this publicati.docx
 
See details below with a minimum of 8 scholarly referencesKey .docx
See details below with a minimum of 8 scholarly referencesKey .docxSee details below with a minimum of 8 scholarly referencesKey .docx
See details below with a minimum of 8 scholarly referencesKey .docx
 
See attatched for part 1Part 2Develop a 10- to 12-slide Micr.docx
See attatched for part 1Part 2Develop a 10- to 12-slide Micr.docxSee attatched for part 1Part 2Develop a 10- to 12-slide Micr.docx
See attatched for part 1Part 2Develop a 10- to 12-slide Micr.docx
 
SEE ATTACHMENTRefer to the Intervention Series #38 Anthony” vid.docx
SEE ATTACHMENTRefer to the Intervention Series #38 Anthony” vid.docxSEE ATTACHMENTRefer to the Intervention Series #38 Anthony” vid.docx
SEE ATTACHMENTRefer to the Intervention Series #38 Anthony” vid.docx
 
SEE Attachment for subject MatterMust be eight to ten double-spa.docx
SEE Attachment for subject MatterMust be eight to ten double-spa.docxSEE Attachment for subject MatterMust be eight to ten double-spa.docx
SEE Attachment for subject MatterMust be eight to ten double-spa.docx
 
See attachment for my proposal and exampleUse the completed prio.docx
See attachment for my proposal and exampleUse the completed prio.docxSee attachment for my proposal and exampleUse the completed prio.docx
See attachment for my proposal and exampleUse the completed prio.docx
 
Securities Fraud Please respond to the followingFrom the e-Acti.docx
Securities Fraud Please respond to the followingFrom the e-Acti.docxSecurities Fraud Please respond to the followingFrom the e-Acti.docx
Securities Fraud Please respond to the followingFrom the e-Acti.docx
 
Security Standards  Please respond to the followingA number of .docx
Security Standards  Please respond to the followingA number of .docxSecurity Standards  Please respond to the followingA number of .docx
Security Standards  Please respond to the followingA number of .docx
 
Section Four Answer all questions thoroughly and hand-write on loos.docx
Section Four Answer all questions thoroughly and hand-write on loos.docxSection Four Answer all questions thoroughly and hand-write on loos.docx
Section Four Answer all questions thoroughly and hand-write on loos.docx
 
Security Support Responsibilities Please respond to the following.docx
Security Support Responsibilities Please respond to the following.docxSecurity Support Responsibilities Please respond to the following.docx
Security Support Responsibilities Please respond to the following.docx
 
Section 1 (200 words)In cognition, one of the major areas of discu.docx
Section 1 (200 words)In cognition, one of the major areas of discu.docxSection 1 (200 words)In cognition, one of the major areas of discu.docx
Section 1 (200 words)In cognition, one of the major areas of discu.docx
 
Second Wave Feminism, gained strength during the 1970s. For this .docx
Second Wave Feminism, gained strength during the 1970s. For this .docxSecond Wave Feminism, gained strength during the 1970s. For this .docx
Second Wave Feminism, gained strength during the 1970s. For this .docx
 

Recently uploaded

Sanyam Choudhary Chemistry practical.pdf
Sanyam Choudhary Chemistry practical.pdfSanyam Choudhary Chemistry practical.pdf
Sanyam Choudhary Chemistry practical.pdfsanyamsingh5019
 
Disha NEET Physics Guide for classes 11 and 12.pdf
Disha NEET Physics Guide for classes 11 and 12.pdfDisha NEET Physics Guide for classes 11 and 12.pdf
Disha NEET Physics Guide for classes 11 and 12.pdfchloefrazer622
 
Sports & Fitness Value Added Course FY..
Sports & Fitness Value Added Course FY..Sports & Fitness Value Added Course FY..
Sports & Fitness Value Added Course FY..Disha Kariya
 
mini mental status format.docx
mini    mental       status     format.docxmini    mental       status     format.docx
mini mental status format.docxPoojaSen20
 
Nutritional Needs Presentation - HLTH 104
Nutritional Needs Presentation - HLTH 104Nutritional Needs Presentation - HLTH 104
Nutritional Needs Presentation - HLTH 104misteraugie
 
9548086042 for call girls in Indira Nagar with room service
9548086042  for call girls in Indira Nagar  with room service9548086042  for call girls in Indira Nagar  with room service
9548086042 for call girls in Indira Nagar with room servicediscovermytutordmt
 
Call Girls in Dwarka Mor Delhi Contact Us 9654467111
Call Girls in Dwarka Mor Delhi Contact Us 9654467111Call Girls in Dwarka Mor Delhi Contact Us 9654467111
Call Girls in Dwarka Mor Delhi Contact Us 9654467111Sapana Sha
 
Activity 01 - Artificial Culture (1).pdf
Activity 01 - Artificial Culture (1).pdfActivity 01 - Artificial Culture (1).pdf
Activity 01 - Artificial Culture (1).pdfciinovamais
 
CARE OF CHILD IN INCUBATOR..........pptx
CARE OF CHILD IN INCUBATOR..........pptxCARE OF CHILD IN INCUBATOR..........pptx
CARE OF CHILD IN INCUBATOR..........pptxGaneshChakor2
 
