SlideShare a Scribd company logo
1 of 51
STAT4610 Project 5
“So You Want to be an Entrepreneur?”
INTRODUCTION
Starting a new business can be an exciting adventure! Who
doesn’t want to find themselves at the top of the next Microsoft,
Apple, Facebook, Myspace, Instagram, Beanie Babies, Pet
Rocks, Roto-Rooter, Enron, WorldCom, (OK, you get the
point!)?
The problem with starting a new business is it carries an
inherent amount of risk. (OK, a considerable amount of risk!)
One-third of businesses fail within the first two years, and over
half are gone in five years. That’s a waste of a significant
amount of toil, sweat, intellectual effort, creativity, and—oh
yes—capital! Failed businesses have stripped entrepreneurs of
their life savings (and the savings of others) for millennia, but
the successes have also lined the pockets of a lot of savvy
investors.
Since most people can’t afford to fund their own startups, a
popular strategy involves convincing others to get behind your
great idea. Venture capital funds are always willing to chase
the next big thing, but unfortunately there is a limit to the
patience of the world’s check-writers, and most of them didn’t
get rich without recognizing when it is time to cut losses.
There is hope, however! Analytics can help every new business
set realistic expectations for their investors, and in fact are
essential in the formulation of a business plan. Because of the
uncertainties involved with startups (in terms of costs,
timelines, returns, rates of success, etc.) it is often difficult to
forecast the progression of a new venture, but with a sufficient
knowledge of the field and the tasks required, a simulation can
frequently get an entrepreneur into the right ballpark.
Sometimes this is enough to start the money flowing!
PROJECT OVERVIEW
In this project you will develop a business plan for an Angel
Investor in order to secure funding for your new startup. You
will require funding until the point that your company generates
a profit and is actually able to return capital (or positive equity)
to its investors. Expenses may include payroll for developers or
designers, facilities and infrastructure, marketing and sales,
purchasing or collecting data, research, and a variety of other
costs that you may want to identify. Obviously, you will have to
identify a viable product around which to build your company,
and if you’re stuck without an idea, feel free to go back to your
Gateway Course and build a business around app development.
Finally, you will need to determine the cost of executing your
business plan across this timeline in order to secure sufficient
funding.
[As an aside, the particular product or service is not essential to
this project—you may use something developed in another
course, some cool idea that you might have in mind, or
something entirely fictional.]
You may make any assumptions you want concerning the
availability of personnel (whether you will be required to hire
them or if they are already onboard as part of your venture) and
you will need to calculate the rates required to retain their
services (market rates for most skill sets are readily available
from a variety of sources).
PROJECT REQUIREMENTS
1. Develop a timeline (flowchart) of the tasks that need to be
accomplished in order to make your company profitable. Build
in as much efficiency as possible by completing tasks in
parallel.
2. Identify appropriate distributions for all of the tasks on your
timeline, to include sales and the potential for them to generate
revenue.
3. Build an Excel-based simulation of your timeline and create
a histogram of possible durations for the company to become a
success.
4. Apply the “Clint Eastwood” rule to your simulation output
(“How lucky do you feel?”) Remember: being too aggressive
can get you burnt if you don’t achieve your goals, but being too
conservative in your estimate and asking for too much can
result in your proposal getting turned down.
5. Generate Cost and Revenue curves for your timeline from
step 4.
You will have two deliverables for the project, which are due
together as documents in Canvas at the beginning of class,
Lesson 19.
DELIVERABLE 1. A business plan proposal and funding
request for your Angel Investor. This should be brief, and
should focus on specifics and results. In particular, specify how
much money will be required to fund this venture, how long it
will take to generate a profit, and how much return can be
expected on the initial investment over time.
[This should be organized to be presented to an Angel Investor.]
DELIVERABLE 2. A summary of the simulation you used to
support your decisions. This should include a flowchart of
tasks, each distribution you used in your simulation, a summary
of the simulation output (to include time to each critical point),
the decisions you make from your output in terms of time to
completion, and the cost and revenue analysis associated with
the timeline.
[This should be organized to be presented to a business partner
(or a Professor).]
critically analyze the article attached and post your article
analysis to the discussion forum. The original post content
should be comprehensive, accurate, and persuasive. Major
points should be clearly stated and well supported based upon
knowledge gained throughout the program of study. You should
not summarize the article but instead, point out areas of
agreement with the article information based upon your
knowledge of the content, areas of weakness and/or
disagreement with the author(s) positions and overall analysis
of the content under discussion.
Supporting research should be appropriate for academic
discussion and address related concepts. As emerging
academics, this is your opportunity to critically evaluate
published research. You should also raise questions and seek
clarifications from your classmates.
Academy of Strategic Management Journal
Volume 16, Number 1, 2017
244
CAN MANAGERS BE REALLY OBJECTIVE?
BIAS IN MULTICRITERIA DECISION ANALYSIS
Silvio Borrero, Universidad Icesi
Felipe Henao, Universidad Icesi
ABSTRACT
Strategic management frequently requires making decisions in
complex situations.
Strategic decision-making is widely assumed to be an objective
process supported by rational
analytical tools. A strategic decision, however, involves both a
structured part, which can be dealt
with using different analytical tools, and an unstructured part,
which must be dealt with by means
of the decision maker’s judgment, intuition and experience.
Therefore, individual leanings are
inherent to complex strategic decisions. To make such
decisions, the human brain appeals to
diverse heuristics or shortcuts to analyze and simplify
contextual information. These mental
strategies are not infallible; on the contrary, there is abundant
evidence that humans’ minds are
prone to cognitive biases or traps that cloud objectivity when
making decisions. In a managerial
context, such biases or traps can result in suboptimal or
inefficient decisions that undermine
organizational value. It is not surprising, therefore, that there
are so many tools and techniques
devised to make (arguably) objective management decisions,
based on evidence. Among other
disciplines that facilitate decision making and thus reduce
cognitive biases, operations research
(OR) offers multiple analytical methodologies and procedures,
such as Multi-Criteria Decision
Analysis (MCDA). However, the relationship between cognitive
biases and these decision-making
tools has not been amply investigated. Empirical evidence on
how individual differences and
cognitive factors influence the effectiveness of these tools in
practice is still lacking, and it is
uncertain whether they contribute effectively to reducing
cognitive biases, or if, on the contrary,
cognitive biases interfere with the tools’ effectiveness. This
paper presents an experimental study
involving undergraduate and graduate students where their
individual differences, as measured
by their hedonic or utilitarian leanings, are linked to two
motivational biases, respectively
known as confirmation and desirability of choice, and how these
biases influence a decision made
through MCDA techniques. Results suggest that experimental
subjects employ MCDA to confirm
previously conceived decisions, rather than using the tool to
explore the problem situation, make
sense of their preferences and choose the most appropriate
course of action accordingly. This is,
it appears that subjects make use of the OR tool to support a
decision they have already made in
their own minds. Consequently, the effectiveness of MCDA and
other strategic or decision-making
tools might be affected by individual differences and
corresponding motivational biases. These
results also suggest that there is a very fine division between
valid, preference-based decisions
and biased preconceptions. In particular, these experimental
results question the effectiveness of
multicriteria analysis tools; more generally, this research
suggests that a totally rational decision
making process—and therefore a completely objective,
analytical, tool-supported, evidence-based
management—might be a utopic endeavor.
Keywords: Strategic management, decision-making, individual
differences, motivational
biases, Multi-Criteria Decision Analysis.
mailto:[email protected]
Academy of Strategic Management Journal
Volume 16, Number 1, 2017
245
INTRODUCTION
Strategic decision-making is often deemed as an objective
exercise, especially when it is
supported by—arguably—rational analytical tools. Classic
rationality and bounded rationality
paradigms assume that top managers approach strategic
decision-making by means of a purposive,
systematic and comprehensive process (Simon, 1955, 1979). Per
this process, managers state
concrete objectives, collect relevant information, develop viable
alternatives, and identify the
optimal course of action, thus maximizing utility. Throughout
the past few decades, numerous
techniques have been developed to facilitate decision making,
including classic tools such as the
strengths-weaknesses-opportunities-threats (SWOT), the
political-economic-social-technological-
environmental-legal (PESTEL), the external factor evaluation
(EFE) or the internal factor
evaluation (IFE) matrices, or more recent extensions, such as
the internal competitive profile
(ICPM), the external competitive profile (ECPM) or the
financial competitive profile (FCPM)
matrices (Capps III & Cassidy, 2016). The introduction and
development of decision support
systems (DSS) and other computer-based decision tools further
enhanced the strategic decision
making processes, and their capability to address increasingly
complex situations (Shim et al.,
2002). In general terms, most of these decision support tools are
intended to deal with the
structured parts of complex managerial problems to make
decision making more efficient.
Strategic decisions, though, involve both structured and
unstructured elements, which must be
dealt with by means of the decision maker’s judgment, intuition
and experience, in addition to any
analytical approach chosen.
It follows that individual leanings or preferences are inherent to
complex strategic
decisions. Therefore, inevitably, cognitive and motivational
biases pervasively affect managerial
decisions and influence strategic outcomes (Das & Teng, 1999).
As the exercise of strategic
management frequently requires making decisions in complex
situations, managers appeal to
diverse heuristics or shortcuts to analyze and simplify
contextual information. Since the
publication of Tversky and Kahneman’s seminal paper (1974),
there has been an increasing interest
in understanding the underlying mechanisms that explain such
heuristics or shortcuts, and the
consequent biases that may affect human behavior in
organizational decision making and strategic
planning contexts (Busenitz & Barney, 1997; Busenitz et al.,
2003). Numerous experimental
studies have been performed within the premises of different
academic fields, such as psychology,
economy, finance, marketing, and—marginally and more
recently—operations research (Franco
& Hämäläinen, 2016; Hämäläinen, Luoma, & Saarinen, 2013).
As a result, researchers have
identified an ample range of human biases that can be classified
as cognitive or motivational, and
which are capable of distorting judgment and decision-making
(Montibeller & von Winterfeldt,
2015). This is known as decision theory. Overall, it could be
argued that its most remarkable
contribution to knowledge is that is has proven that humans are
all vulnerable to fall into these
biases, leading us to make suboptimal or inefficient decisions
that violate rationality and
commonly accepted normative principles (Kahneman, 2011).
In a managerial context, these biases can be especially costly
because they could cloud
objectivity when making decisions and therefore undermine
organizational value (Frederick, 2005;
Montibeller & von Winterfeld, 2015; Montibeller & von
Winterfeldt, 2015). Although media and
popular literature tend to praise gutsy business decisions—and
the gutsy leaders who implement
them—that yield successful market or business moves (Freiberg,
2004; Keyt, 2003), it is difficult
to argue against the idea that firm strategies should be the result
of sound analyses that consider as
Academy of Strategic Management Journal
Volume 16, Number 1, 2017
246
much contextual evidence as possible before making a decision
(Kiron, Ferguson, & Prentice,
2013). Not surprisingly, business literature abounds with tools
and techniques devised to make
management a scientific exercise that eliminates or reduces
biases, and hence aim at achieving
objective business decisions (Rigby & Bilodeau, 2007, 2015).
Operations Research (OR) is one of several disciplines that
offer analytical methodologies
and procedures to facilitate decision making and strategic
planning (Stewart, French, & Rios,
2013). Although OR tools—such as Multi-Criteria Decision
Analysis (MCDA), decision tress or
influence diagrams, among others—focus on helping people
solve problems and make better
decisions under conditions of uncertainty, it appears to have
ignored the behavioral aspects of the
humans involved in its process (Hämäläinen et al., 2013). The
relationship between people’s
individual differences, their biases and decision tools has not
been amply investigated (Franco &
Hämäläinen, 2016). This is, it is unknown whether decision
making tools contribute effectively to
deal with these cognitive factors, or if, on the contrary,
cognitive factors interfere with the
effectiveness of such tools (Montibeller & von Winterfeldt,
2015). It is therefore necessary to
further develop the behavioral OR agenda (Franco &
Hämäläinen, 2016; Hämäläinen et al., 2013)
and to more effectively connect decision making tools with the
managerial reality. This paper
contributes to this need by reporting the design of an
experimental study that links individual
differences, as measured by personal leanings towards hedonic
or utilitarian preferences, with two
motivational biases, known as confirmation and desirability of
choice. More specifically, this study
explores how such personal preferences and biases influence
decisions made supported by MCDA.
In what follows, we first briefly discuss from a theoretical point
of view the known effects
that individual differences and motivational biases have on
decision-making, with particular detail
on confirmation and desirability of choice biases. Then, the
hypotheses for this study and our
experimental approach to evaluating the effects of these two
motivational biases on a MCDA
problem are discussed in detail. We present some results that
suggest that experimental subjects
(undergraduate and graduate students) utilize MCDA to confirm
previously conceived decisions,
rather than to identify the best possible solution for them.
Finally, we finish the paper by
summarizing our main arguments, drawing some conclusions
and presenting opportunities for
extending our research.
ANTECEDENTS
For decades, the strategic and organizational literature has
attempted to model the
managers’ decision-making processes and to propose analytical
approaches to make better
strategic decisions. In an attempt to rationalize and objectivize
decision making, and to optimize
managerial decisions, OR and other disciplines have developed
analytical methodologies and
decision support systems (Shim et al., 2002; Stewart et al.,
2013). Most mid-size and large
companies utilize such methods and systems, in varied settings,
and there is abundant research
on practical applications of decision support tools. The evidence
is relatively scarce, however, on
the potentially undermining effect of the human factor on the
effectiveness of these tools. Extant
literature tends to investigate and describe the firm’s or its
leader’s strategic orientation, as
evidenced by the observed actions (Pleshko & Nickerson, 2008),
or to explore external factors that
affect decision making in particular contexts (Akyürek,
Sawalha, & Ide, 2015), rather than to
understand the strategic decisions as a function of the
manager’s motivational or cognitive
preferences. A manager’s approach to a complex decision will
strongly depend on specific
individual differences, which inevitably affects the decision
process’ outcome. Strategic
Academy of Strategic Management Journal
Volume 16, Number 1, 2017
247
management could certainly benefit from a more thorough
understanding of the personal factors
that affect strategic decisions, and, more specifically, from
context-depending, prescriptive
recommendations aimed at improving the effectiveness of
strategic analytical tools. Such
understanding requires a cross-disciplinary approach that
combines a strategic perspective with
contributions from other fields (Ronda-Pupo, 2015), such as
social psychology, organizational
behavior (OB) or operations research.
Social psychology and OB, for instance, have investigated the
underlying mechanisms that
explain human decisions. Appealing to strategies that facilitate
decision-making processes is
inherent to the human nature. Rather than approaching a novel
problem by means of sequential,
systematic or structured mental processes, the human brain
tends to use heuristics or shortcuts to
analyze contextual information and make decisions (Kahneman,
2011; Kahneman & Tversky,
1979; Tversky & Kahneman, 1974, 1981, 1986). These
facilitating strategies result in more
“efficient” decision processes, especially in complex situations
(Frederick, 2005; Witteman, van
den Bercken, Claes, & Godoy, 2009). However, such facilitating
strategies are not infallible, and
there is abundant evidence that indicates that humans’ minds are
prone to biases or traps that cloud
objectivity when making decisions (Frederick, 2005; Kahneman,
2011).
Within the context of decision and risk analysis, for instance,
Montibeller and von
Winterfeldt discuss the effects of cognitive and motivational
biases and suggest possible ways to
counteract them (2015). A cognitive bias is defined as the
systematic discrepancy between the
actual answer given by a person to a particular judgmental task
and what would be considered the
correct answer after applying a formal normative rule.
Motivational biases, on the other hand, are
more about the influence of people’s desirability or
undesirability towards an event, consequence,
outcome, or choice, in a particular judgmental task. Although
both types of biases, cognitive or
motivational, are likely to cloud objectivity when making
decisions, this study focuses on the latter,
given its prevalence and therefore likelihood to occur in
business or strategic contexts.
Confirmation bias is one of such motivational biases. It is
conceptualized as a mental
shortcut that simplifies complex analyses and arduous
