SlideShare a Scribd company logo
1 of 35
AGGRESSIVE BEHAVIOR
Volume 37, pages 569–577 (2011)
Intolerance of Sexy Peers: Intrasexual Competition
Among Women
Tracy Vaillancourt1,2� and Aanchal Sharma2
1Faculty of Education and School of Psychology, University of
Ottawa, Ottawa, Ontario, Canada
2Department of Psychology, Neuroscience & Behaviour,
McMaster University, Hamilton, Ontario, Canada
: : : : : : : : : : : : : : : : : : : : : : : : : : : : : : : : : : : : : : : : :
Intrasexual competition among males of different species,
including humans, is well documented. Among females, far less
is known.
Recent nonexperimental studies suggest that women are
intolerant of attractive females and use indirect aggression to
derogate
potential rivals. In Study 1, an experimental design was used to
test the evolutionary-based hypothesis that women would be
intolerant of sexy women and would censure those who seem to
make sex too readily available. Results provide strong empirical
support for intrasexual competition among women. Using
independent raters, blind to condition, we found that almost all
women
were rated as reacting negatively (‘‘bitchy’’) to an attractive
female confederate when she was dressed in a sexually
provocative
manner. In contrast, when she was dressed conservatively, the
same confederate was barely noticed by the participants. In
Study 2,
an experimental design was used to assess whether the sexy
female confederate from Study 1 was viewed as a sexual rival
by
women. Results indicated that as hypothesized, women did not
want to introduce her to their boyfriend, allow him to spend
time
alone with her, or be friends with her. Findings from both
studies are discussed in terms of evolutionary theory. Aggr.
Behav.
37:569–577, 2011. r 2011 Wiley Periodicals, Inc.
: : : : : : : : : : : : : : : : : : : : : : : : : : : : : : : : : : : : : : : : :
Keywords: intrasexual competition; indirect aggression; women;
experimental; design; evolution
Intrasexual competition among males of various
species, including humans, is well-documented [Archer,
2009; Daly and Wilson, 1988; Darwin, 1871; Geary,
2010; Wilson and Daly, 1985]. Among females of most
mammalian species less is known [Clutton-Brock,
2007]. We do know that competition among
nonhuman female primates (and other species) is
typically over access to food, and that competition
over the attention of males occurs when male parental
investment is high or when males provide other
valuable and/or limited resources to females [Geary,
2010]. The few non-experimental research studies that
have been conducted on sexual selection in human
females have focused on two competition strategies
used by women: self-promotion (intersexual competi-
tion) and the derogation of rivals (intrasexual competi-
tion). Self-promotion involves epigamic displays of
physical attractiveness such as wearing makeup and
form-fitting clothing that are used to attract the
attention of males [Barber, 1995; Buss, 1988; Buss
and Schmitt, 1993; Fisher and Cox, 2009; Schmitt and
Buss, 1996; Symons, 1979; Walters and Crawford,
1994]. Derogation of competitors takes the form of
indirect aggression [Buss and Dedden, 1990; Campbell,
1995, 1999; Fisher, 2004; Vaillancourt, 2005; Walters
and Crawford, 1994] which is a type of aggression that
tends to be circuitous in nature and is presumably
used to reduce the mate value of a rival [Buss and
Dedden, 1990; Campbell, 1995, 1999; Fisher, 2004;
Vaillancourt, 2005].
Indirect aggression is used by males and females
[Card et al., 2008] and usually directed at same-sex
peers [Gallup and Wilson, 2009]. It includes
behaviors such as spreading rumors that question
the perceived rival’s fidelity or level of promiscuity,
disparaging the competitor’s appearance, excluding
the rival from the peer group, giving her the silent
Published online in Wiley Online Library
(wileyonlinelibrary.com).
DOI: 10.1002/ab.20413
Received 6 May 2011; Revised 29 July 2011; Accepted 16
August
2011
Grant Sponsors: Canadian Institutes of Health Research; Canada
Research Chairs program and the Social Sciences and
Humanities
Research Council of Canada.
�
Correspondence to: Tracy Vaillancourt, Faculty of Education
and
School of Psychology, University of Ottawa, Ottawa, Ontario,
Canada. E-mail: [email protected]
r 2011 Wiley Periodicals, Inc.
treatment, or using derisive body and facial gestures
to make the rival feel badly about herself and thus
less willing to compete. Studies have shown that
even though indirect aggression is covert, it is
nevertheless effective at inflicting harm on targets,
particularly female targets, who are able to pick up
on the subtle nuances which comprise this form of
negative behavior [Vaillancourt, 2005]. Moreover,
recent evidence suggests that women are more likely
than men to engage in this type of behavior when
they face a social threat [e.g., Benenson et al., 2011].
Research on human mate preferences clearly
demonstrates that males show a strong preference
for young, attractive females [Buss, 1989; Buss and
Schmitt, 1993; Grammer and Thornhill, 1994;
Singh, 1993, 1994; Singh and Young, 1995]. In
reaction to this predilection, females derogate rivals
who imbue these qualities. Indeed, studies have
shown that females are particularly intolerant of
attractive peers, using indirect aggression against
them at a greater rate than their less attractive peers
[e.g., Leenaars et al., 2008]. Moreover, at times when
females are maximally fertile (time of ovulation),
they are the most derogating of competitors; rating
female faces as less attractive [Fisher, 2004].
In addition to being intolerant of physically
attractive peers, we hypothesize that women are
particularly intolerant of sexy attractive peers.
According to Baumeister and Twenge [2002], a double
standard of sexual morality exists in which women
‘‘stifle each other’s sexuality because sex is a limited
resource that women use to negotiate with men, and
scarcity gives women an advantage’’ (p. 166). In their
review of relevant literature, Baumeister and Twenge
found support for their theory that females, and not
males, suppress the sexuality of other females.
Females accomplish this by ‘‘punishing’’ other females
who seem to make sex too readily available ‘‘through
informal sanctions such as ostracism and derogatory
gossip’’ (p. 172). In other words, females used indirect
aggression to suppress the sexuality of other females.
We examined the predication that women would
‘‘punish’’ other women who appear to make sex too
readily available by randomly assigning young
women in dyads (with a friend or with a stranger)
to one of two conditions (Study 1). In the first
condition, women were exposed to a conservatively
dressed attractive female confederate. In the second
condition, they were exposed to the same confederate
dressed in sexy clothing. We hypothesized that most,
if not all, women would express negative, derogative
reactions (indirect aggression) toward the attractive
confederate when her appearance emphasized
sexually evocative qualities.
We further hypothesized that these negative
reactions would be particularly pronounced in the
presence of a female friend rather than a stranger.
This hypothesis was based on research demonstrat-
ing that female friendships are characterized by high
intimacy which includes making social comparisons
and gossiping about others [Dunbar et al., 1997;
Hornstein, 1985]. As well, we expected that women
would be more comfortable expressing themselves
negatively in the presence of a friend rather than a
stranger.
We also conducted a manipulation validation
study (Study 2) by randomly assigning women to
one of the three conditions in which they indepen-
dently rated (1) a photograph of the conservatively
dressed confederate from Study 1, (2) a photograph
of the provocatively dressed confederate (sexy-thin)
from Study 1, or (3) a manipulated photograph of
the provocatively dressed confederate from Study 1
in which she appeared overweight (sexy-fat). Con-
sistent with the idea that the sexy confederate from
Study 1 was in fact viewed as a sexual rival, we
hypothesized that women would be less willing to
introduce her to their current or future boyfriend(s),
or have their current or future boyfriend(s) spend
time alone with her than the attractive conservative
confederate or the sexy-fat confederate. We also
hypothesized that compared to the conservatively
dressed confederate, women would not want to
introduce their partner or allow him to spend time
with the sexy-fat woman because her sexy clothing
would be perceived as an indicator of sexual
availability and hence would be threatening
1
[Abbey
et al., 1987].
According to Buss et al. [2000], successful reten-
tion of one’s mate hinges on one’s ability to prevent
the partner from defecting or being unfaithful, and
fending off rivals who may be interested in mating
with that partner. Disparaging a sexy attractive
woman (Study 1) or not allowing your partner to
meet or spend time with such a woman (Study 2) is
mate guarding (i.e., intrasexual competition).
Finally, we hypothesized that women would be
less likely to want to have the sexy-thin or the sexy-
fat confederate as a friend, compared to the
conservatively dressed confederate. This hypothesis
was based on the idea that a woman would not
want to associate with a woman who appears too
sexually available because it would reduce her own
1
It has been shown that the presence of an intrasexual competitor
does indeed represent a threat to fidelity [see Arnocky,
Sunderani,
Miller, & Vaillancourt, in press; Kenrick, Neuberg, Zierk, &
Krones,
1994].
570 Vaillancourt and Sharma
Aggr. Behav.
mate-value (she too would be seen as promiscuous).
As well, having a sexy woman as a friend might
mean that the woman would have to be constantly
on guard to ensure that her partner does not cheat
on her with this seemingly sexually available person.
STUDY 1
Methods
Participants. Eighty-six heterosexual women
ranging in age from 19 to 23 years
(Mean7SE 5 20.1470.26) from varying ethnic
backgrounds participated in Study 1. The women
were recruited from a mid-size university located in
southern Ontario, Canada.
Procedure. A two (friendship, stranger) by two
(conservative, provocative) experimental design was
implemented. Participants were paired together in
dyads to create two participant conditions—friendship
and stranger dyads, based on having or not having an
existing friendship with the woman they were paired
with. Participants were then randomly assigned to
the two experimental conditions. In the first
(conservative) condition (n 5 40; 20 friends and 20
strangers), participants were exposed to an attractive,
conservatively dressed 21-year-old Caucasian female
confederate (independently rated as attractive by
20 female undergraduate students on a scale from 1
to 10, Mean7SE 5 8.670.25) who embodied qualities
considered attractive from an evolutionary perspective
[low waist-to-hip ratio, clear skin, large breasts;
Buss, 1989; Buss and Schmitt, 1993; Grammer and
Thornhill, 1994; Singh, 1993, 1994; Singh and Young,
1995] (Fig. 1A). In the second (sexy) condition
(n 5 46; 23 friends and 23 strangers), the same female
confederate was dressed in a sexually provocative
manner (Fig. 1B).
In both conditions, the confederate’s behavior and
mannerisms were standardized. Each trial was run
with the confederate knocking on the door the same
number of times, taking the same number of steps
across the room and between the dyad (who was
waiting for a moderated discussion on how women
handle conflict in friendships to begin), and using the
same script to ask the dyad the whereabouts of the
study’s experimenter. The confederate then left
the laboratory with the experimenter, a South Asian
woman in her late 20s dressed conservatively across
conditions. Before and after the experimenter and
confederate exited the room the participants
were unknowingly video and audio recorded using
equipment that captured their individual reactions of
their exposure to the confederate.
Coding of observational data. Video clips of
participants’ individual reactions of their exposure to
the confederate were randomly presented to 13
primarily Caucasian women (mean age 5 23,
SE 5 0.74) blind to condition, who classified and
rated participants on two dependent variables:
(1) whether or not they thought the participant was
exhibiting bitchy
2
behavior (i.e., indirect aggression)
and (2) if so, how bitchy her reaction was on a scale
from 0 to 10 (0, not bitchy; 10, extremely bitchy).
Fig. 1. Confederate dressed conservatively (A), provocatively
(sexy-thin) (B), and provocatively (sexy-fat) (C).
2
Because the terms indirect aggression and derogation are
scientific
terms we asked participants to rate reactions using the
colloquial
term ‘‘bitchy’’ and provided them with a comprehensive list of
what
these behaviours entailed (see list provided in main text).
571Intrasexual Competition Among Women
Aggr. Behav.
A participant’s reaction was coded as bitchy if 70%
or more of the coders identified it as so (n 5 36).
A reaction was coded as not bitchy if 70% or more
of the coders deemed it to not be (n 5 42). Cases in
which neither threshold was achieved were coded as
‘‘unsure’’ (n 5 8). Bitchy behavior (use of indirect
aggression) included negative facial expressions (e.g.,
looks of disgust, eye rolls), dismissive/avoidant
behavior, ‘‘once-overs/death stares’’, body posture,
back biting or mocking, sarcastic tone of voice, and
fake smiles. To avoid capturing facial expression
unrelated to the exposure to the confederate, 30 sec
of tape were reviewed from the point of the
confederate’s entrance into the room. If participants
