SlideShare a Scribd company logo
1 of 420
1 of 3
FIN 3302 – Exam I Review Fall 2017
Chapter 1
Chapter 5
-annual periods
Chapter 6
2 of 3
Sample Questions
1) Which of the following goals of the firm are synonymous
(equivalent) to the maximization of
shareholder wealth?
A) profit maximization
B) risk minimization
C) maximization of the total market value of the firm's common
stock
D) none of the above
2) You inherit $300,000 from your parents and want to use the
money to supplement your
retirement. You receive the money on your 65th birthday, the
day you retire. You want to
withdraw equal amounts at the end of each of the next 20 years.
What constant amount can you
withdraw each year and have nothing remaining at the end of 20
years if you are earning 7%
interest per year?
A) $15,000
B) $28,318
C) $33,574
D) $39,113
3) The risk-free rate of interest is 4% and the market risk
premium is 9%. Howard Corporation
has a beta of 2.0, and last year generated a return of 16% with a
standard deviation of returns of
27%. The required return on Howard Corporation stock is
A) 36%.
B) 34%.
C) 26%.
D) 22%.
4) Today is your 21st birthday and your bank account balance is
$25,000. Your account is
earning 6.5% interest compounded monthly. How much will be
in the account on your 50th
birthday?
A) $159,795
B) $162,183
C) $163,823
D) $164,631
5) All of the following statements about agency problems are
true except:
A) Agency problems interfere with the goal of maximizing
shareholder value.
B) Agency costs are paid by the managers who do not act in the
shareholders' best
interest.
C) Agency problems result from the separation of management
and the ownership of a
firm.
D) The root cause of agency problems is conflicts of interest.
3 of 3
6) Which of the following conclusions would be true if you earn
a higher rate of return on your
investments?
A) The greater the present value would be for any lump sum you
would receive in the
future.
B) The lower the present value would be for any lump sum you
would receive in the
future.
C) Your rate of return would not have any effect on the present
value of any sum to be
received in the future.
D) The greater the present value would be for any annuity you
would receive in the
future.
7) Investment A has an expected return of 14% with a standard
deviation of 4%, while
investment B has an expected return of 20% with a standard
deviation of 9%. Therefore,
A) a risk averse investor will definitely select investment A
because the standard
deviation is lower.
B) a rational investor will pick investment B because the return
adjusted for risk (20% -
9%) is higher than the return adjusted for risk for investment A
($14% - 4%).
C) it is irrational for a risk-averse investor to select investment
B because its standard
deviation is more than twice as big as investment A's, but the
return is not twice as big.
D) rational investors could pick either A or B, depending on
their level of risk aversion.
8) Joe purchased 800 shares of Robotics Stock at $3 per share
on 1/1/15. He sold the shares on
12/31/15 for $3.45. Robotics stock has a beta of 1.9, the risk-
free rate of return is 4%, and the
market risk premium is 9%. Joe's holding period return is:
A) 15.0%.
B) 16.5%.
C) 17.6%.
D) 21.1%.
Key
1. C
2. B
3. D
4. C
5. B
6. B
7. D
8. A
PEDIATRIC NURSING/September-October 2017/Vol. 43/No. 5
213
I
n August, our national news was
filled with horrifying images of
racism on display. If racism
weren’t such a serious issue, seeing
people marching with Tiki torches
might have almost seemed comical.
The Nazi flags were chilling. It was
quite the deadly show of visual, ver-
bal, physical, and emotional hatred
that I never thought I would ever see
again in my country.
Yet with this event and similar
ones that followed, we have learned
how deeply racism is imbedded in
American culture. Following the
rebuke from the United Nations on
human rights (Gearan & Wang,
2017). Malik (2017) pointed out the
following:
It took only eight months to go
from a nation that voted for a
black president two terms in a row
to one that is suffering from race
riots and killings, with officials
having to send troops out on to the
streets and declare a state of emer-
gency. The speed with which it
happened is the clue that it was, in
fact, happening all along, unseen.
And the fact that it was lying in
wait is an indicator of how little
racial equality is prized in the
United States’ DNA.
Malik (2017) explains why ostensi-
bly developed countries, once faced
with adversity, a vacuum of authority,
or questionable leadership, tend to
fall apart along the lines of race. In
stable times of prosperity and rational
leadership, “the promotion and en -
shrining of the rights of the more vul-
nerable is cosmetic at best, under-
mined and papered over at worst. The
foundations were there and contin-
ued to be laid even during Obama’s
leadership” (Malik, 2017).
Such a sorry state of affairs. How
does racism develop, what is its
impact, and what can we do about it?
The Development
Of Racism
Racism, a developed set of atti-
tudes, includes antagonism based on
the supposed superiority of one group
or on the supposed inferiority of
another group based solely on skin
color or race (Beswick, 1990). No
human being is born with racist, sex-
ist, and other oppressive attitudes.
Would children even notice differ-
ences if no one said anything about
them? Yes, they would, for the fol-
lowing reasons (Rollins & Mahan,
2010, pp. 70-71):
• Early on, children notice differ-
ences and mentally organize these
observations into categories. This
is how young children make sense
of their ever-expanding world.
• Attitudes about “us and them” are
learned and reinforced in the
home, school, and church, and
through the media.
• By 3 years of age, children have
learned to categorize people into
“good or bad” based on superficial
traits, such as race or gender.
• Children 2 years of age or younger
learn names of colors, then begin
to apply these names to skin color.
• By 3 years of age or even earlier,
children can show signs of being
influenced by what they see and
hear around them. They may even
pick up and exhibit “pre-preju-
dice” toward others based on race
or disability.
• Children 4 and 5 years of age may
use racial reasons for refusing to
interact with others who are dif-
ferent from themselves; they may
act uncomfortable around or even
reject people with disabilities.
• By the time children enter ele-
mentary school, they may have
developed prejudices. Stereotypes
remain until personal experience
or someone attempts to correct
them.
Savard and Aragon (1989) tell us
that parents are the earliest and most
powerful source of racial attitudes
(positive or negative); peers are a close
second. Open-mindedness increases
with age.
The Impact of Racism
On Health
Gee, Walsemann, and Brondolo
(2012) note studies that indicate racism
may influence health inequities.
Growing from infancy into old age,
individuals encounter social institu-
tions that may create new exposures to
racial bias. Gee and colleagues (2012)
view racism and health inequities from
a life course perspective. They found
that repeated exposure to moderate
racial discrimination can cause illness
after a time. Health can also be affected
by social systems, such as education,
the criminal justice system, and the
labor market. Racism leads to housing
and school segregation, limiting a per-
son’s social network, and eventually,
their employment opportunities and
health. Over a lifetime, an individual
who is subjected to racism has longer
periods of unemployment or under-
employment, incarceration, and/or
illness. Compared to someone who
has not experienced racism, someone
who has may have a shorter career
and retirement period, and eventual-
ly, a shorter life expectancy.
Racism also has an impact on the
health of those who migrate to a new
country. Although newly arrived
immigrants are reported to have bet-
ter health than non-immigrants in
North America, their health declines
The Depth of Racism in the U.S.:
What It Means for Children
Judy A. Rollins, PhD, RN
From the Editor
214
PEDIATRIC NURSING/September-October 2017/Vol. 43/No. 5
with increased length of stay. First
attributed to the process of adapting
to a new culture (sedentary North
American lifestyle and increased
intake of fatty food), research now
indicates decline may be due to
racism (Na, 2012).
Ausdale and Feagin (2001) note
that early childhood is a crucial sensi-
tive period when stressors such as
racial discrimination may have an
impact on an individual’s long-term
well-being, affecting brain develop-
ment and the formation of neutral
connections between different regions.
The brain and other parts of the body
do not forget when bad things hap-
pen in early life (Shonkoff, in Kuehn,
2014). In later years, children exposed
to racial discrimination may perceive
their own ethnic group negatively,
become self-conscious, and develop
low self-esteem and symptoms of
depression (Na, 2012).
Implications for Nursing
Research suggests efforts nurses
can take to help limit the effects of
discrimination. Mossakowski (2003)
found that the more strongly people
identified with their own ethnic
group, the less likely they were to dis-
play symptoms of depression. This
study showed that a stronger sense of
ethnic identify meant having a sense
of ethnic pride, being involved in eth-
nic or cultural practices, and having
knowledge about and commitment to
the ethnic group. She concluded that
ethnic identity not only directly pro-
tects individuals from discrimination,
but also buffers the stress of discrimi-
nation on mental health.
Nurses can support children’s eth-
nic identify by learning as much as
possible about the cultural back-
grounds of the populations served by
their practice that are different from
their own. They can encourage chil-
dren’s pride and self-esteem through
an eagerness and curiosity to learn
about children’s cultural heritage from
the children themselves. Healthcare
facilities can use art and design to cel-
ebrate the various cultures served to
provide a sense of welcome and pride
for those who receive treatment there.
Nurses should keep apprised of the
community’s political, social, racial,
and other related issues that could
have an impact on children and teens
in their practice. Talking to children
about discrimination is important for
their health and development, and
nurses should not avoid discussing the
topic. Supporting research-based cur-
ricula for children, such as Teaching
Tolerance, can also be encouraged.
Conducting parenting classes could
prove helpful. Research findings indi-
cate that parents’ responses to their
own experiences of racial discrimina-
tion can influence their parenting
behaviors (Sanders-Phillips, Settles-
Reaves, Walker, & Brownlow, 2009).
Parents who have experienced greater
racial discrimination may become less
sensitive to their children’s needs and
less able to display affection, fail to
prepare them for how to cope with
discrimination, and use harsh disci-
pline. At the same time, research tells
us that more nurturing and involved
parenting can weaken the adverse
outcomes for youth associated with
their own experience of discrimina-
tion (Gibbons et al., 2010).
We must never forget that whether
it is visible or not, hate can be lurking
just below the surface. As pediatric
nurses, we can make a difference to
diminishing racism and discrimina-
tion in our daily interaction with
patients and their families.
References
Ausdale, D.V., & Feagin, J.R. (2001). The
first R: How children learn race and
racism. Lanham, MD: Rowman and
Littlefield Publishers.
Beswick, R. (1990). Racism in America’s
schools. ERIC Digest Series, EA 49.
Retrieved from https://www.ericdigests.
org/pre-9215/racism.htm
Gearan, A., & Wang, A (2017, August 16) In
veiled criticism of Trump, U.N. chief
says racism is ‘poisoning our societies.’
The Washington Post. Retrieved from
https://www.washingtonpost.com/world/
national-security/in-veiled-criticism-of-
trump-un-chief-says-racism-is-poison-
ing-our-societies/2017/ 08/ 16/ ddd03984-
8 2 a 5 - 11 e 7 - a b 2 7 - 1 a 2 1 a 8 e 0 0 6 a b _
story.html?utm_term=.962893eebdfe
Gee, G.C., Walsemann, K.M., & Brondolo, E.
(2012). A life course perspective on how
racism may be related to health inequal-
ities. American Journal of Public Health,
102, 967-974.
Gibbons, F.X., Etcheverry, P.E., Stock, M.L.,
Gerrard, M., Weng, C., Kiviniemi, M., &
O’Hara, R.E. (2010). Exploring the link
between racial discrimination and sub-
stance use: What mediates? What
buffers? Journal of Personality and
Social Psychology, 99(5), 785-801.
Kuehn, B. (2014). AAP: Toxic stress threat-
ens kids’ long-term health. JAMA,
312(6), 585-586.
Malik, N. (2017, August 25). After a UN warn-
ing over racism, America’s self-image
begins to crack. The Guardian. Retrieved
from https://www.theguardian.com/com-
mentisfree/2017/aug/25/un-warns-us-
racism-but-trump-era-bigotry-not-blip-
charlottesville
Mossakowski, K. (2003). Coping with per-
ceived discrimination: Does ethnic iden-
tity protect mental health? Journal of
Health and Social Behavior, 44, 318-
331.
Na, L. (2012). Children and racism: The long-
term impact. Retrieved from http://
www.aboutkidshealth.ca/En/News/
NewsAndFeatures/Pages/children-
racism-long-term-impacts.aspx
Rollins, J., & Mahan, C. (2010). From artist to
artist in residence: Preparing artists to
work in pediatric healthcare settings.
Washington, DC: Rollins & Associates.
Sanders-Phillips, K., Settles-Reaves, B.,
Walker, D., & Brownlow, J. (2009).
Social inequality and racial discrimina-
tion: Risk factors for health disparities in
children of color. Pediatrics, 124(3),
S176-S186.
Savard, W.G., & Aragon, A. (1989). Racial
Justice Survey Final Report. Portland,
OR: Northwest Regional Educational
Laboratory.
Copyright of Pediatric Nursing is the property of Jannetti
Publications, Inc. and its content
may not be copied or emailed to multiple sites or posted to a
listserv without the copyright
holder's express written permission. However, users may print,
download, or email articles for
individual use.
Racism’s Uprooting
White Racial Jeopardy and Democracy
To show what is beneficial,
what is obligatory, what
is good — that is the task of
education. Education concerns
itself w ith the motives
for effective action. For no
action is ever carried out
in the absence of motives
capable of supplying the
indispensable amount
of energy for its execution.
— Simone Weil, The Need for Roots
14
5
B
LA
C
K
R
E
N
A
IS
S
A
N
C
E
N
O
IR
E
To varying degrees, the
approaches used to teach
anti-racism fall short
of their intended goals,
because of the tacit belief
that non-Whites alone
are harmed by racism,
while Whites reap only
its benefits.
This perspective is at odds with, and
unable to fully capture, many aspects
o f our national history and o f our
current reality. It cannot, for example,
account for the race-induced political
behavior o f poor and working-class
Whites. For several decades now,
that behavior has shown itself to be
pointedly self-endangering, culminating
with the election o f Donald J. Trump
to succeed President Barack Obama.
The anguished spectacle o f poor and
working-class Whites vacating their
economic and medical (self-)interest
in order to conform to race-induced
opposition to the pejoratively renamed
“Obamacare,” while pleading for (the
continuance of) Affordable Care, is
revealing. It indicates that for many,
racism has destroyed the ability to
define, assert, or defend their own
survival interests. Despite numerous
similar inconsistencies, the traditional
designation o f victims and beneficiaries
remains unchanged, perhaps, because
it is consistent with racism’s definition
o f the powerless and the powerful.
Insights on the psychology of oppression
developed by the twentieth century
French philosopher Simone Weil
offer a way out o f this impasse. Weil’s
analysis provides an urgently needed
framework for re-examining racism,
one that allows for a more complex
understanding o f the ways in which
people with W hite racial privilege are
endangered by that privilege.
In the collection o f essays translated
and published posthumously as
The N eed fo r Roots: Prelude to a
Declaration o f Duties towards M ankind,
Weil declares that “To be rooted is
perhaps the most im portant and least
recognized need o f the hum an soul”
(Weil 1978, 41). After describing the
familial and social structures through
which individuals are “rooted” so
as to achieve moral, intellectual, and
spiritual stability, Weil examines factors
contributing to its opposite, what she
calls “uprootedness.” Because she is
writing during the Second World War,
Weil focuses on contemporary causes
o f uprootedness, such as military
conquest and colonialism. W hile she
does not mention racism as a cause o f
uprootedness, numerous studies have
confirmed racisms uprooting effects,
most notably the doll studies developed
by Kenneth Clark and Mamie Phipps
Clark that were used in the Brown
v. Board o f Education case to end
state-sponsored school segregation in
the 1950s.' Contrary to expectations of
preference based on “similarity bias,”
the Black children in the Clark study
showed a distinct preference for and
favorable view o f W hite dolls and a
marked rejection o f the Black dolls with
which they nevertheless identified.
I will return to this point later in the
essay, as I examine the “narrative
relationships” deployed to destroy
self-image and self-esteem, and — in
the process — generate a negative
“similarity bias.”
Suffice it to say, Weil’s description o f
uprootedness as a “self-propagating”
social disorder illuminates one o f the
least understood, least examined aspects
o f the ideology o f race — its injurious
psychological, and perhaps neural,
impact on individuals and groups
positioned to internalize, act out, and
ostensibly, benefit from its suggestions.
As she observes, “Uprootedness is
by far the most dangerous malady to
which hum an societies are exposed,
for it is a self-propagating one__
Whoever is uprooted himself uproots
others. Whoever is rooted himself
doesn’t uproot others” (Weil 1978, 45).
Weil’s analysis makes clear that racism’s
uprooting o f Blacks and other racial
minorities is inevitably the secondary
effect o f racism’s uprooting o f Whites,
a latent source o fW h ite racial jeopardy.
That is to say, the ideology o f race must
first have destroyed or dismantled
certain vital hum an capacities within
the W hite population, as an absolute
pre-condition for the sustained
violence o f racism. O f these capacities,
perhaps the most im portant is
the cognitive capacity to perceive
non-W hite people’s equal humanity.
Examining racism through the lenses
provided by Weil’s analysis, new
questions emerge. Exactly how are
W hite people “uprooted” by racism?
How is uprooting manifested or
displayed? W hat are its psychological
contours? And given the new
developments in neuroscience research,
can we go beyond a psychological
mapping to identify the neural correlates
o f racism? M ost importantly, given
the long history during which it has
remained undiagnosed and untreated,
what are the long-term consequences
o f racisms uprooting o f Whites?
RE-EXAMINING RACISM’S
DIVIDE
In the following discussion, I attem pt
to answer these questions by looking
first at the pitfalls o f mainstream
understandings and approaches to
anti-racism and by applying recent
developments in neuroscience research
to expose racism and W hite privilege
as dangers to W hites and, ultimately,
to u.s. democracy. I argue that a
more efficacious design of anti-racist
education can only come from a
comprehensive understanding o f the
jeopardy we all face from racism’s
uprooting effects. For W hites, racism
and W hite privilege threaten those
competencies that enable and sustain
healthy social interactions, professional
performance, and democratic praxis in
a multiracial society, namely, effective
moral reasoning, moral decision-making,
historical thinking, social literacy,
and their prerequisites — emotional
engagement and narrative knowledge.
W ith few exceptions, scholarly analyses
o f racism are shaped by binary
assumptions about its impact: Whites
reap the benefit o f W hite privilege,
while Native Americans, African
Americans, Latinos, Asians, Muslims2,
and others are harmed by it. W hen
injury to W hites is mentioned, it is
generally no more significant than the
harm o f a guilty conscience.
In the best-seller Why Are A ll the Black
Kids Sitting Together in the Cafeteria?
A n d Other Conversations About Race,
psychologist Beverly Tatum recounts
an exchange with a student that
underscores the shortcomings o f most
current approaches to anti-racism.
In examining “The Cost o f Racism,”
Tatum describes a W hite male student
who, after completing her psychology
o f racism course, confessed that he
“now understood in a way he never had
before just how advantaged he was”
(Tatum 1997,13) because o f racism.
The student also shared that “he didn’t
think he would do anything to try
to change the situation. After all, the
system was working in his favor”
(Tatum 1997,13).
After noting that this response was an
anomaly, Tatum goes on to say that
it, nevertheless, raised two im portant
questions: “W hy should Whites who
are advantaged by racism want to end
that system o f advantage? [And] W hat
are the costs o f that system to them?”
(Tatum 1997,13). Tatum answers first
with a financial accounting o f what
racism costs the u.s. economy, citing
a 1989 article, “Race and Money,”
that appeared in Money magazine.
After furnishing anecdotal evidence
o f emotional loss suffered by W hite
women and men, Tatum asserts:
“W hite people are paying a significant
price for the system o f advantage.
The cost is not as high for W hites as
it is for people o f color, but a price
is being paid” (Tatum 1997,14).
Since I first read Tatum’s account of
her W hite male student’s response and
o f the questions it prom pted, I have
remained fascinated by both. I am
fascinated by the student’s response
because, it supplies language for what I
believe is a widely held view: that when
W hite people commit to anti-racism,
they are acting against their own rational
self-interest or are doing so as an
expression o f goodwill toward Blacks,
Latinos, Native Americans, Muslims,
and others who are disadvantaged by
race and racism. The suggestion that
racism is a threat to Whites, that they
too are in danger o f being uprooted by
it, is still a novel one.
I am fascinated by Tatum’s response
because she never poses what I view
as the more urgent questions: why
did her student not recognize what the
system was costing him? And how
might she and other anti-racist educators
be preventing this discovery in their
legitimate focus on defining racism as a
system o f advantage and disadvantage?
W hile she does not address these
questions, Tatum’s analysis does illustrate
just how the myth o f W hite people’s
imm unity to racism’s uprooting
is perpetuated. In discussing how
institutional racism hurt the u.s.
economy and, thereby, W hite Americans,
she mentions only those experiences
o f economic harm that people
disadvantaged by race experience,
namely, “real estate equity lost through
housing discrimination, or the tax
revenue lost in underemployed
communities o f color, or the high cost
o f warehousing hum an talent in prison”
(Tatum 1997,14). Here, Tatum 14
7
B
LA
C
K
R
E
N
A
IS
S
A
N
C
E
N
O
IR
E
suggests that the economic, social, and
psychological injuries sustained by
communities o f color am ount to a cost
to the economy as a whole, which is
the only measure o f (economic)
cost to Whites. To the extent that there
is only a collective — and not an
individualized — economic cost for
Whites, it must mean that relinquishing
W hite privilege or (with more difficulty)
overcoming racism can only be a
gesture of goodwill or economic
patriotism, not one o f self-preservation
or self-interest on the part o f W hite
individuals.
Peggy M cIntosh’s widely respected
essay, “W hite Privilege: Unpacking the
Invisible Knapsack” (1989), provides
further insight into the mythology of
W hite people’s im m unity to racism’s
uprooting effects. The essay opens with
an epigraph in which McIntosh
confesses, “I was taught to see racism
only in individual acts o f meanness,
not in invisible systems conferring
dominance on my group.” The catalogue
that follows is McIntosh’s attempt
to delineate this “invisible system.”
In listing what she calls the “Daily
Effects o f W hite Privilege,” McIntosh
does n ot include a single injury that
might accompany the many “privileges”
she identifies. In fact, the list displays
a peculiar lack o f reflection on, and
lack o f interest in, the harmful effects
o f W hite privilege. McIntosh is able
to avoid this discovery by a steadfast
refusal to expose or identify the hum an
agents implicated in the privileges she
enumerates. Putting the spotlight on
these persons, two details stand out.
First, hatred and/or meanness cannot
account for the spectrum of attitudes
and actions she describes. Second,
the persons dispensing or otherwise
responsible for the privileges she
enjoys have, to varying degrees, been
(psychologically and morally) uprooted.
Among the mundane examples o f
W hite racial privilege McIntosh pulls
out o f her invisible knapsack is the
following: “I can go shopping alone
most o f the time, pretty well assured
that I will not be followed or harassed”
(#4).3 The store clerk who does not
follow McIntosh but who peeps at me
through the fitting room door is
not mean, does not hate me. Disabling
narrative relationships — with
non-W hite characters in television,
cinematic, historical, and other
narratives — have triggered emotions
o f suspicion and impaired her ability
to see me. Uprooted by racism, what
she sees instead is a Black-female-and-
therefore-shoplifter. Favorable narrative
relationships with W hite characters
have also blunted her ability to discern
crime in White-face. So while she
follows me around in a harassing
performance o f helpfulness, the real
shoplifter menaces the merchandise.
Read carefully, one discovers that
the privileges in McIntosh’s invisible
knapsack have been stacked by
“invisible” people psychologically and
morally disabled by racism’s
uprooting, people whose ability to
accurately interpret human behaviors
and motives have likewise been
impaired. Her composition o f such
a comprehensive listing o f assumed
privileges can only mean that McIntosh
knows the stackers, has seen or has
been one o f them. If she had seriously
analyzed the type o f impairment their
actions or attitudes reflect, privilege
#15 — and perhaps the entire essay —
would have been framed differently.
Instead o f simply asserting, "I do not
have to educate my children to be
aware o f systemic racism for their own
daily physical protection,”4 McIntosh
would have added, “But I do have to
educate my children to be aware o f the
daily threat racism and W hite privilege
pose to their psychological, intellectual,
and moral health.”
Two defects weaken McIntosh’s
anti-racist methodology: first, not
identifying the people responsible for
the racial privileges she enumerates,
and second, not analyzing the full
range of psychological dispositions
involved in its operation. These defects
also appear in the structural racism
model championed by legal scholar
John A. Powell, former director o f the
Kirwan Institute for the Study o f Race
and Ethnicity at Ohio State University.
In “Structural Racism: Building
upon the Insights o f John Calmore,”
Powell asserts that “Racism need not
be either intentional or individualist.
Institutional practices and cultural
patterns can perpetuate racial inequity
w ithout relying on racist actors” (795).
Contrasting institutional and structural
racism, Powell notes:
The institutional racism
framework reflects a broader
recognition o f the forms
through which racialized
power is deployed, dispersed,
and entrenched. However,
while illustrating the ways in
which racism is often
non-individualist and
non-intentionalist, this
framework focuses too
heavily on intra-institutional
dynamics, and thus fails to
account for the ways in which
the joint operations o f social
institutions produce critical
racialized outcomes. [...]
Structural racism shifts
our attention from the
single, intra-institutional
setting to inter-institutional
arrangements and
interactions. (795-796)
As an intersectional approach,
Powell’s structural racism model
provides a means o f both identifying
and countering the structures of
compounding jeopardy that constrict
opportunities for poor African
American, Latino, Native American,
and other non-W hite communities.
However, in simultaneously reducing
the m ultitude o f racist dispositions
to the single stereotypical mindset o f
“bad actors” and substituting a focus on
residual effects o f presumably abandoned
mechanics, the structural racism
model moves the discourse from one
shaky leg to another.
The notion that structures can maintain
a racist disposition and continue to
produce racist outcomes w ithout the
involvement o f racist hum an agents
