SlideShare a Scribd company logo
1 of 385
Colorado V. Connelly
479 U.S 157 (1986)
Facts:
· Connelly approached a police officer and showed interest on
confessing about a murder that he committed.
· Connelly had a history of mental illness and had been of his
medication for at least 6 months.
· Before Connelly made a confession, the officer gave Connelly
his Miranda warnings and called a detective that before
interrogating him, read him again his Miranda warnings.
· Connelly acknowledge the understanding of his Miranda rights
and confessed that he murder a young girl.
· The next morning Connelly was disoriented and claimed that
voices made him confess to the murder.
· Connelly then was diagnosed by a psychiatrist with
schizophrenia and wanted to suppress the use of his statements
in court because his psychotic condition made him confess but
he was able to understand his Miranda rights.
Procedural History:
· Supreme court of Colorado suppressed the evidence
· U.S Supreme Court
Law:
· “No person shall be held to answer for a capital, or otherwise
infamous crime, unless on a presentment or indictment of a
Grand Jury, except in cases arising in the land or naval forces,
or in the Militia, when in actual service in time of War or public
danger; nor shall any person be subject for the same offense to
be twice put in jeopardy of life or limb; nor shall be compelled
in any criminal case to be a witness against himself, nor be
deprived of life, liberty, or property, without due process of
law; nor shall private property be taken for public use, without
just compensation.”
-5th amendment
· All persons born or naturalized in the United States, and
subject to the jurisdiction thereof, are citizens of the United
States and of the state wherein they reside. No state shall make
or enforce any law which shall abridge the privileges or
immunities of citizens of the United States; nor shall any state
deprive any person of life, liberty, or property, without due
process of law; nor deny to any person within its jurisdiction
the equal protection of the laws.
-14th amendment
Legal Question:
· Can Connelly’s statement be suppressed, since he made it in a
state of mind in which he was not aware of the situation and the
consequences?
Holding and vote: No (7-2)
Reasoning: (Rehnquist, majority) joined by White, Blackmun,
Powell, Stevens, O’Connor and scalia.
1. The admissibility of statements is governed by state rules of
evidence, rather than previous supreme court decisions
regarding coerced confessions and Miranda waivers.
2. The officers did not use any questionable methods to get a
confession from Connelly, in fact they told him that he had the
right to remain silent, yet he proceeded to confess.
3. Similar cases in which statements were suppressed due to the
defendant’s mental state is because the officers exploited their
mental health to get a confession. I.e Blackburn v. Alabama,
361 U. S. 199 (1960), and Townsend v. Sain, 372 U. S. 293
(1963)
4. The due process clause of the 5th and 14th amendment was
not violated because the defendant did everything voluntarily
and somewhat knowingly because schizophrenia does not
impairs comprehension.
5. Voluntary in the eyes of the law means, not being influenced
by an external force, in this case the defendant was not
influenced by any police officer.
Concurring Opinion: (Blackmun and Stevens)
· Justice Blackmun agrees with everything, except with part III
A, where it was talked about the defendant waived his Miranda
rights. He believes that it was unnecessary.
· Justice Stevens believes that Connelly’s statement were
involuntary. However the fifth amendment was not violated,
therefore, it is not required to suppress the evidence gathered by
the officers.
Dissenting opinion: (Justice Brennan joined by Marshall)
· Justice Brennan believe that the defendant did not made this
confessions in the right state of mind because of his condition
and his withdrawal from medication for a prolonged period of
time. He also believe that his hallucination had some effect in
his ability to comprehend his Miranda rights. The absence of
police misconduct does not necessarily means that the
confessions was made on free will. The court has reversed
similar cases in which a person confessed to a crime due to
medication (Townsend v. Sain,372 U. S. 293, 1963). Allowing
to include mentally ill individual to voluntary confession is an
error because more often than not this confessions are going to
be unreliable
Volume 4 1 /Number 3/July 2019/Pages 242-259/https: 11
ioi.org! IO.I7744/mehc.4l .3.04
NEUROCOUNSELING
Fostering Intrinsic Resilience: A
Neuroscience-Informed Model of
Conceptualizing and Treating Adverse
Childhood Experiences
Eraina Schauss
Greg Horn
Frances Ellmo
Tegan Reeves
Haley Z e ttle r
Debra Bartelli
Pam Cogdal
Steven West
An increasingly common dialogue among mental health
professionals revolves around adverse
childhood experiences (ACEs) and developmental trauma. ACEs
can occur in a number of ways
with a myriad of potential outcomes, often making treatment
choices difficult. During critical
stages of neurodevelopmental growth, trauma makes a mark on
the brain and body at a physi-
ological level. Although the National Institute of Mental
Health’s Research Domain Criteria
have been used to address this brain-body impact, the far-
reaching scope of implications needs
grounding in a theoretical framework. The current paper
discusses developmental trauma and
proposes a new reciprocally determinant model that advocates
for neuroscience-informed coun-
seling interventions such as neurofeedhack therapy. Clinical
implications and considerations
for counselors are discussed.
Eraina Schauss, Department o f Counseling, Educational
Psychology and Research, The University o f
Memphis: Greg Horn, Department o f Counseling, Educational
Psychology and Research, The University o f
Memphis: Frances Ellmo, Department o f Counseling,
Educational Psychology and Research, The University
o f Memphis: Tegan R e e ve s, Department o f Counseling.
Educational Psychology and Research, The
University o f Memphis: Haley Zettler, Department o f
Criminology and Criminal Justice, The University o f
Memphis: Debra Bartelli, School o f Public Health, The
University o f Memphis: Pam Cogdal, Department o f
Counseling. Educational Psychology and Research, The
University o f Memphis: Steven West, Department
o f Counseling, Educational Psychology and Research. The
University o f Memphis.
This research is supported by a Memphis Research Consortium
Grant (Grant #2 3 2 0 0 0 ) to the University
o f Memphis in partnership with the University o f Tennessee H
ealth Science Center. This article also
acknowledges Kristi Nobbman in the conceptual development o
f the reciprocally determinant b ra in -
body model.
Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to
Eraina Schauss, Department o f
Counseling, Educational Psychology and Research, Patterson H
all Room 123, The University o f Memphis,
Memphis, TN 38152. E-mail: [email protected]
242 0 Journal o f Mental Health Counseling
mailto:[email protected]
F o s t e r in g I n t r i n s i c R e s ilie n c e
Mental health professionals increasingly seek information on
approaches
to childhood maltreatment in terms of the prevention and
treatment of trau-
matic response. Counselors seeking applicable information
often find theories
and approaches siloed within specialties and fields of practice.
For instance,
social, interpersonal, and biological theories each provide
useful explanations,
yet are limited in explaining how such variables interact. The
model we present
provides insight into the concept of developmental trauma by
examining the
reciprocal interaction of clients’ environment, behavior, and
personal factors
and a regulatory self-system (Bandura, 1978) that includes
biological and neu-
rological processes. This theoretical framework aims to bridge
important fields
of research including developmental psychology and
neuroscience.
ADVERSE CHILDHOOD EXPERIENCES
AND DEVELOPMENTAL TRAUMA
Adverse childhood experiences (ACEs) include abuse
(emotional, phys-
ical, and sexual), household challenges (e.g., witnessing parent-
to-parent
violence, household substance abuse, mental illness in
household, parental
separation or divorce, incarcerated household member), and
emotional and
physical neglect (Felitti et ah, 1998). Since Felitti et al. (1998)
found in their
seminal study that ACEs were linked to chronic illness in
adulthood and early
morbidity, research examining the profound impact of ACEs has
become a
priority for the Centers for Disease Control and Prevention
(2018) and other
research organizations. In the past two decades, this extensive
field of research
has established early childhood maltreatment as a significant
etiological factor
for a wide range of health and psychological conditions. A
comprehensive
review of this literature is beyond the scope of this article;
however, to summa-
rize, children and adolescents who experience a high number of
ACEs tend to
manifest an increased num ber of psychiatric conditions
(Giaconia et al., 1995)
and a multitude of behavioral and health-related complications
throughout
their life span (Schilling, Aseltine, & Gore, 2007). Research
also indicates
that ACEs are more common among youth with juvenile justice
involvement
(Zettler, Wolff, Baglivio, Craig, & Epps, 2017). The long-term
physiological
response to trauma appears to link ACEs and chronic health
problems (van der
Kolk, 1994). Specifically, a dose effect was found linking
greater ACE exposure
in children and adolescents with greater instances of obesity,
diabetes, heart
disease, maladaptive sleep patterns, autoimmune disorders, and
inflammation,
ultimately leading to a lifetime of chronic physical and mental
illness (Felitti
et al., 1998).
Felitti et al. (1998) further theorized a causal link between
ACEs and early
death mediated by three categories of factors in a linear fashion.
According to
0 Journal o f Mental Health Counseling 243
their model, ACEs lead to social, emotional, and cognitive
impairment, which,
in turn, leads to the adoption of health-risk behaviors, often
resulting in dis-
ease, disability, and ultimately early death. This explanation,
which admirably
accounts for social and psychological factors, represents the
medical model,
which tends to identify (and treat) the root cause of pathology
in a linear
fashion. The medical model is the long-standing dominant
model in health
care (Engel, 1977), has many strengths, and is clearly an
appropriate model for
understanding the factors that lead to early death.
However, the medical model also has significant limitations,
especially
when applied to dynamic social and interpersonal processes. For
instance, due
to the assumption of cause and effect, when applied to ACEs,
the medical
model can imply that “what is done is done” and suggest a sense
of hopeless-
ness to clients and a subsequently defeatist and challenging
position for coun-
selors. In addition, the scale used to measure ACEs assigns the
same weight
to each type of adverse experience (Felitti et ah, 1998).
However, research
shows that not all childhood maltreatment has an equal effect;
some adverse
experiences are more traumatizing than others (Schwartz &
Perry, 1994), and
the degree of long-term traumatic effects can depend on
subjective mediating
factors such as loneliness (Shelvin, McElroy, & Murphy, 2015)
or social sup-
port (Allbaugh et ah, 2018). The medical model does little to
acknowledge the
subjective experience of an individual. In addition, the ACE
scale is additive,
as researchers have argued that they are interested in the
cumulative effect of
ACEs (Thompson, Richards, & Gaysina, 2017). However, an
additive measure
of adverse experience does not acknowledge possible
interaction effects. In
such instances, 1 + 1 does not necessarily equal 2 when adverse
experiences
compound one another and have an exponentially greater
negative effect for
the child. For example, consider a child whose father is
incarcerated and who
is physically abused by his overly stressed single mother, with
no other adult
support to help cope with the physical abuse. Clearly, adverse
experiences can
have an effect on each other, with the resulting impact being
greater than the
sum of its parts. Finally, the medical model does not address
how previous
adverse experiences can make individuals vulnerable to future
adverse expe-
riences. For instance, failure to identify traumatic response
behaviors as such
can exacerbate intense emotions and lead to application of
stigmatizing labels
such as oppositional, rebellious, unmotivated, or antisocial (van
der Kolk, 2005,
p. 404), which can have long-term effects on self-concept
(Rockett, Murrie, &
Boccaccini, 2007).
244 0 Journal o f Mental H ealth Counseling
Fostering In trin s ic Resilience
VIEWING ADVERSE CHILDHOOD EXPERIENCES
THROUGH A
RECIPROCAL DETERMINANT LENS
Social, interpersonal, and attachm ent theories have offered
explanations
for the sequelae of childhood maltreatment and have addressed
many of the
issues the medical model could not (Siegel, 2012; van der Kolk,
2015). The
concept of reciprocal determinism from Bandura’s social
learning theory, in
our view, provides a particularly useful means by which to
address the complex-
ity of issues that arise from ACEs. Bandura (1978) postulated
the self system
within his reciprocal determinism model to describe cognitive
mechanisms
by which the individual uses perception and assessment of past
or potential
future experience to regulate behavior. This process was
theorized to involve
bidirectional relationships among behavior, person, and
environment. Thus,
the self system is used as an analytical self-regulatory process
and describes the
psychosocial interactions within the individual (Bandura, 1978).
Whereas the medical model is limited in that it does not account
for
reciprocal influences, Bandura’s model is limited in that it does
little to address
physical and neurological processes in the body. In this paper
we propose
an expansion of a theoretical model that incorporates an
understanding of
neurological process in the context of developmental trauma and
ACEs. O ur
model will be discussed in detail, and implications for clinical
practice will be
presented.
RESEARCH DOMAIN CRITERIA
An impetus for change in understanding and conceptualizing
develop-
mental trauma is the rise of the Research Domain Criteria
(RDoC) project, a
National Institute of Mental Health (NIMH) movement to
develop research
frameworks incorporating neurobiological, genetic, behavioral,
and cognitive
influences on the classification of mental disorders. The RDoC
initiative
emphasizes neural circuitry, levels of arousal, regulatory
systems, social pro-
cesses, cognitions, developmental trajectories, and the dynamic
interaction
between an individual and their environment. The rise of the
RDoC move-
m ent emphasizes the desire for a precision medicine approach,
“an emerging
approach for disease treatment and prevention that takes into
account individ-
ual variability in genes, environment and lifestyle for each
person” (National
Institutes of Health, as cited in Adams & Petersen, 2016, p.
787), to mental
health treatment.
The RDoC paradigm shift comes at a critical time in mental
health
care. RDoC seeks to foster a more precise way of
conceptualizing and treating
m ental disorders that will transform treatment and public health
outcomes
0 Journal o f Mental Health Counseling 245
through four main objectives. The first strategic objective of
RDoC is to define
the mechanisms of complex behavior. The second objective is to
identify and
characterize sensitive periods of brain development across the
life span with a
goal of determining “the points in time during which the brain
is most sensitive
to intervention and the underlying molecular and system-level
mechanisms
responsible for this sensitivity” (NIMH, 2015, p. 32). The third
strategic
objective of RDoC concerns the areas of treatment, prevention,
and cures.
The objective seeks to integrate neuroscience, genomics, and
evidence-based
behavioral science methodologies into more precise treatments.
Additionally, a
need exists to develop new interventions and tailor current
treatments to indi-
vidualized symptoms. The fourth and final RDoC objective is to
strengthen the
public health impact of NIMH-supported research (NIMH,
2015).
The current ideological shift is to understand and integrate basic
neuro-
science findings into evidence-based clinical practice and
interventions with
the ultimate goal of delivering the most accurate diagnosis at
the earliest point,
followed by precise and individualized treatments. To pave the
way for a pre-
cision medicine approach in mental health counseling,
additional diagnostic
evaluations must be incorporated into treatment, which examine
symptoms,
behaviors, cognition, genotype, physiology, environmental
exposures, cultural
background, and family dynamics (NIMH, 2015). As such, the
task of answer-
ing the call of the RDoC must include counselors and not solely
medical
professionals and scientists.
Our unique counseling perspective brings a much-needed focus
on holis-
tic wellness, individualized treatment, and social justice
considerations and
provides the opportunity to change the way other helping
professions concep-
tualize mental health.
INTEGRATING NEUROSCIENCE AND COUNSELING
Given the RDoC emphasis on precision medicine, neuroscience
is a
logical field for mental health counselors to embrace in order to
understand
the complex mechanisms underlying mental health disorders.
The field of
neurocounseling, the “integration of neuroscience into the
practice of coun-
seling” (Russell-Chapin, 2016, p. 93), although in its infancy,
has already shed
new light on how counselors conceptualize and treat clients.
Neuroscience
findings have profound implications for the field of counseling
and provide a
heuristic for counselors to use evidence-based treatments with
clients with a
history of ACEs (Navalta, McGee, & Underwood, 2018).
Neuroscientists have
identified forms of psychosocial engagement, such as nurturing
and caring,
healthy behaviors, and environmental enrichment, that promote
neuroplasti-
246 ^ Journal o f Mental Health Counseling
Fostering Intrinsic Resilience
city (Gonqalves & Perrone-McGovern, 2014), the brain’s ability
to reorganize
structural neural connections.
One way the brain does this is through a process called memory
recon-
solidation. According to Tronson and Taylor (2007), memories
that have been
learned, or achieve neuronal consolidation, can become labile
when activated.
During this labile phase, a memory can become either
reconsolidated or
transformed if contradictory information is presented within a
window of time.
Tronson and Taylor (2007) argued that this process is distinct
from the extinc-
tion process in classical conditioning. This finding has clear
implications for
emotionally learned processes such as phobias, trauma-related
disorders, and
some aspects of other anxiety disorders.
In addition, neurons are understood to fire when people feel,
think, and
behave. Interestingly, neuroscientists have found that mirror
neurons fire in a
similar pattern when we observe others feeling, thinking, and
behaving (Ivey
& Zalaquett, 2011). Mirror neurons have implications for
counselors’ use of
empathy and for work with clients who have difficulty
empathizing with oth-
ers. The study of brain networks in neuroscience has
strengthened the theory
that the brain does not work in isolated sections, but in
functionally connected
regions. This paradigm has led neuroscientists to hypothesize
“pathways” that
communicate among the brain’s structures. Some of the brain
networks that
have been proposed are the default mode network (resting
state), attention
network, social cognition network, motivational network, and
emotional regu-
lation network (Gongalves & Perrone-McGovern, 2014). As
implied by these
names, these networks facilitate (or do not facilitate) many of
the functions
addressed in counseling.
Though these neuroscience concepts have clear implications for
coun-
seling, counselors may still be at a loss for what to do with
them. One concern
is that integrating neuroscience into counseling would be
reductionist. For
instance, neural bases have been used to explain mental health
disorders such
as posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD), depression, and anxiety
disorders
(Grawe, 1997). Some may be tempted to localize a disorder to a
specific cere-
bral region and neglect social, cultural, and interpersonal
factors. However,
neuroscientists have long viewed the brain as a dynamic system
with complex
networks (Cole, 1978), and more recently as a system that
informs and is
shaped by the social and interpersonal environment (Siegel,
2012). Though
it may be tempting to reduce mental health conditions to a
specific region of
the brain, research supports a reciprocal view of the interaction
between the
brain and the environment. Therefore, a comprehensive and
pragmatic model
is needed, one that integrates social and regulatory factors that
counselors can
use to inform assessments, hypotheses, and treatment decisions.
^ Journal of Mental Health Counseling 247
Linear counseling models have been offered to consider the
complex
neural processes that occur during the counseling process and
how these
processes affect or are affected by the environment. Decety &
Jackson (2004)
outlined the parallel neural processes that occur during key
components of
empathy, such as self-awareness, mental flexibility, and
emotion regulation.
Gonsalves and Perrone-McGovern (2014) presented a linear
model that posits
the interpersonal and environmental factors that influence the
psychological
process, which in turn affect brain networks, which impact
developmental
tasks. Though these models effectively integrate neuroscience
and counseling,
they fail to address the reciprocal interaction between the brain
and the envi-
ronment.
THE RECIPROCALLY DETERMINANT BRAIN-BODY
MODEL
Our approach extends the reach of current counseling
interventions
that address the aftermath of ACE exposure and acknowledges
the interactive
influence of environment and behavioral patterning within the
brain and
body. This addition to current family and social systems
approaches assumes a
reciprocal relationship among environment, behavior, person,
and physiology
(brain-body).
Regarding research on neurobiophysiological factors of
development
(Siegel, 2012; van der Kolk, 2015), we extend the model of
reciprocal deter-
minism to include physiology, or the brain-body connection.
This extended
neurobiophysiological model of reciprocal determinism
provides a theoretical
framework for research examining how integrated therapies,
such as neuro-
feedback (NEB), facilitate the regulation of brainwave activity,
emotional and
behavioral self-regulation, and physiology. Further, the model
provides a con-
text for diagnosing maladaptive stress responses that may
present as a myriad of
symptom clusters and/or co-occurring disorders.
In our model, neurobiophysiological functioning such as the
vagal
response (i.e., fight, flight, or freeze; Porges, 2011) and neuro-
maladaptation
(Siegel, 2012) influences the interaction of the person,
environment, and
behavior. When the self-regulatory system does not adequately
develop or
is interrupted by environmental demands, hyperarousal becomes
chronic
(Levine, 1997), which in turn affects perception and behavior
toward the
current environment. Responses during developmental
experiences involve
neurological and physiological changes in the central and
peripheral nervous
systems (Porges, 2011). Based on an understanding of research
addressing the
brain-body connection, the use of cognitive and behavioral
therapies appears
insufficient to address maladaptive responses, as these therapies
only focus on
change in one aspect of the self system and not the entire
system.
248 0 Journal o f Mental Health Counseling
F o s te rin g In tr in s ic R e s ilie n c e
Person
An individual’s personality and temperament have a
bidirectional rela-
tionship to behavior and environment. An individual who has
been exposed
to trauma during a developmentally sensitive period may have
maladaptive
patterns of coping and dissociation that can lead to poor health.
For example,
stressors early in life can cause dysfunction in the brain-gut
microbiota axis
and therefore increase the likelihood of disease and alter
cognitions, moods,
and emotions (Cryan & Dinan, 2012; Rhee, Pothoulakis, &
Mayer, 2009).
Behavior
An individual’s behavior can influence and be influenced by
their person
and environment. An individual who has been exposed to
trauma in a devel-
opmentally sensitive period may present with a myriad of
bidirectional mal-
adaptive behavioral responses, from aggression to juvenile
delinquency. For an
individual exposed to trauma, the reciprocally deterministic
nature of behavior
with environment and person can be better understood and
influenced through
an integrated lens. Maladaptive behaviors in adolescence, such
as aggression
and poor regulation, are mechanisms in reaction to a stimulus.
Responding to
stress involves internal systems such as the autonomic nervous
system (Porges,
2009) and is often a learned behavior (Bandura, 1978).
Autonomic dysregula-
tion, or hypersensitivity, may explain higher rates of conduct
problems such as
aggression and delinquency (Beauchaine, Gatzke-Kopp, &
Mead, 2007).
Environment
An individual’s environment from prenatal inception to death
includes
physical environment, attachment, and genetic expression. An
individual
who has been exposed to environmental trauma in a
developmentally sen-
sitive period may present a number of social and physiological
maladaptive
responses. Long-standing research on bonding and attachment
underlines the
importance of environment for healthy, adaptive development.
Insufficient
early childhood attunement (Bowlby, 1980) and connection can
lead to physio-
logical developmental delays as well as social maladaptation
later in life. Siegel
(2012) argued that attachment problems in adulthood may be
due to the lack
of secure attachment or bonding in developmentally sensitive
periods, leading
to problems in neurological integration.
Environmental context does not manifest solely in neurological
expres-
sion but also in genetic expression. Epidemiologists estimate
that genes can
explain approximately 10% to 20% of adverse health conditions.
The vast
majority of diseases are caused by a combination of, or
interaction between,
genes and the environment. Epigenetics, or “the study of
changes in gene func-
0 Journal o f Mental Health Counseling 249
tion that are ... heritable and that do not entail a change in DNA
sequence”
(Wu & Morris, 2001), is a growing consideration for mental
health providers
(Simon-Dack & Marmarosh, 2014).
Brain-Body
Neural oscillations, or brainwaves, are one prominent way to
understand
the communication pattern between brain and body. Brainwaves
are theorized
to have a self-regulatory function based on behavior and
demands from the
environment. Looking at the function of each type of brainwave
shows us the
opportunities for regulation within those patterns. For example,
slow delta
waves are prominent during sleep; theta waves have high
amplitude during
light sleep and meditation; alpha waves are associated with
relaxed, detached
awareness; and beta waves are linked to a heightened state of
alertness and
critical thinking (Swingle, 2008).
However, when brainwaves become dysregulated, they can
become
unhelpful (Hamlin, 2011). For instance, Clark, Barry,
McCarthy, & Selikowitz
(1998) found that excess delta and theta bandwidths were
associated with
hyperactivity and inattention. Individuals with more magnitude
of beta waves
are more likely to develop alcoholism (Rangaswamy et ah,
2002). Hamlin
(2011) explained that these individuals seek alcohol, an external
source of per-
ceived relaxation, because they are unable to internally diminish
the amplitude
of beta waves, which have an arousing effect. Likewise, adverse
experiences
may require high beta waves, which are adaptive for being alert;