were still expressing a reaction toward the confeder-
ate after the 30 sec, the coding time was extended
accordingly (Mean7SE 5 38.3977.79 sec).
In addition to coding the video clips on a universal
impression of derogation, the data were also coded
using the Facial Action Coding System [FACS;
Ekman and Friesen, 2003]. FACS is a specialized and
rigorously validated method of taxonomizing facial
expressions. The system describes facial behavior and
expressions of emotions with a series of muscle
contractions termed action units. Two trained and
certified FACS raters independently applied the
FACS to each videotape, while remaining blind to
the experimental condition (k varied from 0.85
to 1.00 across coding categories). Following the
application of the FACS, each coder’s results were
analyzed using the Emotion Facial Action Coding
System [EmFACS; Ekman et al., 2002], a rule-based
approach for assigning emotions to particular
combinations of action units. The specific emotions
coded for were: happiness, sadness, anger, contempt,
surprise, disgust, and rage.
FACS raters also coded for the presence of the
following behavior: (1) whether participants ex-
changed a glare with each other as a way of
conveying information about the confederate
(‘‘exchanging information’’), (2) if they looked at
the confederate up and down (‘‘once-overs’’),
(3) whether they said something negative about
the confederate when she left the room (‘‘negative
verbal comments’’), and (4) if they laughed at the
confederate (‘‘laughed’’). The k score was 1.00 for all
these coding categories.
Manipulation check. At the end of the study,
participants were asked to share their thoughts
about what they felt the purpose of the study was.
None of the participants identified the interruption
by the confederate as a planned, strategic part of the
study, nor were they able to identify the confeder-
ate’s role in the study.
Results
w2 analyses, one-way analysis of variance (ANO-
VA), and multilevel modeling were used to assess the
hypotheses proposed for Study 1. Given the number
of analyses conducted, Po.01 was used as an
indicator of statistical significance.
Bitchy Classification
Examining participants’ global negative reactions
(bitchy or not bitchy) to the confederate we found
striking differences by condition (w2(2) 5 51.71;
Po.0001; see Table I). Specifically, when women
were rated as bitchy, all but 2 were in the sexy
condition. And of those rated as not bitchy, most
were in the conservative condition. The remaining
eight women’s reactions were coded as unsure (two
in the conservative condition and six in sexy
condition).
Bitchy Ratings
When we examined how bitchy the women’s
reactions were on a scale from 0 to 10, we found a
statistically significant main effect for condition
(sexy vs. conservative; F(1, 82) 5 64.00, Po.0001),
a marginal effect for type of dyad (friends vs.
strangers; F(1, 82) 5 4.10, Po.05), and no interac-
tion between conditions (F(1, 82) 5 1.67, P 5 .20).
Women in the sexy condition were rated as being
bitchier (Mean7SE 5 3.4070.28) than women in
the conservative condition (Mean7SE 5 0.2070.27)
and women in friendship dyads were rated as being
slightly bitchier (Mean7SE 5 2.2070.28) than
women in stranger dyads (Mean7SE 5 1.4070.28)
(Fig. 2). The effect size for the sexy condition was
large (Cohen’s d 5 1.74) and for the dyad condition
small (Cohen’s d 5 0.34).
Because participants’ reactions were nested within
dyads, interactions between dyad and condition
were also examined using multilevel modeling with
participants’ global negative reactions (level of
bitchiness) as the outcome. Excluding those in the
unsure group and controlling for the type of dyad,
TABLE I. Relative Frequency of ‘‘Bitchy’’ Classification by
Experimental Condition
Conservative condition
(N 5 40)
Sexy condition
(N 5 46)
Bitchy 2 (5.0%) 34 (74%)
Not bitchy 36 (90%) 6 (13%)
Unsure 2 (5.0%) 6 (13%)
572 Vaillancourt and Sharma
Aggr. Behav.
we found that again, condition (sexy vs. conserva-
tive) was a statistically significant correlate of
outcome (OR 5 exp (4.651) 5 104.69, SE72.60;
Po.001). Unfortunately, we could not test the
initial hypothesis that women would behave more
poorly when with a friend in the sexy condition (i.e.,
the interaction between condition and type of dyad)
because the model did not converge.
EmFACS
Examining emotions coded with EmFACS across
the sexy and conservative conditions we found that
of the 11.6% (n 5 10) of the total sample that
displayed true happiness all were assigned to the
conservative condition (w2(1) 5 13.01, Po.001).
Moreover, of the 12.8% (n 5 11) of the total sample
that displayed anger, all were assigned to the sexy
condition (w2(1) 5 10.97, Po.001). There were no
other statistically significant findings for EmFACS
identified reactions by conditions (friends vs. stranger
or sexy vs. conservative) and no interactions between
conditions.
Other Negative Behavior
As seen in Table II, when examining whether
participants exchanged a look with their partner as a
way of conveying information about the confederate
we found notable differences by condition
(w2(1) 5 11.59, Po.001) with far more women
looking to each other to exchange information in
the sexy condition than in the conservative condi-
tion. As well, women in the sexy condition were far
more likely to have looked the participant over than
those in the conservative condition (w2(1) 5 31.19,
Po.0001). We also found that negative comments
were only made about the confederate in the sexy
condition, w2(1) 5 12.13, Po.001. For example, one
woman implied that the confederate was dressed to
have sex with one of her professors and another said
that the confederate’s ‘‘boobs were about to pop
out’’. Importantly, all comments about the confed-
erate were made after she left the room with one
exception. One woman said ‘‘What the fuck is that?’’
directly to the confederate after blatantly looking
her up and down while showing disgust.
Finally, we found that participants laughed about
the confederate when she left the room far more
often in the sexy condition than in the conservative
condition (w2(1) 5 15.48, Po.001), and when with a
friend than with a stranger (n 5 5; w2(1) 5 11.33,
Po.001).
STUDY 2
Methods
Participants. Sixty-six women ranging in age
from 17 to 28 years (Mean7SE 5 18.7070.04) from
varying ethnic backgrounds participated in the
manipulation validation study. They were recruited
from a mid-size university located in southern
Ontario, Canada.
Procedure. To verify the evolutionary signifi-
cance of the exposure to the sexy confederate,
women were randomly assigned to one of the three
conditions. In all three conditions, participants were
asked to rate a color photograph of the confederate
from Study 1. Specifically, they were asked to rate
the woman’s level of attractiveness (cuteness) and
sexiness in the photograph using a scale of 1–10 with
1 5 not at all descriptive and 10 5 extremely
descriptive. Participants were also asked to rate
how likely, on a scale of 1 to 7 with 1 5 extremely
unlikely and 1 5 extremely likely, would they
Fig. 2. Mean bitchy scores by condition (sexy vs. conservative)
and
dyad (friends vs. strangers).
TABLE II. Relative Frequency of Other Negative Behavior by
Experimental Condition
Type of behavior
Conservative condition
(N 5 40)
Sexy condition
(N 5 46)
Exchanging information 1 (2.5%) 14 (30%)
Once-overs 10 (25%) 39 (85%)
Negative verbal
comments
0 (0%) 12 (26%)
Laughed 3 (7.5%) 21 (46%)
573Intrasexual Competition Among Women
Aggr. Behav.
introduce her to their boyfriend (current or future)
and how likely would they let their boyfriend
(current or future) spend time alone with her. In
the first condition (conservative), a photograph of
the conservatively dressed confederate was provided
(Fig. 1A). In the second condition (sexy-thin), a
photograph of the sexy dressed confederate was
provided (Fig. 1B). Finally, in the third condition
(sexy-fat), a photograph of the sexy dressed con-
federate was manipulated so that she appeared
overweight (Fig. 1C).
Results
ANOVA was used to examine participants’
perception of the confederate from Study 1. Given
the number of analyses conducted, Po.01 was again
used as an indicator of statistical significance. As
expected, Student–Newman–Keuls post-hoc tests
confirmed that even though participants rated the
conservative confederate as cuter than the other two
women (F(2, 61) 5 13.72, Po.001, partial Z2 5 .31),
they nevertheless were less likely to introduce the
sexy-thin confederate to their boyfriend (F(2,
63) 5 24.98, Po.001, partial Z2 5 .44), or let him
spend time alone with her (F(2, 63) 5 8.81, Po.001,
partial Z2 5 .22) than the sexy-fat or conservative
confederate (see Table III for all Means, SE, and
post-hoc comparisons). Moreover, participants also
rated the sexy-thin confederate as being sexier than
the sexy-fat and conservative confederates (F(2,
61) 5 9.74, Po.001, partial Z2 5 .24), with the
post-hoc test revealing that the mean ratings on
sexy for the sexy-fat and conservative confederates
were not statistically significantly different from one
another.
Finally, consistent with our initial hypothesis, we
found that women reported being less likely to be
friends with the sexy-thin or sexy-fat confederate
than the conservatively dressed confederate (F(2,
63) 5 23.66, Po.0001, partial Z2 5 .43).
DISCUSSION
Although the ultimate reason women derogate
rivals is unknown, we strongly suspect that the use
of indirect aggression by human females is rooted in
evolutionary history. It has been noted in the
nonhuman animal literature that female reproduc-
tive competition is most intense within species in
which males invest heavily in their offspring
(with some exceptions see Clutton-Brock, 2007]. In
humans, males are invested in their offspring, albeit
to a lesser extent than females, and because they
invest they also tend to be selective in terms of who
they mate with on a long-term basis [Kenrick et al.,
1990]. This choosiness puts pressure on females to
compete for the most desirable mates and the form
this competition takes is often the derision of
perceived rivals [Campbell, 2002; Vaillancourt,
2005].
Using an experimental design, results of Study 1
provide support for the hypothesis that women do
engage in intrasexual competition and that the form
it takes is indirect aggression. In the presence of an
attractive female who defied social convention by
dressing in a sexually provocative manner, almost
all women randomly assigned to this condition
aggressed against her. The women in this condition
were more likely to roll their eyes at the confederate,
look her up and down, stare at her without
conveying any emotion, and show anger while she
was in the room. When the confederate left the
room, many of them laughed at her, ridiculed her
appearance, and/or suggested that she was sexually
available. In contrast, when the same attractive
confederate was dressed conservatively, the women
assigned to this condition behaved well. They
greeted her in a friendly manner, and none of them
discussed her when she left the room.
Baumeister and Twenge [2002] hypothesized that
women ‘‘stifle each other’s sexuality’’ as a way of
maintaining advantage in the negotiation of
resources. Females who make sex readily available
TABLE III. Means, Standard Errors, and Post-Hoc Results by
Experimental Condition
Condition 1
conservative
(N 5 33)
Condition 2
sexy-thin
(N 5 33)
Condition 3
sexy-fat
(N 5 33)
S-N-K
Post-hoc
analysis
How likely would you introduce her to your boyfriend? 4.73
�
(.29)
��
1.86 (.29) 2.95 (.29) 142, 3 and 342
How likely would you let your boyfriend spend
time alone with her?
3.38 (.29) 1.68 (.28) 2.55 (.28) 142, 3 and 342
How likely would she be a friend of yours? 5.41 (.30) 2.73 (.30)
3.14 (.30) 142, 3
How sexy is she? 4.70 (.46) 6.77 (.44) 4.18 (.44) 241, 3
How cute is she? 7.60 (.47) 4.68 (.45) 4.55 (.45) 142, 3
�
Mean;
��
standard error.
574 Vaillancourt and Sharma
Aggr. Behav.
compromise the power holding position of the
group. It is therefore in the best interest of the
group to punish those who violate this unspoken
rule/convention. Results from Study 1 suggest that
the non-verbal chastisement of violators is an almost
universal phenomenon among women who are at
the height of their reproductive value, a time in
which women are most likely to compete with each
other for access to desirable mates [Campbell, 1995,
1999].
Results of Study 1 raise the question about why
might women use indirect means rather than overt
behavior to aggress against one another (as only one
of the participants did in this study). It has been
suggested that females use indirect rather than overt
aggression because it represents a presumably safer
and more effective intrasexual competition strategy
[Campbell, 1995, 1999; see also Bjorkqvist, 1994].
Physical aggression runs the risk of injury or death,
which historically (and currently in some places
around the world), would have hindered the repro-
ductive success of a female by increasing the vulner-
ability of her offspring. In terms of effectiveness, there
is a convincing literature delineating the negative
effects indirect aggression has on those who fall victim,
which is particularly pronounced in females [e.g., Card
et al., 2008].
In Study 2, we examined whether the sexy
confederate from Study 1 was in fact viewed as a
sexual rival by women. Results from this study
provided additional support for our intrasexual
competition among women hypothesis. Women
independently rated the sexy-thin confederate from
Study 1 as the sexiest of the three women. They were
also two times less likely to want to introduce their