inferential tasks by appealing to strongly
grounded beliefs (Friedrich, 1993; MacCoun, 1998; Wason,
1960). Such beliefs thus serve as a
heuristic that makes evaluation of new information more
expedite and efficient. With a caveat;
because people assume that their existing beliefs are true, the
confirmation bias often results in
poor decisions, given that extant evidence is not considered
thoroughly or in a balanced manner
(Hernandez & Preston, 2013). On the other hand, desirability of
choice, another motivational bias,
occurs when a person desires a particular decision option, and,
consequently, overestimates or
underestimates relevant parameters in order to favor the desired
option.
In a business or strategic context, a person’s bias might result in
decisions which reflect
conscious or subconscious individual preferences (Kahneman,
Lovallo, & Sibony, 2011), rather
than seeking value maximization for the firm. Previous
research, for instance, has explored the
influence of CEOs’ and upper echelons’ preferences—especially
as related to demographic
characteristics—in shaping corporate strategy (Jensen & Zajac,
2004). The marketing literature
provides an interesting framework to better grasp the potential
influence of personal preferences
on business decisions. Indeed, in consumer behavior settings,
there is ample research on the
dichotomy between hedonic and utilitarian goals, which affect
consumers’ perceived value and
hence their purchase decisions (Bridges & Florsheim, 2008;
Jones, Reynolds, & Arnold, 2006;
Overby & Lee, 2006; Sweeney & Soutar, 2001; Ulaga &
Chacour, 2001). There is abundant
evidence that the type of personal goal, hedonic or utilitarian,
has a significant impact on a person’s
purchase decisions. In addition, it is believed that a person’s
motivational orientation for either
Academy of Strategic Management Journal
Volume 16, Number 1, 2017
248
hedonic or utilitarian goals tends to be quite stable through
time. Therefore, it is reasonable to
expect people to favor decision options that match their hedonic
or utilitarian preferences, and
therefore will privilege evidence supporting such motivational
orientation. Moreover,
extrapolating organizational behavior findings on the influence
of personal orientations on
organizational decisions (Polman, 2012), it can be argued that
personal preferences—and hence
biases—will influence not only consumption decisions, but also
decisions in other business or
strategic contexts.
The influence of individual preferences and personal biases on
people’s decisions has
recently captured the attention of OR literature. For instance, it
has been argued that cognitive and
motivational biases (Franco, Rouwette, & Korzilius, 2016), as
well as people’s individual
differences (Franco & Hämäläinen, 2016; Hämäläinen et al.,
2013; Montibeller & von Winterfeldt,
2015), can influence the effectiveness and practical value of
arguably rational decision making
tools. Consequently, individual differences and biases might be
affecting the quality of the decision
modelling process, the transparency of the resulting analyses,
and therefore the effectiveness of
the final decisions made.
There is not much evidence, though, on the undermining effects
of personal biases on
strategic decisions supported by analytical tools, especially in
actual organizational settings. To
assess the potential influence of personal leanings on the
quality of a decision, and, more generally,
on the effectiveness of strategic decisions, in this paper we
study the relationship between
individual differences regarding hedonic or utilitarian
preferences and confirmation and
desirability of choice biases, and the resulting effect on
decisions made with a multicriteria
decision analysis tool.
HYPOTHESES
Building on the aforementioned antecedents and our revision of
previous research on
decision making, multicriteria analysis, and cognitive biases,
we contend that MCDA is sensitive
to both, confirmation and desirability biases. This is, when
considering diverse options; people
will tend to choose the option that matches their personal
preferences, regardless of the decision
technique or tool used. Moreover, we argue that such personal
desire is greatly determined by
the person’s motivational orientation, whether hedonic or
utilitarian. Therefore,
H1 (Motivational bias) There is a positive relationship between
a person’s motivational orientation
and this person’s decision.
Furthermore, people who have a strong personal desire for a
particular option will
consider only those pieces of information that corroborate why
the option should be chosen
(confirmation bias) and will assign more priority to criteria that
further favor that option
(desirability of choice). Therefore,
H2 (Confirmation bias) to make a decision, a person with a
hedonic orientation will consider
experiential criteria rather than performance criteria, whereas a
person with a utilitarian
orientation will consider performance criteria rather than
experiential criteria.
H3 (Desirability of choice bias) To make a decision, a person
with a hedonic orientation will assign
higher priority to experiential criteria, relative to performance
criteria, whereas a person with a
utilitarian orientation will consider performance criteria rather
than experiential criteria.
Academy of Strategic Management Journal
Volume 16, Number 1, 2017
249
Finally, given previous findings on the positive relationship
between desirability of an
outcome and selection of relevant information, we anticipate
that both biases, confirmation and
desirability of choice reinforce each other and jointly influence
people’s decisions. Therefore,
H4 (Motivational interaction) the interaction between criteria
considered and relative priority
assigned to criteria, upon making a decision, will strengthen the
positive relationship between a
person’s motivational orientation and this person’s decision
Figure 1 illustrates these hypotheses as components of a single,
integrated model.
Figure-1
HYPOTHESES OF THE EXPERIMENTAL STUDY
METHODOLOGY
To study the effects of the confirmation and desirability of
choice biases on an MCDA-
supported decision, we designed an experimental intervention
where undergraduate and graduate
students, acting as the decision-makers of a simple decision
problem, employ MCDA to support
their decision. The experiment looks at the effects that both
biases have on the decision making
process and the actual decision made. We designed our
experiment as a vehicle purchase decision,
under the assumption that undergraduate and graduate students
would easily relate to such a
familiar decision context. We infer that confirmation and
desirability of choice biases will
positively relate to a person’s orientation towards hedonism or
utilitarianism and to their final
purchase decision.
Participants
Participants included undergraduate (N=43) and graduate
students (N=72) at a private
university in Colombia. We specifically chose a class on OR
techniques because of its relevance
to the theses tested. The experiment was conducted during the
multicriteria analysis module,
embedded in one of the author’s regular classes. Participants
were recruited at the beginning of
the class using a verbal and written information sheet that told
them about their right not to
participate in the experiment. To fine-tune our experimental
design, which is described below,
we conducted three early pilot versions of the experiment using
undergraduate (N=35) master
students (N=45) during the previous semester. The
questionnaires and tools and manipulation
were thus assessed to come up with the final version for the
experiment with graduate students. All
questionnaires to measure the variables were administered using
on-line Qualtrics software
before and during the class time.
Motivational orientation
Criteria considered
H4: confir
Criteria priority
H1: motivational bias
mation x desirability (+) Decision
Academy of Strategic Management Journal
Volume 16, Number 1, 2017
250
Experimental design
We contrasted individual motivational orientation,
confirmation bias and desirability of
choice bias with hypothetical purchase decisions. The complete
experiment consisted on three
subsequent stages that asked them to make simulated purchase
decisions, following a class
lecture on multicriteria analysis. To analyze our results,
descriptive statistics, pairwise
correlations and analysis of variance (ANOVA) were conducted
using SAS statistical software.
Note that common claims on the variability and effectiveness of
MCDA results are related
to its use either for individual or for group decision making
(Schafer & Gallemore, 2016). This is,
the use of MCDA and similar iterative methods in groups allow
participants to mutually compare
decisions, engage in dialogue, and discuss choices or weights,
on a one-on-one or public manner.
As a result, there is great potential for cross-contamination and
distortion, and people might end
up deciding on behalf of others’ preferences or biases, which
makes it difficult to make
generalizable or standardized inferences. To control for this
issue, respondents read instructions,
analyzed data, and made their decisions in individual
computers. Also, to minimize interaction
between participants during the actual experiment, they were
instructed to not talk with their
classmates and the instructor lead them through successive
stages in an orderly sequence, and with
a reasonably limited time for each stage.
Stage 1
Following a lecture and corresponding material on MCDA and
decision making,
participants were asked to complete a questionnaire that
assessed their motivational orientation.
Then, they were given broad and limited descriptive
specifications of the four purchase decision
options and were asked to choose the one that they found more
appealing (Decision 1).
Motivational orientation (Predictor)
Individual motivational orientation towards either hedonism or
utilitarianism was
assessed using the PERVAL 19-item scale developed to measure
consumer perceived value
(Sweeney & Soutar, 2001). Thus, participants’ were categorized
according to their individual
leanings on four distinct value dimensions: emotional, social,
quality/performance and
price/value for money. For our study, we termed
quality/performance and price/value for money
as utilitarian motives and emotional and social motives as
hedonic motives. The participants
completed the PERVAL questionnaire before the experiment
started.
Decision (Criterion)
Participants’ decisions 1-3 were measured using custom
questionnaires that asked them
to choose between a limited set of purchase options, in a
hypothetical car purchase decision. For
their first decision (Decision1), four distinct car descriptions
similar in price range and type of
vehicle (respectively labeled A, B, C and D), were presented to
the participants along with made-
up specifications relevant to the purchase decision (see Table
1). Each vehicle option’s
specifications were designed to emphasize either performance
(Car A) or user-experience (Car B)
or to present a balance between performance and user-
experience attributes (Cars C and D).
Academy of Strategic Management Journal
Volume 16, Number 1, 2017
251
Table-1
EXPERIMENT STAGE 1: BROAD SPECIFICATIONS GIVEN
FOR
DECISION 1
Car A: Sophisticated style, elegant exterior design,
luxury interior finishes, very good audio system
and highly pleasurable driving.
Car B: Maximum factory warranty, very powerful
engine, optimal price-power, and maximum safety for
passengers.
Car C: Powerful engine, luxury interior finishes,
extended factory warranty, driving pleasure and high
security for passengers.
Car D: Minimum fuel consumption, modern exterior
design, good price-power, upgraded audio system and
extended factory warranty.
Stage 2
Participants were given more detailed, qualitative and
quantitative, specifications of the
four purchase decision options and were asked to choose the one
that sounded more appealing to
them (Decision 2). The information was presented in the form
of a pay-off matrix where
performance-related criteria was listed as factory-catalogue
specs and experience-related criteria
was presented as consumers’ self-reported assessment of their
consumption/driving experience
(Table 2).
Criteria Used (Predictor)
The specific evidence used by each participant to make a
decision was used as a proxy
for an individual’s confirmation bias towards performance or
experience. To measure this
variable, participants were asked during stage 2 to highlight
what specific data cells they had
inspected and used to make their second decision. This was
done by means of a custom tool
based on the made-up specs sheet shown in Table 2. With this
tool, respondents indicated which
criteria they used by clicking on the appropriate cells on the
table.This helps us to identify if
participants systematically overlooked or neglected parts of the
information given in the matrix.
Stage 3
Participants were asked to assign weight factors to the
problem’s criteria by following the
Swing weight method protocol (Goodwin & Wright, 2004) and
then to introduce them into the
HiView3 software (www.catalyze.co.uk) so they would be able
to play with the problem’s data,
which was already preset in the software. Thus, participants
were asked to enter the weight factors
into the software, look at the graphs generated and make a third
and final choice (Decision 3).
Criteria priority (predictor)
The relative weights assigned by each participant were used as a
proxy for an individual’s
desirability of choice bias towards performance or experience.
This was done by means of a
custom tool based on the same made-up specs sheet shown in
Table 2. This allows us not only to
identify if participants learned something from their interplay
with the OR tool that made them
change their minds, but also see if participants systematically
allocated larger weight factors to
the criteria that favored the decision option they had in mind
from the start.
http://www.catalyze.co.uk/
Academy of Strategic Management Journal
Volume 16, Number 1, 2017
252
Table-2
EXPERIMENT STAGE 2: DETAILED SPECIFICATIONS
GIVEN FOR
DECISION 2
Performance criteria
(source: factory-catalogue)
Experience criteria (source: consumers’ self-
reported assessment of driving experience)
Decisio
n
options
Pric
e ($
COP
)
Powe
r
(HP)
Price
/
torqu
e ($
mill)
Warran
ty
(Years
)
Airba
gs
(#)
Fuel
consumpti
on
(km/gal)
Extern
al
desig
n
Luxur
y
interi
or
finish
Audi
o
syste
m
Highly
pleasura
ble
driving
Apparen
t social
status
Car A 63 200 $3.800 3 2 32 Elegant Luxury Full 90 sofisticat
ed upper
class
Car B 61 300 $3.500 5 6 26 Basic Standard Basic 50 upper
middle
class
Car C 65 300 $4.000 5 5 30 Standar
d
Luxury Full 70 upper
middle
class
Car D 59 160 $3.700 4 3 36 Modern Standard Hi-
Tech
60 lower
middle
class
RESULTS
A total of 43 undergraduate students and 72 graduate students
completed both the
individual difference questionnaire that assessed their
motivational orientation and the subsequent
surveys that asked them to make a decision based on either
experience or performance criteria.
The motivational orientation 19-item questionnaire, as well as
the custom instruments applied to
test the desirability of choice hypothesis (weights assigned to
prioritize criteria), showed good
internal reliability, with Cronbach’s alpha standardized values
of .872 and .723, respectively. Table
3 presents the descriptive statistics and pair-wise, Pearson-
product correlations between the
measured variables.
Table-3
DESCRIPTIVE STATISTICS AND PAIR-WISE
CORRELATIONS
Mean SD 1 2 3 4 5 6 7
1. Motivational
1.273 0.844 1
2. Decision 1 -
0.508 .259** 1
3. Decision 2 -
0.549 .106 .441*** 1
4. Decision 3 -
0.542 .174 .464*** .697*** 1
5. Criteria considered -
0.429 .398*** .445*** .520*** .528*** 1
6. Criteria priority 0.024 0.256 .328*** .465*** .604***
.684*** .590*** 1
7. Criteria favored 0.029 2.526 .175 .281** .395*** .502***
.438*** .425*** 1
Valid N (list) = 103; * p-value < 0.05 (2-tailed); ** p-value <
0.01 (2-tailed); *** p-value < 0.001
(2-tailed)
To illustrate how successive decisions maintained (or not) the
first decision made by the
Academy of Strategic Management Journal
Volume 16, Number 1, 2017
253
respondents, and how such decisions compared to the
respondent’s motivational orientation, we
plotted the graph shown in Figure 2. In total, 60% of the
participants adhered to their first decision
on subsequent decisions 2 and 3; 29% changed their decision
once, and 14% changed twice. Also,
as illustrated by the columns in Figure 2, respondents with a
hedonic orientation leaned more
towards an experience-based decision than respondents with a
utilitarian orientation, across all
three decisions.
Figure-2
SEQUENCE OF DECISIONS 1-3, CATEGORIZED BY
INDIVIDUAL MOTIVATIONAL
ORIENTATION (HEDONIC VS. UTILITARIAN) AND FIRST
DECISION MADE
(PERFORMANCE, NEUTRAL OR EXPERIENCE).
1
Utilitarian orientation
Performance option
Hedonistic orientation
0
Decisions 1
Performance
Decision 1
Neutral
Experiance option Decision 1 Decision 2
Decision 3
Decision 1
-1
Experience
To further assess the hypothesized biases, in Figure 3 we
analyzed the criteria used by
respondents, categorized by their motivational orientation, when
making their decision using the
made-up specifications data. Criteria considered refers to the
criteria selected to make their
decisions from the made-up specs table; criteria order refers to
the order assigned to the selected
criteria by the respondents; and criteria priority refers to the
relative weights assigned by the
respondents to all available criteria in the specs table.
Figure-3
EVIDENCE OF COGNITIVE BIAS, CATEGORIZED BY
INDIVIDUAL MOTIVATIONAL
ORIENTATION (HEDONIC VS. UTILITARIAN) AND FIRST
DECISION MADE
(PERFORMANCE, NEUTRAL OR EXPERIENCE).
2
Utilitarian orientation
Performance Criteria
1
Hedonistic orientation
0
Decision 1 performance
Experience Criteria 1
Decision 1 neutral
-2 Decision 1 Experience
Criteria Considered
Criteria order Criteria Priority
Academy of Strategic Management Journal
Volume 16, Number 1, 2017
254
To further test our hypotheses, including our interaction
hypothesis (H4), we conducted an
analysis of variance (ANOVA) regressing motivational
orientation, criteria used and criteria
priority, as independent variables, on the mean of decisions
made, as the dependent variable. That
is, we propose that the relationship between individual
differences, biases and final decision can
be expressed by the equation in (1):
DECISION = ����0 + ����1�������� +
����3�������� + ����4�������� +
����5(�������� ∗ ��������) + ����
(1)
Where MO is motivational orientation (H1), CC is the criteria
considered (H2), CP is the
criteria priority (H3) and CC*CP is the interaction between both
biases (H4).
Table-4
ANOVA - GLM PROCEDURE
Class-level information
Class Levels Values
Criterion priority 2 -1.00, 1.00
Observations read 117
Onbservations used 107
Dependent variable: Decision
Source DF Sum of squares Mean square F-value Pr > F
Model 4 10.7224 2.6806 25.8400 <.0001
Error 102 10.5798 0.1037
Corrected Total 106 21.3022
R-squared Var coeff MSE root Decision mean
0.5033 -240.4226 0.3221 -0.1340
Source DF Type I SS Mean square F-value Pr > F
Motivational orientation 1 0.8945 0.8945 8.6200 0.0041
Criteria priority 1 6.3341 6.3341 61.0700 <.0001
z-criteria considered 1 2.9184 2.9184 28.1400 <.0001
Crit priority*z-crit cons 1 0.5753 0.5753 5.5500 0.0204
Source DF Type III SS Mean square F-value Pr > F
Motivational orientation 1 0.0518 0.0518 0.5000 0.4814
Criteria priority 1 2.6876 2.6876 25.9100 <.0001
z-criteria considered 1 1.9147 1.9147 18.4600 <.0001
Crit priority*z-crit cons 1 0.5753 0.5753 5.5500 0.0204
Minimum mean suqares at z-criteria considered = -1 SD
Criteria priority Decision LS mean H0: LSmean1=LSmean2
