SlideShare a Scribd company logo
1 of 62
Memory is a social and political construction. Discuss.
· The title must appear at the top of your first page as set out in
the list attached (The title must not be altered or changed in any
way)
· You should use spacing (1.5) and font size 12
· Correct spelling and appropriate paragraphing should be
evident
· There should be no widows or orphans, i.e. headings left alone
at the bottom of the page
· You should provide references for every citation within the
text – omissions will be penalised - References should be
written in accord with a referencing convention that is
consistent with your major course of study, e.g. APA
Marks will be awarded for:
· Answering the question
· Observing academic conventions
· Adopting a clear and sophisticated style of writing
· Identifying and addressing key issues raised by the question
· Breadth and depth of relevant knowledge
· Depth of analysis, Clarity of argument, Soundness of argument
· Originality of argument
You might find these articles relevant to the topicBaumeister,
R. F. & Hastings, S. (1997). Distortions of collective memory:
How groups flatter and deceive themselves. In J. W.
Pennebaker, D. Paez, & B. Rimé. Collective memory of political
events: Social psychological perspectives. Mahwah, NJ: LEA.
Liu, J. H., & Hilton, D. J. (2005). How the past weights on the
present: Social representations of history and their role in
identity politics. British Journal of Social Psychology, 44, 537–
556.
Lászlo ́, J. (2003). History, identity and narratives. In J.
Lászlo ́, & W. Wagner (Eds.), Theories and controversies in
societal psychology (pp. 180–192). Budapest, Hungary: New
Mandate.
Pennebaker, J. W., Paez, D., & Rime, B. (Eds.). (1997).
Collective memory of political events: Social psychological
perspectives. Mahwah, NJ: LEA.
1700 WORDS REQUIRED essay] and 300 words reflective
writing, for reflective writing, I will send you the lecture slides
and you will need to reflect upon those slide
to get mark:
Displays a systematic and sophisticated knowledge and
understanding of the subject. Is extensively researched and
shows a critical awareness of current issues within the field.
Original argument reflecting synthetically on the issues raised
in the module. Authoritative and reflective use of supporting
material.
Demonstrates a sophisticated understanding of theoretical and
critical concepts which includes the ability to reflect critically.
Excellently presented work that conforms to all required
academic conventions. Written in a style that is lucid and
precise.
Displays a strong and intelligent knowledge and understanding
ofthe subject. Is extensively researched and referenced in
breadth
and depth.
slides 1.ppt
Epistemology – how do we generate legitimate
knowledge?Physical and metaphysical reality (Metaphysics:
branch of philosophy that investigates the first principles of
nature, e.g. ontology – the science of being)Psychological
realities (experience and perception)Social and political
realitiesHistorical ‘truth’ and historical ‘realities’
The module encourages a discussion about knowledge and
reality…
Critical social psychology (CSP) – challenges ‘taken for granted
assumptions’ about the worldIt analyses social phenomena on
the basis of:
(i) power relations
(ii) difference (how difference is constructed and
maintained, e.g. nationality, race, gender, disability)
Where does the module sit within psychology?
Perceptual Cognitivism
Reality - Perception - Discourse
(language is assumed to be transparent, passive, a mirror)
Discursive Psychology
Discourse - Perception - Reality
Function/purpose of communication, identity and language
employed actively constructs the way we see the world
TWO COMPETING MODELS OF REALITY
Dunkirk: May 1940
DunkirkThe Dunkirk spiritThe Battle of FranceThe fall of
France
What difference does it make if we refer to this event in these
different ways?
Knowledge in all its forms is related to culture (especially
history)Much of what we believe is what we are told by
authorities within the culture, e.g. the writing of history
textbooks.The culture is determined by the politics of the day,
i.e. who is in power. Who decides what history should be
taught? Who decides how it should be taught?
Something to consider:
*
our subjective experience and…agreement with others about
what is real and true (inter-subjectivity/consensus)
N.B. This is not the same as an objectively real view of the
world (God’s eye view)
Our sense of reality is largely determined by:
We usually confine myths to the past assuming previous
civilisations to be primitive, unscientific, uninformed or
mistaken in their representation of the pastAnything that fails to
correspond with our current experience is usually described as a
myth, e.g. supernatural manifestations of the gods‘ – ‘Greek
myths’Some discredit biblical accounts of the past – e.g. The
destruction of Sodom and Gommorah, the global flood, the
miracles of Jesus but…
Myth as rhetoric?
Consider the situation in reverse: Imagine that you are living in
an ancient world and I tell you that I believe that it is possible
to:fly through the sky in a metal container heavier than a house,
from one side of the earth to the other, faster than words can
reach the ear?have a projectile weapon so powerful it could
destroy the earth? i.e. we have recently gained access to
knowledge and powers that the ancients did not understand.Is it
possible that they had access to knowledge and powers that we
do not understand?
To what extent is the word ‘myth’ a form of rhetoric that seeks
to discredit an alternative reality for which we have little or no
understanding ?
gives some credence to alternative takes on reality: the
paranormal, UFO’s, the existence of angels, evil spirits,
miracles and healing– they make reality and what is possible
and impossible a matter of debate
This argument…
An empirical myth (a false belief), e.g. one that can be
demonstrated to be false, e.g. drinking sulphuric acid is good
for the body.
A cultural myth (a belief that defines the culture), e.g.
capitalism and democracy are the best systems to run a society,
the American Dream, ‘might is right’, ‘The Dunkirk spirit’.
A language system perpetuating a discourse that bolsters a myth
(Postmodern / Feminist analysis) e.g. Who is the Chairman of
the company?
(Brown, 2003, p.184)
If language systems endorse mythical beliefs and values
then they need to be studied closely – this is the social
constructionist position (e.g. Burr, Gergen, Berger &
Luckmann)
It may be helpful to think of the word ‘myth’ in three different
ways:
Scientific myths Religious myths Political myths Historical
myths
Could there be ‘modern myths’ within our cultural discourse?
This was the principle question of the Enlightenment (Circa
1637- 1789)How should we determine what is true and
false?People today often use the language of the Enlightenment
– ‘facts, proof and certainty’
How should reliable knowledge be generated?
An understanding of sacred texts that had been passed down as
part of Judeo-Christian and Islamic traditions (The Bible and
the Qu’ran)Philosophical writings of Plato and AristotleMyth
and magic (divination, sorcery etc.) and superstition
Previously the world view had been determined by:
Rationalism – knowledge determined by reason
(Philosophy)Revelation – knowledge revealed by God
(Theology)Empiricism – knowledge acquired through the
senses, i.e. observation (Science)
Philosophers have long argued that there are three legitimate
sources of knowledge:
Knowledge is rational, based on factsTheories are proved;
myths rejectedKnowledge is certainJohn Locke (empiricist)
influences politics and philosophyNewton’s mechanistic
theories of the natural world (natural philosophy)
Scientific revolution (From Copernicus to Newton – Circa 1543-
1727)
The world would progress though this form of knowledge
The project of modernity
?
Scientism – Positivism – Empiricism – Humanism – Anarchism
Absolute truth? Absolute truth is largely
inaccessible
Different versions of
reality
Relativism
Consider the nature of arguments between people – court cases
– competing historical accounts
Philosophies of Science (Knowledge)
Empiricism - Knowledge acquired through the senses
(experience)Observation/ collect data/ formulate
theoriesEmpirical evidence includes experiments, documents,
personal testimonies
The dominant approach in Western culture:
Auguste Comte – French social philosopher – advanced the idea
of a ‘science of humanity’Strict application of ‘the scientific
method’ – leads to absolute certainties – no speculative
knowledge, i.e. religion A more extreme form of empiricism
Positivism
Only scientific statements are meaningfulScientific methods
should be used in every form of intellectual enquiry
Scientism
“No royal road to knowledge” – science is just one useful
approachThere are other types of knowledge that are useful, e.g.
subjective knowledge in counselling
Humanism
“Anything goes” – everything is true from the perspective of the
perceiver – a extreme form of relativism
Anarchism
Science – not absolute knowledge (provisional) – the problem
of inductionWhat can we expect science to explain and what is
beyond its scope? The statement ‘God is good’ is beyond the
scope of science since it can never be tested (Karl Popper)What
has reductionist science done for humans? (Health care &
nuclear weapons)
Limits of Knowledge
Theology – problem of interpretation of the written text
(Competing denominations)Which revelation are we to believe
and why? (Competing religious traditions)What criteria should
we use to evaluate the veracity of religious texts?
Limits of knowledge
Authorship – who by? with what skills? personal motivation and
bias?Authority – by what authority? e.g. biographyPolitical
agenda of taught histories – national superiority?What is to be
recorded /emphasised /excluded?Revisionist history (e.g. denial
of holocaust by David Irving)
The writing of history (Historical knowledge)
Progressivistic writing (The Whig interpretation of history), i.e.
to interpret the past in terms of principles of progress in order
to justify the present Example – humans have progressed in
knowledge and psychology is pushing back the boundaries The
implication - what was past was inferior rather than different
and what is present is superior
Historical knowledge
Perception of history affects/constructs identity and behaviour
Contested histories are often at the heart of conflictThese
interests lead us to consider the social representations of history
(Moscovici,1961/1976)
Historical knowledge is of interest to psychologists because:
slides 2.pptx
G.H. Mead (1863-1931)
Mind and society are inseparable
Society is as much in us as we are in it
Our world view determines what we think and how we think
As society changes, people think differently
People in different places think differently
Broad spectrum of cultural and historic thinking
What role does the past play in the formation of identity?
We can neither function nor be complete without memory, e.g.
the effects of dementia
A relationship with the past provides existential meaning in the
form of a life narrative: Who am I? What have I done? What is
the story of my life?
A psychological relationship with the past starts at birth –
social network provides narratives of the past – families, ethnic
groups religion, nations.
Immersion into these narrative is ”mnemonic socialisation”
(Zerubavel, 1996)
The “past weighs on the present” (Liu & Hilton, 2005)
A reciprocal relationship
There is a reciprocal relationship between social identity (group
membership) and historical understanding
The way we see ourselves and others affects the way we see the
past - and the way we see the past affects the way we see
ourselves and others
Q. What do we have to do to change our perspective on the
past? –to get nearer to the truth
A. Loosen our attachment to certain social identities or
reconfigure them
Q. What would we need to do to see our own group more
objectively?
A. Loosen our attachment to absolute narratives of the past
The development of personal identity
Identity Process Theory (Breakwell, 1986, 1992, 1993)
An interplay between self and experience – two psychological
processes - assimilation-accommodation and evaluation
Assimilation-accommodation process – new knowledge or
experience is either assimilated into an existing identity
structure or the identity structure is altered to accommodate the
the experience.
Included in this “new knowledge” would be accounts of the past
related to family, group or nation.
Orientation is towards “self-interest” rather than accuracy
As each relevant account is heard, political and personal
implications start to accrue.
“The past weighs on the present”
In therapeutic encounters – Freudian perspective
A client’s relationship to their past is foundational to their well
being
What is remembered and how it is remembered becomes
important
How it is re-narrated and its effect on self and others
If we extend this principle to:
larger entities, national groups etc., we observe that political
realities are often deeply affected, influenced even bound by the
past.
Some groups may appear unable to free themselves of the past
(see Hewer & Kut, 2010, Hewer, 2012)
The past is always with us!
The world does not simply receive the past, It is “haunted” by it
(Assmann, 1998; Frosh, 2013)
“Although the past no longer exists, it does not simply
disappear. It is subject to a dynamic process of re-engagement
with the present, transforming itself into a social and
psychological experience that shifts and evolves across
generations. (Hewer, 2017)
The social nature of memory
When a relative gets dementia we do not lose our memory or
identity – because our neurological systems are not connected
However, when we consider the content of memory, we might
draw the opposite conclusion? That despite having independent
memory systems, memories are often shared or depend on the
presence of others
The construction, reconstruction of memories is a feature of
everyday social interaction
A “socialised”memory!
A new concept for many psychology students
Memory as “lived experience” rather than an archive (Tileaga,
2014)
Shared, social or collective memory is not necessarily memory
in the conventional sense - the recall of personal experience.
Often, events directly experienced by others are passed on to us
- we were not there or not yet born.
The memory is socially shared through transmission not
personal experience, e.g. accounts of the First World War.
We construct cultural “remembrance”
Some conceptual issues
“At the one end of the imagined spectrum of approaches, taking
a social view of memory may mean simply recognising that
much of the content of individual remembering refers to things
that are social in character – social occasions, relationships,
conversational exchanges, shared endeavours, and so on. At the
other end, it might mean positing a “group mind” endowed with
a remembering capacity somehow distinct from that of group
members. In between are ranged a variety of ways of thinking of
memory as socially inflected or conditioned, interactive,
culturally mediated, shared or socially distributed. Middling
positions along this spectrum may be thought of as being
reached either by “socializing” the concept of the remembering
individual or by “writing” the individual back into collective
memory.”
(Cubitt 2014, p. 16).
Taxonomies and Classifications
No inter-disciplinary agreement – not everyone likes the term
“collective memory” – it vague and imprecise – has come to
replace “heritage” or “tradition”.
Some scholars use social and collective memory
interchangeably.
Cubitt (2007), uses social memory to refer to social processes
that sustain knowledge of past events within a group or culture
while collective memory is a very specific product of these
processes that presents a particular view or representation of the
past.
Cubitt also reminds us that memory is not an object with
ontological substance therefore we need to guard against
reifying psychological experience into social or psychological
constructs.
Early approaches
Early approaches to the study of collective memory found
expression within French sociology in the 1920s through the
work of Maurice Halbwachs (1877-1945). Halbwachs was
influenced by Emile Durkheim (1858-1917).
In Social Frameworks of Memory (1925), Halbwachs argued
that individual memories are acquired and recalled in a social
milieu and that group settings provide the context that
determines what and how we recall; these social frameworks
include the family, social class or nation.
This is consistent with Bartlett’s (1932) study of memory in
experimental psychology - individual memory reflected patterns
of thinking common to the social and cultural groups of the
participants.
Halbwachs also made a distinction between autobiographical
memory - personal events, which have been experienced
directly, and historical memory - events through which groups
construct a continuous identity over time.
Reinvigorated interest in the 1980s
French historian Pierre Nora reinvigorated interest the
collective past in the 1980s with his work Lieux de mémoire. He
claimed that memory and its values, customarily passed down
through family, school, church and state in France, were in
decline.
The disappearance of “peasant culture” as a source of collective
memory had marked a significant change in French society
leading to an erosion of “real memory” among people.
Nora cites the “Jews of the diaspora” as an example of a
community, which had no need for historians since memory was
lived through everyday tradition and cultural practice. In the
modern age, however, memory was being replaced by history –
the way “our hopelessly forgetful modern societies, propelled
by change, organize the past” (Nora, 1989, p. 8).
As postwar France became “deeply absorbed in its own
transformation and renewal” it valued “the new over the
ancient, the young over the old and the future over the past”
(Nora, 1989, p. 12). What remained, however, were lieux de
mémoire - sites of memory - people, places, institutions,
museums, works of art, holidays and events, which endorse and
encourage, among other things, the creation of archives and the
invention of anniversaries and celebrations.
Nora argued that because there was no memory left, the culture
had become preoccupied with it and the “memory boom” with
its tendency towards nostalgia and incessant memorialization
had taken up a role in contemporary culture (Nora, 1989, 2002).
What is the difference between history and collective
remembering?
Wertsch & Roediger, (2008) make a distinction between
collective memory - a static body of knowledge - and collective
remembering: an active reconstruction of the past, which takes
place in the present.
They also draw a line between history - the study of what
actually happened, which employs critical, complex and
reflective thought - and collective remembering - an identity
project that is intolerant of ambiguity or counter-narratives.
Wertsch & Roediger claim that: “History is willing to change a
narrative in order to be loyal to facts, whereas collective
remembering is willing to change information (even facts) in
order to be loyal to a narrative.” (2008, p. 324)
Selection, omission and distortion
History can be used to construct national identity, which often
involves promoting a particular version of the past that relies
upon and contributes to a knowledge deficit within the general
population (Baumeister & Hastings, 1997).
Different positioning on the past may reflect a degree of
collective narcissism where the narrative of the in-group
invokes a sense of entitlement and superiority (Golec de Zavala,
Cichocka, Eidelson & Jayawickreme, 2009).
Why was collective memory ignored by the Social Sciences
from 1945–1990?
Arguably, there was only one memory in the West - a
metanarrative of Allied victory over the Nazis.
Brown (2005) describes metanarratives as:
“grand, ideological generalized stories by which societies
understand themselves, and which are so normative and all-
consuming, that individuals in a society are not aware of them
as constantly re-circulated. Metanarratives only work when they
become invisible by having no acceptable opposites.” (p. 184).
Little thought was given to the existence of counter-memories
in, for example, the Soviet Union (see Wertsch, 2002) or other
parts of the Eastern bloc (see also Hewer & Kut, 2010) that
eventually emerged to challenge particular aspects of the Allied
account of WWII.
Political events
Eventually, the fall of the Berlin wall in 1989, and the Soviet
Union shortly after, provided the psychological and political
space for other suppressed memories to emerge from within the
Eastern bloc.
In the early 1990s, memory was at the root of political unrest
and ethnic hatred in the former Yugoslavia as nationalist
regimes sought to act upon a historical narrative of conflict,
separation, independence and dominance.
Even in Germany, after the peaceful unification of East and
West in 1992, some aspects of World War II were subject to
subtle differences of interpretation (von Benda-Beckmann,
2011).
Official and Vernacular Memory
State versions of the past, endorsed through education and
commemoration, may be at variance with versions of the past of
ordinary people (Tileagă, 2014).
The distinction between official and vernacular memory (the
memory of ordinary people) occurs in places where either an
authoritarian or hegemonic state has controlled memory, e.g.
Romania under Ciacescu during the communist period, or a
dominant memory silences people whose feelings are not
considered important.
Counter-memories cannot be expressed either because they
would be punished; or because such views are not welcome in
the current political climate, or the group has no “voice” – no
political power to make themselves heard.
Harald Welzer (2002) also refers to two contrasting modes of
memory: lexicon – e.g. the official account of Germany’s role
in World War Two –and album - the narratives of loss and
suffering among the German people, e.g. relatives of German
soldiers.
Communicative and cultural memory
Egyptologist, Jan Assmann uses the term mnemohistory to
describe any form of historical investigation concerned with the
way in which the past is remembered.
In his analysis of European memory of Ancient Egypt, he
Assmann proposes the concept of cultural memory: the idea that
“objectivized culture has the structure of memory” (Assmann &
Czaplicka, 1995, p. 128).
The Greeks viewed culture “not only as based on memory, but
as a form of memory in itself” (p. 15).
Cultural Memory
“Cultural memory is a form of collective memory, in the sense
that it is shared by a number of people and that it conveys to
these people a collective, that is, cultural, identity. Halbwachs,
the inventor of the term “collective memory”, was careful to
keep his concept of collective memory apart from the realm of
traditions, transmissions and transferences, which we propose to
subsume under the term “cultural memory”. We preserve
Halbwachs’s distinction by breaking up his concept of
collective memory into “communicative” and “cultural
memory”, but we insist on including the cultural sphere, which
he excluded in his study of memory. We are therefore not
asking for replacing his idea of “collective memory” with
“cultural memory”: rather we distinguish between both forms as
two different modi memoranda, ways of remembering”.
(Assmann, 1998, p. 210)
Characteristics of communicative and cultural memory (after
Assmann, 1992)
Communicative
Content: Historical experiences in the
individual life course
Form: Informal, natural, based on interpersonal
communication
Media: Human memory immediate, experiences, oral
tradition
Time Structure: 80–100 years, 3-4 generations
Carriers: Contemporaries
Cultural
Myth of origin, archaic history and absolute past
Formal, festive and ceremonial
Recorded, objectified in writing, dancing,
pictures, etc.
Absolute past to mythical times
Professional carriers of tradition
How may collective memory be accessed and studied?
Collective memory has no ontological substance – and therefore
it is not helpful to think of it as stored or located in a three
dimensional space.
Sociologist Jeffrey Olick (1999) distinguishes between
collected memory, which refers to the measurement and
aggregation of individual memories, and collective memory,
which refers to symbols and deeper structures, which are not
reducible to individual psychological processes.
An aggregation approach (collected memory) may be limited.
Olick refers to Durkheim’s position that collective
representations transcend the individual insofar that they are not
dependent on any one individual for their existence. For
Durkheim, culture cannot be reduced to what goes on in the
minds of individuals (Olick, 2010). This may apply to collective
memory
Indeed, while individual memory may be traced back to neural
networks in the brain, there is no equivalent and naturally
occurring substrate or network for group memory. Some may
argue that individual group members comprise a substrate, but
this introduces a different set of assumptions and a different
methodological approach to the study of the collective past.
If collective memory cannot be reduced to what goes on within
the minds of individuals, then it may be inferred from
monuments, museums, street names, media images,
documentary, commemorative practices, libraries, archives and
institutions. Researchers have to decide where they stand on
this issue and to be aware of the implications for their own
research claims.
Social Representations of History
Social representations theory: Moscovici (1961/1976)
We might argue that representations of the past exist both in
people’s heads and in the culture – social representations
approach
e.g. Liu and Hilton (2005)
Dresden: 1991
Image and Iconography in Public Space
Memory as narrative
Narrative thinking is an alternative and complementary form of
human cognition to scientific thinking. It employs a plot, a
theme and an object lesson.
Story telling is an integral part of the construction of social
realities (Bruner, 1986, 1990).
Stories or narratives contain cause and effect explanations,
which are seldom subjected to test or refutation.
Narratives contain universal principles about the world. They
form the basis of assertion and argument: it is a way of thinking
that seeks correspondence and coherence they rather than truth
(László, 2003).
Memory through symbol, metaphor and personalisation
Josef Stalin
Transmission: Memory is a process not an object
Historical legacies through birth (Weissmark, 2004)
Story telling is transmission
Comedy – Springtime for Hitler, Hogan’s Heroes, Allo, Allo.
Journalism - TV, newspaper, blog
Arts - novels, films – dramatic licence
Time Conception
How can the past seem nearer as time passes? How can this
paradox be explained?
Paul Janet in William James Principles of Psychology. Time
perception is related to the length of the life course.
Linear time vs. psychological time. Time perception is
psychological not linear.
Psychological time – 1945 is only 70 years ago (3 life spans if
you are 23) – In 2045, when you are 53, it will be two life spans
– the distant event will have moved forward in psychological
time.
Linear time vs. traumatic time – “You would have thought
they’d have got over by now!” (Holocaust Exhibition: London,
2006)
The Politics of Memory
Who controls public memory?
Why should the history curriculum and the writing of history
textbooks be subject to public scrutiny? (Carretero, 2011)
Is it better to remember or forget historic violence?
How may resolving different histories and or collective
memories be achieved?
Discussion:
How long should we commemorate the war dead of World War
I? Why do you so answer?
How long should we commemorate the war dead of World War
II and why?
Who are we remembering and why? Who is we?
Is this “remembrance” likely to change?
Contents .docx
Contents
Foreword v
Grandpa Wasn’t a Nazi:
The Holocaust
in German Family Remembrance 1
Design of the Study 2
Making Sense of History:
Changes in Transmission
through the Generations 3
Cumulative Heroization 9 The Effects of Heroization 16
How the Jews Are Remembered:
“These People Had Money” 18
How “the Nazis” Are Remembered:
As Others 22
“Empty Talking” 24 A Broadly Representative Survey 25
Postscript: How the Study Was Received 27
Notes
30
iii
Foreword
As Americans and Germans have grappled with the terrifying
events of 1933-45, both have come to understand the importance
of preserving memory of the Holocaust and incorporating the
lessons of history in creat- ing democratic and tolerant
societies. Holocaust remembrance today in Germany takes many
forms: formal educational curricula, television pro- grams,
books, debates in the media, commemorative events, and the
recent opening of a major memorial in the heart of Berlin. Such
a widespread educational effort has borne results. A 2005 AJC
survey found that 77 per- cent of Germans questioned could
correctly identify Auschwitz, Dachau, and Treblinka—higher
than the percentages among American and British respondents—
and 79 percent thought that teaching about the Nazi exter-
mination of the Jews should be required in schools (about the
same as responses in the United States and Britain).
How does one measure the success of Holocaust education? The
research study summarized in this monograph by Prof. Harald
Welzer, director of the Center for Interdisciplinary Memory
Research at the Insti- tute for Cultural Studies in Essen,
suggests that what is learned cognitively is not always absorbed
into the heart. Welzer interviewed forty Western and Eastern
German families, both in a family setting and individually, to
discover how they interpreted their objective knowledge of the
history of the Third Reich in terms of their own family history.
He found that the history transmitted through intergenerational
conversation was quite dif- ferent from the textbook history of
the Holocaust period.
Almost all the younger Germans interviewed believed that their
own grandparents had not been involved or had opposed the
racist policies of Nazism. Even when grandparents admitted
their participation in Nazi crimes, their offspring did not “hear”
them or reinterpreted the stories in ways that turned their
forebears into heroes. This process of “cumulative heroization”
reflects the natural tendency to associate positive elements and
block out negative ones for the people we love.
But there is danger lurking in this all-too-human phenomenon.
First, the story passed down by successive generations
increasingly whitewashes
v
vi Foreword
and distorts history, so that not turning in one’s family doctor,
who was Jewish, as opposed to hiding him, or giving a glass of
milk to passing refugees after the war was over, is portrayed as
a heroic act of resistance. Second, by separating the evil of
Nazism from the good qualities associated with beloved
relatives, the respondents managed to distinguish between the
“Nazis,” who are “others,” and the “Germans” who were
themselves victims of Nazism.
All this suggests that, despite extensive, in-depth Holocaust
education, both in formal settings and in the general culture, the
phenomena of “my grandpa was not a Nazi” and of “politically
correct talk” remain problemat- ic. The good news, on the other
hand, is that this study made a significant impact upon
publication in Germany and entered into the public dis- course
about the residual effects of the war years. From this
discussion, we hope will emerge a more probing appraisal of the
past and a greater vigi- lance toward the future.
The American Jewish Committee has had a long and significant
rela- tionship with Germany since AJC’s founding in 1906,
mainly by Ameri- can Jews of German origin. Shortly after
World War II, AJC became the first Jewish organization to
develop programs with Germany, working to promote
democratization and to spur awareness of the Holocaust. Some
twenty-five years ago, the Konrad Adenauer Foundation and
AJC initiated a series of exchanges between American Jews and
Germans, building bridges by visiting each other’s countries
and engaging in intense dialogue. Since then, AJC has
collaborated with other German political foundations as well. In