Measures of Central Tendency: Mean, Median and Mode
Measures of Central Tendency: Mean, Median and ModeMeasures of Central Tendency: Mean, Median and Mode
Measures of Central Tendency: Mean, Median and ModeThiyagu K
 
A Critique of the Proposed National Education Policy Reform
A Critique of the Proposed National Education Policy ReformA Critique of the Proposed National Education Policy Reform
A Critique of the Proposed National Education Policy ReformChameera Dedduwage
 
The byproduct of sericulture in different industries.pptx
The byproduct of sericulture in different industries.pptxThe byproduct of sericulture in different industries.pptx
The byproduct of sericulture in different industries.pptxShobhayan Kirtania
 
The Most Excellent Way | 1 Corinthians 13
The Most Excellent Way | 1 Corinthians 13The Most Excellent Way | 1 Corinthians 13
The Most Excellent Way | 1 Corinthians 13Steve Thomason
 
microwave assisted reaction. General introduction
microwave assisted reaction. General introductionmicrowave assisted reaction. General introduction
microwave assisted reaction. General introductionMaksud Ahmed
 
Russian Call Girls in Andheri Airport Mumbai WhatsApp 9167673311 💞 Full Nigh...
Russian Call Girls in Andheri Airport Mumbai WhatsApp  9167673311 💞 Full Nigh...Russian Call Girls in Andheri Airport Mumbai WhatsApp  9167673311 💞 Full Nigh...
Russian Call Girls in Andheri Airport Mumbai WhatsApp 9167673311 💞 Full Nigh...Pooja Nehwal
 
Introduction to Nonprofit Accounting: The Basics
Introduction to Nonprofit Accounting: The BasicsIntroduction to Nonprofit Accounting: The Basics
Introduction to Nonprofit Accounting: The BasicsTechSoup
 
Grant Readiness 101 TechSoup and Remy Consulting
Grant Readiness 101 TechSoup and Remy ConsultingGrant Readiness 101 TechSoup and Remy Consulting
Grant Readiness 101 TechSoup and Remy ConsultingTechSoup
 
Organic Name Reactions for the students and aspirants of Chemistry12th.pptx
Organic Name Reactions  for the students and aspirants of Chemistry12th.pptxOrganic Name Reactions  for the students and aspirants of Chemistry12th.pptx
Organic Name Reactions for the students and aspirants of Chemistry12th.pptxVS Mahajan Coaching Centre
 

Recently uploaded (20)

Sanyam Choudhary Chemistry practical.pdf
Sanyam Choudhary Chemistry practical.pdfSanyam Choudhary Chemistry practical.pdf
Sanyam Choudhary Chemistry practical.pdf
 
Disha NEET Physics Guide for classes 11 and 12.pdf
Disha NEET Physics Guide for classes 11 and 12.pdfDisha NEET Physics Guide for classes 11 and 12.pdf
Disha NEET Physics Guide for classes 11 and 12.pdf
 
Mattingly "AI & Prompt Design: The Basics of Prompt Design"
Mattingly "AI & Prompt Design: The Basics of Prompt Design"Mattingly "AI & Prompt Design: The Basics of Prompt Design"
Mattingly "AI & Prompt Design: The Basics of Prompt Design"
 
Sports & Fitness Value Added Course FY..
Sports & Fitness Value Added Course FY..Sports & Fitness Value Added Course FY..
Sports & Fitness Value Added Course FY..
 
mini mental status format.docx
mini    mental       status     format.docxmini    mental       status     format.docx
mini mental status format.docx
 
Nutritional Needs Presentation - HLTH 104
Nutritional Needs Presentation - HLTH 104Nutritional Needs Presentation - HLTH 104
Nutritional Needs Presentation - HLTH 104
 
9548086042 for call girls in Indira Nagar with room service
9548086042  for call girls in Indira Nagar  with room service9548086042  for call girls in Indira Nagar  with room service
9548086042 for call girls in Indira Nagar with room service
 
Código Creativo y Arte de Software | Unidad 1
Código Creativo y Arte de Software | Unidad 1Código Creativo y Arte de Software | Unidad 1
Código Creativo y Arte de Software | Unidad 1
 
Call Girls in Dwarka Mor Delhi Contact Us 9654467111
Call Girls in Dwarka Mor Delhi Contact Us 9654467111Call Girls in Dwarka Mor Delhi Contact Us 9654467111
Call Girls in Dwarka Mor Delhi Contact Us 9654467111
 
Activity 01 - Artificial Culture (1).pdf
Activity 01 - Artificial Culture (1).pdfActivity 01 - Artificial Culture (1).pdf
Activity 01 - Artificial Culture (1).pdf
 
CARE OF CHILD IN INCUBATOR..........pptx
CARE OF CHILD IN INCUBATOR..........pptxCARE OF CHILD IN INCUBATOR..........pptx
CARE OF CHILD IN INCUBATOR..........pptx
 
Measures of Central Tendency: Mean, Median and Mode
Measures of Central Tendency: Mean, Median and ModeMeasures of Central Tendency: Mean, Median and Mode
Measures of Central Tendency: Mean, Median and Mode
 
A Critique of the Proposed National Education Policy Reform
A Critique of the Proposed National Education Policy ReformA Critique of the Proposed National Education Policy Reform
A Critique of the Proposed National Education Policy Reform
 