or that institutions do not reflect the
vision or mindset o f the people by
whom they are governed is simply not
credible. Recent events involving the
three branches of our own democratic
government are instructive. O n the
one hand, the machinations o f the
executive branch demonstrate that if
and when operated by individuals with
an autocratic vision (or personality)
democratic structures will not continue
to support democratic practices
or policies, or produce democratic
outcomes. O n the other hand, recent
rulings from the federal bench regarding
the Trump administrations Muslim
ban indicate that only when structures
are governed by human agents
committed to a democratic vision can
democratic outcomes be assured.
The same is true o f racism. As such,
an approach that combines the
critique o f structural racism with an
examination o f hum an agents
involved in its continuance would
be anchored by two legs and ensure
analytical symmetry.
The discomfort underlying the refusal
to expose hum an beings involved in
the practice o f racism stems from an
ahistorical and simplistic equation
o f racism with hatred or meanness,
as McIntosh admits to having been
taught. Recalling Tatums W hite male
student, however, it is clear that his
resolution to preserve racism and,
inevitably, to become a self-consciously
racist agent was undertaken, one might
say, “with malice toward none.”
Nevertheless, defining racism solely
as a system o f advantage to Whites
creates several significant impediments
to anti-racist education. First, this
definition supports a senseless and
binary positioning of W hite “donors”
and non-W hite “debtors,” with
opposing stakes.
Second, it simultaneously and
paradoxically entreats W hite “donors”
to relinquish certain modes o f racism
as a benefit to others and not on behalf
o f their own psychological, intellectual,
and ethical growth or liberation.
Third, in so doing, it replicates the
structure o f international relations
in the post-colonial era where formerly
colonized nations are defined as
debtors and those former imperial
powers enriched by the colonial
enterprise are defined as donors.
Fourth, it prevents, or else discourages,
W hite people from acknowledging
their own uprootedness, since doing
so carries an automatic indictm ent for
moral failure.
Fifth, it prevents people with race-based
privilege from discovering how they
too are endangered by structures of
thinking and structures of feeling that
directly and indirectly endorse
hierarchical distributions o f hum an
worth and rights.
Sixth, it fails to reveal the dangers
and dispel the potential envy of
a self-endangering racial privilege.
Seventh, it fails to fully interpret the
significance and value o f the intellectual,
psychological, ethical, spiritual, and
material resourcefulness manifested
in African American, Asian, Latino,
Native American and various non-
Western cultural traditions and histories.
In sum, the view o f racism as solely
or primarily a system o f W hite racial
advantage fails to adequately empower
or motivate W hite, Asian, Native
American, Latino, African American,
and other non-W hite communities
for the collaborative task o f ending
racism. The misconception that W hite
people are not individually harmed
by racism, that it is not a threat to
them, inhibits the mass com m itm ent
and participation needed to effectively
counter racisms uprooting.
14
9
B
LA
C
K
R
EN
A
IS
SA
N
C
E
N
O
IR
E
R S D : R A C I S M S P E C T R U M
D I S O R D E R
A key reason for W hite people’s seeming
lack o f concern for their own racial
jeopardy is the assumption that racism
is a moral defect and that having a
sufficient moral foundation confers
immunity. Numerous historical
examples, however, amply demonstrate
that having an extensive moral
foundation is no defense against racist
indoctrination. Recall that despite
being versed in the moral teachings
o f the New Testament that include
instructions to love your neighbor
as yourself and to treat others as you
would like to be treated, the earliest
European arrivants to the Americas
were unable to resist racist conscription.
In tracing the history o f legislative
violence in seventeenth-century
Virginia from which race and racism
ensued, political scientist Joel Olson
notes that “Race as we now know it
did n ot exist when the first colonists
landed on the shores o f the New W orld”
(Olson 2001,165). Racism, Olson
asserts, “was a deliberate, public,
conscious policy o f the Virginia ruling
elite” (Olson 2001,167) invented to
preempt cross-racial economic and
political alliances. In the article titled
“The Democratic Problem o f the W hite
Citizen,” Olson recounts,
Through a series o f acts
from r670 to 1705, the
Virginia assembly made laws
distinguishing African and
Indians from Europeans.
They forbade Africans and
Indians to own Christian
servants and the legal
definition o f “Christian” now
excluded baptized African
and Native Americans...
Through various legislative
measures and social pressures
Virginia elites simultaneously
fastened Africans to a lifetime,
hereditary, degraded status
and created a new group of
relatively privileged people
heretofore unknown in
hum an history. Remarkably,
these measures amassed rich
and poor, planter and servant,
esteemed and lowly into a
single group unified not by
ancestry but by the right to
own property (including
hum an property), the right to
share in the public business,
and a pledge to ensure the
degraded position o f all those
defined as black. (Olson 2001,
166-168)
W hile the legislative acts that
engineered racism and W hite racial
privilege equalized the social status of
all those defined as “W hite,” it did not
equalize the economic and political
interests o f poor W hites and the ruling
(White) elite. That is to say, although
the social identities o f W hite workers
improved, their economic interests
were neither aligned with nor prioritized
by the ruling elite. In acquiescing
to this new regime, therefore, poor
W hites sacrificed their economic and
political self-interest for the mostly
symbolic “wages o f Whiteness.”
In systematically discrediting the
image o f non-Whites and enlisting the
participation o f poor W hites in this
project, the ruling elite launched a
two-pronged psychological assault on
non-Whites and on poor Whites.
By assimilating to this new racialized
self-definition, poor English workers —
and later Irish, Italian, German,
Polish, and other ethnic Europeans
in the u.s. — relinquished their
cultural distinctiveness, class-specific
epistemological standpoint,
and interpretive autonomy.
Their acculturation to the illusion o f
dominance shared with the ruling
elite was the first indication o f racism’s
ability to uproot, to impair intellectual
agency and interpretive competence.
In effect, W hite racial privilege was the
nation’s first designer drug, consuming
vital capacities — intellectual,
psychological, moral — with every
thrilling intake. The “pledge” among
W hites to “ensure the degraded
position” o f all those defined as
non-W hite would find expression and
fulfillment through narrative acts
and the re-configuration o f “empathic
bias.” This would involve scripting
negative narrative relationships toward
non-Whites and positive narrative
relationships toward Whites sufficient
to overshadow the empathic biases
that would otherwise have evolved
from their lived interactions and moral
commitments.
From the outset, therefore, racism was
a psychological, not a moral, disorder
dependent on a conditioned emotional
detachment. W hile this detachment
and resulting disorder can have moral
effects and/or implications, the
disorder is itself psychological with
perhaps identifiable neural correlates.
As a psycho-social spectrum disorder,
racism stems from an involuntary and
symmetrical blindness to one’s own
and to other people’s human worth.
It involves both conditioned emotional
detachment and hyper attachment and
produces disparate forms o f impairment
and disability. From its foundational
measurement o f hum an worth, racism
establishes other “race”-based
measurements o f hum an capacities,
human achievements, hum an potential,
human rights, and hum an responses.
W hile racism’s blindness is typically
assumed to manifest itself in attitudes
and acts o f hatred or meanness, these are
merely points on the racism spectrum.
The socialized blindness that undergirds
racism can manifest in a broad spectrum
o f attitudes and actions, involving
varying degrees o f emotional
detachment or hyper-attachment
towards persons positioned at various
tiers on the hierarchy o f human
worth and entitlement posited by the
ideology o f race.
Attitudes and actions toward persons
positioned at or near the bottom o f
the hierarchy o f hum an worth include,
but are not limited to: suspicion,
fear, unwarranted and frequently
self-endangering mistrust, disinterest,
apathy, a lack o f concern for actual
and potential danger or injury to such
persons, sexual attraction, curiosity,
envy, a desire to help or rescue, a
lack o f self-restraint or gentleness
towards such persons, indifference or
permissiveness about abuse towards
such persons — a spectrum o f
psychological dispositions conditioned
by emotional detachment.
Attitudes and actions toward persons
positioned at or near the top of
the hierarchy, and that constitute
W hite racial privilege, include but are
not limited to: feelings o f comfort,
interestedness, empathy, unwarranted
and frequently self-endangering trust,
feelings o f concern and distress at
real or imagined danger or injury to
such persons, a sense o f the greater
value, greater relevance o f their roles
and contributions — a spectrum of
psychological responses conditioned by
hyper-attachment.
As a psycho-social spectrum disorder,
racism also involves a socialized
blindness to the structures through
which hierarchical distributions o f
advantage, opportunity, immunity, and
material resources are made. As such,
it self-propagates through a concurrent
blindness to its own operation,
an effect with considerable strategic
significance. As Wahneema Lubiano
has observed, it allows W hite people
to see themselves as acting morally
when, according to their own moral
tenets, they are not.5 The tendency to
self-propagate, as Weil emphasizes,
is precisely what makes uprootedness
(through W hite racial privilege)
“the most dangerous malady to which
hum an societies are exposed”
(Weil 1978, 45).
The stark contrast between policy
responses that criminalized the mostly
African American and Latino crack
addicts in the 1980s and 1990s and the
policy responses seeking therapeutic
options for todays mostly W hite
heroin addicts is noteworthy. It attests
to the differing degrees of emotional
attachment to persons situated at
different tiers on the hierarchy of
hum an worth posited by the ideology
o f race. If racism had not created
an emotional detachment toward
non-White people, politicians and
policy-makers would not have been
uprooted by this socialization and
would have experienced feelings of
concern, if not alarm, at the loss o f life
among crack addicts. W ith feelings
and emotion intact, they would have
been able to exercise the type o f moral
reasoning that would have provided
a basis for acquiring “narrative
knowledge” — to be discussed
hereafter — in the quest for appropriate
policy responses. If emotional
detachment had not impaired policy
makers’ ability to formulate effective
moral judgm ent about the addicts
o f the late twentieth century, the
therapeutic solutions now being
pursued would have been proposed
then. Lives would have been saved then
and, with structures already in place,
lives would be saved now.
15
1
B
LA
CK
R
EN
A
IS
SA
N
C
E
NO
IR
E
EXPOSING THE NEUROSCIENCE
OF RACISM
Emotional detachment toward
non-Whites is the foundation — and
catalyst — for a vast structure of
dystopian feeling toward the poor,
prison inmates, the homeless,
immigrants, low-wage workers, people
with intellectual disabilities, and other
groups defined as disposable. The
broad spectrum o f impairment and
disability resulting from the emotional
detachment conditioned by racism
becomes especially significant when
viewed in relation to the work o f the
renowned neuropsychologist Antonio
Damasio on the role o f emotion and
feeling in moral judgment.
In presenting to the public his clinical
findings that “certain aspects o f the
process o f emotion and feeling are
indispensable for rationality” (xiii) and
“that the absence o f emotion and feeling
is no less damaging” than emotional
biases (xii), Damasio mentioned first his
own cultural socialization with regard to
rational decision-making. In Descartes’
Error (1994), Damasio writes,
I had been advised early in
life that sound decisions
came from a cool head, that
emotions and reason did
not mix any more than oil
and water. I had grown up
accustomed to thinking that
the mechanisms o f reason
existed in a separate province
o f the m ind, where emotion
should not be allowed to
intrude, and when I thought
o f the brain behind the
m ind, I envisioned separate
neural systems for reason and
emotion, (xi)
The case that prom pted Damasio to
re-think the role o f emotions and
feeling in effective decision-making
was that o f a patient who “had had
an entirely healthy m ind until a
neurological disease ravaged a specific
sector o f his brain” causing a
“profound defect in decision-making”
(xii). According to Damasio,
The instruments usually
considered necessary and
sufficient for rational behavior
were intact in him. He had
the requisite knowledge,
attention, and memory; his
language was flawless; he
could perform calculations;
he could tackle the logic o f an
abstract problem. There was
only one significant
accompaniment to his
decision-making failure: a
marked alteration o f the
ability to experience feelings.
Flawed reason and impaired
feelings stood out together as the
consequences o f a specific brain
lesion, and this correlation
suggested to me that feeling
was an integral component
o f the machinery o f reason.
Two decades o f clinical and
experimental work with a
large num ber o f neurological
patients have allowed me
to replicate this observation
many times, and to turn a
clue into a testable hypothesis.
(xii) (Emphasis added.)
In the years since its initial publication,
several studies have confirmed
Damasio’s “testable hypothesis.”6
Among other effects, these findings call
into question the preferred formula for
effective decision-making — one that
excludes emotion and feeling — that
has been disseminated and established
as a requisite guideline in almost every
arena o f public, professional, and
personal decision-making from the law
and law enforcement, to business,
international relations, politics,
economics, diplomacy, medicine,
journalism, education, sports, marriage,
and family life.
Given the pervasive influence o f
Descartes’ cogito on Western culture
and its widespread cultural diffusion
as a decision-making protocol, the
neuroscience research confirming the
essential role o f emotion and feeling
in moral decision-making raises new
questions. Has the cogito already
“marked” what Western researchers
view as a healthy/integrated brain?
Can “impaired feelings” be caused by
factors other than disease or physical
injury to the brain? Can socialization,
for example, impair or deactivate
the brain’s emotional circuits?
Can the gender socialization that males
in particular receive about the
incompatibility o f emotion and reason
produce neural effects and a similar
impairment o f feelings as those caused
by injury or disease? Can race or gender
socialization deactivate emotional
systems o f the brain in general or with
regard to specific stimuli? Conversely,
are there deficits in neural processing in
a healthy — non diseased, uninjured —
brain that can be attributed — broadly
speaking — to cultural socialization?
Or, to p ut it succinctly, is socialized
brain injury possible? W hat is the role
o f emotion and feeling in public policy
decision-making? To what extent might
defective public policy decision-making
be attributable to emotional detachment
toward dispossessed and disposable
constituencies? To the extent that
emotion is essential to moral reasoning,
how might emotional detachment
affect historical thinking, social literacy,
and the practice o f democracy, all of
which involve moral reasoning?
O f the many studies inspired by
Damasio’s clinical findings, one o f the
most instructive is a 2001 study by an
interdisciplinary team o f investigators
lead by Harvard professor Joshua
Greene, at the time a doctoral student
in Princeton’s Philosophy department.
In the article “An fMRI Investigation
o f Emotional Engagement in Moral
Judgment,” Greene and his colleagues
describe using functional magnetic
resonance imaging — fMRI — to
observe the neural activity o f test subjects
faced with a variety o f decision-making
scenarios involving “moral-personal
conditions,” “moral-impersonal
conditions,” and “non-moral
conditions.”7 The study focused on two
moral dilemmas representing
moral-impersonal and moral-personal
conditions, respectively. O ne involved a
runaway trolley, the other a footbridge.
As they describe the two dilemmas,
A runaway trolley is headed
for five people who will be
killed if it proceeds on its
present course. The only
way to save them is to hit
a switch that will turn the
trolley onto an alternate set
o f tracks where it will kill
one person instead o f five....
[In] a similar problem, the
footbridge dilem m a... a
trolley threatens to kill five
people. You are standing
next to a large stranger on
a footbridge that spans
the tracks, in between the
oncoming trolley and the five
people. In this scenario, the
only way to save the five
people is to push this stranger
off the bridge, onto the tracks
below. He will die if you do
this, but his body will stop
the trolley from reaching the
others. (2105)
The researchers predicted the absence
o f what they called “emotional
interference” for responses to the
impersonal trolley dilemma, as
measured by both neural activity and
faster response times.
As expected, Greene and his colleagues
found that most respondents said
yes to re-directing the trolley onto
an alternate track in the first
(moral-impersonal) dilemma, while
most respondents said no to pushing
a stranger off the footbridge in the
second (moral-personal) dilemma.
They concluded that,
.. .from a psychological point
o f view, the crucial difference
between the trolley dilemma
and the footbridge dilemma
lies in the latter’s tendency to
engage people’s emotions
in a way the former does not.
The thought o f pushing someone
to his death is, we propose, more
emotionally salient than the
thought o f hitting a switch that
will cause a trolley to produce
similar consequences, and it is
this emotional response that
accounts for people’s tendency
to treat these cases differently.
(2106) [Emphasis added.]
Although not its intended objective,
the Greene study offers a preliminary
basis for speculating about the ways
in which a particular socialization —
whether shaped by race or any
other ideology — might lead to the
type o f emotional detachment that
prevents the neurological engagement
and activity essential for healthy
decision-making about other people’s
lives. This is im portant because
racially-informed responses, misreadings,
behaviors, and actions toward
non-Whites are in some senses an
accumulation o f emotionally detached
(mis)judgments.
Looking closely at the study, one
notices that the “impersonal” character
or framing o f the trolley dilemma
seems to rest upon the social identities
o f the people on the track and the
empathic ability o f particular
respondents, n o t on the dilemma itself.
This can easily be illustrated by
re-framing the dilemma, re-imagining
the unknown person on the alternate
track, and allowing that person to
attain the same hum an distinction as
the stranger on the footbridge whom
test subjects have to touch. W ould test
subjects be as emotionally detached in
reasoning about the runaway trolley
scenario if the person on the alternate
track were an acquaintance — whether
liked or disliked? Would they be as
emotionally disengaged if the five were
five Mexicans crossing the border
illegally and the one were a u.s. border
patrol agent? O r if the “trolley” were a
grenade misfired from a u.s. military
launcher and the five were five Afghan
civilians and the one were a u.s. soldier?
15
3
B
LA
C
K
R
EN
A
IS
SA
N
C
E
NO
IR
E
RE-VIEWING RACISM AS A
THREAT TO DEMOCRACY
My point here is not to suggest that
under these different (narrative) frames
test subjects in the u.s. would make
different judgments and require longer
response times in resolving the trolley
dilemma, although this seems highly
probable. Rather, my point is that to
the extent that the variations suggested
above elicit a more intense emotional
engagement, they also reveal the social
invisibility and/or disposability of
the person on the alternate track and
the resulting detachment with which
test subjects were likely to have
engaged the dilemma. Since social
identities are charged — positively and
negatively — with different emotional
valences, moral judgm ent about
those lives will inevitably be influenced
and perhaps determined by these
pre-set valences. These judgments are
particularly fraught in professional and
public policy deliberations.
Re-examining the results o f the Greene
study, the researchers seem to have
overlooked one o f the most salient
discoveries o f their own research:
the neural similarity displayed between
moral-impersonal and non-moral
decision-making. If emotions are
essential to moral reasoning, the
observed non-involvement o f test
subjects’ emotions in the trolley
dilemma, as measured by neural activity
and faster response times, would suggest
that these decisions are neurologically
defective. Thus, the researchers’ tacit
acceptance o f this defect, their failure
to read these responses as defective, is
perhaps indicative o f persistent cultural
assumptions — and misconceptions —
about the limited utility o f emotions.
Their reference to, and perhaps
preference for, the absence o f “emotional
interference” is revealing.
If the neural and behavioral responses
to the moral-impersonal trolley
dilemma in the Greene study are
(socio-culturally) representative,
they may indicate a significant pattern
o f emotional disengagement in
moral-impersonal decision-making
about socially invisible “strangers,”
the type o f decision-making voters in
a democratic society must routinely
perform. This finding that neural
responses to “moral-impersonal” and
“non-moral” dilemmas involve similar
types o f emotional disengagement is
particularly troubling since professional
and public policy decision-making
tends to occur in situations that resemble
the conditions o f moral-impersonal
decision-making in which the people
at risk on policy-making “trolley tracks”
remain invisible. It should be obvious
that an adult citizen ought n ot to
approach — or (neurologically)
experience — the choice o f whether or
not to fund at-home healthcare
options for people with disabilities
(a moral-impersonal dilemma) in the
same way as deciding whether to
take the 6 p.m. or 7 p.m. flight from
Washington, D C back to Kentucky
or Wisconsin (a non-moral dilemma).
And yet, this appears to be happening
in the Congress on a daily basis.
The Greene’s study and its overlooked
implications provide a basis for
theorizing about the neurological
jeopardies underlying the intellectual
habits that attend W hite racial
privilege and their inevitable impact
on the range o f decision-making
responsibilities accompanying
democratic participation and professional
practice in a multi-racial society.
Most discussions o f racism as a threat
to democracy focus on the cliched
assertion that, as an impediment to
national reconciliation, it prevents
u.s. Americans from achieving “a more
perfect union.” W hile accurate,
this summary view overlooks and
overshadows the more consequential
ways in which racism and W hite racial
privilege jeopardize u.s. democracy
by fostering inept historical thinking,
social illiteracy, defective moral
reasoning, defective (public policy)
decision-making, and self-endangering
voting behaviors. In large measure,
these effects stem from the narrative
relationships that precede and that are
the blueprint for social interactions.
Analyses o f the ways as prompts for
racism tend to focus on the
proliferation o f racist images and
characterization. To fully understand
racism as a psychological spectrum
disorder, however, one must examine
the emotional quality o f narrative
relationships engineered through the
distorted representations o f non-White
and W hite peoples in historical,
literary, theological, cinematic, and
other narratives. Specifically, one must
understand the ways in which narrative
relationships function as both
emotional guides and emotional triggers.
Racism operates through distorted
narrative relationships that undermine
both narrative knowledge and social
literacy. “Narrative knowledge” is the
term used by narrative medicine
pioneer and medical educator, Dr. Rita
Charon, to describe a set o f critical
competencies instilled by literary reading.
In Narrative Medicine: Honoring the
Stories o f Illness (200 6), Dr. Charon, an
internal medicine specialist who later impaired by a long history
o f racist
earned a Ph.D. in English, notes that socialization.
If narratives are stories that
have a teller, a listener, a time
course, a plot, and a point,
then narrative knowledge is
what we naturally use to make
sense o f them. Narrative
knowledge provides one
person with a rich, resonant
grasp o f another persons
situation as it unfolds
in time, whether in such
texts as novels, newspaper
stories, movies, and scripture
or in such life settings as
courtrooms, battlefields,
marriages, and illnesses. (9)®
As a key com ponent o f narrative
medicine, narrative knowledge is
envisioned as a remedy to a medical
education process that many believe
produces doctors with tremendous
technical skills but who “lack the
hum an capacities to recognize the
plights o f their patients, to extend
empathy toward those who suffer, and
to join honestly and courageously
with patients in their struggles toward
recovery, with chronic illness, or in
facing death” (3). In designing narrative
medicine as a response to these deficits,
Charon and other narrative medicine
theorists recognized that “[u]sing
narrative knowledge enables a person
to understand the plight o f another
by participating in his or her story
with complex skills o f imagination,
interpretation, and recognition” (9-10).
It is my belief that the narrative
knowledge supporting both interpretive
and ethical competence in medical
practice is equally im portant to
rehabilitating the range o f social and
professional interactions that have been
In the article titled “How Narrative
Relationships Overcome Empathic
Bias,” literary scholar Mary-Catherine
Harrison makes the case for narrative
relationships as an empathy generator
based on the claim that they can
overcome the limitation of being tied
to similarity bias — that is, the notion
that people are more likely to empathize
with those they view as similar to
themselves. As she sees it, narrative
relationships formed with fictional
characters, especially in literature
and cinema, can be an im portant
mechanism for generating empathy
by circumventing the similarity bias
that is alleged to be normative. In
presenting her argument, Harrison
first outlines the normative parameters
o f narrative relationships, similarity
bias, and empathic bias, citing several
studies that corroborate the association
o f similarity bias with empathy, that
confirm the importance o f empathy as
a precursor to “helping behaviors,” and
that associate the lack o f empathy with
“psychopathy, criminality, aggression,
and anti-social behavior” (256).9
O ne pronounced omission in the
studies Harrison mentions is a lack of
attention to the ways in which racial
narratives, since the seventeenth
century, have re-ordered the importance
o f race-based similarity and, thereby,
re-configured the experience o f
empathy.10 Recall that the experiences
and preferences o f Black children in
the Clark study did not conform to
expectations o f preference/empathy
based on similarity bias. If, as Harrison
and other narrative theorists assert,
narrative relationships can be a vehicle
for overcoming similarity bias
and generating empathy for people
who don’t share prom inent social
characteristics, it must be the case that
narrative relationships o f a different
sort were first used to construct and
cultivate (racialized) similarity bias as
a basis for empathy.
Re-visiting the seventeenth century
legislative acts used to craft race
and racism, it is clear that these alone
could n ot have generated the
widespread emotional detachment
toward non-W hite peoples and the
hyper-attachment toward Whites.
Narrative relationships were and are
a vital mechanism for producing this
outcome. Reverse sequencing from
contemporary understandings of