t value Pr > |t|
-1 -0.5334 -4.8300 <.0001
1 0.0071
Minimum mean suqares at z-criteria considered = +1 SD
Criteria priority Decision LS mean H0: LSmean1=LSmean2
t value Pr > |t|
-1 -0.01667 -2.02 0.0459
1 0.17353
A dichotomous dummy variable was computed for each
individual decision depending on
the type of choice made (performance choice = 1, experience
choice = -1), and the resulting values
were then averaged to obtain an estimate of the respondents’
mean decisions (i.e., their individual
Academy of Strategic Management Journal
Volume 16, Number 1, 2017
255
leanings towards a performance or an experience option). MO,
as measured by the PERVAL 19-
item scale, is a continuous measure. CC was estimated by
adding the total number of performance-
type criteria selected and subtracting the total experience-type
criteria selected. Similarly, CP was
estimated by adding total weight assigned to performance-type
criteria and subtracting total weight
assigned to experience-type criteria. To compare results at high
(performance) and low
(experience) values of criteria priority, CP was dichotomized
about its median. To plot results at
high (performance) and low (experience) values of criteria used,
a standardized score for CC was
computed as a function of standard deviations about 0 (zCC).
Table 4 presents the ANOVA results.
From the ANOVA output, Figure 4 plots the dependent variable
at zCC = -1 SD for
experience criteria and zCC = +1 SD for performance criteria
(i.e., standardized criteria considered
measure at -/+ 1 standard deviations).
Figure-4
EFFECT OF CRITERIA CONSIDERED AND CRITERIA
PRIORITY (EXPERIENCE VS.
PERFORMANCE) ON DECISION, STANDARDIZED (N=109,
P<.001 FOR CC*CP).
DISCUSSION
Overall, the results provide support for our hypotheses. As
shown in Table 3, the positive
correlations between the motivational orientation and the first,
essentially intuitive decision,
suggests that individuals tend to make decisions based on their
personal leanings, thus
supporting hypothesis 1. Likewise, the positive correlations
between subsequent decisions 1-3
provide additional support to this notion as it suggests that
people tend to adhere to the first
decision they make, across different conditions or evidence. On
the other hand, the positive
correlations between the decisions made and both the criteria
selected to make the decision and
the weights assigned to decision criteria, support the
confirmation (H2) and desirability of choice
(H3) hypotheses. This is, participants’ decisions towards either
performance or experience
options were consistent with the criteria they used to support
their decisions and the relative
importance they assigned to such criteria. Finally, the positive
correlation between criteria used
and criteria priority suggests that these two personal biases
might reinforce each other and
together determine a person’s decision. Altogether, the pairwise
correlations support the notion
that people tend to adhere to their first decision, which relates
with subjective personal
preferences, even after being confronted with additional
evidence or analytical tools.
Academy of Strategic Management Journal
Volume 16, Number 1, 2017
256
The correlations are supplemented by the graphs shown in
Figure 2 and Figure 3, which
show how participants, categorized by their first decision
(performance, neutral or experience
option), tend to maintain their initial choice. This figure also
highlight how respondents with a
hedonic orientation lean more towards an experience-based
decision than respondents with a
utilitarian orientation, across all three decisions. These results
further support hypothesis 1.
Also, as shown in Figure 3, results suggest a correspondence
between individual decisions
and both, the criteria used to make the decision and the order
assigned to each criterion used
(hypothesis 2). Also, that both the decision and the criteria
selected are directionally consistent
with the relative weight assigned to all available criteria
(hypothesis 3). This is, as illustrated by
the line graphs, respondents who initially choose a
performance-type option tend to use only
performance-based evidence and to assign a higher weight to
performance data, relative to user-
experience data. Conversely, respondents who opt for an
experience-type option at the beginning
tend to use only experience-based evidence and to assign a
higher weight to user-experience data,
relative to performance data. Results are consistent across
motivational orientation, as utilitarian
participants preferred and assigned more weight to performance
criteria, whereas hedonic
participants preferred and assigned more weight to user-
experience criteria. Although the contrast
is especially clear for criteria priority, as measured by relative
weights assigned, the results are
overall directionally consistent with our hypotheses. Taken
together, these results support
hypotheses 2 and 3.
The ANOVA yields additional support to our hypotheses, with a
significant model
(F=25.84, p<.0001), and significant effects both for the
independent variables zCC and CP (p<.001
in each case) and for their interaction (p<.05). As shown in
Table 4, the results are directionally
consistent with our basic and interaction hypotheses, as people
who choose the performance option
tend to consider and to prioritize performance criteria, rather
than user-experience criteria, and
conversely for those who choose the experience option. Also,
the different slopes and intersecting
curves suggest an interaction effect, which supports our
hypothesis 4. The difference between
people who prioritize performance criteria (CP = +1) and those
who prioritize experience criteria
(CP = -1), however, is clearly stronger for those who consider
experience-related criteria (zCC =
-1 SD) than for those who consider performance-related criteria
(zCC = +1SD), which suggests
intriguing implications. The stronger effect might suggest that
people who are prone to experiences
tend to make more intuitive decisions, compared to people
leaning towards performance.
Conversely, individuals who tend to consider or prioritize
objective, performance-based evidence,
might rely more on analytical procedures and objective evidence
in general and therefore be less
prone to cognitive biases, relative to experience-prone people
(Frederick, 2005; Witteman et al.,
2009).
CONCLUSIONS
Results suggest that decision makers tend to make intuitive
decisions consistent with their
motivational orientation, and that they are inclined to adhere to
such first intuitive decisions despite
getting access to new supporting evidence or additional
analytical tools. In fact, participants
apparently utilized MCDA to confirm previously conceived
decisions, rather than to identify the
best possible solution for them. In other words, participants
appear to have actually sought support
for a decision they have already made in their own minds, rather
than striving to attain a more
thoughtful, better quality decision.
It should be noted that MCDA is not supposed to be a normative
tool that provides a
Academy of Strategic Management Journal
Volume 16, Number 1, 2017
257
“correct” answer, but rather an insightful approach that helps
decision makers learn and make a
more informed choice. Notwithstanding, our results shed doubts
on the participants’ actual
learning from MCDA and whether the tool really added value to
the decision process, since little
learning seems to have happened throughout their interplay with
the tool. One would expect that
if any learning takes place as a result of the participants’
interaction with the MCDA tool, a
significant number of them would have changed their minds
throughout the three stages of the
experiment. However, the evidence suggests that, regardless of
the first option chosen, the number
of participants adhering to their initial impressions was larger
than those who changed their
perception along the experiment. Alternatively, even if we
accept the proposition that MCDA is
devised to reflect its users’ preferences (and help them learn
about them), it seems reasonable to
ask what distinguishes a valid preference-based decision from a
not-so-valid, biased decision.
In sum, it appears that people’s individual differences regarding
hedonic and utilitarian
motivational orientation can lead them to motivational biases,
which in turn may affect the
effectiveness of their use of the MCDA tool. More generally,
the effectiveness of strategic decision
support tools is likely affected by the managers’ leanings and
biases. Strategic decision making
should be understood as a human-machine system, composed
both by structured and unstructured
components (Shim et al., 2002). Although decision-making tools
can enhance the efficiency of
structured information processing and analysis, the unstructured
part of a strategic decision is still
subject to the managers’ leanings and preferences. Therefore,
the objectivity of many strategic
decision that rely on analytical models or tools is doubtful, at
most. This said, we do not intend
to imply that decision support tools are not useful, or even
essential, in modern strategic decision
making. We rather advocate for a grounded, cross-disciplinary
approach that better understands
and addresses such tools’ limitations. It follows that future
research should further explore the
mechanisms underlying strategic decisions supported by
analytical tools, in actual organizational
contexts, in order to propose prescriptive recommendations for
management practitioners.
Our results also suggest that motivational biases might differ
across decision makers
depending on other individual differences, beyond personal
prevailing preferences, such as their
level of rationality or intuition (Frederick, 2005; Witteman et
al., 2009). Future research is
indeed needed in this area (Franco et al., 2016), so in our future
studies we would like to explore
such nuances and try to better grasp the underlying mechanisms
that explain individual
differences, and attempt to predict which types of people are
more or less prone to motivational
biases and perhaps advance custom analytical approaches for
each category. Also, future
research should involve the exploration of strategies to
counteract or de-bias these effects.
Finally, future research should also strive to increase the
generalizability of these results.
Our findings might not hold for other contexts, given the
particular context of the experiment, with
students as respondents and a simulated purchase decision as
experimental scenario. Previous
research suggests that such an artificial setting, involving a
personal consumption choice, might
favor intuitive decisions, whereas a more professional situation,
with real-world consequences,
would motivate more analytical approaches (Sjöberg, 2003).
Future research should therefore try
to replicate and extend these findings in an organizational or
managerial setting, and involving
more strategic or business-specific decisions. Moreover, future
investigations should likely be of
a cross-disciplinary nature, such as the present paper, thus
benefiting the strategy literature with
contributions from other fields (Ronda-Pupo, 2015).
Should our results hold in more varied and generalizable
settings, it would appear that a
totally rational decision making is not feasible. Which, in turn,
would suggest that it is utopic to
expect that management and strategic planning can be
completely objective, analytical, tool-
Academy of Strategic Management Journal
Volume 16, Number 1, 2017
258
supported, evidence-based processes. A more thorough
understanding of their limitations,
however, would certainly enhance the utility and applicability
of analytical approaches, in real-
world, real-people contexts.
REFERENCES
Akyürek ÇE, Sawalha R & Ide S (2015). Factors Affecting the
Decision Making Process in Healthcare Institutions.
Academy of Strategic Management Journal, 14, 1.
Bridges E & Florsheim R (2008). Hedonic and utilitarian
shopping goals: The online experience. Journal of
Business Research 61(4), 309-314.
doi:http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jbusres.2007.06.017
Busenitz LW & Barney JB (1997). Differences between
entrepreneurs and managers in large organizations: Biases
and heuristics in strategic decision-making. Journal of business
venturing, 12(1), 9-30.
Busenitz LW, West GP, Shepherd D, Nelson T, Chandler GN &
Zacharakis A (2003). Entrepreneurship research in
emergence: Past trends and future directions. Journal of
management, 29(3), 285-308.
Capps III CJ & Cassidy CM (2016). Expanding the Competitive
Profile Matrix (Cpm): Introducing the Financial
Competitive Profile Matrix (Fcpm). Academy of Strategic
Management Journal, 15(2), 9.
Das T & Teng BS (1999). Cognitive biases and strategic
decision processes: An integrative perspective. Journal of
management studies, 36(6), 757-778.
Franco LA & Hämäläinen RP (2016). Behavioural operational
research: returning to the roots of the OR profession.
Eur J Oper Res, 249(3), 791-795.
Franco LA, Rouwette EA & Korzilius H (2016). Different paths
to consensus? The impact of need for closure on
model-supported group conflict management. European Journal
of Operational Research, 249(3), 878-
889.
Frederick S (2005). Cognitive reflection and decision making.
The Journal of Economic Perspectives, 19(4), 25-
42.
Freiberg J (2004). How gutsy leaders blow the doors off
business ‐as‐ usual. Leader to Leader, 2004(33), 32-37.
Friedrich J (1993). Primary error detection and minimization
(PEDMIN) strategies in social cognition: A
reinterpretation of confirmation bias phenomena. Psychological
Review, 100(2), 298.
Goodwin P & Wright G (2004). Decision analysis for
management judgement. Chicester: Wiley & Sons.
Hämäläinen RP, Luoma J & Saarinen E (2013). On the
importance of behavioral operational research: The case of
understanding and communicating about dynamic systems.
European Journal of Operational Research,
228(3), 623-634.
Hernandez I & Preston JL (2013). Disfluency disrupts the
confirmation bias. Journal of Experimental Social
Psychology, 49(1), 178-182.
Jensen M & Zajac EJ (2004). Corporate elites and corporate
strategy: How demographic preferences and structural
position shape the scope of the firm. Strategic Management
Journal, 25(6), 507-524.
Jones MA, Reynolds KE & Arnold MJ (2006). Hedonic and
utilitarian shopping value: Investigating differential
effects on retail outcomes. Journal of Business Research, 59(9),
974-981.
doi:http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jbusres.2006.03.006
Kahneman D (2011). Thinking, fast and slow: Macmillan.
Kahneman D, Lovallo D & Sibony O (2011). Before you make
that big decision. Harvard Business Review, 89(6),
50-60.
Kahneman D & Tversky A (1979). Prospect Theory: An
Analysis of Decision under Risk.
Econometrica, 47(2), 263-291.
Keyt A (2003). Decisions, Decisions. Myths and Mortals:
Family Business Leadership and Succession Planning,
135-152.
Kiron D, Ferguson RB & Prentice PK (2013). From value to
vision: Reimagining the possible with data analytics.
MIT Sloan Management Review, 54(3), 1.
MacCoun RJ (1998). Biases in the interpretation and use of
research results. Annual Review of Psychology, 49(1),
259-287.
Montibeller G & von Winterfeld D (2015). Biases and debiasing
in multi-criteria decision analysis. Paper
presented at the Proceedings of the 48th Hawaii International
Conference on System Sciences. Kauai,
Hawaii: IEEE Computer Society.
Montibeller G & von Winterfeldt D (2015). Cognitive and
Motivational Biases in Decision and Risk Analysis. Risk
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jbusres.2007.06.017
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jbusres.2006.03.006
Academy of Strategic Management Journal
Volume 16, Number 1, 2017
259
Analysis.
Overby JW & Lee EJ (2006). The effects of utilitarian and
hedonic online shopping value on consumer preference
and intentions. Journal of Business Research, 59(10–11), 1160-
1166.
doi:http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jbusres.2006.03.008
Pleshko L & Nickerson I (2008). Strategic orientation,
organizational structure, and the associated effects on
performance in industrial firms. Academy of Strategic
Management Journal, 7, 95.
Polman E (2012). Self–other decision making and loss aversion.
Organizational Behavior and Human Decision
Processes, 119(2), 141-150.
Rigby D & Bilodeau B (2007). Selecting Management Tools
Wisely. Harvard Business Review, 85(12), 20-22.
Rigby D & Bilodeau B (2015). Management Tools & Trends
2015. London, Bain & Company. Ronda-Pupo, G. A.
(2015). Growth and Consolidation of Strategic Management
Research: Insights for the Future
Development of Strategic Management. Academy of Strategic
Management Journal, 14(2), 155.
Schafer JG & Gallemore CT (2016). Biases in multicriteria
decision analysis: The case of environmental planning
in Southern Nevada. Environment and Planning C: Government
and Policy, 0263774X16629675.
Shim JP, Warkentin M, Courtney JF, Power DJ, Sharda R &
Carlsson C (2002). Past, present, and future of
decision support technology. Decision support systems, 33(2),
111- 126.
Simon HA (1955). A behavioral model of rational choice. The
quarterly journal of economics, 99-118.
Simon HA (1979). Rational Decision Making in Business
Organizations. The American Economic Review, 69(4),
493-513.
Sjöberg L (2003). Intuitive vs. analytical decision making:
which is preferred? Scandinavian Journal of
Management, 19(1), 17-29.
Stewart TJ, French S & Rios J (2013). Integrating multicriteria
decision analysis and scenario planning—Review and
extension. Omega, 41(4), 679-688.
Sweeney JC & Soutar GN (2001). Consumer perceived value:
The development of a multiple item scale. Journal of
Retailing, 77(2), 203-220. doi:http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0022-
4359(01)00041-0
Tversky A & Kahneman D (1974). Judgment under uncertainty:
Heuristics and biases. Science, 185(4157), 1124-
1131.
Tversky A & Kahneman D (1981). The framing of decisions and
the psychology of choice.
Science, 211(4481), 453-458.
Tversky A & Kahneman D (1986). Rational choice and the
framing of decisions. Journal of Business, S251-S278.
Ulaga W & Chacour S (2001). Measuring Customer-Perceived
Value in Business Markets: A Prerequisite for
Marketing Strategy Development and Implementation. Industrial
Marketing Management, 30(6),
525-540. doi:http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0019-8501(99)00122-
4
Wason PC (1960). On the failure to eliminate hypotheses in a
conceptual task. Quarterly Journal of Experimental
Psychology, 12(3), 129-140.
Witteman C, van den Bercken J, Claes L & Godoy A (2009).
Assessing rational and intuitive thinking styles.
European Journal of Psychological Assessment, 25(1), 39-47.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jbusres.2006.03.008
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0022-4359(01)00041-0
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0022-4359(01)00041-0
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0019-8501(99)00122-4
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0019-8501(99)00122-4CAN
MANAGERS BE REALLY
OBJECTIVE?ABSTRACTINTRODUCTIONANTECEDENTSHY
POTHESESFigure-1HYPOTHESES OF THE EXPERIMENTAL
STUDYStage 1Stage 2Stage 3RESULTSFigure-2SEQUENCE
OF DECISIONS 1-3, CATEGORIZED BY INDIVIDUAL
MOTIVATIONAL ORIENTATION (HEDONIC VS.
UTILITARIAN) AND FIRST DECISION MADE
(PERFORMANCE, NEUTRAL OR EXPERIENCE).Figure-
3EVIDENCE OF COGNITIVE BIAS, CATEGORIZED BY
INDIVIDUAL MOTIVATIONAL ORIENTATION (HEDONIC
VS. UTILITARIAN) AND FIRST DECISION MADE
(PERFORMANCE, NEUTRAL OR EXPERIENCE).Figure-
4EFFECT OF CRITERIA CONSIDERED AND CRITERIA
PRIORITY (EXPERIENCE VS. PERFORMANCE) ON
DECISION, STANDARDIZED (N=109, P<.001 FOR
CC*CP).CONCLUSIONSREFERENCES