boyfriend to her or have him spend time alone with
her compared to the conservative confederate who
was rated as being the most attractive (cute) of the
three women (although she was the same person in
all three photos). Notably, women were also less
likely to introduce the sexy-fat confederate to their
boyfriend or allow him to spend time with her than
the thin attractive, but conservatively dressed,
confederate. It is clear from the research literature
that larger women are not perceived by men as being
attractive. In fact, studies consistently demonstrated
that women with large breasts and a low waist-to-
hip ratio are preferred by men for both short-term
and long-term relationships [e.g., Singh, 1993, 1994;
Singh and Young, 1995]. However, this finding is
consistent with evidence that women are threatened
by, disapprove of, and punish women who appear
and/or act promiscuous [Baumeister and Twenge,
2002]. Recently, Griskevicius et al. [2009] reported
that for women, mating motives increased indirect
aggression use, a finding consistent with Benenson’s
[2009] idea that human females constantly compete
with one another to initiate and maintain a long-
term partnership with a mate. The form this
competition takes is indirect aggression. Across
human history, females have relied heavily on the
investment of males for the provision of resources
and for the protection of themselves and their
offspring [Benenson, 2009, p. 269]. Having a mate
defect often indicates fewer resources for the woman
and her offspring.
Finally, consistent with our initial hypothesis, we
found that women reported being less likely to be
friends with the sexy-thin or sexy-fat confederate
than the conservative confederate. Bleske and
Shackelford [2001] examined mating rivalry in
same-sex friendships and found that women, but
not men, were less willing to be friends with a
woman who was described as sexually promiscuous.
We suspect that women who appear sexually
available are not perceived as ‘‘safe’’ friends—they
are expected to be mate poachers and they likely
devalue a person’s mate value (guilty by association).
More studies examining this specific hypothesis are
needed.
Limitations and Future Directions
Although the effect sizes of this study were large,
there are limitations that ought to be considered.
First, it is possible that participants were reacting to
a norm violation, although there were no differences
between the two conditions on the facial expression
surprise. Women certainly dress provocatively in a
university context; however, most research assistants
(the role played by the confederate in Study 1)
would likely not be dressed in such a sexy manner.
Nevertheless, we suspect that it would be rather
difficult to assess this alternative norm violation
hypothesis in a plausible way. For example, the
confederate could be dressed as a clown (norm
violation) and we would expect a reaction from
participants but we would also expect them to be
suspicious of the intent of the study therefore
compromising validity.
Second, we did not assess the effects of ovulatory
cycle on women’s behavior (Study 1) or ratings
(Study 2). Fisher [2004] found that the time of
ovulation was related to women rating other women
as less attractive, suggesting that intrasexual com-
petition may increase during times of peak fertility.
Durante et al. [2008] found shifts in clothing choice
related to ovulation with women showing greater
575Intrasexual Competition Among Women
Aggr. Behav.
preference for revealing clothing near ovulation.
These authors suggested that this shift in clothing
preference may reflect an increase in ‘‘female–female
competition near ovulation’’ (p. 1451). Replicating
the findings of this study taking into account a
woman’s menstrual cycle seems warranted.
Third, future studies ought to consider the role
dating status has on intrasexual competition.
Intrasexual competition is highest when people are
not in a committed relationship [e.g., Daly and
Wilson, 1988]. However, as Benenson [2009] argues,
for females, this competition extends beyond the
courtship because females need to maintain their
partner’s ‘‘loyalty’’ which discourages the diversion
of resources to other females.
CONCLUSION
Most studies on intrasexual competition across
species have focused on males, and most studies on
humans have not taken an experimental approach in
which participants are randomized to different
conditions. To our knowledge, Study 1 is the first
experimental study to assess female intrasexual
competition through the use of indirect aggression.
We found strong empirical support (effect size;
Cohen’s d 5 1.74) for the hypothesis that women
would be particularly intolerant of a sexy peer and
that this intolerance would take the form of indirect
aggression. With few exceptions, the women in the
sexy condition behaved badly, aggressing against
a woman whose only indiscretion was to be
dressed in a sexually evocative manner. They also
behaved more poorly with a friend than with
a stranger.
In Study 2, we attempted to verify whether the
sexy confederate was perceived as a sexual rival.
Consistent with this idea, we hypothesized that
women would not want to introduce her to their
boyfriend or allow him to spend time with her. We
also hypothesized that they would not let their
partner spend time with the sexy-fat confederate
because her sexy clothing would likely be perceived
as a sign of sexual availability. Manipulating the
appearance of the sexy confederate to by making her
appear overweight but still sexually provocative, we
found strong support for this sexual rival hypothesis
(effect size; partial Z2 5 .44). We also asked parti-
cipants how likely they would be friends with her,
hypothesizing that they would be less inclined if she
was dressed provocatively. Results strongly sup-
ported this hypothesis (effect size; partial Z2 5 .43).
Taken together, the results of this study provide
support for the idea that women do engage in
intrasexual competition through the use of indirect
aggression.
ACKNOWLEDGMENTS
We thank Heather Brittain, Eric Duku, Lisa
Elliot, Amanda Krygsman and Jessie Miller for
their help with this research. We also thank
Charles Cunningham, Wendy Troop-Gordon,
Daniel Quigley, Steven Arnocky, Khrista Boylan,
and Louis Schmidt for their helpful comments
regarding this manuscript. Funding support came
from Canadian Institutes of Health Research,
Canada Research Chairs program and the Social
Sciences and Humanities Research Council of
Canada (awarded to Tracy Vaillancourt).
REFERENCES
Abbey A, Cozzarelli C, McLaughlin K, Harnish J. 1987. The
effect
of clothing and dyad sex composition on perceptions of sexual
intent: Do women evaluate these cues differently. J Appl Soc
Psychol 17:108–126. DOI: 10.1111/j.1559-
1816.1987.tb00304.x.
Archer J. 2009. Does sexual selection explain human sex
differences
in aggression? Behav Brain Sci 32:249–311. DOI: 10.1017/
S0140525X09990951.
Arnocky S, Sunderani S, Miller JL, Vaillancourt T. (2011).
Jealousy
mediates the relationship between attractiveness and females’
indirect aggression. Personal Relationships.
Barber N. 1995. The evolutionary psychology of physical
attractive-
ness: Sexual selection and human morphology. Ethol Sociobiol
16:395–424. DOI: 10.1016/0162-3095(95)00068-2.
Baumeister RF, Twenge JM. 2002. Cultural suppression of
female
sexuality. Rev Gen Psychol 6:166–203. DOI: 10.1037//1089-
2680.6.2.166.
Benenson JF. 2009. Dominating versus eliminating the
competition:
Sex differences in human intrasexual competition. Behav Brain
Sci 32:268–269. DOI: 10.1017/S0140525X0999046X.
Benenson JF, Markovits H, Thompson ME, Wrangham RW.
2011.
Under threat of social exclusion, females exclude more than
males. Psychol Sci 22:538–544. DOI:
10.1177/0956797611402511.
Bjorkqvist K. 1994. Sex differences in physical, verbal and
indirect
aggression: A review of recent research. Sex Roles 30:177–188.
Bleske AL, Shackelford TK. 2001. Poaching, promiscuity, and
deceit: Combating mating rivalry in same- sex friendships.
Personal Relationships 8:407–424. DOI: 10.1111/j.1475-
6811.2001.tb00048.x.
Buss DM. 1988. The evolution of human intrasexual
competition:
Tactics of mate attraction. J Pers Soc Psychol 54:616–628. DOI:
10.1037/0022-3514.54.4.616.
Buss DM. 1989. Sex differences in human mate preferences:
Evolutionary hypotheses tested in 37 cultures. Behav Brain Sci
12:1–49. DOI: 0.1017/S0140525X00023992.
Buss DM, Dedden LA. 1990. Derogation of competitors. J Soc
Pers
Relat 7:395–422. DOI: 10.1177/0265407590073006.
Buss DM, Schmitt DP. 1993. Sexual strategies theory: An
evolu-
tionary perspective on human mating. Psychol Rev 100:204–
232.
DOI: 10.1037/0033-295X.100.2.204.
576 Vaillancourt and Sharma
Aggr. Behav.
Buss DM, Shackelford TK, Choe J, Buunk BP, Dijkstra P. 2000.
Distress about mating rivals. Pers Relat 7:235–243. DOI:
10.1111/j.1475-6811.2000.tb00014.x.
Campbell A. 1995. A few good men: Evolutionary psychology
and
female adolescent aggression. Ethol Sociobiol 16:99–123. DOI:
10.1016/0162-3095(94)00072-F.
Campbell A. 1999. Staying alive: Evolution, culture and
women’s
intra-sexual aggression. Behav Brain Sci 22:203–252. DOI:
10.1017/S014025X9900181.
Campbell A. 2002. A Mind of Her Own: The Evolutionary
Psychology of Women. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Card NA, Stucky BD, Sawalani GM, Little TD. 2008. Direct and
indirect aggression during childhood and adolescence. A meta-
analytic review of gender differences, intercorrelations, and
relations to maladjustment. Child Dev 79:1185–2008. DOI:
10.1111/j.1467-8624.2008.01184.x.
Clutton-Brock TH. 2007. Sexual selection in males and females.
Science 318:1882–1885. DOI: 10.1126/science.1133311.
Daly M, Wilson M. 1988. Homicide. New York: Aldine de
Gruyter.
Darwin C. 1871. The Decent of Man and Selection in Relation
to
Sex. London: John Murray.
Dunbar RIM, Duncan NDC, Marriott A. 1997. Human
controver-
sial behavior. Hum Nat 8:231–246. DOI: 10.1007/BF02912493.
Durante KM, Li NP, Haselton MG. 2008. Changes in women’s
choice of dress across the ovulatory cycle: Naturalistic and
laboratory task-based evidence. Pers Soc Psychol Bull
34:1451–1460. DOI: 10.1177/0146167208323103.
Ekman P, Friesen WV. 2003. Unmasking the Face: A Guide to
Recognizing Emotions From Facial Expressions. Cambridge:
Marlor Books.
Ekman P, Friesen WV, Hager JC. 2002. The Facial Action
Coding
System. London: Research Nexus.
Fisher ML. 2004. Female intrasexual competition decreases
female
facial attractiveness. Proc R Soc Lond Biol Lett 271:283–285.
DOI: 10.1098/rsbl.2004.0160.
Fisher M, Cox A. 2009. The influence of female attractiveness
on
competitor derogation. J Evol Psychol 7:141–155. DOI:
10.1098/
rsbl.2004.0160.
Gallup AC, Wilson DS. 2009. Body mass index (BMI) and peer
aggression in adolescent females: An evolutionary perspective.
J Soc Evol Cult Psychol 3:356–371. Retrieved from http://137.
140.1.71/jsec/
Geary DC. 2010. Male, Female: The Evolution of Human Sex
Differences, 2nd edition. Washington, DC: American Psycholo-
gical Association.
Grammer K, Thornhill R. 1994. Human (homo sapiens) facial
attractiveness and sexual selection: The role of symmetry and
averageness. J Comp Psychol 108:233–242. DOI: 10.1037/0735-
7036.108.3.233.
Griskevicius V, Tybur JM, Gangestad SW, Perea EF, Shapiro
JR,
Kenrick DT. 2009. Aggress to impress: Hostility as an evolved
context-dependent strategy. J Pers Soc Psychol 96:980–994.
DOI: 10.1037/a0013907.
Hornstein GA. 1985. Intimacy in conversational style as a
function
of the degree of closeness between members of a dyad. J Pers
Soc
Psychol 49:671–681. DOI: 10.1037/0022-3514.49.3.671.
Kenrick DT, Sadalla EK, Groth G, Trost MR. 1990. Evolution,
traits, and the stages of human courtship: Qualifying the
parental investment model. J Pers 58:97–116. DOI: 10.1111/
j.1467-6494.1990.tb00909.x.
Kenrick DT, Neuberg SL, Zierk K, Krones J. 1994. Evolution
and
social cognition: Contrast effects as a function of sex,
dominance,
and physical attractiveness. Pers Soc Psychol Bull 20:210–217.
DOI: 10.1177/0146167294202008.
Leenaars LS, Dane AV, Marini ZA. 2008. Evolutionary
perspective on indirect victimization in adolescence: the role of
attractiveness, dating and sexual behavior. Aggr Behav
34:404–415. DOI: 10.1002/ab.20252.
Schmitt DP, Buss DM. 1996. Strategic self-enhancement and
competitor derogation: Sex and context effects on the perceived
effectiveness of mate attraction tactics. J Pers Soc Psychol
70:1185–1204. DOI: 10.1037/0022-3514.70.6.1185.
Singh D. 1993. Adaptive significance of female attractiveness:
Role of waist-to-hip ratio. J Pers Soc Psychol 65:293–307.
DOI: 10.1037/0022-3514.65.2.293.
Singh D. 1994. Is thin really beautiful and good? Relationship
between physical attractiveness and waist-to-hip ratio. Pers
Individ Diff 16:123–132.
Singh D, Young RK. 1995. Body weight, waist-to-hip ratio,
breasts,
and hips: Role in judgments of female attractiveness and
desirability for relationships. Ethol Sociobiol 16:483–507. DOI:
10.1016/0162-3095(95)00074-7.
Symons D. 1979. The Evolution of Human Sexuality. New
York:
Oxford University Press.
Vaillancourt T. 2005. Indirect aggression among human: Social
construct or evolutionary adaptation? In Tremblay RE, Hartup
WW, Archer J (editors). Developmental Origins of Aggression.
New York: Guilford, pp 158–177.
Walters S, Crawford C. 1994. The importance of mate attraction
for
intrasexual competition in men and women. Ethol Sociobiol
15:5–30. DOI: 10.1016/0162-3095(94)90025-6.
Wilson M, Daly M. 1985. Competitiveness, risk taking and
violence: The young male syndrome. Ethol Sociobiol 6:59–73.
DOI: 10.1016/0162-3095(85)90041-X.
577Intrasexual Competition Among Women
Aggr. Behav.