narrative relationships, empathy, and
similarity bias, it is easy to see how
Whites and non-W hites must have
been uprooted by the narratives
relationships with characters that
peopled the W hite supremacist
narratives in literature, law, science,
theology, and cinema in the centuries
and decades before the social
transformations o f the Civil Rights
era. Long before the narrative
transformations o f the Black Arts
Movement and related developments
in the publishing industry enabling
the distribution o f literary works by
non-W hite writers, before curricular
expansions prom pted by the demands
o f Black college students in the 1960s,
dominant cultural narratives constricted
empathic parameters throughout the
society.
In The Bluest Eye, for example,
Toni Morrison depicts how Pauline
Breedlove’s positive narrative
relationships with W hite characters in
1940s cinematic narratives overshadow
her negative social interactions with 15
5
B
LA
C
K
R
E
N
A
IS
S
A
N
C
E
N
O
IR
E
hostile and/or racist Whites and
produce a positive empathic bias
toward them. This positive empathic
bias generates caring behaviors toward
her employers W hite daughter and
a concurrent negative empathic bias
toward and progressive emotional
detachment from her own children.
W ith no similarity bias to overcome,
W hite consumers o f the period’s
cinematic narratives would have been
even more likely to have embraced
the favored narrative relationships with
W hite characters and the negative
narrative relationships with cinematically
“absent” or caricatured non-Whites, and
to have internalized their corresponding
emotional templates.
The common belief today is that these
mechanisms were destroyed with the
mid-twentieth century civil rights
movement. O ne must recall, however,
that the legal remedies, including
school desegregation and busing, that
ended formal apartheid in the u.s. were
not designed to repair the rampant
uprootedness caused by centuries o f
dedicated racism. Taking seriously
Weil’s observation that uprootedness is
a psycho-social disorder, it is difficult
to imagine how it could have been
healed or transformed in the absence
o f deliberate therapeutic interventions.
Recent scholarly studies o f racism’s
impact in several key arenas suggest
that the narrative instruments used to
generate a racist socialization are
re-fashioned and/or re-invented with
each generation, and that the untreated
uprootedness o f earlier decades
continues to self-propagate. In fact,
several studies now confirm significant
patterns o f racialized thinking in the
generation that was born and raised
after the narrative instruments o f the
pre-Civil Rights era were discredited.
In March 2002, the Institute of
Medicine o f the National Academy
o f Sciences released a report titled,
“Unequal Treatment: Confronting
Racial and Ethnic Disparities in Health
Care,” that documented widespread
and substantial disparities in the
treatment doctors dispensed to
Whites and non-W hite patients.
The congressionally mandated study
noted that these disparities persist
“even when insurance status, income,
age, and severity o f conditions are
comparable.” The report’s authors
defined “disparities in healthcare as
racial or ethnic differences in the
quality o f healthcare that are not due
to access-related factors or clinical
needs, preferences, and appropriateness
o f intervention.” They noted that
these disparities are associated with
greater mortality and are, therefore,
“unacceptable.”
In “The Apartheid o f Children’s
Literature,” published in The New
York Times in 2014, writer Christopher
Myers cites a 2013 study by the
Cooperative Children’s Book Center at
the University o f Wisconsin showing
that only 93 o f the 3,200 children’s
books published in 2013 were about
Black people.
A recent study, “W ho believes in me?
The effect o f student-teacher
demographic match on teacher
expectations,” published in the
Economics o f Education Review in 2016,
reveals that “non-black teachers o f
black students have significantly lower
expectations than do black teachers,”
that “ [t]hese effects are larger for black
male students and [non-black] math
teachers,” and that Black teachers have
equal expectations for their non-Black
and Black students.
More recently, a study published in
the Proceedings o f the National Academy
o f Sciences (pnas) in March 2017,
“Language from police body camera
footage shows racial disparities in officer
respect,” confirms that “Police officers
speak significantly less respectfully
to black than to white community
members in everyday traffic stops,
even after controlling for officer race,
infraction severity, stop location, and
stop outcome.”
At the time he shot and killed 32-year
old Philando Castile in July 2016,
M innesota police officer Jeronimo
Yanez was a young man in his late 20s.
Looking at the dash cam and other
footage o f the shooting, it is clear that
Castile’s killer had no mental script
in which a Black man could own
a gun and not be a threat. Narrative
relationships formed through and with
television, cinematic, historical, and
other narrative characterizations o f
Black men as savages, brutes, and thugs
were the likely emotional triggers that
preempted his human response to this
family. Before, during, and after the
killing, Officer Yanez displayed no
empathy for Castile, for his girlfriend,
nor for the traumatized 4-year old
child in the backseat his bullets
narrowly missed hitting, a child who
will undoubtedly need caring and
professional support to become
re-rooted. The jury’s endorsement of
Yanez’s demeanor suggests that racism’s
uprooting is widespread and unabated.
For African-Americans, the public
slaughter o f men, women, and children
by police and by civilians has become
an ongoing roadside holocaust.
O n the one hand, these killings are
consistent with popular understandings
o f the ways in which non-Whites are
endangered by racism. Read more
carefully, however, these developments
are also indicative o f a larger pattern of
detachment, defective moral reasoning,
and misreading responsible for an
alarming decline in the nations
democratic institutions, provisions, and
commitments, and an increase in policies
that have endangered the poor and
middle classes across all racial groups.
As discussed above, these new jeopardies
are the end-results o f a process set in
motion in the seventeenth-century
colonies. In their quest to halt, if not
reverse, this decline, several commentators
have developed lucid descriptions of
the threatening behaviors but have
failed to examine their psychological
foundations in the ideology of race.
A useful example is the book What’s
the Matter with Kansas? (2004), in
which writer Thomas Frank unmasks
the voting behaviors o f working and
middle class W hite men in endorsing
policies and candidates inimical to
their own economic interests. Frank
blames these developments on what
he calls “the Great Backlash, a style of
conservatism that first came snarling
onto the national stage in response to
the partying and protests o f the late
sixties”(5) - As he notes, “W hile earlier
forms o f conservatism emphasized
fiscal sobriety, the backlash mobilizes
voters with explosive social issues —
summoning public outrage over
everything from busing to un-Christian
art — which it then marries to
pro-business economic policies”^ ).
Although he faults the Democratic
Party for becoming “the other
pro-business party,” the group that
comes in for a scathing indictm ent in
Frank’s analysis is the vast and diverse
m ultitude o f working-class voters:
sturdy blue-collar patriots
reciting the Pledge while
they strangle their own life
chances;... small farmers
proudly voting themselves
off the lan d ;... devoted
family men carefully seeing
to it that their children
will never be able to afford
college or proper health
care;... working-class guys
in midwestern cities cheering
as they deliver up a landslide
for a candidate whose policies
will end their way o f life, will
transform their region into
a ‘rust belt,’ will strike people
like them blows from which
they will never recover. (10)
The working-class political behavior
Frank describes can be more fully
understood as the calculated effect of
focusing illusions. As defined by
cognitive psychologist Daniel Kahneman,
a focusing illusion is any single issue
that is given an exaggerated importance
in decision-making, because o f a false
calculation o f its impact on expected
outcome. Historically, race has been
a central focusing illusion for many
W hite working-class voters in the u.s.
who mistakenly believed that their
economic interests and future quality
o f life would be secured by supporting
candidates who endorsed racist
positions. The Trump campaign was
perhaps the most successful among
recent deployments o f race as a focus
illusion. W hite working-class voters
thrilled to Trump’s promises to exclude
Mexicans, ban Muslims, and dismantle
the policies (and legacy) o f the first
Black president, (mistakenly) convinced
that their lives would be improved
by these actions, and, like their
seventeenth-century predecessors, that
their economic interests would be
prioritized and secured by a W hite rich
president.
The widespread vulnerability to focusing
illusions is a direct consequence o f
emotional detachment from those
“other” people who are (mistakenly)
expected to bear the full brunt of
particular policy decisions, a detachment
ffequendy accompanied and exacerbated
by deficits in social literacy and
historical thinking. A recent example
can be observed in the farmers from
California and other Western states
who cheered on the Trump
anti-Mexican, anti-immigrant agenda
with no empathy for the psychological
distress o f the Mexican workers they
had employed for years and whom they
knew. Belatedly recognizing that their
own economic well-being is dependent
on these “disposable” people, these
W hite farmers are now anxious to have
the administration reverse course.
In Historical Thinking and Other
Unnatural Acts (2001), education
psychologist and cognitive scientist,
Samuel W ineburg differentiates
between historical knowledge and
historical thinking. As he sees it, “to
engage in historical thinking [is to
be] called on to see hum an motive in
the texts we read; called on to mine
truth from the quicksand o f innuendo,
half-truth, and falsehood that seeks to
engulf us each day; called on to brave
the fact that certainty, at least in
understanding the social world,
remains elusive and beyond our grasp”
(83). For Wineburg, the ability to
engage in historical thinking is an
essential component o f “social literacy,”
“a literacy n ot o f names and dates but o f
discernment, judgment, and caution” (ix).
15
7
B
LA
C
K
R
EN
A
IS
SA
N
C
E
N
O
IR
E
CODA
In a 2012 lecture that now seems
prescient, retired Supreme Court justice
David Souter expressed his concern that
u.s. democracy would be endangered
not by foreign agents or military force
but by a decline in the populations
social literacy. Prefacing his commentary
with the observation that “Democracy
cannot survive too much ignorance,”
Justice Souter explained,
I don’t worry about our losing
republican government in
the United States because I’m
afraid o f a foreign invasion
[...] or a coup by the military
as has happened in other
places [...] W hat I worry
about is that when problems
are not addressed people will
n ot know who is responsible.
[...] If something is not done
to improve the level o f civic
knowledge, that’s what I
worry about.11
Although Justice Souter mentions
dangers arising from a lack o f “civic”
knowledge, the substance o f his
analysis is about dangers stemming
from a populace whose ability to
engage in historical thinking has been
severely diminished. The paradox
here is that while racism has largely
contributed to the loss o f these
essential competencies, a society
conditioned to recognize only
racialized dangers may not be capable
o f fully appreciating or adequately
responding to ongoing threats to its
democratic institutions when those
threats are in White-face.
Throughout the nation’s history, race
has been the most frequently deployed,
the most effective mechanism for
undermining the interpretive
competence and interpretive agency
o f people with W hite racial privilege.
It does so, first, by its positioning of
particular groups on the lower tiers
on the hierarchy o f hum an worth and
concurrently defining them as
disposable. The resulting emotional
detachment toward these groups infects
and impairs emotional attachment
toward other non-racialized groups,
eroding the capacity for effective moral
reasoning and healthy decision-making.
Second, by generating narratives that
assign blame to these constituencies for
failures in economic and social policy.
Third, by orchestrating narrative
relationships that function as disabling
emotional guides and emotional
triggers, inhibiting narrative knowledge
and social literacy. And finally, by
prescribing indifference regarding
threats to those defined as disposable
and simultaneously obscuring the
connection between such threats and
jeopardy to those defined as valuable.
Now, at the start o f the Trump
presidency, it is becoming all too
apparent that u.s. democracy is
imperiled by the very intellectual
habits that sustain the racial privileges
McIntosh and other W hites carry in
their “invisible knapsacks.”
Despite this, I hope for a future secured
by the possibilities o f re-rooting through
racial self-evaluation, self-diagnosis,
and self-correction: a future in which
a parent might say to a friend,
“I’m worried about my teenage son.
He’s showing signs o f racism spectrum
disorder.” Or, a volunteer might say
o f herself, “I’m beginning to realize
I’m on the racism spectrum and that
the volunteer work I do really stems
from the fact that I don’t see my clients
as having the same capacities that
I do.” A future in which a co-worker
might say, “My parents were on the
racism spectrum and worked really
hard to provide my brother and me
with learning opportunities to make
sure that we wouldn’t be.” A future
in which a colleague might say, “My
students and I are developing a model
to counter r.s.d through an elementary
school reading curriculum.” O ne in
which a political scientist will decide to
measure the ways in which r.s.d. — not
just the Russians — affected the 2016
presidential elections, and in which
doctors and law enforcement officers
will be screened for r.s.d using fMRI
technology and offered appropriate
therapies. A future in which voters who
lack personal knowledge of the many
strangers on the “trolley tracks” o f
domestic and foreign policy debates
will seek new narrative knowledge
and be guided by it in effective moral
reasoning and moral responses. O ne
in which young people will not need
to remind uprooted compatriots that
“Black lives matter!” If not this future,
then it will be, as writer James Baldwin
forewarned, the fire next time. ■
WORKS CITED
Baldwin, James. 1962. The Fire Next Time. New York:
Vintage Books.
Charon, Rita. 2006. Narrative Medicine: Honoring the
Stories o f Illness. New York: Oxford University Press.
Clark, Kenneth B. and Mamie P. Clark. 1947. "Racial
Identification and Preference in Negro Children." E.L.
Hartley, ed. Readings in Social Psychology. New York:
Holt, Rinehart,Winston. Print.
Clore, Gerald L., and Janet Palmer. 2009. “Affective
guidance of intelligent agents: How emotion controls
cognition.” Cognitive Systems Research 10,21-30. Print.
Damasio, Antonio, R. 1994. Descartes’Error: Emotion,
Reason, and the Human Brain. New York: G. P. Putnam’s
Sons. Print.
Frank, Thomas. 2004. What’s the matter with Kansas: how
conservatives stole the heart o f America.
Gershenson, Seth, Stephen B. Holt, and Nicholas W.
Papageorge. 2016. "Who believes in me? The effect
of student-teacher demographic match on teacher
expectations." Economics o f Education Review 52.
209-224. Print.
Greene, Joshua D., R. Brian Sommerville, Leigh E. Nystrom,
John M. Parley, and Jonathan D. Cohen. 2001. "An fMRI
Investigation of Emotional Engagement in Moral
Judgment.”Science Vol. 293, September. 2105-2108.
Print.
ENDNOTES
1 The Clark study showed th a t contrary to psychological
predictions th a t children would prefer dolls that most
closely resembled themselves i.e. would display prefer-
ence based on similarity bias, Black children rejected
as "ugly” the Black dolls with which they identified,
and preferred the White dolls instead.
2 Although the followers of Islam include people from
all races and nations, the term "Muslim” in the
White/Western imagination is as much a signifier
of (non-White) racial difference as of religious
distinction.
3 McIntosh’s first listing of over 50 privileges appeared
in a 1988 essay titled “White Privilege and Male
Privilege: A Personal Account of Coming to See
Correspondences through Work in Women’s Studies.”
A year later, she published an abridged version of
the essay, titled "Unpacking th e Invisible Knapsack,”
with a shorter list of only 26 privileges.
4 This privilege is listed in the 1988 but not the 1989
version.
5 Wahneema Lubiano, “Introduction,” p.vii.
Harrison, Mary-Catherine. 2011. “How Narrative
Relationships Overcome Empathic Bias: Elizabeth
Gaskell’s Empathy across Social Difference.” Poetics
Today 32:2 (Summer). 255-288. Print.
Institute of Medicine. 2003. Unequal Treatment:
Confronting Racial and Ethnic Disparities in Healthcare.
Washington, DC: National Academies Press.
Kahneman, Daniel, and Amos Tversky, eds. 2000. Choices,
Values, and Frames. Cambridge and New York:
Cambridge University Press. Print.
Lubiano, Wahneema, ed. 1997. “Introduction.” The House
That Race Built. New York: Vintage Books. Print.
McIntosh, Peggy. 1989. “White Privilege: Unpacking the
Invisible Knapsack” Peace and Freedom July/August:
io-i2. Print.
Morrison, Toni. 1970. The Bluest Eye. New York: Knopf. Print.
Myers, Christopher. 2014. “The Apartheid of Children’s
Literature.” The New York Times Sunday Review
March 15. Print.
Olson, Joel. 2001. “The Democratic Problem of th e White
Citizen.” Constellations Vol. 8, No. 2. Print.
Powell, John A. 2007. "Structural Racism: Building upon
the Insights of John Calmore." North Carolina Law
Review 86:791- 816. Print.
6 As Gerald L. Clore and Janet Palmer note, "it turns out
that affect and emotion play critical roles in good
judgm ent and in the adaptive regulation of thought."
7 According to Greene and his colleagues, “Typical
examples of non-moral dilemmas posed questions
about whether to travel by bus or by train given
certain tim e constraints and about which of two
coupons to use at a store" (2106).
8 To Charon's listing of “life settings” in which narrative
knowledge is essential, I would add the many other
social and professional arenas — neighborhoods,
k-through-12 classrooms, airports, restaurants,
playgrounds, police precincts, newsrooms, legislative
chambers, and corporate boardrooms — in which
citizens interact in our democracy.
9 It is im portant to note th a t a lack of empathy on
th e part of the ruling elite does not typically involve
th e familiar displays of petty criminals. In fact,
these displays are usually not defined as criminal.
Nevertheless, the characteristics associated with a
lack of empathy are obviously at work in the economic
policies and behaviors of the ruling elite who are
frequently detached from working-class employ-
ees, tenants, patients, students, clients, and other
subordinates.
Souter, David. 2012. “Constitutionally Speaking with
Justice David Souter and PBS senior correspondent
Margaret Warner.” September 14. Capitol Center for
the Arts, Concord, New Hampshire. Audio.
Tatum, Beverly. 1997. Why Are All the Black Kids Sitting
Together in the Cafeteria? And Other Conversations
About Race. Print.
Voigt, Rob, Nicholas P. Camp, Vinodkumar Prabhakaran,
William L. Hamilton, Rebecca C. Hetey, Camilla M.
Griffiths, David Jurgens, Dan Jurafsky, and Jennifer L.
Eberhardt. 2017. "Language from policy body camera
footage shows racial disparities in officer respect."
Proceedings o f the National Academy o f Sciences.
Weil, Simone. 1949. The Need fo r Roots: Prelude to a
Declaration o f Duties toward Mankind. London and
New York: Routledge. Print.
Wineburg, Samuel. 2001. Historical Thinking and Other
Unnatural Acts: Charting the Future o f Teaching the
Past. Philadelphia:Temple University Press. Print.
10 At this historical juncture, it is difficult to tell whether
empathy was/is always dependent and/or limited by
social difference, especially race. The New Testament
narrative of the “good Samaritan" is one of perhaps a
few examples of empathy unrestrained by social dif-
ference. So, too, is Toni Morrison’s revisioning of this
narrative in her depiction of the encounter between
the White teenager Amy Denver and Sethe, the refu-
gee from slavery, in th e novel Beloved.
11 "Constitutionally Speaking with Justice David Souter
and PBS senior correspondent Margaret Warner.”
On September 14,2012 at the Capitol Center for the
Arts in Concord, New Hampshire.
15
9
BL
AC
K
RE
NA
IS
SA
NC
E
NO
IR
E
Copyright of Black Renaissance/Renaissance Noire is the
property of Institute of African-
American Affairs and its content may not be copied or emailed
to multiple sites or posted to a
listserv without the copyright holder's express written
permission. However, users may print,
download, or email articles for individual use.
The Trouble with Gay Rights: Race
and the Politics of Sexual Orientation
in Philadelphia, 1969-1982
Kevin J. Mumford
In 1985 the veteran civil rights activist Bayard Rustin joined a
gay and lesbian coalition in New
York City that was about to make a final push for passage of an
amendment to the city's
administrative code to prohibit discrimination on the basis of
sexual orientation. Rustin wrote
to the city council and to Mayor Edward I. Koch, vowed to
lobby the four opposing African
American councilmen, and called for "the extension of freedom
and justice for all Americans."'
Unusual for Rustin, in this instance his activism ceremoniously
concluded rather than pio-
neered a social movement for change when the city council
passed the bill the next year.
More than fifteen years before, the first gay liberationists had
introduced such a bill
that was defeated by stiff opposition from conservative
politicians and religious leaders.
That mobilization helped spark organizing and demonstrating
for similar laws in cities
and towns during the 1970s and 1980s—a local political
movement and discourse that
remains largely unexamined. In recent writing on sexual
politics, however, Matthew D.
Lassiter, Robert O . Self, Whitney Strub, and others have
analyzed controversies over
gay visibility in the media, conflicts over the regulation of
urban vice, and the political
cooptation of pornography to understand shifts in American
liberalism, and they suggest
that the rise of gay liberation served as a foil against which
liberals reconfigured the
public/private divide and conservatives mobilized a rightward
political turn. From
another perspective, despite the rise of the New Right, the cause
of gays and lesbians did
advance during the 1970s, with the proliferation of community
pride, new national organi-
zations, and the passage of protective ordinances. Utilizing a
case study of Philadelphia, this
essay examines how the pursuit of equal protection led gays and
lesbians to clash with reli-
gious and racial conservatives who challenged not only their
rights but also their legitimacy
Kevin J. Mumford is a member of the Department of History at
the University of Iowa
He wishes to thank the following for encouragement and advice:
Clarissa Atkins, Nan Alamilla Boyd, Zoe Burk-
bolder, Christopher Capozzola, Matthew Countryman, Rachel
Devlin, Steve Estes, Estelle Freedman, Thomas
Guglielmo, Evelyn Brooks Higginbotham, Andrew Kahrl, Linda
Kerber, Scott Kurashige, Kenneth Mack, Charles
Morris, David Roediger, Bob Skiba, Marc Stein, Shelton
Stromquist. the audiences of the Charles Warren Center
for Studies in American History and the 2009 annual meeting of
the Organization of American Historians, and the
anonymous reviewers for theJAH.
' Bayard Rustin, memo, March 11, 1986, in
7^?ß/Z)iari^/?«jft«Pi7/)frj. ed. Bayard Rustin and Nanette
Dobrosky
(Frederick, 1988); Rustin to Edward 1. Koch, Feb. 2, 1985, ibid;
"Bayard Rustin's Statement on Proposed Amend.-
ments to Law Banning Discrimination on the Basis of Sexual
Orientation," April 17, 1986, ibid,; Rustin to Howard
N. Meyer, Dec. 8, 1986, ibid; Rustin to Hon. Mary Pinkett,
March 22, 1985, ibid.; John D'Emilio, Lost Prophet:
The Life and Times of Bayard Rustin (New York, 2003), 489-
90; "Text of the New Bill of Rights," New York Times,
Jan. 23, 1986, p. B2; "Rights Measure Ready for Vote after 12
Years," ibid., March 20, 1986, p. Bl; "Amending the
Homosexual Rights Law," ibid.. May 3, 1986, p. A26.
doi: 10.1O93/jahist/jarl39
© Ihe Author 2011. Published by Oxford University Press on
behalf of the Organization of American Historians.
All rights reserved. For permissions, please e-mail:
journals.permissions^poup.com
June 2011 The Journal of American History 49
50 The Journal of American History June 2011
as a minority. It also explores how they developed racial
diversity to respond to those threats
and ultimately argues that gays and lesbians reconstructed their
identities in the process of
negotiating race relations and extending the liberal impulses of
the 1960s into the 1980s.'
Perhaps more than any other field, gay and lesbian studies
concentrates on the origins
and meanings of identity, creating discussions that often evolve
into what is referred to as
the essentialism versus constructionism debate. Essentialist
scholars conceive of sexuality
as a transhistorical essence located in the self, and they tend to
see the gays, lesbians, or
homosexuals of the past as identifying and desiring in much the
same way as they do in
the present. By contrast, social constructionist scholars argue
that the very categories of
homosexuality and heterosexuality change historically in
relation to the forces of capital-
ism, the family, urbanization, and scientific knowledge. Most
historians are by nature
constructionists, and they generally agree that by the outbreak
of the Stonewall riots in
1969, when New York City police raided the gay-friendly
Stonewall Inn and patrons
fought back, gay and lesbian identities and communities had
formed across metropolitan
America. Even so, sexual historians entertain a variety of
interpretations about which
processes or forces most affected the dynamics of identification.
Anne Enke reconstructed
1970s civic spaces (such as dance halls and ballparks) and
Martin Meeker pointed to contact
zones of literature, periodicals, and photographs to describe
areas of popular interaction that
defined sexual subjectivities. Margot Canaday uncovered the
extensive operation of constitu-
tive regulation by what she termed the "straight state" to
illustrate the powers of government
interdiction and neglect, and Joanne Meyerowitz analyzed
paradigms of racial and sexual
knowledge to uncover changing processes of subjective
interpolation that sometimes served as
self-representations. In turn, my reconstruction of the politics of
sexual orientation in Phila-
delphia highlights the impact of intersections of race and
sexuality on identity formation.
As Matthew J. Countryman demonstrated in his study of civil
rights and black power
movements in Philadelphia, black leadership protested against
workplace discrimination,
promoted educational reform, and gained control in municipal
government. At the same
time, gay organizers, volunteers, witnesses, and audiences in
the city, along with readers of the
gay press and other media, were influenced by black political
actors and gay people of color as
they struggled for a place in the rights revolution of the 1970s.'
By the rights revolution, I not only refer to Jacquelyn Dowd
Hall's phrase the "long civil
rights movement" and her influential reading of racial struggle
that extended the chronology
of the movement, enlarged its scope from the South to the
nation, and stressed the role of
working-class politics, but I also incorporate what the
sociologist John Skrentny conceived
of as a minority rights revolution, or the expansion of the black
civil rights movement
^ Bruce J. Schulman, The Seventies: The Great Shift in
American Culture, Society, and Politics (New York, 2002);
Matthew D. Lassiter, "Inventing Family Values," in Rightward
Bound: Making America Conservative in the 1970s,
ed. Bruce J. Schulman and Julian Zelizer (Cambridge, Mass.,
2008), 13-28; Robert O. Self, "Sex in the City: The
Politics of Sexual Liberalism in Los Angeles, 1963-1979,"
Gender and History, 20 (Aug. 2008), 288-311; Whitney
Strub, Perversion for Profit: The Politics of Pornography and
the Rise of the New Right (New York, 2010), 260-61 ; Josh
Sides, "Excavating the Postwar Sex District in San Francisco,"
Journal of Urban History, 32 (March 2006), 355-79;
Peter Braunstein, '"Adults Only': The Construction of the Erotic
City in New York during the 1970s," in America