however, when
these patterns persist, they can lead to behaviors that are
maladaptive in other
environments (Hamlin, 2011).
CLINICAL IMPLICATIONS
The model provides counselors a framework to take a
reciprocally deter-
minant perspective of their clients’ presenting problems while
being sensitive
to the impact of ACEs (Felitti et ah, 1998) and aware of the role
of neurobio-
physiological processes as a response to interaction with the
environment and
underlying mechanisms for behavior (Siegel, 2012). Awareness
of ACEs has
grown in the counseling profession, and many counselors
include an assess-
ment of ACEs upon intake and through the conceptualization
process. The
model then facilitates client conceptualizations with
consideration for interac-
tions among ACEs, the person, their brain, and their
environment. Counselors
may use the model to make hypotheses about how these
interactions impact
development, mental health, behavior, physical health, and
physiology.
Underpinning the counseling profession are the principles of
wellness,
of a strengths-based focus, of the belief that intrinsic features of
resilience are
250 0 Journal o f Mental H ealth Counseling
F o s t e r i n g I n t r i n s i c R e s i l i e n c e
inherent in an individual, and of the internal capacity for change
and self-
healing (Rogers, 1951). Given these beliefs, many professional
counselors have
embraced the integration of mind-body work in clinical
practice. Examples of
mind-body-based interventions include mindfulness meditation
(Kabat-Zinn,
1982), dance/movement therapy (Payne, 2003), and somatic
experiential
therapy (Levine, 1997), to name a few. More and more
therapeutic modalities
have been developed that incorporate mind-body interventions
alongside
traditional talk-therapy methods such as acceptance and
commitment therapy
(Hayes, Luoma, Bond, Masuda, & Lillis, 2006), coherence
therapy (Ecker &
Toomey, 2008), dialectical behavior therapy (Linehan, 1993),
and eye move-
ment desensitization and reprocessing (Shapiro, 2001).
Counselors practicing
integrative therapeutic techniques are often looking for new
evidence-based
and neuroscience-informed interventions to support brain-body
healing. As
more counselors begin to understand the importance of
including biological
considerations in their assessment and treatment of clients, a
model from
which to conceptualize and treat clients is needed. Our model
illustrates how
biological considerations, such as brain activity, can
complement the counsel-
ing perspective. Our model suggests a need for greater
measurement of neuro-
biophysiological concerns as a part of the presenting problems
of our clients.
Neuro feedback
A readily accessible and safe technology, NFB is an ideal
methodology
integrating the current findings of neuroscience with the body’s
ability for
self-healing. A type of biofeedback, NFB uses
electroencephalogram (EEG)
and operant conditioning to guide clients toward optimal
cognitive flexibility
based on cortical electrical activity, also referred to as
brainwaves (Swingle,
2008). In an NFB session, a clinician applies sensors on the
client’s scalp. The
sensors detect brainwaves and feed the signal to computer
software, which
quantifies and transforms the data into auditory and/or visual
stimuli for the
client to observe, completing one of many reciprocal loops of
ongoing informa-
tion (Angelakis, Hatzis, Panourias, & Sakas, 2007).
The nature of NFB, however, has presented a unique challenge
for
researchers. Studies have found improved outcomes for a wide
range of prob-
lems in research spanning more than four decades. However,
few of these
studies have convincingly attributed the results to the specific
mechanism of
feedback of a particular brainwave (Thibault & Raz, 2017). The
double-blind
randomized clinical trial design is the gold standard for
minimizing most
threats to internal validity, but the reciprocal nature of NFB
presents numerous
challenges for this design. Recent innovations are thought to
have improved
the science of NFB. For instance, live Z-Score NFB training
allows the cli-
0 Journal o f Mental Health Counseling 251
nician to contrast the clients’ brain activity to a normative
database, nesting
an individual’s performance within a social and statistical
context (Collura,
2017). However, a new model is needed for researchers to better
understand
the process of NFB. Although this article is practitioner
oriented, a model that
can be used by researchers and practitioners alike will improve
the ability for
these two professional activities to inform one another. In
addition, research is
needed to test and/or modify the reciprocally determinant brain-
body model.
Random control trial research is specifically needed to
contextualize the model
in order to further address current concerns about the efficacy
and scope of the
impact of NFB.
The reciprocal interaction among brainwaves, the computer
software’s
quantified analysis of the electroencephalogram data, the
perception of the
visual and auditory stimuli of NFB by the individual, and the
normative data-
base is inherently consistent with the reciprocally determinant
brain-body
model. This model may provide a framework for counselors to
integrate NFB
into their practice and easily apply research guided by the same
model.
Applying IS euro feedback
If inclined, counselors may integrate NFB into their evidence-
based prac-
tice. Extensive literature showing the effectiveness of NFB for a
wide range of
conditions dates back to the 1960s (Hammond, 2011). NFB has
been success-
fully integrated into treatment for individuals who have been
exposed to long-
term traumatic experiences during early development (Fisher,
2014). Recent
studies have shown long-term benefits for individuals with
attention-deficit/
hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) after treatment and at 6-month
follow-up (Van
Doren et ah, 2018) and with intractable epilepsy 10 years
following treatment
(Strehl, Birkle, Worz, & Kotchoubey, 2014). Recent meta-
analyses and reviews
have described NFB’s efficaciousness for PTSD (Panisch &
Hai, 2018) and
for criminal behavior associated with ADHD, schizophrenia,
substance use,
and psychopathy (Fielenbach, Donkers, Spreen, Visser, &
Bogaerts, 2018). In
addition, NFB can enhance performance for athletes (Xiang et
ah, 2018) and
visual artists (Shourie, Firoozabadi, & Badie, 2018) and can
improve cognitive
functioning (Reiner, Gruzelier, Bamidis, & Auer, 2018) and
creativity (Agnoli,
Zanon, Mastria, Avenanti, & Corazza, 2018) among healthy
adults.
Using the reciprocally determinant brain-body model, NFB can
be con-
ceptualized as a microcosm of the reciprocal flow of
information that occurs in
clients’ environments. Brainwaves, or cortical electrical
activity, are measured
with quantitative EEG (Niedermeyer & Da Silva, 2004), which
influences the
visual or auditory stimuli, functioning as the environment,
presented to the
client. The client then perceives the stimuli and allows the self
system, includ-
252 0 Journal o f Mental Health Counseling
F o s t e r in g I n t r i n s i c R e s ilie n c e
ing beliefs and brain activity, to adapt to the ongoing
environment. This cycle
occurs in real time and is ongoing throughout an NFB session
(Hammond,
2011).
CONSIDERATIONS FOR COUNSELORS
Adopting NFB into clinical practice requires careful
consideration of a
variety of questions, as Chapin (2016) comprehensively
described. Counselors
practicing in settings that focus on acute or short-term solution-
focused
interventions would not be ideal candidates for NFB.
Counselors working in
settings that provide longer-term therapy (20+ sessions) would
be excellent
candidates to integrate NFB into their practice. NFB therapy
requires multiple
and continuous sessions to activate sustained change in the
brain.
Counselors must consider ethical and legal issues applicable to
NFB.
Counselors are obligated to “practice only within the boundaries
of their com-
petence” (American Counseling Association [ACA], 2014, p. 8).
In keeping
with the ACA code of ethics, we recommend that interested
counselors receive
comprehensive training such as that designed by certification
agencies like the
Biofeedback Certification International Alliance or the
International Society
for Neurofeedback and Research. Certification involves didactic
coursework,
hours of hands-on practice, and supervision by a certified
clinician. No laws
currently require practitioners to be certified in NFB to provide
the service;
however, ACA (2014) ethical standards ask counselors to
practice “in specialty
areas new to them only after appropriate education, training,
and supervised
experience” (p. 8).
The American Mental Health Counselors Association (AMHCA,
2015)
code of ethics states that counselors in practice should maintain
confidential-
ity. Adequate NFB training involves case consultation after the
basic training.
Given that few experts in the field provide such consultation,
this service is
often done via videoconferencing. Therefore, counselors must
take precautions
to ensure they are in line with the AMHCA (2015) code of
ethics when dis-
cussing their clients’ NFB data electronically. Clients should
also be informed
during the consent process if their personal identifying
information will be
shared for case consultation purposes. Mental health counselors
must take
precautions to protect, store, transfer, and dispose of client
records including
NFB session and assessment data in a confidential manner
consistent with
applicable laws and regulations (AMHCA, 2015).
During the informed consent process, mental health counselors
must
provide information to clients that describes any potential risks
that could
occur with NFB. NFB is a Class II device approved by the US
Food and
Drug Administration for relaxation. However, clients should be
informed that
0 Journal of Mental Health Counseling 253
relaxation does not always mean feeling better immediately. For
instance, NFB
could increase self-awareness of anxiety the client had
previously denied. O ther
possible side effects of NFB include headaches, changes in
sleep patterns, or
irritability, but these are usually temporary and reversible
(Hammond, 2011).
T he importance of informing clients of these risks is reinforced
by the ACA
(2014), which states “counselors explicitly explain to clients the
nature of all
sendees provided ... such as ... techniques, procedures,
limitations, potential
risks, and benefits of services” (p. 4). This is especially
important given the lack
of regulations on NFB and the am ount of information that is
still unknown
about brain activity.
Counselors must also consider the age and typical symptom
presentation
of the clients with whom they work. Although NFB has shown
promising find-
ings with children with ADHD, few clinical studies have
included children
younger than 7 in their sample (Van Doren et al., 2018). This is
due in large
part to rapid cortical maturation in gray matter volume during
the critical early
years of child development (Sowell, Thompson, & Toga, 2004),
and NFB may
not be an effective treatment during this time period. Given
this, counselors
who work primarily with small children may choose to forgo
incorporating
NFB into their practice. Additionally, although NFB may be an
effective treat-
m ent for some individuals presenting psychotic symptoms
(McCarthy-Jones,
2012), counselors must be careful to assess the severity of the
symptoms and the
individual’s capacity to understand the treatment so as not to
become triggered
by the treatment procedure itself. Severely acute individuals
suffering from
active phase psychosis would not be appropriate candidates for
NFB treatment.
Counselors must also be mindful of a client’s mental health
history so as not to
trigger a psychotic episode in an individual prone to psychosis.
Another consideration for counselors contemplating NFB is cost
(Chapin,
2016). NFB devices can cost anywhere from $3,000 to $50,000
or more,
depending on the type of technology used. Counselors would
also need NFB
software and a subscription to training packages in addition to
supplies such as
sensors, ear clips, alcohol swabs, and syringes. In an effort to
make NFB devices
more accessible for practitioners, many NFB device distributors
offer monthly
rental or financing plans. Despite these options, the cost can be
a barrier of
entry for many practicing counselors.
DISCUSSION
Counselors have the capacity to utilize theory and research from
other
fields to affect positive change. The clinical application of
counselor training
with a focus on wellness opens the door for the counseling
profession to inte-
grate theory and knowledge from the fields of cognitive
psychology, neurology,
254 0 Journal o f Mental Health Counseling
F o s te rin g In trin s ic R es ilie n ce
and psychiatry. Our model draws from these professions and
provides a deeper
understanding of the part mental health plays in overall
functioning and
well-being. This new conceptualization offers a map to guide
counselors to aim
fora variety of possible treatment outcomes. These potential
outcomes include
improved sleep patterns; adaptive interpersonal relationships;
increased emo-
tional regulation, resilience, self-control, attention, and
executive functioning;
and decreased digestive problems, substance dependence,
anxiety, depression,
and delinquent or aggressive behaviors. Moreover, the model
highlights the
need to incorporate more mind-body theories and interventions
in counseling.
Mind-body interventions can have positive effects in the
domains of physio-
logical response, physical health, social and emotional health,
mental health,
and behavior (Jayawardene, Bebe, Lorhmann, & Torrabi, 2017).
With the
raised awareness of the impact of trauma (van der Kolk, 2015)
and the need
for evidence-based mind-body interventions, counselors have
the opportunity
to advocate for trauma-exposed individuals on an individual and
societal level.
Integrative therapies, such as NFB, when used from a
counseling perspective,
offer an accessible mind-body modality that works within a
whole-person
model of conceptualizing exposure to childhood trauma and
ways to treat it
(Fisher, 2014). Flowever, ethical issues, such as competence
(ACA, 2014),
must be considered before incorporating new specialty areas
like NFB into
counseling practice.
0 Journal of Mental Health Counseling 255
REFERENCES
Adams, A. S., & Petersen, C. (2016). Precision medicine:
Opportunities, possibilities, and
challenges for patients and providers. Journal o f the American
Informatics Association, 23,
787-790. https://doi.org/10.1093/jamia/ocv215
Agnoli, S., Zanon, M., Mastria, S., Avenanti, A., & Corazza, G.
E. (2018). Enhancing creative
cognition with a rapid right-parietal neurofeedback procedure.
Neuropsychologia, 118, 99-106.
https://doi.Org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2018.02.015
Allbaugh, I,. ]., Mack, S. A., Culmone, 11. D., Hosey, A. M.,
Dunn, S. E., & Kaslow, N. J. (2018).
Relational factors critical in the link between childhood
emotional abuse and suicidal ideation.
Psychological Services, 15, 298-304.
https://doi.org/10.1037/ser0000214
American Counseling Association. (2014). AG'A code of ethics:
As approved by the ACA Governing
Council, 2014. Alexandria, VA: Author.
American Mental Health Counselors Association. (2015).
AMHCA Code of Ethics: Revised
October 2015. Retrieved from
http://connections.amhca.org/HigherLogic/System/
DownloadDocumentFile.ashx?DocumentFileKey=d4el0fcb-2Bc-
c701-aald-5d0f53b8bcl4
Angelakis, E., Hatzis, A., Panourias, I. G., & Sakas, D. E.
(2007). Brain-computer interface:
A reciprocal self-regulated neuromodulation. In D. E. Sakas, &
B. A. Simpson, Operative
neuromodulation (pp. 555-559). Vienna, Austria: Springer.
Bandura, A. (1978). The self system in reciprocal determinism.
American Psychologist, 33,
344-358. https://doi.Org/10.1037/0003-066X.33.4.344
Beauchaine, T. P., Gatzke-Kopp, L., & Mead, H. K. (2007).
Polyvagal theory and developmental
psychopathology: Emotion dysregulation and conduct problems
from preschool to
adolescence. Biological Psychology, 74, 174-184.
https://doi.Org/10.1016/j.biopsycho.2005.08.008
Bowlby, J. (1980). Attachment and loss: Loss, sadness and
depression (Vol. 3). New York, NY:
Basic Books.
Centers for Disease Control and Prevention. (2018). About the
CDC-Kaiser ACE study.
Retrieved from
https://www.cdc.gov/violenceprevention/acestudy/about.html
Chapin, T. J. (2016). Developing a specialty in neurofeedback:
Decision points, journal o f Mental
Health Counseling, 38, 155-169.
https://doi.Org/10.17744/mehc.38.2.06
Clarke, A. R., Barr)', R. J.. McCarthy, R., & Selikowitz, M.
(1998). EEC analysis in attention-
deficit/hyperactivity disorder: A comparative study of two
subtypes. Psychiatry Research, 81,
19-29. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0165-1781(98)00072-9
Cole, M. (1978). Alexander Romanovich Luria: 1902-1977.
American Journal o f Psychology, 91,
349-352. Retrieved from
https://www.press.uillinois.edu/journals/ajp.html
Collura, T. F. (2017). Quantitative EEC and live Z-score
neurofeedback—current clinical and
scientific context. Biofeedback, 45, 25-29.
https://doi.org/10.5298/1081-5937-45.L07
Cryan, J. F., & Dinan, T. G. (2012). Mind-altering
microorganisms: The impact of the gut
microbiota on brain and behavior. Nature Reviews
Neuroscience, 13, 701-712.
https://doi.org/10.1038/nrn3346
Decety, ]., & Jackson, P. L. (2004). The functional architecture
of human empathy. Behavioral
and Cognitive Neuroscience Reviews, 3, 71-100.
https://doi.org/10.1177/1534582304267187
Ecker, B., & Toonrey, B. (2008). Depotentiation of symptom-
producing implicit memory in
coherence therapy, journal of Constructivist Psychology, 21,
87-150.
https://doi.org/10.1080/10720530701853685
Engel, G. L. (1977). The need for a new medical model: A
challenge for biomedicine. Science,
196, 129-136. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.847460
Felitti, V. J., Anda, R. F., Nordernberg, D., Williamson, D. F.,
Spiltz, A. M., Edwards, V., ...
Marks, J. S. (1998). Relationship of childhood abuse and
household dysfunction to many of the
causes of death in adults. American Journal o f Preventive
Medicine, 14, 245-258.
https://doi.org/10.1016/S0749-3797(98)00017-8
256 0 Jo u rn a l o f M e n ta l H e a lth C o u n s e lin g
https://doi.org/10.1093/jamia/ocv215
https://doi.Org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2018.02.015
https://doi.org/10.1037/ser0000214
http://connections.amhca.org/HigherLogic/System/
https://doi.Org/10.1037/0003-066X.33.4.344
https://doi.Org/10.1016/j.biopsycho.2005.08.008
https://www.cdc.gov/violenceprevention/acestudy/about.html
https://doi.Org/10.17744/mehc.38.2.06
https://doi.org/10.1016/S0165-1781(98)00072-9
https://www.press.uillinois.edu/journals/ajp.html
https://doi.org/10.5298/1081-5937-45.L07
https://doi.org/10.1038/nrn3346
https://doi.org/10.1177/1534582304267187
https://doi.org/10.1080/10720530701853685
https://doi.org/10.1126/science.847460
https://doi.org/10.1016/S0749-3797(98)00017-8
F o s t e r in g I n t r i n s i c R e s ilie n c e
Fielenbach, S., Donkers, F. C. L., Spreen, M., Visser, H. A., &
Bogaerts, S. (2018).
Neurofeedback training for psychiatric disorders associated
with criminal offending: A review.
Frontiers in Psychiatry, 8, 1-12.
https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyt.2017.00313
Fisher, S. F. (2014). Neurofeedback in the treatment o f
developmental trauma: Calming the fear
driven brain. New York, NY: Norton.
Giaconia, R. M., Reinherz, H. Z., Silverman, A. B., Pakiz, B.,
Frost, A. K., & Cohen, E. (1995).
Traumas and posttraumatic stress disorder in a community
population of older adolescents.
journal o f the American Academy o f Child and Adolescent
Psychiatry, 34, 1369-1380.
https://doi.org/10.1097/00004583-199510000-00023
Gonqalves, O. F., & Perrone-McGovern, K. M. (2014). A
neuroscience agenda for counseling
psychology research, Journal o f Counseling Psychology, 61,
507-512.
https://doi.org/10.1037/cou0000026
Grawe, K. (1997). Research-informed psychotherapy.
Psychotherapy Research, 7, 1-19.
https://doi.org/10.1080/10503309712331331843
Ham lin, E. (2011). Lecture on the electroencephalogram
(EEC): Source and measurement.
Personal collection of E. Hamlin, Institute of Applied
Neuroscience, Asheville, NC.
H am m ond, C. (2011). W hat is neurofeedback? An update,
journal ofNeurotherapy, 15, 305-336
https://d0i.0rg/l 0.1080/10874208.2011.623090
I layes, S. C., Luoma, J. 13., Bond, F. W., Masuda, A., & Lillis,
J. (2006). Acceptance and
com m itm ent therapy: Model, processes and outcomes.
Behaviour Research and Therapy, 44,
1-25. https://doi.0rg/l 0.1016/j.brat.2005.06.006
Ivey, A., & Zalaquett, C. P. (2011). Neuroscience and
counseling: Central issues for social
justice leaders. Journal for Social Action in Counseling and
Psychology, 3, 201-115.
Retrieved from
http://web.b.ebscohost.com.ezproxy.memphis.edu/ehost/pdfview
er/
pdfviewer?vid=3&sid=03d3d312-b8cb-4a3f-a869-
d49dfeffb0d7%40pdc-v-sessmgr03
Jayawardene, W., Bebe, R., Lorhmann, D., & Torrabi, M.
(2017). Use of treatm ent and
counseling services and m ind-body techniques by students with
emotional and behavioral
difficulties. Journal o f School Health, 87, 133-141.
https://doi.Org/10.l 111/josh. 12475
Kabat-Zinn, ]. (1982). An outpatient program in behavioral
medicine for chronic pain patients
based on the practice of mindfulness meditation: Theoretical
considerations and preliminary
results. General Hospital Psychiatry, 4, 33-47.
https://doi.org/10.1016/0163-8343(82)90026-3
Levine, P. (1997). Waking the tiger: Health trauma. Berkeley,
CA: North Atlantic Books.
Linehan, M. M. (1993). Cognitive behavioral therapy oj
borderline personality disorder (Vol. 51).
New York, NY: Guilford Press.
MeCarlhy-Jones, S. (2012). Taking back the brain: C ould
neurofeedback training be effective
for relieving distressing auditory verbal hallucinations in
patients with schizophrenia?
Schizophrenia Bulletin, 38, 678-682.
https://doi.org/10.1093/schbul/sbs006
National Institute of Mental Health. (2015). N IM H strategic
plan for research.
Retrieved from https://www.nimh.nih.gov/about/strategic-
planningreports/nimh_
strategicplanforresearch_508compliant_corrected_final_149979.
pdf
Navalta, C. P., M cGee., L., & Underwood, j. (2018). Adverse
childhood experiences, brain
development, and m ental health: A call for neurocounseling.
Journal o f M ental Health
Counseling, 40, 266-278.
https://doi.Org/10.17744/mehc.40.3.07
Niedermeyer, E., & da Silva, F. L. (2004).
Electroencephalography: Basic principles, clinical
applications, and related fields. Philadelphia, PA: Lippincott
Williams & Wilkins.
Panisch, L. S., & Hai, A. H. (2018). T he effectiveness of using
neurofeedback in the treatment of
post-traumatic stress disorder: A systematic review. Trauma,
Violence, and Abuse, 1-10.
https://doi.org/10.1177/1524838018781103
Payne, 11. (2003). Dance movement therapy: Theory and
practice. New York, NY: Routledge.
Porges, S. W. (2009). T he polyvagal theory: New insights into
adaptive reactions of the automatic
nervous system. Cleveland Clinic Journal o f Medicine, 76, S86-
S90.
https://doi.org/10.3949/ccjm.76.s2.17
Porges, S. W. (2011). The polyvagal theory: Neurophysiological
foundations of emotions,
attachment, communication, and self-regulation. New York,
NY: Norton.
$ Journal o f Mental H ealth Counseling 257
https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyt.2017.00313
https://doi.org/10.1097/00004583-199510000-00023
https://doi.org/10.1037/cou0000026
https://doi.org/10.1080/10503309712331331843
https://d0i.0rg/l
https://doi.0rg/l
http://web.b.ebscohost.com.ezproxy.memphis.edu/ehost/pdfview
er/
https://doi.Org/10.l
https://doi.org/10.1016/0163-8343(82)90026-3
https://doi.org/10.1093/schbul/sbs006
https://www.nimh.nih.gov/about/strategic-
planningreports/nimh_
https://doi.Org/10.17744/mehc.40.3.07
https://doi.org/10.1177/1524838018781103
https://doi.org/10.3949/ccjm.76.s2.17
Rangaswamy, M., Porjesz, B., Chorlian, D. B., Wang, K., Jones,
K. A., Bauer, L. 0 . , . . . Begleiter,
H. (2002). Beta power in the EEG of alcoholics. Biological
Psychiatry, 52, 851-842.
https://doi.org/10.1016/S0006-3223(02)01362-8
Reiner, M., Gruzelier, J., Bamidis, P. D., & Auer, T. (2018).
The science of neurofeedback:
Learnability and effects. Neuroscience, 378, 1-10.
https://doi.Org/10.1016/j.neuroscience.2018.04.024
Rhee, S. H., Pothoulakis, C., & Mayer, E. A. (2009). Principles
and clinical implications of the
brain-gut-enteric microbiota axis. Nature Reviews:
Gastroenterology and Hepatology, 6, 306—
314. https://doi.org/10.1038/nrgastro.2009.35
Rockett, J. L., Murrie, D. C., & Boccaccini, M. T. (2007).
Diagnostic labeling in juvenile justice
settings: Do psychopathology and conduct disorder findings
influence clinicians? Psychological
Services, 4, 107-122. https://doi.Org/10.1037/1541-
1559.4.2.107
Rogers, C. (1951). Client-centered therapy: Its current practice,
implications, and theory. Oxford,
United Kingdom: Houghton Mifflin.
Russell-Chapin, L. A. (2016). Integrating neurocounseling into
the counseling profession: An
introduction. Journal of Mental Health Counseling, 38, 93-102.
https://doi.Org/10.17744/mehc.38.2.01
Schilling, E. A., Aseltine, R. H., & Gore, S. (2007). Adverse
childhood experiences and mental
health in young adults: A longitudinal survey. BMC Public
Health, 7, 30-40.
https://doi.org/10.1186/1471-2458-7-30
Schwartz, E., & Perry, B. (1994). The post-traumatic response
in children and
adolescents. Psychiatric Clinics o f North America, 17, 311-
326.
https://doi.org/10.1016/SO193-95 3X( 18)30117-5
Shapiro, F. (2001). Eye movement desensitization and
reprocessing (EMDR): Basic principles,
protocols, and procedures. New York, NY: Guilford Press.
Shevlin, M., McElroy, E., & Murphy, J. (2015). Loneliness
mediates the relationship between
childhood trauma and adult psychopathology: Evidence from the
adult psychiatric morbidity
survey. Social Psychiatiy and Psychiatric Epidemiology, 59,
591-601.
https://doi.org/10.1007/s00127-014-0951-8
Shourie, N., Firoozabadi, M., & Badie, K. (2018).
Neurofeedback training protocols based on
spectral EEG feature subset and channel selection for
performance enhancement of novice
visual artists. Biomedical Signal Processing and Control, 43,
117-129.
https://doi.Org/10.1016/j.bspc.2018.02.017
Siegel, D. J. (2012). The developing mind: How relationships
and the brain interact to shape who
we are (2"" ed.). New York, NY: Guilford Press.
Simon-Dack, S. L., & Marmarosh, C. L. (2014). Neurosciences
and adult health behaviors:
Recent findings and implications for counseling psychology.
Journal of Counseling Psychology,
61, 528-533. https://doi.org/10.1037/cou0000020
Sowell, E. R., Thompson, P. M., & Toga, A. W. (2004).
Mapping changes in the human cortex
throughout the span of life. The Neuroscientist, 10, 372-392.
https://doi.org/10.1177/1073858404263960
Strehl, U., Birkle, S. M., Worz, S., & Kotchoubey, B. (2014).
Sustained reduction of seizures in
patients with intractable epilepsy after self-regulation training
of slow cortical potentials 10
years after. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 8, 1-7.
https://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2014.00604
Swingle, P. G. (2008). Biofeedback for the brain: How
neurotherapy effectively treats depression,
ADHD, autism, and more. Piscataway, NJ: Rutgers University
Press.
Thibault, R. T., & Raz, A. (2017). The psychology of
neurofeedback: Clinical intervention even if
applied placebo. American Psychologist, 72, 679-688.
https://doi.org/10.1037/amp0000118
Thompson, E. J., Richards, M., & Gaysina, D. (2017).
Cumulative effect of adverse childhood
experiences on affective symptom trajectories in adulthood:
Evidence from a British birth
cohort. Journal of Epidemiology and Community Health, 71
(Suppl. 1), A60.
https://doi.org/10,1136/jech-2017-SS.YlAbstracls. 122
Tronson, N. C., & Taylor, J. R. (2007). Molecular mechanisms
of memory reconsolidation,
Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 8, 262-275.
https://doi.org/10.1038/nrn2090
258 0 Jo u rn a l o f M e n ta l H e a lth C o u n se lin g
