SlideShare a Scribd company logo
1 of 244
1
Annotated Bibliography: Topic (Chosen from the list provided)
[Name]
South University Online
[Template instructions: Replace the information in red with
your work-then delete this line]
2
Annotated Bibliography: Topic (Chosen from the list provided)
[APA formatted reference for source (list in alphabetical order)
using a hanging indent]
[Underneath the reference, give a summary of the article then an
analysis:
Summary of article: 1-2 paragraphs that describe the following
information in your own words
in paragraph format (not bullet points).
• Why the article was written?
• What are the major points of the article?
• If the article was a study, describe:
o The methods used in the research: Include the participants,
how the research question(s)
was tested or measured (e.g. survey, interview, formal
testing…)
o The results of the study: What did the researchers find out?
o The conclusions: What did the researchers conclude from the
study? What were the
limitations of the research?
NOTE: Do not include citations for the article you are
summarizing in an annotated
bibliography. You have already given credit by listing the
reference first. This is different
from a paper.]
[Analysis of the article: 1-2 paragraphs describing the
following: Whether or not the
points made by the author are logical and supported by evidence
and whether the author
demonstrates any bias in presenting the arguments. Were other
arguments or possibilities
considered? Are the author’s conclusions supported? Do they fit
with your understanding
of the topic and your textbook's description (cite the textbook
and any other sources you
use for analyzing your article – include any additional sources
you cite as part of your
analysis in your reference list)? Why or why not (provide
support for your opinion)?]
3
Example of formatting:
Boonstra, A., & Broekhuis, M. (2010). Barriers to the
acceptance of electronic medical records by
physicians from systematic review to taxonomy and
interventions. BMC Health Services
Research, 10(1), 231-248. doi:10.1186/1472-6963-10-231
Authors conducted a systematic review of research papers
between 1998 and 2009 that
examined physician perceptions of barriers to implementation of
electronic medical
records. An examination of 1671 articles….
DeVore, S. D., & Figlioli, K. (2010). Lessons Premier hospitals
learned about implementing electronic
health records. Health Affairs, 29(4), 664-667.
doi:10.1377/hlthaff.2010.0250
Premier healthcare alliance is a network of 2300 non-profit
hospitals and 63,000
outpatient facilities in the United States, This paper summarized
lessons learned from
reviewing implementation practices within their system….
4
References
List any references you cited in your analyses of your chosen
sources. DO NOT list the references for
the articles you chose as you already referenced them in your
annotated bibliograpy.
Personality and Individual Differences 49 (2010) 306–316
Contents lists available at ScienceDirect
Personality and Individual Differences
j o u r n a l h o m e p a g e : w w w . e l s e v i e r . c o m / l o c
a t e / p a i d
The nature of the relationship between personality traits and
political attitudes
Brad Verhulst a, Peter K. Hatemi b,c,*, Nicholas G. Martin b
a Department of Political ScienceStony Brook University, Stony
Brook, NY 11794-4392, USA
b Queensland Institute of Medical Research, Genetic
Epidemiology 300 Herston Road, Q 4029, Australia
c Department of Political Science University of Iowa, 344
Schaeffer Hall, Iowa City, Iowa 52242-1498, USA
a r t i c l e i n f o
Article history:
Received 29 October 2009
Received in revised form 3 November 2009
Accepted 4 November 2009
Available online 9 December 2009
Keywords:
Personality
Ideology
Attitudes
Politics
Genes
0191-8869/$ - see front matter � 2009 Elsevier Ltd. A
doi:10.1016/j.paid.2009.11.013
* Corresponding author. Address: University of Io
City, Iowa 52242-1498, USA. Tel.: +1 319 335 2347.
E-mail address: [email protected] (P.K. H
a b s t r a c t
Building upon a series of works by Thomas J. Bouchard, Lindon
J. Eaves, Hans J. Eysenck and other con-
temporaries, we present strong evidence that the assumed causal
relationship between personality
and left–right ideology is too simplistic. We suggest the
relationship is not predictive and instead is better
understood by dividing the overarching left–right ideological
spectrum into more meaningful attitude
dimensions. In doing so, we find that Psychoticism is strongly
related to conservative positions on Pun-
ishment, Religious, and Sex attitudes, whereas Social
Desirability is related to liberal positions on the
same attitudes. Furthermore, the nature of the covariance
between Psychoticism and social attitudes is
due to a common genetic influence, while covariance between
Social Desirability and these attitudes
in females is largely a function of common shared
environmental covariance.
� 2009 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.
1. Introduction
Psychologists, geneticists, and political scientists have long at-
tempted to identify and explain the associations between
person-
ality and political preferences (Adorno, Frenkel-Brunswik,
Levinson, & Sanford, 1950; Eysenck, 1954; McCloskey, 1958).
Most
of these early attempts to link personality traits with political
atti-
tudes portrayed conservatism as a pathology and focused on
con-
necting conservatives with negative personality traits (e.g.
Adorno et al., 1950; Altemeyer, 1981; Altemeyer, 1996). Such a
view is exemplified by McCloskey (1958), who labeled
conserva-
tives as:
‘‘. . .social isolates, . . . people who think poorly of themselves,
who suffer personal disgruntlement and frustration, who are
submissive, timid, and wanting in confidence, who lack a clear
sense of direction and purpose, who are uncertain about their
values, and who are generally bewildered by the alarming task
of having to thread their way through a society which seems to
them too complex to fathom” (37).
Focusing on the causal connection between negative personality
traits and conservatism and positive traits with liberalism
contin-
ues to dominate research in this area, though the language has
be-
come more subtle (see Block & Block, 2006; Jost, Glaser,
Kruglanski,
& Sulloway, 2003; Jost et al., 2008). For example, researchers
have
ll rights reserved.
wa, 344 Schaeffer Hall, Iowa
atemi).
worked very hard to demonstrate that the positively valued
Open-
ness to Experience dimension has a strong and consistent
negative
relationship with political conservatism (see Gosling, Rentfrow,
&
Swann, 2003; Jost, Federico, & Napier, 2009; McCrae, 1996;
Mondak
& Halperin, 2008; Van Hiel, Kossowska, & Mervielde, 2000;
Van
Hiel, Pandelaere, & Duriez, 2004). Recent work however, has
also
demonstrated that Neuroticism is positively associated with
economic liberalism (Leeson & Heaven, 1999; Riemann,
Grubich,
Hempel, Mergl, & Richter, 1993), though these findings are
often
dismissed or downplayed in favor or a more positive view of
liber-
alism and negative view of conservatism.
The connection between personality and politics historically
rested on the assumption that causality runs from personality
traits to political attitudes. This seems plausible as personality
is
widely understood as some combination of innate dispositions
and personal experiences that, in general, guides behavior in a
sta-
ble predictive manner (Bouchard, 1994; Cattell, 1957; Eysenck,
1990; Eysenck, 1991; Tellegen et al., 1988; Winter &
Barenbaum,
1999). This is not to say behavior is predetermined by
personality;
rather, environmental circumstances provide the impetus for
behaviors, and personality predispositions increase or decrease
the probability of behavior only if the action is appropriate for a
specific situation (Bandura, 2001; Caprara & Cervone, 2000;
Mis-
chel & Shoda, 1998). Political attitudes, on the other hand, have
typically been viewed as much more capricious (Converse,
1964).
Although a plethora of research on attitudes suggest that they
can be quite stable over time, rather than being perceived as
per-
sonal dispositions, political attitudes are typically portrayed as
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2009.11.013
mailto:[email protected]
http://www.sciencedirect.com/science/journal/01918869
http://www.elsevier.com/locate/paid
B. Verhulst et al. / Personality and Individual Differences 49
(2010) 306–316 307
preferences related to the immediate social environment and are
thus perceived as entirely context dependent.
Such a view of attitudes, combined with the widely held
assumption that, unlike personality, parental-offspring correla-
tions of attitudes were entirely functions of cultural
transmission
(e.g., Niemi & Jennings, 1991; for an exception see Martin et
al.,
1986), reinforced the belief that personality traits were causally
prior to social and political attitudes. However, such an
assumption
was unfounded, as the heritability of social and political
attitudes
has been established and replicated across populations
(Bouchard
& McGue, 2003; Eaves, Eysenck, & Martin, 1989; Martin et al.,
1986; Waller, Kojetin, & Bouchard, 1990).
Indeed, the genetic covariation between social attitudes and
personality was explored over 30 years ago. Eaves and Eysenck
(1974) identified a genetic relationship between personality and
conservatism. Typical of a Nature Letter, however, while
profound
and novel, they offered only brief discussion on why this
relation-
ship existed. And, unfortunately, these findings went largely
unad-
dressed for several decades, with the exception of a handful of
scholars. Among these, Bouchard, Lykken, McGue, Segal, and
Telle-
gen (1990) continued to pursue this question (e.g., Bouchard,
1997;
Bouchard, 2009; Bouchard & McGue, 2003; Waller et al.,
1990).
This work has led to a paradigm shift in the connection between
personality and politics.
Combining Religiousness, Authoritarian dispositions, and tradi-
tional Conservatism into a core attitude/world view system,
Bou-
chard (2009, p. 36) drew on other contemporaries (e.g., Saucier,
2000), and synthesized a theory that more accurately integrated
attitude dimensions and personality traits. Such a synthesis im-
plies that whatever relationship exists between personality and
political attitudes cannot be strictly causal; rather, it seems
likely
that the relationships among political attitudes and personality
traits are in part expressions of the same underlying genetic
liabil-
ity. Verhulst, Hatemi, and Eaves (2009) provided strong support
of
such a position and offered the first replication of Eaves and
Ey-
senck’s (1974) findings presented some 35 years earlier. Using
a
large US twin sample (over 6000 pairs), they found that the vast
majority of the covariance between Psychoticism and
Militaristic/
Defense attitudes was due to a common underlying genetic
influence.
Building upon a series of works by Bouchard, Eaves, Eysenck
and others, we examine the assumptions made in the existing
lit-
erature regarding the causal connection between personality
traits
and political attitudes. Specifically, we estimated the genetic
and
environmental sources of covariation between personality traits
and political attitudes and extended the findings of Verhulst
et al. (2009) by focusing on specific attitudinal dimensions of
polit-
ical attitudes and personality traits in a very large sample of
Aus-
tralian Twins.
2. Methods and results
Data were collected from 1988 to 1990 by mailed surveys to
two large cohorts of adult Australian twins enrolled in the
volun-
teer Australian Twin Registry. Each participant completed a
Health
and Lifestyle Questionnaire (HLQ), which contained items on
socio-
political attitudes, personality traits, and wide variety of health-
re-
lated and sociodemographic measures (Eaves et al., 1989; Eaves
et al., 1999; Martin, 1987; Truett et al., 1994). Using only
respon-
dents who completed the political and personality measures, the
sample consists of 20,559 individuals from 5402 families; this
in-
cludes 7234 individual twins, comprising 3254 complete same
sex pairs and 363 unlike sex pairs. The mean age of the entire
sam-
ple was 38.6 (SD = 15.5), while the mean age of the twin
respon-
dents was 34.1 (SD = 14.0). Regarding sex, 57.7% were female
in
the greater population, while in the twin sample 63.8% were fe-
male. For more details on the sample, including ascertainment
and response rates, see Hatemi, Medland, Morley, Heath, and
Mar-
tin (2007).
2.1. Measures
Personality traits were measured by the short form of the Re-
vised version of the Eysenck Personality Questionnaire (EPQ-R-
S,
Eysenck & Eysenck, 1997; Eysenck, Eysenck, & Barrett, 1985)
with
sub-scales for Extraversion, Psychoticism, Neuroticism and
Social
Desirability.
Extraversion is comprised of two central components: affiliation
or sociability, which includes valuing interpersonal bonds and
being warm and affectionate, highly sociable and expressive,
and
agency or impulsiveness, which includes goal-oriented
behavior,
ambition, dogmatism, social dominance, leadership, aggression
and assertiveness (Depue & Collins, 1999). Psychoticism is
posi-
tively related to authoritarianism, risk taking, impulsivity,
tough-
mindedness, practicality, magical ideation, and religiosity and
neg-
atively related to openness to experience. A careful inspection
of
the items that comprise the Psychoticism scale used in this
paper
(as presented in Appendix 1) suggest that the factor is
character-
ized by low levels of introspection and a preoccupation with
rules
and regulations. Neuroticism is associated with feelings of
inferior-
ity, unhappiness, anxiety, dependence, hypochondria, guilt,
emo-
tional instability, and obsessiveness (Eysenck & Eysenck,
1985).
The additional sub-factor of Social Desirability (often dubbed
the
‘‘lie” scale), was also included in the analyses and is
characterized
by social acquiescence or conformity and lack of self-insight
(Fran-
cis, Brown, & Pearson, 1991).
To maximize the empirical validity of the personality dimen-
sions, items were dropped from the personality scales if they
were
explicitly related to one of the attitude dimensions or if they did
not contribute to the intended trait. The nature of the
dimensions
remained unchanged, however. The empirical validity problem
is
most evident with the Psychoticism scale. Although the
Psychoti-
cism sub-scale is established in the extant literature, in many
re-
cent cases it has less than ideal levels of inter-item reliability.
This is most likely because the Psychoticism scale was
developed
in the 1950s, and then revised in the mid 1980s. Thus, the tech-
niques available to assess reliability when the scales were
created
were impoverished. Retaining items that do not meaningfully
con-
tribute to the latent factor introduces error into the model.
There-
fore, dropping these specific items increased the precision with
which we can estimate the trait and the confidence we have in
our analyses.
Another common problem that plagues the study of personality
and politics is the clear distinction between attitudinal items
and
personality items. The tautology problem is more common in
other
personality measures, like the NEO-PI-R Openness to
Experience
scale where several items explicitly tap political concepts (see
Costa & McCrae, 1992). Remaining cognizant of this problem,
we
identified three items in the Psychoticism scale that had also the
possibility of overlapping with attitudinal items. Specifically,
the
items ‘‘Would being in debt worry you?” and ‘‘Do you think
people
spend too much time safeguarding their future with savings and
insurances?” potentially overlap with economic political
attitudes,
while ‘‘Do you think marriage is old-fashioned and should be
done
away with?” potentially overlaps with both social and religious
political attitudes. Importantly, the factor loadings of these
items
are not overwhelming and measures of fit do not decline with
the removal of these items. Removing potentially tautological
items is not meant to fundamentally alter the concept, but rather
ensure that the items that comprise the construct are not inher-
ently political. In other words, this should ensure that the
observed
308 B. Verhulst et al. / Personality and Individual Differences
49 (2010) 306–316
relationship is between the traits and not a function of similar
items.
Political and social attitudes were assessed utilizing a 50-item
index of political and social issues contemporary at the time of
the survey (see Posner, Baker, Heath, & Martin, 1996). In a
Wilson
and Patterson (1968) format, respondents indicated if they
agreed,
disagreed, or were uncertain about their attitudes towards a
wide
range of issues across several ideological dimensions (e.g.,
Death
Penalty, Bible Truth, Legalized Prostitution, Immigration,
Stiffer Jail
Terms, etc.). Of these 50 items, 26 of the most explicitly
political
items were used to construct four political attitude dimensions:
Religious, Sex, Out-groups and Punishment dimensions (see
details
below). To facilitate comparisons between this study and
similar
others, we also created a Liberal–Conservative ideology
measure
using all 50 items as the scale was intended and commonly
found
in the extant literature (see Wilson & Patterson, 1968). A
complete
list of the items can be found in Appendix 2.
Unlike personality, there is no consensual model or theory of
socio-political attitudes. Although there is wide agreement on a
general, abstract left–right spectrum, the attitudes that comprise
this spectrum remain elusive and insofar as specific attitude
facets
go, they remain underdeveloped (see Bouchard, 2009; Eagly &
Chaiken, 1993). As such, to identify the attitude factors, we
relied
heavily on the face validity of the scales and the empirical
results.
2.2. Preliminary analysis
A confirmatory factor analysis (CFA) with missing data was
con-
ducted specifying the four personality factors and the four
political
ideology attitude factors identified above (see Appendices 1 and
2
for the specifics of the CFA). For the personality traits, higher
scores
indicated higher levels of the traits, and for the ideological
dimen-
sions higher scores indicated the liberal position. The model fit
sta-
tistics for this class of Confirmatory Factor Models are
impoverished, especially for relatively complex factor models
such
as those estimated in this paper. To compensate for this, we
esti-
mated a comparable non-nested model with list-wise deletion,
where the factor loadings and the factor correlations were
essen-
tially unchanged. In this model the RMSEA was 0.044, the CFI
was 0.870, and the TLI was 0.917. These fit indices suggest that
the model fits the data very well. As such, we are confident in
the factor structure we estimate.
2.3. Phenotypic correlations between personality traits and
ideological
dimensions
We limited our analyses to the relationships between person-
ality traits and ideological dimensions to those relationships
that had correlations greater than 0.20. This decision was based
on the increased reliably of disentangling covariance between
the phenotypic traits (Cohen, 1988). The first stage of our anal-
ysis focused on identifying the phenotypic relationships be-
tween the personality traits and the social attitudes (see
Table 1). The correlations between our attitude dimensions
and personality traits were much larger than those reported
for correlations between overall Conservatism–Liberalism
(ideol-
ogy) and personality traits in other studies (see Carney, Jost,
Gosling, & Potter, 2008; Verhulst et al., 2009). Further, the
gen-
eral ideology scale also was highly correlated with several per-
sonality dimensions.
As can be seen in Table 1, several relationships stand out. Most
notably, Psychoticism was strongly related to more conservative
Religious, Sex and Punishment attitudes in both sexes. This spe-
cific pattern of relationships was consistent with the original re-
search on the Authoritarian personality (Adorno et al., 1950)
and the magnitude is consistent with the relationship between
authoritarianism and general conservatism (Altemeyer, 1998;
Stenner, 2005). We expected that Psychoticism, by virtue of its
theoretical relationship with Authoritarianism, would be associ-
ated with Religious, Sex, and Punishment attitudes, as these
atti-
tudes are central to the concept of Authoritarianism (see
Bouchard, 2009).
These same political attitudes were also correlated with Social
Desirability in both sexes, although in the opposite (liberal)
direc-
tion and to a lesser extent. Although the personality and politics
literature has generally ignored the connection between Social
Desirability and ideological or attitudinal dimensions, this is
the
second study to link Social Desirability with liberal social
attitudes
in very large genetically informative samples (see Verhulst et
al.,
2009). Thus, it appears that an empirical relationship exists,
even
though the theoretical justifications for this relationship are not
yet resolved.
As a point of comparison with other personality and politics
studies, we also examined the relationship between the additive
50-item general ideology scale and the four personality traits.
The general ideology dimension was also highly related to
Psychot-
icism and Social Desirability. The most interesting feature of
the
pattern of correlations between the general ideology dimension
and the personality traits mirrored the Religious and Sex
attitudes.
Items along these dimensions comprised the majority of the
items
in the scale, and thus appeared to drive the relationships
observed
between the general ideology factor and the separate personality
traits. Importantly, by narrowly focusing in the general ideology
factor the relationships between extraversion and neuroticism
and the political attitudes, which we discuss below, would be
obscured.
The relationship between Neuroticism and liberal Out-group
attitudes also corresponds with recent findings in the emerging
lit-
erature within the personality and politics sub-field.
Specifically,
Neuroticism has been linked with liberal economic ideological
atti-
tudes in several samples and is consistent with Verhulst et al.
(2009) analysis of US twins. Importantly, attitudes on
immigration
were part of the US economic ideology dimension in that study,
but
here in our Australian population they were part of the Out-
group
dimension.
Finally, we found a relationship between Extraversion and con-
servative Sex attitudes in these analyses. Very few
contemporary
studies in the personality and politics literature have found
signif-
icant relationships between Extraversion and attitudinal or ideo-
logical predispositions. However, our finding is consistent with
an older literature showing Extraversion to be positively related
to Authoritarianism and Conservatism (Eysenck & Wilson,
1978;
Pearson & Greatorex, 1981; Ray, 1980; for a recent replication
see Carney et al., 2008). Subsequent analyses often failed to
repli-
cate these earlier findings. We believe part of the failure to
repli-
cate these relationships is due to the left–right generalization of
attitude factors. However, greater Extraversion has been long
established as a significant influence on sex drive, earlier age of
first intercourse, promiscuity, orgasm, and other activities
(Cooper,
Agocha, & Sheldon, 2000; Eysenck, 1976; Miller et al., 2004;
Schenk
& Pfrang, 2005). Thus, regarding the relationship between
Extra-
version and the Sex attitudes factor, the direction of the
correlation
is in the opposite direction than expected. At first glance it
appears
for Australians, the relationship between Extraversion and atti-
tudes on sex are quite different from the relationship between
Extraversion and actual sexual behavior. Future studies are
needed
to replicate this finding.
2.4. Genetic analyses
Structural equation models incorporating genetic, shared and
unique environmental sources of covariation allowed us to ex-
Table 1
Phenotypic correlations between personality and attitude
dimensions for male (N = 3449) and female (N = 6066) twins.
Psychoticism Extraversion Neuroticism Social Desirability
Female twins
Religious attitudes �.534 �.124 �.003 .273
Sex attitudes �.601 �.266 �.049 .359
Out-group attitudes �.106 �.080 .204 .211
Punishment attitudes �.451 �.047 .094 .282
General ideology �.566 �.177 .001 .357
Male twins
Religious attitudes �.496 �.035 .008 .237
Sex attitudes �.570 �.196 �.033 .318
Out-group attitudes �.004 �.010 .166 .100
Punishment attitudes �.421 .047 .021 .231
General ideology �.495 �.061 �.008 .261
Note: Higher scores on the Personality factors denoted higher
levels of the traits. Higher scores on the attitude factors denote
more liberal responses for all attitude
dimensions. All relationships except that between Psychoticism
and Out-group attitudes were statistically significant at .01 or
better. However, conventional levels of
statistical significance do not focus our attention on
substantively meaningful relationships. Here we focus only on
significant relationships where the effect size is medium or
larger (correlations greater P.20 in bold; see Cohen, 1988).
Table 2
Between co-twin correlations by zygosity.
MZM MZF DZM DZF DZOS
Personality traits
Psychoticism .536 .615 .381 .414 .370
Extraversion .516 .488 .193 .212 .211
Neuroticism .367 .438 .142 .213 .085
Social Desirability .383 .520 .318 .394 .267
Social attitudes
Religious .592 .632 .506 .513 .536
Sex .592 .668 .452 .558 .505
Out-groups .600 .559 .482 .402 .361
Punishment .567 .587 .407 .339 .433
General ideology .631 .675 .517 .552 .524
N (twin pairs) 646 1385 384 839 363
B. Verhulst et al. / Personality and Individual Differences 49
(2010) 306–316 309
plore the nature of the relationships among personality and
attitudes dimensions. Table 2 provides the twin correlations
separated by zygosity and sex for the personality traits and so-
cio-political attitude dimensions. The correlations between MZ
twins were substantially larger than the correlations between
same-sex DZ twins, providing initial support for the presence of
genetic influences and reason to explore structural models to
quantify those influences. However, the MZ–DZ correlation
differ-
ences were much greater for most personality dimensions than
for the political attitude sub-factors, suggesting significant
shared
environmental influences on the attitude dimensions. For com-
pleteness we also tabulate the correlations for the DZ opposite-
sex pairs. For most variables the correlations for opposite-sex
DZ twins were similar to the correlations for same-sex DZ
twins,
but for Social Desirability and attitudes to Out-groups the
DZOS
correlation was a bit lower, suggesting that familial effects on
these scales were somewhat different in males and females for
these constructs. For simplicity we therefore present results of
separate analyses of male and female twins, omitting the oppo-
site-sex pairs.
Results of estimating the genetic and shared and non-shared
environmental variance components for each measure alone,
using
maximum likelihood estimation in Mx (Neale, Boker, Xie, &
Maes,
2003), are presented in Table 3. Components of variance due to
Additive Genetic (A), Common environmental (C), and Unique
Environmental (E) influence are tabulated (for detailed explana-
tions of the methodology and theory, along with limitations and
recent criticisms see Medland & Hatemi, 2009; Neale & Cardon,
1992).
The results were largely consistent with the expectations based
on the twin correlations and the long-standing personality
litera-
ture. Specifically, Psychoticism, Extraversion, and Neuroticism
were characterized by large additive genetic and unique
environ-
mental components, while the common environmental
component
was not significant. For Social Desirability, the common
environ-
ment variance was statistically significant but the additive
genetic
component for males was not. In contrast, in both sexes, the
addi-
tive genetic, shared and unique environmental variance compo-
nents for almost all the attitude scales were significant. The
shared environment variance accounted for large portions of the
variance in social attitudes. Importantly, the variance
components
of the general ideology scale are very similar to those of both
the
Sex and the Religious factors. The only attitude that differed
from
this trend was Punishment attitudes, where the common
environ-
ment variance was not significant for females.
2.5. Cholesky decomposition
The preceding steps were necessary to examine the complex
relationships between personality traits and political attitude
dimensions. Several of the personality dimensions were
correlated
with different attitude dimensions at the phenotypic level. Our
main interest, however, lay in exploring the nature of the
pheno-
typic relationships. As such, we undertook a series of bivariate
Cholesky decompositions (see Fig. 1) to evaluate the sources of
covariation. The constraints and model specifications are
explained
in detail in Medland and Hatemi (2009). Since we wished to
know
how much of the variance in attitudes could be explained by
per-
sonality, we put the personality factors first in our analyses.
Tables 4 and 5 display the standardized path coefficients from
the Cholesky analyses for females and males, respectively.
Cells
with dashes indicate that the paths were constrained to zero.
This
was done where the univariate variance decomposition models
de-
scribed in the previous section did not account for a statistically
reliable portion of the variance in the trait. These constraints
pre-
vented model instability arising from very small sources of
shared
variance.
As is shown in Table 4 for males and Table 5 for females, the
relationships between Psychoticism and Religious, Sex, and
Pun-
ishment attitudes were largely due to additive genetic
covariation.
Performing a simple algebraic transformation of the path coeffi-
cients, we found that the majorities of the covariation between
Psychoticism and Punishment, Sex, and Religious attitudes was
due to genetic covariance.
Table 3
Estimated variance components for personality and ideology
dimensions, separately by variable.
Males Females
a2 c2 e2 a2 c2 e2
Psychoticism .559 (.51, .60) – .441 (.39, .49) .602 (.57, .63) –
.399 (.37, .43)
Extraversion .503 (.45, .55) – .497 (.45, .55) .474 (.44, .51) –
.526 (.49, .56)
Neuroticism .362 (.30, .42) – .638 (.58, .70) .438 (.40, .48) –
.562 (.52, .60)
Social Desirability .141 (.00, .36) .230 (.03, .29) .629 (.57, .70)
.368 (.23, .51) .141 (.01, .27) .490 (.46, .53)
Religious attitudes .151 (.00, .32) .435 (.28, .57) .415 (.37, .46)
.226 (.12, .34) .393 (.29, .49) .381 (.35, .41)
Sex attitudes .271 (.11, .45) .316(.15, .46) .413 (.37, .46) .212
(.11, .32) .443 (.34, .53) .346 (.32, .37)
Out-group attitudes .268 (.11, .44) .336 (.18, .47) .395 (.35, .44)
.311 (.19, .44) .244 (.13, .35) .445 (.41, .48)
Punishment attitudes .285 (.11, .47) .277 (.10, .43) .438 (.39,
.61) .491 (.36, .61) .089 (.00, .21) .420 (.39, .45)
General ideology .212 (.06, .37) .408 (.29, .54) .380 (.35, .42)
.244 (.15, .31) .420 (.34, .51) .335 (.31, .36)
Note: Estimates in italics are not significant.
a c e
A11 to the
Personality
Trait
C11 to the
Personality
Trait
E11 to the
Personality
Trait
Personality Trait
and Social
Attitudes A12 to the
Social
Attitude
A22 to the
Social
Attitude
C12 to the
Social
Attitude
C22 to the
Social
Attitude
E12 to the
Social
Attitude
E22 to the
Social
Attitude
Personality
Trait
Social
Attitude
A1 A2 C2C1 E1 E2
Fig. 1. General Cholesky model.
Table 4
Bivariate cholesky results for personality traits and social
attitudes for males.
a c e
Psychoticism and Religious Attitudes .758 – .653
(.72, .79) (.62, .69)
�.476 .615 – – �.201 .596
(�.52, �.43) (.58, .65) (�.24, �.16) (.57, .563)
Psychoticism and Sex Attitudes .757 – .654
(.72, .79) (.62, .69)
�.539 .562 – – �.249 .577
(�.58, �.49) (.52, .60) (�.29, �.21) (.55, .61)
Psychoticism and Punishment Attitudes .760 – .650
(.73, .79) (.61, .69)
�.332 .680 – – �.260 .600
(�.39, �.28) (.65, .71) (�.31, �.22) (.57, .63)
Psychoticism & Ideology .756 – .655
(.72, .78) (.62, .69)
�.486 .632 – – �.192 .572
(�.53, �.44) (.59, .67) (�.24, �.15) (.54, .60)
Extraversion and Sex Attitudes .715 – .700
(.67, .75) (.66, .74)
�.182 .757 – – �.108 .619
(�.24, �.12) (.73, .78) (�.15, �.06) (.59, .65)
Neuroticism and Out�Group Attitudes .610 – .792
(.55, .66) (.75, .83)
.183 .764 – – .061 .615
(.11, .26) (.73, .79) (.02, .11) (.58, .65)
Social Desirability and Religious Attitudes .386 .495 .778
(.08, .60) (.23, .62) (.74, .82)
.362 .138 .106 .651 .055 .642
(.03, .56) (.00, .49) (�.16, .36) (.52, .72) (.01, .10) (.61, .68)
310 B. Verhulst et al. / Personality and Individual Differences
49 (2010) 306–316
Table 5
Bivariate cholesky results for personality traits and social
attitudes for females.
a c e
Psychoticism & Religious Attitudes .783 – .622
(.76, .80) (.60, .65)
�.508 .615 – – �.209 .567
(�.54, �.48) (.59, .64) (�.24, �.18) (.55, .59)
Psychoticism & Sex Attitudes .783 – .623
(.76, .80) (.60, .65)
�.564 .594 – – �.246 .518
(�.59, �.53) (.57, .62) (�.27, �.22) (.50, .54)
Psychoticism & Punishment .787 – .617
(.77, .80) (.59, .64)
�.390 .658 – – �.231 .601
(�.43, �.35) (.63, .68) (�.26, �.20) (.58, .62)
Psychoticism & Ideology .783 – .623
(.76, .80) (.60, .65)
�.580 .586 – – �.170 .540
(�.61, �.55) (.56, .61) (�.20, �.14) (.52, .56)
Extraversion and Sex Attitudes .691 – .723
(.66, .72) (.70, .75)
�.265 .776 – – �.109 .562
(�.31, �.22) (.76, .79) (�.14, .08) (.54, .58)
Neuroticism and Out�Group Attitudes .664 – .748
(.63, .69) (.72, .77)
.204 .726 – – .094 .650
(.16, .25) (.70, .75) (.06, .13) (.63, .67)
Social Desirability and Religious Attitudes .498 .520 .694
(.35, .61) (.40, .61) (.67, .72)
.117 .428 .339 .555 .051 .614
(�.05, .28) (.28, .54) (.20, .50) (.41, .65) (.02, .08) (.59, .64)
Social Desirability and Sex Attitudes .502 .518 .693
(.36, .62) (.39, .61) (.67, .72)
.123 .405 .435 .539 .095 .577
(�.03, .28) (.27, .51) (.30, .60) (.35, .63) (.07, .13) (.55, .60)
Social Desirability and Punishment Attitudes .504 .517 .692
(.37, .61) (.40, .61) (.67, .72)
.057 .671 .355 .000 .096 .642
(�.11, .21) (.59, .72) (.22, .47) (�.31, .31) (.06, .13) (.62, .66)
Social Desirability and Out�Group Attitudes .517 .505 .691
(.38, .63) (.37, .60) (.67, .72)
.011 .563 .290 .393 .080 .662
(�.17, .18) (.44, .67) (.13, .47) (.00, .53) (.05, .11) (.64, .69)
Social Desirability and Ideology .515 .506 .692
(.38, .63) (.38, .60) (.67, .72)
.096 .450 .504 .450 .070 .572
(�.06, .25) (.33, .55) (.36, .69) (.00, .58) (.04, .10) (.55, .60)
Note: Estimates in italics were not significant. Again,
parameters with a lower confidence bound of zero should be
interpreted as not statistically significant.
Table 4 (continued)
a c e
Social Desirability and Sex Attitudes .384 .497 .778
(.10, .60) (.24, .62) (.74, .82)
.445 .274 .137 .548 .104 .631
(.11, .67) (.00, .49) (�.16, .38) (.39, .65) (.06, .15) (.60, .67)
Social Desirability and Punishment Attitudes .399 .484 .779