More Related Content

Similar to STAT4610 Project 5So You Want to be an Entrepreneur”INTROD.docx

Essay Narrative Example. Narrative Essay PDF Essays Narrative
Essay Narrative Example. Narrative Essay  PDF  Essays  NarrativeEssay Narrative Example. Narrative Essay  PDF  Essays  Narrative
Essay Narrative Example. Narrative Essay PDF Essays NarrativeElizabeth Pardue
 
c PJM6610 Foundations of Project Business Analysis.docx
c        PJM6610 Foundations of Project Business Analysis.docxc        PJM6610 Foundations of Project Business Analysis.docx
c PJM6610 Foundations of Project Business Analysis.docxbartholomeocoombs
 
1. what is a feasibility study
1. what is a feasibility study1. what is a feasibility study
1. what is a feasibility studyRudy Flores
 
tools in IDTelated to first year vtu students is useful where they can refer ...
tools in IDTelated to first year vtu students is useful where they can refer ...tools in IDTelated to first year vtu students is useful where they can refer ...
tools in IDTelated to first year vtu students is useful where they can refer ...vinbld123
 
Library worksmay17 2017
Library worksmay17 2017Library worksmay17 2017
Library worksmay17 2017Stephen Abram
 
Appendix ABusiness Plan AssignmentThe Business Plan will be ab.docx
Appendix ABusiness Plan AssignmentThe Business Plan will be ab.docxAppendix ABusiness Plan AssignmentThe Business Plan will be ab.docx
Appendix ABusiness Plan AssignmentThe Business Plan will be ab.docxjustine1simpson78276
 
Business plan template
Business plan templateBusiness plan template
Business plan templateInsightory
 
Pearson HND BTEC Level 5 HNDManaging a Successful Business Pr.docx
Pearson HND BTEC Level 5 HNDManaging a Successful Business Pr.docxPearson HND BTEC Level 5 HNDManaging a Successful Business Pr.docx
Pearson HND BTEC Level 5 HNDManaging a Successful Business Pr.docxAASTHA76
 
Directions_Materiality - Breaking out of the straitjacket
Directions_Materiality - Breaking out of the straitjacketDirections_Materiality - Breaking out of the straitjacket
Directions_Materiality - Breaking out of the straitjacketSamuel Griffin-Flynn
 
[Salterbaxter MSLGROUP Directions] Materiality - Breaking Out of the Strait-J...
[Salterbaxter MSLGROUP Directions] Materiality - Breaking Out of the Strait-J...[Salterbaxter MSLGROUP Directions] Materiality - Breaking Out of the Strait-J...
[Salterbaxter MSLGROUP Directions] Materiality - Breaking Out of the Strait-J...MSL
 
This is a four-Part Project offering you the opportunity to demons.docx
This is a four-Part Project offering you the opportunity to demons.docxThis is a four-Part Project offering you the opportunity to demons.docx
This is a four-Part Project offering you the opportunity to demons.docxchristalgrieg
 
Research Methodology Module-08
Research Methodology Module-08Research Methodology Module-08
Research Methodology Module-08Kishor Ade
 
Presentation on 'Why cant people estimate' event, 23rd June 2016
Presentation on 'Why cant people estimate' event, 23rd June 2016 Presentation on 'Why cant people estimate' event, 23rd June 2016
Presentation on 'Why cant people estimate' event, 23rd June 2016 Association for Project Management
 
OpenStrategies: A simple system for Strategic Planning and Benefits Realisation
OpenStrategies: A simple system for Strategic Planning and Benefits RealisationOpenStrategies: A simple system for Strategic Planning and Benefits Realisation
OpenStrategies: A simple system for Strategic Planning and Benefits RealisationImprovement Skills Consulting Ltd.
 
Type/MBTI and Strategic Planning
Type/MBTI and Strategic PlanningType/MBTI and Strategic Planning
Type/MBTI and Strategic PlanningJennifer Tucker
 
Ultimate+Team+Building+Pack+2.pptx
Ultimate+Team+Building+Pack+2.pptxUltimate+Team+Building+Pack+2.pptx
Ultimate+Team+Building+Pack+2.pptxAshenafiGirma4
 

Similar to STAT4610 Project 5So You Want to be an Entrepreneur”INTROD.docx (20)

Topic 1 xtra note
Topic 1 xtra noteTopic 1 xtra note
Topic 1 xtra note
 
Essay Narrative Example. Narrative Essay PDF Essays Narrative
Essay Narrative Example. Narrative Essay  PDF  Essays  NarrativeEssay Narrative Example. Narrative Essay  PDF  Essays  Narrative
Essay Narrative Example. Narrative Essay PDF Essays Narrative
 
c PJM6610 Foundations of Project Business Analysis.docx
c        PJM6610 Foundations of Project Business Analysis.docxc        PJM6610 Foundations of Project Business Analysis.docx
c PJM6610 Foundations of Project Business Analysis.docx
 
1. what is a feasibility study
1. what is a feasibility study1. what is a feasibility study
1. what is a feasibility study
 
tools in IDTelated to first year vtu students is useful where they can refer ...
tools in IDTelated to first year vtu students is useful where they can refer ...tools in IDTelated to first year vtu students is useful where they can refer ...
tools in IDTelated to first year vtu students is useful where they can refer ...
 
Library worksmay17 2017
Library worksmay17 2017Library worksmay17 2017
Library worksmay17 2017
 
Appendix ABusiness Plan AssignmentThe Business Plan will be ab.docx
Appendix ABusiness Plan AssignmentThe Business Plan will be ab.docxAppendix ABusiness Plan AssignmentThe Business Plan will be ab.docx
Appendix ABusiness Plan AssignmentThe Business Plan will be ab.docx
 
Business plan template
Business plan templateBusiness plan template
Business plan template
 
Pearson HND BTEC Level 5 HNDManaging a Successful Business Pr.docx
Pearson HND BTEC Level 5 HNDManaging a Successful Business Pr.docxPearson HND BTEC Level 5 HNDManaging a Successful Business Pr.docx
Pearson HND BTEC Level 5 HNDManaging a Successful Business Pr.docx
 
Directions_Materiality - Breaking out of the straitjacket
Directions_Materiality - Breaking out of the straitjacketDirections_Materiality - Breaking out of the straitjacket
Directions_Materiality - Breaking out of the straitjacket
 
[Salterbaxter MSLGROUP Directions] Materiality - Breaking Out of the Strait-J...
[Salterbaxter MSLGROUP Directions] Materiality - Breaking Out of the Strait-J...[Salterbaxter MSLGROUP Directions] Materiality - Breaking Out of the Strait-J...
[Salterbaxter MSLGROUP Directions] Materiality - Breaking Out of the Strait-J...
 
This is a four-Part Project offering you the opportunity to demons.docx
This is a four-Part Project offering you the opportunity to demons.docxThis is a four-Part Project offering you the opportunity to demons.docx
This is a four-Part Project offering you the opportunity to demons.docx
 
Research Methodology Module-08
Research Methodology Module-08Research Methodology Module-08
Research Methodology Module-08
 
Presentation on 'Why cant people estimate' event, 23rd June 2016
Presentation on 'Why cant people estimate' event, 23rd June 2016 Presentation on 'Why cant people estimate' event, 23rd June 2016
Presentation on 'Why cant people estimate' event, 23rd June 2016
 
Research
ResearchResearch
Research
 
OpenStrategies: A simple system for Strategic Planning and Benefits Realisation
OpenStrategies: A simple system for Strategic Planning and Benefits RealisationOpenStrategies: A simple system for Strategic Planning and Benefits Realisation
OpenStrategies: A simple system for Strategic Planning and Benefits Realisation
 
Business presentation
Business presentationBusiness presentation
Business presentation
 
Relationship Forecasting
Relationship ForecastingRelationship Forecasting
Relationship Forecasting
 
Type/MBTI and Strategic Planning
Type/MBTI and Strategic PlanningType/MBTI and Strategic Planning
Type/MBTI and Strategic Planning
 
Ultimate+Team+Building+Pack+2.pptx
Ultimate+Team+Building+Pack+2.pptxUltimate+Team+Building+Pack+2.pptx
Ultimate+Team+Building+Pack+2.pptx
 

More from mckellarhastings

Conduct an internet search finding a job you are interested (HR .docx
Conduct an internet search finding a job you are interested (HR .docxConduct an internet search finding a job you are interested (HR .docx
Conduct an internet search finding a job you are interested (HR .docxmckellarhastings
 
Conduct an Internet or library search for information on The Bay of.docx
Conduct an Internet or library search for information on The Bay of.docxConduct an Internet or library search for information on The Bay of.docx
Conduct an Internet or library search for information on The Bay of.docxmckellarhastings
 
Conduct an internet search about the murder of Yeardley Love. After .docx
Conduct an internet search about the murder of Yeardley Love. After .docxConduct an internet search about the murder of Yeardley Love. After .docx
Conduct an internet search about the murder of Yeardley Love. After .docxmckellarhastings
 
At this point, you’ve organized your HR project team and you are.docx
At this point, you’ve organized your HR project team and you are.docxAt this point, you’ve organized your HR project team and you are.docx
At this point, you’ve organized your HR project team and you are.docxmckellarhastings
 
At the beginning of 2012, the Jeater company had the following balan.docx
At the beginning of 2012, the Jeater company had the following balan.docxAt the beginning of 2012, the Jeater company had the following balan.docx
At the beginning of 2012, the Jeater company had the following balan.docxmckellarhastings
 
At many different points throughout the collection Born a Crime, Tre.docx
At many different points throughout the collection Born a Crime, Tre.docxAt many different points throughout the collection Born a Crime, Tre.docx
At many different points throughout the collection Born a Crime, Tre.docxmckellarhastings
 
At least 200 wordss or more per question. Answer UNDER question. And.docx
At least 200 wordss or more per question. Answer UNDER question. And.docxAt least 200 wordss or more per question. Answer UNDER question. And.docx
At least 200 wordss or more per question. Answer UNDER question. And.docxmckellarhastings
 
At least 200 words per question. Chapter 11The Idea .docx
At least 200 words per question. Chapter 11The Idea .docxAt least 200 words per question. Chapter 11The Idea .docx
At least 200 words per question. Chapter 11The Idea .docxmckellarhastings
 
At least 150 words each. Use a reference for each question and us.docx
At least 150 words each.  Use a reference for each question and us.docxAt least 150 words each.  Use a reference for each question and us.docx
At least 150 words each. Use a reference for each question and us.docxmckellarhastings
 
At least 250 words per question. Chapter 11The Idea of Craft A.docx
At least 250 words per question. Chapter 11The Idea of Craft A.docxAt least 250 words per question. Chapter 11The Idea of Craft A.docx
At least 250 words per question. Chapter 11The Idea of Craft A.docxmckellarhastings
 
At its core, pathology is the study of disease. Diseases occur for m.docx
At its core, pathology is the study of disease. Diseases occur for m.docxAt its core, pathology is the study of disease. Diseases occur for m.docx
At its core, pathology is the study of disease. Diseases occur for m.docxmckellarhastings
 
assumptions people make about this topic (homelessness, immigration,.docx
assumptions people make about this topic (homelessness, immigration,.docxassumptions people make about this topic (homelessness, immigration,.docx
assumptions people make about this topic (homelessness, immigration,.docxmckellarhastings
 
At age 12, Freeman Hrabowski marched with Martin Luther King. Now he.docx
At age 12, Freeman Hrabowski marched with Martin Luther King. Now he.docxAt age 12, Freeman Hrabowski marched with Martin Luther King. Now he.docx
At age 12, Freeman Hrabowski marched with Martin Luther King. Now he.docxmckellarhastings
 
At each of the locations listed below, there is evidence of plat.docx
At each of the locations listed below, there is evidence of plat.docxAt each of the locations listed below, there is evidence of plat.docx
At each of the locations listed below, there is evidence of plat.docxmckellarhastings
 
Assume you hold the Special Agent in Charge role of the Joint .docx
Assume you hold the Special Agent in Charge role of the Joint .docxAssume you hold the Special Agent in Charge role of the Joint .docx
Assume you hold the Special Agent in Charge role of the Joint .docxmckellarhastings
 
Assume you are a DFI and you must deliver a presentation to the Stat.docx
Assume you are a DFI and you must deliver a presentation to the Stat.docxAssume you are a DFI and you must deliver a presentation to the Stat.docx
Assume you are a DFI and you must deliver a presentation to the Stat.docxmckellarhastings
 
Assume that you work for the District Board of Education. The Direct.docx
Assume that you work for the District Board of Education. The Direct.docxAssume that you work for the District Board of Education. The Direct.docx
Assume that you work for the District Board of Education. The Direct.docxmckellarhastings
 