More Related Content

Similar to AGGRESSIVE BEHAVIORVolume 37, pages 569–577 (2011)Into.docx

Feminist Theory Group Projec Tfinal Twu
Feminist Theory Group Projec Tfinal TwuFeminist Theory Group Projec Tfinal Twu
Feminist Theory Group Projec Tfinal TwuPJoyceRandolph
 
Running head Examining the reasons 1Examining the reasons 16.docx
Running head Examining the reasons 1Examining the reasons 16.docxRunning head Examining the reasons 1Examining the reasons 16.docx
Running head Examining the reasons 1Examining the reasons 16.docxcharisellington63520
 
Feminist Philosophy Powerpoint
Feminist Philosophy PowerpointFeminist Philosophy Powerpoint
Feminist Philosophy PowerpointARH_Miller
 
FEMINIST FORUMFeminist Ideals for a Healthy Female Adolesc.docx
FEMINIST FORUMFeminist Ideals for a Healthy Female Adolesc.docxFEMINIST FORUMFeminist Ideals for a Healthy Female Adolesc.docx
FEMINIST FORUMFeminist Ideals for a Healthy Female Adolesc.docxssuser454af01
 
Not Apart of the Club Consumer Crossing Over Marketing Niche Boundaries
Not Apart of the Club Consumer Crossing Over Marketing Niche BoundariesNot Apart of the Club Consumer Crossing Over Marketing Niche Boundaries
Not Apart of the Club Consumer Crossing Over Marketing Niche BoundariesRudy Diaz
 
Addressing the Safety and Trauma Issues of Abused Women
Addressing the Safety and Trauma Issues of Abused WomenAddressing the Safety and Trauma Issues of Abused Women
Addressing the Safety and Trauma Issues of Abused WomenLeslie Tutty
 
Internalized Misogyny Complete
Internalized Misogyny CompleteInternalized Misogyny Complete
Internalized Misogyny CompleteSaraPilon
 
ANTY 500 Annotated Bibliography
ANTY 500 Annotated BibliographyANTY 500 Annotated Bibliography
ANTY 500 Annotated BibliographyCarly Ryther
 
1. What are your ideas for your Research Topic inspired by Jac.docx
1. What are your ideas for your Research Topic inspired by Jac.docx1. What are your ideas for your Research Topic inspired by Jac.docx
1. What are your ideas for your Research Topic inspired by Jac.docxambersalomon88660
 
Gender Roles and Sexual Happiness Powerpoint
Gender Roles and Sexual Happiness PowerpointGender Roles and Sexual Happiness Powerpoint
Gender Roles and Sexual Happiness PowerpointMicoleMF
 
Women's perspectives of BDSM power exchange.
Women's perspectives of BDSM power exchange.Women's perspectives of BDSM power exchange.
Women's perspectives of BDSM power exchange.groovyfever001
 
DICK-THESIS-2015
DICK-THESIS-2015DICK-THESIS-2015
DICK-THESIS-2015Amanda Dick
 

Similar to AGGRESSIVE BEHAVIORVolume 37, pages 569–577 (2011)Into.docx (16)

Feminist Theory Group Projec Tfinal Twu
Feminist Theory Group Projec Tfinal TwuFeminist Theory Group Projec Tfinal Twu
Feminist Theory Group Projec Tfinal Twu
 
Running head Examining the reasons 1Examining the reasons 16.docx
Running head Examining the reasons 1Examining the reasons 16.docxRunning head Examining the reasons 1Examining the reasons 16.docx
Running head Examining the reasons 1Examining the reasons 16.docx
 
Feminist Philosophy Powerpoint
Feminist Philosophy PowerpointFeminist Philosophy Powerpoint
Feminist Philosophy Powerpoint
 
FEMINIST FORUMFeminist Ideals for a Healthy Female Adolesc.docx
FEMINIST FORUMFeminist Ideals for a Healthy Female Adolesc.docxFEMINIST FORUMFeminist Ideals for a Healthy Female Adolesc.docx
FEMINIST FORUMFeminist Ideals for a Healthy Female Adolesc.docx
 
Armstrong et al (2006) Sexual Assault on Campus: A Multilevel, Integrative Ap...
Armstrong et al (2006) Sexual Assault on Campus: A Multilevel, Integrative Ap...Armstrong et al (2006) Sexual Assault on Campus: A Multilevel, Integrative Ap...
Armstrong et al (2006) Sexual Assault on Campus: A Multilevel, Integrative Ap...
 
Not Apart of the Club Consumer Crossing Over Marketing Niche Boundaries
Not Apart of the Club Consumer Crossing Over Marketing Niche BoundariesNot Apart of the Club Consumer Crossing Over Marketing Niche Boundaries
Not Apart of the Club Consumer Crossing Over Marketing Niche Boundaries
 
Addressing the Safety and Trauma Issues of Abused Women
Addressing the Safety and Trauma Issues of Abused WomenAddressing the Safety and Trauma Issues of Abused Women
Addressing the Safety and Trauma Issues of Abused Women
 
Internalized Misogyny Complete
Internalized Misogyny CompleteInternalized Misogyny Complete
Internalized Misogyny Complete
 
352 Final paper
352 Final paper 352 Final paper
352 Final paper
 
PSYC482 Paper 2
PSYC482 Paper 2PSYC482 Paper 2
PSYC482 Paper 2
 
ANTY 500 Annotated Bibliography
ANTY 500 Annotated BibliographyANTY 500 Annotated Bibliography
ANTY 500 Annotated Bibliography
 
1. What are your ideas for your Research Topic inspired by Jac.docx
1. What are your ideas for your Research Topic inspired by Jac.docx1. What are your ideas for your Research Topic inspired by Jac.docx
1. What are your ideas for your Research Topic inspired by Jac.docx
 
Gender Roles and Sexual Happiness Powerpoint
Gender Roles and Sexual Happiness PowerpointGender Roles and Sexual Happiness Powerpoint
Gender Roles and Sexual Happiness Powerpoint
 
Social psychology
Social psychologySocial psychology
Social psychology
 
Women's perspectives of BDSM power exchange.
Women's perspectives of BDSM power exchange.Women's perspectives of BDSM power exchange.
Women's perspectives of BDSM power exchange.
 
DICK-THESIS-2015
DICK-THESIS-2015DICK-THESIS-2015
DICK-THESIS-2015
 

More from nettletondevon

Your NamePractical ConnectionYour NameNOTE To insert a .docx
Your NamePractical ConnectionYour NameNOTE To insert a .docxYour NamePractical ConnectionYour NameNOTE To insert a .docx
Your NamePractical ConnectionYour NameNOTE To insert a .docxnettletondevon
 
Your namePresenter’s name(s) DateTITILE Motivatio.docx
Your namePresenter’s name(s) DateTITILE Motivatio.docxYour namePresenter’s name(s) DateTITILE Motivatio.docx
Your namePresenter’s name(s) DateTITILE Motivatio.docxnettletondevon
 
Your nameProfessor NameCourseDatePaper Outline.docx
Your nameProfessor NameCourseDatePaper Outline.docxYour nameProfessor NameCourseDatePaper Outline.docx
Your nameProfessor NameCourseDatePaper Outline.docxnettletondevon
 
Your name _________________________________ Date of submission _.docx
Your name _________________________________ Date of submission _.docxYour name _________________________________ Date of submission _.docx
Your name _________________________________ Date of submission _.docxnettletondevon
 
Your NameECD 310 Exceptional Learning and InclusionInstruct.docx
Your NameECD 310 Exceptional Learning and InclusionInstruct.docxYour NameECD 310 Exceptional Learning and InclusionInstruct.docx
Your NameECD 310 Exceptional Learning and InclusionInstruct.docxnettletondevon
 
Your Name University of the Cumberlands ISOL634-25 P.docx
Your Name University of the Cumberlands ISOL634-25 P.docxYour Name University of the Cumberlands ISOL634-25 P.docx
Your Name University of the Cumberlands ISOL634-25 P.docxnettletondevon
 
Your Name Professor Name Subject Name 06 Apr.docx
Your Name  Professor Name  Subject Name  06 Apr.docxYour Name  Professor Name  Subject Name  06 Apr.docx
Your Name Professor Name Subject Name 06 Apr.docxnettletondevon
 
Your muscular system examassignment is to describe location (su.docx
Your muscular system examassignment is to describe location (su.docxYour muscular system examassignment is to describe location (su.docx
Your muscular system examassignment is to describe location (su.docxnettletondevon
 
Your midterm will be a virtual, individual assignment. You can choos.docx
Your midterm will be a virtual, individual assignment. You can choos.docxYour midterm will be a virtual, individual assignment. You can choos.docx
Your midterm will be a virtual, individual assignment. You can choos.docxnettletondevon
 
Your local art museum has asked you to design a gallery dedicated to.docx
Your local art museum has asked you to design a gallery dedicated to.docxYour local art museum has asked you to design a gallery dedicated to.docx
Your local art museum has asked you to design a gallery dedicated to.docxnettletondevon
 
Your letter should include Introduction – Include your name, i.docx
Your letter should include Introduction – Include your name, i.docxYour letter should include Introduction – Include your name, i.docx
Your letter should include Introduction – Include your name, i.docxnettletondevon
 
Your legal analysis should be approximately 500 wordsDetermine.docx
Your legal analysis should be approximately 500 wordsDetermine.docxYour legal analysis should be approximately 500 wordsDetermine.docx
Your legal analysis should be approximately 500 wordsDetermine.docxnettletondevon
 
Your Last Name 1Your Name Teacher Name English cl.docx
Your Last Name  1Your Name Teacher Name English cl.docxYour Last Name  1Your Name Teacher Name English cl.docx
Your Last Name 1Your Name Teacher Name English cl.docxnettletondevon
 
Your job is to delegate job tasks to each healthcare practitioner (U.docx
Your job is to delegate job tasks to each healthcare practitioner (U.docxYour job is to delegate job tasks to each healthcare practitioner (U.docx
Your job is to delegate job tasks to each healthcare practitioner (U.docxnettletondevon
 
Your job is to look at the routing tables and DRAW (on a piece of pa.docx
Your job is to look at the routing tables and DRAW (on a piece of pa.docxYour job is to look at the routing tables and DRAW (on a piece of pa.docx
Your job is to look at the routing tables and DRAW (on a piece of pa.docxnettletondevon
 
Your job is to design a user interface that displays the lotto.docx
Your job is to design a user interface that displays the lotto.docxYour job is to design a user interface that displays the lotto.docx
Your job is to design a user interface that displays the lotto.docxnettletondevon
 
Your Introduction of the StudyYour Purpose of the stud.docx
Your Introduction of the StudyYour Purpose of the stud.docxYour Introduction of the StudyYour Purpose of the stud.docx
Your Introduction of the StudyYour Purpose of the stud.docxnettletondevon
 
Your instructor will assign peer reviewers. You will review a fell.docx
Your instructor will assign peer reviewers. You will review a fell.docxYour instructor will assign peer reviewers. You will review a fell.docx
Your instructor will assign peer reviewers. You will review a fell.docxnettletondevon
 
Your initial reading is a close examination of the work youve c.docx
Your initial reading is a close examination of the work youve c.docxYour initial reading is a close examination of the work youve c.docx
Your initial reading is a close examination of the work youve c.docxnettletondevon
 
Your initial posting must be no less than 200 words each and is due .docx
Your initial posting must be no less than 200 words each and is due .docxYour initial posting must be no less than 200 words each and is due .docx
Your initial posting must be no less than 200 words each and is due .docxnettletondevon
 

More from nettletondevon (20)

Your NamePractical ConnectionYour NameNOTE To insert a .docx
Your NamePractical ConnectionYour NameNOTE To insert a .docxYour NamePractical ConnectionYour NameNOTE To insert a .docx
Your NamePractical ConnectionYour NameNOTE To insert a .docx
 
Your namePresenter’s name(s) DateTITILE Motivatio.docx
Your namePresenter’s name(s) DateTITILE Motivatio.docxYour namePresenter’s name(s) DateTITILE Motivatio.docx
Your namePresenter’s name(s) DateTITILE Motivatio.docx
 
Your nameProfessor NameCourseDatePaper Outline.docx
Your nameProfessor NameCourseDatePaper Outline.docxYour nameProfessor NameCourseDatePaper Outline.docx
Your nameProfessor NameCourseDatePaper Outline.docx
 
Your name _________________________________ Date of submission _.docx
Your name _________________________________ Date of submission _.docxYour name _________________________________ Date of submission _.docx
Your name _________________________________ Date of submission _.docx
 