https://doi.org/10.1016/S0006-3223(02)01362-8
https://doi.Org/10.1016/j.neuroscience.2018.04.024
https://doi.org/10.1038/nrgastro.2009.35
https://doi.Org/10.1037/1541-1559.4.2.107
https://doi.Org/10.17744/mehc.38.2.01
https://doi.org/10.1186/1471-2458-7-30
https://doi.org/10.1016/SO193-95
https://doi.org/10.1007/s00127-014-0951-8
https://doi.Org/10.1016/j.bspc.2018.02.017
https://doi.org/10.1037/cou0000020
https://doi.org/10.1177/1073858404263960
https://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2014.00604
https://doi.org/10.1037/amp0000118
https://doi.org/10,1136/jech-2017-SS.YlAbstracls
https://doi.org/10.1038/nrn2090
F o s t e r i n g I n t r i n s i c R e s i l i e n c e
van der Kolk, B. A. (1994). T he body keeps the score: Memory
and the evolving psychobiology of
posttraunratic stress. Harvard Review o f Psychiatry, 1, 253-
265.
https://d0 i.0 rg/l 0.3109/10673229409017088
van der Kolk, B. (2005). Developmental trauma disorder.
Psychiatric Annals, 35, 401-408.
https://doi.org/10.3928/00485713-20050501-06
van der Kolk, B. (2015). The body keeps the score: Brain, mind,
and body in the healing o f trauma.
New York, NY: Penguin Group.
Van Doren, J., Arns, M., Heinrich, H., Vollebregt, M. A.,
Strehl, U., & Loo, S. K. (2018).
Sustained effects of neurofeedback in ADHD: A systematic
review and meta-analysis.
European C hild and Adolescent Psychiatry, 28(3), 1-13.
https://doi.org/10.1007/s00787-018-1121-4
W11, C., & Morris, J. R. (2001). Genes, genetics, and
epigenetics: A correspondence. Science, 10,
1 103-1105. https://doi.Org/10.1126/science.296.5532.l 103
Xiang, M., Hou, X., Liao, B., Liao, J., & Hu, M. (2018). T he
effect of neurofeedback training for
sport performance in athletes: A meta-analysis. Psychology o f
Sport and Exercise, 36, 114-122.
https://doi.Org/10.1016/j.psychsport.2018.02.004
Zettler, H. R., Wolff, K., Baglivio, M., Craig, J. M., & Epps, N.
(2017). T he racial and gender
differences in the impact of adverse childhood experiences on
juvenile residential placement.
Youth Violence and Juvenile Justice, 16, 319-337.
https://doi.org/10.! 177/1541204017698213
^ Journal o f Mental Health Counseling 259
https://d0i.0rg/l
https://doi.org/10.3928/00485713-20050501-06
https://doi.org/10.1007/s00787-018-1121-4
https://doi.Org/10.1126/science.296.5532.l
https://doi.Org/10.1016/j.psychsport.2018.02.004
https://doi.org/10
Copyright of Journal of Mental Health Counseling is the
property of American Mental Health
Counselors Association and its content may not be copied or
emailed to multiple sites or
posted to a listserv without the copyright holder's express
written permission. However, users
may print, download, or email articles for individual use.
Full Terms & Conditions of access and use can be found at
https://www.tandfonline.com/action/journalInformation?journal
Code=vjer20
The Journal of Educational Research
ISSN: 0022-0671 (Print) 1940-0675 (Online) Journal homepage:
https://www.tandfonline.com/loi/vjer20
The association among literacy proficiency,
effortful control, and frustration
Su-Ying Huang & Li-Li Yeh
To cite this article: Su-Ying Huang & Li-Li Yeh (2019) The
association among literacy proficiency,
effortful control, and frustration, The Journal of Educational
Research, 112:2, 168-178, DOI:
10.1080/00220671.2018.1461599
To link to this article:
https://doi.org/10.1080/00220671.2018.1461599
Published online: 17 May 2018.
Submit your article to this journal
Article views: 193
View related articles
View Crossmark data
https://www.tandfonline.com/action/journalInformation?journal
Code=vjer20
https://www.tandfonline.com/loi/vjer20
https://www.tandfonline.com/action/showCitFormats?doi=10.10
80/00220671.2018.1461599
https://doi.org/10.1080/00220671.2018.1461599
https://www.tandfonline.com/action/authorSubmission?journalC
ode=vjer20&show=instructions
https://www.tandfonline.com/action/authorSubmission?journalC
ode=vjer20&show=instructions
https://www.tandfonline.com/doi/mlt/10.1080/00220671.2018.1
461599
https://www.tandfonline.com/doi/mlt/10.1080/00220671.2018.1
461599
http://crossmark.crossref.org/dialog/?doi=10.1080/00220671.20
18.1461599&domain=pdf&date_stamp=2018-05-17
http://crossmark.crossref.org/dialog/?doi=10.1080/00220671.20
18.1461599&domain=pdf&date_stamp=2018-05-17
The association among literacy proficiency, effortful control,
and frustration
Su-Ying Huanga and Li-Li Yehb
aDepartment of Clinical Psychology, Fu Jen Catholic
University, New Taipei City, Taiwan; bDepartment of
Audiology and Speech-Language Pathology,
Mackay Medical College, New Taipei City, Taiwan
ARTICLE HISTORY
Received 12 December 2017
Revised 28 March 2018
Accepted 31 March 2018
ABSTRACT
The authors investigated the relationships among effortful
control and frustration and literacy proficiency
of preadolescence to determine which subcomponent of effortful
control and/or emotion might be critical
in achieving academic success. The participants included 72
children recruited from a larger longitudinal
study. Children’s frustration, effortful control, and literacy
skills were assessed at Grade 5, and their
nonverbal IQ levels were assessed at Grade 1. Effortful control
and frustration were measured by a self-
report questionnaire. Literacy proficiency was measured with
tests of reading comprehension, written
language, and the semestral grades in the subject of Chinese
literacy. Results showed that children with
higher effortful control perception had better literacy abilities,
particularly writing and language academic
achievement. The dispositional frustration moderated the
predictive effect of attention control on
children’s reading comprehension. The findings support the
importance of self-regulation and emotions
on literacy proficiency.
KEYWORDS
Effortful control; frustration;
literacy proficiency; self-
regulation; temperament
Preadolescence is a transitional stage between childhood and
adolescence, during which self-regulation skills—often associ-
ated with children’s ability to adjust—continue to develop
(King, Lengua, & Monahan, 2013). Preadolescents may seek
recognition from their families and among their peers by
achieving academic success, which is based on good literacy
skills. Literacy is a new mode of communication that children
need to learn as they begin formal schooling. Reading and writ-
ing are the receptive and expressive aspects of literacy, respec-
tively, and serve as the foundation of one’s learning of other
subjects. A previous study has shown that children with better
effortful control are more likely to perform better academically,
such as in the area of literacy acquisition (Blair & Razza, 2007).
In addition, emotions are functionally important in ensuring
good academic performance (Pekrun, 2009). However, the key
effects of effortful control and negative moods on the literacy
performance of preadolescents have yet to be systematically
investigated.
Effortful control is a dispositional, higher-order self-regula-
tion of one’s temperament. It refers to one’s ability to perform
effectively in a conflict situation, which requires one to demon-
strate attention control, inhibit a dominant response, or acti-
vate a goal-directed response (Posner & Rothbart, 1998;
Rothbart, Derryberry, & Posner, 1994). Effortful control sup-
ports individual attainment of competence-related goals
(Rothbart & Hwang, 2005) and is related to school adaptation
(Blair, 2002). For adolescents, effortful control is constantly
developing and becoming more crucial, particularly for those
who display more intense negative emotions when it comes to
facing challenging situations (Oldehinkel, Hartman, Ferdinand,
Verhulst, & Ormel, 2007).
Effortful control emerges from the end stage of infancy,
markedly increases during preschool, and continues its devel-
opment into adolescence (Rueda, Posner, & Rothbart, 2011). It
is a broad construct consisting of specific components, namely,
attention control, inhibitory control, and activation control
after middle childhood (i.e., around 8 years old; Putnam, Ellis,
& Rothbart, 2001). Effortful control not only is influenced by
biological factors, but also can be shaped by environmental fac-
tors, such as family context and parental practices (Rothbart &
Bates, 2006). Given that literacy acquisition requires complex
processing (e.g., encoding and decoding signals, comprehend-
ing and composing passages) and also emerges from the pre-
school period, there is a growing interest in the regulation
processes involved in children’s literacy acquisition (e.g., Blair
& Razza, 2007; McClelland et al., 2007).
Emotions are crucial contributors to students’ motivation,
interpersonal resources, memory, and learning (Valiente,
Swanson, & Eisenberg, 2012). Specific emotions relate to child-
ren’s development for different reasons, thus requiring an
assessment of specific types of emotions, instead of using broad
composites, such as emotionality (Rothbart & Bate, 2006;
Valiente et al., 2012). Anger or frustration, as negative emo-
tions, involves a tendency to experience an unpleasant affect,
which can be led by goal blocking. The kind of emotion may
increase when children enter school because this is where they
face more academic expectations from themselves or others
(Eisenberg et al., 2005).
Thus far, the direct association between negative emotions
and academic achievement has not been fully examined
(Valiente et al., 2012), particularly for the critical period of
liter-
acy and effortful control acquisition (i.e., at the preadolescence
CONTACT Dr. Li-Li Yeh [email protected] Department of
Audiology and Speech-Language Pathology, Mackay Medical
College, No.46, Sec. 3, Zhongzheng
Rd., Sanzhi Dist., New Taipei City, Taiwan.
© 2018 Taylor & Francis Group, LLC
https://doi.org/10.1080/00220671.2018.1461599
THE JOURNAL OF EDUCATIONAL RESEARCH
2019, VOL. 112, NO. 2, 168–178
https://crossmark.crossref.org/dialog/?doi=10.1080/00220671.2
018.1461599&domain=pdf&date_stamp=2018-05-14
mailto:[email protected]
https://doi.org/10.1080/00220671.2018.1461599
http://www.tandfonline.com
stage). In addition, given that the direct associations between
specific measures of negative emotion and achievement are at a
modest level (Valient et al., 2012), the potential indirect effects
of emotion must also be investigated (Blair, 2002).
Role of effortful control on literacy acquisition
Effortful control is significantly associated with preschoolers’
prerequisite literacy skills, such as phonemic awareness and let-
ter knowledge, and must be strengthened for children from low
socioeconomic status families to avoid possible school failures
(Blair & Razza, 2007). For 7- to 12-year-old children, the
global
effortful control score as reported by parents, is positively
related to children’s grade point average in language, vocabu-
lary, and mathematics (Valiente, Lemery-Chalfant, Swanson, &
Reiser, 2008). Furthermore, global effortful control at Grade 1
significantly predicted academic performance in mathematics
and Chinese four years later among Chinese children (Zhou,
Main, & Wang, 2010). McClelland et al. (2007) indicated that
the specific components of effortful control (e.g., attention con-
trol, inhibitory control) are positively related to letter-word
identification and the vocabulary ability of preschool children.
Attention control, including attention focusing and shifting,
is a more cognitively oriented component of effortful control.
Past studies have reported the direct link between academic
competence and attention control, which serves as a key factor
in promoting academic achievement and learning (Liew, 2012;
Valiente et al., 2008). Children who have difficulty directing
their attention are more likely to experience significant chal-
lenges when trying to learn educational materials (Valiente
et al., 2008). Empirical data have shown that children who have
difficulty focusing their attention often have poor reading and
language skills (McGee, Partridge, Williams, & Silva, 1991;
Tamis-LeMonda & Bornstein, 1989). Attention control can
also regulate emotions and limit the impact of (anxiety) emo-
tion on information processing (Derrybery & Reed, 2002).
Inhibitory control and activation control are two behavioral
regulation components of effortful control. First, inhibitory
control is the ability to appropriately inhibit a dominant
response or desirable behavior (Putnam & Rothbart, 2006).
Associated with emotional adaptation, inhibitory control rap-
idly develops with marked improvement during childhood
(Lengua, 2003; Williams, Ponesse, Schachar, Logan, &
Tannock, 1999). However, its contribution on academic success
are inconsistent. For example, Blair and Razza (2007) indicated
that the inhibitory control by direct measurement is positively
associated with the letter knowledge of preschoolers. Neverthe-
less, the inhibitory control of parents’ rating does not have a
significant association with children’s literacy abilities. The
inconsistency among past studies may be attributed to different
test environments or the design of tasks conducted among vari-
ous studies.
Meanwhile, activation control is the ability to plan new
actions; it also refers to the ability to perform the appropriate
action when there is a tendency to give up, when people are
experiencing challenges or failures, or when individuals are
facing detestable learning tasks (Posner & Rothbart, 1998;
Rothbart et al., 1994). In stressful situations, such as when
people are facing challenges or failures, they may also need to
force themselves to take action to ameliorate the situation.
Activation control is the key that enables an individual to per-
form unpleasant activities, which can be helpful in achieving
one’s long-term goals (Ellis, 2002). Literacy acquisition
requires complicated processing. For example, when writing
an assignment, children need to actively engage and initiate
behaviors to work on tasks that they may otherwise want to
avoid. However, to our knowledge, no study has yet to directly
examine the role of activation control on the literacy abilities
of school-age children.
In summary, although evidence has proven that effortful
control has a direct link with children’s literacy in early child-
hood, few studies have focused on school-age children, particu-
larly preadolescents. Aside from analyzing effortful control in
general, studying both the direct and indirect predictive effects
by examining various components of effortful control can also
improve our understanding of whether any specific component
of effortful control is uniquely predictive of academic achieve-
ment (Valiente et al., 2008), and whether specific components
are associated with literacy performance.
Association between frustration and literacy acquisition
Different negative moods may have varying associations with
academic performance (Schwarz, 2001), and an increasing
number of studies have focused on the association between
anxiety-related emotions and academic performance (e.g.,
Duchesne, Vitaro, Larose, & Tremblay, 2008; Zeidner, 2007),
particularly in how different negative moods may have varying
predictive effects on academic performance (Schwarz, Chang,
& Farver, 2001). Clarifying the relationship between emotions
and achievement by specific types of emotions—instead of
using broad composites, such as negative emotionality—is
helpful in understanding the role of emotions in developing
academic competence (Valiente et al., 2012).
Anger or frustration, as negative moods, may reduce
achievement, because they disrupt cognitive resources, motiva-
tion, and the use of flexible strategies (Blair, 2002; Rice,
Levine,
& Pizarro, 2007). According to previous studies, children’s
frus-
tration at school is correlated negatively with their academic
self-efficacy, self-regulation of learning, and academic perfor-
mance (Boekaerts, 1993; Pekrun, Goetz, Per, Kramer, &
Hochstadt, 2004). Zhou et al. (2010) found that, based on
teachers’ reports, Chinese children who had higher levels of
dis-
positional anger or frustration from 6 to 9 years old also had
lower levels of academic performance (i.e., grade point aver-
age). Furthermore, Blair (2002) proposed that emotions have
an important contribution on the self-regulation of children, as
they have not fully developed their cognitive and higher-order
regulative abilities. Rothbart and Hwang (2005) also claimed
that the efficiency of effortful control depends on the strength
of the dominant response, such as emotional reactivity.
Effortful control demand and frustration reaction during
reading and writing
Reading is an activity that is significantly correlated with both
decoding skills and language comprehension. The correlations
between word recognition and reading tend to decline with
THE JOURNAL OF EDUCATIONAL RESEARCH 169
grade level, and the correlations between comprehension and
reading tend to increase with grade level (Gough, Hoover, &
Peterson, 1996). If frustration is more likely to arise when
ongoing tasks are interrupted or when goal blocking occurs
(e.g., Zhou et al., 2010), factors associated with reading
fluency,
such as word recognition skills or higher-level language com-
prehension skills, may relate to experience of frustration. In
other words, before school-aged children develop mature read-
ing skills, particularly for early adolescents, frustration may
eas-
ily occur, especially when their goal for reading is mismatched
to their academic assignments and texts (Graesser & D’Mello,
2012).
Writing consists of several processes, such as text construc-
tion, handwriting, spelling, executive function, and working
memory for accessing or selecting appropriate vocabulary
(Owens, 2011). Therefore, compared with reading, writing as
actively generating text may demand more self-regulatory skills
to select and sustain attention, organize ideas, and control
social and affective behavior (Ylvisaker & DeBonis, 2000).
Unless it is under time pressure, writing itself emphasizes less
on nonstop or fluent processing than reading. For early adoles-
cents, compared with early writing skills, such as spelling or
orthography knowledge, text generation is more critical to their
writing performance (Owens, 2011).
Aims and research questions for this study
The aim of this study was to investigate whether children’s own
perceptions of their effortful control and frustration had a rela-
tive contribution to their literacy proficiency after their gender,
age, and nonverbal IQ were controlled. The research questions
are as follows:
Research Question 1: Does global effortful control predict
the concurrent literacy proficiency? Is there any signifi-
cant prediction effect between global effortful control
and various indicators of literacy proficiency, such as
reading comprehension, written expression, and semes-
tral literacy test grade, while controlling for gender,
nonverbal IQ, and age?
Research Question 2: How do the specific components of
effortful control relate to different indicators of literacy
proficiency?
Research Question 3: Does children’s dispositional frustra-
tion have not only a direct predictive effect on children’s
literacy proficiency, but also an indirect predictive effect
on the influence of effortful control toward literacy
proficiency?
Methods
Participants
Seventy-two Mandarin-speaking children (33 boys and 39 girls)
in Taiwan, who were earlier recruited for a larger longitudinal
study, were involved in the current longitudinal first- and sec-
ond wave studies. At Wave 1, the children were in Grade 1;
Wave 2 occurred four years later when the children were in
Grade 5, with an average age of 10.77 years (SD D 0.27 years,
age range D 10.17–11.17 years).
Measures
Nonverbal IQ
The children’s nonverbal IQ scores at Grade 1 were assessed
using Tonic Test of Nonverbal Intelligence (TONI-3; Brown,
Sherhenou, & Johnsen, 1996).
Effortful control
The short form of the Chinese version of the self-report ver-
sions of the Early Adolescence Temperament Questionnaire-
Revised (EATQ-R; Putnam et al., 2001) were employed to
assess 9- to 15-year-old children’s effortful control (Ellis &
Rothbart, 2001). The three subtasks of effort control include
attention control (6 items, e.g., “It is easy for me to really con-
centrate on homework problems”), inhibitory control (5 items;
e.g., “When someone tells me to stop doing something, it is
easy for me to stop”), and activation control (5 items; e.g., “If I
have a hard assignment to do, I get started right away”). Items
were rated on a 5-point Likert-type scale ranging from 1
(almost never true or almost always untrue) to 5 (almost always
true). The EATQ-R is a valid and consistent scale employed to
assess children and adolescents’ temperament, including effort-
ful control. The Cronbach’s alpha values ranged from .67–.76
(Muris & Meesters, 2009).
Frustration
The anger-frustration subscale of the negative emotionality in
EATQ-R was used to measure the negative effects related to the
interruption of ongoing tasks or goal blocking in children. The
Cronbach’s alpha value of the 7-item test used in the current
study was .72.
Reading comprehension
Reading comprehension was measured using a paper-and-pen-
cil task from a published Test of Reading Comprehension (Lin
& Chi, 2000), which was designed for pupils in Grades 2–6. It
is a standardized test that was published by the Minister of
Education in Taiwan. In addition, it is a popular test employed
by elementary teachers in Taiwan to assess pupils’ reading
comprehension competence. This was a group test that took
around 45 min to complete. The reading comprehension test
contained six narrative passages and six expositive passages
fol-
lowed by multiple questions. The test assessed the children’s
skills of phonological recoding, syntactic parsing, semantics,
comprehending facts explicitly as stated in the text, deriving
main ideas from the text, making inferences, and comparing or
analyzing the facts stated in the text. The total raw score of the
test is 100. The test manual reports internal consistencies rang-
ing from .88 to .98 and test–retest reliability of .89.
Written expression
Written language skills were assessed by a paper-and-pencil
task from a published Test of Written Language (Lin & Chi,
2002) designed for pupils in Grades 3–6. This was a group test
that took around 1 hr to complete. It is a standardized test that
was published by the Minister of Education in Taiwan. It is one
of the popular tests used by elementary teachers in Taiwan to
assess pupils’ written expression competence. The written lan-
guage test consisted of five subtasks, including listening-
S.-Y. HUANG AND L.-L. YEH170
writing, sentence combination, correction of characters, and
clause and sentence construction. The total raw score of the
test is 100. The test manual reports internal consistencies rang-
ing from .76 to .97 and test–retest reliabilities of .79–.94.
Language academic achievement in terms of semestral
grades
Individual children’s grades were used to obtain measures of
children’s academic competence in the domain of Chinese liter-
acy. At the conclusion of the semester in January boundary
2014, we obtained the mid- and final-term grades, and the aver-
age of the two examinations served as a composite of each stu-
dent’s academic achievement. The semestral grades of Chinese
literacy test consisted of word recognition, semantic discrimi-
nation of a variety of words, reading comprehension of text-
book or passages, the structure and format of articles, and
sentence construction.
Procedure
All participants were recruited from Mandarin-speaking ele-
mentary schools in North Taiwan during the 2009 and 2013
fall semester. At Grade 1, the nonverbal IQ of each child was
individually assessed using the TONI-3 in school settings. At
Grade 5, the children were assessed for Chinese comprehension
and written ability. Self-report versions of the EATQ-R for
children’s frustration and effortful control were also used. Only
the test of nonverbal intelligence and reading comprehension
was administered to groups. The average scores of academic
achievement of language at Grade 5 were also collected.
Analysis
Before the hypothesis testing of the research topic, gender dif-
ference analysis for all the variables was computed by separate
multivariate analyses of variance. Next, the zero-order correla-
tions among study variables were examined. Finally, hierarchi-
cal regressions were conducted to test whether children’s
frustration, overall effortful control, and the three components
of effortful control predicted their reading comprehension,
written expression, and semestral Chinese literacy test grades
after controlling for children’s gender, age, and nonverbal IQ.
Preliminary analysis
Data were combined before analysis because no significant gen-
der difference was found in all variables. Table 1 presents the
means, standard deviations, and the Pearson correlation coeffi-
cients for all study variables. The IQ score of all the children
ranged from 82 to 134, with an average of 102.29 (SD D 10.77),
and all the participants’ scores were above ¡1.5 SD, that is,
within the normal range. The cohort’s performance of reading
comprehension test ranged from 30 to 95, with an average of
76.42 (SD D 11.66). Only one participant had a score lower
than the 15th percentile of the norm. However, the score was
still above ¡1.5 SD according to the norms for 9-year-olds.
Given that EATQ-R is suitable for 9- to 15-year-old preadoles-
cents, it was thought that the participants’ reading comprehen-
sion would allow them to finish the questionnaire and its
results had validity.
Correlation analyses
The analysis of zero-order correlations showed that children’s
age positively correlated with reading comprehension and writ-
ten expression, rs(70) D .27 and .31, ps D .022 and .009, respec-
tively. In addition, nonverbal IQ positively correlated with
written expression and semestral literacy test grades, rs(70) D
.23 and .37, ps D .056 and .001, respectively. There was no sig-
nificant correlation between frustration and children’s reading
comprehension, written expression, and semestral literacy test
grades.
Effortful control was positively associated with attention as
well as inhibitory and activation control, rs(70) D .75, .62, and
.76, respectively, ps < .001. Reading comprehension and writ-
ten expression were positively related with semestral literacy
test grades, rs(70) D .49 and .60, respectively, ps < .001. Effort-
ful control was positively correlated with children’s written
expression and semestral literacy test grades, rs(70) D .27 and
.26, ps D .024 and .03, respectively. Attention control of
Table 1. Descriptive statistics and correlations for all variables
in this study (N D 72).
Variable 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11
1. Gender —
2. Age .04 —
3. Nonverbal IQ .21 ¡.15 —
4. Frustration .02 .10 .06 —
5. Effortful control .00 ¡.07 .08 ¡.18 —
6. Attention control ¡.03 ¡.06 .10 ¡.22y .76*** —
7. Inhibitory control .05 ¡.06 .06 ¡.16 .62*** .45*** —
8. Activation control .04 ¡.10 .03 ¡.20 .76*** .68*** .38** —
9. Reading comprehension ¡.17 .27* .20 .00 .21 .17 ¡.01 .17 —
10. Written expression ¡.14 .31** .23y ¡.03 .27* .32** .12 .23y
.48*** —
11. Semestral score of literacy ¡.08 .21 .37** ¡.06 .26* .22y .13
.23* .49*** .60*** —
Descriptive Statistics
M — 10.77 102.29 3.11 3.46 3.49 3.43 3.46 76.42 65.58 88.1
SD — 0.27 10.77 0.77 0.58 0.66 0.67 0.81 11.66 12.38 7.75
yp < .08.
�p < .05.
��p < .01.
���p < .001.
THE JOURNAL OF EDUCATIONAL RESEARCH 171
effortful control was positively and marginally associated with
written expression, r(70) D .32, p < .01, and semestral literacy
test grades; r(70) D .22, p D .065. Activation control was mar-
ginally and significantly associated positively with written
expression and semestral literacy test grades, rs(70) D .23 and
.23, ps D .054 and .047, respectively.
Regression analyses
Three hierarchical regression equations were created to predict
each of the three indicators of literacy proficiency, including
reading comprehension, written expression, and semestral liter-
acy test grades. Given that gender and IQ were previously cor-
related with children’s literacy ability, and the children’s ages