1998, AJC became the first American Jewish organization to
open a permanent office in Berlin, the Lawrence and Lee Ramer
Center for German-Jewish Relations.
The Berlin office brought this important study to our attention,
and engaged Belinda Cooper to translate it into English. She
performed her task with skill. In sharing Prof. Welzer’s research
with an English-speaking audience, we are attempting to make a
wider public aware of the complex interplay of national and
family historical memories. From this under- standing, we can
grapple more effectively with the complexities of the past while
striving for a brighter future.
David A. Harris
Executive Director, American Jewish Committee August 2005
Grandpa Wasn’t a Nazi: The Holocaust in German Family
Remembrance1
In Germany, Holocaust education in all its forms is very popular
and suc- cessful. Students learn about Nazism and the Shoah
from school curricula, participate in commemorative programs,
and make trips to Holocaust sites. Surveys show that young
Germans are generally quite well informed about the historical
events and can correctly define key terms such as “Auschwitz”
and “SS.” Thus education on the history of the Third Reich
might be considered a success—if one doesn’t ask how the
young targets of this educational campaign actually use the
product. Knowledge and the assimilation of knowledge on a
personal basis are two very different things.
For too long, the tacit assumption was that one needed only to
trans- mit the right message for the lessons to be assimilated.
There was little appreciation for the range of subtexts—
fascinating, daunting, and anes- thetizing—that accompany the
transmission of history. As this study shows, young Germans
acquire knowledge of history in general, and of Nazism and the
Holocaust in particular, in a way very different from what their
educators have intended.
A young person’s awareness of history and his concepts about
the past come from many sources: films, television, novels,
comics, computer games, and family histories, among others.
History lessons are but one source among many. Formal courses
aim to pass on knowledge, but cannot compete with the
emotional impact of images from the past offered by more
immediate sources. Cognitive knowledge of history pales beside
the emotional relationship to the past that come from one’s own
grandparents talking about their lives “before our time.”
Surprisingly, research on histor- ical consciousness has only
recently delved into these other sources.2
Research on the effects of Holocaust education has only slowly
broad- ened its scope. Until recently, the research consisted of
objective studies of historical awareness3 and qualitative
studies of the ways in which young Germans dealt with the
history of this unparalleled crime.4 The questions asked more
recently are not about knowledge of history alone, but about the
use of such knowledge—for example, how history lessons on
Nazism teach students the politically correct way to talk about
the Third Reich,5
1
2 Grandpa Wasn’t a Nazi
how immigrant children absorb history lessons that have little
relevance for their group of origin,6 or how trips to historical
sites impart messages about the past.7
These new studies, while exploratory and limited in scope,
suggest some distressing conclusions. The present
multigenerational study observes the direct communication of
concepts about the past in German families and finds a
pronounced discrepancy between the official and the private
cultures of remembrance in Germany. It documents a clear
tenden- cy on the part of grandchildren to rewrite their
grandparents’ histories into tales of anti-Nazi heroism and
resistance. The pilot study on history teach- ing by Radtke et al.
(2002) suggests that students learn one primary thing in classes
on the Third Reich: how to talk in a politically correct way
about the problematic past. Gudehus’s investigation produces
similar results, and Georgi’s study comes to the remarkable
conclusion that immigrant chil- dren use their study of the Nazi
past as a ticket to seeing themselves as “true Germans.”8
Overall, the beneficiaries of these efforts in historical education
have shown themselves stubborn and unpredictable, which is
why further research on the outcome of history teaching appears
both nec- essary and promising.
Design of the Study
The research project, entitled “Transmitting Historical
Awareness,” dealt with family communication about the Nazi
period in the Federal Republic of Germany. For the study, forty
Western and Eastern German families were interviewed by
researchers both in the context of a whole family dis- cussion
and in separate interviews with at least one member of the
eyewit- ness, child, and grandchild generations in the family.9
The design of the study was quite simple: The members of the
eyewitness generation were asked about their biographical
experience during the wartime period after 1933; then their
children and grandchildren were asked what they had heard
from their parents and grandparents about the same period. A
family discussion was triggered with a brief video of amateur
footage from the Third Reich period.
A total of 182 interviews and family discussions were
conducted. The material was transcribed and evaluated through
a combination of textual
analysis of individual transcripts and computer-based
qualitative content analysis.
The following examples drawn from the interview material will
illus- trate how history is transmitted through conversation
among the genera- tions, how anti-Jewish stereotypes are
similarly passed down, and how Germans interpret the roles of
their parents or grandparents in the Third Reich. The conclusion
will examine how this study entered into public dis- course in
Germany.
Making Sense of History:
Changes in Transmission through the Generations
Johanna Kurz10 was born in 1927. Her father was in the SA
(Sturmab- teilung, the “brown shirts”) and the SS
(Schutzstaffel, originally Hitler’s per- sonal guard), both elite
units within the German military, and her mother had been in
the Nazi women’s organization for a short time, but quit “after
two or three years.”
Johanna Kurz: I only know that we stared at those smoking
ruins.... The synagogue wasn’t destroyed; it was just burned
out. Everything was smolder- ing, and my mother almost went
crazy. She said, “How can they do that?” It was like a church
for her. But it wasn’t just in Hanover; on the contrary, it was
everywhere, you know. And I remember that my mother said to
my father, “I know you were involved; don’t talk to me ever
again!”
Interviewer: But he wasn’t involved, or was he?
Johanna Kurz: I don’t know. I don’t think so, but I don’t know.
I’d like—I
can’t say, I don’t know.
Interviewer: So it was just an expressed threat?
Johanna Kurz: The two of them never came together again, and
then the war broke out, and the marriage just went on the side,
nothing violent. When he came home in 1947, he came back
from prison, and in 1948 they divorced.
Earlier in the interview Johanna Kurz had revealed the fact of
her father’s SA and SS membership. When she got to talking
about the burn- ing synagogue, however, the interviewer, born
in 1971, could not believe that Ms. Kurz’s father was
“involved.” The interviewer’s suggestive follow- up
considerably shook Ms. Kurz’s confidence, and her uncertainty
seems to have strengthened the interviewer in her views: The
interviewer’s inter-
Making Sense of History 3
4 Grandpa Wasn’t a Nazi
pretation that it “was just ... a threat” caused Ms. Kurz to
answer (but at the same time not answer) by reporting the
result. It was not just a threat; apparently, the conflict was
severe enough that her parents “never came together again.”
The interviewer, who noted in her report of the interview that
she had found Johanna Kurz extremely nice, apparently could
not accept the possi- bility that Johanna’s father was an actor in
Kristallnacht. Despite her in- depth knowledge of the history of
the Third Reich, the interviewer resisted the possibility that
even a relative of the old lady she was interviewing could have
been a fellow traveler, much less a perpetrator, of the persecu-
tion of Jews.
This example illustrates how quickly loyalty ties become
generalized within the social situation of a conversation. The
interviewer, with her hopeful follow-up question, was not only
trying to absolve her dialogue partner of any suspicion of
complicity in criminal activity, but was extend- ing the cover to
close relatives of the interviewee, whom she neither knew nor
could have known. It was as if she had not at all absorbed what
Ms. Kurz had just told her about her father’s history. This
phenomenon could often be found in family discussions—even,
or maybe especially—when the explicit theme was murders
committed by the storyteller.
Rainer Hofer, born in 1925, was a NAPOLA (an elite national
socialist school) student and a member of the Waffen-SS and of
the SS-unit “Leib- standarte Adolf Hitler” (named for Hitler’s
personal bodyguard unit). He presented himself, both in the
individual interview and the family discus- sion, as a reformed
Nazi. Although he wrote in his diary, at the news of the
Führer’s death, “My best comrade has fallen,” in his
retrospective telling, he was appalled by the Nazi crimes. Mr.
Hofer, well-read and well-educat- ed, made a career in postwar
Germany as a manager and, in his own way, contributed to the
rebuilding of the state. He speaks unself-consciously about
joining the Waffen-SS, entering the Leibstandarte, participating
in the Russian campaign, and, in 1943, being deployed as an SS
man in the Ukraine. In this connection, the interviewer asks:
Interviewer: Are there any stories that you wouldn’t tell your
daughter or your grandchildren?
Rainer Hofer: No, I would be completely open. I don’t need to
tell them that I shot Jews [he bangs on the table] or that sort of
thing; even if I had done it,
I would tell about it. Why? It’s my daughter and I lived my life.
I can’t let any of it somehow sink into the Hades of the past....
There’s nothing ... I wouldn’t tell her, even if it touched on the
honor of German soldiers. I remember once that we rode to an
attack, and when we came back, attached to infantry, a couple
of Russian soldiers were idiotic enough to surrender. Of course,
they didn’t live a moment longer [knocks on the table]. But that,
of course, was one of those things: Where were they supposed
to ride with us? In the tank? They could have had a hand
grenade hidden somewhere [laughs].... If they had just laid low,
nothing would have happened.
But I’d tell my daughter, even though it actually touched on the
honor of German soldiers. I can’t say there was anything that I
wouldn’t tell her or my granddaughter either. Why should I?
As if to prove his openness, Hofer describes to the interviewer a
crime that might sully “the honor of German soldiers”—but
without questioning the crime from today’s perspective. On the
contrary, Hofer provides a jus- tification for the murder of the
Russian prisoners, assuming that his calcu- lus would be
obvious, even to the interviewer. Anyway, this is all part of his
life as he lived it—so why, asks Hofer rhetorically, shouldn’t he
talk about it?
As further evidence of Mr. Hofer’s openness, the Hofer family
archive even contains letters he sent home from the Eastern
Front, one of which he alludes to in the interview:
Rainer Hofer: I’m horrified today about what I wrote then. What
[laughs] I can’t understand today.... We, of course, saw Russian
women on the oppo- site side, in uniform, with weapons and
armed and, imagine this, at [he knocks on the table] eighteen
years old, I shot one down with my machine gun and wrote very
proudly that “the head and the breast were just a bloody mess,”
or something like that. Today you wonder how you could have
writ- ten something like that.
Note that he questions not how he could do the killing, but how
he could write about it—a subjective assessment of the act. It
should be pointed out that it was not usual for such documents
to be kept in family archives and to be known to the children.
Generally, a rather nebulous for- mulation is used—e.g.,
“something happened”—leaving listeners to draw from vaguely
portrayed events a story that best allows them to live with the
central conflict of German family history a half-century after
the Third Reich: that is, the conflict between the awareness by
members of the chil-
Making Sense of History 5
6 Grandpa Wasn’t a Nazi
dren’s and grandchildren’s generations of the criminal nature of
Nazism and the Holocaust, on the one hand, and the need to
position their own parents or grandparents in a way that they
were untouched by the horror.
This is not an easy task, especially when, as in the case of
Rainer Hofer, the details of the crimes were written down.
Regina Seiler, his daughter, knows the letters, but surprisingly,
emphasizes repeatedly, in her individual interview and in the
family discussion, how important it is for her to “fig- ure out
what people were thinking back then.”
Regina Seiler: I can’t imagine that the German people, even my
father, ... I really think they couldn’t imagine that something
like that could happen.
Although Ms. Seiler refers directly to the letter in her interview
(“and in the war he wrote to his parents that they had just
attacked a Russian vil- lage; he was sixteen or eighteen years
old, I don’t remember exactly, and I was so upset by how
euphorically he talked about it”), she repeatedly spec- ulates
whether Germans could have “imagined” that something like a
war of extermination and a Holocaust could happen.
What motivates this question, which so obviously ignores the
fact that her father did not need to “imagine” crimes that he
himself committed? The question of whether the father “could
have imagined” functions to maneuver him out of the
perpetrator group, into the much less suspect group of
accidental witnesses or perhaps fellow travelers—since a person
imagines only that for which he has no personal knowledge.
Second, Ms. Seiler argues, very similarly to her father, within a
framework of a dual structure of knowledge and ignorance: The
crimes undoubtedly occurred, but no one could imagine them.
Her own father took part in them, but the daughter does not
acknowledge it.
How this dual structure functions in talking about the past is
shown in the family discussion. There Mr. Hofer relates that in
1944, after his deployment to the Ukraine, he was in a tank
division in France and heard from a Sturmbannführer “that in
the East, in Russia, partisans in any case, but also other people,
were killed by a shot to the neck and so forth. I remember
[laughs] that we talked about this later among the comrades, and
we thought, ‘He’s crazy!’”
Having done the exact same thing half a year before, Mr. Hofer
now states he couldn’t believe others were doing this. It would
be an underesti-
mation of the effect of this dual structure to say that Hofer is
lying. The fact that he tells about both his own deeds and his
disbelief at the deeds of others shows that he can integrate his
own deeds subjectively into a mean- ingful context of
rationality and morality, and, as such, he excludes them from
the overall accusation of criminality.
Thus, perpetrators such as Mr. Hofer do not regard their actions
as part of the Holocaust—and this self-perception provides an
interpretative option readily embraced by the following
generations. In the group discus- sion Ms. Seiler concentrates
with quite penetrating questions on finding out from her father
why no one could “imagine” what was happening— thereby
protecting him from the knowledge that he and she herself
actual- ly share. In the Hofer family interaction, what is
transmitted is not knowledge of the crime, but rather knowledge
of how one can at the same time know and not know.
Contrary to the widespread notion that grandparents and parents
do not tell their children and grandchildren problematic wartime
stories— especially ones that highlight their participation in
Nazi crimes—some of the interviewees do talk about their
experiences during the war in ways that show them as
perpetrators. This does not, however, lead their listeners to
dismay, to conflicts, or even to embarrassing situations. It leads
to noth- ing at all. It is as if such tales were not heard by the
family members pres- ent. Apparently, ties of family loyalty
prevent a father or grandfather from appearing to be someone
who killed people a few decades earlier. The images formed
about a beloved relative through socialization and time spent
together are retroactively applied to the earlier period in the
family member’s life as well, before his offspring, who are now
listening and will later pass on the wartime stories, were born.
The ignoring of perpetrator stories occurs accidentally, as if on
automatic pilot—the tape recorder records the stories, but the
family’s memory does not. In other words, wartime memories
are preserved in the family’s lore as stories that can be reshaped
to an idealized vision that succeeding generations have of the
eye- witness who is telling them. And so they are remembered
and retold.
Moving down the generations, stories become so altered that in
the end they undergo a complete change of meaning. This
reconfiguration generally functions to turn grandparents into
people of constant moral
Making Sense of History 7
8 Grandpa Wasn’t a Nazi
integrity, according to today’s standards and normative
appraisal. The reformulation of stories is undertaken precisely
because, in interviews, most members of the children’s and
grandchildren’s generations express no doubt at all that Nazism
was a criminal system and the Holocaust an unparalleled crime.
This assessment of the Nazi past—the standard fare of history
lessons, the media, and the official German culture of
commemo- ration—breaks down when questioning the role
played by one’s own grandparents during the period; it rather
evokes the subjective need to assign one’s grandfather or
grandmother the role of the “good” German in everyday life
under the Nazis.
Thus the paradoxical result of successful education about the
Nazi past comes to this: The more comprehensive the
knowledge about war crimes, persecution, and extermination,
the stronger is the need to develop stories to reconcile the
crimes of “the Nazis” or “the Germans” with the moral integrity
of parents or grandparents.
This reconciliation can only be achieved through stories that
show one’s relatives as human beings who perhaps cautiously,
but also coura- geously, defied contemporary norms and worked
against the system, even if, based on their party membership
and functions, they were anything but opponents of the system.
The eyewitnesses appear in the retellings of their descendants to
be inconspicuous resistance fighters, clever enough to blend in
from the outside but, when push came to shove, ready to help
victims of persecution, hide Jews, or carry out small acts of
resistance.
These stories of “being against” the system are embedded in the
idea that any nonconformist behavior, from “opening one’s
mouth” to “protect- ing Jews,” from continuing to “buy from
Jews” to showing opposition to superiors, could potentially
have brought the harshest consequences. Thus, from the
perspective of their progeny, grandparents who acted
courageous- ly found themselves chronically in danger of career
setbacks, family con- flicts, concentration camp, or even death
sentences as a result of their views and behavior.
Thus the seventeen-year-old grandson of the Groothe family
defends his forebears:
Lars Groothe: I think in any case that most people thought that,
for example, Jews ... are people.... But, as one individual you
couldn’t defend yourself.
As one person, you couldn’t do anything. You could say, “I
think it’s bad,” [but] you’d be locked up and probably shot.
This interpretation allows a synthesis between the image of a
totalitari- an system with its coercive methods and the
reinterpreted grandparents’ roles. It is the product of an
intergenerational chain whereby, in many sto- ries told by the
eyewitness generation, their parents are described as people
who were “against” the system. The rehabilitation of the great-
grandpar- ents’ generation as anti-Nazi can go so far as to
portray an “old fighter” and a “staunch Nazi” who was a Nazi
Ortsgruppenleiter [local political leader] in 1931 as someone
who was always ready to oppose the norms of Nazi soci- ety,
for example, by continuing to shop “from Jews,” by doing
“business with the Jewish cattle trader,” and finally, according
to his great-grandson, “hiding” Jews.
Cumulative Heroization
The term “cumulative heroization” describes the phenomenon of
history becoming ameliorated from generation to generation.
Cumulative heroiza- tion appeared in twenty-six of the forty
families interviewed—that is, in two-thirds of all cases.
Heroizing stories made up roughly 15 percent of all stories told
in the interviews and family discussions; stories of forebears’
victimization accounted for around 50 percent; thus two-thirds
of all the stories were about family members from the
eyewitness generation (or their relatives) who were either
victims of the Nazi past and/or heroes of everyday resistance.
Like her sixty-five-year-old son Bernd Hoffmann, ninety-one-
year-old Elli Krug insists in the individual interview and in the
family discussion that she did not know what a concentration
camp was until the end of the war, even though she lived near
Bergen-Belsen. After the war, however, for- mer inmates of the
camp passed through her village, and Mrs. Krug was forced by
the British occupiers to make her home available to them—a sit-
uation that clearly displeased her.
Elli Krug: The Jews were the worst afterwards. They really
harassed us.... They sat there and made us serve them, and then
they didn’t want.... We had this big hayloft [where] they slept
overnight. ... The Jews and Russians, I always made sure that I
didn’t get them. They were really disgusting, you
Cumulative Heroization 9
10 Grandpa Wasn’t a Nazi
know? I always stood down the street, in front of the gate, and
when they said, “Quarters,” I said, “No, everything’s full!” If
the Jews ... came, I said, “It’s all full of Russians, you can come
in with me!” ... And when the Rus- sians came, then I said the
same thing, that there were Jews here.
Mrs. Krug still recalls how she was able to avoid giving shelter
to “Jews” and “Russians” through a trick, while the
characteristics she attributes to them (“the worst,” “disgusting”)
indicate a clear anti-Semitic or racist atti- tude, even today. The
fact that she is speaking about accommodating pris- oners who
had survived the nearby Bergen-Belsen concentration camp is
not an issue for her at all. The main theme of her story is the
burden that she took on by providing shelter and her clever
technique for keeping the “Jews” and the “Russians” out of her
yard.
Her son also relates that people did not know about the camps
until the end of the war, but he tells a story he heard from his
deceased wife. She had worked on an estate near Bergen-Belsen
and heard that the owner hid escapees from the camp. Bernd
Hoffmann called this person “the grand- ma.”
Bernd Hoffmann: She [his wife] was on a farm in Belsen for a
year. They came right by there. The grandma hid some of them,
and they sat in a wood- en box. Then [the SS-men] went around,
searching everywhere.... They would have shot the grandma
immediately. She put a hot pot with boiling potatoes on top of
the wooden box so they wouldn’t get them.
The twenty-six-year-old granddaughter, Silvia Hoffmann, now
tells her version of what her own grandmother did:
Silvia Hoffmann: Once she told a story I thought was really
interesting: Our village was on the road to Bergen-Belsen, and
she hid someone who escaped from one of those transports, in a
really interesting way, in some grain box with straws sticking
out—she really hid them. Then people came and looked in her
farmyard and she kept quiet. That’s a little thing that I really
give her a lot of credit for.
Her story pieces together elements that were mentioned in her
grand- mother’s and her father’s separate stories: The “road to
Bergen-Belsen,” a stout-hearted woman, the box, even the
haystack, in the form of a straw, have all left a mark on the
granddaughter’s story. But the narrative matrix in which the
actors now appear produces a new story: The estate-owner
grandmother is adopted, wooden boxes and all, and the hay
becomes a
dramatic element in a tale of how her own grandmother tricked
the perse- cutors. Thus, the granddaughter creates an image for
herself of a good grandmother, based on elements present in
neither her grandmother’s nor her father’s stories.
Cumulative heroization occurs rapidly and simply. A
generalized image of a respected grandmother or grandfather
provides a framework in which any point of reference suggested
by family stories can be expanded into a “good story.” As in the
case of Silvia Hoffmann, this can result in a strip- ping away of
the problematic implications of the true tale; plots become
rearranged to reshape the nuanced, ambivalent, often troubling
tales by the eyewitnesses into a morally clear attitude on the
part of the protagonists— a clearly positive one. The tendency
to heroize the grandparents’ genera- tion shows the never-to-be-
underestimated strong effects of ties of loyalty to loved ones on
historical awareness and the retrospective construction of the
past.
In the telling the Grubitsch family history, Sieglinde Grubitsch,
born in 1907, relates:
Sieglinde Grubitsch: But our Dr. Weinberg was a Jew, and his
wife was a teacher; we protected them; they could live until the
end.
This strong statement implies that the grandparents saved the
Wein- berg family from deportation (without, however,
specifying what “protec- tion” consisted of and what was meant
by the family “could live until the end.”11) The grandson, Erich
Grubitsch, Jr., born in 1962, asks a follow-up question: “How
did you protect them?” Whether his question resonated with
skepticism or merely asked for more detail is not clear. The
answer:
Sieglinde Grubitsch: Well, because we never bothered them. We
never felt bothered by them, and they didn’t disturb us. We
didn’t, like those patriots, say, “There are Jews here; we don’t
want anything to do with them.” Or, “Take them away!”
Compared with the statement about “protecting” the Jewish
family, this explanation is sobering in its narrowness. Sieglinde
Grubitsch reports only not “bothering” the Jewish family—a
remarkable comment, since ear- lier she had spoken
empathically of “our Dr. Weinberg.” The “protection” extended
to the Weinberg family, it turns out, consisted merely of not
denouncing them, as “the patriots” might have done. The
Grubitsch fami-
Cumulative Heroization 11
12 Grandpa Wasn’t a Nazi
ly didn’t do anything—and this, from her perspective, was
worth mention- ing, if not calling an act of resistance.
In the individual interview, Erich Grubitsch, Jr., defends this
interpre-
tation:
Erich Grubitsch, Jr.: It was a totalitarian regime. Who knows
what we would have done? ... On the other hand, they
supposedly rescued a couple of Jews. That has to be
acknowledged. OK, we can’t understand everything today— but
at least they tried, or actually managed to do it.
This assessment seizes on the often-repeated belief that it is not
possi- ble to judge behavior under totalitarianism from today’s
perspective. The grandson’s formulation that his grandparents
“supposedly” rescued “a cou- ple of Jews” retains some
skepticism, but the triggering statement, which claimed nothing
more than the failure to denounce, now leads to an
acknowledgment that “at least they tried, or actually managed to
do it.” Once again, the desire to find moral integrity—or, even
better, opposition- al behavior—on the part of the grandparents
leads the offspring to ignore the actual content of the original
tale. After all, not feeling “bothered” by the presence of a
Jewish family—especially when the father was the Gru-
bitsches’ family doctor—is hardly something to be especially
proud of. Ignoring the problematic aspects, the grandchild
leaves open the question of whether the grandparents actually
“saved” the doctor’s family or just “tried” to. In either case,
they would have done something that deserves acknowledgment
by present-day normative standards.
A very similar statement by granddaughter Tina Kunze (born in
1980) occurs in conversation with the Ross family:
Tina Kunze: ...that’s why grandpa might have hidden the two
Jews, like Mama said before.
In the individual interview with Tina Kunze’s father, this
episode was related as follows:
Gerhard Kunze: Once a train went by with prisoners, and people
stayed at the farm once for a short time, people who didn’t
know where else to go, but there was always great danger
connected with that.
In the story as transmitted by Gerhard Kunze (born in 1951),
those who did the acting were “people” who “stayed” at the
grandparents’ farm. The context of persecution and deportation
is established by mention of
the “train with prisoners,” “people who didn’t know where else
to go,” and the “great danger connected with that.” The latter
suggests, in a subtle transformation, that the actions of the
people had endangered the grand- parents. The father’s version
of the story is more ambiguous than the granddaughter’s much
more decisive variant, in which the active ones are the
grandparents who “maybe hid two Jews.”
The eyewitness Margarethe Ross, born in 1919, recounts the
following:
Margarethe Ross: What can I tell you? At night someone
knocked at our door, and there were two concentration camp
women ... the war was proba- bly already over ... and they
knocked at my mother’s door. My mother took them in, and I
think gave them a glass of milk first, and let them sleep in the
house.
The eyewitness account reveals that it was not Tina Kunze’s
“grandpa” but her great-grandmother who was involved, and
that the story of the two Jews she “might have hidden”
happened at the end of the war. They were probably displaced
persons to whom her great-grandmother offered shel- ter. The
episode—in the granddaughter’s view a courageous act of
“hiding” and in the father’s view entailing “great danger”—is
revealed here, in Mar- garethe Ross’s tale, to be an extension of
help involving no danger whatso- ever. Without belittling the
great-grandmother’s act of kindness, one can say that it has
been heroized through successive generations.
Similar exaggeration through retelling is found everywhere in
our material, sometimes as complex stories, sometimes as small
references or fragments of tales. All enjoy the same trajectory:
Harmless, often question- able, and sometimes even scandalous
acts recounted by the eyewitnesses are mediated more abstractly
through the second generation, with details changed and opened
to interpretation; these accounts are then clarified in the
grandchildren’s generation into a finding that the grandparents
“helped,” “hid,” or “saved,” even if it was dangerous for them.
The stress on danger functions as a backdrop to the passing on
of fam- ily history: The dangerous situation highlights more
emphatically the courageous behavior of the forebears. The
invention of ethically motivated behavior by the grandparents
harmonizes with the image the grandchil- dren already have of
them, while reinforcing their knowledge of the absolute tyranny
of the Third Reich, which could turn anyone—even one’s
Cumulative Heroization 13
14 Grandpa Wasn’t a Nazi
own grandparents—into victims. The grandchildren attempt to
preserve this benign image of their relatives at any price.
The Eeven family, too, has passed down a heroic story that goes
back to the great-grandfather, who is portrayed as a notorious
opponent of the Nazi system. Else Eeven, born 1922, says:
Else Eeven: My father was a total opponent.... Already in 1934
he said, “This is leading to war.”... And when the war started,
he said, “One year, then we won’t win! They’re going much,
much too fast through all of Europe; they’ll collapse.”
In the family discussion, her father’s critical attitude, which
applied, at least in this excerpt, not to Nazism per se but to the
war, was addressed again by Ms. Eeven:
Else Eeven: But what my father said every day, I heard so many
things; I did- n’t tell anyone.
Her husband, Albert Eeven, adds:
Albert Eeven: Well, that was extreme. He was really lucky. Else
Eeven: That they didn’t take him away.
Their daughter Claudia Eeven, born in 1949, later in the family
dis- cussion, notes: “Grandpa was arrested again. Because he
didn’t, because he said something that the government.... He
was arrested for two or three days.”
To this her father adds: “Yes, he was once. Yes, he had talked a
little bit too loud, and someone heard him, and the police came
and took him away.”
But a little later Albert Eeven relates that the arrest was
occasioned by the violation of blackout rules, not criticism of
the system, and payment of a fine of 25 marks averted any
further consequences. That this version completely contradicts
the story told only a few minutes earlier creates no stir in the
family discussion nor occasions any retrospective correction.
In the individual interview, Claudia Eeven speaks about her
grandfa- ther as “incredibly critical” and “overly critical.” She
suggests, “I think my mother didn’t have an easy time because
of that.” While Claudia Eeven implies the her father’s critical
remarks created a context of permanent danger to the family,
her son Thomas Eeven, born in 1969, picks up that
his relatives, fortunately, were far from being “yes-men,” and
states what this means to him:
Thomas Eeven: Ultimately it’s positive, of course. One would
have wished that there were more aware Germans at the time. It
makes me personally happy ... that in my family ... there were
people who didn’t just staunchly yell “Heil Hitler.”
This deeply held conviction on the part of the grandson that he
came from a family that had always, even in the great-
grandparents’ generation, been against Nazism led to an absurd
interchange in the family discussion, when the grandfather,
Albert Eeven, asked his grandchildren to ask him critical
questions:
Albert Eeven: Why don’t you ask, “Grandpa, why did you go
along with it all? Why didn’t you do that?” Why don’t you ask
that question?
Thomas Eeven: Oh, Grandpa!
Instead of asking Albert Eeven the question he’d like to hear—
perhaps because he wonders about it himself, perhaps because
he would like the opportunity to justify himself—his daughter
Claudia and his grandson Thomas lecture him as to why his
question makes no sense. Claudia argues from socialization
theory, while the grandson finds other excuses:
Claudia Eeven: But you didn’t protest because you had it drilled
into you: obey and “everything has its system.” I mean, you
can’t compare that with education today!
Thomas Eeven: Grandpa, that’s the simplest question you can
ask.... The answers have so many levels and the stories that
you’ve told are so varied that I can’t ask a question like that. I
know too much about it already for that.
Albert Eeven has nothing to say to this and is silent. As absurd
as this dialogue seems—the eyewitness wants to be critically
questioned and his descendants refuse, explaining why it is not
necessary—it illustrates clearly how knowledge of the Nazi past
creates paradoxical effects in the family: To Claudia Eevens,
information about the authoritarian educational style of that
time—the “drill” and indoctrination—appears sufficient to
explain why her father “went along with it all.” Thomas Eeven
believes that he has so much knowledge of Nazi history that
simple questions and answers seem improper to him: “I know
too much about it already.” Knowledge functions to make its
possessors not want to know any more—particularly
Cumulative Heroization 15
16 Grandpa Wasn’t a Nazi
about family history and its connection to Nazism. This is a
paradoxical consequence of comprehensive education about the
Nazi past and the crimes connected with it. The deeper the
knowledge of history, the greater the subjectively felt need to
protect one’s own family from this knowl- edge—thus removing
them from the historical context about which one knows so
much.
That an eyewitness attempts to be critically questioned is itself
part of the educational process and the culture of
commemoration established over the last two decades. The
grandfather is never asked the question, however, because the
family discourse offers enough points of reference to create the
image of a transgenerational anti-Nazi family, which will now
be preserved into the future. This image seems all the more
reassuring and in contrast to the prevailing norms against the
background of knowledge of the horror of the period. As
Thomas Eeven suggests in the separate inter- view: “Of course,
one would have wished that there were more aware Ger- mans at
the time”—like his grandparents and great-grandparents, that is.
The Effects of Heroization
What conclusions can we draw from the tendency toward
cumulative heroization? One finding, not unimportant to the
pedagogy of history, is that education that delivers a
comprehensive historical knowledge of Nazi crimes
paradoxically evokes a need to remove one’s relatives from this
framework of knowledge. This result need not be assessed only
negatively. Out of the revised history of heroism, resistance,
and civil courage on the part of the grandparents, one can derive
the practical conclusion that indi- vidual resistance is possible
and sensible, even in a totalitarian context— that it is,
emphatically, a question of individual responsibility. To this
extent, the stories of oppositional grandparents and great-
grandparents, regardless of their truth, can serve as examples
motivating people to act courageously when others around them
are threatened or persecuted. In addition, it is clear that a
majority of the grandchildren look favorably on those who
opposed the Nazis—only four of the forty-four grandchildren
interviewed indicated any admiration for or affirmation of “the
Nazis.”
Cumulative heroization, however, has a very different
significance for the historical image of Nazism and the
Holocaust. It represents a restora- tion of the belief, thought to
be long uprooted, that “the Nazis” and “the Germans” were two
different groups; thus it follows that “the Germans” can be seen
to have been seduced, abused, and robbed of their youth, and
they can see themselves as victims of Nazism. That this
historical model apparently holds a secure place in the cultural
memory of the Federal Republic is demonstrated by the
renaissance of historical dramas based on the home-front
perspective, such as the German public television channel ZDF
series “Hitler’s Helpers” and “Hitler’s Children.” While the
Holo- caust has been commemorated in the framework of
international confer- ences such as the 2000 Stockholm
Holocaust Conference, and anniversaries of liberation have
established a liturgical rhetoric of confes- sion and
responsibility, the historical and political contexts that gave rise
to Nazism seem to be disappearing from German historical
awareness.
What is becoming lost is the awareness that it was possible, in a
civi- lized twentieth-century society, with the active
participation of the over- whelming majority of a well-educated
population, to exclude a part of this same population from the
universe of obligations,12 to see them as harmful and
“worthless,” to look on while they were deported, and to accept
their extermination. This is not to speak of the actual
perpetrators (some of whom appear in our interviews) whose
willingness to murder and largely unproblematic reintegration
into postwar West German society recedes from the collective
memory.
The phenomenon of cumulative heroization shows how deep,
emo- tionally based views of the roles of close relatives affect
an individual’s awareness of history, and how detached
cognitive knowledge of history can be. The subjective synthesis
consists of removing one’s own ancestors from one’s knowledge
of history by heroizing them—thus bringing the “evil” of Nazi
rule and the “good” of one’s own grandparents and great-
grandpar- ents into peaceful coexistence.
The Effects of Heroization 17
18 Grandpa Wasn’t a Nazi
How the Jews Are Remembered: “These People Had Money”
How do the formally learned concepts and interpretive patterns
affect cur- rent family discourse about the Third Reich? What is
considered self-evi- dent in family discourse about the Nazi
period differs in many ways from the picture of the Third Reich
portrayed in encyclopedias and history books, and from what
would seem, from the public debate on the past, to be anchored
in cultural memory. This difference can be observed through a
comparison of families in East Germany and West Germany.
Although the official view of history in the former East
Germany was clearly distinct from that in West Germany,13 the
East German families interviewed rarely used different
interpretive patterns and concepts in talking about the Third
Reich from those of their West German counterparts. Regardless
of which encyclopedias or history books were found on the
family bookshelves, within the family discourse, there were
many similarities.
For example, many members of the eyewitness generation still
speak about “the Jews” today in alarmingly anti-Semitic and
racist terms. They make it clear that the discourse established
over the last two decades on the Holocaust and Nazi crimes has
left few traces on the eyewitness genera- tion, at least in this
respect: “Jews” are, in any case, not Germans; the crimes
committed against them were committed against “others,”
strangers; and it was partly as a result of their own behavior
that they became objects of hatred, exclusion, persecution, and
extermination. From the interview process, it becomes clear that
stereotypical images enter into contemporary perceptions, which
can be traced directly to Nazi propagan- da and to later
published film documents from the extermination cam- paign in
the East. For example, Lore Renz, born in 1916, remembers
seeing a “troop of old Jews.”
Lore Renz: They were banded together there.... Some of them
had these beards, long beards, and ... I never forgot ... how
these old men were stand- ing there. They had on big black hats,
like the Jews have, you know.
Helene Stein, born in 1924, relates:
Helene Stein: And when you saw them running like that, with
those long beards, the smock and so forth, as a child, you were
a little afraid, and you thought, ”What kind of people are
these?”
Ms. Stein recalls that in 1945 she saw a train of concentration
camp prisoners:
Helene Stein: There were a lot of Jews among them, you can
always tell, you know. And also foreigners.
Interviewer: How could you tell?
Helene Stein: Well,... You can recognize Jews, real Jews, with
the nose and especially they all had beards, because they can’t
shave and so forth. They looked terrible. And they shot us hate-
filled looks.
Margarethe Haase, born in 1920, tells how in the 1920s her
father sometimes opened the paper in the morning and said,
“That Cohn has him on his conscience, too.” Her father’s
commentary was incomprehensi- ble to her at first, but later she
understood its meaning: “Later I under- stood how he [Cohn]
always lent a lot of money at usurious interest.”
The anti-Semitic stereotypes of the rich, usurious Jew as well as
that of “cowardly, submissive Jews” appear in the eyewitness
generation’s stories:
Margarethe Haase: These children, the Jew children, were
somehow different from us.... I can’t tell you how. One of the
boys, for example, was such a coward.... He was always afraid,
[saying] “Please don’t, please don’t,” and so submissive.
Ms. Haase repeats the boy’s fearful pleading without the
slightest hesi- tation. That his desperate pleas might have been
occasioned by an attack and that he might have had cause to be
afraid is not mentioned. Her description even today lacks
empathy; no awareness of injustice can be read into her words.
Anti-Semitic interpretive patterns also come up in the children’s
gener- ation, though in a more complex form that incorporates
the postwar peri- od’s discourse on the past and philo-Semitic
stereotypes.
Ella Drake: Those two very beautiful people were ... they had
something a bit Semitic about them. Like Jewish.
Erich Grubitsch, born in 1937, developed a complex argument
that identifies “the Jews” as a distinct group of people with
specific characteris- tics who must on the one hand be protected
from anti-Semitic prejudices, but on the other hand must be
protected from themselves:
How the Jews Are Remembered 19
20 Grandpa Wasn’t a Nazi
Erich Grubitsch: You know, sometimes this characteristic
appears that can verge on arrogance.... Maybe that’s because the
Jews sometimes perceive dis- crimination that isn’t even there.
And then they play off almost a cynical arrogance.... This pride,
that they sometimes reproach themselves for, ... must have a
protective function.... I ask myself why it’s expressed that way,
or why it’s shown as if they were better. That’s the danger of
creating anti- Semitism, that at that moment they reveal pride. I
don’t want to talk about the chosen people, but that they reveal
that.
The most comprehensive and frequently voiced stereotype,
across the generations, is that of the “rich Jew,” which appears
often in interviews and family discussions. For example, Peter
Schütz, born in 1954, tells us that “the Jews were among the
richest, that was always true.” It is the eyewit- nesses who
relate that in the 1930s Jews were either in academic profes-
sions (especially doctors) or worked as traders and
businesspeople, lived in “rich areas,” and enjoyed considerable
economic influence. When the eye- witnesses talk about being
personally acquainted with Jews, they generally do not describe
a person, but mention his profession or social status. Even when
this involves schoolmates, who (as most put it) “suddenly”
stopped coming to class, they often refer as a matter of course
to their father’s pro- fessions or assert that they were “rich
people.” Thus Lore Renz, born in 1916, tells of the “last
Jewess” in her class:
Lore Renz: Lilly Schneider, her father was a doctor at the
public health office in Hanover; they lived in Kleefeld. That’s
where all the wealthy people lived, Kleefeld, Kirchrode, all the
Jews lived mainly there.... But they were able to emigrate, you
know.
In this respect, the eyewitness portrayals differ little from one
another. In most eyewitness retellings, a reference to the
person’s or family’s finan- cial situation ends with a report of
their flight before the start of World War II. The older
generation reports are also similar in another respect: In none of
their stories do they mention the discrimination—loss of posi-
tions, property, etc.—that preceded that flight. The flight is
simply referred to as though it requires no explanation.
Furthermore, “flight” is not the word the eyewitnesses use; they
talk about how the Jews “emigrated” or “took off,” were “gone”
or “made a run for it” (aus dem Staub gemacht).
Again and again, “money” appears in this context: The Jews
who had the clothing store on Danzigerstrasse and with whom
her father had
worked, reported Wilhelmine Brinkmann, “took off” “to
America ... because they had money.... These people had
money.”
The stereotype of the “rich Jew” who “emigrated” before the
war, gen- erally to America, is found much more frequently in
eyewitness accounts than in the stories told by their children.
But when the members of the next generation recall their
parents’ stories, the concept reappears. Thus Kurt Jung recalls
that his parents “knew a lot of Jewish families”:
Kurt Jung: ... who were businesspeople in Hanover, who showed
up again in time; many of them emigrated to America before the
war started and then came back, big businesspeople.
Not only is the concept of the “rich Jews” taken up by the
children’s generation. With the same continuity, some members
of the children’s gen- eration also use the Nazi categories of
“half” or “quarter Jews.” These terms are generally incorporated
incidentally into their stories, without ref- erence to the origin
or function of that categorization. As a matter of course, those
interviewed distinguish between “the Jews” and “the Ger-
mans.” Thus Birgit Roth, born in 1939, says:
Birgit Roth: I don’t know why they wanted to throw the Jews
out; Jews were actually the best businesspeople; they had the
biggest share here in Germany. Everywhere big businesses, and
the Germans worked there.
In this interview, Ms. Roth is not openly anti-Semitic; rather,
she attempts, with statements like “Jews were the best
businesspeople,” to explain what she is unable to understand at
the start. Not only does the question “why they wanted to throw
the Jews out” ignore that at issue is mass murder, not expulsion,
but “the Germans” appear only as the employees of Jewish
businesspeople, while “they” who wanted “to throw the Jews
out” remains an open question. The basic distinction between
“Jews” and “Aryans” is maintained through the distinction
between “the Jews” and “the Germans.” What the speaker
intends to be a statement crit- ical of the Nazi period conveys
the very same anti-Semitic stereotypes and Nazi categories:
Jews were successful (read “rich”) entrepreneurs, and Jews are
not considered German.
As self-evidently as “rich Jews” appear in interviews with
eyewitnesses and members of the children’s generation, the
concept no longer appears in the grandchildren’s generation.
Thus the grandchildren of Lore Renz
How the Jews Are Remembered 21
22 Grandpa Wasn’t a Nazi
remember that their grandmother told them about Jewish
classmates, but their grandmother’s automatic reference to the
family’s socio-economic sta- tus is gone. To the interviewer’s
question about what stories her grand- mother had told her from
the Nazi period, Nina Jung, born in 1975, answers:
Nina Jung: For example, that suddenly the young girl ...
disappeared from class.
Nina Jung is the only one of the interviewed grandchildren who
remembers a story told by her grandmother that does not
involve helping Jews, hiding them, or rescuing them in some
way, or some form of resist- ance to their persecution. In all
other cases, it is their own grandmother or grandfather who
treated persecuted Jews humanely, attempted to protect them
from persecution, or, in one case, were against “people being
thrown in a pit like that ... and then just buried” and so the
grandfather “desert- ed.” The image of the good grandparents is
so dominant that no other story told about the Jews by the
eyewitnesses is registered by the grandchil- dren or retold.
How “the Nazis” Are Remembered: As Others
When “the Nazis” are spoken of in our interviews, the term
mostly func- tions to distance the speakers or their families
from the historical events of the Third Reich. It is true that
there are cases where interviewees describe their own family
members as “Nazis” or “National Socialists.” Max Wieck, for
example, speaks of some of his family as “fervent National
Socialists,” and Franz Schmitt emphasizes that one side of his
family were “all pure Nazis.” In both cases, however, the
storyteller, in the same breath, intro- duces the people important
for his concept of himself—his grandfather or father—as
“opponents” of the Nazis. Mr. Wieck presents his grandfather as
“a founder of the SPD [Social Democratic Party],” and Mr.
Schmitt speaks of “my father ... the only Communist.” Thus
family members most signif- icant for their own history are
portrayed as “not Nazis.” “The Nazis” are others—countertypes
to their grandfathers and fathers.
When family members with whom the storyteller identifies are
described as Nazis, there is almost always an immediate
justification. They
were forced to join the Nazi Party; they couldn’t act otherwise
because of their economic situation; they did it because
everyone did. The only excep- tions are if the interviewee still
identifies positively with the Third Reich. In our sample, this
was true only of Mrs. Haase, born in 1920, who thinks the Third
Reich was “a good period,” and describes her father emphatical-
ly as a “staunch National Socialist.”
The extent to which a person’s image can change through the
genera- tions can be illustrated by the Grubitsch family. Like
Mrs. Haase, Mrs. Grubitsch describes her husband as an early
“fervent Nazi.” Unlike Mrs. Haase, however, she notes right
away that he was “in the party” because otherwise “he wouldn’t
have gotten a job.” Economic motives thus justify her husband’s
party membership. Mrs. Grubitsch’s son paints a somewhat
different picture. To him, his father was exactly the opposite of
fervent: “I know that my father did it with a certain amount of
reluctance.... He wore a badge on his lapel, but you couldn’t
always see it on all his suits.” The portrayals by the eyewitness
and her son express ambivalence, each in its own way. The
picture painted by the grandson no longer does: He is sure that
his grandfathers were “not Nazis,” and “my grandpa wasn’t a
party member, on my father’s side, not on my mother’s side
either.” Thus from generation to generation, “the Nazis” become
more the “other,” while the distance of one’s own ancestors
from the horrific events becomes ever greater.
Historians have considered the question of how the
“Volksgenossen” (the “common citizens”) reacted to the
persecution of the Jews—what they knew, could have known, or
wanted to know of their extermination.14 The reconstructed
reality of the Third Reich differs greatly from the answers given
by the eyewitnesses to the question of what they knew or could
have known at the time. But even the eyewitnesses seem to be
aware of this. Few of them act as self-righteously as Rainer
Hofer, born in 1925, who becomes indignant in the group
discussion about the fact that “it is always disputed” that people
didn’t know at the time that concentration and extermination
camps existed. But there are also explicit references to differ-
ent opinions in the statements of other eyewitnesses. Mathilde
Beck, born in 1924 says in the group discussion:
How “the Nazis” Are Remembered 23
24 Grandpa Wasn’t a Nazi
Mathilde Beck: All that about the concentration camps. People
say so often, “But you must have known.” It was all kept so
secret.
“Empty Talking”
In the interviews, there were 106 occurrences of the phrase
“people didn’t know.” More than a third of the eyewitnesses
insist that they did not know there were concentration camps.15
However, most of these statements use an interpretive pattern
similar in vagueness to Mrs. Beck’s (“All that about the
concentration camps”), pointing to a phenomenon that occurred
fre- quently during the discussions, which could be described as
“empty talk- ing.” That is, actors and the contexts of events are
not named, but are called “that,” “they” [sie, “se,” die], or “that
stuff” [das da]. This “empty talking” as a form of speech
typifies the intergenerational discussion about the Third Reich.
Actors, generally the perpetrators, remain shapeless, while
historical events are described in outline form, so what is being
talked about remains unclear, the events seemingly harmless.
“About the Jews,” says Paula Ubaczek, born in 1921, “I only
found out about it when the war was over, when the
concentration camps were opened; that’s when we found out.”
The imprecision of the language, describing developments
associatively and indirectly, characterizes the “empty talking”;
it remains up to the listener to fill in the blanks as to what the
speaker is actually talking about. She was indignant, says Ms.
Wil- helmine Brinkmann, born in 1915, “when all that came
out,” and Ms. Elisabeth Schulze, born in1920, reports:
Elisabeth Schulze: We had no idea what the others were
suffering. We only read that here, when the newspaper came
again. And when we got a radio, we heard it then, but otherwise
we didn’t know anything ... all the things that were going on.
From the point of view of transmission of history, “empty
speaking” perhaps conveys something else: that certain contexts
of history are suffi- ciently discussed even if left imprecise.
“That stuff” and “it” signify con- texts about which it makes no
sense to speak more clearly: “Certainly, it shouldn’t be
forgotten so quickly; we have to talk about it sometimes, so that
it doesn’t come again,” said Elli Krug.
To sum up, the anti-Semitically colored notion of the “rich Jew”
and the interpretive pattern that “Jews” and “Germans”
represent two separate groups of people are ideas that transcend
the generations. Their disturbing resilience can be seen as a
posthumous victory for the Nazi policies of per- secution and
extermination. In addition, the “empty talking” in communi-
cation between generations is a mechanism that allows blank
spaces to be filled with whatever content and ideas best satisfy
the needs of the audi- ence, allowing the listener to ascribe to
the speaker whatever meanings he would most like to impute to
them.
A Broadly Representative Survey
The intergenerational survey presented here was an in-depth
study that necessarily encompassed a small number of cases, to
illuminate the under- lying patterns of how families
communicate about the past. The selective sampling included
only families that had an interest in the subject and whose
members were willing to speak about it together—which is
hardly the case in all German families. To test whether our
findings could be gen- eralized, we reformulated the research
questions for a larger representative survey, carried out in June
2002 by the Emnid Institute in Bielefeld.
The phenomenon of cumulative heroization could not be
reproduced within the representative popular survey, since the
people questioned were randomly chosen and not family
members. But it was possible to ask those surveyed how they
saw the role, attitudes, and behavior of their own rela- tives
during the Third Reich. We posed this question on several
levels: first, by asking what their parents and grandparents, in
their view, thought about Nazism; and second, by querying
about the individual attitudes and behavioral patterns of their
relatives for a range of items—always from the point of view of
the respondent, of course.
The results were even more astonishing than those of the in-
depth study. Roughly half the population surveyed is of the
opinion that their relatives viewed Nazism negatively or even
very negatively; a mere 4 per- cent believe their relatives’
attitude was “more positive,” and only 2 percent believe that
their relatives had a “very positive” attitude. Among people
with upper high school (Abitur) and college diplomas, these
results were even more pronounced—56 percent ascribing
negative attitudes toward
Broadly Representative Survey 25
26 Grandpa Wasn’t a Nazi
Nazism to their relatives.
Another question probed the attitudes, experiences, and
behaviors of
parents and grandparents, as known to the survey participants
from family discussions. Only 3 percent of the respondents
believed that their relatives had been “anti-Jewish,” and only 1
percent thought it was possible that they “were directly
involved in crimes.” Twenty-six percent of those sur- veyed
were convinced that their relatives “helped people who were
perse- cuted,” and 35 percent “didn’t go along with everything
as much as possible.” In contrast, 65 percent of participants
believe their parents and grandparents “suffered a lot in the
war,” and 63 percent stated that their relatives had “experienced
community” during the Third Reich. These results, too, were
even more pronounced among those with an upper high school
(Abitur) or college diploma: Thirty percent believed that their
rela- tives “helped people who were persecuted,” and 71 percent
were convinced that their parents and grandparents “suffered a
lot in the war.”
The results of the broader survey make it clear that the
overwhelming majority believes their own family members were
not Nazis. Anti-Semites and perpetrators appear to be
practically nonexistent in German families. That a higher
frequency of these experiences, attitudes, and behaviors cor-
relates with a high level of education underscores the findings
of our in- depth study that education about Nazi crimes and the
Holocaust has the paradoxical effect of inclining people to turn
their parents and grandpar- ents into opponents of the regime,
helpers, or casual or even explicit resisters.
Some findings of the larger sample also point toward
“cumulative hero- ization”: Thus 14 percent of the youngest
participants (fourteen-to-twen- ty-nine years of age) said that
their grandparents “offered resistance” (compared to 13 percent
overall), and only 4 percent believed that they were “staunch
Nazis” (compared to 6 percent overall). However, in the
youngest group, 6 percent believed that their grandparents
“were anti-Jew- ish,” and 3 percent believed it was possible
“that they were directly involved in crimes” (compared with 3
percent and 1 percent overall).
The finding that two-thirds of the participants in the
representative sample emphasized the sufferings of their own
relatives in the war under- scores the finding that the public
culture of commemoration in Germany
How the Study Was Received 27
differs greatly from private recollections. Whoever was guilty
of the Holo- caust, whoever committed the crimes in the
extermination camps, the forced labor system, and the camps—
one thing is clear to all German citi- zens: My grandpa wasn’t a
Nazi.
Postscript: How the Study Was Received
The results of this study began to have an impact even before
the final report had been published. All the major newspapers
and many radio and television stations carried stories about it.
After the book appeared, the findings entered into the discourse
of politicians and government officials, who showed concern
that the educational efforts of the past two decades had, in their
view, borne astonishingly little fruit. The professional agents of
memory—whether working in schools, foundations, or
Holocaust memorials—were highly disconcerted.
The book itself became the subject of many school projects. In
higher academia, too, the study garnered attention and was the
subject of numer- ous panels and conferences.16
Although the study documents that for the generation of the
grand- children, the role models are resistance fighters, not
Nazis—something that could certainly be viewed as a successful
educational outcome—the public reaction was generally
alarmist. In speeches, high-ranking politi- cians worriedly
demanded more education, and the media latched on with great
interest to the angle that grandchildren use their knowledge of
histo- ry to different ends than their teachers had expected.
Foreign reactions to the study—for example, in the Norwegian
news- papers—interpreted the results as representing a failed
attempt to deal with the past and focused on the recent right-
wing orientation among young Germans. This interpretation
itself illustrates how the study became a pro- jective test for
every group’s difficulties in dealing with the past.
Two years after its original publication in German, the study
continues to receive attention. Even editorials on the 2004 U.S.
elections reflected the background of Grandpa Wasn’t a Nazi:
“Say this about John Kerry: At least his grandfather wasn’t a
Nazi,” wrote Victorino Matus on February 6, 2004, in the
Weekly Standard.17 The occasion for the article, entitled
28 Grandpa Wasn’t a Nazi
“Springtime for Friedrich,” was a minor German scandal
stemming from a statement by the head of the Christian
Democratic Party in parliament, Friedrich Merz, that his
grandfather had been his most important role model; it was
promptly revealed that the grandfather had been a Nazi, at least
if early membership in the SA and the Nazi Party were deemed
suffi- cient grounds. So Merz had a public “grandpa” problem,
and references to our study made it clear how firmly the
research had taken root in the Ger- man commemoration culture
framework.
This was true as well for the counter-commemoration culture of
the far right. The NPD (the National Party of Germany), which
Memory is a social and political construction. Discuss.·.docx
Memory is a social and political construction. Discuss.·.docx
Memory is a social and political construction. Discuss.·.docx
Memory is a social and political construction. Discuss.·.docx
Memory is a social and political construction. Discuss.·.docx