Your NameECD 310 Exceptional Learning and InclusionInstruct.docx
Your NameECD 310 Exceptional Learning and InclusionInstruct.docxYour NameECD 310 Exceptional Learning and InclusionInstruct.docx
Your NameECD 310 Exceptional Learning and InclusionInstruct.docx
 
Your Name University of the Cumberlands ISOL634-25 P.docx
Your Name University of the Cumberlands ISOL634-25 P.docxYour Name University of the Cumberlands ISOL634-25 P.docx
Your Name University of the Cumberlands ISOL634-25 P.docx
 
Your Name Professor Name Subject Name 06 Apr.docx
Your Name  Professor Name  Subject Name  06 Apr.docxYour Name  Professor Name  Subject Name  06 Apr.docx
Your Name Professor Name Subject Name 06 Apr.docx
 
Your muscular system examassignment is to describe location (su.docx
Your muscular system examassignment is to describe location (su.docxYour muscular system examassignment is to describe location (su.docx
Your muscular system examassignment is to describe location (su.docx
 
Your midterm will be a virtual, individual assignment. You can choos.docx
Your midterm will be a virtual, individual assignment. You can choos.docxYour midterm will be a virtual, individual assignment. You can choos.docx
Your midterm will be a virtual, individual assignment. You can choos.docx
 
Your local art museum has asked you to design a gallery dedicated to.docx
Your local art museum has asked you to design a gallery dedicated to.docxYour local art museum has asked you to design a gallery dedicated to.docx
Your local art museum has asked you to design a gallery dedicated to.docx
 
Your letter should include Introduction – Include your name, i.docx
Your letter should include Introduction – Include your name, i.docxYour letter should include Introduction – Include your name, i.docx
Your letter should include Introduction – Include your name, i.docx
 
Your legal analysis should be approximately 500 wordsDetermine.docx
Your legal analysis should be approximately 500 wordsDetermine.docxYour legal analysis should be approximately 500 wordsDetermine.docx
Your legal analysis should be approximately 500 wordsDetermine.docx
 
Your Last Name 1Your Name Teacher Name English cl.docx
Your Last Name  1Your Name Teacher Name English cl.docxYour Last Name  1Your Name Teacher Name English cl.docx
Your Last Name 1Your Name Teacher Name English cl.docx
 
Your job is to delegate job tasks to each healthcare practitioner (U.docx
Your job is to delegate job tasks to each healthcare practitioner (U.docxYour job is to delegate job tasks to each healthcare practitioner (U.docx
Your job is to delegate job tasks to each healthcare practitioner (U.docx
 
Your job is to look at the routing tables and DRAW (on a piece of pa.docx
Your job is to look at the routing tables and DRAW (on a piece of pa.docxYour job is to look at the routing tables and DRAW (on a piece of pa.docx
Your job is to look at the routing tables and DRAW (on a piece of pa.docx
 
Your job is to design a user interface that displays the lotto.docx
Your job is to design a user interface that displays the lotto.docxYour job is to design a user interface that displays the lotto.docx
Your job is to design a user interface that displays the lotto.docx
 
Your Introduction of the StudyYour Purpose of the stud.docx
Your Introduction of the StudyYour Purpose of the stud.docxYour Introduction of the StudyYour Purpose of the stud.docx
Your Introduction of the StudyYour Purpose of the stud.docx
 
Your instructor will assign peer reviewers. You will review a fell.docx
Your instructor will assign peer reviewers. You will review a fell.docxYour instructor will assign peer reviewers. You will review a fell.docx
Your instructor will assign peer reviewers. You will review a fell.docx
 
Your initial reading is a close examination of the work youve c.docx
Your initial reading is a close examination of the work youve c.docxYour initial reading is a close examination of the work youve c.docx
Your initial reading is a close examination of the work youve c.docx
 
Your initial posting must be no less than 200 words each and is due .docx
Your initial posting must be no less than 200 words each and is due .docxYour initial posting must be no less than 200 words each and is due .docx
Your initial posting must be no less than 200 words each and is due .docx
 

Recently uploaded

Contemporary philippine arts from the regions_PPT_Module_12 [Autosaved] (1).pptx
Contemporary philippine arts from the regions_PPT_Module_12 [Autosaved] (1).pptxContemporary philippine arts from the regions_PPT_Module_12 [Autosaved] (1).pptx
Contemporary philippine arts from the regions_PPT_Module_12 [Autosaved] (1).pptxRoyAbrique
 
Z Score,T Score, Percential Rank and Box Plot Graph
Z Score,T Score, Percential Rank and Box Plot GraphZ Score,T Score, Percential Rank and Box Plot Graph
Z Score,T Score, Percential Rank and Box Plot GraphThiyagu K
 
The basics of sentences session 2pptx copy.pptx
The basics of sentences session 2pptx copy.pptxThe basics of sentences session 2pptx copy.pptx
The basics of sentences session 2pptx copy.pptxheathfieldcps1
 
Call Girls in Dwarka Mor Delhi Contact Us 9654467111
Call Girls in Dwarka Mor Delhi Contact Us 9654467111Call Girls in Dwarka Mor Delhi Contact Us 9654467111
Call Girls in Dwarka Mor Delhi Contact Us 9654467111Sapana Sha
 
Grant Readiness 101 TechSoup and Remy Consulting
Grant Readiness 101 TechSoup and Remy ConsultingGrant Readiness 101 TechSoup and Remy Consulting
Grant Readiness 101 TechSoup and Remy ConsultingTechSoup
 
Kisan Call Centre - To harness potential of ICT in Agriculture by answer farm...
Kisan Call Centre - To harness potential of ICT in Agriculture by answer farm...Kisan Call Centre - To harness potential of ICT in Agriculture by answer farm...
Kisan Call Centre - To harness potential of ICT in Agriculture by answer farm...Krashi Coaching
 
Sanyam Choudhary Chemistry practical.pdf
Sanyam Choudhary Chemistry practical.pdfSanyam Choudhary Chemistry practical.pdf
Sanyam Choudhary Chemistry practical.pdfsanyamsingh5019
 
Measures of Central Tendency: Mean, Median and Mode
Measures of Central Tendency: Mean, Median and ModeMeasures of Central Tendency: Mean, Median and Mode
Measures of Central Tendency: Mean, Median and ModeThiyagu K
 
Introduction to AI in Higher Education_draft.pptx
Introduction to AI in Higher Education_draft.pptxIntroduction to AI in Higher Education_draft.pptx
Introduction to AI in Higher Education_draft.pptxpboyjonauth
 
Separation of Lanthanides/ Lanthanides and Actinides
Separation of Lanthanides/ Lanthanides and ActinidesSeparation of Lanthanides/ Lanthanides and Actinides
Separation of Lanthanides/ Lanthanides and ActinidesFatimaKhan178732
 
SOCIAL AND HISTORICAL CONTEXT - LFTVD.pptx
SOCIAL AND HISTORICAL CONTEXT - LFTVD.pptxSOCIAL AND HISTORICAL CONTEXT - LFTVD.pptx
SOCIAL AND HISTORICAL CONTEXT - LFTVD.pptxiammrhaywood
 
Privatization and Disinvestment - Meaning, Objectives, Advantages and Disadva...
Privatization and Disinvestment - Meaning, Objectives, Advantages and Disadva...Privatization and Disinvestment - Meaning, Objectives, Advantages and Disadva...
Privatization and Disinvestment - Meaning, Objectives, Advantages and Disadva...RKavithamani
 
18-04-UA_REPORT_MEDIALITERAСY_INDEX-DM_23-1-final-eng.pdf
18-04-UA_REPORT_MEDIALITERAСY_INDEX-DM_23-1-final-eng.pdf18-04-UA_REPORT_MEDIALITERAСY_INDEX-DM_23-1-final-eng.pdf
18-04-UA_REPORT_MEDIALITERAСY_INDEX-DM_23-1-final-eng.pdfssuser54595a
 
Web & Social Media Analytics Previous Year Question Paper.pdf
Web & Social Media Analytics Previous Year Question Paper.pdfWeb & Social Media Analytics Previous Year Question Paper.pdf
Web & Social Media Analytics Previous Year Question Paper.pdfJayanti Pande
 
A Critique of the Proposed National Education Policy Reform
A Critique of the Proposed National Education Policy ReformA Critique of the Proposed National Education Policy Reform
A Critique of the Proposed National Education Policy ReformChameera Dedduwage
 
Nutritional Needs Presentation - HLTH 104
Nutritional Needs Presentation - HLTH 104Nutritional Needs Presentation - HLTH 104
Nutritional Needs Presentation - HLTH 104misteraugie
 
Software Engineering Methodologies (overview)
Software Engineering Methodologies (overview)Software Engineering Methodologies (overview)
Software Engineering Methodologies (overview)eniolaolutunde
 
Paris 2024 Olympic Geographies - an activity
Paris 2024 Olympic Geographies - an activityParis 2024 Olympic Geographies - an activity
Paris 2024 Olympic Geographies - an activityGeoBlogs
 

Recently uploaded (20)

Contemporary philippine arts from the regions_PPT_Module_12 [Autosaved] (1).pptx
Contemporary philippine arts from the regions_PPT_Module_12 [Autosaved] (1).pptxContemporary philippine arts from the regions_PPT_Module_12 [Autosaved] (1).pptx
Contemporary philippine arts from the regions_PPT_Module_12 [Autosaved] (1).pptx
 
Z Score,T Score, Percential Rank and Box Plot Graph
Z Score,T Score, Percential Rank and Box Plot GraphZ Score,T Score, Percential Rank and Box Plot Graph
Z Score,T Score, Percential Rank and Box Plot Graph
 
The basics of sentences session 2pptx copy.pptx
The basics of sentences session 2pptx copy.pptxThe basics of sentences session 2pptx copy.pptx
The basics of sentences session 2pptx copy.pptx
 
Call Girls in Dwarka Mor Delhi Contact Us 9654467111
Call Girls in Dwarka Mor Delhi Contact Us 9654467111Call Girls in Dwarka Mor Delhi Contact Us 9654467111
Call Girls in Dwarka Mor Delhi Contact Us 9654467111
 
Grant Readiness 101 TechSoup and Remy Consulting
Grant Readiness 101 TechSoup and Remy ConsultingGrant Readiness 101 TechSoup and Remy Consulting
Grant Readiness 101 TechSoup and Remy Consulting
 
Kisan Call Centre - To harness potential of ICT in Agriculture by answer farm...
Kisan Call Centre - To harness potential of ICT in Agriculture by answer farm...Kisan Call Centre - To harness potential of ICT in Agriculture by answer farm...
Kisan Call Centre - To harness potential of ICT in Agriculture by answer farm...
 
Sanyam Choudhary Chemistry practical.pdf
Sanyam Choudhary Chemistry practical.pdfSanyam Choudhary Chemistry practical.pdf
Sanyam Choudhary Chemistry practical.pdf
 
Measures of Central Tendency: Mean, Median and Mode
Measures of Central Tendency: Mean, Median and ModeMeasures of Central Tendency: Mean, Median and Mode
Measures of Central Tendency: Mean, Median and Mode
 
Introduction to AI in Higher Education_draft.pptx
Introduction to AI in Higher Education_draft.pptxIntroduction to AI in Higher Education_draft.pptx
Introduction to AI in Higher Education_draft.pptx
 
Separation of Lanthanides/ Lanthanides and Actinides
Separation of Lanthanides/ Lanthanides and ActinidesSeparation of Lanthanides/ Lanthanides and Actinides
Separation of Lanthanides/ Lanthanides and Actinides
 
SOCIAL AND HISTORICAL CONTEXT - LFTVD.pptx
SOCIAL AND HISTORICAL CONTEXT - LFTVD.pptxSOCIAL AND HISTORICAL CONTEXT - LFTVD.pptx
SOCIAL AND HISTORICAL CONTEXT - LFTVD.pptx
 
Privatization and Disinvestment - Meaning, Objectives, Advantages and Disadva...
Privatization and Disinvestment - Meaning, Objectives, Advantages and Disadva...Privatization and Disinvestment - Meaning, Objectives, Advantages and Disadva...
Privatization and Disinvestment - Meaning, Objectives, Advantages and Disadva...
 