in the Seventies, ed. Beth L. Bailey and David Färber
(Lawrence, 2004), 129-56. esp. 146.
' Anne Enke, Finding the Movement: Sexuality, Contested
Space, and Feminist Activism (Durham, N.C., 2007);
Margot Canaday, The Straight State: Sexuality and Citizenship
in Twentieth Century America (Princeton, 2009); Joanne
Meyerowitz, '"How Common Culture Shapes the Separate
Lives': Sexuality, Race, and Mid-Twentieth-Century
Social Constructionist "Vnon^u Journal of American History, 96
(March 2010), 1057—84; Joanne Meyerowitz,
"Transnational Sex and U.S. Wsx.ov¡^ American Historical
Review, 14 (Dec. 2009), 1273-86; Matthew J. Countryman,
Up South: Civil Rights and Black Power in Philadelphia
(Philadelphia, 2006), 117-52, 236-44, 312-27.
Race and the Politics of Sexual Orientation in Philadelphia,
1969-1982 51
to include women, the disabled, the elderly, and other groups.
His synthesis argued that
"though victimized like blacks, gays and lesbians were not
legitimate victims," in part
because they failed to demonstrate how they were analogous to
recognized groups. Historians
of the legal regulation of sexuality have focused more on the
issues of marriage and sodomy
than on antidiscrimination legislation, while the recent growth
of historiography on the
northern civil rights movement largely overlooks gays and
lesbians. Instead, the study of
sexual orientation ordinances has been taken up by social
scientists who measure discrete
variables—religiosity, population size, socioeconomic status,
and education—to explain
why some locations passed laws and others did not. Following
their lead, my analysis com-
pares the legislative failure in 1975 to the eventual legislative
success in 1982 in Philadelphia
but focuses less on why it occurred than on how it occurred, in
part because of how race
became crucial to arguments against gay rights as well as
central to their passage. Although
historians have examined the impact of race on sexuality,
particularly in studies of "misce-
genation," relatively few studies of gay and lesbian
communities have done so. My analysis
pursues the same sort of questions about homophobia that
historians traditionally ask
about racism: What were the dominant images and logics
presented to the public? What
did it justify? How did it respond to challenges and reform? My
work also locates a kind
of racial conflict between white gays and black gays that
undermined the movement from
within, which the historian of feminism Winifred Breines has
conceptualized as a cycle of
personal misunderstanding that troubled social movements—one
of "profound racial dis-
tance and tentative reconciliation." In Philadelphia and other
cities with significant minor-
ity demographics, sexual politics operated in some relationship
to patterns of race relations."*
Race and Gay Liberation
Since the emergence of modern discourse on gay and lesbian
identity, writers and aca-
demics have often compared the condition of the homosexual to
that of a racial minority.
•• Jacquelyn Dowd Hall, "The Long Civil Rights Movement and
the Political Uses of the Past," Journal of
American History, 91 (March 2005), 1233-63; John D.
Skrentny, The Minority Rights Revolution (Cambridge,
Mass., 2002), 20, 304-5, 315, 326. On the long civil rights
movement, see Sundidata Keita Cha-Jua and Clarence
Lang, "The 'Long Movement' as Vampire: Temporal and Spatial
Fallacies in Recent Black Freedom Smd^its," Journal
of/^can American History, 92 (Spring 2007), 265-88; and
Samuel Walker, The Rights Revolution: Rights and Com-
munity in Modern America (New York, 1998), 44-48. On the
long civil rights movement and working-class politics,
see Nancy MacLean, Ereedom Is Not Enough: The Opening of
the American Workplace (Cambridge, Mass., 2006). For
studies of sexual orientation ordinances, see Kenneth D. Wald,
James W. Button, and Barbara A. Rienzo. "The
Politics of Gay Rights in American Communities: Explaining
Antidiscrimination Ordinances and Policies,"" Ameri-
can Journal of Political Science, 40 (Nov. 1996), 1157-78; Gary
Mucciaroni, Same Sex, Different Politics: Success and
Eailure in the Struggles over Gay Rights (Chicago, 2008), 31;
Craig A. Rimmerman, Kenneth D. Wald, and Clyde
Wilcox, Ihe Politics of Gay Rights (Chicago, 2000) ; and Wayne
Van der Meide, Legist/! ting Equality: A Review of Laws
Affecting Gay, Lesbian, Bisexual, and Transgendered People in
the United States (Washington, 2004). On Philadelphia
gay institutions and sexual orientation rights, see Robert W.
Bailey, Gay Politics, Urban Politics: Identity and Econom-
ics in the Urban Setting (New York, 1999), 249-79. On the
impact of race on sexuality, see Peggy Pa.scoe, What
Comes Naturally: Miscegenation Law and the Making of Race
in America (New York, 2009); Renee C. Romano. Race
Mixing: Black-White Marriage in Postwar America (Cambridge,
Mass., 2003); David !.. Chappell, A Stone of Hope:
Prophetic Religion and the Death of Jim Crow (Chapel Hill,
2004), 114-18; White Interveners in Still v, Savannah-
Chatham County Board of Education, "In Defense of School
Segregation,"" in The Development of Segregationist
Thought, ed. I. A. Newby (Homewood, 1968), 146-53; Pete
Daniel, Lost Revolutions: The South in the 1950s
(Chapel Hill, 2000), 148-75; and 1. A. Newby,//'w Crow's
Defense: Anti-Negro Thought in America, 1900-1930
(Baton Rouge, 1965), 92-95. Winifred Breines, The Trouble
between Us: An Uneasy History of the White and Black
Women in the Eeminist Movement (New York, 2006), 7. On
race and feminism, see also Sara M. Evans, Personal
Politics: The Roots of Women's Liberation in the Civil Rights
Movement and the New Left (New York, 1979); and Anne
M. Valk, Radical Sisters: Second-Wave Eeminism and Black
Liberation in Washington, D.C. (Urbana, 2008).
52 The Journal of American History June 2011
In his influential 1951 treatise. The Homosexual Minority in
America, Daniel Webster
Cory included homosexuals in the so-called minority problem of
Jews and Negroes, as
did some of the writings of the gay black novelist James
Baldwin and brief letters by the
black playwright Lorraine Hansberry that were published in a
1957 issue of a lesbian
journal. Even so, what was known as the homophile movement
(a number of advocacy
organizations founded in the postwar era) remained largely
white and more culturally,
strategically, and personally conservative. In his classic account
of homophile politics,
John D'Emilio argued that they were influenced by the
"headline-making black civil
rights movement," but that the rank and file sought a kind of
invisible integration and
could be compared to only the most conservative of black
organizations. My own reading
of the national organizational journals—Mattachine Review, the
Ladder, and ONE—
revealed that homophiles were more involved in efforts to
achieve civil liberties and gain
public acceptance than demonstrating for rights, and they
infrequently mentioned
African Americans. Conversely, in the 1950s the black popular
periodical/i?i routinely ran
stories on homosexuality or gay sex scandals, though rarely in
connection with civil rights.
O n rare occasions writers observed the intersection of the two
causes. In 1962 the homo-
phile publication ONE published a review that characterized
James Baldwin as "one of a
number of current writers who is discovering a homosexual
dimension to the current
Negro revolution."'
By the 1960s, homophiles in Philadelphia had begun to adapt
what Marc Stein termed
"multiple models of African American resistance" to the
advancement of their cause, and
on July 4, 1965, they held the first Annual Reminder
demonstration at Independence
Hall to demand recognition of their rights of citizenship. Even
before the 1969 Stonewall
riots, Philadelphia gays and lesbians had formed the Homophile
Action League (HAL) to
break away from the civility of the homophiles; a week after the
riots, HAL and other
radicalized activists challenged the conventionality of the fifth
Annual Reminder by reftis-
ing to conform to its usual dress code. The new radical HAL
later discovered less rebellion
and more apathy in its ranks, however, and therefore urged
readers to follow "our fellow
victims of discrimination and oppression—blacks, women, the
poor, political dissi-
dents—[who] are fervently pressing their battles." In 1970 the
HAL announced that it had
testified for a "state Human Rights Act" before the state
platform committees of both the
Democratic and Republican parties. The league also sponsored a
booth where crowds of
supporters "filled 10 pages of the homosexual 'civil rights'
petition." Similarly, Martin
Duberman stressed the connections of race and sexuality in a
narrative on the Stonewall
^ Daniel Webster Cory, The Homosexual in America: A
Subjective Approach (NewYork, 1951), 4; Letter from L.
N. H.[Lorraine Hansberry], Ladder, 1 (May 1957), 26-28. On
Lorraine Hansberry's letters, see also Lisbeth Lipari,
"The Rhetoric of Intersectionality: Lorraine Hansberry's 1957
Letters to the Ladder," in Queering Public Address:
Sexualities in American Historical Discourse, ed. Charles E.
Morris III (Columbia, S.C., 2007), 233-35; and Cheryl
Higashida, "To Be(come) Young, Gay, and Black: Lorraine
Hansberry's Existentialist Routes to Anticolonialism,"
American Quarterly, 60 (Dec. 2008), 899-924. James Baldwin,
Another Country (New York, 1962); James Baldwin,
"The Male Prison," in The Price of the Ticket: Collected
Nonfiction, 1948-1985, by James Baldwin (New York, 1985),
101-05; John D'Emilio, Sexual Politics, Sexual Communities:
The Making of a Homosexual Minority in the United
States, 1940-1970 (Chicago, 1982), 174, 224; Chuck Stanley,
review of Another Country by James Baldwin, One,
10 (Nov. 1962), 21-22. Andrew Bradbury, "Race and Sex," ibid,
12 (Oct. 1964), 17-21; Review of "The Toilet"
by Le Roi Jones, ibid., 13 (March 1965), 16; Richard Mayer,
review o( Giovanni's Room by James Baldwin, Matta-
chine Review, 3 (April 1957), 33; 'A Moral Revolution: For
Homosexuals?," Pursuit and Symposium, 1 (March—April
1966), 31—33. On homophile ethnic analogy, see Barbara
1 of 3  FIN 3302 – Exam I Review Fall 2017 Chapter 1.docx
1 of 3  FIN 3302 – Exam I Review Fall 2017 Chapter 1.docx
1 of 3  FIN 3302 – Exam I Review Fall 2017 Chapter 1.docx
1 of 3  FIN 3302 – Exam I Review Fall 2017 Chapter 1.docx
1 of 3  FIN 3302 – Exam I Review Fall 2017 Chapter 1.docx
1 of 3  FIN 3302 – Exam I Review Fall 2017 Chapter 1.docx
1 of 3  FIN 3302 – Exam I Review Fall 2017 Chapter 1.docx
1 of 3  FIN 3302 – Exam I Review Fall 2017 Chapter 1.docx
1 of 3  FIN 3302 – Exam I Review Fall 2017 Chapter 1.docx
1 of 3  FIN 3302 – Exam I Review Fall 2017 Chapter 1.docx
1 of 3  FIN 3302 – Exam I Review Fall 2017 Chapter 1.docx
1 of 3  FIN 3302 – Exam I Review Fall 2017 Chapter 1.docx
1 of 3  FIN 3302 – Exam I Review Fall 2017 Chapter 1.docx
1 of 3  FIN 3302 – Exam I Review Fall 2017 Chapter 1.docx
1 of 3  FIN 3302 – Exam I Review Fall 2017 Chapter 1.docx
1 of 3  FIN 3302 – Exam I Review Fall 2017 Chapter 1.docx
1 of 3  FIN 3302 – Exam I Review Fall 2017 Chapter 1.docx
1 of 3  FIN 3302 – Exam I Review Fall 2017 Chapter 1.docx
1 of 3  FIN 3302 – Exam I Review Fall 2017 Chapter 1.docx
1 of 3  FIN 3302 – Exam I Review Fall 2017 Chapter 1.docx
1 of 3  FIN 3302 – Exam I Review Fall 2017 Chapter 1.docx
1 of 3  FIN 3302 – Exam I Review Fall 2017 Chapter 1.docx
1 of 3  FIN 3302 – Exam I Review Fall 2017 Chapter 1.docx
1 of 3  FIN 3302 – Exam I Review Fall 2017 Chapter 1.docx
1 of 3  FIN 3302 – Exam I Review Fall 2017 Chapter 1.docx
1 of 3  FIN 3302 – Exam I Review Fall 2017 Chapter 1.docx
1 of 3  FIN 3302 – Exam I Review Fall 2017 Chapter 1.docx
1 of 3  FIN 3302 – Exam I Review Fall 2017 Chapter 1.docx
1 of 3  FIN 3302 – Exam I Review Fall 2017 Chapter 1.docx
1 of 3  FIN 3302 – Exam I Review Fall 2017 Chapter 1.docx
1 of 3  FIN 3302 – Exam I Review Fall 2017 Chapter 1.docx
1 of 3  FIN 3302 – Exam I Review Fall 2017 Chapter 1.docx
1 of 3  FIN 3302 – Exam I Review Fall 2017 Chapter 1.docx
1 of 3  FIN 3302 – Exam I Review Fall 2017 Chapter 1.docx
1 of 3  FIN 3302 – Exam I Review Fall 2017 Chapter 1.docx
1 of 3  FIN 3302 – Exam I Review Fall 2017 Chapter 1.docx
1 of 3  FIN 3302 – Exam I Review Fall 2017 Chapter 1.docx
1 of 3  FIN 3302 – Exam I Review Fall 2017 Chapter 1.docx
1 of 3  FIN 3302 – Exam I Review Fall 2017 Chapter 1.docx
1 of 3  FIN 3302 – Exam I Review Fall 2017 Chapter 1.docx
1 of 3  FIN 3302 – Exam I Review Fall 2017 Chapter 1.docx
1 of 3  FIN 3302 – Exam I Review Fall 2017 Chapter 1.docx
1 of 3  FIN 3302 – Exam I Review Fall 2017 Chapter 1.docx
1 of 3  FIN 3302 – Exam I Review Fall 2017 Chapter 1.docx
1 of 3  FIN 3302 – Exam I Review Fall 2017 Chapter 1.docx
1 of 3  FIN 3302 – Exam I Review Fall 2017 Chapter 1.docx
1 of 3  FIN 3302 – Exam I Review Fall 2017 Chapter 1.docx
1 of 3  FIN 3302 – Exam I Review Fall 2017 Chapter 1.docx
1 of 3  FIN 3302 – Exam I Review Fall 2017 Chapter 1.docx
1 of 3  FIN 3302 – Exam I Review Fall 2017 Chapter 1.docx
1 of 3  FIN 3302 – Exam I Review Fall 2017 Chapter 1.docx
1 of 3  FIN 3302 – Exam I Review Fall 2017 Chapter 1.docx
1 of 3  FIN 3302 – Exam I Review Fall 2017 Chapter 1.docx
1 of 3  FIN 3302 – Exam I Review Fall 2017 Chapter 1.docx
1 of 3  FIN 3302 – Exam I Review Fall 2017 Chapter 1.docx
1 of 3  FIN 3302 – Exam I Review Fall 2017 Chapter 1.docx
1 of 3  FIN 3302 – Exam I Review Fall 2017 Chapter 1.docx
1 of 3  FIN 3302 – Exam I Review Fall 2017 Chapter 1.docx
1 of 3  FIN 3302 – Exam I Review Fall 2017 Chapter 1.docx
1 of 3  FIN 3302 – Exam I Review Fall 2017 Chapter 1.docx
1 of 3  FIN 3302 – Exam I Review Fall 2017 Chapter 1.docx
1 of 3  FIN 3302 – Exam I Review Fall 2017 Chapter 1.docx
1 of 3  FIN 3302 – Exam I Review Fall 2017 Chapter 1.docx
1 of 3  FIN 3302 – Exam I Review Fall 2017 Chapter 1.docx
1 of 3  FIN 3302 – Exam I Review Fall 2017 Chapter 1.docx
1 of 3  FIN 3302 – Exam I Review Fall 2017 Chapter 1.docx
1 of 3  FIN 3302 – Exam I Review Fall 2017 Chapter 1.docx
1 of 3  FIN 3302 – Exam I Review Fall 2017 Chapter 1.docx
1 of 3  FIN 3302 – Exam I Review Fall 2017 Chapter 1.docx
1 of 3  FIN 3302 – Exam I Review Fall 2017 Chapter 1.docx
1 of 3  FIN 3302 – Exam I Review Fall 2017 Chapter 1.docx
1 of 3  FIN 3302 – Exam I Review Fall 2017 Chapter 1.docx
1 of 3  FIN 3302 – Exam I Review Fall 2017 Chapter 1.docx
1 of 3  FIN 3302 – Exam I Review Fall 2017 Chapter 1.docx
1 of 3  FIN 3302 – Exam I Review Fall 2017 Chapter 1.docx
1 of 3  FIN 3302 – Exam I Review Fall 2017 Chapter 1.docx
1 of 3  FIN 3302 – Exam I Review Fall 2017 Chapter 1.docx
1 of 3  FIN 3302 – Exam I Review Fall 2017 Chapter 1.docx
1 of 3  FIN 3302 – Exam I Review Fall 2017 Chapter 1.docx
1 of 3  FIN 3302 – Exam I Review Fall 2017 Chapter 1.docx
1 of 3  FIN 3302 – Exam I Review Fall 2017 Chapter 1.docx
1 of 3  FIN 3302 – Exam I Review Fall 2017 Chapter 1.docx
1 of 3  FIN 3302 – Exam I Review Fall 2017 Chapter 1.docx
1 of 3  FIN 3302 – Exam I Review Fall 2017 Chapter 1.docx
1 of 3  FIN 3302 – Exam I Review Fall 2017 Chapter 1.docx
1 of 3  FIN 3302 – Exam I Review Fall 2017 Chapter 1.docx
1 of 3  FIN 3302 – Exam I Review Fall 2017 Chapter 1.docx
1 of 3  FIN 3302 – Exam I Review Fall 2017 Chapter 1.docx
1 of 3  FIN 3302 – Exam I Review Fall 2017 Chapter 1.docx
1 of 3  FIN 3302 – Exam I Review Fall 2017 Chapter 1.docx
1 of 3  FIN 3302 – Exam I Review Fall 2017 Chapter 1.docx
1 of 3  FIN 3302 – Exam I Review Fall 2017 Chapter 1.docx
1 of 3  FIN 3302 – Exam I Review Fall 2017 Chapter 1.docx
1 of 3  FIN 3302 – Exam I Review Fall 2017 Chapter 1.docx
1 of 3  FIN 3302 – Exam I Review Fall 2017 Chapter 1.docx
1 of 3  FIN 3302 – Exam I Review Fall 2017 Chapter 1.docx
1 of 3  FIN 3302 – Exam I Review Fall 2017 Chapter 1.docx
1 of 3  FIN 3302 – Exam I Review Fall 2017 Chapter 1.docx
1 of 3  FIN 3302 – Exam I Review Fall 2017 Chapter 1.docx
1 of 3  FIN 3302 – Exam I Review Fall 2017 Chapter 1.docx
1 of 3  FIN 3302 – Exam I Review Fall 2017 Chapter 1.docx
1 of 3  FIN 3302 – Exam I Review Fall 2017 Chapter 1.docx
1 of 3  FIN 3302 – Exam I Review Fall 2017 Chapter 1.docx
1 of 3  FIN 3302 – Exam I Review Fall 2017 Chapter 1.docx
1 of 3  FIN 3302 – Exam I Review Fall 2017 Chapter 1.docx
1 of 3  FIN 3302 – Exam I Review Fall 2017 Chapter 1.docx
1 of 3  FIN 3302 – Exam I Review Fall 2017 Chapter 1.docx
1 of 3  FIN 3302 – Exam I Review Fall 2017 Chapter 1.docx
1 of 3  FIN 3302 – Exam I Review Fall 2017 Chapter 1.docx
1 of 3  FIN 3302 – Exam I Review Fall 2017 Chapter 1.docx
1 of 3  FIN 3302 – Exam I Review Fall 2017 Chapter 1.docx
1 of 3  FIN 3302 – Exam I Review Fall 2017 Chapter 1.docx
1 of 3  FIN 3302 – Exam I Review Fall 2017 Chapter 1.docx
1 of 3  FIN 3302 – Exam I Review Fall 2017 Chapter 1.docx
1 of 3  FIN 3302 – Exam I Review Fall 2017 Chapter 1.docx
1 of 3  FIN 3302 – Exam I Review Fall 2017 Chapter 1.docx
1 of 3  FIN 3302 – Exam I Review Fall 2017 Chapter 1.docx
1 of 3  FIN 3302 – Exam I Review Fall 2017 Chapter 1.docx
1 of 3  FIN 3302 – Exam I Review Fall 2017 Chapter 1.docx
1 of 3  FIN 3302 – Exam I Review Fall 2017 Chapter 1.docx
1 of 3  FIN 3302 – Exam I Review Fall 2017 Chapter 1.docx
1 of 3  FIN 3302 – Exam I Review Fall 2017 Chapter 1.docx
1 of 3  FIN 3302 – Exam I Review Fall 2017 Chapter 1.docx
1 of 3  FIN 3302 – Exam I Review Fall 2017 Chapter 1.docx
1 of 3  FIN 3302 – Exam I Review Fall 2017 Chapter 1.docx
1 of 3  FIN 3302 – Exam I Review Fall 2017 Chapter 1.docx
1 of 3  FIN 3302 – Exam I Review Fall 2017 Chapter 1.docx
1 of 3  FIN 3302 – Exam I Review Fall 2017 Chapter 1.docx
1 of 3  FIN 3302 – Exam I Review Fall 2017 Chapter 1.docx
1 of 3  FIN 3302 – Exam I Review Fall 2017 Chapter 1.docx
1 of 3  FIN 3302 – Exam I Review Fall 2017 Chapter 1.docx
1 of 3  FIN 3302 – Exam I Review Fall 2017 Chapter 1.docx
1 of 3  FIN 3302 – Exam I Review Fall 2017 Chapter 1.docx
1 of 3  FIN 3302 – Exam I Review Fall 2017 Chapter 1.docx
1 of 3  FIN 3302 – Exam I Review Fall 2017 Chapter 1.docx
1 of 3  FIN 3302 – Exam I Review Fall 2017 Chapter 1.docx
1 of 3  FIN 3302 – Exam I Review Fall 2017 Chapter 1.docx
1 of 3  FIN 3302 – Exam I Review Fall 2017 Chapter 1.docx
1 of 3  FIN 3302 – Exam I Review Fall 2017 Chapter 1.docx
1 of 3  FIN 3302 – Exam I Review Fall 2017 Chapter 1.docx
1 of 3  FIN 3302 – Exam I Review Fall 2017 Chapter 1.docx
1 of 3  FIN 3302 – Exam I Review Fall 2017 Chapter 1.docx
1 of 3  FIN 3302 – Exam I Review Fall 2017 Chapter 1.docx
1 of 3  FIN 3302 – Exam I Review Fall 2017 Chapter 1.docx
1 of 3  FIN 3302 – Exam I Review Fall 2017 Chapter 1.docx
1 of 3  FIN 3302 – Exam I Review Fall 2017 Chapter 1.docx
1 of 3  FIN 3302 – Exam I Review Fall 2017 Chapter 1.docx
1 of 3  FIN 3302 – Exam I Review Fall 2017 Chapter 1.docx
1 of 3  FIN 3302 – Exam I Review Fall 2017 Chapter 1.docx
1 of 3  FIN 3302 – Exam I Review Fall 2017 Chapter 1.docx
1 of 3  FIN 3302 – Exam I Review Fall 2017 Chapter 1.docx
1 of 3  FIN 3302 – Exam I Review Fall 2017 Chapter 1.docx
1 of 3  FIN 3302 – Exam I Review Fall 2017 Chapter 1.docx
1 of 3  FIN 3302 – Exam I Review Fall 2017 Chapter 1.docx
1 of 3  FIN 3302 – Exam I Review Fall 2017 Chapter 1.docx
1 of 3  FIN 3302 – Exam I Review Fall 2017 Chapter 1.docx
1 of 3  FIN 3302 – Exam I Review Fall 2017 Chapter 1.docx
1 of 3  FIN 3302 – Exam I Review Fall 2017 Chapter 1.docx
1 of 3  FIN 3302 – Exam I Review Fall 2017 Chapter 1.docx
1 of 3  FIN 3302 – Exam I Review Fall 2017 Chapter 1.docx
1 of 3  FIN 3302 – Exam I Review Fall 2017 Chapter 1.docx
1 of 3  FIN 3302 – Exam I Review Fall 2017 Chapter 1.docx
1 of 3  FIN 3302 – Exam I Review Fall 2017 Chapter 1.docx
1 of 3  FIN 3302 – Exam I Review Fall 2017 Chapter 1.docx
1 of 3  FIN 3302 – Exam I Review Fall 2017 Chapter 1.docx
1 of 3  FIN 3302 – Exam I Review Fall 2017 Chapter 1.docx
1 of 3  FIN 3302 – Exam I Review Fall 2017 Chapter 1.docx
1 of 3  FIN 3302 – Exam I Review Fall 2017 Chapter 1.docx
1 of 3  FIN 3302 – Exam I Review Fall 2017 Chapter 1.docx
1 of 3  FIN 3302 – Exam I Review Fall 2017 Chapter 1.docx
1 of 3  FIN 3302 – Exam I Review Fall 2017 Chapter 1.docx
1 of 3  FIN 3302 – Exam I Review Fall 2017 Chapter 1.docx
1 of 3  FIN 3302 – Exam I Review Fall 2017 Chapter 1.docx
1 of 3  FIN 3302 – Exam I Review Fall 2017 Chapter 1.docx
1 of 3  FIN 3302 – Exam I Review Fall 2017 Chapter 1.docx
1 of 3  FIN 3302 – Exam I Review Fall 2017 Chapter 1.docx
1 of 3  FIN 3302 – Exam I Review Fall 2017 Chapter 1.docx
1 of 3  FIN 3302 – Exam I Review Fall 2017 Chapter 1.docx
1 of 3  FIN 3302 – Exam I Review Fall 2017 Chapter 1.docx
1 of 3  FIN 3302 – Exam I Review Fall 2017 Chapter 1.docx
1 of 3  FIN 3302 – Exam I Review Fall 2017 Chapter 1.docx
1 of 3  FIN 3302 – Exam I Review Fall 2017 Chapter 1.docx
1 of 3  FIN 3302 – Exam I Review Fall 2017 Chapter 1.docx
1 of 3  FIN 3302 – Exam I Review Fall 2017 Chapter 1.docx
1 of 3  FIN 3302 – Exam I Review Fall 2017 Chapter 1.docx
1 of 3  FIN 3302 – Exam I Review Fall 2017 Chapter 1.docx
1 of 3  FIN 3302 – Exam I Review Fall 2017 Chapter 1.docx
1 of 3  FIN 3302 – Exam I Review Fall 2017 Chapter 1.docx
1 of 3  FIN 3302 – Exam I Review Fall 2017 Chapter 1.docx
1 of 3  FIN 3302 – Exam I Review Fall 2017 Chapter 1.docx
1 of 3  FIN 3302 – Exam I Review Fall 2017 Chapter 1.docx
1 of 3  FIN 3302 – Exam I Review Fall 2017 Chapter 1.docx
1 of 3  FIN 3302 – Exam I Review Fall 2017 Chapter 1.docx
1 of 3  FIN 3302 – Exam I Review Fall 2017 Chapter 1.docx
1 of 3  FIN 3302 – Exam I Review Fall 2017 Chapter 1.docx
1 of 3  FIN 3302 – Exam I Review Fall 2017 Chapter 1.docx
1 of 3  FIN 3302 – Exam I Review Fall 2017 Chapter 1.docx
1 of 3  FIN 3302 – Exam I Review Fall 2017 Chapter 1.docx
1 of 3  FIN 3302 – Exam I Review Fall 2017 Chapter 1.docx
1 of 3  FIN 3302 – Exam I Review Fall 2017 Chapter 1.docx
1 of 3  FIN 3302 – Exam I Review Fall 2017 Chapter 1.docx
1 of 3  FIN 3302 – Exam I Review Fall 2017 Chapter 1.docx
1 of 3  FIN 3302 – Exam I Review Fall 2017 Chapter 1.docx
1 of 3  FIN 3302 – Exam I Review Fall 2017 Chapter 1.docx
1 of 3  FIN 3302 – Exam I Review Fall 2017 Chapter 1.docx
1 of 3  FIN 3302 – Exam I Review Fall 2017 Chapter 1.docx
1 of 3  FIN 3302 – Exam I Review Fall 2017 Chapter 1.docx
1 of 3  FIN 3302 – Exam I Review Fall 2017 Chapter 1.docx
1 of 3  FIN 3302 – Exam I Review Fall 2017 Chapter 1.docx
1 of 3  FIN 3302 – Exam I Review Fall 2017 Chapter 1.docx
1 of 3  FIN 3302 – Exam I Review Fall 2017 Chapter 1.docx
1 of 3  FIN 3302 – Exam I Review Fall 2017 Chapter 1.docx
1 of 3  FIN 3302 – Exam I Review Fall 2017 Chapter 1.docx
1 of 3  FIN 3302 – Exam I Review Fall 2017 Chapter 1.docx
1 of 3  FIN 3302 – Exam I Review Fall 2017 Chapter 1.docx
1 of 3  FIN 3302 – Exam I Review Fall 2017 Chapter 1.docx
1 of 3  FIN 3302 – Exam I Review Fall 2017 Chapter 1.docx
1 of 3  FIN 3302 – Exam I Review Fall 2017 Chapter 1.docx
1 of 3  FIN 3302 – Exam I Review Fall 2017 Chapter 1.docx
1 of 3  FIN 3302 – Exam I Review Fall 2017 Chapter 1.docx
1 of 3  FIN 3302 – Exam I Review Fall 2017 Chapter 1.docx
1 of 3  FIN 3302 – Exam I Review Fall 2017 Chapter 1.docx
1 of 3  FIN 3302 – Exam I Review Fall 2017 Chapter 1.docx
1 of 3  FIN 3302 – Exam I Review Fall 2017 Chapter 1.docx
1 of 3  FIN 3302 – Exam I Review Fall 2017 Chapter 1.docx
1 of 3  FIN 3302 – Exam I Review Fall 2017 Chapter 1.docx
1 of 3  FIN 3302 – Exam I Review Fall 2017 Chapter 1.docx
1 of 3  FIN 3302 – Exam I Review Fall 2017 Chapter 1.docx
1 of 3  FIN 3302 – Exam I Review Fall 2017 Chapter 1.docx
1 of 3  FIN 3302 – Exam I Review Fall 2017 Chapter 1.docx
1 of 3  FIN 3302 – Exam I Review Fall 2017 Chapter 1.docx
1 of 3  FIN 3302 – Exam I Review Fall 2017 Chapter 1.docx
1 of 3  FIN 3302 – Exam I Review Fall 2017 Chapter 1.docx
1 of 3  FIN 3302 – Exam I Review Fall 2017 Chapter 1.docx
1 of 3  FIN 3302 – Exam I Review Fall 2017 Chapter 1.docx
1 of 3  FIN 3302 – Exam I Review Fall 2017 Chapter 1.docx
1 of 3  FIN 3302 – Exam I Review Fall 2017 Chapter 1.docx
1 of 3  FIN 3302 – Exam I Review Fall 2017 Chapter 1.docx
1 of 3  FIN 3302 – Exam I Review Fall 2017 Chapter 1.docx
1 of 3  FIN 3302 – Exam I Review Fall 2017 Chapter 1.docx
1 of 3  FIN 3302 – Exam I Review Fall 2017 Chapter 1.docx
1 of 3  FIN 3302 – Exam I Review Fall 2017 Chapter 1.docx
1 of 3  FIN 3302 – Exam I Review Fall 2017 Chapter 1.docx
1 of 3  FIN 3302 – Exam I Review Fall 2017 Chapter 1.docx
1 of 3  FIN 3302 – Exam I Review Fall 2017 Chapter 1.docx
1 of 3  FIN 3302 – Exam I Review Fall 2017 Chapter 1.docx
1 of 3  FIN 3302 – Exam I Review Fall 2017 Chapter 1.docx
1 of 3  FIN 3302 – Exam I Review Fall 2017 Chapter 1.docx
1 of 3  FIN 3302 – Exam I Review Fall 2017 Chapter 1.docx
1 of 3  FIN 3302 – Exam I Review Fall 2017 Chapter 1.docx
1 of 3  FIN 3302 – Exam I Review Fall 2017 Chapter 1.docx
1 of 3  FIN 3302 – Exam I Review Fall 2017 Chapter 1.docx
1 of 3  FIN 3302 – Exam I Review Fall 2017 Chapter 1.docx
1 of 3  FIN 3302 – Exam I Review Fall 2017 Chapter 1.docx
1 of 3  FIN 3302 – Exam I Review Fall 2017 Chapter 1.docx
1 of 3  FIN 3302 – Exam I Review Fall 2017 Chapter 1.docx
1 of 3  FIN 3302 – Exam I Review Fall 2017 Chapter 1.docx
1 of 3  FIN 3302 – Exam I Review Fall 2017 Chapter 1.docx
1 of 3  FIN 3302 – Exam I Review Fall 2017 Chapter 1.docx
1 of 3  FIN 3302 – Exam I Review Fall 2017 Chapter 1.docx
1 of 3  FIN 3302 – Exam I Review Fall 2017 Chapter 1.docx
1 of 3  FIN 3302 – Exam I Review Fall 2017 Chapter 1.docx
1 of 3  FIN 3302 – Exam I Review Fall 2017 Chapter 1.docx
1 of 3  FIN 3302 – Exam I Review Fall 2017 Chapter 1.docx
1 of 3  FIN 3302 – Exam I Review Fall 2017 Chapter 1.docx
1 of 3  FIN 3302 – Exam I Review Fall 2017 Chapter 1.docx
1 of 3  FIN 3302 – Exam I Review Fall 2017 Chapter 1.docx
1 of 3  FIN 3302 – Exam I Review Fall 2017 Chapter 1.docx
1 of 3  FIN 3302 – Exam I Review Fall 2017 Chapter 1.docx
1 of 3  FIN 3302 – Exam I Review Fall 2017 Chapter 1.docx
1 of 3  FIN 3302 – Exam I Review Fall 2017 Chapter 1.docx
1 of 3  FIN 3302 – Exam I Review Fall 2017 Chapter 1.docx
1 of 3  FIN 3302 – Exam I Review Fall 2017 Chapter 1.docx
1 of 3  FIN 3302 – Exam I Review Fall 2017 Chapter 1.docx
1 of 3  FIN 3302 – Exam I Review Fall 2017 Chapter 1.docx
1 of 3  FIN 3302 – Exam I Review Fall 2017 Chapter 1.docx
1 of 3  FIN 3302 – Exam I Review Fall 2017 Chapter 1.docx
1 of 3  FIN 3302 – Exam I Review Fall 2017 Chapter 1.docx
1 of 3  FIN 3302 – Exam I Review Fall 2017 Chapter 1.docx
1 of 3  FIN 3302 – Exam I Review Fall 2017 Chapter 1.docx
1 of 3  FIN 3302 – Exam I Review Fall 2017 Chapter 1.docx
1 of 3  FIN 3302 – Exam I Review Fall 2017 Chapter 1.docx
1 of 3  FIN 3302 – Exam I Review Fall 2017 Chapter 1.docx
1 of 3  FIN 3302 – Exam I Review Fall 2017 Chapter 1.docx
1 of 3  FIN 3302 – Exam I Review Fall 2017 Chapter 1.docx
1 of 3  FIN 3302 – Exam I Review Fall 2017 Chapter 1.docx
1 of 3  FIN 3302 – Exam I Review Fall 2017 Chapter 1.docx
1 of 3  FIN 3302 – Exam I Review Fall 2017 Chapter 1.docx
1 of 3  FIN 3302 – Exam I Review Fall 2017 Chapter 1.docx
1 of 3  FIN 3302 – Exam I Review Fall 2017 Chapter 1.docx
1 of 3  FIN 3302 – Exam I Review Fall 2017 Chapter 1.docx
1 of 3  FIN 3302 – Exam I Review Fall 2017 Chapter 1.docx
1 of 3  FIN 3302 – Exam I Review Fall 2017 Chapter 1.docx
1 of 3  FIN 3302 – Exam I Review Fall 2017 Chapter 1.docx
1 of 3  FIN 3302 – Exam I Review Fall 2017 Chapter 1.docx
1 of 3  FIN 3302 – Exam I Review Fall 2017 Chapter 1.docx
1 of 3  FIN 3302 – Exam I Review Fall 2017 Chapter 1.docx
1 of 3  FIN 3302 – Exam I Review Fall 2017 Chapter 1.docx
1 of 3  FIN 3302 – Exam I Review Fall 2017 Chapter 1.docx
1 of 3  FIN 3302 – Exam I Review Fall 2017 Chapter 1.docx
1 of 3  FIN 3302 – Exam I Review Fall 2017 Chapter 1.docx
1 of 3  FIN 3302 – Exam I Review Fall 2017 Chapter 1.docx
1 of 3  FIN 3302 – Exam I Review Fall 2017 Chapter 1.docx
1 of 3  FIN 3302 – Exam I Review Fall 2017 Chapter 1.docx
1 of 3  FIN 3302 – Exam I Review Fall 2017 Chapter 1.docx
1 of 3  FIN 3302 – Exam I Review Fall 2017 Chapter 1.docx
1 of 3  FIN 3302 – Exam I Review Fall 2017 Chapter 1.docx
1 of 3  FIN 3302 – Exam I Review Fall 2017 Chapter 1.docx
1 of 3  FIN 3302 – Exam I Review Fall 2017 Chapter 1.docx
1 of 3  FIN 3302 – Exam I Review Fall 2017 Chapter 1.docx
1 of 3  FIN 3302 – Exam I Review Fall 2017 Chapter 1.docx
1 of 3  FIN 3302 – Exam I Review Fall 2017 Chapter 1.docx
1 of 3  FIN 3302 – Exam I Review Fall 2017 Chapter 1.docx
1 of 3  FIN 3302 – Exam I Review Fall 2017 Chapter 1.docx
1 of 3  FIN 3302 – Exam I Review Fall 2017 Chapter 1.docx
1 of 3  FIN 3302 – Exam I Review Fall 2017 Chapter 1.docx
1 of 3  FIN 3302 – Exam I Review Fall 2017 Chapter 1.docx
1 of 3  FIN 3302 – Exam I Review Fall 2017 Chapter 1.docx
1 of 3  FIN 3302 – Exam I Review Fall 2017 Chapter 1.docx
1 of 3  FIN 3302 – Exam I Review Fall 2017 Chapter 1.docx
1 of 3  FIN 3302 – Exam I Review Fall 2017 Chapter 1.docx
1 of 3  FIN 3302 – Exam I Review Fall 2017 Chapter 1.docx
1 of 3  FIN 3302 – Exam I Review Fall 2017 Chapter 1.docx
1 of 3  FIN 3302 – Exam I Review Fall 2017 Chapter 1.docx
1 of 3  FIN 3302 – Exam I Review Fall 2017 Chapter 1.docx
1 of 3  FIN 3302 – Exam I Review Fall 2017 Chapter 1.docx
1 of 3  FIN 3302 – Exam I Review Fall 2017 Chapter 1.docx
1 of 3  FIN 3302 – Exam I Review Fall 2017 Chapter 1.docx
1 of 3  FIN 3302 – Exam I Review Fall 2017 Chapter 1.docx
1 of 3  FIN 3302 – Exam I Review Fall 2017 Chapter 1.docx