More Related Content

Similar to Memory is a social and political construction. Discuss.·.docx

The Humanities
The HumanitiesThe Humanities
The Humanitiesdcorriher
 
Rough Draft Due April 19th, 2017 Final Draft Due May 3rd,.docx
Rough Draft Due April 19th, 2017  Final Draft Due May 3rd,.docxRough Draft Due April 19th, 2017  Final Draft Due May 3rd,.docx
Rough Draft Due April 19th, 2017 Final Draft Due May 3rd,.docxSUBHI7
 
1Alex SepkowskiProfessor Lewis1152014Annotat.docx
1Alex SepkowskiProfessor Lewis1152014Annotat.docx1Alex SepkowskiProfessor Lewis1152014Annotat.docx
1Alex SepkowskiProfessor Lewis1152014Annotat.docxhyacinthshackley2629
 
Epis Ph D2010 Prof Abdullah
Epis Ph D2010  Prof AbdullahEpis Ph D2010  Prof Abdullah
Epis Ph D2010 Prof AbdullahAnita Adnan
 
Ewrt1 c class 3 post qhq
Ewrt1 c class 3 post qhqEwrt1 c class 3 post qhq
Ewrt1 c class 3 post qhqjordanlachance
 
Lecture Notes 1 - Intro to POR
Lecture Notes 1 - Intro to PORLecture Notes 1 - Intro to POR
Lecture Notes 1 - Intro to PORvjmartin
 
Interpretive frameworks -spring13
Interpretive frameworks -spring13Interpretive frameworks -spring13
Interpretive frameworks -spring13paul_ilsley
 
Philosophy of man(modern, ancient, contemporary)
Philosophy of man(modern, ancient, contemporary)Philosophy of man(modern, ancient, contemporary)
Philosophy of man(modern, ancient, contemporary)EsOr Naujnas
 
Handout 1Understanding Culture, Society, and Politics narra NHS SHS Narra Pal...
Handout 1Understanding Culture, Society, and Politics narra NHS SHS Narra Pal...Handout 1Understanding Culture, Society, and Politics narra NHS SHS Narra Pal...
Handout 1Understanding Culture, Society, and Politics narra NHS SHS Narra Pal...Sham Lumba
 
Topic For Informative Essay. 10 Stunning Ideas For Informative Speech Topics ...
Topic For Informative Essay. 10 Stunning Ideas For Informative Speech Topics ...Topic For Informative Essay. 10 Stunning Ideas For Informative Speech Topics ...
Topic For Informative Essay. 10 Stunning Ideas For Informative Speech Topics ...Kimberly Jabbour
 
On the ethnomathematics � epistemology nexus
On the ethnomathematics � epistemology nexusOn the ethnomathematics � epistemology nexus
On the ethnomathematics � epistemology nexusICEM-4
 
Comm Theory Narrative Paradigm
Comm Theory Narrative Paradigm Comm Theory Narrative Paradigm
Comm Theory Narrative Paradigm danarmcgrath
 
Writing in the humanities
Writing in the humanitiesWriting in the humanities
Writing in the humanitiesmr.mamo
 
Foundation knowledge third paper
Foundation knowledge third paperFoundation knowledge third paper
Foundation knowledge third paperRuslan Leontyev
 
Continental philosophy
Continental philosophyContinental philosophy
Continental philosophytjmartin72768
 
Chapter 1 the communication tradition
Chapter 1   the communication traditionChapter 1   the communication tradition
Chapter 1 the communication traditionxiaoshandanni
 
Analyzing Ethos And Pathos
Analyzing Ethos And PathosAnalyzing Ethos And Pathos
Analyzing Ethos And PathosJustin Knight
 

Similar to Memory is a social and political construction. Discuss.·.docx (20)

The Humanities
The HumanitiesThe Humanities
The Humanities
 
Rough Draft Due April 19th, 2017 Final Draft Due May 3rd,.docx
Rough Draft Due April 19th, 2017  Final Draft Due May 3rd,.docxRough Draft Due April 19th, 2017  Final Draft Due May 3rd,.docx
Rough Draft Due April 19th, 2017 Final Draft Due May 3rd,.docx
 
1Alex SepkowskiProfessor Lewis1152014Annotat.docx
1Alex SepkowskiProfessor Lewis1152014Annotat.docx1Alex SepkowskiProfessor Lewis1152014Annotat.docx
1Alex SepkowskiProfessor Lewis1152014Annotat.docx
 
Epis Ph D2010 Prof Abdullah
Epis Ph D2010  Prof AbdullahEpis Ph D2010  Prof Abdullah
Epis Ph D2010 Prof Abdullah
 
Ewrt1 c class 3 post qhq
Ewrt1 c class 3 post qhqEwrt1 c class 3 post qhq
Ewrt1 c class 3 post qhq
 
Lecture Notes 1 - Intro to POR
Lecture Notes 1 - Intro to PORLecture Notes 1 - Intro to POR
Lecture Notes 1 - Intro to POR
 
Interpretive frameworks -spring13
Interpretive frameworks -spring13Interpretive frameworks -spring13
Interpretive frameworks -spring13
 
110902 theory of science
110902 theory of science110902 theory of science
110902 theory of science
 
Philosophy of man(modern, ancient, contemporary)
Philosophy of man(modern, ancient, contemporary)Philosophy of man(modern, ancient, contemporary)
Philosophy of man(modern, ancient, contemporary)
 
Handout 1Understanding Culture, Society, and Politics narra NHS SHS Narra Pal...
Handout 1Understanding Culture, Society, and Politics narra NHS SHS Narra Pal...Handout 1Understanding Culture, Society, and Politics narra NHS SHS Narra Pal...
Handout 1Understanding Culture, Society, and Politics narra NHS SHS Narra Pal...
 
Neurobiology, layered texts, and correlative cosmologies — a cross cultural f...
Neurobiology, layered texts, and correlative cosmologies — a cross cultural f...Neurobiology, layered texts, and correlative cosmologies — a cross cultural f...
Neurobiology, layered texts, and correlative cosmologies — a cross cultural f...
 
Essay On Violence.pdf
Essay On Violence.pdfEssay On Violence.pdf
Essay On Violence.pdf
 
Topic For Informative Essay. 10 Stunning Ideas For Informative Speech Topics ...
Topic For Informative Essay. 10 Stunning Ideas For Informative Speech Topics ...Topic For Informative Essay. 10 Stunning Ideas For Informative Speech Topics ...
Topic For Informative Essay. 10 Stunning Ideas For Informative Speech Topics ...
 
On the ethnomathematics � epistemology nexus
On the ethnomathematics � epistemology nexusOn the ethnomathematics � epistemology nexus
On the ethnomathematics � epistemology nexus
 
Comm Theory Narrative Paradigm
Comm Theory Narrative Paradigm Comm Theory Narrative Paradigm
Comm Theory Narrative Paradigm
 
Writing in the humanities
Writing in the humanitiesWriting in the humanities
Writing in the humanities
 
Foundation knowledge third paper
Foundation knowledge third paperFoundation knowledge third paper
Foundation knowledge third paper
 
Continental philosophy
Continental philosophyContinental philosophy
Continental philosophy
 
Chapter 1 the communication tradition
Chapter 1   the communication traditionChapter 1   the communication tradition
Chapter 1 the communication tradition
 
Analyzing Ethos And Pathos
Analyzing Ethos And PathosAnalyzing Ethos And Pathos
Analyzing Ethos And Pathos
 

More from ARIV4

Please go through the document completely before providing the answe.docx
Please go through the document completely before providing the answe.docxPlease go through the document completely before providing the answe.docx
Please go through the document completely before providing the answe.docxARIV4
 
Please follow the instruction carefully. APA stile. Mínimum three re.docx
Please follow the instruction carefully. APA stile. Mínimum three re.docxPlease follow the instruction carefully. APA stile. Mínimum three re.docx
Please follow the instruction carefully. APA stile. Mínimum three re.docxARIV4
 
Please follow the instructions attached in MS Word. Font Arial,  .docx
Please follow the instructions attached in MS Word. Font Arial,  .docxPlease follow the instructions attached in MS Word. Font Arial,  .docx
Please follow the instructions attached in MS Word. Font Arial,  .docxARIV4
 
Please help me write a report focusing on photocatalysis of TiO2 .docx
Please help me write a report focusing on photocatalysis of TiO2 .docxPlease help me write a report focusing on photocatalysis of TiO2 .docx
Please help me write a report focusing on photocatalysis of TiO2 .docxARIV4
 
Please follow the directions in the assignment content Environme.docx
Please follow the directions in the assignment content Environme.docxPlease follow the directions in the assignment content Environme.docx
Please follow the directions in the assignment content Environme.docxARIV4
 
Please follow the directions below to complete the project1.).docx
Please follow the directions below to complete the project1.).docxPlease follow the directions below to complete the project1.).docx
Please follow the directions below to complete the project1.).docxARIV4
 
Please follow all directions please. the attachment titled assignme.docx
Please follow all directions please. the attachment titled assignme.docxPlease follow all directions please. the attachment titled assignme.docx
Please follow all directions please. the attachment titled assignme.docxARIV4
 
Please draft a personal message that you would like to appear on you.docx
Please draft a personal message that you would like to appear on you.docxPlease draft a personal message that you would like to appear on you.docx
Please draft a personal message that you would like to appear on you.docxARIV4
 
Please explain how you have met various BSN Essentials for each co.docx
Please explain how you have met various BSN Essentials for each co.docxPlease explain how you have met various BSN Essentials for each co.docx
Please explain how you have met various BSN Essentials for each co.docxARIV4
 
Please Follow directions or I will dispute please answer origina.docx
Please Follow directions or I will dispute please answer origina.docxPlease Follow directions or I will dispute please answer origina.docx
Please Follow directions or I will dispute please answer origina.docxARIV4
 
Please find the attached.Task 1 -  In 150 words comment on att.docx
Please find the attached.Task 1  -  In 150 words comment on att.docxPlease find the attached.Task 1  -  In 150 words comment on att.docx
Please find the attached.Task 1 -  In 150 words comment on att.docxARIV4
 
Please draw primarily from this weeks readings (and use additio.docx
Please draw primarily from this weeks readings (and use additio.docxPlease draw primarily from this weeks readings (and use additio.docx
Please draw primarily from this weeks readings (and use additio.docxARIV4
 
Please explain the reoccurring theme (sub-textual idea) of blin.docx
Please explain the reoccurring theme (sub-textual idea) of blin.docxPlease explain the reoccurring theme (sub-textual idea) of blin.docx
Please explain the reoccurring theme (sub-textual idea) of blin.docxARIV4
 
Please fill the attached Self-Assessment Surveys (TWO) and calcula.docx
Please fill the attached Self-Assessment Surveys (TWO) and calcula.docxPlease fill the attached Self-Assessment Surveys (TWO) and calcula.docx
Please fill the attached Self-Assessment Surveys (TWO) and calcula.docxARIV4
 
Please explain the rules of the calling program (Caller Rules).docx
Please explain the rules of the calling program (Caller Rules).docxPlease explain the rules of the calling program (Caller Rules).docx
Please explain the rules of the calling program (Caller Rules).docxARIV4
 
Please follow directions to receive all possible points!!The int.docx
Please follow directions to receive all possible points!!The int.docxPlease follow directions to receive all possible points!!The int.docx
Please follow directions to receive all possible points!!The int.docxARIV4
 
Please follow instructions A blanch interpersonal record attached..docx
Please follow instructions A blanch interpersonal record attached..docxPlease follow instructions A blanch interpersonal record attached..docx
Please follow instructions A blanch interpersonal record attached..docxARIV4
 
Please explain how you have met various BSN Essentials for each cour.docx
Please explain how you have met various BSN Essentials for each cour.docxPlease explain how you have met various BSN Essentials for each cour.docx
Please explain how you have met various BSN Essentials for each cour.docxARIV4
 
Please explain how you have met various Bachelor of Science in Nur.docx
Please explain how you have met various Bachelor of Science in Nur.docxPlease explain how you have met various Bachelor of Science in Nur.docx
Please explain how you have met various Bachelor of Science in Nur.docxARIV4
 