(.00, .61) (.21, .63) (.74, .82)
.035 .543 .299 .416 .086 .659
(�.64, .65) (.00, .70) (.04, .60) (.00, .60) (.04, .14) (.62, .70)
Social Desirability and Ideology .397 .486 .779
(.00, .61) (.21, .63) (.74, .82)
.179 .427 .323 .549 .066 �.612
(�.53, .58) (.00, .59) (.09, .68) (.00, .68) (.02, .11) (�.65,
�.58)
Note: Estimates in italics were not significant. To solve
estimation problems in a limited number of models we bounded
the parameters to have a lower confidence bound of
zero. Thus, parameters with a lower confidence bound of zero
should be interpreted as not statistically significant.
B. Verhulst et al. / Personality and Individual Differences 49
(2010) 306–316 311
Importantly, although the paths from Psychoticism to the
attitude dimensions (A12) were quite strong for each attitude
dimension, these paths were still smaller than the paths
accounting for the unique influence (A22) on the attitude
dimen-
sions. Thus, although there was a strong relationship between
Psychoticism and each attitude dimension, the majority of the
additive genetic variance in the attitude dimensions was not ac-
counted for by the genetic influences on Psychoticism. The
vari-
ance shared between the unique environmental component of
Psychoticism and the attitude dimensions were minimal (paths
E12), while the residual unique environmental variance in the
attitude dimensions were uniformly high (paths E22), which is
not surprising since they include measurement error. This pat-
tern of results did not vary substantially by sex. Again, the pat-
312 B. Verhulst et al. / Personality and Individual Differences
49 (2010) 306–316
tern of relationships for the general ideology scale mimicked
the
relationships for the Religious and Sex attitudes with Psychoti-
cism. Specifically, the phenotypic relationship between general
ideology and Psychoticism appeared to be driven by the additive
genetic influence.
Similar relationships existed between Extraversion and Sex
attitudes as well as between Neuroticism and Out-group atti-
tudes. In both cases, the connections between the personality
traits and the attitude dimensions were primarily accounted for
by the additive genetic covariance between the constructs rather
than by any environmental covariance. Again, this pattern of re-
sults was remarkably consistent across sexes. In both cases, the
phenotypic relationships were much smaller than the relation-
ships observed between Psychoticism and the attitude dimen-
sions. As such, there were correspondingly smaller paths A12 in
these models.
The relationship between Social Desirability and each attitude
dimension (Sex, Punishment, and Out-groups) deviated slightly
from the pattern of relationships identified for the other
personal-
ity traits. Specifically, for females both the additive genetic and
common environment variance components were significant for
Social Desirability. Thus, this necessitated estimating Cholesky
models with common environmental pathways. To ensure that
the male model was broadly comparable with the female model,
we estimated the same parameters even though the additive ge-
netic variance in Social Desirability was not statistically
significant
when it was measured alone in males.
The Cholesky results for Social Desirability were generally con-
sistent across sexes. Specifically, the relationships between
Social
Desirability and the attitude dimensions rested primarily in the
common environmental covariances. For females, the common
environmental covariations (paths C12) between Social
Desirabil-
ity and the attitude dimensions were substantively large and sta-
tistically significant for all political attitudes. Furthermore, the
additive genetic covariances (paths A12) were substantively
small
and statistically not significant for all political attitudes. The
pat-
tern for males was only slightly different. For the Religious and
Punishment attitude dimensions, the common environmental
covariances (paths C12) were statistically significant and
substan-
tively large. For Sex attitudes, however, the common
environmen-
tal covariance was much smaller and not statistically
significant.
Furthermore, for Religious and Sex attitudes, the additive
genetic
covariances with Social Desirability were statistically
significant.
Remember, however, that the additive genetic component was
not statistically significant for Social Desirability in the
univariate
models for males. Furthermore, we had many more female than
male twins, giving us greater power to detect significant results
in the female sample.
While single population based results should be accepted with
caution, the current results are broadly consistent with analyses
on data from an equally large US twin sample (see Verhulst
et al., 2009). Specifically, the current study replicated the
findings
from a completely separate population of twins in a different
country, and therefore increases generalizability of the effects.
Fi-
nally, as with the other personality traits, the result for the gen-
eral ideology factor mirrored the Religious attitude factor.
Specifically, for both males and females, the primary source of
covariance between Social Desirability and any of the attitude
factors was common environmental covariance rather than addi-
tive genetic covariance.
3. Discussion
The results in this paper offer a more fine grained explanation
for the relationship between personality traits and social atti-
tudes than that captured by looking only at the Conservatism–
Liberalism super factor (ideology). Specifically, ideology was
strongly and negatively correlated with Psychoticism and posi-
tively correlated with Social Desirability. These same relation-
ships were also seen with the Religious, Sex, and Punishment
attitude sub-factors but not with the attitudes toward Out-
groups, where the correlation with Psychoticism was negligible.
In addition we also saw a modest (��0.2) negative correlation
between Extraversion and liberal attitudes to sex and a positive
correlation (�0.2) between Neuroticism and liberal attitudes to
Out-groups. All these correlations were remarkably consistent
between sexes.
Consistent with the existing literature, we showed that individ-
ual differences in both personality traits and political attitudes
could
be decomposed into genetic and environmental variance compo-
nents. For personality traits environmental influences were
over-
whelmingly unique to the individual, except for Social
Desirability
where there was an important component of shared
environmental
influence. For attitudes, on the other hand, we saw substantial
genet-
ic influences, but large components of shared environmental
vari-
ance as well. However, the common environmental covariance
is
likely to be confounded in some part by assortative mating (see
Eaves et al., 1999; Eaves & Hatemi, 2008). Indeed, the spousal
corre-
lations for Social Desirability was 0.25 and ranged from 0.45 to
0.65
for the attitudinal dimensions (p < .001, N = 3535). Finally,
using a
multivariate genetic model, we demonstrated that the
phenotypic
relationships between personality and attitudes were primarily
functions of a single source of covariance, genetic in the case of
the
correlations of Psychoticism, Neuroticism, and Extraversion
with
attitude dimensions in both males and females, and shared
environ-
ment for correlations between Social Desirability and attitudes.
In line with our expectations, Psychoticism was strongly related
to more conservative positions on Religious, Sex, and
Punishment
attitude dimensions. This finding is consistent with previous
theo-
rizing that suggests these attitude dimensions should be central
to
Authoritarian concerns, and that Authoritarianism, Traditional
Conservatism, and Psychoticism are intertwined (Adorno et al.,
1950; Bouchard, 2009).
The correlations between Psychoticism and the attitudinal
dimensions we demonstrate in this paper are larger than those
presented in other similar papers using other omnibus person-
ality theories; however, the reported relationships are consis-
tent with the theoretical relationship between Psychoticism
and various measures of Authoritarianism, like Adorno et al.’s
(1950) F scale, Altemeyer’s (1981, 1996) RWA scale, and Feld-
man’s (2003) Social Conformity–Autonomy Scale. Notably,
both
the F and the RWA scales have been strongly criticized for
including explicit political content in the measurement of
Authoritarianism, while the development of the Social Confor-
mity–Autonomy scale goes to great lengths to remove any
explicitly political content from the measurement of authoritar-
ianism, and thus avoid confounding the personality trait with
attitudinal preferences. Therefore, the magnitude of the rela-
tionships between the attitudinal variables and Psychoticism
are not unexpected.
A similar, but substantially weaker relationship existed be-
tween Extraversion and more conservative attitudes on the Sex
attitude dimension. This finding is opposite to what might be
ex-
pected based on Extraversion’s relationship to mode liberal
sexual
behavior (see Cooper et al., 2000). However, such a finding is
in line
with older studies of conservatism and personality (see Eysenck
&
Wilson, 1978; Pearson & Greatorex, 1981; Ray, 1980). We see
two
possible explanations for this relationship. First, there was a
rea-
sonably large correlation between Psychoticism and
Extraversion
(r = 0.37). As such, the relationship between Extraversion and
Sex
attitudes that we observed may have been a function of the very
B. Verhulst et al. / Personality and Individual Differences 49
(2010) 306–316 313
strong relationship between Psychoticism and Sex attitudes.
Alter-
natively, as noted above, Extraversion has two sub-factors:
affilia-
tion/sociability and agency/impulsiveness. Thus, the phenotypic
relationship may have been driven by the agency/impulsiveness
sub-factor. We leave it to future research to disentangle these
possibilities.
On the other hand, the more Neurotic our twins were, the more
‘‘liberal” their attitudes toward Out-groups (e.g. immigration,
mul-
ticulturalism). This echoed the relationship found between
Neurot-
icism and more liberal attitudes on economic issues in a large
US
sample, as the Australia Out-group dimension and the US Eco-
nomic dimension shared one important item in particular – atti-
tudes to immigration (see Verhulst et al., 2009).
One of the most interesting, but least explored, personality
traits in the context of political attitudes is Social Desirability.
The higher respondents scored on this dimension, the more
likely
they were to support liberal positions on Religious, Sex, and
Pun-
ishment attitudes. Overall the relationships between attitudes
and Social Desirability were the opposite of the relationships
be-
tween Psychoticism and attitudes, though substantially weaker.
In this study, as in Verhulst et al. (2009), Social Desirability
corre-
lated with more liberal social attitudes.
In sum, the relationships between Psychoticism, Extraversion,
and Neuroticism and all attitude dimensions (Sex, Punishment,
and Religious) were largely driven by common genetic influ-
ences. For Social Desirability the pattern that emerged was
the vast majority of covariation between Social Desirability
and each attitude dimension was due to the similarity in shared
environment, though this effect was less consistent in males.
Regardless of the source of covariance between personality
and attitude dimensions, the overwhelming portion of individ-
ual differences remained unique to each trait, with one excep-
tion – Psychoticism and Sex attitudes shared some 40% of
their covariance.
Of central importance to our findings, the relationships between
personality traits and political ideologies were not simple or
uni-
form: different attitude dimensions correlated with different
per-
sonality traits to varying degrees. Importantly, if we had
focused
only on the relationship between the general ideology factor and
the personality traits we would have missed several key
relation-
ships, including the relationship between Neuroticism and more
liberal attitudes, which seemed to be restricted to economic or
group competition attitude dimensions. Furthermore, the
intrigu-
ing relationship between Extraversion and Sex attitudes would
also have gone unnoticed. Recent research on personality and
pol-
itics has missed these findings because it focused only on the
gen-
eral ideological factor.
Do you prefer to go your own way rather than act by the rules?
Do you stop to think things over before doing anything?
Would you take drugs which may have strange or dangerous eff
Do good manners and cleanliness matter much to you?
Would you like other people to be afraid of you?
Is it better to follow society’s rules than go your own way?
Are you a talkative person?
Are you rather lively?
Do you enjoy meeting new people?
ects?
Our analysis has brought together the foundational contribu-
tions of H.J. Eysenck to personality and attitudes
dimensionality,
and the genetic studies of attitudes initiated by L.J. Eaves and
N.G. Martin and further developed by T.J. Bouchard.
Bouchard’s
(2009) argument that combining Authoritarianism,
Religiousness
and Conservatism implies that whatever relationship exists be-
tween personality and attitudes, is not that one predicts the
other,
but that both traits contain elements that are simply different
rep-
resentations of the same constructs. Such a position is supported
by the results we show here.
The finding that the majority of covariation between Psychot-
icism and attitudes is due to genetic variance provides strong
evidence that a simple environmentally driven causal theory
from personality to politics is not suitable for such a complex
relationship. It is important to keep in mind that genes are mul-
tifaceted: there is no specific gene for a personality trait or for
a
political attitude, even if individual genes can be associated
with
specific attitudes or personality traits. However, the
relationship
between Social Desirability and the attitude dimensions exhib-
ited a remarkably different pattern of results. The locus of the
relationship in this case rested on shared environmental influ-
ences on the two traits. Due to the significant spousal correla-
tions for both Social Desirability and socio-political attitudes, it
is likely that some part of the shared environmental covariance
is attributed to assortative mating. The result provides some
support for existing social science expectations about the
impor-
tance of environmental influences for the relationship between
personality traits and social attitudes (see Mondak & Halperin,
2008). The social nature of Social Desirability makes the
connec-
tion between this personality trait and the associated social atti-
tudes reasonable. However, this environmental relationship is
the exception to our understanding of personality and politics
based on our findings here and in other studies (see Verhulst
et al., 2009).
These analyses should be seen as the beginning of a new stage
in the research connecting personality and politics. At this
point,
we know that the patterns of relationships are more complex
than
those suggested by previous models. That is, we cannot assume
causality, nor focus only on a single left–right ideological
spectrum.
Future research focusing on both active and passive gene–
environ-
ment covariation using extended kinship designs, and gene–
envi-
ronment interaction models are underway to further explore
these patterns.
Appendix 1. Confirmatory factor structure of the EPQ
Psychoticism Extraversion Neuroticism Social
Desirability
.57
�.31
.79
�.42
.38
�.46
.66
.71
.60
(continued on next page)
Appendix A (continued)
Psychoticism Extraversion Neuroticism Social
Desirability
Can you usually let yourself go and enjoy yourself at a lively
party? .76
Do you usually take the initiative in making new friends? .69
Can you easily put some life into a rather dull party? .81
Do you tend to keep in the background on social occasions?
�.83
Do you like mixing with people? .73
Do you often take on more activities than you have time for?
.26
Do you like plenty of bustle and excitement around you? .60
Do you like doing things in which you have to act quickly? .57
Are you mostly quiet when you are with other people? �.75
Do other people think of you as being very lively? .81
Can you get a party going? .80
Would you call yourself happy-go-lucky? .49
Does your mood often go up and down? .69
Do you ever feel ‘just miserable’ for no reason? .64
Are you an irritable person? .62
Are your feelings easily hurt? .65
Do you often feel ‘fed-up’? .72
Would you call yourself a nervous person? .63
Are you a worrier? .75
Would you call yourself tense or ‘high-strung’? .65
Do you worry too long after an embarrassing experience? .62
Do you suffer from ‘nerves’? .59
Do you often feel lonely? .66
Are you often troubled about feelings of guilt? .66
If you say you will do something, do you always keep your
promise? .49
Were you every greedy by helping yourself to more than your
share? �.63
Have you ever blamed someone for what was really your fault?
�.59
Are all your habits good and desirable ones? .59
Have you ever taken anything that belonged to someone else?
�.68
Have you ever broken or lost something belonging to someone
else? �.55
Have you ever said anything bad or nasty about anyone? �.71
As a child, were you ever ‘fresh’ towards your parents? �.51
Have you ever cheated at a game? �.70
Have you ever taken advantage of someone? �.76
Do you always practice what you preach? .57
Do you sometimes put off until tomorrow what you ought to do
today?
�.36
Appendix 2. Confirmatory factor structure of the Australian
socio-political attitude index
Religious Sex Out-groups Punishment
Bible Truth .84
Divine Law .74
Sabbath Observance .74
Church Authority .76
Divorce �.60
Evolution Theory �.55
Condom Machine �.86
Abortion �.78
Legalized Prostitution �.69
Casual Sex �.64
Chastity .63
Birth Control �.51
Surrogate Mothers �.50
Test Tube Babies �.44
Gay Rights �.54 �.46
Asian Immigration �.79
Multiculturalism �.73
314 B. Verhulst et al. / Personality and Individual Differences
49 (2010) 306–316
Appendix 2 (continued)
Religious Sex Out-groups Punishment
White Superiority .64
Conservationists �.49
Apartheid .46
Foreign Ownership �.34
Stiffer Jail Terms .82
Strict Rules .68
Death Penalty .59
Caning .48
Teenage Delinquency �.44
Note: In addition to the 26 items above, the following items
comprise the 50-item general conservatism scale: Privatization,
Medicare, Trade Unions, Patriotism, Voluntary
Euthanasia, Nuclear Power, Working Mothers, Inborn
Conscience, Government Welfare, Socialism, Herbal Remedies,
Charity Work, Suicide, Licensing Laws, Fluoridation,
Royalty, Women Judges, Disarmament, Censorship, White Lies,
Teenage Dole, Private Schools, Chiropractors, Defense
Spending, Legalized Modern Art. The items excluded
from the attitudinal factors did not contribute to the latent
attitudes and were thus omitted from the traits.
B. Verhulst et al. / Personality and Individual Differences 49
(2010) 306–316 315
References
Adorno, T. W., Frenkel-Brunswik, E., Levinson, D. J., &
Sanford, R. N. (1950). The
Authoritarian personality. New York: Harper and Row.
Altemeyer, R. A. (1981). Right-wing Authoritarianism.
Winnipeg: University of
Manitoba Press.
Altemeyer, R. A. (1996). The Authoritarian specter. Cambridge,
MA: Harvard
University Press.
Altemeyer, R. A. (1998). The other authoritarian personality.
Advances in
Experimental Social Psychology, 30, 41–92.
Bandura, A. (2001). Social cognitive theory: An agentic
perspective. Annual Review of
Psychology, 52(1), 1.
Block, J., & Block, J. H. (2006). Nursery school personality and
political orientation
two decades later. Journal of Research in Personality, 40, 734–
749.
Bouchard, T. J. Jr., (1994). Genes, environment, and
personality. Science, 264,
1700–1701.
Bouchard, T. J. Jr., (1997). The genetics of personality.
Handbook of psychiatric
genetics. CRC Press.
Bouchard, T. J. Jr, (2009). Authoritarianism religiousness and
conservatism: Is
obedience to authority the explanation for clustering
universality and
evolution? In E. Voland & W. Scheifenhovel (Eds.), The
biological evolution of
religious mind and behavior (pp. 165–180).
Dordrecht/Heidelberg/London/New
York: Springer.
Bouchard, T. J., Jr., Lykken, D. T., McGue, M., Segal, N. L., &
Tellegen, A. (1990).
Sources of human psychological differences: The Minnesota
Study of Twins
Reared Apart. Science, 250(4978), 223–228.
Bouchard, T. J., Jr., & McGue, M. (2003). Genetic and
environmental influences on
human psychological differences. Journal of Neurobiology,
54(1), 4–45.
Caprara, G. V., & Cervone, D. (2000). Personality:
Determinants, dynamics and
potentials. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Carney, D. R., Jost, J. T., Gosling, S. D., & Potter, J. (2008).
The secret lives of liberals
and conservatives: Personality profiles, interaction styles, and
the things they
leave behind. Political Psychology, 29, 807–840.
Cattell, R. B. (1957). Personality and motivation structure and
measurement. New
York: World Book.
Cohen, J. (1988). Statistical power analysis for the behavioral
sciences (2nd ed.).
Hillsdale, NJ: Earlbaum.
Converse, P. E. (1964). The nature of belief systems in mass
publics. In D. E. Apter
(Ed.), Ideology and discontent (pp. 206–261). New York: Free
Press.
Cooper, M. L., Agocha, V. B., & Sheldon, M. S. (2000). A
motivational perspective on
risky behaviors: The role of personality and affect regulatory
processes. Journal
of Personality, 68(6).
Costa, P. T., Jr., & McCrae, R. R. (1992). Normal personality
assessment in clinical
practice: The NEO Personality Inventory. Psychological
Assessment, 4, 5–13.
Depue, R. A., & Collins, P. F. (1999). Neurobiology of the
structure of personality:
Dopamine, facilitation of incentive motivation, and
extraversion. Behavioral
Brain Science, 22(3), 491–517.
Eagly, A., & Chaiken, S. (1993). Psychology of attitudes. NY:
Harcourt, Brace
Jovanovich.
Eaves, L. J., & Eysenck, H. J. (1974). Genetics and the
development of social attitudes.
Nature, 249, 288–289.
Eaves, L. J., Eysenck, H. J., & Martin, N. G. (1989). Genes,
culture and personality; an
empirical approach. London: Academic Press.
Eaves, L. J., & Hatemi, P. K. (2008). Transmission of attitudes
toward abortion and
gay rights: Parental socialization or parental mate selection?
Behavior Genetics,
38, 247–256.
Eaves, L. J., Heath, A. C., Martin, N. G., Maes, H. H., Neale,
M. C., Kendler, K. S., et al.
(1999). Comparing the biological and cultural inheritance of
personality and
social attitudes in the Virginia 30,000 study of twins and their
relatives. Twin
Research, 2, 62–80.
Eysenck, H. J. (1954). The psychology of politics. New
Brunswick, NJ: Routledge.
Eysenck, H. (1976). Sex and personality. London Open Books.
Eysenck, H. J. (1990). Biological dimensions of personality. In
L. A. Pervin (Ed.),
Handbook of personality: Theory and research (pp. 249–276).
New York: Guilford.
Eysenck, H. J. (1991). Dimensions of personality: 16, 5 or 3?
Criteria for a taxonomic
paradigm. Personality and Individual Difference, 12, 773–790.
Eysenck, H. J., & Eysenck, M. W. (1985). Personality and
individual differences: A
natural science approach. New York: Plenum.
Eysenck, H. J., & Eysenck, S. B. G. (1997). Eysenck
Personality Questionnaire-Revised
(EPQ-R) and short scale (EPQ-RS). Madrid: TEA Ediciones.
Eysenck, S. B. G., Eysenck, H. J., & Barrett, P. (1985). A
revised version of the
Psychoticism scale. Personality and Individual Differences,
6(1), 21–29.
Eysenck, H. J., & Wilson, G. D. (1978). The psychological basis
of ideology. Lancaster:
MTP Press.
Feldman, S. (2003). Enforcing social conformity: A theory of
authoritarianism.
Political Psychology, 24, 41–74.
Francis, L. J., Brown, L. B., & Pearson, P. R. (1991). The dual
nature of the EPQ lie scale
among university students in Australia. Personality and
Individual Differences,
12(10), 989–991.
Gosling, S. D., Rentfrow, P. J., & Swann, W. B. Jr., (2003). A
very brief measure of the
big-five personality domains. Journal of Research in
Personality, 37, 504–528.
Hatemi, P. K., Medland, S. E., Morley, K. I., Heath, A. C., &
Martin, N. G. (2007). The
genetics of voting: An Australian Twin Study. Behavior
Genetics, 37(3), 435–448.
Jost, J. T., Federico, C. M., & Napier, J. L. (2009). Political
ideology: Its structure,
functions, and elective affinities. Annual Review of
Psychology, 60, 339–367.
Jost, J. T., Glaser, J., Kruglanski, A. W., & Sulloway, F. J.
(2003). Political conservatism
as motivated social cognition. Psychological Bulletin, 129(3),
339–375.
Jost, J. T., Napier, J. L., Thorisdottir, H., Gosling, S. D., Palfai,
T. B., & Ostafin, B. (2008).
Are needs to manage uncertainty and threat associated with
political
conservatism or ideological extremity? Personality and Social
Psychology
Bulletin, 33, 989–1007.
Leeson, P., & Heaven, P. C. L. (1999). Social attitudes and
personality. Australian
Journal of Psychology, 51, 19–24.
Martin, N. G. (1987). Genetic differences in drinking habits
alcohol metabolism and
sensitivity in unselected samples of twins. In H. W. Goedde &
D. P. Agarwal
(Eds.), Genetics and alcoholism (pp. 109–119). New York: Alan
R. Liss.
Martin, N. G., Eaves, L. J., Heath, A. C., Jardine, R., Feingold,
L. M., & Eysenck, H. J.
(1986). Transmission of social attitudes. Proceedings for the
National Academy of
Sciences, 83, 4364–4368.
McCloskey, H. (1958). Conservatism and personality. American
Political Science
Review, 52, 27–45.
McCrae, R. R. (1996). Social consequences of experiential
openness. Psychological
Bulletin, 120(3), 323–337.
Medland, S. E., & Hatemi, P. K. (2009). Political science,
biometric theory and twin
studies: A methodological introduction. Political Analysis,
17(2), 191–214.
Miller, J. D., Lynam, D., Zimmerman, R. S., Logan, T. K.,
Leukefeld, C., & Clayton, R.
(2004). The utility of the Five Factor Model in understanding
risky sexual
behavior. Personality and Individual Differences, 36(7), 1611–
1626.
Mischel, W., & Shoda, Y. (1998). Reconciling processing
dynamics and personality
dispositions. Annual Review of Psychology, 49, 229–258.
Mondak, J. J., & Halperin, K. D. (2008). A framework for the
study of personality and
political behaviour. British Journal of Political Science, 38,
335–362.
Neale, M. C., Boker, S., Xie, G., & Maes, H. (2003). Mx:
Statistical modeling (5th ed.).
Richmond, VA: Department of Psychiatry, Virginia
Commonwealth University.
Neale, M. C., & Cardon, L. L. (1992). Methodology for genetic
studies of twins and
families. Kluwer Academic.
Niemi, R. G., & Jennings, M. K. (1991). Issues and inheritance
in the formation of
party identification. American Journal of Political Science, 35,
970–988.
Pearson, P. R., & Greatorex, B. J. (1981). Do tough-minded
people hold tough-
minded attitudes? Current Psychology, 1(1), 45–48.
316 B. Verhulst et al. / Personality and Individual Differences
49 (2010) 306–316
Posner, S. F., Baker, L. A., Heath, A. C., & Martin, N. G.
(1996). Social contact; social
attitudes and twin similarity. Behavior Genetics, 26(2), 123.
Ray, J. J. (1980). Are Authoritarians extraverted? British
Journal of Social and Clinical
Psychology, 19, 147–148.
Riemann, R., Grubich, C., Hempel, S., Mergl, S., & Richter, M.
(1993). Personality and
attitudes towards current political topics. Personality and
Individual Differences,
15, 313–321.
Saucier, G. (2000). Isms and the structure of social attitudes.
Journal of Personality
and Social Psychology, 78, 366–385.
Schenk, J., & Pfrang, H. (2005). Extraversion, neuroticism and
sexual behavior:
Interrelationships in a sample of young men. Journal Archives
of Sexual Behavior,
15(6), 449–455.
Stenner, K. (2005). The Authoritarian dynamic. London:
Cambridge University Press.
Tellegen, A., Lykken, D. T., Bouchard, T. J., Jr., Wilcox, K. J.,
Segal, N. L., & Rich, S.
(1988). Personality similarity in twins reared apart and together.
Journal of
Personality and Social Psychology, 54(6), 1031–1039.
Truett, K. R., Eaves, L. J., Walters, E. E., Heath, A. C., Hewitt,
J. K., Meyer, J. M., et al.
(1994). A model system for the analysis of family resemblance
in extended
kinships of twins. Behaviour Genetics, 24, 35–49.
Van Hiel, A., Kossowska, M., & Mervielde, I. (2000). The
relationship between
openness to experience and political ideology. Personality and
Individual
Differences, 28(4), 741–751.
Van Hiel, A., Pandelaere, M., & Duriez, B. (2004). The impact
of need for closure on
conservative beliefs and racism: Differential mediation by
Authoritarian
submission and Authoritarian dominance. Personality and Social
Psychology
Bulletin, 30, 824–837.
Verhulst, B., Hatemi, P. K., & Eaves, L. J. (2009). Personality
traits and political
ideologies. In Paper presented at the 39th annual meeting of the
behavior genetics
association, Minneapolis, MN, June 17–20.
Waller, N. G., Kojetin, B. A., & Bouchard, T. J. (1990). Genetic
and environmental
influences on religious interests, attitudes, and values. A study.
Psychological
Science.
Wilson, G. D., & Patterson, J. R. (1968). A new measure of
conservatism. British
Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology, 7, 264–269.
Winter, D. G., & Barenbaum, N. B. (1999). History of modern
personality
theory and research. In L. A. Pervin & O. P. John (Eds.),
Handbook of
personality: Theory and research. New York, NY: The Guilford
Press (2nd
ed., pp. 3–27).
The nature of the relationship between personality traits and
political attitudesIntroductionMethods and
resultsMeasuresPreliminary analysisPhenotypic correlations
between personality traits and ideological dimensionsGenetic
analysesCholesky decompositionDiscussionConfirmatory factor
structure of the EPQConfirmatory factor structure of the
Australian socio-political attitude indexReferences
REGULAR ARTICLE
Personality Psychology: Lexical Approaches, Assessment
Methods, and Trait Concepts Reveal Only Half
of the Story—Why it is Time for a Paradigm Shift
Jana Uher
Published online: 8 February 2013
# The Author(s) 2013. This article is published with open access
at Springerlink.com
Abstract This article develops a comprehensive philosophy-of-
science for person-
ality psychology that goes far beyond the scope of the lexical
approaches, assessment
methods, and trait concepts that currently prevail. One of the
field’s most important
guiding scientific assumptions, the lexical hypothesis, is
analysed from meta-
theoretical viewpoints to reveal that it explicitly describes two
sets of phenomena
that must be clearly differentiated: 1) lexical repertoires and the
representations that
they encode and 2) the kinds of phenomena that are represented.
Thus far, personality
psychologists largely explored only the former, but have
seriously neglected studying
the latter. Meta-theoretical analyses of these different kinds of
phenomena and their
distinct natures, commonalities, differences, and interrelations
reveal that personality
psychology’s focus on lexical approaches, assessment methods,
and trait concepts
entails a) erroneous meta-theoretical assumptions about what
the phenomena being
studied actually are, and thus how they can be analysed and
interpreted, b) that
contemporary personality psychology is largely based on
everyday psychological
knowledge, and c) a fundamental circularity in the scientific
explanations used in trait
psychology. These findings seriously challenge the widespread
assumptions about the
causal and universal status of the phenomena described by
prominent personality
models. The current state of knowledge about the lexical
hypothesis is reviewed, and
implications for personality psychology are discussed. Ten
desiderata for future
research are outlined to overcome the current paradigmatic
fixations that are substan-
tially hampering intellectual innovation and progress in the
field.
Keywords Personality. Meta-theory. Methodology. Lexical
hypothesis . Lexical
approach . Traits . Personality assessment . Philosophy of
science . Big five model . Five
factor model
Integr Psych Behav (2013) 47:1–55
DOI 10.1007/s12124-013-9230-6
J. Uher (*)
Comparative Differential and Personality Psychology, Freie
Universität Berlin, Habelschwerdter
Allee 45, 14195 Berlin, Germany
e-mail: [email protected]
Science provides a special way of constructing knowledge about
the world. Unlike
nonscientific knowledge construction, science provides a way of
thinking simulta-
neously about phenomena and the means of producing
knowledge about them—this
presupposes meta-theory and methodology (Althusser and
Balibar 1970; Toomela
2011). Meta-theory refers to the philosophical assumptions
about the theoretical
nature of the phenomena to be studied and to the questions that
are asked about
them. Methodology refers to the ways (i.e., approaches) in
which these questions can
be answered and to the techniques (i.e., methods) that can
therefore be used (Sprung
and Sprung 1984).
All sciences have meta-theories. They determine which
elements of real
phenomena can be reduced to precisely those subsets of
elements that are
considered relevant to and defining of concrete scientific
phenomena and the
ways in which they can be reduced (Althusser and Balibar 1970,
p. 84, analysing
the philosophy of science of Marx 1867; Køppe 2012; Weber
1949). Meta-
theories determine what is considered data in a particular field
(in a particular
historical time; Kuhn 1962), and how the thus-defined data can
be analysed and
interpreted (Køppe 2012; Wagoner 2009). Hence, the first step
of reducing real
elements into facts is already a theoretical decision (Weber
1949, p. 173). “Alles
Faktische ist schon Theorie”—all facts are already theory
(Goethe 1907, p. 127).
In other words, “it is the theory which decides what can be
observed” (Einstein
to Heisenberg in 1926, cited in Heisenberg 1989, p. 10). Meta-
theories and
methodologies are the rules that govern the effective practices
of sciences.
Therefore, they should be explicated to enable researchers to
constantly scruti-
nise all levels from epistemology and ontology up to the
specific theories about
the phenomena studied (Toomela 2011). This also includes
rethinking the very
reduction of real phenomena into scientific phenomena (Utz
2005).
Contemporary psychology largely follows meta-theory and
methodology im-
plicitly, sometimes even “blindly” (Toomela 2011, p. 22)—this
occurs in partic-
ular when research methods are decided first and research
questions are adapted
to the methods rather than vice versa (Omi 2012; Westen 1996).
“The under-
standing that research methodology comprises an essential part
of scientific
theories about phenomena that are studied is not always brought
into the center
of theoretical reasoning” (Toomela 2009, p. 45). Ignoring the
meta-theoretical
assumptions underlying particular methods can result in
mismatches between
methods and research questions that hamper the scientific
understanding and
the explanations of the phenomena being studied—and thus
scientific progress
(Loftus 1996; Toomela and Valsiner 2010; Weber 1949). More