were correlated with literacy ability, all three variables were
entered as control variables at Step 1 in each of the regressions.
In Step 2, frustration, overall effortful control, the three compo-
nents of effortful control (attention control, inhibitory control,
and activation control) were entered. Then, the interaction var-
iables of frustration and global effortful control, along with the
three components of effortful control, were respectively entered
in Step 3.
Results
Predicting the contribution of frustration and effortful
control on reading comprehension
Table 2 presents the contribution of frustration, global effortful
control, and the three components of effortful control on read-
ing comprehension. As can be seen, the control variables of
gender, age, and nonverbal IQ made unique contributions to
reading ability in any of the four regressions at Step 1. These
variables respectively accounted for 19% of the variance, F(3,
68) D 5.24, p D .003. Gender (b D –.24), t(71) D –2.13, p D
.037; age (b D .33), t(71) D 2.94, p D .001; and nonverbal IQ (b
D .31), t(71) D 2.66, p D .01; significantly positively predicted
children’s reading comprehension in the four models.
After considering the effects of the three variables, we found
that effortful control, attention control, inhibitory control, and
activation control had no significant contributions (bs D .19,
.15, .01, and .20, respectively; ps > .09), and that frustration
had no significant association with reading comprehension.
Further results for the reading comprehension model indi-
cated that there was a marginally significant two-way interac-
tion between frustration and effortful control (b D –.20), t(71)
D ¡1.91; DR2 D .04; F(1, 65) D 3.63, p D .061. As depicted in
Figure 1, the relation between effortful control and reading
comprehension was positive and stronger for lower frustration
children (b D .42), t(71) D 2.62, p D .011. However, among
children with high levels of frustration, effortful control was
not significantly predictive of reading comprehension (b D
.14), t(71) D 0.89, p D ns. The results indicated that a low-
level,
frustration with high effortful control ameliorated children’s
reading comprehension.
Further results for the reading comprehension model indi-
cated that there was a two-way interaction between frustration
and attention control (b D –.36), t(71) D –3.48; DR2 D .12, F(1,
65) D 12.10, p < .01. The interaction variable significantly
added to the regression model (12%). As depicted in Figure 2,
attention control was positively predictive of reading compre-
hension among children with low frustration. However, the
simple slope analysis showed that such a difference was not sig-
nificant (bs D .11 and .15), ts(71) D 0.28 and 1.37, p D ns.
Prediction of frustration and effortful control on written
expression
Written expression was regressed against the variables that
were entered in blocks, similar to the reading comprehension
variable. Table 3 shows the results of the hierarchical
regression
analysis for predicting written expression. Similar to the results
of reading comprehension, the control variables of gender, age,
and nonverbal IQ made unique contributions to written
expression in any of the four regressions at Step 1. These mod-
els accounted for 22%, F(3, 68) D 6.23, p < .01, of the variance.
Gender (b D –.22), t(71) D –2.01, p D .049; age (b D .36), t(71)
D 3.35, p D .001; and nonverbal IQ at Grade 1 (b D .32), t(71)
D 2.93, p D .005; significantly positively predicted children’s
written expression in the four models.
After considering the above variables, frustration and global
effortful control, attention control, and activation control of
effortful control all added a significant amount of variance to
these three models (7%, 9%, and 7%; bs D .25, .32, and .26), ts
(71) D 2.34, 2.82, and 2.39, ps D .022, .006, and .02, respec-
tively. Children who had higher levels of effortful control,
attention control, and activation control also had better written
expression skills. However, the frustration and inhibitory con-
trol did not have a significant effect. Further results from Step 3
for the written expression model indicated that there was no
significant two-way interaction between frustration and global
effortful control and the three components of effortful control.
Prediction of frustration and effortful control on the
semestral literacy test grades
Table 4 shows the contribution of frustration and effortful con-
trol in predicting the semestral literacy test grades. Results
showed that children’s age and nonverbal IQ were two major
contributors to the children’s semester literacy test grades.
However, the children’s gender did not have a significant asso-
ciation with their grades. Frustration, inhibitory control, and
attention control did not have a significant predictive effect on
children’s semestral literacy test grades; in comparison, global
effortful control and activation control had significant associa-
tion with semestral literacy test grades (bs D .22 and .24), ts
(71) D 2.09 and 2.28, ps D .04 and .026, respectively. The two-
way interaction effect between frustration and effortful control
or any component was not proven.
Discussion
The goal of this study was to investigate whether children’s per-
ceptions of their effortful control and frustration may predict
their literacy proficiency. The key findings revealed that
children
with higher effortful control perception had better literacy abili-
ties, particularly writing and language academic achievement.
The association of effortful control components with children’s
literacy proficiency also proved to be dependent on literacy
S.-Y. HUANG AND L.-L. YEH172
Ta
b
le
2.
Pr
ed
ic
ti
ng
re
ad
in
g
co
m
p
re
he
ns
io
n
fr
om
ch
ild
re
n’
s
fr
us
tr
at
io
n,
ov
er
al
le
ff
or
tf
ul
co
nt
ro
l,
an
d
th
e
th
re
e
co
m
po
ne
nt
s
of
EC
.
Re
ad
in
g
co
m
p
re
he
ns
io
n
Re
ad
in
g
co
m
pr
eh
en
si
on
Re
ad
in
g
co
m
pr
eh
en
si
on
Re
ad
in
g
co
m
pr
eh
en
si
on
Pr
ed
ic
to
rs
B
SE
B
St
an
d
ar
d
iz
ed
b
4
R2
Pr
ed
ic
to
rs
B
SE
B
St
an
d
ar
d
iz
ed
b
4
R2
Pr
ed
ic
to
rs
B
SE
B
St
an
d
ar
d
iz
ed
b
4
R2
Pr
ed
ic
to
rs
B
SE
B
St
an
d
ar
d
iz
ed
b
4
R2
St
ep
1
.1
9*
*
St
ep
1
.1
9*
*
St
ep
1
.1
9*
*
St
ep
1
.1
9*
*
G
en
de
r
¡5
.5
3
2.
60
¡.
24
*
G
en
de
r
¡5
.5
3
2.
60
¡.
24
*
G
en
d
er
¡5
.5
3
2.
60
¡.
24
*
G
en
d
er
¡5
.5
3
2.
60
¡.
24
*
A
g
e
3.
72
1.
26
.3
3*
*
A
g
e
3.
72
1.
26
.3
3*
*
A
ge
3.
72
1.
26
.3
3*
*
A
ge
3.
72
1.
26
.3
3*
*
N
on
ve
rb
al
IQ
3.
31
1.
32
.3
1*
N
on
ve
rb
al
IQ
3.
31
1.
32
.3
1*
N
on
ve
rb
al
IQ
3.
31
1.
32
.3
1*
N
on
ve
rb
al
IQ
3.
31
1.
32
.3
1*
St
ep
2
.0
4
St
ep
2
.0
2
St
ep
2
.1
3
St
ep
2
.0
4
Fr
us
tr
at
io
n
¡.
19
1.
30
¡.
02
Fr
us
tr
at
io
n
¡.
26
1.
32
¡.
02
Fr
us
tr
at
io
n
¡.
66
1.
32
¡.
60
Fr
us
tr
at
io
n
¡.
19
1.
29
¡.
02
EC
2.
27
1.
29
.1
9
A
TC
1.
75
1.
33
.1
5
IC
¡.
06
1.
32
¡.
01
A
C
2.
47
1.
29
.2
0
St
ep
3
.0
4y
St
ep
3
.1
2*
*
St
ep
3
.0
2
St
ep
3
.0
1
Fr
us
tr
at
io
n
x
EC
¡2
.6
1
1.
37
¡.
20
y
Fr
us
tr
at
io
n
x
A
TC
¡4
.8
0
1.
38
¡.
36
*
*
Fr
us
tr
at
io
n
x
IC
¡.
18
1.
36
¡.
02
Fr
us
tr
at
io
n
x
A
C
¡1
.0
3
1.
30
¡.
09
N
ot
e.
A
C
D
ac
ti
va
ti
on
co
nt
ro
l;
A
TC
D
at
te
nt
io
n
co
nt
ro
l;
EC
D
ef
fo
rt
fu
lc
on
tr
ol
;I
C
D
in
hi
b
it
or
y
co
nt
ro
l.
y p
<
.0
6.
� p
<
.0
5.
��
p
<
.0
1.
��
� p
<
.0
01
.
THE JOURNAL OF EDUCATIONAL RESEARCH 173
indicators. Attention control positively predicted children’s
writ-
ing ability. Activation control also had a direct association with
writing ability and semestral language academic performance
after controlling the basic variables. Meanwhile, no significant
association of inhibitory control and any indicator of literacy
pro-
ficiency was found. In addition, dispositional frustration had no
direct predictive effect on preadolescents’ literacy proficiency;
however, frustration moderated the predictive effect of attention
control on children’s reading comprehension.
Role of effortful control in the development of literacy
proficiency
In terms of the first research question, the results showed that
preadolescents’ overall perceptions about their behaviors and
attention regulation did account for the unique variance in
writing- and literacy-related academic achievements in task-
directed performance, even when children’s gender, age, and
IQ were controlled. In line with previous studies (e.g., Valiente
et al., 2008; Zhou et al., 2010), the results implied that effortful
control perception was closely associated with children’s ability
to perform effectively in school. In addition, consistent with
previous studies (e.g., Ylvisaker & DeBonis, 2000), the results
indicated that written expression and semestral literacy test
seemed more closely associated with intentional control than
receptive reading tasks for preadolescents, given that the per-
formance on writing and semestral grading both significantly
related to global effortful control. In other words, the results
suggested that different underlying mechanisms of reading,
writing, or taking a literacy exam may require different compo-
nents or levels of effortful control to perform well.
Association between effortful control components
and indicators of literacy proficiency
Regarding the second research question, our results showed
that the specific component of effortful control was associated
with the different indicators of literacy proficiency. However,
only attention control and activation control were closely tied
to literacy ability for early adolescents. The results implied that
during expressive written tasks, if a preadolescent pays enough
attention and repeatedly activates his or her focus on the tasks
at hand, he or she may have a greater chance of achieving a bet-
ter performance in the writing assignment or literacy exam. In
addition, a possible reason for the absence of a significant rela-
tionship between any literacy indicators and inhibitory control
may be the matureness of inhibitory control for high-grade
pupils who had already adapted to task-conducting situations
(Eisenberg et al., 2005; Williams, Ponesse, Schacha, Logan, &
Tannock, 1999); thus, more inhibitory control would not be
linked to their performance on either the literacy assessment or
literacy exam. It was also possible that inhibitory control had
an indirect predictive effect on academic performance through
social and motivation processes (Valiente et al., 2008).
Relative contribution of dispositional frustration
on literacy proficiency
Finally, in terms of the third research question, the significant
linkage between frustration and attention control, and a mar-
ginal moderator of global effortful control on reading compre-
hension, were found in the study. Given that inhibitory control
and activation control relates to the regulation of impulsive
behaviors and those involved in stressful situations, and less
related to the adjustment of the emotion itself; therefore, no
direct association of frustration was expected. The results sug-
gest that anger-frustration related to the disfluency of reading
(e.g., Zhou et al., 2010) or children’s goal for reading is mis-
matched to their academic assignments and texts (Graesser &
D’Mello, 2012), which is more likely to impede the positive
association between attention regulation and reading compre-
hension. The results support the notion that the indirect model
(Blair, 2002) is valuable in understanding the mechanism of
which emotion correlates with children’s literacy ability and
academic achievement. In addition, the finding also supports
the assumptions that the efficiency of effortful control depends
on the strength of the dominant response or the strength of
emotion reactivity (Rothbart & Hwang, 2005), and that nega-
tive emotion may be associated with lower academic achieve-
ment by disrupting subject’s cognitive resources (Blair, 2002;
Rice et al., 2007).
Clinical implications
The findings of this study have implications on the importance
of facilitating children’s development of effortful control, par-
ticularly attention control and activation control so that their
84.38 83.57
80.04
89.69
70
75
80
85
90
95
Low High
R
ea
di
ng
C
om
pr
eh
en
si
on
Effortful Control
Frustration: High Frustration: Low
Figure 1. Effortful control and reading comprehension for high
and low levels of
frustration.
86.62
81.41
76.80
91.12
70
75
80
85
90
95
Low High
R
ea
di
ng
C
om
pr
eh
en
si
on
Attention control
Frustration: High Frustration: Low
Figure 2. Attention control and reading comprehension for high
and low levels of
frustration.
S.-Y. HUANG AND L.-L. YEH174
Ta
b
le
3.
Pr
ed
ic
ti
ng
w
ri
tt
en
ex
p
re
ss
io
n
fr
om
ch
ild
re
n’
s
fr
us
tr
at
io
n
an
d
th
e
th
re
e
co
m
po
ne
nt
s
of
EC
.
W
ri
ti
ng
ex
p
re
ss
io
n
W
ri
ti
ng
ex
p
re
ss
io
n
W
ri
ti
ng
ex
p
re
ss
io
n
W
ri
ti
ng
ex
p
re
ss
io
n
Pr
ed
ic
to
rs
B
SE
B
St
an
d
ar
d
iz
ed
b
4
R2
Pr
ed
ic
to
rs
B
SE
B
St
an
d
ar
d
iz
ed
b
4
R2
Pr
ed
ic
to
rs
B
SE
B
St
an
d
ar
d
iz
ed
b
4
R2
Pr
ed
ic
to
rs
B
SE
B
St
an
d
ar
d
iz
ed
b
4
R2
St
ep
1
.2
2*
*
St
ep
1
.2
2*
*
St
ep
1
.2
2*
*
St
ep
1
.2
2*
*
G
en
de
r
¡5
.4
5
2.
71
¡.
22
*
G
en
d
er
¡5
.4
5
2.
71
¡.
22
*
G
en
d
er
¡5
.4
5
2.
71
¡.
22
*
G
en
d
er
¡5
.4
5
2.
71
¡.
22
*
A
g
e
4.
42
1.
32
.3
6*
*
A
g
e
4.
42
1.
32
.3
6*
*
A
g
e
4.
42
1.
32
.3
6*
*
A
g
e
4.
42
1.
32
.3
6*
*
N
on
ve
Colorado V. Connelly479 U.S 157 (1986)Facts· Co.docx
Colorado V. Connelly479 U.S 157 (1986)Facts· Co.docx
Colorado V. Connelly479 U.S 157 (1986)Facts· Co.docx
Colorado V. Connelly479 U.S 157 (1986)Facts· Co.docx
Colorado V. Connelly479 U.S 157 (1986)Facts· Co.docx
Colorado V. Connelly479 U.S 157 (1986)Facts· Co.docx
Colorado V. Connelly479 U.S 157 (1986)Facts· Co.docx
Colorado V. Connelly479 U.S 157 (1986)Facts· Co.docx
Colorado V. Connelly479 U.S 157 (1986)Facts· Co.docx
Colorado V. Connelly479 U.S 157 (1986)Facts· Co.docx
Colorado V. Connelly479 U.S 157 (1986)Facts· Co.docx
Colorado V. Connelly479 U.S 157 (1986)Facts· Co.docx
Colorado V. Connelly479 U.S 157 (1986)Facts· Co.docx
Colorado V. Connelly479 U.S 157 (1986)Facts· Co.docx
Colorado V. Connelly479 U.S 157 (1986)Facts· Co.docx
Colorado V. Connelly479 U.S 157 (1986)Facts· Co.docx
Colorado V. Connelly479 U.S 157 (1986)Facts· Co.docx
Colorado V. Connelly479 U.S 157 (1986)Facts· Co.docx
Colorado V. Connelly479 U.S 157 (1986)Facts· Co.docx
Colorado V. Connelly479 U.S 157 (1986)Facts· Co.docx
Colorado V. Connelly479 U.S 157 (1986)Facts· Co.docx
Colorado V. Connelly479 U.S 157 (1986)Facts· Co.docx
Colorado V. Connelly479 U.S 157 (1986)Facts· Co.docx
Colorado V. Connelly479 U.S 157 (1986)Facts· Co.docx
Colorado V. Connelly479 U.S 157 (1986)Facts· Co.docx
Colorado V. Connelly479 U.S 157 (1986)Facts· Co.docx
Colorado V. Connelly479 U.S 157 (1986)Facts· Co.docx
Colorado V. Connelly479 U.S 157 (1986)Facts· Co.docx
Colorado V. Connelly479 U.S 157 (1986)Facts· Co.docx
Colorado V. Connelly479 U.S 157 (1986)Facts· Co.docx
Colorado V. Connelly479 U.S 157 (1986)Facts· Co.docx
Colorado V. Connelly479 U.S 157 (1986)Facts· Co.docx
Colorado V. Connelly479 U.S 157 (1986)Facts· Co.docx
Colorado V. Connelly479 U.S 157 (1986)Facts· Co.docx
Colorado V. Connelly479 U.S 157 (1986)Facts· Co.docx
Colorado V. Connelly479 U.S 157 (1986)Facts· Co.docx
Colorado V. Connelly479 U.S 157 (1986)Facts· Co.docx
Colorado V. Connelly479 U.S 157 (1986)Facts· Co.docx
Colorado V. Connelly479 U.S 157 (1986)Facts· Co.docx
Colorado V. Connelly479 U.S 157 (1986)Facts· Co.docx
Colorado V. Connelly479 U.S 157 (1986)Facts· Co.docx
Colorado V. Connelly479 U.S 157 (1986)Facts· Co.docx
Colorado V. Connelly479 U.S 157 (1986)Facts· Co.docx
Colorado V. Connelly479 U.S 157 (1986)Facts· Co.docx
Colorado V. Connelly479 U.S 157 (1986)Facts· Co.docx
Colorado V. Connelly479 U.S 157 (1986)Facts· Co.docx
Colorado V. Connelly479 U.S 157 (1986)Facts· Co.docx
Colorado V. Connelly479 U.S 157 (1986)Facts· Co.docx
Colorado V. Connelly479 U.S 157 (1986)Facts· Co.docx
Colorado V. Connelly479 U.S 157 (1986)Facts· Co.docx
Colorado V. Connelly479 U.S 157 (1986)Facts· Co.docx
Colorado V. Connelly479 U.S 157 (1986)Facts· Co.docx
Colorado V. Connelly479 U.S 157 (1986)Facts· Co.docx
Colorado V. Connelly479 U.S 157 (1986)Facts· Co.docx
Colorado V. Connelly479 U.S 157 (1986)Facts· Co.docx
Colorado V. Connelly479 U.S 157 (1986)Facts· Co.docx
Colorado V. Connelly479 U.S 157 (1986)Facts· Co.docx
Colorado V. Connelly479 U.S 157 (1986)Facts· Co.docx
Colorado V. Connelly479 U.S 157 (1986)Facts· Co.docx
Colorado V. Connelly479 U.S 157 (1986)Facts· Co.docx
Colorado V. Connelly479 U.S 157 (1986)Facts· Co.docx
Colorado V. Connelly479 U.S 157 (1986)Facts· Co.docx
Colorado V. Connelly479 U.S 157 (1986)Facts· Co.docx
Colorado V. Connelly479 U.S 157 (1986)Facts· Co.docx
Colorado V. Connelly479 U.S 157 (1986)Facts· Co.docx
Colorado V. Connelly479 U.S 157 (1986)Facts· Co.docx
Colorado V. Connelly479 U.S 157 (1986)Facts· Co.docx
Colorado V. Connelly479 U.S 157 (1986)Facts· Co.docx
Colorado V. Connelly479 U.S 157 (1986)Facts· Co.docx
Colorado V. Connelly479 U.S 157 (1986)Facts· Co.docx
Colorado V. Connelly479 U.S 157 (1986)Facts· Co.docx
Colorado V. Connelly479 U.S 157 (1986)Facts· Co.docx
Colorado V. Connelly479 U.S 157 (1986)Facts· Co.docx
Colorado V. Connelly479 U.S 157 (1986)Facts· Co.docx
Colorado V. Connelly479 U.S 157 (1986)Facts· Co.docx
Colorado V. Connelly479 U.S 157 (1986)Facts· Co.docx
Colorado V. Connelly479 U.S 157 (1986)Facts· Co.docx
Colorado V. Connelly479 U.S 157 (1986)Facts· Co.docx
Colorado V. Connelly479 U.S 157 (1986)Facts· Co.docx
Colorado V. Connelly479 U.S 157 (1986)Facts· Co.docx
Colorado V. Connelly479 U.S 157 (1986)Facts· Co.docx
Colorado V. Connelly479 U.S 157 (1986)Facts· Co.docx
Colorado V. Connelly479 U.S 157 (1986)Facts· Co.docx
Colorado V. Connelly479 U.S 157 (1986)Facts· Co.docx
Colorado V. Connelly479 U.S 157 (1986)Facts· Co.docx
Colorado V. Connelly479 U.S 157 (1986)Facts· Co.docx
Colorado V. Connelly479 U.S 157 (1986)Facts· Co.docx
Colorado V. Connelly479 U.S 157 (1986)Facts· Co.docx
Colorado V. Connelly479 U.S 157 (1986)Facts· Co.docx
Colorado V. Connelly479 U.S 157 (1986)Facts· Co.docx
Colorado V. Connelly479 U.S 157 (1986)Facts· Co.docx
Colorado V. Connelly479 U.S 157 (1986)Facts· Co.docx
Colorado V. Connelly479 U.S 157 (1986)Facts· Co.docx
Colorado V. Connelly479 U.S 157 (1986)Facts· Co.docx
Colorado V. Connelly479 U.S 157 (1986)Facts· Co.docx
Colorado V. Connelly479 U.S 157 (1986)Facts· Co.docx
Colorado V. Connelly479 U.S 157 (1986)Facts· Co.docx
Colorado V. Connelly479 U.S 157 (1986)Facts· Co.docx
Colorado V. Connelly479 U.S 157 (1986)Facts· Co.docx
Colorado V. Connelly479 U.S 157 (1986)Facts· Co.docx
Colorado V. Connelly479 U.S 157 (1986)Facts· Co.docx
Colorado V. Connelly479 U.S 157 (1986)Facts· Co.docx
Colorado V. Connelly479 U.S 157 (1986)Facts· Co.docx
Colorado V. Connelly479 U.S 157 (1986)Facts· Co.docx
Colorado V. Connelly479 U.S 157 (1986)Facts· Co.docx
Colorado V. Connelly479 U.S 157 (1986)Facts· Co.docx
Colorado V. Connelly479 U.S 157 (1986)Facts· Co.docx
Colorado V. Connelly479 U.S 157 (1986)Facts· Co.docx
Colorado V. Connelly479 U.S 157 (1986)Facts· Co.docx
Colorado V. Connelly479 U.S 157 (1986)Facts· Co.docx
Colorado V. Connelly479 U.S 157 (1986)Facts· Co.docx
Colorado V. Connelly479 U.S 157 (1986)Facts· Co.docx
Colorado V. Connelly479 U.S 157 (1986)Facts· Co.docx
Colorado V. Connelly479 U.S 157 (1986)Facts· Co.docx
Colorado V. Connelly479 U.S 157 (1986)Facts· Co.docx
Colorado V. Connelly479 U.S 157 (1986)Facts· Co.docx
Colorado V. Connelly479 U.S 157 (1986)Facts· Co.docx
Colorado V. Connelly479 U.S 157 (1986)Facts· Co.docx
Colorado V. Connelly479 U.S 157 (1986)Facts· Co.docx
Colorado V. Connelly479 U.S 157 (1986)Facts· Co.docx
Colorado V. Connelly479 U.S 157 (1986)Facts· Co.docx
Colorado V. Connelly479 U.S 157 (1986)Facts· Co.docx
Colorado V. Connelly479 U.S 157 (1986)Facts· Co.docx
Colorado V. Connelly479 U.S 157 (1986)Facts· Co.docx
Colorado V. Connelly479 U.S 157 (1986)Facts· Co.docx
Colorado V. Connelly479 U.S 157 (1986)Facts· Co.docx
Colorado V. Connelly479 U.S 157 (1986)Facts· Co.docx
Colorado V. Connelly479 U.S 157 (1986)Facts· Co.docx
Colorado V. Connelly479 U.S 157 (1986)Facts· Co.docx
Colorado V. Connelly479 U.S 157 (1986)Facts· Co.docx
Colorado V. Connelly479 U.S 157 (1986)Facts· Co.docx
Colorado V. Connelly479 U.S 157 (1986)Facts· Co.docx
Colorado V. Connelly479 U.S 157 (1986)Facts· Co.docx
Colorado V. Connelly479 U.S 157 (1986)Facts· Co.docx
Colorado V. Connelly479 U.S 157 (1986)Facts· Co.docx
Colorado V. Connelly479 U.S 157 (1986)Facts· Co.docx
Colorado V. Connelly479 U.S 157 (1986)Facts· Co.docx
Colorado V. Connelly479 U.S 157 (1986)Facts· Co.docx
Colorado V. Connelly479 U.S 157 (1986)Facts· Co.docx
Colorado V. Connelly479 U.S 157 (1986)Facts· Co.docx
Colorado V. Connelly479 U.S 157 (1986)Facts· Co.docx
Colorado V. Connelly479 U.S 157 (1986)Facts· Co.docx
Colorado V. Connelly479 U.S 157 (1986)Facts· Co.docx
Colorado V. Connelly479 U.S 157 (1986)Facts· Co.docx
Colorado V. Connelly479 U.S 157 (1986)Facts· Co.docx
Colorado V. Connelly479 U.S 157 (1986)Facts· Co.docx
Colorado V. Connelly479 U.S 157 (1986)Facts· Co.docx
Colorado V. Connelly479 U.S 157 (1986)Facts· Co.docx
Colorado V. Connelly479 U.S 157 (1986)Facts· Co.docx
Colorado V. Connelly479 U.S 157 (1986)Facts· Co.docx
Colorado V. Connelly479 U.S 157 (1986)Facts· Co.docx
Colorado V. Connelly479 U.S 157 (1986)Facts· Co.docx
Colorado V. Connelly479 U.S 157 (1986)Facts· Co.docx
Colorado V. Connelly479 U.S 157 (1986)Facts· Co.docx
Colorado V. Connelly479 U.S 157 (1986)Facts· Co.docx
Colorado V. Connelly479 U.S 157 (1986)Facts· Co.docx
Colorado V. Connelly479 U.S 157 (1986)Facts· Co.docx
Colorado V. Connelly479 U.S 157 (1986)Facts· Co.docx
Colorado V. Connelly479 U.S 157 (1986)Facts· Co.docx
Colorado V. Connelly479 U.S 157 (1986)Facts· Co.docx
Colorado V. Connelly479 U.S 157 (1986)Facts· Co.docx
Colorado V. Connelly479 U.S 157 (1986)Facts· Co.docx
Colorado V. Connelly479 U.S 157 (1986)Facts· Co.docx
Colorado V. Connelly479 U.S 157 (1986)Facts· Co.docx
Colorado V. Connelly479 U.S 157 (1986)Facts· Co.docx
Colorado V. Connelly479 U.S 157 (1986)Facts· Co.docx
Colorado V. Connelly479 U.S 157 (1986)Facts· Co.docx
Colorado V. Connelly479 U.S 157 (1986)Facts· Co.docx
Colorado V. Connelly479 U.S 157 (1986)Facts· Co.docx
Colorado V. Connelly479 U.S 157 (1986)Facts· Co.docx
Colorado V. Connelly479 U.S 157 (1986)Facts· Co.docx
Colorado V. Connelly479 U.S 157 (1986)Facts· Co.docx
Colorado V. Connelly479 U.S 157 (1986)Facts· Co.docx
Colorado V. Connelly479 U.S 157 (1986)Facts· Co.docx
Colorado V. Connelly479 U.S 157 (1986)Facts· Co.docx
Colorado V. Connelly479 U.S 157 (1986)Facts· Co.docx
Colorado V. Connelly479 U.S 157 (1986)Facts· Co.docx
Colorado V. Connelly479 U.S 157 (1986)Facts· Co.docx
Colorado V. Connelly479 U.S 157 (1986)Facts· Co.docx
Colorado V. Connelly479 U.S 157 (1986)Facts· Co.docx
Colorado V. Connelly479 U.S 157 (1986)Facts· Co.docx
Colorado V. Connelly479 U.S 157 (1986)Facts· Co.docx
Colorado V. Connelly479 U.S 157 (1986)Facts· Co.docx
Colorado V. Connelly479 U.S 157 (1986)Facts· Co.docx
Colorado V. Connelly479 U.S 157 (1986)Facts· Co.docx
Colorado V. Connelly479 U.S 157 (1986)Facts· Co.docx
Colorado V. Connelly479 U.S 157 (1986)Facts· Co.docx
Colorado V. Connelly479 U.S 157 (1986)Facts· Co.docx
Colorado V. Connelly479 U.S 157 (1986)Facts· Co.docx
Colorado V. Connelly479 U.S 157 (1986)Facts· Co.docx
Colorado V. Connelly479 U.S 157 (1986)Facts· Co.docx
Colorado V. Connelly479 U.S 157 (1986)Facts· Co.docx
Colorado V. Connelly479 U.S 157 (1986)Facts· Co.docx
Colorado V. Connelly479 U.S 157 (1986)Facts· Co.docx
Colorado V. Connelly479 U.S 157 (1986)Facts· Co.docx
Colorado V. Connelly479 U.S 157 (1986)Facts· Co.docx
Colorado V. Connelly479 U.S 157 (1986)Facts· Co.docx
Colorado V. Connelly479 U.S 157 (1986)Facts· Co.docx
Colorado V. Connelly479 U.S 157 (1986)Facts· Co.docx
Colorado V. Connelly479 U.S 157 (1986)Facts· Co.docx
Colorado V. Connelly479 U.S 157 (1986)Facts· Co.docx
Colorado V. Connelly479 U.S 157 (1986)Facts· Co.docx
Colorado V. Connelly479 U.S 157 (1986)Facts· Co.docx
Colorado V. Connelly479 U.S 157 (1986)Facts· Co.docx
Colorado V. Connelly479 U.S 157 (1986)Facts· Co.docx
Colorado V. Connelly479 U.S 157 (1986)Facts· Co.docx
Colorado V. Connelly479 U.S 157 (1986)Facts· Co.docx
Colorado V. Connelly479 U.S 157 (1986)Facts· Co.docx
Colorado V. Connelly479 U.S 157 (1986)Facts· Co.docx
Colorado V. Connelly479 U.S 157 (1986)Facts· Co.docx
Colorado V. Connelly479 U.S 157 (1986)Facts· Co.docx
Colorado V. Connelly479 U.S 157 (1986)Facts· Co.docx
Colorado V. Connelly479 U.S 157 (1986)Facts· Co.docx
Colorado V. Connelly479 U.S 157 (1986)Facts· Co.docx
Colorado V. Connelly479 U.S 157 (1986)Facts· Co.docx
Colorado V. Connelly479 U.S 157 (1986)Facts· Co.docx
Colorado V. Connelly479 U.S 157 (1986)Facts· Co.docx
Colorado V. Connelly479 U.S 157 (1986)Facts· Co.docx
Colorado V. Connelly479 U.S 157 (1986)Facts· Co.docx
Colorado V. Connelly479 U.S 157 (1986)Facts· Co.docx
Colorado V. Connelly479 U.S 157 (1986)Facts· Co.docx
Colorado V. Connelly479 U.S 157 (1986)Facts· Co.docx
Colorado V. Connelly479 U.S 157 (1986)Facts· Co.docx
Colorado V. Connelly479 U.S 157 (1986)Facts· Co.docx
Colorado V. Connelly479 U.S 157 (1986)Facts· Co.docx
Colorado V. Connelly479 U.S 157 (1986)Facts· Co.docx
Colorado V. Connelly479 U.S 157 (1986)Facts· Co.docx
Colorado V. Connelly479 U.S 157 (1986)Facts· Co.docx
Colorado V. Connelly479 U.S 157 (1986)Facts· Co.docx
Colorado V. Connelly479 U.S 157 (1986)Facts· Co.docx
Colorado V. Connelly479 U.S 157 (1986)Facts· Co.docx
Colorado V. Connelly479 U.S 157 (1986)Facts· Co.docx
Colorado V. Connelly479 U.S 157 (1986)Facts· Co.docx
Colorado V. Connelly479 U.S 157 (1986)Facts· Co.docx
Colorado V. Connelly479 U.S 157 (1986)Facts· Co.docx
Colorado V. Connelly479 U.S 157 (1986)Facts· Co.docx
Colorado V. Connelly479 U.S 157 (1986)Facts· Co.docx
Colorado V. Connelly479 U.S 157 (1986)Facts· Co.docx
Colorado V. Connelly479 U.S 157 (1986)Facts· Co.docx
Colorado V. Connelly479 U.S 157 (1986)Facts· Co.docx
Colorado V. Connelly479 U.S 157 (1986)Facts· Co.docx
Colorado V. Connelly479 U.S 157 (1986)Facts· Co.docx
Colorado V. Connelly479 U.S 157 (1986)Facts· Co.docx
Colorado V. Connelly479 U.S 157 (1986)Facts· Co.docx
Colorado V. Connelly479 U.S 157 (1986)Facts· Co.docx
Colorado V. Connelly479 U.S 157 (1986)Facts· Co.docx
Colorado V. Connelly479 U.S 157 (1986)Facts· Co.docx
Colorado V. Connelly479 U.S 157 (1986)Facts· Co.docx
Colorado V. Connelly479 U.S 157 (1986)Facts· Co.docx
Colorado V. Connelly479 U.S 157 (1986)Facts· Co.docx
Colorado V. Connelly479 U.S 157 (1986)Facts· Co.docx
Colorado V. Connelly479 U.S 157 (1986)Facts· Co.docx
Colorado V. Connelly479 U.S 157 (1986)Facts· Co.docx
Colorado V. Connelly479 U.S 157 (1986)Facts· Co.docx
Colorado V. Connelly479 U.S 157 (1986)Facts· Co.docx
Colorado V. Connelly479 U.S 157 (1986)Facts· Co.docx
Colorado V. Connelly479 U.S 157 (1986)Facts· Co.docx
Colorado V. Connelly479 U.S 157 (1986)Facts· Co.docx
Colorado V. Connelly479 U.S 157 (1986)Facts· Co.docx
Colorado V. Connelly479 U.S 157 (1986)Facts· Co.docx
Colorado V. Connelly479 U.S 157 (1986)Facts· Co.docx
Colorado V. Connelly479 U.S 157 (1986)Facts· Co.docx
Colorado V. Connelly479 U.S 157 (1986)Facts· Co.docx
Colorado V. Connelly479 U.S 157 (1986)Facts· Co.docx
Colorado V. Connelly479 U.S 157 (1986)Facts· Co.docx
Colorado V. Connelly479 U.S 157 (1986)Facts· Co.docx
Colorado V. Connelly479 U.S 157 (1986)Facts· Co.docx
Colorado V. Connelly479 U.S 157 (1986)Facts· Co.docx
Colorado V. Connelly479 U.S 157 (1986)Facts· Co.docx
Colorado V. Connelly479 U.S 157 (1986)Facts· Co.docx
Colorado V. Connelly479 U.S 157 (1986)Facts· Co.docx
Colorado V. Connelly479 U.S 157 (1986)Facts· Co.docx
Colorado V. Connelly479 U.S 157 (1986)Facts· Co.docx
Colorado V. Connelly479 U.S 157 (1986)Facts· Co.docx
Colorado V. Connelly479 U.S 157 (1986)Facts· Co.docx
Colorado V. Connelly479 U.S 157 (1986)Facts· Co.docx
Colorado V. Connelly479 U.S 157 (1986)Facts· Co.docx
Colorado V. Connelly479 U.S 157 (1986)Facts· Co.docx
Colorado V. Connelly479 U.S 157 (1986)Facts· Co.docx
Colorado V. Connelly479 U.S 157 (1986)Facts· Co.docx
Colorado V. Connelly479 U.S 157 (1986)Facts· Co.docx
Colorado V. Connelly479 U.S 157 (1986)Facts· Co.docx
Colorado V. Connelly479 U.S 157 (1986)Facts· Co.docx