Please DiscussWhat are host-based, client-based, client-serv.docx
Please DiscussWhat are host-based, client-based, client-serv.docxPlease DiscussWhat are host-based, client-based, client-serv.docx
Please DiscussWhat are host-based, client-based, client-serv.docxARIV4
 

More from ARIV4 (20)

Please go through the document completely before providing the answe.docx
Please go through the document completely before providing the answe.docxPlease go through the document completely before providing the answe.docx
Please go through the document completely before providing the answe.docx
 
Please follow the instruction carefully. APA stile. Mínimum three re.docx
Please follow the instruction carefully. APA stile. Mínimum three re.docxPlease follow the instruction carefully. APA stile. Mínimum three re.docx
Please follow the instruction carefully. APA stile. Mínimum three re.docx
 
Please follow the instructions attached in MS Word. Font Arial,  .docx
Please follow the instructions attached in MS Word. Font Arial,  .docxPlease follow the instructions attached in MS Word. Font Arial,  .docx
Please follow the instructions attached in MS Word. Font Arial,  .docx
 
Please help me write a report focusing on photocatalysis of TiO2 .docx
Please help me write a report focusing on photocatalysis of TiO2 .docxPlease help me write a report focusing on photocatalysis of TiO2 .docx
Please help me write a report focusing on photocatalysis of TiO2 .docx
 
Please follow the directions in the assignment content Environme.docx
Please follow the directions in the assignment content Environme.docxPlease follow the directions in the assignment content Environme.docx
Please follow the directions in the assignment content Environme.docx
 
Please follow the directions below to complete the project1.).docx
Please follow the directions below to complete the project1.).docxPlease follow the directions below to complete the project1.).docx
Please follow the directions below to complete the project1.).docx
 
Please follow all directions please. the attachment titled assignme.docx
Please follow all directions please. the attachment titled assignme.docxPlease follow all directions please. the attachment titled assignme.docx
Please follow all directions please. the attachment titled assignme.docx
 
Please draft a personal message that you would like to appear on you.docx
Please draft a personal message that you would like to appear on you.docxPlease draft a personal message that you would like to appear on you.docx
Please draft a personal message that you would like to appear on you.docx
 
Please explain how you have met various BSN Essentials for each co.docx
Please explain how you have met various BSN Essentials for each co.docxPlease explain how you have met various BSN Essentials for each co.docx
Please explain how you have met various BSN Essentials for each co.docx
 
Please Follow directions or I will dispute please answer origina.docx
Please Follow directions or I will dispute please answer origina.docxPlease Follow directions or I will dispute please answer origina.docx
Please Follow directions or I will dispute please answer origina.docx
 
Please find the attached.Task 1 -  In 150 words comment on att.docx
Please find the attached.Task 1  -  In 150 words comment on att.docxPlease find the attached.Task 1  -  In 150 words comment on att.docx
Please find the attached.Task 1 -  In 150 words comment on att.docx
 
Please draw primarily from this weeks readings (and use additio.docx
Please draw primarily from this weeks readings (and use additio.docxPlease draw primarily from this weeks readings (and use additio.docx
Please draw primarily from this weeks readings (and use additio.docx
 
Please explain the reoccurring theme (sub-textual idea) of blin.docx
Please explain the reoccurring theme (sub-textual idea) of blin.docxPlease explain the reoccurring theme (sub-textual idea) of blin.docx
Please explain the reoccurring theme (sub-textual idea) of blin.docx
 
Please fill the attached Self-Assessment Surveys (TWO) and calcula.docx
Please fill the attached Self-Assessment Surveys (TWO) and calcula.docxPlease fill the attached Self-Assessment Surveys (TWO) and calcula.docx
Please fill the attached Self-Assessment Surveys (TWO) and calcula.docx
 
Please explain the rules of the calling program (Caller Rules).docx
Please explain the rules of the calling program (Caller Rules).docxPlease explain the rules of the calling program (Caller Rules).docx
Please explain the rules of the calling program (Caller Rules).docx
 
Please follow directions to receive all possible points!!The int.docx
Please follow directions to receive all possible points!!The int.docxPlease follow directions to receive all possible points!!The int.docx
Please follow directions to receive all possible points!!The int.docx
 
Please follow instructions A blanch interpersonal record attached..docx
Please follow instructions A blanch interpersonal record attached..docxPlease follow instructions A blanch interpersonal record attached..docx
Please follow instructions A blanch interpersonal record attached..docx
 
Please explain how you have met various BSN Essentials for each cour.docx
Please explain how you have met various BSN Essentials for each cour.docxPlease explain how you have met various BSN Essentials for each cour.docx
Please explain how you have met various BSN Essentials for each cour.docx
 
Please explain how you have met various Bachelor of Science in Nur.docx
Please explain how you have met various Bachelor of Science in Nur.docxPlease explain how you have met various Bachelor of Science in Nur.docx
Please explain how you have met various Bachelor of Science in Nur.docx
 
Please DiscussWhat are host-based, client-based, client-serv.docx
Please DiscussWhat are host-based, client-based, client-serv.docxPlease DiscussWhat are host-based, client-based, client-serv.docx
Please DiscussWhat are host-based, client-based, client-serv.docx
 

Recently uploaded

Procuring digital preservation CAN be quick and painless with our new dynamic...
Procuring digital preservation CAN be quick and painless with our new dynamic...Procuring digital preservation CAN be quick and painless with our new dynamic...
Procuring digital preservation CAN be quick and painless with our new dynamic...Jisc
 
Enzyme, Pharmaceutical Aids, Miscellaneous Last Part of Chapter no 5th.pdf
Enzyme, Pharmaceutical Aids, Miscellaneous Last Part of Chapter no 5th.pdfEnzyme, Pharmaceutical Aids, Miscellaneous Last Part of Chapter no 5th.pdf
Enzyme, Pharmaceutical Aids, Miscellaneous Last Part of Chapter no 5th.pdfSumit Tiwari
 
How to Make a Pirate ship Primary Education.pptx
How to Make a Pirate ship Primary Education.pptxHow to Make a Pirate ship Primary Education.pptx
How to Make a Pirate ship Primary Education.pptxmanuelaromero2013
 
DATA STRUCTURE AND ALGORITHM for beginners
DATA STRUCTURE AND ALGORITHM for beginnersDATA STRUCTURE AND ALGORITHM for beginners
DATA STRUCTURE AND ALGORITHM for beginnersSabitha Banu
 
Framing an Appropriate Research Question 6b9b26d93da94caf993c038d9efcdedb.pdf
Framing an Appropriate Research Question 6b9b26d93da94caf993c038d9efcdedb.pdfFraming an Appropriate Research Question 6b9b26d93da94caf993c038d9efcdedb.pdf
Framing an Appropriate Research Question 6b9b26d93da94caf993c038d9efcdedb.pdfUjwalaBharambe
 
ECONOMIC CONTEXT - PAPER 1 Q3: NEWSPAPERS.pptx
ECONOMIC CONTEXT - PAPER 1 Q3: NEWSPAPERS.pptxECONOMIC CONTEXT - PAPER 1 Q3: NEWSPAPERS.pptx
ECONOMIC CONTEXT - PAPER 1 Q3: NEWSPAPERS.pptxiammrhaywood
 
Alper Gobel In Media Res Media Component
Alper Gobel In Media Res Media ComponentAlper Gobel In Media Res Media Component
Alper Gobel In Media Res Media ComponentInMediaRes1
 
POINT- BIOCHEMISTRY SEM 2 ENZYMES UNIT 5.pptx
POINT- BIOCHEMISTRY SEM 2 ENZYMES UNIT 5.pptxPOINT- BIOCHEMISTRY SEM 2 ENZYMES UNIT 5.pptx
POINT- BIOCHEMISTRY SEM 2 ENZYMES UNIT 5.pptxSayali Powar
 
EPANDING THE CONTENT OF AN OUTLINE using notes.pptx
EPANDING THE CONTENT OF AN OUTLINE using notes.pptxEPANDING THE CONTENT OF AN OUTLINE using notes.pptx
EPANDING THE CONTENT OF AN OUTLINE using notes.pptxRaymartEstabillo3
 
Organic Name Reactions for the students and aspirants of Chemistry12th.pptx
Organic Name Reactions  for the students and aspirants of Chemistry12th.pptxOrganic Name Reactions  for the students and aspirants of Chemistry12th.pptx
Organic Name Reactions for the students and aspirants of Chemistry12th.pptxVS Mahajan Coaching Centre
 
18-04-UA_REPORT_MEDIALITERAСY_INDEX-DM_23-1-final-eng.pdf
18-04-UA_REPORT_MEDIALITERAСY_INDEX-DM_23-1-final-eng.pdf18-04-UA_REPORT_MEDIALITERAСY_INDEX-DM_23-1-final-eng.pdf
18-04-UA_REPORT_MEDIALITERAСY_INDEX-DM_23-1-final-eng.pdfssuser54595a
 
Like-prefer-love -hate+verb+ing & silent letters & citizenship text.pdf
Like-prefer-love -hate+verb+ing & silent letters & citizenship text.pdfLike-prefer-love -hate+verb+ing & silent letters & citizenship text.pdf
Like-prefer-love -hate+verb+ing & silent letters & citizenship text.pdfMr Bounab Samir
 
How to Configure Email Server in Odoo 17
How to Configure Email Server in Odoo 17How to Configure Email Server in Odoo 17
How to Configure Email Server in Odoo 17Celine George
 
Hierarchy of management that covers different levels of management
Hierarchy of management that covers different levels of managementHierarchy of management that covers different levels of management
Hierarchy of management that covers different levels of managementmkooblal
 
ECONOMIC CONTEXT - LONG FORM TV DRAMA - PPT
ECONOMIC CONTEXT - LONG FORM TV DRAMA - PPTECONOMIC CONTEXT - LONG FORM TV DRAMA - PPT
ECONOMIC CONTEXT - LONG FORM TV DRAMA - PPTiammrhaywood
 
Types of Journalistic Writing Grade 8.pptx
Types of Journalistic Writing Grade 8.pptxTypes of Journalistic Writing Grade 8.pptx
Types of Journalistic Writing Grade 8.pptxEyham Joco
 
Earth Day Presentation wow hello nice great
Earth Day Presentation wow hello nice greatEarth Day Presentation wow hello nice great
Earth Day Presentation wow hello nice greatYousafMalik24
 
Roles & Responsibilities in Pharmacovigilance
Roles & Responsibilities in PharmacovigilanceRoles & Responsibilities in Pharmacovigilance
Roles & Responsibilities in PharmacovigilanceSamikshaHamane
 

Recently uploaded (20)

Procuring digital preservation CAN be quick and painless with our new dynamic...
Procuring digital preservation CAN be quick and painless with our new dynamic...Procuring digital preservation CAN be quick and painless with our new dynamic...
Procuring digital preservation CAN be quick and painless with our new dynamic...
 
Enzyme, Pharmaceutical Aids, Miscellaneous Last Part of Chapter no 5th.pdf
Enzyme, Pharmaceutical Aids, Miscellaneous Last Part of Chapter no 5th.pdfEnzyme, Pharmaceutical Aids, Miscellaneous Last Part of Chapter no 5th.pdf
Enzyme, Pharmaceutical Aids, Miscellaneous Last Part of Chapter no 5th.pdf
 
OS-operating systems- ch04 (Threads) ...
OS-operating systems- ch04 (Threads) ...OS-operating systems- ch04 (Threads) ...
OS-operating systems- ch04 (Threads) ...
 
How to Make a Pirate ship Primary Education.pptx
How to Make a Pirate ship Primary Education.pptxHow to Make a Pirate ship Primary Education.pptx
How to Make a Pirate ship Primary Education.pptx
 
DATA STRUCTURE AND ALGORITHM for beginners
DATA STRUCTURE AND ALGORITHM for beginnersDATA STRUCTURE AND ALGORITHM for beginners
DATA STRUCTURE AND ALGORITHM for beginners
 
Framing an Appropriate Research Question 6b9b26d93da94caf993c038d9efcdedb.pdf
Framing an Appropriate Research Question 6b9b26d93da94caf993c038d9efcdedb.pdfFraming an Appropriate Research Question 6b9b26d93da94caf993c038d9efcdedb.pdf
Framing an Appropriate Research Question 6b9b26d93da94caf993c038d9efcdedb.pdf
 
ECONOMIC CONTEXT - PAPER 1 Q3: NEWSPAPERS.pptx
ECONOMIC CONTEXT - PAPER 1 Q3: NEWSPAPERS.pptxECONOMIC CONTEXT - PAPER 1 Q3: NEWSPAPERS.pptx
ECONOMIC CONTEXT - PAPER 1 Q3: NEWSPAPERS.pptx
 
Alper Gobel In Media Res Media Component
Alper Gobel In Media Res Media ComponentAlper Gobel In Media Res Media Component
Alper Gobel In Media Res Media Component
 
POINT- BIOCHEMISTRY SEM 2 ENZYMES UNIT 5.pptx
POINT- BIOCHEMISTRY SEM 2 ENZYMES UNIT 5.pptxPOINT- BIOCHEMISTRY SEM 2 ENZYMES UNIT 5.pptx
POINT- BIOCHEMISTRY SEM 2 ENZYMES UNIT 5.pptx
 
EPANDING THE CONTENT OF AN OUTLINE using notes.pptx
EPANDING THE CONTENT OF AN OUTLINE using notes.pptxEPANDING THE CONTENT OF AN OUTLINE using notes.pptx
EPANDING THE CONTENT OF AN OUTLINE using notes.pptx
 
ESSENTIAL of (CS/IT/IS) class 06 (database)
ESSENTIAL of (CS/IT/IS) class 06 (database)ESSENTIAL of (CS/IT/IS) class 06 (database)
ESSENTIAL of (CS/IT/IS) class 06 (database)
 
Organic Name Reactions for the students and aspirants of Chemistry12th.pptx
Organic Name Reactions  for the students and aspirants of Chemistry12th.pptxOrganic Name Reactions  for the students and aspirants of Chemistry12th.pptx
Organic Name Reactions for the students and aspirants of Chemistry12th.pptx
 
18-04-UA_REPORT_MEDIALITERAСY_INDEX-DM_23-1-final-eng.pdf
18-04-UA_REPORT_MEDIALITERAСY_INDEX-DM_23-1-final-eng.pdf18-04-UA_REPORT_MEDIALITERAСY_INDEX-DM_23-1-final-eng.pdf
18-04-UA_REPORT_MEDIALITERAСY_INDEX-DM_23-1-final-eng.pdf
 
Like-prefer-love -hate+verb+ing & silent letters & citizenship text.pdf
Like-prefer-love -hate+verb+ing & silent letters & citizenship text.pdfLike-prefer-love -hate+verb+ing & silent letters & citizenship text.pdf
Like-prefer-love -hate+verb+ing & silent letters & citizenship text.pdf
 
How to Configure Email Server in Odoo 17
How to Configure Email Server in Odoo 17How to Configure Email Server in Odoo 17
How to Configure Email Server in Odoo 17
 
Hierarchy of management that covers different levels of management
Hierarchy of management that covers different levels of managementHierarchy of management that covers different levels of management
Hierarchy of management that covers different levels of management
 
ECONOMIC CONTEXT - LONG FORM TV DRAMA - PPT
ECONOMIC CONTEXT - LONG FORM TV DRAMA - PPTECONOMIC CONTEXT - LONG FORM TV DRAMA - PPT
ECONOMIC CONTEXT - LONG FORM TV DRAMA - PPT
 
Types of Journalistic Writing Grade 8.pptx
Types of Journalistic Writing Grade 8.pptxTypes of Journalistic Writing Grade 8.pptx
Types of Journalistic Writing Grade 8.pptx
 
Earth Day Presentation wow hello nice great
Earth Day Presentation wow hello nice greatEarth Day Presentation wow hello nice great
Earth Day Presentation wow hello nice great
 
Roles & Responsibilities in Pharmacovigilance
Roles & Responsibilities in PharmacovigilanceRoles & Responsibilities in Pharmacovigilance
Roles & Responsibilities in Pharmacovigilance
 