18-04-UA_REPORT_MEDIALITERAСY_INDEX-DM_23-1-final-eng.pdf
18-04-UA_REPORT_MEDIALITERAСY_INDEX-DM_23-1-final-eng.pdf18-04-UA_REPORT_MEDIALITERAСY_INDEX-DM_23-1-final-eng.pdf
18-04-UA_REPORT_MEDIALITERAСY_INDEX-DM_23-1-final-eng.pdf
 
Web & Social Media Analytics Previous Year Question Paper.pdf
Web & Social Media Analytics Previous Year Question Paper.pdfWeb & Social Media Analytics Previous Year Question Paper.pdf
Web & Social Media Analytics Previous Year Question Paper.pdf
 
A Critique of the Proposed National Education Policy Reform
A Critique of the Proposed National Education Policy ReformA Critique of the Proposed National Education Policy Reform
A Critique of the Proposed National Education Policy Reform
 
Nutritional Needs Presentation - HLTH 104
Nutritional Needs Presentation - HLTH 104Nutritional Needs Presentation - HLTH 104
Nutritional Needs Presentation - HLTH 104
 
Mattingly "AI & Prompt Design: Structured Data, Assistants, & RAG"
Mattingly "AI & Prompt Design: Structured Data, Assistants, & RAG"Mattingly "AI & Prompt Design: Structured Data, Assistants, & RAG"
Mattingly "AI & Prompt Design: Structured Data, Assistants, & RAG"
 
Software Engineering Methodologies (overview)
Software Engineering Methodologies (overview)Software Engineering Methodologies (overview)
Software Engineering Methodologies (overview)
 
INDIA QUIZ 2024 RLAC DELHI UNIVERSITY.pptx
INDIA QUIZ 2024 RLAC DELHI UNIVERSITY.pptxINDIA QUIZ 2024 RLAC DELHI UNIVERSITY.pptx
INDIA QUIZ 2024 RLAC DELHI UNIVERSITY.pptx
 
Paris 2024 Olympic Geographies - an activity
Paris 2024 Olympic Geographies - an activityParis 2024 Olympic Geographies - an activity
Paris 2024 Olympic Geographies - an activity
 