Assume that you have been tasked by your employer to develop an inci.docx
Assume that you have been tasked by your employer to develop an inci.docxAssume that you have been tasked by your employer to develop an inci.docx
Assume that you have been tasked by your employer to develop an inci.docxmckellarhastings
 
Assume that you generate an authenticated and encrypted message by f.docx
Assume that you generate an authenticated and encrypted message by f.docxAssume that you generate an authenticated and encrypted message by f.docx
Assume that you generate an authenticated and encrypted message by f.docxmckellarhastings
 
Assume that you are in your chosen criminal justice profession, .docx
Assume that you are in your chosen criminal justice profession, .docxAssume that you are in your chosen criminal justice profession, .docx
Assume that you are in your chosen criminal justice profession, .docxmckellarhastings
 

More from mckellarhastings (20)

Conduct an internet search finding a job you are interested (HR .docx
Conduct an internet search finding a job you are interested (HR .docxConduct an internet search finding a job you are interested (HR .docx
Conduct an internet search finding a job you are interested (HR .docx
 
Conduct an Internet or library search for information on The Bay of.docx
Conduct an Internet or library search for information on The Bay of.docxConduct an Internet or library search for information on The Bay of.docx
Conduct an Internet or library search for information on The Bay of.docx
 
Conduct an internet search about the murder of Yeardley Love. After .docx
Conduct an internet search about the murder of Yeardley Love. After .docxConduct an internet search about the murder of Yeardley Love. After .docx
Conduct an internet search about the murder of Yeardley Love. After .docx
 
At this point, you’ve organized your HR project team and you are.docx
At this point, you’ve organized your HR project team and you are.docxAt this point, you’ve organized your HR project team and you are.docx
At this point, you’ve organized your HR project team and you are.docx
 
At the beginning of 2012, the Jeater company had the following balan.docx
At the beginning of 2012, the Jeater company had the following balan.docxAt the beginning of 2012, the Jeater company had the following balan.docx
At the beginning of 2012, the Jeater company had the following balan.docx
 
At many different points throughout the collection Born a Crime, Tre.docx
At many different points throughout the collection Born a Crime, Tre.docxAt many different points throughout the collection Born a Crime, Tre.docx
At many different points throughout the collection Born a Crime, Tre.docx
 
At least 200 wordss or more per question. Answer UNDER question. And.docx
At least 200 wordss or more per question. Answer UNDER question. And.docxAt least 200 wordss or more per question. Answer UNDER question. And.docx
At least 200 wordss or more per question. Answer UNDER question. And.docx
 
At least 200 words per question. Chapter 11The Idea .docx
At least 200 words per question. Chapter 11The Idea .docxAt least 200 words per question. Chapter 11The Idea .docx
At least 200 words per question. Chapter 11The Idea .docx
 
At least 150 words each. Use a reference for each question and us.docx
At least 150 words each.  Use a reference for each question and us.docxAt least 150 words each.  Use a reference for each question and us.docx
At least 150 words each. Use a reference for each question and us.docx
 
At least 250 words per question. Chapter 11The Idea of Craft A.docx
At least 250 words per question. Chapter 11The Idea of Craft A.docxAt least 250 words per question. Chapter 11The Idea of Craft A.docx
At least 250 words per question. Chapter 11The Idea of Craft A.docx
 
At its core, pathology is the study of disease. Diseases occur for m.docx
At its core, pathology is the study of disease. Diseases occur for m.docxAt its core, pathology is the study of disease. Diseases occur for m.docx
At its core, pathology is the study of disease. Diseases occur for m.docx
 
assumptions people make about this topic (homelessness, immigration,.docx
assumptions people make about this topic (homelessness, immigration,.docxassumptions people make about this topic (homelessness, immigration,.docx
assumptions people make about this topic (homelessness, immigration,.docx
 
At age 12, Freeman Hrabowski marched with Martin Luther King. Now he.docx
At age 12, Freeman Hrabowski marched with Martin Luther King. Now he.docxAt age 12, Freeman Hrabowski marched with Martin Luther King. Now he.docx
At age 12, Freeman Hrabowski marched with Martin Luther King. Now he.docx
 
At each of the locations listed below, there is evidence of plat.docx
At each of the locations listed below, there is evidence of plat.docxAt each of the locations listed below, there is evidence of plat.docx
At each of the locations listed below, there is evidence of plat.docx
 
Assume you hold the Special Agent in Charge role of the Joint .docx
Assume you hold the Special Agent in Charge role of the Joint .docxAssume you hold the Special Agent in Charge role of the Joint .docx
Assume you hold the Special Agent in Charge role of the Joint .docx
 
Assume you are a DFI and you must deliver a presentation to the Stat.docx
Assume you are a DFI and you must deliver a presentation to the Stat.docxAssume you are a DFI and you must deliver a presentation to the Stat.docx
Assume you are a DFI and you must deliver a presentation to the Stat.docx
 
Assume that you work for the District Board of Education. The Direct.docx
Assume that you work for the District Board of Education. The Direct.docxAssume that you work for the District Board of Education. The Direct.docx
Assume that you work for the District Board of Education. The Direct.docx
 
Assume that you have been tasked by your employer to develop an inci.docx
Assume that you have been tasked by your employer to develop an inci.docxAssume that you have been tasked by your employer to develop an inci.docx
Assume that you have been tasked by your employer to develop an inci.docx
 
Assume that you generate an authenticated and encrypted message by f.docx
Assume that you generate an authenticated and encrypted message by f.docxAssume that you generate an authenticated and encrypted message by f.docx
Assume that you generate an authenticated and encrypted message by f.docx
 
Assume that you are in your chosen criminal justice profession, .docx
Assume that you are in your chosen criminal justice profession, .docxAssume that you are in your chosen criminal justice profession, .docx
Assume that you are in your chosen criminal justice profession, .docx
 

Recently uploaded

DATA STRUCTURE AND ALGORITHM for beginners
DATA STRUCTURE AND ALGORITHM for beginnersDATA STRUCTURE AND ALGORITHM for beginners
DATA STRUCTURE AND ALGORITHM for beginnersSabitha Banu
 
CARE OF CHILD IN INCUBATOR..........pptx
CARE OF CHILD IN INCUBATOR..........pptxCARE OF CHILD IN INCUBATOR..........pptx
CARE OF CHILD IN INCUBATOR..........pptxGaneshChakor2
 
Interactive Powerpoint_How to Master effective communication
Interactive Powerpoint_How to Master effective communicationInteractive Powerpoint_How to Master effective communication
Interactive Powerpoint_How to Master effective communicationnomboosow
 
Full Stack Web Development Course for Beginners
Full Stack Web Development Course  for BeginnersFull Stack Web Development Course  for Beginners
Full Stack Web Development Course for BeginnersSabitha Banu
 
Types of Journalistic Writing Grade 8.pptx
Types of Journalistic Writing Grade 8.pptxTypes of Journalistic Writing Grade 8.pptx
Types of Journalistic Writing Grade 8.pptxEyham Joco
 
Presiding Officer Training module 2024 lok sabha elections
Presiding Officer Training module 2024 lok sabha electionsPresiding Officer Training module 2024 lok sabha elections
Presiding Officer Training module 2024 lok sabha electionsanshu789521
 
CELL CYCLE Division Science 8 quarter IV.pptx
CELL CYCLE Division Science 8 quarter IV.pptxCELL CYCLE Division Science 8 quarter IV.pptx
CELL CYCLE Division Science 8 quarter IV.pptxJiesonDelaCerna
 
Enzyme, Pharmaceutical Aids, Miscellaneous Last Part of Chapter no 5th.pdf
Enzyme, Pharmaceutical Aids, Miscellaneous Last Part of Chapter no 5th.pdfEnzyme, Pharmaceutical Aids, Miscellaneous Last Part of Chapter no 5th.pdf
Enzyme, Pharmaceutical Aids, Miscellaneous Last Part of Chapter no 5th.pdfSumit Tiwari
 
Organic Name Reactions for the students and aspirants of Chemistry12th.pptx
Organic Name Reactions  for the students and aspirants of Chemistry12th.pptxOrganic Name Reactions  for the students and aspirants of Chemistry12th.pptx
Organic Name Reactions for the students and aspirants of Chemistry12th.pptxVS Mahajan Coaching Centre
 
Solving Puzzles Benefits Everyone (English).pptx
Solving Puzzles Benefits Everyone (English).pptxSolving Puzzles Benefits Everyone (English).pptx
Solving Puzzles Benefits Everyone (English).pptxOH TEIK BIN
 
Biting mechanism of poisonous snakes.pdf
Biting mechanism of poisonous snakes.pdfBiting mechanism of poisonous snakes.pdf
Biting mechanism of poisonous snakes.pdfadityarao40181
 
“Oh GOSH! Reflecting on Hackteria's Collaborative Practices in a Global Do-It...
“Oh GOSH! Reflecting on Hackteria's Collaborative Practices in a Global Do-It...“Oh GOSH! Reflecting on Hackteria's Collaborative Practices in a Global Do-It...
“Oh GOSH! Reflecting on Hackteria's Collaborative Practices in a Global Do-It...Marc Dusseiller Dusjagr
 
Hierarchy of management that covers different levels of management
Hierarchy of management that covers different levels of managementHierarchy of management that covers different levels of management
Hierarchy of management that covers different levels of managementmkooblal
 
ECONOMIC CONTEXT - LONG FORM TV DRAMA - PPT
ECONOMIC CONTEXT - LONG FORM TV DRAMA - PPTECONOMIC CONTEXT - LONG FORM TV DRAMA - PPT
ECONOMIC CONTEXT - LONG FORM TV DRAMA - PPTiammrhaywood
 
Capitol Tech U Doctoral Presentation - April 2024.pptx
Capitol Tech U Doctoral Presentation - April 2024.pptxCapitol Tech U Doctoral Presentation - April 2024.pptx
Capitol Tech U Doctoral Presentation - April 2024.pptxCapitolTechU
 
Pharmacognosy Flower 3. Compositae 2023.pdf
Pharmacognosy Flower 3. Compositae 2023.pdfPharmacognosy Flower 3. Compositae 2023.pdf
Pharmacognosy Flower 3. Compositae 2023.pdfMahmoud M. Sallam
 

Recently uploaded (20)

DATA STRUCTURE AND ALGORITHM for beginners
DATA STRUCTURE AND ALGORITHM for beginnersDATA STRUCTURE AND ALGORITHM for beginners
DATA STRUCTURE AND ALGORITHM for beginners
 
CARE OF CHILD IN INCUBATOR..........pptx
CARE OF CHILD IN INCUBATOR..........pptxCARE OF CHILD IN INCUBATOR..........pptx
CARE OF CHILD IN INCUBATOR..........pptx
 
Interactive Powerpoint_How to Master effective communication
Interactive Powerpoint_How to Master effective communicationInteractive Powerpoint_How to Master effective communication
Interactive Powerpoint_How to Master effective communication
 
Full Stack Web Development Course for Beginners
Full Stack Web Development Course  for BeginnersFull Stack Web Development Course  for Beginners
Full Stack Web Development Course for Beginners
 
Model Call Girl in Tilak Nagar Delhi reach out to us at 🔝9953056974🔝
Model Call Girl in Tilak Nagar Delhi reach out to us at 🔝9953056974🔝Model Call Girl in Tilak Nagar Delhi reach out to us at 🔝9953056974🔝
Model Call Girl in Tilak Nagar Delhi reach out to us at 🔝9953056974🔝
 
Types of Journalistic Writing Grade 8.pptx
Types of Journalistic Writing Grade 8.pptxTypes of Journalistic Writing Grade 8.pptx
Types of Journalistic Writing Grade 8.pptx
 
TataKelola dan KamSiber Kecerdasan Buatan v022.pdf
TataKelola dan KamSiber Kecerdasan Buatan v022.pdfTataKelola dan KamSiber Kecerdasan Buatan v022.pdf
TataKelola dan KamSiber Kecerdasan Buatan v022.pdf
 
Presiding Officer Training module 2024 lok sabha elections
Presiding Officer Training module 2024 lok sabha electionsPresiding Officer Training module 2024 lok sabha elections
Presiding Officer Training module 2024 lok sabha elections
 
CELL CYCLE Division Science 8 quarter IV.pptx
CELL CYCLE Division Science 8 quarter IV.pptxCELL CYCLE Division Science 8 quarter IV.pptx
CELL CYCLE Division Science 8 quarter IV.pptx
 
Enzyme, Pharmaceutical Aids, Miscellaneous Last Part of Chapter no 5th.pdf
Enzyme, Pharmaceutical Aids, Miscellaneous Last Part of Chapter no 5th.pdfEnzyme, Pharmaceutical Aids, Miscellaneous Last Part of Chapter no 5th.pdf
Enzyme, Pharmaceutical Aids, Miscellaneous Last Part of Chapter no 5th.pdf
 
Organic Name Reactions for the students and aspirants of Chemistry12th.pptx
Organic Name Reactions  for the students and aspirants of Chemistry12th.pptxOrganic Name Reactions  for the students and aspirants of Chemistry12th.pptx
Organic Name Reactions for the students and aspirants of Chemistry12th.pptx
 
Solving Puzzles Benefits Everyone (English).pptx
Solving Puzzles Benefits Everyone (English).pptxSolving Puzzles Benefits Everyone (English).pptx
Solving Puzzles Benefits Everyone (English).pptx
 
ESSENTIAL of (CS/IT/IS) class 06 (database)
ESSENTIAL of (CS/IT/IS) class 06 (database)ESSENTIAL of (CS/IT/IS) class 06 (database)
ESSENTIAL of (CS/IT/IS) class 06 (database)
 
Biting mechanism of poisonous snakes.pdf
Biting mechanism of poisonous snakes.pdfBiting mechanism of poisonous snakes.pdf
Biting mechanism of poisonous snakes.pdf
 
“Oh GOSH! Reflecting on Hackteria's Collaborative Practices in a Global Do-It...
“Oh GOSH! Reflecting on Hackteria's Collaborative Practices in a Global Do-It...“Oh GOSH! Reflecting on Hackteria's Collaborative Practices in a Global Do-It...
“Oh GOSH! Reflecting on Hackteria's Collaborative Practices in a Global Do-It...
 