profoundly, a
priori decisions on methods preclude posing the primary
question of what the
phenomena to be studied actually are (Toomela 2011).
These meta-theoretical and methodological challenges are
characteristic of con-
temporary personality psychology and of taxonomic personality
research in particular
(Uher 2008a, b, 2011a, b; Uher, Methodological approaches to
personality taxono-
mies: The Behavioural Repertoire x Environmental Situations
Approach—A non-
lexical alternative, unpublished). The “discovery” of five major
dimensions of indi-
vidual differences is considered a milestone in modern Western
psychology (De Raad
1998; Digman 1990; Goldberg 1990). This “break through” was
made possible by the
assumption that people encode in their everyday languages all
those individual
2 Integr Psych Behav (2013) 47:1–55
differences that they perceive as most salient in everyday
encounters and that they
consider to be socially relevant. This so-called lexical
hypothesis, first articulated by
Galton (1884), has provided a stringent rationale for using the
lexica of human
languages as finite sources of information to unravel a few
major dimensions of
individual differences (Allport and Odbert 1936; Cattell 1943).
In English and some
other languages, the reduction of the pertinent lexical
repertoires to five major
dimensions has received the most support in Western scientific
communities
(Goldberg 1993; John et al. 1988). The lexical hypothesis also
suggests that enquiring
about the everyday psychological ideas that people develop of
themselves and of
other individuals could be a suitable method for the scientific
measurement of
personality (Block 2010; Westen 1996). Assessments by
laypeople have become
the standard methods of investigation (Baumeister et al. 2007;
Matthews et al.
2003) and the “primary source of data” in personality
psychology (Schwarz 1999,
p. 93). The assumptions expressed in the lexical hypothesis
might also contribute to
the fact that the prevailing strategies of the scientific
explanation of individual
differences largely follow structures that are deeply rooted in
everyday psychology.
In spite of its enormous importance as one of the most widely
used theoretical
assumptions to have guided personality psychology (Ashton and
Lee 2005), the
lexical hypothesis has remained untested (Toomela 2010a;
Westen 1996), and still
today, its statements have been considered only partially. This
article systematically
explores the meta-theoretical assumptions that underlie this
hypothesis. It starts by
highlighting the explicit reference of the lexical hypothesis to
two sets of phenomena
that must be clearly differentiated; these are, on the one hand,
people’s lexical
repertoires and the representations that these repertoires encode,
and, on the other
hand, the kinds of phenomena that are perceivable in everyday
life and that are being
represented. Thus far, personality psychology has focused
primarily on just one of
these two sets—on lexically encoded representations—but has
failed to systemati-
cally investigate the second set, namely, the kinds of
phenomena that are being
perceived and represented.
This article elaborates the distinct natures of the different kinds
of phenomena to
which the lexical hypothesis refers and explores their
commonalities, differences, and
interrelations from a philosophy-of-science perspective. This
allows for scrutiny to be
applied to the phenomena that are being lexically encoded and
sheds new light on the
fundamental questions of which kinds of phenomena can
actually be captured by
assessments—and which ones cannot. These analyses reveal that
erroneous meta-
theoretical assumptions underlie the established beliefs about
what these phenomena
actually are. They show that contemporary personality
psychology is largely based on
everyday psychological knowledge. Furthermore, the present
meta-theoretical anal-
yses allow us to scrutinise the prevailing psychological strategy
of explaining indi-
vidual differences—explicitly or implicitly—by assuming the
existence of “traits”.
The philosophy-of-science perspective identifies explanations
based on trait concepts
as fundamentally circular. These findings seriously challenge
the established assump-
tions about the causal and universal status of the phenomena
described by prominent
personality models.
Various researchers have carefully crafted serious concerns
about the established five
factor models of personality (e.g., Block 1995, 2001, 2010;
Eysenck 1992; McAdams
1992; Westen 1996), about assessment data and their
interpretation (e.g., Brower 1949;
Integr Psych Behav (2013) 47:1–55 3
Michell 1997, 2003; Omi 2012; Rosenbaum and Valsiner 2011;
Schwarz 2009; Trendler
2009; Valsiner 2012), and about the explanations provided by
trait psychology (e.g.,
Allport 1961; Bock 2000; Cervone et al. 2001; Lamiell 2003;
Mischel and Shoda 1994,
1995). This article moves these critical objections coherently to
meta-theoretical and
methodological levels of consideration. It expands them
substantially from the
philosophy-of-science perspective and elaborates a fundamental
critique of the lexical
approaches, assessments by laypeople, and trait concepts that
currently dominate per-
sonality psychology. It closes by outlining 10 desiderata for
future research to overcome
the limitations revealed in the analyses and to stimulate new
directions in the field.
Lexical Encodings—Constructs and Representations
The lexical hypothesis states that people encode in their
everyday languages all those
differences between individuals that they perceive to be salient
and that they consider
to be socially relevant in their everyday lives. Encodings about
differences within
individuals over time are not explicitly mentioned, however (see
below). Among the
many phenomena that individuals can perceive, recurrent
patterns are particularly
meaningful because such patterns may allow predictions of
future events while facing
the uncertainty of the future. Individuals therefore seek to
identify recurrent patterns
in their experiences with their personal world (Kelly 1955). To
describe, explain, and
to predict the likely occurrences of events, people construct and
represent their
experiences in private ideas (Valsiner 2012). These ideas are
called subjective or
individual representations (Jovchelovitch 2007; Moscovici
1984). Representations
referring to experiences with one’s own person and the—
especially social—world are
called personal constructs in personality psychology (Kelly
1955).
To communicate their perceptions and mental constructs of what
they perceive and
to negotiate socially shared meanings in order to cope with the
world collectively,
groups of individuals create and use social constructs. These
constructs are called
intersubjective or social representations (Jovchelovitch 2007) or
folk concepts in
personality psychology (Tellegen 1993). Constructs and
representations referring to
the perceptions of persons are called personality constructs. The
lexical hypothesis
states that, over time, socially shared constructs of self- and
other-perception become
encoded in the natural human languages. Pertinent lexical
encodings reflect the body
of everyday psychological ideas, beliefs, values, and practices
that people have
developed about individuals (Block 2010; Wagner et al. 1999;
Westen 1996).
What is Constructed and Represented—Phenomena and Patterns
What is it that is constructed and represented as “personality”?
Psychological defi-
nitions of personality—a few prominent ones picked out of
many—provide the first
insights into what scientific psychologists construct as
personality. Some frequently
cited definitions refer fairly generally and descriptively to
personality as
those characteristics that account for a person’s consistent
patterns of feeling,
thinking, and behaving (Pervin and John 1997, p. 4) or as
4 Integr Psych Behav (2013) 47:1–55
an individual’s characteristic patterns of thought, emotion, and
behaviour,
together with the psychological mechanisms–hidden or not–
behind those pat-
terns (Funder 2004, p. 5).
These definitions include psychological and behavioural
phenomena. Allport
(1937) also included psychophysical systems and environmental
adaptivity by defin-
ing personality as
the dynamic organization within the individual of those
psychophysical systems
that determine his unique adjustments to his environment (p.
48).
Eysenck (1947) furthermore specified genetic and
environmental causality, bio-
logical systems, and ontogenetic development when he defined
personality as
the sum-total of the actual or potential behaviour-patterns of the
organism, as
determined by heredity and environment; [that] originates and
develops through
the functional interaction of the four main sectors into which
these behaviour-
patterns are organized: the cognitive sector (intelligence), the
conative sector
(character), the affective sector (temperament), and the somatic
sector
(constitution) (p. 25).
McCrae and Costa (2008) extended the scope of consideration
by conceptualising
particular personality constructs as reflecting “universals” of
human nature that are
“invariant across human cultures” (McCrae and Costa 1997, p.
510), and thus also
potentially phylogenetic in origin (McCrae 2009).
These definitions are sufficient for highlighting fundamental
issues. First, they
refer descriptively to particular patterns (e.g., those described
as “characteristic”
and “consistent”) and to the different kinds of phenomena in
which these patterns
occur (e.g., physiological, psychological, and behavioural
phenomena). Second,
they include assumptions about the causation of the described
patterns in the
described phenomena by other kinds of phenomena internal to
the individual
(e.g., psychological phenomena) and/or external (e.g.,
environmental conditions)
considering various explanatory perspectives (e.g., proximate,
adaptive, ontoge-
netic, phylogenetic). Astonishingly, there is no mention of
people’s lexical
encodations and representations of individual differences
although these are
central to the lexical hypothesis.
From philosophy-of-science perspectives, the different kinds of
phenomena
studied with regard to personality must be clearly differentiated.
This is essential
because interrelations among them can be untangled only if they
are explored
each in their own right and if a priori assumptions about
specific interrelations
are avoided. This is rarely done in psychology. Behaviourists
have focused too
much on external conditions and behaviours, but have largely
ignored psycho-
logical phenomena. Subsequent researchers have tried to
overcome the limita-
tions of behaviourism and have therefore focused strongly on
psychological
phenomena and on individuals’ representations of their world,
but behaviour
has faded into the background. The prevailing investigative
strategies are preoc-
cupied with causal processes and the conditions in which
behavioural phenomena
Integr Psych Behav (2013) 47:1–55 5
occur—especially psychological ones—with the result that clear
differentiations
of psychological and behavioural phenomena are missing.
The meta-theoretical definition of behavioural phenomena as
“external activities
or externalisations of living organisms that are functionally
mediated by the environ-
ment (Millikan 1993) in the present” (Uher, What is behaviour?
And (when) is
language behaviour? A meta-theoretical definition, unpublished)
allows such differ-
entiations. This definition highlights the intrinsic relatedness of
behaviour to proper-
ties of the immediate external environment that are defined as
environmental
situations and emphasises that it is inherently bound to the
present. Externality
differentiates behaviour from thoughts, emotions, and other
psychological phenom-
ena, which are also bound to the present (Gillespie and Zittaun
2010; Toomela 2010a;
Valsiner 1998, 2012) but are internal to the individual (Toomela
2008). This meta-
theoretical definition generally defines all behavioural
phenomena without specifying
particular ones (e.g., goal-directed action or response) because
many of these con-
cepts include a priori assumptions about causally related
internal processes (e.g.,
psychological phenomena) and external conditions in the
environment (e.g., partic-
ular stimuli) that are separate kinds of phenomena. To scrutinise
which kinds of
phenomena people can perceive in everyday life and construct
as personality, it is
helpful to first specify the particular patterns referred to in
definitions of personality.
Defining Patterns
For scientific definitions, the notions of “a person’s consistent
patterns”, “individual
characteristics”, and “individual uniqueness” are surprisingly
vague. They do not
specify what is meant to be “consistent” with what, nor do they
indicate which patterns
are considered “different, “unique”, and “characteristic” and
why. Moreover, they fail to
mention within-individual variability and structural
complexity—both within and be-
tween individuals. This is remarkable because these patterns
refer to phenomena (e.g.,
those of the psyche and of behaviour) that are heterogeneous,
complex, dynamic, and
thus highly fluctuating. In such phenomena, differences among
individuals are neces-
sarily apparent at any given time (Uher 2011a) and
determinations of meaningful
consistency—both within and between individuals—are matters
of mere convention.
Meta-theoretical analyses help to carve out basic criteria for
determining which patterns
can be considered specific to particular individuals, and thus
different from others,
which ones cannot, and why. So what does individual-
specificity mean?
Probabilistic Patterns In the steadily fluctuating flow of events
in highly dynamic
phenomena, individuals necessarily show considerable within-
individual variability
over time. Consequently, they can be characterised only by
probabilistic patterns.
What types of probability1 are able to describe these patterns?
First, events of
psychological and behavioural phenomena are not random, such
as events in throws
of dice. There are no equipossible elementary events in well-
defined sample spaces
that can be studied in repeated experiments as in Laplacian
theory. Moreover, the
1 For the present meta-theoretical analyses, theories considering
subjectivistic probabilities of the occur-
rences of events (e.g., degrees of belief; Gillies 2000; Rychlak
1968) will not be considered here (for
reasons described in detail below).
6 Integr Psych Behav (2013) 47:1–55
events are not independent from one another—this applies to
events of the same
phenomenon (e.g., several smiles) as well as to events of
different phenomena (e.g., a
smile and a laugh). Rather, different events may co-occur and
even depend upon one
another, particularly if the phenomena are functionally similar
or related. Their events
need not be exclusively disjunctive (e.g., smiles and laughs).
Thus, finite (limiting)
frequencies, which are defined as empirical occurrences relative
to all possible
occurrences (von Mises 1928), are inadequate for describing
individual probabilities
in psychological and behavioural phenomena.
The concept of propensity probabilities seems to be better
suited for this purpose.
Propensities are considered properties of physical objects or
situations that are assumed
to cause empirical frequencies, even if only single-case
observations are made (Popper
1959). They are therefore conceived also as properties of
repeatable conditions that
determine empirical frequencies in the long run (Gillies 2000).2
But this very assump-
tion precludes the idea that propensity probabilities can be
applied to individuals
because their defining property as living systems is the fact that
they undergo continuous
changes during ontogenetic development. This is true for the
molecules in their bodies
and the phenomena constructed as personality alike. In fact,
personality is assumed to
develop and change gradually over the course of the lifespan
(e.g., Caspi and Roberts
2001; Cattell 1950; Soldz and Vaillant 1999). This implies that
the individual’s empirical
frequencies cannot converge (i.e., stabilise) in the long run as
may be true for physical
systems. By contrast, the probabilities themselves change over
time—an idea typically
not considered by theories of propensity probabilities (cf.
Gillies 2000; Popper 1959).
Further differences are essential. In psychological and
behavioural phenomena,
there are no discrete events such as there are in coin tosses.
Well-defined natural
entities on which probability estimations could be based are
lacking. Instead, it is the
scientists who must decide which entities of real phenomena
make up particular
scientific phenomena. The ways in which they define entities as
categories depend
not only on the phenomena themselves, but also on the purposes
of their reduction
(Weber 1949). Moreover, the processual character and
microgenetic development of
many psychological and behavioural phenomena (Rosenthal
2004) entails that their
occurrences can be construed as frequencies only in some cases.
In other cases, they
may be construed more accurately as durations of nonfixed and
often highly variable
length (or as latencies, which are durations with specified start
times).
Consequently, in fluctuating phenomena such as those of the
psyche and of
behaviour, events can be considered only by their empirical
occurrences (i.e., in
terms of frequencies and durations) relative to particular time
periods. This new
probability type will therefore be called a time-relative
probability. For example, in
observations of free play in a kindergarten group, a time-
relative probability of
10 min per hour of observation time during which a child plays
by him/herself may
be determined for a particular 5-year-old child. The time
periods in which time-
relative probabilities are determined must be specified
precisely—both in terms of the
occasions and spans of time in which the data were collected to
determine the
probabilities and in terms of historical times in the individual’s
ontogeny. Both are
2 The concept of propensity probabilities reifies observed
properties into causal entities of an unkown and
undefined kind. As such, the explanatory strategy is inherently
circular and faces the same challenges as
those elaborated for trait psychology in this article.
Integr Psych Behav (2013) 47:1–55 7
essential to define individual-specificity. For example, the 10
min per hour probabil-
ity could have been determined from 20 h of regular observation
obtained across a
time period of 2 weeks; the ontogenetic stage of the individual
is specified by an age
of 5 years, which can be categorised as early childhood.
This new probability type enables ratio-scaled quantifications
that are essential for
quantitative comparisons across time and situational contexts
and between individuals,
groups, populations, and species (Uher, Meta-theoretical
foundations of objectivity
versus subjectivity in quantifications of behaviour and
personality; An integrative
meta-theoretical framework for research on individual
behaviour in context—situa-
tions, populations, species, both unpublished). It also allows for
the merging of
concepts of averages across occasions, as studied in personality
research, with concepts
of ranges, especially maxima, as studied in intelligence and
achievement research3
(Ackerman 1994), and concepts of variability in terms of
differences between consec-
utive occasions (i.e., fluctuations) as studied in physiological
and behavioural research
(De Weerth et al. 1999). For example, the child’s time-relative
probability could be on
average 10 min per hour, ranging from 5 to 20 min per hour on
a daily basis; the
magnitude of day-to-day variability (for comparable occasions
across the days) as
indicated by the coefficient of variation could be CV=0.5. This
coefficient specifies
the standard deviation standardised by the mean (because the
standard deviation is
sensitive to the sample mean) to allow for comparisons between
different samples (for
an overview of methods of analyses of variability, see van Geert
and van Dijk 2002).
Such comprehensive analyses of patterns are important because
within-individual var-
iability in psychological and behavioural phenomena is
pronounced and bears theoret-
ical and empirical importance. For example, within-individual
variability can indicate
ongoing processes and can reflect phenomena that are important
for explaining changes
and development (van Geert and van Dijk 2002, p. 344).
Moreover, within-individual
variability often substantially exceeds between-individual
variability (Shweder and
Sullivan 1990; Uher, An integrative meta-theoretical framework
for research on indi-
vidual behaviour in context—situations, populations, species,
unpublished).
Differential Patterns Time-relative probabilities that
characterise all individuals in
the same way cannot be individual-specific. If all children in
the observed group have
the same time-relative probability of self-playing, then these
probabilities cannot
characterise any one of them individually. Time-relative
probabilities can reflect
individual uniqueness only if they deviate from those of other
individuals of a
particular reference group of interest (e.g., a social group,
culture, or species)—that
is, if they are differential. This is the case if the determined
time-relative probabilities
differ between children such that their individual averages vary
interindividually, for
example, between 2 min and 30 min per hour, their individual
ranges vary, for
example, between 15 min and 50 min, and their individual
within-individual varia-
bilities vary, for example, between CV=0.3 and 1.3.
The concept of time-relative probabilities presupposes that the
number of occa-
sions and periods of time considered for the probability
estimations are comparable
3 Personality research is often distinguished from intelligence
research in American psychology. In
European traditions (e.g., Cattell, Eysenck, Stern, Pawlik),
intelligence and achievement are considered
inherent parts of an individual’s personality.
8 Integr Psych Behav (2013) 47:1–55
between individuals. This is important when considering the
effects of aggregation on
the reliability of probability estimations (cf. Spearman 1910)
and when taking into
account the fact that the individuals’ probabilities themselves
may gradually change
over time (i.e., develop). In the given example, this means that
the time-relative
probabilities of the other children should also be estimated
based on 20 h of
observation across a time span of 2 weeks. These children
should also be of similar
age in order to disentangle differences between individuals from
differences between
age groups. Furthermore, the situational contexts in which
different individuals are
studied should be comparable between them because the average
time-relative
probabilities of particular groups of individuals may generally
shift across different
situations (Uher, An integrative meta-theoretical framework for
research on individ-
ual behaviour in context—situations, populations, species,
unpublished.) Analyses of
differentiality therefore require statistical standardisation of
time-relative probabilities
within the time periods and within the situational contexts in
which they are deter-
mined (Uher 2011a). Statistical standardisation removes the
information on absolute
time-relative probability scores, however, because the data are
converted into relative,
but still ratio-scaled data. Yet, the absolute scores can always
be traced when needed
for interpretation and for later comparisons with other samples
or with future inves-
tigations of the same sample.
Temporal Patterns Differential patterns in time-relative
probabilities that change
rapidly cannot characterise an individual’s uniqueness. To
reflect individual-
specificity, they must be stable across time periods that are
longer than those in
which the probabilities were first ascertained. As with the
analyses of differential
patterns, the analyses of temporal patterns presuppose that
probability estimations are
based on numbers of occasions and periods of time that are
comparable between the
individuals being studied and between the time periods being
contrasted. In the above
example, this means that the differential patterns in the
children’s time-relative
probabilities for self-play should be similar when estimated
again some time later
in a second period of 2 weeks. Temporal stability is an essential
prerequisite for
justifying interpretations of the obtained standardised aggregate
scores as reflecting
individual-specific patterns (Uher 2011a).
Temporal stability can be analysed for differential patterns in
the individuals’
averages, ranges, and variabilities. For example, it is quite
possible that individuals
with similar differential scores in their average probabilities
will differ from one
another in stable ways with regard to their ranges and patterns
of within-individual
variability. The magnitude of temporal stability considered
meaningful to construct
individual-specificity is necessarily a matter of convention that
can and must be
explicitly defined. It depends on the ontogenetic stages of the
individuals under study
as well as on their species-specific life expectancy (Uher 2009).
It also depends on the
phenomena being studied and the meta-theory used. For
example, in biology, weak
test-retest correlations of individual differences in behaviour
across just a few days or
weeks of r=.20 are considered ecologically and evolutionarily
meaningful (Sih and
Bell 2008), whereas in human psychology, such weak
correlations are considered
indicators of unreliable measurements of individual-specific
patterns (Uher 2011a).
In conclusion, for fluctuating and dynamic phenomena such as
those of the psyche
and behaviour, individual-specificity refers to differential
patterns in time-relative
Integr Psych Behav (2013) 47:1–55 9
probabilities that are stable in ways that are considered to be
meaningful. This
concept fits well into Brunswik’s (1955) probabilistic theory for
functional psychol-
ogy. Brunswik ascertained that although the environment may
be lawful in terms of
physical principles, to the individual, “it presents itself as semi-
erratic … therefore all
functional psychology is inherently probabilistic” (Brunswik
1955, p. 193). An
empirical application of this meta-theoretical concept of
individual-specificity to
behaviour was demonstrated in a study on capuchin monkeys
based on 141 con-
textualised behavioural variables (Uher et al., Contextualised
behavioural measure-
ments of personality differences obtained in behavioural tests
and social observations
in adult capuchin monkeys (Cebus apella), unpublished).
Individual-specific patterns that have been identified in these
ways can be subse-
quently studied across longer periods of time to explore patterns
in their gradual
change and ontogenetic development (Uher 2011a). This
presupposes again that the
occasions and time spans as well as the historical times that are
studied should be
specified and should be comparable between the individuals
under study.
The concept of time-relative probabilities allows for the close
examination of individ-
ual cases in specified situations. By studying many individuals
in this way, it also allows
for the study of lawful patterns at the sample level. The former
type of study is commonly
referred to as ideographic; the latter one as nomothetic. But
ideographic approaches in
terms of single case studies in and of themselves need not be
informative about particular
1  Annotated Bibliography Topic (Chosen.docx
1  Annotated Bibliography Topic (Chosen.docx
1  Annotated Bibliography Topic (Chosen.docx
1  Annotated Bibliography Topic (Chosen.docx
1  Annotated Bibliography Topic (Chosen.docx
1  Annotated Bibliography Topic (Chosen.docx
1  Annotated Bibliography Topic (Chosen.docx
1  Annotated Bibliography Topic (Chosen.docx
1  Annotated Bibliography Topic (Chosen.docx
1  Annotated Bibliography Topic (Chosen.docx
1  Annotated Bibliography Topic (Chosen.docx
1  Annotated Bibliography Topic (Chosen.docx
1  Annotated Bibliography Topic (Chosen.docx
1  Annotated Bibliography Topic (Chosen.docx
1  Annotated Bibliography Topic (Chosen.docx
1  Annotated Bibliography Topic (Chosen.docx
1  Annotated Bibliography Topic (Chosen.docx
1  Annotated Bibliography Topic (Chosen.docx
1  Annotated Bibliography Topic (Chosen.docx
1  Annotated Bibliography Topic (Chosen.docx
1  Annotated Bibliography Topic (Chosen.docx
1  Annotated Bibliography Topic (Chosen.docx
1  Annotated Bibliography Topic (Chosen.docx
1  Annotated Bibliography Topic (Chosen.docx
1  Annotated Bibliography Topic (Chosen.docx
1  Annotated Bibliography Topic (Chosen.docx
1  Annotated Bibliography Topic (Chosen.docx
1  Annotated Bibliography Topic (Chosen.docx
1  Annotated Bibliography Topic (Chosen.docx
1  Annotated Bibliography Topic (Chosen.docx
1  Annotated Bibliography Topic (Chosen.docx
1  Annotated Bibliography Topic (Chosen.docx
1  Annotated Bibliography Topic (Chosen.docx
1  Annotated Bibliography Topic (Chosen.docx
1  Annotated Bibliography Topic (Chosen.docx
1  Annotated Bibliography Topic (Chosen.docx
1  Annotated Bibliography Topic (Chosen.docx
1  Annotated Bibliography Topic (Chosen.docx
1  Annotated Bibliography Topic (Chosen.docx
1  Annotated Bibliography Topic (Chosen.docx
1  Annotated Bibliography Topic (Chosen.docx
1  Annotated Bibliography Topic (Chosen.docx
1  Annotated Bibliography Topic (Chosen.docx
1  Annotated Bibliography Topic (Chosen.docx
1  Annotated Bibliography Topic (Chosen.docx
1  Annotated Bibliography Topic (Chosen.docx
1  Annotated Bibliography Topic (Chosen.docx
1  Annotated Bibliography Topic (Chosen.docx
1  Annotated Bibliography Topic (Chosen.docx
1  Annotated Bibliography Topic (Chosen.docx
1  Annotated Bibliography Topic (Chosen.docx
1  Annotated Bibliography Topic (Chosen.docx
1  Annotated Bibliography Topic (Chosen.docx
1  Annotated Bibliography Topic (Chosen.docx
1  Annotated Bibliography Topic (Chosen.docx
1  Annotated Bibliography Topic (Chosen.docx
1  Annotated Bibliography Topic (Chosen.docx
1  Annotated Bibliography Topic (Chosen.docx
1  Annotated Bibliography Topic (Chosen.docx
1  Annotated Bibliography Topic (Chosen.docx
1  Annotated Bibliography Topic (Chosen.docx
1  Annotated Bibliography Topic (Chosen.docx
1  Annotated Bibliography Topic (Chosen.docx
1  Annotated Bibliography Topic (Chosen.docx
1  Annotated Bibliography Topic (Chosen.docx
1  Annotated Bibliography Topic (Chosen.docx
1  Annotated Bibliography Topic (Chosen.docx
1  Annotated Bibliography Topic (Chosen.docx
1  Annotated Bibliography Topic (Chosen.docx
1  Annotated Bibliography Topic (Chosen.docx
1  Annotated Bibliography Topic (Chosen.docx
1  Annotated Bibliography Topic (Chosen.docx
1  Annotated Bibliography Topic (Chosen.docx
1  Annotated Bibliography Topic (Chosen.docx
1  Annotated Bibliography Topic (Chosen.docx
1  Annotated Bibliography Topic (Chosen.docx
1  Annotated Bibliography Topic (Chosen.docx
1  Annotated Bibliography Topic (Chosen.docx
1  Annotated Bibliography Topic (Chosen.docx
1  Annotated Bibliography Topic (Chosen.docx
1  Annotated Bibliography Topic (Chosen.docx
1  Annotated Bibliography Topic (Chosen.docx
1  Annotated Bibliography Topic (Chosen.docx
1  Annotated Bibliography Topic (Chosen.docx
1  Annotated Bibliography Topic (Chosen.docx
1  Annotated Bibliography Topic (Chosen.docx
1  Annotated Bibliography Topic (Chosen.docx
1  Annotated Bibliography Topic (Chosen.docx
1  Annotated Bibliography Topic (Chosen.docx
1  Annotated Bibliography Topic (Chosen.docx
1  Annotated Bibliography Topic (Chosen.docx
1  Annotated Bibliography Topic (Chosen.docx
1  Annotated Bibliography Topic (Chosen.docx
1  Annotated Bibliography Topic (Chosen.docx
1  Annotated Bibliography Topic (Chosen.docx
1  Annotated Bibliography Topic (Chosen.docx
1  Annotated Bibliography Topic (Chosen.docx
1  Annotated Bibliography Topic (Chosen.docx
1  Annotated Bibliography Topic (Chosen.docx
1  Annotated Bibliography Topic (Chosen.docx
1  Annotated Bibliography Topic (Chosen.docx
1  Annotated Bibliography Topic (Chosen.docx
1  Annotated Bibliography Topic (Chosen.docx
1  Annotated Bibliography Topic (Chosen.docx
1  Annotated Bibliography Topic (Chosen.docx
1  Annotated Bibliography Topic (Chosen.docx
1  Annotated Bibliography Topic (Chosen.docx
1  Annotated Bibliography Topic (Chosen.docx
1  Annotated Bibliography Topic (Chosen.docx
1  Annotated Bibliography Topic (Chosen.docx
1  Annotated Bibliography Topic (Chosen.docx
1  Annotated Bibliography Topic (Chosen.docx
1  Annotated Bibliography Topic (Chosen.docx
1  Annotated Bibliography Topic (Chosen.docx
1  Annotated Bibliography Topic (Chosen.docx
1  Annotated Bibliography Topic (Chosen.docx
1  Annotated Bibliography Topic (Chosen.docx
1  Annotated Bibliography Topic (Chosen.docx
1  Annotated Bibliography Topic (Chosen.docx
1  Annotated Bibliography Topic (Chosen.docx
1  Annotated Bibliography Topic (Chosen.docx
1  Annotated Bibliography Topic (Chosen.docx
1  Annotated Bibliography Topic (Chosen.docx
1  Annotated Bibliography Topic (Chosen.docx
1  Annotated Bibliography Topic (Chosen.docx
1  Annotated Bibliography Topic (Chosen.docx
1  Annotated Bibliography Topic (Chosen.docx
1  Annotated Bibliography Topic (Chosen.docx
1  Annotated Bibliography Topic (Chosen.docx
1  Annotated Bibliography Topic (Chosen.docx
1  Annotated Bibliography Topic (Chosen.docx
1  Annotated Bibliography Topic (Chosen.docx
1  Annotated Bibliography Topic (Chosen.docx
1  Annotated Bibliography Topic (Chosen.docx
1  Annotated Bibliography Topic (Chosen.docx
1  Annotated Bibliography Topic (Chosen.docx
1  Annotated Bibliography Topic (Chosen.docx
1  Annotated Bibliography Topic (Chosen.docx
1  Annotated Bibliography Topic (Chosen.docx
1  Annotated Bibliography Topic (Chosen.docx
1  Annotated Bibliography Topic (Chosen.docx
1  Annotated Bibliography Topic (Chosen.docx
1  Annotated Bibliography Topic (Chosen.docx
1  Annotated Bibliography Topic (Chosen.docx
1  Annotated Bibliography Topic (Chosen.docx
1  Annotated Bibliography Topic (Chosen.docx
1  Annotated Bibliography Topic (Chosen.docx
1  Annotated Bibliography Topic (Chosen.docx
1  Annotated Bibliography Topic (Chosen.docx
1  Annotated Bibliography Topic (Chosen.docx
1  Annotated Bibliography Topic (Chosen.docx
1  Annotated Bibliography Topic (Chosen.docx
1  Annotated Bibliography Topic (Chosen.docx
1  Annotated Bibliography Topic (Chosen.docx
1  Annotated Bibliography Topic (Chosen.docx
1  Annotated Bibliography Topic (Chosen.docx
1  Annotated Bibliography Topic (Chosen.docx
1  Annotated Bibliography Topic (Chosen.docx
1  Annotated Bibliography Topic (Chosen.docx
1  Annotated Bibliography Topic (Chosen.docx
1  Annotated Bibliography Topic (Chosen.docx
1  Annotated Bibliography Topic (Chosen.docx
1  Annotated Bibliography Topic (Chosen.docx
1  Annotated Bibliography Topic (Chosen.docx
1  Annotated Bibliography Topic (Chosen.docx
1  Annotated Bibliography Topic (Chosen.docx