More Related Content

More from mccormicknadine86

Option #2Researching a Leader Complete preliminary rese.docx
Option #2Researching a Leader Complete preliminary rese.docxOption #2Researching a Leader Complete preliminary rese.docx
Option #2Researching a Leader Complete preliminary rese.docxmccormicknadine86
 
Option 1 ImperialismThe exploitation of  colonial resources.docx
Option 1 ImperialismThe exploitation of  colonial resources.docxOption 1 ImperialismThe exploitation of  colonial resources.docx
Option 1 ImperialismThe exploitation of  colonial resources.docxmccormicknadine86
 
Option Wireless LTD v. OpenPeak, Inc.Be sure to save an elec.docx
Option Wireless LTD v. OpenPeak, Inc.Be sure to save an elec.docxOption Wireless LTD v. OpenPeak, Inc.Be sure to save an elec.docx
Option Wireless LTD v. OpenPeak, Inc.Be sure to save an elec.docxmccormicknadine86
 
Option A Land SharkWhen is a shark just a shark Consider the.docx
Option A Land SharkWhen is a shark just a shark Consider the.docxOption A Land SharkWhen is a shark just a shark Consider the.docx
Option A Land SharkWhen is a shark just a shark Consider the.docxmccormicknadine86
 
Option 3 Discuss your thoughts on drugs and deviance. Do you think .docx
Option 3 Discuss your thoughts on drugs and deviance. Do you think .docxOption 3 Discuss your thoughts on drugs and deviance. Do you think .docx
Option 3 Discuss your thoughts on drugs and deviance. Do you think .docxmccormicknadine86
 
OPTION 2 Can we make the changes we need to make After the pandemi.docx
OPTION 2 Can we make the changes we need to make After the pandemi.docxOPTION 2 Can we make the changes we need to make After the pandemi.docx
OPTION 2 Can we make the changes we need to make After the pandemi.docxmccormicknadine86
 
Option 1 You will create a PowerPoint (or equivalent) of your p.docx
Option 1 You will create a PowerPoint (or equivalent) of your p.docxOption 1 You will create a PowerPoint (or equivalent) of your p.docx
Option 1 You will create a PowerPoint (or equivalent) of your p.docxmccormicknadine86
 
Option A Description of Dance StylesSelect two styles of danc.docx
Option A Description of Dance StylesSelect two styles of danc.docxOption A Description of Dance StylesSelect two styles of danc.docx
Option A Description of Dance StylesSelect two styles of danc.docxmccormicknadine86
 
Option #2Provide several slides that explain the key section.docx
Option #2Provide several slides that explain the key section.docxOption #2Provide several slides that explain the key section.docx
Option #2Provide several slides that explain the key section.docxmccormicknadine86
 
Option 2 Slavery vs. Indentured ServitudeExplain how and wh.docx
Option 2 Slavery vs. Indentured ServitudeExplain how and wh.docxOption 2 Slavery vs. Indentured ServitudeExplain how and wh.docx
Option 2 Slavery vs. Indentured ServitudeExplain how and wh.docxmccormicknadine86
 
Option 2 ArtSelect any 2 of works of art about the Holocaus.docx
Option 2 ArtSelect any 2 of works of art about the Holocaus.docxOption 2 ArtSelect any 2 of works of art about the Holocaus.docx
Option 2 ArtSelect any 2 of works of art about the Holocaus.docxmccormicknadine86
 
Option #1 Stanford University Prison Experiment Causality, C.docx
Option #1 Stanford University Prison Experiment Causality, C.docxOption #1 Stanford University Prison Experiment Causality, C.docx
Option #1 Stanford University Prison Experiment Causality, C.docxmccormicknadine86
 
Option A  Gender CrimesCriminal acts occur against individu.docx
Option A  Gender CrimesCriminal acts occur against individu.docxOption A  Gender CrimesCriminal acts occur against individu.docx
Option A  Gender CrimesCriminal acts occur against individu.docxmccormicknadine86
 
opic 4 Discussion Question 1 May students express religious bel.docx
opic 4 Discussion Question 1 May students express religious bel.docxopic 4 Discussion Question 1 May students express religious bel.docx
opic 4 Discussion Question 1 May students express religious bel.docxmccormicknadine86
 
Option 1Choose a philosopher who interests you. Research that p.docx
Option 1Choose a philosopher who interests you. Research that p.docxOption 1Choose a philosopher who interests you. Research that p.docx
Option 1Choose a philosopher who interests you. Research that p.docxmccormicknadine86
 
Option #1The Stanford University Prison Experiment Structu.docx
Option #1The Stanford University Prison Experiment Structu.docxOption #1The Stanford University Prison Experiment Structu.docx
Option #1The Stanford University Prison Experiment Structu.docxmccormicknadine86
 
Operationaland Organizational SecurityChapter 3Princ.docx
Operationaland Organizational SecurityChapter 3Princ.docxOperationaland Organizational SecurityChapter 3Princ.docx
Operationaland Organizational SecurityChapter 3Princ.docxmccormicknadine86
 
Open the file (Undergrad Reqt_Individual In-Depth Case Study) for in.docx
Open the file (Undergrad Reqt_Individual In-Depth Case Study) for in.docxOpen the file (Undergrad Reqt_Individual In-Depth Case Study) for in.docx
Open the file (Undergrad Reqt_Individual In-Depth Case Study) for in.docxmccormicknadine86
 
onsider whether you think means-tested programs, such as the Tem.docx
onsider whether you think means-tested programs, such as the Tem.docxonsider whether you think means-tested programs, such as the Tem.docx
onsider whether you think means-tested programs, such as the Tem.docxmccormicknadine86
 
Operations security - PPT should cover below questions (chapter 1 to.docx
Operations security - PPT should cover below questions (chapter 1 to.docxOperations security - PPT should cover below questions (chapter 1 to.docx
Operations security - PPT should cover below questions (chapter 1 to.docxmccormicknadine86
 

More from mccormicknadine86 (20)

Option #2Researching a Leader Complete preliminary rese.docx
Option #2Researching a Leader Complete preliminary rese.docxOption #2Researching a Leader Complete preliminary rese.docx
Option #2Researching a Leader Complete preliminary rese.docx
 
Option 1 ImperialismThe exploitation of  colonial resources.docx
Option 1 ImperialismThe exploitation of  colonial resources.docxOption 1 ImperialismThe exploitation of  colonial resources.docx
Option 1 ImperialismThe exploitation of  colonial resources.docx
 
Option Wireless LTD v. OpenPeak, Inc.Be sure to save an elec.docx
Option Wireless LTD v. OpenPeak, Inc.Be sure to save an elec.docxOption Wireless LTD v. OpenPeak, Inc.Be sure to save an elec.docx
Option Wireless LTD v. OpenPeak, Inc.Be sure to save an elec.docx
 
Option A Land SharkWhen is a shark just a shark Consider the.docx
Option A Land SharkWhen is a shark just a shark Consider the.docxOption A Land SharkWhen is a shark just a shark Consider the.docx
Option A Land SharkWhen is a shark just a shark Consider the.docx
 
Option 3 Discuss your thoughts on drugs and deviance. Do you think .docx
Option 3 Discuss your thoughts on drugs and deviance. Do you think .docxOption 3 Discuss your thoughts on drugs and deviance. Do you think .docx
Option 3 Discuss your thoughts on drugs and deviance. Do you think .docx
 
OPTION 2 Can we make the changes we need to make After the pandemi.docx
OPTION 2 Can we make the changes we need to make After the pandemi.docxOPTION 2 Can we make the changes we need to make After the pandemi.docx
OPTION 2 Can we make the changes we need to make After the pandemi.docx
 
Option 1 You will create a PowerPoint (or equivalent) of your p.docx
Option 1 You will create a PowerPoint (or equivalent) of your p.docxOption 1 You will create a PowerPoint (or equivalent) of your p.docx
Option 1 You will create a PowerPoint (or equivalent) of your p.docx
 
Option A Description of Dance StylesSelect two styles of danc.docx
Option A Description of Dance StylesSelect two styles of danc.docxOption A Description of Dance StylesSelect two styles of danc.docx
Option A Description of Dance StylesSelect two styles of danc.docx
 
Option #2Provide several slides that explain the key section.docx
Option #2Provide several slides that explain the key section.docxOption #2Provide several slides that explain the key section.docx
Option #2Provide several slides that explain the key section.docx
 
Option 2 Slavery vs. Indentured ServitudeExplain how and wh.docx
Option 2 Slavery vs. Indentured ServitudeExplain how and wh.docxOption 2 Slavery vs. Indentured ServitudeExplain how and wh.docx
Option 2 Slavery vs. Indentured ServitudeExplain how and wh.docx
 
Option 2 ArtSelect any 2 of works of art about the Holocaus.docx
Option 2 ArtSelect any 2 of works of art about the Holocaus.docxOption 2 ArtSelect any 2 of works of art about the Holocaus.docx
Option 2 ArtSelect any 2 of works of art about the Holocaus.docx
 
Option #1 Stanford University Prison Experiment Causality, C.docx
Option #1 Stanford University Prison Experiment Causality, C.docxOption #1 Stanford University Prison Experiment Causality, C.docx
Option #1 Stanford University Prison Experiment Causality, C.docx
 
Option A  Gender CrimesCriminal acts occur against individu.docx
Option A  Gender CrimesCriminal acts occur against individu.docxOption A  Gender CrimesCriminal acts occur against individu.docx
Option A  Gender CrimesCriminal acts occur against individu.docx
 
opic 4 Discussion Question 1 May students express religious bel.docx
opic 4 Discussion Question 1 May students express religious bel.docxopic 4 Discussion Question 1 May students express religious bel.docx
opic 4 Discussion Question 1 May students express religious bel.docx
 
Option 1Choose a philosopher who interests you. Research that p.docx
Option 1Choose a philosopher who interests you. Research that p.docxOption 1Choose a philosopher who interests you. Research that p.docx
Option 1Choose a philosopher who interests you. Research that p.docx
 
Option #1The Stanford University Prison Experiment Structu.docx
Option #1The Stanford University Prison Experiment Structu.docxOption #1The Stanford University Prison Experiment Structu.docx
Option #1The Stanford University Prison Experiment Structu.docx
 
Operationaland Organizational SecurityChapter 3Princ.docx
Operationaland Organizational SecurityChapter 3Princ.docxOperationaland Organizational SecurityChapter 3Princ.docx
Operationaland Organizational SecurityChapter 3Princ.docx
 
Open the file (Undergrad Reqt_Individual In-Depth Case Study) for in.docx
Open the file (Undergrad Reqt_Individual In-Depth Case Study) for in.docxOpen the file (Undergrad Reqt_Individual In-Depth Case Study) for in.docx
Open the file (Undergrad Reqt_Individual In-Depth Case Study) for in.docx
 
onsider whether you think means-tested programs, such as the Tem.docx
onsider whether you think means-tested programs, such as the Tem.docxonsider whether you think means-tested programs, such as the Tem.docx
onsider whether you think means-tested programs, such as the Tem.docx
 
Operations security - PPT should cover below questions (chapter 1 to.docx
Operations security - PPT should cover below questions (chapter 1 to.docxOperations security - PPT should cover below questions (chapter 1 to.docx
Operations security - PPT should cover below questions (chapter 1 to.docx
 

Recently uploaded

Sanyam Choudhary Chemistry practical.pdf
Sanyam Choudhary Chemistry practical.pdfSanyam Choudhary Chemistry practical.pdf
Sanyam Choudhary Chemistry practical.pdfsanyamsingh5019
 
CARE OF CHILD IN INCUBATOR..........pptx
CARE OF CHILD IN INCUBATOR..........pptxCARE OF CHILD IN INCUBATOR..........pptx
CARE OF CHILD IN INCUBATOR..........pptxGaneshChakor2
 
Accessible design: Minimum effort, maximum impact
Accessible design: Minimum effort, maximum impactAccessible design: Minimum effort, maximum impact
Accessible design: Minimum effort, maximum impactdawncurless
 
Presentation by Andreas Schleicher Tackling the School Absenteeism Crisis 30 ...
Presentation by Andreas Schleicher Tackling the School Absenteeism Crisis 30 ...Presentation by Andreas Schleicher Tackling the School Absenteeism Crisis 30 ...
Presentation by Andreas Schleicher Tackling the School Absenteeism Crisis 30 ...EduSkills OECD
 
Russian Call Girls in Andheri Airport Mumbai WhatsApp 9167673311 💞 Full Nigh...
Russian Call Girls in Andheri Airport Mumbai WhatsApp  9167673311 💞 Full Nigh...Russian Call Girls in Andheri Airport Mumbai WhatsApp  9167673311 💞 Full Nigh...
Russian Call Girls in Andheri Airport Mumbai WhatsApp 9167673311 💞 Full Nigh...Pooja Nehwal
 
The basics of sentences session 2pptx copy.pptx
The basics of sentences session 2pptx copy.pptxThe basics of sentences session 2pptx copy.pptx
The basics of sentences session 2pptx copy.pptxheathfieldcps1
 
Measures of Central Tendency: Mean, Median and Mode
Measures of Central Tendency: Mean, Median and ModeMeasures of Central Tendency: Mean, Median and Mode
Measures of Central Tendency: Mean, Median and ModeThiyagu K
 
Grant Readiness 101 TechSoup and Remy Consulting
Grant Readiness 101 TechSoup and Remy ConsultingGrant Readiness 101 TechSoup and Remy Consulting
Grant Readiness 101 TechSoup and Remy ConsultingTechSoup
 
Advanced Views - Calendar View in Odoo 17
Advanced Views - Calendar View in Odoo 17Advanced Views - Calendar View in Odoo 17
Advanced Views - Calendar View in Odoo 17Celine George
 
Introduction to Nonprofit Accounting: The Basics
Introduction to Nonprofit Accounting: The BasicsIntroduction to Nonprofit Accounting: The Basics
Introduction to Nonprofit Accounting: The BasicsTechSoup
 
1029 - Danh muc Sach Giao Khoa 10 . pdf
1029 -  Danh muc Sach Giao Khoa 10 . pdf1029 -  Danh muc Sach Giao Khoa 10 . pdf
1029 - Danh muc Sach Giao Khoa 10 . pdfQucHHunhnh
 
POINT- BIOCHEMISTRY SEM 2 ENZYMES UNIT 5.pptx
POINT- BIOCHEMISTRY SEM 2 ENZYMES UNIT 5.pptxPOINT- BIOCHEMISTRY SEM 2 ENZYMES UNIT 5.pptx
POINT- BIOCHEMISTRY SEM 2 ENZYMES UNIT 5.pptxSayali Powar
 
Disha NEET Physics Guide for classes 11 and 12.pdf
Disha NEET Physics Guide for classes 11 and 12.pdfDisha NEET Physics Guide for classes 11 and 12.pdf
Disha NEET Physics Guide for classes 11 and 12.pdfchloefrazer622
 
social pharmacy d-pharm 1st year by Pragati K. Mahajan
social pharmacy d-pharm 1st year by Pragati K. Mahajansocial pharmacy d-pharm 1st year by Pragati K. Mahajan
social pharmacy d-pharm 1st year by Pragati K. Mahajanpragatimahajan3
 
1029-Danh muc Sach Giao Khoa khoi 6.pdf
1029-Danh muc Sach Giao Khoa khoi  6.pdf1029-Danh muc Sach Giao Khoa khoi  6.pdf
1029-Danh muc Sach Giao Khoa khoi 6.pdfQucHHunhnh
 
BAG TECHNIQUE Bag technique-a tool making use of public health bag through wh...
BAG TECHNIQUE Bag technique-a tool making use of public health bag through wh...BAG TECHNIQUE Bag technique-a tool making use of public health bag through wh...
BAG TECHNIQUE Bag technique-a tool making use of public health bag through wh...Sapna Thakur
 
Call Girls in Dwarka Mor Delhi Contact Us 9654467111
Call Girls in Dwarka Mor Delhi Contact Us 9654467111Call Girls in Dwarka Mor Delhi Contact Us 9654467111
Call Girls in Dwarka Mor Delhi Contact Us 9654467111Sapana Sha
 
Organic Name Reactions for the students and aspirants of Chemistry12th.pptx
Organic Name Reactions  for the students and aspirants of Chemistry12th.pptxOrganic Name Reactions  for the students and aspirants of Chemistry12th.pptx
Organic Name Reactions for the students and aspirants of Chemistry12th.pptxVS Mahajan Coaching Centre
 

Recently uploaded (20)

Sanyam Choudhary Chemistry practical.pdf
Sanyam Choudhary Chemistry practical.pdfSanyam Choudhary Chemistry practical.pdf
Sanyam Choudhary Chemistry practical.pdf
 
CARE OF CHILD IN INCUBATOR..........pptx
CARE OF CHILD IN INCUBATOR..........pptxCARE OF CHILD IN INCUBATOR..........pptx
CARE OF CHILD IN INCUBATOR..........pptx
 
Accessible design: Minimum effort, maximum impact
Accessible design: Minimum effort, maximum impactAccessible design: Minimum effort, maximum impact
Accessible design: Minimum effort, maximum impact
 
Presentation by Andreas Schleicher Tackling the School Absenteeism Crisis 30 ...
Presentation by Andreas Schleicher Tackling the School Absenteeism Crisis 30 ...Presentation by Andreas Schleicher Tackling the School Absenteeism Crisis 30 ...
Presentation by Andreas Schleicher Tackling the School Absenteeism Crisis 30 ...
 
Russian Call Girls in Andheri Airport Mumbai WhatsApp 9167673311 💞 Full Nigh...
Russian Call Girls in Andheri Airport Mumbai WhatsApp  9167673311 💞 Full Nigh...Russian Call Girls in Andheri Airport Mumbai WhatsApp  9167673311 💞 Full Nigh...
Russian Call Girls in Andheri Airport Mumbai WhatsApp 9167673311 💞 Full Nigh...
 
The basics of sentences session 2pptx copy.pptx
The basics of sentences session 2pptx copy.pptxThe basics of sentences session 2pptx copy.pptx
The basics of sentences session 2pptx copy.pptx
 
Measures of Central Tendency: Mean, Median and Mode
Measures of Central Tendency: Mean, Median and ModeMeasures of Central Tendency: Mean, Median and Mode
Measures of Central Tendency: Mean, Median and Mode
 
Grant Readiness 101 TechSoup and Remy Consulting
Grant Readiness 101 TechSoup and Remy ConsultingGrant Readiness 101 TechSoup and Remy Consulting
Grant Readiness 101 TechSoup and Remy Consulting
 
Advanced Views - Calendar View in Odoo 17
Advanced Views - Calendar View in Odoo 17Advanced Views - Calendar View in Odoo 17
Advanced Views - Calendar View in Odoo 17
 
Introduction to Nonprofit Accounting: The Basics
Introduction to Nonprofit Accounting: The BasicsIntroduction to Nonprofit Accounting: The Basics
Introduction to Nonprofit Accounting: The Basics
 
1029 - Danh muc Sach Giao Khoa 10 . pdf
1029 -  Danh muc Sach Giao Khoa 10 . pdf1029 -  Danh muc Sach Giao Khoa 10 . pdf
1029 - Danh muc Sach Giao Khoa 10 . pdf
 
POINT- BIOCHEMISTRY SEM 2 ENZYMES UNIT 5.pptx
POINT- BIOCHEMISTRY SEM 2 ENZYMES UNIT 5.pptxPOINT- BIOCHEMISTRY SEM 2 ENZYMES UNIT 5.pptx
POINT- BIOCHEMISTRY SEM 2 ENZYMES UNIT 5.pptx
 
Disha NEET Physics Guide for classes 11 and 12.pdf
Disha NEET Physics Guide for classes 11 and 12.pdfDisha NEET Physics Guide for classes 11 and 12.pdf
Disha NEET Physics Guide for classes 11 and 12.pdf
 
social pharmacy d-pharm 1st year by Pragati K. Mahajan
social pharmacy d-pharm 1st year by Pragati K. Mahajansocial pharmacy d-pharm 1st year by Pragati K. Mahajan
social pharmacy d-pharm 1st year by Pragati K. Mahajan
 
1029-Danh muc Sach Giao Khoa khoi 6.pdf
1029-Danh muc Sach Giao Khoa khoi  6.pdf1029-Danh muc Sach Giao Khoa khoi  6.pdf
1029-Danh muc Sach Giao Khoa khoi 6.pdf
 
Mattingly "AI & Prompt Design: Structured Data, Assistants, & RAG"
Mattingly "AI & Prompt Design: Structured Data, Assistants, & RAG"Mattingly "AI & Prompt Design: Structured Data, Assistants, & RAG"
Mattingly "AI & Prompt Design: Structured Data, Assistants, & RAG"
 
BAG TECHNIQUE Bag technique-a tool making use of public health bag through wh...
BAG TECHNIQUE Bag technique-a tool making use of public health bag through wh...BAG TECHNIQUE Bag technique-a tool making use of public health bag through wh...
BAG TECHNIQUE Bag technique-a tool making use of public health bag through wh...
 