9953330565 Low Rate Call Girls In Rohini Delhi NCR
9953330565 Low Rate Call Girls In Rohini  Delhi NCR9953330565 Low Rate Call Girls In Rohini  Delhi NCR
9953330565 Low Rate Call Girls In Rohini Delhi NCR
 
Hierarchy of management that covers different levels of management
Hierarchy of management that covers different levels of managementHierarchy of management that covers different levels of management
Hierarchy of management that covers different levels of management
 
ECONOMIC CONTEXT - LONG FORM TV DRAMA - PPT
ECONOMIC CONTEXT - LONG FORM TV DRAMA - PPTECONOMIC CONTEXT - LONG FORM TV DRAMA - PPT
ECONOMIC CONTEXT - LONG FORM TV DRAMA - PPT
 
Capitol Tech U Doctoral Presentation - April 2024.pptx
Capitol Tech U Doctoral Presentation - April 2024.pptxCapitol Tech U Doctoral Presentation - April 2024.pptx
Capitol Tech U Doctoral Presentation - April 2024.pptx
 
Pharmacognosy Flower 3. Compositae 2023.pdf
Pharmacognosy Flower 3. Compositae 2023.pdfPharmacognosy Flower 3. Compositae 2023.pdf
Pharmacognosy Flower 3. Compositae 2023.pdf
 