The byproduct of sericulture in different industries.pptx
The byproduct of sericulture in different industries.pptxThe byproduct of sericulture in different industries.pptx
The byproduct of sericulture in different industries.pptx
 
The Most Excellent Way | 1 Corinthians 13
The Most Excellent Way | 1 Corinthians 13The Most Excellent Way | 1 Corinthians 13
The Most Excellent Way | 1 Corinthians 13
 
microwave assisted reaction. General introduction
microwave assisted reaction. General introductionmicrowave assisted reaction. General introduction
microwave assisted reaction. General introduction
 
Russian Call Girls in Andheri Airport Mumbai WhatsApp 9167673311 💞 Full Nigh...
Russian Call Girls in Andheri Airport Mumbai WhatsApp  9167673311 💞 Full Nigh...Russian Call Girls in Andheri Airport Mumbai WhatsApp  9167673311 💞 Full Nigh...
Russian Call Girls in Andheri Airport Mumbai WhatsApp 9167673311 💞 Full Nigh...
 
Introduction to Nonprofit Accounting: The Basics
Introduction to Nonprofit Accounting: The BasicsIntroduction to Nonprofit Accounting: The Basics
Introduction to Nonprofit Accounting: The Basics
 
Grant Readiness 101 TechSoup and Remy Consulting
Grant Readiness 101 TechSoup and Remy ConsultingGrant Readiness 101 TechSoup and Remy Consulting
Grant Readiness 101 TechSoup and Remy Consulting
 
Organic Name Reactions for the students and aspirants of Chemistry12th.pptx
Organic Name Reactions  for the students and aspirants of Chemistry12th.pptxOrganic Name Reactions  for the students and aspirants of Chemistry12th.pptx
Organic Name Reactions for the students and aspirants of Chemistry12th.pptx
 