INDIA QUIZ 2024 RLAC DELHI UNIVERSITY.pptx
INDIA QUIZ 2024 RLAC DELHI UNIVERSITY.pptxINDIA QUIZ 2024 RLAC DELHI UNIVERSITY.pptx
INDIA QUIZ 2024 RLAC DELHI UNIVERSITY.pptx
 
Call Girls in Dwarka Mor Delhi Contact Us 9654467111
Call Girls in Dwarka Mor Delhi Contact Us 9654467111Call Girls in Dwarka Mor Delhi Contact Us 9654467111
Call Girls in Dwarka Mor Delhi Contact Us 9654467111
 
Organic Name Reactions for the students and aspirants of Chemistry12th.pptx
Organic Name Reactions  for the students and aspirants of Chemistry12th.pptxOrganic Name Reactions  for the students and aspirants of Chemistry12th.pptx
Organic Name Reactions for the students and aspirants of Chemistry12th.pptx
 

Colorado V. Connelly479 U.S 157 (1986)Facts· Co.docx

  • 1. Colorado V. Connelly 479 U.S 157 (1986) Facts: · Connelly approached a police officer and showed interest on confessing about a murder that he committed. · Connelly had a history of mental illness and had been of his medication for at least 6 months. · Before Connelly made a confession, the officer gave Connelly his Miranda warnings and called a detective that before interrogating him, read him again his Miranda warnings. · Connelly acknowledge the understanding of his Miranda rights and confessed that he murder a young girl. · The next morning Connelly was disoriented and claimed that voices made him confess to the murder. · Connelly then was diagnosed by a psychiatrist with schizophrenia and wanted to suppress the use of his statements in court because his psychotic condition made him confess but he was able to understand his Miranda rights. Procedural History: · Supreme court of Colorado suppressed the evidence · U.S Supreme Court Law: · “No person shall be held to answer for a capital, or otherwise infamous crime, unless on a presentment or indictment of a Grand Jury, except in cases arising in the land or naval forces, or in the Militia, when in actual service in time of War or public
  • 2. danger; nor shall any person be subject for the same offense to be twice put in jeopardy of life or limb; nor shall be compelled in any criminal case to be a witness against himself, nor be deprived of life, liberty, or property, without due process of law; nor shall private property be taken for public use, without just compensation.” -5th amendment · All persons born or naturalized in the United States, and subject to the jurisdiction thereof, are citizens of the United States and of the state wherein they reside. No state shall make or enforce any law which shall abridge the privileges or immunities of citizens of the United States; nor shall any state deprive any person of life, liberty, or property, without due process of law; nor deny to any person within its jurisdiction the equal protection of the laws. -14th amendment Legal Question: · Can Connelly’s statement be suppressed, since he made it in a state of mind in which he was not aware of the situation and the consequences? Holding and vote: No (7-2) Reasoning: (Rehnquist, majority) joined by White, Blackmun, Powell, Stevens, O’Connor and scalia. 1. The admissibility of statements is governed by state rules of evidence, rather than previous supreme court decisions regarding coerced confessions and Miranda waivers. 2. The officers did not use any questionable methods to get a confession from Connelly, in fact they told him that he had the right to remain silent, yet he proceeded to confess. 3. Similar cases in which statements were suppressed due to the defendant’s mental state is because the officers exploited their mental health to get a confession. I.e Blackburn v. Alabama, 361 U. S. 199 (1960), and Townsend v. Sain, 372 U. S. 293
  • 3. (1963) 4. The due process clause of the 5th and 14th amendment was not violated because the defendant did everything voluntarily and somewhat knowingly because schizophrenia does not impairs comprehension. 5. Voluntary in the eyes of the law means, not being influenced by an external force, in this case the defendant was not influenced by any police officer. Concurring Opinion: (Blackmun and Stevens) · Justice Blackmun agrees with everything, except with part III A, where it was talked about the defendant waived his Miranda rights. He believes that it was unnecessary. · Justice Stevens believes that Connelly’s statement were involuntary. However the fifth amendment was not violated, therefore, it is not required to suppress the evidence gathered by the officers. Dissenting opinion: (Justice Brennan joined by Marshall) · Justice Brennan believe that the defendant did not made this confessions in the right state of mind because of his condition and his withdrawal from medication for a prolonged period of time. He also believe that his hallucination had some effect in his ability to comprehend his Miranda rights. The absence of police misconduct does not necessarily means that the confessions was made on free will. The court has reversed similar cases in which a person confessed to a crime due to medication (Townsend v. Sain,372 U. S. 293, 1963). Allowing to include mentally ill individual to voluntary confession is an error because more often than not this confessions are going to be unreliable Volume 4 1 /Number 3/July 2019/Pages 242-259/https: 11 ioi.org! IO.I7744/mehc.4l .3.04
  • 4. NEUROCOUNSELING Fostering Intrinsic Resilience: A Neuroscience-Informed Model of Conceptualizing and Treating Adverse Childhood Experiences Eraina Schauss Greg Horn Frances Ellmo Tegan Reeves Haley Z e ttle r Debra Bartelli Pam Cogdal Steven West An increasingly common dialogue among mental health professionals revolves around adverse childhood experiences (ACEs) and developmental trauma. ACEs can occur in a number of ways with a myriad of potential outcomes, often making treatment choices difficult. During critical stages of neurodevelopmental growth, trauma makes a mark on the brain and body at a physi- ological level. Although the National Institute of Mental Health’s Research Domain Criteria have been used to address this brain-body impact, the far- reaching scope of implications needs grounding in a theoretical framework. The current paper discusses developmental trauma and proposes a new reciprocally determinant model that advocates for neuroscience-informed coun-
  • 5. seling interventions such as neurofeedhack therapy. Clinical implications and considerations for counselors are discussed. Eraina Schauss, Department o f Counseling, Educational Psychology and Research, The University o f Memphis: Greg Horn, Department o f Counseling, Educational Psychology and Research, The University o f Memphis: Frances Ellmo, Department o f Counseling, Educational Psychology and Research, The University o f Memphis: Tegan R e e ve s, Department o f Counseling. Educational Psychology and Research, The University o f Memphis: Haley Zettler, Department o f Criminology and Criminal Justice, The University o f Memphis: Debra Bartelli, School o f Public Health, The University o f Memphis: Pam Cogdal, Department o f Counseling. Educational Psychology and Research, The University o f Memphis: Steven West, Department o f Counseling, Educational Psychology and Research. The University o f Memphis. This research is supported by a Memphis Research Consortium Grant (Grant #2 3 2 0 0 0 ) to the University o f Memphis in partnership with the University o f Tennessee H ealth Science Center. This article also acknowledges Kristi Nobbman in the conceptual development o f the reciprocally determinant b ra in - body model. Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to Eraina Schauss, Department o f Counseling, Educational Psychology and Research, Patterson H all Room 123, The University o f Memphis, Memphis, TN 38152. E-mail: [email protected] 242 0 Journal o f Mental Health Counseling mailto:[email protected]
  • 6. F o s t e r in g I n t r i n s i c R e s ilie n c e Mental health professionals increasingly seek information on approaches to childhood maltreatment in terms of the prevention and treatment of trau- matic response. Counselors seeking applicable information often find theories and approaches siloed within specialties and fields of practice. For instance, social, interpersonal, and biological theories each provide useful explanations, yet are limited in explaining how such variables interact. The model we present provides insight into the concept of developmental trauma by examining the reciprocal interaction of clients’ environment, behavior, and personal factors and a regulatory self-system (Bandura, 1978) that includes biological and neu- rological processes. This theoretical framework aims to bridge important fields of research including developmental psychology and neuroscience. ADVERSE CHILDHOOD EXPERIENCES AND DEVELOPMENTAL TRAUMA Adverse childhood experiences (ACEs) include abuse (emotional, phys- ical, and sexual), household challenges (e.g., witnessing parent- to-parent violence, household substance abuse, mental illness in household, parental separation or divorce, incarcerated household member), and
  • 7. emotional and physical neglect (Felitti et ah, 1998). Since Felitti et al. (1998) found in their seminal study that ACEs were linked to chronic illness in adulthood and early morbidity, research examining the profound impact of ACEs has become a priority for the Centers for Disease Control and Prevention (2018) and other research organizations. In the past two decades, this extensive field of research has established early childhood maltreatment as a significant etiological factor for a wide range of health and psychological conditions. A comprehensive review of this literature is beyond the scope of this article; however, to summa- rize, children and adolescents who experience a high number of ACEs tend to manifest an increased num ber of psychiatric conditions (Giaconia et al., 1995) and a multitude of behavioral and health-related complications throughout their life span (Schilling, Aseltine, & Gore, 2007). Research also indicates that ACEs are more common among youth with juvenile justice involvement (Zettler, Wolff, Baglivio, Craig, & Epps, 2017). The long-term physiological response to trauma appears to link ACEs and chronic health problems (van der Kolk, 1994). Specifically, a dose effect was found linking greater ACE exposure in children and adolescents with greater instances of obesity, diabetes, heart disease, maladaptive sleep patterns, autoimmune disorders, and
  • 8. inflammation, ultimately leading to a lifetime of chronic physical and mental illness (Felitti et al., 1998). Felitti et al. (1998) further theorized a causal link between ACEs and early death mediated by three categories of factors in a linear fashion. According to 0 Journal o f Mental Health Counseling 243 their model, ACEs lead to social, emotional, and cognitive impairment, which, in turn, leads to the adoption of health-risk behaviors, often resulting in dis- ease, disability, and ultimately early death. This explanation, which admirably accounts for social and psychological factors, represents the medical model, which tends to identify (and treat) the root cause of pathology in a linear fashion. The medical model is the long-standing dominant model in health care (Engel, 1977), has many strengths, and is clearly an appropriate model for understanding the factors that lead to early death. However, the medical model also has significant limitations, especially when applied to dynamic social and interpersonal processes. For instance, due to the assumption of cause and effect, when applied to ACEs, the medical
  • 9. model can imply that “what is done is done” and suggest a sense of hopeless- ness to clients and a subsequently defeatist and challenging position for coun- selors. In addition, the scale used to measure ACEs assigns the same weight to each type of adverse experience (Felitti et ah, 1998). However, research shows that not all childhood maltreatment has an equal effect; some adverse experiences are more traumatizing than others (Schwartz & Perry, 1994), and the degree of long-term traumatic effects can depend on subjective mediating factors such as loneliness (Shelvin, McElroy, & Murphy, 2015) or social sup- port (Allbaugh et ah, 2018). The medical model does little to acknowledge the subjective experience of an individual. In addition, the ACE scale is additive, as researchers have argued that they are interested in the cumulative effect of ACEs (Thompson, Richards, & Gaysina, 2017). However, an additive measure of adverse experience does not acknowledge possible interaction effects. In such instances, 1 + 1 does not necessarily equal 2 when adverse experiences compound one another and have an exponentially greater negative effect for the child. For example, consider a child whose father is incarcerated and who is physically abused by his overly stressed single mother, with no other adult support to help cope with the physical abuse. Clearly, adverse experiences can
  • 10. have an effect on each other, with the resulting impact being greater than the sum of its parts. Finally, the medical model does not address how previous adverse experiences can make individuals vulnerable to future adverse expe- riences. For instance, failure to identify traumatic response behaviors as such can exacerbate intense emotions and lead to application of stigmatizing labels such as oppositional, rebellious, unmotivated, or antisocial (van der Kolk, 2005, p. 404), which can have long-term effects on self-concept (Rockett, Murrie, & Boccaccini, 2007). 244 0 Journal o f Mental H ealth Counseling Fostering In trin s ic Resilience VIEWING ADVERSE CHILDHOOD EXPERIENCES THROUGH A RECIPROCAL DETERMINANT LENS Social, interpersonal, and attachm ent theories have offered explanations for the sequelae of childhood maltreatment and have addressed many of the issues the medical model could not (Siegel, 2012; van der Kolk, 2015). The concept of reciprocal determinism from Bandura’s social learning theory, in our view, provides a particularly useful means by which to address the complex-
  • 11. ity of issues that arise from ACEs. Bandura (1978) postulated the self system within his reciprocal determinism model to describe cognitive mechanisms by which the individual uses perception and assessment of past or potential future experience to regulate behavior. This process was theorized to involve bidirectional relationships among behavior, person, and environment. Thus, the self system is used as an analytical self-regulatory process and describes the psychosocial interactions within the individual (Bandura, 1978). Whereas the medical model is limited in that it does not account for reciprocal influences, Bandura’s model is limited in that it does little to address physical and neurological processes in the body. In this paper we propose an expansion of a theoretical model that incorporates an understanding of neurological process in the context of developmental trauma and ACEs. O ur model will be discussed in detail, and implications for clinical practice will be presented. RESEARCH DOMAIN CRITERIA An impetus for change in understanding and conceptualizing develop- mental trauma is the rise of the Research Domain Criteria (RDoC) project, a National Institute of Mental Health (NIMH) movement to develop research
  • 12. frameworks incorporating neurobiological, genetic, behavioral, and cognitive influences on the classification of mental disorders. The RDoC initiative emphasizes neural circuitry, levels of arousal, regulatory systems, social pro- cesses, cognitions, developmental trajectories, and the dynamic interaction between an individual and their environment. The rise of the RDoC move- m ent emphasizes the desire for a precision medicine approach, “an emerging approach for disease treatment and prevention that takes into account individ- ual variability in genes, environment and lifestyle for each person” (National Institutes of Health, as cited in Adams & Petersen, 2016, p. 787), to mental health treatment. The RDoC paradigm shift comes at a critical time in mental health care. RDoC seeks to foster a more precise way of conceptualizing and treating m ental disorders that will transform treatment and public health outcomes 0 Journal o f Mental Health Counseling 245 through four main objectives. The first strategic objective of RDoC is to define the mechanisms of complex behavior. The second objective is to identify and characterize sensitive periods of brain development across the
  • 13. life span with a goal of determining “the points in time during which the brain is most sensitive to intervention and the underlying molecular and system-level mechanisms responsible for this sensitivity” (NIMH, 2015, p. 32). The third strategic objective of RDoC concerns the areas of treatment, prevention, and cures. The objective seeks to integrate neuroscience, genomics, and evidence-based behavioral science methodologies into more precise treatments. Additionally, a need exists to develop new interventions and tailor current treatments to indi- vidualized symptoms. The fourth and final RDoC objective is to strengthen the public health impact of NIMH-supported research (NIMH, 2015). The current ideological shift is to understand and integrate basic neuro- science findings into evidence-based clinical practice and interventions with the ultimate goal of delivering the most accurate diagnosis at the earliest point, followed by precise and individualized treatments. To pave the way for a pre- cision medicine approach in mental health counseling, additional diagnostic evaluations must be incorporated into treatment, which examine symptoms, behaviors, cognition, genotype, physiology, environmental exposures, cultural background, and family dynamics (NIMH, 2015). As such, the task of answer-
  • 14. ing the call of the RDoC must include counselors and not solely medical professionals and scientists. Our unique counseling perspective brings a much-needed focus on holis- tic wellness, individualized treatment, and social justice considerations and provides the opportunity to change the way other helping professions concep- tualize mental health. INTEGRATING NEUROSCIENCE AND COUNSELING Given the RDoC emphasis on precision medicine, neuroscience is a logical field for mental health counselors to embrace in order to understand the complex mechanisms underlying mental health disorders. The field of neurocounseling, the “integration of neuroscience into the practice of coun- seling” (Russell-Chapin, 2016, p. 93), although in its infancy, has already shed new light on how counselors conceptualize and treat clients. Neuroscience findings have profound implications for the field of counseling and provide a heuristic for counselors to use evidence-based treatments with clients with a history of ACEs (Navalta, McGee, & Underwood, 2018). Neuroscientists have identified forms of psychosocial engagement, such as nurturing and caring, healthy behaviors, and environmental enrichment, that promote neuroplasti-
  • 15. 246 ^ Journal o f Mental Health Counseling Fostering Intrinsic Resilience city (Gonqalves & Perrone-McGovern, 2014), the brain’s ability to reorganize structural neural connections. One way the brain does this is through a process called memory recon- solidation. According to Tronson and Taylor (2007), memories that have been learned, or achieve neuronal consolidation, can become labile when activated. During this labile phase, a memory can become either reconsolidated or transformed if contradictory information is presented within a window of time. Tronson and Taylor (2007) argued that this process is distinct from the extinc- tion process in classical conditioning. This finding has clear implications for emotionally learned processes such as phobias, trauma-related disorders, and some aspects of other anxiety disorders. In addition, neurons are understood to fire when people feel, think, and behave. Interestingly, neuroscientists have found that mirror neurons fire in a similar pattern when we observe others feeling, thinking, and behaving (Ivey & Zalaquett, 2011). Mirror neurons have implications for
  • 16. counselors’ use of empathy and for work with clients who have difficulty empathizing with oth- ers. The study of brain networks in neuroscience has strengthened the theory that the brain does not work in isolated sections, but in functionally connected regions. This paradigm has led neuroscientists to hypothesize “pathways” that communicate among the brain’s structures. Some of the brain networks that have been proposed are the default mode network (resting state), attention network, social cognition network, motivational network, and emotional regu- lation network (Gongalves & Perrone-McGovern, 2014). As implied by these names, these networks facilitate (or do not facilitate) many of the functions addressed in counseling. Though these neuroscience concepts have clear implications for coun- seling, counselors may still be at a loss for what to do with them. One concern is that integrating neuroscience into counseling would be reductionist. For instance, neural bases have been used to explain mental health disorders such as posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD), depression, and anxiety disorders (Grawe, 1997). Some may be tempted to localize a disorder to a specific cere- bral region and neglect social, cultural, and interpersonal factors. However, neuroscientists have long viewed the brain as a dynamic system
  • 17. with complex networks (Cole, 1978), and more recently as a system that informs and is shaped by the social and interpersonal environment (Siegel, 2012). Though it may be tempting to reduce mental health conditions to a specific region of the brain, research supports a reciprocal view of the interaction between the brain and the environment. Therefore, a comprehensive and pragmatic model is needed, one that integrates social and regulatory factors that counselors can use to inform assessments, hypotheses, and treatment decisions. ^ Journal of Mental Health Counseling 247 Linear counseling models have been offered to consider the complex neural processes that occur during the counseling process and how these processes affect or are affected by the environment. Decety & Jackson (2004) outlined the parallel neural processes that occur during key components of empathy, such as self-awareness, mental flexibility, and emotion regulation. Gonsalves and Perrone-McGovern (2014) presented a linear model that posits the interpersonal and environmental factors that influence the psychological process, which in turn affect brain networks, which impact developmental tasks. Though these models effectively integrate neuroscience
  • 18. and counseling, they fail to address the reciprocal interaction between the brain and the envi- ronment. THE RECIPROCALLY DETERMINANT BRAIN-BODY MODEL Our approach extends the reach of current counseling interventions that address the aftermath of ACE exposure and acknowledges the interactive influence of environment and behavioral patterning within the brain and body. This addition to current family and social systems approaches assumes a reciprocal relationship among environment, behavior, person, and physiology (brain-body). Regarding research on neurobiophysiological factors of development (Siegel, 2012; van der Kolk, 2015), we extend the model of reciprocal deter- minism to include physiology, or the brain-body connection. This extended neurobiophysiological model of reciprocal determinism provides a theoretical framework for research examining how integrated therapies, such as neuro- feedback (NEB), facilitate the regulation of brainwave activity, emotional and behavioral self-regulation, and physiology. Further, the model provides a con- text for diagnosing maladaptive stress responses that may present as a myriad of
  • 19. symptom clusters and/or co-occurring disorders. In our model, neurobiophysiological functioning such as the vagal response (i.e., fight, flight, or freeze; Porges, 2011) and neuro- maladaptation (Siegel, 2012) influences the interaction of the person, environment, and behavior. When the self-regulatory system does not adequately develop or is interrupted by environmental demands, hyperarousal becomes chronic (Levine, 1997), which in turn affects perception and behavior toward the current environment. Responses during developmental experiences involve neurological and physiological changes in the central and peripheral nervous systems (Porges, 2011). Based on an understanding of research addressing the brain-body connection, the use of cognitive and behavioral therapies appears insufficient to address maladaptive responses, as these therapies only focus on change in one aspect of the self system and not the entire system. 248 0 Journal o f Mental Health Counseling F o s te rin g In tr in s ic R e s ilie n c e Person An individual’s personality and temperament have a bidirectional rela-
  • 20. tionship to behavior and environment. An individual who has been exposed to trauma during a developmentally sensitive period may have maladaptive patterns of coping and dissociation that can lead to poor health. For example, stressors early in life can cause dysfunction in the brain-gut microbiota axis and therefore increase the likelihood of disease and alter cognitions, moods, and emotions (Cryan & Dinan, 2012; Rhee, Pothoulakis, & Mayer, 2009). Behavior An individual’s behavior can influence and be influenced by their person and environment. An individual who has been exposed to trauma in a devel- opmentally sensitive period may present with a myriad of bidirectional mal- adaptive behavioral responses, from aggression to juvenile delinquency. For an individual exposed to trauma, the reciprocally deterministic nature of behavior with environment and person can be better understood and influenced through an integrated lens. Maladaptive behaviors in adolescence, such as aggression and poor regulation, are mechanisms in reaction to a stimulus. Responding to stress involves internal systems such as the autonomic nervous system (Porges, 2009) and is often a learned behavior (Bandura, 1978). Autonomic dysregula-
  • 21. tion, or hypersensitivity, may explain higher rates of conduct problems such as aggression and delinquency (Beauchaine, Gatzke-Kopp, & Mead, 2007). Environment An individual’s environment from prenatal inception to death includes physical environment, attachment, and genetic expression. An individual who has been exposed to environmental trauma in a developmentally sen- sitive period may present a number of social and physiological maladaptive responses. Long-standing research on bonding and attachment underlines the importance of environment for healthy, adaptive development. Insufficient early childhood attunement (Bowlby, 1980) and connection can lead to physio- logical developmental delays as well as social maladaptation later in life. Siegel (2012) argued that attachment problems in adulthood may be due to the lack of secure attachment or bonding in developmentally sensitive periods, leading to problems in neurological integration. Environmental context does not manifest solely in neurological expres- sion but also in genetic expression. Epidemiologists estimate that genes can explain approximately 10% to 20% of adverse health conditions. The vast majority of diseases are caused by a combination of, or
  • 22. interaction between, genes and the environment. Epigenetics, or “the study of changes in gene func- 0 Journal o f Mental Health Counseling 249 tion that are ... heritable and that do not entail a change in DNA sequence” (Wu & Morris, 2001), is a growing consideration for mental health providers (Simon-Dack & Marmarosh, 2014). Brain-Body Neural oscillations, or brainwaves, are one prominent way to understand the communication pattern between brain and body. Brainwaves are theorized to have a self-regulatory function based on behavior and demands from the environment. Looking at the function of each type of brainwave shows us the opportunities for regulation within those patterns. For example, slow delta waves are prominent during sleep; theta waves have high amplitude during light sleep and meditation; alpha waves are associated with relaxed, detached awareness; and beta waves are linked to a heightened state of alertness and critical thinking (Swingle, 2008). However, when brainwaves become dysregulated, they can become
  • 23. unhelpful (Hamlin, 2011). For instance, Clark, Barry, McCarthy, & Selikowitz (1998) found that excess delta and theta bandwidths were associated with hyperactivity and inattention. Individuals with more magnitude of beta waves are more likely to develop alcoholism (Rangaswamy et ah, 2002). Hamlin (2011) explained that these individuals seek alcohol, an external source of per- ceived relaxation, because they are unable to internally diminish the amplitude of beta waves, which have an arousing effect. Likewise, adverse experiences may require high beta waves, which are adaptive for being alert; however, when these patterns persist, they can lead to behaviors that are maladaptive in other environments (Hamlin, 2011). CLINICAL IMPLICATIONS The model provides counselors a framework to take a reciprocally deter- minant perspective of their clients’ presenting problems while being sensitive to the impact of ACEs (Felitti et ah, 1998) and aware of the role of neurobio- physiological processes as a response to interaction with the environment and underlying mechanisms for behavior (Siegel, 2012). Awareness of ACEs has grown in the counseling profession, and many counselors include an assess- ment of ACEs upon intake and through the conceptualization process. The
  • 24. model then facilitates client conceptualizations with consideration for interac- tions among ACEs, the person, their brain, and their environment. Counselors may use the model to make hypotheses about how these interactions impact development, mental health, behavior, physical health, and physiology. Underpinning the counseling profession are the principles of wellness, of a strengths-based focus, of the belief that intrinsic features of resilience are 250 0 Journal o f Mental H ealth Counseling F o s t e r i n g I n t r i n s i c R e s i l i e n c e inherent in an individual, and of the internal capacity for change and self- healing (Rogers, 1951). Given these beliefs, many professional counselors have embraced the integration of mind-body work in clinical practice. Examples of mind-body-based interventions include mindfulness meditation (Kabat-Zinn, 1982), dance/movement therapy (Payne, 2003), and somatic experiential therapy (Levine, 1997), to name a few. More and more therapeutic modalities have been developed that incorporate mind-body interventions alongside traditional talk-therapy methods such as acceptance and commitment therapy
  • 25. (Hayes, Luoma, Bond, Masuda, & Lillis, 2006), coherence therapy (Ecker & Toomey, 2008), dialectical behavior therapy (Linehan, 1993), and eye move- ment desensitization and reprocessing (Shapiro, 2001). Counselors practicing integrative therapeutic techniques are often looking for new evidence-based and neuroscience-informed interventions to support brain-body healing. As more counselors begin to understand the importance of including biological considerations in their assessment and treatment of clients, a model from which to conceptualize and treat clients is needed. Our model illustrates how biological considerations, such as brain activity, can complement the counsel- ing perspective. Our model suggests a need for greater measurement of neuro- biophysiological concerns as a part of the presenting problems of our clients. Neuro feedback A readily accessible and safe technology, NFB is an ideal methodology integrating the current findings of neuroscience with the body’s ability for self-healing. A type of biofeedback, NFB uses electroencephalogram (EEG) and operant conditioning to guide clients toward optimal cognitive flexibility based on cortical electrical activity, also referred to as brainwaves (Swingle, 2008). In an NFB session, a clinician applies sensors on the