STAT4610 Project 5So You Want to be an Entrepreneur”INTROD.docx

  • 1. STAT4610 Project 5 “So You Want to be an Entrepreneur?” INTRODUCTION Starting a new business can be an exciting adventure! Who doesn’t want to find themselves at the top of the next Microsoft, Apple, Facebook, Myspace, Instagram, Beanie Babies, Pet Rocks, Roto-Rooter, Enron, WorldCom, (OK, you get the point!)? The problem with starting a new business is it carries an inherent amount of risk. (OK, a considerable amount of risk!) One-third of businesses fail within the first two years, and over half are gone in five years. That’s a waste of a significant amount of toil, sweat, intellectual effort, creativity, and—oh yes—capital! Failed businesses have stripped entrepreneurs of their life savings (and the savings of others) for millennia, but the successes have also lined the pockets of a lot of savvy investors. Since most people can’t afford to fund their own startups, a popular strategy involves convincing others to get behind your great idea. Venture capital funds are always willing to chase the next big thing, but unfortunately there is a limit to the patience of the world’s check-writers, and most of them didn’t get rich without recognizing when it is time to cut losses. There is hope, however! Analytics can help every new business set realistic expectations for their investors, and in fact are essential in the formulation of a business plan. Because of the uncertainties involved with startups (in terms of costs, timelines, returns, rates of success, etc.) it is often difficult to forecast the progression of a new venture, but with a sufficient
  • 2. knowledge of the field and the tasks required, a simulation can frequently get an entrepreneur into the right ballpark. Sometimes this is enough to start the money flowing! PROJECT OVERVIEW In this project you will develop a business plan for an Angel Investor in order to secure funding for your new startup. You will require funding until the point that your company generates a profit and is actually able to return capital (or positive equity) to its investors. Expenses may include payroll for developers or designers, facilities and infrastructure, marketing and sales, purchasing or collecting data, research, and a variety of other costs that you may want to identify. Obviously, you will have to identify a viable product around which to build your company, and if you’re stuck without an idea, feel free to go back to your Gateway Course and build a business around app development. Finally, you will need to determine the cost of executing your business plan across this timeline in order to secure sufficient funding. [As an aside, the particular product or service is not essential to this project—you may use something developed in another course, some cool idea that you might have in mind, or something entirely fictional.] You may make any assumptions you want concerning the availability of personnel (whether you will be required to hire them or if they are already onboard as part of your venture) and you will need to calculate the rates required to retain their services (market rates for most skill sets are readily available from a variety of sources). PROJECT REQUIREMENTS 1. Develop a timeline (flowchart) of the tasks that need to be
  • 3. accomplished in order to make your company profitable. Build in as much efficiency as possible by completing tasks in parallel. 2. Identify appropriate distributions for all of the tasks on your timeline, to include sales and the potential for them to generate revenue. 3. Build an Excel-based simulation of your timeline and create a histogram of possible durations for the company to become a success. 4. Apply the “Clint Eastwood” rule to your simulation output (“How lucky do you feel?”) Remember: being too aggressive can get you burnt if you don’t achieve your goals, but being too conservative in your estimate and asking for too much can result in your proposal getting turned down. 5. Generate Cost and Revenue curves for your timeline from step 4. You will have two deliverables for the project, which are due together as documents in Canvas at the beginning of class, Lesson 19. DELIVERABLE 1. A business plan proposal and funding request for your Angel Investor. This should be brief, and should focus on specifics and results. In particular, specify how much money will be required to fund this venture, how long it will take to generate a profit, and how much return can be expected on the initial investment over time. [This should be organized to be presented to an Angel Investor.] DELIVERABLE 2. A summary of the simulation you used to
  • 4. support your decisions. This should include a flowchart of tasks, each distribution you used in your simulation, a summary of the simulation output (to include time to each critical point), the decisions you make from your output in terms of time to completion, and the cost and revenue analysis associated with the timeline. [This should be organized to be presented to a business partner (or a Professor).] critically analyze the article attached and post your article analysis to the discussion forum. The original post content should be comprehensive, accurate, and persuasive. Major points should be clearly stated and well supported based upon knowledge gained throughout the program of study. You should not summarize the article but instead, point out areas of agreement with the article information based upon your knowledge of the content, areas of weakness and/or disagreement with the author(s) positions and overall analysis of the content under discussion. Supporting research should be appropriate for academic discussion and address related concepts. As emerging academics, this is your opportunity to critically evaluate published research. You should also raise questions and seek clarifications from your classmates. Academy of Strategic Management Journal Volume 16, Number 1, 2017 244
  • 5. CAN MANAGERS BE REALLY OBJECTIVE? BIAS IN MULTICRITERIA DECISION ANALYSIS Silvio Borrero, Universidad Icesi Felipe Henao, Universidad Icesi ABSTRACT Strategic management frequently requires making decisions in complex situations. Strategic decision-making is widely assumed to be an objective process supported by rational analytical tools. A strategic decision, however, involves both a structured part, which can be dealt with using different analytical tools, and an unstructured part, which must be dealt with by means of the decision maker’s judgment, intuition and experience. Therefore, individual leanings are inherent to complex strategic decisions. To make such decisions, the human brain appeals to diverse heuristics or shortcuts to analyze and simplify contextual information. These mental strategies are not infallible; on the contrary, there is abundant evidence that humans’ minds are prone to cognitive biases or traps that cloud objectivity when making decisions. In a managerial context, such biases or traps can result in suboptimal or inefficient decisions that undermine organizational value. It is not surprising, therefore, that there are so many tools and techniques devised to make (arguably) objective management decisions, based on evidence. Among other disciplines that facilitate decision making and thus reduce cognitive biases, operations research (OR) offers multiple analytical methodologies and procedures,
  • 6. such as Multi-Criteria Decision Analysis (MCDA). However, the relationship between cognitive biases and these decision-making tools has not been amply investigated. Empirical evidence on how individual differences and cognitive factors influence the effectiveness of these tools in practice is still lacking, and it is uncertain whether they contribute effectively to reducing cognitive biases, or if, on the contrary, cognitive biases interfere with the tools’ effectiveness. This paper presents an experimental study involving undergraduate and graduate students where their individual differences, as measured by their hedonic or utilitarian leanings, are linked to two motivational biases, respectively known as confirmation and desirability of choice, and how these biases influence a decision made through MCDA techniques. Results suggest that experimental subjects employ MCDA to confirm previously conceived decisions, rather than using the tool to explore the problem situation, make sense of their preferences and choose the most appropriate course of action accordingly. This is, it appears that subjects make use of the OR tool to support a decision they have already made in their own minds. Consequently, the effectiveness of MCDA and other strategic or decision-making tools might be affected by individual differences and corresponding motivational biases. These results also suggest that there is a very fine division between valid, preference-based decisions and biased preconceptions. In particular, these experimental results question the effectiveness of multicriteria analysis tools; more generally, this research suggests that a totally rational decision making process—and therefore a completely objective,
  • 7. analytical, tool-supported, evidence-based management—might be a utopic endeavor. Keywords: Strategic management, decision-making, individual differences, motivational biases, Multi-Criteria Decision Analysis. mailto:[email protected] Academy of Strategic Management Journal Volume 16, Number 1, 2017 245 INTRODUCTION Strategic decision-making is often deemed as an objective exercise, especially when it is supported by—arguably—rational analytical tools. Classic rationality and bounded rationality paradigms assume that top managers approach strategic decision-making by means of a purposive, systematic and comprehensive process (Simon, 1955, 1979). Per this process, managers state concrete objectives, collect relevant information, develop viable alternatives, and identify the optimal course of action, thus maximizing utility. Throughout the past few decades, numerous techniques have been developed to facilitate decision making, including classic tools such as the strengths-weaknesses-opportunities-threats (SWOT), the
  • 8. political-economic-social-technological- environmental-legal (PESTEL), the external factor evaluation (EFE) or the internal factor evaluation (IFE) matrices, or more recent extensions, such as the internal competitive profile (ICPM), the external competitive profile (ECPM) or the financial competitive profile (FCPM) matrices (Capps III & Cassidy, 2016). The introduction and development of decision support systems (DSS) and other computer-based decision tools further enhanced the strategic decision making processes, and their capability to address increasingly complex situations (Shim et al., 2002). In general terms, most of these decision support tools are intended to deal with the structured parts of complex managerial problems to make decision making more efficient. Strategic decisions, though, involve both structured and unstructured elements, which must be dealt with by means of the decision maker’s judgment, intuition and experience, in addition to any analytical approach chosen. It follows that individual leanings or preferences are inherent to complex strategic decisions. Therefore, inevitably, cognitive and motivational biases pervasively affect managerial decisions and influence strategic outcomes (Das & Teng, 1999). As the exercise of strategic management frequently requires making decisions in complex situations, managers appeal to diverse heuristics or shortcuts to analyze and simplify contextual information. Since the publication of Tversky and Kahneman’s seminal paper (1974), there has been an increasing interest in understanding the underlying mechanisms that explain such
  • 9. heuristics or shortcuts, and the consequent biases that may affect human behavior in organizational decision making and strategic planning contexts (Busenitz & Barney, 1997; Busenitz et al., 2003). Numerous experimental studies have been performed within the premises of different academic fields, such as psychology, economy, finance, marketing, and—marginally and more recently—operations research (Franco & Hämäläinen, 2016; Hämäläinen, Luoma, & Saarinen, 2013). As a result, researchers have identified an ample range of human biases that can be classified as cognitive or motivational, and which are capable of distorting judgment and decision-making (Montibeller & von Winterfeldt, 2015). This is known as decision theory. Overall, it could be argued that its most remarkable contribution to knowledge is that is has proven that humans are all vulnerable to fall into these biases, leading us to make suboptimal or inefficient decisions that violate rationality and commonly accepted normative principles (Kahneman, 2011). In a managerial context, these biases can be especially costly because they could cloud objectivity when making decisions and therefore undermine organizational value (Frederick, 2005; Montibeller & von Winterfeld, 2015; Montibeller & von Winterfeldt, 2015). Although media and popular literature tend to praise gutsy business decisions—and the gutsy leaders who implement them—that yield successful market or business moves (Freiberg, 2004; Keyt, 2003), it is difficult to argue against the idea that firm strategies should be the result of sound analyses that consider as
  • 10. Academy of Strategic Management Journal Volume 16, Number 1, 2017 246 much contextual evidence as possible before making a decision (Kiron, Ferguson, & Prentice, 2013). Not surprisingly, business literature abounds with tools and techniques devised to make management a scientific exercise that eliminates or reduces biases, and hence aim at achieving objective business decisions (Rigby & Bilodeau, 2007, 2015). Operations Research (OR) is one of several disciplines that offer analytical methodologies and procedures to facilitate decision making and strategic planning (Stewart, French, & Rios, 2013). Although OR tools—such as Multi-Criteria Decision Analysis (MCDA), decision tress or influence diagrams, among others—focus on helping people solve problems and make better decisions under conditions of uncertainty, it appears to have ignored the behavioral aspects of the humans involved in its process (Hämäläinen et al., 2013). The relationship between people’s individual differences, their biases and decision tools has not been amply investigated (Franco & Hämäläinen, 2016). This is, it is unknown whether decision making tools contribute effectively to deal with these cognitive factors, or if, on the contrary, cognitive factors interfere with the effectiveness of such tools (Montibeller & von Winterfeldt,
  • 11. 2015). It is therefore necessary to further develop the behavioral OR agenda (Franco & Hämäläinen, 2016; Hämäläinen et al., 2013) and to more effectively connect decision making tools with the managerial reality. This paper contributes to this need by reporting the design of an experimental study that links individual differences, as measured by personal leanings towards hedonic or utilitarian preferences, with two motivational biases, known as confirmation and desirability of choice. More specifically, this study explores how such personal preferences and biases influence decisions made supported by MCDA. In what follows, we first briefly discuss from a theoretical point of view the known effects that individual differences and motivational biases have on decision-making, with particular detail on confirmation and desirability of choice biases. Then, the hypotheses for this study and our experimental approach to evaluating the effects of these two motivational biases on a MCDA problem are discussed in detail. We present some results that suggest that experimental subjects (undergraduate and graduate students) utilize MCDA to confirm previously conceived decisions, rather than to identify the best possible solution for them. Finally, we finish the paper by summarizing our main arguments, drawing some conclusions and presenting opportunities for extending our research. ANTECEDENTS
  • 12. For decades, the strategic and organizational literature has attempted to model the managers’ decision-making processes and to propose analytical approaches to make better strategic decisions. In an attempt to rationalize and objectivize decision making, and to optimize managerial decisions, OR and other disciplines have developed analytical methodologies and decision support systems (Shim et al., 2002; Stewart et al., 2013). Most mid-size and large companies utilize such methods and systems, in varied settings, and there is abundant research on practical applications of decision support tools. The evidence is relatively scarce, however, on the potentially undermining effect of the human factor on the effectiveness of these tools. Extant literature tends to investigate and describe the firm’s or its leader’s strategic orientation, as evidenced by the observed actions (Pleshko & Nickerson, 2008), or to explore external factors that affect decision making in particular contexts (Akyürek, Sawalha, & Ide, 2015), rather than to understand the strategic decisions as a function of the manager’s motivational or cognitive preferences. A manager’s approach to a complex decision will strongly depend on specific individual differences, which inevitably affects the decision process’ outcome. Strategic Academy of Strategic Management Journal Volume 16, Number 1, 2017 247
  • 13. management could certainly benefit from a more thorough understanding of the personal factors that affect strategic decisions, and, more specifically, from context-depending, prescriptive recommendations aimed at improving the effectiveness of strategic analytical tools. Such understanding requires a cross-disciplinary approach that combines a strategic perspective with contributions from other fields (Ronda-Pupo, 2015), such as social psychology, organizational behavior (OB) or operations research. Social psychology and OB, for instance, have investigated the underlying mechanisms that explain human decisions. Appealing to strategies that facilitate decision-making processes is inherent to the human nature. Rather than approaching a novel problem by means of sequential, systematic or structured mental processes, the human brain tends to use heuristics or shortcuts to analyze contextual information and make decisions (Kahneman, 2011; Kahneman & Tversky, 1979; Tversky & Kahneman, 1974, 1981, 1986). These facilitating strategies result in more “efficient” decision processes, especially in complex situations (Frederick, 2005; Witteman, van den Bercken, Claes, & Godoy, 2009). However, such facilitating strategies are not infallible, and there is abundant evidence that indicates that humans’ minds are prone to biases or traps that cloud objectivity when making decisions (Frederick, 2005; Kahneman, 2011). Within the context of decision and risk analysis, for instance,
  • 14. Montibeller and von Winterfeldt discuss the effects of cognitive and motivational biases and suggest possible ways to counteract them (2015). A cognitive bias is defined as the systematic discrepancy between the actual answer given by a person to a particular judgmental task and what would be considered the correct answer after applying a formal normative rule. Motivational biases, on the other hand, are more about the influence of people’s desirability or undesirability towards an event, consequence, outcome, or choice, in a particular judgmental task. Although both types of biases, cognitive or motivational, are likely to cloud objectivity when making decisions, this study focuses on the latter, given its prevalence and therefore likelihood to occur in business or strategic contexts. Confirmation bias is one of such motivational biases. It is conceptualized as a mental shortcut that simplifies complex analyses and arduous inferential tasks by appealing to strongly grounded beliefs (Friedrich, 1993; MacCoun, 1998; Wason, 1960). Such beliefs thus serve as a heuristic that makes evaluation of new information more expedite and efficient. With a caveat; because people assume that their existing beliefs are true, the confirmation bias often results in poor decisions, given that extant evidence is not considered thoroughly or in a balanced manner (Hernandez & Preston, 2013). On the other hand, desirability of choice, another motivational bias, occurs when a person desires a particular decision option, and, consequently, overestimates or underestimates relevant parameters in order to favor the desired option.
  • 15. In a business or strategic context, a person’s bias might result in decisions which reflect conscious or subconscious individual preferences (Kahneman, Lovallo, & Sibony, 2011), rather than seeking value maximization for the firm. Previous research, for instance, has explored the influence of CEOs’ and upper echelons’ preferences—especially as related to demographic characteristics—in shaping corporate strategy (Jensen & Zajac, 2004). The marketing literature provides an interesting framework to better grasp the potential influence of personal preferences on business decisions. Indeed, in consumer behavior settings, there is ample research on the dichotomy between hedonic and utilitarian goals, which affect consumers’ perceived value and hence their purchase decisions (Bridges & Florsheim, 2008; Jones, Reynolds, & Arnold, 2006; Overby & Lee, 2006; Sweeney & Soutar, 2001; Ulaga & Chacour, 2001). There is abundant evidence that the type of personal goal, hedonic or utilitarian, has a significant impact on a person’s purchase decisions. In addition, it is believed that a person’s motivational orientation for either Academy of Strategic Management Journal Volume 16, Number 1, 2017 248 hedonic or utilitarian goals tends to be quite stable through
  • 16. time. Therefore, it is reasonable to expect people to favor decision options that match their hedonic or utilitarian preferences, and therefore will privilege evidence supporting such motivational orientation. Moreover, extrapolating organizational behavior findings on the influence of personal orientations on organizational decisions (Polman, 2012), it can be argued that personal preferences—and hence biases—will influence not only consumption decisions, but also decisions in other business or strategic contexts. The influence of individual preferences and personal biases on people’s decisions has recently captured the attention of OR literature. For instance, it has been argued that cognitive and motivational biases (Franco, Rouwette, & Korzilius, 2016), as well as people’s individual differences (Franco & Hämäläinen, 2016; Hämäläinen et al., 2013; Montibeller & von Winterfeldt, 2015), can influence the effectiveness and practical value of arguably rational decision making tools. Consequently, individual differences and biases might be affecting the quality of the decision modelling process, the transparency of the resulting analyses, and therefore the effectiveness of the final decisions made. There is not much evidence, though, on the undermining effects of personal biases on strategic decisions supported by analytical tools, especially in actual organizational settings. To assess the potential influence of personal leanings on the quality of a decision, and, more generally, on the effectiveness of strategic decisions, in this paper we
  • 17. study the relationship between individual differences regarding hedonic or utilitarian preferences and confirmation and desirability of choice biases, and the resulting effect on decisions made with a multicriteria decision analysis tool. HYPOTHESES Building on the aforementioned antecedents and our revision of previous research on decision making, multicriteria analysis, and cognitive biases, we contend that MCDA is sensitive to both, confirmation and desirability biases. This is, when considering diverse options; people will tend to choose the option that matches their personal preferences, regardless of the decision technique or tool used. Moreover, we argue that such personal desire is greatly determined by the person’s motivational orientation, whether hedonic or utilitarian. Therefore, H1 (Motivational bias) There is a positive relationship between a person’s motivational orientation and this person’s decision. Furthermore, people who have a strong personal desire for a particular option will consider only those pieces of information that corroborate why the option should be chosen (confirmation bias) and will assign more priority to criteria that
  • 18. further favor that option (desirability of choice). Therefore, H2 (Confirmation bias) to make a decision, a person with a hedonic orientation will consider experiential criteria rather than performance criteria, whereas a person with a utilitarian orientation will consider performance criteria rather than experiential criteria. H3 (Desirability of choice bias) To make a decision, a person with a hedonic orientation will assign higher priority to experiential criteria, relative to performance criteria, whereas a person with a utilitarian orientation will consider performance criteria rather than experiential criteria. Academy of Strategic Management Journal Volume 16, Number 1, 2017 249 Finally, given previous findings on the positive relationship between desirability of an outcome and selection of relevant information, we anticipate
  • 19. that both biases, confirmation and desirability of choice reinforce each other and jointly influence people’s decisions. Therefore, H4 (Motivational interaction) the interaction between criteria considered and relative priority assigned to criteria, upon making a decision, will strengthen the positive relationship between a person’s motivational orientation and this person’s decision Figure 1 illustrates these hypotheses as components of a single, integrated model. Figure-1 HYPOTHESES OF THE EXPERIMENTAL STUDY METHODOLOGY To study the effects of the confirmation and desirability of choice biases on an MCDA- supported decision, we designed an experimental intervention where undergraduate and graduate students, acting as the decision-makers of a simple decision problem, employ MCDA to support their decision. The experiment looks at the effects that both biases have on the decision making process and the actual decision made. We designed our experiment as a vehicle purchase decision,
  • 20. under the assumption that undergraduate and graduate students would easily relate to such a familiar decision context. We infer that confirmation and desirability of choice biases will positively relate to a person’s orientation towards hedonism or utilitarianism and to their final purchase decision. Participants Participants included undergraduate (N=43) and graduate students (N=72) at a private university in Colombia. We specifically chose a class on OR techniques because of its relevance to the theses tested. The experiment was conducted during the multicriteria analysis module, embedded in one of the author’s regular classes. Participants were recruited at the beginning of the class using a verbal and written information sheet that told them about their right not to participate in the experiment. To fine-tune our experimental design, which is described below, we conducted three early pilot versions of the experiment using undergraduate (N=35) master students (N=45) during the previous semester. The questionnaires and tools and manipulation were thus assessed to come up with the final version for the experiment with graduate students. All questionnaires to measure the variables were administered using on-line Qualtrics software before and during the class time. Motivational orientation Criteria considered
  • 21. H4: confir Criteria priority H1: motivational bias mation x desirability (+) Decision Academy of Strategic Management Journal Volume 16, Number 1, 2017 250 Experimental design We contrasted individual motivational orientation, confirmation bias and desirability of choice bias with hypothetical purchase decisions. The complete experiment consisted on three subsequent stages that asked them to make simulated purchase decisions, following a class lecture on multicriteria analysis. To analyze our results, descriptive statistics, pairwise correlations and analysis of variance (ANOVA) were conducted using SAS statistical software. Note that common claims on the variability and effectiveness of MCDA results are related to its use either for individual or for group decision making
  • 22. (Schafer & Gallemore, 2016). This is, the use of MCDA and similar iterative methods in groups allow participants to mutually compare decisions, engage in dialogue, and discuss choices or weights, on a one-on-one or public manner. As a result, there is great potential for cross-contamination and distortion, and people might end up deciding on behalf of others’ preferences or biases, which makes it difficult to make generalizable or standardized inferences. To control for this issue, respondents read instructions, analyzed data, and made their decisions in individual computers. Also, to minimize interaction between participants during the actual experiment, they were instructed to not talk with their classmates and the instructor lead them through successive stages in an orderly sequence, and with a reasonably limited time for each stage. Stage 1 Following a lecture and corresponding material on MCDA and decision making, participants were asked to complete a questionnaire that assessed their motivational orientation. Then, they were given broad and limited descriptive specifications of the four purchase decision options and were asked to choose the one that they found more appealing (Decision 1). Motivational orientation (Predictor) Individual motivational orientation towards either hedonism or utilitarianism was assessed using the PERVAL 19-item scale developed to measure