AGGRESSIVE BEHAVIORVolume 37, pages 569–577 (2011)Into.docx

  • 1. AGGRESSIVE BEHAVIOR Volume 37, pages 569–577 (2011) Intolerance of Sexy Peers: Intrasexual Competition Among Women Tracy Vaillancourt1,2� and Aanchal Sharma2 1Faculty of Education and School of Psychology, University of Ottawa, Ottawa, Ontario, Canada 2Department of Psychology, Neuroscience & Behaviour, McMaster University, Hamilton, Ontario, Canada : : : : : : : : : : : : : : : : : : : : : : : : : : : : : : : : : : : : : : : : : Intrasexual competition among males of different species, including humans, is well documented. Among females, far less is known. Recent nonexperimental studies suggest that women are intolerant of attractive females and use indirect aggression to derogate potential rivals. In Study 1, an experimental design was used to test the evolutionary-based hypothesis that women would be intolerant of sexy women and would censure those who seem to make sex too readily available. Results provide strong empirical support for intrasexual competition among women. Using independent raters, blind to condition, we found that almost all women were rated as reacting negatively (‘‘bitchy’’) to an attractive female confederate when she was dressed in a sexually provocative manner. In contrast, when she was dressed conservatively, the
  • 2. same confederate was barely noticed by the participants. In Study 2, an experimental design was used to assess whether the sexy female confederate from Study 1 was viewed as a sexual rival by women. Results indicated that as hypothesized, women did not want to introduce her to their boyfriend, allow him to spend time alone with her, or be friends with her. Findings from both studies are discussed in terms of evolutionary theory. Aggr. Behav. 37:569–577, 2011. r 2011 Wiley Periodicals, Inc. : : : : : : : : : : : : : : : : : : : : : : : : : : : : : : : : : : : : : : : : : Keywords: intrasexual competition; indirect aggression; women; experimental; design; evolution Intrasexual competition among males of various species, including humans, is well-documented [Archer, 2009; Daly and Wilson, 1988; Darwin, 1871; Geary, 2010; Wilson and Daly, 1985]. Among females of most mammalian species less is known [Clutton-Brock, 2007]. We do know that competition among nonhuman female primates (and other species) is typically over access to food, and that competition over the attention of males occurs when male parental investment is high or when males provide other valuable and/or limited resources to females [Geary, 2010]. The few non-experimental research studies that have been conducted on sexual selection in human females have focused on two competition strategies used by women: self-promotion (intersexual competi- tion) and the derogation of rivals (intrasexual competi- tion). Self-promotion involves epigamic displays of physical attractiveness such as wearing makeup and
  • 3. form-fitting clothing that are used to attract the attention of males [Barber, 1995; Buss, 1988; Buss and Schmitt, 1993; Fisher and Cox, 2009; Schmitt and Buss, 1996; Symons, 1979; Walters and Crawford, 1994]. Derogation of competitors takes the form of indirect aggression [Buss and Dedden, 1990; Campbell, 1995, 1999; Fisher, 2004; Vaillancourt, 2005; Walters and Crawford, 1994] which is a type of aggression that tends to be circuitous in nature and is presumably used to reduce the mate value of a rival [Buss and Dedden, 1990; Campbell, 1995, 1999; Fisher, 2004; Vaillancourt, 2005]. Indirect aggression is used by males and females [Card et al., 2008] and usually directed at same-sex peers [Gallup and Wilson, 2009]. It includes behaviors such as spreading rumors that question the perceived rival’s fidelity or level of promiscuity, disparaging the competitor’s appearance, excluding the rival from the peer group, giving her the silent Published online in Wiley Online Library (wileyonlinelibrary.com). DOI: 10.1002/ab.20413 Received 6 May 2011; Revised 29 July 2011; Accepted 16 August 2011 Grant Sponsors: Canadian Institutes of Health Research; Canada Research Chairs program and the Social Sciences and Humanities
  • 4. Research Council of Canada. � Correspondence to: Tracy Vaillancourt, Faculty of Education and School of Psychology, University of Ottawa, Ottawa, Ontario, Canada. E-mail: [email protected] r 2011 Wiley Periodicals, Inc. treatment, or using derisive body and facial gestures to make the rival feel badly about herself and thus less willing to compete. Studies have shown that even though indirect aggression is covert, it is nevertheless effective at inflicting harm on targets, particularly female targets, who are able to pick up on the subtle nuances which comprise this form of negative behavior [Vaillancourt, 2005]. Moreover, recent evidence suggests that women are more likely than men to engage in this type of behavior when they face a social threat [e.g., Benenson et al., 2011]. Research on human mate preferences clearly demonstrates that males show a strong preference for young, attractive females [Buss, 1989; Buss and Schmitt, 1993; Grammer and Thornhill, 1994; Singh, 1993, 1994; Singh and Young, 1995]. In reaction to this predilection, females derogate rivals who imbue these qualities. Indeed, studies have shown that females are particularly intolerant of attractive peers, using indirect aggression against them at a greater rate than their less attractive peers
  • 5. [e.g., Leenaars et al., 2008]. Moreover, at times when females are maximally fertile (time of ovulation), they are the most derogating of competitors; rating female faces as less attractive [Fisher, 2004]. In addition to being intolerant of physically attractive peers, we hypothesize that women are particularly intolerant of sexy attractive peers. According to Baumeister and Twenge [2002], a double standard of sexual morality exists in which women ‘‘stifle each other’s sexuality because sex is a limited resource that women use to negotiate with men, and scarcity gives women an advantage’’ (p. 166). In their review of relevant literature, Baumeister and Twenge found support for their theory that females, and not males, suppress the sexuality of other females. Females accomplish this by ‘‘punishing’’ other females who seem to make sex too readily available ‘‘through informal sanctions such as ostracism and derogatory gossip’’ (p. 172). In other words, females used indirect aggression to suppress the sexuality of other females. We examined the predication that women would ‘‘punish’’ other women who appear to make sex too readily available by randomly assigning young women in dyads (with a friend or with a stranger) to one of two conditions (Study 1). In the first condition, women were exposed to a conservatively dressed attractive female confederate. In the second condition, they were exposed to the same confederate dressed in sexy clothing. We hypothesized that most, if not all, women would express negative, derogative reactions (indirect aggression) toward the attractive confederate when her appearance emphasized sexually evocative qualities.
  • 6. We further hypothesized that these negative reactions would be particularly pronounced in the presence of a female friend rather than a stranger. This hypothesis was based on research demonstrat- ing that female friendships are characterized by high intimacy which includes making social comparisons and gossiping about others [Dunbar et al., 1997; Hornstein, 1985]. As well, we expected that women would be more comfortable expressing themselves negatively in the presence of a friend rather than a stranger. We also conducted a manipulation validation study (Study 2) by randomly assigning women to one of the three conditions in which they indepen- dently rated (1) a photograph of the conservatively dressed confederate from Study 1, (2) a photograph of the provocatively dressed confederate (sexy-thin) from Study 1, or (3) a manipulated photograph of the provocatively dressed confederate from Study 1 in which she appeared overweight (sexy-fat). Con- sistent with the idea that the sexy confederate from Study 1 was in fact viewed as a sexual rival, we hypothesized that women would be less willing to introduce her to their current or future boyfriend(s), or have their current or future boyfriend(s) spend time alone with her than the attractive conservative confederate or the sexy-fat confederate. We also hypothesized that compared to the conservatively dressed confederate, women would not want to introduce their partner or allow him to spend time with the sexy-fat woman because her sexy clothing would be perceived as an indicator of sexual availability and hence would be threatening 1
  • 7. [Abbey et al., 1987]. According to Buss et al. [2000], successful reten- tion of one’s mate hinges on one’s ability to prevent the partner from defecting or being unfaithful, and fending off rivals who may be interested in mating with that partner. Disparaging a sexy attractive woman (Study 1) or not allowing your partner to meet or spend time with such a woman (Study 2) is mate guarding (i.e., intrasexual competition). Finally, we hypothesized that women would be less likely to want to have the sexy-thin or the sexy- fat confederate as a friend, compared to the conservatively dressed confederate. This hypothesis was based on the idea that a woman would not want to associate with a woman who appears too sexually available because it would reduce her own 1 It has been shown that the presence of an intrasexual competitor does indeed represent a threat to fidelity [see Arnocky, Sunderani, Miller, & Vaillancourt, in press; Kenrick, Neuberg, Zierk, & Krones, 1994]. 570 Vaillancourt and Sharma Aggr. Behav.
  • 8. mate-value (she too would be seen as promiscuous). As well, having a sexy woman as a friend might mean that the woman would have to be constantly on guard to ensure that her partner does not cheat on her with this seemingly sexually available person. STUDY 1 Methods Participants. Eighty-six heterosexual women ranging in age from 19 to 23 years (Mean7SE 5 20.1470.26) from varying ethnic backgrounds participated in Study 1. The women were recruited from a mid-size university located in southern Ontario, Canada. Procedure. A two (friendship, stranger) by two (conservative, provocative) experimental design was implemented. Participants were paired together in dyads to create two participant conditions—friendship and stranger dyads, based on having or not having an existing friendship with the woman they were paired with. Participants were then randomly assigned to the two experimental conditions. In the first (conservative) condition (n 5 40; 20 friends and 20 strangers), participants were exposed to an attractive, conservatively dressed 21-year-old Caucasian female confederate (independently rated as attractive by 20 female undergraduate students on a scale from 1 to 10, Mean7SE 5 8.670.25) who embodied qualities considered attractive from an evolutionary perspective [low waist-to-hip ratio, clear skin, large breasts; Buss, 1989; Buss and Schmitt, 1993; Grammer and
  • 9. Thornhill, 1994; Singh, 1993, 1994; Singh and Young, 1995] (Fig. 1A). In the second (sexy) condition (n 5 46; 23 friends and 23 strangers), the same female confederate was dressed in a sexually provocative manner (Fig. 1B). In both conditions, the confederate’s behavior and mannerisms were standardized. Each trial was run with the confederate knocking on the door the same number of times, taking the same number of steps across the room and between the dyad (who was waiting for a moderated discussion on how women handle conflict in friendships to begin), and using the same script to ask the dyad the whereabouts of the study’s experimenter. The confederate then left the laboratory with the experimenter, a South Asian woman in her late 20s dressed conservatively across conditions. Before and after the experimenter and confederate exited the room the participants were unknowingly video and audio recorded using equipment that captured their individual reactions of their exposure to the confederate. Coding of observational data. Video clips of participants’ individual reactions of their exposure to the confederate were randomly presented to 13 primarily Caucasian women (mean age 5 23, SE 5 0.74) blind to condition, who classified and rated participants on two dependent variables: (1) whether or not they thought the participant was exhibiting bitchy 2 behavior (i.e., indirect aggression)
  • 10. and (2) if so, how bitchy her reaction was on a scale from 0 to 10 (0, not bitchy; 10, extremely bitchy). Fig. 1. Confederate dressed conservatively (A), provocatively (sexy-thin) (B), and provocatively (sexy-fat) (C). 2 Because the terms indirect aggression and derogation are scientific terms we asked participants to rate reactions using the colloquial term ‘‘bitchy’’ and provided them with a comprehensive list of what these behaviours entailed (see list provided in main text). 571Intrasexual Competition Among Women Aggr. Behav. A participant’s reaction was coded as bitchy if 70% or more of the coders identified it as so (n 5 36). A reaction was coded as not bitchy if 70% or more of the coders deemed it to not be (n 5 42). Cases in which neither threshold was achieved were coded as ‘‘unsure’’ (n 5 8). Bitchy behavior (use of indirect aggression) included negative facial expressions (e.g., looks of disgust, eye rolls), dismissive/avoidant behavior, ‘‘once-overs/death stares’’, body posture, back biting or mocking, sarcastic tone of voice, and fake smiles. To avoid capturing facial expression unrelated to the exposure to the confederate, 30 sec
  • 11. of tape were reviewed from the point of the confederate’s entrance into the room. If participants were still expressing a reaction toward the confeder- ate after the 30 sec, the coding time was extended accordingly (Mean7SE 5 38.3977.79 sec). In addition to coding the video clips on a universal impression of derogation, the data were also coded using the Facial Action Coding System [FACS; Ekman and Friesen, 2003]. FACS is a specialized and rigorously validated method of taxonomizing facial expressions. The system describes facial behavior and expressions of emotions with a series of muscle contractions termed action units. Two trained and certified FACS raters independently applied the FACS to each videotape, while remaining blind to the experimental condition (k varied from 0.85 to 1.00 across coding categories). Following the application of the FACS, each coder’s results were analyzed using the Emotion Facial Action Coding System [EmFACS; Ekman et al., 2002], a rule-based approach for assigning emotions to particular combinations of action units. The specific emotions coded for were: happiness, sadness, anger, contempt, surprise, disgust, and rage. FACS raters also coded for the presence of the following behavior: (1) whether participants ex- changed a glare with each other as a way of conveying information about the confederate (‘‘exchanging information’’), (2) if they looked at the confederate up and down (‘‘once-overs’’), (3) whether they said something negative about the confederate when she left the room (‘‘negative verbal comments’’), and (4) if they laughed at the confederate (‘‘laughed’’). The k score was 1.00 for all
  • 12. these coding categories. Manipulation check. At the end of the study, participants were asked to share their thoughts about what they felt the purpose of the study was. None of the participants identified the interruption by the confederate as a planned, strategic part of the study, nor were they able to identify the confeder- ate’s role in the study. Results w2 analyses, one-way analysis of variance (ANO- VA), and multilevel modeling were used to assess the hypotheses proposed for Study 1. Given the number of analyses conducted, Po.01 was used as an indicator of statistical significance. Bitchy Classification Examining participants’ global negative reactions (bitchy or not bitchy) to the confederate we found striking differences by condition (w2(2) 5 51.71; Po.0001; see Table I). Specifically, when women were rated as bitchy, all but 2 were in the sexy condition. And of those rated as not bitchy, most were in the conservative condition. The remaining eight women’s reactions were coded as unsure (two in the conservative condition and six in sexy condition). Bitchy Ratings When we examined how bitchy the women’s reactions were on a scale from 0 to 10, we found a statistically significant main effect for condition
  • 13. (sexy vs. conservative; F(1, 82) 5 64.00, Po.0001), a marginal effect for type of dyad (friends vs. strangers; F(1, 82) 5 4.10, Po.05), and no interac- tion between conditions (F(1, 82) 5 1.67, P 5 .20). Women in the sexy condition were rated as being bitchier (Mean7SE 5 3.4070.28) than women in the conservative condition (Mean7SE 5 0.2070.27) and women in friendship dyads were rated as being slightly bitchier (Mean7SE 5 2.2070.28) than women in stranger dyads (Mean7SE 5 1.4070.28) (Fig. 2). The effect size for the sexy condition was large (Cohen’s d 5 1.74) and for the dyad condition small (Cohen’s d 5 0.34). Because participants’ reactions were nested within dyads, interactions between dyad and condition were also examined using multilevel modeling with participants’ global negative reactions (level of bitchiness) as the outcome. Excluding those in the unsure group and controlling for the type of dyad, TABLE I. Relative Frequency of ‘‘Bitchy’’ Classification by Experimental Condition Conservative condition (N 5 40) Sexy condition (N 5 46) Bitchy 2 (5.0%) 34 (74%) Not bitchy 36 (90%) 6 (13%)
  • 14. Unsure 2 (5.0%) 6 (13%) 572 Vaillancourt and Sharma Aggr. Behav. we found that again, condition (sexy vs. conserva- tive) was a statistically significant correlate of outcome (OR 5 exp (4.651) 5 104.69, SE72.60; Po.001). Unfortunately, we could not test the initial hypothesis that women would behave more poorly when with a friend in the sexy condition (i.e., the interaction between condition and type of dyad) because the model did not converge. EmFACS Examining emotions coded with EmFACS across the sexy and conservative conditions we found that of the 11.6% (n 5 10) of the total sample that displayed true happiness all were assigned to the conservative condition (w2(1) 5 13.01, Po.001). Moreover, of the 12.8% (n 5 11) of the total sample that displayed anger, all were assigned to the sexy condition (w2(1) 5 10.97, Po.001). There were no other statistically significant findings for EmFACS identified reactions by conditions (friends vs. stranger or sexy vs. conservative) and no interactions between conditions. Other Negative Behavior As seen in Table II, when examining whether participants exchanged a look with their partner as a
  • 15. way of conveying information about the confederate we found notable differences by condition (w2(1) 5 11.59, Po.001) with far more women looking to each other to exchange information in the sexy condition than in the conservative condi- tion. As well, women in the sexy condition were far more likely to have looked the participant over than those in the conservative condition (w2(1) 5 31.19, Po.0001). We also found that negative comments were only made about the confederate in the sexy condition, w2(1) 5 12.13, Po.001. For example, one woman implied that the confederate was dressed to have sex with one of her professors and another said that the confederate’s ‘‘boobs were about to pop out’’. Importantly, all comments about the confed- erate were made after she left the room with one exception. One woman said ‘‘What the fuck is that?’’ directly to the confederate after blatantly looking her up and down while showing disgust. Finally, we found that participants laughed about the confederate when she left the room far more often in the sexy condition than in the conservative condition (w2(1) 5 15.48, Po.001), and when with a friend than with a stranger (n 5 5; w2(1) 5 11.33, Po.001). STUDY 2 Methods Participants. Sixty-six women ranging in age from 17 to 28 years (Mean7SE 5 18.7070.04) from varying ethnic backgrounds participated in the manipulation validation study. They were recruited