Memory is a social and political construction. Discuss.·.docx

  • 1. Memory is a social and political construction. Discuss. · The title must appear at the top of your first page as set out in the list attached (The title must not be altered or changed in any way) · You should use spacing (1.5) and font size 12 · Correct spelling and appropriate paragraphing should be evident · There should be no widows or orphans, i.e. headings left alone at the bottom of the page · You should provide references for every citation within the text – omissions will be penalised - References should be written in accord with a referencing convention that is consistent with your major course of study, e.g. APA Marks will be awarded for: · Answering the question · Observing academic conventions · Adopting a clear and sophisticated style of writing · Identifying and addressing key issues raised by the question · Breadth and depth of relevant knowledge · Depth of analysis, Clarity of argument, Soundness of argument · Originality of argument You might find these articles relevant to the topicBaumeister, R. F. & Hastings, S. (1997). Distortions of collective memory: How groups flatter and deceive themselves. In J. W. Pennebaker, D. Paez, & B. Rimé. Collective memory of political events: Social psychological perspectives. Mahwah, NJ: LEA. Liu, J. H., & Hilton, D. J. (2005). How the past weights on the
  • 2. present: Social representations of history and their role in identity politics. British Journal of Social Psychology, 44, 537– 556. Lászlo ́, J. (2003). History, identity and narratives. In J. Lászlo ́, & W. Wagner (Eds.), Theories and controversies in societal psychology (pp. 180–192). Budapest, Hungary: New Mandate. Pennebaker, J. W., Paez, D., & Rime, B. (Eds.). (1997). Collective memory of political events: Social psychological perspectives. Mahwah, NJ: LEA. 1700 WORDS REQUIRED essay] and 300 words reflective writing, for reflective writing, I will send you the lecture slides and you will need to reflect upon those slide to get mark: Displays a systematic and sophisticated knowledge and understanding of the subject. Is extensively researched and shows a critical awareness of current issues within the field. Original argument reflecting synthetically on the issues raised in the module. Authoritative and reflective use of supporting material. Demonstrates a sophisticated understanding of theoretical and critical concepts which includes the ability to reflect critically. Excellently presented work that conforms to all required academic conventions. Written in a style that is lucid and precise.
  • 3. Displays a strong and intelligent knowledge and understanding ofthe subject. Is extensively researched and referenced in breadth and depth. slides 1.ppt Epistemology – how do we generate legitimate knowledge?Physical and metaphysical reality (Metaphysics: branch of philosophy that investigates the first principles of nature, e.g. ontology – the science of being)Psychological realities (experience and perception)Social and political realitiesHistorical ‘truth’ and historical ‘realities’ The module encourages a discussion about knowledge and reality… Critical social psychology (CSP) – challenges ‘taken for granted assumptions’ about the worldIt analyses social phenomena on the basis of: (i) power relations (ii) difference (how difference is constructed and maintained, e.g. nationality, race, gender, disability) Where does the module sit within psychology? Perceptual Cognitivism Reality - Perception - Discourse (language is assumed to be transparent, passive, a mirror) Discursive Psychology Discourse - Perception - Reality Function/purpose of communication, identity and language employed actively constructs the way we see the world
  • 4. TWO COMPETING MODELS OF REALITY Dunkirk: May 1940 DunkirkThe Dunkirk spiritThe Battle of FranceThe fall of France What difference does it make if we refer to this event in these different ways? Knowledge in all its forms is related to culture (especially history)Much of what we believe is what we are told by authorities within the culture, e.g. the writing of history textbooks.The culture is determined by the politics of the day, i.e. who is in power. Who decides what history should be taught? Who decides how it should be taught? Something to consider: * our subjective experience and…agreement with others about what is real and true (inter-subjectivity/consensus)
  • 5. N.B. This is not the same as an objectively real view of the world (God’s eye view) Our sense of reality is largely determined by: We usually confine myths to the past assuming previous civilisations to be primitive, unscientific, uninformed or mistaken in their representation of the pastAnything that fails to correspond with our current experience is usually described as a myth, e.g. supernatural manifestations of the gods‘ – ‘Greek myths’Some discredit biblical accounts of the past – e.g. The destruction of Sodom and Gommorah, the global flood, the miracles of Jesus but… Myth as rhetoric? Consider the situation in reverse: Imagine that you are living in an ancient world and I tell you that I believe that it is possible to:fly through the sky in a metal container heavier than a house, from one side of the earth to the other, faster than words can reach the ear?have a projectile weapon so powerful it could destroy the earth? i.e. we have recently gained access to knowledge and powers that the ancients did not understand.Is it possible that they had access to knowledge and powers that we do not understand? To what extent is the word ‘myth’ a form of rhetoric that seeks to discredit an alternative reality for which we have little or no understanding ? gives some credence to alternative takes on reality: the paranormal, UFO’s, the existence of angels, evil spirits, miracles and healing– they make reality and what is possible and impossible a matter of debate This argument…
  • 6. An empirical myth (a false belief), e.g. one that can be demonstrated to be false, e.g. drinking sulphuric acid is good for the body. A cultural myth (a belief that defines the culture), e.g. capitalism and democracy are the best systems to run a society, the American Dream, ‘might is right’, ‘The Dunkirk spirit’. A language system perpetuating a discourse that bolsters a myth (Postmodern / Feminist analysis) e.g. Who is the Chairman of the company? (Brown, 2003, p.184) If language systems endorse mythical beliefs and values then they need to be studied closely – this is the social constructionist position (e.g. Burr, Gergen, Berger & Luckmann) It may be helpful to think of the word ‘myth’ in three different ways: Scientific myths Religious myths Political myths Historical myths Could there be ‘modern myths’ within our cultural discourse? This was the principle question of the Enlightenment (Circa 1637- 1789)How should we determine what is true and false?People today often use the language of the Enlightenment – ‘facts, proof and certainty’
  • 7. How should reliable knowledge be generated? An understanding of sacred texts that had been passed down as part of Judeo-Christian and Islamic traditions (The Bible and the Qu’ran)Philosophical writings of Plato and AristotleMyth and magic (divination, sorcery etc.) and superstition Previously the world view had been determined by: Rationalism – knowledge determined by reason (Philosophy)Revelation – knowledge revealed by God (Theology)Empiricism – knowledge acquired through the senses, i.e. observation (Science) Philosophers have long argued that there are three legitimate sources of knowledge: Knowledge is rational, based on factsTheories are proved; myths rejectedKnowledge is certainJohn Locke (empiricist) influences politics and philosophyNewton’s mechanistic theories of the natural world (natural philosophy) Scientific revolution (From Copernicus to Newton – Circa 1543- 1727) The world would progress though this form of knowledge The project of modernity ?
  • 8. Scientism – Positivism – Empiricism – Humanism – Anarchism Absolute truth? Absolute truth is largely inaccessible Different versions of reality Relativism Consider the nature of arguments between people – court cases – competing historical accounts Philosophies of Science (Knowledge) Empiricism - Knowledge acquired through the senses (experience)Observation/ collect data/ formulate theoriesEmpirical evidence includes experiments, documents, personal testimonies The dominant approach in Western culture: Auguste Comte – French social philosopher – advanced the idea of a ‘science of humanity’Strict application of ‘the scientific method’ – leads to absolute certainties – no speculative knowledge, i.e. religion A more extreme form of empiricism Positivism Only scientific statements are meaningfulScientific methods should be used in every form of intellectual enquiry Scientism “No royal road to knowledge” – science is just one useful approachThere are other types of knowledge that are useful, e.g. subjective knowledge in counselling
  • 9. Humanism “Anything goes” – everything is true from the perspective of the perceiver – a extreme form of relativism Anarchism Science – not absolute knowledge (provisional) – the problem of inductionWhat can we expect science to explain and what is beyond its scope? The statement ‘God is good’ is beyond the scope of science since it can never be tested (Karl Popper)What has reductionist science done for humans? (Health care & nuclear weapons) Limits of Knowledge Theology – problem of interpretation of the written text (Competing denominations)Which revelation are we to believe and why? (Competing religious traditions)What criteria should we use to evaluate the veracity of religious texts? Limits of knowledge Authorship – who by? with what skills? personal motivation and bias?Authority – by what authority? e.g. biographyPolitical agenda of taught histories – national superiority?What is to be recorded /emphasised /excluded?Revisionist history (e.g. denial of holocaust by David Irving) The writing of history (Historical knowledge) Progressivistic writing (The Whig interpretation of history), i.e. to interpret the past in terms of principles of progress in order to justify the present Example – humans have progressed in
  • 10. knowledge and psychology is pushing back the boundaries The implication - what was past was inferior rather than different and what is present is superior Historical knowledge Perception of history affects/constructs identity and behaviour Contested histories are often at the heart of conflictThese interests lead us to consider the social representations of history (Moscovici,1961/1976) Historical knowledge is of interest to psychologists because: slides 2.pptx G.H. Mead (1863-1931) Mind and society are inseparable Society is as much in us as we are in it Our world view determines what we think and how we think As society changes, people think differently People in different places think differently Broad spectrum of cultural and historic thinking What role does the past play in the formation of identity? We can neither function nor be complete without memory, e.g. the effects of dementia A relationship with the past provides existential meaning in the form of a life narrative: Who am I? What have I done? What is the story of my life? A psychological relationship with the past starts at birth – social network provides narratives of the past – families, ethnic groups religion, nations. Immersion into these narrative is ”mnemonic socialisation” (Zerubavel, 1996)
  • 11. The “past weighs on the present” (Liu & Hilton, 2005) A reciprocal relationship There is a reciprocal relationship between social identity (group membership) and historical understanding The way we see ourselves and others affects the way we see the past - and the way we see the past affects the way we see ourselves and others Q. What do we have to do to change our perspective on the past? –to get nearer to the truth A. Loosen our attachment to certain social identities or reconfigure them Q. What would we need to do to see our own group more objectively? A. Loosen our attachment to absolute narratives of the past The development of personal identity Identity Process Theory (Breakwell, 1986, 1992, 1993) An interplay between self and experience – two psychological processes - assimilation-accommodation and evaluation Assimilation-accommodation process – new knowledge or experience is either assimilated into an existing identity structure or the identity structure is altered to accommodate the the experience. Included in this “new knowledge” would be accounts of the past related to family, group or nation. Orientation is towards “self-interest” rather than accuracy As each relevant account is heard, political and personal implications start to accrue. “The past weighs on the present” In therapeutic encounters – Freudian perspective
  • 12. A client’s relationship to their past is foundational to their well being What is remembered and how it is remembered becomes important How it is re-narrated and its effect on self and others If we extend this principle to: larger entities, national groups etc., we observe that political realities are often deeply affected, influenced even bound by the past. Some groups may appear unable to free themselves of the past (see Hewer & Kut, 2010, Hewer, 2012) The past is always with us! The world does not simply receive the past, It is “haunted” by it (Assmann, 1998; Frosh, 2013) “Although the past no longer exists, it does not simply disappear. It is subject to a dynamic process of re-engagement with the present, transforming itself into a social and psychological experience that shifts and evolves across generations. (Hewer, 2017) The social nature of memory When a relative gets dementia we do not lose our memory or identity – because our neurological systems are not connected However, when we consider the content of memory, we might draw the opposite conclusion? That despite having independent memory systems, memories are often shared or depend on the presence of others The construction, reconstruction of memories is a feature of everyday social interaction
  • 13. A “socialised”memory! A new concept for many psychology students Memory as “lived experience” rather than an archive (Tileaga, 2014) Shared, social or collective memory is not necessarily memory in the conventional sense - the recall of personal experience. Often, events directly experienced by others are passed on to us - we were not there or not yet born. The memory is socially shared through transmission not personal experience, e.g. accounts of the First World War. We construct cultural “remembrance” Some conceptual issues “At the one end of the imagined spectrum of approaches, taking a social view of memory may mean simply recognising that much of the content of individual remembering refers to things that are social in character – social occasions, relationships, conversational exchanges, shared endeavours, and so on. At the other end, it might mean positing a “group mind” endowed with a remembering capacity somehow distinct from that of group members. In between are ranged a variety of ways of thinking of memory as socially inflected or conditioned, interactive, culturally mediated, shared or socially distributed. Middling positions along this spectrum may be thought of as being reached either by “socializing” the concept of the remembering individual or by “writing” the individual back into collective memory.” (Cubitt 2014, p. 16).
  • 14. Taxonomies and Classifications No inter-disciplinary agreement – not everyone likes the term “collective memory” – it vague and imprecise – has come to replace “heritage” or “tradition”. Some scholars use social and collective memory interchangeably. Cubitt (2007), uses social memory to refer to social processes that sustain knowledge of past events within a group or culture while collective memory is a very specific product of these processes that presents a particular view or representation of the past. Cubitt also reminds us that memory is not an object with ontological substance therefore we need to guard against reifying psychological experience into social or psychological constructs. Early approaches Early approaches to the study of collective memory found expression within French sociology in the 1920s through the work of Maurice Halbwachs (1877-1945). Halbwachs was influenced by Emile Durkheim (1858-1917). In Social Frameworks of Memory (1925), Halbwachs argued that individual memories are acquired and recalled in a social milieu and that group settings provide the context that determines what and how we recall; these social frameworks include the family, social class or nation. This is consistent with Bartlett’s (1932) study of memory in experimental psychology - individual memory reflected patterns of thinking common to the social and cultural groups of the participants. Halbwachs also made a distinction between autobiographical memory - personal events, which have been experienced directly, and historical memory - events through which groups construct a continuous identity over time.
  • 15. Reinvigorated interest in the 1980s French historian Pierre Nora reinvigorated interest the collective past in the 1980s with his work Lieux de mémoire. He claimed that memory and its values, customarily passed down through family, school, church and state in France, were in decline. The disappearance of “peasant culture” as a source of collective memory had marked a significant change in French society leading to an erosion of “real memory” among people. Nora cites the “Jews of the diaspora” as an example of a community, which had no need for historians since memory was lived through everyday tradition and cultural practice. In the modern age, however, memory was being replaced by history – the way “our hopelessly forgetful modern societies, propelled by change, organize the past” (Nora, 1989, p. 8). As postwar France became “deeply absorbed in its own transformation and renewal” it valued “the new over the ancient, the young over the old and the future over the past” (Nora, 1989, p. 12). What remained, however, were lieux de mémoire - sites of memory - people, places, institutions, museums, works of art, holidays and events, which endorse and encourage, among other things, the creation of archives and the invention of anniversaries and celebrations. Nora argued that because there was no memory left, the culture had become preoccupied with it and the “memory boom” with its tendency towards nostalgia and incessant memorialization had taken up a role in contemporary culture (Nora, 1989, 2002). What is the difference between history and collective remembering? Wertsch & Roediger, (2008) make a distinction between collective memory - a static body of knowledge - and collective remembering: an active reconstruction of the past, which takes
  • 16. place in the present. They also draw a line between history - the study of what actually happened, which employs critical, complex and reflective thought - and collective remembering - an identity project that is intolerant of ambiguity or counter-narratives. Wertsch & Roediger claim that: “History is willing to change a narrative in order to be loyal to facts, whereas collective remembering is willing to change information (even facts) in order to be loyal to a narrative.” (2008, p. 324) Selection, omission and distortion History can be used to construct national identity, which often involves promoting a particular version of the past that relies upon and contributes to a knowledge deficit within the general population (Baumeister & Hastings, 1997). Different positioning on the past may reflect a degree of collective narcissism where the narrative of the in-group invokes a sense of entitlement and superiority (Golec de Zavala, Cichocka, Eidelson & Jayawickreme, 2009). Why was collective memory ignored by the Social Sciences from 1945–1990? Arguably, there was only one memory in the West - a metanarrative of Allied victory over the Nazis. Brown (2005) describes metanarratives as: “grand, ideological generalized stories by which societies understand themselves, and which are so normative and all- consuming, that individuals in a society are not aware of them as constantly re-circulated. Metanarratives only work when they become invisible by having no acceptable opposites.” (p. 184). Little thought was given to the existence of counter-memories in, for example, the Soviet Union (see Wertsch, 2002) or other parts of the Eastern bloc (see also Hewer & Kut, 2010) that
  • 17. eventually emerged to challenge particular aspects of the Allied account of WWII. Political events Eventually, the fall of the Berlin wall in 1989, and the Soviet Union shortly after, provided the psychological and political space for other suppressed memories to emerge from within the Eastern bloc. In the early 1990s, memory was at the root of political unrest and ethnic hatred in the former Yugoslavia as nationalist regimes sought to act upon a historical narrative of conflict, separation, independence and dominance. Even in Germany, after the peaceful unification of East and West in 1992, some aspects of World War II were subject to subtle differences of interpretation (von Benda-Beckmann, 2011). Official and Vernacular Memory State versions of the past, endorsed through education and commemoration, may be at variance with versions of the past of ordinary people (Tileagă, 2014). The distinction between official and vernacular memory (the memory of ordinary people) occurs in places where either an authoritarian or hegemonic state has controlled memory, e.g. Romania under Ciacescu during the communist period, or a dominant memory silences people whose feelings are not considered important. Counter-memories cannot be expressed either because they would be punished; or because such views are not welcome in the current political climate, or the group has no “voice” – no political power to make themselves heard.
  • 18. Harald Welzer (2002) also refers to two contrasting modes of memory: lexicon – e.g. the official account of Germany’s role in World War Two –and album - the narratives of loss and suffering among the German people, e.g. relatives of German soldiers. Communicative and cultural memory Egyptologist, Jan Assmann uses the term mnemohistory to describe any form of historical investigation concerned with the way in which the past is remembered. In his analysis of European memory of Ancient Egypt, he Assmann proposes the concept of cultural memory: the idea that “objectivized culture has the structure of memory” (Assmann & Czaplicka, 1995, p. 128). The Greeks viewed culture “not only as based on memory, but as a form of memory in itself” (p. 15). Cultural Memory “Cultural memory is a form of collective memory, in the sense that it is shared by a number of people and that it conveys to these people a collective, that is, cultural, identity. Halbwachs, the inventor of the term “collective memory”, was careful to keep his concept of collective memory apart from the realm of traditions, transmissions and transferences, which we propose to subsume under the term “cultural memory”. We preserve Halbwachs’s distinction by breaking up his concept of collective memory into “communicative” and “cultural memory”, but we insist on including the cultural sphere, which he excluded in his study of memory. We are therefore not asking for replacing his idea of “collective memory” with “cultural memory”: rather we distinguish between both forms as two different modi memoranda, ways of remembering”.
  • 19. (Assmann, 1998, p. 210) Characteristics of communicative and cultural memory (after Assmann, 1992) Communicative Content: Historical experiences in the individual life course Form: Informal, natural, based on interpersonal communication Media: Human memory immediate, experiences, oral tradition Time Structure: 80–100 years, 3-4 generations Carriers: Contemporaries Cultural Myth of origin, archaic history and absolute past Formal, festive and ceremonial Recorded, objectified in writing, dancing, pictures, etc. Absolute past to mythical times Professional carriers of tradition How may collective memory be accessed and studied?
  • 20. Collective memory has no ontological substance – and therefore it is not helpful to think of it as stored or located in a three dimensional space. Sociologist Jeffrey Olick (1999) distinguishes between collected memory, which refers to the measurement and aggregation of individual memories, and collective memory, which refers to symbols and deeper structures, which are not reducible to individual psychological processes. An aggregation approach (collected memory) may be limited. Olick refers to Durkheim’s position that collective representations transcend the individual insofar that they are not dependent on any one individual for their existence. For Durkheim, culture cannot be reduced to what goes on in the minds of individuals (Olick, 2010). This may apply to collective memory Indeed, while individual memory may be traced back to neural networks in the brain, there is no equivalent and naturally occurring substrate or network for group memory. Some may argue that individual group members comprise a substrate, but this introduces a different set of assumptions and a different methodological approach to the study of the collective past. If collective memory cannot be reduced to what goes on within the minds of individuals, then it may be inferred from monuments, museums, street names, media images, documentary, commemorative practices, libraries, archives and institutions. Researchers have to decide where they stand on this issue and to be aware of the implications for their own research claims. Social Representations of History Social representations theory: Moscovici (1961/1976) We might argue that representations of the past exist both in people’s heads and in the culture – social representations
  • 21. approach e.g. Liu and Hilton (2005) Dresden: 1991 Image and Iconography in Public Space Memory as narrative Narrative thinking is an alternative and complementary form of human cognition to scientific thinking. It employs a plot, a theme and an object lesson. Story telling is an integral part of the construction of social realities (Bruner, 1986, 1990). Stories or narratives contain cause and effect explanations, which are seldom subjected to test or refutation. Narratives contain universal principles about the world. They form the basis of assertion and argument: it is a way of thinking that seeks correspondence and coherence they rather than truth (László, 2003). Memory through symbol, metaphor and personalisation Josef Stalin
  • 22. Transmission: Memory is a process not an object Historical legacies through birth (Weissmark, 2004) Story telling is transmission Comedy – Springtime for Hitler, Hogan’s Heroes, Allo, Allo. Journalism - TV, newspaper, blog Arts - novels, films – dramatic licence Time Conception How can the past seem nearer as time passes? How can this paradox be explained? Paul Janet in William James Principles of Psychology. Time perception is related to the length of the life course. Linear time vs. psychological time. Time perception is psychological not linear. Psychological time – 1945 is only 70 years ago (3 life spans if you are 23) – In 2045, when you are 53, it will be two life spans – the distant event will have moved forward in psychological time. Linear time vs. traumatic time – “You would have thought they’d have got over by now!” (Holocaust Exhibition: London, 2006) The Politics of Memory Who controls public memory? Why should the history curriculum and the writing of history textbooks be subject to public scrutiny? (Carretero, 2011)
  • 23. Is it better to remember or forget historic violence? How may resolving different histories and or collective memories be achieved? Discussion: How long should we commemorate the war dead of World War I? Why do you so answer? How long should we commemorate the war dead of World War II and why? Who are we remembering and why? Who is we? Is this “remembrance” likely to change? Contents .docx Contents Foreword v Grandpa Wasn’t a Nazi: The Holocaust in German Family Remembrance 1 Design of the Study 2 Making Sense of History: Changes in Transmission through the Generations 3 Cumulative Heroization 9 The Effects of Heroization 16 How the Jews Are Remembered: “These People Had Money” 18 How “the Nazis” Are Remembered: As Others 22 “Empty Talking” 24 A Broadly Representative Survey 25 Postscript: How the Study Was Received 27 Notes 30
  • 24. iii Foreword As Americans and Germans have grappled with the terrifying events of 1933-45, both have come to understand the importance of preserving memory of the Holocaust and incorporating the lessons of history in creat- ing democratic and tolerant societies. Holocaust remembrance today in Germany takes many forms: formal educational curricula, television pro- grams, books, debates in the media, commemorative events, and the recent opening of a major memorial in the heart of Berlin. Such a widespread educational effort has borne results. A 2005 AJC survey found that 77 per- cent of Germans questioned could correctly identify Auschwitz, Dachau, and Treblinka—higher than the percentages among American and British respondents— and 79 percent thought that teaching about the Nazi exter- mination of the Jews should be required in schools (about the same as responses in the United States and Britain). How does one measure the success of Holocaust education? The research study summarized in this monograph by Prof. Harald Welzer, director of the Center for Interdisciplinary Memory Research at the Insti- tute for Cultural Studies in Essen, suggests that what is learned cognitively is not always absorbed into the heart. Welzer interviewed forty Western and Eastern German families, both in a family setting and individually, to discover how they interpreted their objective knowledge of the history of the Third Reich in terms of their own family history. He found that the history transmitted through intergenerational conversation was quite dif- ferent from the textbook history of the Holocaust period. Almost all the younger Germans interviewed believed that their own grandparents had not been involved or had opposed the racist policies of Nazism. Even when grandparents admitted their participation in Nazi crimes, their offspring did not “hear” them or reinterpreted the stories in ways that turned their forebears into heroes. This process of “cumulative heroization” reflects the natural tendency to associate positive elements and
  • 25. block out negative ones for the people we love. But there is danger lurking in this all-too-human phenomenon. First, the story passed down by successive generations increasingly whitewashes v vi Foreword and distorts history, so that not turning in one’s family doctor, who was Jewish, as opposed to hiding him, or giving a glass of milk to passing refugees after the war was over, is portrayed as a heroic act of resistance. Second, by separating the evil of Nazism from the good qualities associated with beloved relatives, the respondents managed to distinguish between the “Nazis,” who are “others,” and the “Germans” who were themselves victims of Nazism. All this suggests that, despite extensive, in-depth Holocaust education, both in formal settings and in the general culture, the phenomena of “my grandpa was not a Nazi” and of “politically correct talk” remain problemat- ic. The good news, on the other hand, is that this study made a significant impact upon publication in Germany and entered into the public dis- course about the residual effects of the war years. From this discussion, we hope will emerge a more probing appraisal of the past and a greater vigi- lance toward the future. The American Jewish Committee has had a long and significant rela- tionship with Germany since AJC’s founding in 1906, mainly by Ameri- can Jews of German origin. Shortly after World War II, AJC became the first Jewish organization to develop programs with Germany, working to promote democratization and to spur awareness of the Holocaust. Some twenty-five years ago, the Konrad Adenauer Foundation and AJC initiated a series of exchanges between American Jews and Germans, building bridges by visiting each other’s countries and engaging in intense dialogue. Since then, AJC has collaborated with other German political foundations as well. In 1998, AJC became the first American Jewish organization to open a permanent office in Berlin, the Lawrence and Lee Ramer