More Related Content

Similar to 1 of 3 FIN 3302 – Exam I Review Fall 2017 Chapter 1.docx

Running head 1-3 FINAL PROJECT MILESTONE ONE .docx
Running head 1-3 FINAL PROJECT MILESTONE ONE                    .docxRunning head 1-3 FINAL PROJECT MILESTONE ONE                    .docx
Running head 1-3 FINAL PROJECT MILESTONE ONE .docxWilheminaRossi174
 
Week 3 - AssignmeCountry Choice for the Final PaperReview .docx
Week 3 - AssignmeCountry Choice for the Final PaperReview .docxWeek 3 - AssignmeCountry Choice for the Final PaperReview .docx
Week 3 - AssignmeCountry Choice for the Final PaperReview .docxendawalling
 
TheHighSchoolDropout_LitReview
TheHighSchoolDropout_LitReviewTheHighSchoolDropout_LitReview
TheHighSchoolDropout_LitReviewJessica Riley
 
Running head IDENTIFYING THE POLICY PROBLEM1IDENTIFYING THE .docx
Running head IDENTIFYING THE POLICY PROBLEM1IDENTIFYING THE .docxRunning head IDENTIFYING THE POLICY PROBLEM1IDENTIFYING THE .docx
Running head IDENTIFYING THE POLICY PROBLEM1IDENTIFYING THE .docxwlynn1
 
Running head IDENTIFYING THE POLICY PROBLEM1IDENTIFYING THE .docx
Running head IDENTIFYING THE POLICY PROBLEM1IDENTIFYING THE .docxRunning head IDENTIFYING THE POLICY PROBLEM1IDENTIFYING THE .docx
Running head IDENTIFYING THE POLICY PROBLEM1IDENTIFYING THE .docxjeanettehully
 
Its Generational Rotary Relationships
Its Generational Rotary RelationshipsIts Generational Rotary Relationships
Its Generational Rotary RelationshipsRILearn
 
Running head MENACE TO SOCIETY .docx
Running head MENACE TO SOCIETY                                .docxRunning head MENACE TO SOCIETY                                .docx
Running head MENACE TO SOCIETY .docxcowinhelen
 
Urban Planning Thesis
Urban Planning ThesisUrban Planning Thesis
Urban Planning ThesisMark Talley
 
Essay Writing Quiz Pdf
Essay Writing Quiz PdfEssay Writing Quiz Pdf
Essay Writing Quiz PdfAmy Bryant
 
Annamaria Lusardi-Educación financiera. Una evidencia internacional
Annamaria Lusardi-Educación financiera. Una evidencia internacionalAnnamaria Lusardi-Educación financiera. Una evidencia internacional
Annamaria Lusardi-Educación financiera. Una evidencia internacionalFundación Ramón Areces
 
BUSI 352Case Study 2Your client, Steven, age 43, has come to.docx
BUSI 352Case Study 2Your client, Steven, age 43, has come to.docxBUSI 352Case Study 2Your client, Steven, age 43, has come to.docx
BUSI 352Case Study 2Your client, Steven, age 43, has come to.docxfelicidaddinwoodie
 
Connecting Education, Welfare, and Health for American Families - Peabody Jou...
Connecting Education, Welfare, and Health for American Families - Peabody Jou...Connecting Education, Welfare, and Health for American Families - Peabody Jou...
Connecting Education, Welfare, and Health for American Families - Peabody Jou...Janet Mulvey
 

Similar to 1 of 3 FIN 3302 – Exam I Review Fall 2017 Chapter 1.docx (12)

Running head 1-3 FINAL PROJECT MILESTONE ONE .docx
Running head 1-3 FINAL PROJECT MILESTONE ONE                    .docxRunning head 1-3 FINAL PROJECT MILESTONE ONE                    .docx
Running head 1-3 FINAL PROJECT MILESTONE ONE .docx
 
Week 3 - AssignmeCountry Choice for the Final PaperReview .docx
Week 3 - AssignmeCountry Choice for the Final PaperReview .docxWeek 3 - AssignmeCountry Choice for the Final PaperReview .docx
Week 3 - AssignmeCountry Choice for the Final PaperReview .docx
 
TheHighSchoolDropout_LitReview
TheHighSchoolDropout_LitReviewTheHighSchoolDropout_LitReview
TheHighSchoolDropout_LitReview
 
Running head IDENTIFYING THE POLICY PROBLEM1IDENTIFYING THE .docx
Running head IDENTIFYING THE POLICY PROBLEM1IDENTIFYING THE .docxRunning head IDENTIFYING THE POLICY PROBLEM1IDENTIFYING THE .docx
Running head IDENTIFYING THE POLICY PROBLEM1IDENTIFYING THE .docx
 
Running head IDENTIFYING THE POLICY PROBLEM1IDENTIFYING THE .docx
Running head IDENTIFYING THE POLICY PROBLEM1IDENTIFYING THE .docxRunning head IDENTIFYING THE POLICY PROBLEM1IDENTIFYING THE .docx
Running head IDENTIFYING THE POLICY PROBLEM1IDENTIFYING THE .docx
 
Its Generational Rotary Relationships
Its Generational Rotary RelationshipsIts Generational Rotary Relationships
Its Generational Rotary Relationships
 
Running head MENACE TO SOCIETY .docx
Running head MENACE TO SOCIETY                                .docxRunning head MENACE TO SOCIETY                                .docx
Running head MENACE TO SOCIETY .docx
 
Urban Planning Thesis
Urban Planning ThesisUrban Planning Thesis
Urban Planning Thesis
 
Essay Writing Quiz Pdf
Essay Writing Quiz PdfEssay Writing Quiz Pdf
Essay Writing Quiz Pdf
 
Annamaria Lusardi-Educación financiera. Una evidencia internacional
Annamaria Lusardi-Educación financiera. Una evidencia internacionalAnnamaria Lusardi-Educación financiera. Una evidencia internacional
Annamaria Lusardi-Educación financiera. Una evidencia internacional
 
BUSI 352Case Study 2Your client, Steven, age 43, has come to.docx
BUSI 352Case Study 2Your client, Steven, age 43, has come to.docxBUSI 352Case Study 2Your client, Steven, age 43, has come to.docx
BUSI 352Case Study 2Your client, Steven, age 43, has come to.docx
 
Connecting Education, Welfare, and Health for American Families - Peabody Jou...
Connecting Education, Welfare, and Health for American Families - Peabody Jou...Connecting Education, Welfare, and Health for American Families - Peabody Jou...
Connecting Education, Welfare, and Health for American Families - Peabody Jou...
 

More from oswald1horne84988

1 Network Analysis and Design This assignment is.docx
1  Network Analysis and Design  This assignment is.docx1  Network Analysis and Design  This assignment is.docx
1 Network Analysis and Design This assignment is.docxoswald1horne84988
 
1 Name _____________________________ MTH129 Fall .docx
1  Name  _____________________________ MTH129 Fall .docx1  Name  _____________________________ MTH129 Fall .docx
1 Name _____________________________ MTH129 Fall .docxoswald1horne84988
 
1 Lab 8 -Ballistic Pendulum Since you will be desig.docx
1  Lab 8 -Ballistic Pendulum Since you will be desig.docx1  Lab 8 -Ballistic Pendulum Since you will be desig.docx
1 Lab 8 -Ballistic Pendulum Since you will be desig.docxoswald1horne84988
 
1 I Samuel 8-10 Israel Asks for a King 8 When S.docx
1  I Samuel 8-10 Israel Asks for a King 8 When S.docx1  I Samuel 8-10 Israel Asks for a King 8 When S.docx
1 I Samuel 8-10 Israel Asks for a King 8 When S.docxoswald1horne84988
 
1 Journal Entry #9 What principle did you select .docx
1  Journal Entry #9 What principle did you select .docx1  Journal Entry #9 What principle did you select .docx
1 Journal Entry #9 What principle did you select .docxoswald1horne84988
 
1 HCA 448 Case 2 for 10042018 Recently, a pat.docx
1  HCA 448 Case 2 for 10042018 Recently, a pat.docx1  HCA 448 Case 2 for 10042018 Recently, a pat.docx
1 HCA 448 Case 2 for 10042018 Recently, a pat.docxoswald1horne84988
 
1 Chapter 2 Understanding Rhetoric Goal To re.docx
1  Chapter 2 Understanding Rhetoric  Goal To re.docx1  Chapter 2 Understanding Rhetoric  Goal To re.docx
1 Chapter 2 Understanding Rhetoric Goal To re.docxoswald1horne84988
 
1 HC2091 Finance for Business Trimester 2 2.docx
1  HC2091 Finance for Business      Trimester 2 2.docx1  HC2091 Finance for Business      Trimester 2 2.docx
1 HC2091 Finance for Business Trimester 2 2.docxoswald1horne84988
 
1 ECE 175 Computer Programming for Engineering Applica.docx
1  ECE 175 Computer Programming for Engineering Applica.docx1  ECE 175 Computer Programming for Engineering Applica.docx
1 ECE 175 Computer Programming for Engineering Applica.docxoswald1horne84988
 
1 Cinemark Holdings Inc. Simulated ERM Program .docx
1  Cinemark Holdings Inc. Simulated ERM Program  .docx1  Cinemark Holdings Inc. Simulated ERM Program  .docx
1 Cinemark Holdings Inc. Simulated ERM Program .docxoswald1horne84988
 
1 Figure 1 Picture of Richard Selzer Richard Selz.docx
1  Figure 1 Picture of Richard Selzer Richard Selz.docx1  Figure 1 Picture of Richard Selzer Richard Selz.docx
1 Figure 1 Picture of Richard Selzer Richard Selz.docxoswald1horne84988
 
1 Films on Africa 1. A star () next to a film i.docx
1  Films on Africa  1. A star () next to a film i.docx1  Films on Africa  1. A star () next to a film i.docx
1 Films on Africa 1. A star () next to a film i.docxoswald1horne84988
 
1 Contemporary Approaches in Management of Risk in .docx
1  Contemporary Approaches in Management of Risk in .docx1  Contemporary Approaches in Management of Risk in .docx
1 Contemporary Approaches in Management of Risk in .docxoswald1horne84988
 
1 Assignment front Sheet Qualification Unit n.docx
1  Assignment front Sheet   Qualification Unit n.docx1  Assignment front Sheet   Qualification Unit n.docx
1 Assignment front Sheet Qualification Unit n.docxoswald1horne84988
 
1 BBS300 Empirical Research Methods for Business .docx
1  BBS300 Empirical  Research  Methods  for  Business .docx1  BBS300 Empirical  Research  Methods  for  Business .docx
1 BBS300 Empirical Research Methods for Business .docxoswald1horne84988
 
1 ASSIGNMENT 7 C – MERGING DATA FILES IN STATA Do.docx
1  ASSIGNMENT 7 C – MERGING DATA FILES IN STATA Do.docx1  ASSIGNMENT 7 C – MERGING DATA FILES IN STATA Do.docx
1 ASSIGNMENT 7 C – MERGING DATA FILES IN STATA Do.docxoswald1horne84988
 
1 Assessment details for ALL students Assessment item.docx
1  Assessment details for ALL students Assessment item.docx1  Assessment details for ALL students Assessment item.docx
1 Assessment details for ALL students Assessment item.docxoswald1horne84988
 
1 CDU APA 6th Referencing Style Guide (Febru.docx
1  CDU APA 6th  Referencing Style Guide (Febru.docx1  CDU APA 6th  Referencing Style Guide (Febru.docx
1 CDU APA 6th Referencing Style Guide (Febru.docxoswald1horne84988
 
1 BIOL 102 Lab 9 Simulated ABO and Rh Blood Typing.docx
1  BIOL 102 Lab 9 Simulated ABO and Rh Blood Typing.docx1  BIOL 102 Lab 9 Simulated ABO and Rh Blood Typing.docx
1 BIOL 102 Lab 9 Simulated ABO and Rh Blood Typing.docxoswald1horne84988
 
1 Business Intelligence Case Project Backgro.docx
1  Business Intelligence Case    Project Backgro.docx1  Business Intelligence Case    Project Backgro.docx
1 Business Intelligence Case Project Backgro.docxoswald1horne84988
 

More from oswald1horne84988 (20)

1 Network Analysis and Design This assignment is.docx
1  Network Analysis and Design  This assignment is.docx1  Network Analysis and Design  This assignment is.docx
1 Network Analysis and Design This assignment is.docx
 
1 Name _____________________________ MTH129 Fall .docx
1  Name  _____________________________ MTH129 Fall .docx1  Name  _____________________________ MTH129 Fall .docx
1 Name _____________________________ MTH129 Fall .docx
 
1 Lab 8 -Ballistic Pendulum Since you will be desig.docx
1  Lab 8 -Ballistic Pendulum Since you will be desig.docx1  Lab 8 -Ballistic Pendulum Since you will be desig.docx
1 Lab 8 -Ballistic Pendulum Since you will be desig.docx
 
1 I Samuel 8-10 Israel Asks for a King 8 When S.docx
1  I Samuel 8-10 Israel Asks for a King 8 When S.docx1  I Samuel 8-10 Israel Asks for a King 8 When S.docx
1 I Samuel 8-10 Israel Asks for a King 8 When S.docx
 
1 Journal Entry #9 What principle did you select .docx
1  Journal Entry #9 What principle did you select .docx1  Journal Entry #9 What principle did you select .docx
1 Journal Entry #9 What principle did you select .docx
 
1 HCA 448 Case 2 for 10042018 Recently, a pat.docx
1  HCA 448 Case 2 for 10042018 Recently, a pat.docx1  HCA 448 Case 2 for 10042018 Recently, a pat.docx
1 HCA 448 Case 2 for 10042018 Recently, a pat.docx
 
1 Chapter 2 Understanding Rhetoric Goal To re.docx
1  Chapter 2 Understanding Rhetoric  Goal To re.docx1  Chapter 2 Understanding Rhetoric  Goal To re.docx
1 Chapter 2 Understanding Rhetoric Goal To re.docx
 
1 HC2091 Finance for Business Trimester 2 2.docx
1  HC2091 Finance for Business      Trimester 2 2.docx1  HC2091 Finance for Business      Trimester 2 2.docx
1 HC2091 Finance for Business Trimester 2 2.docx
 
1 ECE 175 Computer Programming for Engineering Applica.docx
1  ECE 175 Computer Programming for Engineering Applica.docx1  ECE 175 Computer Programming for Engineering Applica.docx
1 ECE 175 Computer Programming for Engineering Applica.docx
 
1 Cinemark Holdings Inc. Simulated ERM Program .docx
1  Cinemark Holdings Inc. Simulated ERM Program  .docx1  Cinemark Holdings Inc. Simulated ERM Program  .docx
1 Cinemark Holdings Inc. Simulated ERM Program .docx
 
1 Figure 1 Picture of Richard Selzer Richard Selz.docx
1  Figure 1 Picture of Richard Selzer Richard Selz.docx1  Figure 1 Picture of Richard Selzer Richard Selz.docx
1 Figure 1 Picture of Richard Selzer Richard Selz.docx
 
1 Films on Africa 1. A star () next to a film i.docx
1  Films on Africa  1. A star () next to a film i.docx1  Films on Africa  1. A star () next to a film i.docx
1 Films on Africa 1. A star () next to a film i.docx
 
1 Contemporary Approaches in Management of Risk in .docx
1  Contemporary Approaches in Management of Risk in .docx1  Contemporary Approaches in Management of Risk in .docx
1 Contemporary Approaches in Management of Risk in .docx
 
1 Assignment front Sheet Qualification Unit n.docx
1  Assignment front Sheet   Qualification Unit n.docx1  Assignment front Sheet   Qualification Unit n.docx
1 Assignment front Sheet Qualification Unit n.docx
 
1 BBS300 Empirical Research Methods for Business .docx
1  BBS300 Empirical  Research  Methods  for  Business .docx1  BBS300 Empirical  Research  Methods  for  Business .docx
1 BBS300 Empirical Research Methods for Business .docx
 
1 ASSIGNMENT 7 C – MERGING DATA FILES IN STATA Do.docx
1  ASSIGNMENT 7 C – MERGING DATA FILES IN STATA Do.docx1  ASSIGNMENT 7 C – MERGING DATA FILES IN STATA Do.docx
1 ASSIGNMENT 7 C – MERGING DATA FILES IN STATA Do.docx
 
1 Assessment details for ALL students Assessment item.docx
1  Assessment details for ALL students Assessment item.docx1  Assessment details for ALL students Assessment item.docx
1 Assessment details for ALL students Assessment item.docx
 
1 CDU APA 6th Referencing Style Guide (Febru.docx
1  CDU APA 6th  Referencing Style Guide (Febru.docx1  CDU APA 6th  Referencing Style Guide (Febru.docx
1 CDU APA 6th Referencing Style Guide (Febru.docx
 
1 BIOL 102 Lab 9 Simulated ABO and Rh Blood Typing.docx
1  BIOL 102 Lab 9 Simulated ABO and Rh Blood Typing.docx1  BIOL 102 Lab 9 Simulated ABO and Rh Blood Typing.docx
1 BIOL 102 Lab 9 Simulated ABO and Rh Blood Typing.docx
 
1 Business Intelligence Case Project Backgro.docx
1  Business Intelligence Case    Project Backgro.docx1  Business Intelligence Case    Project Backgro.docx
1 Business Intelligence Case Project Backgro.docx
 

Recently uploaded

Hybridoma Technology ( Production , Purification , and Application )
Hybridoma Technology  ( Production , Purification , and Application  ) Hybridoma Technology  ( Production , Purification , and Application  )
Hybridoma Technology ( Production , Purification , and Application ) Sakshi Ghasle
 
Paris 2024 Olympic Geographies - an activity
Paris 2024 Olympic Geographies - an activityParis 2024 Olympic Geographies - an activity
Paris 2024 Olympic Geographies - an activityGeoBlogs
 
Q4-W6-Restating Informational Text Grade 3
Q4-W6-Restating Informational Text Grade 3Q4-W6-Restating Informational Text Grade 3
Q4-W6-Restating Informational Text Grade 3JemimahLaneBuaron
 
Introduction to ArtificiaI Intelligence in Higher Education
Introduction to ArtificiaI Intelligence in Higher EducationIntroduction to ArtificiaI Intelligence in Higher Education
Introduction to ArtificiaI Intelligence in Higher Educationpboyjonauth
 
Alper Gobel In Media Res Media Component
Alper Gobel In Media Res Media ComponentAlper Gobel In Media Res Media Component
Alper Gobel In Media Res Media ComponentInMediaRes1
 
mini mental status format.docx
mini    mental       status     format.docxmini    mental       status     format.docx
mini mental status format.docxPoojaSen20
 
Presiding Officer Training module 2024 lok sabha elections
Presiding Officer Training module 2024 lok sabha electionsPresiding Officer Training module 2024 lok sabha elections
Presiding Officer Training module 2024 lok sabha electionsanshu789521
 
SOCIAL AND HISTORICAL CONTEXT - LFTVD.pptx
SOCIAL AND HISTORICAL CONTEXT - LFTVD.pptxSOCIAL AND HISTORICAL CONTEXT - LFTVD.pptx
SOCIAL AND HISTORICAL CONTEXT - LFTVD.pptxiammrhaywood
 
_Math 4-Q4 Week 5.pptx Steps in Collecting Data
_Math 4-Q4 Week 5.pptx Steps in Collecting Data_Math 4-Q4 Week 5.pptx Steps in Collecting Data
_Math 4-Q4 Week 5.pptx Steps in Collecting DataJhengPantaleon
 
BASLIQ CURRENT LOOKBOOK LOOKBOOK(1) (1).pdf
BASLIQ CURRENT LOOKBOOK  LOOKBOOK(1) (1).pdfBASLIQ CURRENT LOOKBOOK  LOOKBOOK(1) (1).pdf
BASLIQ CURRENT LOOKBOOK LOOKBOOK(1) (1).pdfSoniaTolstoy
 
microwave assisted reaction. General introduction
microwave assisted reaction. General introductionmicrowave assisted reaction. General introduction
microwave assisted reaction. General introductionMaksud Ahmed
 
A Critique of the Proposed National Education Policy Reform
A Critique of the Proposed National Education Policy ReformA Critique of the Proposed National Education Policy Reform
A Critique of the Proposed National Education Policy ReformChameera Dedduwage
 
Mastering the Unannounced Regulatory Inspection
Mastering the Unannounced Regulatory InspectionMastering the Unannounced Regulatory Inspection
Mastering the Unannounced Regulatory InspectionSafetyChain Software
 
Accessible design: Minimum effort, maximum impact
Accessible design: Minimum effort, maximum impactAccessible design: Minimum effort, maximum impact
Accessible design: Minimum effort, maximum impactdawncurless
 
Presentation by Andreas Schleicher Tackling the School Absenteeism Crisis 30 ...
Presentation by Andreas Schleicher Tackling the School Absenteeism Crisis 30 ...Presentation by Andreas Schleicher Tackling the School Absenteeism Crisis 30 ...
Presentation by Andreas Schleicher Tackling the School Absenteeism Crisis 30 ...EduSkills OECD
 
APM Welcome, APM North West Network Conference, Synergies Across Sectors
APM Welcome, APM North West Network Conference, Synergies Across SectorsAPM Welcome, APM North West Network Conference, Synergies Across Sectors
APM Welcome, APM North West Network Conference, Synergies Across SectorsAssociation for Project Management
 
Solving Puzzles Benefits Everyone (English).pptx
Solving Puzzles Benefits Everyone (English).pptxSolving Puzzles Benefits Everyone (English).pptx
Solving Puzzles Benefits Everyone (English).pptxOH TEIK BIN
 
The Most Excellent Way | 1 Corinthians 13
The Most Excellent Way | 1 Corinthians 13The Most Excellent Way | 1 Corinthians 13
The Most Excellent Way | 1 Corinthians 13Steve Thomason
 
MENTAL STATUS EXAMINATION format.docx
MENTAL     STATUS EXAMINATION format.docxMENTAL     STATUS EXAMINATION format.docx
MENTAL STATUS EXAMINATION format.docxPoojaSen20
 

Recently uploaded (20)

Hybridoma Technology ( Production , Purification , and Application )
Hybridoma Technology  ( Production , Purification , and Application  ) Hybridoma Technology  ( Production , Purification , and Application  )
Hybridoma Technology ( Production , Purification , and Application )
 
Paris 2024 Olympic Geographies - an activity
Paris 2024 Olympic Geographies - an activityParis 2024 Olympic Geographies - an activity
Paris 2024 Olympic Geographies - an activity
 
Q4-W6-Restating Informational Text Grade 3
Q4-W6-Restating Informational Text Grade 3Q4-W6-Restating Informational Text Grade 3
Q4-W6-Restating Informational Text Grade 3
 
Introduction to ArtificiaI Intelligence in Higher Education
Introduction to ArtificiaI Intelligence in Higher EducationIntroduction to ArtificiaI Intelligence in Higher Education
Introduction to ArtificiaI Intelligence in Higher Education
 
Alper Gobel In Media Res Media Component
Alper Gobel In Media Res Media ComponentAlper Gobel In Media Res Media Component
Alper Gobel In Media Res Media Component
 
mini mental status format.docx
mini    mental       status     format.docxmini    mental       status     format.docx
mini mental status format.docx
 
Presiding Officer Training module 2024 lok sabha elections
Presiding Officer Training module 2024 lok sabha electionsPresiding Officer Training module 2024 lok sabha elections
Presiding Officer Training module 2024 lok sabha elections
 
SOCIAL AND HISTORICAL CONTEXT - LFTVD.pptx
SOCIAL AND HISTORICAL CONTEXT - LFTVD.pptxSOCIAL AND HISTORICAL CONTEXT - LFTVD.pptx
SOCIAL AND HISTORICAL CONTEXT - LFTVD.pptx
 
_Math 4-Q4 Week 5.pptx Steps in Collecting Data
_Math 4-Q4 Week 5.pptx Steps in Collecting Data_Math 4-Q4 Week 5.pptx Steps in Collecting Data
_Math 4-Q4 Week 5.pptx Steps in Collecting Data
 
BASLIQ CURRENT LOOKBOOK LOOKBOOK(1) (1).pdf
BASLIQ CURRENT LOOKBOOK  LOOKBOOK(1) (1).pdfBASLIQ CURRENT LOOKBOOK  LOOKBOOK(1) (1).pdf
BASLIQ CURRENT LOOKBOOK LOOKBOOK(1) (1).pdf
 
Código Creativo y Arte de Software | Unidad 1
Código Creativo y Arte de Software | Unidad 1Código Creativo y Arte de Software | Unidad 1
Código Creativo y Arte de Software | Unidad 1
 
microwave assisted reaction. General introduction
microwave assisted reaction. General introductionmicrowave assisted reaction. General introduction
microwave assisted reaction. General introduction
 
A Critique of the Proposed National Education Policy Reform
A Critique of the Proposed National Education Policy ReformA Critique of the Proposed National Education Policy Reform
A Critique of the Proposed National Education Policy Reform
 
Mastering the Unannounced Regulatory Inspection
Mastering the Unannounced Regulatory InspectionMastering the Unannounced Regulatory Inspection
Mastering the Unannounced Regulatory Inspection
 
Accessible design: Minimum effort, maximum impact
Accessible design: Minimum effort, maximum impactAccessible design: Minimum effort, maximum impact
Accessible design: Minimum effort, maximum impact
 
Presentation by Andreas Schleicher Tackling the School Absenteeism Crisis 30 ...
Presentation by Andreas Schleicher Tackling the School Absenteeism Crisis 30 ...Presentation by Andreas Schleicher Tackling the School Absenteeism Crisis 30 ...
Presentation by Andreas Schleicher Tackling the School Absenteeism Crisis 30 ...
 