  • 16. from a mid-size university located in southern Ontario, Canada. Procedure. To verify the evolutionary signifi- cance of the exposure to the sexy confederate, women were randomly assigned to one of the three conditions. In all three conditions, participants were asked to rate a color photograph of the confederate from Study 1. Specifically, they were asked to rate the woman’s level of attractiveness (cuteness) and sexiness in the photograph using a scale of 1–10 with 1 5 not at all descriptive and 10 5 extremely descriptive. Participants were also asked to rate how likely, on a scale of 1 to 7 with 1 5 extremely unlikely and 1 5 extremely likely, would they Fig. 2. Mean bitchy scores by condition (sexy vs. conservative) and dyad (friends vs. strangers). TABLE II. Relative Frequency of Other Negative Behavior by Experimental Condition Type of behavior Conservative condition (N 5 40) Sexy condition (N 5 46) Exchanging information 1 (2.5%) 14 (30%)
  • 17. Once-overs 10 (25%) 39 (85%) Negative verbal comments 0 (0%) 12 (26%) Laughed 3 (7.5%) 21 (46%) 573Intrasexual Competition Among Women Aggr. Behav. introduce her to their boyfriend (current or future) and how likely would they let their boyfriend (current or future) spend time alone with her. In the first condition (conservative), a photograph of the conservatively dressed confederate was provided (Fig. 1A). In the second condition (sexy-thin), a photograph of the sexy dressed confederate was provided (Fig. 1B). Finally, in the third condition (sexy-fat), a photograph of the sexy dressed con- federate was manipulated so that she appeared overweight (Fig. 1C). Results ANOVA was used to examine participants’ perception of the confederate from Study 1. Given the number of analyses conducted, Po.01 was again used as an indicator of statistical significance. As expected, Student–Newman–Keuls post-hoc tests
  • 18. confirmed that even though participants rated the conservative confederate as cuter than the other two women (F(2, 61) 5 13.72, Po.001, partial Z2 5 .31), they nevertheless were less likely to introduce the sexy-thin confederate to their boyfriend (F(2, 63) 5 24.98, Po.001, partial Z2 5 .44), or let him spend time alone with her (F(2, 63) 5 8.81, Po.001, partial Z2 5 .22) than the sexy-fat or conservative confederate (see Table III for all Means, SE, and post-hoc comparisons). Moreover, participants also rated the sexy-thin confederate as being sexier than the sexy-fat and conservative confederates (F(2, 61) 5 9.74, Po.001, partial Z2 5 .24), with the post-hoc test revealing that the mean ratings on sexy for the sexy-fat and conservative confederates were not statistically significantly different from one another. Finally, consistent with our initial hypothesis, we found that women reported being less likely to be friends with the sexy-thin or sexy-fat confederate than the conservatively dressed confederate (F(2, 63) 5 23.66, Po.0001, partial Z2 5 .43). DISCUSSION Although the ultimate reason women derogate rivals is unknown, we strongly suspect that the use of indirect aggression by human females is rooted in evolutionary history. It has been noted in the nonhuman animal literature that female reproduc- tive competition is most intense within species in which males invest heavily in their offspring (with some exceptions see Clutton-Brock, 2007]. In humans, males are invested in their offspring, albeit to a lesser extent than females, and because they
  • 19. invest they also tend to be selective in terms of who they mate with on a long-term basis [Kenrick et al., 1990]. This choosiness puts pressure on females to compete for the most desirable mates and the form this competition takes is often the derision of perceived rivals [Campbell, 2002; Vaillancourt, 2005]. Using an experimental design, results of Study 1 provide support for the hypothesis that women do engage in intrasexual competition and that the form it takes is indirect aggression. In the presence of an attractive female who defied social convention by dressing in a sexually provocative manner, almost all women randomly assigned to this condition aggressed against her. The women in this condition were more likely to roll their eyes at the confederate, look her up and down, stare at her without conveying any emotion, and show anger while she was in the room. When the confederate left the room, many of them laughed at her, ridiculed her appearance, and/or suggested that she was sexually available. In contrast, when the same attractive confederate was dressed conservatively, the women assigned to this condition behaved well. They greeted her in a friendly manner, and none of them discussed her when she left the room. Baumeister and Twenge [2002] hypothesized that women ‘‘stifle each other’s sexuality’’ as a way of maintaining advantage in the negotiation of resources. Females who make sex readily available TABLE III. Means, Standard Errors, and Post-Hoc Results by Experimental Condition
  • 20. Condition 1 conservative (N 5 33) Condition 2 sexy-thin (N 5 33) Condition 3 sexy-fat (N 5 33) S-N-K Post-hoc analysis How likely would you introduce her to your boyfriend? 4.73 � (.29) �� 1.86 (.29) 2.95 (.29) 142, 3 and 342 How likely would you let your boyfriend spend time alone with her? 3.38 (.29) 1.68 (.28) 2.55 (.28) 142, 3 and 342
  • 21. How likely would she be a friend of yours? 5.41 (.30) 2.73 (.30) 3.14 (.30) 142, 3 How sexy is she? 4.70 (.46) 6.77 (.44) 4.18 (.44) 241, 3 How cute is she? 7.60 (.47) 4.68 (.45) 4.55 (.45) 142, 3 � Mean; �� standard error. 574 Vaillancourt and Sharma Aggr. Behav. compromise the power holding position of the group. It is therefore in the best interest of the group to punish those who violate this unspoken rule/convention. Results from Study 1 suggest that the non-verbal chastisement of violators is an almost universal phenomenon among women who are at the height of their reproductive value, a time in which women are most likely to compete with each other for access to desirable mates [Campbell, 1995, 1999]. Results of Study 1 raise the question about why might women use indirect means rather than overt behavior to aggress against one another (as only one of the participants did in this study). It has been suggested that females use indirect rather than overt aggression because it represents a presumably safer and more effective intrasexual competition strategy [Campbell, 1995, 1999; see also Bjorkqvist, 1994].
  • 22. Physical aggression runs the risk of injury or death, which historically (and currently in some places around the world), would have hindered the repro- ductive success of a female by increasing the vulner- ability of her offspring. In terms of effectiveness, there is a convincing literature delineating the negative effects indirect aggression has on those who fall victim, which is particularly pronounced in females [e.g., Card et al., 2008]. In Study 2, we examined whether the sexy confederate from Study 1 was in fact viewed as a sexual rival by women. Results from this study provided additional support for our intrasexual competition among women hypothesis. Women independently rated the sexy-thin confederate from Study 1 as the sexiest of the three women. They were also two times less likely to want to introduce their boyfriend to her or have him spend time alone with her compared to the conservative confederate who was rated as being the most attractive (cute) of the three women (although she was the same person in all three photos). Notably, women were also less likely to introduce the sexy-fat confederate to their boyfriend or allow him to spend time with her than the thin attractive, but conservatively dressed, confederate. It is clear from the research literature that larger women are not perceived by men as being attractive. In fact, studies consistently demonstrated that women with large breasts and a low waist-to- hip ratio are preferred by men for both short-term and long-term relationships [e.g., Singh, 1993, 1994; Singh and Young, 1995]. However, this finding is consistent with evidence that women are threatened by, disapprove of, and punish women who appear and/or act promiscuous [Baumeister and Twenge,
  • 23. 2002]. Recently, Griskevicius et al. [2009] reported that for women, mating motives increased indirect aggression use, a finding consistent with Benenson’s [2009] idea that human females constantly compete with one another to initiate and maintain a long- term partnership with a mate. The form this competition takes is indirect aggression. Across human history, females have relied heavily on the investment of males for the provision of resources and for the protection of themselves and their offspring [Benenson, 2009, p. 269]. Having a mate defect often indicates fewer resources for the woman and her offspring. Finally, consistent with our initial hypothesis, we found that women reported being less likely to be friends with the sexy-thin or sexy-fat confederate than the conservative confederate. Bleske and Shackelford [2001] examined mating rivalry in same-sex friendships and found that women, but not men, were less willing to be friends with a woman who was described as sexually promiscuous. We suspect that women who appear sexually available are not perceived as ‘‘safe’’ friends—they are expected to be mate poachers and they likely devalue a person’s mate value (guilty by association). More studies examining this specific hypothesis are needed. Limitations and Future Directions Although the effect sizes of this study were large, there are limitations that ought to be considered. First, it is possible that participants were reacting to a norm violation, although there were no differences
  • 24. between the two conditions on the facial expression surprise. Women certainly dress provocatively in a university context; however, most research assistants (the role played by the confederate in Study 1) would likely not be dressed in such a sexy manner. Nevertheless, we suspect that it would be rather difficult to assess this alternative norm violation hypothesis in a plausible way. For example, the confederate could be dressed as a clown (norm violation) and we would expect a reaction from participants but we would also expect them to be suspicious of the intent of the study therefore compromising validity. Second, we did not assess the effects of ovulatory cycle on women’s behavior (Study 1) or ratings (Study 2). Fisher [2004] found that the time of ovulation was related to women rating other women as less attractive, suggesting that intrasexual com- petition may increase during times of peak fertility. Durante et al. [2008] found shifts in clothing choice related to ovulation with women showing greater 575Intrasexual Competition Among Women Aggr. Behav. preference for revealing clothing near ovulation. These authors suggested that this shift in clothing preference may reflect an increase in ‘‘female–female competition near ovulation’’ (p. 1451). Replicating the findings of this study taking into account a woman’s menstrual cycle seems warranted. Third, future studies ought to consider the role
  • 25. dating status has on intrasexual competition. Intrasexual competition is highest when people are not in a committed relationship [e.g., Daly and Wilson, 1988]. However, as Benenson [2009] argues, for females, this competition extends beyond the courtship because females need to maintain their partner’s ‘‘loyalty’’ which discourages the diversion of resources to other females. CONCLUSION Most studies on intrasexual competition across species have focused on males, and most studies on humans have not taken an experimental approach in which participants are randomized to different conditions. To our knowledge, Study 1 is the first experimental study to assess female intrasexual competition through the use of indirect aggression. We found strong empirical support (effect size; Cohen’s d 5 1.74) for the hypothesis that women would be particularly intolerant of a sexy peer and that this intolerance would take the form of indirect aggression. With few exceptions, the women in the sexy condition behaved badly, aggressing against a woman whose only indiscretion was to be dressed in a sexually evocative manner. They also behaved more poorly with a friend than with a stranger. In Study 2, we attempted to verify whether the sexy confederate was perceived as a sexual rival. Consistent with this idea, we hypothesized that women would not want to introduce her to their boyfriend or allow him to spend time with her. We also hypothesized that they would not let their
  • 26. partner spend time with the sexy-fat confederate because her sexy clothing would likely be perceived as a sign of sexual availability. Manipulating the appearance of the sexy confederate to by making her appear overweight but still sexually provocative, we found strong support for this sexual rival hypothesis (effect size; partial Z2 5 .44). We also asked parti- cipants how likely they would be friends with her, hypothesizing that they would be less inclined if she was dressed provocatively. Results strongly sup- ported this hypothesis (effect size; partial Z2 5 .43). Taken together, the results of this study provide support for the idea that women do engage in intrasexual competition through the use of indirect aggression. ACKNOWLEDGMENTS We thank Heather Brittain, Eric Duku, Lisa Elliot, Amanda Krygsman and Jessie Miller for their help with this research. We also thank Charles Cunningham, Wendy Troop-Gordon, Daniel Quigley, Steven Arnocky, Khrista Boylan, and Louis Schmidt for their helpful comments regarding this manuscript. Funding support came from Canadian Institutes of Health Research, Canada Research Chairs program and the Social Sciences and Humanities Research Council of Canada (awarded to Tracy Vaillancourt). REFERENCES Abbey A, Cozzarelli C, McLaughlin K, Harnish J. 1987. The effect
  • 27. of clothing and dyad sex composition on perceptions of sexual intent: Do women evaluate these cues differently. J Appl Soc Psychol 17:108–126. DOI: 10.1111/j.1559- 1816.1987.tb00304.x. Archer J. 2009. Does sexual selection explain human sex differences in aggression? Behav Brain Sci 32:249–311. DOI: 10.1017/ S0140525X09990951. Arnocky S, Sunderani S, Miller JL, Vaillancourt T. (2011). Jealousy mediates the relationship between attractiveness and females’ indirect aggression. Personal Relationships. Barber N. 1995. The evolutionary psychology of physical attractive- ness: Sexual selection and human morphology. Ethol Sociobiol 16:395–424. DOI: 10.1016/0162-3095(95)00068-2. Baumeister RF, Twenge JM. 2002. Cultural suppression of female sexuality. Rev Gen Psychol 6:166–203. DOI: 10.1037//1089- 2680.6.2.166. Benenson JF. 2009. Dominating versus eliminating the
  • 28. competition: Sex differences in human intrasexual competition. Behav Brain Sci 32:268–269. DOI: 10.1017/S0140525X0999046X. Benenson JF, Markovits H, Thompson ME, Wrangham RW. 2011. Under threat of social exclusion, females exclude more than males. Psychol Sci 22:538–544. DOI: 10.1177/0956797611402511. Bjorkqvist K. 1994. Sex differences in physical, verbal and indirect aggression: A review of recent research. Sex Roles 30:177–188. Bleske AL, Shackelford TK. 2001. Poaching, promiscuity, and deceit: Combating mating rivalry in same- sex friendships. Personal Relationships 8:407–424. DOI: 10.1111/j.1475- 6811.2001.tb00048.x. Buss DM. 1988. The evolution of human intrasexual competition: Tactics of mate attraction. J Pers Soc Psychol 54:616–628. DOI: 10.1037/0022-3514.54.4.616. Buss DM. 1989. Sex differences in human mate preferences:
  • 29. Evolutionary hypotheses tested in 37 cultures. Behav Brain Sci 12:1–49. DOI: 0.1017/S0140525X00023992. Buss DM, Dedden LA. 1990. Derogation of competitors. J Soc Pers Relat 7:395–422. DOI: 10.1177/0265407590073006. Buss DM, Schmitt DP. 1993. Sexual strategies theory: An evolu- tionary perspective on human mating. Psychol Rev 100:204– 232. DOI: 10.1037/0033-295X.100.2.204. 576 Vaillancourt and Sharma Aggr. Behav. Buss DM, Shackelford TK, Choe J, Buunk BP, Dijkstra P. 2000. Distress about mating rivals. Pers Relat 7:235–243. DOI: 10.1111/j.1475-6811.2000.tb00014.x. Campbell A. 1995. A few good men: Evolutionary psychology and female adolescent aggression. Ethol Sociobiol 16:99–123. DOI: 10.1016/0162-3095(94)00072-F.
  • 30. Campbell A. 1999. Staying alive: Evolution, culture and women’s intra-sexual aggression. Behav Brain Sci 22:203–252. DOI: 10.1017/S014025X9900181. Campbell A. 2002. A Mind of Her Own: The Evolutionary Psychology of Women. Oxford: Oxford University Press. Card NA, Stucky BD, Sawalani GM, Little TD. 2008. Direct and indirect aggression during childhood and adolescence. A meta- analytic review of gender differences, intercorrelations, and relations to maladjustment. Child Dev 79:1185–2008. DOI: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2008.01184.x. Clutton-Brock TH. 2007. Sexual selection in males and females. Science 318:1882–1885. DOI: 10.1126/science.1133311. Daly M, Wilson M. 1988. Homicide. New York: Aldine de Gruyter. Darwin C. 1871. The Decent of Man and Selection in Relation to Sex. London: John Murray. Dunbar RIM, Duncan NDC, Marriott A. 1997. Human controver-
  • 31. sial behavior. Hum Nat 8:231–246. DOI: 10.1007/BF02912493. Durante KM, Li NP, Haselton MG. 2008. Changes in women’s choice of dress across the ovulatory cycle: Naturalistic and laboratory task-based evidence. Pers Soc Psychol Bull 34:1451–1460. DOI: 10.1177/0146167208323103. Ekman P, Friesen WV. 2003. Unmasking the Face: A Guide to Recognizing Emotions From Facial Expressions. Cambridge: Marlor Books. Ekman P, Friesen WV, Hager JC. 2002. The Facial Action Coding System. London: Research Nexus. Fisher ML. 2004. Female intrasexual competition decreases female facial attractiveness. Proc R Soc Lond Biol Lett 271:283–285. DOI: 10.1098/rsbl.2004.0160. Fisher M, Cox A. 2009. The influence of female attractiveness on competitor derogation. J Evol Psychol 7:141–155. DOI: 10.1098/ rsbl.2004.0160.
  • 32. Gallup AC, Wilson DS. 2009. Body mass index (BMI) and peer aggression in adolescent females: An evolutionary perspective. J Soc Evol Cult Psychol 3:356–371. Retrieved from http://137. 140.1.71/jsec/ Geary DC. 2010. Male, Female: The Evolution of Human Sex Differences, 2nd edition. Washington, DC: American Psycholo- gical Association. Grammer K, Thornhill R. 1994. Human (homo sapiens) facial attractiveness and sexual selection: The role of symmetry and averageness. J Comp Psychol 108:233–242. DOI: 10.1037/0735- 7036.108.3.233. Griskevicius V, Tybur JM, Gangestad SW, Perea EF, Shapiro JR, Kenrick DT. 2009. Aggress to impress: Hostility as an evolved context-dependent strategy. J Pers Soc Psychol 96:980–994. DOI: 10.1037/a0013907. Hornstein GA. 1985. Intimacy in conversational style as a function of the degree of closeness between members of a dyad. J Pers Soc
  • 33. Psychol 49:671–681. DOI: 10.1037/0022-3514.49.3.671. Kenrick DT, Sadalla EK, Groth G, Trost MR. 1990. Evolution, traits, and the stages of human courtship: Qualifying the parental investment model. J Pers 58:97–116. DOI: 10.1111/ j.1467-6494.1990.tb00909.x. Kenrick DT, Neuberg SL, Zierk K, Krones J. 1994. Evolution and social cognition: Contrast effects as a function of sex, dominance, and physical attractiveness. Pers Soc Psychol Bull 20:210–217. DOI: 10.1177/0146167294202008. Leenaars LS, Dane AV, Marini ZA. 2008. Evolutionary perspective on indirect victimization in adolescence: the role of attractiveness, dating and sexual behavior. Aggr Behav 34:404–415. DOI: 10.1002/ab.20252. Schmitt DP, Buss DM. 1996. Strategic self-enhancement and competitor derogation: Sex and context effects on the perceived effectiveness of mate attraction tactics. J Pers Soc Psychol 70:1185–1204. DOI: 10.1037/0022-3514.70.6.1185.
  • 34. Singh D. 1993. Adaptive significance of female attractiveness: Role of waist-to-hip ratio. J Pers Soc Psychol 65:293–307. DOI: 10.1037/0022-3514.65.2.293. Singh D. 1994. Is thin really beautiful and good? Relationship between physical attractiveness and waist-to-hip ratio. Pers Individ Diff 16:123–132. Singh D, Young RK. 1995. Body weight, waist-to-hip ratio, breasts, and hips: Role in judgments of female attractiveness and desirability for relationships. Ethol Sociobiol 16:483–507. DOI: 10.1016/0162-3095(95)00074-7. Symons D. 1979. The Evolution of Human Sexuality. New York: Oxford University Press. Vaillancourt T. 2005. Indirect aggression among human: Social construct or evolutionary adaptation? In Tremblay RE, Hartup WW, Archer J (editors). Developmental Origins of Aggression. New York: Guilford, pp 158–177. Walters S, Crawford C. 1994. The importance of mate attraction
  • 35. for intrasexual competition in men and women. Ethol Sociobiol 15:5–30. DOI: 10.1016/0162-3095(94)90025-6. Wilson M, Daly M. 1985. Competitiveness, risk taking and violence: The young male syndrome. Ethol Sociobiol 6:59–73. DOI: 10.1016/0162-3095(85)90041-X. 577Intrasexual Competition Among Women Aggr. Behav.