  • 26. client’s scalp. The sensors detect brainwaves and feed the signal to computer software, which quantifies and transforms the data into auditory and/or visual stimuli for the client to observe, completing one of many reciprocal loops of ongoing informa- tion (Angelakis, Hatzis, Panourias, & Sakas, 2007). The nature of NFB, however, has presented a unique challenge for researchers. Studies have found improved outcomes for a wide range of prob- lems in research spanning more than four decades. However, few of these studies have convincingly attributed the results to the specific mechanism of feedback of a particular brainwave (Thibault & Raz, 2017). The double-blind randomized clinical trial design is the gold standard for minimizing most threats to internal validity, but the reciprocal nature of NFB presents numerous challenges for this design. Recent innovations are thought to have improved the science of NFB. For instance, live Z-Score NFB training allows the cli- 0 Journal o f Mental Health Counseling 251 nician to contrast the clients’ brain activity to a normative database, nesting an individual’s performance within a social and statistical context (Collura,
  • 27. 2017). However, a new model is needed for researchers to better understand the process of NFB. Although this article is practitioner oriented, a model that can be used by researchers and practitioners alike will improve the ability for these two professional activities to inform one another. In addition, research is needed to test and/or modify the reciprocally determinant brain- body model. Random control trial research is specifically needed to contextualize the model in order to further address current concerns about the efficacy and scope of the impact of NFB. The reciprocal interaction among brainwaves, the computer software’s quantified analysis of the electroencephalogram data, the perception of the visual and auditory stimuli of NFB by the individual, and the normative data- base is inherently consistent with the reciprocally determinant brain-body model. This model may provide a framework for counselors to integrate NFB into their practice and easily apply research guided by the same model. Applying IS euro feedback If inclined, counselors may integrate NFB into their evidence- based prac- tice. Extensive literature showing the effectiveness of NFB for a wide range of conditions dates back to the 1960s (Hammond, 2011). NFB has
  • 28. been success- fully integrated into treatment for individuals who have been exposed to long- term traumatic experiences during early development (Fisher, 2014). Recent studies have shown long-term benefits for individuals with attention-deficit/ hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) after treatment and at 6-month follow-up (Van Doren et ah, 2018) and with intractable epilepsy 10 years following treatment (Strehl, Birkle, Worz, & Kotchoubey, 2014). Recent meta- analyses and reviews have described NFB’s efficaciousness for PTSD (Panisch & Hai, 2018) and for criminal behavior associated with ADHD, schizophrenia, substance use, and psychopathy (Fielenbach, Donkers, Spreen, Visser, & Bogaerts, 2018). In addition, NFB can enhance performance for athletes (Xiang et ah, 2018) and visual artists (Shourie, Firoozabadi, & Badie, 2018) and can improve cognitive functioning (Reiner, Gruzelier, Bamidis, & Auer, 2018) and creativity (Agnoli, Zanon, Mastria, Avenanti, & Corazza, 2018) among healthy adults. Using the reciprocally determinant brain-body model, NFB can be con- ceptualized as a microcosm of the reciprocal flow of information that occurs in clients’ environments. Brainwaves, or cortical electrical activity, are measured with quantitative EEG (Niedermeyer & Da Silva, 2004), which influences the
  • 29. visual or auditory stimuli, functioning as the environment, presented to the client. The client then perceives the stimuli and allows the self system, includ- 252 0 Journal o f Mental Health Counseling F o s t e r in g I n t r i n s i c R e s ilie n c e ing beliefs and brain activity, to adapt to the ongoing environment. This cycle occurs in real time and is ongoing throughout an NFB session (Hammond, 2011). CONSIDERATIONS FOR COUNSELORS Adopting NFB into clinical practice requires careful consideration of a variety of questions, as Chapin (2016) comprehensively described. Counselors practicing in settings that focus on acute or short-term solution- focused interventions would not be ideal candidates for NFB. Counselors working in settings that provide longer-term therapy (20+ sessions) would be excellent candidates to integrate NFB into their practice. NFB therapy requires multiple and continuous sessions to activate sustained change in the brain. Counselors must consider ethical and legal issues applicable to NFB.
  • 30. Counselors are obligated to “practice only within the boundaries of their com- petence” (American Counseling Association [ACA], 2014, p. 8). In keeping with the ACA code of ethics, we recommend that interested counselors receive comprehensive training such as that designed by certification agencies like the Biofeedback Certification International Alliance or the International Society for Neurofeedback and Research. Certification involves didactic coursework, hours of hands-on practice, and supervision by a certified clinician. No laws currently require practitioners to be certified in NFB to provide the service; however, ACA (2014) ethical standards ask counselors to practice “in specialty areas new to them only after appropriate education, training, and supervised experience” (p. 8). The American Mental Health Counselors Association (AMHCA, 2015) code of ethics states that counselors in practice should maintain confidential- ity. Adequate NFB training involves case consultation after the basic training. Given that few experts in the field provide such consultation, this service is often done via videoconferencing. Therefore, counselors must take precautions to ensure they are in line with the AMHCA (2015) code of ethics when dis- cussing their clients’ NFB data electronically. Clients should also be informed
  • 31. during the consent process if their personal identifying information will be shared for case consultation purposes. Mental health counselors must take precautions to protect, store, transfer, and dispose of client records including NFB session and assessment data in a confidential manner consistent with applicable laws and regulations (AMHCA, 2015). During the informed consent process, mental health counselors must provide information to clients that describes any potential risks that could occur with NFB. NFB is a Class II device approved by the US Food and Drug Administration for relaxation. However, clients should be informed that 0 Journal of Mental Health Counseling 253 relaxation does not always mean feeling better immediately. For instance, NFB could increase self-awareness of anxiety the client had previously denied. O ther possible side effects of NFB include headaches, changes in sleep patterns, or irritability, but these are usually temporary and reversible (Hammond, 2011). T he importance of informing clients of these risks is reinforced by the ACA (2014), which states “counselors explicitly explain to clients the nature of all sendees provided ... such as ... techniques, procedures,
  • 32. limitations, potential risks, and benefits of services” (p. 4). This is especially important given the lack of regulations on NFB and the am ount of information that is still unknown about brain activity. Counselors must also consider the age and typical symptom presentation of the clients with whom they work. Although NFB has shown promising find- ings with children with ADHD, few clinical studies have included children younger than 7 in their sample (Van Doren et al., 2018). This is due in large part to rapid cortical maturation in gray matter volume during the critical early years of child development (Sowell, Thompson, & Toga, 2004), and NFB may not be an effective treatment during this time period. Given this, counselors who work primarily with small children may choose to forgo incorporating NFB into their practice. Additionally, although NFB may be an effective treat- m ent for some individuals presenting psychotic symptoms (McCarthy-Jones, 2012), counselors must be careful to assess the severity of the symptoms and the individual’s capacity to understand the treatment so as not to become triggered by the treatment procedure itself. Severely acute individuals suffering from active phase psychosis would not be appropriate candidates for NFB treatment. Counselors must also be mindful of a client’s mental health
  • 33. history so as not to trigger a psychotic episode in an individual prone to psychosis. Another consideration for counselors contemplating NFB is cost (Chapin, 2016). NFB devices can cost anywhere from $3,000 to $50,000 or more, depending on the type of technology used. Counselors would also need NFB software and a subscription to training packages in addition to supplies such as sensors, ear clips, alcohol swabs, and syringes. In an effort to make NFB devices more accessible for practitioners, many NFB device distributors offer monthly rental or financing plans. Despite these options, the cost can be a barrier of entry for many practicing counselors. DISCUSSION Counselors have the capacity to utilize theory and research from other fields to affect positive change. The clinical application of counselor training with a focus on wellness opens the door for the counseling profession to inte- grate theory and knowledge from the fields of cognitive psychology, neurology, 254 0 Journal o f Mental Health Counseling F o s te rin g In trin s ic R es ilie n ce
  • 34. and psychiatry. Our model draws from these professions and provides a deeper understanding of the part mental health plays in overall functioning and well-being. This new conceptualization offers a map to guide counselors to aim fora variety of possible treatment outcomes. These potential outcomes include improved sleep patterns; adaptive interpersonal relationships; increased emo- tional regulation, resilience, self-control, attention, and executive functioning; and decreased digestive problems, substance dependence, anxiety, depression, and delinquent or aggressive behaviors. Moreover, the model highlights the need to incorporate more mind-body theories and interventions in counseling. Mind-body interventions can have positive effects in the domains of physio- logical response, physical health, social and emotional health, mental health, and behavior (Jayawardene, Bebe, Lorhmann, & Torrabi, 2017). With the raised awareness of the impact of trauma (van der Kolk, 2015) and the need for evidence-based mind-body interventions, counselors have the opportunity to advocate for trauma-exposed individuals on an individual and societal level. Integrative therapies, such as NFB, when used from a counseling perspective, offer an accessible mind-body modality that works within a whole-person model of conceptualizing exposure to childhood trauma and ways to treat it
  • 35. (Fisher, 2014). Flowever, ethical issues, such as competence (ACA, 2014), must be considered before incorporating new specialty areas like NFB into counseling practice. 0 Journal of Mental Health Counseling 255 REFERENCES Adams, A. S., & Petersen, C. (2016). Precision medicine: Opportunities, possibilities, and challenges for patients and providers. Journal o f the American Informatics Association, 23, 787-790. https://doi.org/10.1093/jamia/ocv215 Agnoli, S., Zanon, M., Mastria, S., Avenanti, A., & Corazza, G. E. (2018). Enhancing creative cognition with a rapid right-parietal neurofeedback procedure. Neuropsychologia, 118, 99-106. https://doi.Org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2018.02.015 Allbaugh, I,. ]., Mack, S. A., Culmone, 11. D., Hosey, A. M., Dunn, S. E., & Kaslow, N. J. (2018). Relational factors critical in the link between childhood emotional abuse and suicidal ideation. Psychological Services, 15, 298-304. https://doi.org/10.1037/ser0000214 American Counseling Association. (2014). AG'A code of ethics: As approved by the ACA Governing Council, 2014. Alexandria, VA: Author. American Mental Health Counselors Association. (2015).
  • 36. AMHCA Code of Ethics: Revised October 2015. Retrieved from http://connections.amhca.org/HigherLogic/System/ DownloadDocumentFile.ashx?DocumentFileKey=d4el0fcb-2Bc- c701-aald-5d0f53b8bcl4 Angelakis, E., Hatzis, A., Panourias, I. G., & Sakas, D. E. (2007). Brain-computer interface: A reciprocal self-regulated neuromodulation. In D. E. Sakas, & B. A. Simpson, Operative neuromodulation (pp. 555-559). Vienna, Austria: Springer. Bandura, A. (1978). The self system in reciprocal determinism. American Psychologist, 33, 344-358. https://doi.Org/10.1037/0003-066X.33.4.344 Beauchaine, T. P., Gatzke-Kopp, L., & Mead, H. K. (2007). Polyvagal theory and developmental psychopathology: Emotion dysregulation and conduct problems from preschool to adolescence. Biological Psychology, 74, 174-184. https://doi.Org/10.1016/j.biopsycho.2005.08.008 Bowlby, J. (1980). Attachment and loss: Loss, sadness and depression (Vol. 3). New York, NY: Basic Books. Centers for Disease Control and Prevention. (2018). About the CDC-Kaiser ACE study. Retrieved from https://www.cdc.gov/violenceprevention/acestudy/about.html Chapin, T. J. (2016). Developing a specialty in neurofeedback: Decision points, journal o f Mental Health Counseling, 38, 155-169. https://doi.Org/10.17744/mehc.38.2.06
  • 37. Clarke, A. R., Barr)', R. J.. McCarthy, R., & Selikowitz, M. (1998). EEC analysis in attention- deficit/hyperactivity disorder: A comparative study of two subtypes. Psychiatry Research, 81, 19-29. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0165-1781(98)00072-9 Cole, M. (1978). Alexander Romanovich Luria: 1902-1977. American Journal o f Psychology, 91, 349-352. Retrieved from https://www.press.uillinois.edu/journals/ajp.html Collura, T. F. (2017). Quantitative EEC and live Z-score neurofeedback—current clinical and scientific context. Biofeedback, 45, 25-29. https://doi.org/10.5298/1081-5937-45.L07 Cryan, J. F., & Dinan, T. G. (2012). Mind-altering microorganisms: The impact of the gut microbiota on brain and behavior. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 13, 701-712. https://doi.org/10.1038/nrn3346 Decety, ]., & Jackson, P. L. (2004). The functional architecture of human empathy. Behavioral and Cognitive Neuroscience Reviews, 3, 71-100. https://doi.org/10.1177/1534582304267187 Ecker, B., & Toonrey, B. (2008). Depotentiation of symptom- producing implicit memory in coherence therapy, journal of Constructivist Psychology, 21, 87-150. https://doi.org/10.1080/10720530701853685 Engel, G. L. (1977). The need for a new medical model: A challenge for biomedicine. Science,
  • 38. 196, 129-136. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.847460 Felitti, V. J., Anda, R. F., Nordernberg, D., Williamson, D. F., Spiltz, A. M., Edwards, V., ... Marks, J. S. (1998). Relationship of childhood abuse and household dysfunction to many of the causes of death in adults. American Journal o f Preventive Medicine, 14, 245-258. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0749-3797(98)00017-8 256 0 Jo u rn a l o f M e n ta l H e a lth C o u n s e lin g https://doi.org/10.1093/jamia/ocv215 https://doi.Org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2018.02.015 https://doi.org/10.1037/ser0000214 http://connections.amhca.org/HigherLogic/System/ https://doi.Org/10.1037/0003-066X.33.4.344 https://doi.Org/10.1016/j.biopsycho.2005.08.008 https://www.cdc.gov/violenceprevention/acestudy/about.html https://doi.Org/10.17744/mehc.38.2.06 https://doi.org/10.1016/S0165-1781(98)00072-9 https://www.press.uillinois.edu/journals/ajp.html https://doi.org/10.5298/1081-5937-45.L07 https://doi.org/10.1038/nrn3346 https://doi.org/10.1177/1534582304267187 https://doi.org/10.1080/10720530701853685 https://doi.org/10.1126/science.847460 https://doi.org/10.1016/S0749-3797(98)00017-8 F o s t e r in g I n t r i n s i c R e s ilie n c e Fielenbach, S., Donkers, F. C. L., Spreen, M., Visser, H. A., & Bogaerts, S. (2018). Neurofeedback training for psychiatric disorders associated with criminal offending: A review.
  • 39. Frontiers in Psychiatry, 8, 1-12. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyt.2017.00313 Fisher, S. F. (2014). Neurofeedback in the treatment o f developmental trauma: Calming the fear driven brain. New York, NY: Norton. Giaconia, R. M., Reinherz, H. Z., Silverman, A. B., Pakiz, B., Frost, A. K., & Cohen, E. (1995). Traumas and posttraumatic stress disorder in a community population of older adolescents. journal o f the American Academy o f Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 34, 1369-1380. https://doi.org/10.1097/00004583-199510000-00023 Gonqalves, O. F., & Perrone-McGovern, K. M. (2014). A neuroscience agenda for counseling psychology research, Journal o f Counseling Psychology, 61, 507-512. https://doi.org/10.1037/cou0000026 Grawe, K. (1997). Research-informed psychotherapy. Psychotherapy Research, 7, 1-19. https://doi.org/10.1080/10503309712331331843 Ham lin, E. (2011). Lecture on the electroencephalogram (EEC): Source and measurement. Personal collection of E. Hamlin, Institute of Applied Neuroscience, Asheville, NC. H am m ond, C. (2011). W hat is neurofeedback? An update, journal ofNeurotherapy, 15, 305-336 https://d0i.0rg/l 0.1080/10874208.2011.623090 I layes, S. C., Luoma, J. 13., Bond, F. W., Masuda, A., & Lillis, J. (2006). Acceptance and
  • 40. com m itm ent therapy: Model, processes and outcomes. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 44, 1-25. https://doi.0rg/l 0.1016/j.brat.2005.06.006 Ivey, A., & Zalaquett, C. P. (2011). Neuroscience and counseling: Central issues for social justice leaders. Journal for Social Action in Counseling and Psychology, 3, 201-115. Retrieved from http://web.b.ebscohost.com.ezproxy.memphis.edu/ehost/pdfview er/ pdfviewer?vid=3&sid=03d3d312-b8cb-4a3f-a869- d49dfeffb0d7%40pdc-v-sessmgr03 Jayawardene, W., Bebe, R., Lorhmann, D., & Torrabi, M. (2017). Use of treatm ent and counseling services and m ind-body techniques by students with emotional and behavioral difficulties. Journal o f School Health, 87, 133-141. https://doi.Org/10.l 111/josh. 12475 Kabat-Zinn, ]. (1982). An outpatient program in behavioral medicine for chronic pain patients based on the practice of mindfulness meditation: Theoretical considerations and preliminary results. General Hospital Psychiatry, 4, 33-47. https://doi.org/10.1016/0163-8343(82)90026-3 Levine, P. (1997). Waking the tiger: Health trauma. Berkeley, CA: North Atlantic Books. Linehan, M. M. (1993). Cognitive behavioral therapy oj borderline personality disorder (Vol. 51). New York, NY: Guilford Press. MeCarlhy-Jones, S. (2012). Taking back the brain: C ould neurofeedback training be effective
  • 41. for relieving distressing auditory verbal hallucinations in patients with schizophrenia? Schizophrenia Bulletin, 38, 678-682. https://doi.org/10.1093/schbul/sbs006 National Institute of Mental Health. (2015). N IM H strategic plan for research. Retrieved from https://www.nimh.nih.gov/about/strategic- planningreports/nimh_ strategicplanforresearch_508compliant_corrected_final_149979. pdf Navalta, C. P., M cGee., L., & Underwood, j. (2018). Adverse childhood experiences, brain development, and m ental health: A call for neurocounseling. Journal o f M ental Health Counseling, 40, 266-278. https://doi.Org/10.17744/mehc.40.3.07 Niedermeyer, E., & da Silva, F. L. (2004). Electroencephalography: Basic principles, clinical applications, and related fields. Philadelphia, PA: Lippincott Williams & Wilkins. Panisch, L. S., & Hai, A. H. (2018). T he effectiveness of using neurofeedback in the treatment of post-traumatic stress disorder: A systematic review. Trauma, Violence, and Abuse, 1-10. https://doi.org/10.1177/1524838018781103 Payne, 11. (2003). Dance movement therapy: Theory and practice. New York, NY: Routledge. Porges, S. W. (2009). T he polyvagal theory: New insights into adaptive reactions of the automatic
  • 42. nervous system. Cleveland Clinic Journal o f Medicine, 76, S86- S90. https://doi.org/10.3949/ccjm.76.s2.17 Porges, S. W. (2011). The polyvagal theory: Neurophysiological foundations of emotions, attachment, communication, and self-regulation. New York, NY: Norton. $ Journal o f Mental H ealth Counseling 257 https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyt.2017.00313 https://doi.org/10.1097/00004583-199510000-00023 https://doi.org/10.1037/cou0000026 https://doi.org/10.1080/10503309712331331843 https://d0i.0rg/l https://doi.0rg/l http://web.b.ebscohost.com.ezproxy.memphis.edu/ehost/pdfview er/ https://doi.Org/10.l https://doi.org/10.1016/0163-8343(82)90026-3 https://doi.org/10.1093/schbul/sbs006 https://www.nimh.nih.gov/about/strategic- planningreports/nimh_ https://doi.Org/10.17744/mehc.40.3.07 https://doi.org/10.1177/1524838018781103 https://doi.org/10.3949/ccjm.76.s2.17 Rangaswamy, M., Porjesz, B., Chorlian, D. B., Wang, K., Jones, K. A., Bauer, L. 0 . , . . . Begleiter, H. (2002). Beta power in the EEG of alcoholics. Biological Psychiatry, 52, 851-842. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0006-3223(02)01362-8 Reiner, M., Gruzelier, J., Bamidis, P. D., & Auer, T. (2018).
  • 43. The science of neurofeedback: Learnability and effects. Neuroscience, 378, 1-10. https://doi.Org/10.1016/j.neuroscience.2018.04.024 Rhee, S. H., Pothoulakis, C., & Mayer, E. A. (2009). Principles and clinical implications of the brain-gut-enteric microbiota axis. Nature Reviews: Gastroenterology and Hepatology, 6, 306— 314. https://doi.org/10.1038/nrgastro.2009.35 Rockett, J. L., Murrie, D. C., & Boccaccini, M. T. (2007). Diagnostic labeling in juvenile justice settings: Do psychopathology and conduct disorder findings influence clinicians? Psychological Services, 4, 107-122. https://doi.Org/10.1037/1541- 1559.4.2.107 Rogers, C. (1951). Client-centered therapy: Its current practice, implications, and theory. Oxford, United Kingdom: Houghton Mifflin. Russell-Chapin, L. A. (2016). Integrating neurocounseling into the counseling profession: An introduction. Journal of Mental Health Counseling, 38, 93-102. https://doi.Org/10.17744/mehc.38.2.01 Schilling, E. A., Aseltine, R. H., & Gore, S. (2007). Adverse childhood experiences and mental health in young adults: A longitudinal survey. BMC Public Health, 7, 30-40. https://doi.org/10.1186/1471-2458-7-30 Schwartz, E., & Perry, B. (1994). The post-traumatic response in children and adolescents. Psychiatric Clinics o f North America, 17, 311- 326.
  • 44. https://doi.org/10.1016/SO193-95 3X( 18)30117-5 Shapiro, F. (2001). Eye movement desensitization and reprocessing (EMDR): Basic principles, protocols, and procedures. New York, NY: Guilford Press. Shevlin, M., McElroy, E., & Murphy, J. (2015). Loneliness mediates the relationship between childhood trauma and adult psychopathology: Evidence from the adult psychiatric morbidity survey. Social Psychiatiy and Psychiatric Epidemiology, 59, 591-601. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00127-014-0951-8 Shourie, N., Firoozabadi, M., & Badie, K. (2018). Neurofeedback training protocols based on spectral EEG feature subset and channel selection for performance enhancement of novice visual artists. Biomedical Signal Processing and Control, 43, 117-129. https://doi.Org/10.1016/j.bspc.2018.02.017 Siegel, D. J. (2012). The developing mind: How relationships and the brain interact to shape who we are (2"" ed.). New York, NY: Guilford Press. Simon-Dack, S. L., & Marmarosh, C. L. (2014). Neurosciences and adult health behaviors: Recent findings and implications for counseling psychology. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 61, 528-533. https://doi.org/10.1037/cou0000020 Sowell, E. R., Thompson, P. M., & Toga, A. W. (2004). Mapping changes in the human cortex throughout the span of life. The Neuroscientist, 10, 372-392. https://doi.org/10.1177/1073858404263960
  • 45. Strehl, U., Birkle, S. M., Worz, S., & Kotchoubey, B. (2014). Sustained reduction of seizures in patients with intractable epilepsy after self-regulation training of slow cortical potentials 10 years after. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 8, 1-7. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2014.00604 Swingle, P. G. (2008). Biofeedback for the brain: How neurotherapy effectively treats depression, ADHD, autism, and more. Piscataway, NJ: Rutgers University Press. Thibault, R. T., & Raz, A. (2017). The psychology of neurofeedback: Clinical intervention even if applied placebo. American Psychologist, 72, 679-688. https://doi.org/10.1037/amp0000118 Thompson, E. J., Richards, M., & Gaysina, D. (2017). Cumulative effect of adverse childhood experiences on affective symptom trajectories in adulthood: Evidence from a British birth cohort. Journal of Epidemiology and Community Health, 71 (Suppl. 1), A60. https://doi.org/10,1136/jech-2017-SS.YlAbstracls. 122 Tronson, N. C., & Taylor, J. R. (2007). Molecular mechanisms of memory reconsolidation, Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 8, 262-275. https://doi.org/10.1038/nrn2090 258 0 Jo u rn a l o f M e n ta l H e a lth C o u n se lin g https://doi.org/10.1016/S0006-3223(02)01362-8 https://doi.Org/10.1016/j.neuroscience.2018.04.024 https://doi.org/10.1038/nrgastro.2009.35
  • 46. https://doi.Org/10.1037/1541-1559.4.2.107 https://doi.Org/10.17744/mehc.38.2.01 https://doi.org/10.1186/1471-2458-7-30 https://doi.org/10.1016/SO193-95 https://doi.org/10.1007/s00127-014-0951-8 https://doi.Org/10.1016/j.bspc.2018.02.017 https://doi.org/10.1037/cou0000020 https://doi.org/10.1177/1073858404263960 https://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2014.00604 https://doi.org/10.1037/amp0000118 https://doi.org/10,1136/jech-2017-SS.YlAbstracls https://doi.org/10.1038/nrn2090 F o s t e r i n g I n t r i n s i c R e s i l i e n c e van der Kolk, B. A. (1994). T he body keeps the score: Memory and the evolving psychobiology of posttraunratic stress. Harvard Review o f Psychiatry, 1, 253- 265. https://d0 i.0 rg/l 0.3109/10673229409017088 van der Kolk, B. (2005). Developmental trauma disorder. Psychiatric Annals, 35, 401-408. https://doi.org/10.3928/00485713-20050501-06 van der Kolk, B. (2015). The body keeps the score: Brain, mind, and body in the healing o f trauma. New York, NY: Penguin Group. Van Doren, J., Arns, M., Heinrich, H., Vollebregt, M. A., Strehl, U., & Loo, S. K. (2018). Sustained effects of neurofeedback in ADHD: A systematic review and meta-analysis. European C hild and Adolescent Psychiatry, 28(3), 1-13. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00787-018-1121-4
  • 47. W11, C., & Morris, J. R. (2001). Genes, genetics, and epigenetics: A correspondence. Science, 10, 1 103-1105. https://doi.Org/10.1126/science.296.5532.l 103 Xiang, M., Hou, X., Liao, B., Liao, J., & Hu, M. (2018). T he effect of neurofeedback training for sport performance in athletes: A meta-analysis. Psychology o f Sport and Exercise, 36, 114-122. https://doi.Org/10.1016/j.psychsport.2018.02.004 Zettler, H. R., Wolff, K., Baglivio, M., Craig, J. M., & Epps, N. (2017). T he racial and gender differences in the impact of adverse childhood experiences on juvenile residential placement. Youth Violence and Juvenile Justice, 16, 319-337. https://doi.org/10.! 177/1541204017698213 ^ Journal o f Mental Health Counseling 259 https://d0i.0rg/l https://doi.org/10.3928/00485713-20050501-06 https://doi.org/10.1007/s00787-018-1121-4 https://doi.Org/10.1126/science.296.5532.l https://doi.Org/10.1016/j.psychsport.2018.02.004 https://doi.org/10 Copyright of Journal of Mental Health Counseling is the property of American Mental Health Counselors Association and its content may not be copied or emailed to multiple sites or posted to a listserv without the copyright holder's express written permission. However, users may print, download, or email articles for individual use.
  • 48. Full Terms & Conditions of access and use can be found at https://www.tandfonline.com/action/journalInformation?journal Code=vjer20 The Journal of Educational Research ISSN: 0022-0671 (Print) 1940-0675 (Online) Journal homepage: https://www.tandfonline.com/loi/vjer20 The association among literacy proficiency, effortful control, and frustration Su-Ying Huang & Li-Li Yeh To cite this article: Su-Ying Huang & Li-Li Yeh (2019) The association among literacy proficiency, effortful control, and frustration, The Journal of Educational Research, 112:2, 168-178, DOI: 10.1080/00220671.2018.1461599 To link to this article: https://doi.org/10.1080/00220671.2018.1461599 Published online: 17 May 2018. Submit your article to this journal Article views: 193 View related articles View Crossmark data
  • 49. https://www.tandfonline.com/action/journalInformation?journal Code=vjer20 https://www.tandfonline.com/loi/vjer20 https://www.tandfonline.com/action/showCitFormats?doi=10.10 80/00220671.2018.1461599 https://doi.org/10.1080/00220671.2018.1461599 https://www.tandfonline.com/action/authorSubmission?journalC ode=vjer20&show=instructions https://www.tandfonline.com/action/authorSubmission?journalC ode=vjer20&show=instructions https://www.tandfonline.com/doi/mlt/10.1080/00220671.2018.1 461599 https://www.tandfonline.com/doi/mlt/10.1080/00220671.2018.1 461599 http://crossmark.crossref.org/dialog/?doi=10.1080/00220671.20 18.1461599&domain=pdf&date_stamp=2018-05-17 http://crossmark.crossref.org/dialog/?doi=10.1080/00220671.20 18.1461599&domain=pdf&date_stamp=2018-05-17 The association among literacy proficiency, effortful control, and frustration Su-Ying Huanga and Li-Li Yehb aDepartment of Clinical Psychology, Fu Jen Catholic University, New Taipei City, Taiwan; bDepartment of Audiology and Speech-Language Pathology, Mackay Medical College, New Taipei City, Taiwan ARTICLE HISTORY Received 12 December 2017 Revised 28 March 2018 Accepted 31 March 2018 ABSTRACT
  • 50. The authors investigated the relationships among effortful control and frustration and literacy proficiency of preadolescence to determine which subcomponent of effortful control and/or emotion might be critical in achieving academic success. The participants included 72 children recruited from a larger longitudinal study. Children’s frustration, effortful control, and literacy skills were assessed at Grade 5, and their nonverbal IQ levels were assessed at Grade 1. Effortful control and frustration were measured by a self- report questionnaire. Literacy proficiency was measured with tests of reading comprehension, written language, and the semestral grades in the subject of Chinese literacy. Results showed that children with higher effortful control perception had better literacy abilities, particularly writing and language academic achievement. The dispositional frustration moderated the predictive effect of attention control on children’s reading comprehension. The findings support the importance of self-regulation and emotions on literacy proficiency. KEYWORDS Effortful control; frustration; literacy proficiency; self- regulation; temperament Preadolescence is a transitional stage between childhood and adolescence, during which self-regulation skills—often associ- ated with children’s ability to adjust—continue to develop (King, Lengua, & Monahan, 2013). Preadolescents may seek recognition from their families and among their peers by achieving academic success, which is based on good literacy skills. Literacy is a new mode of communication that children need to learn as they begin formal schooling. Reading and writ- ing are the receptive and expressive aspects of literacy, respec-