APM Welcome, APM North West Network Conference, Synergies Across Sectors
APM Welcome, APM North West Network Conference, Synergies Across SectorsAPM Welcome, APM North West Network Conference, Synergies Across Sectors
APM Welcome, APM North West Network Conference, Synergies Across Sectors
 
Solving Puzzles Benefits Everyone (English).pptx
Solving Puzzles Benefits Everyone (English).pptxSolving Puzzles Benefits Everyone (English).pptx
Solving Puzzles Benefits Everyone (English).pptx
 
The Most Excellent Way | 1 Corinthians 13
The Most Excellent Way | 1 Corinthians 13The Most Excellent Way | 1 Corinthians 13
The Most Excellent Way | 1 Corinthians 13
 
MENTAL STATUS EXAMINATION format.docx
MENTAL     STATUS EXAMINATION format.docxMENTAL     STATUS EXAMINATION format.docx
MENTAL STATUS EXAMINATION format.docx
 

1 of 3 FIN 3302 – Exam I Review Fall 2017 Chapter 1.docx

  • 1. 1 of 3 FIN 3302 – Exam I Review Fall 2017 Chapter 1 Chapter 5 -annual periods Chapter 6
  • 2. 2 of 3 Sample Questions 1) Which of the following goals of the firm are synonymous (equivalent) to the maximization of shareholder wealth? A) profit maximization B) risk minimization C) maximization of the total market value of the firm's common stock D) none of the above 2) You inherit $300,000 from your parents and want to use the money to supplement your retirement. You receive the money on your 65th birthday, the
  • 3. day you retire. You want to withdraw equal amounts at the end of each of the next 20 years. What constant amount can you withdraw each year and have nothing remaining at the end of 20 years if you are earning 7% interest per year? A) $15,000 B) $28,318 C) $33,574 D) $39,113 3) The risk-free rate of interest is 4% and the market risk premium is 9%. Howard Corporation has a beta of 2.0, and last year generated a return of 16% with a standard deviation of returns of 27%. The required return on Howard Corporation stock is A) 36%. B) 34%. C) 26%. D) 22%.
  • 4. 4) Today is your 21st birthday and your bank account balance is $25,000. Your account is earning 6.5% interest compounded monthly. How much will be in the account on your 50th birthday? A) $159,795 B) $162,183 C) $163,823 D) $164,631 5) All of the following statements about agency problems are true except: A) Agency problems interfere with the goal of maximizing shareholder value. B) Agency costs are paid by the managers who do not act in the shareholders' best interest. C) Agency problems result from the separation of management and the ownership of a firm. D) The root cause of agency problems is conflicts of interest.
  • 5. 3 of 3 6) Which of the following conclusions would be true if you earn a higher rate of return on your investments? A) The greater the present value would be for any lump sum you would receive in the future. B) The lower the present value would be for any lump sum you would receive in the future. C) Your rate of return would not have any effect on the present value of any sum to be received in the future. D) The greater the present value would be for any annuity you would receive in the future. 7) Investment A has an expected return of 14% with a standard deviation of 4%, while
  • 6. investment B has an expected return of 20% with a standard deviation of 9%. Therefore, A) a risk averse investor will definitely select investment A because the standard deviation is lower. B) a rational investor will pick investment B because the return adjusted for risk (20% - 9%) is higher than the return adjusted for risk for investment A ($14% - 4%). C) it is irrational for a risk-averse investor to select investment B because its standard deviation is more than twice as big as investment A's, but the return is not twice as big. D) rational investors could pick either A or B, depending on their level of risk aversion. 8) Joe purchased 800 shares of Robotics Stock at $3 per share on 1/1/15. He sold the shares on 12/31/15 for $3.45. Robotics stock has a beta of 1.9, the risk- free rate of return is 4%, and the market risk premium is 9%. Joe's holding period return is: A) 15.0%. B) 16.5%.
  • 7. C) 17.6%. D) 21.1%. Key 1. C 2. B 3. D 4. C 5. B 6. B 7. D 8. A PEDIATRIC NURSING/September-October 2017/Vol. 43/No. 5 213 I n August, our national news was filled with horrifying images of racism on display. If racism weren’t such a serious issue, seeing
  • 8. people marching with Tiki torches might have almost seemed comical. The Nazi flags were chilling. It was quite the deadly show of visual, ver- bal, physical, and emotional hatred that I never thought I would ever see again in my country. Yet with this event and similar ones that followed, we have learned how deeply racism is imbedded in American culture. Following the rebuke from the United Nations on human rights (Gearan & Wang, 2017). Malik (2017) pointed out the following: It took only eight months to go from a nation that voted for a black president two terms in a row to one that is suffering from race riots and killings, with officials having to send troops out on to the streets and declare a state of emer- gency. The speed with which it happened is the clue that it was, in fact, happening all along, unseen. And the fact that it was lying in wait is an indicator of how little racial equality is prized in the United States’ DNA. Malik (2017) explains why ostensi- bly developed countries, once faced with adversity, a vacuum of authority, or questionable leadership, tend to
  • 9. fall apart along the lines of race. In stable times of prosperity and rational leadership, “the promotion and en - shrining of the rights of the more vul- nerable is cosmetic at best, under- mined and papered over at worst. The foundations were there and contin- ued to be laid even during Obama’s leadership” (Malik, 2017). Such a sorry state of affairs. How does racism develop, what is its impact, and what can we do about it? The Development Of Racism Racism, a developed set of atti- tudes, includes antagonism based on the supposed superiority of one group or on the supposed inferiority of another group based solely on skin color or race (Beswick, 1990). No human being is born with racist, sex- ist, and other oppressive attitudes. Would children even notice differ- ences if no one said anything about them? Yes, they would, for the fol- lowing reasons (Rollins & Mahan, 2010, pp. 70-71): • Early on, children notice differ- ences and mentally organize these observations into categories. This is how young children make sense of their ever-expanding world.
  • 10. • Attitudes about “us and them” are learned and reinforced in the home, school, and church, and through the media. • By 3 years of age, children have learned to categorize people into “good or bad” based on superficial traits, such as race or gender. • Children 2 years of age or younger learn names of colors, then begin to apply these names to skin color. • By 3 years of age or even earlier, children can show signs of being influenced by what they see and hear around them. They may even pick up and exhibit “pre-preju- dice” toward others based on race or disability. • Children 4 and 5 years of age may use racial reasons for refusing to interact with others who are dif- ferent from themselves; they may act uncomfortable around or even reject people with disabilities. • By the time children enter ele- mentary school, they may have developed prejudices. Stereotypes remain until personal experience or someone attempts to correct them.
  • 11. Savard and Aragon (1989) tell us that parents are the earliest and most powerful source of racial attitudes (positive or negative); peers are a close second. Open-mindedness increases with age. The Impact of Racism On Health Gee, Walsemann, and Brondolo (2012) note studies that indicate racism may influence health inequities. Growing from infancy into old age, individuals encounter social institu- tions that may create new exposures to racial bias. Gee and colleagues (2012) view racism and health inequities from a life course perspective. They found that repeated exposure to moderate racial discrimination can cause illness after a time. Health can also be affected by social systems, such as education, the criminal justice system, and the labor market. Racism leads to housing and school segregation, limiting a per- son’s social network, and eventually, their employment opportunities and health. Over a lifetime, an individual who is subjected to racism has longer periods of unemployment or under- employment, incarceration, and/or illness. Compared to someone who has not experienced racism, someone who has may have a shorter career
  • 12. and retirement period, and eventual- ly, a shorter life expectancy. Racism also has an impact on the health of those who migrate to a new country. Although newly arrived immigrants are reported to have bet- ter health than non-immigrants in North America, their health declines The Depth of Racism in the U.S.: What It Means for Children Judy A. Rollins, PhD, RN From the Editor 214 PEDIATRIC NURSING/September-October 2017/Vol. 43/No. 5 with increased length of stay. First attributed to the process of adapting to a new culture (sedentary North American lifestyle and increased intake of fatty food), research now indicates decline may be due to racism (Na, 2012). Ausdale and Feagin (2001) note that early childhood is a crucial sensi- tive period when stressors such as racial discrimination may have an impact on an individual’s long-term well-being, affecting brain develop-
  • 13. ment and the formation of neutral connections between different regions. The brain and other parts of the body do not forget when bad things hap- pen in early life (Shonkoff, in Kuehn, 2014). In later years, children exposed to racial discrimination may perceive their own ethnic group negatively, become self-conscious, and develop low self-esteem and symptoms of depression (Na, 2012). Implications for Nursing Research suggests efforts nurses can take to help limit the effects of discrimination. Mossakowski (2003) found that the more strongly people identified with their own ethnic group, the less likely they were to dis- play symptoms of depression. This study showed that a stronger sense of ethnic identify meant having a sense of ethnic pride, being involved in eth- nic or cultural practices, and having knowledge about and commitment to the ethnic group. She concluded that ethnic identity not only directly pro- tects individuals from discrimination, but also buffers the stress of discrimi- nation on mental health. Nurses can support children’s eth- nic identify by learning as much as possible about the cultural back- grounds of the populations served by
  • 14. their practice that are different from their own. They can encourage chil- dren’s pride and self-esteem through an eagerness and curiosity to learn about children’s cultural heritage from the children themselves. Healthcare facilities can use art and design to cel- ebrate the various cultures served to provide a sense of welcome and pride for those who receive treatment there. Nurses should keep apprised of the community’s political, social, racial, and other related issues that could have an impact on children and teens in their practice. Talking to children about discrimination is important for their health and development, and nurses should not avoid discussing the topic. Supporting research-based cur- ricula for children, such as Teaching Tolerance, can also be encouraged. Conducting parenting classes could prove helpful. Research findings indi- cate that parents’ responses to their own experiences of racial discrimina- tion can influence their parenting behaviors (Sanders-Phillips, Settles- Reaves, Walker, & Brownlow, 2009). Parents who have experienced greater racial discrimination may become less sensitive to their children’s needs and less able to display affection, fail to prepare them for how to cope with
  • 15. discrimination, and use harsh disci- pline. At the same time, research tells us that more nurturing and involved parenting can weaken the adverse outcomes for youth associated with their own experience of discrimina- tion (Gibbons et al., 2010). We must never forget that whether it is visible or not, hate can be lurking just below the surface. As pediatric nurses, we can make a difference to diminishing racism and discrimina- tion in our daily interaction with patients and their families. References Ausdale, D.V., & Feagin, J.R. (2001). The first R: How children learn race and racism. Lanham, MD: Rowman and Littlefield Publishers. Beswick, R. (1990). Racism in America’s schools. ERIC Digest Series, EA 49. Retrieved from https://www.ericdigests. org/pre-9215/racism.htm Gearan, A., & Wang, A (2017, August 16) In veiled criticism of Trump, U.N. chief says racism is ‘poisoning our societies.’ The Washington Post. Retrieved from https://www.washingtonpost.com/world/ national-security/in-veiled-criticism-of- trump-un-chief-says-racism-is-poison-
  • 16. ing-our-societies/2017/ 08/ 16/ ddd03984- 8 2 a 5 - 11 e 7 - a b 2 7 - 1 a 2 1 a 8 e 0 0 6 a b _ story.html?utm_term=.962893eebdfe Gee, G.C., Walsemann, K.M., & Brondolo, E. (2012). A life course perspective on how racism may be related to health inequal- ities. American Journal of Public Health, 102, 967-974. Gibbons, F.X., Etcheverry, P.E., Stock, M.L., Gerrard, M., Weng, C., Kiviniemi, M., & O’Hara, R.E. (2010). Exploring the link between racial discrimination and sub- stance use: What mediates? What buffers? Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 99(5), 785-801. Kuehn, B. (2014). AAP: Toxic stress threat- ens kids’ long-term health. JAMA, 312(6), 585-586. Malik, N. (2017, August 25). After a UN warn- ing over racism, America’s self-image begins to crack. The Guardian. Retrieved from https://www.theguardian.com/com- mentisfree/2017/aug/25/un-warns-us- racism-but-trump-era-bigotry-not-blip- charlottesville Mossakowski, K. (2003). Coping with per- ceived discrimination: Does ethnic iden- tity protect mental health? Journal of Health and Social Behavior, 44, 318- 331.
  • 17. Na, L. (2012). Children and racism: The long- term impact. Retrieved from http:// www.aboutkidshealth.ca/En/News/ NewsAndFeatures/Pages/children- racism-long-term-impacts.aspx Rollins, J., & Mahan, C. (2010). From artist to artist in residence: Preparing artists to work in pediatric healthcare settings. Washington, DC: Rollins & Associates. Sanders-Phillips, K., Settles-Reaves, B., Walker, D., & Brownlow, J. (2009). Social inequality and racial discrimina- tion: Risk factors for health disparities in children of color. Pediatrics, 124(3), S176-S186. Savard, W.G., & Aragon, A. (1989). Racial Justice Survey Final Report. Portland, OR: Northwest Regional Educational Laboratory. Copyright of Pediatric Nursing is the property of Jannetti Publications, Inc. and its content may not be copied or emailed to multiple sites or posted to a listserv without the copyright holder's express written permission. However, users may print, download, or email articles for individual use.
  • 18. Racism’s Uprooting White Racial Jeopardy and Democracy To show what is beneficial, what is obligatory, what is good — that is the task of education. Education concerns itself w ith the motives for effective action. For no action is ever carried out in the absence of motives capable of supplying the indispensable amount of energy for its execution. — Simone Weil, The Need for Roots 14 5 B LA C K R E N A IS S
  • 19. A N C E N O IR E To varying degrees, the approaches used to teach anti-racism fall short of their intended goals, because of the tacit belief that non-Whites alone are harmed by racism, while Whites reap only its benefits. This perspective is at odds with, and unable to fully capture, many aspects o f our national history and o f our current reality. It cannot, for example, account for the race-induced political behavior o f poor and working-class Whites. For several decades now, that behavior has shown itself to be pointedly self-endangering, culminating with the election o f Donald J. Trump to succeed President Barack Obama. The anguished spectacle o f poor and
  • 20. working-class Whites vacating their economic and medical (self-)interest in order to conform to race-induced opposition to the pejoratively renamed “Obamacare,” while pleading for (the continuance of) Affordable Care, is revealing. It indicates that for many, racism has destroyed the ability to define, assert, or defend their own survival interests. Despite numerous similar inconsistencies, the traditional designation o f victims and beneficiaries remains unchanged, perhaps, because it is consistent with racism’s definition o f the powerless and the powerful. Insights on the psychology of oppression developed by the twentieth century French philosopher Simone Weil offer a way out o f this impasse. Weil’s analysis provides an urgently needed framework for re-examining racism, one that allows for a more complex understanding o f the ways in which people with W hite racial privilege are endangered by that privilege. In the collection o f essays translated and published posthumously as The N eed fo r Roots: Prelude to a Declaration o f Duties towards M ankind, Weil declares that “To be rooted is perhaps the most im portant and least recognized need o f the hum an soul” (Weil 1978, 41). After describing the familial and social structures through
  • 21. which individuals are “rooted” so as to achieve moral, intellectual, and spiritual stability, Weil examines factors contributing to its opposite, what she calls “uprootedness.” Because she is writing during the Second World War, Weil focuses on contemporary causes o f uprootedness, such as military conquest and colonialism. W hile she does not mention racism as a cause o f uprootedness, numerous studies have confirmed racisms uprooting effects, most notably the doll studies developed by Kenneth Clark and Mamie Phipps Clark that were used in the Brown v. Board o f Education case to end state-sponsored school segregation in the 1950s.' Contrary to expectations of preference based on “similarity bias,” the Black children in the Clark study showed a distinct preference for and favorable view o f W hite dolls and a marked rejection o f the Black dolls with which they nevertheless identified. I will return to this point later in the essay, as I examine the “narrative relationships” deployed to destroy self-image and self-esteem, and — in the process — generate a negative “similarity bias.” Suffice it to say, Weil’s description o f uprootedness as a “self-propagating” social disorder illuminates one o f the least understood, least examined aspects
  • 22. o f the ideology o f race — its injurious psychological, and perhaps neural, impact on individuals and groups positioned to internalize, act out, and ostensibly, benefit from its suggestions. As she observes, “Uprootedness is by far the most dangerous malady to which hum an societies are exposed, for it is a self-propagating one__ Whoever is uprooted himself uproots others. Whoever is rooted himself doesn’t uproot others” (Weil 1978, 45). Weil’s analysis makes clear that racism’s uprooting o f Blacks and other racial minorities is inevitably the secondary effect o f racism’s uprooting o f Whites, a latent source o fW h ite racial jeopardy. That is to say, the ideology o f race must first have destroyed or dismantled certain vital hum an capacities within the W hite population, as an absolute pre-condition for the sustained violence o f racism. O f these capacities, perhaps the most im portant is the cognitive capacity to perceive non-W hite people’s equal humanity. Examining racism through the lenses provided by Weil’s analysis, new questions emerge. Exactly how are W hite people “uprooted” by racism? How is uprooting manifested or displayed? W hat are its psychological contours? And given the new developments in neuroscience research, can we go beyond a psychological
  • 23. mapping to identify the neural correlates o f racism? M ost importantly, given the long history during which it has remained undiagnosed and untreated, what are the long-term consequences o f racisms uprooting o f Whites? RE-EXAMINING RACISM’S DIVIDE In the following discussion, I attem pt to answer these questions by looking first at the pitfalls o f mainstream understandings and approaches to anti-racism and by applying recent developments in neuroscience research to expose racism and W hite privilege as dangers to W hites and, ultimately, to u.s. democracy. I argue that a more efficacious design of anti-racist education can only come from a comprehensive understanding o f the jeopardy we all face from racism’s uprooting effects. For W hites, racism and W hite privilege threaten those competencies that enable and sustain healthy social interactions, professional performance, and democratic praxis in a multiracial society, namely, effective moral reasoning, moral decision-making, historical thinking, social literacy, and their prerequisites — emotional engagement and narrative knowledge.
  • 24. W ith few exceptions, scholarly analyses o f racism are shaped by binary assumptions about its impact: Whites reap the benefit o f W hite privilege, while Native Americans, African Americans, Latinos, Asians, Muslims2, and others are harmed by it. W hen injury to W hites is mentioned, it is generally no more significant than the harm o f a guilty conscience. In the best-seller Why Are A ll the Black Kids Sitting Together in the Cafeteria? A n d Other Conversations About Race, psychologist Beverly Tatum recounts an exchange with a student that underscores the shortcomings o f most current approaches to anti-racism. In examining “The Cost o f Racism,” Tatum describes a W hite male student who, after completing her psychology o f racism course, confessed that he “now understood in a way he never had before just how advantaged he was” (Tatum 1997,13) because o f racism. The student also shared that “he didn’t think he would do anything to try to change the situation. After all, the system was working in his favor” (Tatum 1997,13). After noting that this response was an anomaly, Tatum goes on to say that it, nevertheless, raised two im portant questions: “W hy should Whites who
  • 25. are advantaged by racism want to end that system o f advantage? [And] W hat are the costs o f that system to them?” (Tatum 1997,13). Tatum answers first with a financial accounting o f what racism costs the u.s. economy, citing a 1989 article, “Race and Money,” that appeared in Money magazine. After furnishing anecdotal evidence o f emotional loss suffered by W hite women and men, Tatum asserts: “W hite people are paying a significant price for the system o f advantage. The cost is not as high for W hites as it is for people o f color, but a price is being paid” (Tatum 1997,14). Since I first read Tatum’s account of her W hite male student’s response and o f the questions it prom pted, I have remained fascinated by both. I am fascinated by the student’s response because, it supplies language for what I believe is a widely held view: that when W hite people commit to anti-racism, they are acting against their own rational self-interest or are doing so as an expression o f goodwill toward Blacks, Latinos, Native Americans, Muslims, and others who are disadvantaged by race and racism. The suggestion that racism is a threat to Whites, that they too are in danger o f being uprooted by it, is still a novel one.
  • 26. I am fascinated by Tatum’s response because she never poses what I view as the more urgent questions: why did her student not recognize what the system was costing him? And how might she and other anti-racist educators be preventing this discovery in their legitimate focus on defining racism as a system o f advantage and disadvantage? W hile she does not address these questions, Tatum’s analysis does illustrate just how the myth o f W hite people’s imm unity to racism’s uprooting is perpetuated. In discussing how institutional racism hurt the u.s. economy and, thereby, W hite Americans, she mentions only those experiences o f economic harm that people disadvantaged by race experience, namely, “real estate equity lost through housing discrimination, or the tax revenue lost in underemployed communities o f color, or the high cost o f warehousing hum an talent in prison” (Tatum 1997,14). Here, Tatum 14 7 B LA C K R
  • 27. E N A IS S A N C E N O IR E suggests that the economic, social, and psychological injuries sustained by communities o f color am ount to a cost to the economy as a whole, which is the only measure o f (economic) cost to Whites. To the extent that there is only a collective — and not an individualized — economic cost for Whites, it must mean that relinquishing W hite privilege or (with more difficulty) overcoming racism can only be a gesture of goodwill or economic patriotism, not one o f self-preservation or self-interest on the part o f W hite
  • 28. individuals. Peggy M cIntosh’s widely respected essay, “W hite Privilege: Unpacking the Invisible Knapsack” (1989), provides further insight into the mythology of W hite people’s im m unity to racism’s uprooting effects. The essay opens with an epigraph in which McIntosh confesses, “I was taught to see racism only in individual acts o f meanness, not in invisible systems conferring dominance on my group.” The catalogue that follows is McIntosh’s attempt to delineate this “invisible system.” In listing what she calls the “Daily Effects o f W hite Privilege,” McIntosh does n ot include a single injury that might accompany the many “privileges” she identifies. In fact, the list displays a peculiar lack o f reflection on, and lack o f interest in, the harmful effects o f W hite privilege. McIntosh is able to avoid this discovery by a steadfast refusal to expose or identify the hum an agents implicated in the privileges she enumerates. Putting the spotlight on these persons, two details stand out. First, hatred and/or meanness cannot account for the spectrum of attitudes and actions she describes. Second, the persons dispensing or otherwise responsible for the privileges she enjoys have, to varying degrees, been (psychologically and morally) uprooted.
  • 29. Among the mundane examples o f W hite racial privilege McIntosh pulls out o f her invisible knapsack is the following: “I can go shopping alone most o f the time, pretty well assured that I will not be followed or harassed” (#4).3 The store clerk who does not follow McIntosh but who peeps at me through the fitting room door is not mean, does not hate me. Disabling narrative relationships — with non-W hite characters in television, cinematic, historical, and other narratives — have triggered emotions o f suspicion and impaired her ability to see me. Uprooted by racism, what she sees instead is a Black-female-and- therefore-shoplifter. Favorable narrative relationships with W hite characters have also blunted her ability to discern crime in White-face. So while she follows me around in a harassing performance o f helpfulness, the real shoplifter menaces the merchandise. Read carefully, one discovers that the privileges in McIntosh’s invisible knapsack have been stacked by “invisible” people psychologically and morally disabled by racism’s uprooting, people whose ability to accurately interpret human behaviors and motives have likewise been impaired. Her composition o f such a comprehensive listing o f assumed
  • 30. privileges can only mean that McIntosh knows the stackers, has seen or has been one o f them. If she had seriously analyzed the type o f impairment their actions or attitudes reflect, privilege #15 — and perhaps the entire essay — would have been framed differently. Instead o f simply asserting, "I do not have to educate my children to be aware o f systemic racism for their own daily physical protection,”4 McIntosh would have added, “But I do have to educate my children to be aware o f the daily threat racism and W hite privilege pose to their psychological, intellectual, and moral health.” Two defects weaken McIntosh’s anti-racist methodology: first, not identifying the people responsible for the racial privileges she enumerates, and second, not analyzing the full range of psychological dispositions involved in its operation. These defects also appear in the structural racism model championed by legal scholar John A. Powell, former director o f the Kirwan Institute for the Study o f Race and Ethnicity at Ohio State University. In “Structural Racism: Building upon the Insights o f John Calmore,” Powell asserts that “Racism need not be either intentional or individualist. Institutional practices and cultural patterns can perpetuate racial inequity
  • 31. w ithout relying on racist actors” (795). Contrasting institutional and structural racism, Powell notes: The institutional racism framework reflects a broader recognition o f the forms through which racialized power is deployed, dispersed, and entrenched. However, while illustrating the ways in which racism is often non-individualist and non-intentionalist, this framework focuses too heavily on intra-institutional dynamics, and thus fails to account for the ways in which the joint operations o f social institutions produce critical racialized outcomes. [...] Structural racism shifts our attention from the single, intra-institutional setting to inter-institutional arrangements and interactions. (795-796) As an intersectional approach, Powell’s structural racism model provides a means o f both identifying and countering the structures of compounding jeopardy that constrict
  • 32. opportunities for poor African American, Latino, Native American, and other non-W hite communities. However, in simultaneously reducing the m ultitude o f racist dispositions to the single stereotypical mindset o f “bad actors” and substituting a focus on residual effects o f presumably abandoned mechanics, the structural racism model moves the discourse from one shaky leg to another. The notion that structures can maintain a racist disposition and continue to produce racist outcomes w ithout the involvement o f racist hum an agents or that institutions do not reflect the vision or mindset o f the people by whom they are governed is simply not credible. Recent events involving the three branches of our own democratic government are instructive. O n the one hand, the machinations o f the executive branch demonstrate that if and when operated by individuals with an autocratic vision (or personality) democratic structures will not continue to support democratic practices or policies, or produce democratic outcomes. O n the other hand, recent rulings from the federal bench regarding the Trump administrations Muslim ban indicate that only when structures are governed by human agents committed to a democratic vision can
  • 33. democratic outcomes be assured. The same is true o f racism. As such, an approach that combines the critique o f structural racism with an examination o f hum an agents involved in its continuance would be anchored by two legs and ensure analytical symmetry. The discomfort underlying the refusal to expose hum an beings involved in the practice o f racism stems from an ahistorical and simplistic equation o f racism with hatred or meanness, as McIntosh admits to having been taught. Recalling Tatums W hite male student, however, it is clear that his resolution to preserve racism and, inevitably, to become a self-consciously racist agent was undertaken, one might say, “with malice toward none.” Nevertheless, defining racism solely as a system o f advantage to Whites creates several significant impediments to anti-racist education. First, this definition supports a senseless and binary positioning of W hite “donors” and non-W hite “debtors,” with opposing stakes. Second, it simultaneously and paradoxically entreats W hite “donors” to relinquish certain modes o f racism as a benefit to others and not on behalf
  • 34. o f their own psychological, intellectual, and ethical growth or liberation. Third, in so doing, it replicates the structure o f international relations in the post-colonial era where formerly colonized nations are defined as debtors and those former imperial powers enriched by the colonial enterprise are defined as donors. Fourth, it prevents, or else discourages, W hite people from acknowledging their own uprootedness, since doing so carries an automatic indictm ent for moral failure. Fifth, it prevents people with race-based privilege from discovering how they too are endangered by structures of thinking and structures of feeling that directly and indirectly endorse hierarchical distributions o f hum an worth and rights. Sixth, it fails to reveal the dangers and dispel the potential envy of a self-endangering racial privilege. Seventh, it fails to fully interpret the significance and value o f the intellectual, psychological, ethical, spiritual, and material resourcefulness manifested in African American, Asian, Latino, Native American and various non-