  • 26. Center for German-Jewish Relations. The Berlin office brought this important study to our attention, and engaged Belinda Cooper to translate it into English. She performed her task with skill. In sharing Prof. Welzer’s research with an English-speaking audience, we are attempting to make a wider public aware of the complex interplay of national and family historical memories. From this under- standing, we can grapple more effectively with the complexities of the past while striving for a brighter future. David A. Harris Executive Director, American Jewish Committee August 2005 Grandpa Wasn’t a Nazi: The Holocaust in German Family Remembrance1 In Germany, Holocaust education in all its forms is very popular and suc- cessful. Students learn about Nazism and the Shoah from school curricula, participate in commemorative programs, and make trips to Holocaust sites. Surveys show that young Germans are generally quite well informed about the historical events and can correctly define key terms such as “Auschwitz” and “SS.” Thus education on the history of the Third Reich might be considered a success—if one doesn’t ask how the young targets of this educational campaign actually use the product. Knowledge and the assimilation of knowledge on a personal basis are two very different things. For too long, the tacit assumption was that one needed only to trans- mit the right message for the lessons to be assimilated. There was little appreciation for the range of subtexts— fascinating, daunting, and anes- thetizing—that accompany the transmission of history. As this study shows, young Germans acquire knowledge of history in general, and of Nazism and the Holocaust in particular, in a way very different from what their educators have intended. A young person’s awareness of history and his concepts about the past come from many sources: films, television, novels, comics, computer games, and family histories, among others. History lessons are but one source among many. Formal courses
  • 27. aim to pass on knowledge, but cannot compete with the emotional impact of images from the past offered by more immediate sources. Cognitive knowledge of history pales beside the emotional relationship to the past that come from one’s own grandparents talking about their lives “before our time.” Surprisingly, research on histor- ical consciousness has only recently delved into these other sources.2 Research on the effects of Holocaust education has only slowly broad- ened its scope. Until recently, the research consisted of objective studies of historical awareness3 and qualitative studies of the ways in which young Germans dealt with the history of this unparalleled crime.4 The questions asked more recently are not about knowledge of history alone, but about the use of such knowledge—for example, how history lessons on Nazism teach students the politically correct way to talk about the Third Reich,5 1 2 Grandpa Wasn’t a Nazi how immigrant children absorb history lessons that have little relevance for their group of origin,6 or how trips to historical sites impart messages about the past.7 These new studies, while exploratory and limited in scope, suggest some distressing conclusions. The present multigenerational study observes the direct communication of concepts about the past in German families and finds a pronounced discrepancy between the official and the private cultures of remembrance in Germany. It documents a clear tenden- cy on the part of grandchildren to rewrite their grandparents’ histories into tales of anti-Nazi heroism and resistance. The pilot study on history teach- ing by Radtke et al. (2002) suggests that students learn one primary thing in classes on the Third Reich: how to talk in a politically correct way about the problematic past. Gudehus’s investigation produces similar results, and Georgi’s study comes to the remarkable conclusion that immigrant chil- dren use their study of the Nazi past as a ticket to seeing themselves as “true Germans.”8
  • 28. Overall, the beneficiaries of these efforts in historical education have shown themselves stubborn and unpredictable, which is why further research on the outcome of history teaching appears both nec- essary and promising. Design of the Study The research project, entitled “Transmitting Historical Awareness,” dealt with family communication about the Nazi period in the Federal Republic of Germany. For the study, forty Western and Eastern German families were interviewed by researchers both in the context of a whole family dis- cussion and in separate interviews with at least one member of the eyewit- ness, child, and grandchild generations in the family.9 The design of the study was quite simple: The members of the eyewitness generation were asked about their biographical experience during the wartime period after 1933; then their children and grandchildren were asked what they had heard from their parents and grandparents about the same period. A family discussion was triggered with a brief video of amateur footage from the Third Reich period. A total of 182 interviews and family discussions were conducted. The material was transcribed and evaluated through a combination of textual analysis of individual transcripts and computer-based qualitative content analysis. The following examples drawn from the interview material will illus- trate how history is transmitted through conversation among the genera- tions, how anti-Jewish stereotypes are similarly passed down, and how Germans interpret the roles of their parents or grandparents in the Third Reich. The conclusion will examine how this study entered into public dis- course in Germany. Making Sense of History: Changes in Transmission through the Generations Johanna Kurz10 was born in 1927. Her father was in the SA (Sturmab- teilung, the “brown shirts”) and the SS (Schutzstaffel, originally Hitler’s per- sonal guard), both elite
  • 29. units within the German military, and her mother had been in the Nazi women’s organization for a short time, but quit “after two or three years.” Johanna Kurz: I only know that we stared at those smoking ruins.... The synagogue wasn’t destroyed; it was just burned out. Everything was smolder- ing, and my mother almost went crazy. She said, “How can they do that?” It was like a church for her. But it wasn’t just in Hanover; on the contrary, it was everywhere, you know. And I remember that my mother said to my father, “I know you were involved; don’t talk to me ever again!” Interviewer: But he wasn’t involved, or was he? Johanna Kurz: I don’t know. I don’t think so, but I don’t know. I’d like—I can’t say, I don’t know. Interviewer: So it was just an expressed threat? Johanna Kurz: The two of them never came together again, and then the war broke out, and the marriage just went on the side, nothing violent. When he came home in 1947, he came back from prison, and in 1948 they divorced. Earlier in the interview Johanna Kurz had revealed the fact of her father’s SA and SS membership. When she got to talking about the burn- ing synagogue, however, the interviewer, born in 1971, could not believe that Ms. Kurz’s father was “involved.” The interviewer’s suggestive follow- up considerably shook Ms. Kurz’s confidence, and her uncertainty seems to have strengthened the interviewer in her views: The interviewer’s inter- Making Sense of History 3 4 Grandpa Wasn’t a Nazi pretation that it “was just ... a threat” caused Ms. Kurz to answer (but at the same time not answer) by reporting the result. It was not just a threat; apparently, the conflict was severe enough that her parents “never came together again.” The interviewer, who noted in her report of the interview that she had found Johanna Kurz extremely nice, apparently could
  • 30. not accept the possi- bility that Johanna’s father was an actor in Kristallnacht. Despite her in- depth knowledge of the history of the Third Reich, the interviewer resisted the possibility that even a relative of the old lady she was interviewing could have been a fellow traveler, much less a perpetrator, of the persecu- tion of Jews. This example illustrates how quickly loyalty ties become generalized within the social situation of a conversation. The interviewer, with her hopeful follow-up question, was not only trying to absolve her dialogue partner of any suspicion of complicity in criminal activity, but was extend- ing the cover to close relatives of the interviewee, whom she neither knew nor could have known. It was as if she had not at all absorbed what Ms. Kurz had just told her about her father’s history. This phenomenon could often be found in family discussions—even, or maybe especially—when the explicit theme was murders committed by the storyteller. Rainer Hofer, born in 1925, was a NAPOLA (an elite national socialist school) student and a member of the Waffen-SS and of the SS-unit “Leib- standarte Adolf Hitler” (named for Hitler’s personal bodyguard unit). He presented himself, both in the individual interview and the family discus- sion, as a reformed Nazi. Although he wrote in his diary, at the news of the Führer’s death, “My best comrade has fallen,” in his retrospective telling, he was appalled by the Nazi crimes. Mr. Hofer, well-read and well-educat- ed, made a career in postwar Germany as a manager and, in his own way, contributed to the rebuilding of the state. He speaks unself-consciously about joining the Waffen-SS, entering the Leibstandarte, participating in the Russian campaign, and, in 1943, being deployed as an SS man in the Ukraine. In this connection, the interviewer asks: Interviewer: Are there any stories that you wouldn’t tell your daughter or your grandchildren? Rainer Hofer: No, I would be completely open. I don’t need to tell them that I shot Jews [he bangs on the table] or that sort of thing; even if I had done it,
  • 31. I would tell about it. Why? It’s my daughter and I lived my life. I can’t let any of it somehow sink into the Hades of the past.... There’s nothing ... I wouldn’t tell her, even if it touched on the honor of German soldiers. I remember once that we rode to an attack, and when we came back, attached to infantry, a couple of Russian soldiers were idiotic enough to surrender. Of course, they didn’t live a moment longer [knocks on the table]. But that, of course, was one of those things: Where were they supposed to ride with us? In the tank? They could have had a hand grenade hidden somewhere [laughs].... If they had just laid low, nothing would have happened. But I’d tell my daughter, even though it actually touched on the honor of German soldiers. I can’t say there was anything that I wouldn’t tell her or my granddaughter either. Why should I? As if to prove his openness, Hofer describes to the interviewer a crime that might sully “the honor of German soldiers”—but without questioning the crime from today’s perspective. On the contrary, Hofer provides a jus- tification for the murder of the Russian prisoners, assuming that his calcu- lus would be obvious, even to the interviewer. Anyway, this is all part of his life as he lived it—so why, asks Hofer rhetorically, shouldn’t he talk about it? As further evidence of Mr. Hofer’s openness, the Hofer family archive even contains letters he sent home from the Eastern Front, one of which he alludes to in the interview: Rainer Hofer: I’m horrified today about what I wrote then. What [laughs] I can’t understand today.... We, of course, saw Russian women on the oppo- site side, in uniform, with weapons and armed and, imagine this, at [he knocks on the table] eighteen years old, I shot one down with my machine gun and wrote very proudly that “the head and the breast were just a bloody mess,” or something like that. Today you wonder how you could have writ- ten something like that. Note that he questions not how he could do the killing, but how he could write about it—a subjective assessment of the act. It should be pointed out that it was not usual for such documents
  • 32. to be kept in family archives and to be known to the children. Generally, a rather nebulous for- mulation is used—e.g., “something happened”—leaving listeners to draw from vaguely portrayed events a story that best allows them to live with the central conflict of German family history a half-century after the Third Reich: that is, the conflict between the awareness by members of the chil- Making Sense of History 5 6 Grandpa Wasn’t a Nazi dren’s and grandchildren’s generations of the criminal nature of Nazism and the Holocaust, on the one hand, and the need to position their own parents or grandparents in a way that they were untouched by the horror. This is not an easy task, especially when, as in the case of Rainer Hofer, the details of the crimes were written down. Regina Seiler, his daughter, knows the letters, but surprisingly, emphasizes repeatedly, in her individual interview and in the family discussion, how important it is for her to “fig- ure out what people were thinking back then.” Regina Seiler: I can’t imagine that the German people, even my father, ... I really think they couldn’t imagine that something like that could happen. Although Ms. Seiler refers directly to the letter in her interview (“and in the war he wrote to his parents that they had just attacked a Russian vil- lage; he was sixteen or eighteen years old, I don’t remember exactly, and I was so upset by how euphorically he talked about it”), she repeatedly spec- ulates whether Germans could have “imagined” that something like a war of extermination and a Holocaust could happen. What motivates this question, which so obviously ignores the fact that her father did not need to “imagine” crimes that he himself committed? The question of whether the father “could have imagined” functions to maneuver him out of the perpetrator group, into the much less suspect group of accidental witnesses or perhaps fellow travelers—since a person imagines only that for which he has no personal knowledge.
  • 33. Second, Ms. Seiler argues, very similarly to her father, within a framework of a dual structure of knowledge and ignorance: The crimes undoubtedly occurred, but no one could imagine them. Her own father took part in them, but the daughter does not acknowledge it. How this dual structure functions in talking about the past is shown in the family discussion. There Mr. Hofer relates that in 1944, after his deployment to the Ukraine, he was in a tank division in France and heard from a Sturmbannführer “that in the East, in Russia, partisans in any case, but also other people, were killed by a shot to the neck and so forth. I remember [laughs] that we talked about this later among the comrades, and we thought, ‘He’s crazy!’” Having done the exact same thing half a year before, Mr. Hofer now states he couldn’t believe others were doing this. It would be an underesti- mation of the effect of this dual structure to say that Hofer is lying. The fact that he tells about both his own deeds and his disbelief at the deeds of others shows that he can integrate his own deeds subjectively into a mean- ingful context of rationality and morality, and, as such, he excludes them from the overall accusation of criminality. Thus, perpetrators such as Mr. Hofer do not regard their actions as part of the Holocaust—and this self-perception provides an interpretative option readily embraced by the following generations. In the group discus- sion Ms. Seiler concentrates with quite penetrating questions on finding out from her father why no one could “imagine” what was happening— thereby protecting him from the knowledge that he and she herself actual- ly share. In the Hofer family interaction, what is transmitted is not knowledge of the crime, but rather knowledge of how one can at the same time know and not know. Contrary to the widespread notion that grandparents and parents do not tell their children and grandchildren problematic wartime stories— especially ones that highlight their participation in Nazi crimes—some of the interviewees do talk about their
  • 34. experiences during the war in ways that show them as perpetrators. This does not, however, lead their listeners to dismay, to conflicts, or even to embarrassing situations. It leads to noth- ing at all. It is as if such tales were not heard by the family members pres- ent. Apparently, ties of family loyalty prevent a father or grandfather from appearing to be someone who killed people a few decades earlier. The images formed about a beloved relative through socialization and time spent together are retroactively applied to the earlier period in the family member’s life as well, before his offspring, who are now listening and will later pass on the wartime stories, were born. The ignoring of perpetrator stories occurs accidentally, as if on automatic pilot—the tape recorder records the stories, but the family’s memory does not. In other words, wartime memories are preserved in the family’s lore as stories that can be reshaped to an idealized vision that succeeding generations have of the eye- witness who is telling them. And so they are remembered and retold. Moving down the generations, stories become so altered that in the end they undergo a complete change of meaning. This reconfiguration generally functions to turn grandparents into people of constant moral Making Sense of History 7 8 Grandpa Wasn’t a Nazi integrity, according to today’s standards and normative appraisal. The reformulation of stories is undertaken precisely because, in interviews, most members of the children’s and grandchildren’s generations express no doubt at all that Nazism was a criminal system and the Holocaust an unparalleled crime. This assessment of the Nazi past—the standard fare of history lessons, the media, and the official German culture of commemo- ration—breaks down when questioning the role played by one’s own grandparents during the period; it rather evokes the subjective need to assign one’s grandfather or grandmother the role of the “good” German in everyday life under the Nazis.
  • 35. Thus the paradoxical result of successful education about the Nazi past comes to this: The more comprehensive the knowledge about war crimes, persecution, and extermination, the stronger is the need to develop stories to reconcile the crimes of “the Nazis” or “the Germans” with the moral integrity of parents or grandparents. This reconciliation can only be achieved through stories that show one’s relatives as human beings who perhaps cautiously, but also coura- geously, defied contemporary norms and worked against the system, even if, based on their party membership and functions, they were anything but opponents of the system. The eyewitnesses appear in the retellings of their descendants to be inconspicuous resistance fighters, clever enough to blend in from the outside but, when push came to shove, ready to help victims of persecution, hide Jews, or carry out small acts of resistance. These stories of “being against” the system are embedded in the idea that any nonconformist behavior, from “opening one’s mouth” to “protect- ing Jews,” from continuing to “buy from Jews” to showing opposition to superiors, could potentially have brought the harshest consequences. Thus, from the perspective of their progeny, grandparents who acted courageous- ly found themselves chronically in danger of career setbacks, family con- flicts, concentration camp, or even death sentences as a result of their views and behavior. Thus the seventeen-year-old grandson of the Groothe family defends his forebears: Lars Groothe: I think in any case that most people thought that, for example, Jews ... are people.... But, as one individual you couldn’t defend yourself. As one person, you couldn’t do anything. You could say, “I think it’s bad,” [but] you’d be locked up and probably shot. This interpretation allows a synthesis between the image of a totalitari- an system with its coercive methods and the reinterpreted grandparents’ roles. It is the product of an intergenerational chain whereby, in many sto- ries told by the
  • 36. eyewitness generation, their parents are described as people who were “against” the system. The rehabilitation of the great- grandpar- ents’ generation as anti-Nazi can go so far as to portray an “old fighter” and a “staunch Nazi” who was a Nazi Ortsgruppenleiter [local political leader] in 1931 as someone who was always ready to oppose the norms of Nazi soci- ety, for example, by continuing to shop “from Jews,” by doing “business with the Jewish cattle trader,” and finally, according to his great-grandson, “hiding” Jews. Cumulative Heroization The term “cumulative heroization” describes the phenomenon of history becoming ameliorated from generation to generation. Cumulative heroiza- tion appeared in twenty-six of the forty families interviewed—that is, in two-thirds of all cases. Heroizing stories made up roughly 15 percent of all stories told in the interviews and family discussions; stories of forebears’ victimization accounted for around 50 percent; thus two-thirds of all the stories were about family members from the eyewitness generation (or their relatives) who were either victims of the Nazi past and/or heroes of everyday resistance. Like her sixty-five-year-old son Bernd Hoffmann, ninety-one- year-old Elli Krug insists in the individual interview and in the family discussion that she did not know what a concentration camp was until the end of the war, even though she lived near Bergen-Belsen. After the war, however, for- mer inmates of the camp passed through her village, and Mrs. Krug was forced by the British occupiers to make her home available to them—a sit- uation that clearly displeased her. Elli Krug: The Jews were the worst afterwards. They really harassed us.... They sat there and made us serve them, and then they didn’t want.... We had this big hayloft [where] they slept overnight. ... The Jews and Russians, I always made sure that I didn’t get them. They were really disgusting, you Cumulative Heroization 9 10 Grandpa Wasn’t a Nazi know? I always stood down the street, in front of the gate, and
  • 37. when they said, “Quarters,” I said, “No, everything’s full!” If the Jews ... came, I said, “It’s all full of Russians, you can come in with me!” ... And when the Rus- sians came, then I said the same thing, that there were Jews here. Mrs. Krug still recalls how she was able to avoid giving shelter to “Jews” and “Russians” through a trick, while the characteristics she attributes to them (“the worst,” “disgusting”) indicate a clear anti-Semitic or racist atti- tude, even today. The fact that she is speaking about accommodating pris- oners who had survived the nearby Bergen-Belsen concentration camp is not an issue for her at all. The main theme of her story is the burden that she took on by providing shelter and her clever technique for keeping the “Jews” and the “Russians” out of her yard. Her son also relates that people did not know about the camps until the end of the war, but he tells a story he heard from his deceased wife. She had worked on an estate near Bergen-Belsen and heard that the owner hid escapees from the camp. Bernd Hoffmann called this person “the grand- ma.” Bernd Hoffmann: She [his wife] was on a farm in Belsen for a year. They came right by there. The grandma hid some of them, and they sat in a wood- en box. Then [the SS-men] went around, searching everywhere.... They would have shot the grandma immediately. She put a hot pot with boiling potatoes on top of the wooden box so they wouldn’t get them. The twenty-six-year-old granddaughter, Silvia Hoffmann, now tells her version of what her own grandmother did: Silvia Hoffmann: Once she told a story I thought was really interesting: Our village was on the road to Bergen-Belsen, and she hid someone who escaped from one of those transports, in a really interesting way, in some grain box with straws sticking out—she really hid them. Then people came and looked in her farmyard and she kept quiet. That’s a little thing that I really give her a lot of credit for. Her story pieces together elements that were mentioned in her grand- mother’s and her father’s separate stories: The “road to
  • 38. Bergen-Belsen,” a stout-hearted woman, the box, even the haystack, in the form of a straw, have all left a mark on the granddaughter’s story. But the narrative matrix in which the actors now appear produces a new story: The estate-owner grandmother is adopted, wooden boxes and all, and the hay becomes a dramatic element in a tale of how her own grandmother tricked the perse- cutors. Thus, the granddaughter creates an image for herself of a good grandmother, based on elements present in neither her grandmother’s nor her father’s stories. Cumulative heroization occurs rapidly and simply. A generalized image of a respected grandmother or grandfather provides a framework in which any point of reference suggested by family stories can be expanded into a “good story.” As in the case of Silvia Hoffmann, this can result in a strip- ping away of the problematic implications of the true tale; plots become rearranged to reshape the nuanced, ambivalent, often troubling tales by the eyewitnesses into a morally clear attitude on the part of the protagonists— a clearly positive one. The tendency to heroize the grandparents’ genera- tion shows the never-to-be- underestimated strong effects of ties of loyalty to loved ones on historical awareness and the retrospective construction of the past. In the telling the Grubitsch family history, Sieglinde Grubitsch, born in 1907, relates: Sieglinde Grubitsch: But our Dr. Weinberg was a Jew, and his wife was a teacher; we protected them; they could live until the end. This strong statement implies that the grandparents saved the Wein- berg family from deportation (without, however, specifying what “protec- tion” consisted of and what was meant by the family “could live until the end.”11) The grandson, Erich Grubitsch, Jr., born in 1962, asks a follow-up question: “How did you protect them?” Whether his question resonated with skepticism or merely asked for more detail is not clear. The answer:
  • 39. Sieglinde Grubitsch: Well, because we never bothered them. We never felt bothered by them, and they didn’t disturb us. We didn’t, like those patriots, say, “There are Jews here; we don’t want anything to do with them.” Or, “Take them away!” Compared with the statement about “protecting” the Jewish family, this explanation is sobering in its narrowness. Sieglinde Grubitsch reports only not “bothering” the Jewish family—a remarkable comment, since ear- lier she had spoken empathically of “our Dr. Weinberg.” The “protection” extended to the Weinberg family, it turns out, consisted merely of not denouncing them, as “the patriots” might have done. The Grubitsch fami- Cumulative Heroization 11 12 Grandpa Wasn’t a Nazi ly didn’t do anything—and this, from her perspective, was worth mention- ing, if not calling an act of resistance. In the individual interview, Erich Grubitsch, Jr., defends this interpre- tation: Erich Grubitsch, Jr.: It was a totalitarian regime. Who knows what we would have done? ... On the other hand, they supposedly rescued a couple of Jews. That has to be acknowledged. OK, we can’t understand everything today— but at least they tried, or actually managed to do it. This assessment seizes on the often-repeated belief that it is not possi- ble to judge behavior under totalitarianism from today’s perspective. The grandson’s formulation that his grandparents “supposedly” rescued “a cou- ple of Jews” retains some skepticism, but the triggering statement, which claimed nothing more than the failure to denounce, now leads to an acknowledgment that “at least they tried, or actually managed to do it.” Once again, the desire to find moral integrity—or, even better, opposition- al behavior—on the part of the grandparents leads the offspring to ignore the actual content of the original tale. After all, not feeling “bothered” by the presence of a Jewish family—especially when the father was the Gru-
  • 40. bitsches’ family doctor—is hardly something to be especially proud of. Ignoring the problematic aspects, the grandchild leaves open the question of whether the grandparents actually “saved” the doctor’s family or just “tried” to. In either case, they would have done something that deserves acknowledgment by present-day normative standards. A very similar statement by granddaughter Tina Kunze (born in 1980) occurs in conversation with the Ross family: Tina Kunze: ...that’s why grandpa might have hidden the two Jews, like Mama said before. In the individual interview with Tina Kunze’s father, this episode was related as follows: Gerhard Kunze: Once a train went by with prisoners, and people stayed at the farm once for a short time, people who didn’t know where else to go, but there was always great danger connected with that. In the story as transmitted by Gerhard Kunze (born in 1951), those who did the acting were “people” who “stayed” at the grandparents’ farm. The context of persecution and deportation is established by mention of the “train with prisoners,” “people who didn’t know where else to go,” and the “great danger connected with that.” The latter suggests, in a subtle transformation, that the actions of the people had endangered the grand- parents. The father’s version of the story is more ambiguous than the granddaughter’s much more decisive variant, in which the active ones are the grandparents who “maybe hid two Jews.” The eyewitness Margarethe Ross, born in 1919, recounts the following: Margarethe Ross: What can I tell you? At night someone knocked at our door, and there were two concentration camp women ... the war was proba- bly already over ... and they knocked at my mother’s door. My mother took them in, and I think gave them a glass of milk first, and let them sleep in the house. The eyewitness account reveals that it was not Tina Kunze’s
  • 41. “grandpa” but her great-grandmother who was involved, and that the story of the two Jews she “might have hidden” happened at the end of the war. They were probably displaced persons to whom her great-grandmother offered shel- ter. The episode—in the granddaughter’s view a courageous act of “hiding” and in the father’s view entailing “great danger”—is revealed here, in Mar- garethe Ross’s tale, to be an extension of help involving no danger whatso- ever. Without belittling the great-grandmother’s act of kindness, one can say that it has been heroized through successive generations. Similar exaggeration through retelling is found everywhere in our material, sometimes as complex stories, sometimes as small references or fragments of tales. All enjoy the same trajectory: Harmless, often question- able, and sometimes even scandalous acts recounted by the eyewitnesses are mediated more abstractly through the second generation, with details changed and opened to interpretation; these accounts are then clarified in the grandchildren’s generation into a finding that the grandparents “helped,” “hid,” or “saved,” even if it was dangerous for them. The stress on danger functions as a backdrop to the passing on of fam- ily history: The dangerous situation highlights more emphatically the courageous behavior of the forebears. The invention of ethically motivated behavior by the grandparents harmonizes with the image the grandchil- dren already have of them, while reinforcing their knowledge of the absolute tyranny of the Third Reich, which could turn anyone—even one’s Cumulative Heroization 13 14 Grandpa Wasn’t a Nazi own grandparents—into victims. The grandchildren attempt to preserve this benign image of their relatives at any price. The Eeven family, too, has passed down a heroic story that goes back to the great-grandfather, who is portrayed as a notorious opponent of the Nazi system. Else Eeven, born 1922, says: Else Eeven: My father was a total opponent.... Already in 1934 he said, “This is leading to war.”... And when the war started, he said, “One year, then we won’t win! They’re going much,