Psychological Science2014, Vol. 25(3) 817 –823© The Author.docx

  • 1. Psychological Science 2014, Vol. 25(3) 817 –823 © The Author(s) 2014 Reprints and permissions: sagepub.com/journalsPermissions.nav DOI: 10.1177/0956797613515681 pss.sagepub.com Research Report Diagnosis of ailments based on tasting and smelling bodily fluids goes back to ancient history. More recently, several diseases have been reported to yield characteris- tic odors, such as Scrofula (which smells like stale beer), Typhoid fever (which smells like baked bread) and Yellow fever (which smells like a butcher’s shop; Penn & Potts, 1998). Today, the idea of medical diagnosis of infectious diseases and other disorders through analysis of volatile organic compounds from skin, breath, feces, or urine by aid of electronic noses has refocused atten- tion on these observations, and the results hold promise for disease-specific volatile biomarkers to be of wide- spread clinical use in the future (Shirasu & Touhara, 2011). In rodents, a wide array of infections, ranging from gastrointestinal nematodes to viruses, are known to alter body odor. This alteration results in a lowered preference for the infected individual during initial investigation (Ehman & Scott, 2001, 2002; Kiesecker, Skelly, Beard, & Preisser, 1999). Chemosensory-mediated avoidance of
  • 2. sick conspecifics in animals is now well established (Arakawa, Cruz, & Deak, 2011; Kavaliers & Colwell, 1995a, 1995b). To investigate whether the first line of defense to microbes entails detectable chemosensory sickness cues, researchers in animal studies have used lipopolysaccharide (LPS) to activate the innate immune 515681PSSXXX10.1177/0956797613515681Olsson et al.Scent of Disease research-article2014 Corresponding Author: Mats J. Olsson, Department of Clinical Neuroscience, Division of Psychology, Karolinska Institutet, Nobels väg 9, SE-17177 Stockholm, Sweden E-mail: [email protected] The Scent of Disease: Human Body Odor Contains an Early Chemosensory Cue of Sickness Mats J. Olsson1, Johan N. Lundström1,2,3, Bruce A. Kimball2,4, Amy R. Gordon1,2, Bianka Karshikoff1, Nishteman Hosseini1, Kimmo Sorjonen1, Caroline Olgart Höglund1, Carmen Solares1, Anne Soop1, John Axelsson1, and Mats Lekander1,5 1Department of Clinical Neuroscience, Karolinska Institutet; 2Monell Chemical Senses Center, Philadelphia, Pennsylvania; 3Department of Psychology, University of Pennsylvania; 4U.S. Department of Agriculture, Animal and Plant Health Inspection Service, Wildlife Services, National Wildlife Research Center, Philadelphia, Pennsylvania; and 5Stress Research Institute, Stockholm University
  • 3. Abstract Observational studies have suggested that with time, some diseases result in a characteristic odor emanating from different sources on the body of a sick individual. Evolutionarily, however, it would be more advantageous if the innate immune response were detectable by healthy individuals as a first line of defense against infection by various pathogens, to optimize avoidance of contagion. We activated the innate immune system in healthy individuals by injecting them with endotoxin (lipopolysaccharide). Within just a few hours, endotoxin-exposed individuals had a more aversive body odor relative to when they were exposed to a placebo. Moreover, this effect was statistically mediated by the individuals’ level of immune activation. This chemosensory detection of the early innate immune response in humans represents the first experimental evidence that disease smells and supports the notion of a “behavioral immune response” that protects healthy individuals from sick ones by altering patterns of interpersonal contact. Keywords olfactory perception, human body, health Received 6/9/13; Revision accepted 10/27/13 at ARKANSAS STATE UNIV on January 29, 2015pss.sagepub.comDownloaded from http://pss.sagepub.com/ 818 Olsson et al. system and an inflammatory response (Beutler, 2009; Suffredini, Fantuzzi, Badolato, Oppenheim, & O’Grady,
  • 4. 1999). Indeed, in the rat, it has been shown that LPS injection influences the body odor of an individual such that other rats avoid contact (Arakawa, Blandino, & Deak, 2009; Dantzer, 2009). Therefore, the innate immune response is also relevant to the investigation of olfactory markers in humans. With this background, we set out to—for the first time—experimentally test the idea of a chemosensory sickness cue in humans. We hypothesized that humans are able to perceptually dissociate between healthy and sick individuals’ body odors. Moreover, under the assumption that this capacity has evolved to reduce con- tamination risks, we hypothesized that it should be pres- ent at an early stage of the sickness response. We tested this hypothesis by comparing body-odor samples from individuals following LPS treatment to samples from the same individuals following saline treatment. Because inflammatory cytokines are involved in sickness behavior (Avitsur, Cohen, & Yirmiya, 1997) and are also implicated in the expression of aversive odor cues in response to a number of pathogens in animals (Dantzer, 2004), we measured proinflammatory cytokines as key mediators of these odor cues. Method Sampling of body odors Eight healthy volunteers (7 men, 1 woman; mean age = 24 years, SD = 3.71) were recruited for donation of body odor during two sessions. To be included in the study, participants had to be between 18 and 45 years old, right- handed, and nonsmoking and to neither be taking medi- cation (including nonbarrier contraceptives for female participants) nor have a history of drug abuse, chronic