More Related Content

Similar to 1 Annotated Bibliography Topic (Chosen.docx

Conservatism and Physical Fitness: Political Science Senior Research, Adrian ...
Conservatism and Physical Fitness: Political Science Senior Research, Adrian ...Conservatism and Physical Fitness: Political Science Senior Research, Adrian ...
Conservatism and Physical Fitness: Political Science Senior Research, Adrian ...Josh Emington
 
BIOETHICS METHODS IN THE ETHICAL, LEGAL, AND SOCIALIMPLICATI.docx
BIOETHICS METHODS IN THE ETHICAL, LEGAL, AND SOCIALIMPLICATI.docxBIOETHICS METHODS IN THE ETHICAL, LEGAL, AND SOCIALIMPLICATI.docx
BIOETHICS METHODS IN THE ETHICAL, LEGAL, AND SOCIALIMPLICATI.docxAASTHA76
 
Cultural Significance Of Art
Cultural Significance Of ArtCultural Significance Of Art
Cultural Significance Of ArtKaty Allen
 
FINAL SENIOR SEMINAR PROPOSAL
FINAL SENIOR SEMINAR PROPOSALFINAL SENIOR SEMINAR PROPOSAL
FINAL SENIOR SEMINAR PROPOSALMargaret O'Brien
 
PAGE 52What is Action ResearchViaA review of the Literat.docx
PAGE  52What is Action ResearchViaA review of the Literat.docxPAGE  52What is Action ResearchViaA review of the Literat.docx
PAGE 52What is Action ResearchViaA review of the Literat.docxgerardkortney
 
Page 291LEARNING OBJECTIVES· Discuss the issues created by.docx
Page 291LEARNING OBJECTIVES· Discuss the issues created by.docxPage 291LEARNING OBJECTIVES· Discuss the issues created by.docx
Page 291LEARNING OBJECTIVES· Discuss the issues created by.docxkarlhennesey
 
Evaluation Of A Good Scientific Research
Evaluation Of A Good Scientific ResearchEvaluation Of A Good Scientific Research
Evaluation Of A Good Scientific ResearchCrystal Torres
 
Timothy l. o’briena and shiri noyb
Timothy l. o’briena and shiri noybTimothy l. o’briena and shiri noyb
Timothy l. o’briena and shiri noybJosias Espinoza
 
Spirals_of_silence_Editorial.pdf
Spirals_of_silence_Editorial.pdfSpirals_of_silence_Editorial.pdf
Spirals_of_silence_Editorial.pdfMaksudHossain2
 
Influences of strong tie with opinion leaders in an interconnected network of...
Influences of strong tie with opinion leaders in an interconnected network of...Influences of strong tie with opinion leaders in an interconnected network of...
Influences of strong tie with opinion leaders in an interconnected network of...Myunggoon Choi
 
For my final project I am choosing the environmental influences on.docx
For my final project I am choosing the environmental influences on.docxFor my final project I am choosing the environmental influences on.docx
For my final project I am choosing the environmental influences on.docxrhetttrevannion
 
(Discussion 1)Describe a social theory that was discussed in the Socia.docx
(Discussion 1)Describe a social theory that was discussed in the Socia.docx(Discussion 1)Describe a social theory that was discussed in the Socia.docx
(Discussion 1)Describe a social theory that was discussed in the Socia.docxchristina345678
 
Thesis Complete.adobe.
Thesis Complete.adobe.Thesis Complete.adobe.
Thesis Complete.adobe.R Murphy
 

Similar to 1 Annotated Bibliography Topic (Chosen.docx (20)

Conservatism and Physical Fitness: Political Science Senior Research, Adrian ...
Conservatism and Physical Fitness: Political Science Senior Research, Adrian ...Conservatism and Physical Fitness: Political Science Senior Research, Adrian ...
Conservatism and Physical Fitness: Political Science Senior Research, Adrian ...
 
Selected Research Sample Writing
Selected Research Sample WritingSelected Research Sample Writing
Selected Research Sample Writing
 
BIOETHICS METHODS IN THE ETHICAL, LEGAL, AND SOCIALIMPLICATI.docx
BIOETHICS METHODS IN THE ETHICAL, LEGAL, AND SOCIALIMPLICATI.docxBIOETHICS METHODS IN THE ETHICAL, LEGAL, AND SOCIALIMPLICATI.docx
BIOETHICS METHODS IN THE ETHICAL, LEGAL, AND SOCIALIMPLICATI.docx
 
Social Contagion Theory
Social Contagion TheorySocial Contagion Theory
Social Contagion Theory
 
ARTIKEL 1.pdf
ARTIKEL 1.pdfARTIKEL 1.pdf
ARTIKEL 1.pdf
 
Cultural Significance Of Art
Cultural Significance Of ArtCultural Significance Of Art
Cultural Significance Of Art
 
FINAL SENIOR SEMINAR PROPOSAL
FINAL SENIOR SEMINAR PROPOSALFINAL SENIOR SEMINAR PROPOSAL
FINAL SENIOR SEMINAR PROPOSAL
 
PAGE 52What is Action ResearchViaA review of the Literat.docx
PAGE  52What is Action ResearchViaA review of the Literat.docxPAGE  52What is Action ResearchViaA review of the Literat.docx
PAGE 52What is Action ResearchViaA review of the Literat.docx
 
Page 291LEARNING OBJECTIVES· Discuss the issues created by.docx
Page 291LEARNING OBJECTIVES· Discuss the issues created by.docxPage 291LEARNING OBJECTIVES· Discuss the issues created by.docx
Page 291LEARNING OBJECTIVES· Discuss the issues created by.docx
 
Evaluation Of A Good Scientific Research
Evaluation Of A Good Scientific ResearchEvaluation Of A Good Scientific Research
Evaluation Of A Good Scientific Research
 
Timothy l. o’briena and shiri noyb
Timothy l. o’briena and shiri noybTimothy l. o’briena and shiri noyb
Timothy l. o’briena and shiri noyb
 
Spirals_of_silence_Editorial.pdf
Spirals_of_silence_Editorial.pdfSpirals_of_silence_Editorial.pdf
Spirals_of_silence_Editorial.pdf
 
Influences of strong tie with opinion leaders in an interconnected network of...
Influences of strong tie with opinion leaders in an interconnected network of...Influences of strong tie with opinion leaders in an interconnected network of...
Influences of strong tie with opinion leaders in an interconnected network of...
 
For my final project I am choosing the environmental influences on.docx
For my final project I am choosing the environmental influences on.docxFor my final project I am choosing the environmental influences on.docx
For my final project I am choosing the environmental influences on.docx
 
Interviewing ethnic minorities
Interviewing ethnic minoritiesInterviewing ethnic minorities
Interviewing ethnic minorities
 
Researcher positionality
Researcher positionalityResearcher positionality
Researcher positionality
 
Researcher Positionality
Researcher PositionalityResearcher Positionality
Researcher Positionality
 
(Discussion 1)Describe a social theory that was discussed in the Socia.docx
(Discussion 1)Describe a social theory that was discussed in the Socia.docx(Discussion 1)Describe a social theory that was discussed in the Socia.docx
(Discussion 1)Describe a social theory that was discussed in the Socia.docx
 
Thesis Complete.adobe.
Thesis Complete.adobe.Thesis Complete.adobe.
Thesis Complete.adobe.
 