  • 35. Western cultural traditions and histories. In sum, the view o f racism as solely or primarily a system o f W hite racial advantage fails to adequately empower or motivate W hite, Asian, Native American, Latino, African American, and other non-W hite communities for the collaborative task o f ending racism. The misconception that W hite people are not individually harmed by racism, that it is not a threat to them, inhibits the mass com m itm ent and participation needed to effectively counter racisms uprooting. 14 9 B LA C K R EN A IS SA N C E
  • 36. N O IR E R S D : R A C I S M S P E C T R U M D I S O R D E R A key reason for W hite people’s seeming lack o f concern for their own racial jeopardy is the assumption that racism is a moral defect and that having a sufficient moral foundation confers immunity. Numerous historical examples, however, amply demonstrate that having an extensive moral foundation is no defense against racist indoctrination. Recall that despite being versed in the moral teachings o f the New Testament that include instructions to love your neighbor as yourself and to treat others as you would like to be treated, the earliest European arrivants to the Americas were unable to resist racist conscription. In tracing the history o f legislative violence in seventeenth-century Virginia from which race and racism ensued, political scientist Joel Olson notes that “Race as we now know it did n ot exist when the first colonists
  • 37. landed on the shores o f the New W orld” (Olson 2001,165). Racism, Olson asserts, “was a deliberate, public, conscious policy o f the Virginia ruling elite” (Olson 2001,167) invented to preempt cross-racial economic and political alliances. In the article titled “The Democratic Problem o f the W hite Citizen,” Olson recounts, Through a series o f acts from r670 to 1705, the Virginia assembly made laws distinguishing African and Indians from Europeans. They forbade Africans and Indians to own Christian servants and the legal definition o f “Christian” now excluded baptized African and Native Americans... Through various legislative measures and social pressures Virginia elites simultaneously fastened Africans to a lifetime, hereditary, degraded status and created a new group of relatively privileged people heretofore unknown in hum an history. Remarkably, these measures amassed rich and poor, planter and servant, esteemed and lowly into a single group unified not by
  • 38. ancestry but by the right to own property (including hum an property), the right to share in the public business, and a pledge to ensure the degraded position o f all those defined as black. (Olson 2001, 166-168) W hile the legislative acts that engineered racism and W hite racial privilege equalized the social status of all those defined as “W hite,” it did not equalize the economic and political interests o f poor W hites and the ruling (White) elite. That is to say, although the social identities o f W hite workers improved, their economic interests were neither aligned with nor prioritized by the ruling elite. In acquiescing to this new regime, therefore, poor W hites sacrificed their economic and political self-interest for the mostly symbolic “wages o f Whiteness.” In systematically discrediting the image o f non-Whites and enlisting the participation o f poor W hites in this project, the ruling elite launched a two-pronged psychological assault on non-Whites and on poor Whites. By assimilating to this new racialized self-definition, poor English workers — and later Irish, Italian, German, Polish, and other ethnic Europeans in the u.s. — relinquished their
  • 39. cultural distinctiveness, class-specific epistemological standpoint, and interpretive autonomy. Their acculturation to the illusion o f dominance shared with the ruling elite was the first indication o f racism’s ability to uproot, to impair intellectual agency and interpretive competence. In effect, W hite racial privilege was the nation’s first designer drug, consuming vital capacities — intellectual, psychological, moral — with every thrilling intake. The “pledge” among W hites to “ensure the degraded position” o f all those defined as non-W hite would find expression and fulfillment through narrative acts and the re-configuration o f “empathic bias.” This would involve scripting negative narrative relationships toward non-Whites and positive narrative relationships toward Whites sufficient to overshadow the empathic biases that would otherwise have evolved from their lived interactions and moral commitments. From the outset, therefore, racism was a psychological, not a moral, disorder dependent on a conditioned emotional detachment. W hile this detachment and resulting disorder can have moral effects and/or implications, the disorder is itself psychological with
  • 40. perhaps identifiable neural correlates. As a psycho-social spectrum disorder, racism stems from an involuntary and symmetrical blindness to one’s own and to other people’s human worth. It involves both conditioned emotional detachment and hyper attachment and produces disparate forms o f impairment and disability. From its foundational measurement o f hum an worth, racism establishes other “race”-based measurements o f hum an capacities, human achievements, hum an potential, human rights, and hum an responses. W hile racism’s blindness is typically assumed to manifest itself in attitudes and acts o f hatred or meanness, these are merely points on the racism spectrum. The socialized blindness that undergirds racism can manifest in a broad spectrum o f attitudes and actions, involving varying degrees o f emotional detachment or hyper-attachment towards persons positioned at various tiers on the hierarchy o f human worth and entitlement posited by the ideology o f race. Attitudes and actions toward persons positioned at or near the bottom o f the hierarchy o f hum an worth include, but are not limited to: suspicion, fear, unwarranted and frequently
  • 41. self-endangering mistrust, disinterest, apathy, a lack o f concern for actual and potential danger or injury to such persons, sexual attraction, curiosity, envy, a desire to help or rescue, a lack o f self-restraint or gentleness towards such persons, indifference or permissiveness about abuse towards such persons — a spectrum o f psychological dispositions conditioned by emotional detachment. Attitudes and actions toward persons positioned at or near the top of the hierarchy, and that constitute W hite racial privilege, include but are not limited to: feelings o f comfort, interestedness, empathy, unwarranted and frequently self-endangering trust, feelings o f concern and distress at real or imagined danger or injury to such persons, a sense o f the greater value, greater relevance o f their roles and contributions — a spectrum of psychological responses conditioned by hyper-attachment. As a psycho-social spectrum disorder, racism also involves a socialized blindness to the structures through which hierarchical distributions o f advantage, opportunity, immunity, and material resources are made. As such, it self-propagates through a concurrent blindness to its own operation, an effect with considerable strategic
  • 42. significance. As Wahneema Lubiano has observed, it allows W hite people to see themselves as acting morally when, according to their own moral tenets, they are not.5 The tendency to self-propagate, as Weil emphasizes, is precisely what makes uprootedness (through W hite racial privilege) “the most dangerous malady to which hum an societies are exposed” (Weil 1978, 45). The stark contrast between policy responses that criminalized the mostly African American and Latino crack addicts in the 1980s and 1990s and the policy responses seeking therapeutic options for todays mostly W hite heroin addicts is noteworthy. It attests to the differing degrees of emotional attachment to persons situated at different tiers on the hierarchy of hum an worth posited by the ideology o f race. If racism had not created an emotional detachment toward non-White people, politicians and policy-makers would not have been uprooted by this socialization and would have experienced feelings of concern, if not alarm, at the loss o f life among crack addicts. W ith feelings and emotion intact, they would have been able to exercise the type o f moral reasoning that would have provided a basis for acquiring “narrative
  • 43. knowledge” — to be discussed hereafter — in the quest for appropriate policy responses. If emotional detachment had not impaired policy makers’ ability to formulate effective moral judgm ent about the addicts o f the late twentieth century, the therapeutic solutions now being pursued would have been proposed then. Lives would have been saved then and, with structures already in place, lives would be saved now. 15 1 B LA CK R EN A IS SA N C E NO IR
  • 44. E EXPOSING THE NEUROSCIENCE OF RACISM Emotional detachment toward non-Whites is the foundation — and catalyst — for a vast structure of dystopian feeling toward the poor, prison inmates, the homeless, immigrants, low-wage workers, people with intellectual disabilities, and other groups defined as disposable. The broad spectrum o f impairment and disability resulting from the emotional detachment conditioned by racism becomes especially significant when viewed in relation to the work o f the renowned neuropsychologist Antonio Damasio on the role o f emotion and feeling in moral judgment. In presenting to the public his clinical findings that “certain aspects o f the process o f emotion and feeling are indispensable for rationality” (xiii) and “that the absence o f emotion and feeling is no less damaging” than emotional biases (xii), Damasio mentioned first his own cultural socialization with regard to rational decision-making. In Descartes’ Error (1994), Damasio writes,
  • 45. I had been advised early in life that sound decisions came from a cool head, that emotions and reason did not mix any more than oil and water. I had grown up accustomed to thinking that the mechanisms o f reason existed in a separate province o f the m ind, where emotion should not be allowed to intrude, and when I thought o f the brain behind the m ind, I envisioned separate neural systems for reason and emotion, (xi) The case that prom pted Damasio to re-think the role o f emotions and feeling in effective decision-making was that o f a patient who “had had an entirely healthy m ind until a neurological disease ravaged a specific sector o f his brain” causing a “profound defect in decision-making” (xii). According to Damasio, The instruments usually considered necessary and sufficient for rational behavior were intact in him. He had the requisite knowledge, attention, and memory; his language was flawless; he
  • 46. could perform calculations; he could tackle the logic o f an abstract problem. There was only one significant accompaniment to his decision-making failure: a marked alteration o f the ability to experience feelings. Flawed reason and impaired feelings stood out together as the consequences o f a specific brain lesion, and this correlation suggested to me that feeling was an integral component o f the machinery o f reason. Two decades o f clinical and experimental work with a large num ber o f neurological patients have allowed me to replicate this observation many times, and to turn a clue into a testable hypothesis. (xii) (Emphasis added.) In the years since its initial publication, several studies have confirmed Damasio’s “testable hypothesis.”6 Among other effects, these findings call into question the preferred formula for effective decision-making — one that excludes emotion and feeling — that has been disseminated and established as a requisite guideline in almost every arena o f public, professional, and personal decision-making from the law
  • 47. and law enforcement, to business, international relations, politics, economics, diplomacy, medicine, journalism, education, sports, marriage, and family life. Given the pervasive influence o f Descartes’ cogito on Western culture and its widespread cultural diffusion as a decision-making protocol, the neuroscience research confirming the essential role o f emotion and feeling in moral decision-making raises new questions. Has the cogito already “marked” what Western researchers view as a healthy/integrated brain? Can “impaired feelings” be caused by factors other than disease or physical injury to the brain? Can socialization, for example, impair or deactivate the brain’s emotional circuits? Can the gender socialization that males in particular receive about the incompatibility o f emotion and reason produce neural effects and a similar impairment o f feelings as those caused by injury or disease? Can race or gender socialization deactivate emotional systems o f the brain in general or with regard to specific stimuli? Conversely, are there deficits in neural processing in a healthy — non diseased, uninjured — brain that can be attributed — broadly speaking — to cultural socialization? Or, to p ut it succinctly, is socialized
  • 48. brain injury possible? W hat is the role o f emotion and feeling in public policy decision-making? To what extent might defective public policy decision-making be attributable to emotional detachment toward dispossessed and disposable constituencies? To the extent that emotion is essential to moral reasoning, how might emotional detachment affect historical thinking, social literacy, and the practice o f democracy, all of which involve moral reasoning? O f the many studies inspired by Damasio’s clinical findings, one o f the most instructive is a 2001 study by an interdisciplinary team o f investigators lead by Harvard professor Joshua Greene, at the time a doctoral student in Princeton’s Philosophy department. In the article “An fMRI Investigation o f Emotional Engagement in Moral Judgment,” Greene and his colleagues describe using functional magnetic resonance imaging — fMRI — to observe the neural activity o f test subjects faced with a variety o f decision-making scenarios involving “moral-personal conditions,” “moral-impersonal conditions,” and “non-moral conditions.”7 The study focused on two moral dilemmas representing moral-impersonal and moral-personal
  • 49. conditions, respectively. O ne involved a runaway trolley, the other a footbridge. As they describe the two dilemmas, A runaway trolley is headed for five people who will be killed if it proceeds on its present course. The only way to save them is to hit a switch that will turn the trolley onto an alternate set o f tracks where it will kill one person instead o f five.... [In] a similar problem, the footbridge dilem m a... a trolley threatens to kill five people. You are standing next to a large stranger on a footbridge that spans the tracks, in between the oncoming trolley and the five people. In this scenario, the only way to save the five people is to push this stranger off the bridge, onto the tracks below. He will die if you do this, but his body will stop the trolley from reaching the others. (2105) The researchers predicted the absence o f what they called “emotional interference” for responses to the impersonal trolley dilemma, as measured by both neural activity and
  • 50. faster response times. As expected, Greene and his colleagues found that most respondents said yes to re-directing the trolley onto an alternate track in the first (moral-impersonal) dilemma, while most respondents said no to pushing a stranger off the footbridge in the second (moral-personal) dilemma. They concluded that, .. .from a psychological point o f view, the crucial difference between the trolley dilemma and the footbridge dilemma lies in the latter’s tendency to engage people’s emotions in a way the former does not. The thought o f pushing someone to his death is, we propose, more emotionally salient than the thought o f hitting a switch that will cause a trolley to produce similar consequences, and it is this emotional response that accounts for people’s tendency to treat these cases differently. (2106) [Emphasis added.] Although not its intended objective, the Greene study offers a preliminary basis for speculating about the ways in which a particular socialization — whether shaped by race or any other ideology — might lead to the
  • 51. type o f emotional detachment that prevents the neurological engagement and activity essential for healthy decision-making about other people’s lives. This is im portant because racially-informed responses, misreadings, behaviors, and actions toward non-Whites are in some senses an accumulation o f emotionally detached (mis)judgments. Looking closely at the study, one notices that the “impersonal” character or framing o f the trolley dilemma seems to rest upon the social identities o f the people on the track and the empathic ability o f particular respondents, n o t on the dilemma itself. This can easily be illustrated by re-framing the dilemma, re-imagining the unknown person on the alternate track, and allowing that person to attain the same hum an distinction as the stranger on the footbridge whom test subjects have to touch. W ould test subjects be as emotionally detached in reasoning about the runaway trolley scenario if the person on the alternate track were an acquaintance — whether liked or disliked? Would they be as emotionally disengaged if the five were five Mexicans crossing the border illegally and the one were a u.s. border patrol agent? O r if the “trolley” were a grenade misfired from a u.s. military launcher and the five were five Afghan
  • 52. civilians and the one were a u.s. soldier? 15 3 B LA C K R EN A IS SA N C E NO IR E RE-VIEWING RACISM AS A THREAT TO DEMOCRACY My point here is not to suggest that under these different (narrative) frames test subjects in the u.s. would make
  • 53. different judgments and require longer response times in resolving the trolley dilemma, although this seems highly probable. Rather, my point is that to the extent that the variations suggested above elicit a more intense emotional engagement, they also reveal the social invisibility and/or disposability of the person on the alternate track and the resulting detachment with which test subjects were likely to have engaged the dilemma. Since social identities are charged — positively and negatively — with different emotional valences, moral judgm ent about those lives will inevitably be influenced and perhaps determined by these pre-set valences. These judgments are particularly fraught in professional and public policy deliberations. Re-examining the results o f the Greene study, the researchers seem to have overlooked one o f the most salient discoveries o f their own research: the neural similarity displayed between moral-impersonal and non-moral decision-making. If emotions are essential to moral reasoning, the observed non-involvement o f test subjects’ emotions in the trolley dilemma, as measured by neural activity and faster response times, would suggest that these decisions are neurologically defective. Thus, the researchers’ tacit acceptance o f this defect, their failure
  • 54. to read these responses as defective, is perhaps indicative o f persistent cultural assumptions — and misconceptions — about the limited utility o f emotions. Their reference to, and perhaps preference for, the absence o f “emotional interference” is revealing. If the neural and behavioral responses to the moral-impersonal trolley dilemma in the Greene study are (socio-culturally) representative, they may indicate a significant pattern o f emotional disengagement in moral-impersonal decision-making about socially invisible “strangers,” the type o f decision-making voters in a democratic society must routinely perform. This finding that neural responses to “moral-impersonal” and “non-moral” dilemmas involve similar types o f emotional disengagement is particularly troubling since professional and public policy decision-making tends to occur in situations that resemble the conditions o f moral-impersonal decision-making in which the people at risk on policy-making “trolley tracks” remain invisible. It should be obvious that an adult citizen ought n ot to approach — or (neurologically) experience — the choice o f whether or not to fund at-home healthcare options for people with disabilities (a moral-impersonal dilemma) in the
  • 55. same way as deciding whether to take the 6 p.m. or 7 p.m. flight from Washington, D C back to Kentucky or Wisconsin (a non-moral dilemma). And yet, this appears to be happening in the Congress on a daily basis. The Greene’s study and its overlooked implications provide a basis for theorizing about the neurological jeopardies underlying the intellectual habits that attend W hite racial privilege and their inevitable impact on the range o f decision-making responsibilities accompanying democratic participation and professional practice in a multi-racial society. Most discussions o f racism as a threat to democracy focus on the cliched assertion that, as an impediment to national reconciliation, it prevents u.s. Americans from achieving “a more perfect union.” W hile accurate, this summary view overlooks and overshadows the more consequential ways in which racism and W hite racial privilege jeopardize u.s. democracy by fostering inept historical thinking, social illiteracy, defective moral reasoning, defective (public policy) decision-making, and self-endangering voting behaviors. In large measure, these effects stem from the narrative relationships that precede and that are the blueprint for social interactions.
  • 56. Analyses o f the ways as prompts for racism tend to focus on the proliferation o f racist images and characterization. To fully understand racism as a psychological spectrum disorder, however, one must examine the emotional quality o f narrative relationships engineered through the distorted representations o f non-White and W hite peoples in historical, literary, theological, cinematic, and other narratives. Specifically, one must understand the ways in which narrative relationships function as both emotional guides and emotional triggers. Racism operates through distorted narrative relationships that undermine both narrative knowledge and social literacy. “Narrative knowledge” is the term used by narrative medicine pioneer and medical educator, Dr. Rita Charon, to describe a set o f critical competencies instilled by literary reading. In Narrative Medicine: Honoring the Stories o f Illness (200 6), Dr. Charon, an internal medicine specialist who later impaired by a long history o f racist earned a Ph.D. in English, notes that socialization. If narratives are stories that have a teller, a listener, a time
  • 57. course, a plot, and a point, then narrative knowledge is what we naturally use to make sense o f them. Narrative knowledge provides one person with a rich, resonant grasp o f another persons situation as it unfolds in time, whether in such texts as novels, newspaper stories, movies, and scripture or in such life settings as courtrooms, battlefields, marriages, and illnesses. (9)® As a key com ponent o f narrative medicine, narrative knowledge is envisioned as a remedy to a medical education process that many believe produces doctors with tremendous technical skills but who “lack the hum an capacities to recognize the plights o f their patients, to extend empathy toward those who suffer, and to join honestly and courageously with patients in their struggles toward recovery, with chronic illness, or in facing death” (3). In designing narrative medicine as a response to these deficits, Charon and other narrative medicine theorists recognized that “[u]sing narrative knowledge enables a person to understand the plight o f another by participating in his or her story with complex skills o f imagination, interpretation, and recognition” (9-10).
  • 58. It is my belief that the narrative knowledge supporting both interpretive and ethical competence in medical practice is equally im portant to rehabilitating the range o f social and professional interactions that have been In the article titled “How Narrative Relationships Overcome Empathic Bias,” literary scholar Mary-Catherine Harrison makes the case for narrative relationships as an empathy generator based on the claim that they can overcome the limitation of being tied to similarity bias — that is, the notion that people are more likely to empathize with those they view as similar to themselves. As she sees it, narrative relationships formed with fictional characters, especially in literature and cinema, can be an im portant mechanism for generating empathy by circumventing the similarity bias that is alleged to be normative. In presenting her argument, Harrison first outlines the normative parameters o f narrative relationships, similarity bias, and empathic bias, citing several studies that corroborate the association o f similarity bias with empathy, that confirm the importance o f empathy as a precursor to “helping behaviors,” and that associate the lack o f empathy with “psychopathy, criminality, aggression, and anti-social behavior” (256).9
  • 59. O ne pronounced omission in the studies Harrison mentions is a lack of attention to the ways in which racial narratives, since the seventeenth century, have re-ordered the importance o f race-based similarity and, thereby, re-configured the experience o f empathy.10 Recall that the experiences and preferences o f Black children in the Clark study did not conform to expectations o f preference/empathy based on similarity bias. If, as Harrison and other narrative theorists assert, narrative relationships can be a vehicle for overcoming similarity bias and generating empathy for people who don’t share prom inent social characteristics, it must be the case that narrative relationships o f a different sort were first used to construct and cultivate (racialized) similarity bias as a basis for empathy. Re-visiting the seventeenth century legislative acts used to craft race and racism, it is clear that these alone could n ot have generated the widespread emotional detachment toward non-W hite peoples and the hyper-attachment toward Whites. Narrative relationships were and are a vital mechanism for producing this outcome. Reverse sequencing from contemporary understandings of
  • 60. narrative relationships, empathy, and similarity bias, it is easy to see how Whites and non-W hites must have been uprooted by the narratives relationships with characters that peopled the W hite supremacist narratives in literature, law, science, theology, and cinema in the centuries and decades before the social transformations o f the Civil Rights era. Long before the narrative transformations o f the Black Arts Movement and related developments in the publishing industry enabling the distribution o f literary works by non-W hite writers, before curricular expansions prom pted by the demands o f Black college students in the 1960s, dominant cultural narratives constricted empathic parameters throughout the society. In The Bluest Eye, for example, Toni Morrison depicts how Pauline Breedlove’s positive narrative relationships with W hite characters in 1940s cinematic narratives overshadow her negative social interactions with 15 5 B LA C K
  • 61. R E N A IS S A N C E N O IR E hostile and/or racist Whites and produce a positive empathic bias toward them. This positive empathic bias generates caring behaviors toward her employers W hite daughter and a concurrent negative empathic bias toward and progressive emotional detachment from her own children. W ith no similarity bias to overcome, W hite consumers o f the period’s cinematic narratives would have been even more likely to have embraced
  • 62. the favored narrative relationships with W hite characters and the negative narrative relationships with cinematically “absent” or caricatured non-Whites, and to have internalized their corresponding emotional templates. The common belief today is that these mechanisms were destroyed with the mid-twentieth century civil rights movement. O ne must recall, however, that the legal remedies, including school desegregation and busing, that ended formal apartheid in the u.s. were not designed to repair the rampant uprootedness caused by centuries o f dedicated racism. Taking seriously Weil’s observation that uprootedness is a psycho-social disorder, it is difficult to imagine how it could have been healed or transformed in the absence o f deliberate therapeutic interventions. Recent scholarly studies o f racism’s impact in several key arenas suggest that the narrative instruments used to generate a racist socialization are re-fashioned and/or re-invented with each generation, and that the untreated uprootedness o f earlier decades continues to self-propagate. In fact, several studies now confirm significant patterns o f racialized thinking in the generation that was born and raised after the narrative instruments o f the pre-Civil Rights era were discredited.
  • 63. In March 2002, the Institute of Medicine o f the National Academy o f Sciences released a report titled, “Unequal Treatment: Confronting Racial and Ethnic Disparities in Health Care,” that documented widespread and substantial disparities in the treatment doctors dispensed to Whites and non-W hite patients. The congressionally mandated study noted that these disparities persist “even when insurance status, income, age, and severity o f conditions are comparable.” The report’s authors defined “disparities in healthcare as racial or ethnic differences in the quality o f healthcare that are not due to access-related factors or clinical needs, preferences, and appropriateness o f intervention.” They noted that these disparities are associated with greater mortality and are, therefore, “unacceptable.” In “The Apartheid o f Children’s Literature,” published in The New York Times in 2014, writer Christopher Myers cites a 2013 study by the Cooperative Children’s Book Center at the University o f Wisconsin showing that only 93 o f the 3,200 children’s books published in 2013 were about
  • 64. Black people. A recent study, “W ho believes in me? The effect o f student-teacher demographic match on teacher expectations,” published in the Economics o f Education Review in 2016, reveals that “non-black teachers o f black students have significantly lower expectations than do black teachers,” that “ [t]hese effects are larger for black male students and [non-black] math teachers,” and that Black teachers have equal expectations for their non-Black and Black students. More recently, a study published in the Proceedings o f the National Academy o f Sciences (pnas) in March 2017, “Language from police body camera footage shows racial disparities in officer respect,” confirms that “Police officers speak significantly less respectfully to black than to white community members in everyday traffic stops, even after controlling for officer race, infraction severity, stop location, and stop outcome.” At the time he shot and killed 32-year old Philando Castile in July 2016, M innesota police officer Jeronimo Yanez was a young man in his late 20s. Looking at the dash cam and other footage o f the shooting, it is clear that
  • 65. Castile’s killer had no mental script in which a Black man could own a gun and not be a threat. Narrative relationships formed through and with television, cinematic, historical, and other narrative characterizations o f Black men as savages, brutes, and thugs were the likely emotional triggers that preempted his human response to this family. Before, during, and after the killing, Officer Yanez displayed no empathy for Castile, for his girlfriend, nor for the traumatized 4-year old child in the backseat his bullets narrowly missed hitting, a child who will undoubtedly need caring and professional support to become re-rooted. The jury’s endorsement of Yanez’s demeanor suggests that racism’s uprooting is widespread and unabated. For African-Americans, the public slaughter o f men, women, and children by police and by civilians has become an ongoing roadside holocaust. O n the one hand, these killings are consistent with popular understandings o f the ways in which non-Whites are endangered by racism. Read more carefully, however, these developments are also indicative o f a larger pattern of detachment, defective moral reasoning, and misreading responsible for an