  • 5. pain, or psychiatric disorders. The eight donors took part in two sessions, both conducted at 1 p.m. and separated by 28 days, in which they received either LPS or saline injections. Participants wore tight T-shirts to allow for body-odor sampling. The sampling took place in a hos- pital environment and lasted for a total of 4 hr. A physi- cian supervised the donors (for details, see Body-Odor Sampling in the Supplemental Material available online). The study was approved by the regional ethical review board in Stockholm. Written informed consent was obtained from all participants. Measures of sickness response During the sampling period, tympanic temperature was measured (using a Thermoscan PRO-1; Braun, Inc., San Diego, CA) before injection and once every hour following injection. Four hours after injection with LPS, participants’ body temperature had increased by about 1 °C (Fig. 1d). Plasma samples were provided 0 hr (baseline), 1 hr, 1.5 hr, 2 hr, 3 hr, and 4 hr after injection; they were frozen at −70 °C and were later thawed for analysis with Millipore’s MILLIPLEX MAP high-sensitivity human-cytokine kit (Millipore, Billerica, MA) using Luminex xMAP methodol- ogy (Luminex, Austin, TX). In response to LPS, clear rises in levels of tumor necrosis factor-alpha (TNF-a), interleu- kin-6 (IL-6), and IL-8, peaking between 1.5 and 2 hr after injection, were observed, confirming an inflammatory response to LPS (see Figs. 1a–1c). Chemical assays We conducted chemical assays of body-odor samples to assess the relative abundance of potentially odorous (i.e., volatile) compounds with gas chromatography–mass
  • 6. spectrometry (GC-MS; see GC-MS Analysis in the Supplemental Material for details). Participants Forty participants (28 women, 12 men; mean age = 26.2 years, SD = 6.3) were recruited from the Karolinska Institutet university campus to take part in the body- odor-assessment portion of the study. To be included, participants had to be nonsmokers and to have self- reported good health and functional sense of smell. Procedure Using a double-blind, within-group experimental design, we tested participants separately with 18 unique odor stimuli (8 LPS body-odor samples, 8 placebo body-odor samples, and 2 samples from unworn T shirts, which served as controls) in squeeze bottles. For each participant, the odor stimuli were first pre- sented one at a time in a uniquely randomized order with an intertrial interval of 30 s. After a short break of 1.5 min, the odor stimuli were presented a second time, again in a uniquely randomized order. On each trial, the participant could squeeze the bottle and smell the head- space a maximum of two times to prevent sensory adap- tation. After smelling the sample, they rated its perceived intensity (using a scale from 0 to 7), pleasantness (using a scale from −7 to 7), and health (using a scale from −4 to 4). Unique scales were used to avoid the potential confound that participants would convert ratings from one scale to another. The extremes of the scales were referred to as “maximal experiences.” The value of 0 on the pleasantness and health scales was referred to as “neither pleasant nor unpleasant” and “neither healthy
  • 7. nor sick,” respectively. We used a measure of perceived intensity to assess whether there was a quantitative rather than qualitative difference between sick and healthy at ARKANSAS STATE UNIV on January 29, 2015pss.sagepub.comDownloaded from http://pss.sagepub.com/ Scent of Disease 819 body odors, and we used a measure of pleasantness because it is the primary dimension of the olfactory per- ceptual space and is therefore at the base of olfactory functioning (Khan et al., 2007). Results and Discussion Participants’ ratings of the perceived intensity, pleasant- ness, and health of odor samples from the three experi- mental conditions (LPS, placebo, and control; see Fig. 2) were submitted to analyses. Control odors (unworn T-shirts) were rated as smelling significantly less intense, more pleasant, and healthier than the LPS and placebo odors (worn T-shirts; see Additional Analyses of Ratings of Control Shirts and Table S1 in the Supplemental Material), which indicates that the body-odor-sampling technique was adequate. Further analyses, therefore, were focused on the difference between LPS and placebo body odors. Linear mixed model analyses, using both donors and rat- ers as statistical units (see Choice of Statistical Analysis of Body-Odor Ratings in the Supplemental Material for details) with Bonferroni-Holm correction for multiple test- ing (Holm, 1979), showed that the LPS body odors smelled
  • 8. significantly more unpleasant, d = 0.259, t(592) = 4.487, a 0 25 50 75 100 0 25 50 75 100 TN F- α L ev el (p g/ m
  • 9. l) Before 1 1.5 2 3 4 Time After Injection (hours) b 0 200 400 600 Before 1 1.5 2 3 4 Time After Injection (hours) c Before 1 1.5 2 3 4 Time After Injection (hours) d 36 37 38 Ty m
  • 10. pa ni c B od y Te m pe ra tu re (° C ) Before 1 2 3 4 Time After Injection (hours) Placebo LPS IL -8 L ev el
  • 11. (p g/ m l) IL -6 L ev el (p g/ m l) Fig. 1. Mean levels of proinflammatory cytokines (a) tumor necrosis factor-alpha (TNF-a), (b) interleukin-6 (IL-6), and (c) IL-8 and (d) tympanic body temperature as a function of time after injection and treatment (lipopolysaccharide, LPS, vs. placebo). Error bars represent standard errors. at ARKANSAS STATE UNIV on January 29, 2015pss.sagepub.comDownloaded from http://pss.sagepub.com/ 820 Olsson et al.