STDEV . I3.pdf
STDEV . I3.pdfSTDEV . I3.pdf
STDEV . I3.pdf
 

More from mercysuttle

1 Question Information refinement means taking the system requi.docx
1 Question Information refinement means taking the system requi.docx1 Question Information refinement means taking the system requi.docx
1 Question Information refinement means taking the system requi.docxmercysuttle
 
1 pageApaSourcesDiscuss how an organization’s marketing i.docx
1 pageApaSourcesDiscuss how an organization’s marketing i.docx1 pageApaSourcesDiscuss how an organization’s marketing i.docx
1 pageApaSourcesDiscuss how an organization’s marketing i.docxmercysuttle
 
1 R120V11Vac0Vdc R2100VC13mE.docx
1 R120V11Vac0Vdc R2100VC13mE.docx1 R120V11Vac0Vdc R2100VC13mE.docx
1 R120V11Vac0Vdc R2100VC13mE.docxmercysuttle
 
1 PSYC499SeniorCapstoneTheImpactoftheSocial.docx
1 PSYC499SeniorCapstoneTheImpactoftheSocial.docx1 PSYC499SeniorCapstoneTheImpactoftheSocial.docx
1 PSYC499SeniorCapstoneTheImpactoftheSocial.docxmercysuttle
 
1 Politicking is less likely in organizations that have· adecl.docx
1 Politicking is less likely in organizations that have· adecl.docx1 Politicking is less likely in organizations that have· adecl.docx
1 Politicking is less likely in organizations that have· adecl.docxmercysuttle
 
1 page2 sourcesReflect on the important performance management.docx
1 page2 sourcesReflect on the important performance management.docx1 page2 sourcesReflect on the important performance management.docx
1 page2 sourcesReflect on the important performance management.docxmercysuttle
 
1 of 402.5 PointsUse Cramer’s Rule to solve the following syst.docx
1 of 402.5 PointsUse Cramer’s Rule to solve the following syst.docx1 of 402.5 PointsUse Cramer’s Rule to solve the following syst.docx
1 of 402.5 PointsUse Cramer’s Rule to solve the following syst.docxmercysuttle
 
1 of 6 LAB 5 IMAGE FILTERING ECE180 Introduction to.docx
1 of 6  LAB 5 IMAGE FILTERING ECE180 Introduction to.docx1 of 6  LAB 5 IMAGE FILTERING ECE180 Introduction to.docx
1 of 6 LAB 5 IMAGE FILTERING ECE180 Introduction to.docxmercysuttle
 
1 Objectives Genetically transform bacteria with for.docx
1 Objectives Genetically transform bacteria with for.docx1 Objectives Genetically transform bacteria with for.docx
1 Objectives Genetically transform bacteria with for.docxmercysuttle
 
1 of 8 Student name ……………. St.docx
1 of 8 Student name …………….               St.docx1 of 8 Student name …………….               St.docx
1 of 8 Student name ……………. St.docxmercysuttle
 
1 MATH 106 QUIZ 4 Due b.docx
1 MATH 106 QUIZ 4                                 Due b.docx1 MATH 106 QUIZ 4                                 Due b.docx
1 MATH 106 QUIZ 4 Due b.docxmercysuttle
 
1 MN6003 Levis Strauss Case Adapted from Does Levi St.docx
1 MN6003 Levis Strauss Case Adapted from Does Levi St.docx1 MN6003 Levis Strauss Case Adapted from Does Levi St.docx
1 MN6003 Levis Strauss Case Adapted from Does Levi St.docxmercysuttle
 
1 NAME__________________ EXAM 1 Directi.docx
1 NAME__________________ EXAM 1  Directi.docx1 NAME__________________ EXAM 1  Directi.docx
1 NAME__________________ EXAM 1 Directi.docxmercysuttle
 
1 pageapasources2Third Party LogisticsBriefly describe .docx
1 pageapasources2Third Party LogisticsBriefly describe .docx1 pageapasources2Third Party LogisticsBriefly describe .docx
1 pageapasources2Third Party LogisticsBriefly describe .docxmercysuttle
 
1 Pageapasources2Review the Food Environment Atlas maps for.docx
1 Pageapasources2Review the Food Environment Atlas maps for.docx1 Pageapasources2Review the Food Environment Atlas maps for.docx
1 Pageapasources2Review the Food Environment Atlas maps for.docxmercysuttle
 
1 Lab 3 Newton’s Second Law of Motion Introducti.docx
1 Lab 3 Newton’s Second Law of Motion  Introducti.docx1 Lab 3 Newton’s Second Law of Motion  Introducti.docx
1 Lab 3 Newton’s Second Law of Motion Introducti.docxmercysuttle
 
1 Marks 2 A person can be prosecuted for both an attempt and .docx
1 Marks 2 A person can be prosecuted for both an attempt and .docx1 Marks 2 A person can be prosecuted for both an attempt and .docx
1 Marks 2 A person can be prosecuted for both an attempt and .docxmercysuttle
 
1 Marks 1 Post Traumatic Stress Disorder (PTSD)Choose one .docx
1 Marks 1 Post Traumatic Stress Disorder (PTSD)Choose one .docx1 Marks 1 Post Traumatic Stress Disorder (PTSD)Choose one .docx
1 Marks 1 Post Traumatic Stress Disorder (PTSD)Choose one .docxmercysuttle
 
1 List of Acceptable Primary Resources for the Week 3 .docx
1 List of Acceptable Primary Resources for the Week 3 .docx1 List of Acceptable Primary Resources for the Week 3 .docx
1 List of Acceptable Primary Resources for the Week 3 .docxmercysuttle
 

More from mercysuttle (20)

1 Question Information refinement means taking the system requi.docx
1 Question Information refinement means taking the system requi.docx1 Question Information refinement means taking the system requi.docx
1 Question Information refinement means taking the system requi.docx
 
1 pageApaSourcesDiscuss how an organization’s marketing i.docx
1 pageApaSourcesDiscuss how an organization’s marketing i.docx1 pageApaSourcesDiscuss how an organization’s marketing i.docx
1 pageApaSourcesDiscuss how an organization’s marketing i.docx
 
1 R120V11Vac0Vdc R2100VC13mE.docx
1 R120V11Vac0Vdc R2100VC13mE.docx1 R120V11Vac0Vdc R2100VC13mE.docx
1 R120V11Vac0Vdc R2100VC13mE.docx
 
1 PSYC499SeniorCapstoneTheImpactoftheSocial.docx
1 PSYC499SeniorCapstoneTheImpactoftheSocial.docx1 PSYC499SeniorCapstoneTheImpactoftheSocial.docx
1 PSYC499SeniorCapstoneTheImpactoftheSocial.docx
 
1 Politicking is less likely in organizations that have· adecl.docx
1 Politicking is less likely in organizations that have· adecl.docx1 Politicking is less likely in organizations that have· adecl.docx
1 Politicking is less likely in organizations that have· adecl.docx
 
1 page2 sourcesReflect on the important performance management.docx
1 page2 sourcesReflect on the important performance management.docx1 page2 sourcesReflect on the important performance management.docx
1 page2 sourcesReflect on the important performance management.docx
 
1 of 402.5 PointsUse Cramer’s Rule to solve the following syst.docx
1 of 402.5 PointsUse Cramer’s Rule to solve the following syst.docx1 of 402.5 PointsUse Cramer’s Rule to solve the following syst.docx
1 of 402.5 PointsUse Cramer’s Rule to solve the following syst.docx
 
1 of 6 LAB 5 IMAGE FILTERING ECE180 Introduction to.docx
1 of 6  LAB 5 IMAGE FILTERING ECE180 Introduction to.docx1 of 6  LAB 5 IMAGE FILTERING ECE180 Introduction to.docx
1 of 6 LAB 5 IMAGE FILTERING ECE180 Introduction to.docx
 
1 Objectives Genetically transform bacteria with for.docx
1 Objectives Genetically transform bacteria with for.docx1 Objectives Genetically transform bacteria with for.docx
1 Objectives Genetically transform bacteria with for.docx
 
1 of 8 Student name ……………. St.docx
1 of 8 Student name …………….               St.docx1 of 8 Student name …………….               St.docx
1 of 8 Student name ……………. St.docx
 
1 MATH 106 QUIZ 4 Due b.docx
1 MATH 106 QUIZ 4                                 Due b.docx1 MATH 106 QUIZ 4                                 Due b.docx
1 MATH 106 QUIZ 4 Due b.docx
 
1 MN6003 Levis Strauss Case Adapted from Does Levi St.docx
1 MN6003 Levis Strauss Case Adapted from Does Levi St.docx1 MN6003 Levis Strauss Case Adapted from Does Levi St.docx
1 MN6003 Levis Strauss Case Adapted from Does Levi St.docx
 
1 NAME__________________ EXAM 1 Directi.docx
1 NAME__________________ EXAM 1  Directi.docx1 NAME__________________ EXAM 1  Directi.docx
1 NAME__________________ EXAM 1 Directi.docx
 
1 Name .docx
1 Name                                                 .docx1 Name                                                 .docx
1 Name .docx
 
1 pageapasources2Third Party LogisticsBriefly describe .docx
1 pageapasources2Third Party LogisticsBriefly describe .docx1 pageapasources2Third Party LogisticsBriefly describe .docx
1 pageapasources2Third Party LogisticsBriefly describe .docx
 
1 Pageapasources2Review the Food Environment Atlas maps for.docx
1 Pageapasources2Review the Food Environment Atlas maps for.docx1 Pageapasources2Review the Food Environment Atlas maps for.docx
1 Pageapasources2Review the Food Environment Atlas maps for.docx
 
1 Lab 3 Newton’s Second Law of Motion Introducti.docx
1 Lab 3 Newton’s Second Law of Motion  Introducti.docx1 Lab 3 Newton’s Second Law of Motion  Introducti.docx
1 Lab 3 Newton’s Second Law of Motion Introducti.docx
 
1 Marks 2 A person can be prosecuted for both an attempt and .docx
1 Marks 2 A person can be prosecuted for both an attempt and .docx1 Marks 2 A person can be prosecuted for both an attempt and .docx
1 Marks 2 A person can be prosecuted for both an attempt and .docx
 
1 Marks 1 Post Traumatic Stress Disorder (PTSD)Choose one .docx
1 Marks 1 Post Traumatic Stress Disorder (PTSD)Choose one .docx1 Marks 1 Post Traumatic Stress Disorder (PTSD)Choose one .docx
1 Marks 1 Post Traumatic Stress Disorder (PTSD)Choose one .docx
 
1 List of Acceptable Primary Resources for the Week 3 .docx
1 List of Acceptable Primary Resources for the Week 3 .docx1 List of Acceptable Primary Resources for the Week 3 .docx
1 List of Acceptable Primary Resources for the Week 3 .docx
 

Recently uploaded

Incoming and Outgoing Shipments in 1 STEP Using Odoo 17
Incoming and Outgoing Shipments in 1 STEP Using Odoo 17Incoming and Outgoing Shipments in 1 STEP Using Odoo 17
Incoming and Outgoing Shipments in 1 STEP Using Odoo 17Celine George
 
Painted Grey Ware.pptx, PGW Culture of India
Painted Grey Ware.pptx, PGW Culture of IndiaPainted Grey Ware.pptx, PGW Culture of India
Painted Grey Ware.pptx, PGW Culture of IndiaVirag Sontakke
 
Introduction to AI in Higher Education_draft.pptx
Introduction to AI in Higher Education_draft.pptxIntroduction to AI in Higher Education_draft.pptx
Introduction to AI in Higher Education_draft.pptxpboyjonauth
 
Framing an Appropriate Research Question 6b9b26d93da94caf993c038d9efcdedb.pdf
Framing an Appropriate Research Question 6b9b26d93da94caf993c038d9efcdedb.pdfFraming an Appropriate Research Question 6b9b26d93da94caf993c038d9efcdedb.pdf
Framing an Appropriate Research Question 6b9b26d93da94caf993c038d9efcdedb.pdfUjwalaBharambe
 
internship ppt on smartinternz platform as salesforce developer
internship ppt on smartinternz platform as salesforce developerinternship ppt on smartinternz platform as salesforce developer
internship ppt on smartinternz platform as salesforce developerunnathinaik
 
DATA STRUCTURE AND ALGORITHM for beginners
DATA STRUCTURE AND ALGORITHM for beginnersDATA STRUCTURE AND ALGORITHM for beginners
DATA STRUCTURE AND ALGORITHM for beginnersSabitha Banu
 
ECONOMIC CONTEXT - LONG FORM TV DRAMA - PPT
ECONOMIC CONTEXT - LONG FORM TV DRAMA - PPTECONOMIC CONTEXT - LONG FORM TV DRAMA - PPT
ECONOMIC CONTEXT - LONG FORM TV DRAMA - PPTiammrhaywood
 
EPANDING THE CONTENT OF AN OUTLINE using notes.pptx
EPANDING THE CONTENT OF AN OUTLINE using notes.pptxEPANDING THE CONTENT OF AN OUTLINE using notes.pptx
EPANDING THE CONTENT OF AN OUTLINE using notes.pptxRaymartEstabillo3
 
CELL CYCLE Division Science 8 quarter IV.pptx
CELL CYCLE Division Science 8 quarter IV.pptxCELL CYCLE Division Science 8 quarter IV.pptx
CELL CYCLE Division Science 8 quarter IV.pptxJiesonDelaCerna
 
Historical philosophical, theoretical, and legal foundations of special and i...
Historical philosophical, theoretical, and legal foundations of special and i...Historical philosophical, theoretical, and legal foundations of special and i...
Historical philosophical, theoretical, and legal foundations of special and i...jaredbarbolino94
 
Presiding Officer Training module 2024 lok sabha elections
Presiding Officer Training module 2024 lok sabha electionsPresiding Officer Training module 2024 lok sabha elections
Presiding Officer Training module 2024 lok sabha electionsanshu789521
 
Computed Fields and api Depends in the Odoo 17
Computed Fields and api Depends in the Odoo 17Computed Fields and api Depends in the Odoo 17
Computed Fields and api Depends in the Odoo 17Celine George
 
Solving Puzzles Benefits Everyone (English).pptx
Solving Puzzles Benefits Everyone (English).pptxSolving Puzzles Benefits Everyone (English).pptx
Solving Puzzles Benefits Everyone (English).pptxOH TEIK BIN
 
POINT- BIOCHEMISTRY SEM 2 ENZYMES UNIT 5.pptx
POINT- BIOCHEMISTRY SEM 2 ENZYMES UNIT 5.pptxPOINT- BIOCHEMISTRY SEM 2 ENZYMES UNIT 5.pptx
POINT- BIOCHEMISTRY SEM 2 ENZYMES UNIT 5.pptxSayali Powar
 
18-04-UA_REPORT_MEDIALITERAСY_INDEX-DM_23-1-final-eng.pdf
18-04-UA_REPORT_MEDIALITERAСY_INDEX-DM_23-1-final-eng.pdf18-04-UA_REPORT_MEDIALITERAСY_INDEX-DM_23-1-final-eng.pdf
18-04-UA_REPORT_MEDIALITERAСY_INDEX-DM_23-1-final-eng.pdfssuser54595a
 
Hierarchy of management that covers different levels of management
Hierarchy of management that covers different levels of managementHierarchy of management that covers different levels of management
Hierarchy of management that covers different levels of managementmkooblal
 
Introduction to ArtificiaI Intelligence in Higher Education
Introduction to ArtificiaI Intelligence in Higher EducationIntroduction to ArtificiaI Intelligence in Higher Education
Introduction to ArtificiaI Intelligence in Higher Educationpboyjonauth
 

Recently uploaded (20)

Incoming and Outgoing Shipments in 1 STEP Using Odoo 17
Incoming and Outgoing Shipments in 1 STEP Using Odoo 17Incoming and Outgoing Shipments in 1 STEP Using Odoo 17
Incoming and Outgoing Shipments in 1 STEP Using Odoo 17
 
OS-operating systems- ch04 (Threads) ...
OS-operating systems- ch04 (Threads) ...OS-operating systems- ch04 (Threads) ...
OS-operating systems- ch04 (Threads) ...
 
Painted Grey Ware.pptx, PGW Culture of India
Painted Grey Ware.pptx, PGW Culture of IndiaPainted Grey Ware.pptx, PGW Culture of India
Painted Grey Ware.pptx, PGW Culture of India
 
Introduction to AI in Higher Education_draft.pptx
Introduction to AI in Higher Education_draft.pptxIntroduction to AI in Higher Education_draft.pptx
Introduction to AI in Higher Education_draft.pptx
 
Framing an Appropriate Research Question 6b9b26d93da94caf993c038d9efcdedb.pdf
Framing an Appropriate Research Question 6b9b26d93da94caf993c038d9efcdedb.pdfFraming an Appropriate Research Question 6b9b26d93da94caf993c038d9efcdedb.pdf
Framing an Appropriate Research Question 6b9b26d93da94caf993c038d9efcdedb.pdf
 
internship ppt on smartinternz platform as salesforce developer
internship ppt on smartinternz platform as salesforce developerinternship ppt on smartinternz platform as salesforce developer
internship ppt on smartinternz platform as salesforce developer
 
DATA STRUCTURE AND ALGORITHM for beginners
DATA STRUCTURE AND ALGORITHM for beginnersDATA STRUCTURE AND ALGORITHM for beginners
DATA STRUCTURE AND ALGORITHM for beginners
 
ECONOMIC CONTEXT - LONG FORM TV DRAMA - PPT
ECONOMIC CONTEXT - LONG FORM TV DRAMA - PPTECONOMIC CONTEXT - LONG FORM TV DRAMA - PPT
ECONOMIC CONTEXT - LONG FORM TV DRAMA - PPT
 
EPANDING THE CONTENT OF AN OUTLINE using notes.pptx
EPANDING THE CONTENT OF AN OUTLINE using notes.pptxEPANDING THE CONTENT OF AN OUTLINE using notes.pptx
EPANDING THE CONTENT OF AN OUTLINE using notes.pptx
 
CELL CYCLE Division Science 8 quarter IV.pptx
CELL CYCLE Division Science 8 quarter IV.pptxCELL CYCLE Division Science 8 quarter IV.pptx
CELL CYCLE Division Science 8 quarter IV.pptx
 
Historical philosophical, theoretical, and legal foundations of special and i...
Historical philosophical, theoretical, and legal foundations of special and i...Historical philosophical, theoretical, and legal foundations of special and i...
Historical philosophical, theoretical, and legal foundations of special and i...
 
Model Call Girl in Bikash Puri Delhi reach out to us at 🔝9953056974🔝
Model Call Girl in Bikash Puri  Delhi reach out to us at 🔝9953056974🔝Model Call Girl in Bikash Puri  Delhi reach out to us at 🔝9953056974🔝
Model Call Girl in Bikash Puri Delhi reach out to us at 🔝9953056974🔝
 
Presiding Officer Training module 2024 lok sabha elections
Presiding Officer Training module 2024 lok sabha electionsPresiding Officer Training module 2024 lok sabha elections
Presiding Officer Training module 2024 lok sabha elections
 
Computed Fields and api Depends in the Odoo 17
Computed Fields and api Depends in the Odoo 17Computed Fields and api Depends in the Odoo 17
Computed Fields and api Depends in the Odoo 17
 
Solving Puzzles Benefits Everyone (English).pptx
Solving Puzzles Benefits Everyone (English).pptxSolving Puzzles Benefits Everyone (English).pptx
Solving Puzzles Benefits Everyone (English).pptx
 
POINT- BIOCHEMISTRY SEM 2 ENZYMES UNIT 5.pptx
POINT- BIOCHEMISTRY SEM 2 ENZYMES UNIT 5.pptxPOINT- BIOCHEMISTRY SEM 2 ENZYMES UNIT 5.pptx
POINT- BIOCHEMISTRY SEM 2 ENZYMES UNIT 5.pptx
 
18-04-UA_REPORT_MEDIALITERAСY_INDEX-DM_23-1-final-eng.pdf
18-04-UA_REPORT_MEDIALITERAСY_INDEX-DM_23-1-final-eng.pdf18-04-UA_REPORT_MEDIALITERAСY_INDEX-DM_23-1-final-eng.pdf
18-04-UA_REPORT_MEDIALITERAСY_INDEX-DM_23-1-final-eng.pdf
 
Model Call Girl in Tilak Nagar Delhi reach out to us at 🔝9953056974🔝
Model Call Girl in Tilak Nagar Delhi reach out to us at 🔝9953056974🔝Model Call Girl in Tilak Nagar Delhi reach out to us at 🔝9953056974🔝
Model Call Girl in Tilak Nagar Delhi reach out to us at 🔝9953056974🔝
 
Hierarchy of management that covers different levels of management
Hierarchy of management that covers different levels of managementHierarchy of management that covers different levels of management
Hierarchy of management that covers different levels of management
 
Introduction to ArtificiaI Intelligence in Higher Education
Introduction to ArtificiaI Intelligence in Higher EducationIntroduction to ArtificiaI Intelligence in Higher Education
Introduction to ArtificiaI Intelligence in Higher Education
 