  • 23. consumer perceived value (Sweeney & Soutar, 2001). Thus, participants’ were categorized according to their individual leanings on four distinct value dimensions: emotional, social, quality/performance and price/value for money. For our study, we termed quality/performance and price/value for money as utilitarian motives and emotional and social motives as hedonic motives. The participants completed the PERVAL questionnaire before the experiment started. Decision (Criterion) Participants’ decisions 1-3 were measured using custom questionnaires that asked them to choose between a limited set of purchase options, in a hypothetical car purchase decision. For their first decision (Decision1), four distinct car descriptions similar in price range and type of vehicle (respectively labeled A, B, C and D), were presented to the participants along with made- up specifications relevant to the purchase decision (see Table 1). Each vehicle option’s specifications were designed to emphasize either performance (Car A) or user-experience (Car B) or to present a balance between performance and user- experience attributes (Cars C and D). Academy of Strategic Management Journal Volume 16, Number 1, 2017
  • 24. 251 Table-1 EXPERIMENT STAGE 1: BROAD SPECIFICATIONS GIVEN FOR DECISION 1 Car A: Sophisticated style, elegant exterior design, luxury interior finishes, very good audio system and highly pleasurable driving. Car B: Maximum factory warranty, very powerful engine, optimal price-power, and maximum safety for passengers. Car C: Powerful engine, luxury interior finishes, extended factory warranty, driving pleasure and high security for passengers. Car D: Minimum fuel consumption, modern exterior design, good price-power, upgraded audio system and extended factory warranty. Stage 2 Participants were given more detailed, qualitative and quantitative, specifications of the four purchase decision options and were asked to choose the one that sounded more appealing to them (Decision 2). The information was presented in the form of a pay-off matrix where
  • 25. performance-related criteria was listed as factory-catalogue specs and experience-related criteria was presented as consumers’ self-reported assessment of their consumption/driving experience (Table 2). Criteria Used (Predictor) The specific evidence used by each participant to make a decision was used as a proxy for an individual’s confirmation bias towards performance or experience. To measure this variable, participants were asked during stage 2 to highlight what specific data cells they had inspected and used to make their second decision. This was done by means of a custom tool based on the made-up specs sheet shown in Table 2. With this tool, respondents indicated which criteria they used by clicking on the appropriate cells on the table.This helps us to identify if participants systematically overlooked or neglected parts of the information given in the matrix. Stage 3 Participants were asked to assign weight factors to the problem’s criteria by following the Swing weight method protocol (Goodwin & Wright, 2004) and then to introduce them into the HiView3 software (www.catalyze.co.uk) so they would be able to play with the problem’s data, which was already preset in the software. Thus, participants were asked to enter the weight factors into the software, look at the graphs generated and make a third
  • 26. and final choice (Decision 3). Criteria priority (predictor) The relative weights assigned by each participant were used as a proxy for an individual’s desirability of choice bias towards performance or experience. This was done by means of a custom tool based on the same made-up specs sheet shown in Table 2. This allows us not only to identify if participants learned something from their interplay with the OR tool that made them change their minds, but also see if participants systematically allocated larger weight factors to the criteria that favored the decision option they had in mind from the start. http://www.catalyze.co.uk/ Academy of Strategic Management Journal Volume 16, Number 1, 2017 252 Table-2 EXPERIMENT STAGE 2: DETAILED SPECIFICATIONS
  • 27. GIVEN FOR DECISION 2 Performance criteria (source: factory-catalogue) Experience criteria (source: consumers’ self- reported assessment of driving experience) Decisio n options Pric e ($ COP ) Powe r (HP) Price / torqu e ($ mill) Warran ty (Years ) Airba gs
  • 29. ed upper class Car B 61 300 $3.500 5 6 26 Basic Standard Basic 50 upper middle class Car C 65 300 $4.000 5 5 30 Standar d Luxury Full 70 upper middle class Car D 59 160 $3.700 4 3 36 Modern Standard Hi- Tech 60 lower middle class RESULTS A total of 43 undergraduate students and 72 graduate students completed both the individual difference questionnaire that assessed their motivational orientation and the subsequent surveys that asked them to make a decision based on either experience or performance criteria. The motivational orientation 19-item questionnaire, as well as the custom instruments applied to test the desirability of choice hypothesis (weights assigned to prioritize criteria), showed good
  • 30. internal reliability, with Cronbach’s alpha standardized values of .872 and .723, respectively. Table 3 presents the descriptive statistics and pair-wise, Pearson- product correlations between the measured variables. Table-3 DESCRIPTIVE STATISTICS AND PAIR-WISE CORRELATIONS Mean SD 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 1. Motivational 1.273 0.844 1 2. Decision 1 - 0.508 .259** 1 3. Decision 2 - 0.549 .106 .441*** 1 4. Decision 3 - 0.542 .174 .464*** .697*** 1 5. Criteria considered - 0.429 .398*** .445*** .520*** .528*** 1
  • 31. 6. Criteria priority 0.024 0.256 .328*** .465*** .604*** .684*** .590*** 1 7. Criteria favored 0.029 2.526 .175 .281** .395*** .502*** .438*** .425*** 1 Valid N (list) = 103; * p-value < 0.05 (2-tailed); ** p-value < 0.01 (2-tailed); *** p-value < 0.001 (2-tailed) To illustrate how successive decisions maintained (or not) the first decision made by the Academy of Strategic Management Journal Volume 16, Number 1, 2017 253 respondents, and how such decisions compared to the respondent’s motivational orientation, we plotted the graph shown in Figure 2. In total, 60% of the participants adhered to their first decision on subsequent decisions 2 and 3; 29% changed their decision once, and 14% changed twice. Also, as illustrated by the columns in Figure 2, respondents with a hedonic orientation leaned more towards an experience-based decision than respondents with a utilitarian orientation, across all three decisions.
  • 32. Figure-2 SEQUENCE OF DECISIONS 1-3, CATEGORIZED BY INDIVIDUAL MOTIVATIONAL ORIENTATION (HEDONIC VS. UTILITARIAN) AND FIRST DECISION MADE (PERFORMANCE, NEUTRAL OR EXPERIENCE). 1 Utilitarian orientation Performance option Hedonistic orientation 0 Decisions 1 Performance Decision 1 Neutral Experiance option Decision 1 Decision 2 Decision 3 Decision 1 -1 Experience
  • 33. To further assess the hypothesized biases, in Figure 3 we analyzed the criteria used by respondents, categorized by their motivational orientation, when making their decision using the made-up specifications data. Criteria considered refers to the criteria selected to make their decisions from the made-up specs table; criteria order refers to the order assigned to the selected criteria by the respondents; and criteria priority refers to the relative weights assigned by the respondents to all available criteria in the specs table. Figure-3 EVIDENCE OF COGNITIVE BIAS, CATEGORIZED BY INDIVIDUAL MOTIVATIONAL ORIENTATION (HEDONIC VS. UTILITARIAN) AND FIRST DECISION MADE (PERFORMANCE, NEUTRAL OR EXPERIENCE). 2 Utilitarian orientation Performance Criteria 1 Hedonistic orientation 0 Decision 1 performance
  • 34. Experience Criteria 1 Decision 1 neutral -2 Decision 1 Experience Criteria Considered Criteria order Criteria Priority Academy of Strategic Management Journal Volume 16, Number 1, 2017 254 To further test our hypotheses, including our interaction hypothesis (H4), we conducted an analysis of variance (ANOVA) regressing motivational orientation, criteria used and criteria priority, as independent variables, on the mean of decisions made, as the dependent variable. That is, we propose that the relationship between individual differences, biases and final decision can be expressed by the equation in (1): DECISION = ����0 + ����1�������� + ����3�������� + ����4�������� + ����5(�������� ∗ ��������) + ���� (1) Where MO is motivational orientation (H1), CC is the criteria
  • 35. considered (H2), CP is the criteria priority (H3) and CC*CP is the interaction between both biases (H4). Table-4 ANOVA - GLM PROCEDURE Class-level information Class Levels Values Criterion priority 2 -1.00, 1.00 Observations read 117 Onbservations used 107 Dependent variable: Decision Source DF Sum of squares Mean square F-value Pr > F Model 4 10.7224 2.6806 25.8400 <.0001 Error 102 10.5798 0.1037 Corrected Total 106 21.3022 R-squared Var coeff MSE root Decision mean 0.5033 -240.4226 0.3221 -0.1340 Source DF Type I SS Mean square F-value Pr > F Motivational orientation 1 0.8945 0.8945 8.6200 0.0041 Criteria priority 1 6.3341 6.3341 61.0700 <.0001 z-criteria considered 1 2.9184 2.9184 28.1400 <.0001 Crit priority*z-crit cons 1 0.5753 0.5753 5.5500 0.0204 Source DF Type III SS Mean square F-value Pr > F Motivational orientation 1 0.0518 0.0518 0.5000 0.4814 Criteria priority 1 2.6876 2.6876 25.9100 <.0001
  • 36. z-criteria considered 1 1.9147 1.9147 18.4600 <.0001 Crit priority*z-crit cons 1 0.5753 0.5753 5.5500 0.0204 Minimum mean suqares at z-criteria considered = -1 SD Criteria priority Decision LS mean H0: LSmean1=LSmean2 t value Pr > |t| -1 -0.5334 -4.8300 <.0001 1 0.0071 Minimum mean suqares at z-criteria considered = +1 SD Criteria priority Decision LS mean H0: LSmean1=LSmean2 t value Pr > |t| -1 -0.01667 -2.02 0.0459 1 0.17353 A dichotomous dummy variable was computed for each individual decision depending on the type of choice made (performance choice = 1, experience choice = -1), and the resulting values were then averaged to obtain an estimate of the respondents’ mean decisions (i.e., their individual Academy of Strategic Management Journal Volume 16, Number 1, 2017 255 leanings towards a performance or an experience option). MO,
  • 37. as measured by the PERVAL 19- item scale, is a continuous measure. CC was estimated by adding the total number of performance- type criteria selected and subtracting the total experience-type criteria selected. Similarly, CP was estimated by adding total weight assigned to performance-type criteria and subtracting total weight assigned to experience-type criteria. To compare results at high (performance) and low (experience) values of criteria priority, CP was dichotomized about its median. To plot results at high (performance) and low (experience) values of criteria used, a standardized score for CC was computed as a function of standard deviations about 0 (zCC). Table 4 presents the ANOVA results. From the ANOVA output, Figure 4 plots the dependent variable at zCC = -1 SD for experience criteria and zCC = +1 SD for performance criteria (i.e., standardized criteria considered measure at -/+ 1 standard deviations). Figure-4 EFFECT OF CRITERIA CONSIDERED AND CRITERIA PRIORITY (EXPERIENCE VS. PERFORMANCE) ON DECISION, STANDARDIZED (N=109, P<.001 FOR CC*CP). DISCUSSION Overall, the results provide support for our hypotheses. As shown in Table 3, the positive
  • 38. correlations between the motivational orientation and the first, essentially intuitive decision, suggests that individuals tend to make decisions based on their personal leanings, thus supporting hypothesis 1. Likewise, the positive correlations between subsequent decisions 1-3 provide additional support to this notion as it suggests that people tend to adhere to the first decision they make, across different conditions or evidence. On the other hand, the positive correlations between the decisions made and both the criteria selected to make the decision and the weights assigned to decision criteria, support the confirmation (H2) and desirability of choice (H3) hypotheses. This is, participants’ decisions towards either performance or experience options were consistent with the criteria they used to support their decisions and the relative importance they assigned to such criteria. Finally, the positive correlation between criteria used and criteria priority suggests that these two personal biases might reinforce each other and together determine a person’s decision. Altogether, the pairwise correlations support the notion that people tend to adhere to their first decision, which relates with subjective personal preferences, even after being confronted with additional evidence or analytical tools. Academy of Strategic Management Journal Volume 16, Number 1, 2017
  • 39. 256 The correlations are supplemented by the graphs shown in Figure 2 and Figure 3, which show how participants, categorized by their first decision (performance, neutral or experience option), tend to maintain their initial choice. This figure also highlight how respondents with a hedonic orientation lean more towards an experience-based decision than respondents with a utilitarian orientation, across all three decisions. These results further support hypothesis 1. Also, as shown in Figure 3, results suggest a correspondence between individual decisions and both, the criteria used to make the decision and the order assigned to each criterion used (hypothesis 2). Also, that both the decision and the criteria selected are directionally consistent with the relative weight assigned to all available criteria (hypothesis 3). This is, as illustrated by the line graphs, respondents who initially choose a performance-type option tend to use only performance-based evidence and to assign a higher weight to performance data, relative to user- experience data. Conversely, respondents who opt for an experience-type option at the beginning tend to use only experience-based evidence and to assign a higher weight to user-experience data, relative to performance data. Results are consistent across motivational orientation, as utilitarian participants preferred and assigned more weight to performance criteria, whereas hedonic participants preferred and assigned more weight to user- experience criteria. Although the contrast
  • 40. is especially clear for criteria priority, as measured by relative weights assigned, the results are overall directionally consistent with our hypotheses. Taken together, these results support hypotheses 2 and 3. The ANOVA yields additional support to our hypotheses, with a significant model (F=25.84, p<.0001), and significant effects both for the independent variables zCC and CP (p<.001 in each case) and for their interaction (p<.05). As shown in Table 4, the results are directionally consistent with our basic and interaction hypotheses, as people who choose the performance option tend to consider and to prioritize performance criteria, rather than user-experience criteria, and conversely for those who choose the experience option. Also, the different slopes and intersecting curves suggest an interaction effect, which supports our hypothesis 4. The difference between people who prioritize performance criteria (CP = +1) and those who prioritize experience criteria (CP = -1), however, is clearly stronger for those who consider experience-related criteria (zCC = -1 SD) than for those who consider performance-related criteria (zCC = +1SD), which suggests intriguing implications. The stronger effect might suggest that people who are prone to experiences tend to make more intuitive decisions, compared to people leaning towards performance. Conversely, individuals who tend to consider or prioritize objective, performance-based evidence, might rely more on analytical procedures and objective evidence in general and therefore be less prone to cognitive biases, relative to experience-prone people (Frederick, 2005; Witteman et al.,
  • 41. 2009). CONCLUSIONS Results suggest that decision makers tend to make intuitive decisions consistent with their motivational orientation, and that they are inclined to adhere to such first intuitive decisions despite getting access to new supporting evidence or additional analytical tools. In fact, participants apparently utilized MCDA to confirm previously conceived decisions, rather than to identify the best possible solution for them. In other words, participants appear to have actually sought support for a decision they have already made in their own minds, rather than striving to attain a more thoughtful, better quality decision. It should be noted that MCDA is not supposed to be a normative tool that provides a Academy of Strategic Management Journal Volume 16, Number 1, 2017 257 “correct” answer, but rather an insightful approach that helps decision makers learn and make a more informed choice. Notwithstanding, our results shed doubts on the participants’ actual
  • 42. learning from MCDA and whether the tool really added value to the decision process, since little learning seems to have happened throughout their interplay with the tool. One would expect that if any learning takes place as a result of the participants’ interaction with the MCDA tool, a significant number of them would have changed their minds throughout the three stages of the experiment. However, the evidence suggests that, regardless of the first option chosen, the number of participants adhering to their initial impressions was larger than those who changed their perception along the experiment. Alternatively, even if we accept the proposition that MCDA is devised to reflect its users’ preferences (and help them learn about them), it seems reasonable to ask what distinguishes a valid preference-based decision from a not-so-valid, biased decision. In sum, it appears that people’s individual differences regarding hedonic and utilitarian motivational orientation can lead them to motivational biases, which in turn may affect the effectiveness of their use of the MCDA tool. More generally, the effectiveness of strategic decision support tools is likely affected by the managers’ leanings and biases. Strategic decision making should be understood as a human-machine system, composed both by structured and unstructured components (Shim et al., 2002). Although decision-making tools can enhance the efficiency of structured information processing and analysis, the unstructured part of a strategic decision is still subject to the managers’ leanings and preferences. Therefore, the objectivity of many strategic decision that rely on analytical models or tools is doubtful, at
  • 43. most. This said, we do not intend to imply that decision support tools are not useful, or even essential, in modern strategic decision making. We rather advocate for a grounded, cross-disciplinary approach that better understands and addresses such tools’ limitations. It follows that future research should further explore the mechanisms underlying strategic decisions supported by analytical tools, in actual organizational contexts, in order to propose prescriptive recommendations for management practitioners. Our results also suggest that motivational biases might differ across decision makers depending on other individual differences, beyond personal prevailing preferences, such as their level of rationality or intuition (Frederick, 2005; Witteman et al., 2009). Future research is indeed needed in this area (Franco et al., 2016), so in our future studies we would like to explore such nuances and try to better grasp the underlying mechanisms that explain individual differences, and attempt to predict which types of people are more or less prone to motivational biases and perhaps advance custom analytical approaches for each category. Also, future research should involve the exploration of strategies to counteract or de-bias these effects. Finally, future research should also strive to increase the generalizability of these results. Our findings might not hold for other contexts, given the particular context of the experiment, with students as respondents and a simulated purchase decision as experimental scenario. Previous research suggests that such an artificial setting, involving a
  • 44. personal consumption choice, might favor intuitive decisions, whereas a more professional situation, with real-world consequences, would motivate more analytical approaches (Sjöberg, 2003). Future research should therefore try to replicate and extend these findings in an organizational or managerial setting, and involving more strategic or business-specific decisions. Moreover, future investigations should likely be of a cross-disciplinary nature, such as the present paper, thus benefiting the strategy literature with contributions from other fields (Ronda-Pupo, 2015). Should our results hold in more varied and generalizable settings, it would appear that a totally rational decision making is not feasible. Which, in turn, would suggest that it is utopic to expect that management and strategic planning can be completely objective, analytical, tool- Academy of Strategic Management Journal Volume 16, Number 1, 2017 258 supported, evidence-based processes. A more thorough understanding of their limitations, however, would certainly enhance the utility and applicability of analytical approaches, in real- world, real-people contexts. REFERENCES
  • 45. Akyürek ÇE, Sawalha R & Ide S (2015). Factors Affecting the Decision Making Process in Healthcare Institutions. Academy of Strategic Management Journal, 14, 1. Bridges E & Florsheim R (2008). Hedonic and utilitarian shopping goals: The online experience. Journal of Business Research 61(4), 309-314. doi:http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jbusres.2007.06.017 Busenitz LW & Barney JB (1997). Differences between entrepreneurs and managers in large organizations: Biases and heuristics in strategic decision-making. Journal of business venturing, 12(1), 9-30. Busenitz LW, West GP, Shepherd D, Nelson T, Chandler GN & Zacharakis A (2003). Entrepreneurship research in emergence: Past trends and future directions. Journal of management, 29(3), 285-308. Capps III CJ & Cassidy CM (2016). Expanding the Competitive Profile Matrix (Cpm): Introducing the Financial Competitive Profile Matrix (Fcpm). Academy of Strategic Management Journal, 15(2), 9. Das T & Teng BS (1999). Cognitive biases and strategic decision processes: An integrative perspective. Journal of management studies, 36(6), 757-778. Franco LA & Hämäläinen RP (2016). Behavioural operational research: returning to the roots of the OR profession. Eur J Oper Res, 249(3), 791-795. Franco LA, Rouwette EA & Korzilius H (2016). Different paths to consensus? The impact of need for closure on
  • 46. model-supported group conflict management. European Journal of Operational Research, 249(3), 878- 889. Frederick S (2005). Cognitive reflection and decision making. The Journal of Economic Perspectives, 19(4), 25- 42. Freiberg J (2004). How gutsy leaders blow the doors off business ‐as‐ usual. Leader to Leader, 2004(33), 32-37. Friedrich J (1993). Primary error detection and minimization (PEDMIN) strategies in social cognition: A reinterpretation of confirmation bias phenomena. Psychological Review, 100(2), 298. Goodwin P & Wright G (2004). Decision analysis for management judgement. Chicester: Wiley & Sons. Hämäläinen RP, Luoma J & Saarinen E (2013). On the importance of behavioral operational research: The case of understanding and communicating about dynamic systems. European Journal of Operational Research, 228(3), 623-634. Hernandez I & Preston JL (2013). Disfluency disrupts the confirmation bias. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 49(1), 178-182. Jensen M & Zajac EJ (2004). Corporate elites and corporate strategy: How demographic preferences and structural position shape the scope of the firm. Strategic Management Journal, 25(6), 507-524. Jones MA, Reynolds KE & Arnold MJ (2006). Hedonic and utilitarian shopping value: Investigating differential effects on retail outcomes. Journal of Business Research, 59(9),
  • 47. 974-981. doi:http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jbusres.2006.03.006 Kahneman D (2011). Thinking, fast and slow: Macmillan. Kahneman D, Lovallo D & Sibony O (2011). Before you make that big decision. Harvard Business Review, 89(6), 50-60. Kahneman D & Tversky A (1979). Prospect Theory: An Analysis of Decision under Risk. Econometrica, 47(2), 263-291. Keyt A (2003). Decisions, Decisions. Myths and Mortals: Family Business Leadership and Succession Planning, 135-152. Kiron D, Ferguson RB & Prentice PK (2013). From value to vision: Reimagining the possible with data analytics. MIT Sloan Management Review, 54(3), 1. MacCoun RJ (1998). Biases in the interpretation and use of research results. Annual Review of Psychology, 49(1), 259-287. Montibeller G & von Winterfeld D (2015). Biases and debiasing in multi-criteria decision analysis. Paper presented at the Proceedings of the 48th Hawaii International Conference on System Sciences. Kauai, Hawaii: IEEE Computer Society. Montibeller G & von Winterfeldt D (2015). Cognitive and Motivational Biases in Decision and Risk Analysis. Risk http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jbusres.2007.06.017 http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jbusres.2006.03.006
  • 48. Academy of Strategic Management Journal Volume 16, Number 1, 2017 259 Analysis. Overby JW & Lee EJ (2006). The effects of utilitarian and hedonic online shopping value on consumer preference and intentions. Journal of Business Research, 59(10–11), 1160- 1166. doi:http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jbusres.2006.03.008 Pleshko L & Nickerson I (2008). Strategic orientation, organizational structure, and the associated effects on performance in industrial firms. Academy of Strategic Management Journal, 7, 95. Polman E (2012). Self–other decision making and loss aversion. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 119(2), 141-150. Rigby D & Bilodeau B (2007). Selecting Management Tools Wisely. Harvard Business Review, 85(12), 20-22. Rigby D & Bilodeau B (2015). Management Tools & Trends 2015. London, Bain & Company. Ronda-Pupo, G. A. (2015). Growth and Consolidation of Strategic Management Research: Insights for the Future Development of Strategic Management. Academy of Strategic Management Journal, 14(2), 155. Schafer JG & Gallemore CT (2016). Biases in multicriteria
  • 49. decision analysis: The case of environmental planning in Southern Nevada. Environment and Planning C: Government and Policy, 0263774X16629675. Shim JP, Warkentin M, Courtney JF, Power DJ, Sharda R & Carlsson C (2002). Past, present, and future of decision support technology. Decision support systems, 33(2), 111- 126. Simon HA (1955). A behavioral model of rational choice. The quarterly journal of economics, 99-118. Simon HA (1979). Rational Decision Making in Business Organizations. The American Economic Review, 69(4), 493-513. Sjöberg L (2003). Intuitive vs. analytical decision making: which is preferred? Scandinavian Journal of Management, 19(1), 17-29. Stewart TJ, French S & Rios J (2013). Integrating multicriteria decision analysis and scenario planning—Review and extension. Omega, 41(4), 679-688. Sweeney JC & Soutar GN (2001). Consumer perceived value: The development of a multiple item scale. Journal of Retailing, 77(2), 203-220. doi:http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0022- 4359(01)00041-0 Tversky A & Kahneman D (1974). Judgment under uncertainty: Heuristics and biases. Science, 185(4157), 1124- 1131. Tversky A & Kahneman D (1981). The framing of decisions and the psychology of choice. Science, 211(4481), 453-458. Tversky A & Kahneman D (1986). Rational choice and the
  • 50. framing of decisions. Journal of Business, S251-S278. Ulaga W & Chacour S (2001). Measuring Customer-Perceived Value in Business Markets: A Prerequisite for Marketing Strategy Development and Implementation. Industrial Marketing Management, 30(6), 525-540. doi:http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0019-8501(99)00122- 4 Wason PC (1960). On the failure to eliminate hypotheses in a conceptual task. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 12(3), 129-140. Witteman C, van den Bercken J, Claes L & Godoy A (2009). Assessing rational and intuitive thinking styles. European Journal of Psychological Assessment, 25(1), 39-47. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jbusres.2006.03.008 http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0022-4359(01)00041-0 http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0022-4359(01)00041-0 http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0019-8501(99)00122-4 http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0019-8501(99)00122-4CAN MANAGERS BE REALLY OBJECTIVE?ABSTRACTINTRODUCTIONANTECEDENTSHY POTHESESFigure-1HYPOTHESES OF THE EXPERIMENTAL STUDYStage 1Stage 2Stage 3RESULTSFigure-2SEQUENCE OF DECISIONS 1-3, CATEGORIZED BY INDIVIDUAL MOTIVATIONAL ORIENTATION (HEDONIC VS. UTILITARIAN) AND FIRST DECISION MADE (PERFORMANCE, NEUTRAL OR EXPERIENCE).Figure- 3EVIDENCE OF COGNITIVE BIAS, CATEGORIZED BY INDIVIDUAL MOTIVATIONAL ORIENTATION (HEDONIC VS. UTILITARIAN) AND FIRST DECISION MADE (PERFORMANCE, NEUTRAL OR EXPERIENCE).Figure- 4EFFECT OF CRITERIA CONSIDERED AND CRITERIA PRIORITY (EXPERIENCE VS. PERFORMANCE) ON
  • 51. DECISION, STANDARDIZED (N=109, P<.001 FOR CC*CP).CONCLUSIONSREFERENCES