  • 12. p < .001, more intense, d = 0.212, t(592) = 4.423, p < .001, and more unhealthy, d = 0.133, t(592) = 2.025, p = .043. These results indicate that humans can indeed dissoci- ate between the odors of sick and healthy individuals within 4 hr of innate immune-system activation; there are at least two possible reasons for this effect. The body odors of sick and healthy people may differ in perceived intensity, reflecting that the sick body emits more of the same types of volatile and odorous substances as the healthy body. The higher perceived intensity of the sick body-odor samples supports such a “more-of-the-same” model. The other possible, and more intriguing, explana- tion for the LPS-induced changes in body odor is that the pattern of substance concentrations emitted from the body has changed and forms a cue of sickness, reflecting the activation of the innate immune system. Such a quali- tative shift, independent of overall odorant concentra- tion, would be a more ecologically viable candidate to modulate behavioral adaptations. The observation that a sick individual’s body odor smells more unpleasant and unhealthy supports such a “sickness-cue” model. However, it is well established that odor pleasantness— in addition to odor intensity—changes as a function of odorant concentration (Doty, 1975). Whereas intensity increases with odor concentration, pleasantness follows a more complicated model: Pleasant odors tend to have an optimum along the stimulus-concentration range, and unpleasant odors simply get more unpleasant as the con- centration increases (Lawless, 1977). Given that body odors are on the unpleasant end of the olfactory contin- uum (Fig. 2), we expect that an increase in body-odor intensity would be accompanied by an increase in unpleas- antness. Hence, a more-of-the-same model could also pre- dict a shift in odor pleasantness.
  • 13. To answer the question of whether or not there was a treatment-related shift in body-odor pleasantness inde- pendent of the changes in odor intensity, we performed an analysis similar to that used to assess the effect of body-odor type (LPS, placebo) on pleasantness ratings, but we added odor intensity as a covariate. The results showed a significant and separate effect of LPS treatment on body-odor pleasantness, d = −0.118, t(597) = 2.424, p = .016, and this effect could not be explained by differ- ences in intensity. The same test of perceived health using intensity as a covariate did not reveal a significant separate effect of treatment, d = −0.046, t(598) = −0.730, p = .465. Moreover, to directly test the more-of-the-same model, we analyzed the body-odor samples (using GC-MS) to assess the concentration of odorous compounds. Because the samples had been used in the behavioral test and were therefore potentially depleted and contaminated, the analysis was restricted to determining the overall abundance of volatile compounds in the LPS and placebo samples. The results indicated that the concentrations of the LPS samples were, on average, lower than those in placebo samples across all compounds identified, but insignificantly so, d = 0.134, t(16) = 0.715, n.s. In other words, LPS-treated participants did not seem to sweat more, but rather less, than those who were placebo treated. Thus, these results fail to support a more-of-the- same model as the explanation of LPS-induced changes in body odor and are consistent with the notion that dur- ing a generalized sickness response, humans emit a chemical cue. Dedicated studies should target these chemicals in the future. In order to link the change in body-odor composition
  • 14. not only to the LPS treatment but also to the actual Placebo LPS 0 1 2 3 4 In te ns ity a *** Placebo LPS –2.5 –2.0 –1.5 –1.0 –0.5 0.0 Pl ea
  • 15. sa nt ne ss b Placebo LPS –0.45 –0.60 –0.30 –0.15 0.00 H ea lth c *** * Fig. 2. Mean ratings of the perceived (a) intensity, (b) pleasantness, and (c) health of odors of T-shirts worn by individuals exposed to lipo- polysaccharide (LPS) and a placebo. Error bars show standard errors. Asterisks indicate significant differences between conditions (*p < .05; ***p < .001).
  • 16. at ARKANSAS STATE UNIV on January 29, 2015pss.sagepub.comDownloaded from http://pss.sagepub.com/ Scent of Disease 821 treatment-induced inflammatory response, we performed mediation analyses of cytokines. Cytokines IL-6 and TNF-a, but not IL-8, significantly mediated the effect of treatment on body-odor pleasantness and intensity (Table 1). Taken together, these results strongly support that humans emit a chemical cue during a generalized sickness response that can be perceived by others. According to participants’ verbal reports, rating body- odor intensity and pleasantness was an easier task than rating the perceived health of body-odor samples. Moreover, the effect size of LPS treatment on health rat- ings, albeit significant, was relatively small. In fact, there was no significant effect of treatment on health ratings when either pleasantness or intensity was controlled for; thus, these results do not support a direct perception of health status. Instead, health ratings may have been based on inferences from the other aspects of the odor, such as its pleasantness. It has also been suggested that the emotion of disgust has evolved as a disease-avoid- ance mechanism (Oaten, Stevenson, & Case, 2009). The current results may be relevant to this hypothesis, in that a disgust-driven, negative response promotes withdrawal from and avoidance of a sick individual by healthy ones. In concert with the social withdrawal exhibited by infected individuals, such mechanisms are modeled to be highly effective in containing an epidemic—
  • 17. particularly if instigated soon after infection (Cole, 2006). In this study, when participants rated body odors of “sick” individuals, they found them significantly more unpleasant than “healthy” body odors; future studies should focus on the role of disgust in such responses to “sick” body odor. The exact nature of the cue or signal has yet to be determined. For instance, the volatile substances in the skin mediating the effect from an inflammatory response to body odor need careful investigation. Moreover, with regard to theory, there are two assumptions one can make about a sickness odor: It could be viewed as either a cue or a signal. As a cue, it would be inadvertent on behalf of the sender and beneficial to the receiver. In line with that notion, most animal studies have revealed avoidance behavior in response to a sick body odor. However, the avoidance behavior is typically seen in response to body odors of unfamiliar conspecifics; in contrast, increased maternal licking of LPS-treated rat pups has also been reported (Breivik et al., 2002). This pattern of results supports the idea that the sickness odor can also be a signal beneficial to the sender. Some evi- dence even suggests that, rather than being aversive, the odor of infected males simply loses its attractiveness, which suggests a reduced signal of health rather than an increased signal of sickness (Kavaliers & Colwell, 1995a; Penn, Schneider, White, Slev, & Potts, 1998). It is not yet clear whether these changes are best characterized as a cue of illness beneficial to recipients or simply as a reduced signal of health from a sender whose resources necessary for health-signal maintenance have been real- located (Penn & Potts, 1998). It should also be noted that the specificity of the type of olfactory cue indicated here and in animal models remains to be determined.