1 Annotated Bibliography Topic (Chosen.docx

  • 1. 1 Annotated Bibliography: Topic (Chosen from the list provided) [Name] South University Online [Template instructions: Replace the information in red with your work-then delete this line]
  • 2. 2 Annotated Bibliography: Topic (Chosen from the list provided) [APA formatted reference for source (list in alphabetical order) using a hanging indent] [Underneath the reference, give a summary of the article then an analysis: Summary of article: 1-2 paragraphs that describe the following information in your own words in paragraph format (not bullet points). • Why the article was written? • What are the major points of the article? • If the article was a study, describe: o The methods used in the research: Include the participants, how the research question(s) was tested or measured (e.g. survey, interview, formal testing…) o The results of the study: What did the researchers find out? o The conclusions: What did the researchers conclude from the study? What were the limitations of the research? NOTE: Do not include citations for the article you are
  • 3. summarizing in an annotated bibliography. You have already given credit by listing the reference first. This is different from a paper.] [Analysis of the article: 1-2 paragraphs describing the following: Whether or not the points made by the author are logical and supported by evidence and whether the author demonstrates any bias in presenting the arguments. Were other arguments or possibilities considered? Are the author’s conclusions supported? Do they fit with your understanding of the topic and your textbook's description (cite the textbook and any other sources you use for analyzing your article – include any additional sources you cite as part of your analysis in your reference list)? Why or why not (provide support for your opinion)?] 3 Example of formatting:
  • 4. Boonstra, A., & Broekhuis, M. (2010). Barriers to the acceptance of electronic medical records by physicians from systematic review to taxonomy and interventions. BMC Health Services Research, 10(1), 231-248. doi:10.1186/1472-6963-10-231 Authors conducted a systematic review of research papers between 1998 and 2009 that examined physician perceptions of barriers to implementation of electronic medical records. An examination of 1671 articles…. DeVore, S. D., & Figlioli, K. (2010). Lessons Premier hospitals learned about implementing electronic health records. Health Affairs, 29(4), 664-667. doi:10.1377/hlthaff.2010.0250 Premier healthcare alliance is a network of 2300 non-profit hospitals and 63,000 outpatient facilities in the United States, This paper summarized lessons learned from reviewing implementation practices within their system…. 4
  • 5. References List any references you cited in your analyses of your chosen sources. DO NOT list the references for the articles you chose as you already referenced them in your annotated bibliograpy. Personality and Individual Differences 49 (2010) 306–316 Contents lists available at ScienceDirect Personality and Individual Differences j o u r n a l h o m e p a g e : w w w . e l s e v i e r . c o m / l o c a t e / p a i d The nature of the relationship between personality traits and political attitudes Brad Verhulst a, Peter K. Hatemi b,c,*, Nicholas G. Martin b a Department of Political ScienceStony Brook University, Stony Brook, NY 11794-4392, USA b Queensland Institute of Medical Research, Genetic Epidemiology 300 Herston Road, Q 4029, Australia c Department of Political Science University of Iowa, 344 Schaeffer Hall, Iowa City, Iowa 52242-1498, USA a r t i c l e i n f o Article history: Received 29 October 2009 Received in revised form 3 November 2009
  • 6. Accepted 4 November 2009 Available online 9 December 2009 Keywords: Personality Ideology Attitudes Politics Genes 0191-8869/$ - see front matter � 2009 Elsevier Ltd. A doi:10.1016/j.paid.2009.11.013 * Corresponding author. Address: University of Io City, Iowa 52242-1498, USA. Tel.: +1 319 335 2347. E-mail address: [email protected] (P.K. H a b s t r a c t Building upon a series of works by Thomas J. Bouchard, Lindon J. Eaves, Hans J. Eysenck and other con- temporaries, we present strong evidence that the assumed causal relationship between personality and left–right ideology is too simplistic. We suggest the relationship is not predictive and instead is better understood by dividing the overarching left–right ideological spectrum into more meaningful attitude dimensions. In doing so, we find that Psychoticism is strongly related to conservative positions on Pun- ishment, Religious, and Sex attitudes, whereas Social Desirability is related to liberal positions on the same attitudes. Furthermore, the nature of the covariance between Psychoticism and social attitudes is due to a common genetic influence, while covariance between Social Desirability and these attitudes in females is largely a function of common shared environmental covariance.
  • 7. � 2009 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved. 1. Introduction Psychologists, geneticists, and political scientists have long at- tempted to identify and explain the associations between person- ality and political preferences (Adorno, Frenkel-Brunswik, Levinson, & Sanford, 1950; Eysenck, 1954; McCloskey, 1958). Most of these early attempts to link personality traits with political atti- tudes portrayed conservatism as a pathology and focused on con- necting conservatives with negative personality traits (e.g. Adorno et al., 1950; Altemeyer, 1981; Altemeyer, 1996). Such a view is exemplified by McCloskey (1958), who labeled conserva- tives as: ‘‘. . .social isolates, . . . people who think poorly of themselves, who suffer personal disgruntlement and frustration, who are submissive, timid, and wanting in confidence, who lack a clear sense of direction and purpose, who are uncertain about their values, and who are generally bewildered by the alarming task of having to thread their way through a society which seems to them too complex to fathom” (37). Focusing on the causal connection between negative personality traits and conservatism and positive traits with liberalism contin- ues to dominate research in this area, though the language has be- come more subtle (see Block & Block, 2006; Jost, Glaser, Kruglanski, & Sulloway, 2003; Jost et al., 2008). For example, researchers
  • 8. have ll rights reserved. wa, 344 Schaeffer Hall, Iowa atemi). worked very hard to demonstrate that the positively valued Open- ness to Experience dimension has a strong and consistent negative relationship with political conservatism (see Gosling, Rentfrow, & Swann, 2003; Jost, Federico, & Napier, 2009; McCrae, 1996; Mondak & Halperin, 2008; Van Hiel, Kossowska, & Mervielde, 2000; Van Hiel, Pandelaere, & Duriez, 2004). Recent work however, has also demonstrated that Neuroticism is positively associated with economic liberalism (Leeson & Heaven, 1999; Riemann, Grubich, Hempel, Mergl, & Richter, 1993), though these findings are often dismissed or downplayed in favor or a more positive view of liber- alism and negative view of conservatism. The connection between personality and politics historically rested on the assumption that causality runs from personality traits to political attitudes. This seems plausible as personality is widely understood as some combination of innate dispositions and personal experiences that, in general, guides behavior in a sta- ble predictive manner (Bouchard, 1994; Cattell, 1957; Eysenck, 1990; Eysenck, 1991; Tellegen et al., 1988; Winter &
  • 9. Barenbaum, 1999). This is not to say behavior is predetermined by personality; rather, environmental circumstances provide the impetus for behaviors, and personality predispositions increase or decrease the probability of behavior only if the action is appropriate for a specific situation (Bandura, 2001; Caprara & Cervone, 2000; Mis- chel & Shoda, 1998). Political attitudes, on the other hand, have typically been viewed as much more capricious (Converse, 1964). Although a plethora of research on attitudes suggest that they can be quite stable over time, rather than being perceived as per- sonal dispositions, political attitudes are typically portrayed as http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2009.11.013 mailto:[email protected] http://www.sciencedirect.com/science/journal/01918869 http://www.elsevier.com/locate/paid B. Verhulst et al. / Personality and Individual Differences 49 (2010) 306–316 307 preferences related to the immediate social environment and are thus perceived as entirely context dependent. Such a view of attitudes, combined with the widely held assumption that, unlike personality, parental-offspring correla- tions of attitudes were entirely functions of cultural transmission (e.g., Niemi & Jennings, 1991; for an exception see Martin et al., 1986), reinforced the belief that personality traits were causally prior to social and political attitudes. However, such an assumption
  • 10. was unfounded, as the heritability of social and political attitudes has been established and replicated across populations (Bouchard & McGue, 2003; Eaves, Eysenck, & Martin, 1989; Martin et al., 1986; Waller, Kojetin, & Bouchard, 1990). Indeed, the genetic covariation between social attitudes and personality was explored over 30 years ago. Eaves and Eysenck (1974) identified a genetic relationship between personality and conservatism. Typical of a Nature Letter, however, while profound and novel, they offered only brief discussion on why this relation- ship existed. And, unfortunately, these findings went largely unad- dressed for several decades, with the exception of a handful of scholars. Among these, Bouchard, Lykken, McGue, Segal, and Telle- gen (1990) continued to pursue this question (e.g., Bouchard, 1997; Bouchard, 2009; Bouchard & McGue, 2003; Waller et al., 1990). This work has led to a paradigm shift in the connection between personality and politics. Combining Religiousness, Authoritarian dispositions, and tradi- tional Conservatism into a core attitude/world view system, Bou- chard (2009, p. 36) drew on other contemporaries (e.g., Saucier, 2000), and synthesized a theory that more accurately integrated attitude dimensions and personality traits. Such a synthesis im- plies that whatever relationship exists between personality and political attitudes cannot be strictly causal; rather, it seems likely that the relationships among political attitudes and personality
  • 11. traits are in part expressions of the same underlying genetic liabil- ity. Verhulst, Hatemi, and Eaves (2009) provided strong support of such a position and offered the first replication of Eaves and Ey- senck’s (1974) findings presented some 35 years earlier. Using a large US twin sample (over 6000 pairs), they found that the vast majority of the covariance between Psychoticism and Militaristic/ Defense attitudes was due to a common underlying genetic influence. Building upon a series of works by Bouchard, Eaves, Eysenck and others, we examine the assumptions made in the existing lit- erature regarding the causal connection between personality traits and political attitudes. Specifically, we estimated the genetic and environmental sources of covariation between personality traits and political attitudes and extended the findings of Verhulst et al. (2009) by focusing on specific attitudinal dimensions of polit- ical attitudes and personality traits in a very large sample of Aus- tralian Twins. 2. Methods and results Data were collected from 1988 to 1990 by mailed surveys to two large cohorts of adult Australian twins enrolled in the volun- teer Australian Twin Registry. Each participant completed a Health and Lifestyle Questionnaire (HLQ), which contained items on
  • 12. socio- political attitudes, personality traits, and wide variety of health- re- lated and sociodemographic measures (Eaves et al., 1989; Eaves et al., 1999; Martin, 1987; Truett et al., 1994). Using only respon- dents who completed the political and personality measures, the sample consists of 20,559 individuals from 5402 families; this in- cludes 7234 individual twins, comprising 3254 complete same sex pairs and 363 unlike sex pairs. The mean age of the entire sam- ple was 38.6 (SD = 15.5), while the mean age of the twin respon- dents was 34.1 (SD = 14.0). Regarding sex, 57.7% were female in the greater population, while in the twin sample 63.8% were fe- male. For more details on the sample, including ascertainment and response rates, see Hatemi, Medland, Morley, Heath, and Mar- tin (2007). 2.1. Measures Personality traits were measured by the short form of the Re- vised version of the Eysenck Personality Questionnaire (EPQ-R- S, Eysenck & Eysenck, 1997; Eysenck, Eysenck, & Barrett, 1985) with sub-scales for Extraversion, Psychoticism, Neuroticism and Social Desirability. Extraversion is comprised of two central components: affiliation or sociability, which includes valuing interpersonal bonds and being warm and affectionate, highly sociable and expressive,
  • 13. and agency or impulsiveness, which includes goal-oriented behavior, ambition, dogmatism, social dominance, leadership, aggression and assertiveness (Depue & Collins, 1999). Psychoticism is posi- tively related to authoritarianism, risk taking, impulsivity, tough- mindedness, practicality, magical ideation, and religiosity and neg- atively related to openness to experience. A careful inspection of the items that comprise the Psychoticism scale used in this paper (as presented in Appendix 1) suggest that the factor is character- ized by low levels of introspection and a preoccupation with rules and regulations. Neuroticism is associated with feelings of inferior- ity, unhappiness, anxiety, dependence, hypochondria, guilt, emo- tional instability, and obsessiveness (Eysenck & Eysenck, 1985). The additional sub-factor of Social Desirability (often dubbed the ‘‘lie” scale), was also included in the analyses and is characterized by social acquiescence or conformity and lack of self-insight (Fran- cis, Brown, & Pearson, 1991). To maximize the empirical validity of the personality dimen- sions, items were dropped from the personality scales if they were explicitly related to one of the attitude dimensions or if they did
  • 14. not contribute to the intended trait. The nature of the dimensions remained unchanged, however. The empirical validity problem is most evident with the Psychoticism scale. Although the Psychoti- cism sub-scale is established in the extant literature, in many re- cent cases it has less than ideal levels of inter-item reliability. This is most likely because the Psychoticism scale was developed in the 1950s, and then revised in the mid 1980s. Thus, the tech- niques available to assess reliability when the scales were created were impoverished. Retaining items that do not meaningfully con- tribute to the latent factor introduces error into the model. There- fore, dropping these specific items increased the precision with which we can estimate the trait and the confidence we have in our analyses. Another common problem that plagues the study of personality and politics is the clear distinction between attitudinal items and personality items. The tautology problem is more common in other personality measures, like the NEO-PI-R Openness to Experience scale where several items explicitly tap political concepts (see Costa & McCrae, 1992). Remaining cognizant of this problem, we identified three items in the Psychoticism scale that had also the possibility of overlapping with attitudinal items. Specifically, the items ‘‘Would being in debt worry you?” and ‘‘Do you think
  • 15. people spend too much time safeguarding their future with savings and insurances?” potentially overlap with economic political attitudes, while ‘‘Do you think marriage is old-fashioned and should be done away with?” potentially overlaps with both social and religious political attitudes. Importantly, the factor loadings of these items are not overwhelming and measures of fit do not decline with the removal of these items. Removing potentially tautological items is not meant to fundamentally alter the concept, but rather ensure that the items that comprise the construct are not inher- ently political. In other words, this should ensure that the observed 308 B. Verhulst et al. / Personality and Individual Differences 49 (2010) 306–316 relationship is between the traits and not a function of similar items. Political and social attitudes were assessed utilizing a 50-item index of political and social issues contemporary at the time of the survey (see Posner, Baker, Heath, & Martin, 1996). In a Wilson and Patterson (1968) format, respondents indicated if they agreed, disagreed, or were uncertain about their attitudes towards a wide range of issues across several ideological dimensions (e.g., Death Penalty, Bible Truth, Legalized Prostitution, Immigration, Stiffer Jail Terms, etc.). Of these 50 items, 26 of the most explicitly
  • 16. political items were used to construct four political attitude dimensions: Religious, Sex, Out-groups and Punishment dimensions (see details below). To facilitate comparisons between this study and similar others, we also created a Liberal–Conservative ideology measure using all 50 items as the scale was intended and commonly found in the extant literature (see Wilson & Patterson, 1968). A complete list of the items can be found in Appendix 2. Unlike personality, there is no consensual model or theory of socio-political attitudes. Although there is wide agreement on a general, abstract left–right spectrum, the attitudes that comprise this spectrum remain elusive and insofar as specific attitude facets go, they remain underdeveloped (see Bouchard, 2009; Eagly & Chaiken, 1993). As such, to identify the attitude factors, we relied heavily on the face validity of the scales and the empirical results. 2.2. Preliminary analysis A confirmatory factor analysis (CFA) with missing data was con- ducted specifying the four personality factors and the four political ideology attitude factors identified above (see Appendices 1 and 2 for the specifics of the CFA). For the personality traits, higher scores indicated higher levels of the traits, and for the ideological
  • 17. dimen- sions higher scores indicated the liberal position. The model fit sta- tistics for this class of Confirmatory Factor Models are impoverished, especially for relatively complex factor models such as those estimated in this paper. To compensate for this, we esti- mated a comparable non-nested model with list-wise deletion, where the factor loadings and the factor correlations were essen- tially unchanged. In this model the RMSEA was 0.044, the CFI was 0.870, and the TLI was 0.917. These fit indices suggest that the model fits the data very well. As such, we are confident in the factor structure we estimate. 2.3. Phenotypic correlations between personality traits and ideological dimensions We limited our analyses to the relationships between person- ality traits and ideological dimensions to those relationships that had correlations greater than 0.20. This decision was based on the increased reliably of disentangling covariance between the phenotypic traits (Cohen, 1988). The first stage of our anal- ysis focused on identifying the phenotypic relationships be- tween the personality traits and the social attitudes (see Table 1). The correlations between our attitude dimensions and personality traits were much larger than those reported for correlations between overall Conservatism–Liberalism (ideol- ogy) and personality traits in other studies (see Carney, Jost, Gosling, & Potter, 2008; Verhulst et al., 2009). Further, the gen- eral ideology scale also was highly correlated with several per- sonality dimensions.
  • 18. As can be seen in Table 1, several relationships stand out. Most notably, Psychoticism was strongly related to more conservative Religious, Sex and Punishment attitudes in both sexes. This spe- cific pattern of relationships was consistent with the original re- search on the Authoritarian personality (Adorno et al., 1950) and the magnitude is consistent with the relationship between authoritarianism and general conservatism (Altemeyer, 1998; Stenner, 2005). We expected that Psychoticism, by virtue of its theoretical relationship with Authoritarianism, would be associ- ated with Religious, Sex, and Punishment attitudes, as these atti- tudes are central to the concept of Authoritarianism (see Bouchard, 2009). These same political attitudes were also correlated with Social Desirability in both sexes, although in the opposite (liberal) direc- tion and to a lesser extent. Although the personality and politics literature has generally ignored the connection between Social Desirability and ideological or attitudinal dimensions, this is the second study to link Social Desirability with liberal social attitudes in very large genetically informative samples (see Verhulst et al., 2009). Thus, it appears that an empirical relationship exists, even though the theoretical justifications for this relationship are not yet resolved. As a point of comparison with other personality and politics studies, we also examined the relationship between the additive 50-item general ideology scale and the four personality traits. The general ideology dimension was also highly related to Psychot-
  • 19. icism and Social Desirability. The most interesting feature of the pattern of correlations between the general ideology dimension and the personality traits mirrored the Religious and Sex attitudes. Items along these dimensions comprised the majority of the items in the scale, and thus appeared to drive the relationships observed between the general ideology factor and the separate personality traits. Importantly, by narrowly focusing in the general ideology factor the relationships between extraversion and neuroticism and the political attitudes, which we discuss below, would be obscured. The relationship between Neuroticism and liberal Out-group attitudes also corresponds with recent findings in the emerging lit- erature within the personality and politics sub-field. Specifically, Neuroticism has been linked with liberal economic ideological atti- tudes in several samples and is consistent with Verhulst et al. (2009) analysis of US twins. Importantly, attitudes on immigration were part of the US economic ideology dimension in that study, but here in our Australian population they were part of the Out- group dimension. Finally, we found a relationship between Extraversion and con- servative Sex attitudes in these analyses. Very few contemporary studies in the personality and politics literature have found signif-
  • 20. icant relationships between Extraversion and attitudinal or ideo- logical predispositions. However, our finding is consistent with an older literature showing Extraversion to be positively related to Authoritarianism and Conservatism (Eysenck & Wilson, 1978; Pearson & Greatorex, 1981; Ray, 1980; for a recent replication see Carney et al., 2008). Subsequent analyses often failed to repli- cate these earlier findings. We believe part of the failure to repli- cate these relationships is due to the left–right generalization of attitude factors. However, greater Extraversion has been long established as a significant influence on sex drive, earlier age of first intercourse, promiscuity, orgasm, and other activities (Cooper, Agocha, & Sheldon, 2000; Eysenck, 1976; Miller et al., 2004; Schenk & Pfrang, 2005). Thus, regarding the relationship between Extra- version and the Sex attitudes factor, the direction of the correlation is in the opposite direction than expected. At first glance it appears for Australians, the relationship between Extraversion and atti- tudes on sex are quite different from the relationship between Extraversion and actual sexual behavior. Future studies are needed to replicate this finding. 2.4. Genetic analyses Structural equation models incorporating genetic, shared and unique environmental sources of covariation allowed us to ex-
  • 21. Table 1 Phenotypic correlations between personality and attitude dimensions for male (N = 3449) and female (N = 6066) twins. Psychoticism Extraversion Neuroticism Social Desirability Female twins Religious attitudes �.534 �.124 �.003 .273 Sex attitudes �.601 �.266 �.049 .359 Out-group attitudes �.106 �.080 .204 .211 Punishment attitudes �.451 �.047 .094 .282 General ideology �.566 �.177 .001 .357 Male twins Religious attitudes �.496 �.035 .008 .237 Sex attitudes �.570 �.196 �.033 .318 Out-group attitudes �.004 �.010 .166 .100 Punishment attitudes �.421 .047 .021 .231 General ideology �.495 �.061 �.008 .261 Note: Higher scores on the Personality factors denoted higher levels of the traits. Higher scores on the attitude factors denote more liberal responses for all attitude dimensions. All relationships except that between Psychoticism and Out-group attitudes were statistically significant at .01 or better. However, conventional levels of statistical significance do not focus our attention on substantively meaningful relationships. Here we focus only on significant relationships where the effect size is medium or larger (correlations greater P.20 in bold; see Cohen, 1988). Table 2 Between co-twin correlations by zygosity. MZM MZF DZM DZF DZOS
  • 22. Personality traits Psychoticism .536 .615 .381 .414 .370 Extraversion .516 .488 .193 .212 .211 Neuroticism .367 .438 .142 .213 .085 Social Desirability .383 .520 .318 .394 .267 Social attitudes Religious .592 .632 .506 .513 .536 Sex .592 .668 .452 .558 .505 Out-groups .600 .559 .482 .402 .361 Punishment .567 .587 .407 .339 .433 General ideology .631 .675 .517 .552 .524 N (twin pairs) 646 1385 384 839 363 B. Verhulst et al. / Personality and Individual Differences 49 (2010) 306–316 309 plore the nature of the relationships among personality and attitudes dimensions. Table 2 provides the twin correlations separated by zygosity and sex for the personality traits and so- cio-political attitude dimensions. The correlations between MZ twins were substantially larger than the correlations between same-sex DZ twins, providing initial support for the presence of genetic influences and reason to explore structural models to quantify those influences. However, the MZ–DZ correlation differ- ences were much greater for most personality dimensions than for the political attitude sub-factors, suggesting significant shared environmental influences on the attitude dimensions. For com- pleteness we also tabulate the correlations for the DZ opposite- sex pairs. For most variables the correlations for opposite-sex DZ twins were similar to the correlations for same-sex DZ twins, but for Social Desirability and attitudes to Out-groups the DZOS
  • 23. correlation was a bit lower, suggesting that familial effects on these scales were somewhat different in males and females for these constructs. For simplicity we therefore present results of separate analyses of male and female twins, omitting the oppo- site-sex pairs. Results of estimating the genetic and shared and non-shared environmental variance components for each measure alone, using maximum likelihood estimation in Mx (Neale, Boker, Xie, & Maes, 2003), are presented in Table 3. Components of variance due to Additive Genetic (A), Common environmental (C), and Unique Environmental (E) influence are tabulated (for detailed explana- tions of the methodology and theory, along with limitations and recent criticisms see Medland & Hatemi, 2009; Neale & Cardon, 1992). The results were largely consistent with the expectations based on the twin correlations and the long-standing personality litera- ture. Specifically, Psychoticism, Extraversion, and Neuroticism were characterized by large additive genetic and unique environ- mental components, while the common environmental component was not significant. For Social Desirability, the common environ- ment variance was statistically significant but the additive genetic component for males was not. In contrast, in both sexes, the addi- tive genetic, shared and unique environmental variance compo- nents for almost all the attitude scales were significant. The shared environment variance accounted for large portions of the variance in social attitudes. Importantly, the variance components
  • 24. of the general ideology scale are very similar to those of both the Sex and the Religious factors. The only attitude that differed from this trend was Punishment attitudes, where the common environ- ment variance was not significant for females. 2.5. Cholesky decomposition The preceding steps were necessary to examine the complex relationships between personality traits and political attitude dimensions. Several of the personality dimensions were correlated with different attitude dimensions at the phenotypic level. Our main interest, however, lay in exploring the nature of the pheno- typic relationships. As such, we undertook a series of bivariate Cholesky decompositions (see Fig. 1) to evaluate the sources of covariation. The constraints and model specifications are explained in detail in Medland and Hatemi (2009). Since we wished to know how much of the variance in attitudes could be explained by per- sonality, we put the personality factors first in our analyses. Tables 4 and 5 display the standardized path coefficients from the Cholesky analyses for females and males, respectively. Cells with dashes indicate that the paths were constrained to zero. This was done where the univariate variance decomposition models de- scribed in the previous section did not account for a statistically reliable portion of the variance in the trait. These constraints pre-
  • 25. vented model instability arising from very small sources of shared variance. As is shown in Table 4 for males and Table 5 for females, the relationships between Psychoticism and Religious, Sex, and Pun- ishment attitudes were largely due to additive genetic covariation. Performing a simple algebraic transformation of the path coeffi- cients, we found that the majorities of the covariation between Psychoticism and Punishment, Sex, and Religious attitudes was due to genetic covariance. Table 3 Estimated variance components for personality and ideology dimensions, separately by variable. Males Females a2 c2 e2 a2 c2 e2 Psychoticism .559 (.51, .60) – .441 (.39, .49) .602 (.57, .63) – .399 (.37, .43) Extraversion .503 (.45, .55) – .497 (.45, .55) .474 (.44, .51) – .526 (.49, .56) Neuroticism .362 (.30, .42) – .638 (.58, .70) .438 (.40, .48) – .562 (.52, .60) Social Desirability .141 (.00, .36) .230 (.03, .29) .629 (.57, .70) .368 (.23, .51) .141 (.01, .27) .490 (.46, .53) Religious attitudes .151 (.00, .32) .435 (.28, .57) .415 (.37, .46) .226 (.12, .34) .393 (.29, .49) .381 (.35, .41) Sex attitudes .271 (.11, .45) .316(.15, .46) .413 (.37, .46) .212 (.11, .32) .443 (.34, .53) .346 (.32, .37)
  • 26. Out-group attitudes .268 (.11, .44) .336 (.18, .47) .395 (.35, .44) .311 (.19, .44) .244 (.13, .35) .445 (.41, .48) Punishment attitudes .285 (.11, .47) .277 (.10, .43) .438 (.39, .61) .491 (.36, .61) .089 (.00, .21) .420 (.39, .45) General ideology .212 (.06, .37) .408 (.29, .54) .380 (.35, .42) .244 (.15, .31) .420 (.34, .51) .335 (.31, .36) Note: Estimates in italics are not significant. a c e A11 to the Personality Trait C11 to the Personality Trait E11 to the Personality Trait Personality Trait and Social Attitudes A12 to the Social Attitude A22 to the Social
  • 27. Attitude C12 to the Social Attitude C22 to the Social Attitude E12 to the Social Attitude E22 to the Social Attitude Personality Trait Social Attitude A1 A2 C2C1 E1 E2 Fig. 1. General Cholesky model. Table 4 Bivariate cholesky results for personality traits and social attitudes for males.
  • 28. a c e Psychoticism and Religious Attitudes .758 – .653 (.72, .79) (.62, .69) �.476 .615 – – �.201 .596 (�.52, �.43) (.58, .65) (�.24, �.16) (.57, .563) Psychoticism and Sex Attitudes .757 – .654 (.72, .79) (.62, .69) �.539 .562 – – �.249 .577 (�.58, �.49) (.52, .60) (�.29, �.21) (.55, .61) Psychoticism and Punishment Attitudes .760 – .650 (.73, .79) (.61, .69) �.332 .680 – – �.260 .600 (�.39, �.28) (.65, .71) (�.31, �.22) (.57, .63) Psychoticism & Ideology .756 – .655 (.72, .78) (.62, .69) �.486 .632 – – �.192 .572 (�.53, �.44) (.59, .67) (�.24, �.15) (.54, .60) Extraversion and Sex Attitudes .715 – .700 (.67, .75) (.66, .74) �.182 .757 – – �.108 .619 (�.24, �.12) (.73, .78) (�.15, �.06) (.59, .65) Neuroticism and Out�Group Attitudes .610 – .792 (.55, .66) (.75, .83) .183 .764 – – .061 .615 (.11, .26) (.73, .79) (.02, .11) (.58, .65) Social Desirability and Religious Attitudes .386 .495 .778 (.08, .60) (.23, .62) (.74, .82) .362 .138 .106 .651 .055 .642 (.03, .56) (.00, .49) (�.16, .36) (.52, .72) (.01, .10) (.61, .68)