  • 66. alarming decline in the nations democratic institutions, provisions, and commitments, and an increase in policies that have endangered the poor and middle classes across all racial groups. As discussed above, these new jeopardies are the end-results o f a process set in motion in the seventeenth-century colonies. In their quest to halt, if not reverse, this decline, several commentators have developed lucid descriptions of the threatening behaviors but have failed to examine their psychological foundations in the ideology of race. A useful example is the book What’s the Matter with Kansas? (2004), in which writer Thomas Frank unmasks the voting behaviors o f working and middle class W hite men in endorsing policies and candidates inimical to their own economic interests. Frank blames these developments on what he calls “the Great Backlash, a style of conservatism that first came snarling onto the national stage in response to the partying and protests o f the late sixties”(5) - As he notes, “W hile earlier forms o f conservatism emphasized fiscal sobriety, the backlash mobilizes voters with explosive social issues — summoning public outrage over everything from busing to un-Christian art — which it then marries to pro-business economic policies”^ ).
  • 67. Although he faults the Democratic Party for becoming “the other pro-business party,” the group that comes in for a scathing indictm ent in Frank’s analysis is the vast and diverse m ultitude o f working-class voters: sturdy blue-collar patriots reciting the Pledge while they strangle their own life chances;... small farmers proudly voting themselves off the lan d ;... devoted family men carefully seeing to it that their children will never be able to afford college or proper health care;... working-class guys in midwestern cities cheering as they deliver up a landslide for a candidate whose policies will end their way o f life, will transform their region into a ‘rust belt,’ will strike people like them blows from which they will never recover. (10) The working-class political behavior Frank describes can be more fully understood as the calculated effect of focusing illusions. As defined by cognitive psychologist Daniel Kahneman, a focusing illusion is any single issue that is given an exaggerated importance in decision-making, because o f a false calculation o f its impact on expected
  • 68. outcome. Historically, race has been a central focusing illusion for many W hite working-class voters in the u.s. who mistakenly believed that their economic interests and future quality o f life would be secured by supporting candidates who endorsed racist positions. The Trump campaign was perhaps the most successful among recent deployments o f race as a focus illusion. W hite working-class voters thrilled to Trump’s promises to exclude Mexicans, ban Muslims, and dismantle the policies (and legacy) o f the first Black president, (mistakenly) convinced that their lives would be improved by these actions, and, like their seventeenth-century predecessors, that their economic interests would be prioritized and secured by a W hite rich president. The widespread vulnerability to focusing illusions is a direct consequence o f emotional detachment from those “other” people who are (mistakenly) expected to bear the full brunt of particular policy decisions, a detachment ffequendy accompanied and exacerbated by deficits in social literacy and historical thinking. A recent example can be observed in the farmers from California and other Western states who cheered on the Trump
  • 69. anti-Mexican, anti-immigrant agenda with no empathy for the psychological distress o f the Mexican workers they had employed for years and whom they knew. Belatedly recognizing that their own economic well-being is dependent on these “disposable” people, these W hite farmers are now anxious to have the administration reverse course. In Historical Thinking and Other Unnatural Acts (2001), education psychologist and cognitive scientist, Samuel W ineburg differentiates between historical knowledge and historical thinking. As he sees it, “to engage in historical thinking [is to be] called on to see hum an motive in the texts we read; called on to mine truth from the quicksand o f innuendo, half-truth, and falsehood that seeks to engulf us each day; called on to brave the fact that certainty, at least in understanding the social world, remains elusive and beyond our grasp” (83). For Wineburg, the ability to engage in historical thinking is an essential component o f “social literacy,” “a literacy n ot o f names and dates but o f discernment, judgment, and caution” (ix). 15 7 B
  • 70. LA C K R EN A IS SA N C E N O IR E CODA In a 2012 lecture that now seems prescient, retired Supreme Court justice David Souter expressed his concern that u.s. democracy would be endangered not by foreign agents or military force but by a decline in the populations social literacy. Prefacing his commentary with the observation that “Democracy cannot survive too much ignorance,”
  • 71. Justice Souter explained, I don’t worry about our losing republican government in the United States because I’m afraid o f a foreign invasion [...] or a coup by the military as has happened in other places [...] W hat I worry about is that when problems are not addressed people will n ot know who is responsible. [...] If something is not done to improve the level o f civic knowledge, that’s what I worry about.11 Although Justice Souter mentions dangers arising from a lack o f “civic” knowledge, the substance o f his analysis is about dangers stemming from a populace whose ability to engage in historical thinking has been severely diminished. The paradox here is that while racism has largely contributed to the loss o f these essential competencies, a society conditioned to recognize only racialized dangers may not be capable o f fully appreciating or adequately responding to ongoing threats to its democratic institutions when those threats are in White-face. Throughout the nation’s history, race has been the most frequently deployed,
  • 72. the most effective mechanism for undermining the interpretive competence and interpretive agency o f people with W hite racial privilege. It does so, first, by its positioning of particular groups on the lower tiers on the hierarchy o f hum an worth and concurrently defining them as disposable. The resulting emotional detachment toward these groups infects and impairs emotional attachment toward other non-racialized groups, eroding the capacity for effective moral reasoning and healthy decision-making. Second, by generating narratives that assign blame to these constituencies for failures in economic and social policy. Third, by orchestrating narrative relationships that function as disabling emotional guides and emotional triggers, inhibiting narrative knowledge and social literacy. And finally, by prescribing indifference regarding threats to those defined as disposable and simultaneously obscuring the connection between such threats and jeopardy to those defined as valuable. Now, at the start o f the Trump presidency, it is becoming all too apparent that u.s. democracy is imperiled by the very intellectual habits that sustain the racial privileges McIntosh and other W hites carry in their “invisible knapsacks.” Despite this, I hope for a future secured
  • 73. by the possibilities o f re-rooting through racial self-evaluation, self-diagnosis, and self-correction: a future in which a parent might say to a friend, “I’m worried about my teenage son. He’s showing signs o f racism spectrum disorder.” Or, a volunteer might say o f herself, “I’m beginning to realize I’m on the racism spectrum and that the volunteer work I do really stems from the fact that I don’t see my clients as having the same capacities that I do.” A future in which a co-worker might say, “My parents were on the racism spectrum and worked really hard to provide my brother and me with learning opportunities to make sure that we wouldn’t be.” A future in which a colleague might say, “My students and I are developing a model to counter r.s.d through an elementary school reading curriculum.” O ne in which a political scientist will decide to measure the ways in which r.s.d. — not just the Russians — affected the 2016 presidential elections, and in which doctors and law enforcement officers will be screened for r.s.d using fMRI technology and offered appropriate therapies. A future in which voters who lack personal knowledge of the many strangers on the “trolley tracks” o f domestic and foreign policy debates will seek new narrative knowledge
  • 74. and be guided by it in effective moral reasoning and moral responses. O ne in which young people will not need to remind uprooted compatriots that “Black lives matter!” If not this future, then it will be, as writer James Baldwin forewarned, the fire next time. ■ WORKS CITED Baldwin, James. 1962. The Fire Next Time. New York: Vintage Books. Charon, Rita. 2006. Narrative Medicine: Honoring the Stories o f Illness. New York: Oxford University Press. Clark, Kenneth B. and Mamie P. Clark. 1947. "Racial Identification and Preference in Negro Children." E.L. Hartley, ed. Readings in Social Psychology. New York: Holt, Rinehart,Winston. Print. Clore, Gerald L., and Janet Palmer. 2009. “Affective guidance of intelligent agents: How emotion controls cognition.” Cognitive Systems Research 10,21-30. Print. Damasio, Antonio, R. 1994. Descartes’Error: Emotion, Reason, and the Human Brain. New York: G. P. Putnam’s Sons. Print. Frank, Thomas. 2004. What’s the matter with Kansas: how conservatives stole the heart o f America. Gershenson, Seth, Stephen B. Holt, and Nicholas W.
  • 75. Papageorge. 2016. "Who believes in me? The effect of student-teacher demographic match on teacher expectations." Economics o f Education Review 52. 209-224. Print. Greene, Joshua D., R. Brian Sommerville, Leigh E. Nystrom, John M. Parley, and Jonathan D. Cohen. 2001. "An fMRI Investigation of Emotional Engagement in Moral Judgment.”Science Vol. 293, September. 2105-2108. Print. ENDNOTES 1 The Clark study showed th a t contrary to psychological predictions th a t children would prefer dolls that most closely resembled themselves i.e. would display prefer- ence based on similarity bias, Black children rejected as "ugly” the Black dolls with which they identified, and preferred the White dolls instead. 2 Although the followers of Islam include people from all races and nations, the term "Muslim” in the White/Western imagination is as much a signifier of (non-White) racial difference as of religious distinction. 3 McIntosh’s first listing of over 50 privileges appeared in a 1988 essay titled “White Privilege and Male Privilege: A Personal Account of Coming to See Correspondences through Work in Women’s Studies.” A year later, she published an abridged version of the essay, titled "Unpacking th e Invisible Knapsack,” with a shorter list of only 26 privileges. 4 This privilege is listed in the 1988 but not the 1989 version.
  • 76. 5 Wahneema Lubiano, “Introduction,” p.vii. Harrison, Mary-Catherine. 2011. “How Narrative Relationships Overcome Empathic Bias: Elizabeth Gaskell’s Empathy across Social Difference.” Poetics Today 32:2 (Summer). 255-288. Print. Institute of Medicine. 2003. Unequal Treatment: Confronting Racial and Ethnic Disparities in Healthcare. Washington, DC: National Academies Press. Kahneman, Daniel, and Amos Tversky, eds. 2000. Choices, Values, and Frames. Cambridge and New York: Cambridge University Press. Print. Lubiano, Wahneema, ed. 1997. “Introduction.” The House That Race Built. New York: Vintage Books. Print. McIntosh, Peggy. 1989. “White Privilege: Unpacking the Invisible Knapsack” Peace and Freedom July/August: io-i2. Print. Morrison, Toni. 1970. The Bluest Eye. New York: Knopf. Print. Myers, Christopher. 2014. “The Apartheid of Children’s Literature.” The New York Times Sunday Review March 15. Print. Olson, Joel. 2001. “The Democratic Problem of th e White Citizen.” Constellations Vol. 8, No. 2. Print. Powell, John A. 2007. "Structural Racism: Building upon the Insights of John Calmore." North Carolina Law Review 86:791- 816. Print.
  • 77. 6 As Gerald L. Clore and Janet Palmer note, "it turns out that affect and emotion play critical roles in good judgm ent and in the adaptive regulation of thought." 7 According to Greene and his colleagues, “Typical examples of non-moral dilemmas posed questions about whether to travel by bus or by train given certain tim e constraints and about which of two coupons to use at a store" (2106). 8 To Charon's listing of “life settings” in which narrative knowledge is essential, I would add the many other social and professional arenas — neighborhoods, k-through-12 classrooms, airports, restaurants, playgrounds, police precincts, newsrooms, legislative chambers, and corporate boardrooms — in which citizens interact in our democracy. 9 It is im portant to note th a t a lack of empathy on th e part of the ruling elite does not typically involve th e familiar displays of petty criminals. In fact, these displays are usually not defined as criminal. Nevertheless, the characteristics associated with a lack of empathy are obviously at work in the economic policies and behaviors of the ruling elite who are frequently detached from working-class employ- ees, tenants, patients, students, clients, and other subordinates. Souter, David. 2012. “Constitutionally Speaking with Justice David Souter and PBS senior correspondent Margaret Warner.” September 14. Capitol Center for the Arts, Concord, New Hampshire. Audio. Tatum, Beverly. 1997. Why Are All the Black Kids Sitting Together in the Cafeteria? And Other Conversations
  • 78. About Race. Print. Voigt, Rob, Nicholas P. Camp, Vinodkumar Prabhakaran, William L. Hamilton, Rebecca C. Hetey, Camilla M. Griffiths, David Jurgens, Dan Jurafsky, and Jennifer L. Eberhardt. 2017. "Language from policy body camera footage shows racial disparities in officer respect." Proceedings o f the National Academy o f Sciences. Weil, Simone. 1949. The Need fo r Roots: Prelude to a Declaration o f Duties toward Mankind. London and New York: Routledge. Print. Wineburg, Samuel. 2001. Historical Thinking and Other Unnatural Acts: Charting the Future o f Teaching the Past. Philadelphia:Temple University Press. Print. 10 At this historical juncture, it is difficult to tell whether empathy was/is always dependent and/or limited by social difference, especially race. The New Testament narrative of the “good Samaritan" is one of perhaps a few examples of empathy unrestrained by social dif- ference. So, too, is Toni Morrison’s revisioning of this narrative in her depiction of the encounter between the White teenager Amy Denver and Sethe, the refu- gee from slavery, in th e novel Beloved. 11 "Constitutionally Speaking with Justice David Souter and PBS senior correspondent Margaret Warner.” On September 14,2012 at the Capitol Center for the Arts in Concord, New Hampshire. 15 9 BL
  • 79. AC K RE NA IS SA NC E NO IR E Copyright of Black Renaissance/Renaissance Noire is the property of Institute of African- American Affairs and its content may not be copied or emailed to multiple sites or posted to a listserv without the copyright holder's express written permission. However, users may print, download, or email articles for individual use. The Trouble with Gay Rights: Race and the Politics of Sexual Orientation in Philadelphia, 1969-1982 Kevin J. Mumford
  • 80. In 1985 the veteran civil rights activist Bayard Rustin joined a gay and lesbian coalition in New York City that was about to make a final push for passage of an amendment to the city's administrative code to prohibit discrimination on the basis of sexual orientation. Rustin wrote to the city council and to Mayor Edward I. Koch, vowed to lobby the four opposing African American councilmen, and called for "the extension of freedom and justice for all Americans."' Unusual for Rustin, in this instance his activism ceremoniously concluded rather than pio- neered a social movement for change when the city council passed the bill the next year. More than fifteen years before, the first gay liberationists had introduced such a bill that was defeated by stiff opposition from conservative politicians and religious leaders. That mobilization helped spark organizing and demonstrating for similar laws in cities and towns during the 1970s and 1980s—a local political movement and discourse that remains largely unexamined. In recent writing on sexual politics, however, Matthew D. Lassiter, Robert O . Self, Whitney Strub, and others have analyzed controversies over gay visibility in the media, conflicts over the regulation of urban vice, and the political cooptation of pornography to understand shifts in American liberalism, and they suggest that the rise of gay liberation served as a foil against which liberals reconfigured the public/private divide and conservatives mobilized a rightward political turn. From another perspective, despite the rise of the New Right, the cause
  • 81. of gays and lesbians did advance during the 1970s, with the proliferation of community pride, new national organi- zations, and the passage of protective ordinances. Utilizing a case study of Philadelphia, this essay examines how the pursuit of equal protection led gays and lesbians to clash with reli- gious and racial conservatives who challenged not only their rights but also their legitimacy Kevin J. Mumford is a member of the Department of History at the University of Iowa He wishes to thank the following for encouragement and advice: Clarissa Atkins, Nan Alamilla Boyd, Zoe Burk- bolder, Christopher Capozzola, Matthew Countryman, Rachel Devlin, Steve Estes, Estelle Freedman, Thomas Guglielmo, Evelyn Brooks Higginbotham, Andrew Kahrl, Linda Kerber, Scott Kurashige, Kenneth Mack, Charles Morris, David Roediger, Bob Skiba, Marc Stein, Shelton Stromquist. the audiences of the Charles Warren Center for Studies in American History and the 2009 annual meeting of the Organization of American Historians, and the anonymous reviewers for theJAH. ' Bayard Rustin, memo, March 11, 1986, in 7^?ß/Z)iari^/?«jft«Pi7/)frj. ed. Bayard Rustin and Nanette Dobrosky (Frederick, 1988); Rustin to Edward 1. Koch, Feb. 2, 1985, ibid; "Bayard Rustin's Statement on Proposed Amend.- ments to Law Banning Discrimination on the Basis of Sexual Orientation," April 17, 1986, ibid,; Rustin to Howard N. Meyer, Dec. 8, 1986, ibid; Rustin to Hon. Mary Pinkett, March 22, 1985, ibid.; John D'Emilio, Lost Prophet: The Life and Times of Bayard Rustin (New York, 2003), 489- 90; "Text of the New Bill of Rights," New York Times,
  • 82. Jan. 23, 1986, p. B2; "Rights Measure Ready for Vote after 12 Years," ibid., March 20, 1986, p. Bl; "Amending the Homosexual Rights Law," ibid.. May 3, 1986, p. A26. doi: 10.1O93/jahist/jarl39 © Ihe Author 2011. Published by Oxford University Press on behalf of the Organization of American Historians. All rights reserved. For permissions, please e-mail: journals.permissions^poup.com June 2011 The Journal of American History 49 50 The Journal of American History June 2011 as a minority. It also explores how they developed racial diversity to respond to those threats and ultimately argues that gays and lesbians reconstructed their identities in the process of negotiating race relations and extending the liberal impulses of the 1960s into the 1980s.' Perhaps more than any other field, gay and lesbian studies concentrates on the origins and meanings of identity, creating discussions that often evolve into what is referred to as the essentialism versus constructionism debate. Essentialist scholars conceive of sexuality as a transhistorical essence located in the self, and they tend to see the gays, lesbians, or homosexuals of the past as identifying and desiring in much the same way as they do in the present. By contrast, social constructionist scholars argue that the very categories of homosexuality and heterosexuality change historically in
  • 83. relation to the forces of capital- ism, the family, urbanization, and scientific knowledge. Most historians are by nature constructionists, and they generally agree that by the outbreak of the Stonewall riots in 1969, when New York City police raided the gay-friendly Stonewall Inn and patrons fought back, gay and lesbian identities and communities had formed across metropolitan America. Even so, sexual historians entertain a variety of interpretations about which processes or forces most affected the dynamics of identification. Anne Enke reconstructed 1970s civic spaces (such as dance halls and ballparks) and Martin Meeker pointed to contact zones of literature, periodicals, and photographs to describe areas of popular interaction that defined sexual subjectivities. Margot Canaday uncovered the extensive operation of constitu- tive regulation by what she termed the "straight state" to illustrate the powers of government interdiction and neglect, and Joanne Meyerowitz analyzed paradigms of racial and sexual knowledge to uncover changing processes of subjective interpolation that sometimes served as self-representations. In turn, my reconstruction of the politics of sexual orientation in Phila- delphia highlights the impact of intersections of race and sexuality on identity formation. As Matthew J. Countryman demonstrated in his study of civil rights and black power movements in Philadelphia, black leadership protested against workplace discrimination, promoted educational reform, and gained control in municipal government. At the same time, gay organizers, volunteers, witnesses, and audiences in
  • 84. the city, along with readers of the gay press and other media, were influenced by black political actors and gay people of color as they struggled for a place in the rights revolution of the 1970s.' By the rights revolution, I not only refer to Jacquelyn Dowd Hall's phrase the "long civil rights movement" and her influential reading of racial struggle that extended the chronology of the movement, enlarged its scope from the South to the nation, and stressed the role of working-class politics, but I also incorporate what the sociologist John Skrentny conceived of as a minority rights revolution, or the expansion of the black civil rights movement ^ Bruce J. Schulman, The Seventies: The Great Shift in American Culture, Society, and Politics (New York, 2002); Matthew D. Lassiter, "Inventing Family Values," in Rightward Bound: Making America Conservative in the 1970s, ed. Bruce J. Schulman and Julian Zelizer (Cambridge, Mass., 2008), 13-28; Robert O. Self, "Sex in the City: The Politics of Sexual Liberalism in Los Angeles, 1963-1979," Gender and History, 20 (Aug. 2008), 288-311; Whitney Strub, Perversion for Profit: The Politics of Pornography and the Rise of the New Right (New York, 2010), 260-61 ; Josh Sides, "Excavating the Postwar Sex District in San Francisco," Journal of Urban History, 32 (March 2006), 355-79; Peter Braunstein, '"Adults Only': The Construction of the Erotic City in New York during the 1970s," in America in the Seventies, ed. Beth L. Bailey and David Färber (Lawrence, 2004), 129-56. esp. 146. ' Anne Enke, Finding the Movement: Sexuality, Contested Space, and Feminist Activism (Durham, N.C., 2007); Margot Canaday, The Straight State: Sexuality and Citizenship
  • 85. in Twentieth Century America (Princeton, 2009); Joanne Meyerowitz, '"How Common Culture Shapes the Separate Lives': Sexuality, Race, and Mid-Twentieth-Century Social Constructionist "Vnon^u Journal of American History, 96 (March 2010), 1057—84; Joanne Meyerowitz, "Transnational Sex and U.S. Wsx.ov¡^ American Historical Review, 14 (Dec. 2009), 1273-86; Matthew J. Countryman, Up South: Civil Rights and Black Power in Philadelphia (Philadelphia, 2006), 117-52, 236-44, 312-27. Race and the Politics of Sexual Orientation in Philadelphia, 1969-1982 51 to include women, the disabled, the elderly, and other groups. His synthesis argued that "though victimized like blacks, gays and lesbians were not legitimate victims," in part because they failed to demonstrate how they were analogous to recognized groups. Historians of the legal regulation of sexuality have focused more on the issues of marriage and sodomy than on antidiscrimination legislation, while the recent growth of historiography on the northern civil rights movement largely overlooks gays and lesbians. Instead, the study of sexual orientation ordinances has been taken up by social scientists who measure discrete variables—religiosity, population size, socioeconomic status, and education—to explain why some locations passed laws and others did not. Following their lead, my analysis com- pares the legislative failure in 1975 to the eventual legislative success in 1982 in Philadelphia but focuses less on why it occurred than on how it occurred, in
  • 86. part because of how race became crucial to arguments against gay rights as well as central to their passage. Although historians have examined the impact of race on sexuality, particularly in studies of "misce- genation," relatively few studies of gay and lesbian communities have done so. My analysis pursues the same sort of questions about homophobia that historians traditionally ask about racism: What were the dominant images and logics presented to the public? What did it justify? How did it respond to challenges and reform? My work also locates a kind of racial conflict between white gays and black gays that undermined the movement from within, which the historian of feminism Winifred Breines has conceptualized as a cycle of personal misunderstanding that troubled social movements—one of "profound racial dis- tance and tentative reconciliation." In Philadelphia and other cities with significant minor- ity demographics, sexual politics operated in some relationship to patterns of race relations."* Race and Gay Liberation Since the emergence of modern discourse on gay and lesbian identity, writers and aca- demics have often compared the condition of the homosexual to that of a racial minority. •• Jacquelyn Dowd Hall, "The Long Civil Rights Movement and the Political Uses of the Past," Journal of American History, 91 (March 2005), 1233-63; John D. Skrentny, The Minority Rights Revolution (Cambridge, Mass., 2002), 20, 304-5, 315, 326. On the long civil rights
  • 87. movement, see Sundidata Keita Cha-Jua and Clarence Lang, "The 'Long Movement' as Vampire: Temporal and Spatial Fallacies in Recent Black Freedom Smd^its," Journal of/^can American History, 92 (Spring 2007), 265-88; and Samuel Walker, The Rights Revolution: Rights and Com- munity in Modern America (New York, 1998), 44-48. On the long civil rights movement and working-class politics, see Nancy MacLean, Ereedom Is Not Enough: The Opening of the American Workplace (Cambridge, Mass., 2006). For studies of sexual orientation ordinances, see Kenneth D. Wald, James W. Button, and Barbara A. Rienzo. "The Politics of Gay Rights in American Communities: Explaining Antidiscrimination Ordinances and Policies,"" Ameri- can Journal of Political Science, 40 (Nov. 1996), 1157-78; Gary Mucciaroni, Same Sex, Different Politics: Success and Eailure in the Struggles over Gay Rights (Chicago, 2008), 31; Craig A. Rimmerman, Kenneth D. Wald, and Clyde Wilcox, Ihe Politics of Gay Rights (Chicago, 2000) ; and Wayne Van der Meide, Legist/! ting Equality: A Review of Laws Affecting Gay, Lesbian, Bisexual, and Transgendered People in the United States (Washington, 2004). On Philadelphia gay institutions and sexual orientation rights, see Robert W. Bailey, Gay Politics, Urban Politics: Identity and Econom- ics in the Urban Setting (New York, 1999), 249-79. On the impact of race on sexuality, see Peggy Pa.scoe, What Comes Naturally: Miscegenation Law and the Making of Race in America (New York, 2009); Renee C. Romano. Race Mixing: Black-White Marriage in Postwar America (Cambridge, Mass., 2003); David !.. Chappell, A Stone of Hope: Prophetic Religion and the Death of Jim Crow (Chapel Hill, 2004), 114-18; White Interveners in Still v, Savannah- Chatham County Board of Education, "In Defense of School Segregation,"" in The Development of Segregationist Thought, ed. I. A. Newby (Homewood, 1968), 146-53; Pete Daniel, Lost Revolutions: The South in the 1950s (Chapel Hill, 2000), 148-75; and 1. A. Newby,//'w Crow's
  • 88. Defense: Anti-Negro Thought in America, 1900-1930 (Baton Rouge, 1965), 92-95. Winifred Breines, The Trouble between Us: An Uneasy History of the White and Black Women in the Eeminist Movement (New York, 2006), 7. On race and feminism, see also Sara M. Evans, Personal Politics: The Roots of Women's Liberation in the Civil Rights Movement and the New Left (New York, 1979); and Anne M. Valk, Radical Sisters: Second-Wave Eeminism and Black Liberation in Washington, D.C. (Urbana, 2008). 52 The Journal of American History June 2011 In his influential 1951 treatise. The Homosexual Minority in America, Daniel Webster Cory included homosexuals in the so-called minority problem of Jews and Negroes, as did some of the writings of the gay black novelist James Baldwin and brief letters by the black playwright Lorraine Hansberry that were published in a 1957 issue of a lesbian journal. Even so, what was known as the homophile movement (a number of advocacy organizations founded in the postwar era) remained largely white and more culturally, strategically, and personally conservative. In his classic account of homophile politics, John D'Emilio argued that they were influenced by the "headline-making black civil rights movement," but that the rank and file sought a kind of invisible integration and could be compared to only the most conservative of black organizations. My own reading of the national organizational journals—Mattachine Review, the Ladder, and ONE—
  • 89. revealed that homophiles were more involved in efforts to achieve civil liberties and gain public acceptance than demonstrating for rights, and they infrequently mentioned African Americans. Conversely, in the 1950s the black popular periodical/i?i routinely ran stories on homosexuality or gay sex scandals, though rarely in connection with civil rights. O n rare occasions writers observed the intersection of the two causes. In 1962 the homo- phile publication ONE published a review that characterized James Baldwin as "one of a number of current writers who is discovering a homosexual dimension to the current Negro revolution."' By the 1960s, homophiles in Philadelphia had begun to adapt what Marc Stein termed "multiple models of African American resistance" to the advancement of their cause, and on July 4, 1965, they held the first Annual Reminder demonstration at Independence Hall to demand recognition of their rights of citizenship. Even before the 1969 Stonewall riots, Philadelphia gays and lesbians had formed the Homophile Action League (HAL) to break away from the civility of the homophiles; a week after the riots, HAL and other radicalized activists challenged the conventionality of the fifth Annual Reminder by reftis- ing to conform to its usual dress code. The new radical HAL later discovered less rebellion and more apathy in its ranks, however, and therefore urged readers to follow "our fellow victims of discrimination and oppression—blacks, women, the poor, political dissi-
  • 90. dents—[who] are fervently pressing their battles." In 1970 the HAL announced that it had testified for a "state Human Rights Act" before the state platform committees of both the Democratic and Republican parties. The league also sponsored a booth where crowds of supporters "filled 10 pages of the homosexual 'civil rights' petition." Similarly, Martin Duberman stressed the connections of race and sexuality in a narrative on the Stonewall ^ Daniel Webster Cory, The Homosexual in America: A Subjective Approach (NewYork, 1951), 4; Letter from L. N. H.[Lorraine Hansberry], Ladder, 1 (May 1957), 26-28. On Lorraine Hansberry's letters, see also Lisbeth Lipari, "The Rhetoric of Intersectionality: Lorraine Hansberry's 1957 Letters to the Ladder," in Queering Public Address: Sexualities in American Historical Discourse, ed. Charles E. Morris III (Columbia, S.C., 2007), 233-35; and Cheryl Higashida, "To Be(come) Young, Gay, and Black: Lorraine Hansberry's Existentialist Routes to Anticolonialism," American Quarterly, 60 (Dec. 2008), 899-924. James Baldwin, Another Country (New York, 1962); James Baldwin, "The Male Prison," in The Price of the Ticket: Collected Nonfiction, 1948-1985, by James Baldwin (New York, 1985), 101-05; John D'Emilio, Sexual Politics, Sexual Communities: The Making of a Homosexual Minority in the United States, 1940-1970 (Chicago, 1982), 174, 224; Chuck Stanley, review of Another Country by James Baldwin, One, 10 (Nov. 1962), 21-22. Andrew Bradbury, "Race and Sex," ibid, 12 (Oct. 1964), 17-21; Review of "The Toilet" by Le Roi Jones, ibid., 13 (March 1965), 16; Richard Mayer, review o( Giovanni's Room by James Baldwin, Matta- chine Review, 3 (April 1957), 33; 'A Moral Revolution: For Homosexuals?," Pursuit and Symposium, 1 (March—April 1966), 31—33. On homophile ethnic analogy, see Barbara