  • 51. tively, and serve as the foundation of one’s learning of other subjects. A previous study has shown that children with better effortful control are more likely to perform better academically, such as in the area of literacy acquisition (Blair & Razza, 2007). In addition, emotions are functionally important in ensuring good academic performance (Pekrun, 2009). However, the key effects of effortful control and negative moods on the literacy performance of preadolescents have yet to be systematically investigated. Effortful control is a dispositional, higher-order self-regula- tion of one’s temperament. It refers to one’s ability to perform effectively in a conflict situation, which requires one to demon- strate attention control, inhibit a dominant response, or acti- vate a goal-directed response (Posner & Rothbart, 1998; Rothbart, Derryberry, & Posner, 1994). Effortful control sup- ports individual attainment of competence-related goals (Rothbart & Hwang, 2005) and is related to school adaptation (Blair, 2002). For adolescents, effortful control is constantly developing and becoming more crucial, particularly for those who display more intense negative emotions when it comes to facing challenging situations (Oldehinkel, Hartman, Ferdinand, Verhulst, & Ormel, 2007). Effortful control emerges from the end stage of infancy, markedly increases during preschool, and continues its devel- opment into adolescence (Rueda, Posner, & Rothbart, 2011). It is a broad construct consisting of specific components, namely, attention control, inhibitory control, and activation control after middle childhood (i.e., around 8 years old; Putnam, Ellis, & Rothbart, 2001). Effortful control not only is influenced by biological factors, but also can be shaped by environmental fac- tors, such as family context and parental practices (Rothbart & Bates, 2006). Given that literacy acquisition requires complex processing (e.g., encoding and decoding signals, comprehend- ing and composing passages) and also emerges from the pre-
  • 52. school period, there is a growing interest in the regulation processes involved in children’s literacy acquisition (e.g., Blair & Razza, 2007; McClelland et al., 2007). Emotions are crucial contributors to students’ motivation, interpersonal resources, memory, and learning (Valiente, Swanson, & Eisenberg, 2012). Specific emotions relate to child- ren’s development for different reasons, thus requiring an assessment of specific types of emotions, instead of using broad composites, such as emotionality (Rothbart & Bate, 2006; Valiente et al., 2012). Anger or frustration, as negative emo- tions, involves a tendency to experience an unpleasant affect, which can be led by goal blocking. The kind of emotion may increase when children enter school because this is where they face more academic expectations from themselves or others (Eisenberg et al., 2005). Thus far, the direct association between negative emotions and academic achievement has not been fully examined (Valiente et al., 2012), particularly for the critical period of liter- acy and effortful control acquisition (i.e., at the preadolescence CONTACT Dr. Li-Li Yeh [email protected] Department of Audiology and Speech-Language Pathology, Mackay Medical College, No.46, Sec. 3, Zhongzheng Rd., Sanzhi Dist., New Taipei City, Taiwan. © 2018 Taylor & Francis Group, LLC https://doi.org/10.1080/00220671.2018.1461599 THE JOURNAL OF EDUCATIONAL RESEARCH 2019, VOL. 112, NO. 2, 168–178 https://crossmark.crossref.org/dialog/?doi=10.1080/00220671.2 018.1461599&domain=pdf&date_stamp=2018-05-14
  • 53. mailto:[email protected] https://doi.org/10.1080/00220671.2018.1461599 http://www.tandfonline.com stage). In addition, given that the direct associations between specific measures of negative emotion and achievement are at a modest level (Valient et al., 2012), the potential indirect effects of emotion must also be investigated (Blair, 2002). Role of effortful control on literacy acquisition Effortful control is significantly associated with preschoolers’ prerequisite literacy skills, such as phonemic awareness and let- ter knowledge, and must be strengthened for children from low socioeconomic status families to avoid possible school failures (Blair & Razza, 2007). For 7- to 12-year-old children, the global effortful control score as reported by parents, is positively related to children’s grade point average in language, vocabu- lary, and mathematics (Valiente, Lemery-Chalfant, Swanson, & Reiser, 2008). Furthermore, global effortful control at Grade 1 significantly predicted academic performance in mathematics and Chinese four years later among Chinese children (Zhou, Main, & Wang, 2010). McClelland et al. (2007) indicated that the specific components of effortful control (e.g., attention con- trol, inhibitory control) are positively related to letter-word identification and the vocabulary ability of preschool children. Attention control, including attention focusing and shifting, is a more cognitively oriented component of effortful control. Past studies have reported the direct link between academic competence and attention control, which serves as a key factor in promoting academic achievement and learning (Liew, 2012; Valiente et al., 2008). Children who have difficulty directing their attention are more likely to experience significant chal-
  • 54. lenges when trying to learn educational materials (Valiente et al., 2008). Empirical data have shown that children who have difficulty focusing their attention often have poor reading and language skills (McGee, Partridge, Williams, & Silva, 1991; Tamis-LeMonda & Bornstein, 1989). Attention control can also regulate emotions and limit the impact of (anxiety) emo- tion on information processing (Derrybery & Reed, 2002). Inhibitory control and activation control are two behavioral regulation components of effortful control. First, inhibitory control is the ability to appropriately inhibit a dominant response or desirable behavior (Putnam & Rothbart, 2006). Associated with emotional adaptation, inhibitory control rap- idly develops with marked improvement during childhood (Lengua, 2003; Williams, Ponesse, Schachar, Logan, & Tannock, 1999). However, its contribution on academic success are inconsistent. For example, Blair and Razza (2007) indicated that the inhibitory control by direct measurement is positively associated with the letter knowledge of preschoolers. Neverthe- less, the inhibitory control of parents’ rating does not have a significant association with children’s literacy abilities. The inconsistency among past studies may be attributed to different test environments or the design of tasks conducted among vari- ous studies. Meanwhile, activation control is the ability to plan new actions; it also refers to the ability to perform the appropriate action when there is a tendency to give up, when people are experiencing challenges or failures, or when individuals are facing detestable learning tasks (Posner & Rothbart, 1998; Rothbart et al., 1994). In stressful situations, such as when people are facing challenges or failures, they may also need to force themselves to take action to ameliorate the situation. Activation control is the key that enables an individual to per- form unpleasant activities, which can be helpful in achieving
  • 55. one’s long-term goals (Ellis, 2002). Literacy acquisition requires complicated processing. For example, when writing an assignment, children need to actively engage and initiate behaviors to work on tasks that they may otherwise want to avoid. However, to our knowledge, no study has yet to directly examine the role of activation control on the literacy abilities of school-age children. In summary, although evidence has proven that effortful control has a direct link with children’s literacy in early child- hood, few studies have focused on school-age children, particu- larly preadolescents. Aside from analyzing effortful control in general, studying both the direct and indirect predictive effects by examining various components of effortful control can also improve our understanding of whether any specific component of effortful control is uniquely predictive of academic achieve- ment (Valiente et al., 2008), and whether specific components are associated with literacy performance. Association between frustration and literacy acquisition Different negative moods may have varying associations with academic performance (Schwarz, 2001), and an increasing number of studies have focused on the association between anxiety-related emotions and academic performance (e.g., Duchesne, Vitaro, Larose, & Tremblay, 2008; Zeidner, 2007), particularly in how different negative moods may have varying predictive effects on academic performance (Schwarz, Chang, & Farver, 2001). Clarifying the relationship between emotions and achievement by specific types of emotions—instead of using broad composites, such as negative emotionality—is helpful in understanding the role of emotions in developing academic competence (Valiente et al., 2012). Anger or frustration, as negative moods, may reduce achievement, because they disrupt cognitive resources, motiva-
  • 56. tion, and the use of flexible strategies (Blair, 2002; Rice, Levine, & Pizarro, 2007). According to previous studies, children’s frus- tration at school is correlated negatively with their academic self-efficacy, self-regulation of learning, and academic perfor- mance (Boekaerts, 1993; Pekrun, Goetz, Per, Kramer, & Hochstadt, 2004). Zhou et al. (2010) found that, based on teachers’ reports, Chinese children who had higher levels of dis- positional anger or frustration from 6 to 9 years old also had lower levels of academic performance (i.e., grade point aver- age). Furthermore, Blair (2002) proposed that emotions have an important contribution on the self-regulation of children, as they have not fully developed their cognitive and higher-order regulative abilities. Rothbart and Hwang (2005) also claimed that the efficiency of effortful control depends on the strength of the dominant response, such as emotional reactivity. Effortful control demand and frustration reaction during reading and writing Reading is an activity that is significantly correlated with both decoding skills and language comprehension. The correlations between word recognition and reading tend to decline with THE JOURNAL OF EDUCATIONAL RESEARCH 169 grade level, and the correlations between comprehension and reading tend to increase with grade level (Gough, Hoover, & Peterson, 1996). If frustration is more likely to arise when ongoing tasks are interrupted or when goal blocking occurs (e.g., Zhou et al., 2010), factors associated with reading fluency,
  • 57. such as word recognition skills or higher-level language com- prehension skills, may relate to experience of frustration. In other words, before school-aged children develop mature read- ing skills, particularly for early adolescents, frustration may eas- ily occur, especially when their goal for reading is mismatched to their academic assignments and texts (Graesser & D’Mello, 2012). Writing consists of several processes, such as text construc- tion, handwriting, spelling, executive function, and working memory for accessing or selecting appropriate vocabulary (Owens, 2011). Therefore, compared with reading, writing as actively generating text may demand more self-regulatory skills to select and sustain attention, organize ideas, and control social and affective behavior (Ylvisaker & DeBonis, 2000). Unless it is under time pressure, writing itself emphasizes less on nonstop or fluent processing than reading. For early adoles- cents, compared with early writing skills, such as spelling or orthography knowledge, text generation is more critical to their writing performance (Owens, 2011). Aims and research questions for this study The aim of this study was to investigate whether children’s own perceptions of their effortful control and frustration had a rela- tive contribution to their literacy proficiency after their gender, age, and nonverbal IQ were controlled. The research questions are as follows: Research Question 1: Does global effortful control predict the concurrent literacy proficiency? Is there any signifi- cant prediction effect between global effortful control and various indicators of literacy proficiency, such as reading comprehension, written expression, and semes- tral literacy test grade, while controlling for gender,
  • 58. nonverbal IQ, and age? Research Question 2: How do the specific components of effortful control relate to different indicators of literacy proficiency? Research Question 3: Does children’s dispositional frustra- tion have not only a direct predictive effect on children’s literacy proficiency, but also an indirect predictive effect on the influence of effortful control toward literacy proficiency? Methods Participants Seventy-two Mandarin-speaking children (33 boys and 39 girls) in Taiwan, who were earlier recruited for a larger longitudinal study, were involved in the current longitudinal first- and sec- ond wave studies. At Wave 1, the children were in Grade 1; Wave 2 occurred four years later when the children were in Grade 5, with an average age of 10.77 years (SD D 0.27 years, age range D 10.17–11.17 years). Measures Nonverbal IQ The children’s nonverbal IQ scores at Grade 1 were assessed using Tonic Test of Nonverbal Intelligence (TONI-3; Brown, Sherhenou, & Johnsen, 1996). Effortful control The short form of the Chinese version of the self-report ver- sions of the Early Adolescence Temperament Questionnaire- Revised (EATQ-R; Putnam et al., 2001) were employed to assess 9- to 15-year-old children’s effortful control (Ellis &
  • 59. Rothbart, 2001). The three subtasks of effort control include attention control (6 items, e.g., “It is easy for me to really con- centrate on homework problems”), inhibitory control (5 items; e.g., “When someone tells me to stop doing something, it is easy for me to stop”), and activation control (5 items; e.g., “If I have a hard assignment to do, I get started right away”). Items were rated on a 5-point Likert-type scale ranging from 1 (almost never true or almost always untrue) to 5 (almost always true). The EATQ-R is a valid and consistent scale employed to assess children and adolescents’ temperament, including effort- ful control. The Cronbach’s alpha values ranged from .67–.76 (Muris & Meesters, 2009). Frustration The anger-frustration subscale of the negative emotionality in EATQ-R was used to measure the negative effects related to the interruption of ongoing tasks or goal blocking in children. The Cronbach’s alpha value of the 7-item test used in the current study was .72. Reading comprehension Reading comprehension was measured using a paper-and-pen- cil task from a published Test of Reading Comprehension (Lin & Chi, 2000), which was designed for pupils in Grades 2–6. It is a standardized test that was published by the Minister of Education in Taiwan. In addition, it is a popular test employed by elementary teachers in Taiwan to assess pupils’ reading comprehension competence. This was a group test that took around 45 min to complete. The reading comprehension test contained six narrative passages and six expositive passages fol- lowed by multiple questions. The test assessed the children’s skills of phonological recoding, syntactic parsing, semantics, comprehending facts explicitly as stated in the text, deriving main ideas from the text, making inferences, and comparing or analyzing the facts stated in the text. The total raw score of the
  • 60. test is 100. The test manual reports internal consistencies rang- ing from .88 to .98 and test–retest reliability of .89. Written expression Written language skills were assessed by a paper-and-pencil task from a published Test of Written Language (Lin & Chi, 2002) designed for pupils in Grades 3–6. This was a group test that took around 1 hr to complete. It is a standardized test that was published by the Minister of Education in Taiwan. It is one of the popular tests used by elementary teachers in Taiwan to assess pupils’ written expression competence. The written lan- guage test consisted of five subtasks, including listening- S.-Y. HUANG AND L.-L. YEH170 writing, sentence combination, correction of characters, and clause and sentence construction. The total raw score of the test is 100. The test manual reports internal consistencies rang- ing from .76 to .97 and test–retest reliabilities of .79–.94. Language academic achievement in terms of semestral grades Individual children’s grades were used to obtain measures of children’s academic competence in the domain of Chinese liter- acy. At the conclusion of the semester in January boundary 2014, we obtained the mid- and final-term grades, and the aver- age of the two examinations served as a composite of each stu- dent’s academic achievement. The semestral grades of Chinese literacy test consisted of word recognition, semantic discrimi- nation of a variety of words, reading comprehension of text- book or passages, the structure and format of articles, and sentence construction. Procedure
  • 61. All participants were recruited from Mandarin-speaking ele- mentary schools in North Taiwan during the 2009 and 2013 fall semester. At Grade 1, the nonverbal IQ of each child was individually assessed using the TONI-3 in school settings. At Grade 5, the children were assessed for Chinese comprehension and written ability. Self-report versions of the EATQ-R for children’s frustration and effortful control were also used. Only the test of nonverbal intelligence and reading comprehension was administered to groups. The average scores of academic achievement of language at Grade 5 were also collected. Analysis Before the hypothesis testing of the research topic, gender dif- ference analysis for all the variables was computed by separate multivariate analyses of variance. Next, the zero-order correla- tions among study variables were examined. Finally, hierarchi- cal regressions were conducted to test whether children’s frustration, overall effortful control, and the three components of effortful control predicted their reading comprehension, written expression, and semestral Chinese literacy test grades after controlling for children’s gender, age, and nonverbal IQ. Preliminary analysis Data were combined before analysis because no significant gen- der difference was found in all variables. Table 1 presents the means, standard deviations, and the Pearson correlation coeffi- cients for all study variables. The IQ score of all the children ranged from 82 to 134, with an average of 102.29 (SD D 10.77), and all the participants’ scores were above ¡1.5 SD, that is, within the normal range. The cohort’s performance of reading comprehension test ranged from 30 to 95, with an average of 76.42 (SD D 11.66). Only one participant had a score lower than the 15th percentile of the norm. However, the score was
  • 62. still above ¡1.5 SD according to the norms for 9-year-olds. Given that EATQ-R is suitable for 9- to 15-year-old preadoles- cents, it was thought that the participants’ reading comprehen- sion would allow them to finish the questionnaire and its results had validity. Correlation analyses The analysis of zero-order correlations showed that children’s age positively correlated with reading comprehension and writ- ten expression, rs(70) D .27 and .31, ps D .022 and .009, respec- tively. In addition, nonverbal IQ positively correlated with written expression and semestral literacy test grades, rs(70) D .23 and .37, ps D .056 and .001, respectively. There was no sig- nificant correlation between frustration and children’s reading comprehension, written expression, and semestral literacy test grades. Effortful control was positively associated with attention as well as inhibitory and activation control, rs(70) D .75, .62, and .76, respectively, ps < .001. Reading comprehension and writ- ten expression were positively related with semestral literacy test grades, rs(70) D .49 and .60, respectively, ps < .001. Effort- ful control was positively correlated with children’s written expression and semestral literacy test grades, rs(70) D .27 and .26, ps D .024 and .03, respectively. Attention control of Table 1. Descriptive statistics and correlations for all variables in this study (N D 72). Variable 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 1. Gender — 2. Age .04 — 3. Nonverbal IQ .21 ¡.15 — 4. Frustration .02 .10 .06 — 5. Effortful control .00 ¡.07 .08 ¡.18 — 6. Attention control ¡.03 ¡.06 .10 ¡.22y .76*** —
  • 63. 7. Inhibitory control .05 ¡.06 .06 ¡.16 .62*** .45*** — 8. Activation control .04 ¡.10 .03 ¡.20 .76*** .68*** .38** — 9. Reading comprehension ¡.17 .27* .20 .00 .21 .17 ¡.01 .17 — 10. Written expression ¡.14 .31** .23y ¡.03 .27* .32** .12 .23y .48*** — 11. Semestral score of literacy ¡.08 .21 .37** ¡.06 .26* .22y .13 .23* .49*** .60*** — Descriptive Statistics M — 10.77 102.29 3.11 3.46 3.49 3.43 3.46 76.42 65.58 88.1 SD — 0.27 10.77 0.77 0.58 0.66 0.67 0.81 11.66 12.38 7.75 yp < .08. �p < .05. ��p < .01. ���p < .001. THE JOURNAL OF EDUCATIONAL RESEARCH 171 effortful control was positively and marginally associated with written expression, r(70) D .32, p < .01, and semestral literacy test grades; r(70) D .22, p D .065. Activation control was mar- ginally and significantly associated positively with written expression and semestral literacy test grades, rs(70) D .23 and .23, ps D .054 and .047, respectively. Regression analyses Three hierarchical regression equations were created to predict each of the three indicators of literacy proficiency, including reading comprehension, written expression, and semestral liter- acy test grades. Given that gender and IQ were previously cor- related with children’s literacy ability, and the children’s ages were correlated with literacy ability, all three variables were entered as control variables at Step 1 in each of the regressions. In Step 2, frustration, overall effortful control, the three compo-
  • 64. nents of effortful control (attention control, inhibitory control, and activation control) were entered. Then, the interaction var- iables of frustration and global effortful control, along with the three components of effortful control, were respectively entered in Step 3. Results Predicting the contribution of frustration and effortful control on reading comprehension Table 2 presents the contribution of frustration, global effortful control, and the three components of effortful control on read- ing comprehension. As can be seen, the control variables of gender, age, and nonverbal IQ made unique contributions to reading ability in any of the four regressions at Step 1. These variables respectively accounted for 19% of the variance, F(3, 68) D 5.24, p D .003. Gender (b D –.24), t(71) D –2.13, p D .037; age (b D .33), t(71) D 2.94, p D .001; and nonverbal IQ (b D .31), t(71) D 2.66, p D .01; significantly positively predicted children’s reading comprehension in the four models. After considering the effects of the three variables, we found that effortful control, attention control, inhibitory control, and activation control had no significant contributions (bs D .19, .15, .01, and .20, respectively; ps > .09), and that frustration had no significant association with reading comprehension. Further results for the reading comprehension model indi- cated that there was a marginally significant two-way interac- tion between frustration and effortful control (b D –.20), t(71) D ¡1.91; DR2 D .04; F(1, 65) D 3.63, p D .061. As depicted in Figure 1, the relation between effortful control and reading comprehension was positive and stronger for lower frustration children (b D .42), t(71) D 2.62, p D .011. However, among children with high levels of frustration, effortful control was
  • 65. not significantly predictive of reading comprehension (b D .14), t(71) D 0.89, p D ns. The results indicated that a low- level, frustration with high effortful control ameliorated children’s reading comprehension. Further results for the reading comprehension model indi- cated that there was a two-way interaction between frustration and attention control (b D –.36), t(71) D –3.48; DR2 D .12, F(1, 65) D 12.10, p < .01. The interaction variable significantly added to the regression model (12%). As depicted in Figure 2, attention control was positively predictive of reading compre- hension among children with low frustration. However, the simple slope analysis showed that such a difference was not sig- nificant (bs D .11 and .15), ts(71) D 0.28 and 1.37, p D ns. Prediction of frustration and effortful control on written expression Written expression was regressed against the variables that were entered in blocks, similar to the reading comprehension variable. Table 3 shows the results of the hierarchical regression analysis for predicting written expression. Similar to the results of reading comprehension, the control variables of gender, age, and nonverbal IQ made unique contributions to written expression in any of the four regressions at Step 1. These mod- els accounted for 22%, F(3, 68) D 6.23, p < .01, of the variance. Gender (b D –.22), t(71) D –2.01, p D .049; age (b D .36), t(71) D 3.35, p D .001; and nonverbal IQ at Grade 1 (b D .32), t(71) D 2.93, p D .005; significantly positively predicted children’s written expression in the four models. After considering the above variables, frustration and global effortful control, attention control, and activation control of
  • 66. effortful control all added a significant amount of variance to these three models (7%, 9%, and 7%; bs D .25, .32, and .26), ts (71) D 2.34, 2.82, and 2.39, ps D .022, .006, and .02, respec- tively. Children who had higher levels of effortful control, attention control, and activation control also had better written expression skills. However, the frustration and inhibitory con- trol did not have a significant effect. Further results from Step 3 for the written expression model indicated that there was no significant two-way interaction between frustration and global effortful control and the three components of effortful control. Prediction of frustration and effortful control on the semestral literacy test grades Table 4 shows the contribution of frustration and effortful con- trol in predicting the semestral literacy test grades. Results showed that children’s age and nonverbal IQ were two major contributors to the children’s semester literacy test grades. However, the children’s gender did not have a significant asso- ciation with their grades. Frustration, inhibitory control, and attention control did not have a significant predictive effect on children’s semestral literacy test grades; in comparison, global effortful control and activation control had significant associa- tion with semestral literacy test grades (bs D .22 and .24), ts (71) D 2.09 and 2.28, ps D .04 and .026, respectively. The two- way interaction effect between frustration and effortful control or any component was not proven. Discussion The goal of this study was to investigate whether children’s per- ceptions of their effortful control and frustration may predict their literacy proficiency. The key findings revealed that children with higher effortful control perception had better literacy abili- ties, particularly writing and language academic achievement.
  • 67. The association of effortful control components with children’s literacy proficiency also proved to be dependent on literacy S.-Y. HUANG AND L.-L. YEH172 Ta b le 2. Pr ed ic ti ng re ad in g co m p re he ns io n fr om ch ild re
  • 88. THE JOURNAL OF EDUCATIONAL RESEARCH 173 indicators. Attention control positively predicted children’s writ- ing ability. Activation control also had a direct association with writing ability and semestral language academic performance after controlling the basic variables. Meanwhile, no significant association of inhibitory control and any indicator of literacy pro- ficiency was found. In addition, dispositional frustration had no direct predictive effect on preadolescents’ literacy proficiency; however, frustration moderated the predictive effect of attention control on children’s reading comprehension. Role of effortful control in the development of literacy proficiency In terms of the first research question, the results showed that preadolescents’ overall perceptions about their behaviors and attention regulation did account for the unique variance in writing- and literacy-related academic achievements in task- directed performance, even when children’s gender, age, and IQ were controlled. In line with previous studies (e.g., Valiente et al., 2008; Zhou et al., 2010), the results implied that effortful control perception was closely associated with children’s ability to perform effectively in school. In addition, consistent with previous studies (e.g., Ylvisaker & DeBonis, 2000), the results indicated that written expression and semestral literacy test seemed more closely associated with intentional control than receptive reading tasks for preadolescents, given that the per- formance on writing and semestral grading both significantly related to global effortful control. In other words, the results
  • 89. suggested that different underlying mechanisms of reading, writing, or taking a literacy exam may require different compo- nents or levels of effortful control to perform well. Association between effortful control components and indicators of literacy proficiency Regarding the second research question, our results showed that the specific component of effortful control was associated with the different indicators of literacy proficiency. However, only attention control and activation control were closely tied to literacy ability for early adolescents. The results implied that during expressive written tasks, if a preadolescent pays enough attention and repeatedly activates his or her focus on the tasks at hand, he or she may have a greater chance of achieving a bet- ter performance in the writing assignment or literacy exam. In addition, a possible reason for the absence of a significant rela- tionship between any literacy indicators and inhibitory control may be the matureness of inhibitory control for high-grade pupils who had already adapted to task-conducting situations (Eisenberg et al., 2005; Williams, Ponesse, Schacha, Logan, & Tannock, 1999); thus, more inhibitory control would not be linked to their performance on either the literacy assessment or literacy exam. It was also possible that inhibitory control had an indirect predictive effect on academic performance through social and motivation processes (Valiente et al., 2008). Relative contribution of dispositional frustration on literacy proficiency Finally, in terms of the third research question, the significant linkage between frustration and attention control, and a mar- ginal moderator of global effortful control on reading compre- hension, were found in the study. Given that inhibitory control and activation control relates to the regulation of impulsive behaviors and those involved in stressful situations, and less
  • 90. related to the adjustment of the emotion itself; therefore, no direct association of frustration was expected. The results sug- gest that anger-frustration related to the disfluency of reading (e.g., Zhou et al., 2010) or children’s goal for reading is mis- matched to their academic assignments and texts (Graesser & D’Mello, 2012), which is more likely to impede the positive association between attention regulation and reading compre- hension. The results support the notion that the indirect model (Blair, 2002) is valuable in understanding the mechanism of which emotion correlates with children’s literacy ability and academic achievement. In addition, the finding also supports the assumptions that the efficiency of effortful control depends on the strength of the dominant response or the strength of emotion reactivity (Rothbart & Hwang, 2005), and that nega- tive emotion may be associated with lower academic achieve- ment by disrupting subject’s cognitive resources (Blair, 2002; Rice et al., 2007). Clinical implications The findings of this study have implications on the importance of facilitating children’s development of effortful control, par- ticularly attention control and activation control so that their 84.38 83.57 80.04 89.69 70 75 80
  • 91. 85 90 95 Low High R ea di ng C om pr eh en si on Effortful Control Frustration: High Frustration: Low Figure 1. Effortful control and reading comprehension for high and low levels of frustration. 86.62 81.41
  • 93. Attention control Frustration: High Frustration: Low Figure 2. Attention control and reading comprehension for high and low levels of frustration. S.-Y. HUANG AND L.-L. YEH174 Ta b le 3. Pr ed ic ti ng w ri tt en ex p re ss io n fr om