  • 42. much too fast through all of Europe; they’ll collapse.” In the family discussion, her father’s critical attitude, which applied, at least in this excerpt, not to Nazism per se but to the war, was addressed again by Ms. Eeven: Else Eeven: But what my father said every day, I heard so many things; I did- n’t tell anyone. Her husband, Albert Eeven, adds: Albert Eeven: Well, that was extreme. He was really lucky. Else Eeven: That they didn’t take him away. Their daughter Claudia Eeven, born in 1949, later in the family dis- cussion, notes: “Grandpa was arrested again. Because he didn’t, because he said something that the government.... He was arrested for two or three days.” To this her father adds: “Yes, he was once. Yes, he had talked a little bit too loud, and someone heard him, and the police came and took him away.” But a little later Albert Eeven relates that the arrest was occasioned by the violation of blackout rules, not criticism of the system, and payment of a fine of 25 marks averted any further consequences. That this version completely contradicts the story told only a few minutes earlier creates no stir in the family discussion nor occasions any retrospective correction. In the individual interview, Claudia Eeven speaks about her grandfa- ther as “incredibly critical” and “overly critical.” She suggests, “I think my mother didn’t have an easy time because of that.” While Claudia Eeven implies the her father’s critical remarks created a context of permanent danger to the family, her son Thomas Eeven, born in 1969, picks up that his relatives, fortunately, were far from being “yes-men,” and states what this means to him: Thomas Eeven: Ultimately it’s positive, of course. One would have wished that there were more aware Germans at the time. It makes me personally happy ... that in my family ... there were people who didn’t just staunchly yell “Heil Hitler.” This deeply held conviction on the part of the grandson that he came from a family that had always, even in the great-
  • 43. grandparents’ generation, been against Nazism led to an absurd interchange in the family discussion, when the grandfather, Albert Eeven, asked his grandchildren to ask him critical questions: Albert Eeven: Why don’t you ask, “Grandpa, why did you go along with it all? Why didn’t you do that?” Why don’t you ask that question? Thomas Eeven: Oh, Grandpa! Instead of asking Albert Eeven the question he’d like to hear— perhaps because he wonders about it himself, perhaps because he would like the opportunity to justify himself—his daughter Claudia and his grandson Thomas lecture him as to why his question makes no sense. Claudia argues from socialization theory, while the grandson finds other excuses: Claudia Eeven: But you didn’t protest because you had it drilled into you: obey and “everything has its system.” I mean, you can’t compare that with education today! Thomas Eeven: Grandpa, that’s the simplest question you can ask.... The answers have so many levels and the stories that you’ve told are so varied that I can’t ask a question like that. I know too much about it already for that. Albert Eeven has nothing to say to this and is silent. As absurd as this dialogue seems—the eyewitness wants to be critically questioned and his descendants refuse, explaining why it is not necessary—it illustrates clearly how knowledge of the Nazi past creates paradoxical effects in the family: To Claudia Eevens, information about the authoritarian educational style of that time—the “drill” and indoctrination—appears sufficient to explain why her father “went along with it all.” Thomas Eeven believes that he has so much knowledge of Nazi history that simple questions and answers seem improper to him: “I know too much about it already.” Knowledge functions to make its possessors not want to know any more—particularly Cumulative Heroization 15 16 Grandpa Wasn’t a Nazi about family history and its connection to Nazism. This is a
  • 44. paradoxical consequence of comprehensive education about the Nazi past and the crimes connected with it. The deeper the knowledge of history, the greater the subjectively felt need to protect one’s own family from this knowl- edge—thus removing them from the historical context about which one knows so much. That an eyewitness attempts to be critically questioned is itself part of the educational process and the culture of commemoration established over the last two decades. The grandfather is never asked the question, however, because the family discourse offers enough points of reference to create the image of a transgenerational anti-Nazi family, which will now be preserved into the future. This image seems all the more reassuring and in contrast to the prevailing norms against the background of knowledge of the horror of the period. As Thomas Eeven suggests in the separate inter- view: “Of course, one would have wished that there were more aware Ger- mans at the time”—like his grandparents and great-grandparents, that is. The Effects of Heroization What conclusions can we draw from the tendency toward cumulative heroization? One finding, not unimportant to the pedagogy of history, is that education that delivers a comprehensive historical knowledge of Nazi crimes paradoxically evokes a need to remove one’s relatives from this framework of knowledge. This result need not be assessed only negatively. Out of the revised history of heroism, resistance, and civil courage on the part of the grandparents, one can derive the practical conclusion that indi- vidual resistance is possible and sensible, even in a totalitarian context— that it is, emphatically, a question of individual responsibility. To this extent, the stories of oppositional grandparents and great- grandparents, regardless of their truth, can serve as examples motivating people to act courageously when others around them are threatened or persecuted. In addition, it is clear that a majority of the grandchildren look favorably on those who opposed the Nazis—only four of the forty-four grandchildren
  • 45. interviewed indicated any admiration for or affirmation of “the Nazis.” Cumulative heroization, however, has a very different significance for the historical image of Nazism and the Holocaust. It represents a restora- tion of the belief, thought to be long uprooted, that “the Nazis” and “the Germans” were two different groups; thus it follows that “the Germans” can be seen to have been seduced, abused, and robbed of their youth, and they can see themselves as victims of Nazism. That this historical model apparently holds a secure place in the cultural memory of the Federal Republic is demonstrated by the renaissance of historical dramas based on the home-front perspective, such as the German public television channel ZDF series “Hitler’s Helpers” and “Hitler’s Children.” While the Holo- caust has been commemorated in the framework of international confer- ences such as the 2000 Stockholm Holocaust Conference, and anniversaries of liberation have established a liturgical rhetoric of confes- sion and responsibility, the historical and political contexts that gave rise to Nazism seem to be disappearing from German historical awareness. What is becoming lost is the awareness that it was possible, in a civi- lized twentieth-century society, with the active participation of the over- whelming majority of a well-educated population, to exclude a part of this same population from the universe of obligations,12 to see them as harmful and “worthless,” to look on while they were deported, and to accept their extermination. This is not to speak of the actual perpetrators (some of whom appear in our interviews) whose willingness to murder and largely unproblematic reintegration into postwar West German society recedes from the collective memory. The phenomenon of cumulative heroization shows how deep, emo- tionally based views of the roles of close relatives affect an individual’s awareness of history, and how detached cognitive knowledge of history can be. The subjective synthesis
  • 46. consists of removing one’s own ancestors from one’s knowledge of history by heroizing them—thus bringing the “evil” of Nazi rule and the “good” of one’s own grandparents and great- grandpar- ents into peaceful coexistence. The Effects of Heroization 17 18 Grandpa Wasn’t a Nazi How the Jews Are Remembered: “These People Had Money” How do the formally learned concepts and interpretive patterns affect cur- rent family discourse about the Third Reich? What is considered self-evi- dent in family discourse about the Nazi period differs in many ways from the picture of the Third Reich portrayed in encyclopedias and history books, and from what would seem, from the public debate on the past, to be anchored in cultural memory. This difference can be observed through a comparison of families in East Germany and West Germany. Although the official view of history in the former East Germany was clearly distinct from that in West Germany,13 the East German families interviewed rarely used different interpretive patterns and concepts in talking about the Third Reich from those of their West German counterparts. Regardless of which encyclopedias or history books were found on the family bookshelves, within the family discourse, there were many similarities. For example, many members of the eyewitness generation still speak about “the Jews” today in alarmingly anti-Semitic and racist terms. They make it clear that the discourse established over the last two decades on the Holocaust and Nazi crimes has left few traces on the eyewitness genera- tion, at least in this respect: “Jews” are, in any case, not Germans; the crimes committed against them were committed against “others,” strangers; and it was partly as a result of their own behavior that they became objects of hatred, exclusion, persecution, and extermination. From the interview process, it becomes clear that stereotypical images enter into contemporary perceptions, which can be traced directly to Nazi propagan- da and to later published film documents from the extermination cam- paign in
  • 47. the East. For example, Lore Renz, born in 1916, remembers seeing a “troop of old Jews.” Lore Renz: They were banded together there.... Some of them had these beards, long beards, and ... I never forgot ... how these old men were stand- ing there. They had on big black hats, like the Jews have, you know. Helene Stein, born in 1924, relates: Helene Stein: And when you saw them running like that, with those long beards, the smock and so forth, as a child, you were a little afraid, and you thought, ”What kind of people are these?” Ms. Stein recalls that in 1945 she saw a train of concentration camp prisoners: Helene Stein: There were a lot of Jews among them, you can always tell, you know. And also foreigners. Interviewer: How could you tell? Helene Stein: Well,... You can recognize Jews, real Jews, with the nose and especially they all had beards, because they can’t shave and so forth. They looked terrible. And they shot us hate- filled looks. Margarethe Haase, born in 1920, tells how in the 1920s her father sometimes opened the paper in the morning and said, “That Cohn has him on his conscience, too.” Her father’s commentary was incomprehensi- ble to her at first, but later she understood its meaning: “Later I under- stood how he [Cohn] always lent a lot of money at usurious interest.” The anti-Semitic stereotypes of the rich, usurious Jew as well as that of “cowardly, submissive Jews” appear in the eyewitness generation’s stories: Margarethe Haase: These children, the Jew children, were somehow different from us.... I can’t tell you how. One of the boys, for example, was such a coward.... He was always afraid, [saying] “Please don’t, please don’t,” and so submissive. Ms. Haase repeats the boy’s fearful pleading without the slightest hesi- tation. That his desperate pleas might have been occasioned by an attack and that he might have had cause to be
  • 48. afraid is not mentioned. Her description even today lacks empathy; no awareness of injustice can be read into her words. Anti-Semitic interpretive patterns also come up in the children’s gener- ation, though in a more complex form that incorporates the postwar peri- od’s discourse on the past and philo-Semitic stereotypes. Ella Drake: Those two very beautiful people were ... they had something a bit Semitic about them. Like Jewish. Erich Grubitsch, born in 1937, developed a complex argument that identifies “the Jews” as a distinct group of people with specific characteris- tics who must on the one hand be protected from anti-Semitic prejudices, but on the other hand must be protected from themselves: How the Jews Are Remembered 19 20 Grandpa Wasn’t a Nazi Erich Grubitsch: You know, sometimes this characteristic appears that can verge on arrogance.... Maybe that’s because the Jews sometimes perceive dis- crimination that isn’t even there. And then they play off almost a cynical arrogance.... This pride, that they sometimes reproach themselves for, ... must have a protective function.... I ask myself why it’s expressed that way, or why it’s shown as if they were better. That’s the danger of creating anti- Semitism, that at that moment they reveal pride. I don’t want to talk about the chosen people, but that they reveal that. The most comprehensive and frequently voiced stereotype, across the generations, is that of the “rich Jew,” which appears often in interviews and family discussions. For example, Peter Schütz, born in 1954, tells us that “the Jews were among the richest, that was always true.” It is the eyewit- nesses who relate that in the 1930s Jews were either in academic profes- sions (especially doctors) or worked as traders and businesspeople, lived in “rich areas,” and enjoyed considerable economic influence. When the eye- witnesses talk about being personally acquainted with Jews, they generally do not describe a person, but mention his profession or social status. Even when
  • 49. this involves schoolmates, who (as most put it) “suddenly” stopped coming to class, they often refer as a matter of course to their father’s pro- fessions or assert that they were “rich people.” Thus Lore Renz, born in 1916, tells of the “last Jewess” in her class: Lore Renz: Lilly Schneider, her father was a doctor at the public health office in Hanover; they lived in Kleefeld. That’s where all the wealthy people lived, Kleefeld, Kirchrode, all the Jews lived mainly there.... But they were able to emigrate, you know. In this respect, the eyewitness portrayals differ little from one another. In most eyewitness retellings, a reference to the person’s or family’s finan- cial situation ends with a report of their flight before the start of World War II. The older generation reports are also similar in another respect: In none of their stories do they mention the discrimination—loss of posi- tions, property, etc.—that preceded that flight. The flight is simply referred to as though it requires no explanation. Furthermore, “flight” is not the word the eyewitnesses use; they talk about how the Jews “emigrated” or “took off,” were “gone” or “made a run for it” (aus dem Staub gemacht). Again and again, “money” appears in this context: The Jews who had the clothing store on Danzigerstrasse and with whom her father had worked, reported Wilhelmine Brinkmann, “took off” “to America ... because they had money.... These people had money.” The stereotype of the “rich Jew” who “emigrated” before the war, gen- erally to America, is found much more frequently in eyewitness accounts than in the stories told by their children. But when the members of the next generation recall their parents’ stories, the concept reappears. Thus Kurt Jung recalls that his parents “knew a lot of Jewish families”: Kurt Jung: ... who were businesspeople in Hanover, who showed up again in time; many of them emigrated to America before the war started and then came back, big businesspeople.
  • 50. Not only is the concept of the “rich Jews” taken up by the children’s generation. With the same continuity, some members of the children’s gen- eration also use the Nazi categories of “half” or “quarter Jews.” These terms are generally incorporated incidentally into their stories, without ref- erence to the origin or function of that categorization. As a matter of course, those interviewed distinguish between “the Jews” and “the Ger- mans.” Thus Birgit Roth, born in 1939, says: Birgit Roth: I don’t know why they wanted to throw the Jews out; Jews were actually the best businesspeople; they had the biggest share here in Germany. Everywhere big businesses, and the Germans worked there. In this interview, Ms. Roth is not openly anti-Semitic; rather, she attempts, with statements like “Jews were the best businesspeople,” to explain what she is unable to understand at the start. Not only does the question “why they wanted to throw the Jews out” ignore that at issue is mass murder, not expulsion, but “the Germans” appear only as the employees of Jewish businesspeople, while “they” who wanted “to throw the Jews out” remains an open question. The basic distinction between “Jews” and “Aryans” is maintained through the distinction between “the Jews” and “the Germans.” What the speaker intends to be a statement crit- ical of the Nazi period conveys the very same anti-Semitic stereotypes and Nazi categories: Jews were successful (read “rich”) entrepreneurs, and Jews are not considered German. As self-evidently as “rich Jews” appear in interviews with eyewitnesses and members of the children’s generation, the concept no longer appears in the grandchildren’s generation. Thus the grandchildren of Lore Renz How the Jews Are Remembered 21 22 Grandpa Wasn’t a Nazi remember that their grandmother told them about Jewish classmates, but their grandmother’s automatic reference to the family’s socio-economic sta- tus is gone. To the interviewer’s question about what stories her grand- mother had told her from
  • 51. the Nazi period, Nina Jung, born in 1975, answers: Nina Jung: For example, that suddenly the young girl ... disappeared from class. Nina Jung is the only one of the interviewed grandchildren who remembers a story told by her grandmother that does not involve helping Jews, hiding them, or rescuing them in some way, or some form of resist- ance to their persecution. In all other cases, it is their own grandmother or grandfather who treated persecuted Jews humanely, attempted to protect them from persecution, or, in one case, were against “people being thrown in a pit like that ... and then just buried” and so the grandfather “desert- ed.” The image of the good grandparents is so dominant that no other story told about the Jews by the eyewitnesses is registered by the grandchil- dren or retold. How “the Nazis” Are Remembered: As Others When “the Nazis” are spoken of in our interviews, the term mostly func- tions to distance the speakers or their families from the historical events of the Third Reich. It is true that there are cases where interviewees describe their own family members as “Nazis” or “National Socialists.” Max Wieck, for example, speaks of some of his family as “fervent National Socialists,” and Franz Schmitt emphasizes that one side of his family were “all pure Nazis.” In both cases, however, the storyteller, in the same breath, intro- duces the people important for his concept of himself—his grandfather or father—as “opponents” of the Nazis. Mr. Wieck presents his grandfather as “a founder of the SPD [Social Democratic Party],” and Mr. Schmitt speaks of “my father ... the only Communist.” Thus family members most signif- icant for their own history are portrayed as “not Nazis.” “The Nazis” are others—countertypes to their grandfathers and fathers. When family members with whom the storyteller identifies are described as Nazis, there is almost always an immediate justification. They were forced to join the Nazi Party; they couldn’t act otherwise because of their economic situation; they did it because
  • 52. everyone did. The only excep- tions are if the interviewee still identifies positively with the Third Reich. In our sample, this was true only of Mrs. Haase, born in 1920, who thinks the Third Reich was “a good period,” and describes her father emphatical- ly as a “staunch National Socialist.” The extent to which a person’s image can change through the genera- tions can be illustrated by the Grubitsch family. Like Mrs. Haase, Mrs. Grubitsch describes her husband as an early “fervent Nazi.” Unlike Mrs. Haase, however, she notes right away that he was “in the party” because otherwise “he wouldn’t have gotten a job.” Economic motives thus justify her husband’s party membership. Mrs. Grubitsch’s son paints a somewhat different picture. To him, his father was exactly the opposite of fervent: “I know that my father did it with a certain amount of reluctance.... He wore a badge on his lapel, but you couldn’t always see it on all his suits.” The portrayals by the eyewitness and her son express ambivalence, each in its own way. The picture painted by the grandson no longer does: He is sure that his grandfathers were “not Nazis,” and “my grandpa wasn’t a party member, on my father’s side, not on my mother’s side either.” Thus from generation to generation, “the Nazis” become more the “other,” while the distance of one’s own ancestors from the horrific events becomes ever greater. Historians have considered the question of how the “Volksgenossen” (the “common citizens”) reacted to the persecution of the Jews—what they knew, could have known, or wanted to know of their extermination.14 The reconstructed reality of the Third Reich differs greatly from the answers given by the eyewitnesses to the question of what they knew or could have known at the time. But even the eyewitnesses seem to be aware of this. Few of them act as self-righteously as Rainer Hofer, born in 1925, who becomes indignant in the group discussion about the fact that “it is always disputed” that people didn’t know at the time that concentration and extermination camps existed. But there are also explicit references to differ- ent opinions in the statements of other eyewitnesses. Mathilde
  • 53. Beck, born in 1924 says in the group discussion: How “the Nazis” Are Remembered 23 24 Grandpa Wasn’t a Nazi Mathilde Beck: All that about the concentration camps. People say so often, “But you must have known.” It was all kept so secret. “Empty Talking” In the interviews, there were 106 occurrences of the phrase “people didn’t know.” More than a third of the eyewitnesses insist that they did not know there were concentration camps.15 However, most of these statements use an interpretive pattern similar in vagueness to Mrs. Beck’s (“All that about the concentration camps”), pointing to a phenomenon that occurred fre- quently during the discussions, which could be described as “empty talk- ing.” That is, actors and the contexts of events are not named, but are called “that,” “they” [sie, “se,” die], or “that stuff” [das da]. This “empty talking” as a form of speech typifies the intergenerational discussion about the Third Reich. Actors, generally the perpetrators, remain shapeless, while historical events are described in outline form, so what is being talked about remains unclear, the events seemingly harmless. “About the Jews,” says Paula Ubaczek, born in 1921, “I only found out about it when the war was over, when the concentration camps were opened; that’s when we found out.” The imprecision of the language, describing developments associatively and indirectly, characterizes the “empty talking”; it remains up to the listener to fill in the blanks as to what the speaker is actually talking about. She was indignant, says Ms. Wil- helmine Brinkmann, born in 1915, “when all that came out,” and Ms. Elisabeth Schulze, born in1920, reports: Elisabeth Schulze: We had no idea what the others were suffering. We only read that here, when the newspaper came again. And when we got a radio, we heard it then, but otherwise we didn’t know anything ... all the things that were going on. From the point of view of transmission of history, “empty speaking” perhaps conveys something else: that certain contexts
  • 54. of history are suffi- ciently discussed even if left imprecise. “That stuff” and “it” signify con- texts about which it makes no sense to speak more clearly: “Certainly, it shouldn’t be forgotten so quickly; we have to talk about it sometimes, so that it doesn’t come again,” said Elli Krug. To sum up, the anti-Semitically colored notion of the “rich Jew” and the interpretive pattern that “Jews” and “Germans” represent two separate groups of people are ideas that transcend the generations. Their disturbing resilience can be seen as a posthumous victory for the Nazi policies of per- secution and extermination. In addition, the “empty talking” in communi- cation between generations is a mechanism that allows blank spaces to be filled with whatever content and ideas best satisfy the needs of the audi- ence, allowing the listener to ascribe to the speaker whatever meanings he would most like to impute to them. A Broadly Representative Survey The intergenerational survey presented here was an in-depth study that necessarily encompassed a small number of cases, to illuminate the under- lying patterns of how families communicate about the past. The selective sampling included only families that had an interest in the subject and whose members were willing to speak about it together—which is hardly the case in all German families. To test whether our findings could be gen- eralized, we reformulated the research questions for a larger representative survey, carried out in June 2002 by the Emnid Institute in Bielefeld. The phenomenon of cumulative heroization could not be reproduced within the representative popular survey, since the people questioned were randomly chosen and not family members. But it was possible to ask those surveyed how they saw the role, attitudes, and behavior of their own rela- tives during the Third Reich. We posed this question on several levels: first, by asking what their parents and grandparents, in their view, thought about Nazism; and second, by querying about the individual attitudes and behavioral patterns of their
  • 55. relatives for a range of items—always from the point of view of the respondent, of course. The results were even more astonishing than those of the in- depth study. Roughly half the population surveyed is of the opinion that their relatives viewed Nazism negatively or even very negatively; a mere 4 per- cent believe their relatives’ attitude was “more positive,” and only 2 percent believe that their relatives had a “very positive” attitude. Among people with upper high school (Abitur) and college diplomas, these results were even more pronounced—56 percent ascribing negative attitudes toward Broadly Representative Survey 25 26 Grandpa Wasn’t a Nazi Nazism to their relatives. Another question probed the attitudes, experiences, and behaviors of parents and grandparents, as known to the survey participants from family discussions. Only 3 percent of the respondents believed that their relatives had been “anti-Jewish,” and only 1 percent thought it was possible that they “were directly involved in crimes.” Twenty-six percent of those sur- veyed were convinced that their relatives “helped people who were perse- cuted,” and 35 percent “didn’t go along with everything as much as possible.” In contrast, 65 percent of participants believe their parents and grandparents “suffered a lot in the war,” and 63 percent stated that their relatives had “experienced community” during the Third Reich. These results, too, were even more pronounced among those with an upper high school (Abitur) or college diploma: Thirty percent believed that their rela- tives “helped people who were persecuted,” and 71 percent were convinced that their parents and grandparents “suffered a lot in the war.” The results of the broader survey make it clear that the overwhelming majority believes their own family members were not Nazis. Anti-Semites and perpetrators appear to be practically nonexistent in German families. That a higher
  • 56. frequency of these experiences, attitudes, and behaviors cor- relates with a high level of education underscores the findings of our in- depth study that education about Nazi crimes and the Holocaust has the paradoxical effect of inclining people to turn their parents and grandpar- ents into opponents of the regime, helpers, or casual or even explicit resisters. Some findings of the larger sample also point toward “cumulative hero- ization”: Thus 14 percent of the youngest participants (fourteen-to-twen- ty-nine years of age) said that their grandparents “offered resistance” (compared to 13 percent overall), and only 4 percent believed that they were “staunch Nazis” (compared to 6 percent overall). However, in the youngest group, 6 percent believed that their grandparents “were anti-Jew- ish,” and 3 percent believed it was possible “that they were directly involved in crimes” (compared with 3 percent and 1 percent overall). The finding that two-thirds of the participants in the representative sample emphasized the sufferings of their own relatives in the war under- scores the finding that the public culture of commemoration in Germany How the Study Was Received 27 differs greatly from private recollections. Whoever was guilty of the Holo- caust, whoever committed the crimes in the extermination camps, the forced labor system, and the camps— one thing is clear to all German citi- zens: My grandpa wasn’t a Nazi. Postscript: How the Study Was Received The results of this study began to have an impact even before the final report had been published. All the major newspapers and many radio and television stations carried stories about it. After the book appeared, the findings entered into the discourse of politicians and government officials, who showed concern that the educational efforts of the past two decades had, in their view, borne astonishingly little fruit. The professional agents of memory—whether working in schools, foundations, or Holocaust memorials—were highly disconcerted.
  • 57. The book itself became the subject of many school projects. In higher academia, too, the study garnered attention and was the subject of numer- ous panels and conferences.16 Although the study documents that for the generation of the grand- children, the role models are resistance fighters, not Nazis—something that could certainly be viewed as a successful educational outcome—the public reaction was generally alarmist. In speeches, high-ranking politi- cians worriedly demanded more education, and the media latched on with great interest to the angle that grandchildren use their knowledge of histo- ry to different ends than their teachers had expected. Foreign reactions to the study—for example, in the Norwegian news- papers—interpreted the results as representing a failed attempt to deal with the past and focused on the recent right- wing orientation among young Germans. This interpretation itself illustrates how the study became a pro- jective test for every group’s difficulties in dealing with the past. Two years after its original publication in German, the study continues to receive attention. Even editorials on the 2004 U.S. elections reflected the background of Grandpa Wasn’t a Nazi: “Say this about John Kerry: At least his grandfather wasn’t a Nazi,” wrote Victorino Matus on February 6, 2004, in the Weekly Standard.17 The occasion for the article, entitled 28 Grandpa Wasn’t a Nazi “Springtime for Friedrich,” was a minor German scandal stemming from a statement by the head of the Christian Democratic Party in parliament, Friedrich Merz, that his grandfather had been his most important role model; it was promptly revealed that the grandfather had been a Nazi, at least if early membership in the SA and the Nazi Party were deemed suffi- cient grounds. So Merz had a public “grandpa” problem, and references to our study made it clear how firmly the research had taken root in the Ger- man commemoration culture framework. This was true as well for the counter-commemoration culture of the far right. The NPD (the National Party of Germany), which