  • 29. 310 B. Verhulst et al. / Personality and Individual Differences 49 (2010) 306–316 Table 5 Bivariate cholesky results for personality traits and social attitudes for females. a c e Psychoticism & Religious Attitudes .783 – .622 (.76, .80) (.60, .65) �.508 .615 – – �.209 .567 (�.54, �.48) (.59, .64) (�.24, �.18) (.55, .59) Psychoticism & Sex Attitudes .783 – .623 (.76, .80) (.60, .65) �.564 .594 – – �.246 .518 (�.59, �.53) (.57, .62) (�.27, �.22) (.50, .54) Psychoticism & Punishment .787 – .617 (.77, .80) (.59, .64) �.390 .658 – – �.231 .601 (�.43, �.35) (.63, .68) (�.26, �.20) (.58, .62) Psychoticism & Ideology .783 – .623 (.76, .80) (.60, .65) �.580 .586 – – �.170 .540 (�.61, �.55) (.56, .61) (�.20, �.14) (.52, .56) Extraversion and Sex Attitudes .691 – .723 (.66, .72) (.70, .75) �.265 .776 – – �.109 .562 (�.31, �.22) (.76, .79) (�.14, .08) (.54, .58)
  • 30. Neuroticism and Out�Group Attitudes .664 – .748 (.63, .69) (.72, .77) .204 .726 – – .094 .650 (.16, .25) (.70, .75) (.06, .13) (.63, .67) Social Desirability and Religious Attitudes .498 .520 .694 (.35, .61) (.40, .61) (.67, .72) .117 .428 .339 .555 .051 .614 (�.05, .28) (.28, .54) (.20, .50) (.41, .65) (.02, .08) (.59, .64) Social Desirability and Sex Attitudes .502 .518 .693 (.36, .62) (.39, .61) (.67, .72) .123 .405 .435 .539 .095 .577 (�.03, .28) (.27, .51) (.30, .60) (.35, .63) (.07, .13) (.55, .60) Social Desirability and Punishment Attitudes .504 .517 .692 (.37, .61) (.40, .61) (.67, .72) .057 .671 .355 .000 .096 .642 (�.11, .21) (.59, .72) (.22, .47) (�.31, .31) (.06, .13) (.62, .66) Social Desirability and Out�Group Attitudes .517 .505 .691 (.38, .63) (.37, .60) (.67, .72) .011 .563 .290 .393 .080 .662 (�.17, .18) (.44, .67) (.13, .47) (.00, .53) (.05, .11) (.64, .69) Social Desirability and Ideology .515 .506 .692 (.38, .63) (.38, .60) (.67, .72) .096 .450 .504 .450 .070 .572 (�.06, .25) (.33, .55) (.36, .69) (.00, .58) (.04, .10) (.55, .60) Note: Estimates in italics were not significant. Again, parameters with a lower confidence bound of zero should be interpreted as not statistically significant. Table 4 (continued)
  • 31. a c e Social Desirability and Sex Attitudes .384 .497 .778 (.10, .60) (.24, .62) (.74, .82) .445 .274 .137 .548 .104 .631 (.11, .67) (.00, .49) (�.16, .38) (.39, .65) (.06, .15) (.60, .67) Social Desirability and Punishment Attitudes .399 .484 .779 (.00, .61) (.21, .63) (.74, .82) .035 .543 .299 .416 .086 .659 (�.64, .65) (.00, .70) (.04, .60) (.00, .60) (.04, .14) (.62, .70) Social Desirability and Ideology .397 .486 .779 (.00, .61) (.21, .63) (.74, .82) .179 .427 .323 .549 .066 �.612 (�.53, .58) (.00, .59) (.09, .68) (.00, .68) (.02, .11) (�.65, �.58) Note: Estimates in italics were not significant. To solve estimation problems in a limited number of models we bounded the parameters to have a lower confidence bound of zero. Thus, parameters with a lower confidence bound of zero should be interpreted as not statistically significant. B. Verhulst et al. / Personality and Individual Differences 49 (2010) 306–316 311 Importantly, although the paths from Psychoticism to the attitude dimensions (A12) were quite strong for each attitude dimension, these paths were still smaller than the paths accounting for the unique influence (A22) on the attitude dimen- sions. Thus, although there was a strong relationship between Psychoticism and each attitude dimension, the majority of the additive genetic variance in the attitude dimensions was not ac- counted for by the genetic influences on Psychoticism. The
  • 32. vari- ance shared between the unique environmental component of Psychoticism and the attitude dimensions were minimal (paths E12), while the residual unique environmental variance in the attitude dimensions were uniformly high (paths E22), which is not surprising since they include measurement error. This pat- tern of results did not vary substantially by sex. Again, the pat- 312 B. Verhulst et al. / Personality and Individual Differences 49 (2010) 306–316 tern of relationships for the general ideology scale mimicked the relationships for the Religious and Sex attitudes with Psychoti- cism. Specifically, the phenotypic relationship between general ideology and Psychoticism appeared to be driven by the additive genetic influence. Similar relationships existed between Extraversion and Sex attitudes as well as between Neuroticism and Out-group atti- tudes. In both cases, the connections between the personality traits and the attitude dimensions were primarily accounted for by the additive genetic covariance between the constructs rather than by any environmental covariance. Again, this pattern of re- sults was remarkably consistent across sexes. In both cases, the phenotypic relationships were much smaller than the relation- ships observed between Psychoticism and the attitude dimen- sions. As such, there were correspondingly smaller paths A12 in these models. The relationship between Social Desirability and each attitude dimension (Sex, Punishment, and Out-groups) deviated slightly from the pattern of relationships identified for the other personal- ity traits. Specifically, for females both the additive genetic and
  • 33. common environment variance components were significant for Social Desirability. Thus, this necessitated estimating Cholesky models with common environmental pathways. To ensure that the male model was broadly comparable with the female model, we estimated the same parameters even though the additive ge- netic variance in Social Desirability was not statistically significant when it was measured alone in males. The Cholesky results for Social Desirability were generally con- sistent across sexes. Specifically, the relationships between Social Desirability and the attitude dimensions rested primarily in the common environmental covariances. For females, the common environmental covariations (paths C12) between Social Desirabil- ity and the attitude dimensions were substantively large and sta- tistically significant for all political attitudes. Furthermore, the additive genetic covariances (paths A12) were substantively small and statistically not significant for all political attitudes. The pat- tern for males was only slightly different. For the Religious and Punishment attitude dimensions, the common environmental covariances (paths C12) were statistically significant and substan- tively large. For Sex attitudes, however, the common environmen- tal covariance was much smaller and not statistically significant. Furthermore, for Religious and Sex attitudes, the additive genetic covariances with Social Desirability were statistically significant. Remember, however, that the additive genetic component was not statistically significant for Social Desirability in the
  • 34. univariate models for males. Furthermore, we had many more female than male twins, giving us greater power to detect significant results in the female sample. While single population based results should be accepted with caution, the current results are broadly consistent with analyses on data from an equally large US twin sample (see Verhulst et al., 2009). Specifically, the current study replicated the findings from a completely separate population of twins in a different country, and therefore increases generalizability of the effects. Fi- nally, as with the other personality traits, the result for the gen- eral ideology factor mirrored the Religious attitude factor. Specifically, for both males and females, the primary source of covariance between Social Desirability and any of the attitude factors was common environmental covariance rather than addi- tive genetic covariance. 3. Discussion The results in this paper offer a more fine grained explanation for the relationship between personality traits and social atti- tudes than that captured by looking only at the Conservatism– Liberalism super factor (ideology). Specifically, ideology was strongly and negatively correlated with Psychoticism and posi- tively correlated with Social Desirability. These same relation- ships were also seen with the Religious, Sex, and Punishment attitude sub-factors but not with the attitudes toward Out- groups, where the correlation with Psychoticism was negligible. In addition we also saw a modest (��0.2) negative correlation between Extraversion and liberal attitudes to sex and a positive correlation (�0.2) between Neuroticism and liberal attitudes to Out-groups. All these correlations were remarkably consistent between sexes.
  • 35. Consistent with the existing literature, we showed that individ- ual differences in both personality traits and political attitudes could be decomposed into genetic and environmental variance compo- nents. For personality traits environmental influences were over- whelmingly unique to the individual, except for Social Desirability where there was an important component of shared environmental influence. For attitudes, on the other hand, we saw substantial genet- ic influences, but large components of shared environmental vari- ance as well. However, the common environmental covariance is likely to be confounded in some part by assortative mating (see Eaves et al., 1999; Eaves & Hatemi, 2008). Indeed, the spousal corre- lations for Social Desirability was 0.25 and ranged from 0.45 to 0.65 for the attitudinal dimensions (p < .001, N = 3535). Finally, using a multivariate genetic model, we demonstrated that the phenotypic relationships between personality and attitudes were primarily functions of a single source of covariance, genetic in the case of the correlations of Psychoticism, Neuroticism, and Extraversion with attitude dimensions in both males and females, and shared environ- ment for correlations between Social Desirability and attitudes. In line with our expectations, Psychoticism was strongly related to more conservative positions on Religious, Sex, and
  • 36. Punishment attitude dimensions. This finding is consistent with previous theo- rizing that suggests these attitude dimensions should be central to Authoritarian concerns, and that Authoritarianism, Traditional Conservatism, and Psychoticism are intertwined (Adorno et al., 1950; Bouchard, 2009). The correlations between Psychoticism and the attitudinal dimensions we demonstrate in this paper are larger than those presented in other similar papers using other omnibus person- ality theories; however, the reported relationships are consis- tent with the theoretical relationship between Psychoticism and various measures of Authoritarianism, like Adorno et al.’s (1950) F scale, Altemeyer’s (1981, 1996) RWA scale, and Feld- man’s (2003) Social Conformity–Autonomy Scale. Notably, both the F and the RWA scales have been strongly criticized for including explicit political content in the measurement of Authoritarianism, while the development of the Social Confor- mity–Autonomy scale goes to great lengths to remove any explicitly political content from the measurement of authoritar- ianism, and thus avoid confounding the personality trait with attitudinal preferences. Therefore, the magnitude of the rela- tionships between the attitudinal variables and Psychoticism are not unexpected. A similar, but substantially weaker relationship existed be- tween Extraversion and more conservative attitudes on the Sex attitude dimension. This finding is opposite to what might be ex- pected based on Extraversion’s relationship to mode liberal sexual behavior (see Cooper et al., 2000). However, such a finding is in line
  • 37. with older studies of conservatism and personality (see Eysenck & Wilson, 1978; Pearson & Greatorex, 1981; Ray, 1980). We see two possible explanations for this relationship. First, there was a rea- sonably large correlation between Psychoticism and Extraversion (r = 0.37). As such, the relationship between Extraversion and Sex attitudes that we observed may have been a function of the very B. Verhulst et al. / Personality and Individual Differences 49 (2010) 306–316 313 strong relationship between Psychoticism and Sex attitudes. Alter- natively, as noted above, Extraversion has two sub-factors: affilia- tion/sociability and agency/impulsiveness. Thus, the phenotypic relationship may have been driven by the agency/impulsiveness sub-factor. We leave it to future research to disentangle these possibilities. On the other hand, the more Neurotic our twins were, the more ‘‘liberal” their attitudes toward Out-groups (e.g. immigration, mul- ticulturalism). This echoed the relationship found between Neurot- icism and more liberal attitudes on economic issues in a large US sample, as the Australia Out-group dimension and the US Eco- nomic dimension shared one important item in particular – atti- tudes to immigration (see Verhulst et al., 2009).
  • 38. One of the most interesting, but least explored, personality traits in the context of political attitudes is Social Desirability. The higher respondents scored on this dimension, the more likely they were to support liberal positions on Religious, Sex, and Pun- ishment attitudes. Overall the relationships between attitudes and Social Desirability were the opposite of the relationships be- tween Psychoticism and attitudes, though substantially weaker. In this study, as in Verhulst et al. (2009), Social Desirability corre- lated with more liberal social attitudes. In sum, the relationships between Psychoticism, Extraversion, and Neuroticism and all attitude dimensions (Sex, Punishment, and Religious) were largely driven by common genetic influ- ences. For Social Desirability the pattern that emerged was the vast majority of covariation between Social Desirability and each attitude dimension was due to the similarity in shared environment, though this effect was less consistent in males. Regardless of the source of covariance between personality and attitude dimensions, the overwhelming portion of individ- ual differences remained unique to each trait, with one excep- tion – Psychoticism and Sex attitudes shared some 40% of their covariance. Of central importance to our findings, the relationships between personality traits and political ideologies were not simple or uni- form: different attitude dimensions correlated with different per- sonality traits to varying degrees. Importantly, if we had focused only on the relationship between the general ideology factor and the personality traits we would have missed several key
  • 39. relation- ships, including the relationship between Neuroticism and more liberal attitudes, which seemed to be restricted to economic or group competition attitude dimensions. Furthermore, the intrigu- ing relationship between Extraversion and Sex attitudes would also have gone unnoticed. Recent research on personality and pol- itics has missed these findings because it focused only on the gen- eral ideological factor. Do you prefer to go your own way rather than act by the rules? Do you stop to think things over before doing anything? Would you take drugs which may have strange or dangerous eff Do good manners and cleanliness matter much to you? Would you like other people to be afraid of you? Is it better to follow society’s rules than go your own way? Are you a talkative person? Are you rather lively? Do you enjoy meeting new people? ects? Our analysis has brought together the foundational contribu- tions of H.J. Eysenck to personality and attitudes dimensionality, and the genetic studies of attitudes initiated by L.J. Eaves and N.G. Martin and further developed by T.J. Bouchard. Bouchard’s (2009) argument that combining Authoritarianism, Religiousness and Conservatism implies that whatever relationship exists be- tween personality and attitudes, is not that one predicts the other, but that both traits contain elements that are simply different rep- resentations of the same constructs. Such a position is supported
  • 40. by the results we show here. The finding that the majority of covariation between Psychot- icism and attitudes is due to genetic variance provides strong evidence that a simple environmentally driven causal theory from personality to politics is not suitable for such a complex relationship. It is important to keep in mind that genes are mul- tifaceted: there is no specific gene for a personality trait or for a political attitude, even if individual genes can be associated with specific attitudes or personality traits. However, the relationship between Social Desirability and the attitude dimensions exhib- ited a remarkably different pattern of results. The locus of the relationship in this case rested on shared environmental influ- ences on the two traits. Due to the significant spousal correla- tions for both Social Desirability and socio-political attitudes, it is likely that some part of the shared environmental covariance is attributed to assortative mating. The result provides some support for existing social science expectations about the impor- tance of environmental influences for the relationship between personality traits and social attitudes (see Mondak & Halperin, 2008). The social nature of Social Desirability makes the connec- tion between this personality trait and the associated social atti- tudes reasonable. However, this environmental relationship is the exception to our understanding of personality and politics based on our findings here and in other studies (see Verhulst et al., 2009). These analyses should be seen as the beginning of a new stage in the research connecting personality and politics. At this point, we know that the patterns of relationships are more complex
  • 41. than those suggested by previous models. That is, we cannot assume causality, nor focus only on a single left–right ideological spectrum. Future research focusing on both active and passive gene– environ- ment covariation using extended kinship designs, and gene– envi- ronment interaction models are underway to further explore these patterns. Appendix 1. Confirmatory factor structure of the EPQ Psychoticism Extraversion Neuroticism Social Desirability .57 �.31 .79 �.42 .38 �.46 .66 .71 .60 (continued on next page) Appendix A (continued) Psychoticism Extraversion Neuroticism Social
  • 42. Desirability Can you usually let yourself go and enjoy yourself at a lively party? .76 Do you usually take the initiative in making new friends? .69 Can you easily put some life into a rather dull party? .81 Do you tend to keep in the background on social occasions? �.83 Do you like mixing with people? .73 Do you often take on more activities than you have time for? .26 Do you like plenty of bustle and excitement around you? .60 Do you like doing things in which you have to act quickly? .57 Are you mostly quiet when you are with other people? �.75 Do other people think of you as being very lively? .81 Can you get a party going? .80 Would you call yourself happy-go-lucky? .49 Does your mood often go up and down? .69 Do you ever feel ‘just miserable’ for no reason? .64 Are you an irritable person? .62 Are your feelings easily hurt? .65 Do you often feel ‘fed-up’? .72 Would you call yourself a nervous person? .63 Are you a worrier? .75 Would you call yourself tense or ‘high-strung’? .65 Do you worry too long after an embarrassing experience? .62 Do you suffer from ‘nerves’? .59 Do you often feel lonely? .66 Are you often troubled about feelings of guilt? .66 If you say you will do something, do you always keep your promise? .49 Were you every greedy by helping yourself to more than your share? �.63 Have you ever blamed someone for what was really your fault?
  • 43. �.59 Are all your habits good and desirable ones? .59 Have you ever taken anything that belonged to someone else? �.68 Have you ever broken or lost something belonging to someone else? �.55 Have you ever said anything bad or nasty about anyone? �.71 As a child, were you ever ‘fresh’ towards your parents? �.51 Have you ever cheated at a game? �.70 Have you ever taken advantage of someone? �.76 Do you always practice what you preach? .57 Do you sometimes put off until tomorrow what you ought to do today? �.36 Appendix 2. Confirmatory factor structure of the Australian socio-political attitude index Religious Sex Out-groups Punishment Bible Truth .84 Divine Law .74 Sabbath Observance .74 Church Authority .76 Divorce �.60 Evolution Theory �.55 Condom Machine �.86 Abortion �.78 Legalized Prostitution �.69 Casual Sex �.64 Chastity .63 Birth Control �.51 Surrogate Mothers �.50 Test Tube Babies �.44
  • 44. Gay Rights �.54 �.46 Asian Immigration �.79 Multiculturalism �.73 314 B. Verhulst et al. / Personality and Individual Differences 49 (2010) 306–316 Appendix 2 (continued) Religious Sex Out-groups Punishment White Superiority .64 Conservationists �.49 Apartheid .46 Foreign Ownership �.34 Stiffer Jail Terms .82 Strict Rules .68 Death Penalty .59 Caning .48 Teenage Delinquency �.44 Note: In addition to the 26 items above, the following items comprise the 50-item general conservatism scale: Privatization, Medicare, Trade Unions, Patriotism, Voluntary Euthanasia, Nuclear Power, Working Mothers, Inborn Conscience, Government Welfare, Socialism, Herbal Remedies, Charity Work, Suicide, Licensing Laws, Fluoridation, Royalty, Women Judges, Disarmament, Censorship, White Lies, Teenage Dole, Private Schools, Chiropractors, Defense Spending, Legalized Modern Art. The items excluded from the attitudinal factors did not contribute to the latent attitudes and were thus omitted from the traits.
  • 45. B. Verhulst et al. / Personality and Individual Differences 49 (2010) 306–316 315 References Adorno, T. W., Frenkel-Brunswik, E., Levinson, D. J., & Sanford, R. N. (1950). The Authoritarian personality. New York: Harper and Row. Altemeyer, R. A. (1981). Right-wing Authoritarianism. Winnipeg: University of Manitoba Press. Altemeyer, R. A. (1996). The Authoritarian specter. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press. Altemeyer, R. A. (1998). The other authoritarian personality. Advances in Experimental Social Psychology, 30, 41–92. Bandura, A. (2001). Social cognitive theory: An agentic perspective. Annual Review of Psychology, 52(1), 1. Block, J., & Block, J. H. (2006). Nursery school personality and political orientation two decades later. Journal of Research in Personality, 40, 734– 749. Bouchard, T. J. Jr., (1994). Genes, environment, and personality. Science, 264, 1700–1701. Bouchard, T. J. Jr., (1997). The genetics of personality. Handbook of psychiatric
  • 46. genetics. CRC Press. Bouchard, T. J. Jr, (2009). Authoritarianism religiousness and conservatism: Is obedience to authority the explanation for clustering universality and evolution? In E. Voland & W. Scheifenhovel (Eds.), The biological evolution of religious mind and behavior (pp. 165–180). Dordrecht/Heidelberg/London/New York: Springer. Bouchard, T. J., Jr., Lykken, D. T., McGue, M., Segal, N. L., & Tellegen, A. (1990). Sources of human psychological differences: The Minnesota Study of Twins Reared Apart. Science, 250(4978), 223–228. Bouchard, T. J., Jr., & McGue, M. (2003). Genetic and environmental influences on human psychological differences. Journal of Neurobiology, 54(1), 4–45. Caprara, G. V., & Cervone, D. (2000). Personality: Determinants, dynamics and potentials. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. Carney, D. R., Jost, J. T., Gosling, S. D., & Potter, J. (2008). The secret lives of liberals and conservatives: Personality profiles, interaction styles, and the things they leave behind. Political Psychology, 29, 807–840. Cattell, R. B. (1957). Personality and motivation structure and measurement. New York: World Book.
  • 47. Cohen, J. (1988). Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences (2nd ed.). Hillsdale, NJ: Earlbaum. Converse, P. E. (1964). The nature of belief systems in mass publics. In D. E. Apter (Ed.), Ideology and discontent (pp. 206–261). New York: Free Press. Cooper, M. L., Agocha, V. B., & Sheldon, M. S. (2000). A motivational perspective on risky behaviors: The role of personality and affect regulatory processes. Journal of Personality, 68(6). Costa, P. T., Jr., & McCrae, R. R. (1992). Normal personality assessment in clinical practice: The NEO Personality Inventory. Psychological Assessment, 4, 5–13. Depue, R. A., & Collins, P. F. (1999). Neurobiology of the structure of personality: Dopamine, facilitation of incentive motivation, and extraversion. Behavioral Brain Science, 22(3), 491–517. Eagly, A., & Chaiken, S. (1993). Psychology of attitudes. NY: Harcourt, Brace Jovanovich. Eaves, L. J., & Eysenck, H. J. (1974). Genetics and the development of social attitudes. Nature, 249, 288–289. Eaves, L. J., Eysenck, H. J., & Martin, N. G. (1989). Genes,
  • 48. culture and personality; an empirical approach. London: Academic Press. Eaves, L. J., & Hatemi, P. K. (2008). Transmission of attitudes toward abortion and gay rights: Parental socialization or parental mate selection? Behavior Genetics, 38, 247–256. Eaves, L. J., Heath, A. C., Martin, N. G., Maes, H. H., Neale, M. C., Kendler, K. S., et al. (1999). Comparing the biological and cultural inheritance of personality and social attitudes in the Virginia 30,000 study of twins and their relatives. Twin Research, 2, 62–80. Eysenck, H. J. (1954). The psychology of politics. New Brunswick, NJ: Routledge. Eysenck, H. (1976). Sex and personality. London Open Books. Eysenck, H. J. (1990). Biological dimensions of personality. In L. A. Pervin (Ed.), Handbook of personality: Theory and research (pp. 249–276). New York: Guilford. Eysenck, H. J. (1991). Dimensions of personality: 16, 5 or 3? Criteria for a taxonomic paradigm. Personality and Individual Difference, 12, 773–790. Eysenck, H. J., & Eysenck, M. W. (1985). Personality and individual differences: A natural science approach. New York: Plenum. Eysenck, H. J., & Eysenck, S. B. G. (1997). Eysenck Personality Questionnaire-Revised (EPQ-R) and short scale (EPQ-RS). Madrid: TEA Ediciones.
  • 49. Eysenck, S. B. G., Eysenck, H. J., & Barrett, P. (1985). A revised version of the Psychoticism scale. Personality and Individual Differences, 6(1), 21–29. Eysenck, H. J., & Wilson, G. D. (1978). The psychological basis of ideology. Lancaster: MTP Press. Feldman, S. (2003). Enforcing social conformity: A theory of authoritarianism. Political Psychology, 24, 41–74. Francis, L. J., Brown, L. B., & Pearson, P. R. (1991). The dual nature of the EPQ lie scale among university students in Australia. Personality and Individual Differences, 12(10), 989–991. Gosling, S. D., Rentfrow, P. J., & Swann, W. B. Jr., (2003). A very brief measure of the big-five personality domains. Journal of Research in Personality, 37, 504–528. Hatemi, P. K., Medland, S. E., Morley, K. I., Heath, A. C., & Martin, N. G. (2007). The genetics of voting: An Australian Twin Study. Behavior Genetics, 37(3), 435–448. Jost, J. T., Federico, C. M., & Napier, J. L. (2009). Political ideology: Its structure, functions, and elective affinities. Annual Review of Psychology, 60, 339–367. Jost, J. T., Glaser, J., Kruglanski, A. W., & Sulloway, F. J.
  • 50. (2003). Political conservatism as motivated social cognition. Psychological Bulletin, 129(3), 339–375. Jost, J. T., Napier, J. L., Thorisdottir, H., Gosling, S. D., Palfai, T. B., & Ostafin, B. (2008). Are needs to manage uncertainty and threat associated with political conservatism or ideological extremity? Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 33, 989–1007. Leeson, P., & Heaven, P. C. L. (1999). Social attitudes and personality. Australian Journal of Psychology, 51, 19–24. Martin, N. G. (1987). Genetic differences in drinking habits alcohol metabolism and sensitivity in unselected samples of twins. In H. W. Goedde & D. P. Agarwal (Eds.), Genetics and alcoholism (pp. 109–119). New York: Alan R. Liss. Martin, N. G., Eaves, L. J., Heath, A. C., Jardine, R., Feingold, L. M., & Eysenck, H. J. (1986). Transmission of social attitudes. Proceedings for the National Academy of Sciences, 83, 4364–4368. McCloskey, H. (1958). Conservatism and personality. American Political Science Review, 52, 27–45. McCrae, R. R. (1996). Social consequences of experiential openness. Psychological Bulletin, 120(3), 323–337.
  • 51. Medland, S. E., & Hatemi, P. K. (2009). Political science, biometric theory and twin studies: A methodological introduction. Political Analysis, 17(2), 191–214. Miller, J. D., Lynam, D., Zimmerman, R. S., Logan, T. K., Leukefeld, C., & Clayton, R. (2004). The utility of the Five Factor Model in understanding risky sexual behavior. Personality and Individual Differences, 36(7), 1611– 1626. Mischel, W., & Shoda, Y. (1998). Reconciling processing dynamics and personality dispositions. Annual Review of Psychology, 49, 229–258. Mondak, J. J., & Halperin, K. D. (2008). A framework for the study of personality and political behaviour. British Journal of Political Science, 38, 335–362. Neale, M. C., Boker, S., Xie, G., & Maes, H. (2003). Mx: Statistical modeling (5th ed.). Richmond, VA: Department of Psychiatry, Virginia Commonwealth University. Neale, M. C., & Cardon, L. L. (1992). Methodology for genetic studies of twins and families. Kluwer Academic. Niemi, R. G., & Jennings, M. K. (1991). Issues and inheritance in the formation of party identification. American Journal of Political Science, 35, 970–988.
  • 52. Pearson, P. R., & Greatorex, B. J. (1981). Do tough-minded people hold tough- minded attitudes? Current Psychology, 1(1), 45–48. 316 B. Verhulst et al. / Personality and Individual Differences 49 (2010) 306–316 Posner, S. F., Baker, L. A., Heath, A. C., & Martin, N. G. (1996). Social contact; social attitudes and twin similarity. Behavior Genetics, 26(2), 123. Ray, J. J. (1980). Are Authoritarians extraverted? British Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology, 19, 147–148. Riemann, R., Grubich, C., Hempel, S., Mergl, S., & Richter, M. (1993). Personality and attitudes towards current political topics. Personality and Individual Differences, 15, 313–321. Saucier, G. (2000). Isms and the structure of social attitudes. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 78, 366–385. Schenk, J., & Pfrang, H. (2005). Extraversion, neuroticism and sexual behavior: Interrelationships in a sample of young men. Journal Archives of Sexual Behavior, 15(6), 449–455. Stenner, K. (2005). The Authoritarian dynamic. London: Cambridge University Press. Tellegen, A., Lykken, D. T., Bouchard, T. J., Jr., Wilcox, K. J., Segal, N. L., & Rich, S.
  • 53. (1988). Personality similarity in twins reared apart and together. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 54(6), 1031–1039. Truett, K. R., Eaves, L. J., Walters, E. E., Heath, A. C., Hewitt, J. K., Meyer, J. M., et al. (1994). A model system for the analysis of family resemblance in extended kinships of twins. Behaviour Genetics, 24, 35–49. Van Hiel, A., Kossowska, M., & Mervielde, I. (2000). The relationship between openness to experience and political ideology. Personality and Individual Differences, 28(4), 741–751. Van Hiel, A., Pandelaere, M., & Duriez, B. (2004). The impact of need for closure on conservative beliefs and racism: Differential mediation by Authoritarian submission and Authoritarian dominance. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 30, 824–837. Verhulst, B., Hatemi, P. K., & Eaves, L. J. (2009). Personality traits and political ideologies. In Paper presented at the 39th annual meeting of the behavior genetics association, Minneapolis, MN, June 17–20. Waller, N. G., Kojetin, B. A., & Bouchard, T. J. (1990). Genetic and environmental influences on religious interests, attitudes, and values. A study. Psychological Science.
  • 54. Wilson, G. D., & Patterson, J. R. (1968). A new measure of conservatism. British Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology, 7, 264–269. Winter, D. G., & Barenbaum, N. B. (1999). History of modern personality theory and research. In L. A. Pervin & O. P. John (Eds.), Handbook of personality: Theory and research. New York, NY: The Guilford Press (2nd ed., pp. 3–27). The nature of the relationship between personality traits and political attitudesIntroductionMethods and resultsMeasuresPreliminary analysisPhenotypic correlations between personality traits and ideological dimensionsGenetic analysesCholesky decompositionDiscussionConfirmatory factor structure of the EPQConfirmatory factor structure of the Australian socio-political attitude indexReferences REGULAR ARTICLE Personality Psychology: Lexical Approaches, Assessment Methods, and Trait Concepts Reveal Only Half of the Story—Why it is Time for a Paradigm Shift Jana Uher Published online: 8 February 2013 # The Author(s) 2013. This article is published with open access at Springerlink.com Abstract This article develops a comprehensive philosophy-of- science for person- ality psychology that goes far beyond the scope of the lexical