  • 18. Among the physiological adaptations that occur after immune challenge is an activation of the hypothalamic- pituitary-adrenal (HPA) axis (Maier, Watkins, & Nance, 2001), and consequently, increased cortisol can be seen after LPS administration in humans (Grigoleit et al., 2011). This overlap between sickness and the fight-flight response is expected, given that mobilization and redi- rection of energy is central to handling threats from both within and without (Segerstrom & Miller, 2004). Future studies should therefore investigate the extent to which internal and external challenges result in similar olfactory changes. Table 1. Crude and Mediated Effects on Ratings of Body-Odor Pleasantness, Intensity, and Health Effect and mediator Pleasantness Intensity Health Crude effect LPS –0.259 (0.058)*** 0.212 (0.048)*** –0.133 (0.066)* Temperature –0.120 (0.034)*** 0.091 (0.029)** –0.041 (0.039) IL-6 –0.181 (0.034)*** 0.145 (0.029)*** –0.080 (0.039)* IL-8 –0.144 (0.033)*** 0.107 (0.027)*** –0.059 (0.037) TNF-a –0.153 (0.031)*** 0.126 (0.026)*** –0.075 (0.035)* Mediator of effects of LPS IL-6 –0.149 (0.051)** 0.113 (0.043)* –0.049 (0.057) IL-8 –0.070 (0.061) 0.017 (0.051) 0.013 (0.068) TNF-a –0.154 (0.077)*a 0.127 (0.064)*,a –0.058 (0.085) Note: Tests of the significance of mediation effects were conducted using Sobel tests and were based on MacKinnon and Fritz (2007). Standard deviations are shown in parentheses. LPS =
  • 19. lipopolysaccharide; IL-6 = interleukin-6; IL-8 = interleukin-8; TNF-a = tumor necrosis factor- alpha. aSignificance does not withstand a Bonferroni-Holm correction for multiple testing. *p < .05. **p < .01. ***p < .001. at ARKANSAS STATE UNIV on January 29, 2015pss.sagepub.comDownloaded from http://pss.sagepub.com/ 822 Olsson et al. Altogether, the results of this experimental study sug- gest that, akin to rodents, humans are able to detect a social cue of sickness from body odor alone, which can be used for avoidance of infected conspecifics. Moreover, this social information can be triggered by the innate immune response, which is observable just a few hours after innate immune-system activation and is a general response to a variety of pathogens. Human olfaction may thus prove to be a signaling route to a “behavioral immune response” (Breivik et al., 2002) that protects healthy individuals by altering patterns of interpersonal contact and, possibly, by heightening the immune-system response to infection in the receiver, as has been shown for other disease-related stimuli (Schaller & Park, 2011; Stevenson et al., 2012). Author Contributions M. J. Olsson, J. N. Lundström, B. Karshikoff, N. Hosseini, C. Olgart Höglund, A. Soop, J. Axelsson, and M. Lekander designed the method for data collection; data collection was carried out
  • 20. by A. R. Gordon, B. Karshikoff, C. Solares, N. Hosseini, and B. A. Kimball under the supervision of M. J. Olsson. A. Soop administered lipopolysaccharide. M. J. Olsson and K. Sorjonen wrote the statistical-analysis plan and carried out the statistical analyses. Data analyses were conducted by A. R. Gordon, C. Solares, and B. A. Kimball, and J. Axelsson conducted GC-MS analyses. M. J. Olsson, C. Olgart Höglund, and M. Lekander obtained funding with help from J. Axelsson. The manuscript was drafted by M. J. Olsson, N. Hosseini, and K. Sorjonen and revised by J. N. Lundström, A. R. Gordon, B. Karshikoff, C. Solares, N. Hosseini, K. Sorjonen, B. A. Kimball, J. Axelsson, and M. Lekander. M. J. Olsson served as the study’s guarantor. All authors approved the final version of the manuscript for submission. Acknowledgments Thanks are due to Malin Schütze for technical assistance and to Andreas Olsson for commenting on a previous version of the manuscript. Declaration of Conflicting Interests The authors declared that they had no conflicts of interest with respect to their authorship or the publication of this article. Funding This study was supported by the Swedish Research Council (Grant 421-2012-1125) and the Bank of Sweden Tercentenary Foundation (Grant P12-1017; M. J. Olsson); by the Knut and Alice Wallenberg Foundation (Grant KAW 2012.0141; J. N. Lundström); by the Osher Center for Integrative Medicine (J. Axelsson); and by the Swedish Heart-Lung Foundation, the Center for Allergy Research and the Petrus and Augusta Hedlund’s Foundation, the Swedish Asthma and Allergy Foundation, the Stockholm Stress Center, and the Swedish
  • 21. Council for Working Life and Social Research (M. Lekander and C. Olgart Höglund). Supplemental Material Additional supporting information may be found at http://pss .sagepub.com/content/by/supplemental-data References Arakawa, H., Blandino, P., Jr., & Deak, T. (2009). Central infusion of interleukin-1 receptor antagonist blocks the reduction in social behavior produced by prior stressor exposure. Physiology & Behavior, 98, 139–146. doi:10.1016/j .physbeh.2009.04.024 Arakawa, H., Cruz, S., & Deak, T. (2011). From models to mech- anisms: Odorant communication as a key determinant of social behavior in rodents during illness-associated states. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 35, 1916–1928. doi:10.1016/j.neubiorev.2011.03.007 Avitsur, R., Cohen, E., & Yirmiya, R. (1997). Effects of inter- leukin-1 on sexual attractivity in a model of sickness behavior. Physiology & Behavior, 63, 25–30. doi:10.1016/ S0031-9384(97)00381-8 Beutler, B. A. (2009). TLRs and innate immunity. Blood, 113, 1399–1407. doi:10.1182/blood-2008-07-019307 Breivik, T., Stephan, M., Brabant, G. E., Straub, R. H., Pabst, R., & von Hörsten, S. (2002). Postnatal lipopolysaccha- ride-induced illness predisposes to periodontal disease in adulthood. Brain, Behavior, and Immunity, 16, 421–438.
  • 22. doi:10.1006/brbi.2001.0642 Cole, S. W. (2006). The complexity of dynamic host networks. In T. S. Deisboeck & J. Y. Kresh (Eds.), Complex systems sci- ence in biomedicine (pp. 605–629). New York, NY: Kluwer Academic. doi:10.1007/978-0-387-33532-2_26 Dantzer, R. (2004). Cytokine-induced sickness behaviour: A neuroimmune response to activation of innate immu- nity. European Journal of Pharmacology, 500, 399–411. doi:10.1016/j.ejphar.2004.07.040 Dantzer, R. (2009). Cytokine, sickness behavior, and depres- sion. Immunology and Allergy Clinics of North America, 29, 247–264. doi:10.1016/j.iac.2009.02.002 Doty, R. L. (1975). An examination of relationships between the pleasantness, intensity, and concentration of 10 odor- ous stimuli. Perception & Psychophysics, 17, 492–496. doi:10.3758/BF03203300 Ehman, K. D., & Scott, M. E. (2001). Urinary odour preferences of MHC congenic female mice, Mus domesticus: Implications for kin recognition and detection of parasitized males. Animal Behaviour, 62, 781–789. doi:10.1006/anbe.2001.1805 Ehman, K. D., & Scott, M. E. (2002). Female mice mate pref- erentially with non-parasitized males. Parasitology, 125, 461–466. doi:10.1017/S003118200200224X Grigoleit, J. S., Kullmann, J. S., Wolf, O. T., Hammes, F., Wegner, A., Jablonowski, S., . . . Schedlowski, M. (2011). Dose-dependent effects of endotoxin on neurobehavioral functions in humans. PLoS ONE, 6, e28330. Retrieved from http://www.plosone.org/article/info%3Adoi%2F10.1371
  • 23. %2Fjournal.pone.0028330 Holm, S. (1979). A simple sequentially rejective multiple test procedure. Scandinavian Journal of Statistics, 6, 65–70. Retrieved from http://www.jstor.org/discover/10.2307/461 at ARKANSAS STATE UNIV on January 29, 2015pss.sagepub.comDownloaded from http://www.plosone.org/article/info%3Adoi%2F10.1371%2Fjour nal.pone.0028330 http://www.jstor.org/discover/10.2307/4615733?uid=3738984&; uid=2134&uid=2129&uid=2&uid=70&uid=4&sid=21102598283 337 http://pss.sagepub.com/content/by/supplemental-data http://pss.sagepub.com/ Scent of Disease 823 5733?uid=3738984&;uid=2134&uid=2129&uid=2&uid=70&u id=4&sid=21102598283337 Kavaliers, M., & Colwell, D. D. (1995a). Discrimination by female mice between the odours of parasitized and non-parasitized males. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 261, 37–41. doi:10.1098/ rspb.1995.0113 Kavaliers, M., & Colwell, D. D. (1995b). Odours of parasit- ized males induce aversive responses in female mice. Animal Behaviour, 50, 1161–1169. doi:10.1016/0003- 3472(95)80032-8 Khan, R. M., Luk, C. H., Flinker, A., Aggarwal, A., Lapid, H., Haddad, R., & Sobel, N. (2007). Predicting odor pleasant-
  • 24. ness from odorant structure: Pleasantness as a reflection of the physical world. Journal of Neuroscience, 27, 10015– 10023. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1158-07.2007 Kiesecker, J. M., Skelly, D. K., Beard, K. H., & Preisser, E. (1999). Behavioral reduction of infection risk. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, USA, 96, 9165–9168. doi:10.1073/pnas.96.16.9165 Lawless, H. (1977). The pleasantness of mixtures in taste and olfaction. Sensory Processes, 1, 227–237. MacKinnon, D. P., Fritz, M. S., Williams, J., & Lockwood, C. M. (2007). Distribution of the product confidence lim- its for the indirect effect: Program PRODCLIN. Behavior Research Methods, 39, 384–389. doi:10.3758/BF03193007 Maier, S. F., Watkins, L. R., & Nance, D. M. (2001). Multiple routes of action of Interleukin-1 on the nervous system. In R. Ader, D. L. Felten, & L. Cohen (Eds.), Psychoneuroimmunology (3rd ed., pp. 563–583). San Diego, CA: Academic Press. Oaten, M., Stevenson, R. J., & Case, T. I. (2009). Disgust as a disease-avoidance mechanism. Psychological Bulletin, 135, 303–321. doi:10.1037/a0014823 Penn, D., & Potts, W. K. (1998). Chemical signals and parasite- mediated sexual selection. Trends in Ecology & Evolution, 13, 391–396. doi:10.1016/S0169-5347(98)01473-6 Penn, D., Schneider, G., White, K., Slev, P., & Potts, W. (1998). Influenza infection neutralizes the attractiveness of male odour to female mice (Mus musculus). Ethology, 104, 685–694. doi:10.1111/j.1439-0310.1998.tb00102.x
  • 25. Schaller, M., & Park, J. H. (2011). The behavioral immune system (and why it matters). Current Directions in Psychological Science, 20, 99–103. doi:10.1177/0963721411402596 Segerstrom, S. C., & Miller, G. E. (2004). Psychological stress and the human immune system: A meta-analytic study of 30 years of inquiry. Psychological Bulletin, 130, 601–630. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.130.4.601 Shirasu, M., & Touhara, K. (2011). The scent of disease: Volatile organic compounds of the human body related to dis- ease and disorder. Journal of Biochemistry, 150, 257–266. doi:10.1093/jb/mvr090 Stevenson, R. J., Hodgson, D., Oaten, M. J., Moussavi, M., Langberg, R., Case, T. I., & Barouei, J. (2012). Disgust elevates core body temperature and up-regulates certain oral immune markers. Brain, Behavior, and Immunity, 26, 1160–1168. doi:10.1016/j.bbi.2012.07.010 Suffredini, A. F., Fantuzzi, G., Badolato, R., Oppenheim, J. J., & O’Grady, N. P. (1999). New insights into the biology of the acute phase response. Journal of Clinical Immunology, 19, 203–214. doi:10.1023/A:1020563913045 at ARKANSAS STATE UNIV on January 29, 2015pss.sagepub.comDownloaded from http://www.jstor.org/discover/10.2307/4615733?uid=3738984&; uid=2134&uid=2129&uid=2&uid=70&uid=4&sid=21102598283 337 http://pss.sagepub.com/