  • 55. approaches, assessment methods, and trait concepts that currently prevail. One of the field’s most important guiding scientific assumptions, the lexical hypothesis, is analysed from meta- theoretical viewpoints to reveal that it explicitly describes two sets of phenomena that must be clearly differentiated: 1) lexical repertoires and the representations that they encode and 2) the kinds of phenomena that are represented. Thus far, personality psychologists largely explored only the former, but have seriously neglected studying the latter. Meta-theoretical analyses of these different kinds of phenomena and their distinct natures, commonalities, differences, and interrelations reveal that personality psychology’s focus on lexical approaches, assessment methods, and trait concepts entails a) erroneous meta-theoretical assumptions about what the phenomena being studied actually are, and thus how they can be analysed and interpreted, b) that contemporary personality psychology is largely based on everyday psychological knowledge, and c) a fundamental circularity in the scientific explanations used in trait psychology. These findings seriously challenge the widespread assumptions about the causal and universal status of the phenomena described by prominent personality models. The current state of knowledge about the lexical hypothesis is reviewed, and implications for personality psychology are discussed. Ten desiderata for future research are outlined to overcome the current paradigmatic
  • 56. fixations that are substan- tially hampering intellectual innovation and progress in the field. Keywords Personality. Meta-theory. Methodology. Lexical hypothesis . Lexical approach . Traits . Personality assessment . Philosophy of science . Big five model . Five factor model Integr Psych Behav (2013) 47:1–55 DOI 10.1007/s12124-013-9230-6 J. Uher (*) Comparative Differential and Personality Psychology, Freie Universität Berlin, Habelschwerdter Allee 45, 14195 Berlin, Germany e-mail: [email protected] Science provides a special way of constructing knowledge about the world. Unlike nonscientific knowledge construction, science provides a way of thinking simulta- neously about phenomena and the means of producing knowledge about them—this presupposes meta-theory and methodology (Althusser and Balibar 1970; Toomela 2011). Meta-theory refers to the philosophical assumptions about the theoretical nature of the phenomena to be studied and to the questions that are asked about them. Methodology refers to the ways (i.e., approaches) in which these questions can be answered and to the techniques (i.e., methods) that can
  • 57. therefore be used (Sprung and Sprung 1984). All sciences have meta-theories. They determine which elements of real phenomena can be reduced to precisely those subsets of elements that are considered relevant to and defining of concrete scientific phenomena and the ways in which they can be reduced (Althusser and Balibar 1970, p. 84, analysing the philosophy of science of Marx 1867; Køppe 2012; Weber 1949). Meta- theories determine what is considered data in a particular field (in a particular historical time; Kuhn 1962), and how the thus-defined data can be analysed and interpreted (Køppe 2012; Wagoner 2009). Hence, the first step of reducing real elements into facts is already a theoretical decision (Weber 1949, p. 173). “Alles Faktische ist schon Theorie”—all facts are already theory (Goethe 1907, p. 127). In other words, “it is the theory which decides what can be observed” (Einstein to Heisenberg in 1926, cited in Heisenberg 1989, p. 10). Meta- theories and methodologies are the rules that govern the effective practices of sciences. Therefore, they should be explicated to enable researchers to constantly scruti- nise all levels from epistemology and ontology up to the specific theories about the phenomena studied (Toomela 2011). This also includes rethinking the very reduction of real phenomena into scientific phenomena (Utz
  • 58. 2005). Contemporary psychology largely follows meta-theory and methodology im- plicitly, sometimes even “blindly” (Toomela 2011, p. 22)—this occurs in partic- ular when research methods are decided first and research questions are adapted to the methods rather than vice versa (Omi 2012; Westen 1996). “The under- standing that research methodology comprises an essential part of scientific theories about phenomena that are studied is not always brought into the center of theoretical reasoning” (Toomela 2009, p. 45). Ignoring the meta-theoretical assumptions underlying particular methods can result in mismatches between methods and research questions that hamper the scientific understanding and the explanations of the phenomena being studied—and thus scientific progress (Loftus 1996; Toomela and Valsiner 2010; Weber 1949). More profoundly, a priori decisions on methods preclude posing the primary question of what the phenomena to be studied actually are (Toomela 2011). These meta-theoretical and methodological challenges are characteristic of con- temporary personality psychology and of taxonomic personality research in particular (Uher 2008a, b, 2011a, b; Uher, Methodological approaches to personality taxono- mies: The Behavioural Repertoire x Environmental Situations Approach—A non-
  • 59. lexical alternative, unpublished). The “discovery” of five major dimensions of indi- vidual differences is considered a milestone in modern Western psychology (De Raad 1998; Digman 1990; Goldberg 1990). This “break through” was made possible by the assumption that people encode in their everyday languages all those individual 2 Integr Psych Behav (2013) 47:1–55 differences that they perceive as most salient in everyday encounters and that they consider to be socially relevant. This so-called lexical hypothesis, first articulated by Galton (1884), has provided a stringent rationale for using the lexica of human languages as finite sources of information to unravel a few major dimensions of individual differences (Allport and Odbert 1936; Cattell 1943). In English and some other languages, the reduction of the pertinent lexical repertoires to five major dimensions has received the most support in Western scientific communities (Goldberg 1993; John et al. 1988). The lexical hypothesis also suggests that enquiring about the everyday psychological ideas that people develop of themselves and of other individuals could be a suitable method for the scientific measurement of personality (Block 2010; Westen 1996). Assessments by laypeople have become the standard methods of investigation (Baumeister et al. 2007;
  • 60. Matthews et al. 2003) and the “primary source of data” in personality psychology (Schwarz 1999, p. 93). The assumptions expressed in the lexical hypothesis might also contribute to the fact that the prevailing strategies of the scientific explanation of individual differences largely follow structures that are deeply rooted in everyday psychology. In spite of its enormous importance as one of the most widely used theoretical assumptions to have guided personality psychology (Ashton and Lee 2005), the lexical hypothesis has remained untested (Toomela 2010a; Westen 1996), and still today, its statements have been considered only partially. This article systematically explores the meta-theoretical assumptions that underlie this hypothesis. It starts by highlighting the explicit reference of the lexical hypothesis to two sets of phenomena that must be clearly differentiated; these are, on the one hand, people’s lexical repertoires and the representations that these repertoires encode, and, on the other hand, the kinds of phenomena that are perceivable in everyday life and that are being represented. Thus far, personality psychology has focused primarily on just one of these two sets—on lexically encoded representations—but has failed to systemati- cally investigate the second set, namely, the kinds of phenomena that are being perceived and represented.
  • 61. This article elaborates the distinct natures of the different kinds of phenomena to which the lexical hypothesis refers and explores their commonalities, differences, and interrelations from a philosophy-of-science perspective. This allows for scrutiny to be applied to the phenomena that are being lexically encoded and sheds new light on the fundamental questions of which kinds of phenomena can actually be captured by assessments—and which ones cannot. These analyses reveal that erroneous meta- theoretical assumptions underlie the established beliefs about what these phenomena actually are. They show that contemporary personality psychology is largely based on everyday psychological knowledge. Furthermore, the present meta-theoretical anal- yses allow us to scrutinise the prevailing psychological strategy of explaining indi- vidual differences—explicitly or implicitly—by assuming the existence of “traits”. The philosophy-of-science perspective identifies explanations based on trait concepts as fundamentally circular. These findings seriously challenge the established assump- tions about the causal and universal status of the phenomena described by prominent personality models. Various researchers have carefully crafted serious concerns about the established five factor models of personality (e.g., Block 1995, 2001, 2010; Eysenck 1992; McAdams 1992; Westen 1996), about assessment data and their interpretation (e.g., Brower 1949;
  • 62. Integr Psych Behav (2013) 47:1–55 3 Michell 1997, 2003; Omi 2012; Rosenbaum and Valsiner 2011; Schwarz 2009; Trendler 2009; Valsiner 2012), and about the explanations provided by trait psychology (e.g., Allport 1961; Bock 2000; Cervone et al. 2001; Lamiell 2003; Mischel and Shoda 1994, 1995). This article moves these critical objections coherently to meta-theoretical and methodological levels of consideration. It expands them substantially from the philosophy-of-science perspective and elaborates a fundamental critique of the lexical approaches, assessments by laypeople, and trait concepts that currently dominate per- sonality psychology. It closes by outlining 10 desiderata for future research to overcome the limitations revealed in the analyses and to stimulate new directions in the field. Lexical Encodings—Constructs and Representations The lexical hypothesis states that people encode in their everyday languages all those differences between individuals that they perceive to be salient and that they consider to be socially relevant in their everyday lives. Encodings about differences within individuals over time are not explicitly mentioned, however (see below). Among the many phenomena that individuals can perceive, recurrent patterns are particularly
  • 63. meaningful because such patterns may allow predictions of future events while facing the uncertainty of the future. Individuals therefore seek to identify recurrent patterns in their experiences with their personal world (Kelly 1955). To describe, explain, and to predict the likely occurrences of events, people construct and represent their experiences in private ideas (Valsiner 2012). These ideas are called subjective or individual representations (Jovchelovitch 2007; Moscovici 1984). Representations referring to experiences with one’s own person and the— especially social—world are called personal constructs in personality psychology (Kelly 1955). To communicate their perceptions and mental constructs of what they perceive and to negotiate socially shared meanings in order to cope with the world collectively, groups of individuals create and use social constructs. These constructs are called intersubjective or social representations (Jovchelovitch 2007) or folk concepts in personality psychology (Tellegen 1993). Constructs and representations referring to the perceptions of persons are called personality constructs. The lexical hypothesis states that, over time, socially shared constructs of self- and other-perception become encoded in the natural human languages. Pertinent lexical encodings reflect the body of everyday psychological ideas, beliefs, values, and practices that people have developed about individuals (Block 2010; Wagner et al. 1999;
  • 64. Westen 1996). What is Constructed and Represented—Phenomena and Patterns What is it that is constructed and represented as “personality”? Psychological defi- nitions of personality—a few prominent ones picked out of many—provide the first insights into what scientific psychologists construct as personality. Some frequently cited definitions refer fairly generally and descriptively to personality as those characteristics that account for a person’s consistent patterns of feeling, thinking, and behaving (Pervin and John 1997, p. 4) or as 4 Integr Psych Behav (2013) 47:1–55 an individual’s characteristic patterns of thought, emotion, and behaviour, together with the psychological mechanisms–hidden or not– behind those pat- terns (Funder 2004, p. 5). These definitions include psychological and behavioural phenomena. Allport (1937) also included psychophysical systems and environmental adaptivity by defin- ing personality as the dynamic organization within the individual of those psychophysical systems that determine his unique adjustments to his environment (p.
  • 65. 48). Eysenck (1947) furthermore specified genetic and environmental causality, bio- logical systems, and ontogenetic development when he defined personality as the sum-total of the actual or potential behaviour-patterns of the organism, as determined by heredity and environment; [that] originates and develops through the functional interaction of the four main sectors into which these behaviour- patterns are organized: the cognitive sector (intelligence), the conative sector (character), the affective sector (temperament), and the somatic sector (constitution) (p. 25). McCrae and Costa (2008) extended the scope of consideration by conceptualising particular personality constructs as reflecting “universals” of human nature that are “invariant across human cultures” (McCrae and Costa 1997, p. 510), and thus also potentially phylogenetic in origin (McCrae 2009). These definitions are sufficient for highlighting fundamental issues. First, they refer descriptively to particular patterns (e.g., those described as “characteristic” and “consistent”) and to the different kinds of phenomena in which these patterns occur (e.g., physiological, psychological, and behavioural phenomena). Second, they include assumptions about the causation of the described
  • 66. patterns in the described phenomena by other kinds of phenomena internal to the individual (e.g., psychological phenomena) and/or external (e.g., environmental conditions) considering various explanatory perspectives (e.g., proximate, adaptive, ontoge- netic, phylogenetic). Astonishingly, there is no mention of people’s lexical encodations and representations of individual differences although these are central to the lexical hypothesis. From philosophy-of-science perspectives, the different kinds of phenomena studied with regard to personality must be clearly differentiated. This is essential because interrelations among them can be untangled only if they are explored each in their own right and if a priori assumptions about specific interrelations are avoided. This is rarely done in psychology. Behaviourists have focused too much on external conditions and behaviours, but have largely ignored psycho- logical phenomena. Subsequent researchers have tried to overcome the limita- tions of behaviourism and have therefore focused strongly on psychological phenomena and on individuals’ representations of their world, but behaviour has faded into the background. The prevailing investigative strategies are preoc- cupied with causal processes and the conditions in which behavioural phenomena
  • 67. Integr Psych Behav (2013) 47:1–55 5 occur—especially psychological ones—with the result that clear differentiations of psychological and behavioural phenomena are missing. The meta-theoretical definition of behavioural phenomena as “external activities or externalisations of living organisms that are functionally mediated by the environ- ment (Millikan 1993) in the present” (Uher, What is behaviour? And (when) is language behaviour? A meta-theoretical definition, unpublished) allows such differ- entiations. This definition highlights the intrinsic relatedness of behaviour to proper- ties of the immediate external environment that are defined as environmental situations and emphasises that it is inherently bound to the present. Externality differentiates behaviour from thoughts, emotions, and other psychological phenom- ena, which are also bound to the present (Gillespie and Zittaun 2010; Toomela 2010a; Valsiner 1998, 2012) but are internal to the individual (Toomela 2008). This meta- theoretical definition generally defines all behavioural phenomena without specifying particular ones (e.g., goal-directed action or response) because many of these con- cepts include a priori assumptions about causally related internal processes (e.g., psychological phenomena) and external conditions in the environment (e.g., partic-
  • 68. ular stimuli) that are separate kinds of phenomena. To scrutinise which kinds of phenomena people can perceive in everyday life and construct as personality, it is helpful to first specify the particular patterns referred to in definitions of personality. Defining Patterns For scientific definitions, the notions of “a person’s consistent patterns”, “individual characteristics”, and “individual uniqueness” are surprisingly vague. They do not specify what is meant to be “consistent” with what, nor do they indicate which patterns are considered “different, “unique”, and “characteristic” and why. Moreover, they fail to mention within-individual variability and structural complexity—both within and be- tween individuals. This is remarkable because these patterns refer to phenomena (e.g., those of the psyche and of behaviour) that are heterogeneous, complex, dynamic, and thus highly fluctuating. In such phenomena, differences among individuals are neces- sarily apparent at any given time (Uher 2011a) and determinations of meaningful consistency—both within and between individuals—are matters of mere convention. Meta-theoretical analyses help to carve out basic criteria for determining which patterns can be considered specific to particular individuals, and thus different from others, which ones cannot, and why. So what does individual- specificity mean?
  • 69. Probabilistic Patterns In the steadily fluctuating flow of events in highly dynamic phenomena, individuals necessarily show considerable within- individual variability over time. Consequently, they can be characterised only by probabilistic patterns. What types of probability1 are able to describe these patterns? First, events of psychological and behavioural phenomena are not random, such as events in throws of dice. There are no equipossible elementary events in well- defined sample spaces that can be studied in repeated experiments as in Laplacian theory. Moreover, the 1 For the present meta-theoretical analyses, theories considering subjectivistic probabilities of the occur- rences of events (e.g., degrees of belief; Gillies 2000; Rychlak 1968) will not be considered here (for reasons described in detail below). 6 Integr Psych Behav (2013) 47:1–55 events are not independent from one another—this applies to events of the same phenomenon (e.g., several smiles) as well as to events of different phenomena (e.g., a smile and a laugh). Rather, different events may co-occur and even depend upon one another, particularly if the phenomena are functionally similar or related. Their events need not be exclusively disjunctive (e.g., smiles and laughs). Thus, finite (limiting) frequencies, which are defined as empirical occurrences relative
  • 70. to all possible occurrences (von Mises 1928), are inadequate for describing individual probabilities in psychological and behavioural phenomena. The concept of propensity probabilities seems to be better suited for this purpose. Propensities are considered properties of physical objects or situations that are assumed to cause empirical frequencies, even if only single-case observations are made (Popper 1959). They are therefore conceived also as properties of repeatable conditions that determine empirical frequencies in the long run (Gillies 2000).2 But this very assump- tion precludes the idea that propensity probabilities can be applied to individuals because their defining property as living systems is the fact that they undergo continuous changes during ontogenetic development. This is true for the molecules in their bodies and the phenomena constructed as personality alike. In fact, personality is assumed to develop and change gradually over the course of the lifespan (e.g., Caspi and Roberts 2001; Cattell 1950; Soldz and Vaillant 1999). This implies that the individual’s empirical frequencies cannot converge (i.e., stabilise) in the long run as may be true for physical systems. By contrast, the probabilities themselves change over time—an idea typically not considered by theories of propensity probabilities (cf. Gillies 2000; Popper 1959). Further differences are essential. In psychological and behavioural phenomena,
  • 71. there are no discrete events such as there are in coin tosses. Well-defined natural entities on which probability estimations could be based are lacking. Instead, it is the scientists who must decide which entities of real phenomena make up particular scientific phenomena. The ways in which they define entities as categories depend not only on the phenomena themselves, but also on the purposes of their reduction (Weber 1949). Moreover, the processual character and microgenetic development of many psychological and behavioural phenomena (Rosenthal 2004) entails that their occurrences can be construed as frequencies only in some cases. In other cases, they may be construed more accurately as durations of nonfixed and often highly variable length (or as latencies, which are durations with specified start times). Consequently, in fluctuating phenomena such as those of the psyche and of behaviour, events can be considered only by their empirical occurrences (i.e., in terms of frequencies and durations) relative to particular time periods. This new probability type will therefore be called a time-relative probability. For example, in observations of free play in a kindergarten group, a time- relative probability of 10 min per hour of observation time during which a child plays by him/herself may be determined for a particular 5-year-old child. The time periods in which time- relative probabilities are determined must be specified
  • 72. precisely—both in terms of the occasions and spans of time in which the data were collected to determine the probabilities and in terms of historical times in the individual’s ontogeny. Both are 2 The concept of propensity probabilities reifies observed properties into causal entities of an unkown and undefined kind. As such, the explanatory strategy is inherently circular and faces the same challenges as those elaborated for trait psychology in this article. Integr Psych Behav (2013) 47:1–55 7 essential to define individual-specificity. For example, the 10 min per hour probabil- ity could have been determined from 20 h of regular observation obtained across a time period of 2 weeks; the ontogenetic stage of the individual is specified by an age of 5 years, which can be categorised as early childhood. This new probability type enables ratio-scaled quantifications that are essential for quantitative comparisons across time and situational contexts and between individuals, groups, populations, and species (Uher, Meta-theoretical foundations of objectivity versus subjectivity in quantifications of behaviour and personality; An integrative meta-theoretical framework for research on individual behaviour in context—situa- tions, populations, species, both unpublished). It also allows for the merging of
  • 73. concepts of averages across occasions, as studied in personality research, with concepts of ranges, especially maxima, as studied in intelligence and achievement research3 (Ackerman 1994), and concepts of variability in terms of differences between consec- utive occasions (i.e., fluctuations) as studied in physiological and behavioural research (De Weerth et al. 1999). For example, the child’s time-relative probability could be on average 10 min per hour, ranging from 5 to 20 min per hour on a daily basis; the magnitude of day-to-day variability (for comparable occasions across the days) as indicated by the coefficient of variation could be CV=0.5. This coefficient specifies the standard deviation standardised by the mean (because the standard deviation is sensitive to the sample mean) to allow for comparisons between different samples (for an overview of methods of analyses of variability, see van Geert and van Dijk 2002). Such comprehensive analyses of patterns are important because within-individual var- iability in psychological and behavioural phenomena is pronounced and bears theoret- ical and empirical importance. For example, within-individual variability can indicate ongoing processes and can reflect phenomena that are important for explaining changes and development (van Geert and van Dijk 2002, p. 344). Moreover, within-individual variability often substantially exceeds between-individual variability (Shweder and Sullivan 1990; Uher, An integrative meta-theoretical framework
  • 74. for research on indi- vidual behaviour in context—situations, populations, species, unpublished). Differential Patterns Time-relative probabilities that characterise all individuals in the same way cannot be individual-specific. If all children in the observed group have the same time-relative probability of self-playing, then these probabilities cannot characterise any one of them individually. Time-relative probabilities can reflect individual uniqueness only if they deviate from those of other individuals of a particular reference group of interest (e.g., a social group, culture, or species)—that is, if they are differential. This is the case if the determined time-relative probabilities differ between children such that their individual averages vary interindividually, for example, between 2 min and 30 min per hour, their individual ranges vary, for example, between 15 min and 50 min, and their individual within-individual varia- bilities vary, for example, between CV=0.3 and 1.3. The concept of time-relative probabilities presupposes that the number of occa- sions and periods of time considered for the probability estimations are comparable 3 Personality research is often distinguished from intelligence research in American psychology. In European traditions (e.g., Cattell, Eysenck, Stern, Pawlik), intelligence and achievement are considered inherent parts of an individual’s personality.
  • 75. 8 Integr Psych Behav (2013) 47:1–55 between individuals. This is important when considering the effects of aggregation on the reliability of probability estimations (cf. Spearman 1910) and when taking into account the fact that the individuals’ probabilities themselves may gradually change over time (i.e., develop). In the given example, this means that the time-relative probabilities of the other children should also be estimated based on 20 h of observation across a time span of 2 weeks. These children should also be of similar age in order to disentangle differences between individuals from differences between age groups. Furthermore, the situational contexts in which different individuals are studied should be comparable between them because the average time-relative probabilities of particular groups of individuals may generally shift across different situations (Uher, An integrative meta-theoretical framework for research on individ- ual behaviour in context—situations, populations, species, unpublished.) Analyses of differentiality therefore require statistical standardisation of time-relative probabilities within the time periods and within the situational contexts in which they are deter- mined (Uher 2011a). Statistical standardisation removes the information on absolute time-relative probability scores, however, because the data are
  • 76. converted into relative, but still ratio-scaled data. Yet, the absolute scores can always be traced when needed for interpretation and for later comparisons with other samples or with future inves- tigations of the same sample. Temporal Patterns Differential patterns in time-relative probabilities that change rapidly cannot characterise an individual’s uniqueness. To reflect individual- specificity, they must be stable across time periods that are longer than those in which the probabilities were first ascertained. As with the analyses of differential patterns, the analyses of temporal patterns presuppose that probability estimations are based on numbers of occasions and periods of time that are comparable between the individuals being studied and between the time periods being contrasted. In the above example, this means that the differential patterns in the children’s time-relative probabilities for self-play should be similar when estimated again some time later in a second period of 2 weeks. Temporal stability is an essential prerequisite for justifying interpretations of the obtained standardised aggregate scores as reflecting individual-specific patterns (Uher 2011a). Temporal stability can be analysed for differential patterns in the individuals’ averages, ranges, and variabilities. For example, it is quite possible that individuals with similar differential scores in their average probabilities
  • 77. will differ from one another in stable ways with regard to their ranges and patterns of within-individual variability. The magnitude of temporal stability considered meaningful to construct individual-specificity is necessarily a matter of convention that can and must be explicitly defined. It depends on the ontogenetic stages of the individuals under study as well as on their species-specific life expectancy (Uher 2009). It also depends on the phenomena being studied and the meta-theory used. For example, in biology, weak test-retest correlations of individual differences in behaviour across just a few days or weeks of r=.20 are considered ecologically and evolutionarily meaningful (Sih and Bell 2008), whereas in human psychology, such weak correlations are considered indicators of unreliable measurements of individual-specific patterns (Uher 2011a). In conclusion, for fluctuating and dynamic phenomena such as those of the psyche and behaviour, individual-specificity refers to differential patterns in time-relative Integr Psych Behav (2013) 47:1–55 9 probabilities that are stable in ways that are considered to be meaningful. This concept fits well into Brunswik’s (1955) probabilistic theory for functional psychol- ogy. Brunswik ascertained that although the environment may
  • 78. be lawful in terms of physical principles, to the individual, “it presents itself as semi- erratic … therefore all functional psychology is inherently probabilistic” (Brunswik 1955, p. 193). An empirical application of this meta-theoretical concept of individual-specificity to behaviour was demonstrated in a study on capuchin monkeys based on 141 con- textualised behavioural variables (Uher et al., Contextualised behavioural measure- ments of personality differences obtained in behavioural tests and social observations in adult capuchin monkeys (Cebus apella), unpublished). Individual-specific patterns that have been identified in these ways can be subse- quently studied across longer periods of time to explore patterns in their gradual change and ontogenetic development (Uher 2011a). This presupposes again that the occasions and time spans as well as the historical times that are studied should be specified and should be comparable between the individuals under study. The concept of time-relative probabilities allows for the close examination of individ- ual cases in specified situations. By studying many individuals in this way, it also allows for the study of lawful patterns at the sample level. The former type of study is commonly referred to as ideographic; the latter one as nomothetic. But ideographic approaches in terms of single case studies in and of themselves need not be informative about particular